- Aug 2015
-
langsci-press.org langsci-press.org
-
is
-
form
-
second
'second oldest' - well, actually it's the oldest because it's just traditional school grammar, as in 19th-century Reed and Kellogg diagramming (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sentence_diagram). Tesniere's main contribution was to unify the analysis by taking the verb as root, instead of treating the top verb and its subject as equals. See attached notes from Percival (1999).
-
would have to be
Why? I don't see why this is more of a problem for DG than for PSG.
-
fronting should betreated by special mechanisms
Precisely as suggested in Word Grammar. As I say, there's not only one version of DG!
-
So, in addition to the dependency structure thatis described in Dependency Syntax, one needs other levels. One level is the level ofsemantics and another one is linearization
I think everyone in DG would agree that syntax and semantics are separate.
-
This is indeed true: Dependency Grammar is well-suited for teaching grammar in introductory classes
-
there is no reading in which the adjunct scopes over each event individually
How about this ?: Robin's goal was to take the same time to do A, B and C; and at last she did it: she did A, did B and did C in exactly 30 seconds each.
-
for
-
a phrase structure componen
Not really 'phrase structure' because the larger unit need not be a phrase.
-
looks rather strange
Precisely - and that's probably why T didn't adopt your analysis!
-
An alternative to such a special treatment
But Tesniere was happy with junction as a special relation, so why are you revising his theory?
-
Groß & Osborne(2009: 80) use the concepts ofchain(calledcatenain later work (Osborne, Putnam & Groß2012))
I think it's misleading to present Gross and Osborne's analysis, as it's neither typical nor particularly successful. Nobody else distinguishes heads and governors in that way, and G&O don't show how 'head' translates into linearization. Nor do they explain how their principle excludes examples such as "great with difficulty", where "with" qualifies as the head of "great difficulty". And their 'g' notation implies the underlying dependency even though their diagram doesn't show it. In contrast, there are well-developed analyses in other theories, including my own Word Grammar analysis which I've published in several places, including a whole chapter of discussion in Hudson 1990, which you mention in your footnote 13.
-
By assigning pronouns the category N such a disjunctive specification is avoided
This (fn 9) is standard Word Grammar, e.g. Hudson 1990:167, Hudson 2007:190.
-
Dependency grammar does not focus on linearization aspects.
This is like saying that dependency grammar doesn't focus on word classes. No 'dependency grammar' discusses nothing but dependencies, just as no PS-based grammar discusses nothing but part-whole relations. A 'dependency grammar' is one that uses dependencies, and the fact is, as you've already said, that different dependency grammars make different assumptions e.g. on linearization. Since everyone agrees that linearization is important, it's just a question of how to handle it, so it must be part of any dependency grammar, whether implicit or explicit. E.g. in Word Grammar (2015) linearization is handled by 'landmark' relations which are separate from dependency relations.
What you can say is that dependency structure, as such, is independent of linearization. Just as with the ID/LP split in GPSG.
-
assume that sub-jects depend on auxiliaries
And Hudson 1990 etc
-
it is assumed
You're suddenly assuming a very particular version of DG. DG is just a tradition, not a theory; it's no more unified than PS-based theories. As soon as you discuss details such as passives, you have to say which version you're discussing. The analysis in Fig 11.7 is very different from the one in Word Grammar.
-
use glue seman-tics
But every dependency-based theory includes a separate semantic structure which dependencies are mapped onto. It's quite easy, especially since the asymmetry of dependency is motivated by meaning, but as you say, the mapping is sensitive to word order.
-
his basically reintroduces the con-cept of constituency into the framework
Not really. Projectivity doesn't invoke a separate phrase node, so the dependent and head aren't part of a larger node, as required by constituency.
-
we do not need to use arrows
You do need them if you recognise mutual dependency!
-
rather than part of speech information
Strictly speaking, the part of speech information is included as well, though sometimes it's omitted just to simplify diagrams. This diagramming system is only used in Word Grammark, I think.
-
English
You should also mention Karlsson's ependency-based Constraint Grammar which as fully functioning parsers for several languages. It's described at ttp://beta.visl.sdu.dk/constraint_grammar.html and the languages and parsers are listed here: http://beta.visl.sdu.dk/constraint_grammar_languages.html . This online system has also been used in school teaching.
-
Hudson(1989)
Hudson 1989 certainly doesn't deserve a mention - it was just a mickey-mouse parser with a tiny vocabulary.
-
(?)
Lafferty, John D., Daniel D. Sleator, and Dennis Grinberg. “A Robust Parsing Algorithm for Link Grammars,” 1995. Sleator, Daniel D., and David Temperley. “Proceedings of the Third International Workshop on Parsing Technologies.” In Parsing English with a Link Grammar, 277–92. Tilburg, 1993
-
dead
-
towards
-
Richard Hudson
Hudson followed Anderson: Anderson, John. The Grammar of Case: Towards a Localistic Theory. . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1971.
-