5,377 Matching Annotations
  1. May 2025
    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) 8 molar urea not only denatures proteins but also denatures DNA. Obviously, this does not affect the ChIP, since antibodies often recognize small linear epitopes and the proteins are crosslinked. However, under high urea conditions the BUR elements should be rendered single-stranded, and one wonders whether this has any effect on the procedure. The authors should alert the reader of these circumstances.

      Thank you for raising this important question about the effects of 8M urea. We have added a brief paragraph explaining this point in the revised manuscript. Despite common misconceptions, 8M urea by itself does not actively convert double-stranded DNA to single-stranded DNA. For this conversion to occur, a heat denaturation step is required. Once DNA is heat-denatured to become single-stranded, urea can maintain this configuration. This is why the addition of 8M urea to acrylamide gel electrophoresis is a standard method for analyzing single-stranded oligonucleotides, but the DNA must first be denatured by heat (Summer et al., J. Vis. Exp. (32), e1485, DOI : 10.3791/1485). This is clearly described in published work comparing the status of DNA with and without heat treatment in an 8M urea-containing buffer (Hegedus et al., Nucl.Acids Res. 2009 (doi:10.1093/nar/gkp539).

      We have additional evidence supporting this conclusion in the context of our urea ultracentrifugation experiment. Both crosslinked and un-crosslinked genomic DNA purified by 8M urea centrifugation can be digested with restriction enzymes, which indicates that the DNA remains double-stranded. For instance, we previously published SATB1 ChIP-3C results using Sau3A-digested DNA after urea purification. In the current paper, we used HindIII to digest urea-purified DNA for urea4C-seq. The BUR reference map can also be generated after restriction digestion of urea-purified DNA and isolating and sequencing SATB1-bound restriction fragments in vitro. If genomic DNA were denatured by 8M urea ultracentrifugation, we would not have been able to digest it with restriction enzymes to obtain these results.

      We have now added a sentence noting that SATB1 is a double-stranded DNA-binding protein that does not bind to single-stranded DNA, as we have previously shown (Dickinson et al., 1992, Ref 32).

      (2) An important conclusion is that urea-ChIP reveals direct DNA binding events, whereas standard ChIP shows indirect binding (which is stripped off by urea). I do not see any evidence for direct binding. At low resolution, predicted BUR elements are enriched in domains where SATB-1 is mapped by urea-ChIP. A statement like 'In a zoomed-in view, covering a 430 kb region, SATB1 sites identified from urea ChIP-seq precisely coincided with BUR peaks' is certainly not correct: most BUR peaks do not show significant SATB-1 binding. The randomly chosen regions shown in Figure 4 – Supplement 1 show how poor the overlap of SATB-1 and BURs is; indeed, they show that SATB-1 binds DNA mostly at non-BUR sites. I see Figure 2D, but such cumulative plots can be highly biased by very few cases. I suggest showing these data in heat maps instead.

      We believe there may be some confusion regarding the interpretation of our figures. Looking at Track 3 (BUR reference map, RED peaks) and urea SATB1 Tracks 4 and 5 (replicas from two independent experiments) in Fig. 2B, the SATB1 peaks detected by urea ChIP-seq do indeed coincide with BUR peaks. In the revised manuscript, we have provided a further ‘zoomed-in’ view to better illustrate this point and also provided the underlying BUR sequence from one of these SATB1-bound regions (Figure 2—supplement figure 1).

      It is true that many more BURs exist than SATB1-bound BURs, especially in gene-poor regions where BURs are clustered. However, from the perspective of SATB1-bound peaks, the majority of these coincide with BURs, as shown by both deepTools analyses and new heatmap, as suggested (Figure 2E, and Figure 7—supplement figure 3).

      The results from our genome-wide quantitative analyses using deepTools to compare peaks from urea SATB1 ChIP-seq data and the BUR reference map shown in Supplementary Tables 1 and 2 are consistent with the heatmap analyses.

      We must apologize for an error in the scaling of the y-axis in Figure 4-supplement figure 1 that likely contributed to some confusion. We have corrected our mistake in the revised manuscript. As we were preparing our figures, when placed in the figure and axes relabeled for legibility, the BUR reference peaks were mislabeled on their y-axis. In the figure the peaks were erroneously labeled on a scale of 0.1-1 read counts/million reads, but the data shown is actually scaled at 0.1 to 2 read counts per million reads. Unfortunately, we did not realize this error and, using the figure as a guide for scaling, provided urea SATB1 ChIP-seq peaks at a scale of 0.1-1 read counts/million reads to match the mislabeled BUR reference track. This had the effect of reducing the signal/noise in the SATB1 ChIP-seq data (Figure 1). We have now standardized the y-axis for fair comparison using a scaling of the y-axis at 0.1-2 for all tracks.  This will more clearly show that there are indeed more BUR peaks than SATB1-bound sites, consistent with our quantitative analysis.

      We hope that these clarifications as well as the added heatmaps and binding site example allay the concerns about the specificity and overlap of SATB1 binding on BURS.

      (3) In Figure 6C 'peaks' are compared. However, looking at Figure 4 - Supplement 1 again it is clear that peak calling can yield a misleading impression. Figure 6D suggests that there are more BURs than SATB-1 peaks but this is not true from looking at the browser.

      We thank the reviewer for this observation. As noted in our response to point 2 above, the inconsistent y-axis scaling in Figure 4-supplement figure 1 created a misleading impression, which we have corrected in the revised manuscript. When properly displayed with consistent y-axis scaling, the browser view aligns with our quantitative data showing that there are indeed many more BURs than SATB1-bound sites. As mentioned under 2 above, we have performed genome-wide quantitative analysis by deepTools (Supplementary Tables 1 and 2) to confirm the results shown by bar graphs in Fig. 6C, 6D and Fig. 2D. 

      In Figure 6C, the bars show the percentage of SATB1-bound peaks in each cell type (denominator) that overlap with confirmed BUR sites in the BUR reference map (numerator). In Figure 6D, we show the percentage of total BUR sites in the BUR reference map (denominator) that are bound by SATB1 from urea ChIP-seq (numerator). To avoid any confusion, we have added brief subtitles to Figures 6C and 6D in the revised manuscript.

      (4) An important conclusion is that urea-ChIP reveals direct DNA binding events, whereas standard ChIP shows indirect binding (which is stripped off by urea). I do not yet see any evidence for direct binding. It cannot be excluded that the binding is RNA-mediated. The authors mention in passing that urea-ChIP material still contains (specific!) RNA. Given that this is a new procedure, the authors should document the RNA content of urea-ChIP and RNase-treat their samples prior to ChIP to monitor an RNA contribution.

      Thank you for raising this important point. The direct binding of SATB1 to BURs is well-established in our previous work. Indeed, this was the main motivation to explore the reason for the lack of evidence for genome-wide SATB1 binding to BURs in the DNA-binding profile by standard ChIP-seq. This has been a major point of confusion for us for many years.

      SATB1 was originally identified through a search for mammalian proteins that could recognize BURs specifically and not just any A+T-rich sequence. The Satb1 gene was originally cloned by an expression cDNA library and encoded SATB1 protein bound the BUR probe but not a mutated AT-rich BUR (control) probe.  Subsequent experiments confirmed that SATB1 specifically binds to many BURs without requiring additional factors. Furthermore, SATB1 recognizes BURs by binding in the minor groove of double-stranded DNA, presumably recognizing the altered phosphate backbone structure of BUR DNA, rather than accessing nucleotide bases (Dickinson et al, 1992).

      We do agree with the reviewer, however, that there is a possibility that RNA can redirect SATB1 to different subsets of BURs and/or to interact indirectly with different regulatory regions depending on cell type or developmental stage. Although urea ultracentrifugation clearly separates most RNA (found in the middle region of the tube) from genomic DNA (pelleted at the bottom) (de Belle et al., 1998), upon crosslinking cells, a small quantity of RNA is found co-pelleted with DNA (our recent unpublished results). This RNA, tightly associated with crosslinked chromatin, may have some impact on SATB1 function.

      Based on our preliminary data, we are currently planning to study the impact of RNA using RNase A as well as by targeting specific RNAs employing an anti-sense approach. We believe that thoroughly addressing the impact of RNA warrants a full paper, including the potential roles of specific non-coding RNAs in SATB1 function, and thus is beyond the scope of the current paper. However, we have now added discussion of this important point in the manuscript.

      (5) An important aspect of the model is that SATB1 tethers active genes to inactive LADs. However, in the 4C experiment the BUR elements used to anchor the looping are both in the accessible, active chromatin domain. If the authors want to maintain their statement, they must show a 4C result that connects the 2 distinct domains and transverses A/B domain boundaries. Currently, the data only show a looping within accessible chromatin.

      We appreciate REVIEWER 1 for bringing up the important point that our model could potentially be interpreted as “SATB1 tethers active genes to inactive LADs.” Since we describe that BURs are enriched in LADs and that SATB1 binds a subset of BURs, readers may assume that we aim to demonstrate, through urea 4C-seq, that SATB1 tethers active genes to transcriptionally-inactive LADs (via BURs). However, this is not our intention in the model (Figure 8). In the experiment we designed for our present study,  we selected BUR-1 and BUR-2 as viewpoints from a non-LAD gene-rich region (inter-LAD). Because these BURs are bound by SATB1, it indicates that these BURs are part of the “hard-to-access” SATB1-rich subnuclear structure, which resists extraction, in contrast to accessible chromatin. Thus, we illustrate in the model that BURs anchored to the SATB1-rich nuclear substructure make contact with accessible chromatin over long distances in a SATB1-dependent manner. Therefore, we do not intend to conclude that SATB1 mediates interactions between LADs and inter-LADs (accessible chromatin) from our current study: this would be a topic for future research. In the original model in the submitted manuscript, we used the terms “inaccessible” and “accessible.” In the revised version, we clarified this in the model by changing “inaccessible” to “SATB1-rich subnuclear structure” and carefully revised  the text in the Figure 8 legend to clarify the model. 

      At this time, we do not know exactly how LADs and SATB1 nuclear architecture are related spatially and functionally. While LADs are mapped as genomic domains in proximity to Lamin B1 by LaminB1-DamID, BURs are mapped at ~300-500 bp resolution by urea ChIP-seq. To gain further insight into this important question, a large body of DNA-FISH and immunoDNA-FISH experiments will be required, comparing different cell types to see whether and how specific BURs move between LADs and SATB1 nuclear architecture. Such experiments may benefit from testing the Gabrg1 and Gabra2 loci, where many BURs are anchored to SATB1 in neurons but not in thymocytes, for instance.  This is included in Discussion in the revised manuscript.

      Regarding the reviewer's second point about showing more extended domains for 4C interactions, we would like to highlight that Figure 5—supplement figure 3 in our submitted manuscript addresses this concern. This figure shows that BUR-interactions extend to multiple gene-rich regions across intervening gene-poor regions. Interestingly, BUR-1 and BUR-2 interactions skip a transcriptionally silent gene-rich region containing olfactory receptor genes but interact with subsequent gene-rich regions containing active genes. These data demonstrate that BUR-interactions do indeed traverse A- and B-compartment boundaries.  In the revised manuscript (in Figure 5—supplement figure 3), we newly added a Lamin B1-DamID (thymocyte) track.  Comparing with LADs, BUR-1 interactions occur mostly in non-LAD regions. Some minor overlap with LADs was detected in high resolution views (not shown). Future experiments testing BUR viewpoints that reside within LADs are required to assess whether SATB1 mediates interactions between B and A compartments.

      (6) The description of the urea-co-immunoprecipitation experiment (Figure 3C) could be improved to make it unequivocally clear that co-binding to chromatin is tested, not protein-protein interaction (which is destroyed by urea).

      Thank you for this helpful suggestion. We have revised the text in the manuscript by stating “Distinct from protein-protein co-immunoprecipitation (co-IP) using whole cell or nuclear extracts, we examined the direct co-binding status on chromatin in vivo of SATB1 and CTCF or cohesin by urea ChIP-Western”.

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) Since SATB1 has been described to interact with beta-catenin, I wonder if the authors have looked at TCF4/TCF7l2 binding patterns and their potential overlap with SATB1 binding patterns. This might appear a trivial request. However, uncontrolled WNT signalling is a major feature of cancer undergoing metastasis - a process that the authors have earlier associated with unscheduled SATB1 expression in triple-negative breast cancer.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting this important point about the potential relationship between SATB1 and TCF4/TCF7l2 binding patterns. Based on published observations with other factors (Rad21, CTCF, BRG1, RUNX) that show substantial overlap with SATB1 in standard ChIP-seq peaks(Kakugawa et al., Cell Rep 19, 1176-1188 (2017). DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.04.038. Poterlowicz et al., PLoS Genet, 2017 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006966), we would anticipate that TCF4 might also show significant overlap with SATB1. An important question is whether the DNA binding profile of TCF4 depends on SATB1.

      We have not yet generated ChIP-seq data for TCF4 in the presence and absence of SATB1, but we agree that such experiments could provide important insights into cancer progression as well as brain function. This represents an interesting direction for future work. We have added this point in our discussion based on your kind suggestion.

      (2) The CTCF sizes indicated in the western blot analyses of Figures 3C and Figure 3 - supplement figure 2 do not display the normal size, which is around 130 kDa. Either the issue is erroneous marking or a so-called salt effect to slow the migration in the gel. Alternatively, it reflects a slower migrating form of CTCF generated by for example PARylation (by PARP1) that is known to approach 180 kDa. It would be useful if the authors could clarify this minor issue.

      We appreciate the reviewer pointing out this discrepancy. As the reviewer correctly noted, CTCF can appear at a higher molecular weight due to post-translational modifications such as PARylation and O-GlcNAcylation, which alter its migration during electrophoresis.

      Upon re-examination of our raw data for Figure 3—supplement figure 2A, we discovered that the marker lane for the CTCF panel was broken, and the 150kDa band was erroneously assigned. This led to the 150kDa marker being placed below the CTCF migration position, which is clearly an error. We thank the reviewer for bringing this to our attention.

      We have checked our other data and consistently observe CTCF migrating below the 150kDa band, similar to the pattern shown on the Abcam website for the antibody we used (ab128873) (Figure 2). For Figure 3-supplement figure 2, we will use a marker lane from a parallel gel with identical composition and run time to correctly indicate the molecular weight. We havealso corrected the marker position in Figure 3C.

      Reviewing Editor (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The introduction states that urea ChIP-seq is "unbiased", which is difficult to unambiguously determine and therefore might be an overstatement. Maybe the authors could consider rephrasing.

      We agree with the reviewer's assessment and have rephrased our description of the urea ChIP-seq method to avoid using the term "unbiased."

      (2) The authors propose that in standard ChIP, most SATB1 is in the insoluble fraction. This seems easy to test and demonstrating it may help to further clarify the differences between the protocols.

      We appreciate this suggestion and would like to clarify our description. What we stated in the manuscript was:

      "We envision that SATB1 bound to inaccessible nuclear regions may be lost in the insoluble fraction."

      This refers specifically to a subpopulation of SATB1 that is bound to the high-salt extraction-resistant nuclear substructure, not to the total SATB1 protein. We also noted elsewhere in the manuscript that:

      "SATB1 proteins are found in high salt-resistant fraction as well as salt-extracted fraction (40). Thus, it is possible that soluble SATB1 may associate with open chromatin."

      Our unpublished results show that SATB1 proteins exist in at least two distinct forms based on protein mobility: SATB1 with high mobility and another with very low or no mobility. While we have identified the SATB1 domain responsible for each of these distinct mobility patterns, we have not yet identified biochemical differences that would allow us to distinguish them conclusively. Therefore, an experiment to test the distribution of SATB1 in soluble versus insoluble fractions would show SATB1 in both fractions but would not necessarily provide information about the functional significance of these different populations. We believe this is an important area for future research and are working to develop tools to specifically distinguish and characterize SATB1 in the soluble versus insoluble fractions.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This work studies representations in a network with one recurrent layer and one output layer that needs to path-integrate so that its position can be accurately decoded from its output. To formalise this problem, the authors define a cost function consisting of the decoding error and a regularisation term. They specify a decoding procedure that at a given time averages the output unit center locations, weighted by the activity of the unit at that time. The network is initialised without position information, and only receives a velocity signal (and a context signal to index the environment) at each timestep, so to achieve low decoding error it needs to infer its position and keep it updated with respect to its velocity by path integration.

      The authors take the trained network and let it explore a series of environments with different geometries while collecting unit activities to probe learned representations. They find localised responses in the output units (resembling place fields) and border responses in the recurrent units. Across environments, the output units show global remapping and the recurrent units show rate remapping. Stretching the environment generally produces stretched responses in output and recurrent units. Ratemaps remain stable within environments and stabilise after noise injection. Low-dimensional projections of the recurrent population activity forms environment-specific clusters that reflect the environment's geometry, which suggests independent rather than generalised representations. Finally, the authors discover that the centers of the output unit ratemaps cluster together on a triangular lattice (like the receptive fields of a single grid cell), and find significant clustering of place cell centers in empirical data as well.

      The model setup and simulations are clearly described, and are an interesting exploration of the consequences of a particular set of training requirements - here: path integration and decodability. But it is not obvious to what extent the modelling choices are a realistic reflection of how the brain solves navigation. Therefore it is not clear whether the results generalize beyond the specifics of the setup here.

      Strengths:

      The authors introduce a very minimal set of model requirements, assumptions, and constraints. In that sense, the model can function as a useful 'baseline', that shows how spatial representations and remapping properties can emerge from the requirement of path integration and decodability alone. Moreover, the authors use the same formalism to relate their setup to existing spatial navigation models, which is informative.

      The global remapping that the authors show is convincing and well-supported by their analyses. The geometric manipulations and the resulting stretching of place responses, without additional training, are interesting. They seem to suggest that the recurrent network may scale the velocity input by the environment dimensions so that the exact same path integrator-output mappings remain valid (but maybe there are other mechanisms too that achieve the same).

      The clustering of place cell peaks on a triangular lattice is intriguing, given there is no grid cell input. It could have something to do with the fact that a triangular lattice provides optimal coverage of 2d space? The included comparison with empirical data is valuable, although the authors only show significant clustering - there is no analysis of its grid-like regularity.

      First of all, we would like to thank the reviewer for their comprehensive feedback, and their insightful comments. Importantly, as you point out, our goal with this model was to build a minimal model of place cell representations, where representations were encouraged to be place-like, but free to vary in tuning and firing locations. By doing so, we could explore what upstream representations facilitate place-like representations, and even remapping (as it turned out) with minimal assumptions. However, we agree that our task does not capture some of the nuances of real-world navigation, such as sensory observations, which could be useful extensions in future work. Then again, the simplicity of our setup makes it easier to interpret the model, and makes it all the more surprising that it learns many behaviors exhibited by real world place cells.

      As to the distribution of phases - we also agree that a hexagonal arrangement likely reflects some optimal configuration for decoding of location.

      And we agree that the symmetry within the experimental data is important; we have revised analyses on experimental phase distributions, and included an analysis of ensemble grid score, to quantify any hexagonal symmetries within the data.

      Weaknesses:

      The navigation problem that needs to be solved by the model is a bit of an odd one. Without any initial position information, the network needs to figure out where it is, and then path-integrate with respect to a velocity signal. As the authors remark in Methods 4.2, without additional input, the only way to infer location is from border interactions. It is like navigating in absolute darkness. Therefore, it seems likely that the salient wall representations found in the recurrent units are just a consequence of the specific navigation task here; it is unclear if the same would apply in natural navigation. In natural navigation, there are many more sensory cues that help inferring location, most importantly vision, but also smell and whiskers/touch (which provides a more direct wall interaction; here, wall interactions are indirect by constraining velocity vectors). There is a similar but weaker concern about whether the (place cell like) localised firing fields of the output units are a direct consequence of the decoding procedure that only considers activity center locations.

      Thank you for raising this point; we absolutely agree that the navigation task is somewhat niche. However, this was a conscious decision, to minimize any possible confounding from alternate input sources, such as observations. In part, this experimental design was inspired by the suggestion that grid cells support navigation/path integration in open-field environments with minimal sensory input (as they could, conceivably do so with no external input). This also pertains to your other point, that boundary interactions are necessary for navigation. In our model, using boundaries is one solution, but there is another way around this problem, which is conceivably better: to path integrate in an egocentric frame, starting from your initial position. Since the locations of place fields are inferred only after a trajectory has been traversed, the network is free to create a new or shifted representation every time, independently of the arena. In this case, one might have expected generalized solutions, such as grid cells to emerge. That this is not the case, seems to suggest that grid cells may somehow not be optimal for pure path integration, or at the very least, hard to learn (but may still play a part, as alluded to by place field locations). We have tried to make these points more evident in the revised manuscript.

      As for the point that the decoding may lead to place-like representations, this is a fair point. Indeed, we did choose this form of decoding, inspired by the localized firing of place cells, in the hope that it would encourage minimally constrained, place-like solutions. However, compared to other works (Sorscher and Xu) hand tuning the functional form of their place cells, our (although biased towards centralized tuning curves) allows for flexible functional forms such as the position of the place cell centers, their tuning width, whether or not it is center-surround activity, and how they should tune to different environments/rooms. This allows us to study several features of the place cell system, such as remapping and field formation. We have revised to make this more clear in the model description.

      The conclusion that 'contexts are attractive' (heading of section 2) is not well-supported. The authors show 'attractor-like behaviour' within a single context, but there could be alternative explanations for the recovery of stable ratemaps after noise injection. For example, the noise injection could scramble the network's currently inferred position, so that it would need to re-infer its position from boundary interactions along the trajectory. In that case the stabilisation would be driven by the input, not just internal attractor dynamics. Moreover, the authors show that different contexts occupy different regions in the space of low-dimensional projections of recurrent activity, but not that these regions are attractive.

      We agree that boundary interactions could facilitate the convergence of representations after noise injection. We did try to moderate this claim by the wording “attractor-like”, but we agree that boundaries could confound this result. We have therefore performed a modified noise injection experiment, where we let the network run for an extended period of time, before noise injection (and no velocity signal), see Appendix Velocity Ablation in the revised text. Notably, representations converge to their pre-scrambled state after noise injection, even without a velocity signal. However, place-like representations do not converge for all noise levels in this case, possibly indicating that boundary interactions do serve an error-correcting function, also. Thank you for pointing this out.

      As for the attractiveness of contexts, we agree that more analyses were required to demonstrate this. We have therefore conducted a supplementary analysis where we run the trained network with a mismatch in context/geometry, and demonstrate that the context signal fixes the representation, up to geometric distortions.

      The authors report empirical data that shows clustering of place cell centers like they find for their output units. They report that 'there appears to be a tendency for the clusters to arrange in hexagonal fashion, similar to our computational findings'. They only quantify the clustering, but not the arrangement. Moreover, in Figure 7e they only plot data from a single animal, then plot all other animals in the supplementary. Does the analysis of Fig 7f include all animals, or just the one for which the data is plotted in 7e? If so, why that animal? As Appendix C mentions that the ratemap for the plotted animal 'has a hexagonal resemblance' whereas other have 'no clear pattern in their center arrangements', it feels like cherrypicking to only analyse one animal without further justification.

      Thank you for pointing this out; we agree that this is not sufficiently explained and explored in the current version. We have therefore conducted a grid score analysis of the experimental place center distributions, to uncover possible hexagonal symmetries. The reason for choosing this particular animal was in part because it featured the largest number of included cells, while also demonstrating the most striking phase distribution, while including all distributions in the supplementary. Originally, this was only intended as a preliminary analysis, suggesting non-uniformity in experimental place field distributions, but we realize that these may all provide interesting insight into the distributional properties of place cells.

      We have explained these choices in the revised text, and expanded analyses on all animals to showcase these results more clearly.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors proposed a neural network model to explore the spatial representations of the hippocampal CA1 and entorhinal cortex (EC) and the remapping of these representations when multiple environments are learned. The model consists of a recurrent network and output units (a decoder) mimicking the EC and CA1, respectively. The major results of this study are: the EC network generates cells with their receptive fields tuned to a border of the arena; decoder develops neuron clusters arranged in a hexagonal lattice. Thus, the model accounts for entorhinal border cells and CA1 place cells. The authors also suggested the remapping of place cells occurs between different environments through state transitions corresponding to unstable dynamical modes in the recurrent network.

      Strengths:

      The authors found a spatial arrangement of receptive fields similar to their model's prediction in experimental data recorded from CA1. Thus, the model proposes a plausible mechanisms to generate hippocampal spatial representations without relying on grid cells. This result is consistent with the observation that grid cells are unnecessary to generate CA1 place cells.

      The suggestion about the remapping mechanism shows an interesting theoretical possibility.

      We thank the reviewer for their kind feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      The explicit mechanisms of generating border cells and place cells and those underlying remapping were not clarified at a satisfactory level.

      The model cannot generate entorhinal grid cells. Therefore, how the proposed model is integrated into the entire picture of the hippocampal mechanism of memory processing remains elusive.

      We appreciate this point, and hope to clarify: From a purely architectural perspective, place-like representations are generated by linear combinations of recurrent unit representations, which, after training, appear border-like. During remapping, the network is simply evaluated/run in different geometries/contexts, which, it turns out, causes the network to exhibit different representations, likely as solutions to optimally encoding position in the different environments. We have attempted to revise the text to make some of these interpretations more clear. We have also conducted a supplementary analysis to demonstrate how representations are determined by the context signal directly, which helps to explain how recurrent and output units form their representations.

      We also agree that our model does not capture the full complexity of the Hippocampal formation. However, we would argue that its simplicity (focusing on a single cell type and a pure path integration task), acts as a useful baseline for studying the role of place cells during spatial navigation. The fact that our model captures a range of place cell behaviors (field formation, remapping and geometric deformation) without grid cells also point to several interesting possibilities, such that grid cells may not be strictly necessary for place cell formation and remapping, or that border cells may account for many of the peculiar behaviors of place cells. However, we wholeheartedly agree that including e.g. sensory information and memory storage/retrieval tasks would prove a very interesting extension of our model to more naturalistic tasks and settings. In fact, our framework could easily accommodate this, e.g. by decoding contexts/observations/memories from the network state, alongside location.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors used recurrent neural network modelling of spatial navigation tasks to investigate border and place cell behaviour during remapping phenomena.

      Strengths:

      The neural network training seemed for the most part (see comments later) well-performed, and the analyses used to make the points were thorough.

      The paper and ideas were well explained.

      Figure 4 contained some interesting and strong evidence for map-like generalisation as environmental geometry was warped.

      Figure 7 was striking, and potentially very interesting.

      It was impressive that the RNN path-integration error stayed low for so long (Fig A1), given that normally networks that only work with dead-reckoning have errors that compound. I would have loved to know how the network was doing this, given that borders did not provide sensory input to the network. I could not think of many other plausible explanations... It would be even more impressive if it was preserved when the network was slightly noisy.

      Thank you for your insightful comments! Regarding the low path integration error, there is a slight statistical signal from the boundaries, as trajectories tend to turn away from arena boundaries. However, we agree, that studying path integration performance in the face of noise would make for a very interesting future development.

      Weaknesses:

      I felt that the stated neuroscience interpretations were not well supported by the presented evidence, for a few reasons I'll now detail.

      First, I was unconvinced by the interpretation of the reported recurrent cells as border cells. An equally likely hypothesis seemed to be that they were positions cells that are linearly encoding the x and y position, which when your environment only contains external linear boundaries, look the same. As in figure 4, in environments with internal boundaries the cells do not encode them, they encode (x,y) position. Further, if I'm not misunderstanding, there is, throughout, a confusing case of broken symmetry. The cells appear to code not for any random linear direction, but for either the x or y axis (i.e. there are x cells and y cells). These look like border cells in environments in which the boundaries are external only, and align with the axes (like square and rectangular ones), but the same also appears to be true in the rotationally symmetric circular environment, which strikes me as very odd. I can't think of a good reason why the cells in circular environments should care about the particular choice of (x,y) axes... unless the choice of position encoding scheme is leaking influence throughout. A good test of these would be differently oriented (45 degree rotated square) or more geometrically complicated (two diamonds connected) environments in which the difference between a pure (x,y) code and a border code are more obvious.

      Thank you for pointing this out. This is an excellent point, that we agree could be addressed more rigorously. Note that there is no position encoding in our model; the initial state of the network is a vector of zeros, and the network must infer its location from boundary interactions and context information alone. So there is no way for positional information to leak through to the recurrent layer directly. However, one possible reason for the observed symmetry breaking, is the fact that the velocity input signal is aligned with the cardinal directions. To investigate this, we trained a new model, wherein input velocities are rotated 45 degrees relative to the horizontal, as you suggest. The results, shown and discussed in appendix E (Learned recurrent representations align with environment boundaries), do indicate that representations are tuned to environment boundaries, and not the cardinal directions, which hopefully improves upon this point.

      Next, the decoding mechanism used seems to have forced the representation to learn place cells (no other cell type is going to be usefully decodable?). That is, in itself, not a problem. It just changes the interpretation of the results. To be a normative interpretation for place cells you need to show some evidence that this decoding mechanism is relevant for the brain, since this seems to be where they are coming from in this model. Instead, this is a model with place cells built into it, which can then be used for studying things like remapping, which is a reasonable stance.

      This is a great point, and we agree. We do write that we perform this encoding to encourage minimally constrained place-like representations (to study their properties), but we have revised to make this more evident.

      However, the remapping results were also puzzling. The authors present convincing evidence that the recurrent units effectively form 6 different maps of the 6 different environments (e.g. the sparsity of the code, or fig 6a), with the place cells remapping between environments. Yet, as the authors point out, in neural data the finding is that some cells generalise their co-firing patterns across environments (e.g. grid cells, border cells), while place cells remap, making it unclear what correspondence to make between the authors network and the brain. There are existing normative models that capture both entorhinal's consistent and hippocampus' less consistent neural remapping behaviour (Whittington et al. and probably others), what have we then learnt from this exercise?

      Thanks for raising this point! We agree that this finding is surprising, but we hold that it actually shows something quite important: that border-type units are sufficient to create place-like representations, and learns several of the behaviors associated with place cells and remapping (including global remapping and field stretching). In other words, a single cell type known to exist upstream of place cells is sufficient to explain a surprising range of phenomena, demonstrating that other cell types are not strictly necessary. However, we agree that understanding why the boundary type units sometimes rate remap, and whether that can be true for some border type cells in the brain (either directly, or through gating mechanisms) would be important future developments. Related to this point, we also expanded upon the influence of the context signal for representation selection (appendix F)

      Concerning the relationship to other models, we would argue that the simplicity of our model is one of its core strengths, making it possible to disentangle what different cell types are doing. While other models, including TEM, are highly important for understanding how different cell types and brain regions interact to solve complex problems, we believe there is a need for minimal, understandable models that allows us to investigate what each cell type is doing, and this is where we believe our work is important. As an example, our model not only highlights the sufficiency of boundary-type cells as generators of place cells, its lack of e.g. grid cells also suggest that grid cells may not be strictly necessary for e.g. open-field/sensory-deprived navigation, as is often claimed.

      One striking result was figure 7, the hexagonal arrangement of place cell centres. I had one question that I couldn't find the answer to in the paper, which would change my interpretation. Are place cell centres within a single clusters of points in figure 7a, for example, from one cell across the 100 trajectories, or from many? If each cluster belongs to a different place cell then the interpretation seems like some kind of optimal packing/coding of 2D space by a set of place cells, an interesting prediction. If multiple place cells fall within a single cluster then that's a very puzzling suggestion about the grouping of place cells into these discrete clusters. From figure 7c I guess that the former is the likely interpretation, from the fact that clusters appear to maintain the same colour, and are unlikely to be co-remapping place cells, but I would like to know for sure!

      This is a good point, and you are correct: one cluster tends to correspond to one unit. To make this more clear, we have revised Fig. 7, so that each decoded center is shaded by unit identity, which makes this more evident. And yes, this is, seemingly in line with some form of optimal packing/encoding of space, yes!

      I felt that the neural data analysis was unconvincing. Most notably, the statistical effect was found in only one of seven animals. Random noise is likely to pass statistical tests 1 in 20 times (at 0.05 p value), this seems like it could have been something similar? Further, the data was compared to a null model in which place cell fields were randomly distributed. The authors claim place cell fields have two properties that the random model doesn't (1) clustering to edges (as experimentally reported) and (2) much more provocatively, a hexagonal lattice arrangement. The test seems to collude the two; I think that nearby ball radii could be overrepresented, as in figure 7f, due to either effect. I would have liked to see a computation of the statistic for a null model in which place cells were random but with a bias towards to boundaries of the environment that matches the observed changing density, to distinguish these two hypotheses.

      Thanks for raising this point. We agree that we were not clear enough in our original manuscript. We included additional analyses in one animal, to showcase one preliminary case of non-uniform phases. To mitigate this, we have performed the same analyses for all animals, and included a longer discussion of these results (included in the supplementary material). We have also moderated the discussion on Ripley’s H to encompass only non-uniformity, and added a grid score analysis to showcase possible rotational symmetries in the data. We hope this gets our findings across more clearly

      Some smaller weaknesses:

      - Had the models trained to convergence? From the loss plot it seemed like not, and when including regularisors recent work (grokking phenomena, e.g. Nanda et al. 2023) has shown the importance of letting the regularisor minimise completely to see the resulting effect. Else you are interpreting representations that are likely still being learnt, a dangerous business.

      Longer training time did not seem to affect representations. However, due to the long trajectories and statefulness involved, training was time-intensive and could become unstable for very long training. We therefore stopped training at the indicated time.

      - Since RNNs are nonlinear it seems that eigenvalues larger than 1 doesn't necessarily mean unstable?

      This is a good point; stability is not guaranteed. We have updated the text to reflect this.

      - Why do you not include a bias in the networks? ReLU networks without bias are not universal function approximators, so it is a real change in architecture that doesn't seem to have any positives?

      We found that bias tended to have a detrimental effect on training, possibly related to the identity initialization used (see e.g. Le et al. 2015), and found that training improved when biases were fixed to zero.

      - The claim that this work provided a mathematical formalism of the intuitive idea of a cognitive map seems strange, given that upwards of 10 of the works this paper cite also mathematically formalise a cognitive map into a similar integration loss for a neural network.

      We agree that other works also provide ways of formalizing this concepts. However, our goal by doing so was to elucidate common features across these seemingly disparate models. We also found that the concept of a learned and target map made it easier to come up with novel models, such as one wherein place cells are constructed to match a grid cell label.

      Aim Achieved? Impact/Utility/Context of Work

      Given the listed weaknesses, I think this was a thorough exploration of how this network with these losses is able to path-integrate its position and remap. This is useful, it is good to know how another neural network with slightly different constraints learns to perform these behaviours. That said, I do not think the link to neuroscience was convincing, and as such, it has not achieved its stated aim of explaining these phenomena in biology. The mechanism for remapping in the entorhinal module seemed fundamentally different to the brain's, instead using completely disjoint maps; the recurrent cell types described seemed to match no described cell type (no bad thing in itself, but it does limit the permissible neuroscience claims) either in tuning or remapping properties, with a potentially worrying link between an arbitrary encoding choice and the responses; and the striking place cell prediction was unconvincingly matched by neural data. Further, this is a busy field in which many remapping results have been shown before by similar models, limiting the impact of this work. For example, George et al. and Whittington et al. show remapping of place cells across environments; Whittington et al. study remapping of entorhinal codes; and Rajkumar Vasudeva et al. 2022 show similar place cell stretching results under environmental shifts. As such, this papers contribution is muddied significantly.

      Thank you for this perspective; we agree that all of these are important works that arrive at complementary findings. We hold that the importance of our paper lies in its minimal nature, and its focus on place cells, via a purpose-built decoding that enables place-like representations. In doing so, we can point to possibly under explored relationships between cell types, in particular place cells and border cells, while challenging the necessity of other cell types for open-field navigation (i.e. grid cells). In addition, our work points to a novel connection between grid cells, place cells and even border cells, by way of the hexagonal arrangement of place unit centers. However, we agree that expanding our model to include more biologically plausible architectures and constraints would make for a very interesting extension in the future.

      Thank you again for your time, as well as insightful comments.  

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Even after reading Methods 5.3, I found it hard to understand how the ratemap population vectors that produce Fig 3e and Fig 5 are calculated. It's unclear to me how there can be a ratemap at a single timestep, because calculating a ratemap involves averaging the activity in each location, which would take a whole trajectory and not a single timestep. But I think I've understood from Methods 5.1 that instead the ratemap is calculated by running multiple 'simultaneous' trajectories, so that there are many visited locations at each timestep. That's a bit confusing because as far as I know it's not a common way to calculate ratemaps in rodent experiments (probably because it would be hard to repeat the same task 500 times, while the representations remain the same), so it might be worth explaining more in Methods 5.3.

      We understand the confusion, and have attempted to make this more clear in the revised manuscript. We did indeed create ratemaps over many trajectories for time-dependent plots, for the reasons you mentioned. We also agree that this would be difficult to do experimentally, but found it an interesting way to observe convergence of representations in our simulated scenario.

      Fig 3b-d shows multiple analyses to support output unit global remapping, but no analysis to support the claim that recurrent units remap by rate changes. The examples in Fig 3ai look pretty convincing, but it would be useful to also have a more quantitative result.

      We agree, and only showed that units turn off/become silent using ratemaps. We have therefore added an explicit analysis, showcasing rate remapping in recurrent units (see appendix G; Recurrent units rate remap)

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Some parts of the current manuscript are hard to follow. Particularly, the model description is not transparent enough. See below for the details.

      Major comments:

      (1) Mathematical models should be explained more explicitly and carefully. I had to guess or desperately search for the definitions of parameters. For instance, define the loss function L in eq.(1). Though I can assume L represents the least square error (in A.8), I could not find the definition in Model & Objective. N should also be defined explicitly in equation (3). Is this the number of output cells?

      Thank you for pointing this out, we have revised to make it more clear.

      (2) In Fig. 1d, how were the velocity and context inputs given to individual neurons in the network? The information may be described in the Methods, but I could not identify it.

      This was described in the methods section (Neural Network Architecture and Training), but we realize that we used confusing notation, when comparing with Fig. 1d. We have therefore changed the notation, and it should hopefully be clearer now. Thanks for pointing out this discrepancy.

      (3) I took a while to understand equations (3) and (4) (for instance, t is not defined here). The manuscript would be easier to read if equations (5) and (6) are explained in the main text but not on page 18 (indeed, these equations are just copies of equations 3 and 4). Otherwise, the authors may replace equations (3) and (4) with verbal explanations similar to figure legend for Fig. 1b.

      (4) Is there any experimental evidence for uniformly strong EC-to-CA1 projections assumed in the non-trainable decoder? This point should be briefly mentioned.

      Thank you for raising this point. The decoding from EC (the RNN) to CA1 (the output layer) consists of a trainable weight matrix, and may thus be non-uniform in magnitude. The non-trainable decoding acts on the resulting “CA1” representation only. We hope that improvements to the model description also makes this more evident.  

      (5) The explanation of Fig. 3 in the main text is difficult to follow because subpanels are explained in separate paragraphs, some of which are very short, as short as just a few lines.

      This presentation style makes it difficult to follow the logical relationships between the subpanels. This writing style is obeyed throughout the manuscript but is not popular in neuroscience.

      Thanks for pointing this out, we have revised to accommodate this.

      (6) Why do field centers cluster near boundaries? No underlying mechanisms are discussed in the manuscript.

      This is a good point; we have added a note on this; it likely reflects the border tuning of upstream units.

      (7) In Fig. 4, the authors presented how cognitive maps may vary when the shape and size of open arenas are modified. The results would be more interesting if the authors explained the remapping mechanism. For instance, on page 8, the authors mentioned that output units exhibit global remapping between contexts, whereas recurrent units mainly rate remapping.

      Why do such representational differences emerge?

      We agree! Thanks for raising this point. We have therefore expanded upon this discussion in section 2.4.

      (8) In the first paragraph of page 10, the authors stated ".. some output units display distinct field doubling (see both Fig. 4c), bottom right, and Fig. 4d), middle row)". I could not understand how Fig. 4d, middle row supports the argument. Similarly, they stated "..some output units reflect their main boundary input (with greater activity near one boundary)." I can neither understand what the authors mean to say nor which figures support the statement. Please clarify.

      This is a good point, there was an identifier missing; we have updated to refer to the correct “magnification”. Thanks!

      (9) The underlying mechanism of generating the hexagonal representation of output cells remains unclear. The decoder network uses a non-trainable decoding scheme based on localized firing patterns of output units. To what extent does the hexagonal representation depend on the particular decoding scheme? Similarly, how does the emergence of the hexagonal representation rely on the border representation in the upstream recurrent network? Showing several snapshots of the two place representations during learning may answer these questions.

      This is an interesting point, and we have added some discussion on this matter. In particular, we speculate whether it’s an optimal configuration for position reconstruction, which is demanded by the task and thus highly likely dependent on the decoding scheme. We have not reached a conclusive method to determine the explicit dependence of the hexagonal arrangement on the choice of decoding scheme. Still, it seems this would require comparison with other schemes. In our framework, this would require changing the fundamental operation of the model, which we leave as inspiration for future work. We have also added additional discussion concerning the relationship between place units, border units, and remapping in our model. As for exploring different training snapshots, the model is randomly initialized, which suggests that earlier training steps should tend to reveal unorganized/uninformative phase arrangements, as phases are learned as a way of optimizing position reconstruction. However, we do call for more analysis of experimental data to determine whether this is true in animals, which would strongly support this observation. We also hope that our work inspires other models studying the formation and remapping of place cells, which could serve as a starting point for answering this question in the future.

      (10) Figure 7 requires a title including the word "hexagonal" to make it easier to find the results demonstrating the hexagonal representations. In addition, please clarify which networks, p or g, gave the results shown here.

      We agree, and have added it!

      Minor comments:

      (11) In many paragraphs, conclusions appear near their ends. Stating the conclusion at the beginning of each paragraph whenever possible will improve the readability.

      We have made several rewrites to the manuscript, and hope this improves readability.

      (12) Figure A4 is important as it shows evidence of the CA1 spatial representation predicted by the model. However, I could not find where the figure is cited in the manuscript. The authors can consider showing this figure in the main text.

      We agree, and we have added more references to the experimental data analyses in the main text, as well as expanded this analysis.

      (13) The main text cites figures in the following format: "... rate mapping of Fig. 3a), i), boundary ...." The parentheses make reading difficult.

      We have removed the overly stringent use of double parentheses, thanks for letting us know.

      (14) It would be nice if the authors briefly explained the concept of Ripley's H function on page 14.

      Yes, we have added a brief descriptor.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Review 1:

      Weaknesses:

      The weaknesses of the study also stem from the methodological approach, particularly the use of whole-brain Calcium imaging as a measure of brain activity. While epilepsy and seizures involve network interactions, they typically do not originate across the entire brain simultaneously. Seizures often begin in specific regions or even within specific populations of neurons within those regions. Therefore, a whole-brain approach, especially with Calcium imaging with inherited limitations, may not fully capture the localized nature of seizure initiation and propagation, potentially limiting the understanding of Galanin's role in epilepsy.

      We agree with the reviewers that the whole brain imaging approach is both a strength and a weakness. This manuscript and our previously published paper (Hotz et al., 2022) show indeed that the seizures have a initiation point and spread throughout the brain, interestingly affecting the telencephalon last. Localized seizure initiation was not the scope of this manuscript, however also here we would have to rely on imaging techniques. Using cell type specific drivers for specific neuronal subpopulation are an interesting approach, but outside of the scope of this study. An interesting approach would also include a more detailed analysis of glia in the context of epilepsy.

      Furthermore, Galanin's effects may vary across different brain areas, likely influenced by the predominant receptor types expressed in those regions. Additionally, the use of PTZ as a "stressor" is questionable since PTZ induces seizures rather than conventional stress. Referring to seizures induced by PTZ as "stress" might be a misinterpretation intended to fit the proposed model of stress regulation by receptors other than Galanin receptor 1 (GalR1).

      We also agree, that a more regional approach, after having more reliable information on the expression domains of the different galanin receptors, including more information on their respective role, is an important future research direction.

      The description of the EAAT2 mutants is missing crucial details. EAAT2 plays a significant role in the uptake of glutamate from the synaptic cleft, thereby regulating excitatory neurotransmission and preventing excitotoxicity. Authors suggest that in EAAT2 knockout (KO) mice galanin expression is upregulated 15-fold compared to wild-type (WT) mice, which could be interpreted as galanin playing a role in the hypoactivity observed in these animals.

      However, the study does not explore the misregulation of other genes that could be contributing to the observed phenotype. For instance, if AMPA receptors are significantly downregulated, or if there are alterations in other genes critical for brain activity, these changes could be more important than the upregulation of galanin. The lack of wider gene expression analysis leaves open the possibility that the observed hypoactivity could be due to factors other than, or in addition to, galanin upregulation.

      We are in the process of preparing a manuscript describing a more detailed gene expression study of this and a chemically induced seizure model. Surprisingly we did not observe strong effects on glutamate receptor related genes. This does not preclude and indeed we deem it likely that additional factors play a role, e.g. other neuropeptides.

      Moreover, the observation that in double KO mice for both EAAT2 and galanin there was little difference in seizure susceptibility compared to EAAT2 KO mice alone further supports the idea that galanin upregulation might not be the reason to the observed phenotype. This indicates that other regulatory mechanisms or gene expressions might be playing a more pivotal role in the manifestation of hypoactivity in EAAT2 mutants.

      Yes, we agree that galanin is likely not the only player. This warrants further investigations.

      These methodological shortcomings and conceptual inconsistencies undermine the perceived strengths of the study, and hinders understanding of Galanin's role in epilepsy and stress regulation.

      Review 2:

      Previous concerns about sex or developmental biological variables were addressed, as their model's seizure phenotype emerges rapidly and long prior to the establishment of zebrafish sexual maturity. However, in the course of re-review, some additional concerns (below) were detected that, if addressed, could further improve the manuscript. These concerns relate to how seizures were defined from the measurement of fluorescent calcium imaging data. Overall, this study is important and convincing, and carries clear value for understanding the multifaceted functions that neuronal galanin can perform under homeostatic and disease conditions.

      We are pleased that we could dispel the initial concerns.

      Additional Concerns:

      - The authors have validated their ability to measure behavioral seizures quantitatively in their 2022 Glia paper but the information provided on defining behavioral seizures was limited. The definition of behavioral seizure activity is not expanded upon in this paper, but could provide detail about how the behavioral seizures relate to a seizure detected via calcium imaging.

      In this paper we indeed do not address behavioral seizures but focus completely on neuronal seizures as defined in the material and methods section (“seizures were defined as calcium fluctuations reaching at least 100% of ΔF/F0 in the whole brain.”). Epileptic seizures in zebrafish, either evoked by pharmacological means or the result of genetic mutations, evoke stereotyped locomotor behavior in zebrafish as described in multiple publications (e.g. Baraban et al., 2005, Berghmans et al., 2007, Baxendale et al., 2012 and references therein).

      - Related to the previous point, for the calcium imaging, the difference between an increase in fluorescence that the authors think reflects increased neuronal activity and the fluorescence that corresponds to seizures is not very clear. This detail is necessary because exactly when the term "seizure" describes a degree of increased activity can be difficult to distinguish objectively.

      In our material and methods section, we describe our working definition of a seizure. Seizures are easily distinguished from increased activity by being synchronized.

      - The supplementary movies that were added were very useful, but raised some questions. For example, what brain regions were pulsating? What areas seemed to constantly exhibit strong fluorescence and was this an artifact? It seemed that sometimes there was background fluorescence in the body. Perhaps an anatomical diagram could be provided for the readers. In addition, there were some movies with much greater fluorescence changes - are these the seizures? These are some reasons for our request for clarified definitions of the term "seizure".

      The ”pulsating” (or “flickering”) brain activity is spontaneous neuronal activity. Some areas may appear to be more active, probably by a denser packing of neurons and intrinsically more spontaneous neuronal activity. However, since we only use normalized data, this does not affect our measurements.

      - While it is not critical to change, I will again note the possible confusion that the use of the word "sedative" in this context may cause. However, I do understand this is a stylistic choice.

      - Supplementary Figure 1B: the N values along the x-axis appear to have been duplicated and the duplications are offset and overlapping with one another by mistake.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have corrected the figure accordingly.

      Review 3:

      (1) Although the relationship between galanin and brain activity during interictal or seizure-free periods was clear, the revised manuscript still lacks mechanistic insight in the role of galanin during seizure-like activity induced by PTZ.

      We agree that the mechanistic role of galanin still needs to be defined. The role is more complex that we expected, mainly due to its negative feedback properties. A complete mechanistic understanding will require a number of additional studies and is unfortunately outside of the scope of this manuscript.

      (2) The revised manuscript continues to heavily rely on calcium imaging of different mutant lines. Confirmation of knockouts has been provided with immunostaining in a new supplementary figure. Additional methods could strengthen the data, translational relevance, and interpretation (e.g., acute pharmacology using galanin agonists or antagonists, brain or cell recordings, biochemistry, etc).

      Cell recordings and biochemistry is challenging in the small larval zebrafish brain. We deem the genetic manipulations that we describe to be more informative than pharmacological experiments due to specificity issues.

    1. Author response

      eLife Assessment

      The authors investigated KLF Transcription Factor 16 (KLF16) as an inhibitor of osteogenic differentiation, which plays a critical role in bone development, metabolism and repair. The results of the study are valuable as they could help to facilitate future research on the regulation of osteogenesis in vitro and in vivo. However, the evidence overall is incomplete, as validation by knockout mouse models would help to strengthen the conclusions.

      We appreciate the editors’ evaluation and recognition of the importance of our research. The primary goal and value of our study is to provide robust bioinformatics analyses of 20 independent iPSC lines, which can lead to the identification of novel genes involved in osteogenic differentiation. The identification of KLF16 serves to illustrate this goal. A thorough analysis of the function of any single gene both in vitro and in vivo is beyond the initial scope of this study. To validate KLF16’s inhibitory role in osteogenic differentiation, we provided evidence showing overexpression of Klf16 suppressed osteogenic differentiation in vitro, and Klf16<sup>+/-</sup> mice exhibited enhanced bone mineral content and density in vivo.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Ru and colleagues investigated regulatory gene interactions during osteogenic differentiation. By profiling transcriptomic changes during mesenchymal stem cell differentiation, they identified KLF16 as a key transcription factor that inhibits osteogenic differentiation and mineralization. It was found that overexpression of KLF16 suppressed osteogenesis in vitro, while Klf16<sup>+/-</sup> mice exhibited enhanced bone density, underscoring its regulatory role in bone formation.

      Strengths:

      (1) Bioinformatics is strong and comprehensive.

      (2) Identification of KLF16 in osteoblast differentiation is exciting and innovative.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments on our bioinformatic analyses of MSC osteogenic differentiation and the identification of KLF16 as a new osteogenesis regulator. The differentiation of iPSC-derived MSCs to OBs serves as a valuable model for investigating gene expression and regulatory networks in osteogenic differentiation. This study provides insights into the complex and dynamic regulation of the transcriptomic landscape in osteogenic differentiation and supplies a foundational resource for additional investigation into normal bone formation and the mechanisms underlying pathological conditions.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The mechanism of KLF16 function is not studied.

      (2) Studies of KLF16 in bone development, from both in vitro and in vivo perspectives, are descriptive.

      Our study aims to apply rigorous bioinformatic analyses of 20 iPSC lines to identify novel genes involved in osteogenic differentiation. With this strategy, we successfully identified KLF16 as a regulator of osteogenic differentiation. We validated this with both in vitro and in vivo models even though we had limited availability of Klf16 knockout mice when the study was conducted. We demonstrated that overexpression of Klf16 suppressed osteogenesis in vitro, while Klf16<sup>+/-</sup> mice exhibited increased bone mineral density, trabecular number, and cortical bone area, highlighting its role in bone formation. With these mice now available, further investigation into the mechanism of KLF16's function is possible.

      (3) Findings in bioinformatics analysis are mostly redundant with previous studies in the field, and can be simplified.

      We compared our bulk RNA-seq data with our previously published single-cell RNA-seq (scRNA-seq) data generated from iPSC-induced cells during osteogenic differentiation (Housman et al., 2022). The purpose is to corroborate the expression patterns of the genes we focused on during osteogenic differentiation. We found similar differential expression patterns in a pseudobulk analysis of the scRNA-seq data, even though there are significant differences between these two studies, including: cell culture conditions, sequencing approaches (bulk vs. single cell), goals of the studies (key TF drivers of osteoblast differentiation vs. mapping differentiation stages and inter-species gene programs in human and chimp), and findings (identification of TFs vs. identification of interspecific regulatory differences) .

      Importantly, we performed network analyses to identify key transcription factors, which were not redundant with previous studies. We constructed a transcription factor regulatory network analysis during human osteogenic differentiation, and identified a network organized into five interactive modules. The most exciting finding was the identification of KLF16 as one of the strongest regulators in Module 5 (Figure 3), which previously was not demonstrated to be involved in bone formation. We also demonstrated known TF genes regulating osteogenic differentiation in these modules, and performed gene ontology (GO) and reactome pathway (RP) analyses for regulatory functions and pathways specific to each module. To clarify that our findings do not overlap with previous studies, we will revise the manuscript focusing on Module 5 and simplify the description of the bioinformatics analysis as the reviewer suggested.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In their manuscript with the title "Integrated transcriptomic analysis of human induced pluripotent stem cell (iPSC)-derived osteogenic differentiation reveals a regulatory role of KLF16", Ru et al. have analyzed the gene expression changes during the osteogenic differentiation of iPSC-derived mesenchymal stem/stromal cells into preosteoblasts and osteoblasts. As part of the computational analyses, they have investigated the transcription factor regulatory network mediating this differentiation process, which has also led to the identification of the transcription factor KLF16. Overexpression experiments in vitro and the analysis of heterozygous KLF16 knockout mice in vivo indicate that KLF16 is an inhibitor of osteogenic differentiation.

      The integrated analysis of iPSC bulk transcriptomic data is a major strength of the study, and it is also great that the authors provide deeper functional characterization of the transcription factor KLF16, one of the newly identified candidate regulators of osteogenic differentiation.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s summary and comments on the strength of our bioinformatic analyses of iPSC/MSC osteogenic differentiation and the deep functional characterization of the KLF16, as well as the novelty of our findings.

      However, characterization of KLF16 expression in the mouse and validation of the knockout model are currently lacking. Alternative explanations for the mutant phenotype should be considered to improve the strength of the conclusions.

      If all issues can be addressed, the study would provide an important resource for the field that would facilitate future research on the regulation of osteogenesis in vitro and in vivo, with potential implications for preclinical and clinical research as well as bioengineering.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s valuable suggestions. Klf16 is highly expressed in mandibular, maxillary and tail mesenchyme at embryonic Day 12 (D'Souza et al., 2002), indicating its role in early bone development. We will further characterize the expression of Klf16 in mice, especially in the developing bones.

      We identified Klf16 as a potential regulator of osteogenic differentiation, and then validated this with both in vitro and in vivo models. Overexpression of Klf16 suppressed osteogenesis in vitro, and Klf16<sup>+/-</sup> mice showed increased bone mineral content and density, indicating its regulatory role in bone formation. We agree with the reviewer that the bone phenotypes of Klf16 knockout mice potentially can be affected by other factors in addition to osteogenic differentiation. As both bone formation and resorption are critical for bone development, we evaluated osteoclastogenesis in the Klf16<sup>+/-</sup> mice by analyzing the expression of osteoclast marker CALCR and regulator RANKL in the femurs of the Klf16<sup>+/-</sup> mice. Neither CALCR nor RANKL decreased in the bone of Klf16<sup>+/-</sup> mice, indicating that osteoclastogenesis is not decreased; therefore, increased bone mineral content and density in the mutant mice is more likely attributed to enhanced bone formation rather than reduced resorption by osteoclasts. Additionally, we will discuss other alternative explanations for the bone phenotypes of Klf16 knockout mice as suggested by the reviewer.

      References

      D'Souza, U. M., Lammers, C.-H., Hwang, C. K., Yajima, S. and Mouradian, M. M. (2002). Developmental expression of the zinc finger transcription factor DRRF (dopamine receptor regulating factor). Mechanisms of Development 110, 197-201.

      Housman, G., Briscoe, E. and Gilad, Y. (2022). Evolutionary insights into primate skeletal gene regulation using a comparative cell culture model. PLOS Genetics 18, e1010073-e1010073.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We thank all the reviewers for their time and valuable feedback, which helped us improve our manuscript. Based on the comments, we have made several critical changes to the revised manuscript.

      (1) We have changed our threshold for detecting freezing epochs from 1 cm/s to 0 cm/s in this revised manuscript. This change allows us to capture periods when animals are completely still on the treadmill, better matching the "true freezing" behavior seen in freely moving set-ups. We have added a new supplementary video (Supplementary Video 2) that better demonstrates the freezing response we observe. All results and figures in the revised manuscript reflect this updated threshold (Figure 2-6, Supplementary Figures 16, Tables 1-6). Our main findings remain robust, demonstrating that freezing serves as a reliable conditioned response in our paradigms, comparable to freely moving animals. Specifically, freezing behavior increased reliably in the fear-conditioned environment following CFC across all paradigms. We have also added data from a no-shock control group (Supplementary Figure 2) which, when compared to the conditioned group, shows that freezing responses in the conditioned group result from fear conditioning rather than immobility. We do observe other avoidance behaviors unique to our treadmill-based task— such as hesitation, backward movement, and slow crawls. These conditioned behaviors are captured through a separate metric: the time taken to complete a lap.

      (2) As suggested by the reviewers, we have separately analyzed fear discrimination and extinction dynamics across recall days (Supplementary Figures 2, 5 and 6, Table 1-6). To assess fear discrimination, we use within-group comparisons to evaluate how well animals differentiate between the two VRs across days. For extinction, we use within-VR comparisons to examine freezing dynamics over time. Freezing across recall days is compared to baseline freezing (pre-conditioning) using a Linear Mixed Effects model (Tables 1-6), with recall days as fixed effects and mouse as a random effect, using baseline freezing as the reference.

      (3) We have expanded the behavioral dataset in Paradigm 1 to investigate the effect of shock amplitude on the conditioned fear response (Supplementary Figure 2 C-E). Consistent with findings in freely moving animals, our data show that increasing shock intensity from 0.6 mA to 1.0 mA leads to stronger freezing. For the revised manuscript, we specifically increased the sample size in the 0.6 mA group (n = 8) in Paradigm 1, as this intensity is used in Paradigm 3. These additional data demonstrate that combining a lower shock amplitude with shorter inter-shock intervals and retaining the tail-coat during recall can enhance freezing, suggesting that these parameters help compensate for lower shock intensity.

      (4) We have added more sample sizes to the imaging dataset (now n = 8, Figures 7-8).

      Finally, we acknowledge that many aspects of this paradigm still require optimization. The headfixed CFC paradigm is in its early stages compared to the decades of research dedicated to understanding fear learning parameters in freely moving CFC paradigms. While there are numerous parameters that could be tested—both those identified through our own discussions and those raised by the reviewers—it is not feasible for a single lab to conduct a full evaluation of all the possible factors that could influence CFC in the head-fixed prep. A key limitation is that our approach requires robust navigation behavior in the VR without rewards, which requires weeks of training per mouse. It also necessitates larger sample sizes at the outset as not all animals will make it through our behavioral criteria required for CFC. Another important consideration is scalability. Unlike freely moving CFC paradigms, which allow parallel testing of many animals with minimal pre-training, the VR-CFC setup requires several weeks of behavior training and involves a more complex integration of hardware and software to accurately track behavior in virtual space. The number of VR rigs that can be operated simultaneously in a single lab is often limited, making high-throughput testing more challenging. These factors mean that the testing of a single parameter in a group of animals requires approximately 3–4 months to complete. Despite these constraints, we are committed to continue refining this paradigm over time. With this manuscript, our main aim was to provide a detailed framework, initial parameters, and evidence for conditioned behavior in the head-fixed preparation. By doing so, we hope to facilitate the adoption of this paradigm by researchers interested in studying the neural correlates of learning and memory using multiphoton imaging and stimulation techniques. This approach enables investigations that are not possible in freely moving animals, while the presence of freezing as a conditioned response allows for direct comparisons to the extensive body of work done in freely moving paradigms. Moving forward, we anticipate that optimizing this paradigm and identifying the key parameters that drive learning will be a collaborative, community-led effort.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors set out to develop a contextual fear learning (CFC) paradigm in head-fixed mice that would produce freezing as the conditioned response. Typically, lick suppression is the conditioned response in such designs, but this (1) introduces a potential confounding influence of reward learning on neural assessments of aversion learning and (2) does not easily allow comparison of head-fixed studies with extensive previous work in freely moving animals, which use freezing as the primary conditioned response.

      The first part of this study is a report on the development and outcomes of 3 variations of the CFC paradigm in a virtual reality environment. The fundamental design is strong, with headfixed mice required to run down a linear virtual track to obtain a water reward. Once trained, the water reward is no longer necessary and mice will navigate virtual reality environments. There are rigorous performance criteria to ensure that mice that make it to the experimental stage show very low levels of inactivity prior to fear conditioning. These criteria do result in only 40% of the mice making it to the experimental stage, but high rates of activity in the VR environment are crucial for detecting learning-related freezing. It is possible that further adjustments to the procedure could improve attrition rates.

      We acknowledge that further adjustments to the procedure could improve attrition rates, and we will continue to work on improving the paradigm.

      Paradigm versions 1 and 2 vary the familiarity of the control context while paradigm versions 2 and 3 vary the inter-shock interval. Paradigm version 1 is the most promising, showing the greatest increase in conditioned freezing (~40%) and good discrimination between contexts (delta ~15-20%). Paradigm version 2 showed no clear evidence of learning - average freezing at recall day 1 was not different than pre-shock freezing. First-lap freezing showed a difference, but this single-lap effect is not useful for many of the neural circuit questions for which this paradigm is meant to facilitate. Also, the claim that mice extinguished first-lap freezing after 1 day is weak. Extinction is determined here by the loss of context discrimination, but this was not strong to begin with. First-lap freezing does not appear to be different between Recall Day 1 and 2, but this analysis was not done.

      This is an important point. Following reviewer suggestions, we have replotted our figures for all paradigms to show within-VR freezing (see Supplementary Figures 2, 5 and 6) as the appropriate method for quantifying fear extinction across days. Using an LME model (Tables 16), we quantify freezing during recall days against baseline freezing levels measured before fear conditioning within each VR. In Paradigm 2, while some fear discrimination persists across days, extinction does occur rapidly. After the first lap in the CFC VR, we observed no significant differences in freezing compared to the baseline. These results are shown in the revised Supplementary Figure 5, and the revised text is in lines 393-399.

      Paradigm version 3 has some promise, but the magnitude of the context discrimination is modest (~10% difference in freezing). Thus, further optimization of the VR CFC will be needed to achieve robust learning and extinction. This could include factors not thoroughly tested in this study, including context pre-exposure timing and duration and shock intensity and frequency.

      We acknowledge that many aspects of this paradigm still need optimization, as virtual reality CFC is in its early stages, and we have not explored all of the parameter space. We describe above the reasoning for this. However, for this revised version of the paper we have added new behavioral data (Supplementary Figure 2 C-E) showing that increasing shock intensities from 0.6 mA to 1 mA enhances freezing, both in the first lap and on average. There are of course many other parameters that are likely important, like the ones pointed out here by the reviewer, but exploring the entire parameter space will take many years and will likely require many labs. The purpose of this paper is to show that VR-CFC fundamentally works and is a starting point from which the field can build on. We have now pointed out in the introduction (lines 54-58) and discussion (lines 730-737, 810-814) that there remains significant scope for improving this paradigm and optimizing parameters in the future.

      The second part of the study is a validation of the head-fixed CFC VR protocol through the demonstration that fear conditioning leads to the remapping of dorsal CA1 place fields, similar to that observed in freely moving subjects. The results support this aim and largely replicate previous findings in freely moving subjects. One difference from previous work of note is that VR CFC led to the remapping of the control environment, not just the conditioning context. The authors present several possible explanations for this lack of specificity to the shock context, further underscoring the need for further refinement of the CFC protocol before it can be widely applied. While this experiment examined place cell remapping after fear conditioning, it did not attempt to link neural activity to the learned association or freezing behavior.

      This is an interesting observation. We think that the remapping observed in the control context likely occurred due to the absence of reward in a previously rewarded environment. Our prior work has demonstrated that removal of reward causes increased remapping (Krishnan et al., 2022, Krishnan and Sheffield, 2023). In other words, the continued presence of reward within an environment stabilizes CA1 place fields. The Moita et al. (2004) paper, which showed remapping only in the fear conditioned context and not in the control context, provided rats with food pellets throughout the experimental session in both the control and conditioned context— likely to increase exploration necessary for identifying place cells. The presence of reward in the Moita et al experiment could explain the minimal remapping observed in their control context compared to our control context which lacked reward. Another possibility could lie in the differences in the intervals between place cell activity recordings in our study and that of Moita et al. While Moita et al. separated their recordings by just one hour, our recordings were separated by a full day, with a sleep period in between. The absence of sleep and the shorter time interval between conditioning and retrieval sessions in their study could explain the minimal remapping observed by Moita et al. compared to our findings. We have now addressed this discrepancy explicitly in lines 596-606.

      Although we agree with the reviewer that it would be informative to perform analysis of how neural activity correlates with freezing responses, we think this warrants its own stand-alone manuscript as the neural dynamics and methods to appropriately analyze them are complicated. We are in the midst of analyzing this data further and will present these findings in a separate publication.

      In summary, this is an important study that sets the initial parameters and neuronal validation needed to establish a head-fixed CFC paradigm that produces freezing behaviors. In the discussion, the authors note the limitations of this study, suggest the next steps in refinement, and point to several future directions using this protocol to significantly advance our understanding of the neural circuits of threat-related learning and behavior.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Krishnan et al devised three paradigms to perform contextual fear conditioning in head-fixed mice. Each of the paradigms relied on head-fixed mice running on a treadmill through virtual reality arenas. The authors tested the validity of three versions of the paradigms by using various parameters. As described below, I think there are several issues with the way the paradigms are designed and how the data are interpreted. Moreover, as Paradigm 3 was published previously in a study by the same group, it is unclear to me what this manuscript offers beyond the validations of parameters used for the previous publication. Below, I list my concerns point-by-point, which I believe need to be addressed to strengthen the manuscript.

      Major comments

      (1) In the analysis using the LME model (Tables 1 and 2), I am left wondering why the mice had increased freezing across recall days as well as increased generalization (increased freezing to the familiar context, where shock was never delivered). Would the authors expect freezing to decrease across recall days, since repeated exposure to the shock context should drive some extinction? This is complicated by the analysis showing that freeing was increased only on retrieval day 1 when analyzing data from the first lap only. Since reward (e.g., motivation to run) is removed during the conditioning and retrieval tests, I wonder if what the authors are observing is related to decreased motivation to perform the task (mice will just sit, immobile, not necessarily freezing per se). I think that these aspects need to be teased out.

      This is an important point and we agree teasing out a lack of motivation versus fearful freezing would be useful. To address the possibility that reduced motivation to run without reward could contribute to the observed freezing behavior, we have now included a no-shock control group in the revised manuscript (n = 7; Supplementary Figure 2A-B, H–I). These control mice experienced the same protocol, including the wearing of a tail coat, but did not receive any shocks. We observed no increases in freezing across days in these controls, confirming that the increased freezing in the Familiar context of our experimental group stems from fear conditioning rather than the removal of reward from a previously rewarded context. If reduced motivation from reward removal were the primary driver, similar freezing patterns would have emerged in the no-shock controls. We have added lines 248-261 in the revised manuscript, discussing this point, and we thank the reviewer for motivating us to do this experiment and analysis.

      That said, the precise mechanisms underlying the fear generalization observed in the nonconditioned context—particularly its emergence during later recall days—remain unclear. Studies in freely moving animals have shown that fear memories initially specific to the conditioned context can become generalized with repeated exposures, which may be occurring here (Biedenkapp & Rudy, 2007; Wiltgen & Silva, 2007). Alternatively, it is possible that the combination of fear conditioning and the removal of expected reward contributes to a delayed generalization effect. This may reflect a limitation of our approach, which relies on reward to motivate initial training. As noted by another reviewer, we have now addressed this potential drawback of reward-based training in the discussion (see lines 809-817). Clearly, unique factors specific to the head-fixed VR paradigm may contribute to this phenomenon. Understanding the mechanisms underlying fear generalization in the head-fixed VR CFC paradigm will be a valuable direction for future research.

      (2) Related to point 1, the authors actually point out that these changes could be due to the loss of the water reward. So, in line 304, is it appropriate to call this freezing? I think it will be very important for the authors to exactly define and delineate what they consider as freezing in this task, versus mice just simply sitting around, immobile, and taking a break from performing the task when they realize there is no reward at the end.

      As noted in point 1 above, we have added a no-shock control group (n = 7; Supplementary Figure 2A-B, H–I) to determine whether the observed freezing was driven by fear conditioning or by reduced motivation to run in the absence of reward. The absence of increased freezing in these controls supports the interpretation that the behavior in the conditioned group is fearrelated. In future studies, incorporating additional physiological measures—such as heart rate monitoring—could further help distinguish fear-related freezing from other forms of immobility.

      (3) In the second paradigm, mice are exposed to both novel and (at the time before conditioning) neutral environments just before fear conditioning. There is a big chance that the mice are 'linking' the memories (Cai et al 2016) of the two contexts such that there is no difference in freezing in the shock context compared to the neutral context, which is what the authors observe (Lines 333-335). The experiment should be repeated such that exposure to the contexts does not occur on the conditioning day.

      This is an interesting idea. However, if memory linking were driving the observed freezing patterns, we would expect to see similarly reduced fear discrimination across all three paradigms, as mice experience both contexts sequentially in each case. However, this effect appears to be specific to Paradigm 2, suggesting this may be due to other factors. We agree it would be informative to eliminate pre-conditioning exposure to both environments—to assess whether this improves fear discrimination and helps clarify the potential contribution of memory linking. This is something we plan to do in future studies that are beyond the scope of this initial paper on VR-CFC.

      (4) On lines 360-361, the authors conclude that extinction happens rapidly, within the first lap of the VR trial. To my understanding, that would mean that extinction would happen within the first 5-10 seconds of the test (according to Figure S1E). That seems far too fast for extinction to occur, as this never occurs in freely behaving mice this quickly.

      We agree with the reviewer that extinction in Paradigm 2 appears to occur relatively rapidly.

      However, the average time to complete the first lap in the fear-conditioned context in Paradigm 2 is 25.68 ± 5.55 seconds (as stated in line 384), indicating that extinction occurs within approximately the first 30 seconds of context exposure—not within 5–10 seconds. This is specific to Paradigm 2 and does not happen in either of the other paradigms, as shown in Supplementary Figure 4. For clarification, Figure S1E pertains to baseline running in Paradigm 1 and does not apply to Paradigm 2.

      As the reviewer points out, even at 30 seconds, extinction seems to be happening more quickly in Paradigm 2 than seen in freely moving setups. This may be due to a key structural difference in our setup. The VR-CFC task is organized into discrete trials, with mice being teleported back to the start after reaching the end of the virtual track. Completing a full lap without receiving a shock could serve as a clear signal that the threat is no longer present within the environment as the completion of a lap means that the animals have surveyed all locations within the environment. This structure could accelerate extinction compared to freely moving setups, where animals take longer to explore their complete environment due to the lack of discrete trials. Although this is true for all our paradigms, the accelerated extinction seen in paradigm 2 versus 1 and 3 may be driven by other factors. As noted by the reviewers, other task parameters—such as context pre-exposure timing, shock intensity, and conditioning duration— are likely to play a role in shaping extinction dynamics. These factors warrant further investigation, and we plan to explore them in future studies to better understand the conditions influencing extinction in the VR-CFC paradigm.

      (5) Throughout the different paradigms, the authors are using different shock intensities. This can lead to differences in fear memory encoding as well as in levels of fear memory generalization. I don't think that comparisons can be made across the different paradigms as too many variables (including shock intensity - 0.5/0.6mA can be very different from 1.0 mA) are different. How can the authors pinpoint which works best? Indeed, they find Paradigm 3 'works' better than Paradigm 2 because mice discriminate better between the neutral and shock contexts. This can definitely be driven by decreased generalization from using a 0.6mA shock in Paradigm 3 compared to 1.0 mA shock in Paradigm 2.

      The reviewer brings up important points here. We have now added new data evaluating 0.6 mA shocks in Paradigm 1 (Supplementary Figure 2A–E, n=8). These data show that 1.0 mA shocks produced stronger conditioned responses and greater fear discrimination compared to 0.6 mA. Our goal in Paradigm 3 was to begin with a lower shock intensity and assess whether additional modifications—specifically the shorter ISI and retention of the tail-coat during recall—could enhance fear conditioning. Surprisingly, despite the weaker shock intensity, Paradigm 3 resulted in improved discrimination and freezing behavior relative to Paradigm 2. We have now clarified this point in the manuscript (lines 466-470), and we interpret this outcome as evidence that the shorter ISIs and contextual cue continuity (tail-coat) likely play a more significant role in enhancing learning and recall. However, as noted in the text (lines 511-514), further testing is needed to determine the individual contributions of each parameter to successful VR-CFC. Fully optimizing the parameter settings will take additional time and resources, and we aim to continually refine the parameter space in the future, as has been done over the years for freely moving animals.

      (6) There are some differences in the calcium imaging dataset compared to other studies, and the authors should perform additional testing to determine why. This will be integral to validating their head-fixed paradigm(s) and showing they are useful for modeling circuit dynamics/behaviors observed in freely behaving mice. Moreover, the sample size (number of mice) seems low.

      The one notable difference between our imaging study and that done in freely moving animals is that we observed remapping of place cells in the control context. In contrast, Moita et al. (2004) reported more stable place fields in the control context. A key distinction is that their study included rewards in the control context, which may have contributed to the spatial stability. We now discuss this difference in the manuscript (lines 599-605).

      It should be noted that there are many key distinctions among paradigms that study neural activity during fear conditioning in freely moving animals. These include varying exposure times to environments (1–6 days), the time interval between neural activity recordings, and the use of food rewards during the experiment stages in freely moving animals to encourage exploration for place cell identification. Although freely moving paradigms that investigate fear conditioning and place cells are heterogeneous, we were encouraged by the replication of several key findings. This validates VR-based CFC as a viable tool for neural circuit investigations. While future work will include more thorough analyses, our current findings demonstrate the paradigm's effectiveness for modeling circuit dynamics and behavior. We have now expanded our dataset, which includes four additional mice, further corroborating these original findings.

      (7) It appears that the authors have already published a paper using Paradigm 3 (Ratigan et al 2023). If they already found a paradigm that is published and works, it is unclear to me what the current manuscript offers beyond that initial manuscript.

      The reviewer is correct that we have published a paper using Paradigm 3. However, this manuscript goes beyond that one and provides a much more comprehensive description and fundamental analysis of the behavior and experimental parameters regarding VR-CFC, allowing the research community to adapt our paradigm reproducibly. While Ratigan et al. (2023) offered only a minimal description of behavior and included just Paradigm 3, we present two additional paradigms along with neuronal validation using hippocampal place cells. We have now explicitly stated this in the introduction (lines 50-55).

      (8) As written, the manuscript is really difficult to follow with the averages and standard error reported throughout the text. This reporting in the text occurred heterogeneously throughout the text, as sometimes it was reported and other times it was not. Cleaning this reporting up throughout the paper would greatly improve the flow of the text and qualitative description of the results.

      We completely agree with this point and have now cleaned up the text, leaving details only in a few places we felt were important.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Krishnan et al. present a novel contextual fear conditioning (CFC) paradigm using a virtual reality (VR) apparatus to evaluate whether conditioned context-induced freezing can be elicited in head-fixed mice. By combining this approach with two-photon imaging, the authors aim to provide high-resolution insights into the neural mechanisms underlying learning, memory, and fear. Their experiments demonstrate that head-fixed mice can discriminate between threat and non-threat contexts, exhibit fear-related behavior in VR, and show context-dependent variability during extinction. Supplemental analyses further explore alternative behaviors and the influence of experimental parameters, while hippocampal neuron remapping is tracked throughout the experiments, showcasing the paradigm's potential for studying memory formation and extinction processes.

      Strengths:

      Methodological Innovation: The integration of a VR-based CFC paradigm with real-time twophoton imaging offers a powerful, high-resolution tool for investigating the neural circuits underlying fear, learning, and memory.

      Versatility and Utility: The paradigm provides a controlled and reproducible environment for studying contextual fear learning, addressing challenges associated with freely moving paradigms.

      Potential for Broader Applications: By demonstrating hippocampal neuron remapping during fear learning and extinction, the study highlights the paradigm's utility for exploring memory dynamics, providing a strong foundation for future studies in behavioral neuroscience.

      Comprehensive Data Presentation: The inclusion of supplemental figures and behavioral analyses (e.g., licking behaviors and variability in extinction) strengthens the manuscript by addressing additional dimensions of the experimental outcomes.

      Weaknesses:

      Characterization of Freezing Behavior: The evidence supporting freezing behavior as the primary defensive response in VR is unclear. Supplementary videos suggest the observed behaviors may include avoidance-like actions (e.g., backing away or stopping locomotion) rather than true freezing. Additional physiological measurements, such as EMG or heart rate, are necessary to substantiate the claim that freezing is elicited in the paradigm.

      To strengthen our claim that freezing is a conditioned response in this task, we have taken three key steps:

      (1) We adjusted our freezing detection threshold from 1 cm/s to near 0 cm/s to capture only periods where the animal is virtually motionless on the treadmill. We validated this approach in Figure 2, particularly in the zoomed-in track position trace in Figure 2A, which clearly shows that the identified freezing epochs correspond to no change in track position. All analyses and figures have been updated to reflect this more stringent threshold.

      (2) We have added a no-shock control group in the revised manuscript (n = 7; Supplementary Figure 2A-B, H–I) where mice experienced the same protocol, including wearing a tail-coat, but received no shocks. These mice showed no increases in freezing behavior, which further demonstrates that the increased freezing we observe is a result of fear conditioning.

      (3) We have added a new supplementary video (Supplementary Video 2) that better illustrates the freezing behavior in our task.

      That said, we fully agree with the reviewer that freezing is not the only defensive response observed. Other behaviors—such as hesitation, backward movement, and slowing down—also emerge that are unique to our treadmill-based paradigm. We chose to focus on freezing in this manuscript to align with convention in freely moving fear conditioning studies and to facilitate direct comparisons. We agree that additional physiological measurements (e.g., EMG or heart rate) would provide further validation and could help distinguish between different forms of defensive responses. We view this as an important future direction and plan to incorporate such measures in upcoming studies. We highlight this in the results section (lines 175-179, 262-268) and in the discussion (lines 739-750).

      Analysis of Extinction: Extinction dynamics are only analyzed through between-group comparisons within each Recall day, without addressing within-group changes in behavior across days. Statistical comparisons within groups would provide a more robust demonstration of extinction processes.

      This is an important distinction and we have now added figures (Supplementary Figures 2H-I, 5C-D, 6C-D) showing within-VR behavior across Recall days, along with statistical comparisons and a description of the extinction process based on these results.

      Low Sample Sizes: Paradigm 1 includes conditions with very low sample sizes (N=1-3), limiting the reliability of statistical comparisons regarding the effects of shock number and intensity.

      Increasing sample sizes or excluding data from mice that do not match the conditions used in Paradigms 2 and 3 would improve the rigor of the analysis.

      While we included all conditions in Figure 2 for completeness, we have separated these conditions in Supplementary Figure 2 to ensure clarity. This allows researchers interested in this paradigm to see the approximate range of conditioned responses observed across different parameters. When comparing Paradigm 1 with Paradigms 2 and 3, we have only used data from 1mA, 6 shocks condition.

      Potential Confound of Water Reward: The authors critique the use of reward in conjunction with fear conditioning in prior studies but do not fully address the potential confound introduced by using water reward during the training phase in their own paradigm.

      We agree this is a point that needs discussion. We have now noted the limitation of using water rewards during training in the discussion section, particularly its effect on the animal’s motivation in the long term and on place cell activity (lines 814-820).

      Recommendations for the authors

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I suggest changing "3 paradigms" to "3 versions of a CFC paradigm," as the paradigm is fundamentally the same, but parameters were adjusted towards finding an optimal protocol.

      We have changed this phrasing where applicable.

      Figure S2: There appear to be different sets of shock parameters for different mice, most with an n of 1 or 2. This is not reliable for making a decision for optimal shock parameters and should not be discussed in that way until a full-powered comparison is completed. Also, the N adds up to 19, yet only 18 are described as being included in the study.

      We thank the reviewer for this important point. We agree that the current study is not powered to definitively identify optimal parameter settings. We have been careful not to interpret it in that way in the text. Rather, we adopted a commonly used starting point from the freely moving literature—1 mA with six shocks—as our initial condition (lines 196-199). To provide context for others interested in pursuing this work, we have presented a range of conditioned responses from different parameter combinations to illustrate potential variability. In most cases, these data are intended for illustrative purposes only and are not meant to support firm conclusions. We agree that a systematic and fully powered investigation of each parameter would be highly valuable, and we plan to pursue this in future work (and hope other labs contribute to this goal, too), much like the iterative optimizations performed in freely moving paradigms over time.

      We thank the reviewer for catching the sample size discrepancy and have now corrected it.

      The number of animals for the no-shock condition should be included.

      Thank you. We have now included this.

      A possible explanation for the lower fear and poorer discrimination in versions 2 and 3 could be that 10 min pre-exposure to the CFC context on day -1 led to latent inhibition. Shorter (or eliminated) pre-exposure may improve outcomes.

      We agree that the exposure time is a parameter that we should explore. We have highlighted this in the discussion (lines 729-736) as a parameter that is worth testing in the future.

      For analysis of extinction, it is best to establish this within condition - is freezing to the CFC context significantly reduced compared with initial recall and similar to pre-training freezing? By using discrimination as your index of extinction, increases in control context freezing/inactivity can eliminate context discrimination without the conditioned response of freezing actually undergoing extinction.

      This is a good point, and we have now included analysis and conclusions based on a within-VR comparison for the analysis of fear extinction (Supplementary Figures 2H-I, 5C-D, 6C-D).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Clarification of Treadmill Shape: The manuscript describes the treadmill as "spherical" throughout. However, based on representative images and videos, the treadmill appears cylindrical. This discrepancy should be clarified to ensure consistency between the text and visuals.

      The reviewer is correct that the treadmill is cylindrical, and this was an error on our part. We have corrected it throughout.

      Figure and Legend Labeling: To improve clarity, all figures and their legends should be explicitly labeled with the corresponding paradigm (1, 2, or 3) to facilitate interpretation.

      We have now added a label on all figures that clarifies which Paradigm the figures are referring to. We have also explicitly added this to the figure legends.

      Objective Language: Subjective language, such as "since we wanted animals to" (Line 850), should be revised to reflect an objective tone (e.g., "to allow animals to"). Similarly, phrases like "We believe" (Line 896) should be avoided to maintain an unbiased presentation.

      We have removed subjective language from our text.

      Placement of Future Directions: Speculations on future experimental plans, such as the use of sex as a biological variable (Lines 895-903), should be included in the Discussion section rather than the Methods. Additionally, remarks about the responsiveness of female mice to tail shocks should be moved to the main text for proper contextualization.

      We have moved these lines as suggested by the reviewer.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Guo and colleagues used a cell rounding assay to screen a library of compounds for inhibition of TcdB, an important toxin produced by Clostridioides difficile. Caffeic acid and derivatives were identified as promising leads, and caffeic acid phenethyl ester (CAPE) was further investigated.

      Strengths:

      Considering the high morbidity rate associated with C. difficile infections (CDI), this manuscript presents valuable research in the investigation of novel therapeutics to combat this pressing issue. Given the rising antibiotic resistance in CDI, the significance of this work is particularly noteworthy. The authors employed a robust set of methods and confirmatory tests, which strengthen the validity of the findings. The explanations provided are clear, and the scientific rationale behind the results is well-articulated. The manuscript is extremely well written and organized. There is a clear flow in the description of the experiments performed. Also, the authors have investigated the effects of CAPE on TcdB in careful detail, and reported compelling evidence that this is a meaningful and potentially useful metabolite for further studies.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors have made some changes in the revised version. However, many of the changes were superficial, and some concerns still need to be addressed. Important details are still missing from the description of some experiments. Authors should carefully revise the manuscript to ascertain that all details that could affect interpretation of their results are presented clearly. For instance, authors still need to include details of how the metabolomics analyses were performed. Just stating that samples were "frozen for metabolomics analyses" is not enough. Was this mass-spec or NMR-based metabolomics. Assuming it was mass-spec, what kind? How was metabolite identity assigned, etc? These are important details, which need to be included. Even in cases where additional information was included, the authors did not discuss how the specific way in which certain experiments were performed could affect interpretation of their results. One example is the potential for compound carryover in their experiments. Another important one is the fact that CAPE affects bacterial growth and sporulation. Therefore, it is critical that authors acknowledge that they cannot discard the possibility that other factors besides compound interactions with the toxin are involved in their phenotypes. As stated previously, authors should also be careful when drawing conclusions from the analysis of microbiota composition data, and changes to the manuscript should be made to reflect this. Ascribing causality to correlational relationships is a recurring issue in the microbiome field. Again, I suggest authors carefully revise the manuscript and tone down some statements about the impact of CAPE treatment on the gut microbiota.

      Thanks for your constructive suggestion. We have carefully revised the manuscript according to your suggestions.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      I appreciate the author's responses to my original review. This is a comprehensive analysis of CAPE on C. difficile activity. It seems like this compound affects all aspects of C. difficile, which could make it effective during infection but also make it difficult to understand the mechanism. Even considering the authors responses, I think it is critical for the authors to work on the conclusions regarding the infection model. There is some protection from disease by CAPE but some parameters are not substantially changed. For instance, weight loss is not significantly different in the C. difficile only group versus the C. difficile + CAPE group. Histology analysis still shows a substantial amount of pathology in the C. difficile + CAPE group. This should be discussed more thoroughly using precise language.

      Thanks for your constructive suggestion. We have carefully revised the manuscript according to your suggestions.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study is well written, and the results are solid and well demonstrated. It shows a field that can be explored for the treatment of CDI

      Strengths:

      Results are really good, and the CAPE shows a good and promising alternative for treating CDI.

      Weaknesses:

      Some references are too old or missing.

      Comments on revisions:

      I have read your study after comments made by all referees, and I noticed that all questions and suggestions addressed to the authors were answered and well explained. Some of the minor and major issues related to the article were also solved. I am satisfied with all the effort given by the authors to improve their manuscript.

      Thanks again for your review.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The legend of Figure 3SB is incorrect. It should read "Growth curves of C. difficile BAA-1870 in the presence of varying concentrations of CAPE (0-64 µg/mL)". Also, there is something wrong with the symbols in this figure. I suspect what is happening is that the symbols for the concentrations of 32 and 64 µg/mL are superimposing, but this is a problem because the lower line looks like a closed circle, which is supposed to represent the condition where no CAPE was added. The authors should change the symbols to allow clear distinction between each of the conditions.

      Thanks for your constructive suggestion. We have modified the panel and figure legend in Figure 3SB. The concentrations of 32 μg/mL and 64 μg/mL are quite similar, which makes it challenging to differentiate between the corresponding data points on the graph. To enhance clarity, we have utilized distinct colors to help distinguish these closely valued lines as effectively as possible.

      Since the authors observed a significant effect of CAPE on both bacterial growth and spore production, their discussion and conclusions need to reflect the fact that the effects observed can no longer be attributed solely to toxin inhibition.

      Thanks for your comments. We have modified the corresponding description according to your suggestions.

      In lines 43-45, authors state that "CAPE treatment of C. difficile-challenged mice induces a remarkable increase in the diversity and composition of the gut microbiota (e.g., Bacteroides spp.)". It is still unclear to this reviewer why mention Bacteroides between parentheses. Does this mean that there was an increase in the abundance of Bacteroides? If that is the case this needs to be stated more clearly.

      Thanks for your comments. Treatment with CAPE indeed significantly increased the abundance of Bacteroides spp. in the gut microbiota (Figure 7H-J). However, to avoid ambiguity in the abstract, we have chosen to delete the specific mention of Bacteroides spp. within the parentheses.

      The modifications made to lines 132-135 still do not address my concern. Authors stated in the manuscript that "compounds that were not bound to TcdB were removed". But how was this done? This needs to be clearly explained in the manuscript. In the response to reviewers document, authors state that this was done through centrifugation. But given that the goal here is to separate excess of small molecule from a protein target, just stating that centrifugation was used is not enough. Did the authors use ultracentrifugation? What were the conditions employed. This is critical so that the reader can assess the degree of compound carryover that may have occurred. Also, authors need to clearly acknowledge the caveats of their experimental design by stating that they cannot rule out the contribution of compound carryover to their results.

      Thanks for your comments. We employed ultrafiltration centrifugal partition to remove the unbound small molecule compounds. Due to the large molecular weight of TcdB, approximately 270 kDa, we selected a 100 kDa molecular weight cutoff ultrafiltration membrane. The centrifugation was performed at 4000 g for 5 min to eliminate the compounds that did not bind to TcdB. We have incorporated the relevant methods and discussed the potential impacts on the respective sections of the manuscript.

      In line 142, authors added the molar concentration of caffeic acid, as requested. Although this helps, it is even more important that molar concentrations are added every time a compound concentration is mentioned. For instance, just 2 lines down there is another mention of a compound concentration. It would be informative if authors also added molar concentrations here and throughout the manuscript.

      Thanks for your comments. In our initial test design, we have utilized the concentration unit of μg/mL. However, during the conversion to μM using the dilution method, some values do not result in neat, whole numbers. For instance, the conversion of 32 μg/mL of caffeic acid phenyl ethyl ester yields 112.55 μM, which appears somewhat irregular when expressed in this manner.

      Line 277. For the sake of clarity, I would strongly suggest that authors use the term "control mice" instead of "model mice".

      Thanks for your comments. We have modified “model mice” to “control mice” throughout the manuscript.

      In line 302, the word taxa should not be capitalized. I capitalized it in my original comments simply to draw attention to it.

      Thanks for your comments. We have modified this word.

      In the section starting in line 318, authors still need to include details of how the metabolomics analyses were performed. Just stating that samples were "frozen for metabolomics analyses" is not enough. Was this mass-spec or NMR-based metabolomics. Assuming it was mass-spec, what kind? How was metabolite identity assigned? Etc, etc. These are important details, which need to be included.

      Thanks for your comments. We have added some metabolomics methods in the corresponding section.

      In line 338, the authors misunderstood my original comment. This sentence should read "...the final product of purine degradation, were markedly decreased in mice after...".

      Thanks for your comments. We have modified this sentence.

      Panels of figure 3 are still incorrectly labeled. The secondary structure predictions are shown in A and C, not A and B as is currently stated in the legend.

      Thanks for your comments. We have modified the figure legend in Figure 3.

      About Figure 5C, I think the authors for the clarification, but this explanation should be included in the figure legend.

      Thanks for your comments. We have added the relevant information to the figure legend.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Brdar, Osterburg, Munick, et al. present an interesting cellular and biochemical investigation of different p53 isoforms. The authors investigate the impact of different isoforms on the in-vivo transcriptional activity, protein stability, induction of the stress response, and hetero-oligomerization with WT p53. The results are logically presented and clearly explained. Indeed, the large volume of data on different p53 isoforms will provide a rich resource for researchers in the field to begin to understand the biochemical effects of different truncations or sequence alterations.

      Strengths:

      The authors achieved their aims to better understand the impact/activity of different p53 is-forms, and their data will support their statements. Indeed, the major strengths of the paper lie in its comprehensive characterization of different p53 isoforms and the different assays that are measured. Notably, this includes p53 transcriptional activity, protein degradation, induction of the chaperone machinery, and hetero-oligomerization with wtp53. This will provide a valuable dataset where p53 researchers can evaluate the biological impact of different isoforms in different cell lines. The authors went to great lengths to control and test for the effect of (1) p53 expression level, (2) promotor type, and (3) cell type. I applaud their careful experiments in this regard.

      Weaknesses:

      One thing that I would have liked to see more of is the quantification of the various pull-down/gel assays - to better quantify the effect of, e.g., hetero-oligomerization among the various isoforms. In addition, a discussion about the role of isoforms that contain truncations in the IDRs is not available. It is well known that these regions function in an auto-inhibitory manner (e.g. work by Wright/Dyson) and also mediate many PPIs, which likely have functional roles in vivo (e.g. recruiting p53 to various complexes). The discussion could be strengthened by focusing on some of these aspects of p53 as well.

      Thank you for these comments. In this paper we have focused on the importance of the integrity of the folded domains of p53 for their function. The unfolded regions in the N- and the C-terminus have not been our main target but the reviewer is right that they play important regulatory functions that are lost in the corresponding isoforms. We have, therefore, added a few sentences in the Discussion section.

      With respect to a better quantification, we have re-evaluated the quantification and adjusted where necessary (see also reviewer 2). With respect to the hetero-oligomerization we have run a new mass spectrometry experiment in which we only focus on the p53 peptides. These have been now quantitatively evaluated and the results are provided in this manuscript Fig. 5.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript entitled "p53 isoforms have a high aggregation propensity, interact with chaperones and lack 1 binding to p53 interaction partners", the authors suggest that the p53 isoforms have high aggregation propensity and that they can co-aggregate with canonical p53 (FLp53), p63 and p73 thus exerting a dominant-negative effect.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the paper is interesting as it provides some characterization of most p53 isoforms DNA binding (when expressed alone), folding structure, and interaction with chaperones. The data presented support their conclusion and bring interesting mechanistic insight into how p53 isoforms may exert some of their activity or how they may be regulated when they are expressed in excess.

      Weaknesses:

      The main limitation of this manuscript is that the isoforms are highly over-expressed throughout the manuscript, although the authors acknowledge that the level of expression is a major factor in the aggregation phenomenon and "that aggregation will only become a problem if the expression level surpasses a certain threshold level" (lines 273-274 and results shown in Figures S3D, 6E). The p53 isoforms are physiologically expressed in most normal human cell types at relatively low levels which makes me wonder about the physiological relevance of this phenomenon.

      Furthermore, it was previously reported that some isoforms clearly induce transcription of target genes which are not observed here. For example, p53β induces p21 expression (Fujita K. et al. p53 isoforms Delta133p53 and p53beta are endogenous regulators of replicative cellular senescence. Nat Cell Biol. 2009 Sep;11(9):1135-42), and Δ133p53α induces RAD51, RAD52, LIG4, SENS1 and SOD1 expression (Gong, L. et al. p53 isoform D113p53/D133p53 promotes DNA double-strand break repair to protect cell from death and senescence in response to DNA damage. Cell Res. 2015, 25, 351-369. / Gong, L. et al. p53 isoform D133p53 promotes the efficiency of induced pluripotent stem cells and ensures genomic integrity during reprogramming. Sci. Rep. 2016, 6, 37281. / Horikawa, I. et al. D133p53 represses p53-inducible senescence genes and enhances the generation of human induced pluripotent stem cells. Cell Death Differ. 2017, 24, 1017-1028. / Gong, L. p53 coordinates with D133p53 isoform to promote cell survival under low-level oxidative stress. J. Mol. Cell Biol. 2016, 8, 88-90. / Joruiz et al. Distinct functions of wild-type and R273H mutant Δ133p53α differentially regulate glioblastoma aggressiveness and therapy-induced senescence. Cell Death Dis. 2024 Jun 27;15(6):454.) which demonstrates that some isoforms can induce target genes transcription and have defined normal functions (e.g. Cellular senescence or DNA repair).

      However, in this manuscript, the authors conclude that isoforms are "largely unfolded and not capable of fulfilling a normal cellular function" (line 438), that they do not have "well defined physiological roles" (line 456), and that they only "have the potential to inactivate members of the p53 protein family by forming inactive hetero complexes with wtp53" (line 457-458).

      Therefore, I think it is essential that the authors better discuss this major discrepancy between their study and previously published research.

      This manuscript is not about hunting for the next “signal transduction pathway” that is “regulated” by a specific p53 isoform. For such a project work has indeed to be conducted at the endogenous level. However, our manuscript is about the basic thermodynamic behavior of these isoforms in in vitro assays and in some cell culture assays.

      What, however, depends on the expression level is the interaction with chaperones as well as the tendency to aggregate. And this we actually show in our manuscript by using two different promotors with very different strength: Strong overexpression leads to aggregation, much weaker expression to soluble isoforms. For the mass spectrometry experiments we have established stable expressing cell lines and not used transiently overexpressing ones.

      The level from which on the chaperone systems of the cell cannot keep these isoforms soluble and they start to aggregate is certainly an important question, and we have experimental evidence that if we use different chaperone inhibitors the percentage of the aggregating isoforms in the insoluble fraction increases.

      Proteins have to follow the basic physicochemical rules also in cells. And this manuscript sets the stage for re-interpreting the observed cellular effects – not in terms of specific interaction with certain promoters but as causing a stress response and non-specific interaction with other not-well folded domains of other proteins.

      With respect to this discussion about the physiological relevance, it is interesting to look at a study that was published in Cell:

      Rohaly, G., Chemnitz, J., Dehde, S., Nunez, A.M., Heukeshoven, J., Deppert, W. and Dornreiter, I. (2005) A novel human p53 isoform is an essential element of the ATR-intra-S phase checkpoint. Cell, 122, 21-32.

      This manuscript describes how a specific isoform regulates an important pathway. Two other studies also focused on the same isoform but showed that it lacks the nuclear localization signal and therefore does not enter the nucleus. And even if it would, it would have no transcriptional activity due to the unfolding of the DBD.

      Chan, W.M. and Poon, R.Y. (2007) The p53 Isoform Deltap53 lacks intrinsic transcriptional activity and reveals the critical role of nuclear import in dominant-negative activity. Cancer Res, 67, 1959-1969.

      Garcia-Alai, M.M., Tidow, H., Natan, E., Townsley, F.M., Veprintsev, D.B. and Fersht, A.R. (2008) The novel p53 isoform "delta p53" is a misfolded protein and does not bind the p21 promoter site. Protein Sci, 17, 1671-1678.

      This example shows that it is important to re-consider the basic principles of protein structure and protein folding. And that is exactly what this manuscript is about.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Does the p53g C-terminus (322-346) form cross-beta amyloid structures? The strong fluorescence signal in the presence of ThT suggests this may be forming amyloid. I wonder if any amyloid sequence predictors identify this region as amyloidogenic.

      Using the Waltz predictor (https://doi.org/10.1038/nmeth.1432), the amino acids 339-346 have been identified as potentially amyloidogenic. We have added this information to the manuscript.

      (2) The chaperone binding results in Figure 5 are interesting and indeed suggest that many p53 isoforms interact with chaperones in vivo to counteract their destabilized nature. For the 5 p53 isoforms shown in Figure 5D, do they present any HSP70-binding motifs that may not exist in wtp53? These motifs can be predicted from the sequence with established software in a similar manner as the authors performed for TANGO.

      Author response image 1.

      Predicted Chaperon binding sites using the LIMBO prediction tool. (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/19696878)

      We have analyzed the sequence of p53 and the isoforms for potential HSP70 binding sites using the LIMBO prediction tool. The results are shown in the figure above. Wild type p53 has a very strong site that is lost in the β- and ɣ-isoforms. The ɣ-isoform in addition loses another predicted binding site which is replaced with a ɣ-specific one. Overall, this analysis does not provide a very clear picture due to the loss of some and the creation of new, isoform-specific binding sites. We have, therefore, not included this analysis in the manuscript but show it here for the reviewers.

      (3) The mixed hetero-tetramers detected by the MS is very interesting. Also the pull-down experiments in Figure 6. However, the extent of hetero-oligomerization is at times hard to follow. Could you more clearly summarize and/or quantify the results of the hetero-oligomerization experiments?

      We have conducted a new mass spectrometry experiment that was focused only on the analysis of p53 peptides. These data are now shown in Figure 5 and Supplementary Figure 6. They show that peptides not present in the Δ133p53α isoform and therefore must come from wild type p53 can be detected. For the Δ133p53β isoform these peptides are absent, suggesting that this isoform does not hetero-oligomerize with wild type p53. Furthermore, all β- and ɣ- isoforms do not show peptides derived from wild type p53, again suggesting that they cannot hetero-oligomerize due to the lack of a functional oligomerization domain.

      (4) There is a typo in Figure 5. The figure title (top of page) says "Figure 4: Chaperons". Also, "chaperons" appears in the legend.

      Thank you for making us aware of this problem. This has been corrected.

      (5) The figures are often quite small with a lot of white space. Figure 4 in particular is arranged in a confusing way with A, D, B, C, E, F, G in T->B L->R order. Perhaps some figures could be expanded or re-arranged to make better use of the available space. E.g. could move B, C above panel D, and then shift F, G to be next to E. This would give you A, [B, C, D], [E, F, G] in a 2x2 format.

      We have rearranged figures 2, 4, 5 and 6 to be able to enlarge the individual figure panels.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figure 2C: Why is the p21-Luc reporter assay performed in SAOS-2 cells when all other assays are performed in H1299?

      The assays we have performed in this study are independent of the cell type because we investigate very basic principles of protein folding and stability. If one removes a third of a folded domain, this domain will no longer fold, independent of the cell type it is in. However, to show, that the cell type indeed does not play any role, we have repeated the experiments in H1299 cells. These data are now shown in Figure 2C and the original data in SAOS cells we have moved to Supplementary Figure 1E.

      (2) Figure 3: I find the statistics on this figure very confusing... It looks like every isoform is compared to the "WT", but in that case, in Figure 3B for example, how can the Δ40p53β be ****, Δ133p53γ be *** while the Δ133p53α, more different to WT and narrower error bars is non-specific? I guess this comes from the normalization of the GST expression of each isoform but in this case, the isoforms should not be compared to the WT, but to their respective GST sample.

      There was indeed a mistake in the statistics, thank you for pointing this out.

      We repeated the statistical analysis and the relative protein level within each sample is now calculated using the ratio between the respective GST sample and the sample containing E6. Significance for each isoform was assessed by comparing the relative protein level to the protein level of the WT.

      (3) Figures 3D and 3E: the authors did not perform the assays on Δ40p53 isoforms because they "contain a fully folded DBD" (lines 218-219). This may be true for Δ40p53α as shown by the pAB240 binding figure 3C, but it is speculative for Δ40p53β and Δ40p53γ since these were not tested in Figure 3C either... Furthermore, Figure 3B suggests that there may be differences between Δ40p53α, Δ40p53β and Δ40p53γ and therefore these two isoforms should be tested for pAB240 IP at least (and DARPin as well if the pAB240 IP shows differences). Also, why were the TAp53β and TAp53γ not tested in Figures 3D and 3E?

      Here we disagree with the reviewer. The PDB is full of structures of the p53 DNA binding domain. All of them – including many structures of the same domain from other species – span residues ~90 to 294 (or the equivalent residues in other species). That means that the β- and ɣ- versions of p53 contain the full DNA binding domain. In contrast to the DNA binding domain, the oligomerization domain, however, is truncated and therefore does not form functional tetramers. This is the reason for the reduced binding affinity to DNA.

      The pAB240 antibody recognizes and binds to an epitope that becomes exposed upon the unfolding of the DBD. This manuscript shows by multiple experiments that the DBD of the β- and the ɣ-isoforms are not compromised but that the oligomerization domain is not functional. In figures 3D and 3E we have not included the TA β- and the ɣ-isoforms, because, again, they have a folded DBD and their inclusion would not provide any additional information compared to TAp53α.

      (4) Figures 4B and 4C are small and extremely difficult to read.

      We agree and have rearranged and enlarged these and other figures. Please see also answer to comment (5) of reviewer 1.

      (5) Figure 5C: the authors claim that "the isoform induced cellular stress that triggers the expression of chaperones" (line 320). However, if the induction of the HSP70 promoter is shown, there is no evidence that this is due to cellular stress. Evidence to support that claim should be shown.

      The expression and accumulation of unfolded, aggregation prone sequences is a stress situation for the cell which triggers the expression of chaperones. The expression of isoforms that are not well folded or of p53 mutants that are not well folded increases expression both from the HSP70 promoter and the heat shock promoter. This shows that the expression of unfolded isoforms induces cellular stress.

      (6) Figure 5D: why was this experiment performed in SAOS2 cells when the whole paper was otherwise performed in H1299 cells?<br /> Also, about this figure, the authors write "In addition to this common set, Δ133p53α and Δ40p53α showed only very few additional interaction partners. This situation was very different for Δ133α, Δ133β and TAp53γ." (lines 331 to 333). My feeling is that we should instead read "In addition to this common set, TAp53β and Δ40p53α showed only very few additional interaction partners. This situation was very different for Δ133p53α, Δ133p53β and TAp53γ"

      Thank you for spotting this mistake. Indeed, the correct wording is TAp53β and Δ40p53α and we have corrected the manuscript.

      The mass spectrometry experiments were actually not carried out in SAOS cells, but in U2OS cells. The reason for not using the H1299 cell line was that these cells do not contain functional p53. In contrast, U2OS cells express wild type p53. We have repeated the mass spectrometry analysis and analyzed the data with a special focus on p53 peptides. This information is now added as Figure 5E. In this analysis we show that the Δ133p53α samples contain peptides from the DBD that are not part of this truncated isoform and must therefore originate from wild type p53 with which this isoform hetero-oligomerizes. The corresponding peptides are absent from Δ133p53β, showing that without a functional oligomerization domain this isoform does not interact with wild type p53. Likewise, the data demonstrate that the β- and the ɣ-isoforms do not form hetero-oligomers.

      (7) Supplementary Table 2: the authors claim "For Δ133p53α we could identify peptides between amino acids 102 and 132 that must originate from wild type p53". SAOS2 has a WT TP53 gene and expresses all isoforms endogenously. Therefore, peptides between amino acids 102 and 132 can actually originate from "WT p53" but also TAp53β, TAp53γ, Δ40p53α, Δ40p53β or Δ40p53γ (most likely a mix of these).

      We have not used SAOS cells but U2OS cells. As mentioned above the data show that the Δ133p53α sample contains peptides from wild type p53 and that these peptides cannot be found in the Δ133p53β sample. In addition, peptides originating from the oligomerization domain are only found in the samples of isoforms containing an oligomerization domain but not in samples of β- and ɣ-isoforms. The data are presented in Figure 5 E-G and Supplementary Figure S5.

      Since the Biotin ligase is directly fused to a specific isoform, peptides from other isoforms can only be detected if these directly interact with the isoform fused to the ligase (and contain unique peptides, not present in the isoform fused to the ligase). The data confirm that only isoforms that have a functional oligomerization domain can interact with wild type p53 (or potentially other isoforms with a functional oligomerization domain).

      (8) Figure 6: Why not conduct these luciferase reporter assays using the MDM-2 and p21 promoters like in Figure 2B and 2C since there may be promoter-specific regulation?

      This would be particularly important for the p21 promoter as TAp53β is known to induce it (Fujita K. et al. p53 isoforms Delta133p53 and p53beta are endogenous regulators of replicative cellular senescence. Nat Cell Biol. 2009 Sep;11(9):1135-42) and the Δ133p53α, Δ133p53β and Δ133p53γ isoforms were shown to reduce p21 transcription by TAp73β when co-expressed in H1299 cells (Zorić A. et al. Differential effects of diverse p53 isoforms on TAp73 transcriptional activity and apoptosis. Carcinogenesis. 2013 Mar;34(3):522-9.). Neither of these regulations appears here on the pBDS2 reporter, which is puzzling.

      The main point of this paper is that all isoforms without a complete DNA binding domain and without a complete oligomerization domain do not bind to DNA with high affinity and do not show transcriptional activity and that is independent of the promotor. There might be effects of expressing certain isoforms in some cells, but that is most likely by inducing a stress response via expression of chaperones etc. High affinity sequence specific DNA binding does not play a role here (see results in Figure 2) and we have therefore not conducted these suggested experiments.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Batra, Cabrera and Spence et al. present a model which integrates histone posttranslational modification (PTM) data across cell models to predict gene expression with the goal of using this model to better understand epigenetic editing. This gene expression prediction model approach is useful if a) it predicts gene expression in specific cell lines b) it predicts expression values rather than a rank or bin, c) if it helps us to better understand the biology of gene expression or d) it helps us to understand epigenome editing activity. Problematically for point a) and b) it is easier to directly measure gene expression than to measure multiple PTMs and so the real usefulness of this approach mostly relates to c) and d).

      We appreciate this point from Reviewer #1 and the instructive comments and helpful feedback on our study. We designed our approach keeping in mind that the primary use case is to understand how epigenome editing would affect gene expression.

      Other approaches have been published that use histone PTM to predict expression (e.g. PMID 27587684, 36588793). Is this model better in some way? No comparisons are made although a claim is made that direct comparisons are difficult. I appreciate that the authors have not used the histone PTM data to predict gene expression levels of an "average cell" but rather that they are predicting expression within specific cell types or for unseen cell types. Approaches that predict expression levels are much more useful whereas some previous approaches have only predicted expressed or not expressed or a rank order or bin-based ranking. The paper does not seem to have substantial novel insights into understanding the biology of gene expression.

      We thank Reviewer #1 again for this insightful comment. We have included citations for a series of papers (PMIDs: 27587684, 30147283, 36588793) that performed gene expression prediction using histone PTM data. However, each of these methods performs classification of gene expression as opposed to predicting the actual gene expression value via regression. Additionally, the referenced studies all work with Roadmap Epigenomics read-depth data as opposed to p-values obtained from the ENCODE pipelines, making it difficult to make direct comparisons. We outline in the Discussion section that by creating a comprehensive dataset of epigenome editing outcomes, which include quantification of histone PTMs before and after in situ 1 perturbations, will improve our understanding of the effects of dCas9-p300 on gene expression and assist in the design of gRNAs for achieving fine-tuned control over gene expression levels. In this revised version of our study, we have also added new data (Figure 3 – figure supplement 3) to further benchmark our model against others.

      The approach of using this model to predict epigenetic editor activity on transcription is interesting and to my knowledge novel although only examined in the context of a p300 editor. As the author point out the interpretation of the epigenetic editing data is convoluted by things like sgRNA activity scoring and to fully understand the results likely would require histone PTM profiling and maybe dCas9 ChIP-seq for each sgRNA which would be a substantial amount of work.

      We agree with the Reviewer and view these experiments as important components of future studies.

      Furthermore from the model evaluation of H3K9me3 is seems the model is performing modestly for other forms of epigenetic or transcriptional editing- e.g. we know for the best studied transcriptional editor which is CRISPRi (dCas9-KRAB) that recruitment to a locus is associated with robust gene repression across the genome and is associated with H3K9me3 deposition by recruitment of KAP1/HP1/SETDB1 (PMID: 35688146, 31980609, 27980086, 26501517).

      This is an interesting point. We have included new data (Figure 4 – figure supplement 1), that quantifies how sensitive the trained gene expression model is to perturbations in H3K9me3. Indeed our data suggests that the model predictions are sensitive to perturbations in H3K9me3. For instance, there is a clear decrease and a gradual increase as the position where the perturbation is performed moves from upstream to downstream of the TSS. Additionally, the magnitude of the predicted fold-change is a function of how much the H3K9me3 is perturbed and hence the magnitude of change would be even higher if the perturbation magnitude is increased. However, this precise magnitude is hard to estimate In the absence of experimental perturbation data for H3K9me3. Leveraging our model in combination with KRAB-based CRISPRi is an exciting and important aspect of future studies.

      One concern overall with this approach is that dCas9-p300 has been observed to induce sgRNA independent off target H3K27Ac (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC8349887/ see Figure S5D) which could convolute interpretation of this type of experiment for the model.

      This remains an excellent point and indeed, we and others have observed that dCas9-p300 can result in off-target H3K27ac levels (both increased and suppressed) across the genome. Our study focused on p300, because the molecule is one of the few known proteins that can catalyze H3K27ac in the human genome, and H3K27ac remains a proxy for active genomic regulatory elements. Nevertheless, any off target activity of dCas9-p300 could certainly convolute our analyses. We have included language to address this caveat in our discussion.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors build a gene expression model based on histone post-translational modifications, and find that H3K27ac is correlated with gene expression. They proceed to perturb H3K27ac at 13 gene promoters in two cell types, and measure gene expression changes to test their model.

      We remain appreciative of the constructive feedback and input from Reviewer #2 on our manuscript.

      Strengths:

      The combination of multiple methods to model expression, along with utilizing 6 histone datasets in 13 cell types allowed the authors to build a model that correlates between 0.7-0.79 with gene expression. They use dCas9-p300 fusions to perturb H3K27ac and monitor gene expression to test their model. Ranked correlations of the HEK293 data showed some support for the predictions after perturbation of H3K27ac.

      Weaknesses:

      The perturbation of 5 genes in K562 with perturb-seq data shows a modest correlation of ~0.5 and isn't included in the main figures. The authors are then left to speculate reasons why the outcome of epigenome editing doesn't fit their predictions, which highlights the limited value in the current version of this method.

      We agree with the reviewer’s suggestion and highlight in our conclusion that generating epigenome editing data across a variety of cell types and across many genes will help uncover the underlying mechanisms of gene expression modulation.

      As mentioned before, testing genes that were not expressed being most activated by dCas9-p300 weaken the correlations vs. looking at a broad range of different gene expression as the original model was trained on.

      We appreciate this comment from Reviewer #2. We note that the data generated from this dCas9-p300 perturb-seq experiment used gRNAs from a pre-existing library published previously (PMID: 37034704). While this library enabled deeper interrogation of dCas9-p300 driven effects compared to our previous revision, the gRNAs in this library were designed against genes associated with haploinsufficiency in neuronal cell types, and which were generally lowly-expressed in K562 cells. Further, we restricted our analysis here to promoter-proximal gRNAs (as opposed to enhancer-targeted gRNAs in the library), focusing our scope even more so. Thus the genes ultimately used for analysis are enriched for low expression.

      If the authors want this method to be used to predict outcomes of epigenome editing, expanding to dCas9-KRAB and other CRISPRa methods (SAM and VPR) would be useful. Those datasets are published and could be analyzed for this manuscript.

      This is an exciting suggestion from Reviewer #2. We agree, and view this as a component of future work in this area.

      The authors don't compare their method to other prediction methods.

      In this revised version of our study, we have also added new data (Figure 3 – figure supplement 3) to further benchmark our model against others. These data demonstrate that our CNN model outperforms existing approaches across multiple cell types.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Looking at the individual genes in K562 shows a random looking range of predictions and observed, with the exception of Bcl11A which is one of two genes in this set of 5 that are not expressed. I will repeat my earlier comment, that epigenome editing and CRISPRa methods generally show the most upregulation with the lowest expressed genes. I speculate that plotting endogenous expression vs. outcome (assuming using all gRNAs within a reasonable and similar distance to TSS) would produce a correlation of -0.5 or greater and be as useful as this method.

      We agree, and believe that this demonstrates more work is needed in this emerging research area.

      The methods describe Perturb-seq analysis but not the bench experiments.

      We have added the bench methods related to our Perturb-seq experiments to our revised manuscript under the Experimental Methods section in the Appendix.

      I don't understand why the authors can't compare to other methods as that is fairly standard in new prediction papers. I get that others used REMC vs. ENCODE, and were rank or binary based, but the authors could use REMC data and/or convert their data to ranked or binary and still compare. Lacking that it's hard to judge this manuscript.

      We have added benchmarking against existing methods as Figure 3 – figure supplement 3.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Our revised manuscript thoroughly addresses all comments and suggestions raised by the reviewers, as detailed in our point-by-point response. To strengthen our findings, we have conducted additional in vivo experiments to evaluate the presence of fibro-adipogenic progenitors (FAPs) at different time points during HO formation in control and BYL719-treated mice. Our results indicate that BYL719 reduces the accumulation of FAPs and promotes muscle fiber regeneration in vivo. We have also expanded our discussion on BYL719’s effects on mTOR signaling, further clarifying key points raised by Reviewer #1, and have addressed all minor comments.

      Additionally, in response to Reviewer #2, we have employed an orthogonal and complementary approach using a new model. We conducted chondrogenic differentiation experiments with murine MSCs expressing either ACVR1wt or ACVR1<sup>R206H</sup>. qPCR analysis of chondrogenic gene markers (Sox9, Acan, Col2a1) demonstrates that Activin A enhances their expression in ACVR1<sup>R206H</sup> cells, whereas BYL719 strongly suppresses their expression, regardless of ACVR1 mutational status. These new data further confirm that BYL719 effectively inhibits genes involved in ossification and osteoblast differentiation, independent of the ACVR1 mutation. We have also expanded our discussion to further clarify points raised by Reviewer #2 and have addressed all remaining minor comments.

      Below, we provide a detailed point-by-point response to the reviewers’ comments:

      Rreviewer #1:

      Point 1: In this revised manuscript, the authors clearly showed that BYL719 suppressed the proliferation and differentiation of murine myoblasts, C2C12 cells, in addition to human MSCs in vitro. Furthermore, BYL719 decreased migratory activity in vitro in monocytes and macrophages without suppressing proliferation. Overall, these data suggested that BYL719 is not a specific chemical compound for cell types or signaling pathways as mentioned in the manuscript by the authors themselves. Therefore, it was still unclear how to explain the molecular mechanisms in inhibition of HO by the compound in a specific signaling pathway in a specific cell type, MSCs, contradicting many other possibilities. The authors should add logical explanations in the manuscript.

      Regarding its selectivity, BYL719 is a potent and highly selective inhibitor of PI3Kα. It has been demonstrated in multiple studies and in several in vitro kinase assay panels (Furet et al. PMID: 23726034, Fritsch et al. PMID: 24608574). The IC50 or Kd values for BYL719 against PI3Kα were at least 50 times lower than for most of other kinases tested. Moreover, BYL719 is also highly selective for PI3Kα (IC50 = 4.6 nmol/L) compared to other class I PI3K (PI3Kβ (IC50 = 1,156 nmol/L), PI3Kδ (IC50 = 290 nmol/L), PI3Kγ (IC50 = 250 nmol/L)) (Fritsch et al). Consistent with these data, we show that, at the concentrations tested, BYL719 does not have a direct effect on any kinase receptor within the TGF-b superfamily, including ACVR1 or ACVR1<sup>R206H</sup>.

      Rather than blocking ACVR1 kinase activity, in our manuscript we provide evidence that BYL719 has the potential to inhibit osteochondroprogenitor specification and prevent an exacerbated inflammatory response in vivo (Valer et al., 2019a PMID: 31373426, and this manuscript) through different mechanisms, such as (i) increasing SMAD1/5 degradation, (ii) reducing transcriptional responsiveness to BMPs and Activin, (iii) blocking non-canonical ACVR1 responses such as the activation of AKT/mTOR. All these defined molecular mechanisms contribute to suppress HO in vitro and in vivo, as we report and explain throughout the manuscript. Selective PI3Kα inhibition is at the core of the different molecular pathways described. As such, PI3Kα blockade inhibits the phosphorylation of GSK3 and compromises SMAD1 protein stability, thereby altering canonical responsiveness and osteochondroprogenitor specification (Gamez et al PMID: 26896753; Valer et al PMID: 31373426). Moreover, PI3Kα blockade downregulates Akt/mTOR signalling, which is critical for FOP and non‐genetic (trauma induced) HO in preclinical models (Hino et al, 2017 PMID: 28758906; Hino et al. PMID: 30392977). Finally, PI3Kα inhibition hampers a number of proinflammatory pathways, thereby limiting the expression of pro-inflammatory cytokines, reducing the proliferation of monocytes, macrophages and mast cells, and partially blocking the migration of monocytes. As we suggest in the discussion of the manuscript, this effect likely causes a poor recruitment of monocytes and macrophages at injury sites and throughout the in vivo ossification process.

      Noteworthy, in our manuscript we do not refer to a “specific chemical compound for cell types”. Rather, in the Discussion we write “the administration of BYL719 prevented an exacerbated inflammatory response in vivo, possibly due to specific effects observed on immune cell populations.” This sentence did not intend to imply that BYL719 only affects these specific cell types, but aimed to emphasize the effects observed on those cell populations, even though systemic BYL719 may affect all populations. We rephrased it to “the administration of BYL719 prevented an exacerbated inflammatory response in vivo, possibly due to the effects observed on immune cell populations.” to provide a clearer message as suggested by the reviewer. We thank the reviewer for these questions and hope that these explanations and changes in the text improve the clarity of the message.

      Mesenchymal stem/stromal cells (MSCs) are osteochondroprogenitor cells that can follow distinct differentiation paths. In this study, we use these cells as an in vitro model for the study of osteochondrogenitor specification. MSCs, and induced MSCs (iMSCs), have been widely used as in vitro cellular models of osteochondroprogenitor specification for the analysis of markers, signaling, modulation, and differentiation potential or capacity. Their use as models for this purpose has been extensively studied in wild type MSCs, and in the presence of FOP mutations (Boeuf and Richter PMID: 20959030; Schwartzl et al. PMID: 37923731).

      Point 2: Related to comment #1, the effects of BYL719 on the proliferation and differentiation of fibro-adipogenic cells in skeletal muscle, which are potential progenitor cells of HO, should be important to support the claim of the authors.

      We have performed additional in vivo experiments to assess the presence of fibro-adipogenic precursors (FAPs) at different time-points during HO formation in control and BYL719-treated in the mouse model of heterotopic ossification. We analyzed the number of fibro-adipogenic progenitor (FAPs) during the progression of the HO. These data are shown in the new Figure3-Figure Supplement 1. We demonstrate that BYL719 reduces the number of PDGFRA+ cells (FAPs, red) throughout the ossification process in vivo. Moreover, now we also show an enlargement of the diameter of myofibers (labelled with wheat germ agglutinin, green) when animals were treated with BYL719, indicating improved muscle regeneration and further validating the data reported as supplementary figures that were added in the first revision of this manuscript.

      Point 3: BYL719 inhibited signaling through not only ACVR1-R206H and ACVR1-Q207D but also wild type ACVR1 and suppressed the chondrogenic differentiation of parental MSCs regardless of the expression of wild type or mutant ACVR1. Again, these findings suggest that BYL719 inhibits HO through a multiple and nonspecific pathway in multiple types of cells in vivo. The authors are encouraged to explain logically the use of bone marrow-derived MSCs to examine the effects of BYL719.

      As detailed in main point 1, we consider that the main target, molecular mechanisms and inhibited pathways by BYL719 are specific and well characterised in other research articles and further defined in this manuscript, including the generation of PI3Ka deficient mice in an FOP background, that undoubtedly demonstrates an essential role for PI3Ka in ACVR1-driven heterotopic ossification in vivo. Altogether, we are confident that BYL719 inhibits HO through multiple and specific pathways that arise from the PI3Kα inhibition. As a systemically administrated drug, BYL719 affects the multiple types of cells in vivo that express PI3Kα. It is well known that PI3Kα is exquisitely required for chondrogenesis and osteogenesis (Zuscik et al. PMID; Gamez et al PMID: 26896753 1824619). Accordingly, throughout the manuscript we refrain from suggesting a specific effect on ACVR1-R206H cells but instead an inhibitory effect on cell number and differentiation regardless on the ACVR1 form expressed.

      Similarly, as detailed in main point 1, MSCs and hiPSCs have been extensible used as in vitro cellular models of osteochondroprogenitor specification for the analysis of markers, signaling, modulation, and differentiation potential or capacity (Barruet et al., PMID: 28716551; Kan et al., PMID: 39308190).

      Point 4: BYL719 clearly inhibits an mTOR pathway. Is there a possibility that BYL719 suppresses HO by inhibiting mTOR rather than PI3K? The authors are encouraged to show the unique role of PI3K in BYL719-suppressed HO formation.

      As clarified above, BYL719 is a potent and selective inhibitor of PI3Kα, with minimal off-target inhibition against other kinases, as it has been demonstrated in multiple studies and in several in vitro kinase assay panels. In the same study, while IC50 of BYL719 against PI3Kα was (IC50 = 4.6 nmol/L), IC50 against mTOR was (IC50= >9,100 nmol/L), indicating that it was not directly inhibited. mTOR is one of the well-known pathways that are activated downstream of PI3K. Therefore, there is no surprise that blocking PI3Kα will block mTOR signalling. This potential effect was already demonstrated in previous publications (Valer et al., 2019a PMID: 31373426) and discussed throughout the first revision. We consider that the additive effect of mTOR inhibition and other molecular mechanisms downstream of PI3Kα, including reduced SMAD1/5 protein levels, contribute to the in vivo HO inhibition by BYL719.

      Reviewer #2:

      Point 1: It is also important to note that, in most of the data, there is no significant difference between cells with wild-type ACVR1 and those with the R206H mutation. The authors demonstrated that ACVR1 is not a target of BYL719 based on NanoBRET assay data, suggesting that BYL719's effect is not specific to FOP cells, even though they used an FOP mouse model to show in vivo effects.

      The main effect of R206H mutation is the gain of function in response to Activin A. For most of the responses to other ACVR1 ligands (e.g. BMP6/7), we observe a slightly increased response in the presence of the mutation (which is consistent with previous research, usually labelling RH as a “weak activating mutant” unless Activin A is added (Song et al., PMID: 20463014)). Therefore, as expected, most of the differences between WT and RH mutant cells can be observed mostly upon Activin A addition, as observed, for example, in Figure 3 of our manuscript.

      We agree with the reviewer that, at the concentrations used, BYL719 does not specifically target FOP cells. However, we believe that it targets downstream pathways of PI3Kα inhibition that are essential for osteochondrogenic specification, regardless of mutation status. This therapeutic strategy aligns with other experimental drugs, including Palovarotene (validated for FOP) and Garetosmab and Saracatinib (in advanced clinical trials), which target Activin A function, ACVR1 activity, or osteochondrogenic differentiation irrespective of the mutant allele. Unlike these molecules, BYL719 has been chronically administered to patients (including children) without major side effects (Gallagher et al.; PMID: 38297009), further supporting its potential for safe long-term use.

      The authors should consider that the effect of Activin A on R206H cells is not identical to that of BMP6 on WT cells. If the authors aim to identify the target of BYL719 in FOP cells, they should compare R206H cells treated with Activin A/BYL719 to WT cells treated with BMP6/BYL719.

      We use Activin A and BMP6, both high-affinity ACVR1 ligands, to demonstrate, as observed in figure 6, that PI3Kα inhibition can inhibit the expression of genes within GO terms ossification and osteoblast differentiation. It is important to note, however, that Activin A canonical signaling receptor is ACVR1B. Since BYL719 blocks the induction of a heterotopic ossification gene expression signature common to Activin A and BMP6, in the context of the FOP mutation R206H, our results indicate that BYL719 inhibition affects a signaling pathway downstream of ACVR1, activated by either BMP6 (wild type receptor, relevant for non-genetic heterotopic ossifications) or Activin (R206H mutant receptor, relevant for FOP).

      We consider that the comparison (RH ACTA BYL vs WT BMP6 BYL) would provide confounding results raised from intrinsic model differences in basal expression programs (WT vs RH), and differences in the quantitative level of signaling of the different ligands at these specific doses. First, if we only consider SMAD1/5 signaling, Activin A and BMP6 won’t have identical signaling, and differences will arise from the strength of that signaling. Secondly, in the suggested comparison we would find, mostly, all the differential gene expression promoted by Activin A canonical signaling through type I receptors ACVR1B/ALK4 in complex with ACVR2A or ACVR2B, promoting SMAD2/3 activation (in addition to the altered signaling that ACVR1-R206H could promote). Examples of differential response in pSMAD1/5 in ACVR1-WT or RH with BMP ligands and R206H with Activin A ligand, and examples of pSMAD2/3 canonical signaling in R206H cells have been described in Ramachandran et al, PMID: 34003511; Hatsell et al., PMID: 26333933).

      Point 2: The interpretation of the data in the new Figure 5 is inappropriate. Based on the expression levels of SOX9, COL2A1, and ACAN, it is unclear whether the effect of BYL719 is due to the inhibition of differentiation or proliferation. The addition of Activin A showed no difference between ACVR1/WT and ACVR1/R206H cells, suggesting that these cells did not accurately replicate the FOP condition.

      To gain consistency in our manuscript, we decided to use an orthogonal and complementary approach in a completely new model. We performed new experiments of chondrogenic differentiation using murine MSCs from UBC-Cre-ERT2/ACVR1<sup>R206H</sup> knock-in mice. These cells, when treated with 4OH-tamoxifen, express the intracellular exons of human ACVR1<sup>R206H</sup> in the murine Acvr1 locus. Therefore, we can compare differentiation of wild type and R206H MSCs isolated form the same mice. We initiated the chondrogenic differentiation assay from confluent cells to minimize changes in cell proliferation throughout the process. These new results are shown in the new Figure 5F. Mutant (RH) cells display an enhanced chondrogenic response to activin A compared to wild type cells. The treatment with BYL719 decreased the expression of chondrogenic markers irrespective of the mutational status of ACVR1 in the cells, further supporting our previous results in this manuscript and published article (Valer et al., 2019a PMID: 31373426).

      Point 3: The additional investigation of RNA-seq data provided useful information but was insufficient to fully address the purpose of this study. The authors should identify downregulated genes by comparing WT cells treated with Activin A/BYL719 and Activin A alone and then compare these identified genes with those shown in Figure 5E. Additionally, they should compare R206H cells treated with Activin A/BYL719 to WT cells treated with BMP6/BYL719. These comparisons will clarify whether there are FOP-specific BYL719-regulated genes.

      We thank the reviewer for considering that RNAseq data provides useful information. As already discussed in our answer above, our results indicate that regardless of the ligand (Activin A or BMP6) and regardless of the ACVR1 mutation (WT, relevant for non-genetic heterotopic ossifications or RH, relevant for FOP), BYL719 can inhibit the expression of the genes relevant to endochondral ossification. In our opinion, this is a very relevant conclusion of this study.

      We have deeply considered the strategy proposed by the reviewer, comparing “WT cells treated with Activin A/BYL719 and Activin A alone and then compare these identified genes with those shown in Figure 5E” and/or comparing “R206H cells treated with Activin A/BYL719 to WT cells treated with BMP6/BYL719”. While we have discussed why we do not consider appropriate the first comparison proposed, there are a number of reasons why we are not confident that the second comparison would provide a straightforward conclusion.

      Regarding the second suggested comparison already in Main point 1, we consider that it would provide confounding results due to all the arguments detailed in Main point 1. Regarding the first suggested comparison, we also consider that it would provide confounding results. There are several reasons why we do not consider that the genes only found in the RH comparison can be confidently considered genes that are only affected by BYL719 in RH cells.

      First, the effect of BYL719 in an osteogenic-prone sample (for example, RH-ActA) is higher than the effect that we can observe in absence of this activation (for example, WT-ActA), as observed in the higher number of significantly downregulated genes in RH ActA BYL vs RH ActA comparison, compared to WT ActA BYL vs WT ActA. Similar results are observed in figure 3C, where the expressions of the genes are significantly inhibited in RH ActA compared to RH ActA BYL. This inhibition is not significantly observed in in WT ActA compared to WT ActA BYL because the osteogenic expression of these genes is already very weak in the absence of ACVR1 R206H. This weak signaling of pSMAD1/5 in the absence of osteogenic signaling (RH without ligand or, especially, WT with Activin A) has already been described (Ramachandran et al. MID: 34003511). Therefore, even though the inhibition is present in both comparisons, as observed in figure 6C, the extent of the observed effect is different. Second, we are comparing a different number of DEGs for each comparison between them. If we compare the 67 downregulated genes from one comparison and 38 downregulated genes from the other comparison, the unequal list size may inflate the number of unique genes in the group with more downregulated genes. To prove these concerns, we performed the comparison that the reviewer suggested and we found, for example, that amongst the 38 differentially downregulated ossification genes in (WT_ActA_BYL vs WT_ActA) and 67 differentially downregulated ossification genes in (RH_ActA_BYL vs RH_ActA), 39 genes were only found in the RH comparison, while 10 were only found in the WT comparison, and 28 were found in both.

      These effects are present, for example, when studying the ID genes, well-known downstream mediators of BMP signaling. In this case, ID1 is downregulated in both comparisons, while ID2, ID3, and ID4, are downregulated only in the RH-group, despite the fact that all ID1, ID2, ID3, and ID4 are similarly regulated and increase their expression with similar time curves upon BMP signaling activation (Yang et al., PMID: 23771884). Therefore, we consider that the comparisons proposed will not help us to identify specific BYL719-regulated genes relevant for FOP and/or ACVR1 R206H signaling. Again, we consider that BYL719 effect is not specific of FOP cells. Our results show that regardless of the ligand (Activin A or BMP6) and regardless of the ACVR1 mutation (WT, relevant for non-genetic heterotopic ossifications or RH, relevant for FOP), BYL719 can inhibit the expression of the genes linked to ossification and osteoblast differentiation, which could be important for the treatment of FOP and non-genetic heterotopic ossifications.

      Point 4: The data in Figure 7 are not relevant to the aim of this study because the cell lines used in these experiments did not have ACVR1/R206H mutations. The authors mentioned that BMP6 is a ligand for ACVR1 and, therefore, these experiments reflect the situation of inflammatory cells in FOP. This is inappropriate and not rational. As mentioned above, the effect of Activin A on FOP cells is not identical to the effect of BMP6 in wild-type cells. The data in Figure 7 indicated that the effect of BYL719 is unrelated to the presence of BMP6, clearly demonstrating that these experiments are not related to the activation of ACVR1. In the gene expression analyses, almost all genes showed no changes with the addition of BMP6. Only TGF and CCL2 showed upregulation in THP1 cells, and the treatment with BYL719 failed to inhibit the effect of BMP6, suggesting that these experiments merely demonstrate the effect of BYL719 on inflammatory cells irrespective of the presence of the HO signal.

      We consider that Figure 7 is relevant to the aim of this study. As shown in Fig. 8, treatment of FOP mice with BYL719 led to a decreased recruitment of immune cells within the FOP lesions, suggesting a direct effect of BYL719 in immune cells. This is very relevant for the FOP pathology, since flare-ups have been linked with inflammatory episodes since the very early characterization of the disease (Mejias-Rivera et al., PMID: 38672135). Given the technical difficulties to transduce THP1, RAW264 and HMC1 cell lines with lentiviral particles carrying ACVR1 R206H, we decided to partially recapitulate ACVR1 R206H activation with recombinant BMP6 and to test the effect of BYL719 in these conditions. In these models, we found that BYL719 inhibited the expression of key genes driving immune cell activation, in a cell-type and ligand independent manner. To clarify this rationale, we have swapped Figures 7 and 8 and adjusted our conclusions accordingly. We have softened our interpretations, emphasizing the absence of the ACVR1 R206H mutant receptor in these experiments.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Summary of Revisions

      We sincerely thank the editors and reviewers for their thorough assessment and constructive feedback, which has greatly improved our manuscript. We have carefully addressed all concerns as summarized below:

      In response to the requests made by Reviewer #1:

      • Clarified task design and acknowledged its limitations regarding endpoint accuracy control.

      • Included analysis comparing the effects of cerebellar block on within-trial versus inter-trial movements.

      • Clearly defined target groupings, replacing the term “single-joint” with “movements with low coupling torques” and “multi-joint” with “movements with high coupling torques”: definitions which are now supported by a supplementary material describing the net torque data as a function of the targets.

      • Added detailed descriptions of trial success criteria, based on timing, and positional constraints.

      • Expanded figures illustrating the effect of the cerebellar block on movement decomposition and variability in joint space and across different target directions.

      In response to the requests made by Reviewer #2:

      • Included an explicit discussion highlighting why the acute reduction in muscle torque during cerebellar block is likely due to agonist weakness rather than cocontraction, emphasizing the rationale behind our torque-centric analysis.

      • Clearly defined trial success criteria and included the timing and accuracy constraints used in our study.

      • Clarified our rationale for grouping targets based on shoulder flexion/extension, clearly justified by interaction torque analysis.

      • Revised the caption and legend of Figure 3d for clarity and included partial correlation results to account for the variability across monkeys for the analysis of reduction in hand velocity vs. coupling torque in control. 

      In response to the requests made by Reviewer #3:

      • Included electrophysiological validation of the accuracy of targeting the superior cerebellar peduncle from one of the monkeys used in the experiment.

      • Provided new analyses comparing movement decomposition and variability between slower and faster movements within the cerebellar block condition.

      • Revised manuscript text to clarify terminology and clearly explained the rationale behind target groupings and torque analyses.

      • Expanded discussion sections to better explain the relationships between timing deficits, movement decomposition, trajectory variability, and faulty motor commands.

      • Clarified methodological choices regarding our analysis timeframe and acknowledged limitations related to the distinction between feedforward and feedback control.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary:

      In a previous work, Prut and colleagues had shown that during reaching, high-frequency stimulation of the cerebellar outputs resulted in reduced reach velocity. Moreover, they showed that the stimulation produced reaches that deviated from a straight line, with the shoulder and elbow movements becoming less coordinated. In this report, they extend their previous work by the addition of modeling results that investigate the relationship between the kinematic changes and torques produced at the joints. The results show that the slowing is not due to reductions in interaction torques alone, as the reductions in velocity occur even for movements that are single joints. More interestingly, the experiment revealed evidence for the decomposition of the reaching movement, as well as an increase in the variance of the trajectory.

      Strengths:

      This is a rare experiment in a non-human primate that assessed the importance of cerebellar input to the motor cortex during reaching.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive feedback on our study. We particularly appreciate their recognition of the novelty and importance of our experimental approach in non-human primates, as well as their insightful summary of our key findings.

      Weaknesses:

      My major concerns are described below.

      If I understand the task design correctly, the monkeys did not need to stop their hand at the target. I think this design may be suboptimal for investigating the role of the cerebellum in control of reaching because a number of earlier works have found that the cerebellum's contributions are particularly significant as the movement ends, i.e., stopping at the target. For example, in mice, interposed nucleus neurons tend to be most active near the end of the reach that requires extension, and their activation produces flexion forces during the reach (Becker and Person 2019). Indeed, the inactivation of interposed neurons that project to the thalamus results in overshooting of reaching movements (Low et al. 2018). Recent work has also found that many Purkinje cells show a burst-pause pattern as the reach nears its endpoint, and stimulation of the mossy fibers tends to disrupt endpoint control (Calame et al. 2023). Thus, the fact that the current paper has no data regarding endpoint control of the reach is puzzling to me.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s point that cerebellar contributions can be particularly critical near the endpoint of a reach. In our task design, monkeys were indeed required to hold at the target briefly—100 ms for Monkeys S and P, and 150 ms for Monkeys C and M—before receiving the reward. However,  given the size of the targets and the velocity of movements, it often happened that the monkeys didn’t have to stop their movements fully to obtain the reward. Importantly, we relaxed the task’s requirements (by increasing the target size and reducing the temporal constraints) to enable the monkeys to perform a sufficient number of successful trials under both the control and the cerebellar block conditions. This was necessary as we found that strict criteria regarding these parameters yielded a very low success rate in the cerebellar block condition. Nevertheless, as we appreciate now, this task design is suboptimal for studying endpoint accuracy which is an important aspect of cerebellar control. In the methods section of our revised manuscript, we have clarified this aspect of the task design and acknowledged that it is sub-optimal for examining the role of the cerebellum in end-point control (lines 475-485). The task design of our future studies will explicitly address this point more carefully.

      Because stimulation continued after the cursor had crossed the target, it is interesting to ask whether this disruption had any effects on the movements that were task-irrelevant. The reason for asking this is because we have found that whereas during task-relevant eye or tongue movements the Purkinje cells are strongly modulated, the modulations are much more muted when similar movements are performed but are task-irrelevant (Pi et al., PNAS 2024; Hage et al. Biorxiv 2024). Thus, it is interesting to ask whether the effects of stimulation were global and affected all movements, or were the effects primarily concerned with the task-relevant movements.

      This is an insightful suggestion. The behavioral task in the present study was designed with a focus on task-relevant, reward-associated reaching movements. Nevertheless, we also have data on the inter-trial movements (e.g., return-to-center reaches) under continued cerebellar stimulation, which were not directly associated with reward. In response to the reviewer’s comment, we compared the effects of cerebellar block on endpoint velocities between these two types of movements. We found that reductions in peak hand velocity during inter-trial movements were significantly smaller than those observed during the target directed reaches. We have updated the Results section of our manuscript (lines 125-137) and expanded our supplementary document (Supplementary Figure S1) to include this analysis. 

      If the schematic in Figure 1 is accurate, it is difficult for me to see how any of the reaching movements can be termed single joint. In the paper, T1 is labeled as a single joint, and T2T4 are labeled as dual-joint. The authors should provide data to justify this.

      The reviewer is correct. Movements to all targets involved both shoulder and elbow joints, but the degree to which each joint participated varied in a targetspecific manner. In our original manuscript, we used the term “single-joint” to refer to movements in which one joint was nearly stationary, resulting in minimal coupling torque at the adjacent joint. Specifically, for Targets 1 and 5, the net torque—and thus acceleration— at the elbow was negligible, causing the shoulder to experience low coupling torques (as illustrated in Figure 3c of our revised manuscript). Following this comment and  to avoid confusion, we have now explained this explicitly in the revised manuscript (lines 178-187). This is supported by Supplementary Figure S2 demonstrating the net torques at the shoulder and elbow for movements to each target. We have also replaced the term ‘singlejoint movements’  and ‘multi-joint movements’  with  ‘movements with low coupling torques’ and ‘movements with high coupling torques’ respectively in our revised manuscript (lines 178-180, 204-207, 225-227, 230-232, 305-307, and 362-365).  

      Because at least part of this work was previously analyzed and published, information should be provided regarding which data are new.

      While some of the same animals and stimulation protocol were presented in prior work, the inverse-dynamics modeling, the analyses exploring progressive velocity changes across trials under a cerebellar block, and the relationship of motor noise to movement velocity are newly reported in this manuscript. We have included a clear statement in the Methods section specifying which components of the dataset and analyses are entirely new (lines 582-589).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Before the results are presented, it is useful to present the experimental paradigm in more detail. For example, after the center-out movement was completed, was the monkey required to hold at the target location? How did the next trial begin (re-centering movement)? Next, specify the stimulation protocol, noting that each session was divided into 3-4 blocks of stimulation and not stimulation, with each block 50-80 trials.

      We have updated the results section of our revised manuscript (lines 91-104) to present the experimental paradigm in more detail according to the reviewer’s advice.

      (2) Figure 1. Hand velocity does not show how the reach was completed. Did the subjects stop at the target or simply shoot through it and turn around without stopping? Why are the traces cut off?

      Monkeys were indeed required to hold at the target briefly (100-150 ms) before receiving the reward. However,  given the size of the targets and the velocity of movements, it often happened that the monkeys didn’t have to stop their movements fully to obtain the reward. The hand velocity profile shown in Figure 1b and the torque profiles shown in Figures 2a and 2b correspond to the period from movement onset to the entry of the control cursor into the peripheral target which marked the end of the movement for the trial. Since the monkeys didn’t have to stop their movements fully for the trial to end, the traces appear cut off at the beginning of the deceleration/stopping phase of the movement. We have updated the captions of Figures 1b, 2a, and 2b to include this information (lines 869-872 and 882-884).  

      (3) Maybe state that the data regarding reaction times are not presented because of the task design in which the go signal was predictable.

      In monkeys M and C, the timing of the go signal was fixed and therefore predictable. Furthermore, they were also allowed a grace period of 200 ms before the go signal to facilitate predictive timing which often resulted in negative reaction times. However, in Monkeys S and P, the go signal was variable in timing and the monkeys were not allowed to initiate the movements before the go signal. In our previous studies (Nashef et al., 2019; Israely et al. 2025), we reported increased reaction times under cerebellar block. However, since the present study focuses specifically on execution-related motor deficits, we did not analyze reaction time data. 

      (4) Please provide the data and analysis regarding the entire reach, including the period after the cursor crosses the target and returns to the center position.

      We compared the peak hand velocity of the target-directed movements to the inter-trial return-to-center movements. Cerebellar block produced significantly smaller reductions in peak hand velocity during inter-trial movements compared to within-trial reaches. The results section of our revised manuscript (lines 125137) and the supplementary material (Supplementary Figure S1) have been updated accordingly. While the behavioral task in the present study was designed with a focus on task-relevant, reward-associated reaching movements, it will be interesting to examine in detail the effect of cerebellar block on spontaneous movements in a future study.

      (5) Figure 5. To illustrate the decomposition of multijoint movements into a sequence of single joint movements, I suggest plotting movements in joint space (in addition to Cartesian space as you have done now). The results in Figure 5 are most interesting and thus should be expanded. Please provide this data using the format in Figure 1C, that is, as a function of direction.

      Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have plotted sample trajectories in joint-velocity (Supplementary Figures 3a and b) and position space (Supplementary Figures 4a and b) to highlight the decomposition of multi-joint movements and increased inter-trial trajectory variability respectively during the cerebellar block. Additionally, we also analyzed movement decomposition and trajectory variability as a function of target direction (Supplementary Figures 3c and 4c respectively). The corresponding text in the Results section has been updated accordingly (lines 256-261, 267-271, 277-278 and 280-288).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This manuscript asks an interesting and important question: what part of 'cerebellar' motor dysfunction is an acute control problem vs a compensatory strategy to the acute control issue? The authors use a cerebellar 'blockade' protocol, consisting of high-frequency stimuli applied to the cerebellar peduncle which is thought to interfere with outflow signals. This protocol was applied in monkeys performing center outreaching movements and has been published from this laboratory in several preceding studies. I found the takehome-message broadly convincing and clarifying - that cerebellar block reduces muscle activation acutely particularly in movements that involve multiple joints and therefore invoke interaction torques, and that movements progressively slow down to in effect 'compensate' for these acute tone deficits. The manuscript was generally well written, and the data was clear, convincing, and novel. My comments below highlight suggestions to improve clarity and sharpen some arguments.

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful and constructive feedback. We are grateful for their recognition of the significance of our findings regarding acute and compensatory motor responses following a cerebellar block.

      Primary comments:

      (1) Torque vs. tone: Is it known whether this type of cerebellar blockade is reducing muscle tone or inducing any type of acute co-contraction that could influence limb velocity through mechanisms different than 'atonia'? If so, the authors should discuss this information in the discussion section starting around line 336, and clarify that this motivates (if it does) the focus on 'torques' rather than muscle activation. Relatedly, besides the fact that there are joints involved, is there a reason there is so much emphasis on torque per se? If the muscle is deprived of sufficient drive, it would seem that it would be more straightforward to conceptualize the deficit as one of insufficient timed drive to a set of muscles than joint force. Some text better contextualizing the choices made here would be sufficient to address this concern. I found statements like those in the introduction "hand velocity was low initially, reflecting a primary muscle torque deficit" to be lacking in substance. Either that statement is self-evident or the alternative was not made clear. Finally, emphasize that it is a loss of self-generated torque at the shoulder that accounts for the velocity deficits. At times the phrasing makes it seem that there is a loss of some kind of passive torque.

      We appreciate the reviewer's emphasis on distinguishing between reduced muscle tone and altered co-contraction patterns as potential explanations for decreased limb velocity. Our focus on torques per se arises from previous studies suggesting that a core deficit in cerebellar ataxia is impaired prediction of passive coupling torques (Bastian et al., 1996). In our study, we demonstrate that motor deficits in cerebellar ataxia result in fact from both the inability to compensate for passive coupling torques and an acute insufficiency in the ability to generate active muscle torques.

      The muscle torque, representing the sum of all muscle forces acting at a joint, can indeed be reduced by any of the two mechanisms: (i) co-contraction of agonist and antagonist muscles, and/or (ii) insufficient agonist muscle activity (i.e., agonist weakness). In cerebellar ataxia, co-contraction has been proposed as a simplifying strategy to stabilize stationary joints during decomposed multi-joint movements (Bastian et al., 1996). In our experiments, this strategy would likely emerge gradually following cerebellar block similar to the adaptive slowing of movements aimed at reducing inter-joint interactions. However, we found that irrespective of the magnitude of coupling torques involved, reduction in the velocity of movements also occurred immediately following cerebellar block—a pattern less consistent with gradually emerging compensatory strategies. We therefore argue that this acute onset of movement slowing was mainly driven by agonist weakness. Our argument is further supported by previous studies which attributed reduced agonist muscle activity as a cause for the slowing of voluntary movements in individuals with cerebellar lesions (Hallet et al. 1991; Wild et al., 1996). Additionally, early studies have also reported muscle weakness (asthenia) and hypotonia acutely following cerebellar injury in humans (Haines et al., 2007) and experimental lesions in animals (Luciani, 1893; Bremer et al., 1935; Fulton & Dow, 1937; Granit et al., 1955).

      We have modified the discussion section of our revised manuscript (lines 366-376) to explain/clarify this. Additionally, we have also underscored that the observed velocity deficits primarily reflect a reduction of self-generated torque at the shoulder (whether acute or adaptive), rather than any reduction in passive torque (lines 350-352).

      (2) Please clarify some of the experimental metrics: Ln 94 RESULTS. The success rate is used as a primary behavioral readout, but what constitutes success is not clearly defined in the methods. In addition to providing a clear definition in the methods section, it would also be helpful for the authors to provide a brief list of criteria used to determine a 'successful' movement in the results section before the behavioral consequences of stimulation are described. In particular, the time and positional error requirements should be clear.

      Successful trials were defined as trials in which monkeys didn’t leave the center position before the “Go” signal and entered the peripheral target within a permitted movement time. We have updated the results (lines 91-104) and methods (lines 475-485) section of our revised manuscript to include (i) the timing criteria of each phase of the trials and (ii) the size of the peripheral targets indicating the tolerance for endpoint accuracy.  

      (3) Based on the polar plot in Figure 1c, it seemed odd to consider Targets 1-4 outward and 5-8 inward movements, when 1 and 5 are side-to-side. Is there a rationale for this grouping or might results be cleaner by cleanly segregating outward (targets 2-4) and inward (targets 6-8) movements? Indeed, by Figure 3 where interaction torques are measured, this grouping would seem to align with the hypothesis much more cleanly since it is with T2,T3,and T4 where clear coupling torques deficits are seen with cerebellar block.

      We acknowledge the reviewer's observation regarding the classification of targets 1 and 5 as side-to-side movements rather than strictly "outward" or "inward." In the initial section of our results, we grouped the targets based on shoulder joint movements: "outward" targets involved shoulder flexion, while "inward" targets involved shoulder extension. This classification highlighted the more pronounced effect of cerebellar block on movements requiring shoulder flexion compared to those requiring shoulder extension. For subsequent analyses, we focused on the effects of cerebellar block on movements to "outward" targets, which included directions involving low (target 1) or high (targets 2–4) coupling torques. To clarify this aspect, we have revised our manuscript to explain our definition of "outward" (targets 1–4) and "inward" (targets 5–8) target groupings based on shoulder  flexion and extension movements respectively (lines 117-120).

      (4) I did not follow Figure 3d. Both the figure axis labels and the description in the main text were difficult to follow. Furthermore, the color code per animal made me question whether the linear regression across the entire dataset was valid, or would be better performed within animal, and the regressions summarized across animals. The authors should look again at this section and figure.

      We have revised the legend of Figure 3d to include a detailed explanation of how the value along each axis is computed  (lines 908-920 of the revised manuscript). Please note that  the color coding of the data points is as per the target number (T1-T4) and not the monkey number (as denoted in the figure legend). Also, pooling of data across monkeys was done after confirming that data from each animal expressed a similar trend. Specifically, the correlation coefficients were all positive but statistically significant in 3 out of the 4 monkeys. Following the reviewers’ feedback, we now performed  a partial correlation analysis (which controls for the variability across monkeys) and found a significant correlation (r = 0.32, p < 0.001) between reduction in peak hand velocities during cerebellar block and the net coupling torque impulse. We have updated the manuscript to include the result of the partial correlation analysis (lines 173-176).  

      (5) Line 206+ The rationale for examining movement decomposition with a cerebellar block is presented as testing the role of the cerebellum in timing. Yet it is not spelled out what movement decomposition and trajectory variability have to do with motor timing per se.

      The reviewer is right and the relations between timing, decomposition and variability need to be explicitly explained. In the results  section of our revised manuscript, we have explained how decomposed movements and trajectory variability may reflect impaired temporal coordination across multiple joints—a critical cerebellar function (lines 235-244).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Rephrase the findings, starting Line 232. Here the authors state, "Next, we asked whether movement decomposition was mainly due to lower hand velocities. We therefore selected a subset of control trials that matched the cerebellar block trials in their peak velocity. However, even though movement decomposition in these control trials was higher compared to all control trials, it was still significantly lower than velocity matched cerebellar block trials." I suggest inverting the final sentence to: "Movement decomposition in control trials was significantly lower than velocity-matched cerebellar block trials, even though these control trials themselves had somewhat higher decomposition indices than all control trials together." A similar issue pops up with trajectory variability below that simply requires some editing to be less clunky.

      Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have revised the sentences related to movement decomposition and trajectory variability. These sentences now reads as follows: 

      (lines 267-271 in the revised manuscript): “Movement decomposition in control trials was significantly lower than velocity-matched cerebellar block trials (p < 0.001; Figure 5c), even though these control trials themselves had 11.0% (CI [5.2, 17.0], p = 0.03) higher decomposition than the mean value calculated across all control trials.” 

      (lines 280-288 in the revised manuscript): “ When we compared the subset of velocitymatched control and cerebellar block trials, we found that cerebellar block trials exhibited 34.6% (CI [26.2, 43.2], p < 0.001) higher trajectory variability (Figure 5e). Normally, slower movements are also less variable due to the speed-accuracy tradeoff (Plamondon and Alimi 1997). Indeed, the trajectory variability in this subset of slower control trials was 5.5% (CI [0.9, 9.9], p = 0.02) lower than that of all control trials. In other words, despite slower movements, cerebellar block led to increased trajectory variability.”

      (2) Typo: Ln 73 sequences, not sequence.

      Typo error was corrected (line 75 of revised manuscript). 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In their manuscript, "Disentangling acute motor deficits and adaptive responses evoked by the loss of cerebellar output," Sinha and colleagues aim to identify distinct causes of motor impairments seen when perturbing cerebellar circuits. This goal is an important one, given the diversity of movement-related phenotypes in patients with cerebellar lesions or injuries, which are especially difficult to dissect given the chronic nature of the circuit damage. To address this goal, the authors use high-frequency stimulation (HFS) of the superior cerebellar peduncle in monkeys performing reaching movements. HFS provides an attractive approach for transiently disrupting cerebellar function previously published by this group. First, they found a reduction in hand velocities during reaching, which was more pronounced for outward versus inward movements. By modeling inverse dynamics, they find evidence that shoulder muscle torques are especially affected. Next, the authors examine the temporal evolution of movement phenotypes over successive blocks of HFS trials. Using this analysis, they find that in addition to the acute, specific effects on muscle torques in early HFS trials, there was an additional progressive reduction in velocity during later trials, which they interpret as an adaptive response to the inability to effectively compensate for interaction torques during cerebellar block. Finally, the authors examine movement decomposition and trajectory, finding that even when low-velocity reaches are matched to controls, HFS produces abnormally decomposed movements and higher than expected variability in trajectory.

      Strengths:

      Overall, this work provides important insight into how perturbation of cerebellar circuits can elicit diverse effects on movement across multiple timescales.

      The HFS approach provides temporal resolution and enables analysis that would be hard to perform in the context of chronic lesions or slow pharmacological interventions. Thus, this study describes an important advance over prior methods of circuit disruption, and their approach can be used as a framework for future studies that delve deeper into how additional aspects of sensorimotor control are disrupted (e.g., response to limb perturbations).

      In addition, the authors use well-designed behavioral approaches and analysis methods to distinguish immediate from longer-term adaptive effects of HFS on behavior. Moreover, inverse dynamics modeling provides important insight into how movements with different kinematics and muscle dynamics might be differentially disrupted by cerebellar perturbation.

      We thank the reviewer for their detailed assessment and thoughtful comments and greatly appreciate their positive feedback.  

      Weaknesses:

      The argument that there are acute and adaptive effects to perturbing cerebellar circuits is compelling, but there seems to be a lost opportunity to leverage the fast and reversible nature of the perturbations to further test this idea and strengthen the interpretation. Specifically, the authors could have bolstered this argument by looking at the effects of terminating HFS - one might hypothesize that the acute impacts on muscle torques would quickly return to baseline in the absence of HFS, whereas the longer-term adaptive component would persist in the form of aftereffects during the 'washout' period. As is, the reversible nature of the perturbation seems underutilized in testing the authors' ideas.

      We agree that our approach could more explicitly exploit the rapid reversibility of high-frequency stimulation (HFS) by examining post-stimulation ‘washout’ periods. However, for the present dataset, we ended the session after the set of cerebellar block trials without using an explicit washout period. We plan to study the effect of the cerebellar block on immediate post-block washout trials in the future.    

      The analysis showing that there is a gradual reduction in velocity during what the authors call an adaptive phase is convincing. That said, the argument is made that this is due to difficulty in compensating for interaction torques. Even if the inward targets (i.e., targets 68) do not show a deficit during the acute phase, these targets still have significant interaction torques (Figure 3c). Given the interpretation of the data as presented, it is not clear why disruption of movement during the adaptive phase would not be seen for these targets as well since they also have large interaction torques. Moreover, it is difficult to delve into this issue in more detail, as the analyses in Figures 4 and 5 omit the inward targets.

      The reviewer is right and  movements to Targets 6–8 (inward) were seemingly unaffected despite also involving significant interaction torques. Specifically, we noted that while outward targets (2–4) tend to involve higher coupling torque impulses on average, this alone does not fully explain the differential impact of cerebellar block, as illustrated by discrepancies at the individual target level (e.g., target 7 vs. target 1). We propose two possible explanations: (1) a bias toward shoulder flexion in the effect of cerebellar block—consistent with earlier studies showing ipsilateral flexor activation or tone changes following stimulation or lesioning of the deep cerebellar nuclei; and (2) posture-related facilitation of inward (shoulder extension) movements from the central starting position. This point is addressed in the Discussion section (lines 404-433  in the revised manuscript).

      The text in the Introduction and in the prior work developing the HFS approach overstates the selectivity of the perturbations. First, there is an emphasis on signals transmitted to the neocortex. As the authors state several times in the Discussion, there are many subcortical targets of the cerebellar nuclei as well, and thus it is difficult to disentangle target-specific behavioral effects using this approach. Second, the superior cerebellar peduncle contains both cerebellar outputs and inputs (e.g., spinocerebellar). Therefore, the selectivity in perturbing cerebellar output feels overstated. Readers would benefit from a more agnostic claim that HFS affects cerebellar communication with the rest of the nervous system, which would not affect the major findings of the study.

      The reviewer is right that the superior cerebellar peduncle carries both descending and ascending fibers, and that cerebellar nuclei project to subcortical as well as cortical targets. Therefore, we cannot rule out the fact that the effect of HFS  may be mediated in part through pathways other than the cerebello-thalamo-cortical pathway (as mentioned in the Discussion section). However, it is also important to note that in primates the cerebellar-thalamo-cortical (CTC) pathway greatly expanded (at the expense of the cerbello-rubro-spinal tract) in mediating cerebellar control of voluntary movements (Horne and Butler, 1995). The cerebello-subcortical pathways diminished in importance over the course of evolution (Nathan and Smith, 1982, Padel et al., 1981, ten Donkelaar, 1988). Previously we found that the ascending spinocerebellar axons which enter the cerebellum through the superior cerebellar peduncle (SCP) are weakly task-related and the descending system is quite small (Cohen et al, 2017). We have clarified these points and acknowledged that HFS disrupts cerebellar communication broadly, rather than solely the cerebellothalamo-cortical pathway in the methods section of our revised manuscript (lines 531544).  

      The text implies that increased movement decomposition and variability must be due to noise. However, this assumption is not tested. It is possible that the impairments observed are caused by disrupted commands, independent of whether these command signals are noisy. In other words, commands could be low noise but still faulty.

      We recognize the reviewer’s concern about linking movement decomposition and trial-to-trial trajectory variability with motor noise. We interpret these motor abnormalities as a form of motor noise in the sense that they are generated by faulty motor commands. We draw our interpretation from the findings of previous research work which show that the cerebellum aids in the state estimation of the limb and subsequent generation of accurate feedforward commands. Therefore, disruption of the cerebellar output may lead to faulty motor commands resulting in the observed asynchronous joint activations (i.e., movement decomposition) and unpredictable trajectories (i.e., increased trial-to-trial variability). Both observed deficits resemble increased motor noise. This point is presented in our Discussion section (lines 436-458 of the revised manuscript),

      Throughout the text, the use of the term 'feedforward control' seems unnecessary. To dig into the feedforward component of the deficit, the authors could quantify the trajectory errors only at the earliest time points (e.g., in Figure 5d), but even with this analysis, it is difficult to disentangle feedforward- and feedback-mediated effects when deficits are seen throughout the reach. While outside the scope of this study, it would be interesting to explore how feedback responses to limb perturbation are affected in control versus HFS conditions. However, as is, these questions are not explored, and the claim of impaired feedforward control feels overstated.

      We agree that to strictly focus on feedforward control, we could have examined the measured variables in the first 50-100 ms of the movement which has been shown to be unaffected by feedback responses (Pruszynski et al. 2008, Todorov and Jordan 2002,  Pruszynski  and Scott 2012, Crevecoeur  et al. 2013). However, in our task, the amplitude of movements made by the monkeys was small, and therefore the response measures in the first 50-100 ms were too small for a robust estimation. Also, fixing a time window led to an unfair comparison between control and cerebellar block trials, in which velocity was significantly reduced and therefore movement time was longer.  Therefore, we used the peak velocity, torque impulse at the peak velocity, and maximum deviation of the hand trajectory as response measures. We have acknowledged this point in the methods section of our revised manuscript (lines 590-600). We have also refrained from using the term feedforward control throughout the text of our revised manuscript as suggested by the reviewer.

      The terminology 'single-joint' movement is a bit confusing. At a minimum, it would be nice to show kinematics during different target reaches to demonstrate that certain targets are indeed single joint movements. More of an issue, however, is that it seems like these are not actually 'single-joint' movements. For example, Figure 2c shows that target 1 exhibits high elbow and shoulder torques, but in the text, T1 is described as a 'single-joint' reach (e.g. lines 155-156). The point that I think the authors are making is that these targets have low interaction torques. If that is the case, the terminology should be changed or clarified to avoid confusion.

      Indeed, as reviewer #1 also noted, movements to targets 1 and 5 are not purely single-joint but rather have relatively low coupling torques. Movements to all targets involved both shoulder and elbow joints, but the degree to which each joint participated varied in a target-specific manner. In our original manuscript, we used the term “single-joint” to refer to movements in which one joint was largely stationary, resulting in minimal coupling torque at the adjacent joint. Specifically, for Targets 1 and 5, the net torque—and thus acceleration—at the elbow was negligible, causing the shoulder to experience low coupling torques (as illustrated in Figure 3c of our revised manuscript). Following this comment and  to avoid confusion, we have now explained this explicitly in the revised manuscript (lines 178-187). This is supported by Supplementary Figure S2 demonstrating the net torques at the shoulder and elbow for movements to each target. We have also replaced the term ‘single-joint movements’  and ‘multi-joint movements’  with  ‘movements with low coupling torques’ and ‘movements with high coupling torques’ respectively in our revised manuscript (lines 178-180, 204-207, 225-227, 230-232, 305-307, and 362-365).

      The labels in Figure 3d are confusing and could use more explanation in the figure legend. In Figure 3d, it is stated that data from all monkeys is pooled. However, if there is a systematic bias between animals, this could generate spurious correlations. Were correlations also calculated for each animal separately to confirm the same trend between velocity and coupling torques holds for each animal?

      We have revised the legend of Figure 3d to include a detailed explanation of how the values along each axis are computed  (lines 908-920 of the revised manuscript). Please note that the pooling of data across monkeys was done after confirming that data from each animal expressed a similar trend. Specifically, the correlation coefficients were all positive but statistically significant in 3 out of the 4 monkeys. Moreover, following the reviewers’ feedback, we also did a partial correlation analysis (which controls for the variability across monkeys) and found a significant correlation (r = 0.32, p < 0.001) between reduction in peak hand velocities during cerebellar block and the net coupling torque impulse. We have updated the manuscript to include the result of the partial correlation analysis (lines 173-176).  

      In Table S1, it would be nice to see target-specific success rates. The data would suggest that targets with the highest interaction torques will have the largest reduction in success rates, especially during later HFS trials. Is this the case?

      The breakdown of the percentage increase in failure rate due to cerebellar block as a function of target direction is shown in Author response image 1 inserted to this response. 

      Author response image 1.

      Effect of cerebellar block on failure rate. The change in failure rate for the cerebellar block trials was computed relative to the control trials per session per target. The depicted values are the mean ± 95% confidence intervals across all sessions pooled from all four monkeys. The individual means of each monkey are overlaid. Statistical significance is denoted as follows: p ≥ 0.05NS, p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001*** [T1-8: Targets 1-8]

      The increase in failure rate due to cerebellar block was not affected by the target direction (linear mixed model analysis,  target x trial-type interaction effect: p  = 0.44).  However, it should be noted that success/failure depends on several factors beyond just the execution related impaired limb dynamics. In a previous study (Nashef et al. 2019) we identified several causes of failure such as (i) not entering the central target in time, (ii) premature exit from the central target before the ‘go’ signal,  (iii) reaction time longer than the time permitted to reach the peripheral target after the ‘go’ signal, or (iv) not holding at the peripheral target for the required time at the end of the movement.   

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) It would be helpful to provide some supplemental information on electrophysiological validation of the targeting in each monkey. Was any variability in targeting observed (e.g., some targeting was more effective at eliciting cortical responses)? If so, does targeting variability relate to any of the variability in behavioral effects of HFS across monkeys?

      Although we currently do not have an exact measure of the proportion of fibers blocked by HFS, our targeting approach consistently elicited robust cortical responses across monkeys. Specifically, we implanted the stimulating electrode at the location that produced the maximum peak-to-peak evoked responses in the primary motor cortex. Author response image 2 in this response demonstrates that even a slight deviation (~0.5 mm) from this optimal site reduced these responses substantially.:

      Author response image 2.

      Evoked responses in the primary motor cortex as a function of the location of the stimulation site. [LEFT] Coronal T2-weighted MRI showing the planned trajectory to target the superior cerebellar peduncle (location marked by the tip of the arrowhead) through a round chamber suitably positioned over the skull. [RIGHT] Evoked multi-unit (300-7500 Hz) responses from one of the recording electrodes in the primary motor cortex are used to guide the stimulating electrode to the correct implant site. As the stimulating electrode was lowered deeper, maximum peak-to-peak evoked responses were obtained at a depth of 32.5 mm relative to the cortical surface. This was chosen as the implant site. Elevating or lowering the electrode by ~0.5 mm from this depth reduced the peak-to-peak response amplitude. 

      (2) The emphasis in the Introduction that HFS provides direct insight into deficits seen in patients with cerebellar disease or injury is a bit overstated. Patients have very diverse etiologies, only a modest number of which might be faithfully mimicked by SCP HFS. I would suggest some text acknowledging that this is only a limited model for cerebellar disease or injury.

      We agree with the reviewer that the high-frequency stimulation of the superior cerebellar peduncle provides a limited model that does not fully replicate the diverse pathologies seen in cerebellar disease or injury. In fact, in the introduction section (lines 53-59 of our revised manuscript) we have mentioned that the discrepancy in the conclusions of various clinical studies may reflect the heterogeneity of the individuals with cerebellar lesions who often have differences in lesion etiology and associated damage beyond the cerebellum itself. While this may preclude the generalization of our findings to the wider clinical population per se, our approach offers a precise and controlled method to investigate the immediate and adaptive changes in motor behavior following the disruption of cerebellar signals.

      (3) Do animals with HFS show less decomposition and trajectory variability in their slower movements when compared to their faster movements? Comparisons are only made with velocity-matched control blocks, but the comparison of slower vs. faster reaches during HFS blocks would also be informative.

      To answer this point we classified movements during cerebellar block as either slow or fast based on the median peak hand velocity of the cerebellar block trials per target per session. We then computed the decomposition index and trajectory variability for the fast and slow movements during cerebellar block relative to control in the same way as in Figure 5 of our manuscript (i.e., the percentage change relative to control). Our analysis revealed significantly lower movement decomposition (p < 0.001) and reduced trajectory variability (p < 0.001) for slower movements compared to faster ones within the cerebellar block condition (Author response image 3).

      Author response image 3.

      Effect of slow and fast movements during cerebellar block on movement decomposition and trajectory variability. [LEFT] Change in decomposition index (i.e., the proportion of the movement time during which the movement was decomposed) for slow and fast cerebellar block trials relative to all control trials. The change in median decomposition was computed per session per target and then averaged across all eight targets to arrive at one value per session. The depicted values are the mean ± 95% confidence intervals across all sessions pooled from all four monkeys. The individual means of each monkey are overlaid. [RIGHT] Change in inter-trial trajectory variability for slow and fast cerebellar block trials relative to all control trials. The trajectory variability was measured as the standard deviation of the maximum perpendicular distance of the trajectories from the Y-axis after transforming them as in Figure 5d of the main text. The change in trajectory variability for the fast and slow cerebellar block trials was then computed per session per target and averaged across all eight targets to arrive at one value per session. The depicted values are the mean ± 95% confidence intervals across all sessions pooled from all four monkeys. The individual means of each monkey are overlaid. Statistical significance is denoted as follows: p ≥ 0.05NS, p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***. [Cbl: Cerebellar block].

      (4) Line 220- 'velocity' should be 'speed' or 'absolute velocity'?

      The term velocity was changed to speed in  the revised manuscript (line 255).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer 1 (Public review)

      Summary

      The results offer compelling evidence that L5-L5 tLTD depends on presynaptic NMDARs, a concept that has previously been somewhat controversial. It documents the novel finding that presynaptic NMDARs facilitate tLTD through their metabotropic signaling mechanism.

      We thank Reviewer 1 for their kind words and thoughtful feedback!

      Strengths

      The experimental design is clever and clean. The approach of comparing the results in cell pairs where NMDA is deleted either presynaptically or postsynaptically is technically insightful and yields decisive data. The MK801 experiments are also compelling.

      We are very grateful for this kind feedback!

      Weaknesses

      No major weaknesses were noted by this reviewer.

      We were happy to see that Reviewer 1 had no concerns in the Public Review. We address their Recommendations here below.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      There is one minor issue that the authors might want to address. In Figure 6C, the average time course of the controls (blue symbols) shows a clear decline in the baseline. The rate of this decline appears to be similar to the initial decline rate observed after inducing tLTD.

      Sorry, the x-axis was truncated so the first data points were not visible. We fixed Fig 6C as well as 6G, which suffered from the same problem.

      Reviewer 2 (Public review)

      Summary

      The study characterized the dependence of spike-timing-dependent long-term depression (tLTD) on presynaptic NMDA receptors and the intracellular cascade after NMDAR activation possibly involved in the observed decrease in glutamate probability release at L5-L5 synapses of the visual cortex in mouse brain slices.

      We are grateful for Reviewer 2’s thoughtful and detailed feedback!

      Strengths

      The genetic and electrophysiological experiments are thorough. The experiments are well-reported and mainly support the conclusions. This study confirms and extends current knowledge by elucidating additional plasticity mechanisms at cortical synapses, complementing existing literature.

      We were thrilled to see that the reviewer thinks our experiments are “thorough”, “well-reported” and they “mainly support the conclusions”!

      Weaknesses

      While one of the main conclusions (preNMDARs mediating presynaptic LTD) is resolved in a very convincing genetic approach, the second main conclusion of the manuscript (non-ionotropic preNMDARs) relies on the use of a high concentration of extracellular blockers (MK801, 2 mM; 7-clorokinurenic acid: 100 microM), but no controls for the specific actions of these compounds are shown.

      We thank the reviewer for calling our genetic approach “very convincing”!

      Regarding the pharmacological controls: for MK-801, we deliberately used a high extracellular concentration in the mM-range to match the intracellular concentrations used both in our own experiments and in prior studies (Berretta and Jones, 1996; Brasier and Feldman, 2008; Buchanan et al., 2012; Corlew et al., 2007; Humeau et al., 2003; Larsen et al., 2011; Rodríguez-Moreno et al., 2011; Rodríguez-Moreno and Paulsen, 2008). Our goal was to isolate the variable of application site (internal vs. external) while keeping concentration constant. If we had used the lower, more conventional µM-range extracellular concentrations (e.g., Huettner and Bean, 1988; Kemp et al., 1988; Tovar and Westbrook, 1999), differences in outcome might have reflected differences in drug efficacy rather than localization — particularly since failure to observe an effect at low concentrations would be hard to interpret.

      We now clarify this rationale in the revised manuscript (lines 578-585).

      As for 7-chlorokynurenic acid (7-CK), the 100 µM concentration we used is standard for effectively blocking the glycine-binding site of NMDARs (e.g., Nabavi et al., 2013).

      We also added two supplementary figures to show the effects of washing in MK-801 and 7-CK. In MK-801, responses are stable at low frequency (clarified in the manuscript lines 155-157 and Supp Fig 1 caption text). However, 7-CK suppresses responses appreciably, which takes time to stabilize. We clarify in the revised manuscript that in 7-CK experiments, we waited for this stabilization before inducing tLTD (lines 167-172 and Supp Fig 2 caption text). This additional suppression is consistent with 7-CK also acting as a potent competitive inhibitor of L-glutamate transport into synaptic vesicles (Bartlett et al., 1998).

      In addition, no direct testing for ions passing through preNMDAR has been performed.

      Sorry for being unclear, we have previously tested directly for ions passing through preNMDARs. For example, we showed blockade with Mg<sup>2+</sup> before (Abrahamsson et al., 2017; Wong et al., 2024), and we showed preNMDAR Ca<sup>2+</sup> supralinearities before (Abrahamsson et al., 2017; Buchanan et al., 2012). To improve the manuscript, we clarified the text accordingly (lines 140-141).

      It is not known if the results can be extrapolated to adult brain as the data were obtained from 11-18 days-old mice slices, a period during which synapses are still maturing and the cortex is highly plastic.

      Thank you, this is a good point. We address this point in the revised manuscript (lines 428-432). While our study focuses on the early postnatal period (P11–P18), when plasticity mechanisms are prominent and synaptic maturation is ongoing, we agree that extrapolation to the adult brain should be made with caution.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Points 1-3 were also found in the Public Review so are not addressed again here.

      (4) Results seem to be obtained in the absence of inhibition blocking and the role of inhibition in tLTD is not described. It should be indicated whether present results are obtained with or without the functional inhibitory synapse activation. If GABAergic synapses are not blocked authors need to show what happens when this inhibition is blocked.

      We agree that extracellular stimulation can inadvertently recruit inhibitory circuits. However, in our paired whole-cell recordings, synaptic responses are always subthreshold and exclusively reflect the direct connection between the two recorded neurons (Chou et al., 2024; Song et al., 2005). Under these conditions, inhibitory synapses are not activated, and we therefore did not apply GABAergic blockers. We thank the reviewer for raising this, which is now clarified in the Methods (lines 539-541) of the revised manuscript.

      (5) In some figures, the number of experiments seems to be low, and this number of experiments might be increased (Figures 1C, 3C, 4B).

      We acknowledge that the number of experiments in these figures is modest, but these recordings are technically demanding, and the data are carefully curated. Importantly, the observed effects were statistically significant, indicating that the sample sizes were sufficient. We also note that concerns about statistical power are typically more critical in the case of negative or null results, whereas our findings were positive.

      (6) The discussion is detailed but it is not clear that the activation of JNK2 needs to be achieved by a non-ionotropic action of NMDAR as activation after ionotropic NMDAR activation has been described in the literature. This point needs to be clarified and expanded.

      Sorry that we were unclear on this point. We clarified this on lines 371-372 of the manuscript.

      (7) Adding a cartoon/schematic summarizing the proposed mechanism for tLTD would help the reading of the manuscript.

      We appreciate this suggestion and agree that a schematic would be helpful. However, we prefer to hold off on including one at this stage, as aspects of the underlying mechanism — particularly the role of CB1 receptors in presynaptic pyramidal cells (Sjöström et al., 2003) — are currently under active investigation in a separate project. To avoid potentially misleading oversimplifications, we would prefer to revisit a summary schematic once these uncertainties have been resolved.

      Minor:

      (1) Concentration of compounds is recommended to be included in the figures or in the text. This would make it easy to follow the results.

      We appreciate the suggestion. However, we avoid repeating concentrations to emphasize that conditions are consistent unless otherwise stated. All compound concentrations are clearly listed in the Methods and remain unchanged across experiments. We believe this streamlined approach avoids redundancy while keeping the results clear.

      (2) In some figures, failures in synaptic transmission can be observed (and changes after tLTD). The authors may analyse changes in a number of failures in synaptic transmission after tLTD as an additional indication of a presynaptic expression of this form of tLTD. PPR may also be included in all figures.

      While failures in synaptic transmission are occasionally visible, we chose to focus on CV analysis, which is mathematically equivalent to failure rate analysis, as both rely on the same underlying variability in synaptic responses (Brock et al., 2020). Provided failures are reliably extracted (which requires sufficient signal-to-noise), CV and failure rate analyses should yield consistent conclusions.

      In contrast, PPR analysis is not mathematically equivalent to CV analysis and may offer complementary insights into presynaptic mechanisms. However, the presence of preNMDARs complicates the use of paired-pulse stimulation during baseline: preNMDARs enhance release during high-frequency activity (Abrahamsson et al., 2017; Sjöström et al., 2003; Wong et al., 2024), so repeated stimulation can suppress synaptic responses when preNMDARs are blocked, potentially confounding interpretation. For this reason, we limited PPR analysis to Figures 5 and 6, where conditions were appropriate.

      Admittedly, our manuscript was previously not clear on when we did paired-pulse stimulation and when we did not. We have clarified this in the revised manuscript (lines 548- 551 and lines 569-574).

      (3) Discussion: Line 363-64, hippocampal (SC-CA1 synapses) results exist where postsynaptic MK801 blocks presynaptic tLTD, this may be added here and in the references.

      While we acknowledge that postsynaptic MK-801 has been shown to block presynaptic tLTD at hippocampal SC–CA1 synapses, we note that the hippocampus is part of the archicortex, whereas our study focuses on neocortical circuits, as highlighted in the manuscript title. Given the substantial anatomical and functional differences between these regions, we prefer to keep our discussion focused on the neocortex to maintain conceptual coherence.

      (4) Discussion: While authors indicate "non-ionotropic" they do not discuss whether this action can be named properly "metabotropic" and whether G-proteins may be in fact needed for this action. The authors may briefly discuss this point.

      We previously referred to non-ionotropic NMDAR signaling as “metabotropic,” but reconsidered after discussions with colleagues, including Juan Lerma, who pointed out that the term typically implies G-protein coupling, which has not been definitively shown in this context. While the term “metabotropic” is used inconsistently in the literature (Heuss and Gerber, 2000; Heuss et al., 1999) — sometimes broadly to indicate non-ion flow signaling — we prefer to avoid potential confusion and therefore use “non-ionotropic” unless and until G-protein involvement is clearly demonstrated. We clarified this on lines 423-427 of the Discussion.

      (5) Page 19, line 451 NMDR needs to be corrected to NMDAR.

      Thanks! This was corrected.

      Reviewer 3 (Public review)

      Summary

      In this manuscript, "Neocortical Layer-5 tLTD Relies on Non-Ionotropic Presynaptic NMDA Receptor Signaling", Thomazeau et al. seek to determine the role of presynaptic NMDA receptors and the mechanism by which they mediate expression of frequency-independent timing-dependent long-term depression (tLTD) between layer-5 (L5) pyramidal cells (PCs) in the developing mouse visual cortex. By utilizing sophisticated methods, including sparse Cre-dependent deletion of GluN1 subunit via neonatal iCre-encoding viral injection, in vitro quadruple patch clamp recordings, and pharmacological interventions, the authors elegantly show that L5 PC->PC tLTD is (1) dependent on presynaptic NMDA receptors, (2) mediated by non-ionotropic NMDA receptor signaling, and (3) is reliant on JNK2/Syntaxin-1a (STX1a) interaction (but not RIM1αβ) in the presynaptic neuron. The study elegantly and pointedly addresses a long-standing conundrum regarding the lack of frequency dependence of tLTD.

      We thank the reviewer for calling our methods “sophisticated” and our study “elegant”! We appreciate the kind feedback!

      Strengths

      The authors did a commendable job presenting a very polished piece of work with high-quality data that this Reviewer feels enthusiastic about. The manuscript has several notable strengths. Firstly, the methodological approach used in the study is highly sophisticated and technically challenging and successfully produced high-quality data that were easily accessible to a broader audience. Secondly, the pharmacological interventions used in the study targeted specific players and their mechanistic roles, unveiling the mechanism in question step-by-step. Lastly, the manuscript is written in a well-organized manner that is easy to follow. Overall, the study provides a series of compelling evidence that leads to a clear illustration of mechanistic understanding.

      We are elated that the reviewer described our study with words such as “polished”, “high-quality”, “sophisticated”, and “compelling”!

      Minor comments

      (1) For the broad readership, a brief description of JNK2-mediated signaling cascade underlying tLTD, including its intersection with CB1 receptor signaling may be desired.

      Thank you, this is a great suggestion for improving clarity. We briefly address this point in the revised manuscript (lines 360-363).

      (2) The authors used juvenile mice, P11 to P18 of age. It is a typical age range used for plasticity experiments, but it is also true that this age range spans before and after eye-opening in mice (~P13) and is a few days before the onset of the classical critical period for ocular dominance plasticity in the visual cortex. Given the mechanistic novelty reported in the study, can authors comment on whether this signaling pathway may be age-dependent?

      Thanks, Reviewer 2 also raised this point. In the revised manuscript, we discuss this point (lines 428-432).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Minor typos: page 4 line 101: sensitivity -> sensitive.

      We fixed this typo.

      (2) Page 15 line 333: sensitivity -> sensitive.

      We fixed this typo.

      (3) Minor aesthetic suggestion: On the scale bars for all examples, LTP and LTD data are easily confused with the letter L. I'd suggest flipping them left to right.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We flipped the scale bars in all figures.

      References

      Abrahamsson, T., Chou, C.Y.C., Li, S.Y., Mancino, A., Costa, R.P., Brock, J.A., Nuro, E., Buchanan, K.A., Elgar, D., Blackman, A.V., et al. 2017. Differential Regulation of Evoked and Spontaneous Release by Presynaptic NMDA Receptors. Neuron 96: 839-855 e835

      Bartlett, R.D., Esslinger, C.S., Thompson, C.M., and Bridges, R.J. 1998. Substituted quinolines as inhibitors of L-glutamate transport into synaptic vesicles. Neuropharmacology 37: 839-846

      Berretta, N., and Jones, R.S. 1996. Tonic facilitation of glutamate release by presynaptic N-methyl-D-aspartate autoreceptors in the entorhinal cortex. Neuroscience 75: 339-344.

      Brasier, D.J., and Feldman, D.E. 2008. Synapse-specific expression of functional presynaptic NMDA receptors in rat somatosensory cortex. J Neurosci 28: 2199-2211

      Brock, J.A., Thomazeau, A., Watanabe, A., Li, S.S.Y., and Sjöström, P.J. 2020. A Practical Guide to Using CV Analysis for Determining the Locus of Synaptic Plasticity. Frontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience 12:11 10.3389/fnsyn.2020.00011

      Buchanan, K.A., Blackman, A.V., Moreau, A.W., Elgar, D., Costa, R.P., Lalanne, T., Tudor Jones, A.A., Oyrer, J., and Sjöström, P.J. 2012. Target-Specific Expression of Presynaptic NMDA Receptors in Neocortical Microcircuits. Neuron 75: 451-466

      Chou, C.Y.C., Wong, H.H.W., Guo, C., Boukoulou, K.E., Huang, C., Jannat, J., Klimenko, T., Li, V.Y., Liang, T.A., Wu, V.C., and Sjöström, P.J. 2024. Principles of visual cortex excitatory microcircuit organization. The Innovation 6: 1-11

      Corlew, R., Wang, Y., Ghermazien, H., Erisir, A., and Philpot, B.D. 2007. Developmental switch in the contribution of presynaptic and postsynaptic NMDA receptors to long-term depression. J Neurosci 27: 9835-9845

      Heuss, C., and Gerber, U. 2000. G-protein-independent signaling by G-protein-coupled receptors. Trends in Neurosciences 23: 469-475

      Heuss, C., Scanziani, M., Gähwiler, B.H., and Gerber, U. 1999. G-protein-independent signaling mediated by metabotropic glutamate receptors. Nature Neuroscience 2: 1070-1077

      Huettner, J.E., and Bean, B.P. 1988. Block of N-methyl-D-aspartate-activated current by the anticonvulsant MK-801: selective binding to open channels. PNAS 85: 1307-1311.

      Humeau, Y., Shaban, H., Bissière, S., and Lüthi, A. 2003. Presynaptic induction of heterosynaptic associative plasticity in the mammalian brain. Nature 426: 841-845

      Kemp, J.A., Foster, A.C., Leeson, P.D., Priestley, T., Tridgett, R., Iversen, L.L., and Woodruff, G.N. 1988. 7-Chlorokynurenic acid is a selective antagonist at the glycine modulatory site of the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor complex. PNAS 85: 6547-6550

      Larsen, R.S., Corlew, R.J., Henson, M.A., Roberts, A.C., Mishina, M., Watanabe, M., Lipton, S.A., Nakanishi, N., Perez-Otano, I., Weinberg, R.J., and Philpot, B.D. 2011. NR3A-containing NMDARs promote neurotransmitter release and spike timing-dependent plasticity. Nat Neurosci 14: 338-344

      Nabavi, S., Kessels, H.W., Alfonso, S., Aow, J., Fox, R., and Malinow, R. 2013. Metabotropic NMDA receptor function is required for NMDA receptor-dependent long-term depression. PNAS 110: 4027-4032

      Rodríguez-Moreno, A., Kohl, M.M., Reeve, J.E., Eaton, T.R., Collins, H.A., Anderson, H.L., and Paulsen, O. 2011. Presynaptic induction and expression of timing-dependent long-term depression demonstrated by compartment-specific photorelease of a use-dependent NMDA receptor antagonist. J Neurosci 31: 8564-8569

      Rodríguez-Moreno, A., and Paulsen, O. 2008. Spike timing-dependent long-term depression requires presynaptic NMDA receptors. Nat Neurosci 11: 744-745

      Sjöström, P.J., Turrigiano, G.G., and Nelson, S.B. 2003. Neocortical LTD via coincident activation of presynaptic NMDA and cannabinoid receptors. Neuron 39: 641-654

      Song, S., Sjöström, P.J., Reigl, M., Nelson, S., and Chklovskii, D.B. 2005. Highly nonrandom features of synaptic connectivity in local cortical circuits. PLoS biology 3: e68

      Tovar, K.R., and Westbrook, G.L. 1999. The incorporation of NMDA receptors with a distinct subunit composition at nascent hippocampal synapses in vitro. J Neurosci 19: 4180-4188

      Wong, H.H., Watt, A.J., and Sjöström, P.J. 2024. Synapse-specific burst coding sustained by local axonal translation. Neuron 112: 264-276 e266

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer 1 (Public review):

      (1) “It is likely that metabolism changes ex vivo vs in vivo, and therefore stable isotope tracing experiments in the explants may not reflect in vivo metabolism.”

      We agree with the reviewer that metabolic changes may differ ex vivo versus in vivo. We now state: “Lastly, an important caveat to our study is that metabolism changes ex vivo versus in vivo, and thus, in the future, in vivo studies can be performed to assess metabolic changes.” (lines 591-593).

      (2) “The retina at P0 is composed of both progenitors and differentiated cells. It is not clear if the results of the RNA-seq and metabolic analysis reflect changes in the metabolism of progenitors, or of mature cells, or changes in cell type composition rather than direct metabolic changes in a specific cell type.”

      We have clarified that the metabolic changes may be in RPCs or in other retinal cell types on lines 149-152: “Since these measurements were performed in bulk, and the ratio of RPCs to differentiated cells declines as development proceeds, it is not clear whether glycolytic activity is temporally regulated within RPCs or in other retinal cell types.”

      However, since we mined a single cell (sc) RNA-seq dataset, we are able to attribute gene expression specifically within RPCs (Figure 1).

      (3) “The biochemical links between elevated glycolysis and pH and beta-catenin stability are unclear. White et al found that higher pH decreased beta-catenin stability (JCB 217: 3965) in contrast to the results here. Oginuma et al found that inhibition of glycolysis or beta-catenin acetylation does not affect beta-catenin stability (Nature 584:98), again in contrast to these results. Another paper showed that acidification inhibits Wnt signaling by promoting the expression of a transcriptional repressor and not via beta-catenin stability (Cell Discovery 4:37). There are also additional papers showing increased pH can promote cell proliferation via other mechanisms (e.g. Nat Metab 2:1212). It is possible that there is organ-specificity in these signaling pathways however some clarification of these divergent results is warranted.”

      We have added the information and references brought up by the reviewer in our discussion (lines 529-549 and 570-574). We have also suggested future experiments to further analyse our system in line with the studies now referenced (lines 580-589).

      (4) The gene expression analysis is not completely convincing. E.g. the expression of additional glycolytic genes should be shown in Figure 1. It is not clear why Hk1 and Pgk1 are specifically shown, and conclusions about changes in glycolysis are difficult to draw from the expression of these two genes. The increase in glycolytic gene expression in the Pten-deficient retina is generally small.

      We have expanded the list of glycolytic genes analysed, in modified Figure 1B, and expanded the description of these results on lines 156-166.

      (5) Is it possible that glycolytic inhibition with 2DG slows down the development and production of most newly differentiated cells rather than specifically affecting photoreceptor differentiation?

      We added a comment to this effect to the discussion: “It is possible that glycolytic inhibition with 2DG slows down the development and production of most newly differentiated cells rather than specifically affecting photoreceptor differentiation, which we could assess in the future.“ (lines 600-603).

      (6) “Likewise the result that an increase in pH from 7.4 to 8.0 is sufficient to increase proliferation implies that pH regulation may have instructive roles in setting the tempo of retinal development and embryonic cell proliferation. Similarly, the results show that acetate supplementation increases proliferation (I think this result should be moved to the main figures).”

      We have added the acetate data to main Figure 7E.

      We added a supplemental data table that was inadvertently not included in our last submission. Figure 2– Data supplement 1.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major points

      (1) Assuming that increased glycolysis gets RPCs to exit from the proliferative stage earlier, the total number of retinal cells, notably that of the rod photoreceptors, should be reduced since the pool of proliferating cells is depleted earlier. Is that really the case for a mature retina? To address this question, the authors should perform quantifications of photoreceptors at a stage where most developmental cell death has concluded (i.e. at P14 or later; Young, J. Comp. Neurol. 229:362-373, 1984) and check whether or not there are more or less photoreceptors present.

      We have previously quantified numbers of each cell type in Pten RPC-cKO retinas, and as suggested by the reviewer, there are fewer rod photoreceptors at P7 (Tachibana et al. 2016. J Neurosci 36 (36) 9454-9471) and P21 (Hanna et al. 2025. IOVS. Mar 3;66(3):45). We have edited the following sentence: “Using cellular birthdating, we previously showed that Pten-cKO RPCs are hyperproliferative and differentiate on an accelerated schedule between E12.5 and E18.5, yet fewer rod photoreceptors are ultimately present in P7 (Tachibana et al., 2016) and P21 (Hanna et al., 2025) retinas, suggestive of a developmental defect. (lines 184-187).

      (2) Figure 1B, 1H: On what data are these two figures based? The plots suggest that a high-density time series of gene expression and rod photoreceptor birth was performed, yet it is not clear where and how this was done. The authors should provide the data, plot individual data points, and, if applicable perform a statistical analysis to support their idea that glycolytic gene expression (as a surrogate for glycolysis) overlaps in time with rod photoreceptor birth (Figure 1B) and that in Pten KO the glycolytic gene expression is shifted forward in time (Figure 1H). If the data required to construct these plots (min. 5 data points, min 3 repeats each) does not exist or cannot be generated (e.g. from reanalysis of previously published datasets), then these graphs should be removed.

      We have removed the previous Figure 1B and Figure 1H.

      (3) Figure 2E: Which PKM isozyme was analyzed here? Does the genetic analysis allow us to distinguish between PKM1 and PKM2? Since PKM governs the key rate-limiting step of glycolysis but was not significantly upregulated, does this not contradict the authors' main hypothesis? If PKM at some point was inhibited (see also below comment to Figure 5) one would expect an accumulation of glycolytic intermediates, including phosphoenolpyruvate. Was such an effect observed?

      The data in Figure 2E is bulk RNA-seq data. Since there is only a single Pkm gene that is alternatively spliced, the RNA-sequencing data cannot distinguish between the four PK isozymes that arise from alternative splicing. Specifically, we used Illumina NextSeq 500 for sequencing of 75bp Single-End reads that will sequence transcripts for alternatively spliced Pkm1 and Pkm2 mRNAs, which carry a common 3’end. We added a statement to this effect: “However, since we employed 75 bp single-end sequencing, we could not distinguish between alternatively spliced Pkm1 and Pkm2 mRNAs.“ (lines 215-216).

      We have not performed metabolic analyses of glycolytic intermediates, but we have proposed such a strategy as an important avenue of investigation for future studies in the Discussion: “Lastly, an important caveat to our study is that metabolism changes ex vivo versus in vivo, and thus, in the future, in vivo studies can be performed to assess metabolic changes.” (lines 591-593).

      (4) Figure 3 and materials & methods: For the retinal explant cultures, was the RPE included in the cultured explants? If so, how can the authors distinguish drug effects on neuroretina and RPE? If the RPE was not included, then the authors should discuss how the missing RPE - neuroretina interaction could have influenced their results.

      We remove the RPE from the retinal explants, as indicated in the Methods section. The RPE is a metabolic hub that allows transport of nutrients for the retina, so in the absence of the RPE, there is not an immediate source of energy, such as glucose, to the retina. However, the media (DMEM) contains 25 mM glucose to replace the RPE as an energy source, and we now show that RPCs express GLUT1, which allows uptake of glucose (see new Figure 3A).

      We added the following sentence “P0 explants were mounted on Nucleopore membranes and cultured on top of retinal explant media, providing a source of nutrients, growth factors and glucose. “(lines 241-243).

      (5) Figure 3: It seems rather odd that, if glycolysis was so important for retinal proliferation, differentiation, and metabolism in general, the inhibition of glycolysis with 2DG should not produce a strong degeneration. However, since 2DG competes with glucose, and must be used at nearly equimolar concentration to block glycolysis in a meaningful way, it is possible that the 2DG concentration used simply was not high enough to substantially inhibit glycolysis. Since the inhibitory effect of 2DG depends on the glucose concentration, the authors should measure and provide the concentration of glucose in the explant culture medium. This value should be given either in results or materials and methods.

      We recently published a manuscript showing that 2DG treatments at the same concentrations employed in this study are effective at reducing lactate production in the developing retina in vivo, which is the expected effect of reduced glycolysis (Hanna et al. 2025. IOVS). However, in this study, we did not observe an impact on cell survival.

      We do not agree that it is necessary to measure glucose in the media since the anti-proliferative effect of 2DG is well known, and we are working in the effective range established by multiple groups. We have clarified that we are in the effective range by adding the following sentences: “2DG is typically used in the range of 5-10 mM in cell culture studies and in general, has anti-proliferative effects. To test whether 2DG treatment was in the effective range, explants were exposed to BrdU, which is incorporated into S-phase cells, for 30 minutes prior to harvesting. 2DG treatment resulted in a dose-dependent inhibition of RPC proliferation as evidenced by a reduction in BrdU<sup>+</sup> cells (Figure 3D), indicating that our treatment was in the effective range.” (lines 246-251).

      (6) Figure 3F: The authors use immunostaining for cleaved, activated caspase-3 to assess the amount of apoptotic cell death. However, there are many different possible mechanisms for neuronal cells to die, the majority of which are caspase-independent. To assess the amount of cell death occurring, the authors should perform a TUNEL assay (which labels apoptotic and non-apoptotic forms of cell death; Grasl-Kraupp et al., Hepatology 21:1465-8, 1995), quantify the numbers of TUNEL-positive cells in the retina, and compare this to the numbers of cells positive for activated caspase-3.

      We agree with the reviewer that there are more ways for a cell to die than just apoptosis, and TUNEL would pick up dying cells that may undergo apoptosis or necrosis, for example, our data with cleaved caspase-3, an executioner protease for apoptosis, provides us with clear evidence of cell death in our different conditions. Since this manuscript is not focused on cell death pathways, we have not performed the additional TUNEL assay.

      (7) Figure 4F and 4I: At post-natal day P7 the rod outer segments (OSs) only just start to grow out and the characteristic, rhodopsin-filled disk stacks are not yet formed. To test whether the PFKB3 gain-of function or the Pten KO has a marked effect on OS formation and length, the authors should perform the same tests on older, more mature retina at a time when rod OS show their characteristic disk structures (e.g. somewhere between P14 to P30). The same applies to the 2DG inhibition on the Pten KO retina.

      The precocious differentiation of rod outer segments observed in P7 Pten-cKO retinas does not persist in adulthood, and instead reflects a developmental acceleration. Indeed, we found that in Pten cKO retinas at 3-, 6- and 12-months of age, rod and cone photoreceptors degenerate, and cone outer segments are shorter (Hanna et al., 2025; Tachibana et al., 2016). These data demonstrate that Pten is required to support rod and cone survival.

      (8) Figure 5: Lowering media pH is a rather coarse and untargeted intervention that will have multiple metabolic consequences independent of PKM2. It is thus hardly possible to attribute the effects of pH manipulation to any specific enzyme. To assess this and possibly confirm the results obtained with low pH, the authors should perform a targeted inhibition experiment, for instance using Shikonin (Chen et al., Oncogene 30:4297-306, 2011), to selectively inhibit PKM2. If the retinal explant cultures contained the RPE, an additional question would be how the changes in RPE would alter lactate flux and metabolization between RPE and neuroretina (see also question 4 above).

      We have reframed the rationale for the pH manipulation experiments, highlighting the importance of pH in cell fate specification, and indicating that the aggregation of PKM2 is only one possible effect of lower pH.

      We wrote: “Given that altered glycolysis influences intracellular pH, which in turn controls cell fate decisions, we set out to assess the impact of manipulating pH on cell fate selection in the retina. One of the expected impacts of lowering pH was the aggregation of PKM2, a rate-limiting enzyme for glycolysis, which aggregates in reversible, inactive amyloids (Cereghetti et al., 2024).” (lines 362-366). 

      We have also added a discussion point “Whether pH manipulations also impact the stability of other retinal proteins, such as PKM2, can be further investigated in the future using specific PKM2 inhibitors, such as Shikonin (Chen et al., 2011). (lines 545-547).

      (9) Figure 5G: As for Figure 3F, the authors should perform TUNEL assays to assess the number of cells dying independent of caspase-3.

      Please see response to point 6.

      (10) Figure 7E: In the figure legend "K" should read "E". From the figure and the legend, it is not clear to which cell type this diagram should refer. This must be specified. Importantly, the insulin-dependent glucose-transporter 4 (GLUT4) highlighted in Figure 7E, while expressed on inner retinal vasculature endothelial cells, is not expressed in retinal neurons. What GLUTs exactly are expressed in what retinal neurons may still be to some extent contentious (cf. Chen et al., elife, https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.91141.3 ; and reviewer comments therein), yet RPE cells clearly express GLUT1, photoreceptors likely express GLUT3, Müller glia cells may express GLUT1, while horizontal cells likely express GLUT2 (Yang et al., J Neurochem. 160:283-296, 2022).’

      We have removed this summary schematic for simplicity.

      (11) Materials and methods: The retinal explant culture system must be described in more detail. Important questions concern the use of medium and serum for which the providers, order numbers, and batch/lot numbers (whichever is applicable) must be given. The glucose concentration in the medium (including the serum content) should be measured. A key concern is whether the explants were cultivated submerged into the medium - this would prevent sufficient oxygenation and drive metabolism towards glycolysis (i.e. the Pasteur effect) - or whether they were cultivated on top of the liquid medium, at the interface between air and liquid (i.e. a situation that would favor OXPHOS).

      We have added further detail to the methods section for the explant assay (lines 686-689). We cultured the retinal explants on membranes on top of the media, which is the standard methodology in the field and in our laboratory (Cantrup et al., 2012; Tachibana et al., 2016; Touahri et al., 2024). Typically, RPCs undergo aerobic glycolysis, meaning that even in the presence of oxygen, they still prefer glycolysis rather than OXPHOS. We demonstrated that 2DG blocks RPC proliferation when treated with 2DG, indicating that RPCs are indeed favoring glycolysis in our assay system.

      (12) A point the authors may want to discuss additionally is the potential relevance of their data for the pathogenesis of human diseases, especially early developmental defects such as they occur in oxygen-induced retinopathy of prematurity.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for their valuable comment. Given that retinopathy of prematurity (ROP) is primarily vascular in nature, and we have not investigated vascular defects in this study, we have elected not to add a discussion of ROP to our manuscript.

      Minor points

      (1) Please add a label indicating the ages of the retina to images showing the entire retina (i.e. "P7"; e.g. in Figures 1F, 3, 4D, 5, etc.).

      Figure 1:

      1D: E18.5 indicated at the bottom of the two panels

      1F – P0 is indicated at the bottom of the two panels.

      Figure 3C-H: P0 explant stage and days of culture indicated

      Figure 4D: E12.5 BrdU and P7 harvest date indicated

      Figure 5C-H: P0 explant stage and days of culture indicated

      Figure 7A-E: P0 explant stage and days of culture indicated

      (2) The term Ctnnb1 should be introduced also in the abstract.

      We now state that Ctnnb1 encodes for b-catenin in the abstract.

      (3) Line 249: "...remaining..." should probably read "...remained...".

      Changed (now line 260).

      (4) Line 381: The sentence "...correlating with the propensity of some RPCs to continue to proliferate while others to differentiate.", should probably be rewritten to something like "...correlating with the propensity of some RPCs to continue to proliferate while others differentiate.".

      We have corrected this sentence.

      (5) The structure of the discussion might benefit from the introduction of subheadings.

      We have introduced subheadings.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figure 1H shows the kinetics of rod photoreceptor production as accelerated, but does not represent the fact that fewer rods are ultimately produced, which appears to be the case from the data. If so, the Pten cKO curve should probably be lower than WT to reflect that difference.

      We have removed this graph (as per Reviewer #2, point 2).

      (2) KEGG analysis also showed that the HIF-1 signaling pathway is altered in the Pten cKO retina. What is the significance of that, and is it related to metabolic dysregulation? It has been shown that lactate can promote vessel growth, which initiates at birth in the mouse retina.

      We have added some information on HIF-1 to the Discussion. “The increased glycolytic gene expression in Pten-cKO retinas is likely tied to the increased expression of hypoxia-induced-factor-1-alpha (Hif1a), a known target of mTOR signaling that transcriptionally activates Slc1a3 (GLUT1) and glycolytic genes (Hanna et al., 2022). Indeed, mTOR signaling is hyperactive in Pten-cKO retinas (Cantrup et al., 2012; Tachibana et al., 2016; Tachibana et al., 2018; Touahri et al., 2024), and likewise, in Tsc1-cKO retinas, which also increase glycolysis via HIF-1A (Lim et al., 2021).” (lines 489-494).

      Cantrup, R., Dixit, R., Palmesino, E., Bonfield, S., Shaker, T., Tachibana, N., Zinyk, D., Dalesman, S., Yamakawa, K., Stell, W. K., Wong, R. O., Reese, B. E., Kania, A., Sauve, Y., & Schuurmans, C. (2012). Cell-type specific roles for PTEN in establishing a functional retinal architecture. PLoS One, 7(3), e32795. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0032795

      Cereghetti, G., Kissling, V. M., Koch, L. M., Arm, A., Schmidt, C. C., Thüringer, Y., Zamboni, N., Afanasyev, P., Linsenmeier, M., Eichmann, C., Kroschwald, S., Zhou, J., Cao, Y., Pfizenmaier, D. M., Wiegand, T., Cadalbert, R., Gupta, G., Boehringer, D., Knowles, T. P. J., Mezzenga, R., Arosio, P., Riek, R., & Peter, M. (2024). An evolutionarily conserved mechanism controls reversible amyloids of pyruvate kinase via pH-sensing regions. Dev Cell. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.devcel.2024.04.018

      Chen, J., Xie, J., Jiang, Z., Wang, B., Wang, Y., & Hu, X. (2011). Shikonin and its analogs inhibit cancer cell glycolysis by targeting tumor pyruvate kinase-M2. Oncogene, 30(42), 4297-4306. https://doi.org/10.1038/onc.2011.137

      Hanna, J., Touahri, Y., Pak, A., David, L. A., van Oosten, E., Dixit, R., Vecchio, L. M., Mehta, D. N., Minamisono, R., Aubert, I., & Schuurmans, C. (2025). Pten Loss Triggers Progressive Photoreceptor Degeneration in an mTORC1-Independent Manner. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci, 66(3), 45. https://doi.org/10.1167/iovs.66.3.45

      Tachibana, N., Cantrup, R., Dixit, R., Touahri, Y., Kaushik, G., Zinyk, D., Daftarian, N., Biernaskie, J., McFarlane, S., & Schuurmans, C. (2016). Pten Regulates Retinal Amacrine Cell Number by Modulating Akt, Tgfbeta, and Erk Signaling. J Neurosci, 36(36), 9454-9471. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0936-16.2016

      Touahri, Y., Hanna, J., Tachibana, N., Okawa, S., Liu, H., David, L. A., Olender, T., Vasan, L., Pak, A., Mehta, D. N., Chinchalongporn, V., Balakrishnan, A., Cantrup, R., Dixit, R., Mattar, P., Saleh, F., Ilnytskyy, Y., Murshed, M., Mains, P. E., Kovalchuk, I., Lefebvre, J. L., Leong, H. S., Cayouette, M., Wang, C., Sol, A. D., Brand, M., Reese, B. E., & Schuurmans, C. (2024). Pten regulates endocytic trafficking of cell adhesion and Wnt signaling molecules to pattern the retina. Cell Rep, 43(4), 114005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2024.114005

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper describes the cryoEM structure of RAD51 filament on the recombination intermediate. In the RAD51 filament, the insertion of a DNA-binding loop called the L2 loop stabilizes the separation of the complementary strand for the base-pairing with an incoming ssDNA and the non-complementary strand, which is captured by the second DNA-binding channel called the site II. The molecular structure of the RAD51 filament with a recombination intermediate provides a new insight into the mechanism of homology search and strand exchange between ssDNA and dsDNA.

      Strengths:

      This is the first human RAD51 filament structure with a recombination intermediate called the D-loop. The work has been done with great care, and the results shown in the paper are compelling based on cryo-EM and biochemical analyses. The paper is really nice and important for researchers in the field of homologous recombination, which gives a new view on the molecular mechanism of RAD51-mediated homology search and strand exchange.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors need more careful text writing. Without page and line numbers, it is hard to give comments.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for their kind words of appreciation of our work.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Homologous recombination (HR) is a critical pathway for repairing double-strand DNA breaks and ensuring genomic stability. At the core of HR is the RAD51-mediated strand-exchange process, in which the RAD51-ssDNA filament binds to homologous double-stranded DNA (dsDNA) to form a characteristic D-loop structure. While decades of biochemical, genetic, and single-molecule studies have elucidated many aspects of this mechanism, the atomic-level details of the strand-exchange process remained unresolved due to a lack of atomic-resolution structure of RAD51 D-loop complex.

      In this study, the authors achieved this by reconstituting a RAD51 mini-filament, allowing them to solve the RAD51 D-loop complex at 2.64 Å resolution using a single particle approach. The atomic resolution structure reveals how specific residues of RAD51 facilitate the strand exchange reaction. Ultimately, this work provides unprecedented structural insight into the eukaryotic HR process and deepens the understanding of RAD51 function at the atomic level, advancing the broader knowledge of DNA repair mechanisms.

      Strengths:

      The authors overcame the challenge of RAD51's helical symmetry by designing a minifilament system suitable for single-particle cryo-EM, enabling them to resolve the RAD51 D-loop structure at 2.64 Å without imposed symmetry. This high resolution revealed precise roles of key residues, including F279 in Loop 2, which facilitates strand separation, and basic residues on site II that capture the displaced strand. Their findings were supported by mutagenesis, strand exchange assays, and single-molecule analysis, providing strong validation of the structural insights.

      Weaknesses:

      Despite the detailed structural data, some structure-based mutagenesis data interpretation lacks clarity. Additionally, the proposed 3′-to-5′ polarity of strand exchange relies on assumptions from static structural features, such as stronger binding of the 5′-arm-which are not directly supported by other experiments. This makes the directional model compelling but contradicts several well-established biochemical studies that support a 5'-to-3' polarity relative to the complementary strand (e.g., Cell 1995, PMID: 7634335; JBC 1996, PMID: 8910403; Nature 2008, PMID: 18256600).

      Overall:

      The 2.6 Å resolution cryoEM structure of the RAD51 D-loop complex provides remarkably detailed insights into the residues involved in D-loop formation. The high-quality cryoEM density enables precise placement of each nucleotide, which is essential for interpreting the molecular interactions between RAD51 and DNA. Particularly, the structural analysis highlights specific roles for key domains, such as the N-terminal domain (NTD), in engaging the donor DNA duplex.

      This structural interpretation is further substantiated by single-molecule fluorescence experiments using the KK39,40AA NTD mutant. The data clearly show a significant reduction in D-loop formation by the mutant compared to wild-type, supporting the proposed functional role of the NTD observed in the cryoEM model.

      However, the strand exchange activity interpretation presented in Figure 5B could benefit from a more rigorous experimental design. The current assay measures an increase in fluorescence intensity, which depends heavily on the formation of RAD51-ssDNA filaments. As shown in Figure S6A, several mutants exhibit reduced ability to form such filaments, which could confound the interpretation of strand exchange efficiency. To address this, the assay should either: (1) normalize for equivalent levels of RAD51-ssDNA filaments across samples, or (2) compare the initial rates of fluorescence increase (i.e., the slope of the reaction curve), rather than endpoint fluorescence, to better isolate the strand exchange activity itself.

      Based on the structural features of the D-loop, the authors propose that strand pairing and exchange initiate at the 3'-end of the complementary strand in the donor DNA and proceed with a 3'-to-5' polarity. This conclusion, drawn from static structural observations, contrasts with several well-established biochemical studies that support a 5'-to-3' polarity relative to the complementary strand (e.g., Cell 1995, PMID: 7634335; JBC 1996, PMID: 8910403; Nature 2008, PMID: 18256600). While the structural model is compelling and methodologically robust, this discrepancy underscores the need for further experiments.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for highlighting the importance of our findings to our understanding of the mechanism of homologous recombination.

      We agree with the reviewer that the reduced filament-forming ability of some of the RAD51 mutants complicates a straightforward interpretation of their strand-exchange assay. Interestingly, the RAD51 mutants that appear most impaired are the esDNA-capture mutants that do not contact the ssDNA in the structure of the pre-synaptic filament. However, the RAD51 NTD mutants, that display the most severe defect in strand-exchange, have a near-WT filament forming ability.

      The reviewer correctly points out that the polarity of strand exchange by RecA and RAD51 is an extensively researched topic that has been characterised in several authoritative studies. In our paper, we simply describe the mechanistic insights obtained from the structural D-loop models of RAD51 (our work) and RecA (Yang et al, PMID: 33057191).The structures illustrate a very similar mechanism of D-loop formation that proceeds with opposite polarity of strand exchange for RAD51 and RecA. Comparison of the D-loop structures for RecA and RAD51 provides an attractive explanation for the opposite polarity, as caused by the different positions of their dsDNA-binding domains in the filament structure. We agree with the reviewer that further investigation will be needed for an adequate rationalisation of the available evidence. We will mention the relevant literature in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Built on their previous pioneer expertise in studying RAD51 biology, in this paper, the authors aim to capture and investigate the structural mechanism of human RAD51 filament bound with a displacement loop (D-loop), which occurs during the dynamic synaptic state of the homologous recombination (HR) strand-exchange step. As the structures of both pre- and post-synaptic RAD51 filaments were previously determined, a complex structure of RAD51 filaments during strand exchange is one of the key missing pieces of information for a complete understanding of how RAD51 functions in the HR pathway. This paper aims to determine the high-resolution cryo-EM structure of RAD51 filament bound with the D-loop. Combined with mutagenesis analysis and biophysical assays, the authors aim to investigate the D-loop DNA structure, RAD51-mediated strand separation and polarity, and a working model of RAD51 during HR strand invasion in comparison with RecA.

      Strengths:

      (1) The structural work and associated biophysical assays in this paper are solid, elegantly designed, and interpreted.  These results provide novel insights into RAD51's function in HR.

      (2) The DNA substrate used was well designed, taking into consideration the nucleotide number requirement of RAD51 for stable capture of donor DNA. This DNA substrate choice lays the foundation for successfully determining the structure of the RAD51 filament on D-loop DNA using single-particle cryo-EM.

      (3) The authors utilised their previous expertise in capping DNA ends using monomeric streptavidin and combined their careful data collection and processing to determine the cryo-EM structure of full-length human RAD51 bound at the D-loop in high resolution. This interesting structure forms the core part of this work and allows detailed mapping of DNA-DNA and DNA-protein interaction among RAD51, invading strands, and donor DNA arms (Figures 1, 2, 3, 4). The geometric analysis of D-loop DNA bound with RAD51 and EM density for homologous DNA pairing is also impressive (Figure S5). The previously disordered RAD51's L2-loop is now ordered and traceable in the density map and functions as a physical spacer when bound with D-loop DNA. Interestingly, the authors identified that the side chain position of F279 in the L2_loop of RAD51_H differs from other F279 residues in L2-loops of E, F, and G protomers. This asymmetric binding of L2 loops and RAD51_NTD binding with donor DNA arms forms the basis of the proposed working model about the polarity of csDNA during RAD51-mediated strand exchange.

      (4) This work also includes mutagenesis analysis and biophysical experiments, especially EMSA, single-molecule fluorescence imaging using an optical tweezer, and DNA strand exchange assay, which are all suitable methods to study the key residues of RAD51 for strand exchange and D-loop formation (Figure 5).

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The proposed model for the 3'-5' polarity of RAD51-mediated strand invasion is based on the structural observations in the cryo-EM structure. This study lacks follow-up biochemical/biophysical experiments to validate the proposed model compared to RecA or developing methods to capture structures of any intermediate states with different polarity models.

      (2) The functional impact of key mutants designed based on structure has not been tested in cells to evaluate how these mutants impact the HR pathway.

      The significance of the work for the DNA repair field and beyond:

      Homologous recombination (HR) is a key pathway for repairing DNA double-strand breaks and involves multiple steps. RAD51 forms nucleoprotein filaments first with 3' overhang single-strand DNA (ssDNA), followed by a search and exchange with a homologous strand. This function serves as the basis of an accurate template-based DNA repair during HR. This research addressed a long-standing challenge of capturing RAD51 bound with the dynamic synaptic DNA and provided the first structural insight into how RAD51 performs this function. The significance of this work extends beyond the discovery of biology for the DNA repair field, into its medical relevance. RAD51 is a potential drug target for inhibiting DNA repair in cancer cells to overcome drug resistance. This work offers a structural understanding of RAD51's function with the D-loop and provides new strategies for targeting RAD51 to improve cancer therapies.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive comments on the significance of our work. Concerning the proposed polarity of strand exchange based on our structural finding, please see our reply to the previous reviewer; we agree with the reviewer that further experimentation will be needed to reach a settled view on this.

      Testing the functional effects of the RAD51 mutants on HR in cells was not an aim of the current work but we agree that it would be a very interesting experiment, which would likely provide further important insights into the mechanism of strand exchange at the core of the HR reaction.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer 1:

      (1) The initial high accumulation by all cells followed by the emergence of a sub-population that has reduced its intracellular levels of tachyplesin is a key observation and I agree with the authors' conclusion that this suggests an induced response to the AMP is important in facilitating the bimodal distribution. However, I think the conclusion that upregulated efflux is driving the reduction in signal in the "low accumulator" subpopulation is not fully supported. Steady-state amounts of intracellular fluorescent AMP are determined by the relative rates of influx and efflux and a decrease could be caused by decreasing influx (while efflux remained unchanged), increasing efflux (while influx remained unchanged), or both decreasing influx and increasing efflux. Given the transcriptomic data suggest possible changes in the expression of enzymes that could affect outer membrane permeability and outer membrane vesicle formation as well as efflux, it seems very possible that changes to both influx and efflux are important. The "efflux inhibitors" shown to block the formation of the low accumulator subpopulation have highly pleiotropic or incompletely characterised mechanisms of action so they also do not exclusively support a hypothesis of increased efflux.

      We agree with the reviewer that the emergence of low accumulators after 30 min in the presence of extracellular tachyplesin-NBD (Figure 4A) could be due to either decreased influx while efflux remained unchanged, increased efflux while influx remained unchanged, or both decreasing influx and increasing efflux. Increased proteolytic activity or increased secretion of OMVs could also play a role.

      We have now acknowledged that “Reduced intracellular accumulation of tachyplesin-NBD in the presence of extracellular tachyplesin-NBD could be due to decreased drug influx, increased drug efflux, increased proteolytic activity or increased secretion of OMVs.” (lines 313-315).

      However, the emergence of low accumulators after 60 min in the absence of extracellular tachyplesin-NBD in our efflux assays (Figure 4C) cannot be due to decreased influx while efflux remained unchanged because of the absence of extracellular tachyplesin-NBD. We acknowledge that in our original manuscript we did not explicitly state that the efflux assays reported in Figure 4C-D were performed in the absence of tachyplesin-NBD in the extracellular environment. We have now clarified this point in our manuscript, we have added illustrations in Figure 4A, 4C-D and we have also carried out efflux assays using ethidium bromide (EtBr) to further support our conclusions about the primary role played by efflux in reducing tachyplesin accumulation in low accumulators. We have added the following paragraphs to our revised manuscript:

      “Next, we performed efflux assays using ethidium bromide (EtBr) by adapting a previously described protocol [62]. Briefly, we preloaded stationary phase E. coli with EtBr by incubating cells at a concentration of 254 µM EtBr in M9 medium for 90 min. Cells were then pelleted and resuspended in M9 to remove extracellular EtBr. Single-cell EtBr fluorescence was measured at regular time points in the absence of extracellular EtBr using flow cytometry. This analysis revealed a progressive homogeneous decrease of EtBr fluorescence due to efflux from all cells within the stationary phase E. coli population (Figure S13A). In contrast, when we performed efflux assays by preloading cells with tachyplesin-NBD (46 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> or 18.2 μM), followed by pelleting and resuspension in M9 to remove extracellular tachyplesin-NBD, we observed a heterogeneous decrease in tachyplesin-NBD fluorescence in the absence of extracellular tachyplesin-NBD: a subpopulation retained high tachyplesin-NBD fluorescence, i.e. high accumulators; whereas another subpopulation displayed decreased tachyplesin-NBD fluorescence, 60 min after the removal of extracellular tachyplesin-NBD (Figure 4B). Since these assays were performed in the absence of extracellular tachyplesin-NBD, decreased tachyplesin-NBD fluorescence could not be ascribed to decreased drug influx or increased secretion of OMVs in low accumulators, but could be due to either enhanced efflux or proteolytic activity in low accumulators.

      Next, we repeated efflux assays using EtBr in the presence of 46 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> (or 20.3 µM) extracellular tachyplesin-1. We observed a heterogeneous decrease of EtBr fluorescence with a subpopulation retaining high EtBr fluorescence (i.e. high tachyplesin accumulators) and another population displaying reduced EtBr fluorescence (i.e. low tachyplesin accumulators, Figure S14B) when extracellular tachyplesin-1 was present. Moreover, we repeated tachyplesin-NBD efflux assays in the presence of M9 containing 50 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> (244 μM) carbonyl cyanide m-chlorophenyl hydrazone (CCCP), an ionophore that disrupts the proton motive force (PMF) and is commonly employed to abolish efflux and found that all cells retained tachyplesin-NBD fluorescence (Figure S15B). However, it is important to note that CCCP does not only abolish efflux but also other respiration-associated and energy-driven processes [63].

      Taken together, our data demonstrate that in the absence of extracellular tachyplesin, stationary phase E. coli homogeneously efflux EtBr, whereas only low accumulators are capable of performing efflux of intracellular tachyplesin after initial tachyplesin accumulation. In the presence of extracellular tachyplesin, only low accumulators can perform efflux of both intracellular tachyplesin and intracellular EtBr. However, it is also conceivable that besides enhanced efflux, low accumulators employ proteolytic activity, OMV secretion, and variations to their bacterial membrane to hinder further uptake and intracellular accumulation of tachyplesin in the presence of extracellular tachyplesin.”

      These amendments can be found on lines 316-350 and in the new Figure S13 and Figure 4. We have also carried out more tachyplesin-NBD accumulation assays using single and double gene-deletion mutants lacking efflux components, please see Response 3 to reviewer 2 and the data reported in Figure 4B.

      (2) A conclusion of the transcriptomic analysis is that the lower accumulating subpopulation was exhibiting "a less translationally and metabolically active state" based on less upregulation of a cluster of genes including those involved in transcription and translation. This conclusion seems to borrow from well-described relationships referred to as bacterial growth laws in which the expression of genes involved in ribosome production and translation is directly related to the bacterial growth (and metabolic) rate. However, the assumptions that allow the formulation of the bacterial growth laws (balanced, steady state, exponential growth) do not hold in growth arrest. A non-growing cell could express no genes at all or could express ribosomal genes at a very low level, or efflux pumps at a high level. The distribution of transcripts among the functional classes of genes does not reveal anything about metabolic rates within the context of growth arrest - it only allows insight into metabolic rates when the constraint of exponential growth can be assumed. Efflux pumps can be highly metabolically costly; for example, Tn-Seq experiments have repeatedly shown that mutants for efflux pump gene transcriptional repressors have strong fitness disadvantages in energy-limited conditions. There are no data presented here to disprove a hypothesis that the low accumulators have high metabolic rates but allocate all of their metabolic resources to fortifying their outer membranes and upregulating efflux. This could be an important distinction for understanding the vulnerabilities of this subpopulation. Metabolic rates can be more directly estimated for single cells using respiratory dyes or pulsed metabolic labelling, for example, and these data could allow deeper insight into the metabolic rates of the two subpopulations. My main recommendation for additional experiments to strengthen the conclusions of the paper would be to attempt to directly measure metabolic or translational activity in the high- and low-accumulating populations. I do not think that the transcriptomic data are sufficient to draw conclusions about this but it would be interesting to directly measure activity. Otherwise, it might be reasonable to simply soften the language describing the two populations as having different activity levels. They do seem to have different transcriptional profiles, and this is already an interesting observation.

      We agree with the reviewer that it might be misleading to draw conclusions on bacterial metabolic states solely based on transcriptomic data. We have therefore removed the statement “low accumulators displayed a less translationally and metabolically active state”. We have instead stated the following: “Our transcriptomics analysis showed that low tachyplesin accumulators downregulated protein synthesis, energy production, and gene expression processes compared to high accumulators”. Moreover, we have employed the membrane-permeable redox-sensitive dye C<sub>12</sub>-resazurin, which is reduced to the fluorescent C<sub>12</sub>-resorufin in metabolically active cells, to obtain a more direct estimate of the metabolic state of low and high accumulators of tachyplesin. We have added the following paragraph reporting our new data:

      “Our transcriptomics analysis also showed that low tachyplesin accumulators downregulated protein synthesis, energy production, and gene expression compared to high accumulators. To gain further insight on the metabolic state of low tachyplesin accumulators, we employed the membrane-permeable redox-sensitive dye, resazurin, which is reduced to the highly fluorescent resorufin in metabolically active cells. We first treated stationary phase E. coli with 46 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> (18.2 μM) tachyplesin-NBD for 60 min, then washed the cells, and then incubated them in 1 μM resazurin for 15 min and measured single-cell fluorescence of resorufin and tachyplesin-NBD simultaneously via flow cytometry. We found that low tachyplesin-NBD accumulators also displayed low fluorescence of resorufin, whereas high tachyplesin-NBD accumulators also displayed high fluorescence of resorufin (Figure S16), suggesting lower metabolic activity in low tachyplesin-NBD accumulators.”

      These amendments can be found on lines 398-408 and in Figure S16.

      (3) The observation that adding nutrients to the stationary phase cultures pushes most of the cells to the "high accumulator" state is presented as support of the hypothesis that the high accumulator state is a higher metabolism/higher translational activity state. However, it is important to note that adding nutrients will cause most or all of the cells in the population to start to grow, thus re-entering the familiar regime in which bacterial growth laws apply. This is evident in the slightly larger cell sizes seen in the nutrient-amended condition. In contrast to stationary phase cells, growing cells largely do not exhibit the bimodal distribution, and they are much more sensitive to tachyplesin, as demonstrated clearly in the supplement. Growing cells are not necessarily the same as the high-accumulating subpopulation of non-growing cells.

      Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we are no longer using the nutrient supplementation data to support the hypothesis that high accumulators possess higher metabolism or translational activity.

      The nutrient supplementation data is now only used to investigate whether tachyplesin-NBD accumulation and efficacy can be increased, and not to show that high tachyplesin-NBD accumulators are more metabolically or translationally active.

      Furthermore, our previous statement “Our data suggests that such slower-growing subpopulations might display lower antibiotic accumulation and thus enhanced survival to antibiotic treatment.” has now been removed from the discussion.

      (4) It might also be worth adding some additional context around the potential to employ efflux inhibitors as therapeutics. It is very clear that obtaining sufficient antimicrobial drug accumulation within Gram-negative bacteria is a substantial barrier to effective treatments, and large concerted efforts to find and develop therapeutic efflux pump inhibitors have been undertaken repeatedly over the last 25 years. Sufficiently selective inhibitors of bacterial efflux pumps with appropriate drug-like properties have been challenging to find and none have entered clinical trials. Multiple psychoactive drugs have been shown to impact efflux in bacteria but usually using concentrations in the 10-100 uM range (as here). Meanwhile, the Ki values for their human targets are usually in the sub- to low-nanomolar range. The authors rightly note that the concentration of sertraline they have used is higher than that achieved in patients, but this is by many orders of magnitude, and it might be worth expanding a bit on the substantial challenge of finding efflux inhibitors that would be specific and non-toxic enough to be used therapeutically. Many advances in structural biology, molecular dynamics, and medicinal chemistry may make the quest for therapeutic efflux inhibitors more fruitful than it has been in the past but it is likely to remain a substantial challenge.

      We agree with this comment and we have now added the following statement:

      “This limitation underscores the broader challenge of identifying EPIs that are both effective and minimally toxic within clinically achievable concentrations, while also meeting key therapeutic criteria such as broad-spectrum efficacy against diverse efflux pumps, high specificity for bacterial targets, and non-inducers of AMR [117]. However, advances in biochemical, computational, and structural methodologies hold the potential to guide rational drug design, making the search for effective EPIs more promising [118]. Therefore, more investigation should be carried out to further optimise the use of sertraline or other EPIs in combination with tachyplesin and other AMPs.”

      This amendment can be found on lines 535-542.

      (5) My second recommendation is that the transcriptomic data should be made available in full and in a format that is easier for other researchers to explore. The raw data should also be uploaded to a sequence repository, such as the NCBI Geo database or the EMBL ENA. The most useful format for sharing transcriptomic data is a table (such as an excel spreadsheet) of transcripts per million counts for each gene for each sample. This allows other researchers to do their own analyses and compare expression levels to observations from other datasets. When only fold change data are supplied, data cannot be compared to other datasets at all, because they are relative to levels in an untreated control which are not known. The cluster analysis is one way of gaining insight into biological function revealed by transcriptional profile, but it can hide interesting additional complexities. For example, rpoS is named as one of the transcription-associated genes that are higher in the high accumulator subpopulation and evidence of generally increased activity. But RpoS is the stress sigma factor that drives much lower levels of expression generally than the housekeeping sigma factor RpoD, even though it recognises many of the same promoters (and some additional stress-specific promoters). Therefore, increased RpoS occupancy of RNAP would be expected to result in overall lower levels of transcription. However, it is also true that the transcript level for the rpoS gene is a particularly poor indicator of expression - rpoS is largely post-transcriptionally regulated. More generally, annotations are always evolving and key functional insights related to each gene might change in the future, so the results are a more durable resource if they are presented in a less analysed form as well as showing the analysis steps. It can also be important to know which genes were robustly expressed but did not change, versus genes that were not detected.

      Sequencing data associated with this study have now been uploaded and linked under NCBI BioProject accession number PRJNA1096674 (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/bioproject/PRJNA1096674).

      We have added this link to the methods under subheading “Accession Numbers” on lines 858-860. Additionally, transcripts per million counts for each gene for each sample have been added to the Figure 3 - Source Data file as requested by the reviewer.

      (6) In the introduction, the susceptibility of AMP efficacy to resistance mechanisms is discussed:

      "However, compared to small molecule antimicrobials, AMP resistance genes typically confer smaller increases in resistance, with polymyxin-B being a notable exception 7, 8. Moreover, mobile resistance genes against AMPs are relatively rare, and horizontal acquisition of AMP resistance is hindered by phylogenetic barriers owing to functional incompatibility with the new host bacteria9, again with plasmid-transmitted polymyxin resistance being a notable exception."

      It seems worth pointing out that polymixins are the only AMPs that can reasonably be compared with small molecule antibiotics in terms of resistance acquisition since they are the only AMPs that have been widely used as drugs and therefore had similar chances to select for resistance among diverse global microbial populations.

      We have now clarified that we are referring to laboratory evolutionary analyses of resistance towards small molecule antibiotics and AMPs (Spohn et al., 2019) and that polymyxins are the only AMPs that have been used in antibiotic treatment to date.

      We have added the following statement to address this point:

      “Bacteria have developed genetic resistance to AMPs, including proteolysis by proteases, modifications in membrane charge and fluidity to reduce affinity, and extrusion by AMP transporters. However, compared to small molecule antimicrobials, AMP resistance genes typically confer smaller increases in resistance in experimental evolution analyses, with polymyxin-B and CAP18 being notable exceptions [8]. Moreover, mobile resistance genes against AMPs are relatively rare and horizontal acquisition of AMP resistance is hindered by phylogenetic barriers owing to functional incompatibility with the new host bacteria [9]. Plasmid-transmitted polymyxin resistance constitutes a notable exception [10], possibly because polymyxins are the only AMPs that have been in clinical use to date [9].”

      This amendment can be found on lines 57-65.

      (7) In the description of Figure 4, " tachyplesin monotherapy" is mentioned. It is not really appropriate to describe the treatment of a planktonic culture of bacteria in a test tube as a therapy since there is no host that is benefitting.

      We have now replaced “tachyplesin monotherapy” with “tachyplesin treatment”.

      (8) In the discussion, it is stated that " tachyplesin accumulates intracellularly only in bacteria that do not survive tachyplesin exposure" but this is clearly not true. All bacteria accumulate tachyplesin intracellularly initially, but if the bacteria are non-growing during the exposure, some of them are able to reduce their intracellular levels. The fraction of survivors is roughly correlated with the fraction of bacteria that do not maintain high intracellular levels of tachyplesin and that do not stain with propidium iodide, but for any given cell it seems that there is no clear point at which a high intracellular level of tachyplesin means that it will definitely not survive.

      We have now clarified this statement as follows: “We show that after an initial homogeneous tachyplesin accumulation within a stationary phase E. coli population, tachyplesin is retained intracellularly by bacteria that do not survive tachyplesin exposure, whereas tachyplesin is retained only in the membrane of bacteria that survive tachyplesin exposure.”

      This amendment can be found on lines 443-446.

      (9) Also in the discussion: " Our data suggests that such slower-growing subpopulations might display lower antibiotic accumulation and thus enchanced [sic] survival to antibiotic treatment." This does not really relate to the results here because the bimodal distributions were primarily studied in the absence of growth. In the LB/exponential growth situations where the population was growing but a very small subpopulation of low accumulators was observed, no measurements were made to indicate subpopulation growth rates.

      We have now removed this statement from the manuscript.

      (10) In discussion, L-Ara4N appears to be referred to as both positively charged and negatively charged; this should be clarified.

      We have now clarified that L-Ara4N is positively charged.

      This amendment can be found on line 496.

      (11) Discussion of TF analysis seems to overstate what is supported by the evidence. The correlation of up- and downregulated genes with previously described TF regulons (probably measured in very different conditions) does not really demonstrate TF activity. This could be measured directly with additional experiments but in the absence of those experiments claims about detecting TF activity should probably be avoided. The attempts to directly demonstrate the importance of those transcription factors to the observed accumulation activity were not successful.

      We have now removed from the discussion the previous paragraph related to the TF analysis. We have also modified the results section reported the TF analysis as follows: “Next, we sought to infer transcription factor (TF) activities via differential expression of their known regulatory targets [61]. A total of 126 TFs were inferred to exhibit differential activity between low and high accumulators (Data Set S4). Among the top ten TFs displaying higher inferred activity in low accumulators compared to high accumulators, four regulate transport systems, i.e. Nac, EvgA, Cra, and NtrC (Figure S12). However, further experiments should be carried out to directly measure the activity of these TFs.”

      Finally, we have also moved the TFs’ data from Figure 3 to Figure S12 in the Supplementary information.

      These amendments can be found on lines 288-293.

      (12) When discussing the possibility of nutrient supplementation versus efflux inhibition as a potential therapeutic strategy, it could be noted that nutrient supplementation cannot be done in many infection contexts. The host immune system and host/bacterial cell density control nutrient access.

      We have now added the following statement: “Moreover, nutrient supplementation as a therapeutic strategy may not be viable in many infection contexts, as host density and the immune system often regulate access to nutrients [3]”.

      These amendments can be found on lines 553-555.

      Reviewer 2:

      (1) Some questions regarding the mechanism remain. One shortcoming of the setup of the transcriptomics experiment is that the tachyplesin-NBD probe itself has antibiotic efficacy and induces phenotypes (and eventually cell death) in the ´high accumulator´cells. This makes it challenging to interpret whether any differences seen between the two groups are causative for the observed accumulation pattern or if they are a consequence of differential accumulation and downstream phenotypic effects.

      We agree with the reviewer and we have now acknowledged that “tachyplesin-NBD has antibiotic efficacy (see Figure 2) and has an impact on the E. coli transcriptome (Figure 3). Therefore, we cannot conclude whether the transcriptomic differences reported between low and high accumulators of tachyplesin-NBD are causative for the distinct accumulation patterns or if they are a consequence of differential accumulation and downstream phenotypic effects.”

      These amendments can be found on lines 283-287.

      (2) It would be relevant to test and report the MIC of sertraline for the strain tested, particularly since in Figure 4G an initial reduction in CFUs is observed for sertraline treatment, which suggests the existence of biological effects in addition to efflux inhibition.

      We have now measured the MIC of sertraline against E. coli BW25113 finding the MIC value to be 128 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> (418 µM). This value is more than four times higher compared to the sertraline concentration employed in our study, i.e. 30 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> (98 μM).

      These amendments can be found on lines 389-391 and data has been added to Figure 4 – Source Data.

      (3) The role of efflux systems is further supported by the finding that efflux pump inhibitors sensitize E. coli to tachyplesin and prevent the occurrence of the tolerant ´low accumulator´ subpopulations. In principle, this is a great way of validating the role of efflux pumps, but the limited selectivity of these inhibitors (CCCP is an uncoupling agent, and for sertraline direct antimicrobial effects on E. coli have been reported by Bohnert et al.) leaves some ambiguity as to whether the synergistic effect is truly mediated via efflux pump inhibition. To strengthen the mechanistic angle of the work analysis of tachyplesin-NBD accumulation in mutants of the identified efflux components would be interesting.

      We have now performed tachyplesin-NBD accumulation assays using 28 single and 4 double E. coli BW25113 gene-deletion mutants of efflux components and transcription factors regulating efflux. While for the majority of the mutants we recorded bimodal distributions of tachyplesin-NBD accumulation similar to the distribution recorded for the E. coli BW25113 parental strain (Figure 4B and Figure S13), we found unimodal distributions of tachyplesin-NBD accumulation constituted only of high accumulators for both DqseB and DqseBDqseC mutants as well as reduced numbers of low accumulators for the DacrADtolC mutant (Figure 4B). Considering that the AcrAB-TolC tripartite RND efflux system is known to confer genetic resistance against AMPs like protamine and polymyxin-B [29,30] and that the quorum sensing regulators qseBC might control the expression of acrA [64] , these data further corroborate the hypothesis that low accumulators can efflux tachyplesin and survive treatment with this AMP.

      These amendments can be found on lines 351-361, in the new Figure 4B and in the new Figure S14.

      Moreover, we have also carried out further efflux assays with both ethidium bromide and tachyplesin-NBD to further demonstrate the role of efflux in reduced accumulation of tachyplesin as well as acknowledging that other mechanisms (i.e reduced influx, increased protease activity or increased secretion of OMVs) could play an important role, please see Response 1 to Reviewer 1.

      (4) The authors imply that protease could contribute to the low accumulator mechanism. Proteases could certainly cleave and thus inactivate AMPs/tachyplesin, but would this effect really lead to a reduction in fluorescence levels since the fluorophore itself would not be affected by proteolytic cleavage?

      We agree with the reviewer that nitrobenzoxadiazole (NBD) might not be cleaved by proteases that inactivate tachyplesin and other AMPs. Therefore, inactivation of tachyplesin by proteases might not affect cellular fluorescence levels unless efflux of NBD is possible following the cleavage of tachyplesin-NBD. We have therefore removed the statement “Conversely, should efflux or proteolytic activities by proteases underpin the functioning of low accumulators, we should observe high initial tachyplesin-NBD fluorescence in the intracellular space of low accumulators followed by a decrease in fluorescence due to efflux or proteolytic degradation.” We have now stated the following: “Low accumulators displayed an upregulation of peptidases and proteases compared to high accumulators, suggesting a potential mechanism for degrading tachyplesin (Table S1 and Data Set S3).”

      These amendments can be found on lines 280-282.

      (5) To facilitate comparison with other literature (e.g. papers on sertraline) it would be helpful to state compound concentrations also as molar concentrations.

      We have now added the molar concentrations alongside all instances where concentrations are stated in μg mL<sup>-1</sup>.

      (6) The authors tested a series of efflux pump inhibitors and found that CCCP and sertraline prevented the generation of the low accumulator subpopulation, whereas other inhibitors did not. An overview and discussion of the known molecular targets and mode of action of the different selected inhibitors could reveal additional insights into the molecular mechanism underlying the synergy with tachyplesin.

      We have now added molecular targets and mode of action of the different inhibitors where known. “Moreover, we repeated tachyplesin-NBD efflux assays in the presence of M9 containing 50 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> (244 μM) carbonyl cyanide m-chlorophenyl hydrazone (CCCP), an ionophore that disrupts the proton motive force (PMF) and is commonly employed to abolish efflux and found that all cells retained tachyplesin-NBD fluorescence (Figure S15B). However, it is important to note that CCCP does not only abolish efflux but also other respiration-associated and energy-driven processes [63].” And “Interestingly, M9 containing 30 µg mL<sup>-1</sup> (98 μM) sertraline (Figure 4D and S15C), an antidepressant which inhibits efflux activity of RND pumps, potentially through direct binding to efflux pumps [65] and decreasing the PMF [66], or 50 µg mL<sup>-1</sup> (110 μM) verapamil (Figure S15D), a calcium channel blocker that inhibits MATE transporters [67] by a generally accepted mechanism of PMF generation interference [68,69], was able to prevent the emergence of low accumulators. Furthermore, tachyplesin-NBD cotreatment with sertraline simultaneously increased tachyplesin-NBD accumulation and PI fluorescence levels in individual cells (Figure 4E and F, p-value < 0.0001 and 0.05, respectively). The use of berberine, a natural isoquinoline alkaloid that inhibits MFS transporters [70] and RND pumps [71], potentially by inhibiting conformational changes required for efflux activity [70], and baicalein, a natural flavonoid compound that inhibits ABC [72] and MFS [73,74] transporters, potentially through PMF dissipation [75], prevented the formation of a bimodal distribution of tachyplesin accumulation, however displayed reduction in fluorescence of the whole population (Figure S15E and F). Phenylalanine-arginine beta-naphthylamide (PAbN), a synthetic peptidomimetic compound that inhibits RND pumps [76] through competitive inhibition [77], reserpine, an indole alkaloid that inhibits ABC and MFS transporters, and RND pumps [78], by altering the generation of the PMF [69], and 1-(1-naphthylmethyl)piperazine (NMP), a synthetic piperazine derivative that inhibits RND pumps [79], through non-competitive inhibition [80], did not prevent the emergence of low accumulators (Figure S15G-I).”

      These amendments can be found on lines 337-342 and 367-385.

      (7) Page 8. The term ´medium accumulators´ for a 1:1 mix of low and high accumulators is misleading.

      We have now replaced the term “medium accumulators” with “a 1:1 (v/v) mixture of low and high accumulators”.

      These amendments to the description can be found on lines 238-239.

      (8) Figure 3. It may be more appropriate to rephrase the title of the figure to ´biological processes associated with low tachyplesin accumulation´ (rather than ´facilitate accumulation´). The same applies to the section title on page 8.

      We have amended the title of Figure 3 as requested by the reviewer.

      (9) The fact that the low accumulation phenotype depends on the growth media and conditions and can be prevented by nutrients is highly relevant. I would encourage the authors to consider showing the corresponding data in the main manuscript rather than in the SI.

      We have created a new Figure 5, displaying the impact of the nutritional environment and bacterial growth phase on both tachyplesin-NBD accumulation and efficacy.

      (10) In the discussion the authors state´ Heterogeneous expression of efflux pumps within isogenic bacterial populations has been reported 29,32,33,67-69. However, recent reports have suggested that efflux is not the primary mechanism of antimicrobial resistance within stationary-phase bacteria 31,70.´. In light of the authors´ findings that the response to tachyplesin is induced by exposure and is not pre-selected, could they speculate on why this specific response can be induced in stationary, but not exponential cells? Could there be a combination of pre-existing traits and induced responses at play? Could e.g. the reduced growth rate/metabolism in these cells render these cells less susceptible to the intracellular effects of tachyplesin and slow down the antibiotic efficacy, giving the cells enough time to mount additional protective responses that then lead to the low accumulation phenotype?

      We have now acknowledged that it is conceivable that other pre-existing traits of low accumulators also contribute to reduced tachyplesin accumulation. For example, reduced protein synthesis, energy production and gene expression in low accumulators could slow down tachyplesin efficacy, giving low accumulators more time to mount efflux as an additional protective response.

      “As our accumulation assay did not require the prior selection for phenotypic variants, we have demonstrated that low accumulators emerge subsequent to the initial high accumulation of tachyplesin-NBD, suggesting enhanced efflux as an induced response. However, it is conceivable that other pre-existing traits of low accumulators also contribute to reduced tachyplesin accumulation. For example, reduced protein synthesis, energy production, and gene expression in low accumulators could slow down tachyplesin efficacy, giving low accumulators more time to mount efflux as an additional protective response.”

      This amendment can be found on lines 482-489.

      (11) In the abstract: Is it true that low accumulators ´sequester´ the drug in their membrane? In my understanding ´sequestering´ would imply that low accumulators would bind higher levels of tachyplesin-NBD in their membrane compared to high accumulators (and thereby preventing it from entering the cells). According to Figure 1 J, K, it rather seems that the fluorescent signal around the membrane is also stronger in high accumulators.

      We have now removed the sentence “low accumulators sequester the drug in their membrane” from the abstract. We have instead stated: “These phenotypic variants display enhanced efflux activity to limit intracellular peptide accumulation.”

      These amendments can be found on lines 34-35.

      Reviewer 3:

      (1) The authors' claims about high efflux being the main mechanism of survival are unconvincing, given the current data. There can be several alternative hypotheses that could explain their results, such as lower binding of the AMP, lower rate of internalization, metabolic inactivity, etc. It is unclear how efflux can be important for survival against a peptide that the authors claim binds externally to the cell. The addition of efflux assays would be beneficial for clear interpretations. Given the current data, the authors' claims about efflux being the major mechanism in this resistance are unconvincing (in my humble opinion). Some direct evidence is necessary to confirm the involvement of efflux. The data with CCCP in Figure 4C can only indicate accumulation, not efflux. The authors are encouraged to perform direct efflux assays using known methods (e.g., PMIDs 20606071, 30981730, etc.). Figure 4A: The data does not support the broad claims about efflux. First, if the peptide is accumulated on the outside of the outer membrane, how will efflux help in survival? The dynamics shown in 4A may be due to lower binding, lower entry, or lower efflux. These mechanisms are not dissected here. Second, the heterogeneity can be preexisting or a result of the response to this stress. Either way, whether active efflux or dynamic transcriptomic changes are responsible for these patterns is not clear. Direct efflux assays are crucial to conclude that efflux is a major factor here.

      This important comment is similar in scope to the first comment of reviewer 1 and it is partly due to the fact that we had not clearly explained our efflux assays reported in Figure 4 in the original manuscript. We kindly refer this reviewer to our extensive response 1 to reviewer 1 and corresponding amendments on lines 316-350 and in the new Figure S13 and Figure 4 (reported in the response 1 to reviewer 1 above), where we have now fully addressed this reviewer’s and reviewer 1 concerns, as well as performing new experiments following their important suggestions and the methods described in PMIDs 20606071 suggested by this reviewer.

      (2) The fluorescent imaging experiments can be conducted in the presence of externally added proteases, such as proteinase K, which has multiple cleavage sites on tachyplesin. This would ensure that all the external peptides (both free and bound) are removed. If the signal is still present, it can be concluded that the peptide is present internally. If the peptide is primarily external, the authors need to explain how efflux could help with externally bound peptides. Figure 1J-K: How are the authors sure about the location of the intensity? The peptide can be inside or outside and still give the same signal. To prove that the peptide is inside or outside, a proteolytic cleavage experiment is necessary (proteinase K, Arg-C proteinase, clostripain, etc.).

      We thank the reviewer for this important suggestion.

      We have now performed experiments where stationary phase E. coli was incubated in 46 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> (18.2 μM) tachyplesin-NBD in M9 for 60 min. Next, cells were pelleted and washed to remove extracellular tachyplesin-NBD and then incubated in either M9 or 20 μg mL<sup>-1</sup> (0.7 μΜ) proteinase K in M9 for 120 min. We found that the fluorescence of low accumulators decreased over time in the presence of proteinase K; in contrast, the fluorescence of high accumulators did not decrease over time in the presence of proteinase K. These data therefore suggest that tachyplesin-NBD is present only on the cell membrane of low accumulators and both on the membrane and intracellularly in high accumulators.

      Moreover, confocal microscopy using tachyplesin-NBD along with the membrane dye FM™ 4-64FX further confirmed that tachyplesin-NBD is present only on the cell membrane of low accumulators and both on the membrane and intracellularly in high accumulators.

      These amendments can be found on lines 173-179, lines 188-192 and in the new Figures S4 and S6.

      (3) Further genetic experiments are necessary to test whether efflux genes are involved at all. The genetic data presented by the authors in Figure S11 is crucial and should be further extended. The problem with fitting this data to the current hypothesis is as follows: If specific efflux pumps are involved in the resistance mechanism, then single deletions would cause some changes to the resistance phenotype, and the data in Figure S11 would look different. If there is redundancy (as is the case in many efflux phenotypes), the authors may consider performing double deletions on the major RND regulators (for example, evgA and marA). Additionally, the deletion of pump components such as TolC (one of the few OM components) and adaptors (such as acrA/D) might also provide insights. If the peptide is present in the periplasm, then deletions involving outer components would become important.

      This important comment is similar in scope to the third comment of reviewer 2. We have now performed tachyplesin-NBD accumulation assays using 28 single and 4 double E. coli BW25113 gene-deletion mutants of efflux components and transcription factors regulating efflux. While for the majority of the mutants we recorded bimodal distributions of tachyplesin-NBD accumulation similar to the distribution recorded for the E. coli BW25113 parental strain (Figure 4B and Figure S13), we found unimodal distributions of tachyplesin-NBD accumulation constituted only of high accumulators for both DqseB and DqseBDqseC mutants as well as reduced numbers of low accumulators for the DacrADtolC mutant.

      These amendments can be found on lines 351-361, in the new Figure 4B and in the new Figure S14, please also see our response to comment 3 of reviewer 2.

      (4) Line numbers would have been really helpful. Please mention the size of the peptide (length and spatial) for readers.

      We have now added line numbers to the revised manuscript. The length and molecular weight of tachyplesin-1 have now been added on lines 75.

      (5) Figure S4 is unclear. How were the low accumulators collected? What prompted the low-temperature experiment? The conclusion that it accumulates at the outer membrane is unjustified. Where is the data for high accumulators?

      We have now corrected the results section to state that tachyplesin-NBD accumulates on the cell membranes, rather than at the outer membrane of E. coli cells.

      These amendments can be found on lines 178 and 190.

      We would like to clarify that in Figure S4 we compare the distribution of tachyplesin-NBD single-cell fluorescence at low temperature versus 37 °C across the whole stationary phase E. coli population, we did not collect low accumulators only.

      The low-temperature experiment was prompted by a previous publication paper (Zhou Y et al. 2015: doi: 10.1021/ac504880r. Epub 2015 Mar 24. PMID: 25753586) that showed non-specific adherence of antimicrobials to the bacterial surface occurs at low temperatures and that passive and active transport of antimicrobials across the membrane is significantly diminished. Additionally, there are previous reports that suggest low temperatures inhibit post-binding peptide-lipid interactions, but not the primary binding step (PMID: 16569868; PMCID: PMC1426969; PMID: 3891625; PMCID: PMC262080).

      Therefore, the low-temperature experiment was performed to quantify the fluorescence of cells due to non-specific binding. This quantification allowed us to deduce that fluorescence levels of high accumulators are above the measured non-specific binding fluorescence (measured in the low-temperature experiment for the whole stationary phase E. coli population) is the result of intracellular tachyplesin-NBD accumulation. In contrast, the comparable fluorescence levels between all the cells in the low-temperature experiment and the low accumulator subpopulation at 37 °C suggest that tachyplesin-NBD is predominantly accumulated on the cell membranes of low accumulators instead of intracellularly.

      Please also see our response to comment 2 above for further evidence supporting that tachyplesin-NBD accumulates only on the cell membranes of low accumulators and both on the cell membranes and intracellularly in low accumulators.

      (6) Figure S5: Describe the microfluidic setup briefly. Why did the distribution pattern change (compared to Figure 1A)? Now, there are more high accumulators. Does the peptide get equally distributed between daughter cells?

      We have now added a brief description of the microfluidic setup on lines 182-184.

      The difference in the abundance of low and high accumulators between the microfluidics and flow cytometry measurements is likely due to differences in cell density, i.e. a few cells per channel vs millions of cells in a tube. A second major difference is that tachyplesin-NBD is continuously supplied in the microfluidic device for the entire duration of the experiment, therefore, the extracellular concentration of tachyplesin-NBD does not decrease over time. In contrast, tachyplesin-NBD is added to the tube only at the beginning of the experiment, therefore, the extracellular concentration of tachyplesin-NBD likely decreases in time as it is accumulated by the bacteria. The relative abundance of low and high accumulators changes with the extracellular concentration of tachyplesin-NBD as shown in Figure 1A.

      We have added a sentence to acknowledge this discrepancy on lines 186-187.

      No instances of cell division were observed in stationary phase E. coli in the absence of nutrients in all microfluidics assays. Therefore, we cannot comment on the distribution of tachyplesin-NBD across daughter cells.

      (7) How did the authors conclude this: "tachyplesin accumulation on the bacterial membrane may not be sufficient for bacterial eradication"? It is completely unclear to this reviewer.

      We presented this hypothesis at the end of the section “Tachyplesin accumulates primarily in the membranes of low accumulators” as a link to the following section “Tachyplesin accumulation on the bacterial membranes is insufficient for bacterial eradication” where we test this hypothesis. For clarity, we have now moved this sentence to the beginning of the section “Tachyplesin accumulation on the bacterial membranes is insufficient for bacterial eradication”.

      (8) What is meant by membrane accumulation? Outside, inside, periplasm? Where? Figure 2H conclusions are unjustified. Bacterial killing with many antibiotics is associated with membrane damage, which is an aftereffect of direct antibiotic action. How can the authors state that "low accumulators primarily accumulate tachyplesin-NBD on the bacterial membrane, maintaining an intact membrane, strongly contributing to the survival of the bacterial population"? This reviewer could not find justifications for the claims about the location of the accumulation or cells actively maintaining an intact membrane. Also, PI staining reports damage both membranes.

      Based on the experiments that we have carried out after this reviewer’s suggestions, please see response 2 above, it is likely that tachyplesin-NBD is present only on the bacterial surface, i.e. in or on the outer membrane of low accumulators, considering that their fluorescence decreases during treatment with proteinase K. However, to take a more conservative approach we have now written on the cell membranes throughout the manuscript, i.e. either the outer or the inner membrane.

      We have also rephrased the statement reported by the reviewer as follows:

      “Taken together with PI staining data indicating membrane damage caused by high tachyplesin accumulation, these data demonstrate that low accumulators, which primarily accumulate tachyplesin-NBD on the bacterial membranes, maintain membrane integrity and strongly contribute to the survival of the bacterial population in response to tachyplesin treatment.”

      These amendments can be found on lines 228-232.

      (9) Figure 3: The findings about cluster 2 and cluster 4 genes do not correlate logically. If the cells are in a metabolically low active state, how are the cells getting enough energy for active efflux and membrane transport? This scenario is possible, but the authors must confirm the metabolic activity by measuring respiration rates. Also, metabolically less-active cells may import a lower number of peptides to begin with. That also may contribute to cell survival. Additionally, lowered metabolism is a known strategy of antibiotic survival that is distinctly different from efflux-mediated survival.

      Following this reviewer’s comment and comment 2 of reviewer 1, we have now carried out further experiments to estimate the metabolic activity of low and high accumulators. Please see our response to comment 2 of reviewer 1 above.

      (10) Figure S10: How did the authors test their hypothesis that cardiolipin is involved in the binding of the peptide to the membrane? The transcriptome data does not confirm it. Genetic experiments are necessary to confirm this claim.

      We would like to clarify that we have not set out to test the hypothesis that cardiolipin is involved in the binding of tachyplesin-NBD. We have only stated that cardiolipin could bind tachyplesin due to its negative charge. We have now cited two previous studies that suggest that tachyplesin has an increased affinity for lipids mixtures containing either cardiolipin (Edwards et al. ACS Inf Dis 2017) or PG lipids (Matsuzaki et al. BBA 1991), i.e. the main constituents of cardiolipins.

      These amendments can be found on lines 264-267.

      (11) Figure 4B-F: There are several controls missing. For Sertraline treatment, the authors must test that the metabolic profile, transcriptomic changes, or import of the peptide are not responsible for enhanced survival. CCCP will not only abolish efflux but also many other respiration-associated or all other energy-driven processes.

      Figure 4D presents data acquired in efflux assays in the absence of extracellular tachyplesin-NBD. Therefore, altered tachyplesin-NBD import cannot contribute to the lack of formation of the low accumulator subpopulation.

      We have now acknowledged that it is conceivable that increased tachyplesin efficacy is due to metabolic and transcriptomic changes induced by sertraline.

      These amendments can be found on lines 396-397.

      We have also acknowledged that CCCP does not only abolish efflux but also other respiration-associated and energy-driven processes.

      These amendments can be found on lines 341-342.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a very well-written paper presenting interesting findings related to the recovery following the end-Permian event in continental settings, from N China. The finding is timely as the topic is actively discussed in the scientific community. The data provides additional insights into the faunal, and partly, floral global recovery following the EPE, adding to the global picture.

      Strengths:

      The conclusions are supported by an impressive amount of sedimentological and paleontological data (mainly trace fossils) and illustrations.

      We thank Reviewer #1 for the positive assessments.

      Weaknesses: [eliminated in revision]

      We thank Reviewer #1.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors made a thorough revision of the manuscript, strengthening the message. They also considered all the comments made by the reviewers and provided appropriate and convincing arguments.

      Strengths:

      The revised manuscript clarifies all the major points raised by the reviewers, and the way the information is presented (in the text, figures and tables) is clear.

      We thank Reviewer #2 for the positive comments on our work.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors provided an appropriate and convincing rebuttal regarding the potential weakness I pointed out in the first review of the manuscript. Therefore, I do not see any major issue in their work.

      Introduction

      (1) P. 2, L. 32: Replace "to migrated" with "to migrate".

      Revised as suggested.

      (2) P. 3, L. 43-44: We recently published a review article on the tetrapod terrestrial record from the Central European Basin, showing that Olenekian tetrapod faunas (and ichnofaunas) were already quite rich and diverse. Article: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earscirev.2025.105085

      Yes, we have read this paper. This summary is very important for the understanding of the biotic recovery after the PTME, especially in the early stage. We have added the new result in our manuscript.

      (3) P. 3, L. 57: Replace "recovered terrestrial ecosystems in tropical" with "recovered tropical terrestrial ecosystems".

      Revised as suggested.

      Results and Discussion

      (4) P. 6, L. 118: Replace "declined" with "decline".

      Revised as suggested.

      (5) P. 7, L. 131: Replace "microbial" with "microbially".

      Revised as suggested.

      Conclusions

      (6) P. 11, L. 224: Replace "as little as" with "as early as".

      Revised as suggested.

      (7) P. 11, L. 227: Replace "not only results in" with "not only result in".

      Revised as suggested.

      (8) 11, L. 230: Replace "suggesting" with "suggest".

      Revised as suggested.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Guo and colleagues features the documentation and interpretation of three successions of continental to marginal marine deposits spanning the P/T transition and their respective ichnofaunas. Based on these new data inferences concerning end-Permian mass extinction and Triassic recovery in the tropical realm are discussed.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is well-written and organized and includes a large amount of new lithological and ichnological data that illuminate ecosystem evolution in a time of large-scale transition. The lithological documentations, facies interpretations, and ichnotaxonomic assignments look okay (with a few exceptions).

      We thank Reviewer #3 for the positive assessments.

      Weaknesses:

      Weaknesses: [all eliminated in revision]

      We thank Reviewer #3.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The authors found that IL-1b signaling is pivotal for hypoxemia development and can modulate NETs formation in LPS+HVV ALI model.  

      Strengths: 

      They used IL1R1 ko mice and proved that IL1R1 is involved in ALI model proving that IL1b signalling leads towards ARDS. In addition, hypothermia reduces this effect, suggesting a therapeutic option.  

      We thank the Reviewer for recognizing the strengths of our study and their positive feedback.

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) IL1R1 binds IL1a and IL1b. What would be the role of IL1a in this scenario? 

      Thank you for asking this question. We have addressed this in our previous paper (Nosaka et al. Front Immunol 2020;11; 207) where we used  anti-IL-1a and IL-1a KO mice (Nosaka et al. Front Immunol 2020;11; 207) in our model and found that neither anti-IL-1a treated mice nor IL-1a KO mice were protected. Thus, IL-1b plays a role in inducing hypoxemia during LPS+HVV but not IL-1a. We will now add this point in our revised manuscript discussion.

      (2) The authors depleted neutrophils using anti-Ly6G. What about MDSCs? Do these latter cells be involved in ARDS and VILI?  

      Anti-Ly6G neutrophils depletion may potentially affect G-MDSCs as well (Blood Adv 2022 Jul 29;7(1):73–86), however, we have not looked directly at G-MDSCs.  If these cells were depleted we would have expected to see an increase in inflammation, which we did not.   Instead, anti-Ly6G treated mice were protected. Thus, we can not comment on any presumed role of G-MDSCs in LPS+HVV induced severe ALI model that we used.  

      (3) The authors found that TH inhibited IL-1β release from macrophages led to less NETs formation and albumin leakage in the alveolar space in their lung injury model. A graphical abstract could be included suggesting a cellular mechanism.  

      Thanks for summarizing our findings and the suggestion. Unfortunately, eLIFE does not publish a graphical abstract.  

      (4) If Macrophages are responsible for IL1b release that via IL1R1 induces NETosis, what happens if you deplete macrophages? what is the role of epithelial cells?  

      Previous studies have found that macrophage depletion is protective in several models of ALI (Eyal. Intensive Care Med. 2007;33:1212–1218., Lindauer.  J Immunol. 2009;183:1419–1426.), and other researchers have found that airway epithelial cells did not contribute to IL-1β secretion (Tang. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e37689.). We have previously reported that epithelial cells produce IL-18 without LPS priming signal during LPS+HVV (Nosaka et al. Front Immunol 2020;11; 207). Thus, IL-18 is not sufficient to induce Hypoxemia as Saline+HVV treated mice do not develop hypoxemia (Nosaka et al. Front Immunol 2020;11; 207). We will now add this point to the revised discussion of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The manuscript by Nosaka et al is a comprehensive study exploring the involvement of IL1beta signaling in a 2-hit model of lung injury + ventilation, with a focus on modulation by hypothermia. 

      Strengths: 

      The authors demonstrate quite convincingly that interleukin 1 beta plays a role in the development of ventilator-induced lung injury in this model, and that this role includes the regulation of neutrophil extracellular trap formation. The authors use a variety of in vivo animal-based and in vitro cell culture work, and interventions including global gene knockout, cell-targeted knockout and pharmacological inhibition, which greatly strengthen the ability to make clear biological interpretations. 

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive feedback 

      Weaknesses: 

      A primary point for open discussion is the translatability of the findings to patients. The main model used, one of intratracheal LPS plus mechanical ventilation is well accepted for research exploring the pathogenesis and potential treatments for acute respiratory distress syndrome (ARDS). However, the interpretation may still be open to question - in the model here, animals were exposed to LPS to induce inflammation for only 2 hours, and seemingly displayed no signs of sickness, before the start of ventilation. This would not be typical for the majority of ARDS patients, and whether hypothermia could be effective once substantial injury is already present remains an open question. The interaction between LPS/infection and temperature is also complicated - in humans, LPS (or infection) induces a febrile, hyperthermic response, whereas in mice LPS induces hypothermia (eg. Ganeshan K, Chawla A. Nat Rev Endocrinol. 2017;13:458-465). Given this difference in physiological response, it is therefore unclear whether hypothermia in mice and hypothermia in humans are easily comparable. Finally, the use of only young, male animals such as in the current study has been typical but may be criticised as limiting translatability to people. 

      Therefore while the conclusions of the paper are well supported by the data, and the biological pathways have been impressively explored, questions still remain regarding the ultimate interpretations.  

      We agree with the reviewer that at two hours post LPS, there is only minimal pulmonary inflammation at that time (Dagvadorj et al Immunity 42, 640–653). This is a limitation to the experimental model we used in our study. Additionally, as the reviewer pointed out that LPS induces hyperthermia in human, but it is also well-established that physiological hypothermia occurs in humans with severe infections and sepsis (Baisse. Am J Emerg Med. 2023 Sep: 71: 134-138., Werner.  Am J Emerg Med. 2025 Feb;88:64-78.). Therefore, the difference between human and mouse responses to sepsis or infections may be more nuanced.  Furthermore, it is important to distinguish between physiological hypothermia (just <36°C) and therapeutic hypothermia (typically 32-34°C). We will add to the discussion whether hypothermia serves as a protective response, and the transition from normothermia to hyperthermia could have detrimental effects. We only used young male mice in our study as the Reviewer points out; we will also add this point to the revised discussion as a limitation of our study.

      Recommendations for the authors: 

      (i) With hypothermia, metabolic activity would be expected to be reduced and therefore presumably impact on CO2/pH. These may have an impact on outcomes from ventilation, so could the authors include this data and discuss as appropriate? 

      We have now included these data in Suppl Fig 6.  While we observed significant differences in blood pH and  PaCO<sub>2</sub> in Hypothermia treatment group, these values remained within clinically normal range (PaCO<sub>2</sub> : 35 - 45 mmHg, pH : 7.35 - 7.45). Neither Alkalosis (PaCO<sub>2</sub> < 35 mmHg , pH> 7.45) nor Acidosis (PaCO<sub>2</sub> > 45 mmHg, pH < 7.35) was observed.

      (ii) It is noticeable that there are quite large differences in experimental numbers between groups - typically 7-12, 5-12 in Figure 2. How were these N determined? For example is there a reason why there is apparently N = 8 for BALF neutrophils in the saline + HVV group (Figure 1c) but N = 12 for LPS + HVV group? Did any animals die during any of the protocols for example? 

      We conducted experiments with 4 mice per experiment (2 mice per group x2  or 4 mice per group) for ventilation experiments, and pooled data from 5-6 independent experiments or 3-4 independent experiments, respectively. No mouse mortality was observed (unless otherwise noted). However, in the severe ARDS group, some mice were dehydrated by the endpoint of experiments, preventing blood or BALF collections. As a result sample sizes were unequal in some case. Nevertheless, no data were selectively excluded.

      (iii) Discussion - On page 13 you refer to data involving Cl-amidine administration. This does not seem to be related to any experiments reported in the manuscript. 

      We apology for this mistake and have removed it.

      (iv) Methods - authors state that BALF was obtained after 150 minutes of ventilation, yet the experiments apparently lasted for 180 minutes. Presumably this is an error? 

      We apology for this inconsistency.  We collected blood for measuring blood gas at 30 min and 150 min after ventilation. However, mice were kept on ventilator 30 min longer, and then mice were euthanized and BALF were collected.  Thus, BALF were collected at 180 min, 30 minutes after the final blood draw. We have corrected the methods in revised manuscript.  

      (v) Statistical methods - authors state that sometimes Mann-Whitney U-test was used and sometimes unpaired t-test, presumably reflecting that some data were normally distributed and some were not. Could the authors please describe the tests used to confirm distribution of data. 

      We have clarified which stattistcal methods were used in our revised manuscript. 

      Briefly, Normality within the groups was assessed using the Shapiro-Wilk and KolmogorovSmirnov tests. Three-way ANOVA (Figure 1B; Supplemental Figure 1B-D; Supplemental Figure 6), one-way ANOVA (Supplemental Figure 4D-E; Supplemental Figure 5C), and two-way ANOVA were performed for data with more than two groups, followed by Tukey's post hoc test. Some groups analyzed by two-way ANOVA in Figure 1 and Supplemental Figure 1 failed the normality tests due to zero values (analyte not detected by ELISA) or the relatively small sample size, as samples were distributed across multiple measurements. However, the primary group of interest, LPS+HVV, showed significant differences from other groups with consistently low P-values in most datasets, supporting the decision to retain the ANOVA analyses. For comparisons between two groups, the Mann-Whitney U test was used when one or both groups failed the Shapiro-Wilk normality test, while the unpaired Student's t-test was applied to the remaining normally distributed data.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript presents a significant and rigorous investigation into the role of CHMP5 in regulating bone formation and cellular senescence. The study provides compelling evidence that CHMP5 is essential for maintaining endolysosomal function and controlling mitochondrial ROS levels, thereby preventing the senescence of skeletal progenitor cells.

      Strengths:

      The authors demonstrate that the deletion of Chmp5 results in endolysosomal dysfunction, elevated mitochondrial ROS, and ultimately enhanced bone formation through both autonomous and paracrine mechanisms. The innovative use of senolytic drugs to ameliorate musculoskeletal abnormalities in Chmp5-deficient mice is a novel and critical finding, suggesting potential therapeutic strategies for musculoskeletal disorders linked to endolysosomal dysfunction.

      Weaknesses:

      The manuscript requires a deeper discussion or exploration of CHMP5's roles and a more refined analysis of senolytic drug specificity and effects. This would greatly enhance the comprehensiveness and clarity of the manuscript.

      We thank the reviewer for these insightful comments. In the revised manuscript, we have expanded the discussion of the distinct roles of CHMP5 in different cell types. Specifically, we add the following sentences (Lines 433-439 in the combined manuscript):

      “Also, a previous study by Adoro et al. did not detect endolysosomal abnormalities in Chmp5 deficient developmental T cells [1]. Since both osteoclasts and T cells are of hematopoietic origin, and meanwhile osteogenic cells and MEFs, which show endolysosomal abnormalities after CHMP5 deficiency, are of mesenchymal origin, it turns out that the function of CHMP5 in regulating endolysosomal pathway could be cell lineage-specific, which remains clarified in future studies.”

      In addition, we tested another senolytic drug Navitoclax (ABT-263), which is a BCL-2 family inhibitor and induces apoptosis of senescent cells, in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice. Micro-CT analysis showed that ABT-263 could also improve periskeletal bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice (Fig. 5F). Furthermore, we have also discussed the potential off-target effects of senolytic drugs in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we added the following paragraph (Lines 441-451):

      “Furthermore, it is unclear whether the effect of senolytic drugs in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice involves targeting osteoclasts other than osteogenic cells, as osteoclast senescence has not yet been evaluated. However, the efficacy of Q + D in targeting osteogenic cells, which is the focus of the current study, was confirmed in Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice (Fig. 5C-E). Additionally, Q + D caused a higher cell apoptotic ratio in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> compared to wild-type periskeletal progenitors in ex vivo culture (Fig. 5A), demonstrating the effectiveness of Q + D in targeting osteogenic cells in the Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> model. Furthermore, an alternative senolytic drug ABT-263 could also ameliorate periskeletal bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice (Fig. 5F). Together, these results confirm that osteogenic cell senescence is responsible for the bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> and Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice, and senolytic treatments are effective in alleviating these skeletal disorders.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors try to show the importance of CHMP5 for skeletal development.

      Strengths:

      The findings of this manuscript are interesting. The mouse phenotypes are well done and are of interest to a broader (bone) field.

      Weaknesses:

      The mechanistic insights are mediocre, and the cellular senescence aspect poor.

      In total, it has not been shown that there are actual senescent cells that are reduced after D+Qtreatment. These statements need to be scaled back substantially.

      We thank the reviewer for these suggestive comments. We have added additional results to strengthen the senescent phenotypes of Chmp5-deficient skeletal progenitor cells, including significant enrichment of the SAUL_SEN_MAYO geneset (positively correlated with cell senescence) and the KAMMINGA_SENESCENCE geneset (negatively correlated with cell senescence) at the transcriptional level by GSEA analysis of RNA-seq data (Fig. S3C), and the increase of γH2Ax<sup>+</sup>;GFP<sup>+</sup> cells at periskeletal overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice vs. the periosteum of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk/+</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> control mice (Fig. 3E). These results further advocate for the senescent phenotypes of Chmp5-deficient skeletal progenitors.

      Furthermore, the combination of Q + D caused a higher cell apoptotic ratio in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> vs. wildtype periskeletal progenitors in ex vivo culture (Fig. 5A), suggesting their effectiveness in targeting periskeletal progenitor cell senescence in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice. Furthermore, we tested an alternative senolytic drug ABT-263, which is an inhibitor of the BCL-2 family and induces apoptosis of senescent cells, in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice, and ABT-263 could also alleviate periskeletal bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice (Fig. 5F). Together, these results demonstrate that osteogenic cell senescence is responsible for abnormal bone overgrowth in Chmp5-deficient mice and that senolytic drugs are effective in improving these skeletal disorders.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Zhang et al. reported that CHMP5 restricts bone formation by controlling endolysosomemitochondrion-mediated cell senescence. The effects of CHMP5 on osteoclastic bone resorption and bone turnover have been reported previously (PMID: 26195726), in which study the aberrant bone phenotype was observed in the CHMP5-ctsk-CKO mouse model, using the same mouse model, Zhang et al., report a novel role of CHMP5 on osteogenesis through affecting cell senescence. Overall, it is an interesting study and provides new insights in the field of cell senescence and bone.

      Strengths:

      Analyzed the bone phenotype OF CHMP5-periskeletal progenitor-CKO mouse model and found the novel role of senescent cells on osteogenesis and migration.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) There are a lot of papers that have reported that senescence impairs osteogenesis of skeletal stem cells. In this study, the author claimed that Chmp5 deficiency induces skeletal progenitor cell senescence and enhanced osteogenesis. Can the authors explain the controversial results?

      Different skeletal stem cell populations in time and space have been identified and reported [2-6]. The present study shows that Chmp5 deficiency in periskeletal (Ctsk-Cre) and endosteal (Dmp1-Cre) osteogenic cells causes cell senescence and aberrant bone formation. Although cell senescence during aging can impair the osteogenesis of marrow stromal cells (MSCs), which contributes to diseases with low bone mass such as osteoporosis, aging can also increase heterotopic ossification or mineralization in musculoskeletal soft tissues such as ligaments and tendons [7]. Notably, the abnormal periskeletal bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice was mainly mapped to insertion sites of tendons and ligaments on the bone (Fig. 1A and E), consistent with changes during aging. More broadly, aging can also cause abnormal ossification or mineralization in other body tissues, such as the heart valve [8, 9]. These different results reflect an aberrant state of ossification or mineralization in musculoskeletal tissues and throughout the body during aging. Based on the reviewer’s comment, we have discussed these results in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we add the following paragraph (Lines 453-462 in the combined manuscript):

      “Notably, aging is associated with decreased osteogenic capacity in marrow stromal cells, which is related to conditions with low bone mass, such as osteoporosis. Rather, aging is also accompanied by increased ossification or mineralization in musculoskeletal soft tissues, such as tendons and ligaments [7]. In particular, the abnormal periskeletal overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice was predominantly mapped to insertion sites of tendons and ligaments on the bone (Fig. 1A and E), which is consistent with changes during aging and suggests that mechanical stress at these sites could contribute to the aberrant bone growth. These results suggest that skeletal stem/progenitor cells at different sites of musculoskeletal tissues could demonstrate different, even opposite outcomes in osteogenesis, due to cell senescence.”

      (2) Co-culture of Chmp5-KO periskeletal progenitors with WT ones should be conducted to detect the migration and osteogenesis of WT cells in response to Chmp5-KO-induced senescent cells. In addition, the co-culture of WT periskeletal progenitors with senescent cells induced by H2O2, radiation, or from aged mice would provide more information.

      In the present study, the increased proliferation and osteogenesis of CD45-;CD31-;GFP- periskeletal progenitors were shown as paracrine mechanisms of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors to promote bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice (Figs. 4F, G, and S4C-E). According to the reviewer’s suggestion, we have carried out the coculture experiment and the coculture of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> with wild-type skeletal progenitors could promote osteogenesis of wild-type cells (Fig. S4B), which further supports the paracrine effect of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors.

      In addition, the cause and outcome of cell senescence could be highly heterogeneous, and different causes of cell senescence can cause significantly distinct, even opposite outcomes. Although the coculture experiments of WT periskeletal progenitors with senescent cells induced by H2O2, radiation, or from aged mice are very interesting, these are beyond the scope of the current study.

      (3) Many EVs were secreted from Chmp5-deleted periskeletal progenitors, compared to the rarely detected EVs around WT cells. Since EVs of BMSCs or osteoprogenitors show strong effects of promoting osteogenesis, did the EVs contribute to the enhanced osteogenesis induced by Chmp5defeciency? Author’s response:

      This is an interesting question. Although we did not separately test the effect of EVs from Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors on the osteogenesis of WT skeletal progenitors, the CD45-;CD31-;GFP- skeletal progenitor cells from Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice have an increased capacity of osteogenesis compared to corresponding cells from control animals (Figs. 4G and S4D). Also, the coculture of Chmp5-deficient with wild-type skeletal progenitors could enhance the osteogenesis of wild-type cells (Fig. S4B). These results suggest that EVs from Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors could promote osteogenesis of neighboring WT skeletal progenitors. The specific functions of EVs of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors in regulating osteogenesis will be further investigated in future studies.

      (4) EVs secreted from senescent cells propagate senescence and impair osteogenesis, why do EVs secreted from senescent cells induced by Chmp5-defeciency have opposite effects on osteogenesis?

      The question is similar to comments #1 and #3 from this reviewer. First, the manifestations (including the secretory phenotype) and outcomes of cell senescence could be highly heterogeneous depending on inducers, tissue and cell contexts, and other factors such as “time”. Different causes of cell senescence could lead to different manifestations and outcomes, which have been discussed in the manuscript (Lines 381-383). Similarly, as mentioned above, skeletal stem/progenitor cells at different sites of musculoskeletal tissues could also demonstrate distinct, even opposite outcomes, as a result of cell senescence (Line 453-462). Second, CD45-;CD31-;GFP- periskeletal progenitor cells from Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice have an increased capacity of proliferation and osteogenesis compared to corresponding cells from control animals (Figs. 4F, G and S4C-E). Furthermore, the conditioned medium of Chmp5-deficient skeletal progenitors promoted the proliferation of ATDC5 cells (Fig. 4E) and the coculture of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> and wild-type periskeletal progenitors could enhance the osteogenesis of wild-type cells (Fig. S4B). Taken together, these results show paracrine actions of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors in promoting aberrant bone growth in Chmp5 conditional knockout mice. We also refer the reviewer to our responses to comments #1 and #3.

      (5) The Chmp5-ctsk mice show accelerated aging-related phenotypes, such as hair loss and joint stiffness. Did Ctsk also label cells in hair follicles or joint tissue?

      This is an interesting question. Although we did not check the expression of CHMP5 in hair follicles, which is outside the scope of the present study, the result in Fig. 1E showed the expression of Ctsk in joint ligaments, tendons, and their insertion sites on the bone (Lines 108-111). Notably, the periskeletal bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice was mainly mapped to insertion sites of ligaments and tendons on the bone, which have been discussed in the revised manuscript (Lines 456-460).

      (6) Fifteen proteins were found to increase and five proteins to decrease in the cell supernatant of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> periskeletal progenitors. How about SASP factors in the secretory profile?

      The SASP phenotype and related factors of senescent cells could be highly heterogeneous depending on inducers, cell types, and timing of senescence [10, 11]. Most of the proteins we identified in the secretome analysis have previously been reported in the secretory profile of osteoblasts or involved in the regulation of osteogenesis. Although we were interested in changes in common SASP factors, such as cytokines and chemokines, the experiment did not detect these factors, probably due to their small molecular weights and the technical limitations of the mass-spec analysis. We have clarified this in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we add the following sentences (Lines 258-261):

      “Notably, the secretome analysis did not detect common SASP factors, such as cytokines and chemokines, in the secretory profile of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> periskeletal progenitors, probably due to their small molecular weights and the technical limitations of the mass-spec analysis.”

      (7) D+Q treatment mitigates musculoskeletal pathologies in Chmp5 conditional knockout mice. In the previously published paper (CHMP5 controls bone turnover rates by dampening NF-κB activity in osteoclasts), inhibition of osteoclastic bone resorption rescues the aberrant bone phenotype of the Chmp5 conditional knockout mice. Whether the effects of D+Q on bone overgrowth is because of the inhibition of bone resorption?

      This is an important question. We have discussed the potential off-target effect of senolytic drugs in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we add the following paragraph (Lines 441451):

      “Furthermore, it is unclear whether the effect of senolytic drugs in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice involves targeting osteoclasts other than osteogenic cells, as osteoclast senescence has not yet been evaluated. However, the efficacy of Q + D in targeting osteogenic cells, which is the focus of the current study, was confirmed in Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice (Fig. 5C-E). Additionally, Q + D caused a higher cell apoptotic ratio in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> compared to wild-type periskeletal progenitors in ex vivo culture (Fig. 5A), demonstrating the effectiveness of Q + D in targeting osteogenic cells in the Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> model. Furthermore, an alternative senolytic drug ABT-263 could also ameliorate periskeletal bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice (Fig. 5F). Together, these results confirm that osteogenic cell senescence is responsible for the bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> and Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice and senolytic treatments are effective in alleviating these skeletal disorders.”

      (8) The role of VPS4A in cell senescence should be measured to support the conclusion that CHMP5 regulates osteogenesis by affecting cell senescence.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. The current study mainly reports the function of CHMP5 in the regulation of skeletal progenitor cell senescence and osteogenesis. The roles of VPS4A in cell senescence and skeletal biology will be further explored in future studies. We have discussed this in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we add the following sentence (Lines 407-409):

      “The roles of VPS4A in regulating musculoskeletal biology and cell senescence should be further explored in future studies.”

      (9) Cell senescence with markers, such as p21 and H2AX, co-stained with GFP should be performed in the mouse models to indicate the effects of Chmp5 on cell senescence in vivo.

      According to the reviewer’s suggestion, we have already performed immunostaining of γH2AX and colocalization with GFP in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> and Chmp5<sup>Ctsk/+</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice. The results showed that there are more γH2AX+;GFP+ cells in the periskeletal overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice compared to the periosteum of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk/+</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> control animals. Because the γH2AX staining could stand as one of the critical results supporting the senescent phenotype of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors. We have added these results to Fig. 3E and put Fig. 3F in the original manuscript into Fig. S3E due to the space limitation in Figure 3. In sum, these results further enrich the senescent manifestations of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors.

      (10) ADTC5 cell as osteochondromas cells line, is not a good cell model of periskeletal progenitors.

      Maybe primary periskeletal progenitor cell is a better choice.

      ATDC5 cells are typically used as a chondrocyte progenitor cell line. However, our previous study showed that ATDC5 cells could also be used as a reasonable cell model for periskeletal progenitors [12], which was mentioned in the manuscript (Lines 202-204). In addition, the results of ATDC5 cells were also verified in primary periskeletal progenitor cells in this study.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Despite the robust experimental framework and intriguing findings, there are several areas that require further attention to enhance the manuscript's overall quality and clarity:

      (1) The manuscript could benefit from a more in-depth discussion of the tissue-specific roles of CHMP5, particularly in addressing why CHMP5 deficiency results in distinct outcomes in osteogenic cells as opposed to other cell types, such as osteoclasts. Expanding the discussion would greatly enhance the comprehensiveness and clarity of the manuscript.

      Based on the reviewer’s suggestion, we have expanded the discussion of the distinct roles of CHMP5 in different cell types. Specifically, we state (Lines 433-439):

      “Also, a previous study by Adoro et al. did not detect endolysosomal abnormalities in _Chmp5_deficient developmental T cells [1]. Since both osteoclasts and T cells are of hematopoietic origin, and meanwhile osteogenic cells and MEFs, which show endolysosomal abnormalities after CHMP5 deficiency, are of mesenchymal origin, it turns out that the function of CHMP5 in regulating the endolysosomal pathway could be cell lineage-specific, which remains clarified in future studies.”

      (2) Given that Figures 1 and 2 suggest that the absence of Chmp5 (CHMP5Ctsk & CHMP5Dmp1) leads to disordered proliferation or mineralization of bone or osteoblasts, the manuscript should delve deeper into the potential links between these findings and aging-related processes, such as age-associated fibrosis. Providing clearer explanations and discussion on these connections would help present a more cohesive understanding of the results in the context of aging.

      We thank the reviewer for this favorable suggestion. A feature of aging is heterotopic ossification or mineralization in musculoskeletal soft tissues, including tendons and ligaments [7]. Notably, the abnormal periskeletal bone formation in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice in this study was mostly mapped to the insertion sites of tendons and ligaments on the bone (Fig. 1A and E), which is consistent with changes during aging and suggests that mechanical stress at these sites could be a contributor to periskeletal overgrowth. We have discussed these results in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we add the following paragraph (Lines 453-462):

      “Notably, aging is associated with decreased osteogenic capacity in marrow stromal cells, which is related to conditions with low bone mass, such as osteoporosis. Rather, aging is also accompanied by increased ossification or mineralization in musculoskeletal soft tissues, such as tendons and ligaments [7]. In particular, the abnormal periskeletal overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice was predominantly mapped to the insertion sites of tendons and ligaments on the bone (Fig. 1A and E), which is consistent with changes during aging and suggests that mechanical stress at these sites could contribute to the aberrant bone growth. These results suggest that skeletal stem/progenitor cells at different sites of musculoskeletal tissues could demonstrate different, even opposite outcomes in osteogenesis, due to cell senescence.”

      (3) The manuscript would be improved by a more refined analysis in Figures 3 and 5, particularly in relation to the use of senolytic drugs. Furthermore, a detailed discussion of the specificity and potential off-target effects of quercetin and dasatinib treatments in Chmp5-deficient mice would strengthen the therapeutic claims of these drugs.

      In Figure 3, we have added additional experiments and results to strengthen the senescent phenotypes of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors, including significant enrichment of the SAUL_SEN_MAYO geneset (positively correlated with cell senescence) and the KAMMINGA_SENESCENCE geneset (negatively correlated with cell senescence) at the transcriptional level by GSEA analysis of RNA-seq data (Fig. S3F), and an increase of γH2AX+;GFP+ cells at the site of periskeletal overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice compared to the periosteum of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk/+</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> control mice (Fig. 3E). These results further enrich the senescent molecular manifestations of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors.

      In Figure 5, we used an alternative senolytic drug ABT-263 to treat Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice, and this antisenescence treatment could also alleviate periskeletal bone overgrowth in this mouse model (Fig. 5F). Furthermore, we have also discussed the potential off-target effects of senolytic drugs in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice. Specifically, we add the following paragraph (Lines 441-451):

      “Furthermore, it is unclear whether the effect of senolytic drugs in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice involves targeting osteoclasts other than osteogenic cells, as osteoclast senescence has not yet been evaluated. However, the efficacy of Q + D in targeting osteogenic cells, which is the focus of the current study, was confirmed in Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice (Fig. 5C-E). Additionally, Q + D caused a higher cell apoptotic ratio in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> compared to wild-type periskeletal progenitors in ex vivo culture (Fig. 5A), demonstrating the effectiveness of Q + D in targeting osteogenic cells in the Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> model. Furthermore, an alternative senolytic drug ABT-263 could also ameliorate periskeletal bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice (Fig. 5F). Together, these results confirm that osteogenic cell senescence is responsible for the bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> and Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice and senolytic treatments are effective in alleviating these skeletal disorders.”

      (4) The manuscript could be further enhanced by providing more details into how CHMP5 specifically regulates VPS4A protein levels. Notably, this is a central aspect of the paper linking CHMP5 to endolysosomal dysfunction.

      We thank the reviewer for this important suggestion. One of the novel findings of this study is that CHMP5 regulates the protein level of VPS4A without affecting its RNA transcription. The mechanism of CHMP5 in the regulation of VPS4A protein will be reported in a separate study. However, we have discussed the potential mechanism in the manuscript (Lines 399-409). Specifically, we state:

      “However, the mechanism of CHMP5 in the regulation of the VPS4A protein has not yet been studied. Since CHMP5 can recruit the deubiquitinating enzyme USP15 to stabilize IκBα in osteoclasts by suppressing ubiquitination-mediated proteasomal degradation [13], it is also possible that CHMP5 stabilizes the VPS4A protein by recruiting deubiquitinating enzymes and regulating the ubiquitination of VPS4A, which needs to be clarified in future studies. Notably, mutations in the VPS4A gene in humans can cause multisystemic diseases, including musculoskeletal abnormalities [14] (OMIM: 619273), suggesting that normal expression and function of VPS4A are important for musculoskeletal physiology. The roles of VPS4A in regulating musculoskeletal biology and cell senescence should be further explored in future studies.”

      (5) The discussion section could be enriched by more thoroughly integrating the current findings with previous studies on CHMP5, particularly those exploring its role in osteoclast differentiation and NF-κB signaling.

      The comment is similar to comment #1 of this reviewer. We have expanded the discussion of the distinct functions of CHMP5 in osteoclasts and osteogenic cells (Lines 424-439). We also refer the reviewer to our response to comment #1.

      (6) Figure S4 D is incorrectly arranged and should be revised accordingly.

      Sorry for the confusion. We have added additional annotations to make the images clearer. Now it is Fig. S4E in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Abstract A clinical perspective or at least an outline is desirable.

      The clinical importance of the findings of this study in understanding and treating musculoskeletal disorders of lysosomal storage diseases has been highlighted at the end of the abstract (Line 38).

      (2) Introduction Header missing.

      The protein name is BCL2, not Bcl2.

      These have been corrected in the revised manuscript (Lines 41, 66).

      (3) Results

      The mouse phenotype experiments are well done.

      Hmga1, Hmga2, Trp53, Ets1, and Txn1 are no typical senescence-associated genes. How about

      Cdkn2a and Cdkn1a? These could easily be highlighted in Figure 3B.

      Hmga1, Hmga2, Trp53, Ets1, and Txn1 are within the geneset of Reactome Cellular Senescence. Notably, only the protein levels of CDKN2A (p16) and CDKN1A (p21) showed significant changes (Fig. 3D) and the mRNA levels of Cdkn2a and Cdkn1a did not show significant changes according to RNAseq data. We have added the result of Cdkn2a and Cdkn1a mRNA levels to Fig. S3D in the revised manuscript. Also, we add the following sentences in the text (Lines 193-195):

      “However, the mRNA levels of Cdkn2a (p16) and Cdkn1a (p21) did not show significant changes according to the RNA-seq analysis (Fig. S3D).”

      Figure 3C: Which gene set was used for SASP?

      The SASP geneset in Fig. 3C was from the Reactome database. We have clarified this in the figure legend of Fig. 3 in the revised manuscript (Line 1013).

      The symptom "joint stiffness/contracture" could also be due to skeletal abnormalities related to Chmp5Ctsk.

      Joint stiffness/contracture during aging is mainly the result of heterotopic ossification or mineralization in musculoskeletal soft tissues, including ligaments, tendons, joint capsules, and their insertion sites on the bone. Notably, the periskeletal bone overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice was mainly mapped to the insertion sites of tendons, ligaments, and joint capsules on the bone, which are consistent with changes during aging. These results have been discussed in the revised manuscript (Lines 456-460).

      Overall, cellular senescence needs at least Cdkn2a and/or Cdkn1a and another marker, i.e. SenMayo or telomere-associated foci or senescence-associated distortion of satellites.

      We have run GSEA with the SenMayo geneset and the result is added in Fig. S3F in the revised manuscript. Also, we ran another geneset KAMMINGA_SENESCENCE which includes genes downregulated in cell senescence. Both genesets are significantly enriched in Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors based on RNA-seq data (Fig. S3F).

      In addition, we also performed immunostaining for another senescence marker γH2AX and the results showed that there are more γH2AX+;GFP+ cells in periskeletal overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>mTmG/+</sup> mice compared to the periosteum of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk/+</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> control animals (Fig. 3E).

      Together, these results further support the senescent phenotypes of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors.

      For Figure 4A: What is the NES?

      The value of NES has been added in Fig. 4A.

      The existence of vesicles does not necessarily indicate more SASP. Author’s response:

      We agree with the reviewer that the secretion of extracellular vesicles is not directly correlated with the SASP. In this study, the increased secretory vesicles around Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> periskeletal progenitors represent a secretory phenotype of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors and have paracrine effects in the abnormal bone growth in Chmp5 conditional knockout mice as shown in Figs. 4 and S4.

      The Chmp5-deficient cells COULD promote the proliferation and osteogenesis of other progenitors, but they might as well not. And if this is through the SASP, is completely unresolved.

      CD45<sup>-</sup>;CD31<sup>-</sup>;GFP<sup>-</sup> periskeletal progenitor cells from Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice showed an increased capacity of proliferation and osteogenesis compared to the corresponding cells from control animals (Figs. 4F, G, and S4C-E). Also, the conditioned medium of Chmp5-deficient skeletal progenitors promoted the proliferation of ATDC5 cells (Fig. 4E). In addition, the coculture of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> and wild-type periskeletal progenitors could enhance the osteogenesis of wild-type cells (Fig. S4B). These results demonstrate the paracrine actions of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors in promoting aberrant bone growth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> and Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice. However, factors that mediate the paracrine effects of Chmp5-deficient periskeletal progenitors remain further clarified in future studies.

      This has been mentioned in the revised manuscript (Lines 263-265).

      Figure 5C: The time points are not labelled.

      The time point of 16 weeks was mentioned in the Method section and now it has been added in the legend of Fig. 5C (Line 1063).

      Figure B: Was the bone's overall thickness quantified?

      In Fig. 5B, bone morphology in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> mice is irregular and difficult to quantify. Therefore, we did not qualify the overall bone thickness in these animals. However, the thickness of the cortical bone was measured by micro-CT analysis in Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice after treatment with Q + D (Fig. 5E). Also, we have added the image of the gross femur thickness of Chmp5<sup>Dmp1</sup> mice before and after treatment with Q + D in Fig. 5E.

      It needs to be demonstrated that the actual cell number was reduced after D+Q treatment.

      The Q + D treatment caused a higher cell apoptotic ratio in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup> vs. wild-type skeletal progenitors in ex vivo culture (Fig. 5A), suggesting its effectiveness in targeting the senescent periskeletal progenitors.

      Figure 7A: What is the NES?

      The value of NES has been added in Fig. 7A.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The WB analysis should be quantified in the Figure 3D.

      In Fig. 3D, the numbers above the lanes of p16 and p21 are the results of the quantification of the band intensity after normalization by β-Actin, which has been indicated in the Figure legend (Lines 10151017).

      (2) The osteoblast detection should be measured with antibody against osteocalcin.

      This comment did not specify what result the reviewer was referring to. However, most of the experiments in this study were performed in primary skeletal progenitor cells or cell lines. Osteoblasts were not specifically involved in the current study.

      (3) Co-culture of Chmp5-KO periskeletal progenitors with WT ones should be conducted to detect the migration and osteogenesis of WT cell in response to Chmp5-KO induced senescent cells. In addition, co-culture of WT periskeletal progenitors with senescent cells induced by H2O2, radiation, or from aged mice would provide more information.

      This comment is the same as comment #2 in the Public Reviews of this Reviewer. We already carried out the coculture experiment of Chmp5-deficient and wild-type periskeletal progenitors and the result was added in Fig. S4B. We refer the reviewer to our response to comment #2 in the Public Reviews for more details.

      (4) D+Q treatment mitigates musculoskeletal pathologies in Chmp5 conditional knockout mice. In the previously published paper (CHMP5 controls bone turnover rates by dampening NF-κB activity in osteoclasts), inhibition of osteoclastic bone resorption rescues the aberrant bone phenotype of the Chmp5 conditional knockout mice. Is the effect of D+Q on bone overgrowth because of the inhibition of bone resorption?

      This comment is the same as comment #7 in the Public Reviews of this Reviewer, where we already address this question.

      (5) The role of VPS4A in cell senescence should be measured to support the conclusion that CHMP5 regulates osteogenesis through affecting cell senescence.

      This comment is the same as comment #8 in the Public Reviews of this Reviewer. We refer the reviewer to our response to that comment.

      (6) Cell senescence with the markers, such as p21 and H2AX, co-stained with GFP should be performed in the mouse models to indicate the effects of Chmp5 on cell senescence in vivo.

      This comment is the same as comment #9 in the Public Reviews of this Reviewer. We have performed immunostaining of γH2AX and colocalization with GFP in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice and Chmp5<sup>Ctsk/+</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice. The results showed that there were more γH2AX+;GFP+ cells at the site of periskeletal overgrowth in Chmp5<sup>Ctsk</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> mice compared to the periosteum of Chmp5<sup>Ctsk/+</sup>;Rosa26<sup>26mTmG/+</sup> control mice (Fig. 3E). We also refer the reviewer to our response to comment #9 in Public Reviews.

      (7) ADTC5 cell as osteochondromas cells line, is not a good cell model of periskeletal progenitors.

      Maybe primary periskeletal progenitor cell is a better choice.

      This comment is the same as comment #10 in the Public Reviews of this Reviewer. Our previous study showed that ATDC5 cells could be used as a reasonable cell model for periskeletal progenitors [12]. Also, most of the results of ATDC5 cells in the current study were verified in primary periskeletal progenitors.

      References

      (1) Adoro S, Park KH, Bettigole SE, Lis R, Shin HR, Seo H, et al. Post-translational control of T cell development by the ESCRT protein CHMP5. Nat Immunol. 2017;18(7):780-90. doi: 10.1038/ni.3764. PubMed PMID: 28553951.

      (2) Kassem M, Bianco P. Skeletal stem cells in space and time. Cell. 2015;160(1-2):17-9. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.12.034. PubMed PMID: 25594172.

      (3) Chan CKF, Gulati GS, Sinha R, Tompkins JV, Lopez M, Carter AC, et al. Identification of the Human Skeletal Stem Cell. Cell. 2018;175(1):43-56 e21. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2018.07.029. PubMed PMID: 30241615.

      (4) Debnath S, Yallowitz AR, McCormick J, Lalani S, Zhang T, Xu R, et al. Discovery of a periosteal stem cell mediating intramembranous bone formation. Nature. 2018;562(7725):133-9. Epub 20180924. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0554-8. PubMed PMID: 30250253; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC6193396.

      (5) Mizuhashi K, Ono W, Matsushita Y, Sakagami N, Takahashi A, Saunders TL, et al. Resting zone of the growth plate houses a unique class of skeletal stem cells. Nature. 2018;563(7730):254-8. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0662-5. PubMed PMID: 30401834; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC6251707.

      (6) Zhang F, Wang Y, Zhao Y, Wang M, Zhou B, Zhou B, et al. NFATc1 marks articular cartilage progenitors and negatively determines articular chondrocyte differentiation. Elife. 2023;12. Epub 20230215. doi: 10.7554/eLife.81569. PubMed PMID: 36790146; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC10076019.

      (7) Dai GC, Wang H, Ming Z, Lu PP, Li YJ, Gao YC, et al. Heterotopic mineralization (ossification or calcification) in aged musculoskeletal soft tissues: A new candidate marker for aging. Ageing Res Rev. 2024;95:102215. Epub 20240205. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2024.102215. PubMed PMID: 38325754.

      (8) Mohler ER, 3rd, Adam LP, McClelland P, Graham L, Hathaway DR. Detection of osteopontin in calcified human aortic valves. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol. 1997;17(3):547-52. doi: 10.1161/01.atv.17.3.547. PubMed PMID: 9102175.

      (9) Mohler ER, 3rd, Gannon F, Reynolds C, Zimmerman R, Keane MG, Kaplan FS. Bone formation and inflammation in cardiac valves. Circulation. 2001;103(11):1522-8. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.103.11.1522. PubMed PMID: 11257079.

      (10) Paramos-de-Carvalho D, Jacinto A, Saude L. The right time for senescence. Elife. 2021;10. Epub 2021/11/11. doi: 10.7554/eLife.72449. PubMed PMID: 34756162; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC8580479.

      (11) Wiley CD, Campisi J. The metabolic roots of senescence: mechanisms and opportunities for intervention. Nat Metab. 2021;3(10):1290-301. Epub 2021/10/20. doi: 10.1038/s42255-021-00483-8. PubMed PMID: 34663974; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC8889622.

      (12) Ge X, Tsang K, He L, Garcia RA, Ermann J, Mizoguchi F, et al. NFAT restricts osteochondroma formation from entheseal progenitors. JCI Insight. 2016;1(4):e86254. doi: 10.1172/jci.insight.86254. PubMed PMID: 27158674; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4855520.

      (13) Greenblatt MB, Park KH, Oh H, Kim JM, Shin DY, Lee JM, et al. CHMP5 controls bone turnover rates by dampening NF-kappaB activity in osteoclasts. J Exp Med. 2015;212(8):1283-301. Epub 20150720. doi: 10.1084/jem.20150407. PubMed PMID: 26195726; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC4516796.

      (14) Rodger C, Flex E, Allison RJ, Sanchis-Juan A, Hasenahuer MA, Cecchetti S, et al. De Novo VPS4A Mutations Cause Multisystem Disease with Abnormal Neurodevelopment. Am J Hum Genet. 2020;107(6):1129-48. Epub 20201112. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2020.10.012. PubMed PMID: 33186545; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC7820634.

    1. Author response:

      eLife Assessment

      This manuscript introduces a useful protein-stability-based fitness model for simulating protein evolution and unifying non-neutral models of molecular evolution with phylogenetic models. The model is applied to four viral proteins that are of structural and functional importance. The justification of some hypotheses regarding fitness is incomplete, as well as the evidence for the model's predictive power, since it shows little improvement over neutral models in predicting protein evolution.

      We thank for the constructive comments that helped improve our study. Regarding the comment about justification of fitness, we will include in the revised manuscript additional information to support the relevance of modeling protein evolution accounting for protein folding stability. We agree that increasing the parameterization of the developed birth-death model is interesting, if it does not lead to overfitting. The model presented considers the fitness of protein variants to determine their reproductive success through the corresponding birth and death rates, varying among lineages, and it is biologically meaningful and technically correct (Harmon 2019). Following a suggestion of the first reviewer to allow variation of the global birth-death rate among lineages, we will additionally incorporate this aspect into the model and evaluate its performance with the data for the evaluation of the models. The integration of structurally constrained substitution models of protein evolution, as Markov models, into the birth-death process was made following standards approaches of molecular evolution in population genetics (Yang 2006; Carvajal-Rodriguez 2010; Arenas 2012; Hoban, et al. 2012) and we will provide more information about it in the revised manuscript. Regarding the predictive power, our study showed good accuracy in predicting the real folding stability of forecasted protein variants. On the other hand, predicting the exact sequences proved to be more challenging, indicating needs in the field of substitution models of molecular evolution. Altogether, we believe our findings provide a significant contribution to the field, as accurately forecasting the folding stability of future real proteins is fundamental for predicting their protein function and enabling a variety of applications. Additionally, we implemented the models into a freely available computer framework, with detailed documentation and diverse practical examples.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Ferreiro et al. present a method to simulate protein sequence evolution under a birth-death model where sequence evolution is constrained by structural constraints on protein stability. The authors then use this model to explore the predictability of sequence evolution in several viral structural proteins. In principle, this work is of great interest to molecular evolution and phylodynamics, which have struggled to couple non-neutral models of sequence evolution to phylodynamic models like birth-death. Unfortunately, though, the model shows little improvement over neutral models in predicting protein evolution, and this ultimately appears to be due to fundamental conceptual problems with how fitness is modeled and linked to the phylodynamic birth-death model.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments about our work.

      Regarding predictive power, the study showed a good accuracy in predicting the real folding stability of forecasted protein variants under a selection model, but not under a neutral model. However, predicting the exact sequences was more challenging. For example, amino acids with similar physicochemical properties can result in similar folding stability while differ in the specific sequence, more accurate substitution models of molecular evolution are required in the field. We consider that forecasting the folding stability of future real proteins is an important advancement in forecasting protein evolution, given the essential role of folding stability in protein function and its variety of applications. Regarding the conceptual concerns related to fitness modeling, we clarify this issue in detail in our responses to the specific comments below.

      Major concerns:

      (1) Fitness model: All lineages have the same growth rate r = b-d because the authors assume b+d=1. But under a birth-death model, the growth r is equivalent to fitness, so this is essentially assuming all lineages have the same absolute fitness since increases in reproductive fitness (b) will simply trade off with decreases in survival (d). Thus, even if the SCS model constrains sequence evolution, the birth-death model does not really allow for non-neutral evolution such that mutations can feed back and alter the structure of the phylogeny.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment that aims to improve the realism of our model. In the model presented (but see later for another model derived from the proposal of the reviewer and that we are now implementing into the framework and applying to the data used for the evaluation of the models), the fitness predicted from a protein variant is used to obtain the corresponding birth rate of that variant. In this way, protein variants with high fitness have high birth rates leading to overall more birth events, while protein variants with low fitness have low birth rates resulting in overall more extinction events, which has biological meaning for the study system. The statement “All lineages have the same growth rate r = b-d” in our model is incorrect because, in our model, b and d can vary among lineages according to the fitness. For example, a lineage might have b=0.9, d=0.1, r=0.8, while another lineage could have b=0.6, d=0.4, r=0.2. Indeed, the statement “this is essentially assuming all lineages have the same absolute fitness” is incorrect. Clearly, assuming that all lineages have the same fitness would not make sense, in that situation the folding stability of the forecasted protein variants would be similar under any model, which is not the case as shown in the results. In our model, the fitness affects the reproductive success, where protein variants with a high fitness have higher birth rates leading to more birth events, while those with lower fitness have higher death rates leading to more extinction events. This parameterization is meaningful for protein evolution because the fitness of a protein variant can affect its survival (birth or extinction) without necessarily affecting its rate of evolution. While faster growth rate can sometimes be associated with higher fitness, a variant with high fitness does not necessarily accumulate substitutions at a faster rate. Regarding the phylogenetic structure, the model presented considers variable birth and death events across different lineages according to the fitness of the corresponding protein variants, and this alters the derived phylogeny (i.e., protein variants selected against can go extinct while others with high fitness can produce descendants). We are not sure about the meaning of the term “mutations can feed back” in the context of our system. Note that we use Markov models of evolution, which are well-stablished in the field (despite their limitations), and substitutions are fixed mutations, which still could be reverted later if selected by the substitution model (Yang 2006). Altogether, we find that the presented birth-death model is technically correct and appropriate for modeling our biological system. Its integration with structurally constrained substitution (SCS) models of protein evolution, as Markov models, is correct following general approaches of molecular evolution in population genetics (Yang 2006; Carvajal-Rodriguez 2010; Arenas 2012; Hoban, et al. 2012). We will provide a more detailed description of the model in the revised manuscript.

      Apart from these clarifications about the birth-death model used, we understand the point of the reviewer and following the suggestion we are now incorporating an additional birth-death model that accounts for variable global birth-death rate among lineages. Specifically, we are following the model proposed by Neher et al (2014), where the death rate is considered as 1 and the birth rate is modeled as 1 + fitness. In this model, the global birth-death rate varies among lineages. We are now implementing this model into the computer framework and applying it to the data used for the evaluation of the models. Preliminary results, which will be finally presented in the revised manuscript, indicate that this model yields similar predictive accuracy compared to the previous birth-death model. If this is confirmed, accounting for variability in the global birth-death rate does not appear to play a major role in the studied systems of protein evolution. We will present this additional birth-death model and its results in the revised manuscript.

      (2) Predictive performance: Similar performance in predicting amino acid frequencies is observed under both the SCS model and the neutral model. I suspect that this rather disappointing result owes to the fact that the absolute fitness of different viral variants could not actually change during the simulations (see comment #1).

      The study shows similar performance in predicting the sequences of the forecasted proteins under both the SCS model and the neutral model, but shows differences in predicting the folding stability of the forecasted proteins between these models. Indeed, as explained in the previous answer, the birth-death model accounts for variation in fitness among lineages, leading to differences among lineages in reproductive success. The new birth-death model that we are now implementing, which incorporates variation of the global birth-death rate among lineages, is producing similar preliminary results. In addition to these considerations, it is known that SCS models applied to phylogenetics (such as ancestral molecular reconstruction) can model protein evolution with high accuracy in terms of folding stability. However, inferring sequences (i.e., ancestral sequences) is considerably more challenging even for ancestral molecular reconstruction (Arenas, et al. 2017; Arenas and Bastolla 2020). The observed sequence diversity is much greater than the observed structural diversity (Illergard, et al. 2009; Pascual-Garcia, et al. 2010), and substitutions among amino acids with similar physicochemical properties can result in protein variants with similar folding stability but different specific amino acid sequences; further work is demanded in the field of substitution models of molecular evolution. We will expand the discussion of this aspect in the revised manuscript.

      (3) Model assessment: It would be interesting to know how much the predictions were informed by the structurally constrained sequence evolution model versus the birth-death model. To explore this, the authors could consider three different models: 1) neutral, 2) SCS, and 3) SCS + BD. Simulations under the SCS model could be performed by simulating molecular evolution along just one hypothetical lineage. Seeing if the SCS + BD model improves over the SCS model alone would be another way of testing whether mutations could actually impact the evolutionary dynamics of lineages in the phylogeny.

      In the present study, we compare the neutral model + birth-death (BD) with the SCS model + BD. Markov substitution models Q are applied upon an evolutionary time (i.e., branch length, t) and this allows to determine the probability of substitution events during that time period [P(t) = exp (Qt)]. This approach is traditionally used in phylogenetics to model the incorporation of substitutions over time. Therefore, to compare the neutral and SCS models, an evolutionary time is required, in this case it is provided by the birth-death process. The suggestions 1) and 2) cannot be compared without an underlined evolutionary history. However, comparisons in terms of likelihood, and other aspects, between models that ignore the protein structure and the implemented SCS models are already available in our previous studies based on coalescent simulations or given phylogenetic trees (Arenas, et al. 2013; Arenas, et al. 2015). There, SCS models produced proteins with more realistic folding stability than models that ignore evolutionary constraints from the protein structure, and those findings are consistent with the results from the present study where we explore the application of these models to forecasting protein evolution. We would like to emphasize that forecasting the folding stability of future real proteins is a significant and novel finding, folding stability is fundamental to protein function and has diverse implications. While accurately forecasting the exact sequences would indeed be ideal, this remains a challenging task with current substitution models. In this regard, we will discuss in the revised manuscript the need of developing more accurate substitution models.

      (4) Background fitness effects: The model ignores background genetic variation in fitness. I think this is particularly important as the fitness effects of mutations in any one protein may be overshadowed by the fitness effects of mutations elsewhere in the genome. The model also ignores background changes in fitness due to the environment, but I acknowledge that might be beyond the scope of the current work.

      This comment made us realize that more information about the features of the implemented SCS models should be included in the manuscript. In particular, the implemented SCS models consider a negative design based on the observed residue contacts in nearly all proteins available in the Protein Data Bank (Arenas, et al. 2013; Arenas, et al. 2015). This data is provided as an input file and it can be updated to incorporate new structures (see the framework documentation and the practical examples). Therefore, the prediction of folding stability is a combination of positive design (direct analysis of the target protein) and negative design (consideration of background proteins to reduce biases), thus incorporating background molecular diversity. This important feature was not sufficiently described in the manuscript, and we will add more details in the revised version. Regarding the fitness caused by the environment, we agree with the reviewer. This is a challenge for any method aiming to forecast evolution, as future environmental shifts are inherently unpredictable and may impact the accuracy of the predictions. Although one might attempt to incorporate such effects into the model, doing so risks overparameterization, especially when the additional factors are uncertain or speculative. We will include a discussion in the revised manuscript about our perspective on the potential effects of environmental changes on forecasting evolution.

      (5) In contrast to the model explored here, recent work on multi-type birth-death processes has considered models where lineages have type-specific birth and/or death rates and therefore also type-specific growth rates and fitness (Stadler and Bonhoeffer, 2013; Kunhert et al., 2017; Barido-Sottani, 2023). Rasmussen & Stadler (eLife, 2019) even consider a multi-type birth-death model where the fitness effects of multiple mutations in a protein or viral genome collectively determine the overall fitness of a lineage. The key difference with this work presented here is that these models allow lineages to have different growth rates and fitness, so these models truly allow for non-neutral evolutionary dynamics. It would appear the authors might need to adopt a similar approach to successfully predict protein evolution.

      We agree with the reviewer that robust birth-death models have been developed applying statistics and, in many cases, the primary aim of those studies is the development and refinement of the model itself. Regarding the study by Rasmussen and Stadler 2019, it incorporates an external evaluation of mutation events where the used fitness is specific for the proteins investigated in that study, which may pose challenges for users interested in analyzing other proteins. In contrast, our study takes a different approach. We implement a fitness function that can be predicted and evaluated for any type of protein (Goldstein 2013), making it broadly applicable. In addition, we provide a freely available and well-documented computational framework to facilitate its use. The primary aim of our study is not the development of novel or complex birth-death models. Rather, we aim to explore the integration of a standard birth-death model with structurally constrained substitution models for the purpose of predicting protein evolution. In the context of protein evolution, substitution models are a critical factor (Liberles, et al. 2012; Wilke 2012; Bordner and Mittelmann 2013; Echave, et al. 2016; Arenas, et al. 2017; Echave and Wilke 2017), and their combination with a birth-death model constitutes a first approximation upon which next studies can build to better understand this biological system. We will include these considerations in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, "Forecasting protein evolution by integrating birth-death population models with structurally constrained substitution models", David Ferreiro and co-authors present a forward-in-time evolutionary simulation framework that integrates a birth-death population model with a fitness function based on protein folding stability. By incorporating structurally constrained substitution models and estimating fitness from ΔG values using homology-modeled structures, the authors aim to capture biophysically realistic evolutionary dynamics. The approach is implemented in a new version of their open-source software, ProteinEvolver2, and is applied to four viral proteins from HIV-1 and SARS-CoV-2.

      Overall, the study presents a compelling rationale for using folding stability as a constraint in evolutionary simulations and offers a novel framework and software to explore such dynamics. While the results are promising, particularly for predicting biophysical properties, the current analysis provides only partial evidence for true evolutionary forecasting, especially at the sequence level. The work offers a meaningful conceptual advance and a useful simulation tool, and sets the stage for more extensive validation in future studies.

      We also thank this reviewer for the positive comments on our study. Regarding the predictive power, our results showed good accuracy in predicting the folding stability of the forecasted protein variants. However, predicting the specific sequences of these variants is more challenging. For example, forecasting in amino acids with similar physicochemical properties can result in different sequences but in similar folding stability. We believe that these findings are realistic and interesting as they indicate that while forecasting folding stability is feasible, forecasting the specific sequence evolution is more complex that one could anticipate.

      Strengths:

      The results demonstrate that fitness constraints based on protein stability can prevent the emergence of unrealistic, destabilized variants - a limitation of traditional, neutral substitution models. In particular, the predicted folding stabilities of simulated protein variants closely match those observed in real variants, suggesting that the model captures relevant biophysical constraints.

      We agree with the reviewer and appreciate the consideration that forecasting the folding stability of future real proteins is a relevant finding. For instance, folding stability is fundamental for protein function and affects several other molecular properties.

      Weaknesses:

      The predictive scope of the method remains limited. While the model effectively preserves folding stability, its ability to forecast specific sequence content is not well supported.

      It is known that structurally constrained substitution (SCS) models applied to phylogenetics (such as ancestral molecular reconstruction) can model protein evolution with high accuracy in terms of folding stability, while inferring sequences (i.e., ancestral sequences) remains considerably more challenging (Arenas, et al. 2017; Arenas and Bastolla 2020). The observed sequence diversity is much higher than the observed structural diversity (Illergard, et al. 2009; Pascual-Garcia, et al. 2010), and substitutions between amino acids with similar physicochemical properties can result in protein variants with similar folding stability but with different specific amino acid composition. We will expand the discussion of this aspect in the manuscript.

      Only one dataset (HIV-1 MA) is evaluated for sequence-level divergence using KL divergence; this analysis is absent for the other proteins. The authors use a consensus Omicron sequence as a representative endpoint for SARS-CoV-2, which overlooks the rich longitudinal sequence data available from GISAID. The use of just one consensus from a single time point is not fully justified, given the extensive temporal and geographical sampling available. Extending the analysis to include multiple timepoints, particularly for SARS-CoV-2, would strengthen the predictive claims. Similarly, applying the model to other well-sampled viral proteins, such as those from influenza or RSV, would broaden its relevance and test its generalizability.

      The evaluation of forecasting evolution using real datasets is complex due to several conceptual and practical aspects. In contrast to traditional phylogenetic reconstruction of past evolutionary events and ancestral sequences, forecasting evolution often begins with a variant that is evolved forward in time and requires a rough fitness landscape to select among possible future variants (Lässig, et al. 2017). Another concern for validating the method is the need to know the initial variant that gives rise to the corresponding forecasted variants, and it is not always known. Thus, we investigated systems where the initial variant, or a close approximation, is known, such as scenarios of in vitro monitored evolution. In the case of SARS-CoV-2, the Wuhan variant is commonly used as the starting variant of the pandemic. Next, since forecasting evolution is highly dependent on the used model of evolution, unexpected external factors can be dramatic for the predictions. For this reason, systems with minimal external influences provide a more controlled context for evaluating forecasting evolution. For instance, scenarios of in vitro monitored virus evolution avoid some external factors such as host immune response. Another important aspect is the availability of data at two (i.e., present and future) or more time points along the evolutionary trajectory, with sufficient genetic divergence between them to identify clear evolutionary signatures. Additionally, using consensus sequences can help mitigate effects from unfixed mutations, which should not be modeled by a substitution model of evolution. Altogether, not all datasets are appropriate to properly evaluate forecasting evolution. We will include these considerations in the revised manuscript.

      Sequence comparisons based on the KL divergence require, at the studied time point, an observed distribution of amino acid frequencies among sites and an estimated distribution of amino acid frequencies among sites. In the study datasets, this is only the case for the HIV-1 MA dataset, which belongs to a previous study from one of us and collaborators where we obtained at least 20 independent sequences at each sampling point (Arenas, et al. 2016). We will provide additional information on this aspect in the manuscript.

      Regarding the Omicron dataset, we used 384 curated sequences of the Omicron variant of concern to construct the study dataset and we believe that it is a representative sample. The sequence used for the initial time point was the Wuhan variant (Wu, et al. 2020), which is commonly assumed to be the origin of the pandemic in SARS-CoV-2 studies. As previously indicated, the use of consensus sequences is convenient to avoid variants with unfixed mutations. Regarding extending the analysis to other timepoints (other variants of concern), we kindly disagree because Omicron is the variant of concern with the highest genetic distance to the Wuhan variant, and a high genetic distance is required to properly evaluate the prediction method. We noted that earlier variants of concern show a small number of fixed mutations in the study proteins, despite the availability of large numbers of sequences in databases such as GISAID.

      Additionally, we investigated the evolutionary trajectories of HIV-1 protease (PR) in 12 intra-host viral populations.

      Next, following the proposal of the reviewer, we will incorporate the analysis of an additional viral dataset (probably influenza following the suggestion of the reviewer) to further assess the generalizability of the method. Still, as previously indicated, not all datasets are suitable for a proper evaluation of forecasting evolution. Factors such as the shape of the fitness landscape and the amount of genetic variation over time can influence the accuracy of predictions. We will present the results of the analysis of the new data in the revised manuscript.

      It would also be informative to include a retrospective analysis of the evolution of protein stability along known historical trajectories. This would allow the authors to assess whether folding stability is indeed preserved in real-world evolution, as assumed in their model.

      Our present study is not focused on investigating the evolution of the folding stability over time, although it provides this information indirectly at the studied time points. Instead, the present study shows that the folding stability of the forecasted protein variants is similar to the folding stability of the corresponding real protein variants for diverse viral proteins, which is an important evaluation of the method. Next, the folding stability can indeed vary over time in both real and modeled evolutionary scenarios, and our present study is not in conflict with this. In that regard, which is not the aim of our present study, some previous phylogenetic-based studies have reported temporal fluctuations in folding stability for diverse data (Arenas, et al. 2017; Olabode, et al. 2017; Arenas and Bastolla 2020; Ferreiro, et al. 2022).

      Finally, a discussion on the impact of structural templates - and whether the fixed template remains valid across divergent sequences - would be valuable. Addressing the possibility of structural remodeling or template switching during evolution would improve confidence in the model's applicability to more divergent evolutionary scenarios.

      This is an important point. For the datasets that required homology modeling (in several cases it was not necessary because the sequence was present in a protein structure of the PDB), the structural templates were selected using SWISS-MODEL, and we applied the best-fitting template. We will include additional details about the parameters of the homology modeling in the revised version. Indeed, our method assumes that the protein structure is maintained over the studied evolutionary time, which can be generally reasonable for short timescales where the structure is conserved (Illergard, et al. 2009; Pascual-Garcia, et al. 2010). Over longer evolutionary timescales, structural changes may occur, and in such cases, modeling the evolution of the protein structure would be necessary. To our knowledge, modeling the evolution of the protein structure remains a challenging task that requires substantial methodological developments. Recent advances in artificial intelligence, particularly in protein structure prediction from sequence, may offer promising tools for addressing this challenge. However, we believe that evaluating such approaches in the context of structural evolution would be difficult, especially given the limited availability of real data with known evolutionary trajectories involving structural change. In any case, this is probably an important direction for future research. We will include this discussion in the revised manuscript.

      Cited references

      Arenas M. 2012. Simulation of Molecular Data under Diverse Evolutionary Scenarios. PLoS Comput Biol 8:e1002495.

      Arenas M, Bastolla U. 2020. ProtASR2: Ancestral reconstruction of protein sequences accounting for folding stability. Methods Ecol Evol 11:248-257.

      Arenas M, Dos Santos HG, Posada D, Bastolla U. 2013. Protein evolution along phylogenetic histories under structurally constrained substitution models. Bioinformatics 29:3020-3028.

      Arenas M, Lorenzo-Redondo R, Lopez-Galindez C. 2016. Influence of mutation and recombination on HIV-1 in vitro fitness recovery. Molecular Phylogenetics and Evolution 94:264-270.

      Arenas M, Sanchez-Cobos A, Bastolla U. 2015. Maximum likelihood phylogenetic inference with selection on protein folding stability. Molecular Biology and Evolution 32:2195-2207.

      Arenas M, Weber CC, Liberles DA, Bastolla U. 2017. ProtASR: An Evolutionary Framework for Ancestral Protein Reconstruction with Selection on Folding Stability. Systematic Biology 66:1054-1064.

      Bordner AJ, Mittelmann HD. 2013. A new formulation of protein evolutionary models that account for structural constraints. Molecular Biology and Evolution 31:736-749.

      Carvajal-Rodriguez A. 2010. Simulation of genes and genomes forward in time. Current Genomics 11:58-61.

      Echave J, Spielman SJ, Wilke CO. 2016. Causes of evolutionary rate variation among protein sites. Nature Reviews Genetics 17:109-121.

      Echave J, Wilke CO. 2017. Biophysical Models of Protein Evolution: Understanding the Patterns of Evolutionary Sequence Divergence. Annu Rev Biophys 46:85-103.

      Ferreiro D, Khalil R, Gallego MJ, Osorio NS, Arenas M. 2022. The evolution of the HIV-1 protease folding stability. Virus Evol 8:veac115.

      Goldstein RA. 2013. Population Size Dependence of Fitness Effect Distribution and Substitution Rate Probed by Biophysical Model of Protein Thermostability. Genome Biol Evol 5:1584-1593.

      Harmon LJ. 2019. Introduction to birth-death models. In. Phylogenetic Comparative Methods. p. https://lukejharmon.github.io/pcm/chapter10_birthdeath/.

      Hoban S, Bertorelle G, Gaggiotti OE. 2012. Computer simulations: tools for population and evolutionary genetics. Nature Reviews Genetics 13:110-122.

      Illergard K, Ardell DH, Elofsson A. 2009. Structure is three to ten times more conserved than sequence--a study of structural response in protein cores. Proteins 77:499-508.

      Lässig M, Mustonen V, Walczak AM. 2017. Predicting evolution. Nature Ecology & Evolution 1:0077.

      Liberles DA, Teichmann SA, Bahar I, Bastolla U, Bloom J, Bornberg-Bauer E, Colwell LJ, de Koning AP, Dokholyan NV, Echave J, et al. 2012. The interface of protein structure, protein biophysics, and molecular evolution. Protein Science 21:769-785.

      Neher RA, Russell CA, Shraiman BI. 2014. Predicting evolution from the shape of genealogical trees. Elife 3.

      Olabode AS, Kandathil SM, Lovell SC, Robertson DL. 2017. Adaptive HIV-1 evolutionary trajectories are constrained by protein stability. Virus Evol 3:vex019.

      Pascual-Garcia A, Abia D, Mendez R, Nido GS, Bastolla U. 2010. Quantifying the evolutionary divergence of protein structures: the role of function change and function conservation. Proteins 78:181-196.

      Wilke CO. 2012. Bringing molecules back into molecular evolution. PLoS Comput Biol 8:e1002572.

      Wu F, Zhao S, Yu B, Chen YM, Wang W, Song ZG, Hu Y, Tao ZW, Tian JH, Pei YY, et al. 2020. A new coronavirus associated with human respiratory disease in China. Nature 579:265-269.

      Yang Z. 2006. Computational Molecular Evolution. Oxford, England.: Oxford University Press.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) The broader significance of the findings needs to be better articulated. While the authors emphasize that comparing adaptive traits in sympatry and allopatry provides insights into selective processes shaping reproductive isolation and coexistence, it is unclear what key conceptual or theoretical questions are being addressed. Are these patterns expected under certain evolutionary scenarios? Have they been empirically demonstrated in other systems? The authors should explicitly state the overarching research question, incorporate some predictions, and better contextualize their findings within the existing literature. If the results challenge or support previous work, that should be highlighted to strengthen the study's importance in a broader context.

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable feedback. We understand that the framing of the results and the discussion did not allow to highlight the broader significance of our findings. In the revised version of the manuscript, we will explicitly mention the theoretical questions asked and our hypotheses in the introduction, and better compare our results to pre-existing examples from the literature.

      (2) The motivation for studying visual signals and mate choice in allopatric populations (i.e., at the intraspecific level) is not well articulated, leaving their role in the broader narrative unclear. In particular, the rationale behind experiments 1, 2, and 3 is not well defined, as the authors have not made a strong case for the need for these intraspecific comparisons in the introduction. This issue is further compounded by the authors' primary focus on signal evolution in sympatry throughout both the results and the discussion. For instance, the divergence of iridescence in allopatry is a potentially interesting result. But the authors have not discussed its implications.

      Overall, given that the primary conclusions are based on results and analyses in sympatry, the role of allopatric populations in shaping these conclusions needs to be better integrated and justified.

      Without a stronger link between the comparative framework and the study's key takeaways, the use of allopatric populations feels somewhat peripheral rather than central to the study's aim.

      Since the primary conclusions remain valid even without the allopatric comparisons, their inclusion requires a clearer rationale.

      We recognize that the current manuscript places more emphasis on the sympatric Morpho population, and that the analysis and the discussion of the results regarding the allopatric Morpho population were underdeveloped. In the revised version, we plan to address this by (1) developing the rationale behind the male choice experiments performed on the allopatric population. We will argue that intraspecific comparison helps identify the traits involved in mate preference within species (iridescent color and/or wing pattern) and that those results can be compared to the interspecific mate choice results to identify the traits involved in species recognition. To explain the relevance of the comparison with the allopatric population, we will also (2) strengthen expectations on the effect of species interactions on the evolution of traits and mate recognition in sympatric populations vs. allopatric populations.

      (3) While the authors demonstrate that iridescence is indistinguishable to predators in sympatry, they overstate the role of predation in driving convergence. The present study does not experimentally demonstrate that iridescence in this species has a confusion effect or contributes to evasive mimicry. Alternatively, convergence could result from other selective forces, such as signal efficacy due to environmental conditions, rather than being solely driven by predation.

      We acknowledge that this study neither demonstrates that iridescence contributes to evasive mimicry nor that predation is the driver of the convergence in iridescence. We will tone down the interpretation of the results in the discussion and state that predation is not the only selective pressure that could have promoted a convergent evolution of iridescence in sympatric species, although this observation is consistent with the evasive mimicry hypothesis.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      My only major comment concerns the authors' favoured explanation for aposematism (or evasive mimicry) for convergence among species, which is based upon the you-can't-catch-me hypothesis first presented by Young 1971. Although there is supporting work showing that iridescent-like stimuli are more difficult to precisely localize by a range of viewers, most of the evidence as applied to the Morpho system is circumstantial, and I'm not certain that there is widespread acceptance of this hypothesis. Given that the present study deals with closely-related (sub)species, one alternative explanation - a "null" hypothesis of sorts - is for a lack of divergence (from a common starting point) as opposed to evolutionary convergence per se. in other words, two subspecies are likely to retain ancestral character states unless there is selection that causes them to diverge. I feel that the manuscript would benefit from a discussion of this alternative, if not others. Signalling to predators could very well be involved in constraining the extent of convergence, but this seems a little premature to state as an up-front conclusion of this work. There is also the result of a *dorsal* wing manipulation by Vieira-Silva et al. 2024 (https://doi.org/10.1111/eth.13517), which seems difficult to reconcile in light of this explanation. Whereas this paper is cited by the authors, a more nuanced discussion of their experimental results would seem appropriate here.

      We thank the reviewer for their constructive comments on our manuscript. We appreciate the reviewer’s concern regarding the way iridescence convergence between sympatric species is discussed in our manuscript, which aligns with similar concerns raised by Reviewer 1. We will improve the discussion on the different evolutionary forces that could have favored this convergent iridescent signal in sympatry to bring more nuance to the discussion.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      First, when using allopatric and sympatric (sub)species pairs to test evolutionary hypotheses, replication is important. Ideally, multiple allopatric and sympatric (sub)species pairs are compared to avoid outlier (sub)species or pairs that lead to biased conclusions. Unfortunately, the current study compares 1 allopatric and 1 sympatric (sub)species pair, hence having poor (no) replication on the level of allopatric and sympatric (sub)species pairs.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for their constructive feedbacks. We agree that replication is important to test evolutionary hypotheses and that our study lacks replication for allopatric and sympatric Morpho populations. Ideally, one would require several allopatric and sympatric replicates pointing respectively toward divergence and convergence of Morpho iridescence to conclude on the effect of species interaction in trait evolution. Our study is a first attempt at answering this question, covering few Morpho populations but proposing a broad assessment of iridescence and mate preference for those populations. We will make sure to mention this limitation more clearly in the revised version of our manuscript.

      Second, chemical profiles were only measured for sympatric species and not for allopatric (sub)species, which limits the interpretation of this data. The allopatric (sub)species could have been measured as non-coexistence "control". If coexistence and convergence in wing colouration drives the evolution of alternative mate recognition signals, such alternative signals should not evolve/diverge for allopatric (sub)species where wing colouration is still a reliable mate recognition cue. More importantly, no details are provided on the quantification of butterfly chemical profiles, which is essential to understand such data. It is unclear how the chemical profiles were quantified and what data (concentrations, ratios, proportions) were used to perform NDMS and generate Figure 5 and the associated statistical tests.

      We recognize that having the chemical profiles of the genitalia of the Morpho from the allopatric population would have made a stronger case arguing in favor of reinforcement acting on the divergence of the chemical compounds found on the genitalia of the sympatric Morpho species. Due to limited access to the biological material needed by the time of the chromatography, we could not test for lower divergence in the chemical profiles of allopatric Morpho butterflies. We will mention this limitation in the results, and clarify the protocol used to extract the chemical profiles, by mentioning the use of concentration data to generate Figure 5 and the associated statistical tests.

      Third, throughout the discussion, the authors mention that their results support natural selection by predators on iridescent wing colouration, without measuring natural selection by predators or any other measure related to predation. It is unclear by what predators any of the butterfly species are predated on at this point.

      We will mention in the next version of the manuscript previous predation experiments performed on Morpho and other butterflies showing evidence that birds can be predators for those species. Those observations lead us to test for the putative effect of predation on the evolution of their color pattern, without directly testing predatory rates. We will make sure this information is transparent in the revised manuscript.

      To continue on the interpretation of the data related to selection on specific traits by specific selection agents: This study did not measure any form of selection or any selection agent. Hence, it is not known if iridescent wing colouration is actually under selection by predators and/or mates, if maybe other selection agents are involved or if these traits converge due to genetic correlations with other traits under selection. For example, Iridescent colouration in ground beetles has functions as antipredator defence but also thermo- and water regulation. None of these issues are recognized or discussed.

      We acknowledge that the lack of discussion on alternative evolutionary forces involved in the evolution of iridescence has been highlighted by all reviewers. We will discuss how environmental factors, genetic factors or the correlation with others traits as explanatory variables might explain the convergent signal of iridescence found in sympatric Morpho species, and not only focus on the putative effect of predation.

      Finally, some of the results are weakly supported by statistics or questionable methodology. Most notably, the perception of the iridescence coloration of allopatric subspecies by bird visual systems. Although for females, means and errors (not indicated what exactly, SD, SE or CI) are clearly above the 1 JND line, for males, means are only slightly above this line and errors or CIs clearly overlap with the 1 JND line. Since there is no additional statistical support, higher means but overlap of SD, SE or CI with the baseline provides weak statistical support for differences.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing interpretation issues concerning the chromatic distances of allopatric Morpho species measured with a bird vision model. We will make sure to bring nuance to the interpretation of this graph, and clearly mention in the figure’s legend that the error bars represent the confidence intervals obtained after performing a bootstrap analysis.

      Regarding the assortative mating experiment, the results are clearly driven by M. bristowi. For M. theodorus, females mate equally often with conspecifics (6 times) as with M. bristowi (5 times). For males, the ratio is slightly better (6 vs 3), but with such low numbers, I doubt this is statistically testable. Overall low mating for M. bristowi could indicate suboptimal experimental conditions, and hence results should be interpreted with care.

      Regarding the wing manipulation experiment, M. theodorus does not show a preference when dummies with non-modified wings are presented and prefers non-modified dummies over modified dummies. This is acknowledged by the authors but not further discussed. Certainly, some control treatment for wing modification could have been added.

      We recognize that the tetrad experiment results are mainly driven by M. bristowi’s behavior. This experiment would have benefited from more replicates. We will mention that the conclusions we draw for this experiment are mainly driven by male M. bristowi behavior, and that it is more difficult to test for assortative or disassortative mating in M. theodorus, adding more nuance to our interpretation. We will also make sure to discuss further the effect of wing modification in the discussion.

      Overall, the fact that certain measurements only provide evidence for 1 of the 2 (sub)species (assortative mating, wing manipulation) or one sex of one of the species (bird visual systems) means overall interpretation and overgeneralization of the results to both allopatric or sympatric species should be done with care, and such nuances should ideally be discussed.

      The aim of the authors, "to investigate the antagonistic effects of selective pressures generated by mate recognition and shared predation" has not been achieved, and the conclusions regarding this aim are not supported by the results. Nevertheless, the iridescence colour measurements are solid, and some of the behavioural experiments and chemical profile measurements seem to yield interesting results. The study would benefit from less overinterpretation of the results in the framework of predation and more careful consideration of methodological difficulties, statistical insecurities, and nuances in the results.

      Overall, we would like to thank all reviewers for their thorough assessment of our work. We understand that the imbalance between mate choice data, visual model data and chemical data only give us a partial assessment of species recognition in Morpho butterflies, thus requiring more precision in the interpretation and the discussion of our results. We will implement all the comments made by the reviewers in the next version of our manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this work, the authors have developed SPLASH+, a micro-assembly and biological interpretation framework that expands on their previously published reference-free statistical approach (SPLASH) for sequencing data analysis.

      Thank you for this thorough overview of our work.

      Strengths:

      (1) The methodology developed by the authors seems like a promising approach to overcome many of the challenges posed by reference-based single-cell RNA-seq analysis methods.

      Thank you for your positive comment on the potential of our approach to address the limitations of reference-based methods for scRNA-Seq analysis.

      (2) The analysis of the RNU6 repetitive small nuclear RNA provides a very compelling example of a type of transcript that is very challenging to analyze with standard reference-based methods (e.g., most reads from this gene fail to align with STAR, if I understood the result correctly).

      We thank the reviewer for their positive comment. We agree that the variation in RNU6 detected by SPLASH+ underscores the potential of our reference-free method to make discoveries in cases where reference-based approaches fall short.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The manuscript presents a number of case studies from very diverse domains of single-cell RNA-seq analysis. As a result, the manuscript has been challenging to review, because it requires domain expertise in centromere biology, RNA splicing, RNA editing, V(D)J transcript diversity, and repeat polymorphisms.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s effort in thoroughly evaluating this manuscript, especially given the broad range of biological domains discussed. Our main goal in presenting a wide range of applications was to highlight the key strength of the SPLASH+ framework: its ability to unify diverse biological discoveries within a single method that operates directly on sequencing reads.

      (2) Although the paper focuses on SmartSeq2 full-length single-cell RNA-seq data analysis, the vast majority of single-cell RNA-seq data that is currently being generated comes from droplet-based methods (e.g., 10x Genomics) that sequence only the 3' or 5' ends of transcripts. As a result, it is unclear if SPLASH+ is also applicable to these types of data.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. Due to the specific data format of barcoded single-cell sequencing platforms such as 10x Genomics, extending the SPLASH framework to support 10x analysis required engineering a specialized preprocessing tool. We have addressed this in a recent work, which is now available as a preprint (https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.12.24.630263).

      (3) The criteria used for the selection of the 10 'core genes' have not been sufficiently justified.

      We chose these genes as SPLASH+ detected regulated splicing for them in nearly all tissues (18 out of 19)  analyzed in our study (i.e., identifying anchors classified as splicing anchors in those tissues). Our subsequent analysis showed that all these genes are involved in either splicing regulation or histone modification. We will further clarify this selection criterion in the revision. 

      (4) It is currently unclear how the splicing diversity discovered in this paper relates to the concept of noisy splicing (i.e., there are likely many low-frequency transcripts and splice junctions that are unlikely to have a significant functional impact beyond triggering nonsense-mediated decay).

      In our analysis, to ensure sufficient read coverage, we considered significant anchors supported by more than 50 reads and detected in over 10 cells. Additionally, our downstream analyses (including splicing analysis) are based on assembled sequences (compactors) generated through our micro-assembly step. This process effectively acts as a denoising step by filtering out sequences likely caused by sequencing errors or with very low read support. However, we agree that the detected splice variants have not been fully functionally characterized, and further functional experiments may be needed.

      (5) The paper presents only a very superficial discussion of the potential weaknesses of the SPLASH+ method.

      We discussed two potential limitations of SPLASH+ in the Conclusions section: (1) it is not suitable for differential gene expression analysis, and (2) although we provide a framework for interpreting and analyzing SPLASH results, further work is still needed to improve the annotation of calls lacking BLAST matches. We will add more discussion for these in the revision. 

      (6) The cursory mention of metatranscriptome in the conclusion of the paper is confusing, as it might suggest the presence of microbial cells in sterile human tissues (which has recently been discredited in cancer, see e.g. https://www.science.org/content/article/journal-retracts-influential-cancer-microbiome-paper).

      We will remove the mention of metatranscriptome in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The authors extend their SPLASH framework with single-cell RNA-seq in mind, in two ways. First, they introduce "compactors", which are possible paths branching out from an anchor. Second, they introduce a workflow to classify compactors according to the type of biological sequence variation represented (splicing, SNV, etc). They focus on simulated data for fusion detection, and then focus on analyzing the Tabula sapiens Smart-seq2 data, showing extensive results on alternative splicing analysis, VDJ, and repeat elements.

      This is strong work with an impressive array of biological investigations and results for a methods paper. I have various concerns about terminology and comparisons, as follows (in a somewhat arbitrary order, apologies).

      Thank you for this thorough overview of our work and your positive comment on the strength of our work.

      (1) The discussion of the weaknesses of the consensus sequence approach of SPLASH is an odd way to motivate SPLASH+ in my opinion, in that SPLASH is not yet so widely used, so the baseline for SPLASH+ is really standard alignment-based approaches. It is fine to mention consensus sequence issues briefly, but it felt belabored.

      We thank the reviewer and agree that the primary comparison for SPLASH+ is with reference-based methods. However, since SPLASH+ builds upon SPLASH, we also aimed to highlight the limitations of the consensus step in original SPLASH and how SPLASH+ addresses them. To maintain the main focus of the paper on comparison with reference-based methods and biological investigations, this discussion with consensus was provided in a Supplementary Figure. We will shorten this discussion in the revision.

      (2) Regarding compactors reducing alignment cost: the comparison should really be between compactor construction and alignment vs read alignment (and maybe vs modern contig construction algorithms and alignment).

      Since the SPLASH framework is fundamentally reference-free and does not require read alignment, we compared the number of sequence alignments for compactors to the total read alignments required by a reference-based method to show that while compactors are aligned to the reference, the number of alignments needed is still orders of magnitude less than a reference-based approach requiring alignment of all the reads.

      (3) The language around "compactors" is a bit confusing, where the authors sometimes refer to the tree of possibilities from an anchor as a "compactor", and sometimes a compactor is a single branch. Presumably, ideally, compactors should be DAGs, not trees, i.e., they can connect back together. Perhaps the authors could comment on whether this matters/would be a valuable extension.

      We thank the reviewer for their comment. We refer to each generated assembled sequence as “a compactor”, and we attempted to make this clear in the paper. We will review the text further to ensure this definition is clear in the revised version.

      (4) The main oddness of the splicing analysis to me is not using cell-type/state in any way in the statistical testing. This need not be discrete cell types: psiX, for example, tested whether exonic PSI was variable with reference to a continuous gene expression embedding. Intuitively, such transcriptome-wide signal should be valuable for a) improving power and b) distinguishing cell-type intrinsic/"noisy" from cell-type specific splicing variation. A straightforward way of doing this would be pseudobulking cell types. Possibly a more sophisticated hierarchical model could be constructed also.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concern regarding SPLASH+ not using cell type metadata. SPLASH, which performs the core statistical inference in SPLASH+, is an unsupervised tool specifically designed to make biological discoveries without relying on metadata (such as cell type annotations in scRNA-Seq). This is particularly useful in scRNA-seq, where cell type labels could be missing, imprecise, or may miss important within-cell-type variation. As shown in the paper, even without using metadata, SPLASH+ demonstrated improved performance than both SpliZ and Leafcutter (two metadata-dependent tools) in terms of achieving higher concordance and identifying more differentially spliced genes. Regarding pseudobulking, as has been shown in the SpliZ paper (https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-022-01400-x), pseudobulking requires multiple pseudobulked replicates per cell type for reliable inference, which is often not feasible in scRNA-seq settings, making such methods statistically suboptimal for single-cell studies. We will add a discussion on pseudobulking in the revision. 

      (5) A secondary weakness is that some informative reads will not be used, for example, unspliced reads aligning to an alterantive exons. This relates to the broader weakness of SPLASH that it is blind to changes in coverage that are not linked to a specific anchor (which should be acknowledged somewhere, maybe in the Discussion). In the deeply sequenced SS2 data, this is likely not an issue, but might be more limiting in sparser data. A related issue is that coverage change indicative of, e.g., alternative TSS or TES (that do not also include a change in splice junction use) will not be detected. In fairness, all these weaknesses are shared by LeafCutter. It would be valuable to have a comparison to a more "traditional" splicing analysis approach (pick your favorite of rMATS, MISO, SUPPA).

      We thank the reviewer for their comment. As noted in the Conclusion, the SPLASH framework is not designed for differential gene expression analysis, which relies on quantifying read coverage. Rather, it focuses on detecting differential sequence diversity arising from mechanisms like alternative splicing or RNA editing. We will clarify this limitation further in the revised Conclusion. 

      Regarding splicing evaluation, we have performed extensive comparisons with two widely used and recent methods—SpliZ and Leafcutter—for both bulk and single-cell splicing analysis. While we appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to include an additional method, given the current length of the paper and the fact that leafcutter has previously been shown to outperform rMATS, MAJIQ, and Cufflinks2

      (https://www.nature.com/articles/s41588-017-0004-9), we believe the current comparisons provide sufficient support for the evaluation of the splicing detection by SPLASH+.

      (6) "We should note that there is no difference between gene fusions and other RNA variants (e.g., RNA splicing) from a sequence assembly viewpoint". Maybe this is true in an abstract sense, but I don't think it is in reality. AS can produce hundreds of isoforms from the same gene, and be variable across individual cells. Gene fusions are generally less numerous/varied and will be shared across clonal populations, so the complexity is lower. That simplicity is balanced against the challenge that any genes could, in principle, fuse.

      We selected the fusion benchmarking dataset solely to evaluate how well compactors reconstruct sequences. Since our goal was to assess the accuracy of reconstructed compactor sequences, we needed a benchmarking dataset with ground truth sequences, which this dataset provides. We had explained our main reason and purpose for selecting fusion dataset in the text, but we will clarify it further in the revision.

      (7) For the fusion detection assessment, SPLASH+ is given the correct anchor for detection. This feels like cheating since this information wouldn't usually be available. Can the authors motivate this? Are the other methods given comparable information? Also, TPM>100 seems like a very high expression threshold for the assessment.

      We agree with the reviewer that the fusion benchmarking dataset should not be used to assess the entire SPLASH+ framework. In fact, we did not use this dataset to evaluate SPLASH+; it was used exclusively to evaluate the performance of compactors as a standalone module. Specifically, we tested how well compactors can reconstruct fusion sequences when provided with seed sequences corresponding to fusion junctions. This aligns with our expectation from compactors in SPLASH+, that they should correctly reconstruct the sequence context for the detected anchors. As noted in our previous response, since our goal was to assess the accuracy of reconstructed compactor sequences, we required a benchmarking dataset with ground truth sequences, which this dataset provides. We will clarify this further in the revision.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concern that a TPM of 100 is high. In Figure 1C, we presented the full TPM distribution for fusions missed or detected by compactors. The 100 threshold was an arbitrary benchmark to illustrate the clear difference in TPM profiles between these two sets of fusions. We will clarify this point in the revised manuscript.

      (8) Why are only 3'UTRs considered and not 5'? Is this because the analysis is asymmetric, i.e., only considering upstream anchors and downstream variation? If so, that seems like a limitation: how much additional variation would you find if including the other direction?

      We thank the reviewer for their comment. SPLASH+ can, in principle, detect variation in 5’ UTR regions, as demonstrated by the variations observed in the 5’ UTRs of the genes ANPC16 and ARPC2. If sequence variation exists in the 5′ UTR, SPLASH+ can still detect it by identifying an anchor upstream of the variable region, as it directly parses sequencing reads to find anchors with downstream sequence diversity. Even when the variation occurs near the 5′ end of the 5′ UTR, SPLASH+ can still capture this diversity if the user selects a shorter anchor length.

      (9) I don't find the theoretical results very meaningful. Assuming independent reads (equivalently binomial counts) has been repeatedly shown to be a poor assumption in sequencing data, likely due to various biases, including PCR. This has motivated the use of overdispersed distributions such as the negative Binomial and beta binomial. The theory would be valuable if it could say something at a specified level of overdispersion. If not, the caveat of assuming no overdispersion should be clearly stated.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment. We will clarify this in the revised paper.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      General response 

      Our modeling study integrates recent experimental advances on dendritic physiology, biophysical plasticity rules, and network connectivity motifs into a single model, aiming to clarify their hypothesized inseparable functional roles in neocortical learning. By modelling excitatory plasticity in multi-synaptic connections on dendrites within a network with biologically constrained higher-order structure, we show these aspects are sufficient to account for a wide range of interesting phenomena: First, the calcium-based plasticity rule acted sparsely and specifically, keeping the network stable without requiring homeostatic mechanisms or inhibitory plasticity, as usually employed for models based on STDP rules. Most importantly, simulations of the network initiated in a recurrent-excitation induced synchronous state transitioned to an in vivo-like asynchronous state, and remained there. Second, plastic changes were stimulus-dependent and could be predicted by neurons’ membership in functional assemblies, spatial clustering of synapses on dendrites, and the topology of the network’s connectivity. Several of our predictions could be confirmed by comparison to the MICrONS dataset.

      Our study thus aims to provide a first broad exploration of these phenomena and their interactions in a model, as well as a foundation for future studies that examine specific aspects more deeply. Specific concerns of the reviewers about parameter choices (reviewer 2’s 2nd point - 2.2), claims about stability (2.1 and 3.1), the STDP control (1.5), and the motivation behind network metrics (1.8, 2.3) are addressed in detail below and in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      This paper investigates the dynamics of excitatory synaptic weights under a calcium-based plasticity rule, in long (up to 10 minutes) simulations of a 211,000-neuron biophysically detailed model of a rat cortical network. 

      Strengths 

      (1) A very detailed network model, with a large number of neurons, connections, synapses, etc., and with a huge number of biological considerations implemented in the model. 

      (2) A carefully developed calcium-based plasticity rule, which operates with biologically relevant variables like calcium concentration and NMDA conductances. 

      (3) The study itself is detailed and thorough, covering many aspects of the cellular and network anatomy and properties and investigating their relationships to plasticity. 

      (4) The model remains stable over long periods of simulations, with the plasticity rule maintaining reasonable synaptic weights and not pushing the network to extremes. 

      (5) The variety of insights the authors derive in terms of relationships between the cellular and network properties and dynamics of the synaptic weights are potentially interesting for the field. 

      (6) Sharing the model and the associated methods and tools is a big plus. 

      We thank the reviewer for their comments.

      Weaknesses 

      (1) Conceptually, there seems to be a missed opportunity here in that it is not clear what the network learns to do. The authors present 10 different input patterns, the network does some plasticity, which is then analyzed, but we do not know whether the learning resulted in anything functionally significant. Did the network learn to discriminate the patterns much better than at the beginning, to capture or anticipate the timing of pattern presentation, detect similarities between patterns, etc.? This is important to understand if one wants to assess the significance of synaptic changes due to plasticity. For example, if the network did not learn much new functionally, relative to its initial state, then the observed plasticity could be considered minor and possibly insufficient. In that case, were the network to learn something substantial, one would potentially observe much more extensive plasticity, and the results of the whole study could change, possibly including the stability of the network. While this could be a whole separate study, this issue is of central importance, and it is hard to judge the value of the results when we do not know what the network learned to do, if anything. 

      (1.1) The reviewer raises a very interesting point of discussion. As they remarked, it is very hard to judge what the network learned to do. However, our model was not designed to solve a specific task and even defining precisely what "learning" entails in a primary sensory region is still an open question. As many before us, we hypothesized that one of the roles of the primary somatosensory cortex would be to represent stimuli features and that most of the learning process would happen in an unsupervised manner. This is indeed what we have demonstrated by showing the stimulus-specificity of changes as well as an increase of reliability of assembly sequences between repetitions after plasticity. We have added this to the Discussion in lines 523-525.

      (2) In this study, plasticity occurs only at E-to-E connections but not at others. However, it is well known that inhibitory connections in the cortex exhibit at the very least a substantial short-term plasticity. One would expect that not including these phenomena would have substantial consequences on the results.

      (1.2) This is indeed well known. Please consider that we do have short-term plasticity (called synapse dynamics in the manuscript) at all connections, including inhibitory ones. We thank the reviewer for pointing out this potential confusion in the wording. We have now clarified this  in the Methods in lines: 691-697. Furthermore, we have listed not having long-term plasticity at inhibitory connections in the limitations part of the Discussion in line: 593.

      (3) Lines 134-135: "We calibrated layer-wise spontaneous firing rates and evoked activity to brief VPM inputs matching in vivo data from Reyes-Puerta et al. (2015)."

      (4) Can the authors show these results? It is an important comparison, and so it would be great to see firing rates (ideally, their distributions) for all the cell types and layers vs. experimental data, for the evoked and spontaneous conditions. 

      (1.3) The layer- and cell type specific spontaneous firing rates were indeed hidden in the Methods and on Supplementary Figure S3. We now reference that figure in the Results in line: 136. Furthermore, we have amended Supplementary Figure S3 (panel A2), to show these rates in the evoked state as well.

      (5) That being said, the Reyes-Puerta et al. paper reports firing rates for the barrel cortex, doesn't it? Whereas here, the authors are simulating a non-barrel cortex. Is such a comparison appropriate?

      (1.4) As correctly pointed out by the reviewer, we made the assumption that these rates would generalize to the whole S1 because of the sparsity of experimental data. This assumption is discussed in length in Isbister et al. (2023) and now in the limitations part of the Discussion in lines: 564-568.

      (6) Comparison with STDP on pages 5-7 and Figure 2: if I got this right, the authors applied STDP to already generated spikes, that is, did not run a simulation with STDP. That seems strange. The spikes they use here were generated by the system utilizing their calcium-based plasticity rule. Obviously, the spikes would be different if STDP was utilized instead. The traces of synaptic weights would then also be different. The comparison therefore is not quite appropriate, is it?

      (1.5) Yes, the reviewer's understanding is correct. However, considering the findings of Morrison et al. 2007 [PMID: 17444756], and Zenke et al. 2017 [PMID: 28431369] (cited in the manuscript in lines: 165-166), running STDP in a closed loop simulation would most likely make the network “blow up” because of the positive feedback loop. Thus, we argue that our comparison is more conservative, since by using pre-generated spikes, we opened the loop and avoided positive feedback. This is now further explained in lines: 166-167.

      (7) Section 2.3 and Figure 5: I am not sure this analysis adds much. The main finding is that plasticity occurs more among cells in assemblies than among all cells. But isn't that expected given what was shown in the previous figures? Specifically, the authors showed that for cells that fire more, plasticity is more prominent. Obviously, cells that fire little or not at all won't belong to any assemblies. Therefore, we expect more plasticity in assemblies.

      (1.6) We thank the reviewer for this comment. We added additional panels (G1 and G2) to Figure 5 (and describe their content in lines: 329-337) showing that this is not the case. Firing-rate alone is indeed predictive of plastic changes, but co-firing in assemblies is even more so.

      (8) Section 2.4 and Figure 6: It is not clear that the results truly support the formulation of the section's title ("Synapse clustering contributes to the emergence of cell assemblies, and facilitates plasticity across them") and some of the text in the section. What I can see is that the effect on rho is strong for non-clustered synapses (Figure 6C and Figure S8A). In some cases, it is substantially higher than what is seen for clustered synapses. Furthermore, the wording "synapse clustering contributes to the emergence of cell assemblies" suggests some kind of causal role of clustered synapses in determining which neurons form specific cell assemblies. I do not see how the data presented supports that. Overall, it appears that the story about clustered synapses is quite complicated, with both clustered and non-clustered synapses driving changes in rho across the board. 

      (1.7) We agree with the reviewer, it is “quite complicated” and we also see that the writing could have been better/more precise and supported by the data shown on the Figure. We updated both the section title and a big chunk of the text to take the suggestions into account in lines: 361-373.

      (9) Section 2.5 and Figure 7: Can we be certain that it is the edge participation that is a particularly good predictor of synaptic changes and/or strength, as opposed to something simpler? For example, could it be the overall number of synapses, excitatory synapses, or something along these lines, that the source and/or target neurons receive, that determine the rho dynamics? And then, I do not understand the claim that edge participation allows one to "delineate potentiation from depression". The only related data I can find is in Figure 7A3, about which the authors write "this effect was stronger for potentiation than depression". But I don't see what they mean. For both depression and facilitation, the changes observed are in the range of ~12% of probability values. And even if the effect is stronger, does it mean one can "delineate" potentiation from depression better? What does it mean, to "delineate"? If it is some kind of decoding based on the edge participation, then the authors did not show that.  

      (1.8) We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have included an analysis of the predictive power of indegree of the pre and postsynaptic neuron of a connection on the rho dynamics in Figure 7 (panel B). Please consider, that the rho dynamics are described on the level of connections, while properties like indegree are on the level of nodes. Any procedure transferring a node based property to an edge based property involves choices e.g., should the values be added, multiplied, should one be preferential over the other, or should they be considered independently? As edge-based metrics avoid these arbitrary choices, we would argue that they are - ultimately - the simpler and more natural choice in this context.

      Though we believe that the metric of edge participation is simple, we recognize it is perhaps not common. Thus, we have switched to using a version of it that is perhaps more intuitive for the community at large i.e., as a metric of common innervation.  Moreover, we have changed the name “(k+2) edge participation” to “(k)-edge indegree”, to make it even more accessible. For k=0, this is the number of neurons that commonly innervate the connection, i.e., a common neighbour. And for k=1, this is the number of connections that commonly innervate the connection.  This is equivalent to edge participation from the next to last to the last neuron in a simplex.  Furthermore, in lines: 391-418 we have added additional text and references explaining the intuition of why we think this metric is relevant, as it has been shown to affect correlated activity of pairs of neurons, as well as assembly formation.

      Furthermore, we have clarified the language referring to potentiation and depression in lines: 420-422 and 448.

      (10) "test novel predictions in the MICrONS (2021) dataset, which while pushing the boundaries of big data neuroscience, was so far only analyzed with single cells in focus instead of the network as a whole (Ding et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2023)." That is incorrect. For example, the whole work of Ding et al. analyzes connectivity and its relation to the neuron's functional properties at the network level. 

      (1.9) We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. Indeed, the sentence was improperly worded. We have appropriately changed this phrasing in lines: 616-618.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This paper aims to understand the effects of plasticity in shaping the dynamics and structure of cortical circuits, as well as how that depends on aspects such as network structure and dendritic processing. 

      Strengths: 

      The level of biological detail included is impressive, and the numerical simulations appear to be well executed. Additionally, they have done a commendable job in open-sourcing the model.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments.

      Weaknesses: 

      The main result of this work is that activity in their network model remains stable without the need for a homeostatic mechanism. However, as the authors acknowledge, this has been  demonstrated in previous studies (e.g., Higgins et al. 2014). In those studies, stability was attributed to calcium-based rules combined with calcium concentrations at in vivo levels and background neuronal activity. Since the authors use the same calcium-based rule, it is unclear what new result, if any, is being presented. If the authors are suggesting that the mechanism in their simulations differs, that should be stated clearly, and evidence supporting that claim should be provided. 

      (2.1) We do not see this as the main result of our study, but rather a critical validation step, since our calcium rule, while similar to previous ones, is not exactly the same (see equations (1) and especially (2) in Methods). This has been clarified in the text in lines: 150-151. Note in particular, that one of the main differences is the stochastic synaptic transmission and the role of calcium concentration on the release probability. Furthermore, our model involves multicompartmental neurons instead of point neuron models, which to our knowledge was never tested before with calcium-based plasticity rules at the network level. Moreover, determining the time required for stability to be reached is a necessary step to set up the simulation parameters to test the main hypotheses about rules governing the plastic changes.

      The other findings discussed in the paper are related to a characterization of the dependency of plastic changes on network structure. While this analysis is potentially interesting, it has the following limitations. 

      First, I believe the authors should include an analysis of the generality and specificity of their results. All the findings seem to be derived from a single run of the simulation. How do the results vary with different network initializations, simulation times, or parameter choices? 

      (2.2) All simulations were run with 3 different random seeds (mentioned in the Methods) and now shown in Supplementary Figure S8 for some selected analyses. The maximum duration of our simulations were limited by our hardware constraints.  However, from the long (10 minutes) simulation we concluded that most changes happen within the first minute. This is how we determined 2 minutes as the simulation time for all other experiments. Parameters determining both the spontaneous and evoked network state are discussed in length in Isbister et al. (2023) and while we acknowledge that they are only shown in Supplementary Figure S3, we did not want to lengthen the manuscript with redundant details but rather refer to reader to the manuscript where this is discussed at large. 

      Crucially, we tried slightly different parameters of the plasticity model in the early phases of the research, and while they changed the exact numerical values of our results, the main trends (i.e., stabilization time, assemblies, synapse clustering, and network topology influencing plastic changes) remained unchanged. This is now shown in Supplementary Figure S13 and referenced in the Discussion in lines: 572-575.

      Second, the presentation of the results is difficult to follow. The characterization comes across as a long list of experiments, making it hard to identify a central message or distinguish key findings from minor details. The authors provide little intuition about why certain outcomes arise, and the complexity of the simulation makes it challenging - if not impossible - to determine which model elements are essential for specific results and which mechanisms drive emergent properties. Additionally, the text often lacks crucial details. For instance, the description of k-edge participation should be expanded, and an explanation of what this method quantifies should be included. Overall, I believe the authors should focus on a smaller set of significant results and provide a more in-depth discussion. 

      (2.3) We acknowledge the complexity of these large-scale simulations and the interpretation of their results. We appreciate the reviewer's feedback on the areas that needed more detail. To address this, we have extended the Results section describing k-edge indegree with more background and intuition in lines: 391-418. See also our reply to reviewer 1 (1.8) above. 

      While the manuscript may appear to be "a long list of experiments," it is actually guided by the following logic: We choose a calcium-based rule because it was the natural choice in a multicompartmental model which already included calcium dynamics and NMDA receptors. After setting up the main network state, verifying stability (Figure 2), doing traditional basic analysis (Figure 3), and verifying that the changes are non-random (Figure 4); we elaborated on long-standing ideas about co-firing in cell assemblies (Figure 5) and spatial clustering of synapse on dendrites (Figure 6) interacting with plasticity. Finally as we had access to the network’s non-random connectivity we tried to link the network's topology to the observed plastic changes. This was done with a higher order perspective, given that there was previous evidence for the relevance of these structures on cofiring and correlated activity.

      While we understand the frustration, we would highlight that the study is the first of its kind at this scale and level of biological detail. Our goal was to offer a broad exploration of the factors influencing plasticity and their interactions at this scale. Thus, laying the groundwork for future studies to investigate specific aspects more deeply. 

      The comparison of the model with the MICrONS dataset could be improved. In Figure 7B, the authors should show how the same quantification looks in a network model without plasticity. In Figure 8B, the data aligns with the model before plasticity, so it's unclear how this serves as a verification of the theoretical predictions.

      (2.4) Our only claim is that by being used to working with both functional and structural data we were able to develop a metric (k-edge indegree) that could be utilized to study the non-random, high-order topology of the MICrONS connectivity as well. On Figure 8, spike correlations in MICrONS more or less align with both cases (before vs. after plasticity); the only difference is that spike correlations looked different enough in the model so we thought they are worth showing for both cases. Moreover, as the changes are sparse (Figure 2 and 3) the synapse strength panel of Figure 7(D) looks almost exactly the same before plasticity (see first two panels of Author response image 1). In line with our results, the small and significant changes increase as k-edge indegree increases (last panel of Author response image 1). As the first two panels look almost the same and the third one is shown in a slightly different way (Figure 7C2) we would prefer not to include this in the manuscript, but only in our response.

      Author response image 1.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      Ecker et al. utilized a biologically realistic, large-scale cortical model of the rat's non-barrel somatosensory cortex, incorporating a calcium-dependent plasticity rule to examine how various factors influence synaptic plasticity under in vivo-like conditions. Their analysis characterized the resulting plastic changes and revealed that key factors, including the co-firing of stimulus-evoked neuronal ensembles, the spatial organization of synaptic clusters, and the overall network topology, play an important role in affecting the extent of synaptic plasticity. 

      Strengths: 

      The detailed, large-scale model employed in this study enables the evaluation of diverse factors across various levels that influence the extent of plastic changes. Specifically, it facilitates the assessment of synaptic organization at the subcellular level, network topology at the macroscopic level, and the co-activation of neuronal ensembles at the activity level. Moreover, modeling plasticity under in vivo-like conditions enhances the model's relevance to experiments. 

      We thank the reviewer for their comments.

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) The authors claimed that, under in vivo-like conditions and in the presence of plasticity, firing rates and weight distributions remain stable without additional homeostatic mechanisms during a 10-minute stimulation period. However, the weights do not reach the steady state immediately after the 10-minute stimulation. Therefore, extended simulations are necessary to substantiate the claim. 

      (3.1) We thank the reviewer for this comment, as it gave us the opportunity to clarify in the text our stabilization criteria. Indeed, the dynamical system of weight changes has not reached a zero-change steady state because the changes, while small, are non-zero. However, in a stochastic system with ongoing activity (stimulus- or noise-driven), non-zero changes are expected. Thus, we consider the system to be at steady state when changes become negligible relative to a null model given by a random walk. Our results show that this condition is met around the 2-minute mark, with negligible changes in the subsequent 8 minutes.

      Moreover, for spontaneous activity, we showed that an unstable network exhibiting synchronous activity can be stabilized into an asynchronous regime by the calcium-based plasticity rule within 10 minutes. These results show that the system reaches a stochastic steady state within 10 minutes without requiring homeostatic mechanisms. Our work reveals that incorporating more biological detail (i.e. calcium-based plasticity), reduces the need for additional mechanisms to stabilize network activity (e.g. fast homeostatic mechanisms).

      Interestingly, one might argue that after 10 minutes of stimulation the network might transition to a different weight configuration if the stimuli change or cease. We agree this is an intriguing question, which we added to the Discussion in lines 611-613. However, this scenario concerns continuous learning, not the system’s steady-state dynamics.

      (2) Another major limitation of the paper lies in its lack of mechanistic insights into the observed phenomena (particularly on aspects that are typically impossible to assess in traditional simplified models, like layer-specific and layer-to-layer pathways-specific plasticity changes), as well as the absence of discussions on the potential computational implications of the corresponding observed plastic changes.

      (3.2) Our study integrates recent experimental advances aiming to clarify their hypothesized inseparable functional roles in neocortical learning. In particular, we study three different kinds of mechanistic insight: co-firing in assemblies (Figure 5), synapse clustering on postsynaptic dendrites (Figure 6), and high-order network topology (Figure 7). Furthermore, layer specificity is shown (Figure 3A1, B1, B2, D1) and so is layer-to-layer specificity (Figure 4A2). In addition we also describe synapse clustering on postsynaptic dendrites (Figure 6) which is not available in simplified models either.

      As such, the mechanistic insights provided in our work are integrative in nature and aim to provide a first broad exploration of these phenomena and their interactions-which are rarely considered together in experimental or modelling studies.  This foundation paves the way for future studies that examine specific aspects more deeply in this level of biological detail.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) I would suggest the authors explain more explicitly that their study uses plasticity for E-to-E connections and not others. Doing so in multiple places in the paper, but certainly in Methods and early in Results, would be helpful. This is stated in lines 117-119 ("To simulate long-term plasticity, we integrated our recently published calcium-based plasticity model that was used to describe functional long-term potentiation and depression between pairs of pyramidal cells"), but could be highlighted more.

      We have added it to several lines in the Methods: 621, 648, 649.

      (2) "Simulations were always repeated at least three times to assess the consistency of the results." This sounds important. How is this used for the analysis? Do the results reported combine the data from the 3 simulations? How did the authors check the "consistency of the results"? Did they run any statistical tests comparing the results between the 3 simulations or was it more of a visual check?

      The reported results come from a single simulation. Three simulations were run to check that no obvious qualitative differences could be found, such as a change of network regime, association between stimuli and assemblies. No statistical tests can be run with samples of size three. These are now shown in Supplementary Figure S8, and additional clarifying text has been added in Methods line: 722. 

      (3) "We needed 12M core hours to run the simulation presented in this manuscript." The Methods section mentions ~2.4 M core hours for a 10-minute simulation, which may be confusing. It might be helpful to provide a table with all the simulations run for this study.

      We wanted to provide a rough estimate of the runtime, but did not run a deep profiling of all campaigns. The results depend on the actual hardware and configurations used (e.g., temporal resolution of synapse reporting).  We understand the potential source of confusion and have clarified this in the Methods in lines 719-721 (and took it out from the Discussion).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) I found the paper somewhat challenging to follow, as there are many small points, making it unclear what the main message is. It sometimes feels like a list of 'we did this and found that.' It might be helpful if the authors focused on a smaller number of key results with more in-depth discussion. For instance, the discussion of network topology on page 9 is intriguing but condensed into a single, dense paragraph that is hard to follow. Clarifying how the random control is generated would also be beneficial.

      See our response to the public review’s third point (2.3).

      (2) Line 245: typo? "Furthermore, the maximal simplex dimension found in the subgraph was two higher than expected by chance.".

      We changed the grammar in line: 249.

      (3) Line 410: typo? "It has been previously shown before that  assemblies have many edges".

      Noted and fixed in line: 463.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The authors claimed that plasticity operates in a sparse and specific manner, with firing rates and weight distributions remaining stable without additional homeostatic mechanisms. However, as shown in Figure 2D inset, the weights do not reach their steady-state values immediately after the 10-minute stimulation. A similar issue is observed in Figure 2G. It would be necessary to show the claim is indeed true as the weights reach the steady states.

      See our response to the public review’s first point (3.1).

      (2) In the model, synapses undergo both short- and long-term plasticity, but the contribution of short-term plasticity to the stated claim is unclear. It would be helpful to demonstrate how the results of Figure 2 are affected when short-term plasticity is excluded.

      STP is needed to achieve the asynchronous in vivo-like firing state in our model (and is intimately linked to the fitting procedure of the plasticity rules - mean-field approximation is not possible due to the important role of synaptic failures in thresholded plasticity outcomes), thus it cannot be excluded. We have added this to the Methods in lines: 691-697.

      (3) It would be helpful to include a supplementary plot, similar to Figure 2F, illustrating the corresponding results for STDP.

      This is not possible as we did not run a different simulation with STDP, only evaluated the changes in connections with an STDP model using spikes from our simulation. We did not incorporate the STDP equations into our detailed network, as there is no canonical or unambiguous way for doing so (e.g., one would need to handle the fact the connections are multi-synaptic). Note however, that considering the findings of Morrison et al. 2007 [PMID: 17444756], and Zenke et al. 2017 [PMID: 28431369] (cited in the manuscript in lines: 165-166), running STDP in a closed loop simulation would most likely make the network “blow up” because of the positive feedback loop.

      (4) It would be helpful to provide mechanistic insights into the current observations and to discuss the potential computational implications of the observed plastic changes. Particularly on aspects that are typically impossible to examine in traditional models, like layer-specific plastic changes presented in Fig. 3A1, B1, B2, D1, and layer-to-layer pathways-specific plastic changes illustrated in Figure 4A2.

      See our response to the public review’s second point (3.2).

      (5) The use of the term 'assembly' in most places of the manuscript may cause confusion. To enhance clarity and foster effective discussions in the field, I would recommend replacing it with 'ensemble,' as suggested in Miehl et al. (2023), 'Formation and computational implications of assemblies in neural circuits' (The Journal of Physiology, 601(15), 3071-3090), which should also be cited.

      We read the mentioned manuscript when it was published (and appreciated it a lot), now reference it, and explain why we did not exactly follow the suggestion in lines: 293-299.

      (6) The title of Figure 5 is not directly supported by the current figure. To strengthen the alignment, it would be helpful to present the results from lines 303-306 in bar plots and incorporate them into Figure 5 to better substantiate the figure title.

      While the mentioned lines compare maximum values to those within the whole dataset, we think those 2*12*12 values are better presented in condensed matrices than bar plots (while the maximum values are still easily grasped from the colorbars). We have added panel G2 to the figure to address a comment by reviewer 1 (1.7), we believe that this further supports the title of the Figure.

      (7) Line 326, cite "Kirchner, J. H., & Gjorgjieva, J. (2021). Emergence of local and global synaptic organization on cortical dendrites. Nature Communications, 12(1), 4005." and "Kirchner, J. H., & Gjorgjieva, J. (2022). Emergence of synaptic organization and computation in dendrites. Neuroforum, 28(1), 21-30."

      Although we were aware of the mentioned manuscripts, we did not include them originally because they are models of a different species. However, we have now cited these in line: 347.

      (8) The contrast results for ensembles 11 and 12 do not appear to support the claims made in lines 339-341. Clarification on this point would be helpful.

      The reviewer is right, we have updated lines: 360-361, to clarify the difference between the two late assemblies.

      (9) For Figure 6C and 6D in Section 2.4, rather than presenting the results for individual ensembles (which could be moved to the supplementary materials), it would be easier if the authors could summarize the results by grouping them into three categories: early, middle, and late ensembles.

      We agree with the reviewer’s suggestion and tried it before, but as the results slightly depend on functional assembly size as well (not only temporal order) averaging them loses information (see different xlims of the panels). Given that the issue is complex we decided to show all the data on the Figure, but we have revised the text now to provide  a more high-level interpretation.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      In this study, the authors conducted a single-cell RNA sequencing analysis of the cellular and transcriptional landscape of the gastric cancer tumor microenvironment, stratifying patients according to their H. pylori status into currently infected, previously infected, and non-infected patients. The authors comprehensively dissect various cellular compartments, including epithelial, stromal, and immune cells, and describe specific cell types and signatures to be associated with H. pylori infection, including i) inflammatory and EMT signatures in malignant epithelial cells, ii) inflammatory CAFs in stromal cells, iii) Angio-TAMs, TREM2+ TAMs, exhausted and suppressive T cells in immune cells. Looking at ligand-receptor interactions as well as correlations between cell type abundances, they suggest that iCAFs interact with immunosuppressive T cells via a NECTIN2-TIGIT axis, as well as Angio-TAMs through a VEGFA/B-VEGFR1 axis and thereby promote immune escape, tumor angiogenesis and resistance to immunotherapy.

      We sincerely appreciate the Reviewer's interest in our study and their valuable insights on how we can further enhance our work.

      The authors conduct a comprehensive and thorough analysis of the complex tumor microenvironment of gastric cancer, both single-cell RNA sequencing data as well as the analysis seem of high quality and according to best practices. The authors validate their findings using external datasets, and include some prognostic value of the identified signatures and cell types. However, most of their conclusions throughout the manuscript are based on the comparison between HPGC and healthy controls, which is not a valid comparison to determine which of the phenotypes are specifically driven by HP infection, e.g. Tregs are high in all GC types, independent of HP status. The same holds true for TREM+ TAMs and iCAFs, which are higher in GC in general. This makes it very difficult to assess the actual HP-driven signatures and cell types. Also, when looking at the correlation/transcriptional differences across different cell types and cellular interactions, the authors do not explicitly define if they are looking at the whole dataset (including healthy controls?) or only at certain patients (HPGC?), which again makes it difficult to interpret the results.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's thorough assessment and valuable feedback on our study. During our analysis, although we did not specifically identify cell types unique to non-HpGC, ex-HpGC, or HpGC, we found that TREM+ TAMs and iCAFs were enriched in H. pylori-infected GC, with an even higher proportion in HpGC. This suggests that the enrichment of TREM+ TAMs and iCAFs is correlated with H. pylori infection status.

      However, gastric cancer is driven by multiple complex factors, including environmental influences, genetic mutations, and pathogenic infections. As single factor, the H. pylori infection does not significantly alter T cell proportions at the cellular level; rather, it affects the expression of immune checkpoint molecules (Author response image 1A-B). Importantly, we evaluated key molecules mediating the interaction among the iCAF with the angio-TAM and Tregs, the results show that the expression of NECTIN, PVR, VEGF, IL11 and IL24 are higher in ex-HpGC compared to the non-HpGC, with the highest expression observed in HpGC, which further validate the H. pylori -driven signatures (Author response image 1C).

      The correlation analysis among different cell types was conducted within different groups based on their H. pylori infection status (Author response image 1C). However, transcriptional differences across different cell types and cellular interactions were analyzed using the entire dataset, including healthy controls. This approach ensured an unbiased identification of molecular and cellular-level differences among cell subtypes before determining whether these subtypes originated from HpGC or ex-HpGC.

      Author response image 1.

      A. The dot plot illustrates the enrichment of the TIGIT-PVR/NECTIN axis in the interaction between malignant epithelial cells and immunosuppressive T cells. B. T Dotplot showing the expression of NECTIN2 and PVR in non-HpGC, ex-HpGC, and HpGC cells. C. The bubble plot showing the expression of NECTIN, PVR, VEGF, IL11 and IL24 in the CAF within non-HpGC, ex-HpGC, and HpGC sample. D. The correlation of cell type (percentage) between Tregs, Angio-TAM, TREM2+ TAM and iCAF.

      The authors aim to confirm some of their findings via immunofluorescence, which in principle is a great approach to validate their results. However, to be able to conclude that e.g. suppressive TIGIT+ T cells are located close to NECTIN2+ malignant epithelium and that this might facilitate immune escape in HPGC (Figure 4K), the authors should include stains that show that this is not the case in the other groups (nonHPGC, exHPGC and HC). The same holds true for Figure 5G.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. We have add the immunostaining of the ligand TIGIT and the receptor NECTIN2 on suppressive T cells and on the malignant epithelium, as well as signature marker of Angio-TAM and TREM2+ TAM including TREM2, SPP1, VEGF and CD68, in the non-HpGC, ex-HpGC and HC sample (Figure S3, Figure S5). We could find that TIGIT and NECTIN2 exclusively express in HpGC and ex-HpGC samples compared with non-HpGC and HC, with extremely higher in HpGC. Furthermore, the Angio-TAM and TREM2+ TAM were exclusively enriched in HpGC and ex-HpGC samples, barely expressed in non-HpGC and HC. The above results also support our finding that the H.p infection statue determinate the enrichment of Angio-TAM and TREM2+ TAM, also the interaction between suppressive T cells and malignant epithelium guided by TIGIT-NECTIN.

      In summary, this study provides a valuable resource on the cellular and transcriptional heterogeneity of the tumor microenvironment in gastric cancers, distinguishing between positive, negative, and previously positive HP-infected gastric cancer patients. Given that HP is the main risk factor for gastric cancer development, the study provides valuable insights into HP-driven transcriptional signatures and how these might contribute to this increased risk, however, the study would highly benefit from a clearer and more stringent comparison between HPGC and nonHPGC.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study aims to describe the single-cell transcriptomes of H pylori-associated (Hp) gastric cancers and tumor microenvironment (TME), as a starting point to understand TME diversity stratified by Hp status.  RNA-seq was performed for gastric cancers with current Hp+ (from N=9 people), ex-Hp+ (N=6), non-Hp (N=6), and healthy gastric tissue (N=6).

      The study expands on previous single-cell transcriptomic studies of gastric cancers and was motivated by previous observations about the effect of H pylori status on therapeutic outcomes. The study includes a brief review of previous work and provides valuable context for this study.

      We thank the Reviewer for recognizing the interest of the topic, and for sharing their views on how we might further strengthen our work.

      Strengths:

      The observations are supported by solid RNAseq study design and analysis. The authors describe correlations between Hp status and inferred molecular characteristics including cell lineages, enrichment for cell subclusters identified as tumour-infiltrating lymphocyte cell types, tumour-infiltrating myeloid cells, and cancer-associated fibroblasts.

      The observed correlations between Hp status and enrichment of cell subclusters were broadly corroborated using comparisons to deconvolved bulk RNAseq from publicly available gastric cancer data, providing a convincing starting point for understanding the diversity of tumour microenvironment by Hp-status.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors acknowledge several limitations of this study.<br /> The correlations with HP-status are based on a small number of participants per Hp category (N=9 with current Hp+; N=6 for ex-HP+ and non-HP), and would benefit from further validation to establish reproducibility in other cohorts.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestions. We acknowledge that this may limit the generalizability and statistical power of our findings. However, despite the limited sample size, our analysis revealed statistically significant trends (e.g., p-value < 0.05) or consistent patterns in the data. The sample size in this study was constrained by the availability of participants meeting the inclusion criteria, particularly in the ex-HP+ and non-HP groups. We view these findings as hypothesis-generating and aim to validate them in future studies with larger cohorts.

      The ligand-receptor cross-talk analysis and the suggestion that suppressive T cells could interact with the malignant epithelium through TIGIT-NECTIN2/PVR pairs, are preliminary findings based on transcriptomic analysis and immunostaining and will require further validation.

      We appreciate the reviewer's comment and agree that the ligand-receptor cross-talk analysis and the proposed interaction between suppressive T cells and malignant epithelium via TIGIT-NECTIN2/PVR pairs are preliminary findings. These insights were derived from transcriptomic data and immunostaining, which provide valuable but indirect evidence of potential interactions. Our analysis revealed co-expression patterns of TIGIT in suppressive T cells and NECTIN2/PVR in malignant epithelial cells, and immunostaining demonstrated spatial proximity between these cell types. Previous studies have established the functional significance of TIGIT-NECTIN2/PVR interactions in immune regulation (PMID: 19815499, 27978489), supporting the biological plausibility of our observations. While our current data provide a foundation for this hypothesis, future studies involving functional assays or in vivo models would be valuable to confirm the biological relevance of these interactions. We view these findings as exploratory and aimed at guiding future research into the role of suppressive T cells in the tumor microenvironment.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Software versions are missing from the scRNAseq section of the Methods.

      Thank you for your feedback. The bioinformation analysis are performed by Seurat 4.1 version, we have annotated the software version in the revised manuscript.

      (2) There is a data link to a deposit in Zenodo, subject to data access request to the authors. Do the authors intend to publish the scRNAseq data?

      Thank you for your inquiry regarding the data availability. We fully intend to make the scRNA-seq data publicly accessible. Currently, the dataset has been deposited in Zenodo and is available upon request to ensure compliance with institutional and ethical guidelines. We are in the process of finalizing the necessary approvals for unrestricted public release. Once completed, we will update the Raw data with an open-access link to facilitate direct download.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript discusses the role of phosphorylated ubiquitin (pUb) by PINK1 kinase in neurodegenerative diseases. It reveals that elevated levels of pUb are observed in aged human brains and those affected by Parkinson's disease (PD), as well as in Alzheimer's disease (AD), aging, and ischemic injury. The study shows that increased pUb impairs proteasomal degradation, leading to protein aggregation and neurodegeneration. The authors also demonstrate that PINK1 knockout can mitigate protein aggregation in aging and ischemic mouse brains, as well as in cells treated with a proteasome inhibitor. While this study provided some interesting data, several important points should be addressed before being further considered.

      Strengths:

      (1) Reveals a novel pathological mechanism of neurodegeneration mediated by pUb, providing a new perspective on understanding neurodegenerative diseases.

      (2) The study covers not only a single disease model but also various neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's disease, aging, and ischemic injury, enhancing the breadth and applicability of the research findings.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) PINK1 has been reported as a kinase capable of phosphorylating Ubiquitin, hence the expected outcome of increased p-Ub levels upon PINK1 overexpression. Figures 5E-F do not demonstrate a significant increase in Ub levels upon overexpression of PINK1 alone, whereas the evident increase in Ub expression upon overexpression of S65A is apparent. Therefore, the notion that increased Ub phosphorylation leads to protein aggregation in mouse hippocampal neurons is not yet convincingly supported.

      Indeed, overexpression of sPINK1 alone resulted in minimal changes in Ub levels in the soluble fraction (Figure 5E), which is expected given that the soluble Ub pool remains relatively stable and buffered. However, sPINK1* overexpression led to a marked increase in Ub levels in the insoluble fraction, indicative of increased protein aggregation (Figure 5F). The molecular weight distribution of Ub in the insoluble fraction was predominantly below 70 kDa, suggesting that phosphorylation inhibits Ub chain elongation.

      To further validate this mechanism, we utilized the Ub/S65A mutant to antagonize Ub phosphorylation and observed a significant reduction in the intensity of aggregated bands at low molecular weights, indicating restored proteasomal activity. The observed increase in Ub levels in the soluble fraction upon Ub/S65A overexpression is likely due to enhanced ubiquitination driven by elevated Ub-S65A, and notably, Ub/S65A was also detectable using an antibody against wild-type Ub.

      Consistent with these findings, overexpression of Ub/S65E resulted in a further increase in Ub levels in the insoluble fraction, with intensified low molecular weight bands. The effect was even more pronounced than that observed with sPINK1 transfection, likely resulting from the complete phosphorylation mimicry achieved by Ub/S65E, compared to the relatively low levels of phosphorylation by PINK1.

      These findings collectively support the conclusion that sPINK1 promotes protein aggregation via Ub phosphorylation. We have updated the Results and Discussion sections to more clearly present the data and explain the various controls.

      (2) The specificity of PINK1 and p-Ub antibodies requires further validation, as a series of literature indicate that the expression of the PINK1 protein is relatively low and difficult to detect under physiological conditions.

      We acknowledge the challenges in achieving high specificity with commercially available and customgenerated antibodies targeting PINK1 and pUb, particularly given their low endogenous expression under physiological conditions. However, in our study, we observed robust immunofluorescent staining for PINK1 (Figures 1A, 1C, and 1G) and pUb (Figures 1B, 1D, and 1G) in human brain samples from Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients, as well as in mouse models of AD and cerebral ischemia. The clear visualization can be partly attributed to the pathological upregulation of PINK1 and pUb under disease conditions. Importantly, the images from pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice exhibit much weaker staining.

      Additionally, we detected a significant elevation in the pUb levels in aged mouse brains compared to younger ones (Figures 1E and 1F). In contrast, pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice showed no change in pUb levels with aging, despite some background signals, demonstrating that pUb accumulation during aging is PINK1dependent. Collectively, these results support the specificity of the antibodies used in detecting pathophysiological changes in PINK1 and pUb levels.

      For cultured cells, pink1<sup>-/-</sup> cells served as a negative control for both PINK1 (Figures 2B and 2C) and pUb (Figures 2D and 2E). While the pUb Western blot exhibited some nonspecific background, pUb levels in pink1<sup>-/-</sup> cells remained unchanged across all MG132 treatment conditions (Figures 2D and 2E), further attesting the usability of the antibodies in conjunction with appropriated controls.

      We have updated the manuscript with higher-resolution images; individual image files have been uploaded separately.

      (3) In Figure 6, relying solely on Western blot staining and Golgi staining under high magnification is insufficient to prove the impact of PINK1 overexpression on neuronal integrity and cognitive function. The authors should supplement their findings with immunostaining results for MAP2 or NeuN to demonstrate whether neuronal cells are affected.

      We included NeuN immunofluorescent staining at 10, 30, and 70 days post transfection in Figure 5— figure supplement 2. The results clearly demonstrate a significant loss of NeuN-positive cells in the hippocampus following Ub/S65E overexpression, while no apparent reduction was observed with sPINK1 transfection alone. 

      We have also quantified MAP2 protein levels via Western blotting and examined morphology of neuronal dendrite and synaptic structure using Golgi staining. These analyses revealed a significant reduction in MAP2 levels and synaptic damage upon sPINK1 or Ub/S65E overexpression (Figures 6F and 6H), consistent with the proteomics analysis (Figure 5—figure supplementary 5). Notably, these detrimental effects could be rescued by co-expression of Ub/S65A, reinforcing the role of pUb in mediating these structural changes.

      Together, our findings from NeuN immunostaining, MAP2 protein analysis, proteomics analysis, and Golgi staining provide strong evidence for the impact of PINK1 overexpression and pUb elevation on neuronal integrity and synaptic structure.

      (4) The authors should provide more detailed figure captions to facilitate the understanding of the results depicted in the figures.

      Figure captions have been updated with more details incorporated in the revised manuscript.

      (5) While the study proposes that pUb promotes neurodegeneration by affecting proteasomal function, the specific molecular mechanisms and signaling pathways remain to be elucidated.

      The molecular mechanisms and signaling pathways through which pUb promotes neurodegeneration are likely multifaceted and interconnected. Our findings suggest that mitochondrial dysfunction plays a central role following sPINK1* overexpression. This is supported by (1) an observed increase in full-length PINK1, indicative of impaired mitochondrial quality control, and (2) proteomic data showing enhanced mitophagy at 30 days post-transfection, followed by substantial mitochondrial injuries at 70 days post-transfection (Figure 5—figure supplement 5 and Supplementary Data). The progressive mitochondrial damage caused by protein aggregates would exacerbate neuronal injury and degeneration.

      Additionally, reduced proteasomal activity may lead to the accumulation of inhibitory proteins that are normally degraded by the ubiquitin-proteasome system. Our proteomics analysis identified a >50fold increase in CamK2n1 (UniProt ID: Q6QWF9), an endogenous inhibitor of CaMKII activation, following sPINK1* overexpression. The accumulation of CamK2n1 suppresses CaMKII activation, thereby inhibiting the CREB signaling pathway (Figure 7), which is essential for synaptic plasticity and neuronal survival. This disruption can further contribute to neurodegenerative processes.

      Thus, our findings underscore the complexity of pUb-mediated neurodegeneration and call for further investigation into downstream consequences.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Suggestions for improved or additional experiments, data or analyses.

      We have performed additional experiments to investigate how the impairment of ubiquitinproteasomal activity contributes to neurodegeneration. Specifically, we investigated CamK2n1, an endogenous inhibitor of CaMKII, which is normally degraded by the proteasome to allow CaMKII activation. Our proteomics analysis revealed a significant (>50-fold) elevation of CamKI2n1 following sPINK1 overexpression (Figure 5—figure supplement 5 and Supplementary Data).

      To validate this mechanism, we conducted immunofluorescence and Western blot analyses, demonstrating reduced levels of phosphorylated CaMKII (pCaMKII) and phosphorylated CREB (pCREB), as well as reduced levels of downstream proteins such as BDNF and ERK. These results have been incorporated into the revised manuscript (Figure 7).

      As the proteasome is crucial in maintaining proteostasis, its dysregulation would trigger neurodegeneration through multiple pathways, contributing to a broad cascade of pathological events.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript makes the claim that pUb is elevated in a number of degenerative conditions including Alzheimer's Disease and cerebral ischemia. Some of this is based on antibody staining which is poorly controlled and difficult to accept at this point. They confirm previous results that a cytosolic form of PINK1 accumulates following proteasome inhibition and that this can be active. Accumulation of pUb is proposed to interfere with proteostasis through inhibition of the proteasome. Much of the data relies on over-expression and there is little support for this reflecting physiological mechanisms.

      Weaknesses:

      The manuscript is poorly written. I appreciate this may be difficult in a non-native tongue, but felt that many of the problems are organizational. Less data of higher quality, better controls and incision would be preferable. Overall the referencing of past work is lamentable. Methods are also very poor and difficult to follow.

      Until technical issues are addressed I think this would represent an unreliable contribution to the field.

      (1) Antibody specificity and detection under pathological conditions

      We recognize the limitations of commercially available antibodies for detecting PINK1 and pUb. Nevertheless, our findings reveal a significant elevation in PINK1 and pUb levels under pathological conditions, such as Alzheimer's disease (AD) and ischemia. Additionally, we observed an increase in pUb level during brain aging, further demonstrating its relevance and a potentially causative role for this special pathological condition. Similarly, elevated pUb levels were observed for cultured cells following pharmacological treatment or oxygen-glucose deprivation (OGD).

      In contrast, in pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice and HEK293 cells used as negative controls, PINK1 and pUb levels remained consistently low. Therefore, the observed elevation of PINK1 and pUb are associated with special pathological conditions, rather than an antibody-detection anomaly.

      (2) Overexpression as a model for pathological conditions

      To investigate whether the inhibitory effects of sPINK1 on the ubiquitin-proteasome system (UPS) depend on its kinase activity, we employed a kinase-dead version of sPINK1* as a negative control. Given that PINK1 targets multiple substrates, we also investigated whether its effects on UPS inhibition were specifically mediated by ubiquitin phosphorylation. To this end, we used Ub/S65A (a phospho-null mutant) to block Ub phosphorylation by sPINK1, and Ub/S65E (a phospho-mimetic mutant) to mimic phosphorylated Ub. These well-defined controls ensured the robustness of our conclusions.

      Although overexpression does not perfectly replicate physiological conditions, it provides a valuable model for studying pathological scenarios such as neurodegeneration and brain aging, where pUb levels are elevated. For example, we observed a 30.4% increase in pUb levels in aged mouse brains compared to young brains (Figure 1F). Similarly, in our sPINK1 overexpression model, pUb levels increased by 43.8% and 59.9% at 30- and 70-days post-transfection, respectively, compared to controls (Figures 5A and 5C). Notably, co-expression of sPINK1* with Ub/S65A almost entirely prevented sPINK1* accumulation (Figure 5B), indicating that an active UPS can efficiently degrade this otherwise stable variant of sPINK1.

      Together, our findings demonstrate that sPINK1 accumulation inhibits UPS activity, an effect that can be reversed by the phospho-null Ub mutant. The overexpression model mimics pathological conditions and provides valuable insights into pUb-mediated proteasomal dysfunction.

      (3) Organization of the manuscript

      Following your suggestion, we have restructured the manuscript to present the key findings in a more logical and cohesive sequence:

      (a) Evidence for elevated PINK1 and pUb levels across a broad spectrum of pathological and neurodegenerative conditions;

      (b) The effects of pUb elevation in cultured cells, focusing on the proteasome;

      (c) Mechanistic insights into how pUb elevation inhibits proteasomal activity;

      (d) The absence of PINK1 and pUb alleviates protein aggregation;

      (e) Evidence for the causative relationship between elevated pUb levels and proteasomal inhibition;

      (f) Demonstration that pUb elevation directly contributes to neuronal degeneration;

      (g) Give an additional evidence to explain the mechanism of neuronal degeneration post sPINK1* over-expression. The downstream effects of elevated CamK2n1, an inhibitor of CaMKII, resulting from proteasomal inhibition.

      This reorganization should ensure a clear and progressive narrative, and enhance the overall coherence and impact of the revised manuscript.

      (4) Revisions to writing, referencing, and methodology

      We have made a great effort to enhance the clarity and flow of the manuscript, including the addition of references to appropriately acknowledge prior work. We have also expanded the Methods section with additional details to improve readability and ensure reproducibility. We believe these revisions effectively address the concerns raised and strengthen the overall quality of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Figure 1: PINK1 is a poorly expressed protein and difficult to detect by Western blot let alone by immunofluorescence. I have direct experience of the antibody used in this study and do not consider it reliable. There are much cleaner reagents out there, although they still have many challenges. The minimal requirement here is for the PINK1 antibody staining to be compared in wild-type and knockout mice. One would also expect to see a mitochondrial staining which would require higher magnification to be definitive, but it does not look like it to me. This is a key foundational figure and is unreliable. The pUb antibody also has a high background, see for example figure 2E.

      Under physiological conditions, PINK1 and pUb levels are indeed low, making their detection challenging. However, under pathological conditions, their expression is significantly elevated, correlating with disease severity. Given the limitations of available reagents, using appropriate controls is a standard approach in biological research.

      Nevertheless, we observed robust immunofluorescent staining for PINK1 (Figures 1A, 1C, and 1G) and pUb (Figures 1B, 1D, and 1G) in human brain samples from Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients and mouse models of AD and cerebral ischemia. Compared to healthy controls, the significant elevation of PINK1 and pUb under these pathological conditions accounts for their clear visualization. To validate antibody specificity, we have included images from pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice as negative controls (Figure 1C and 1D, third panel).

      Furthermore, we analyzed pUb levels in both young and aged mice, using pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice as controls.

      Our results revealed a significant increase in pUb levels in aged wild-type mice (Figures 1E and 1F), In contrast, pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice exhibited relatively low pUb levels, with no notable change between young and aged groups. These findings reinforce the conclusion that pUb accumulation during aging is dependent on PINK1.Furthermore, we analyzed pUb levels in both young and aged mice, using pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice as controls.

      For HEK293 cells, pink1<sup>-/-</sup> cells were used as a negative control for assessing PINK1 (Figures 2B and 2C) and pUb levels (Figures 2D and 2E). While the pUb Western blot did show some nonspecific background, as you have noted, pUb levels significantly increased following MG132 treatment of the wildtype cells. In contrast, no such increase was observed in pink1<sup>-/-</sup> cells (Figure 2D and 2E). These results further validate the reliability of our findings.

      Regarding mitochondrial staining, we recognize that PINK1 localization can vary depending on the pathological context. For example, in Alzheimer’s disease, PINK1 exhibits relatively high nuclear staining, while in cerebral ischemia and brain aging, it is predominantly cytoplasmic and punctate. In contrast, in young, healthy mouse brains, PINK1 is more uniformly distributed. The observed elevation in pUb levels could arise from mitochondrial PINK1 or soluble sPINK1 in the cytoplasm, and it remains unclear whether nuclear PINK1 contributes to pUb accumulation. Investigating the role of PINK1 in different forms and subcellular localizations will be an important avenue for future research.

      To enhance clarity, we have updated our images and replaced them with higher-resolution versions in the revised manuscript.

      Please also confirm that the GAPDH loading controls represent the same gels, to my eye they do not match.

      We have reviewed all the bands, and confirmed that the GAPDH loading controls correspond to the same gels. For different gels, we use separate GAPDH loading controls. There are two experimental scenarios to consider:

      (1) When there is a large difference in molecular weight between target proteins, we cut the gel into sections and incubate each section with different antibodies separately.

      (2) When the molecular weight difference is small and cutting is not feasible, we first probe the membrane with one antibody, strip it, and then re-incubate the membrane with a second antibody.

      These approaches ensure accurate and reliable detection of target proteins with various molecular weights relative to GAPDH.

      1H. Ponceau.

      We have corrected the spelling.

      Figure 2 many elements are confirmation of work already reported and this must be made clearer in the text. 

      Indeed, the elevation of sPINK1 and pUb upon proteasomal inhibition has been previously reported, and these studies have been acknowledged (Gao, et al, 2016; Dantuma, et al, 2000). In the present study, we expand on these findings by conducting a detailed analysis of the time- and concentrationdependent effects of MG132 on sPINK1 and pUb levels, establishing a causative relationship between pUb accumulation and proteasomal inhibition. Furthermore, we demonstrate that sPINK1 overexpression and MG132-induced proteasomal inhibition exhibit no additive effect, indicating that both converge on the same pathway, resulting in the impairment of proteasomal activity.

      It has been established that ubiquitin phosphorylation inhibits Ub chain elongation (Wauer, et al, 2015). However, our study provides novel insights by identifying an additional mechanism: phosphorylated Ub also interferes with the noncovalent interactions between Ub chain and Ub receptors in the proteasome, which further contributes to the impairment of UPS function.

      The PINK1 kinase-dead mutant construction (Figure 2F) and the use of Ub-GFP as a proteasomal substrate were based on established methodologies, which have been appropriately cited in the manuscript (Beilina, etal 2005 for KD sPINK1; Yamano, et al for endogenous PINK1; Samant, et al, 2018 and Dantuma, et al, 2000 for Ub-GFP probe). Similarly, our use of puromycin and BALA treatments follows previously reported protocols (Gao, et al, 2016), which allowed us to dissect the relative contributions of sPINK1* overexpression to proteasomal vs. autophagic dysfunction.

      As you have noted, our study has built upon prior findings while introducing new mechanistic insights into sPINK1 and pUb-mediated proteasomal dysfunction.

      2C 24h MG132 not recommended, most cells are dead by then.

      We used MG132 treatment for 24 hours to evaluate the time-course effects of proteasomal inhibition on PINK1 and pUb levels in HEK293 cells (Figures 2C and 2E). We did observe some decrease in both PINK1 and pUb levels at 24 hours compared to 12 hours, which may result from some extend of cell death at the longer treatment duration.

      In SH-SY5Y cells, we collected cells at 24 hours after MG132 administration (Figure 5—figure supplementary 1). Though protein aggregation was evident in these cells, we did not observe pronounced cell death under these conditions, justifying our treatment.

      Our findings are consistent with previous studies demonstrating that MG132 at 5 µM for 24 hours effectively induces proteasomal inhibition without substantial cytotoxicity. For example, studies using human esophageal squamous cancer cells have reported that this treatment condition inhibits cell proliferation while maintaining cell viability, with cell viability >70% after 24-hour treatment with 5 µM MG132 (Int J Mol Med 33: 1083-1088, 2014). 

      MG132 has been commonly used at concentrations ranging from 5 to 50 µM for durations of 1 to 24 hours, as stated at the vendor’s website (https://www.cellsignal.com/products/activatorsinhibitors/mg-132/2194).

      2I what is BALA do they mean bafilomycin. This is a v-ATPase inhibitor, not just an autophagy inhibitor.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment regarding the use of BALA in Figure 2I. To clarify, BALA refers to bafilomycin A1, a well-established v-ATPase inhibitor that blocks lysosomal acidification. While bafilomycin A1 is commonly used as an autophagy inhibitor, its primary mechanism involves inhibiting lysosomal function, which is critical for autophagosome-lysosome fusion and subsequent degradation of autophagic cargo.

      In our study, we used bafilomycin A1 in conjunction with puromycin to dissect the relative contributions of sPINK1 overexpression on proteasomal and autophagic activities. Puromycin induces protein misfolding and aggregation, causing stress on both degradation pathways. By inhibiting lysosomal function with bafilomycin A1 and blocking the protein degradation load at various stages, we can tell the relative contributions of autophagy and UPS pathways.

      We acknowledge that bafilomycin A1’s effects extend beyond autophagy, as it also inhibits v-ATPase activity. However, its inhibition of lysosomal degradation is integral to distinguishing autophagy’s contribution under the experimental conditions, and BALA treatment has been used in extensively in previous studies (Mauvezin and Neufeld, 2015). 

      We have further clarified this treatment in the revised manuscript.

      Figure 3. Legend or text needs to be more explicit about how chains have been produced. From what I can gather from methods only a single E2 has been trialed. Authors should use at least one of the criteria used by Wauer et al. (2014) to confirm the stoichiometry of phosphorylation. The concept that pUb can interfere with E2 discharging is not new, but not universal across E2s.

      We have cited in the manuscript that PINK1-mediated ubiquitin phosphorylation can interfere with ubiquitin chain elongation for certain E2 enzymes (Wauer et al., 2015). 

      To clarify, the focus of our current work is on how elevation of Ub phosphorylation impacts UPS activity, rather than exploring the broader effects of Ub phosphorylation on Ub chain elongation. For this reason, we have used the standard E2 that is well-established for generating K48-linked polyUb chain (Pickart CM, 2005). Moreover, our findings go further and by demonstrate that phosphorylated K48-linked polyubiquitin exhibits weaker non-covalent interactions with proteasomal ubiquitin receptors. This dual effect—on both covalent chain elongation and non-covalent interactions— contributes to the observed reduction in ubiquitin-proteasome activity, a novel aspect of our study.

      To address the reviewer’s concerns, we have added details in the Methods section and figure legends regarding the generation of ubiquitin chains. Specifically, we used ubiquitin-activating enzyme E1 (UniProt ID: P22314) and ubiquitin-conjugating enzyme E2-25K (UniProt ID: P61086) to generate K48-linked ubiquitin chains. 

      Our ESI-MS analysis showed that only 1–2 phosphoryl groups were incorporated into the K48-linked tetra-ubiquitin chains (Figure 3—figure supplement 2). This is consistent with our in vivo findings, where pUb levels increased by 30.4% in aged mouse brains compared to young brains (Figure 1F). Notably, even sub-stoichiometric phosphorylation onto the K48-linked ubiquitin chain significantly weakens the non-covalent interactions with the proteasome (Figures 3E and 3H).

      Figure 4. I could find no definition of the insoluble fraction, nor details on how it is prepared.

      The insoluble fraction primarily contains proteins that are aggregated or associated with hydrophobic interactions and cannot be solubilized by RIPA buffer. We have provided more details in the Methods of the revised manuscript about how the insoluble fraction was prepared. Our approach was based on established protocols for fractionating soluble and insoluble proteins from brain tissues (Wirths, 2017). Here is an outline of the procedure, which enables the separation and subsequent analysis of distinct protein populations:

      • Lysis and preparation of soluble fraction: Cells and brain tissues were lysed using RIPA buffer (Beyotime Biotechnology, cat# P0013B) containing protease (P1005) and phosphatase inhibitors (P1081) on ice for 30 minutes, with gentle vortexing every 10 minutes. Brain samples were homogenized using a precooled TissuePrep instrument (TP-24, Gering Instrument Company). Lysates were centrifuged at 12,000 rpm for 30 minutes at 4°C. The supernatant was collected as the soluble protein fraction.

      • Preparation of insoluble fraction: The pellet was resuspended in 20 µl of SDS buffer (2% SDS, 50 mM Tris-HCl, pH 7.5) and subjected to ultrasonic pyrolysis at 4°C for 8 cycles (10 seconds ultrasound, 30 seconds interval). The samples were then centrifuged at 12,000 rpm for 30 minutes at 4°C. The supernatant obtained after this step was designated as the insoluble protein fraction.

      • Protein quantification: Protein concentrations for both soluble and insoluble fractions were determined using the BCA Protein Assay Kit (Beyotime Biotechnology, cat# P0009).

      Figure 5. What is the transfection efficiency? How many folds is sPINK1 over-expressed? Typically, a neuron will have only a few hundred copies of PINK1 at the basal state. How much mutant ubiquitin is expressed relative to wild type, seeing the free ubiquitin signals on the gels might be helpful here, but they seem to have been cut off. 

      We appreciate the reviewer's insightful comments regarding transfection efficiency, the extent of sPINK1 overexpression, and the expression levels of mutant ubiquitin relative to wild-type ubiquitin. Below, we provide detailed responses to each point:

      Transfection Efficiency: Our immunofluorescent staining for NeuN, a neuronal marker, demonstrated that over 90% of NeuN-positive cells were co-localized with GFP (Figure 5—figure supplement 2), indicating a high transfection efficiency in our neuronal cultures.

      Extent of sPINK1 Overexpression: Quantifying the exact fold increase of sPINK1 upon overexpression is inherently difficult due to its low basal expression under physiological conditions, making the relative increase difficult to measure (small denominator effect). However, our Western blot analysis shows that ischemic events can cause a substantial elevation of PINK1 levels, including both full-length and cleaved forms (Figure 1H). This suggests that our overexpression model recapitulates the pathological increase in PINK1, making it a relevant system for studying disease mechanisms.

      From Figure 5B, it is evident that sPINK1 levels differ significantly between neurons overexpressing sPINK1 alone and those co-expressing sPINK1 + Ub/S65A (70 days post-transfection). Overexpression of sPINK1 alone results in multiple PINK1 bands, consistent with sPINK1, endogenous PINK1 (induced by mitochondrial damage), and ubiquitinated sPINK1. In comparison, co-expressing Ub/S65A leads to faint PINK1 bands, suggesting that in the presence of a functionally restored proteasome, overexpressed sPINK1 is rapidly degraded. Therefore, actual accumulation of sPINK1 depends on proteasomal activity, and the “over-expressed” PINK1 level can be comparable to levels observed under native, pathological conditions.

      Expression Levels of Mutant Ubiquitin Relative to Wild-Type: Assessing the expression levels of mutant versus wild-type ubiquitin is indeed valuable. In Figure 5E, we observed a 38.9% increase in high-molecular-weight ubiquitin conjugates in the soluble fraction when comparing the sPINK1+Ub/S65A group to the control. This increase suggests that mutant ubiquitin is actively incorporated into polyubiquitin chains.

      Regarding free monomeric ubiquitin, its low abundance and rapid incorporation into polyubiquitin chains make it difficult to visualize in Western blots. Additionally, its low molecular weight and lower antibody binding valency further reduce its visibility.

      General: a number of effects are shown following over-expression but no case is made that these levels of pUb are ever attained physiologically. I am very unconvinced by these findings and think the manuscript needs to be improved at multiple levels before being added to the record.

      We understand the reviewer’s concerns regarding the relevance of pUb levels observed in our overexpression model. To clarify, our study is not focused on physiological levels of pUb, but rather on pathologically elevated levels, which have been documented in various neurodegenerative conditions. While overexpression is not a perfect replication of pathological states, it provides a valuable tool to investigate mechanisms that become relevant under disease conditions. Moreover, we have taken steps to ensure the validity of our findings and to address potential limitations associated with overexpression models:

      Pathological Relevance: Besides several reported literatures, we observed significant increases in PINK1 and pUb levels in human brain samples from Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients, as well as in mouse models of AD, cerebral ischemia (including mouse middle cerebral artery occlusion ischemic model and oxygen glucose deprivation cell model), and aging (e.g., Figures 1E, 1F, and 1H). All these data show that pUb levels are elevated under pathological conditions. Our overexpression model mimics these pathological scenarios by recreating the high levels of pUb, which lead to the impairment of proteasomal activity and subsequent disruption of proteostasis.

      Use of Robust Controls: To ensure the reliability of our results and interpretations, we employed multiple controls for our experiments. We have used pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice and cells to confirm that pUb accumulation is PINK1-dependent (Figures 1C and 2C). We have also included kinase-dead sPINK1 mutant and Ub/S65A phospho-null mutants to negate/counteract the specific roles of PINK1 activity and pUb in proteasomal dysfunction. On the other hand, we have used Ub/S65E for phosphomimetic mutant, corresponding to a 100% Ub phosphorylation.

      Importantly, we have compared sPINK1 overexpression with both baseline and disease-mimicking conditions, thus to ensure that the observed effects are consistent with pathological changes. Furthermore, our findings are supported by complementary evidences from human brain samples, model animals, cell cultures, and molecular assays. Integrating the different controls and various approaches, we have provided mechanistic insights into how elevated pUb levels causes proteasomal impairment and contributes to neurodegeneration.

      Our findings elucidate how elevated pUb level contributes to the disruption of proteostasis in neurodegenerative conditions. While overexpression may have limitations, it remains a powerful tool for dissecting pathological mechanisms and testing hypotheses. Our results align with and expand upon previous studies suggesting pUb as a biomarker of neurodegeneration (Hou, et al, 2018; Fiesel, et al, 2015), and provide mechanistic insights into how elevated pUb and sPINK1 drive a viscous feedforward cycle, ultimately leading to proteasomal dysfunction and neurodegeneration. 

      We hope these clarifications highlight the relevance and rigor of our study, and welcome additional suggestions to improve the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study aims to explore the role of phosphorylated ubiquitin (pUb) in proteostasis and its impact on neurodegeneration. By employing a combination of molecular, cellular, and in vivo approaches, the authors demonstrate that elevated pUb levels contribute to both protective and neurotoxic effects, depending on the context. The research integrates proteasomal inhibition, mitochondrial dysfunction, and protein aggregation, providing new insights into the pathology of neurodegenerative diseases.

      Strengths:

      - The integration of proteomics, molecular biology, and animal models provides comprehensive insights.

      - The use of phospho-null and phospho-mimetic ubiquitin mutants elegantly demonstrates the dual effects of pUb.

      - Data on behavioral changes and cognitive impairments establish a clear link between cellular mechanisms and functional outcomes.

      Weaknesses:

      - While the study discusses the reciprocal relationship between proteasomal inhibition and pUb elevation, causality remains partially inferred.

      It has been well-established that protein aggregates, particularly neurodegenerative fibrils, can impair proteasomal activity (McDade, et al., 2024; Kinger, et al., 2024; Tseng, et al., 2008). Other contributing factors, including ATP depletion, reduced proteasome component expression, and covalent modifications of proteasomal subunits, can also lead to declined proteasomal function. Additionally, mitochondrial injury serves as an important source of elevated PINK1 and pUb levels. Recent studies have demonstrated that efficient mitophagy is essential to prevent pUb accumulation, whereas partial mitophagy failure results in elevated PINK1 levels (Chin, et al, 2023; Pollock, et al. 2024).

      While pathological conditions can impair proteasomal function and slow sPINK1 degradation, leading to its accumulation, our results demonstrate that overexpression of sPINK1 or PINK1 can initiate this cycle as well. Once this cycle is initiated, it becomes self-perpetuating, as sPINK1 and pUb accumulation progressively impair proteasomal function, leading to more protein aggregates and mitochondrial damages.

      Importantly, we show that co-expression of Ub/S65A effectively rescues cells from this cycle, which further illustrates the pivotal role of pUb in driving proteasomal inhibition and the causality between pUb elevation and proteasomal inhibition. At the animal level, pink1 knockout prevents protein aggregation under aging and cerebral ischemia conditions (Figures 1E and 1G). 

      Together, by controlling at protein, cell, and animal levels, our findings support this self-reinforcing and self-amplifying cycle of pUb elevation, proteasomal inhibition, protein aggregation, mitochondrial damage, and ultimately, neurodegeneration.

      - The role of alternative pathways, such as autophagy, in compensating for proteasomal dysfunction is underexplored.

      Indeed, previous studies have shown that elevated sPINK1 can enhance autophagy (Gao, et al., 2016,), potentially compensating for impaired UPS function. One mechanism involves PINK1mediated phosphorylation of p62, which enhances autophagic activity.

      In our study, we observed increased autophagic activity upon sPINK1 overexpression, as shown in Figure 2I (middle panel, without BALA). This increase in autophagy may facilitate the degradation of ubiquitinated proteins induced by puromycin, partially mitigating proteasomal dysfunction. This compensation might also explain why protein aggregation, though statistically significant, increased only slightly at 70 days post-sPINK1 transfection (Figure 5F). Additionally, we detected a mild but statistically insignificant increase in LC3II levels in the hippocampus of mouse brains at 70 days postsPINK1 transfection (Figure 5—figure supplement 6), further supporting the notion of autophagy activation.

      However, while autophagy may provide some compensation, its effect is likely limited. The UPS and autophagy serve distinct roles in protein degradation:

      • Autophagy is a bulk degradation pathway, primarily targeting damaged organelles, intracellular pathogens, and protein aggregates, often in a non-selective manner.

      • The UPS, in contrast, is highly selective, degrading short-lived regulatory proteins, misfolded proteins, and proteins tagged for degradation via ubiquitination.

      Thus, while sPINK1 overexpression enhances autophagy-mediated degradation, it simultaneously impairs UPS-mediated degradation. This suggests that autophagy partially compensates for proteasomal dysfunction but is insufficient to counterbalance the UPS's selective degradation function. We have incorporated additional discussion in the revised manuscript.

      - The immunofluorescence images in Figure 1A-D lack clarity and transparency. It is not clear whether the images represent human brain tissue, mouse brain tissue, or cultured cells. Additionally, the DAPI staining is not well-defined, making it difficult to discern cell nuclei or staging. To address these issues, lower-magnification images that clearly show the brain region should be provided, along with improved DAPI staining for better visualization. Furthermore, the Results section and Figure legends should explicitly indicate which brain region is being presented. These concerns raise questions about the reliability of the reported pUb levels in AD, which is a critical aspect of the study's findings.

      We have taken steps to address the concerns regarding clarity and transparency in Figure 1A-D. We have already addressed the source of tissues at the left of each images. For example, we have written “human brain with AD” at the left side of Figure 1A, and “mouse brains with AD” at the left side of Figure 1C.

      Briefly, the human brain samples in Figure 1 originate from the cingulate gyrus of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) patients. Our analysis revealed that PINK1 is primarily localized within cell bodies, whereas pUb is more abundant around Aβ plaques, likely in nerve terminals. For the mouse brain samples, we have now explicitly indicated in the figure legends and Results section that the images represent the neocortex of APP/PS1 mice, a mouse model relevant to AD pathology, as well as the corresponding regions in wild-type and pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice. We have ensured that the brain regions and sources are clearly stated throughout the manuscript.

      Regarding image clarity, we have uploaded higher-resolution versions of the images in the revised manuscript to improve visualization of key features, including DAPI staining. We believe these revisions enhance the reliability and interpretability of our findings, particularly in relation to the reported pUb levels in AD. 

      - Figure 4B should also indicate which brain region is being presented.

      The images were taken for layer III-IV in the neocortex of mouse brains. We have included this information in the figure legend of the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      - Expand on the potential compensatory role of autophagy in response to proteasomal dysfunction.

      Upon proteasomal inhibition, cells may activate autophagy as an alternative pathway of degradation to help clear damaged or misfolded proteins. Autophagy is a bulk degradation process that targets long-lived proteins, damaged organelles, and aggregated proteins for lysosomal degradation. While this pathway can provide some compensation, it is distinct from the ubiquitin-proteasome system (UPS), which specializes in the selective degradation of short-lived regulatory proteins and misfolded proteins.

      In our study, we observed increased autophagic activity following sPINK1 overexpression (Figure 2J, middle panel, without BALA) and a slight, though statistically insignificant, increase in LC3II levels in the hippocampus of mouse brains at 70 days post-sPINK1 transfection (Figure 5—figure supplement 6). These findings suggest that autophagy is indeed upregulated as a compensatory response to proteasomal dysfunction, potentially facilitating the degradation of aggregated ubiquitinated proteins. Additionally, gene set enrichment analysis (GSEA) revealed similar enrichment of autophagy pathways at 30 and 70 days post-sPINK1 overexpression (Figure 5—figure supplement 5).

      However, the compensatory capacity of autophagy is likely limited. While autophagy can reduce protein aggregation, it is an inherently non-selective process and cannot fully replace the targeted functions of the UPS. Moreover, as we illustrate in Figure 7 of the revised manuscript, UPS is essential for degrading specific regulatory and inhibitory proteins and plays a critical role in cellular proteostasis, particularly in signaling regulation, cell cycle control, and stress responses.

      Together, while autophagy activation provides some degree of compensation, it cannot fully restore cellular proteostasis. The interplay between these two degradation pathways is an important area for future investigation. For the present study, our focus is on how pUb elevations impact proteasomal activity and elicits downstream effects.

      We have incorporated these additional discussions on this topic in the revised manuscript.

      - Simplify the discussion of complex mechanisms to improve accessibility for readers.

      We have revised the Discussion to present the mechanisms in a more coherent and accessible manner, ensuring clarity for a broader readership. These revisions should make the discussion more intuitive while preserving the depth of our findings.

      - Statistical analyses could benefit from clarifying how technical replicates and biological replicates were accounted for across experiments.

      We have clarified our statistical analysis in the Methods section and figure legends, explicitly detailing how many biological replicates were accounted for across experiments. These revisions should enhance transparency and clarity, ensuring that our findings are robust and reproducible.

      - The image in Figure 3D is too small to distinguish any signals. A larger and clearer image should be presented.

      We have expanded the images in Figure 3D. Additionally, we have replaced figures with version of better resolutions throughout the manuscript.

      - NeuN expression in Figure 4B differs between wildtype and pink-/- mice. Additional validation is needed to determine whether pink-/- enhances NeuN expression.

      The difference in NeuN immunofluorescence intensity between wild-type and pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice in Figure 4B may simply result from variations in image acquisition rather than an actual difference in NeuN expression.

      Our single nuclei RNA-seq analyses of wild-type and pink1<sup>-/-</sup> mice at 3 and 18 months of age reveal no significant differences in NeuN expression at the transcript level (data provided below). This confirms that the observed variation in fluorescence intensity is unlikely to reflect an authentic upregulation of NeuN expression. Thus, factors like the concentration of antibody, image exposure and processing may contribute to differences in staining intensity.

      Author response image 1.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript entitled "Phosphodiesterase 1A Physically Interacts with YTHDF2 and Reinforces the Progression of Non-Small Cell Lung Cancer" explores the role of PDE1A in promoting NSCLC progression by binding to the m6A reader YTHDF2 and regulating the mRNA stability of several novel target genes, consequently activating the STAT3 pathway and leading to metastasis and drug resistance.

      Strengths:

      The study addresses a novel mechanism involving PDE1A and YTHDF2 interaction in NSCLC, contributing to our understanding of cancer progression.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary

      This revised manuscript investigates the role and the mechanism by which PDE1 impacts NSCLC progression. They provide evidence to demonstrate that PDE1 binds to m6A reader YTHDF2, in turn, regulating STAT3 signaling pathway through its interaction, promoting metastasis and angiogenesis.

      Strength:

      The study uncovers a novel PDE1A/YTHDF2/SOCS2/STAT3 pathway in NSCLC progression and the findings provide a potential treatment strategy for NSCLC patients with metastasis.

      Weakness:

      In discussion, it is stated in the revised version that "the role of YTHDF2 in PDE1A-driven tumor metastasis should be elucidated in future studies", however, given that physical interaction of PDE1A and YTHDF2 plays a critical role in PDE1A-mediated NSCLC metastasis, whether YTHDF2 mimicking the effect of PDE1A in metastasis will strength the manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) In Figure 1A, the y-axis should be "IOD/Area" instead of "IDO/Area".

      Figure 1A was revised as suggested.

      (2) Figure 3A legend for (F) and (G) was switched.

      Figure 3A legend was revised as suggested “(F-G) The mRNA (F) and protein (G) levels of indicated genes were determined in P3 and P0 NSCLC cells.”.

      (3) The statistical analysis should be performed for Figure 3H.

      Figure 3H was revised as suggested.

      (4) Figure 4F, Y-axis has a typo for "vessels" and statistical analysis should be performed on this data.

      Figure 4F was revised as suggested.

      (5) Figure 6 E, typo for "migrated" on the y-axis.

      Figure 6E was revised as suggested.

      (6) Figure 7 C, typos for "expression" on y-aixs in both figures need to be fixed.

      Figure 7C was revised as suggested.

      (7) P-values for Figure 7B need to be stated.

      Figure 7B was revised as suggested.

      (8) m6A should be consistent throughout the manuscript.

      m6A was consistent throughout the manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      IKK is the key signaling node for inflammatory signaling. Despite the availability of molecular structures, how the kinase achieves its specificity remains unclear. This paper describes a dynamic sequence of events in which autophosphorylation of a tyrosine near the activate site facilitates phosphorylation of the serine on the substrate via a phosphor-transfer reaction. The proposed mechanism is conceptually novel in several ways, suggesting that the kinase is dual specificity (tyrosine and serine) and that it mediates a phospho-transfer reaction. While bacteria contain phosphorylation-transfer enzymes, this is unheard of for mammalian kinases. However, what the functional significance of this enzymatic activity might remain unaddressed.

      The revised manuscript adequately addresses all the points I suggested in the review of the first submission.

      Response: Authors thank the reviewer for their valuable comments and constructive criticisms for the betterment of the manuscript. We also thank them for appreciating our work. We agree with the reviewer that the functional significance of this particular enzymatic activity of IKK2 is yet to be fully realized. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The authors investigate the phosphotransfer capacity of Ser/Thr kinase IkB kinase (IKK), a mediator of cellular inflammation signaling. Canonically, IKK activity is promoted by activation loop phosphorylation at Ser177/Ser181. Active IKK can then unleash NF-kB signaling by phosphorylating repressor IkBα at residues Ser32/Ser26. Noting the reports of other IKK phosphorylation sites, the authors explore the extent of autophosphorylation.

      Semi-phosphorylated IKK purified from Sf9 cells, exhibits the capacity for further autophosphorylation. Anti-phosphotyrosine immunoblotting indicated unexpected tyrosine phosphorylation. Contaminating kinase activity was tested by generating a kinase-dead K44M variant, supporting the notion that the unexpected phosphorylation was IKK-dependent. In addition, the observed phosphotyrosine signal required phosphorylated IKK activation loop serines.

      Two candidate IKK tyrosines were examined as the source of the phosphotyrosine immunoblotting signal. Activation loop residues Tyr169 and Tyr188 were each rendered non-phosphorylatable by mutation to Phe. The Tyr variants decreased both autophosphorylation and phosphotransfer to IkBα. Likewise, Y169F and Y188F IKK2 variants immunoprecipitated from TNFa-stimulated cells also exhibited reduced activity in vitro.

      The authors further focus on Tyr169 phosphorylation, proposing a role as a phospho-sink capable of phosphotransfer to IkBα substrate. This model is reminiscent of the bacterial two-component signaling phosphotransfer from phosphohistidine to aspartate. Efforts are made to phosphorylate IKK2 and remove ATP to assess the capacity for phosphotransfer. Phosphorylation of IkBα is observed after ATP removal, although there are ambiguous requirements for ADP.

      Strengths:

      Ultimately, the authors draw together the lines of evidence for IKK2 phosphotyrosine and ATP-independent phosphotransfer to develop a novel model for IKK2-mediated phosphorylation of IkBα. The model suggests that IKK activation loop Ser phosphorylation primes the kinase for tyrosine autophosphorylation. With the assumption that IKK retains the bound ADP, the phosphotyrosine is conformationally available to relay the phosphate to IkBα substrate. The authors are clearly aware of the high burden of evidence required for this unusual proposed mechanism. Indeed, many possible artifacts (e.g., contaminating kinases or ATP) are anticipated and control experiments are included to address many of these concerns. The analysis hinges on the fidelity of pan-specific phosphotyrosine antibodies, and the authors have probed with two different anti-phosphotyrosine antibody clones. Taken together, the observations are thought-provoking, and I look forward to seeing this model tested in a cellular system.

      Weaknesses:

      Multiple phosphorylated tyrosines in IKK2 were apparently identified by mass spectrometric analyses. LC-MS/MS spectra are presented, but fragments supporting phospho-Y188 and Y325 are difficult to distinguish from noise. It is common to find non-physiological post-translational modifications in over-expressed proteins from recombinant sources. Are these IKK2 phosphotyrosines evident by MS in IKK2 immunoprecipitated from TNFa-stimulated cells? Identifying IKK2 phosphotyrosine sites from cells would be especially helpful in supporting the proposed model.

      Authors thank the reviewer for their elaborate comments and constructive criticisms that helped enrich the manuscript. We also thank them for pointing out the critical points in the model. We agree with the reviewer that testing this model in a cellular system is required to bolster this concept. However, an appropriate cellular assay system to investigate and monitor this mode of phosphotransfer is still elusive. We agree with the reviewer’s concerns on the identification of Y188 and Y325 as potential phosphosites. They have been omitted in the current version and relevant changes have been incorporated. IKK2’s tyrosine phosphorylation status in cells is reported earlier. Although we have not analyzed IKK2 from TNF-a treated cells in this study, a different study of phospho-status of cellular IKK2 indicated tyrosine phosphorylation (Meyer et al 2013).   

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigate the kinase activity of IKK2, a crucial regulator of inflammatory cell signaling. They describe a novel tyrosine kinase activity of this well-studied enzyme and a highly unusual phosphotransfer from phosphorylated IKK2 onto substrate proteins in the absence of ATP as a substrate.

      Strengths:

      The authors provide an extensive biochemical characterization of the processes with recombinant protein, western blot, autoradiography, protein engineering and provide MS data now.

      Weaknesses:

      The identity and purity of the used proteins has improved in the revised work. Since the findings are so unexpected and potentially of wide-reaching interest - this is important. Similar specific detection of phospho-Ser/Thr vs phospho-Tyr relies largely on antibodies which can have varying degrees of specificity. Using multiple antibodies and MS improves the quality of the data.

      Authors thank the reviewer for their crisp comments and constructive criticisms that helped improve the manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Generally, the paper is well written, but the first 4 figures are slow going and could be condensed to show the key points, so that reader gets to Figure 6 and 7 which contain the "meat" of the paper.

      Specific points:

      Several figures should be quantified and experimental reproducibility is not always clear.

      I understand that Figure 3 shows that K44M abolishes both S32/26 phosphorylation and tyrosine phosphorylation, but not PEST region phosphorylation. This suggests that autophosphorylation is reflective of its known specific biological role in signal transduction. But I do not understand why "these results strongly suggest that IKK2-autophosphorylation is critical for its substrate specificity". That statement would be supported by a mutant that no longer autophosphorylates, and as a result shows a loss of substrate specificity, i.e. phosphorylates non-specific residues more strongly. Is that the case? Maybe Darwech et al 2010 or Meyer et al 2013 showed this? Later figures seem to address this point, so maybe this conclusion should be stated later in the paper.

      Page 10: mentions DFG+1 without proper introduction. The Chen et al 2014 paper appears to inform the author's interest in Y169 phosphorylation, or is just an additional interesting finding? Does this publication belong in the Introduction or the Discussion?

      To understand the significance of Figure 4D, we need a WT IKK2 control: or is there prior literature to cite?

      This is relevant for the conclusion that Y169 phosphorylation is particularly important for S32 phosphorylation.

      The cold ATP quenching experiment is nice for testing the model that Y169 functions as a phospho sink that allows for a transfer reaction. However, there is only a single timepoint and condition, which does not allow for a quantitative analysis. Furthermore, a positive control would make this experiment more compelling, and Y169F mutant should show that cold ATP quenching reduces the phosphorylation of IkBa.

      Note after revision: I thank the authors for addressing these points. The manuscript is thereby improved.

      We thank the reviewer for appreciating our efforts in addressing their concerns.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In the revisions, the authors provide LC-MS/MS spectra for putative phospho-Y325 and phospho-Y188. The details are hard to see at the scale provided, but the fragment ions for pY188 and pY325 peptides are unconvincing. Phospho-Y169, on the other hand, is much more credible. In addition, the revision rebuttal clarifies that Y188 would be packed into a catalytically important core, and Y188F is likely to disrupt the fold. Taken together, it seems doubtful that Y188 is subject to any significant autophosphorylation, and presenting the Y188F data (and discussion) seems like a distraction.

      We agree with the reviewer’s concerns on the identification of Y188 and Y325 as potential phosphosites. They have been omitted in the current version and relevant sections in the manuscript text and figures have been edited.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) Potential bleed-over across frequencies in the spectral domain is a major concern for all of the results in this paper. The fact that alpha power, 36Hz and 40Hz frequency-tagged amplitude and 4Hz intermodulation frequency power is generally correlated with one another amplifies this concern. The authors are attaching specific meaning to each of these frequencies, but perhaps there is simply a broadband increase in neural activity when anticipating an auditory target compared to a visual target?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment regarding the potential bleed-over across frequencies in the spectral domain. We fully acknowledge that the trade-off between temporal and frequency resolution is a challenge, particularly given the proximity of the frequencies we are examining.

      To address this concern, we performed additional analyses to investigate whether there is indeed a broadband increase in neural activity when anticipating an auditory target as compared to a visual target, as opposed to distinct frequency-specific effects. Our results show that the bleed-over between frequencies is minimal and does not significantly affect our findings. Specifically, we repeated the analyses using the same filter and processing steps for the 44 Hz frequency. At this frequency, we did not observe any significant differences between conditions.

      These findings suggest that the effects we report are indeed specific to the 40 Hz frequency band and not due to a general broadband increase in neural activity. We hope this addresses the reviewer’s concern and strengthens the validity of our frequency-specific results. We have now added this analysis to the methods section of our manuscript.

      Line 730: To confirm that 4 Hz is a sufficient distance between tagging frequencies, we repeated to analysis for 43.5 to 44.5. We found no indication of frequency-bleeding over, as the effects observed at 40 Hz, were not present at 44 Hz (see SUPPL Fig. 11).

      We do, however, not specifically argue against the possibility of a broadband increase in sensory processing when anticipating an auditory compared to a visual target. But even a broadband-increase would directly contradict the alpha inhibition hypothesis, which poses that an increase in alpha completely disengage the whole cortex. We have made this clearer in the text now.

      Line 491: As auditory targets were significantly more difficult than visual targets in our first study and of comparable difficulty in our second study, these results strongly speak to a vigilance increase of sensory processing independent of modality and an inability to selectively disengage one sensory modality in anticipation of a demanding task. This view is consistent with previous work in which visual SSEPs elicited by irrelevant background stimulation increased with task load in an auditory discrimination task (Jacoby et al., 2012).

      (2) Moreover, 36Hz visual and 40Hz auditory signals are expected to be filtered in the neocortex. Applying standard filters and Hilbert transform to estimate sensory evoked potentials appears to rely on huge assumptions that are not fully substantiated in this paper. In Figure 4, 36Hz "visual" and 40Hz "auditory" signals seem largely indistinguishable from one another, suggesting that the analysis failed to fully demix these signals.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful concern regarding the filtering and demixing of the 36 Hz visual and 40 Hz auditory signals, and we share the same reservations about the reliance on standard filters and the Hilbert transform method.

      To address this, we would like to draw attention to SUPPL Fig. 11, which demonstrates that a 4 Hz difference is sufficient to effectively demix the signals using our chosen filtering and Hilbert transform approach. We argue that the reason the 36 Hz visual and 40 Hz auditory signals show similar topographies lies not in incomplete demixing but rather in the possibility that this condition difference reflects sensory integration, rather than signal contamination.

      This interpretation is further supported by our findings with the intermodulation frequency at 4 Hz, which also suggests cross-modal integration. Furthermore, source localization analysis revealed that the strongest condition differences were observed in the precuneus, an area frequently associated with sensory integration processes. We have now expanded on this in the discussion section to better clarify this point.

      Line 578: Previous research has shown that simultaneous frequency-tagging at multiple frequencies can evoke a response at the intermodulation frequency (f1 – f2), which in multimodal settings is thought to reflect cross-modal integration (Drijvers et al., 2021). This concept aligns closely with our findings, where increased vigilance in the sensory system, prompted by anticipation of a difficult auditory target, resulted in an increase in the intermodulation frequency. Similarly, our data shows that visual signal enhancement was localized in the precuneus, further supporting the role of this region in sensory integration (Al-Ramadhani et al., 2021; Xie et al., 2019).

      (3) The asymmetric results in the visual and auditory modalities preclude a modality-general conclusion about the function of alpha. However, much of the language seems to generalize across sensory modalities (e.g., use of the term 'sensory' rather than 'visual').

      We agree that in some cases we have not made a sufficient distinction between visual and sensory. We have now made sure, that when using ‘sensory’, we either describe overall theories, which are not visual-exclusive or refer to the possibility of a broad sensory increase. However, when directly discussing our results and the interpretation thereof, we now use ‘visual’.

      (4) In this vein, some of the conclusions would be far more convincing if there was at least a trend towards symmetry in source-localized analyses of MEG signals. For example, how does alpha power in primary auditory cortex (A1) compare when anticipating auditory vs visual target? What do the frequency tagged visual and auditory responses look like when just looking at primary visual cortex (V1) or A1?

      We thank the reviewer for this important suggestion and have added a virtual channel analysis. We were however, not interested in alpha power in primary auditory cortex, as we were specifically interested in the posterior alpha, which is usually increased when expecting an auditory compared to a visual target (and used to be interpreted as a blanket inhibition of the visual cortex). We have now improved upon the clarity concerning this point in the manuscript.

      We have however, followed the reviewer’s suggestion of a virtual channel analysis, showing that the condition differences are not observable in primary visual cortex for the 36 Hz visual signal and in primary auditory cortex for the 40 Hz auditory signal. Our data clearly shows that there is an alpha condition difference in V1, while there no condition difference for 36 Hz in V1 and for 40 Hz in Heschl’s Gyrus.

      Line 356: Additionally, we replicated this effect with a virtual channel analysis in V1 (see SUPPL Fig. 12)

      Line 403: Furthermore, a virtual channel analysis in V1 and Heschl’s gyrus confirmed that there were no condition differences in primary visual and auditory areas (see SUPPL Fig. 12).

      (5) Blinking would have a huge impact on the subject's ability to ignore the visual distractor. The best thing to do would be to exclude from analysis all trials where the subjects blinked during the cue-to-target interval. The authors mention that in the MEG experiment, "To remove blinks, trials with very large eye-movements (> 10 degrees of visual angle) were removed from the data (See supplement Fig. 5)." This sentence needs to be clarified, since eye-movements cannot be measured during blinking. In addition, it seems possible to remove putative blink trials from EEG experiments as well, since blinks can be detected in the EEG signals.

      We agree with the reviewer that this point has been phrased in a confusing way. From the MEG-data, we removed eyeblinks using ICA. Along for the supplementary Fig. 5 analysis, we used the eye-tracking data to make sure that participants were in fact fixating the centre of the screen. For this analysis, we removed trials with blinks (which can be seen in the eye-tracker as huge amplitude movements or as large eye-movements in degrees of visual angle; see figure below to show a blink in the MEG data and the according eye-tracker data in degrees of visual angle). We have now clarified this in the methods section.

      As for the concern closed eyes to ignore visual distractors, in both experiments we can observe highly significant distractor cost in accuracy for visual distractors, which we hope will convince the reviewer that our visual distractors were working as intended.

      Author response image 1.

      Illustration of eye-tracker data for a trial without and a trial with a blink. All data points recorded during this trial are plottet. A, ICA component 1, which reflects blinks and its according data trace in a trial. No blink is visible. B, eye-tracker data transformed into degrees of visual angle for the trial depicted in A. C, ICA component 1, which reflects blinks and its according data trace in a trial. A clear blink is visible. D, eye-tracker data transformed into degrees of visual angle for the trial depicted in C.

      Line 676: To confirm that participants had focused on the fixation cross during the cue-to-target interval, we incorporated eye-tracking into our MEG-experiment (EyeLink 1000 Plus). Correct trials of the second block were analysed for vertical and horizontal eye-movements. To exclude blinks from this analysis, trials with very large eye-movements (> 10 degrees of visual angle) were removed from the eye-tracking data (See suppl Fig. 5).

      (6) It would be interesting to examine the neutral cue trials in this task. For example, comparing auditory vs visual vs neutral cue conditions would be indicative of whether alpha was actively recruited or actively suppressed. In addition, comparing spectral activity during cue-to-target period on neutral-cue auditory correct vs incorrect trials should mimic the comparison of auditory-cue vs visual-cue trials. Likewise, neutral-cue visual correct vs incorrect trials should mimic the attention-related differences in visual-cue vs auditory-cue trials.

      We have analysed the neutral cue trials in the EEG dataset (see suppl. Fig. 1). There were no significant differences to auditory or visual cues, but descriptively alpha power was higher for neutral cues compared to visual cues and lower for neutral cues compared to auditory cues. While this may suggest that for visual trials alpha is actively suppressed and for auditory trials actively recruited, we do not feel comfortable to make this claim, as the neutral condition may not reflect a completely neutral state. The neutral task can still be difficult, especially because of the uncertainty of the target modality.

      As for the analysis of incorrect versus correct trials, we appreciate the idea, but unfortunately the accuracy rate was quite high so that the number of incorrect trials is insufficient to perform a reliable analysis.

      (7) In the abstract, the authors state that "This implies that alpha modulation does not solely regulate 'gain control' in early sensory areas but rather orchestrates signal transmission to later stages of the processing stream." However, I don't see any supporting evidence for the latter claim, that alpha orchestrates signal transmission to later stages of the processing stream. If the authors are claiming an alternative function to alpha, this claim should be strongly substantiated.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out, that we have not sufficiently explained our case. The first point refers to gain control as elucidated by the alpha inhibition hypothesis, which claims that increases in alpha disengage an entire cortical area. Since we have confirmed the alpha increase in our data to originate from primary visual cortex through source analysis, this should lead to decreased visual processing. The increase in 36 Hz visual processing therefore directly contradicts the alpha inhibition hypothesis. We propose an alternative explanation for the functionality of alpha activity in this task. Through pulsed inhibition, information packages of relevant visual information could be transmitted down the processing stream, thereby enhancing relevant visual signal transmission. We argue the fact that the enhanced visual 36 Hz signal we found correlated with visual alpha power on a trial-by-trial basis, and did not originate from primary visual cortex, but from areas known for sensory integration supports our claim.

      We have now tried to make this point clearer by rephrasing our manuscript. Additionally, we have also now further clarified this point in our discussion.

      Line 527: Our data provides evidence in favour of this view, as we can show that early sensory alpha activity covaries over trials with SSEP magnitude in higher order sensory areas. If alpha activity exerted gain control in early visual regions, increased alpha activity would have to lead to a decrease in SSEP responses. In contrast, we observe that increased alpha activity originating from early visual cortex is related to enhanced visual processing. Source localization confirmed that this enhancement was not originating from early visual areas, but from areas associated with later stages of the processing stream such as the precuneus, which has been connected to sensory integration (Al-Ramadhani et al., 2021; Xie et al., 2019). While we cannot completely rule out alternative explanations, it seems plausible to assume that inhibition of other task-irrelevant communication pathways leads to prioritised and thereby enhanced processing over relevant pathways. In line with previous literature (Morrow et al., 2023; Peylo et al., 2021; Zhigalov & Jensen, 2020b), we therefore suggest that alpha activity limits task-irrelevant feedforward communication, thereby enhancing processing capabilities in relevant downstream areas (see Fig. 1A).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):Minor Concerns:

      (1) I suggest adding more details about the task in the Results and/or Figure 1 legend. Specifically, when describing the task, I think it would help the readers if the authors specified what the participants had to do to get a trial correct (e.g., press left / down / right arrow if the tone pitch was low (500Hz) / medium (1000Hz) / high (2000Hz).)

      (2) Please clarify whether Gaboar patch was drifting.

      (3) Figure 2C-D: I suggest clarifying in the X-tick labels that + and - trials are in separate blocks (e.g., put 'Block1 visual-' instead of 'visual-').

      We followed the suggestions of the reviewer detailed in point 1-3, which indeed greatly improves the clarity and readability of these parts.

      (4) "Interestingly, auditory distractors reduced reaction times to visual targets, which could be explained by a generally faster processing of auditory targets (Jain et al., 2015), possibly probing faster responses in visual tasks (Naue et al., 2011)." - Please elaborate on how faster processing of auditory targets could lead to the probing of faster responses in visual tasks. Further, if I understand correctly, this should result in a speed-accuracy trade-off, which is not observed in the MEG experiments. If there is a learning effect due to the blocked structure in the MEG experiments, why is it not observed on auditory trials?

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting clarifying this paragraph. We have now rephrased this part and added additional information.

      Concerning the reviewer’s theory, intersensory facilitation can occur in the absence of a speed-accuracy trade-off, as it can affect the motor execution after a decision has been made. Nevertheless, learning effects could also have led to this result in the MEG experiment. Our difficulty calibration did not lead to comparable accuracies in block 1, where auditory targets wetre now less difficult than visual targets. Whith the addition of distractors in block 2, accuracy for auditory targets decreased, while it increased for visual targets. Indeed, one interpretation could be that there was a learning effect for visual targets, which was not prevalent for auditory targets. However, the speed increase when visual targets are coupled with auditory distractors is prevalent in both experiments. Accordingly, we find the intersensory facilitation account more likely.

      line 148: Interestingly, auditory distractors reduced reaction times to visual targets, which could be explained by a generally faster processing of auditory targets (Jain et al., 2015). As such, the auditory distractor possibly caused intersensory facilitation (Nickerson., 1973), whereby reaction times to a target can be facilitated when accompanied by stimuli of other sensory modalities, even if they are irrelevant or distracting.

      (5) Please briefly describe the cluster permutation analysis in the results section.

      We have now added a brief description of the cluster permutation analysis we performed in the results section.

      Line 166: We then applied cluster permutation analysis, whereby real condition differences were tested against coincidental findings by randomly permutating the condition labels to the data and testing for condition differences 1000 times (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007).

      (6) Figure 4A legend: "auditory steady-state evoked potential (ASSEP) averaged over 6 central electrodes displaying the highest 40 Hz power (Fz, FC1, FC2, F11, F2, FCz)." - I suggest marking these 6 electrodes in the scalp map on the figure panel.

      We have followed the suggestion of the reviewer and marked the electrodes/sensors used to illustrate the steady-state responses.

      (7) Lines 281-283: "It was highly significant for the visual 36 Hz response (Fig. 5A, middle columns, p = .033; t(19) = 2.29; BF(10) = 1.91) but did not reach significance for the visual 40 Hz response (Fig. 5B, middle column; p = 0.20; t(19) = 1.32; BF(10) = 0.49)." - Was "visual 40Hz response" a typo? I believe 40Hz pertains to auditory, not visual?

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this error and agree that the phrasing was sometimes confusing. We have now used the terms VSSEP and ASSEP to make things clearer throughout the manuscript.

      L. 224-229: The median split was highly significant for the 36 Hz VSSEP response (Fig. 5A, middle columns, p \= .033; t<sub>(19)</sub> = 2.29; BF<sub>(10)</sub> = 1.91) but did not reach significance for the 40 Hz ASSEP response (Fig. 5B, middle column; p = 0.20; t<sub>(19)</sub> = 1.32; BF<sub>(10)</sub> = 0.49).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Brickwedde et al. investigate the role of alpha oscillations in allocating intermodal attention. A first EEG study is followed up with an MEG study that largely replicates the pattern of results (with small to be expected differences). They conclude that a brief increase in the amplitude of auditory and visual stimulus-driven continuous (steady-state) brain responses prior to the presentation of an auditory - but not visual - target speaks to the modulating role of alpha that leads them to revise a prevalent model of gating-by-inhibition.

      Overall, this is an interesting study on a timely question, conducted with methods and analysis that are state-of-the-art. I am particularly impressed by the author's decision to replicate the earlier EEG experiment in MEG following the reviewer's comments on the original submission. Evidently, great care was taken to accommodate the reviewers suggestions.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive feedback and expression of interest in the topic of our manuscript.

      Nevertheless, I am struggling with the report for two main reasons: It is difficult to follow the rationale of the study, due to structural issues with the narrative and missing information or justifications for design and analysis decisions, and I am not convinced that the evidence is strong, or even relevant enough for revising the mentioned alpha inhibition theory. Both points are detailed further below.

      We have now revised major parts of the introduction and results in line with the reviewer’s suggestions, hoping that our rationale is now easier to follow and that our evidence will now be more convincing. We have separated our results section into the first study (EEG) and to second study (MEG), to enhance the rationale of our design choices and readability. We have clarified all mentioned ambiguous parts in our methods section. Additionally, we have revised the introduction to now explain more clearly what results to expect under the alpha inhibition theory in contrast to our alternative account.

      Strength/relevance of evidence for model revision: The main argument rests on 1) a rather sustained alpha effect following the modality cue, 2) a rather transient effect on steady-state responses just before the expected presentation of a stimulus, and 3) a correlation between those two. Wouldn't the authors expect a sustained effect on sensory processing, as measured by steady-state amplitude irrespective of which of the scenarios described in Figure 1A (original vs revised alpha inhibition theory) applies? Also, doesn't this speak to the role of expectation effects due to consistent stimulus timing? An alternative explanation for the results may look like this: Modality-general increased steady-state responses prior to the expected audio stimulus onset are due to increased attention/vigilance. This effect may be exclusive (or more pronounced) in the attend-audio condition due to higher precision in temporal processing in the auditory sense or, vice versa, too smeared in time due to the inferior temporal resolution of visual processing for the attend-vision condition to be picked up consistently. As expectation effects will build up over the course of the experiment, i.e., while the participant is learning about the consistent stimulus timing, the correlation with alpha power may then be explained by a similar but potentially unrelated increase in alpha power over time.

      We thank the reviewer for raising these insightful questions and suggestions.

      It is true that our argument rests on a rather sustained alpha effect and a rather transient effect on steady-state responses ,and a correlation between the two. However, this connection would not be expected under the alpha inhibition hypothesis, which states that alpha activity would inhibit a whole cortical area (when irrelevant to the task), exerting “gain control”. This notion directly contradicts our results of the “irrelevant” visual information a) being transmitted at all and b) increasing.

      However, it has been shown in various reports (see for instance Dugué et al., 2011; Haegens et al., 2011; Spaak et al., 2012) that alpha activity exerts pulsed inhibition, so we proposed an alternative theory of an involvement in signal transmission. In this case, the cyclic inhibition would serve as an ordering system, which only allows for high-priority information to pass, resulting in higher signal-to-noise ratio. We do not make a claim about how fast or when these signals are transmitted in relation to alpha power. For instance, it could be that alpha power increases as a preparatory state even before signal is actually transmitted.  Zhigalov (2020 Hum. Brain M.) has shown that in V1, frequency-tagging responses were up-and down regulated with attention – independent of alpha activity.

      However, we do believe that visual alpha power correlates on a trial-by-trial level with visual 36 Hz frequency-tagging increases (see Fig. 5 and 10 in our manuscript) - a relationship which has not been found in V1 by us and others (see SUPPL Fig. 12 and Zhigalov 2020, Hum. Brain Mapp.) suggest a strong connection. Furthermore, the fact that the alpha modulation originates from early visual areas and occurs prior to any frequency-tagging changes, while the increase in frequency-tagging can be observed in areas which are later in the processing stream (such as the precuneus) is strongly indicative for an involvement of alpha power in the transmission of this signal. We cannot fully exclude alternative accounts and mechanisms which effect both alpha power and frequency-tagging responses.  

      The alternative account described by the reviewer does not contradict our theory, as we argue that the alpha power modulation reflects an expectation effect (and the idea that it could be related to the resolution of auditory versus visual processing is very interesting!). It is also possible that this expectation is, as the reviewer suggests, related to attention/vigilance and might result in a modality-general signal increase. By way of support, we observed an increase in the frequency-tagging response in sensory integration areas. Accordingly, we argue that the alternative explanation provided by the reviewer contradicts the alpha inhibition hypothesis, but not necessarily our alternative theory.

      We have now revised the discussion and are confident our case is now stronger and easier to follow. Additionally, we mentioned the possibility for alternative explanations as well as the possibility, that alpha networks fulfil different roles in different locations/task environments.

      Line 523: Here we propose that alpha activity, rather than modulating early primary sensory processing, exhibits its inhibitory effects at later stages of the processing stream (Antonov et al., 2020; Gundlach et al., 2020; Zhigalov & Jensen, 2020a; Zumer et al., 2014), gating feedforward or feedback communication between sensory areas (Bauer et al., 2020; Haegens et al., 2015; Uemura et al., 2021). Our data provides evidence in favour of this view, as we can show that early sensory alpha activity covaries over trials with SSEP magnitude in higher order sensory areas. If alpha activity exerted gain control in early visual regions, increased alpha activity would have to lead to a decrease in SSEP responses. In contrast, we observe that increased alpha activity originating from early visual cortex is related to enhanced visual processing. Source localization confirmed that this enhancement was not originating from early visual areas, but from areas associated with later stages of the processing stream such as the precuneus, which has been connected to sensory integration (Al-Ramadhani et al., 2021; Xie et al., 2019). While we cannot completely rule out alternative explanations, it seems plausible to assume that inhibition of other task-irrelevant communication pathways leads to prioritised and thereby enhanced processing over relevant pathways. In line with previous literature (Morrow et al., 2023; Peylo et al., 2021; Zhigalov & Jensen, 2020b), we therefore suggest that alpha activity limits task-irrelevant feedforward communication, thereby enhancing processing capabilities in relevant downstream areas (see Fig. 1A).

      References:

      Dugué, L., Marque, P., & VanRullen, R. (2011). The phase of ongoing oscillations mediates the causal relation between brain excitation and visual perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(33), 11889–11893. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1161-11.2011

      Haegens, S., Nácher, V., Luna, R., Romo, R., & Jensen, O. (2011). α-Oscillations in the monkey sensorimotor network influence discrimination performance by rhythmical inhibition of neuronal spiking. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(48), 19377–19382. https://doi.org/10.1073/PNAS.1117190108

      Spaak, E., Bonnefond, M., Maier, A., Leopold, D. A., & Jensen, O. (2012). Layer-Specific Entrainment of Gamma-Band Neural Activity by the Alpha Rhythm in Monkey Visual Cortex. Current Biology, 22(24), 2313–2318. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.CUB.2012.10.020

      Zhigalov, A., & Jensen, O. (2020). Alpha oscillations do not implement gain control in early visual cortex but rather gating in parieto-occipital regions. Human Brain Mapping, 41(18), 5176–5186. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25183

      Structural issues with the narrative and missing information: Here, I am mostly concerned with how this makes the research difficult to access for the reader. I list the some major, followed by more specific points below:

      In the introduction the authors pit the original idea about alpha's role in gating against some recent contradictory results. If it's the aim of the study to provide evidence for either/or, predictions for the results from each perspective are missing. Also, it remains unclear how this relates to the distinction between original vs revised alpha inhibition theory (Fig. 1A). Relatedly, if this revision is an outcome rather than a postulation for this study, it shouldn't be featured in the first figure.

      We agree with the reviewer that we have not sufficiently clarified our goal as well as how different functionalities of alpha oscillations would lead to different outcomes. We have revised the introduction and restructured the results part and hope that it is now easier to follow. The results part now follows study 1 (EEG) and study 2 (MEG) chronologically, so that results can more easily be differentiated and our design choices for the second study can be explained better.

      Line 50: Recent evidence challenged a direct connection between alpha activity and visual information processing in early visual cortex. As such, both visual steady-state responses and alpha power were modulated by attention, but did not covary when investigating individual trials (Zhigalov & Jensen, 2020). Unfortunately, very few studies have investigated direct connections between alpha activity, attention and sensory signals, especially over trials. Furthermore, results seem to depend on timing of alpha activity in relation to sensory responses as well as stimulus type and outcome measure (Morrow et al., 2023).

      Accordingly, the objective of the current study is to test the alpha inhibition hypothesis compared to an alternative theory. Based on the alpha inhibition hypothesis, alpha modulation is connected to ‘gain control’ in early visual areas through modulation of excitability (Foxe & Snyder, 2011; Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010; Van Diepen et al., 2019).  In contrast, we propose that inhibitory effects of alpha modulation are exhibited at later stages of the processing stream (Peylo et al., 2021; Yang et al., 2023; Zhigalov & Jensen, 2020a; Zumer et al., 2014), gating feedforward or feedback communication between sensory areas (see Fig. 1B; Bauer et al., 2020; Haegens et al., 2015; Uemura et al., 2021).

      Line 80: The aim of our study was to directly test the alpha inhibition hypothesis by investigating if cue-induced modulation of alpha activity coincides with the suppression of frequency-tagging responses in task-irrelevant modalities.

      Line 99: In brief, while we observed the expected cue-induced early-visual alpha modulation, the amplitude of auditory and visual SSEP/SSEFs as well as their intermodulation frequency increased just prior to the onset of the auditory target, contradicting the alpha inhibition hypothesis. The difference between conditions of visual SSEP/SSEFs originated from sensory integration areas and correlated with early sensory alpha activity on a trial-by-trial basis, speaking to an effect of alpha modulation on signal transmission rather than inhibition of early visual areas.

      The analysis of the intermodulation frequency makes a surprise entrance at the end of the Results section without an introduction as to its relevance for the study. This is provided only in the discussion, but with reference to multisensory integration, whereas the main focus of the study is focussed attention on one sense. (Relatedly, the reference to "theta oscillations" in this sections seems unclear without a reference to the overlapping frequency range, and potentially more explanation.) Overall, if there's no immediate relevance to this analysis, I would suggest removing it.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out and have now added information about this frequency to the introduction. We believe that the intermodulation frequency analysis is important, as it potentially supports the notion that condition differences in the visual-frequency tagging response are related to downstream processing rather than overall visual information processing in V1. We would therefore prefer to leave this analysis in the manuscript.

      Line 75: Furthermore, when applying two different frequencies for two different sensory modalities, their intermodulation frequency (f1-f2) has been suggested to reflect cross-modal integration (Drijvers et al., 2021). Due to distinct responses, localisation and attention-dependence, frequency-tagging provides an optimal tool to study sensory signal processing and integration over time.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      As detailed in several points below, I found that I didn't get the information I needed to fully understand design/analysis decisions. In some cases, this may just be a case of re-organising the manuscript, in others crucial info should be added:

      Specific issues:

      Page 2, line 51: How does recent evidence contradict this? Please explain.

      We have added a section that describes the results contradicting the alpha inhibition hypothesis.

      Line 50: Recent evidence challenged a direct connection between alpha activity and visual information processing in early visual cortex. As such, both visual steady-state responses and alpha power were modulated by attention, but did not covary when investigating individual trials (Zhigalov & Jensen, 2020).

      Page 3, line 78-80: "... also interested in relationships [...] on a trial-by-trial basis" - why? Please motivate.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting this section, which we feel was not very well phrased. We have rewritten this whole paragraph and hope that our motivation for this study is now clear.

      Line 50: Recent evidence challenged a direct connection between alpha activity and visual information processing in early visual cortex. As such, both visual steady-state responses and alpha power were modulated by attention, but did not covary when investigating individual trials (Zhigalov & Jensen, 2020). Unfortunately, very few studies have investigated direct connections between alpha activity, attention and sensory signals, especially over trials. Furthermore, results seem to depend on timing of alpha activity in relation to sensory responses as well as stimulus type and outcome measure (Morrow et al., 2023).

      Page 4, line 88-92: "... implementing a blocked design" - unclear why? This is explained to some extent in the next few lines but remains unclear without knowing outcomes of the EEG experiment with more detail. Overall, it seems like this methodological detail may be better suited for a narrative in the Results section, that follows a more chronological order from the findings of the EEG experiment to the design of the MEG study.

      More generally, and maybe I missed it, I couldn't find a full account of why a block design was chosen and what the added value was. I believe that re-organising the Results section would allow precisely stating how that was an improvement over the EEG experiment.

      In line with the reviewer’s suggestion, we have now restructured the results section. The first section of the study 2 results now explains our design choices with direct reference to the results of the EEG experiment.

      Line 298: To test the robustness of our results and to employ additional control analyses, we replicated our experiment using MEG (see Fig. 7A). While an increase in visual information processing parallel to an increase in alpha modulation already contradicts the notion of alpha inhibition exerting “gain control”, affecting the whole visual cortex, our claim that alpha modulation instead affects visual information at later processing stages still required further validation. As such, our goal was to perform source analyses showing alpha modulation originating from primary visual areas affected visual information at later processing stages (e.g. not in primary visual cortex). Additionally, to exclude that the uncertainty over possible distractors affected our results, we employed a block design, where block 1 consisted only of trials without distractors and in block 2 targets were always accompanied by a distractor. Furthermore, we aligned the visual and auditory task to be more similar, both of them now featuring frequency-discrimination, which related to sound pitch (frequency) in the auditory condition and stripe-frequency of the Gabor patch in the visual condition. Lastly, to make sure our effects were driven by sensory modality-differences rather than task-difficulty differences, we included a short calibration phase. Prior to the experiment, difficulty of pitch sounds, and Gabor patch frequency were calibrated for each individual, ascertaining a success rate between 55% to 75%.

      The point above also applies to lines 95-97 where it's unclear what "aligning the visual with the auditory task" means. Also, what would be the predictions for "more nuanced interactions [...]"

      We agree that this phrasing was more than confusing and in the process of restructuring our results section, we have now revised this passage (see cited text from our manuscript to the point just above).

      Page 9, line 207-209: One of the few mentions of the "ambivalent" condition (attention to audio+vision?). To what end was that condition added to the experiment originally? The explanation that this condition was dropped from analysis because it did not show significant results does not seem methodologically sound.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out, as we had changed the name from ambivalent to non-specific, but this word had slipped our attention. The condition was added to the experiment as a control, which enables us to verify that our cues as well as our distractors work as intended. While interesting to analyse (and we did not drop it completely, the condition comparisons are in the supplementary material), we felt that further analysis of this condition would not contribute to addressing our research question. To be specific, the prerequisite to analysing the effect of alpha modulation is a significant effect of alpha modulation in the first place. We have now clarified the rationale for this condition, as well as our reasoning for omitting it from correlation and source analysis.

      Line 173 When presenting unspecified cues, alpha power changes were not significant, but descriptively larger compared to visual target conditions and lower compared to auditory target conditions (see suppl Fig. 2). However as significant alpha modulation was a prerequisite to test our hypotheses, we excluded this condition from further analysis.

      Page 9, line 209-212: "condition differences in alpha were only significant in block 2 [...] therefore we performed the [...] analysis [...] only for the second half of the experiment." This sounds like double-dipping. Maybe just an issue of phrasing?

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out that it may appear like ‘double dipping’. The reasoning was the same as the point above, we require a significant alpha modulation to test the effect of alpha modulation on further processing. We have revised this part to be clearer.

      Line 345: In line with previous studies (van Diepen & Mazaheri, 2017), condition differences in alpha activity were only significant in block 2, where distractors were present. As alpha modulation was a prerequisite to test our hypotheses, we performed the following analyses solely with data from block 2 (see Fig. 8).

      Page 12, line 281: Bayes factors are used here (and elsewhere), in addition to NHST. May be worthwhile to mention that briefly before use and give an intro sentence on its use, value and interpretation, and why these are added sometimes but not for all tests reported.

      We agree that we did not introduce this at all and have now added a section, which explains the inclusion as well as the interpretation of the Bayes factor.

      Line 218: To estimate the robustness of these results, we additionally conducted median split analyses between trials with high and low alpha power for each participant, as well as averaged the correlation coefficient of each participant and calculated a one-sample t-test against 0. For each analysis we provided the Bayes Factor, which estimates the strength of support for or against the null hypothesis (BF > 3.2 is considered as substantial evidence and BF > 10 is considered as strong evidence; Kass & Raftery, 1995).

      Throughout the Results section, it's not always clear which results are from the EEG or from the MEG study. Adopting the recommendation in point c) may help with that.

      According to the reviewer’s recommendation, we have restructured our results section and first present the EEG study and afterwards the MEG study.

      Similarly, it seems pivotal to add "visual" and "auditory" when mentioning the 36/40-Hz steady-state responses (or stimulation) to help the reader.

      We agree that visual/auditory 36 Hz / 40 Hz frequency-tagging responses, expecting visual/auditory target becomes lengthy and confusing very quickly. We therefore decided to introduce the abbreviation of visual steady-state evoked potentials/fields (VSSEP/VSSEF) and auditory steady-state evoked potentials/fields (ASSEP/ASSEF).

      Figure 5 - showing the same cluster as "early" and "late" in the margin for the MEG data is potentially confusing.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out and have now adapted the figure to just show one cluster, as we only found this one cluster in our MEG analysis.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This paper seems very strong, particularly given that the follow-up MEG study both (a) clarifies the task design and separates the effect of distractor stimuli into other experimental blocks, and (b) provides source-localization data to more concretely address whether alpha inhibition is occurring at or after the level of sensory processing, and (c) replicates most of the EEG study's key findings.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive feedback and evaluation of our work.

      There are some points that would be helpful to address to bolster the paper. First, the introduction would benefit from a somewhat deeper review of the literature, not just reviewing when the effects of alpha seem to occur, but also addressing how the effect can change depending on task and stimulus design (see review by Morrow, Elias & Samaha (2023).

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion and agree. We have now added a paragraph to the introduction that refers to missing correlation studies and the impact of task design.

      Line 53: Unfortunately, very few studies have investigated direct connections between alpha activity, attention and sensory signals, especially over trials. Furthermore, results seem to depend on timing of alpha activity in relation to sensory responses as well as stimulus type and outcome measure (Morrow et al., 2023).

      Additionally, the discussion could benefit from more cautionary language around the revision of the alpha inhibition account. For example, it would be helpful to address some of the possible discrepancies between alpha and SSEP measures in terms of temporal specificity, SNR, etc. (see Peylo, Hilla, & Sauseng, 2021). The authors do a good job speculating as to why they found differing results from previous cross-modal attention studies, but I'm also curious whether the authors think that alpha inhibition/modulation of sensory signals would have been different had the distractors been within the same modality or whether the cues indicated target location, rather than just modality, as has been the case in so much prior work?

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting these interesting discussion points and have included a paragraph in our discussion that clarifies these issues.

      Line 543: It should be noted, the comparison between modulation in alpha activity and in SSEP/SSEFs is difficult, especially concerning timing. This is largely owed to differences in signal-to-noise due to trial averaging in the frequency versus the time domain and temporal and frequency lag in the estimation of alpha activity (Peylo et al., 2021). It is further noteworthy, that the majority of evidence for the alpha inhibition hypothesis focused on the effect of pre-target alpha modulation on behaviour and target-related potentials (Morrow et al., 2023). However, in our data alpha modulation occurs clearly ahead of SSVEP/SSVEF modulation on a scale that could not be simply explained by temporal or frequency smearing. Additionally, significant trial-by-trial correlations, which occur in the frequency domain for both signal types, underline the strong relationship between both measurements.

      Interestingly, we could show that the magnitude of the correlation between alpha power and visual information processing varied between conditions, suggesting a dynamic and adaptive regime. This notion supports the view that alpha oscillations represent a mechanism rather than a specific function, which can fulfil different roles depending on task demand and network location, which has been confirmed in a recent study revealing functionally distinct alpha networks (Clausner et al., 2024). As such, it is conceivable that alpha oscillations can in some cases inhibit local processing, while in other cases, depending on network location, connectivity and demand, alpha oscillation can facilitate signal transmission. In different contexts, utilizing unimodal targets and distractors, spatial cueing, or covert attention, different functional processes could be involved (Morrow et al., 2023). Future research should intensify efforts to disentangle these effects, investigating localized alpha networks intracranially or through combinations of fMRI, EEG and MEG, to clearly measure their effects on sensory processing and behaviour.

      Overall, the analyses and discussion are quite comprehensive, and I believe this paper to be an excellent contribution to the alpha-inhibition literature.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Overall, the paper is well-written, and the analyses and interpretations are strong. I think that the end of the introduction would feel more complete and more read more easily if you outlined all of your main hypotheses (not just trials signaling an auditory stimulus, but visual trials too, and what about distractor trials? This could help justify changes to task design in the MEG study), and then the key findings that motivated the follow-up design, which you then discuss (as opposed to introducing a new aim in this paragraph).

      We thank the reviewer for this positive evaluation. Based on feedback und suggestions from all reviewers, we have revised the structure of the manuscript. The introduction now states more clearly which results would be expected under the alpha inhibition theory and how our results contradict this. The results section has now been divided into two studies, which will make the rationale for our follow-up design easier to follow.

      Line 80: The aim of our study was to directly test the alpha inhibition hypothesis by investigating if cue-induced modulation of alpha activity coincides with the suppression of frequency-tagging responses in task-irrelevant modalities.

      Line 96: In brief, while we observed the expected cue-induced early-visual alpha modulation, the amplitude of auditory and visual SSEP/SSEFs as well as their intermodulation frequency increased just prior to the onset of the auditory target, contradicting the alpha inhibition hypothesis. The difference between conditions of visual SSEP/SSEFs originated from sensory integration areas and correlated with early sensory alpha activity on a trial-by-trial basis, speaking to an effect of alpha modulation on signal transmission rather than inhibition of early visual areas.

      Minor issues:

      L84 - "is" should be "was"

      L93 - "allows" should be "allowed"

      L113 - I think "changed" would suffice

      Fig 1A (text within figure on top) - "erea" should be "area" and caption title should include "of" (Illustration of the...)

      L213 - time window could be clarified

      Fig 4 -captions inconsistently capitalize words and use ) and , following the caption letters

      L253-255 - give you are looking at condition differences, do you mean the response was larger before an auditory target than before a visual target? It currently reads as if you mean that it was larger in that window right before the target as opposed to other time windows

      L368 - "behaviorally" should be "behavioral"

      L407-408 - I think auditory SSEP/SSVEFs should be auditory or visual SSEP/SSEFs, unless you are specifically only talking about auditory SSEPs and visual SSEFs

      L411 - also uses SSVEFs

      L413 - "frequently, or in the case of..."

      L555 - "predicting" should be predicted? Or do you mean only cues that correctly predicted the target?

      We are very grateful for the reviewer for pointing out these mistakes, all of which we have remedied in our manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Hearing and balance rely on specialized ribbon synapses that transmit sensory stimuli between hair cells and afferent neurons. Synaptic adhesion molecules that form and regulate transsynaptic interactions between inner hair cells (IHCs) and spiral ganglion neurons (SGNs) are crucial for maintaining auditory synaptic integrity and, consequently, for auditory signaling. Synaptic adhesion molecules such as neurexin-3 and neuroligin-1 and -3 have recently been shown to play vital roles in establishing and maintaining these synaptic connections ( doi: 10.1242/dev.202723 and DOI: 10.1016/j.isci.2022.104803). However, the full set of molecules required for synapse assembly remains unclear.

      Karagulan et al. highlight the critical role of the synaptic adhesion molecule RTN4RL2 in the development and function of auditory afferent synapses between IHCs and SGNs, particularly regarding how RTN4RL2 may influence synaptic integrity and receptor localization. Their study shows that deletion of RTN4RL2 in mice leads to enlarged presynaptic ribbons and smaller postsynaptic densities (PSDs) in SGNs, indicating that RTN4RL2 is vital for synaptic structure. Additionally, the presence of "orphan" PSDs-those not directly associated with IHCs-in RTN4RL2 knockout mice suggests a developmental defect in which some SGN neurites fail to form appropriate synaptic contacts, highlighting potential issues in synaptic pruning or guidance. The study also observed a depolarized shift in the activation of CaV1.3 calcium channels in IHCs, indicating altered presynaptic functionality that may lead to impaired neurotransmitter release. Furthermore, postsynaptic SGNs exhibited a deficiency in GluA2/3 AMPA receptor subunits, despite normal Gria2 mRNA levels, pointing to a disruption in receptor localization that could compromise synaptic transmission. Auditory brainstem responses showed increased sound thresholds in RTN4RL2 knockout mice, indicating impaired hearing related to these synaptic dysfunctions.

      The findings reported here significantly enhance our understanding of synaptic organization in the auditory system, particularly concerning the molecular mechanisms underlying IHC-SGN connectivity. The implications are far-reaching, as they not only inform auditory neuroscience but also provide insights into potential therapeutic targets for hearing loss related to synaptic dysfunction.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for appreciating the work and the advice that helped us to further improve the manuscript. We have carefully addressed all concerns, please see our point-per-point response below and the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Kargulyan et al. investigate the function of the transsynaptic adhesion molecule RTN4RL2 in the formation and function of ribbon synapses between type I spiral ganglion neurons (SGNs) and inner hair cells. For this purpose, they study constitutive RTN4RL2 knock-out mice. Using immunohistochemistry, they reveal defects in the recruitment of protein to ribbon synapses in the knockouts. Serial block phase EM reveals defects in SGN projections in mutants. Electrophysiological recordings suggest a small but statistically significant depolarized shift in the activation of Cav1.3 Ca<sup>2+</sup> channels. Auditory thresholds are also elevated in the mutant mice. The authors conclude that RTN4RL2 contributes to the formation and function of auditory afferent synapses to regulate auditory function.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for appreciating the work and the advice that helped us to further improve the manuscript. We have carefully addressed all concerns, please see our point-per-point response below and the revised manuscript.

      Strengths:

      The authors have excellent tools to analyze ribbon synapses.

      Weaknesses:

      However, there are several concerns that substantially reduce my enthusiasm for the study.

      (1) The analysis of the expression pattern of RTN4RL2 in Figure 1 is incomplete. The authors should show a developmental time course of expression up into maturity to correlate gene expression with major developmental milestones such as axon outgrowth, innervation, and refinement. This would allow the development of models supporting roles in axon outgrowth versus innervation or both.

      We agree that it would be valuable to show the developmental time course of RTN4RL2 expression. In response to the reviewer’s comment, we are providing RNAscope data from developmental ages E11.5, E12.5 and E16 in Figure 1. RTN4RL2 shows expression at E11.5/E12.5 both in the spiral ganglion and hair cell region, with first onset in the hair cells. We conclude that RTN4RL2 is expressed highest during fiber growth at embryonic stages and is downregulated during postnatal development maintaining low levels of expression during adulthood.

      (2) It would be important to improve the RNAscope data. Controls should be provided for Figure 1B to show that no signal is observed in hair cells from knockouts. The authors apparently already have the sections because they analyzed gene expression in SGNs of the knock-outs (Figure 1C).

      In Figure 1C gene expression in SGNs was assessed at p40, while the expression in hair cells is provided for p1 animals. Unfortunately, we do not have KO controls for p1 animals. However, as indicated in our manuscript, previously published RNA expression datasets do find RTN4RL2 expression in hair cells. Therefore, we think it is unlikely that our results are unspecific.

      (3) It is unclear from the immunolocalization data in Figure 1D if all type I SGNs express RTN4RL2. Quantification would be important to properly document the presence of RTN4RL2 in all or a subset of type I SGNs. If only a subset of SGNs express RTN4RL2, it could significantly affect the interpretation of the data. For example, SGNs selectively projecting to the pillar or modiolar side of hair cells could be affected. These synapses significantly differ in their properties.

      According to already published single cell RNAseq dataset from Shrestha et al., 2018, RTN4RL2 expression does not seem to show a clear type I SGN subtype specificity (Author response image 1). In response to the reviewer’s comment, we have further performed anti-Parvalbumin (PV) and anti-calretinin (CR) immunostainings in mid-modiolar cryosections of RTN4RL2<sup>+/+</sup> and RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> cochleae. Parvalbumin was chosen to label all SGNs and CALB2 was chosen primarily as a type Ia SGN marker (Sun et al., 2018). We present the data from all analyzed samples below (figure 2 of this rebuttal letter). Cell segmentation masks of PV positive cells were obtained using Cellpose 2.0 and the average CR intensity was calculated in those masks. While the distributions of CR intensity and the ratio of CR and PV intensities are slightly shifted in RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> cochleae, we take the data to suggest that the composition of the spiral ganglion by molecular type I SGN subtypes is largely unchanged in RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> mice.

      Author response image 1.

      Author response image 1 cites single cell RNAseq data of Brikha R Shrestha, Chester Chia, Lorna Wu, Sharon G Kujawa, M Charles Liberman, Lisa V Goodrich. Sensory neuron diversity in the inner ear is shaped by activity. Cell. 2018 Aug 23; 174(5):1229-1246.e17. doi: 10.1016/j.cell/2018.07.007

      Author response image 2.

      Calretinin intensity distribution in spiral ganglion of RTN4RL2<sup>+/+</sup> and RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> mice. (A) Mid-modiolar cochlear cryosections from RTN4RL2<sup>+/+</sup> (top) and RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> (bottom) mice immunolabeled against Parvalbumin (PV) and Calretinin (CR). Scale bar = 20 mm. (B) Distribution of CR intensity in PV positive cells (N = 3 for each genotype). (C) Distribution of the ratio of CR and PV intensities (N = 3 for each genotype).

      (4) It is important to show proper controls for the RTN4RL2 immunolocalization data to show that no staining is observed in knockouts.

      Unfortunately, our recent attempts to perform RTN4RL2 immunostainings on cryosections failed and therefore, we decided to remove the RTNr4RL2 immunostainings from Figure 1. We have adjusted the results section accordingly.

      (5) The authors state in the discussion that no staining for RTN4RL2 was observed at synaptic sites. This is surprising. Did the authors stain multiple ages? Was there perhaps transient expression during development? Or in axons indicative of a role in outgrowth, not synapse formation?

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We have now tried RTN4RL2 immunostainings on cryosections at several developmental stages, but unfortunately this time did not succeed to obtain reproducible and reliable results. Therefore, we decided to also remove the previous immunostainings from Figure 1. We have adjusted the results section as well as removed our statement of not detecting RTN4RL2 near the synaptic regions from the discussion.

      (6) In Figure 2 it seems that images in mutants are brighter compared to wildtypes. Are exposure times equivalent? Is this a consistent result?

      Yes, the samples were prepared in parallel, imaged and analyzed in the same manner.

      No, we did not observe consistent differences in brightness and also did not find it in the exemplary images of figure 2.

      (7) The number of synaptic ribbons for wildtype in Figure 2 is at 10/IHCs, and in Figure 2 Supplementary Figure 2 at 20/IHCs (20 is more like what is normally reported in the literature). The value for mutant similarly drastically varies between the two figures. This is a significant concern, especially because most differences that are reported in synaptic parameters between wild-type and mutants are far below a 2-fold difference.

      The key message is that there is no difference in the numbers of ribbons and synapses between the genotypes for the cochlear apex (~10 ribbons/IHCs, Figure 2 and Figure 2-figure supplement 2) and the mid- and base of the cochlea (more ribbons/IHCs, Figure 2-figure supplement 2). Figure 2-figure supplement 3 (now Figure 3) shows that there is a massive reduction of postsynaptic GluA2, while both Figure 2 and Figure 2-figure supplement 2 indicate that the number synapses is normal. These are two different data sets and while we closely collaborated and also shared the Moser lab protocols and analysis routines, we agree that there is a difference in the absolute synapse count, which most likely was an observer difference and different choice of tonotopic positions of analysis. In Figure 2 only the apical hair cells have been analyzed. The Moser lab, since establishing the immunofluorescence-based quantification of synapse number (Khimich et al., 2005) reported tonotopic differences in synapse counts (focus of Meyer et al., 2009 and reported by others: e.g. Kujawa and Liberman, 2009): apical and basal IHCs lower synapse numbers than mid-cochlear IHCs.

      (8) The authors report differences in ribbon volume between wild-type and mutant. Was there a difference between the modiolar/pillar region of hair cells? It is known that synaptic size varies across the modiolar-pillar axis. Maybe smaller synapses are preferentially lost?

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. Unfortunately, our already acquired datasets from 3-week-old mice did not allow us to check whether the previously described modiolar-pillar gradient of the ribbon size was collapsed in RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> mice due to the not so well-preserved morphology of the inner hair cells in our preparations. However, since the number of the ribbons is not changed in the RTN4RL2 KO mice, we do not think that the increase in the ribbon size is due to the loss of small ribbons. In response to the reviewers comment we have analyzed the modiolar-pillar gradient of the ribbon size in IHCs of middle turn of the cochlea form a newly acquired dataset of 14-week-old mice. We took the fluorescence intensity of Ctbp2 positive puncta as a proxy for the ribbon size. In these older mice we found a preserved modiolar-pillar gradient of the ribbon size (larger ribbons at the modiolar side). We summarized the results in the below Author response image 3.

      Author response image 3.

      The modiolar-pillar gradient of ribbon size is preserved in RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> IHCs. (A) Maximum intensity projections of approximately 2 IHCs stained against Vglut3 and Ctbp2 from 14-week-old RTN4RL2<sup>+/+</sup> (left) and RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> (right) mice. Scale bar = 5 mm. (B) Synaptic ribbons on the modiolar side show higher fluorescence intensity than the ones on the pillar side of mid-cochlear IHCs in both RTN4RL2<sup>+/+</sup> (left, N=2) RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> (right, N=2) mice. (C) Average fluorescence intensity of modiolar ribbons per IHC is higher than the average fluorescence intensity of pillar ribbons (paired t-test, p < 0.001).

      (9) The authors show in Figure 2 - Supplement 3 that GluA2/3 staining is absent in the mutants. Are GluA4 receptors upregulated? Otherwise, synaptic transmission should be abolished, which would be a dramatic phenotype. Antibodies are available to analyze GluA4 expression, the experiment is thus feasible. Did the authors carry out recordings from SGNs?

      In response to the reviewer’s comment, we have performed GluA4 stainings in RTN4LR2<sup>-/-</sup> mice and did not detect any GluA4 positive signal in the mutants (new Figure 3-figure supplement 1). Unfortunately, our animal breeding license was expired at the time we received the reviews and that is why our results are from 14-week-old animals. To verify that the absence of GluA4 signal is not due to potential PSD loss in 14-week-old RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup>, we have additionally performed anti-Ctbp2, anti-Homer1 and anti-Vglut3 stainings in 14-week-old animals. Despite the reduced number, we still observed juxtaposing pre- and postsynaptic puncta. We assume that the reviewer asks for patch-clamp recordings from SGNs, which are, as we are confident the reviewer is aware of, technically very challenging and beyond the scope of the present study but an important objective for future studies.  In response to the reviewers comment we have added a statement to the discussion pointing to these patch-clamp recordings from SGNs as important objective for future studies.

      (10) The authors use SBEM to analyze SGN projections and synapses. The data suggest that a significant number of SGNs are not connected to IHCs. A reconstruction in Figure 3 shows hair cells and axons. It is not clear how the outline of hair cells was derived, but this should be indicated. Also, is this a defect in the formation of synapses and subsequent retraction of SGN projections? Or could RTN4RL2 mutants have a defect in axonal outgrowth and guidance that secondarily affects synapses? To address this question, it would be useful to sparsely label SGNs in mutants, for example with AAV vectors expression GFP, and to trace the axons during development. This would allow us to distinguish between models of RTN4RL2 function. As it stands, it is not clear that RTN4RL2 acts directly at synapses.

      We agree with the reviewer on the value of a developmental study of afferent connectivity but consider this beyond the scope of the present study. In response to the reviewer's comment, we have replaced the IHC outlines with volume-reconstructed IHCs in Figure 3B (now Figure 4B). Moreover, as shown in Figure 3F (now Figure 4F), most if not all type-I SGNs (both with and without ribbon) were unbranched in the mutants just like in wildtype (also shown for a larger sample in Hua et al., 2021), arguing against morphological abnormality during development.

      (11) The authors observe a tiny shift in the operation range of Ca<sup>2+</sup> channels that has no effect on synaptic vesicle exocytosis. It seems very unlikely that this difference can explain the auditory phenotype of the mutant mice.

      We assume that the statement refers to the normal exocytosis of mutant IHCs at the potential of maximal Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx (Figure 3G and H, now Figure 4G and H). We would like to note that this experiment was performed to probe for a deficit of synapse function beyond that of the Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel activation, but did not address the impact of the altered voltage—dependence of Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel activation. In response to the reviewer’s comment, we have now added further discussion to more clearly communicate that for the range of receptor potentials achieved near sound threshold we expect impaired IHC exocytosis as the Ca<sup>2+</sup> channels require slightly more depolarization for activation in the mutant IHCs.

      (12) ABR recordings were conducted in whole-body knockouts. Effects on auditory thresholds could be a secondary consequence of perturbation along the auditory pathway. Conditional knockouts or precisely designed rescue experiments would go a long way to support the authors' hypothesis. I realize that this is a big ask and floxed mice might not be available to conduct the study.

      Thanks for this helpful comment and, indeed, unfortunately, we do not have conditional KO mice at our disposal. We totally agree that this will be important also for clarifying the role of IHC vs. SGN expression of RTN4RL2. In response to the reviewer’s comment, we now discussed the shortcoming of using constitutive RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> mice and added this important experiment on IHC and SGN specific deletion of RTN4RL2 as an objective of future studies.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In this study, the authors used RNAscope and immunostaining to confirm the expression of RTN4RL2 RNA and protein in hair cells and spiral ganglia. Through RTN4RL2 gene knockout mice, they demonstrated that the absence of RTN4RL2 leads to an increase in the size of presynaptic ribbons and a depolarized shift in the activation of calcium channels in inner hair cells. Additionally, they observed a reduction in GluA2/3 AMPA receptors in postsynaptic neurons and identified additional "orphan PSDs" not paired with presynaptic ribbons. These synaptic alterations ultimately resulted in an increased hearing threshold in mice, confirming that the RTN4RL2 gene is essential for normal hearing. These data are intriguing as they suggest that RTN4RL2 contributes to the proper formation and function of auditory afferent synapses and is critical for normal hearing. However, a thorough understanding of the known or postulated roles of RTN4Rl2 is lacking.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for appreciating the work and the advice that helped us to further improve the manuscript. We have carefully addressed all concerns, please see our point-per-point response below and the revised manuscript.

      While the conclusions of this paper are generally well supported by the data, several aspects of the data analysis warrant further clarification and expansion.

      (1) A quantitative assessment is necessary in Figure 1 when discussing RNA and protein expression. It would be beneficial to show that expression levels are quantitatively reduced in KO mice compared to wild-type mice. This suggestion also applies to Figure 2-supplement 3.D, which examines expression levels.

      The processing of our control and KO samples for RNAscope was not strictly done in parallel and therefore we would like to refrain from quantitative comparison.

      (2) In Figure 2, the authors present a morphological analysis of synapses and discuss the presence of "orphan PSDs." I agree that Homer1 not juxtaposed with Ctbp2 is increased in KO mice compared to the control group. However, in quantifying this, they opted to measure the number of Homer1 juxtaposed with Ctbp2 rather than directly quantifying the number of Homer1 not juxtaposed with Ctbp2. Quantifying the number of Homer1 not juxtaposed with Ctbp2 would more clearly represent "orphan PSDs" and provide stronger support for the discussion surrounding their presence.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment. We did not perform this analysis primarily because “orphan” Homer1 puncta, as seen in our immunostainings, are distributed away from hair cells in diverse morphologies and sizes. This makes distinguishing them from unspecific immunofluorescent spots—also present in wild-type samples—challenging. In response to the reviewer’s request, we analyzed the number of “orphan” Homer1 puncta in our previously acquired RTN4RL2<sup>+/+</sup> and RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> samples. Using the surface algorithm in Imaris software, we applied identical parameters across all samples to create surfaces for Homer1-positive puncta (total Homer1 puncta). We quantified “orphan” Homer1 puncta as the difference between total and ribbon-juxtaposing Homer1 puncta and normalized this number to the IHC count. Our results showed 4.3 vs. 26.8 “orphan” Homer1 puncta per IHC in RTN4RL2<sup>+/+</sup> and RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> samples, respectively. We note that variations in acquired volumes between samples may introduce confounding effects.

      (3) In Figure 2, Supplementary 3, the authors discuss GluA2/3 puncta reduction and note that Gria2 RNA expression remains unchanged. However, there is an issue with the lack of quantification for Gria2 RNA expression. Additionally, it is noted that RNA expression was measured at P4. While the timing for GluA2/3 puncta assessment is not specified, if it was assessed at 3 weeks old as in Figure 2's synaptic puncta analysis, it would be inappropriate to link Gria2 RNA expression with GluA2/3 protein expression at P4. If RNA and protein expression were assessed at P4, please indicate this timing for clarity.

      GluA2/3 immunostainings were performed in 1 to 1.5-month-old animals. We apologize for not indicating this before and have now included it in Figure 3 legend. The processing of our control and KO samples for RNAscope was not strictly done in parallel and therefore we would like to refrain from quantitative comparison.

      (4) In Figure 3, the authors indicate that RTN4RL2 deficiency reduces the number of type 1 SGNs connected to ribbons. Given that the number of ribbons remains unchanged (Figure 2), it is important to clearly explain the implications of this finding. It is already known that each type I SGN forms a single synaptic contact with a single IHC. The fact that the number of ribbons remains constant while additional "orphan PSDs" are present suggests that the overall number of SGNs might need to increase to account for these findings. An explanation addressing this would be helpful.

      In Figure 3 (now Figure 4), we found additional type-1 SGNs that are unconnected to IHC, in good agreement with “orphan PSDs” observed under the light microscope. Indeed, we also confirmed monosynaptic, unbranched fiber morphology (Figure 3F, now Figure 4F). Together, these results imply about a 20% increase in the overall number of SGNs, which however we did not observe in SGN soma counting.

      (5) In Figure 4F and 5Cii, could you clarify how voltage sensitivity (k) was calculated? Additionally, please provide an explanation for the values presented in millivolts (mV).

      Voltage sensitivity (k) was calculated as the slope of the Boltzmann fit to the fractional activation curves: , Where G is conductance, G<sub>max</sub> is the maximum conductance, V<sub>m</sub> is the membrane potential, V<sub>half</sub> is the voltage corresponding to the half maximal activation of Ca<sup>2+</sup> channels and k (slope of the curve) is the voltage sensitivity of Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel activation. We have now added this to our Materials and Methods section.

      (6) In Figure 6, the author measured the threshold of ABR at 2-4 months old. Since previous figures confirming synaptic morphology and function were all conducted on 3-week-old mice, it would be better to measure ABR at 3 weeks of age if possible.

      ABR measurements for comparisons in a cohort of age-matched mice require fully developed individuals. 3 weeks is the minimum age that is regarded for a mature ear. However, variation in developmental differences among one litter is very frequent that affects normal hearing thresholds. From our own experience we do not regard the ear fully functional before 6 weeks of age. Then hearing thresholds are lowest indicating full functionality. Since the C57BL/6 background strain has a genetic defect in the Cadherin 23-coding gene (Cdh23) at the ahl locus of mouse chromosome 10 these mice exhibit early onset and progression of age-related hearing loss starting at 5–8 months (Hunter & Willott, 1987). Therefore, we chose a “safe” time window for stable and unaffected ABR recordings of 2-4 months to provide most representative data.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Please include information on the validation of all the antibodies used in this study, or reference the relevant work where the antibodies were previously validated.

      In response to the reviewer’s comment, we have now included a table listing all primary antibodies used in this study. Where possible, we provide references for knockout (KO) validation. Otherwise, we refer to the manufacturer’s information, as provided in the respective datasheets.

      (2) Figure 2 illustrates the pre- and postsynaptic changes observed in RTN4RL2 knockout (KO) mice. Please specify the age of the mice and the cochlear region depicted and analyzed in Figure 2.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. The IHCs of apical cochlear region were analyzed in mice at 3 weeks of age. We have now added this to the figure legend.

      (3) The discovery of orphan SGN neurites in RTN4RL2 KO mice is particularly intriguing. I wonder whether the additional Homer1-positive puncta illustrated in Figure 2 are present in these orphan SGN neurites, which would suggest that they may be functional. Conducting immunohistochemistry (IHC) labeling for type I SGN neurites using an anti-Tuj1 antibody, along with Homer1, would help localize the additional Homer1 puncta shown in Figure 2. Additionally, the "extra" Homer1 puncta appears less striking in the data presented in Figure 2-Supplement 2. Quantifying the number of Homer1 puncta in wild-type versus KO mice across different cochlear regions will help visualize the Figure 2-Supplement 2 data and relate the presence of extra neurites to the increased auditory brainstem response (ABR) thresholds observed at all frequencies.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment and we agree that localizing orphan PSDs on the SGN neurites would be very useful. Unfortunately, the animal breeding license in the Göttingen lab had expired. At the time we received the reviews we only had access to 14-week-old animals and could not perform the stainings in animals which would have comparable age range to the rest of the study (3-4 weeks). The phenotype of extra Homer1 puncta was not as drastic in 14-week-old animals as it was in previously stained 3-week-old animals. Nevertheless, we still tried NF200, Homer1 and Vglut3 immunostainings in 14-week-old animals. We present representative single imaging planes of NF200, Homer1 and Vglut3 stainings in Author response image 4. Additionally, we provide exemplary images from 7-week-old RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup>, where it looks like that the orphan Homer1 puncta are found on calretinin positive neurites.

      Author response image 4.

      Attempts to localize “orphan” Homer1 patches on type I SGN neurites. (A) Single exemplary imaging planes of apical IHC region from RTN4RL2<sup>+/+</sup> (left) and RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> (right) mice immunolabeled against NF200, Vglut3 and Homer1. White arrows show putative “orphan” Homer1 puncta on NF200 positive neurites. Scale bar = 5 mm. (B) Maximum intensity projections of representative confocal stacks of IHCs from RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> mice immunolabeled against Calretinin and Homer1. Scale bars = 5 mm. White arrows show possible “orphan” Homer1 puncta on Calretinin positive boutons.

      (4) The authors noted a reduction in the number of GluA2/3-positive puncta in RTN4RL2 KOs, as shown in Figure 2-Supplement 3. However, in the Results section (page 5, line 124), it is unclear whether the authors refer to a reduction in fluorescence intensity or the number of puncta. Please clarify this.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We refer to the number and have now added this to the manuscript.

      (5) I find it particularly interesting that, despite the presence of smaller but synaptically engaged Homer1-positive SGN neurites, these appear to lack or present a reduction in the number of GluA2/3 puncta, and that GluA2/3 puncta are observed in non-ribbon juxtaposed neurites. Therefore, I suggest including GluA2/3 (Fig2 supplement 3) data in the main figure. It would be valuable to determine whether the orphan neurites express both Homer1 and GluA2/3, which could indicate that the defect is not solely due to reduced GluA2/3 expression at the formed synapses, but also to the presence of additional orphan synapses. I would also mention in the discussion how the phenotype of the RTN4L2 KO compares to the GluA2/3 KO and if the lack of GluA2/3 at the AZ could explain the increase in ABR threshold. Quantification of GluA2/3 puncta at the apical, middle, and basal region would also help understand the auditory phenotype of the KO mice.

      We have changed Figure2-figure supplement 3 to become a main figure (Figure 3) based on the recommendation of the reviewer. We agree, that it would be valuable to perform immunohistochemistry combining anti-GluA2/3 and anti-Homer1 and anti-Ctbp2 antibodies to see if the “orphan” Homer1 patches house GluA2/3 not juxtaposing synaptic ribbons. Unfortunately, as mentioned above, due to the expiration of our animal breeding and experimentation licenses we did not manage to do those experiments. We have however performed stainings with anti-GluA4 antibodies and could not detect GluA4 signal in RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> mice (Figure 3-figure supplement 1). This potentially could explain the more drastic ABR threshold elevation in RTN4RL2<sup>-/-</sup> mice compared to e.g. GluA3 KO mice. We have now made this clearer in our discussion.

      (6) I suggest considering the use of color-blind friendly palettes for figures and graphs in this manuscript to enhance clarity and ensure that the findings are accessible to a wider audience and improve the overall effectiveness of the presentation. Please use color-blind-friendly schemes in Figure 1 and Figure 2 Supplement 3.

      Done.

      (7) Could you please explain what "XX {plus minus} Y, SD = W" means in the figure legends?

      Mean ± SEM (standard error of the mean), SD (standard deviation) are indicated in the legends. In response to the reviewer comment we have now added an explanation in the Materials and Methods –> Data analysis and statistics section.

      (8) Please include information about the ear tested (left or right or both).

      Both ears were tested. Since there was no significant difference between right and left ear we did not further consider this factor. We will add this fact more precisely in the Material and methods section.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 90: Why not show this control, it is a nice control.

      Unfortunately, our recent attempts to perform RTN4RL2 immunostaining on cryosections were unsuccessful. Therefore, we decided to remove RTN4RL2 immunostaining from Figure 1 and have adjusted the results section accordingly.

      (2) Line 94: Please provide a reference for these interactions.

      Done.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors used a multi-alternative decision task and a multidimensional signaldetection model to gain further insight into the cause of perceptual impairments during the attentional blink. The model-based analyses of behavioural and EEG data show that such perceptual failures can be unpacked into distinct deficits in visual detection and discrimination, with visual detection being linked to the amplitude of late ERP components (N2P and P3) and discrimination being linked to the coherence of fronto-parietal brain activity.

      Strengths:

      The main strength of this paper lies in the fact that it presents a novel perspective on the cause of perceptual failures during the attentional blink. The multidimensional signal detection modelling approach is explained clearly, and the results of the study show that this approach offers a powerful method to unpack behavioural and EEG data into distinct processes of detection and discrimination.

      Thank you.

      Weaknesses:

      (1.1) While the model-based analyses are compelling, the paper also features some analyses that seem misguided, or, at least, insufficiently motivated and explained. Specifically, in the introduction, the authors raise the suggestion that the attentional blink could be due to a reduction in sensitivity or a response bias. The suggestion that a response bias could play a role seems misguided, as any response bias would be expected to be constant across lags, while the attentional blink effect is only observed at short lags. Thus, it is difficult to understand why the authors would think that a response bias could explain the attentional blink.

      In the revision, we seek to better motivate the bias component. A deficit in T2 identification accuracy could arise from either sensitivity or criterion effects at short lags. For example, in short T1-T2 lag trials participants may adopt a more conservative choice criterion for reporting the presence of T2 thereby yielding lower accuracies for short lags. Criterion effects need not be uniform across lags: A participant could infer the T1-T2 lag on each trial based on various factors, such as trial length, and systematically adjust their choice criterion across lags, prior to making a response.

      Below, we present a simple schematic for how a conservative choice criterion impacts accuracy. Consider a conventional attentional blink paradigm where the task is to detect and report T2's presence. For simplicity, we assume that prior probabilities for T2’s occurrence are equal, such that the number of “T2 present” and “T2 absent” trials are equal.

      We model this task with a one-dimensional signal detection theory (SDT) model (left panel). Here, ψ represents the decision variable and the red and gray Gaussians represent the conditional density of ψ for the T2 present (“signal”) and T2 absent (“noise”) conditions, respectively. We increase the criterion from its optimal value (here, midpoint of signal and noise means), to reflect increasingly conservative choices. As the criterion increases and deviates further from its optimal value – here, reflecting a conservative bias – accuracy drops systematically (right panel).

      Author response image 1.

      We have revised the Introduction as follows:

      “Distinguishing between sensitivity and criterion effects is crucial because a change in either of these parameters can produce a change in the proportion of correct responses[41,42]. A lower proportion of correct T2 detections may reflect not only a lower detection d’ at short lags but also a sub-optimal choice criterion corresponding, for instance, to a conservative detection bias (Fig. 1, right, top). Importantly, such criterion effects need not be uniform across intertarget lags: the lag on each trial could be inferred based on various factors, such as trial length, allowing participants to adopt different choice criteria for the different lags prior to making a response.”

      (1.2) A second point of concern regards the way in which the measures for detection and discrimination accuracy were computed. If I understand the paper correctly, a correct detection was defined as either correctly identifying T2 (i.e., reporting CW or CCW if T2 was CW or CCW, respectively, see Figure 2B), or correctly reporting T2's absence (a correct rejection).

      Here, it seems that one should also count a misidentification (i.e., incorrect choice of CW or CCW when T2 was present) as a correct detection, because participants apparently did detect T2, but failed to judge/remember its orientation properly in case of a misidentification. Conversely, the manner in which discrimination performance is computed also raises questions. Here, the authors appear to compute accuracy as the average proportion of T2present trials on which participants selected the correct response option for T2, thus including trials in which participants missed T2 entirely. Thus, a failure to detect T2 is now counted as a failure to discriminate T2. Wouldn't a more proper measure of discrimination accuracy be to compute the proportion of correct discriminations for trials in which participants detected T2?

      Indeed, detection and discrimination accuracies were computed with precisely the same procedure, and under the same conditions, as described by the Reviewer. We regret our poor description. For clarity, we have revised the following line in the Results section; we have also updated the Methods (section on Behavioral data analysis: Measuring attentional blink effects on psychometric quantities).

      “Detection accuracies were calculated based on the proportion of trials in which T2 was correctly detected (Methods). Briefly, we computed the average proportion of hits, misidentifications, and correct rejections; misidentifications were included because, although incorrectly identified, the target was nevertheless correctly detected. In contrast, discrimination accuracies were derived from T2 present trials, based on the proportion of correct identifications alone (Methods).”

      (1.3) My last point of critique is that the paper offers little if any guidance on how the inferred distinction between detection and discrimination can be linked to existing theories of the attentional blink. The discussion mostly focuses on comparisons to previous EEG studies, but it would be interesting to know how the authors connect their findings to extant, mechanistic accounts of the attentional blink. A key question here is whether the finding of dissociable processes of detection and discrimination would also hold with more meaningful stimuli in an identification task (e.g., the canonical AB task of identifying two letters shown amongst digits).

      There is evidence to suggest that meaningful stimuli are categorized just as quickly as they are detected (Grill-Spector & Kanwisher, 2005; Grill-Spector K, Kanwisher N. Visual recognition: as soon as you know it is there, you know what it is. Psychol Sci. 2005 Feb;16(2):152-60. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00796.x. PMID: 15686582.). Does that mean that the observed distinction between detection and discrimination would only apply to tasks in which the targets consist of otherwise meaningless visual elements, such as lines of different orientations?

      Our results are consistent with previous literature suggested by the reviewer. Specifically, we model detection and discrimination not as sequential processes, but as concurrent computations (Figs. 3A-B). Yet, our results suggest that these processes possess distinct neural bases. We have further revised the Discussion in context of this literature in the revised manuscript.

      “…Interestingly, we found no evidence indicating that these two computations (detection and discrimination) were sequential; in fact, the modulation of beta coherence occurred almost immediately after T2 onset, and lasted well afterwards (>400 ms from T2 onset) (Fig. 5A-B) suggesting that an analysis of T2’s features proceeded in parallel with its detection and consolidation. We also modeled detection and discrimination as concurrent computations in our SDT model (Fig. 3A-B). Previous work suggests that while object detection and categorization processes proceed in parallel, detection and identification processes occur sequentially[77]. Our results are in line with this literature, if we consider T2’s discrimination judgement – clockwise versus counterclockwise of vertical – to be a categorization, rather than an identification judgement. Moreover, this earlier study[75] observed significant trial-wise correlations between detection and categorization responses, suggesting that the two processes involve the operation of the same perceptual filters (“analyzers”). Our study, on the other hand, reports distinct neural bases for detection and discrimination computations. Yet, the two sets of findings are not mutually contradictory.

      In many conventional attentional blink tasks[3,20,25], complex visual stimuli, like letters, must be detected among a stream of background distractors with closely similar features, such as digits. In this case, target detection would require the operation of shape-selective perceptual filters for feature analysis. These same shape-selective filters would be involved also for discriminating between distinct, but related target stimuli (e.g., two designated candidate letters). In our task, target gratings needed to be distinguished in a stream of plainly distinct background distractors (plaids), whereas the discrimination judgement involved analysis of grating orientation. As a result, our task design likely precludes the need for the same perceptual filters in the detection and the discrimination judgements. Absent this common feature analysis, our results suggest distinct electrophysiological correlates for the detection and discrimination of targets.”

      Reviewer #2 Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors had two aims: First, to decompose the attentional blink (AB) deficit into the two components of signal detection theory; sensitivity and bias. Second, the authors aimed to assess the two subcomponents of sensitivity; detection and discrimination. They observed that the AB is only expressed in sensitivity. Furthermore, detection and discrimination were doubly dissociated. Detection modulated N2p and P3 ERP amplitude, but not frontoparietal beta-band coherence, whereas this pattern was reversed for discrimination.

      Strengths:

      The experiment is elegantly designed, and the data - both behavioral and electrophysiological - are aptly analyzed. The outcomes, in particular the dissociation between detection and discrimination blinks, are consistently and clearly supported by the results. The discussion of the results is also appropriately balanced.

      Thank you.

      Weaknesses:

      (2.1) The lack of an effect of stimulus contrast does not seem very surprising from what we know of the nature of AB already. Low-level perceptual factors are not thought to cause AB. This is fine, as there are also other, novel findings reported, but perhaps the authors could bolster the importance of these (null) findings by referring to AB-specific papers, if there are indeed any, that would have predicted different outcomes in this regard.

      While there is consensus that the low-level perceptual factors are not affected by the attentional blink, other studies have suggested evidence to the contrary (e.g., Chua et al, Percept. Psychophys., 2005)[1]. We have mentioned the significance of our findings in the context of such conflicting evidence in literature, in the revised Discussion.

      “Surprisingly, we found no significant effect of contrast on either type of deficit (Figs. 2A-B). In other words, high (100%) contrast T2 stimuli were also strongly susceptible to the detection and discrimination bottlenecks associated with the attentional blink. Thus, despite a clear contrast-dependent encoding of T2 in early sensory cortex, the attentional blink produced a significant deficit with downstream processing, even for targets of high contrast. While at odds with some earlier work, which suggest an early-stage perceptual bottleneck [82–84], these results are largely consistent with findings from the majority of previous studies [3,7,9,11,19,20,82,85,86] which suggest a late-stage bottleneck.”

      (2.2) On an analytical note, the ERP analysis could be finetuned a little more. The task design does not allow measurement of the N2pc or N400 components, which are also relevant to the AB, but the N1 component could additionally be analyzed. In doing so, I would furthermore recommend selecting more lateral electrode sites for both the N1, as well as the P1. Both P1 and N1 are likely not maximal near the midline, where the authors currently focused their P1 analysis.

      We performed these suggested analysis. Whereas in the original submission we had used the O1, O2 and Oz electrodes, we now estimate the P1 and N1 with the more lateral P7 and P8 electrodes[2], as suggested by the reviewer.

      Even with these more lateral electrodes, we did not observe a significant N1 component in a 90-160 ms window[3] in the long lag trials (p=0.207, signed rank test for amplitude less than zero); a one-tailed Bayes factor (BF=1.35) revealed no clear evidence for or against an N1 component. Analysis of the P1 component with these more lateral electrodes also yielded no statistically significant blink-induced modulation (P1(short lag-long lag) = 0.25 ± 0.16, uV, p=0.231, BF=0.651) (SI Figure S3, revised).

      These updated analyses are now reported in the revised Results (lines 317-319) and Methods (lines 854-855). In addition, we have revised SI Table S2 with the new P1 component analysis.

      (2.3) Impact & Context:

      The results of this study will likely influence how we think about selective attention in the context of the AB phenomenon. However, I think its impact could be further improved by extending its theoretical framing. In particular, there has been some recent work on the nature of the AB deficit, showing that it can be discrete (all-or-none) and gradual (Sy et al., 2021; Karabay et al., 2022, both in JEP: General). These different faces of target awareness in the AB may be linked directly to the detection and discrimination subcomponents that are analyzed in the present paper. I would encourage the authors to discuss this potential link and comment on the bearing of the present work on these behavioural findings.

      Thank you. We have now discussed our findings in the context of these recent studies in the revised manuscript.

      “…In line with this hypothesis, we discovered that the attentional blink induced dissociable detection and discrimination deficits. There was no statistically significant correlation between these two types of deficits within and across participants and evidence for such a correlation was weak, at best. Unlike previous target identification designs that conflated attentional blink’s effect on detection versus discrimination performance[3,4,9,25,37], our 3-AFC task, and associated signal detection model enabled quantifying each of these deficits separately and identifying a double dissociation between their respective neural correlates. Our dissociation of the attentional blink into distinct subcomponents is complementary to recent studies, which examined whether the attentional blink reflects an all-or-none phenomenon[73,74]. For example, the T2 deficit induced by the attentional blink can be either all-or-none or graded, depending on whether T1 and T2 judgements involve distinct or common features, respectively[73]. While a graded change in precision could reflect sensitivity effects, an all-or-none change in guess rates – without a concomitant change in precision – may reflect a criterion increase (conservative detection bias) effect. Future experiments that incorporate a three-alternative response, with concurrent detection and discrimination, along with key task elements of these earlier studies, may further help resolve these findings.”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the present study, the authors aimed to achieve a better understanding of the mechanisms underlying the attentional blink, that is, a deficit in processing the second of two target stimuli when they appear in rapid succession. Specifically, they used a concurrent detection and identification task in- and outside of the attentional blink and decoupled effects of perceptual sensitivity and response bias using a novel signal detection model. They conclude that the attentional blink selectively impairs perceptual sensitivity but not response bias, and link established EEG markers of the attentional blink to deficits in stimulus detection (N2p, P3) and discrimination (fronto-parietal high-beta coherence), respectively. Taken together, their study suggests distinct mechanisms mediating detection and discrimination deficits in the attentional blink.

      Strengths:

      Major strengths of the present study include its innovative approach to investigating the mechanisms underlying the attentional blink, an elegant, carefully calibrated experimental paradigm, a novel signal detection model, and multifaceted data analyses using state-of-the art model comparisons and robust statistical tests. The study appears to have been carefully conducted and the overall conclusions seem warranted given the results. In my opinion, the manuscript is a valuable contribution to the current literature on the attentional blink. Moreover, the novel paradigm and signal detection model are likely to stimulate future research.

      Thank you.

      Weaknesses:

      Weaknesses of the present manuscript mainly concern the negligence of some relevant literature, unclear hypotheses, potentially data-driven analyses, relatively low statistical power, potential flaws in the EEG methods, and the absence of a discussion of limitations. In the following, I will list some major and minor concerns in detail.

      (3.1) Hypotheses: I appreciate the multifaceted, in-depth analysis of the given dataset including its high amount of different statistical tests. However, neither the Introduction nor the Methods contain specific statistical hypotheses. Moreover, many of the tests (e.g., correlations) rely on selected results of previous tests. It is unclear how many of the tests were planned a priori, how many more were performed, and how exactly corrections for multiple tests were implemented. Thus, I find it difficult to assess the robustness of the results.

      We hypothesized that neural computations associated with target detection would be characterized by regional (local) neuronal markers (e.g., parietal or occipital ERPs), whereas computations linked to feature discrimination would involve neural coordination across multiple brain regions (e.g. fronto-parietal coherence) (lines 135-138). We planned and conducted our statistical tests based on this hypothesis. All multiple comparison corrections (Bonferroni-Holm correction, see Methods) were performed separately for each class of analyses.

      Based on this overarching hypothesis, the following tests were planned and conducted.

      ERP analysis: Based on an extensive review of recent literature[4] (Zivony et al., 2022 we performed the following tests: i) We tested whether four ERP component amplitudes (parietal P1, fronto-central P2, occipito-parietal N2p, and parietal P3) were significantly different between short and long lags with a Wilcoxon signed rank test followed by Bonferroni-Holm multiple comparison correction; ii) We correlated the ERPs whose amplitudes showed a significant difference in analysis (i) with detection and discrimination d’ deficits (six correlations) using robust (bend) correlations[5]; again, this was followed by a Bonferroni-Holm multiple comparison correction. Note that there is no circularity with planning analysis (ii) based on the results of analysis (i) because the latter is agnostic to detection versus discrimination blink deficits. In case (i), where no a priori hypothesis about directionality were available, all p-values were based on two-tailed tests but for case (ii), where we had an a priori directional hypothesis, p-values were computed from one-tailed tests. This has now been clarified in the revised Methods lines 937-940 and 950-952.

      Coherence analysis: Based on a seminal study of long-range synchrony modulation by the attentional blink[6], we examined fronto-parietal coherence in the beta (13-30 Hz) band, separately for the left and right hemispheres, and performed the following comparisons. i) We computed differences between the fronto-parietal coherogram (time-frequency representation of coherence, Fig. 5A-D) between short-lag and long-lag conditions, and performed a twodimensional cluster-based permutation test[7]; this method inherently corrects for multiple comparisons across time-frequency windows. ii) Because the analysis in (i) revealed the clearest evidence for coherence differences in the canonical high-beta (20-30 Hz band) in the left fronto-parietal electrodes (Figs. 5C-D; 0-300 ms following target onset), we correlated power in this band with detection and discrimination d’ deficits; this was followed by a Bonferroni-Holm multiple comparison correction. As before there is no circularity with planning analysis (ii) based on the results of analysis (i) because the latter is agnostic to detection versus discrimination blink deficits. Again, in case (i), where no a priori hypothesis about directionality was made, all p-values were based on two-tailed tests but for case (ii), where we had an a priori directional hypothesis, p-values were computed from one-tailed tests.

      For completeness, we performed all of the other correlations, for example, correlations with coherence in the low-beta band or with the right fronto-parietal electrodes (SI Table 3). These latter analyses were not planned, nor did they yield significant results.

      Neural distance analysis: This was a novel analysis designed to test the hypothesis that detection and discrimination deficits would be correlated with neural distances along distinct dimensions. i) First, we compared neural distances across lag conditions at different timepoints following target onset with a one-dimensional cluster-based permutation test[7] ; ii) Next, we correlated the neural distances along the detection and discrimination dimension with the detection and discrimination d’ deficits (Fig. 6E-F, 6G-H), as well as with the ERP and coherence markers (Fig. 7A-B, 7C-D). For each of these analyses, we employed robust (bend) correlations[5] followed by a Bonferroni-Holm multiple comparison correction. As before, pvalues were computed using two-tailed tests for case (i) and one-tailed tests for case (ii), based on the absence or presence of an a priori directional hypothesis.

      (3.2) Power: Some important null findings may result from the rather small sample sizes of N = 24 for behavioral and N = 18 for ERP analyses. For example, the correlation between detection and discrimination d' deficits across participants (r=0.39, p=0.059) (p. 12, l. 263) and the attentional blink effect on the P1 component (p=0.050, no test statistic) (p. 14, 301) could each have been significant with one more participant. In my opinion, such results should not be interpreted as evidence for the absence of effects.

      We have modified these claims in the revised Results. In addition, we now compute and report Bayes factors, which enable evaluating evidence for the presence versus absence of effects.

      “Detection and discrimination d’ deficits were not statistically significantly correlated (r=0.39, t=2.28, p=0.059); Bayes factor analysis revealed no clear evidence for or against a correlation between these subcomponent deficits (BF=1.18) (SI Fig. S2, left).”

      “Discrimination accuracy deficits were not statistically significantly different between high and low detection accuracy deficit blocks (z=1.97, p=0.067), and the Bayes factor revealed no strong evidence for or against such a difference (BF=1.42) (Fig. 3G).”

      In addition, the results are interpreted as follows (lines 294-296):

      “Moreover, detection and discrimination d’ deficits were not significantly correlated both within and across participants, with no clear evidence for or against a correlation, based on the Bayes factor.”

      The null result on the P1 has changed because of the analysis with the alternative electrode set suggested by Reviewer #2 (see comment #2.2). We now report these results as follows:

      “By contrast, the P1, an early sensory component, showed no statistically significant blinkinduced modulation (P1= 0.25 ± 0.16µV, z = 1.19, p=0.231, BF = 0.651) (SI Fig. S3).”

      (3.3) Neural basis of the attentional blink: The introduction (e.g., p. 4, l. 56-76) and discussion (e.g., p. 19, 427-447) do not incorporate the insights from the highly relevant recent review by Zivony & Lamy (2022), which is only cited once (p. 19, l. 428). Moreover, the sections do not mention some relevant ERP studies of the attentional blink (e.g., Batterink et al., 2012; Craston et al., 2009; Dell'Acqua et al., 2015; Dellert et al., 2022; Eiserbeck et al., 2022; Meijs et al., 2018).

      We have now cited these previous studies at the appropriate places in the revised Introduction.

      “The effect of the attentional blink on the processing of the second target is well studied. In particular, previous studies have investigated the stage at which attentional blink affects T2’s processing (early or late) [14–17] and the neural basis of this effect, including the specific brain regions involved[15,18–20]. Several theoretical frameworks characterize a sequence of phases of the attentional blink, including target selection based on relevance, detection, feature processing, and encoding into working memory[9,21]. Overall, there is little support for attentional blink deficits at an early, sensory encoding[14] stage; by contrast, the vast majority of literature suggests that T2’s processing is affected at a late stage[8,10]. Consistent with these behavioral results, scalp electroencephalography (EEG) studies have reported partial or complete suppression of late event-related potential (ERP) components, particularly those linked to attentional engagement (P2, N2, N2pc or VAN)[15,22–25], working memory (P3) [20,26–30] or semantic processing (N400)[31]; early sensory components (P1/N1) are virtually unaffected[20,24] (reviewed in detail in Zivony and Lamy, 2022[32]) .”

      (3.4) Detection versus discrimination: Concerning the neural basis of detection versus discrimination (e.g., p. 6, l. 98-110; p. 18, l. 399-412), relevant existing literature (e.g., Broadbent & Broadbent, 1987; Hillis & Brainard, 2007; Koivisto et al., 2017; Straube & Fahle, 2011; Wiens et al., 2023) is not included.

      Thank you for these suggestions. We have now cited these studies in the revised Discussion.

      “It is increasingly clear that detection and discrimination are separable processes, each mediated by distinct neural mechanisms. Behaviorally, accurately identifying the first target, versus merely detecting it, produces stronger deficits with identifying the second target[59]. Moreover, dissociable mechanisms have been reported to mediate object detection and discrimination in visual adaptation contexts[60]. Neurally, shape detection and identification judgements produce activations in non-overlapping clusters in various brain regions in the visual cortex, inferior parietal cortex, and the medial frontal lobe[61]. Similarly, occipital ERPs associated with conscious awareness also show clear differences between detection and discrimination. For instance, an early posterior negative component (200-300 ms) was significantly modulated in amplitude by success in detection, but not in identification[62]. The closely related visual awareness negativity (VAN) was substantially stronger at the detection, compared to the discrimination, threshold[63].

      Furthermore, a significant body of previous work has reported dissociable behavioural and neural mechanisms underlying attention’s effects on target detection versus discrimination. Behavioral studies have reported distinct effects on target detection versus discrimination in both endogenous[64] and exogenous[65] attention tasks.”

      (3.5) Pooling of lags and lags 1 sparing: I wonder why the authors chose to include 5 different lags when they later pooled early (100, 300 ms) and late (700, 900 ms) lags, and whether this pooling is justified. This is important because T2 at lag 1 (100 ms) is typically "spared" (high accuracy) while T2 at lag 3 (300 ms) shows the maximum AB (for reviews, see, e.g., Dux & Marois, 2009; Martens & Wyble, 2010). Interestingly, this sparing was not observed here (p. 43, Figure 2). Nevertheless, considering the literature and the research questions at hand, it is questionable whether lag 1 and 3 should be pooled.

      Lag-1 sparing is not always observed in attentional blink studies; there are notable exceptions to reports of lag-1 sparing[8,9]. Our statistical tests revealed no significant difference in accuracies between short lag (100 and 300 ms) trials or between long lag (700 and 900 ms) trials but did reveal significant differences between the short and long lag trials (ANOVA, followed by post-hoc tests). To simplify the presentation of the findings, we pooled together the short lag (100 and 300 ms) and, separately, the long lag (700 and 900 ms) trials. We have presented these analyses, and clarified the motivation for pooling these lags in the revised Methods.

      “Based on these psychometric measures, we computed detection and discrimination accuracies as follows. Detection accuracies were computed as the average proportion of the hits, misidentification and correct rejection responses; misidentifications were included because not missing the target reflected accurate detection. By contrast, discrimination accuracies were computed based on the average proportion of the two correct identifications (hits) on T2 present trials alone. We performed 2-way ANOVAs on both detection and discrimination accuracies with the inter-target lag (5 values) and T2 contrast independent factors. We found main effects of both lag (F(4,92)=18.81, p<0.001) and contrast (F(1,92)=21.78, p<0.001) on detection accuracy, but no interaction effect between lag and contrast (F(4,92)=1.92, p=0.113). Similarly, we found main effects of both lag (F(4,92)=25.08, p<0.001) and contrast (F(1,92)=16.58, p<0.001) on discrimination accuracy, but no interaction effect between lag and contrast (F(4,92)=0.93, p=0.450). Post-hoc tests based on Tukey’s HSD revealed a significant difference in discrimination accuracies between the two shortest lags (100 ms and 300 ms) and the two longest lags (700 and 900 ms) for both low and high contrast targets, and for both detection and discrimination accuracies (p<0.01). But they revealed no significant difference between the two shortest lags (p>0.25) or the two longest lags (p>0.40) for either target contrast or for either accuracy type. As a result, for subsequent analyses, we pooled together the “short lag” (100 ms and 300 ms) and the “long lag” (700 ms and 900 ms) trials. We quantified the effect of the attentional blink on each of the psychometric measures as well as detection and discrimination accuracies by comparing their respective, average values between the short lag and long lag trials, separately for the high and low T2 contrasts.”

      (3.6) Discrimination in the attentional blink. Concerning the claims that previous attentional blink studies conflated detection and discrimination (p. 6, l. 111-114; p. 18, l. 416), there is a recent ERP study (Dellert et al., 2022) in which participants did not perform a discrimination task for the T2 stimuli. Moreover, since the relevance of all stimuli except T1 was uncertain in this study, irrelevant distractors could not be filtered out (cf. p. 19, l. 437). Under these conditions, the attentional blink was still associated with reduced negativities in the N2 range (cf. p. 19, l. 427-437) but not with a reduced P3 (cf. p. 19, l 439-447).

      We have addressed the relationship between our findings and those of Dellert et al (2022)[10] in the revised Discussion.

      “… In the present study, we observed that the parietal P3 amplitude was correlated selectively with detection, rather than discrimination deficits. This suggests that the P3 deficit indexes a specific bottleneck with encoding and consolidating T2 into working memory, rather than an inability to reliably maintain its features. In this regard, a recent study[22] measured ERP correlates of the perceptual awareness of the T2 stimulus whose relevance was uncertain at the time of its presentation. In contrast to earlier work, this study observed no change in P3b amplitude across seen (detected) and unseen targets. Taken together with this study, our findings suggest that rather than indexing visual awareness, the P3 may index detection, but only when information about the second target, or a decision about its appearance, needs to be maintained in working memory. Additional experiments, involving targets of uncertain relevance, along with our behavioral analysis framework, may help further evaluate this hypothesis.”

      (3.7) General EEG methods: While most of the description of the EEG preprocessing and analysis (p. 31/32) is appropriate, it also lacks some important information (see, e.g., Keil et al., 2014). For example, it does not include the length of the segments, the type and proportion of artifacts rejected, the number of trials used for averaging in each condition, specific hypotheses, and the test statistics (in addition to p-values).

      We regret the lack of details. We have included these in the revised Methods, and expanded on the description of the trial rejection (SCADS) algorithm.

      The revised Methods section on EEG Preprocessing mentions the type and proportion of artifacts rejected:

      “We then epoched the data into trials and applied SCADS (Statistical Control of Artifacts in Dense Array EEG/MEG Studies[90]) to identify bad epochs and artifact contaminated channels. SCADS detects artifacts based on three measures: maximum amplitude over time, standard deviation over time, and first derivative (gradient) over time. Any electrode or trial exhibiting values outside the specified boundaries for these measures was excluded. The boundaries were defined as M ± n*λ, where M is the grand median across electrodes and trials for each of the three measures, and λ is the root mean square (RMS) of the deviation of medians across sensors relative to the grand median. We set n to 3, allowing data within three boundaries to be retained. The percentage of electrodes per participant rejected was 6.3 ± 0.43% (mean ± s.e.m. across participants), whereas the percentage of trials rejected per electrode and participant was 3.4 ± 0.33% (mean ± s.e.m.).”

      The revised Methods section on ERP analysis mentions the number of trials for averaging in each condition and the length of the segments:

      “First trials were sorted based on inter-target lags (100, 300, 500, 700 and 900 ms). This yielded an average of (200±13, 171±9.71, 145 ± 7.54, 117 ± 5.43, 87 ± 4.51 ) (mean ± s.e.m. across participants) trials for each of the 5 lags, respectively.”

      “Then, EEG traces were epoched from -300 ms before to +700 ms after either T1 onset or T2 onset and averaged across trials to estimate T1-evoked and T2-evoked ERPs, respectively.”

      Specific hypotheses are mentioned in response #3.1; we also now mention the test statistic associated with each test at the appropriate places in the Results. For example:

      “Among these ERP components, the N2p component and the P2 component were both significantly suppressed during the blink (∆amplitude, short-lag – long-lag: N2p=-0.47 ± 0.12 µV, z=-3.20, p=0.003, BF=40, P2=-0.19 ± 0.07 µV, z=-2.54, p=0.021, BF=4.83, signed rank test) (Fig. 4A, right). Similarly, the parietal P3 also showed a significant blink-induced suppression (P3= -0.45 ± 0.09µV, z=-3.59, p < 0.001, BF>10<sup>2</sup>) (Fig. 4B, right).”

      “Neural inter-class distances (||η||) along both the detection and discrimination dimensions decreased significantly during the blink (short lag-long lag: ∆||ηdet|| = -1.30 ± 0.70, z=-3.68, p=0.006, BF=20; ∆||ηdis|| = -1.23 ± 0.42, z=-3.54, p<0.001, BF>10<sup>2</sup>) (Figs. 6C-D).”

      (3.8) EEG filters: P. 31, l. 728: "The data were (...) bandpass filtered between 0.5 to 18 Hz (...). Next, a bandstop filter from 9-11 Hz was applied to remove the 10 Hz oscillations evoked by the RSVP presentation." These filter settings do not follow common recommendations and could potentially induce filter distortions (e.g., Luck, 2014; Zhang et al., 2024). For example, the 0.5 high-pass filter could distort the slow P3 wave. Mostly, I am concerned about the bandstop filter. Since the authors commendably corrected for RSVP-evoked responses by subtracting T2-absent from T2-present ERPs (p. 31, l. 746), I wonder why the additional filter was necessary, and whether it might have removed relevant peaks in the ERPs of interest.

      Thank you for this suggestion. Originally, the 9-11 Hz bandstop filter was added to remove the strong 10 Hz evoked oscillation from the EEG response for obtaining a cleaner signal for the other analyses, like the analysis of neural dimensions (Fig. 6)

      We performed two control ERP analyses to address the reviewers’ concern:

      (1) We removed the bandstop filter and re-evaluated the P1, P2, N2pc and P3 ERP amplitudes. We observed no statistically significant difference in the modulation of any of the 4 ERP components (P1: p=0.031, BF=0.692, P2: p=0.038, BF=1.21, N2pc: p=0.286, BF=0.269, P3: p=0.085, BF=0.277). In particular, Bayes Factor analysis revealed substantial evidence against a difference in the N2pc and P3 amplitudes before versus after the bandstop filter removal (BF<0.3).

      (2) We removed the bandstop filter and repeated all of the same analyses as reported in the Results and summarized in SI Table S2. We observed a virtually identical pattern of results, summarized in an analogous table, below (compare with SI Table S2, revised, in the Supplementary Information).

      Author response table 2.

      We have now mentioned this control analysis briefly in the Methods (lines 863-865).

      (3.9) Coherence analysis: P. 33, l. 786: "For subsequent, partial correlation analyses of coherence with behavioral metrics and neural distances (...), we focused on a 300 ms time period (0-300 ms following T2 onset) and high-beta frequency band (20-30 Hz) identified by the cluster-based permutation test (Fig. 5A-C)." I wonder whether there were any a priori criteria for the definition and selection of such successive analyses. Given the many factors (frequency bands, hemispheres) in the analyses and the particular shape of the cluster (p. 49, Fig 5C), this focus seems largely data-driven. It remains unclear how many such tests were performed and whether the results (e.g., the resulting weak correlation of r = 0.22 in one frequency band and one hemisphere in one part of a complexly shaped cluster; p. 15, l. 327) can be considered robust.

      Please see responses to comments #3.1 and #3.2 (above). In addition to reporting further details regarding statistical tests, their hypotheses, and multiple comparisons corrections, we computed Bayes factors to quantify the strength of the evidence for correlations, as appropriate. Interpretations have been rephrased depending on whether the evidence for the null or alternative hypothesis is strong or equivocal. For example:

      “Bayes factor analysis revealed no clear evidence for or against a correlation between these subcomponent deficits (BF=1.18) (SI Fig. S2, left).”

      “Discrimination accuracy deficits were not statistically significantly different between high and low detection accuracy deficit blocks (z=1.97, p=0.067), and the Bayes factor revealed no strong evidence for or against such a difference (BF=1.42) (Fig. 3G).”

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1.a) Line 76-79: "Despite this extensive literature, previous studies have essentially treated the attentional blink as a unitary, monolithic phenomenon. As a result, fundamental questions regarding the component mechanisms of the attentional blink remain unanswered." This statement seems antithetical to the fact that theories of the AB suggest a variety of different mechanisms as possible causes of the effect.

      The statement has been revised as follows:

      “Despite this extensive literature, many previous studies have[ studied the attentional blink as a unitary phenomenon. While some theoretical models9,21,32] and experimental studies[38,39] have explored distinct mechanisms underlying the attentional blink, several fundamental questions about its distinct component mechanisms remain unanswered.”

      (1.b) Line 95-97: Here, the authors should explain in more detail how a response bias could fluctuate across lags.

      Addressed in response to public reviews, #1.1.

      (1.c) Line 98: I found this second question a much more compelling motivation for the study than the earlier stated question of whether the AB reflects a reduction in sensitivity or a fluctuation (?) of response bias.

      Thank you.

      (1.d) Line 143: What do the authors mean by "geometric" distribution of lags? In virtually all AB studies, the distribution of lags is uniform. Wasn't that the case in this study?

      We employed a geometric distribution for the trials of different lags, and verified that the sampled distribution of lags was well fit by this distribution (χ<sup>2</sup>(3, 312)=0.22, p=0.974). We chose a geometric distribution – with a flat hazard function[11] – over the uniform distribution to avoid conflating the effects of temporal expectation with those of the attention blink on criterion[12] at different lags.

      (1.e) Line 158-160: Explain why incorrect discrimination responses were not counted as correct detection. Explain why failure to detect T2 was counted as a discrimination error.

      Addressed in response to public reviews, #1.2.

      (1.f) Line 167: The results do not show lag-1 sparing, which is a typical property of the AB.

      The authors should report this, and explain why their paradigm did not show a sparing effect.

      Addressed in response to public reviews, #3.5.

      (1.g) Line 262-263: With only 24 participants, the study appears to be underpowered to reliably detect correlations. This should be noted as a limitation.

      Addressed in response to public reviews, #3.2.

      (1.h) Line 399-412: This section could be moved to the introduction to explain and motivate the aim of examining the distinct contributions of detection and discrimination to the AB.

      We have revised the Introduction to better motivate the aims of the study.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (2.a) A small note about the writing: as a matter of style, I would advise editing the generic phrasing (e.g., "shedding new light", "complex interplay") in abstract and general discussion.

      These are now revised as follows (for example):

      Line 26 - “These findings provide detailed insights into the subcomponents of the attentional blink….”

      Line 596 - “More broadly, these findings contribute to our understanding of the relationship between attention and perception….”

      (2.b) Some references appear double and/or without volume or page numbers (e.g., 44/61).

      Thank you. Amended now.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (3.a) Suggestions for additional analyses:

      I appreciate that the authors have quantified the evidence for null effects in simple comparisons using Bayes factors. In my opinion, the study would additionally benefit from Bayesian ANOVAs, which can also easily be implemented in JASP (Keysers et al., 2020), which the authors have already used for the other tests. As a result, they could further substantiate some of their claims related to null effects (e.g., p. 9, l. 175; p. 12, l. 246).

      Thank you. We have added Bayes factor values for ANOVAs (implemented in JASP[13]) wherever applicable in the revised manuscript. For example:

      “While we found a main effect of both lag (detection: F(1,23)=29.8, p<0.001, BF >10<sup>3</sup> discrimination: F(1,23)=54.1, p<0.001, BF >10<sup>3</sup>) and contrast (detection: F(1,23)=21.02, p<0.001, BF>10<sup>2</sup>, discrimination: F(1,23) =13.75, p=0.001, BF=1.22), we found no significant interaction effect between lag and contrast (detection: F(1,23)=1.92, p=0.113, BF=0.49, discrimination: F(1,23) = 0.93, p=0.450, BF=0.4).”

      “A two-way ANOVA with inter-target lag and T2 contrast as independent factors revealed a main effect of lag on both d’<sub>det</sub> (F(1,23)=30.3, p<0.001, BF>10<sup>3</sup>) and d’<sub>dis</sub> (F(1,23)=100.3, p<0.001, BF>10<sup>3</sup>). Yet, we found no significant interaction effect between lag and contrast for d’<sub>det</sub> (F(1,23)=2.3, p=0.141, BF=0.44).”

      Minor points

      (3.b) Statistics: Many p-values are reported without the respective test statistics (e.g., p. 9, l. 164; p. 12, l. 241-244 and 252-258; p. 13, l. 271, etc.).

      Addressed in response to public reviews, #3.7.

      (3.c) P. 4, l. 58: It is not entirely clear how the authors define "early or late". For example, while they consider the P2/N2/N2pc complex as "late" (l. 62-64), these ERP components are considered "early" in the debate on "early vs. late" neural correlates of consciousness (for a review, see Förster et al., 2020).

      We appreciate the debate. Our naming convention follows these seminal works[3,14–16].

      (3.d) P. 5., l. 77: "previous studies have essentially treated the attentional blinks as a unitary, monolithic phenomenon": There are previous studies in which both the presence and identity of T2 were queried (e.g., Eiserbeck et al., 2022; Harris et al., 2013).

      Addressed in response to recommendations for authors, #1.a.

      (3.e) P. 9, l. 169-177: The detection and discrimination accuracies are analyzed using twoway ANOVAs with the factors lags and contrast. I wonder why the lag effects are additionally analyzed using Wilcoxon signed rank tests using data pooled across the T2 contrasts (p., 9, l. 161-168)? If I understand it correctly, these tests should correspond to the main effects of lag in the ANOVAs. Indeed, both analyses lead to the same conclusions (l. 167 and l. 176).

      Our motivation was to first establish the attentional blink effect, with data pooled across contrasts. The subsequent ANOVA allowed delving deeper into contrast and interaction effects. Indeed, the results were consistent across both tests.

      (3.f) P. 12, l. 242: I wonder why the T2 contrasts are pooled in the statistical tests (but plotted separately, p. 45, Figure 3C).

      Model selection analysis distinct d’<sub>det</sub> parameter values across contrasts, as reflected in Fig. 3C. As mentioned in response #3.e contrasts effects were analyzed with an ANOVA.

      (3.g) P. 13, l. 287: "high and low contrast T2 trials were pooled to estimate reliable ERPs". The amount of trials per condition is not provided.

      Addressed in response to public reviews, #3.7.

      (3.h) P. 45, Figure 3D/F: In my opinion, plotting the contrasts and lags separately (despite the results of the model selection) would have provided a better idea of the data.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion, but followed the results of model selection for consistency.

      (3.i) P. 21, l. 470: "the left index finger to report clockwise orientations and the right index finger to report counter-clockwise orientations": This left/right mapping seems counterintuitive to me, and the authors also used the opposite mapping in Figures 1 and 2. It is not described in the Methods (p. 25) and thus is unclear.

      We regret the typo. Revised as follows:

      “...the left index finger to report counter-clockwise orientations and the right index finger to report clockwise orientations.”

      (3.j) P. 22, l. 514: "Taken together, these results suggest the following, testable schema (SI Figure S5)." Figure S5 seems to be missing.

      Amended. This is Fig. 8 in the revised manuscript.

      (3.k) P. 25, l. 559: I do not understand why the circular placeholders around the stimuli were included, and they are not mentioned in Figure 2A (p. 43). When I saw the figure and read the inscription, I wondered whether they were actually part of the stimulus presentation or symbolized something else.

      The placeholder was described in the earlier Methods section. We have now also mentioned it in caption for Fig. 2A.

      “All plaids were encircled by a circular placeholder. The fixation dot and the placeholder were present on the screen throughout the trial.”

      This avoided spatial uncertainty with estimating stimulus dimensions during the presentation.

      (3.l) P. 32, l. 754: The interval of interest for the P1 from 40 to 140 ms seems unusually early to me. The component usually peaks at 100 ms (e.g., at 96 ms in the cited study by Sergent et al., 2005), which also seems to be the case in the present study (Fig. S3, p. 57). I wonder how they were defined.

      For our analyses, we employed the peak value of the P1 ERP component in a window from 40-140 ms. The peak occurred around 100 ms (SI Fig. S3), which aligns with the literature.

      Additional minor comments:

      These comments have been all addressed, and typos corrected, by revising the manuscript at the appropriate places.

      3.m.1. L. 14: In my opinion, this sentence is difficult to read due to the nested combination of singular and plural forms. Importantly, as the authors also acknowledge (e.g., l. 83), perceptual sensitivity and choice bias could both be compromised, so I would suggest using plural and adding "or both" as a third option for clarity. See also p. 10, l. 204.

      3.m.2. L. 14: The comma before "As a result" should be replaced by a period.

      3.m.3. L. 45 "to guide Behavior" should be lowercase.

      3.m.4. L. 67: "Activity in the parietal, lateral prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex" could be read as if there was a "parietal, prefrontal cortex", so I would suggest removing the first "cortex".

      Revised/amended.

      3.m.5. L. 77: "fundamental questions regarding the component mechanisms of the attentional blink remain unanswered": The term "component mechanisms" is a bit unclear to me.

      We elaborate on this term in the very next set of paragraphs in the Introduction.

      3.m.6. L. 88: "a lower proportion of correct T2 detections can arise from a lower detection d'". "Arise from" sounds a bit off given that d' is a function of hits and false alarms.

      3.m.7. L. 95: I would suggest citing the updated edition of the classic "Detection Theory: A User's Guide" by Hautus, Macmillan & Creelman (2021).

      3.m.8. L. 102: "a oriented grating" should be "an".

      3.m.9. L. 126: "key neural markers - a local neural marker (event-related potentials) potentials" should be rephrased/corrected.

      3.m.10. L. 129: There are inconsistent tenses (mostly past tense but "we synthesize").

      3.m.11. L. 138: Perhaps the abbreviations (e.g., dva, cpd) should be introduced here (first mention) rather than in the Methods below.

      3.m.12. L. 148: "at the end of each trial participants first, indicated": The comma position should be changed.

      3.m.13. L. 176 "attentional blink-induced both a ...": The hyphen should be removed.

      3.m.14. L. 396: I think "but neither of them affects" would be better here.

      3.m.15. L. 383: "Detection deficits were signaled by ERP components such as the occipitoparietal N2p and the parietal P3": In my opinion, "such as" is too vague here.

      Revised/amended.

      3.m.16. L. 403: "Neurally, improved detection of attended targets is accompanied by (...) higher ERP amplitudes". Given the different mechanisms underlying the ERP, this section would benefit from more details.

      Addressed in response to public reviews, #3.4.

      3.m.17.    L. 924: References 18 and 46 seem to be the same.

      3.m.18.    L. 1181: I think d'det should be d'dis here.

      3.m.19.    L. 1284: "détection" should be "detection".

      3.m.20.    I found some Figure legends a bit confusing. For example, 5E refers to 4E, but 4E refers to 4C.

      3.m.21.    In Figures 4A/B and 6C/D, some conditions are hidden due to the overlap of CIs. Could they be made more transparent?

      Revised/amended.

      References:

      (1) Fook K.Chua. The effect of target contrast on the attentional blink. Percept Psychophys 5, 770–788 (2005).

      (2) Chmielewski, W. X., Mückschel, M., Dippel, G. & Beste, C. Concurrent information affects response inhibition processes via the modulation of theta oscillations in cognitive control networks. Brain Struct Funct 221, 3949–3961 (2016).

      (3) Sergent, C., Baillet, S. & Dehaene, S. Timing of the brain events underlying access to consciousness during the attentional blink. Nat Neurosci 8, 1391–400 (2005).

      (4) Zivony, A. & Lamy, D. What processes are disrupted during the attentional blink? An integrative review of event-related potential research. Psychon Bull Rev 29, 394–414 (2022).

      (5) Pernet, C. R., Wilcox, R. & Rousselet, G. A. Robust Correlation Analyses: False Positive and Power Validation Using a New Open Source Matlab Toolbox. Front Psychol 3, (2013).

      (6) Gross, J. et al. Modulation of long-range neural synchrony reflects temporal limitations of visual attention in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 101, 13050–13055 (2004).

      (7) Eric Maris and Robert Oostenveld. Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG and MEG data. J Neurosci Methods 164, 177–190 (2007).

      (8) Hommel, B. & Akyürek, E. G. Lag-1 sparing in the attentional blink: Benefits and costs of integrating two events into a single episode. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A 58, 1415–1433 (2005).

      (9) Livesey, E. J. & Harris, I. M. Target sparing effects in the attentional blink depend on type of stimulus. Atten Percept Psychophys 73, 2104–2123 (2011).

      (10) Dellert, T. et al. Neural correlates of consciousness in an attentional blink paradigm with uncertain target relevance. Neuroimage 264, 119679 (2022).

      (11) Nobre, A., Correa, A. & Coull, J. The hazards of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol 17, 465– 470 (2007).

      (12) Bang, J. W. & Rahnev, D. Stimulus expectation alters decision criterion but not sensory signal in perceptual decision making. Sci Rep 7, 17072 (2017).

      (13) JASP Team. JASP (version 0.19.0.) [Computer Software]. Preprint at (2022).

      (14) Luck, S. J. Electrophysiological Correlates of the Focusing of Attention within Complex Visual Scenes: N2pc and Related ERP Components. (Oxford University Press, 2011). doi:10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195374148.013.0161.

      (15) Brydges, C. R., Fox, A. M., Reid, C. L. & Anderson, M. Predictive validity of the N2 and P3 ERP components to executive functioning in children: a latent-variable analysis. Front Hum Neurosci 8, (2014).

      (16) Michalewski, H. J., Prasher, D. K. & Starr, A. Latency variability and temporal interrelationships of the auditory event-related potentials (N1, P2, N2, and P3) in normal subjects. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section 65, 59–71 (1986).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We thank the reviewers for the careful review of our manuscript. Overall, they were positive about our use of cutting-edge methods to identify six inversions segregating in Lake Malawi. Their distribution in ~100 species of Lake Malawi species demonstrated that they were differentially segregating in different ecogroups/habitats and could potentially play a role in local adaptation, speciation, and sex determination. Reviewers were positive about our finding that the chromosome 10 inversion was associated with sex-determination in a deep benthic species and its potential role in regulating traits under sexual selection. They agree that this work is an important starting point in understanding the role of these inversions in the amazing phenotypic diversity found in the Lake Malawi cichlid flock.

      There were two main criticisms that were made which we summarize:

      (1) Lack of clarity. It was noted that the writing could be improved to make many technical points clearer. Additionally, certain discussion topics were not included that should be.

      We will rewrite the text and add additional figures and tables to address the issues that were brought up in a point-by-point response. We will improve/include (1) the nomenclature to understand the inversions in different lineages, (2) improved descriptions for various genomic approaches, (3) a figure to document the samples and technologies used for each ecogroup, and 4) integration of LR sequences to identify inversion breakpoints to the finest resolution possible.

      (2) We overstate the role that selection plays in the spread of these inversions and neglect other evolutionary processes that could be responsible for their spread.

      We agree with the overarching point. We did not show that selection is involved in the spread of these inversions and other forces can be at play. Additionally, there were concerns with our model that the inversions introgressed from a Diplotaxodon ancestor into benthic ancestors and incomplete lineage sorting or balancing selection (via sex determination) could be at play. Overall, we agree with the reviewers with the following caveats. 1. Our analysis of the genetic distance between Diplotaxodons and benthic species in the inverted regions is more consistent with their spread through introgression versus incomplete lineage sorting or balancing selection. 2. Further the role of these inversions is likely different in different species. For example, the inversion of 10 and 11 play a role in sex determination in some species but not others and the potential pressures acting on the inverted and non-inverted haplotypes will be very different. These are very interesting and important questions booth for understanding the adaptive radiations in Lake Malawi and in general, and we are actively studying crosses to understand the role of these inversions in phenotypic variation between two species. We will modify the text to make all of these points clearer.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Using high-quality genomic data (long-reads, optical maps, short-reads) and advanced bioinformatic analysis, the authors aimed to document chromosomal rearrangements across a recent radiation (Lake Malawi Cichlids). Working on 11 species, they achieved a high-resolution inversion detection and then investigated how inversions are distributed within populations (using a complementary dataset of short-reads), associated with sex, and shared or fixed among lineages. The history and ancestry of the inversions is also explored.

      On one hand, I am very enthusiastic about the global finding (many inversions well-characterized in a highly diverse group!) and impressed by the amount of work put into this study. On the other hand, I have struggled so much to read the manuscript that I am unsure about how much the data supports some claims. I'm afraid most readers may feel the same and really need a deep reorganisation of the text, figures, and tables. I reckon this is difficult given the complexity brought by different inversions/different species/different datasets but it is highly needed to make this study accessible.

      The methods of comparing optical maps, and looking at inversions at macro-evolutionary scales can be useful for the community. For cichlids, it is a first assessment that will allow further tests about the role of inversions in speciation and ecological specialisation. However, the current version of the manuscript is hardly accessible to non-specialists and the methods are not fully reproducible.

      Strengths:

      (1) Evidence for the presence of inversion is well-supported by optical mapping (very nice analysis and figure!).

      (2) The link between sex determination and inversion in chr 10 in one species is very clearly demonstrated by the proportion in each sex and additional crosses. This section is also the easiest to read in the manuscript and I recommend trying to rewrite other result sections in the same way.

      (3) A new high-quality reference genome is provided for Metriaclima zebra (and possibly other assemblies? - unclear).

      (4) The sample size is great (31 individuals with optical maps if I understand well?).

      (5) Ancestry at those inversions is explored with outgroups.

      (6) Polymorphism for all inversions is quantified using a complementary dataset.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Lack of clarity in the paper: As it currently reads, it is very hard to follow the different species, ecotypes, samples, inversions, etc. It would be useful to provide a phylogeny explicitly positioning the samples used for assembly and the habitat preference. Then the text would benefit from being organised either by variant or by subgroups rather than by successive steps of analysis.

      We have extensively rewritten the paper to improve the clarity. With respect to this point, we moved Figure 6 to Figure 1, which places the phylogeny of Lake Malawi cichlids at the beginning of the paper. We incorporated information about samples/technologies by ecogroup into this figure to help the reader gain an overview of the technologies involved. We added information about habitat for each ecogroup as well. While we considered a change to the text organization suggested here, we thought it was clearer to keep the original headings.

      (2) Lack of information for reproducibility: I couldn't find clearly the filters and parameters used for the different genomic analyses for example. This is just one example and I think the methods need to be re-worked to be reproducible. Including the codes inside the methods makes it hard to follow, so why not put the scripts in an indexed repository?

      We now provide a link to a github repository (https://github.com/ptmcgrat/CichlidSRSequencing/tree/Kumar_eLife) containing the scripts used for the major analysis in the paper. Because our data is behind a secure Dropbox account, readers will not be able to run the analysis, however, they can see the exact programs, filters, and parameters used for manuscript embedded within each script.

      (3) Further confirmation of inversions and their breakpoints would be valuable. I don't understand why the long-reads (that were available and used for genome assembly) were not also used for SV detection and breakpoint refinement.

      We did use long reads to confirm the presence of the inversions by creating five new genome assemblies from the PacBio HiFi reads: two additional Metriaclima zebra samples and three Aulonocara samples. Alignment of these five genomes to the MZ_GT3 reference is shown in Figures S2 – S7. These genome assemblies were also used to identify the breakpoints of the inversions. However, because of the extensive amount of repetitive DNA at the breakpoints (which is known to be important for the formation of large inversions), our ability to resolve the breakpoints was limited.

      (4) Lack of statistical testing for the hypothesis of introgression: Although cichlids are known for high levels of hybridization, inversions can also remain balanced for a long time. what could allow us to differentiate introgression from incomplete lineage sorting?

      The coalescent time between the inversions between Diplotaxodons and benthics should allow us to distinguish these two mechanisms. Our finding that the genetic distance, which is related to coalescent time, is closer within the inversions than the whole genome is supportive of introgression. However, we did not perform any simulations or statistical tests. We make it clearer in the text that incomplete lineage sorting remains a possible mechanism for the distribution of inversions within these ecogroups.

      (5) The sample size is unclear: possibly 31 for Bionano, 297 for short-reads, how many for long-reads or assemblies? How is this sample size split across species? This would deserve a table.

      We have included this information in the new Figure 1.

      (6) Short read combines several datasets but batch effect is not tested.

      We do not test for batch effect. However, we do note that all of the datasets were analyzed by the same pipeline starting from alignment so batch effects would be restricted to aspects of the reads themselves. Additionally, samples from the different data sets clustered as expected by lineage and inferred inversion, so for these purposes unlikely to have affected analysis.

      (7) It is unclear how ancestry is determined because the synteny with outgroups is not shown.

      Ancestry analysis was determined using the genome alignments of two outgroups from outside of Lake Malawi. This is shown in Figure S8.

      (8) The level of polymorphism for the different inversions is difficult to interpret because it is unclear whether replicated are different species within an eco-group or different individuals from the same species. How could it be that homozygous references are so spread across the PCA? I guess the species-specific polymorphism is stronger than the ancestral order but in such a case, wouldn't it be worth re-doing the PCa on a subset?

      The genomic PCA plots reflect the evolutionary histories that are observed in the whole genome phylogenies. Because the distribution of the inverted alleles violate the species tree, they form separate clusters on the PCA plots that can be used to genotype specific species. We have also performed this analysis on benthics (utaka/shallow benthics/deep benthics) and the distribution matches the expectation.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Chromosomal inversions have been predicted to play a role in adaptive evolution and speciation because of their ability to "lock" together adaptive alleles in genomic regions of low recombination. In this study, the authors use a combination of cutting-edge genomic methods, including BioNano and PacBio HiFi sequencing, to identify six large chromosomal inversions segregating in over 100 species of Lake Malawi cichlids, a classic example of adaptive radiation and rapid speciation. By examining the frequencies of these inversions present in species from six different linages, the authors show that there is an association between the presence of specific inversions with specific lineages/habitats. Using a combination of phylogenetic analyses and sequencing data, they demonstrate that three of the inversions have been introduced to one lineage via hybridization. Finally, genotyping of wild individuals as well as laboratory crosses suggests that three inversions are associated with XY sex determination systems in a subset of species. The data add to a growing number of systems in which inversions have been associated with adaptation to divergent environments. However, like most of the other recent studies in the field, this study does not go beyond describing the presence of the inversions to demonstrate that the inversions are under sexual or natural selection or that they contribute to adaptation or speciation in this system.

      Strengths:

      All analyses are very well done, and the conclusions about the presence of the six inversions in Lake Malawi cichlids, the frequencies of the inversions in different species, and the presence of three inversions in the benthic lineages due to hybridization are well-supported. Genotyping of 48 individuals resulting from laboratory crosses provides strong support that the chromosome 10 inversion is associated with a sex-determination locus.

      Weaknesses:

      The evidence supporting a role for the chromosome 11 inversion and the chromosome 9 inversion in sex determination is based on relatively few individuals and therefore remains suggestive. The authors are mostly cautious in their interpretations of the data. However, there are a few places where they state that the inversions are favored by selection, but they provide no evidence that this is the case and there is no consideration of alternative hypotheses (i.e. that the inversions might have been fixed via drift).

      We have removed mention of chromosome 9’s potential role in sex determination from the paper. While our analysis of sex association with chromosome 11 was limited compared to our analysis of chromosome 10, it was still statistically significant, and we believe it should be left in the paper. The role of 11 (and 9 and 10) in sex determination was also demonstrated using an independent dataset by Blumer et al (https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.07.28.605452)

      We agree that we did not properly consider alternative hypothesis in the original submission and have rewritten the Discussion substantially to consider various alternative hypothesis.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This is a very interesting paper bringing truly fascinating insight into the genomic processes underlying the famous adaptive radiation seen in cichlid fishes from Lake Malawi. The authors use structural and sequence information from species belonging to distinct ecotypic categories, representing subclades of the radiation, to document structural variation across the evolutionary tree, infer introgression of inversions among branches of the clade, and even suggest that certain rearrangements constitute new sex-determining loci. The insight is intriguing and is likely to make a substantial contribution to the field and to seed new hypotheses about the ecological processes and adaptive traits involved in this radiation.

      I think the paper could be clarified in its prose, and that the discussion could be more informative regarding the putative roles of the inversions in adaptation to each ecotypic niche. Identifying key, large inversions shared in various ways across the different taxa is really a great step forward. However, the population genomics analysis requires further work to describe and decipher in a more systematic way the evolutionary forces at play and their consequences on the various inversions identified.

      The model of evolution involving multiple inversions putatively linking together co-adapted "cassettes" could be better spelled out since it is not entirely clear how the existing theory on the recruitment of inversions in local adaptation (e.g. Kirkpatrick and Barton) operates on multiple unlinked inversions. How such loci correspond to distinct suites of integrated traits, or not, is not very easy to envision in the current state of the manuscript.

      This is a very interesting point, and we agree creates complications for a simple model of local adaptation. We imagine though that the actual evolutionary history was much more complicated than a single Rhamphochromis-type species separating from a single Diplotaxodon-type species and could have occurred sequentially involving multiple species that are now extinct. A better understanding of the role each of these inversions play in phenotypic diversity could potentially help us determine if different inversions carry variation that could be linked to distinct habit differences. We have added a line to the discussion.

      The role of one inversion in sex determination is apparent and truly intriguing. However, the implication of such locus on ecological adaptation is somewhat puzzling. Also, whether sex determination loci can flow across species via introgression seems quite important as a route to chromosomal sex determination, so this could be discussed further.

      Another very interesting point. If the inversions are involved in ecological adaptation (an important caveat), then potentially the inverted and non-inverted haplotypes play dual roles in the Aulonocara animals with the inverted haplotype carrying adaptive alleles to deep water and the non-inverted haplotype carrying alleles resolving sexual conflict. We have broadened our discussion about their function at the origin including non-adaptive roles.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Overall, the paper is well-written and clear. I do have a few suggestions for changes that would help the reader:

      (1) Figure 1: the figure legend could be expanded here to help the reader; what are the blue and yellow lines? Why are there two lines for the GT3a assembly? And, I had to somehow read the legend a few times to understand that the top line is the UMD2a reference assembly, and the next line is the new Bionano map.

      Fixed in what is now Figure 2

      (2) Paragraph starting on line 133: you use the word "test" to refer to the Bionano analyses; it is not clear whether anything is being tested. Perhaps "analyse the maps" or just "map" would be more clear? Or more explanation?

      The text has been modified to address this point

      (3) L145-146: perhaps change "a single inversion" and "a double inversion" to "single inversions" and "double inversions".

      The text has been modified to address this point

      (4) L157: suppression of recombination in inversion heterozygotes is "textbook" material and perhaps does not need a reference. Or, you could reference an empirical paper that demonstrates this point. Though I love the Kirkpatrick and Barton paper, it certainly is not the correct reference for this point.

      The Kirkpatrick reference was incorrectly included here. The correct reference was an empirical demonstration (Conte) that there were regions of suppressed recombination that have been observed in the location of the inversions. We have also moved this reference further up in the sentence to a more appropriate position

      (5) L173: how do you know this is an assembly error and not polymorphism?

      The text has been modified to address this point

      (6) L277(?): "currently growing in the lab" is probably unnecessary.

      The text has been modified to address this point

      (7) L298: "the inversion on 10 acts as an XY sex determiner": the inversion itself is not the sex determination gene; rather, it is linked. I think it would be more precise, here and throughout the paper, to say that these inversions likely harbor the sex determination locus (for example, the wording on lines 369-370 is misleading).

      We agree with the larger point that the inversion might not be causal for sex determination, however, it could still be causal through positional effects. We have modified the text to make it clear that it could also carry the causal locus (or loci).

      (8) Figure 6: overall, this figure is very helpful! However, it contains several problematic statements. In no case do you have evidence that these inversions are "favored by selection"; such statements should be deleted. Also, in point 3, you state that inversions 9, 11, and 20 are transferred to benthic lineages, and then that these inversions are involved in sex determination. But, your data suggests that it is chromosomes 9, 10, and 11 that are linked to sex determination.

      This figure is now Figure 1. We have remove these problematic statements.

      (9) L356-360: I would move the references that are currently at the end of the sentence to line 357 after the statement about the previous work on hybridization. Otherwise, it reads as if these previous papers demonstrated what you have demonstrated in your work.

      The text has been modified to address this point

      (10) Overall, the discussion focuses completely on adaptive explanations for your results, and I would like to see at least an acknowledgement that drift could also be involved unless you have additional data to support adaptive explanations.

      We have rewritten the text to account for the possibility of drift (line 404 and 405).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The paper utilizes heterogeneous datasets coming from different sources, and it is not always clear which specimens were used to generate structural information (bionano) or sequence information. A diagram summarizing the sequence data, methodologies, and research questions would be beneficial for the reader to navigate in this paper.

      Much of this information has been added to what is now Figure 1. All of this data is also found in Table S2.

      The authors performed genome alignments to analyze and homologize inversion, but this process is not clearly described. For the PCA, SNP information likely involves mapping onto a common reference genome. However, it is not clear how this was achieved given the different species and varying divergence times involved.

      We now include a link to the github that contains the commands that were run. Because the overall level of sequence divergence between cichlid species is quite low (2*10^-3 – Milansky et al), mapping different species onto a common reference is commonly performed in Lake Malawi cichlids.

      The introgression scenario is very intriguing but its role in local adaptation of the ecogroup types is not easy to understand. I understand this is still an outstanding question, but it is unclear how the directionality of introgressions was estimated. This can be substantiated using tree topology analysis, comparative estimates of sequence divergence, and accumulation of DNA insertions. The diagram does not clearly indicate which ones are polymorphic. In some cases, polymorphic inversions could result from the coexistence of native and introgressed haplotypes.

      We agree that this analysis would be interesting but is beyond the scope of this paper.

      The alternative model of introgression proposed in the cited preprint is interesting and should deserve a formal analysis here. The authors consider unclear what would drive "back" introgressions of non-inverted haplotypes, but this would depend on the selection regimes acting on the inversions themselves, which can include forms of balancing selection and a role for recessive lethals (heterozygote advantage). For instance, a standard haplotype could be favored if it shelters deleterious mutations carried by an inversion. Testing the introgression history over a wider range of branches and directions would provide further insights.

      We agree that this analysis would be interesting but is beyond the scope of this paper.

      The prose in the paper is occasionally muddled and somewhat unclear. Referring to chromosomes solely by their numbers (e.g.. "inversion on 11") complicates readability.

      This is the standard way to refer to chromosomes in cichlids and we believe while it complicates readability, any other method would be inconsistent with other papers. Changes to nomenclature might improve the readability of this paper, but would make it more difficult to compare results for these chromosomes from other papers with what we have found.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The current work explored the link between the pulvinar intrinsic organisation and its functional and structural connectivity patterns of the cortex using different dimensional reduction techniques. Overall they find relationships between pulvinar-cortical organization and cortico-cortical organization, and little evidence for clustered organization. Moreover, they investigate PET maps to understand how neurotransmitter/receptor distributions vary within the pulvinar and along its structural and functional connectivity axes.

      Strengths:

      There is a replication dataset and different modalities are compared against each other to understand the structural and functional organisation of the pulvinar complex.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) What is the motivation of the study and how does this work extend previous assessments of the organization of the complete thalamus within the gradient framework?

      Thank you for raising this central question. As already mentioned in the main text, pulvinar is one of the largest and prototypical associative nuclei, yet its organizational principles in the human brain remain relatively unexplored. The substantial body of anatomical research conducted in primate species suggests the coexistence of multiple coexisting and overlapping corticotopic representations on the pulvinar complex.

      Existing connectivity-based parcellation studies of pulvinar organization often overlook these organizational principles, as the resulting parcellation may reflect a linear combination of single overlapping connectopies rather than accurately capturing their distinct and unique spatial arrangement.

      Investigations of thalamic connectivity have already revealed overarching organizational principles within the thalamus, which are partially reflected in its cytoarchitecture subdivision. These principles are associated with core and matrix thalamic neuronal subpopulation, and their distinct contributions to large-scale connectivity networks.

      Since gradient selection relies on the explained variance of the diffusion embeddings, and pulvinar-cortical connectivity likely accounts for only a limited portion of the variance in thalamocortical connectivity, we chose to focus specifically on the pulvinar nucleus. This approach was intended to ensure that the local connectivity principles of the pulvinar are not overshadowed by the broader connectotopical organization of the entire thalamus.

      This rationale aligns with findings in topographically organized regions of the cerebral cortex, such as M1, S1 or visual areas. In these regions, distinct principles of topographical organization are not readily apparent when analyzing whole-brain connectivity embedding but emerge when dimensionality reduction is applied to region-specific connectivity data.

      (2) Why is the current atlas chosen for the delineation of the pulvinar and individualized maps not considered? Given the size of the pulvinar, more validation of the correctness of the atlas may be helpful.

      To improve signal-to-noise ratio and in alignment with previous studies, we performed diffusion embedding on the group-level, averaged connectivity matrices rather than estimating gradients at the individual subject level.

      The decision to use a standard-space atlas for pulvinar delineation, rather than individualized parcellation, was driven by technical considerations: 1) functional MRI data were already transformed to MNI space; and 2) individualized parcellation of thalamic nuclei can result in varying pulvinar volumes across subjects, complicating the averaging of connectivity data. By using a standard-space atlas, we ensured that connectivity was consistently extracted from the same set of voxels across all subjects.

      We selected the AAL3 atlas (Rolls et al., 2020)over other existing thalamic atlases for practical reasons: the atlas incorporates an ex-vivo thalamic parcellation (Iglesias et al., 2018) with a specific delineation of pulvinar nuclei, which was necessary for subsequent analyses. In the revised version of the manuscript, to validate our findings, we replicated the pulvinar gradient using a different pulvinar delineation from a recent, thalamus-specific atlas (Su et al., 2019). Notably, the spatial distribution of pulvinar connectivity and coexpression gradients remained consistent, regardless of the choice of the thalamic atlas, underscoring the robustness of our results.

      (3) Overall the study feels a little incremental and a repetition of what others have done already in the thalamus. It would be good to know how focusing only on the pulvinar changes interpretation, for example by comparing thalamic and pulvinar gradients?

      The authors acknowledge the existing body of literature that has examined thalamic connectivity under the lens of the connectivity gradient framework. While these studies may provide valuable insights into the functional topography of the pulvinar complex -given its prominent role within the thalamus - we contend that a focused analysis of pulvinar connectivity offers a unique opportunity to uncover the specific organization principles of this nuclear complex. By isolating the pulvinar, we aimed to avoid the potential overshadowing of its local connectivity patterns by the broader connectotopical organization of the entire thalamus. However, as we believe that our findings are best interpreted within the broader context of general thalamic connectivity organization, we have included an additional paragraph in the Discussion section, which explores the similarities and differences between thalamic and pulvinar gradients, offering a more integrative perspective on our results.

      “In recent years, different works have explored the spatial arrangement of thalamic connectivity within a connectivity gradient framework. Diffusion embedding of thalamocortical functional connectivity has revealed a principal, medio-lateral gradient that was found correlated to thalamic structural subdivisions, and a secondary, antero-posterior gradient associated with thalamic functional subfields, and showing progression from unimodal sensorimotor cortical networks to multimodal attention and associative networks. Interestingly, the principal thalamic gradient shows a medio-lateral arrangement on the pulvinar axis while the secondary gradients correspond more to a ventral-dorsal pulvinar axis (Yang et al. 2020). In particular, further independent investigations have suggested that the progressing pattern of thalamic connectivity from unimodal to transmodal cortices is strongly associated to the local density of core and matrix cell types, thus establishing a link between molecular properties and functional connectivity dynamics (Müller et al. 2020; Huang et al. 2024). Our findings complement and expand the existing literature by revealing a similar arrangement of cortical connectivity patterns on the pulvinar complex, and elucidating its relationship to in-vivo estimates of molecular markers of neurotransmission. We found that the gradient associated to unimodal-transmodal cortical connectivity accounted for the highest percentage of variance of variance in cortico-pulvinar connectivity, in line with its well-acknowledged role of associative nucleus. It is noteworthy that, in analyses of thalamocortical gradients, the pulvinar complex is situated towards the “sensorimotor” extreme of the unimodal-to-transmodal thalamic gradient (Yang et al., 2020). This likely reflects its prominent connectivity to visual and sensory areas compared to other thalamic nuclei. Nevertheless, the extensive and intricate association of pulvinar with multiple cortical networks emerges is strongly evident in various functional connectivity investigations (Basile et al., 2021; Kumar et al., 2017, 2022). By isolating pulvinar-cortical from broader thalamocortical connectivity, our analysis was able to provide additional insights into the spatial organization of its connectivity with different cortical networks, highlighting the pulvinar's remarkable functional diversity and complexity.”

      (4) Could it be that the gradient patterns stem from lacking anatomical and functional resolutions (or low SNR) therefore generating no sharp boundaries?

      The gradient organization described in our results is aligns with anatomical evidence on non-human primates (Shipp, 2003), and with existing neuroimaging studies in humans, which report limited correspondence between connectivity-based hard clustering solutions and histological delineation of pulvinar nuclei. However, we recognize the critical importance of assessing the impact of SNR on connectivity measures derived from functional and structural MRI. In the revised manuscript, we have included an additional analysis to investigate the potential impact of local noise on gradient reconstruction. This analysis involved sampling voxel-wise SNR estimates in the pulvinar from both BOLD and diffusion-weighted MRI data, averaging these estimates to generate group-level, modality-specific SNR maps. We then assessed spatial correlations between these maps and the gradient embeddings using the same methodological framework employed throughout the study. Our findings indicate that functional connectivity gradients are weakly, but significantly correlated to SNR, with the strongest correlation observed for the third gradient (left hemisphere G<sub>FC</sub>1 r= -0.30, SA-corrected p < 0.001, G<sub>FC</sub>2 r= 0.22, SA-corrected p = 0.05, G<sub>FC</sub>3 r= 0.55, SA-corrected p < 0.001; right hemisphere G<sub>FC</sub>1 r= -0.41, SA-corrected p < 0.001, G<sub>FC</sub>2 r= 0.22, SA-corrected p = 0.008, G<sub>FC</sub>3 r= 0.52, SA-corrected p = 0.017). In contrast, structural connectivity gradients showed no significant correlation with SNR (left hemisphere G<sub>SC</sub>1 r= 0.06, SA-corrected p = 0.82, G<sub>SC</sub>2 r= -0.33, SA-corrected p = 0.01; right hemisphere G<sub>SC</sub>1 r= 0.40, SA-corrected p = 0.28, G<sub>SC</sub>2 r=-0.19, SA-corrected p = 0.31).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Please add more literature on thalamus gradients and interpret this with care.

      Thank you for the suggestion. We have added the following paragraph in the Discussion section:

      “In recent years, different works have explored the spatial arrangement of thalamic connectivity within a connectivity gradient framework. Diffusion embedding of thalamocortical functional connectivity has revealed a principal, medio-lateral gradient that was found correlated to thalamic structural subdivisions, and a secondary, antero-posterior gradient associated with thalamic functional subfields, and showing progression from unimodal sensorimotor cortical networks to multimodal attention and associative networks. Interestingly, the principal thalamic gradient shows a medio-lateral arrangement on the pulvinar axis while the secondary gradients correspond more to a ventral-dorsal pulvinar axis (Yang et al. 2020). In particular, further independent investigations have suggested that the progressing pattern of thalamic connectivity from unimodal to transmodal cortices is strongly associated to the local density of core and matrix cell types, thus establishing a link between molecular properties and functional connectivity dynamics (Müller et al. 2020; Huang et al. 2024). Our findings complement and expand the existing literature by revealing a similar arrangement of cortical connectivity patterns on the pulvinar complex, and elucidating its relationship to in-vivo estimates of molecular markers of neurotransmission. We found that the gradient associated to unimodal-transmodal cortical connectivity accounted for the highest percentage of variance of variance in cortico-pulvinar connectivity, in line with its well-acknowledged role of associative nucleus. It is noteworthy that, in analyses of thalamocortical gradients, the pulvinar complex is situated towards the “sensorimotor” extreme of the unimodal-to-transmodal thalamic gradient (Yang et al., 2020). This likely reflects its prominent connectivity to visual and sensory areas compared to other thalamic nuclei. Nevertheless, the extensive and intricate association of pulvinar with multiple cortical networks emerges is strongly evident in various functional connectivity investigations (Basile et al., 2021; Kumar et al., 2017, 2022). By isolating pulvinar-cortical from broader thalamocortical connectivity, our analysis was able to provide additional insights into the spatial organization of its connectivity with different cortical networks, highlighting the pulvinar's remarkable functional diversity and complexity.

      As regards structural connectivity, existing accounts describe a medio-lateral organization of thalamocortical connections, corresponding to an antero-posterior gradient on the cortical mantle. This gradient organization appears to be anchored to genetic markers of different cell types (Oldham and Ball 2023). In line with their findings, we describe a principal axis of structural connectivity in the pulvinar complex that is arranged on the mediolateral axis, and we enforce the notion of a deep relationship between structural connections and molecular expression of neurotransmission markers. On the other hand, the patterns of connectivity with the cerebral cortex do not correspond to a clear antero-posterior axis on the cerebral cortex, probably showing the predominance of local connectivity over the global thalamic structural topography. Further investigations are warranted to ascertain whether the structural gradients of the pulvinar complex may be in continuity with this general cortico-thalamic connectivity gradient.”

      (2) Please state the motivation of the work more clearly and what makes it different from related literature.

      Thank you for pointing us to this lack of clarity. We have added the following paragraph in the Introduction section:

      “In particular, investigations of thalamic connectivity within the gradient framework have uncovered general organizational principles within the thalamus, which are partially reflected in thalamic cytoarchitecture subdivisions. These principles have been linked to core and matrix thalamic neuronal subpopulation, and to their differential contribution to large-scale connectivity networks (Müller et al., 2020; Yang et al., 2020). However, given the remarkable functional complexity and diversity of the pulvinar complex, these global spatial organization patterns likely capture only part of its functional topography. With this in mind, isolating pulvinar connectivity from the remaining thalamocortical connectome would ensure that local organizational principles are not obscured by the global connectotopic structure of the entire thalamus.”

      (3) Why did the authors opt for a whole brain labelling atlas, would a thalamus-specific atlas not be more suitable?

      Despite being a large-scale whole brain atlas, the labeling atlas of choice (AAL3) incorporates a thalamus-specific parcellation from previous work (Iglesias et al., 2018), derived from ex-vivo data and including subdivision of the pulvinar complex into anterior, inferior, lateral and medial nuclei. In the revised version of the manuscript, to validate our findings, we replicated the pulvinar gradient using a different pulvinar delineation from a recent, thalamus-specific atlas (Su et al., 2019). We show these results in Supplementary Figure 1. Notably, the spatial distribution of pulvinar connectivity and coexpression gradients remained consistent, regardless of the choice of the thalamic atlas, underscoring the robustness of our results.

      (4) How did the authors account for the potential low sensitivity of subcortical signals in the PET data?

      We acknowledge the inherent limitations in spatial sensitivity that are a common drawback of PET imaging. However, the PET data employed in the present study were derived from a high-quality dataset collected across multiple studies, predominantly acquired using high resolution scanners (Hansen et al., 2022; see supplementary material at https://static-content.springer.com/esm/art%3A10.1038%2Fs41593-022-01186-3/MediaObjects/41593_2022_1186_MOESM3_ESM.xlsx for technical details). Furthermore, the reliability of neurotransmission markers measurements at the subcortical level has been validated against genetic transcription markers (Hansen, Markello, et al., 2022; Hansen, Shafiei, et al., 2022), ensuring robust and biologically meaningful results.

      (5) What about SNR of the metrics within the pulvinar?

      The referee raises a crucial and complex point, prompting us to conduct additional analyses. We recognize the critical importance of assessing the impact of SNR on connectivity measures derived from functional and structural MRI. In the revised manuscript, we have included an additional analysis to investigate the potential impact of local noise on gradient reconstruction. Therefore, we have incorporated the following text into the manuscript:

      Results (5. Reliability and Reproducibility):

      “To assess the influence of local noise on functional and structural connectivity gradients, we calculated the spatial correlation between gradient values and averaged voxel-wise estimates of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) from functional and structural MRI data, respectively. We found that functional connectivity gradients are weakly, but significantly correlated with the SNR, with the strongest correlation observed for the third gradient (left hemisphere G<sub>FC</sub>1 r= -0.30, SA-corrected p < 0.001, G<sub>FC</sub>2 r= 0.22, SA-corrected p = 0.05, G<sub>FC</sub>3 r= 0.55, SA-corrected p < 0.001; right hemisphere G<sub>FC</sub>1 r= -0.41, SA-corrected p < 0.001, G<sub>FC</sub>2 r= 0.22, SA-corrected p = 0.008, G<sub>FC</sub>3 r= 0.52, SA-corrected p = 0.017). In contrast, structural connectivity gradients were not significantly associated with SNR (left hemisphere G<sub>SC</sub>1 r= 0.06, SA-corrected p = 0.82, G<sub>SC</sub>2 r= -0.33, SA-corrected p = 0.01; right hemisphere G<sub>SC</sub>1 r= 0.40, SA-corrected p = 0.28, G<sub>SC</sub>2 r=-0.19, SA-corrected p = 0.31) (Supplementary Figure 5).”

      Methods (4. Reliability and reproducibility assessment):

      “To evaluate the possible influence of SNR on connectivity-derived diffusion embeddings, we have performed a voxel-wise,

      modality-specific, SNR assessment to investigate correlation between spatial distribution of noise and diffusion embeddings. For each subject, we separately calculated voxel-wise SNR maps for the left and right pulvinar, using both functional (BOLD) volumes and DWI data. For BOLD volumes, we employed the widely accepted definition of temporal signal to noise (tSNR) (Murphy et al., 2006):

      where T<sub>mean</sub> and T<sub>std</sub> are, respectively, the mean and the standard deviation of each voxel’s signal across the time series.

      For the DWI data, we applied a similar approach (Cai et al., 2021) that allows estimation of SNR from multiple b=0 diffusion weighted volumes:

      where S is the voxel’s signal intensity, and the mean (S<sub>mean</sub>) and standard deviation (S<sub>std</sub>) were computed across all the b0-weighted volumes (18 for HCP dataset; 7 for LEMON dataset). Individual pulvinar SNR maps were then averaged to generate group-level estimates of SNR spatial distribution. The resulting, modality-specific average SNR maps were correlated with the diffusion gradients derived from the corresponding modality, following the same approach described in the previous section (Pearson’s correlation; p-values corrected using spatial null models for spatial autocorrelation, and Benjamini-Hochberg correction for FWE).”

      (6) The numbers of the screeplot / numbers in figures are quite small and not so easy to read.

      Thank you for highlighting this point. We have fixed this issue in the revised version of the Figures.

      (7) How do you know the pulvinar mask is not also picking up on the cortical spinal tract?

      To ensure that pulvinar masks did not pick up streamlines from the corticospinal tracts, we performed a thorough visual inspection of the tractograms that were employed for structural connectivity estimation. For each subject-specific tractogram, we randomly subsampled 10000 streamlines after transformation into MNI standard space and summed up these results to generate a group-level tractogram in standard space. The resulting track-density images (Author response image 1) demonstrate only minimal involvement of descending/ascending tracts from/to the brainstem and spinal cord, confirming the specificity of the pulvinar masks.

      Author response image 1.

      Group-level structural connectivity of the pulvinar complex. Track-density images have been normalized and overlaid on the MNI152 standard template.

      (8) There is no mention of the within pulvinar gradients that then are correlated with PET patterns or across gradients are tested to spatial autocorrelation? I believe it is only mentioned for the cortex.

      Thanks for providing us with the opportunity to clarify this important aspect, which is mentioned in the Methods section (3. Gradient analysis and statistics):

      “To account for the spatial autocorrelation (SA) properties of gradient maps, for all the correlations described, statistical significance was assessed using the permutational approach described in Burt et al. (2020). Briefly, this method takes as input geometric distance matrices for SA estimation and involves the generation of a given number of SA-preserving permuted surrogate maps, which are then employed as nulls to estimate a permutational null distribution of the test statistic (Burt et al. 2020). Pairwise Euclidean distances between left or right pulvinar voxel coordinates were employed for pulvinar null models, while for cortical parcellated connectivity data Euclidean distances were estimated between centroids of each cortical ROI. In both cases, 1000 surrogates were generated to estimate the null distribution. Statistical tests were controlled for false discovery rate (FDR) using Benjamini and Hochberg’s correction.”

      However, to enhance readability, we have highlighted this concept in the Results section (3. The unimodal-to-transmodal gradient (G<sub>FC</sub>1) aligns with receptor expression on the dorso-ventral pulvinar axis):

      “To take into account the effects of spatial autocorrelation, we corrected the resulting p-values using a method based on SA-preserving spatial null models (Burt et al. 2020)”.

      (9) I don't fully understand why the mappings are so patchy of the structural connectivity gradient? Maybe some normalisation went wrong? Other papers on thalamic gradients show smoother patterns.

      We thank the Reviewer for the observation. After thoroughly reviewing the related codes, we found no normalization errors. However, we identified a visualization issue, which has been addressed in the revised version. Specifically, the structural gradient representations showed in the figures were based on the averaged values of left and right pulvinar gradients both of which include structural connectivity to either the ipsilateral or contralateral cerebral cortex. Since ipsilateral connectivity is more prominently represented than contralateral connectivity, this led to asymmetric gradient patterns between ipsilateral and contralateral cortical gradients, resulting in a patchy representation when gradients were averaged between left and right pulvinar. To resolve this, we adjusted the visualization by flipping the right pulvinar gradient representations along the x axis, aligning all the ipsilateral cortical connectivity on the left side and all the contralateral connectivity on the right. This adjustment produced smoother, more readable, and interpretable visualizations. Additionally, it allowed the asymmetry between ipsilateral and contralateral connections to be more clearly appreciated.

      (10) The final statement of the abstract is misleading as we at this point don't know how making spatial pattern maps in the pulvinar may help understand the role of the pulvinar in health and disease.

      We appreciate the Reviewer’s suggestion and have updated the expression accordingly:

      “Our findings represent a significant step forward in advancing the understanding of pulvinar anatomy and function, offering an exploratory framework to investigate the role of this structure in both health and disease.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to explore and better understand the complex topographical organization of the human pulvinar, a brain region crucial for various high-order functions such as perception and attention. They sought to move beyond traditional histological subdivisions by investigating continuous 'gradients' of cortical connections along the dorsoventral and mediolateral axes. Using advanced imaging techniques and a comprehensive PET atlas of neurotransmitter receptors, the study aimed to identify and characterize these gradients in terms of structural connections, functional coactivation, and molecular binding patterns. Ultimately, the authors targeted to provide a more nuanced understanding of pulvinar anatomy and its implications for brain function in both healthy and diseased states.

      Strengths:

      A key strength of this study lies in the authors' effort to comprehensively combine multimodal data, encompassing both functional and structural connectomics, alongside the analysis of major neurotransmitter distributions. This approach enabled a more nuanced understanding of the overarching organizational principles of the pulvinar nucleus within the broader context of whole-brain connectivity. By employing cortex-wide correlation analyses of multimodal embedding patterns derived from 'gradients,' which provide spatial maps reflecting the underlying connectomic and molecular similarities across voxels, the study offers a thorough characterization of the functional neuroanatomy of the pulvinar.

      Weaknesses:

      Despite its strengths, the current manuscript falls short in presenting the authors' unique perspectives on integrating the diverse biological principles derived from the various neuroimaging modalities. The findings are predominantly reported as correlations between different gradient maps, without providing the in-depth interpretations that would allow for a more comprehensive understanding of the pulvinar's role as a central hub in the brain's network. Another limitation of the study is the lack of clarity regarding the application of pulvinar and its subnuclei segmentation maps to individual brains prior to BOLD signal extraction and gradient reconstruction. This omission raises concerns about the precision and reproducibility of the findings, leaving their robustness less transparently evaluable.

      We thank the Reviewer for the valuable comments. While commonalities and discrepancies between structural and functional connectivity have been extensively explored in the literature, the relationship between functional connectivity and modulatory neurotransmission remains poorly understood. Specifically, while the role of thalamic modulatory neurotransmission has been thoroughly investigated in experimental animal models from an electrophysiological perspective, it remains relatively underexplored in the human brain. In our study, we identified significant associations between the spatial distribution of serotonergic, noradrenergic, dopaminergic and mu-opioid systems and functional pulvinar-cortical connectivity to specific functional networks. Evidence from pharmacological challenge studies using resting-state fMRI suggests that these neurotransmission systems may modulate network-specific thalamocortical connectivity directly or influence neural gain in cortico-cortical connectivity, a process partially dependent on thalamocortical connections to associative thalamic nuclei. However, the limitations of spatial and receptor specificity inherent to this approach, coupled with the predominantly correlational nature of our study design, prevented us from drawing more definitive conclusions on the biological relationship between neurotransmitter expression and functional connectivity. As regards the lack of clarity concerning signal extraction, we have now clarified that all the relevant steps of time series extraction were performed in standard space, without any further registration to individual subjects.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In line with the weaknesses that I raised above, my recommendation to authors are two-fold:

      (1) Please provide readers with a more holistic viewpoint to better digest all the correlation analyses. For instance, in p18, the summary says:

      "G<sub>FC</sub>1, GRC1, and G<sub>SC</sub>2 substantially delineate multiscale differences between the ventral and dorsal aspects of the pulvinar. Moving along the ventral-dorsal axis of the pulvinar complex, more ventral regions showed higher functional connectivity to unimodal sensory processing networks, higher levels of 5HTT and NAT expression, and preferentially higher structural connectivity to modality-independent or low-level sensory processing cortices."

      We already knew somehow the existence of the dorsoventral axis in the pulvinar, as the authors already specified in the introduction. Beyond this simple report on phenomenological observation, one may provide a more integrated discussion to pinpoint what commonality or discrepancy the GFC, GRC, and GSC map show and potential common principles explaining their biological relationship (e.g., the 5HTT and NAT's high expression and functional connectivity). Such digested perspectives will grant the study unique insights into the functional system of the pulvinar.

      We have expanded on this topic in the Discussion section (Neurochemical correlates of pulvinar-cortical topographical organization) as follows:

      “Indeed, while commonalities and discrepancies between structural and functional connectivity have been extensively investigated, the relationship between functional connectivity and modulatory neurotransmission remains poorly understood. Our findings reveal stronger associations between pulvinar-cortical connectivity to specific functional networks and the spatial distribution of markers of serotonergic, noradrenergic, dopaminergic and opioid systems. Pharmacological challenge studies using resting-state functional MRI suggest that each of these neurotransmission systems may either directly modulate thalamocortical connectivity or influence neuronal gain in cortico-cortical functional connectivity, which is known to depend, in part, on cortical connections to associative thalamic nuclei, including the pulvinar.”

      (2) Specify the details if there was a QC procedure to check the signal extraction from the pulvinar subnuclei by applying the segmentation atlas at each individual.

      Preprocessed BOLD volumes were available in standard-space, and time series were extracted for each voxel within a standard-space mask of the pulvinar complex. All volumes underwent visual inspection to ensure the accuracy of the registration process. Regarding the pulvinar subnuclei, these structures were not segmented at the individual level.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary of the Study:

      The authors investigate the organization of the human pulvinar by analyzing DWI, fMRI, and PET data. The authors explore the hypothesis of the "replication principle" in the pulvinar.

      Strengths and Weaknesses of the Methods and Results:

      The study effectively integrates diverse imaging modalities to provide a view of the pulvinar's organization. The use of analysis techniques, such as diffusion embedding-driven gradients combined with detailed interpretations of the pulvinar, is a strength.

      Even though the study uses the best publicly available resolution possible with current MR-technology, the pulvinar is densely packed with many cell bodies, requiring even higher spatial resolution. In addition, the model order selection of gradients may vary with the acquired data quality. Therefore, the pulvinar's intricate organization needs further exploration with even higher spatial resolution to capture gradients closer to the biological organization of the pulvinar.

      Appraisal of the Study's Aims and Conclusions:

      The authors delineate the gradient organization of the pulvinar. The study provides a basis for understanding the pulvinar's role in mediating brain network communication.

      Impact and Utility of the Work:

      This work contributes to the field by offering insights into pulvinar organization.

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive assessment and constructive feedback. The Authors agree with the Reviewer that the spatial resolution of currently available in-vivo imaging methods is limited, and that gradient representation would indeed benefit from higher resolution data. However, we also note that the resolution of structural and functional volumes used in our study is consistent with existing literature on pulvinar connectivity. Additionally, the PET data employed in our work include multi-centric studies collected worldwide from healthy populations, and are primarily acquired using high-resolution scanners that allow spatial resolution up to 2 mm<sup>2</sup>. Notwithstanding, further investigations employing finer resolution imaging techniques, such as ultra-high field fMRI, may provide more detailed insights into pulvinar topographical organization at a finer scale.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The HCP data contains genetically related datasets. Please mention whether the data-selection criteria for the selected 210 healthy subjects followed the genetically unrelated criteria.

      The HCP sample employed in this study consists of an initial cohort of 100 unrelated subjects, as provided in the HCP database, along with an additional random sample of 110 subjects. Subjects were selected without following a genetic criterion, as the family structure of the HCP dataset was part of a restricted access subset that we did not have access to at the time of processing. Subsequently, we obtained access to this information and determined that 178 out of 210 subjects (85%) are genetically unrelated. Of the remaining, genetically related subjects, 22 (~10% of the total sample) were included with another subject from the same family group (11 pairs); 6 (3%) were included with two other family members (2 triplets) and 4 (2%) were all parts of the same family group. This information has been included in the Methods section for clarity.

      (2) The study uses HCP data with an fMRI resolution of 2mm isotropic and diffusion MRI with 1.25mm. Additionally, the LEMON dataset includes 1.7mm isotropic DWI data and fMRI with 2.3mm isotropic resolution. Furthermore, the available PET data from the Hansen et al. 2022b study has a rather coarser spatial resolution. Therefore, it may be important to mention in the discussion that the pulvinar is densely packed with cell bodies and that their gradient organization might be better reflected with even higher spatial resolution or improved measurement techniques used in the study.

      We have revised the conclusive section of the Discussion into a paragraph title “Future perspectives and limitations”, and added the following text:

      “One notable limitation of this study lies in the relatively small size of the pulvinar complex compared to other larger cortical or subcortical structures. The high cellular density of the pulvinar poses a challenge for the relatively coarse resolution of currently available imaging techniques. Although the generally high quality of both the main and validation datasets, including rs-fMRI data (Uǧurbil et al. 2013; Babayan et al. 2019), align with current standards for imaging investigations of pulvinar connectivity, higher-resolution imaging approaches may offer more granular insights. Advanced techniques, such as ultra-high-field fMRI, hold promise for uncovering the fine-scale topographical organization of the pulvinar complex.”

      (3) The functional multiplicity of the Pulvinar nuclei among other thalamus nuclei is also illustrated in https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-04126-w

      We thank the Reviewer for suggesting this important reference. We have added the following text in the Discussion section:

      “It is noteworthy that, in analyses of thalamocortical gradients, the pulvinar complex is situated towards the “sensorimotor” extreme of the unimodal-to-transmodal thalamic gradient (Yang et al., 2020). This likely reflects its prominent connectivity to visual and sensory areas compared to other thalamic nuclei. Nevertheless, the extensive and intricate association of pulvinar with multiple cortical networks emerges is strongly evident in various functional connectivity investigations (Basile et al., 2021; Kumar et al., 2017, 2022). By isolating pulvinar-cortical from broader thalamocortical connectivity, our analysis was able to provide additional insights into the spatial organization of its connectivity with different cortical networks, highlighting the pulvinar's remarkable functional diversity and complexity.”

      (4) In addition to DWI/DSI and PET, the study also uses fMRI, which allows for functional interaction in time. It may be worth reflecting in the discussion that the observed gradient organization of the pulvinar could have detailed aspects in the temporal domain, which might not be fully captured in the time-averaged embeddings.

      We thank the Reviewer for their insightful observation. The authors recognize that the exploration of brain temporal dynamics is a compelling area of research due to its extensive correlation with multiple hierarchical aspects of brain information processing. Examining the functional organization of the pulvinar complex lies beyond the scope of the present work and will be subject of further investigation. On the other hand, it is possible that certain aspects of the spatial organization of pulvinar connectivity may be influenced by temporal dynamics of cortico-thalamic information processing. Intrinsic timescales have been consistently showed to progressively increase from unimodal to multimodal associative cortical regions. Furthermore, cortico-thalamic connectivity in matrix-rich regions has been correlated with cortical time scales.

      To address this point, we have added the following lines to the Discussion section:

      “In this context, it could be hypothesized that the observed gradient organization of the pulvinar may also exhibit specific patterns in the temporal domain. Indeed, multiple investigations have linked the temporal dynamics of cortical regions to different aspects of information processing (Rossi-Pool et al., 2021; Soltani et al., 2021). Notably, intrinsic neural timescales of functional activity have been associated with the functional specialization and gradient organization of the cerebral cortex (Golesorkhi et al., 2021), with shorter timescales in unimodal sensory regions and longer ones in transmodal networks (Ito et al., 2020; Murray et al., 2014). Moreover, thalamocortical connectivity has been showed to correlate with these patterns of intrinsic time scale (Müller et al., 2020). In addition, modulatory neurotransmitters such as serotonin and dopamine have been demonstrated to play a significant role in modulating functional cortical dynamics across different timescales (Hansen, Shafiei, et al., 2022; Luppi et al., 2023). Exploring how the spatial organization of the pulvinar relates to temporal dynamics and timescale modulation could provide valuable insights and represents a promising avenue for future investigations.”

      (5) The K-means clustering (Supplementary Figure 1) used has limitations, particularly with respect to the structure of the data. Another aspect is the reproducibility of the model-order selection. Did the reliability and reproducibility assessment produce a similar number of clusters with the LEMON data as with the HCP data?

      We acknowledge the limitations of k-means clustering, particularly regarding the stability and reproducibility of the model order. To address the concerns, we iteratively ran the clustering algorithm 50 times on bootstrap resamples to enhance the stability of the silhouette score estimates. In addition, we have now replicated the analysis on the secondary dataset, as suggested by the Reviewer (Author response image 2). The Silhouette plots show similar number of clusters between the two different datasets for functional connectivity gradients, with minor differences observed in the results for structural connectivity gradients and multimodal gradient clustering. Notably, we did not find high a high degree of similarity between the results of gradient clustering and histologically defined nuclei, further underscoring the distinct organizational patterns identified through our analysis.

      This reinforces the relevance of using gradient-based approaches to reveal insights into the functional and structural organization of the pulvinar complex that may not align strictly with discrete, histologically defined subdivisions.

      Author response image 2.

      K-means clustering of pulvinar gradients on the secondary dataset (LEMON) and their correspondence with histological pulvinar nuclei. Panels on the left show the silhouette plots for left and right pulvinar clustering solutions; error bars are standard error calculated across 50 resamples. Panels on the right show matrix plots of Dice similarity coefficients for pulvinar clusters against histological nuclei (AAL3 atlas). INF: inferior; ANT: anterior; LAT: lateral; MED: medial.

      (6) The pulvinar correlates of the unimodal-transmodal cortical gradient (Figure 4) show an association with almost the entire brain (Figure 4C, violin plot). It would be interesting to back this association with known anatomical connectivity studies in animals that show connections to these network areas. To my limited knowledge, I am not aware of pulvinar tracer studies showing such extensive connectivity across the entire cortex.

      As our structural connectivity estimates are based on tractography, they are subject to the known limitation of potentially overestimating anatomical connectivity. A technical clarification is warranted: since structural connectivity is grouped by networks, it is strongly influenced by connections to specific cortical regions within each network. This explains the uneven and asymmetric distribution of structural gradient-weighted connectivity observed in our results and does not imply widespread connectivity across the entire cortex.

      Nonetheless, structural connectivity of the pulvinar to cortical regions in primates encompasses a remarkably broad array of cortical areas, including predominantly occipital (Adams et al., 2000; Benevento, 1976; Casanova et al., 1989), temporal (Berman & Wurtz, 2010; Gattass et al., 2018; Homman-Ludiye et al., 2020) and parietal cortices (Asanuma et al., 1985; Baleydier & Morel, 1992). Additionally, to a more limited extent, connections to the cingulate gyrus, and portions of the lateral prefrontal cortex have also been documented (Baleydier & Mauguiere, 1985; Baleydier & Mauguire, 1987). These connectivity patterns are in line with prior accounts of structural connectivity of the human pulvinar (Arcaro et al., 2015; Basile et al., 2021; Leh et al., 2008; Tamietto et al., 2012), and with the patterns identified in our work (Author response image 1). Such findings provide further validation of the structural connectivity profiles explored in the present study.

      References

      Adams, M. M., Hof, P. R., Gattass, R., Webster, M. J., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2000). Visual cortical projections and chemoarchitecture of macaque monkey pulvinar. The Journal of Comparative Neurology, 419(3), 377–393. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(20000410)419:3<377::AID-CNE9>3.0.CO;2-E

      Arcaro, M. J., Pinsk, M. A., & Kastner, S. (2015). The anatomical and functional organization of the human visual pulvinar. Journal of Neuroscience. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1575-14.2015

      Asanuma, C., Andersen, R. A., & Cowan, W. M. (1985). The thalamic relations of the caudal inferior parietal lobule and the lateral prefrontal cortex in monkeys: Divergent cortical projections from cell clusters in the medial pulvinar nucleus. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 241(3), 357–381. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902410309

      Baleydier, C., & Mauguiere, F. (1985). Anatomical evidence for medial pulvinar connections with the posterior cingulate cortex, the retrosplenial area, and the posterior parahippocampal gyrus in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Neurology. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.902320207

      Baleydier, C., & Mauguiere, F. (1987). Network organization of the connectivity between parietal area 7, posterior cingulate cortex and medial pulvinar nucleus: A double fluorescent tracer study in monkey. Experimental Brain Research, 66(2). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00243312

      Baleydier, C., & Morel, A. (1992). Segregated thalamocortical pathways to inferior parietal and inferotemporal cortex in macaque monkey. Visual Neuroscience, 8(5), 391–405. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952523800004922

      Basile, G. A., Bertino, S., Bramanti, A., Anastasi, G. P., Milardi, D., & Cacciola, A. (2021). In Vivo Super-Resolution Track-Density Imaging for Thalamic Nuclei Identification. Cerebral Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhab184

      Benevento. (1976). The Cortical Projections of the Inferior Pulvinar and Adjacent Lateral Pulvinar in the Rhesus Monkey ( Macaca. October, 108, 1–24.

      Berman, R. A., & Wurtz, R. H. (2010). Functional Identification of a Pulvinar Path from Superior Colliculus to Cortical Area MT. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30(18), 6342–6354. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6176-09.2010

      Cai, L. Y., Yang, Q., Hansen, C. B., Nath, V., Ramadass, K., Johnson, G. W., Conrad, B. N., Boyd, B. D., Begnoche, J. P., Beason-Held, L. L., Shafer, A. T., Resnick, S. M., Taylor, W. D., Price, G. R., Morgan, V. L., Rogers, B. P., Schilling, K. G., & Landman, B. A. (2021). PreQual: An automated pipeline for integrated preprocessing and quality assurance of diffusion weighted MRI images. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 86(1), 456. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.28678

      Casanova, C., Freeman, R. D., & Nordmann, J. P. (1989). Monocular and binocular response properties of cells in the striate-recipient zone of the cat’s lateral posterior-pulvinar complex. Journal of Neurophysiology. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.1989.62.2.544

      Gattass, R., Soares, J. G. M., & Lima, B. (2018). Comparative Pulvinar Organization Across Different Primate Species (pp. 37–37). https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-70046-5_8

      Golesorkhi, M., Gomez-Pilar, J., Tumati, S., Fraser, M., & Northoff, G. (2021). Temporal hierarchy of intrinsic neural timescales converges with spatial core-periphery organization. Communications Biology, 4(1), 277. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-021-01785-z

      Hansen, J. Y., Markello, R. D., Tuominen, L., Nørgaard, M., Kuzmin, E., Palomero-Gallagher, N., Dagher, A., & Misic, B. (2022). Correspondence between gene expression and neurotransmitter receptor and transporter density in the human brain. NeuroImage, 264, 119671. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119671

      Hansen, J. Y., Shafiei, G., Markello, R. D., Smart, K., Cox, S. M. L., Nørgaard, M., Beliveau, V., Wu, Y., Gallezot, J.-D., Aumont, É., Servaes, S., Scala, S. G., DuBois, J. M., Wainstein, G., Bezgin, G., Funck, T., Schmitz, T. W., Spreng, R. N., Galovic, M., … Misic, B. (2022). Mapping neurotransmitter systems to the structural and functional organization of the human neocortex. Nature Neuroscience, 25(11), 1569–1581. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-022-01186-3

      Homman-Ludiye, J., Mundinano, I. C., Kwan, W. C., & Bourne, J. A. (2020). Extensive Connectivity Between the Medial Pulvinar and the Cortex Revealed in the Marmoset Monkey. Cerebral Cortex, 30(3), 1797–1812. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhz203

      Iglesias, J. E., Insausti, R., Lerma-Usabiaga, G., Bocchetta, M., Van Leemput, K., Greve, D. N., van der Kouwe, A., Fischl, B., Caballero-Gaudes, C., & Paz-Alonso, P. M. (2018). A probabilistic atlas of the human thalamic nuclei combining ex vivo MRI and histology. NeuroImage, 183, 314–326. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.012

      Ito, T., Hearne, L. J., & Cole, M. W. (2020). A cortical hierarchy of localized and distributed processes revealed via dissociation of task activations, connectivity changes, and intrinsic timescales. NeuroImage, 221, 117141. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117141

      Kumar, V. J., Beckmann, C. F., Scheffler, K., & Grodd, W. (2022). Relay and higher-order thalamic nuclei show an intertwined functional association with cortical-networks. Communications Biology, 5(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-04126-w

      Kumar, V. J., van Oort, E., Scheffler, K., Beckmann, C. F., & Grodd, W. (2017). Functional anatomy of the human thalamus at rest. NeuroImage, 147, 678–691. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.12.071

      Leh, S. E., Chakravarty, M. M., & Ptito, A. (2008). The Connectivity of the Human Pulvinar: A Diffusion Tensor Imaging Tractography Study. International Journal of Biomedical Imaging, 2008, 1–5. https://doi.org/10.1155/2008/789539

      Luppi, A. I., Hansen, J. Y., Adapa, R., Carhart-Harris, R. L., Roseman, L., Timmermann, C., Golkowski, D., Ranft, A., Ilg, R., Jordan, D., Bonhomme, V., Vanhaudenhuyse, A., Demertzi, A., Jaquet, O., Bahri, M. A., Alnagger, N. L. N., Cardone, P., Peattie, A. R. D., Manktelow, A. E., … Stamatakis, E. A. (2023). In vivo mapping of pharmacologically induced functional reorganization onto the human brain’s neurotransmitter landscape. Science Advances, 9(24), eadf8332. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.adf8332

      Müller, E. J., Munn, B., Hearne, L. J., Smith, J. B., Fulcher, B., Arnatkevičiūtė, A., Lurie, D. J., Cocchi, L., & Shine, J. M. (2020). Core and matrix thalamic sub-populations relate to spatio-temporal cortical connectivity gradients. NeuroImage, 222, 117224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117224

      Murphy, K., Bodurka, J., & Bandettini, P. A. (2006). How long to scan? The relationship between fMRI temporal signal to noise and necessary scan duration. NeuroImage, 34(2), 565. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.032

      Murray, J. D., Bernacchia, A., Freedman, D. J., Romo, R., Wallis, J. D., Cai, X., Padoa-Schioppa, C., Pasternak, T., Seo, H., Lee, D., & Wang, X.-J. (2014). A hierarchy of intrinsic timescales across primate cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 17(12), 1661–1663. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3862

      Oldham, S., & Ball, G. (2023). A phylogenetically-conserved axis of thalamocortical connectivity in the human brain. Nature Communications, 14(1), 6032. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-41722-8

      Rolls, E. T., Huang, C.-C., Lin, C.-P., Feng, J., & Joliot, M. (2020). Automated anatomical labelling atlas 3. NeuroImage, 206, 116189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116189

      Rossi-Pool, R., Zainos, A., Alvarez, M., Parra, S., Zizumbo, J., & Romo, R. (2021). Invariant timescale hierarchy across the cortical somatosensory network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(3), e2021843118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2021843118

      Shipp, S. (2003). The functional logic of cortico-pulvinar connections. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1438), 1605–1624. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1213

      Soltani, A., Murray, J. D., Seo, H., & Lee, D. (2021). Timescales of cognition in the brain. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 41, 30–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.03.003

      Su, J. H., Thomas, F. T., Kasoff, W. S., Tourdias, T., Choi, E. Y., Rutt, B. K., & Saranathan, M. (2019). Thalamus Optimized Multi Atlas Segmentation (THOMAS): Fast, fully automated segmentation of thalamic nuclei from structural MRI. NeuroImage, 194, 272–282. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.021

      Tamietto, M., Pullens, P., de Gelder, B., Weiskrantz, L., & Goebel, R. (2012). Subcortical Connections to Human Amygdala and Changes following Destruction of the Visual Cortex. Current Biology, 22(15), 1449–1455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.06.006

      Yang, S., Meng, Y., Li, J., Li, B., Fan, Y.-S., Chen, H., & Liao, W. (2020). The thalamic functional gradient and its relationship to structural basis and cognitive relevance. NeuroImage, 218, 116960. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116960

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1:

      This paper seeks to address the question of how quantitative trait variation and expression variation are related. scRNAseq represents an appealing approach to eQTL mapping as it is possible to simultaneously genotype individual cells and measure expression in the same cell. As eQTL mapping requires large sample sizes to identify statistical relationships, the use of scRNAseq is likely to dramatically increase the statistical power of such studies. However, there are several technical challenges associated with scRNAseq and the authors' study is focused on addressing those challenges. Most of the points raised by my review of the initial version have been addressed. However, one point remains and one additional point should be considered. In this version the authors have introduced the use of data imputation using a published algorithm, DISCERN. This has greatly increased the variation explained by their model as presented in figure 3. However, it is possible that the explained variance is now an overestimation as a result of using the imputed expression data. I think that it would be appropriate to present figure 3 using the sparse data presented in the initial version of the paper and the newly presented imputed data so that the reader can draw their own conclusions about the interpretation.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out and decided to present the results obtained from the sparse data in the main Figure 3 to avoid any overestimation. We also performed the variance partitioning at different sample sizes and used an optimized implementation of the GREML method to be able to handle high sample sizes instead of having to use a bootstrap estimate. As for the benefits of denoising the expression data, we illustrated it in the supplementary figure S6 so that people can draw their own conclusions about this imputation method. The imputation generally increases the contribution of the expressiongenotype interaction and decreases the residuals of the model by up to 8%.

      Reviewer #1:

      Given that the authors overcame many technical and analytical challenges in the course of this research, the study would be greatly strengthened through analysis of at least one, and ideally several, more conditions which would expand the conclusions that could be drawn from the study and demonstrate the power of using scRNAseq to efficiently quantify expression in different environments.

      Our aim was to illustrate the benefit of one-pot scRNA-seq for eQTL mapping and the association of transcriptomic variation to trait variation. We think we have reached this goal with the current study. We understand that performing another scRNA-seq experiment in a new environment would help expand/validate our conclusions, but we think this would be a better fit for a future study. 

      Reviewer #2:

      The authors now say the main take-home for their work is (1) they have established methods for linkage mapping with scRNA-seq and that these (2) "can help gain insights about the genotype-phenotype map at a broader scale." My opinion in this revision is much the same as it was in the first round: I agree that they have met the first goal, and the second theme has been so well explored by other literature that I'm not convinced the authors' results meet the bar for novelty and impact. To my mind, success for this manuscript would be to support the claim that the scRNA-seq approach helps "reveal hidden components of the yeast genotype-to-phenotype map." I'm not sure the authors have achieved this. I agree that the new Figure 3 is a nice addition-a result that apparently hasn't been reported elsewhere (30% of growth trait variation can't be explained by expression). The caveats are that this is a negative result that needs to be interpreted with caution; and that it would be useful for the authors to clarify whether the ability to do this calculation is a product of the scRNA-seq method per se or whether they could have used any bulk eQTL study for it. Beside this, I regret to say that I still find that the results in the revision recapitulate what the bulk eQTL literature has already found, especially for the authors' focal yeast cross: heritability, expression hotspots, the role of cis and transacting variation, etc.

      We agree with the reviewer that this study does not reveal new modes of transcription regulation or phenomena that were not highlighted or hypothesized in the literature. To avoid confusion, we refrained from using the word “reveal” for such cases. However, we provide convincing evidence that one-pot scRNA-seq helps refining our understanding of genotype-phenotype map in two ways. First, the larger scalability of this approach allowed us to find a median number of eQTL per gene that is ~4 times higher than the largest bulk-eQTL mapping in the same genetic background. For 60% of these genes, i.e. the ones with higher expression heritability in our dataset, the ability to explain their transcriptomic variation from SNPs increased by ~16% on average, which is substantial. This gain in power can thus improve our understanding of the gene network by highlighting new downstream effects of mutations or transcriptome variation. Second, by performing one-pot eQTL as opposed to large-scale bulk eQTL, thousands of transcriptomes can be collected simultaneously without having to use batching strategies. This enables the association between phenotype, genotype and expression variation, which we show in figure 3 through variance partitioning. While it is possible that the growth trait variation not being fully explained by expression could be an artifact of scRNA-seq, we do not believe this is the case because most transcriptional variation is explained by genotype (~76%).

      Furthermore, we show that by having to control expression for growth, by missing some hotspots of regulation and by missing multiple eQTL for each gene, previous bulk-eQTL analysis could not replicate the significant association between eQTL hotspots and QTL hotspot, which this study highlights. Thus, we agree in general that many of the insights about transcriptional regulation have been obtained through ‘brute-force’, bulk RNA-seq, which fundamentally can reach tens of thousands of transcriptomes as well, but we believe the one-pot scRNA-seq approach is much easier and expedient once genotyping the single-cells and other challenges regarding denoising and low coverage have been solved (which we believe we did). There is indeed another reviewed preprint [Boocock et al, eLife] that has used similar approaches as our study since the publication of our manuscript (in October 2023).

      Likewise, when in the first round of review I recommended that the authors repeat their analyses on previous bulk RNA-seq data from Albert et al., my point was to lead the authors to a means to provide rigorous, compelling justification for the scRNA-seq approach. The response to reviewers and the text (starting on line 413) says the comparison in its current form doesn't serve this purpose because Albert et al. studied fewer segregants. Wouldn't down-sampling the current data set allow a fair comparison? Again, to my mind what the current manuscript needs is concrete evidence that the scRNA-seq method per se affords truly better insights relative to what has come before.

      We agree that down-sampling the current dataset would allow for a fair comparison. Thus, we illustrate the results of the variance partitioning at different sample sizes. While the total variance explained is similar, the contribution of the genotype-expression interaction increases with sample size, highlighting the increase in the confidence of the associations between expression and genotype that contributed to trait variation. We also showed that a lot of important low-effect sizes eQTL are missing at a sample size of 1000 compared to a sample size 4000. Indeed, by increasing the scale of eQTL mapping by ~4, about 60% of genes have increased heritability and this increase is due to eQTLs that cumulatively explain more than 15% of transcript level variation.

      I also recommend that the authors take care to improve the main text for readability and professionalism. It would benefit from further structural revision throughout (especially in the figure captions) to allow high-impact conclusions to be highlighted and low-impact material to be eliminated. Figure 4 and the results text sections from line 319 onward could be edited for concision or perhaps moved to supplementary if they obscure the authors' case for the scRNA-seq approach. The text could also benefit from copy editing (e.g. three clauses starting with "while" in the paragraph starting on line 456; "od ratio" on line 415). I appreciate the authors' work on the discussion, including posing big picture questions for the field (lines 426-429), but I don't see how they have anything to do with the current scRNA-seq method.

      We thank the reviewer for their suggestions for improving the readability of the text. We edited some of the figure captions and result section titles to better highlight the main results. However, we do not think that the last result section obscures our findings but rather supports the fact that scRNA-seq refines our understanding of the GPM. Indeed, we discovered many new eQTLs that are related to both expression and trait variation, highlighting the potential for understanding the downstream effects of mutations on the gene network and on trait variation through multiple trans-regulation paths.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This work provides a new Python toolkit for combining generative modeling of neural dynamics and inversion methods to infer likely model parameters that explain empirical neuroimaging data. The authors provided tests to show the toolkit's broad applicability and accuracy; hence, it will be very useful for people interested in using computational approaches to better understand the brain.

      Strengths:

      The work's primary strength is the tool's integrative nature, which seamlessly combines forward modelling with backward inference. This is important as available tools in the literature can only do one and not the other, which limits their accessibility to neuroscientists with limited computational expertise. Another strength of the paper is the demonstration of how the tool can be applied to a broad range of computational models popularly used in the field to interrogate diverse neuroimaging data, ensuring that the methodology is not optimal to only one model. Moreover, through extensive in-silico testing, the work provided evidence that the tool can accurately infer ground-truth parameters, which is important to ensure results from future hypothesis testing are meaningful.

      We are happy to hear the positive feedback on our effort to provide an open-source and widely accessible tool for both fast forward simulations and flexible model inversion, applicable across popular models of large-scale brain dynamics.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the tool itself is the main strength of the work, the paper lacked a thorough analysis of issues concerning robustness and benchmarking relative to existing tools.

      The first issue is the robustness to the choice of features to be included in the objective function. This choice significantly affects the training and changes the results, as the authors even acknowledged themselves multiple times (e.g., Page 17 last sentence of first paragraph or Page 19 first sentence of second paragraph). This brings the question of whether the accurate results found in the various demonstrations are due to the biased selection of features (possibly from priors on what worked in previous works). The robustness of the neural estimator and the inference method to noise was also not demonstrated. This is important as most neuroimaging measurements are inherently noisy to various degrees.

      The second issue is on benchmarking. Because the tool developed is, in principle, only a combination of existing tools specific to modeling or Bayesian inference, the work failed to provide a more compelling demonstration of its added value. This could have been demonstrated through appropriate benchmarking relative to existing methodologies, specifically in terms of accuracy and computational efficiency.

      We fully agree with the reviewer that the VBI estimation heavily depends on the choice of data features, and this is the core of the inference procedure, not its weakness. We have demonstrated different scenarios showing how the informativeness of features (commonly used in the literature) results in varying uncertainty quantification. For instance, using summary statistics of functional connectivity (FC) and functional connectivity dynamics (FCD) matrices to estimate global coupling parameter leads to fast convergence; however, it is not sufficient to accurately estimate the whole-brain heterogeneous excitability parameter, which requires features such as statistical moments of time series. VBI provides a taxonomy of data features that users can employ to test their hypotheses. It is important to note that one major advantage of VBI is its ability to make estimation using a battery of data features, rather than relying on a limited set (such as only FC or FCD) as is often the case in the literature. In the revised version, we will elaborate further by presenting additional scenarios to demonstrate the robustness of the estimation. We will also evaluate the robustness of the neural density estimators to (dynamical/additive) noise.

      More importantly, relative to benchmarking, we would like to draw attention to a key point regarding existing tools and methods. The literature often uses optimization for fitting whole-brain network models, and its limitations for reliable causal hypothesis testing have been pointed out in the Introduction/Discussion. As also noted by the reviewer under strengths, and to the best of our knowledge, there are no existing tools other than VBI that can scale and generalize to operate across whole-brain models for Bayesian model inversion. Previously, we developed Hamiltonian Monte Carlo (HMC) sampling for Epileptor model in epilepsy (Hashemi et al., 2020, Jha et al., 2022). This phenomenological model is very well-behaved in terms of numerical integration, gradient calculation, and dynamical system properties (Jirsa et al., 2014). However, this does not directly generalize to other models, particularly the Montbrió model for resting-state, which exhibits bistability with noise driving transitions between states. As shown in Baldy et al., 2024, even at the level of a single neural mass model (i.e., one brain region), gradient-based HMC failed to capture such switching behaviour, particularly when only one state variable (membrane potential) was observed while the other (firing rate) was missing. Our attempts to use other methods (e.g., the second-derivative-based Laplace approximation used in Dynamic Causal Modeling) also failed, due to divergence in gradient calculation. Nevertheless, reparameterization techniques (Baldy et al., 2024) and hybrid algorithms (Gabrié et al., 2022) could offer improvements, although this remains an open problem for these classes of computational models.

      In sum, for oscillatory systems, it has been shown previously that SBI approach used in VBI substantially outperforms both gradient-based and gradient-free alternative methods (Gonçalves et al., 2020, Hashemi et al., 2023, Baldy et al., 2024). Importantly, for bistable systems with switching dynamics, gradient-based methods fail to converge, while gradient-free methods do not scale to the whole-brain level (Hashemi et al., 2020). Hence, the generalizability of VBI relies on the fact that neither the model nor the data features need to be differentiable. We will clarify this point in the revised version. Moreover, we will provide better explanations for some terms mentioned by the reviewer in Recommendations.

      Hashemi, M., Vattikonda, A. N., Sip, V., Guye, M., Bartolomei, F., Woodman, M. M., & Jirsa, V. K. (2020). The Bayesian Virtual Epileptic Patient: A probabilistic framework designed to infer the spatial map of epileptogenicity in a personalized large-scale brain model of epilepsy spread. NeuroImage, 217, 116839.

      Jha, J., Hashemi, M., Vattikonda, A. N., Wang, H., & Jirsa, V. (2022). Fully Bayesian estimation of virtual brain parameters with self-tuning Hamiltonian Monte Carlo. Machine Learning: Science and Technology, 3(3), 035016.

      Jirsa, V. K., Stacey, W. C., Quilichini, P. P., Ivanov, A. I., & Bernard, C. (2014). On the nature of seizure dynamics. Brain, 137(8), 2210-2230.

      Baldy, N., Breyton, M., Woodman, M. M., Jirsa, V. K., & Hashemi, M. (2024). Inference on the macroscopic dynamics of spiking neurons. Neural Computation, 36(10), 2030-2072.

      Baldy, N., Woodman, M., Jirsa, V., & Hashemi, M. (2024). Dynamic Causal Modeling in Probabilistic Programming Languages. bioRxiv, 2024-11.

      Gabrié, M., Rotskoff, G. M., & Vanden-Eijnden, E. (2022). Adaptive Monte Carlo augmented with normalizing flows. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(10), e2109420119.

      Gonçalves, P. J., Lueckmann, J. M., Deistler, M., Nonnenmacher, M., Öcal, K., Bassetto, G., ... & Macke, J. H. (2020). Training deep neural density estimators to identify mechanistic models of neural dynamics. eLife, 9, e56261.

      Hashemi, M., Vattikonda, A. N., Jha, J., Sip, V., Woodman, M. M., Bartolomei, F., & Jirsa, V. K. (2023). Amortized Bayesian inference on generative dynamical network models of epilepsy using deep neural density estimators. Neural Networks, 163, 178-194.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Whole-brain network modeling is a common type of dynamical systems-based method to create individualized models of brain activity incorporating subject-specific structural connectome inferred from diffusion imaging data. This type of model has often been used to infer biophysical parameters of the individual brain that cannot be directly measured using neuroimaging but may be relevant to specific cognitive functions or diseases. Here, Ziaeemehr et al introduce a new toolkit, named "Virtual Brain Inference" (VBI), offering a new computational approach for estimating these parameters using Bayesian inference powered by artificial neural networks. The basic idea is to use simulated data, given known parameters, to train artificial neural networks to solve the inverse problem, namely, to infer the posterior distribution over the parameter space given data-derived features. The authors have demonstrated the utility of the toolkit using simulated data from several commonly used whole-brain network models in case studies.

      Strengths:

      (1) Model inversion is an important problem in whole-brain network modeling. The toolkit presents a significant methodological step up from common practices, with the potential to broadly impact how the community infers model parameters.

      (2) Notably, the method allows the estimation of the posterior distribution of parameters instead of a point estimation, which provides information about the uncertainty of the estimation, which is generally lacking in existing methods.

      (3) The case studies were able to demonstrate the detection of degeneracy in the parameters, which is important. Degeneracy is quite common in this type of model. If not handled mindfully, they may lead to spurious or stable parameter estimation. Thus, the toolkit can potentially be used to improve feature selection or to simply indicate the uncertainty.

      (4) In principle, the posterior distribution can be directly computed given new data without doing any additional simulation, which could improve the efficiency of parameter inference on the artificial neural network if well-trained.

      We thank the reviewer for the careful consideration of important aspects of the VBI tool, such as uncertainty quantification, degeneracy detection, parallelization, and amortization strategy.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) While the posterior estimator was trained with a large quantity of simulated data, the testing/validation is only demonstrated with a single case study (one point in parameter space) per model. This is not sufficient to demonstrate the method's accuracy and reliability, but only its feasibility. Demonstrating the accuracy and reliability of the posterior estimation in large test sets would inspire more confidence.

      (2) The authors have only demonstrated validation of the method using simulated data, but not features derived from actual EEG/MEG or fMRI data. So, it is unclear if the posterior estimator, when applied to real data, would produce results as sensible as using simulated data. Human data can often look quite different from the simulated data, which may be considered out of distribution. Thus, the authors should consider using simulated test data with out-of-distribution parameters to validate the method and using real human data to demonstrate, e.g., the reliability of the method across sessions.

      (3) The z-scores used to measure prediction error are generally between 1-3, which seems quite large to me. It would give readers a better sense of the utility of the method if comparisons to simpler methods, such as k-nearest neighbor methods, are provided in terms of accuracy.

      (4) A lot of simulations are required to train the posterior estimator, which seems much more than existing approaches. Inferring from Figure S1, at the required order of magnitudes of the number of simulations, the simulation time could range from days to years, depending on the hardware. Although once the estimator is well-trained, the parameter inverse given new data will be very fast, it is not clear to me how often such use cases would be encountered. Because the estimator is trained based on an individual connectome, it can only be used to do parameter inversion for the same subject. Typically, we only have one session of resting state data from each participant, while longitudinal resting state data where we can assume the structural connectome remains constant, is rare. Thus, the cost-efficiency and practical utility of training such a posterior estimator remains unclear.

      We agree with the reviewer that it is necessary to show results on larger synthetic test sets, and we will elaborate further by presenting additional scenarios to demonstrate the robustness of the estimation. However, there are some points raised by the reviewer that we need to clarify.

      The validation on empirical data was beyond the scope of this study, as it relates to model validation rather than the inversion algorithms. This is also because we aimed to avoid repetition, given that we have previously demonstrated model validation on empirical data using these techniques, for invasive sEEG (Hashemi et al., 2023), MEG (Sorrentino et al., 2024), EEG (Angiolelli et al., 2025) and fMRI (Lavanga et al., 2024, Rabuffo et al., 2025). Note that if the features of the observed data are not included during training, VBI ignores them, as it requires an invertible mapping function between parameters and data features.

      We have used z-scores and posterior shrinkage to measure prediction performance, as these are Bayesian metrics that take into account the variance of both prior and posterior rather than only the mean value or thresholding for ranking of the prediction used in k-NN or confusion matrix methods. This helps avoid biased accuracy estimation, for instance, if the mean posterior is close to the true value but there is no posterior shrinkage. Although shrinkage is bounded between 0 and 1, we agree that z-scores have no upper bound for such diagnostics.

      Finally, the number of required simulations depends on the dimensionality of the parameter space and the informativeness of the data features. For instance, estimating a single global scaling parameter requires around 100 simulations, whereas estimating whole-brain heterogeneous parameters requires substantially more simulations. Nevertheless, we have provided fast simulations, and one key advantage of VBI is that simulations can be run in parallel (unlike MCMC sampling, which is more limited in this regard). Hence, with commonly accessible CPUs/GPUs, the fast simulations and parallelization capabilities of the VBI tool allow us to run on the order of 1 million simulations within 2–3 days on desktops, or in less than half a day on supercomputers at cohort level, rather than over several years! It has been previously shown that the SBI method used in VBI provides an order-of-magnitude faster inversion than HMC for whole-brain epilepsy spread (Hashemi et al., 2023). Moreover, after training, the amortized strategy is critical for enabling hypothesis testing within seconds to minutes. We agree that longitudinal resting-state data under the assumption of a constant structural connectome is rare; however, this strategy is essential in brain diseases such as epilepsy, where experimental hypothesis testing is prohibitive.

      We will clarify these points and better explain some terms mentioned by the reviewer in the revised manuscript.

      Hashemi, M., Vattikonda, A. N., Jha, J., Sip, V., Woodman, M. M., Bartolomei, F., & Jirsa, V. K. (2023). Amortized Bayesian inference on generative dynamical network models of epilepsy using deep neural density estimators. Neural Networks, 163, 178-194.

      Sorrentino, P., Pathak, A., Ziaeemehr, A., Lopez, E. T., Cipriano, L., Romano, A., ... & Hashemi, M. (2024). The virtual multiple sclerosis patient. Iscience, 27(7).

      Angiolelli, M., Depannemaecker, D., Agouram, H., Regis, J., Carron, R., Woodman, M., ... & Sorrentino, P. (2025). The virtual parkinsonian patient. npj Systems Biology and Applications, 11(1), 40.

      Lavanga, M., Stumme, J., Yalcinkaya, B. H., Fousek, J., Jockwitz, C., Sheheitli, H., ... & Jirsa, V. (2023). The virtual aging brain: Causal inference supports interhemispheric dedifferentiation in healthy aging. NeuroImage, 283, 120403.

      Rabuffo, G., Lokossou, H. A., Li, Z., Ziaee-Mehr, A., Hashemi, M., Quilichini, P. P., ... & Bernard, C. (2025). Mapping global brain reconfigurations following local targeted manipulations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 122(16), e2405706122.

    1. Author response:

      We thank all three reviewers for providing excellent suggestions that we feel will enhance the clarity and impact of our manuscript. When we submit the revised manuscript, we plan to respond to each comment and provide additional data and discussion points as requested. Below, we include an outline of the main points that we intend to address.

      (1) Reviewers 1 and 2 both suggested investigating degenerative changes in Purkinje cells that are more resistant to age-related loss. We will look for hallmarks of neurodegeneration, such as shrunken dendrites and axonal swellings, in two areas: surviving Purkinje cells adjacent to stripes of cell loss, and the Purkinje cells in aged mice without Purkinje cell loss.

      (2) We agree with Reviewer 2’s point that our manuscript would benefit from discussion of the differences in vulnerability between individual mice.  Therefore, we will elaborate upon possible reasons why some aged mice are more resistant to age-related Purkinje cell loss than others.

      (3) We will take Reviewer 3’s suggestion to perform zebrin II co-staining in our GFP reporter mice, given our findings that calbindin staining can be unreliable in this context. 4) We appreciate Reviewer 3’s comment that quantification would support the observations made in our study. To provide quantitative evidence for our categorization of mice with striped and non-striped Purkinje cell loss, we will measure the gaps (or lack thereof) between Purkinje cell bodies in the anterior zone.

      (4) We will also incorporate several minor but important changes suggested by all three reviewers.

      Thank you to the reviewers and editors for taking the time and effort to review our manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study presents compelling evidence for a novel treatment approach in a challenging patient population with MSS/pMMR mCRC, where traditional immunotherapy has often fallen short. The combination of SBRT and tislelizumab not only yielded a high disease control rate but also indicated significant improvements in the tumor's immune landscape. The safety profile appears favorable, which is crucial for patients who have already undergone multiple lines of therapy.

      Strengths:

      The results underscore the potential of leveraging radiation therapy to enhance the effectiveness of immunotherapy, especially in tumor environments previously deemed hostile to immune interventions. Future research should focus on larger cohorts to validate these findings and explore the underlying mechanisms of immune modulation post-treatment.

      Weaknesses:

      I believe the author's work is commendable and should be considered with some minor modifications:

      (1) While the author categorized patients based on the type of RAS mutation and the location of colorectal cancer metastasis, the article does not adequately address how these classifications influence treatment outcomes. Such as whether KRAS or NRAS mutations, as well as the type of metastatic lesions, affect the sensitivity to gamma-ray treatment and lead to varying responses.

      Thank you very much for your question. Therefore, in the revised manuscript, we added an analysis of the impact of RAS mutation types and different metastatic sites on patient prognosis, but unfortunately, due to the limited number of samples, we were unable to obtain satisfactory results. We also placed the relevant results in the supplementary figure.

      (2) In Figure 2, clarification is needed on how the author differentiated between on-target and off-target lesions. I observed that some images depicted both lesion types at the same level, which could lead to confusion.

      We sincerely apologize for any oversight in our previous submission. To clarify, during the process of radiotherapy planning, we pre-select target lesions at the CT image level, and subsequently define the planning treatment volume (PTV) by marking these pre-selected areas with the 50% isodose lines. In our efficacy evaluation, we distinguish between the target lesions inside the PTV and any lesions outside the target area. In response to your valuable feedback, we have now added the isodose lines for the target lesions to the supplementary figure for greater clarity.

      (3) The author performed only a basic difference analysis. A more comprehensive analysis, including calculations of markers related to treatment efficacy, could offer additional insights for clinical practice.

      To identify potential markers associated with treatment efficacy, we attempted to establish a Cox proportional hazards model and conducted both univariate and multivariate Cox regression analyses. Unfortunately, due to the constraints of sample size and sequencing depth, the analyses did not yield statistically significant results, and we were unable to identify markers that could clearly predict treatment outcomes.

      (4) The transcriptome sequencing analysis provides insights into how stereotactic radiotherapy sensitizes immunotherapy; however, it currently relies on a simple pre- and post-treatment group comparison. It would be beneficial to include additional subgroups to explore more nuanced findings.

      We acknowledge the limitations in the depth of our analysis. In addition to performing differential analysis between the responder group (PR) and the non-responder group (Non-PR), we also conducted differential gene expression analysis on samples before and after treatment. The results revealed a consistent increase in the expression of NOS2 in both groups following Gamma Knife combined with immunotherapy, suggesting that this gene may serve as a potential prognostic factor influencing treatment outcomes. However, given the limited number of studies exploring the role of NOS2 in this context, we recognize that further research is necessary to better understand its involvement and to substantiate its potential as a predictive marker.

      (5) The author briefly discusses the effects of changes in tumor fibrosis and angiogenesis on treatment outcomes. Further experiments may be necessary to validate these findings and investigate the underlying mechanisms of immune regulation following treatment.

      We sincerely appreciate your thoughtful feedback on our results. In response, we conducted additional experiments, including immunohistochemical analysis of patient samples before and after combined treatment. The results demonstrated a reduction in the expression of CD31, a marker of tumor angiogenesis, following the combined treatment. This finding further supports our hypothesis that Gamma Knife treatment, in combination with immunotherapy, may effectively inhibit tumor angiogenesis, contributing to an improved therapeutic outcome.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This Phase II clinical trial investigates the combination of Gamma Knife Stereotactic Body Radiation Therapy (SBRT) with Tislelizumab for the treatment of metastatic colorectal cancer (mCRC) in patients with proficient mismatch repair (pMMR). The study addresses a critical clinical challenge in the management of pMMR CRC, focusing on the selection of appropriate candidates. The results suggest that the combination of Gamma Knife SBRT and Tislelizumab provides a safe and potent treatment option for patients with pMMR/MSS/MSI-L mCRC who have become refractory to first- and second-line chemotherapy. The study design is rigorous, and the outcomes are promising.

      Advantage:

      The trial design was meticulously structured, and appropriate statistical methods were employed to rigorously analyze the results. Bioinformatics approaches were utilized to further elucidate alterations in the patient's tumor microenvironment and to explore the underlying factors contributing to the observed differences in treatment efficacy. The conclusions drawn from this trial offer valuable insights for managing advanced colorectal cancer in patients who have not responded to first- and second-line therapies.

      Weakness:

      (1) Clarity and Structure of the Abstract<br /> - Results Section: The results section should contain important data, I suggest some important sequencing data should be shown to enhance understanding.

      Thank you for your insightful question. In response, we have revised the content of the article and restructured the abstract to enhance its scientific clarity and make it more accessible to readers.

      (2) As the author using the NanoString assay for transcriptome analysis, more detail should be shown such as the version of R, and the bioinformatics analysis methods.

      We have also addressed the missing details in our research methodology. The revised manuscript now includes a complete description of the research methods, along with the specific software and versions used.

      (3) It is interesting for included patients that PD-L1 increase expression after Gamma Knife Stereotactic Body Radiation Therapy (SBRT) treatment, How to explain it?

      Thank you for your thought-provoking question. PD-L1 plays a crucial role in tumor cell immune evasion, and anti-PD-1/PD-L1 inhibitors have emerged as effective immune checkpoint inhibitors, widely used in cancer therapy. In our clinical trials, we observed an increase in PD-L1 expression in some patients following combined treatment. Existing literature suggests that activation of various carcinogenic and stress response pathways, along with post-transcriptional modifications of PD-L1 (such as phosphorylation, glycosylation, acetylation, ubiquitination, and palmitoylation), can influence its expression[1]. We hypothesize that the increase in PD-L1 expression may be attributed to the activation of specific signaling pathways induced by the radiation from Gamma Knife treatment, as well as the enhanced tumor stress in response to the treatment. However, the precise mechanisms underlying this observation require further experimental investigation. A deeper understanding of these processes could potentially optimize our clinical treatment strategies.

      (4) It would be helpful to include a brief discussion of the limitations of the study, such as sample size constraints and their impact on the generalizability of the results. This will give readers a more comprehensive understanding of the findings.

      Thank you for highlighting the limitations of the article. In response, we have added a detailed discussion of the constraints arising from the limited number of experimental samples and insufficient sequencing depth. This addition aims to provide readers with a clearer understanding of the study's limitations and the context of our research findings.

      (5) Language Accuracy: There are a few instances where wording could be more professional or precise.

      Regarding the language deficiency, we are very sorry that the wording of the professional content in the article is not careful and accurate enough due to the difference in the native language environment. We have checked our article again and revised the wording and grammar in the hope that you and other readers can grasp our research content more accurately.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The research presented in this article is commendable; however, I would like to propose several revisions for consideration:

      Consideration of Concomitant Medications: It is imperative to ascertain whether enrolled patients utilized additional pharmacological agents alongside the trial regimen. Such concurrent drug use could potentially influence the final outcomes. A concise discussion of this aspect is warranted within the manuscript.

      Clinical Characterization of Response Groups: An examination of the clinical characteristics distinguishing the effective and non-responsive cohorts within the trial is essential. This inquiry merits further exploration, as it may elucidate factors influencing treatment efficacy.

      Tumor Microenvironment Analysis: The authors highlight the implications of tumor fibrosis and angiogenesis on therapeutic response. Identification of specific biomarkers associated with these phenotypes is crucial. I recommend undertaking straightforward testing and validation to substantiate these observations.

      Thank you very much for your valuable suggestions, many of which have been incorporated into the revised manuscript. Regarding the consideration of concurrent medication, we would like to clarify that all patients included in the study were advanced CRC patients who had progressed during first- or second-line treatments. As such, targeted therapy or chemotherapy was used concurrently in the trial. Previous studies have not indicated that different targeted therapies influence the efficacy of Gamma Knife treatment, though some chemotherapy agents may vary in their side effects. However, we believe these differences do not significantly impact the final outcomes. Given that existing chemotherapy regimens do not substantially affect patient prognosis, we considered the combined drug treatment regimen to be an irrelevant variable in our analysis.

      Additionally, we have carefully examined the clinical characteristics of patients across different groups. We have also included an analysis of the impact of various mutation types and metastatic sites in the revised manuscript. Furthermore, we plan to perform CD31 staining on lesions from both the responder and non-responder groups before and after Gamma Knife treatment to assess the role of angiogenesis in treatment response.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The abstract should be revised for greater clarity and include key results that substantiate the conclusions. The discussion section needs to more thoroughly address the limitations of the clinical trial, providing readers with a deeper understanding of the trial's findings and implications. Additionally, the methods section should be more rigorous and detailed, offering sufficient information to enhance the transparency and robustness of the experimental design.

      Thank you for your constructive suggestions regarding the shortcomings in our manuscript. In response, we have thoroughly reviewed the article and addressed the missing content, including revisions to the abstract, results, discussion, and methods sections. Additionally, we have refined the grammar and wording throughout the manuscript to enhance its professionalism and ensure it aligns with the standards expected for publication.

      (1)  YAMAGUCHI H, HSU J M, YANG W H, et al. Mechanisms regulating PD-L1 expression in cancers and associated opportunities for novel small-molecule therapeutics [J]. Nature reviews Clinical oncology, 2022, 19(5): 287-305.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The Authors investigated the anatomical features of the excitatory synaptic boutons in layer 1 of the human temporal neocortex. They examined the size of the synapse, the macular or the perforated appearance and the size of the synaptic active zone, the number and volume of the mitochondria, the number of the synaptic and the dense core vesicles, also differentiating between the readily releasable, the recycling and the resting pool of synaptic vesicles. The coverage of the synapse by astrocytic processes was also assessed, and all the above parameters were compared to other layers of the human temporal neocortex. The Authors conclude that the subcellular morphology of the layer 1 synapses is suitable for the functions of the neocortical layer, i.e. the synaptic integration within the cortical column. The low glial coverage of the synapses might allow the glutamate spillover from the synapses enhancing synaptic crosstalk within this cortical layer.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of this paper are the abundant and very precious data about the fine structure of the human neocortical layer 1. Quantitative electron microscopy data (especially that derived from the human brain) are very valuable, since this is a highly time- and energy consuming work. The techniques used to obtain the data, as well as the analyses and the statistics performed by the Authors are all solid, strengthen this manuscript, and mainly support the conclusions drawn in the discussion.

      Comments on latest version:

      The corrected version of the article titled “Ultrastructural sublaminar specific diversity of excitatory synaptic boutons in layer 1 of the adult human temporal lobe neocortex" has been improved thanks to the comments and suggestions of the reviewers. The Authors implemented several of my comments and suggestions. However, many of them were not completed. It is understandable that the Authors did not start a whole new series of experiments investigating inhibitory synapses (as it was a misunderstanding affecting 2 reviewers from the three). But the English text is still very hard to understand and has many mistakes, although I suggested to extensively review the use of English. Furthermore, my suggestion about avoiding many abbreviations in the abstract, analyse and discuss more the perforated synapses, the figure presentation (Figure 3) and including data about the astrocytic coverage in the Results section were not implemented. My questions about the number of docked vesicles and p10 vesicles, as well as about the different categories of the vesicle pools have not been answered neither. Many other minor comments and suggestions were answered, corrected and implemented, but I think it could have been improved more if the Authors take into account all of the reviewers' suggestions, not only some of them. I still have several main and minor concerns, with a few new ones as well I did not realize earlier, but still think it is important.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for the comments.

      - We worked on the English again and tried to improve the language.

      - We avoided to use too many abbreviations in the Abstract and reduced them to a minimum.

      - We included a small paragraph about non-perforated vs. perforated active zones in both the Results and Discussion sections. However, since the majority of active zones in all cortical layers of the human TLN were of the macular type, we concluded that it is not relevant to describe their function in more detail.

      - In Figure 3 A-C we added contour lines to the boutons to make their outlines more visible.

      - We completed the data about the astrocytic coverage in the Results section (see also below).

      - Concerning the vesicle pools please see below.

      Main concerns:

      (1) Epileptic patients:

      As all patients were epileptic, it is not correct to state in the abstract that non-epileptic tissue was investigated. Even if the seizure onset zone was not in the region investigated, seizures usually invade the temporal lobe in TLE. If you can prove that no spiking activity occurred in the sample you investigated and the seizures did not invade that region, then you can write that it is presumably non-epileptic. I would suggest to write “L1 of the human temporal lobe neocortical biopsy tissue". See also Methods lines 608-612. Write only “non-epileptic" or “non-affected" if you verified it with EcoG. If this was the case, please write a few sentences about it in the Methods.

      We rephrased Material and Methods concerning this point and added that patients were monitored with EEG, MRI and multielectrode recordings. In addition, we stated that the epileptic focus was always far away from the neocortical tissue samples. Furthermore, we added a small paragraph that functional studies using the same methodology have shown that neocortical access tissue samples taken from epilepsy surgery do not differ in electrophysiological properties and synaptic physiology when compared with acute slice preparations in experimental animals and we quoted the relevant papers.

      We hope that the reviewer is now convinced that our tissue samples can be regarded as non-affected.

      (2) About the inhibitory/excitatory synapses.

      Since our focus was on excitatory synaptic boutons as already stated in the title we have not analyzed inhibitory SBs. Now, I do understand that only excitatory synapses were investigated. Although it was written in the title, I did not realized, since all over the manuscript the Authors were writing synapses, and were distinguishing between inhibitory and excitatory synapses in the text and showing numerous excitatory and inhibitory synapses on Figure 2 and discussing inhibitory interneurons in the Discussion as well. Maybe this was the reason why two reviewers out of the three (including myself) thought you investigated both types of synapses but did not differentiated between them. So, please, emphasize in the Abstract (line 40), Introduction (for ex. line 92-97) and the Discussion (line 369) that only excitatory synaptic boutons were investigated.

      As this paper investigated only excitatory synaptic boutons, I think it is irrelevant to write such a long section in the Discussion about inhibitory interneurons and their functions in the L1 of the human temporal lobe neocortex. Same applies to the schematic drawing of the possible wiring of L1 (Figure 7). As no inhibitory interneurons were examined, neither the connection of the different excitatory cells, only the morphology of single synaptic boutons without any reference on their origin, I think this figure does not illustrate the work done in this paper. This could be a figure of a review paper about the human L1, but is inappropriate in this study.

      We followed the reviewer’s suggestion and pointed out explicitly that we only investigated excitatory synaptic boutons. We also changed the Discussion and focused more on circuitry in L1 and the role of CR-cells.

      (3) Perforated synapses

      The findings of the Geinismann group suggesting that perforated synapses are more efficient than non-perforated ones is nowadays very controversially discussed” I did not ask the Authors to say that perforated synapses are more efficient. However, based on the literature (for ex. Harris et al, 1992; Carlin and Siekievitz, 1982; Nieto-Sampedro et al., 1982) the presence of perforated synapses is indeed a good sign of synapse division/formation - which in turn might be coupled to synaptic plasticity (Geinisman et al, 1993), increased synaptic activity (Vrensen and Cardozo, 1981), LTP (Geinisman et al, 1991, Harris et al, 2003), pathological axonal sprouting (Frotscher et al, 2006), etc. I think it is worth mentioning this at least in the Discussion.

      We agree with the reviewer and added a small paragraph in the Results section about the two types of AZs in L1 of the human TLN. We pointed out that there are both types, macular non-perforated and perforated AZs, but the majority in all layers were of the non-perforated type. In the Discussion we added some paper pointing out the role of perforated synapses.

      (4) Question about the vesicle pools

      Results, Line 271: Still not understandable, why the RRP was defined as {less than or equal to}10 nm and {less than or equal to}20nm. Why did you use two categories? One would be sufficient (for example {less than or equal to}20nm). Or the vesicles between 10 and 20nm were considered to be part of RRP? In this case there is a typo, it should be {greater than or equal to}10 nm and {less than or equal to}20nm.

      The answer of the Authors was to my question raised: We decided that also those very close within 10 and 20 nm away from the PreAZ, which is less than a SV diameter may also contribute to the RRP since it was shown that SVs are quite mobile.

      This does not clarify why did you use two categories. Furthermore, I did not receive answer (such as Referee #2) for my question on how could you have 3x as many docked vesicles than vesicles {less than or equal to}10nm. The category {less than or equal to}10nm should also contain the docked vesicles. Or if this is not the case, please, clarify better what were your categories.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out that mentioning two distance criteria (p10 and p20) to define one physiological entity (RRP) is somewhat confusing and we acknowledge that the initial response to the reviewers falls short of explaining this choice. This is indeed only understandable in the context of the original paper by Sätzler et al. 2002, where these criteria were first introduced. We therefore referenced this publication more prominently in the paragraph in question.

      So to explain this, we first would like to clarify the definition of the two RRP classification criteria used (p10 and p20), which has caused some confusion amongst the reviewers as to which vesicles where included or not:

      - p10 criterion: p£10 nm (SVs have a minimum distance less than or equal to 10 nm from the PreAZ), including ‘docked’ vesicles which have a distance of zero or less (p0)

      - p20 criterion: p£20 nm (SVs have a minimum distance less than or equal to 20 nm from the PreAZ), including vesicles of the p10 criterion.

      As mentioned, these criteria were introduced first in Sätzler et al. 2002 looking at the Calyx of Held synapse. In that paper, we tried to establish a morphological correlate to existing physiological measurements, which included the RRP. As there is no known marker that would allow to discriminate between vesicles that contribute to the RRP anatomically, we looked at existing physiological experiments such as Schneggenburger et al. 1999; Wu and Borst 1999; Sun and Wu 2001 and compared their total numbers to our measurements. As the number of docked vesicles (p0, see above) was on the lower side of these physiological estimates, we also looked at vesicles close to the AZ, which we think could be recruited within a short time (£ 10 msec). Comparing with existing literature, we found that at p20 we get pool sizes comparable to midrange estimates of reported RRP sizes. In order to account for the variability of the observed physiological pool sizes, we reported all three measurements (p0, p10, p20) not only in the original Calyx of Held, but in all subsequent studies of different CNS synapses of our group since then.

      As it remains uncertain if such correlate indeed exists, we therefore followed the suggestion to rephrase RRP and RP to putative RRP and putative RP (see also Rollenhagen et al. 2007). We thank both reviewers for pointing out this omission.

      Concerning the difference between ‘docked’ vesicles and vesicles within the p10 perimeter criterion. First of all, the reviewer is right in saying that the category p10 ({less than or equal to}10nm) should also contain the docked vesicles (see above). The fact to have 3x as many ‘docked’ vesicles in our TEM tomography than in the p10 distance analysis could be partly explained, on the one hand, by a very high variability between patients (as expressed by the high SD, table 1) and, on the other hand, by a high intraindividual synaptic bouton variability. In both sublayers, there is a huge difference in the number of vesicles within the p10 criterion of individual synaptic boutons ranging from 0 to ~40 with a mean value of ~1 to ~4 (calculated per patient), the upper level being close to the values calculated with TEM tomography for the ‘docked’ vesicles.

      (5) Astrocytic coverage

      On Fig. 6 data are presented on the astrocytic coverage derived from L1 and L4. In my previous review I asked to include this in the text of the Results as well, but I still do not see it. It is also lacking from the Results how many samples from which layer were investigated in this analysis. Only percentages are given, and only for L1 (but how many patients, L1a and/or L1b and/or L4 is not provided). In contrast, Figure 6 and Supplementary Table 2 (patient table) contains the information that this analysis has been made in L4 as well. Please, include this information in the text as well (around lines 348-360).

      In our previous revised version, we had included the values shown in Fig. 6 for both L1 and L4 in the Results section (L4: lines 352 – 355: ‘The findings in L1…’). However, we agree with the reviewer and have now also added the number of patients and synapses investigated (now lines 359 – 365).

      About how to determine glial elements. I cannot agree with the Authors that glial elements can be determined with high certainty based only on the anatomical features of the profiles seen in the EM. “With 25 years of experience in (serial) EM work" I would say, that glial elements can be very similar to spine necks and axonal profiles.

      All in all, if similar methods were used to determine the glial coverage in the different layers of the human neocortex, than it can be compared (I guess this is the case). However, I would say in the text that proper determination would need immunostaining and a new analysis. This only gives an estimation with the possibility of a certain degree of error.

      We do not entirely agree with the reviewer on this point. As stated in the text, there are structural criteria to identify astrocytic elements (see citations quoted). These golden standard criteria are commonly used also by other well-known groups (DeFelipe and co-workers, Francisco Clasca and co-workers; Michael Frotscher the late and co-workers etc.). However, in a past paper about astrocytic coverage of synaptic complexes in L5 of the human TLN, immunohistochemistry against glutamine synthetase, a key enzyme in astrocytes, was carried out to describe the coverage. This experiment supports our findings in the other cortical layers of the human TLN. As the reviewer might know, immunohistochemistry always led to a reduction in ultrastructural preservation, so we decided not to use immunohistochemistry for the further publications of the other cortical layers. We added a short notice on this in the Material and Methods section.

      (6) Large interindividual differences in the synapse density should be discussed in the Discussion.

      As suggested by the reviewer we have included a sentence in the Discussion that interindividual differences can be either related to differences in age, gender and the use of different methodology as suggested by DeFelipe and co-workers (1999)

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study of Rollenhagen et al examines the ultrastructural features of Layer 1 of human temporal cortex. The tissue was derived from drug-resistant epileptic patients undergoing surgery, and was selected as further from the epilepsy focus, and as such considered to be non-epileptic. The analyses has included 4 patients with different age, sex, medication and onset of epilepsy. The MS is a follow-on study with 3 previous publications from the same authors on different layers of the temporal cortex:

      Layer 4 - Yakoubi et al 2019 eLife

      Layer 5 - Yakoubi et al 2019 Cerebral Cortex,

      Layer 6 - Schmuhl-Giesen et al 2022 Cerebral Cortex

      They find, the L1 synaptic boutons mainly have single active zone a very large pool of synaptic vesicles and are mostly devoid of astrocytic coverage.

      Strengths:

      The MS is well written easy to read. Result section gives a detailed set of figures showing many morphological parameters of synaptic boutons and surrounding glial elements. The authors provide comparative data of all the layers examined by them so far in the Discussion. Given that anatomical data in human brain are still very limited, the current MS has substantial relevance.

      The work appears to be generally well done, the EM and EM tomography images are of very good quality. The analyses is clear and precise.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors made all the corrections required, answered most of my concerns, included additional data sets, and clarified statements where needed.

      My remaining points are:

      Synaptic vesicle diameter (that has been established to be ~40nm independent of species) can properly be measured with EM tomography only, as it provides the possibility to find the largest diameter of every given vesicle. Measuring it in 50 nm thick sections result in underestimation (just like here the values are ~25 nm) as the measured diameter will be smaller than the true diameter if the vesicle is not cut in the middle, (which is the least probable scenario). The authors have the EM tomography data set for measuring the vesicle diameter properly.

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful comments. We followed the recommendation to measure the vesicle diameter using our TEM tomography tilt series, but came to similar results concerning this synaptic parameter. As stated in our Material and Methods section, we only counted (measured) clear ring-link structures according to a paper by Abercrombie (1963). Since our results are similar for both methods, we do believe that our measurements are correct. Even random single measurements on the original 3D tilt-series yielded comparable results (Lübke and co-workers, personal observation). Furthermore, our results are within ranges, although with high variability, also described by other groups (see discussion lines 436 - 449). We therefore hope that the reviewer will now accept our measurements.

      It is a bit misleading to call vesicle populations at certain arbitrary distances from the presynaptic active zone as readily releasable pool, recycling pool and resting pool, as these are functional categories, and cannot directly be translated to vesicles at certain distances. Even it is debated whether the morphologically docked vesicles are the ones, that are readily releasable, as further molecular steps, such as proper priming is also a prerequisite for release.

      It would help to call these pools as "putative" correlates of the morphological categories.

      We followed the suggestion by the reviewer and renamed our vesicle pools as putative RRP, putative RP and putative resting pools.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Rollenhagen at al. offer a detailed description of layer 1 of the human neocortex. They use electron microscopy to assess the morphological parameters of presynaptic terminals, active zones, vesicle density/distribution, mitochondrial morphology and astrocytic coverage. The data is collected from tissue from four patients undergoing epilepsy surgery. As the epileptic focus was localized in all patients to the hippocampus, the tissue examined in this manuscript is considered non-epileptic (access) tissue.

      Strengths:

      The quality of the electron microscopic images is very high, and the data is analyzed carefully. Data from human tissue is always precious and the authors here provide a detailed analysis using adequate approaches, and the data is clearly presented.

      Weaknesses:

      The text connects functional and morphological characteristics in a very direct way. For example, connecting plasticity to any measurement the authors present would be rather difficult without any additional functional experiments. References to various vesicle pools based on the location of the vesicles is also more complex than it is suggested in the manuscript. The text should better reflect the limitations of the conclusions that can be drawn from the authors' data.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Astrocytic coverage

      On Fig. 6 data are presented on the astrocytic coverage derived from L1 and L4. In my previous review I asked to include this in the text of the Results as well, but I still do not see it. It is also lacking from the Results how many samples from which layer were investigated in this analysis. Only percentages are given, and only for L1 (but how many patients, L1a and/or L1b and/or L4 is not provided). In contrast, Figure 6 and Supplementary Table 2 (patient table) contains the information that this analysis has been made in L4 as well. Please, include this information in the text as well (around lines 348-360).

      See above.

      About how to determine glial elements. I cannot agree with the Authors that glial elements can be determined with high certainty based only on the anatomical features of the profiles seen in the EM. “With 25 years of experience in (serial) EM work" I would say, that glial elements can be very similar to spine necks and axonal profiles. Please, see the photos below, out of the 16 circled profiles (2nd picture, very similar to each other) only 3 belong to an astroglial cell (last picture, purple profiles-purple cell), 10 are spines/spine necks/small caliber dendrites of pyramidal cells, 3 are axonal profiles (last but one picture, blue profiles, marked with arrows on the right side). If you follow in your serial sections those elements which you think are glial processes and indeed they are attached to a confidently identifiable glial cell, I agree, it is a glial process. But identifying small, almost empty profiles without any specific staining, from one single EM section, as glial process is very uncertain. Please, check the database of the Allen Institute made from the V1 visual cortex of a mouse. It is a large series of EM sections where they reconstructed thousands of neurons, astroglial and microglial cells. It is possible to double click on the EM picture on a profile and it will show the cell to which that profile belongs. https://portal.brain-map.org/connectivity/ultrastructural-connectomics Pictures included here: https://elife-rp.msubmit.net/eliferp_files/2024/11/25/00132644/02/132644_2_attach_21_29456_convrt.pdf

      All in all, if similar methods were used to determine the glial coverage in the different layers of the human neocortex, than it can be compared (I guess this is the case). However, I would say in the text that proper determination would need immunostaining and a new analysis. This only gives an estimation with the possibility of a certain degree of error.

      As stated above, we carried out glutamine synthetase immunohistochemistry in L5 of the human TLN and came to the same results. However, we added a sentence on this in the chapter on astrocytic coverage in the Material and Methods section. Additionally, we modified this chapter according to the reviewer’s suggestion.

      Minor comments

      Introduction: Last sentence is not understandable (lines 101-103), please rephrase. (contribute to understand or contribute in understanding or contribute to the understanding of..., but definitely not contribute to understanding). The authors should check and review extensively for improvements to the use of English, or use a program such as Grammarly.

      Results: Grammar (line 107): L1 in the adult mammalian neocortex represents a relatively...

      Line 173: “Some SBs in both sublaminae were seen to establish either two or three SBs on the same spine, spines 173 of other origin or dendritic shafts." - Some SBs established two or three SBs? I would write Some SBs established two or three synapses on...

      Line 243: “The synaptic cleft size were slightly, but non-significantly different"

      Line 260: “DCVs play an important role in endo- and exocytosis, the build-up of PreAZs by releasing Piccolo and Bassoon (Schoch and Gundelfinger 2006; Murkherjee et al. 2010)," - please, correct this.

      We have done corrections as suggested by the reviewer.

      Line 374: No point at the end of the last phrase.

      Discussion:

      Lines 400-404: “The majority of SBs in L1 of the human TLN had a single at most three AZs that could be of the non perforated macular or perforated type comparable with results for other layers in the human TLN but by ~1.5-fold larger than in rodent and non-human primates." - What is comparable with the other layers, but different from animals? Please rephrase this sentence, it is not understandable. I already mentioned this sentence in my previous review, but nothing happened.

      Lines 435-437: “Remarkably, the total pool sizes in the human TLN were significantly larger by more than 6-fold (~550 SVs/AZ), and ~4.7-fold (~750 SVs/AZ;) than those in L4 and L5 (Yakoubi et al. 2019a, b; see also Rollenhagen et al. 2018) in rats." Please rethink what you wished to say and compare to the sentence meaning. I think you wanted to compare human TLN L1 pool size to L4 and L5 in the human TLN (Yakoubi 2019a and b) and to rat (Rollenhagen 2018). Instead, you compared all layers of the human TLN to L4 and L5 in rats (with partly wrong references). Please rephrase this. Lines 483-484: “Astrocytes serve as both a physical barrier to glutamate diffusion and as mediate neurotransmitter uptake via transporters".

      This sentence is grammatically incorrect, please rephrase.

      We corrected the sentences as suggested by the reviewer.

      Methods:

      In the text, there are only 4 patients (lines 603-604), but in the supplementary table there are 9 patients (5 new included for L4 astrocytic coverage). Please, correct it in the text.

      Lines 608-609: “neocortical access tissue samples were resected to control the seizures for histological inspection by neuropathologists." - What is the meaning of this? Please, rephrase.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment and included the 5 patients used for L4 to the Material and Methods section, as well as in the Results section.

      The reviewer is right, and we rephrased and corrected the sentence concerning the inspection by neuropathologists.

      Figures

      Figures 5B: The legend says “SB (sb) synapsing on a stubby spine (sp) with a prominent spine apparatus (framed area) and a thick dendritic segment (de) in L1b" - In my opinion this is not one synaptic bouton, but two. Clearly visible membranes separate them, close to the spine.

      Supplemental Table 2 (patient table). If there is no information about Hu_04 patient's epilepsy, please write N/A (=non available) instead of - (which means it does not exist).

      The reviewer is right, and we corrected the figure and the legend, as well as the table accordingly.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors addressed almost all of my concern, only this one remained:

      If there is, however, relevant literature on "methods based on EM tomography" and "stereological methods to estimate both types of error" (over- and underestimates) that we are missing out on, we would appreciate the reviewer providing us with the corresponding references so that we can include such calculations in our paper.

      There is a very detailed new study on calculating correction for TEM 2D 3D, Rothman et al 2023 PLOS One. That addresses most of these issues.

      We thank the reviewer for drawing our attention to the publication by Rothman et al. 2023, which is a very detailed and comprehensive study looking at accurately estimating distributions of 3D size and densities of particles from 2D measurements using – amongst others – ET and TEM images as well as synaptic vesicles for validating their method. However, we do not see how this would be relevant to the reported mean diameters and their corresponding variances. And even if we would have reported on vesicle size/diameter distributions (referred to as G(d) in Rothmann et al. 2023), the authors themselves state that “… the results from our ET and TEM image analysis highlight the difficulty in computing a complete G(d) of MFT vesicles due to their small size…

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Mackie and colleagues compare chemosensory preferences between C. elegans and P. pacificus, and the cellular and molecular mechanisms underlying them. The nematodes have overlapping and distinct preferences for different salts. Although P. pacificus lacks the lsy-6 miRNA important for establishing asymmetry of the left/right ASE salt-sensing neurons in C. elegans, the authors find that P. pacificus ASE homologs achieve molecular (receptor expression) and functional (calcium response) asymmetry by alternative means. This work contributes an important comparison of how these two nematodes sense salts and highlights that evolution can find different ways to establish asymmetry in small nervous systems to optimize the processing of chemosensory cues in the environment.

      Strengths:

      The authors use clear and established methods to record the response of neurons to chemosensory cues. They were able to show clearly that ASEL/R are functionally asymmetric in P. pacificus, and combined with genetic perturbation establish a role for che-1-dependent gcy-22.3 in in the asymmetric response to NH<sub>4</sub>Cl.

      Weaknesses:

      The mechanism of lsy-6-independent establishment of ASEL/R asymmetry in P. pacificus remains uncharacterized.

      We thank the reviewer for recognizing the novel contributions of our work in revealing the existence of alternative pathways for establishing neuronal lateral asymmetry without the lsy-6 miRNA in a divergent nematode species. We are certainly encouraged now to search for genetic factors that alter the exclusive asymmetric expression of gcy-22.3.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Mackie et al. investigate gustatory behavior and the neural basis of gustation in the predatory nematode Pristionchus pacificus. First, they show that the behavioral preferences of P. pacificus for gustatory cues differ from those reported for C. elegans. Next, they investigate the molecular mechanisms of salt sensing in P. pacificus. They show that although the C. elegans transcription factor gene che-1 is expressed specifically in the ASE neurons, the P. pacificus che-1 gene is expressed in the Ppa-ASE and Ppa-AFD neurons. Moreover, che-1 plays a less critical role in salt chemotaxis in P. pacificus than C. elegans. Chemogenetic silencing of Ppa-ASE and Ppa-AFD neurons results in more severe chemotaxis defects. The authors then use calcium imaging to show that both Ppa-ASE and Ppa-AFD neurons respond to salt stimuli. Calcium imaging experiments also reveal that the left and right Ppa-ASE neurons respond differently to salts, despite the fact that P. pacificus lacks lsy-6, a microRNA that is important for ASE left/right asymmetry in C. elegans. Finally, the authors show that the receptor guanylate cyclase gene Ppa-gcy-23.3 is expressed in the right Ppa-ASE neuron (Ppa-ASER) but not the left Ppa-ASE neuron (Ppa-ASEL) and is required for some of the gustatory responses of Ppa-ASER, further confirming that the Ppa-ASE neurons are asymmetric and suggesting that Ppa-GCY-23.3 is a gustatory receptor. Overall, this work provides insight into the evolution of gustation across nematode species. It illustrates how sensory neuron response properties and molecular mechanisms of cell fate determination can evolve to mediate species-specific behaviors. However, the paper would be greatly strengthened by a direct comparison of calcium responses to gustatory cues in C. elegans and P. pacificus, since the comparison currently relies entirely on published data for C. elegans, where the imaging parameters likely differ. In addition, the conclusions regarding Ppa-AFD neuron function would benefit from additional confirmation of AFD neuron identity. Finally, how prior salt exposure influences gustatory behavior and neural activity in P. pacificus is not discussed.

      Strengths:

      (1) This study provides exciting new insights into how gustatory behaviors and mechanisms differ in nematode species with different lifestyles and ecological niches. The results from salt chemotaxis experiments suggest that P. pacificus shows distinct gustatory preferences from C. elegans. Calcium imaging from Ppa-ASE neurons suggests that the response properties of the ASE neurons differ between the two species. In addition, an analysis of the expression and function of the transcription factor Ppa-che-1 reveals that mechanisms of ASE cell fate determination differ in C. elegans and P. pacificus, although the ASE neurons play a critical role in salt sensing in both species. Thus, the authors identify several differences in gustatory system development and function across nematode species.

      (2) This is the first calcium imaging study of P. pacificus, and it offers some of the first insights into the evolution of gustatory neuron function across nematode species.

      (3) This study addresses the mechanisms that lead to left/right asymmetry in nematodes. It reveals that the ASER and ASEL neurons differ in their response properties, but this asymmetry is achieved by molecular mechanisms that are at least partly distinct from those that operate in C. elegans. Notably, ASEL/R asymmetry in P. pacificus is achieved despite the lack of a P. pacificus lsy-6 homolog.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The authors observe only weak attraction of C. elegans to NaCl. These results raise the question of whether the weak attraction observed is the result of the prior salt environment experienced by the worms. More generally, this study does not address how prior exposure to gustatory cues shapes gustatory responses in P. pacificus. Is salt sensing in P. pacificus subject to the same type of experience-dependent modulation as salt sensing in C. elegans?

      We tested if starving animals in the presence of a certain salt will result in those animals avoiding it. However, under our experimental conditions we were unable to detect experiencedependent modulation either in P. pacificus or in C. elegans.

      Author response image 1.

      (2) A key finding of this paper is that the Ppa-CHE-1 transcription factor is expressed in the PpaAFD neurons as well as the Ppa-ASE neurons, despite the fact that Ce-CHE-1 is expressed specifically in Ce-ASE. However, additional verification of Ppa-AFD neuron identity is required. Based on the image shown in the manuscript, it is difficult to unequivocally identify the second pair of CHE-1-positive head neurons as the Ppa-AFD neurons. Ppa-AFD neuron identity could be verified by confocal imaging of the CHE-1-positive neurons, co-expression of Ppa-che1p::GFP with a likely AFD reporter, thermotaxis assays with Ppa-che-1 mutants, and/or calcium imaging from the putative Ppa-AFD neurons.

      In the revised manuscript, we provide additional and, we believe, conclusive evidence for our correct identification of Ppa-AFD neuron being another CHE-1 expressing neuron. Specifically, we have constructed and characterized 2 independent reporter strains of Ppa-ttx-1, a putative homolog of the AFD terminal selector in C. elegans. There are two pairs of ttx-1p::rfp expressing amphid neurons. The anterior neuronal pair have finger-like endings that are unique for AFD neurons compared to the dendritic endings of the 11 other amphid neuron pairs (no neuron type has a wing morphology in P. pacificus). Their cell bodies are detected in the newly tagged TTX-1::ALFA strain that co-localize with the anterior pair of che-1::gfp-expressing amphid neurons (n=15, J2-Adult).

      We note that the identity of the posterior pair of amphid neurons differs between the ttx-1p::rfp promoter fusion reporter and TTX-1::ALFA strains– the ttx-1p::rfp posterior amphid pair overlaps with the gcy-22.3p::gfp reporter (ASER) but the TTX-1::ALFA posterior amphid pair do not overlap with the posterior pair of che-1::gfp-expressing amphid neurons (n=15). Given that there are 4 splice forms detected by RNAseq (Transcriptome Assembly Trinity, 2016; www.pristionchus.org), this discrepancy between the Ppa-ttx-1 promoter fusion reporter and the endogenous expression of the Ppa-TTX-1 C-terminally tagged to the only splice form containing Exon 18 (ppa_stranded_DN30925_c0_g1_i5, the most 3’ exon) may be due to differential expression of different splice variants in AFD, ASE, and another unidentified amphid neuron types.  

      Although we also made reporter strains of two putative AFD markers, Ppa-gcy-8.1 (PPA24212)p::gfp; csuEx101 and Ppa-gcy-8.2 (PPA41407)p::gfp; csuEx100, neither reporter showed neuronal expression.

      (3) Loss of Ppa-che-1 causes a less severe phenotype than loss of Ce-che-1. However, the loss of Ppa-che-1::RFP expression in ASE but not AFD raises the question of whether there might be additional start sites in the Ppa-che-1 gene downstream of the mutation sites. It would be helpful to know whether there are multiple isoforms of Ppa-che-1, and if so, whether the exon with the introduced frameshift is present in all isoforms and results in complete loss of Ppa-CHE-1 protein.

      According to www.pristionchus.org (Transcriptome Assembly Trinity), there is only a single detectable splice form by RNAseq. Once we have a Ppa-AFD-specific marker, we would be able to determine how much of the AFD terminal effector identify (e.g. expression of gcy-8 paralogs) is effected by the loss of Ppa-che-1 function.

      (4) The authors show that silencing Ppa-ASE has a dramatic effect on salt chemotaxis behavior. However, these data lack control with histamine-treated wild-type animals, with the result that the phenotype of Ppa-ASE-silenced animals could result from exposure to histamine dihydrochloride. This is an especially important control in the context of salt sensing, where histamine dihydrochloride could alter behavioral responses to other salts.

      We have inadvertently left out this important control. Because the HisCl1 transgene is on a randomly segregating transgene array, we have scored worms with and without the transgene expressing the co-injection marker (Ppa-egl-20p::rfp, a marker in the tail) to show that the presence of the transgene is necessary for the histamine-dependent knockdown of NH<sub>4</sub>Br attraction. This control is added as Figure S2.

      (5) The calcium imaging data in the paper suggest that the Ppa-ASE and Ce-ASE neurons respond differently to salt solutions. However, to make this point, a direct comparison of calcium responses in C. elegans and P. pacificus using the same calcium indicator is required. By relying on previously published C. elegans data, it is difficult to know how differences in growth conditions or imaging conditions affect ASE responses. In addition, the paper would be strengthened by additional quantitative analysis of the calcium imaging data. For example, the paper states that 25 mM NH<sub>4</sub>Cl evokes a greater response in ASEL than 250 mM NH<sub>4</sub>Cl, but a quantitative comparison of the maximum responses to the two stimuli is not shown.

      We understand that side-by-side comparisons with C. elegans using the same calcium indicator would lend more credence to the differences we observed in P. pacificus versus published findings in C. elegans from the past decades, but are not currently in a position to conduct these experiments in parallel.

      (6) It would be helpful to examine, or at least discuss, the other P. pacificus paralogs of Ce-gcy22. Are they expressed in Ppa-ASER? How similar are the different paralogs? Additional discussion of the Ppa-gcy-22 gene expansion in P. pacificus would be especially helpful with respect to understanding the relatively minor phenotype of the Ppa-gcy-22.3 mutants.

      In P. pacificus, there are 5 gcy-22-like paralogs and 3 gcy-7-like paralogs, which together form a subclade that is clearly distinct from the 1-1 Cel-gcy-22, Cel-gcy-5, and Cel-gcy-7 orthologs in a phylogenetic tree containing all rGCs in P. pacificus, C. elegans, and C. briggssae (Hong et al, eLife, 2019). In Ortiz et al (2006 and 2009), Cel-gcy-22 stands out from other ASER-type gcy genes (gcy-1, gcy-4, gcy-5) in being located on a separate chromosome (Chr. V) as well as in having a wider range of defects in chemoattraction towards salt ions. Given that the 5 P. pacificus gcy-22-like paralogs are located on 3 separate chromosomes without clear synteny to their C. elegans counterparts, it is likely that the gcy-22 paralogs emerged from independent and repeated gene duplication events after the separation of these Caenorhabditis and Pristionchus lineages. Our reporter strains for two other P. pacificus gcy-22-like paralogs either did not exhibit expression in amphid neurons (Ppa-gcy-22.1p::GFP, ) or exhibited expression in multiple neuron types in addition to a putative ASE neuron (Ppa-gcy-22.4p::GFP). We have expanded the discussion on the other P. pacificus gcy-22 paralogs.

      (7) The calcium imaging data from Ppa-ASE is quite variable. It would be helpful to discuss this variability. It would also be helpful to clarify how the ASEL and ASER neurons are being conclusively identified during calcium imaging.

      For each animal, the orientation of the nose and vulva were recorded and used as a guide to determine the ventral and dorsal sides of the worm, and subsequently, the left and right sides of the worm. Accounting for the plane of focus of the neuron pairs as viewed through the microscope, it was then determined whether the imaged neuron was the worm’s left or right neuron of each pair. We added this explanation to the Methods.

      (8) More information about how the animals were treated prior to calcium imaging would be helpful. In particular, were they exposed to salt solutions prior to imaging? In addition, the animals are in an M9 buffer during imaging - does this affect calcium responses in Ppa-ASE and Ppa-AFD? More information about salt exposure, and how this affects neuron responses, would be very helpful.

      Prior to calcium imaging, animals were picked from their cultivation plates (using an eyelash pick to minimize bacteria transfer) and placed in loading solution (M9 buffer with 0.1% Tween20 and 1.5 mM tetramisole hydrochloride, as indicated in the Method) to immobilize the animals until they were visibly completely immobilized.

      (9) In Figure 6, the authors say that Ppa-gcy-22.3::GFP expression is absent in the Ppa-che1(ot5012) mutant. However, based on the figure, it looks like there is some expression remaining. Is there a residual expression of Ppa-gcy-22.3::GFP in ASE or possibly ectopic expression in AFD? Does Ppa-che-1 regulate rGC expression in AFD? It would be helpful to address the role of Ppa-che-1 in AFD neuron differentiation.

      In Figure 6C, the green signal is autofluorescence in the gut, and there is no GFP expression detected in any of the 55 che-1(-) animals we examined. We are currently developing AFDspecific rGC markers (gcy-8 homologs) to be able to examine the role of Ppa-CHE-1 in regulating AFD identity.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Abstract: 'how does sensory diversity prevail within this neuronal constraint?' - could be clearer as 'numerical constraint' or 'neuron number constraint'.

      We have clarified this passage as ‘…constraint in neuron number’.

      (2) 'Sensory neurons in the Pristionchus pacificus' - should get rid of the 'the'.

      We have removed the ‘the’.

      (3) Figure 2: We have had some good results with the ALFA tag using a similar approach (tagging endogenous loci using CRISPR). I'm not sure if it is a Pristionchus thing, or if it is a result of our different protocols, but our staining appears stronger with less background. We use an adaptation of the Finney-Ruvkin protocol, which includes MeOH in the primary fixation with PFA, and overcomes the cuticle barrier with some LN2 cracking, DTT, then H2O2. No collagenase. If you haven't tested it already it might be worth comparing the next time you have a need for immunostaining.

      We appreciate this suggestion. Our staining protocol uses paraformaldehyde fixation. We observed consistent and clear staining in only 4 neurons in CHE-1::ALFA animals but more background signals from TTX-1::ALFA in Figure 2I-J in that could benefit from improved immunostaining protocol.

      (4) Page 6: 'By crossing the che-1 reporter transgene into a che-1 mutant background (see below), we also found that che-1 autoregulates its own expression (Figure 2F), as it does in C. elegans' - it took me some effort to understand this. It might make it easier for future readers if this is explained more clearly.

      We understand this confusion and have changed the wording along with a supporting table with a more detailed account of che-1p::RFP expression in both ASE and AFD neurons in wildtype and che-1(-) backgrounds in the Results.

      (5) Line numbers would make it easier for reviewers to reference the text.

      We have added line numbers.

      (6) Page 7: is 250mM NH<sub>4</sub>Cl an ecologically relevant concentration? When does off-target/nonspecific activation of odorant receptors become an issue? Some discussion of this could help readers assess the relevance of the salt concentrations used.

      This is a great question but one that is difficult to reconcile between experimental conditions that often use 2.5M salt as point-source to establish salt gradients versus ecologically relevant concentrations that are very heterogenous in salinity. Efforts to show C. elegans can tolerate similar levels of salinity between 0.20-0.30 M without adverse effects have been recorded previously (Hu et al., Analytica Chimica Acta 2015; Mah et al. Expedition 2017).

      (7) It would be nice for readers to have a short orientation to the ecological relevance of the different salts - e.g. why Pristionchus has a particular taste for ammonium salts.

      Pristionchus species are entomophilic and most frequently found to be associated with beetles in a necromenic manner. Insect cadavers could thus represent sources of ammonium in the soil. Additionally, ammonium salts could represent a biological signature of other nematodes that the predatory morphs of P. pacificus could interpret as prey. We have added the possible ecological relevance of ammonium salts into the Discussion.

      (8) Page 11: 'multiple P. pacificus che-1p::GCaMP strains did not exhibit sufficient basal fluorescence to allow for image tracking and direct comparison'. 500ms exposure to get enough signal from RCaMP is slow, but based on the figures it still seems enough to capture things. If image tracking was the issue, then using GCaMP6s with SL2-RFP or similar in conjunction with a beam splitter enables tracking when the GCaMP signal is low. Might be an option for the future.

      These are very helpful suggestions and we hope to eventually develop an improved che1p::GCaMP strain for future studies.

      (9) Sometimes C. elegans genes are referred to as 'C. elegans [gene name]' and sometimes 'Cel [gene name]'. Should be consistent. Same with Pristionchus.

      We have now combed through and corrected the inconsistencies in nomenclature.

      (10) Pg 12 - '...supports the likelihood that AFD receives inputs, possibly neuropeptidergic, from other amphid neurons' - the neuropeptidergic part could do with some justification.

      Because the AFD neurons are not exposed directly to the environment through the amphid channel like the ASE and other amphid neurons, the calcium responses to salts detected in the AFD likely originate from sensory neurons connected to the AFD. However, because there is no synaptic connection from other amphid neurons to the AFD neurons in P. pacificus (unlike in C. elegans; Hong et al, eLife, 2019), it is likely that neuropeptides connect other sensory neurons to the AFDs. To avoid unnecessary confusion, we have removed “possibly neuropeptidergic.”

      (11) Pg16: the link to the Hallam lab codon adaptor has a space in the middle. Also, the paper should be cited along with the web address (Bryant and Hallam, 2021).

      We have now added the proper link, plus in-text citation. https://hallemlab.shinyapps.io/Wild_Worm_Codon_Adapter/ (Bryant and Hallem, 2021)

      Full citation:

      Astra S Bryant, Elissa A Hallem, The Wild Worm Codon Adapter: a web tool for automated codon adaptation of transgenes for expression in non-Caenorhabditis nematodes, G3 Genes|Genomes|Genetics, Volume 11, Issue 7, July 2021, jkab146, https://doi.org/10.1093/g3journal/jkab146

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) In Figure 1, the legend states that the population tested was "J4/L4 larvae and young adult hermaphrodites," whereas in the main text, the population was described as "adult hermaphrodites." Please clarify which ages were tested.

      We have tested J4-Adult stage hermaphrodites and have made the appropriate corrections in the text.

      (2) The authors state that "in contrast to C. elegans, we find that P. pacificus is only moderately and weakly attracted to NaCl and LiCl, respectively." However, this statement does not reflect the data shown in Figure 1, where there is no significant difference between C. elegans and P. pacificus - both species show at most weak attraction to NaCl.

      Although there is no statistically significant difference in NaCl attraction between P. pacificus and C. elegans, NaCl attraction in P. pacificus is significantly lower than its attraction to all 3 ammonium salts when compared to C. elegans. We have rephrased this statement as relative differences in the Results and updated the Figure legend.

      (3) In Figure 1, the comparisons between C. elegans and P. pacificus should be made using a two-way ANOVA rather than multiple t-tests. Also, the sample sizes should be stated (so the reader does not need to count the circles) and the error bars should be defined.

      We performed the 2-way ANOVA to detect differences between C. elegans and P. pacificus for the same salt and between salts within each species. We also indicated the sample size on the figure and defined the error bars.

      Significance:

      For comparisons of different salt responses within the same species:

      - For C. elegans, NH<sub>4</sub>Br vs NH<sub>4</sub>Cl (**p<0.01), NH<sub>4</sub>Cl vs NH<sub>4</sub>I (* p<0.05), and NH<sub>4</sub>Cl vs NaCl (* p<0.05). All other comparisons are not significant.

      - For P. pacificus, all salts showed (****p<0.0001) when compared to NaAc and to NH<sub>4</sub>Ac, except for NH<sub>4</sub>Ac and NaAc compared to each other (ns). Also, NH<sub>4</sub>Cl showed (*p<0.05) and NH<sub>4</sub>I showed (***p<0.001) when compared with LiCl and NaCl. All other comparisons are not significant.

      For comparisons of salt responses between different species (N2 vs PS312):

      - NH<sub>4</sub>I and LiCl (*p<0.05); NaAc and NH<sub>4</sub>Ac (****p<0.0001)

      (4) It might be worth doing a power analysis on the data in Figure 3B. If the data are underpowered, this might explain why there is a difference in NH<sub>4</sub>Br response with one of the null mutants but not the other.

      For responses to NH<sub>4</sub>Cl, since both che-1 mutants (rather than just one) showed significant difference compared to wildtype, we conducted a power analysis based on the effect size of that difference (~1.2; large). Given this effect size, the sample size for future experiments should be 12 (ANOVA).

      For responses to NH<sub>4</sub>Br and given the effect size of the difference seen between wildtype (PS312) and ot5012 (~0.8; large), the sample size for future experiments should be 18 (ANOVA) for a power value of 0.8. Therefore, it is possible that the sample size of 12 for the current experiment was too small to detect a possible difference between the ot5013 alleles and wildtype.

      (5) It would be helpful to discuss why silencing Ppa-ASE might result in a switch from attractive to repulsive responses to some of the tested gustatory cues.

      For similar assays using Ppa-odr-3p::HisCl1, increasing histamine concentration led to decreasing C.I. for a given odorant (myristate, a P. pacificus-specific attractant). It is likely that the amount of histamine treatment for knockdown to zero (i.e. without a valence change) will differ depending on the attractant.

      (6) The statistical tests used in Figure 3 are not stated.

      Figure 3 used Two-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s post hoc test. We have now added the test in the figure legend.

      (7) It would be helpful to examine the responses of ASER to the full salt panel in the Ppa-gcy-22.3 vs. wild-type backgrounds.

      We understand that future experiments examining neuron responses to the full salt panel for wildtype and gcy-22.3 mutants would provide further information about the salts and specific ions associated with the GCY-22.3 receptor. However, we have tested a broader range of salts (although not yet the full panel) for behavioral assays in wildtype vs gcy-22.3 mutants, which we have included as part of an added Figure 8.

      (8) The controls shown in Figure S1 may not be adequate. Ideally, the same sample size would be used for the control, allowing differences between control worms and experimental worms to be quantified.

      Although we had not conducted an equal number of negative controls using green light without salt stimuli due to resource constraints (6 control vs ~10-19 test), we provided individual recordings with stimuli to show that conditions we interpreted as having responses rarely showed responses resembling the negative controls. Similarly, those we interpreted as having no responses to stimuli mostly resembled the no-stimuli controls (e.g. WT to 25 mM NH<sub>4</sub>Cl, gcy22.3 mutant to 250 mM NH<sub>4</sub>Cl).

      (9) An osmolarity control would be helpful for the calcium imaging experiments.

      We acknowledge that future calcium imaging experiments featuring different salt concentrations could benefit from osmolarity controls.

      (10) In Figure S7, more information about the microfluidic chip design is needed.

      The chip design features a U-shaped worm trap to facilitate loading the worm head-first, with a tapered opening to ensure the worm fits snugly and will not slide too far forward during recording. The outer two chip channels hold buffer solution and can be switched open (ON) or closed (OFF) by the Valvebank. The inner two chip channels hold experimental solutions. The inner channel closer to the worm trap holds the control solution, and the inner channel farther from the worm trap holds the stimulant solution.

      We have added an image of the chip in Figure S7 and further description in the legend.

      (11) Throughout the manuscript, the discussion of the salt stimuli focuses on the salts more than the ions. More discussion of which ions are eliciting responses (both behavioral and neuronal responses) would be helpful.

      In Figure 7, the gcy-22.3 defect resulted in a statistically significant reduction in response only towards NH<sub>4</sub>Cl but not towards NaCl, which suggests ASER is the primary neuron detecting NH<sub>4</sub><sup>+</sup> ions. To extend the description of the gcy-22.3 mutant defects to other ions, we have added a Figure 8: chemotaxis on various salt backgrounds. We found only a mild increase in attraction towards NH<sub>4</sub><sup>+</sup> by both gcy-22.3 mutant alleles, but wild-type in their responses toward Cl<sup>-</sup>, Na<sup>+</sup>, or I<sup>-</sup>. The switch in the direction of change between the behavioral (enhanced) and calcium imaging result (reduced) suggests the behavioral response to ammonium ions likely involves additional receptors and neurons.

      Minor comments:

      (1) The full species name of "C. elegans" should be written out upon first use.

      We have added ‘Caenorhabditis elegans’ to its first mention.

      (2) In the legend of Figure 1, "N2" should not be in italics.

      We have made the correction.

      (3) The "che-1" gene should be in lowercase, even when it is at the start of the sentence.

      We have made the correction.

      (4) Throughout the manuscript, "HisCl" should be "HisCl1."

      We have made these corrections to ‘HisCl1’.

      (5) Figure 3A would benefit from more context, such as the format seen in Figure 7A. It would also help to have more information in the legend (e.g., blue boxes are exons, etc.).

      (6) "Since NH<sub>4</sub>I sensation is affected by silencing of che-1(+) neurons but is unaffected in che-1 mutants, ASE differentiation may be more greatly impacted by the silencing of ASE than by the loss of che-1": I don't think this is exactly what the authors mean. I would say, "ASE function may be more greatly impacted...".

      We have changed ‘differentiation’ to ‘function’ in this passage.

      (7) In Figure 7F-G, the AFD neurons are referred to as AFD in the figure title but AM12 in the graph. This is confusing.

      Thank you for noticing this oversight. We have corrected “AM12” to “AFD”.

      (8) In Figure 7, the legend suggests that comparisons within the same genotype were analyzed. I do not see these comparisons in the figure. In which cases were comparisons within the same genotype made?

      Correct, we performed additional tests between ON and OFF states within the same genotypes (WT and mutant) but did not find significant differences. To avoid unnecessary confusion, we have removed this sentence.

      (9) The nomenclature used for the transgenic animals is unconventional. For example, normally the calcium imaging line would be listed as csuEx93[Ppa-che-1p::optRCaMP] instead of Ppache-1p::optRCaMP(csuEx93).

      We have made these corrections to the nomenclature.

      (10) Figure S6 appears to come out of order. Also, it would be nice to have more of a legend for this figure. The format of the figure could also be improved for clarity.

      We have corrected Figure S6 (now S8) and added more information to the legend.

      (11) Methods section, Chemotaxis assays: "Most assays lasted ~3.5 hours at room temperature in line with the speed of P. pacificus without food..." It's not clear what this means. Does it take the worms 3.5 hours to crawl across the surface of the plate?

      Correct, P. pacificus requires 3-4 hours to crawl across the surface of the plate, which is the standard time for chemotaxis assays for some odors and all salts. We have added this clarification to the Methods.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      It would be helpful for the authors to highlight why their technique (large scale analysis of one emm type) can yield more information than a typical GWAS analysis of invasive vs. non-invasive strains. Are SNPs easier to identify using a large-scale core genome? Is it more likely evolutionarily to find mutations in non-coding regions as opposed to the core genome and accessory genes, and this is what this technique allows? Did the analysis yield unexpected genes or new genes that had not been previously identified in other GWAS analyses? These points may need to be made more apparent in the results and deserve some thought in the discussion section.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the importance of this study. By focusing on bacteria within a single emm type, false positives caused by confounding lineage effects can be minimized, which contributes to greater accuracy of the pan-GWAS. We have added relevant text describing the strong points of our pan-GWAS approach to the Results and Discussion sections, as shown following:

      “The present pan-GWAS of bacteria within a single emm type minimized lineage effects, thus reducing false positives.” (lines 204–205)

      The present study focused on emm89 S. pyogenes, known to cause increasing rates of invasive infections worldwide, and also assessed differences between emm89 strains causing invasive and non-invasive infections. By focusing on bacteria within closed phylogenies, false positives caused by confounding lineage effects were minimized, thus contributing to a higher level of accuracy of the pan-GWAS.” (lines 420–424)

      In addition, we would like to comment more regarding the reviewer’s question, "Is it more likely evolutionarily to find mutations in non-coding regions as opposed to the core genome and accessory genes, and this is what this technique allows?". Mutations are generally considered to be more frequent in non-coding than coding regions. However, the actual mutation frequencies in both types of regions were not assessed in this study. Nevertheless, exploring non-coding regions using the k-mer method is of considerable importance, as variations significantly associated with infectious phenotypes may contribute to alterations in gene expression and other regulatory mechanisms.

      The Alpha-fold data does not demonstrate why the mutations the authors identified could contribute to the invasive phenotype. It would be helpful to show an overlay of the predicted structures containing the different SNPs to demonstrate the potential structural differences that can occur due to the SNP. This would make the data more convincing that the SNP has a potential impact on the function of the protein. Similarly, the authors discuss modification of the hydrophobicity of the side chain in the ferrichrome transporter (lines 317-318) due to a SNP, but this is not immediately obvious in the figure (Fig. 5).

      As the reviewer suggested, we have substituted Figure 5E in the previous version with a figure illustrating the molecular surface within proximity of the mutation. We speculated that the mutation may induce a small indentation on the surface, and thus attenuate the stability of the hydrophobic bound between FhuB and FhuD by invasion of solvent into the indentation. Additionally, images showing the wild-type and mutated models have been separated for better visibility instead of as an overlay of the predicted models suggested by the reviewer. Relevant text in the Results section and legend of Figure 5E have been accordingly revised, as shown following:

      “The mutation was predicted to induce formation of a small indentation on the molecular surface, thus increasing the surface area accessible to the solvent, and is considered to potentially affect the stability of the hydrophobic bond between FhuB and FhuD, and thus ferrichrome transport (Figure 5E).” (lines 360–363)

      “The 73rd valine in FhuB, shown in magenta, was substituted with alanine. The molecular surface is illustrated with a wireframe and that of the predicted indentation is shown with an arrowhead.” (lines 1162–1164)

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the author):

      The figure legend for Fig. 3C needs to be explained so that it is similarly laid out as in Fig. 2C. Fig. 2C should indicate that the magenta color represents the invasive phenotype.

      Based on this helpful suggestion, more detailed information about the magenta color representing the invasive phenotype has been added to the legends of Fig. 2C and 3C, with relevant text also included in the revised legends, as shown following:

      “Colored bars above indicate countries and phenotypes, and magenta bars represent invasive phenotypes. Using the Roary program, gene names starting with “Group_” were automatically assigned. Position indicates the location of each SNP/indel on the core gene alignment. The full results are shown in Table S6.” (lines 1116–1120)

      “Colored bars above indicate countries and phenotypes, and magenta bars represent invasive phenotypes. Using the Roary program, gene names starting with “Group_” were automatically assigned. The full results are shown in Table S8. (lines 1130–1133)

      The wording and organization of results in the k-mer section started to get confusing around lines 270-271. It begins to be a list of results and would be better served by some interpretation or explanation of the significance (why it is important to find such mutations). For example, for mutations you find in non-coding regions, do you expect them to have a detrimental effects on gene expression/regulation?

      As the reviewer kindly suggested, we have added interpretation or explanation of the significance of Comp_6 and Comp_24 to the Results section. We analyzed the function of the non-coding region of Comp_6 by employing web-based in silico tools, including MLDSPP and BacPP, though no promoter sequences could be identified. Next, using BLAST, a search for known promoter sequences of S. pyogenes M1 strain SF370 of the CDBProm database was attempted, because the web-based in silico promoter prediction tools are not suitable for S. pyogenes. However, neither identical nor homologous sequences were detected. Thus, the significance of this region remains unknown. In Comp_24, group_141 was also identified in the COGs-based pan-GWAS as a non-invasiveness related gene. Furthermore, group_141 showed high levels of correlation with group_139 and group_467, encoding transposase and uncharacterized protein, respectively, which suggests that the presence of an MGE is associated with a non-invasive phenotype.

      Relevant text has been added to the Materials and Methods (lines 653–657) and Results (lines 308–311 and 314–319) sections, as shown following:

      “Promoter sequences in intergenic regions were predicted using web-based tools, MLDSPP and BacPP[29,30]. Additionally, BLAST was employed to search the promoter sequences of S. pyogenes strain SF370 registered in the CDBProm database (https://aw.iimas.unam.mx/cdbprom/)[69]” (lines 653–657)

      “We speculated that this region is related to regulation of gene expression. However, no promoter sequences were identified by utilizing MLDSPP, BacPP, and BLAST, thus the significance of this region remains to be clarified[29,30].” (lines 308–311)

      “Furthermore, group_141 was also identified in the COGs-based pan-GWAS as a non-invasiveness-related gene along with group_139 and group_467, which encode transposase and uncharacterized protein, respectively (Table S8 and Figure S4). Taken together, the absence of an MGE containing group_141, and the presence of another MGE harboring group_142 and group_143 may result in an invasive phenotype.” (lines 314–319)

      Additionally, new references (#29, 30, and 69) concerning bacterial promoter prediction have been included in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Because there is no difference in intracellular free ferric ions in the fhuB mutant compared with the wild-type, the authors speculate that the upregulation of the fhuBCD operon can compensate for the loss of function of the fhuB gene, but there is insufficient data to support this claim.

      As the reviewer indicate, the data presented in the previous version were insufficient to support our speculation. Therefore, the following sentence has been deleted from the manuscript (previous version line 367):

      “Therefore, the upregulation of fhuBCD may compensate for the impaired function mediated by SNP T218C.”

      The authors mention that there was no direct association between invasiveness and acquisition of genes (lines 451-455), including antibiotic resistance genes from prophages and MGEs (lines 467-469). These data should be moved to the results section to focus the results on the correlation between invasiveness and mutation of existing DNA vs acquisition of new DNA.

      Accordingly, we have added relevant text to the Results section, as shown following:

      “On the other hand, the present pan-GWAS found no genes encoding known virulence factors significantly associated with invasiveness, thus further analysis of the relationships of detected distribution patterns with prophages and MGEs was performed.” (lines 264–267)

      Minor spelling error at line 210 ("waws" instead of "was").

      As the reviewer kindly pointed out, the spelling has been corrected. (line 233)

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor comments:

      Line 55: Does this rate apply to all types of infections?

      The authors appreciate this question from the reviewer. We checked what types of infections the mortality rate is applied to and confirmed that it only represents STSS. Therefore, relevant text has been revised, as shown following:

      However, even with proper treatment, the mortality rate of patients with STSS remains high, ranging from 23–81%[6]”. (lines 72–73)

      Line 58: Could you explain the protein encoded by the emm gene and the role of the hypervariable region in pathogenesis?

      As requested, relevant text regarding the pathogenic role of the hypervariable region of M protein has been added, as shown following:

      S. pyogenes has been classified into at least 240 emm types based on a hypervariable region sequence of the emm gene, which encodes the M protein. This hypervariable region of the M protein is responsible for type-specific antigenicity and binds with high affinity to C4b-binding protein, a major fluid phase inhibitor of the classical and lectin pathways of the complement system that confers resistance to opsonophagocytosis[8].” (lines 76–81)

      Line 161: Figure 1C does not show the strain with the different pattern.

      The authors apologize for the lack of clarity. In Fig. 1C, the strain is shown by a pale pink color bar used to indicate the related clade. For clarity, an arrowhead pointing to the strain from outside of the tree has been added along with the following text in the legend:

      “Arrowhead indicates strain belonging to the novel clade.” (lines 1102–1103)

      Line 239: It could be interesting to examine the genes in the region between the mobile elements found in the global cohort, as the result profile was very different from the Japanese group, which revealed more specific genes. Consider adding this to the results section.

      Based on the reviewer’s insightful suggestion, we attempted to find regions between the mobile genetic element-related genes. However, contigs generated from short reads were not adequate to identify such a genome structure. Therefore, calculations to analyze the pairwise correlation of the presence of significant COGs in the 666 strains to predict genes on prophages and MGEs were performed, and the results added to Figure S4. Eight clusters were detected as coexisting COG groups, seven of which comprised phage- or MGE-related genes. Furthermore, a cluster with antimicrobial-resistant genes was shown to be correlated with non-invasive infections. It is thus speculated that gain or loss of gene sets via phages and MGEs rather than acquisition of virulence genes may lead to changes in fitness to the environment and bacterial phenotypes. Relevant text has been added to the revised versions of the Results, Discussion, and Materials and Methods sections, as shown following:

      “On the other hand, the present pan-GWAS found no genes encoding known virulence factors significantly associated with invasiveness, thus further analysis of the relationships of detected distribution patterns with prophages and MGEs was performed. For calculating the pairwise correlation of the presence of significant COGs in the 666 strains, the COGs were clustered into eight coexisting groups, seven of which contained phage- and/or MGEs-related genes (Figure S4). The largest group comprised 65 genes including phage proteins, while the second largest with 42 genes was found to be associated with non-invasive infections, and included group_2689, group_1833, and ermA1, encoding TetR/AcrR family transcriptional regulator, multidrug efflux system permease protein, and rRNA adenine N-6-methyltransferase, respectively.” (lines 264–273)

      “On the other hand, a cluster comprising 49 non-invasiveness-associated genes including antibiotic-resistance genes was identified. Furthermore, among the genes showing a significant correlation with the infectious phenotype, approximately 90% (152 of 169) were associated with non-invasiveness. One possible explanation is that significantly related genes reflect the process of not only gain of factors but also loss of those affecting fitness cost.” (lines 517–522)

      “The correlation of the presence of significant COGs was calculated and visualized using the R program.” (lines 643–644)

      Line 548: What cutoff values were used in Fastp?

      The default cutoff value for Fastp (Q>15) was used, and relevant text has been added to the Materials and Methods section in the revised version, as shown following:

      “All collected sequences were subjected to quality checks using Fastp v.0.20.1, with a default cutoff value of Q>15[53].” (lines 600–601)

      Line 635: Were the transcriptome experiments performed in triplicate?

      We apologize for the confusion. The transcriptome experiment was performed only once with three samples for each condition. The notation “(n=3 for each condition)” has been added to the relevant text portion in the Materials and Methods section (line 696).

      Discussion section: I believe the authors should place more emphasis on the fact that FhuB is associated with non-invasiveness, to provide clearer context in the discussion.

      Based on this helpful suggestion, we have revised relevant text in the Discussion section, as shown following:

      “Transcriptomic analysis findings suggested that the Japan-specific fhuB mutation associated with non-severe invasive infections contributes to the growth rate of S. pyogenes in human blood by adapting to the environment.” (lines 457–459)

      Also, “V73A” has been removed from the relevant text in the Discussion section to provide a more clear and precise context, with the revised sentence shown following:

      “Two possible roles of the FhuB mutation in the pathogenesis of severe invasive infections are thus proposed.” (lines 470–471)

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      We would like to respond to just one remaining concern from Reviewer 1 and Reviewer 2 regarding a potential overfitting in Test Set 1, which involves combinations already present in the training set. DIPx’s (and TAIJI’s) performance in Test Set 1 is better than in Test Set 2, which involves combinations not present in the training set. Let’s consider two general points to highlight why the improved performance is not the result of overfitting. 

      (1) Suppose we are testing the e ect of one drug D; the training may involve, for example, selecting an optimal dose. A validated e ect of D in an independent test set is not an overfit, even though we are using the same drug in the training and the test set. Testing one drug is an extreme case, but the same idea holds for any number of drugs. What matters is the independence of the test set. 

      (2) A prediction model P1 will legitimately perform better than model P2, if P1 uses better or more informative features than P2. The features could be those used directly in the model, but they could also be other observable characteristics not directly used in the model, such as optimal subregions of the feature space. DPIx or TAIJI results indicate that the identity of previously trained combinations is one such informative feature. The set of previously trained combinations corresponds to a subregion of the feature space. DIPx’s prediction performance for known combinations would be expected to follow the results from Test Set 1; we cannot expect that if there is an overfitting issue. Finally, we note that Test Set 1 was established and used in the AstraZeneca Dream Challenge for rigorously testing the prediction of known combinations.

    1. Author response:

      We appreciate the constructive and thoughtful reviews provided by the reviewers and editorial team. We thank you for the opportunity to submit a provisional response and are grateful for the detailed and critical feedback that will strengthen our work. Below, we provide a summary of our planned revisions in response to the public reviews from Reviewer #1 and Reviewer #2.

      Reviewer #1 – Public Review Response Plan

      (1) Sample Overlap (MR Bias):

      We plan to replace several non-overlapping GWAS data sources to validate the association between aneurysms and atherosclerosis, thereby eliminating bias and Type I errors caused by sample overlap.

      (2) Multivariable MR (MVMR):<br /> We will attempt to incorporate known confounding factors (e.g., hypertension, smoking, diabetes) within the multivariable MR framework to verify the robustness of our results.

      (3) Clarifications and Presentation:

      - We will correct eTable citations.

      - Distinguish correctly between "incidence" and "prevalence".

      - Reorganize results to consistently present primary analyses first (IVW), followed by sensitivity results.

      - Expand the methods section to fully reflect all analyses.

      Reviewer #2 – Public Review Response Plan

      (1) Justification of MMP Selection:<br /> We will provide a detailed rationale for the inclusion of the 12 MMPs, based on prior literature and biological relevance.

      (2) Multiple Testing Clarification:<br /> We will clarify the Bonferroni correction strategy, explicitly accounting for all tests (e.g., 72 comparisons × multiple MR methods).

      (3) Instrument Selection Threshold:

      - We agree with the reviewer and will revise the SNP selection strategy, starting from p < 5×10⁻⁸ and only relaxing thresholds when fewer than 3 instruments are found.

      - Clarify the reasons why we do not use LD proxies.

      (4) Pleiotropy and Heterogeneity Tests:

      - We will add Egger's intercept results alongside MR-PRESSO.

      - Specify the R packages used (e.g., TwoSampleMR).

      - To prevent cluttered data presentation, we have included both heterogeneity and pleiotropy p-values in the supplementary tables.

      - Supplement forest plots showing outlier exclusion effects.

      (5) Clarifications in Figures and Tables:

      - Fix the duplicated “simple mode” entry in Figure 2.

      - Correct inconsistencies in p-values between figures and text.

      - Improve figure legends (e.g., color bar labels, panel identifiers).

      - Revise Table 4 title for clarity.

      - Remove the term "causal" where associations are nominal (e.g., p ~ 0.05).

    1. Author response:

      We would like to express our sincere gratitude to the editor and reviewers for their thoughtful comments and suggestions on our manuscript. Below is our interim response to the reviewers’ public review:

      Reviewer 1:

      (1) We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment on the consideration of RAS mutation type and lesion metastasis site in our study. We will undertake a more comprehensive review of the literature and conduct a detailed analysis to assess how these factors influence treatment efficacy in our cohort.

      (2) Regarding the radiotherapy planning process, we will provide further clarification in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we select the target lesion using CT imaging and delineate it by marking the 50% isodose line to define the planning target volume (PTV). In assessing treatment efficacy, we differentiate between target lesions (within the PTV) and off-target lesions (outside the PTV). We will update the figures to include the isodose line display for better clarity.

      (3 & 4) We acknowledge the limitations of our study, particularly with respect to the sample size, which may hinder the statistical power required for a comprehensive analysis of treatment effect markers and subgroup variations. Nonetheless, we will continue to refine our analyses in the revised manuscript to provide additional insights and strengthen the conclusions where possible.

      (5) During the early stages of our research, our team conducted a series of investigations into the impact of tumor fibrosis and angiogenesis on treatment outcomes. We have accumulated a substantial body of data, and we will summarize these findings in the revised manuscript to provide further context and support for our current study.

      Reviewer 2:

      (1, 4 & 5) We greatly appreciate the reviewer’s careful reading of the manuscript. We will revise the abstract, methods, and results sections to improve clarity and precision. Additionally, we will refine the overall wording of the manuscript to enhance its scientific rigor and professionalism.

      (2) We also appreciate the reviewer’s suggestions regarding the methods and results. These will be incorporated into the revised manuscript, with additional detail in the methods section to clarify our experimental approach and strengthen the discussion of our findings.

      (3) This is an intriguing point raised by the reviewer. We agree that the upregulation of PD-L1 expression following SBRT treatment could potentially enhance the efficacy of subsequent immunotherapy. To explore this further, we will conduct a detailed literature review and provide a more in-depth analysis of our data to elucidate the underlying mechanisms.

      We trust that the clarifications provided above partially address the reviewers' concerns. We are committed to fully resolving the raised issues through more comprehensive revisions in the subsequent manuscript update.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Ning et al. reported that Bcas2 played an indispensable role in zebrafish primitive hematopoiesis via sequestering β-catenin in the nucleus. The authors showed that loss of Bcas2 caused primitive hematopoietic defects in zebrafish. They unraveled that Bcas2 deficiency promoted β-catenin nuclear export via a CRM1-dependent manner in vivo and in vitro. They further validated that BCAS2 directly interacted with β-catenin in the nucleus and enhanced β-catenin accumulation through its CC domains. They unveil a novel insight into Bcas2, which is critical for zebrafish primitive hematopoiesis via regulating nuclear β-catenin stabilization rather than its canonical pre-mRNA splicing functions. Overall, the study is impressive and well-performed, although there are also some issues to address.

      Strengths:

      The study unveils a novel function of Bcas2, which is critical for zebrafish primitive hematopoiesis by sequestering β-catenin. The authors validated the results in vivo and in vitro. Most of the figures are clear and convincing. This study nicely complements the function of Bcas2 in primitive hematopoiesis.

      Weaknesses:

      A portion of the figures were over-exposed.

      Thank you for the time reviewing our manuscript. We agree with your suggestion and the exposure of Figure 5C and Figure 7E has been reduced. We hope that the revisions will meet your expectation.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Ning and colleagues present studies supporting a role for breast carcinoma amplified sequence 2 (Bcas2) in positively regulating primitive wave hematopoiesis through amplification of beta-catenin-dependent (canonical) Wnt signaling. The authors present compelling evidence that zebrafish bcas2 is expressed at the right time and place to be involved in primitive hematopoiesis, that there are primitive hematopoietic defects in hetero- and homozygous mutant and knockdown embryos, that Bcas2 mechanistically positively regulates canonical Wnt signaling, and that Bcas2 is required for nuclear retention of B-cat through physical interaction involving armadillo repeats 9-12 of B-cat and the coiled-coil domains of Bcas2. Overall, the data and writing are clean, clear, and compelling. This study is a first-rate analysis of a strong phenotype with highly supportive mechanistic data. The findings shed light on the controversial question of whether, when, and how canonical Wnt signaling may be involved in hematopoietic development. We detail some minor concerns and questions below, which if answered, we believe would strengthen the overall story and resolve some puzzling features of the phenotype. Notwithstanding these minor concerns, we believe this is an exceptionally well-executed and interesting manuscript that is likely suitable for publication with minor additional experimental detail and commentary.

      Strengths:

      (1) The study features clear and compelling phenotypes and results.

      (2) The manuscript narrative exposition and writing are clear and compelling.

      (3) The authors have attended to important technical nuances sometimes overlooked, for example, focusing on different pools of cytosolic or nuclear b-catenin.

      (4) The study sheds light on a controversial subject: regulation of hematopoietic development by canonical Wnt signaling and presents clear evidence of a role.

      (5) The authors present evidence of phylogenetic conservation of the pathway.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The authors present compelling data that Bcas2 regulates nuclear retention of B-cat through physical association involving binding between the Bcas2 CC domains and B-cat arm repeats 9-12. Transcriptional activation of Wnt target genes by B-cat requires physical association between B-cat and Tcf/Lef family DNA binding factors involving key interactions in Arm repeats 2-9 (Graham et al., Cell 2000). Mutually exclusive binding by B-cat regulatory factors, such as ICAT that prevent Tcf-binding is a documented mechanism (e.g. Graham et al., Mol Cell 2002). It would appear - based on the arm repeat usage by Bcas2 (repeats 9-12)-that Bcas2 and Tcf binding might not be mutually exclusive, which would support their model that Bcas2 physical association with B-cat to retain it in the nucleus would be compatible with co-activation of genes by allowing association with Tcf. It might be nice to attempt a three-way co-IP of these factors showing that B-cat can still bind Tcf in the presence of Bcas2, or at least speculate on the plausibility of the three-way interaction.

      We appreciate your assessment and generous comments for the manuscript. As you mentioned, the binding sites for TCF on β-catenin almost do not overlap with those for BCAS2. It is likely that BCAS2-mediated nuclear sequestration of β-catenin would be compatible with the initiation of gene transcription by allowing TCF to associate with β-catenin. To test this possibility, we have taken your suggestion and performed co-IP assays. The results showed that β-catenin still bound with TCF4 in the presence of BCAS2 (Supplemental Figure 12), confirming that the binding of BCAS2 to β-catenin would not interfere with the formation of β-catenin/TCF complex.

      (2) A major way that canonical Wnt signaling regulates hematopoietic development is through regulation of the LPM hematopoietic competence territories by activating expression of cdx1a, cdx4, and their downstream targets hoxb5a and hoxa9a (Davidson et al., Nature 2003; Davidson et al., Dev Biol 2006; Pilon et al., Dev Biol 2006; Wang et al., PNAS 2008). Could the authors assess (in situ) the expression of cdx1a, cdx4, hoxb5a, and hoxa9a in the bcas2 mutants?

      We agree with your suggestion and have examined the expression of cdx4 and hoxa9a by performing WISH. Diminished expression of cdx4 and hoxa9a was detected in the lateral plate mesoderm of bcas2<sup>+/-</sup> embryos at the 6-somite stage (Supplemental Figure 7).

      (3) The authors show compellingly that even heterozygous loss of bcas2 has strong Wnt-inhibitory effects. If Bcas2 is required for canonical Wnt signaling and bcas2 is expressed ubiquitously from the 1-cell stage through at least the beginning of gastrulation, why do bcas2 KO embryos not have morphological axis specification defects consistent with loss of early Wnt signaling, like loss of head (early), or brain anteriorization (later)? Could the authors provide some comments on this puzzle? Or if they do see any canonical Wnt signaling patterning defects in het- or homozygous embryos, could they describe and/or present them?

      You have raised an interesting question. In fact, we did not observe ventralization or axis determination defects in the early embryos of bcas2<sup>+/-</sup> mutants. Even in the very small number of homozygous mutant embryos, we did not find such morphological defects. Given that the homozygous and heterozygous mutant embryos were derived from crossing bcas2<sup>+/-</sup> males with bcas2<sup>+/-</sup> females, maternal Bcas2 might still remain and function in these embryos during gastrulation when axis determination and neural patterning took place. Accordingly, we have expanded our discussion to incorporate these insights (Line 565-572).

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript utilized zebrafish bcas2 mutants to study the role of bcas2 in primitive hematopoiesis and further confirms that it has a similar function in mice. Moreover, they showed that bcas2 regulates the transition of hematopoietic differentiation from angioblasts via activating Wnt signaling. By performing a series of biochemical experiments, they also showed that bcas2 accomplishes this by sequestering b-catenin within the nucleus, rather than through its known function in pre-mRNA splicing.

      Strengths:

      The work is well-performed, and the manuscript is well-written.

      Weaknesses:

      Several issues need to be clarified.

      (1) Is wnt signaling also required during hematopoietic differentiation from angioblasts? Can the authors test angioblast and endothelial markers in embryos with wnt inhibition? Also, can the authors add export inhibitor LMB to the mouse mutants to test if sequestering of b-catenin by bcas2 is conserved during primitive hematopoiesis in mice?

      Thank you very much for your appreciation and detailed assessment. To test whether Wnt signaling is also required during hematopoietic differentiation from angioblasts, wild-type embryos were exposed to 10 µM CCT036477, a small molecule β-catenin antagonist, from 9 hpf and then collected for WISH experiments. As shown in Supplemental Figure 8, the expression of hemangioblast markers npas4l, scl, and gata2 and endothelial marker fli1a remained unchanged, but the expression of erythroid progenitor marker gata1 was significantly reduced. These results suggest that canonical Wnt pathway may not be required for the generation of hemangioblasts or their endothelial differentiation, but is pivotal for their hematopoietic differentiation.

      It is quite difficult to validate the conserve role of BCAS2 during primitive hematopoiesis in mice, because the toxicity of LMB may cause severe adverse effects in mice.[1,2]

      (2) Bcas2 is required for primitive myelopoiesis in ALM. Does bcas2 play a similar function in primitive myelopoiesis, or is bcas2/b-catenin interaction more important for hematopoietic differentiation in PLM?

      You have raised an important question. In our study, we have demonstrated that the expression of myeloid progenitor marker pu.1 was significantly decreased in bcas2 mutants, hinting that Bcas2 is pivotal for primitive myelopoiesis. To further clarify the function of Bcas2 in primitive myelopoiesis, we injected 8 ng of bcas2 morpholino into Tg(coro1a:GFP) embryos at the 1-cell stage and examined β-catenin distribution at 17 hpf via immunostaining. We observed a significant decline of nuclear β-catenin in primitive myeloid cells (Supplemental Figure 9), indicating that Bcas2 is highly likely to play a similar role in sequestering β-catenin within the nucleus during primitive myelopoiesis.

      (3) Is it possible that CC1-2 fragment sequester b-catenin? The different phenotypes between this manuscript and the previous article (Yu, 2019) may be due to different mutations in bcas2. Is it possible that the bcas2 mutation in Yu's article produces a complete CC1-2 fragment, which might sequester b-catenin?

      This is an interesting perspective. To test the possibility that CC1-2 sequesters β-catenin, mRNA expressing the CC domains of BCAS2 has been co-injected with bcas2 morpholino into Tg(gata1:GFP) embryo at the one-cell stage. Increased nuclear β-catenin levels were detected in the GFP-positive hematopoietic progenitor cells at 16 hpf (Supplemental Figure 11). Our findings support that CC1-2 fragment of BCAS2 can sequester β-catenin within the nucleus.

      In the previous article (Yu, 2019), a deletion 5 bases mutation in the third exon of BCAS2 was produced by TALEN, therefore the CC domains of this mutant should be affected. It is difficult to conclude that the mutant BCAS2 protein in Yu’s study still remains association with β-catenin.

      (4) Can the author clarify what embryos the arrows point to in SI Figure 2D? In SI Figure 6B and B', can the author clarify how the nucleus and cytoplasm are bleached? In B, the nucleus also appears to be bleached.

      Thank you for your query and suggestion. In our revisions, the corresponding clarifications have been supplemented (Line 239-242; Line 978-979).

      We acknowledge that the nuclei in both the BCAS2 overexpression group and control group were slightly bleached. Given that we have performed real-time analysis for fluorescent recovery after photobleaching, and we have observed a much slower recovery of cytoplasmic fluorescence in BCAS2 overexpressed cells, the conclusion that BCAS2 inhibits the nuclear export of β-catenin but not its nuclear import, remains changed.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Major concerns:

      (1) In this study, the authors detected β-catenin distribution in erythrocytes (gata1-GFP+ cells). Estimating the β-catenin distribution in the myeloid cells is recommended.

      Thank you for your assessment and we have taken your suggestion. Tg(coro1a:GFP) embryos, which is commonly used to track both macrophages and neutrophils,[3] were injected with 8 ng of bcas2 morpholino into at the 1-cell stage and collected for immunostaining to examine the β-catenin distribution at 17 hpf. We observed a significant decline of nuclear β-catenin in primitive myeloid cells (Supplemental Figure 9). This result indicates that Bcas2 is highly likely to play a similar role in sequestering β-catenin within the nucleus during primitive myelopoiesis.

      (2) The reduced nuclear localization of β-catenin in Figure 3H required further evidence. It would be helpful if the authors quantified the fluorescence intensity in the cell nucleus and cytoplasm. Meanwhile, the figures (Figure 5C, Figure 7E) were over-exposed. Please validate these figures.

      Thank you for your suggestions. We agree with you that the fluorescence intensity of β-catenin in the nucleus and cytoplasm should be quantified. However, as the nucleus comprises a large part of the cell, we believe it would be more appropriate to quantify the relative fluorescence intensity by dividing the fluorescence intensity of nuclear β-catenin by the fluorescence intensity of DAPI.

      Such quantifications have been added for Figure 3G, 5C, 7E, S9A, and S13A. In addition, we have reduced the exposure of Figure 5C and Figure 7E. We hope that you will be satisfied with the revisions.

      (3) The authors used cKO mice to validate that the erythrocytes were eliminated. It would be interesting to detect β-catenin distribution by immunofluorescent staining in primitive hematopoietic cells in cKO mice. Addressing this issue can provide further evidence to support the conservation of Bcas2.

      We appreciate your suggestion. However, we found that red blood cells were almost eliminated in the yolk sac of Bcas2<sup>F/F</sup>;Flk1-Cre mice at E12.5. It is difficult to further detect β-catenin distribution in primitive erythroid cells in these mice.

      (4) The authors discovered that Bcas2 mediated β-catenin nuclear export in a CRM1-dependent manner. CRM1 is a key regulator involved in the majority of factors of nuclear export via recognizing specific nuclear export signals (NES). Validating the NES of Bcas2 is recommended. Furthermore, I wonder about the relationship between Bcas2 and CRM1 in regulating β-catenin nuclear export. One possibility is that Bcas2 covers the NES to inhibit the interaction between CRM1 and β-catenin, thus leading to β-catenin accumulation in the cell nucleus. The authors should discuss this possibility accordingly.

      Thank you for providing an interesting perspective. CRM1-mediated nuclear export of β-catenin usually requires CRM1 recognition and binding with the NES sequences in chaperon proteins, such as APC, Axin and Chibby.[4-6] Moreover, CRM1 can bind directly to and function as an efficient nuclear exporter for β-catenin.[7] Since BCAS2 has not been reported to contain any recognizable NES sequences, it will be interesting to investigate whether BCAS2 competitively inhibits β-catenin from associating with CRM1, or with the chaperone proteins. We have rewritten the discussion on CRM1-dependent nuclear export of β-catenin in line with your comments (Line 572-578).

      (5) It would be interesting if the authors could answer the specificity in Bcas2-mediated protein nuclear export pathway. The authors should detect other classical factors (CRM1 mediated) distribution when loss of Bcas2.

      Thank you for bringing up this point. To test whether BCAS2 specifically regulates CRM1-mediated nuclear export of β-catenin, we have investigated the nucleocytoplasmic distribution of other known CRM1 cargoes, such as ATG3 and CDC37L.[8] BCAS2 overexpression in HeLa cells slightly enhanced the nuclear localization of CDC37L, and had no significant impact on that of ATG3 (Supplemental Figure 11), indicating the specificity of BCAS2 in the regulation of CRM1-dependent nuclear export of β-catenin.

      Minor concerns:

      (1) The name "bcas2Δ7+/- and bcas2Δ14+/-" should be changed into "bcas2+/Δ7 and bcas2+/Δ14"(+/Δ7 or +/Δ14 should be superior on the right).

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have changed the names of the mutants throughout the manuscript.

      (2) The scale bar position in the figures should be unified.

      We agree with your suggestion and have unified the scale bar position in all figures.

      (3) In Figure 4E, "Nuclear" should be changed into "Nucleus".

      We apologize for the mistake and Figure 4E has been revised.

      (4) There are some unaesthetic issues in the figures. The figures need to be further edited. Figure 3H "β-catenin and Merge", Figure 4D "Merge". All these words should be centered in the figures.

      Thank you. We have edited all the figures to ensure that the text is centered.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) It would be nice to have whole blot images for the Westerns in Supplementary Info.

      Thank you for your suggestion. Whole images for immunoblotting have been supplemented as Source data.

      (2) Line 292 change 5 hpf to 5 dpf.

      (3) Line 301 change "primary" to "primitive"?

      We apologize for the mistakes. We have incorporated these suggestions in the revised manuscript and reexamined spelling throughout the paper.

      (4) Figure S2C: is "Maker" a typographical error? Change to "ladder"?

      We apologize for this typographical error and we have revised it in Figure S2C.

      Reference

      (1) Ishizawa J, Kojima K, Hail N, Tabe Y, Andreeff M. Expression, function, and targeting of the nuclear exporter chromosome region maintenance 1 (CRM1) protein. Pharmacology & Therapeutics. 2015;153:25-35.

      (2) Li X, Feng Y, Yan MF, et al. Inhibition of Autism-Related Crm1 Disrupts Mitosis and Induces Apoptosis of the Cortical Neural Progenitors. Cerebral Cortex. 2020;30(7):3960-3976.

      (3) Li L, Yan B, Shi YQ, Zhang WQ, Wen ZL. Live Imaging Reveals Differing Roles of Macrophages and Neutrophils during Zebrafish Tail Fin Regeneration. Journal of Biological Chemistry. 2012;287(30):25353-25360.

      (4) Neufeld KL, Nix DA, Bogerd H, et al. Adenomatous polyposis coli protein contains two nuclear export signals and shuttles between the nucleus and cytoplasm. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2000;97(22):12085-12090.

      (5) Li FQ, Mofunanya A, Harris K, Takemaru KI. Chibby cooperates with 14-3-3 to regulate β-catenin subcellular distribution and signaling activity. Journal of Cell Biology. 2008;181(7):1141-1154.

      (6) Cong F, Varmus H. Nuclear-cytoplasmic shuttling of Axin regulates subcellular localization of β-catenin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2004;101(9):2882-2887.

      (7) Ki H, Oh M, Chung SW, Kim K. β-Catenin can bind directly to CRM1 independently of adenomatous polyposis coli, which affects its nuclear localization and LEF-1/β-catenin-dependent gene expression. Cell Biology International. 2008;32(4):394-400.

      (8) Kirli K, Karaca S, Dehne HJ, et al. A deep proteomics perspective on CRM1-mediated nuclear export and nucleocytoplasmic partitioning. Elife. 2015;4.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Using a cross-modal sensory selection task in head-fixed mice, the authors attempted to characterize how different rules reconfigured representations of sensory stimuli and behavioral reports in sensory (S1, S2) and premotor cortical areas (medial motor cortex or MM, and ALM). They used silicon probe recordings during behavior, a combination of single-cell and population-level analyses of neural data, and optogenetic inhibition during the task.

      Strengths:

      A major strength of the manuscript was the clarity of the writing and motivation for experiments and analyses. The behavioral paradigm is somewhat simple but well-designed and wellcontrolled. The neural analyses were sophisticated, clearly presented, and generally supported the authors' interpretations. The statistics are clearly reported and easy to interpret. In general, my view is that the authors achieved their aims. They found that different rules affected preparatory activity in premotor areas, but not sensory areas, consistent with dynamical systems perspectives in the field that hold that initial conditions are important for determining trial-based dynamics.

      Weaknesses:

      The manuscript was generally strong. The main weakness in my view was in interpreting the optogenetic results. While the simplicity of the task was helpful for analyzing the neural data, I think it limited the informativeness of the perturbation experiments. The behavioral read-out was low dimensional -a change in hit rate or false alarm rate- but it was unclear what perceptual or cognitive process was disrupted that led to changes in these read-outs. This is a challenge for the field, and not just this paper, but was the main weakness in my view. I have some minor technical comments in the recommendations for authors that might address other minor weaknesses.

      I think this is a well-performed, well-written, and interesting study that shows differences in rule representations in sensory and premotor areas and finds that rules reconfigure preparatory activity in the motor cortex to support flexible behavior.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Chang et al. investigate neuronal activity firing patterns across various cortical regions in an interesting context-dependent tactile vs visual detection task, developed previously by the authors (Chevee et al., 2021; doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.11.013). The authors report the important involvement of a medial frontal cortical region (MM, probably a similar location to wM2 as described in Esmaeili et al., 2021 & 2022; doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.05.005; doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001667) in mice for determining task rules.

      Strengths:

      The experiments appear to have been well carried out and the data well analysed. The manuscript clearly describes the motivation for the analyses and reaches clear and well-justified conclusions. I find the manuscript interesting and exciting!

      Weaknesses:

      I did not find any major weaknesses.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      This study examines context-dependent stimulus selection by recording neural activity from several sensory and motor cortical areas along a sensorimotor pathway, including S1, S2, MM, and ALM. Mice are trained to either withhold licking or perform directional licking in response to visual or tactile stimulus. Depending on the task rule, the mice have to respond to one stimulus modality while ignoring the other. Neural activity to the same tactile stimulus is modulated by task in all the areas recorded, with significant activity changes in a subset of neurons and population activity occupying distinct activity subspaces. Recordings further reveal a contextual signal in the pre-stimulus baseline activity that differentiates task context. This signal is correlated with subsequent task modulation of stimulus activity. Comparison across brain areas shows that this contextual signal is stronger in frontal cortical regions than in sensory regions. Analyses link this signal to behavior by showing that it tracks the behavioral performance switch during task rule transitions. Silencing activity in frontal cortical regions during the baseline period impairs behavioral performance.

      Overall, this is a superb study with solid results and thorough controls. The results are relevant for context-specific neural computation and provide a neural substrate that will surely inspire follow-up mechanistic investigations. We only have a couple of suggestions to help the authors further improve the paper.

      (1) We have a comment regarding the calculation of the choice CD in Fig S3. The text on page 7 concludes that "Choice coding dimensions change with task rule". However, the motor choice response is different across blocks, i.e. lick right vs. no lick for one task and lick left vs. no lick for the other task. Therefore, the differences in the choice CD may be simply due to the motor response being different across the tasks and not due to the task rule per se. The authors may consider adding this caveat in their interpretation. This should not affect their main conclusion.

      We thank the Reviewer for the suggestion. We have discussed this caveat and performed a new analysis to calculate the choice coding dimensions using right-lick and left-lick trials (Fig. S3h) on page 8. 

      “Choice coding dimensions were obtained from left-lick and no-lick trials in respond-to-touch blocks and right-lick and no-lick trials in respond-to-light blocks. Because the required lick directions differed between the block types, the difference in choice CDs across task rules (Fig. S4f) could have been affected by the different motor responses. To rule out this possibility, we did a new version of this analysis using right-lick and left-lick trials to calculate the choice coding dimensions for both task rules. We found that the orientation of the choice coding dimension in a respond-to-touch block was still not aligned well with that in a respond-to-light block (Fig. S4h;  magnitude of dot product between the respond-to-touch choice CD and the respond-to-light choice CD, mean ± 95% CI for true vs shuffled data: S1: 0.39 ± [0.23, 0.55] vs 0.2 ± [0.1, 0.31], 10 sessions; S2: 0.32 ± [0.18, 0.46] vs 0.2 ± [0.11, 0.3], 8 sessions; MM: 0.35 ± [0.21, 0.48] vs 0.18 ± [0.11, 0.26], 9 sessions; ALM: 0.28 ± [0.17, 0.39] vs 0.21 ± [0.12, 0.31], 13 sessions).”

      We also have included the caveats for using right-lick and left-lick trials to calculate choice coding dimensions on page 13.

      “However, we also calculated choice coding dimensions using only right- and left-lick trials. In S1, S2, MM and ALM, the choice CDs calculated this way were also not aligned well across task rules (Fig. S4h), consistent with the results calculated from lick and no-lick trials (Fig. S4f). Data were limited for this analysis, however, because mice rarely licked to the unrewarded water port (# of licksunrewarded port  / # of lickstotal , respond-to-touch: 0.13, respond-to-light: 0.11). These trials usually came from rule transitions (Fig. 5a) and, in some cases, were potentially caused by exploratory behaviors. These factors could affect choice CDs.”

      (2) We have a couple of questions about the effect size on single neurons vs. population dynamics. From Fig 1, about 20% of neurons in frontal cortical regions show task rule modulation in their stimulus activity. This seems like a small effect in terms of population dynamics. There is somewhat of a disconnect from Figs 4 and S3 (for stimulus CD), which show remarkably low subspace overlap in population activity across tasks. Can the authors help bridge this disconnect? Is this because the neurons showing a difference in Fig 1 are disproportionally stimulus selective neurons?

      We thank the Reviewer for the insightful comment and agree that it is important to link the single-unit and population results. We have addressed these questions by (1) improving our analysis of task modulation of single neurons  (tHit-tCR selectivity) and (2) examining the relationship between tHit-tCR selective neurons and tHit-tCR subspace overlaps.  

      Previously, we averaged the AUC values of time bins within the stimulus window (0-150 ms, 10 ms bins). If the 95% CI on this averaged AUC value did not include 0.5, this unit was considered to show significant selectivity. This approach was highly conservative and may underestimate the percentage of units showing significant selectivity, particularly any units showing transient selectivity. In the revised manuscript, we now define a unit as showing significant tHit-tCR selectivity when three consecutive time bins (>30 ms, 10ms bins) of AUC values were significant. Using this new criterion, the percentage of tHittCR selective neurons increased compared with the previous analysis. We have updated Figure 1h and the results on page 4:

      “We found that 18-33% of neurons in these cortical areas had area under the receiver-operating curve (AUC) values significantly different from 0.5, and therefore discriminated between tHit and tCR trials (Fig. 1h; S1: 28.8%, 177 neurons; S2: 17.9%, 162 neurons; MM: 32.9%, 140 neurons; ALM: 23.4%, 256 neurons; criterion to be considered significant: Bonferroni corrected 95% CI on AUC did not include 0.5 for at least 3 consecutive 10-ms time bins).”

      Next, we have checked how tHit-tCR selective neurons were distributed across sessions. We found that the percentage of tHit-tCR selective neurons in each session varied (S1: 9-46%, S2: 0-36%, MM:25-55%, ALM:0-50%). We examined the relationship between the numbers of tHit-tCR selective neurons and tHit-tCR subspace overlaps. Sessions with more neurons showing task rule modulation tended to show lower subspace overlap, but this correlation was modest and only marginally significant (r= -0.32, p= 0.08, Pearson correlation, n= 31 sessions). While we report the percentage of neurons showing significant selectivity as a simple way to summarize single-neuron effects, this does neglect the magnitude of task rule modulation of individual neurons, which may also be relevant. 

      In summary, the apparent disconnect between the effect sizes of task modulation of single neurons and of population dynamics could be explained by (1) the percentages of tHit-tCR selective neurons were underestimated in our old analysis, (2) tHit-tCR selective neurons were not uniformly distributed among sessions, and (3) the percentages of tHit-tCR selective neurons were weakly correlated with tHit-tCR subspace overlaps. 

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      For the analysis of choice coding dimensions, it seems that the authors are somewhat data limited in that they cannot compare lick-right/lick-left within a block. So instead, they compare lick/no lick trials. But given that the mice are unable to initiate trials, the interpretation of the no lick trials is a bit complicated. It is not clear that the no lick trials reflect a perceptual judgment about the stimulus (i.e., a choice), or that the mice are just zoning out and not paying attention. If it's the latter case, what the authors are calling choice coding is more of an attentional or task engagement signal, which may still be interesting, but has a somewhat different interpretation than a choice coding dimension. It might be worth clarifying this point somewhere, or if I'm totally off-base, then being more clear about why lick/no lick is more consistent with choice than task engagement.

      We thank the Reviewer for raising this point. We have added a new paragraph on page 13 to clarify why we used lick/no-lick trials to calculate choice coding dimensions, and we now discuss the caveat regarding task engagement.  

      “No-lick trials included misses, which could be caused by mice not being engaged in the task. While the majority of no-lick trials were correct rejections (respond-to-touch: 75%; respond-to-light: 76%), we treated no-licks as one of the available choices in our task and included them to calculate choice coding dimensions (Fig. S4c,d,f). To ensure stable and balanced task engagement across task rules, we removed the last 20 trials of each session and used stimulus parameters that achieved similar behavioral performance for both task rules (Fig. 1d; ~75% correct for both rules).”

      In addition, to address a point made by Reviewer 3 as well as this point, we performed a new analysis to calculate choice coding dimensions using right-lick vs left-lick trials. We report this new analysis on page 8:

      “Choice coding dimensions were obtained from left-lick and no-lick trials in respond-to-touch blocks and right-lick and no-lick trials in respond-to-light blocks. Because the required lick directions differed between the block types, the difference in choice CDs across task rules (Fig. S4f) could have been affected by the different motor responses. To rule out this possibility, we did a new version of this analysis using right-lick and left-lick trials to calculate the choice coding dimensions for both task rules. We found that the orientation of the choice coding dimension in a respond-to-touch block was still not aligned well with that in a respond-to-light block (Fig. S4h;  magnitude of dot product between the respond-to-touch choice CD and the respond-to-light choice CD, mean ± 95% CI for true vs shuffled data: S1: 0.39 ± [0.23, 0.55] vs 0.2 ± [0.1, 0.31], 10 sessions; S2: 0.32 ± [0.18, 0.46] vs 0.2 ± [0.11, 0.3], 8 sessions; MM: 0.35 ± [0.21, 0.48] vs 0.18 ± [0.11, 0.26], 9 sessions; ALM: 0.28 ± [0.17, 0.39] vs 0.21 ± [0.12, 0.31], 13 sessions).” 

      We added discussion of the limitations of this new analysis on page 13:

      “However, we also calculated choice coding dimensions using only right- and left-lick trials. In S1, S2, MM and ALM, the choice CDs calculated this way were also not aligned well across task rules (Fig. S4h), consistent with the results calculated from lick and no-lick trials (Fig. S4f). Data were limited for this analysis, however, because mice rarely licked to the unrewarded water port (# of licksunrewarded port  / # of lickstotal , respond-to-touch: 0.13, respond-to-light: 0.11). These trials usually came from rule transitions (Fig. 5a) and, in some cases, were potentially caused by exploratory behaviors. These factors could affect choice CDs.”

      The authors find that the stimulus coding direction in most areas (S1, S2, and MM) was significantly aligned between the block types. How do the authors interpret that finding? That there is no major change in stimulus coding dimension, despite the change in subspace? I think I'm missing the big picture interpretation of this result.

      That there is no significant change in stimulus coding dimensions but a change in subspace suggests that the subspace change largely reflects a change in the choice coding dimensions.

      As I mentioned in the public review, I thought there was a weakness with interpretation of the optogenetic experiments, which the authors generally interpret as reflecting rule sensitivity. However, given that they are inhibiting premotor areas including ALM, one might imagine that there might also be an effect on lick production or kinematics. To rule this out, the authors compare the change in lick rate relative to licks during the ITI. What is the ITI lick rate? I assume pretty low, once the animal is welltrained, in which case there may be a floor effect that could obscure meaningful effects on lick production. In addition, based on the reported CI on delta p(lick), it looks like MM and AM did suppress lick rate. I think in the future, a task with richer behavioral read-outs (or including other measurements of behavior like video), or perhaps something like a psychological process model with parameters that reflect different perceptual or cognitive processes could help resolve the effects of perturbations more precisely.

      Eighteen and ten percent of trials had at least one lick in the ITI in respond-to-touch and  respond-tolight blocks, respectively. These relatively low rates of ITI licking could indeed make an effect of optogenetics on lick production harder to observe. We agree that future work would benefit from more complex tasks and measurements, and have added the following to make this point (page 14):

      “To more precisely dissect the effects of perturbations on different cognitive processes in rule-dependent sensory detection, more complex behavioral tasks and richer behavioral measurements are needed in the future.”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      I have the following minor suggestions that the authors might consider in revising this already excellent manuscript :

      (1) In addition to showing normalised z-score firing rates (e.g. Fig 1g), I think it is important to show the grand-average mean firing rates in Hz.

      We thank the Reviewer for the suggestion and have added the grand-average mean firing rates as a new supplementary figure (Fig. S2a). To provide more details about the firing rates of individual neurons, we have also added to this new figure the distribution of peak responses during the tactile stimulus period (Fig. S2b).

      (2) I think the authors could report more quantitative data in the main text. As a very basic example, I could not easily find how many neurons, sessions, and mice were used in various analyses.

      We have added relevant numbers at various points throughout the Results, including within the following examples:

      Page 3: “To examine how the task rules influenced the sensorimotor transformation occurring in the tactile processing stream, we performed single-unit recordings from sensory and motor cortical areas including S1, S2, MM and ALM (Fig. 1e-g, Fig. S1a-h, and Fig. S2a; S1: 6 mice, 10 sessions, 177 neurons, S2: 5 mice, 8 sessions, 162 neurons, MM: 7 mice, 9 sessions, 140 neurons, ALM: 8 mice, 13 sessions, 256 neurons).”

      Page 5: “As expected, single-unit activity before stimulus onset did not discriminate between tactile and visual trials (Fig. 2d; S1: 0%, 177 neurons; S2: 0%, 162 neurons; MM: 0%, 140 neurons; ALM: 0.8%, 256 neurons). After stimulus onset, more than 35% of neurons in the sensory cortical areas and approximately 15% of neurons in the motor cortical areas showed significant stimulus discriminability (Fig. 2e; S1: 37.3%, 177 neurons; S2: 35.2%, 162 neurons; MM: 15%, 140 neurons; ALM: 14.1%, 256 neurons).”

      Page 6: “Support vector machine (SVM) and Random Forest classifiers showed similar decoding abilities

      (Fig. S3a,b; medians of classification accuracy [true vs shuffled]; SVM: S1 [0.6 vs 0.53], 10 sessions, S2

      [0.61 vs 0.51], 8 sessions, MM [0.71 vs 0.51], 9 sessions, ALM [0.65 vs 0.52], 13 sessions; Random

      Forests: S1 [0.59 vs 0.52], 10 sessions, S2 [0.6 vs 0.52], 8 sessions, MM [0.65 vs 0.49], 9 sessions, ALM [0.7 vs 0.5], 13 sessions).”

      Page 6: “To assess this for the four cortical areas, we quantified how the tHit and tCR trajectories diverged from each other by calculating the Euclidean distance between matching time points for all possible pairs of tHit and tCR trajectories for a given session and then averaging these for the session (Fig. 4a,b; S1: 10 sessions, S2: 8 sessions, MM: 9 sessions, ALM: 13 sessions, individual sessions in gray and averages across sessions in black; window of analysis: -100 to 150 ms relative to stimulus onset; 10 ms bins; using the top 3 PCs; Methods).” 

      Page 8: “In contrast, we found that S1, S2 and MM had stimulus CDs that were significantly aligned between the two block types (Fig. S4e; magnitude of dot product between the respond-to-touch stimulus CDs and the respond-to-light stimulus CDs, mean ± 95% CI for true vs shuffled data: S1: 0.5 ± [0.34, 0.66] vs 0.21 ± [0.12, 0.34], 10 sessions; S2: 0.62 ± [0.43, 0.78] vs 0.22 ± [0.13, 0.31], 8 sessions; MM: 0.48 ± [0.38, 0.59] vs 0.24 ± [0.16, 0.33], 9 sessions; ALM: 0.33 ± [0.2, 0.47] vs 0.21 ± [0.13, 0.31], 13 sessions).”  Page 9: “For respond-to-touch to respond-to-light block transitions, the fractions of trials classified as respond-to-touch for MM and ALM decreased progressively over the course of the transition (Fig. 5d; rank correlation of the fractions calculated for each of the separate periods spanning the transition, Kendall’s tau, mean ± 95% CI: MM: -0.39 ± [-0.67, -0.11], 9 sessions, ALM: -0.29 ± [-0.54, -0.04], 13 sessions; criterion to be considered significant: 95% CI on Kendall’s tau did not include 0).

      Page 11: “Lick probability was unaffected during S1, S2, MM and ALM experiments for both tasks, indicating that the behavioral effects were not due to an inability to lick (Fig. 6i, j; 95% CI on Δ lick probability for cross-modal selection task: S1/S2 [-0.18, 0.24], 4 mice, 10 sessions; MM [-0.31, 0.03], 4 mice, 11 sessions; ALM [-0.24, 0.16], 4 mice, 10 sessions; Δ lick probability for simple tactile detection task: S1/S2 [-0.13, 0.31], 3 mice, 3 sessions; MM [-0.06, 0.45], 3 mice, 5 sessions; ALM [-0.18, 0.34], 3 mice, 4 sessions).”

      (3) Please include a clearer description of trial timing. Perhaps a schematic timeline of when stimuli are delivered and when licking would be rewarded. I may have missed it, but I did not find explicit mention of the timing of the reward window or if there was any delay period.

      We have added the following (page 3): 

      “For each trial, the stimulus duration was 0.15 s and an answer period extended from 0.1 to 2 s from stimulus onset.”

      (4) Please include a clear description of statistical tests in each figure legend as needed (for example please check Fig 4e legend).

      We have added details about statistical tests in the figure legends:

      Fig. 2f: “Relationship between block-type discriminability before stimulus onset and tHit-tCR discriminability after stimulus onset for units showing significant block-type discriminability prior to the stimulus. Pearson correlation: S1: r = 0.69, p = 0.056, 8 neurons; S2: r = 0.91, p = 0.093, 4 neurons; MM: r = 0.93, p < 0.001, 30 neurons; ALM: r = 0.83, p < 0.001, 26 neurons.” 

      Fig. 4e: “Subspace overlap for control tHit (gray) and tCR (purple) trials in the somatosensory and motor cortical areas. Each circle is a subspace overlap of a session. Paired t-test, tCR – control tHit: S1: -0.23, 8 sessions, p = 0.0016; S2: -0.23, 7 sessions, p = 0.0086; MM: -0.36, 5 sessions, p = <0.001; ALM: -0.35, 11 sessions, p < 0.001; significance: ** for p<0.01, *** for p<0.001.”  

      Fig. 5d,e: “Fraction of trials classified as coming from a respond-to-touch block based on the pre-stimulus population state, for trials occurring in different periods (see c) relative to respond-to-touch → respondto-light transitions. For MM (top row) and ALM (bottom row), progressively fewer trials were classified as coming from the respond-to-touch block as analysis windows shifted later relative to the rule transition. Kendall’s tau (rank correlation): MM: -0.39, 9 sessions; ALM: -0.29, 13 sessions. Left panels: individual sessions, right panels: mean ± 95% CI. Dash lines are chance levels (0.5). e, Same as d but for respond-to-light → respond-to-touch transitions. Kendall’s tau: MM: 0.37, 9 sessions; ALM: 0.27, 13 sessions.”

      Fig. 6: “Error bars show bootstrap 95% CI. Criterion to be considered significant: 95% CI did not include 0.”

      (5) P. 3 - "To examine how the task rules influenced the sensorimotor transformation occurring in the tactile processing stream, we performed single-unit recordings from sensory and motor cortical areas including S1, S2, MM, and ALM using 64-channel silicon probes (Fig. 1e-g and Fig. S1a-h)." Please specify if these areas were recorded simultaneously or not.

      We have added “We recorded from one of these cortical areas per session, using 64-channel silicon probes.”  on page 3.  

      (6) Figure 4b - Please describe what gray and black lines show.

      The gray traces are the distance between tHit and tCR trajectories in individual sessions and the black traces are the averages across sessions in different cortical areas. We have added this information on page 6 and in the Figure 4b legend. 

      Page 6: “To assess this for the four cortical areas, we quantified how the tHit and tCR trajectories diverged from each other by calculating the Euclidean distance between matching time points for all possible pairs of tHit and tCR trajectories for a given session and then averaging these for the session (Fig. 4a,b; S1: 10 sessions, S2: 8 sessions, MM: 9 sessions, ALM: 13 sessions, individual sessions in gray and averages across sessions in black; window of analysis: -100 to 150 ms relative to stimulus onset; 10 ms bins; using the top 3 PCs; Methods).

      Fig. 4b: “Distance between tHit and tCR trajectories in S1, S2, MM and ALM. Gray traces show the time varying tHit-tCR distance in individual sessions and black traces are session-averaged tHit-tCR distance (S1:10 sessions; S2: 8 sessions; MM: 9 sessions; ALM: 13 sessions).”

      (7) In addition to the analyses shown in Figure 5a, when investigating the timing of the rule switch, I think the authors should plot the left and right lick probabilities aligned to the timing of the rule switch time on a trial-by-trial basis averaged across mice.

      We thank the Reviewer for suggesting this addition. We have added a new figure panel to show the probabilities of right- and left-licks during rule transitions (Fig. 5a).

      Page 8: “The probabilities of right-licks and left-licks showed that the mice switched their motor responses during block transitions depending on task rules (Fig. 5a, mean ± 95% CI across 12 mice).” 

      (8) P. 12 - "Moreover, in a separate study using the same task (Finkel et al., unpublished), high-speed video analysis demonstrated no significant differences in whisker motion between respond-to-touch and respond-to-light blocks in most (12 of 14) behavioral sessions.". Such behavioral data is important and ideally would be included in the current analysis. Was high-speed videography carried out during electrophysiology in the current study?

      Finkel et al. has been accepted in principle for publication and will be available online shortly. Unfortunately we have not yet carried out simultaneous high-speed whisker video and electrophysiology in our cross-modal sensory selection task.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Minor point. For subspace overlap calculation of pre-stimulus activity in Fig 4e (light purple datapoints), please clarify whether the PCs for that condition were constructed in matched time windows. If the PCs are calculated from the stimulus period 0-150ms, the poor alignment could be due to mismatched time windows.

      We thank the Reviewer for the comment and clarify our analysis here. We previously used timematched windows to calculate subspace overlaps. However, the pre-stimulus activity was much weaker than the activity during the stimulus period, so the subspaces of reference tHit were subject to noise and we were not able to obtain reliable PCs. This caused the subspace overlap values between the reference tHit and control tHit to be low and variable (mean ± SD, S1:  0.46± 0.26, n = 8 sessions, S2: 0.46± 0.18, n = 7 sessions, MM: 0.44± 0.16, n = 5 sessions, ALM: 0.38± 0.22, n = 11 sessions).  Therefore, we used the tHit activity during the stimulus window to obtain PCs and projected pre-stimulus and stimulus activity in tCR trials onto these PCs. We have now added a more detailed description of this analysis in the Methods (page 32). 

      “To calculate the separation of subspaces prior to stimulus delivery, pre-stimulus activity in tCR trials (100 to 0 ms from stimulus onset) was projected to the PC space of the tHit reference group and the subspace overlap was calculated. In this analysis, we used tHit activity during stimulus delivery (0 to 150 ms from stimulus onset) to obtain reliable PCs.”   

      We acknowledge this time alignment issue and have now removed the reported subspace overlap between tHit and tCR during the pre-stimulus period from Figure 4e (light purple). However, we think the correlation between pre- and post- stimulus-onset subspace overlaps should remain similar regardless of the time windows that we used for calculating the PCs. For the PCs calculated from the pre-stimulus period (-100 to 0 ms), the correlation coefficient was 0.55 (Pearson correlation, p <0.01, n = 31 sessions). For the PCs calculated from the stimulus period (0-150 ms), the correlation coefficient was 0.68 (Figure 4f, Pearson correlation, p <0.001, n = 31 sessions). Therefore, we keep Figure 4f.  

      (2) Minor point. To help the readers follow the logic of the experiments, please explain why PPC and AMM were added in the later optogenetic experiment since these are not part of the electrophysiology experiment.

      We have added the following rationale on page 9.

      “We recorded from AMM in our cross-modal sensory selection task and observed visually-evoked activity (Fig. S1i-k), suggesting that AMM may play an important role in rule-dependent visual processing. PPC contributes to multisensory processing51–53 and sensory-motor integration50,54–58.  Therefore, we wanted to test the roles of these areas in our cross-modal sensory selection task.”

      (3) Minor point. We are somewhat confused about the timing of some of the example neurons shown in figure S1. For example, many neurons show visually evoked signals only after stimulus offset, unlike tactile evoked signals (e.g. Fig S1b and f). In addition, the reaction time for visual stimulus is systematically slower than tactile stimuli for many example neurons (e.g. Fig S1b) but somehow not other neurons (e.g. Fig S1g). Are these observations correct?

      These observations are all correct. We have a manuscript from a separate study using this same behavioral task (Finkel et al., accepted in principle) that examines and compares (1) the onsets of tactile- and visually-evoked activity and (2) the reaction times to tactile and visual stimuli. The reaction times to tactile stimuli were slightly but significantly shorter than the reaction times to visual stimuli (tactile vs visual, 397 ± 145 vs 521 ± 163 ms, median ± interquartile range [IQR], Tukey HSD test, p = 0.001, n =155 sessions). We examined how well activity of individual neurons in S1 could be used to discriminate the presence of the stimulus or the response of the mouse. For discriminability for the presence of the stimulus, S1 neurons could signal the presence of the tactile stimulus but not the visual stimulus. For discriminability for the response of the mouse, the onsets for significant discriminability occurred earlier for tactile compared with visual trials (two-sided Kolmogorov-Smirnov test, p = 1x10-16, n = 865 neurons with DP onset in tactile trials, n = 719 neurons with DP onset in visual trials).

    1. Author Response

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Public Comments

      Reviewer 1

      (1) Despite the well-established role of Netrin-1 and UNC5C axon guidance during embryonic commissural axons, it remains unclear which cell type(s) express Netrin-1 or UNC5C in the dopaminergic axons and their targets. For instance, the data in Figure 1F-G and Figure 2 are quite confusing. Does Netrin-1 or UNC5C express in all cell types or only dopamine-positive neurons in these two mouse models? It will also be important to provide quantitative assessments of UNC5C expression in dopaminergic axons at different ages.

      Netrin-1 is a secreted protein and in this manuscript we did not examine what cell types express Netrin-1. This question is not the focus of the study and we consider it irrelevant to the main issue we are addressing, which is where in the forebrain regions we examined Netrin-1+ cells are present. As per the reviewer’s request we include below images showing Netrin-1 protein and Netrin-1 mRNA expression in the forebrain. In Figure 1 below, we show a high magnification immunofluorescent image of a coronal forebrain section showing Netrin-1 protein expression.

      Author response image 1.

      This confocal microscope image shows immunofluorescent staining for Netrin-1 (green) localized around cell nuclei (stained by DAPI in blue). This image was taken from a coronal section of the lateral septum of an adult male mouse. Scale bar = 20µm

      In Figures 2 and 3 below we show low and high magnification images from an RNAscope experiment confirming that cells in the forebrain regions examined express Netrin-1 mRNA.

      Author response image 2.

      This confocal microscope image of a coronal brain section of the medial prefrontal cortex of an adult male mouse shows Netrin-1 mRNA expression (green) and cell nuclei (DAPI, blue). Brain regions are as follows: Cg1: Anterior cingulate cortex 1, DP: dorsopeduncular cortex, fmi: forceps minor of the corpus callosum, IL: Infralimbic Cortex, PrL: Prelimbic Cortex

      Author response image 3.

      A higher resolution image from the same sample as in Figure 2 shows Netrin-1 mRNA (green) and cell nuclei (DAPI; blue). DP = dorsopeduncular cortex

      Regarding UNC5c, this receptor homologue is expressed by dopamine neurons in the rodent ventral tegmental area (Daubaras et al., 2014; Manitt et al., 2010; Phillips et al., 2022). This does not preclude UNC5c expression in other cell types. UNC5c receptors are ubiquitously expressed in the brain throughout development, performing many different developmental functions (Kim and Ackerman, 2011; Murcia-Belmonte et al., 2019; Srivatsa et al., 2014). In this study we are interested in UNC5c expression by dopamine neurons, and particularly by their axons projecting to the nucleus accumbens. We therefore used immunofluorescent staining in the nucleus accumbens, showing UNC5 expression in TH+ axons. This work adds to the study by Manitt et al., 2010, which examined UNC5 expression in the VTA. Manitt et al. used Western blotting to demonstrate that UNC5 expression in VTA dopamine neurons increases during adolescence, as can be seen in the following figure:

      References:

      Daubaras M, Bo GD, Flores C. 2014. Target-dependent expression of the netrin-1 receptor, UNC5C, in projection neurons of the ventral tegmental area. Neuroscience 260:36–46. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.007

      Kim D, Ackerman SL. 2011. The UNC5C Netrin Receptor Regulates Dorsal Guidance of Mouse Hindbrain Axons. J Neurosci 31:2167–2179. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5254-10.20110.2011

      Manitt C, Labelle-Dumais C, Eng C, Grant A, Mimee A, Stroh T, Flores C. 2010. Peri-Pubertal Emergence of UNC-5 Homologue Expression by Dopamine Neurons in Rodents. PLoS ONE 5:e11463-14. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011463

      Murcia-Belmonte V, Coca Y, Vegar C, Negueruela S, Romero C de J, Valiño AJ, Sala S, DaSilva R, Kania A, Borrell V, Martinez LM, Erskine L, Herrera E. 2019. A Retino-retinal Projection Guided by Unc5c Emerged in Species with Retinal Waves. Current Biology 29:1149-1160.e4. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.052

      Phillips RA, Tuscher JJ, Black SL, Andraka E, Fitzgerald ND, Ianov L, Day JJ. 2022. An atlas of transcriptionally defined cell populations in the rat ventral tegmental area. Cell Reports 39:110616. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2022.110616

      Srivatsa S, Parthasarathy S, Britanova O, Bormuth I, Donahoo A-L, Ackerman SL, Richards LJ, Tarabykin V. 2014. Unc5C and DCC act downstream of Ctip2 and Satb2 and contribute to corpus callosum formation. Nat Commun 5:3708. doi:10.1038/ncomms4708

      (2) Figure 1 used shRNA to knockdown Netrin-1 in the Septum and these mice were subjected to behavioral testing. These results, again, are not supported by any valid data that the knockdown approach actually worked in dopaminergic axons. It is also unclear whether knocking down Netrin-1 in the septum will re-route dopaminergic axons or lead to cell death in the dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta?

      First we want to clarify and emphasize, that our knockdown approach was not designed to knock down Netrin-1 in dopamine neurons or their axons. Our goal was to knock down Netrin-1 expression in cells expressing this guidance cue gene in the dorsal peduncular cortex.

      We have previously established the efficacy of the shRNA Netrin-1 knockdown virus used in this experiment for reducing the expression of Netrin-1 (Cuesta et al., 2020). The shRNA reduces Netrin-1 levels in vitro and in vivo.

      We agree that our experiments do not address the fate of the dopamine axons that are misrouted away from the medial prefrontal cortex. This research is ongoing, and we have now added a note regarding this to our manuscript.

      Our current hypothesis, based on experiments being conducted as part of another line of research in the lab, is that these axons are rerouted to a different brain region which they then ectopically innervate. In these experiments we are finding that male mice exposed to tetrahydrocannabinol in adolescence show reduced dopamine innervation in the medial prefrontal cortex in adulthood but increased dopamine input in the orbitofrontal cortex. In addition, these mice show increased action impulsivity in the Go/No-Go task in adulthood (Capolicchio et al., Society for Neuroscience 2023 Abstracts)

      References:

      Capolicchio T., Hernandez, G., Dube, E., Estrada, K., Giroux, M., Flores, C. (2023) Divergent outcomes of delta 9 - tetrahydrocannabinol in adolescence on dopamine and cognitive development in male and female mice. Society for Neuroscience, Washington, DC, United States [abstract].

      Cuesta S, Nouel D, Reynolds LM, Morgunova A, Torres-Berrío A, White A, Hernandez G, Cooper HM, Flores C. 2020. Dopamine Axon Targeting in the Nucleus Accumbens in Adolescence Requires Netrin-1. Frontiers Cell Dev Biology 8:487. doi:10.3389/fcell.2020.00487

      (3) Another issue with Figure1J. It is unclear whether the viruses were injected into a WT mouse model or into a Cre-mouse model driven by a promoter specifically expresses in dorsal peduncular cortex? The authors should provide evidence that Netrin-1 mRNA and proteins are indeed significantly reduced. The authors should address the anatomic results of the area of virus diffusion to confirm the virus specifically infected the cells in dorsal peduncular cortex.

      All the virus knockdown experiments were conducted in wild type mice, we added this information to Figure 1k.

      The efficacy of the shRNA in knocking down Netrin-1 was demonstrated by Cuesta et al. (2020) both in vitro and in vivo, as we show in our response to the reviewer’s previous comment above.

      We also now provide anatomical images demonstrating the localization of the injection and area of virus diffusion in the mouse forebrain. In Author response image 4 below the area of virus diffusion is visible as green fluorescent signal.

      Author response image 4.

      Fluorescent microscopy image of a mouse forebrain demonstrating the localization of the injection of a virus to knock down Netrin-1. The location of the virus is in green, while cell nuclei are in blue (DAPI). Abbreviations: DP: dorsopeduncular cortex IL: infralimbic cortex

      References:

      Cuesta S, Nouel D, Reynolds LM, Morgunova A, Torres-Berrío A, White A, Hernandez G, Cooper HM, Flores C. 2020. Dopamine Axon Targeting in the Nucleus Accumbens in Adolescence Requires Netrin-1. Frontiers Cell Dev Biology 8:487. doi:10.3389/fcell.2020.00487

      (4) The authors need to provide information regarding the efficiency and duration of knocking down. For instance, in Figure 1K, the mice were tested after 53 days post injection, can the virus activity in the brain last for such a long time?

      In our study we are interested in the role of Netrin-1 expression in the guidance of dopamine axons from the nucleus accumbens to the medial prefrontal cortex. The critical window for these axons leaving the nucleus accumbens and growing to the cortex is early adolescence (Reynolds et al., 2018b). This is why we injected the virus at the onset of adolescence, at postnatal day 21. As dopamine axons grow from the nucleus accumbens to the prefrontal cortex, they pass through the dorsal peduncular cortex. We disrupted Netrin-1 expression at this point along their route to determine whether it is the Netrin-1 present along their route that guides these axons to the prefrontal cortex. We hypothesized that the shRNA Netrin-1 virus would disrupt the growth of the dopamine axons, reducing the number of axons that reach the prefrontal cortex and therefore the number of axons that innervate this region in adulthood.

      We conducted our behavioural tests during adulthood, after the critical window during which dopamine axon growth occurs, so as to observe the enduring behavioral consequences of this misrouting. This experimental approach is designed for the shRNa Netrin-1 virus to be expressed in cells in the dorsopeduncular cortex when the dopamine axons are growing, during adolescence.

      References:

      Capolicchio T., Hernandez, G., Dube, E., Estrada, K., Giroux, M., Flores, C. (2023) Divergent outcomes of delta 9 - tetrahydrocannabinol in adolescence on dopamine and cognitive development in male and female mice. Society for Neuroscience, Washington, DC, United States [abstract].

      Reynolds LM, Yetnikoff L, Pokinko M, Wodzinski M, Epelbaum JG, Lambert LC, Cossette M-P, Arvanitogiannis A, Flores C. 2018b. Early Adolescence is a Critical Period for the Maturation of Inhibitory Behavior. Cerebral cortex 29:3676–3686. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy247

      (5) In Figure 1N-Q, silencing Netrin-1 results in less DA axons targeting to infralimbic cortex, but why the Netrin-1 knocking down mice revealed the improved behavior?

      This is indeed an intriguing finding, and we have now added a mention of it to our manuscript. We have demonstrated that misrouting dopamine axons away from the medial prefrontal cortex during adolescence alters behaviour, but why this improves their action impulsivity ability is something currently unknown to us. One potential answer is that the dopamine axons are misrouted to a different brain region that is also involved in controlling impulsive behaviour, perhaps the dorsal striatum (Kim and Im, 2019) or the orbital prefrontal cortex (Jonker et al., 2015).

      We would also like to note that we are finding that other manipulations that appear to reroute dopamine axons to unintended targets can lead to reduced action impulsivity as measured using the Go No Go task. As we mentioned above, current experiments in the lab, which are part of a different line of research, are showing that male mice exposed to tetrahydrocannabinol in adolescence show reduced dopamine innervation in the medial prefrontal cortex in adulthood, but increased dopamine input in the orbitofrontal cortex. In addition, these mice show increased action impulsivity in the Go/No-Go task in adulthood (Capolicchio et al., Society for Neuroscience 2023 Abstracts)

      References

      Capolicchio T., Hernandez, G., Dube, E., Estrada, K., Giroux, M., Flores, C. (2023) Divergent outcomes of delta 9 - tetrahydrocannabinol in adolescence on dopamine and cognitive development in male and female mice. Society for Neuroscience, Washington, DC, United States [abstract].

      Jonker FA, Jonker C, Scheltens P, Scherder EJA. 2015. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in cognition and behavior. Rev Neurosci 26:1–11. doi:10.1515/revneuro2014-0043 Kim B, Im H. 2019. The role of the dorsal striatum in choice impulsivity. Ann N York Acad Sci 1451:92–111. doi:10.1111/nyas.13961

      (6) What is the effect of knocking down UNC5C on dopamine axons guidance to the cortex?

      We have found that mice that are heterozygous for a nonsense Unc5c mutation, and as a result have reduced levels of UNC5c protein, show reduced amphetamine-induced locomotion and stereotypy (Auger et al., 2013). In the same manuscript we show that this effect only emerges during adolescence, in concert with the growth of dopamine axons to the prefrontal cortex. This is indirect but strong evidence that UNC5c receptors are necessary for correct adolescent dopamine axon development.

      References

      Auger ML, Schmidt ERE, Manitt C, Dal-Bo G, Pasterkamp RJ, Flores C. 2013. unc5c haploinsufficient phenotype: striking similarities with the dcc haploinsufficiency model. European Journal of Neuroscience 38:2853–2863. doi:10.1111/ejn.12270

      (7) In Figures 2-4, the authors only showed the amount of DA axons and UNC5C in NAcc. However, it remains unclear whether these experiments also impact the projections of dopaminergic axons to other brain regions, critical for the behavioral phenotypes. What about other brain regions such as prefrontal cortex? Do the projection of DA axons and UNC5c level in cortex have similar pattern to those in NAcc?

      UNC5c receptors are expressed throughout development and are involved in many developmental processes (Kim and Ackerman, 2011; Murcia-Belmonte et al., 2019; Srivatsa et al., 2014). We cannot say whether the pattern we observe here is unique to the nucleus accumbens, but it is certainly not universal throughout the brain.

      The brain region we focus on in our manuscript, in addition to the nucleus accumbens, is the medial prefrontal cortex. Close and thorough examination of the prefrontal cortices of adult mice revealed practically no UNC5c expression by dopamine axons. However, we did observe very rare cases of dopamine axons expressing UNC5c. It is not clear whether these rare cases are present before or during adolescence.

      Below is a representative set of images of this observation, which is now also included as Supplementary Figure 4:

      Author response image 5.

      Expression of UNC5c protein in the medial prefrontal cortex of an adult male mouse. Low (A) and high (B) magnification images demonstrate that there is little UNC5c expression in dopamine axons in the medial prefrontal cortex. Here we identify dopamine axons by immunofluorescent staining for tyrosine hydroxylase (TH, see our response to comment #9 regarding the specificity of the TH antibody for dopamine axons in the prefrontal cortex). This figure is also included as Supplementary Figure 4 in the manuscript. Abbreviations: fmi: forceps minor of the corpus callosum, mPFC: medial prefrontal cortex.

      References:

      Kim D, Ackerman SL. 2011. The UNC5C Netrin Receptor Regulates Dorsal Guidance of Mouse Hindbrain Axons. J Neurosci 31:2167–2179. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5254- 10.20110.2011

      Murcia-Belmonte V, Coca Y, Vegar C, Negueruela S, Romero C de J, Valiño AJ, Sala S, DaSilva R, Kania A, Borrell V, Martinez LM, Erskine L, Herrera E. 2019. A Retino-retinal Projection Guided by Unc5c Emerged in Species with Retinal Waves. Current Biology 29:1149-1160.e4. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.052

      Srivatsa S, Parthasarathy S, Britanova O, Bormuth I, Donahoo A-L, Ackerman SL, Richards LJ, Tarabykin V. 2014. Unc5C and DCC act downstream of Ctip2 and Satb2 and contribute to corpus callosum formation. Nat Commun 5:3708. doi:10.1038/ncomms4708

      (8) Can overexpression of UNC5c or Netrin-1 in male winter hamsters mimic the observations in summer hamsters? Or overexpression of UNC5c in female summer hamsters to mimic the winter hamster? This would be helpful to confirm the causal role of UNC5C in guiding DA axons during adolescence.

      This is an excellent question. We are very interested in both increasing and decreasing UNC5c expression in hamster dopamine axons to see if we can directly manipulate summer hamsters into winter hamsters and vice versa. We are currently exploring virus-based approaches to design these experiments and are excited for results in this area.

      (9) The entire study relied on using tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) as a marker for dopaminergic axons. However, the expression of TH (either by IHC or IF) can be influenced by other environmental factors, that could alter the expression of TH at the cellular level.

      This is an excellent point that we now carefully address in our methods by adding the following:

      In this study we pay great attention to the morphology and localization of the fibres from which we quantify varicosities to avoid counting any fibres stained with TH antibodies that are not dopamine fibres. The fibres that we examine and that are labelled by the TH antibody show features indistinguishable from the classic features of cortical dopamine axons in rodents (Berger et al., 1974; 1983; Van Eden et al., 1987; Manitt et al., 2011), namely they are thin fibres with irregularly-spaced varicosities, are densely packed in the nucleus accumbens, sparsely present only in the deep layers of the prefrontal cortex, and are not regularly oriented in relation to the pial surface. This is in contrast to rodent norepinephrine fibres, which are smooth or beaded in appearance, relatively thick with regularly spaced varicosities, increase in density towards the shallow cortical layers, and are in large part oriented either parallel or perpendicular to the pial surface (Berger et al., 1974; Levitt and Moore, 1979; Berger et al., 1983; Miner et al., 2003). Furthermore, previous studies in rodents have noted that only norepinephrine cell bodies are detectable using immunofluorescence for TH, not norepinephrine processes (Pickel et al., 1975; Verney et al., 1982; Miner et al., 2003), and we did not observe any norepinephrine-like fibres.

      Furthermore, we are not aware of any other processes in the forebrain that are known to be immunopositive for TH under any environmental conditions.

      To reduce confusion, we have replaced the abbreviation for dopamine – DA – with TH in the relevant panels in Figures 1, 2, 3, and 4 to clarify exactly what is represented in these images. As can be seen in these images, fluorescent green labelling is present only in axons, which is to be expected of dopamine labelling in these forebrain regions.

      References:

      Berger B, Tassin JP, Blanc G, Moyne MA, Thierry AM (1974) Histochemical confirmation for dopaminergic innervation of the rat cerebral cortex after destruction of the noradrenergic ascending pathways. Brain Res 81:332–337.

      Berger B, Verney C, Gay M, Vigny A (1983) Immunocytochemical Characterization of the Dopaminergic and Noradrenergic Innervation of the Rat Neocortex During Early Ontogeny. In: Proceedings of the 9th Meeting of the International Neurobiology Society, pp 263–267 Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier.

      Levitt P, Moore RY (1979) Development of the noradrenergic innervation of neocortex. Brain Res 162:243–259.

      Manitt C, Mimee A, Eng C, Pokinko M, Stroh T, Cooper HM, Kolb B, Flores C (2011) The Netrin Receptor DCC Is Required in the Pubertal Organization of Mesocortical Dopamine Circuitry. J Neurosci 31:8381–8394.

      Miner LH, Schroeter S, Blakely RD, Sesack SR (2003) Ultrastructural localization of the norepinephrine transporter in superficial and deep layers of the rat prelimbic prefrontal cortex and its spatial relationship to probable dopamine terminals. J Comp Neurol 466:478–494.

      Pickel VM, Joh TH, Field PM, Becker CG, Reis DJ (1975) Cellular localization of tyrosine hydroxylase by immunohistochemistry. J Histochem Cytochem 23:1–12.

      Van Eden CG, Hoorneman EM, Buijs RM, Matthijssen MA, Geffard M, Uylings HBM (1987) Immunocytochemical localization of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex of the rat at the light and electron microscopical level. Neurosci 22:849–862.

      Verney C, Berger B, Adrien J, Vigny A, Gay M (1982) Development of the dopaminergic innervation of the rat cerebral cortex. A light microscopic immunocytochemical study using anti-tyrosine hydroxylase antibodies. Dev Brain Res 5:41–52.

      (10) Are Netrin-1/UNC5C the only signal guiding dopamine axon during adolescence? Are there other neuronal circuits involved in this process?

      Our intention for this study was to examine the role of Netrin-1 and its receptor UNC5C specifically, but we do not suggest that they are the only molecules to play a role. The process of guiding growing dopamine axons during adolescence is likely complex and we expect other guidance mechanisms to also be involved. From our previous work we know that the Netrin-1 receptor DCC is critical in this process (Hoops and Flores, 2017; Reynolds et al., 2023). Several other molecules have been identified in Netrin-1/DCC signaling processes that control corpus callosum development and there is every possibility that the same or similar molecules may be important in guiding dopamine axons (Schlienger et al., 2023).

      References:

      Hoops D, Flores C. 2017. Making Dopamine Connections in Adolescence. Trends in Neurosciences 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2017.09.004

      Reynolds LM, Hernandez G, MacGowan D, Popescu C, Nouel D, Cuesta S, Burke S, Savell KE, Zhao J, Restrepo-Lozano JM, Giroux M, Israel S, Orsini T, He S, Wodzinski M, Avramescu RG, Pokinko M, Epelbaum JG, Niu Z, Pantoja-Urbán AH, Trudeau L-É, Kolb B, Day JJ, Flores C. 2023. Amphetamine disrupts dopamine axon growth in adolescence by a sex-specific mechanism in mice. Nat Commun 14:4035. doi:10.1038/s41467-023-39665-1

      Schlienger S, Yam PT, Balekoglu N, Ducuing H, Michaud J-F, Makihara S, Kramer DK, Chen B, Fasano A, Berardelli A, Hamdan FF, Rouleau GA, Srour M, Charron F. 2023. Genetics of mirror movements identifies a multifunctional complex required for Netrin-1 guidance and lateralization of motor control. Sci Adv 9:eadd5501. doi:10.1126/sciadv.add5501

      (11) Finally, despite the authors' claim that the dopaminergic axon project is sensitive to the duration of daylight in the hamster, they never provided definitive evidence to support this hypothesis.

      By “definitive evidence” we think that the reviewer is requesting a single statistical model including measures from both the summer and winter groups. Such a model would provide a probability estimate of whether dopamine axon growth is sensitive to daylight duration. Therefore, we ran these models, one for male hamsters and one for female hamsters.

      In both sexes we find a significant effect of daylength on dopamine innervation, interacting with age. Male age by daylength interaction: F = 6.383, p = 0.00242. Female age by daylength interaction: F = 21.872, p = 1.97 x 10-9. The full statistical analysis is available as a supplement to this letter (Response_Letter_Stats_Details.docx).

      Reviewer 3

      (1) Fig 1 A and B don't appear to be the same section level.

      The reviewer is correct that Fig 1B is anterior to Fig 1A. We have changed Figure 1A to match the section level of Figure 1B.

      (2) Fig 1C. It is not clear that these axons are crossing from the shell of the NAC.

      We have added a dashed line to Figure 1C to highlight the boundary of the nucleus accumbens, which hopefully emphasizes that there are fibres crossing the boundary. We also include here an enlarged image of this panel:

      Author response image 6.

      An enlarged image of Figure1c in the manuscript. The nucleus accumbens (left of the dotted line) is densely packed with TH+ axons (in green). Some of these TH+ axons can be observed extending from the nucleus accumbens medially towards a region containing dorsally oriented TH+ fibres (white arrows).

      (3) Fig 1. Measuring width of the bundle is an odd way to measure DA axon numbers. First the width could be changing during adult for various reasons including change in brain size. Second, I wouldn't consider these axons in a traditional bundle. Third, could DA axon counts be provided, rather than these proxy measures.

      With regards to potential changes in brain size, we agree that this could have potentially explained the increased width of the dopamine axon pathway. That is why it was important for us to use stereology to measure the density of dopamine axons within the pathway. If the width increased but no new axons grew along the pathway, we would have seen a decrease in axon density from adolescence to adulthood. Instead, our results show that the density of axons remained constant.

      We agree with the reviewer that the dopamine axons do not form a traditional “bundle”. Therefore, throughout the manuscript we now avoid using the term bundle.

      Although we cannot count every single axon, an accurate estimate of this number can be obtained using stereology, an unbiassed method for efficiently quantifying large, irregularly distributed objects. We used stereology to count TH+ axons in an unbiased subset of the total area occupied by these axons. Unbiased stereology is the gold-standard technique for estimating populations of anatomical objects, such as axons, that are so numerous that it would be impractical or impossible to measure every single one. Here and elsewhere we generally provide results as densities and areas of occupancy (Reynolds et al., 2022). To avoid confusion, we now clarify that we are counting the width of the area that dopamine axons occupy (rather than the dopamine axon “bundle”).

      References:

      Reynolds LM, Pantoja-Urbán AH, MacGowan D, Manitt C, Nouel D, Flores C. 2022. Dopaminergic System Function and Dysfunction: Experimental Approaches. Neuromethods 31–63. doi:10.1007/978-1-0716-2799-0_2

      (4) TH in the cortex could also be of noradrenergic origin. This needs to be ruled out to score DA axons

      This is the same comment as Reviewer 1 #9. Please see our response below, which we have also added to our methods:

      In this study we pay great attention to the morphology and localization of the fibres from which we quantify varicosities to avoid counting any fibres stained with TH antibodies that are not dopamine fibres. The fibres that we examine and that are labelled by the TH antibody show features indistinguishable from the classic features of cortical dopamine axons in rodents (Berger et al., 1974; 1983; Van Eden et al., 1987; Manitt et al., 2011), namely they are thin fibres with irregularly-spaced varicosities, are densely packed in the nucleus accumbens, sparsely present only in the deep layers of the prefrontal cortex, and are not regularly oriented in relation to the pial surface. This is in contrast to rodent norepinephrine fibres, which are smooth or beaded in appearance, relatively thick with regularly spaced varicosities, increase in density towards the shallow cortical layers, and are in large part oriented either parallel or perpendicular to the pial surface (Berger et al., 1974; Levitt and Moore, 1979; Berger et al., 1983; Miner et al., 2003). Furthermore, previous studies in rodents have noted that only norepinephrine cell bodies are detectable using immunofluorescence for TH, not norepinephrine processes (Pickel et al., 1975; Verney et al., 1982; Miner et al., 2003), and we did not observe any norepinephrine-like fibres.

      References:

      Berger B, Tassin JP, Blanc G, Moyne MA, Thierry AM (1974) Histochemical confirmation for dopaminergic innervation of the rat cerebral cortex after destruction of the noradrenergic ascending pathways. Brain Res 81:332–337.

      Berger B, Verney C, Gay M, Vigny A (1983) Immunocytochemical Characterization of the Dopaminergic and Noradrenergic Innervation of the Rat Neocortex During Early Ontogeny. In: Proceedings of the 9th Meeting of the International Neurobiology Society, pp 263–267 Progress in Brain Research. Elsevier.

      Levitt P, Moore RY (1979) Development of the noradrenergic innervation of neocortex. Brain Res 162:243–259.

      Manitt C, Mimee A, Eng C, Pokinko M, Stroh T, Cooper HM, Kolb B, Flores C (2011) The Netrin Receptor DCC Is Required in the Pubertal Organization of Mesocortical Dopamine Circuitry. J Neurosci 31:8381–8394.

      Miner LH, Schroeter S, Blakely RD, Sesack SR (2003) Ultrastructural localization of the norepinephrine transporter in superficial and deep layers of the rat prelimbic prefrontal cortex and its spatial relationship to probable dopamine terminals. J Comp Neurol 466:478–494.

      Pickel VM, Joh TH, Field PM, Becker CG, Reis DJ (1975) Cellular localization of tyrosine hydroxylase by immunohistochemistry. J Histochem Cytochem 23:1–12.

      Van Eden CG, Hoorneman EM, Buijs RM, Matthijssen MA, Geffard M, Uylings HBM (1987) Immunocytochemical localization of dopamine in the prefrontal cortex of the rat at the light and electron microscopical level. Neurosci 22:849–862.

      Verney C, Berger B, Adrien J, Vigny A, Gay M (1982) Development of the dopaminergic innervation of the rat cerebral cortex. A light microscopic immunocytochemical study using anti-tyrosine hydroxylase antibodies. Dev Brain Res 5:41–52.

      (5) Netrin staining should be provided with NeuN + DAPI; its not clear these are all cell bodies. An in situ of Netrin would help as well.

      A similar comment was raised by Reviewer 1 in point #1. Please see below the immunofluorescent and RNA scope images showing expression of Netrin-1 protein and mRNA in the forebrain.

      Author response image 7.

      This confocal microscope image shows immunofluorescent staining for Netrin-1 (green) localized around cell nuclei (stained by DAPI in blue). This image was taken from a coronal section of the lateral septum of an adult male mouse. Scale bar = 20µm

      Author response image 8.

      This confocal microscope image of a coronal brain section of the medial prefrontal cortex of an adult male mouse shows Netrin-1 mRNA expression (green) and cell nuclei (DAPI, blue). RNAscope was used to generate this image. Brain regions are as follows: Cg1: Anterior cingulate cortex 1, DP: dorsopeduncular cortex, IL: Infralimbic Cortex, PrL: Prelimbic Cortex, fmi: forceps minor of the corpus callosum

      Author response image 9.

      A higher resolution image from the same sample as in Figure 2 shows Netrin-1 mRNA (green) and cell nuclei (DAPI; blue). DP = dorsopeduncular cortex

      (6) The Netrin knockdown needs validation. How strong was the knockdown etc?

      This comment was also raised by Reviewer 1 #1.

      We have previously established the efficacy of the shRNA Netrin-1 knockdown virus used in this experiment for reducing the expression of Netrin-1 (Cuesta et al., 2020). The shRNA reduces Netrin-1 levels in vitro and in vivo.

      References:

      Cuesta S, Nouel D, Reynolds LM, Morgunova A, Torres-Berrío A, White A, Hernandez G, Cooper HM, Flores C. 2020. Dopamine Axon Targeting in the Nucleus Accumbens in Adolescence Requires Netrin-1. Frontiers Cell Dev Biology 8:487. doi:10.3389/fcell.2020.00487

      (7) If the conclusion that knocking down Netrin in cortex decreases DA innervation of the IL, how can that be reconciled with Netrin-Unc repulsion.

      This is an intriguing question and one that we are in the planning stages of addressing with new experiments.

      Although we do not have a mechanistic answered for how a repulsive receptor helps guide these axons, we would like to note that previous indirect evidence from a study by our group also suggests that reducing UNC5c signaling in dopamine axons in adolescence increases dopamine innervation to the prefrontal cortex (Auger et al, 2013).

      References

      Auger ML, Schmidt ERE, Manitt C, Dal-Bo G, Pasterkamp RJ, Flores C. 2013. unc5c haploinsufficient phenotype: striking similarities with the dcc haploinsufficiency model. European Journal of Neuroscience 38:2853–2863. doi:10.1111/ejn.12270

      (8) The behavioral phenotype in Fig 1 is interesting, but its not clear if its related to DA axons/signaling. IN general, no evidence in this paper is provided for the role of DA in the adolescent behaviors described.

      We agree with the reviewer that the behaviours we describe in adult mice are complex and are likely to involve several neurotransmitter systems. However, there is ample evidence for the role of dopamine signaling in cognitive control behaviours (Bari and Robbins, 2013; Eagle et al., 2008; Ott et al., 2023) and our published work has shown that alterations in the growth of dopamine axons to the prefrontal cortex leads to changes in impulse control as measured via the Go/No-Go task in adulthood (Reynolds et al., 2023, 2018a; Vassilev et al., 2021).

      The other adolescent behaviour we examined was risk-like taking behaviour in male and female hamsters (Figures 4 and 5), as a means of characterizing maturation in this behavior over time. We decided not to use the Go/No-Go task because as far as we know, this has never been employed in Siberian Hamsters and it will be difficult to implement. Instead, we chose the light/dark box paradigm, which requires no training and is ideal for charting behavioural changes over short time periods. Indeed, risk-like taking behavior in rodents and in humans changes from adolescence to adulthood paralleling changes in prefrontal cortex development, including the gradual input of dopamine axons to this region.

      References:

      Bari A, Robbins TW. 2013. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in neurobiology 108:44–79. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005

      Eagle DM, Bari A, Robbins TW. 2008. The neuropsychopharmacology of action inhibition: cross-species translation of the stop-signal and go/no-go tasks. Psychopharmacology 199:439–456. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1127-6

      Ott T, Stein AM, Nieder A. 2023. Dopamine receptor activation regulates reward expectancy signals during cognitive control in primate prefrontal neurons. Nat Commun 14:7537. doi:10.1038/s41467-023-43271-6

      Reynolds LM, Hernandez G, MacGowan D, Popescu C, Nouel D, Cuesta S, Burke S, Savell KE, Zhao J, Restrepo-Lozano JM, Giroux M, Israel S, Orsini T, He S, Wodzinski M, Avramescu RG, Pokinko M, Epelbaum JG, Niu Z, Pantoja-Urbán AH, Trudeau L-É, Kolb B, Day JJ, Flores C. 2023. Amphetamine disrupts dopamine axon growth in adolescence by a sex-specific mechanism in mice. Nat Commun 14:4035. doi:10.1038/s41467-023-39665-1

      Reynolds LM, Pokinko M, Torres-Berrío A, Cuesta S, Lambert LC, Pellitero EDC, Wodzinski M, Manitt C, Krimpenfort P, Kolb B, Flores C. 2018a. DCC Receptors Drive Prefrontal Cortex Maturation by Determining Dopamine Axon Targeting in Adolescence. Biological psychiatry 83:181–192. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.06.009

      Vassilev P, Pantoja-Urban AH, Giroux M, Nouel D, Hernandez G, Orsini T, Flores C. 2021. Unique effects of social defeat stress in adolescent male mice on the Netrin-1/DCC pathway, prefrontal cortex dopamine and cognition (Social stress in adolescent vs. adult male mice). Eneuro ENEURO.0045-21.2021. doi:10.1523/eneuro.0045-21.2021

      (9) Fig2 - boxes should be drawn on the NAc diagram to indicate sampled regions. Some quantification of Unc5c would be useful. Also, some validation of the Unc5c antibody would be nice.

      The images presented were taken medial to the anterior commissure and we have edited Figure 2 to show this. However, we did not notice any intra-accumbens variation, including between the core and the shell. Therefore, the images are representative of what was observed throughout the entire nucleus accumbens.

      To quantify UNC5c in the accumbens we conducted a Western blot experiment in male mice at different ages. A one-way ANOVA analyzing band intensity (relative to the 15-day-old average band intensity) as the response variable and age as the predictor variable showed a significant effect of age (F=5.615, p=0.01). Posthoc analysis revealed that 15-day-old mice have less UNC5c in the nucleus accumbens compared to 21- and 35-day-old mice.

      Author response image 10.

      The graph depicts the results of a Western blot experiment of UNC5c protein levels in the nucleus accumbens of male mice at postnatal days 15, 21 or 35 and reveals a significant increase in protein levels at the onset adolescence.

      Our methods for this Western blot were as follows: Samples were prepared as previously (Torres-Berrío et al., 2017). Briefly, mice were sacrificed by live decapitation and brains were flash frozen in heptane on dry ice for 10 seconds. Frozen brains were mounted in a cryomicrotome and two 500um sections were collected for the nucleus accumbens, corresponding to plates 14 and 18 of the Paxinos mouse brain atlas. Two tissue core samples were collected per section, one for each side of the brain, using a 15-gauge tissue corer (Fine surgical tools Cat no. NC9128328) and ejected in a microtube on dry ice. The tissue samples were homogenized in 100ul of standard radioimmunoprecipitation assay buffer using a handheld electric tissue homogenizer. The samples were clarified by centrifugation at 4C at a speed of 15000g for 30 minutes. Protein concentration was quantified using a bicinchoninic acid assay kit (Pierce BCA protein assay kit, Cat no.PI23225) and denatured with standard Laemmli buffer for 5 minutes at 70C. 10ug of protein per sample was loaded and run by SDS-PAGE gel electrophoresis in a Mini-PROTEAN system (Bio-Rad) on an 8% acrylamide gel by stacking for 30 minutes at 60V and resolving for 1.5 hours at 130V. The proteins were transferred to a nitrocellulose membrane for 1 hour at 100V in standard transfer buffer on ice. The membranes were blocked using 5% bovine serum albumin dissolved in tris-buffered saline with Tween 20 and probed with primary (UNC5c, Abcam Cat. no ab302924) and HRP-conjugated secondary antibodies for 1 hour. a-tubulin was probed and used as loading control. The probed membranes were resolved using SuperSignal West Pico PLUS chemiluminescent substrate (ThermoFisher Cat no.34579) in a ChemiDoc MP Imaging system (Bio-Rad). Band intensity was quantified using the ChemiDoc software and all ages were normalized to the P15 age group average.

      Validation of the UNC5c antibody was performed in the lab of Dr. Liu, from whom it was kindly provided. Briefly, in the validation study the authors showed that the anti-UNC5C antibody can detect endogenous UNC5C expression and the level of UNC5C is dramatically reduced after UNC5C knockdown. The antibody can also detect the tagged-UNC5C protein in several cell lines, which was confirmed by a tag antibody (Purohit et al., 2012; Shao et al., 2017).

      References:

      Purohit AA, Li W, Qu C, Dwyer T, Shao Q, Guan K-L, Liu G. 2012. Down Syndrome Cell Adhesion Molecule (DSCAM) Associates with Uncoordinated-5C (UNC5C) in Netrin-1mediated Growth Cone Collapse. The Journal of biological chemistry 287:27126–27138. doi:10.1074/jbc.m112.340174

      Shao Q, Yang T, Huang H, Alarmanazi F, Liu G. 2017. Uncoupling of UNC5C with Polymerized TUBB3 in Microtubules Mediates Netrin-1 Repulsion. J Neurosci 37:5620–5633. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2617-16.2017

      (10) "In adolescence, dopamine neurons begin to express the repulsive Netrin-1 receptor UNC5C, and reduction in UNC5C expression appears to cause growth of mesolimbic dopamine axons to the prefrontal cortex".....This is confusing. Figure 2 shows a developmental increase in UNc5c not a decrease. So when is the "reduction in Unc5c expression" occurring?

      We apologize for the mistake in this sentence. We have corrected the relevant passage in our manuscript as follows:

      In adolescence, dopamine neurons begin to express the repulsive Netrin-1 receptor UNC5C, particularly when mesolimbic and mesocortical dopamine projections segregate in the nucleus accumbens (Manitt et al., 2010; Reynolds et al., 2018a). In contrast, dopamine axons in the prefrontal cortex do not express UNC5c except in very rare cases (Supplementary Figure 4). In adult male mice with Unc5c haploinsufficiency, there appears to be ectopic growth of mesolimbic dopamine axons to the prefrontal cortex (Auger et al., 2013). This miswiring is associated with alterations in prefrontal cortex-dependent behaviours (Auger et al., 2013).

      References:

      Auger ML, Schmidt ERE, Manitt C, Dal-Bo G, Pasterkamp RJ, Flores C. 2013. unc5c haploinsufficient phenotype: striking similarities with the dcc haploinsufficiency model. European Journal of Neuroscience 38:2853–2863. doi:10.1111/ejn.12270

      Manitt C, Labelle-Dumais C, Eng C, Grant A, Mimee A, Stroh T, Flores C. 2010. Peri-Pubertal Emergence of UNC-5 Homologue Expression by Dopamine Neurons in Rodents. PLoS ONE 5:e11463-14. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011463

      Reynolds LM, Pokinko M, Torres-Berrío A, Cuesta S, Lambert LC, Pellitero EDC, Wodzinski M, Manitt C, Krimpenfort P, Kolb B, Flores C. 2018a. DCC Receptors Drive Prefrontal Cortex Maturation by Determining Dopamine Axon Targeting in Adolescence. Biological psychiatry 83:181–192. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.06.009

      (11) In Fig 3, a statistical comparison should be made between summer male and winter male, to justify the conclusions that the winter males have delayed DA innervation.

      This analysis was also suggested by Reviewer 1, #11. Here is our response:

      We analyzed the summer and winter data together in ANOVAs separately for males and females. In both sexes we find a significant effect of daylength on dopamine innervation, interacting with age. Male age by daylength interaction: F = 6.383, p = 0.00242. Female age by daylength interaction: F = 21.872, p = 1.97 x 10-9. The full statistical analysis is available as a supplement to this letter (Response_Letter_Stats_Details.docx).

      (12) Should axon length also be measured here (Fig 3)? It is not clear why the authors have switched to varicosity density. Also, a box should be drawn in the NAC cartoon to indicate the region that was sampled.

      It is untenable to quantify axon length in the prefrontal cortex as we cannot distinguish independent axons. Rather, they are “tangled”; they twist and turn in a multitude of directions as they make contact with various dendrites. Furthermore, they branch extensively. It would therefore be impossible to accurately quantify the number of axons. Using unbiased stereology to quantify varicosities is a valid, well-characterized and straightforward alternative (Reynolds et al., 2022).

      References:

      Reynolds LM, Pantoja-Urbán AH, MacGowan D, Manitt C, Nouel D, Flores C. 2022. Dopaminergic System Function and Dysfunction: Experimental Approaches. Neuromethods 31–63. doi:10.1007/978-1-0716-2799-0_2

      (13) In Fig 3, Unc5c should be quantified to bolster the interesting finding that Unc5c expression dynamics are different between summer and winter hamsters. Unc5c mRNA experiments would also be important to see if similar changes are observed at the transcript level.

      We agree that it would be very interesting to see how UNC5c mRNA and protein levels change over time in summer and winter hamsters, both in males, as the reviewer suggests here, and in females. We are working on conducting these experiments in hamsters as part of a broader expansion of our research in this area. These experiments will require a lengthy amount of time and at this point we feel that they are beyond the scope of this manuscript.

      (14) Fig 4. The peak in exploratory behavior in winter females is counterintuitive and needs to be better discussed. IN general, the light dark behavior seems quite variable.

      This is indeed a very interesting finding, which we have expanded upon in our manuscript as follows:

      When raised under a winter-mimicking daylength, hamsters of either sex show a protracted peak in risk taking. In males, it is delayed beyond 80 days old, but the delay is substantially less in females. This is a counterintuitive finding considering that dopamine development in winter females appears to be accelerated. Our interpretation of this finding is that the timing of the risk-taking peak in females may reflect a balance between different adolescent developmental processes. The fact that dopamine axon growth is accelerated does not imply that all adolescent maturational processes are accelerated. Some may be delayed, for example those that induce axon pruning in the cortex. The timing of the risk-taking peak in winter female hamsters may therefore reflect the amalgamation of developmental processes that are advanced with those that are delayed – producing a behavioural effect that is timed somewhere in the middle. Disentangling the effects of different developmental processes on behaviour will require further experiments in hamsters, including the direct manipulation of dopamine activity in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex.

      Full Reference List

      Auger ML, Schmidt ERE, Manitt C, Dal-Bo G, Pasterkamp RJ, Flores C. 2013. unc5c haploinsufficient phenotype: striking similarities with the dcc haploinsufficiency model. European Journal of Neuroscience 38:2853–2863. doi:10.1111/ejn.12270

      Bari A, Robbins TW. 2013. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in neurobiology 108:44–79. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005

      Cuesta S, Nouel D, Reynolds LM, Morgunova A, Torres-Berrío A, White A, Hernandez G, Cooper HM, Flores C. 2020. Dopamine Axon Targeting in the Nucleus Accumbens in Adolescence Requires Netrin-1. Frontiers Cell Dev Biology 8:487. doi:10.3389/fcell.2020.00487

      Daubaras M, Bo GD, Flores C. 2014. Target-dependent expression of the netrin-1 receptor, UNC5C, in projection neurons of the ventral tegmental area. Neuroscience 260:36–46. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.007

      Eagle DM, Bari A, Robbins TW. 2008. The neuropsychopharmacology of action inhibition: crossspecies translation of the stop-signal and go/no-go tasks. Psychopharmacology 199:439– 456. doi:10.1007/s00213-008-1127-6

      Hoops D, Flores C. 2017. Making Dopamine Connections in Adolescence. Trends in Neurosciences 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2017.09.004

      Jonker FA, Jonker C, Scheltens P, Scherder EJA. 2015. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in cognition and behavior. Rev Neurosci 26:1–11. doi:10.1515/revneuro-2014-0043

      Kim B, Im H. 2019. The role of the dorsal striatum in choice impulsivity. Ann N York Acad Sci 1451:92–111. doi:10.1111/nyas.13961

      Kim D, Ackerman SL. 2011. The UNC5C Netrin Receptor Regulates Dorsal Guidance of Mouse Hindbrain Axons. J Neurosci 31:2167–2179. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5254-10.2011

      Manitt C, Labelle-Dumais C, Eng C, Grant A, Mimee A, Stroh T, Flores C. 2010. Peri-Pubertal Emergence of UNC-5 Homologue Expression by Dopamine Neurons in Rodents. PLoS ONE 5:e11463-14. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0011463

      Murcia-Belmonte V, Coca Y, Vegar C, Negueruela S, Romero C de J, Valiño AJ, Sala S, DaSilva R, Kania A, Borrell V, Martinez LM, Erskine L, Herrera E. 2019. A Retino-retinal Projection Guided by Unc5c Emerged in Species with Retinal Waves. Current Biology 29:1149-1160.e4. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2019.02.052

      Ott T, Stein AM, Nieder A. 2023. Dopamine receptor activation regulates reward expectancy signals during cognitive control in primate prefrontal neurons. Nat Commun 14:7537. doi:10.1038/s41467-023-43271-6

      Phillips RA, Tuscher JJ, Black SL, Andraka E, Fitzgerald ND, Ianov L, Day JJ. 2022. An atlas of transcriptionally defined cell populations in the rat ventral tegmental area. Cell Reports 39:110616. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2022.110616

      Purohit AA, Li W, Qu C, Dwyer T, Shao Q, Guan K-L, Liu G. 2012. Down Syndrome Cell Adhesion Molecule (DSCAM) Associates with Uncoordinated-5C (UNC5C) in Netrin-1-mediated Growth Cone Collapse. The Journal of biological chemistry 287:27126–27138. doi:10.1074/jbc.m112.340174

      Reynolds LM, Hernandez G, MacGowan D, Popescu C, Nouel D, Cuesta S, Burke S, Savell KE, Zhao J, Restrepo-Lozano JM, Giroux M, Israel S, Orsini T, He S, Wodzinski M, Avramescu RG, Pokinko M, Epelbaum JG, Niu Z, Pantoja-Urbán AH, Trudeau L-É, Kolb B, Day JJ, Flores C. 2023. Amphetamine disrupts dopamine axon growth in adolescence by a sex-specific mechanism in mice. Nat Commun 14:4035. doi:10.1038/s41467-023-39665-1

      Reynolds LM, Pantoja-Urbán AH, MacGowan D, Manitt C, Nouel D, Flores C. 2022. Dopaminergic System Function and Dysfunction: Experimental Approaches. Neuromethods 31–63. doi:10.1007/978-1-0716-2799-0_2

      Reynolds LM, Pokinko M, Torres-Berrío A, Cuesta S, Lambert LC, Pellitero EDC, Wodzinski M, Manitt C, Krimpenfort P, Kolb B, Flores C. 2018a. DCC Receptors Drive Prefrontal Cortex Maturation by Determining Dopamine Axon Targeting in Adolescence. Biological psychiatry 83:181–192. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.06.009

      Reynolds LM, Yetnikoff L, Pokinko M, Wodzinski M, Epelbaum JG, Lambert LC, Cossette M-P, Arvanitogiannis A, Flores C. 2018b. Early Adolescence is a Critical Period for the Maturation of Inhibitory Behavior. Cerebral cortex 29:3676–3686. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy247

      Schlienger S, Yam PT, Balekoglu N, Ducuing H, Michaud J-F, Makihara S, Kramer DK, Chen B, Fasano A, Berardelli A, Hamdan FF, Rouleau GA, Srour M, Charron F. 2023. Genetics of mirror movements identifies a multifunctional complex required for Netrin-1 guidance and lateralization of motor control. Sci Adv 9:eadd5501. doi:10.1126/sciadv.add5501

      Shao Q, Yang T, Huang H, Alarmanazi F, Liu G. 2017. Uncoupling of UNC5C with Polymerized TUBB3 in Microtubules Mediates Netrin-1 Repulsion. J Neurosci 37:5620–5633. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.2617-16.2017

      Srivatsa S, Parthasarathy S, Britanova O, Bormuth I, Donahoo A-L, Ackerman SL, Richards LJ, Tarabykin V. 2014. Unc5C and DCC act downstream of Ctip2 and Satb2 and contribute to corpus callosum formation. Nat Commun 5:3708. doi:10.1038/ncomms4708

      Torres-Berrío A, Lopez JP, Bagot RC, Nouel D, Dal-Bo G, Cuesta S, Zhu L, Manitt C, Eng C, Cooper HM, Storch K-F, Turecki G, Nestler EJ, Flores C. 2017. DCC Confers Susceptibility to Depression-like Behaviors in Humans and Mice and Is Regulated by miR-218. Biological psychiatry 81:306–315. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.017

      Vassilev P, Pantoja-Urban AH, Giroux M, Nouel D, Hernandez G, Orsini T, Flores C. 2021. Unique effects of social defeat stress in adolescent male mice on the Netrin-1/DCC pathway, prefrontal cortex dopamine and cognition (Social stress in adolescent vs. adult male mice). Eneuro ENEURO.0045-21.2021. doi:10.1523/eneuro.0045-21.2021

      Private Comments

      Reviewer #1

      (12) The language should be improved. Some expression is confusing (line178-179). Also some spelling errors (eg. Figure 1M).

      We have removed the word “Already” to make the sentence in lines 178-179 clearer, however we cannot find a spelling error in Figure 1M or its caption. We have further edited the manuscript for clarity and flow.

      Reviewer #2

      (1) The authors claim to have revealed how the 'timing of adolescence is programmed in the brain'. While their findings certainly shed light on molecular, circuit and behavioral processes that are unique to adolescence, their claim may be an overstatement. I suggest they refine this statement to discuss more specifically the processes they observed in the brain and animal behavior, rather than adolescence itself.

      We agree with the reviewer and have revised the manuscript to specify that we are referring to the timing of specific developmental processes that occur in the adolescent brain, not adolescence overall.

      (2) Along the same lines, the authors should also include a more substantiative discussion of how they selected their ages for investigation (for both mice and hamsters), For mice, their definition of adolescence (P21) is earlier than some (e.g. Spear L.P., Neurosci. and Beh. Reviews, 2000).

      There are certainly differences of opinion between researchers as to the precise definition of adolescence and the period it encompasses. Spear, 2000, provides one excellent discussion of the challenges related to identifying adolescence across species. This work gives specific ages only for rats, not mice (as we use here), and characterizes post-natal days 28-42 as being the conservative age range of “peak” adolescence (page 419, paragraph 1). Immediately thereafter the review states that the full adolescent period is longer than this, and it could encompass post-natal days 20-55 (page 419, paragraph 2).

      We have added the following statement to our methods:

      There is no universally accepted way to define the precise onset of adolescence. Therefore, there is no clear-cut boundary to define adolescent onset in rodents (Spear, 2000). Puberty can be more sharply defined, and puberty and adolescence overlap in time, but the terms are not interchangeable. Puberty is the onset of sexual maturation, while adolescence is a more diffuse period marked by the gradual transition from a juvenile state to independence. We, and others, suggest that adolescence in rodents spans from weaning (postnatal day 21) until adulthood, which we take to start on postnatal day 60 (Reynolds and Flores, 2021). We refer to “early adolescence” as the first two weeks postweaning (postnatal days 21-34). These ranges encompass discrete DA developmental periods (Kalsbeek et al., 1988; Manitt et al., 2011; Reynolds et al., 2018a), vulnerability to drug effects on DA circuitry (Hammerslag and Gulley, 2014; Reynolds et al., 2018a), and distinct behavioral characteristics (Adriani and Laviola, 2004; Makinodan et al., 2012; Schneider, 2013; Wheeler et al., 2013).

      References:

      Adriani W, Laviola G. 2004. Windows of vulnerability to psychopathology and therapeutic strategy in the adolescent rodent model. Behav Pharmacol 15:341–352. doi:10.1097/00008877-200409000-00005

      Hammerslag LR, Gulley JM. 2014. Age and sex differences in reward behavior in adolescent and adult rats. Dev Psychobiol 56:611–621. doi:10.1002/dev.21127

      Hoops D, Flores C. 2017. Making Dopamine Connections in Adolescence. Trends in Neurosciences 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2017.09.004

      Kalsbeek A, Voorn P, Buijs RM, Pool CW, Uylings HBM. 1988. Development of the Dopaminergic Innervation in the Prefrontal Cortex of the Rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 269:58–72. doi:10.1002/cne.902690105

      Makinodan M, Rosen KM, Ito S, Corfas G. 2012. A critical period for social experiencedependent oligodendrocyte maturation and myelination. Science 337:1357–1360. doi:10.1126/science.1220845

      Manitt C, Mimee A, Eng C, Pokinko M, Stroh T, Cooper HM, Kolb B, Flores C. 2011. The Netrin Receptor DCC Is Required in the Pubertal Organization of Mesocortical Dopamine Circuitry. J Neurosci 31:8381–8394. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0606-11.2011

      Reynolds LM, Flores C. 2021. Mesocorticolimbic Dopamine Pathways Across Adolescence: Diversity in Development. Front Neural Circuit 15:735625. doi:10.3389/fncir.2021.735625

      Reynolds LM, Yetnikoff L, Pokinko M, Wodzinski M, Epelbaum JG, Lambert LC, Cossette MP, Arvanitogiannis A, Flores C. 2018. Early Adolescence is a Critical Period for the Maturation of Inhibitory Behavior. Cerebral cortex 29:3676–3686. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy247

      Schneider M. 2013. Adolescence as a vulnerable period to alter rodent behavior. Cell and tissue research 354:99–106. Doi:10.1007/s00441-013-1581-2

      Spear LP. 2000. Neurobehavioral Changes in Adolescence. Current directions in psychological science 9:111–114. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00072

      Wheeler AL, Lerch JP, Chakravarty MM, Friedel M, Sled JG, Fletcher PJ, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW. 2013. Adolescent Cocaine Exposure Causes Enduring Macroscale Changes in Mouse Brain Structure. J Neurosci 33:1797–1803. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3830-12.2013

      (3) Figure 1 - the conclusions hinge on the Netrin-1 staining, as shown in panel G, but the cells are difficult to see. It would be helpful to provide clearer, more zoomed images so readers can better assess the staining. Since Netrin-1 expression reduces dramatically after P4 and they had to use antigen retrieval to see signal, it would be helpful to show some images from additional brain regions and ages to see if expression levels follow predicted patterns. For instance, based on the allen brain atlas, it seems that around P21, there should be high levels of Netrin-1 in the cerebellum, but low levels in the cortex. These would be nice controls to demonstrate the specificity and sensitivity of the antibody in older tissue.

      We do not study the cerebellum and have never stained this region; doing so now would require generating additional tissue and we’re not sure it would add enough to the information provided to be worthwhile. Note that we have stained the forebrain for Netrin-1 previously, providing broad staining of many brain regions (Manitt et al., 2011)

      References:

      Manitt C, Mimee A, Eng C, Pokinko M, Stroh T, Cooper HM, Kolb B, Flores C. 2011. The Netrin Receptor DCC Is Required in the Pubertal Organization of Mesocortical Dopamine Circuitry. J Neurosci 31:8381–8394. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0606-11.2011

      (4) Figure 3 - Because mice tend to avoid brightly-lit spaces, the light/dark box is more commonly used as a measure of anxiety-like behavior than purely exploratory behavior (including in the paper they cited). It is important to address this possibility in their discussion of their findings. To bolster their conclusions about the coincidence of circuit and behavioral changes in adolescent hamsters, it would be useful to add an additional measure of exploratory behaviors (e.g. hole board).

      Regarding the light/dark box test, this is an excellent point. We prefer the term “risk taking” to “anxiety-like” and now use the former term in our manuscript. Furthermore, our interest in the behaviour is purely to chart the development of adolescent behaviour across our treatment groups, not to study a particular emotional state. Regardless of the specific emotion or emotions governing the light/dark box behaviour, it is an ideal test for charting adolescent shifts in behaviour as it is well-characterized in this respect, as we discuss in our manuscript.

      (5) Supplementary Figure 4,5 The authors defined puberty onset using uterine and testes weights in hamsters. While the weights appear to be different for summer and winter hamsters, there were no statistical comparison. Please add statistical analyses to bolster claims about puberty start times. Also, as many studies use vaginal opening to define puberty onset, it would be helpful to discuss how these measurements typically align and cite relevant literature that described use of uterine weights. Also, Supplementary Figures 4 and 5 were mis-cited as Supp. Fig. 2 in the text (e.g. line 317 and others).

      These are great suggestions. We have added statistical analyses to Supplementary Figures 5 and 6 and provided Vaginal Opening data as Supplementary Figure 7. The statistical analyses confirm that all three characters are delayed in winter hamsters compared to summer hamsters.

      We have also added the following references to the manuscript:

      Darrow JM, Davis FC, Elliott JA, Stetson MH, Turek FW, Menaker M. 1980. Influence of Photoperiod on Reproductive Development in the Golden Hamster. Biol Reprod 22:443–450. doi:10.1095/biolreprod22.3.443

      Ebling FJP. 1994. Photoperiodic Differences during Development in the Dwarf Hamsters Phodopus sungorus and Phodopus campbelli. Gen Comp Endocrinol 95:475–482. doi:10.1006/gcen.1994.1147

      Timonin ME, Place NJ, Wanderi E, Wynne-Edwards KE. 2006. Phodopus campbelli detect reduced photoperiod during development but, unlike Phodopus sungorus, retain functional reproductive physiology. Reproduction 132:661–670. doi:10.1530/rep.1.00019

      (6) The font in many figure panels is small and hard to read (e.g. 1A,D,E,H,I,L...). Please increase the size for legibility.

      We have increased the font size of our figure text throughout the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3

      (15) Fig 1 C,D. Clarify the units of the y axis

      We have now fixed this.

      Full Reference List

      Adriani W, Laviola G. 2004. Windows of vulnerability to psychopathology and therapeutic strategy in the adolescent rodent model. Behav Pharmacol 15:341–352. doi:10.1097/00008877-200409000-00005

      Hammerslag LR, Gulley JM. 2014. Age and sex differences in reward behavior in adolescent and adult rats. Dev Psychobiol 56:611–621. doi:10.1002/dev.21127

      Hoops D, Flores C. 2017. Making Dopamine Connections in Adolescence. Trends in Neurosciences 1–11. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2017.09.004

      Kalsbeek A, Voorn P, Buijs RM, Pool CW, Uylings HBM. 1988. Development of the Dopaminergic Innervation in the Prefrontal Cortex of the Rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology 269:58–72. doi:10.1002/cne.902690105

      Makinodan M, Rosen KM, Ito S, Corfas G. 2012. A critical period for social experiencedependent oligodendrocyte maturation and myelination. Science 337:1357–1360. doi:10.1126/science.1220845

      Manitt C, Mimee A, Eng C, Pokinko M, Stroh T, Cooper HM, Kolb B, Flores C. 2011. The Netrin Receptor DCC Is Required in the Pubertal Organization of Mesocortical Dopamine Circuitry. J Neurosci 31:8381–8394. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0606-11.2011

      Reynolds LM, Flores C. 2021. Mesocorticolimbic Dopamine Pathways Across Adolescence: Diversity in Development. Front Neural Circuit 15:735625. doi:10.3389/fncir.2021.735625 Reynolds LM, Yetnikoff L, Pokinko M, Wodzinski M, Epelbaum JG, Lambert LC, Cossette M-P, Arvanitogiannis A, Flores C. 2018. Early Adolescence is a Critical Period for the Maturation of Inhibitory Behavior. Cerebral cortex 29:3676–3686. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhy247

      Schneider M. 2013. Adolescence as a vulnerable period to alter rodent behavior. Cell and tissue research 354:99–106. doi:10.1007/s00441-013-1581-2

      Spear LP. 2000. Neurobehavioral Changes in Adolescence. Current directions in psychological science 9:111–114. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00072

      Wheeler AL, Lerch JP, Chakravarty MM, Friedel M, Sled JG, Fletcher PJ, Josselyn SA, Frankland PW. 2013. Adolescent Cocaine Exposure Causes Enduring Macroscale Changes in Mouse Brain Structure. J Neurosci 33:1797–1803. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3830-12.2013

    1. Author Response

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      eLife assessment

      This important study combines a range of advanced ultrastructural imaging approaches to define the unusual endosomal system of African trypanosomes. Compelling images show that instead of a distinct set of compartments, the endosome of these protists comprises a continuous system of membranes with functionally distinct subdomains as defined by canonical markers of early, late and recycling endosomes. The findings suggest that the endocytic system of bloodstream stages has evolved to facilitate the extraordinarily high rates of membrane turnover needed to remove immune complexes and survive in the blood, which is of interest to anyone studying infectious diseases.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Bloodstream stages of the parasitic protist, Trypanosoma brucei, exhibit very high rates of constitutive endocytosis, which is needed to recycle the surface coat of Variant Surface Glycoproteins (VSGs) and remove surface immune complexes. While many studies have shown that the endo-lysosomal systems of T. brucei BF stages contain canonical domains, as defined by classical Rab markers, it has remained unclear whether these protists have evolved additional adaptations/mechanisms for sustaining these very high rates of membrane transport and protein sorting. The authors have addressed this question by reconstructing the 3D ultrastructure and functional domains of the T. brucei BF endosome membrane system using advanced electron tomography and super-resolution microscopy approaches. Their studies reveal that, unusually, the BF endosome network comprises a continuous system of cisternae and tubules that contain overlapping functional subdomains. It is proposed that a continuous membrane system allows higher rates of protein cargo segregation, sorting and recycling than can otherwise occur when transport between compartments is mediated by membrane vesicles or other fusion events.

      Strengths:

      The study is a technical tour-de-force using a combination of electron tomography, super-resolution/expansion microscopy, immune-EM of cryo-sections to define the 3D structures and connectivity of different endocytic compartments. The images are very clear and generally support the central conclusion that functionally distinct endocytic domains occur within a dynamic and continuous endosome network in BF stages.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors suggest that this dynamic endocytic network may also fulfil many of the functions of the Golgi TGN and that the latter may be absent in these stages. Although plausible, this comment needs further experimental support. For example, have the authors attempted to localize canonical makers of the TGN (e.g. GRIP proteins) in T. brucei BF and/or shown that exocytic carriers bud directly from the endosomes?

      We agree with the criticism and have shortened the discussion accordingly and clearly marked it as speculation. However, we do not want to completely abandon our hypothesis.

      The paragraph now reads:

      Lines 740 – 751:

      “Interestingly, we did not find any structural evidence of vesicular retrograde transport to the Golgi. Instead, the endosomal ‘highways’ extended throughout the posterior volume of the trypanosomes approaching the trans-Golgi interface. It is highly plausible that this region represents the convergence point where endocytic and biosynthetic membrane trafficking pathways merge. A comparable merging of endocytic and biosynthetic functions has been described for the TGN in plants. Different marker proteins for early and recycling endosomes were shown to be associated and/ or partially colocalized with the TGN suggesting its function in both secretory and endocytic pathways (reviewed in Minamino and Ueda, 2019). As we could not find structural evidence for the existence of a TGN we tentatively propose that trypanosomes may have shifted the central orchestrating function of the TGN as a sorting hub at the crossroads of biosynthetic and recycling pathways to the endosome. Although this is a speculative scenario, it is experimentally testable.”

      Furthermore, we removed the lines 51 - 52, which included the suggestion of the TGN as a master regulator, from the abstract.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The authors suggest that the African trypanosome endomembrane system has unusual organisation, in that the entire system is a single reticulated structure. It is not clear if this is thought to extend to the lysosome or MVB. There is also a suggestion that this unusual morphology serves as a trans-(post)Golgi network rather than the more canonical arrangement.

      The work is based around very high-quality light and electron microscopy, as well as utilising several marker proteins, Rab5A, 11 and 7. These are deemed as markers for early endosomes, recycling endosomes and late or pre-lysosomes. The images are mostly of high quality but some inconsistencies in the interpretation, appearance of structures and some rather sweeping assumptions make this less easy to accept. Two perhaps major issues are claims to label the entire endosomal apparatus with a single marker protein, which is hard to accept as certainly this reviewer does not really even know where the limits to the endosomal network reside and where these interface with other structures. There are several additional compartments that have been defined by Rob proteins as well, and which are not even mentioned. Overall I am unconvinced that the authors have demonstrated the main things they claim.<br /> The endomembrane system in bloodstream form T. brucei is clearly delimited. Compared to mammalian cells it is tidy and confined to the posterior part of the spindleshaped cell. The endoplasmic reticulum is linked to one side of the longitudinal cell axis, marked by the attached flagellum, while the mitochondrion locates to the opposite side. Glycosomes are easily identifiable as spheres, as are acidocalcisomes, which are smaller than glycosomes and – in electron micrographs – are characterized by high electron density. All these organelles extend beyond the nucleus, which is not the case for the endosomal compartment, the lysosome and the Golgi. The vesicles found in the posterior half of the trypanosome cell are quantitatively identifiable as COP1, CCVI or CCVII vesicles, or exocytic carriers. The lysosome has a higher degree of morphological plasticity, but this is not topic of the present work. Thus, the endomembrane system in T. brucei is comparatively well structured and delimited, which is why we have chosen trypanosomes as cell biological model.

      We have published EP1::GFP as marker for the endosome system and flagellar pocket back in 2004. We have defined the fluid phase volume of the trypanosome endosome in papers published between 2002 and 2007. This work was not intended to represent the entirety of RAB proteins. We were only interested in 3 canonical markers for endosome subtypes. We do not claim anything that is not experimentally tested, we have clearly labelled our hypotheses as such, and we do not make sweeping assumptions.

      The approaches taken are state-of-the-art but not novel, and because of the difficulty in fully addressing the central tenet, I am not sure how much of an impact this will have beyond the trypanosome field. For certain this is limited to workers in the direct area and is not a generalisable finding.

      To the best of our knowledge, there is no published research that has employed 3D Tokuyasu or expansion microscopy (ExM) to label endosomes. The key takeaway from our study, which is the concept that "endosomes are continuous in trypanosomes" certainly is novel. We are not aware of any other report that has demonstrated this aspect.

      The doubts formulated by the reviewer regarding the impact of our work beyond the field of trypanosomes are not timely. Indeed, our results, and those of others, show that the conclusions drawn from work with just a few model organisms is not generalisable. We are finally on the verge of a new cell biology that considers the plethora of evolutionary solutions beyond ophistokonts. We believe that this message should be widely acknowledged and considered. And we are certainly not the only ones who are convinced that the term "general relevance" is unscientific and should no longer be used in biology.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      As clearly highlighted by the authors, a key plank in the ability of trypanosomes to evade the mammalian host’s immune system is its high rate of endocytosis. This rapid turnover of its surface enables the trypanosome to ‘clean’ its surface removing antibodies and other immune effectors that are subsequently degraded. The high rate of endocytosis is likely reflected in the organisati’n and layout of the endosomal system in these parasites. Here, Link et al., sought to address this question using a range of light and three-dimensional electron microscopy approaches to define the endosomal organisation in this parasite.

      Before this study, the vast majority of our information about the make-up of the trypanosome endosomal system was from thin-section electron microscopy and immunofluorescence studies, which did not provide the necessary resolution and 3D information to address this issue. Therefore, it was not known how the different structures observed by EM were related. Link et al., have taken advantage of the advances in technology and used an impressive combination of approaches at the LM and EM level to study the endosomal system in these parasites. This innovative combination has now shown the interconnected-ness of this network and demonstrated that there are no ‘classical’ compartments within the endosomal system, with instead different regions of the network enriched in different protein markers (Rab5a, Rab7, Rab11).

      Strengths:

      This is a generally well-written and clear manuscript, with the data well-presented supporting the majority of the conclusions of the authors. The authors use an impressive range of approaches to address the organisation of the endosomal system and the development of these methods for use in trypanosomes will be of use to the wider parasitology community.

      I appreciate their inclusion of how they used a range of different light microscopy approaches even though for instance the dSTORM approach did not turn out to be as effective as hoped. The authors have clearly demonstrated that trypanosomes have a large interconnected endosomal network, without defined compartments and instead show enrichment for specific Rabs within this network.

      Weaknesses:

      My concerns are:

      i) There is no evidence for functional compartmentalisation. The classical markers of different endosomal compartments do not fully overlap but there is no evidence to show a region enriched in one or other of these proteins has that specific function. The authors should temper their conclusions about this point.

      The reviewer is right in stating that Rab-presence does not necessarily mean Rabfunction. However, this assumption is as old as the Rab literature. That is why we have focused on the 3 most prominent endosomal marker proteins. We report that for endosome function you do not necessarily need separate membrane compartments. This is backed by our experiments.

      ii) The quality of the electron microscopy work is very high but there is a general lack of numbers. For example, how many tomograms were examined? How often were fenestrated sheets seen? Can the authors provide more information about how frequent these observations were?

      The fenestrated sheets can be seen in the majority of the 37 tomograms recorded of the posterior volume of the parasites. Furthermore, we have randomly generated several hundred tiled (= very large) electron micrographs of bloodstream form trypanosomes for unbiased analyses of endomembranes. In these 2D-datasets the “footprint” of the fenestrated flat and circular cisternae is frequently detectable in the posterior cell area.

      We now have included the corresponding numbers in all EM figure legends.

      iii) The EM work always focussed on cells which had been processed before fixing. Now, I understand this was important to enable tracers to be used. However, given the dynamic nature of the system these processing steps and feeding experiments may have affected the endosomal organisation. Given their knowledge of the system now, the authors should fix some cells directly in culture to observe whether the organisation of the endosome aligns with their conclusions here.

      This is a valid criticism; however, it is the cell culture that provides an artificial environment. As for a possible effect of cell harvesting by centrifugation on the integrity and functionality of the endosome system, we consider this very unlikely for one simple reason. The mechanical forces acting in and on the parasites as they circulate in the extremely crowded and confined environment of the mammalian bloodstream are obviously much higher than the centrifugal forces involved in cell preparation. This becomes particularly clear when one considers that the mass of the particle to be centrifuged determines the actual force exerted by the g-forces. Nevertheless, the proposed experiment is a good control, although much more complex than proposed, since tomography is a challenging technique. We have performed the suggested experiment and acquired tomograms of unprocessed cells. The corresponding data is now included as supplementary movie 2, 3 and 4. We refer to it in lines 202 – 206: To investigate potential impacts of processing steps (cargo uptake, centrifugation, washing) on endosomal organization, we directly fixed cells in the cell culture flask, embedded them in Epon, and conducted tomography. The resulting tomograms revealed endosomal organization consistent with that observed in cells fixed after processing (see Supplementary movie 2, 3, and 4).

      We furthermore thank the reviewer for the experiment suggestion in the acknowledgments.

      iv) The discussion needs to be revamped. At the moment it is just another run through of the results and does not take an overview of the results presenting an integrated view. Moreover, it contains reference to data that was not presented in the results.

      We have improved the discussion accordingly.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      The reviewers concurred about the high calibre of the work and the importance of the findings.

      They raised some issues and made some suggestions to improve the paper without additional experiments - key issues include

      (1) Better referencing of the trypanosome endocytosis/ lysosomal trafficking literature.

      The literature, especially the experimental and quantitative work, is very limited. We now provide a more complete set of references. However, we would like to mention that we had cited a recent review that critically references the trypanosome literature with emphasis on the extensive work done with mammalian cells and yeast.

      (2) Moving the dSTORM data that detracts from otherwise strong data in a supplementary figure.

      We have done this.

      (3) Removal of the conclusion that the continuous endosome fulfils the functions of TGN, without further evidence.

      As stated above, this was not a conclusion in our paper, but rather a speculation, which we have now more clearly marked as such. Lines 740 to 751 now read:

      “Interestingly, we did not find any structural evidence of vesicular retrograde transport to the Golgi. Instead, the endosomal ‘highways’ extended throughout the posterior volume of the trypanosomes approaching the trans-Golgi interface. It is highly plausible that this region represents the convergence point where endocytic and biosynthetic membrane trafficking pathways merge. A comparable merging of endocytic and biosynthetic functions was already described for the TGN in plants. Different marker proteins for early and recycling endosomes were shown to be associated and/ or partially colocalized with the TGN suggesting its function in both secretory and endocytic pathways (reviewed in Minamino and Ueda, 2019). As we could not find structural evidence for the existence of a TGN we tentatively propose that trypanosomes may have shifted the central orchestrating function of the TGN as a sorting hub at the crossroads of biosynthetic and recycling pathways to the endosome. Although this is a speculative scenario, it is experimentally testable.”

      (4) Broader discussion linking their findings to other examples of organelle maturation in eukaryotes (e.g cisternal maturation of the Golgi)

      We have improved the discussion accordingly.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      What are the multi-vesicular vesicles that surround the marked endosomal compartments in Fig 1. Do they become labelled with fluid phase markers with longer incubations (e.g late endosome/ lysosomal)?

      The function of MVBs in trypanosomes is still far from being clear. They are filled with fluid phase cargo, especially ferritin, but are devoid of VSG. Hence it is likely that MVBs are part of the lysosomal compartment. In fact, this part of the endomembrane system is highly dynamic. MVBs can be physically connected to the lysosome or can form elongated structures. The surprising dynamics of the trypanosome lysosome will be published elsewhere.

      Figure 2. The compartments labelled with EP1::Halo are very poorly defined due to the low levels of expression of the reporter protein and/or sensitivity of detection of the Halo tag. Based on these images, it would be hard to conclude whether the endosome network is continuous or not. In this respect, it is unclear why the authors didn't use EP1-GFP for these analyses? Given the other data that provides more compelling evidence for a single continuous compartment, I would suggest removing Fig 2A.

      We have used EP1::GFP to label the entire endosome system (Engstler and Boshart, 2004). Unfortunately, GFP is not suited for dSTORM imaging. By creating the EP1::Halo cell line, we were able to utilize the most prominent dSTORM fluorescent dye, Alexa 647. This was not primarily done to generate super resolution images, but rather to measure the dynamics of the GPI-anchored, luminal protein EP with single molecule precision. The results from this study will be published separately. But we agree with the reviewer and have relocated the dSTORM data to the supplementary material.

      The observation that Rab5a/7 can be detected in the lumen of lysosome is interesting. Mechanistically, this presumably occurs by invagination of the limiting membrane of the lysosome. Is there any evidence that similar invagination of cytoplasmic markers occurs throughout or in subdomains of the endocytic network (possibly indicative of a 'late endosome' domain)?

      So far, we have not observed this. The structure of the lysosome and the membrane influx from the endosome are currently being investigated.

      The authors note that continuity of functionally distinct membrane compartments in the secretory/endocytic pathways has been reported in other protists (e.g T. cruzi). A particular example that could be noted is the endo-lysosomal system of Dictyostelium discoideum which mediates the continuous degradation and eventual expulsion of undigested material.

      We tried to include this in the discussion but ultimately decided against it because the Dictyostelium system cannot be easily compared to the trypanosome endosome.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Abstract

      Not sure that 'common' is the correct term here. Frequent, near-universal..... it would be true that endocytosis is common across most eukaryotes.

      We have changed the sentence to “common process observed in most eukaryotes” (line 33).

      Immune evasion - the parasite does not escape the immune system, but does successfully avoid its impact, at least at the population level.

      We have replaced the word “escape” with “evasion” (line 35).

      The third sentence needs to follow on correctly from the second. Also, more than Igs are internalised and potentially part of immune evasion, such as C3, Factor H, ApoL1 etcetera.

      We believe that there may be a misunderstanding here. The process of endocytic uptake and lysosomal degradation has so far only been demonstrated in the context of VSGbound antibodies, which is why we only refer to this. Of course, the immune system comprises a wide range of proteins and effector molecules, all of which could be involved in immune evasion.

      I do not follow the logic that the high flux through the endocytic system in trypanosomes precludes distinct compartmentalisation - one could imagine a system where a lot of steps become optimised for example. This idea needs expanding on if it is correct.

      Membrane transport by vesicle transfer between several separate membrane compartments would be slower than the measured rate of membrane flux.

      Again I am not sure 'efficient' on line 40. It is fast, but how do you measure efficiency? Speed and efficiency are not the same thing.

      We have replaced the word “efficient” with “fast” (line 42).

      The basis for suggesting endosomes as a TGN is unclear. Given that there are AP complexes, retromer, exocyst and other factors that are part of the TGN or at least post-G differentiation of pathways in canonical systems, this seems a step too far. There really is no evidence in the rest of the MS that seems to support this.

      Yes, we agree and have clarified the discussion accordingly. We have not completely removed the discussion on the TGN but have labelled it more clearly as speculation.

      I am aware I am being pedantic here, but overall the abstract seems to provide an impression of greater novelty than may be the case and makes several very bold claims that I cannot see as fully valid.

      We are not aware of any claim in the summary that we have not substantiated with experiments, or any hypothesis that we have not explained.

      Moreover, the concept of fused or multifunctional endosomes (or even other endomembrane compartments) is old, and has been demonstrated in metazoan cells and yeast. The concept of rigid (in terms of composition) compartments really has been rejected by most folks with maturation, recycling and domain structures already well-established models and concepts.

      We agree that the (transient) presence of multiple Rab proteins decorating endosomes has been demonstrated in various cell types. This finding formed the basis for the endosomal maturation model in mammals and yeast, which has replaced the previous rigid compartment model.

      However, we do not appreciate attempts to question the originality of our study by claiming that similar observations have been made in metazoans or yeast. This is simply wrong. There are no reports of a functionally structured, continuous, single and large endosome in any other system. The only membrane system that might be similar was described in the American parasite Trypanosoma cruzi, however, without the use of endosome markers or any functional analysis. We refer to this study in the discussion.

      In summary, the maturation model falls short in explaining the intricacies of the membrane system we have uncovered in trypanosomes. Therefore, one plausible interpretation of our data is that the overall architecture of the trypanosome endosomes represents an adaptation that enables the remarkable speed of plasma membrane recycling observed in these parasites. In our view, both our findings and their interpretation are novel and worth reporting. Again, modern cell biology should recognize that evolution has developed many solutions for similar processes in cells, about whose diversity we have learned almost nothing because of our reductionist view. A remarkable example of this are the Picozoa, tiny bipartite eukaryotes that pack the entire nutritional apparatus into one pouch and the main organelles with the locomotor system into the other. Another one is the “extreme” cell biology of many protozoan parasites such as Giardia, Toxpoplasma or Trypanosoma.

      Higher plants have been well characterised, especially at the level of Rab/Arf proteins and adaptins.

      We now mention plant endosomes in our brief discussion of the trypanosome TGN. Lines 744 – 747:

      “A comparable merging of endocytic and biosynthetic functions was already described for the TGN in plants. Different marker proteins for early and recycling endosomes were shown to be associated and/ or partially colocalized with the TGN suggesting its function in both secretory and endocytic pathways (reviewed in Minamino and Ueda, 2019).”

      The level of self-citing in the introduction is irritating and unscholarly. I have no qualms with crediting the authors with their own excellent contributions, but work from Dacks, Bangs, Field and others seems to be selectively ignored, with an awkward use of the authors' own publications. Diversity between organisms for example has been a mainstay of the Dacks lab output, Rab proteins and others from Field and work on exocytosis and late endosomal systems from Bangs. These efforts and contributions surely deserve some recognition?

      This is an original article and not a review. For a comprehensive overview the reviewer might read our recent overview article on exo- and endocytic pathways in trypanosomes, in which we have extensively cited the work of Mark Field, Jay Bangs and Joel Dacks. In the present manuscript, we have cited all papers that touch on our results or are otherwise important for a thorough understanding of our hypotheses. We do not believe that this approach is unscientific, but rather improves the readability of the manuscript. Nevertheless, we have now cited additional work.

      For the uninitiated, the posterior/anterior axis of the trypanosome cell as well as any other specific features should be defined.

      In lines 102 - 110 we wrote:

      “This process of antibody clearance is driven by hydrodynamic drag forces resulting from the continuous directional movement of trypanosomes (Engstler et al., 2007). The VSG-antibody complexes on the cell surface are dragged against the swimming direction of the parasite and accumulate at the posterior pole of the cell. This region harbours an invagination in the plasma membrane known as the flagellar pocket (FP) (Gull, 2003; Overath et al., 1997). The FP, which marks the origin of the single attached flagellum, is the exclusive site for endo- and exocytosis in trypanosomes (Gull, 2003; Overath et al., 1997). Consequently, the accumulation of VSG-antibody complexes occurs precisely in the area of bulk membrane uptake.”

      We think this sufficiently introduces the cell body axes.

      I don't understand the comment concerning microtubule association. In mammalian cells, such association is well established, but compartments still do not display precise positioning. This likely then has nothing to do with the microtubule association differences.

      We have clarified this in the text (lines 192 – 199). There is no report of cytoplasmic microtubules in trypanosomes. All microtubules appear to be either subpellicular or within the flagellum. To maintain the structure and position of the endosomal apparatus, they should be associated either with subpellicular microtubules, as is the case with the endoplasmic reticulum, or with the more enigmatic actomyosin system of the parasites. We have been working on the latter possibility and intend to publish a follow-up paper to the present manuscript.

      The inability to move past the nucleus is a poor explanation. These compartments are dynamic. Even the nucleus does interesting things in trypanosomes and squeezes past structures during development in the tsetse fly.

      The distance between the nucleus and the microtubule cytoskeleton remains relatively constant even in parasites that squeeze through microfluidic channels. This is not unexpected as the nucleus can be highly deformed. A structure the size of the endosome will not be able to physically pass behind the nucleus without losing its integrity. In fact, the recycling apparatus is never found in the anterior part of the trypanosome, most probably because the flagellar pocket is located at the posterior cell pole.

      L253 What is the evidence that EP1 labels the entire FP and endosomes? This may be extensive, but this claim requires rather more evidence. This is again suggested at l263. Again, please forgive me for being pedantic, but this is an overstatement unless supported by evidence that would be incredibly difficult to obtain. This is even sort of acknowledged on l271 in the context of non-uniform labelling. This comes again in l336.

      The evidence that EP1 labels the entire FP and endosomes is presented here: Engstler and Boshart, 2004; 10.1101/gad.323404).

      Perhaps I should refrain from comments on the dangers of expansion microscopy, or asking what has actually been gained here. Oddly, the conclusion on l290 is a fair statement that I am happy with.

      An in-depth discussion regarding the advantages and disadvantages of expansion microscopy is beyond the manuscript's intended scope. Our approach involved utilizing various imaging techniques to confirm the validity of our findings. We appreciate that our concluding sentence is pleasing.

      F2 - The data in panel A seem quite poor to me. I also do not really understand why the DAPI stain in the first and second columns fails to coincide or why the kinetoplast is so diffuse in the second row. The labelling for EP1 presents as very small puncta, and hence is not evidence for a continuum. What is the arrow in A IV top? The data in panel B are certainly more in line with prior art, albeit that there is considerable heterogeneity in the labelling and of the FP for example. Again, I cannot really see this as evidence for continuity. There are gaps.... Albeit I accept that labelling of such structures is unlikely to ever be homogenous.

      We agree that the dSTORM data represents the least robust aspect of the findings we have presented, and we concur with relocating it to the supplementary material.

      F3 - Rather apparent, and specifically for Rab7, that there is differential representation - for example, Cell 4 presents a single Rab7 structure while the remaining examples demonstrate more extensive labelling. Again, I am content that these are highly dynamic strictures but this needs to be addressed at some level and commented upon. If the claim is for continuity, the dynamics observed here suggest the usual; some level of obvious overlap of organellar markers, but the representation in F3 is clever but not sure what I am looking at. Moreover, the title of the figure is nothing new. What is also a bit odd is that the extent of the Rab7 signal, and to some extent the other two Rabs used, is rather variable, which makes this unclear to me as to what is being detected. Given that the Rab proteins may be defining microdomains or regions, I would also expect a region of unique straining as well as the common areas. This needs to at least be discussed.

      The differences in the representation result from the dynamics of the labelled structures. Therefore, we have selected different cells to provide examples of what the labelling can look like. We now mention this in the results section.

      The overlap of the different Rab signals was perhaps to be expected, but we now have demonstrated it experimentally. Importantly, we performed a rigorous quantification by calculating the volume overlaps and the Pearson correlation coefficients.

      In previous studies the data were presented as maximal intensity projections, which inherently lack the complete 3D information.

      We found that Rab proteins define microdomains and that there are regions of unique staining as well as common areas, as shown in Figure 3. The volumes do not completely overlap. This is now more clearly stated in lines 315 – 319:

      “These objects showed areas of unique staining as well as partially overlapping regions. The pairwise colocalization of different endosomal markers is shown in Figure 3 A, XI - XIII and 3 B. The different cells in Figure 3 B were selected to represent the dynamic nature of the labelled structures. Consequently, the selected cells provide a variety of examples of how the labelling can appear.”

      This had already been stated in lines 331 – 336:

      “In summary, the quantitative colocalization analyses revealed that on the one hand, the endosomal system features a high degree of connectivity, with considerable overlap of endosomal marker regions, and on the other hand, TbRab5A, TbRab7, and TbRab11 also demarcate separated regions in that system. These results can be interpreted as evidence of a continuous endosomal membrane system harbouring functional subdomains, with a limited amount of potentially separated early, late or recycling endosomes.”

      F4-6 - Fabulous images. But a couple of issues here; first, as the authors point out, there is distance between the gold and the antigen. So, this of course also works in the z-plane as well as the x/y-planes and some of the gold may well be associated with membraneous figures that are out of the plane, which would indicate an absence of colinearity on one specific membrane. Secondly, in several instances, we have Rab7 essentially mixed with Rab11 or Rab5 positive membrane. While data are data and should be accepted, this is difficult to reconcile when, at least to some level, Rab7 is a marker for a late-endosomal structure and where the presence of degradative activity could reside. As division of function is, I assume, the major reason for intracellular compartmentalisation, such a level of admixture is hard to rationalise. A continuum is one thing but the data here seem to be suggesting something else, i.e. almost complete admixture.

      We are grateful for the positive feedback regarding the image quality. It is true that the "linkage error," representing the distance between the gold and the antigen, also functions to some extent in the z-axis. However, it's important to note that the zdimension of the section in these Figures is 55 nm. Nevertheless, it's interesting to observe that membranes, which may not be visible within the section itself but likely the corresponding Rab antigen, is discernible in Figure 4C (indicated by arrows).

      We have clarified this in lines 397 – 400:

      “Consequently, gold particles located further away may represent cytoplasmic TbRab proteins or, as the “linkage error” can also occur in the z-plane, correspond to membranes that are not visible within the 55 nm thickness of the cryosection (Figure 4, panel C, arrows). “

      The coexistence of different Rabs is most likely concentrated in regions where transitions between different functions are likely. Our focus was primarily on imaging membranes labelled with two markers. We wanted to show that the prevailing model of separate compartments in the trypanosome literature is not correct.

      F7 - Not sure what this adds beyond what was published by Grunfelder.

      First, this figure is an important control that links our results to published work (Grünfelder et al. (2003)). Second, we include double staining of cargo with Rab5, Rab7, and Rab11, whereas Grünfelder focused only on Rab11. Therefore, our data is original and of such high quality that it warrants a main figure.

      F8 - and l583. This is odd as the claim is 'proof' which in science is a hard thing to claim (and this is definitely not at a six sigma level of certainty, as used by the physics community). However, I am seeing structures in the tomograms which are not contiguous - there are gaps here between the individual features (Green in the figure).

      We have replaced the term "proof". It is important to note that the structures in individual tomograms cannot all be completely continuous because the sections are limited to a thickness of 250 nm. Therefore, it is likely that they have more connectivity above and below the imaged section. Nevertheless, we believe that the quality of the tomograms is satisfactory, considering that 3D Tokuyasu is a very demanding technique and the production of serial Tokuyasu tomograms is not feasible in practice.

      Discussion - Too long and the self-citing of four papers from the corresponding author to the exclusion of much prior work is again noted, with concerns about this as described above. Moreover, at least four additional Rab proteins are known associated with the trypanosome endosomal system, 4, 5B, 21 and 28. These have been completely ignored.

      We have outlined our position on referencing in original articles above. We also explained why we focused on the key marker proteins associated with early (Rab5), late (Rab7) and recycling endosomes (Rab11). We did not ignore the other Rabs, we just did not include them in the present study.

      Overall this is disappointing. I had expected a more robust analysis, with a clearer discussion and placement in context. I am not fully convinced that what we have here is as extreme as claimed, or that we have a substantial advance. There is nothing here that is mechanistic or the identification of a new set of gene products, process or function.

      We do not think that this is constructive feedback.

      This MS suggests that the endosomal system of African trypanosomes is a continuum of membrane structures rather than representing a set of distinct compartments. A combination of light and electron microscopy methods are used in support. The basic contention is very challenging to prove, and I'm not convinced that this has been. Furthermore, I am also unclear as to the significance of such an organisation; this seems not really addressed.

      We acknowledge and respect varying viewpoints, but we hold a differing perspective in this matter. We are convinced that the data decisively supports our interpretation. May future work support or refute our hypothesis.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Line 81 - delete 's

      Done.

      Generally, the introduction was very well written and clearly summarised our current understanding but the paragraph beginning line 134 felt out of place and repeated some of the work mentioned earlier.

      We have removed this paragraph.

      For the EM analysis throughout quantification would be useful as highlighted in the public review. How many tomograms were examined, and how often were types of structures seen? I understand the sample size is often small but this would help the reader appreciate the diversity of structures seen.

      We have included the numbers.

      Following on from this how were the cells chosen for tomogram analysis? For example, the dividing cell in 1D has palisades associating with the new pocket - is this commonly seen? Does this reflect something happening in dividing cells. This point about endosomal division was picked up in the discussion but there was little about in the main results.

      This issue is undoubtedly inherent to the method itself, and we have made efforts to mitigate it by generating a series of tomograms recorded randomly. We have refrained from delving deeper into the intricacies of the cell cycle in this manuscript, as we believe that it warrants a separate paper.

      As the authors prosecute, the co-localisation analysis highlights the variable nature of the endosome and the overlap of different markers. When looking at the LM analysis, I was struck by the variability in the size and number of labelled structures in the different cells. For example, in 3A Rab7 is 2 blobs but in 3B Cell 1 it is 4/5 blobs. Is this just a reflection of the increase in the endosome during the cell cycle?

      The variability in representation is a direct consequence of the dynamic nature of the labelled structures. For this reason, we deliberately selected different cells to represent examples of how the labelling can look like. We have decided not to mention the dynamics of the endosome during the cell cycle. This will be the subject of a further report.

      Moreover, Rab 11 looks to be the marker covering the greatest volume of the endosomal system - is this true? I think there's more analysis of this data that could be done to try and get more information about the relative volumes etc of the different markers that haven't been drawn out. The focus here is on the co-localisation.

      Precisely because we recognize the importance of this point, we intend to turn our attention to the cell cycle in a separate publication.

      I appreciate that it is an awful lot of work to perform the immuno-EM and the data is of good quality but in the text, there could be a greater effort to tie this to the LM data. For example, from the Rab11 staining in LM you would expect this marker to be the most extensive across the networks - is this reflected in the EM?

      For the immuno-EM there were no numbers, the authors had measured the position of the gold but what was the proportion of gold that was in/near membranes for each marker? This would help the reader understand both the number of particles seen and the enrichment of the different regions.

      Our original intent was to perform a thorough quantification (using stereology) of the immuno-EM data. However, we later realized that the necessary random imaging approach is not suitable for Tokuyasu sections of trypanosomes. In short, the cells are too far apart, and the cell sections are only occasionally cut so that the endosomal membranes are sufficiently visible. Nevertheless, we continue to strive to generate more quantitative data using conventional immuno-EM.

      The innovative combination of Tokuyasu tomograms with immuno-EM was great. I noted though that there was a lack of fenestration in these models. Does this reflect the angle of the model or the processing of these samples?

      We are grateful to the referee, as we have asked ourselves the same question. However, we do not attribute the apparent lack of fenestration to the viewing angle, since we did not find fenestration in any of the Tokuyasu tomograms. Our suspicion is more directed towards a methodological problem. In the Tokuyasu workflow, all structures are mainly fixed with aldehydes. As a result, lipids are only effectively fixed through their association with membrane proteins. We suggest that the fenestration may not be visible because the corresponding lipids may have been lost due to incomplete fixation.

      We now clearly state this in the lines 563 – 568.

      “Interestingly, these tomograms did not exhibit the fenestration pattern identified in conventional electron tomography. We suspect that this is due to methodological reasons. The Tokuyasu procedure uses only aldehydes to fix all structures. Consequently, effective fixation of lipids occurs only through their association with membrane proteins. Thus, the lack of visible fenestration is likely due to possible loss of lipids during incomplete fixation.”

      The discussion needs to be reworked. Throughout it contains references to results not in the main results section such as supplementary movie 2 (line 735). The explicit references to the data and figures felt odd and more suited to the results rather than the discussion. Currently, each result is discussed individually in turn and more effort needs to be made to integrate the results from this analysis here but also with previous work and the data from other organisms, which at the moment sits in a standalone section at the end of the discussion.

      We have improved the discussion and removed the previous supplementary movies 2 and 3. Supplementary movie 1 is now mentioned in the results section.

      Line 693 - There was an interesting point about dividing cells describing the maintenance of endosomes next to the old pocket. Does that mean there was no endosome by the new pocket and if so where is this data in the manuscript? This point relates back to my question about how cells were chosen for analysis - how many dividing cells were examined by tomography?

      The fate of endosomes during the cell cycle is not the subject of this paper. In this manuscript we only show only one dividing cell using tomography. An in-depth analysis focusing on what happens during the cell cycle will be published separately.

      Line 729 - I'm unclear how this represents a polarization of function in the flagellar pocket. The pocket I presume is included within the endosomal system for this analysis but there was no specific mention of it in the results and no marker of each position to help define any specialisation. From the results, I thought the focus was on endosomal co-localisation of the different markers. If the authors are thinking about specialisation of the pocket this paper from Mark Field shows there is evidence for the exocyst to be distributed over the entire surface of the pocket, which is relevant to the discussion here. Boehm, C.M. et al. (2017) The trypanosome exocyst: a conserved structure revealing a new role in endocytosis. PLoS Pathog. 13, e1006063

      We have formulated our statement more cautiously. However, we are convinced that membrane exchange cannot physically work without functional polarization of the pocket. We know that Rab11, for example, is not evenly distributed on the pocket. By the way, in Boehm et al. (2017) the exocyst is not shown to cover the entire pocket (as shown in Supplementary Video 1).

      We now refer to Boehm et al. (Lines 700 – 703):

      “Boehm et al (2017) report that in the flagellar pocket endocytic and exocytic sites are in close proximity but do not overlap. We further suggest that the fusion of EXCs with the flagellar pocket membrane and clathrin-mediated endocytosis take place on different sites of the pocket. This disparity explains the lower colocalization between TbRab11 and TbRab5A.”

      Line 735 - link to data not previously mentioned I think. When I looked at this data I couldn't find a key to explain what all the different colours related to.

      We have removed the previous supplementary movies 2 and 3. We now reference supplementary movie 1 in the results section.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Du et al. address the cell cycle-dependent clearance of misfolded protein aggregates mediated by the endoplasmic reticulum (ER) associated Hsp70 chaperone family and ER reorganisation. The observations are interesting and impactful to the field.

      Strength:

      The manuscript addresses the connection between the clearance of misfolded protein aggregates and the cell cycle using a proteostasis reporter targeted to ER in multiple cell lines. Through imaging and some biochemical assays, they establish the role of BiP, an

      Hsp70 family chaperone, and Cdk1 inactivation in aggregate clearance upon mitotic exit.

      Furthermore, the authors present an initial analysis of the role of ER reorganisation in this clearance. These are important correlations and could have implications for ageingassociated pathologies. Overall, the results are convincing and impactful to the field.

      Weakness:

      The manuscript still lacks a mechanistic understanding of aggregate clearance. Even though the authors have provided the role of different cellular components, such as BiP, Cdk1 and ATL2/3 through specific inhibitors, at least an outline establishing the sequence of events leading to clearance is missing. Moreover, the authors show that the levels of ERFlucDM-eGFP do not change significantly throughout the cell cycle, indicating that protein degradation is not in play. Therefore, addressing/elaborating on the mechanism of disassembly can add value to the work. Also, the physiological relevance of aggregate clearance upon mitotic exit has not been tested, nor have the cellular targets of this mode of clearance been identified or discussed.

      Thank you for your suggestions. 

      We have added descriptions about the sequence of events leading to clearance in the abstract (line 33) and discussion (line 316). 

      We have commented on the future work that could address the molecular mechanisms behind the aggregate clearance in the discussion (line 388). 

      It has been difficult to address the physiological relevance of aggregate clearance during cell division, as the inhibition of BiP or depletion of ATL2/3 that prevent aggregate clearance cause cellular consequences not specific to aggregate clearance. Future work that lead to understanding of aggregate clearance at the molecular level may allow us to address this more specifically. Furthermore, we have commented about the potential defects that could arise in cells expressing ER-FlucDM-eGFP that have a perturbed cellular health based on the proteomic analysis (line 359). 

      To identify pathological targets that undergo clearance as the ER-FlucDM-eGFP, we tested three pathological mutants (CFTR-∆F508, AAT S and Z variants) that are known to mis-fold and accumulate in the ER. Unfortunately, expression of these mutants did not result in the confinement of aggregates in the nucleus. The data related to this have been added as Figure S1E and S1F (line 102) in this revised manuscript. We have also commented in the discussion that pathological targets are yet to be identified and could be a part of future work (line 392).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This paper describes an interesting observation that ER-targeted misfolded proteins are trapped within vesicles inside nucleus to facilitate quality control during cell division. This work supports the concept that transient sequestration of misfolded proteins is a fundamental mechanism of protein quality control. The authors satisfactorily addressed several points asked in the review of first submission. The manuscript is improved but still unable to fully address the mechanisms.

      Strengths:

      The observations in this manuscript are very interesting and open up many questions on proteostasis biology.

      Weaknesses:

      Despite inclusions of several protein-level experiments, the manuscript remained a microscopy-driven work and missed the opportunity to work out the mechanisms behind the observations.

      Thank you for your suggestions. We believe that our study has provided a genetic basis for the involvement of ER reorganization and BiP during cell division in aggregate clearance, which is a new observation. We have also commented in this revised manuscript about the future work that could address the molecular mechanisms behind the aggregate clearance in the discussion (line 388).  

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This paper describes a new mechanism for the clearance of protein aggregates associated to endoplasmic reticulum re-organization that occurs during mitosis.

      Experimental data showing clearance of protein aggregates during mitosis is solid, statistically significant, and very interesting. The authors made several new experiments included in the revised version to address the concerns raised by reviewers. A new proteomic analysis, co-localization of the aggregates with the ER membrane Sec61beta protein, expression of the aggregate-prone protein in the nucleus does not result in accumulation of aggregates, detection of protein aggregates in the insoluble faction after cell disruption and mostly importantly knockdown of ATL proteins involved in the organization of ER shape and structure impaired the clearance mechanism. This last observation addresses one of the weakest points of the original version which was the lack of experimental correlation between ER structure capability to re-shape and the clearance mechanism.

      In conclusion, this new mechanism of protein aggregate clearance from the ER was not completely understood in this work but the manuscript presented, particularly in the revised version, an ensemble of solid observations and mechanistic information to scaffold future studies that clarify more details of this mechanism. As stated by the authors: "How protein aggregates are targeted and assembled into the intranuclear membranous structure waits for future investigation". This new mechanism of aggregate clearance from the ER is not expected to be fully understood in a single work but this paper may constitute one step to better comprehend the cell capability to resolve protein aggregates in different cell compartments.

      We thank the reviewer for the comments.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The manuscript presents a very interesting set of observations that could have significant implications on age-related protein misfolding and aggregate clearance. There are a few places in the manuscript that still need more clarity. Some are listed below, which I think can improve the manuscript.

      - The new data associated with proteomic analysis is appreciated, but the information gained has not been explored or elaborated sufficiently in the manuscript. Based on the differential expression of cell cycle proteins, how the authors interpret cellular health is unclear. Also, the physiological role of this mode of aggregate clearance remains unclear.

      We have added our interpretation of perturbed cellular health in cells expressing ERFlucDM-eGFP in the discussion (line 359). 

      It has been difficult to address the physiological relevance of aggregate clearance during cell division, as the inhibition of BiP or depletion of ATL2/3 that prevent aggregate clearance cause cellular consequences not specific to aggregate clearance. Future work that lead to understanding of aggregate clearance at the molecular level may allow us to address this more specifically.

      - In Figure 3A, have the authors measured the total GFP intensity from interphase through early G1? Even though the number and area of the aggregates decrease significantly, the cytoplasmic GFP signal does not seem to increase. Considering new CHX chase experiments and total Fluorescence intensity calculations (Figure S7D), which indicate no difference, one would expect an increase in cytoplasmic signal upon the disassembly of aggregates. Therefore, the data from Figures 3A and 7D seem contradictory. Can the authors please explain?

      We apologized for the confusion. The images in Figure 3A were derived from fixed cells. So, different cells were shown in every cell cycle phases and were not suitable for quantification. Fluorescence intensity changes could be better appreciated in Figure 3C or 4D as these were time-lapse microscopy images of live cells progressing through mitosis and cytokinesis. Data used in the quantification of fluorescence intensity in Figure S7D were derived from live cells taken from specific time points to avoid unwanted fluorescence bleaching during time-lapse microscopy. 

      - Do the authors expect a similar clearance of pathological aggregates such as mutant FUS or TDP43 condensates? Showing aggregate disassembly of disease-relevant aggregates would be an excellent addition to the manuscript, but it might be beyond the scope of the current version. However, the authors can comment/speculate how their study might extend to pathological condensates.

      We tested three pathological mutants (CFTR-∆F508, AAT S and Z variants) that are known to mis-fold and accumulate in the ER. Unfortunately, expression of these mutants did not result in the confinement of aggregates in the nucleus. The data related to this have been added as Figure S1E and S1F (line 102) in this revised manuscript. We have commented that pathological targets are yet to be identified and could be a part of future work (line 392).

      - The presence of ER membrane around these aggregates is an interesting observation. This membrane is retained even after nuclear membrane breakdown. What could be the relevance of membrane-bound aggregates, especially since the membrane can limit the access of chaperones involved in disassembly? This observation becomes more important since the depletion of ER membrane fusion proteins also leads to the accumulation of aggregates. Are the membranes a beacon for disassembly? The authors may comment/ speculate. This could also be an important aspect of the mechanism of clearance.

      We think that the ER membranes around the aggregates are disassembled when the ER networks reorganize during mitotic exit and this may allow accessibility of BiP to disaggregate the aggregates. We have added this in the discussion (line 316).

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      For many years, there has been extensive electrophysiological research investigating the relationship between local field potential patterns and individual cell spike patterns in the hippocampus. In this study, using state-of-the-art imaging techniques, they examined spike synchrony of hippocampal cells during locomotion and immobility states. In contrast to conventional understanding of the hippocampus, the authors demonstrated that hippocampal place cells exhibit prominent synchronous spikes locked to theta oscillations.

      Strengths:

      The voltage imaging used in this study is a highly novel method that allows recording not only suprathreshold-level spikes but also subthreshold-level activity. With its high frame rate, it offers time resolution comparable to electrophysiological recordings. Moreover, it enables the visualization of actual cell locations, allowing for the examination of spatial properties (e.g., Figure 4G).

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the technical novelty of this work.

      Weaknesses:

      There is a notable deviation from several observations obtained through conventional electrophysiological recordings. Particularly, as mentioned below in detail, the considerable differences in baseline firing rates and no observations of ripple-triggered firing patterns raise some concerns about potential artifacts from imaging and analysis, such as cell toxicity, abnormal excitability, and false detection of spikes. While these findings are intriguing if the validity of these methods is properly proven, accepting the current results as new insights is challenging.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments regarding the intriguing aspect of our findings. Indeed, the emergence of a novel form of CA1 population synchrony presents exciting implications for hippocampal memory research and beyond.

      While we acknowledge the deviations from conventional electrophysiological recordings, we respectfully contend that these differences do not necessarily imply methodological flaws. All experiments and analyses were conducted with meticulous adherence to established standards in the field.

      Regarding the observed variations in averaging firing rates, it is important to note the well-documented heterogeneity in CA1 pyramidal neuron firing rates, spanning from 0.01 to 10 Hz, with a skewed distribution toward lower frequencies (Mizuseki et al., 2013). Our exclusion criteria for neurons with low estimated firing rates may have inadvertently biased the selection towards more active neurons. Moreover, prior research has indicated that averaging firing rates tend to increase during exposure to novel environments (Karlsson et al., 2008), and among deep-layer CA1 pyramidal neurons (Mizuseki et al., 2011). Given our recording setup in a highly novel environment and the predominance of deep CA1 pyramidal neurons in our sample, the observed higher averaging firing rates could be influenced by these factors. Considering these points, our mean firing rates (3.2 Hz) are reasonable estimations compared to previously reported values obtained from electrophysiological recordings (2.1 Hz in McHugh et al., 1996 and 2.4-2.6 Hz in Buzsaki et al., 2003).

      Regarding concerns about potential cell toxicity, previous studies have shown that Voltron expression and illumination do not significantly alter membrane resistance, membrane capacitance, resting membrane potentials, spike amplitudes, and spike width (see Abdelfattah 2019, Science, Supplementary Figure 11 and 12). In our recordings, imaged neurons exhibit preserved membrane and dendritic morphology during and after experiments (Author response image 1), supporting the absence of significant toxicity.

      Author response image 1.

      Voltron-expressing neurons exhibit preserved membrane and dendritic morphology. (A) Images of two-photon z-stack maximum intensity projection showing Voltron-expressing neurons taken after voltage image experiments in vivo. (B) Post-hoc histological images of neurons being voltage-imaged.

      Regarding spike detection, we use validated algorithms (Abdelfattah et al., 2019 and 2023) to ensure robust and reliable detection of spikes. Spiking activity was first separated from slower subthreshold potentials using high-pass filtering. This way, a slow fluorescence increase will not be detected as a spike, even if its amplitude is large. We benchmarked the detection algorithm in computer simulation. The sensitivity and specificity of the algorithm exceed 98% at the level of signal-to-noise ratio of our recordings. While we acknowledge that a small number of spikes, particularly those occurring later in a burst, might be missed due to their smaller amplitudes (as illustrated in Figure 1 and 2 of the manuscript), we anticipate that any missed spikes would lead to a decrease rather than an increase in synchrony between neurons. Overall, we are confident that spike detection is performed in a rigorous and robust manner.

      To further strengthen these points, we will include the following in the revision:

      (1) Histological images of recorded neurons during and after experiments.

      (2) Further details regarding the validation of spike detection algorithms.

      (3) Analysis of publicly available electrophysiological datasets.

      (4) Discussion regarding the reasons behind the novelty of some of our findings compared to previous observations.

      In conclusion, we assert that our experimental and analysis approach upholds rigorous standards. We remain committed to reconciling our findings with previous observations and welcome further scrutiny and engagement from the scientific community to explore the intriguing implications of our findings.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study employed voltage imaging in the CA1 region of the mouse hippocampus during the exploration of a novel environment. The authors report synchronous activity, involving almost half of the imaged neurons, occurred during periods of immobility. These events did not correlate with SWRs, but instead, occurred during theta oscillations and were phased-locked to the trough of theta. Moreover, pairs of neurons with high synchronization tended to display non-overlapping place fields, leading the authors to suggest these events may play a role in binding a distributed representation of the context.

      We thank the reviewer for a thorough and thoughtful review of our paper.

      Strengths:

      Technically this is an impressive study, using an emerging approach that allows single-cell resolution voltage imaging in animals, that while head-fixed, can move through a real environment. The paper is written clearly and suggests novel observations about population-level activity in CA1.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the technical strength and the novelty of our observations.

      Weaknesses:

      The evidence provided is weak, with the authors making surprising population-level claims based on a very sparse data set (5 data sets, each with less than 20 neurons simultaneously recorded) acquired with exciting, but less tested technology. Further, while the authors link these observations to the novelty of the context, both in the title and text, they do not include data from subsequent visits to support this. Detailed comments are below:

      We understand the reviewer’s concerns regarding the size of the dataset. Despite this limitation, it is important to note that synchronous ensembles beyond what could be expected from chance (jittering) were detected in all examined data. In the revision, we plan to add more data, including data from subsequent visits, to further strengthen our findings.

      (1) My first question for the authors, which is not addressed in the discussion, is why these events have not been observed in the countless extracellular recording experiments conducted in rodent CA1 during the exploration of novel environments. Those data sets often have 10x the neurons simultaneously recording compared to these present data, thus the highly synchronous firing should be very hard to miss. Ideally, the authors could confirm their claims via the analysis of publicly available electrophysiology data sets. Further, the claim of high extra-SWR synchrony is complicated by the observation that their recorded neurons fail to spike during the limited number of SWRs recorded during behavior- again, not agreeing with much of the previous electrophysiological recordings.

      We understand the reviewer’s concern. We will examine publicly available electrophysiology datasets to gain further insights into any similarities and differences to our findings. Based on these results, we will discuss why these events have not been previously observed/reported.

      (2) The authors posit that these events are linked to the novelty of the context, both in the text, as well as in the title and abstract. However, they do not include any imaging data from subsequent days to demonstrate the failure to see this synchrony in a familiar environment. If these data are available it would strengthen the proposed link to novelty if they were included.

      We thank the reviewer’s constructive suggestion. We will acquire more datasets from subsequent visits to gain further insights into these synchronous events.

      3) In the discussion the authors begin by speculating the theta present during these synchronous events may be slower type II or attentional theta. This can be supported by demonstrating a frequency shift in the theta recording during these events/immobility versus the theta recording during movement.

      We thank the reviewer’s constructive suggestion. We did demonstrate a frequency shift to a lower frequency in the synchrony-associated theta during immobility than during locomotion (see Fig. 4B, the red vs. blue curves). We will enlarge this panel and specifically refer to it in the corresponding discussion paragraph.

      (4) The authors mention in the discussion that they image deep-layer PCs in CA1, however, this is not mentioned in the text or methods. They should include data, such as imaging of a slice of a brain post-recording with immunohistochemistry for a layer-specific gene to support this.

      We thank the reviewer’s constructive suggestion. We do have images of brain slices post-recordings (Author response image 2). Imaged neurons are clearly located in the deep CA1 pyramidal layer. We will add these images and quantification in the revised manuscript.

      Author response image 2.

      Imaged neurons are located in the deep pyramidal layer of the dorsal hippocampal CA1 region.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In the present manuscript, the authors use a few minutes of voltage imaging of CA1 pyramidal cells in head-fixed mice running on a track while local field potentials (LFPs) are recorded. The authors suggest that synchronous ensembles of neurons are differentially associated with different types of LFP patterns, theta and ripples. The experiments are flawed in that the LFP is not "local" but rather collected in the other side of the brain, and the investigation is flawed due to multiple problems with the point process analyses. The synchrony terminology refers to dozens of milliseconds as opposed to the millisecond timescale referred to in prior work, and the interpretations do not take into account theta phase locking as a simple alternative explanation.

      We genuinely appreciate the reviewer’s feedback and acknowledge the concerns raised. However, we believe these concerns can be effectively addressed without undermining the validity of our conclusions. With this in mind, we respectfully disagree with the assessment that our experiments and investigation are flawed. Please allow us to address these concerns and offer additional context to support the validity of our study.

      Weaknesses:

      The two main messages of the manuscript indicated in the title are not supported by the data. The title gives two messages that relate to CA1 pyramidal neurons in behaving head-fixed mice: (1) synchronous ensembles are associated with theta (2) synchronous ensembles are not associated with ripples.

      There are two main methodological problems with the work:

      (1) Experimentally, the theta and ripple signals were recorded using electrophysiology from the opposite hemisphere to the one in which the spiking was monitored. However, both signals exhibit profound differences as a function of location: theta phase changes with the precise location along the proximo-distal and dorso-ventral axes, and importantly, even reverses with depth. And ripples are often a local phenomenon - independent ripples occur within a fraction of a millimeter within the same hemisphere, let alone different hemispheres. Ripples are very sensitive to the precise depth - 100 micrometers up or down, and only a positive deflection/sharp wave is evident.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s consideration regarding the collection of LFP from the contralateral hemisphere. While we acknowledge the limitation of this design, we believe that our findings still offer valuable insights into the dynamics of synchronous ensembles. Despite potential variations in theta phases with recording locations and depth, we find that the occurrence and amplitudes of theta oscillations are generally coordinated across hemispheres (Buzsaki et al., Neurosci., 2003). Therefore, the presence of prominent contralateral LFP theta around the times of synchronous ensembles in our study (see Figure 4A of the manuscript) strongly supports our conclusion regarding their association with theta oscillations, despite the collection of LFP from the opposite hemisphere.

      In addition, in our manuscript, we specifically mentioned that the “preferred phases” varied from session to session, likely due to the variability of recording locations (see Line 254-256). Therefore, we think that the reviewer’s concern regarding theta phase variability has already been addressed in the present manuscript.

      Regarding ripple oscillations, while we recognize that they can sometimes occur locally, the majority of ripples occur synchronously in both hemispheres (up to 70%, see Szabo et al., Neuron, 2022; Buzsaki et al., Neurosci., 2003). Therefore, using contralateral LFP to infer ripple occurrence on the ipsilateral side has been a common practice in the field, employed by many studies published in respectable journals (Szabo et al., Neuron, 2022; Terada et al., Nature, 2021; Dudok et al., Neuron, 2021; Geiller et al., Neuron, 2020). Furthermore, our observation that 446 synchronous ensembles during immobility do not co-occur with contralateral ripples, and the remaining 313 ensembles during locomotion are not associated with ripples, as ripples rarely occur during locomotion. Therefore, our conclusion that synchronous ensembles are not associated with ripple oscillations is supported by data.

      (2) The analysis of the point process data (spike trains) is entirely flawed. There are many technical issues: complex spikes ("bursts") are not accounted for; differences in spike counts between the various conditions ("locomotion" and "immobility") are not accounted for; the pooling of multiple CCGs assumes independence, whereas even conditional independence cannot be assumed; etc.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding spike train analysis. Indeed, complex bursts or different behavioral conditions can lead to differences in spike counts that could potentially affect the detection of synchronous ensembles. However, our jittering procedure (see Line 121-132) is designed to control for the variation of spike counts. Importantly, while the jittered spike trains also contain the same spike count variations, we found 7.8-fold more synchronous events in our data compared to jitter controls (see Figure 1G of the manuscript), indicating that these factors cannot account for the observed synchrony.

      To explicitly demonstrate that complex bursts cannot account for the observed synchrony, we have performed additional analysis to remove all latter spikes in bursts and only count the single and the first spikes of bursts. Importantly, we found that this procedure did not change the rate and size of synchronous ensembles, nor did it significantly alter the grand-average CCG (see Author response image 3). The results of this analysis explicitly rule out a significant effect of complex spikes on the analysis of synchronous ensembles.

      Author response image 3.

      Population synchrony remains after the removal of spikes in bursts. (A) The grand-average cross correlogram (CCG) was calculated using spike trains without latter spikes in bursts. The gray line represents the mean grand average CCG between reference cells and randomly selected cells from different sessions. (B) Pairwise comparison of the event rates of population synchrony between spike trains containing all spikes and spike trains without latter spikes in bursts. Bar heights indicate group means (n=10 segments, p=0.036, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). (C) Histogram of the ensemble sizes as percentages of cells participating in the synchronous ensembles.

      Beyond those methodological issues, there are two main interpretational problems: (1) the "synchronous ensembles" may be completely consistent with phase locking to the intracellular theta (as even shown by the authors themselves in some of the supplementary figures).

      We agree with the reviewer that the synchronous ensembles are indeed consistent with theta phase locking. However, it is important to note that theta phase locking alone does not necessarily imply population synchrony. In fact, theta phase locking has been shown to “reduce” population synchrony in a previous study (Mizuseki et al., 2014, Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B.). Thus, the presence of theta phase locking cannot be taken as a simple alternative explanation of the synchronous ensembles.

      To directly assess the contribution of theta phase locking to synchronous ensembles, we have performed a new analysis to randomize the specific theta cycles in which neurons spike, while keeping the spike phases constant. This manipulation disrupts spike co-occurrence while preserving theta phase locking, allowing us to test whether theta phase locking alone can explain the population synchrony, or whether spike co-occurrence in specific cycles is required. The grand-average CCG shows a much smaller peak compared to the original peak (Author response image 4A). Moreover, synchronous event rates show a 4.5-fold decrease in the randomized data compared to the original event rates (Author response image 4B). Thus, the new analysis reveals theta phase locking alone cannot account for the population synchrony.

      Author response image 4.

      Drastic reduction of population synchrony by randomizing spikes to other theta cycles while preserving the phases. (A) The grand-average cross correlogram (CCG) was calculated using original spike trains (black) and randomized spike trains where theta phases of the spikes are kept the same but spike timings were randomly moved to other theta cycles (red). (B) Pairwise comparison of the event rates of population synchrony between the original spike trains and randomized spike trains (n=10 segments, p=0.002, Wilcoxon signed-rank test). Bar heights indicate group means. ** p<0.01

      (2) The definition of "synchrony" in the present work is very loose and refers to timescales of 20-30 ms. In previous literature that relates to synchrony of point processes, the timescales discussed are 1-2 ms, and longer timescales are referred to as the "baseline" which is actually removed (using smoothing, jittering, etc.).

      Regarding the timescale of synchronous ensembles, we acknowledge that it varies considerably across studies and cell types. However, it is important to note that a timescale of dozens, or even hundreds of milliseconds is common for synchrony terminology in CA1 pyramidal neurons (see Csicsvari et al., Neuron, 2000; Harris et al., Science, 2003; Malvache et al., Science, 2016; Yagi et al., Cell Reports, 2023). In fact, a timescale of 20-30 ms is considered particularly important for information transmission and storage in CA1, as it matches the membrane time constant of pyramidal neurons, the period of hippocampal gamma oscillations, and the time window for synaptic plasticity. Therefore, we believe that this timescale is relevant and in line with established practices in the field.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Responses to Editors:

      We appreciate the editors’ concern regarding the difficulty of disentangling the contributions of tightly-coupled brain regions to the speech-gesture integration process—particularly due to the close temporal and spatial proximity of the stimulation windows and the potential for prolonged disruption. While we agree with that stimulation techniques, such as transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), can evoke or modulate neuronal activity both locally within the target region and in remote connected areas of the network. This complex interaction makes drawing clear conclusions about the causal relationship between stimulation and cognitive function more challenging. However, we believe that cause-and-effect relationships in cognitive neuroscience studies using non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) can still be robustly established if key assumptions are explicitly tested and confounding factors are rigorously controlled (Bergmann & Hartwigsen et al., 2021, J Cogn Neurosci).

      In our experiment, we addressed these concerns by including a sham TMS condition, an irrelevant control task, and multiple control time points. The results showed that TMS selectively disrupted the IFG-pMTG interaction during specific time windows of the task related to gesture-speech semantic congruency, but not in the sham TMS condition or the control task (gender congruency effect) (Zhao et al., 2021, JN). This selective disruption provides strong evidence for a causal link between IFG-pMTG connectivity and gesture-speech integration in the targeted time window.

      Regarding the potential for transient artifacts from TMS, we acknowledge that previous research has demonstrated that single-pulse TMS induces brief artifacts (0–10 ms) due to direct depolarization of cortical neurons, which momentarily disrupts electrical activity in the stimulated area (Romero et al., 2019, NC). However, in the case of paired-pulse TMS (ppTMS), the interaction between the first and second pulses is more complex. The first pulse increases membrane conductance in the target neurons via shunting inhibition mediated by GABAergic interneurons. This effectively lowers neuronal membrane resistance, “leaking” excitatory current and diminishing the depolarization induced by the second pulse, leading to a reduction in excitability during the paired-pulse interval. This mechanism suppresses the excitatory response to the second pulse, which is reflected in a reduced motor evoked potential (MEP) (Paulus & Rothwell, 2016, J Physiol).

      Furthermore, ppTMS has been widely used in previous studies to infer causal temporal relationships and explore the neural contributions of both structurally and functionally connected brain regions, across timescales as brief as 3–60 ms. We have reviewed several studies that employed paired-pulse TMS to investigate neural dynamics in regions such as the tongue and lip areas of the primary motor cortex (M1), as well as high-level semantic regions like the pMTG, PFC, and ATL (Table 1). These studies consistently demonstrate the methodological rigor and precision of double-pulse TMS in elucidating the temporal dynamics between different brain regions within short temporal windows.

      Given these precedents and the evidence provided, we respectfully assert the validity of the methods employed in our study. We therefore kindly request the editors to reconsider the assessment that “the methods are insufficient for studying tightly-coupled brain regions over short timescales.” We hope that the editors’ concerns about the complexities of TMS-induced effects have been adequately addressed, and that our study’s design and results provide a clear and convincing causal argument for the role of IFG-pMTG in gesture-speech integration.

      Author response table 1.

      Double-pulse TMS studies on brain regions over 3-60 ms time interval

      Reference

      Teige, C., Mollo, G., Millman, R., Savill, N., Smallwood, J., Cornelissen, P. L., & Jefferies, E. (2018). Dynamic semantic cognition: Characterising coherent and controlled conceptual retrieval through time using magnetoencephalography and chronometric transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cortex, 103, 329-349.

      Amemiya, T., Beck, B., Walsh, V., Gomi, H., & Haggard, P. (2017). Visual area V5/hMT+ contributes to perception of tactile motion direction: a TMS study. Scientific reports, 7(1), 40937.

      Muessgens, D., Thirugnanasambandam, N., Shitara, H., Popa, T., & Hallett, M. (2016). Dissociable roles of preSMA in motor sequence chunking and hand switching—a TMS study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 116(6), 2637-2646.

      Vernet, M., Brem, A. K., Farzan, F., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2015). Synchronous and opposite roles of the parietal and prefrontal cortices in bistable perception: a double-coil TMS–EEG study. Cortex, 64, 78-88.

      Pitcher, D. (2014). Facial expression recognition takes longer in the posterior superior temporal sulcus than in the occipital face area. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(27), 9173-9177.

      Bardi, L., Kanai, R., Mapelli, D., & Walsh, V. (2012). TMS of the FEF interferes with spatial conflict. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 24(6), 1305-1313.

      D’Ausilio, A., Bufalari, I., Salmas, P., & Fadiga, L. (2012). The role of the motor system in discriminating normal and degraded speech sounds. Cortex, 48(7), 882-887.

      Pitcher, D., Duchaine, B., Walsh, V., & Kanwisher, N. (2010). TMS evidence for feedforward and feedback mechanisms of face and body perception. Journal of Vision, 10(7), 671-671.

      Gagnon, G., Blanchet, S., Grondin, S., & Schneider, C. (2010). Paired-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation over the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex interferes with episodic encoding and retrieval for both verbal and non-verbal materials. Brain Research, 1344, 148-158.

      Kalla, R., Muggleton, N. G., Juan, C. H., Cowey, A., & Walsh, V. (2008). The timing of the involvement of the frontal eye fields and posterior parietal cortex in visual search. Neuroreport, 19(10), 1067-1071.

      Pitcher, D., Garrido, L., Walsh, V., & Duchaine, B. C. (2008). Transcranial magnetic stimulation disrupts the perception and embodiment of facial expressions. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(36), 8929-8933.

      Til Ole Bergmann, Gesa Hartwigsen; Inferring Causality from Noninvasive Brain Stimulation in Cognitive Neuroscience. J Cogn Neurosci 2021; 33 (2): 195–225. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01591

      Romero, M.C., Davare, M., Armendariz, M. et al. Neural effects of transcranial magnetic stimulation at the single-cell level. Nat Commun 10, 2642 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-10638-7

      Paulus W, Rothwell JC. Membrane resistance and shunting inhibition: where biophysics meets state-dependent human neurophysiology. J Physiol. 2016 May 15;594(10):2719-28. doi: 10.1113/JP271452. PMID: 26940751; PMCID: PMC4865581.

      Staat, C., Gattinger, N., & Gleich, B. (2022). PLUSPULS: A transcranial magnetic stimulator with extended pulse protocols. HardwareX, 13. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ohx.2022.e00380

      Zhao, W., Li, Y., and Du, Y. (2021). TMS reveals dynamic interaction between inferior frontal gyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus in gesture-speech semantic integration. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10356-10364. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1355-21.2021.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors quantified information in gesture and speech, and investigated the neural processing of speech and gestures in pMTG and LIFG, depending on their informational content, in 8 different time-windows, and using three different methods (EEG, HD-tDCS and TMS). They found that there is a time-sensitive and staged progression of neural engagement that is correlated with the informational content of the signal (speech/gesture).

      Strengths:

      A strength of the paper is that the authors attempted to combine three different methods to investigate speech-gesture processing.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for recognizing our efforts in conducting three experiments to explore the neural activity linked to the amount of information processed during multisensory gesture-speech integration. In Experiment 1, we observed that the extent of inhibition in the pMTG and LIFG was closely linked to the overlapping gesture-speech responses, as quantified by mutual information. Building on the established roles of the pMTG and LIFG in our previous study (Zhao et al., 2021, JN), we then expanded our investigation to determine whether the dynamic neural engagement between the pMTG and LIFG during gesture-speech processing was also associated with the quality of the information. This hypothesis was further validated through high-temporal resolution EEG, where we examined ERP components related to varying information contents. Notably, we observed a close time alignment between the ERP components and the time windows of the TMS effects, which were associated with the same informational matrices in gesture-speech processing.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) One major issue is that there is a tight anatomical coupling between pMTG and LIFG. Stimulating one area could therefore also result in stimulation of the other area (see Silvanto and Pascual-Leone, 2008). I therefore think it is very difficult to tease apart the contribution of these areas to the speech-gesture integration process, especially considering that the authors stimulate these regions in time windows that are very close to each other in both time and space (and the disruption might last longer over time).

      Response 1: We greatly appreciate the reviewer’s careful consideration. We trust that the explanation provided above has clarified this issue (see Response to Editors for detail).

      (2) Related to this point, it is unclear to me why the HD-TDCS/TMS is delivered in set time windows for each region. How did the authors determine this, and how do the results for TMS compare to their previous work from 2018 and 2023 (which describes a similar dataset+design)? How can they ensure they are only targeting their intended region since they are so anatomically close to each other?

      Response 2: The current study builds on a series of investigations that systematically examined the temporal and spatial dynamics of gesture-speech integration. In our earlier work (Zhao et al., 2018, J. Neurosci), we demonstrated that interrupting neural activity in the IFG or pMTG using TMS selectively disrupted the semantic congruency effect (reaction time costs due to semantic incongruence), without affecting the gender congruency effect (reaction time costs due to gender incongruence). These findings identified the IFG and pMTG as critical hubs for gesture-speech integration. This informed the brain regions selected for subsequent studies.

      In Zhao et al. (2021, J. Neurosci), we employed a double-pulse TMS protocol, delivering stimulation within one of eight 40-ms time windows, to further examine the temporal involvement of the IFG and pMTG. The results revealed time-window-selective disruptions of the semantic congruency effect, confirming the dynamic and temporally staged roles of these regions during gesture-speech integration.

      In Zhao et al. (2023, Frontiers in Psychology), we investigated the semantic predictive role of gestures relative to speech by comparing two experimental conditions: (1) gestures preceding speech by a fixed interval of 200 ms, and (2) gestures preceding speech at its semantic identification point. We observed time-window-selective disruptions of the semantic congruency effect in the IFG and pMTG only in the second condition, leading to the conclusion that gestures exert a semantic priming effect on co-occurring speech. These findings underscored the semantic advantage of gesture in facilitating speech integration, further refining our understanding of the temporal and functional interplay between these modalities.

      The design of the current study—including the choice of brain regions and time windows—was directly informed by these prior findings. Experiment 1 (HD-tDCS) targeted the entire gesture-speech integration process in the IFG and pMTG to assess whether neural activity in these regions, previously identified as integration hubs, is modulated by changes in informativeness from both modalities (i.e., entropy) and their interactions (mutual information, MI). The results revealed a gradual inhibition of neural activity in both areas as MI increased, evidenced by a negative correlation between MI and the tDCS inhibition effect in both regions. Building on this, Experiments 2 and 3 employed double-pulse TMS and ERPs to further assess whether the engaged neural activity was both time-sensitive and staged. These experiments also evaluated the contributions of various sources of information, revealing correlations between information-theoretic metrics and time-locked brain activity, providing insights into the ‘gradual’ nature of gesture-speech integration.

      We acknowledge that the rationale for the design of the current study was not fully articulated in the original manuscript. In the revised version, we provided a more comprehensive and coherent explanation of the logic behind the three experiments, as well as the alignment with our previous findings in Lines 75-102:

      ‘To investigate the neural mechanisms underlying gesture-speech integration, we conducted three experiments to assess how neural activity correlates with distributed multisensory integration, quantified using information-theoretic measures of MI. Additionally, we examined the contributions of unisensory signals in this process, quantified through unisensory entropy. Experiment 1 employed high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) to administer Anodal, Cathodal and Sham stimulation to either the IFG or the pMTG. HD-tDCS induces membrane depolarization with anodal stimulation and membrane hyperpolarization with cathodal stimulation[26], thereby increasing or decreasing cortical excitability in the targeted brain area, respectively. This experiment aimed to determine whether the overall facilitation (Anodal-tDCS minus Sham-tDCS) and/or inhibitory (Cathodal-tDCS minus Sham-tDCS) of these integration hubs is modulated by the degree of gesture-speech integration, as measure by MI.

      Given the differential involvement of the IFG and pMTG in gesture-speech integration, shaped by top-down gesture predictions and bottom-up speech processing [23], Experiment 2 was designed to further assess whether the activity of these regions was associated with relevant informational matrices. Specifically, we applied inhibitory chronometric double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to specific temporal windows associated with integration processes in these regions[23], assessing whether the inhibitory effects of TMS were correlated with unisensory entropy or the multisensory convergence index (MI).

      Experiment 3 complemented these investigations by focusing on the temporal dynamics of neural responses during semantic processing, leveraging high-temporal event-related potentials (ERPs). This experiment investigated how distinct information contributors modulated specific ERP components associated with semantic processing. These components included the early sensory effects as P1 and N1–P2[27,28], the N400 semantic conflict effect[14,28,29], and the late positive component (LPC) reconstruction effect[30,31]. By integrating these ERP findings with results from Experiments 1 and 2, Experiment 3 aimed to provide a more comprehensive understanding of how gesture-speech integration is modulated by neural dynamics.’

      Although the IFG and pMTG are anatomically close, the consistent differentiation of their respective roles, as evidenced by our experiment across various time windows (TWs) and supported by previous research (see Response to editors for details), reinforces the validity of the stimulation effect observed in our study.

      References

      Zhao, W.Y., Riggs, K., Schindler, I., and Holle, H. (2018). Transcranial magnetic stimulation over left inferior frontal and posterior temporal cortex disrupts gesture-speech integration. Journal of Neuroscience 38, 1891-1900. 10.1523/Jneurosci.1748-17.2017.

      Zhao, W., Li, Y., and Du, Y. (2021). TMS reveals dynamic interaction between inferior frontal gyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus in gesture-speech semantic integration. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10356-10364. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1355-21.2021.

      Zhao, W. (2023). TMS reveals a two-stage priming circuit of gesture-speech integration. Front Psychol 14, 1156087. 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1156087.

      Bikson, M., Inoue, M., Akiyama, H., Deans, J.K., Fox, J.E., Miyakawa, H., and Jefferys, J.G.R. (2004). Effects of uniform extracellular DC electric fields on excitability in rat hippocampal slices. J Physiol-London 557, 175-190. 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.055772.

      Federmeier, K.D., Mai, H., and Kutas, M. (2005). Both sides get the point: hemispheric sensitivities to sentential constraint. Memory & Cognition 33, 871-886. 10.3758/bf03193082.

      Kelly, S.D., Kravitz, C., and Hopkins, M. (2004). Neural correlates of bimodal speech and gesture comprehension. Brain and Language 89, 253-260. 10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00335-3.

      Wu, Y.C., and Coulson, S. (2005). Meaningful gestures: Electrophysiological indices of iconic gesture comprehension. Psychophysiology 42, 654-667. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00356.x.

      Fritz, I., Kita, S., Littlemore, J., and Krott, A. (2021). Multimodal language processing: How preceding discourse constrains gesture interpretation and affects gesture integration when gestures do not synchronise with semantic affiliates. J Mem Lang 117, 104191. 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104191.

      Gunter, T.C., and Weinbrenner, J.E.D. (2017). When to take a gesture seriously: On how we use and prioritize communicative cues. J Cognitive Neurosci 29, 1355-1367. 10.1162/jocn_a_01125.

      Ozyurek, A., Willems, R.M., Kita, S., and Hagoort, P. (2007). On-line integration of semantic information from speech and gesture: Insights from event-related brain potentials. J Cognitive Neurosci 19, 605-616. 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.605.

      (3) As the EEG signal is often not normally distributed, I was wondering whether the authors checked the assumptions for their Pearson correlations. The authors could perhaps better choose to model the different variables to see whether MI/entropy could predict the neural responses. How did they correct the many correlational analyses that they have performed?

      Response 3: We greatly appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful comments.

      (1) Regarding the questioning of normal distribution of EEG signals and the use of Pearson correlation, in Figure 5 of the manuscript, we have already included normal distribution curves to illustrate the relationships between average ERP amplitudes across each ROI or elicited cluster and the three information models.

      Additionally, we performed the Shapiro-Wilk test, a widely accepted method for assessing bivariate normality, on both the MI/entropy and averaged ERP data. The p-values for all three combinations were greater than 0.05, indicating that the sample data from all bivariate combinations were normally distributed (Author response table 2).

      Author response table 2.

      Shapiro-Wilk results of bivariable normality test

      To further consolidate the relationship between entropy/MI and various ERP components, we also conducted a Spearman rank correlation analysis (Author response table 3-5). While the correlation between speech entropy and ERP amplitude in the P1 component yielded a p-value of 0.061, all other results were consistent with those obtained from the Pearson correlation analysis across the three experiments. Therefore, our conclusion that progressive neural responses reflected the degree of information remains robust. Although the Spearman rank and Pearson correlation analyses yielded similar results, we opted to report the Pearson correlation coefficients throughout the manuscript to maintain consistency.

      Author response table 3.

      Comparison of Pearson and Spearman results in Experiment 1

      Author response table 4.

      Comparison of Pearson and Spearman results in Experiment 2

      Author response table 5.

      Comparison of Pearson and Spearman results in Experiment 3

      (2) Regarding the reviewer’s comment ‘choose to model the different variables to see whether MI/entropy could predict the neural responses’, we employed Representational Similarity Analysis (RSA) (Popal et.al, 2019) with MI and entropy as continuous variables. This analysis aimed to build a model to predict neural responses based on these feature metrics.

      To capture dynamic temporal features indicative of different stages of multisensory integration, we segmented the EEG data into overlapping time windows (40 ms in duration with a 10 ms step size). The 40 ms window was chosen based on the TMS protocol used in Experiment 2, which also employed a 40 ms time window. The 10 ms step size (equivalent to 5 time points) was used to detect subtle shifts in neural responses that might not be captured by larger time windows, allowing for a more granular analysis of the temporal dynamics of neural activity.

      Following segmentation, the EEG data were reshaped into a four-dimensional matrix (42 channels × 20 time points × 97 time windows × 20 features). To construct a neural similarity matrix, we averaged the EEG data across time points within each channel and each time window. The resulting matrix was then processed using the pdist function to compute pairwise distances between adjacent data points. This allowed us to calculate correlations between the neural matrix and three feature similarity matrices, which were constructed in a similar manner. These three matrices corresponded to (1) gesture entropy, (2) speech entropy, and (3) mutual information (MI). This approach enabled us to quantify how well the neural responses corresponded to the semantic dimensions of gesture and speech stimuli at each time window.

      To determine the significance of the correlations between neural activity and feature matrices, we conducted 1000 permutation tests. In this procedure, we randomized the data or feature matrices and recalculated the correlations repeatedly, generating a null distribution against which the observed correlation values were compared. Statistical significance was determined if the observed correlation exceeded the null distribution threshold (p < 0.05). This permutation approach helps mitigate the risk of spurious correlations, ensuring that the relationships between the neural data and feature matrices are both robust and meaningful.

      Finally, significant correlations were subjected to clustering analysis, which grouped similar neural response patterns across time windows and channels. This clustering allowed us to identify temporal and spatial patterns in the neural data that consistently aligned with the semantic features of gesture and speech stimuli, thus revealing the dynamic integration of these multisensory modalities across time. Results are as follows:

      (1) Two significant clusters were identified for gesture entropy (Author response image 1 left). The first cluster was observed between 60-110 ms (channels F1 and F3), with correlation coefficients (r) ranging from 0.207 to 0.236 (p < 0.001). The second cluster was found between 210-280 ms (channel O1), with r-values ranging from 0.244 to 0.313 (p < 0.001).

      (2) For speech entropy (Author response image 1 middle), significant clusters were detected in both early and late time windows. In the early time windows, the largest significant cluster was found between 10-170 ms (channels F2, F4, F6, FC2, FC4, FC6, C4, C6, CP4, and CP6), with r-values ranging from 0.151 to 0.340 (p = 0.013), corresponding to the P1 component (0-100 ms). In the late time windows, the largest significant cluster was observed between 560-920 ms (across the whole brain, all channels), with r-values ranging from 0.152 to 0.619 (p = 0.013).

      (3) For mutual information (MI) (Author response image 1 right), a significant cluster was found between 270-380 ms (channels FC1, FC2, FC3, FC5, C1, C2, C3, C5, CP1, CP2, CP3, CP5, FCz, Cz, and CPz), with r-values ranging from 0.198 to 0.372 (p = 0.001).

      Author response image 1.

      Results of RSA analysis.

      These additional findings suggest that even using a different modeling approach, neural responses, as indexed by feature metrics of entropy and mutual information, are temporally aligned with distinct ERP components and ERP clusters, as reported in the current manuscript. This alignment serves to further consolidate the results, reinforcing the conclusion we draw. Considering the length of the manuscript, we did not include these results in the current manuscript.

      (3) In terms of the correction of multiple comparisons, in Experiment 1, two separate participant groups were recruited for HD-tDCS applied over either the IFG or pMTG. FDR correction was performed separately for each group, resulting in six comparisons for each brain region (three information matrices × two tDCS effects: anodal-sham or cathodal-sham). In Experiment 2, six comparisons (three information matrices × two sites: IFG or pMTG) were submitted for FDR correction. In Experiment 3, FDR correction was applied to the seven regions of interest (ROIs) within each component, resulting in five comparisons.

      Reference:

      Wilk, M.B. (2015). The Shapiro Wilk And Related Tests For Normality.

      Popal, H., Wang, Y., & Olson, I. R. (2019). A guide to representational similarity analysis for social neuroscience. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 14(11), 1243-1253.

      (4) The authors use ROIs for their different analyses, but it is unclear why and on the basis of what these regions are defined. Why not consider all channels without making them part of an ROI, by using a method like the one described in my previous comment?

      Response 4: For the EEG data, we conducted both a traditional ROI analysis and a cluster-based permutation approach. The ROIs were defined based on a well-established work (Habets et al., 2011), allowing for hypothesis-driven testing of specific regions. In addition, we employed a cluster-based permutation methods, which is data-driven and helps enhance robustness while addressing multiple comparisons. This method serves as a complement to the hypothesis-driven ROI analysis, offering an exploratory, unbiased perspective. Notably, the results from both approaches were consistent, reinforcing the reliability of our findings.

      To make the methods more accessible to a broader audience, we clarified the relationship between these approaches in the revised manuscript in Lines 267-270: ‘To consolidate the data, we conducted both a traditional region-of-interest (ROI) analysis, with ROIs defined based on a well-established work40, and a cluster-based permutation approach, which utilizes data-driven permutations to enhance robustness and address multiple comparisons’

      Additionally, we conducted an RSA analysis without defining specific ROIs, considering all channels in the analysis. This approach yielded consistent results, further validating the robustness of our findings across different analysis methods. See Response 3 for detail.

      Reference:

      Habets, B., Kita, S., Shao, Z.S., Ozyurek, A., and Hagoort, P. (2011). The Role of Synchrony and Ambiguity in Speech-Gesture Integration during Comprehension. J Cognitive Neurosci 23, 1845-1854. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21462

      (5) The authors describe that they have divided their EEG data into a "lower half" and a "higher half" (lines 234-236), based on entropy scores. It is unclear why this is necessary, and I would suggest just using the entropy scores as a continuous measure.

      Response 5: To identify ERP components or spatiotemporal clusters that demonstrated significant semantic differences, we split each model into higher and lower halves based on entropy scores. This division allowed us to capture distinct levels of information processing and explore how different levels of entropy or mutual information (MI) related to neural activity. Specifically, the goal was to highlight the gradual activation process of these components and clusters as they correlate with changes in information content. Remarkably, consistent results were observed between the ERP components and clusters, providing robust evidence that semantic information conveyed through gestures and speech significantly influenced the amplitude of these components or clusters. Moreover, the semantic information was shown to be highly sensitive, varying in tandem with these amplitude changes.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Comment:

      Summary:

      The study is an innovative and fundamental study that clarified important aspects of brain processes for integration of information from speech and iconic gesture (i.e., gesture that depicts action, movement, and shape), based on tDCS, TMS, and EEG experiments. They evaluated their speech and gesture stimuli in information-theoretic ways and calculated how informative speech is (i.e., entropy), how informative gesture is, and how much shared information speech and gesture encode. The tDCS and TMS studies found that the left IFG and pMTG, the two areas that were activated in fMRI studies on speech-gesture integration in the previous literature, are causally implicated in speech-gesture integration. The size of tDC and TMS effects are correlated with the entropy of the stimuli or mutual information, which indicates that the effects stem from the modulation of information decoding/integration processes. The EEG study showed that various ERP (event-related potential, e.g., N1-P2, N400, LPC) effects that have been observed in speech-gesture integration experiments in the previous literature, are modulated by the entropy of speech/gesture and mutual information. This makes it clear that these effects are related to information decoding processes. The authors propose a model of how the speech-gesture integration process unfolds in time, and how IFG and pMTG interact with each other in that process.

      Strengths:

      The key strength of this study is that the authors used information theoretic measures of their stimuli (i.e., entropy and mutual information between speech and gesture) in all of their analyses. This made it clear that the neuro-modulation (tDCS, TMS) affected information decoding/integration and ERP effects reflect information decoding/integration. This study used tDCS and TMS methods to demonstrate that left IFG and pMTG are causally involved in speech-gesture integration. The size of tDCS and TMS effects are correlated with information-theoretic measures of the stimuli, which indicate that the effects indeed stem from disruption/facilitation of the information decoding/integration process (rather than generic excitation/inhibition). The authors' results also showed a correlation between information-theoretic measures of stimuli with various ERP effects. This indicates that these ERP effects reflect the information decoding/integration process.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for recognizing our efforts and the innovation of employing information-theoretic measures to elucidate the brain processes underlying the multisensory integration of gesture and speech.

      Weaknesses:

      The "mutual information" cannot fully capture the interplay of the meaning of speech and gesture. The mutual information is calculated based on what information can be decoded from speech alone and what information can be decoded from gesture alone. However, when speech and gesture are combined, a novel meaning can emerge, which cannot be decoded from a single modality alone. When example, a person produces a gesture of writing something with a pen, while saying "He paid". The speech-gesture combination can be interpreted as "paying by signing a cheque". It is highly unlikely that this meaning is decoded when people hear speech only or see gestures only. The current study cannot address how such speech-gesture integration occurs in the brain, and what ERP effects may reflect such a process. Future studies can classify different types of speech-gesture integration and investigate neural processes that underlie each type. Another important topic for future studies is to investigate how the neural processes of speech-gesture integration change when the relative timing between the speech stimulus and the gesture stimulus changes.

      We greatly appreciate Reviewer2 ’s thoughtful concern regarding whether "mutual information" adequately captures the interplay between the meanings of speech and gesture. We would like to clarify that the materials used in the present study involved gestures that were performed without actual objects, paired with verbs that precisely describe the corresponding actions. For example, a hammering gesture was paired with the verb “hammer”, and a cutting gesture was paired with the verb “cut”. In this design, all gestures conveyed redundant information relative to the co-occurring speech, creating significant overlap between the information derived from speech alone and that from gesture alone.

      We understand the reviewer’s concern about cases where gestures and speech might provide complementary, rather than redundant, information. To address this, we have developed an alternative metric for quantifying information gains contributed by supplementary multisensory cues, which will be explored in a subsequent study. However, for the present study, we believe that the observed overlap in information serves as a key indicator of multisensory convergence, a central focus of our investigation.

      Regarding the reviewer’s concern about how neural processes of speech-gesture integration may change with varying relative timing between speech and gesture stimuli, we would like to highlight findings from our previous study (Zhao, 2023, Frontiers in Psychology). In that study, we explored the semantic predictive role of gestures relative to speech under two timing conditions: (1) gestures preceding speech by a fixed interval of 200 ms, and (2) gestures preceding speech at its semantic identification point. Interestingly, only in the second condition did we observe time-window-selective disruptions of the semantic congruency effect in the IFG and pMTG. This led us to conclude that gestures play a semantic priming role for co-occurring speech. Building on this, we designed the present study with gestures deliberately preceding speech at its semantic identification point to reflect this semantic priming relationship. Additionally, ongoing research in our lab is exploring gesture and speech interactions in natural conversational settings to investigate whether the neural processes identified here remain consistent across varying contexts.

      To address potential concerns and ensure clarity regarding the limitations of the MI measurement, we have included a discussion of tthis in the revised manuscript in Lines 543-547: ‘Furthermore, MI quantifies overlap in gesture-speech integration, primarily when gestures convey redundant meaning. Consequently, the conclusions drawn in this study are constrained to contexts in which gestures serve to reinforce the meaning of the speech. Future research should aim to explore the neural responses in cases where gestures convey supplementary, rather than redundant, semantic information.’ This is followed by a clarification of the timing relationship between gesture and speech: ‘Note that the sequential cortical involvement and ERP components discussed above are derived from a deliberate alignment of speech onset with gesture DP, creating an artificial priming effect with gesture semantically preceding speech. Caution is advised when generalizing these findings to the spontaneous gesture-speech relationships, although gestures naturally precede speech[34].’ (Lines 539-543).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In this useful study, Zhao et al. try to extend the evidence for their previously described two-step model of speech-gesture integration in the posterior Middle Temporal Gyrus (pMTG) and Inferior Frontal Gyrus (IFG). They repeat some of their previous experimental paradigms, but this time quantifying Information-Theoretical (IT) metrics of the stimuli in a stroop-like paradigm purported to engage speech-gesture integration. They then correlate these metrics with the disruption of what they claim to be an integration effect observable in reaction times during the tasks following brain stimulation, as well as documenting the ERP components in response to the variability in these metrics.

      The integration of multiple methods, like tDCS, TMS, and ERPs to provide converging evidence renders the results solid. However, their interpretation of the results should be taken with care, as some critical confounds, like difficulty, were not accounted for, and the conceptual link between the IT metrics and what the authors claim they index is tenuous and in need of more evidence. In some cases, the difficulty making this link seems to arise from conceptual equivocation (e.g., their claims regarding 'graded' evidence), whilst in some others it might arise from the usage of unclear wording in the writing of the manuscript (e.g. the sentence 'quantitatively functional mental states defined by a specific parser unified by statistical regularities'). Having said that, the authors' aim is valuable, and addressing these issues would render the work a very useful approach to improve our understanding of integration during semantic processing, being of interest to scientists working in cognitive neuroscience and neuroimaging.

      The main hurdle to achieving the aims set by the authors is the presence of the confound of difficulty in their IT metrics. Their measure of entropy, for example, being derived from the distribution of responses of the participants to the stimuli, will tend to be high for words or gestures with multiple competing candidate representations (this is what would presumptively give rise to the diversity of responses in high-entropy items). There is ample evidence implicating IFG and pMTG as key regions of the semantic control network, which is critical during difficult semantic processing when, for example, semantic processing must resolve competition between multiple candidate representations, or when there are increased selection pressures (Jackson et al., 2021). Thus, the authors' interpretation of Mutual Information (MI) as an index of integration is inextricably contaminated with difficulty arising from multiple candidate representations. This casts doubt on the claims of the role of pMTG and IFG as regions carrying out gesture-speech integration as the observed pattern of results could also be interpreted in terms of brain stimulation interrupting the semantic control network's ability to select the best candidate for a given context or respond to more demanding semantic processing.

      Response 1: We sincerely thank the reviewer for pointing out the confound of difficulty. The primary aim of this study is to investigate whether the degree of activity in the established integration hubs, IFG and pMTG, is influenced by the information provided by gesture-speech modalities and/or their interactions. While we provided evidence for the differential involvement of the IFG and pMTG by delineating their dynamic engagement across distinct time windows of gesture-speech integration and associating these patterns with unisensory information and their interaction, we acknowledge that the mechanisms underlying these dynamics remain open to interpretation. Specifically, whether the observed effects stem from difficulties in semantic control processes, as suggested by the reviewer, or from resolving information uncertainty, as quantified by entropy, falls outside the scope of the current study. Importantly, we view these two interpretations as complementary rather than mutually exclusive, as both may be contributing factors. Nonetheless, we agree that addressing this question is a compelling avenue for future research.

      In the revised manuscript, we have included an additional analysis to assess whether the confounding effects of lexical or semantic control difficulty—specifically, the number of available responses—affect the neural outcomes. To address this, we performed partial correlation analyses, controlling for the number of responses.

      We would like to clarify an important distinction between the measure of entropy derived from the distribution of responses and the concept of response diversity. Entropy, in our analysis, is computed based on the probability distribution of each response, as captured by the information entropy formula. In contrast, response diversity refers to the simple count of different responses provided. Mutual Information (MI), by its nature, is also an entropy measure, quantifying the overlap in responses. For reference, although we observed a high correlation between the three information matrices and the number of responses (gesture entropy & gesture response number: r = 0.976, p < 0.001; speech entropy & speech response number: r = 0.961, p < 0.001; MI & total response number: r = 0.818, p < 0.001), it is crucial to emphasize that these metrics capture different aspects of the semantic information represented. In the revised manuscript, we have provided a table detailing both entropy and response numbers for each stimulus, to allow for greater transparency and clarity.

      Furthermore, we have added a comprehensive description of the partial correlation analysis conducted across all three experiments in the methodology section: for Experiment 1, please refer to Lines 213–222: ‘To account for potential confounds related to multiple candidate representations, we conducted partial correlation analyses between the tDCS effects and gesture entropy, speech entropy, and MI, controlling for the number of responses provided for each gesture and speech, as well as the total number of combined responses. Given that HD-tDCS induces overall disruption at the targeted brain regions, we hypothesized that the neural activity within the left IFG and pMTG would be progressively affected by varying levels of multisensory convergence, as indexed by MI. Moreover, we hypothesized that the modulation of neural activity by MI would differ between the left IFG and pMTG, as reflected in the differential modulation of response numbers in the partial correlations, highlighting their distinct roles in semantic processing[37].’

      Experiment 2: ‘To control for potential confounds, partial correlations were also performed between the TMS effects and gesture entropy, speech entropy, and MI, controlling for the number of responses for each gesture and speech, as well as the total number of combined responses. By doing this, we can determine how the time-sensitive contribution of the left IFG and pMTG to gesture–speech integration was affected by gesture and speech information distribution.’ (Lines 242–246).

      Experiment 3: ‘Additionally, partial correlations were conducted, accounting for the number of responses for each respective metric’ (Lines 292–293).

      As anticipated by the reviewer, we observed a consistent modulation of response numbers across both regions as well as across the four ERP components and associated clusters. The detailed results are presented below:

      Experiment 1: ‘However, partial correlation analysis, controlling for the total response number, revealed that the initially significant correlation between the Cathodal-tDCS effect and MI was no longer significant (r = -0.303, p = 0.222, 95% CI = [-0.770, 0.164]). This suggests that the observed relationship between Cathodal-tDCS and MI may be confounded by semantic control difficulty, as reflected by the total number of responses. Specifically, the reduced activity in the IFG under Cathodal-tDCS may be driven by variations in the difficulty of semantic control rather than a direct modulation of MI.’ (Lines 310-316) and ‘’Importantly, the reduced activity in the pMTG under Cathodal-tDCS was not influenced by the total response number, as indicated by the non-significant correlation (r = -0.253, p = 0.295, 95% CI = [-0.735, 0.229]). This finding was further corroborated by the unchanged significance in the partial correlation between Cathodal-tDCS and MI, when controlling for the total response number (r = -0.472, p = 0.048, 95% CI = [-0.903, -0.041]). (Lines 324-328).

      Experiment 2:’ Notably, inhibition of pMTG activity in TW2 was not influenced by the number of speech responses (r = -0.539, p = 0.087, 95% CI = [-1.145, 0.067]). However, the number of speech responses did affect the modulation of speech entropy on the pMTG inhibition effect in TW2. This was evidenced by the non-significant partial correlation between pMTG inhibition and speech entropy when controlling for speech response number (r = -0.218, p = 0.545, 95% CI = [-0.563, 0.127]).

      In contrast, the interrupted IFG activity in TW6 appeared to be consistently influenced by the confound of semantic control difficulty. This was reflected in the significant correlation with both gesture response number (r = -0.480, p = 0.032, 95% CI = [-904, -0.056]), speech response number (r = -0.729, p = 0.011, 95% CI = [-1.221, -0.237]), and total response number (r = -0.591, p = 0.008, 95% CI = [-0.993, -0.189]). Additionally, partial correlation analyses revealed non-significant relationship between interrupted IFG activity in TW6 and gesture entropy (r = -0.369, p = 0.120, 95% CI = [-0.810, -0.072]), speech entropy (r = -0.455, p = 0.187, 95% CI = [-1.072, 0.162]), and MI (r = -0.410, p = 0.091, 95% CI = [-0.856, -0.036]) when controlling for response numbers.’ (Lines 349-363)

      Experiment 3: ‘To clarify potential confounds of semantic control difficulty, partial correlation analyses were conducted to examine the relationship between the elicited ERP components and the relevant information matrices, controlling for response numbers. Results consistently indicated modulation by response numbers in the relationship of ERP components with the information matrix, as evidenced by the non-significant partial correlations between the P1 amplitude (P1 component over ML: r = -0.574, p = 0.082, 95% CI = [-1.141, -0.007]) and the P1 cluster (r = -0.503, p = 0.138, 95% CI = [-1.102, 0.096]) with speech entropy; the N1-P2 amplitude (N1-P2 component over LA: r = -0.080, p = 0.746, 95% CI = [-0.554, 0.394]) and N1-P2 cluster (r \= -0.179, p = 0.464, 95% CI = [-0.647, 0.289]) with gesture entropy; the N400 amplitude (N400 component over LA: r = 0.264, p = 0.247, 95% CI = [-0.195,0.723]) and N400 cluster (r = 0.394, p = 0.095, 95% CI = [-0.043, 0.831]) with gesture entropy; the N400 amplitude (N400 component over LA: r = -0.134, p = 0.595, 95% CI = [-0.620, 0.352]) and N400 cluster (r = -0.034, p = 0.894, 95% CI = [-0.524,0.456]) with MI; and the LPC amplitude (LPC component over LA: r \= -0.428, p = 0.217, 95% CI = [-1.054, 0.198]) and LPC cluster (r \= -0.202, p = 0.575, 95% CI = [-0.881, 0.477]) with speech entropy.’ (Lines 424-438)

      Based on the above results, we conclude that there is a dynamic interplay between the difficulty of semantic representation and the control pressures that shape the resulting neural responses. Furthermore, while the role of the IFG in control processes remains consistent, the present study reveals a more segmented role for the pMTG. Specifically, although the pMTG is well-established in the processing of distributed speech information, the integration of multisensory convergence, as indexed by MI, did not elicit the same control-related modulation in pMTG activity. A comprehensive discussion of the control process in shaping neural responses, as well as the specific roles of the IFG and pMTG in this process, is provided in the Discussion section in Lines (493-511): ‘Given that control processes are intrinsically integrated with semantic processing50, a distributed semantic representation enables dynamic modulation of access to and manipulation of meaningful information, thereby facilitating flexible control over the diverse possibilities inherent in a concept. Accordingly, an increased number of candidate responses amplifies the control demands necessary to resolve competing semantic representations. This effect was observed in the present study, where the association of the information matrix with the tDCS effect in IFG, the inhibition of pMTG activity in TW2, disruption of IFG activity in TW6, and modulation of four distinct ERP components collectively demonstrated that response quantity modulated neural activity. These results underscore the intricate interplay between the difficulty of semantic representation and the control pressures that shape the resulting neural responses. 

      The IFG and pMTG, central components of the semantic control network, have been extensively implicated in previous research 50-52. While the role of the IFG in managing both unisensory information and multisensory convergence remains consistent, as evidenced by the confounding difficulty results across Experiments 1 and 2, the current study highlights a more context-dependent function for the pMTG. Specifically, although the pMTG is well-established in the processing of distributed speech information, the multisensory convergence, indexed by MI, did not evoke the same control-related modulation in pMTG activity. These findings suggest that, while the pMTG is critical to semantic processing, its engagement in control processes is likely modulated by the specific nature of the sensory inputs involved’

      Reference:

      Tesink, C.M.J.Y., Petersson, K.M., van Berkum, J.J.A., van den Brink, D., Buitelaar, J.K., and Hagoort, P. (2009). Unification of speaker and meaning in language comprehension: An fMRI study. J Cognitive Neurosci 21, 2085-2099. 10.1162/jocn.2008.21161

      Jackson, R.L. (2021). The neural correlates of semantic control revisited. Neuroimage 224, 117444. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117444.

      Jefferies, E. (2013). The neural basis of semantic cognition: converging evidence from neuropsychology, neuroimaging and TMS. Cortex 49, 611-625. 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.008.

      Noonan, K.A., Jefferies, E., Visser, M., and Lambon Ralph, M.A. (2013). Going beyond inferior prefrontal involvement in semantic control: evidence for the additional contribution of dorsal angular gyrus and posterior middle temporal cortex. J Cogn Neurosci 25, 1824-1850. 10.1162/jocn_a_00442.

      In terms of conceptual equivocation, the use of the term 'graded' by the authors seems to be different from the usage commonly employed in the semantic cognition literature (e.g., the 'graded hub hypothesis', Rice et al., 2015). The idea of a graded hub in the controlled semantic cognition framework (i.e., the anterior temporal lobe) refers to a progressive degree of abstraction or heteromodal information as you progress through the anatomy of the region (i.e., along the dorsal-to-ventral axis). The authors, on the other hand, seem to refer to 'graded manner' in the context of a correlation of entropy or MI and the change in the difference between Reaction Times (RTs) of semantically congruent vs incongruent gesture-speech. The issue is that the discourse through parts of the introduction and discussion seems to conflate both interpretations, and the ideas in the main text do not correspond to the references they cite. This is not overall very convincing. What is it exactly the authors are arguing about the correlation between RTs and MI indexes? As stated above, their measure of entropy captures the spread of responses, which could also be a measure of item difficulty (more diverse responses imply fewer correct responses, a classic index of difficulty). Capturing the diversity of responses means that items with high entropy scores are also likely to have multiple candidate representations, leading to increased selection pressures. Regions like pMTG and IFG have been widely implicated in difficult semantic processing and increased selection pressures (Jackson et al., 2021). How is this MI correlation evidence of integration that proceeds in a 'graded manner'? The conceptual links between these concepts must be made clearer for the interpretation to be convincing.

      Response 2: Regarding the concern of conceptual equivocation, we would like to emphasize that this study represents the first attempt to focus on the relationship between information quantity and neural engagement, a question addressed in three experiments. Experiment 1 (HD-tDCS) targeted the entire gesture-speech integration process in the IFG and pMTG to assess whether neural activity in these regions, previously identified as integration hubs, is modulated by changes in informativeness from both modalities (i.e., entropy) and their interactions (MI). The results revealed a gradual inhibition of neural activity in both areas as MI increased, evidenced by a negative correlation between MI and the tDCS inhibition effect in both regions. Building on this, Experiments 2 and 3 employed double-pulse TMS and ERPs to further assess whether the engaged neural activity was both time-sensitive and staged. These experiments also evaluated the contributions of various sources of information, revealing correlations between information-theoretic metrics and time-locked brain activity, providing insights into the ‘gradual’ nature of gesture-speech integration.

      Therefore, the incremental engagement of the integration hub of IFG and pMTG along with the informativeness of gesture and speech during multisensory integration is different from the "graded hub," which refers to anatomical distribution. We sincerely apologize for this oversight. In the revised manuscript, we have changed the relevant conceptual equivocation in Lines 44-60: ‘Consensus acknowledges the presence of 'convergence zones' within the temporal and inferior parietal areas [1], or the 'semantic hub' located in the anterior temporal lobe[2], pivotal for integrating, converging, or distilling multimodal inputs. Contemporary theories frame the semantic processing as a dynamic sequence of neural states[3], shaped by systems that are finely tuned to the statistical regularities inherent in sensory inputs[4]. These regularities enable the brain to evaluate, weight, and integrate multisensory information, optimizing the reliability of individual sensory signals[5]. However, sensory inputs available to the brain are often incomplete and uncertain, necessitating adaptive neural adjustments to resolve these ambiguities [6]. In this context, neuronal activity is thought to be linked to the probability density of sensory information, with higher levels of uncertainty resulting in the engagement of a broader population of neurons, thereby reflecting the brain’s adaptive capacity to handle diverse possible interpretations[7,8]. Although the role of 'convergence zones' and 'semantic hubs' in integrating multimodal inputs is well established, the precise functional patterns of neural activity in response to the distribution of unified multisensory information—along with the influence of unisensory signals—remain poorly understood.

      To this end, we developed an analytic approach to directly probe the cortical engagement during multisensory gesture-speech semantic integration.’  

      Furthermore, in the Discussion section, we have replaced the term 'graded' with 'incremental' (Line 456,). Additionally, we have included a discussion on the progressive nature of neural engagement, as evidenced by the correlation between RTs and MI indices in Lines 483-492: ‘The varying contributions of unisensory gesture-speech information and the convergence of multisensory inputs, as reflected in the correlation between distinct ERP components and TMS time windows (TMS TWs), are consistent with recent models suggesting that multisensory processing involves parallel detection of modality-specific information and hierarchical integration across multiple neural levels[4,48]. These processes are further characterized by coordination across multiple temporal scales[49]. Building on this, the present study offers additional evidence that the multi-level nature of gesture-speech processing is statistically structured, as measured by information matrix of unisensory entropy and multisensory convergence index of MI, the input of either source would activate a distributed representation, resulting in progressively functioning neural responses.’

      Reference:

      Damasio, H., Grabowski, T.J., Tranel, D., Hichwa, R.D., and Damasio, A.R. (1996). A neural basis for lexical retrieval. Nature 380, 499-505. DOI 10.1038/380499a0.

      Patterson, K., Nestor, P.J., and Rogers, T.T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 8, 976-987. 10.1038/nrn2277.

      Brennan, J.R., Stabler, E.P., Van Wagenen, S.E., Luh, W.M., and Hale, J.T. (2016). Abstract linguistic structure correlates with temporal activity during naturalistic comprehension. Brain and Language 157, 81-94. 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.04.008.

      Benetti, S., Ferrari, A., and Pavani, F. (2023). Multimodal processing in face-to-face interactions: A bridging link between psycholinguistics and sensory neuroscience. Front Hum Neurosci 17, 1108354. 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1108354.

      Noppeney, U. (2021). Perceptual Inference, Learning, and Attention in a Multisensory World. Annual Review of Neuroscience, Vol 44, 2021 44, 449-473. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-100120-085519.

      Ma, W.J., and Jazayeri, M. (2014). Neural coding of uncertainty and probability. Annu Rev Neurosci 37, 205-220. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014017.

      Fischer, B.J., and Pena, J.L. (2011). Owl's behavior and neural representation predicted by Bayesian inference. Nat Neurosci 14, 1061-1066. 10.1038/nn.2872.

      Ganguli, D., and Simoncelli, E.P. (2014). Efficient sensory encoding and Bayesian inference with heterogeneous neural populations. Neural Comput 26, 2103-2134. 10.1162/NECO_a_00638.

      Meijer, G.T., Mertens, P.E.C., Pennartz, C.M.A., Olcese, U., and Lansink, C.S. (2019). The circuit architecture of cortical multisensory processing: Distinct functions jointly operating within a common anatomical network. Prog Neurobiol 174, 1-15. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.01.004.

      Senkowski, D., and Engel, A.K. (2024). Multi-timescale neural dynamics for multisensory integration. Nat Rev Neurosci 25, 625-642. 10.1038/s41583-024-00845-7.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I have a number of small suggestions to make the paper more easy to understand.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for their careful reading and thoughtful consideration. All suggestions have been thoroughly addressed and incorporated into the revised manuscript.

      (1) Lines 86-87, please clarify whether "chronometric double-pulse TMS" should lead to either excitation or inhibition of neural activities

      Double-pulse TMS elicits inhibition of neural activities (see responses to editors), which has been clarified in the revised manuscript in Lines 90-93: ‘we applied inhibitory chronometric double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to specific temporal windows associated with integration processes in these regions[23], assessing whether the inhibitory effects of TMS were correlated with unisensory entropy or the multisensory convergence index (MI)’

      (2) Line 106 "validated by replicating the semantic congruencey effect". Please specify what the task was in the validation study.

      The description of the validation task has been added in Lines 116-119: ‘To validate the stimuli, 30 participants were recruited to replicate the multisensory index of semantic congruency effect, hypothesizing that reaction times for semantically incongruent gesture-speech pairs would be significantly longer than those for congruent pairs.’

      (3) Line 112. "30 subjects". Are they Chinese speakers?

      Yes, all participants in the present study, including those in the pre-tests, are native Chinese speakers.

      (4) Line 122, "responses for each item" Please specify whether you mean here "the comprehensive answer" as you defined in 118-119.

      Yes, and this information has been added in Lines 136-137: ‘comprehensive responses for each item were converted into Shannon's entropy (H)’

      (5) Line 163 "one of three stimulus types (Anodal, Cathodal or Sham)". Please specify whether the order of the three conditions was counterbalanced across participants. Or, whether the order was fixed for all participants.

      The order of the three conditions was counterbalanced across participants, a clearer description has been added in the revised manuscript in Lines 184-189: ‘Participants were divided into two groups, with each group undergoing HD-tDCS stimulation at different target sites (IFG or pMTG). Each participant completed three experimental sessions, spaced one week apart, during which 480 gesture-speech pairs were presented across various conditions. In each session, participants received one of three types of HD-tDCS stimulation: Anodal, Cathodal, or Sham. The order of stimulation site and type was counterbalanced using a Latin square design to control for potential order effects.’

      (6) Line 191-192, "difference in reaction time between semantic incongruence and semantic congruent pairs)" Here, please specify which reaction time was subtracted from which one. This information is very crucial; without it, you cannot interpret your graphs.

      (17) Figure 3. Figure caption for (A). "The semantic congruence effect was calculated as the reaction time difference between...". You need to specify which condition was subtracted from what condition; otherwise, you cannot interpret this figure. "difference" is too ambiguous.

      Corrections have been made in the revised manuscript in Lines 208-211: ‘Neural responses were quantified based on the effects of HD-tDCS (active tDCS minus sham tDCS) on the semantic congruency effect, defined as the difference in reaction times between semantic incongruent and congruent conditions (Rt(incongruent) - Rt(congruent))’ and Line 796-798: ‘The semantic congruency effect was calculated as the reaction time (RT) difference between semantically incongruent and semantically congruent pairs (Rt(incongruent) - Rt(congruent))’.

      (7) Line 363 "progressive inhibition of IFG and pMTG by HD-tDCS as the degree of gesture-speech interaction, indexed by MI, advanced." This sentence is very hard to follow. I don't understand what part of the data in Figure 3 speaks to "inhibition of IFG". And what is "HD-tDCS"? I think it is easier to read if you talk about correlation (not "progressive" and "advanced").

      High-Definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) was applied to modulate the activity of pMTG and IFG, with cathodal stimulation inducing inhibitory effects and anodal stimulation facilitating neural activity. In Figure 3, we examined the relationship between the tDCS effects on pMTG and IFG and the three information matrices (entropy and MI). Our results revealed significant correlations between MI and the cathodal-tDCS effects in both regions. We acknowledge that the original phrasing may have been unclear, and in the revised manuscript, we have provided a more explicit explanation to enhance clarity in Lines 443-445: ‘Our results, for the first time, revealed that the inhibition effect of cathodal-tDCS on the pMTG and IFG correlated with the degree of gesture-speech multisensory convergence, as indexed by MI’.

      (8) Lines 367-368 I don't understand why gesture is top down and speech is bottom up. Is that because gesture precedes speech (gesture is interpretable at the point of speech onset)?

      Yes, since we employed a semantic priming paradigm by aligning speech onset with the gesture comprehension point, we interpret the gesture-speech integration process as an interaction between the top-down prediction from gestures and the bottom-up processing of speech. In the revised manuscript, we have provided a clearer and more coherent description that aligns with the results. Lines 445-449: ‘Moreover, the gradual neural engagement was found to be time-sensitive and staged, as evidenced by the selectively interrupted time windows (Experiment 2) and the distinct correlated ERP components (Experiment 3), which were modulated by different information contributors, including unisensory entropy or multisensory MI’

      (9) Line 380 - 381. Can you spell out "TW" and "IP"?

      (16) Line 448, NIBS, Please spell out "NIBS".

      "TW" have been spelled out in Lines 459: ‘time windows (TW)’,"IP" in Line 460: ‘identification point (IP)’. The term "NIBS" was replaced with "HD-tDCS and TMS" to provide clearer specification of the techniques employed: ‘Consistent with this, the present study provides robust evidence, through the application of HD-tDCS and TMS, that the integration hubs for gesture and speech—the pMTG and IFG—operate in an incremental manner.’ (Lines 454-457). 

      (10) Line 419, The higher certainty of gesture => The higher the certainty of gesture is

      (13) Line 428, "a larger MI" => "a larger MI is"

      (12) Line 427-428, "the larger overlapped neural populations" => "the larger, the overlapped neural populations"

      Changes have been made in Line 522 ‘The higher the certainty of gesture is’ , Line 531: ‘a larger MI is’ and Line 530 ‘the larger, overlapped neural populations’

      (11) Line 423 "Greater TMS effect over the IFG" Can you describe the TMS effect?

      TMS effect has been described as ‘Greater TMS inhibitory effect’ (Line 526)

      (14) Line 423 "reweighting effect" What is this? Please describe (and say which experiment it is about).

      Clearer description has been provided in Lines 535-538: ‘As speech entropy increases, indicating greater uncertainty in the information provided by speech, more cognitive effort is directed towards selecting the targeted semantic representation. This leads to enhanced involvement of the IFG and a corresponding reduction in LPC amplitude’.

      (15) Line 437 "the graded functionality of every disturbed period is not guaranteed" (I don't understand this sentence).

      Clearer description has been provided in Lines 552-557: ‘Additionally, not all influenced TWs exhibited significant associations with entropy and MI. While HD-tDCS and TMS may impact functionally and anatomically connected brain regions[55,56], whether the absence of influence in certain TWs can be attributed to compensation by other connected brain areas, such as angular gyrus[57] or anterior temporal lobe[58], warrants further investigation. Therefore, caution is needed when interpreting the causal relationship between inhibition effects of brain stimulation and information-theoretic metrics (entropy and MI).

      References:

      Humphreys, G. F., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Simons, J. S. (2021). A Unifying Account of Angular Gyrus Contributions to Episodic and Semantic Cognition. Trends in neurosciences, 44(6), 452–463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2021.01.006

      Bonner, M. F., & Price, A. R. (2013). Where is the anterior temporal lobe and what does it do?. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(10), 4213–4215. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0041-13.2013

      (18) Figure 4. "TW1", "TW2", etc. are not informative. Either replace them with the actual manuscript or add manuscript information (either in the graph itself or in the figure title).

      Information was added into the figure title ‘Figure 4. TMS impacts on semantic congruency effect across various time windows (TW).’ (Line 804), included a detailed description of each time window in Lines 805-807: ‘(A) Five time windows (TWs) showing selective disruption of gesture-speech integration were chosen: TW1 (-120 to -80 ms relative to speech identification point), TW2 (-80 to -40 ms), TW3 (-40 to 0 ms), TW6 (80 to 120 ms), and TW7 (120 to 160 ms).’

      (19) Table 2C.

      The last column is titled "p(xi, yi)". I don't understand why the authors use this label for this column.

      In the formula, at the very end, there is "p(xi|yi). I wonder why it is p(xi|yi), as opposed to p(yi|xi).

      Mutual Information (MI) was calculated by subtracting the entropy of the combined gesture-speech dataset (Entropy(gesture + speech)) from the sum of the individual entropies of gesture and speech (Entropy(gesture) + Entropy(speech)). Thus, the p(xi,yi) aimed to describe the entropy of the combined dataset. We acknowledge the potential ambiguity in the original description, and in the revised manuscript, we have changed the formula of p(xi,yi) into ‘p(xi+yi)’ (Line 848) in Table 2C, and the relevant equation of MI ‘’. Also we provided a clear MI calculation process in Lines 143-146: ‘MI was used to measure the overlap between gesture and speech information, calculated by subtracting the entropy of the combined gesture-speech dataset (Entropy(gesture + speech)) from the sum of their individual entropies (Entropy(gesture) + Entropy(speech)) (see Appendix Table 2C)’.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The authors should try and produce data showing that the confound of difficulty due to the number of lexical or semantic representations is not underlying high-entropy items if they wish to improve the credibility of their claim that the disruption of the congruency effect is due to speech-gesture integration. Additionally, they should provide more evidence either in the form of experiments or references to better justify why mutual information is an index for integration in the first place.

      Response 1: An additional analysis has been conducted to assess whether the number of lexical or semantic representations affect the neural outcomes, please see details in the Responses to Reviewer 3 (public review) response 1.

      Mutual information (MI), a concept rooted in information theory, quantifies the reduction in uncertainty about one signal when the other is known, thereby capturing the statistical dependence between them. MI is calculated as the difference between the individual entropies of each signal and their joint entropy, which reflects the total uncertainty when both signals are considered together. This metric aligns with the core principle of multisensory integration: different modalities reduce uncertainty about each other by providing complementary, predictive information. Higher MI values signify that the integration of sensory signals results in a more coherent and unified representation, while lower MI values indicate less integration or greater divergence between the modalities. As such, MI serves as a robust and natural index for assessing the degree of multisensory integration.

      To date, the use of MI as an index of integration has been limited, with one notable study by Tremblay et al. (2016), cited in the manuscript, using pointwise MI to quantify the extent to which two syllables mutually constrain each other. While MI has been extensively applied in natural language processing to measure the co-occurrence strength between words (e.g., Lin et al., 2012), its application as an index of multisensory convergence—particularly in the context of gesture-speech integration as employed in this study—is novel. In the revised manuscript, we have clarified the relationship between MI and multisensory convergence: ‘MI assesses share information between modalities[25],indicating multisensory convergence and acting as an index of gesture-speech integration’ (Lines 73-74).

      Also, in our study, we calculated MI as per its original definition, by subtracting the entropy of summed dataset of gesture-speech from the combined entropies of gesture and speech. The detailed calculation method is provided in Lines 136-152: ‘To quantify information content, comprehensive responses for each item were converted into Shannon's entropy (H) as a measure of information richness (Figure 1A bottom). With no significant gender differences observed in both gesture (t(20) = 0.21, p = 0.84) and speech (t(20) = 0.52, p = 0.61), responses were aggregated across genders, resulting in 60 answers per item (Appendix Table 2). Here, p(xi) and p(yi) represent the distribution of 60 answers for a given gesture (Appendix Table 2B) and speech (Appendix Table 2A), respectively. High entropy indicates diverse answers, reflecting broad representation, while low entropy suggests focused lexical recognition for a specific item (Figure 2B). MI was used to measure the overlap between gesture and speech information, calculated by subtracting the entropy of the combined gesture-speech dataset (Entropy(gesture + speech)) from the sum of their individual entropies (Entropy(gesture) + Entropy(speech)) (see Appendix Table 2C). For specific gesture-speech combinations, equivalence between the combined entropy and the sum of individual entropies (gesture or speech) indicates absence of overlap in response sets. Conversely, significant overlap, denoted by a considerable number of shared responses between gesture and speech datasets, leads to a noticeable discrepancy between combined entropy and the sum of gesture and speech entropies. Elevated MI values thus signify substantial overlap, indicative of a robust mutual interaction between gesture and speech.’

      Additional examples outlined in Appendix Table 2 in Lines 841-848:

      This novel application of MI as a multisensory convergence index offers new insights into how different sensory modalities interact and integrate to shape semantic processing.

      Reference:

      Tremblay, P., Deschamps, I., Baroni, M., and Hasson, U. (2016). Neural sensitivity to syllable frequency and mutual information in speech perception and production. Neuroimage 136, 106-121. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.05.018

      Lin, W., Wu, Y., & Yu, L. (2012). Online Computation of Mutual Information and Word Context Entropy. International Journal of Future Computer and Communication, 167-169.

      (2) Finally, if the authors wish to address the graded hub hypothesis as posited by the controlled semantic cognition framework (e.g., Rice et al., 2015), they would have to stimulate a series of ROIs progressing gradually through the anatomy of their candidate regions showing the effects grow along this spline, more than simply correlate MI with RT differences.

      Response 2: We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful consideration. The incremental engagement of the integration hub of IFG and pMTG along with the informativeness of gesture and speech during multisensory integration is different from the concept of "graded hub," which refers to anatomical distribution. See Responses to reviewer 3 (public review) response 2 for details.

      (3) The authors report significant effects with p values as close to the threshold as p=0.49 for the pMTG correlation in Experiment 1, for example. How confident are the authors these results are reliable and not merely their 'statistical luck'? Especially in view of sample sizes that hover around 22-24 participants, which have been called into question in the field of non-invasive brain stimulation (e.g., Mitra et al, 2021)?

      Response 3: In Experiment 1, a total of 52 participants were assigned to two groups, each undergoing HD-tDCS stimulation over either the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) or posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG), yielding 26 participants per group for correlation analysis. Power analysis, conducted using G*Power, indicated that a sample size of 26 participants per group would provide sufficient power (0.8) to detect a large effect size (0.5) at an alpha level of 0.05, justifying the chosen sample size. To control for potential statistical artifacts, we compared the results to those from the unaffected control condition.

      In the Experiment 1, participants were tasked with a gender categorization task, where they responded as accurately and quickly as possible to the gender of the voice they saw, while gender congruency (e.g., a male gesture paired with a male voice or a female gesture with a male voice) was manipulated. This manipulation served as direct control, enabling the investigation of automatic and implicit semantic interactions between gesture and speech. This relevant information was provided in the manuscript in Lines 167-172:‘An irrelevant factor of gender congruency (e.g., a man making a gesture combined with a female voice) was created[22,23,35]. This involved aligning the gender of the voice with the corresponding gender of the gesture in either a congruent (e.g., male voice paired with a male gesture) or incongruent (e.g., male voice paired with a female gesture) manner. This approach served as a direct control mechanism, facilitating the investigation of the automatic and implicit semantic interplay between gesture and speech[35]’. Correlation analyses were conducted to examine the TMS disruption effects on gender congruency, comparing reaction times for gender-incongruent versus congruent trials. No significant correlations were found between TMS disruption effects on either the IFG (Cathodal-tDCS effect with MI: r = 0.102, p = 0.677; Anodal-tDCS effect with MI: r = 0.178, p = 0.466) or pMTG (Cathodal-tDCS effect with MI: r \= -0.201, p = 0.410; Anodal-tDCS effect with MI: r = -0.232, p = 0.338).

      Moreover, correlations between the TMS disruption effect on semantic congruency and both gesture entropy, speech entropy, and mutual information (MI) were examined. P-values of 0.290, 0.725, and 0.049 were observed, respectively.  

      The absence of a TMS effect on gender congruency, coupled with the lack of significance when correlated with the other information matrices, highlights the robustness of the significant finding at p = 0.049.

      (4) The distributions of entropy for gestures and speech are very unequal. Whilst entropy for gestures has high variability, (.12-4.3), that of speech is very low (ceiling effect?) with low variance. Can the authors comment on whether they think this might have affected their analyses or results in any way? For example, do they think this could be a problem when calculating MI, which integrates both measures? L130-131.'

      Response 4: We sincerely thank the reviewer for raising this insightful question. The core premise of the current study is that brain activity is modulated by the degree of information provided. Accordingly, the 20 entropy values for gesture and speech represent a subset of the overall entropy distribution, with the degree of entropy correlating with a distributed pattern of neural activity, regardless of the scale of variation. This hypothesis aligns with previous studies suggesting that neuronal activity is linked to the probability density of sensory information, with higher levels of uncertainty resulting in the engagement of a broader population of neurons, thereby reflecting the brain’s adaptive capacity to handle diverse possible interpretations (Fischer & Pena, 2011; Ganguli & Simoncelli, 2014).

      Importantly, we conducted another EEG experiment with 30 subjects. Given the inherent differences between gesture and speech, it is important to note that speech, being more structurally distinct, tends to exhibit lower variability than gesture. To prevent an imbalance in the distribution of gesture and speech, we manipulated the information content of each modality. Specifically, we created three conditions for both gesture and speech (i.e., 0.75, 1, and 1.25 times the identification threshold), thereby ensuring comparable variance between the two modalities: gesture (mean entropy = 2.91 ± 1.01) and speech (mean entropy = 1.82 ± 0.71) (Author response table 6).

      Full-factorial RSA analysis revealed an early P1 effect (0-100 ms) for gesture and a late LPC effect (734-780 ms) for speech (Author response image 2b). Crucially, the identified clusters showed significant correlations with both gesture (Author response image 2c1) and speech entropy (Author response image 2c3), respectively. These findings replicate the results of the present study, demonstrating that, irrespective of the variance in gesture and speech entropy, both modalities elicited ERP amplitude responses in a progressive manner that aligned with their respective information distributions.

      Regarding the influence on MI values, since MI was calculated based on the overlapping responses between gesture and speech, a reduction in uncertainty during speech comprehension would naturally result in a smaller contribution to the MI value. However, as hypothesized above, the MI values were also assumed to represent a subset of the overall distribution, where the contributions of both gesture and speech are expected to follow a normal distribution. This hypothesis was further supported by our replication experiment. When the contributions of gesture and speech were balanced, a correlation between MI values and N400 amplitude was observed (Author response image 2c2), consistent with the results reported in the present manuscript. These findings not only support the idea that the correlation between MI and ERP components is unaffected by the subset of MI values but also confirm the replicability of our results.

      Author response table 6.

      Quantitative entropy for each gesture stimulus (BD: before discrimination point; DP: discrimination point; AD: after discrimination point) and speech stimulus (BI: before identification point; IP: identification point; AI: after identification point).

      Author response image 2.

      Results of group-level analysis and full-factorial RSA. a: The full-factorial representational similarity analysis (RSA) framework is illustrated schematically. Within the general linear model (GLM), the light green matrix denotes the representational dissimilarity matrix (RDM) for gesture semantic states, while light blue matrix represents speech semantic states, and the light red matrix illustrates the semantic congruency effect. The symbol ‘e’ indicates the random error term. All matrices, including the neural dissimilarity matrix, are structured as 18 * 18 matrices, corresponding to 18 conditions (comprising 3 gesture semantic states, 3 speech semantic states, and 2 congruency conditions). b: Coding strength for gesture states, speech states and congruency effect. Shaded clusters represent regions where each factor exhibited significant effects. Clusters with lower opacity correspond to areas where the grand-mean ERP amplitudes across conditions showed the highest correlation with unimodal entropy or MI. c1-c6: Topographical correlation maps illustrate the four significant RSA clusters (top), accompanied by the highest correlations between ERP amplitudes within the significant RSA clusters and the information matrices (bottom). Black dots represent electrodes exhibiting significant correlations, while black stars highlight the electrode with the highest correlation coefficient.

      (5) L383: Why are the authors calling TW2 pre-lexical and TW6 post-lexical? I believe they must provide evidence or references justifying calling these periods pre- and post-lexical. This seems critical given the argument they're trying to make in this paragraph.

      Response 5: The time windows (TWs) selected for the current study were based on our previous work (Zhao et al., 2021, J. Neurosci). In that study, we employed a double-pulse TMS protocol, delivering stimulation across eight 40-ms time windows: three windows preceding the speech identification point (TWs 1-3) and five windows following it (TWs 4-8). The pre-lexical time windows (TWs 1-3) occur before speech identification, while the post-lexical time windows (TWs 4-8) occur after this point. in the revised manuscript, we have made that clear in Lines 462-466:

      “In TW2 of gesture-speech integration, which precedes the speech identification point23 and represents a pre-lexical stage, the suppression effect observed in the pMTG was correlated with speech entropy. Conversely, during TW6, which follows the speech identification point23 and represents a post-lexical stage, the IFG interruption effect was influenced by both gesture entropy, speech entropy, and their MI”

      Reference:

      Zhao, W., Li, Y., and Du, Y. (2021). TMS reveals dynamic interaction between inferior frontal gyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus in gesture-speech semantic integration. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10356-10364. 10.1523/jneurosci.1355-21.2021.

      (6) Below, I recommend the authors improve their description of the criteria employed to select ROIs. This is important for several reasons. For example, the lack of a control ROI presumably not implicated in integration makes the interpretation of the specificity of the results difficult. Additionally, other regions have been proposed more consistently by recent evidence as multimodal integrators, like for example, the angular gyrus (Humphreys, 2021), or the anterior temporal lobe. The inclusion of IFG as a key region for integration and the oversight of angular gyrus seems to me unjustified in the light of recent evidence.

      Response 6: We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful consideration. The selection of IFG and pMTG as ROIs was based on a meta-analysis of multiple fMRI studies on gesture-speech integration, in which these two locations were consistently identified as activated. See Table 2 for details of the studies and coordinates of brain locations reported.

      Author response table 7.

      Meta-analysis of previous studies on gesture-speech integration.

      Based on the meta-analysis of previous studies, we selected the IFG and pMTG as ROIs for gesture-speech integration. The rationale for selecting these brain regions is outlined in the introduction in Lines 65-68: ‘Empirical studies have investigated the semantic integration between gesture and speech by manipulating their semantic relationship[15-18] and revealed a mutual interaction between them[19-21] as reflected by the N400 latency and amplitude[14] as well as common neural underpinnings in the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG)[15,22,23]’.

      And further described in Lines 79-80: ‘_Experiment 1 employed high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) to administer Anodal, Cathodal and Sham stimulation to either the IFG or the pMTG ’._ And Lines 87-90: ‘Given the differential involvement of the IFG and pMTG in gesture-speech integration, shaped by top-down gesture predictions and bottom-up speech processing [23], Experiment 2 was designed to assess whether the activity of these regions was associated with relevant informational matrices’.

      In the Methods section, we clarified the selection of coordinates in Lines 193-199: ‘Building on a meta-analysis of prior fMRI studies examining gesture-speech integration[22], we targeted Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) coordinates for the left IFG at (-62, 16, 22) and the pMTG at (-50, -56, 10). In the stimulation protocol for HD-tDCS, the IFG was targeted using electrode F7 as the optimal cortical projection site[36], with four return electrodes placed at AF7, FC5, F9, and FT9. For the pMTG, TP7 was selected as the cortical projection site36, with return electrodes positioned at C5, P5, T9, and P9.’

      The selection of IFG or pMTG as integration hubs for gesture and speech has also been validated in our previous studies. Specifically, Zhao et al. (2018, J. Neurosci) applied TMS to both areas. Results demonstrated that disrupting neural activity in the IFG or pMTG via TMS selectively impaired the semantic congruency effect (reaction time costs due to semantic incongruence), while leaving the gender congruency effect unaffected. These findings identified the IFG and pMTG as crucial hubs for gesture-speech integration, guiding the selection of brain regions for our subsequent studies.

      In addition, Zhao et al. (2021, J. Neurosci) employed a double-pulse TMS protocol across eight 40-ms time windows to explore the temporal dynamics of the IFG and pMTG. The results revealed time-window-selective disruptions of the semantic congruency effect, further supporting the dynamic and temporally staged involvement of these regions in gesture-speech integration.

      While we have solid rationale for selecting the IFG and pMTG as key regions, we acknowledge the reviewer's point that the involvement of additional functionally and anatomically brain areas, cannot be excluded. We have included in the discussion as limitations in Lines 552-557: ‘Additionally, not all influenced TWs exhibited significant associations with entropy and MI. While HD-tDCS and TMS may impact functionally and anatomically connected brain regions[55,56], whether the absence of influence in certain TWs can be attributed to compensation by other connected brain areas, such as angular gyrus[57] or anterior temporal lobe[58], warrants further investigation. Therefore, caution is needed when interpreting the causal relationship between inhibition effects of brain stimulation and information-theoretic metrics (entropy and MI).

      References:

      Willems, R.M., Ozyurek, A., and Hagoort, P. (2009). Differential roles for left inferior frontal and superior temporal cortex in multimodal integration of action and language. Neuroimage 47, 1992-2004. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.066.

      Drijvers, L., Jensen, O., and Spaak, E. (2021). Rapid invisible frequency tagging reveals nonlinear integration of auditory and visual information. Human Brain Mapping 42, 1138-1152. 10.1002/hbm.25282.

      Drijvers, L., and Ozyurek, A. (2018). Native language status of the listener modulates the neural integration of speech and iconic gestures in clear and adverse listening conditions. Brain and Language 177, 7-17. 10.1016/j.bandl.2018.01.003.

      Drijvers, L., van der Plas, M., Ozyurek, A., and Jensen, O. (2019). Native and non-native listeners show similar yet distinct oscillatory dynamics when using gestures to access speech in noise. Neuroimage 194, 55-67. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.03.032.

      Holle, H., and Gunter, T.C. (2007). The role of iconic gestures in speech disambiguation: ERP evidence. J Cognitive Neurosci 19, 1175-1192. 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.7.1175.

      Kita, S., and Ozyurek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. J Mem Lang 48, 16-32. 10.1016/S0749-596x(02)00505-3.

      Bernardis, P., and Gentilucci, M. (2006). Speech and gesture share the same communication system. Neuropsychologia 44, 178-190. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.05.007.

      Zhao, W.Y., Riggs, K., Schindler, I., and Holle, H. (2018). Transcranial magnetic stimulation over left inferior frontal and posterior temporal cortex disrupts gesture-speech integration. Journal of Neuroscience 38, 1891-1900. 10.1523/Jneurosci.1748-17.2017.

      Zhao, W., Li, Y., and Du, Y. (2021). TMS reveals dynamic interaction between inferior frontal gyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus in gesture-speech semantic integration. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10356-10364. 10.1523/jneurosci.1355-21.2021.

      Hartwigsen, G., Bzdok, D., Klein, M., Wawrzyniak, M., Stockert, A., Wrede, K., Classen, J., and Saur, D. (2017). Rapid short-term reorganization in the language network. Elife 6. 10.7554/eLife.25964.

      Jackson, R.L., Hoffman, P., Pobric, G., and Ralph, M.A.L. (2016). The semantic network at work and rest: Differential connectivity of anterior temporal lobe subregions. Journal of Neuroscience 36, 1490-1501. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2999-15.2016.

      Humphreys, G. F., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Simons, J. S. (2021). A Unifying Account of Angular Gyrus Contributions to Episodic and Semantic Cognition. Trends in neurosciences, 44(6), 452–463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2021.01.006

      Bonner, M. F., & Price, A. R. (2013). Where is the anterior temporal lobe and what does it do?. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(10), 4213–4215. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0041-13.2013

      (7) Some writing is obscure or unclear, in part due to superfluous words like 'intricate neural processes' on L74. Or the sentence in L47 - 48 about 'quantitatively functional mental states defined by a specific parser unified by statistical regularities' which, even read in context, fails to provide clarity about what a quantitatively functional mental state is, or how it is defined by specific parsers (or what these are), and what is the link to statistical regularities. In some cases, this lack of clarity leads to difficulties assessing the appropriateness of the methods, or the exact nature of the claims. For example, do they mean degree of comprehension instead of comprehensive value? I provide some more examples below:

      Response 7: We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful consideration. The revised manuscript now includes a clear description and a detailed explanation of the association with the statistical logic, addressing the concerns raised in Lines 47-55: ‘Contemporary theories frame the semantic processing as a dynamic sequence of neural states[3], shaped by systems that are finely tuned to the statistical regularities inherent in sensory inputs[4]. These regularities enable the brain to evaluate, weight, and integrate multisensory information, optimizing the reliability of individual sensory signals [5]. However, sensory inputs available to the brain are often incomplete and uncertain, necessitating adaptive neural adjustments to resolve these ambiguities[6]. In this context, neuronal activity is thought to be linked to the probability density of sensory information, with higher levels of uncertainty resulting in the engagement of a broader population of neurons, thereby reflecting the brain’s adaptive capacity to handle diverse possible interpretations[7,8].’

      References:

      Brennan, J.R., Stabler, E.P., Van Wagenen, S.E., Luh, W.M., and Hale, J.T. (2016). Abstract linguistic structure correlates with temporal activity during naturalistic comprehension. Brain and Language 157, 81-94. 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.04.008.

      Benetti, S., Ferrari, A., and Pavani, F. (2023). Multimodal processing in face-to-face interactions: A bridging link between psycholinguistics and sensory neuroscience. Front Hum Neurosci 17, 1108354. 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1108354.

      Noppeney, U. (2021). Perceptual Inference, Learning, and Attention in a Multisensory World. Annual Review of Neuroscience, Vol 44, 2021 44, 449-473. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-100120-085519.

      Ma, W.J., and Jazayeri, M. (2014). Neural coding of uncertainty and probability. Annu Rev Neurosci 37, 205-220. 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014017.

      Fischer, B.J., and Pena, J.L. (2011). Owl's behavior and neural representation predicted by Bayesian inference. Nat Neurosci 14, 1061-1066. 10.1038/nn.2872.

      Ganguli, D., and Simoncelli, E.P. (2014). Efficient sensory encoding and Bayesian inference with heterogeneous neural populations. Neural Comput 26, 2103-2134. 10.1162/NECO_a_00638.

      Comment 7.1: a) I am not too sure what they mean by 'response consistently provided by participants for four to six consecutive instances' [L117-118]. They should be clearer with the description of these 'pre-test' study methods.

      Response 7.1: Thank you for this insightful question. An example of a participant's response to the gesture 'an' is provided below (Table 3). Initially, within 240 ms, the participant provided the answer "an," which could potentially be a guess. To ensure that the participant truly comprehends the gesture, we repeatedly present it until the participant’s response stabilizes, meaning the same answer is given consistently over several trials. While one might consider fixing the number of repetitions (e.g., six trials), this could lead to participants predicting the rule and providing the same answer out of habit. To mitigate this potential bias, we allow the number of repetitions to vary flexibly between four and six trials. 

      We understand that the initial phrase might be ambiguous, in the revised manuscript, we have changed the phrase into: ‘For each gesture or speech, the action verb consistently provided by participants across four to six consecutive repetitions—with the number of repetitions varied to mitigate learning effects—was considered the comprehensive response for the gesture or speech.’ (Lines 130-133)

      Author response table 8.

      Example of participant's response to the gesture 'an'

      Comment 7.2: b) I do not understand the paragraph in L143 - 146. This is important to rephrase for clarification. What are 'stepped' neural changes? What is the purpose of 'aggregating' neural responses with identical entropy / MI values?

      Response 7.2: It is important to note that the 20 stimuli exhibit 20 increments of gesture entropy values, 11 increments of speech entropy values, and 19 increments of mutual information values (Appendix Table 3). This discrepancy arises from the calculation of entropy and mutual information, where the distributions were derived from the comprehensive set of responses contributed by all 30 participants. As a result, these values were impacted not only by the distinct nameabilities of the stimuli but also by the entirety of responses provided. Consequently, in the context of speech entropy, 9 items demonstrate the nameability of 1, signifying unanimous comprehension among all 30 participants, resulting in an entropy of 0. Moreover, stimuli 'ning' and 'jiao' share an identical distribution, leading to an entropy of 0.63. Regarding MI, a value of 0.66 is computed for the combinations of stimuli 'sao' (gesture entropy: 4.01, speech entropy: 1.12, Author response image 32) and 'tui' (gesture entropy: 1.62, speech entropy: 0, Author response image 4). This indicates that these two sets of stimuli manifest an equivalent degree of integration.

      Author response image 3.

      Example of gesture answers (gesture sao), speech answers (speech sao), and mutual information (MI) for the ‘sao’ item

      Author response image 4.

      Example of gesture answers (gesture tui), speech answers (speech tui), and mutual information (MI) for the ‘tui’ item

      To precisely assess whether lower entropy/MI corresponds to a smaller or larger neural response, neural responses (ERP amplitude or TMS inhibition effect) with identical entropy or MI values were averaged before undergoing correlational analysis. We understand that the phrasing might be ambiguous. Clear description has been changed in the revised manuscript in Lines 157-160: ‘To determine whether entropy or MI values corresponds to distinct neural changes, the current study first aggregated neural responses (including inhibition effects of tDCS and TMS or ERP amplitudes) that shared identical entropy or MI values, prior to conducting correlational analyses.’

      Comment 7.3: c) The paragraph in L160-171 is confusing. Is it an attempt to give an overview of all three experiments? If so, consider moving to the end or summarising what each experiment is at the beginning of the paragraph giving it a name (i.e., TMS). Without that, it is unclear what each experiment is counterbalancing or what 'stimulation site' refers to, for example, leading to a significant lack of clarity.

      Response 7.3: We are sorry for the ambiguity, in the revised manuscript, we have moved the relevant phrasing to the beginning of each experiment.

      ‘Experiment 1: HD-tDCS protocol and data analysis

      Participants were divided into two groups, with each group undergoing HD-tDCS stimulation at different target sites (IFG or pMTG). Each participant completed three experimental sessions, spaced one week apart, during which 480 gesture-speech pairs were presented across various conditions. In each session, participants received one of three types of HD-tDCS stimulation: Anodal, Cathodal, or Sham. The order of stimulation site and type was counterbalanced using a Latin square design to control for potential order effects’ (Lines 183-189)

      ‘Experiment 2: TMS protocol and data analysis

      Experiment 2 involved 800 gesture-speech pairs, presented across 15 blocks over three days, with one week between sessions. Stimulation was administered at three different sites (IFG, pMTG, or Vertex). Within the time windows (TWs) spanning the gesture-speech integration period, five TWs that exhibited selective disruption of integration were selected: TW1 (-120 to -80 ms relative to the speech identification point), TW2 (-80 to -40 ms), TW3 (-40 to 0 ms), TW6 (80 to 120 ms), and TW7 (120 to 160 ms)23 (Figure 1C). The order of stimulation site and TW was counterbalanced using a Latin square design.’ (Lines 223-230)

      ‘Experiment 3: Electroencephalogram (EEG) recording and data analysis

      Experiment 3, comprising a total of 1760 gesture-speech pairs, was completed in a single-day session.’ (Lines 249-250)

      Comment 7.4: d) L402-406: This sentence is not clear. What do the authors mean by 'the state of [the neural landscape] constructs gradually as measured by entropy and MI'? How does this construct a neural landscape? The authors must rephrase this paragraph using clearer language since in its current state it is very difficult to assess whether it is supported by the evidence they present.

      Response 7.4: We are sorry for the ambiguity, in the revised manuscript we have provided clear description in Lines 483-492: ‘The varying contributions of unisensory gesture-speech information and the convergence of multisensory inputs, as reflected in the correlation between distinct ERP components and TMS time windows (TMS TWs), are consistent with recent models suggesting that multisensory processing involves parallel detection of modality-specific information and hierarchical integration across multiple neural levels[4,48]. These processes are further characterized by coordination across multiple temporal scales[49]. Building on this, the present study offers additional evidence that the multi-level nature of gesture-speech processing is statistically structured, as measured by information matrix of unisensory entropy and multisensory convergence index of MI, the input of either source would activate a distributed representation, resulting in progressively functioning neural responses’

      References:

      Benetti, S., Ferrari, A., and Pavani, F. (2023). Multimodal processing in face-to-face interactions: A bridging link between psycholinguistics and sensory neuroscience. Front Hum Neurosci 17, 1108354. 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1108354.

      Meijer, G.T., Mertens, P.E.C., Pennartz, C.M.A., Olcese, U., and Lansink, C.S. (2019). The circuit architecture of cortical multisensory processing: Distinct functions jointly operating within a common anatomical network. Prog Neurobiol 174, 1-15. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.01.004.

      Senkowski, D., and Engel, A.K. (2024). Multi-timescale neural dynamics for multisensory integration. Nat Rev Neurosci 25, 625-642. 10.1038/s41583-024-00845-7.

      (8) Some writing suffers from conceptual equivocation. For example, the link between 'multimodal representation' and gesture as a type of multimodal extralinguistic information is not straightforward. What 'multimodal representations' usually refer to in semantic cognition is not the co-occurrence of gesture and speech, but the different sources or modalities that inform the structure of a semantic representation or concept (not the fact we use another modality vision to perceive gestures that enrich the linguistic auditory communication of said concepts). See also my comment in the public review regarding the conceptual conflation of the graded hub hypothesis.

      Response 8: We aimed to clarify that the integration of gesture and speech, along with the unified representation it entails, is not merely a process whereby perceived gestures enhance speech comprehension. Rather, there exists a bidirectional influence between these two modalities, affecting both their external forms (Bernaidis et al., 2006) and their semantic content (Kita et al., 2003; Kelly et al., 2010). Given that multisensory processing is recognized as an interplay of both top-down and bottom-up mechanisms, we hypothesize that this bidirectional semantic influence between gesture and speech operates similarly. Consequently, we recorded neural responses—specifically the inhibitory effects observed through TMS/tDCS or ERP components—beginning at the onset of speech, which marks the moment when both modalities are accessible.

      We prioritize gesture for two primary reasons. Firstly, from a naturalistic perspective, speech and gesture are temporally aligned; gestures typically precede their corresponding speech segments by less than one second (Morrelsamuls et al., 1992). This temporal alignment has prompted extensive research aimed at identifying the time windows during which integration occurs (Obermeier et al., 2011, 2015). Results indicate that local integration of gesture and speech occurs within a time frame extending from -200 ms to +120 ms relative to gesture-speech alignment, where -200 ms indicates that gestures occur 200 ms before speech onset, and +120 ms signifies gestures occurring after the identification point of speech.

      Secondly, in our previous study (Zhao, 2023), we investigated this phenomenon by manipulating gesture-speech alignment across two conditions: (1) gestures preceding speech by a fixed interval of 200 ms, and (2) gestures preceding speech at its semantic identification point. Notably, only in the second condition did we observe time-window-selective disruptions of the semantic congruency effect in the IFG and pMTG. This led us to conclude that gestures serve a semantic priming function for co-occurring speech.

      We recognize that our previous use of the term "co-occurring speech" may have led to ambiguity. Therefore, in the revised manuscript, we have replaced those sentences with a detailed description of the properties of each modality in Lines 60-62: ‘Even though gestures convey information in a global-synthetic way, while speech conveys information in a linear segmented way, there exists a bidirectional semantic influence between the two modalities[9,10]’

      Conceptual conflation of the graded hub hypothesis has been clarified in the Response to Reviewer 3 (public review) response 2.

      References:

      Bernardis, P., & Gentilucci, M. (2006). Speech and gesture share the same communication system. Neuropsychologia, 44(2), 178-190

      Kelly, S. D., Ozyurek, A., & Maris, E. (2010b). Two sides of the same coin: speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension. Psychological Science, 21(2), 260-267. doi:10.1177/0956797609357327

      Kita, S., & Ozyurek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 16-32. doi:10.1016/s0749-596x(02)00505-3

      Obermeier, C., & Gunter, T. C. (2015). Multisensory Integration: The Case of a Time Window of Gesture-Speech Integration. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(2), 292-307. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00688

      Obermeier, C., Holle, H., & Gunter, T. C. (2011). What Iconic Gesture Fragments Reveal about Gesture-Speech Integration: When Synchrony Is Lost, Memory Can Help. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(7), 1648-1663. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21498

      Morrelsamuels, P., & Krauss, R. M. (1992). WORD FAMILIARITY PREDICTS TEMPORAL ASYNCHRONY OF HAND GESTURES AND SPEECH. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 18(3), 615-622. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.615

      Hostetter, A., and Mainela-Arnold, E. (2015). Gestures occur with spatial and Motoric knowledge: It's more than just coincidence. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education 22, 42-49. doi:10.1044/lle22.2.42.

      McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and though (University of Chicago Press). 10.7208/chicago/9780226514642.001.0001.

      Zhao, W. (2023). TMS reveals a two-stage priming circuit of gesture-speech integration. Front Psychol 14, 1156087. 10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1156087.

      (9) The last paragraph of the introduction lacks a conductive thread. The authors describe three experiments without guiding the reader through a connecting thread underlying the experiments. Feels more like three disconnected studies than a targeted multi-experiment approach to solve a problem. What is each experiment contributing to? What is the 'grand question' or thread unifying these?

      Response 9: The present study introduced three experiments to explore the neural activity linked to the amount of information processed during multisensory gesture-speech integration. In Experiment 1, we observed that the extent of inhibition in the pMTG and LIFG was closely linked to the overlapping gesture-speech responses, as quantified by mutual information. Building on the established roles of the pMTG and LIFG in our previous study (Zhao et al., 2021, JN), we then expanded our investigation to determine whether the dynamic neural engagement between the pMTG and LIFG during gesture-speech processing was also associated with the quality of the information. This hypothesis was further validated through high-temporal resolution EEG, where we examined ERP components related to varying information qualities. Notably, we observed a close time alignment between the ERP components and the time windows of the TMS effects, which were associated with the same informational matrices in gesture-speech processing.

      Linkage of the three experiments has been clarified in the introduction in Lines 75-102: ‘

      To investigate the neural mechanisms underlying gesture-speech integration, we conducted three experiments to assess how neural activity correlates with distributed multisensory integration, quantified using information-theoretic measures of MI. Additionally, we examined the contributions of unisensory signals in this process, quantified through unisensory entropy. Experiment 1 employed high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) to administer Anodal, Cathodal and Sham stimulation to either the IFG or the pMTG. HD-tDCS induces membrane depolarization with anodal stimulation and membrane hyperpolarization with cathodal stimulation[26], thereby increasing or decreasing cortical excitability in the targeted brain area, respectively. This experiment aimed to determine whether the overall facilitation (Anodal-tDCS minus Sham-tDCS) and/or inhibitory (Cathodal-tDCS minus Sham-tDCS) of these integration hubs is modulated by the degree of gesture-speech integration, as measure by MI.

      Given the differential involvement of the IFG and pMTG in gesture-speech integration, shaped by top-down gesture predictions and bottom-up speech processing [23], Experiment 2 was designed to further assess whether the activity of these regions was associated with relevant informational matrices. Specifically, we applied inhibitory chronometric double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to specific temporal windows associated with integration processes in these regions[23], assessing whether the inhibitory effects of TMS were correlated with unisensory entropy or the multisensory convergence index (MI).

      Experiment 3 complemented these investigations by focusing on the temporal dynamics of neural responses during semantic processing, leveraging high-temporal event-related potentials (ERPs). This experiment investigated how distinct information contributors modulated specific ERP components associated with semantic processing. These components included the early sensory effects as P1 and N1–P2[27,28], the N400 semantic conflict effect[14,28,29], and the late positive component (LPC) reconstruction effect[30,31]. By integrating these ERP findings with results from Experiments 1 and 2, Experiment 3 aimed to provide a more comprehensive understanding of how gesture-speech integration is modulated by neural dynamics’

      References:

      Bikson, M., Inoue, M., Akiyama, H., Deans, J.K., Fox, J.E., Miyakawa, H., and Jefferys, J.G.R. (2004). Effects of uniform extracellular DC electric fields on excitability in rat hippocampal slices. J Physiol-London 557, 175-190. 10.1113/jphysiol.2003.055772.

      Federmeier, K.D., Mai, H., and Kutas, M. (2005). Both sides get the point: hemispheric sensitivities to sentential constraint. Memory & Cognition 33, 871-886. 10.3758/bf03193082.

      Kelly, S.D., Kravitz, C., and Hopkins, M. (2004). Neural correlates of bimodal speech and gesture comprehension. Brain and Language 89, 253-260. 10.1016/s0093-934x(03)00335-3.

      Wu, Y.C., and Coulson, S. (2005). Meaningful gestures: Electrophysiological indices of iconic gesture comprehension. Psychophysiology 42, 654-667. 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00356.x.

      Fritz, I., Kita, S., Littlemore, J., and Krott, A. (2021). Multimodal language processing: How preceding discourse constrains gesture interpretation and affects gesture integration when gestures do not synchronise with semantic affiliates. J Mem Lang 117, 104191. 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104191.

      Gunter, T.C., and Weinbrenner, J.E.D. (2017). When to take a gesture seriously: On how we use and prioritize communicative cues. J Cognitive Neurosci 29, 1355-1367. 10.1162/jocn_a_01125.

      Ozyurek, A., Willems, R.M., Kita, S., and Hagoort, P. (2007). On-line integration of semantic information from speech and gesture: Insights from event-related brain potentials. J Cognitive Neurosci 19, 605-616. 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.4.605.

      Zhao, W., Li, Y., and Du, Y. (2021). TMS reveals dynamic interaction between inferior frontal gyrus and posterior middle temporal gyrus in gesture-speech semantic integration. The Journal of Neuroscience, 10356-10364. 10.1523/jneurosci.1355-21.2021.

      (10) The authors should provide a clearer figure to appreciate their paradigm, illustrating clearly the stimulus presentation (gesture and speech).

      Response 10: To reduce ambiguity, unnecessary arrows were deleted from Figure 1.

      Comment 11.1: (11) Required methodological clarifications to better assess the strength of the evidence presented:

      a) Were the exclusion criteria only handedness and vision? Did the authors exclude based on neurological and psychiatric disorders? Psychoactive drugs? If not, do they think the lack of these exclusion criteria might have influenced their results?

      Response 11.1: Upon registration, each participant is required to complete a questionnaire alongside the consent form and handedness questionnaire. This procedure is designed to exclude individuals with potential neurological or psychiatric disorders, as well as other factors that may affect their mental state or reaction times. Consequently, all participants reported in the manuscript do not have any of the aforementioned neurological or psychiatric disorders. The questionnaire is attached below:

      Author response image 4.

      Comment 11.2: b) Are the subjects from the pre-tests (L112-113) and the replication study (L107) a separate sample or did they take part in Experiments 1-3?

      Response 11.2: The participants in each pre-test and experiment were independent, resulting in a total of 188 subjects. Since the stimuli utilized in this study were previously validated and reported (Zhao et al., 2021), the 90 subjects who participated in the three pre-tests are not included in the final count for the current study, leaving a total of 98 participants reported in the manuscript in Lines 103-104: ‘Ninety-eight young Chinese participants signed written informed consent forms and took part in the present study’.

      Comment 11.3: c) L176. The authors should explain how they selected ROIs. This is very important for the reasons outlined above.

      Response 11.3: Please see Response to Comment 6 for details.

      Comment 11.4: d) The rationale for Experiment 1 and its analysis approach should be explicitly described. Why perform Pearson correlations? What is the conceptual explanation of the semantic congruency effect and why should it be expected to correlate with the three information-theoretic metrics? What effects could the authors expect to find and what would they mean? There is a brief description in L187-195 but it is unclear.

      Response 11.4: We thank the reviewer for their rigorous consideration. The semantic congruency effect is widely used as an index of multisensory integration. Therefore, the effects of HD-tDCS on the IFG and pMTG, as measured by changes in the semantic congruency effect, serve as an indicator of altered neural responses to multisensory integration. In correlating these changes with behavioral indices of information degree, we aimed to assess whether the integration hubs (IFG and pMTG) function progressively during multisensory gesture-speech integration. The rationale for using Pearson correlations is based on the hypothesis that the 20 sets of stimuli used in this study represent a sample from a normally distributed population. Thus, even with changes in the sample (e.g., using another 20 values), the gradual relationship between neural responses and the degree of information would remain unchanged. This hypothesis is supported by the findings from another experiment (see details in Response to Comment 4).

      In the revised manuscript, we have provided a clear description of the rationale for Experiment 1 in Lines 206-219: ‘To examine the relationship between the degree of information and neural responses, we conducted Pearson correlation analyses using a sample of 20 sets. Neural responses were quantified based on the effects of HD-tDCS (active tDCS minus sham tDCS) on the semantic congruency effect, defined as the difference in reaction times between semantic incongruent and congruent conditions (Rt(incongruent) - Rt(congruent)). This effect served as an index of multisensory integration[35] within the left IFG and pMTG. The variation in information was assessed using three information-theoretic metrics. To account for potential confounds related to multiple candidate representations, we conducted partial correlation analyses between the tDCS effects and gesture entropy, speech entropy, and MI, controlling for the number of responses provided for each gesture and speech, as well as the total number of combined responses. Given that HD-tDCS induces overall disruption at the targeted brain regions, we hypothesized that the neural activity within the left IFG and pMTG would be progressively affected by varying levels of multisensory convergence, as indexed by MI.’

      Additionally, in the introduction, we have rephrased the relevant rationale in Lines 75-86: _‘_To investigate the neural mechanisms underlying gesture-speech integration, we conducted three experiments to assess how neural activity correlates with distributed multisensory integration, quantified using information-theoretic measures of MI. Additionally, we examined the contributions of unisensory signals in this process, quantified through unisensory entropy. Experiment 1 employed high-definition transcranial direct current stimulation (HD-tDCS) to administer Anodal, Cathodal and Sham stimulation to either the IFG or the pMTG. HD-tDCS induces membrane depolarization with anodal stimulation and membrane hyperpolarization with cathodal stimulation[26], thereby increasing or decreasing cortical excitability in the targeted brain area, respectively. This experiment aimed to determine whether the overall facilitation (Anodal-tDCS minus Sham-tDCS) and/or inhibitory (Cathodal-tDCS minus Sham-tDCS) of these integration hubs is modulated by the degree of gesture-speech integration, as measure by MI

      Reference:

      Kelly, S.D., Creigh, P., and Bartolotti, J. (2010). Integrating speech and iconic gestures in a Stroop-like task: Evidence for automatic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22, 683-694. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21254.

      Comment 11.5: e) The authors do not mention in the methods if FDR correction was applied to the Pearson correlations in Experiment 1. There is a mention in the Results Figure, but it is unclear if it was applied consistently. Can the authors confirm, and explicitly state the way they carried out FDR correction for this family of tests in Experiment 1? This is especially important in the light of some of their results having a p-value of p=.049.

      Response 11.5: FDR correction was applied to Experiment 1, and all reported p-values were corrected using this method. In the revised manuscript, we have included a reference to FDR correction in Lines 221-222: ‘False discovery rate (FDR) correction was applied for multiple comparisons.’

      In Experiment 1, since two separate participant groups (each N = 26) were recruited for the HD-tDCS over either the IFG or pMTG, FDR correction was performed separately for each group. Therefore, for each brain region, six comparisons (three information matrices × two tDCS effects: anodal-sham or cathodal-sham) were submitted for FDR correction.

      In Experiment 2, six comparisons (three information matrices × two sites: IFG or pMTG) were submitted for FDR correction. In Experiment 3, FDR correction was applied to the seven regions of interest (ROIs) within each component, resulting in five comparisons

      The confidence of a p-value of 0.049 was clarified in Response to Comment 3.

      Comment 11.6: f) L200. What does the abbreviation 'TW' stands for in this paragraph? When was it introduced in the main text? The description is in the Figure, but it should be moved to the main text.]

      Comment 11.7: g) How were the TWs chosen? Is it the criterion in L201-203? If so, it should be moved to the start of the paragraph. What does the word 'selected' refer to in that description? Selected for what? The explanation seems to be in the Figure, but it should be in the main text. It is still not a complete explanation. What were the criteria for assigning TWs to the IFG or pMTG?

      Response 11.6& 11.7: Since the two comments are related, we will provide a synthesized response. 'TW' refers to time window, the selection of which was based on our previous study (Zhao et al., 2021, J. Neurosci). In Zhao et al. (2021), we employed the same experimental protocol—using inhibitory double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) over the IFG and pMTG in one of eight 40-ms time windows relative to the speech identification point (IP; the minimal length of lexical speech), with three time windows before the speech IP and five after. Based on this previous work, we believe that these time windows encompass the potential gesture-speech integration process. Results demonstrated a time-window-selective disruption of the semantic congruency effect (i.e., reaction time costs driven by semantic conflict), with no significant modulation of the gender congruency effect (i.e., reaction time costs due to gender conflict), when stimulating the left pMTG in TW1, TW2, and TW7, and when stimulating the left IFG in TW3 and TW6. Based on these findings, the present study selected the five time windows that showed a selective disruption effect during gesture-speech integration.

      Note that in the present study, we applied stimulation to both the IFG and pMTG across all five time windows, and further correlated the TMS disruption effects with the three information matrices.

      We recognize that the rationale for the choice of time windows was not sufficiently explained in the original manuscript. In the revised manuscript, we have added the relevant description in Lines 223-228: ‘Stimulation was administered at three different sites (IFG, pMTG, or Vertex). Within the time windows (TWs) spanning the gesture-speech integration period, five TWs that exhibited selective disruption of integration were selected: TW1 (-120 to -80 ms relative to the speech identification point), TW2 (-80 to -40 ms), TW3 (-40 to 0 ms), TW6 (80 to 120 ms), and TW7 (120 to 160 ms)[23] (Figure 1C). The order of stimulation site and TW was counterbalanced using a Latin square design.’

      Comment 11.8: h) Again, the rationale for the Pearson correlations of semantic congruency with information-theoretic metrics should be explicitly outlined. What is this conceptually?

      Response 11.8: Given that the rationale behind Experiment 1 and Experiment 2 is similar—both investigating the correlation between interrupted neural effects and the degree of information—we believe that the introduction of the Pearson correlation between semantic congruency and information-theoretic metrics, as presented in Experiment 1 (see Response to Comment 11.4 for details), is sufficient for both experiments.

      Comment 11.9: i)What does 'gesture stoke' mean in the Figure referring to Experiment 3? Figure 1D is not clear. What are the arrows referring to?

      Response 11.9: According to McNeill (1992), gesture phases differ based on whether the gesture depicts imagery. Iconic and metaphoric gestures are imagistic and typically consist of three phases: a preparation phase, a stroke phase, and a retraction phrase. Figure 4 provides an example of these three phases using the gesture ‘break’. In the preparation phase, the hand and arm move away from their resting position to a location in gesture space where the stroke begins. As illustrated in the first row of Figure 4, during the preparation phase of the ‘break’ gesture, the hands, initially in a fist and positioned downward, rise to a center-front position. In the stroke phase, the meaning of the gesture is conveyed. This phase occurs in the central gesture space and is synchronized with the linguistic segments it co-expresses. For example, in the stroke phase of the ‘break’ gesture (second row of Figure 4), the two fists move 90 degrees outward before returning to a face-down position. The retraction phase involves the return of the hand from the stroke position to the rest position. In the case of the ‘break’ gesture, this involves moving the fists from the center front back into the resting position (see third row of Figure 4).

      Therefore, in studies examining gesture-speech integration, gestures are typically analyzed starting from the stroke phase (Habets et al., 2011; Kelly et al., 2010), a convention also adopted in our previous studies (Zhao et al., 2018, 2021, 2023). We acknowledge that this should be explained explicitly, and in the revised manuscript, we have added the following clarification in Lines 162-166: ‘Given that gestures induce a semantic priming effect on concurrent speech[33], this study utilized a semantic priming paradigm in which speech onset was aligned with the DP of each gesture[23,33], the point at which the gesture transitions into a lexical form[34]. The gesture itself began at the stroke phase, a critical moment when the gesture conveys its primary semantic content[34].’

      Additionally, Figure 1 has been revised in the manuscript to eliminate ambiguous arrows. (see Response 10 for detail).

      Author response image 5.

      An illustration of the gesture phases of the 'break' gesture.

      References:

      Habets, B., Kita, S., Shao, Z. S., Ozyurek, A., & Hagoort, P. (2011). The Role of Synchrony and Ambiguity in Speech-Gesture Integration during Comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(8), 1845-1854. doi:10.1162/jocn.2010.21462

      Kelly, S. D., Creigh, P., & Bartolotti, J. (2010). Integrating Speech and Iconic Gestures in a Stroop-like Task: Evidence for Automatic Processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(4), 683-694. doi:DOI 10.1162/jocn.2009.21254

      Comment 11.10: j) L236-237: "Consequently, four ERP components were predetermined" is very confusing. Were these components predetermined? Or were they determined as a consequence of the comparison between the higher and lower halves for the IT metrics described above in the same paragraph? The description of the methods is not clear.

      Response 11.10: The components selected were based on a comparison between the higher and lower halves of the information metrics. By stating that these components were predetermined, we aimed to emphasize that the components used in our study are consistent with those identified in previous research on semantic processing. We acknowledge that the phrasing may have been unclear, and in the revised manuscript, we have provided a more explicit description in Lines 267-276: ‘To consolidate the data, we conducted both a traditional region-of-interest (ROI) analysis, with ROIs defined based on a well-established work[40], and a cluster-based permutation approach, which utilizes data-driven permutations to enhance robustness and address multiple comparisons.

      For the traditional ROI analysis, grand-average ERPs at electrode Cz were compared between the higher (≥50%) and lower (<50%) halves for gesture entropy (Figure 5A1), speech entropy (Figure 5B1), and MI (Figure 5C1). Consequently, four ERP components were determined: the P1 effect observed within the time window of 0-100 ms[27,28], the N1-P2 effect observed between 150-250ms[27,28], the N400 within the interval of 250-450ms[14,28,29], and the LPC spanning from 550-1000ms[30,31].’

      Reference: Habets, B., Kita, S., Shao, Z.S., Ozyurek, A., and Hagoort, P. (2011). The Role of Synchrony and Ambiguity in Speech-Gesture Integration during Comprehension. J Cognitive Neurosci 23, 1845-1854. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21462.

      (12) In the Results section for Experiment 2 (L292-295), it is not clear what the authors mean when they mention that a more negative TMS effect represents a stronger interruption of the integration effect. If I understand correctly, the correlation reported for pMTG was for speech entropy, which does not represent integration (that would be MI).

      Response 12: Since the TMS effect was defined as active TMS minus Vertex TMS, the inhibitory TMS effect is inherently negative. A greater inhibitory TMS effect corresponds to a larger negative value, such that a more negative TMS effect indicates a stronger disruption of the integration process. We acknowledge that the previous phrasing was somewhat ambiguous. In the revised manuscript, we have rephrased the sentence as follows: ‘a larger negative TMS effect signifies a greater disruption of the integration process’ (Lines 342-343)

      Multisensory integration transcends simple data amalgamation, encompassing complex interactions at various hierarchical neural levels and the parallel detection and discrimination of raw data from each modality (Benetti et al., 2023; Meijer et al., 2019). Therefore, we regard the process of gesture-speech integration as involving both unisensory processing and multisensory convergence. The correlation of gesture and speech entropy reflects contributions from unisensory processing, while the mutual information (MI) index indicates the contribution of multisensory convergence during gesture-speech integration. The distinction between these various source contributions will be the focus of Experiment 2 and Experiment 3, as described in the revised manuscript Lines 87-102: ‘Given the differential involvement of the IFG and pMTG in gesture-speech integration, shaped by top-down gesture predictions and bottom-up speech processing [23], Experiment 2 was designed to further assess whether the activity of these regions was associated with relevant informational matrices. Specifically, we applied inhibitory chronometric double-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to specific temporal windows associated with integration processes in these regions[23], assessing whether the inhibitory effects of TMS were correlated with unisensory entropy or the multisensory convergence index (MI).

      Experiment 3 complemented these investigations by focusing on the temporal dynamics of neural responses during semantic processing, leveraging high-temporal event-related potentials (ERPs). This experiment investigated how distinct information contributors modulated specific ERP components associated with semantic processing. These components included the early sensory effects as P1 and N1–P2[27,28], the N400 semantic conflict effect[14,28,29], and the late positive component (LPC) reconstruction effect[30,31]. By integrating these ERP findings with results from Experiments 1 and 2, Experiment 3 aimed to provide a more comprehensive understanding of how gesture-speech integration is modulated by neural dynamics’.  

      References:

      Benetti, S., Ferrari, A., and Pavani, F. (2023). Multimodal processing in face-to-face interactions: A bridging link between psycholinguistics and sensory neuroscience. Front Hum Neurosci 17, 1108354. 10.3389/fnhum.2023.1108354.

      Meijer, G.T., Mertens, P.E.C., Pennartz, C.M.A., Olcese, U., and Lansink, C.S. (2019). The circuit architecture of cortical multisensory processing: Distinct functions jointly operating within a common anatomical network. Prog Neurobiol 174, 1-15. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2019.01.004.

      (13) I find the description of the results for Experiment 3 very hard to follow. Perhaps if the authors have decided to organise the main text by describing the components from earliest to latest, the Figure organisation should follow suit (i.e., organise the Figure from the earliest to the latest component, instead of gesture entropy/speech entropy / mutual information). This might make the description of the results easier to follow.

      Response 13: As suggested, we have reorganized the results of experiment 3 based on components from earliest to latest, together with an updated Figure 5.

      The results are detailed in Lines 367-423: ‘Topographical maps illustrating amplitude differences between the lower and higher halves of speech entropy demonstrate a central-posterior P1 amplitude (0-100 ms, Figure 5B). Aligning with prior findings[27], the paired t-tests demonstrated a significantly larger P1 amplitude within the ML ROI (t(22) = 2.510, p = 0.020, 95% confidence interval (CI) = [1.66, 3.36]) when contrasting stimuli with higher 50% speech entropy against those with lower 50% speech entropy (Figure 5D1 left). Subsequent correlation analyses unveiled a significant increase in the P1 amplitude with the rise in speech entropy within the ML ROI (r = 0.609, p = 0.047, 95% CI = [0.039, 1.179], Figure 5D1 right). Furthermore, a cluster of neighboring time-electrode samples exhibited a significant contrast between the lower 50% and higher 50% of speech entropy, revealing a P1 effect spanning 16 to 78 ms at specific electrodes (FC2, FCz, C1, C2, Cz, and CPz, Figure 5D2 middle) (t(22) = 2.754, p = 0.004, 95% confidence interval (CI) = [1.65, 3.86], Figure 5D2 left), with a significant correlation with speech entropy (r = 0.636, p = 0.035, 95% CI = [0.081, 1.191], Figure 5D2 right).

      Additionally, topographical maps comparing the lower 50% and higher 50% gesture entropy revealed a frontal N1-P2 amplitude (150-250 ms, Figure 5A). In accordance with previous findings on bilateral frontal N1-P2 amplitude[27], paired t-tests displayed a significantly larger amplitude for stimuli with lower 50% gesture entropy than with higher 50% entropy in both ROIs of LA (t(22) = 2.820, p = 0.011, 95% CI = [2.21, 3.43]) and RA (t(22) = 2.223, p = 0.038, 95% CI = [1.56, 2.89]) (Figure 5E1 left).  Moreover, a negative correlation was found between N1-P2 amplitude and gesture entropy in both ROIs of LA (r = -0.465, p = 0.039, 95% CI = [-0.87, -0.06]) and RA (r = -0.465, p = 0.039, 95% CI = [-0.88, -0.05]) (Figure 5E1 right). Additionally, through a cluster-permutation test, the N1-P2 effect was identified between 184 to 202 ms at electrodes FC4, FC6, C2, C4, C6, and CP4 (Figure 5E2 middle) (t(22) = 2.638, p = 0.015, 95% CI = [1.79, 3.48], (Figure 5E2 left)), exhibiting a significant correlation with gesture entropy (r = -0.485, p = 0.030, 95% CI = [-0.91, -0.06], Figure 5E2 right).

      Furthermore, in line with prior research[42], a left-frontal N400 amplitude (250-450 ms) was discerned from topographical maps of gesture entropy (Figure 5A). Specifically, stimuli with lower 50% values of gesture entropy elicited a larger N400 amplitude in the LA ROI compared to those with higher 50% values  (t(22) = 2.455, p = 0.023, 95% CI = [1.95, 2.96], Figure 5F1 left). Concurrently, a negative correlation was noted between the N400 amplitude and gesture entropy (r = -0.480, p = 0.032, 95% CI = [-0.94, -0.03], Figure 5F1 right) within the LA ROI. The identified clusters showing the N400 effect for gesture entropy (282 – 318 ms at electrodes FC1, FCz, C1, and Cz, Figure 5F2 middle) (t(22) = 2.828, p = 0.010, 95% CI = [2.02, 3.64], Figure 5F2 left) also exhibited significant correlation between the N400 amplitude and gesture entropy (r = -0.445, p = 0.049, 95% CI = [-0.88, -0.01], Figure 5F2 right).

      Similarly, a left-frontal N400 amplitude (250-450 ms) [42] was discerned from topographical maps for MI (Figure 5C). A larger N400 amplitude in the LA ROI was observed for stimuli with lower 50% values of MI compared to those with higher 50% values (t(22) = 3.00, p = 0.007, 95% CI = [2.54, 3.46], Figure 5G1 left). This was accompanied by a significant negative correlation between N400 amplitude and MI (r = -0.504, p = 0.028, 95% CI = [-0.97, -0.04], Figure 5G1 right) within the LA ROI. The N400 effect for MI, observed in the 294–306 ms window at electrodes F1, F3, Fz, FC1, FC3, FCz, and C1 (Figure 5G2 middle) (t(22) = 2.461, p = 0.023, 95% CI = [1.62, 3.30], Figure 5G2 left), also showed a significant negative correlation with MI (r = -0.569, p = 0.011, 95% CI = [-0.98, -0.16], Figure 5G2 right).

      Finally, consistent with previous findings[30], an anterior LPC effect (550-1000 ms) was observed in topographical maps comparing stimuli with lower and higher 50% speech entropy (Figure 5B). The reduced LPC amplitude was evident in the paired t-tests conducted in ROIs of LA (t(22) = 2.614, p = 0.016, 95% CI = [1.88, 3.35]); LC (t(22) = 2.592, p = 0.017, 95% CI = [1.83, 3.35]); RA (t(22) = 2.520, p = 0.020, 95% CI = [1.84, 3.24]); and ML (t(22) = 2.267, p = 0.034, 95% CI = [1.44, 3.10]) (Figure 5H1 left). Simultaneously, a marked negative correlation with speech entropy was evidenced in ROIs of LA (r = -0.836, p =   0.001, 95% CI = [-1.26, -0.42]); LC (r = -0.762, p = 0.006, 95% CI = [-1.23, -0.30]); RA (r = -0.774, p = 0.005, 95% CI = [-1.23, -0.32]) and ML (r = -0.730, p = 0.011, 95% CI = [-1.22, -0.24]) (Figure 5H1 right). Additionally, a cluster with the LPC effect (644 - 688 ms at electrodes Cz, CPz, P1, and Pz, Figure 5H2 middle) (t(22) = 2.754, p = 0.012, 95% CI = [1.50, 4.01], Figure 5H2 left) displayed a significant correlation with speech entropy (r = -0.699, p = 0.017, 95% CI = [-1.24, -0.16], Figure 5H2 right).’

      (14) In the Discussion (L394 - 395) the authors mention for the first time their task being a semantic priming paradigm. This idea of the task as a semantic priming paradigm allowing top-down prediction of gesture over speech should be presented earlier in the paper, perhaps during the final paragraph of the introduction (as part of the rationale) or during the explanation of the task. The authors mention top-down influences earlier and this is impossible to understand before this information about the paradigm is presented. It would also make the reading of the paper significantly clearer. Critically, an appropriate description of the paradigm is missing in the Methods (what are the subjects asked to do? It states that it replicates an effect in Ref 28, but this manuscript does not contain a clear description of the task). To further complicate things, the 'Experimental Procedure' section of the methods states this is a semantic priming paradigm of gestures onto speech (L148) and proceeds to provide two seemingly irrelevant references (for example, the Pitcher reference is to a study that employed faces and houses as stimuli). How is this a semantic priming paradigm? The study where I found the first mention of this paradigm seems to clearly classify it as a Stroop-like task (Kelly et al, 2010).

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thorough consideration. The experimental paradigm employed in the current study differs from the Stroop-like task utilized by Kelly et al. (2010). In their study, the video presentation started with the stroke phase of the gesture, while speech occurred 200 ms after the gesture onset.

      As detailed in our previous study (Zhao et al., 2023, Frontiers in Psychology), we confirmed the semantic predictive role of gestures in relation to speech by contrasting two experimental conditions: (1) gestures preceding speech by a fixed 200 ms interval, and (2) gestures preceding speech at the semantic identification point of the gesture. Our findings revealed time-window-selective disruptions in the semantic congruency effect in the IFG and pMTG, but only in the second condition, suggesting that gestures exert a semantic priming effect on concurrent speech.

      This work highlighted the semantic priming role of gestures in the integration of speech found in Zhao et al. (2021, Journal of Neuroscience). In the study, a comparable approach was adopted by segmenting speech into eight 40-ms time windows based on the speech discrimination point, while manipulating the speech onset to align with the gesture identification point. The results revealed time-window-selective disruptions in the semantic congruency effect, providing support for the dynamic and temporally staged roles of the IFG and pMTG in gesture-speech integration.

      Given that the present study follows the same experimental procedure as our prior work (Zhao et al., 2021, Journal of Neuroscience; Zhao et al., 2023, Frontiers in Psychology), we refer to this design as a "semantic priming" of gesture upon speech. We agree with the reviewer that a detailed description should be clarified earlier in the manuscript. To address this, we have added a more explicit description of the semantic priming paradigm in the methods section of the revised manuscript in Lines 162-166: ‘Given that gestures induce a semantic priming effect on concurrent speech[33], this study utilized a semantic priming paradigm in which speech onset was aligned with the DP of each gesture[23,33], the point at which the gesture transitions into a lexical form[34]. The gesture itself began at the stroke phase, a critical moment when the gesture conveys its primary semantic content [34].’

      The task participants completed was outlined immediately following the explanation of the experimental paradigm: ‘Gesture–speech pairs were presented randomly using Presentation software (www.neurobs.com). Participants were asked to look at the screen but respond with both hands as quickly and accurately as possible merely to the gender of the voice they heard’ (Lines:177-180).

      Wrongly cited references have been corrected.

      (15) L413-417: How do the authors explain that they observe this earlier ERP component and TMS effect over speech and a later one over gesture in pMTG when in their task they first presented gesture and then speech? Why mention STG/S when they didn't assess this?

      (19) L436-440: This paragraph yields the timing of the findings represented in Figure 6 even more confusing. If gesture precedes speech in the paradigm, why are the first TMS and ERP results observed in speech?

      Response 15 &19: Since these two aspects are closely related, we offer a comprehensive explanation. Although gestures were presented before speech, the integration process occurs once both modalities are available. Consequently, ERP and TMS measurements were taken after speech onset to capture the integration of the two modalities. Neural responses were used as the dependent variable to reflect the degree of integration—specifically, gesture-speech semantic congruency in the TMS study and high-low semantic variance in the ERP study. Therefore, the observed early effect can be interpreted as an interaction between the top-down influence of gesture and the bottom-up processing of speech.

      To isolate the pure effect of gesture, neural activity would need to be recorded from gesture onset. However, if one aims to associate the strength of neural activity with the degree of gesture information, recording from the visual processing areas would be more appropriate.

      To avoid unnecessary ambiguity, the phrase "involved STG/S" has been removed from the manuscript.

      (16) L427-428: I find it hard to believe that MI, a behavioural metric, indexes the size of overlapped neural populations activated by gesture and speech. The authors should be careful with this claim or provide evidence in favour.

      Response 16: Mutual information (MI) is a behavioral metric that indexes the distribution of overlapping responses between gesture and speech (for further details, please see the Response to Comment 1). In the present study, MI was correlated with neural responses evoked by gesture and speech, with the goal of demonstrating that neural activity progressively reflects the degree of information conveyed, as indexed by MI.

      (17) Why would you have easier integration (reduced N400) with larger gesture entropy in IFG (Figure 6(3))? Wouldn't you expect more difficult processing if entropy is larger?

      (18) L431-432: The claim that IFG stores semantic information is controversial. The authors provide two references from the early 2000s that do not offer support for this claim (the IFG's purported involvement according to these is in semantic unification, not storage).

      Response 17 &18: As outlined in the Responses to Comment 1 of the public review, we have provided a re-explanation of the IFG as a semantic control region. Additionally, we have clarified the role of the IFG in relation to the various stages of gesture-speech integration in Lines 533-538: ‘Last, the activated speech representation would disambiguate and reanalyze the semantic information and further unify into a coherent comprehension in the pMTG[12,37]. As speech entropy increases, indicating greater uncertainty in the information provided by speech, more cognitive effort is directed towards selecting the targeted semantic representation. This leads to enhanced involvement of the IFG and a corresponding reduction in LPC amplitude’

      (20) Overall, the grammar makes some parts of the discussion hard to follow (e.g. the limitation in L446-447: 'While HD tDCS and TMS may impact functionally and anatomically connected brain regions, the graded functionality of every disturbed period is not guaranteed')

      Response 20: Clear description has been provided in the revised manuscript in Lines 552-557: ‘Additionally, not all influenced TWs exhibited significant associations with entropy and MI. While HD-tDCS and TMS may impact functionally and anatomically connected brain regions[55,56],  whether the absence of influence in certain TWs can be attributed to compensation by other connected brain areas, such as angular gyrus[57] or anterior temporal lobe[58], warrants further investigation. Therefore, caution is needed when interpreting the causal relationship between inhibition effects of brain stimulation and information-theoretic metrics (entropy and MI).’

      References:

      Hartwigsen, G., Bzdok, D., Klein, M., Wawrzyniak, M., Stockert, A., Wrede, K., Classen, J., and Saur, D. (2017). Rapid short-term reorganization in the language network. Elife 6. 10.7554/eLife.25964.

      Jackson, R.L., Hoffman, P., Pobric, G., and Ralph, M.A.L. (2016). The semantic network at work and rest: Differential connectivity of anterior temporal lobe subregions. Journal of Neuroscience 36, 1490-1501. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2999-15.2016

      Humphreys, G. F., Lambon Ralph, M. A., & Simons, J. S. (2021). A Unifying Account of Angular Gyrus Contributions to Episodic and Semantic Cognition. Trends in neurosciences, 44(6), 452–463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2021.01.006

      Bonner, M. F., & Price, A. R. (2013). Where is the anterior temporal lobe and what does it do?. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 33(10), 4213–4215. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0041-13.2013

      (21) Inconsistencies between terminology employed in Figures and main text (e.g., pre-test study in text, gating study in Figure?)

      Response 21: Consistence has been made by changing the ‘gating study’ into ‘pre-tests’ in Figure 1 (Lines 758).

    1. eLife Assessment

      This important manuscript uses circuit mapping, chemogenetics, and optogenetics to demonstrate a novel hippocampal lateral septal circuit that regulates social novelty behaviours and shows that downstream of the hippocampal septal circuit, septal projections to the ventral tegmental area are necessary for general novelty discrimination. The strength of the evidence supporting the claims is convincing but would be strengthened by the inclusion of additional functional assays. The work will be of interest to systems and behavioural neuroscientists who are interested in the brain mechanisms of social behaviours.

      We thank the reviewers for their thoughtful and constructive feedback. We are excited that both reviewers thought that the manuscript was of “interest to specialists in the field and to the broad readership of the journal”, that the paper was “well-written and logically organized” and that the “study opens an avenue to study these circuits further to uncover the plasticity and synaptic mechanisms regulating social novelty preference.” Additionally, the reviewers wrote that the experiments were “well-designed” “with clever controls and conditions to provide compelling evidence for their conclusion.” The reviewers additionally provided constructive feedback, which we address in our responses below.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The study investigated the neural circuits underlying social novelty preference in mice. Using viral circuit tracing, chemogenetics, and optogenetics in the vHPC, LS, and VTA, the authors found that vHPC to LS projections may contribute to the salience of social novelty investigations. In addition, the authors identify LS projections to the VTA involved in social novelty and familiar food responses. Finally, via viral tracing, they demonstrate that vHPC-LS neurons may establish direct monosynaptic connections with VTA dopaminergic neurons. The experiments are well-designed, and the conclusions are mostly very clear. The manuscript is well-written and logically organized, and the content will be of interest to specialists in the field and to the broad readership of the journal.

      Strengths:

      (1) The vHPC has been involved in social memory for novel and familiar conspecifics. Yet, how the vHPC conveys this information to drive motivation for novel social investigations remains unclear. The authors identified a pathway from the vHPC to the LS and eventually the VTA, that may be involved in this process.

      (2) Mice became familiar with a novel conspecific by co-housing for 72h. This represents a familiarization session with a longer duration as compared to previous literature. Using this new protocol, the authors found robust social novelty preference when animals were given a choice between a novel and familiar conspecific.

      (3) The effects of vHPC-LS inhibition are specific to novel social stimuli. The authors included novel food and novel object control experiments and those were not affected by neuronal manipulations.

      (4) For optogenetic studies, the authors applied closed-loop photoinhibition only when the animals investigated either the novel conspecific or the familiar. This optogenetic approach allowed for the investigation of functional manipulations to selective novel or familiar stimuli approaches.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The abstract and the overall manuscript pose that the authors identified a novel vHPC-LS-VTA pathway that is necessary for mice to preferentially investigate novel conspecifics. However, the authors assessed the functional manipulations of vHPC-LS and LS-VTA circuits independently and the sentence could be misleading. Therefore, a viral strategy specifically designed to target the vHPC-LS-VTA circuit combined with optogenetic/chemogenetic tools and behavior may be necessary for the statement of this conclusion.

      The reviewer raises an important point. Although Figure 3 shows that vHPC (vCA1 and vCA3) is the source of the greatest number of monosynaptic inputs onto LS-VTA neurons, we did not perform any experiments that specifically manipulated vHPC neurons that project to LS-VTA neurons. While these experiments would be extremely interesting, they are technically challenging and beyond the scope of this study.

      (2) The authors combined males and females in their analysis, as neural circuit manipulation affected novelty discrimination ratios in both sexes. However, supplementary Figure 1 demonstrates the chemogentic inhibition of vHPC-LS circuit may cause stronger effects in male mice as compared to females.

      The reviewer makes an interesting point. We can confirm that we found no significant differences in the effectiveness of our vHPC-LS inhibition between the males and females (2-factor ANOVA with sex (male/female) and drug condition (saline/CNO) as factors on the discrimination scores of hM4Di expressing animals: interaction p=0.2241, sex: p=0.1233, drug condition: p=0.0166). These data suggest that there are no significant sex differences in the effectiveness of inhibition of the vHPC-LS neurons.

      (3) In most experiments, the same animals were used for social novelty preference, for food or object novelty responses but washout periods between experiments are not mentioned in the methods section. In this line, the authors did not mention the time frame between the closed-loop optogenetic experiments that silenced the vHPC-LS only during familiar and then only novel social investigations. When using the same animals tested for social experiments in the same context there may be an effect of context-dependent social behaviors that could affect future outcomes.

      We thank the reviewer for this important clarification. We apologize for not including these crucial details in our Methods section. For both the chemogenetic and optogenetic inhibition experiments, all conditions were separated by a minimum of 24 hours. In the chemogenetic inhibition experiments, saline and CNO conditions were counterbalanced between animals. Similarly, we counterbalanced the order of light ON vs light OFF conditions across animals during our optogenetic inhibition experiments.

      (4) All the experiments were performed in a non-cell-type-specific manner. The viral strategies used targeted multiple neuronal subpopulations that could have divergent effects on social novelty preference. This constraint could be added in the discussion section.

      The reviewer raises an important point. In our study, while we specifically manipulate projection populations (either vHPC-LS or LS-VTA), it is possible that these projection populations themselves are composed of heterogeneous cell types. It would be an interesting direction of study to pursue in the future.

      (5) The authors' assumptions were all based on experiments of necessity. The authors could use an experiment of sufficiency by targeting for instance the LS-VTA circuit and assess if animals reduce novel social investigations with LS-VTA photostimulation.

      We agree with the reviewers that it would be interesting to determine if LS-VTA neurons are sufficient, in addition to being necessary, to drive social novelty. These will be interesting experiments to pursue in the future.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Rashid and colleagues demonstrate a novel hippocampal lateral septal circuit that is important for social recognition and drives the exploration of novel conspecifics. Their study spans from neural tracing to close-loop optogenetic experiments with clever controls and conditions to provide compelling evidence for their conclusion. They demonstrate that downstream of the hippocampal septal circuit, septal projections to the ventral tegmental area are necessary for general novelty discrimination. The study opens an avenue to study these circuits further to uncover the plasticity and synaptic mechanisms regulating social novelty preference.

      Strengths:

      Chemogenetic and optogenetic experiments have excellent behavioral controls. The synaptic tracing provides important information that informs the narrative of experiments presented and invites future studies to investigate the effects of septal input on dopaminergic activity.

      Weaknesses:

      There are unclear methodological important details for circuit manipulation experiments and analyses where multiple measures are needed but missing. Based on the legends, the chemogenetic experiment is done in a within-animal design. That is the same mouse receives SAL and CNO. However, the data is not presented in a within-animal manner such that we can distinguish if the behavior of the same animal changes with drug treatment. Similarly, the methods specify that the optogenetic manipulations were done in three different conditions, but the analyses do not report within-animal changes across conditions nor account for multiple measures within subjects.

      Thank you for raising this important point. We agree that a repeated measures ANOVA would be ideal, but there is sufficient behavioral variability that such analyses will be difficult without very large sample sizes.

      Finally, it is unclear if the order of drug treatment and conditions were counterbalanced across subjects.

      As mentioned in the above response to Reviewer 1, for both the chemogenetic and optogenetic inhibition experiments, all conditions were separated by a minimum of 24 hours and we counterbalanced the order of chemogenetic (saline/CNO) and optogenetic (light ON/light OFF) experimental manipulations across animals.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study investigates the potential of targeting specific regions within the RNA genome of the Porcine Epidemic Diarrhea Virus (PEDV) for antiviral drug development. The authors used SHAPE-MaP to analyze the structure of the PEDV RNA genome in infected cells. They categorized different regions of the genome based on their structural characteristics, focusing on those that might be good targets for drugs or small interfering RNAs (siRNAs).

      They found that dynamic single-stranded regions can be stabilized by compounds (e.g., to form G-quadruplexes), which inhibit viral proliferation. They demonstrated this by targeting a specific G4-forming sequence with a compound called Braco-19. The authors also describe stable (structured) single-stranded regions that they used to design siRNAs showing that they effectively inhibited viral replication.

      Strengths:

      There are a number of strengths to highlight in this manuscript.

      (1) The study uses a sophisticated technique (SHAPE-MaP) to analyze the PEDV RNA genome in situ, providing valuable insights into its structural features.

      (2) The authors provide a strong rationale for targeting specific RNA structures for antiviral development.

      (3) The study includes a range of experiments, including structural analysis, compound screening, siRNA design, and viral proliferation assays, to support their conclusions.

      (4) Finally, the findings have potential implications for the development of new antiviral therapies against PEDV and other RNA viruses.

      Overall, this interesting study highlights the importance of considering RNA structure when designing antiviral therapies and provides a compelling strategy for identifying promising RNA targets in viral genomes.

      Weaknesses:

      I have some concerns about the utility of the 3D analyses, the effects of their synonymous mutants on expression/proliferation, a potentially missed control for studies of mutants, and the therapeutic utility of the compound they tested vs. Gquadruplexes.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive assessment and insightful comments. Below, we address each point of concern:

      (1) The utility of the 3D analyses:

      In the revised manuscript, we have toned down this discussion and moved Figure 3A to the supplementary materials to reduce any sense of fragmentation in the overall story. While SHAPE-MaP technology is mature and convenient to use and can indeed capture some RNA structural elements with special functions in certain case; we acknowledge that its application for 3D analyses requires further validation. We believe this approach will become more prevalent in future research.

      (2) The effects of synonymous mutants on expression/proliferation:

      In the PEDV genome, the PQS1 mutation site encodes lysine (AAG). Given that lysine has only two codons (AAG and AAA), the G3109A synonymous mutation represented our sole viable option. Published studies (Ding et al., 2024) confirm that neither AAG nor AAA are classified as rare or dominant codons in mammalian cells. Therefore, the observed changes in viral proliferation levels are likely to stem from alterations in RNA secondary structure rather than codon usage effects.

      REFERENCES:

      Ding W, Yu W, Chen Y, et al. Rare codon recoding for efficient noncanonical amino acid incorporation in mammalian cells. Science. 2024;384(6700):1134-1142. 

      (3) Potentially missed control for studies of mutants:

      In the revised manuscript, we have incorporated additional control experiments evaluating Braco-19's therapeutic effects on the PQS3 mutant strain (Figure 4 – figure supplement 3):

      (4) The therapeutic utility of Braco-19 vs. G-quadruplexes:

      While Braco-19 is indeed a broad-spectrum G4 ligand, our data clearly show that not all PQSs in the viral genome can form G4 structures. Our findings primarily provide proof-of-concept that sequences with high G4-forming potential in viral genomes represent viable targets for antiviral therapy. Future studies could leverage SHAPEguided structural insights to design ligands with enhanced specificity for viral G4s, potentially improving therapeutic utility while minimizing off-target effects.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Luo et. al. use SHAPE-MaP to find suitable RNA targets in Porcine Epidemic Diarrhoea Virus. Results show that dynamic and transient structures are good targets for small molecules, and that exposed strand regions are adequate targets for siRNA. This work is important to segment the RNA targeting.

      Strengths:

      This work is well done and the data supports its findings and conclusions. When possible, more than one technique was used to confirm some of the findings.

      Weaknesses:

      The study uses a cell line that is not porcine (not the natural target of the virus).

      We thank the reviewer for their insightful comments and recognition of our study's value. The most commonly employed cell models for in vitro PEDV studies are monkey-derived Vero E6 cells and porcine PK1 cells. However, PEDV (particularly our strain) exhibits significantly lower replication efficiency in PK1 cells compared to Vero cells, and no cytopathic effects were observed in PK1 cells. In our preliminary attempts to perform SHAPE-MaP experiments using infected PK1 cells, the sequencing data showed less than 0.03% alignment to the PEDV genome, rendering subsequent analysis and downstream experiments unfeasible.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Luo et al. applied SHAPE-Map to analyze the secondary structure of the Porcine Epidemic Diarrhoea Virus (PEDV) RNA genome in infected cells. By combining SHAPE reactivity and Shannon entropy, the study indicated that the folding of the PEDV genomic RNA was nonuniform, with the 5' and 3' untranslated regions being more compactly structured, which revealed potentially antiviral targetable RNA regions. Interestingly, the study also suggested that compounds bound to well-folded RNA structures in vitro did not necessarily exhibit antiviral activity in cells, because the binding of these compounds did not necessarily alter the functions of the well-folded RNA regions. Later in the manuscript, the authors focus on guanine-rich regions, which may form G-quadruplexes and be potential targets for small interfering RNA (siRNA). The manuscript shows the binding effect of Braco-19 (a G-quadruplex-binding ligand) to a predicted G4 region in vitro, along with the inhibition of PEDV proliferation in cells. This suggests that targeting high SHAPE-high Shannon G4 regions could be a promising approach against RNA viruses. Lastly, the manuscript identifies 73 singlestranded regions with high SHAPE and low Shannon entropy, which demonstrated high success in antiviral siRNA targeting.

      Strengths:

      The paper presents valuable data for the community. Additionally, the experimental design and data analysis are well documented.

      Weakness:

      The manuscript presents the effect of Braco-19 on PQS1, a single G4 region with high SHAPE and high Shannon entropy, to suggest that "the compound can selectively target the PQS1 of the high SHAPE-high Shannon region in cells" (lines 625-626). While the effect of Braco-19 on PQS1 is supported by strong evidence in the manuscript, the conclusion regarding the G4 region with high SHAPE and high Shannon entropy is based on a single target, PQS1.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive assessment of our methodology and dataset. We propose that dynamic RNA structures in high SHAPE-high Shannon regions, when stabilized by small molecules, can serve as viable targets for antiviral therapy. Gquadruplexes represent a characteristic type of such dynamic structures that compete with local stem-loop formations in the genome. While we identified seven highly conserved PQSs in the PEDV genome, only PQS1 was located within a high SHAPEhigh Shannon region. To further validate this concept, we have supplemented the revised manuscript with Thioflavin T (ThT) fluorescence turn-on assays (Figures 3D, 3E, and Figure 3 – figure supplement 6), which provide additional evidence for the differential G4-forming capabilities of PQSs across regions with distinct structural features.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major Comments:

      (1) It could be valuable for the authors to spend some more effort comparing their approach to siRNA target discovery and design to current methods for siRNA design. It would be good to highlight which components are novel, and which might offer superior performance with respect to other existing methods.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting this important point. In response, we have rewritten the relevant section in the discussion:

      “Our approach uniquely integrates in situ RNA structural data (SHAPE reactivity and Shannon entropy) to prioritize siRNA targets within stable single-stranded regions (high SHAPE reactivity, low Shannon entropy), which are experimentally validated as accessible in infected cells. This represents a significant departure from traditional siRNA design methods that rely primarily on sequence conservation, thermodynamic rules (e.g., Tuschl rules), or in vitro structural predictions (Ali Zaidi et al., 2023; Qureshi et al., 2018; Tang and Khvorova, 2024),which may not accurately reflect intracellular RNA accessibility. Bowden-Reid et al. designed 39 antiviral siRNAs against various SARS-CoV-2 variants based on sequence conservation, ultimately identifying 8 highly effective sequences (Bowden-Reid et al., 2023). Notably, five of these effective sequences targeted regions that were located in high SHAPE-high Shannon regions according to SARS-CoV-2 SHAPE datasets (Supplementary Table 8) (Manfredonia et al., 2020). This independent finding aligns perfectly with our conclusions and demonstrates that SHAPE-based siRNA design outperforms sequence/structureagnostic approaches, at least in terms of significantly improving antiviral siRNA screening efficiency. Given the growing availability of SHAPE datasets for numerous viruses, we are confident that our methodology will facilitate more precise design of antiviral siRNAs.”

      (2) The section targeting their discovered G4 structure with Braco-19 is interesting, particularly showing effects on viral proliferation; however, it's not clear to me how this compound could be used therapeutically against PEDV, as it is a non-selective binder of G4 structures. Their results are good support for the presence and functionality of a G4 structure in PEDV, but I don't see any strategy outlined in the manuscript on how this could be specifically targeted with Braco-19.

      While Braco-19 is indeed a broad-spectrum G4 ligand, our data demonstrate that not all PQSs in the viral genome can form G4 structures under physiological conditions. Our results specifically show that Braco-19 exerts its anti-PEDV activity by targeting PQS1, which is located in a high SHAPE-high Shannon entropy region. This target specificity was further confirmed by the complete resistance of the PQS1mut strain (lacking G4-forming ability) to Braco-19 treatment in our in vitro assays. 

      Additionally, previous studies have reported that during rapid viral replication, viral RNA accumulates to levels that significantly exceed host RNA concentrations. This "concentration advantage" suggests that G4 ligands like Braco-19 would preferentially bind viral G4 structures over host targets, thereby enhancing their antiviral specificity in vivo. In summary, our data provide proof-of-concept that viral genomic regions with high G4-forming potential - particularly those in high SHAPE-high Shannon entropy regions - represent promising targets for antiviral therapy.

      (3) The section where they proposed 3D RNA structures based on sequence similarity feels "tacked on" and I don't see how it adds to the overall story. The authors identify a short RNA hairpin in the PEDV genome with some sequence similarity to the CPEB3 nuclease P4 hairpin. However, they don't provide any evidence that this motif functions in a similar way or that it's important for the virus's life cycle. They also don't explain how this similarity could be exploited for antiviral drug development. It's not clear whether targeting this motif would have any effect on the virus. It's interesting that these two sequences share nucleotides, but it's unlikely that they share any homology...perhaps they convergently evolved (or were captured), but the similarity could also be coincidental.

      We appreciate the reviewer's insightful observation regarding this section. While our intention was to demonstrate that flexible conformations in high SHAPE-high Shannon regions could potentially be targeted, we acknowledge that extensive discussion of these motifs' functions would exceed the scope of this study, resulting in some disconnection from the main narrative. In response to this valuable feedback, we have consequentially removed it from the manuscript.

      (4) The authors should consider the optimality of the synonymous mutation (G3109A) that they introduced, as G3109A could swap a rare codon for a more optimal one. Even though the protein sequence is unaffected, the translation rate (and ability to proliferate) could be very different due to altered codon optimality. Additionally, to show the inactivity of the PQS3 mutant, the Braco-19 treatment studies performed on the PQS1 mutants could be repeated with PQS3 - using this as a control for these experiments.

      We appreciate the reviewer's insightful comment regarding codon optimization. In the PEDV genome, the PQS1 mutation site encodes lysine (AAG). Since lysine has only two codons (AAG and AAA), the G3109A synonymous mutation was our only viable option. Published literature (Ding et al. 2024) confirms that neither AAG nor AAA are classified as either preferred or rare codons in mammalian cells. Therefore, this substitution should have minimal direct impact on translation efficiency. Compared to nonsynonymous mutations that would alter amino acid sequences, we believe this synonymous mutation represents the optimal approach for maintaining native protein function while introducing the desired structural modification.

      REFERENCES:

      Ding W, Yu W, Chen Y, et al. Rare codon recoding for efficient noncanonical amino acid incorporation in mammalian cells. Science. 2024;384(6700):1134-1142.

      In the revised version, we have added control experiments showing the inhibitory activity of Braco-19 against the PQS3 mutant strain (Figure 4—figure supplement 3C) and discussed it in the results section.

      “Furthermore, as a control, we observed nearly identical inhibitory activity of Braco19 against both the PQS3 mutant strain (AJ1102-PQS3mut) and wild-type virus (Figure 4—figure supplement 3C), demonstrating the specificity of Braco-19's action on PQS1.”

      Minor Comments:

      (5) The authors' description of the Shannon Entropy could be improved. The current description makes it seem like the Shannon Entropy only provides information on base pairing, however, the Shannon entropy quantifies the uncertainty of structural states at each position and is calculated based on the probabilities of the different states (paired or unpaired) that a nucleotide can adopt.

      We have revised the description of Shannon entropy in the manuscript:

      "The pairing probability of each nucleotide derived from SHAPE reactivities was subsequently used to calculate Shannon entropy. Regions with high Shannon entropy may adopt alternative conformations, while those with low Shannon entropy correspond to either well-defined RNA structures or persistently single-stranded regions (MATHEWS, 2004; Siegfried et al., 2014)."

      (6) The overall writing of the manuscript is very good, but there are some minor grammatical issues throughout, e.g., here are some of the ones that I caught:

      a) Lines 71-3: "various types of RNA structures such as hairpin structure, RNA singlestrand, RNA pseudoknot and RNA G-quadruplex (G4)" - the examples should be plural and, rather than "hairpins" (or in addition), perhaps add "helixes" to be more generically correct(?).

      We have revised the relevant description: 

      "various types of RNA structures such as stem-loop structures (with double-helical stems), RNA single-strand, RNA pseudoknot and RNA G-quadruplex (G4)"

      b) Lines 74-5: "Of these, RNA G4 has shown considerable promise because of the high stability and modulation by small molecules" should be "Of these, RNA G4 has shown considerable promise because of its high stability and ability for modulation by small molecules."

      We have revised the sentence:

      “Of these, RNA G4 has shown considerable promise because of its high stability and ability for modulation by small molecules.”

      c) Line 76: "have" should be "has".

      We have revised the sentence.

      d) Lines 104-5 (and elsewhere): "frameshift stimulation element (FSE)" should be "frameshift stimulatory element (FSE)".

      We have revised the sentence.

      e) Lines 428-9: following the Manfredonia's methods" should be "following Manfredonia's method" or "following the Manfredonia method".

      We have made the appropriate edit.

      These edits ensure grammatical accuracy and consistency with standard scientific terminology. We appreciate the reviewer's attention to detail, which has significantly improved the clarity of our manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) There are some important references missing, on shape-seq from Julius Lucks.

      We have added citations to the foundational work by Lucks et al. (2011, PNAS) that pioneered in vitro RNA structure probing using SHAPE-seq.

      (2) Describe the acronym "SHAPE",

      We have now included the full name of SHAPE:“Selective 2’-Hydroxyl Acylation and Primer Extension”.

      (3) Line 81: 2"-hydroxyl-selective - the prime is incorrect.

      We thank the reviewer for catching this technical error. We have corrected "2"hydroxyl" to "2'-hydroxyl".

      (4) Explaining a bit better how shape reagent works would be beneficial (one sentence should suffice).

      We have revised the Introduction section:

      “SHAPE reagents like NAI selectively modify flexible, unpaired 2′-OH groups in RNA, and these modifications are detected as mutations during reverse transcription, enabling precise mapping of RNA secondary structures through sequencing.”

      (5) Line 128: cite the paper that introduced NAI.

      We have now properly cited the original publication introducing NAI(Spitale et al., 2012).

      (6) Line 243: Can you describe what the compound is?

      The compound is Braco-19. This has now been included in the methods section. 

      (7) Line 272: describe what 3Dpol is and the source of it.

      We have supplemented the relevant information as follows:

      "3Dpol (recombinant RNA-dependent RNA polymerase; Abcam, ab277617, 0.02 mg/reaction)"

      (8) Figure 1 legend: For both C and D, the explanation of the G4 structure and the RISC complex should be added, otherwise, it becomes unclear why they are there.

      We have revised the captions for Figure 1 as follows:

      "(A) Well-folded regions (low SHAPE reactivity and low Shannon entropy; 26.40% of genome). These regions represent stably folded RNA structures with minimal conformational flexibility, likely serving as structural scaffolds or functional elements in viral replication. (B) Dynamic structured regions (low SHAPE reactivity and high Shannon entropy; 11.70% of genome). These conformationally plastic domains likely mediate regulatory switches between alternative secondary structures during infection. (C) Dynamic unpaired regions (high SHAPE reactivity and high Shannon entropy; 26.90% of genome). These regions are prone to form non-canonical nucleic acid structures (e.g., G-quadruplexes), which can be stabilized by small-molecule ligands to inhibit viral replication. (D) Persistent unpaired regions (high SHAPE reactivity and low Shannon entropy; 9.67% of genome). These regions are more accessible for siRNA binding, facilitating recruitment of Argonaute proteins and Dicer to form the RNAinduced silencing complex (RISC) for targeted cleavage."

      (9) Figure S2 panel A should be in Figure 1. This is a nice picture showing the backbone of the research.

      In the revised manuscript, we have reorganized Figure 1 and Figure S2 by incorporating the SHAPE-MaP workflow diagram (previously Figure S2A) into Figure 1 as panel (A): 

      (10) Please add the citation to Braco-19.

      We have now added the appropriate citation for Braco-19 (Gowan et al., 2002) in the revised manuscript.

      (11) Figure 5 legend: could you add in parenthesis the what ds means (and call Figure S28).

      We appreciate the reviewer's attention to detail. In the revised manuscript, we have clarified the abbreviations in the Figure 5 legend: ss (single-stranded targeting siRNAs); ds (dual-stranded targeting siRNAs). 

      (12) Line 107: I would argue that the "stabilization of a G4" inhibited viral proliferation. And that supports the point of the paper, that a small molecule that stabilizes the G4 can be used to reduce viral replication. I suggest emphasizing this thorough the paper.

      We fully concur with the reviewer's insightful perspective. In the revised manuscript, we have comprehensively strengthened the point of 'G4 stabilization' as an antiviral mechanism through the following enhancements:

      (1) In the Results section: We present Thioflavin T (ThT) fluorescence assays demonstrating the G4-forming capability of PQSs in the full-length PEDV genomic RNA context:

      “These findings indicate that although most PQSs can form G4 structures in vitro, PQS1—located in the high SHAPE-high Shannon entropy region—demonstrates the most robust G4-forming capability when competing with local secondary structures in the genomic context.”

      (2) In the Results section: The inclusion of Braco-19 inhibition assays using PQS3 mutant virus as control provides robust evidence that Braco-19 exerts its antiviral effects specifically through PQS1 stabilization:

      “Furthermore, as a control, we observed nearly identical inhibitory activity of Braco-19 against both the PQS3 mutant strain (AJ1102-PQS3mut) and wild-type virus, demonstrating the specificity of Braco-19's action on PQS1.”

      (3) In the Discussion section: We have rewritten the mechanistic interpretation to emphasize: 

      "Crucially, Braco-19 showed no inhibitory activity against the PQS1-mutant strain while maintaining potent activity against the PQS3-mutant strain (Figure 4E, Figure 4—figure supplement 3C). This suggests that the compound can selectively target the PQS1 of the high SHAPE-high Shannon region in cells." 

      (13) For PQS1, it's suggested that it is indeed a competing and transient conformation that forms the G4. I wonder if using an extended PQS1 (perhaps what is shown in Figure 3E) and using fluorescence, and/or K+ vs Li+, and/or in-vitro SHAPE could tell us more about this dynamic structure. Thioflavin T or any other fluorescent molecule that binds to G4s could be easily used to show how the formation of G4 may happen or not. In addition, how Braco-19 could really lock the dynamic structure in-vitro as well. I think the field would benefit from a deeper investigation of it.

      To address the dynamic competition between G4 and alternative RNA conformations, we performed Thioflavin T (ThT) fluorescence turn-on assay (now in Figure 3D-E and Figure 3—figure supplement 6) under physiological K<sup>+</sup> conditions (100 mM), with PRRSV-G4 RNA as a positive control. This reads as:

      “To validate whether SHAPE analysis could reflect the competitive conformational folding of PQSs in the PEDV genome, we performed in vitro transcription to obtain local intact structures containing PQSs within dynamic single-stranded regions and stable double-stranded regions (Table S6). Thioflavin T (ThT) fluorescence turn-on assays were conducted under physiological K<sup>+</sup> conditions (100 mM), with the G4 sequence of porcine reproductive and respiratory syndrome virus (PRRSV) serving as a positive control (Control-G4)(Fang et al., 2023). The results demonstrated that for short PQSs sequences containing only G4-forming motifs (Table S7), PQS1, PQS3, PQS4, and PQS6 all induced significant ThT fluorescence enhancement (Figure 3D-E, Figure 3—figure supplement 6), confirming their ability to form G4 structures. However, in long RNA fragments encompassing PQSs and their flanking sequences, only PQS1 and PQS4 exhibited pronounced ThT fluorescence responses (Figure 3DE), whereas PQS2, PQS3, and PQS6 showed negligible signals (Figure 3E, Figure 3— figure supplement 6). Notably, the PQS1-long chain displayed the strongest fluorescence signal, while its mutant counterpart (PQS1mut-long chain) exhibited the lowest background fluorescence (Figure 3D). These findings indicate that although most PQSs can form G4 structures in vitro, PQS1—located in the high SHAPE-high Shannon entropy region—demonstrates the most robust G4-forming capability when competing with local secondary structures in the genomic context. Therefore, PQS1 was selected for further structural and functional validation.”

      (14) Figure S29 is nice and informative. Consider moving it to the main text.

      We appreciate the reviewer's positive assessment of Figure S29. Now we have renamed this figure as "Figure 5—Supplement 2".

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) Changes in blood volume due to brain activity are indirectly related to neuronal responses. The exact relationship is not clear, however, we do know two things for certain: (a) each measurable unit of blood volume change depends on the response of hundreds or thousands of neurons, and (b) the time course of the volume changes are slow compared to the potential time course of the underlying neuronal responses. Both of these mean that important variability in neuronal responses will be averaged out when measuring blood changes. For example, if two neighbouring neurons have opposite responses to a given stimulus, this will produce opposite changes in blood volume, which will cancel each other out in the blood volume measurement due to (a). This is important in the present study because blood volume changes are implicitly being used as a measure of coding in the underlying neuronal population. The authors need to acknowledge that this is a coarse measure of neuronal responses and that important aspects of neuronal responses may be missing from the blood volume measure.

      The reviewer is correct: we do not measure neuronal firing, but use blood volume as a proxy for bulk local neuronal activity, which does not capture the richness of single neuron responses. We will highlight this point in the manuscript. This is why the paper focuses on large-scale spatial representations as well as cross-species comparison. For this latter purpose, fMRI responses are on par with our fUSI data, with both neuroimaging techniques showing the same weakness.

      (2) More importantly for the present study, however, the effect of (b) is that any rapid changes in the response of a single neuron will be cancelled out by temporal averaging. Imagine a neuron whose response is transient, consisting of rapid excitation followed by rapid inhibition. Temporal averaging of these two responses will tend to cancel out both of them. As a result, blood volume measurements will tend to smooth out any fast, dynamic responses in the underlying neuronal population. In the present study, this temporal averaging is likely to be particularly important because the authors are comparing responses to dynamic (nonstationary) stimuli with responses to more constant stimuli. To a first approximation, neuronal responses to dynamic stimuli are themselves dynamic, and responses to constant stimuli are themselves constant. Therefore, the averaging will mean that the responses to dynamic stimuli are suppressed relative to the real responses in the underlying neurons, whereas the responses to constant stimuli are more veridical. On top of this, temporal following rates tend to decrease as one ascends the auditory hierarchy, meaning that the comparison between dynamic and stationary responses will be differently affected in different brain areas. As a result, the dynamic/stationary balance is expected to change as you ascend the hierarchy, and I would expect this to directly affect the results observed in this study.

      It is not trivial to extrapolate from what we know about temporal following in the cortex to know exactly what the expected effect would be on the authors' results. As a first-pass control, I would strongly suggest incorporating into the authors' filterbank model a range of realistic temporal following rates (decreasing at higher levels), and spatially and temporally average these responses to get modelled cerebral blood flow measurements. I would want to know whether this model showed similar effects as in Figure 2. From my guess about what this model would show, I think it would not predict the effects shown by the authors in Figure 2. Nevertheless, this is an important issue to address and to provide control for.

      We understand the reviewer’s concern about potential differences in response dynamics in stationary vs non-stationary sounds. In particular, it seems that the reviewer is concerned that responses to foregrounds may be suppressed in non-primary fields because foregrounds are not stationary, and non-primary regions could struggle to track and respond to these sounds. Nevertheless, we  observed the contrary, with non-primary regions over-representing non-stationary (dynamic) sounds, over stationary ones. For this reason, we are inclined to think that this explanation cannot falsify our findings.

      Furthermore, background sounds are not completely constant: they are still dynamic sounds, but their temporal modulation rates are usually faster (see Figure 3B). Similarly, neural responses to these two types of sounds are dynamic (see for example Hamersky et al., 2025, Figure 1).  Thus, we are not sure that blood volume would transform the responses to these types of sounds non-linearly.

      We understand the comment that temporal following rates might differ across regions in the auditory hierarchy and agree. In fact, we show that tuning to temporal rates differ across regions and partly explains the differences in background invariance we observe. We think the reviewer’s suggestion is already implemented by our spectrotemporal model, which incorporates the full range of realistic temporal following rates (up to 128 Hz). The temporal averaging is done as we take the output of the model (which varies continuously through time) and average it in the same window as we used for our fUSI data. When we fit this model to the ferret data, we find that voxels in non-primary regions, especially VP (tertiary auditory cortex), tend to be more tuned to low temporal rates (Figure 2F, G), and that background invariance is stronger in voxels tuned to low rates. This is, however, not true in humans, suggesting that background invariance in humans rely on different computational mechanisms.

      (3) I do not agree with the equivalence that the authors draw between the statistical stationarity of sounds and their classification as foreground or background sounds. It is true that, in a common foreground/background situation - speech against a background of white noise - the foreground is non-stationary and the background is stationary. However, it is easy to come up with examples where this relationship is reversed. For example, a continuous pure tone is perfectly stationary, but will be perceived as a foreground sound if played loudly. Background music may be very non-stationary but still easily ignored as a background sound when listening to overlaid speech. Ultimately, the foreground/background distinction is a perceptual one that is not exclusively determined by physical characteristics of the sounds, and certainly not by a simple measure of stationarity. I understand that the use of foreground/background in the present study increases the likely reach of the paper, but I don't think it is appropriate to use this subjective/imprecise terminology in the results section of the paper.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment that the classification of our sounds into foregrounds and backgrounds is not verified by any perceptual experiments. We use those terms to be consistent with the literature, including the paper we derived this definition from (Kell et al., 2019). These terms are widely used in studies where no perceptual or behavioral experiments are included, and even when animals are anesthetized. However, we will emphasize the limits of this definition when introducing it, as well as in the discussion.

      (4) Related to the above, I think further caveats need to be acknowledged in the study. We do not know what sounds are perceived as foreground or background sounds by ferrets, or indeed whether they make this distinction reliably to the degree that humans do. Furthermore, the individual sounds used here have not been tested for their foreground/background-ness. Thus, the analysis relies on two logical jumps - first, that the stationarity of these sounds predicts their foreground/background perception in humans, and second, that this perceptual distinction is similar in ferrets and humans. I don't think it is known to what degree these jumps are justified. These issues do not directly affect the results, but I think it is essential to address these issues in the Discussion, because they are potentially major caveats to our understanding of the work.

      We agree with the reviewer that the foreground-background distinction might be different in ferrets. In anticipation of that issue, we had enriched the sound set with more ecologically relevant sounds, such as ferret and other animal vocalizations. Nevertheless, the point remains valid and is already raised in the discussion. We will emphasize this limitation in addition to the limitation of our definition of foregrounds and backgrounds.

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) Interpretation of the cerebral blood volume signal: While the results are compelling, more caution should be exercised by the authors in framing their results, given that they are measuring an indirect measure of neural activity, this is the difference between stating "CBV in area MEG was less background invariant than in higher areas" vs. saying "MEG was less background invariant than other areas". Beyond framing, the basic properties of the CBV signal should be better explored:

      a) Cortical vasculature is highly structured (e.g. Kirst et al.( 2020) Cell). One potential explanation for the results is simply differences in vasculature and blood flow between primary and secondary areas of auditory cortex, even if fUS is sensitive to changes in blood flow, changes in capillary beds, etc (Mace et al., 2011) Nat. Methods.. This concern could be addressed by either analyzing spontaneous fluctuations in the CBV signal during silent periods or computing a signal-to-noise ratio of voxels across areas across all sound types. This is especially important given the complex 3D geometry of gyri and sulci in the ferret brain.

      We agree with the reviewers that there could be differences in vasculature across subregions of the auditory cortex. We will run analyses providing comparisons of basic signal properties across our different regions of interest. We note that this point would also be valid for the human fMRI data, for which we cannot run these controls. Nevertheless, this should not affect our analyses and results, which should be independent of local vascular density. First, we normalize the signal in each voxel before any analysis, so that the absolute strength of the signal, or blood volume in a given voxel, does not matter. Second, we do see sound-evoked responses in all regions (Figure S2) and only focus on reliable voxels in each region. Third, our analysis mostly relies on voxel-based correlation across sounds, which is independent of the mean and variance of the voxel responses. Thus, we believe that differences in vascular architecture across regions are unlikely to affect our results.

      b) Figure 1 leaves the reader uncertain what exactly is being encoded by the CBV signal, as temporal responses to different stimuli look very similar in the examples shown. One possibility is that the CBV is an acoustic change signal. In that case, sounds that are farther apart in acoustic space from previous sounds would elicit larger responses, which is straightforward to test. Another possibility is that the fUS signal reflects time-varying features in the acoustic signal (e.g. the low-frequency envelope). This could be addressed by cross-correlating the stimulus envelope with fUS waveform. The third possibility, which the authors argue, is that the magnitude of the fUS signal encodes the stimulus ID. A better understanding of the justification for only looking at the fUS magnitude in a short time window (2-4.8 s re: stimulus onset) would increase my confidence in the results.

      We thank the reviewer for raising that point as it highlights that the layout of Figure 1 is misleading. While Figure 1B shows an example snippet of our sound streams, Figure 1D shows the average timecourse of CBV time-locked to a change in sound (foreground or background, isolated or in a mixture). This is the average across all voxels and sounds, and the point is just to illustrate the dynamics for the three broad categories. In Figure 1E however, we show the cross-validated cross-correlation of CBV  across sounds (and different time lags). To obtain this, we compute for each voxel the response to each sound at each time lag, thus obtaining two vector of size number of sounds per lag, one per repeat. Then, we correlate all these vectors across the two repeats, obtaining one cross-correlation matrix per neuron. We finally average these matrices across all neurons. The fact that you see red squares demonstrates that the signal encodes sound identity, since CBV is more similar across two repeats of the same sound (for e.g., in the foreground only matrix, 0-5 s vs 0-5 s), than two different sounds (0-5 s vs. 7-12 s). We will modify the figure layout as well as the legend to improve clarity.

      (2) Interpretation of the human data: The authors acknowledge in the discussion that there are several differences between fMRI and fUS. The results would be more compelling if they performed a control analysis where they downsampled the Ferret fUS data spatially and temporally to match the resolution of fMRI and demonstrated that their ferret results hold with lower spatiotemporal resolution.

      We agree with the reviewer that the use of different techniques might come in the way of cross-species comparison. We will add additional discussion on this point. We already control for the temporal aspect by using the average of stimulus-evoked activity across time (note that due to scanner noise, sounds are presented cut into small pieces in the fMRI experiments). Regarding the spatial aspect, there are several things to consider. First, both species have brains of very different sizes, a factor that is conveniently compensated for by the higher spatial resolution of fUSI compared to fMRI (0.1 vs 2 mm). Downsampling to fMRI resolution would lead to having one voxel per region per slice, which is not feasible. We also summarize results with one value per region, which is a form of downsampling that is fairer across species. Furthermore, we believe that we already established in a previous study (Landemard et al, 2021 eLife) that fUSI and fMRI data are comparable signals. We indeed could predict human fMRI responses to most sounds from ferret fUSI responses to the same identical sounds.

      Reviewer #3:

      As mentioned above, interpretation of the invariance analyses using predictions from the spectrotemporal modulation encoding model hinges on the model's ability to accurately predict neural responses. Although Figure S5 suggests the encoding model was generally able to predict voxel responses accurately, the authors note in the introduction that, in human auditory cortex, this kind of tuning can explain responses in primary areas but not in non-primary areas (Norman-Haignere & McDermott, PLOS Biol. 2018). Indeed, the prediction accuracy histograms in Figure S5C suggest a slight difference in the model's ability to predict responses in primary versus non-primary voxels. Additional analyses should be done to a) determine whether the prediction accuracies are meaningfully different across regions and b) examine whether controlling for prediction accuracy across regions (i.e., sub-selecting voxels across regions with matched prediction accuracy) affects the outcomes of the invariance analyses.

      The reviewer is correct: the spectrotemporal model tends to perform less well in human non-primary cortex. We believe this does not contradict our results but goes in the same direction: while there is a gradient in invariance in both ferrets and humans, this gradient is predicted by the spectrotemporal model in ferrets, but not in humans (possibly indeed because predictions are less good in human non-primary auditory cortex). Regardless of the mechanism, this result points to a difference across species. We will clarify these points by quantifying potential differences in prediction accuracy in both species and comment on those in the manuscript.

      A related concern is the procedure used to train the encoding model. From the methods, it appears that the model may have been fit using responses to both isolated and mixture sounds. If so, this raises questions about the interpretability of the invariance analyses. In particular, fitting the model to all stimuli, including mixtures, may inflate the apparent ability of the model to "explain" invariance, since it is effectively trained on the phenomenon it is later evaluated on. Put another way, if a voxel exhibits invariance, and the model is trained to predict the voxel's responses to all types of stimuli (both isolated sounds and mixtures), then the model must also show invariance to the extent it can accurately predict voxel responses, making the result somewhat circular. A more informative approach would be to train the encoding model only on responses to isolated sounds (or even better, a completely independent set of sounds), as this would help clarify whether any observed invariance is emergent from the model (i.e., truly a result of low-level tuning to spectrotemporal features) or simply reflects what it was trained to reproduce.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion and will run an additional prediction using only the sounds presented in isolation. This will be included in the next version of the manuscript.

      Finally, the interpretation of the foreground invariance results remains somewhat unclear. In ferrets (Figure 2I), the authors report relatively little foreground invariance, whereas in humans (Figure 5G), most participants appear to show relatively high levels of foreground invariance in primary auditory cortex (around 0.6 or greater). However, the paper does not explicitly address these apparent cross-species differences. Moreover, the findings in ferrets seem at odds with other recent work in ferrets (Hamersky et al. 2025 J. Neurosci.), which shows that background sounds tend to dominate responses to mixtures, suggesting a prevalence of foreground invariance at the neuronal level. Although this comparison comes with the caveat that the methods differ substantially from those used in the current study, given the contrast with the findings of this paper, further discussion would nonetheless be valuable to help contextualize the current findings and clarify how they relate to prior work.

      We thank the reviewer for this point. We will indeed add further discussion of the  difference between ferrets and humans in foreground invariance in primary auditory cortex. In addition, while we found a trend for higher background invariance than foreground invariance in ferret primary auditory cortex, this difference was not significant and many voxels exhibit similar levels of background and foreground invariance (for example in Figure 2D, G). Thus, we do not think our results are inconsistent with Hamersky et al., 2025, though we agree the bias towards background sounds is not as strong in our data. This might indeed reflect differences in methodology, both in the signal that is measured (blood volume vs spikes), and the sound presentation paradigm. We will add this point to our discussion.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The ventral nerve cord (VNC) of organisms like Drosophila is an invaluable model for studying neural development and organisation in more complex organisms. Its well-defined structure allows researchers to investigate how neurons develop, differentiate, and organise into functional circuits. As a critical central nervous system component, the VNC plays a key role in controlling motor functions, reflexes, and sensory integration.

      Particularly relevant to this work, the VNC provides a unique opportunity to explore neuronal hemilineages - groups of neurons that share molecular, genetic, and functional identities. Understanding these hemilineages is crucial for elucidating how neurons cooperate to form specialized circuits, essential for comprehending normal brain function and dysfunction.<br /> A significant challenge in the field has been the lack of developmentally stable, hemilineage-specific driver lines that enable precise tracking and measurement of individual VNC hemilineages. The authors address this need by generating and validating a comprehensive, lineage-specific split-GAL4 driver library.

      Strengths and weaknesses

      The authors select new marker genes for hemilineages from previously published single-cell data of the VNC. They generate and validate specific and temporally stable lines for almost all the hemilineages in the VNC. They successfully achieved their aims, and their results support their conclusions. This will be a valuable resource for investigating neural circuit formation and function.

      We thank the reviewer for her/his positive comments and time reviewing our manuscript. We are pleased that the reviewer recognized the value of our work in generating a comprehensive, lineage-specific split-GAL4 driver library for VNC hemilineages. We agree that this will be a critical resource for investigating neural circuit formation and function, and we are encouraged by the positive comments regarding the novelty and potential impact of our approach.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I have no suggestions for further experiments, data, or analyses. There are some grammatical errors and referencing issues throughout, but the editors will hopefully catch them.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments regarding the grammatical errors and referencing issues and have carefully checked the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      It is my pleasure to review this manuscript from Soffers, Lacin, and colleagues, in which they identify pairs of transcription factors unique to (almost) every ventral nerve cord hemilineage in Drosophila and use these pairs to create reagents to label and manipulate these cells. The advance is sold as largely technical-as a pipeline for identifying durably expressed transcription factor codes in postmitotic neurons from single cell RNAseq data, generating knock-in alleles in the relevant genes, using these to match transcriptional cell types to anatomic cell types, and then using the alleles as a genetic handle on the cells for downstream explication of their function. Yet I think the work is gorgeous in linking the expression of genes that are causal for neuron-type-specific characteristics to the anatomic instantiations of those neurons. It is astounding that the authors are able to use their deep collective knowledge of hemilineage anatomy and gene expression to match 33 of 34 transcriptional profiles. Together with other recent studies, this work drives a major course correction in developmental biology, away from empirically identified cell type "markers" (in Drosophila neuroscience, often genomic DNA fragments that contain enhancers found to be expressed in specific neurons at specific times), and towards methods in which the genes that generate neuronal type identity are actually used to study those neurons. Because the relationship between fate and form/function is built into the tools, I believe that this approach will be a trojan horse to integrate the fields of neural development and systems neuroscience.

      We thank the reviewer for their time reviewing our manuscript, generous compliments, and appreciation of the potential of our study to drive a major shift in developmental biology, moving away from traditional marker-based methods toward utilizing the genes that mark neuronal type identity in “omics” datasets. Much like the Trojan Horse, which, though initially a concealed and subtle tool, we hope that the strategy outlined here will have continued impact, as we and others plan to leverage future high-resolution and developmental series of scRNAseq datasets to generate driver lines to target neuronal cell types with uttermost precision.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Line 126-127: I'm not sure if it is true to say "most TFs in the CNS are expressed in a hemilineage-specific manner." As the authors haven't formally interrogated how different neuronal features relate to expression patterns of all ~600 Drosophila TFs, how about replacing "most" with "many?"

      The reviewer makes an excellent point. Work by Lacin and colleagues has demonstrated via genetic studies that lineage-specific transcription factors that regulate the specification and differentiation of postembryonic neurons are stably expressed during development. This was documented for 15 transcription factors in Lacin et al., 2014, and our lab has identified additional examples since. When we refer to the stable expression of transcription factors, we refer to such transcription factors, not the complete set of ~600 transcription factors described to date. We have added this citation to clarify this statement and replaced p6 line 135 ”Most”  by “Many”. We have also address this now in the introduction (p5 line 109-116). Of note, as we conducted this study, we found that is closer to be a rule than an exception that if a transcription factor acted cluster as marker, it was also stably expressed during development. Thus, a growing number of transcription factors is now documented to be stably expressed in a hemilineage-specific manner

      Line 265: Typo? 334 should be 34?

      We thank the reviewer for noting this type error. We have corrected this typographical error.

      Line 522: Refs 56, 57 here related to chinmo, mamo, br-c don't show br-c or mamo mark temporal cohorts of postmitotic neurons. Consider adding PMID: 19883497, 18510932, and 31545163.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out and have added these references that demonstrate that broad, Mamo and Chinmo mark temporal cohorts in the developing adult CNS (p17 line 535).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Soffers et al. developed a comprehensive genetic toolkit that enables researchers to access neuronal hemilineages during developmental and adult time points using scRNA-seq analysis to guide gene cassette exchange-based or CRISPR-based tool building. Currently, research groups studying neural circuit development are challenged with tying together findings in the development and mature circuit function of hemilineage-related neurons. Here, authors leverage publicly available scRNA-seq datasets to inform the development of a split-Gal4 library that targets 32 of 34 hemilineages in development and adult stages. The authors demonstrated that the split-Gal4 library, or genetic toolkit, can be used to assess the functional roles, neurotransmitter identity, and morphological changes in targeted cells. The tools presented in this study should prove to be incredibly useful to Drosophila neurobiologists seeking to link neural developmental changes to circuit assembly and mature circuit function. Additionally, some hemilineages have more than one split-Gal4 combination that will be advantageous for studies seeking to disrupt associated upstream genes.

      Strengths:

      Informing genetic tool development with publicly available scRNA-seq datasets is a powerful approach to creating specific driver lines. Additionally, this approach can be easily replicated by other researchers looking to generate similar driver lines for more specific subpopulations of cells, as mentioned in the Discussion.

      The unification of optogenetic stimulation data of 8B neurons and connectomic analysis of the Giant-Fiber-induced take-off circuit was an excellent example of the utility of this study. The link between hemilineage-specific functional assays and circuit assembly has been limited by insufficient genetic tools. The tools and data present in this study will help better understand how collections of hemilineages develop in a genetically constrained manner to form circuits amongst each other selectively.

      Weaknesses:

      Although cell position, morphology (to some extent), and gene expression are good markers to track cell identity across developmental time, there are genetic tools available that could have been used to permanently label cells that expressed genes of interest from birth, ensuring that the same cells are being tracked in fixed tissue images.

      Although gene activation is a good proxy for assaying neurochemical features, relying on whether neurochemical pathway genes are activated in a cell to determine its phenotype can be misleading given that the Trojan-Gal4 system commandeers the endogenous transcriptional regulation of a gene but not its post-transcriptional regulation. Therefore, neurochemical identity is best identified via protein detection. (strong language used in this section of the paper).

      The authors mainly rely on the intersectional expression of transcription factors to generate split-Gal4 lines and target hemilineages specifically. However, the Introduction (Lines 97-99) makes a notable point about how driver lines in the past, which have also predominantly relied on the regulatory sequences of transcription factors, lack the temporal stability to investigate hemilineages across time. This point seems to directly conflict with the argument made in the Results (Lines 126-127) that states that most transcription factors are stably expressed in hemilineage neurons that express them. It is generally known that transcription factors can be expressed stably or transiently depending on the context. It is unclear how using the genes of transcription factors in this study circumvents the issue of creating temporally stable driver lines.<br />

      We thank the reviewer for their time to thoroughly and carefully review our manuscript. We appreciate the reviewer’s comments on its strengths, and we to hope that this body of work will prove to be incredibly useful to Drosophila neurobiologists seeking to link neural developmental changes to circuit assembly and mature circuit function. Likewise, we also appreciate the reviews careful consideration of its weaknesses, as the reviewer raises valid points. We have addressed these in our revised manuscript and believe this has significantly improved our manuscript.

      Weakness 1: Although cell position, morphology (to some extent), and gene expression are good markers to track cell identity across developmental time, there are genetic tools available that could have been used to permanently label cells that expressed genes of interest from birth, ensuring that the same cells are being tracked in fixed tissue images.

      The reviewer is fully correct, and we are aware of techniques developed by the laboratories of U. Banerjee, T. Lee, and J. Truman that can make transient GAL4 expression permanent, such as G-TRACE and lineage filtering. A common feature of these techniques is that effector activity is permanent (FLP-mediated removal of the FRT-flanked stop codon preceding GFP in G-TRACE or LexA in lineage filtering) but not the GAL4 activity, which is needed to take advantage of the vast UAS based effector lines such as RNAi libraries. For example, the study of Harris et al., 2015 from the Truman lab beautifully showed the strength of this kind of approaches for labeling the hemilineages but their approach cannot be used for functional studies for the reasons mentioned above. Fly lines using these approaches already have several transgenes and require the addition of several more to be used for functional studies. Our approach requires only two transgenes and is compatible with all UAS lines. One additional advantage of the splitGAL4 combinations that we identify here is that they are inserted in genes that are stably expressed throughout larval and pupal development in postmitotic cells, such that they can be used for functional manipulations during development. We emphasized this point in the discussion on page 16 under the heading “Mapping and manipulating morphological outgrowth patterns of hemilineages during development”. 

      Weakness 2: Although gene activation is a good proxy for assaying neurochemical features, relying on whether neurochemical pathway genes are activated in a cell to determine its phenotype can be misleading given that the Trojan-Gal4 system commandeers the endogenous transcriptional regulation of a gene but not its post-transcriptional regulation. Therefore, neurochemical identity is best identified via protein detection. (strong language used in this section of the paper).

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up this important point. We agree that the Trojan-GAL4 approach will not faithfully recapitulate expression of genes that undergo posttranscriptional regulation. Our previous eLife paper (Lacin et al., 2019) showed that this is the case for Trojan driver lines for the ChAT gene. This study demonstrated that ChAT drivers unexpectedly but strongly labeled many GABAergic and Glutamatergic neurons in both the brain and VNC. With RNA in situ hybridization and immunostainings approaches, we showed that these neurons indeed express ChAT mRNA but not the protein. After our publication, another group showed a class of miRNA binds to the 3’UTR of the ChAT gene and regulates its expression post-transcriptionally (Griffith 2023). We believe that one major reason the Trojan driver lines do not faithfully recapitulate this expression pattern is due to the presence of the Hsp70 transcriptional terminator located at the 5’ end of the trojan exon which prematurely ends the transcript and affects the host gene’s 3’ UTR regulation. For this reason, we have recently generated new Trojan plasmids which allow the retention of the 3’UTR of the host gene in the transcript. We have revised the result section “Neurotransmitter use on pages 11-12 to address this point and have modified the language.

      Weakness 3: The authors mainly rely on the intersectional expression of transcription factors to generate split-Gal4 lines and target hemilineages specifically. However, the Introduction (Lines 97-99) makes a notable point about how driver lines in the past, which have also predominantly relied on the regulatory sequences of transcription factors, lack the temporal stability to investigate hemilineages across time. This point seems to directly conflict with the argument made in the Results (Lines 126-127) that states that most transcription factors are stably expressed in hemilineage neurons that express them. It is generally known that transcription factors can be expressed stably or transiently depending on the context. It is unclear how using the genes of transcription factors in this study circumvents the issue of creating temporally stable driver lines.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this apparent paradox, which we have clarified in the manuscript (p4. lines 94-102). Driver lines in the past have relied on the intersection of genes to label a defined set of neurons, which helped marking more narrow cell populations compared to enhancer traps in the adult CNS. Elegant and elaborate screening methods have been devised to identify hemidriver combinations that mark specific subset of neurons in the adult (Meissner et al, 2025 (eLife 98405.2) and citations therein). However, these hemidrivers do not leverage the expression pattern of hemilineage marker genes. Instead, their expression is controlled by random 2-3 kb genomic fragments. We and others observed that these drivers are not stably expressed during development. Hence, hemidrivers combinations that work beautifully to target adult neuronal cel populations can oftentimes not be directly used for developmental studies. Work by Lacin et al. 2014 has demonstrated that transcription factors that mark hemilineages are oftentimes stably expressed in the embryo larvae and even adult. When we made driver lines for these TF, using artificial exons, its complete endogenous enhancers elements remain intact. Consequently, we find that Trojan driver lines recapitulate the expression pattern of the transcription factor gene in which it was inserted, and the hemidrivers are stably expressed during development. Hence, leveraging scRNAseq cluster markers for hemilineages and converting them to Trojan driver lines, the approach we took in this paper, has proven a powerful method to generate stable driver lines for developmental studies.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 14: Affiliations typo should be correct to "St. Louis".

      We thank the reviewer for catching this and have corrected the typo.

      (2) Line 26: "model systems have focused on only on a few".

      We have replaced the words “a few regions” by “select regions” to better contrast that studies to date have been performed, but not at CNS level, due to the lack of genetic driver lines.

      (3) Line 52: The use of "medium" here is ambiguous without a comparison.

      We agree that the term “medium” in line 52 could be ambiguous without context, and we appreciate your suggestion to clarify this. The revised sentence now reads: “Drosophila has served as a powerful model system to investigate how neuronal circuits function due to its medium complexity compared to vertebrate models”

      (4) Line 91-92: Consider shortening to "of behavioral circuit assembly".

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have revised p4 lines 90-91 to: “Thus, taking a hemilineage-based approach is essential for a systematic and comprehensive understanding of behavioral circuit assembly during development in complex nervous systems.”

      (5) Line 216-217: Consider establishing what the expected morphology and neurochemical phenotype for 2A neurons is before presenting findings.

      This suggestion is well-taken, and agree that this paragraph did not fully get the point across we were trying to make. This purpose of this paragraph is to explain our workflow of how we assigned 16 hemilineages to orphan clusters, which is why we present the data in this order and present the morphology of hemilineage 2A last. To accommodate the reviewer’s suggestion, we have now clarified our approach before diving into the results to improve the flow of this paragraph (p8 lines 218-223). Briefly, the starting point to annotate the 16 orphan scRNAseq clusters was each time taking one orphan scRNAseq cluster, picking its top cluster marker genes that had not been established yet as marker genes for any hemilineage, and visualizing the morphology of the neurons that expressed such cluster marker using a reporter line for the cluster marker or an antibody stain for its protein. We then compared this to documented hemilineage morphologies, and to narrow down our search, we compared the observed trajectories to those of unannotated hemilineages that used the same neurotransmitter as the orphan scRNAseq. The evaluation of the documented morphologies of the hemilineages came at the last part of our method to annotate the hemilineages to orphan scRNAseq clusters, which is why we chose to present the expected morphology of a hemilineage at the end.

      (6) If "neurochemical" phenotype and "neurotransmitter" identity are sometimes used interchangeably but seem to mean the same thing. Consider choosing one term throughout.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion and have changed the terminology to “neurotransmitter use” (p11-12 lines 326-359).

      (7) Line 235: MARCM technique citation needed.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out, the citation (no. 37, p9 line 249) was present in the method section, but we had inadvertently omitted it in the main text and we have now corrected this.

      (8) Line 281: typo, should be "patterns".

      We thank the reviewer for noting this and have corrected this.

      (9) Line 469: End of sentence needs a ".".

      We have added the punctuation mark.

      (10) Line 516: "driver line combinations to express...".

      We have inserted the word “to” to correct it.

      (11) Please make sure that the correct genotypes are matched in the figure legends and Table 1. For instance, knot-GAL4-DBD is listed as the hemi driver for 10B neurons in Figure 3 but only knot-p65.AD is listed in Table 1.

      We thank the reviewer for catching this, we made a mistake and the correct hemidriver combination used in Figure 3L i: knot-GAL4-AD with hb9-GAL4-DBD. We have updated the legend and carefully checked the legends and tables.

      (12) Consider making different color choices for readability when possible and be consistent with labeling CadN. For instance, in Figure 1 the magenta color has three separate meanings: CadN, Acj6, and unc-4. Either of the three genes can be mistaken for the other for a reader mainly paying attention to the magenta color. I find that one color can mean two things in a figure if organized properly but any more begs for confusion. Also, CadN can be easily labeled if used in a new figure (e.g. Figure 1-Supplment 1).

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful observation and have adjusted figure 1 so that cadN is displayed in blue and reporter genes expressing Acj6, Unc-4 or their intersection in green. The legend is modified to reflect these changes.

      (13) If Seurat object changes or additional quality control steps were taken from the original studies, please provide these changes. Similarly, provide any scRNA-seq code used or cite code used for readers to access. Also, provide a section in the methods briefly describing how genes were chosen (criteria) for tool development.

      We thank the reviewer for nothing we had not described our scRNA analysis pipeline and criteria to select transcription factors in the methods section of the manuscript. We have added this section at p19 lines 548-558. Briefly, we used the Seurat object generated by Allen et al., 2015, and did not change quality control steps, normalizations or scaling. Candidate genes to make split-GAL4 drivers from were chosen based on their ability to mark the clusters defined by Allen et al. We did not use computer-based algorithms and made a list of the top cluster markers. Then, we made binary combinations amongst these cluster markers and with hemilineages markers we had identified before (Lacin et al, 2014; Lacin et al 2019), and used the code generated by Allen et al., 2015 (deposited on Github) with Seurat v5 to test if these combinations marked unique clusters. We then prioritized testing these combinations based on the availability of antibodies, BAC lines and CRiMIC/MiMIC constructs to validate their expression pattern prior to creating split-GAL4 lines for these candidates.

      (14) In regard to the seemingly contradictory argument that most transcription factors are stably expressed when most drivers of the past used regulatory elements of transcription factors: the paper could be strengthened by either a) describing how older driver lines differ from the lines presented in the paper or b) remarking on the endogenous temporal stability of the transcription factors used in this study.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out, and we agree that it is necessary to clarify this apparent paradox since it is essential for understanding the impact of the present work. We have revised our manuscript described in our response to weakness 1.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Oor et al. report the potentially independent effects of the spatial and feature-based selection history on visuomotor choices. They outline compelling evidence, tracking the dynamic history effects based on their clever experimental design (urgent version of the search task). Their finding broadens the framework to identify variables contributing to choice behavior and their neural correlates in future studies.

      Strengths:

      In their urgent search task, the variable processing time of the visual cue leads to a dichotomy in choice performance - uninformed guesses vs. informed choices. Oor et al. did rigorous analyses to find a stronger influence of the location-based selection history on the uninformed guesses and a stronger influence of the feature-based selection history on the informed choices. It is a fundamental finding that contributes to understanding the drivers of behavioral variance. The results are clear.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) In this urgent search task, as the authors stated in line 724, the variability in performance was mainly driven by the amount of time available for processing the visual cue. The authors used processing time (PT) as the proxy for this "time available for processing the visual cue." But PT itself is already a measure of behavioral variance since it is also determined by the subject's reaction time (i.e., PT = Reaction time (RT) - Gap). In that sense, it seems circular to explain the variability in performance using the variability in PT. I understand the Gap time and PT are correlated (hinted by the RT vs. Gap in Figure 1C), but Gap time seems to be more adequate to use as a proxy for the (imposed) time available for processing the visual cue, which drives the behavioral variance. Can the Gap time better explain some of the results? It would be important to describe how the results are different (or the same) if Gap time was used instead of PT and also discuss why the authors would prefer PT over Gap time (if that's the case).

      Thanks to Rev 1 for requesting clarification of this important point. As Rev 1 notes, PT is a derived variable, computed for each trial by subtracting the Gap interval from RT (PT=RT‒Gap). While it is true that Gap and PT are correlated (inversely), it is precisely because of the variance in RT that Gap alone is not an adequate (or certainly not the best) predictor of choice outcome. First, note that, if the Gap were fixed, there would still be variance in RT and in outcome, and any dependence of outcome on time would be explained necessarily by the PT. This is true at any Gap. So, clearly, the PT predicts outcome in a way that the Gap cannot. It is easy to see why: the Gap is the part of the RT interval during which no cue information is present, whereas the PT is the part of the same interval during which it is. Therefore, if one accepts the logical premise that the likelihood of a correct choice depends on the amount of time available to view the Cue before making that choice (i.e., the definition of PT), it follows that the relationship between PT and performance should be tighter than that between performance and Gap. And, indeed, this is the case. Mean accuracy declines systematically as a function of Gap, as expected, but its correlation with performance is much weaker than for PT.

      Rev 1’s request for a comparison of how accuracy varies as function of PT versus how it varies with Gap has appeared in earlier publications (Stanford et al., 2010; Shankar et al., 2011; Salinas et al., 2014) and we now include it here for the current dataset by adding plots of accuracy versus Gap as a new panel in Fig. 1 (Fig. 1c). That PT (not Gap) better predicts the likelihood of success on a given trial is evident in comparing the tachometric (Fig. 1b) and psychometric curves (Fig. 1c). The tachometric curves vary from chance to asymptotic performance and do so over a short range of PT (~75 ms) with well-defined inflection points identifying key transitions in performance (e.g., from guesses to increasingly informed choices). In contrast, the psychometric function plotting average accuracy versus Gap (Fig. 1c) varies much more gradually, a reduction in temporal definition attributable to the failure to account for the RT’s contribution to determining PT for each trial at a given Gap.

      (2) The authors provide a compelling account of how the urgent search task affords

      (i) more pronounced selection history effects on choice and

      (ii) dissociating the spatial and feature-based history effects by comparing their different effects on the tachometric curves. However, the authors didn't discuss the limits of their task design enough. It is a contrived task (one of the "laboratory tasks"), but the behavioral variability in this simple task is certainly remarkable. Yet, is there any conclusion we should avoid from this study? For instance, can we generalize the finding in more natural settings and say, the spatial selection history influences the choice under time pressure? I wonder whether the task is simple yet general enough to make such a conclusion.

      As Rev. 1 notes, the CO task is a laboratory task that produces large history effects. But importantly, we don't think urgency is causal or essential to the existence of such effects (this is now more explicitly stated in the first section of the Results); it is simply a powerful tool for revealing and characterizing them. As noted in the Discussion, our results are consistent with studies that, based on simpler, non-urgent tasks, demonstrated either reward-driven spatial biases or color priming effects. The CO task uses urgency to generate a psychometric function that time resolves perceptually informed from perceptually uninformed choices, and thereby provides the logical key to disambiguating the simultaneous contributions of perceptual and non-perceptual biases to performance. Such was essential to our demonstration that distinct biases act independently on the same saccade choices.

      In a natural setting, we would certainly expect the respective magnitudes of such non-volitional history-based biases to be highly context dependent, but it would be difficult, if not impossible, to discern their relative impact on natural behavior. That said, we think that the biases revealed by the CO task are exemplary of those that would manifest in natural behaviors depending on the real-world context to which such behaviors correspond. Here, it is important to emphasize that the spatial- and feature-based biases we observed were not strategic, on average neither helping nor hindering overall performance. Thus, in the real-world we might expect the expression of similar biases to be an important source of behavioral variance. These observations are now summarized in the penultimate paragraph of the Discussion.

      (3) Although the authors aimed to look at both inter- and intra-trial temporal dynamics, I'm not sure if the results reflect the true within-trial dynamics. I expected to learn more about how the spatial selection history bias develops as the Gap period progresses (as the authors mentioned in line 386, the spatial history bias must develop during the Gap interval). Does Figure 3 provide some hints in this within-trial temporal dynamics?

      Because it is based on the location of the saccadic choice(s) on previous trial(s), we might expect a signal of spatial bias to be present before and during the Gap period and perhaps even before a trial begins (i.e., intertrial interval). However, because behavioral bias is a probabilistic measure of saccade tendency, we have no way of knowing if such a signal is present during periods devoid of saccadic choices. Note that, for both monkey subjects, average RT exceeded the duration of the longest Gap employed (Fig. 1), and this means that relatively few saccades occurred prior to Cue onset. That said, it's clear in both Figs. 2, 3, and 6 that location bias is evident for saccades initiated at the transition between Gap and Cue intervals (PT=0). Anecdotally, we can report that that spatial bias is evident when we extend our analysis back further into the range of negative PTs (i.e., Gap interval), but the statistics are weak given the paucity of trials at that point. Nevertheless, this is consistent with a bias that exists from the beginning of the trial, as would be expected based on neurophysiological studies from Hikosaka's lab in a simpler but comparable spatial bias task.

      Although our data do not unequivocally identify the temporal origin of the spatial bias, they clearly show that the bias is present early (at short PTs) and diminishes rapidly as the perceptual information accrues (at long PTs). Thus, the PT-dependent temporal dynamics that are revealed clearly suggest that spatial and perceptual biases operate over different intra-trial time frames, one decreasing and the other increasing. As mentioned by Rev. 1, Fig. 3 emphasizes this dichotomy.

      (4) The monkeys show significant lapse rates (enough error trials for further analyses). Do the choices in the error trials reflect the history bias? For example, if errors are divided in terms of PTs, do the errors with short PT reflect more pronounced spatial history bias (choosing the previously selected location) compared to the errors with long PT?

      The short answer is “yes”. Errors generally show a PT-dependent influence of history bias. However, correct and error trials are the result of the same biased dynamics, and analyzing them separately post-hoc does not provide much additional insight about the history effects beyond that provided by the tachometric curves themselves.

      To see this, first consider the figure below (Author response image 1). Two tachometric curves conditioned on color history are shown (left). These are the two extreme curves plotted in Fig. 2a, which correspond to the 4S (i.e., 4 repeats of the current target color) and 4D (4 color repeats and then a switch) conditions. Each of these curves already shows the probability of making an error at each PT but, indeed, we can compare the proportions of correct and error trials at short PTs (guesses) and long PTs (informed choices). These are indicated by the bar graphs on the right. Now, the effect of a bias would be to create a difference in success rate between repetitions (4S, blue) and switches (4D, red) relative to the overall, unbiased expectation (indicated by dotted lines). For color-based history, there is no bias at short PT: the proportions of correct choices are almost exactly at the expected chance level (filled bars coincide with dotted line). In contrast, at long PTs, there is a differential effect, but it is due both to a proportion of correct trials that is higher than expected in the 4S case (filled blue bar above dotted line) and to a proportion of correct trials that is lower than expected in the 4D case (filled orange bar below dotted line). This is exactly as one would expect if the current choice was biased by target color history.

      Author response image 1.

      A similar analysis can be done for location history (Author response image 2, which shows the two extreme curves from Fig. 2e). In this case the bias is much stronger at short PTs, and the difference between repeats (4S, blue) and switches (4D, red) is largely explained by a proportion of correct choices that is much higher than expected by chance in the 4S condition (filled blue bar well above dotted line). This makes sense, because a rewarded location is likely to become the next guess, so if the target happens to appear again at that same location, the subsequent guess is more likely than chance to be correct. At longer PTs, the differential effect is smaller, as would be expected for more informed choices, but it is again driven by the 4S condition. Importantly, in the case of location the total number of S trials is much smaller than the total number of D trials (because a target-location repetition has a probability of 0.25 only), so it only makes sense to compare the proportions of correct (or error) trials, not the absolute numbers, between those conditions.

      Author response image 2.

      In summary, although it is possible to examine the separate dependencies of correct and error trials on history and PT, the distinction is not very useful. Only the frequency of errors relative to that of correct choices makes complete sense, not so much, say, the frequency of short PT errors relative to that of long PT errors.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a clear and systematic study of trial history influences on the performance of monkeys in a target selection paradigm. The primary contribution of the paper is to add a twist in which the target information is revealed after, rather than before, the cue to make a foveating eye movement. This twist results in a kind of countermanding of an earlier "uninformed" saccade plan by a new one occurring right after the visual information is provided. As with countermanding tasks in general, time now plays a key factor in the success of this task, and it is time that allows the authors to quantitatively assess the parametric influences of things like previous target location, previous target identity, and previous correctness rate on choice performance. The results are logical and consistent with the prior literature, but the authors also highlight novelties in the interpretation of prior-trial effects that they argue are enabled by the use of their paradigm.

      Strengths:

      Careful analysis of a multitude of variables influencing behavior

      Weaknesses:

      Results appear largely confirmatory.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The authors provide comprehensive accounts of the urgent search task in multiple places in the manuscript. But the description can be simpler and more consistent throughout. I found it confusing when the authors compared their task with previous search tasks used by Bichot and Schall, McPeek et al. I believe the authors wanted to explain that it is not just the urgency but the fact that the target color being randomly interleaved also contributes to the pronounced history bias in their task. I appreciate their thorough comparison with previous studies but it can be distracting or lose focus. It might read better if this statement can be expanded in the Discussion, not in the Results (lines 366-376).

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We agree that the paragraph in question was ambiguous and appeared to elaborate a Discussion point, which was not our intent. Indeed, as the reviewer noted, the main point was that the randomization of the target colors (and not urgency) is the critical aspect of the task that makes it surprisingly difficult for the monkeys. We have revised the paragraph to emphasize this conclusion and the two empirical results from our own data that support it. The agreement with prior studies, which is somewhat tangential, is now briefly mentioned at the end of the paragraph. It should now be clear that the text mainly describes current data that are relevant to the interpretation of the main results.

      (2) It's important to state that feature-based selection history bias is not merely due to the monkey's intrinsic bias to one color over the other (red vs green). The authors did a nice job controlling that, as mentioned in Methods (lines 194-196) and supplementary figure (Figure 1 - Figure Supplement 2). It would be helpful for readers to read in Results as well.

      Thank you for the suggestion. We now mention this in the second section of the Results.

      (3) D trial examples for the location history in Results can be confusing to readers (lines 407-409; left-left-right, up-up-left). The examples in Methods (lines 224-229; left-up-right, up-down-left) are better to convey the preceding (different) trials can be of any kind.

      Indeed. Both types of example are now mentioned in the Results.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I have only minor comments:

      (1) In the abstract, I'm not sure what "when combined" means in the last sentence. What is combined? Selection history and stimulus salience? If so, this is not very clear. Also, it might be nice to end the abstract on how the study addresses the three components of attention that the abstract started with in the first place (salience, task, and history). Otherwise, I spent multiple abstract reads (before even reading the rest of the paper) trying to see whether indeed the paper addresses the three components of attention that were so prominently described at the beginning of the abstract or not. And, I still could not convince myself of whether all three were addressed by the study or not (I then resorted to proceeding with a reading of the rest of the paper).

      Thanks for pointing this out. We have reworded the abstract to clarify that we are focusing on selection history, not salience or top-down attention.

      (2) Line 72: isn't stimulus location still a feature????

      Our nomenclature here is intended to be consistent with the commonly applied distinction between “spatial” and “feature” -based attention that underscores the distinct mechanistic underpinnings of “where” and “what”.

      (3) Lines 76-79: I'm very confused here. The part about "guesses can be strongly biased toward an arbitrary location early on". However, I expected the later part of the sentence to still stick to location and mention what the temporal dynamic is. Instead, it discusses perceptual bias, which I presume is the color thing. So, the net result is that I'm a bit confused about how *both* location and color behave in *both* early and late times.

      We have rewritten the end of this paragraph to clarify when and how location and feature biases manifest in behavior. It may be useful to note the following. The tachometric curve describes different types of choices distinguished by their timing, guesses at short PTs vs informed decisions at long PTs. However, this also corresponds to the degree to which perceptual information becomes available over time within a single trial. Namely, perceptual information is initially absent but arrives later on. The revised text now reflects this distinction, making the logic for the expected results clearer.

      (4) Last paragraph of the introduction (lines 80-82): it would be helpful to justify here why the psychophysics were done in monkeys in this study, instead of humans.

      We now allude to the reason these studies were done in monkeys but feel that more elaboration of this point is better left to Discussion. The Discussion now more explicitly states that the current data are closely related to neurophysiological studies of spatial attention and color priming in monkeys (beginning of 4th paragraph).

      - Line 389: this kind of formulation is much clearer to me than lines 76-79 mentioned above.

      As noted, the above-mentioned section has been revised.

      - I'm a bit confused by Figure 4 in the sense that some of the effect sizes are not too different from Figure 2, even when there are some intermediate inconsistent trials. I guess the problem is aggravated by the different axis ranges in Figures 2, and 4.

      All the 1S and 1D data points are the same in both figures, as they should, but the problem is that, otherwise, the two figures are just not comparable. Apples and oranges. To see this, note that the trends for the difference between S and D conditions should go in opposite directions as trials go further into the past, and indeed they do. In Figures 2c, f, the differences between 1S and 1D results are small, and those between 4S and 4D results are the largest because both S and D effects grow away from the average with more repetitions. In contrast, in Figure 4b-d, the differences between S and D shrink as the effect of a single trial becomes more distant (differences are largest between 1S and 1D results, smallest between 1S9x and 1D9x results). The only slightly ambiguous trend is that of Figure 2g, because the S data are more noisy. We have expanded the text surrounding Figure 4 to highlight the different expected trends for this analysis in contrast to that presented in Figure 2. This should clarify the qualitative difference between the two.

      - On a related note, it is odd that the summary figures (e.g. Figures. 2, 4, etc) are vertically aligned such that the dependent measure is on the x-axis rather than the y-axis. For example, looking at Figure 2, it would make much more sense if panels b-d and f-h were rotated by 90 deg, such that the vertical axis is indeed the low asymptote or high asymptote or RT. This would directly correlate with the same data in panels a and e in the same figure and would be much easier to follow. Then, later in the paper, Fig. 8 suddenly does the dependent measure on the y-axis, as I said. I think it can help to use similarly consistent plotting approaches across all (or most) analyses.

      We tried other formats but settled on the current one because we felt it made it (slightly) easier to compare the patterns across history conditions between any two of the 6 bar graphs in each figure (in Figs 2, 5, 6), in part because it prevents any confusion with the PT axes. As this does not make a substantial difference either way, we prefer to maintain the present arrangement. Additional labels are now included, which should make the figures a bit more friendly.

      - At the beginning of the paper, I was under the impression that this will really be a free viewing search task (e.g. Wolfe search arrays or old Nakayama search arrays), but then it became clear later that it was still an instructed task, with the only difference being that the target onset is now 4 targets. I think this distinction should be clarified very early on, in order to avoid confusion by the readers. The reason I say this is that with enforced fixation, there are other factors in this task that come into play, like the monkey's individual microsaccade rates etc, which can modulate performance since they also have a form of countermanding that is like the one imposed by the compelled saccade task. So, better alert the readers to the context of the task early on.

      Thanks. We have provided additional detail when introducing the task for the first time in the Introduction, along with a citation to an earlier publication in which the specific task is described. There should be no ambiguity now.

      Reviewing Editor Comments:

      Short Assessment:

      This important study makes compelling use of the monkey animal model to capture the long-time course over which trial history affects decision-making under time pressure, showing decisions are affected by the stimulus sequence extending back as many as four trials previously.

      Summary:

      Decision-making is variable, but how much of this variability can be accounted for by the immediate previous history is not well known. Using an "urgent" saccade, Oor et al manipulated how much time monkeys had to process evidence, and evaluated what they did when there was too little time to make an evidence-based decision. They report that the history affected performance as far back as 4 previous trials and that different aspects of the stimulus history (color and location) affected performance differently.

      Strengths:

      The key strengths of this paper are that the monkey paradigm permitted a study under highly controlled conditions with stable performance across sessions and enough trials to conduct the history analysis farther back in time than is possible with smaller data sets. While the fact that prior history affects decisions was previously known, this study provides a careful quantification of the effect -- which proves to be quite large - as well as an assessment of both location and feature histories in combination with each other. The manuscript is well-written and easy to follow.

      Weaknesses and recommendations for the authors:

      (1) The figures are lovely but could use some more text/design elements to clarify, and there is space to do so. e.g., in Figure 2, there could be titles to indicate that the top row involves the color history and the bottom row involves location history. The information is there, in the y labels of panels B and F, but it takes a while to see that.

      Done. Titles have been added to Figure 2 and several others.

      (2) Furthermore, the abbreviations 1D, 4S, etc are explained in the legend but it seems there is room to spell them out or include a graphic to indicate what they mean.

      The labels 1D, 4S, etc are difficult to spell out because each one represents multiple conditions; for instance, 2S may correspond to green-green or red-red target colors, and so on. Figure legends have been edited to more clearly indicate that S and D labels correspond to repeat and switch trials, respectively, and that the associated number indicates how far back the history goes.

      (3) The terms "low asymptote" and "high asymptote" could be indicated in a graphic of a tachymetric function, smoothing the transition to the rightmost panels. (Consider also alternative terms - perhaps "floor" and "ceiling" might be more readily understandable than asymptote to the student reader??).

      Thanks for the suggested terms, “floor” and “ceiling”, which we’ve adopted. They are indeed more natural. Figure 2a now indicates that floor and ceiling accuracies correspond to opposite ends of the PT axis.

      (4) The units for the asymptotes are not indicated - I assume these are "% correct" but that would be helpful to clarify.

      Yes. Units for floor and ceiling (and RT) are now indicated in all figures.

      (5) Figure 3 - "PT", and "1S-1D" could be spelled out, and the meaning of the two colored traces could be in the figure itself rather than only in the legend. Similar suggestions apply about labeling, abbreviations apply in subsequent figures.

      PT is now spelled out in all figures other than Figure 1, and labels for the two traces were added to Figure 3. Thanks for all the detailed suggestions.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a very well-written paper presenting interesting findings related to the recovery following the end-Permian event in continental settings, from N China. The finding is timely as the topic is actively discussed in the scientific community. The data provides additional insights into the faunal, and partly, floral global recovery following the EPE, adding to the global picture.

      Strengths:

      The conclusions are supported by an impressive amount of sedimentological and paleontological data (mainly trace fossils) and illustrations.

      We thank Reviewer #1 for the positive assessments.

      Weaknesses:

      The occurrence of MISS (Microbially Induced Sedimentary Structures) could be discussed more in detail as these provide interesting information directly linked to the delayed recovery of the biota.

      We appreciate the reviewer for highlighting this important point. In the Phanerozoic, increase of microbial abundances generally occurred with rapid warming when documented and those hyperthermal events had causal links to mass extinction in continental realms, including the Permian–Triassic mass extinction (Mays et al., 2021). Accumulations of cyanobacteria and other microbes was favored by low dissolved oxygen concentrations (Pacton et al., 2011) and the produced secondary metabolites may also be toxic to animals (Paerl and Otten, 2013). Therefore, repeated algal and bacterial blooms in the post-extinction interval could disrupt ecological stability and inhibit the restoration of ecosystems.

      So, the sentence from Lines 127–130 “The depauperate ichnofauna of the late Smithian were monospecific, representing initial recolonization of empty niches by opportunists, but the coeval thrived microbial mats indicated harsh environments, which might have inhibited the recovery of freshwater ecosystems (Tu et al., 2016; Chu et al., 2017; Mays et al., 2021).” is rephased by:

      “The depauperate ichnofauna of the late Smithian were monospecific, representing initial recolonization of empty niches by opportunists. However, recurrent occurrences of microbial induced sedimentary structures (MISS) in the Liujiagou Formation imply that depressed ecosystems persisted until the Smithian (Tu et al., 2016; Chu et al., 2017). Studies revealed that the increase in microbial abundances were generally associated with hyperthermals, which would be the principal causes for mass extinction on land (Mays et al., 2021). Accumulations of microbes were favored by low dissolved oxygen concentration condition and their secondary metabolites could also be toxic to animals (Pacton et al., 2011; Paerl and Otten, 2013). Therefore, repeated thriving of MISS during the Dienerian–Smithian disrupted ecological stability in freshwater ecosystem and delayed biotic recovery in North China.”

      References:

      Mays, C., et al. 2021. Lethal microbial blooms delayed freshwater ecosystem recovery following the end-Permian extinction. Nat. Commun. 12, 5511. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-25711-3

      Pacton, M., et al. 2011. Amorphous organic matter—Experimental data on formation and the role of microbes. Rev. Palaeobot. Palynol. 166, 253–267. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.revpalbo.2011.05.011

      Paerl, H. W. & Otten, T. G. 2013. Harmful cyanobacterial blooms: causes, consequences, and controls. Microb. Ecol. 65, 995–1010. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00248-012-0159-y

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      A rapid recovery of the ecosystems during the late Early Triassic, in the aftermath of the end-Permian mass extinction, is discussed based on different types of fossils.

      Strengths:

      The combined study of invertebrate trace fossils, tetrapod bones, and plant remains together with their stratigraphic distribution in different sections provides a convincing case to support a rapid recovery as the authors hypothesize.

      We thank Reviewer #2 for the positive comments on our work.

      Weaknesses:

      The study is based on three regions with Triassic successions from the North China block. While a first-hand study of other localities of similar age would be ideal, this is of course a difficult task. Instead, the authors provide comparisons with other worldwide regions to build their case and support the initial hypothesis.

      Globally, ichnoassemblages reported from the Lower Triassic are relatively impoverished (Guo et al., 2019). We have compiled ichnoassemblages from several continental basins before, including South Africa, Antarctica, North America, European Basin and North China (Fig. 14 in Guo et al., 2019). However, most of the Early Triassic strata lack bioturbation (e.g., Guo et al., 2019, Buatois et al., 2021). On the contrary, the coeval deposits in North China contain diverse trace fossils, making it an ideal place for ichnological investigations. Hence, this study mainly focuses on the ichnological records in North China, but we hope more work will be done in other basins. 

      References:

      Guo, W.W, et al. 2019. Secular variations of ichnofossils from the terrestrial Late Permian–Middle Triassic succession at the Shichuanhe section in Shaanxi Province, North China. Glob. Planet. Change 181, 102978. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloplacha.2019.102978

      Buatois, L.A., et al. 2021. Impact of Permian mass extinctions on continental invertebrate infauna. Terra Nova 33, 455–464. https://doi.org/10.1111/ter.12530

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Guo and colleagues features the documentation and interpretation of three successions of continental to marginal marine deposits spanning the P/T transition and their respective ichnofaunas. Based on these new data inferences concerning end-Permian mass extinction and Triassic recovery in the tropical realm are discussed.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is well-written and organized and includes a large amount of new lithological and ichnological data that illuminate ecosystem evolution in a time of large-scale transition. The lithological documentations, facies interpretations, and ichnotaxonomic assignments look okay (with a few exceptions).

      We thank Reviewer #3 for the positive assessments.

      Weaknesses:

      Some interpretations in Table 1 could be questioned: For facies association FA2 the interpretation as „terrestrial facies with periodical flooding" should be put into the right column and, given the fossil content, other interpretations, such as "marine facies" or "lagoonal environment" with some plant debris and (terrestrial) animal remains washed in, could also be possible. For FA3 the statement "bioturbation is absent" is in conflict with the next statement "strata are moderately reworked". For FA5 the observation of a "monospecific ichnoassemblage" contradicts the listing of several ichnotaxa.

      We thank the reviewer for this feedback. The “FA2: terrestrial facies with periodical flooding” has been moved to the right column. As for the interpretation of depositional environment of FA2, this interval was basically terrestrial accordingly to the well-developed paleosols (Yu et al., 2022). Meanwhile, regional geological surveys have shown a faunal transition in this interval among a series of successions, from typical marine fauna containing Lingula, Eumorphotis, etc. in the southwest to a marine bivalve-terrestrial conchostracan mixed fauna in the northeast (Yin and Lin, 1979; Chu et al., 2019). Therefore, occurrence of episodic transgressions is suggested.

      The FA3: Costal mudplain facies distributed to both the upper Sunjiagou Formation and Lower Heshang Formation (Fig S1), where the former lack bioturbation and the latter were moderately disturbed. We have stated this clearly in the table S1.

      Ichnofauna in FA5 are dominated by Skolithos, Lockeia and Gordia, with only one poorly preserved specimen of Palaeophycus, which are distributed at the Shichuanhe and Liulin sections. However, there ichnotaxa were distributed separately, characterized by low diversity (single ichnogenus) and high density. We have deleted the “monospecific ichnoassemblage” for clarity.

      References:

      Chu, D., et al. 2019, Mixed continental-marine biotas following the Permian-Triassic mass extinction in South and North China: Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology, v. 519, p. 95–107, doi:10.1016/j.palaeo.2017.10.028.

      Yu, Y., et al. 2021, Latest Permian–Early Triassic paleoclimatic reconstruction by sedimentary and isotopic analyses of paleosols from the Shichuanhe section in central North China Basin: Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology, v. 585, p. 110726, doi:10.1016/j.palaeo.2021.110726.

      Yin, H.F., Lin, H.M., 1979. Marine Triassic faunas and the geologic time from Shihchienfeng Group in the northern Weihe River Basin, Shaanxi Province. Acta Stratigr. Sin. 3, 233–241 (in Chinese).

      Concerning the structure of the manuscript, certain hypotheses related to the end-Permian mass extinction and the process of the P/T extinction and recovery, namely the existence of a long-persisting "tropic dead zone" are introduced as a foregone conclusion to which the new data seemingly shall be fit as corroborating evidence. Some of the data - e.g. the presence of a supposedly Smithian-age ichnofauna are interpreted as a fast recovery shortening the duration of the "tropic dead zone" episode - but these interpretations could also be interpreted as contradicting the idea of a "dead zone" sensu stricto in favour of a "normal" post-extinction environment with low diversity and occurrence of typical disaster taxa. Due to their large error bars the early Triassic radiometric ages did not put much of a constraint on the age determination of the earliest post-extinction ichnofaunas discussed here.

      In the first ~5 Myr of the Triassic, there is evidence for a broad equatorial belt (30°N-40°S) where marine and terrestrial animals were nearly absent (namely “equatorial tetrapod gap”; Sun et al., 2012). However, the nature, duration and range of the “equatorial tetrapod gap” remain debated. Allen et al. (2020) show poleward migrations of terrestrial tetrapods during the Late Permian to Middle Triassic, with marine reptile diversity peak still restricted to northern low latitudes. Romano et al. (2020) argued that the Early Triassic equatorial terrestrial tetrapod gap would be narrower and restricted the “death belt” between 15° N and about 31° S, while Liu et al. (2022) consider that the exact boundaries of this gap likely varied with climate change (hot phases). Moreover, duration of the gap is also questioned, it’s long-lasting (Late Permian to Middle Triassic), during Induan (Bernardi et al., 2018), or from Induan to the early Spathian (Liu et al., 2022). Regardless of these discrepancies, all the related studies show the existence of the “low latitudinal tetrapod gap”, which is mentioned as background information. On this basis, this study aims to reveal when and how terrestrial ecosystems recovered from the “tropic dead zone” from the ecological point of view, rather than tetrapods only.

      The fast recovered terrestrial ecosystems are represented by diverse traces, and concurrent tetrapods and plants found in the Heshanggou Formation. We acknowledge that the chronostratigraphy of the Lower Triassic in North China (and most of continental basins globally) are not controlled by precise ages, this formation, however, could be constrained to Spathian (or even straddle to earliest Middle Triassic), based on integrated magnetostratigraphic correlation, fossil records and geochemical data (Liu, 2018; Guo et al., 2022). The Smithian-age ichnofaunas here are not interpreted as a rapidly recovering biota, but early occurring opportunist-dominated communities that explore the empty ecospace under inhospitable environments. Our study also constrains roughly the “tropical dead zone” from Induan to late Smithian in North China (Fig. 4).

      References:

      Allen, B.J., et al. 2020. The latitudinal diversity gradient of tetrapods across the Permo-Triassic mass extinction and recovery interval. Proc Biol Sci 287, 20201125. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1125

      Bernardi, M., et al. 2018. Tetrapod distribution and temperature rise during the Permian-Triassic mass extinction. Proc Biol Sci 285, 20172331. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.2331

      Guo, W., et al. 2022. Late Permian–Middle Triassic magnetostratigraphy in North China and its implications for terrestrial-marine correlations. Earth Planet. Sci. Lett. 585, 117519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.epsl.2022.117519

      Liu, J. 2018. New progress on the correlation of Chinese terrestrial Permo-Triassic strata. Vertebrata Palasiatica, 56, 327-342. 10.19615/j.cnki.1000-3118.180709

      Liu, J., et al. 2021. Permo-Triassic tetrapods and their climate implications. Glob. Planet. Change 103618. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloplacha.2021.103618

      Romano, M., et al. 2020. Early Triassic terrestrial tetrapod fauna: a review. Earth-Sci. Rev. 210, 103331. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earscirev.2020.103331

      Sun, Y., er al. 2012. Lethally hot temperatures during the early triassic greenhouse. Science 338, 366–70. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1224126

      Considering the somewhat equivocal evidence and controversial ideas about the P/T transition, the introduction could be improved by describing how the idea of a "tropic dead zone" arose against the background of earlier ideas, alternative views, and conflicting data. In the discussion section, alternative interpretations of the extensive data presented here - e.g. proximal-distal shifts in lithofacies with respect to the sediment source, sea level changes, preservation bias, the local occurrence of hostile environments instead of a regional scale, etc. should be discussed, also to avoid the impression that the author's conclusion was driven by confirmation bias.

      As mentioned above, it’s still controversial about the nature, duration and range of the “equatorial tetrapod gap”, which primarily derived from the database (body fossils only vs. both skeletal and footprint data) and analytical methods. However, detailed discussions about these differences are beyond the scope of our study. This paper provides new evidence for the "tropical dead zone" from the ecological perspective (invertebrate ichnology, paleobotany and newly found tetrapods). Our results show that the "tropical dead zone" in North China terminated in the Smithian, followed by the reappearance of many animals in the Spathian, shedding light on the more rapidly recovering terrestrial ecosystems than previously thought.

      We have improved the Introduction section by providing a summary of the “equatorial tetrapod gap”. Lines 33-35: “A tropical “tetrapod gap”, spanning between 15°N and ~31°S, prevailed through the Early Triassic, or at least during particular intervals of intense global warming (Bernardi et al., 2018; Allen et al., 2020; Romano et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2022).” is revised to:

      “A tropical “tetrapod gap”, spanning between 15°N and ~31°S, prevailed in the Early Triassic, or at particular interval of intense global warming, even though the nature, duration and range remain debated (Bernardi et al., 2018; Allen et al., 2020; Romano et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2022).”

      In the Discussion section, Lines 180-181: “Although the specimens are not yet fully prepared for taxonomic description, they clearly show the existence of tetrapod at this level” is revised to:

      “Although the specimens are not yet fully prepared for taxonomic description, they clearly show the existence of tetrapods at this level, narrowing the “tetrapod gap” to the Spathian.”

      we also add a new paragraph from Line 208:

      “Our results also shed light on the timing of the tropical dead zone. The late Smithian-age ichnofauna, although impoverished, represents early opportunist-dominated communities that explored empty ecospace under inhospitable environments, which constrains the equatorial death belt to the late Smithian in North China.”

      Contrary to the authors' claim, Figures S7 and S8 suggest that burrow size does not vary much within the studied sections. Size decreases and increases in the Shichuanhe and Liulin sections do not contemporaneously, are usually within the error-bar range, and might be driven by ichnotaxa composition, i.e. the presence or absence of larger ichnotaxa, rather than by size changes in the same ichnotaxon (and producer group). Here the measurement data would be needed as well to check the basis of the authors' interpretations.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting this important point. We have checked the accuracy of our raw data. Both the average size of all ichnogenera and single ichnogenera do not change obviously, but increase slightly upwards in the Spathian (Figures S7c and S8). This tendency is congruent with other coeval studies in North China (e.g., Shu et al., 2018; Xing et al., 2020). The presence of larger ichnotaxa will indeed improve the average sizes of fossil-bearing horizons, however, burrows of single ichnogenera in the Spathian generally show wider size distributions than in the Smithian, which might be associated with enriched producer groups or different growth stages of the same biota.

      The asynchronous burrow size changes in the Shichuanhe and Liulin sections could be attributed to sedimentary facies. Late Permian deposits at Shichuanhe are finer than those at Linlin, which is located at the basin margin. As a result, tiny traces, like Helminthoidichnites, which were widely distributed at Shichuanhe, are absent at Linlin section. Those traces significantly reduce the average sizes in this interval, leading to inconsistent size variation patterns.

      References:

      Shu, W., et al. 2018. Limuloid trackways from Permian-Triassic continental successions of North China. Palaeogeogr. Palaeoclimatol. Palaeoecol. 508, 71–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.palaeo.2018.07.022

      Xing, Z.F., et al. 2020. Trace fossils from the Lower Triassic of North China—a potential signature of the gradual recovery of a terrestrial ecosystem. Palaeoworld 30, 95–105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.palwor.2020.06.002

      Some arthropod tracks assigned here to Kouphichnium might not represent limulid traces but other (non-marine) arthropod taxa in accordance with their occurrence in terrestrial facies/non-marine units of the succession. More generally, the ichnotaxonomy of arthropod trackways is not yet well reserved - beyond Kouphichnium and Diplichnites various similar-looking types may occur that can have a variety of distinct insect, crustacean, millipede, etc. producers (including larval stages).

      Well, individual trace-makers can produce different traces, and different organisms can make morphologically similar traces. In consideration of this, it’s hard to give a one-on-one relationship between trace fossils and their producers in most cases, especially for the invertebrates. So, Kouphichnium could be made by arthropods other than limuloidss.

      However, horseshoe crabs, originating in the early Ordovician, invaded freshwater environments twice in the Paleozoic and once in the Mesozoic (Lamsdell, 2016), and their body fossils have been found from the Early Triassic of Germany (e.g., Hauschke and Wilde, 2008) and North China (which occur with their traces; unpublished data). Accordingly, we tentatively speculate Kouphichnium found in this interval could be primarily produced by limuloids.

      References:

      Hauschke, N., Wilde, V. 2008. Limuliden aus dem Oberen Buntsandstein von Süddeutschland. Hallesches Jahrb. Für Geowiss. 30, 21–26.

      Lamsdell, J.C. 2016. Horseshoe crab phylogeny and independent colonizations of fresh water: ecological invasion as a driver for morphological innovation. Palaeontology 59, 181–194. https://doi.org/10.1111/pala.12220

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for The Authors):

      (1)  Line 112 - was identified during..; please change to ...was identified in successions of late Changsian-early Smithian age.

      Revised as suggested.

      (2)  Line 116 - change prolong to prolonged.

      Revised as suggested.

      (3) Line 121 - change ichnofaunal to ichnofauna (check the entire sentence).

      We checked the manuscript thoroughly and revised as suggested.

      (4) Figure 1 caption - check sentence starting with - Base map...(delete 'of is')

      Revised as suggested.

      (5) Line 471 - tiny instead of tinny.

      Revised as suggested.

      (6) Figure S9 - would it be possible to include this reconstruction in the main manuscript?

      We have moved the artistic illustration to the main text as Figure 5.

      (7) Add the illustrators name / or indicate if it is produced by AI.

      We have added the sentence “The artistic illustration is credited to J. Sun” at the end.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for The Authors):

      (1) Line 15 – change 252 million years ago to ca. 252 million years ago.

      Revised as suggested.

      (2) Line 18 – change low-latitude North China to low-latitude present-day North China.

      Actually, the paleolatitude of North China during the Early Triassic is about 17-18°N according to paleomagnetic results (Huang et al., 2018; Guo et al., 2022,).

      References:

      Guo, W., et al. 2022. Late Permian–Middle Triassic magnetostratigraphy in North China and its implications for terrestrial-marine correlations. Earth Planet. Sci. Lett. 585, 117519. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.epsl.2022.117519

      Huang, B., et al. 2018. Paleomagnetic constraints on the paleogeography of the east asian blocks during Late Paleozoic and Early Mesozoic times. Earth-Sci. Rev. 186, 8–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earscirev.2018.02.004

      (3) Line 25 - "possible" doesn't seem the appropriate term here for the structure of the sentence. Could it be "to make possible" that it meant? Or otherwise you could write "possibly". Please revise this.

      Revised “possible” to “possibly”.

      (4) Line 33 – change “are” to “were”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (5) Line 43 – There are other, more appropriate articles that should (also) be cited here, especially because Mujal et al. (2017) doesn't deal with the Central European Basin (so you could even remove this reference). For sure this one should be cited:

      Scholze, F., Wang, Z., Kirscher, U., Kraft, J., Schneider, J.W., Götz, A.E., Joachimski, M.M., Bachtadse, V., 2017. A multistratigraphic approach to pinpoint the Permian-Triassic boundary in continental deposits: the Zechstein–Lower Buntsandstein transition in Germany. Glob. Planet. Chang. 152, 129–151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.gloplacha.2017.03.004.

      We have replaced Mujal’s paper with Scholze et al., (2017) in the main text.

      (6) Line 46 – change “Roopnarinev et al., 2019” to “Roopnarine et al., 2019”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (7) Line 53 – Here Mujal et al. (2017) would be more appropriate, since it deals with a basin from the western peri-Tethys, also, this other article by Mujal et al. (2017) discussed the recovery in the western peri-Tethys based on tetrapod footprints:

      Mujal, E., Fortuny, J., Bolet, A., Oms, O., López, J.Á., 2017. An archosauromorph dominated ichnoassemblage in fluvial settings from the late Early Triassic of the Catalan Pyrenees (NE Iberian Peninsula). PLoS One 12 (4), e0174693. http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0174693.

      Revised as suggested.

      (8) Line 58 – change “relatively diversified trace fossils have been found during the late Early Triassic” to “because relatively diversified trace fossils have been found in upper Lower Triassic deposits”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (9) Line 58 – change “recovered” to “ecosystems recovered”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (10) Line 81 – These two paragraphs could be under a section named Geological setting or similar.

      Yes, these two paragraphs are brief introductions of the geological background of North China, so we change the section name to “Geological Settings and Methods”.

      (11) Line 99 – change “behavioural” to “behavioral”.

      Revised as suggested and check spelling throughout.

      (12) Line 103 – add “is” before adopted.

      The sentence “Tiering, referring to the life position of an animal vertically in the sediment, is divided into surficial, semi-infaunal (0–0.5 cm), shallow (0.5–6 cm), intermediate (6–12 cm) and deep infaunal tiers (> 12 cm), adopted from Minter et al. (2017).” is changed to “…, based on Minter et al. (2017).”

      (13) Line 113 –change “mainly” to “were mainly”.

      Revised as suggested

      (14) Line 116 - change prolong to prolonged.

      Revised as suggested.

      (15) Line 121 – add “preserved” before in.

      Revised as suggested.

      (16) Line 123 - change “were” to “are”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (17) Line 127 – “Kouphichnium” instead of “Kouphichnim”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (18) Line 135 – change to “Occupied by”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (19) Line 140 – change “bioturbations” to “bioturbated deposits”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (20) Line 145 – “Spathian” rather than “Spthian”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (21) Line 140 – change “displayed” to “displays”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (22) Line 160 – change “continental” to “terrestrial”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (23) Line 165 – “Marchetti” rather than “Marchettti”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (24) Line 168 – change “relationships” to “relation”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (25) Line 177 – “including” instead of “includes”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (26) Line 181 and Line 214– change “tetrapod” to “tetrapods”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (27) Line 195 and Line 218 – change “cooccurred” to “co-occurring”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (28) Line 540 – delete “herein”.

      Revised as suggested.

      (28) Line 559 – “Helminthoidichnites tenuis”, it should be in italics.

      Revised as suggested.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper is an elegant, mostly observational work, detailing observations that polysome accumulation appears to drive nucleoid splitting and segregation. Overall I think this is an insightful work with solid observations.

      Thank you for your appreciation and positive comments. In our view, an appealing aspect of this proposed biophysical mechanism for nucleoid segregation is its self-organizing nature and its ability to intrinsically couple nucleoid segregation to biomass growth, regardless of nutrient conditions.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of this paper are the careful and rigorous observational work that leads to their hypothesis. They find the accumulation of polysomes correlates with nucleoid splitting, and that the nucleoid segregation occurring right after splitting correlates with polysome segregation. These correlations are also backed up by other observations:

      (1) Faster polysome accumulation and DNA segregation at faster growth rates.

      (2) Polysome distribution negatively correlating with DNA positioning near asymmetric nucleoids.

      (3) Polysomes form in regions inaccessible to similarly sized particles.

      These above points are observational, I have no comments on these observations leading to their hypothesis.

      Thank you!

      Weaknesses:

      It is hard to state weaknesses in any of the observational findings, and furthermore, their two tests of causality, while not being completely definitive, are likely the best one could do to examine this interesting phenomenon.

      It is indeed difficult to prove causality in a definitive manner when the proposed coupling mechanism between nucleoid segregation and gene expression is self-organizing, i.e., does not involve a dedicated regulatory molecule (e.g., a protein, RNA, metabolite) that we could have eliminated through genetic engineering to establish causality. We are grateful to the reviewer for recognizing that our two causality tests are the best that can be done in this context.

      Points to consider / address:

      Notably, demonstrating causality here is very difficult (given the coupling between transcription, growth, and many other processes) but an important part of the paper. They do two experiments toward demonstrating causality that help bolster - but not prove - their hypothesis. These experiments have minor caveats, my first two points.

      (1) First, "Blocking transcription (with rifampicin) should instantly reduce the rate of polysome production to zero, causing an immediate arrest of nucleoid segregation". Here they show that adding rifampicin does indeed lead to polysome loss and an immediate halting of segregation - data that does fit their model. This is not definitive proof of causation, as rifampicin also (a) stops cell growth, and (b) stops the translation of secreted proteins. Neither of these two possibilities is ruled out fully.

      That’s correct; cell growth also stops when gene expression is inhibited, which is consistent with our model in which gene expression within the nucleoid promotes nucleoid segregation and biomass growth (i.e., cell growth), inherently coupling these two processes. This said, we understand the reviewer’s point: the rifampicin experiment doesn’t exclude the possibility that protein secretion and cell growth drive nucleoid segregation. We are assuming that the reviewer is envisioning an alternative model in which sister nucleoids would move apart because they would be attached to the membrane through coupled transcription-translation-protein secretion (transertion) and the membrane would expand between the separating nucleoids, similar to the model proposed by Jacob et al in 1963 (doi:10.1101/SQB.1963.028.01.048). There are several observations arguing against cell elongation/transertion acting a predominant mechanism of nucleoid segregation.

      (1) For this alternative mechanism to work, membrane growth must be localized at the middle of the splitting nucleoids (i.e., midcell position for slow growth and ¼ and ¾ cell positions for fast growth) to create a directional motion. To our knowledge, there is no evidence of such localized membrane incorporation. Furthermore, even if membrane growth was localized at the right places, the fluidity of the cytoplasmic membrane (PMID: 6996724, 20159151, 24735432, 27705775) would be problematic. To circumvent the membrane fluidity issue, one could potentially evoke an additional connection to the rigid peptidoglycan, but then again, peptidoglycan growth would have to be localized at the middle of the splitting nucleoid. However, peptidoglycan growth is dispersed early in the cell division cycle when the nucleoid splitting happens in fast growing cells and only appears to be zonal after the onset of cell constriction (PMID: 35705811, 36097171, 2656655).

      (2) Even if we ignore the aforementioned caveats, Paul Wiggins’s group ruled out the cell elongation/transertion model by showing that the rate of cell elongation is slower than the rate of chromosome segregation (PMID: 23775792). In our revised manuscript, we clarify this point and provide confirmatory data showing that the cell elongation rate is indeed slower than the nucleoid segregation rate (Figure 1H and Figure 1 - figure supplement 5A), indicating that it cannot be the main driver.

      (3) The asymmetries in nucleoid compaction that we described in our paper are predicted by our model. We do not see how they could be explained by cell growth or protein secretion.

      (4) We also show that polysome accumulation at ectopic sites (outside the nucleoid) results in correlated nucleoid dynamics, consistent with our proposed mechanism. It is not clear to us how such nucleoid dynamics could be explained by cell growth or protein secretion (transertion).

      (1a) As rifampicin also stops all translation, it also stops translational insertion of membrane proteins, which in many old models has been put forward as a possible driver of nucleoid segregation, and perhaps independent of growth. This should at last be mentioned in the discussion, or if there are past experiments that rule this out it would be great to note them.

      It is not clear to us how the attachment of the DNA to the cytoplasmic membrane could alone create a directional force to move the sister nucleoids. We agree that old models have proposed a role for cell elongation (providing the force) and transertion (providing the membrane tether). Please see our response above for the evidence (from the literature and our work) against it. This was mentioned in the Introduction and Results section, but we agree that this was not well explained. We have now put emphasis on the related experimental data (Figure 1H, Figure 1 – figure supplement 5A, ) and revised the text (lines 199 - 210) to clarify these points.

      (1b) They address at great length in the discussion the possibility that growth may play a role in nucleoid segregation. However, this is testable - by stopping surface growth with antibiotics. Cells should still accumulate polysomes for some time, it would be easy to see if nucleoids are still segregated, and to what extent, thereby possibly decoupling growth and polysome production. If successful, this or similar experiments would further validate their model.

      We reviewed the literature and could not find a drug that stops cell growth without stopping gene expression. Any drug that affects the integrity or potential of the membrane depletes cells of ATP; without ATP, gene expression is inhibited. However, our experiment in which we drive polysome accumulation at ectopic sites decouples polysome accumulation from cell growth. In this experiment, by redirecting most of chromosome gene expression to a single plasmid-encoded gene, we reduce the rate of cell growth but still create a large accumulation of polysomes at an ectopic location. This ectopic polysome accumulation is sufficient to affect nucleoid dynamics in a correlated fashion. In the revised manuscript, we have clarified this point and added model simulations (Figure 7 – figure supplement 2) to show that our experimental observations are predicted by our model.

      (2) In the second experiment, they express excess TagBFP2 to delocalize polysomes from midcell. Here they again see the anticorrelation of the nucleoid and the polysomes, and in some cells, it appears similar to normal (polysomes separating the nucleoid) whereas in others the nucleoid has not separated. The one concern about this data - and the differences between the "separated" and "non-separated" nuclei - is that the over-expression of TagBFP2 has a huge impact on growth, which may also have an indirect effect on DNA replication and termination in some of these cells. Could the authors demonstrate these cells contain 2 fully replicated DNA molecules that are able to segregate?

      We have included new flow cytometry data of fluorescently labeled DNA to show that DNA replication is not impacted.

      (3) What is not clearly stated and is needed in this paper is to explain how polysomes do (or could) "exert force" in this system to segregate the nucleoid: what a "compaction force" is by definition, and what mechanisms causes this to arise (what causes the "force") as the "compaction force" arises from new polysomes being added into the gaps between them caused by thermal motions.

      They state, "polysomes exert an effective force", and they note their model requires "steric effects (repulsion) between DNA and polysomes" for the polysomes to segregate, which makes sense. But this makes it unclear to the reader what is giving the force. As written, it is unclear if (a) these repulsions alone are making the force, or (b) is it the accumulation of new polysomes in the center by adding more "repulsive" material, the force causes the nucleoids to move. If polysomes are concentrated more between nucleoids, and the polysome concentration does not increase, the DNA will not be driven apart (as in the first case) However, in the second case (which seems to be their model), the addition of new material (new polysomes) into a sterically crowded space is not exerting force - it is filling in the gaps between the molecules in that region, space that needs to arise somehow (like via Brownian motion). In other words, if the polysome region is crowded with polysomes, space must be made between these polysomes for new polysomes to be inserted, and this space must be made by thermal (or ATP-driven) fluctuations of the molecules. Thus, if polysome accumulation drives the DNA segregation, it is not "exerting force", but rather the addition of new polysomes is iteratively rectifying gaps being made by Brownian motion.

      We apologize for the understandable confusion. In our picture, the polysomes and DNA (conceptually considered as small plectonemic segments) basically behave as dissolved particles. If these particles were noninteracting, they would simply mix. However, both polysomes and DNA segments are large enough to interact sterically. So as density increases, steric avoidance implies a reduced conformational entropy and thus a higher free energy per particle. We argue (based on Miangolarra et al. 2021 PMID: 34675077 and Xiang et al. 2021 PMID: 34186018) that the demixing of polysomes and DNA segments occurs because DNA segments pack better with each other than they do with polysomes. This raises the free energy cost associated with DNA-polysome interactions compared to DNA-DNA interactions. We model this effect by introducing a term in the free energy χ_np, which refers to as a repulsion between DNA and polysomes, though as explained above it arises from entropic effects. At realistic cellular densities of DNA and polysomes, this repulsive interaction is strong enough to cause the DNA and polysomes to phase separate.

      This same density-dependent free energy that causes phase separation can also give rise to forces, just in the way that a higher pressure on one side of a wall can give rise to a net force on the wall. Indeed, the “compaction force” we refer to is fundamentally an osmotic pressure difference. At some stages during nucleoid segregation, the region of the cell between nucleoids has a higher polysome concentration, and therefore a higher osmotic pressure, than the regions near the poles. This results in a net poleward force on the sister nucleoids that drives their migration toward the poles. This migration continues until the osmotic pressure equilibrates. Therefore, both phase separation (due to the steric repulsion described above) and nonequilibrium polysome production and degradation (which creates the initial accumulation of polysomes around midcell) are essential ingredients for nucleoid segregation.

      This has been clarified in the revised text, with the support of additional simulation results showing how the asymmetry in polysome distribution causes a compaction force (Figure 4A).

      The authors use polysome accumulation and phase separation to describe what is driving nucleoid segregation. Both terms are accurate, but it might help the less physically inclined reader to have one term, or have what each of these means explicitly defined at the start. I say this most especially in terms of "phase separation", as the currently huge momentum toward liquid-liquid interactions in biology causes the phrase "phase separation" to often evoke a number of wider (and less defined) phenomena and ideas that may not apply here. Thus, a simple clear definition at the start might help some readers.

      In our case, phase separation means that the DNA-polysome steric repulsion is strong enough to drive their demixing, which creates a compact nucleoid. As mentioned in a previous point, this effect is captured in the free energy by the χ_np term, which is an effective repulsion between DNA and polysomes, though it arises from entropic effects.

      In the revised manuscript, we now illustrate this with our theoretical model by initializing a cell with a diffuse nucleoid and low polysome concentration. For the sake of simplicity, we assume that the cell does not elongate. We observe that the DNA-polysome steric repulsion is sufficient to compact the nucleoid and place it at mid-cell (new Figure 4A).

      (4) Line 478. "Altogether, these results support the notion that ectopic polysome accumulation drives nucleoid dynamics". Is this right? Should it not read "results support the notion that ectopic polysome accumulation inhibits/redirects nucleoid dynamics"?

      We think that the ectopic polysome accumulation drives nucleoid dynamics. In our theoretical model, we can introduce polysome production at fixed sources to mimic the experiments where ectopic polysome production is achieved by high plasmid expression. The model is able to recapitulate the two main phenotypes observed in experiments (Figure 7). These new simulation results have been added to the revised manuscript (Figure 7 – figure supplement 2).

      (5) It would be helpful to clarify what happens as the RplA-GFP signal decreases at midcell in Figure 1- is the signal then increasing in the less "dense" parts of the cell? That is, (a) are the polysomes at midcell redistributing throughout the cell? (b) is the total concentration of polysomes in the entire cell increasing over time?

      It is a redistribution—the RplA-GFP signal remains constant in concentration from cell birth to division (Figure 1 – Figure Supplement 1E). This is now clarified in the revised text.

      (6) Line 154. "Cell constriction contributed to the apparent depletion of ribosomal signal from the mid-cell region at the end of the cell division cycle (Figure 1B-C and Movie S1)" - It would be helpful if when cell constriction began and ended was indicated in Figures 1B and C.

      Good idea. We have added markers in Figure 1C to indicate the average start of cell constriction. This relative time from birth to division was estimated as described in the new Figure 1 – figure supplement 2. We have also indicated that cell birth and division correspond to the first and last images/timepoint in Figure 1B and C, respectively. The two-imensional average cell projections presented in Figure 3D also indicate the average timing of cell constriction, consistent with our analysis in Figure 1 – figure supplement 2.

      (7) In Figure 7 they demonstrate that radial confinement is needed for longitudinal nucleoid segregation. It should be noted (and cited) that past experiments of Bacillus l-forms in microfluidic channels showed a clear requirement role for rod shape (and a given width) in the positing and the spacing of the nucleoids.

      Wu et al, Nature Communications, 2020. "Geometric principles underlying the proliferation of a model cell system" https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-17988-7

      Good point! We have revised the text to mention this work. Thank you.

      (8) "The correlated variability in polysome and nucleoid patterning across cells suggests that the size of the polysome-depleted spaces helps determine where the chromosomal DNA is most concentrated along the cell length. This patterning is likely reinforced through the displacement of the polysomes away from the DNA dense region"

      It should be noted this likely functions not just in one direction (polysomes dictating DNA location), but also in the reverse - as the footprint of compacted DNA should also exclude (and thus affect) the location of polysomes

      We agree that the effects could go both ways at this early stage of the story. We have revised the text accordingly.

      (9) Line 159. Rifampicin is a transcription inhibitor that causes polysome depletion over time. This indicates that all ribosomal enrichments consist of polysomes and therefore will be referred to as polysome accumulations hereafter". Here and throughout this paper they use the term polysome, but cells also have monosomes (and 2 somes, etc). Rifampicin stops the assembly of all of these, and thus the loss of localization could occur from both. Thus, is it accurate to state that all transcription events occur in polysomes? Or are they grouping all of the n-somes into one group?

      In the original discussion, we noted that our term “polysomes” also includes monosomes for simplicity, but we agree that the term should have been defined much earlier. The manuscript has been revised accordingly. Furthermore, in the revised manuscript, we have included additional simulation results with three different diffusion coefficients that reflect different polysome sizes to show that different polysome species with less or more ribosomes give similar results (Figure 4 – figure supplement 4). This shows that the average polysome description in our model is sufficient.

      Thank you for the valuable comments and suggestions!

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors perform a remarkably comprehensive, rigorous, and extensive investigation into the spatiotemporal dynamics between ribosomal accumulation, nucleoid segregation, and cell division. Using detailed experimental characterization and rigorous physical models, they offer a compelling argument that nucleoid segregation rates are determined at least in part by the accumulation of ribosomes in the center of the cell, exerting a steric force to drive nucleoid segregation prior to cell division. This evolutionarily ingenious mechanism means cells can rely on ribosomal biogenesis as the sole determinant for the growth rate and cell division rate, avoiding the need for two separate 'sensors,' which would require careful coupling.

      Terrific summary! Thank you for your positive assessment.

      Strengths:

      In terms of strengths; the paper is very well written, the data are of extremely high quality, and the work is of fundamental importance to the field of cell growth and division. This is an important and innovative discovery enabled through a combination of rigorous experimental work and innovative conceptual, statistical, and physical modeling.

      Thank you!

      Weaknesses:

      In terms of weaknesses, I have three specific thoughts.

      Firstly, my biggest question (and this may or may not be a bona fide weakness) is how unambiguously the authors can be sure their ribosomal labeling is reporting on polysomes, specifically. My reading of the work is that the loss of spatial density upon rifampicin treatment is used to infer that spatial density corresponds to polysomes, yet this feels like a relatively indirect way to get at this question, given rifampicin targets RNA polymerase and not translation. It would be good if a more direct way to confirm polysome dependence were possible.

      The heterogeneity of ribosome distribution inside E. coli cells has been attributed to polysomes by many labs (PMID: 25056965, 38678067, 22624875, 31150626, 34186018, 10675340). The attribution is also consistent with single-molecule tracking experiments showing that slow-moving ribosomes (polysomes) are excluded by the nucleoid whereas fast-diffusing ribosomes (free ribosomal subunits) are distributed throughout the cytoplasm (PMID: 25056965, 22624875). These points are now mentioned in the revised manuscript.

      Second, the authors invoke a phase separation model to explain the data, yet it is unclear whether there is any particular evidence supporting such a model, whether they can exclude simpler models of entanglement/local diffusion (and/or perhaps this is what is meant by phase separation?) and it's not clear if claiming phase separation offers any additional insight/predictive power/utility. I am OK with this being proposed as a hypothesis/idea/working model, and I agree the model is consistent with the data, BUT I also feel other models are consistent with the data. I also very much do not think that this specific aspect of the paper has any bearing on the paper's impact and importance.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment, but the output of our reaction-diffusion model is a bona fide phase separation (spinodal decomposition). So, we feel that we need to use the term when reporting the modeling results. Inside the cell, the situation is more complicated. As the reviewer points out, there are likely entanglements (not considered in our model) and other important factors (please see our discussion on the model limitations). This said, we have revised our text to clarify our terms and proposed mechanism.

      Finally, the writing and the figures are of extremely high quality, but the sheer volume of data here is potentially overwhelming. I wonder if there is any way for the authors to consider stripping down the text/figures to streamline things a bit? I also think it would be useful to include visually consistent schematics of the question/hypothesis/idea each of the figures is addressing to help keep readers on the same page as to what is going on in each figure. Again, there was no figure or section I felt was particularly unclear, but the sheer volume of text/data made reading this quite the mental endurance sport! I am completely guilty of this myself, so I don't think I have any super strong suggestions for how to fix this, but just something to consider.

      We agree that there is a lot to digest. We could not come up with great ideas for visuals others than the schematics we already provide. However, we have revised the text to clarify our points and added a simulation result (Figure 4A) to help explain biophysical concepts.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Papagiannakis et al. present a detailed study exploring the relationship between DNA/polysome phase separation and nucleoid segregation in Escherichia coli. Using a combination of experiments and modelling, the authors aim to link physical principles with biological processes to better understand nucleoid organisation and segregation during cell growth.

      Strengths:

      The authors have conducted a large number of experiments under different growth conditions and physiological perturbations (using antibiotics) to analyse the biophysical factors underlying the spatial organisation of nucleoids within growing E. coli cells. A simple model of ribosome-nucleoid segregation has been developed to explain the observations.

      Weaknesses:

      While the study addresses an important topic, several aspects of the modelling, assumptions, and claims warrant further consideration.

      Thank you for your feedback. Please see below for a response to each concern.

      Major Concerns:

      Oversimplification of Modelling Assumptions:

      The model simplifies nucleoid organisation by focusing on the axial (long-axis) dimension of the cell while neglecting the radial dimension (cell width). While this approach simplifies the model, it fails to explain key experimental observations, such as:

      (1) Inconsistencies with Experimental Evidence:

      The simplified model presented in this study predicts that translation-inhibiting drugs like chloramphenicol would maintain separated nucleoids due to increased polysome fractions. However, experimental evidence shows the opposite-separated nucleoids condense into a single lobe post-treatment (Bakshi et al 2014), indicating limitations in the model's assumptions/predictions. For the nucleoids to coalesce into a single lobe, polysomes must cross the nucleoid zones via the radial shells around the nucleoid lobes.

      We do not think that the results from chloramphenicol-treated cells are inconsistent with our model. Our proposed mechanism predicts that nucleoids will condense in the presence of chloramphenicol, consistent with experiments. It also predicts that nucleoids that were still relatively close at the time of chloramphenicol treatment could fuse if they eventually touched through diffusion (thermal fluctuation) to reduce their interaction with the polysomes and minimize their conformational energy. Fusion is, however, not expected for well-separated nucleoids since their diffusion is slow in the crowded cytoplasm. This is consistent with our experimental observations: In the presence of a growth-inhibitory concentration of chloramphenicol (70 μg/mL), nucleoids in relatively close proximity can fuse, but well-separated nucleoids condense and do not fuse. Since the growth rate inhibition is not immediate upon chloramphenicol treatment, many cells with well-separated condensed nucleoids divide during the first hour. As a result, the non-fusion phenotype is more obvious in non-dividing cells, achieved by pre-treating cells with the cell division inhibitor cephalexin (50μg/mL). In these polyploid elongated cells, well-separated nucleoids condensed but did not fuse, not even after an hour in the presence of chloramphenicol. We have revised the manuscript to add these data (illustrative images + a quantitative analysis) in Figure 4 – figure supplement 1.

      (2) The peripheral localisation of nucleoids observed after A22 treatment in this study and others (e.g., Japaridze et al., 2020; Wu et al., 2019), which conflicts with the model's assumptions and predictions. The assumption of radial confinement would predict nucleoids to fill up the volume or ribosomes to go near the cell wall, not the nucleoid, as seen in the data.

      The reviewer makes a good point that DNA attachment to the membrane through transertion could contribute to the nucleoid being peripherally localized in A22 cells. We have revised the text to add this point. However, we do not think that this contradicts the proposed nucleoid segregation mechanism described in our model. On the contrary, by attaching the nucleoid to the cytoplasmic membrane along the cell width, transertion might help reduce the diffusion and thus exchange of polysomes across nucleoids. We have revised the text to discuss transertion over radial confinement.

      (3) The radial compaction of the nucleoid upon rifampicin or chloramphenicol treatment, as reported by Bakshi et al. (2014) and Spahn et al. (2023), also contradicts the model's predictions. This is not expected if the nucleoid is already radially confined.

      We originally evoked radial confinement to explain the observation that polysome accumulations do not equilibrate between DNA-free regions. We agree that transertion is an alternative explanation. Thank you for bringing it to our attention. However, please note that this does not contradict the model. In our view, it actually supports the 1D model by providing a reasonable explanation for the slow exchange of polysomes across DNA-free regions. The attachment of the nucleoid to the membrane along the cell width may act as diffusion barrier. We have revised the text and the title of the manuscript accordingly.

      (4) Radial Distribution of Nucleoid and Ribosomal Shell:

      The study does not account for well-documented features such as the membrane attachment of chromosomes and the ribosomal shell surrounding the nucleoid, observed in super-resolution studies (Bakshi et al., 2012; Sanamrad et al., 2014). These features are critical for understanding nucleoid dynamics, particularly under conditions of transcription-translation coupling or drug-induced detachment. Work by Yongren et al. (2014) has also shown that the radial organisation of the nucleoid is highly sensitive to growth and the multifork nature of DNA replication in bacteria.

      We have revised the manuscript to discuss the membrane attachment. Please see the previous response.

      The omission of organisation in the radial dimension and the entropic effects it entails, such as ribosome localisation near the membrane and nucleoid centralisation in expanded cells, undermines the model's explanatory power and predictive ability. Some observations have been previously explained by the membrane attachment of nucleoids (a hypothesis proposed by Rabinovitch et al., 2003, and supported by experiments from Bakshi et al., 2014, and recent super-resolution measurements by Spahn et al.).

      We agree—we have revised the text to discuss membrane attachment in the radial dimension. See previous responses.

      Ignoring the radial dimension and membrane attachment of nucleoid (which might coordinate cell growth with nucleoid expansion and segregation) presents a simplistic but potentially misleading picture of the underlying factors.

      Please see above.

      This reviewer suggests that the authors consider an alternative mechanism, supported by strong experimental evidence, as a potential explanation for the observed phenomena:

      Nucleoids may transiently attach to the cell membrane, possibly through transertion, allowing for coordinated increases in nucleoid volume and length alongside cell growth and DNA replication. Polysomes likely occupy cellular spaces devoid of the nucleoid, contributing to nucleoid compaction due to mutual exclusion effects. After the nucleoids separate following ter separation, axial expansion of the cell membrane could lead to their spatial separation.

      This “membrane attachment/cell elongation” model is reminiscent to the hypothesis proposed by Jacob et al in 1963 (doi:10.1101/SQB.1963.028.01.048). There are several lines of evidence arguing against it as the major driver of nucleoid segregation:

      (Below is a slightly modified version of our response to a comment from Reviewer 1—see page 3)

      (1) For this alternative model to work, axial membrane expansion (i.e., cell elongation) would have to be localized at the middle of the splitting nucleoids (i.e., midcell position for slow growth and ¼ and ¾ cell positions for fast growth) to create a directional motion. To our knowledge, there is no evidence of such localized membrane incorporation. Furthermore, even if membrane growth was localized at the right places, the fluidity of the cytoplasmic membrane (PMID: 6996724, 20159151, 24735432, 27705775) would be problematic. To go around this fluidity issue, one could potentially evoke a potential connection to the rigid peptidoglycan, but then again, peptidoglycan growth would have to be localized at the middle of the splitting nucleoid to “push” the sister nucleoid apart from each other. However, peptidoglycan growth is dispersed prior to cell constriction (PMID: 35705811, 36097171, 2656655).

      (2) Even if we ignore the aforementioned caveats, Paul Wiggins’s group ruled out the cell elongation/transertion model by showing that the rate of cell elongation is slower than the rate of chromosome segregation (PMID: 23775792). In the revised manuscript, we confirm that the cell elongation rate is indeed overall slower than the nucleoid segregation rate (see Figure 1 - figure supplement 5A where the subtraction of the cell elongation rate to the nucleoid segregation rate at the single-cell level leads to positive values).

      (3) Furthermore, our correlation analysis comparing the rate of nucleoid segregation to the rate of either cell elongation or polysome accumulation argues that polysome accumulation plays a larger role than cell elongation in nucleoid segregation. These data were already shown in the original manuscript (Figure 1I and Figure 1 – figure supplement 5B) but were not highlighted in this context. We have revised the text to clarify this point.

      (4) The membrane attachment/cell elongation model does not explain the nucleoid asymmetries described in our paper (Figure 3), whereas they can be recapitulated by our model.

      (5) The cell elongation/transertion model cannot predict the aberrant nucleoid dynamics observed when chromosomal expression is largely redirected to plasmid expression (Figure 7). In the revised manuscript, we have added simulation results showing that these nucleoid dynamics are predicted by our model (Figure 7 – figure supplement 2).

      Based on these arguments, we do not believe that a mechanism based on membrane attachment and cell elongation is the major driver of nucleoid segregations. However, we do believe that it may play a complementary role (see “Nucleoid segregation likely involves multiple factors” in the Discussion). We have revised the text to clarify our thoughts and mention the potential role of transertion.

      Incorporating this perspective into the discussion or future iterations of the model may provide a more comprehensive framework that aligns with the experimental observations in this study and previous work.

      As noted above, we have revised the text to mention transertion.

      Simplification of Ribosome States:

      Combining monomeric and translating ribosomes into a single 'polysome' category may overlook spatial variations in these states, particularly during ribosome accumulation at the mid-cell. Without validating uniform mRNA distribution or conducting experimental controls such as FRAP or single-molecule measurements to estimate the proportions of ribosome states based on diffusion, this assumption remains speculative.

      Indeed, for simplicity, we adopt an average description of all polysomes with an average diffusion coefficient and interaction parameters, which is sufficient for capturing the fundamental mechanism underlying nucleoid segregation. To illustrate that considering multiple polysome species does not change the physical picture, we have considered an extension of our model, which contains three polysome species, each with a different diffusion coefficient (D<sub>P</sub> = 0.018, 0.023, or 0.028 μm<sup>2</sup>/s), reflecting that polysomes with more ribosomes will have a lower diffusion coefficient. Simulation of this model reveals that the different polysome species have essentially the same concentration distribution, suggesting that the average description in our minimal model is sufficient for our purposes. We present these new simulation results in Figure 4 – figure supplement 4 of the revised manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Does the polysome density correlate with the origins? If the majority of ribosomal genes are expressed near the origins,

      This is indeed an interesting point that we mention in the discussion. The fact that the chromosomal origin is surrounded by highly expressed genes (PMID: 30904377) and is located near the middle of the nucleoid prior to DNA replication (PMID: 15960977, 27332118, 34385314, 37980336) can only help the model that we propose by increasing the polysome density at the mid-nucleoid position.

      (2) Red lines in 3C are hard to resolve - can the authors make them darker?

      Absolutely. Sorry about that.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors use rifampicin treatment as a mechanism to trigger polysome disassembly and show this leads to homogenous RplA distribution. This is a really important experiment as it is used to link RplA localization to polysomes, and tp argue that RplA density is reporting on polysomes. Given rifampicin inhibits RNA polymerase, and given the only reference of the three linking rifampicin to polysome disassembly is the 1971 Blundell and Wild ref), it would perhaps be useful to more conclusively show that polysome depletion (as opposed to inhibition of mRNA synthesis, which is upstream of polysome assembly) by using an alternative compound more commonly linked to polysome disassembly (e.g., puromycin) and show timelapse loss of density as a function of treatment time. This is not a required experiment, but given the idea that RplA density reports on polysomes is central to the authors' interpretation, it feels like this would be a thing worth being certain of. An alternative model is that ribosomes undergo self-assembly into local storage depots when not being used, but those depots are not translationally active/lack polysomes. I don't know if I think this is likely, but I'm not convinced the rifampicin treatment + waiting for a relatively long period of time unambiguously excludes other possible mechanisms given the large scale remodeling of the intracellular environment upon mRNA inhibition. I 100% buy the relationship between ribosomal distribution and nucleoid segregation (and the ectopic expression experiments are amazing in this regard), so my own pause for thought here is "do we know those ribosomes are in polysomes in the ribosome-dense regions". I'm not sure the answer to this question has any bearing on the impact and importance of this work (in my mind, it doesn't, but perhaps there's a reason it does?). The way to unambiguously show this would really be to do CryoET and show polysomes in the dense ribosomal regions, but I would never suggest the authors do that here (that's an entire other paper!).

      We agree that mRNAs play a role, as mRNAs are major components of polysomes and most mRNAs are expected to be in the form of polysomes (i.e., in complex with ribosomes). In addition, as mentioned above, the enrichments of ribosome distribution are known to be associated with polysomes (PMID: 25056965, 38678067, 22624875, 31150626, 34186018, 10675340). The attribution is consistent with single-molecule tracking experiments showing that slow-moving ribosomes (polysomes) are excluded by the nucleoid whereas fast-diffusing ribosomes (free ribosomal subunits) are distributed throughout the cytoplasm (PMID: 25056965, 22624875). This is also consistent with cryo-ET results that we actually published (see Figure S5, PMID: 34186018). We have added this information to the revised manuscript. Thank you for alerting us of this oversight.

      On line 320 the authors state "Our single-cell studies provided experimental support that phase separation between polysomes and DNA contributes to nucleoid segregation." - this comes pretty out of left field? I didn't see any discussion of this hypothesis leading up to this sentence, nor is there evidence I can see that necessitates phase separation as a mechanistic explanation unless we are simply using phase separation to mean cellular regions with distinct cellular properties (which I would advise against). If the authors really want to pursue this model I think much more support needs to be provided here, including (1) defining what the different phases are, (2) providing explicit description of what the attractive/repulsive determinants of these different phases could be/are, and (3) ruling out a model where the behavior observed is driven by a combination of DNA / polysome entanglement + steric exclusion; if this is actually the model, then being much more explicit about this being a locally arrested percolation phenomenon would be essential. Overall, however, I would probably dissuade the authors from pursuing the specific underlying physics of what drives the effects they're seeing in a Results section, solely because I think ruling in/out a model unambiguously is very difficult. Instead, this would be a useful topic for a Discussion, especially couched under a "our data are consistent with..." if they cannot exclude other models (which I think is unreasonably difficult to do).

      Thank you for your advice. We have revised the text to more carefully choose our words and define our terms.

      Minor comments:

      The results in "Cell elongation may also contribute to sister nucleoid migration near the end of the division cycle" are really interesting, but this section is one big paragraph, and I might encourage the authors to divide this paragraph up to help the reader parse this complex (and fascinating) set of results!

      We have revised this section to hopefully make it more accessible.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Technical Controls:

      The authors should conduct a photobleaching control to confirm that the perceived 'higher' brightness of new ribosomes at the mid-cell position is not an artefact caused by older ribosomes being photobleached during the imaging process. Comparing results at various imaging frequencies and intensities is necessary to address this issue.

      The ribosome localization data across 30 nutrient conditions (Figure 2, Figure 1 – figure supplement 6, Figure 2 – Figure supplement 1, Figure 2 – Figure supplement 3 and Figure 5) are from snapshot images, which do not have any photobleaching issue. They confirm the mid-cell accumulation seen by time-lapse microscopy. We have revised the text to clarify this point.

      Novelty of Experimental Measurements:

      While the scale of the study is unprecedented, claims of novelty (e.g., line 142) regarding ribosome-nucleoid segregation tracking are overstated. Similar observations have been made previously (e.g., Bakshi et al., 2012; Bakshi et al., 2014; Chai et al., 2014).

      Our apologies. The text in line 142 oversimplified our rationale. This has been corrected in the revised manuscript.

    1. Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript reports experiments designed to dissect the function of N-cadherin during mammalian folliculogenesis, using the mouse as a model system. Prior studies have shown that this is the principal cadherin expressed by the follicular granulosa cells. Two main strategies are used - small-molecule inhibitors that target N-cadherin and a conditional knockout where the gene encoding N-cad is deleted in granulosa cells. The authors also take advantage of the ability to reproduce key events of folliculogenesis, such as oocyte meiotic maturation, in vitro. Four main conclusions are drawn from the studies: (i) cadherin-based cell contact is required to maintain cadherin (N-cad in the granulosa cells; E-cad in the oocyte) at the plasma membrane; (ii) N-cad is required for cumulus layer expansion; (iii) N-cad is required for meiotic maturation of the oocyte; (iv) N-cad is required for ovulation.

      Strengths:

      The experiments are logically conceived, clearly described and presented, and carefully interpreted. A key strength of the paper is that multiple approaches have been used (drugs, knockouts, immunofluorescence, PLA, in vitro and in vivo studies). Taken together, they clearly establish essential roles for N-cadherin during folliculogenesis.

      It is intriguing that, when cadherin activity is impaired, the cadherins are lost from the plasma membrane. This suggests that, in a multicellular context, interactions with other cadherins, either in cis within the same cell or in trans with a neighboring cell, are required to maintain cadherins at the membrane. Hence, beyond their significance for understanding female reproductive biology, these experiments have broader implications for cell biology.

      Weaknesses:

      A few points could be considered or clarified by the authors:

      The YAP experiments were confusing to the reviewer. CRS-066 increased YAP activity, as indicated by increased expression of target genes. Since CRS-066 prevents expansion, this result suggests that YAP antagonizes expansion. Therefore, blocking YAP should favor expansion. Yet, the YAP inhibitor impaired expansion. In the reviewer's eyes, these results seem to be contradictory.

      The mechanism through which N-cadherin inhibitors block cumulus expansion isn’t fully elucidated but isn’t deemed to be through YAP alone. The transcriptional changes indicate crosstalk between N-cadherin, β-catenin and Hippo/YAP pathways, as well as impacting on the signalling between cumulus cells and the oocyte.

      It is intriguing that the inhibitors were able to efficiently block oocyte maturation. Oocytes from which the cumulus granulosa cells have been removed (denuded) will mature in vitro in the absence of LH or EGF. Since the effect of the inhibitors is to break the contact between the cumulus cells and oocyte, one might have predicted that this would not impair the ability of the oocytes to mature. Perhaps the authors could comment on this.

      Indeed, removal of cumulus cells permits oocyte meiotic maturation by reducing oocyte cAMP, leading to activation of meiosis promoting factor (MPF). A hypothesis would be that cyclic nucleotides and MPF arrest in the oocyte are maintained when N-cadherin contacts are blocked but this was not determined.

      Regarding the experiments where the inhibitors were administered intra-peritoneally, the authors might comment on the rationale for choosing the doses that were used. An additional point to consider is that, since N-cadherin is expressed in a variety of tissues, an effect of interfering with N-cadherin at these non-ovarian sites could indirectly influence ovarian function.

      Doses were chosen based on previous reported use of these inhibitors in vivo (Mrozik et al. 2020). Possible effects of the N-cadherin antagonists in other tissues was a carefully considered in this and the previous Mrozik et al study. While we saw no evidence of effects in gross morphological observations, or closer examination of vasculature or blood in these studies, this potential is not excluded.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript entitled "N-cadherin mechanosensing in ovarian follicles controls oocyte maturation and ovulation" aimed to investigate the role of N-cadherin in different ovarian physiological processes, including cumulus oocyte expansion, oocyte maturation, and ovulation. The authors performed several in vitro and in vivo mice experiments, using diverse techniques to reinforce their results.

      First, they identified two compounds (N-cadherin antagonists) that block the adhesion of periovulatory COCs to fibronectin through screening a small molecule library, using the xCELLigenceTM system, performing proper and complementary controls. Second, the authors showed the presence of N-cadherin adherens junctions between granulosa cells and cumulus cells and at the interface of cumulus cell transzonal projections and the oocyte throughout folliculogenesis. And that these adherens complexes between cumulus cells and oocytes were disrupted when inhibited N-cadherin, as observed by nice representative confocal images. Then, the authors assessed COC expansion and oocyte meiotic maturation to determine whether the loss of oocyte membrane β-catenin and E-cadherin upon N-cadherin inhibitor treatment disrupts the bi-directional communication between cumulus cells and the oocyte. Indeed, N-cadherin antagonists disrupted both processes (cumulus expansion and oocyte meiotic). However, the expression of known mediators of COC expansion (E.g., Areg and Ptgs2) were either increased or unaffected. Nevertheless, RNA-Seq showed consistent effects on cell signaling mRNA genes by the antagonist CRS-066.

      In vivo studies using mice were also achieved using stimulated protocols (together with one of the antagonists or vehicle) or granulosa-specific Cdh2 Knockouts to further analyze the role of N-cadherin. N-cadherin antagonist CRS-066 (but not LCRF-0006) significantly reduced mouse ovulation compared to controls. RNA-sequencing data analysis identified distinct gene expression profiles in CRS-066 treated compared to control ovaries. Ovulation in CdhFl/FL; Amhr2Cre mice after stimulation were also significantly reduced; multiple large unruptured follicles were observed in these granulosa-specific Cdh2 mutant ovaries, and the mRNA expression of Areg and Ptgs2 were reduced.

      The authors conclude that their study identified N-cadherin as a mechanosensory regulator important in ovarian granulosa cell differentiation able to respond to hormone stimuli both in vivo and in vitro, demonstrating a critical role for N-cadherin in ovarian follicular development and ovulation. They highlighted the potential to inhibit ovulation by targeting this signaling mechanism.

      Strengths:

      This remarkable manuscript is very well designed, performed, and discussed. The authors analyzed different aspects, and their data supports their conclusions.

      Weaknesses:

      This study was performed using the mouse as a research model; further studies in larger animals and humans would be interesting and warranted.

      Indeed, this would be interesting. Ongoing research into therapeutic applications of N-cadherin targeting is reviewed in Blaschuk OW. Front Cell Dev Biol. 2022 Mar 3;10:866200

      Minor comments:

      Some results are intriguing. While the AREG y PTGS2 mRNA increased within the COC in vitro by the N-cadherin antagonists, in vivo, the treatment induced a significant increase in both genes when analyzing the whole ovary. What are the authors' ideas that could explain these discrepancies in outcomes?

      Comparing the responses in IVM COCs to in vivo whole ovaries carries multiple caveats, though as noted, the observations are consistent with altered mechanotransduction in each case. It is important to note the change in pre-ovulatory follicle gene expression in vivo, which likely affects the response of follicles to ovulatory stimulus.

      The authors stated that the ovaries from mice treated in the same manner and collected either before hCG treatment (eCG 44 h) or 11 h after hCG showed equivalent numbers of follicles at each stage of development from primary to antral. However, in Panel l from Figure 5, there is a significant increase in the number of antral follicles in the CRS-066 group (hCG 11 h) compared to the vehicle. Could the author discuss it in the manuscript?

      A small change in these follicle types was significant in hCG 11h treated mice and is consistent with the altered response to the ovulatory stimulus and reduced ovulation resulting in persistent antral follicles.

      Recommendations For The Authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Is the mechanism by which the small molecules block N-cad's adhesive activity known? And is the stable residence of cadherins in the plasma membrane known to depend on their engagement with other cadherins either in cis or in trans?

      Adhesion interactions between N-cadherin in Cis or Trans results in their clustering and enrichment at the membrane. Molecular docking models of the small molecule N-cadherin inhibitors are not available. However, these inhibitors were designed as peptidomimetics of the N-cadherin amino-terminus that is shown to interacts in Trans with N-cadherin on neighbouring cells (Blaschuk OW. Front Cell Dev Biol. 2022 Mar 3;10:866200).

      Since the inhibitors are blocking cadherin activity, one might have expected the cumulus cell mass to disaggregate into individual cells. Yet, Figures 3a and 3c show that this does not happen. Could the authors speculate how the cells are being held together?

    1. Author Response

      We appreciate the insightful feedback provided by the editors and reviewers who have recognized the novelty of our study. We have mapped the spatial distribution of six endogenous somatic histone H1 variants within the nuclei of several human cell lines using specific antibodies, which strongly suggest functional differences between variants. We will submit a reviewed version of the manuscript to accommodate the reviewers comments.

      To answer the reviewers comments at this stage:

      1. We do have investigated co-localization of H1 variants with HP1 proteins and we are eager to add some of this data in a revised version of this manuscript.

      2. Respect to the functional significance of the results presented here, we want to stress that as a consequence of the differential distribution and abundance of H1 variants among cell types, depletion of different variants has different consequences. For example, H1.2 depletion but not others has a great impact on chromatin compaction. Besides, cell lines lacking H1.3/H1.5 expression present a basal up-regulation of some Interferon stimulated genes (ISGs) and particular repetive elements, as it was previously described upon induced depletion of H1.2/H1.4 in a breast cancer cell line or in pancreatic adenocarcinomas with lower levels of replication-dependent H1 variants (Izquierdo et al. 2017 NAR 45:11622). So, our results reinforce the existing link between H1 content and immune signature. We are eager to add this data in a revised version of this manuscript. Moreover, we also analyzed the chromatin structural changes upon combined depletion of H1.2 and H1.4. Combined H1.2/H1.4 depletion triggers a global chromatin decompaction, which supports previous observations from ATAC-Seq and Hi-C experiments in these cells (Izquierdo et al. 2017 NAR 45:11622; Serna-Pujol et al. 2022 NAR 50:3892). Although H1 content is more compromised in these cells (30% total H1 reduction) compared to single H1 KDs, the phenotype observed could not be recapitulated when other H1 KD combinations, in which total H1 content was reduced similarly, were investigated (Izquierdo et al. 2017 NAR 45:11622), supporting that the deleterious defects were due to the non-redundant role of H1.2 and H1.4 proteins. Indeed, this manuscript supports this notion, as H1.2 and H1.4 show a different genome-wide and nuclear distribution.

      3. We totally agree with the reviewers that the use of commercially available antibodies does not guarantee their quality and specificity. As this issue was crucial for our studies, we extensively assayed performance and specificity of the antibodies, using different approaches. The validations were shown in our previous publications where these antibodies where successfully used for ChIP-seq (Serna et al. 2022 NAR 50:3892; Salinas-Pena et al, under revision). In summary, performance of H1.0 (05-629l, Millipore), H1.2 (ab4086, abcam), H1.4 (702876; Invitrogen), H1.5 (711912, Invitrogen) and H1X (ab31972; abcam) antibodies was tested by Western-Blot, ChIP and proteomic analyses (all the results are included in Supplementary Figure 1 in Serna et al. 2022 NAR 50:3892). Concretely, we tested specificity using inducible KDs for the depletion of each of the somatic H1 variants in T47D. We also checked that the antibodies did not recognize additional H1 variants using recombinant proteins or cell lines naturally lacking some of the variants. All the experiments confirmed that antibodies were variant-specific. In addition, when the corresponding epitope was absent, the antibodies did not gain new cross-reactivity with other variants. More recently, validation of the specificicity of the H1.3 antibody (ab203948) was performed following the same experimental approaches described for the rest of antibodies (Salinas-Pena et al, under revision).

      4. Our immunofluorescence data, together with ChIP-seq data, do not discard binding of H1 variants to a great variety of chromatin, but show enrichment or preferential binding to certain regions or chromatin types. Our data on the interphase nuclei does not suggest at all any type of quenching or saturation. Obviously, detection with antibodies depends on epitope accessibility, just like all immunofluorescence data ever published, and we have acknowledged that post-translational modifications of H1 may occlude antibody accessibility as some phospho-H1 antibodies give distribution patterns different than total/unmodified H1 antibodies. Thus, we cannot exclude that specific modified-H1s exhibit particular distribution patterns that are not being recapitulated in our data. This represents another layer of complexity in H1 diversity and we agree that exploration of the repertoire of H1 PTMs and their functional roles are an interesting matter of study that needs to be addressed. Still, our data is highly relevant as it demonstrates for the first time the unique distribution patterns of H1 variants among multiple cell lines and it does not use overexpression of tagged H1 variants that in our experience produces mislocalization of H1s.

      5. We will further explain how the relative quantification of H1 variants in different cell lines was performed if not clear enough. We agree that more sophisticated mass spectrometry-based quantification is desirable and we are collaborating to do this using internal H1 peptide controls, but this is out of the scope of this manuscript as the observed patterns of distribution of H1 variants do not depend on mild differences in variants abundance. Only the absence of H1.3 and H1.5 in some cell lines alters the distribution of other variants.

      6. We have also studied the spatial distribution of H1 variants in non-tumorogenic cell lines and we are eager to add this in a revised version of the manuscript.

    1. Author Response

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer 1:

      Public review:

      In this study, Porter et al report on outcomes from a small, open-label, pilot randomized clinical trial comparing dornase-alfa to the best available care in patients hospitalized with COVID-19 pneumonia. As the number of randomized participants is small, investigators describe also a contemporary cohort of controls and the study concludes about a decrease of inflammation (reflected by CRP levels) aJer 7 days of treatment but no other statistically significant clinical benefit.

      Suggestions to the authors:

      • The RCT does not follow CONSORT statement and reporting guidelines

      We thank you for this suggestion and have now amended the order and content of the manuscript to follow the CONSORT statement as closely as possible.

      • The authors have chosen a primary outcome that cannot be at least considered as clinically relevant or interesting. AJer 3 years of the pandemic with so much research, why investigate if a drug reduces CRP levels as we already have marketed drugs that provide beneficial clinical outcomes such as dexamethasone, anakinra, tocilizumab and baricitinib.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up this central topic. The answer to this question has both a historical and practical component. This trial was initiated in June of 2020 and was completed in June of 2021. At that time there were no known treatments for the severe immune pathology of COVID19 pneumonia. In June 2020, dexamethasone data came out and we incorporated dexamethasone into the study design. It took much longer for all other anti-inflammatories to be tested. Hence, our decision to trial an approved endonuclease was based purely on basic science work on the pathogenic role of cell-free chromatin and NETs in murine sepsis and flu models and the ability of DNase I to clear them and reduce pathology in these animal models. In addition, evidence for the presence of cell-free chromatin components in COVID-19 patient plasma had already been communicated in a pre-print. Finally, several studies had reported the anti-inflammatory effects of dornase treatment in CF patients. Hence there was a strong case for a cheap, safe, pulmonary noninvasive treatment that could be self-administered outside the clinical se]ng.

      The Identification of novel/repurposed treatments effective for COVID-19 were hampered by patient recruitment to competing studies during a pandemic. This resulted in small studies with inconclusive or contrary findings. In general, effective treatments were only picked up in very large RCTs. For example, demonstrating dexamethasone as effective in COVID-19 required recruitment of 6,425 patients into the RECOVERY study. Multiple trials with anti-IL-6 gave conflicting evidence until RECOVERY recruited 4116 adults with COVID-19 (n=2022, tocilizumab and 2094, control) similar for Baracitinib (4,148 randomised to treatment and 4,008 to standard care). Anakinra is approved for patients with elevated suPAR, based on data from one randomized clinical trial of 594 patients, of whom 405 had active treatment (PMID: 34625750). However, a systematic review analysing over 1,627 patients (anakinra 888, control 739) with COVID-19 showed no benefit (PMID: 36841793). Regarding the choice of the primary endpoint, there is a wealth of clinical evidence to support the relevance of CRP as a prognostic marker for COVID-19 pneumonia patients and it is a standard diagnostic and prognostic clinical parameter in infectious disease wards. This choice in March 2020 was supported by evidence of the prognostic value of IL-6; CRP is a surrogate of IL-6. We also provide our own data from a large study of severe COVID-19 pneumonia in figure 1, showing how well CRP correlates with survival.

      In summary, our data suggest that Dornase yields an anti-inflammatory effect that is comparable or potentially superior to cytokine-blocking monotherapies at a fraction of the cost and potentially without the additional adverse effects such as the increase for co-infections.

      We now provide additional justification on these points in the introduction on pg.4 as follows:

      “The trial was ini.ated in June 2020 and was completed in September of 2021. At the start of the trial only dexamethasone had been proven to benefit hospitalized COVID-19 pneumonia pa.ents and was thus included in both arms of the trial. To increase the chance of reaching significance under challenging constraints in pa.ent access, we opted to increase our sample size by using a combina.on of randomized individuals and available CRP data from matched contemporary controls (CC) hospitalized at UCL but not recruited to a trial. These approaches demonstrated that when combined with dexamethasone, nebulized DNase treatment was an effec.ve an.-inflammatory treatment in randomized individuals with or without the implementa.on of CC data.”

      We also added the following explanation in the discussion on pg. 16:

      “Our study design offered a solution to the early screening of compounds for inclusion in larger platform trials. The study took advantage of frequent repeated measures of quantifiable CRP in each patient, to allow a smaller sample size to determine efficacy/futility than if powered on clinical outcomes. We applied a CRP-based approach that was similar to the CATALYST and ATTRACT studies. CATALYST showed in much smaller groups (usual care, 54, namilumab, 57 and infliximab, 35) that namilumab that is an antibody that blocks the cytokine GM-CSF reduced CRP even in participants treated with dexamethasone whereas infliximab that targets TNF-α had no significant effect on CRP. This led to a suggestion that namilumab should be considered as an agent to be prioritised for further investigation in the RECOVERY trial. A direct comparison of our results with CATALYST is difficult due to the different nature of the modelling employed in the two studies. However, in general Dornase alfa exhibited comparable significance in the reduction in CRP compared to standard of care as described for namilumab at a fraction of the cost. Furthermore, endonuclease therapies may prove superior to cytokine blocking monotherapies, as they are unlikely to increase the risk for microbial co-infections that have been reported for antibody therapies that neutralize cytokines that are critical for immune defence such as IL-1β, IL-6 or GM-CSF. “

      • Please provide in Methods the timeframe for the investigation of the primary endpoint

      This information is provided in the analysis on pg. 8:

      “The primary outcome was the least square (LS) mean CRP up to 7 days or at hospital discharge whichever was sooner.”

      • Why day 35 was chosen for the read-out of the endpointt?

      We now state on pg. 8 that “Day 35 was chosen as being likely to include most early mortality due to COVID-19 being 4 weeks after completion of a week of treatment. ( i.e. d7 of treatment +28 (4 x 7 days))”

      • The authors performed an RCT but in parallel chose to compare also controls. They should explain their rationale as this is not usual. I am not very enthusiastic to see mixed results like Figures 2c and 2d.

      We initially aimed at a fully randomized trial. However, the swiJ implementation of trial prioritization strategies towards large and pre-established trial plamorms in the UK made the recruitment COVID19 patients to small studies extremely challenging. Thus, we struggled to gain access to patients. Our power calculations suggested that a mixed trial with randomized and contemporary controls was the best way forward under these restrictions in patient access that could provide sufficient power.

      That being said, we also provide the primary endpoint (CRP) results in Fig. 3B as well as the results for the length of hospitalization (Fig. S3D) for the randomized subjects only.

      • Analysis is performed in mITT; this is a major limitation. The authors should provide at least ITT results. And they should describe in the main manuscript why they chose mITT analysis.

      We apologize if this point was confusing. We performed the analysis on the ITT as defined in our SAP: “The primary analysis population will be all evaluable patients randomised to BAC + dornase alfa or BAC only who have at least one post-baseline CRP measurement, as well as matched historical comparators.”

      We understand that the reason this might be mistaken as an mITT is because the N in the ITT (39) doesn’t match the number randomised and because we had stated on pg. 8 that “ Efficacy assessments of primary and secondary outcomes in the modified inten.on-to-treat popula.on were performed.”

      However, we did randomise 41 participants, but:

      One participant in the DA arm never received treatment. The individual withdrew consent and was replaced. We also have no CRP data for this participant in the database, so they were unevaluable, and we couldn’t include them in the baseline table even if we wanted to. In addition, 1 participant in BAC only had a baseline CRP measurement available. Hence not evaluable as we only have a baseline CRP measurement for this participant.

      We have corrected the confusing statement on pg. 8 and added an additional explanation.

      “Efficacy assessments of primary and secondary outcomes in the inten.on-to-treat (ITT) popula.on were performed on all randomised par.cipants who had received at least one dose of dornase alfa if randomized to treatment. For full details see Sta.s.cal Analysis Plan. The ITT was adjusted to mi.gate the following protocol viola.ons where one par.cipant in the BAC arm and one in the DA arm withdrew before they received treatment and provided only a baseline CRP measurement available. The par.cipant in the DA arm was replaced with an addi.onal recruited pa.ent. Exploratory endpoints were only available in randomised par.cipants and not in the CC. In this case, a post hoc within group analysis was conducted to compare baseline and post-baseline measurements.”

      • It is also not usual to exclude patients from analysis because investigators just do not have serial measurements. This is lost to follow up and investigators should have pre-decided what to do with lost-to-follow-up.

      Our protocol pre-specified that the primary analysis population should have at least one postbaseline CRP measurement (pg. 13 of protocol). The patient that was excluded was one that initially joined the trial but withdrew consent after the first treatment but before the first post-treatment blood sample could be drawn. Hence, the pre-treatment CRP of this patient alone provided no useful information.

      • In Table 1 I would like to see all randomized patients (n=39), which is missing. There are also baseline characteristics that are missing, like which other treatments as BAT received by those patients except for dexamethasone.

      Table 1 includes all 39 patients plus 60 CCs.<br /> Table 2 shows additional treatments given for COVID-19 as part of BAC.

      • In the first paragraph of clinical outcomes, the authors refer to a cohort that is not previously introduced in the manuscript. This is confusing. And I do not understand why this analysis is performed in the context of this RCT although I understand its pilot nature.

      One of the main criticisms we have encountered in this study has been the choice of the primary endpoint. The best way respond to these questions was to provide data to support the prognostic relevance of CRP in COVID-19 pneumonia from a separate independent study where no other treatments such as dexamethasone, anakinra or anti-IL6 therapies were administered. We think this is very useful analysis and provides essential context for the trial and the choice of the primary endpoint, indicating that CRP has good enough resolution to predict clinical outcomes.

      • Propensity-score selected contemporary controls may introduce bias in favor of the primary study analysis, since controls are already adjusted for age, sex and comorbidities.

      The contemporary controls were selected to best match the characteristics of the randomized patients including that the first CRP measurement upon admission surpassed the trial threshold, so we do not see how this selection process introduces biases, as it was blinded with regards to the course of the CRP measurements. Given that this was a small trial, matching for baseline characteristics is necessary to minimize confounding effects.

      • The authors do not clearly present numerically survivors and non-survivors at day 34, even though this is one of the main secondary outcomes.

      We now provide the mortality numbers in the following paragraph on pg. 13.

      “Over 35 days follow up, 1 person in the BAC + dornase-alfa group died, compared to 8 in the BAC group. The hazard ra.o observed in the Cox propor.onal hazards model (95% CI) was 0.47 (0.06, 3.86), which es.mates that throughout 35 days follow-up, there was a 53% reduced chance of death at any given .mepoint in the BAC + dornase-alfa group compared to the BAC group, though the confidence intervals are wide due to a small number of events. The p-value from a log-rank test was 0.460, which does not reach sta.s.cal significance at an alpha of 0.05.”

      • It is unclear why another cohort (Berlin) was used to associate CRP with mortality. CRP association with mortality should (also) be performed within the current study.

      As we explained above, the Berlin cohort CRP data serve to substantiate the relevance of CRP as a primary endpoint in a cohort that experienced sufficient mortality as this cohort did not receive any approved anti-inflammatory therapy. Mortality in our COVASE trial was minimal, since all patients were on dexamethasone and did not reach the highest severity grade, since we opted to treat patients before they deteriorated further. The overall mortality was 8% across all arms of our study, which does not provide enough events for mortality measurements. In contrast the Berlin cohort did not receive dexamethasone and all patients had reached a WHO severity grade 7 category with mortality at 30%.

      My other concerns are:

      • This report is about an RCT and the authors should follow the CONSORT reporting guidelines. Please amend the manuscript and Figure 1b accordingly and provide a CONSORT checklist.

      We now provide a CONSORT checklist and have amended the CONSORT diagram accordingly.

      • Please provide in brief the exclusion criteria in the main manuscript

      We have now included the exclusion criteria in the manuscript on pg. 6.

      “1.1.1 Exclusion criteria

      1. Females who are pregnant, planning pregnancy or breasmeeding

      2. Concurrent and/or recent involvement in other research or use of another experimental inves.ga.onal medicinal product that is likely to interfere with the study medica.on within (specify .me period e.g. last 3 months) of study enrolment 3. Serious condi.on mee.ng one of the following:

      a. Respiratory distress with respiratory rate >=40 breaths/min

      b. oxygen satura.on<=93% on high-flow oxygen

      1. Require mechanical invasive or non-invasive ven.la.on at screening

      2. Concurrent severe respiratory disease such as asthma, COPD and/or ILD

      3. Any major disorder that in the opinion of the Inves.gator would interfere with the evalua.on of the results or cons.tute a health risk for the trial par.cipant

      4. Terminal disease and life expectancy <12 months without COVID-19

      5. Known allergies to dornase alfa and excipients

      6. Par.cipants who are unable to inhale or exhale orally throughout the en.re nebulisa.on period So briefly Patients were excluded if they were:

      7. pregnant, planning pregnancy or breasmeeding

      8. Serious condition meeting one of the following:

      a. Respiratory distress with respiratory rate >=40 breaths/min

      b. oxygen satura.on<=93% on high-flow oxygen

      1. Require ven.la.on at screening

      2. Concurrent severe respiratory disease such as asthma, COPD and/or ILD

      3. Terminal disease and life expectancy <12 months without COVID-19

      4. Known allergies to dornase alfa and excipients

      5. Participants who are unable to inhale or exhale orally throughout the en.re nebulisa.on period”

      • "The final trial visit occurred at day 35." "Analysis included mortality at day 35". I am not sure I understand why. In clinicaltrials.gov all endpoints are meant to be studies at day 7 except for mortality rate day 28. Why day 35 was chosen? Please be consistent.

      Thank you for identifying this inconsistency. We have amended the record on clinicaltrials.gov to read ‘’the time to event data was censored at 28 days post last dose (up to d35) for the randomised participants and at the date of the last electronic record for the CC.”

      • Please provide in Methods the timeframe for the investigation of the primary endpoint

      • The authors performed an RCT but in parallel chose to compare also controls. They should explain their rationale as this is not usual. I am not very enthusiastic to see mixed results like Figures 2c and 2d.

      • Analysis is performed in mITT; this is a major limitation. The authors should provide at least ITT results. And they should describe in the main manuscript why they chose mITT analysis.

      • It is also not usual to exclude patients from analysis because investigators just do not have serial measurements. This is lost to follow up and investigators should have pre-decided what to do with lost-to-follow-up.

      • Figure 1b as in CONSORT statement, please provide reasons why screened patients were not enrolled.

      • In Table 1 I would like to see all randomized patients (n=39), which is missing. There are also baseline characteristics that are missing, like which other treatment as BAT received those patients except for dexamethasone.

      • In the first paragraph of clinical outcomes, the authors refer to a cohort that is not previously introduced in the manuscript. This is confusing. And I do not understand why this analysis is performed in the context of this RCT although I understand its pilot nature.

      • In Figure 2 the authors draw results about ITT although in methods describe that they performed an mITT analysis. Please be consistent.

      Please see answers provided to these queries above.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      1) Suppl Figure 2B would be more informative if presented as a Table with N of patients with per day sampling

      We now provide the primary end point daily sampling table in Table 3.

      2) The numbers at risk should figure under the KM curves

      The numbers at risk for figures 1E, 2C, 2D have been added as graphs either in the main figures or in the supplement.

      3) HD in Supplementary figure 3 should be explained

      We apologize for this omission. We now provide a description for the healthy donor samples that we used in the cell-free DNA measurements in figure S3B on pg. 14:

      “Compared to the plasma of anonymized healthy donors volunteers at the Francis Crick ins.tute (HD), plasma cf-DNA levels were elevated in both BAC and DA-treated COVASE par.cipants.

      4) Presentation is inappropriate for Table S4

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this issue. We have now formaxed Table S4 to be consistent with all other tables.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript is a focused investigation of the phosphor-regulation of a C. elegans kinesin-2 motor protein, OSM-3. In C-elegans sensory ciliary, kinesin-2 motor proteins Kinesin-II complex and OSM-3 homodimer transport IFT trains anterogradely to the ciliary tip. Kinesin-II carries OSM-3 as an inactive passenger from the ciliary base to the middle segment, where kinesin-II dissociates from IFT trains and OSM-3 gets activated and transports IFT trains to the distal segment. Therefore, activation/inactivation of OSM-3 plays an essential role in its ciliary function.

      Strengths:

      In this study, using mass spectrometry, the authors have shown that the NEKL-3 kinase phosphorylates a serine/threonine patch at the hinge region between coiled coils 1 and 2 of an OSM-3 dimer, referred to as the elbow region in ubiquitous kinesin-1. Phosphomimic mutants of these sites inhibit OSM-3 motility both in vitro and in vivo, suggesting that this phosphorylation is critical for the autoinhibition of the motor. Conversely, phospho-dead mutants of these sites hyperactivate OSM-3 motility in vitro and affect the localization of OSM3 in C. elegans. The authors also showed that Alanine to Tyrosine mutation of one of the phosphorylation rescues OS-3 function in live worms.

      Weaknesses:

      Collectively, this study presents evidence for the physiological role of OSM-3 elbow phosphorylation in its autoregulation, which affects ciliary localization and function of this motor. Overall, the work is well performed, and the results mostly support the conclusions of this manuscript. However, the work will benefit from additional experiments to further support conclusions and rule out alternative explanations, filling some logical gaps with new experimental evidence and in-text clarifications, and improving writing before I can recommend publication.

      We appreciate Reviewer #1’s comments and suggestions. We have now provided additional evidences and discussions to further support our conclusions and fill the logical gaps. We have also provided alternative explanations to our data and improved writing.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The regulation of kinesin is fundamental to cellular morphogenesis. Previously, it has been shown that OSM-3, a kinesin required for intraflagellar transport (IFT), is regulated by autoinhibition. However, it remains totally elusive how the autoinhibition of OSM-3 is released. In this study, the authors have shown that NEKL-3 phosphorylates OSM-3 and releases its autoinhibition.

      The authors found NEKL-3 directly phosphorylates OSM-3 (although the method is not described clearly) (Figure 1). The phophorylated residue is the "elbow" of OSM-3. The authors introduced phospho-dead (PD) and phospho-mimic (PM) mutations by genome editing and found that the OSM-3(PD) protein does not form cilia, and instead, accumulates to the axonal tips. The phenotype is similar to another constitutive active mutant of OSM-3, OSM-3(G444A) (Imanishi et al., 2006; Xie et al., 2024). osm-3(PM) has shorter cilia, which resembles with loss of function mutants of osm-3 (Figure 3). The authors did structural prediction and showed that G444E and PD mutations change the conformation of OSM-3 protein (Figure 3). In the single-molecule assays G444E and PD mutations exhibited increased landing rate (Figure 4). By unbiased genetic screening, the authors identified a suppressor mutant of osm-3(PD), in which A489T occurs. The result confirms the importance of this residue. Based on these results, the authors suggest that NEKL-3 induces phosphorylation of the elbow domain and inactivates OSM-3 motor when the motor is synthesized in the cell body. This regulation is essential for proper cilia formation.

      Strengths:

      The finding is interesting and gives new insight into how the IFT motor is regulated.

      Weaknesses:

      The methods section has not presented sufficient information to reproduce this study.

      We appreciate that Reviewer #2 is also positive to our study. We have now provided sufficient information in the revised Methods section.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major Concerns

      (1) Why do the authors think that NEKL-3 phosphorylates OSM-3 in the first place? This seems to come out of nowhere and prior evidence indicating that NEKL-3 may be phosphorylating OSM-3 is not even mentioned in the Introduction.

      We thank the Reviewer for raising this important point. Our hypothesis that NEKL-3 phosphorylates OSM-3 stems from prior findings in our lab. In a previous study (Yi et al., Traffic, 2018, PMID: 29655266), we identified NEKL-4, a member of the NIMA kinase family, as a suppressor of the OSM-3(G444E) hyperactive mutation. This discovery prompted us to explore the broader role of NIMA kinases in regulating OSM3. Subsequent genetic screens (Xie et al., EMBO J, 2024, PMID: 38806659) revealed that both NEKL-3 and NEKL-4 suppress multiple OSM-3 mutations, further supporting their functional interaction. Given the established role of NIMA kinases in phosphorylation-dependent processes (Fry et al., JCS, 2012, PMID: 23132929; Chivukula et al., Nat. Med., 2020, PMID: 31959991; Thiel, C. et al. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2011, PMID: 21211617; Smith, L. A. et al., J. Am. Soc. Nephrol., 2006, PMID: 16928806), we hypothesized that NEKL-3/4 may directly phosphorylate OSM-3 to modulate its activity.

      To test this hypothesis, we expressed recombinant C. elegans NEKL-3 and OSM-3 proteins and conducted in vitro phosphorylation assays. While we were unable to obtain active recombinant NEKL-4 (limitations noted in the revised text), our experiments with NEKL-3 revealed phosphorylation at residues 487-490 (YSTT motif) in OSM-3’s tail region, as confirmed by mass spectrometry. These findings are now explicitly contextualized in the Introduction and Results sections of the revised manuscript.

      Page #4, Line #11:

      “...In our previous study (Yi et al., Traffic, 2018, PMID: 29655266), a genetic screen targeting the OSM-3(G444E) hyperactive mutation identified NEKL-4, a member of the NIMA kinase family, as a suppressor of this phenotype. This finding, combined with reports that NIMA kinases regulate ciliary processes independently of their canonical mitotic roles (Fry et al., JCS, 2012, PMID: 23132929; Chivukula et al., Nat. Med., 2020, PMID: 31959991; Thiel, C. et al. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2011, PMID: 21211617; Smith, L. A. et al., J. Am. Soc. Nephrol., 2006, PMID: 16928806), prompted us to investigate whether NIMA kinases modulate OSM-3-driven intraflagellar transport. We hypothesized that NEKL-3/4, as paralogs within this family, might directly phosphorylate OSM-3 to regulate its motility...”

      Page #4, line #26:  

      “... To determine whether NIMA kinase family members could directly phosphorylate

      OSM-3, we purified prokaryotic recombinant C. elegans NEKL-3/NEKL-4 and OSM3 protein in order to perform in vitro phosphorylation assays. We were able to obtain active recombinant NEKL-3 but not NEKL-4. The in vitro phosphorylation assays showed that NEKL-3, directly phosphorylates OSM-3 (Fig. 1A-B, Appendix Table S1). Subsequent mass spectrometric analysis revealed phosphorylation at residues 487-490, which localize to the conserved "YSTT" motif within OSM-3’s C-terminal tail region ...”

      (2) The authors need to characterize the proteins they expressed and purified for in vitro ATPase and motility assays. Are these proteins monomers or dimers?

      For our in vitro ATPase and motility assays, OSM-3 was expressed in E. coli BL21(DE3) and purified using established protocols (Xie et al., EMBO J, 2024, PMID: 38806659; Imanishi et al., JCB, 2006, PMID: 17000874). To confirm its oligomeric state, we analyzed recombinant OSM-3 by size-exclusion chromatography coupled with multiangle light scattering (SEC-MALS). As reported in Xie et al. (2024), OSM-3 (~80 kDa monomer) elutes with a molecular weight of 173–193 kDa under physiological buffer conditions, consistent with a homodimeric assembly. These findings confirm that the functional unit used in our assays is the biologically relevant dimer. This characterization has been added to the revised manuscript on Page #35, Line #7.

      “…OSM-3 was expressed in E. coli BL21(DE3) and purified for in vitro assays using established protocols (REFs). Size-exclusion chromatography coupled with multiangle light scattering (SEC-MALS) (Xie et al., EMBO J., 2024) confirmed that recombinant OSM-3 forms a homodimer (173–193 kDa) under physiological conditions, ensuring its dimeric state remained intact....” 

      (3) The authors primarily used PD and PM mutations, which affect all four amino acids in the region. This may or may not be physiologically relevant. Figure 5 indicates that T489 is a critical regulatory site. However, this conclusion is undermined by reliance on PD mutations, which affect all four amino acids. Creating PM (T489E) and PD (T489A) mutations based on WT OSM-3 would better reflect physiological relevance. In vitro assays with a single phosphomimic or phosphor-dead mutation at residue 489 are missing at the end of this story. This would better link Figure 5 with the rest of the manuscript.

      We thank the reviewer for this constructive critique. Below, we address the concerns and integrate new data to strengthen the link between T489 and autoinhibition:

      To probe the regulatory role of T489 phosphorylation, we generated osm-3(T489E) (phosphomimetic, PM) and osm-3(T489A) (phospho-dead, PD) mutant animals. Strikingly, both mutants formed axonal puncta (Figure S7), recapitulating the hyperactive phenotype of the OSM-3G444E mutant. While the similar puncta formation in PM and PD mutants initially appeared paradoxical, this observation underscores the necessity of dynamic phosphorylation cycling at T489 for proper autoinhibition. Specifically, the PD mutant (T489A) likely disrupts phosphorylationdependent autoinhibition stabilization, leading to constitutive activation, where as the PM mutant (T489E) may mimic a "locked" phosphorylated state, preventing dephosphorylation-dependent release of autoinhibition in cilia and trapping OSM-3 in an aggregation-prone conformation. These results highlight T489 as a structural linchpin whose post-translational modification dynamically regulates motor activity. While the precise molecular mechanism—such as how phosphorylation modulates tailmotor domain interactions—remains to be elucidated, our data conclusively demonstrate that perturbing T489 (even in isolation) destabilizes autoinhibition, driving puncta formation and the constitutive activity.

      We have integrated the above paragraph in the revised manuscript on page #8, line #27.

      (4) There seems to be a disconnect between the MT gliding assays in Figure 4C and single molecule motility assays in Figure 4E. The gliding assays show that all constructs can glide microtubules at near WT speeds. Yet, the motility assays show that WT and PM cannot land or walk on MTs. The authors need to explain why this is the case. Is this because surface immobilization of kinesin from its tail disrupts autoinhibition? Alternatively, the protein preparation may include monomers that cannot be autoinhibited and cannot land and processively walk on surface-immobilized microtubules (because they only have one motor domain) but can glide microtubules when immobilized on the surface from their tail.

      The surface immobilization of OSM-3 via its tail domain disrupts autoinhibition, a phenomenon previously observed in other kinesins such as kinesin-1 (Nitzsche et al, Methods Cell Biol., 2010, PMID: 20466139). In our assays, OSM-3 was nonspecifically immobilized on glass surfaces, enabling microtubule gliding by motors whose autoinhibition was relieved through tail anchoring. Critically, the PD and PM mutations reside in the tail region and do not alter the intrinsic properties of the motor head domain. Consequently, once autoinhibition is released via immobilization, the gliding velocities reflect the conserved motor head activity, which is expected to remain comparable across all constructs. While we cannot entirely rule out the presence of monomeric OSM-3 in solution, several lines of evidence argue against this possibility. First, the mutations are located in the elbow region, which is dispensable for motor dimerization. Second, SEC-MALS analysis from prior studies confirms that purified OSM-3 exists predominantly as dimers in solution. 

      We have discussed these issues in the revised text on page #10, line #18: 

      “…In our gliding assays, OSM-3PM has an increased gliding speed of 0.69 ± 0.07 μm/s (Fig. 4 C-D), similar to PD mutant. PD and PM mutations are confined to the elbow region, leaving the motor head’s mechanochemical properties intact. Upon tail immobilization—which releases autoinhibition—the gliding speeds reflect motor head activity. Single-molecule assays, however, directly resolve their native regulatory states: PD mutants are constitutively active, whereas PM mutants persist in an autoinhibited state (Fig. 4E-G). Although monomeric OSM-3 could theoretically mediate singlemotor gliding, the previous SEC-MALS data demonstrate that OSM-3 purifies as stable dimers (Xie et al., EMBO J, 2024, PMID: 38806659). Thus, dimeric OSM-3 is perhaps the predominant functional species in our assays…”

      (5) An alternative explanation for the data is that both PD and PM mutations result in loss-of-function effects, disrupting OSM-3 activity. For instance:

      a) In Figure 2C, both mutations cause shorter cilia than the wild type (WT).

      b) In Figure 4A, both mutations result in higher ATPase activity than WT.

      c) In Figure 4D, both mutations show increased gliding velocity compared to WT. These results suggest the observed effects could stem from loss of function rather than phosphorylation-specific regulation.

      Although PD and PM mutations exhibit superficially similar "loss-of-function" phenotypes in certain assays, they mechanistically disrupt motor regulation in distinct ways:

      a) Ciliary Length (Figure 2C) PD Mutants: Hyperactivation causes OSM-3-PD to prematurely aggregate into axonal puncta, preventing ciliary entry. Consequently, cilia are built solely by the weaker Kinesin-II motor, which only constructs shorter middle segments.

      PM Mutants: OSM-3-PM retains autoinhibition during transport (enabling ciliary entry) but cannot be dephosphorylated in cilia. This blocks activation, leaving OSM-3-PM partially functional and resulting in cilia intermediate in length between WT and PD.

      We have discussed this issue in the revised text on page #5, line #30:

      “…These findings indicate that OSM-3-PM is in an autoinhibited state capable of ciliary delivery, yet fails to achieve full activation due to defective dephosphorylation. This incomplete activation results in suboptimal motor function and intermediate ciliary length phenotypes (Fig.2 B-C). In contrast, OSM-3-PD exhibits constitutive activation leading to aggregation into axonal puncta, which completely abolishes its ciliary entry capacity (Fig.2 A-B)...”

      b) ATPase Activity (Figure 4A)

      PD Mutants: Fully autoinhibition-released (98.15% of KHC ATPase activity), consistent with constitutive activation.

      PM Mutants: Show partial ATPase activity (34.28% of KHC), reflecting imperfect phosphomimicry. While the DDEE substitution introduces negative charges, it fails to fully replicate the steric/kinetic effects of phosphorylated tyrosine (Y486; phenyl ring absent), resulting in incomplete autoinhibition stabilization. Despite this, the residual inhibition is sufficient to phenocopy shorter cilia in vivo.

      We have discussed this issue in the revised text on page #7, line#19:

      “…The PM mutant’s partial ATPase activity (34.28% of KHC) might arise from imperfect phosphomimicry—while the DDEE substitution introduces negative charges, it lacks the steric bulk of phosphorylated tyrosine (pY487). And this incomplete mimicry allows residual autoinhibition, sufficient to limit ciliary construction in vivo...”

      c) Microtubule Gliding Velocity (Figure 4D)

      Gliding Assay Limitation: Tail immobilization artificially releases autoinhibition, masking regulatory differences. Thus, all constructs (PD, PM) exhibit similar velocities (~0.7 µm/s), reflecting conserved motor head activity.

      Single-Molecule Assay (Figure 4E): Directly resolves native autoinhibition states:

      PD mutants show robust motility (autoinhibition released).

      PM mutants remain largely inactive (autoinhibition retained).

      We have discussed this issue in the revised text on page #10, line#18:

      “…In our gliding assays, OSM-3PM has an increased gliding speed of 0.69 ± 0.07 μm/s (Fig. 4 C-D), similar to PD mutant. PD and PM mutations are confined to the elbow region, leaving the motor head’s mechanochemical properties intact. Upon tail immobilization—which releases autoinhibition—the gliding speeds reflect motor head activity. Single-molecule assays, however, directly resolve their native regulatory states: PD mutants are constitutively active, whereas PM mutants persist in an autoinhibited state (Fig. 4E-G)...”

      Minor Suggestions and Concerns

      (1) Lines 60-66: References that support these observations are missing from this section.

      We have added the relevant references.

      (2) Lines 66-67: I would revise this sentence as "It remains unclear how OSM-3 becomes enriched...".

      We have made the changes.

      (3) Line 85: The authors should describe how they perform these assays (i.e. recombinantly expressed NEKL-3 and OSM-3, are these C. elegans proteins, and which expression system was used...).

      We have described them in the main text and methods

      Page #4 line #26

      “...To determine whether NIMA kinase family members could directly phosphorylate OSM-3, we purified prokaryotic recombinant C. elegans NEKL-3/NEKL-4 and OSM-3 protein in order to perform in vitro phosphorylation assays...”

      Page #35 line#12

      “...Basically, point mutations was introduced in to pET.M.3C OSM-3-eGFP-His6 plasmid for prokaryotic expression. Plasmid transformed E. coli (BL21) was cultured at 37°C and induced overnight at 23°C with 0.2 mM IPTG. Cells were lysed in lysis buffer (50 mM NaPO4 pH8.0, 250 mM NaCl, 20 mM imidazole, 10 mM bME, 0.5 mM ATP, 1 mM MgCl¬2, Complete Protease Inhibitor Cocktail (Roche)) and Ni-NTA beads were applied for affinity purification. After incubation, beads were washed with wash buffer (50 mM NaPO4 pH6.0, 250 mM NaCl, 10 mM bME, 0.1 mM ATP, 1 mM MgCl¬2) and eluted with elute buffer (50 mM NaPO4 pH7.2, 250 mM NaCl, 500 mM imidazole, 10 mM bME, 0.1 mM ATP, 1 mM MgCl¬2). Protein concentration was determined by standard Bradford assay. C elegans nekl-3 cDNA was cloned in to pGEX-6P GST vector and expressed in E. coli BL21 (DE3) and purified for in vitro phosphorylation assays. Plasmid transformed E. coli (BL21) was cultured at 37°C and induced overnight at 18°C with 0.5 mM IPTG. Cells were lysed in lysis buffer (50 mM NaPO4 pH8.0, 250 mM NaCl, 1 mM DTT, Complete Protease Inhibitor Cocktail (Roche)) and GST beads were applied for affinity purification. After incubation, beads were washed with wash buffer (50 mM NaPO4 pH6.0, 250 mM NaCl, 1 mM DTT) and eluted with elute buffer (50 mM NaPO4 pH7.2, 150 mM NaCl, 10 mM GSH, 1 mM DTT). Purified proteins were dialyzed against storge buffer (50 mM Tris-HCl, pH 8.0, 150 mM NaCl). Protein concentration was determined by standard Bradford assay...”

      (4) Line 141: The first sentence of this paragraph lacks motivation. I would start this sentence with "To directly observe the effects of phosphor mutants in the elbow region in microtubule binding and motility of OSM-3, we...".

      We have made the change.

      (5) Figure 1B: The mass spectrometry data in Figure 1B lacks adequate explanation. The Methods section should detail the experimental protocol, data interpretation, and any databases used. Additionally, the manuscript should list all identified phosphorylation sites on OSM-3 to provide context, including whether Y487_T490 is the major site.

      We have provided the detailed experimental protocol, data interpretation, and databases used in methods. We have provided all identified sites as Appendix table S1.

      (6) Figure 1C: Is it possible to model the effect of PM and PD mutations using AlphaFold? The authors should also show PAE or pLDDT scores of their model.

      AlphaFold cannot well model the effect of mutants, but we conducted the Rosetta relax to capture their possible conformational changes, as shown in the revised Figure 3. We have provided PAE and pLDDT as a new figure, Figure S2.

      (7) Figure 2D: The unit for speed should use a lowercase "s" for seconds.

      We have fixed it.

      (8) Figure 3: I am not sure whether this figure stands for a main text figure on its own, as it is only a Rosetta prediction and is not supported by any experimental data. In addition, it remains unclear what the labels on the x-axis mean.

      We have updated the figure and explain the labels on the x-axis in Figure S4 to make it more reader-friendly.

      (9) Figure 4: NEKL-3-treated OSM-1 should be included as a positive control in the in vitro experiments.

      We suspect that the Reviewer asked for NEKL-3-treated OSM-3. 

      In our other study which has just been accepted by the Journal of Cell Biology, NEKL3-treated OSM-3 significantly reduced the affinity between OSM-3 motor and microtubules and showed very low ATPase activity. We have cited and discussed this in the revised text on page #10, line #28: 

      “…As demonstrated in our recent study (Huang et al., JCB, 2025, In press, attached), phosphorylation of OSM-3 by NEKL-3 at two distinct regions—Ser96 and the conserved "elbow" motif—differentially regulates its activity and localization. Phosphorylation at Ser96 reduces OSM-3’s ATPase activity and alters its ciliary distribution from the distal segment to a uniform localization, while elbow phosphorylation induces autoinhibition, retaining OSM-3 in the cell body. Strikingly, in vitro phosphorylation of OSM-3 by NEKL-3 significantly reduces its microtubulebinding affinity, likely arising from combined modifications at both sites. We propose a model wherein elbow phosphorylation ensures anterograde ciliary transport, while Ser96 phosphorylation fine-tunes distal segment targeting. This multistep regulation may involve distinct phosphatases to reverse phosphorylation at specific sites, a hypothesis warranting further investigation….”

      (10) Figure 4C, D, and F: The unit of velocity is wrong. The authors should use the same units they used in the table shown in Figure 4B.

      We have fixed these errors

      (11) Figure 4F: The velocity of PD is a lot lower than G444E. Therefore, it would be more appropriate to refer to PD as partially active, rather than hyperactive.

      We have made the change. 

      (12) Figure 5: There is too much genetics jargon on this figure (EMF, F2, 100%Dyf,...). How are the alleles numbered? Is it OK to refer to them as Alleles 1 and 2 for simplicity?

      According to the established C. elegans allele nomenclature, each worm allele has a unique number named after the lab code for identification. We have simplified the labels and updated the figure to make it more reader-friendly.

      (13) Figure 5E: A plot would be more reader-friendly than a table. Additionally, the legend for Fig. 5E mistakenly refers to it as "D."

      We have changed the table to a plot and fixed the mistakes. We thank the Reviewer for pointing them out.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The model appears as if NEKL-3 induces dephosphorylation of OSM-3 (Figure 6). This is not consistent with the conclusions described in the Discussion and is confusing.

      We have updated the model figure and fixed the error.

      (2) It should be described why the authors hypothesized NEKL-3 phosphorylates OSM3. Was there genetic evidence? Did the authors screened cilia-related kinases? or Did the authors identify it incidentally? Providing this information would help readers to understand the context of the research.

      We appreciate both Reviewers for pointing out this issue. 

      Our hypothesis that NEKL-3 phosphorylates OSM-3 stems from prior findings in our lab. In a previous study (Yi et al., Traffic, 2018, PMID: 29655266), we identified NEKL-4, a member of the NIMA kinase family, as a suppressor of the OSM-3(G444E) hyperactive mutation. This discovery prompted us to explore the broader role of NIMA kinases in regulating OSM-3. Subsequent genetic screens (Xie et al., EMBO J, 2024, PMID: 38806659) revealed that both NEKL-3 and NEKL-4 suppress multiple OSM-3 mutations, further supporting their functional interaction. Given the established role of NIMA kinases in phosphorylation-dependent processes (Fry et al., JCS, 2012, PMID: 23132929; Chivukula et al., Nat. Med., 2020, PMID: 31959991; Thiel, C. et al. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2011, PMID: 21211617; Smith, L. A. et al., J. Am. Soc. Nephrol., 2006, PMID: 16928806), we hypothesized that NEKL-3/4 may directly phosphorylate OSM3 to modulate its activity.

      To test this hypothesis, we expressed recombinant C. elegans NEKL-3 and OSM-3 proteins and conducted in vitro phosphorylation assays. While we were unable to obtain active recombinant NEKL-4 (limitations noted in the revised text), our experiments with NEKL-3 revealed phosphorylation at residues 487-490 (YSTT motif) in OSM-3’s tail region, as confirmed by mass spectrometry. These findings are now explicitly contextualized in the Introduction and Results sections of the revised manuscript.

      Page #4, Line #11:

      “... In our previous study (Yi et al., Traffic, 2018, PMID: 29655266), a genetic screen targeting the OSM-3(G444E) hyperactive mutation identified NEKL-4, a member of the NIMA kinase family, as a suppressor of this phenotype. This finding, combined with reports that NIMA kinases regulate ciliary processes independently of their canonical mitotic roles (Fry et al., JCS, 2012, PMID: 23132929; Chivukula et al., Nat. Med., 2020, PMID: 31959991; Thiel, C. et al. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 2011, PMID: 21211617; Smith, L. A. et al., J. Am. Soc. Nephrol., 2006, PMID: 16928806), prompted us to investigate whether NIMA kinases modulate OSM-3-driven intraflagellar transport. We hypothesized that NEKL-3/4, as paralogs within this family, might directly phosphorylate OSM-3 to regulate its motility...”

      Page #4, line #26: 

      “... To determine whether NIMA kinase family members could directly phosphorylate OSM-3, we purified prokaryotic recombinant C. elegans NEKL-3/NEKL-4 and OSM3 protein in order to perform in vitro phosphorylation assays. We were able to obtain active recombinant NEKL-3 but not NEKL-4. The in vitro phosphorylation assays showed that NEKL-3, directly phosphorylates OSM-3 (Fig. 1A-B, Appendix Table S1). Subsequent mass spectrometric analysis revealed phosphorylation at residues 487-490, which localize to the conserved "YSTT" motif within OSM-3’s C-terminal tail region...”

      (3) It is curious the authors have not addressed the cilia phenotype and the localization of OSM-3 in nekl-3 mutant. Regardless of whether these observations agrees with the proposed mechanisms, it is essential for the authors to show and discuss the cilia phenotype and OSM-3 localization in nekl-3 mutants.

      We thank the Reviewer for highlighting this critical point. Indeed, nekl-3 null mutants are inviable due to essential mitotic roles (Barstead et al., 2012, PMID: 23173093), precluding direct analysis of ciliary phenotypes. To bypass this limitation, we recently generated nekl-3 conditional knockouts (cKOs) in ciliated neurons (Huang et al., JCB, 2025 in press, attached). In these mutants, OSM-3—which is normally enriched in the ciliary distal segment—becomes uniformly distributed along the cilium. This redistribution correlates with premature activation of OSM-3-driven anterograde motility in the ciliary middle region, consistent with our proposed model where NEKL3 phosphorylation suppresses OSM-3 activity. We have now integrated this result and discussion into the revised manuscript, reinforcing the physiological relevance of NEKL-3-mediated regulation in ciliary transport. 

      Page #6 line #10

      “… While nekl-3 null mutants are inviable due to essential mitotic roles (Barstead et al., 2012, PMID: 23173093), conditional knockout (cKO) of nekl-3 in ciliated neurons (Huang et al., JCB, 2025 in press, attached) revealed its critical role in regulating OSM3 dynamics. In nekl-3 cKO animals, OSM-3—normally enriched in the ciliary distal segment—redistributed uniformly along the cilium, concomitant with premature activation of anterograde motility in the middle ciliary region. This phenotype aligns with our model wherein NEKL-3 phosphorylation suppresses OSM-3 activity, ensuring spatiotemporal regulation of IFT.…”

      (4) The methods section lacks some information, which is critical to reproducing this study.

      We have now provided detailed information in the methods section in the revised manuscript.

      (a) It is not described how the authors determined phosphorylation of OSM-3 by NEKL-3. In methods, nothing is described about the assay.

      We performed in vitro phosphorylation assays using recombinant OSM-3 and NEKL3 purified from bacteria. We then used LC-MS/MS for identification of phosphorylation sites. We have now updated the methods section to include all the information.

      Page #4 line #26

      “... To determine whether NIMA kinase family members could directly phosphorylate OSM-3, we purified prokaryotic recombinant C. elegans NEKL-3/NEKL-4 and OSM3 protein in order to perform in vitro phosphorylation assays. We were able to obtain active recombinant NEKL-3 but not NEKL-4. The in vitro phosphorylation assays showed that NEKL-3, directly phosphorylates OSM-3 (Fig. 1A-B, Appendix Table S1). Subsequent mass spectrometric analysis revealed phosphorylation at residues 487-490, which localize to the conserved "YSTT" motif within OSM-3’s C-terminal tail region...”

      Page #36, line #19

      “In vitro phosphorylation assay 20 μM purified OSM-3 was incubated with 1 μM GST-NEKL-3 at 30 °C in 100 μL reaction buffer (50 mM Tris-HCl pH 8.0, 10 mM MgCl2, 150 mM NaCl, and 2 mM ATP) for 30 min. The reaction was terminated by boiling for 5 min with an SDS-sample buffer.

      Mass spectrometry

      Following NEKL-3 treatment, OSM-3 proteins were resolved by SDS-PAGE and visualized with Coomassie Brilliant Blue staining. Protein bands corresponding to OSM-3 were excised and subjected to digestion using the following protocol: reduction with 5 mM TCEP at 56°C for 30 min; alkylation with 10 mM iodoacetamide in darkness for 45 min at room temperature, and tryptic digestion at 37°C overnight with a 1:20 enzyme-to-protein ratio. The resulting peptides were subjected to mass spectrometry analysis. Briefly, the peptides were analyzed using an UltiMate 3000 RSLCnano system coupled to an Orbitrap Fusion Lumos mass spectrometer (Thermo Fisher Scientific). We applied an in-house proteome discovery searching algorithm to search the MS/MS data against the C. elegans database. Phosphorylation sites were determined using PhosphoRS algorithm with manual validation of MS/MS spectra.”

      (b) The method of structural prediction by Alfafold2 and LocalColabFold needs clarification. In general, the prediction gives several candidates. How did the authors choose one of these candidates?

      We generated five candidate models and all of them showed similar conformation. We thus chose the model with the highest confidence. We have provided PAE and pLDDT as additional data in Figure S2 and discussed them in the revised text on, Page #4, line #32: 

      “...To gain structural insights from this motif, we employed LocalColabFold based on AlphaFold2 to predict the dimeric structure of OSM-3 (Evans et al., 2022; Jumper et al., 2021; Mirdita et al., 2022). The highest-confidence model was selected for further analysis (Fig. 1C, Fig. S2)...”

      (c) The methods to predict conformational changes by introducing various point mutations are interesting (Figure 3). However, the methods require more detailed descriptions. In the current form, the manuscript only lists the tools used. The pipelines and parameters need to be described. This information is important because AlphaFoldbased predictions often give folded conformations because the training data are mainly composed of folded proteins. It is surprising that the methods applied here give open conformations induced by point mutations.

      We have described the pipelines in the revised Methods section on page#34, line#25: 

      “…OSM-3 model was predicted using LocalColabFold (Evans et al., 2022; Jumper et al., 2021; Mirdita et al., 2022). Mutated proteins were designed by Pymol 2.6, choosing the rotamer of the mutated residues in G444E, PM and PD models with the least clash as the initial conformation. To predict mutation-induced conformational changes, the initial models were subjected to Pyrosetta (Chaudhury et al., 2010). The energies of pre-relaxed models were evaluated with Rosetta Energy Function 2015 (Alford et al., 2017), and then the relax procedure were applied to the models with default parameters to obtain the relaxed models visualized by Pymol to minimize the energy of these models. In detail, to obtain the relaxed models visualized by Pymol and minimize the energy of these models, the classic relax mover was used in the procedure mentioned above with default settings. The relax script has been uploaded to Github: https://github.com/young55775/RosettaRelax_for_OSM3...”

      (5) The authors have purified proteins. Do they show different properties in gel filtration that are consistent with the structural prediction? It is anticipated that open-form mutants are eluted from earlier than closed forms.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful suggestion. Indeed, our recent study supported that the open-from of the active OSM-3 G444E mutation were eluted earlier than the wild-type closed form (Xie et al., EMBO J., 2024). While the current study did not perform gel filtration chromatography (SEC) to directly compare the hydrodynamic properties of the OSM-3 mutants, our functional assays provide robust evidence for conformational changes predicted by structural modeling. For example: ATPase activity assays revealed that the open-state mutants (e.g., G444E and PD muatnts) exhibited significantly enhanced enzymatic activity (Figure 4A), consistent with structural predictions of an active, destabilized autoinhibitory interface (Figure 3A). These functional readouts collectively validate the predicted structural states. While SEC could further corroborate these findings by distinguishing compact (closed) versus extended (open) conformations, we prioritized assays that directly link structural predictions to in vitro enzymatic activity and in vivo ciliary transport dynamics. Future studies incorporating SEC or cryo-EM will provide additional biophysical validation of these states.

      We have revised the text in the manuscript (Page #7, Lines #22): 

      “…Notably, the open-state OSM-3 mutants (e.g., G444E) displayed elevated ATPase activity, consistent with structural predictions of autoinhibition release (Fig. 3A, Fig. 4A) (Xie et al., 2024). While hydrodynamic profiling (e.g., SEC) could further resolve conformational states, our functional assays directly connect predicted structural changes to altered biochemical and cellular activity...”

      Minor point

      (1) Line 85 "MIMA kinase family" should be "NIMA kinase family".

      We have corrected the typo and appreciate that the Reviewer for pointing it out. 

      (2) M.S. and D.S. need to be defined in Figure 2D.

      We have updated the figures.

    1. Author Response

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      1) The main issue relates to Set2, and how STIM1 expression rescues Set2-dependent functions in Set2 KO flies. If Set2 is downstream of STIM1, how would STIM1 over-expression rescue a Set2-dependent effect?

      STIM rescue is of Set2 knockdown (RNAi) and NOT Set2 Knockout flies. Over expression of STIM raises SOCE in primary cultures of Drosophila neurons (as demonstrated in previous publications from our group: Agrawal et al., 2010; Chakraborty et al, 2016; Deb et al., 2016). The higher SOCE drives greater expression of Set2 from the endogenous locus thus reducing the efficacy of Set2 RNAi. Hence the rescue by STIM of Set2 KD flies in Figure S2E. We have explained this in lines 227-234.

      2) There is still no characterization of SOCE in fpDANs from flies expressing native Orai or the dominant negative OraiE180A mutant.

      Measurement of SOCE is not technically feasible in ex-vivo preps due to the presence of extracellular calcium in the brain milieu. In the past we have measured SOCE from primary cultures of central dopaminergic neurons expressing either native Orai OR OraiE180A mutant (Pathak et al., 2015) where we found that all dopaminergic neurons expressing OraiE180A exhibit very low SOCE. This is the reason we have not measured SOCE in the fewer cells of the fpDAN subset marked by THD' GAL4. This point has been specifically mentioned and explained in the section on “limitations of the study” at the end of the manuscript.

      3) The revised version does not include an analysis of the STIM:Orai stoichiometry, which has been demonstrated to be essential for SOCE.

      To measure such stoichiometry we would need to perform direct measurements of STIM and Orai levels by protein extraction from the fpDANs of all appropriate genotypes. This is not feasible due to the small number of cells available from each brain.

      I confirm that there are no changes to the text OR figures from the previous version of the manuscript.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      […]

      The manuscript by Mitra and coworkers analyses the functional role of Orai in the excitability of central dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila. The authors show that a dominant-negative mutant of Orai (OraiE180A) significantly alters the gene expression profile of flight-promoting dopaminergic neurons (fpDANs). Among them, OraiE180A attenuates the expression of Set2 and enhances that of E(z) shifting the level of epigenetic signatures that modulate gene expression. The present results also demonstrate that Set2 expression via Orai involves the transcription factor Trl. The Orai-Trl-Set1 pathway modulates the expression of VGCC, which, in turn, are involved in dopamine release. The topic investigated is interesting and timely and the study is carefully performed and technically sound; however, there are several major concerns that need to be addressed:

      1) In Figure S2E, STIM is overexpressed in the absence of Set2 and this leads to rescue. It is presumed that STIM overexpression causes excess SOCE, yet this is rarely the case. Perhaps the bigger concern, however, is how excess SOCE might overcome the loss of SET2 if SET2 mediates SOCE-induced development of flight. These data are more consistent with something other than SET2 mediating this function.

      Our statement that STIM overexpression overcomes deficits in SOCE is based on the following published work, which has been highlighted in the revised version of the manuscript (see Lines 226-233):

      1. Studies of SOCE in wildtype cultured larval Drosophila neurons demonstrated that overexpression of STIM raised SOCE to the same extent as co-expression of STIM and Orai in the WT background (Chakraborty et al, 2016; Figure 1D).

      2. Both Carbachol-induced IP3-mediated Ca2+ release and SOCE (measured by Ca2+ add back after Thapsigargin-induced store depletion) were rescued in primary cultures of IP3R hypomorphic mutant (itprku) Drosophila neurons by overexpression of STIM (Agrawal et al., 2010; Figure 8A-G).

      3. Deb et al., 2016 (Supplementary Figure 2h,i) reaffirmed that overexpression of STIM significantly improves SOCE after Thapsigargin-induced passive store-depletion in Drosophila neurons expressing IP3RRNAi.

      4. Consistent with the cellular rescue of SOCE, defects in flight initiation and physiology observed in the heteroallelic IP3R hypomorphic background (itprku) could be rescued by overexpression of STIM (Agrawal et al., 2010; Figure 3A-E) as well as Orai (Venkiteswaran and Hasan, 2009; Figure 3).

      5. In Figure S2E, we show that flight deficits arising from THD’> Set2RNAi are rescued upon overexpression of STIM (i.e. THD’>Set2RNAi; STIMOE). Here and in another recent publication (Mitra et al., 2021) we show that neurons expressing Set2RNAi exhibit reduced expression of the IP3R and reduced ER-Ca2+ release presumably leading to reduced SOCE. As mentioned above we have consistently found that STIM overexpression raises both IP3-mediated Ca2+ release and SOCE in Drosophila neurons.

      In this study, we propose that Ca2+ release through the IP3R followed by SOCE are part of a positive feedback loop (described in the revised manuscript- see Lines 302-307) driving expression of Set2 which in turn upregulates expression of mAChR and IP3R (Figure 3F) to regulate dopaminergic neuron function. Our observation that loss of Set2 (THD’>Set2RNAi) can be rescued by STIM overexpression is consistent with this model because:

      1. Loss of Set2 (THD’>Set2RNAi) results in downregulation of several genes including mAChR and IP3R leading to decreased SOCE.

      2. As evident from our previous studies increased STIM expression in the Set2RNAi background (THD’>Set2RNAi; STIMOE) is expected to enhance SOCE which we predict would rescue Set2 expression leading to rescue of other Set2 dependent downstream functions like flight (Figure 2D).

      2) In Figure 3, data is provided linking SET2 expression and Cch-induced Ca2+ responses. The presentation of these data is confusing. In addition, the results may be a simple side effect of SET2-dependent expression of IP3R. Given that this article is about SOCE, why isn't SOCE shown here? More generally, there are no measurements of SOCE in this entire article. Measuring SOCE (not what is measured in response to Cch) could help eliminate some of this confusion.

      This section has been re-written in the revised version for better clarity and we have explained how Set2-dependent IP3R expression is an important component of Orai-mediated Ca2+ entry in fpDANs (see Lines 302-307). Here, we propose that IP3-mediated Ca2+ release and SOCE, through Orai, are together part of a positive feedback loop (see Lines 286-307) driving transcription of Set2 which in turn upregulates mAChR and IP3R expression (Figure 3F). We hypothesized that the observed loss of CCh-induced Ca2+ response in the Set2RNAi background (Figure 3B-D; THD’>Set2RNAi) results from decreased itpr and mAChR expression and verified this in Figure 3E. This is further validated by the rescue of CCh-induced Ca2+ response and itpr/mAChR expression in the OraiE180A background upon Set2 overexpression (Figure 3B-E; THD’>OraiE180A; Set2OE). We were constrained to measure CCh-induced Ca2+ responses in OraiE180A expressing neurons for the following reasons (highlighted in the revised version of the manuscript- (See Lines 307-313; ‘Limitations of the study’-Lines 719-735):

      1. SOCE measurements through Tg mediated store Ca2+ release followed by Ca2+ add back require a 0 Ca2+ environment that can only be achieved in culture. The Drosophila brain is bathed in hemolymph which contains Ca2+ and there do not exist any methods to readily deplete Ca2+ from the tissue to create a 0 Ca2+ environment without also effecting the health of the neurons.

      2. Cultures of the subset of dopaminergic neurons (THD’) we have focused on in this study were not feasible due to the small number of neurons being studied from the total number of dopaminergic neurons in the brain (~35/400). In previous studies we have shown that SOCE post-Tg induced store depletion is abrogated in cultured dopaminergic neurons from Drosophila upon expression of OraiE180A (Pathak et al., 2015). Furthermore, Carbachol-induced IP3-mediated Ca2+ release is tightly coupled to SOCE in Drosophila neurons (Venkiteswaran and Hasan, 2009) and Ca2+ release from the IP3R is physiologically relevant for flight behavior in THD’ neurons (Sharma and Hasan, 2020).

      3) A significant gap in the study relates to the conclusion that trl is a SOCE-regulated transcription factor. This conclusion is entirely based on genetic analysis of STIMKO heterozygous flies in which a copy of the trl13C hypomorph allele is introduced. While these results suggest a genetic interaction between the expression of the two genes, the evidence that expression translates into a functional interaction that places trl immediately downstream of SOCE is not rigorous or convincing. All that can be said is that the double mutant shows a defect in flight which could arise from an interruption of the circuit. Further, it is not clear whether the trl13C hypomorph is only introduced during the critical 72-96 hour time window when the Orai1E180E phenotype shows up. The same applies to the over-expression of Set2 and the other genes. If the expression is not temporally controlled, then the phenotype could be due to the blockade of an entirely different aspect of flight neuron function.

      The idea that Trl functions downstream of Orai-mediated Ca2+ entry in THD’ neurons is based on the following genetic evidence (highlighted in the revised version; see Lines 339-341; 351-367; 647-65; ‘Limitations of the study’: 736-739)

      1. In Figure 4D, we show evidence of genetic interaction between trl-STIM and trl-Set2. The rescue of trl13c/STIMKO with STIM overexpression in THD’ neurons indicates that excess SOCE (driven by STIMOE) may activate the residual Trl (there exists a WT Trl copy in this genetic background) to rescue THD’ flight function. This is further supported by the rescue of trl/STIMKO with Set2 overexpression in THD’ neurons, which is consistent with the feedback loop model proposed in Figure 5C (see Lines 390-396) where we propose that reduced SOCE leads to reduced ‘activated’ Trl and thus reduced Set2 expression, and the latter is rescued by SET2OE . The manner in which SOCE ‘activates’ Trl is the subject of ongoing investigations.

      2. The trl hypomorphic alleles (including trl13C) exist as genetic mutants and they affect Trl function in all tissues throughout development. While we concede that these mutant alleles would affect multiple functions at other stages of development, which may impinge on the phenotypes noted in Figure S4B, we have used a targeted RNAi approach to validate Trl function specifically in the THD’ neurons (see Figure 4C; Lines 339-341).

      3. Overexpression mediated rescues (including Set2) were not induced only during the critical 72-96 hrs APF developmental window. Having established that Orai function drives critical gene expression during this window (Figure 1), it is reasonable to assume that Set2 rescue of loss of flight in OraiE180A occurs in the same time window where flight is disrupted (see Lines 221-224).

      4) In Figure 4, data is shown that SOCE compensates for the loss of Trl, the presumed mediator of SOCE-dependent flight. The fact that flight deficits are rescued by raising SOCE in the absence of Trl is very inconsistent with this conclusion.

      We apologise for this confusion and have clarified in the revision (see Lines 346-367). trl13c is a recessive allele of Trl and has been written as such throughout the text and in the figures (i.e trl13c and NOT Trl13c). In all cases of Trl mutant rescue by STIMOE and Set2OE there exists residual Trl that can be activated by excess SOCE thus leading to the rescue. This is true for trl13C/ STIMKO where each mutant is present as a heterozygote (the complete genotype of this strain is STIMKO/+; trl13c/+; this has been corrected in the revision). Similarly, for TrlRNAi we expect reduced levels (but not complete loss) of Trl. Thus the SOCE rescue of loss of Trl occurs in conditions where Trl levels are reduced but NOT absent. Homozygous trl null mutants are lethal.

      5) In Figure 5 (A-C), data is provided that Trl transcripts are unaffected by loss of SOCE and that overexpression cannot rescue flightlessness. From this, the authors conclude that this gene "must" be calcium responsive. While that is one possibility, it is also possible that these genes are not functionally linked.

      The idea that Trl is functionally linked to SOCE is based on the following evidence (included in the revised version- see Lines 339-341; 346-367; 391-396)

      1. In Figure 4C we show that flight defects caused by partial loss of Trl (THD’>TrlRNAi) were rescued by STIM overexpression (THD’>TrlRNAi; STIMOE). As mentioned above we have found that STIM overexpression raises SOCE.

      2. Heteroalleles of the trl13C hypomorph exhibit a strong genetic interaction with a single copy of the null allele of STIMKO as shown by the flight deficit of trl13c/+; STIMKO/+ (trl13C/STIMKO ) flies (Figure 4D). The genotypes will be corrected in the revision.

      3. Flight defects in trl13C/STIMKO flies could be rescued by STIM overexpression in the THD’ neurons (trl13C/STIMKO; THD’>STIMOE)

      4. In Figure 4E, we show that partial loss of Trl in THD’ neurons (THD’>TrlRNAi) leads to decreased expression of the Ca2+ responsive genes mAChR, itpr, and Set2 genes indicating that Trl is a constituent of the SOCE-driven transcriptional feedback loop (see Figure 5C).

      Since we could not detect a well-defined Ca2+ binding domain in Trl, we hypothesize that it could be activated by a Ca2+ dependent post-translational modification. Phosphoproteome analysis of Trl demonstrated that it does indeed undergo phosphorylation at a Threonine residue (T237; Zhai et al., 2008), which lies within a potential site for CaMKII. Independently, CaMKII has been identified as a binding partner of Trl from a Trl interactome study (Lomaev et al., 2018). Past work from our group (Ravi et al., 2018) identified a role for CaMKII in THD’ neurons in the context of flight. We are currently testing if CaMKII functions downstream of SOCE in THD’ neurons to mediate flight and will update this information in the next version of the manuscript.

      Now included in the revised version of the manuscript as Figure S5; Lines 397-424)

      6) There is no characterization of SOCE in fpDANs from flies expressing native Orai or the dominant negative OraiE180A mutant. While the authors refer to previous studies, as the manuscript is essentially based on Orai function thapsigargin-induced SOCE should be tested using the Ca2+ add-back protocol in order to assess the release of Ca2+ from the ER in response to thapsigargin as well as the subsequent SOCE.

      The fpDANs consist of 16-19 neurons in each hemisphere (PPL1 are 10-12 and PPM3 are 6-7 cells; Pathak et al., 2015). Measuring SOCE from these neurons in vivo is not possible due to the presence of abundant extracellular Ca2+ in the brain. Given their sparse number, it proved technically challenging to isolate the fpDANs in culture to perform SOCE measurements using the Ca2+ add back protocol. Due to these reasons, we have relied upon using Carbachol to elicit IP3-mediated Ca2+ release and SOCE as a proxy for in vivo SOCE. In previous studies we have shown that Carbachol treatment of cultured Drosophila neurons elicits IP3-mediated Ca2+ release and SOCE (Agrawal et al., 2010; Figure 8). Moreover, expression of OraiE180A completely blocks SOCE as measured in primary cultures of dopaminergic neurons (Pathak et al., 2015; Figure 1E). Hence we have not repeated SOCE measurements from all dopaminergic neurons in this work. In the revised version we have explicitly stated this weakness of our study and the reasons for it (See Lines 307-313; ‘Limitations of the study’-Lines 719-735).

      7) In the experiments performed to rescue flight duration in Set2RNAi individuals the authors overexpress STIM and attribute the effect to "Excess STIM presumably drives higher SOCE sufficient to rescue flight bout durations caused by deficient Set2 levels.". This should be experimentally tested as the STIM:Orai stoichiometry has been demonstrated as essential for SOCE.

      The assumption that STIM overexpression drives higher SOCE is based upon previously published work from Drosophila neurons (Agrawal et al., 2010; Chakraborty et al, 2016; Deb et al., 2016) which demonstrates that excess WT STIM overcomes IP3R deficiencies (RNAi or hypomorphic mutants) to rescue SOCE. We agree that STIM-Orai stoichiometry is essential for SOCE, and propose that the rescue backgrounds possess sufficient WT Orai, which is recruited by the excess STIM to mediate the rescue. We have referenced the earlier work to validate our use of STIMOE for rescue of SOCE (See Lines 226-233).

      Here, we propose that Set2 is part of a positive feedback loop (see Lines 286-307) driving transcription of mAChR and IP3R (Figure 3F). In keeping with this hypothesis, we posit that the phenotypes observed in the Set2RNAi background (Figure 2D) result from decreased itpr and mAChR expression (validated in Figure 3E). This is further validated by the Set2 overexpression mediated rescue of OraiE180A (Figure 2D) and rescue of itpr/mAChR expression in the OraiE180A background (Figure 3B-E; THD’>OraiE180A; Set2OE).

      8) The authors show that overexpression of OraiE108A results in Stim downregulation at a mRNA level. What about the protein level? And more important, how does OraiE108A downregulate Stim expression? Does it promote Stim degradation? Does it inhibit Stim expression?

      We hypothesize that changes in STIM mRNA observed in the THD’ > OraiE180A neurons stems from an overall reduction in IP3-mediated Ca2+ release and SOCE due to loss of Trl-Set2 driven gene expression detailed in our transcriptional feedback loop model (Figure 5C; see Lines 286-307; 581-591). We have attempted to explain this aspect more clearly in the revised version of the manuscript. While we agree that measuring levels of STIM protein would be helpful, estimation of protein levels from a limited number of neurons (~35 cells per brain) is technically challenging. The STIM antibody does not work well in immunohistochemistry. In the absence of any experimental evidence we cannot comment on how expression of OraiE180A might affect STIM protein turnover (see Lines 307-313).

      9) Lines 271-273, the authors state "whereas overexpression of a transgene encoding Set2 in THD' neurons either with loss of SOCE (OraiE180A) or with knockdown of the IP3R (itprRNAi), lead to significant rescue of the Ca2+ response". This is attributed to a positive effect of Set2 expression on IP3R expression and the authors show a positive correlation between these two parameters; however, there is no demonstration that Set2 expression can rescue IP3R expression in cells where the IP3R is knocked down (itprRNAi). This should be further demonstrated.

      The rescue of IP3R expression by Set2 overexpression in itprRNAi was demonstrated in a different set of Drosophila neurons in an earlier study (Mitra et al., 2021) and has not been repeated specifically in THD’ neurons (see Lines 286-307). Similar to the previous study, here we tested CCh stimulated Ca2+ responses of THD’ neurons with itprRNAi and itprRNAi; SetOE (Fig S3), which are indeed rescued by SET2OE see Lines 280-285)

      10) The data presented in Figure 3E should be functionally demonstrated by analyzing the ability of CCh to release Ca2+ from the intracellular stores in the absence of extracellular Ca2+.

      CCh-mediated Ca2+ release from the intracellular stores in the absence of extracellular Ca2+ has been described in primary cultures of Drosophila neurons in previously published work (Venkiteswaran and Hasan, 2009; Agrawal et al., 2010) This work focuses on a set of 16-19 dopaminergic neurons in a hemisphere of the Drosophila central brain. It is technically challenging to generate a 0 Ca2+ environment in vivo, which is essential for measuring store Ca2+ release. Given their meagre numbers, primary cultures of these neurons is not readily feasible. (see Lines 307-313; ‘Limitations of the study’-Lines 719-735)

      11) The conclusion that SOCE regulates the neuronal excitability threshold is based entirely on either partial behavioral rescue of flight, or measurements of KCl-induced Ca2+ rises monitored by GCaMP6m in DAN neurons. The threshold for neuronal excitability is a precise parameter based on rheobase measurements of action potentials in current-clamp. Measurements of slow calcium signals using a slow dye such as GCaMp6m should not be equated with neuronal excitability. What is measured is a loss of the calcium response in high K depolarization experiments, which occurs due to the loss of expression of Cav channels. Hence, the use of this term is not accurate and will confuse readers. The use of terms referring to neuronal excitability needs to be changed throughout the manuscript. As such, the conclusions regarding neuronal excitability should be strongly tempered and the data reinterpreted as there are no true measurements of neuronal excitability in the manuscript. All that can be said is that expression of certain ion channel genes is suppressed. Since both Na+ channels and K+ channel expression is down-regulated, it is hard to say precisely how membrane excitability is altered without action potential analysis.

      The claim that SOCE influences neuronal excitability is based on the following observations:

      1. Interruption of the transcriptional feedback loop involving SOCE, Trl, and Set2 through loss of any of its constituents, results in the downregulation of VGCCs (Figure 5G, 6H), which are essential components of action potentials.

      2. OraiE180A mediated loss of SOCE in THD’ neurons abrogates the KCl-evoked depolarization response (Figure 6B, C) measured using GCaMP6m. We verified that this response requires VGCC function using pharmacological inhibition of L-type VGCCs (Figure 6E, F).

      3. SOCE deficient THD’ neurons, which were presumably compromised in their ability to evoke action potentials could be rescued to undergo KCl-evoked depolarisation by expression of NachBac, which lowers the depolarization threshold (Figure 7C, D) or through optogenetic stimulation using CsChrimson (Figure 7F).

      We agree that ‘neuronal excitability threshold’ is a precise electrophysiological parameter that has not been directly investigated here by measurement of action potentials. Therefore, references to neuronal excitability have been tempered throughout the revised manuscript and be replaced with a more generic reference to ‘neuronal activity’. In this context we have included further evidence supporting reduced activity of THD’ neurons upon loss of SOCE in the revision.

      Since one of the key functional outcomes of activity during critical developmental periods such as the 72-96 hrs APF developmental window identified in this study, is remodelling of neuronal morphology, we decided to investigate the same in our context. Neuronal activity can drive changes in neurite complexity and axonal arborization (Depetris-Chauvin et al., 2011) especially during critical developmental periods (Sachse et al., 2007). To understand if Orai mediated Ca2+ entry and downstream gene expression through Set2 affects this activity-driven parameter, we investigated the morphology of fpDANs, and specifically measured the complexity of presynaptic terminals within the 2’1 lobe MB using super-resolution microscopy. We found striking changes in the neurite volume upon expression of OraiE180A which could be rescued by restoring either Set2 (OraiE180A; Set2OE) or by inducing hyperactivity through NachBac expression (OraiE180A ; NachBacOE). These data have been included in the revised manuscript (Figure 8 B, C, D; see Lines 481-482; 519-534; 584-591; 701-704).

      12) Related, since trl does not contain any molecular domains that could be regulated by Ca2+ signaling, it is unclear whether trl is directly regulated by SOCE or the regulation is highly indirect. Reporter assays evaluating trl activation upon Ca2+ rises would provide much stronger and more direct evidence for the conclusion that trl is a SOCE-regulated TF. As such the evidence is entirely based on RNAi downregulation of trl which indicates that trl is essential but has no bearing on exactly what point of the signaling cascade it is involved.

      We agree that luciferase Trl reporters would provide a direct method to test SOCE-mediated activation. Future investigations will be targeted in this direction. Regarding possible mechanisms of Trl activation - since we could not detect a well-defined Ca2+ binding domain in Trl, we hypothesize that it may be phosphorylation by a Ca2+ sensitive kinase. Phosphoproteome analysis of Trl indicates that it does indeed undergo phosphorylation at a Threonine reside (T237; Zhai et al., 2008), which may be mediated by the Ca2+ sensitive kinase-CaMKII based on binding partners identified in the Trl interactome (Lomaev et al., 2018; Past work (Ravi et al., 2018) has indeed demonstrated a requirement for CaMKII in THD’ neurons for flight. We are currently testing whether CaMKII functions downstream of SOCE in these neurons to mediate flight, and will be updating this information in the next version of the manuscript.

      New data and analysis has been included - see Figure S5; ‘Limitations of the study’- Lines 397-424; 736-739).

      13) Are NFAT levels altered in the Orai1 loss of function mutant? If not, this should be explicitly stated. It would seem based on previous literature that some gene regulation may be related to the downregulation of this established Ca2+-dependent transcription factor. Same for NFkb.

      As mentioned in the revised version of the manuscript (see Lines 315-326), Drosophila NFAT lacks a calcineurin binding site and is therefore not sensitive to Ca2+ (Keyser et al., 2007). In the past we tested if knockdown of NF-kB in dopaminergic neurons gave a flight phenotype and did not observe any measurable deficit. From the RNAseq data we find a slight downregulation of NFAT (0.49 fold, p value=0.048) and NF-kb (0.26 fold, p value =0.258) the significance of which is unclear at this point. We did not find any consensus binding sites for these two factors in the regulatory regions of downregulated genes from THD’ neurons.

      14) Does over-expression of Set2 restore ion channel expression especially those of the VGCCs? This would provide rigorous, direct evidence that SOCE-mediated regulation of VGCCs through Set2 controls voltage-gated calcium channel signaling.

      Set2 overexpression in the OraiE180A background indeed restores the expression of VGCC genes (see Figure 6H; Lines 461-468).

      15) All 6 representative panels from Figure 3B are duplicated in Figure 4G. Likewise, 2 representative panels from Figure 5H are duplicated in Figure 6D. Although these panels all represent the results from control experiments, the relevant experiments were likely not conducted at the same time and under the same conditions. Thus, control images from other experiments should not be used simply because they correspond to controls. This situation should be clarified.

      We regret the confusion caused by the same representative images for the control experiments. These have been replaced by new representative images for Figure 4G and 6D in the updated version of the manuscript.

      16) The figures are unusually busy and difficult to follow. In part this is because they usually have many panels (Fig. 1: A-I; Fig. 2, A-J, etc) but also because the arrangement of the panels is not consistent: sometimes the following panel is found to the right, other times it is below. It would help the reader to make the order of the panels consistent, and, if possible, reduce the number of panels and/or move some of the panels to new figures (eLife does not limit the number of display items).

      The image panels have been rearranged for ease of reading in the updated version of the manuscript.

      17) As a final recommendation, the reviewers suggest that the authors a- Reword the text that refers to membrane excitability since membrane excitability was not directly measured here. b-Explain why STIM1 rescues the partial loss of flight in Set2 RNAi flies (Fig. S2E); and c- Explain how/why trl is calcium regulated and test using luciferase (or other) reporter assays whether Orai activation leads to trl activation.

      a. Textual references to membrane excitability have been appropriately modified and some new data has been included in this regard (see Figure 8 B, C, D; Lines 481-483; 519-534; 584-591; 701-704).

      b. We have provided a detailed explanation for how STIM overexpression might rescue the phenotypes caused by Set2RNAi in Point 1 (see Lines 226-233). In short, these phenotypes depend upon IP3R mediated Ca2+ entry driving a transcriptional feedback loop. We relied upon past reports that STIM overexpression upregulates IP3R-mediated Ca2+ release and SOCE in Drosophila itpr mutant neurons (Agrawal et al., 2010; Chakraborty et al, 2016; Deb et al, 2016). We therefore propose that STIM overexpression in the Set2RNAi background rescues IP3R mediated Ca2+ release followed by SOCE, which drives enhanced Set2 transcription, counteracting the effects of the RNAi. We will explain this more clearly with past references in the next revision.

      c. We have provided a detailed response to this comment in Point 12. Briefly, we agree that building luciferase reporters for Trl could be an ideal strategy to test for its responsiveness to SOCE and needs to be done in future. As an alternate strategy, we have looked at data from existing studies of interacting partners of Trl (Lomaev et al., 2017) and identified CamKII, which is both Ca2+ responsive (Braun and Schulman, 1995; Yasuda et al., 2022), and thus might activate Trl through a phosphorylation-switch like mechanism (see Figure S5; ‘Limitations of the study’-736-739; Lines 397-424). Moreover, a previous publication identified a requirement for CamKII in THD’ neurons for Drosophila flight (Ravi et al., 2018). We have tested the ability of a dominant active version of CamKII to rescue THD’>E180A flight deficits and have included this information in the next version of the manuscript.

      References

      1. Agrawal N, Venkiteswaran G, Sadaf S, Padmanabhan N, Banerjee S, Hasan G. Inositol 1,4,5-Trisphosphate Receptor and dSTIM Function in Drosophila Insulin-Producing Neurons Regulates Systemic Intracellular Calcium Homeostasis and Flight. J Neurosci. 2010;30:1301-1313. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3668-09.2010

      2. Braun AP, Schulman H. A non-selective cation current activated via the multifunctional Ca(2+)-calmodulin-dependent protein kinase in human epithelial cells. J Physiol. 1995. 488:37-55. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020944

      3. Chakraborty S, Deb BK, Chorna T, Konieczny V, Taylor CW, Hasan G. Mutant IP3 receptors attenuate store-operated Ca2+ entry by destabilizing STIM-Orai interactions in Drosophila neurons. J Cell Sci. 2016. 129:3903-3910. doi:10.1242/jcs.191585

      4. Deb BK, Pathak T, Hasan G. Store-independent modulation of Ca2+ entry through Orai by Septin 7. Nat Commun. 2016. 7:11751. doi:10.1038/ncomms11751

      5. Depetris-Chauvin A, Berni J, Aranovich EJ, Muraro NI, Beckwith EJ, Ceriani MF. Adult-specific electrical silencing of pacemaker neurons uncouples molecular clock from circadian outputs. Curr Biol. 2011. 21:1783-1793. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.09.027.

      6. Keyser P, Borge-Renberg K, Hultmark D. The Drosophila NFAT homolog is involved in salt stress tolerance. Insect Biochem Mol Biol. 2007. 37:356-362. doi:10.1016/j.ibmb.2006.12.009

      7. Kilo L, Stürner T, Tavosanis G, Ziegler AB. Drosophila Dendritic Arborisation Neurons: Fantastic Actin Dynamics and Where to Find Them. Cells. 2021. 10:2777. doi:10.3390/cells10102777

      8. Lomaev D, Mikhailova A, Erokhin M, et al. The GAGA factor regulatory network: Identification of GAGA factor associated proteins. PLoS One. 2017. 12:e0173602. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0173602

      9. Mitra R, Richhariya S, Jayakumar S, Notani D, Hasan G. IP3/Ca2+ signals regulate larval to pupal transition under nutrient stress through the H3K36 methyltransferase dSET2. Development. 2021. 148:dev199018. doi:10.1101/2020.11.25.399329

      10. Pathak T, Agrawal T, Richhariya S, Sadaf S, Hasan G. Store-Operated Calcium Entry through Orai Is Required for Transcriptional Maturation of the Flight Circuit in Drosophila. J Neurosci. 2015. 35:13784-13799. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1680-15.2015

      11. Ravi P, Trivedi D, Hasan G. FMRFa receptor stimulated Ca2+ signals alter the activity of flight modulating central dopaminergic neurons in Drosophila melanogaster. Barsh GS, ed. PLOS Genet. 2018. 14:e1007459. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1007459

      12. Sachse S, Rueckert E, Keller A, Okada R, Tanaka NK, Ito K, Vosshall LB. Activity-dependent plasticity in an olfactory circuit. Neuron. 2007. 56:838-50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.035.

      13. Sharma A, Hasan G. Modulation of flight and feeding behaviours requires presynaptic IP3Rs in dopaminergic neurons. Elife. 2020;9. e62297.doi:10.7554/elife.62297

      14. Venkiteswaran G, Hasan G. Intracellular Ca2+ signalling and store operated Ca2+ entry are required in Drosophila neurons for flight. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2009.106:10326-10331. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0902982106

      15. Yasuda R, Hayashi Y, Hell JW. CaMKII: a central molecular organizer of synaptic plasticity, learning and memory. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2022. 23: 666-682 doi:10.1038/s41583-022-00624-2

      16. Zhai B, Villén J, Beausoleil SA, Mintseris J, Gygi SP. Phosphoproteome Analysis of Drosophila melanogaster Embryos. J Proteome Res. 2008. 7:1675-1682. doi:10.1021/pr700696a

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Ma & Yang et al. report a new investigation aimed at elucidating one of the key nutrients S. Typhimurium (STM) utilizes with the nutrient-poor intracellular niche within macrophage, focusing on the amino acid beta-alanine. From these data, the authors report that beta-alanine plays important roles in mediating STM infection and virulence. The authors employ a multidisciplinary approach that includes some mouse studies, and ultimately propose a mechanism by which panD, involved in B-Ala synthesis, mediates regulation of zinc homeostatisis in Salmonella.

      Strengths and weaknesses:

      The results and model are adequately supported by the authors' data. Further work will need to be performed to learn whether the Zn2+ functions as proposed in their mechanism. By performing a small set of confirmatory experiments in S. Typhi, the authors provide some evidence of relevance to human infections.

      Impact:

      This work adds to the body of literature on the metabolic flexibility of Salmonella during infection that enable pathogenesis.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      No further suggestions. The authors have adequately addressed my prior concerns through new data and revisions to the text.

      Thank you for considering this work. We appreciate your efforts in aiding us to improve our manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Salmonella is interesting due to its life within a compact compartment, which we call SCV or Salmonella containing vacuole in the field of Salmonella. SCV is a tight-fitting vacuole where the acquisition of nutrients is a key factor by Salmonella. The authors among many nutrients, focussed on beta-alanine. It is also known that Salmonella requires beta-alanine from many other studies. The authors have done in vitro RAW macrophage infection assays and In vivo mouse infection assays to see the life of Salmonella in the presence of beta-alanine. They concluded by comprehending that beta-alanine modulates the expression of many genes including zinc transporters which is required for pathogenesis.

      Strengths:

      Made a couple of knockouts in Salmonella and did transcriptomic to understand the global gene expression pattern

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Transport of Beta-alanine to SCV is not yet elucidated. Is it possible to determine whether the Zn transporter is involved in B-alanine transport?

      Thank you for the comment. Following your suggestion, we investigated the growth of Salmonella WT and the ∆znuA mutant cultured in N-minimal and M9 minimal medium, with β-alanine as the sole carbon source. We observed no significant difference in growth kinetics between the ∆znuA mutant and WT strain under either culture condition (please refer to Author response image 1). The results indicate that ZnuA is not involved in β-alanine transport in Salmonella.

      Author response image 1.

      (2) Beta-alanine can also be shuttled to form carnosine along with histidine. If beta-alanine is channelled to make more carnosine, then the virulence phenotypes may be very different.

      Our study reveals that β-alanine availability, whether obtained from the host or synthesized de novo via the panD-dependent pathway, is important for Salmonella pathogenesis. We have shown that β-alanine influences Salmonella intracellular replication and in vivo virulence partly by enhancing the expression of the zinc transporter genes.

      Although β-alanine can also be shuttled to form carnosine along with histidine in animals, the Salmonella genome lacks canonical carnosine synthase (CARNS) orthologs that catalyze the condensation of β-alanine and histidine into carnosine. Therefore, we believe that the carnosine biosynthetic pathway does not influence the virulence phenotypes of Salmonella.

      (3) Some amino acid transporters can be knocked out to see if beta-alanine uptake is perturbed. Like ArgT transport Arginine, and its mutation perturbs the uptake of beta-alanine. What is the beta-alanine concentration in the SCV? SCVS can be purified at different time points, and the Beta-alanine concentration can be measured

      Thank you for the comment. As suggested, we have investigated the role of other amino acid transporters in the uptake of β-alanine. In E. coli, GabP transports γ-aminobutyric acid (GABA), a structural analogue of β-alanine, and may also transport β-alanine (J Bacteriol. 2021, 203(4):e00642-20). Nevertheless, SalmonellagabP mutant displayed no growth defect in minimal medium with β-alanine as the sole carbon source (Figure 1_figure Supplement 7, Figure 1_figure Supplement 8), indicating that GabP is not involved in β-alanine uptake in Salmonella. Strikingly, the Δ_argT_ mutant—defective in arginine uptake—showed markedly decreased growth in the minimal medium with β-alanine as the sole carbon source (Figure 1F),suggesting that ArgT also transports β-alanine in Salmonella. We have added the results in the revised manuscript (lines 167-179).

      It has been reported that ArgT is essential for Salmonella replication within macrophages and full virulence in vivo (PloS one. 2010, 5(12):e15466). Given that ArgT is involved in both arginine and β-alanine uptake (as verified in this study), whether the attenuated virulence of the ∆argT mutant is due to a deficiency in β-alanine or arginine requires further investigation. We have also included a discussion on this issue (lines 409-415).

      In this work, to avoid delays and alterations in metabolite concentrations during the isolation of bacterial contents from macrophages, we directly assessed the combined metabolite concentrations within infected cells and Salmonella. It has been previously verified that these metabolites are primarily of host origin (Nat Commun. 2021, 12(1):879.). We noted a decrease in β-alanine levels in macrophages infected with Salmonella. The process of separating SCV is intricate and encompasses dissociation and sonication (Nat Commun. 2018, 9(1):2091). These steps may potentially result in alterations of metabolite concentrations during the separation procedure. Therefore, we did not measure the β-alanine concentration in the SCV.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The Authors have done meticulous experiments to address the questions asked by the reviewers. My one question of beta-alanine transport inside the SCV remains undone, though the authors have tried.

      Was Zinc transporter mutant checked? It is possible that the Zn transporter can take up Beta-alanine.

      Thank you for the comment. Following your suggestion, we investigated the growth of Salmonella WT and the ∆znuA mutant cultured in N-minimal and M9 minimal medium, with β-alanine as the sole carbon source. We observed no significant difference in growth kinetics between the ∆znuA mutant and WT strain under either culture condition (please refer to Author response image 1). The results indicate that ZnuA is not involved in β-alanine transport in Salmonella.

      Additionally, we have investigated the role of other amino acid transporters in the uptake of β-alanine and have ultimately identified that ArgT, the arginine transporter, is involved in the uptake of β-alanine in Salmonella (please refer to our previous response).

    1. Author Response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:<br /> The global decline of amphibians is primarily attributed to deadly disease outbreaks caused by the chytrid fungus, Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis (Bd). It is unclear whether and how skin-resident immune cells defend against Bd. Although it is well known that mammalian mast cells are crucial immune sentinels in the skin and play a pivotal role in the immune recognition of pathogens and orchestrating subsequent immune responses, the roles of amphibian mast cells during Bd infections are largely unknown. The current study developed a novel way to enrich X. laevis skin mast cells by injecting the skin with recombinant stem cell factor (SCF), a KIT ligand required for mast cell differentiation and survival. The investigators found an enrichment of skin mast cells provides X. laevis substantial protection against Bd and mitigates the inflammation-related skin damage resulting from Bd infection. Additionally, the augmentation of mast cells leads to increased mucin content within cutaneous mucus glands and shields frogs from the alterations to their skin microbiomes caused by Bd.

      Strengths:<br /> This study underscores the significance of amphibian skin-resident immune cells in defenses against Bd and introduces a novel approach to examining interactions between amphibian hosts and fungal pathogens.

      Weaknesses:<br /> The main weakness of the study is the lack of functional analysis of X. laevis mast cells. Upon activation, mast cells have the characteristic feature of degranulation to release histamine, serotonin, proteases, cytokines, and chemokines, etc. The study should determine whether X. laevis mast cells can be degranulated by two commonly used mast cell activators IgE and compound 48/80 for IgE-dependent and independent pathways. This can be easily done in vitro. It is also important to assess whether in vivo these mast cells are degranulated upon Bd infection using avidin staining to visualize vesicle releases from mast cells. Figure 3 only showed rSCF injection caused an increase in mast cells in naïve skin. They need to present whether Bd infection can induce mast cell increase and rSCF injection under Bd infection causes a mast cell increase in the skin. In addition, it is unclear how the enrichment of mast cells provides protection against Bd infection and alternations to skin microbiomes after infection. It is important to determine whether skin mast cells release any contents mentioned above.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for taking the time to review our work and for providing us with valuable feedback.

      Please note that amphibians do not possess the IgE antibody isotype1.

      To our knowledge there have been no published studies using approaches for studying mammalian mast cell degranulation to examine amphibian mast cells. Notably, several studies suggest that amphibian mast cells lack histamine2, 3, 4, 5 and serotonin2, 6. While there are commercially available kits and reagents for examining mammalian mast cell granule content, most of these reagents may not cross-react with their amphibian counterparts. This is especially true of cytokines and chemokines, which diverged quickly with evolution and thus do not share substantial protein sequence identity across species as divergent as frogs and mammals. Respectfully, while following up on these findings is possible, it would involve considerable additional work to find reagents that would detect amphibian mast cell contents.

      We would also like to respectfully point out that while mast cell degranulation is a feature most associated with mammalian mast cells, this is not the only means by which mammalian mast cells confer their immunological effects. While we agree that defining the biology of amphibian mast cell degranulation is important, we anticipate that since the anti-Bd protection conferred by enriching frog mast cells is seen after 21 days of enrichment, it is quite possible that degranulation may not be the central mechanism by which the mast cells are mediating this protection.

      As noted in our manuscript, frog mast cells upregulate their expression of interleukin-4 (IL4), which is a hallmark cytokine associated with mammalian mast cells7. We are presently exploring the role of the frog IL4 in the observed mast cell anti-Bd protection. Should we generate meaningful findings in this regard, we will add them to the revised version of this manuscript.

      We are also exploring the heparin content of frog mast cells and capacities of these cells to degranulate in vitro in response to compound 48/80. In addition, we are exploring in vivo mast cell degranulation via histology and avidin-staining. Should these studies generate significant findings, we will include them in the revised version of this manuscript.

      Per the reviewer’s suggestion, in our revised manuscript we also plan to include data showing whether Bd infections affect skin mast cell numbers and how rSCF injection impacts skin mast cell numbers in the context of Bd infections.

      In regard to how mast cells impact Bd infections and skin microbiomes, our data indicate that mast cells are augmenting skin integrity during Bd infections and promoting mucus production, as indicated by the findings presented in Figure 4A-C and Figure 5A-C, respectively. There are several mammalian mast cell products that elicit mucus production. In mammals, this mucus production is mediated by goblet cells while the molecular control of amphibian skin mucus gland content remains incompletely understood. Interleukin-13 (IL13) is the major cytokine associated with mammalian mucus production8, while to our knowledge this cytokine is either not encoded by amphibians or else has yet to be identified and annotated in these animals’ genomes. IL4 signaling also results in mucus production9 and we are presently exploring the possible contribution of the X. laevis IL4 to skin mucus gland filling. Any significant findings on this front will be included in the revised manuscript. Histamine release contributes to mast cell-mediated mucus production10, but as we outline above, several studies indicate that amphibian mast cells may lack histamine2, 3, 4, 5. Mammalian mast cell-produced lipid mediators also play a critical role in eliciting mucus secretion11 and our transcriptomic analysis indicates that frog mast cells express several enzymes associated with production of such mediators. We will highlight this observation in our revised manuscript.

      We anticipate that X. laevis mast cells influence skin integrity, microbial composition and Bd susceptibility in a myriad of ways. Considering the substantial differences between amphibian and mammalian evolutionary histories and physiologies, we anticipate that many of the mechanisms by which X. laevis mast cells confer anti-Bd protection will prove to be specific to amphibians and some even unique to X. laevis. We are most interested in deciphering what these mechanisms are but foresee that they will not necessarily reflect what one would expect based on what we know about mammalian mast cells in the context of mammalian physiologies.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:<br /> In this study, Hauser et al investigate the role of amphibian (Xenopus laevis) mast cells in cutaneous immune responses to the ecologically important pathogen Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis (Bd) using novel methods of in vitro differentiation of bone marrow-derived mast cells and in vivo expansion of skin mast cell populations. They find that bone marrow-derived myeloid precursors cultured in the presence of recombinant X. laevis Stem Cell Factor (rSCF) differentiate into cells that display hallmark characteristics of mast cells. They inject their novel (r)SCF reagent into the skin of X. laevis and find that this stimulates the expansion of cutaneous mast cell populations in vivo. They then apply this model of cutaneous mast cell expansion in the setting of Bd infection and find that mast cell expansion attenuates the skin burden of Bd zoospores and pathologic features including epithelial thickness and improves protective mucus production and transcriptional markers of barrier function. Utilizing their prior expertise with expanding neutrophil populations in X. laevis, the authors compare mast cell expansion using (r)SCF to neutrophil expansion using recombinant colony-stimulating factor 3 (rCSF3) and find that neutrophil expansion in Bd infection leads to greater burden of zoospores and worse skin pathology.

      Strengths: <br /> The authors report a novel method of expanding amphibian mast cells utilizing their custom-made rSCF reagent. They rigorously characterize expanded mast cells in vitro and in vivo using histologic, morphologic, transcriptional, and functional assays. This establishes solid footing with which to then study the role of rSCF-stimulated mast cell expansion in the Bd infection model. This appears to be the first demonstration of the exogenous use of rSCF in amphibians to expand mast cell populations and may set a foundation for future mechanistic studies of mast cells in the X. laevis model organism. 

      We thank the reviewer for recognizing the breadth and extent of the undertaking that culminated in this manuscript. Indeed, this manuscript would not have been possible without considerable reagent development and adaptation of techniques that had previously not been used for amphibian immunity research. In line with the reviewer’s sentiment, to our knowledge this is the first report of using molecular approaches to augment amphibian mast cells, which we hope will pave the way for new areas of research within the fields of comparative immunology and amphibian disease biology.

      Weaknesses:<br /> The conclusions regarding the role of mast cell expansion in controlling Bd infection would be stronger with a more rigorous evaluation of the model, as there are some key gaps and remaining questions regarding the data. For example:

      1. Granulocyte expansion is carefully quantified in the initial time courses of rSCF and rCSF3 injections, but similar quantification is not provided in the disease models (Figures 3E, 4G, 5D-G). A key implication of the opposing effects of mast cell vs neutrophil expansion is that mast cells may suppress neutrophil recruitment or function. Alternatively, mast cells also express notable levels of csfr3 (Figure 2) and previous work from this group (Hauser et al, Facets 2020) showed rG-CSF-stimulated peritoneal granulocytes express mast cell markers including kit and tpsab1, raising the question of what effect rCSF3 might have on mast cell populations in the skin. Considering these points, it would be helpful if both mast cells and neutrophils were quantified histologically (based on Figure 1, they can be readily distinguished by SE or Giemsa stain) in the Bd infection models.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful suggestion. We are performing a further examination of skin granulocyte content during Bd infections and plan on including any significant findings in our revised manuscript.

      We predict that rSCF administration results in the accumulation of mast cells that are polarized such that they ablate the inflammatory response elicited by Bd infection. Mammalian mast cells, including peritonea-resident mast cells, express csf3r12, 13. Although the X. laevis animal model does not permit nearly the degree of immune cell resolution afforded by mammalian animal models, we do know that the adult X. laevis peritonea contain heterogenous leukocyte populations. We anticipate that the high kit expression reported by Hauser et al., 2020 in the rCSF3-recruited peritoneal leukocytes reflects the presence of mast cells therein. As such and in acknowledgement of the reviewer’s suggestion, we also think that the cells recruited by rCSF3 into the skin may include not only neutrophils but also mast cells. Possibly, these mast cells have distinct polarization states from those enriched by rSCF. While the lack of antibodies against frog neutrophils or mast cells has limited our capacity to address this question, we will attempt to reexamine by histology the proportions of skin neutrophils and mast cells in the skins of frogs under the conditions described in our manuscript. Any new findings in this regard will be included in the revised version of this work.

      2. Epithelial thickness and inflammation in Bd infection are reported to be reduced by rSCF treatment (Figure 3E, 5A-B) or increased by rCSF3 treatment (Figure 4G) but quantification of these critical readouts is not shown.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We will score epithelial thickness under the distinct conditions described in our manuscript and present the quantified data in the revised paper.

      3. Critical time points in the Bd model are incompletely characterized. Mast cell expansion decreases zoospore burden at 21 dpi, while there is no difference at 7 dpi (Figure 3E). Conversely, neutrophil expansion increases zoospore burden at 7 dpi, but no corresponding 21 dpi data is shown for comparison (Figure 4G). Microbiota analysis is performed at a third time point,10 dpi (Figure 5D-G), making it difficult to compare with the data from the 7 dpi and 21 dpi time points. Reporting consistent readouts at these three time points is important to draw solid conclusions about the relationship of mast cell expansion to Bd infection and shifts in microbiota.

      Because there were no significant effects of mast cell enrichment at 7 days post Bd infection, we chose to look at the microbiome composition in a subsequent experiment at 10 days and 21 days post Bd infection, with 10 days being a bit more of a midway point between the initial exposure and day 21, when we see the effect on Bd loads. We will clarify this rationale in the revised manuscript.

      The enrichment of neutrophils in frog skins resulted in prompt (12 hours post enrichment) skin thickening (in absence of Bd infection) and increased frog Bd susceptibility by 7 days of infection. Conversely, mast cell enrichment stabilized skin mucosal and symbiotic microbial environment, presumably accounting at least in part for the lack of further Bd growth on mast cell-enriched animals by 21 days of infection. Our question regarding the roles of inflammatory granulocytes/neutrophils during Bd infections was that of ‘how’ rather ‘when’ these cells affect Bd infections. Because the central focus of this work was mast cells and not other granulocyte subsets, when we saw that rCSF3-recruited granulocytes adversely affected Bd infections at 7 days post infection, we did not pursue the kinetics of these responses further. We plan to explore the roles of inflammatory mediators and disparate frog immune cell subsets during the course of Bd infections, but we feel that these future studies are more peripheral to the central thesis of the present manuscript regarding the roles of frog mast cells during Bd infections.

      4. Although the effect of rSCF treatment on Bd zoospores is significant at 21 dpi (Figure 3E), bacterial microbiota changes at 21 dpi are not (Figure S3B-C). This discrepancy, how it relates to the bacterial microbiota changes at 10 dpi, and why 7, 10, and 21 dpi time points were chosen for these different readouts (Figure 5F-G), is not discussed.

      Our results indicate that after 10 days of Bd infection, control Bd-challenged animals exhibited reduced microbial richness, while skin mast cell-enriched Bd-infected frogs were protected from this disruption of their microbiome. The amphibian microbiome serves as a major barrier to these fungal infections14, and we anticipate that Bd-mediated disruption of microbial richness and composition facilitates host skin colonization by this pathogen. Control and mast cell-enriched animals had similar skin Bd loads at 10 days post infection. However, by 21 days of Bd infection the mast cells-enriched animals maintained their Bd loads to levels observed at 10 days post infection, whereas the control animals had significantly greater Bd loads. Thus, we anticipate that frog mast cells are conferring the observed anti-Bd protection in part by preventing microbial disassembly and thus interfering with optimal Bd colonization and growth on frog skins. In other words, maintained microbial composition at 10 days of infection may be preventing additional Bd colonization/growth, as seen when comparing skins of control and mast cell-enriched frogs at 21 days post infection. By 21 days of infection, control animals rebounded from the Bd-mediated reduction in bacterial richness seen at 10 days. Considering that after 21 days of infection control animals also had significantly greater Bd loads than mast-cell enriched animals suggests that there may be a critical earlier window during which microbial composition is able to counteract _Bd_growth. 

      While the current draft of our manuscript has a paragraph to this effect (see below), we appreciate the reviewer conveying to us that our perspective on the relationship between skin mast cells and the kinetics of microbial composition and _Bd_loads could be better emphasized. We plan to revise our manuscript to include the above discussion points. 

      Bd infections caused major reductions in bacterial taxa richness, changes in composition and substantial increases in the relative abundance of Bd-inhibitory bacteria early in the infection. Similar changes to microbiome structure occur during experimental Bd infections of red-backed salamanders and mountain yellow-legged frogs15, 16. In turn, progressing Bd_infections corresponded with a return to baseline levels of _Bd-inhibitory bacteria abundance and rebounding microbial richness, albeit with dissimilar communities to those seen in control animals. These temporal changes indicate that amphibian microbiomes are dynamic, as are the effects of Bd infections on them. Indeed, Bd infections may have long-lasting impacts on amphibian microbiomes15. While Bd infections manifested in these considerable changes to frog skin microbiome structure, mast cell enrichment appeared to counteract these deleterious effects to their microbial composition. Presumably, the greater skin mucosal integrity and mucus production observed after mast cell enrichment served to stabilize the cutaneous environment during Bd infections, thereby ameliorating the Bd-mediated microbiome changes. While this work explored the changes in established antifungal flora, we anticipate the mast cell-mediated inhibition of Bd may be due to additional, yet unidentified bacterial or fungal taxa. Intriguingly, while mammalian skin mast cell functionality depends on microbiome elicited SCF production by keratinocytes17, our results indicate that frog skin mast cells in turn impact skin microbiome structure and likely their function. It will be interesting to further explore the interdependent nature of amphibian skin microbiomes and resident mast cells.

      5. The time course of rSCF or rCSF3 treatments relative to Bd infection in the experiments is not clear. Were the treatments given 12 hours prior to the final analysis point to maximize the effect? For example, in Figure 3E, were rSCF injections given at 6.5 dpi and 20.5 dpi? Or were treatments administered on day 0 of the infection model? If the latter, how do the authors explain the effects at 7 dpi or 21 dpi given mast cell and neutrophil numbers return to baseline within 24 hours after rSCF or rCSF3 treatment, respectively?

      Please find the schematic of the immune manipulation, Bd infection, and sample collection times below. We will include a figure like this in our revised manuscript.

      The title of the manuscript may be mildly overstated. Although Bd infection can indeed be deadly, mortality was not a readout in this study, and it is not clear from the data reported that expanding skin mast cells would ultimately prevent progression to death in Bd infections.

      We acknowledge this point. The revised manuscript will be titled: “Amphibian mast cells: barriers to chytrid fungus infections”.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:<br /> Hauser et al. provide an exceptional study describing the role of resident mast cells in amphibian epidermis that produce anti-inflammatory cytokines that prevent Batrachochytrium dendrobatidis (Bd) infection from causing harmful inflammation, and also protect frogs from changes in skin microbiomes and loss of mucin in glands and loss of mucus integrity that otherwise cause changes to their skin microbiomes. Neutrophils, in contrast, were not protective against Bd infection. Beyond the beautiful cytology and transcriptional profiling, the authors utilized elegant cell enrichment experiments to enrich mast cells by recombinant stem cell factor, or to enrich neutrophils by recombinant colony-stimulating factor-3, and examined respective infection outcomes in Xenopus.

      Strengths:<br /> Through the use of recombinant IL4, the authors were able to test and eliminate the hypothesis that mast cell production of IL4 was the mechanism of host protection from Bd infection. Instead, impacts on the mucus glands and interaction with the skin microbiome are implicated as the protective mechanism. These results will press disease ecologists to examine the relative importance of this immune defense among species, the influence of mast cells on the skin microbiome and mucosal function, and open the potential for modulating mucosal defense.

      We thank the reviewer for recognizing the significance and utility of the findings presented in our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:<br /> A reduction of bacterial diversity upon infection, as described at the end of the results section, may not always be an "adverse effect," particularly given that anti-Bd function of the microbiome increased. Some authors (see Letourneau et al. 2022 ISME, or Woodhams et al. 2023 DCI) consider these short-term alterations as encoding ecological memory, such that continued exposure to a pathogen would encounter an enriched microbial defense. Regardless, mast cell-initiated protection of the mucus layer may negate the need for this microbial memory defense.

      We thank the reviewer their insightful comment. We will revise our discussion to include this possible interpretation.

      While the description of the mast cell location in the epidermal skin layer in amphibians is novel, it is not known how representative these results are across species ranging in chytridiomycosis susceptibility. No management applications are provided such as methods to increase this defense without the use of recombinant stem cell factor, and more discussion is needed on how the mast cell component (abundance, distribution in the skin) of the epidermis develops or is regulated.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment and would like to point out that the work presented in our manuscript was driven by comparative immunology questions more than by conservation biology.

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting expanding our discussion to include potential management applications and potential mechanisms for regulating frog skin mast cells. While any content to these effects would be highly speculative, we agree that it may spark new interest and pave new avenues for research. To this end, our revised manuscript will include a paragraph to this effect.

      References:

      1. Flajnik, M.F. A cold-blooded view of adaptive immunity. Nat Rev Immunol 18, 438-453 (2018).

      2. Mulero, I., Sepulcre, M.P., Meseguer, J., Garcia-Ayala, A. & Mulero, V. Histamine is stored in mast cells of most evolutionarily advanced fish and regulates the fish inflammatory response. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 104, 19434-19439 (2007).

      3. Reite, O.B. A phylogenetical approach to the functional significance of tissue mast cell histamine. Nature 206, 1334-1336 (1965).

      4. Reite, O.B. Comparative physiology of histamine. Physiol Rev 52, 778-819 (1972).

      5. Takaya, K., Fujita, T. & Endo, K. Mast cells free of histamine in Rana catasbiana. Nature 215, 776-777 (1967).

      6. Galli, S.J. New insights into "the riddle of the mast cells": microenvironmental regulation of mast cell development and phenotypic heterogeneity. Lab Invest 62, 5-33 (1990).

      7. Babina, M., Guhl, S., Artuc, M. & Zuberbier, T. IL-4 and human skin mast cells revisited: reinforcement of a pro-allergic phenotype upon prolonged exposure. Archives of dermatological research 308, 665-670 (2016).

      8. Lai, H. & Rogers, D.F. New pharmacotherapy for airway mucus hypersecretion in asthma and COPD: targeting intracellular signaling pathways. J Aerosol Med Pulm Drug Deliv 23, 219-231 (2010).

      9. Rankin, J.A. et al. Phenotypic and physiologic characterization of transgenic mice expressing interleukin 4 in the lung: lymphocytic and eosinophilic inflammation without airway hyperreactivity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93, 7821-7825 (1996).

      10. Church, M.K. Allergy, Histamine and Antihistamines. Handb Exp Pharmacol 241, 321-331 (2017).

      11. Nakamura, T. The roles of lipid mediators in type I hypersensitivity. J Pharmacol Sci 147, 126-131 (2021).

      12. Aponte-Lopez, A., Enciso, J., Munoz-Cruz, S. & Fuentes-Panana, E.M. An In Vitro Model of Mast Cell Recruitment and Activation by Breast Cancer Cells Supports Anti-Tumoral Responses. Int J Mol Sci 21 (2020).

      13. Jamur, M.C. et al. Mast cell repopulation of the peritoneal cavity: contribution of mast cell progenitors versus bone marrow derived committed mast cell precursors. BMC Immunol 11, 32 (2010).

      14. Walke, J.B. & Belden, L.K. Harnessing the Microbiome to Prevent Fungal Infections: Lessons from Amphibians. PLoS Pathog 12, e1005796 (2016).

      15. Jani, A.J. et al. The amphibian microbiome exhibits poor resilience following pathogen-induced disturbance. ISME J 15, 1628-1640 (2021).

      16. Muletz-Wolz, C.R., Fleischer, R.C. & Lips, K.R. Fungal disease and temperature alter skin microbiome structure in an experimental salamander system. Mol Ecol 28, 2917-2931 (2019).

      17. Wang, Z. et al. Skin microbiome promotes mast cell maturation by triggering stem cell factor production in keratinocytes. J Allergy Clin Immunol 139, 1205-1216 e1206 (2017).

    1. Author Response

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study investigated behavioural performance on a competing speech task and neural attentional filtering over the course of two years in a group of middle-aged to older adults. Neural attentional filtering was quantified using EEG by comparing neural envelope tracking to an attended vs. an unattended sentence. This dataset was used to examine the stability of the link between behavior and neural filtering over time. They found that neural filtering and behavior were correlated during each measurement, but EEG measures at the first time point did not predict behavioural performance two years later. Further, while behavioural measures showed relatively high test-retest reliability, the neural filtering reliability was weak with an r-value of 0.21. The authors conclude that neural tracking-based metrics have limited ability to predict longitudinal changes in listening behavior.

      Strengths:

      This study is novel in its tracking of behavioural performance and neural envelope tracking over time, and it includes an impressively large dataset of 105 participants. The manuscript is clearly written.

      Weaknesses:

      The weaknesses are minor, primarily concerning how the reviewers interpret their data. Specifically, the envelope tracking measure is often quite low, close to the noise floor, and this may affect testretest reliability. Furthermore, the trajectories may be affected by accelerated age-related declines that are more apparent in neural tracking than in behaviour.

      We thank the reviewer for their supportive assessment of our work. We describe in detail how we have addressed the two main concerns raised here—neural filtering’s low test-retest reliability and differences in age-related behavioural vs. neural change—in our response to the more detailed recommendations below.

      To briefly summarise here:

      (1) In Figure 5, we now illustrate more transparently how the employed structural equation framework helps to overcome the issue of low test-retest reliability of neural filtering as originally reported.

      (2) We include two additional control analyses, one of which relates neural tracking of attended speech (featuring a moderately high T1–T2 correlation of r = .64 even outside of latent modelling) to behavioural change. Importantly, this analysis provides critical empirical support for the apparent independence of neural and behavioural trajectories.

      (3) We more clearly describe how the latent-variable modelling strategy accounts for differences in age-related change along the neural and behavioural domain. Moreover, the results of the of 18 additional control analysis also suggest that the absence of a change-change relationship is not primarily due to differential effects of age on brain and behaviour.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      1) Figure 3:

      Does the 70-year range reach a tipping point?

      Is that why neural filtering drops dramatically in this age group, whereas the other groups do not change or increase slightly?

      This can also be seen with behavioral accuracy to a lesser extent. Perhaps test-retest reliability is affected by accelerated age-related declines in older listeners, as was found for envelope tracking measures in Decruy et al. 2019.

      We agree with the reviewer that at first glance the data seem to suggest a critical tipping point in the age range above 70 years. It is important to emphasize, however, that the four age bins were not based on equal number of data points. In fact, the >70 age group included the fewest participants, leading to a less reliable estimate of change. Together with the known observation of increasing interindividual differences with increasing age, the results do not allow for any strong conclusions regarding a potential tipping point. For the same reasons, we used the four age bins for illustrative purposes, only, and did not include them in any statistical modelling.

      We did however include chronological age as a continuous predictor in latent change score modelling. Here, we modelled its influence on participants’ T1 neural and behavioural status, as well as its effect on their respective change, thereby accounting for any differential (linear) effects of age on neural vs. behavioural functioning and its change.

      On p.14 of the revised manuscript, we now state more clearly that the latent change score model did in fact account for the potential influence of age on the change-related relationships:

      "In line with our hypotheses, we modelled the longitudinal impact of T1 neural functioning on the change in speed, and tested for a change-change correlation. Since the analyses conducted up to this point have either directly shown or have suggested that longitudinal change per domain may be affected by age, we included individuals’ age as a time-invariant covariate in the final model. We modelled the influence of age on neural and behavioural functioning at T1 but also on individual change per domain. By accounting for linear effects of age on longitudinal change, we also minimize its potential impact on the estimation of change-change relationship of interest. Note that we refrained from fitting separate models per age group due to both limited and different number of data points per age group."

      2) Would good test-retest reliability be expected when the actual values of envelope tracking for attended vs. unattended speech are so low? The investigators address this by including measurement errors in the models, but I am not certain this kind adequately deals with envelope tracking values that are close to the noise floor.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We addressed the concerns regarding the low re-test reliability of our neural-attentional metric (and its potential impact on observing a systematic changechange relationship) in two separate ways.

      The major outcome of these tests is that low re-test reliability of neural tracking is (i) not generally true, and (ii) is not the cause of the main finding, i.e., a low or absent correlations of behavioural vs. neural changes over time.

      In more detail, to show how latent change score modelling improves test-retest reliability by explicitly modelling measurement error, we first extracted and correlated T1 and T2 latent factors scores from the respective univariate models of neural filtering and response speed.

      Indeed, at the latent level, the correlation of T1–T2 neural filtering was moderately high at r = .65 (compared to r = .21 at the manifest level). The correlation of T1–T2 response speed was estimated as r = .75 (compared to r = .71).

      Figure 5A, reproduced below for the reviewer’s convenience, now includes insets quantifying these latent-level correlations over time.

      Author response image 1.

      Modelling of univariate and bivariate change. A Univariate latent change score models for response speed (left) and neural filtering (right). All paths denoted with Latin letters refer to freely estimated but constrained to be equal parameters of the respective measurement models. Greek letters refer to freely estimated parameters of the structural model. Highlighted in black is the estimated mean longitudinal change from T1 to T2. Scatterplots in the top left corner illustrate how capturing T1 and T2 neural and behavioural functioning as latent factors improves their respective test-retest reliability. B Latent change score model (LCSM) relating two-year changes in neural filtering strength to changes in response speed. Black arrows indicate paths or covariances of interest. Solid black arrows reflect freely estimated and statistically significant effects, dashed black arrows reflect non-significant effects. All estimates are standardised. Grey arrows show paths that were freely estimated or fixed as part of the structural model but that did not relate to the main research questions. For visual clarity, manifest indicators of the measurement model and all symbols relating to the estimated mean structure are omitted but are identical to those shown in panel A. p<.001, p<.01, p<.05, p=.08. C Scatterplots of model-predicted factor scores that refer to the highlighted paths in panel B. Top panel shows that baseline-level neural filtering did not predict two-year change in behavioural functioning, bottom panel shows the absence of a significant change-change correlation.

      Second, we ran a control analysis that includes the neural tracking of attended speech in selectiveattention trials rather than the neural filtering index averaged across all trials. The results are shown as part of a new main figure (and two new supplemental figures) reproduced below (see in particular Figure 6, panels C and D).

      This analysis serves two purposes: On the one hand, it allows for a more direct evaluation of the actual strength of neural speech tracking as quantified by the Pearson’s correlation coefficient. Note that these individual averages fall well within the to be expected range given that the neural tracking estimates are based on relatively short sentences (i.e., duration of ~2.5 sec) (O’Sullivan et al., 2014).

      On the other hand, neural tracking of attended speech showed a moderately high, r = .64, T1–T2 correlation even outside of latent modelling. Note that the magnitude of this T1–T2 reliability is close to the short-term test-retest reliability recently reported by Panela et al. (2023). Still, when including neural tracking of attended speech in the bivariate model of change, the change-change correlation with response speed was now estimated as close to 0 (𝜙 = –.03, n.s). This observation suggests that manifest-level high re-test reliability does not necessarily improve chances of observing a significant change-change correlation.

      Lastly, we would like to point out that these bivariate model results also help to shed light on the question of whether non-linear effects of age on neural / behavioural change may affect the chance of observing a systematic change-change relationship. As shown in Fig. 6C, for neural tracking of attended speech, we observed a fairly consistent longitudinal increase across age groups. Yet, as detailed above, the change-change correlation was virtually absent.

      In sum, these new results provide compelling evidence for the absence of a systematic changechange relationship.

      The respective control analysis results section reads as follows, and is accompanied by Figure 6 reproduced below:

      "Control analyses: The weak correlation of behavioural and neural change is robust against different quantifications of neural filtering

      Taken together, our main analyses revealed that inter-individual differences in behavioural change could only be predicted by baseline age and baseline behavioural functioning, and did not correlate with contemporaneous neural changes.

      However, one could ask in how far core methodological decisions taken in the current study, namely our focus on (i) the differential neural tracking of relevant vs. irrelevant speech as proxy of neural filtering, and (ii) on its trait-level characterization that averaged across different spatial-attention conditions may have impacted these results. Specifically, if the neural filtering index (compared to the neural tracking of attended speech alone) is found to be less stable generally, would this also impact the chances of observing a systematic change-change relationship? Relatedly, did the analysis of neural filtering across all trials underestimate the effects of interest?

      To evaluate the impact of these consideration on our main findings, we conducted two additional control analyses: First, we repeated the main analyses using the neural filtering index (and response speed) averaged across selective-attention trials, only. Second, we repeated the main analyses using the neural tracking of attended speech, again averaged across selective-attention trials, only.

      As shown in Figure 6, taken together, the control analyses provide compelling empirical support for the robustness of our main results: Linking response speed and neural filtering under selective attention strengthened their relationship at T1 (𝜙 = .54, SE = .15, Dc2(df = 1) = 2.74, p = .1; see. Fig 6B) but did not yield any significant effects for the influence of T1 neural filtering on behavioural change (β = .13, SE = .21, Dc2(df = 1) = .43, p = .51), or for the relationship of neural and behavioural change (𝜙 = .26, SE = .14, Dc2(df = 1) = 3.1, p = .08; please note the close correspondence to path estimates reported in Fig. 5). The second control analysis revealed a substantially higher manifest-level test-retest reliability of neural tracking of attended speech (r = .65, p<.001; Fig. 6C) compared to that of the neural tracking index. However, when linked to longitudinal changes in response speed, this analysis provided even less evidence for systematic change-related relationships: Baseline-levels of attended-speech tracking did not predict future change in response speed (β = .18, SE = .11, Dc2(df = 1) = 2.73, p = .10), and changes in neural and behavioural functioning occurred independently of one another (𝜙 = –.03, SE = .12, Dc2(df = 1) = .06, p = .81).

      In sum, the two control analyses provide additional empirical support for the results revealed by our main analysis."

      Author response image 2.

      Control analyses corroborate the independence of neural and behavioural trajectories under selective attention. Cross-sectional and longitudinal change in neural filtering (A) and neural tracking of attended speech (C) averaged across selective-attention trials, only. Coloured vectors (colour-coding four age groups for illustrative purposes, only) in the left subpanels show individual T1–T2 change along with the cross-sectional trend plus 95% confidence interval (CI) separately for T1 (dark grey) and T2 (light grey). Top right, correlation of T1 and T2 as measure of test-retest reliability along with the 45° line (grey) and individual data points (black circles). Bottom right, mean longitudinal change per age group and grand mean change (grey). B, D Latent change score model (LCSM) relating two-year changes in neural filtering (B) /neural tracking (D) strength to changes in response speed. Black arrows show the paths or covariances of interest that were freely estimates, grey arrows show paths that were freely estimated or fixed as part of the structural model but did not relate to the main research questions. Solid arrows indicate statistically significant effects, dashed arrows reflect nonsignificant paths. All estimates are standardised. p<.001, p<.01, p<.05.

      3) The authors conclude that the temporal instability of the neural filtering measure precludes its use for diagnostic/therapeutic intervention. I agree that test-retest reliability is needed for a clinical intervention. However, given the relationship with behavior at a specific point in time, would it not be a possible target for intervention to improve performance? Even if there are different trajectories, an individual may benefit from enhanced behavioral performance in the present.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We would agree that the observation of robust betweensubject (or even more desirable: within-subject) brain–behaviour relationships is a key desideratum in identifying potential interventional targets. At the same time, we would argue that the most direct way of evaluating a neural signature’s translational potential is by focusing on how it predicts or is linked to individual change. In revising both the Introduction and Discussion section, we hope to now better motivate our reasoning.

      Other minor comments:

      4) Lines 106-107 What is the basis for the prediction regarding neural filtering?

      In our previous analysis of T1 data (Tune et al., 2021), we found inter-individual differences in neural filtering itself, and also in its link to behaviour, to be independent of chronological age and hearing loss. On the basis of these results, we did not expect any systematic decrease or increase in neural filtering over time.<br /> We rephrased the respective sentence as follows:

      Since we previously observed inter-individual differences in neural filtering to be independent of age and hearing status, we did not expect any systematic longitudinal change in neural filtering.

      5) Line 414: Replace "relevant" with "relevance".

      Thank you, this has been corrected.

      6) What was the range of presentation levels? Stimuli presented at 50 dB above individual sensation level could result in uncomfortably loud levels for people with mild to moderate hearing loss.

      Unfortunately, we didn’t have the means to estimate the precise dB SPL level at which our stimuli were presented. Due to the use of in-ear headphones, we did not aim to measure the exact sound pressure level of presentation but instead ensured that even if stimuli were presented at the maximally possible intensity given our hardware, this would not result in subjectively uncomfortably loud stimulus presentation levels. The described procedure estimated per individual how far the maximal sound pressure level needed to be attenuated to arrive at a comfortable and easy-tounderstand presentation level.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study examined the longitudinal brain-behaviour link between attentional neural filtering and listening behaviour among a sample of aging individuals. The results based on the latent change score modeling showed that neither attentional neural filtering at T1 nor its T1-T2 change predicted individual two-year listening performance change. The findings suggest that neural filtering and listening behaviour may follow independent developmental trajectories. This study focuses on an interesting topic and has the potential to contribute a better understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms of successful communication across the lifespan.

      Strengths:

      Although research suggests that speech comprehension is neurally supported by an attentionguided filter mechanism, the evidence of their causal association is limited. This study addresses this gap by testing the longitudinal stability of neural filtering as a neural mechanism upholding listening performance, potentially shedding light on translational efforts aiming at the preservation of speech comprehension abilities among aging individuals.

      The latent change score modeling approach is appropriately used as a tool to examine key developmental questions and distinguish the complex processes underlying lifespan development in brain and behaviour with longitudinal data.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the paper does have strengths in principle, the weaknesses of the paper are that the findings are merely based on a single listening task. Since both neural and behavioral indicators are derived from the same task, the results may be applicable only to this specific task, and it is difficult to extrapolate them to cognitive and listening abilities measured by the other tasks. Therefore, more listening tasks are required to comprehensively measure speech comprehension and neural markers.

      The age span of the sample is relatively large. Although no longitudinal change from T1 to T2 was found at the group-level, from the cross-sectional and longitudinal change results (see Figure 3), individuals of different age groups showed different development patterns. Particularly, individuals over the age of 70 show a clear downward trend in both neural filtering index and accuracy. Therefore, different results may be found based on different age groups, especially older groups. However, due to sample limitations, this study was unable to examine whether age has a moderating effect on this brain-behaviour link.

      In the Dichotic listening task, valid and invalid cues were manipulated. According to the task description, the former could invoke selective attention, whereas the latter could invoke divided attention. It is possible that under the two conditions, the neural filtering index may reflect different underlying cognitive processes, and thus may differ in its predictive effect on behavioral performance. The author could perform a more in-depth data analysis on indicators under different conditions.

      We thank the reviewer for their critical yet positive assessment of our work that also appreciates its potential to further our understanding of key determinants of successful communication in healthy aging. Please also see our more in-depth responses to the detailed recommendations that relate to the three main concern raised above.

      Regarding the first concern of the reviewer about the limited generalizability of our brain–behaviour results, we would argue that there are two sides to this argument.

      On the one hand, the results do not directly speak to the generalizability of the observed complex brain–behaviour relationships to other listening tasks. This may be perceived as a weakness. Unfortunately, as part of our large-scale projects, we did not collect data from another listening task suitable for such a generalization test. Using any additional cognitive tests would shift the focus away from the goal of understanding the determinants of successful communication, and rather speak more generally to the relationship of neural and cognitive change.

      On the other hand, we would argue the opposite, namely that the focus on the same listening task is in fact a major strength of the present study: The key research questions were motivated by our timepoint 1 findings of a brain-behaviour link both at the within-subject (state) and at the between subject (trait) level (Tune et al., 2021). Notably, in the current study, we show that both, the state- and the trait-level results, were replicated at timepoint 2. This observed stability of results provides compelling empirical evidence for the functional relevance of neural filtering to the listening outcome and critically sets the stage for the inquiry into the complex longitudinal change relationships. We now spell this out more clearly in the Introduction and the Discussion.

      Here, we briefly summarise how we have addressed the two remaining main concerns.

      (1) Please refer to our response R1’s comment #1 on the influence of (differential) age effects on brain and behaviour. These effects were in fact already accounted for by our modelling strategy which included the continuously (rather than binned by age group) modelled effect of age. We now communicate this more clearly in the revised manuscript.

      (2) We added two control analyses, one of which replicated the main analysis using selective attention trials, only. Critically, as shown in Figure 6, while the strength of the relationship of neural filtering and behaviour at a given timepoint increased, the key change-related relationships of interest remained not only qualitatively unchanged, but resulted in highly similar quantitative estimates.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      1) Theoretically, the relationship between brain and behavior may not be just one-way, but probably bi-directional. In this study, the authors only considered the unidirectional predictive effect of neural filtering on changes in listening task performance. However, it is possible that lower listening ability may limit information processing in older adults, which may lead to a decline in neural filtering abilities. The authors may also consider this theoretical hypothesis.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. While we did not have any specific hypotheses about influence of the behavioural state at timepoint 1 on the change in neural filtering, we ran control analysis that freely estimates the respective path (rather than implicitly assuming it to be 0). However, the results did not provide evidence for such a relationship. We report the results on p. 14 of the revised manuscript:

      "We did not have any a priori hypotheses on the influence of T1 speed on the individual T1–T2 change in neural filtering. Still in a control analysis that freely estimated the respective path, we found that an individual’s latent T1 level of response speed was not predictive of the ensuing latent T1–T2 change in neural filtering (β = –.11, SE = .21, Dc2(df = 1) = .31, p = .58)."

      2) The necessity of exploring the longitudinal relationship between attentional neural filtering and listening behaviour needs to be further clarified. That is, why choose attentional filtering (instead of the others) as an indicator to predict listening performance?

      We are not quite certain we understood which ‘other’ metrics the reviewer was referring to here exactly. But we would like to reiterate our argument from above: we believe that focusing on neural and behavioural metrics that are (i) derived from the same task, and (ii) were previously shown to be linked at both the trait- and state-level provided strong empirical ground for our inquiries into their longitudinal change-related relationships.

      Please note that we agree that the neural filtering index as a measure of attention-guided neural encoding of relevant vs. irrelevant speech signals is only one potential candidate neural measure but one that was clearly motivated by previous results. Nevertheless, in the revised manuscript we now also report on the relationship of neural tracking of attended speech and listening performance (see also our response to the reviewer’s comment #5 below).

      Apart of this, by making the entire T1–T2 dataset openly available, we invite researchers to conduct any potential follow-up analyses focused on metrics not reported here.

      3) Regarding the Dichotic listening task, further clarification is needed.

      (1) The task procedure and key parameters need to be supplemented.

      We have added a new supplemental Figure S6 which details the experimental design and procedure. We have also added further listening task details to the Methods section on p.23:

      At each timepoint, participants performed a previously established dichotic listening task20. We provide full details on trial structure, stimulus construction, recording and presentation in our previously published study on the first (N = 155) wave of data collection (but see also Fig. S6)12.

      In short, in each of 240 trials, participants listened to two competing, dichotically presented five-word sentences spoken by the same female speaker. They were probed on the sentence-final noun in one of the two sentences. Participants were instructed to respond within a given 4 s time window beginning with the onset of a probe screen showing four alternatives. They were not explicitly instructed to respond as quickly as possible. The probe screen showed four alternative words presented either on the left or right side of the screen, indicating the probed ear. Two visual cues preceded auditory presentation (…)

      We also note that the task and key parameters have been published additionally in (Tune et al., 2021) and Alavash et al. (2019). We have made sure these citations are placed prominently at the beginning of the methods section.

      Author response image 3.

      Experimental design and procedure.

      (2) Prior to the task, were the participants instructed to respond quickly and correctly? Was there a speed-accuracy trade-off? Was it possible to consider an integrated ACC-RT indicator?

      We instructed participants to respond within a 4-sec time window following the response screen onset but we did not explicitly instruct them to respond as quickly as possible. We also state this more explicitly in the revised Method section on p. 23 (see also our response to comment #3 by R3 on p. 15 below).

      In a between-subjects analysis we observed, both within T1 and T2, a significant positive correlation (rT1 = .33, p<.01; rT2 = .40, p<.001) of participants’ overall accuracy and response speed, speaking against a speed-accuracy trade-off. For this reason, we did not consider an integrated speed–accuracy measure as behavioural indicator for modelling.

      (3) The correlation between neural filtering at T1 and T2 was weak, which may be due to the low reliability of this indicator. The generally low reliability of the difference score is a notorious measurement problem recognized in the academic community.

      We fully agree with the reviewer on their assessment of notoriously noisy difference scores. It is the very reason that motivated our application of the latent change score model approach. This framework elegantly supersedes the manual calculation of differences scores, and by explicitly

      modelling measurement error also removes the impact of varying degrees of reliability on the estimation of change and how it varies as a function of different influences.

      While we had already detailed this rationale in the original manuscript, we now more prominently describe the advantages of the latent variable approach in the first paragraph of the Results section:

      Third and final, we integrate and extend the first two analysis perspectives in a joint latent change score model (LCSM) to most directly probe the role of neural filtering ability as a predictor of future attentive listening ability. Addressing our key change-related research questions at the latent rather than the manifest level supersedes the manual calculation of notoriously noisy differences scores, and effectively removes the influence of each metric’s reliability on the estimation of change-related relationships.

      We also kindly refer the reviewer to our in-depth response to R1’s comment #2 regarding the concern of neural filtering’s low test-rest reliability and its impact on estimating change-change relationships.

      1. For the latent change score model, it is recommended that the authors:<br /> (1) Supplement the coefficients of each path in Figure 5. For details, please refer to the figures in the papers of Kievit et al. (2017, 2019)

      This information has been added to Figure 5.

      (2) In Figure 5 and Figure S2, why should the two means of the observed 2nd half scores be estimated?

      In longitudinal modelling, special care needs to be applied to the pre-processing/transformation of raw data for the purpose of change score modelling. While it is generally desirable to bring all variables onto the same scale (typically achieved by standardising all variables), one needs to be careful not to remove the mean differences of interest in such a data transformation step. We therefore followed the procedure recommended by Little (2013) and rescaled variables stacked across T1 and T2 using the proportion of maximum scale (‘POMS’) methods. This procedure, however, results in mean values per timepoint ≠ 0, so the mean of the second half needed to be freely estimated to avoid model misfit. Note that the mean of the first half manifest variables was set to 0 (using the ‘marker method’; see Little, 2013) to ensure model identification.

      We have added the following more detailed description to the Method section on p. 26:

      To bring all manifest variables onto the same scale while preserving mean differences over time, we first stacked them across timepoint and then rescaled them using the proportion of maximum scale (‘POMS’) method99,100 (…) Given our choice of POMS-transformation of raw to preserve mean differences over time, the mean of the second manifest variable had to be freely estimated (rather than implicitly assumed to be 0) to avoid severe model misfit.

      (3) The authors need to clarify whether the latent change factor in Figure 5 is Δ(T1-T2) or Δ(T2-T1)?

      Thank you for this comment. Our notation here was indeed confusing. The latent change factor quantifies the change from T1 to T2, so it is Δ(T2–T1). We have accordingly re-named the respective latent variables in all corresponding figures.

      1. For data analysis, the author combined the trials under different conditions (valid and invalid cues) in the dichotic listening task and analyzed them together, which may mask the variations between different attention levels (selective vs. divided attention). It is recommended that the authors analyze the relationship between various indicators under different conditions.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment which prompted us to (i) more clearly motivate our decision to model neural filtering across all trials, and (ii) nevertheless report the results of an additional control analyses that focused on neural filtering (or the neural tracking of attended speech) in selective-attention trials, only.

      Our decision to analyse neural filtering across all spatial-attention conditions was motivated by two key considerations: First, previous T1 results (Tune et al., 2021) suggested that irrespective of the spatial-attention condition, stronger neural filtering boosted behavioural performance. Second, analysing neural filtering (and associated behaviour) across all trials provided the most direct way of probing the trait-like nature of individual neural filtering ability. <br /> We have included the following paragraph to the Results section on p. 6 to motivate this decision more clearly:

      Our main analyses focus on neural filtering and listening performance averaged across all trials and thereby also across two separate spatial-attention conditions. This choice allowed us to most directly probe the trait-like nature and relationships of neural filtering. It was additionally supported by our previous observation of a general boost in behavioural performance with stronger neural filtering, irrespective of spatial attention.

      On the other hand, one could argue that the effects of interest are underestimated by jointly analysing neural and behavioural functioning derived from both selective- and divided-attention conditions. After all, it is reasonable to expect a more pronounced neural filtering response in selective-attention trials.

      For this reason, we now report, in the revised version, two additional control analyses that replicate the key analyses for the neural filtering index and for the tracking of attended speech, both averaged across selective-attention trials, only: In summary, analysing neural filtering under selective attention strengthened the brain-behaviour link within a given time-point but resulted in highly similar quantitative estimated for the key relationships of interest. The analysis of attended speech tracking notably improved the neural metric’s manifest-level re-test reliability (r = .64, p<.001) – but resulted in an estimated change-change correlation close to 0.

      Taken together, these control analyses provide compelling support for our main conclusion that neural and behavioural functioning follow largely independent developmental trajectories.

      We kindly refer the reviewer to our detailed response to R1 for the text of the added control analysis section on p. 4f. above. The additional Figure 6 is reproduced again below for the reviewer’s convenience.

      Author response image 4.

      Control analyses corroborate the independence of neural and behavioural trajectories under selective attention. Cross-sectional and longitudinal change in neural filtering (A) and neural tracking of attended speech (C) averaged across selective-attention trials, only. Coloured vectors (colour-coding four age groups for illustrative purposes, only) in the left subpanels show individual T1–T2 change along with the cross-sectional trend plus 95% confidence interval (CI) separately for T1 (dark grey) and T2 (light grey). Top right, correlation of T1 and T2 as measure of test-retest reliability along with the 45° line (grey) and individual data points (black circles). Bottom right, mean longitudinal change per age group and grand mean change (grey). B, D Latent change score model (LCSM) relating two-year changes in neural filtering (B) /neural tracking (D) strength to changes in response speed. Black arrows show the paths or covariances of interest that were freely estimates, grey arrows show paths that were freely estimated or fixed as part of the structural model but did not relate to the main research questions. Solid arrows indicate statistically significant effects, dashed arrows reflect nonsignificant paths. All estimates are standardised. p<.001, p<.01, p<.05.

      Figure 6 has also been supplemented by two additional figures showing behavioural functioning (Fig. S4) and neural tracking of ignored speech (Fig. S5) under selective-attention trials, only. These figures are reproduced below for the reviewer’s convenience.

      Author response image 5.

      Cross-sectional and longitudinal change in listening behaviour under selective attention.

      Author response image 6.

      Cross-sectional and longitudinal change in neural tracking of ignored speech under selective attention.

      6) As can be seen from the Methods section, there were still other cognitive tasks in this database that can be included in the data analysis to further determine the predictive validity of neural filtering.

      We kindly refer the reviewer to our response to their public review and comment # 2 above where we motivate our decision to focus on manifest indicators of neural and behavioural functioning that are derived from the same task.

      We believe that the analysis of several additional indicators of cognitive functioning would have distracted from our main goal of the current study focused on understanding how individual trajectories of listening performance may be explained and predicted.

      7) "Magnitudes > 1 are taken as moderate, > 2.3 as strong evidence for either of the alternative or null hypotheses, respectively." Which papers are referenced by these criteria? The interpretation of BF values seems inconsistent with existing literature.

      It may deserve emphasis that these are log Bayes Factors (logBF). Our interpretation of logarithmic Bayes Factors (logBF) follows Lee and Wagenmakers’ (2013) classic heuristic scheme for the interpretation of (non-logarithmic, ‘raw’) BF10 values. We have added the respective reference to the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The study investigates the longitudinal changes in hearing threshold, speech recognition behavior, and speech neural responses in 2 years, and how these changes correlate with each other. A slight change in the hearing threshold is observed in 2 years (1.2 dB on average) but the speech recognition performance remains stable. The main conclusion is that there is no significant correlation between longitudinal changes in neural and behavioral measures.

      Strengths:

      The sample size (N>100) is remarkable, especially for longitudinal studies.

      Weaknesses:

      The participants are only tracked for 2 years and relatively weak longitudinal changes are observed, limiting how the data may shed light on the relationships between basic auditory function, speech recognition behavior, and speech neural responses.

      Suggestions

      First, it's not surprising that a 1.2 dB change in hearing threshold does not affect speech recognition, especially for the dichotic listening task and when speech is always presented 50 dB above the hearing threshold. For the same listener, if the speech level is adjusted for 1.2 dB or much more, the performance will not be influenced during the dichotic listening task. Therefore, it is important to mention in the abstract that "sensory acuity" is measured using the hearing threshold and the change in hearing threshold is only 1.2 dB.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have added the respective information to the abstract and have toned down our interpretation of the observed behavioural stability despite the expected decline in auditory acuity.

      Second, the lack of correlation between age-related changes in "neuronal filtering" and behavior may not suggest that they follow independent development trajectories. The index for "neuronal filtering" does not seem to be stable and the correlation between the two tests is only R = 0.21. This low correlation probably indicates low test-retest reliability, instead of a dramatic change in the brain between the two tests. In other words, if the "neuronal filtering" index only very weakly correlates with itself between the two tests, it is not surprising that it does not correlate with other measures in a different test. If the "neuronal filtering" index is measured on two consecutive days and the index remains highly stable, I'm more convinced that it is a reliable measure that just changes a lot within 2 years, and the change is dissociated with the changes in behavior.

      The authors attempted to solve the problem in the section entitled "Neural filtering reliably supports listening performance independent of age and hearing status", but I didn't follow the logic. As far as I could tell, the section pooled together the measurements from two tests and did not address the test-retest stability issue.

      Please see our detailed response to R1’s comment #2 regarding the concern of how low (manifestlevel) reliability of our neural metric may have impacted the chance of observing a significant changechange correlation.

      In addition, we would like to emphasize that the goal of the second step of our analysis procedure, featuring causal mediation analysis, was not to salvage the perhaps surprisingly low reliability of neural filtering. Instead, this section addressed a different research question, namely, whether the link of neural filtering to behaviour would hold across time, irrespective of the observed stability of the measure itself. The stability of the observed between-subjects brain-behaviour relationships was assessed by testing for an interaction with timepoint.

      We have revised the respective Results section to more clearly state our scientific questions, and how our analysis procedure helped to address them:

      "The temporal instability of neural filtering challenges its status as a potential trait-like neural marker of attentive listening ability. At the same time, irrespective of the degree of reliability of neural filtering itself, across individuals it may still be reliably linked to the behavioural outcome (see Fig. 1). This is being addressed next.

      On the basis of the full T1–T2 dataset, we aimed to replicate our key T1 results and test whether the previously observed between-subjects brain-behaviour relationship would hold across time: We expected an individual’s neural filtering ability to impact their listening outcome (accuracy and response speed) independently of age or hearing status12. (…) To formally test the stability of direct and indirect relationships across time, we used a moderated mediation analysis. In this analysis, the inclusion of interactions by timepoint tested whether the influence of age, sensory acuity, and neural filtering on behaviour varied significantly across time."

      Third, the behavioral measure that is not correlated with "neuronal filtering" is the response speed. I wonder if the participants are asked to respond as soon as possible (not mentioned in the method). If not, the response speed may strongly reflect general cognitive function or a personal style, which is not correlated with the changes in auditory functions. This can also explain why the hearing threshold affects speech recognition accuracy but not the response speed (lines 263-264).

      Participants were asked to response within a given time window limited to 4 s but were not implicitly instructed to respond as quickly as possible. This is now stated more clearly in the Methods section (please also refer to our response to R2 on a similar question). It is important to emphasize—as shown in Figure 4A and Figure 5B —both at the manifest and latent variable level neural filtering (and in fact also the neural tracking of attended speech, see Fig. 6C) was reliably linked to response speed at T1 and T2. These results providing important empirical ground for the question of whether changes in neural filtering are systematically related to changes in response speed, and whether the fidelity of neural filtering at T1 represents a precursor of behavioural changes.

      Moreover, an interpretation of response speed as an indicator of general cognitive function is not at all incompatible with the cognitive demands imposed by the task. As the reviewer rightly stated above, performance in a dichotic listening task does not simply hinge on how auditory acuity may limit perceptual encoding of speech inputs but also on how the goal-directed application of attention modulates the encoding of relevant vs. irrelevant inputs. We here focus on one candidate neural strategy we here termed ‘neural filtering’ in line with an influential metaphor of how auditory attention may be neurally implemented (Cherry, 1953; Erb & Obleser, 2020; Fernandez-Duque & Johnson, 1999).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Other issues:

      The authors should consider using terminology that the readers are more familiar with and avoid unsubstantiated claims.

      For example, the Introduction mentions that "The observation of such brain-behaviour relationships critically advances our understanding of the neurobiological foundation of cognitive functioning. Their translational potential as neural markers predictive of behaviour, however, is often only implicitly assumed but seldomly put to the test. Using auditory cognition as a model system, we here overcome this limitation by testing directly the hitherto unknown longitudinal stability of neural filtering as a neural compensatory mechanism upholding communication success."

      For the first sentence, please be clear about which aspects of "our understanding of the neurobiological foundation of cognitive functioning" is critically advanced by such brain-behaviour relationships, and why such brain-behaviour relationships are so critical given that so many studies have analyzed brain-behaviour relationships. The following two sentences seem to suggest that the current study is a translational study, but the later questions do not seem to be quite translational.

      The uncovering of robust between- and within-subject brain behaviour-relationships is a key scientific goal that unites basic and applied neuroscience. From a basic neuroscience standpoint, the observation of such brain–behaviour links provides important mechanistic insight into the neurobiological implementation of higher order cognition – here the application of auditory spatial attention in the service of speech comprehension. At the same time, they provide fruitful ground for translational inquiries of applied neuroscience. We therefore don’t consider it contradictory at all that the current study addressed both more basic and applied/translational neuroscientific research questions.

      We have rephrased the respective section as follows:

      "The observation of such brain–behaviour relationships critically advances our understanding of the neurobiological foundation of cognitive functioning by showing, for example, how neural implementations of auditory selective attention support attentive listening. They also provide fruitful ground for scientific inquiries into the translational potential of neural markers. However, the potency of neural markers to predict future behavioural outcomes is often only implicitly assumed but seldomly put to the test15."

      More importantly, "neuronal filtering" is a key concept in the paper but I'm not sure what it means. The authors have only mentioned that auditory cognition is a model system for "neuronal filtering", but not what "neuronal filtering" is. Even for auditory cognition, I'm not sure what "neuronal filtering" is and why the envelope response is representative of "neuronal filtering".

      As spelled out in the Introduction, we define our ‘neural filtering’ metric of interest as neural manifestation of the attention-guided segregation of behaviourally relevant from irrelevant sounds. By terming this signature neural ‘filtering’, we take up on a highly influential algorithmic metaphor of how auditory attention may be implemented at the neurobiological level (Cherry, 1953; Erb & Obleser, 2020; Fernandez-Duque & Johnson, 1999).

      We now provide more mechanistic detail in our description of the neural filtering signature analysed in the current study:

      "Recent research has focused on the neurobiological mechanisms that promote successful speech comprehension by implementing ‘neural filters’ that segregate behaviourally relevant from irrelevant sounds. Such neural filter mechanisms act by selectively increasing the sensory gain for behaviourally relevant inputs or by inhibiting the processing of irrelevant inputs5-7. A growing body of evidence suggests that speech comprehension is neurally supported by an attention-guided filter mechanism that modulates sensory gain and arises from primary auditory and perisylvian brain regions: By synchronizing its neural activity with the temporal structure of the speech signal of interest, the brain ‘tracks’ and thereby better encodes behaviourally relevant auditory inputs to enable attentive listening 8-11."

      Figure 1C should be better organized and the questions mentioned in the Introduction should be numbered.

      We have revised both the respective section of the Introduction and corresponding Figure 1 in line with the reviewer’s suggestions. The revised text and figure are reproduced below for the reviewer’s convenience:

      "First, by focusing on each domain individually, we ask how sensory, neural, and behavioural functioning evolve cross-sectionally across the middle and older adult life span (Fig. 1B). More importantly, we also ask how they change longitudinally across the studied two-year period (Fig. 1C, Q1), and whether aging individuals differ significantly in their degree of change (Q2). We expect individuals’ hearing acuity and behaviour to decrease from T1 to T2. Since we previously observed inter-individual differences in neural filtering to be independent of age and hearing status, we did not expect any systematic longitudinal change in neural filtering.

      Second, we test the longitudinal stability of the previously observed age- and hearing-loss–independent effect of neural filtering on both accuracy and response speed (Fig. 1A). To this end, we analyse the multivariate direct and indirect relationships of hearing acuity, neural filtering and listening behaviour within and across timepoints.

      Third, leveraging the strengths of latent change score modelling16,17, we fuse cross-sectional and longitudinal perspectives to probe the role of neural filtering as a precursor of behavioural change in two different ways: we ask whether an individual’s T1 neural filtering strength can predict the observed behavioural longitudinal change (Q3), and whether two-year change in neural filtering can explain concurrent change in listening behaviour (Q4). Here, irrespective of the observed magnitude and direction of T1–T2 developments, two scenarios are conceivable: Intra-individual neural and behavioural change may be either be correlated—lending support to a compensatory role of neural filtering—or instead follow independent trajectories18 (see Fig. 1C)."

      Author response image 7.

      Schematic illustration of key assumptions and research questions. A Listening behaviour at a given timepoint is shaped by an individuals’ sensory and neural functioning. Increased age decreases listening behaviour both directly, and indirectly via age-related hearing loss. Listening behaviour is supported by better neural filtering ability, independently of age and hearing acuity. B Conceptual depiction of individual two-year changes along the neural (blue) and behavioural (red) domain. Thin coloured lines show individual trajectories across the adult lifespan, thick lines and black arrows highlight two-year changes in a single individual. C Left, Schematic diagram highlighting the key research questions detailed in the introduction and how they are addressed in the current study using latent change score modelling. Right, across individuals, co-occurring changes in the neural and behavioural domain may be correlated (top) or independent of one another (bottom).

      Figure 3, the R-value should also be labeled on the four main plots.

      This information has been added to Figure 3, reproduced below.

      Author response image 8.

      Characterizing cross-sectional and longitudinal change along the auditory sensory (A), neural (B), and behavioural (C, D) domain. For each domain, coloured vectors (colour-coding four age groups for illustrative purposes, only) in the respective left subpanels show an individual’s change from T1 to T2 along with the cross-sectional trend plus 95% confidence interval (CI) separately for T1 (dark grey) and T2 (light grey). Top right subpanels: correlation of T1 and T2 as measure of test-retest reliability along with the 45° line (grey) and individual data points (black circles). Bottom right panels: Mean longitudinal change per age group (coloured vectors) and grand mean change (grey). Note that accuracy is expressed here as proportion correct for illustrative purposes, but was analysed logit-transformed or by applying generalized linear models.

      T1 and T2 should be briefly defined in the abstract or where they first appear.

      We have changed the abstract accordingly.

      References

      Alavash, M., Tune, S., & Obleser, J. (2019). Modular reconfiguration of an auditory control brain network supports adaptive listening behavior. [Clinical Trial]. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the United States of America, 116(2), 660-669. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1815321116

      Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25(5), 975-979. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1907229

      Erb, J., & Obleser, J. (2020). Neural filters for challening listening situations. In M. Gazzaniga, G. R. Mangun, & D. Poeppel (Eds.), The cognitive neurosciences (6th ed.). MIT Press.

      Fernandez-Duque, D., & Johnson, M. L. (1999). Attention metaphors: How metaphors guide the cognitive psychology of attention. Cognitive Science, 23(1), 83-116. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2301_4<br /> O’Sullivan, J. A., Power, A. J., Mesgarani, N., Rajaram, S., Foxe, J. J., Shinn-Cunningham, B. G., Slaney, M., Shamma,

      S. A., & Lalor, E. C. (2014). Attentional Selection in a Cocktail Party Environment Can Be Decoded from Single-Trial EEG. Cerebral Cortex, 25(7), 1697-1706. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht355

      Panela, R. A., Copelli, F., & Herrmann, B. (2023). Reliability and generalizability of neural speech tracking in younger and older adults. Nature Communications, 2023.2007.2026.550679. https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.07.26.550679

      Tune, S., Alavash, M., Fiedler, L., & Obleser, J. (2021). Neural attentional-filter mechanisms of listening success in middle-aged and older individuals. Nature Communications, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467021-24771-9

    1. Author Response

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1:

      Watanuki et al used metabolomic tracing strategies of U-13C6-labeled glucose and 13C-MFA to quantitatively identify the metabolic programs of HSCs during steady-state, cell-cycling, and OXPHOS inhibition. They found that 5-FU administration in mice increased anaerobic glycolytic flux and decreased ATP concentration in HSCs, suggesting that HSC differentiation and cell cycle progression are closely related to intracellular metabolism and can be monitored by measuring ATP concentration. Using the GO-ATeam2 system to analyze ATP levels in single hematopoietic cells, they found that PFKFB3 can accelerate glycolytic ATP production during HSC cell cycling by activating the rate-limiting enzyme PFK of glycolysis. Additionally, by using Pfkfb3 knockout or overexpressing strategies and conducting experiments with cytokine stimulation or transplantation stress, they found that PFKFB3 governs cell cycle progression and promotes the production of differentiated cells from HSCs in proliferative environments by activating glycolysis. Overall, in their study, Watanuki et al combined metabolomic tracing to quantitatively identify metabolic programs of HSCs and found that PFKFB3 confers glycolytic dependence onto HSCs to help coordinate their response to stress. Even so, several important questions need to be addressed as below:

      We sincerely appreciate the constructive feedback from the reviewer. Additional experiments and textual improvements have been made to the manuscript based on your valuable suggestions. In particular, the major revisions are as follows: First, we investigated the extent to which other metabolites, not limited to the glycolytic system, affect metabolism in HSCs after 5-FU treatment. Second, the extent to which PFKFB3 contributes to the expansion of the HSPC pool in the bone marrow was adjusted to make the description more accurate based on the data. Finally, we overexpressed PFKFB3 in HSCs derived from GO-ATeam2 mice and confirmed that PRMT1 inhibition did not reduce the ATP concentration. We believe that the reviewer's valuable comments have further deepened our knowledge of the significance of glycolytic activation by PFKFB3 that we have demonstrated. Our response to the "Recommendations for Authors" is listed first, followed by our responses to all "Public Review" comments as follows:

      (Recommendations For The Authors):

      1. The methods used in key experiments should be described in more detail. For example, in the section on ‘Conversion of GO-ATeam2 fluorescence to ATP concentration’, the knock-in strategy for GO-ATeam2 should be described, as well as U-13C6 -glucose tracer assays.

      As per your recommendation, we have described the key experimental method in more detail in the revised manuscript: the GO-ATeam2 knock-in method was reported by Yamamoto et al. 1. Briefly, they used a CAG promoter-based knock-in strategy targeting the Rosa26 locus to generate GO-ATeam2 knock-in mice. A description of the method has been added to Methods and the reference has been added to the citation.

      For the U-13C6-glucose tracer analysis, the following points were added to describe the details of the analysis: First, a note was added that the number of cells used for the in vitro tracer analysis was the number of cells used for each sample. Second, we added the solution from which the cells were collected by sorting. We added that the incubation was performed under 1% O2 and 5% CO2.

      1. Confusing image label of Supplemental Figure 1H should be corrected in line 253.

      We have corrected the incorrect figure caption on line 217 in the revised manuscript to "Supplemental Figure 1N" as you suggested.

      1. The percentage of the indicated cell population should also be shown in Figure S1B.

      As you indicated, we have included the percentages for each population in Supplemental Figure 1B.

      Author response image 1.

      1. Please pay attention to the small size of the marks in the graph, such as in Figure S1F and so on.

      As you indicated, we have corrected the very small text contained in Figure S1F. Similar corrections have been made to Figures S1B and S5A.

      1. Please pay attention to the label of line in Figure S6A-D.

      Thank you very much for the advice. We have added line labels to the graph in the original Figures S6A–D.

      (Specific comments)

      1. Based on previous reports, the authors expanded the LSK gate to include as many HSCs as possible (Supplemental Figure 1B). However, while they showed the gating strategy on Day 6 after 5-FU treatment, results from other time-points should also be displayed to ensure the strict selection of time-points.

      Thank you for pointing this out. First, we did not enlarge the Sca-1 gating in this study. We apologize for any confusion caused by the incomplete description. The gating of c-Kit is based on that shown by Umemoto et al (Figure EV1A) 2, who used 250 mg/kg 5-FU, so their c-Kit reduction is more pronounced than ours.

      We followed this study and compared c-Kit expression in Lin-Sca-1+CD150+CD48-EPCR+ gates to BMMNCs on day 6 after 5-FU administration (150 mg/kg). The results are shown below.

      Author response image 2.

      Since the MFI of c-Kit was downregulated, we used gating that extended the c-Kit gate to lower-expression regions on day 6 after 5-FU administration (revised Figure S1C). At other time points, LSK gating was the same as in the PBS-treated group, as noted in the Methods.

      1. In Figure 1, the authors examined the metabolite changes on Day 6 after 5-FU treatment. However, it is important to consider whether there are any dynamic adjustments to metabolism during the early and late stages of 5-FU treatment in HSCs compared to PBS treatment, in order to coordinate cell homeostasis despite no significant changes in cell cycle progression at other time-points.

      Thank you for pointing this out. Below are the results of the GO-ATeam2 analysis during the very early phase (day 3) and late phase (day 15) after 5-FU administration (revised Figures S7A–H).

      Author response image 3.

      In the very early phase, such as day 3 after 5-FU administration, cell cycle progression had not started (Figure S1C) and was not preceded by metabolic changes. Meanwhile, in the late phase, such as day 15 after 5-FU administration, the cell cycle and metabolism returned to a steady state. In summary, the timing of the metabolic changes coincided with that of cell cycle progression. This point is essential for discussing the cell cycle-dependent metabolic system of HSCs and has been newly included in the Results (page 11, lines 321-323).

      1. As is well known, ATP can be produced through various pathways, including glycolysis, the TCA cycle, the PPP, NAS, lipid metabolism, amino acid metabolism and so on. Therefore, it is important to investigate whether treatment with 5-FU or oligomycin affects these other metabolic pathways in HSCs.

      As the reviewer pointed out, ATP production by systems other than the glycolytic system of HSCs is also essential. In this revised manuscript, we examined the effects of the FAO inhibitor (Etomoxir, 100 µM) and the glutaminolysis inhibitor 6-diazo-5-oxo-L-norleucine (DON, 2mM) alone or in combination on the ATP concentration of HSCs after PBS or 5-FU treatment. As shown below, there was no apparent decrease in ATP concentration (revised Figures S7J–M).

      Author response image 4.

      Fatty acid β-oxidation activity was also measured in 5-FU-treated HSCs using the fluorescent probe FAOBlue and was unchanged compared to PBS-treated HSCs (revised Figure S7N).

      Author response image 5.

      Notably, the addition of 100 µM etomoxir plus glucose and Pfkfb3 inhibitors resulted in a rapid decrease in ATP concentration in HSCs (revised Figures S7O–P). This indicates that etomoxir partially mimics the effect of oligomycin, suggesting that at a steady state, OXPHOS is driven by FAO, but can be compensated by the acceleration of the glycolytic system by Pfkfb3. Meanwhile, the exposure of HSCs to Pfkfb3 inhibitors in addition to 2 mM DON, which is an extremely high dose considering that the Ki value of DON for glutaminase is 6 µM, did not reduce ATP (revised Figures S7O–P). This suggests that ATP production from glutaminolysis is limited in HSCs at a steady state.

      Author response image 6.

      These points suggest that OXPHOS is driven by fatty acids at a steady state, but unlike the glycolytic system, FAO is not further activated by HSCs after 5-FU treatment. The results of these analyses and related descriptions are included in the revised manuscript (page 11, lines 332-344).

      1. In part 2, they showed that oligomycin treatment of HSCs exhibited activation of the glycolytic system, but what about the changes in ATP concentration under oligomycin treatment? Are other metabolic systems affected by oligomycin treatment?

      Thank you for your thoughtful comments. The relevant results we have obtained so far with the GO-ATeam2 system are as follows: First, OXPHOS inhibition in the absence of glucose significantly decreases the ATP concentration of HSCs (Figure 4C). Meanwhile, OXPHOS inhibition in the presence of glucose maintains the ATP concentration of HSCs (Figure 5B). Since it is difficult to imagine a completely glucose-free environment in vivo, it is thought that ATP concentration is maintained by the acceleration of the glycolytic system even under hypoxic or other conditions that inhibit OXPHOS.

      Meanwhile, glucose tracer analysis shows that OXPHOS inhibition suppresses nucleic acid synthesis (NAS) except for the activation of the glycolytic system (Figures 2C–F). This is because phosphate groups derived from ATP are transferred to nucleotide mono-/di-phosphate in NAS, but OXPHOS, the main source of ATP production, is impaired, along with the enzyme conjugated with OXPHOS in the process of NAS (dihydroorotate dehydrogenase, DHODH). We have added a new paragraph in the Discussion section (page 17, lines 511-515) to provide more insight to the reader by summarizing and discussing these points.

      1. In Figure 5M, it would be helpful to include a control group that was not treated with 2-DG. Additionally, if Figure 5L is used as the control, it is unclear why the level of ATP does not show significant downregulation after 2-DG treatment. Similarly, in Figure 5O, a control group with no glucose addition should be included.

      Thank you for your advice. The experiments corresponding to the control groups in Figures 5M and O were in Figures 5L and N, respectively, but we have combined them into one graph (revised Figures 5L–M). The results more clearly show that PFKFB3 overexpression enhances sensitivity to 2-DG, but also enhances glycolytic activation upon oligomycin administration.

      Author response image 7.

      1. In this study, their findings suggest that PFKFB3 is required for glycolysis of HSCs under stress, including transplantation. In Figure 7B, the results showed that donor-derived chimerism in PB cells decreased relative to that in the WT control group during the early phase (1 month post-transplant) but recovered thereafter. Although the transplantation cell number is equal in two groups of donor cells, it is unclear why the donor-derived cell count decreased in the 2-week post-transplantation period and recovered thereafter in the Pfkgb3 KO group. Therefore, they should provide an explanation for this. Additionally, they only detected the percentage of donor-derived cells in PB but not from BM, which makes it difficult to support the argument for Increasing the HSPC pool.

      As pointed out by the reviewer, it is interesting to note that the decrease in peripheral blood chimerism in the PFKFB3 knockout is limited to immediately after transplantation and then catches up with the control group (Figure 7B). We attribute this to the fact that HSPC proliferation is delayed immediately after transplantation in PFKFB3 deficiency, but after a certain time, PB cells produced by the delayed proliferating HSPCs are supplied. In support of this, the PFKFB3 knockout HSPCs did not exhibit increased cell death after transplantation (Figure 7K), while a delayed cell cycle was observed (Figures 7G–J). A description of this point has been added to the Discussion (page 19, lines 573-579).

      In addition, the knockout efficiency in bone marrow cells could not be verified because the number of cells required for KO efficiency analysis was not available. Therefore, we have added a statement on this point and have toned down our overall claim regarding the extent to which PFKFB3 is involved in the expansion of the HSPC pool (page 15, lines 474-476).

      1. In Figure 7E, they collected the BM reconstructed with Pfkfb3- or Rosa-KO HSPCs two months after transplantation, and then tested their resistance to 5-FU. However, the short duration of the reconstruction period makes it difficult to draw conclusions about the effects on steady-state blood cell production.

      We agree that we cannot conclude from this experiment alone that PFKFB3 is completely unnecessary in steady state because, as you pointed out, the observation period of the experiment in Figure 7E is not long. We have toned down the claim by stating that PFKFB3 is only less necessary in steady-state HSCs compared to proliferative HSCs (page 15, lines 460-461).

      1. PFK is allosterically activated by PFKFB, and other members of the PFKFB family could also participate in the glycolytic program. Therefore, they should investigate their function in contributing to glycolytic plasticity in HSCs during proliferation. Additionally, they should also analyze the protein expression and modification levels of other members. Although PFKFB3 is the most favorable for PFK activation, the role of other members should also be explored in HSC cell cycling to provide sufficient reasoning for choosing PFKFB3.

      To further justify why we chose PFKFB3 among the PFKFB family members, we reviewed our data and the publicly available Gene Expression Commons (GEXC) 3. PFKFB3 is the most highly expressed member of the PFKFB family in HSCs (revised Figure 4F), and its expression increases with proliferation (Author response image 9). In addition to this, we have also cited the literature 4 indicating that AZ PFKFB3 26 is a Pfkfb3-specific inhibitor that we used in this paper, and added a note to this point (that it is specific) (page 11, lines 327-329). Through these revisions, we sought to strengthen the rationale for Pfkfb3 as the primary target of the analysis.

      Author response image 8.

      Author response image 9.

      1. In this study, the authors identified PRMT1 as the upstream regulator of PFKFB3 that is involved in the glycolysis activation of HSCs. However, PRMT1 is also known to participate in various transcriptional activations. Thus, it is important to determine whether PRMT1 affects glycolysis through transcriptional regulation or through its direct regulation of PFKFB3? Additionally, the authors should investigate whether PRMT1i inhibits ATP production in normal HSCs. Moreover, could we combine Figure 6I and 6J for analysis. Finally, the authors could conduct additional rescue experiments to demonstrate that the effect of PRMT1 inhibitors on ATP production can be rescued by overexpression of PFKFB3.

      Although PRMT1 inhibition reduced m-PFKFB3 levels in HSCs, 5-FU treatment also reduced or did not alter Pfkfb3 transcript levels (Figures 6B, G) and the expression of genes such as Hoxa7/9/10, Itga2b, and Nqo1, which are representative transcriptional targets of PRMT1, in proliferating HSCs after 5-FU treatment (revised Figure S9).

      Author response image 10.

      These results suggest that PRMT1 promotes PFKFB3 methylation, which increases independently of transcription in HSCs after 5-FU treatment.

      A summary analysis of the original Figures 6I and 6J is shown below (revised Figure 6I).

      Author response image 11.

      Finally, we tested whether the inhibition of the glycolytic system and the decrease in ATP concentration due to PRMT1 inhibition could be rescued by the retroviral overexpression of PFKFB3. We found that PFKFB3 overexpression did not decrease the ATP concentration in HSCs due to PRMT1 inhibition (revised Figure 6J). Therefore, PFKFB3 overexpression mitigated the decrease in ATP concentration caused by PRMT1 inhibition. These data and related statements have been added to the revised manuscript (page 14, lines 427-428).

      Author response image 12.

      Reviewer #2:

      In the manuscript Watanuki et al. want to define the metabolic profile of HSCs in stress/proliferative (myelosuppression with 5-FU), and mitochondrial inhibition and homeostatic conditions. Their conclusions are that during proliferation HSCs rely more on glycolysis (as other cell types) while HSCs in homeostatic conditions are mostly dependent on mitochondrial metabolism. Mitochondrial inhibition is used to demonstrate that blocking mitochondrial metabolism results in similar features of proliferative conditions.

      The authors used state-of-the-art technologies that allow metabolic readout in a limited number of cells like rare HSCs. These applications could be of help in the field since one of the major issues in studying HSCs metabolism is the limited sensitivity of the“"standard”" assays, which make them not suitable for HSC studies.

      However, the observations do not fully support the claims. There are no direct evidence/experiments tackling cell cycle state and metabolism in HSCs. Often the observations for their claims are indirect, while key points on cell cycle state-metabolism, OCR analysis should be addressed directly.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's constructive comments. Thank you for highlighting the importance of the highly sensitive metabolic assay developed in this study and the findings based on it. Meanwhile, the reviewer's comments have made us aware of areas where we can further improve this manuscript. In particular, in the revised manuscript, we have performed further studies to demonstrate the link between the cell cycle and metabolic state. Specifically, we further subdivided HSCs by the uptake of in vivo-administered 2-NBDG and performed cell cycle analysis. Next, HSCs after PBS or 5-FU treatment were analyzed by a Mito Stress test using the Seahorse flux analyzer, including ECAR and OCR, and a more direct relationship between the cell cycle state and the metabolic system was found. We believe that the reviewer's valuable suggestions have helped us clarify more directly the importance of the metabolic state of HSCs in response to cell cycle and stress that we wanted to show and emphasize the usefulness of the GO-ATeam2 system. Our response to "Recommendations For The Authors" is listed first, followed by our responses to all comments in "Public Review" as follows:

      (Recommendations For The Authors):

      In general, I believe it would be important:

      1. to directly associate cell cycle state with metabolic state. For example, by sorting HSC (+/- 5FU) based on their cell cycle state (exploiting the mouse model presented in the manuscript or by defining G0/G1/G2-S-M via Pyronin/Hoechst staining which allow to sort live cells) and follow the fate of radiolabeled glucose.

      Thank you for raising these crucial points. Unfortunately, it was difficult to perform the glucose tracer analysis by preparing HSCs with different cell cycle states as you suggested due to the amount of work involved. In particular, in the 5-FU group, more than 60 mice per group were originally required for an experiment, and further cell cycle-based purification would require many times that number of mice, which we felt was unrealistic under current technical standards. As an alternative, we administered 2-NBDG to mice and fractionated HSCs at the 2-NBDG fluorescence level for cell cycle analysis. The results are shown below (revised Figure S1M). Notably, even in the PBS-treated group, HSCs with high 2-NBDG uptake were more proliferative than those with low 2-NBDG uptake and are comparable to HSCs after 5-FU treatment, although the overall population of HSCs exiting the G0 phase and entering the G1 phase increased after 5-FU treatment. In both PBS/5-FU-treated groups, these large differences in cell cycle glucose utilization suggest a direct link between HSC proliferation and glycolysis activation. If a more sensitive type of glucose tracer analysis becomes available in the future, it may be possible to directly address the reviewer's comments. We see this as a topic for the future. The descriptions of the above findings and perspectives have been added to the Results and Discussion section (page 7, lines 208-214, page 20, lines 607-610).

      Author response image 13.

      1. Use other radio labeled substrates (fatty acid, glutamate)

      Thank you very much for your suggestion. While this is an essential point for future studies, we believe it is not the primary focus of the paper. We are planning another research project on tracer analysis using labeled fatty acids and glutamates, which we will report on in the near future. We have clearly stated in the Abstract and Introduction of the revised manuscript, that the focus of this study is on changes in glucose metabolism when HSCs are stressed (page 3, line 75 and 87, page 5, lines 135).

      Instead, we added the following analyses of metabolic changes in fatty acids and glutamate using the GO-ATeam2 system. HSCs derived from GO-ATeam2 mice treated with PBS or 5-FU were used to measure changes in ATP concentrations after exposure to the fatty acid beta-oxidation (FAO) inhibitor etomoxir and the glutaminolysis inhibitor 6-diazo-5-oxo-L-norleucine (DON). Etomoxir was used at 100 µM, a concentration that inhibits FAO without inhibiting mitochondrial electron transfer complex I, as previously reported 5. DON was used at 2 mM, a concentration that sufficiently inhibits the enzyme as the Ki for glutaminase is 6 µM. In this experiment, etomoxir alone, DON alone, or etomoxir and DON in combination did not decrease the ATP concentration of HSCs in the PBS and 5-FU groups (revised Figures S7J–M), suggesting that FAO and glutaminolysis were not essential for ATP production in HSCs in the short term. Thus, according to the analysis using the GO-Ateam2 system, HSCs exposed to acute stresses change the efficiency of glucose utilization (accelerated glycolytic ATP production) rather than other energy sources. Since there are reports that FAO and glutaminolysis are required for HSC maintenance in the long term 5,6, compensatory pathways may be able to maintain ATP levels in the short term. A description of these points has been added to the Discussion (page 11, lines 332-344).

      Author response image 14.

      1. Include OCR analyses.

      In addition to the ECAR data of the Mito Stress test (original Figures 2G–H), OCR data were added to the revised manuscript (revised Figures 2H, S3D). Compared to c-Kit+ myeloid progenitors (LKS- cells), HSC showed a similar increase in ECAR, while the decrease in OCR was relatively limited. A possible explanation for this is that glycolytic and mitochondrial metabolism are coupled in c-Kit+ myeloid progenitors, whereas they are decoupled in HSCs. This is also suggested by the glucose plus oligomycin experiment in Figures 5B, C, and S6A–D (orange lines). In summary, in HSCs, glycolytic and mitochondrial ATP production are decoupled and can maintain ATP levels by glycolytic ATP production alone, whereas in progenitors including GMPs, the two ATP production systems are constantly coupled, and glycolysis alone cannot maintain ATP concentration. We have added descriptions of these points in the Results and Discussion section (page 8, lines 240-243, page 18, lines 558-561).

      Author response image 15.

      Next, a Mito Stress test was performed using HSCs derived from PBS- or 5-FU-treated mice in the presence or absence of oligomycin (revised Figures 1G–H, S3A–B). Without oligomycin treatment, ECAR in 5-FU-treated HSCs was higher than in PBS-treated HSCs, and OCR was unchanged. Oligomycin treatment increased ECAR in both PBS- and 5-FU-treated HSCs, whereas OCR was unchanged in PBS-treated HSCs, but significantly decreased in 5-FU-treated HSCs. Changes in ECAR in response to oligomycin differed between HSC proliferation or differentiation: ECAR increased in 5-FU-treated HSCs but not in LKS- progenitors (original Figures 2G–H). This suggests a metabolic feature of HSCs in which the coupling of OXPHOS with glycolysis seen in LKS- cells is not essential in HSCs even after cell cycle entry. The results and discussion of this experiment have been added to page 7, lines 194-201 and page 18, lines 558-561).

      Author response image 16.

      1. Correlate proliferation-mitochondrial inhibition-metabolic state

      We agree that it is important to clarify this point. First, OXPHOS inhibition and proliferation similarly accelerate glycolytic ATP production with PFKFB3 (Figures 4G, I, and 5F–I). Meanwhile, oligomycin treatment rapidly decreases ATP in HSCs with or without 5-FU administration (Figure 4C). These results suggest that OXPHOS is a major source of ATP production both at a steady state and during proliferation, even though the analysis medium is pre-saturated with hypoxia similar to that in vivo. This has been added to the Discussion section (page 17, lines 520-523).

      1. Tune down the claim on HSCs in homeostatic conditions since from the data it seems that HSCs rely more on anaerobic glycolysis.

      Thanks for the advice. The original Figures S2C, D, F, and G show that HSC is dependent on the anaerobic glycolytic system even at a steady state, so we have toned down our claims (page 7, lines 192-194).

      1. For proliferative HSCs mitochondrial are key. When you block mitochondria with oligomycin there's the biggest drop in ATP.

      In the revised manuscript, we have tried to highlight the key findings that you have pointed out. First, we mentioned in the Discussion (page 17, lines 523-525) that previous studies suggested the importance of mitochondria in proliferating HSCs. Meanwhile, the GO-ATeam2 and glucose tracer analyses in this study newly revealed that the glycolytic system activated by PFKFB3 is activated during the proliferative phase, as shown in Figure 4C. We also confirmed that mitochondrial ATP production is vital in proliferating HSCs, and we hope to clarify the balance between ATP-producing pathways and nutrient sources in future studies.

      1. To better clarify this point authors, authors should do experiments in hypoxic conditions and compare it to oligomycin treatment and showing that mito-inhibition acts differently on HSCs (considering that all these drugs are toxic for mitochondria and induce rapidly stress responses ex: mitophagy).

      We apologize for any confusion caused by not clearly describing the experimental conditions. As pointed out by the reviewer, we also recognize the importance of experiments in a hypoxic environment. All GO-ATeam2 analyses were performed in a medium saturated sufficiently under hypoxic conditions and analyzed within minutes, so we believe that the medium did not become oxygenated (page S5-S6, lines 160-163 in the Methods). Despite being conducted under such hypoxic conditions, the substantial decrease in ATP after oligomycin treatment is intriguing (original Figures 4C, 5B, 5C). The p50 value of mitochondria (the partial pressure of oxygen at which respiration is half maximal) is 0.1 kPa, which is less than 0.1% of the oxygen concentration at atmospheric pressure 7. Thus, biochemically, it is consistent that OXPHOS can maintain sufficient activity even in a hypoxic environment like the bone marrow. We are currently embarking on a study to determine ATP concentration in physiological hypoxic conditions using in vivo imaging within the bone marrow, which we hope to report in a separate project. We have discussed these points, technical limitations, and perspectives in the Discussion section (page 20, lines 610-612).

      • In Figure 1 C, D, E and F, the comparison should be done as unpaired t test and the control group should not be 1 as the cells comes from different individuals.

      Thank you very much for pointing this out. We have reanalyzed and revised the figures (revised Figures 1C–F)

      Author response image 17.

      • In Figure S2A, the post-sorting bar of 6PG, R5P and S7P are missing.

      Metabolites below the detection threshold (post-sorting samples of 6PG, R5P, and S7P) are now indicated as N.D. (not detected) (revised Figure S2A).

      Author response image 18.

      • In the 2NBDG experiments, authors should add the appropriate controls, since it has been shown that 2NBDG cellular uptake do not correctly reflect glucose uptake (Sinclair LV, Immunometabolism 2020) (a cell type dependent variations) thus inhibitors of glucose transporters should be added as controls (cytochalasin B; 4,6-O-ethylidene-a-D-glucose) it would be quite challenging to test it in vivo but it would be sufficient to show that in vitro in the different HSPCs analyzed.

      We appreciate the essential technical point raised by the reviewer. In the revised manuscript, we performed a 2-NBDG assay with cytochalasin B and phloretin as negative controls. After PBS treatment, 2-NBDG uptake was higher in 5-FU-treated HSCs compared to untreated HSCs. This increase was inhibited by both cytochalasin B and phloretin. In PBS-treated HSCs, cytochalasin B did not downregulate 2-NBDG uptake, whereas phloretin did. Although cytochalasin B inhibits glucose transporters (GLUTs), it is also an inhibitor of actin polymerization. Therefore, its inhibitory effect on GLUTs may be weaker than that of phloretin. We have revised the figure (revised Figure S1L) and added the corresponding description (page 7, lines 207-208).

      Author response image 19.

      • S5C: authors should show the cell number for each population. If there's a decreased in % in Lin- that will be reflected in all HSPCs. Comparing the proportion of the cells doesn't show the real impact on HSPCs.

      Thank you for your insightful point. In the revision, we compared the numbers, not percentages, of HSPCs and found no difference in the number of cells in the major HSPC fractions in Lin-. The figure has been revised (revised Figure S6C) and the corresponding description has been added (page 10, lines 296-299).

      Author response image 20.

      Minor:

      1. In S1 F-G is not indicated in which day post 5FU injection is done the analysis. I assume on day 6 but it should be indicated in the figure legend and/or text.

      Thank you for pointing this out. As you assumed, the analysis was performed on day 6. The description has been added to the legend of the revised Figure S1G.

      1. S1K is not described in the text. What are proliferative and quiescence-maintaining conditions? The analyses are done by flow using LKS SLAM markers after culture? How long was the culture?

      Thank you for your comments. First, the figure citation on line 250 was incorrect and has been corrected to Figure S1N. Regarding the proliferative and quiescence-maintaining conditions, we have previously reported on these 8. In brief, these are culture conditions that maintain HSC activity at a high level while allowing for the proliferation or maintenance of HSCs in quiescence, achieved by culturing under fatty acid-rich, hypoxic conditions with either high or low cytokine concentrations. Analysis was performed after one week of culture, with the HSC number determined by flow cytometry based on the LSK-SLAM marker. While these are mentioned in the Methods section, we have added a description in the main text to highlight these points for the reader (page 7, lines 214-217).

      1. In Figure 5G, why does the blue line (PFKFB3 inhibitor) go up in the end of the real-time monitoring? Does it mean that other compensatory pathway is turned on?

      As you have pointed out, we cannot rule out the possibility that other unknown compensatory ATP production pathways were activated. We have added a note in the Discussion section to address this (page 18, lines 555-556).

      1. In Figure S6H&J, the reduction is marginal. Does it mean that PKM2 is not important for ATP production in HSCs?

      The activity of the inhibitor is essential in the GO-ATeam2 analysis. The commercially available PKM2 inhibitors have a higher IC50 value (IC50 = 2.95 μM in this case). Nevertheless, the effect of reducing the ATP concentration was observed in progenitor cells, but not in HSCs. The report by Wang et al. 9 on the analysis using a PKM2-deficient model suggests a stronger effect on progenitor cells than on HSCs. Our results are similar to those of the previous report.

      (Specific comments)

      Specifically, there are several major points that rise concerns about the claims:

      1. The gating strategy to select HSCs with enlarged Sca1 gating is not convincing. I understand the rationale to have a sufficient number of cells to analyze, however this gating strategy should be applied also in the control group. From the FACS plot seems that there are more HSCs upon 5FU treatment (Figure S1b). How that is possible? Is it because of the 20% more of cycling cells at day 6? To prove that this gating strategy still represents a pure HSC population, authors should compare the blood reconstitution capability of this population with a "standard" gated population. If the starting population is highly heterogeneous then the metabolic readout could simply reflect cell heterogeneity.

      Thank you for pointing this out. First, we did not enlarge the Sca-1 gating in this study. We apologize for any confusion caused by the incomplete description. The gating of c-Kit is based on that shown by Umemoto et al (Figure EV1A) 2, who used 250 mg/kg 5-FU, so their c-Kit reduction is more pronounced than ours.

      We followed this study and compared c-Kit expression in the Lin-Sca-1+CD150+CD48-EPCR+ gates to BMMNCs on day 6 after 5-FU administration (150 mg/kg). The results are shown below.

      Author response image 21.

      Since the MFI of c-Kit was downregulated, we used gating that extended the c-Kit gate to lower expression regions on day 6 after 5-FU administration (revised Figure S1C).

      At other time points, LSK gating was the same as in the PBS-treated group, as noted in the Methods.

      The reason why the number of HSCs appears to be higher in the 5-FU group is because most of the differentiated blood cells were lost due to 5-FU administration and the same number of cells as in the PBS group were analyzed by FACS, resulting in a relatively higher number of HSCs. The legend of Figure S1 shows that the number of HSCs in both the PBS and 5-FU groups appeared to increase because the same number of BMMNCs was obtained at the time of analysis (page S22, lines 596-598).

      Regarding cellular heterogeneity, from a metabolic point of view, the heterogeneity in HSCs is rather reduced by 5-FU administration. As shown in Figure S3A–C, this is simulated under stress conditions, such as after 5-FU administration or during OXPHOS inhibition, where the flux variability in each enzymatic reaction is significantly reduced. GO-ATeam2 analysis after 5-FU treatment showed no increase in cell population variability. After 2-DG treatment, ATP concentrations in HSCs were widely distributed from 0 mM to 0.8 mM in the PBS group, while more than 80% of those in the 5-FU group were less than 0.4 mM (Figures 4B, D). HSCs may have a certain metabolic diversity at a steady state, but under stress conditions, they may switch to a more specialized metabolism with less cellular heterogeneity in order to adapt.

      1. S2 does not show major differences before and after sorting. However, a key metabolite like Lactate is decreased, which is also one of the most present. Wouldn't that mean that HSCs once they move out from the hypoxic niche, they decrease lactate production? Do they decrease anaerobic glycolysis? How can quiescent HSC mostly rely on OXPHOS being located in hypoxic niche?

      2. Since HSCs in the niche are located in hypoxic regions of the bone marrow, would that not mimic OxPhos inhibition (oligomycin)? Would that not mean that HSCs in the niche are more glycolytic (anaerobic glycolysis)?

      3. In Figure 5B, the orange line (Glucose+OXPHOS inhibition) remains stable, which means HSCs prefer to use glycolysis when OXPHOS is inhibited. Which metabolic pathway would HSCs use under hypoxic conditions? As HSCs resides in hypoxic niche, does it mean that these steady-state HSCs prefer to use glycolysis for ATP production? As mentioned before, mitochondrial inhibition can be comparable at the in vivo condition of the niche, where low pO2 will "inhibit" mitochondria metabolism.

      Thank you for the first half of comment 2 on the technical features of our approach. First, as you have pointed out, there is minimal variation and stable detection of many metabolites before and after sorting (Figure S2A), suggesting that isolation from the hypoxic niche and sorting stress do not significantly alter metabolite detection performance. This is consistent with a previous report by Jun et al. 10. Meanwhile, lactate levels decreased by sorting. Therefore, if the activity of anaerobic glycolysis was suppressed in stressed HSCs, it may be difficult to detect these metabolic changes with our tracer analysis. However, in this study, several glycolytic metabolites, including an increase in lactate, were detected in HSCs from 5-FU-treated mice compared with HSCs from PBS-treated mice that were similarly sorted and prepared, suggesting an increase in glycolytic activity. In other words, we may have been fortunate to detect the stress-induced activation of the glycolytic system beyond the characteristic of our analysis system that lactate levels tend to appear lower than they are. Given that damage to the bone marrow hematopoiesis tends to alleviate the low-oxygen status of the niche 11, we postulate that this upregulated aerobic glycolysis arises intrinsically in HSCs rather than from external conditions.

      The second half of comment 2, and comments 7 and 10, are essential and overlapping comments and will be answered together. Although genetic analyses have shown that HSCs produce ATP by anaerobic glycolysis in low-oxygen environments 9,12, our GO-ATeam2 analysis in this study confirmed that HSCs also generate ATP via mitochondria. This is also supported by Ansó's prior findings where the knockout of the Rieske iron–sulfur protein (RISP), a constituent of the mitochondrial electron transport chain, impairs adult HSC quiescence and bone marrow repopulation 13. Bone marrow is a physiologically hypoxic environment (9.9–32.0 mmHg 11). However, the p50 value of mitochondria (the partial pressure of oxygen at which respiration is half maximal) is below 0.1% oxygen concentration at atmospheric pressure (less than 1 mmHg) 7. This suggests that OXPHOS can retain sufficient activity even under physiologically hypoxic conditions. We are currently initiating efforts to discern ATP concentrations in vivo within the bone marrow under physiological hypoxia. This will be reported in a separate project in the future. Admittedly, when we began this research, we did not anticipate the significant mitochondrial reliance of HSCs. As we previously reported, the metabolic uncoupling of glycolysis and mitochondria 12 may enable HSCs to activate only glycolysis, and not mitochondria, under stress conditions such as post-5-FU administration, suggesting a unique metabolic trait of HSCs. We have included these technical limitations and perspectives in the Discussion section (page 17, lines 520-523).

      1. The authors performed challenging experiments to track radiolabeled glucose, which are quite remarkable. However, the data do not fully support the conclusions. Mitochondrial metabolism in HSCs can be supported by fatty acid and glutamate, thus authors should track the fate of other energy sources to fully discriminate the glycolysis vs mito-metabolism dependency. From the data on S2 and Fig1 1C-F, the authors can conclude that upon 5FU treatment HSCs increase glycolytic rate.

      2. FIG.2B-C: Increase of Glycolysis upon oligomycin treatment is common in many different cell types. As explained before, other radiolabeled substrates should be used to understand the real effect on mitochondria metabolism.

      Thank you for your suggestion. While this is essential for future studies, we believe it is not the primary focus of the paper. We are planning another research project on tracer analysis using labeled fatty acids and glutamates, which we will report on in the near future. We have clearly stated in the Abstract and Introduction of the revised manuscript that the focus of this study is on changes in glucose metabolism when HSCs are stressed (page 3, line 75 and 87, page 5, lines 135).

      Instead, we have added the following analyses of metabolic changes in fatty acids and glutamate using the GO-ATeam2 system: HSCs derived from GO-ATeam2 mice treated with PBS or 5-FU were used to measure changes in ATP concentrations after exposure to the fatty acid beta-oxidation (FAO) inhibitor etomoxir and the glutaminolysis inhibitor 6-diazo-5-oxo-L-norleucine (DON). Etomoxir was used at 100 µM, a concentration that inhibits FAO without inhibiting mitochondrial electron transfer complex I, as previously reported 5. DON was used at 2 mM, a concentration that sufficiently inhibits the enzyme as the Ki for glutaminase is 6 µM. In this experiment, etomoxir alone, DON alone, or etomoxir and DON in combination did not decrease the ATP concentration of HSCs in the PBS and 5-FU groups (revised Figures S7J–M), suggesting that FAO and glutaminolysis were not essential for ATP production in HSCs in the short term. Thus, according to the analysis using the GO-Ateam2 system, HSCs exposed to acute stresses change the efficiency of glucose utilization (accelerated glycolytic ATP production) rather than other energy sources. Since there are reports that FAO and glutaminolysis are required for HSC maintenance in the long term 5,6, compensatory pathways may be able to maintain ATP levels in the short term. A description of these points has been added to the Discussion (page 17, lines 525-527).

      Author response image 22.

      Fatty acid β-oxidation activity was also measured in 5-FU-treated HSCs using the fluorescent probe FAOBlue and was unchanged compared to PBS-treated HSCs (revised Figure S7N).

      Author response image 23.

      Notably, the addition of 100 µM etomoxir plus glucose and Pfkfb3 inhibitors resulted in a rapid decrease in ATP concentration in HSCs (revised Figures S7O–P). This indicates that etomoxir partially mimics the effect of oligomycin, suggesting that at a steady state, OXPHOS is driven by FAO, but can be compensated by the acceleration of the glycolytic system by Pfkfb3. Meanwhile, the exposure of HSCs to Pfkfb3 inhibitors in addition to 2 mM DON did not reduce ATP (revised Figures S7O–P). This suggests that ATP production from glutaminolysis is limited in HSCs at a steady state.

      Author response image 24.

      These points suggest that OXPHOS is driven by fatty acids at a steady state, but unlike the glycolytic system, FAO is not further activated by HSCs after 5-FU treatment. The results of these analyses and related descriptions are included in the revised manuscript (page 11, lines 332-344).

      1. In Figure S1, 5-FU leads to the induction of cycling HSCs and in figure 1, 5-FU results in higher activation of glycolysis. Would it be possible to correlate these two phenotypes together? For example, by sorting NBDG+ cells and checking the cell cycle status of these cells?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments. We administered 2-NBDG to mice and fractionated HSCs at the 2-NBDG fluorescence level for cell cycle analysis. The results are shown below (revised Figure S1M). Notably, even in the PBS-treated group, HSCs with high 2-NBDG uptake were more proliferative than HSCs with low 2-NBDG uptake and were comparable to HSCs after 5-FU treatment, although the overall population of HSCs that exited the G0 phase and entered the G1 phase increased after 5-FU treatment. In both PBS/5-FU-treated groups, these profound differences in cell cycle glucose utilization suggest a direct link between HSC proliferation and glycolysis activation. Descriptions of the above findings and perspectives have been added to the Results and Discussion section (page 7, lines 208-214, page 20, lines 607-610).

      Author response image 25.

      1. Why are only ECAR measurements (and not OCR measurements) shown? In Fig.2G, why are HSCs compared with cKit+ myeloid progenitors, and not with MPP1? The ECAR increased observed in HSC upon oligomycin treatment is shared with many other types of cells. However, cKit+ cells have a weird behavior. Upon oligo treatment cKit+ cells decrease ECAR, which is quite unusual. The data of both HSCs and cKit+ cells could be clarified by adding OCR curves. Moreover, it is recommended to run glycolysis stress test profile to assess the dependency to glycolysis (Glucose, Oligomycin, 2DG).

      In addition to the ECAR data of the Mito Stress test (original Figures 2G–H), OCR data were added in the revised manuscript (revised Figures 2H, S3D). Compared to c-Kit+ myeloid progenitors (LKS- cells), HSC exhibited a similar increase in ECAR, while the decrease in OCR was relatively limited. This may be because glycolytic and mitochondrial metabolism are coupled in c-Kit+ myeloid progenitors, whereas they are decoupled in HSCs. This is also suggested by the glucose plus oligomycin experiment in Figures 5B, C, and S6A–D (orange lines). In summary, in HSCs, glycolytic and mitochondrial ATP production are decoupled and can maintain ATP levels by glycolytic ATP production alone, whereas in progenitors including GMPs, the two ATP production systems are constantly coupled, and glycolysis alone cannot maintain the ATP concentration. While we could not conduct a glycolysis stress test, we believe that Pfkfb3-dependent glycolytic activation, which is evident in the oligomycin+glucose+Pfkfb3i experiment, is only apparent in HSCs when subjected to glucose+oligomycin treatment (original Figures 5F–I). We have added descriptions of these points in the Results and Discussion section (page 8, lines 240-243, page 18, lines 558-561).

      Author response image 26.

      FIG.3 A-C. As mentioned previously, the flux analyses should be integrated with data using other energy sources. If cycling HSCs are less dependent to OXPHOS, what happen if you inhibit OXHPHOS in 5-FU condition? Since the authors are linking OXPHOS inhibition and upregulation of Glycolysis to increase proliferation, do HSCs proliferate more when treated with oligomycin?

      First, please see our response to comments 3 and 5 regarding the first part of this comment about the flux analysis of other energy sources. According to the analysis using the GO-Ateam2 system, stressed HSCs change the efficiency of glucose utilization (accelerated glycolytic ATP production) rather than other energy sources. The change in ATP concentration after OXPHOS inhibition for 5-FU-treated HSCs is shown in Figures 4C and E, suggesting that the activity of OXPHOS itself does not increase. HSCs after oligomycin treatment and HSCs after 5-FU treatment are similar in that they activate glycolytic ATP production. However, inhibition of OXPHOS did not induce the proliferation of HSCs (original Figure S1K). This suggests that proliferation activates glycolysis and not that activation of the glycolytic system induces proliferation. This similarity and dissimilarity of glycolytic activation upon proliferation and OXPHOS inhibition is discussed in the Discussion section (page 16-17, lines 505-515).

      1. FIG.4 shows that in vivo administration of radiolabeled glucose especially marks metabolites of TCA cycle and Glycolysis. The authors interpret enhanced anaerobic glycolysis, but I am not sure this is correct; if more glycolysis products go in the TCA cycle, it might mean that HSC start engaging mitochondrial metabolism. What do the authors think about that?

      Thank you for pointing this out. We believe that the data are due to two differences in the experimental features between in vivo (Figure S5) and in vitro (Figures 1 and S2) tracer analysis. The first difference is that in in vivo tracer analysis, unlike in vitro, all cells can metabolize U-13C6-glucose. Another difference is that after glucose labeling in vivo, it takes approximately 120–180 minutes to purify HSCs to extract metabolites, and processing on ice may result in a gradual progression of metabolic reactions within HSCs. As a result, in vivo tracer analysis may detect an increased influx of labeled carbon derived from U-13C6-glucose into the TCA cycle over an extended period. However, it is difficult to interpret whether this influx of labeled carbon is derived from the direct influx of glycolysis or the re-uptake by HSCs of metabolites that have been metabolized to other metabolites in other cells. Meanwhile, as shown in Figure 4C using the GO-ATeam2 system, ATP production from mitochondria is not upregulated by 5-FU treatment. This suggests that even if the direct influx from glycolysis into the TCA cycle is increased, the rate of ATP production does not exceed that of glycolysis. Despite these technical caveats in interpretation, the results of in vivo and in vitro tracer analyses are considered essential. In particular, we consider the increased labeling of metabolites involved in glycolysis and nucleotide synthesis to be crucial. We have added a discussion of these points, including experimental limitations (page 17-18, lines 530-545).

      1. FIG.4: the experimental design is not clear. Are BMNNCs stained and then put in culture? Is it 6-day culture or BMNNCs are purified at day 6 post 5FU? FIG-4B-C The difference between PBS vs 5FU conditions are the most significant; however, the effect of oligomycin in both conditions is the most dramatic one. From this readout, it seems that HSCs are more dependent on mitochondria for energy production both upon 5FU treatment and in PBS conditions.

      We apologize for the incomplete description of the experimental details. The experiment involved dispensing freshly stained BMMNC with surface antigens into the medium and immediately subjecting them to flow cytometry analysis. For post-5-FU treatment HSCs, mice were administered with 5-FU (day 1), and freshly obtained BMMNCs were analyzed on day 6. The analysis of HSCs and progenitors was performed by gating each fraction within the BMMNC (original Figure S5A). We have added these details to ensure that readers can grasp these aspects more clearly (page S5, lines 155-158).

      As pointed out by the reviewer, we understand that HSCs produce more ATP through OXPHOS. However, ATP production by glycolysis, although limited, is observed under steady-state conditions (post-PBS treatment HSC), and its reliance increases during the proliferation phase (post-5-FU treatment HSC) (original Figures 4B, D). Until now, discussions on energy production in HSCs have focused on either glycolysis or mitochondrial functions. However, with the GO-ATeam2 system, it has become possible for the first time to compare their contributions to ATP production and evaluate compensatory pathways. As a result, it became evident that while OXPHOS is the main source of ATP production, the reliance on glycolysis plastically increases in response to stress. This has led to a better understanding of HSC metabolism. These points are included in the Discussion as well (page 16, lines 479-488).

      1. FIG.6H should be extended with cell cycle analyses. There are no differences between 5FU and ctrl groups. If 5FU induces HSCs cycling and increases glycolysis I would expect higher 2-NBDG uptake in the 5FU group. How do the authors explain this?

      Thank you for your comments. In the original Figure 6H, we found that 2-NBDG uptake correlated with mPFKFB3 levels in both the 5-FU and PBS groups. mPfkfb3 levels remained low in the few HSCs with low 2-NBDG uptake in the 5-FU group.

      In the revised manuscript, to directly relate glucose utilization to the cell cycle, we administered 2-NBDG to mice and fractionated HSCs at the 2-NBDG fluorescence level for cell cycle analysis. The results are shown below (revised Figure S1M). Notably, even in the PBS-treated group, HSCs with high 2-NBDG uptake were more proliferative than those with low 2-NBDG uptake and are comparable to HSCs after 5-FU treatment, although the overall population of HSCs that exited the G0 phase and entered the G1 phase increased after 5-FU treatment. The large differences in glucose utilization per cell cycle observed in both PBS/5-FU-treated groups suggest a direct link between HSC proliferation and glycolysis activation. Descriptions of the above findings have been added to the Results and Discussion ((page 7, lines 208-214, page 20, lines 607-610).

      Author response image 27.

      1. In S7 the experimental design is not clear. What are quiescent vs proliferative conditions? What does it mean "cell number of HSC-derived colony"? Is it a CFU assay? Then you should show colony numbers. When HSCs proliferate, they need more energy thus inhibition of metabolism will impact proliferation. What happens if you inhibit mitochondrial metabolism with oligomycin?

      Regarding the proliferative and quiescence-maintaining conditions, we have previously reported on these 8. In brief, these are culture conditions that maintain HSC activity at a high level while allowing for the proliferation or maintenance of HSCs in quiescence, achieved by culturing under fatty acid-rich, hypoxic conditions with either high or low cytokine concentrations. Analysis was performed after one week of culture, with the HSC number determined by flow cytometry based on the LSK-SLAM marker. While these are mentioned in the Methods section, we have added a description in the main text to highlight these points for the reader (page 7, lines 214-217).

      In vitro experiments with the oligomycin treatment of HSCs showed that OXPHOS inhibition activates the glycolytic system, but does not induce HSC proliferation (original Figure S1K). This suggests that proliferation activates glycolysis and not that activation of the glycolytic system induces proliferation. This similarity and dissimilarity of glycolytic activation upon proliferation and OXPHOS inhibition is discussed in the Discussion (page 16-17, lines 505-515).

      1. In FIG 7 since homing of HSCs is influenced by the cell cycle state, should be important to show if in the genetic model for PFKFB3 in HSCs there's a difference in homing efficiency.

      In response to the reviewer's comments, we knocked out PFKFB3 in HSPCs derived from Ubc-GFP mice, transplanted 200,000 HSPCs into recipients (C57BL/6 mice) post-8.5Gy irradiation, and harvested the bone marrow of recipients after 16 h to compare homing efficiency (revised Figure S10H). Even with the knockout of PFKFB3, no significant difference in homing efficiency was detected compared to the control group (Rosa knockout group). These results suggest that the short-term reduction in chimerism due to PFKFB3 knockout is not due to decreased homing efficiency or cell death by apoptosis (Figure 7K) but a transient delay in cell cycle progression. We have added descriptions regarding these findings in the Results and Discussion sections (page 15, lines 470-471, page 19, lines 576-578).

      Author response image 28.

      1. Yamamoto M, Kim M, Imai H, Itakura Y, Ohtsuki G. Microglia-Triggered Plasticity of Intrinsic Excitability Modulates Psychomotor Behaviors in Acute Cerebellar Inflammation. Cell Rep. 2019;28(11):2923-2938 e2928.

      2. Umemoto T, Johansson A, Ahmad SAI, et al. ATP citrate lyase controls hematopoietic stem cell fate and supports bone marrow regeneration. EMBO J. 2022:e109463.

      3. Seita J, Sahoo D, Rossi DJ, et al. Gene Expression Commons: an open platform for absolute gene expression profiling. PLoS One. 2012;7(7):e40321.

      4. Boyd S, Brookfield JL, Critchlow SE, et al. Structure-Based Design of Potent and Selective Inhibitors of the Metabolic Kinase PFKFB3. J Med Chem. 2015;58(8):3611-3625.

      5. Ito K, Carracedo A, Weiss D, et al. A PML–PPAR-δ pathway for fatty acid oxidation regulates hematopoietic stem cell maintenance. Nat Med. 2012;18(9):1350-1358.

      6. Oburoglu L, Tardito S, Fritz V, et al. Glucose and glutamine metabolism regulate human hematopoietic stem cell lineage specification. Cell Stem Cell. 2014;15(2):169-184.

      7. Gnaiger E, Mendez G, Hand SC. High phosphorylation efficiency and depression of uncoupled respiration in mitochondria under hypoxia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(20):11080-11085.

      8. Kobayashi H, Morikawa T, Okinaga A, et al. Environmental Optimization Enables Maintenance of Quiescent Hematopoietic Stem Cells Ex Vivo. Cell Rep. 2019;28(1):145-158 e149.

      9. Wang YH, Israelsen WJ, Lee D, et al. Cell-state-specific metabolic dependency in hematopoiesis and leukemogenesis. Cell. 2014;158(6):1309-1323.

      10. Jun S, Mahesula S, Mathews TP, et al. The requirement for pyruvate dehydrogenase in leukemogenesis depends on cell lineage. Cell Metab. 2021;33(9):1777-1792 e1778.

      11. Spencer JA, Ferraro F, Roussakis E, et al. Direct measurement of local oxygen concentration in the bone marrow of live animals. Nature. 2014;508(7495):269-273.

      12. Takubo K, Nagamatsu G, Kobayashi CI, et al. Regulation of glycolysis by Pdk functions as a metabolic checkpoint for cell cycle quiescence in hematopoietic stem cells. Cell Stem Cell. 2013;12(1):49-61.

      13. Anso E, Weinberg SE, Diebold LP, et al. The mitochondrial respiratory chain is essential for haematopoietic stem cell function. Nat Cell Biol. 2017;19(6):614-625.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      eLife Assessment

      For decades it has been accepted that only the growth-arrested "stumpy" form of Trypanosoma brucei can infect the arthropod vector, the Tsetse fly, but this was recently challenged by a demonstration that - under artificial conditions that are known to enhance infectivity - the proliferative "slender" form can also establish Tsetse infections. The infectiousness of the two forms is a fundamental question in trypanosome biology and epidemiology, concerning both infection dynamics and parasite differentiation. The authors of the current study provide compelling evidence that without artificial enhancement, the "stumpy" form is indeed much more infective for Tsetse than the slender form; they suggest that this is probably also true in the wild.

      Since the authors of this paper did not themselves test the effect of enhancing conditions, the precise reason for the discrepancy in results between the two laboratories has not been demonstrated conclusively.

      This specific comment was addressed in the revision and illustrated with new data.

      Differences between the strain clones, the cell culture conditions and/or the fly colony maintenance conditions could explain part of the differences in infection rates observed here as compared to the Schuster et al. study (1). However, the use of the lectin-inhibitory sugar N-acetyl-glucosamine to enhance infection rates in the latter study could be a more likely explanation. To assess this hypothesis, an additional experimental challenge was performed to compare infection rates in teneral versus adult flies, with or without N-acetyl-glucosamine supplement in an infective meal containing 10<sup>5</sup> slender parasites / ml (Figure 2). Whereas no infection was detected in adult flies, the N-acetyl-glucosamine supplementation of the infective meal led to an increase of the infection rates from 2,4% to 13,3% in teneral flies (Figure 2).

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Ngoune et al. present compelling evidence that Slender cells are challenged to infect tsetse flies. They explore the experimental context of a recent important paper in the field, Schuster et al., that presents evidence suggesting the proliferative Slender bloodstream T.brucei can infect juvenile tsetse flies. Schuster et al. was disruptive to the widely accepted paradigm that the Stumpy bloodstream form is solely responsible for tsetse infection and T.brucei transmission potential. Evidence presented here shows that in all cases, Stumpy form parasites are exponentially more capable of infecting tsetse flies. They further show that Slender cells do not infect mature flies.

      However, they raise questions of immature tsetse immunological potential and field transmission potential that their experiments do not address. Specifically, they do not show that teneral tsetse flies are immunocompromised, that tsetse flies must be immunocompromised for Slender infection nor that younger teneral tsetse infection is not pertinent to field transmission.

      All these specific comments were addressed in the revision and illustrated with new data and references.

      - The limited immunocompetence of teneral flies has been extensively studied by the labs of S. Aksoy at Yale and M. Lehane at Liverpool. In the discussion, we provide key references from these two labs 19-22.

      - Differences between the strain clones, the cell culture conditions and/or the fly colony maintenance conditions could explain part of the differences in infection rates observed here as compared to the Schuster et al. study (1). However, the use of the lectin-inhibitory sugar N-acetyl-glucosamine to enhance infection rates in the latter study could be a more likely explanation. To assess this hypothesis, an additional experimental challenge was performed to compare infection rates in teneral versus adult flies, with or without N-acetyl-glucosamine supplement in an infective meal containing 10<sup>5</sup> slender parasites / ml (Figure 2). Whereas no infection was detected in adult flies, the N-acetyl-glucosamine supplementation of the infective meal led to an increase of the infection rates from 2,4% to 13,3% in teneral flies (Figure 2).

      - Our comment on the relevance to field transmission is simply based on field observations of the fly biology. For example, according to the capture-recapture experiments described in HARGROVE JW insect sci applic 1990 (new ref 23), wild female mortality was reported 6.8% shortly after emergence, <1% for ages 20-50 days and rose to 5% by 130 day (a pattern similar to that for laboratory reared tsetse), while wild male daily mortality was 8.3% after emergence, fell to 5.5% by 9 days, then rose continuously to more than 10% by 30 days. This means that adult flies represent the majority of individuals in a wild tsetse population. Hence, knowing that both males and females are strictly hematophagous and that they can live up to nine months, the impact of teneral flies (up to 4 days after emergence) on trypanosome transmission appears limited, if not incidental.

      Strengths:

      Experimental Design is precise and elegant, outcomes are convincing. Discussion is compelling and important to the field. This is a timely piece that adds important data to a critical discussion of host:parasite interactions, of relevance to all parasite transmission.

      Thank you

      Weaknesses:

      As above, the authors dispute the biological relevance of teneral tsetse infection in the wild, without offering evidence to the contrary. Statements need to be softened for claims regarding immunological competence or relevance to field transmission.

      All these specific comments were addressed in the revision and illustrated with new data and references.

      - The limited immunocompetence of teneral flies has been extensively studied by the labs of S. Aksoy at Yale and M. Lehane at Liverpool. In the discussion, we provide key references from these two labs 19-22.

      - Differences between the strain clones, the cell culture conditions and/or the fly colony maintenance conditions could explain part of the differences in infection rates observed here as compared to the Schuster et al. study (1). However, the use of the lectin-inhibitory sugar N-acetyl-glucosamine to enhance infection rates in the latter study could be a more likely explanation. To assess this hypothesis, an additional experimental challenge was performed to compare infection rates in teneral versus adult flies, with or without N-acetyl-glucosamine supplement in an infective meal containing 10<sup>5</sup> slender parasites / ml (Figure 2). Whereas no infection was detected in adult flies, the N-acetyl-glucosamine supplementation of the infective meal led to an increase of the infection rates from 2,4% to 13,3% in teneral flies (Figure 2).

      - Our comment on the relevance to field transmission is simply based on field observations of the fly biology. For example, according to the capture-recapture experiments described in HARGROVE JW insect sci applic 1990 (new ref 23), wild female mortality was reported 6.8% shortly after emergence, <1% for ages 20-50 days and rose to 5% by 130 day (a pattern similar to that for laboratory reared tsetse), while wild male daily mortality was 8.3% after emergence, fell to 5.5% by 9 days, then rose continuously to more than 10% by 30 days. This means that adult flies represent the majority of individuals in a wild tsetse population. Hence, knowing that both males and females are strictly hematophagous and that they can live up to nine months, the impact of teneral flies (up to 4 days after emergence) on trypanosome transmission appears limited, if not incidental.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In contrast to the recent findings reported by Schuster S et al., this brief paper presents evidence suggesting that the stumpy form of T. brucei is likely the most pre-adapted form to progress through the life cycle of this parasite in the tsetse vector.

      Strengths:

      One significant experimental point is that all fly infection experiments are conducted in the absence of "boosting" metabolites like GlcNAc or S-glutathione. As a result, flies infected with slender trypanosomes present very low or nonexistent infection rates. This provides important experimental evidence that the findings of Schuster S and colleagues may need to be revisited.

      Thank you

      Weaknesses:

      However, I believe the authors should have included their own set of experiments demonstrating that the presence of these metabolites in the infectious bloodmeal enhances infection rates in flies receiving blood meals containing slender trypanosomes. Considering the well-known physiological variabilities among flies from different facilities, including infection rates, this would have strengthened the experimental evidence presented by the authors.

      This specific comment was addressed in the revision and illustrated with new data.

      Differences between the strain clones, the cell culture conditions and/or the fly colony maintenance conditions could explain part of the differences in infection rates observed here as compared to the Schuster et al. study (1). However, the use of the lectin-inhibitory sugar N-acetyl-glucosamine to enhance infection rates in the latter study could be a more likely explanation. To assess this hypothesis, an additional experimental challenge was performed to compare infection rates in teneral versus adult flies, with or without N-acetyl-glucosamine supplement in an infective meal containing 10<sup>5</sup> slender parasites / ml (Figure 2). Whereas no infection was detected in adult flies, the N-acetyl-glucosamine supplementation of the infective meal led to an increase of the infection rates from 2,4% to 13,3% in teneral flies (Figure 2).

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      The dogma in the Trypanosome field is that transmission by Tsetse flies is ensured by stumpy forms. This has been recently challenged by the Engstler lab (Schuster et al.), who showed that slender forms can also be transmitted by teneral flies. In this work, the authors aimed to test whether transmission by slender forms is possible and frequent. The authors observed that most stumpy forms infections with teneral and adult flies were successful while only 1 out of 24 slender form infections were successful.

      In this revised version of the manuscript, the authors made some text changes and included statistical testing as a new section of the Materials and Methods. It seems the comparison of midgut infection in adult vs teneral flies was significant in most of the conditions. However, the critical comparison is still missing: within each type of fly (adult or teneral), was the MG infection significantly different between slender and stumpy forms?

      An ANOVA statistical analysis was performed and a dedicated section added to the revised version. MG infection rate comparisons were statistically significant between teneral and adult flies infected with ST in each amount (p<0.02 with 10 parasites; p<0.0001 with 100 and 1,000 parasites) and with 1,000 SL (p<0.0001). MG infection rate comparisons were statistically significant (p<0.0001) between parasite stages (SL and ST) in each amount (10, 100 and 1,000) and for each fly group (teneral and adult), excepted in teneral flies infected with 1,000 parasites (p=0.2356).

      Given no additional experiments were performed, it remains unknown why this work and Schuster et al. reached different conclusions. As a result it remains unclear in which conditions slender forms could be important for transmission. Several variables could explain differences between the two groups: the strain used, the presence or absence of N-acetylglucosamine and/or glutathione, how Tsetse colonies were maintained, thorough molecular and cellular characterisation of slender and stumpy forms (to avoid using intermediate forms as slender forms), comparison to recent field parasite strains.

      This specific comment was addressed in the revision and illustrated with new data.

      Differences between the strain clones, the cell culture conditions and/or the fly colony maintenance conditions could explain part of the differences in infection rates observed here as compared to the Schuster et al. study (1). However, the use of the lectin-inhibitory sugar N-acetyl-glucosamine to enhance infection rates in the latter study could be a more likely explanation. To assess this hypothesis, an additional experimental challenge was performed to compare infection rates in teneral versus adult flies, with or without N-acetyl-glucosamine supplement in an infective meal containing 10<sup>5</sup> slender parasites / ml (Figure 2). Whereas no infection was detected in adult flies, the N-acetyl-glucosamine supplementation of the infective meal led to an increase of the infection rates from 2,4% to 13,3% in teneral flies (Figure 2).

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The manuscript is improved, but the author has not addressed much of the constructive criticism offered that would benefit the manuscript.

      To clarify, evidence from Schuster et al did not demonstrate, rather it suggested. That is a major point of this paper - that the previous evidence presented had caveats. Terms such as demonstrate or prove are inappropriate in most biological contexts, unless evidence is without caveat.

      This specific comment was addressed in the revision and illustrated with new data.

      Differences between the strain clones, the cell culture conditions and/or the fly colony maintenance conditions could explain part of the differences in infection rates observed here as compared to the Schuster et al. study (1). However, the use of the lectin-inhibitory sugar N-acetyl-glucosamine to enhance infection rates in the latter study could be a more likely explanation. To assess this hypothesis, an additional experimental challenge was performed to compare infection rates in teneral versus adult flies, with or without N-acetyl-glucosamine supplement in an infective meal containing 10<sup>5</sup> slender parasites / ml (Figure 2). Whereas no infection was detected in adult flies, the N-acetyl-glucosamine supplementation of the infective meal led to an increase of the infection rates from 2,4% to 13,3% in teneral flies (Figure 2).

      Statements regarding teneral flies in the field are softened. Yet the referenced papers pertain more to commensurate coinfections rather than reduced immunocapacity of immature teneral flies in the field. This should be clarified.

      The limited immunocompetence of teneral flies has been extensively studied by the labs of S. Aksoy at Yale and M. Lehane at Liverpool. In the discussion, we provide key references from these two labs 19-22.

      The text remains convoluted to read with grammatical errors in places. For example, it is incorrect to begin a sentence with However. There are far too many run-on sentences in the manuscript that confuse this straightforward story.

      The revised text was improved as much as possible.

      All text requires grammatical refinement and softer claims unless additional experiments are undertaken.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      I continue to endorse the publication of this manuscript; however, I am somewhat disappointed by the authors' justifications for not conducting additional experiments or exploring other factors that might influence the infection phenotypes in the fly.

      This specific comment was addressed in the revision and illustrated with new data.

      Differences between the strain clones, the cell culture conditions and/or the fly colony maintenance conditions could explain part of the differences in infection rates observed here as compared to the Schuster et al. study (1). However, the use of the lectin-inhibitory sugar N-acetyl-glucosamine to enhance infection rates in the latter study could be a more likely explanation. To assess this hypothesis, an additional experimental challenge was performed to compare infection rates in teneral versus adult flies, with or without N-acetyl-glucosamine supplement in an infective meal containing 10<sup>5</sup> slender parasites / ml (Figure 2). Whereas no infection was detected in adult flies, the N-acetyl-glucosamine supplementation of the infective meal led to an increase of the infection rates from 2,4% to 13,3% in teneral flies (Figure 2).

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors performed an integration of 48 scRNA-seq public datasets and created a single-cell transcriptomic atlas for AML (222 samples comprising 748,679 cells). This is important since most AML scRNA-seq studies suffer from small sample size coupled with high heterogeneity. They used this atlas to further dissect AML with t(8;21) (AML-ETO/RUNX1-RUNX1T1), which is one of the most frequent AML subtypes in young people. In particular, they were able to predict Gene Regulatory Networks in this AML subtype using pySCENIC, which identified the paediatric regulon defined by a distinct group of hematopoietic transcription factors (TFs) and the adult regulon for t(8;21). They further validated this in bulk RNA-seq with AUCell algorithm and inferred prenatal signature to 5 key TFs (KDM5A, REST, BCLAF1, YY1, and RAD21), and the postnatal signature to 9 TFs (ENO1, TFDP1, MYBL2, KLF1, TAGLN2, KLF2, IRF7, SPI1, and YXB1). They also used SCENIC+ to identify enhancer-driven regulons (eRegulons), forming an eGRN, and found that prenatal origin shows a specific HSC eRegulon profile, while a postnatal origin shows a GMP profile. They also did an in silico perturbation and found AP-1 complex (JUN, ATF4, FOSL2), P300, and BCLAF1 as important TFs to induce differentiation. Overall, I found this study very important in creating a comprehensive resource for AML research.

      Strengths:

      (1) The generation of an AML atlas integrating multiple datasets with almost 750K cells will further support the community working on AML.

      (2) Characterisation of t(8;21) AML proposes new interesting leads.

      We thank the reviewer for a succinct summary of our work and highlighting its strengths.

      Weaknesses:

      Were these t(8;21) TFs/regulons identified from any of the single datasets? For example, if the authors apply pySCENIC to any dataset, would they find the same TFs, or is it the increase in the number of cells that allows identification of these?

      The purpose of our study was to gain biological insights by integrating multiple datasets, to overcome limitations from small sample size. We expect that the larger dataset would improve network inference, which is what we implemented in the manuscript, hence we have not looked at individual datasets. However, we will investigate this further in the revised manuscript by running pySCENIC on individual datasets and comparing to the results drawn from the whole atlas.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors assemble 222 publicly available bone marrow single-cell RNA sequencing samples from healthy donors and primary AML, including pediatric, adolescent, and adult patients at diagnosis. Focusing on one specific subtype, t(8;21), which, despite affecting all age classes, is associated with better prognosis and drug response for younger patients, the authors investigate if this difference is reflected also in the transcriptomic signal. Specifically, they hypothesize that the pediatric and part of the young population acquires leukemic mutations in utero, which leads to a different leukemogenic transformation and ultimately to differently regulated leukemic stem cells with respect to the adult counterpart. The analysis in this work heavily relies on regulatory network inference and clustering (via SCENIC tools), which identifies regulatory modules believed to distinguish the pre-, respectively, post-natal leukemic transformation. Bulk RNA-seq and scATAC-seq datasets displaying the same signatures are subsequently used for extending the pool of putative signature-specific TFs and enhancer elements. Through gene set enrichment, ontology, and perturbation simulation, the authors aim to interpret the regulatory signatures and translate them into potential onset-specific therapeutic targets. The putative pre-natal signature is associated with increased chemosensitivity, RNA splicing, histone modification, stem-ness marker SMARCA2, and potentially maintained by EP300 and BCLAF1.

      Strengths:

      The main strength of this work is the compilation of a pediatric AML atlas using the efficient Cellxgene interface. Also, the idea of identifying markers for different disease onsets, interpreting them from a developmental angle, and connecting this to the different therapy and relapse observations, is interesting. The results obtained, the set of putative up-regulated TFs, are biologically coherent with the mechanisms and the conclusions drawn. I also appreciate that the analysis code was made available and is well documented.

      We thank the reviewer for reviewing our work, and highlighting its key features, including creation of AML atlas, downstream analysis and interpretation for t(8;21) subtype.

      We also appreciate useful critique of our paper provided below.

      Weaknesses:

      There were fundamental flaws in how methods and samples were applied, a general lack of critical examination of both the results and the appropriateness of the methods for the data at hand, and in how results were presented. In particular:

      (1) Cell type annotation:

      a) The 2-phase cell type annotation process employed for the scRNA-seq sample collection raised concerns. Initially annotated cells are re-labeled after a second round with the same cell types from the initial label pool (Figure 1E). The automatic annotation tools were used without specifying the database and tissue atlases used as a reference, and no information was shown regarding the consensus across these tools.

      We believe that most of the reviewer’s criticisms stem from a misunderstanding, and we apologize for not explaining certain aspects of our work more clearly.

      The two types of cell type annotation applied were different and served distinct purposes:

      • One was using general bone marrow/blood reference datasets to annotate blood subtype lineage clusters.

      • The other was using a CD34 purified AML specific reference dataset which included leukaemia-associated annotations, to identify HSPC subpopulations. We also implemented this on a single-cell level to allow more robust identification of these rare populations in a large dataset.

      This is probably not well explained in the methods and figure presentation. We will clearly indicate in the revised manuscript that different HSPC annotations represent separate analysis and will update the figures to highlight this. We will provide a comprehensive review of the annotation strategies implemented, including the automated tool outputs, which may be useful for the single-cell community.

      b) Expression of the CD34 marker is only reported as a selection method for HSPCs, which is not in line with common practice. The use of only is admitted as a surface marker, while robust annotation of HSPCs should be done on the basis of expression of gene sets.

      We used CD34 expression in conjunction with other cell type annotations and marker sets to identify LSCs, although results are same when we use HSPC annotated cells without condition on CD34 expression.  In the revised manuscript, we will simplify this analysis to use HSPC clusters as suggested by the reviewer.

      c) During several analyses, the cell types used were either not well defined or contradictory, such as in Figure 2D, where it is not clear if pySCENIC and AUC scores were computed on HSPCs alone or merged with CMPs. In other cases, different cell type populations are compared and used interchangeably: comparing the HSPC-derived regulons with bulk (probably not enriched for CD34+ cells) RNA samples could be an issue if there are no valid assumptions on the cell composition of the bulk sample.

      As mentioned in the Methods, we only excluded lymphoid cell types from the pySCENIC analysis to overcome the bias that some samples were enriched using CD34 selection when preparing them for scRNA-seq. We will make this clearer in the text and figures of the revised manuscript. It is difficult to overcome this bias when using bulk RNA samples, which may explain why some of our samples do not fit into our defined signature groups. However, as we do not have access to primary samples ourselves, we cannot provide a better matched experimental cohort for validation.

      (2) Method selection:

      a) The authors should explain why they use pySCENIC and not any other approach. They should briefly explain how pySCENIC works and what they get out in the main text. In addition they should explain the AUCell algorithm and motivate its usage.

      pySCENIC is state-of-the-art method for network inference from scRNA data and is widely used within the single-cell community (over 5000 citations for both versions of the SCENIC pipeline). The pipeline has been benchmarked as one of the top performers for GRN analysis (Nguyen et al, 2021. Briefings in Bioinformatics). AUCELL is a module within the pySCENIC pipeline to summarise the activity of a set of genes (a regulon) into a single number which helps compare and visualise different regulons. We agree with reviewer that this could have been more clearly explained within the manuscript. We will update text in the revised manuscript to add more explanation.

      b) The obtained GRN signatures were not critically challenged on an external dataset. Therefore, the evidence that supports these signatures to be reliable and significant to the investigated setting is weak.

      These signatures were inferred from the best suitable AML single-cell RNA datasets available to date, and we used two independent datasets to validate our findings (the TARGET AML bulk RNA sequencing cohort, and the Lambo et al. scRNA-seq dataset). To our knowledge, there are no other better suited datasets for validation. Experimental validations on patient samples are beyond the scope of this study.

      (3) There are some issues with the analysis & visualization of the data.

      We will provide new statistical tests to improve robustness of the analysis as well as presentation and visualization of the data in the revised manuscript.

      (4) Discussion:

      a) What exactly is the 'regulon signature' that the authors infer? How can it be useful for insights into disease mechanisms?

      The ’regulon signature’ here refers to a gene regulatory program (multiple gene modules, each defined by a transcription factor and its targets) which are specific to different age groups. Further investigation into this can be useful for understanding why patients of different ages confer a different clinical course. We will add more text on the utility of our discovered 'regulon signature' in the discussion section of revised manuscript.

      b) The authors write 'Together this indicates that EP300 inhibition may be particularly effective in t(8;21) AML, and that BCLAF1 may present a new therapeutic target for t(8;21) AML, particularly in children with inferred pre-natal origin of the driver translocation.' I am missing a critical discussion of what is needed to further test the two targets. Put differently: Would the authors take the risk of a clinical study given the evidence from their analysis?

      Of course, many extensive studies would be required before these findings are clinically translatable. We can include some perspectives on what further work is required in terms of further experimental validation and potential subsequent clinical study.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Thank you for your valuable comments, which helped us improve our manuscript. We will make the following modifications in the revised manuscript:

      (1) In the first paragraph of the Result section, we will provide a summary of trimeric G proteins in Ciona and explain how we focused on Gαs and Gαq in the initial phase of this study.

      We added a summary of trimeric G proteins in Ciona in the initial part of the Results section (page 6, line 23 to page 8, line 5). In this summary, we added the following sentence explaining the reason we focused on Gas and Gaq in the initial phase of this study: "Among them, we prioritized examining the Gα proteins having an excitatory function (Gαq and Gαs) rather than inhibitory roles since previous studies suggested that excitatory events like Ca<sup>2+</sup> transient and neuropeptide secretion occur when Ciona metamorphose."

      (2) As the reviewer 1 suggests, the polymodal roles of papilla neurons are interesting. Although we could not address this through functional analyses in this study, we will add a discussion regarding this aspect. The sentences will be something like the following:

      “The recent study (Hoyer et al., 2024) provided several lines of evidence suggesting that PSNs can serve as the sensors of several chemicals in addition to the mechanical stimuli. This finding and our model could be mutually related because these chemicals could modify Ca<sup>2+</sup> and cAMP production. The use of G protein signaling allows Ciona to reflect various environmental stimuli to initiate metamorphosis in the appropriate situation, both mechanically and chemically.”

      We added a discussion related to the recent publication by Hoyer and colleagues on page 23, lines 13-18: " A recent study[19] provided several lines of evidence suggesting that PNs can serve as the sensors of several chemicals in addition to mechanical stimuli. This finding and our model could be mutually related because these chemicals could modify Ca<sup>2+</sup> and cAMP production. G protein signaling allows Ciona to reflect various environmental stimuli to initiate metamorphosis either mechanically or chemically according to the situation."

      (3) As both reviewers suggested, imaging cAMP on the backgrounds of some G protein knockdowns is essential, and we will conduct the experiments.

      We added the data on cAMP imaging in Gas, Gaq, and dvGai_Chr2 knockdown larvae in Supplementary Figure S4C-D and Figure 6E.

      (4) We carefully modify the text throughout the manuscript so that the descriptions suitably reflect the results.

      We modified the descriptions of experimental results so that the text reflects the results more precisely.

      Reviewer #1:

      Pg1 - need to add an additional '6' to the author list to clarify which two or more authors contributed equally.

      We added a 6 as suggested. Thank you for pointing this out.

      Pg3 - note that larval adhesive organ applies to not all benthic adults, but to benthic sessile adults this makes it sound like the adhesive organ can trigger metamorphosis but has that been shown? In Ciona or others? Need to specify the role of cells secreting adhesive, vs sensory cells that trigger metamorphosis?

      We divided the corresponding sentence into two to clearly state that adhesion and triggering metamorphosis are related but could be different events. Moreover, we modified the sentence to state that physical contact is one example of a cue triggering metamorphosis. We then added another example of a factor triggering metamorphosis—i.e., chemicals from the organisms surrounding the adherence site (page 3, lines 16-20 of the revised version):

      "Many marine invertebrates exhibit a benthic lifestyle at the adult stage[4]. Their planktonic larvae have an adhesive organ that secretes adhesives and adheres to a substratum. The cues associated with the adhesion, such as the physical contact with the substratum and a chemical from organisms surrounding the adherence site, can trigger their metamorphosis."

      Pg 4 - although mechanosensation is the focus here, could there also be chemoreception/chemoreceptors involved in Ciona metamorphosis? For example, Hoyer et al. 2024 (Current Biology 34(6):1168-1182) concluded that some palp sensory neurons were multimodal and could be both chemo- and mechano-sensory.

      We added statements about this recent finding in the Introduction and Discussion sections. In the Introduction (page 4, lines 16-18), however, we also stated that a mechanical stimulus can trigger metamorphosis in the lab without the need to supply these chemicals. This is to emphasize that the mechanical stimulus is the focus of this study. In the Discussion, we added a statement that G-protein signaling could also be used to receive the chemical stimuli (page 23, lines 13-18).

      Pg 6 - Before starting functional characterizations, it would be useful to give an overview (table?) of the G proteins found in papillae, and what receptor they are suspected of binding to, or if this is completely unknown, and which downstream pathways they likely activate. That is, to show some results about which G proteins are found in Ciona, and which are found in papillae. In this way, it will make more sense for readers when the Gai is suddenly introduced later, following the sections of Gaq and Gas.

      Thank you for your idea to improve the readability of this manuscript. In the initial part of the Results section (page 6, line 22 to page 8, line 5), we added descriptions of the repertoire of trimeric G-proteins in Ciona, including phylogenetic analyses, and expression in the papillae based on RNA-seq data, followed by the reason why we initially focused on Gaq and Gas. The data are displayed in Supplementary Figure S1. The phylogenetic analyses were modified from those shown in Supplementary Figure S5 of the previous version. We also added the general downstream activities of Gas, Gai and Gaq in the Introduction section (page 6, lines 10-12). Considering the contents, the general function of Ga12/13 was stated in the Results section (page 8, lines 2-3).

      We did not add the information about their partner receptors in this early section. This is because there are many candidates, and we could not pick some of them. Instead, we described our current suppositions about their possible partners in the Discussion (page 23, line 22 to page 24, line 19). However, we suspect that there are more candidates, and we wish to promote unbiased research in the future.

      Pg 9 - would be good to know the timing of this PF fluorescence increase and the timing of stimulation in the text here, relevant to the 30-min gap before metamorphosis initiation

      We added the start times for the cAMP reduction and re-upregulation in the following sentence (page 11, lines 17-18): "The cAMP reduction and increase respectively started at 35 seconds and 4 min 40 seconds after stimulation on average."

      Pg 28 - Phylogenetic analysis: Given that the results may be of interest to metamorphosis in other marine invertebrates as discussed in the last paragraph of the paper, it would be useful to include G proteins from these other animal phyla where available in the phylogenetic tree. Similarly, in Figure S5A it would be useful to highlight further all the different Ciona G proteins, and the different protein families, through the use of additional colour/labelling (regardless of whether this remains Fig S5A, or becomes part of the main figures)

      We drew a phylogenetic tree of G-proteins including those in some sessile and benthic animals (barnacle, sea anemone, hydra, sponge, sea urchin and shell). However, we decided not to add the tree in the revised version because, unfortunately, the bootstrap values of many branches were not high enough to have confidence in the results. We hope you understand our decision. Ciona divergent G-proteins are likely to be specific to Ciona.

      According to your comment, we highlighted all Ciona G alpha proteins in red in Figure S5A, which is now Figure S1A in the revised version.

      Figure 3E and Figure S3 - is the data shown as an average of all larvae measured (n=5 and n=4) or is it data from one representative larva out of the 4-5 measured? This needs clarification.

      The original graphs in Figure 3E and Figure S3 are typical examples. We added the graphs summarizing data of all larvae in each experimental condition in Supplementary Figure S4 (corresponding to Supplementary Figure S3 of the original version). Figure 3E remains as a typical example of the result of a single larva to explain our data analysis in detail.

      Experimental suggestion - As mentioned above, one missing detail seems to be the need for evidence that cAMP is elevated in the papillae directly as a result of Gs activation- this could be shown with measurement of cAMP via PF in Gs knockdown larvae that are mechanically stimulated compared to wildtype stimulated and non-stimulated?

      Thank you for your suggestion. The experiments are indeed important. We added the data of Pink Flamindo imaging in the Gas, Gaq and dvGai_Chr2 knockdown conditions. The results of Gas and Gaq knockdowns are described in page 11, line 24 to page 12, line 5, and are displayed in Supplementary Figure S4C-D. The result of dvGai_Chr2 knockdown is given on page 16, lines 20-22 and shown in Figure 6E.

      In order to insert the data of cAMP imaging of dvGai_Chr2 knockdown larvae, we transferred some panels of Figure 6 to Supplementary Figure S6. In addition, the knockdown data of dvGαi_Chr4 and double knockdowns of Gai genes are also included in Supplementary Figure S6.

      Reviewer #2:

      Page 6, line 3-4 in the first paragraph of the "Results"; the authors state "Neither morphant showed any signature of metamorphosis even though both were allowed to adhere to the base of culture dishes...". However, judging from Fig. 1E, "the percentage of metamorphosis initiation" (indicated by the initiation of tail regression) in Gαq morphans is not close to 0 (average about 40%), thus I am not convinced this observation can be described as "Neither morphant showed any signature of metamorphosis..." in this sentence.

      Thank you for your suggestion. In writing the original text, we oversimplified some of the descriptions when trying to improve the readability. We agree this resulted in imprecision in places. We have revised all these passages in our revision. In this particular case, we softened the overly emphatic statement to better reflect the results, changing “... any signature of metamorphosis...” to “... reduced rate of metamorphosis initiation...” In addition, we stated that the effect of G_α_q MO was weaker than that of G_α_s MO on page 8, lines 10-12. The weaker effect of Gaq MO was due to the redundant role of the Gi pathway, which is shown on page 17, lines 10-17, and in Figure 6G-H.

      Similarly, in the next paragraph describing the knockdown of PLCβ1/2/3, PLCβ4, and IP3R genes, the authors appear to neglect there is a weaker effect of the PLCβ4 MO, and simply described the results as "The knockdown larvae of these three genes failed to start metamorphosis". Based on Fig. 1H, about 30% of the PLCβ4 MO-injected animals still initiated tail regeneration. This difference may have some biological meanings and thus should be described more precisely.

      We added the following sentence on page 8, lines 18-19 of the revised version: “The effect of PLCβ4 MO was weaker than those of the other MOs, suggesting that this PLC plays an auxiliary role.”

      Page 7, second paragraph, on the description of GCaMP8 fluorescence and also at the end of Fig. 1O legend, the citation to "Figure S1" is confusing; Fig. S1 is the phylogenetic tree of PLCβ proteins. Is there additional data regarding this Gαq MO plus GCaMP8 mRNA injection experiment?

      Figure S1 of the original version corresponds to Figure S2 of the revised version. To avoid confusion, we deleted this citation from the legend of Figure 1O. By this modification, the sentence stating the repertoire of PLCb and IP3R in Ciona (page 8, lines 15-16) is the only sentence citing Figure S2 in the revised version.

      Page 8, first sentence; The purpose of theophylline treatment is not to prevent larvae from adhesion, thus I would suggest modifying this sentence to: "We treated wild-type larvae with theophylline after tail amputation, and we observed that most theophylline-treated larvae completed tail regression without adhesion (Figure 2D-F)".

      We modified the sentence according to your comment. Thank you for your suggestion.

      Page 9, second paragraph; judging from the data presented in Fig. 3C, I think this description: "when papillae were removed from larvae, theophylline failed to induce metamorphosis" is not accurate, because about ~30% of the Papilla cut +Theophylline-treated larvae still initiated their tail regression. This needs to be explained clearly.

      We modified the sentence (page 11, lines 2-3) as follows: “...the average rate of metamorphosis induction by theophylline was reduced from 100% to 30%...”

      Similarly in the next few sentences regarding the results presented in Fig, 3D, the effects of overexpressing those genes are not uniform. While amputation of papillae in larvae overexpressing caPLCβ1/2/3 could inhibit metamorphosis almost completely, papilla cut seems to have a weaker effect on caGαq, caGαs, and bPAC-overexpressing larvae.

      We added a description explaining that caPLCβ1/2/3 was the most sensitive to papilla amputation, and the possibility that PLCβ1/2/3 works specifically in the papillae (page 11, lines 9-11): “Among these experiments, caPLCβ1/2/3 overexpression was the most sensitive to papilla amputation, suggesting that PLCβ1/2/3 acts specifically in the papillae during metamorphosis.”

      Page 9, the paragraph on using the fluorescent cAMP indicator; there is a discrepancy between the described developmental time when the authors conducted this experiment and the metamorphosis competent timing (after 24hpf) described on page 7. On page 26, the authors describe "The Pink Flamindo mRNA-injected larvae were immobilized on Poly L lysine-coated glass bottom dishes at 20-21 hpf...". Did the authors start stimulating the larvae to observe the fluorescent signal soon after immobilization, or wait several hours until the larvae passed 24hpf and then conduct the experiment?

      The latter is the case. The immobilized larvae were kept until they acquired the competence for metamorphosis and then stimulation/recording was carried out. This point is described in the Materials and Methods section of the revised version (page 29, lines 16-18):

      "The Pink Flamindo mRNA-injected larvae were immobilized on Poly L lysine-coated glass-bottom dishes at 20-21 hpf, and stimulated their adhesive papillae around 25 hpf."

      Page 10, the description "...Gαq morphants initiated metamorphosis when caGαs was overexpressed in the nervous system (Figure 4F)". It should be noted that the result is only a partial rescue. To be precise, this description needs to be modified.

      We changed the sentence to reflect the results more precisely (page 14, lines 2-3): “Moreover, caGαs overexpression in the nervous system significantly, although not perfectly, ameliorated the effect of Gαq MO (Figure 4F).”

      Page 12-13, This description and the figure 5E presented is a bit confusing to me. The figure legend for 5E: "GABA is necessary for Ca2+ transient in the adhesive papillae (arrow)" But the arrow in this image points to a place with no fluorescent signal, and on the upper corner it labeled as "29% (n=17)". Does that mean the proportion of "no Ca2+ increase after stimulation" was 29% among the 17 samples examined? Or actually, is the other way around that 81% of the examined larvae did not show Ca2+ signal increase after stimulation?

      The latter is the case. We added a caption explaining this clearly in the Figure legend: “The percentage and number exhibit the rate of animals showing Ca<sup>2+</sup> transient in the papillae.”

      Page 13, second paragraph; I do not agree with the overly simplified description that "GABA significantly ameliorated the metamorphosis-failed phenocopies of Gαq, PLCβ, and Gαs morphants". As shown in Fig. 5F-H, adding GABA exerts different levels of partial rescue effect on each morphant, and thus should be described clearly.

      When the outliers are neglected, the effect of GABA is most evident in Gαs knockdowns. This suggests that the target(s) of GABA signaling is more likely to be Gq pathway components. We added the following sentence to the revised version (page 15, lines 14-16):

      “Among the three morphants, GABA exhibited the most effective rescues in Gαs knockdowns than Gαq and PLCβ.”

      In addition, we think this sentence establishes a more logical connection with the sentence that follows it: “These results could be explained by assuming enhancement of the Gq pathway by GABA through PLCβ and another GABA-mediated metamorphic pathway bypassing Gq components.” Thank you for your suggestion.

      The section "Contribution of Gi to metamorphosis" confirmed the possibility that GABA signaling targets Gq pathway components.

      Page 13, the first paragraph on "Contribution of Gi to metamorphosis"; the description that "The knockdown of this gene (Gαi) exhibited a significantly reduced rate of metamorphosis;..." is misleading. I would suggest modifying the entire sentence as "The knockdown of this gene (Gαi) exhibited a moderate (although statistically significant) reduction of metamorphosis rate, suggesting the presence of another Gαi regulating metamorphosis".

      Thank you for your suggestion. We modified the sentence (page 16, lines 2-4 in the revised version) as recommended. We believe the description is much improved.

      Page 20, the last sentence about Ciona papilla neurons expressing transcription factor Islet; the authors seem to attempt to make some comparison with the vertebrate pancreatic beta cells in this paragraph, but the comparison and the argument are not fully developed in this current format.

      To deepen this discussion, we added the following sentence (page 23, lines 10-12): “The atypical secretion of GABA might depend on the transcription factor like Islet shared between Ciona papilla neurons and vertebrate beta cells.”

      However, we would like to limit the depth of our discussion on this point, as we hope to expand on it further in future studies.

      Other suggestions:

      Page 3, second paragraph: as they become unable to "move" after metamorphosis -> "relocate"

      We corrected the word as suggested.

      Page 4, second paragraph: In the first sentence, the author states the current understanding of chordate phylogeny and cites Delsuc et al. 2006 Nature paper at the end of this sentence. However, in this paper cephalochordates were erroneously grouped with echinoderms, and thus chordates did not form a monophyletic clade. A later paper by Bourlat et al, (Nature 444:85-88, 2006) corrected this problem, and subsequently Dulsuc et al. also published another paper (genesis, 46:592-604, 2008) with broader sampling to overcome this problem. These later publications need to be included for the sake of correctness.

      We added this reference.

      Page 14, regarding the redundant function of the typical Gαi protein in the papillae; the authors may try double KD of Gαi and dvGαi_Chr2 in their experimental system to test this idea.

      We carried out double knockdown of typical Gai and dvGαi_Chr2. However, we could not address their redundant role sufficiently because most of the double knockdown larvae exhibited severe shape malformation.

      dvGαi_Chr4 is also expressed in the papillae. We carried out knockdown of this gene, to find that the knockdown resulted in very minor but statistically significant reduction of the metamorphosis rate, suggesting that this Gai also plays a supportive role in metamorphosis. We also carried out double knockdown of dvGαi_Chr2 and dvGαi_Chr4. The double KD larvae exhibited responsiveness to GABA, probably because of the presence of typical Gai.

      These results are described on page 16, lines 2-18, and the data are shown in Supplementary Figure S6A-D of the revised version.

      Responses to the Reviewing editor's comments:

      "Larvae of the ascidian Ciona initiate metamorphosis tens of minutes after adhesion to a substratum via its adhesive organ." - Larvae is plural so change to 'via their adhesive organ'

      The sentence was corrected as suggested.

      "Metamorphosis is a widespread feature of animal development that allows them" - revise the sentence, e.g. "Metamorphosis is a widespread feature of development that allows animals"

      The sentence was corrected as suggested.

      "GABA synthase (GAD)" GAD is not called GABA synthase but glutamate decarboxylase - clarify, e.g. encoding the enzyme synthesizing GABA called glutamate decarboxylase (GAD)

      This part was corrected exactly as suggested. Thank you.

      "IP3 is received by its receptor on the endoplasmic reticulum (ER) and releases calcium ion (Ca2+ )" revise to "IP3 is received by its receptor on the endoplasmic reticulum (ER) that releases calcium ion (Ca2+ )"

      The sentence was corrected as suggested.

      "Moreover, GPCR is implicated as the mediator of settlement" - GPCRs are implicated

      This sentence was modified as suggested.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1(Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript details the results of a small pilot study of neoadjuvant radiotherapy followed by combination treatment with hormone therapy and dalpiciclib for early-stage HR+/HER2-negative breast cancer.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of the manuscript include the scientific rationale behind the approach and the inclusion of some simple translational studies.

      Weaknesses:

      The main weakness of the manuscript is that overly strong conclusions are made by the authors based on a very small study of twelve patients. A study this small is not powered to fully characterize the efficacy or safety of a treatment approach, and can, at best, demonstrate feasibility. These data need validation in a larger cohort before they can have any implications for clinical practice, and the treatment approach outlined should not yet be considered a true alternative to standard evidence-based approaches.

      I would urge the authors and readers to exercise caution when comparing results of this 12-patient pilot study to historical studies, many of which were much larger, and had different treatment protocols and baseline patient characteristics. Cross-trial comparisons like this are prone to mislead, even when comparing well powered studies. With such a small sample size, the risk of statistical error is very high, and comparisons like this have little meaning.

      We greatly appreciate your evaluation of our study and fully agree with the limitations you have pointed out. We have clearly stated the limitations of the small sample size and emphasized the need for a larger population to validate our preliminary findings in the discussion section (Lines 311-316).

      We acknowledge that this small sample size is not powered to characterize this regimen as a promising alternative regimen in the treatment of patients with HR-positive, HER2-negative breast cancer. Therefore, we have revised the description of this regimen to serve as a feasible option for neoadjuvant therapy in HR-positive, HER2-negative breast cancers both in the discussion (Lines 317-320) and the abstract (Lines 71-72).

      We agree with you that cross-trial comparisons should be approached with caution due to differences in study designs and patient populations. In our discussion section, we acknowledge that small sample size limited the comparison of our data with historical data in the literature due to the potential bias (Lines 312-313). We clearly state that such comparisons hold limited significance (Lines 313-314) and suggest a larger population to validate our preliminary findings.

      • Why was dalpiciclib chosen, as opposed to another CDK4/6 inhibitor?

      Thank you for your comments. The rationale for selecting dalpiciclib over other CDK4/6 inhibitors in our study is primarily based on the following considerations:

      (1) Clinical Efficacy: In several clinical trials, including DAWNA-1 and DAWNA-2, the combination of dalpiciclib with endocrine therapies such as fulvestrant, letrozole, or anastrozole has been shown to significantly extend the progression-free survival (PFS) in patients with hormone receptor-positive, HER2-negative advanced breast cancer [1-2].

      (2) Tolerability and Management of Adverse Reactions: The primary adverse reactions associated with dalpiciclib are neutropenia, leukopenia, and anemia. Despite these potential side effects, the majority of patients are able to tolerate them, and with proper monitoring and management, these reactions can be effectively mitigated [1-2].

      (3) Comparable pharmacodynamic with other CDK4/6 inhibitors: The combination of CDK4/6 inhibitors, including palbociclib, ribociclib, and abemaciclib, with aromatase inhibitors has demonstrated an enhanced ability to suppress tumor proliferation and increase the rate of clinical response in neoadjuvant therapy for HR-positive, HER2-negative breast cancer [3-5]. Furthermore, preclinical studies have shown that dalpiciclib has comparable in vivo and in vitro pharmacodynamic activity to palbociclib, suggesting its potential effectiveness in similar treatment regimens [6].

      (4) Accessibility and Regulatory Approval: Dalpiciclib has gained marketing approval in China on December 31, 2021, which facilitates the accessibility of this medication, making it a more convenient option when considering treatment plans.

      References:

      (1) Zhang P, Zhang Q, Tong Z, et al. Dalpiciclib plus letrozole or anastrozole versus placebo plus letrozole or anastrozole as first-line treatment in patients with hormone receptor-positive, HER2-negative advanced breast cancer (DAWNA-2): a multicentre, randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled, phase 3 trial[J]. The Lancet Oncology, 2023, 24(6): 646-657.

      (2) Xu B, Zhang Q, Zhang P, et al. Dalpiciclib or placebo plus fulvestrant in hormone receptor-positive and HER2-negative advanced breast cancer: a randomized, phase 3 trial[J]. Nature medicine, 2021, 27(11): 1904-1909.

      (3) Hurvitz S A, Martin M, Press M F, et al. Potent cell-cycle inhibition and upregulation of immune response with abemaciclib and anastrozole in neoMONARCH, phase II neoadjuvant study in HR+/HER2− breast cancer[J]. Clinical Cancer Research, 2020, 26(3): 566-580.

      (4) Prat A, Saura C, Pascual T, et al. Ribociclib plus letrozole versus chemotherapy for postmenopausal women with hormone receptor-positive, HER2-negative, luminal B breast cancer (CORALLEEN): an open-label, multicentre, randomised, phase 2 trial[J]. The lancet oncology, 2020, 21(1): 33-43.

      (5) Ma C X, Gao F, Luo J, et al. NeoPalAna: neoadjuvant palbociclib, a cyclin-dependent kinase 4/6 inhibitor, and anastrozole for clinical stage 2 or 3 estrogen receptor–positive breast cancer[J]. Clinical Cancer Research, 2017, 23(15): 4055-4065.

      (6) Long F, He Y, Fu H, et al. Preclinical characterization of SHR6390, a novel CDK 4/6 inhibitor, in vitro and in human tumor xenograft models[J]. Cancer science, 2019, 110(4): 1420-1430.

      • The eligibility criteria are not consistent throughout the manuscript, sometimes saying early breast cancer, other times saying stage II/III by MRI criteria.

      Thank you for pointing out the inconsistencies in the description of the eligibility criteria in our manuscript. We deeply apologize for any confusion caused by these inconsistencies. We have revised the term from “early-stage HR-positive, HER2-negative breast cancer” to “early or locally advanced HR-positive, HER2-negative breast cancer” (Lines 128 and 150). The term “early or locally advanced” encompasses two different stages of breast cancer, whereas “Stage II/III by MRI criteria” refers to specific stages within the TNM staging system.

      • The authors should emphasize the 25% rate of conversion from mastectomy to breast conservation and also report the type and nature of axillary lymph node surgery performed. As the authors note in the discussion section, rates of pathologic complete response/RCB scores are less prognostic for hormone-receptor-positive breast cancer than other subtypes, so one of the main rationales for neoadjuvant medical therapy is for surgical downstaging. This is a clinically relevant outcome.

      We appreciate your constructive comments. Based on your suggestions, we have made the following revisions and additions to the article.

      The breast conservation rate serves as a secondary endpoint in our study (Line 62 and 179). We have highlighted the significant 25% conversion rate from mastectomy to breast conservation in both the results (Lines 229-230) and discussion sections (Lines 290-292).

      In our study, all patients underwent lymph node surgery, including sentinel lymph node biopsy or axillary lymph node dissection. Among them, 58.3% of patients (7/12) underwent sentinel lymph node biopsies.

      We agree with your point that the prognostic value of pathologic complete response/RCB score is lower for hormone receptor-positive breast cancer compared to other subtypes, we have revised the discussion section to clarify that one of the principal objectives for neoadjuvant therapy in this patient population is to facilitate downstaging and enhance the rate of breast conservation (Lines 289-290). And also emphasized that this neoadjuvant therapeutic regiment appeared to improve the likelihood of pathological downstaging and achieve a margin-free resection, particularly for those with locally advanced and high-risk breast cancer (Lines 293-295).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Firstly, as this is a single-arm preliminary study, we are curious about the order of radiotherapy and the endocrine therapy. Besides, considering the radiotherapy, we also concern about the recovery of the wound after the surgery and whether related data were collected.

      Thanks for the comments. The treatment sequence in this study is to first administer radiotherapy, followed by endocrine therapy. A meta-analysis has indicated that concurrent radiotherapy with endocrine therapy does not significantly impact the incidence of radiation-induced toxicity or survival rates compared to a sequential approach [1]. In light of preclinical research suggesting enhanced therapeutic efficacy when radiotherapy is delivered prior to CDK4/6 inhibitors, we have opted to administer radiotherapy before the combination therapy of CDK4/6 inhibitors and hormone therapy [2].

      In our study, we collected data on surgical wound recovery. All 12 patients had Class I incisions, which healed by primary intention. The wounds exhibited no signs of redness, swelling, exudate, or fat necrosis.

      References:

      (1) Li Y F, Chang L, Li W H, et al. Radiotherapy concurrent versus sequential with endocrine therapy in breast cancer: A meta-analysis[J]. The Breast, 2016, 27: 93-98.

      (2) Petroni G, Buqué A, Yamazaki T, et al. Radiotherapy delivered before CDK4/6 inhibitors mediates superior therapeutic effects in ER+ breast cancer[J]. Clinical Cancer Research, 2021, 27(7): 1855-1863.

      Secondly, in the methodology, please describe the sample size estimation of this study and follow up details.

      Thanks for pointing out this crucial omission. Sample size estimation for this study and follow-up details have been added in the methodology section. The section on sample size estimation has been revised to state in Statistical analysis: “This exploratory study involves 12 patients, with the sample size determined based on clinical considerations, not statistical factors (Lines 210-211).” The section on follow up has been revised to state in Procedures section “A 5-year follow-up is conducted every 3 months during the first 2 years, and every 6 months for the subsequent 3 years. Additionally, safety data are collected within 90 days after surgery for subjects who discontinue study treatment (Lines 169-172).”

      Thirdly, in Table 1, the item HER2 expression, it's better to categorise HER2 into 0, 1+, 2+ and FISH-.

      Thank you very much for pointing out this issue. The item HER2 expression in Table 1 has been revised from “negative, 1+, 2+ and FISH-” to “0, 1+, 2+ and FISH-”.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In their paper, Zhan et al. have used Pf genetic data from simulated data and Ghanaian field samples to elucidate a relationship between multiplicity of infection (MOI) (the number of distinct parasite clones in a single host infection) and force of infection (FOI). Specifically, they use sequencing data from the var genes of Pf along with Bayesian modeling to estimate MOI individual infections and use these values along with methods from queueing theory that rely on various assumptions to estimate FOI. They compare these estimates to known FOIs in a simulated scenario and describe the relationship between these estimated FOI values and another commonly used metric of transmission EIR (entomological inoculation rate).

      This approach does fill an important gap in malaria epidemiology, namely estimating the force of infection, which is currently complicated by several factors including superinfection, unknown duration of infection, and highly genetically diverse parasite populations. The authors use a new approach borrowing from other fields of statistics and modeling and make extensive efforts to evaluate their approach under a range of realistic sampling scenarios. However, the write-up would greatly benefit from added clarity both in the description of methods and in the presentation of the results. Without these clarifications, rigorously evaluating whether the author's proposed method of estimating FOI is sound remains difficult. Additionally, there are several limitations that call into question the stated generalizability of this method that should at minimum be further discussed by authors and in some cases require a more thorough evaluation.

      Major comments:

      (1) Description and evaluation of FOI estimation procedure.

      a. The methods section describing the two-moment approximation and accompanying appendix is lacking several important details. Equations on lines 891 and 892 are only a small part of the equations in Choi et al. and do not adequately describe the procedure notably several quantities in those equations are never defined some of them are important to understand the method (e.g. A, S as the main random variables for inter-arrival times and service times, aR and bR which are the known time average quantities, and these also rely on the squared coefficient of variation of the random variable which is also never introduced in the paper). Without going back to the Choi paper to understand these quantities, and to understand the assumptions of this method it was not possible to follow how this works in the paper. At a minimum, all variables used in the equations should be clearly defined.

      We thank the reviewer for this useful comment. We have clarified the method and defined all relevant variables in the revised manuscript (Line 537-573). The reviewer correctly pointed out additional sections and equations in Choi et al., including the derivation of an exact expression for the steady-state queue-length distribution and the two-moment approximation. Since our work directly utilized the two-moment approximation, our previous manuscript included only material on that section. However, we agree that providing additional details on the derivation of the exact expression would benefit readers. Therefore, we have summarized this derivation in the revised manuscript (Line 561-564). Additionally, we clarified the method’s assumptions, particularly those involved in transitioning from the exact expression to the two-moment approximation (Line 565-570).

      b. Additionally, the description in the main text of how the queueing procedure can be used to describe malaria infections would benefit from a diagram currently as written it's very difficult to follow.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we included a diagram illustrating the connection between the queueing procedure and malaria transmission (Appendix 1-Figure 8).

      c. Just observing the box plots of mean and 95% CI on a plot with the FOI estimate (Figures 1, 2, and 10-14) is not sufficient to adequately assess the performance of this estimator. First, it is not clear whether the authors are displaying the bootstrapped 95%CIs or whether they are just showing the distribution of the mean FOI taken over multiple simulations, and then it seems that they are also estimating mean FOI per host on an annual basis. Showing a distribution of those per-host estimates would also be helpful. Second, a more quantitative assessment of the ability of the estimator to recover the truth across simulations (e.g. proportion of simulations where the truth is captured in the 95% CI or something like this) is important in many cases it seems that the estimator is always underestimating the true FOI and may not even contain the true value in the FOI distribution (e.g. Figure 10, Figure 1 under the mid-IRS panel). But it's not possible to conclude one way or the other based on this visualization. This is a major issue since it calls into question whether there is in fact data to support that these methods give good and consistent FOI estimates.

      There seems to be some confusion on what we display in some key figures. Figures 1-2 and 10-14 (labeled as Figure 1-2 and Appendix 1-Figure 11-15 in the revised manuscript) display bootstrapped distributions including the 95% CIs, not the distribution of the mean FOI taken over multiple simulations. To estimate the mean FOI per host on an annual basis, the two proposed methods require either the steady-state queue length distribution (MOI distribution) or the moments of this distribution. Obtaining such a steady-state queue length distribution necessitates either densely tracked time-series observations per host or many realizations at the same sampling time per host. However, under the sparse sampling schemes, we only have two one-time-point observations per host: one at the end of wet/high-transmission and another at the end of dry/low-transmission. This is typically the case for empirical data, although numerical simulations could circumvent this limitation and generate such output. Nonetheless, we have a population-level queue length distribution from both simulation outputs and empirical data by aggregating MOI estimates across all sampled individuals. We use this population-level distribution to represent and approximate the steady-state queue length distribution at the individual level, not explicitly considering any individual heterogeneity due to transmission. The estimated FOI is per host in the sense of representing the FOI experienced by an individual host whose queue length distribution is approximated from the collection of all sampled individuals. The true FOI per host per year in the simulation is the total FOI of all hosts per year divided by the number of hosts. Therefore, our estimator, combined with the demographic information on population size, estimates the total number of Plasmodium falciparum infections acquired by all individual hosts in the population of interest per year. We clarified this point in the revised manuscript in the subsection of the Materials and Methods, entitled ‘Population-level MOI distribution for approximating time-series observation of MOI per host or many realizations at the same sampling time per host’ (Line 623-639).

      We evaluated the impact of individual heterogeneity due to transmission on FOI inference using simulation outputs (Line 157-184, Figure 1-2 and Appendix 1-Figure 11-15). Even with significant heterogeneity among individuals (2/3 of the population receiving approximately 94% of all bites whereas the remaining 1/3 receives the rest of the bites), our methods performed comparably to scenarios with homogeneous transmission. Furthermore, our methods demonstrated similar performance for both non-seasonal and seasonal transmission scenarios.

      Regarding the second point, we quantitatively assessed the ability of the estimator to recover the truth across simulations and included this information in a supplementary table in the revised manuscript (supplementary file 3-FOImethodsPerformance.xlsx). Specifically, we indicated whether the truth lies within the bootstrap distribution and provided a measure of relative deviation, which is defined as the true FOI value minus the median of the bootstrap distribution for the estimate, normalized by the true FOI value .  This assessment is a valuable addition which enhances clarity, but please note that our previous graphical comparisons do illustrate the ability of the methods to estimate “sensible” values, close to the truth despite multiple sources of errors. “Close” here is relative to the scale of variation of FOI in the field and to the kind of precision that would be useful in an empirical context. From a practical perspective based on the potential range of variation of FOI, the graphical results already illustrate that the estimated distributions would be informative.

      We also thank the reviewer for highlighting instances where our proposed methods for FOI inference perform sub-optimally (e.g. Figure 10, Figure 1 under the mid-IRS panel in the previous manuscript). This feedback prompted us to examine these instances more closely and identify the underlying causes related to the stochastic impact introduced during various sampling processes. These include sampling the host population and their infections at a specific sampling depth in the simulated output, matching the depth used for collecting empirical data. In addition, previously, we imputed MOI estimates for treated individuals by sampling only once from non-treated individuals. This time, we conducted 200 samplings and used the final weighted MOI distribution for FOI inference. By doing so, we reduced the impact of extreme single-sampling efforts on MOI distribution and FOI inference. In other words, some of these suboptimal instances correspond to the scenarios where the one-time sampled MOIs from non-treated individuals do not fully capture the MOI distribution of non-treated individuals. We added a section titled ‘Reducing stochastic impact in sampling processes’ to Appendix 1 on this matter (Line 841-849).

      The reviewer correctly noted that our proposed methods tend to underestimate FOI (Figure 1-2, 10-14, ‘Estimated All Errors’ and ‘Estimated Undersampling of Var’ panels in the previous manuscript, corresponding to Figure 1-2 and Appendix 1-Figure 11-15 in the revised manuscript). This underestimation arises from the underestimation of MOI. The Bayesian formulation of the varcoding method does not account for the limited overlap between co-infecting strains, an additional factor that reduces the number of var genes detected per individual. We have elaborated on this matter in the Results and Discussion sections of the revised manuscript (Line 142-149, 252-256).

      d. Furthermore the authors state in the methods that the choice of mean and variance (and thus second moment) parameters for inter-arrival times are varied widely, however, it's not clear what those ranges are there needs to be a clear table or figure caption showing what combinations of values were tested and which results are produced from them, this is an essential component of the method and it's impossible to fully evaluate its performance without this information. This relates to the issue of selecting the mean and variance values that maximize the likelihood of observing a given distribution of MOI estimates, this is very unclear since no likelihoods have been written down in the methods section of the main text, which likelihood are the authors referring to, is this the probability distribution of the steady state queue length distribution? At other places the authors refer to these quantities as Maximum Likelihood estimators, how do they know they have found the MLE? There are no derivations in the manuscript to support this. The authors should specify the likelihood and include in an appendix an explanation of why their estimation procedure is in fact maximizing this likelihood, preferably with evidence of the shape of the likelihood, and how fine the grid of values they tested is for their mean and variance since this could influence the overall quality of the estimation procedure.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out these aspects of the work that can be further clarified. In response, we maximized the likelihood of observing the population-level MOI distribution in the sampled population (see our responses to your previous comment c), given queue length distributions, derived from the two-moment approximation method for various mean and variance combinations of inter-arrival times. We added a new section to the Materials and Methods in the revised manuscript with an explicit likelihood formulation (Line 574-585).

      Additionally, we specified the ranges for the mean and variance parameters for inter-arrival times and provided the grid of values tested in a supplementary table (supplementary file 4-meanVarianceParams.xlsx). Example figures illustrating the shape of the likelihood have also been included in Appendix 1-Figure 9. We tested the impact of different grid value choices on estimation quality by refining the grid to include more points, ensuring the FOI inference results are consistent. The results of the test are documented in the revised manuscript (Line 587-593, Appendix 1-Figure 10).

      (2) Limitation of FOI estimation procedure.

      a. The authors discuss the importance of the duration of infection to this problem. While I agree that empirically estimating this is not possible, there are other options besides assuming that all 1-5-year-olds have the same duration of infection distribution as naïve adults co-infected with syphilis. E.g. it would be useful to test a wide range of assumed infection duration and assess their impact on the estimation procedure. Furthermore, if the authors are going to stick to the described method for duration of infection, the potentially limited generalizability of this method needs to be further highlighted in both the introduction, and the discussion. In particular, for an estimated mean FOI of about 5 per host per year in the pre-IRS season as estimated in Ghana (Figure 3) it seems that this would not translate to 4-year-old being immune naïve, and certainly this would not necessarily generalize well to a school-aged child population or an adult population.

      We thank the reviewer for this useful comment. The reviewer correctly noted the challenge in empirically measuring the duration of infection for 1-5-year-olds and comparing it to that of naïve adults co-infected with syphilis. We nevertheless continued to use the described method for the duration of infection, while more thoroughly acknowledging and discussing the limitations this aspect of the method introduces. We have highlighted this potential limitation in the Abstract, Introduction, and Discussion sections of the revised manuscript (Line 26-28, 99-103, 270-292). It is important to note that the infection duration from the historical clinical data we have relied on has been used, and is still used, in the malaria modeling community as a credible source for this parameter in untreated natural infections of malaria-naïve individuals in endemic settings of Africa (e.g. in the agent-based model OpenMalaria, see 1).

      To reduce misspecification in infection duration and fully utilize our proposed methods, future data collection and sampling could prioritize subpopulations with minimal prior infections and an immune profile similar to naïve adults, such as infants and toddlers. As these individuals are also the most vulnerable, prioritizing them aligns with the priority of all intervention efforts in the short term, which is to monitor and protect the most vulnerable individuals from severe symptoms and death. We discuss this aspect in detail in the Discussion section of the revised manuscript (Line 287-292).

      In the pre-IRS phase of Ghana surveys, an estimated mean FOI of about 5 per host per year indicates that a 4-year-old child would have experienced around 20 infections, which could suggest they are far from naïve. The extreme diversity of circulating var genes (2) implies, however, that even after 20 infections, a 4-year-old may have only developed immunity to a small fraction of the variant surface antigens (PfEMP1, Plasmodium falciparum erythrocyte membrane protein 1) encoded by this important gene family. Consequently, these children are not as immunologically experienced as it might initially seem. Moreover, studies have shown that long-lived infections in older children and adults can persist for months or even years, including through the dry season. This persistence is driven by high antigenic variation of var genes and associated incomplete immunity. Additionally, parasites can skew PfEMP1 expression to produce less adhesive erythrocytes, enhancing splenic clearance, reducing virulence, and maintaining sub-clinical parasitemia (3, 4, 5). The impact of immunity on infection duration with age for falciparum malaria remains a challenging open question.

      Lastly, the FOI for naïve hosts is a key basic parameter for epidemiological models of complex infectious diseases like falciparum malaria, in both agent-based and equation-based formulations. This is because FOI for non-naïve hosts is typically a function of their immune status, body size, and the FOI of naïve hosts. Thus, knowing the FOI of naïve hosts helps parameterize and validate these models by reducing degrees of freedom.

      b. The evaluation of the capacity parameter c seems to be quite important and is set at 30, however, the authors only describe trying values of 25 and 30, and claim that this does not impact FOI inference, however it is not clear that this is the case. What happens if the carrying capacity is increased substantially? Alternatively, this would be more convincing if the authors provided a mathematical explanation of why the carrying capacity increase will not influence the FOI inference, but absent that, this should be mentioned and discussed as a limitation.

      Thank you for this question. This parameter represents the carrying capacity of the queuing system, or the maximum number of blood-stage strains with which an individual human host can be co-infected. Empirical evidence, estimated using the varcoding method, suggests this value is 20 (2), providing a lower bound for parameter c. However, the varcoding method does not account for the limited overlap between co-infecting strains, which reduces the number of var genes detected in an individual, thereby affecting the basis of MOI estimation. Additional factors, such as the synchronicity of clones in their 48-hour life cycle on alternate days (6) and within-host competition of strains leading to low-parasitemia levels (7, 8), contribute to under-sampling of strains and are not accounted for in MOI estimation (9). To address these potential under-sampling issues, we previously tested values of 25 and 30.

      This time, we systematically investigated a wider range of values, including substantially higher ones: 25, 30, 40, and 60. We found that the FOI inference results are similar across these values. Figure 3 in the main text and supplementary figures (Appendix 1-Figure 16-18) illustrates these findings.

      The parameter c influences the steady-state queue length distribution based on the two-moment approximation with specific mean and variance combinations, primarily affecting the distribution’s tail when customer or infection flows are high. Smaller values of c lower the maximum possible queue length, making the system more prone to “overflow”. In such cases, customers or infections may find no space available upon their arrival, hence not incrementing the queue length.

      Empirical MOI distributions for high-transmission endemic regions center around 4 or 5, mostly remaining below 10, with only a small fraction between 15-20 (2). These distributions do not support parameter combinations resulting in frequent overflow for a system with c equal to 25 or 30. As one increases the value of c further, these parameter combinations would cause the MOI distributions to shift to larger values inconsistent with the empirical MOI distributions. We therefore do not expect substantially higher values for parameter c to noticeably change either the relative shape of the likelihood or the MLE.

      We have included a subsection on parameter c in the Materials and Methods section of the revised manuscript (Line 596-612).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors combine a clever use of historical clinical data on infection duration in immunologically naive individuals and queuing theory to infer the force of infection (FOI) from measured multiplicity of infection (MOI) in a sparsely sampled setting. They conduct extensive simulations using agent-based modeling to recapitulate realistic population dynamics and successfully apply their method to recover FOI from measured MOI. They then go on to apply their method to real-world data from Ghana before and after an indoor residual spraying campaign.

      Strengths:

      (1) The use of historical clinical data is very clever in this context.

      (2) The simulations are very sophisticated with respect to trying to capture realistic population dynamics.

      (3) The mathematical approach is simple and elegant, and thus easy to understand.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The assumptions of the approach are quite strong and should be made more clear. While the historical clinical data is a unique resource, it would be useful to see how misspecification of the duration of infection distribution would impact the estimates.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up the limitation of our proposed methods due to their reliance on a known and fixed duration of infection distribution from historical clinical data. Please see our response to Reviewer 1, Comment 2a, for a detailed discussion on this matter.

      (2) Seeing as how the assumption of the duration of infection distribution is drawn from historical data and not informed by the data on hand, it does not substantially expand beyond MOI. The authors could address this by suggesting avenues for more refined estimates of infection duration.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out a potential improvement to our work. We acknowledge that FOI is inferred from MOI and thus depends on the information contained in MOI. However, MOI by definition is a number and not a rate parameter. FOI for naïve hosts is a fundamental parameter for epidemiological models of complex infectious diseases like falciparum malaria, in both agent-based and equation-based formulations. FOI of non-naïve hosts is typically a function of their immune status, body size, and the FOI of naïve hosts. Thus, knowing the FOI of naïve hosts helps parameterize and validate these models by reducing degrees of freedom. In this sense, we believe the transformation from MOI to FOI is valuable.

      Measuring infection duration is challenging, making the simultaneous estimation of infection duration and FOI an attractive alternative, as the referee noted. This, however, would require closely monitored cohort studies or densely sampled cross-sectional surveys to reduce issues like identifiability. For instance, a higher arrival rate of infections paired with a shorter infection duration could generate a similar MOI distribution to a lower arrival rate with a longer infection duration. In some cases, incorrect combinations of rate and duration might even produce an MOI distribution that appears closer to the targeted distribution. Such cohort studies and densely sampled cross-sectional surveys have not been and will not be widely available across different geographical locations and times. This work utilizes more readily available data from sparsely sampled single-time-point cross-sectional surveys, which precludes more sophisticated derivation of time-varying average arrival rates of infections and lacks the resolution to simultaneously estimate arrival rates and infection duration. In the revised manuscript, we have elaborated on this matter and added a paragraph in the Discussion section (Line 306-309).

      (3) It is unclear in the example how their bootstrap imputation approach is accounting for measurement error due to antimalarial treatment. They supply two approaches. First, there is no effect on measurement, so the measured MOI is unaffected, which is likely false and I think the authors are in agreement. The second approach instead discards the measurement for malaria-treated individuals and imputes their MOI by drawing from the remaining distribution. This is an extremely strong assumption that the distribution of MOI of the treated is the same as the untreated, which seems unlikely simply out of treatment-seeking behavior. By imputing in this way, the authors will also deflate the variability of their estimates.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out aspects of the work that can be further clarified. Disentangling the effect of drug treatment on measurements like infection duration is challenging. Since our methods rely on the known and fixed distribution of infection duration from historical data of naïve patients with neurosyphilis infected with malaria as a therapy, drug treatment can potentially violate this assumption. In the previous manuscript, we did not attempt to directly address the impact of drug treatment. Instead, we considered two extreme scenarios that bound reality, well summarized by the reviewer. Reality lies somewhere in between these two extremes, with antimalarial treatment significantly affecting measurements in some individuals but not in others. Nonetheless, the results of FOI inference do not differ significantly across both extremes.

      The impact of the drugs likely depends on their nature, efficiency, and duration. We note that treatment information was collected via a routine questionnaire, with participant self-reporting that they had received an antimalarial treatment in the previous two-weeks before the surveys (i.e., participants that reported they were sick, sought treatment, and were provided with an antimalarial treatment). No confirmation through hospital or clinic records was conducted, as it was beyond the scope of the study. Additionally, many of these sick individuals seek treatment at local chemists, which may limit the relevance of hospital or clinic records, if they are even available. Consequently, information on the nature, efficiency, and duration of administrated drugs was incomplete or lacking. As this is not the focus of this work, we do not elaborate on the impact of drug treatment in the revised manuscript.

      The reviewer correctly noted that this imputation might not add additional information and could reduce MOI variability. Therefore, in the revised manuscript, we reported FOI estimates with drug-treated 1-5-year-olds excluded. Additionally, we discarded the infection status and MOI values of treated individuals and sampled their MOI from non-treated microscopy-positive individuals, imputing a positive MOI for treated and uninfected individuals. We also reported FOI estimates based on these MOI values. This scenario provides an upper bound for FOI estimates. Note that we do not assume that the MOI distribution for treated individuals is the same as that for untreated individuals. Rather, we aim to estimate what their MOI would have been, and consequently, determine what the FOI per individual per year in the combined population would be, had these individuals not received antimalarial treatment. The results of FOI inference do not differ significantly between these two approaches. They can serve as general solutions to antimalarial treatment issues for others applying our FOI inference methods. These details can be found in the revised manuscript (Line 185-210, 462-484).

      - For similar reasons, their imputation of microscopy-negative individuals is also questionable, as it also assumes the same distributions of MOI for microscopy-positive and negative individuals.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. The reviewer correctly noted that we imputed the MOI values for microscopy-negative but PCR-positive 1-5-year-olds by sampling from the microscopy-positive 1-5-year-olds, under the assumption that both groups have similar MOI distributions. This approach was motivated by the analysis of our Ghana surveys, which shows no clear relationship between MOI (or the number of var genes detected within an individual host, on the basis of which our MOI values were estimated) and the parasitemia levels of those hosts. Parasitemia levels underlie the difference in detection sensitivity between PCR and microscopy.

      In the revised manuscript, we elaborated on this issue and included formal regression tests showing the lack of a relationship between MOI/the number of var genes detected within an individual host and the parasitemia levels of those hosts (Line 445-451, Appendix 1-Figure 7). We also described potential reasons or hypotheses behind this observation (Line 452-461).

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      It has been proposed that the FOI is a method of using parasite genetics to determine changes in transmission in areas with high asymptomatic infection. The manuscript attempts to use queuing theory to convert multiplicity of infection estimates (MOI) into estimates of the force of infection (FOI), which they define as the number of genetically distinct blood-stage strains. They look to validate the method by applying it to simulated results from a previously published agent-based model. They then apply these queuing theory methods to previously published and analysed genetic data from Ghana. They then compare their results to previous estimates of FOI.

      Strengths:

      It would be great to be able to infer FOI from cross-sectional surveys which are easier and cheaper than current FOI estimates which require longitudinal studies. This work proposes a method to convert MOI to FOI for cross-sectional studies. They attempt to validate this process using a previously published agent-based model which helps us understand the complexity of parasite population genetics.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) I fear that the work could be easily over-interpreted as no true validation was done, as no field estimates of FOI (I think considered true validation) were measured. The authors have developed a method of estimating FOI from MOI which makes a number of biological and structural assumptions. I would not call being able to recreate model results that were generated using a model that makes its own (probably similar) defined set of biological and structural assumptions a validation of what is going on in the field. The authors claim this at times (for example, Line 153) and I feel it would be appropriate to differentiate this in the discussion.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment, although we think there is a mis-understanding on what can and cannot be practically validated in the sense of a “true” measure of FOI that would be free from assumptions for a complex disease such as malaria. We would not want the results to be over-interpreted, and we have extended the discussion of what we have done to test the methods in the revised manuscript (Line 314-328). Performance evaluation via simulation output is common and often necessary for statistical methods. These simulations can come from dynamical or descriptive models, each making their own assumptions to simplify reality. Our stochastic agent-based model (ABM) of malaria transmission, used in this study, has successfully replicated several key patterns from high-transmission endemic regions in the field, including aspects of strain diversity not represented and captured by simpler models (10).

      In what sense this ABM makes a set of biological and structural assumptions that are “probably similar” to those of the queuing methods we present is not clear to us. We agree that using models with different structural assumptions from the method being tested is ideal. Our FOI inference methods based on queuing theory require the duration of infection distribution and the MOI distribution among sampled individuals. However, these FOI inference methods are agnostic to the specific biological mechanisms governing these distributions.

      Another important point raised by this comment is what would be the “true” FOI value against which to validate our methods. Empirical MOI-FOI pairs from cohort studies tracking FOI directly are still lacking. Direct FOI measurements are prone to errors because differentiating new infections from the temporary absence of an old infection in the peripheral blood and its subsequent re-emergence remains challenging. Reasons for this challenge include the low resolution of the polymorphic markers used in cohort studies, which cannot fully differentiate hyper-diverse antigenic strains, and the complexity of within-host dynamics and competitive interaction of co-infecting strains (6, 8, 9). Alternative approaches also do not provide a “true” FOI estimation free from assumptions. These approaches involve fitting simplified epidemiological models to densely sampled/repeated cross-sectional surveys for FOI inference. In this case, no FOI is measured directly, and thus, there are no FOI values available for benchmarking against fitted FOI values. The evaluation or validation of these model-fitting approaches is typically based on their ability to capture other epidemiological quantities that are easier to sample or measure, such as prevalence or incidence, with criteria such as the Akaike information criterion (AIC). This type of evaluation is similar to the one done in this work. We selected FOI values that maximize the likelihood of observing the given MOI distribution. Furthermore, we paired our estimated FOI values for Ghana surveys with the independently measured EIR (Entomological Inoculation Rate), a common field measure of transmission intensity. We ensured that our resulting FOI-EIR points align with existing FOI-EIR pairs and the relationship between these quantities from previous studies. We acknowledge that, like model-fitting approaches, our validation for the field data is also indirect and further complicated by high variance in the relationship between EIR and FOI from previous studies.

      Prompted by the reviewer’s comment, we elaborated on these points in the revised manuscript, emphasizing the indirect nature and existing constraints of our validation with field data in the Discussion section (Line 314-328). Additionally, we clarified certain basic assumptions of our agent-based model in Appendix 1-Simulation data.

      (2) Another aspect of the paper is adding greater realism to the previous agent-based model, by including assumptions on missing data and under-sampling. This takes prominence in the figures and results section, but I would imagine is generally not as interesting to the less specialised reader. The apparent lack of impact of drug treatment on MOI is interesting and counterintuitive, though it is not really mentioned in the results or discussion sufficiently to allay my confusion. I would have been interested in understanding the relationship between MOI and FOI as generated by your queuing theory method and the model. It isn't clear to me why these more standard results are not presented, as I would imagine they are outputs of the model (though happy to stand corrected - it isn't entirely clear to me what the model is doing in this manuscript alone).

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. Please refer to our response to Reviewer 2, comment (3), as we made changes in the revised manuscript regarding antimalarial drug treated individuals. We reported two sets of FOI estimates. In the first, we excluded these treated individuals from the analysis as suggested by Reviewer 2. In the second, we discarded their infection status and MOI estimates and sampling from non-treated individuals.

      The reviewer correctly noted the surprising lack of impact of antimalarial treatment on MOI estimates. This pattern is indeed interesting and counterintuitive. The impact of the drugs likely depends on their nature, efficiency, and duration. We note that treatment information was collected via a routine questionnaire, with participant self-reporting that they had received an antimalarial treatment in the previous two-weeks before the surveys (i.e., participants that reported they were sick, sought treatment, and were provided with an antimalarial treatment). No confirmation through hospital or clinic or pharmacy records was conducted, as it was beyond the scope of the study. Additionally, many of these sick individuals seek treatment at local chemists, which may limit the relevance of hospital or clinic records, if they are even available. Consequently, information on the nature, efficiency, and duration of administrated drugs was incomplete or lacking. As this is not the focus of this work, we do not elaborate on the impact of drug treatment in the revised manuscript.

      Regarding the last point of the reviewer, on understanding the relationship between MOI and FOI, we are not fully clear about what was meant. We are also confused about the statement on what the “model is doing in this manuscript alone”. We interpret the overall comment as the reviewer suggesting a better understanding of the relationship between MOI and FOI generated by the two-moment approximation method and the agent-based model. This could involve exploring the relationship between the moments of their distributions, possibly by fitting models such as simple linear regression models. Although this approach is in principle possible, it falls outside the focus of our work. Moreover, it would be challenging to evaluate the performance of this alternative approach given the lack of MOI-FOI pairs from empirical settings with directly measured FOI values (from large cohort studies). Nonetheless, we note that the qualitative relationship between the two quantities is intuitive. Higher FOI values should correspond to higher MOI values. Less variable FOI values should result in more narrow or concentrated MOI distributions, whereas more variable FOI values should lead to more spread-out MOI distributions. We described this qualitative relationship between MOI and FOI in the revised manuscript (Line 499-502).

      As mentioned in the response to the reviewer’s previous point (1), we hope that our clarification of the basic assumptions underlying our agent-based model in Appendix 1-Simulation data helps the reviewer gain a better sense of the model. We appreciate agent-based models involve more assumptions and parameters than typical equation-based models in epidemiology, and their description can be difficult to follow. We have extended this description to rely less on previous publications. As for other ABMs, the population dynamics of the disease is followed over time by tracking individual hosts and strains. This allows us to implement specific immune memory to the large number of strains arising from the var multigene family. There is no equation-based formulation of the transmission dynamics that can incorporate immune memory in the presence of such large variation as well as recombination of the strains. We rely on this model because large strain diversity at high transmission underlies superinfection of individual hosts, and therefore, MOI values larger than one. We relied on the estimation of MOI with a method based on var gene sampling, and therefore, simulated such sampling for individual hosts (which requires an ABM and one that represents such genes and resulting strains explicitly).

      (3) I would suggest that outside of malaria geneticists, the force of infection is considered to be the entomological inoculation rate, not the number of genetically distinct blood-stage strains. I appreciate that FOI has been used to explain the latter before by others, though the authors could avoid confusion by stating this clearly throughout the manuscript. For example, the abstract says FOI is "the number of new infections acquired by an individual host over a given time interval" which suggests the former, please consider clarifying.

      We thank the reviewer for this helpful comment, as it is crucial to avoid any confusion regarding basic definitions. EIR, the entomological inoculation rate, is closely related to the FOI, force of infection, but they are not equivalent. EIR focuses on the rate of arrival of infectious bites and is measured as such by focusing on the mosquito vectors that are infectious and arrive to bite a given host. Not all these bites result in actual infection of the human host. Epidemiological models of malaria transmission clearly make this distinction, as FOI is defined as the rate at which a host acquires infection. This definition comes from more general models of the population dynamics of infectious diseases. For simpler diseases without super-infection, the typical SIR models define FOI as the rate at which a susceptible individual becomes infected. In the context of malaria, FOI refers to the number of new infections acquired by an individual host over a given time interval. This distinction between EIR and FOI is the reason why studies have investigated their relationship, with the nonlinearity of this relationship reflecting the complexity of the underlying biology and how host immunity influences the outcome of an infectious bite.

      We added “blood-stage strains” to the definition of FOI in the previous manuscript, as pointed out by the reviewer, for the following reason. After an individual host acquires an infection/strain from an infectious mosquito bite, the strain undergoes a multi-stage life cycle within the host, including the liver stage and asexual blood stage. Liver-stage infections can fail to advance to the blood stage due to immunity or exceeding the blood-stage carrying capacity. Only active blood-stage infections are detectable in all direct measures of FOI. Quantities used in indirect model-fitting approaches for estimating FOI are also based on or reflect these blood-stage strains/infections. Only these blood-stage strains/infections are transmissible to other individuals, impacting disease dynamics. Ultimately, the FOI we seek to estimate is the one defined as specified above, as well as in both the previous and revised manuscripts, consistent with the epidemiological literature. We expanded on this point in the revised manuscript (Line 641-656).

      (4) Line 319 says "Nevertheless, overall, our paired EIR (directly measured by the entomological team in Ghana (Tiedje et al., 2022)) and FOI values are reasonably consistent with the data points from previous studies, suggesting the robustness of our proposed methods". I would agree that the results are consistent, given that there is huge variation in Figure 4 despite the transformed scales, but I would not say this suggests a robustness of the method.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and have modified the relevant sentences to use “consistent” instead of “robust” (Line 229-231).

      (5) The text is a little difficult to follow at times and sometimes requires multiple reads to understand. Greater precision is needed with the language in a few situations and some of the assumptions made in the modelling process are not referenced, making it unclear whether it is a true representation of the biology.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. As mentioned in the response to Reviewer 1 and in response to your previous points, we have shortened, reorganized and rewritten parts of the text in the revised manuscript to improve clarity and readability.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Minor comments:

      Bar graphs in Figures 6 and 7 are not an appropriate way to rigorously compare whether your estimated MOI (under different approaches) is comparable to your true MOIs. Particularly in Figure 6 it is very difficult to clearly compare what is going on. If anything in Figure 7 it looks like as MOI gets higher, Bayesian methods and barcoding are overestimating relative to the truth. The large Excel file that shows KS statistics could be better summarized (and include p-values not in a separate table) and further discussion of how these methods perform on metrics other than the mean value would be important given that MOI distributions can be heavily right skewed and these high MOI values contain a large proportion of genetic diversity which can be highly informative for the purposes of this estimation.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment. It appears there may have been some misinterpretation of the pattern in Figure 7 in the previous manuscript. We believe the reviewer meant “as MOI gets higher, Bayesian methods and varcoding are UNDERESTIMATING relative to the truth” rather than “OVERESTIMATING”.

      We agree with the reviewer that the comparison of MOI distributions can be improved. To better quantify the difference between the MOI distribution from the original varcoding method and its Bayesian formulation relative to true MOIs, we replaced the KS test conducted in the previous manuscript with two alternative, more powerful tests: the Cramer-von Mises Test and the Anderson-Darling Test. The Cramer-von Mises Test quantifies the sum of the squared differences between the two cumulative distribution functions, while the Anderson-Darling Test, a modification of the Cramer-von Mises Test, gives more weight to the tails of the distribution, as noted by the reviewer. We have summarized the results, including test statistics and their associated p-values, in a supplementary table (Line 135-149, Line 862-883, supplementary file 1-MOImethodsPerformance.xlsx and supplementary file 7-BayesianImprovement.xlsx).

      Throughout the text the authors use "consistent" to describe their estimation of FOI, I know this is meant in the colloquial use of the word but consider changing this word to replicable or something similar. When talking about estimators, usually, consistency implies asymptotic convergence in probability which we do not know whether the proposed estimator does.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We changed “consistent” to “replicable” in the revised manuscript.

      I think there is an issue with the numbering of the figures, they are just numbered continuously between the main text and appendix between 1 and 15, but in the text, there is a different numbering system between the main text and appendix figures.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have double-checked to ensure that the numbering of the figures is consistent with the text in the revised manuscript. Figures are numbered continuously between the main text and the appendix. When referring to these figures in the text, we provide a prefix (i.e., Appendix 1) indicating whether the figure is in the main text or Appendix 1, followed by the figure number.

      The description of the bootstrap for 95% CI is a bit sparse, did bootstrap distributions look symmetric? If not did authors use a skewness adjustment to ensure good coverage? Also, is the bootstrap unit of resampling at the individual level, the simulation scenario level, population level?

      We checked the bootstrap distributions and calculated their skewness. The majority fall within the range of -0.5 to 0.5, with a few exceptions falling within the range of 0.5-0.75 (supplementary file 6-FOIBootstrapSkewness.xlsx). We considered them as fairly symmetric and thus did not use a skewness adjustment.

      In Figures 8 and 9 the x-axes seem to imply there are both the true and estimated MOI distributions on the plot but only 1 color of grey is clearly visible. If there are 2 distributions the color or size needs to be changed or if not consider re-labeling the x-axis.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. There was a mistake in the x-axis labels in Figure 8 and 9. Only the estimated MOI distributions were shown because the true ones are not available for the Ghana field surveys. The labels should simply be “Estimated MOIvar”.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Throughout the results section there are lots of vague statements such as "differ only slightly", "exhibit a somewhat larger, but still small, difference", etc. Please include the exact values and ranges within the text where appropriate because it can be difficult to discern from the figure.

      We thank the reviewer for this useful comment. In the revised manuscript, we have provided exact values and ranges where appropriate (supplementary file 1- MOImethodsPerformance.xlsx, supplementary file 3- FOImethodsPerformance.xlsx, and supplementary file 7-BayesianImprovement.xlsx).

      (2) Truncate decimals to 2 places.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. In the revised manuscript, we have truncated decimals to two places where applicable.

      (3) The queueing theory notation in the methods section is unfamiliar, specifically things like "M/M/c/k", please define the variables used.

      We thank the reviewer for this useful comment. In the revised manuscript, we have defined all the variables used. Please refer to our responses to Reviewer 1 Point (1) a.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The work takes many of the models and data from a previous paper published in eLife in 2023 (the 4 most senior authors of this previous manuscript are the 4 authors of the current manuscript). This previous paper introduced some new terminology "census population" which was highlighted as being potentially confusing by 2 of the 3 reviewers of the original article. This was somewhat rebuffed by the authors, though their response was ambiguous about whether the terminology would be changed in any potential future revision. The census population terminology does not appear in this manuscript, though the same data is being used. Publication of similar papers with the same data and different terminology could generate confusion, so I would encourage authors to be consistent and make sure the two papers are in line. To this end, it feels like this paper would be better suited to be classified as a "Research Advances" on this original manuscript and linked, which is a nice functionality that eLife offers.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment, but we do not think our work would fall under the criteria of “Research Advances” based on our previous paper pointed out by the reviewer. The reviewer correctly noted that the current work and the previous paper used the same datasets. However, they have different goals and are not related in terms of content.

      The previous paper examined how epidemiological quantities and diversity measurements of the local parasite population change following the initiation of effective control interventions and subsequently as this control wanes. These quantities included MOI and census population size (MOI was estimated using the Bayesian formulation of the varcoding method, and the census population size was derived from summing MOIvar across individuals in the human population). In contrast, our current work focused on a different goal: inferring FOI based on MOI. We proposed two methods from queuing theory and illustrated them with MOI estimates obtained with the Bayesian formulation of the "varcoding" method. Although the method applied to estimate MOI is indeed the same as that of the paper mentioned by the reviewer, the proposed methods should be applicable to MOI estimates obtained in any other way, as stated in the Abstract in the previous manuscript. That is, the methods we present in the current paper are independent from the way the MOI estimation has been carried out. Our results are not about the MOI values themselves but rather on an illustration of the methods for converting those MOI values to FOI. In fact, there are different ways to obtain MOI estimates for Plasmodium falciparum (9). The most common approach for determining MOI involves size-polymorphic antigenic markers, such as msp1, msp2, msp3, glurp, ama1, and csp. Similarly, microsatellites, also termed simple sequence repeat (SSR), are another type of size-polymorphic marker that can be amplified to estimate MOI by determining the number of alleles detected. Combinations of genome-wide single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) have also been used to estimate MOI.

      The result section of the current manuscript begins by evaluating how different kinds of errors/sampling limitations affect the estimation of MOI using the Bayesian formulation of the varcoding method. Only that brief section, which is not the core or primary objective of the manuscript, could be considered an extension and an advancement related to the other paper. We considered the effect of these errors on the resulting estimates of FOI.

      We further note that, as the reviewer pointed out, the census population size is not utilized at all in our current work. We are unclear on why this quantity is mentioned here. Our previous paper has been revised and can be found in eLife as such. We have not changed this terminology and have provided a clear explanation for why we chose it. The reviewer seems to have read the previous response to version 1 posted on December 28, 2023 (Note that version 2 and the associated response was posted on November 20, 2024). Regardless, this is not the place for a discussion on another paper on a quantity that is irrelevant to the current work being reviewed.

      We understand that the reviewer’s impression may have been influenced by the previous emphasis on the Bayesian formulation of the varcoding method in our manuscript. With the reorganization and rewriting of parts of the manuscript, we hope the revised version will clearly convey the central goal of our work.

      (2) Similar statements that could be toned down. 344 ".... two-moment approximation approach and Little's law are shown to provide consistent and good FOI estimates,.....", 374 "Thus, the flexibility and generality of these two proposed methods allow robust estimation of an important metric for malaria transmission"

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have modified the descriptive terms for the performance of our methods. Please also refer to our responses to Reviewer 1, Point (1) c and your previous Point (1).

      (3) Various assumptions seem to have been made which are not justified. For example, heterogeneous mixing is defined as 2/3rd of the population receives 90% of the bites. A reference for this would be good.

      In this work, we considered heterogenous transmission arising from 2/3 of the population receiving approximately 94% of all bites, because we believe this distribution introduces a reasonable and sufficient amount of heterogeneity in exposure risk across individuals. We are not aware of field studies justifying this degree of heterogeneity.

      (4) The work assumes children under 5 have no immunity (Line 648 says "It is thus safe to consider negligible the impact of immune memory accumulated from previous infections on the duration of a current infection." ). Is there supporting evidence for this and what would happen if this wasn't the case?

      We thank the reviewer for this helpful comment. Please refer to our responses to Reviewer 1 Point (2) a.

      (5) Similarly, there are a few instances of a need for more copy-editing. The text says "We continue with the result of the heterogeneous exposure risk scenarios in which a high-risk group ( 2/3 of the total population) receives around 94% of all bites whereas a low-risk group ( 1/3 of the total population) receives the remaining bites (Appendix 1-Figure 5C)." whereas the referenced caption says "For example, heterogeneous mixing is defined as 2/3rd of population receives 90% of the bites."

      We believe there was a misinterpretation of the legend caption. In the referenced caption, we stated “2/3rd of population receives MORE THAN 90% of the bites”, which aligns with “around 94% of all bites”. Nonetheless, to maintain consistency in the revised manuscript, we have updated the description to uniformly state “approximately 94% of all bites” throughout.

      (6) The term "measurement error" is used to describe the missing potential under-sampling of var genes. Given this would only go one way isn't the term "bias" more appropriate?

      We understand that, in general English, “bias” might seem more precise for describing a deviation in one direction. However, in malaria epidemiology and in models for malaria and other infectious diseases, “measurement error” is a general term that describes deviations introduced in the process of measurement and sampling, which can confound or add noise to the true values being collected. This term is commonly used, and we have adhered to it in the revised manuscript.

      (7) Line 739 "Though FOI and EIR both reflect transmission intensity, the former refers directly to detectable blood-stage infections whereas the latter concerns human-vector contact rates." In my mind this is not true, the EIR is the number of potentially invading parasites (a contact rate between parasites in mosquitoes and humans if you will). The human-vector contact rate is the human biting rate.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have clarified the definition regarding FOI and EIR in our response to your previous comment (3) and in the revised manuscript. We agree that the term “human-vector contact rates” was not precise enough for EIR. We intended “human-infectious vector contact rates”, and we have updated the text to reflect this change (Line 644-645).

      References and Notes

      (1) Maire, N. et al. A model for natural immunity to asexual blood stages of Plasmodium falciparum malaria in endemic areas. Am J Trop Med Hyg., 75(2 Suppl):19-31 (2006).

      (2) Tiedje, K. E. et al. Measuring changes in Plasmodium falciparum census population size in response to sequential malaria control interventions. eLife, 12 (2023).

      (3) Andrade C. M. et al. Infection length and host environment influence on Plasmodium falciparum dry season reservoir. EMBO Mol Med.,16(10):2349-2375 (2024).

      (4) Zhang X. and Deitsch K. W. The mystery of persistent, asymptomatic Plasmodium falciparum infections, Current Opinion in Microbiology, 70:102231 (2022).

      (5) Tran, T. M. et al. An Intensive Longitudinal Cohort Study of Malian Children and Adults Reveals No Evidence of Acquired Immunity to Plasmodium falciparum Infection, Clinical Infectious Diseases, 57(1):40–47 (2013).

      (6) Farnert, A., Snounou, G., Rooth, I., Bjorkman, A. Daily dynamics of Plasmodium falciparum subpopulations in asymptomatic children in a holoendemic area. Am J Trop Med Hyg., 56(5):538-47 (1997).

      (7) Read, A. F. and Taylor, L. H. The Ecology of Genetically Diverse Infections, Science, 292:1099-1102 (2001).

      (8) Sondo, P. et al. Genetically diverse Plasmodium falciparum infections, within-host competition and symptomatic malaria in humans. Sci Rep 9(127) (2019).

      (9) Labbe, F. et al. Neutral vs. non-neutral genetic footprints of Plasmodium falciparum multiclonal infections. PLoS Comput Biol, 19(1) (2023).

      (10) He, Q. et al. Networks of genetic similarity reveal non-neutral processes shape strain structure in Plasmodium falciparum. Nat Commun 9(1817) (2018).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      I can find no problems with the experiments performed in this study, but there are several results that are not easily explained. I would like to see more consideration of possible explanations. For example, one of the major differences between the the CESA structure from primary and secondary cell walls is the displacement of TM7 in the primary cell wall CESAs that leads to the formation of lipid exposed channel. Why does this vary between primary and secondary cell wall CESA proteins? Could it explain differences in the properties, such as crystallinity between primary and secondary cell wall cellulose?

      At this time, the different position of TM helix 7 observed in our GmCesA structures is just an observation. We have some emerging evidence that this helix is also flexible in POCesA8 under certain conditions; however, we do not know whether this affects catalytic activity or cellulose coalescence. We have revised the text to avoid the interpretation that TM 7 repositioning is a characteristic feature of primary cell wall CesAs only.

      Similarly, regarding the formation of the larger structures from mixtures of different CESA trimers. Why do they not form roseOes? Par;cularly as these appear to be forming 2-dimensional structures.

      We have included additional data on the interaction between different CesA isoform trimers (Figure 6). To answer the reviewer’s ques;on, the most likely reasons for not observing closely packed roseOe-like structures are (a) steric interferences between the micelles harboring the individual CesA trimers, and (b) the lack of a stabilizing cellulose fiber.  This interpretation is supported by 2D class averages of dimers of CesA1 and CesA3 trimers (now shown in Fig. 6). The class averages show an ‘upside-down and side-by-side’ orientation of the two trimers, consistent with interferences between the solubilizing detergent micelles. The implica;ons of this non-physiological arrangement are discussed in the revised manuscript. In a biological membrane, the CesA trimers are confined to the same plane in the same orientation, which is likely necessary to form ordered arrangements.

      What role does the NTD play in trimer formation given its apparent very high class specificity?

      We have no data suggesting any contribution of the NTD to trimer formation. Recent work on moss CesA5 and similar AlphaFold predic;ons suggest that, for some CesAs, an extreme Nterminal region can interact with the beta sheet of the catalytic domain via beta-strand augmentation. Whether this interaction can contribute to CesA-CesA interactions remains unknown.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The authors provide PDB codes but not EMDB codes for the EM maps, also I would encourage the authors to upload the raw micrographs to the EMPIAR database.

      The EMDB codes are shown in Table 1 and data transfer to EMPIAR is ongoing.  

      Page 6 line 144, the statement "All CesA isoforms show greatest catalytic activity at neutral pH" seems to contradict the data in Figure 1e and the subsequent statements. This sentence should be removed.

      The text has been revised to indicate that CesA1 and CesA6 show highest activity under mild alkaline conditions.  

      Page 6, line 150, the authors state "The affinities for substrate binding range from 1.4 mM for CesA1 to 0.6 and 2.4 mM for CesA3 and CesA6, respectively." How were the affinities determined? Is this the affinities or the Michaelis constants? Is it known whether CesAs are rapid equilibrium enzymes? This should be clarified.

      The text now states that we performed Michaelis Menten kine;cs using the ‘UDP-Glo’ glycosyltransferase assay kit. We are uncertain about whether CesAs can be classified as rapid equilibrium enzymes. The rate-limiting step of cellulose biosynthesis has been proposed to be glycosyl transfer, rather than cellulose transloca;on.  To avoid any confusion, we changed the text from '…reveals Michaelis Menten constants for substrate binding of CesA1 and CesA3' to '…reveals Michaelis Menten constants for CesA1 and CesA3 with respect to UDP-Glc'.

      Page 6, line 153, the authors state "CesA1's apparent Ki for UDP is roughly 0.8 mM, whereas this concentration is increased to about 1.2 to 1.5 mM for CesA6 and CesA3, respectively." From the Figure 1g legend, it appears that the authors performed additional experiments at different UDP-Glc concentrations in order to determine Ki that are not shown. This data should be included as a figure supplement as the data presented are insufficient to determine Ki (only IC50).

      The UDP inhibition data show apparent IC50 values, and this has been corrected in the text. For each CesA isoform, the titration was done at one UDP-Glc concentration only.    

      Page 8, line 202, the authors state that TM helix 7 of the primary cell wall CesAs is more flexible "as evidenced by weaker density." The density for the TM helix 7 should be shown. If the density shown in Supplementary Figure 3 corresponds to TM helices the number of the helices should be indicated as it is not immediately obvious from the amino acid residue numbers.

      The densities for TM helix 7 of all CesA isoforms are shown in Supplemental Figure 3. The helices are now labeled to orient the reader.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review)

      The authors demonstrate via truncation that the N-terminus of the CesA is not involved in the interactions between the isoforms and propose that the CSR hook-like extensions are the primary mediator of trimer-trimer interactions. This argument would be strengthened by equivalent truncation experiments in which the CSR region is removed.

      We performed the suggested experiment. We replaced the CSR in N-terminally truncated GmCesA1 and GmCesA3 with a 20-residue long linker. The resulting constructs assemble into homotrimeric complexes as observed for the wild type and only N-terminally truncated versions. However, the CSR-truncated constructs of the different isoforms do not interact with each other in vitro. Further, CSR-deleted GmCesA3 also does not interact with full-length CesA1, suggesting that two CSR domains of different isoforms are necessary for homotrimer interaction. This data is now shown as Fig. 5.  

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Major Points

      (1) The authors state on Line 354 that they were unable to isolate heterotrimers, but they need to provide the data to support this claim; for example, it is important for readers to understand whether co-expression of all three CESAs leads to only homotrimers or only monomers. This information is essential to exclude model C in Figure 6.

      We have revised the corresponding discussion and toned down the statement that heterotrimeric complexes did not form in our recombinant expression system. Co-expression of differently tagged secondary or primary cell wall CesAs in Sf9 cells has consistently resulted in negligible amounts of material that can be purified sequentially over different affinity matrices (corresponding to the tags on the recombinantly expressed CesAs – His, Strep, Flag). While this does not exclude the formation of a small fraction of hetero-oligomeric complexes (which could be trimers as observed in the structures or monomers interacting via their CSR regions), it demonstrates that CesAs favor the same isoform for trimer formation, rather than partnering with other isoforms. An example of such a purification is now shown as Supplemental Figure 8.

      Determining whether heterotrimers are formed upon co-expression of different CesA isoforms requires high resolution structural analysis because co-purification of different isoforms can also be due to interactions between different homo-trimeric complexes, as demonstrated in this study.

      While we cannot exclude that factors exist in planta that may prevent the formation of homotrimers and favor the formation of hetero-trimers, it is important to keep in mind that currently no experimental data supports the formation of hetero-trimeric complexes. Instead, our work demonstrates that existing data on CesA isoform interactions can be explained by the interaction of homotrimers of different isoforms.

      (2) The evidence that the products of GmCEA1, GmCESA3, and GmCESA6 homotrimers are cellulose is that they consume UDP-glucose and produce a beta-glucanase-sensitive product. Other beta-glucans synthesized by similar GT2 family proteins (e.g. CSLDs, Yang et al., 2020 Plant Cell or CSLCs, Kim et al., 2020 PNAS) would be sensitive to this enzyme, and the product cannot truly be called cellulose unless it forms microfibrils. Previous reports of CESA activity in vitro have demonstrated that the products form genuine cellulose microfibrils rather than amorphous beta-glucan (via electron microscopy); extensively documented that the product is sensitive to beta-glucanase, but not other enzymes (e.g., callose or MLG degrading enzymes); provided linkage analysis of the product to conclusively demonstrate that it is a beta1,4-linked glucan; and documented a loss of activity when key catalytic residues were mutated (Purushotham et al., 2016 PNAS; Cho et al., 2017 Plant Phys; Purushotham et al., 2020 Science).

      Other GT2 characterization efforts have documented activity to similar standards (e.g. CSLDs, Yang et al., 2020 Plant Cell or CSLFs, Purushotham et al., 2022 Science Advances). At least one independent method should be provided, and the TEM of the product is necessary for readers to appreciate whether the product forms true cellulose microfibrils.

      There may be some confusion regarding the nomenclature. Therefore, we revised the second sentence of the Introduction to define ‘cellulose’ as a beta-1,4 linked glucose polymer, in accordance with the ‘Essentials of Glycobiology’. This is also consistent with enzyme nomenclature as the primary product of cellulose synthase is a single glucose polymer, and not a fibril. For example, most bacterial cellulose synthases only produce amorphous (single chain) cellulose. 

      We show that the GmCesA products can be degraded with a beta-1,4 specific glucanase (cellulase), which demonstrates the formation of authentic cellulose. This study does not focus on the formation of fibrillar cellulose apart from suggesting a revised model for a microfibrilforming CSC.       

      (3) The position of isoxaben-resistant mutations implies that primary cell wall CESAs form heterotrimers (Shim et al., 2018 Frontiers in Plant Biology). Indeed, in their previous description of the POCESA8 structure (Purushotham et al., 2020 Science), the authors discussed the position of isoxaben-resistant mutations as a way to justify the way that TM7 of one CESA can contribute to forming the cellulose translocation pore in the neighbouring CESA within a heterotrimer. However, in this manuscript, the authors document a different location for TM7 in the GmCEA1, GmCESA3, and GmCESA6 homotrimers, which would change the position of these resistance mutations. Please discuss.

      As stated in the manuscript, we do not know what the functional implication of the TM7 flexibility may be, but we speculate that it could affect the alignment of the synthesized cellulose polymers. Regarding the previously reported POCesA8 structure, the mapping of one of the reported isoxaben resistance mutants to the C-terminus of TM7 was not used to justify the structure; the structure with its position of TM7 stands on its own.  Considering recent observations suggesting that isoxaben may affect cellulose biosynthesis via secondary effects, we prefer not to speculate on the mechanism by which these mutations cause the apparent resistance to isoxaben (PMID: 37823413).

      (4) The authors present no evidence that GmCESA1/3/6 are involved in primary cell wall synthesis. Please include gene expression information (documenting widespread expression consistent with primary CESAs) and rigorous molecular phylogenetic analysis (or references to these published data) to clarify that these are indeed primary cell wall CESAs.

      This has been addressed. We have included additional figures (Fig. 1 and S1B) that show the strong and wide distribution of the selected CesAs in soybean leaves, their co-expression with primary cell wall markers, and their phylogenetic clustering with Arabidopsis primary cell wall CesAs.  

      (5) Several small changes need to be made to the abstract to ensure that it aligns with the data: Line 28: add "in vitro" arer "their assembly into homotrimeric complexes" Line 28: change "stabilized by the PCR" to "presumably stabilized by the PCR".

      We inserted ‘in vitro’ as requested. We did not insert the second modification as requested since CesA trimers are stabilized by the PCR. This is a fact arising from several experimentally determined CesA trimer structures.  

      (6) In all graphs in all figures it is unclear what the sample size is and what the bars represent. These must be stated in the figure legends. It is best practice to plot individual data points so that readers can easily interpret both the sample size and the variation.

      The sample sizes and error bars are now defined in the relevant figure legends.

      (7) The methods need to unambiguously define GmCESA1, GmCESA3, GmCESA6 protein identities using appropriate accession numbers.

      The accession codes are now provided in the Methods.

      Minor Points

      (1) Does CESA1 have higher activity in Figure 1D because of the pH at which the assay was conducted (see Figure 1E)? Could this difference in activity or pH preference have also affected their capacity to resolve TM7 of CESA1?

      We consistently observe higher in vitro catalytic activity of CesA1, compared to CesA3 and CesA6. Activity assays are performed at a pH of 7.5, roughly halfway between the activity maxima of CesA3 and CesA1/6. At this pH, we expect activity differences to arise from factors other than the buffer pH. As detailed above, we do not know whether the conformational flexibility of TM helix 7 affects catalytic activity.

      (2) Line 55: The authors should cite additional papers that also provide insight into CESA structure (e.g. Qiao et al 2021 PNAS).

      A recent publication on moss CesA5 has been included. Qiao et al unfortunately report on a dimeric assembly of a fragment of Arabidopsis thaliana’s CesA3 catalytic domain, which we consider non-physiological. We added a brief statement in the Discussion explaining that our GmCesA3 structure is inconsistent with the dimeric arrangement reported by Qiao et al.

      (3) Line 95: these references are about secondary cell wall CESA isoforms, but there are more appropriate references for the primary CESAs that should be included in place of these papers.

      Fagard et al report on growth defects in roots and dark-grown hypocotyls linked to Arabidopsis CesA 1 and CesA6, which are primary cell wall CesAs. Nevertheless, we have included two additional recent publications from the Meyerowitz and Persson labs.

      (4) Line 121-122: Please cite a specific figure that supports this claim, since the (Purushotham et al., 2020) reference refers to POCESA8 enrichment results, but the claims are about the GmCESA1/3/6 enrichment.

      The POCesA8 reference has been removed. The classification into monomers and trimers arises from the data processing described in this manuscript and is consistent with similar results obtained for POCesA8.

      (5) Line 314: It is more appropriate to use "enzyme activity" rather than "cellulose synthesis".

      We prefer to use cellulose biosynthesis since the enzyme produces cellulose.

      (6) Figure 1: please add colour to the graphs to clarify which trend lines belong to which data series (especially Figure 1G).

      The figure (now Fig. 2) has been revised as suggested.  

      (7) Figure 2D: It's not clear which parts are GmCESA and which are POCESA8; please clarify the figure legend.

      Thank you, the legend has been revised accordingly (now Fig. 3).

      (8) In Figure 5, It's not clear that the one CESA is maintained at a steady concentration throughout the assay since there is only a bar for that CESA at the highest concentration (e.g. in Figure 5A, the blue bar for CESA1 only appears on the right-most assay, but there was CESA1 in all assays, so this should be indicated).

      In the panel the reviewer is referring to, the blue bar corresponds to the activity measured for only CesA1 at a concentration of 20 µM. The red columns (indicated as ‘Mix’) represent the activities measured in the presence of 20 µM of CesA1 plus increasing concentrations of CesA3. The purple columns represent activities obtained for only CesA3 at the indicated concentrations. Numerical addition of the activities of CesA1 alone at 20 µM (blue column) and CesA 3 alone (purple columns) gives rise to the gray columns, now indicated by a capital ‘sigma’ sign. We are unclear on how the figure could be improved, but we have revised the legend to avoid confusion.    

      (9) Figure 5 legend needs to be clarified to indicate whether monomers or homotrimers were used in the assays.

      This is now shown as Fig. 7 and the legend has been revised as requested. The experiments were performed with the trimeric CesA fractions.

      (10) There seem to be some random dots near the top of Figures 6B & 6C

      Removed. Thank you.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Using lineage tracing and single-cell RNA sequencing, Li et al. reported brain ECs can differentiate into pericytes after stroke. This finding is novel and important to the field.

      Strengths:

      Detailed characterization of each time point and genetic manipulation of genes for study role of ECs and E-pericyte.

      Weaknesses:

      Genetic evidence for lineage tracing of ECs and E-pericytes requires more convincing data that includes staining, FACS, and scRNA-seq analysis.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s recommendation to explore more convincing data, including staining, FACS, and scRNA-seq analysis. We initially employed traditional lineage tracing methods to demonstrate that endothelial cells can transform into pericytes after stroke. We utilized Cdh5CreERT2;Ai47 mice, Tie2-Dre;Mfsd2aCreER;Ai47 mice, and AAV-BI30 virus-infected Ai47 mice. However, in our validation of the transformed cells as pericytes, there are limitations to our results. While three pericyte markers (CD13, NG2, and PDGFRβ) were used in Cdh5CreERT2;Ai47 mice, only one marker (CD13) was applied in Tie2Dre; Mfsd2aCreER;Ai47 and AAV-BI30 virus-infected Ai47 mice. This is insufficient, and the other two pericyte markers (NG2 and PDGFRβ) need to be verified in these models.

      At scRNA-seq, although we observed an increased proportion of pericyte/EGFP<sup>+</sup> cells after stroke, we did not rule out potential contamination by pericyte cells, nor did we include sufficient replicates. To address these issues, we can explore additional methods for analyzing scRNA-seq data, increasing sample replicates, and eliminating pericyte contamination using advanced algorithms. Furthermore, we can use chimeric-related mutations to compare normal endothelial cells, normal pericytes, endothelial-derived pericytes (E-pericytes), and intermediate fibroblast-like cells at the DNA level. This approach will help identify and trace chimeric-related mutations across different cell types and developmental stages. Finally, we can track the entire process of endothelial cell transformation into pericytes using two-photon imaging in vivo.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Li and colleagues study the fate of endothelial cells in a mouse model of ischemic stroke. Using genetic lineage tracing approaches, they found that endothelial cells give rise to non-endothelial cells, which they term "E-pericytes." They further show that depleting these cells exacerbates blood-brain barrier leakage and worsens functional recovery. The authors also provide evidence that endothelial-to-mesenchymal transition, myeloid cell-derived TGFβ1, and endothelial TGFβRII are involved in this process. These are potentially interesting findings, however, the experimental evidence that endothelial cells undergo transdifferentiation to non-endothelial cells is weak, as is the evidence that these cells are pericytes. Addressing this foundational weakness will facilitate the interpretation of the other findings.

      Strengths:

      (1) The authors address an important question about blood vessel function and plasticity in the context of stroke.

      (2) The authors use a variety of genetic approaches to understand cell fate in the context of stroke. Particularly commendable is the use of several complementary lineage tracing strategies, including an intersectional strategy requiring both endothelial Cre activity and subsequent mural cell NG2 promoter activity.

      (3) The authors address upstream cellular and molecular mechanisms, including roles for myeloid-derived TGFβ.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The authors use Cdh5-CreERT2; Ai47 mice to permanently label endothelial cells and their progeny with eGFP. They then isolate eGFP<sup>+</sup> cells from control and MCAO RP7D and RP34D brains, and use single-cell RNA-seq to identify the resulting cell types. Theoretically, all eGFP<sup>+</sup> cells should be endothelial cells or their progeny. This is a very powerful and well-conceived experiment. The authors use the presence of a pericyte cluster as evidence that endothelial-to-pericyte transdifferentiation occurs. However, pericytes are also present in the scRNA-seq data from sham mice, as are several other cell types such as fibroblasts and microglia. This suggests that pericytes and these other cell types might have been co-purified (e.g., as doublets) with eGFP<sup>+</sup> endothelial cells during FACS and may not themselves be eGFP<sup>+</sup>. Pericyte-endothelial doublets are common in scRNA-seq given that these cell types are closely and tightly associated. Additionally, tight association (e.g., via peg-socket junctions) can cause fragments of endothelial cells to be retained on pericytes (and vice-versa) during dissociation. Finally, it is possible that after stroke or during the dissociation process, endothelial cells lyse and release eGFP that could be taken up by other cell types. All of these scenarios could lead to the purification of cells that were not derived (transdifferentiated) from endothelial cells. The authors note that the proportion of pericytes increased in the stroke groups, but it does not appear this experiment was replicated and thus this conclusion is not supported by statistical analysis. The results of pseudotime and trajectory analyses rely on the foundation that the pericytes in this dataset are endothelial-derived, which, as discussed above, has not been rigorously demonstrated.

      Thank you for your thoughtful comment.

      Indeed, we face the challenge of obtaining pure cells. As the reviewer has pointed out, several factors may contribute to cell contamination. For instance, the meninges of adult mice are difficult to remove completely, which may lead to fibroblast contamination. Although Cdh5CreERT2 can specifically label endothelial cells in the normal brain parenchyma, there may still be very few unspecific cells in certain brain regions, such as the choroid plexus and periventricular areas, resulting in the presence of ependymal cells. To address these issues, we can improve our methodology by carefully removing the meninges, choroid plexus, and periventricular cells during sample preparation. Additionally, we need to increase the N of the transcriptome samples to enhance the reliability of our data.

      (2) I have the same concern regarding the inadvertent purification of cells that were not derived from endothelial cells in the context of the bulk RNA-seq experiment (Figure S4), especially given the sample-to-sample variability in gene expression in the RP34D, eGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs-group (e.g., only 2/5 samples are enriched for mesenchymal transcription factor Tbx18, only 1/5 samples are enriched for mural cell TF Heyl). If the sorted eGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs were pericytes, I would expect a strong and consistent pericyte-like gene expression profile.

      This is an interesting question.

      Indeed, significant differences were observed in the expression of pericyte-related transcriptional profiles within the eGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs group. For instance, transcription factors such as Hic1 and Fosl1 were nearly absent in the eGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs group. We propose several potential explanations for these observations:

      (1) The sorted eGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs group may contain other cell types, leading to contamination.

      (2) The eGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs group may not uniformly express all pericyte-related transcriptional profiles.

      (3) The temporal dynamics of transcription factor expression (i.e., different factors being expressed at different stages) could contribute to the observed variability.

      (4) The heterogeneity in the timing of endothelial-to-pericyte transformation (i.e., some cells have already transformed into pericytes while others are in the process of transformation at the early stage) may result in significant differences in transcriptional profiles.

      (3) The authors use immunohistochemistry to understand localization, morphology, and marker expression of eGFP<sup>+</sup> cells in situ. The representative "E-pericytes" shown in Figure 3A-D are not associated with blood vessels, and the authors' quantification also shows that the majority of such cells are not vessel-associated ("avascular"). By definition, pericytes are a component of blood vessels and are embedded within the vascular basement membrane. Thus, concluding that these cells are pericytes ("E-pericytes") may be erroneous.

      Yes, we found that 72.2% of E-pericytes were free and not associated with blood vessels. Normally, pericytes surround blood vessels and connect to endothelial cells. However, in certain diseases, such as Alzheimer's disease, stroke, and diabetic encephalopathy, pericytes can detach from blood vessels. In our stroke model, we observed that pericytes detach from blood vessels. This phenomenon can be explained by two possible scenarios:

      (1) After endothelial cells transform into E-pericytes, the E-pericytes detach from blood vessels due to the pathological environment following stroke.

      (2) After stroke, blood vessel function is impaired, leading to vascular degeneration. Endothelial cells shed from the blood vessels and subsequently transform into E-pericytes.

      Therefore, preventing pericyte detachment from blood vessels after stroke represents an important scientific challenge.

      (4) CD13 flow cytometry and immunohistochemistry are used extensively to identify pericytes. In the context of several complementary lineage tracing strategies noted in Strength #2, CD13 immunohistochemistry is the only marker used to identify putative pericytes (Figure S3J-M). In stroke, CD13 is not specific to pericytes; dendritic cells and other monocyte-derived cells express CD13 (Anpep) in mouse brain after stroke (PMID: 38177281, https://anratherlab.shinyapps.io/strokevis/).

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable input. In the context of stroke, CD13 is not specific to pericytes. Additionally, pericytes lack a single specific marker; instead, their identity is determined by a combination of multiple markers. To more convincingly validate the identity of pericytes, it is necessary to incorporate additional pericyte markers alongside several complementary lineage tracing strategies.

      (5) The authors conclude that "EC-specific overexpression of the Tgfbr2 protein by a virus (Tgfbr2) decreases Evans blue leakage, promotes CBF recovery, alleviates neurological deficits and facilitates spontaneous behavioral recovery after stroke by increasing the number of E-pericytes." All data in Figure 10, however, compare endothelial Tgfbr2 overexpression to a DsRed overexpression control. There is no group in which Tgfbr2 is overexpressed but "E-pericytes" are eliminated with DTA (this is done in Figure 9B, but this experiment lacks the Tgfbr2 overexpression-only control). Thus, the observed functional outcomes cannot be ascribed to "E-pericytes"; it remains possible that endothelial Tgfbr2 overexpression affects EB leakage, CBF, and behavior through alternative mechanisms.

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable comment. Although in Figures 9A-B, we observed no significant difference in Evans blue leakage between the Tgfbr2 overexpression group and the Tgfbr2 overexpression + DTA group (P=0.8153), this suggests that the impact of Tgfbr2 overexpression on the blood-brain barrier (BBB) is primarily attributed from the E-pericytes generated by Tgfbr2 expression. Furthermore, in Figure 10A, the inclusion of the Tgfbr2 overexpression + DTA group would provide stronger evidence that the effects of Tgfbr2 overexpression on the BBB and neurobehavioral outcomes are mainly due to the E-pericytes derived from Tgfbr2 expression.

      (6) Single-cell and bulk RNA-seq data are not available in a public repository (such as GEO). Depositing these data would facilitate their independent reevaluation and reuse.

      Thank you for the suggestion and we have uploaded Single-cell and bulk RNA-seq data (The assignment of GEO number is pending).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The data and experiments presented in that study convincingly show that a subpopulation of endothelial cells undergo transformation into pericyte-like cells after stroke in mice. These so-called "E-pericytes" are protective and might present a new target for stroke recovery. The authors used a huge battery of different techniques and modified signaling pathways and cellular interactions using several genetic and pharmacological tools to show that TGFbeta and EndoMT are causes of this transformation.

      Strengths:

      The amount of different genetic and pharmacological approaches in combination with sophisticated techniques such as single-cell RNAseq is impressive and convincing. The results support their conclusions and the authors achieved their aims. The findings will strongly impact the field of cerebrovascular recovery after stroke and might open up new therapeutic targets.

      Weaknesses:

      The written and graphic presentation of the findings needs substantial improvement. Language editing is strongly recommended (there are a lot of spelling and grammatical errors in the text and illustrations, including legends).

      Thank you for raising this important point and we will place greater emphasis on the written and graphic presentation of the findings.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In this study, Li et al. reported that endothelial cells in the brain can differentiate into pericytes to promote the restoration of blood-brain barrier (BBB) function after stroke. Understanding the mechanisms underlying BBB restoration post-stroke is crucial to the field. Using lineage tracing, RNA sequencing (RNA-seq), and immunostaining, Li et al. detected the transdifferentiation of endothelial cells (ECs) into E-pericytes in the middle cerebral artery occlusion (MCAO) model. The specific knockout of Tgfbr2 in ECs reduced the number of E-pericytes, exacerbated BBB leakage, and worsened neurological deficits. This observation of EC to pericyte differentiation is novel; however, the conclusions at this stage are not fully supported by the evidence provided.

      (1) The authors claimed, based on the EdU assay, that 12.9% of pericytes present at RP34D originated from self-proliferation, while the origin of the remaining 27.6% of new pericytes remains unclear. This raises concerns, as the EdU assay is not 100% efficient in detecting all proliferating cells. If EdU<sup>+</sup> ECs account for fewer than 10% of all ECs, it follows that other EdU-ECs must have alternative origins.

      That is an interesting question. To address this issue, we need to consider the following aspects:

      (1) The EdU assay is not 100% efficient in detecting all proliferating cells, which means that the actual proportion of proliferating pericytes may be higher than 12.9%, while the proportion of pericytes from other sources may be lower than 27.6% (as determined by FACS). This is consistent with the observation in Figure 3H (immunofluorescence analysis), where EGFP<sup>+</sup> pericytes accounted for only 24.5% of all pericytes.

      (2) The dose of EdU administered in our study was relatively high (200 mg/kg, intraperitoneal injection, daily), which may increase the efficiency of EdU labeling.

      (3) When EdU<sup>+</sup> endothelial cells (ECs) constitute less than 10% of all ECs, it does suggest that EdU-ECs could be a source of pericytes. However, at least EdU<sup>+</sup> ECs cannot transform into pericytes, as we did not detect any EdU<sup>+</sup>EGFP<sup>+</sup> pericytes.

      (2) The reference for Cdh5CreERT2 is cited as 25, which is a review article published in ATVB. This review lists many different drivers, and the specific Cdh5CreERT2 line used in this study is not identified. This specificity is critical for accurate lineage tracing of ECs.

      Although the review I mentioned did not address this, the specificity of Cdh5CreERT2 in the brain has been demonstrated in other studies (Boyé K, et al. Nat Commun. 2022 Mar 4;13(1):1169; Patel A, et al. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2024 Dec 3;121(49):e2322124121). We have further confirmed that Cdh5CreERT2 specifically labels endothelial cells in the brain parenchyma (Figure S1). Additionally, we found nonspecific labeling in the blood (less than 1% CD45+ blood cells, primarily myeloid cells) and meninges outside the brain parenchyma. We ruled out nonspecific transdifferentiation labeling in the blood through bone marrow reconstitution experiments and in the meninges using in vivo two-photon imaging (results not shown).

      (3) The scRNA-seq data should include GFP signals to track the increasing number of pericytes from early to late stages post-injury. This is the only independent method from staining to verify that the pericytes are indeed derived from GFP<sup>+</sup> ECs after brain injury. Sham samples should be utilized as strict side-by-side controls.

      This is a valuable suggestion. We observed that, despite being positive for EGFP protein, only 50% of the sorted cells expressed the EGFP gene at the transcriptome level. This phenomenon has also been reported in other studies (Rodor J,et al a. Cardiovasc Res. 2022 Aug 24;118(11):2519-2534.). For these reasons, we did not rely on GFP signals to track the increase in pericyte numbers from early to late stages post-injury.

      (4) Since Ai47 is employed, there are three different variants of green fluorescent proteins, including ZsGreen, which may result in signals being spotted in the staining. The GFP signal detected could also represent dead cells that have lost CD31 expression.

      The detected GFP signal could also originate from dead cells that have lost CD31 expression, which is a plausible explanation. As shown in Figure 3I, EGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs peak at RP14D and then decline, suggesting that some EGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs either die or revert to endothelial cells (ECs). Therefore, it cannot be ruled out that we captured some dead EGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs; however, as indicated in Figure 3I, this proportion is likely less than 25%. Additionally, pericytes are prone to death in ischemic and hypoxic environments (Figure 1A), which explains why some of the transformed EGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs may die. Nevertheless, at RP514D, we can still detect EGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs, indicating that a subset of these cells can survive for an extended period (Figure S3F).

      (5) The quality of the staining images is not convincing, as some non-ECs and ECs are in close proximity, leading to potential artifacts in signal interpretation. The reviewer cannot rely solely on single staining techniques to be convinced of EC differentiation into pericytes. Although it has been reported that ECs can differentiate into pericytes during development, this phenomenon in the adult brain is surprising; thus, more rigorous evidence with strong lineage tracing data should be provided through multiple measurements.

      Why some non-ECs and ECs are located nearby:

      (1) Non-ECs exhibit characteristics of pericytes, which are typically adjacent to ECs.

      (2) Could this proximity lead to potential artifacts in signal interpretation? We believe this is unlikely, as we also observed a significant number of non-ECs located far from ECs on blood vessels (Figure 3A-B, Figure S3M).

      (3) Three pericyte markers (CD13, NG2, and PDGFRβ) were also used to verify the transformed cells, while the three pericyte markers were not expressed in normal endothelial cells.

      (6) FACS (Fluorescence-activated cell sorting) should be employed to quantitatively assess the contribution of GFP<sup>+</sup> ECs to pericytes at each stage after injury, compared to sham controls.

      Yes, if the contribution of GFP<sup>+</sup> ECs to pericytes could be assessed at each time point, the role of E-pericytes in the pericyte pool could be better explained, and the proportion of E-pericytes would become more prominent. In Figure 3, we did not use FACS to evaluate the contribution of GFP<sup>+</sup> ECs to pericytes at each stage post-injury. Instead, we only assessed the ratio of EGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs to all EGFP<sup>+</sup> cells. However, we did verify the contribution of GFP<sup>+</sup> ECs (E-pericytes) to pericytes at RP34D using FACS (CD13+ DsRed/CD13 = 25.6%, Figure 4C). This ratio is consistent with the immunofluorescence data (Figure 3H).

      (7) In Tie2Dre;Mfsd2aCrexER;Ai47 mice, ECs in the brain are specifically labeled, indicating that ECs could give rise to CD13+ EGFP<sup>+</sup> non-ECs at RP34D (Figure S3L). However, the GFP signal for Ai47 is not homogeneous, displaying many spotted patterns. Using tdTomato as an alternative for detection could enhance clarity.

      We repeated the experiment using tdTomato as the reporter gene in mice and observed results consistent with those obtained using Ai47 as the reporter gene. For consistency, all results presented are based on Ai47. Regarding the spotted patterns observed with Ai47, this phenomenon can be attributed to the relatively low laser intensity (2%). Higher laser intensity would cause overexposure of EGFP<sup>+</sup> ECs. To address the issue of spotted patterns in Ai47 imaging, we can improve the visualization of complete cell morphology (as shown in Figure S3M) by increasing the gain value, which enhances the background signal.

      (8) The data concerning the genetic ablation of pericytes lacks specificity. There is insufficient evidence to support that DTA is specifically expressed in E-pericytes. The authors should utilize DTR (Diphtheria Toxin Receptor) and confirm that DTR expression is restricted to pericytes derived from GFP<sup>+</sup> ECs. Treatment with diphtheria toxin, but not PBS as a control, should specifically ablate these E-pericytes without affecting any other GFP-pericytes in the brain following injury.

      We did not verify that DTA expression was restricted to E-pericytes. To ensure that DTA is only expressed in converted E-pericytes, we employed two strategies:

      (1) Specific Targeting of Endothelial Cells: We used the AAV-BI30 virus to specifically infect endothelial cells. Although not 100% exclusive, 98.5% of the expression occurred in endothelial cells, with minimal infection in neurons and microglia. Additionally, we combined this with Cdh5CreERT2 to control the DIO action in the virus. This means that only endothelial cells expressing both Cdh5CreERT2 and infected with AAV-BI30 could undergo cell fate changes and transform into pericytes, subsequently expressing markers such as NG2 and driving DTA expression in E-pericytes (Figure 4A).

      (2) Validation of DTA Expression: To prevent off-target expression of DTA in other cell types, we plan to verify DTA protein expression using specific antibodies to confirm whether DTA is expressed in unintended cells. Alternatively, as suggested, we could utilize the Diphtheria Toxin Receptor (DTR) system. By ensuring that DTR expression is restricted to pericytes derived from GFP<sup>+</sup> ECs, treatment with diphtheria toxin would specifically ablate these E-pericytes without affecting other GFP- pericytes in the brain post-injury.

      (9) There is currently no convincing genetic data demonstrating that Tgfb signaling overexpression or deletion modulates the transdifferentiation of ECs to pericytes.

      Yes, this is an important consideration. Although we knocked out the TGFβ receptor in endothelial cells (ECs) and observed a reduction in the formation of E-pericytes (Figure 6D and 6G), it would be more informative to specifically knockout the Tgfb gene in myeloid cells or monocyte-macrophage lineages to determine whether these cells are the primary source of TGFβ driving endothelial cell transformation. Additionally, injecting TGFβ protein directly into the brains of mice could help explore whether exogenous TGFβ promotes the formation of E-pericytes.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figure 1D, there does not appear to be a clear PDGFRβ-positive population. In this case, it is necessary to include the negative control that served as the basis for drawing the positive gate.

      Author response image 1 below show the negative control for CD31 and PDGFRβ.

      Author response image 1.

      (2) Figures 3A-D, Figures S3J-M, the authors statistically compare % negative to % positive. It appears % negative = 100% - % positive. If this is the case, these groups are not independent and should not be statistically compared.

      This is a very important point, and such a comparison is not appropriate. The statistical comparison mentioned above has now been removed.

      (3) Figure 4B, in addition to the cells indicated with arrows, there is a substantial additional DsRed+ signal of similar intensity in this image. It would be helpful to show a negative control.

      Author response image 2 below show the contralateral and ipsilateral, respectively. In the contralateral, DsRed has few signals, no complete cell morphology, and is separated from the Hoechst+ nucleus. in the ipsilateral, DsRed signals are strong, have intact cell morphology, and are tightly bound to the Hoechst+ nucleus. In the ipsilateral, some DsRed signals may come from dying cells.

      Author response image 2.

      (4) Figure 6G, the y-axis title is "E-pericytes/all EGFP<sup>+</sup> cells (%)" but the y-axis scale goes from 0 to 900. Is this an error?

      Thank you. We want to calculate the number of pericytes per unit area, it should be E-pericyte/mm2.

      (5) Figure 9B, in the representative images, the 6th group is labeled "Tgfb2 + DTA" but in the plot below, the 6th group is labeled Tgfbr2 + DsRed. Which is correct?

      Thank you. The "Tgfb2 + DTA" is right. We have changed it to "Tgfb2 + DTA" in the 6th group, Figure 9B.

      (6) Figure S1I, error bars and/or individual data points should be shown.

      The purpose of this diagram is to demonstrate the number of mice in which EGFP<sup>+</sup> cells are 100% co-labeled with endothelial markers (CD31, ERG, GLUT1, and VE-Cadherin), as EGFP<sup>+</sup> cells are exclusively found in endothelial cells within the brain parenchyma. Additionally, the diagram illustrates the number of mice in which EGFP<sup>+</sup> cells show no co-labeling (0%) with mural cell markers (CD13, PDGFRβ, α-SMA, and NG2), as EGFP<sup>+</sup> cells are not present in mural cells within the brain parenchyma.

      (7) The authors write: "When Tgfbr2 was overexpressed and DTA was expressed specifically in the same ECs, DTA prevented the EC-specific overexpression of the Tgfbr2 gene and increased the proportion of E-pericytes.". The authors' strategy for DTA expression involves the NG2 promoter, which, in principle, is not active in ECs. Thus how can DTA be "expressed specifically in the same ECs" and how can DTA "prevent EC-specific overexpression" of Tgfbr2?

      Our purpose is not clearly expressed. The statement should be revised to: “When Tgfbr2 was overexpressed to increase E-pericytes and DTA was expressed in transformed cells to deplete E-pericytes, we found that there was no significant change in the number of E-pericytes in the Tgfbr2 + DTA group compared with the DTA group.”

      (8) The interpretation of Evans blue leakage as "low molecular weight" leakage should be revised since Evans blue binds serum albumin and thus it is the molecular weight of this complex (~67 kDa) that is relevant.

      We agree with the reviewer. Yes, it should not be stated that Evans blue is low molecular weight, as it binds to serum albumin to form complexes. The text has been revised to: “Interestingly, no obvious leakage of dextran-rhodamine B (~70 kDa) (Figure S8C) or Texas Red (~71 kDa) was detected (Figure S8D). However, the elimination of E-pericytes allowed evans blue and trypan blue to cross the blood-brain barrier (BBB).”

      (9) It is critical that the sequencing data be made available through a public repository (such as GEO).

      Thank you. Now we've uploaded it to GEO.

      (10) It would be extremely helpful if the authors would make their viral plasmids available through a public repository (such as Addgene).

      Thank you. Now we've uploaded it to Addgene (The assignment of Addgene number is pending).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The distribution and expression of pericytic and fibroblast markers at different time points after stroke is confusing while reading the manuscript, e.g., vimentin is not expressed on day 34 but on day 8, whereas CD13 is expressed on day 34 but not on day 8, if I understood the text correctly. To make it easier to follow, the authors could add a label of the day after stroke to each of the subfigures which show images and co-expression of different markers (e.g. Figures 3 and S3).

      Below are the expressions of different specific markers in each cell.

      “√” stand for positive, “×” stand for positive

      Author response table 1.

      (2) The authors need to check the N numbers again, e.g., Figure S3L: 4 dots per group are shown in the graph but an N of 3 is mentioned in the legend.

      Thank you for raising this important point. N=4 has been corrected in the legend of Figure 3S. We also checked other N numbers.

      (3) Labelling of graphs should be consistent (e.g., S4C: "I-ECs" vs. S4F: "Ipsi-ECs") and correct (e.g., "DsRed" instead of "DeRed" in Figure 4B).

      Yes, we need a uniform name with "Ipsi-ECs" and "DsRed". Thank you.

      (4) Figure 4: In the text, the injection is described to be done on day 34 whereas in Figure 4A the injections are described to take place before MCAO, please clarify. Does day 34 mean 34 days after injection or after MCAO (as in the former experiments)?

      In the text, the sentence, “Then we used AAV2/9-BI30-NG2 promoter-DIO-DTA (DTA) to deplete E-pericytes at RP34D (Figure 4D),” could be misinterpreted as suggesting that the virus was injected at RP34D. To avoid confusion, it has been revised to: “We used the AAV2/9-BI30-NG2 promoter-DIO-DTA (DTA) virus, which was injected before MCAO (Figure 4A), to deplete E-pericytes (Figure 4D).” Yes, day 34 means 34 days after injection or after MCAO and we unify to 34 days.

      (5) Some images are too dark to recognize clear structures and prove the findings (e.g., Figure S6B).

      Thank you for raising this important point.

      (6) There is no Figure S8D (as mentioned in the text).

      Thank you for raising this important point. This problem has been corrected.

      (7) Figure S9: the text only states, that Tgfbr2 overexpression increases CBF recovery and effective perfusion. Also with the legend, it is not clear what was done and measured, especially in Figure S9B - what do the graphs show? Also, the y-axis labeling is missing for the traces.

      In Figure S9A, we assessed changes in blood flow using laser speckle imaging. Laser speckle imaging relies on random interference patterns formed by scattered light when a laser strikes tissue. Moving red blood cells alter the contrast of the speckle pattern: faster blood flow results in quicker speckle changes and lower contrast, while slower blood flow leads to slower speckle changes and higher contrast. By analyzing these changes in speckle contrast, blood flow dynamics can be evaluated in real-time and non-invasively.

      In Figure S9B, we measured blood flow changes using Laser Doppler flowmetry. When a laser interacts with flowing blood, the moving red blood cells scatter the light, causing a frequency shift (Doppler shift). Faster blood flow results in a greater frequency shift, while slower blood flow leads to a smaller frequency shift. By detecting the frequency shift of the scattered light, blood flow velocity and changes can be measured in real time and non-invasively. In Laser Doppler Flowmetry (LDF), the unit of the vertical axis is typically Perfusion Units (PU). PU is a relative unit used to represent changes in blood flow rather than absolute blood flow velocity. These methods have now been further explained in the diagram.

      (8) Which regions of the brain were used to take images (e.g., to count neurons)?

      We captured images and quantified neurons in the cortex and striatum of the brain. Our statistical analysis further demonstrated that, at RP34D, the presence of E-pericytes in the brain does not exhibit region-specificity. Instead, the formation of E-pericytes is driven by TGFβ1, which is regulated by immune cells. Ultimately, the distribution and activity of these immune cells are influenced by the severity of ischemia and hypoxia.

      (9) The sentence "Protein C receptor-expressing (Procr+) ECs could give rise to de novo formation of ECs and pericytes in the mammary gland13." is repeated almost identically in three different places in the text. However, whether Procr+ cells are involved in the described transdifferentiation or whether "E-pericytes" do express the protein C receptor is not shown and needs additional investigation.

      The reason for referencing this literature is to highlight that endothelial cells (ECs) during breast development can give rise to pericytes, which serves as background knowledge supporting our research. To further explore this phenomenon in brain, we used ProcrCreERT2;Ai47 mice subjected to MCAO (middle cerebral artery occlusion) to investigate whether Procr+ ECs could transform into pericytes, similar to what occurs in mammary glands. However, since ProcrCreERT2 labels not only ECs but also pericytes in the brain, the results did not achieve our goal and were therefore not included in the study.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      We appreciate the reviewers' thoughtful comments and suggestions. Below, we provide point-by-point responses to the recommendations and outline the updates made to the manuscript.

      (1) Discussion, "the obvious experiment is to manipulate a neuron's anatomical embedding while leaving stimulus information intact."] The epiphenomenon can arise from the placement and types of a neuron's neurotransmitters and neuromodulators, too.

      The content of vesicles released by a neuron is obviously of great importance in determining postsynaptic impact. However, we’re suggesting that (assuming vesicular content is held constant) the anatomically-relevant patterning of spiking might additionally affect the postsynaptic neuron’s integration of the presynaptic input. To avoid confusion, we updated the text accordingly: “the obvious experiment is to manipulate a neuron's anatomical embedding while minimally impacting external and internal variables, such as stimulus information and levels of neurotransmitters or neuromodulators” (Line 594 - 596).

      (2) “In all conditions, the slope of the input duration versus sensitivity line was still positive at 1,800 seconds (Fig. 3B)". This may suggest that the estimate of the calculated statistics (ISI, PSTH) is more reliable with more data, rather than (or in addition to) specific information being extracted from faraway time points. Another potential confound is the training statistics were calculated from all training data, so the test data is a better match to training data when test statistics are calculated from more data. Overall, the validity of the conclusions following this observation is not clear to me.

      This is a great point. Accordingly, we revised the text to include this possibility: “Because the training data were of similar duration, this could be explained by either of two possibilities. First, the signal is relatively short, but noisy—in this case, extended sampling will increase reliability. Second, the anatomical signal is, itself, distributed over time scales of tens to hundreds of seconds.” (Line 252 - 255).

      (3) "This further suggests that there is a latent neural code for anatomical location embedded within the spike train, a feature that could be practically applied to determining the brain region of a recording electrode without the need for post-hoc histology". The performance of the model at the subregion level, which is a typical level of desired precision in locating cells, does not seem to support such a practical application. Please clarify to avoid confusion.

      The current model should not be considered a replacement for traditional methods, such as histology. Our intention is to convey that, with the inclusion of multimodal data and additional samples, a computational approach to anatomical localization has great promise. We updated the manuscript to clarify this point: “While significantly above chance, the structure-level model still lacks the accuracy for immediate practical application. However, it is highly likely that the incorporation of datasets with diverse multi-modal features and alternative regions from other research groups will increase the accuracy of such a model. In addition, a computational approach can be combined with other methods of anatomical reconstruction.” (Line 355 - 359).

      Additionally, we directly addressed this point in our original manuscript (Discussion section: Line 498 - 505 in the current version). Furthermore, following the release of our preprint, independent efforts have adopted a multimodal strategy with qualitatively similar results (Yu et al., 2024). Other recent work expands on the idea of utilizing single-neuron features for brain region/structure characterization (La Merre et al., 2024).

      Yu, H., Lyu, H., Xu, E. Y., Windolf, C., Lee, E. K., Yang, F., ... & Hurwitz, C. (2024). In vivo cell-type and brain region classification via multimodal contrastive learning. bioRxiv, 2024-11.

      Le Merre, P., Heining, K., Slashcheva, M., Jung, F., Moysiadou, E., Guyon, N., ... & Carlén, M. (2024). A Prefrontal Cortex Map based on Single Neuron Activity. bioRxiv, 2024-11.

      (4) "These results support the notion the meaningful computational division in murine visuocortical regions is at the level of VISp versus secondary areas.". The use of the word "meaningful" is vague and this conclusion is not well justified because it is possible that subregions serve different functional roles without having different spiking statistics.

      Precisely! It is well established that different subregions serve different functional purposes - but they do not necessitate different regional embeddings. It is important to note the difference between stimulus encoding and the embedding that we are describing. As a rough analogy, the regional embedding might be considered a language, while the stimulus is the content of the spoken words. However, to avoid vague words, we revised the sentence to “These results suggest that the computational differentiability of murine visuocortical regions is at the level of VISp versus secondary areas.” (Line 380 - 381)

      (5) Figure 3D left/right halves look similar. A measure of the effect size needs to accompany these p-values.

      We assume the reviewer is referring to Figure 3E. Although some of the violin plots in Figure 3E look similar, they are not identical. In the revision, we include effect sizes in the caption.

      (6) Figure 3A, 3F: Could uncertainty estimates be provided?

      Yes. We added uncertainty estimates to the text (Line 272 - 294) and to the caption of Figure S2, which displays confusion matrices corresponding to Figure 3A. The inclusion of similar estimates for 3F would be so unwieldy as to be a disservice to the reader—there are 240 unique combinations of stimulus parameters and structures. In the context of the larger figure, 3F serves to illustrate a relationship between stimulus, region, and the anatomical embedding.

      (7) Page 21. "semi-orthogonal". Please reword or explain if this usage is technical.

      We replaced “semi-orthogonal” with “dissociable” (Line 549).

      (8) Page 11, "This approach tested whether..."] Unclear sentence. Please reword.

      We changed “This approach tested whether the MLP’s performance depended on viewing the entire ISI distribution or was enriched in a subset of patterns” to “This approach identified regions of the ISI distribution informative for classification” (Line 261).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments and summary of the results. We agree that the introductory results (Figs. 1-3) are not particularly compelling when considered in isolation. They provide a baseline of comparison for the subsequent results. Our intention was to approach the problem systematically, progressing from well-established, basic methods to more advanced approaches. This allows us to clearly test a baseline and avoid analytical leaps or untested assumptions. Specifically:

      ● Figure 1 provides an evaluation of the standard dimensionality reduction methods. As expected, these methods yield minimal results, serving as a clear baseline. This is consistent, for example, with an understanding of single units as rate-varying Poisson processes.

      ● Figures 2 and 3 then build upon these results with spiking features frequent in neuroscience literature such as firing rate, coefficient of variation, etc using linear supervised and more detailed spiking features such as ISI distribution using nonlinear supervised machine learning methods.

      By starting from the standpoint of the status quo, we are better able to contextualize the significance of our later findings in Figures 4–6.

      Response to Specific Points in the Summary

      (6) Separability of VISp vs. Secondary Visual Areas

      I found the entire argument about visual areas somewhat messy and unclear. The stimuli used might not drive the secondary visual areas particularly well and might necessitate task engagement.

      We appreciate your feedback that the dissection of visual cortical structures is unclear. To summarize, as shown in the bottom three rows of Figure 6, there is a notable lack of diagonality in visuocortical structures. This means that our model was unable to learn signatures to reliably predict these classes. In contrast, visuocortical layer is returned well above chance, and superstructures (primary and secondary areas) are moderately well identified, albeit still well above chance.

      Consider a thought experiment, if Charlie Gross had not shown faces to monkeys to find IT, or Newsome and others shown motion to find MT and Zeki and others color stimuli to find V4, we would conclude that there are no differences.

      The thought experiment is misleading. The results specifically do not arise from stimulus selectivity—much of Newsome’s own work suggests that the selectivity of neurons in IT etc. is explained by little more than rate varying Poisson processes. In this case, there should be no fundamental anatomical difference in the “language” of the neurons in V4 and IT, only a difference in the inputs driving those neurons. In contrast, our work suggests that the “language” of neurons varies as a function of some anatomical divisions. In other words, in contrast to a Poisson rate code, our results predict that single neuron spike patterns might be remarkably different in MT and IT— and that this is not a function of stimulus selectivity. Notably, the anatomical (and functional) division between V1 and secondary visual areas does not appear to manifest in a different “language”, thus constituting an interesting result in and of itself.

      We regret a failure to communicate this in a tight and compelling fashion on the first submission, but hope that the revision is limpid and accessible.

      Barberini, C. L., Horwitz, G. D., & Newsome, W. T. (2001). A comparison of spiking statistics in motion sensing neurones of flies and monkeys. Motion Vision: Computational, Neural, and Ecological Constraints, 307-320.

      Bair, W., Zohary, E., & Newsome, W. T. (2001). Correlated firing in macaque visual area MT: time scales and relationship to behavior. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(5), 1676-1697.

      Similarly, why would drifting gratings be a good example of a stimulus for the hippocampus, an area thought to be involved in memory/place fields?

      The results suggest that anatomical “language” is not tied to stimuli. It is imperative to recall that neurons are highly active absent experimentally imposed stimuli, such as when an animal is at rest, when an animal is asleep, and when an animal is in the dark (relevant to visual cortices). With this in mind, also recall that, despite the lack of stimuli tailored to the hippocampus, neurons therein were still reliably separable from neurons in seven nuclei in the thalamus, 6 of which are not classically considered visual regions. Should these regions (including hippocampus) have been inert during the presentation of visual stimuli, there would have been very little separability.

      (7) Generalization across laboratories

      “[C]omparison across laboratories was somewhat underwhelming. It does okay but none of the results are particularly compelling in terms of performance.

      Any result above chance is a rejection of the null hypothesis: that a model trained on a set of animals in Laboratory A will be ineffective in identifying brain regions when tested on recordings collected in Laboratory B (in different animals and under different experimental conditions). As an existence proof, the results suggest conserved principles (however modest) that constrain neuronal activity as a function of anatomy. That models fail to achieve high accuracy (in this context) is not surprising (given the limitations of available recordings)---that models achieve anything above chance, however, is.

      Thus, after reading the paper many times, I think part of the problem is that the study is not cohesive, and the authors need to either come up with a tool or demonstrate a scientific finding.

      We demonstrate that neuronal spike trains carry robust anatomical information. We developed an ML architecture for this and that architecture is publicly available.

      They try to split the middle and I am left somewhat perplexed about what exact scientific problem they or other researchers are solving.

      We humbly suggest that the question of a neurons “language” is highly important and central to an understanding of how brains work. From a computational perspective, there is no reason for a vast diversity of cell types, nor a differentiation of the rules that dictate neuronal activity in one region versus another. A Turing Complete system can be trivially constructed from a small number of simple components, such as an excitatory and inhibitory cell type. This is the basis of many machine learning tools.

      Please do not confuse stimulus specificity with the concept of a neuron’s language. Neurons in VISp might fire more in response to light, while those in auditory cortex respond to sound. This does not mean that these neurons are different - only that their inputs are. Given the lack of a literature describing our main effect—that single neuron spiking carries information about anatomical location—it is difficult to conclude that our results are either commonplace or to be expected.

      I am also unsure why the authors think some of these results are particularly important.

      See above.

      For instance, has anyone ever argued that brain areas do not have different spike patterns?

      Yes. In effect, by two avenues. The first is a lack of any argument otherwise (please do not conflate spike patterns with stimulus tuning), and the second is the preponderance of, e.g., rate codes across many functionally distinct regions and circuits.

      Is that not the premise for all systems neuroscience?

      No. The premise for all systems neuroscience (from our perspective) is that the brain is a) a collection of interacting neurons and b) the collective system of neurons gives rise to behavior, cognition, sensation, and perception. As stated above, these axiomatic first principles fundamentally do not require that neurons, as individual entities, obey different rules in different parts of the brain.

      I could see how one could argue no one has said ISIs matter but the premise that the areas are different is a fundamental part of neuroscience.

      Based on logic and the literature, we fundamentally disagree. Consider: while systems neuroscience operates on the principle that brain regions have specialized functions, there is no a priori reason to assume that these functions must be reflected in different underlying computational rules. The simplest explanation is that a single language of spiking exists across regions, with functional differences arising from processing distinct inputs rather than fundamentally different spiking rules. For example, an identical spike train in the amygdala and Layer 5 of M1 would have profoundly different functional impacts, yet the spike timing itself could be identical (even as stimulus response). Until now, evidence for region-specific spiking patterns has been lacking, and our work attempts to begin addressing this gap. There is extensive further work to be conducted in this space, and it is certain that models will improve, rules will be clarified, and mechanisms will be identified.

      Detailed major comments

      (1) Exploratory trends in spiking by region and structure across the population:

      The argument in this section is that unsupervised analyses might reveal subtle trends in the organization of spiking patterns by area. The authors show 4 plots from t-SNE and claim to see subtle organization. I have concerns. For Figure 1C, it is nearly impossible to see if a significant structure exists that differentiates regions and structures. So this leads certain readers to conclude that the authors are looking at the artifactual structure (see Chari et al. 2024) - likely to contribute to large Twitter battles. Contributing to this issue is that the hyperparameter for tSNE was incorrectly chosen. I do think that a different perplexity should be used for the visualization in order to better show the underlying structure; the current visualization just looks like a single "blob". The UMAP visualizations in the supplement make this point more clearly. I also think the authors should include a better plot with appropriate perplexity or not include this at all. The color map of subtle shades of green and yellow is hard to see as well in both Figure S1 and Figure 1.

      In response to the feedback, we replaced t-SNE/UMAP with LDA, while keeping PCA for dimensionality reduction.

      As stated in the original methods, t-SNE/UMAP hyperparameters were chosen based on the combination that led to the greatest classifiable separability of the regions/structures in the space (across a broad range of possible combinations). It just so happens that the maximally separable structure from a regions/structures perspective is the “blob”. This suggests that perhaps the predominant structure the t-SNE finds in the data is not driven by anatomy. If we selected hyperparameters in some other way that was not based specifically on regions/structures (e.g. simple visual inspection of the plots) the conformation would of course be different and not blob-like. However, we removed the t-SNE and UMAP to avoid further confusion.

      The “muddy appearance” is not an issue with the color map. As seen in Figure 1B, the chosen colors are visibly distinct. Figure 1C (previous version) appeared muddy yellow/green because of points that overlap with transparency, resulting in a mix of clearly defined classes (e.g., a yellow point on top of a blue point creating green). This overlap is a meaningful representation of the separability observed in this analysis. We also tried using 2D KDE for visualization, but it did not improve the impression of visual separability.

      We are removing p-values from the figures because they lead to the impression that we over-interpret these results quantitatively. However, we calculated p-values based on label permutation similar to the way R2 suggests (see previous methods). The conflation with the Wasserstein distances is an understandable misunderstanding. These are unrelated to p-values and used for the heatmaps in S1 only (see previous methods).

      Instead of p-values, we now use the adjusted rand index, which measures how accurately neurons within the same region are clustered together (see Line 670 - 671, Figure 1C, and Figure S1) (Hubert & Arabie 1985). This quantifies the extent to which the distribution of points in dimensionally-reduced space is shaped by region/structure.

      Hubert, L., & Arabie, P. (1985). Comparing partitions. Journal of Classification, 2(1), 193–218. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01908075

      (2) Logistic classifiers:

      The results in this section are somewhat underwhelming. Accuracy is around 40% and yes above chance but I would be very surprised if someone is worried about separating visual structures from the thalamus. Such coarse brain targeting is not difficult. If the authors want to include this data, I recommend they show it as a control in the ISI distribution section. The entire argument here is that perhaps one should not use derived metrics and a nonlinear classifier on more data is better, which is essentially the thrust of the next section.

      As outlined above, our work systematically increases in model complexity. The logistic result is an intermediate model, and it returns intermediate results. This is an important stepping stone between the lack of a result based on unsupervised linear dimensionality reduction and the performance of supervised nonlinear models.

      From a purely utilitarian perspective, the argument could be framed as “one should not use derived metrics, and a nonlinear classifier on more data is better.” However, please see all of our notes above.

      (3) MLP classifiers:

      Even in this section, I was left somewhat underwhelmed that a nonlinear classifier with large amounts of data outperforms a linear classifier with small amounts of data. I found the analysis of the ISIs and which timescales are driving the classifier interesting but I think the classifier with smoothing is more interesting. So with a modest chance level decodability of different brain areas in the visual system, I found it somewhat grandiose to claim a "conserved" code for anatomy in the brain. If there is conservation, it seems to be at the level of the coarse brain organization, which in my opinion is not particularly compelling.

      The sample size used for both the linear and nonlinear classifiers is the same; however, the nonlinear classifier leverages the detailed spiking time information from ISIs. Our goal here was to systematically evaluate how classical spike metrics compare to more detailed temporal features in their ability to decode brain areas. We chose a linear classifier for spike metrics because, with fewer features, nonlinear methods like neural networks often offer very modest advantages over linear methods, less interpretability, and are prone to overfitting.

      Respectfully, we stand by our word choice. The term “conserved” is appropriate given that our results hold appreciably, i.e., statistically above chance, across animals.

      (4) Generalization section:

      The authors suggest that a classifier learned from one set of data could be used for new data. I was unsure if this was a scientific point or the fact that they could use it as a tool.

      It can be both. We are more driven by the scientific implications of a rejection of the null.

      Is the scientific argument that ISIs are similar across areas even in different tasks?

      It appears so - despite heterogeneity in the tuning of single neurons, their presynaptic inputs, and stimuli, there is identifiable information about anatomical location in the spike train.

      Why would one not learn a classifier from every piece of available data: like LFP bands, ISI distributions, and average firing rates, and use that to predict the brain area as a comparison?

      Because this would obfuscate the ability to conclude that spike trains embed information about anatomy.

      Considering all features simultaneously and adding additional data modalities—such as LFP bands and spike waveforms—has potential to improve classification accuracy at the cost of understanding the contribution of each feature. The spike train as a time series is the most fundamental component of neuronal communication. As a result, this is the only feature of neuronal activity of concern for the present investigation.

      Or is the argument that the ISIs are a conserved code for anatomy? Unfortunately, even in this section, the data are underwhelming.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments, but arrive at a very different conclusion. We were quite surprised to find any generalizability whatsoever.

      Moreover, for use as a tool, I think the authors need to seriously consider a control that is either waveforms from different brain areas or the local field potentials. Without that, I am struggling to understand how good this tool is. The authors said "because information transmission in the brain arises primarily from the timing of spiking and not waveforms (etc)., our studies involve only the timestamps of individual spikes from well-isolated units ". However, we are not talking about information transmission and actually trying to identify and assess brain areas from electrophysiological data.

      While we are not blind to the “tool” potential that is suggested by our work, this is not the primary motivation or content in any section of the paper. As stated clearly in the abstract, our motivation is to ask “whether individual neurons [...] embed information about their own anatomical location within their spike patterns”. We go on to say “This discovery provides new insights into the relationship between brain structure and function, with broad implications for neurodevelopment, multimodal integration, and the interpretation of large-scale neuronal recordings. Immediately, it has potential as a strategy for in-vivo electrode localization.” Crucially, the last point we make is a nod to application. Indeed, our results suggest that in-vivo electrode localization protocols may benefit from the incorporation of such a model.

      In light of the reviewer’s concerns, we have further dampened the weight of statements about our model as a consumer-ready tool.

      Example 1: The final sentence of the abstract now reads: “Computational approximations of anatomy have potential to support in-vivo electrode localization.”

      Example 2: The results sections now contains the following text: “While significantly above chance, the structure-level model still lacks the accuracy for immediate practical application. However, it is highly likely that the incorporation of datasets with diverse multi-modal features and alternative regions from other research groups will increase the accuracy of such a model. In addition, a computational approach can be combined with other methods of anatomical reconstruction.” (Line 355 - 359).

      Example 3: We replaced the phrase "because information transmission in the brain arises primarily from the timing of spiking and not waveforms (etc) " with the phrase “because information is primarily encoded by the firing rate or the timing of spiking and not waveforms (etc)” (Line 116 - 118).

      (5) Discussion section:

      In the discussion, beginning with "It is reasonable to consider . . ." all the way to the penultimate paragraph, I found the argumentation here extremely hard to follow. Furthermore, the parts of the discussion here I did feel I understood, I heavily disagreed with. They state that "recordings are random in their local sampling" which is almost certainly untrue when it comes to electrophysiology which tends to oversample task-modulated excitatory neurons (https://elifesciences.org/articles/69068). I also disagree that "each neuron's connectivity is unique, and vertebrate brains lack 'identified neurons' characteristic of simple organisms. While brains are only eutelic and "nameable" in only the simplest organisms (C. elegans), cell types are exceedingly stereotyped in their connectivity even in mammals and such connectivity defines their computational properties. Thus I don't find the premise the authors state in the next sentence to be undermined ("it seems unlikely that a single neuron's happenstance imprinting of its unique connectivity should generalize across stimuli and animals"). Overall, I found this subsection to rely on false premises and in my opinion it should be removed.

      At the suggestion of R2, we removed the paragraph in question. However, we would like to address some points of disagreement:

      We agree that electrophysiology, along with spike-sorting, quality metrics, and filtering of low-firing neurons, leads to oversampling of task-modulated neurons. However, when we stated that recordings are random in their local sampling, we were referring to structural (anatomical) randomness, not functional randomness. In other words, the recorded neurons were not specifically targeted (see below).

      Electrode arrays, such as Neuropixels, record from hundreds of neurons within a small volume relative to the total number of neurons and the volume of a given brain region. For instance, the paper R2 referenced includes a statement supporting this: “... assuming a 50-μm ‘listening radius’ for the probes (radius of half-cylinder around the probe where the neurons’ spike amplitude is sufficiently above noise to trigger detection) …, the average yield of 116 regular-spiking units/probe (prior to QC filtering) would imply a density of 42,000 neurons/mm³, much lower than the known density of ~90,000 neurons/mm³ for excitatory cells in mouse visual cortex….”

      If we take the estimated volume of V1 to be approximately 3 mm³, this region could theoretically be subdivided into multiple cylinders with a 100-μm diameter. While stereotaxic implantation of the probe mitigates some variability, the natural anatomical variability across individual animals introduces spatially random sampling. This was the randomness we were referring to, and thus, we disagree with the assertion that our claim is “almost certainly untrue.”

      Additionally, each cortical pyramidal neuron is understood to have ~ 10,000 presynaptic partners. It is highly unlikely that these connections are entirely pre-specified, perfectly replicated within the same animal, and identical across all members of species. Further, there is enormous diversity in the activity properties of even neighboring cells of the same type. Consider pyramidal neurons in V1. Single neuron firing rates are log normally distributed, there are many of combinations of tuning properties (i.e., direction, orientation) that must occupy each point in retinotopic space, and there is powerful experience dependent change in the connectivity of these cells. We suggest that it is inconceivable that any two neurons, even within a small region of V1, have identical connectivity.

      Minor Comments:

      (1) Although the description of confusion matrices is good from a didactic perspective, some of this could be moved to methods to simplify the paper.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. However, given the broad readership of eLife, we gently suggest that confusion matrices are not a trivial and universally appreciated plotting format. For the purpose of accessibility, a brief and didactic 2-sentence description will make the paper far more comprehensible to many readers at little cost to experts.

      (2) Figure 3A: It is concluded in their subsequent figure that the longer the measured amount of time, the better the decoding performance. Thus it makes sense why the average PSTHs do not show significant decoding of areas or structures

      That is a good observation. However, all features were calculated from the same duration of data, except in Figure 3B, where we tested the effect of duration. The averaged PSTH was calculated from the same length of data as the ISI distribution and binned to have the same number of feature lengths as the ISI distribution (refer to Methods section). Therefore, we interpreted this as an indication of information degradation through averaging, rather than an effect of data length (Line 234 - 237).

      (3) Figure 3D: A Gaussian is used to fit the ISI distributions here but ISI distributions do not follow a normal distribution, they follow an inverse gamma distribution.

      We agree with the reviewer and we are familiar with the literature that the ISI distribution is best fitted by a gamma family distribution (as a recent, but not earliest example: Li et al. 2018). However, we did not fit a gaussian (or any distribution) to the data, we just calculated the sample mean and variance. Reporting sample mean and variance (or standard deviation) is not something that is only done for Gaussian distributions. They are broadly used metrics that simply have additional intrinsic meaning for Gaussian distributions. We used the schematic illustration in Fig 3D because mean and variance are much more familiar in Gaussian distribution context, but ultimately that does not affect our analyses in Fig 3 E-F. Alternatively, the alpha and beta intrinsic parameters of a gamma distribution could have been used, but they are known by a much smaller portion of neuroscientists.

      Li, M., Xie, K., Kuang, H., Liu, J., Wang, D., Fox, G. E., ... & Tsien, J. Z. (2018). Spike-timing pattern operates as gamma-distribution across cell types, regions and animal species and is essential for naturally-occurring cognitive states. Biorxiv, 145813(10.1101), 145813.

      (4) Figure 3G: Something is wrong with this figure as each vertical bar is supposed to represent a drifting grating onset but yet, they are all at 5 hz despite the PSTH being purportedly shown at many different frequencies from 1 to 15 hz.

      We appreciate your attention to detail, but we are not representing the onset of individual drifting gratings in this. We just meant to represent the overall start\end of the drifting grating session. We did not intend to signal the temporal frequency of the drifting gratings (or the spatial frequency, orientation, or contrast).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study focuses on characterizing a previously identified gene, encoding the secreted protein Ppe1, that may play a role in rice infection by the blast fungus Magnaporthe oryzae. Magnaporthe oryzae is a hemibiotrophic fungus that infects living host cells before causing disease. Infection begins with the development of a specialized infection cell, the appressorium, on the host leaf surface. The appressorium generates enormous internal turgor that acts on a thin penetration peg at the appressorial base, forcing it through the leaf cuticle. Once through this barrier, the peg elaborates into bulbous invasive hyphae that colonizes the first infected cell before moving to neighboring cells via plasmodesmata. During this initial biotrophic growth stage, invasive hyphae invaginate the host plasma membrane, which surrounds growing hyphae as the extra-invasive hyphae membrane (EIHM). To avoid detection, the fungus secretes apoplastic effectors into the EIHM matrix via the conventional ER-Golgi secretion pathway. The fungus also forms a plant-derived structure called the biotrophic interfacial complex (BIC) that receives cytoplasmic effectors through an unconventional secretion route before they are delivered into the host cell. Together, these secreted effector proteins act to evade or suppress host innate immune responses. Here the authors contribute to our understanding of M. oryzae infection biology by showing how Ppe1, which localizes to both the appressorial penetration peg and to the appressorial-like transpressoria associated with invasive hyphal movements into adjacent cells, maximizes host cell penetration and disease development and is thus a novel contributor to rice blast disease.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful evaluation of our work. We are grateful for your recognition of Ppe1 as a novel contributor to M. oryzae infection biology and your insightful summary of its spatio-temporal localization and functional importance in host penetration. We also appreciate devoting your time to provide us with constructive feedback, which greatly strengthens our manuscript.

      Strengths:

      A major goal of M. oryzae research is to understand how the fungus causes disease, either by determining the physiological underpinnings of the fungal infection cycle or by identifying effectors and their host targets. Such new knowledge may point the way to novel mitigation strategies. Here, the authors make an interesting discovery that bridges both fungal physiology and effector biology research by showing how a secreted protein Ppe1, initially considered an effector with potential host targets, associates with its own penetration peg (and transpressoria) to facilitate host invasion. In a previous study, the authors had identified a small family of small secreted proteins that may function as effectors. Here they suggest Ppe1 (and, later in the manuscript, Ppe2/3/5) localizes outside the penetration peg when appressoria develops on surfaces that permit penetration, but not on artificial hard surfaces that prevent peg penetration. Deleting the PPE1 gene reduced (although did not abolish) penetration, and a fraction of those that penetrated developed invasive hyphae that were reduced in growth compared to WT. Using fluorescent markers, the authors show that Ppe1 forms a ring underneath appressoria, likely where the peg emerges, which remained after invasive hyphae had developed. The ring structure is smaller than the width of the appressorium and also lies within the septin ring known to form during peg development. This so-called penetration ring also formed at the transpressorial penetration point as invasive hyphae moved to adjacent cells. This structure is novel, and required for optimum penetration during infection. Furthermore, Ppe1, which carries a functional signal peptide, may form on the periphery of the peg, together suggesting it is secreted and associated with the peg to facilitate penetration. Staining with aniline blue also suggests Ppe1 is outside the peg. Together, the strength of the work lies in identifying a novel appressorial penetration ring structure required for full virulence.

      We are deeply grateful to the reviewer for the clear understanding and insightful evaluation of our work. Your recognition of the novel contribution and scientific merit of our study is both encouraging and motivating. We sincerely appreciate the time, expertise and constructive feedback dedicated to reviewing our manuscript, as the comments have been instrumental in enhancing the quality of this work.

      Weaknesses:

      The main weakness of the paper is that, although Ppe1 is associated with the peg and optimizes penetration, the function of Ppe1 is not known. The work starts off considering Ppe1 a secreted effector, then a facilitator of penetration by associating with the peg, but what role it plays here is only often speculated about. For example, the authors consider at various times that it may have a structural role, a signaling role orchestrating invasive hyphae development, or a tethering role between the peg and the invaginated host plasma membrane (called throughout the host cytoplasmic membrane, a novel term that is not explained). However, more effort should be expended to determine which of these alternative roles is the most likely. Otherwise, as it stands, the paper describes an interesting phenomenon (the appressorial ring) but provides no understanding of its function.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s comments. We have revised "host cytoplasmic membrane" to "host plasma membrane" throughout the manuscript for consistency. To further investigate the role of the Ppe1 in the interaction between M. oryzae and rice, we overexpressed PPE1 in rice ZH11. A pCXUN-SP-GFP-Ppe1 vector containing a signal peptide and an N-terminal GFP tag was constructed (pCXUN-SP-GFP-Ppe1), and 35 GFP-PPE1-OX plants (T0) were subsequently obtained through Agrobacterium-mediated rice transformation. Subsequently, PCR and qRT-PCR validation were performed on the T0 transgenic plants. The PCR results showed that the inserted plasmid could be amplified from the genomic DNA extracted from the leaves of all the resulting T0 plants (Author response image 1A). qRT-PCR results indicated that most T0 transgenic plants could transcriptionally express PPE1 (Author response image 1B). T0 plants with higher expression levels were selected for western blot analysis, which confirmed the presence of GFP-Ppe1 bands of the expected size (Author response image 1C). To further explore the targets of Ppe1 in rice, the leaf sheaths of T0 plants were inoculated with M. oryzae strain Guy11. Total proteins were extracted at 24 hours post-inoculation (hpi) and subjected to immunoprecipitation using GFP magnetic beads. Silver staining revealed more interacting protein bands in T0 plants compared to ZH11 and GFP-OX controls (Author response image 1D). These samples were then analyzed by mass spectrometry in which 331 rice proteins that potentially interact with Ppe1 were identified (Author response image 1E). Subsequently, yeast two-hybrid assays were performed on 13 putative interacting proteins with higher coverage, but no interaction was detected between Ppe1 and these proteins (Author response image 1F-G). Considering that the identification and functional validation of interacting proteins is a labor-intensive and time-consuming endeavor, we will focus our future efforts on in-depth studies of Ppe1's function in rice.

      Author response image 1.

      Screening of Ppe1 candidate targets in rice. (A) The determination of GFP-PPE1 construct in transgenic rice. (B) The expression of PPE1 transgenic rice (T0) was verified by qRT-PCR. (C) Western blot analysis of Ppe1 expression in transgenic rice. (D) Rapid silver staining for detection of the purified proteins captured by the GFP-beads. (E) Venn diagram comparing the number of proteins captured in the different samples. (F) Identity of the potential targets of Ppe1 in rice. (G) Yeast two-hybrid assay showing negative interaction of Ppe1 with rice candidate proteins.

      The inability to nail down the function of Ppe1 likely stems from two underlying assumptions with weak support. Firstly, the authors assume that Ppe1 is secreted and associated with the peg to form a penetration ring between the plant cell wall and cytoplasm membrane. However, the authors do not demonstrate it is secreted (for instance by blocking Ppe1 secretion and its association with the peg using brefeldin A).

      To investigate the secretion pathway of Ppe1 in M. oryzae, we determined the inhibitory effects of Brefeldin A (BFA) on conventional ER-to-Golgi secretion in fungi as suggested by the reviewer. We inoculated rice leaf sheaths with conidia suspensions from the Ppe1-mCherry and PBV591 strains (containing a Pwl2-mCherry-NLS and Bas4-GFP co-expressing constructs) and treated them with BFA. We found that, even after exposure to BFA for 5 to 11 hours, the Ppe1-mCherry still formed its characteristic ring conformation (Author response image 2). Similarly, in the BFA-treated samples, the cytoplasmic effector Pwl2-mCherry accumulated at the BIC, while the apoplastic effector Bas4-GFP was retained in the invasive hyphae (Author response image 2). These results indicate that Ppe1 is not secreted through the conventional ER-Golgi secretion pathway.

      Author response image 2.

      The secretion of Ppe1 is not affected by BFA treatment. (A) and (B) The Ppe1-mCherry fluorescent signal was still observed both in the presence and absence of BFA. (C) Following BFA treatment, the secretion of the apoplastic effector Bas4-GFP was blocked while that of the cytoplasmic effector Pwl2-mCherry was not affected. The rice leaf sheath tissue was inoculated with 50 μg/mL BFA (0.1% DMSO) at 17 hpi. Images were captured at 22 hpi for A and 28 hpi for B and C. Scale bars = 10 µm.

      Also, they do not sufficiently show that Ppe1 localizes on the periphery of the peg. This is because confocal microscopy is not powerful enough to see the peg. The association they are seeing (for example in Figure 4) shows localization to the bottom of the appressorium and around the primary hyphae, but the peg cannot be seen. Here, the authors will need to use SEM, perhaps in conjunction with gold labeling of Ppe1, to show it is associating with the peg and, indeed, is external to the peg (rather than internal, as a structural role in peg rigidity might predict). It would also be interesting to repeat the microscopy in Figure 4C but at much earlier time points, just as the peg is penetrating but before invasive hyphae have developed - Where is Ppe1 then? Finally, the authors speculate, but do not show, that Ppe1 anchors penetration pegs on the plant cytoplasm membrane. Doing so may require FM4-64 staining, as used in Figure 2 of Kankanala et al, 2007 (DOI: 10.1105/tpc.106.046300), to show connections between Ppe1 and host membranes. Note that the authors also do not show that the penetration ring is a platform for effector delivery, as speculated in the Discussion.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's valuable suggestion regarding SEM with immunogold labeling to precisely visualize Ppe1's association with penetration peg. While we fully acknowledge this would be an excellent approach, after consulting several experts in the field, we realized that the specialized equipment and technical expertise required for fungal immunogold-SEM are currently unavailable to us. We sincerely hope that the reviewer will understand this technical limitation.

      To further strengthen our evidence for the role of Ppe1's in anchoring penetration peg to the plant plasma membrane, we provided new co-localization images of Ppe1 and penetration peg (Fig. S7). At 16 hours post-inoculation (hpi), when the penetration peg was just forming and prior to the development of invasive hyphae, the Ppe1-mCherry fluorescence forms a tight ring-like structure closely associated with the base of the appressorium. As at 23 hpi, the circular Ppe1-mCherry signal was still detectable beneath the appressorium, and around the penetration peg which differentiated into the primary invasive hyphae. Furthermore, we obtained 3D images of the strain expressing both Ppe1-mCherry and Lifeact-GFP during primary invasive hyphal development. The results revealed that Ppe1 forms a ring-like structure that remains anchored to the penetration peg during fungal invasion (Fig. S6).

      We also conducted FM4-64 staining experiment as recommended by the reviewer. Although the experiment provided valuable insights, we found that the resolution was insufficient to precisely delineate the spatial relationship between Ppe1 and host membranes at the penetration peg (Author response image 3). To optimize this colocalization, we tested the localization between Ppe1-mCherry ring and rice plasma membrane marker GFP-OsPIP2 (Fig. S8). These new results provide compelling complementary evidence supporting our conclusion that Ppe1 functions extracellularly at the host-pathogen interface. We hope these additional data will help address the reviewer's concerns regarding Ppe1's localization.

      Author response image 3.

      FM4-64-stained rice leaf sheath inoculated with M. oryzae strain expressing Ppe1-GFP. Ppe1-GFP ring was positioned above the primary invasive hyphae. Scale bar = 5 µm.

      Secondly, the authors assume Ppe1 is required for host infection due to its association with the peg. However, its role in infection is minor. The majority of appressoria produced by the mutant strain penetrate host cells and elaborate invasive hyphae, and lesion sizes are only marginally reduced compared to WT (in fact, the lesion density of the 70-15 WT strain itself seems reduced compared to what would be expected from this strain). The authors did not analyze the lesions for spores to confirm that the mutant strains were non-pathogenic (non-pathogenic mutants sometimes form small pinprick-like lesions that do not sporulate). Thus, the pathogenicity phenotype of the knockout mutant is weak, which could contribute to the inability to accurately define the molecular and cellular function of Ppe1.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments. To ensure the reliability of our findings, we conducted spray inoculation experiments with multiple independent repeats. Our results consistently demonstrated that deletion of the PPE1 gene significantly attenuates the virulence of M. oryzae. Further analysis of lesion development and sporulation in the Δ_ppe1_ mutant revealed that it retains the ability to produce conidia. To validate these observations, we generated a PPE1 knockout in the wild-type reference strain Guy11. Similarly, we observed a significant decrease in the pathogenicity of the Δ_ppe1_ mutants generated from the wild-type Guy11 strain compared to Guy11 in the spray assay (Fig S2). These results collectively indicate the importance of Ppe1 in the pathogenicity of M. oryzae to rice.

      In summary, it is important that the role of Ppe1 in infection be determined.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The article focuses on the study of Magnaporthe oryzae, the fungal pathogen responsible for rice blast disease, which poses a significant threat to global food security. The research delves into the infection mechanisms of the pathogen, particularly the role of penetration pegs and the formation of a penetration ring in the invasion process. The study highlights the persistent localization of Ppe1 and its homologs to the penetration ring, suggesting its function as a structural feature that facilitates the transition of penetration pegs into invasive hyphae. The article provides a thorough examination of the infection process of M. oryzae, from the attachment of conidia to the development of appressoria and the formation of invasive hyphae. The discovery of the penetration ring as a structural element that aids in the invasion process is a significant contribution to the understanding of plant-pathogen interactions. The experimental methods are well-documented, allowing for reproducibility and validation of the results.

      We sincerely appreciate the thoughtful and insightful evaluation of our work. Thank you for recognizing the significance of our findings regarding the penetration ring and the functional role of Ppe1 during host invasion.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Line 48: "after appressorium- or transpressorium-mediated penetration of plant cell wall" - transpressoria do not penetrate the plant cell wall.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. For improved clarity, we have rephrased the sentence as follows: In this study, we showed that a penetration ring is formed by penetration pegs after appressorium-mediated penetration of plant cell wall.

      Line 143: "approximately 25% of the 143 appressoria formed by the Δppe1 mutant had no penetration peg" - It is not possible to see the penetration peg by confocal microscopy.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. We have revised the sentence as follows: In contrast, approximately 25% of the appressoria formed by the Δ_ppe1_ mutant had no penetration.

      Line 159: "inner cycle" -should be inner circle?

      We gratefully acknowledge the reviewer's careful reading. The typographical error has been corrected throughout the revised manuscript.

      Line 255: "These results indicate that initiation of penetration peg formation is necessary for the formation of the penetration ring." Actually, more precisely, they indicate that penetration is necessary.

      We appreciate this suggestion and have revised the text to be more concise: These results indicate that penetration is necessary for the formation of the penetration ring.

      Line 282: "unlike subcellular localizations of other effectors"- is this an effector if no plant targets are known?

      We appreciate this suggestion and have revised the text as follows: unlike subcellular localizations of Bas4, Slp1, Pwl2, and AvrPiz-t.

      Line 299: "it may function as a novel physical structure for anchoring penetration pegs on the surface of plant cytoplasm membrane after cell wall penetration" - an interaction with the plant plasma membrane was not shown and this is speculative.

      We have provided new evidence to show the spatial positioning of Ppe1-mCherry ring with the rice plasma membrane (see figure S8)

      Line 301: "It is also possible that this penetration ring functions as a collar or landmark that is associated with the differentiation of penetration pegs (on the surface of cytoplasm membrane) into primary invasive hyphae enveloped in the EIHM cytoplasm membrane (Figure 7)." The alternative conclusions for Ppe1 function, either interacting with host membranes or acting as a developmental landmark, need to be resolved here.

      We appreciate this suggestion and have revised the text as follows: It is also possible that this penetration ring functions as a collar that is associated with the differentiation of penetration pegs into primary invasive hyphae enveloped in the EIHM (Figure 7).

      Line 317: "is likely a structural feature or component for signaling the transition of penetration pegs to invasive hyphae",- if the authors think Ppe1 has these roles, why do they refer to Ppe1 as an effector?

      Many thanks for these comments. We have revised this and refer to Ppe1 as a secreted protein throughout the revised manuscript.

      Line 337: "After the penetration of plant cell wall, the penetration ring may not only function as a physical structure but also serve as an initial effector secretion site for the release of specific effectors to overcome plant immunity in early infection stages"- which is it? Also, no evidence is provided to suggest it is a platform for effector secretion.

      We sincerely appreciate your valuable suggestion. We have revised this sentence as follows: After the penetration of plant cell wall, the penetration ring may not only function as a physical structure but also serve as a secretion site for the release of specific proteins to overcome plant immunity during the early infection stages.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) While the study suggests the penetration ring as a structural feature, it remains unclear whether it also serves as a secretion site for effectors. Further exploration of this aspect would strengthen the conclusions.

      We thank the reviewer for this useful suggestion. In this study, we demonstrated that Ppe1 proteins form a distinct penetration ring structure at the site where the penetration peg contacts the plant plasma membrane prior to differentiation into primary invasive hyphae (Figs. 2 and 7). Thus, we reasoned that penetration ring may function as a novel physical structure. Notably, additional Ppe family members (Ppe2, Ppe3, and Ppe5) were also found to localize to this penetration ring (Fig. 6B), suggesting that it also serves as a secretion site for releasing proteins. To test whether Ppe1 and Ppe2 label to the same site, we analyzed the colocalization between Ppe1-GFP and Ppe2-mCherry. The results showed that Ppe1-GFP and Ppe2-mCherry are well colocalized (Author response image 4). This study primarily focuses on the discovery and characterization of the penetration ring. The potential role of this structure in effector translocation will be investigated in future studies.

      Author response image 4.

      Ppe1 co-localizes with Ppe2 at the penetration ring in M. oryzae. Line graphs were generated at the directions pointed by the white arrows. Scale bar = 2μm.

      (2) The article could benefit from a discussion on the broader implications of these findings for developing resistant crop varieties or new fungicidal strategies.

      We have incorporated this discussion as suggested (lines 358-360).

      (3) What is the significance of the formation of the penetration ring in the pathogenicity of the rice blast fungus? Or, how does it assist the fungus in its infection process?

      Our findings have several significant implications. First, we believe that the discovery of the penetration ring as a novel physical structure associated with the differentiation of invasive hyphae represents a breakthrough in plant-pathogen interactions that will be of interest to fungal biologists, pathologists and plant biologists. Secondly, our study presents new role of the peg as a specialized platform for secretory protein deployment, in addition to its commonly known role as a physical penetration tool for the pathogen. Thirdly, we identify Ppe1 as a potential molecular target for controlling the devastating rice blast disease, as Ppe homologs are absent in plants and mammals. We have incorporated this discussion in the revised manuscript (lines 354-362).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The mechanism, as I understand it, is different from what the authors described before in the RNN with tonic gain changes. As uncertainty increases, the network enters a regime in which the two excitatory populations start to oscillate. My intuition is that this oscillation arises from the feedback loop created by the new gain control mechanism. If my intuition is correct, I think it would be worth to explain this mechanism in the paper more explicitly.

      While interesting, this intuition is not correct. The oscillations are generated by the interaction between excitatory and inhibitory nodes in the network and occur in the model even with stationary gain. All of the plots in figure 3 exploring the dynamical regime of the network at different input x gain combinations (i.e., where the oscillatory regime is characterised) are simulations run with stationary gain.

      To ensure that this intuition is more clearly presented in the manuscript, we have edited the description in the text.

      P. 12: “Because of the large size of the network, we could not solve for the fixed points or study their stability analytically. Instead, we opted for a numerical approach and characterised the dynamical regime (i.e. the location and existence of approximate fixed-point attractors) across all combinations of (static) gain and  visited by the network.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      - The demonstration of the causal role of gain modulation in perceptual switches is partial. This causality is clearly demonstrated in the simulation work with the RNN. However, it is not fully demonstrated in the pupil analysis and the fMRI analysis. One reason is that this work is correlative (which is already very informative). An analysis of the timing of the effect might have overcome this limitation. For example, in a previous study, the same group showed that fMRI activity in the LC region precedes changes in the energy landscape of fMRI dynamics, which is a step towards investigating causal links between gain modulation, changes in the energy landscape and perceptual switches.

      Thank you for the suggestion, which we considered in detail. Unfortunately, the  temporal and spatial resolution of the fMRI data collected for this study precluded the same analyses we’ve run in previous work, however this is an important question for future work.

      - Some effects may reflect the expectation of a perceptual switch rather than the perceptual switch itself. To mitigate this risk, the design of the fMRI task included catch trials, in which no switch occurs, to reduce the expectation of a switch. The pupil study, however, did not include such catch trials.

      We agree that this is a limitation of the current study, which we previously highlighted in the methods section.

      - The paper uses RNN-based modelling to provide mechanistic insight into the role of gain modulation in perceptual switches. However, the RNN solves a task that differs markedly from that performed by human participants, which may limit the explanatory value of the model. The RNN is provided with two inputs characterising the sensory evidence supporting the first and last image category in the sequence (e.g. plane and shark). In contrast, observers in the task were naïve as to the identity of the last image at the beginning of the sequence. The brain first receives sensory evidence about the image category (e.g. plane) with which the sequence begins, which is very easy to recognise, then it sees a sequence of morphed images and has to discover what the final image category will be. To discover the final image category, the brain has to search a vast space of possible second images (it is a shark?, a frog?, a bird?, etc.), rather than comparing the likelihood of just two categories. This search process and the perceptual switch in the task appear to be mechanistically different from the competition between two inputs in the RNN.

      We appreciate the critical analysis of the experimental paradigm but disagree with the reviewers conclusions for two keys reasons: 1) Participants prior exposure to the images, such that they could create an expectation about what stimulus category a particular image would transition into (i.e., the image could not switch into any possible category); and 2) even if the reviewers’ concern was founded, models of K winner-take-all decision making are structured identically irrespective of whether the options are 2 or K options all that changes is the simulated reaction times which depend linearly on the K (for an example model see Hugh Wilson’s textbook Spikes, Decisions, and Actions, 1999, p.89-91). For these reasons, we maintain that the RNN is a sensible representation of the behavioural task.

      - Another aspect of the motivation for the RNN model remains unclear. The authors introduce dynamic gain modulation in the RNN, but it is not clear what the added value of dynamic gain modulation is. Both static (Fig. S1) and dynamic (Fig. 2F) gain modulation lead to the predicted effect: faster switching when the gain is larger.

      While we agree that the effect is observable with both static and dynamic gain, the stronger construct validity associated with the dynamic approach, including a stronger link with the observed pupil dynamics and a rich literature associated with modelling the behavioural consequences of surprise/uncertainty led us to the conclusion that the dynamical approach was a better representation of our hypothesis.

      - Fig 1C: I don't see a "top grey bar" indicating significance.

      Thank you for catching this, the caption has been amended. The text was from an older version of the manuscript.

      - p. 10, reference to fig 3F seems incorrect: there is Fig 3F upper and Fig 3F lower, and nothing on Fig 3 and its legend mention the lesion of units

      This has been amended. We meant to refer to 2F.

      - In the response letter you mention a MATLAB tutorial, but I could not find it.

      This has been amended. Github repository can be found at https://github.com/ShineLabUSYD/AmbiguousFigures

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript reports that expression of the E. coli operon topAI/yjhQ/yjhP is controlled by the translation status of a small open reading frame, that authors have discovered and named toiL, located in the leader region upstream of the operon. Authors propose the following model for topAI activation: Under normal conditions, toiL is translated but topAI is not expressed because of Rho-dependent transcription termination within the topAI ORF and because its ribosome binding site and start codon are trapped in an mRNA hairpin. Ribosome stalling at various codons of the toiL ORF, prompted in this work by some ribosome-targeting antibiotics, triggers an mRNA conformational switch which allows translation of topAI and, in addition, activation of the operon's transcription because presence of translating ribosomes at the topAI ORF blocks Rho from terminating transcription. The model is appealing and several of the experimental data mainly support it. However, it remains unanswered what is the true trigger of the translation arrest at toiL and what is the physiological role of the induced expression of the topAI/yjhQ/yjhP operon.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Baniulyte and Wade describe how translation of an 8-codon uORF denoted toiL upstream of the topAI-yjhQP operon is responsive to different ribosome-targeting antibiotics, consequently controlling translation of the TopAI toxin as well as Rho-dependent termination with the gene.

      Strengths:

      The authors used multiple different approaches such as a genetic screen to identify factors such as 23S rRNA mutations that affect topA1 expression and ribosome profiling to examine the consequences of various antibiotics on toiL-mediated regulation.

      Weaknesses:

      Future experiments will be needed to better understand the physiological role of the toiL-mediated regulation and elucidate the mechanism of specific antibiotic sensing.

      The results are clearly described, and the revisions have helped to improve the presentation of the data.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In this revised manuscript, the authors provide convincing data to support an elegant model in which ribosome stalling by ToiL promotes downstream topAI translation and prevents premature Rho-dependent transcription termination. However, the physiological consequences of activating topAI-yjhQP expression upon exposure to various ribosome-targeting antibiotics remain unresolved. The authors have satisfactorily addressed all major concerns raised by the reviewers, particularly regarding the SHAPE-seq data. Overall, this study underscores the diversity of regulatory ribosome-stalling peptides in nature, highlighting ToiL's uniqueness in sensing multiple antibiotics and offering significant insights into bacterial gene regulation coordinated by transcription and translation.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      - Showing the ribosome density profiles of topAI/yjhQP and toiL in control and tetracycline treated cells is necessary to support that ribosome arrest at toiL increases translation of topAI/yjhQP.

      Figure 7B shows ribosome density around the start of toiL. Ribosome density increases across topAI in the presence of tetracycline, but we have opted not to show this region because we cannot say whether the increase in ribosome occupancy (represented in Figure 7A) is due to an increase in translation efficiency, RNA level, or both.

      - The subinhibitory antibiotic concentrations used in the reporter assays were based on MICs reported in the literature. This is not appropriate since MICs can greatly vary between strains, antibiotic solution stocks, and experimental conditions.

      Reported MICs were used as an initial guide for selecting antibiotic concentrations to test in our reporter assays. We have added text to indicate this, and to highlight that MICs vary considerably between strains.

      - toiL sequence may have evolved to maintain base-pairing with the topAI upstream region rather than, as authors suggest in Discussion, to respond to antibiotic-mediated arrest in an amino acid sequence specific manner.

      We have chosen to frame this as speculation.

      - Authors may consider commenting on the possibility that chloramphenicol does not induce because ToiL lacks alanine residues, whose presence at specific places of a nascent protein have been shown to promote chloramphenicol action (2016 PNAS 113:12150; 2022 NSMB 29:152).

      This is a great point as none of our stalling reporters included an ORF with alanine. We now include a short paragraph in the Discussion section to raise this possibility.

      - Tetracycline was added at the "subinhibitory concentration" of 8 ug/mL for the reporter assays but at 1 ug/mL for the ribosome profiling experiments. Authors should explain what was the rational for this.

      We think the reviewer is mixing up the epidemiological cut-off value of 8 ug/mL with the concentration used in experiments (0.5-1 ug/mL for reporter assays and ribosome profiling). The text was confusing, so we have added a sentence to the Methods section to indicate that epidemiological cut-off values and MICs were only a guide for selecting antibiotic concentrations to test.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I wish the authors had been slightly less dismissive of the reviewers' comments. At a minimum, it would be nice if the authors could be consistent about the ribosome representation throughout the manuscript;

      We apologize if our previous responses gave the impression of being dismissive. That was certainly not our intention. We greatly value the reviewers' feedback, and we appreciate the opportunity to clarify any misunderstandings. We believe the reviewer is referring to the different shape and color of the ribosome in Figures 8 and 9, and Figure 8 figure supplement 2, which we have now corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Comments on revisions:

      Although the authors have revealed the sulfane sulfur content in native MT-3, my question, namely, whether canonical MT-1 and MT-2 contained sulfane sulfur after the induction has been left.

      The authors argue that the biological significance of sulfane sulfur in MTs lies in its ability to contribute to metal binding affinity, provide a sensing mechanism against oxidative stress, and aid in the regulation of the protein. Due to their biological roles, induced MT-1 and MT-2 could contain sulfane sulfur in their molecules. Thus, I expect the authors to evaluate or explain the sulfane sulfur content in induced MT-1 and MT-2.

      Thank you for your valuable comments. In this study, we were not able to examine the role of sulfane sulfur in the induced forms of MT-1 and MT-2. However, this topic is undoubtedly important and intriguing; therefore, we will continue to explore it in future studies.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Comments on revisions:

      The revised manuscript is only slightly changed from the original, with the inclusion of a supplementary figure (Fig. S2) and minor changes in the text. The authors did not choose to carry out the quantitative Zn binding experiment (which I really wanted to see), but given the complexities of the experiment, I'll let it go.

      Fig. 9: the authors imply in the mechanistic "redox-switch" figure that Trx/TR can not reduce persulfide linkages. A number of groups have shown this to be the case. I recommend modifying the figure legend or text to make this clear to the reader.

      Thank you for your understanding. Regarding the "redox-switch" figure, although some groups have demonstrated the ability of Trx to reduce persulfide moieties, as you pointed out, we have addressed this discrepancy in the Discussion section as follows (lines 357-361): “In contrast, Trx has been proposed to reduce the persulfide moiety of PTP1B (37) and albumin (38, 39). A possible explanation for this discrepancy is that apo-GIF/MT-3-persulfide is rapidly changed into a different conformation that is topologically resistant to Trx reduction. In other words, Trx may exhibit substrate specificity.” Additionally, we have inserted the following sentence just before the above discussion to further clarify this point:“This suggests that the persulfide moiety in GIF/MT-3 appears to be relatively stable against Trx reduction.”

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)): 

      Summary: 

      Laura Morano and colleagues have performed a screen to identify compounds that interfere with the formation of TopBP1 condensates. TopBP1 plays a crucial role in the DNA damage response, and specifically the activation of ATR. They found that the GSK-3b inhibitor AZD2858 reduced the formation of TopBP1 condensates and activation of ATR and its downstream target CHK1 in colorectal cancer cell lines treated with the clinically relevant irinotecan active metabolite SN-38. This inhibition of TopBP1 condensates by AZD2858 was independent from its effect on GSK-3b enzymatic activity. Mechanistically, they show that AZD2858 thus can interfere with intra-S-phase checkpoint signaling, resulting in enhanced cytostatic and cytotoxic effects of SN-38 (or SN-38+Fluoracil aka FOLFIRI) in vitro in colorectal carcinoma cell lines. 

      Major comments: 

      Overall the work is rigorous and the main conclusions are convincing. However, they only show the effects of their combination treatments on colorectal cancer cell lines. I'm worried that blocking the formation of TopB1 condensates will also be detrimental in non-transformed cells. Furthermore it is somewhat disappointing that it remains unclear how AZD2858 blocks selfassembly of TopBP1 condensates, although I understand that unraveling this would be complex and somewhat out-of-reach for now. 

      We appreciate your feedback and fully recognize the importance of understanding how AZD2858 blocks the assembly of TopBP1 condensates. While we understand your disappointment, addressing this question remains a key focus for us. Keeping in mind that unravelling such a mechanism in vitro or in vivo is rather challenging, we have consulted an expert who has made efforts to predict the potential docking sites of AZD2858 on TopBP1, which may provide valuable insights for future experimental investigations. Using an AlphaFold model (no crystal or cryo-EM structure available) and looking for suitable pockets or cavities in which AZD2858 could bind, the analyses, though requiring cautious interpretation, suggested that AZD2858 may target the BRCT1 and BRCT8 domains (as shown below, two pockets n°1 and 7 with sufficient volume and surrounded by b-sheets structures like other GSK3 inhibitor) of TopBP1.

      However, these are preliminary results that require further exploration and experimental validation to confirm their significance and mechanistic implications.

      Author response image 1.

      Here are some specific points for improvement: 

      (1) The authors conclude that "These data supports [sic] the feasibility of targeting condensates formed in response to DNA damage to improve chemotherapy-based cancer treatments". To support this conclusion the authors need to show that proliferating non-transformed cells (e.g. primary cell cultures or organoids) can tolerate the combination of AZD2858 + SN-38 (or FOLFIRI) better than colorectal cancer cells. 

      We would like to thank the reviewer for this vital suggestion to prove that this combination is effective on tumor cells and not very toxic on healthy cells. We therefore used a healthy colon cell line (CCD841) and tested the efficacy of each treatment alone (FOLFIRI and AZD2858) as well as the combination FOLFIRI+AZD2858. We compared the results obtained in the CCD841 cell line with those obtained in the HCT116 colorectal cancer cell line. The results presented below show not only that each treatment alone is much less effective on CCD841 lines, but also that the combination is not synergistic.

      Author response image 2.

      Page 19 "This suggests that the combination... arrests the cell cycle before mitosis in a DNAPKsc-dependent manner." I find the remark that this arrest would be DNA-PKcs-dependent too speculative. I suppose that the authors base this claim on reference 55 but if they want to support this claim they need to prove this by adding DNA-PKcs inhibitors to their treated cells. 

      Thank you for your thoughtful comment. We agree with the reviewer that claiming the G2/M arrest is DNA-PKcs-dependent without direct experimental evidence is speculative. While we initially based this hypothesis on reference 55, we acknowledge that further experiments, such as the use of DNA-PKcs inhibitors, would be necessary to robustly support this claim.

      Given that this observation was intended as a potential explanation for the G2/M arrest observed at 6 and 12 hours of treatment with AZD2858 + SN-38 (compared to SN-38 alone), and considering that exploring this pathway is not the primary focus of our study, we have decided to remove this hypothesis from both the figure and the text to avoid any ambiguity.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s input and will consider investigating this pathway in future studies.

      (2) When discussing Figure S5B the authors claim that SN-38 + AZD2858 progressively increases the fractions of BrdU positive cells, but this is not supported by statistical analysis.

      The fractions are still very small, so I would like to see statistics on these data. Alternatively, the authors could take out this conclusion. 

      Thank you for your valuable comment. In response, we have conducted a statistical analysis (Mann-Whitney test) on the data, and the results have been added to Figure S5C for the 6-hour time point and Figure S5D for the 12-hour time point, based on three independent biological replicates. We hope this provides the necessary clarification.

      Minor comments: 

      - Page 5 Materials and methods - Cell culture. Last sentence "Add in what medium you cultured them" looks like an internal review remark and should probably be removed? 

      We apologize for this oversight. The medium has now been specified, and the sentence has been removed.

      - The numbers in all the synergy matrices (in white font) are extremely small and virtually unreadable, and visually distracting. I recommend taking these out altogether. 

      We believe that the reduction in figure quality may be due to the PDF compression, which affected the resolution of the figures. We are happy to provide high-resolution versions of the figures separately for clarity. If the issue persists even with the higher resolution, we will consider removing the numbers, as suggested.

      - The legends of the synergy matrices (for example Fig 1D, 4E, 5, 6) are often extremely small, making it difficult to understand them intuitively. Please enlarge them and label them more clearly, and use larger fonts. In the legend of Figure 5D,E a green matrix indicating % live cells is mentioned but I don't see it. Do they mean the grey matrix? 

      We have enlarged the figure legends and will provide high-resolution versions of the figures to ensure all details are clearly readable. Regarding Figure 5D,E: we acknowledge that the color may appear differently (more green or gray) depending on the display or printer settings. To avoid any confusion, we have corrected the legend to specify that the color in question is khaki, rather than green. Moreover, following suggestions of the reviewer #2, these figures have been respectively moved to Figure S6B and S6C.

      - Figure S2. Perhaps I misunderstand the PML body experiment but the authors seem to use PML body formation to support their idea that AZD2858 blocks TopBP1 condensate formation and not just any condensate formation. However, if this is the case they would need a proper positive control, i.e. an additional experimental condition in which they do see PLM bodies. 

      Arsenic is a well-known positive control for experiments involving PML bodies due to its ability to induce specific responses in PML proteins and modify PML nuclear bodies (NBs) structure and function (Jaffray et al., 2023, JCB ; Zhu et al., 1997, PNAS). Thus, we used Arsenic as a positive control and observed a significant increase in PML NBs vs the other conditions (Kruskal-Wallis test) as indicated below. We thus implemented the results in the corresponding figure S2B and text.

      Author response image 3.

      PML condensates were tested after 2 h of incubation. AZD2858 : 100nM ; SN-38 : 300nM ; Arsenic : 6µM. ****: p<0.0001 (Kruskal-Wallis test).

      - The quantification of the flow cytometry data needs to be clarified. I find it strange that in the figures (for example Figure 3A and 3C) representative examples are shown of apparently 3 replicates, and that the percentages shown in these examples are then the given in the text as the overall numbers; for example on page 18 "...BrdU incorporation increased from 16.11% (SN38 alone) to 41.83% (combination)...". This type of description is done in multiple places in the Results section and is confusing. It would be clearer if the authors show proper quantifications (mean +/- sem) of the percentages of (the relevant) gated populations. Besides, I don't think it make a lot of sense to mention in the text the percentages with 2 decimals behind the comma. This suggests a level of precision that does not seem justified in flow cytometry data. Finally, all flow cytometry plots look visually very busy and all the text is crammed in with really small fonts. Cleaning them up and enlarging the fonts of the remaining text/numbers would really improve the readability of the figures. 

      Thank you for your helpful comments. We understand your concern regarding the flow cytometry quantification. Indeed, the percentages presented in the figures are derived from representative replicates, and we acknowledge that this presentation could be confusing. To address this, we have included a table summarizing the data from all replicates to improve readability [Table S2 and S3 in the new version]. Second, we specified in the text that the data are representative biological replicates when needed. Third, we have performed statistical analyses on the three replicates when necessary, as shown in Supplementary Figure S5C-F in the new version. The text has been revised to reflect the correct statistical interpretation.

      Regarding the use of two decimal, we are unable to remove them due to limitations in the software (Kaluza) used for flow cytometry analysis. However, we agree that this level of precision may not be warranted, and we have revised the text where appropriate to reduce confusion.

      - In Figure 5G the authors show that FOLFIRI + AZD2858 are synergistic in two SN-38-resistant cell lines. They conclude that this combination may overcome drug resistance. But tried to figure out the used FOLFIRI concentrations used in these cell lines and they still seem far higher than the SN-38-sensitive HCT116 cell lines, so I would like to see a bit more nuance in their interpretation. I think overcoming drug resistance is an overstatement, and perhaps alleviating would be a better term 

      Thank you for highlighting this important point; we have adjusted the text accordingly.

      - The legend in Table S2 refers to Figure 5A-B; this should be Figure 4A-B. 

      Thank you, this has been corrected and Table S2 is now moved to Table S4 .

      Reviewer #1 (Significance (Required)): 

      The finding that AZD2858 block TOPbp1 condensate formation via a pleiotropic effect of this compound is interesting and convincing. To my best knowledge it's a novel finding which is interesting to the potential target audience mentioned below. Their findings that inhibition of TOPbp1 condensation and ATR signaling via AZD2858 may synergize with FOLFIRI therapy in colorectal cancer cells are still very preliminary, because the effects on non-cancerous cells are not tested. 

      Researchers involved in early cancer drug discovery and cell biologists studying DNA damage responses in cancer cells seem to me typical audience interested and influenced by this paper. 

      I'm a cell biologist studying cell cycle fate decisions, and adaptation of cancer cells & stem cells to (drug-induced) stress. My expertise aligns well with the work presented throughout this paper. 

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)): 

      The authors have extended their previous research to develop TOPBP1 as a potential drug target for colorectal cancer by inhibiting its condensation. Utilizing an optogenetic approach, they identified the small molecule AZD2858, which inhibits TOPBP1 condensation and works synergistically with first-line chemotherapy to suppress colorectal cancer cell growth. The authors investigated the mechanism and discovered that disrupting TOPBP1 assembly inhibits the ATR/Chk1 signaling pathway, leading to increased DNA damage and apoptosis, even in drug-resistant colorectal cancer cell lines. Addressing the following concerns would enhance clarity and further in vivo work may improve significance: 

      (1) How does the optogenetic method for inducing condensates compare to the DNA damage induction mechanism? 

      Optogenetics provides a versatile and precise approach for controlling the condensation of scaffold proteins in both space and time. This method enables us to study the role of biomolecular condensates with minute-scale resolution, separating their formation from potentially confounding upstream events, such as DNA damage, and providing valuable insights into their specific function. Importantly, based on our previous publications on TopBP1 or SLX4 optogenetic condensates, we have substantial evidence indicating that light-induced condensates closely mimic those formed in response to DNA damage:

      - Functional similarity: Optogenetic condensates recapitulate endogenous condensates formed upon exposure of the cells of DNA damaging agents, and include most known partner proteins involved in the DNA damage response. It was shown for light induced-TopBP1 and SLX4 condensates (1-3).

      - Dynamic reversibility: Optogenetic condensates and DNA damage induced condensates are both dynamic and reversible. They dissolve within 15 minutes of light deactivation or after removal of the damaging agent (1,3).

      - Chromatin association: Both optogenetic and DNA damage-induced condensates are bound to chromatin or localized at sites of DNA damage (3).

      - Regulation: Both types of condensates are regulated similarly, with their formation triggered by the same signaling pathways. ATR basal activity drives the nucleation of opto-TopBP1 condensates and endogenous TopBP1 structures upon light exposure (1). Likewise, sumoylation modifications regulate the formation of opto-SLX4 condensates and endogenous SLX4 condensates (3).

      - Structurally: Using super-resolution imaging by stimulation-emission-depletion (STED) microscopy, we observed that endogenous SLX4 nanocondensates formed globular clusters that were indistinguishable from recombinant light induced SLX4 condensates (1,3).  

      (1) Frattini C, Promonet A, Alghoul E, Vidal-Eychenie S, Lamarque M, Blanchard MP, et al. TopBP1 assembles nuclear condensates to switch on ATR signaling. Molecular Cell. 18 mars 2021;81(6):1231-1245.e8. 

      (2) Alghoul E, Basbous J, Constantinou A. An optogenetic proximity labeling approach to probe the composition of inducible biomolecular condensates in cultured cells. STAR Protocols. 2021;2(3):100677. 

      (3) Alghoul E, Basbous J, Constantinou A. Compartmentalization of the DNA damage response: Mechanisms and functions. DNA Repair. août 2023;128:103524.

      (2) Why wasn't the initial screen conducted on the HCT116-SN50 resistant cell line? 

      Thank you for raising this important question, which we also considered at the outset of the project. After careful consideration, we decided to use the HCT116 WT cells in order to obtain initial data from an unmodified cell line. It is worth mentioning that HCT116-SN50 cells exhibit slower proliferation compared to WT cells, and they also express an efflux pump capable of pumping out SN38. We were concerned that these factors might interfere with the optogenetic assay, which is why we chose to perform the screen using the WT HCT116 cells.

      (3) The labels in Fig. 1D are difficult to recognize. 

      This issue was also raised by Reviewer #1. We suspect that the PDF conversion may have reduced the resolution of the figures, so we will provide them separately in high resolution. In addition, we have increased the size of some labels to improve their clarity.

      The selected cell image in Fig. 2A for SN-38 seems over-representative; unselected cells appear similar to other groups. Why does AZD2858 itself induce TopBP1 condensates in the plot, yet this is not evident in the images? 

      Thank you for your comment; we have updated the figure with a more representative image. We indeed observe that AZD2858 alone induces a slight increase in TopBP1 condensates. However, this increase did not lead to the activation of the ATR/Chk1 signaling pathway, as shown by the Western blot data presented in Fig. 2B. In addition, AZD2858 specifically prevents the formation of TopBP1 condensates induced by SN38 treatment, and the level of TopBP1 condensates does not return to the basal levels observed in untreated cells, but rather to those observed with AZD2858 treatment. During the 2-hour AZD2858 treatment, the progression of replication forks was unaffected (Fig. 3A and 3B). However, when AZD2858 was added alone to the Xenopus egg extracts, there was increased recruitment of TopBP1 to the chromatin (Fig. 2E). This result suggests that AZD2858 alone can induce the assembly of TopBP1 on chromatin to initiate DNA replication (a well-established role of TopBP1), but the number and concentration of TopBP1 molecules did not reach levels sufficient to activate the ATR/Chk1 pathway.

      (4) In Fig. 3A, despite the drastic change in the FACS plot shape, the quantifications appear quite similar. 

      Thank you for this insightful observation. The gates for the S phase were intentionally set wider to avoid biasing the results and inadvertently excluding the population that incorporates BrdU weakly (but still incorporates it) in the SN-38 only condition. As a result, the percentage of cells within this gate remains similar, even though the overall shape of the FACS plot changes, reflecting a shift in the distribution of BrdU incorporation. This point has now been clarified in the legend of the Figure 3A.

      This effect can also be attributed to the relatively short treatment time (2 hours), which captures early changes in DNA synthesis. The effect becomes more pronounced at later time points, as shown in Figure 3C. For example, after 6 hours of treatment, the percentage of BrdU-positive cells increases from 15% with SN-38 alone to 41% with the AZD2858 combination, demonstrating a clearer impact on DNA synthesis. A graph summarizing the statistical analysis has been added to Figure S5C for the 6-hour time point and Figure S5D for the 12-hour time point, based on data from three independent biological replicates.

      (5) The results section is imbalanced; Figs. 5 and 6 could be combined into one figure. 

      We have combined Figures 5 and 6 into a single figure to optimize the presentation of results. To avoid overloading the new figure, some of the data have been moved to supplementary figures, ensuring the main figure remains clear and focused.

      (6) An in vivo study is anticipated to assess the drug's efficacy. 

      Although AZD2858 was developed a few years ago, there is a limited amount of in vivo data available, which led us to consider potential issues related to the drug's biodistribution or its pharmacokinetics (PK). Despite these concerns, we proceeded with preliminary in vivo studies, testing various diluents and injection routes for AZD2858. However, we observed that the compound was not effective in vivo. Given the strong synergistic effects observed in vitro, we concluded that AZD2858 was likely not being distributed properly in the mice. As a result, we have decided to conduct a more detailed investigation into the pharmacokinetics (PK), pharmacodynamics (PD), and absorption, distribution, metabolism, and excretion (ADME) of AZD2858 to better understand its in vivo behavior and efficacy. Therefore, the in vivo evaluation of AZD2858 will be addressed in a separate study specifically focused on this aspect.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance (Required)): 

      Addressing the stated concerns would enhance clarity and further in vivo work may improve significance. 

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity (Required)): 

      Summary 

      In 2021 (PMID: 33503405) and 2024 (PMID: 38578830) Constantinou and colleagues published two elegant papers in which they demonstrated that the Topbp1 checkpoint adaptor protein could assemble into mesoscale phase-separated condensates that were essential to amplify activation of the PIKK, ATR, and its downstream effector kinase, Chk1, during DNA damage signalling. A key tool that made these studies possible was the use of a chimeric Topbp1 protein bearing a cryptochrome domain, Cry2, which triggered condensation of the chimeric Topbp1 protein, and thus activation of ATR and Chk1, in response to irradiation with blue light without the myriad complications associated with actually exposing cells to DNA damage. 

      In this current report Morano and co-workers utilise the same optogenetic Topbp1 system to investigate a different question, namely whether Topbp1 phase-condensation can be inhibited pharmacologically to manipulate downstream ATR-Chk1 signalling. This is of interest, as the therapeutic potential of the ATR-Chk1 pathway is an area of active investigation, albeit generally using more conventional kinase inhibitor approaches. 

      The starting point is a high throughput screen of 4730 existing or candidate small molecule anticancer drugs for compounds capable of inhibiting the condensation of the Topbp1-Cry2mCherry reporter molecule in vivo. A surprisingly large number of putative hits (>300) were recorded, from which 131 of the most potent were selected for secondary screening using activation of Chk1 in response to DNA damage induced by SN-38, a topoisomerase inhibitor, as a surrogate marker for Topbp1 condensation. From this the 10 most potent compounds were tested for interactions with a clinically used combination of SN-38 and 5-FU (FOLFIRI) in terms of cytotoxicity in HCT116 cells. The compound that synergised most potently with FOLFIRI, the GSK3-beta inhibitor drug AZD2858, was selected for all subsequent experiments. 

      AZD2858 is shown to suppress the formation of Topbp1 (endogenous) condensates in cells exposed to SN-38, and to inhibit activation of Chk1 without interfering with activation of ATM or other endpoints of damage signalling such as formation of gamma-H2AX or activation of Chk2 (generally considered to be downstream of ATM). AZD2858 therefore seems to selectively inhibit the Topbp1-ATR-Chk1 pathway without interfering with parallel branches of the DNA damage signalling system, consistent with Topbp1 condensation being the primary target. Importantly, neither siRNA depletion of GSK3-beta, or other GSK3-beta inhibitors were able to recapitulate this effect, suggesting it was a specific non-canonical effect of AZD2858 and not a consequence of GSK3-beta inhibition per se. 

      To understand the basis for synergism between AZD2858 and SN-38 in terms of cell killing, the effect of AZD2858 on the replication checkpoint was assessed. This is a response, mediated via ATR-Chk1, that modulates replication origin firing and fork progression in S-phase cell under conditions of DNA damage or when replication is impeded. SN-38 treatment of HCT116 cells markedly suppresses DNA replication, however this was partially reversed by co-treatment with AZD2858, consistent with the failure to activate ATR-Chk1 conferring a defect in replication checkpoint function. 

      Figures 4 and 5 demonstrate that AZD2858 can markedly enhance the cytotoxic and cytostatic effects of SN-38 and FOLFIRI through a combination of increased apoptosis and growth arrest according to dosage and treatment conditions. Figure 6 extends this analysis to cells cultured as spheroids, sometimes considered to better represent tumor responses compared to single cell cultures. 

      Major comments 

      Most of the data presented is of good technical quality and supports the conclusions drawn. There are however a small number of instances where this is not true; ie where the data are of insufficient technical quality, or where the description or interpretation of the results is at variance with the data which is presented. Some examples: 

      (1) Fig.2E - the claim that "we observed an increase in RPA, Topb1 and Pol-epsilon levels when CPT and AZD2858 were added together" do not seem to be justified by the data provided. It is also unclear what the purpose/ significance of this experiment is. 

      Thank you for pointing out the contradiction in Figure 2E. Upon review, we identified an error in the labeling of conditions (CPT and AZD2858 were inadvertently swapped). The corrected figure now clearly shows that, at the 60-minute timepoint after starting replication, the combination of

      CPT and AZD2858 results in a greater accumulation of TopBP1, Pol ε, and RPA on chromatin compared to CPT alone. We have revised the sentence to: "Our data demonstrate that combining CPT and AZD2858 earlier enhances the accumulation of replication-related factors (RPA, TopBP1, and Pol ε) on chromatin compared to CPT treatment alone, particularly visible at the 60minute after starting replication."

      The significance of this experiment lies in its connection to the earlier observation that AZD2858 restores BrdU incorporation when combined with SN-38, as shown in flow cytometry data (Figure 3A). At a molecular level, this was further supported by DNA fiber assays, which revealed that replication tracks (CldU tracts) were longer in the combination treatment compared to SN-38 alone (Figure 3B).

      To strengthen and validate these findings, we chose to employ the Xenopus egg extract system for several reasons. This model provides a highly controlled environment where DNA replication occurs without confounding effects from transcription or translation. Moreover, replication is limited to a single round, offering a unique opportunity to specifically interrogate replication mechanisms. These attributes make the Xenopus model an ideal system to confirm that AZD2858 facilitates replication recovery in the presence of replication stress induced by agents like CPT. This will lead, in longer treatment, to accumulation of DNA damage and apoptosis (Figure 3D-E and Figure 4A-D)

      (2) Figs. 3 A and C certainly show that the SN-38-mediated suppression of DNA synthesis is modified and partially alleviated by co-treatment with AZD2858. The statement however that "prolonged co-incubation with AZD2858 for 6 and 12 hours effectively abolished the SN-38 induced S-phase checkpoint" is clearly misleading. If this were true, then the BrdU incorporation profiles of the respective samples would be similar or identical to control, which clearly they are not. Clearly AZD2858 is affecting the imposition of the S-phase checkpoint in some way, but not "abolishing" it. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s detailed observations regarding Figures 3A and 3C and the phrasing in our manuscript. We agree that the term "abolished" is not precise in describing the effects of AZD2858 on the SN-38-induced S-phase checkpoint.

      To clarify: our data indicate that co-treatment with AZD2858 modifies and partially alleviates the SN-38-induced suppression of DNA synthesis, as demonstrated by increased BrdU incorporation relative to SN-38 treatment alone. However, as the reviewer correctly points out, the BrdU incorporation profiles of the co-treated samples do not fully return to control non treated cells levels. This suggests that while AZD2858 significantly mitigates the S-phase checkpoint, it does not completely abolish it.

      We have revised the statement in the manuscript to better reflect these findings, as follows: "Prolonged co-incubation with AZD2858 for 6 and 12 hours significantly alleviated the SN-38induced S-phase checkpoint, as evidenced by the partially increased BrdU incorporation. However, the population of co-treated cells is heterogeneous: some cells exhibit BrdU incorporation levels similar to those of untreated control cells, while others incorporate BrdU at levels comparable to cells treated with SN-38 alone. This indicates that AZD2858 does not fully restore DNA synthesis to control levels across the entire cell population."

      This revised phrasing aligns with the data presented and acknowledges the partial recovery of DNA synthesis observed. Thank you for bringing this to our attention and helping us improve the accuracy of our conclusions.

      (3) Fig. 3 E. The western blots of pDNA-PKcs (S2056) and total DNA-PKcs are really not interpretable. It is possible to sympathise that these reagents are probably extremely difficult to work with and obtain clear results, however uninterpretable results are not acceptable. 

      We agree that the data presented in the Fig3E are difficult to interpret. As noted by Reviewer 1, we recognize the challenge of obtaining clear and reliable results with these specific reagents. Based on this feedback, and to ensure the robustness of our conclusions, we have decided to exclude these specifics blots from the revised manuscript.

      We believe that this adjustment will enhance the clarity and reliability of the manuscript while focusing on the other, more interpretable data presented. Thank you for pointing this out, and we appreciate your understanding.

      (4) Fig. 3D. This is a puzzling image. Described as a PFGE assay, it presumably depicts an agarose gel, with intact genomic DNA at the top and a discrete band below representing fragmented genomic DNA. This is a little surprising, as fragmented genomic DNA does not usually appear as a specific band but as a heterogenous population or "smear". Nevertheless, even if one accepts this premise, it is unclear what is meant by "DSBs remained elevated after the combined treatment" when the intensity of this band is equivalent for both SN-38 and SN-38 + AZD2858 treatments. 

      We thank the reviewer for his insightful comments regarding the PFGE results in Figure 3D. We agree that the appearance of a discrete band, rather than a heterogeneous smear, is atypical for fragmented genomic DNA in this assay. However, by enhancing the signal intensity (as shown below), the expected smear becomes more appreciable.

      Author response image 4.

      Regarding the interpretation of the band intensities, we agree that the signals for SN-38 and SN38 + AZD2858 appear similar under these specific conditions. At the relatively high concentration of SN-38 used in this experiment (300 nM), it is indeed challenging to observe a more pronounced effect on DNA breaks. This is why we proposed the "DSBs remained elevated after the combined treatment" because the band intensity of SN-38 single agent treated cells or combined with AZD2858 is comparable. However, we note a slightly more intense γH2AX signal over time when AZD2858 is combined with SN-38 compared to SN-38 alone (Figure 3E). Furthermore, under lower, sub-optimal doses of SN-38 and over extended incubation treatment (48h), we observe a clearer increase in fragmented DNA bands, as demonstrated in Figure 4D.

      Minor comments 

      (1) Fig. 1. A surprisingly large number of compounds scored positive in the primary screen for inhibition of Topbp1 condensation (>300). Of the 131 of these selected for secondary screening using Chk1 activation (S345 phosphorylation) as a readout approximately 2/3 were negative, implying that a majority of the tested compounds inhibited Topbp1 condensation but not Chk1 activation. What could explain that?

      Thank you for this thoughtful comment. The discrepancy between the large number of compounds scoring positive for TopBP1 condensation inhibition and the smaller number inhibiting Chk1 activation (S345 phosphorylation) could be attributed to several factors:

      • Different cell lines and induction methods: The initial screen was conducted in HEK293 TrexFlpin cells overexpressing optoTopBP1, while the secondary screen used HCT116 cells. In addition, the methods used to induce the respective pathways were distinct: in the primary screen, we employed a blue light induction of opto-TopBP1 condensates, whereas in the secondary screen, we used an SN-38 treatment to induce DNA replication stress and activate the Chk1 pathway. These differences could account for the varying responses observed in the two screens.

      • The compounds that inhibited TopBP1 condensation might not fully block Chk1 activation. While they disrupt TopBP1 condensation, they may still allow for partial activation of Chk1 or Chk1 activation through alternative mechanisms. For instance, Chk1 activation could be mediated by other signaling pathways or molecules, such as ETAA1, a known Chk1 activator (1). Thus, TopBP1 condensation inhibition does not necessarily translate to complete inhibition of Chk1 activation, especially if ETAA1 is employed by cells as a rescue activator.

      • Some compounds may affect chromosome dynamics, potentially generating mechanical forces or torsional stress that could activate the ATR/Chk1 pathway independently of TopBP1

      (2).

      These factors suggest that while the compounds effectively disrupt TopBP1 condensation, they may not always fully inhibit the downstream Chk1 activation, pointing to the complexity of the DNA damage response pathways. 

      (1) Bass, T. E. et al. ETAA1 acts at stalled replication forks to maintain genome integrity. Nat Cell Biol 18, 1185–1195 (2016).

      (2) Kumar, A. et al. ATR Mediates a Checkpoint at the Nuclear Envelope in Response to Mechanical Stress. Cell 158, 633–646 (2014).

      (2) Fig. 2D. The protein-protein interaction assay shown demonstrates that AZD2858 ablates the light-induced auto-interaction between exogenous opto-Topbp1 molecules and ATR plus or minus SN-38, but clearly endogenous Topbp1 molecules do not participate. Why is this? 

      The biotin proximity labeling assay was conducted without exposing cells to light, using a TurboID module fused to TopBP1-mCherry-CRY2. Stable cell lines were then generated in HEK293 TrexFlpIn cells, where endogenous TopBP1 is still expressed. Upon adding doxycycline, the recombinant TurboID-TopBP1-mCherry-Cry2 (opto-TopBP1) is induced at levels comparable to endogenous TopBP1 (Fig 2D).

      Since the opto-TopBP1 construct exhibits behavior similar to that of endogenous TopBP1 (1), we used it to investigate whether TopBP1 self-assembly and its interaction with ATR are influenced by AZD2858 alone or in combination with SN38. Our results show that treatment with SN38 increases the proximity between opto-TopBP1 and the endogenous TopBP1 (not fused to TurboID). However, AZD2858, either alone or in combination with SN38, disrupts the selfassembly of recombinant TopBP1 with itself as well as its interaction with endogenous TopBP1.

      (1) Frattini C, Promonet A, Alghoul E, Vidal-Eychenie S, Lamarque M, Blanchard MP, et al. TopBP1 assembles nuclear condensates to switch on ATR signaling. Molecular Cell. 18 mars 2021;81(6):1231-1245.e8.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance (Required)): 

      Significance 

      Liquid phase separation of protein complexes is increasingly recognised as a fundamental mechanism in signal transduction and other cellular processes. One recent and important example was that of Topbp1, whose condensation in response to DNA damage is required for efficient activation of the ATR-Chk1 pathway. The current study asks a related but distinct question; can protein condensation be targeted by drugs to manipulate signalling pathways which in the main rely on protein kinase cascades? 

      Here, the authors identify an inhibitor of GSK3-beta as a novel inhibitor of DNA damage-induced Topbp1 condensation and thus of ATR-Chk1 signalling. 

      This work will be of interest to researchers in the fields of DNA damage signalling, biophysics of protein condensation, and cancer chemotherapy.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer 1:

      We thank the reviewer for his/her very positive comments.

      Reviewer 2:

      We thank the reviewer for his/her positive evaluation. We plan to add RNAseq data of yeast wild-type and JDP mutant strains as more direct readout for the role of Apj1 in controlling Hsf1 activity. We agree with the reviewer that our study includes one major finding: the central role of Apj1 in controlling the attenuation phase of the heat shock response. In accordance with the reviewer we consider this finding highly relevant and interesting for a broad readership. We agree that additional studies are now necessary to mechanistically dissect how the diverse JDPs support Hsp70 in controlling Hsf1 activity. We believe that such analysis should be part of an independent study but we will indicate this aspect as part of an outlook in the discussion section of a revised manuscript.

      Reviewer 3:

      We thank the reviewer for his/her suggestions. We agree that it is sometimes difficult to distinguish direct effects of JDP mutants on heat shock regulation from indirect ones, which can result from the accumulation of misfolded proteins that titrate Hsp70 capacity. We also agree that an in vitro reconstitution of Hsf1 displacement from DNA by Apj1/Hsp70 will be important, also to dissect Apj1 function mechanistically. We will add this point as outlook to the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      eLife Assessment

      This important and creative study finds that the uplift of the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau-via its resultant monsoon system rather than solely its high elevation-has shifted avian migratory directions from a latitudinal to a longitudinal orientation. However, the main claims are incomplete and only partially supported, as the reliance on eBird data-which lacks the resolution to capture population-specific teleconnections-combined with a limited tracking dataset covering only seven species leaves key aspects of the argument underdetermined, and the critical assumption of niche conservatism is not sufficiently foregrounded in the manuscript. More clearly communicating these limitations would significantly enhance the interpretability of the results, ensuring that the major conclusions are presented in the context of these essential caveats.

      We appreciate your positive comments and constructive suggestions. We fully acknowledge your concerns about clearly communicating the limitations associated with the data used and analytical assumptions. We will try to get more satellite tracking data of birds migrating across the plateau. We will carefully consider the insights that our paper can deliver and make sure the limitations of our datasets and the critical assumption of niche conservatism are clearly presented. By explicitly clarifying these caveats, we believe the transparency and interpretability of the findings will be much improved.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors have done a good job of responding to the reviewer's comments, and the paper is now much improved.

      Again, we thank the reviewer for constructive comments during review.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      I would like to thank the authors for the revision and the input they invested in this study.

      We are grateful for your thoughtful feedback and enthusiasms, which will help us improve our manuscript.

      With the revised text of the study, my earlier criticism holds, and your arguments about the counterfactual approach are irrelevant to that. The recent rise of the counterfactual approach might likely mirror the fact that there are too many scientists behind their computers, and few go into the field to collect in situ data. Studies like the one presented here are a good intellectual exercise but the real impact is questionable.

      We understand your question about the relevance of the counterfactual approach used in our study. Our intent in using a counterfactual scenario (reconstructing migration patterns assuming pre-uplift conditions on the QTP) was to isolate the potential influence of the plateau’s geological history on current migration routes. We agree that such an approach must be used properly. In the revision, we will explicitly clarify why this counterfactual comparison is useful – namely, it provides a theoretical baseline to test how much the QTP’s uplift (and the associated monsoon system) might have redirected migration paths. We acknowledge that the counterfactual results are theoretical and will explicitly emphasise the assumptions involved (e.g. species–environment relationships hold between pre- and post- lift environments) in the main text. Nonetheless, we defend the approach as a valuable study design: it helps generate testable hypotheses about migration (for instance, that the plateau’s monsoon-driven climate, rather than just its elevation, introduces an east–west shift en route). We will also tone down the language around this analysis to avoid overstating its real-world relevance. In summary, we will clarify that the counterfactual analysis is meant to complement, not replace, empirical observations, and we will discuss its limitations so that its role is appropriately bounded in the paper.

      All your main conclusions are inferred from published studies on 7! bird species. In addition, spatial sampling in those seven species was not ideal in relation to your target questions. Thus, no matter how fancy your findings look, the basic fact remains that your input data were for 7 bird species only! Your conclusion, “our study provides a novel understanding of how QTP shapes migration patterns of birds” is simply overstretching.

      Thank you for your comments. We apologise for any confusion regarding the scope of our dataset. Our main conclusions are not solely derived from seven bird species. Rather, we integrated a full list of 50 bird species that migrate across the QTP and analysed their migratory patterns with eBird data. We studied the factors influencing their choices of migratory routes with seven species that were among the few with available tracking data across the QTP. In this revision, we will clarify the role of these seven species and the rationale for their selection. Additionally, we attempt to include more satellite tracking data to improve spatial coverage, as recommended by the reviewer and editor. Based on discussions with potential collaborators, we will hopefully include a number of at least 10 more species with available tracking data.

      The way you respond to my criticism on L 81-93 is something different than what you admit in the rebuttal letter. The text of the ms is silent about the drawbacks and instead highlights your perspective. I understand you; you are trying to sell the story in a nice wrapper. In the rebuttal you state: “we assume species' responses to environments are conservative and their evolution should not discount our findings.” But I do not see that clearly stated in the main text.

      Thanks, as suggested we will clearly state the assumptions of niche conservatism in the Introduction.

      In your rebuttal, you respond to my criticism of "No matter how good the data eBird provides is, you do not know population-specific connections between wintering and breeding sites" when you responded: ... "we can track the movement of species every week, and capture the breeding and wintering areas for specific populations" I am having a feeling that you either play with words with me or do not understand that from eBird data nobody will be ever able to estimate population-specific teleconnections between breeding and wintering areas. It is simply impossible as you do not track individuals. eBird gives you a global picture per species but not for particular populations. You cannot resolve this critical drawback of your study.

      We agree that inferring population-specific migratory connections (teleconnections) from eBird data is challenging and inherently limited. eBird provides occurrence records for species, but it generally cannot distinguish which breeding population an individual bird came from or exactly where it goes for winter. However, in this study we intend to infer broad-scale movement patterns (e.g. general directions and stopover regions) rather than precise one-to-one population linkages. In the revision, we will carefully rephrase those sections to make clear that our inferences are at the species level and at large spatial scales. We will also explicitly state in the Discussion that confirming population connectivity would require targeted tracking or genetic studies, and that our eBird-based analysis can only suggest plausible routes and region-to-region linkages. We will contrast migratory routes identified by using eBird data and satellite tracking for the same species to check their similarity. We argue that, even with its limits, the eBird dataset can still yield useful insights (such as identifying major flyway corridors over the QTP).

      I am sorry that you invested so much energy into this study, but I see it as a very limited contribution to understanding the role of a major barrier in shaping migration.

      Thank you for recognising our efforts in the study. By integrating both satellite tracking and community-contributed data, we explored how the uplift of the QTP could shape avian migration across the area. We believe our findings provide important insights of how birds balance their responses to large-scale climate change and geological barrier, which yields the most comprehensive picture to date of how the QTP uplift shapes migratory patterns of birds. We will also acknowledge the study’s limitations to ensure that readers understand the context and constraints of our findings.

      My modest suggestion for you is: go into the field. Ideally use bird radars along the plateau to document whether the birds shift the directions when facing the barrier.

      We appreciate your suggestions to incorporate field tracking or radar studies to strengthen our results. All coauthors have years of field experiences, even on the QTP and Arctic. For example, the tracking data of peregrine falcons (Falco peregrinus) that we will incorporate in the revision are collected with during our own fieldwork in the Arctic for more than six years. We agree that more direct tracking (through GPS tagging or radar) would be an ideal way to validate migration pathways and population connectivity. In this revision, as stated above we will try to more species with satellite tracking data. We will also note that future studies should build on our findings by using dedicated tracking of more individual birds and radar monitoring of migration over the QTP. We will cite recent advances in these techniques and suggest that incorporating more tracking data could further test the hypotheses generated by our analyses.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      L55 "an important animal movement behaviour is.." Is there any unimportant animal movement? I mean this sentence is floppy, empty.

      We will rewrite this sentence to remove any ambiguous phrasing.

      L 152-154 This sentence is full of nonsense or you misinterpretation. First of all, the issue of inflexible initiation of migration was related to long-distance migrants only! The way you present it mixes apples and oranges (long- and short-distance migrants). It is not "owing to insufficient responses" but due to inherited patterns of when to take off, photoperiod and local conditions.

      We will remove the sentence to avoid misinterpretation.

      L 158 what is a migration circle? I do not know such a term.

      We will amend it as “annual migration cycle”, which is a more common way to describe the yearly round-trip journey between breeding and wintering grounds of birds.

      L 193 The way you present and mix capital and income breeding theory with your simulation study is quite tricky and super speculative.

      We will present this idea as an inference rather than a conclusion: “This pattern could be consistent with a ‘capital breeding’ strategy — where birds rely on energy reserves acquired before breeding — rather than an ‘income’ strategy that depends on food acquired during breeding. However, we note that this interpretation would require further study.” By adding this caution, we will make it clear that we are not asserting this link as proven fact, only suggesting it as one possible explanation. We will also double-check that the rest of the discussion around this point is framed appropriately.


      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      eLife Assessment

      This study addresses a novel and interesting question about how the rise of the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau influenced patterns of bird migration, employing a multi-faceted approach that combines species distribution data with environmental modeling. The findings are valuable for understanding avian migration within a subfield, but the strength of evidence is incomplete due to critical methodological assumptions about historical species-environment correlations, limited tracking data, and insufficient clarity in species selection criteria. Addressing these weaknesses would significantly enhance the reliability and interpretability of the results.

      We would like to thank you and two anonymous reviewers for your careful, thoughtful, and constructive feedback on our manuscript. These reviews made us revisit a lot of our assumptions and we believe the paper is much improved as a result. In addition to minor points, we have made three main changes to our manuscript in response to the reviews. First, we addressed the concerns on the assumptions of historical species-environment correlations from perspectives of both theoretical and empirical evidence. Second, we discussed the benefits and limitations of using tracking data in our study and demonstrate how the findings of our study are consolidated with results of previous studies. Third, we clarified our criteria for selecting species in terms of both eBird and tracking data.

      Below, we respond to each comment in turn. Once again, we thank you all for your feedback.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Strengths:

      This is an interesting topic and a novel theme. The visualisations and presentation are to a very high standard. The Introduction is very well-written and introduces the main concepts well, with a clear logical structure and good use of the literature. The methods are detailed and well described and written in such a fashion that they are transparent and repeatable.

      We are appreciative of the reviewer’s careful reading of our manuscript, encouraging comments and constructive suggestions.

      Weaknesses:

      I only have one major issue, which is possibly a product of the structure requirements of the paper/journal. This relates to the Results and Discussion, line 91 onwards. I understand the structure of the paper necessitates delving immediately into the results, but it is quite hard to follow due to a lack of background information. In comparison to the Methods, which are incredibly detailed, the Results in the main section reads as quite superficial. They provide broad overviews of broad findings but I found it very hard to actually get a picture of the main results in its current form. For example, how the different species factor in, etc.

      Yes, it is the journal request to format in this way (Methods follows the Results and Discussion) for the article type of short reports. As suggested, in the revision we have elaborated on details of our findings, in terms of (i) shifts of distribution of avian breeding and wintering areas under the influence of the uplift of the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau (Lines 102-116), and (ii) major factors that shape current migration patterns of birds in the plateau (Lines 118-138). We have also better referenced the approaches we used in the study.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study tries to assess how the rise of the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau affected patterns of bird migration between their breeding and wintering sites. They do so by correlating the present distribution of the species with a set of environmental variables. The data on species distributions come from eBird. The main issue lies in the problematic assumption that species correlations between their current distribution and environment were about the same before the rise of the Plateau. There is no ground truthing and the study relies on Movebank data of only 7 species which are not even listed in the study. Similarly, the study does not outline the boundaries of breeding sites NE of the Plateau. Thus it is absolutely unclear potentially which breeding populations it covers.

      We are very grateful for the careful review and helpful suggestions. We have revised the manuscript carefully in response to the reviewer’s comments and believe that it is much improved as a result. Below are our point-by-point replies to the comments.

      Strengths:

      I like the approach for how you combined various environmental datasets for the modelling part.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s encouragement.

      Weaknesses:

      The major weakness of the study lies in the assumption that species correlations between their current distribution and environments found today are back-projected to the far past before the rise of the Q-T Plateau. This would mean that species responses to the environmental cues do not evolve which is clearly not true. Thus, your study is a very nice intellectual exercise of too many ifs.

      This is a valid concern. We have addressed this from both the perspectives of the theoretical design of our study and empirical evidence.

      First, we agree with the reviewer that species responses to environmental cues might vary over time. Nonetheless, the simulated environments before the uplift of the plateau serve as a counterfactual state in our study. Counterfactual is an important concept to support causation claims by comparing what happened to what would have happened in a hypothetical situation: “If event X had not occurred, event Y would not have occurred” (Lewis 1973). Recent years have seen an increasing application of the counterfactual approach to detect biodiversity change, i.e., comparing diversity between the counterfactual state and real estimates to attribute the factors causing such changes (e.g., Gonzalez et al. 2023). Whilst we do not aim to provide causal inferences for avian distributional change, using the counterfactual approach, we are able to estimate the influence of the plateau uplift by detecting the changes of avian distributions, i.e., by comparing where the birds would have distributed without the plateau to where they currently distributed. We regard the counterfactual environments as a powerful tool for eliminating, to the extent possible, vagueness, as opposed to simply description of current distributions of birds. Therefore, we assume species’ responses to environments are conservative and their evolution should not discount our findings. We have clarified this in the Introduction (Lines 81-93).

      Second, we used species distribution modelling to contrast the distributions of birds before and after the uplift of the plateau under the assumption that species tend to keep their ancestral ecological traits over time (i.e., niche conservatism). This indicates a high probability for species to distribute in similar environments wherever suitable. Particularly, considering bird distributions are more likely to be influenced by food resources and vegetation distributions (Qu et al. 2010, Li et al. 2021, Martins et al. 2024), and the available food and vegetation before the uplift can provide suitable habitats for birds (Jia et al. 2020), we believe the findings can provide valuable insights into the influence of the plateau rise on avian migratory patterns. Having said that, we acknowledge other factors, e.g., carbon dioxide concentrations (Zhang et al. 2022), can influence the simulations of environments and our prediction of avian distribution. We have clarified the assumptions and evidence we have for the modelling in Methods (Lines 362-370).

      The second major drawback lies in the way you estimate the migratory routes of particular birds. No matter how good the data eBird provides is, you do not know population-specific connections between wintering and breeding sites. Some might overwinter in India, some populations in Africa and you will never know the teleconnections between breeding and wintering sites of particular species. The few available tracking studies (seven!) are too coarse and with limited aspects of migratory connectivity to give answer on the target questions of your study.

      We agree with the reviewer that establishing interconnections for birds is important for estimating the migration patterns of birds. We employed a dynamic model to assess their weekly distributions. Thus, we can track the movement of species every week, and capture the breeding and wintering areas for specific populations. That being said, we acknowledge that our approach can be subjected to the patchy sampling of eBird data. In contrast, tracking data can provide detailed information of the movement patterns of species but are limited to small numbers of species due to the considerable costs and time needed. We aimed to adopt the tracking data to examine the influence of focal factors on avian migration patterns, but only seven species, to the best of our ability, were acquired. Moreover, similar results were found in studies that used tracking data to estimate the distribution of breeding and wintering areas of birds in the plateau (e.g., Prosser et al. 2011, Zhang et al. 2011, Zhang et al. 2014, Liu et al. 2018, Kumar et al. 2020, Wang et al. 2020, Pu and Guo 2023, Yu et al. 2024, Zhao et al. 2024). We believe the conclusions based on seven species are rigour, but their implications could be restricted by the number of tracking species we obtained. We have better demonstrated how our findings on breeding and wintering areas of birds are reinforced by other studies reporting the locations of those areas. We have also added a separate caveat section to discuss the limitations stated above (Lines 202-215).

      Your set of species is unclear, selection criteria for the 50 species are unknown and variability in their migratory strategies is likely to affect the direction of the effects.

      In this revision, we have clarified the selection criteria for the 50 species and outlined the boundaries of the breeding areas of all birds (Lines 243-249). Briefly, we first obtained a full list of birds in the plateau from Prins and Namgail (2017). We then extracted species identified as full migrants in Birdlife International (https://datazone.birdlife.org/species/spcdistPOS) from the full list. Migratory birds may follow a capital or income migratory strategy depending on how much birds ingest endogenous reserved energy gained prior to reproduction. We have added discussions on how these migratory strategies might influence the effects of environment on migratory direction (Lines 183-200).

      In addition, the position of the breeding sites relative to the Q-T plate will affect the azimuths and resulting migratory flyways. So in fact, we have no idea what your estimates mean in Figure 2.

      We calculated the azimuths not only by the angles between breeding sites and wintering sites but also based on the angles between the stopovers of birds. Therefore, the azimuths are influenced by the relative positions of breeding, wintering and stopover sites. This would minimize the possible errors by just using breeding areas such as the biases caused by relative locations of breeding areas to the QTP as the reviewer pointed. We have better explained this both in the Introduction, Methods and legend of Figure 2.

      There is no way one can assess the performance of your statistical exercises, e.g. performances of the models.

      As suggested, we have reported Area Under the Curve (AUC) of the Receiver Operator Characteristic (ROC)assess the performances of the models (Table S1). AUC is a threshold-independent measurement for discrimination ability between presence and random points (Phillips et al. 2006). When the AUC value is higher than 0.75, the model was considered to be good (Elith et al. 2006). (Lines 379-383).

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      This is an interesting topic and a novel theme. The visualisations and presentation are to a very high standard. The Introduction is very well-written and introduces the main concepts well, with a clear logical structure and good use of the literature. The Methods are detailed and well described and written in such a fashion that they are transparent and repeatable.

      I only have one major issue, which is possibly a product of the structure requirements of the paper/journal. With the Results and Discussion, line 91 onwards. I understand the structure of the paper necessitates delving immediately into the results, but it is quite hard to follow due to a lack of background information. In comparison to the Methods, which are incredibly detailed, the Results in the main section read quite superficial. They provide broad overviews of broad findings but I found it very hard to actually get a picture of the main results in its current form. For example, how the different species factor in, etc.

      Please see our responses above.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Methodological issues:

      Line 219 Why have you selected only 64 species and what were the selection criteria?

      We have clarified the selection criteria (Lines 243-248). Briefly, we first obtained a full list of birds in the plateau from Prins and Namgail (2017). We then extracted species identified as full migrants in Birdlife International (https://datazone.birdlife.org/species/spcdistPOS) from the full list.

      Minor:

      Line 219 eBird has very uneven distribution, especially in vast areas of Russia. How can your exercise on Lines 232-238 overcome this issue?

      Yes, eBird data can be biased due to patchy sampling and variation of observers’ skills in identifying species. To address this issue, we have developed an adaptive spatial-temporal modelling (stemflow; Chen et al. 2024) to correct the imbalance distribution of data and modelled the observer experience to address the bias in recognising species. The stemflow was developed based on a machine learning modelling framework (AdaSTEM) which leverages the spatio-temporal adjacency information of sample points to model occurrence or abundance of species at different scales. It has been frequently used in modelling eBird data (Fink et al. 2013, Johnston et al. 2015, Fink et al. 2020) and has been proven to be efficient and advanced in multi-scale spatiotemporal data modelling. We have better explained this (Lines 251-270; Lines 307-321).

      Line 54 This sentence sounds very empty and in fact does not tell us much.

      We have adjusted this sentenced to “Animal movement underpins species’ spatial distributions and ecosystem processes”.

      Line 55 Again a sentence that implies a causality of the annual cycle to make the species migrate. It does not make sense.

      We have revised this sentence as “An important animal movement behaviour is migrating between breeding and wintering grounds”.

      Line 58 How is our fascination with migratory journeys related to the present article? I think this line is empty.

      We have changed this sentence to “Those migratory journeys have intrigued a body of different approaches and indicators to describe and model migration, including migratory direction, speed, timing, distance, and staging periods”.

      Figure 1 - ABC insets are OK, but a combination of lati- and longitudinal patterns is possible, e.g. in species with conservative strategies or for whatever other reason.

      Thank you for the suggestion. We kept the ABC insets rather than combining them together as we believe this can deliver a clear structure of influence of QTP uplift under different scenarios.

      The legend to Figure 2 is not self-explanatory. Please make it clear what the response variable is and its units. The first line of the legend should read something like The influence of environmental factors on the direction of avian migration.

      Thank you. We have amended the legends of Figure 2 as suggested:

      “Figure 2. The influence of environmental factors on the direction of avian migration.  Migratory directions are calculated based on the azimuths between each adjacent stopover, breeding and wintering areas for each species. We employ multivariate linear regression models under the Bayesian framework to measure the correlation between environmental factors and avian migratory directions. Wind represents the wind cost calculated by wind connectivity. Vegetation is measured by the proportion of average vegetation cover in each pixel (~1.9° in latitude by 2.5° in longitude). Temperature is the average annual temperature. Precipitation is the average yearly precipitation. All environmental layers are obtained using the Community Earth System Model. West QTP, central QTP, and East QTP denote areas in the areas west (longitude < 73°E), central (73°E ≤ longitude < 105°E), and east of (longitude ≥ 105°E) the Qinghai-Tibet Plateau, respectively.”

      References

      Chen, Y., Z. Gu, and X. Zhan. 2024. stemflow: A Python Package for Adaptive Spatio-Temporal Exploratory Model. Journal of Open Source Software 9:6158.

      Elith, J., C. H. Graham, R. P. Anderson, M. Dudík, S. Ferrier, A. Guisan, R. J. Hijmans, F. Huettmann, J. R. Leathwick, A. Lehmann, J. Li, L. G. Lohmann, B. A. Loiselle, G. Manion, C. Moritz, M. Nakamura, Y. Nakazawa, J. McC. M. Overton, A. Townsend Peterson, S. J. Phillips, K. Richardson, R. Scachetti-Pereira, R. E. Schapire, J. Soberón, S. Williams, M. S. Wisz, and N. E. Zimmermann. 2006. Novel methods improve prediction of species' distributions from occurrence data. Ecography 29:129-151.

      Fink, D., T. Auer, A. Johnston, V. Ruiz-Gutierrez, W. M. Hochachka, and S. Kelling. 2020. Modeling avian full annual cycle distribution and population trends with citizen science data. Ecological Applications 30:e02056.

      Fink, D., T. Damoulas, and J. Dave. 2013. Adaptive Spatio-Temporal Exploratory Models: Hemisphere-wide species distributions from massively crowdsourced eBird data. Pages 1284-1290 in Proceedings of the AAAI Conference on Artificial Intelligence.

      Gonzalez, A., J. M. Chase, and M. I. O'Connor. 2023. A framework for the detection and attribution of biodiversity change. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 378.

      Jia, Y., H. Wu, S. Zhu, Q. Li, C. Zhang, Y. Yu, and A. Sun. 2020. Cenozoic aridification in Northwest China evidenced by paleovegetation evolution. Palaeogeography, Palaeoclimatology, Palaeoecology 557:109907.

      Johnston, A., D. Fink, M. D. Reynolds, W. M. Hochachka, B. L. Sullivan, N. E. Bruns, E. Hallstein, M. S. Merrifield, S. Matsumoto, and S. Kelling. 2015. Abundance models improve spatial and temporal prioritization of conservation resources. Ecological Applications 25:1749-1756.

      Kumar, N., U. Gupta, Y. V. Jhala, Q. Qureshi, A. G. Gosler, and F. Sergio. 2020. GPS-telemetry unveils the regular high-elevation crossing of the Himalayas by a migratory raptor: implications for definition of a “Central Asian Flyway”. Scientific Reports 10:15988.

      Lewis, D. 1973. Counterfactuals. Oxford: Blackwell.

      Li, S.-F., P. J. Valdes, A. Farnsworth, T. Davies-Barnard, T. Su, D. J. Lunt, R. A. Spicer, J. Liu, W.-Y.-D. Deng, J. Huang, H. Tang, A. Ridgwell, L.-L. Chen, and Z.-K. Zhou. 2021. Orographic evolution of northern Tibet shaped vegetation and plant diversity in eastern Asia. Science Advances 7:eabc7741.

      Liu, D., G. Zhang, H. Jiang, and J. Lu. 2018. Detours in long-distance migration across the Qinghai-Tibetan Plateau: individual consistency and habitat associations. PeerJ 6:e4304.

      Martins, L. P., D. B. Stouffer, P. G. Blendinger, K. Böhning-Gaese, J. M. Costa, D. M. Dehling, C. I. Donatti, C. Emer, M. Galetti, R. Heleno, Í. Menezes, J. C. Morante-Filho, M. C. Muñoz, E. L. Neuschulz, M. A. Pizo, M. Quitián, R. A. Ruggera, F. Saavedra, V. Santillán, M. Schleuning, L. P. da Silva, F. Ribeiro da Silva, J. A. Tobias, A. Traveset, M. G. R. Vollstädt, and J. M. Tylianakis. 2024. Birds optimize fruit size consumed near their geographic range limits. Science 385:331-336.

      Phillips, S. J., R. P. Anderson, and R. E. Schapire. 2006. Maximum entropy modeling of species geographic distributions. Ecological Modelling 190:231-259.

      Prins, H. H. T., and T. Namgail. 2017. Bird migration across the Himalayas : wetland functioning amidst mountains and glaciers. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.

      Prosser, D. J., P. Cui, J. Y. Takekawa, M. Tang, Y. Hou, B. M. Collins, B. Yan, N. J. Hill, T. Li, Y. Li, F. Lei, S. Guo, Z. Xing, Y. He, Y. Zhou, D. C. Douglas, W. M. Perry, and S. H. Newman. 2011. Wild Bird Migration across the Qinghai-Tibetan Plateau: A Transmission Route for Highly Pathogenic H5N1. Plos One 6:e17622.

      Pu, Z., and Y. Guo. 2023. Autumn migration of black-necked crane (Grus nigricollis) on the Qinghai-Tibetan and Yunnan-Guizhou plateaus. Ecology and Evolution 13:e10492.

      Qu, Y., F. Lei, R. Zhang, and X. Lu. 2010. Comparative phylogeography of five avian species: implications for Pleistocene evolutionary history in the Qinghai-Tibetan plateau. Molecular Ecology 19:338-351.

      Wang, Y., C. Mi, and Y. Guo. 2020. Satellite tracking reveals a new migration route of black-necked cranes (Grus nigricollis) in Qinghai-Tibet Plateau. PeerJ 8:e9715.

      Yu, X., G. Song, H. Wang, Q. Wei, C. Jia, and F. Lei. 2024. Migratory flyways and connectivity of Brown Headed Gulls (Chroicocephalus brunnicephalus) revealed by GPS tracking. Global Ecology and Conservation 56:e03340.

      Zhang, G.-G., D.-P. Liu, Y.-Q. Hou, H.-X. Jiang, M. Dai, F.-W. Qian, J. Lu, T. Ma, L.-X. Chen, and Z. Xing. 2014. Migration routes and stopover sites of Pallas’s Gulls Larus ichthyaetus breeding at Qinghai Lake, China, determined by satellite tracking. Forktail 30:104-108.

      Zhang, G.-G., D.-P. Liu, Y.-Q. Hou, H.-X. Jiang, M. Dai, F.-W. Qian, J. Lu, Z. Xing, and F.-S. Li. 2011. Migration Routes and Stop-Over Sites Determined with Satellite Tracking of Bar-Headed Geese (Anser indicus) Breeding at Qinghai Lake, China. Waterbirds 34:112-116, 115.

      Zhang, R., D. Jiang, C. Zhang, and Z. Zhang. 2022. Distinct effects of Tibetan Plateau growth and global cooling on the eastern and central Asian climates during the Cenozoic. Global and Planetary Change 218:103969.

      Zhao, T., W. Heim, R. Nussbaumer, M. van Toor, G. Zhang, A. Andersson, J. Bäckman, Z. Liu, G. Song, M. Hellström, J. Roved, Y. Liu, S. Bensch, B. Wertheim, F. Lei, and B. Helm. 2024. Seasonal migration patterns of Siberian Rubythroat (Calliope calliope) facing the Qinghai–Tibet Plateau. Movement Ecology 12:54.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Overall, I find only two minor weaknesses. First, the insights of this study are, first and foremost, of feed-forward nature, and a feed-forward network would have been enough (and the more parsimonious model) to illustrate the results. While using a recurrent neural network (RNN) shows that the results are, in general, compatible with recurrent dynamics, the specific limitations imposed by RNNs (e.g., dynamical stability, low-dimensional internal dynamics) are not the focus of this study. Indeed, the additional RNN models in the supplementary material show that under more constrained conditions for the RNN (low-dimensional dynamics), using the input control alone runs into difficulties.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point. While we agree that recurrent dynamics were not the focus of this study, we would like to point out that 1) dynamics, of some kind, are necessary to simulate the decoder fitting process and 2) recurrent neural networks (RNNs) are valuable for obtaining general insights on how biological constraints shape the reachable manifold:

      (1) To simulate the decoder fitting process, we had to simulate neural activity during the so-called “calibration task”. Some dynamics to these responses are necessary to produce a population response with dimensionality resembling what was found in experiments (10 dimensions). Moreover, dynamics are necessary to create a common direction of high variance across population responses to the calibration task stimuli (see Supplementary Figure 2a and surrounding discussion), which is necessary to reproduce the biases in readouts demonstrated in Figure 4 (as many within-manifold decoder perturbations are aligned with it; Supplementary Figure 2b).

      Because feed-forward networks lack dynamics, reproducing our results with a feed-forward network would require using an input with dynamics. Rather than making an arbitrary choice for these input dynamics, we chose to keep the input static and instead generate the dynamics with a RNN, which is in line with recent models of motor cortex.

      We agree, however, that this is an important point worth clarifying in the manuscript. In our revision we will aim to add a demonstration of how to reproduce a subset of our results with a feed-forward network and a dynamic input.

      (2) While we agree that RNNs impose certain limitations over feed-forward networks, we see these limitations as an advantage because they provide a framework for understanding the structure of the reachable manifold in terms of biological constraints. For example, our simulations in Supplementary Figure 1 show that the dimensionality of the reachable manifold is highly dependent on recurrent connectivity: inhibition-stabilized connectivity makes it higher-dimensional whereas task-specific optimized connectivity makes it lower-dimensional. Such insights are valuable to understand the broader implications and experimental predictions of the re-aiming strategy.

      Because feed-forward networks are untied from the reality of recurrent cortical circuitry, they cannot be characterized in terms of such biological constraints. For instance, as the reviewer points out, dynamical stability is not a well-defined property of feed-forward networks. Such models therefore cannot provide any insight into how the biological constraint of dynamical stability could influence the reachable manifold (which we show it does in Figure 5b). Relatedly, feed-forward networks cannot be optimized to solve complex spatiotemporal tasks like the ballistic reaching task we used for our task-optimized RNN (Supplementary Figure 1, right column), so cannot be used to understand how such behavioral constraints would influence the reachable manifold.

      We agree that these reasons for using RNNs are subtle and left implicit in how they are currently exposed in the text. We will add a discussion point clarifying these in our revision.

      Second, explaining the quantitative differences between the model and data for shifts in tuning curves seems to take the model a bit too literally. The model serves greatly for qualitative observations. I assume, however, that many of the unconstrained aspects of the model would yield quantitatively different results.

      We completely agree: our model is best used to provide a qualitative description of the capabilities of the re-aiming strategy. We will be sure to revise our manuscript to keep such quantitative comparisons at a minimum.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The authors mention alternative models (eg, based on synaptic plasticity in the RNN and/or input weights) that can explain the same experimental data that they do, they do not provide any direct comparisons to those models. Thus, the main argument that the authors have in favor of their model is the fact that it is more plausible because it relies on performing the optimization in a low-dimensional space. It would be nice to see more quantitative arguments for why the re-aiming strategy may be more plausible than synaptic plasticity (either by showing that it explains data better, or explaining why it may be more optimal in the context of fast learning).

      We agree this remains a limitation of our study. To contrast our re-aiming model with models of synaptic plasticity (in the input and/or recurrent weights), we have included substantial discussion of these alternative models in two sections of the manuscript:

      • Introduction, where we elaborate on the argument that synaptic plasticity requires solving an exceptionally difficult optimization problem in high dimensions

      • Discussion section “The role of synaptic plasticity in BCI learning”, where we review a number of synaptic plasticity models and experimental results they can account for

      We fully agree that more quantitative comparisons remain an important follow-up to this line of research. However, it is worth noting that there are many such models out there. Moreover, as is the case with many computational models, the results one can achieve with any given model can be highly sensitive to a number of different hyperparameters (e.g. learning rates). We therefore feel that a more rigorous comparison requires deeper study and is out of scope of this manuscript.

      In particular, the authors model the adaptation to outside-manifold perturbations (OMPs) through a "generalized re-aiming strategy". This assumes the existence of additional command variables, which are not used in the original decoding task, but can then be exploited to adapt to these OMPs. While this model is meant to capture the fact that optimization is occurring in a low-dimensional subspace, the fact that animals take longer to adapt to OMPs suggests that WMPs and OMPs may rely on different learning mechanisms, and that synaptic plasticity may actually be a better model of adaptation to OMPs. 

      We thank the reviewer for raising this question. We agree that the fact that animals take longer to adapt to OMPs suggests that the underlying learning strategy is somehow different. But the argument we try to make in this section of the paper is that it in fact does not require an entirely different mechanism. Our simulations show that the same mechanism of re-aiming can suffice to learn OMPs, but it simply requires re-aiming in the larger space of all command variables available to the motor system (rather than just the two command variables evoked by the calibration task). Because this is a much higher-dimensional search space (10-20 vs. 2 dimensions, which is a substantial difference due to the curse of dimensionality), we argue that learning should be slower, even though the mechanism (i.e. re-aiming) is the same.

      This is an important and somewhat surprising takeaway from these simulations, which we will try to bring up more explicitly and clearly in the revision.

      It would be important to discuss how exactly generalized re-aiming would differ from allowing plasticity in the input weights, or in all weights in the network. Do those models make different predictions, and could they be differentiated in future experiments?

      They do in fact make different predictions, and we thank the reviewer for asking and pointing out the lack of discussion of this point. The key difference between these two learning mechanisms is demonstrated in Figure 5b: under generalized re-aiming, there is a fundamental limit to the set of activity patterns one can learn to produce in the brain-computer interface (BCI) learning task. This is quantified in that analysis by the asymptotic participation ratio of the reachable manifold as K increases, which indicates that there is a limited ~12-dimensional subspace that the reachable manifold can occupy. The specific orientation of this subspace is determined by the (recurrent and input) connectivity of the recurrent neural network. With synaptic plasticity in any of the weight matrices (Wrec,Win,U), this subspace could be re-oriented in any arbitrary direction. Our theory of “generalized re-aiming” therefore predicts that the reachable manifold is 1) constrained to a low-d subspace and 2) is not modified when learning BCIs with outside-manifold perturbations.

      Experimentally testing this would require a within-/outside- manifold perturbation BCI learning task akin to that of Sadtler et al, but where the “intrinsic manifold” is measured from population responses evoked by every possible motor command so as to entirely contain the full reachable manifold at max K. This would require measuring motor cortical activity during naturalistic behavior under a wide range of conditions, rather than just in response to the 2D cursor movements on the screen used in the calibration task of the original study. In this case, learning outside-manifold perturbations would require re-orienting the reachable manifold, so a pure generalized re-aiming strategy would fail to learn them. Synaptic plasticity, on the other hand, would not.

      We will be sure to elaborate further on this claim in the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The manuscript's logical flow is challenging and hard to follow, and key arguments could be more clearly structured, particularly in transitions between mechanistic components.

      We will revise our manuscript so as to make it easy to follow the logical flow in transitions between mechanistic components.

      (2) The causality between stress-induced α2A-AR internalization and the enhanced MAO-A remains unclear. Direct experimental evidence is needed to determine whether α2A-AR internalization itself or Ca<sup>2+</sup> drives MAO-A activation, and how they activate MAO-A should be considered.

      We believe that the causality between stress-induced α2A-AR internalization and the enhancement of MAO-A is clearly demonstrated by our current experiments, while our explanations may be improved by making them easier to understand especially for those who are not expert on electrophysiology.

      Firstly, it is well established that autoinhibition in LC neurons is mediated by α2A-AR coupled-GIRK (Arima et al., 1998, J Physiol; Williams et al., 1985, Neuroscience). We found that spike frequency adaptation in LC neurons was also mediated by α2A-AR coupled GIRK-I (Fig. 1A-I), and that α2A-AR coupled GIRK-I underwent [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub>-dependent rundown (Figs. 2, S1, S2), leading to an abolishment of spike-frequency adaptation (Figs. S4). [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub>-dependent rundown of α2A-AR coupled GIRK-I was prevented by barbadin (Fig 2G-J), which prevents the internalization of G-protein coupled receptor (GPCR) channels.

      Abolishment of spike frequency adaptation itself, i.e., “increased spike activity” can increase [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> because [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> is entirely dependent on the spike activity as shown by Ca<sup>2+</sup> imaging method in Figure S3.

      Thus, α2A-AR internalization can increase [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> through the abolishment of autoinhibition or spike frequency adaptation, and a [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> increase drives MAO-A activation as reported previously (Cao et al., 2007, BMC Neurosci). The mechanism how Ca<sup>2+</sup> activates MAO-A is beyond the scope of the current study.

      Our study just focused on the mechanism how chronic or sever stress can cause persistent overexcitation and how it results in LC degeneration.

      (3) The connection between α2A-AR internalization and increased cytosolic NA levels lacks direct quantification, which is necessary to validate the proposed mechanism.

      Direct quantification of the relationship between α2A-AR internalization and increased cytosolic NA levels may not be possible, and may not be necessarily needed to be demonstrated as explained below.

      The internalization of α2A-AR can increase [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> through the abolishment of autoinhibition or spike frequency adaptation, and [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> increases can facilitate NA autocrine (Huang et al., 2007), similar to the transmitter release from nerve terminals (Kaeser & Regehr, 2014, Annu Rev Physiol).

      Autocrine released NA must be re-uptaken by NAT (NA transporter), which is firmly established (Torres et al., 2003, Nat Rev Neurosci). Re-uptake of NA by NAT is the only source of intracellular NA, and NA re-uptake by NAT should be increased as the internalization of NA biding site (α2A-AR) progresses in association with [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> increases (see page 11, lines 334-336).

      Thus, the connection between α2A-AR internalization and increased cytosolic NA levels is logically compelling, and the quantification of such connection may not be possible at present (see the response to the comment made by the Reviewer #1 as Recommendations for the authors (2) and beyond the scope of our current study.

      (4) The chronic stress model needs further validation, including measurements of stress-induced physiological changes (e.g., corticosterone levels) to rule out systemic effects that may influence LC activity. Additional behavioral assays for spatial memory impairment should also be included, as a single behavioral test is insufficient to confirm memory dysfunction.

      It is well established that restraint stress (RS) increases corticosterone levels depending on the period of RS (García-Iglesias et al., 2014, Neuropharmacology), although we are not reluctant to measure the corticosterone levels. In addition, there are numerous reports that showed the increased activity of LC neurons in response to various stresses (Valentino et al., 1983; Valentino and Foote, 1988; Valentino et al., 2001; McCall et al., 2015), as described in the text (page 4, lines 96-98). Measurement of cortisol levels may not be able to rule out systemic effects of CRS on the whole brain.

      We had already done another behavioral test using elevated plus maze (EPM) test.

      By combining the two tests, it may be possible to more accurately evaluate the results of Y-maze test by differentiating the memory impairment from anxiety. However, the results obtained by these behavioral tests are just supplementary to our current aim to elucidate the cellular mechanisms for the accumulation of cytosolic free NA. Its subsequent anxiety and memory impairment are just supplementary to our current study. We will soften the implication of anxiety and memory impairment.

      (5) Beyond b-arrestin binding, the role of alternative internalization pathways (e.g., phosphorylation, ubiquitination) in α2A-AR desensitization should be considered, as current evidence is insufficient to establish a purely Ca<sup>2+</sup>-dependent mechanism.

      We can hardly agree with this comment.

      It was clearly demonstrated that repeated application of NA itself did not cause desensitization of α2A-AR (Figure S1A-D), and that the blockade of b-arrestin binding by barbadin completely suppressed the Ca<sup>2+</sup>-dependent downregulation of GIRK (Fig. 2G-K). These observations can clearly rule out the possible involvement of phosphorylation or ubiquitination for the desensitization.

      Not only the barbadin experiment, but also the immunohistochemistry and western blot method clearly demonstrated the decrease of α2A-AR expression on the cell membrane (Fig. 3).

      Ca<sup>2+</sup>-dependent mechanism of the rundown of GIRK was convincingly demonstrated by a set of different protocols of voltage-clamp study, in which Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx was differentially increased. The rundown of GIRK-I was orderly potentiated or accelerated by increasing the number of positive command pulses each of which induces Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx (compare Figure S1E-J, Figure S2A-E and Figure S2F-K along with Fig. 2A-F). The presence or absence of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents and the amount of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents determined the trend of the rundown of GIRK-I (Figs. 2, S1 and S2). Because the same voltage protocol hardly caused the rundown when it did not induce Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents in the absence of TEA (Fig. S1F; compare with Fig. 2B), blockade of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents by nifedipine would not be so beneficial.

      We believe the series of voltage-clamp protocols convincingly demonstrated the orderly involvement of [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> in accelerating the rundown of GIRK-I.

      (6) NA leakage for free NA accumulation is also influenced by NAT or VMAT2. Please discuss the potential role of VMAT2 in NA accumulation within the LC in AD.

      We will discuss the role of VMAT2 in NA accumulation, especially when VMAT2 was impaired. Indeed, it has been demonstrated that reduced VMAT2 levels increased susceptibility to neuronal damage: VMAT2 heterozygote mice displayed increased vulnerability to MPTP as evidenced by reductions in nigral dopamine cell counts (Takahashi et al, 1997, PNAS). Thus, when the activity of VMAT2 in LC neurons were impaired by chronic restraint stress, cytosolic NA levels in LC neurons would increase. We will add such discussion in the revised manuscript.

      (7) Since the LC is a small brain region, proper staining is required to differentiate it from surrounding areas. Please provide a detailed explanation of the methodology used to define LC regions and how LC neurons were selected among different cell types in brain slices for whole-cell recordings.

      LC neurons were identified immunohistochemically and electrophysiologically as we previously reported (see Fig. 2 in Front. Cell. Neurosci. 16:841239. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2022.841239). A delayed spiking pattern in response to depolarizing pulses (Figure S9) applied at a hyperpolarized membrane potential was commonly observed in LC neurons in many studies (Masuko et al., 1986; van den Pol et al., 2002; Wagner-Altendorf et al., 2019).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The manuscript reports that chronic stress for 5 days increases MAO-A levels in LC neurons, leading to the production of DOPEGAL, activation of AEP, and subsequent tau cleavage into the tau N368 fragment, ultimately contributing to neuronal damage. However, the authors used wild-type C57BL/6 mice, and previous literature has indicated that AEP-mediated tau cleavage in wild-type mice is minimal and generally insufficient to cause significant behavioral alterations. Please clarify and discuss this apparent discrepancy.

      In our study, normalized relative value of AEP-mediated tau cleavage (Tau N368) was much higher in CRS mice than non-stress wild-type mice. It is not possible to compare AEP-mediated tau cleavage between our non-stress wild type mice and those observed in previous study (Zhang et al., 2014, Nat Med), because band intensity is largely dependent on the exposure time and its numerical value is the normalized relative value. In view of such differences, our apparent band expression might have been intensified to detect small changes.

      (2) It is recommended that the authors include additional experiments to examine the effects of different durations and intensities of stress on MAO-A expression and AEP activity. This would strengthen the understanding of stress-induced biochemical changes and their thresholds.

      GIRK rundown was almost saturated after 3-day RS and remained the same in 5-day RS mice (Fig. 4A-G), which is consistent with the downregulation of α2A-AR and GIRK1 expression by 3-day RS (Fig. 3C, F and G; Fig. 4J and K). However, we examine the protein levels of MAO-A, pro/active-AEP and Tau N368 only in 5-day RS mice without examining in 3-day RS mice. This is because we considered the possibility that 3-day RS may be insufficient to induce changes in MAO-A, AEP and Tau N368 and some period of high [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> condition may be necessary to induce such changes. We will discuss this in the revised manuscript.

      (3) Please clarify the rationale for the inconsistent stress durations used across Figures 3, 4, and 5. In some cases, a 3-day stress protocol is used, while in others, a 5-day protocol is applied. This discrepancy should be addressed to ensure clarity and experimental consistency.

      Please see our response to the comment (2).

      (4) The abbreviation "vMAT2" is incorrectly formatted. It should be "VMAT2," and the full name (vesicular monoamine transporter 2) should be provided at first mention.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We will revise accordingly.

    1. Author response:

      We thank the referees for finding our work well written and systematic. We are planning a revision of the manuscript based on the public review and the confidential recommendations of the referees.

      The role of axons:

      Indeed, radial axon projections appear before mature epithelial stripes in the cornea (Iannaccone et al., 2012). Our claim is, however, not that guidance cues are absent, but that global cues are unnecessary. The alignment term in our model, together with evidence that corneal epithelial cells follow contact-mediated substrate cues (Walczysko et al., 2016), show that corneal cells migration is responsive to external forces, and the underlying patterns of axonal projections could be one of those cues.

      Experiments (Collinson et al., 2002) and simulations in this work show that a rapid spiral epithelial flow forms first, with cells migrating radially for ~2 weeks before stripes become visible. Axons seeking the path of least resistance within this moving basal layer would therefore appear radial early on. By contrast, establishing visible stripes requires an entire cohort of epithelial cells to travel from the limbus to the central cornea (Fig. 7). Extensive in-vivo studies (Song et al., 2004; Leiper et al., 2009) find no evidence that axons direct epithelial migration; if anything, epithelial flow dictates axonal trajectories.

      Geometry and boundaries:

      The spiral also forms on a flat disc, but its exact shape changes with curvature and cap angle; this variation is seen across mammals, including humans (Dua et al., 1993) and in diseases such as keratoconus. On a spherical cap the boundary winding number fixes the interior index, so ongoing limbal influx keeps the total index = 1. 

      In the revised version, we will therefore simulate a range of curvatures, cap angles, a prolate ellipsoid, and cases without limbal division, then compare with published data and disease states.

      In-vitro data and parameter fits:

      Although our dataset is limited, the inferred parameters match three independent invitro estimates (Kostanjevec et al. 2020; Saraswathibhatla et al. 2021; Kammeraat et al. in prep.). Spatial correlations exceed those expected from persistence alone, implying some polar alignment - consistent with Saraswathibhatla et al. 2021.  Slide-scanner images that we will include in the revision show cells are neither elongated nor nematically ordered. In the revision we will detail our parameter extraction, highlight evidence for alignment, stress the substrate-based activity mechanism, and draw attention to the supplementary videos.

      Topological clarification:

      Stagnation points can be seen as topological defects because classification depends only on vector directions. Boundary conditions can remove such defects in fluids, yet two sources/sinks still interact via the same logarithmic Green’s function that governs disclinations, despite di^erent physics. The Euler characteristic is a property of the surface; while the boundary winding number fixes the field index, it does not alter the surface’s Euler characteristic. 

      In the revision, we will add a concise primer on the di^erential-geometric concepts to make these points explicit.

    1. Author response:

      We thank the reviewers for their thoughtful and generous assessment of our work. Overall, the reviewers found our work to be novel and relevant. In particular: reviewer #1 found that our manuscript “It is timely and highly valuable for the telomere field” reviewer #2 stated, “Overall, I find this manuscript worthy of publication, as the optimized END-seq methods described here will likely be widely utilized in the telomere field.” Reviewer #3 stated that “The study is original, the experiments were well-controlled and excellently executed.”

      We are extremely grateful for these comments and want to thank all the reviewers and the editors for their time and effort in reviewing our work.

      The reviewers had a number of suggestions to improve our work. We have addressed all the points as highlighted in the point-by-point responses below.

      Reviewer 1:

      One minor question would be whether the authors could expand more on the application of END-Seq to examine the processive steps of the ALT mechanism? Can they speculate if the ssDNA detected in ALT cells might be an intermediate generated during BIR (i.e., is the ssDNA displaced strand during BIR) or a lesion? Furthermore, have the authors assessed whether ssDNA lesions are due to the loss of ATRX or DAXX, either of which can be mutated in the ALT setting?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful questions regarding the application of our assays to investigate the nature of the ssDNA detected in ALT telomeres. Our primary aim in this study was to establish the utility of END-seq and S1-END-seq in telomere biology and to demonstrate their applicability across both ALT-positive and -negative contexts. We agree that exploring the mechanistic origins of ssDNA would be highly informative, and we anticipate that END-seq–based approaches will be well suited for such future studies. However, it remains unclear whether the resolution of S1-END-seq is sufficient to capture transient intermediates such as those generated during BIR. We have now included a brief speculative statement in the revised discussion addressing the potential nature of ssDNA at telomeres in ALT cells.

      Reviewer #2:

      How can we be sure that all telomeres are equally represented? The authors seem to assume that END-seq captures all chromosome ends equally, but can we be certain of this? While I do not see an obvious way to resolve this experimentally, I recommend discussing this potential bias more extensively in the manuscript.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point. END-seq and S1-END-seq are unbiased methods designed to capture either double-stranded or single-stranded DNA that can be converted into blunt-ended double-stranded DNA and ligated to a capture oligo. As such, if a subset of telomeres cannot be processed using this approach, it is possible that these telomeres may be underrepresented or lost. However, to our knowledge, there are no proposed telomeric structures that would prevent capture using this method. For example, even if a subset of telomeres possesses a 5′ overhang, it would still be captured by END-seq. Indeed, we observed the consistent presence of the 5′-ATC motif across multiple cell lines and species (human, mouse, and dog). More importantly, we detected predictable and significant changes in sequence composition when telomere ends were experimentally altered, either in vivo (via POT1 depletion) or in vitro (via T7 exonuclease treatment). Together, these findings support the robustness of the method in capturing a representative and dynamic view of telomeres across different systems.

      That said, we have now included a brief statement in the revised discussion acknowledging that we cannot fully exclude the possibility that a subset of telomeres may be missed due to unusual or uncharacterized structures

      I believe Figures 1 and 2 should be merged.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to merge Figures 1 and 2. However, we feel that keeping them as separate figures better preserves the logical flow of the manuscript and allows the validation of END-seq and its application to be presented with appropriate clarity and focus. We hope the reviewer agrees that this layout enhances the clarity and interpretability of the data.

      Scale bars should be added to all microscopy figures.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We have now added scale bars to all the microscopy panels in the figures and included the scale details in the figure legends.

      Reviewer #3:

      Overall, the discussion section is lacking depth and should be expanded and a few additional experiments should be performed to clarify the results.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestions. Based on this reviewer’s comments and comments for the other reviewers, we incorporated several points into the discussion. As a result, we hope that we provide additional depth to our conclusions.

      (1) The finding that the abundance of variant telomeric repeats (VTRs) within the final 30 nucleotides of the telomeric 5' ends is similar in both telomerase-expressing and ALT cells is intriguing, but the authors do not address this result. Could the authors provide more insight into this observation and suggest potential explanations? As the frequency of VTRs does not seem to be upregulated in POT1-depleted cells, what then drives the appearance of VTRs on the C-strand at the very end of telomeres? Is CST-Pola complex responsible?

      The reviewer raises a very interesting and relevant point. We are hesitant at this point to speculate on why we do not see a difference in variant repeats in ALT versus non-ALT cells, since additional data would be needed. One possibility is that variant repeats in ALT cells accumulate stochastically within telomeres but are selected against when they are present at the terminal portion of chromosome ends. However, to prove this hypothesis, we would need error-free long-read technology combined with END-seq. We feel that developing this approach would be beyond the scope of this manuscript.

      (2) The authors also note that, in ALT cells, the frequency of VTRs in the first 30 nucleotides of the S1-END-SEQ reads is higher compared to END-SEQ, but this finding is not discussed either. Do the authors think that the presence of ssDNA regions is associated with the VTRs? Along this line, what is the frequency of VTRs in the END-SEQ analysis of TRF1-FokI-expressing ALT cells? Is it also increased? Has TRF1-FokI been applied to telomerase-expressing cells to compare VTR frequencies at internal sites between ALT and telomerase-expressing cells?

      Similarly to what is discussed above, short reads have the advantage of being very accurate but do not provide sufficient length to establish the relative frequency of VTRs across the whole telomere sequence. The TRF1-FokI experiment is a good suggestion, but it would still be biased toward non-variant repeats due to the TRF1-binding properties. We plan to address these questions in a future study involving long-read sequencing and END-seq capture of telomeres.

      Finally, in these experiments (S1-END-SEQ or END-SEQ in TRF1-Fok1), is the frequency of VTRs the same on both the C- and the G-rich strands? It is possible that the sequences are not fully complementary in regions where G4 structures form.

      We thank the reviewer for this observation. While we do observe a higher frequency of variant telomeric repeats (VTRs) in the first 30 nucleotides of S1-END-seq reads compared to END-seq in ALT cells, we are currently unable to determine whether this difference is significant, as an appropriate control or matched normalization strategy for this comparison is lacking. Therefore, we refrain from overinterpreting the biological relevance of this observation.

      The reviewer is absolutely correct. Our calculation did not exclude the possibility of extrachromosomal DNA as a source of telomeric ssDNA. We have now addressed this point in our discussion.

      The reviewer is correct in pointing out that we still do not know what causes ssDNA at telomeres in ALT cells. Replication stress seems the most logical explanation based on the work of many labs in the field. However, our data did not reveal any significant difference in the levels of ssDNA at telomeres in non-ALT cells based on telomere length. We used the HeLa1.2.11 cell line (now clarified in the Materials section), which is the parental line of HeLa1.3 and has similarly long telomeres (~20 kb vs. ~23 kb). Despite their long telomeres and potential for replication-associated challenges such as G-quadruplex formation, HeLa1.2.11 cells did not exhibit the elevated levels of telomeric ssDNA that we observed in ALT cells (Figure 4B). Additional experiments are needed to map the occurrence of ssDNA at telomeres in relation to progression toward ALT.

      (3) Based on the ratio of C-rich to G-rich reads in the S1-END-SEQ experiment, the authors estimate that ALT cells contain at least 3-5 ssDNA regions per chromosome end. While the calculation is understandable, this number could be discussed further to consider the possibility that the observed ratios (of roughly 0.5) might result from the presence of extrachromosomal DNA species, such as C-circles. The observed increase in the ratio of C-rich to G-rich reads in BLM-depleted cells supports this hypothesis, as BLM depletion suppresses C-circle formation in U2OS cells. To test this, the authors should examine the impact of POLD3 depletion on the C-rich/G-rich read ratio. Alternatively, they could separate high-molecular-weight (HMW) DNA from low-molecular-weight DNA in ALT cells and repeat the S1-END-SEQ in the HMW fraction.

      The reviewer is absolutely correct. Our calculation did not exclude the possibility of extrachromosomal DNA as a source of telomeric ssDNA. We have now addressed this point in our discussion.

      (4) What is the authors' perspective on the presence of ssDNA at ALT telomeres? Do they attribute this to replication stress? It would be helpful for the authors to repeat the S1-END-SEQ in telomerase-expressing cells with very long telomeres, such as HeLa1.3 cells, to determine if ssDNA is a specific feature of ALT cells or a result of replication stress. The increased abundance of G4 structures at telomeres in HeLa1.3 cells (as shown in J. Wong's lab) may indicate that replication stress is a factor. Similar to Wong's work, it would be valuable to compare the C-rich/G-rich read ratios in HeLa1.3 cells to those in ALT cells with similar telomeric DNA content.

      The reviewer is correct in pointing out that we still do not know what causes ssDNA at telomeres in ALT cells. Replication stress seems the most logical explanation based on the work of many labs in the field. However, our data did not reveal any significant difference in the levels of ssDNA at telomeres in non-ALT cells based on telomere length. We used the HeLa1.2.11 cell line (now clarified in the Materials section), which is the parental line of HeLa1.3 and has similarly long telomeres (~20 kb vs. ~23 kb). Despite their long telomeres and potential for replication-associated challenges such as G-quadruplex formation, HeLa1.2.11 cells did not exhibit the elevated levels of telomeric ssDNA that we observed in ALT cells (Figure 4B). Additional experiments are needed to map the occurrence of ssDNA at telomeres in relation to progression toward ALT.

      Finally, Reviewer #3 raises a list of minor points:

      (1) The Y-axes of Figure 4 have been relabeled to account for the G-strand reads.

      (2) Statistical analyses have been added to the figures where applicable.

      (3) The manuscript has been carefully proofread to improve clarity and consistency throughout the text and figure legends.

      (4) We have revised the text to address issues related to the lack of cross-referencing between the supplementary figures and their corresponding legends.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      (1) This study uses structural and functional approaches to investigate the regulation of the Na/Ca exchanger NCX1 by an activator, PIP2, and an inhibitor, SEA0400.  State-of-the-art methods are employed, and the data are of high quality and presented very clearly. The manuscript combines two rather different studies (one on PIP2; and one on SEA0400) neither of which is explored in the depth one might have hoped to form robust conclusions and significantly extend knowledge in the field.

      We combined the study of PIP2 and SEA0400 in this manuscript because both ligands inhibit or activate NCX1 by affecting the Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation of the exchanger - SEA0400 promotes inactivation by stabilizing the cytosolic inactivation assembly whereas PIP2 mitigates inactivation by destabilizing the assembly. The current study aims to provide structural insights into these ligand binding. We didn’t perform extensive electrophysiological analysis as the functional effects of both ligands have been extensively characterized over the last thirty years.

      (2) The novel aspect of this work is the study of PIP2. Unfortunately, technical limitations precluded structural data on binding of the native PIP2, so an unnatural short-chained analog, diC8 PIP2, was used instead. This raises the question of whether these two molecules, which have similar but very distinctly different profiles of activation, actually share the same binding pocket and mode of action. In an effort to address this, the authors mutate key residues predicted to be important in forming the binding site for the phosphorylated head group of PIP2. However, none of these mutations prevent PIP2 activation. The only ones that have a significant effect also influence the Na-dependent inactivation process independently of PIP2, thus casting doubt on their role in PIP2 binding, and thus identification of the PIP2 binding site. A more extensive mutagenic study, based on the diC8 PIP2 binding site, would have given more depth to this work and might have been more revealing mechanistically.

      The reviewer raises the important question of whether the short-chain PIP2 diC8 and long-chain native PIP2 share the same binding site. We have performed a pilot experiment to address this question. The data indicate that PIP2 diC8 competes with native brain PIP2 for its binding site (Author response image 1).  We believe that the mild effects of diC8 on the biophysical properties of NCX1 are due to its decreased affinity as compared to the long-chain PIP2. We have included this competition assay in the revised manuscript.

      The acyl-chain length-dependent PIP2 activation is consistent with some previous studies. Before PIP2 was demonstrated to regulate NCX1, some earlier studies showed that negatively charged long-chain lipids such as phosphatidylserine (PS) or phosphatidic acid (PA) could have the same potentiation effects on NCX1 as PIP2 (PMID: 1474504; PMID: 3276350). A later study showed that long-chain acyl-CoAs could also have the same potentiation effects on NCX1 as PIP2 (PMID: 16977318).  All these studies demonstrated that activation of NCX by the anionic lipids depends on their chain length with the short chain being ineffective or less effective. These findings have two implications. First, it is the negative surface charge rather than the specific IP3 head group of the lipid that is important for stimulating NCX1 activity. This would imply non-specific electrostatic interactions between the negatively charged lipids and those positively charged residues at the binding site. Second, a longer acyl chain is required for the high-affinity binding of PIP2 or negatively charged lipids. As further discussed in the revised manuscript (Discussion section), we suspect the tail of the long acyl chain from the native anionic lipids can enter the same binding pocket for SEA0400 thereby rendering higher affinity lipid binding than shorter chain lipids.

      As the interactions between PIP2 and NCX1 are both electrostatic involving multiple charged residues as well as hydrophobic involving the long lipid acyl chain, single amino acid substitutions likely only decrease the affinity of PIP2 rather than completely disrupt its binding. Our data demonstrated that mutants R220A, K225A, and R220A/K225A do show a significantly decreased potentiation effect of PIP2 (Figure 3 in the manuscript). We also conducted an experiment with a mutant exchanger in which all four amino were mutated. This K164A/R167A/R220A/K225A mutant is insensitive to PIP2 and shows no Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation (Figure 3A). The unresponsiveness to PIP2 and lack of Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation in this mutant is consistent with previous studies demonstrating that PIP2 activates NCX by tuning the amount of Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation and any mutation that decreases NCX sensitivity to PIP2 will affect the extent of Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation (PMID: 10751315). Such studies show that the two processes cannot be dissected from each other, making more extensive mutagenesis investigation unlikely to provide new mechanistic insights. A brief discussion related to this quadruple mutant has been added in the revised manuscript.

      Author response image 1.

      Giant patch recording of the human WT exchanger. Currents were first activated by intracellular application of 10 µM brain PIP2. Afterwards, a solution containing 100 mM Na<sup>+</sup> and 12 µM Ca<sup>2+</sup> was perfused for about 5 min (washout). The PIP2 effects was not reversible during this time. The same patch was then perfused internally with the same solution in presence of 10 µM di-C8. Application of the shorted-chained di-C8, partially decreased the current suggesting that that PIP2 and diC8 compete for the binding site.

      (3) The SEA0400 aspect of the work does not integrate particularly well with the rest of the manuscript. This study confirms the previously reported structure and binding site for SEA0400 but provides no further information. While interesting speculation is presented regarding the connection between SEA0400 inhibition and Na-dependent inactivation, further experiments to test this idea are not included here.

      Our SEA0400-bound NCX structure was determined and deposited in 2023, along with our previous study on the apo NCX published in 2023 (PMID: 37794011). We decided to combine the SEA0400-bound structure with the later study of PIP2 binding because both represent ligand modulation of NCX by affecting the Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation of the exchanger. The SEA0400 inhibition of NCX1 has been extensively investigated previously, which demonstrated a strong connection between SEA0400 and the Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation. As discussed in the manuscript, SEA0400 is ineffective in an exchanger lacking Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation. Conversely, enhancing the extent of Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation increases the affinity for SEA0400. Our structural analysis provides explanations for these pharmacological features of SEA0400 inhibition.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      (1) The study by Xue et al. reports the structural basis for the regulation of the human cardiac sodium-calcium exchanger, NCX1, by the endogenous activator PIP2 and the small molecule inhibitor SEA400. This well-written study contextualizes the new data within the existing literature on NCX1 and the broader NCX family. This work builds upon the authors' previous study (Xue et al., 2023), which presented the cryo-EM structures of human cardiac NCX1 in both inactivated and activated states. The 2023 study highlighted key structural differences between the active and inactive states and proposed a mechanism where the activity of NCX1 is regulated by the interactions between the ion-transporting transmembrane domain and the cytosolic regulatory domain. Specifically, in the inward-facing state and at low cytosolic calcium levels, the transmembrane (TM) and cytosolic domains form a stable interaction that results in the inactivation of the exchanger. In contrast, calcium binding to the cytosolic domain at high cytosolic calcium levels disrupts the interaction with the TM domain, leading to active ion exchange.

      In the current study, the authors present two mechanisms explaining how both PIP2 stimulates NCX1 activity by destabilizing the protein's inactive state (i.e., by disrupting the interaction between the TM domain and the cytosolic domain) and how SEA400 stabilizes this interaction, thereby acting as a specific inhibitor of the system.

      The first part of the results section addresses the effect of PIP2 and PIP2 diC8 on NCX1 activity. This is pertinent as the authors use the diC8 version of this lipid (which has a shorter acyl chain) in their subsequent cryo-EM structure due to the instability of native PIP2. I am not an electrophysiology expert; however, my main comment would be to ask whether there is sufficient data here to characterise fully the differences between PIP2 and PIP2 diC8 on NCX1 function. It appears from the text that this study is the first to report these differences, so perhaps this data needs to be more robust. The spread of the data points in Figure 1B is possibly a little unconvincing given that only six measurements were taken. Why is there one outlier in Figure 1A? Were these results taken using the same batch of oocytes? Are these technical or biological replicates? Is the convention to use statistical significance for these types of experiments?

      Oocytes were isolated from at least 3 different frogs and each data point shown in Fig. 1 A or 1B of the manuscript represents a recording obtained from a single oocyte. For clarity, we have added this information to the Methods section. We understand that 6 observations (Fig. 1B) are a small sample size but electrophysiological recordings of NCX currents are extremely challenging and technically difficult due to the low transport activity of the exchanger. Because of these circumstances, this type of study relies on a small sample of observations. Nevertheless, our data clearly show that native PIP2 and the short-chain PIP2 diC8 can activate NCX activity although with different affinity. The spread of the steady state current data points is due to the variability in the extent of Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation within each patch, likely due to slightly different levels of endogenous PIP2 or other regulatory mechanisms that control this allosteric process. As PIP2 acts on the Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation this will lead to varying levels of potentiation. Because of that, we did occasionally observe some outliers in our recordings. Rather than cherry-picking in data analysis, we presented all the data points from patches with measurable NCX1 currents. Despite this variability, a T-test indicates that the effects of PIP2 are more pronounced on the steady-state current than peak current.  The differences between native PIP2 and PIP2 diC8 on NCX1 function are consistent with previous investigations showing that both PIP2 and anionic lipids enhance NCX current by antagonizing the Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation and long-chain lipids are more effective in potentiating NCX1 activity (PMID: 1474504; PMID: 3276350; PMID: 16977318). A discussion related to the chain length-dependent lipid activation of NCX1 is added in the Discussion of the revised manuscript. 

      (2) I am also somewhat skeptical about the modelling of the PIP2 diC8 molecule. The authors state, "The density of the IP3 head group from the bound PIP2 diC8 is well-defined in the EM map. The acyl chains, however, are flexible and could not be resolved in the structure (Fig. S2)."

      However, the density appears rather ambiguous to me, and the ligand does not fit well within the density. Specifically, there is a large extension in the volume near the phosphate at the 5' position, with no corresponding volume near the 4' phosphate. Additionally, there is no bifurcation of the volume near the lipid tails. I attempted to model cholesterol hemisuccinate (PDB: Y01) into this density, and it fits reasonably well - at least as well as PIP2 diC8. I am also concerned that if this site is specific for PIP2, then why are there no specific interactions with the lipid phosphates? How can the authors explain the difference between PIP2 and PIP2 diC8 if the acyl chains don't make any direct interactions with the TM domain? In short, the structures do not explain the functional differences presented in Figure 1.

      The side chain densities for Arg167 and Arg220 are also quite weak. While there is some density for the side chain of Lys164, it is also very weak. I would expect that if this site were truly specific for PIP2, it should exhibit greater structural rigidity - otherwise, how is this specific?

      Given this observation, have the authors considered using other PIP2 variants to determine if the specificity lies with PI4,5P<sub>2</sub> as opposed to PI3,5P<sub>2</sub> or PI3,4P<sub>2</sub>? A lack of specificity may explain the observed poor density.

      The map we provided to the editor in the initial submission is the overall map for PIP2-bound NCX1. Due to the relative flexibility between the cytosolic CBD and TM regions, we also performed local refinement on each region in data processing to improve the map quality as illustrated in Fig. S2.  The local-refined map focused on the TM domain provides a much better density for PIP2 diC8 and its surrounding residues than the overall map. The map quality allowed us to unambiguously identify the lipid as PIP2 with the IP3 head group having phosphate groups at the 4,5 positions. Furthermore, no lipid density is observed at the equivalent location in the local-refined map from the apo NCX1 TM region as shown in Fig. S3 in the revision. In the revised manuscript, the density for the bound PIP2 is shown in Fig. 2A. Those local-refined maps for PIP2-bound NCX1 were also deposited as additional maps along with the overall map in the Electron Microscopy Data Bank under accession numbers EMD-60921. The local-refined maps for the apo-NCX1 were deposited in the Electron Microscopy Data Bank under accession numbers EMD-40457 in our previous study (https://www.ebi.ac.uk/emdb/EMD-40457?tab=interpretation).

      As discussed in our response to reviewer #1, the acyl-chain length-dependent PIP2 activation is consistent with some previous studies. Before PIP2 was identified as a physiological regulator of NCX1, some earlier studies showed that negatively charged long-chain lipids such as phosphatidylserine (PS) or phosphatidic acid (PA) could have the same potentiation effects on NCX as PIP2 (PMID: 1474504; PMID: 3276350). A later study also showed that acyl-CoA could also have the same potentiation effects on NCX as PIP2 (PMID: 16977318). All these studies demonstrated that activation of NCX1 by the anionic lipids depends on their chain length with the short chain being ineffective.  These findings have two implications. First, it is the negative surface charge rather than the specific IP3 head group of the lipid that is important for stimulating NCX activity. This would imply non-specific electrostatic interactions between the negatively charged lipids and those positively charged residues at the binding site.  Second, a longer acyl chain is required for the high-affinity binding of PIP2 or negatively charged lipids. As further discussed in the revised manuscript (Discussion section), we suspect the tail of the long acyl chain can enter the same binding pocket for SEA0400 thereby rendering higher affinity lipid binding than shorter chain lipids. In light of the equivalent potentiating effect of various anionic lipids on NCX1, PI(4,5)P2 activation of NCX1 is likely non-specific and PI(3,5)P2 or PI(3,4)P2 may also activate the exchanger. However, as a key player in membrane signaling, PI(4,5)P2 has been demonstrated to be a physiological regulator of NCX1 in many studies.

      (3) I also noticed many lipid-like densities in the maps for this complex. Is it possible that the authors overlooked something? For instance, there is a cholesterol-like density near Val51, as well as something intriguing near Trp763, where I could model PIP2 diC8 (though this leads to a clash with Trp763). I wonder if the authors are working with mixed populations in their dataset. The accompanying description of the structural changes is well-written (assuming it is accurate).

      Densities from endogenous lipids and cholesterols are commonly observed in membrane protein structures. Other than the bound PIP2, those lipid and cholesterol densities are present in both the apo and PIP2-bound structures, including the density around Trp763 and Val53. Whether those bound lipids/cholesterols play any functional roles or just stabilize the protein is beyond the scope of this study.  We have added a supporting figure (Fig. S3) showing a side-by-side comparison of the density at the PIP2 binding site between the PIP2-bound and apo structures.

      I would recommend that the authors update the figures associated with this section, as they are currently somewhat difficult to interpret without prior knowledge of NCX architecture. My suggestions include:

      - Including the density for the PIP2 diC8 in Figure 2A.

      As suggested, we have included the density of PIP2 diC8 in Figure 2A.

      - Adding membrane boundaries (cytosolic vs. extracellular) in Figure 2B.

      - Labeling the cytosolic domains in Figure 2B.

      - Adding hydrogen bond distances in Figure 2A.

      We have added and labeled the boundaries for the TM and cytosolic domains in Figure 2B as suggested. Although we can identify those positively charged residues in the vicinity of the PIP2 head group and observe local structural changes, the poorly defined side-chain densities of these residues won’t allow us to properly determine the hydrogen bond distances.

      - Detailing the domain movements in Figure 2B (what is the significance of the grey vs. blue structures?).

      There is a rigid-body downward swing movement at CBDs between the apo (grey) and PIP2-bound (cyan) structures. The movement at the TM region is subtle. We have added the description in the legend for Figure 2B and also marked the movement at the tip of CBD1 in the figure.

      The section on the mechanism of SEA400-induced inactivation is strong. The maps are of better quality than those for the PIP2 diC8 complex, and the ligand fits well. However, I noticed a density peak below F02 on SEA400 that lies within the hydrogen bonding distance of Asp825. Is this a water molecule? If so, is this significant?

      The structure of SEA0400-bound NCX1 was determined at a higher resolution likely because the drug stabilize the exchanger in the inactivated state.  The mentioned density could be an ordered water molecule. We don’t know if it is functionally significant.

      Furthermore, there are many unmodeled regions that are likely cholesterol hemisuccinate or detergent molecules, which may warrant further investigation.

      We constantly observed partial densities from bound lipids, cholesterols, or detergents in our structures. Most of them are difficult to be unambiguously identified and modeled. Whether they play any functional roles is beyond the scope of this study.  

      The authors introduce SEA400 as a selective inhibitor of NCX1; however, there is little to no comparison between the binding sites of the different NCX proteins. This section could be expanded. Perhaps Fig. 4C could include sequence conservation data.

      SEA0400 is more specific for NCX1 than NCX2 and NCX3 as demonstrated in an early study (PMID: 14660663). The lack of structure information for NCX2 or NCX3 makes it difficult to make a direct comparison to reveal the structural basis of SEA0400 specificity.

      Additionally, is the fenestration in the membrane physiological, or is it merely a hole forced open by the binding of SEA400? I was unclear as to whether the authors were suggesting a physiological role for this feature, similar to those observed in sodium channels.

      The fenestration likely serves as the portal for SEA0400 binding as discussed in the manuscript. As further discussed in the revised manuscript, we suspect this fenestration also allows the tail of a long-chain lipid to enter the same binding pocket for SEA0400 and results in higher affinity binding of a long-chain lipid than a short-chain lipid.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      NCXs are key Ca<sup>2+</sup> transporters located on the plasma membrane, essential for maintaining cellular Ca<sup>2+</sup> homeostasis and signaling. The activities of NCX are tightly regulated in response to cellular conditions, ensuring precise control of intracellular Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels, with profound physiological implications. Building upon their recent breakthrough in determining the structure of human NCX1, the authors obtained cryo-EM structures of NCX1 in complex with its modulators, including the cellular activator PIP2 and the small molecule inhibitor SEA0400. Structural analyses revealed mechanistically informative conformational changes induced by PIP2 and elucidated the molecular basis of inhibition by SEA0400. These findings underscore the critical role of the interface between the transmembrane and cytosolic domains in NCX regulation and small molecule modulation. Overall, the results provide key insights into NCX regulation, with important implications for cellular Ca<sup>2+</sup> homeostasis.

      We appreciate this reviewer’s positive comments.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The manuscript would be strengthened enormously by a much deeper focus on the novel and very interesting PIP2 work, as noted above, and perhaps the removal of the SEA0400 data.

      If that is beyond the scope of the authors' options, then a more robust discussion of limitations of the current work, perhaps speculation regarding other future experiments, a clearer presentation of how these data on SEA0400 are different from/extend from the previously published work, and a better effort to link the two disparate aspects of the work into a more cohesive manuscript should be attempted.

      As discussed in our response to this reviewer’s public review, we combined the study of PIP2 and SEA0400 in this manuscript because both ligands activate or inhibit NCX1 by affecting the Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation of the exchanger. The functional effects of both ligands on NCX1 have been extensively characterized over the last thirty years. Thus the current study is focused on providing structural explanations for some unique pharmacological features of these ligands. In the revised manuscript, we have added an extra paragraph of discussion that provides a plausible explanation for chain length-dependent PIP2 activation.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      A few comments to consider:

      (1) The short-chain PIP2 appears to have lower potency, but the mechanism remains unclear. Based on structural analyses, are there potential binding sites for the acyl chains of PIP2 that could contribute to this difference?

      As discussed in our response to other reviewers, long-chain anionic lipids can have the same potentiation effect on NCX1 activity as PIP2, but the short-chain ones are ineffective just like short-chain PIP2 diC8. We suspect the tail of a long acyl chain from the native PIP2 can enter the same binding pocket for SEA0400 thereby rendering higher affinity binding for a long-chain lipid than a short-chain lipid. A discussion related to this point has been added to the revised manuscript.

      (2) It is unclear why mutating residues that interact with the IP3 head group retain PIP2 activation. Would it be possible to assess PIP2 and C8 PIP2 binding to these NCX1 variants? Identifying a mutant that abolishes C8 PIP2 binding would be valuable in interpreting those results.

      As the interactions between PIP2 and NCX1 are both electrostatic involving multiple charged residues and hydrophobic involving the long lipid acyl chain, single amino acid substitutions likely only decrease the affinity of PIP2 rather than completely disrupt its binding.  Individual mutants R220A and K225A show a 5-fold decrease in their response to PIP2 application indicating that their replacement alters the affinity of NCX for PIP2.  We have added a new experiment showing that an exchanger with all four residues mutated is insensitive to PIP2 in the revision.

      (3) What are the functional effects of mutating Y226 and R247, residues that seem to play an important role in PIP2-mediated activation?

      In a previous study, mutation at Y226 (Y226T), which is found within the XIP region of NCX, has been shown to have enhanced Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation (PMID: 9041455).  To our knowledge, the R247 mutation has not been investigated. Also positioned in the XIP region, we suspect its mutation could directly affect Na<sup>+</sup>-dependent inactivation. This would make it difficult to determine if the function effect of the mutation is caused by changing the stability of the XIP region or by changing the binding of PIP2.

      (4) Is there any overlap between the PIP2 and SEA0400 binding regions? Both appear to involve TM4, TM5, and TMD-beta hub interfaces. It might be interesting to discuss any shared mechanisms and why this region might serve as a hotspot for modulation.

      As mentioned in our previous response, we suspect the tail of a long acyl chain from the native PIP2 can enter the same binding pocket for SEA0400 thereby rendering higher affinity binding for a long-chain lipid than a short-chain lipid. A more detailed discussion related to this point has been included in the revision.

      (5) It would be helpful to show the density at the PIP2-binding site in the apo and PIP2-bound structures side by side

      This figure has been added in the revision as Fig. S3.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, authors have tried to repurpose cipargamin (CIP), a known drug against Plasmodium and Toxoplasma against Babesia. They proved the efficacy of CIP on Babesia in nanomolar range. In silico analyses revealed the drug resistance mechanism through a single amino acid mutation at amino acid position 921 on the ATP4 gene of Babesia. Overall, the conclusions drawn by the authors are well justified by their data. I believe this study opens up a novel therapeutic strategy against babesiosis.

      Strengths:

      Authors have carried out a comprehensive study. All the experiments performed were carried out methodically and logically.

      We appreciate your positive feedback. Your acknowledgment reinforces our commitment to rigor and thoroughness in our research.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aim to establish that cipargamin can be used for the treatment of infection caused by Babesia organisms.

      Strengths:

      The study provides strong evidence that cipargamin is effective against various Babesia species. In vitro growth assays were used to establish that cipargamin is effective against Babesia bovis and Babesia gibsoni. Infection of mice with Babesia microti demonstrated that cipargamin is as effective as the combination of atovaquone plus azithromycin. Cipargamin protected mice from lethal infection with Babesia rodhaini. Mutations that confer resistance to cipargamin were identified in the gene encoding ATP4, a P-type Na+ ATPase that is found in other apicomplexan parasites, thereby validating ATP4 as the target of cipargamin. A 7-day treatment of cipagarmin, when combined with a single dose of tafenoquine, was sufficient to eradicate Babesia microti in a mouse model of severe babesiosis caused by lack of adaptive immunity.

      Thank you for the comments and for your time to review our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      Cipargamin was tested in vivo at a single dose administered daily for 7 days. Despite the prospect of using cipargamin for the treatment of human babesiosis, there was no attempt to identify the lowest dose of cipagarmin that protects mice from Babesia microti infection. In the SCID mouse model, cipargamin was tested in combination with tafenoquine but not with atovaquone and/or azithromycin, although the latter combination is often used as first-line therapy for human babesiosis caused by Babesia microti.

      Thank you for your insightful comments. We agree that using a single daily dose over 7 days is one of the limitations in the in vivo trial. Our main goals were to demonstrate cipargamin's efficacy and understand its antibabesial agent mechanism. For future work, we plan to conduct dose‐optimization studies to determine the lowest effective dose in vivo. Regarding the drug combination in the SCID mouse model, although atovaquone and/or azithromycin are frequently used as first-line therapies for human babesiosis, resistance to these traditional drugs is emerging. Based on this challenge, we opted to evaluate a combination with tafenoquine as a novel partner, aiming to overcome resistance issues and improve therapeutic outcomes.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      None other than some minor grammatical mistakes.

      We have corrected the grammatical mistakes.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The revised manuscript is much improved. I have the following comments.

      Comment 1: Atovaquone plus azithromycin is effective against Babesia microti (Figure 1C) but not against Babesia rodhaini (Figure 1E). It would be valuable to provide a possible explanation.

      Thank you for highlighting this issue. One potential explanation is that B. microti and B. rodhaini might have intrinsic differences in drug sensitivity and susceptibility. A previous study reported that both species possess a unique linear monomeric mitochondrial genome with a dual flip-flop inversion system, which generates four distinct genome structures (Hikosaka et al., 2012). In addition, previous studies have shown that mitochondria-associated energy production is greater in B. microti than in B. rodhaini (Shikano et al., 1998). This suggests that B. microti, whose metabolism is largely driven by mitochondrial function, may be more susceptible to drugs (like atovaquone) that induce parasite death by disrupting mitochondrial targets such as cytochrome b (Wormser et al., 2010). Moreover, B. rodhaini tends to proliferate more rapidly and causes acute infections, which may outpace any drug effects. Further, the rapid proliferation of apicomplexan parasites, as is the case in Plasmodium (Salcedo-Sora et al., 2014), Theileria (Metheni et al., 2015), and B. rodhaini (Rickard, 1970; Shikano et al., 1995), has been ascribed to glycolysis as the primary energy source. This may have contributed to the reduced efficacy of atovaquone and azithromycin in B. rodhaini-infected mice in the current study. Nonetheless, we plan to explore these interspecies differences in our future work.

      Comment 2: The relapse that follows a 7-day treatment with cipargamin is transient in BALB/ mice infected with Babesia rodhaini (Figure 1E) but persistent in SCID mice infected with Babesia microti (Figure 5C). It would be valuable to provide a possible explanation.

      Thank you for your insightful comment. One possible explanation is the difference in immune status between the two mouse models. BALB/c mice have a fully functional immune system that can likely clear residual parasites following a transient relapse after cipargamin treatment. In contrast, SCID mice lack an adaptive immune response, which might allow residual B. microti parasites to persist and cause a sustained relapse. Additionally, intrinsic differences between B. rodhaini and B. microti, such as growth rate or drug susceptibility, could also play a role. We plan to explore these factors in future studies.

      Comment 3: The effect of cipargamin on parasite pH is the greatest when assessed 4 to 8 min after exposure is initiated (Figure 3E). Yet, resistance of parasites that carry a mutation in ATP4, the target of cipargamin, was assessed 20 min after cipargamin addition. At this time point, cipargamin has very little effect (Figure 3E). Accordingly, data reported in Figure 3G are of limited value.

      Thank you for your comment. The initial pH increase we see around 4 to 8 minutes likely reflects the rapid inhibition of ATP4-mediated Na⁺/H⁺ exchange by cipargamin, which quickly alkalinizes the cell. However, after the initial increase, compensatory processes, such as proton influx or metabolic acid production, gradually restored the pH, resulting in a later decline. Although assessing the pH level at 20 minutes may have recorded less dramatic changes, it still allowed us to compare the sustained differences between wild-type and mutant strains. We agree that including earlier time points for the mutants might provide further insight and we will consider this in our future work.

      Comment 4: In Figure 3H, please report the lack of statistical significance between wild-type parasites and parasites that carry the mutation L921V.

      In Figure 3H, the ATPase activity in erythrocytes infected with wild-type parasites (6.31 ± 1.20 nmol Pi/mg protein/min) is higher than that of the L921V mutation (5.11 ± 0.50 nmol Pi/mg protein/min), but the difference is not statistically significant (P = 0.095), so no asterisk was added.

      Comment 5: Tafenoquine was administered as a single 20 mg/kg dose. Please specify whether this dose is for tafenoquine succinate or tafenoquine base.

      Thank you for raising this point. In our study, the single 20 mg/kg dose refers to tafenoquine succinate. We have clarified this detail in the revised manuscript (Line 40).

      Comment 6: A single dose of 20 mg/kg tafenoquine succinate was first tested in the SCID mouse model of severe babesiosis by Mordue et al (JID 2019), not by Liu et al. (JID 2024). Please amend discussion accordingly (line 311). As correctly stated in the discussion, the single 20 mg/kg dose was not sufficient to prevent relapse of Babesia microti in the study by Mordue et al. Please provide a possible explanation for why no parasitemia was detected for 90 days in your SCID model (Figure 5C).

      Thank you for your comment. We have modified the suggested citation (Line 309). As noted by Mordue et al. (JID 2019), a single 20 mg/kg dose of tafenoquine succinate was insufficient to prevent relapse in their SCID mouse model using B. microti (ATCC 30221 Gray strain). In our study, however, no parasitemia was detected for 90 days (Figure 5C) using the B. microti Peabody mjr strain (ATCC PRA-99). Differences in the parasite strain and the timing of treatment relative to infection may have contributed to the extended suppression of parasitemia observed in our study. We plan to explore these aspects in future work.

      Comment 7: Real-time PCR was used to confirm eradication of Babesia microti infection (Figure 5D). Please specify the blood volume from which genomic DNA was extracted for each mouse. Please specify the amount of genomic DNA (i.e., not the volume) used in each reaction. Please explain how/why the cut-off was set at 35 cycles. What were the Ct values when blood was obtained from uninfected mice? For infected mice treated with cipargamin plus tafenoquine, there was no amplification. Was each reaction subjected to a maximum of 40 cycles (as suggested by Figure 5D)?

      In our qPCR assay, genomic DNA was extracted from 200 µL of blood per mouse (Line 458). In each reaction, we used 100 ng of genomic DNA (Line 464), and the thermocycling conditions were set at 40 cycles. We set the cut-off at 35 cycles based on our optimization experiments: samples with Ct values ≤ 35 consistently indicated the presence of parasite DNA, while samples without parasite DNA (distilled water and DNA from uninfected mice) had CT values > 35 cycles or undetectable. Although each reaction was run for 40 cycles, for our analysis, we defined samples as negative if no signal was observed beyond cycle 35. In mice treated with cipargamin plus tafenoquine, no signal was detected until 40 cycles, indicating the absence of parasite DNA in the samples.

      Comment 8:  Persistence of parasite DNA in blood of tafenoquine treated mice highlights the limitation of PCR to assess persistence of infection. That is, PCR cannot distinguish between viable parasites and non-viable (or dead) parasites. An adoptive transfer of blood to immunocompromised mice can help determine whether persistence of DNA is due to persistence of viable parasites. Because the experiment was carried out in SCID mice, no adoptive transfer is needed. Few parasites are required for a successful infection of immunocompromised mice (SCID mice included). Given that parasitemia never rose following treatment of SCID mice with a single dose of tafenoquine, it is highly likely that parasite DNA detected on day 90 post-infection in these tafenoquine treated mice came from persistent non-viable/dead parasites.

      We appreciate your comment and acknowledge that the use of PCR has limitations in differentiating between live and dead parasites. It is possible that the residual DNA may represent a small population of dormant parasites that are not actively replicating and thus remain below the detection threshold of parasitemia. Even in highly immunocompromised SCID mice, such dormant parasites might persist without causing overt infection under our experimental conditions. An adoptive transfer experiment in SCID mice, although not strictly necessary, could validate whether the detection of low levels of DNA comes from viable parasites capable of reactivating under different circumstances. Future studies using more sensitive viability assays or adoptive transfer approaches could provide further insights into this possibility.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This study examined the interaction between two key cortical regions in the mouse brain involved in goal-directed movements, the rostral forelimb area (RFA) - considered a premotor region involved in movement planning, and the caudal forelimb area (CFA) - considered a primary motor region that more directly influences movement execution. The authors ask whether there exists a hierarchical interaction between these regions, as previously hypothesized, and focus on a specific definition of hierarchy - examining whether the neural activity in the premotor region exerts a larger functional influence on the activity in the primary motor area than vice versa. They examine this question using advanced experimental and analytical methods, including localized optogenetic manipulation of neural activity in either region while measuring both the neural activity in the other region and EMG signals from several muscles involved in the reaching movement, as well as simultaneous electrophysiology recordings from both regions in a separate cohort of animals.

      The findings presented show that localized optogenetic manipulation of neural activity in either RFA or CFA resulted in similarly short-latency changes in the muscle output and in firing rate changes in the other region. However, perturbation of RFA led to a larger absolute change in the neural activity of CFA neurons. The authors interpret these findings as evidence for reciprocal, but asymmetrical, influence between the regions, suggesting some degree of hierarchy in which RFA has a greater effect on the neural activity in CFA. They go on to examine whether this asymmetry can also be observed in simultaneously recorded neural activity patterns from both regions. They use multiple advanced analysis methods that either identify latent components at the population level or measure the predictability of firing rates of single neurons in one region using firing rates of single neurons in the other region. Interestingly, the main finding across these analyses seems to be that both regions share highly similar components that capture a high degree of variability of the neural activity patterns in each region. Single units' activity from either region could be predicted to a similar degree from the activity of single units in the other region, without a clear division into a leading area and a lagging area, as one might expect to find in a simple hierarchical interaction. However, the authors find some evidence showing a slight bias towards leading activity in RFA. Using a two-region neural network model that is fit to the summed neural activity recorded in the different experiments and to the summed muscle output, the authors show that a network with constrained (balanced) weights between the regions can still output the observed measured activities and the observed asymmetrical effects of the optogenetic manipulations, by having different within-region local weights. These results put into question whether previous and current findings that demonstrate asymmetry in the output of regions can be interpreted as evidence for asymmetrical (and thus hierarchical) inputs between regions, emphasizing the challenges in studying interactions between any brain regions.

      Strengths:

      The experiments and analyses performed in this study are comprehensive and provide a detailed examination and comparison of neural activity recorded simultaneously using dense electrophysiology probes from two main motor regions that have been the focus of studies examining goal-directed movements. The findings showing reciprocal effects from each region to the other, similar short-latency modulation of muscle output by both regions, and similarity of neural activity patterns without a clear lead/lag interaction, are convincing and add to the growing body of evidence that highlight the complexity of the interactions between multiple regions in the motor system and go against a simple feedforward-like network and dynamics. The neural network model complements these findings and adds an important demonstration that the observed asymmetry can, in theory, also arise from differences in local recurrent connections and not necessarily from different input projections from one region to the other. This sheds an important light on the multiple factors that should be considered when studying the interaction between any two brain regions, with a specific emphasis on the role of local recurrent connections, that should be of interest to the general neuroscience community.

      Weaknesses:

      While the similarity of the activity patterns across regions and lack of a clear leading/lagging interaction are interesting observations that are mostly supported by the findings presented (however, see comment below for lack of clarity in CCA/PLS analyses), the main question posed by the authors - whether there exists an endogenous hierarchical interaction between RFA and CFA - seems to be left largely open. 

      The authors note that there is currently no clear evidence of asymmetrical reciprocal influence between naturally occurring neural activity patterns of the two regions, as previous attempts have used non-natural electrical stimulation, lesions, or pharmacological inactivation. The use of acute optogenetic perturbations does not seem to be vastly different in that aspect, as it is a non-natural stimulation of inhibitory interneurons that abruptly perturbs the ongoing dynamics.

      We do believe that our optogenetic inactivation identifies a causal interaction between the endogenous activity patterns in the excitatory projection neurons, which we have largely silenced, and the downstream endogenous activity that is perturbed. The effect in the downstream region results directly from the silencing of activity in the excitatory projection neurons that mediate each region’s interaction with other regions. Here we have performed a causal intervention common in biology: a loss-of-function experiment. Such experiments generally reveal that a causal interaction of some sort is present, but often do not clarify much about the nature of the interaction, as is true in our case. By showing that a silencing of endogenous activity in one motor cortical region causes a significant change to the endogenous activity in another, we establish a causal relationship between these activity patterns. This is analogous to knocking out the gene for a transcription factor and observing causal effects on the expression of other genes that depend on it. 

      Moreover, our experiments are, to our knowledge, the first that localize a causal relationship to endogenous activity in motor cortex at a particular point during a motor behavior. Lesion and pharmacological or chemogenetic inactivation have long-lasting effects, and so their consequences on firing in other regions cannot be attributed to a short-latency influence of activity at a particular point during movement. Moreover, the involvement of motor cortex in motor learning and movement preparation/initiation complicates the interpretation of these consequences in relation to movement execution, as disturbance to processes on which execution depends can impede execution itself. Stimulation experiments generate spiking in excitatory projection neurons that is not endogenous.

      That said, we would agree that the form of the causal interaction between RFA and CFA remains unaddressed by our results. These results do not expose how the silenced activity patterns affect activity in the downstream region, just as knocking out a transcription factor gene does not expose how the transcription factor influences the expression of other genes. To show evidence for a specific type of interaction dynamics between RFA and CFA, a different sort of experiment would be necessary. See Jazayeri and Afraz, Neuron, 2017 for more on this issue.

      Furthermore, the main finding that supports a hierarchical interaction is a difference in the absolute change of firing rates as a result of the optogenetic perturbation, a finding that is based on a small number of animals (N = 3 in each experimental group), and one which may be difficult to interpret. 

      Though N = 3, we do show statistical significance. Moreover, using three replicates is not uncommon in biological experiments that require a large technical investment.

      As the authors nicely demonstrate in their neural network model, the two regions may differ in the strength of local within-region inhibitory connections. Could this theoretically also lead to a difference in the effect of the artificial light stimulation of the inhibitory interneurons on the local population of excitatory projection neurons, driving an asymmetrical effect on the downstream region? 

      We (Miri et al., Neuron, 2017) and others (Guo et al., Neuron, 2014) have shown that the effect of this inactivation on excitatory neurons in CFA is a near-complete silencing (90-95% within 20 ms). There thus is not much room for the effects on projection neurons in RFA to be much larger. We have measured these local effects in RFA as part of other work (Kristl et al., biorxiv, 2025), verifying that the effects on RFA projection neuron firing are not larger.

      Moreover, the manipulation was performed upon the beginning of the reaching movement, while the premotor region is often hypothesized to exert its main control during movement preparation, and thus possibly show greater modulation during that movement epoch. It is not clear if the observed difference in absolute change is dependent on the chosen time of optogenetic stimulation and if this effect is a general effect that will hold if the stimulation is delivered during different movement epochs, such as during movement preparation.

      We agree that the dependence of RFA-CFA interactions on movement phase would be interesting to address in subsequent experiments. While a strong interpretation of lesion results might lead to a hypothesis that premotor influence on primary motor cortex is local to, or stronger during, movement preparation as opposed to execution, at present there is to our knowledge no empirical support from interventional experiments for this hypothesis. Moreover, existing results from analysis of activity in these two regions have produced conflicting results on the strength of interaction between these regions during preparation. Compare for example BachschmidRomano et al., eLife, 2023 to Kaufman et al., Nature Neuroscience, 2014.

      That said, this lesion interpretation would predict the same asymmetry we have observed from perturbations at the beginning of a reach - a larger effect of RFA on CFA than vice versa.

      Another finding that is not clearly interpretable is in the analysis of the population activity using CCA and PLS. The authors show that shifting the activity of one region compared to the other, in an attempt to find the optimal leading/lagging interaction, does not affect the results of these analyses. Assuming the activities of both regions are better aligned at some unknown groundtruth lead/lag time, I would expect to see a peak somewhere in the range examined, as is nicely shown when running the same analyses on a single region's activity. If the activities are indeed aligned at zero, without a clear leading/lagging interaction, but the results remain similar when shifting the activities of one region compared to the other, the interpretation of these analyses is not clear.

      Our results in this case were definitely surprising. Many share the intuition that there should be a lag at which the correlations in activity between regions may be strongest. The similarity in alignment across lags we observed might be expected if communication between regions occurs over a range of latencies as a result of dependence on a broad diversity of synaptic paths that connect neurons. In the Discussion, we offer an explanation of how to reconcile these findings with the seemingly different picture presented by DLAG.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      While technical advances have enabled large-scale, multi-site neural recordings, characterizing inter-regional communication and its behavioral relevance remains challenging due to intrinsic properties of the brain such as shared inputs, network complexity, and external noise. This work by Saiki-Ishkawa et al. examines the functional hierarchy between premotor (PM) and primary motor (M1) cortices in mice during a directional reaching task. The authors find some evidence consistent with an asymmetric reciprocal influence between the regions, but overall, activity patterns were highly similar and equally predictive of one another. These results suggest that motor cortical hierarchy, though present, is not fully reflected in firing patterns alone.

      Strengths:

      Inferring functional hierarchies between brain regions, given the complexity of reciprocal and local connectivity, dynamic interactions, and the influence of both shared and independent external inputs, is a challenging task. It requires careful analysis of simultaneous recording data, combined with cross-validation across multiple metrics, to accurately assess the functional relationships between regions. The authors have generated a valuable dataset simultaneously recording from both regions at scale from mice performing a cortex-dependent directional reaching task.

      Using electrophysiological and silencing data, the authors found evidence supporting the traditionally assumed asymmetric influence from PM to M1. While earlier studies inferred a functional hierarchy based on partial temporal relationships in firing patterns, the authors applied a series of complementary analyses to rigorously test this hierarchy at both individual neuron and population levels, with robust statistical validation of significance.

      In addition, recording combined with brief optogenetic silencing of the other region allowed authors to infer the asymmetric functional influence in a more causal manner. This experiment is well designed to focus on the effect of inactivation manifesting through oligosynaptic connections to support the existence of a premotor to primary motor functional hierarchy.

      Subsequent analyses revealed a more complex picture. CCA, PLS, and three measures of predictivity (Granger causality, transfer entropy, and convergent cross-mapping) emphasized similarities in firing patterns and cross-region predictability. However, DLAG suggested an imbalance, with RFA capturing CFA variance at a negative time lag, indicating that RFA 'leads' CFA. Taken together these results provide useful insights for current studies of functional hierarchy about potential limitations in inferring hierarchy solely based on firing rates.

      While I would detail some questions and issues on specifics of data analyses and modeling below, I appreciate the authors' effort in training RNNs that match some behavioral and recorded neural activity patterns including the inactivation result. The authors point out two components that can determine the across-region influence - 1) the amount of inputs received and 2) the dependence on across-region input, i.e., the relative importance of local dynamics, providing useful insights in inferring functional relationships across regions.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Trial-averaging was applied in CCA and PLS analyses. While trial-averaging can be appropriate in certain cases, it leads to the loss of trial-to-trial variance, potentially inflating the perceived similarities between the activity in the two regions (Figure 4). Do authors observe comparable degrees of similarity, e.g., variance explained by canonical variables? Also, the authors report conflicting findings regarding the temporal relationship between RFA and CFA when using CCA/PLS versus DLAG. Could this discrepancy be due to the use of trial-averaging in former analyses but not in the latter?

      We certainly agree that the similarity in firing patterns is higher in trial averages than on single trials, given the variation in single-neuron firing patterns across trials. Here, we were trying to examine the similarity of activity variance that is clearly movement dependent, as trial averages are, and to use an approach aligned with those applied in the existing literature. We would also agree that there is more that can be learned about interactions from trial-by-trial analysis. It is possible that the activity components identified by DLAG as being asymmetric somehow are not reflected strongly in trial averages. In our Discussion we offer another potential explanation that is based on other differences in what is calculated by DLAG and CCA/PLS.

      We also note here that all of the firing pattern predictivity analysis we report (Figure 6) was done on single trial data, and in all cases the predictivity was symmetric. Thus, our results in aggregate are not consistent with symmetry purely being an artifact of trial averaging.

      (2) A key strength of the current study is the precise tracking of forelimb muscle activity during a complex motor task involving reaching for four different targets. This rich behavioral data is rarely collected in mice and offers a valuable opportunity to investigate the behavioral relevance of the PM-M1 functional interaction, yet little has been done to explore this aspect in depth. For example, single-trial time courses of inter-regional latent variables acquired from DLAG analysis can be correlated with single-trial muscle activity and/or reach trajectories to examine the behavioral relevance of inter-regional dynamics. Namely, can trial-by-trial change in inter-regional dynamics explain behavioral variability across trials and/or targets? Does the inter-areal interaction change in error trials? Furthermore, the authors could quantify the relative contribution of across-area versus within-area dynamics to behavioral variability. It would also be interesting to assess the degree to which across-area and within-area dynamics are correlated. Specifically, can acrossarea dynamics vary independently from within-area dynamics across trials, potentially operating through a distinct communication subspace?

      These are all very interesting questions. Our study does not attempt to parse activity into components predictive of muscle activity and others that may reflect other functions. Distinct components of RFA and CFA activity may very well rely on distinct interactions between them.

      (3) While network modeling of RFA and CFA activity captured some aspects of behavioral and neural data, I wonder if certain findings such as the connection weight distribution (Figure 7C), across-region input (Figure 7F), and the within-region weights (Figure 7G), primarily resulted from fitting the different overall firing rates between the two regions with CFA exhibiting higher average firing rates. Did the authors account for this firing rate disparity when training the RNNs?

      The key comparison in Figure 7 is shown in 7F, where the firing rates are accounted for in calculating the across-region input strength. Equalizing the firing rates in RFA and CFA would effectively increase RFA rates. If the mean firing rates in each region were appreciably dependent on across-region inputs, we would then expect an off-setting change in the RFA→CFA weights, such that the RFA→CFA distributions in 7F would stay the same. We would also expect the CFA→RFA weights would increase, since RFA neurons would need more input. This would shift the CFA→RFA (blue) distributions up. Thus, if anything, the key difference in this panel would only get larger. 

      We also generally feel that it is a better approach to fit the actual firing rates, rather than normalizing, since normalizing the firing rates would take us further from the actual biology, not closer.

      (4) Another way to assess the functional hierarchy is by comparing the time courses of movement representation between the two regions. For example, a linear decoder could be used to compare the amount of information about muscle activity and/or target location as well as time courses thereof between the two regions. This approach is advantageous because it incorporates behavior rather than focusing solely on neural activity. Since one of the main claims of this study is the limitation of inferring functional hierarchy from firing rate data alone, the authors should use the behavior as a lens for examining inter-areal interactions.

      As we state above, we agree that examining interactions specific to movement-related activity components could reveal interesting structure in interregional interactions. Since it remains a challenge to rigorously identify a subset of neural activity patterns specifically related to driving muscle activity, any such analysis would involve an additional assumption. It remains unclear how well the activity that decoders use for predicting muscle activity matches the activity that actually drives muscle activity in situ.

      To address this issue, which related to one raised by Reviewer #3 below, we have added an additional paragraph to the Discussion (see “Manifestations of hierarchy in firing patterns”).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This study investigates how two cortical regions that are central to the study of rodent motor control (rostral forelimb area, RFA, and caudal forelimb area, CFA) interact during directional forelimb reaching in mice. The authors investigate this interaction using

      (1) optogenetic manipulations in one area while recording extracellularly from the other, (2) statistical analyses of simultaneous CFA/RFA extracellular recordings, and (3) network modeling.

      The authors provide solid evidence that asymmetry between RFA and CFA can be observed, although such asymmetry is only observed in certain experimental and analytical contexts.

      The authors find asymmetry when applying optogenetic perturbations, reporting a greater impact of RFA inactivation on CFA activity than vice-versa. The authors then investigate asymmetry in endogenous activity during forelimb movements and find asymmetry with some analytical methods but not others. Asymmetry was observed in the onset timing of movement-related deviations of local latent components with RFA leading CFA (computed with PCA) and in a relatively higher proportion and importance of cross-area latent components with RFA leading than CFA leading (computed with DLAG). However, no asymmetry was observed using several other methods that compute cross-area latent dynamics, nor with methods computed on individual neuron pairs across regions. The authors follow up this experimental work by developing a twoarea model with asymmetric dependence on cross-area input. This model is used to show that differences in local connectivity can drive asymmetry between two areas with equal amounts of across-region input.

      Overall, this work provides a useful demonstration that different cross-area analysis methods result in different conclusions regarding asymmetric interactions between brain areas and suggests careful consideration of methods when analyzing such networks is critical. A deeper examination of why different analytical methods result in observed asymmetry or no asymmetry, analyses that specifically examine neural dynamics informative about details of the movement, or a biological investigation of the hypothesis provided by the model would provide greater clarity regarding the interaction between RFA and CFA.

      Strengths:

      The authors are rigorous in their experimental and analytical methods, carefully monitoring the impact of their perturbations with simultaneous recordings, and providing valid controls for their analytical methods. They cite relevant previous literature that largely agrees with the current work, highlighting the continued ambiguity regarding the extent to which there exists an asymmetry in endogenous activity between RFA and CFA.

      A strength of the paper is the evidence for asymmetry provided by optogenetic manipulation. They show that RFA inactivation causes a greater absolute difference in muscle activity than CFA interaction (deviations begin 25-50 ms after laser onset, Figure 1) and that RFA inactivation causes a relatively larger decrease in CFA firing rate than CFA inactivation causes in RFA (deviations begin <25ms after laser onset, Figure 3). The timescales of these changes provide solid evidence for an asymmetry in the impact of inactivating RFA/CFA on the other region that could not be driven by differences in feedback from disrupted movement (which would appear with a ~50ms delay).

      The authors also utilize a range of different analytical methods, showing an interesting difference between some population-based methods (PCA, DLAG) that observe asymmetry, and single neuron pair methods (granger causality, transfer entropy, and convergent cross mapping) that do not. Moreover, the modeling work presents an interesting potential cause of "hierarchy" or "asymmetry" between brain areas: local connectivity that impacts dependence on across-region input, rather than the amount of across-region input actually present.

      Weaknesses:

      There is no attempt to examine neural dynamics that are specifically relevant/informative about the details of the ongoing forelimb movement (e.g., kinematics, reach direction). Thus, it may be preemptive to claim that firing patterns alone do not reflect functional influence between RFA/CFA. For example, given evidence that the largest component of motor cortical activity doesn't reflect details of ongoing movement (reach direction or path; Kaufman, et al. PMID: 27761519) and that the analytical tools the authors use likely isolate this component (PCA, CCA), it may not be surprising that CFA and RFA do not show asymmetry if such asymmetry is related to the control of movement details. 

      An asymmetry may still exist in the components of neural activity that encode information about movement details, and thus it may be necessary to isolate and examine the interaction of behaviorally-relevant dynamics (e.g., Sani, et al. PMID: 33169030).

      To clarify, we are not claiming that firing patterns in no way reflect the asymmetric functional influence that we demonstrate with optogenetic inactivation. Instead, we show that certain types of analysis that we might expect to reflect such influence, in fact, do not. Indeed, DLAG did exhibit asymmetries that matched those seen in functional influence (at least qualitatively), though other methods we applied did not.

      As we state above, we do think that there is more that can be gleaned by looking at influence specifically in terms of activity related to movement. However, if we did find that movement-related activity exhibited an asymmetry following functional influence, our results imply that the remaining activity components would exhibit an opposite asymmetry, such that the overall balance is symmetric. This would itself be surprising. We also note that the components identified by CCA and PLS do show substantial variation across reach targets, indicating that they are not only reflecting condition-invariant components. These analyses were performed on components accounting for well over 90% of the total activity variance, suggesting that both conditiondependent and condition-invariant components should be included.

      To address the concern about condition-dependent and condition-invariant components, we have added a sentence to the Results section reporting our CCA and PLS results: “Because our results here involve the vast majority of trial-averaged activity variance, we expect that they encompass both components of activity that vary for different movement conditions (condition-dependent), and those that do not (condition-invariant).” To address the general concerns about potential differences in activity components specifically related to muscle activity, we have also added an additional paragraph to the Discussion (see “Manifestations of hierarchy in firing patterns”).

      The idea that local circuit dynamics play a central role in determining the asymmetry between RFA and CFA is not supported by experimental data in this paper. The plausibility of this hypothesis is supported by the model but is not explored in any analyses of the experimental data collected. Given the focus on this idea in the discussion, further experimental investigation is warranted.

      While we do not provide experimental support for this hypothesis, the data we present also do not contradict this hypothesis. Here we used modeling as it is often used - to capture experimental results and generate hypotheses about potential explanation. We do feel that our Discussion makes clear where the hypothesis derives from and does not misrepresent the lack of experimental support. We expect readers will take our engagement with this hypothesis with the appropriate grain of salt. The imaginable experiments to support such a hypothesis would constitute another substantial study, requiring numerous controls - a whole other paper in itself.

      Recommendations for the authors:  

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) There are a few small text/figure caption modifications that can be made for clarity of reading:

      (2) Unclear sentence in the second paragraph of the introduction: "For example, stimulation applied in PM has been shown to alter the effects on muscles of stimulation in M1 under anesthesia, both in monkeys and rodents."

      This sentence has been rephrased for clarity: “For example, in anesthetized monkeys34 and rodents35, stimulation in PM alters the effects of stimulation in M1 on muscles.”

      (3) The first section of the results presents the optogenetic manipulation. However, the critical control that tests whether this was strictly a local manipulation that did not affect cells in the other region is introduced only much later. It may be helpful to add a comment in this section noting that such a control was performed, even if it is explained in detail later when introducing the recordings.

      We have added the following to the first Results section: “we show below that direct optogenetic effects were only seen in the targeted forelimb area and not the other.”

      (4) Figure 1D - I imagine these averages are from a single animal, but this is not stated in the figure caption.

      “For one example mouse,” has been added to the beginning of the Figure 1D legend.

      (5) Figure 2F - N=6 is not stated in the panel's caption (though it can make it clearer), while it is stated in the caption of 2H.

      “n = 6 mice” has been added to the Figure 2F legend.

      (6) There's some inconsistency with the order of RFA/CFA in the figures, sometimes RFA is presented first (e.g., Figure 1D and 1F), and sometimes CFA is presented first (e.g., panels of Figure 2).

      We do not foresee this leading to confusion.

      (7) "As expected, the majority of recorded neurons in each region exhibited an elevated average firing rate during movement as compared to periods when forelimb muscles were quiescent (Figure 2D,E; Figure S1A,B)" - Figure S1A,B show histograms of narrow vs. wide waveforms, is this the relevant figure here?

      We apologize for the cryptic reference. The waveform width histograms were referred to here because they enabled the separation of narrow- and wide-waveform cells shown in Figure 2D,E. We have added the following clause to the referenced sentence to make this explicit:  “, both for narrow-waveform, putative interneurons and wide-waveform putative pyramidal neurons.”

      (8) Figure 2I caption - "The fraction of activity variance from 150 ms before reach onset to 150 ms after it that occurs before reach onset" - this sentence is not clear.

      The Figure 2I legend has been updated to “The activity variance in the 150 ms before muscle activity onset, defined as a fraction of the total activity variance from 150 ms before to 150 ms after muscle activity onset, for each animal (circles) and the mean across animals (black bars, n = 6 mice).”

      (9) Figure 4B-G - is this showing results across the 6 animals? Not stated clearly.

      Yes - the 21 sessions we had referred to are drawn from all six mice. We have updated the legend here to make this explicit.

      (10) DLAG analysis - is there any particular reasoning behind choosing four across-region and four within-region components?

      In actuality, we completed this analysis for a broad range of component numbers and obtained similar results in all cases. Four fell in the center of our range, and so we focused the illustrations shown in the figure on this value. In general, the number of components is arbitrary. The original paper from Gokcen et al. describes a method for identifying a lower bound on the number of distinct components the method can identify. However, this method yields different results for each individual recording session. For the comparisons we performed, we needed to use the same range of values for each session.

      (11) Figure 5A seems to show 11 across-session components, it's unclear from the caption but I imagine this should show 12 (4 components times 3 sessions?)

      As we state in the Methods, any across-region latent variable with a lag that failed to converge between the boundary values of ±200 ms was removed from the analysis. In the case illustrated in this panel, the lag for one of the components failed to converge and is not shown. We have now clarified this both in the relevant Results paragraph and in the figure legend.

      (12) Figure 5B - is each marker here the average variance explained by all across/within components that were within the specified lag criteria across sessions per mouse? In other words, what does a single marker here stand for?

      We apologize for the lack of clarity here. These values reflect the average across sessions for each mouse. We have updated the legend to make this explicit.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      As I have addressed most of my major recommendations in the public review, I will use this section to include relatively minor points for the authors to consider.

      (1) The EMG data in Figure 1C shows distinct patterns across spouts, both in the magnitude and complexity of muscle activations. It would be interesting to investigate whether these differences in muscle activity lead to behavioral variations (e.g., reaction time, reach duration) and how they relate to the relative involvement of the two areas.

      We agree that it would be interesting to examine how the interactions between areas vary as behavior varies. While the differences between reaches here are limited, we have addressed this question for two substantially different motor behaviors (reaching and climbing) in a follow-up study that was recently preprinted (Kristl et al., biorxiv, 2025).

      (2) How do the authors account for the lingering impact of RFA inactivation on muscle activity, which persists for tens of milliseconds after laser offset? Could this effect be due to compensatory motor activity following the perturbation? A further illustration of how the raw limb trajectories and/or muscle activity are perturbed and recovered would help readers better understand the impact of motor cortical inactivation.

      To clarify the effects of inactivation on a longer timescale, we have added a new supplemental figure showing the plots from Figure 1D over a longer time window extending to 500 ms after trial onset (new Figure S1). Lingering effects do persist, at least in certain cases. In general, we find it hard to ascertain the source of optogenetic effects on longer timescales like this. On the shortest timescales, effects will be mediated by relatively direct connections between regions. However, on these longer timescales, effects could be due to broader changes in brain and behavioral state that can influence muscle activity. For example, attempts to compensate for the initial disturbance to muscle activity could cause divergence from controls on these longer timescales. Muscle tissue itself is also known to have long timescale relaxation dynamics, and it would not be surprising if the relevant control circuits here also had long timescales dynamics, such that we would not expect an immediate return to control when the light pulse ends. Because of this ambiguity, we generally avoid interpretation of optogenetic effects on these longer timescales.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Page 9: ". We measured the time at which the activity state deviated from baseline preceding reach onset," - I cannot find how this deviation was defined (neither the baseline nor the threshold).

      We have added text to the Figure 2G legend that explicitly states how the baseline and activity onset time were defined.

      (2) Given the shape of the curves in Figure 2G, the significance of this result seems susceptible to slight modifications of what defines a baseline or a deviation threshold. For example, it looks like the circle for CFA has a higher y-axis value, suggesting the baseline deviance is higher, but it is unclear why that would be from the plot. If the threshold for deviation in neural activity state were held uniform between CFA and RFA is the difference still significant across animals?

      We have repeated the analysis using the same absolute threshold for each region. We used the higher of the two thresholds from each region. The difference remains significant. This is now described in the last paragraph of the Results section for Figure 2.

      (3) Since summed deviation of the top 3 PCs is used to show a difference in activity onset between CFA/RFA, but only a small proportion of variance is explained pre-movement (<2% in most animals), it seems relevant to understand what percentage of CFA/RFA neuron activity actually is modulated and deviates from baseline prior to movement and to show the distribution of activity onsets at the single neuron level in CFA/RFA. Can an onset difference only be observed using PCA? 

      Because many neurons have low firing rates, estimating the time at which their firing rate begins to rise near reach onset is difficult to do reliably. It is also true that not all neurons show an increase around onset - some show a decrease and others show no discernible change. Using PCs to measure onset avoids both of these problems, since they capture both increases and decreases in individual neuron firing rates and are much less noisy than individual neuron firing rates. 

      However, based on this comment, we have repeated this analysis on a single-neuron level using only neurons with relatively high average firing rates. Specifically, we analyzed neurons with mean firing rates above the 90th percentile across all sessions within an animal. Neurons whose activity never crossed threshold were excluded. Results matched those using PCs, with RFA neurons showing an earlier average activity onset time. This is now described in the last paragraph of the Results section for Figure 2.

      (4) It is stated that to study the impact of inactivation on CFA/RFA activity, only the 50 highest average firing rate neurons were used (and maybe elsewhere too, e.g., convergent cross mapping). It is unclear why this subselection is necessary. It is justified by stating that higher firing rate neurons have better firing rate estimates. This may be supportable for very low firing rate units that spike sorting tools have a hard time tracking, but I don't think this is supported by data for most of the distribution of firing rates. It therefore seems like the results might be biased by a subselection of certain high firing rate neuron populations. It would be useful to also compute and mention if the results for all neurons/neuron pairs are the same. If there is worry about low-quality units being those with low firing rates, a threshold for firing rate as used elsewhere in the paper (at least 1 spike / 2 trials) seems justified.

      The issue here is that as firing rates decrease and firing rate estimates get noisier, estimates of the change in firing rate get more variable. Here we are trying to estimate the fraction of neurons for which firing rates decreased upon inactivation of the other region. Variability in estimates of the firing rate change will bias this estimate toward 50%, since in the limit when the change estimates are entirely based on noise, we expect 50% to be decreases. As expected, when we use increasingly liberal thresholds for this analysis, the fraction of decreases trends closer to 50%. 

      As a consequence of this, we cannot easily distinguish whether higher firing rate neurons might for some reason have a greater tendency to exhibit decreases in firing compared to lower firing rate neurons. However, we see no positive reason to expect such a difference. We have added a sentence noting this caveat in interpreting our findings to the relevant paragraph of the Results.

      The lack of min/max axis values in Figure 3B-F makes it hard to interpret - are these neurons almost silent when near the bottom of the plot or are they still firing a substantial # of spikes?

      To aid interpretation of the relative magnitude of firing rate changes, we have added minimum firing rates for the averages depicted in Figure 3B,C,E and F to the legend. Our original thinking was that the plots in Figure 3G and H would provide an indication of the relative changes in firing.

      It would be interesting to know if the impact of optogenetic stimulation changed with exposure to the manipulation. Are all results presented only from the first X number of sessions in each animal? Or is the effect robust over time and (within the same animal) you can get the same results of optogenetic inactivation over time? This information seems critical for reproducibility.

      We have now performed brief optogenetic inactivations in several brain areas in several different behavioral paradigms, and have found that inactivation effects are stable both within and across sessions, almost surprisingly so. This includes cases where the inactivations were more frequent (every ~1.25 s on average) and more numerous (>15,000 trials per animal) than in the present manuscript. Thus we did not restrict our analysis here to the first X sessions or trials within a session. We have added additional plots as Figure S3T-AA showing the stability of optogenetic effects both within and across sessions.

      Given that it can be difficult to record from interneurons (as the proportion of putative interneurons in Figure S1 attests), the SALT analyses would be more convincing if a few recordings had been performed in the same region as optogenetic stimulation to show a "positive control" of what direct interneuron stimulation looks like. Could also use this to validate the narrow/wide waveform classification.

      We have verified that using SALT as we have in the present manuscript does detect vGAT+ interneurons directly responding to light. This is included in a recent preprint from the lab (Kristl et al., biorxiv, 2025). We (Warriner et al., Cell Reports, 2022) and others (Guo et al., Neuron, 2014) have previously used direct ChR2 activation to validate waveform-based classification.

      Simultaneous CFA/RFA recordings during optogenetic perturbation would also allow for time courses of inhibition to be compared in RFA/CFA. Does it take 25ms to inhibit locally, and the cross-area impact is fast, or does it inactivate very fast locally and takes ~25ms to impact the other region?

      Latencies of this sort are difficult to precisely measure given the statistical limits of this sort of data, but there does appear to be some degree of delay between local and downstream effects. We do not have a statistical foundation as of yet for concluding that this is the case. It will be interesting to examine this issue more rigorously in the future.

      Given the difference in the analytical methods, the authors should share data in a relatively unprocessed format (e.g., spike times from sorted units relative to video tracking + behavioral data), along with analysis code, to allow others to investigate these differences.

      We plan to post the data and code to our lab’s Github site once the Version of Record is online.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In this paper, the authors reveal that the MK2 inhibitor CMPD1 can inhibit the growth, migration, and invasion of breast cancer cells both in vitro and in vivo by inducing microtubule depolymerization, preferentially at the microtubule plus-end, leading to cell division arrest, mitotic defects, and apoptotic cell death. They also showed that CMPD1 treatment upregulates genes associated with cell migration and cell death, and downregulates genes related to mitosis and chromosome segregation in breast cancer cells, suggesting a potential mechanism of CMPD1 inhibition in breast cancer. Besides, they used the combination of an MK2-specific inhibitor, MK2-IN-3, with the microtubule depolymerizer vinblastine to simultaneously disrupt both the MK2 signaling pathway and microtubule dynamics, and they claim that inhibiting the p38-MK2 pathway may help to enhance the efficacy of MTAs in the treatment of breast cancer. However, there are a few concerns, including:

      (1) What is the effect of CMPD1 on breast cancer metastasis?

      In this study, we hypothesized that the MK2 signaling pathway could synergize with microtubule-targeting agents (MTAs) to enhance anti-cancer efficacy. We utilized CMPD1 as a potent dual-function inhibitor, targeting both MK2 and microtubule dynamics. By simultaneously inhibiting these pathways, CMPD1 not only shows the therapeutic impact of MTAs, but also significantly suppresses breast cancer cell migration and invasion. Therefore, we propose that CMPD1, through its dual inhibition of MK2 activity and microtubule dynamics, may offer enhanced specificity and efficacy in preventing breast cancer metastasis and limiting tumor progression.

      (2) The mechanism is lacking as to how MK2 inhibitors enhance the efficacy of MTAs.

      Thank you for the valuable suggestion. We agree that our current findings do not fully elucidate the underlying mechanism by which MK2 inhibition synergistically enhances the efficacy of MTAs. We recognize this as an important area for further investigation and are committed to exploring the molecular interplay between MK2 signaling and microtubule dynamics in future studies. A deeper mechanistic understanding will be critical to establishing a strong rationale for the potential co-treatment of MK2 inhibitors and MTAs in clinical breast cancer therapy.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study explores the potential of inhibiting the p38-MK2 signaling pathway to enhance the efficacy of microtubule-targeting agents (MTAs) in breast cancer treatment using a dual-target inhibitor.

      Strengths:

      The study identifies the p38-MK2 pathway as a promising target to enhance the efficacy of microtubule-targeting agents (MTAs), offering a novel therapeutic strategy for breast cancer treatment. In addition, the study employs a wide range of techniques, especially live-cell imaging, to assess the microtubule dynamics in TNBC cells.

      We sincerely appreciate your recognition of the significance and impact of our work.

      Weaknesses:

      The study primarily uses RPE1 cells as the control for normal cells, which may not fully capture the response of normal mammary epithelial cells. While CMPD1 is shown to be effective in suppressing tumor growth in MDA-MB-231 xenograft, the study lacks detailed toxicity data to confirm its safety profile in vivo.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestions. In the revised manuscript, we have included CMPD1 treatment in MCF10A cells, a more appropriate non-transformed control line commonly used in breast cancer research. Notably, MCF10A cells exhibited results similar to those observed in RPE1 cells, further reinforcing our conclusion that breast cancer cells display increased sensitivity to CMPD1 treatment. These new findings are presented in Figure 2-Supplement 1A-C. Additionally, we performed further xenograft experiments using CAL-51 and MDA-MB-231 cells. We collected data on tumor growth, mouse body weight, survival rates, and other relevant parameters to comprehensively assess toxicity. The newly obtained results are presented in Figure 3F-G and Figure 3-Supplement 1-3.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors demonstrated MK2i could enhance the therapeutic efficacy of MTAs. With Tumor xenograft and migration assay, the author suggested that the p38-MK2 pathway may serve as a promising therapeutic target in combination with MTAs in cancer treatment.

      Strengths:

      The authors provided a potential treatment for breast cancer.

      Thank you for recognizing the importance and significance of our work.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) In Figure 2, the authors used a human retinal pigment epithelial-1 (RPE1) cell line to show that breast cancer cells are more sensitive to CMPD1 treatment. MCF10A cells would be suggested here as a suitable control. Besides, to compare the sensitivity, IC50 indifferent cell lines should be measured.

      In the revised manuscript, we have addressed these points by determining the IC50 values for CMPD1 in MDA-MB-231, CAL-51, MCF10A, and CAL-51 p53 knockout cells. These new results are presented in Figure 2-Supplement Figure 3.

      (2) The data of MDA-MB-231 in Figure 1D is not consistent with CAL-51 and T47D, also not consistent with the data in Figures 2B-C.

      In the revised manuscript, we have included all relevant statistical analyses in Figure 1D. In MDA-MB-231 cells, there are no statistically significant differences in mitotic duration between 1 µM and 5 µM, 5 µM and 10 µM, or 1 µM and 10 µM CMPD1 treatments. Similarly, no significant differences are observed between 1 µM and 5 µM or 5 µM and 10 µM CMPD1 treatments in CAL-51 cells, and between 5 µM and 10 µM in T-47D cells. These results suggest that mitotic duration does not exhibit a clear dose-dependent relationship within the 1–10 µM range, likely because mitotic arrest has reached a near-plateau effect at these concentrations.

      It is also important to note that the experimental conditions in Figures 1 and 2 are fundamentally different. Figure 1 investigates the effects of higher concentrations of CMPD1 (≥1 µM), which severely disrupt microtubule organization and result in robust mitotic arrest, with cells arrested in mitosis for over 8 hours. In contrast, the conditions in Figure 2 utilize much lower concentrations of CMPD1 (10–50 nM), which are insufficient to cause complete microtubule depolymerization, but are capable of inducing a subtle yet statistically significant mitotic delay, particularly in breast cancer cell lines. These lower concentrations were chosen to mimic clinically relevant intratumoral drug levels. Previous studies have reported that paclitaxel (PTX) concentrations in patient tumors approximate ~50 nM when modeled in vitro. At these physiologically relevant levels, PTX does not induce strong mitotic arrest but instead causes moderate delays that result in division errors and chromosomal instability, ultimately contributing to cancer cell death. In this study, the conditions used in Figure 2 emulate these clinically relevant concentrations for CMPD1. We found that, similar to PTX, low-dose CMPD1 induces a slight but significant mitotic delay without triggering a full mitotic arrest. Notably, unlike PTX, CMPD1 appears to exert this effect selectively in breast cancer cells, contributing to mitotic errors and potentially enhancing therapeutic efficacy through targeted chromosomal instability.

      (3) To support the authors' conclusion in Figure 5, an additional animal experiment performed by tail vein injection would be helpful.

      While current technical limitations have precluded us from conducting this suggested experiment in this study, we have performed complementary xenograft studies using CAL-51 cells treated with CMPD1. These experiments included a comprehensive toxicity analysis. Furthermore, we carried out an in vitro migration assay using CAL-51 cells under combined treatment with the MK2 inhibitor and vinblastine. These additional findings are presented in Figure 3–Supplement 1–3 and Figure 6–Supplement 3. We recognize the importance of the suggested tail vein injection approach and are actively pursuing further mechanistic studies, including this experiment, in our ongoing and future work.

      (4) Page 14, to evaluate the combination result of MK2i and vinblastine, an in vivo animal assay must be performed.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s valuable suggestion. We are actively investigating the synergistic mechanisms between the MK2 inhibitor and microtubule-targeting agents (MTAs). In future studies, we plan to extend our findings by conducting xenograft experiments to further evaluate their therapeutic potential in vivo.

      (5) The authors used RNA-seq to show some pathways affected by CMPD1. What are the key/top genes that were affected? How about the mechanism?

      In the revised manuscript, we have included the top 20 upregulated and downregulated genes identified from RNA-seq analysis using MDA-MB-231 cells. This new data is presented in Figure 6-Supplement Figure 4. Gene Ontology (GO) Biological Process (BP) pathway enrichment analysis revealed that the most significantly enriched pathways among upregulated genes are associated with cell migration, whereas the downregulated genes are primarily involved in mitosis and chromosome segregation. These transcriptional changes are consistent with the phenotypic outcomes observed in our experiments, supporting the functional relevance of CMPD1 treatment. However, further investigation will be necessary to elucidate the detailed molecular mechanisms underlying these effects.

      (6) Line 127, more experiments should be involved to support the conclusion.

      In the revised manuscript, we have addressed this point by performing additional experiments, including determination of the IC₅₀ values of CMPD1 in MDA-MB-231, CAL-51, MCF10A, and CAL-51 p53 knockout cells. We also conducted live-cell imaging analyses using MCF10A cells. These new results further reinforce our conclusion that breast cancer cells are more sensitive to CMPD1 treatment than normal breast epithelial cells, and that this sensitivity is independent of p53 status. The new data are presented in Figure 2-Supplement Figures 1 and 3.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figure 1D: As the concentration of CMPD1 increased, the mitotic duration of MDA-MB-231 cells decreased, why was that?

      Although there appears to be a slight decrease in mitotic duration with increasing concentrations of CMPD1, our quantitative analysis reveals no statistically significant differences among the 1 to 10 µM treatment groups in MDA-MB-231 cells. In the revised manuscript, we have included all relevant statistical analyses in Figure 1D for clarity. Importantly, all CMPD1-treated groups exhibit a pronounced and statistically significant prolongation of mitosis compared to the DMSO-treated control. While the average mitotic duration in control cells is approximately 30 minutes, cells exposed to 1–10 µM CMPD1 consistently display mitotic durations exceeding 8 hours, indicating a strong and sustained mitotic arrest across this concentration range.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The rationale for using RPE1 as normal cell control instead of normal mammary epithelial cells as control is unclear. Using normal mammary epithelial cells such as MCF10A for the study is recommended.

      Thank you for this valuable suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have included additional experiments using non-transformed mammary epithelial MCF10A cells. The new data, presented in Figure 2-Supplement Figures 1 and 3, include both IC50 measurements and live-cell imaging analyses. These results further support our conclusion that breast cancer cells are significantly more sensitive to CMPD1 treatment compared to normal mammary epithelial cells.

      (2) It is intriguing that CAL-51 cells are more sensitive to CMPD1 than MDA-MB-231 cells; examining how p53 signaling changes in these cells would be worthwhile.

      We appreciate this insightful comment. In the revised manuscript, we have measured the IC₅₀ values for both CAL-51 and CAL-51 p53 knockout (p53KO) cells. The results show no significant difference in CMPD1 sensitivity between the two, suggesting that the enhanced sensitivity of CAL-51 cells is independent of p53 status. These new findings are presented in Figure 2—Supplement Figure 3.

      (3) Figures S1A and B are not described and cited in the main text.

      We apologize for this oversight. In the revised manuscript, we have correctly cited and described Figures S1A and B (Figure 2-Supplement Figure 2 A-B in revised manuscript) in the main text.

      (4) I'm not that convinced by the conclusion made from Lines 201-204. First, Figure S2C, which is the growth of tumor volume, does not reflect the toxicity of the drug treatment. No additional data evaluating the toxicity (such as body weight change) under the regimen was shown. Second, although the tumor weight by the endpoint indicated some anti-tumor effect in the MDA-MB-231 xenograft model, the tumor volume does not show the same pattern (the dot lines do not well distinguish which group from which). I would suggest repeating the in vivo experiment using CAL-51 cells since it is more sensitive to CMPD1 according to the previous data.

      Thank you for this thoughtful and constructive feedback. In the revised manuscript, we have addressed these concerns through several additional experiments. We performed new xenograft studies using CAL-51 TNBC cells, in parallel with further toxicity-focused analyses in the MDA-MB-231 model. Consistent with previous results, CMPD1 treatment significantly suppressed tumor growth in CAL-51 xenografts (Figure 3F-G), further supporting its efficacy in a more sensitive cell line. To evaluate drug-associated toxicity, we measured body weight changes throughout the course of treatment. CMPD1-treated mice maintained a comparable weight gain to the control group, whereas mice treated with paclitaxel (PTX) showed significantly reduced body weight (Figure 3-Supplement Figure 2A). Notably, animal deaths occurred only in the PTX-treated groups in both MDA-MB-231 and CAL-51 models (Figure 3-Supplement Figure 2B). We also assessed organ toxicity, including both anatomical and functional evaluations of the kidney and liver, and observed no significant damage in CMPD1-treated mice (Figure 3-Supplement Figures 3A-B and 3D). Furthermore, white blood cell (WBC) counts remained stable in the CMPD1 group, while PTX treatment led to a significant reduction (Figure 3-Supplement Figures 3C-D). These additional data provide strong evidence for the anti-tumor efficacy and lower toxicity of CMPD1 in vivo.

      (5) While I appreciate the combination effect of treating cells with the MK2 inhibitor with vinblastine. I would consider using genetic knockdown as a complementary approach to demonstrate that inhibiting the p38-MK2 pathway synergized with microtubule depolymerizing agents. In addition, could inhibition of the p38-MK2 pathway alone induce the cell growth inhibition observed with CMPD1 treatment?

      Thank you for these important suggestions. In the revised manuscript, we have incorporated siRNA-mediated knockdown of MK2 in combination with vinblastine treatment. This genetic approach revealed synergistic effects on mitotic index and mitotic errors, closely mirroring the phenotypes observed with pharmacological co-treatment using the MK2 inhibitor and vinblastine (Figure 6-Supplement Figure 2A-C). These results further validate the role of the p38-MK2 pathway in modulating mitotic progression in the presence of MTAs. To address whether MK2 inhibition alone is sufficient to impair cell growth, we performed validation experiments using the MK2 inhibitor at 10 µM. At this concentration, the inhibitor effectively blocked phosphorylation of Hsp27, a major downstream substrate of MK2, under H2O2-induced ROS stress conditions (Figure 6-Supplement Figure 1A-B), confirming MK2 signaling pathway inhibition. However, treatment with the MK2 inhibitor alone did not significantly affect cell proliferation, as shown by a 4-day growth curve analysis in CAL-51 cells (Figure 6-Supplement Figure 1C). These findings suggest that inhibition of the p38-MK2 pathway alone is not sufficient to suppress cancer cell growth, and that its synergistic interaction with MTAs, such as vinblastine, is essential for the observed anti-proliferative effects.

      (6) Phenotypic studies (such as anchorage-independent growth and cell migration and invasion assay) of combining MK2 inhibitor with vinblastine in TNBC cells are recommended.

      Thank you for this valuable suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have conducted cancer cell migration assays using CAL-51 TNBC cells treated with control, MK2 inhibitor alone, vinblastine alone, or the combination of both. Our results demonstrate that the combination treatment significantly enhances the inhibition of cell migration compared to either agent alone (Figure 6-Supplement Figure 3A-C). These findings provide additional phenotypic evidence supporting the synergistic interaction between MK2 inhibition and microtubule-targeting agents in TNBC cells.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors can utilize diverse experiments to support their conclusions.

      Thank you for this important suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have conducted a series of additional experiments to robustly support our conclusions.

      These include:

      (1) Xenograft studies using CAL-51 TNBC cells, along with comprehensive toxicity evaluations.

      (2) CMPD1 sensitivity analysis in non-transformed MCF10A mammary epithelial cells.

      (3) IC50 measurements in MDA-MB-231, CAL-51, CAL-51 p53 knockout, and MCF10A cells.

      (4) Cell migration assays assessing the combination effects of MK2 inhibitor and vinblastine

      (5) siRNA-mediated genetic knockdown of MK2 to complement pharmacological findings

      Collectively, these additional data sets substantially strengthen the evidence base for our conclusions and provide a more comprehensive mechanistic understanding.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In this study, Marocco and colleagues perform a deep characterization of the complex molecular mechanism guiding the recognition of a particular CELLmotif previously identified in hepatocytes in another publication. Having miR-155-3p with or without this CELLmotif as the initial focus, the authors identify 21 proteins differentially binding to these two miRNA versions. From there, they decided to focus on PCBP2. They elegantly demonstrate PCBP2 binding to the miR-155-3p WT version but not to the CELLmotif-mutated version. miR-1553p contains a hEXOmotif identified in a different report, whose recognition is largely mediated by another RNA-binding protein called SYNCRIP. Interestingly, mutation of the hEXOmotif contained in miR-155-3p did not only blunt SYNCRIP binding but also PCBP2 binding despite the maintenance of the CELLmotif. This indicates that somehow SYNCRIP binding is a pre-requisite for PCBP2 binding. EMSA assay confirms that SYNCRIP is necessary for PCBP2 binding to miR-155-3p, while PCBP2 is not needed for SYNCRIP binding. The authors aim to extend these findings to other miRNAs containing both motifs. For that, they perform a small-RNA-Seq of EVs released from cells knockdown for PCBP2 versus control cells, identifying a subset of miRNAs whose expression either increases or decreases. The assumption is that those miRNAs containing PCBP2-binding CELLmotif should now be less retained in the cell and go more to extracellular vesicles, thus reflecting a higher EV expression. The specific subset of miRNAs having both the CELLmotif and hEXOmotif (9 miRNAs) whose expressions increase in EVs due to PCBP2 reduction is also affected by knocking-down SYNCRIP in the sense that reduction of SYNCRIP leads to lower EV sorting. Further experiments confirm that PCBP2 and SYNCRIP bind to these 9 miRNAs and that knocking down SYNCRIP impairs their EV sorting.

      We thank this Reviewer for the time spent on our manuscript and for having appreciated our characterization of the present molecular mechanism controlling miRNA export/cellretention in hepatocytes.

      While the process studied in this work is novel and interesting, there are several aspects of this manuscript that should be improved:

      (1) First of all, the nature of the CELLmotif and the hEXOmotif they are studying is extremely confusing. For the CELLmotif, the authors seem to focus on the Core CELLmotif AUU A/G in some experiments and the extended 7-nucleotide version in others. The fact that these CELLmotif and hEXOmotif are not shown anywhere in the figures (I mean with the full nucleotide variability described in the original publications) but only referred to in the text further complicates the identification of the motifs and the understanding of the experiments. Moreover, I am not convinced that the sequences they highlight in grey correspond to the original CELLmotif in all cases. For instance, in the miR-155-3p sequence, GCAUU is highlighted in grey. However, the original CELLmotif is basically 7-nucleotide long: C, A/U, G/A/C, U, U/A, C/G/A, A/U/C or CAGUUCA in its more abundant version. I can only see clearly the presence of the Core CELLmotif AUUA in miR-155-3p; however, the last A is not highlighted in grey. It is true that there is some nucleotide variability in each position in the originally reported CELLmotif by the authors in ref. 5 and the hEXOmotifs in ref. 7; however, not all nucleotides are equally likely to be found in each position. This fact seems to be not to be taken into account by the authors as they took basically any sequence with any length and almost sequence combination as valid CELLmotif. This means that I cannot identify the CELLmotif in many cases among the ones they highlight in grey. Instead, they should really focus on the most predominant CELLmotif sequence or, instead, take a reduced subset of "more abundant" CELLmotif versions from the ones that could fit in the originally described CELLmotif. Altogether, the authors need to explain much better what they have considered as the CELLmotif, what is the Core CELLmotif and what is hEXOmotif in each case and restrict to the most likely versions of the CELLmotif and hEXOmotif.

      We thank the Reviewer for having raised this concern and indeed we must agree with her/him and therefore, we modified the text and the figure accordingly. 

      In brief, as now stated, with respect to the CELL motif, miR 155-3p, miR-155-5p, miR-181d5p, miR-3084-5p, miR-122b-3p, miR-192-5p, miR-26b-3p, miR-31-3p, miR-195a-5p and miR-421-3p have the Core CELL motif (AUUA/G) described by Garcia- Martin and colleagues (ref. 5). 

      Other miRNAs (miR-345-3p, miR-23a-5p and miR-214-3p) share the described CELL motif (ref.5) with the most frequent nucleotides, considering also the reported variability. Also for the hEXO motif described by Santangelo and collaborators (ref.7), the most frequent nucleotides defining the motif sequence have been taken into consideration. The motifs have been better highlighted in the new version of Fig. 1 panel C.

      (2) Validation of EV isolation method: first, a large part of Supplementary Figure 2 is not readable. EV markers seem to be enriched in EV isolates; however, more EV and cell markers should be assayed to fulfill MISEV guidelines.

      We apologize for the low quality of the figure. In order to address this issue, we replaced the Supplementary Figure 2 panel A (now panel B) and we added further EV markers (TSG101, Alix, Flotillin) in Supplementary Figure 2 panel B (now panel A). Notably, in the same Western blot analysis we also addressed the expression of SYNCRIP and PCBP2 (that were found in the cellular end EV compartments or only in the intracellular compartment respectively).

      (3) A key variable is missing in Supplementary Figure 2, which is whether PCBP2 or SYNCRIP knockdowns impair EV secretion rates. A quantification of the nr vesicles released per cell upon knocking down each of these factors would be essential to rule out that any of the effects seen throughout the paper are not due to reduced or enhanced EV production rather than miRNA sorting/retention.

      We addressed this issue by quantifying the number of EVs per cell in shPCBP2 or shSYNCRIP with respect to the shCTR conditions. Data are shown in the new Supplementary Figure 2 panel C and indicate that there are not significant differences on EVs production rate upon PCBP2 or SYNCRIP knockdown.

      (4) The EMSA experiment is important to support their claims. Given the weak bands that are shown, the authors need to show all their replicates to convince the readers that it is reproducible.

      We are aware that the signals appear faint; the experimental replicates showing the robustness of the observation are reported below. 

      Author response image 1.

      (5) Although the bindings of SYNCRIP and PCBP2 to miR-155-3p and other miRNAs having both hEXOmotif and CELLmotif seem clear, the need for SYNCRIP binding to allow for PCBP2-mediated cellular retention is counterintuitive. What happens to those miRNAs that only contain a CELLmotif in terms of cellular retention and SYNCRIP dependence for cellular retention? In this regard, a representative miRNA (miR-31-3p) is analyzed in several experiments, showing that PCBP2 does not bind to it unless a hEXOmotif is introduced (Figure 3). However, this type of experiment should definitely be extended to other miRNAs containing only CELLmotif without hEXOmotif.

      Based on the Reviewer’s suggestion we confirmed previous findings by extending the observation to further two miRNAs embedding the sole CELL-motif (miR-195a-5p and 4213p) whose sequences are reported in Figure 4C. Data relative to qPCR amplification are reported in Figure 4D, Figure 5 panels A-B, Figure 6 and new Supplementary Figure 3. They confirm that miRNAs only containing CELL motif are not EV-exported in dependence of SYNCRIP and are cell-retained independently of PCBP2 silencing. 

      (6) Along the same line, I am missing another important experiment: the artificial incorporation of CELLmotif. For example, miR-365-2-5p lacks a CELLmotif but has a hEXOmotif. Does PCBP2 bind to this miRNA upon incorporation of CELLmotif? Does this lead now to enhanced cellular retention of this miRNA?

      We are grateful for the Reviewer's concern. As suggested, we added RNA pull-down experiments for miR-365-2-5p in wild type form and in mutated form (with the inclusion of CELL motif). As reported in the new Figure 1 panel E, the addition of the CELL motif maintains SYNCRIP binding and allows PCBP2 interaction with this miRNA. 

      (7) What would be the net effect of knocking down both SYNCRIP and PCBP2 at the same time? Would this neutralize each other's effect or would the lack of one impose on the other? That could help in understanding the complex interplay between these two factors for mediating cellular retention and EV sorting.

      SYNCRIP and PCBP2 play opposite roles in the dynamics of miRNA retention/export. SYNCRIP is involved in the loading of miRNAs into EVs through the recognition of hEXO motif. Instead, PCBP2 is involved in cellular retention of miRNAs, acting as a negative regulator of SYNCRIP activity. PCBP2 binding and function requires both CELL-motif and SYNCRIP binding in order to negatively regulate miRNAs export into EVs.

      Being SYNCRIP silencing sufficient to cause miRNA retention (as shown in Supplementary Figure 3), we believe that the contemporary silencing of PCBP2 should not disclose any additional aspect on cellular retention and EV sorting dynamics.

      (8) The authors have here a great opportunity to shed some light on an unclear aspect of miRNA EV sorting and cellular retention: whether the RBPs go together with the miRNA to the EVs or not. While the original paper describing hEXOmotif found SYNCRIP in EVs, another publication (Jeppesen et al, Cell 2019; PMID: 30951670) later found this RBP being very scarce in small EVs compared to cellular bodies or large EVs (Supplementary Tables 3 and 4 in that publication). Can the authors find SYNCRIP and PCBP2 in the EVs? Another important question would be the colocalization of these RBPs in the place where the miRNA selection is supposed to take place: in multivesicular bodies (MVB). Is there a colocalization of these RBPs with MVBs in the cell?

      We are thankful for the Reviewer’s suggestions. As reported in Supplementary Figure 2A SYNCRIP is present in both the intracellular end EV compartment and PCBP2 is detectable only in the intracellular one.

      (9) In Figure 4C, the authors state in the text that CELLmotif and hEXOmotif are present in extra-seed region; however, for miR-181d-5p and miR-122-3p this is not true as their CELLmotifs fall within the seed sequence.

      We apologize for our mistake. While for hEXO motif, it is confirmed that it is present in extraseed region on all analyzed miRNAs (as in ref. 7), the CELL motif on the cited miRNAs is overlapping with the seed sequence. We modified the text accordingly.

      (10) The authors need to describe how they calculate the EV/cell ratio in gene expression in some experiments (for instance, Figures 1H, 4D, etc). Did they use any housekeeping gene for EV RNA content, the same RNA load, or some other alternative method to normalize EV vs cell RNA content?

      We apologize for having not well clarified the calculation of EV/cell ratio in the cited figures. Data are shown as ratio of miRNAs expression in EVs with respect to the intracellular compartment. Expression of miRNAs in both compartments are normalized with respect to the spike-in sequence (cel-miR-39-3p), included in miRNAs sample (EVs and intracellular samples). This is also better clarified in the Materials and Methods section.

      (11) I would suggest that the authors speculate a bit in the discussion section on how the interaction between PCBP2 and SYNCRIP takes place. Do they contain any potential interacting domain? The binding of one to the miRNA would impose a topological interference on the binding of the other?

      We now speculate on the interaction between PCBP2 and SYNCRIP in the discussion section. Briefly, we described that PCBP2 interaction with several proteins have been reported (as in PMID 19881509 and 10772858), indicating the C-terminal domain including also the two KH1 and KH2 regions as the domains with the highest propensity interaction with proteins. Also in the case of SYNCRIP binding, the domains of interaction with proteins have been reported (as in PMID 10734137, 29483512 and 16765914) and we should hypothesize that these domains represent conserved regions responsible for its interaction also with PCBP2. Moreover, we also discussed that upon the interaction between SYNCRIP and the miRNAs a topological switch can occur, impacting the affinity of PCBP2 for the same miRNAs. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The author of this manuscript aimed to uncover the mechanisms behind miRNA retention within cells. They identified PCBP2 as a crucial factor in this process, revealing a novel role for RNA-binding proteins. Additionally, the study discovered that SYNCRIP is essential for PCBP2's function, demonstrating the cooperative interaction between these two proteins. This research not only sheds light on the intricate dynamics of miRNA retention but also emphasizes the importance of protein interactions in regulating miRNA behavior within cells.

      We thank this Reviewer for having appreciated our characterization of the molecular dynamics governing miRNA export/cell-retention in hepatocytes.

      Strengths:

      This paper makes important progress in understanding how miRNAs are kept inside cells. It identifies PCBP2 as a key player in this process, showing a new role for proteins that bind RNA. The study also finds that SYNCRIP is needed for PCBP2 to work, highlighting how these proteins work together. These discoveries not only improve our knowledge of miRNA behavior but also suggest new ways to develop treatments by controlling miRNA locations to influence cell communication in diseases. The use of liver cell models and thorough experiments ensures the results are reliable and show their potential for RNA-based therapies

      Weaknesses:

      Despite its strengths, the manuscript has several notable limitations. The study's exclusive focus on hepatocytes limits the applicability of the findings to other cell types and physiological contexts. While the interaction between PCBP2 and SYNCRIP is wellcharacterized, the manuscript lacks detailed insights into the structural basis of this interaction and the dynamic regulation of their binding. The generalization of the findings to a broader spectrum of miRNAs and RNA-binding proteins (RBPs) remains underexplored, leaving gaps in understanding the full scope of miRNA compartmentalization.

      Furthermore, the therapeutic implications of these findings, though promising, are not directly connected to specific disease models or clinical scenarios, reducing their immediate translational impact. The manuscript would also benefit from a deeper discussion of potential upstream regulators of PCBP2 and SYNCRIP and the influence of cellular or environmental factors on their activity. Additionally, it is important to note that SYNCRIP has already been recognized as a major regulator of miRNA loading in extracellular vesicles (EVs). However, the purity of EVs is a concern, as the author only performed crude extraction methods without further purification using an iodixanol density gradient. The study also lacks in vivo evidence of PCBP2's role in exosomal miRNA export.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Improve figure quality in some cases (Figures 1A, 4B, Supplementary Figure 2).

      Figures have been improved accordingly. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Questions for the Authors:

      (1) Why was hepatocyte-specific data prioritized, and how generalizable are the findings to other cell types?

      This work is based on our previous publication (Santangelo et al., Cell Reports), concerning the identification of the RBP SYNCRIP as an actor in the loading machinery of miRNAs in Extracellular Vesicles in hepatocytes. Since  both SYNCRIP and  PCBP2 are  expressed in different cell types  (Keerthikumar et al., 2016, PMID: 26434508; https://www.bgee.org/gene/ENSMUSG00000056851), is conceivable  that our  findings can be translated also in other cellular systems. To formally proof this hypothesis seems out of the scope of this manuscript. 

      (2) Can the authors elaborate on the functional impact of PCBP2-mediated miRNA retention? Which biological pathways are directly influenced by miRNAs retained by PCBP2?

      We appreciate the suggestion; in line with this comment, we performed a Gene Ontology enrichment analysis of the targets of the retained miRNAs. In order to be the most exhaustive, we included both validated and predicted targets, respectively obtained from TarBase v9.0 database and DIANA-microT web server. As reported in the new figure 4, panel E, and in the new supplementary figure 4, the analysis highlighted several biological pathways collectively influenced by the PCBP2-dependent cell-retained miRNAs, including establishment of organelle localization, regulation of cell cycle and lymphocyte differentiation. 

      (3) What criteria were used to select the miRNAs (e.g., miR-155-3p) for this study?

      miR-155-3p was selected as initial bait for RNA pulldown based on the reported presence of Core CELL motif in AML12 cell line (PMID: 34937935).

      (4) How do the results using recombinant PCBP2 in RNA pull-down assays compare with those using native PCBP2 in cellular extracts?

      The RNA-pull down with recombinant PCBP2 confirms the evidence obtained by RNA-pull downs with cellular extracts. Indeed, PCBP2 interacts with miR-155-3p in the wild type form and this interaction is lost upon the mutation of the CELL motif. Moreover, this experiment highlights a direct and sequence specific interaction.  

      (5) How much protein was loaded for Western blot analysis?

      We’re sorry for not explaining the experimental procedure in depth. For protein expression analysis, as reported in supplementary figure 1 and 2, we loaded 30 µg of proteins. Half of the amount of the protein obtained upon either RNA pull-down or protein immunoprecipitation experiments performed using 2 mg of protein extract were analyzed. This information has been added to the methods section.

      Suggested experiments to strengthen the manuscript:

      (1) Purify EVs using an iodixanol density gradient to eliminate the possibility of soluble PCBP2 contamination.

      We appreciate this suggestion. In order to avoid the effect of PCBP2 contamination that represents a source of variability in the experiments, we evaluated its presence in the purified extracellular vesicles protein extracts. As reported in the new figure Supplementary 2A, PCBP2 is completely absent in EV extracts as assessed by Western Blot; thus, accordingly with MISEV guideline we followed the differential ultracentrifugation method for EV purification. 

      (2) Perform gain- and loss-of-function assays by overexpressing or silencing PCBP2 in various models to observe downstream changes in miRNA-dependent pathways.

      We chose to silence PCBP2 protein since its high expression in our cellular model. Overexpression of PCBP2 would probably have no other significant readout. We are aware that PCBP2 silencing would perturb miRNA biogenesis and in turn miRNA downstream pathway modulation. Indeed, its association with Dicer has been reported as propaedeutic to miRNA processing (Li, et al 2012, Cell metabolism). However, this aspect is out of the scope of the present manuscript, here we focus exclusively on PCBP2 role in the regulation of miRNA EV export. Moreover, to overcome the effects on miRNA processing we evaluated the expression level of each miRNA as ratio between the extracellular and intracellular compartment. 

      (3) Use a murine model with hepatocyte-specific PCBP2 knockout and track changes in EV miRNA content and their functional effects in target tissues.

      We took advantage of our murine cells silenced for PCBP2 and evaluated miRNA content.

      New functional assays (now included in the new figure 4F) with leukocytes obtained from C57BL/6J mice livers show a higher percentage of IFN-+ T cell, NK and myeloid cells upon shPCBP2 EVs treatment in comparison to the shCTR EVs; this suggests that PCBP2 silencing results into an EV-mediated modulation of the immune response.

      (4) Conduct co-culture experiments to assess EV-mediated intercellular communication between donor and recipient cells.

      We reasoned that co-culture experiments don’t limit the observed effect on EVs since the contribution of soluble factors can have a role on recipient cells. Conversely  the treatments with purified EVs, here performed,  allow the evaluation of the sole EV-mediated downstream effects.

      These experiments would provide insights into the PCBP2-SYNCRIP axis, broaden the applicability of the findings, and enhance their translational relevance.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors use the teleost medaka as an animal model to study the effect of seasonal changes in day-length on feeding behaviour and oocyte production. They report a careful analysis of how day-length affects female medakas and a thorough molecular genetic analysis of genes potentially involved in this process. They show a detailed analysis of two genes and include a mutant analysis of one gene to support their conclusions

      Strengths:

      The authors pick their animal model well and exploit the possibilities to examine in this laboratory model the effect of a key environmental influence, namely the seasonal changes of day-length. The phenotypic changes are carefully analysed and well-controlled. The mutational analysis of the agrp1 by a ko-mutant provides important evidence to support the conclusions. Thus this report exceeds previous findings on the function of agrp1 and npyb as regulators of food-intake and shows how in medaka these genes are involved in regulating the organismal response to an environmental change. It thus furthers our understanding of how animals react to key exogenous stimuli for adaptation.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors are too modest when it comes to underscoring the importance of their findings. Previous animal models used to study the effect of these neuropeptides on feeding behaviour have either lost or were most likely never sensitive to seasonal changes of day length. Considering the key importance of this parameter on many aspects of plant and animal life it could be better emphasised that a suitable animal model is at hand that permits this. The molecular characterization of the agrp1 ko-mutant that the authors have generated lacks some details that would help to appreciate the validity of the mutant phenotype. Additional data would help in this respect.

      We would like to thank Reviewer #1 for the really constructive advice. In the revised manuscript, we provided more information on the molecular characterization of the agrp1 KO-mutant and to emphasize the importance of our present animal model that permits the analysis of neuropeptide effects on feeding behavior in response to seasonal changes of day length.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigated the mechanisms behind breeding season-dependent feeding behavior using medaka, a well-known photoperiodic species, as a model. Through a combination of molecular, cellular, and behavioral analyses, including tests with mutants, they concluded that AgRP1 plays a central role in feeding behavior, mediated by ovarian estrogenic signals.

      Strengths:

      This study offers valuable insights into the neuroendocrine mechanisms that govern breeding season-dependent feeding behavior in medaka. The multidisciplinary approach, which includes molecular and physiological analyses, enhances the scientific contribution of the research.

      Weaknesses:

      While medaka is an appropriate model for studying seasonal breeding, the results presented are insufficient to fully support the authors' conclusions.

      Specifically, methods and data analyses are incomplete in justifying the primary claims:<br /> - the procedure for the food intake assay is unclear;

      - the sample size is very small;

      - the statistical analysis is not always adequate.

      Additionally, the discussion fails to consider the possible role of other hormones that may be involved in the feeding mechanism.

      We would like to thank Reviewer #2 for the helpful comments. As the reviewer suggested, we revised the paragraph describing the procedure for the food intake assay to make it much easier for the readers to understand in the revised manuscript. In Figure 1-Supplementary figure 2, RNAseq was performed to search for the candidate neuropeptides, and that’s why the sample size was the minimum. On the other hand, each group in the other experiments consist of n ≥ 5 samples, which is usually accepted to be adequate sample size in various studies (cf. Kanda et al., Gen Comp Endocrinol., 2011, Spicer et al., Biol Reprod., 2017). As for the statistical analyses, we revised our manuscript so that the readers may be convinced with the validity of our statistical analyses.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Understanding the mechanisms whereby animals restrict the timing of their reproduction according to day length is a critical challenge given that many of the most relevant species for agriculture are strongly photoperiodic. However, the principal animal models capable of detailed genetic analysis do not respond to photoperiod so this has inevitably limited progress in this field. The fish model medaka occupies a uniquely powerful position since its reproduction is strictly restricted to long days and it also offers a wide range of genetic tools for exploring, in depth, various molecular and cellular control mechanisms.

      For these reasons, this manuscript by Tagui and colleagues is particularly valuable. It uses the medaka to explore links bridging photoperiod, feeding behaviour, and reproduction. The authors demonstrate that in female, but not male medaka, photoperiod-induced reproduction is associated with an increase in feeding, presumably explained by the high metabolic cost of producing eggs on a daily basis during the reproductive period. Using RNAseq analysis of the brain, they reveal that the expression of the neuropeptides agrp and npy that have been previously implicated in the regulation of feeding behaviour in mice are upregulated in the medaka brain during exposure to long photoperiod conditions. Unlike the situation in mice, these two neuropeptides are not co-expressed in medaka neurons, and food deprivation in medaka led to increases in agrp but also a decrease in npy expression. Furthermore, the situation in fish may be more complicated than in mice due to the presence of multiple gene paralogs for each neuropeptide. Exposure to long-day conditions increases agrp1 expression in medaka as the result of increases in the number of neurons expressing this neuropeptide, while the increase in npyb levels results from increased levels of expression in the same population of cells. Using ovariectomized medaka and in situ hybridization assays, the authors reveal that the regulation of agrp1 involves estrogen acting via the estrogen receptor esr2a. Finally, a loss of agrp1 function mutant is generated where the female mutants fail to show the characteristic increase in feeding associated with long-day enhanced reproduction as well as yielding reduced numbers of eggs during spawning.

      Strengths:

      This manuscript provides important foundational work for future investigations aiming to elucidate the coordination of photoperiod sensing, feeding activity, and reproduction function. The authors have used a combination of approaches with a genetic model that is particularly well suited to studying photoperiodic-dependent physiology and behaviour. The data are clear and the results are convincing and support the main conclusions drawn. The findings are relevant not only for understanding photopriodic responses but also provide more general insight into links between reproduction and feeding behaviour control.

      Weaknesses:

      Some experimental models used in this study, namely ovariectomized female fish and juvenile fish have not been analysed in terms of their feeding behaviour and so do not give a complete view of the position of this feeding regulatory mechanism in the context of reproduction status. Furthermore, the scope of the discussion section should be expanded to speculate on the functional significance of linking feeding behaviour control with reproductive function.

      We would like to thank Reviewer #3 for the insightful advice. We added several pertinent sentences describing the ovariectomized female fish and juvenile fish, and our revised manuscript will give more complete view of their feeding regulatory mechanism in the context of reproduction status. In addition, we revised the discussion section to incorporate the valuable suggestion of the Reviewer #3.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      General: the text could profit from a careful editing of errors, including adjusting singular and plural status of nouns and verbs: examples are line 107 noun, line 96 verb suitable text editing software is available to do this task

      Thank you for your suggestion. We thoroughly read the entire manuscript and corrected such errors in the revised manuscript.

      As medaka is a unique genetic vertebrate model to study seasonal effects, it would be interesting to know whether the authors found novel or rather unexpected genes with a differential expression between LD and SD. It is understandable that the authors focused on argrp1 and npyb, as these have already been well studied in mammalian models although not in this context. Novel insights with genes previously not implicated in feeding regulation could underscore the unique nature of medaka as a model.

      We appreciate your kind comments, which we found really encouraging to us. Since we focused on feeding-related peptides, we did not find any novel genes that have not been reported.

      ISH is unreliable as a methodology to quantify expression levels. Yet the authors use this to compare fed and starved females to compare expression levels of agrp1. They use a temporal staining comparison and compare 90-minute and 300-minute staining reactions. However, they do not explain why they use the 90-minute staining time point and why 300 minutes of staining is the "saturation point of staining". They should provide compelling data for their claim and the selection of time points or else refrain from using these (at best) semi-quantitative ISH and provide more detailed (using serial sections) data to quantify the number of expressing cells.

      Anyhow, the quantification of mRNA expression levels may not be that significant when trying to compare different states of gene function, as translational and post-translational steps can have large effects on gene function. This should be discussed adequately.

      Thank you very much for your comments. We conducted ISH by using medaka under LD or SD, not using those under fed or starved conditions. In addition, our previous study demonstrated that the slopes of the increase in the number of cells stained by ISH are also different if there is a difference in the expression level (Mitani et al., 2010). Although we do not have quantitative data of cell numbers, we confirmed that the number of cells expressing agrp1 was saturated around 300 mins in our preliminary experiments, and therefore we terminated the chemogenic reactions at 300 mins. Based on these, we compared the cell ratio of 90 min (beginning of coloring) /300 min (saturation). However, since this analysis may not be worth discussing in detail, we moved this part to the supplementary figure as the reviewer suggested.

      The molecular characterization of the agrp1 ko mutant is a bit thin.

      Line 221: "We obtained agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> medaka, which has lots of amino acid changes in functional site for AgRP1" is a bit vague as a description for the ko-mutation. It would be really helpful if the authors could provide a scheme showing the wt protein with the relevant functional sites alongside the presumptive mutant protein.

      How did the authors verify the molecular nature of their mutation? They should use suitable antibodies and western-blot analysis (maybe reagents from Shainer et al., 2019 work in medaka); in case this is not possible they could isolate & clone the mutant transcript and use in-vitro translation systems to show that the presumptive mutant protein can actually be translated from this transcript. Another strategy could be to use a second non-allelic and (hopefully) non-complementing mutation (ko1/ko2 heterozygots for example) to show that ko-mutation acts the way the authors presume. The authors mention agrp1 ko medaka lines (plural!) in line 520, thus they may have an additional ko allele at hand.

      Thank you very much for your comments. We explained the mutation site in Figure 6-Supplementary Figure 1 (A: DNA sequences and B: predicted amino acid sequence, of WT and mutants). In addition, we added immunohistochemistry data of WT and mutant using anti-AgRP antibody (Figure 6-Supplementary Figure 1C). While AgRP-immunoreactive signals were observed in WT, those were not in agrp1<sup>−/−</sup>. This result suggests that AgRP1 is not functional in agrp1<sup>−/−</sup>.

      Presumably, the authors analysed heterozygous agrp1<sup>+/−</sup> females and found they are as wt. If so the authors should say so.

      Yes, we analyzed food intake of agrp1<sup>+/−</sup>. We added a supplementary figure (Figure 6-Supplementary Figure 2) and a sentence in L. 233-234.

      How about agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> medaka males: do they show a discernible phenotype?

      We analyzed the phenotypes of agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> males but did not describe the results, since the present paper only focused on female-specific feeding behavior.

      agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> females show no significant sensitivity of food intake to day length (Figure 6C). Does their (reduced) oocyte production react to day length? With other words: how much of the seasonal sensitivity is left in agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> females. The authors suggest that E2 acts upstream of agrp1 and therefore some seasonality may still be left in agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> females.

      Although agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> female is suggested to display abnormal seasonality of food intake, agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> female in LD spawns and that in SD does not, indicating that seasonality of gonadal maturation still remains in agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> female.

      The authors show that fshb and lhb are downregulated in agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> females. Is this also the case in wt females at SD?

      Thank you very much for your comment. As described above, agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> can spawn, which indicates that mechanisms for the downregulation of gonadotropins in agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> may be different from that in SD female.

      Figure 1_Supplementary Figure 2: the trends are visible in B and C, however, there is quite some variance between LD1, 2, and 3; the same for SD 1, 2, and 3. Can the authors give an explanation for this?

      Since the data for LD1, 2, and 3 (SD1, 2, and 3) were obtained from different individual fish, the variance may be reasonable. We conducted expression analyses by using RNA-seq to find candidate genes that show larger differences than individual ones.

      Figure 7E: the ovaries are difficult to see and the size bar in the wt picture is missing.

      Thank you very much for your comments. We added a scale bar in the wt picture.

      509 ff: the authors do not describe what exactly the "sham operation" encompasses: were the females just anesthetised or was there an actual operation without removing the ovaries?

      The sham operation group was anesthetized, received an abdominal incision without removing the ovaries, and received skin suture by using a silk thread. We added this explanation in the Method section.

      519 ff: was the agrp1<sup>−/−</sup> ko induced in the d-rR strain to have the same genetic background as the wt fish?

      Exactly. As the reviewer pointed out, the genetic background of agrp1 -/- was the same as that of WT.

      Minor points (Text edits):

      Line 42: change "when" into "where".

      Line: 54 "under the fixed appropriate ambient temperature" change into "while keeping an appropriate temperature constant".

      Line 55: here it would be good to briefly explain what long-day and short-day is so that the reader has an idea about the changes required without having to scroll down to the M&M section. For example LD 14/10 light-dark cycle, SD 10/14 light-dark cycle.

      Line 88: change "measurement" into "measuring".

      Line 96 change eats -> eat.

      Line 107 change female -> females.

      We deeply appreciate the reviewer’s suggestions described above. We corrected them as the reviewer suggested (L. 42, L. 54, L. 55, L. 89, L. 96, L. 107).

      Line 144-145: the sentence "since hypothalamic npy control..." does not make sense. Please correct.

      Thank you very much for your suggestion. We corrected the sentence so that it makes sense (L. 145-146).

      Line 180 and 185: the term here should be "LD induced sexual activity" rather than maturity. Age is the main determinant of maturity whereas light (LD) determines activity, in other words SD females are sexually mature if they are post-puberty stage.

      Thank you very much for your suggestion. Since the sentence “LD-induced sexual maturity” made the reviewer confused, we corrected the sentence “substance(s) from LD-induced mature ovary” or “ovarian maturity”. Even though SD females are at post-puberty stage, their ovaries are immature and do not possess mature oocytes (L. 181).

      Line 222: the authors should include the relevant information about the females: presumably agrp1.

      In Line 226-228, we explained the phenotypes of agrp1 knockout and added information for AgRP1 protein in Figure 6-Supplementary figure 1C.

      Lines 449 ff: authors should state that the analysis was done in females, instead of just writing "medaka". This is also in line with the preceding paragraph of the M&M section.

      Thank you very much for your suggestions. We corrected the sentence as the reviewer suggested (L.469)

      Line 305: change like other mammals -> like in mammals.

      Thank you very much for your suggestion. We corrected the sentence as the reviewer suggested (L. 320)

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The procedure of the food intake assay is not clear.

      - Habituation Period: Medaka were placed into a white cup containing 100 mL of water and allowed to habituate for 5 minutes. However, is 5 minutes sufficient to reduce stress in the fish? A stressed fish does not exhibit the same feeding behavior as an unstressed one.

      Thank you for your comment. We confirmed that 5 minutes is enough for habituation in medaka, since medaka can swim freely in a few minutes after replacement from the tank and show normal feeding behavior.

      - Feeding Protocol: Medaka were fed with 200 μL aliquots of brine shrimp-containing water. This procedure was repeated multiple times. How many times was this feeding procedure repeated? Was it 3, 10, or 100 times?

      Although there was a small variation in each trial, we usually applied tubes about 5 times or so.

      - Brine Shrimp Counting: You collected 10 mL of the breeding water to count the number of uneaten brine shrimp. Can you confirm that sampling 10% of the total volume is representative? Were any tests conducted to validate this? Given that you developed an automated tool to count the brine shrimp, why didn't you count them in all 100 mL?

      The reason for collecting 10 mL is to collect the leftover shrimp as soon as possible. Ten mins after the start of the experiment, we quickly placed a magnetic bar to stir the breeding water so that the shrimp concentration will be constant. Then we collected 10 mL aliquot from the experimental cup by using a micro pipette. In preliminary trials, we applied shrimps, the amount of which is almost the same as that applied to WT medaka in LD, to a white cup containing 100 mL water, and we divided it into 10 mL and 90 mL aliquots and separately counted the number of shrimps in each aliquot. Here, we confirmed that the variance between the numbers calculated by counting the shrimps in 10 mL aliquot and the total volume of 100 mL falls within the range of the variance of total applied shrimp. Thus, our present counting method can be considered reasonable.

      - Brine Shrimp Aliquot Measurement: You mentioned counting the number of brine shrimp in the 200 μL solution three times before and after the experiments. What does this mean? Did you use this procedure to calculate the mean number of brine shrimp in each 200 μL aliquot?

      Thank you for your comment. As the reviewer commented, to calculate the mean number of brine shrimp in each 200 µL aliquot, we counted the number of brine shrimp in the 200 µL solution three times before and after the experiments.

      - How did you normalize the food intake data? This procedure is not detailed in the methods section.

      Thank you very much for pointing it out. We normalized food intake by subtracting the amount of shrimp by the average of those in LD or WT fish. This explanation was added in the Method section (L. 439).

      (2) Sample Size. Various tests were conducted with a low number of medaka (e.g., 2 brains for RNA-seq, 8 females for ovariectomy). Are these sample sizes sufficient to draw reliable conclusions?

      In Figure 1-Supplementary figure 2, RNAseq was performed to search for the candidate neuropeptides, and that’s why the sample size was the minimum; we pooled two brains as one sample and used three samples per group. On the other hand, each group in the other experiments consist of n ≥ 5 samples, which is usually accepted to be adequate sample size in various studies (cf. Kanda et al., Gen Comp Endocrinol., 2011, Spicer et al., Biol Reprod., 2017).

      (3) Statistical Analysis.

      - The authors used both parametric and non-parametric tests but did not specify how they assessed the normal distribution of the data. For example, if I understood correctly, a t-test was used to compare a small dataset (n=3). In such cases, a U-test would be more appropriate.

      Thank you for your comment. As for Figure 1 -Supplementary Figure 2C, we showed the graphs just to show you candidates. To avoid misunderstanding, we deleted statistical statements in that panel.

      - It is unclear why the Steel-Dwass test was used instead of the Kruskal-Wallis test for comparing agrp1 and npyb expressions in control, OVX, and E2-administered medaka.

      While the authors mentioned using non-parametric tests, they did not specify in which contexts or conditions they were applied.

      Thank you very much for your comment. Kruskal-Wallis test statistically shows whether or not there are differences among any of three groups. To perform multiple comparisons among the three groups, we used Steel-Dwass test.

      - The results section lacks details on the statistical tests used, including the specific test (e.g., Z, U, or W values) and degrees of freedom.

      Thank you for your comment. As the reviewer pointed out, we added such statements in all the figure legends containing statistics.

      (4) Previous studies have shown that photoperiod treatments alter the production of various hormones in medaka (e.g., Lucon-Xiccato et al., 2022; Shimmura et al., 2017), some of which, like growth hormone (GH), have been shown to influence feeding behavior (Canosa et al., 2007).

      In your RNA-seq analysis, did you observe any changes in the expression of genes involved in other hormone synthesis pathways, such as pituitary hormones (GH and TSH), leptin, or ghrelin (e.g., see Volkoff, 2016; Blanco, 2020; Bertolucci et al., 2019)?

      Including such evidence in the discussion would provide a broader perspective on the hormonal regulation of food intake in medaka.

      We appreciate your constructive comments. Unfortunately, since we performed RNA-seq using the whole brain after removal of the pituitary, we could not check such changes in the expression of pituitary hormone-related genes. As additional information about the feeding-related hormones, leptin did not show significant difference in our RNA-seq analysis, and we could not analyze ghrelin because ghrelin has not been annotated in medaka (NCBI and ensembl).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      There are some parts of the study that need to be developed further in order to provide a more comprehensive analysis.

      (1) In the juvenile as well as ovariectomized female fish, the authors should confirm experimentally whether day length influences feeding activity.

      Thank you very much for your suggestion. We analyzed feeding behavior of juvenile (Figure 4-Supplementary Figure 1) and OVX female (Figure 5-Supplementary Figure 1). As shown in these figures, food intake in juvenile and OVX were not significantly different between LD and SD.

      (2) More discussion as to the relevance of increasing feeding activity to support reproductive functions such as sustained egg production would be valuable. One assumes the metabolic costs of producing eggs on a daily basis in this species would inevitably require increased food intake. Is this a reasonable prediction?

      We deeply appreciate your suggestion. We strongly agree with this argument, and we added such discussion in “Discussion” section (L. 406-408).

      Editor's note:

      Should you choose to revise your manuscript, if you have not already done so, please include full statistical reporting including exact p-values wherever possible alongside the summary statistics (test statistic and df) and 95% confidence intervals. These should be reported for all key questions and not only when the p-value is less than 0.05 in the main manuscript.

      We appreciate the editor’s suggestion. We added P-value in the main manuscript, where statistical analyses were performed. In addition, we described test statics in the figure legends. We did not use df values for the statistics used in the present analyses, and therefore did not describe it in the main text.

    1. Author response:

      We will revise the statements of novelty in the introduction by more clearly emphasizing how our model addresses gaps in the existing literature. In addition, we will clarify the description of the dispersal process. Briefly, we use the same dispersal gene β to represent the likelihood an individual will either leave or join a group, thereby quantifying both dispersal and immigration using the same parameter. Specifically, individuals with higher β are more likely to remain as floaters (i.e., disperse from their natal group to become a breeder elsewhere), whereas those with lower β are either more likely to remain in their natal group as subordinates (i.e., queue in a group for the breeding position) or join another group if they dispersed. Immigrants that join a group as a subordinate help and queue for a breeding position, as does any natal subordinate born into the group. To follow the suggestion of the referee and more fully explore the impact of competition between subordinates born in the group and subordinate immigrants, we will explore extending our model to allow dispersers to leave their natal group and join another as subordinates, by incorporating a reaction norm based on their age or rank (D = 1 / (1 + exp (β<sub>t</sub> * t – β<sub>0</sub>)) . This approach will allow individuals to adjust also their dispersal strategy to their competitiveness and to avoid kin competition by remaining as a subordinate in another group.

      We apologize that there was some confusion with terminology. We use the term “disperser” to describe individuals that disperse from their natal group. Dispersers can assume one of three roles: (1) they can migrate to another group as "subordinates"; (2) they can join another group as "breeders" if they successfully outcompete other candidates; or (3) they can remain as "floaters" if they fail to join a group. "Floaters" are individuals who persist in a transient state without access to a breeding territory, waiting for opportunities to join a group in an established territory. Therefore, dispersers do not work when they are floaters, but they may later help if they immigrate to a group as a subordinate. Consequently, immigrant subordinates have no inherent competitive advantage over natal subordinates (as step 2.2. “Join a group” is followed by step 3. “Help”, which occurs before step 5. “Become a breeder”). Nevertheless, floaters can potentially outcompete subordinates of the same age if they attempt to breed without first queuing as a subordinate (step 5) when subordinates are engaged in work tasks. We believe that this assumption is realistic and constitutes part of the costs associated with work tasks. However, floaters are at a disadvantage for becoming a breeder because: (1) floaters incur higher mortality than individuals within groups (eq. 3); and (2) floaters may only attempt to become breeders in some breeding cycles (versus subordinate groups members, who are automatically candidates for an open breeding position in the group in each cycle). Therefore, due to their higher mortality, floaters are rarely older than individuals within groups, which heavily influences dominance value and competitiveness. Additionally, any competitive advantage that floaters might have over other subordinate group members is unlikely to drive the kin selection-only results because subordinates would preferably choose defense tasks instead of work tasks so as not to be at a competitive disadvantage compared to floaters.

      We note that reviewers also mention that floaters often aren't usually high resource holding potential (RHP) individuals and, therefore, our assumptions might be unrealistic. As we explain above, floaters are not inherently at a competitive advantage in our model. In any case, empirical work in a number of species has shown that dispersers are not necessarily those of lower RHP or of lower quality. In fact, according to the ecological constraints hypothesis, one might predict that high quality individuals are the ones that disperse because only individuals in good condition (e.g., larger body size, better energy reserves) can afford the costs associated with dispersal (Cote et al., 2022). By adding a reaction norm approach to explore the role of age or rank in the revised version, we can also determine whether higher or lower quality individuals are the ones dispersing. We will address the issues of terminology and clarity of the relative competitive advantage of floaters versus subordinates, and also include more information in the Supplementary Tables (e.g., the number of floaters). As a side note, the “scramble context” we mention was an additional implementation that we decided to remove from the final manuscript, but we forgot to remove from Table 1 before submission.

      The reviewers also raised a question about asexual reproduction and relatedness more generally. As we showed in the Supplementary Tables and the section on relatedness in the SI (“Kin selection and the evolution of division of labor"), high relatedness does not appear to explain our results. In evolutionary biology generally and in game theory specifically (with the exception of models on sexual selection or sex-specific traits), asexual reproduction is often modelled because it reduces unnecessary complexity. To further study the effect of relatedness on kin structures more closely resembling those of vertebrates, however, we will create an additional “relatedness structure level”, where we will shuffle half of the philopatric offspring using the same method used to remove relatedness completely. This approach will effectively reduce relatedness structure by half and overcome the concerns with our decision to model asexual reproduction.

      Briefly, we will elaborate on the concept of division of labor and the tasks that cooperative breeders perform. In nature, multiple tasks are often necessary to successfully rear offspring. For example, in many cooperatively breeding birds, the primary reasons that individuals fail to produce offspring are (1) starvation, which is mitigated by the feeding of offspring, and (2) nest depredation, which is countered by defensive behavior. Consequently, both types of tasks are necessary to successfully produce offspring, and focusing solely on one while neglecting the other is likely to result in lower reproductive success than if both tasks are performed by individuals within the group. We simplify this principle in the model by maximizing reproductive output when both tasks are carried out to a similar extent, allowing for some flexibility from the mean. In response to the reviewer suggestion about making fecundity a function of work tasks and offspring survival as a function of defensive tasks, these are actually equivalent in model terms, as it’s the same whether breeders produce three offspring and two die, or if they only produce one. This represents, of course, a simplification of the natural context, where breeding unsuccessfully is more costly (in terms of time and energy investment) than not breeding at all, but this is approach is typically used in models of this sort.

      The scope of this paper was to study division of labor in cooperatively breeding species with fertile workers, in which help is exclusively directed towards breeders to enhance offspring production (i.e., alloparental care). Our focus is in line with previous work in most other social animals, including eusocial insects and humans, which emphasizes how division of labor maximizes group productivity. Other forms of “general” help are not considered in the paper, and such forms of help are rarely considered in cooperatively breeding vertebrates or in the division of labor literature, as they do not result in task partitioning to enhance productivity.

      How do we model help? Help provided is an interaction between H (total effort) and T (proportion of total effort invested in each type of task). We will make this definition clearer in the revised manuscript. Thank you for pointing out an error in Eq. 1. This inequality was indeed written incorrectly in the paper (but is correct in the model code); it is dominance rank instead of age (see code in Individual.cpp lines 99-119). We will correct this mistake in the revision.

      There was also a question about bounded and unbounded helping costs. The difference in costs is inherent to the nature of the different task (work or defense): while survival is naturally bounded, with death as the lower bound, dominance costs are potentially unbounded, as they are influenced by dynamic social contexts and potential competitors. Therefore, we believe that the model’s cost structure is not too different to that in nature.

      Thank you for your comments about the parameter landscape. It is important to point out that variations in the mutation rate do not qualitatively affect our results, as this is something we explored in previous versions of the model (not shown). Briefly, we find that variations in the mutation rates only alter the time required to reach equilibrium. Increasing the step size of mutation diminishes the strength of selection by adding stochasticity and reducing the genetic correlation between offspring and their parents. Population size could, in theory, affect our results, as small populations are more prone to extinction. Since this was not something we planned to explore in the paper directly, we specifically chose a large population size, or better said, a large number of territories (i.e. 5000) that can potentially host a large population.

      During the exploratory phase of the model development, various parameters and values were also assessed. However, the manuscript only details the ranges of values and parameters where changes in the behaviors of interest were observed, enhancing clarity and conciseness. For instance, variation in y<sub>h</sub> (the cost of help on dominance when performing “work tasks”) led to behavioral changes similar to those caused by changes in x<sub>h</sub> (the cost of help in survival when performing “defensive tasks”), as both are proportional to each other. Specifically, since an increase in defense costs raises the proportion of work relative to defense tasks, while an increase in the costs of work task has the opposite effect, only results for the variation of x<sub>h</sub> were included in the manuscript to avoid redundancy. We will make this clearer in the revision.

      Finally, following the advice from the reviewers, we will add the symbols of the variables to the figure axes, and clarify whether the values shown represent a genetic or phenotypic trait. In Figure 2, the x-axis is H and the y-axis is T. In Figure 3A, the subindex t in x-axis is incorrect; it should be subindex R (reaction norm to dominance rank instead of age), the y-axis is T. In Figure 3B, the x-axis is R, and the y-axis is T. All values of T, H and R are phenotypic expressed values (see Table 1). For instance, T values are the phenotypic expressed values from the individuals in the population according to their genetic gamma values and their current dominance rank at a given time point.

      References

      Cote, J., Dahirel, M., Schtickzelle, N., Altermatt, F., Ansart, A., Blanchet, S., Chaine, A. S., De Laender, F., De Raedt, J., & Haegeman, B. (2022). Dispersal syndromes in challenging environments: A cross‐species experiment. Ecology Letters, 25(12), 2675–2687.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript presents a practical modification of the orthogonal hybridization chain reaction (HCR) technique, a promising yet underutilized method with broad potential for future applications across various fields. The authors advance this technique by integrating peptide ligation technology and nanobody-based antibody mimetics - cost-effective and scalable alternatives to conventional antibodies - into a DNA-immunoassay framework that merges oligonucleotide-based detection with immunoassay methodologies. Notably, they demonstrate that this approach facilitates a modified ELISA platform capable of simultaneously quantifying multiple target protein expression levels within a single protein mixture sample.

      Strengths:

      The hybridization chain reaction (HCR) technique was initially developed to enable the simultaneous detection of multiple mRNA expression levels within the same tissue. This method has since evolved into immuno-HCR, which extends its application to protein detection by utilizing antibodies. A key requirement of immuno-HCR is the coupling of oligonucleotides to antibodies, a process that can be challenging due to the inherent difficulties in expressing and purifying conventional antibodies.

      In this study, the authors present an innovative approach that circumvents these limitations by employing nanobody-based antibody mimetics, which recognize antibodies, instead of directly coupling oligonucleotides to conventional antibodies. This strategy facilitates oligonucleotide conjugation - designed to target the initiator hairpin oligonucleotide of HCR -through peptide ligation and click chemistry.

      Weaknesses:

      The sandwich-format technique presented in this study, which employs a nanobody that recognizes primary IgG antibodies, may have limited scalability compared to existing methods that directly couple oligonucleotides to primary antibodies. This limitation arises because the C-region types of primary antibodies are relatively restricted, meaning that the use of nanobody-based detection may constrain the number of target proteins that can be analyzed simultaneously. In contrast, the conventional approach of directly conjugating oligonucleotides to primary antibodies allows for a broader range of protein targets to be analyzed in parallel.

      We would like to clarify that MaMBA was specifically designed to address and overcome the limitations imposed by relying on primary antibodies’ Fc types for multiplexing. MaMBA utilizes DNA oligo-conjugated nanobodies that selectively and monovalently bind to the Fc region of IgG. This key feature allows us to barcode primary IgGs targeting different antigens independently. These barcoded IgGs can then be pooled together after barcoding, effectively minimizing the potential for cross-reactivity or crossover. Therefore, IgGs barcoded using MaMBA are functionally equivalent to those barcoded via conventional direct conjugation approaches with respect to multiplexing capability.

      Additionally, in the context of HCR-based protein detection, the number of proteins that can be analyzed simultaneously is inherently constrained by fluorescence wavelength overlap in microscopy, which limits its multiplexing capability. By comparison, direct coupling of oligonucleotides to primary antibodies can facilitate the simultaneous measurement of a significantly greater number of protein targets than the sandwich-based nanobody approach in the barcode-ELISA/NGS-based technique.

      As we have responded above, MaMBA barcoding of primary IgGs that target various antigens can be conducted separately. Once barcoded, these IgGs can then be combined into a single pool. Therefore, for BLISA (i.e., the barcode-ELISA/NGS-based technique), IgGs barcoded through MaMBA offer the same multiplexing capability as those barcoded using traditional direct conjugation methods.

      In in situ protein imaging, spectral overlap can indeed limit the throughput of multiplexed HCR fluorescent imaging. There are two strategies to address this challenge. As demonstrated in this work with misHCR and misHCRn, removing the HCR amplifiers allows for multiplexed detection using a limited number of fluorescence wavelengths. This is achieved through sequential rounds of HCR amplification and imaging. Alternatively, recent computational approaches offer promising solutions for “one-shot” multiplexed imaging. These include combinatorial multiplexing (PMID: 40133518) and spectral unmixing (PMID: 35513404), which can be applied to misHCR to deconvolute overlapping spectra and increase multiplexing capacity in a single imaging acquisition.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study puts forth the model that under IFN-B stimulation, liquid-phase WTAP coordinates with the transcription factor STAT1 to recruit MTC to the promoter region of interferon-stimulated genes (ISGs), mediating the installation of m<sup>6</sup>A on newly synthesized ISG mRNAs. This model is supported by strong evidence that the phosphorylation state of WTAP, regulated by PPP4, is regulated by IFN-B stimulation, and that this results in interactions between WTAP, the m<sup>6</sup>A methyltransferase complex, and STAT1, a transcription factor that mediates activation of ISGs. This was demonstrated via a combination of microscopy, immunoprecipitations, m<sup>6</sup>A sequencing, and ChIP. These experiments converge on a set of experiments that nicely demonstrate that IFN-B stimulation increases the interaction between WTAP, METTL3, and STAT1, that this interaction is lost with the knockdown of WTAP (even in the presence of IFN-B), and that this IFN-B stimulation also induces METTL3-ISG interactions.

      Strengths:

      The evidence for the IFN-B stimulated interaction between METTL3 and STAT1, mediated by WTAP, is quite strong. Removal of WTAP in this system seems to be sufficient to reduce these interactions and the concomitant m<sup>6</sup>A methylation of ISGs. The conclusion that the phosphorylation state of WTAP is important in this process is also quite well supported.

      Weaknesses:

      The evidence that the above mechanism is fundamentally driven by different phase-separated pools of WTAP (regulated by its phosphorylation state) is weaker. These experiments rely relatively heavily on the treatment of cells with 1,6-hexanediol, which has been shown to have some off-target effects on phosphatases and kinases (PMID 33814344).

      Given that the model invoked in this study depends on the phosphorylation (or lack thereof) of WTAP, this is a particularly relevant concern.

      We are grateful for the reviewer’s positive comment and constructive feedback. 1,6-hexanediol (hex) was considered an inhibitor of hydrophobic interaction, thereby capable of dissolving protein phase separation condensates. Hex (5%-10% w/v) was still widely used to explore the phase separation characteristic and function on various protein, including some phosphorylated proteins such as pHSF1, or kinases such as NEMO1-3. Since hydrophobic interactions involved in various types of protein-protein interaction, the off-target effects of hex were inevitable. It has been reported that hex impaired RNA polymerase II CTD-specific phosphatase and kinase activity at 5% concentration4, which led to the reviewer’s concern. In response to this concern, we investigated the phosphorylation level of WTAP under hex concentration gradient treatment. Surprisingly, we found that both 2% and 5% hex maintained the PPP4c-mediated dephosphorylation level of WTAP but still led to the dissolution of WTAP LLPS condensates (Figure 5-figure supplement 1H; Author response image 1), indicating that hex dispersed WTAP phase separation in a phosphorylation-independent manner. Consistent with our findings, Ge et al. used 10% hex to dissolve WTAP phase separation condensates5. Additionally, we found the phosphorylation level of STAT1 was not affected by both 2% and 5% hex, revealing the off-target and impairment function of hex on kinases or phosphatases might not be universal (Figure 5-figure supplement 1H). Collectively, since the 5% hex we used did not influence the de-phosphorylation event of WTAP, function of WTAP LLPS mediating interaction between methylation complex and STAT1 revealed by our results was independent from its phosphorylation status.

      Author response image 1.

      mCherry-WTAP-rescued HeLa cells were treated with 10 ng/mL IFN-β together with or without 2% or 5% hex and 20 μg/mL digitonin for 1 hour or left untreated. Phase separation of mCherry-WTAP was observed through confocal microscopy. The number of WTAP condensates that diameter over 0.4 μm of n = 20 cells were counted through ImageJ and shown. Scale bars indicated 10 μm. All error bars, mean values ± SD, P-values were determined by unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test of n = 20 cells in (B). For (A), similar results were obtained for three independent biological experiments.

      Related to this point, it is also interesting (and potentially concerning for the proposed model) that the initial region of WTAP that was predicted to be disordered is in fact not the region that the authors demonstrate is important for the different phase-separated states.

      A considerable number of proteins undergo phase separation via interactions between intrinsically disordered regions (IDRs). IDR contains more charged and polar amino acids to present multiple weakly interacting elements, while lacking hydrophobic amino acids to show flexible conformations6. In our study, we used PLAAC websites (http://plaac.wi.mit.edu/) to predict IDR domain of WTAP, and a fragment (234-249 amino acids) was predicted as prion-like domain (PLD). However, deletion of this fragment failed to abolish the phase separation properties of WTAP, which might be the main confusion to reviewers. To explain this issue, we checked the WTAP structure (within part of MTC complex) from protein data bank (https://www.rcsb.org/structure/7VF2) and found that the prediction of IDR has been renewed due to the update of different algorithm. IDR of WTAP expanded to 245-396 amino acids, encompassing the entire CTD region. Our results demonstrate that the CTD was critical for WTAP LLPS, as CTD deficiency significantly inhibited the formation of liquid condensates both in vitro and in cells. Also, phosphorylation sites on CTD were important for the phase transition of WTAP. These observations highlight the phosphorylation status on CTD region as a key driving force of phase separation, consistent with the established role of IDR in regulating LLPS. We have revised our description on WTAP IDR in our article following the reviewers’ suggestion.

      Taking all the data together, it is also not clear to me that one has to invoke phase separation in the proposed mechanism.

      In this article, we observed that WTAP underwent phase transition during virus infection and IFN-β treatment, and proposed a novel mechanism whereby post translational modification (PTM)-driven WTAP LLPS was required for the m<sup>6</sup>A modification of ISG mRNAs. To verify the hypothesis, we first demonstrated the relationship between PTM and phase separation of WTAP. We constructed WTAP 5ST-D and 5ST-A mutant to mimic WTAP phosphorylation and dephosphorylation status respectively, and figured out that dephosphorylated WTAP underwent LLPS. We also found that PPP4 was the main phosphatase to regulate WTAP dephosphorylation. To further investigation, we introduced the potent PPP4 inhibitor, fostriecin. Consistent with our findings in PPP4 deficient model, fostriecin treatment significantly inhibited the IFN-β-induced dephosphorylation of WTAP and disrupted its LLPS condensates, indicating that PPP4 was the key phosphatase recruited by IFN-β to regulate WTAP, confirming that PTM governs WTAP LLPS dynamics (Figure 2-figure supplement 1C and H). Furthermore, fostriecin-induced impairment of WTAP phosphorylation and phase separation correlated with reduced m<sup>6</sup>A modification level and elevated ISGs expression level (Figure 4C and Figure 4-figure supplement 1E). These findings together emphasized that dephosphorylation is required for WTAP LLPS.

      As for the function of WTAP LLPS, we assumed that WTAP might undergo LLPS to sequester STAT1 together with m<sup>6</sup>A methyltransferase complex (MTC) for mediating m<sup>6</sup>A deposition on ISG mRNAs co-transcriptionally under IFN-β stimulation. Given that hex dissolved WTAP LLPS condensates without affecting dephosphorylation status (Figure 5-figure supplement 1H; Author response image 1), various experiments we performed previously actually confirmed the critical role of WTAP LLPS during m<sup>6</sup>A modification (Figure 4A), as well as the mechanism that WTAP phase separation enhanced the interaction between MTC and STAT1 (Figure 5E-F). Subsequent to reviewer’s comments, more experiments were conducted for further validation. We found the WTAP liquid condensates formed by wild type (WT) WTAP or WTAP 5ST-A mutant (which mimics dephosphorylated-WTAP) could be dissembled by hex, which also led to the impairment of the interaction between STAT1, METTL3 and WTAP (Figure 5-figure supplement 1E). In addition, in vitro experiments demonstrated that hex treatment significantly disrupted the interaction between recombinant GFP-STAT1 and mCherry-WTAP which expressed in the E. coli system (Figure 5F and Figure 5-figure supplement 1G). Notably, this prokaryotic expression system lacks endogenous phosphorylation machinery, resulting in non-phosphorylated mCherry-WTAP. For further validation, we performed the interaction of WTAP WT or 5ST-A with the promoter regions of ISG as well as the m<sup>6</sup>A modification of ISG mRNAs were attenuated by WTAP LLPS dissolution (Figure 4E and Figure 6E). These findings together revealed that WTAP LLPS were the critical mediators of the STAT1-MTC interactions, ISG promoter regions binding and the co-transcription m<sup>6</sup>A modification.

      Collectively, our results demonstrated that IFN-β treatment recruited PPP4c to dephosphorylate WTAP, thereby driving the formation of WTAP liquid condensates which were essential for facilitating STAT1-MTC interaction and m<sup>6</sup>A deposition, subsequently mediated ISG expression. Since we revealed a strong association between PTM-regulated WTAP phase transition and its m<sup>6</sup>A modification function, WTAP LLPS was a novel and functionally distinct mechanism that must be reckoned with in this study.

      Author response image 2.

      Wild type (WT) WTAP or 5ST-A mutant-rescued WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> THP-1-derived macrophages are stimulated with or without with 10 ng/mL IFN-β together followed with 2% or 5% 1,6-hexanediol (hex) and 20 μg/mL digitonin for 1 hour or left untreated. antibody and imaged using confocal microscope. Scale bars indicated 10 μm.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this study, Cai and colleagues investigate how one component of the m<sup>6</sup>A methyltransferase complex, the WTAP protein, responds to IFNb stimulation. They find that viral infection or IFNb stimulation induces the transition of WTAP from aggregates to liquid droplets through dephosphorylation by PPP4. This process affects the m<sup>6</sup>A modification levels of ISG mRNAs and modulates their stability. In addition, the WTAP droplets interact with the transcription factor STAT1 to recruit the methyltransferase complex to ISG promoters and enhance m<sup>6</sup>A modification during transcription. The investigation dives into a previously unexplored area of how viral infection or IFNb stimulation affects m<sup>6</sup>A modification on ISGs. The observation that WTAP undergoes a phase transition is significant in our understanding of the mechanisms underlying m<sup>6</sup>A's function in immunity. However, there are still key gaps that should be addressed to fully accept the model presented.

      Major points:

      (1) More detailed analyses on the effects of WTAP sgRNA on the m<sup>6</sup>A modification of ISGs:

      a. A comprehensive summary of the ISGs, including the percentage of ISGs that are m<sup>6</sup>A-modified. merip-isg percentage

      b. The distribution of m<sup>6</sup>A modification across the ISGs. Topology

      c. A comparison of the m<sup>6</sup>A modification distribution in ISGs with non-ISGs. Topology

      In addition, since the authors propose a novel mechanism where the interaction between phosphorylated STAT1 and WTAP directs the MTC to the promoter regions of ISGs to facilitate co-transcriptional m<sup>6</sup>A modification, it is critical to analyze whether the m<sup>6</sup>A modification distribution holds true in the data.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s summary of our manuscript and the constructive assessment. We have conducted the related analysis accordingly to present the m<sup>6</sup>A modification in ISGs in our model as reviewers suggested. Our results showed that about 64.29% of core ISGs were m<sup>6</sup>A modified under IFN-β stimulation (Figure 3-figure supplement 1B; Figure 3G), which was consistent with the similar percentage in previous studies[7,8]. The m<sup>6</sup>A distribution of the ISGs transcripts including IFIT1, IFIT2, OAS1 and OAS2 was validated (Figure 3-figure supplement 1H).

      Generally, m<sup>6</sup>A deposition preferentially located in the vicinity of stop codon, while m<sup>6</sup>A modification was highly dynamic under different cellular condition. However, we compared the topology of m<sup>6</sup>A deposition of ISGs with non-ISGs, and found that m<sup>6</sup>A modification of ISG mRNAs showed higher preference of coding sequences (CDS) localization compared to global m<sup>6</sup>A deposition (Figure 3H). Similarly, various researches uncovered the m<sup>6</sup>A deposition regulated by co-transcriptionally m<sup>6</sup>A modification preferred to locate in the CDS [9-11]. Since our results of m<sup>6</sup>A modification distribution of ISGs was consistent with the co-transcriptional m<sup>6</sup>A modification characteristics, we believed that our hypothesis and results were correlated and consistent.

      (2) Since a key part of the model includes the cytosol-localized STAT1 protein undergoing phosphorylation to translocate to the nucleus to mediate gene expression, the authors should focus on the interaction between phosphorylated STAT1 and WTAP in Figure 4, rather than the unphosphorylated STAT1. Only phosphorylated STAT1 localizes to the nucleus, so the presence of pSTAT1 in the immunoprecipitate is critical for establishing a functional link between STAT1 activation and its interaction with WTAP.

      Thank you for the constructive comments. As we showed in Figure 4, we found the enhanced interaction between phase-separated WTAP and the nuclear-translocated STAT1 which were phosphorylated under IFN-β stimulation, indicating the importance of phosphorylation of STAT1. We repeated the immunoprecipitation experiments to clarify the function of pSTAT1 in WTAP interaction. Our results showed that IFN-β stimulation induced the interaction of WTAP with both pSTAT1 and STAT1 (Figure 5-figure supplement 1C), indicating that most of the WTAP-associated STAT1 was phosphorylated. Taken together, our data proved that LLPS WTAP bound with nuclear-translocated pSTAT1 under IFN-β stimulation.

      (3) The authors should include pSTAT1 ChIP-seq and WTAP ChIP-seq on IFNb-treated samples in Figure 5 to allow for a comprehensive and unbiased genomic analysis for comparing the overlaps of peaks from both ChIP-seq datasets. These results should further support their hypothesis that WTAP interacts with pSTAT1 to enhance m<sup>6</sup>A modifications on ISGs.

      Thank you for raising this thoughtful comment. In this study, MeRIP-seq and RNA-seq along with immunoprecipitation and immunofluorescence experiments supported that phase transition of WTAP enhanced its interaction to pSTAT1, thus mediating ISGs m<sup>6</sup>A modification and expression by enhancing its interaction with nuclear-translocated pSTAT1 during virus infection and IFN-β stimulation. However, how WTAP-mediated m<sup>6</sup>A modification was related to STAT1-mediated transcription remained unclear. Several researches have revealed the recruitment of m<sup>6</sup>A methyltransferase complex (MTC) to transcription start sites (TSS) of coding genes and R-loop structure by interacting with transcriptional factors STAT5B, SMAD2/3, DNA helicase DDX21, indicating the engagement of MTC mediated m<sup>6</sup>A modification on nascent transcripts at the very beginning of transcription [11-13]. These researches inspired us that phase-separated WTAP could be recruited to the ISG promoter regions via interacting with nuclear-translocated pSTAT1. To validate this mechanism, we have conducted ChIP-qPCR experiments targeting STAT1 and WTAP, revealed that IFN-β induced the comparable recruitment dynamics of both STAT1 and WTAP to ISG promoter regions (Figure 6A-B). Notably, STAT1 deficiency significantly abolished the bindings between WTAP and ISG promoter regions (Figure 6C). These findings established nuclear-translocated STAT1-dependent recruitment of phase separated WTAP and ISG promoter region, substantiated our hypothesis within the current study. We totally agree that ChIP-seq data will be more supportive to explore the mechanism in depth. We will continuously focus on the whole genome chromatin distribution of WTAP and explore more functional effect of transcriptional factor-dependent WTAP-promoter regions interaction in the future.

      Minor points:

      (1) Since IFNb is primarily known for modulating biological processes through gene transcription, it would be informative if the authors discussed the mechanism of how IFNb would induce the interaction between WTAP and PPP4.

      Protein phosphatase 4 (PPP4) is a multi-subunit serine/threonine phosphatase complex that participates in diverse biologic process, including DDR, cell cycle progression, and apoptosis[14]. Several signaling pathway such as NF-κB and mTOR signaling, can be regulated by PPP4. Previous research showed that deficiency of PPP4 enhanced IFN-β downstream signaling and ISGs expression, which was consistent with our findings that knockdown of PPP4C impaired WTAP-mediated m<sup>6</sup>A modification, enhanced the ISGs expression[15,16]. Since there was no significant enhancement in PPP4 expression level during 0-3 hours of IFN-β stimulation in our results, we explored the PTM that may influence the protein-protein interaction, such as ubiquitination. Intriguingly, we found the ubiquitination level of PPP4 was enhanced after IFN-β stimulation, which may affect the interaction between PPP4 and WTAP (Author response image 3). Further investigation between PPP4 and WTAP will be conducted in our future study.

      Author response image 3.

      HEK 293T transfected with HA-ubiquitin (HA-Ub) and Flag-PPP4 were treated with 10 ng/mL IFN-β or left untreated. Whole cell lysate (WCL) was collected and immunoprecipitation (IP) experiment using anti-Flag antibody was performed, followed with immunoblot. Similar results were obtained for three independent biological experiments.

      (2) The authors should include mCherry alone controls in Figure 1D to demonstrate that mCherry does not contribute to the phase separation of WTAP. Does mCherry have or lack a PLD?

      According to the crystal structure of mCherry protein in protein structure database RCSB-PDB, mCherry protein presents as a β-barrel protein, with no IDRs or multivalent interaction domains including PLD, indicating that mCherry protein has no capability to undergo phase separation. This characteristic makes it a suitable protein to tag and trace the localization or expression levels of proteins, and a negative control for protein phase separation studies. As the reviewer suggested, we include mCherry alone controls in the revised version (Figure 1D).

      (3) The authors should clarify the immunoprecipitation assays in the methods. For example, the labeling in Figure 2A suggests that antibodies against WTAP and pan-p were used for two immunoprecipitations. Is that accurate?

      Due to the lack of phosphorylated-WTAP antibody, the detection of phosphorylation of WTAP was conducted by WTAP-antibody-mediated immunoprecipitation. We conducted immunoprecipitation to pull-down WTAP protein by WTAP antibody, then used antibody against phosphoserine/threonine/tyrosine (pan-p) to detect the phosphorylation level of WTAP. To avoid the misunderstanding, we have modified the description from pan-p to pWTAP (pan-p) in figures and revised the figure legends.

      (4) The authors should include overall m<sup>6</sup>A modification levels quantified of GFP<sup>sgRNA</sup> and WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> cells, either by mass spectrometry (preferably) or dot blot.

      We thank reviewer for raising these useful suggestions. As we showed in Figure 3F and J-K, the m<sup>6</sup>A modification event and degradation of ISG mRNAs were significantly depleted in WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> cell lines, indicating that function of WTAP was deficient. Thus, we used the WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> #2 cell line to perform most of our experiment. Furthermore, we also found 46.4% of global m<sup>6</sup>A modification was decreased in WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> THP-1 cells than that of control cells with or without IFN-β stimulation (Author response image 4), further validating that level of m<sup>6</sup>A modification was significantly affected in WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> cells. Taken together, our data confirmed that m<sup>6</sup>A methyltransferase activity was efficiently inhibited in our WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> cells.

      Author response image 4.

      Control (GFP<sup>sgRNA</sup>) and WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> #2 THP-1-derived macrophages were treated with 10 ng/mL IFN-β for 4 hours. Global m<sup>6</sup>A level was detected and quantified through ELISA assays. All error bars, mean values ± SEM, P-values were determined by two-way ANOVA test independent biological experiments.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study presents a valuable finding on the mechanism used by WTAP to modulate the IFN-β stimulation. It describes the phase transition of WTAP driven by IFN-β-induced dephosphorylation. The evidence supporting the claims of the authors is solid, although major analysis and controls would strengthen the impact of the findings. Additionally, more attention to the figure design and to the text would help the reader to understand the major findings.

      Strength:

      The key finding is the revelation that WTAP undergoes phase separation during virus infection or IFN-β treatment. The authors conducted a series of precise experiments to uncover the mechanism behind WTAP phase separation and identified the regulatory role of 5 phosphorylation sites. They also succeeded in pinpointing the phosphatase involved.

      Weaknesses:

      However, as the authors acknowledge, it is already widely known in the field that IFN and viral infection regulate m<sup>6</sup>A mRNAs and ISGs. Therefore, a more detailed discussion could help the reader interpret the obtained findings in light of previous research.

      We are grateful for the positive comments and the unbiased advice by the reviewer. To interpret our findings in previous research, we have revised the manuscript carefully and added more detailed discussion on ISG mRNAs m<sup>6</sup>A modification during virus infection or IFN stimulation.

      It is well-known that protein concentration drives phase separation events. Similarly, previous studies and some of the figures presented by the authors show an increase in WTAP expression upon IFN treatment. The authors do not discuss the contribution of WTAP expression levels to the phase separation event observed upon IFN treatment. Similarly, METTL3 and METTL14, as well as other proteins of the MTC are upregulated upon IFN treatment. How does the MTC protein concentration contribute to the observed phase separation event?

      We thank reviewer for pointing out the importance of the concentration dependent phase transition. Previously, Ge et al. discovered that expression level of WTAP was up-regulated during LPS stimulation, thereby promoting WTAP phase separation ability and m<sup>6</sup>A modification, indicating that WTAP concentration indeed affects the phase separation event. In our article, we have generated the phase diagram with different concentration of recombinant mCherry-WTAP in vitro (Figure 1-figure supplement 1C). For in cells experiments, we constructed the TRE-mCherry-WTAP HeLa cells in which the expression of mCherry-WTAP was induced by doxycycline (Dox) in a dose-dependent manner (Author response image 5A). We detected the LLPS of mCherry-WTAP at different concentrations by increasing the doses of Dox, and found that WTAP automatically underwent LLPS only in an artificially high expression level (Author response image 5B). However, the cells we used to detect the WTAP phase separation in our article was mCherry-WTAP-rescued HeLa cells, in which mCherry-WTAP was introduced in WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> HeLa cells to stably express mCherry-WTAP. We had adjusted and verified the mCherry-WTAP expression level precisely to make the protein abundance similar to endogenous WTAP in wild type (WT) HeLa cells (Author response image 5C). We also repeated the IFN-β stimulation experiments and found no significant increase of WTAP protein level (Figure 5-figure supplement 1A). These findings indicated the phase separation of WTAP in our article was not artificially induced due to the extremely high protein expression level.

      MTC protein expression level was crucial for m<sup>6</sup>A modification during virus infection event. Rubio et al. and Winkler et al. revealed that WTAP, METTL3 and METTL14 were up-regulated after 24 hours of HCMV infection[8,17]. Recently, Ge et al. proposed that WTAP protein was degraded after 12 hours of VSV and HSV stimulation5,18. However, these studies only focused on the virus infection event, how the MTC protein expression change after direct IFN-β stimulation was still unclear. Our study investigated the transition change of WTAP under IFNβ stimulation in a short time, we detected the expression level of MTC proteins within 6 hours of IFN-β treatment, and found no significant enhancement of WTAP, METTL3 or METTL14 protein level and mRNA level (Figure 5-figure supplement 1B and Figure 5-figure supplement 1A;). Our in vitro experiments showed that introducing CFP-METTL3 protein have no significant influence on WTAP phase separation (Figure 4H). Additionally, we performed in cells experiments and found that increased expression of METTL3 had no effect on WTAP phase separation event (Author response image 5D). Taken together, WTAP phase separation can be promoted by dramatically increased concentration of WTAP both in vitro and in cells, but the phase separation of WTAP under IFN-β stimulation in our study was not related with the expression level of MTC proteins.

      Author response image 5.

      (A) Immunoblot analysis of the expression of mCherry-WTAP in TRE-mCherry-WTAP HeLa cells treated with different doses of doxycycline (Dox). Protein level of mCherry-WTAP was quantified and normalized to β-actin of n=3 independent biological experiments. Results were obtained for three independent biological experiments. (B) Phase separation diagram of mCherry-WTAP in TRE-mCherry-WTAP HeLa cells treated with different doses of Dox were observed through confocal microscopy. Representative images for three independent biological experiments were shown in b while number of WTAP condensates that diameter over 0.4 μm of n=80 cells were counted and shown as medium with interquartile range. Dotted white lines indicated the location of nucleus. Scale bars indicated 10 μm. (C) Immunoblot analysis of the expression of endogenous WTAP in wildtype (WT) HeLa cells and mCherry-WTAP-rescued WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> HeLa cells. (D) mCherry-WTAP-rescued HeLa cells were transfected with 0, 200 or 400 ng of Flag-METTL3, followed with 10 ng/mL IFN-β for 1 hour or left untreated (UT). Phase separation of mCherry-WTAP was observed through confocal microscopy. The number of WTAP condensates that diameter over 0.4 μm of n = 20 cells were counted through ImageJ and shown. Representative images of n=20 cells were shown. All error bars, mean values ± SD were determined by unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test of n = 3 independent biological experiments in (A). For (A, C), similar results were obtained for three independent biological experiments.

      How is PP4 related to the IFN signaling cascade?

      Both reviewer #2 and reviewer #3 raised a similar point on the relationship between PPP4 and IFN signaling. In short, protein phosphatase 4 (PPP4) participates in diverse biologic process, including DDR, cell cycle progression and apoptosis14 and several signaling pathway. Previous research showed that deficiency of PPP4 enhanced IFN-β downstream signaling and ISGs expression, which was consistent with our findings that knockdown of PPP4C impaired WTAP-mediated m<sup>6</sup>A modification, and enhanced the ISGs expression[15,16]. Since there was no significant enhancement in PPP4C expression level during 0-3 hours of IFN-β stimulation in our results, we tried to explore the post-translation modification which may influence the protein-protein interaction, such as ubiquitination. Intriguingly, we found the ubiquitination level of PPP4 was enhanced after IFN-β stimulation, which may affect the interaction between PPP4 and WTAP (Author response image 4). Investigation between PPP4 and WTAP will be conducted in our further study (also see minor points 1 of reviewer#2).

      In general, it is very confusing to talk about WTAP KO as WTAPgRNA.

      As we describe above, all WTAP-deficient THP-1 cells were generated using the CRISPR-Cas9 system with WTAP-specific sgRNA, and used bulk cells instead of the monoclonal knockout cell for further experiments. Since monoclonal knockout cells were not obtained, we refer it as WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> THP-1 cells rather than WTAP-KO THP-1 cells. We confirmed that WTAP expression was efficiently knocked down in WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> THP-1 cells, and the m<sup>6</sup>A modification level was significantly impaired (Figure 3I, Figure 3-figure supplement 1A and Author response image 4), which was suitable for mechanism investigation.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Related to the points raised in 'weaknesses' above, if the authors want to claim that this mechanism is reliant on WTAP phase-separated states, additional controls should be done to demonstrate this. Based on the available data it seems that it is just as likely that the phosphorylation state of WTAP is mediating interactions with other factors and/or altering its subcellular localization, which may or may not be related to phase separation.

      We are grateful for the constructive suggestions. As we showed in , Figure 5-figure supplement 1H; Author response image 1 along with the explanation above, 5% hex dispersed the phase separation condensates of WTAP without affecting its phosphorylation status, proving the interaction between STAT1 and methylation complex impaired by hex was depended on WTAP LLPS but not its phosphorylation status (Figure 5E-H). To further confirmed the recruitment of WTAP LLPS on ISG promoter region, we performed the immunoprecipitation and ChIP-qPCR experiments of wild type (WT) WTAP, 5ST-D and 5ST-A rescued THP-1 cells. Our results uncovered the interaction between de-phosphorylated-mimic WTAP mutant and STAT1, and its binding ability with ISG promoter regions were depleted by hex without affecting its phosphorylation status (Author response image 2, Figure 5-figure supplement 1 F, Figure 6E). Taken together, we identified the de-phosphorylation event that regulated phase transition of WTAP during IFN-β stimulation, and proposed that LLPS of WTAP mediated by dephosphorylation was the key mechanism to bind with STAT1 and mediate the m<sup>6</sup>A modification on ISG mRNAs.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The author order is different for the main text and the supplementary file.

      Thank you for pointing out this mistake. We have corrected it in our revised version.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Signaling molecules? Do you mean RNA or protein post-translational modification?

      Thank you for pointing out this problem. In this sentence, we mean the post-translational modification of signaling proteins. We have corrected this mistake in our revised version.

      Line 145: Do you mean Figure 1C?

      We have corrected it in our revised version.

      Line 214: Are the cells KO for WTAP? Do you mean CRISPR KO? In general, it is easier to present WTAPgRNA as WTAPKO.

      Thank you for the constructive suggestion. As we explained above, in this project, all WTAP-deficient THP-1 cells were generated using the CRISPR-Cas9 system with WTAP-specific sgRNA, and used bulk cells instead of the monoclonal knockout cells. We confirmed that WTAP expression was efficiently knocked down in WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> THP-1 cells, and the m<sup>6</sup>A modification level was significantly impaired (Figure 3I, Figure3-figure supplement 1A and Author response image 4). Since monoclonal knockout cells were not obtained, we refer it as WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> THP-1 cells rather than WTAP-KO THP-1 cells.

      Line 221: WTAP KO has no effect on the expression level of transcription factors?

      Thank you for pointing out the uncritical expression. We have corrected this in our revised version.

      Figure 3C: I would suggest removing the tracks and showing the expression levels in TPMs.

      According to the reviewer’s suggestion, we have analyzed the results and showed the ISGs expression levels in fold change of TPMs (Figure 3D).

      Figure 4C: It seems that the IP efficiency from METTL3 is lower in WTAP KO cells. That may impact the author's conclusions.

      We have repeated the immunoprecipitation experiments of METTL3 and confirmed the immunoprecipitation (IP) efficiency from METTL3 had no difference between WTAP<sup>sgRNA</sup> cells and the control cells (Figure 5C).

      I would suggest performing an IP of WTAP with the 5StoA mutation to confirm the missing interaction with WTAP.

      According to the reviewer’s suggestion, we investigated the interaction between STAT1 and WTAP in WT cells and WTAP 5ST-A-rescued THP-1 cells. Our results showed that interaction between STAT1, METTL3 and WTAP were enhanced with WTAP 5ST-A mutation, which was depleted by hex treatment (Figure 5-figure supplement 1E). Thus, the interaction of WTAP WT or 5ST-A with the promoter regions of ISG were attenuated by WTAP LLPS dissolution (Figure 6E). Taken together, the interaction between STAT1 and MTC were relied on LLPS of WTAP.

      In the graphical abstract, it is confusing to represent WTAP as a line. All other proteins are presented as circles. It is easy to confuse WTAP protein with an RNA. Additionally, m<sup>6</sup>A is too big in size. It should be smaller than a protein.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this suggestion. We have modified the graphical abstract to avoid the confusion in our revised version (Figure 6F).

      References

      (1) Wegmann, S., Eftekharzadeh, B., Tepper, K., Zoltowska, K.M., Bennett, R.E., Dujardin, S., Laskowski, P.R., MacKenzie, D., Kamath, T., Commins, C., et al. (2018). Tau protein liquid-liquid phase separation can initiate tau aggregation. The EMBO journal 37. 10.15252/embj.201798049.

      (2) Lu, Y., Wu, T., Gutman, O., Lu, H., Zhou, Q., Henis, Y.I., and Luo, K. (2020). Phase separation of TAZ compartmentalizes the transcription machinery to promote gene expression. Nat Cell Biol 22, 453-464. 10.1038/s41556-020-0485-0.

      (3) Zhang, H., Shao, S., Zeng, Y., Wang, X., Qin, Y., Ren, Q., Xiang, S., Wang, Y., Xiao, J., and Sun, Y. (2022). Reversible phase separation of HSF1 is required for an acute transcriptional response during heat shock. Nat Cell Biol 24, 340-352. 10.1038/s41556-022-00846-7.

      (4) Duster, R., Kaltheuner, I.H., Schmitz, M., and Geyer, M. (2021). 1,6-Hexanediol, commonly used to dissolve liquid-liquid phase separated condensates, directly impairs kinase and phosphatase activities. J Biol Chem 296, 100260. 10.1016/j.jbc.2021.100260.

      (5) Ge, Y., Chen, R., Ling, T., Liu, B., Huang, J., Cheng, Y., Lin, Y., Chen, H., Xie, X., Xia, G., et al. (2024). Elevated WTAP promotes hyperinflammation by increasing m<sup>6</sup>A modification in inflammatory disease models. J Clin Invest 134. 10.1172/JCI177932.

      (6) Hou, S., Hu, J., Yu, Z., Li, D., Liu, C., and Zhang, Y. (2024). Machine learning predictor PSPire screens for phase-separating proteins lacking intrinsically disordered regions. Nat Commun 15, 2147. 10.1038/s41467-024-46445-y.

      (7) McFadden, M.J., McIntyre, A.B.R., Mourelatos, H., Abell, N.S., Gokhale, N.S., Ipas, H., Xhemalce, B., Mason, C.E., and Horner, S.M. (2021). Post-transcriptional regulation of antiviral gene expression by N6-methyladenosine. Cell Rep 34, 108798. 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.108798.

      (8) Winkler, R., Gillis, E., Lasman, L., Safra, M., Geula, S., Soyris, C., Nachshon, A., Tai-Schmiedel, J., Friedman, N., Le-Trilling, V.T.K., et al. (2019). m(6)A modification controls the innate immune response to infection by targeting type I interferons. Nat Immunol 20, 173-182. 10.1038/s41590-018-0275-z.

      (9) Li, Y., Xia, L., Tan, K., Ye, X., Zuo, Z., Li, M., Xiao, R., Wang, Z., Liu, X., Deng, M., et al. (2020). N(6)-Methyladenosine co-transcriptionally directs the demethylation of histone H3K9me2. Nat Genet 52, 870-877. 10.1038/s41588-020-0677-3.

      (10) Huang, H., Weng, H., Zhou, K., Wu, T., Zhao, B.S., Sun, M., Chen, Z., Deng, X., Xiao, G., Auer, F., et al. (2019). Histone H3 trimethylation at lysine 36 guides m(6)A RNA modification co-transcriptionally. Nature 567, 414-419. 10.1038/s41586-019-1016-7.

      (11) Barbieri, I., Tzelepis, K., Pandolfini, L., Shi, J., Millan-Zambrano, G., Robson, S.C., Aspris, D., Migliori, V., Bannister, A.J., Han, N., et al. (2017). Promoter-bound METTL3 maintains myeloid leukaemia by m(6)A-dependent translation control. Nature 552, 126-131. 10.1038/nature24678.

      (12) Hao, J.D., Liu, Q.L., Liu, M.X., Yang, X., Wang, L.M., Su, S.Y., Xiao, W., Zhang, M.Q., Zhang, Y.C., Zhang, L., et al. (2024). DDX21 mediates co-transcriptional RNA m(6)A modification to promote transcription termination and genome stability. Mol Cell 84, 1711-1726 e1711. 10.1016/j.molcel.2024.03.006.

      (13) Bhattarai, P.Y., Kim, G., Lim, S.C., and Choi, H.S. (2024). METTL3-STAT5B interaction facilitates the co-transcriptional m(6)A modification of mRNA to promote breast tumorigenesis. Cancer Lett 603, 217215. 10.1016/j.canlet.2024.217215.

      (14) Dong, M.Z., Ouyang, Y.C., Gao, S.C., Ma, X.S., Hou, Y., Schatten, H., Wang, Z.B., and Sun, Q.Y. (2022). PPP4C facilitates homologous recombination DNA repair by dephosphorylating PLK1 during early embryo development. Development 149. 10.1242/dev.200351.

      (15) Zhan, Z., Cao, H., Xie, X., Yang, L., Zhang, P., Chen, Y., Fan, H., Liu, Z., and Liu, X. (2015). Phosphatase PP4 Negatively Regulates Type I IFN Production and Antiviral Innate Immunity by Dephosphorylating and Deactivating TBK1. J Immunol 195, 3849-3857. 10.4049/jimmunol.1403083.

      (16) Raja, R., Wu, C., Bassoy, E.Y., Rubino, T.E., Jr., Utagawa, E.C., Magtibay, P.M., Butler, K.A., and Curtis, M. (2022). PP4 inhibition sensitizes ovarian cancer to NK cell-mediated cytotoxicity via STAT1 activation and inflammatory signaling. J Immunother Cancer 10. 10.1136/jitc-2022-005026.

      (17) Rubio, R.M., Depledge, D.P., Bianco, C., Thompson, L., and Mohr, I. (2018). RNA m(6) A modification enzymes shape innate responses to DNA by regulating interferon beta. Genes Dev 32, 1472-1484. 10.1101/gad.319475.118.

      (18) Ge, Y., Ling, T., Wang, Y., Jia, X., Xie, X., Chen, R., Chen, S., Yuan, S., and Xu, A. (2021). Degradation of WTAP blocks antiviral responses by reducing the m(6) A levels of IRF3 and IFNAR1 mRNA. EMBO Rep 22, e52101. 10.15252/embr.202052101.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Nuclear depletion and cytoplasmic mislocalization/aggregation of the DNA and RNA binding protein TDP-43 are pathological hallmarks of multiple neurodegenerative diseases. Prior work has demonstrated that depletion of TDP-43 from the nucleus leads to alterations in transcription and splicing. Conversely, cytoplasmic mislocalization/aggregation can contribute to toxicity by impairing mRNA transport and translation as well as miRNA dysregulation. However, to date, models of TDP-43 proteinopathy rely on artificial knockdown- or overexpression-based systems to evaluate either nuclear loss or cytoplasmic gain of function events independently. Few model systems authentically reproduce both nuclear depletion and cytoplasmic miscloalization/aggregation events. In this manuscript, the authors generate novel iPSC-based reagents to manipulate the localization of endogenous TDP-43. This is a valuable resource for the field to study pathological consequences of TDP-43 proteinopathy in a more endogenous and authentic setting. However, in the current manuscript, there are a number of weaknesses that should be addressed to further validate the ability of this model to replicate human disease pathology and demonstrate utility for future studies.

      Strengths:

      The primary strength of this paper is the development of a novel in vitro tool.

      Weaknesses:

      There are a number of weaknesses detailed below that should be addressed to thoroughly validate these new reagents as more authentic models of TDP-43 proteinopathy and demonstrate their utility for future investigations.

      (1) The authors should include images of their engineered TDP-43-GFP iPSC line to demonstrate TDP-43 localization without the addition of any nanobodies (perhaps immediately prior to addition of nanobodies). Additionally, it is unclear whether simply adding a GFP tag to endogenous TDP-43 impact its normal function (nuclear-cytoplasmic shuttling, regulation of transcription and splicing, mRNA transport etc).

      We have included images of the untransduced day 20 MNs derived from the engineered TDP43-GFP iPSC lines and the unedited line (Supplementary Fig. 1B).

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern about the potential impact of the GFP tag on TDP43's normal function. To address this, we have validated the functionality of TDP43 by assessing the inclusion of cryptic exons in highly sensitive targets such as UNC13A and STMN2, both of which are known to be directly regulated by TDP43.

      We compared MNs derived from the unedited parent line with the TDP43-GFP MNs prior to nanobody addition. As measured by qPCR, cryptic exon inclusion in UNC13A and STMN2 was not observed in the unedited or edited TDP43-GFP MNs (Supplementary Fig.1C), confirming that the tagging does not induce splicing defects by itself. The cryptic exon inclusion in UNC13A and STMN2 were only observed in TDP43-GFP MNs expressing the NES nanobody (Supplementary Fig. 2D). These findings were further supported by our next-generation sequencing data, which also showed that cryptic exon inclusion was specific to the TDP43 mislocalization condition (Supplementary Fig.3 and 4).

      Thus, we have strong evidence that the GFP-tagged TDP43 behaves similarly to the wild-type protein and does not interfere with its function in our model.

      (2) Can the authors explain why there is a significant discrepancy in time points selected for nanobody transduction and immunostaining or cell lysis throughout Figure 1 and 2? This makes interpretation and overall assessment of the model challenging.

      For the phenotypic data shown in Fig.1, we added the AAVs at day 18 or 20 and analyzed the cells at day 40. For the phosphorylated TDP43 western blot (revised Fig. 3D), cells were treated with doxycycline at day 20 to induce nanobody expression and samples were harvested at day 40. Thus, cells were harvested between days 20 or 22 after adding the nanobodies. The onset of transgene expression when using AAVs in neurons typically display slow kinetics. We observed TDP43 mislocalization in less than 50% of the neurons after 7 days post-transduction that peaked at 10-12 days after addition of the nanobodies, when more than 80% of the cells displayed TDP43 mislocalization. Hence, we do not believe that a two-day difference significantly alters the interpretation of the data.

      The decision to harvest neurons at day 30 for the qPCR data was taken to investigate whether the splicing changes seen at day 40 from the transcriptomics analysis can be detected well before the phenotypes observed at day 40.

      (3) The authors should further characterize their TDP-43 puncta. TDP-43 immunostaining is typically punctate so it is unclear if the puncta observed are physiologic or pathologic based on the analyses carried out in the current version of this manuscript. Additionally, do these puncta co-localize with stress granule markers or RNA transport granule markers? Are these puncta phosphorylated (which may be more reminiscent of end-stage pathologic observations in humans)?

      We have tried immunostaining neurons for phosphorylated TDP43. However, our immunostaining attempts were unsuccessful. Depending on the antibody, we either saw no signal (antibody from Cosmo Bio, TIP-PTD-M01A) or even the control neurons displayed detectable phosphorylation within the nucleus (antibody from Proteintech 22309-1-AP). Consequently, we performed western blot analysis using an antibody from Cosmo Bio, (TIP-PTD-M01A) that clearly shows hyperphosphorylation of TDP43 in whole cell lysates (Fig. 3D, E). Hence, we have referred to these structures as puncta and not aggregates (Page 4).

      To assess co-localization of the puncta with stress granules, we immunostained for the stress granule marker G3BP1. This was done in MNs that were treated with sodium arsenite (SA) or PBS as a control. In the PBS treated control MN cultures, TDP43 mislocalization alone did not induce stress granule formation. G3BP1+ stress granules were only observed following SA stress (0.5 mM, 60 minutes). Further, only a subset of TDP43 puncta overlapped with these stress granules (Supplementary Fig. 7) (Page 6).

      (4) The authors should include multiple time points in their evaluation of TDP-43 loss of function events and aggregation. Does loss of function get worse over time? Is there a time course by which RNA misprocessing events emerge or does everything happen all at once? Does aggregation get worse over time? Do these neurons die at any point as a result of TDP-43 proteinopathy?

      We agree that a time course to analyze TDP43 mislocalization and its consequences would be ideal. However, the mislocalization of TDP43 across neurons is not a coordinated process. At each given time instance, neurons display varying levels of TDP43 mislocalization. Answering the questions raised by the reviewer would require tracking individual neurons in real time in a controlled environment over weeks. Unfortunately, we currently do not have the hardware to run these experiments. However, we do observe increased levels of cleaved caspase 3 in MNs expressing the NES nanobody, indicating that these neurons indeed undergo apoptosis by day 40 (Fig.1).

      We have, however, analyzed changes in splicing using qPCR for 12 genes over a time course starting as early as 4 hours after inducing mislocalization. We detect time-dependent cryptic splicing events in all genes as early as 8 hours after doxycycline addition, coinciding with the appearance TDP43 mislocalization (Fig. 4A, B).

      (5) Can the authors please comment on whether or not their model is "tunable"? In real human disease, not every neuron displays complete nuclear depletion of TDP-43. Instead there is often a gradient of neurons with differing magnitudes of nuclear TDP-43 loss. Additionally, very few neurons (5-10%) harbor cytoplasmic TDP-43 aggregates at end-stage disease. These are all important considerations when developing a novel authentic and endogenous model of TDP-43 proteinopathy which the current manuscript fails to address.

      As shown in Fig .1, the neurons expressing the NES-nanobody display a wide range of mislocalization as assessed by the % of nuclear TDP43 present. By titrating the amount of AAVs added to the culture, the model can be tuned to achieve a wide gradient of TDP43 mislocalization.

      We calculated the size and percentage of neurons displaying TDP43 puncta. The size and the number of aggregates varies across the neurons that display TDP43 mislocalization. Around 50% of the neurons displayed small (1  um<sup>2</sup>) puncta while large puncta (> 5  um<sup>2</sup>) were observed in <10% of the cells, similar to observations in patient tissue (Fig. 1F).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      TDP-43 mislocalization occurs in nearly all of ALS, roughly half of FTD, and as a co-pathology in roughly half of AD cases. Both gain-of-function and loss-of-function mechanisms associated with this mislocalization likely contribute to disease pathogeneisis.

      Here, the authors describe a new method to induce TDP-43 mislocalization in cellular models. They endogenously tagged TDP-43 with a C-terminal GFP tag in human iPSCs. They then expressed an intrabody - fused with a nuclear export signal (NES) - that targeted GFP to the cytosol. Expression of this intrabody-NES in human iPSC-derived neurons induced nuclear depletion of homozygous TDP-43-GFP, caused its mislocalization to the cytosol, and at least in some cells appeared to cause cytosolic aggregates. This mislocalization was accompanied by induction of cryptic exons in well characterized transcripts known to be regulated by TDP-43, a hallmark of functional TDP-43 loss and consistent with pathological nuclear TDP-43 depletion. Interestingly, in heterozygous TDP-43-GFP neurons, expression of intrabody-NES appeared to also induce the mislocalization of untagged TDP-43 in roughly half of the neurons, suggesting that this system can also be used to study effects on untagged endogenous TDP-43 as well as TDP-43-GFP fusion protein.

      Strengths:

      A clearer understanding of how TDP-43 mislocalization alters cellular function, as well as pathways that mitigate clearance of TDP-43 aggregates, is critical. But modeling TDP-43 mislocalization in disease-relevant cellular systems has proven to be challenging. High levels of overexpression of TDP-43 lacking an NES can drive endogenous TDP-43 mislocalization, but such overexpression has direct and artificial consequences on certain cellular features (e.g. altered exon skipping) not seen in diseased patients. Toxic small molecules such as MG132 and arsenite can induce TDP-43 mislocalization, but co-induce myriad additional cellular dysfunctions unrelated to TDP-43 or ALS. TDP-43 binding oligonucleotides can cause cytosolic mislocalization as well. Each system has pros and cons, and additional ways to induce TDP-43 mislocalization would be useful for the field. The method described in this manuscript could provide researchers with a powerful way to study the combined biology of cytosolic TDP-43 mislocalization and nuclear TDP-43 depletion, with additional temporal control that is lacking in current method. Indeed, the authors see some evidence of differences in RNA splicing caused by pure TDP-43 depletion versus their induced mislocalization model. Finally, their method may be especially useful in determining how TDP-43 aggregates are cleared by cells, potentially revealing new biological pathways that could be therapeutically targeted.

      Weaknesses:

      The method and supporting data have limitations in its current form, outlined below, and in its current form the findings are rather preliminary.

      (1) Tagging of TDP-43 with a bulky GFP tag may alter its normal physiological functions, for example phase separation properties and functions within complex ribonucleoprotein complexes. In addition, alternative isoforms of TDP-43 (e.g. "short" TDP-43, would not be GFP tagged and therefore these species would not be directly manipulatable or visualizable with the tools currently employed in the manuscript.

      With reference to our answer above, we have confirmed using qPCR and RNA-seq analysis that adding a GFP tag to the C-terminus of TDP43 does not result in an appreciable loss of functionality. We do not observe any cryptic exon inclusion in STMN2 and UNC13A. Cryptic exon inclusion in these genes, especially STMN2, has been recognized as a very sensitive indicator of TDP43 loss of function (Supplementary Fig 1C, Supplementary 2D, Fig. 3, Fig.4)

      We acknowledge that truncated alternatively spliced versions of TDP43 will lose the GFP-tag and cannot be manipulated with our system. Since our GFP tag is positioned on the C-terminus, our system cannot manipulate these truncated fragments as the tag is lost in these isoforms. But these isoforms, if present, should be detectable using the Proteintech antibody against total TDP43, which recognizes N-terminal TDP43 epitopes. However, western blot analysis, even 20 days after inducing TDP43 mislocalization, showed no truncated fragments. This suggests that TDP43 mislocalization alone is insufficient to generate significant levels of truncated isoforms. We have added this section to the Limitations paragraph (page 9).

      (2) The data regarding potential mislocalization of endogenous TDP-43 in the heterozygous TDP-43-GFP lines is especially intriguing and important, yet very little characterization was done. Does untagged TDP-43 co-aggregate with the tagged TDP-43? Is localization of TDP-43 immunostaining the same as the GFP signal in these cells?

      The purpose of the heterozygous experiments was to see whether mislocalized TDP43 could potentially trap the untagged TDP43. If this was not the case, we would have seen a maximum of 50% of the TDP43 signal mislocalized to the cytoplasm. The fact that a sizeable proportion of cells had significantly higher levels of TDP43 loss from the nucleus, indicates that mislocalized TDP43 can indeed trap the untagged protein fraction. We used GFP immunostaining to identify the tagged TDP43 while an antibody against the endogenous TDP43 protein was used to detect total TDP43 levels. In the cells that show near complete loss of nuclear TDP43, the total TDP43 signal coincides with the GFP (tagged TDP43) signal. We are unable to distinguish the untagged fraction selectively as we do not have an antibody that can detect this directly.  

      But we agree with the reviewer that these observations need further detailed follow-up that we are unable to provide currently. Hence, we have removed this figure from the manuscript.

      (3) The experiments in which dox was used to induce the nanobody-NES, then dox withdrawn to study potential longer-lasting or self-perpetuating inductions of aggregation is potentially interesting. However, the nanobody was only measured at the RNA level. We know that protein half lives can be very long in neurons, and therefore residual nanobody could be present at these delayed time points. The key measurement to make would be at the protein level of the nanobody if any conclusions are be made from this experiment.

      The reviewer has highlighted an important point. To address this issue, we tagged the nanobodies with a V5 tag that allowed us to directly measure nanobody levels within cells. After Dox withdrawal, we indeed observed significant expression of the nanobody within cells even after two weeks of Dox withdrawal. Extending the time point to three weeks allowed complete loss of the nanobody in most neurons. However, in contrast to our observations at two weeks, this was accompanied by a reversal of TDP43 mislocalization in these neurons at three weeks (Fig. 5).

      Surprisingly, in less than 10% of the neurons, we observed >80% of the total TDP43 still mislocalized to the cytoplasm, despite nearly undetectable levels of the nanobody. Super-resolution microscopy further revealed persistent cytoplasmic TDP43 in these neurons that did not overlap with residual nanobody signal. This suggests that in these neurons, the nanobody was no longer required to maintain TDP43 mislocalization (Fig. 5, page 7)

      (4) Potential differences in splicing and microRNAs between TDP-43 knockdown and TDP-43 mislocalization are potentially interesting. However, different patterns of dysregulated RNA splicing can occur at different levels of TDP-knockdown, thus it is difficult to assess whether the changes observed in this paper are due to mislocalization per se, or rather just reflect differences in nuclear TDP-43 abundance.

      This a fair point. It is possible that microRNA dysregulation might require a greater loss of nuclear TDP43 and maybe more resilient to TDP43 loss as compared to splicing. We have acknowledged this in the discussion section (page 9).

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) It would be helpful to include nuclear vs cytoplasmic ratios of TDP-43 instead of simply "% nuclear TDP-43"

      We have used % nuclear TDP43 as these values have biologically meaningful upper and lower bounds, which makes it easier to compare across experiments. We found that using a ratio of nuclear vs cytoplasmic TDP43 intensities displayed higher variability and a wider range.

      We have re-labelled the y-axis as “% Nuclear TD43 / soma TDP43” to make our quantification clearer. The conversion from % nuclear TDP43 to N/C is straightforward. If the % nuclear TDP43 is X, then the N/C ratio can be calculated as X / (100-X). For example, a % nuclear TDP43 of 80% would amount to an N/C ratio of 80/20 = 4.

      (2) The axis descriptions in Figure 1D are very unclear. While this is described better in the figure legend, it would be beneficial to have a more descriptive y-axis title in the figure (which may mean increasing the number of graphs).

      Axis descriptions and figures changed as recommended.

      (3) In Figure 1, the time points at which iPSNs were transduced with nanobody and/or fixed for immunostaining is somewhat inconsistent across all panels. This hinders interpretation of the figure as a whole. The authors should use same transduction and immunostaining time points for consistency or demonstrate that the same phenotype is observed regardless of transduction and immunostaining day as long as the time in between (time of nano body expression) is consistent. Subsequently, in Figure 2, a different set of time points is used.

      Please see our response in the public comments above

      (4) In Figure 1, please show individual data points for each independent differentiation to demonstrate the level of reproducibility from batch to batch.

      Data points have been shown per replicate (Supplementary Fig. 2)

      We have refined our approach for phenotypic analysis to improve consistency across different clones. Previously, we set thresholds on % nuclear TDP43 to distinguish MNs with nuclear versus mislocalized TDP43. This was done by ranking all cells based on % nuclear TDP43 and applying quantile-based thresholds—designating the top 25% as control and the bottom 25% as mislocalized, ensuring equal number of cells per category. However, we observed significant variability in thresholds across clones. For instance, the E8 clone had thresholds of 96% and 29%, while the E5 clone had 93% and 40%.

      To address this, we reanalysed the data using a standardized three-bin approach:

      (1) Control: MNs expressing the control nanobody.

      (2) Low-Moderate Mislocalization: MNs expressing the NES nanobody with > 40% nuclear TDP43.

      (3) Severe Mislocalization: MNs expressing the NES nanobody with < 40% nuclear TDP43.

      This approach ensured a more reliable comparison of TDP43 mislocalization effects across experiments. The conclusions remain the same.

      (5) In Figure 2, please show individual data points.

      Data points for all the qPCR analyses in the paper have been included as a supplementary text file.

      (6) In Figure 3, please show individual data points.

      Data points for the western blot data have been included as a supplementary data file.

      All other comments are within the public review.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) In general more robust quantification of many of the described phenotypes are necessary. In particular, no apparent quantification of cytosolic mislocalization was performed in Figure 1, or quantification of mislocalization of Figure 3F. It is unclear in the western blot in Fig 1G if TDP-43 signal were normalized to total protein, and of note it seems that expression of the intrabody-NES reduced total proteins in the western blots that were shown. No quantification or measurement of the insoluble material was done or shown.

      We have quantified cytosolic mislocalization of TDP43 (Fig. 1C). The y-axis indicates the total TDP43 signal observed in the nucleus as a percentage of the total signal observed in the soma (including the nucleus). This value has the advantage of ranging between 100% (perfectly nuclear) to 0% (complete nuclear loss). The boxplots indicate that expression of the NES-nanobody results in a range of cytosolic mislocalization with a median value around 40% of the TDP43 remaining in the nucleus.

      Western blot data in previous Fig. 1G was normalized to alpha-tubulin. We were unable to get a good signal for the insoluble fraction. From the alpha-tubulin alone, it cannot be concluded that NES-nanobody results in a decrease in total protein levels. In the revised western blot for phosphorylated TDP43 (Fig. 3D, E), we have quantified total and phosphorylated TDP43. Here, we observe a six-fold increase in the levels of phosphorylated TDP43 without a significant change in total TDP43 protein levels.

      To avoid potential mis-interpretation of our results, we have now removed the previous Fig. 1G.

      (2) Additional images of nearly all microscopy data at higher magnifications would be required to better evaluate TDP-43 localization. Ideally including images for each channel in addition to merged images, and especially for key figures such as Figure 1B, 3B, 3F.

      Better images have been provided.

      (3) No control images were shown for Figure 1F and 3F. It is unclear what the bright punctate spots of cytoplasmic TDP-43 GFP signal represent. Are these true aggregates? If so, additional characterization would be required before such conclusions can be made, beyond the relatively superficial western blot analysis that was done in Figure 1.

      Control images have now been provided (Figure 1E). As we mentioned above, immunostaining analysis to characterize whether the aggregates are phosphorylated failed to provide a clear signal. However, we have now confirmed that the mislocalized TDP43 is indeed hyper-phosphorylated (Figure 3D, E). We have acknowledged this in the main text, and have referred to these as puncta reminiscent of aggregates (Page 4, Page 6).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:  

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This paper reports an intracranial SEEG study of speech coordination, where participants synchronize their speech output with a virtual partner that is designed to vary its synchronization behavior. This allows the authors to identify electrodes throughout the left hemisphere of the brain that have activity (both power and phase) that correlates with the degree of synchronization behavior. They find that high-frequency activity in the secondary auditory cortex (superior temporal gyrus) is correlated to synchronization, in contrast to primary auditory regions. Furthermore, activity in the inferior frontal gyrus shows a significant phase-amplitude coupling relationship that is interpreted as compensation for deviation from synchronized behavior with the virtual partner.

      Strengths:

      (1) The development of a virtual partner model trained for each individual participant, which can dynamically vary its synchronization to the participant's behavior in real-time, is novel and exciting.

      (2) Understanding real-time temporal coordination for behaviors like speech is a critical and understudied area.

      (3) The use of SEEG provides the spatial and temporal resolution necessary to address the complex dynamics associated with the behavior.

      (4) The paper provides some results that suggest a role for regions like IFG and STG in the dynamic temporal coordination of behavior both within an individual speaker and across speakers performing a coordination task.

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive comments on our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The main weakness of the paper is that the results are presented in a largely descriptive and vague manner. For instance, while the interpretation of predictive coding and error correction is interesting, it is not clear how the experimental design or analyses specifically support such a model, or how they differentiate that model from the alternatives. It's possible that some greater specificity could be achieved by a more detailed examination of this rich dataset, for example by characterizing the specific phase relationships (e.g., positive vs negative lags) in areas that show correlations with synchronization behavior. However, as written, it is difficult to understand what these results tell us about how coordination behavior arises.

      We understand the reviewer’s comment. It is true that this work, being the first in the field using real-time adapting synchronous speech and intracerebral neural data, is a descriptive work, that hopefully will pave the way for further studies. We have now added more statistical analyses (see point 2) to go beyond a descriptive approach and we have also rewritten the discussion to clarify how this work can possibly contribute to disentangle different models of language interaction. Most importantly we have also run new analyses taking into account the specific phase relationship, as suggested.

      We already had an analysis using instantaneous phase difference in the phase-amplitude coupling approach, that bridges phase of behaviour to neural responses (amplitude in the high-frequency range). However, this analysis, as the reviewer noted, does not distinguish between positive and negative lags, but rather uses the continuous fluctuations of coordinative behaviour. Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have now run a new analysis estimating the average delay (between virtual partner speech and patient speech) in each trial, using a cross-correlation approach. This gives a distribution of delays across trials that can then be “binned” as positive or negative. We have thus rerun the phase-amplitude coupling analyses on positive and negative trials separately, to assess whether the phase amplitude relationship depends upon the anticipatory (negative lags) or compensatory (positive lags) behaviour. Our new analysis (now in the supplementary, see figure below) does not reveal significant differences between positive and negative lags. This lack of difference, although not easy to interpret, is nonetheless interesting because it seems to show that the IFG does not have a stronger coupling for anticipatory trials. Rather the IFG seems to be strongly involved in adjusting behaviour, minimizing the error, independently of whether this is early or late.

      We have updated the “Coupling behavioural and neurophysiological data” section in Materials and methods as follows:  

      “In the third approach, we assessed whether the phase-amplitude relationship (or coupling) depends upon the anticipatory (negative delays) or compensatory (positive delays) behaviour between the VO and the patients’ speech. We computed the average delay in each trial using a cross-correlation approach on speech signals (between patient and VP) with the MATLAB function xcorr. A median split (patient-specific ; average median split = 0ms, average sd = 24ms) was applied to conserve a sufficient amount of data, classifying trials below the median as “anticipatory behaviour” and trials above the median as “compensatory behaviour”. Then we conducted the phase-amplitude coupling analyses on positive and negative trials separately.”

      We also added a paragraph on this finding in the Discussion:

      “Our results highlight the involvement of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) bilaterally, in particular the BA44 region, in speech coordination. First, trials with a weak verbal coordination (VCI) are accompanied by more prominent high frequency activity (HFa, Fig.4; Fig.S4). Second, when considering the within-trial time-resolved dynamics, the phase-amplitude coupling (PAC) reveals a tight relation between the low frequency behavioural dynamics (phase) and the modulation of high-frequency neural activity (amplitude, Fig.5B ; Fig.S5). This relation is strongest when considering the phase adjustments rather than the phase of speech of the VP per se : larger deviations in verbal coordination are accompanied by increase in HFa. Additionally, we also tested for potential effects of different asynchronies (i.e., temporal delay) between the participant's speech and that of the virtual partner but found no significant differences (Fig.S6). While lack of delay-effect does not permit to conclude about the sensitivity of BA44 to absolute timing of the partner’s speech, its neural dynamics are linked to the ongoing process of resolving phase deviations and maintaining synchrony.”

      (2) In the results section, there's a general lack of quantification. While some of the statistics reported in the figures are helpful, there are also claims that are stated without any statistical test. For example, in the paragraph starting on line 342, it is claimed that there is an inverse relationship between rho-value and frequency band, "possibly due to the reversed desynchronization/synchronization process in low and high frequency bands". Based on Figure 3, the first part of this statement appears to be true qualitatively, but is not quantified, and is therefore impossible to assess in relation to the second part of the claim. Similarly, the next paragraph on line 348 describes optimal clustering, but statistics of the clustering algorithm and silhouette metric are not provided. More importantly, it's not entirely clear what is being clustered - is the point to identify activity patterns that are similar within/across brain regions? Or to interpret the meaning of the specific patterns? If the latter, this is not explained or explored in the paper.

      The reviewer is right. We have now added statistical analyses showing that:

      (1) the ratio between synchronization and desynchronization evolves across frequencies (as often reported in the literature).

      (2) the sign of rho values also evolves across frequencies.

      (3) the clustering does indeed differ when taking into account behaviour. We have also clarified the use of clustering and the reasoning behind it.

      We have updated the Materials and methods section as follows:

      “The statistical difference between spatial clustering in global effect and brain-behaviour correlation was estimated with linear model using the R function lm (stat package), post-hoc comparisons were corrected for multiple comparisons using the Tukey test (lsmeans R package ; Lenth, 2016). The statistical difference between clustering in global effect and behaviour correlation across the number of clusters was estimated using permutation tests (N=1000) by computing the silhouette score difference between the two conditions.” We have updated the Results section as follows:

      (1) “This modulation between synchronization and desynchronization across frequencies was significant (F(5) = 6.42, p < .001 ; estimated with linear model using the R function lm).”

      (2) “The first observation is a gradual transition in the direction of correlations as we move up frequency bands, from positive correlations at low frequencies to negative ones at high frequencies (F(5) = 2.68, p = .02). This effect, present in both hemispheres, mimics the reversed desynchronization/synchronization process in low and high frequency bands reported above.”

      (3) “Importantly, compared to the global activity (task vs rest, Fig 3A), the neural spatial profile of the behaviour-related activity (Fig 3B) is more clustered, in the left hemisphere. Indeed, silhouette scores are systematically higher for behaviour-related activity compared to global activity, indicating greater clustering consistency across frequency bands (t(106) = 7.79, p < .001, see Figure S3). Moreover, silhouette scores are maximal, in particular for HFa, for five clusters (p < .001), located in the IFG BA44, the IPL BA 40 and the STG BA 41/42 and BA22 (see Figure S3).”

      (3) Given the design of the stimuli, it would be useful to know more about how coordination relates to specific speech units. The authors focus on the syllabic level, which is understandable. But as far as the results relate to speech planning (an explicit point in the paper), the claims could be strengthened by determining whether the coordination signal (whether error correction or otherwise) is specifically timed to e.g., the consonant vs the vowel. If the mechanism is a phase reset, does it tend to occur on one part of the syllable?

      Thank you for this thoughtful feedback. We agree that the relationship between speech coordination and specific speech units, such as consonants versus vowels, is an intriguing question. However, in our study, both interlocutors (the participant and the virtual partner) are adapting their speech production in real-time. This interactive coordination makes it difficult to isolate neural signatures corresponding to precise segments like consonants or vowels, as the adjustments occur in a continuous and dynamic context.

      The VP's ability to adapt depends on its sensitivity to spectral cues, such as the transition from one phonetic element to another. This is likely influenced by the type of articulation, with certain transitions being more salient (e.g., between a stop consonant like "p" and a vowel like "a") and others being less distinct (e.g., between nasal consonants like "m" and a vowel). Thus, the VP’s spectral adaptation tends to occur at these transitions, which are more prominent in some cases than in others.

      For the participants, previous studies have shown a greater sensitivity during the production of stressed vowels (Oschkinat & Hoole, 2022; Li & Lancia, 2024), which may reflect a heightened attentional or motor adjustment to stressed syllables.

      Here, we did not specifically address the question of coordination at the level of individual linguistic units. Moreover, even if we attempted to focus on this level, it would be challenging to relate neural dynamics directly to specific speech segments. The question of how synchronization at the level of individual linguistic units might relate to neural data is complex. The lack of clear, unit-specific predictions makes it difficult to parse out distinct neural signatures tied to individual segments, particularly when both interlocutors are continuously adjusting their speech in relation to one another.

      Therefore, while we recognize the potential importance of examining synchronization at the level of individual phonetic elements, the design of our task and the nature of the coordination in this interactive context (realtime bidirection adaptation) led us to focus more broadly on the overall dynamics of speech synchronization at the syllabic level, rather than on specific linguistic units.

      We now state at the end of the Discussion section:

      “It is worth noting that the influence of specific speech units, such as consonants versus vowels, on speech coordination remains to be explored. In non-interactive contexts, participants show greater sensitivity during the production of stressed vowels, possibly reflecting heightened attentional or motor adjustments (Oschkinat & Hoole, 2022; Li & Lancia, 2024). In this study, the VP’s adaptation relies on sensitivity to spectral cues, particularly phonetic transitions, with some (e.g., formant transitions) being more salient than others. However, how these effects manifest in an interactive setting remains an open question, as both interlocutors continuously adjust their speech in real time. Future studies could investigate whether coordination signals, such as phase resets, preferentially align with specific parts of the syllable.” References cited:

      – Oschkinat, M., & Hoole, P. (2022). Reactive feedback control and adaptation to perturbed speech timing in stressed and unstressed syllables. Journal of Phonetics, 91, 101133.

      – Li, J., & Lancia, L. (2024). A multimodal approach to study the nature of coordinative patterns underlying speech rhythm. In Proc. Interspeech, 397-401.

      (4) In the discussion the results are related to a previously-described speech-induced suppression effect. However, it's not clear what the current results have to do with SIS, since the speaker's own voice is present and predictable from the forward model on every trial. Statements such as "Moreover, when the two speech signals come close enough in time, the patient possibly perceives them as its own voice" are highly speculative and apparently not supported by the data.

      We thank the reviewer for raising thoughtful concerns about our interpretation of the observed neural suppression as related to speaker-induced suppression (SIS). We agree that our study lacks a passive listening condition, which limits direct comparisons to the original SIS effect, traditionally defined as the suppression of neural responses to self-produced speech compared to externally-generated speech (Meekings & Scott, 2021).

      In response, we have reconsidered our terminology and interpretation. In the revised Discussion section, we refer to our findings as a "SIS-related phenomenon specific to the synchronous speech context". Unlike classic SIS paradigms, our interactive task involves simultaneous monitoring of self- and externally-generated speech, introducing additional attentional and coordinative demands.

      The revised Discussion also incorporates findings by Ozker et al. (2022, 2024), which link SIS and speech monitoring, suggesting that suppressing responses to self-generated speech facilitates error detection. We propose that the decrease in high-frequency activity (HFa) as verbal coordination increases reflects reduced error signals due to closer alignment between perceived and produced speech. Conversely, HFa increases with reduced coordination may signify greater prediction error.

      Additionally, we relate our findings to the "rubber voice" effect (Zheng et al., 2011; Lind et al., 2014; Franken et al., 2021), where temporally and phonetically congruent external speech can be perceived as self-generated. We speculate that this may occur in synchronous speech tasks when the participant's and VP's speech signals closely align. However, this interpretation remains speculative, as no subjective reports were collected to confirm this perception. Future studies could include participant questionnaires to validate this effect and relate subjective experience to neural measures of synchronization.

      Overall, our findings extend the study of SIS to dynamic, interactive contexts and contribute to understanding internal forward models of speech production in more naturalistic scenarios.

      We have now added these points to the discussion as follows:

      “The observed negative correlation between verbal coordination and high-frequency activity (HFa) in STG BA22 suggests a suppression of neural responses as the degree of behavioural synchrony increases. This result is reminiscent of findings on speaker-induced suppression (SIS), where neural activity in auditory cortex decreases during self-generated speech compared to externally-generated speech (Meekings & Scott, 2021; Niziolek et al., 2013). However, our paradigm differs from traditional SIS studies in two critical ways: (1) the speaker's own voice is always present and predictable from the forward model, and (2) no passive listening condition was included. Therefore, our findings cannot be directly equated with the original SIS effect.

      Instead, we propose that the suppression observed here reflects a SIS-related phenomenon specific to the synchronous speech context. Synchronous speech requires simultaneous monitoring of self- and externallygenerated speech, a task that is both attentionally demanding and coordinative. This aligns with evidence from Ozker et al. (2024, 2022), showing that the same neural populations in STG exhibit SIS and heightened responses to feedback perturbations. These findings suggest that SIS and speech monitoring are related processes, where suppressing responses to self-generated speech facilitates error detection. In our study, suppression of HFa as coordination increases may reflect reduced prediction errors due to closer alignment between perceived and produced speech signals. Conversely, increased HFa during poor coordination may signify greater mismatch, consistent with prediction error theories (Houde & Nagarajan, 2011; Friston et al., 2020). Furthermore, when self- and externally-generated speech signals are temporally and phonetically congruent, participants may perceive external speech as their own. This echoes the "rubber voice" effect, where external speech resembling self-produced feedback is perceived as self-generated (Zheng et al., 2011; Lind et al., 2014; Franken et al., 2021). While this interpretation remains speculative, future studies could incorporate subjective reports to investigate this phenomenon in more detail.” References cited:

      – Franken, M. K., Hartsuiker, R. J., Johansson, P., Hall, L., & Lind, A. (2021). Speaking With an Alien Voice: Flexible Sense of Agency During Vocal Production. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human perception and performance, 47(4), 479-494. https://doi.org/10.1037/xhp0000799

      – Houde, J. F., & Nagarajan, S. S. (2011). Speech production as state feedback control. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 5, 82.

      – Lind, A., Hall, L., Breidegard, B., Balkenius, C., & Johansson, P. (2014). Speakers' acceptance of real-time speech exchange indicates that we use auditory feedback to specify the meaning of what we say. Psychological Science, 25(6), 1198-1205. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614529797

      – Meekings, S., & Scott, S. K. (2021). Error in the Superior Temporal Gyrus? A Systematic Review and Activation Likelihood Estimation Meta-Analysis of Speech Production Studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 33(3), 422-444. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_01661

      – Niziolek C. A., Nagarajan S. S., Houde J. F (2013) What does motor efference copy represent? Evidence from speech production Journal of Neuroscience 33:16110–16116Ozker M., Doyle W., Devinsky O., Flinker A (2022) A cortical network processes auditory error signals during human speech production to maintain fluency PLoS Biology 20.

      – Ozker, M., Yu, L., Dugan, P., Doyle, W., Friedman, D., Devinsky, O., & Flinker, A. (2024). Speech-induced suppression and vocal feedback sensitivity in human cortex. eLife, 13, RP94198. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.94198

      – Zheng, Z. Z., MacDonald, E. N., Munhall, K. G., & Johnsrude, I. S. (2011). Perceiving a Stranger's Voice as Being One's Own: A 'Rubber Voice' Illusion? PLOS ONE, 6(4), e18655.

      (5) There are some seemingly arbitrary decisions made in the design and analysis that, while likely justified, need to be explained. For example, how were the cutoffs for moderate coupling vs phase-shifted coupling (k ~0.09) determined? This is noted as "rather weak" (line 212), but it's not clear where this comes from. Similarly, the ROI-based analyses are only done on regions "recorded in at least 7 patients" - how was this number chosen? How many electrodes total does this correspond to? Is there heterogeneity within each ROI?

      The reviewer is correct, we apologize for this missing information. We now specify that the coupling values were empirically determined on the basis of a pilot experiment in order to induce more or less synchronization, but keeping the phase-shifted coupling at a rather implicit level.  

      Concerning the definition of coupling as weak, one should consider that, in the Kuramoto model, the strength of coupling (k) is relative to the spread of the natural frequencies (Δω) in the system. In our study, the natural frequencies of syllables range approximately from 2 Hz to 10Hz, resulting in a frequency spread of Δω = 8 Hz. For coupling to strongly synchronize oscillators across such a wide range, k must be comparable to or exceed Δω. Thus, since k = 0.1 is far much smaller than Δω, it is therefore classified as weak coupling.

      We have now modified the Materials and methods section as follows:

      “More precisely, for a third of the trials the VP had a neutral behaviour (close to zero coupling: k = +/- 0.01). For a third it had a moderate coupling, meaning that the VP synchronised more to the participant speech (k = -0.09). And for the last third of the trials the VP had a moderate coupling but with a phase shift of pi/2, meaning that it moderately aimed to speak in between the participant syllables (k = + 0.09). The coupling values were empirically determined on the basis of a pilot experiment in order to induce more or less synchronization but keeping the phase-shifted coupling at a rather implicit level. In other terms, while participants knew that the VP would adapt, they did not necessarily know in which direction the coupling went.”

      Regarding the criterion of including regions recorded in at least 7 patients, our goal was to balance data completeness with statistical power. Given our total sample of 16 patients, this threshold ensures that each included region is represented in at least ~44% of the cohort, reducing the likelihood of spurious findings due to extremely small sample sizes. This choice also aligns with common neurophysiological analysis practices, where a minimum number of subjects (at least 2 in extreme cases) is required to achieve meaningful interindividual comparisons while avoiding excessive data exclusion. Additionally, this threshold maintains a reasonable tradeoff between maximizing patient inclusion and ensuring that statistical tests remain robust.

      We have now added more information in the Results section “Spectral profiles in the language network are nuanced by behaviour” on this point as follows:

      “To balance data completeness and statistical power, we included only brain regions recorded in at least 7 patients (~44% of the cohort) for the left hemisphere and at least 5 patients for the right hemisphere (~31% of the cohort), ensuring sufficient representation while minimizing biases due to sparse data.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This paper investigates the neural underpinnings of an interactive speech task requiring verbal coordination with another speaker. To achieve this, the authors recorded intracranial brain activity from the left hemisphere in a group of drug-resistant epilepsy patients while they synchronised their speech with a 'virtual partner'. Crucially, the authors were able to manipulate the degree of success of this synchronisation by programming the virtual partner to either actively synchronise or desynchronise their speech with the participant, or else to not vary its speech in response to the participant (making the synchronisation task purely one-way). Using such a paradigm, the authors identified different brain regions that were either more sensitive to the speech of the virtual partner (primary auditory cortex), or more sensitive to the degree of verbal coordination (i.e. synchronisation success) with the virtual partner (secondary auditory cortex and IFG). Such sensitivity was measured by (1) calculating the correlation between the index of verbal coordination and mean power within a range of frequency bands across trials, and (2) calculating the phase-amplitude coupling between the behavioural and brain signals within single trials (using the power of high-frequency neural activity only). Overall, the findings help to elucidate some of the left hemisphere brain areas involved in interactive speaking behaviours, particularly highlighting the highfrequency activity of the IFG as a potential candidate supporting verbal coordination.

      Strengths:

      This study provides the field with a convincing demonstration of how to investigate speaking behaviours in more complex situations that share many features with real-world speaking contexts e.g. simultaneous engagement of speech perception and production processes, the presence of an interlocutor, and the need for inter-speaker coordination. The findings thus go beyond previous work that has typically studied solo speech production in isolation, and represent a significant advance in our understanding of speech as a social and communicative behaviour. It is further an impressive feat to develop a paradigm in which the degree of cooperativity of the synchronisation partner can be so tightly controlled; in this way, this study combines the benefits of using prerecorded stimuli (namely, the high degree of experimental control) with the benefits of using a live synchronisation partner (allowing the task to be truly two-way interactive, an important criticism of other work using pre-recorded stimuli). A further key strength of the study lies in its employment of stereotactic EEG to measure brain responses with both high temporal and spatial resolution, an ideal method for studying the unfolding relationship between neural processing and this dynamic coordination behaviour.

      We sincerely appreciate the Reviewer's thoughtful and positive feedback on our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      One major limitation of the current study is the lack of coverage of the right hemisphere by the implanted electrodes. Of course, electrode location is solely clinically motivated, and so the authors did not have control over this. However, this means that the current study neglects the potentially important role of the right hemisphere in this task. The right hemisphere has previously been proposed to support feedback control for speech (likely a core process engaged by synchronous speech), as opposed to the left hemisphere which has been argued to underlie feedforward control (Tourville & Guenther, 2011). Indeed, a previous fMRI study of synchronous speech reported the engagement of a network of right hemisphere regions, including STG, IPL, IFG, and the temporal pole (Jasmin et al., 2016). Further, the release from speech-induced suppression during a synchronous speech reported by Jasmin et al. was found in the right temporal pole, which may explain the discrepancy with the current finding of reduced leftward high-frequency activity with increasing verbal coordination (suggesting instead increased speech-induced suppression for successful synchronisation). The findings should therefore be interpreted with the caveat that they are limited to the left hemisphere, and are thus likely missing an important aspect of the neural processing underpinning verbal coordination behaviour.

      We have now included, in the supplementary materials, data from the right hemisphere, although the coverage is a bit sparse (Figures S2, S4, S5, see our responses in the ‘Recommendation for the authors’ section, below). We have also revised the Discussion section to add the putative role of right temporal regions (see below as well).

      A further limitation of this study is that its findings are purely correlational in nature; that is, the results tell us how neural activity correlates with behaviour, but not whether it is instrumental in that behaviour. Elucidating the latter would require some form of intervention such as electrode stimulation, to disrupt activity in a brain area and measure the resulting effect on behaviour. Any claims therefore as to the specific role of brain areas in verbal coordination (e.g. the role of the IFG in supporting online coordinative adjustments to achieve synchronisation) are therefore speculative.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s observation regarding the correlational nature of our findings and agree that this is a common limitation of neuroimaging studies. While elucidating causal relationships would indeed require intervention techniques such as electrical stimulation, our study leverages the unique advantages of intracerebral recordings, offering the best available spatial and temporal resolution alongside a high signal-tonoise ratio. These attributes ensure that our data accurately reflect neural activity and its temporal dynamics, providing a robust foundation for understanding the relationship between neural processes and behaviour. Therefore, while causal claims are beyond the scope of this study, the precision of our methodology allows us to make well-supported observations about the neural correlates of synchronous speech tasks.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewing Editor Comment:

      After joint consultation, we are seeing the potential for the report to be strengthened and the evidence here to be deemed ultimately at least 'solid': to us (editors and reviewers) it seems that this would require both (1) clarifying/acknowledging the limitations of not having right hemisphere data, and (2) running some of the additional analyses the reviewers suggest, which should allow for richer examination of the data e.g. phase relationships in areas that correlate with synchronisation.

      We have now added data on the right hemisphere (RH) that we did not previously report due to a rather sparse sampling of the RH. These results are now reported in the Results section as well as in the Supplementary section, where we put all right hemisphere figures for all analyses (Figure S2, S4, S5). We have also run additional analyses digging into the phase relationship in areas that correlate with synchronisation (Figure S6). These additional analyses allowed us to improve the Discussion section as well.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      In some sections, the writing is a bit unclear, with both typos and vague statements that could be fixed with careful proofreading.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out areas where the writing could be improved. We carefully proofread the manuscript to address typos and clarify any vague statements. Specific sections identified as unclear have been rephrased for better precision and readability.

      In Figure 1, the colors repeat, making it impossible to tell patients apart.

      We have now updated Figure 1 colormap to avoid redundancy and added the right hemisphere.

      Line 132: "16 unilateral implantations (9 left, 7 bilateral implantations)". Should this say 7 right hemisphere? If so, the following sentence stating that there was "insufficient cover [sic] of the right hemisphere" is unclear, since the number of patients between LH and RH is similar.

      The confusion was due to the fact that the lateralization refers to the presence/absence of electrodes in the Heschl’s gyrus (left : H’ ; right : H) exclusively.

      We have thus changed this section as follows:

      “16 patients (7 women, mean age 29.8 y, range 17 - 50 y) with pharmacoresistant epilepsy took part in the study. They were included if their implantation map covered at least partially the Heschl's gyrus and had sufficiently intact diction to support relatively sustained language production.” The relevant part (previously line 132) now states:

      “Sixteen patients with a total of 236 electrodes (145 in the left hemisphere) and 2395 contacts (1459 in the left hemisphere, see Figure 1). While this gives a rather sparse coverage of the right hemisphere, we decided, due to the rarity of this type of data, to report results for both hemispheres, with figures for the left hemisphere in the main text and figures for the right hemisphere in the supplementary section.”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) To address the concern regarding the absence of data from the right hemisphere, I would advise the authors to directly acknowledge this limitation in their Discussion section, citing relevant work suggesting that the right hemisphere has an important role to play in this task (e.g. Jasmin et al., 2016). You should also make this clear in your abstract e.g. you could rewrite the sentence in line 40 to be: "Then, we recorded the intracranial brain activity of the left hemisphere in 16 patients with drug-resistant epilepsy...".

      We are grateful to the reviewer for this comment that incited us to look into the right hemisphere data. We have now included results in the right hemisphere, although the coverage is a bit sparse. We have also revised the Discussion section to add the putative role of right temporal regions. Interestingly, our results show, as suggested by the reviewer, a clear involvement of the RH in this task.

      First, the full brain analyses show a very similar implication of the RH as compared to the LH (see Figure below). We have now added in the Results section:

      “As expected, the whole language network is strongly involved, including both dorsal and ventral pathways (Fig 3A). More precisely, in the left temporal lobe the superior, middle and inferior temporal gyri, in the left parietal lobe the inferior parietal lobule (IPL) and in the left frontal lobe the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and the middle frontal gyrus (MFG). Similar results are observed in the right hemisphere, neural responses being present across all six frequency bands with medium to large modulation in activity compared to baseline (Figure S2A) in the same regions. Desynchronizations are present in the theta, alpha and beta bands while the low gamma and HFa bands show power increases.”

      As to compared to the left hemisphere, assessing brain-behaviour correlations in the right hemisphere does not provide the same statistical power, because some anatomical regions have very few electrodes. Nonetheless, we observe a strong correlation in the right IFG, similar to the one we previously reported in the left hemisphere, and we now report in the Results section:

      “The decrease in HFa along the dorsal pathway is replicated in the right hemisphere (Figure S4). However, while both the right STG BA41/42 and STG BA22 present a power increase (compared to baseline) — with a stronger increase for the STG BA41/42 — neither shows a significant correlation with verbal coordination (t(45)=-1.65, p=.1 ; t(8)=-0.67, p=.5 ; Student’s T test, FDR correction). By contrast, results in the right IFG BA44 are similar to the one observed in the left hemisphere with a significant power increase associated with a negative brainbehaviour correlation (t(17) = -3.11, p = .01 ; Student’s T test, FDR correction).”

      Interestingly, the phase-amplitude coupling analysis yields very similar results in both hemispheres (exception made for BA22). We have thus updated the Results section as follows:

      “Notably, when comparing – within the regions of interest previously described – the PAC with the virtual partner speech and the PAC with the phase difference, the coupling relationship changes when moving along the dorsal pathway: a stronger coupling in the auditory regions with the speech input, no difference between speech and coordination dynamics in the IPL and a stronger coupling for the coordinative dynamics compared to speech signal in the IFG (Figure 5B ). When looking at the right hemisphere, we observe the same changes in the coupling relationship when moving along the dorsal pathway, except that no difference between speech and coordination dynamics is present in the right secondary auditory regions (STG BA22; Figure S5).”

      We also included in the Discussion section the right hemisphere results also mentioning previous work of Guenther and the one of Jasmin. On the section “Left secondary auditory regions are more sensitive to coordinative behaviour” one can read:

      “Furthermore, the absence of correlation in the right STG BA22 (Figure S4) seems in first stance to challenge influential speech production models (e.g. Guenther & Hickok, 2016) that propose that the right hemisphere is involved in feedback control. However, one needs to consider the the task at stake heavily relied upon temporal mismatches and adjustments. In this context, the left-lateralized sensitivity to verbal coordination reminds of the works of Floegel and colleagues (2020, 2023) suggesting that both hemispheres are involved depending on the type of error: the right auditory association cortex monitoring preferentially spectral speech features and the left auditory association cortex monitoring preferentially temporal speech features. Nonetheless, the right temporal pole seems to be sensitive to speech coordinative behaviour, confirming previous findings using fMRI (Jasmin et al., 2016) and thus showing that the right hemisphere has an important role to play in this type of tasks (e.g. Jasmin et al., 2016).”

      References cited:

      – Floegel, M., Fuchs, S., & Kell, C. A. (2020). Differential contributions of the two cerebral hemispheres to temporal and spectral speech feedback control. Nature Communications, 11(1), 2839.

      – Floegel, M., Kasper, J., Perrier, P., & Kell, C. A. (2023). How the conception of control influences our understanding of actions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 24(5), 313-329.

      – Guenther, F. H., & Hickok, G. (2016). Neural models of motor speech control. In Neurobiology of language (pp. 725-740). Academic Press.

      (2) When discussing previous work on alignment during synchronous speech, you may wish to include a recently published paper by Bradshaw et al (2024); this manipulated the acoustics of the accompanist's voice during a synchronous speech task to show interactions between speech motor adaptation and phonetic convergence/alignment.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing to this recent and interesting paper. We added the article as reference as follows

      “Furthermore, synchronous speech favors the emergence of alignment phenomena, for instance of the fundamental frequency or the syllable onset (Assaneo et al., 2019 ; Bradshaw & McGettigan, 2021 ; Bradshaw et al., 2023; Bradshaw et al., 2024).”

      (3) Line 80: "Synchronous speech resembles to a certain extent to delayed auditory feedback tasks"- I think you mean "altered auditory feedback tasks" here.

      In the case of synchronous speech it is more about timing than altered speech signals, that is why the comparison is done with delayed and not altered auditory feedback. Nonetheless, we understand the Reviewer’s point and we have now changed the sentence as follows:

      “Synchronous speech resembles to a certain extent to delayed/altered auditory feedback tasks”

      (4) When discussing superior temporal responses during such altered feedback tasks, you may also want to cite a review paper by Meekings and Scott (2021).

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion, indeed this was a big oversight!

      The paper is now quoted in the introduction as follows:

      “Previous studies have revealed increased responses in the superior temporal regions compared to normal feedback conditions (Hirano et al., 1997 ; Hashimoto & Sakai, 2003 ; Takaso et al., 2010 ; Ozerk et al., 2022 ; Floegel et al., 2020 ; see Meekings & Scott, 2021 for a review of error-monitoring and feedback control in the STG during speech production).”

      Furthermore, we updated the discussion part concerning the speaker-induced suppression phenomenon (see below our response to the point 10).

      (5) Line 125: "The parameters and sound adjustment were set using an external low-latency sound card (RME Babyface Pro Fs)". Can you please report the total feedback loop latency in your set-up? Or at the least cite the following paper which reports low latencies with this audio device.

      Kim, K. S., Wang, H., & Max, L. (2020). It's About Time: Minimizing Hardware and Software Latencies in Speech Research With Real-Time Auditory Feedback. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(8), 25222534. https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00419

      We now report the total feedback loop latency (~5ms) and also cite the relevant paper (Kim et al., 2020).

      (6) Line 127 "A calibration was made to find a comfortable volume and an optimal balance for both the sound of the participant's own voice, which was fed back through the headphones, and the sound of the stimuli." What do you mean here by an 'optimal balance'? Was the participant's own voice always louder than the VP stimuli? Can you report roughly what you consider to be a comfortable volume in dB?

      This point was indeed unlcear. We have now changed as follows:

      “A calibration was made to find a comfortable volume and an optimal balance for both the sound of the participant's own voice, which was fed back through the headphones, and the sound of the stimuli. The aim of this procedure was that the patient would subjectively perceive their voice and the VP-voice in equal measure. VP voice was delivered at approximately 70dB.”

      (7) Relatedly, did you use any noise masking to mask the air-conducted feedback from their own voice (which would have been slightly out of phase with the feedback through the headphones, depending on your latency)?

      Considering the low-latency condition allowed with the sound card (RME Babyface Pro Fs), we did not use noise masking to mask the air-conducted feedback from the self-voice of the patients.

      (8) Line 141: "four short sentences were pre-recorded by a woman and a man." Did all participants synchronise with both the man and woman or was the VP gender matched to that of the participant/patient?

      We thank the reviewer for this important missing detail. We know changed the text as follows:

      “Four stimuli corresponding to four short sentences were pre-recorded by both a female and a male speaker. This allowed to adapt to the natural gender differences in fundamental frequency (i.e. so that the VP gender matched that of the patients). All stimuli were normalised in amplitude.”

      (9) Can you clarify what instructions participants were given regarding the VP? That is, were they told that this was a recording or a real live speaker? Were they naïve to the manipulation of the VP's coupling to the participant?

      We have now added this information to the task description as follows:

      “Participants, comfortably seated in a medical chair, were instructed that they would perform a real-time interactive synchronous speech task with an artificial agent (Virtual Partner, henceforth VP, see next section) that can modulate and adapt to the participant’s speech in real time.”

      “The third step was the actual experiment. This was identical to the training but consisted of 24 trials (14s long, speech rate ~3Hz, yielding ~1000 syllables). Importantly, the VP varied its coupling behaviour to the participant. More precisely, for a third of the sequences the VP had a neutral behaviour (close to zero coupling : k = +/- 0.01). For a third it had a moderate coupling, meaning that the VP synchronised more to the participant speech (k = - 0.09). And for the last third of the sequences the VP had a moderate coupling but with a phase shift of pi/2, meaning that it moderately aimed to speak in between the participant syllables (k = + 0.09). The coupling values were empirically determined on the basis of a pilot experiment in order to induce more or less synchronization, but keeping the phase-shifted coupling at a rather implicit level. In other terms, while participants knew that the VP would adapt, they did not necessarily know in which direction the coupling went.”  

      (10) The paragraph from line 438 entitled "Secondary auditory regions are more sensitive to coordinative behaviour" includes an interesting discussion of the relation of the current findings to the phenomenon of speech-induced suppression (SIS). However, the authors appear to equate the observed decrease in highfrequency activity as speech coordination increases with the phenomenon of SIS (in lines 456-457), which is quite a speculative leap. I would encourage the authors to temper this discussion by referring to SIS as a potentially related phenomenon, with a need for more experimental work to determine if this is indeed the same phenomenon as the decreases in high-frequency power observed here. I believe that the authors are arguing here for an interpretation of SIS as reflecting internal modelling of sensory input regardless of whether this is self-generated or other-generated; if this is indeed the case, I would ask the authors to be more explicit here that these ideas are not a standard part of the traditional account of SIS, which only includes internal modelling of self-produced sensory feedback.

      As stated in the public review, we thank both reviewers for raising thoughtful concerns about our interpretation of the observed neural suppression as related to speaker-induced suppression (SIS). We agree that our study lacks a passive listening condition, which limits direct comparisons to the original SIS effect, traditionally defined as the suppression of neural responses to self-produced speech compared to externally-generated speech (Meekings & Scott, 2021).

      In response, we have reconsidered our terminology and interpretation. In the revised discussion, we refer to our findings as a "SIS-related phenomenon specific to the synchronous speech context." Unlike classic SIS paradigms, our interactive task involves simultaneous monitoring of self- and externally-generated speech, introducing additional attentional and coordinative demands.

      The revised discussion also incorporates findings by Ozker et al. (2024, 2022), which link SIS and speech monitoring, suggesting that suppressing responses to self-generated speech facilitates error detection. We propose that the decrease in high-frequency activity (HFa) as verbal coordination increases reflects reduced error signals due to closer alignment between perceived and produced speech. Conversely, HFa increases with reduced coordination may signify greater prediction error.

      Additionally, we relate our findings to the "rubber voice" effect (Zheng et al., 2011; Lind et al., 2014; Franken et al., 2021), where temporally and phonetically congruent external speech can be perceived as self-generated. We speculate that this may occur in synchronous speech tasks when the participant's and VP's speech signals closely align. However, this interpretation remains speculative, as no subjective reports were collected to confirm this perception. Future studies could include participant questionnaires to validate this effect and relate subjective experience to neural measures of synchronization.

      Overall, our findings extend the study of SIS to dynamic, interactive contexts and contribute to understanding internal forward models of speech production in more naturalistic scenarios.

      We have now added these points to the discussion as follows:

      “The observed negative correlation between verbal coordination and high-frequency activity (HFa) in STG BA22 suggests a suppression of neural responses as the degree of synchrony increases. This result aligns with findings on speaker-induced suppression (SIS), where neural activity in auditory cortex decreases during self-generated speech compared to externally-generated speech (Meekings & Scott, 2021; Niziolek et al., 2013). However, our paradigm differs from traditional SIS studies in two critical ways: (1) the speaker's own voice is always present and predictable from the forward model, and (2) no passive listening condition was included. Therefore, our findings cannot be directly equated with the original SIS effect.

      Instead, we propose that the suppression observed here reflects a SIS-related phenomenon specific to the synchronous speech context. Synchronous speech requires simultaneous monitoring of self- and externally generated speech, a task that is both attentionally demanding and coordinative. This aligns with evidence from Ozker et al. (2024, 2022), showing that the same neural populations in STG exhibit SIS and heightened responses to feedback perturbations. These findings suggest that SIS and speech monitoring are related processes, where suppressing responses to self-generated speech facilitates error detection.

      In our study, suppression of HFa as coordination increases may reflect reduced prediction errors due to closer alignment between perceived and produced speech signals. Conversely, increased HFa during poor coordination may signify greater mismatch, consistent with prediction error theories (Houde & Nagarajan, 2011; Friston et al., 2020).”

      (11) Within this section, you also speculate in line 460 that "Moreover, when the two speech signals come close enough in time, the patient possibly perceives them as its own voice." I would recommend citing studies on the 'rubber voice' effect to back up this claim (e.g. Franken et al., 2021; Lind et al., 2014; Zheng et al., 2011).

      We are grateful to the Reviewer for this interesting suggestion. Directly following the previous comment, the section now states:

      “Furthermore, when self- and externally-generated speech signals are temporally and phonetically congruent, participants may perceive external speech as their own. This echoes the "rubber voice" effect, where external speech resembling self-produced feedback is perceived as self-generated (Zheng et al., 2011; Lind et al., 2014; Franken et al., 2021). While this interpretation remains speculative, future studies could incorporate subjective reports to investigate this phenomenon in more detail.”

      (12) As noted in my public review, since your methods are correlational, you need to be careful about inferring the causal role of any brain areas in supporting a specific aspect of functioning e.g. line 501-504: "By contrast, in the inferior frontal gyrus, the coupling in the high-frequency activity is strongest with the input-output phase difference (input of the VP - output of the speaker), a metric that reflects the amount of error in the internal computation to reach optimal coordination, which indicates that this region optimises the predictive and coordinative behaviour required by the task." I would argue that the latter part of this sentence is a conclusion that, although consistent with, goes beyond the current data in this study, and thus needs tempering.

      We agree with the Reviewer and changed the sentence as follows:

      “By contrast, in the inferior frontal gyrus, the coupling in the high-frequency activity is strongest with the inputoutput phase difference (input of the VP - output of the speaker), a metric that could possibly reflect the amount of error in the internal computation to reach optimal coordination. This indicates that this region could have an implication in the optimisation of the predictive and coordinative behaviour required by the task.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors set out to explore the role of upstream open reading frames (uORFs) in stabilizing protein levels during Drosophila development and evolution. By utilizing a modified ICIER model for ribosome translation simulations and conducting experimental validations in Drosophila species, the study investigates how uORFs buffer translational variability of downstream coding sequences. The findings reveal that uORFs significantly reduce translational variability, which contributes to gene expression stability across different biological contexts and evolutionary timeframes.

      We thank the reviewer for carefully reading our manuscript and providing thoughtful and constructive feedback. We believe the manuscript has been significantly improved by incorporating your suggestions. Please find our detailed responses and corresponding revisions below.

      Strengths:

      (1) The study introduces a sophisticated adaptation of the ICIER model, enabling detailed simulation of ribosomal traffic and its implications for translation efficiency.

      (2) The integration of computational predictions with empirical data through knockout experiments and translatome analysis in Drosophila provides a compelling validation of the model's predictions.

      (3) By demonstrating the evolutionary conservation of uORFs' buffering effects, the study provides insights that are likely applicable to a wide range of eukaryotes.

      We appreciate your positive feedback and thoughtful summary of the strengths of our study.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Although the study is technically sound, it does not clearly articulate the mechanisms through which uORFs buffer translational variability. A clearer hypothesis detailing the potential molecular interactions or regulatory pathways by which uORFs influence translational stability would enhance the comprehension and impact of the findings.

      Thanks for your constructive comments. In the Discussion section of our previous submission (Original Lines 470-489), we proposed that uORFs function as “molecular dams” to smooth out fluctuations in ribosomal flow toward downstream CDS regions, primarily via mechanisms involving ribosome collision and dissociation. To further address your concern, we have expanded the Discussion and included a new model figure (Fig. 9) to more clearly articulate the potential biological and mechanistic basis by which translating 80S ribosomes may induce the dissociation of 40S ribosomes. The revised section (Lines 540–557) now reads:

      “Ribosome slowdown or stalling on mRNA due to rare codons [56,96-98] or nascent blocking peptides [99-102] frequently triggers ribosome collisions genome-wide [103-105]. Such collisions, especially among elongating 80S ribosomes, often activate ribosome quality control (RQC) pathways that recognize collision interfaces on the 40S subunit, leading to ribosomal subunit dissociation and degradation [106-108]. In mammals, ZNF598 specifically identifies collided ribosomes to initiate ubiquitin-dependent protein and mRNA quality control pathways [109-113]. Analogously, yeast employs Hel2-mediated ubiquitination of uS10, initiating dissociation via the RQC-trigger complex (RQT) [114]. Furthermore, the human RQT (hRQT) complex recognizes ubiquitinated ribosomes and induces subunit dissociation similarly to yeast RQT [115]. However, transient ribosome collisions can evade RQC by promoting resumed elongation through mechanical force provided by trailing ribosomes, thereby mitigating stalling [116]. Beyond 80S collisions, evidence increasingly highlights a distinct collision type involving scanning 40S subunits or pre-initiation (43S) complexes. Recently, an initiation RQC pathway (iRQC) targeting the small ribosomal subunit (40S) has been described, particularly involving collisions between scanning 43S complexes or between stalled 43S and elongating 80S ribosomes (Figure 9B) [117,118]. During iRQC, E3 ubiquitin ligase RNF10 ubiquitinates uS3 and uS5 proteins, resulting in 40S degradation [118]. This mechanism aligns closely with our ICIER model, proposing collision-driven 43S dissociation in the 5' UTRs. Future studies exploring these mechanisms in greater detail will clarify how uORFs modulate translational regulation through buffering effects.”

      (2) The study could be further improved by a discussion regarding the evolutionary selection of uORFs. Specifically, it would be beneficial to explore whether uORFs are favored evolutionarily primarily for their role in reducing translation efficiency or for their capability to stabilize translation variability. Such a discussion would provide deeper insights into the evolutionary dynamics and functional significance of uORFs in genetic regulation.

      Thank you for this insightful suggestion. We agree that understanding whether uORFs are evolutionarily favored for their role in translational repression or for their capacity to buffer translational variability is a compelling and unresolved question. Our study suggests that translational buffering, rather than translational repression alone, can also drive evolutionary selection favoring uORFs, although it remains challenging to empirically disentangle these functions due to their inherent linkage. We have expanded the discussion in the revised manuscript to address this point in more detail (Lines 494-513), which is reproduced as follows:

      “Previous studies have shown that a significant fraction of fixed uORFs in the populations of D. melanogaster and humans were driven by positive Darwinian selection 63,67, suggesting active maintenance through adaptive evolution rather than purely neutral or deleterious processes. While uORFs have traditionally been recognized for their capacity to attenuate translation of downstream CDSs, accumulating evidence now underscores their critical role in stabilizing gene expression under fluctuating cellular and environmental conditions [43,55,56]. Whether the favored evolutionary selection of uORFs acts primarily through their role in translational repression or translational buffering remains a compelling yet unresolved question, as these two functions are inherently linked. Indeed, highly conserved uORFs tend to be translated at higher levels, resulting not only in stronger inhibition of CDS translation [34,45,67] but also in a more pronounced buffering effect, as demonstrated in this study. This buffering capacity of uORFs potentially provides selective advantages by reducing fluctuations in protein synthesis, thus minimizing gene-expression noise and enhancing cellular homeostasis. This suggests that selection may favor uORFs that contribute to translational robustness, a hypothesis supported by findings in yeast and mammals showing that uORFs are significantly enriched in stressresponse genes and control the translation of certain master regulators of stress responses [41,42,94,95]. Our study suggests that translational buffering, rather than translational repression alone, can also drive evolutionary selection favoring uORFs, although it remains challenging to empirically disentangle these functions. Future comparative genomic analyses, coupled with experimental approaches such as ribosome profiling and functional mutagenesis, will be crucial in elucidating the precise evolutionary forces driving uORF conservation and adaptation.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      uORFs, short open reading frames located in the 5' UTR, are pervasive in genomes. However, their roles in maintaining protein abundance are not clear. In this study, the authors propose that uORFs act as "molecular dam", limiting the fluctuation of the translation of downstream coding sequences. First, they performed in silico simulations using an improved ICIER model, and demonstrated that uORF translation reduces CDS translational variability, with buffering capacity increasing in proportion to uORF efficiency, length, and number. Next, they analzed the translatome between two related Drosophila species, revealing that genes with uORFs exhibit smaller fluctuations in translation between the two species and across different developmental stages within the same specify. Moreover, they identified that bicoid, a critical gene for Drosophila development, contains a uORF with substantial changes in translation efficiency. Deleting this uORF in Drosophila melanogaster significantly affected its gene expression, hatching rates, and survival under stress condition. Lastly, by leveraging public Ribo-seq data, the authors showed that the buffering effect of uORFs is also evident between primates and within human populations. Collectively, the study advances our understanding of how uORFs regulate the translation of downstream coding sequences at the genome-wide scale, as well as during development and evolution.

      The conclusions of this paper are mostly well supported by data, but some definitions and data analysis need to be clarified and extended.

      We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful and constructive review. Your summary accurately captures the key findings of our study. We have carefully addressed all your concerns in the revised manuscript, and we believe it has been significantly improved based on your valuable input.

      (1) There are two definitions of translation efficiency (TE) in the manuscript: one refers to the number of 80S ribosomes that complete translation at the stop codon of a CDS within a given time interval, while the other is calculated based on Ribo-seq and mRNA-seq data (as described on Page 7, line 209). To avoid potential misunderstandings, please use distinct terms to differentiate these two definitions.

      Thank you for highlighting this important point, and we apologize for the confusion. The two definitions of translation efficiency (TE) in our manuscript arise from methodological differences between simulation and experimental analyses. To clarify, in the revised manuscript, we use “translation rate” in the context of simulations to describe the number of 80S ribosomes completing translation at the CDS stop codon per unit time. We retain the conventional “translation efficiency (TE)” for Ribo-seq–based measurements. 

      In this revised manuscript, we have added a more detailed explanation of TE in the revised manuscript (Lines 202–206), which now reads:

      “For each sample, we followed established procedures [62-66] to calculate the translational efficiency (TE) for each feature (CDS or uORF). TE serves as a proxy for the translation rate at which ribosomes translate mRNA into proteins, typically quantified by comparing the density of ribosome-protected mRNA fragment (RPF) to the mRNA abundance for that feature (see Materials and Methods).”

      (2) Page 7, line 209: "The translational efficiencies (TEs) of the conserved uORFs were highly correlated between the two species across all developmental stages and tissues examined, with Spearman correlation coefficients ranging from 0.478 to 0.573 (Fig. 2A)." However, the authors did not analyze the correlation of translation efficiency of conserved CDSs between the two species, and compare this correlation to the correlation between the TEs of CDSs. These analyzes will further support the authors conclusion regarding the role of conserved uORFs in translation regulation.

      In the revised manuscript, we have incorporated a comparison of translational efficiency (TE) correlations for conserved CDSs between the two species. We found that CDSs exhibit significantly higher interspecific TE correlations than uORFs, with Spearman’s rho ranging from 0.588 to 0.806. This suggests that uORFs tend to show greater variability in TE than CDSs, consistent with our model in which uORFs buffer fluctuations in downstream CDS translation. The updated results were included in the revised manuscript (Lines 223-227) as follows:

      “In contrast, TE of CDSs exhibited a significantly higher correlation between the two species in the corresponding samples compared to that of uORFs, with Spearman’s rho ranging from 0.588 to 0.806 (P = 0.002, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Figure 2A). This observation is consistent with our simulation results, which indicate that uORFs experience greater translational fluctuations than their downstream CDSs.”

      (3) Page 8, line 217: "Among genes with multiple uORFs, one uORF generally emerged as dominant, displaying a higher TE than the others within the same gene (Fig. 2C)." The basis for determining dominance among uORFs is not explained and this lack of clarification undermines the interpretation of these findings.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We apologize for the confusion. In our study, a “dominant” uORF is defined as the one with the highest translation efficiency (TE) among all uORFs within the same gene. This designation is based solely on TE, which we consider a key metric for uORF activity, as it directly reflects translational output and potential regulatory impact. We have revised the manuscript to clarify this definition (Lines 232–244), now stating:

      “Among genes with multiple uORFs, we defined the uORF with the highest TE as the dominant uORF for that gene, as TE is one of the most relevant metrics for assessing uORF function 45,67…… These results suggest that genes with multiple uORFs tend to retain the same dominant uORF across developmental stages, indicating that the dominant uORFs may serve as the key translational regulator of the downstream CDS.

      (4) According to the simulation, the translation of uORFs should exhibit greater variability than that of CDSs. However, the authors observed significantly fewer uORFs with significant TE changes compared to CDSs. This discrepancy may be due to lower sequencing depth resulting in fewer reads mapped to uORFs. Therefore, the authors may compare this variability specifically among highly expressed genes.

      Thank you for this thoughtful observation. We agree that the lower proportion of uORFs showing significant TE changes compared to CDSs, as reported in Table 1, appears inconsistent with our conclusion that uORFs exhibit greater translational variability. However, this discrepancy is largely attributable to differences in sequencing depth and feature length—uORFs are generally much shorter and more weakly expressed than CDSs, resulting in fewer mapped reads and reduced statistical power (Figure S18A).

      To address this issue, we first followed your suggestion and restricted our analysis to genes with both mRNA and RPF RPKM values above the 50th percentile in D. melanogaster and D. simulans. While this filtering increased the total proportion of features with significant TE changes (due to improved read coverage), the proportion of significant uORFs still remained lower than that of CDSs (Table R1). This suggests that even among highly expressed genes, the disparity in read counts between uORFs and CDSs persists (Figure S18B), and thus the issue is not fully resolved.

      To better capture biological relevance, we compared the absolute values of log2(TE changes) between D. melanogaster and D. simulans for uORFs and their corresponding CDSs. Across all samples, uORFs consistently exhibit larger TE shifts than their downstream CDSs, supporting our model that uORFs act as translational buffers (Figure 3B).

      We have made relevant changes to report the new analysis in this revised manuscript. Specifically, in our original submission, we stated this observation with the sentence “The smaller number of uORFs showing significant TE changes compared to CDSs between D. melanogaster and D. simulans likely reflects their shorter length and reduced statistical power, rather than indicating that uORFs are less variable in translation than CDSs.” To make this point clearer, in the revised version (Lines 275-284), we rephrased this sentence which read as follows: 

      “Note that due to their shorter length and generally lower TE, uORFs had considerably lower read counts than CDSs, limiting the statistical power to detect significant interspecific TE differences for uORFs. This trend consistently holds whether analyzing all expressed uORFs (Figure S18A) or only highly expressed genes (Figure S18B). Thus, the fewer uORFs showing significant TE divergence likely reflects lower read counts and statistical sensitivity rather than reduced translational variability relative to CDSs. In fact, the absolute values of log2(fold change) of TE for uORFs between D. melanogaster and D. simulans were significantly greater than those observed for corresponding CDSs across all samples (P < 0.001, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Figure 3B), suggesting that the magnitude of

      TE changes in CDSs is generally smaller than that in uORFs, due to the buffering effect of uORF.”

      Author response table 1.

      Proportion of uORFs and CDSs with significant TE changes before and after selecting HEGs

      (5) If possible, the author may need to use antibodies against bicoid to test the effect of ATG deletion on bicoid expression, particularly under different developmental stages or growth conditions.

      According to the authors' conclusions, the deletion mutant should exhibit greater variability in bicoid protein abundance. This experiment could provide strong support for the proposed mechanisms.

      Thank you for this excellent suggestion. We fully agree that testing Bcd protein levels across developmental stages or stress conditions using antibodies would be a strong validation of our model, which predicts greater variability in Bcd protein abundance upon uORF deletion.

      In fact, we attempted such experiments in both wild-type and mutant backgrounds. However, we encountered substantial difficulties in obtaining a reliable anti-Bcd antibody. Some Bcd antibodies referenced in the published literature were homemade and often shared among research groups as gifts [1-3] and some commercially available antibodies cited in previous studies are no longer supplied by vendors [4-6]. We managed to obtain a custom-made antibody from Professor Feng Liu, but unfortunately, it produced inconsistent and unsatisfactory results. Despite considerable effort—including during the COVID-19 pandemic—we were unable to identify a reagent suitable for robust and reproducible detection of Bcd protein.

      As an alternative, we used sucrose gradient fractionation followed by qPCR to directly measure the translation efficiency of bicoid in vivo. We believe this approach offers a clear and quantitative readout of translational activity, and it avoids potential confounding from protein degradation, which may vary across conditions and developmental stages. Nonetheless, we recognize the value of antibody-based validation and will pursue this direction in future work if reliable antibodies become available. We have added this limitation to the revised Discussion section (Lines 563–568) as follows:

      “We demonstrated that the bcd uORF represses CDS translation using sucrose gradient fractionation followed by qPCR—an approach that directly measures translation efficiency while minimizing confounding from RNA/protein degradation. However, detecting Bcd protein levels with antibodies across developmental stages or conditions in the mutants and wild-type controls would provide an even stronger validation of our model and should be explored in future studies.”

      Recommendations for the authors:  

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The authors should provide a more detailed explanation for the modifications made to the ICIER model. Specifically, an explanation of the biological or mechanistic rationale behind the ability of the 80S ribosome to cause upstream 40S ribosomes to dissociate from mRNA would help clarify this aspect of the model.

      Thank you for this suggestion. In the original submission, we described our modifications to the ICIER model in the section titled “An extended ICIER model for quantifying uORF buffering in CDS translation” (Lines 88-124 of the revised manuscript). 

      To further clarify the biological rationale behind this mechanism, we have now included a conceptual model figure (Figure 9) illustrating mechanistically how uORF translation can buffer downstream translation within a single mRNA molecule. Additionally, we expanded the Discussion to summarize the current understanding of how collisions between translating 80S ribosomes and scanning 40S subunits may lead to dissociation, referencing known initial ribosome quality control (iRQC) pathways. These revisions provide a clearer mechanistic framework for interpreting the buffering effects modeled in our simulations. The relevant part is reproduced from Discussion (Lines 540-557) which reads as follows:

      “Ribosome slowdown or stalling on mRNA due to rare codons [56,96-98] or nascent blocking peptides [99-102] frequently triggers ribosome collisions genome-wide [103-105]. Such collisions, especially among elongating 80S ribosomes, often activate ribosome quality control (RQC) pathways that recognize collision interfaces on the 40S subunit, leading to ribosomal subunit dissociation and degradation [106-108]. In mammals, ZNF598 specifically identifies collided ribosomes to initiate ubiquitin-dependent protein and mRNA quality control pathways [109-113]. Analogously, yeast employs Hel2-mediated ubiquitination of uS10, initiating dissociation via the RQC-trigger complex (RQT) [114]. Furthermore, the human RQT (hRQT) complex recognizes ubiquitinated ribosomes and induces subunit dissociation similarly to yeast RQT [115]. However, transient ribosome collisions can evade RQC by promoting resumed elongation through mechanical force provided by trailing ribosomes, thereby mitigating stalling [116]. Beyond 80S collisions, evidence increasingly highlights a distinct collision type involving scanning 40S subunits or pre-initiation (43S) complexes. Recently, an initiation RQC pathway (iRQC) targeting the small ribosomal subunit (40S) has been described, particularly involving collisions between scanning 43S complexes or between stalled 43S and elongating 80S ribosomes (Figure 9B) [117,118]. During iRQC, E3 ubiquitin ligase RNF10 ubiquitinates uS3 and uS5 proteins, resulting in 40S degradation [118]. This mechanism aligns closely with our ICIER model, proposing collision-driven 43S dissociation in the 5' UTRs. Future studies exploring these mechanisms in greater detail will clarify how uORFs modulate translational regulation through buffering effects.”

      (2) The figure legend references Figure 5C; however, this figure appears to be missing from the document.

      We apologize for the oversight. The missing panel previously referred to as Figure 5C has now been incorporated into the revised Figure 6A. The figure and its corresponding legend have been corrected accordingly in the updated manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      This is an important study that enhances our understanding of the roles of uORFs in translational regulation. In addition to the suggestions provided in the public review, the following minor points should be addressed before publication in eLife:

      (1) Page 7, line 207: "We identified 18,412 canonical uORFs shared between the two species (referred to as conserved uORFs hereafter)." The term "canonical uORFs" requires clarification. Does this refer to uORFs with specific sequence features, conservation, or another defining characteristic?

      Thank you for pointing this out. We apologize for the lack of clarity. In our study, a canonical uORF is defined as an open reading frame (ORF) that initiates with a canonical AUG start codon located in the 5′ untranslated region (UTR) and terminates with a stop codon (UAA, UAG, or UGA) within the same mRNA. Conservation of uORFs is defined solely based on the presence of AUG start codons at orthologous positions in the 5′ UTR across species, regardless of differences in the stop codon.

      To clarify this definition, we have revised the sentence as follows (Lines 213-219): “We focused on canonical uORFs that initiate with an ATG start codon in the 5′ UTR and terminate with a stop codon (TAA, TAG, or TGA). Because the ATG start codon is the defining feature of a canonical uORF and tends to be more conserved than its downstream sequence [67], we defined uORF conservation based on the presence of the ATG start codon in the 5′ UTR of D. melanogaster and its orthologous positions in D. simulans, regardless of differences in the stop codon. Using this criterion, we identified 18,412 canonical uORFs with conserved start codons between the two species.”

      (2) Page 8, line 227: "Furthermore, the dominant uORFs showed a higher proportion of conserved uATGs than the other translated uORFs." There appears to be a typographical error. Should "other uATGs" instead read "other uORFs"?

      Thank you for pointing this out. As we addressed in response to your previous concern, in this study, we defined uORF conservation primarily based on the presence of their start codon (uATG) both in D. melanogaster and the orthologous sites of D. simulans, as the start codon is the defining feature of a uORF and tends to be more conserved than the remaining sequence, as demonstrated in our previous study [7]. We used the term “conserved uATGs” to reflect this definition and believe it accurately conveys the intended meaning in this context.

      (3) Page 8, line 240: "uORFs exhibited a significant positive correlation with the TE of their downstream CDSs in all samples analyzed (P < 0.001, Spearman's correlation)." A Spearman's rho of 0.11 or 0.21 may not practically represent a "significant" positive correlation. Consider rephrasing this as "a positive correlation."

      Thank you for the suggestion. We have revised the sentence in the manuscript to read (Lines 257-259): “uORFs exhibited a modest, yet statistically significant, positive correlation with the TE of their downstream CDSs across all samples analyzed (P < 0.001, Spearman’s correlation).”

      (4) Page 9, line 269: The analysis of interspecific TE changes between uORFs and their corresponding CDSs is a crucial piece of evidence supporting the authors' conclusions. Presenting this analysis as part of the figures, rather than in "Table 1," would improve clarity and accessibility.

      Thank you for this suggestion. In Table 1, we originally presented the number of uORFs and CDSs that showed significant differences in TE between D. melanogaster and D. simulans during various developmental stages. One key point we aimed to emphasize was that, although TE changes in uORFs and their downstream CDSs are positively correlated, there is a notable difference in the magnitude of these changes. To better convey this, we have summarized the core findings of Table 1 in graphical form.

      In Figure 3B of the revised version, we compared the absolute values of interspecific TE changes between CDS and uORF, showing that CDSs consistently exhibit smaller shifts than their upstream uORFs. This result further supports the translational buffering effect of uORFs on downstream CDS expression. We have included the updated results in the revised manuscript (Lines 281-284) as follows:

      “In fact, the absolute values of log2(fold change) of TE for uORFs between D. melanogaster and D. simulans was significantly greater than that observed for corresponding CDSs across all samples (P < 0.001, Wilcoxon signed-rank test; Figure 3B), suggesting that the magnitude of TE changes in CDSs is generally smaller than that in uORFs, due to the buffering effect of uORF.”

      (5) Page 9, line 279: The phrase "dominantly translated" needs clarification. Does it refer to Figure 2C, where one uORF is dominantly translated within a gene, or does it mean that the uORF's translation is higher than that of its corresponding CDS?

      We apologize for the obscurity. The phrase "dominantly translated" means one uORF with the highest TE compared to other uORFs within a gene. We have rephrased the relevant sentence in the revised version (Lines 299-304), which now reads:

      “To investigate how the conservation level and translation patterns of uORFs influence their buffering capacity on CDS translation, we categorized genes expressed in each pair of samples into three classes:

      Class I, genes with conserved uORFs that are dominantly translated (i.e., exhibiting the highest TE among all uORFs within the same gene) in both Drosophila species; Class II, genes with conserved uORFs that are translated in both species but not dominantly translated in at least one; and Class III, the remaining expressed genes.”

      (6) The sequencing data and analysis code should be made publicly available before publication to ensure transparency and reproducibility.

      Thank you for this suggestion. As described in the Data availability section, all deepsequencing data generated in this study, including single-ended mRNA-Seq and Ribo-Seq data of 10 developmental stages and tissues of Drosophila simulans and paired-end mRNA-Seq data of 0-2 h, 26 h, 6-12 h, and 12-24 h Drosophila melanogaster embryos, were deposited in the China National Genomics Data Center Genome Sequence Archive (GSA) under accession numbers CRA003198, CRA007425, and CRA007426. The mRNA-Seq and Ribo-Seq data for the different developmental stages and tissues of Drosophila melanogaster were published in our previous paper [8] and were deposited in the Sequence Read Archive (SRA) under accession number SRP067542.

      All original code has been deposited on GitHub: https://github.com/lujlab/uORF_buffer; https://github.com/lujlab/Buffer_eLife2025.

      Response reference

      (1) Li, X.Y., MacArthur, S., Bourgon, R., Nix, D., Pollard, D.A., Iyer, V.N., Hechmer, A., Simirenko, L., Stapleton, M., Luengo Hendriks, C.L., et al. (2008). Transcription factors bind thousands of active and inactive regions in the Drosophila blastoderm. PLoS Biol 6, e27. 10.1371/journal.pbio.0060027.

      (2) Horner, V.L., Czank, A., Jang, J.K., Singh, N., Williams, B.C., Puro, J., Kubli, E., Hanes, S.D., McKim, K.S., Wolfner, M.F., and Goldberg, M.L. (2006). The Drosophila calcipressin sarah is required for several aspects of egg activation. Curr Biol 16, 1441-1446. 10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.024.

      (3) Lee, K.M., Linskens, A.M., and Doe, C.Q. (2022). Hunchback activates Bicoid in Pair1 neurons to regulate synapse number and locomotor circuit function. Curr Biol 32, 2430-2441 e2433. 10.1016/j.cub.2022.04.025.

      (4) Wharton, T.H., Nomie, K.J., and Wharton, R.P. (2018). No significant regulation of bicoid mRNA by Pumilio or Nanos in the early Drosophila embryo. PLoS One 13, e0194865. 10.1371/journal.pone.0194865.

      (5) Wang, J., Zhang, S., Lu, H., and Xu, H. (2022). Differential regulation of alternative promoters emerges from unified kinetics of enhancer-promoter interaction. Nat Commun 13, 2714. 10.1038/s41467-022-30315-6.

      (6) Xu, H., Sepulveda, L.A., Figard, L., Sokac, A.M., and Golding, I. (2015). Combining protein and mRNA quantification to decipher transcriptional regulation. Nat Methods 12, 739-742. 10.1038/nmeth.3446.

      (7) Zhang, H., Wang, Y., Wu, X., Tang, X., Wu, C., and Lu, J. (2021). Determinants of genomewide distribution and evolution of uORFs in eukaryotes. Nat Commun 12, 1076. 10.1038/s41467-021-21394-y.

      (8) Zhang, H., Dou, S., He, F., Luo, J., Wei, L., and Lu, J. (2018). Genome-wide maps of ribosomal occupancy provide insights into adaptive evolution and regulatory roles of uORFs during Drosophila development. PLoS Biol 16, e2003903. 10.1371/journal.pbio.2003903.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study aimed at replicating two previous findings that showed (1) a link between prediction tendencies and neural speech tracking, and (2) that eye movements track speech. The main findings were replicated which supports the robustness of these results. The authors also investigated interactions between prediction tendencies and ocular speech tracking, but the data did not reveal clear relationships. The authors propose a framework that integrates the findings of the study and proposes how eye movements and prediction tendencies shape perception.

      Strengths:

      This is a well-written paper that addresses interesting research questions, bringing together two subfields that are usually studied in separation: auditory speech and eye movements. The authors aimed at replicating findings from two of their previous studies, which was overall successful and speaks for the robustness of the findings. The overall approach is convincing, methods and analyses appear to be thorough, and results are compelling.

      Weaknesses:

      Linking the new to the previous studies could have been done in more detail, and the extent to which results were replicated could have been discussed more thoroughly.

      Eye movement behavior could have been presented in more detail and the authors could have attempted to understand whether there is a particular component in eye movement behavior (e.g., microsaccades) that drives the observed effects.

      We would like to thank you for your time and effort in reviewing our work and we appreciate the positive comments!

      We extended our manuscript, now providing intermediate results on individual prediction tendency, which can be compared to our results from Schubert et al., (2023).

      Furthermore, we expanded our discussion now detailing the extent to which our results (do not) replicate the previous findings (e.g. differences in horizontal vs. vertical ocular speech tracking, lack of distractor tracking, link between ocular speech tracking and behavioral outcomes).

      While we agree with the reviewer that it is an important and most interesting question, to what extent individual features of gaze behavior (such as microsaccades, blinks etc.) contribute to the ocular speech tracking effect, it is beyond the scope of the current manuscript. It will be methodologically and conceptually challenging to distinguish these features from one another and to relate them to diverse cognitive processes. We believe that a separate manuscript is needed to give these difficult questions sufficient space for new methodological approaches and control analyses. The primary goal of this manuscript was to replicate the findings of Gehmacher et al. (2024) using similar methods and to relate them to prediction tendencies, attention, and neural speech tracking. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary

      Schubert et al. recorded MEG and eye-tracking activity while participants were listening to stories in single-speaker or multi-speaker speech. In a separate task, MEG was recorded while the same participants were listening to four types of pure tones in either structured (75% predictable) or random (25%) sequences. The MEG data from this task was used to quantify individual 'prediction tendency': the amount by which the neural signal is modulated by whether or not a repeated tone was (un)predictable, given the context. In a replication of earlier work, this prediction tendency was found to correlate with 'neural speech tracking' during the main task. Neural speech tracking is quantified as the multivariate relationship between MEG activity and speech amplitude envelope. Prediction tendency did not correlate with 'ocular speech tracking' during the main task. Neural speech tracking was further modulated by local semantic violations in the speech material, and by whether or not a distracting speaker was present. The authors suggest that part of the neural speech tracking is mediated by ocular speech tracking. Story comprehension was negatively related to ocular speech tracking.

      Strengths

      This is an ambitious study, and the authors' attempt to integrate the many reported findings related to prediction and attention in one framework is laudable. The data acquisition and analyses appear to be done with great attention to methodological detail (perhaps even with too much focus on detail-see below). Furthermore, the experimental paradigm used is more naturalistic than was previously done in similar setups (i.e. stories instead of sentences).

      Weaknesses

      For many of the key variables and analysis choices (e.g. neural/ocular speech tracking, prediction tendency, mediation) it is not directly clear how these relate to the theoretical entities under study, and why they were quantified in this particular way. Relatedly, while the analysis pipeline is outlined in much detail, an overarching rationale and important intermediate results are often missing, which makes it difficult to judge the strength of the evidence presented. Furthermore, some analysis choices appear rather ad-hoc and should be made uniform and/or better motivated.

      We would like to thank you very much for supporting our paper and your thoughtful feedback!

      To address your concerns, that our theoretical entities as well as some of our analytical choices lack transparency, we expanded our manuscript in several ways:

      (1) We now provide the intermediate results of our prediction tendency analysis (see new Figure 2 of our manuscript). These results are comparable to our findings from Schubert et al. (2023), demonstrating that on a group level there is a tendency to pre-activate auditory stimuli of high probability and illustrating the distribution of this tendency value in our subject population.

      (2) We expanded our methods section in order to explain our analytical choices (e.g. why this particular entropy modulation paradigm was used to measure individual prediction tendency).

      (3) We now provide an operationalisation of the terms “neural speech tracking” and “ocular speech tracking” at their first mention, to make these metrics more transparent to the reader.

      (4) We are summarizing important methodological information ahead of each results section, in order to provide the reader with a comprehensible background, without the necessity to read through the detailed methods section. 

      (5) We expanded our discussion section, with a special emphasis on relating the key variables of the current investigation to theoretical entities.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, the authors measured neural activity (using MEG) and eye gaze while individuals listened to speech from either one or two speakers, which sometimes contained semantic incongruencies.

      The stated aim is to replicate two previous findings by this group: (1) that there is "ocular speech tracking" (that eye-movements track the audio of the speech), (2) that individual differences in neural response to tones that are predictable vs. not-predictable in their pitch is linked to neural response to speech. In addition, here they try to link the above two effects to each other, and to link "attention, prediction, and active sensing".

      Strengths:

      This is an ambitious project, that tackles an important issue and combines different sources of data (neural data, eye-movements, individual differences in another task) in order to obtain a comprehensive "model" of the involvement of eye-movements in sensory processing.

      The authors use many adequate methods and sophisticated data-analysis tools (including MEG source analysis and multivariate statistical models) in order to achieve this.

      Weaknesses:

      Although I sympathize with the goal of the paper and agree that this is an interesting and important theoretical avenue to pursue, I am unfortunately not convinced by the results and find that many of the claims are very weakly substantiated in the actual data.

      Since most of the analyses presented here are derivations of statistical models and very little actual data is presented, I found it very difficult to assess the reliability and validity of the results, as they currently stand. I would be happy to see a thoroughly revised version, where much more of the data is presented, as well as control analyses and rigorous and well-documented statistical testing (including addressing multiple comparisons).

      We thank you for your thoughtful feedback. We appreciate your concerns and will address them below in greater detail.

      These are the main points of concern that I have regarding the paper, in its current format.

      (1) Prediction tendencies - assessed by listening to sequences of rhythmic tones, where the pitch was either "predictable" (i.e., followed a fixed pattern, with 25% repetition) or "unpredictable" (no particular order to the sounds). This is a very specific type of prediction, which is a general term that can operate along many different dimensions. Why was this specific design selected? Is there theoretical reason to believe that this type of prediction is also relevant to "semantic" predictions or other predictive aspects of speech processing?

      Theoretical assumptions and limitations of our quantification of individual prediction tendency are now shortly summarized in the first paragraph of our discussion section. With this paradigm we focus on anticipatory “top-down” predictions, whilst controlling for possibly confounding “bottom-up” processes. Since this study aimed to replicated our previous work we chose the same entropy-modulation paradigm as in other studies from our group (e.g. Demarchi et al. 2019, Schubert et al. 2023;2024, Reisinger et al. 2024), which has proven to give reproducible findings of feature-specific preactivations of sounds in a context of low entropy. One advantage of this design is that it gives us the opportunity to directly compare the processing of “predictable” and “unpredictable” sounds of the same frequency in a time-resolved manner (this argument is now also included in the Methods section).

      Regarding the question to what extent this type of prediction might also be relevant to “semantic” predictions we would like to refer to our previous study (Schubert et al., 2023), where we explicitly looked at the interaction between individual prediction tendency and encoding of semantic violations in the cortex. (In short, there we found a spatially dissociable interaction effect, indicating an increased encoding of semantic violations that scales with prediction tendency in the left hemisphere, as well as a disrupted encoding of semantic violations for individuals with stronger prediction tendency in the right hemisphere.) We did not aim to replicate all our findings in the current study, but instead we focused on merging the most important results from two independent phenomena in the domain of speech processing and bringing them into a common framework. However, as now stated in our discussion, we believe that “predictions are directly linked to the interpretation of sensory information. This interpretation is likely to occur at different levels along the cognitive (and anatomical) hierarchy…” and that “this type of prediction is relevant for acoustic processing such as speech and music, whose predictability unfolds over time.”

      (2) On the same point - I was disappointed that the results of "prediction tendencies" were not reported in full, but only used later on to assess correlations with other metrics. Even though this is a "replication" of previous work, one would like to fully understand the results from this independent study. On that note, I would also appreciate a more detailed explanation of the method used to derive the "prediction tendency" metric (e.g, what portion of the MEG signal is used? Why use a pre-stimulus and not a post-stimulus time window? How is the response affected by the 3Hz steady-state response that it is riding on? How are signals integrated across channels? Can we get a sense of what this "tendency" looks like in the actual neural signal, rather than just a single number derived per participant (an illustration is provided in Figure 1, but it would be nice to see the actual data)? How is this measure verified statistically? What is its distribution across the sample? Ideally, we would want enough information for others to be able to replicate this finding).

      We now included a new figure (similar to Schubert et al. 2023) showing the interim results of the “prediction tendency” effect as well as individual prediction tendency values of all subjects.

      Furthermore we expanded the description of the “prediction tendency” metric in the Methods section, where we explain our analytical choices in more detail. In particular we used a pre-stimulus time window in order to capture “anticipatory predictions”. The temporally predictably design gives us the opportunity to capture this type of predictions. The integration across channels is handled by the multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA), which inherently integrates multidimensional data (as mentioned in the methods section we used data from 102 magnetometers) and links it to (in this case) categorical information.

      (3) Semantic violations - half the nouns ending sentences were replaced to create incongruent endings. Can you provide more detail about this - e.g., how were the words selected? How were the recordings matched (e.g., could they be detected due to audio editing?)? What are the "lexically identical controls that are mentioned"? Also, is there any behavioral data to know how this affected listeners? Having so many incongruent sentences might be annoying/change the nature of listening. Were they told in advance about these?

      We expanded the Methods section and included the missing information: 

      “We randomly selected half of the nouns that ended a sentence (N = 79) and replaced them with the other half to induce unexpected semantic violations. The swap of nouns happened in the written script before the audio material was recorded in order to avoid any effects of audio clipping. Narrators were aware of the semantic violations and had been instructed to read out the words as normal. Consequently all target words occurred twice in the text, once in a natural context (serving as lexical controls) and once in a mismatched context (serving as semantic violations) within each trial, resulting in two sets of lexically identical words that differed greatly in their contextual probabilities (see Figure 1F for an example). Participants were unaware of these semantic violations.” Since we only replaced 79 words with semantic violations in a total of ~ 24 minutes of audio material we believe that natural listening was not impaired. In fact none of the participants mentioned to have noticed the semantic violations during debriefing (even though they had an effect on speech tracking in the brain). 

      (4) TRF in multi-speaker condition: was a univariate or multivariate model used? Since the single-speaker condition only contains one speech stimulus - can we know if univariate and multivariate models are directly comparable (in terms of variance explained)? Was any comparison to permutations done for this analysis to assess noise/chance levels?

      For mTRF models it depends on the direction (“encoding” vs. “decoding”) whether or not the model is comparable to a univariate model. In our case of an encoding model the TRFs are fitted to each MEG channel independently. This gives us the possibility to explore the effect over different areas (whereas a multivariate “decoding” model would result in only one speech reconstruction value).

      In both conditions (single and multi speaker) a single input feature (the envelope of the attended speech stream) was used. Of course it would be possible to fit the model to use a multivariate encoding model, predicting the brain’s response to the total input of sounds. This would, however, target a slightly different question than ours as we aimed to investigate how much of the attended speech is tracked.

      Regarding your suggestion of a comparison to permutations to assess noise levels we would like to point out that we chose the same methodological approach as in our previous studies, that we aimed to replicate here. Indeed in these original studies no permuted versions (with exception of the mediation analysis where comparing a model with an additional input predictor to a single predictor model would not result in a fair comparison) have been used. We conducted the mTRF approach considering the guidelines of Crosse et al. (2016) to the best of our knowledge and in accordance with similar studies in this field.

      Crosse, M. J., Di Liberto, G. M., Bednar, A., & Lalor, E. C. (2016). The multivariate temporal response function (mTRF) toolbox: a MATLAB toolbox for relating neural signals to continuous stimuli. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 10, 604.

      (5) TRF analysis at the word level: from my experience, 2-second segments are insufficient for deriving meaningful TRFs (see for example the recent work by Mesik & Wojtczak). Can you please give further details about how the analysis of the response to semantic violations was conducted? What was the model trained on (the full speech or just the 2-second long segments?) Is there a particular advantage to TRFs here, relative - say - to ERPs (one would expect a relatively nice N400 response, not)? In general, it would be nice to see the TRF results on their own (and not just the modulation effects).

      We fully agree with the reviewers statement that 2-second segments would have been too short to derive meaningful TRFs. To investigate the effect of semantic violations, we used the same TRFs trained on the whole dataset (with 4-fold cross validation). The resulting true as well as the predicted data was segmented into single word epochs of 2 seconds. We selected semantic violations as well as their lexically identical controls and correlated true with predicted responses for every word. Thus, we conducted the same analysis as for the overall encoding effect, focusing on only part of the data. We have reformulated the Methods section accordingly to clear up this misunderstanding. Since the TRFs are identical to the standard TRFs from the overall neural speech tracking, they are not informative to the semantic violation effect. However, since the mTRF approach is the key method throughout the manuscript (and our main focus is not on the investigations of brain responses to semantic violations) we have favoured this approach over the classical ERF analysis. 

      (6) Another related point that I did not quite understand - is the dependent measure used for the regression model "neural speech envelope tracking" the r-value derived just from the 2sec-long epochs? Or from the entire speech stimulus? The text mentions the "effect of neural speech tracking" - but it's not clear if this refers to the single-speaker vs. twospeaker conditions or to the prediction manipulation. Or is it different in the different analyses? Please spell out exactly what metric was used in each analysis.

      As suggested we now provide a clear definition of each dependent metric for each analysis.

      “Neural speech tracking” refers to the correlation coefficients between predicted and true brain responses from the aforementioned encoding model, trained and tested on the whole audio material within condition (single vs. multi-speaker).

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewing Editor Comments:

      The reviewers have provided a number of recommendations to improve the manuscript, particularly requesting that more data be reported, with an emphasis on the measurements themselves (eye movements and TRFs) rather than just the numerical outputs of mathematical models.

      We appreciate all the reviewers' and editor’s comments and effort to improve our manuscript. In the revised version we provide interim findings and missing data, updated figures that include an intuitive illustration of the metrics (such as TRFs), and a thoroughly revised discussion section where we focus on the relationship between our observed quantities and theoretical entities. We now offer operationalized definitions of the relevant concepts (“prediction tendency”, “active ocular sensing” and “selective attention”) and suggest how these entities might be related in the context of speech processing, based on the current findings. We are confident that this revision has improved the quality of our paper a lot and we are grateful for all the feedback and suggestions. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Participants had to fixate throughout the tasks. How did the authors deal with large eye movements that violated the instructed fixation?

      As described in the Methods section: “Participants were instructed to look at a black fixation cross at the center of a grey screen.” This instruction was not intended to enforce strict fixation but rather to provide a general reference point, encouraging participants to keep their gaze on the grey screen and avoid freely scanning the room or closing their eyes. Unlike trial-based designs, where strict fixation is feasible due to shorter trial durations, this approach did not impose rigid fixation requirements. Consequently, the threshold for "instruction violation" was inherently more flexible, and no additional preprocessing was applied to the gaze vectors.

      Fixating for such an extended period of time (1.5 hours?) is hard. Did fixation behavior change over time? Could (fixation) fatigue affect the correlations between eye movements and speech tracking? For example, fatigued participants had to correct their fixation more often and this drives, in part, the negative correlation with comprehension?

      Yes, participants spent approximately 2 hours in the MEG, including preparation time (~30 minutes). However, participants were given opportunities to rest their eyes between different parts and blocks of the experiment (e.g., resting state, passive listening, and audiobook blocks), which should help mitigate fatigue to some extent.

      That said, we agree that it is an intriguing idea that fatigue could drive the ocular speech tracking effect, with participants potentially needing to correct their gaze more as the experiment progresses. However, our analysis suggests this is unlikely for several reasons:

      (1) Cross-validation in encoding models: Ocular speech tracking effects were calculated using a 4-fold cross-validation approach (this detail has now been added to the Methods section; please see our response to public review #3). This approach reduces the influence of potential increases in gaze corrections over time, as the models are trained and validated on independent data splits.  Moreover, if there were substantial differences in underlying response magnitudes between folds - for instance, between the first and fourth fold - this would likely compromise the TRF's ability to produce valid response functions for predicting the left-out data. Such a scenario would not result in significant tracking, further supporting the robustness of the observed effects.

      (2) TRF time-course stability: If fatigue were driving increased gaze corrections, we would expect this to be reflected in a general offset (capturing the mean difference between folds) in the TRF time-courses shown in Figure 4 (right panel). However, no such trend / offset is evident.

      (3) Comparison of eye movement data: To directly investigate this possibility, we compared the amount of total eye movements between the first and last blocks for both the single and multi-speaker conditions. Total movement was calculated by first calculating the differences in pixel values between consecutive eye positions on both the x- and y-axes. The Euclidean distance was then computed for each difference, providing a measure of movement between successive time points. Summing these distances yielded the total movement for each block. Statistical analysis was performed separately for the single speaker (ASS) and multi-speaker (AMS) conditions. For each condition, paired comparisons were made between the first and last blocks (we resorted to non-parametric tests, if assumptions of normality were violated):

      For the single speaker condition (ASS), the normality assumption was not satisfied (p≤0.05p, Kolmogorov-Smirnov test). Consequently, a Wilcoxon signedrank test was conducted, which revealed no significant difference in total movements between the first and last blocks (z=−1.330, p=0.184). For the multi-speaker condition (AMS), the data met the normality assumption (p>0.05), allowing the use of a paired t-test. The results showed no significant difference in total movements between the first and last blocks (t=−0.184, p=0.855).

      The results are visualized in a bar plot (see below), where individual data points are displayed alongside the mean and standard error for each block. Statistical annotations indicate that neither condition demonstrated significant differences between the blocks. These findings suggest that total eye movements remained stable across the experimental conditions, regardless of whether participants were exposed to a single or multiple speakers.

      Author response image 1.

      (4) Behavioral responses: Participants’ behavioral responses did not indicate any decrease in comprehensibility for later blocks compared to earlier ones. Specifically, a comparison of comprehension scores between the first and last blocks revealed no significant difference in either the single-speaker condition (ASS; Wilcoxon signed-rank test Z=−0.5911, p=0.5545) or the multi-speaker condition (AMS; Wilcoxon signed-rank test: Z=0.5018, p=0.6158). These findings suggest that participants maintained consistent levels of comprehension throughout the experiment, regardless of the condition or block order. The results are visualized in a bar plot (see below), where individual data points are displayed alongside the mean and standard error for each block. Statistical annotations indicate that neither condition demonstrated significant differences between the blocks.

      Author response image 2.

      Together, these factors suggest that fatigue is unlikely to be a significant driver of the ocular speech tracking effects observed in this study.

      (2) The authors should provide descriptive statistics of fixation behavior /fixational eye movements. What was the frequency and mean direction of microsaccades, do they follow the main sequence, etc., quantify drift and tremor?

      Thank you for their suggestion regarding descriptive statistics. To address this, we computed the rates of microsaccades (which were extracted using the microsaccade detection algorithm as proposed in Liu, B., Nobre, A. C. & van Ede, F. Functional but not obligatory link between microsaccades and neural modulation by covert spatial attention. Nat. Commun. 13, 3503 (2022)) and fixations as these metrics are directly relevant to our study and the requests above.

      Microsaccade Rates:

      - Single speaker Condition: Mean = 2.306 Hz, SD = 0.363 Hz. ○ Multi speaker: Mean = 2.268 Hz, SD = 0.355 Hz.

      Fixation Rates:

      - Single speaker Condition: Mean = 2.858 Hz, SD = 1.617 Hz. ○ Multi speaker Condition: Mean = 2.897 Hz, SD = 1.542 Hz.

      These values fall within the expected ranges reported in the literature (fixation rates: 2– 4 Hz, microsaccade rates: ~0.5–2.5 Hz) and serve as a sanity check, confirming the plausibility of our eye-tracking data. Regarding the reviewer’s request for additional metrics (e.g., microsaccade direction, main sequence analysis, drift, and tremor), extracting these features would require advanced algorithms and analyses not supported by our current preprocessing pipeline or dataset. We hope that the provided metrics, which were the main focus of this study, serve as a sufficient sanity check and highlight the robustness of our data.

      Related to this, I am wondering whether microsaccades are the feature that drives speech tracking.

      This is an important and pressing question that we aim to address in future publications. Currently, our understanding - and the reason microsaccades and blinks are not analysed in this manuscript - is limited by methodological constraints. Specifically, microsaccades are binary response vectors, which are not compatible with TRF analyses. Addressing this would require adapting future models to handle timecontinuous binary response data or exploring alternative approaches, such as regression-based ERFs (for example as in Heilbron et al. 2022). As the primary goal of this manuscript was to replicate the findings of Gehmacher et al. (2024) using similar methods and to integrate these findings into an initial unified framework, we did not investigate additional eye movement features here. However, we agree that microsaccades (and also blinks, see below) likely contribute, at least in part, to the observed ocular speech tracking effects, and we now suggest this in the Discussion:  

      “Relatedly, it remains an open question whether microsaccades are a key feature driving ocular speech tracking. However, our current study does not analyze microsaccades due to methodological constraints: microsaccades are binary response vectors, which are incompatible with TRF analyses used here. Addressing this would require adapting models to handle time-continuous binary response data or potentially exploring alternative approaches, such as regression-based ERFs (e.g., as in Heilbron et al., 2022). While these limitations preclude microsaccade analysis in the current study, we hypothesize that they could enhance temporal precision and selectively amplify relevant sensory input, supporting auditory perception. Future studies should explore this possibility to uncover the specific contributions of microsaccades to speech tracking.”

      (3) Can the authors make sure that interpolated blinks did not drive any of the effects? Can interpolated blink trials be excluded?

      Using continuous audiobooks as stimuli meant that we could not exclude blink periods from the analysis without introducing substantial continuation artifacts in the TRF analysis. Importantly, the concept of covert motor routines and active sensing suggests that participants engage more strongly in motor routines - including ocular behaviors such as microsaccades and blinks - during tasks like speech tracking. These motor routines are inherently tied to individual gaze patterns, making microsaccades and blinks correlated with other ocular behaviors. This complicates efforts to disentangle their individual contributions to the observed ocular speech tracking effects.

      Engagement in these motor routines, as posited by active sensing, would naturally load onto various viewing behaviors, further intertwining their roles.

      Even if we were to examine correlations, such as the amount of blinks with the ocular speech tracking effect, it is unlikely to provide a clearer understanding due to these inherent overlaps. The methodological and conceptual challenge lies in distinguishing these features from one another and understanding their respective roles in driving the observed effects.

      However, the aim of this manuscript was not to dissect the ocular speech tracking effect in greater detail, but rather to relate it - based on similar analytical choices as in Gehmacher et al - to prediction tendencies, attention, and neural speech tracking. While it will be crucial in future work to differentiate these patterns and their connections to diverse cognitive processes, it is beyond the scope of this study to address all these questions comprehensively.

      We acknowledge that eye movements, including microsaccades and blinks (however, see challenges for this in response 2), remain underexplored in many experimental paradigms. Their interplay with cognitive processes - such as attention, prediction, and sensory integration - will undoubtedly be an important focus for future studies. 

      (4) Could the authors provide more details on how time shuffling was done for the eyemovement predictor, and include a circularly shifted version (or a version that does not destroy temporal contiguity) in their model comparisons? Some types of shuffling can result in unrealistic time series, which would end up in an unfair comparison with the model that has the real eye movement traces as predictors.

      We thank the reviewer for their insightful question regarding the time-shuffling procedure for the eye-movement predictor and for suggesting the inclusion of a circularly shifted version in our model comparisons. Below, we provide further details about our approach and the rationale behind it:

      (1) Random Shuffling: In our analysis, the eye-movement predictor was randomly shuffled over time, meaning that individual samples were randomly replaced. This method completely disrupts the temporal structure of the signal, providing a null model that directly tests whether the temporal mediation observed is due to the specific temporal relationship between ocular movements and envelope tracking.

      (2) Circular Shifting: While circular shifting maintains temporal contiguity, it introduces certain challenges in the context of TRF analysis. Specifically:

      - Adaptation to Shifts: The TRF model could adapt to the introduced shift, potentially reducing the validity of the null comparison.

      - Similarity due to Repetition: The broadband envelope exhibits strong repetitive patterns over time, such as rhythms inherent to speech. Circular shifting can therefore produce predictors that are very similar to the original signal. As a result, this similarity may lead to null distributions that do not adequately disrupt the temporal mediation we aim to test, making it less robust as a control.

      (3) Rationale for Random Shuffling: The primary goal of our mediation analysis is to determine whether there is a temporal mediation of envelope tracking by ocular movements. By deliberately destroying the temporal structure through random shuffling, we ensure that the null model tests for the specific temporal relationship that is central to our hypothesis. Circularly shifted predictors, on the other hand, may partially preserve temporal dependencies, making them less suitable for this purpose.

      In summary, while circular shifting is a valuable approach in other contexts, it is less appropriate for the specific goals of this study. We hope this explanation clarifies our methodological choices and demonstrates their alignment with the aims of our analysis.

      (5) Replication: I want to point out that it is great that the previous findings were in principle replicated. However, I would like to suggest a more nuanced evaluation of the replication:

      a) Instead of a (direct) replication, the present study should be called a 'conceptual replication', since modifications in design and procedure were made.

      Thank you very much for this suggestion! We now use the term ‘conceptual replication’ throughout the manuscript.

      b) Not all the findings from the Gehmacher et al., 2024 study were replicated to a full extent:

      Did the authors find indications of a vertical vs. horizontal tracking difference in the Gehmacher 2024 data? Could they check this in the Gehmacher 2024 data?

      The findings for horizontal and vertical gaze tracking in Gehmacher et al. (2024) are detailed in the supplementary material of that publication. Both single-speaker and multi-speaker target conditions showed significant speech tracking effects in both horizontal and vertical directions. However, there was a slightly stronger tracking effect for the single-speaker condition in the vertical direction. Due to the highly predictable structure of words in Gehmacher et al. effects here were probably overall boosted as compared to continuous audiobook listening, likely leading to the differentiation of horizontal and vertical gaze. See figures in Gehmacher et al. supplementary file for reference.

      c) Another difference between their previous and this study is the non-existent tracking of the multi-speaker distractor in this study. The authors should point this out clearly in the discussion and potentially provide an explanation.

      Thank you for highlighting this point! We now address this in the discussion:

      “Importantly, in contrast to Gehmacher et al. (2024), we did not observe ocular tracking of the multi-speaker distractor in this study. This difference is likely attributable to the simplistic single-trial, 5-word task structure in Gehmacher et al., which resulted in high temporal overlap between the target and distractor speech streams and likely drove the significant distractor-tracking effects observed in that study. The absence of such an effect during continuous listening in our study suggests that ocular tracking is indeed more specific to selective attention.”

      Minor:

      (1) I was a little surprised to not see an indication of eyes/eye movements in Figure 6. The intention of the authors might have been to create a general schematic illustration, but I find this a bit misleading. This paper provides nice evidence for a specific ocular effect in speech tracking. There is, to my knowledge, no indication that speech would be influenced by different kinds of active sensing (if there are, please include them in the discussion). Given that the visuomotor system is quite dominant in humans, it might actually be the case that the speech tracking the authors describe is specifically ocular.

      Taking into account all the reviewers' remarks on the findings and interpretations, we have updated this figure (now Fig. 7) in the manuscript to make it more specific and aligned with the revised discussion section. Throughout the manuscript, we now explicitly refer to active ocular sensing in relation to speech processing and have avoided the broader term 'active sensing' in this context. We hope these revisions address the concerns raised.

      (2) I find the part in the discussion (page 2, last paragraph) on cognitive processes hard to follow. I don't agree that 'cognitive processes' are easily separable from any of the measured responses (eye and brain). Referring to the example they provide, there is evidence that eye movements are correlated with brain activity that is correlated with memory performance. How, and more importantly, why would one separate those?

      Thank you for raising this important point. We have carefully considered your comments, particularly regarding the interplay between cognitive processes and measured responses (eye and brain), as well as the challenge of conceptually separating them. Additionally, we have incorporated Reviewer #2's query (13) into a unified and complementary reasoning. In response, we have rewritten the relevant paragraph in the discussion to provide a clearer and more detailed explanation of how ocular and neural responses contribute to speech processing in an interdependent manner. We hope this revision addresses your concerns and offers a more precise and coherent discussion on this topic:

      “Despite the finding that eye movements mediate neural speech tracking, the behavioural relevance for semantic comprehension appears to differ between ocular and neural speech tracking. Specifically, we found a negative association between ocular speech tracking and comprehension, indicating that participants with lower comprehension performance exhibited increased ocular speech tracking. Interestingly, no significant relationship was observed between neural tracking and comprehension.

      In this context, the negative association between ocular tracking and comprehension might reflect individual differences in how participants allocate cognitive resources. Participants with lower comprehension may rely more heavily on attentional mechanisms to process acoustic features, as evidenced by increased ocular tracking. This reliance could represent a compensatory strategy when higher-order processes, such as semantic integration or memory retrieval, are less effective. Importantly, our comprehension questions (see Experimental Procedure) targeted a broad range of processes, including intelligibility and memory, suggesting that this relationship reflects a trade-off in resource allocation between low-level acoustic focus and integrative cognitive tasks.

      Rather than separating eye and brain responses conceptually, our analysis highlights their complementary contributions. Eye movements may enhance neural processing by increasing sensitivity to acoustic properties of speech, while neural activity builds on this foundation to integrate information and support comprehension. Together, these systems form an interdependent mechanism, with eye and brain responses working in tandem to facilitate different aspects of speech processing.

      This interpretation is consistent with the absence of a difference in ocular tracking for semantic violations (e.g., words with high surprisal versus lexically matched controls), reinforcing the view that ocular tracking primarily reflects attentional engagement with acoustic features rather than direct involvement in semantic processing. This aligns with previous findings that attention modulates auditory responses to acoustic features (e.g., Forte et al., 2017), further supporting the idea that ocular tracking reflects mechanisms of selective attention rather than representations of linguistic content.

      Future research should investigate how these systems interact and explore how ocular tracking mediates neural responses to linguistic features, such as lexical or semantic processing, to better understand their joint contributions to comprehension.”.  

      (3) Attention vs. predictive coding. I think the authors end up with an elegant description of the observed effects, "as an "active sensing" mechanism that implements the attentional optimization of sensory precision." However, I feel the paragraph starts with the ill-posed question "whether ocular speech tracking is modulated not by predictive, but other (for example attentional) processes". If ocular tracking is the implementation of a process (optimization of sensory precision, aka attention), how could it be at the same time modulated by that process? In my opinion, adding the notion that there is a modulation by a vague cognitive concept like attention on top of what the paper shows does not improve our understanding of how speech tracking in humans works.

      Thank you for raising this point. We agree that it is critical to clarify the relationship between ocular speech tracking, attention, and predictive processes, and we appreciate the opportunity to refine this discussion.  

      To avoid the potential confusion that active ocular sensing represents on the one hand an implementation of selective attention on the other it seems to be modulated by it, we now use  the formulation “ocular speech tracking reflects attentional mechanisms rather than predictive processes.”

      To address your concern that the conceptualization of attention seems rather vague, we have revised the whole paragraph in order to redefine the theoretical entities in question (including selective attention) and to provide a clearer and more precise picture (see also our revised version of Fig. 6, now Fig. 7). We now focus on highlighting the distinct yet interdependent roles of selective attention and individual prediction tendencies for speech tracking.:

      “With this speculative framework we attempt to describe and relate three important phenomena with respect to their relevance for speech processing: 1) “Anticipatory predictions” that are created in absence of attentional demands and contain probabilistic information about stimulus features (here, inferred from frequency-specific pre-activations during passive listening to sound sequences). 2) “Selective attention” that allocates resources towards relevant (whilst suppressing distracting) information (which was manipulated by the presence or absence of a distractor speaker). And finally 3) “active ocular sensing”, which refers to gaze behavior that is temporally aligned to attended (but not unattended) acoustic speech input (inferred from the discovered phenomenon of ocular speech tracking). We propose that auditory inflow is, at a basic level, temporally modulated via active ocular sensing, which “opens the gates” in the sensory periphery at relevant timepoints. How exactly this mechanism is guided (for example where the information about crucial timepoints comes from, if not from prediction, and whether it requires habituation to a speechstream etc.) is yet unclear. Unlike predictive tendencies, active ocular sensing appears to reflect selective attention, manifesting as a mechanism that optimizes sensory precision. Individual differences with respect to anticipatory predictions on the other hand, seem to be independent from the other two entities, but nevertheless relevant for speech processing. We therefore support the notion that representational content is interpreted based on prior probabilistic assumptions. If we consider the idea that “a percept” of an (auditory) object is actually temporally and spatially distributed (across representational spacetime - see Fig. 7), the content of information depends on where and when it is probed (see for example Dennett, 1991 for similar ideas on consciousness). Having to select from multiple interpretations across space and time requires a careful balance between the weighting of internal models and the allocation of resources based on current goals. We suggest that in the case of speech processing, this challenge results in an independent adaptation of feature-based precision-weighting by predictions on the one hand and temporal precision-weighting by selective attention on the other.”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      My main recommendation is outlined in the Weaknesses above: the overarching rationale for many analysis choices should be made explicit, and intermediate results should be shown where appropriate, so the reader can follow what is being quantified and what the results truly mean. Specifically, I recommend to pay attention to the following (in no particular order):

      (1) Define 'neural speech tracking' early on. (e.g.: 'The amount of information in the MEG signal that can multivariately be explained by the speech amplitude envelope.' (is that correct?))

      Thank you for pointing out that this important definition is missing. It is now defined at the first mention in the Introduction as follows: “Here (and in the following) “neural speech tracking” refers to a correlation coefficient between actual brain responses and responses predicted from an encoding model based solely on the speech envelope”.

      (2) Same for 'ocular speech tracking'. Here even reading the Methods does not make it unambiguous how this term is used.

      It is now defined at the first mention in the Introduction as follows: “Ocular speech tracking” (similarly to “neural speech tracking” refers to the correlation coefficient between actual eye movements and movements predicted from an encoding model based on the speech envelope”.

      In addition also define both (neural and ocular speech tracking) metrics in the Methods Section.

      (3) Related to this: for ocular speech tracking, are simply the horizontal and vertical eye traces compared to the speech envelope? If so, this appears somewhat strange: why should the eyes move more rightward/upward with a larger envelope? And the direction here depends on the (arbitrary) sign of right = positive, etc. (It would make more sense to quantify 'amount of movement' in some way, but if this is done, I missed it in Methods.)

      Thank you for your insightful comments. You are correct that the horizontal and vertical traces were used for ocular speech tracking, and no additional details were included in the Methods. While we agree that the observed rightward/upward movement may seem unusual, this pattern is consistent with previous findings, including those reported in Gehmacher et al. (2024). In that study, we discussed how ocular speech tracking could reflect a broader engagement of the motor system during speech perception. For example, we observed a general right-lateralized gaze bias when participants attended to auditory speech, which we hypothesized might resemble eye movements during text reading, with a similar temporal alignment (~200 ms). We also speculated that this pattern might differ in cultures that read text from right to left.

      We appreciate your suggestion to explore alternative methods for quantifying gaze patterns, such as the "amount of movement" or microsaccades. While these approaches hold promise for future studies, our primary aim here was to replicate previous findings using the same signal and analysis methods to establish a basis for further exploration.  

      (4) In the Introduction, specifically blink-related ocular activity is mentioned as being related to speech tracking (for which a reference is, incidentally, missing), while here, any blink-related activity is excluded from the analysis. This should be motivated, as it appears in direct contradiction.

      Thank you for pointing this out. The mention of blink-related ocular activity in the Introduction refers to findings by Jin et al. (2018), where such activity was shown to align with higher-order syntactic structures in artificial speech. We have now included the appropriate reference for clarity.

      While Jin et al. focused on blink-related activity, in the present study, we focused on gaze patterns to investigate ocular speech tracking, replicating findings from

      Gehmacher et al. (2024). This approach was motivated by our goal to validate previous results using the same methodology. Importantly to this point, the exclusion of blinks in our analysis was due to methodological constraints of TRF analysis, which requires a continuous response signal; blinks, being discrete and artifact-prone, are incompatible with this approach.

      To address your concern, we revised the Introduction to clarify this distinction and provide explicit motivation for focusing on gaze patterns. It now reads:

      “Along these lines, It has been shown that covert, mostly blink related eye activity aligns with higher-order syntactic structures of temporally predictable, artificial speech (i.e. monosyllabic words; Jin et al, 2018). In support of ideas that the motor system is actively engaged in speech perception (Galantucci et al., 2006; Liberman & Mattingly, 1985), the authors suggest a global entrainment across sensory and (oculo)motor areas which implements temporal attention. 

      In another recent study from our lab (Gehmacher et al., 2024), we showed that eye movements continuously track intensity fluctuations of attended natural speech, a phenomenon we termed ocular speech tracking. In the present study, we focused on gaze patterns rather than blink-related activity, both to replicate findings from

      Gehmacher et al. (2024) and because blink activity is unsuitable for TRF analysis due to its discrete and artifact-prone nature. Hence, “Ocular speech tracking” (similarly to “neural speech tracking” refers to the correlation coefficient between actual eye movements and movements predicted from an encoding model based on the speech envelope.”

      Jin, P., Zou, J., Zhou, T., & Ding, N. (2018). Eye activity tracks task-relevant structures during speech and auditory sequence perception. Nature communications, 9(1), 5374.

      (5) The rationale for the mediation analysis is questionable. Let speech envelope = A, brain activity = B, eye movements = C. The authors wish to claim that A -> C -> B. But it is equally possible that A -> B -> C. They reflect on this somewhat in Discussion, but throughout the rest of the paper, the mediation analysis is presented as specifically testing whether A -> B is mediated by C, which is potentially misleading.

      Indeed we share your concern regarding the directionality of the relationships in the mediation analysis. Our choice of ocular movements as a mediator was motivated by the fact that the relationship between acoustic speech and neural activity is well established, as well as previous results indicating that oculomotor activity contributes to cognitive effects in auditory attention (Popov et al., 2022). 

      Indeed, here we treat both interpretations (“ocular movements contribute to neural speech tracking” versus “neural activity contributes to ocular speech tracking”) as equal.  We now emphasise this point in our discussion quite thoroughly:

      “It is important to note that our current findings do not allow for inference on directionality. Our choice of ocular movements as a mediator was motivated by the fact that the relationship between acoustic speech and neural activity is well established, as well as previous results indicating that oculomotor activity contributes to cognitive effects in auditory attention (Popov et al., 2022). However, an alternative model may suggest that neural activity mediates the effect of ocular speech tracking. Hence, it is possible that ocular mediation of speech tracking may reflect a) active (ocular) sensing for information driven by (top-down) selective attention or b) improved neural representations as a consequence of temporally aligned increase of sensory gain or c) (not unlikely) both. In fact, when rejecting the notion of a single bottom-up flow of information and replacing it with a model of distributed parallel and dynamic processing, it seems only reasonable to assume that the direction of communication (between our eyes and our brain) will depend on where (within the brain) as well as when we look at the effect. Thus, the regions and time-windows reported here should be taken as an illustration of oculo-neural communication during speech processing rather than an attempt to "explain" neural speech processing by ocular movements.”

      (6) The mediation analysis can be improved by a proper quantification of the effect (sizes or variance explained). E.g. how much % of B is explained by A total, and how much of that can in turn be explained by C being involved? For drawing directional conclusions perhaps Granger causality could be used.

      In Figure 4 (now Figure 5) of our manuscript we use standardized betas (which correspond to effect sizes) to illustrate the mediation effect. With the current mTRF approach it is however not possible (or insightful) to compare the variance explained. It is reasonable to assume that variance in neural activity will be explained better when including oculomotor behavior as a second predictor along with acoustic simulation. However this increase gives no indication to what extent this oculomotor behavior was task relevant or irrelevant (since all kinds of “arbitrary” movements will be captured with brain activity and therefore lead to an increase in variance explained). For this reason we chose to pursue the widely accepted framework of mediation (Baron & Kenny, 1986). This (correlational) approach is indeed limited in its interpretations (see prev. response), however the goal of the current study was to replicate and illustrate the triad relationship of acoustic speech input, neural activity and ocular movements with no particular hypotheses on directionality.

      (7) Both prediction tendency and neural speech tracking depend on MEG data, and thus on MEG signal-to-noise ratio (SNR). It is possible some participants may have higher SNR recordings in both tasks, which may result in both higher (estimated) prediction tendency and higher (estimated) speech tracking. This would result in a positive correlation, as the authors observe. This trivial explanation should be ruled out, by quantifying the relative SNR and testing for the absence of a mediation here.

      We agree that for both approaches (MVPA and mTRF models) individual MEG SNR plays an important role. This concern has been raised previously and addressed in our previous manuscript (Schubert et al., 2023). First, it should be noted that our prediction tendency value is the result of a condition contrast (rather than simple decoding accuracy) which compensates for the influence of subject specific signal-to-noise ratio (as no vacuous difference in SNR is to be expected between conditions). Second, in our previous study we also used frequency decoding accuracy as a control variable to correlate with speech tracking variables of interest and found no significant effect.

      (8) Much of the analysis pipeline features temporal response functions (TRFs). These should be shown in a time-resolved manner as a key intermediate step.

      We now included the Neural Speech tracking TRFs into the Figure (now Figure 3).

      (9) Figure 2 shows much-condensed results from different steps in the pipeline. If I understand correctly, 2A shows raw TRF weights (averaged over some time window?), while 2B-F shows standardized mean posterior regressor weights after Bayesian stats? It would be very helpful to make much more explicit what is being shown here, in addition to showing the related TRFs.

      Thank you for pointing this out! The figure description so far has been indeed not very insightful on this issue. We now adapted the caption and hope this clarifies the confusion: “ Neural speech tracking is related to prediction tendency and word surprisal, independent of selective attention. A) Envelope (x) - response (y) relationships are estimated using deconvolution (Boosting). The TRF (filter kernel, h) models how the brain processes the envelope over time. This filter is used to predict neural responses via convolution. Predicted responses are correlated with  actual neural activity to evaluate model fit and the TRF's ability to capture response dynamics. Correlation coefficients from these models are then used as dependent variables in Bayesian regression models. (Panel adapted from Gehmacher et al., 2024b). B) Temporal response functions (TRFs) depict the time-resolved neural tracking of the speech envelope for the single speaker and multi speaker target condition, shown here as absolute values averaged across channels. Solid lines represent the group average. Shaded areas represent 95% Confidence Intervals. C–H) The beta weights shown in the sensor plots are derived from Bayesian regression models in A). For Panel C, this statistical model is based on correlation coefficients computed from the TRF models (further details can be found in the Methods Section). C) In a single speaker condition, neural tracking of the speech envelope was significant for widespread areas, most pronounced over auditory processing regions. D) The condition effect indicates a decrease in neural speech tracking with increasing noise (1 distractor). E) Stronger prediction tendency was associated with increased neural speech tracking over left frontal areas. F) However, there was no interaction between prediction tendency and conditions of selective attention. G) Increased neural tracking of semantic violations was observed over left temporal areas. H) There was no interaction between word surprisal and speaker condition, suggesting a representation of surprising words independent of background noise. Marked sensors indicate ‘significant’ clusters, defined as at least two neighboring channels showing a significant result. N = 29.”

      Gehmacher, Q., Schubert, J., Kaltenmaier, A., Weisz, N., & Press, C. (2024b). The "Ocular Response Function" for encoding and decoding oculomotor related neural activity. bioRxiv, 2024-11.

      (10) Bayesian hypothesis testing is not done consistently. Some parts test for inclusion of 0 in 94% HDI, while some parts adopt a ROPE approach. The same approach should be taken throughout. Additionally, Bayes factors would be very helpful (I appreciate these depend on the choice of priors, but the default Bambi priors should be fine).

      Our primary aim in this study was to replicate two recent findings: (1) the relationship between individual prediction tendencies and neural speech tracking, and (2) the tracking of the speech envelope by eye movements. To maintain methodological consistency with the original studies, we did not apply a ROPE approach when analyzing these replication effects. Instead, we followed the same procedures as the original work, focusing on the inclusion of 0 in the HDI for the neural effects and using the same methods for the ocular effects. Additionally, we were not specifically interested in potential null effects in these replication analyses, as our primary goal was to test whether we could reproduce the previously reported findings.

      For the mediation analysis, however, we chose to extend the original approach by not only performing the analysis in a time-resolved manner but also applying a ROPE approach. This decision was motivated by our interest in gaining more comprehensive insights — beyond the replication goals — by also testing for potential null effects, which can provide valuable information about the presence or absence of mediation effects.

      We appreciate your thoughtful feedback and hope this clarifies our rationale for the differing approaches in our Bayesian hypothesis testing. 

      Regarding Bayes Factors, 

      We understand that some researchers find Bayes Factors appealing, as they offer a seemingly simple and straightforward way to evaluate the evidence in favor of/ or against H0 in relation to H1 (e.g. BF10 > 102 =  Decisive; according to the Jeffreys Scale). However, in practice Bayes Factors are often misunderstood e.g. by interpreting Bayes Factor as posterior odds or not acknowledging the notion of relative evidence in the Bayes Factor (see Wong et al. 2022). Instead of using Bayes Factors, we prefer to rely on estimating and reporting the posterior distribution of parameters given the data, prior and model assumptions (in form of the 94% HDI). This allows for a continuous evaluation of evidence for a given hypothesis that is in our eyes easier to interpret as a Bayes Factor.

      Jeffreys, Harold (1998) [1961]. The Theory of Probability (3rd ed.). Oxford, England. p. 432. ISBN 9780191589676.

      Wong, T. K., Kiers, H., & Tendeiro, J. (2022). On the Potential Mismatch Between the Function of the Bayes Factor and Researchers’ Expectations. Collabra: Psychology, 8(1), 36357. https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.36357

      (11) It would be helpful if Results could be appreciated without a detailed read of Methods. I would recommend a recap of each key methodological step before introducing the relevant Result. (This may also help in making the rationale explicit.)

      In addition to the short recaps of methods that were already present, and information on quantifications of neural and ocular tracking and bayes statistics (see responses 1, 2, 9), we now added the following parts below to the results sections. Please refer to them in the context of the manuscript where they should now complement a key recap of methodological steps necessary to readily understand each analysis and rational that led to the results:

      Individual prediction tendency is related to neural speech tracking:

      “Thus, this measure is a single value per subject, which comprises a) differences between two contextual probabilities (i.e. ordered vs. random) in b) feature-specific tone representations c) in advance of their observation (summed over a time-window of -0.3 - 0 s). Importantly, this prediction tendency was assessed in an independent entropy modulation paradigm (see Fig. 1). On a group level we found an increased tendency to pre-activate a stimulus of high probability (i.e. forward transition) in an ordered context compared to a random context (see Fig, 2A). This effect replicates results from our previous work (Schubert et al., 2023, 2024). Using the summed difference between entropy levels (ordered - random) across pre-stimulus time, one value was extracted per subject (Fig. 2B). This value was used as a proxy for “individual prediction tendency” and correlated with encoding of clear speech across different MEG sensors. [...]

      Neural speech tracking, quantified as the correlation coefficients between predicted and observed MEG responses to the speech envelope, was used as the dependent variable in Bayesian regression models. These models included condition (single vs. multi-speaker) as a fixed effect, with either prediction tendency or word surprisal as an additional predictor, and random effects for participants.”

      Eye movements track acoustic speech in selective attention:

      “For this, we separately predicted horizontal and vertical eye movements from the acoustic speech envelope using temporal response functions (TRFs). The resulting model fit (i.e. correlation between true and predicted eye movements) is commonly referred to as “speech tracking”. Bayesian regression models were applied to evaluate tracking effects under different conditions of selective attention (single speaker, attended multi-speaker, unattended multi-speaker). Furthermore, we assessed whether individual prediction tendency or semantic word surprisal influenced ocular speech tracking.”

      Neural speech tracking is mediated by eye movements:

      “This model evaluates to what extent gaze behaviour functions as a mediator between acoustic speech input and brain activity.”

      Neural and ocular speech tracking are differently related to comprehension: “Bayesian regression models were used to investigate relationships between neural/ocular speech tracking and comprehension or difficulty. Ocular speech tracking was analyzed separately for horizontal and vertical eye movements.”

      (12) The research questions in the Introduction should be sharpened up, to make explicit when a question concerns a theoretical entity, and when it concerns something concretely measured/measurable.

      We sharpened them up:

      “Taking into account the aforementioned study by Schubert and colleagues (2023), the two recently uncovered predictors of neural tracking (individual prediction tendency and ocular tracking) raise several empirical questions regarding the relationship between predictive processes, selective attention, and active ocular sensing in speech processing:

      (1) Are predictive processes related to active ocular sensing in the same way they are to neural speech tracking? Specifically, do individuals with a stronger tendency to anticipate predictable auditory features, as quantified through prestimulus neural representations in an independent tone paradigm, show increased or even decreased ocular speech tracking, measured as the correlation between predicted and actual eye movements? Or is there no relationship at all?

      (2) To what extent does selective attention influence the relationship between prediction tendency, neural speech tracking, and ocular speech tracking? For example, does the effect of prediction tendency or ocular speech tracking on neural tracking differ between a single-speaker and multi-speaker listening condition?

      (3) Are individual prediction tendency and ocular speech tracking related to behavioral outcomes, such as comprehension and perceived task difficulty? Speech comprehension is assessed through accuracy on comprehension questions, and task difficulty is measured through subjective ratings.

      Although predictive processes, selective attention, and active sensing have been shown to contribute to successful listening, their potential interactions and specific roles in naturalistic speech perception remain unclear. Addressing these questions will help disentangle their contributions and establish an integrated framework for understanding how neural and ocular speech tracking support speech processing.”

      (13) The negative relationship between story comprehension and ocular speech tracking appears to go against the authors' preferred interpretation, but the reflection on this in the Discussion is very brief and somewhat vague.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have taken your comments into careful consideration and also incorporated Reviewer #1's query (Minor point 2) into a unified and complementary reasoning. We have rewritten the relevant paragraph in the discussion to provide a clearer and more detailed explanation. We hope this revision offers a more precise and less vague discussion on this important point.

      “Despite the finding that eye movements mediate neural speech tracking, the behavioural relevance for semantic comprehension appears to differ between ocular and neural speech tracking. Specifically, we found a negative association between ocular speech tracking and comprehension, indicating that participants with lower comprehension performance exhibited increased ocular speech tracking. Interestingly, no significant relationship was observed between neural tracking and comprehension.

      In this context, the negative association between ocular tracking and comprehension might reflect individual differences in how participants allocate cognitive resources. Participants with lower comprehension may rely more heavily on attentional mechanisms to process acoustic features, as evidenced by increased ocular tracking. This reliance could represent a compensatory strategy when higher-order processes, such as semantic integration or memory retrieval, are less effective. Importantly, our comprehension questions (see Experimental Procedure) targeted a broad range of processes, including intelligibility and memory, suggesting that this relationship reflects a trade-off in resource allocation between low-level acoustic focus and integrative cognitive tasks.

      Rather than separating eye and brain responses conceptually, our analysis highlights their complementary contributions. Eye movements may enhance neural processing by increasing sensitivity to acoustic properties of speech, while neural activity builds on this foundation to integrate information and support comprehension. Together, these systems form an interdependent mechanism, with eye and brain responses working in tandem to facilitate different aspects of speech processing.

      This interpretation is consistent with the absence of a difference in ocular tracking for semantic violations (e.g., words with high surprisal versus lexically matched controls), reinforcing the view that ocular tracking primarily reflects attentional engagement with acoustic features rather than direct involvement in semantic processing. This aligns with previous findings that attention modulates auditory responses to acoustic features (e.g., Forte et al., 2017), further supporting the idea that ocular tracking reflects mechanisms of selective attention rather than representations of linguistic content.

      Future research should investigate how these systems interact and explore how ocular tracking mediates neural responses to linguistic features, such as lexical or semantic processing, to better understand their joint contributions to comprehension.”.  

      (14) Page numbers would be helpful.

      We added the page numbers.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Results

      (1) Figure 2 - statistical results are reported in this figure, but they are not fully explained in the text, nor are statistical values provided for any of the analyses (as far as I can tell).

      Also, how were multiple comparisons dealt with (the choice of two neighboring channels seems quite arbitrary)? Perhaps for this reason, the main result - namely the effect of "prediction tendency" and "semantic violations" - is quite sparse and might not survive more a rigorous statistical criterion. I would feel more comfortable with these results if the reporting of the statistical analysis had been more thorough (ideally, including comparison to control models).

      We would like to thank you again for your detailed queries, comments, and questions on our work. We first of all adapted this figure (now Figure 3 in the manuscript, please see responses 8 and 9 to Reviewer #2) to help readers understand the metrics and values within each statistical analysis. In addition, we indeed did not include the detailed statistics in the text! We now added the missing statistic reports calculated as averages over ‘clusters’:

      “Replicating previous findings (Schubert et al., 2023), we found widespread encoding of clear speech (average over cluster: β = 0.035, 94%HDI = [0.024, 0.046]), predominantly over auditory processing regions (Fig. 3C), that was decreased (β = -0.018, 94%HDI = [0.029, -0.006]) in a multi-speaker condition (Fig. 3D). Furthermore, a stronger prediction tendency was associated with increased neural speech tracking (β = 0.014, 94%HDI = [0.004, 0.025]) over left frontal sensors (see Fig. 3E). We found no interaction between prediction tendency and condition (see Fig. 3F).” [...] “In a direct comparison with lexically identical controls, we found an increased neural tracking of semantic violations (β = 0.039, 94%HDI = [0.007, 0.071]) over left temporal areas (see Fig. 3G). Furthermore, we found no interaction between word surprisal and speaker condition (see Fig. 3H).”

      Regarding the "prediction tendency" effect, it is important to note that this finding replicates a result from Schubert et al. (2023). The left frontal location of this effect is also consistent over studies, which convinces us of the robustness of the finding. Furthermore, testing this relationship properly requires a mixed-effects model in order to account for the variability across subjects that is not explained by fixed effects and the repeated measures design. For this reason a random Intercept had to be fitted for each subject (1|subject in the respective model formula). This statistical requirement motivated our decision to use bayesian statistics as (at least to our knowledge) there is no implementation of a cluster-based permutation mixed effects model (yet). In order to provide a more conservative criterion (as bayesian statistics don’t require a multiple comparison correction) we chose to impose in addition the requirement of a “clustered” effect.

      The choice of using two neighboring channels is consistent with the default parameter settings in FieldTrip’s cluster-based permutation testing (cfg.minnbchan = 2). This parameter specifies the minimum number of neighboring channels required for a sample to be included in the clustering algorithm, ensuring spatial consistency in the identified clusters. This alignment ensures that our methodology is comparable to numerous prior studies in the field, where such thresholds are standard. While it is true that all statistical analyses involve some degree of arbitrariness in parameter selection (e.g., alpha levels or clustering thresholds), our approach reflects established conventions and ensures comparability with previous findings.

      While the original study utilized source space analyses, we replicated this effect using only 102 magnetometers. This choice was made for computational simplicity, demonstrating that the effect is robust even without source-level modeling. Similarly, the "semantic violation" effect, while perceived as sparse, is based solely on magnetometer data and - in our opinion - should not be viewed as overly sparse given the methods employed. This effect aligns with the two-neighbor clustering approach, ensuring spatial consistency across magnetometers. The results reflect the robustness of the effects within the constraints of magnetometer-level analyses.

      Overall, the methodological choices, including the choice of a bayesian linear mixed effects model, the use of two neighboring channels and the reliance on magnetometers, are grounded in established practices and methodological considerations. While stricter thresholds or alternative approaches might yield different results, our methods align with best practices in the field and ensure the robustness, comparability, and replicability of our findings.

      (2) Figure 3 - the difference between horizontal and vertical eye-movements. This result is quite confusing and although the authors do suggest a possible interpretation for this in the discussion, I do wonder how robust this difference is or whether the ocular signal (in either direction) is simply too noisy or the effect too small to be detected consistently across conditions. Also, the ocular-TRFs themselves are not entirely convincing in suggesting reliable response/tracking of the audio - despite the small-but-significant increase in prediction accuracy.

      The horizontal versus vertical comparison was conducted to explore potential differences in how these dimensions contribute to ocular tracking of auditory stimuli (please also see our response to Reviewer #1, Response 5b, that includes the vertical vs. horizontal effects of Gehmacher at al. 2024). It would indeed be interesting to develop a measure that combines the two directions into a more natural representation of 'viewing,' such as a combined vector. However, this approach would require the use of complex numbers to represent both magnitude and direction simultaneously, hence the development of novel TRF algorithms capable of modeling this multidimensional signal. While beyond the scope of the current study, this presents an exciting avenue for future research and would allow us to move closer to understanding ocular speech tracking and the robustness of these effects, above and beyond the already successful replication.

      It is also important to emphasize that ocular-TRFs are derived from (viewing) behavioral data rather than neural signals, and are thus inherently subject to greater variability across participants and time. This higher variability does not necessarily indicate a small or unreliable effect but reflects the dynamic and task-dependent nature of eye movement behavior. The TRFs with shaded error margins represent this variability, highlighting how eye movements are influenced by both individual differences and moment-to-moment changes in task engagement.

      Despite this inherent variability, the significant prediction accuracy improvements confirm that ocular-TRFs reliably capture meaningful relationships between eye movements and auditory stimuli. The observed differences between horizontal and vertical TRFs further support the hypothesis that these dimensions are differentially involved in the task, possibly driven by the specific roles they play in sensorimotor coupling.

      (3) Figure 4 - this figure shows source distribution of 3 PCA components, derived from the results of the mediation effect of eye movements on the speech-tracking. Here too I am having difficulty in interpreting what the results actually are. For one, all three components are quite widespread and somewhat overlapping, so although they are statistically "independent" it is hard to learn much from them about the brain regions involved and whether they truly represent separable contributions. Similarly difficult to interpret are the time courses, which share some similarities with the known TRFs to speech (especially PC3). I would have expected to find a cleaner "auditory" response, and clearer separation between sensory regions and regions involved in the control of eye movements. I also wonder why the authors chose not to show the sourcelocalization of the neural and ocular speech-tracking responses alone - this could have helped us between understand what "mediation" of the neural response might look like.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s interest in better understanding the source distribution and time courses of the PCA components. While we acknowledge that the widespread and overlapping nature of the components may make a more fine grained interpretation challenging, it is important to emphasize that our analysis simply reflects the data, hence we can only present and interpret what the analysis revealed.

      Regarding your suggestion to show the source localization of ocular speech tracking and neural speech tracking alone, we would like to point out that ocular tracking is represented by only one channel for vertical and one channel for horizontal eye movements. Thus, in this case the estimated source of the effect are the eyes themselves. We believe that the source localization of neural speech tracking has been a thoroughly studied topic in research so far (locating it to perisylvian, auditory areas with a stronger preference for the left hemisphere) and can also be seen in Schubert et al., (2023). Nevertheless, we believe the observed PCA components still provide valuable, and most importantly novel insights into the interplay between eye movements and neural responses in speech tracking.  

      Discussion/interpretation

      (1) Although I appreciate the authors' attempt to propose a "unified" theoretical model linking predictions about low-level features to higher features, and the potential involvement of eye movements in 'active sensing' I honestly think that this model is overambitious, given the data presented in the current study. Moreover, there is very little discussion of past literature and existing models of active sensing and hierarchical processing of speech, that could have helped ground the discussion in a broader theoretical context. The entire discussion contains fewer than 20 citations (some of which are by these authors) and needs to be substantially enriched in order to provide context for the authors' claims.

      Thank you very much for your thoughtful feedback and for appreciating our approach. We hope that the revised manuscript addresses your concerns. Specifically, we now emphasize that our proposal is a conceptual framework, with the main goal to operationale “prediction tendency”, “active ocular sensing”, and “selective attention” and to “organise these entities according to their assumed function for speech processing and to describe their relationship with each other.” We did this by thoroughly revising our discussion section with a clear emphasis on the definition of terms, for example: 

      “With this speculative framework we attempt to describe and relate three important phenomena with respect to their relevance for speech processing: 1) “Anticipatory predictions” that are created in absence of attentional demands and contain probabilistic information about stimulus features (here, inferred from frequency-specific pre-activations during passive listening to sound sequences). 2) “Selective attention” that allocates resources towards relevant (whilst suppressing distracting) information (which was manipulated by the presence or absence of a distractor speaker). And finally 3) “active ocular sensing”, which refers to gaze behavior that is temporally aligned to attended (but not unattended) acoustic speech input (inferred from the discovered phenomenon of ocular speech tracking).”

      Our theoretical proposals are now followed by a recap of our results that support the respective idea, for example: 

      “...these predictions are formed in parallel and carry high feature-specificity but low temporal precision (as they are anticipatory in nature). This idea is supported by our finding that pure-tone anticipation is visible over a widespread prestimulus interval, instead of being locked to sound onset”

      “....we suggest that active (ocular) sensing does not necessarily convey feature- or content-specific information, it is merely used to boost (and conversely filter) sensory input at specific timescales (similar to neural oscillations). This assumption is supported by our finding that semantic violations are not differentially encoded in gaze behaviour than lexical controls.”

      And we put a strong focus on highlighting the boundaries of these ideas, in order to avoid theoretical confusion, misunderstandings or implicit theoretical assumption that are not grounded in data, in particular: 

      “In fact, when rejecting the notion of a single bottom-up flow of information and replacing it with a model of distributed parallel and dynamic processing, it seems only reasonable to assume that the direction of communication (between our eyes and our brain) will depend on where (within the brain) as well as when we look at the effect. Thus, the regions and time-windows reported here should be taken as an illustration of oculo-neural communication during speech processing rather than an attempt to "explain" neural speech processing by ocular movements.”

      “Even though the terminology [“hierarchy”] is suggestive of a fixed sequence (similar to a multi storey building) with levels that must be traversed one after each other (and even the more spurious idea of a rooftop, where the final perceptual experience is formed and stored into memory), we distance ourselves from these (possibly unwarranted) ideas. Our usage of “higher” or “lower” simply refers to the observation that the probability of a feature at a higher (as in more associative) level affects the interpretation (and thus the representation and prediction) of a feature at lower (as in more segregated) levels (Caucheteux et al., 2023).”

      Additionally, we have made substantial efforts to present complementary results (see response to Reviewer #2, point 8) to further substantiate our interpretation. Importantly, we have updated the illustration of the model (see response to Reviewer #, minor point 1) and refined both our interpretations and the conceptual language in the Discussion. Furthermore, we have included additional citations where appropriate to strengthen our argument.

      We would also like to briefly note that this section of the Discussion aimed to highlight existing literature that bridges the gap our model seeks to address. However, as this is a relatively underexplored area, the references available are necessarily limited.

      (2) Given my many reservations about the data, as presented in the current version of the manuscript, I find much of the discussion to be an over-interpretation of the results. This might change if the authors are able to present more robust results, as per some of my earlier comments.

      We sincerely hope that our comprehensive revisions have addressed your concerns and improved the manuscript to your satisfaction.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Recommendations For The Authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      Recommendations  Analysis: 

      (1) Given that a MER21B/C LTR was not immediately identified at the start site of the Liz lncRNA in the mouse, and its match is only 46%, this raises the question of whether an analogous LTR would be identified at the homologous location in other species on deeper analysis. The authors need to argue that what has been conserved in the LTR alone in mouse is the essential element conferring the ability to initiate transcription of Liz. A transient reporter assay might be sufficient to do this. 

      We believe that the 46% identity between the first exon of mouse Liz and the consensus sequence of MER21C is so weak that its traces as MER21C are too attenuated to be detected by standard in silico analyses, such as homology searches. For instance, when pairwise alignments are performed between the first exon of mouse Liz and the consensus sequences of solo-LTRs other than MER21C, MER21C does not emerge as the most similar sequence (Figure 5 – figure supplement 1). This is in stark contrast to similar analyses involving the first exon of human and rabbit GPR1AS (which overlaps with MER21C), where MER21C is identified as the most similar sequence. [pages: 26, 31-32]

      The positions of these LTRs were initially annotated using RepeatMasker. To ensure robust analysis, we performed additional searches with RepeatMasker under more sensitive conditions, adjusting search engines (e.g., RMblast to HMMER or Cross-match) and sensitivity settings. Nevertheless, MER21C or closely related LTRs were still undetectable in mouse, rat, and hamster (Figure 4 – figure supplement 1). However, a multiple genome alignment generated by Cactus/UCSC revealed a syntenic region corresponding to the first exon of human GPR1-AS, overlapping with LTR21C, in the genomes of mice, as well as rats and hamsters (Figure 4 – figure supplement 2). Although RepeatMasker did not annotate MER21C at the GPR1 locus in these species, homologous regions were observed across all selected Euarchontoglires. Due to the limitations of the Cactus alignment track in delineating precise homologous boundaries across species, extracting sequences for evolutionary tree construction was not feasible. Nevertheless, these findings support the hypothesis that the first exon of GPR1-AS (Liz in mice) originated from a MER21C insertion in the common ancestor of Euarchontoglires. [pages: 21, 24-25]

      A combination of traditional annotation of repetitive elements using RepeatMasker and the reconstruction of ancestral genomes through multiple genome alignment can reveal highly degenerated LTR relics. This approach is likely to point to significant future directions for research. This point is further elaborated in the discussion section. [page 42]

      Furthermore, in response to the reviewer's suggestion, we investigated the promoter activity of the GPR1-AS and Liz first exons, which are hypothesized to have originated from the same MER21C insertion. Using a dual reporter assay, we demonstrated that the first exon of mouse Liz exhibits promoter activity in a human cell line comparable to that of the human GPR1-AS promoter. Thus, despite the relatively low sequence similarity between the Liz first exon and the MER21C consensus sequence (46% as determined by pairwise alignment, Figure 5 – figure supplement 2), the promoter activity remains functionally conserved. We further discuss the potential functional motifs within the putative MER21C LTR-derived sequences in Figure 4B-D. Taken together, these findings suggest that despite a high level of degeneracy of the promoter region in rodents, including mice, the most parsimonious explanation for the origin of this regulatory element in rodents is the presence of the same LTR relic detectable in humans/primates, which is essential for robust transcription initiation of Liz and GPR1-AS, respectively. [pages: 27, 32]

      (2) Imprinting will depend on an initiating mechanism in the germline, in addition to events in the embryo that induce the secondary DMR at ZDBF2. The authors should therefore examine as far as possible the presence of a gDMR in the species with/without GPR1-AS1 and ZDBF2 imprinting. Whole-genome bisulphite sequencing data from oocytes and sperm should exist for some of the relevant species (e.g., pig, cow: Ivanova et al. 2020 PMID: 32393379; Lu et al. 2012 PMID: 34818044). 

      As the reviewer noted, the presence of a gDMR is essential for the establishment of imprinting. Following another reviewer's suggestion, we have now demonstrated that the ZDBF2 gene in rhesus monkeys is also subject to imprinting (see Figure 3C-D). We also acquired whole genome bisulfite sequencing data for rhesus monkey sperm and oocytes, identified DMRs between them, and discovered an oocyte-specifically methylated gDMR in the first exon of GPR1-AS (which overlaps with MER21C)(Figure 3 – figure supplement 1A). This finding is consistent with observations in humans and mice. Conversely, we obtained similar sequencing data for porcine and bovine sperm and oocytes and conducted the same analysis (Figure 3 – figure supplement 1A,B). However, we did not detect any oocyte-specific methylated gDMRs in the GPR1 intragenic region (where GPR1AS is transcribed from an intron of GPR1) in these species of the Laurasiatheria superorder. These results support the hypothesis that ZDBF2 is not imprinted in lineages outside the Euarchontoglires, the superorder which includes both rodents and primates. We have included these important DMR results as a supplement to Figure 3. [pages 16-21]

      Presentation: 

      (1) The first section of the Introduction would benefit from the inclusion of some additional general references on genomic imprinting. 

      We have added two review articles, Tucci et al. (2019) and Kobayashi (2021), as references in the first section of the Introduction. [page 5]

      (2) Introduction statement: "....nearly 200 imprinted genes have been identified in mice and humans. However, less than half of these genes overlapped in both species." This was the conclusion of one study (Tucci et al. 2016), so it would be better to provide a caveat to the statement "However, one comparative analysis suggested that fewer than half of these genes overlapped in both species". 

      The point being that the actual number of imprinted genes is still a matter of debate (see Edwards et al. 2023 PMID: 36916665), and the extent of overlap will depend on the strength of the evidence for each gene in the human and mouse imprinted gene lists. So, it is very difficult to put an accurate figure on the extent of overlap - but the authors' point is valid that there are species- or lineage-specific imprinted genes. 

      We have revised this sentence following reviewer #1's suggestion. [page 5]

      (3) Introduction statement: "The establishment of species-specific imprinting.....can be driven by various evolutionary events, including.....differences in the function of DNA methyltransferases". I am not aware that this has been described as an evolutionary event causing species-specific imprinting - without supporting evidence, I recommend to remove this suggestion. 

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and realize that we should have been more explicit here. We were referring to DNMT3C, a rodent-specific member of the DNMT3 family, which is responsible for the paternal methylation imprinting of Rasgrf1 in mice (Barau et al., Science, 2016), in association with the piRNA pathway and targeting of a specific retrotransposon within the DMR (Watanabe et al. Science, 2011). The Rasgrf1 gene is imprinted in mice but not considered imprinted in humans (though some conflicting data exist). While it is likely that the emergence of DNMT3C was a pre-requisite to the establishment of Rasgrf1 imprinting in evolutionary terms, clear evidence is lacking. Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have removed the phrase "differences in the function of DNA methyltransferases" from the text. However, we have reintroduced this point in the Introduction section as a potential mechanism that may contribute to the establishment of species-specific imprinted genes, alongside the roles of ZNF445 and ZFP57, which regulate the maintenance of imprinting with partially divided roles between humans and mice. [page 6]

      (4) It would be very useful for readers to have a schema of the Gpr1/Zdbf2 locus that indicates the locations of the germline and somatic DMRs and their relationship to the Liz transcript. 

      (5) There is a summary figure amongst the Supplementary Figures (Suppl. Fig. 7) - it would be beneficial to readers to have this summary figure in the main text rather than the supplement. 

      Following reviewer #1’s suggestion, we have moved the regulatory system schema at the Gpr1/Zdbf2 locus, originally shown in Supplementary Figure 7, to the main text as Figure 7. In addition, in response to comment 4, we have revised the figure to explicitly depict the relationship between the Liz transcript and the establishment of the somatic DMR (sDMR), enhancing the clarity of the regulatory interactions at this locus. [page 38]

      (6) With a focus of the study on LTRs as cis-regulatory elements having been co-opted in genomic imprinting mechanisms - whether in the female germline (as in Bogutz et al. 2019) or in the current study as an activating element post-fertilisation - it is a real omission that the authors do not to refer to the role of tissue-specific LTRs as the candidate regulatory elements in non-canonical imprinting (see Hanna et al. 2019 PMID: 31665063). Please include in Introduction and/or Discussion. 

      We added a sentence explaining canonical and non-canonical imprinting and the cases where LTRs act as regulatory elements in non-canonical imprinting, with reference to the study of Hanna et al., as suggested. [page 6]

      (7) Discussion statement: "Two paternally expressed imprinted genes, PEG10/SIRH1 and PEG11/RTL1/SIRH2 have been identified in mammals. They encode GAG-POL proteins of sushi-ichi LTR retrotransposons and are essential for mammalian placenta formation and maintenance." 

      These sentences should be combined: "Two paternally expressed imprinted genes, PEG10/SIRH1, and PEG11/RTL1/SIRH2, that encode GAG-POL proteins of sushi-ichi LTR retrotransposons have been identified in mammals and are essential for mammalian placenta formation and maintenance." 

      We have revised this sentence according to reviewer #1's suggestion. [page 41]

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      When showing assembled GPR1-AS transcripts via genome browser tracks, it would be valuable to add normalized counts of reads mapping to each strand, in order to more convincingly demonstrate the existence of these transcripts. I ask for this because in my experience Stringtie will assemble transcripts that are only marginally supported by reads. 

      In response to Reviewer #2's suggestion, FPKM and TPM values for all StringTiepredicted GPR1-AS-like transcripts are now included in Figure 6. Each of these transcripts has a TPM value greater than 1, supporting their validity. [pages: 35]

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      (1) The tree in Figure 5A is one of the main arguments supporting the divergence of the mouse Liz promoter from a common MER21C element, but this contains only a handful of species, making it difficult to appreciate the full extent of its evolution. Presumably its faster mutation rate in mouse would also be supported by other closely related rodents, which would solidify the conclusion that the Liz promoter is derived from an ancient MER21C insertion. So my suggestion is to expand this tree substantially to other species, comparing sequences syntenic to the GPR1-AS/Liz promoter. 

      (2) It may also be worth trying different TE/LTR annotation tools and/or running Repeatmasker with different parameters, to see if an MER21C element is detected in mouse using a more sensitive approach. 

      In response to this suggestion, we performed computational analyses with RepeatMasker under various settings (e.g., switching search engines from RMblast to HMMER or Crossmatch, adjusting speed/sensitivity settings from default to slow). Despite these modifications, a MER21C element was not detected near the mouse Liz promoter. However, a multiple genome alignment track generated by Cactus/UCSC revealed a syntenic region, corresponding to the first exon of human GPR1-AS, which overlaps with LTR21C, also present in the genomes of mouse, rat, and hamster (Figure 4 – figure supplement 1). While RepeatMasker did not identify MER21C at the GPR1 locus in these species, homologous regions were observed across all selected Euarchontoglires. Although the Cactus alignment track does not delineate the exact boundaries of homologous regions across species (relative to humans) and thus precludes extracting each homologous sequence to construct an evolutionary tree, these findings support the hypothesis that the first exon of GPR1-AS (referred to as Liz in mice) originated from an ancient MER21C insertion in the common ancestor of Euarchontoglires. [pages: 21, 24-25]

      (3) According to Dfam, MER21C is not common to all eutherians, but specific to Boroeutheria, whilst MER21B is presumably specific to Euarchontoglires. To clarify MER21C/B evolution, it would be useful to show the number of elements present in select species from each group (including an outgroup). 

      (7) In Figure 4 it is hard to distinguish between red and purple. 

      Initially, we referenced Repbase (e.g., MER21C: Origin/Eutheria), but, as Reviewer #3 noted, Dfam should be the primary reference. We have now included the copy numbers of MER21C and MER21B for each genome in Figure 4, providing a clearer understanding of their evolutionary appearance (MER21C appears specific to Boroeutheria, while MER21B is specific to Euarchontoglires). Additionally, we adjusted the MER21B position color from purple to dark purple to improve visibility. Furthermore, we have also underlined the copy number of MER21C or MER21B located within the GPR1 region in each species. For example, in the Treeshrew genome, the LTR overlapping with GPR1-AS is annotated as MER21B, so we underlined the copy number of MER21B (2,305). These changes now clearly indicate whether homologous sequences to the first exon of GPR1-AS are annotated as MER21C or MER21B in each genome. [page 22]

      (4) Could the imprinting status of ZDBF2 not be determined in chimpanzees and rabbits? Or is it already known? Either way, a clarification would be useful to further support the concordance between GPR1-AS-like transcripts and ZDBF2 imprinting.

      The imprinting status of ZDBF2 had not previously been reported in chimpanzees, rhesus macaques, or rabbits, where GPR1-AS-like transcripts were identified. Therefore, we conducted allele-specific expression analysis of ZDBF2 using blood samples from rhesus macaques and rabbits. As expected, paternal-allele-specific expression of ZDBF2 was observed in both species, consistent with findings in humans and mice. These results have been added to Figure 3. Although we did not analyze the imprinting status in chimpanzees, we believe the existing data sufficiently support our hypothesis. [pages: 16, 19-20]

      (5) The authors briefly discuss the role of KRAB-ZFPs in controlling TE expression. An interesting addition would be to analyse the expression of the main KRAB-ZFP that binds to MER21C (ZFP789, according to data from PMID 28273063). This could be linked to the temporal control of MER21C expression. 

      In response to Reviewer #3's suggestion, we focused on the expression pattern of ZNF789 (noted by the reviewer as ZFP789), the primary KRAB-ZFP known to bind MER21C, as identified by Didier Trono’s group (PMID 28273063). Strikingly, our analysis reveals that ZNF789 is specifically downregulated at the 4-cell stage, which aligns with the timing of MER21C reactivation. While it remains to be determined whether this downregulation directly influences MER21C reactivation or the initiation of GPR1-AS expression, this finding is significant and consistent with our model. We have incorporated this information in Figure 5 – figure supplement 3. [pages: 33]

      (6) The sentence "Liz directs DNA methylation at the somatic DMR, which competes with ZDBF2 to repress the paternal allele" (introduction) was confusing to me. 

      This sentence has been revised to be more accurate as follows; Liz transcription counteracts the H3K27me3-mediated repression of Zdbf2 by promoting the deposition of antagonistic DNA methylation at the secondary DMR. [page 7]

      (8) In Figure 5 I take it that 'consensus motif' refers to ELF1/2? Maybe change the legend. 

      To clarify potential confusion around the term 'consensus motif,' which may have been mistaken for 'consensus MER21C' (the consensus sequence of MER21C-LTR from the Dfam database), we have revised the figure legend. We now refer to the motif as the "common motif," indicating the sequence common to all MER21C-derived sequences and overlapping with the first exon of GPR1-AS. [page 29]

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      Glaser et al present ExA-SPIM, a light-sheet microscope platform with large volumetric coverage (Field of view 85mm^2, working distance 35mm), designed to image expanded mouse brains in their entirety. The authors also present an expansion method optimized for whole mouse brains and an acquisition software suite. The microscope is employed in imaging an expanded mouse brain, the macaque motor cortex, and human brain slices of white matter. 

      This is impressive work and represents a leap over existing light-sheet microscopes. As an example, it offers a fivefold higher resolution than mesoSPIM (https://mesospim.org/), a popular platform for imaging large cleared samples. Thus while this work is rooted in optical engineering, it manifests a huge step forward and has the potential to become an important tool in the neurosciences. 

      Strengths: 

      - ExA-SPIM features an exceptional combination of field of view, working distance, resolution, and throughput. 

      - An expanded mouse brain can be acquired with only 15 tiles, lowering the burden on computational stitching. That the brain does not need to be mechanically sectioned is also seen as an important capability. 

      - The image data is compelling, and tracing of neurons has been performed. This demonstrates the potential of the microscope platform. 

      Weaknesses: 

      - There is a general question about the scaling laws of lenses, and expansion microscopy, which in my opinion remained unanswered: In the context of whole brain imaging, a larger expansion factor requires a microscope system with larger volumetric coverage, which in turn will have lower resolution (Figure 1B). So what is optimal? Could one alternatively image a cleared (non-expanded) brain with a high-resolution ASLM system (Chakraborty, Tonmoy, Nature Methods 2019, potentially upgraded with custom objectives) and get a similar effective resolution as the authors get with expansion? This is not meant to diminish the achievement, but it was unclear if the gains in resolution from the expansion factor are traded off by the scaling laws of current optical systems. 

      Paraphrasing the reviewer: Expanding the tissue requires imaging larger volumes and allows lower optical resolution. What has been gained?

      The answer to the reviewer’s question is nuanced and contains four parts. 

      First, optical engineering requirements are more forgiving for lenses with lower resolution. Lower resolution lenses can have much larger fields of view (in real terms: the number of resolvable elements, proportional to ‘etendue’) and much longer working distances. In other words, it is currently more feasible to engineer lower resolution lenses with larger volumetric coverage, even when accounting for the expansion factor. 

      Second, these lenses are also much better corrected compared to higher resolution (NA) lenses. They have a flat field of view, negligible pincushion distortions, and constant resolution across the field of view. We are not aware of comparable performance for high NA objectives, even when correcting for expansion.

      Third, although clearing and expansion render tissues ‘transparent’, there still exist refractive index inhomogeneities which deteriorate image quality, especially at larger imaging depths. These effects are more severe for higher optical resolutions (NA), because the rays entering the objective at higher angles have longer paths in the tissue and will see more aberrations. For lower NA systems, such as ExaSPIM, the differences in paths between the extreme and axial rays are relatively small and image formation is less sensitive to aberrations. 

      Fourth, aberrations are proportional to the index of refraction inhomogeneities (dn/dx). Since the index of refraction is roughly proportional to density, scattering and aberration of light decreases as M^3, where M is the expansion factor. In contrast, the imaging path length through the tissue only increases as M. This produces a huge win for imaging larger samples with lower resolutions. 

      To our knowledge there are no convincing demonstrations in the literature of diffraction-limited ASLM imaging at a depth of 1 cm in cleared mouse brain tissue, which would be equivalent to the ExA-SPIM imaging results presented in this manuscript.  

      In the discussion of the revised manuscript we discuss these factors in more depth. 

      - It was unclear if 300 nm lateral and 800 nm axial resolution is enough for many questions in neuroscience. Segmenting spines, distinguishing pre- and postsynaptic densities, or tracing densely labeled neurons might be challenging. A discussion about the necessary resolution levels in neuroscience would be appreciated. 

      We have previously shown good results in tracing the thinnest (100 nm thick) axons over cm scales with 1.5 um axial resolution. It is the contrast (SNR) that matters, and the ExaSPIM contrast exceeds the block-face 2-photon contrast, not to mention imaging speed (> 10x).  

      Indeed, for some questions, like distinguishing fluorescence in pre- and postsynaptic structures, higher resolutions will be required (0.2 um isotropic; Rah et al Frontiers Neurosci, 2013). This could be achieved with higher expansion factors.

      This is not within the intended scope of the current manuscript. As mentioned in the discussion section, we are working towards ExA-SPIM-based concepts to achieve better resolution through the design and fabrication of a customized imaging lens that maintains a high volumetric coverage with increased numerical aperture.  

      - Would it be possible to characterize the aberrations that might be still present after whole brain expansion? One approach could be to image small fluorescent nanospheres behind the expanded brain and recover the pupil function via phase retrieval. But even full width half maximum (FWHM) measurements of the nanospheres' images would give some idea of the magnitude of the aberrations. 

      We now included a supplementary figure highlighting images of small axon segments within distal regions of the brain.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      In this manuscript, Glaser et al. describe a new selective plane illumination microscope designed to image a large field of view that is optimized for expanded and cleared tissue samples. For the most part, the microscope design follows a standard formula that is common among many systems (e.g. Keller PJ et al Science 2008, Pitrone PG et al. Nature Methods 2013, Dean KM et al. Biophys J 2015, and Voigt FF et al. Nature Methods 2019). The primary conceptual and technical novelty is to use a detection objective from the metrology industry that has a large field of view and a large area camera. The authors characterize the system resolution, field curvature, and chromatic focal shift by measuring fluorescent beads in a hydrogel and then show example images of expanded samples from mouse, macaque, and human brain tissue. 

      Strengths: 

      I commend the authors for making all of the documentation, models, and acquisition software openly accessible and believe that this will help assist others who would like to replicate the instrument. I anticipate that the protocols for imaging large expanded tissues (such as an entire mouse brain) will also be useful to the community. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The characterization of the instrument needs to be improved to validate the claims. If the manuscript claims that the instrument allows for robust automated neuronal tracing, then this should be included in the data. 

      The reviewer raises a valid concern. Our assertion that the resolution and contrast is sufficient for robust automated neuronal tracing is overstated based on the data in the paper. We are hard at work on automated tracing of datasets from the ExA-SPIM microscope. We have demonstrated full reconstruction of axonal arbors encompassing >20 cm of axonal length.  But including these methods and results is out of the scope of the current manuscript. 

      The claims of robust automated neuronal tracing have been appropriately modified.  

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      Smaller questions to the authors: 

      - Would a multi-directional illumination and detection architecture help? Was there a particular reason the authors did not go that route?

      Despite the clarity of the expanded tissue, and the lower numerical aperture of the ExA-SPIM microscope, image quality still degrades slightly towards the distal regions of the brain relative to both the excitation and detection objective. Therefore, multi-directional illumination and detection would be advantageous. Since the initial submission of the manuscript, we have undertaken re-designing the optics and mechanics of the system. This includes provisions for multi-directional illumination and detection. However, this new design is beyond the scope of this manuscript. We now mention this in L254-255 of the Discussion section.

      - Why did the authors not use the same objective for illumination and detection, which would allow isotropic resolution in ASLM? 

      The current implementation of ASLM requires an infinity corrected objective (i.e. conjugating the axial sweeping mechanism to the back focal plane). This is not possible due to the finite conjugate design of the ExA-SPIM detection lens.

      More fundamentally, pushing the excitation NA higher would result in a shorter light sheet Rayleigh length, which would require a smaller detection slit (shorter exposure time, lower signal to noise ratio). For our purposes an excitation NA of 0.1 is an excellent compromise between axial resolution, signal to noise ratio, and imaging speed. 

      For other potentially brighter biological structures, it may be possible to design a custom infinity corrected objective that enables ASLM with NA > 0.1.

      - Have the authors made any attempt to characterize distortions of the brain tissue that can occur due to expansion? 

      We have not systematically characterized the distortions of the brain tissue pre and post expansion. Imaged mouse brain volumes are registered to the Allen CCF regardless of whether or not the tissue was expanded. It is beyond the scope of this manuscript to include these results and processing methods, but we have confirmed that the ExA-SPIM mouse brain volumes contain only modest deformation that is easily accounted for during registration to the Allen CCF. 

      - The authors state that a custom lens with NA 0.5-0.6 lens can be designed, featuring similar specifications. Is there a practical design? Wouldn't such a lens be more prone to Field curvature? 

      This custom lens has already been designed and is currently being fabricated. The lens maintains a similar space bandwidth product as the current lens (increased numerical aperture but over a proportionally smaller field of view). Over the designed field of view, field curvature is <1 µm. However, including additional discussion or results of this customized lens is beyond the scope of this manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      System characterization: 

      - Please state what wavelength was used for the resolution measurements in Figure 2.

      An excitation wavelength of 561 nm was used. This has been added to the manuscript text.

      - The manuscript highlights that a key advance for the microscope is the ability to image over a very large 13 mm diameter field of view. Can the authors clarify why they chose to characterize resolution over an 8diameter mm field rather than the full area? 

      The 13 mm diameter field of view refers to the diagonal of the 10.6 x 8.0 mm field of view. The results presented in Figure 1c are with respect to the horizontal x direction and vertical y direction. A note indicating that the 13 mm is with respect to the diagonal of the rectangular imaging field has been added to the manuscript text. The results were presented in this way to present the axial and lateral resolution as a function of y (the axial sweeping direction).

      - The resolution estimates seem lower than I would expect for a 0.30 NA lens (which should be closer to ~850 nm for 515 nm emission). Could the authors clarify the discrepancy? Is this predicted by the Zemax model and due to using the lens in immersion media, related to sampling size on the camera, or something else? It would be helpful if the authors could overlay the expected diffraction-limited performance together with the plots in Figure 2C. 

      As mentioned previously, the resolution measurements were performed with 561 nm excitation and an emission bandpass of ~573 – 616 nm (595 nm average). Based on this we would expect the full width half maximum resolution to be ~975 nm. The resolution is in fact limited by sampling on the camera. The 3.76 µm pixel size, combined with the 5.0X magnification results in a sampling of 752 nm. Based on the Nyquist the resolution is limited to ~1.5 µm. We have added clarifying statements to the text.

      - I'm confused about the characterization of light sheet thickness and how it relates to the measured detection field curvature. The authors state that they "deliver a light sheet with NA = 0.10 which has a width of 12.5 mm (FWHM)." If we estimate that light fills the 0.10 NA, it should have a beam waist (2wo) of ~3 microns (assuming Gaussian beam approximations). Although field curvature is described as "minimal" in the text, it is still ~10-15 microns at the edge of the field for the emission bands for GFP and RFP proteins. Given that this is 5X larger than the light sheet thickness, how do the authors deal with this? 

      The generated light sheet is flat, with a thickness of ~ 3 µm. This flat light sheet will be captured in focus over the depth of focus of the detection objective. The stated field curvature is within 2.5X the depth of focus of the detection lens, which is equivalent to the “Plan” specification of standard microscope objectives.

      - In Figure 2E, it would be helpful if the authors could list the exposure times as well as the total voxels/second for the two-camera comparison. It's also worth noting that the Sony chip used in the VP151MX camera was released last year whereas the Orca Flash V3 chosen for comparison is over a decade old now. I'm confused as to why the authors chose this camera for comparison when they appear to have a more recent Orca BT-Fusion that they show in a picture in the supplement (indicated as Figure S2 in the text, but I believe this is a typo and should be Figure S3). 

      This is a useful addition, and we have added exposure times to the plot. We have also added a note that the Orca Flash V3 is an older generation sCMOS camera and that newer variants exist. Including the Orca BT-Fusion. The BT-Fusion has a read noise of 1.0 e- rms versus 1.6 e- rms, and a peak quantum efficiency of ~95% vs. 85%. Based on the discussion in Supplementary Note S1, we do not expect that these differences in specifications would dramatically change the data presented in the plot. In addition, the typo in Figure S2 has been corrected to Figure S3.

      - In Table S1, the authors note that they only compare their work to prior modalities that are capable of providing <= 1 micron resolution. I'm a bit confused by this choice given that Figure 2 seems to show the resolution of ExA-SPIM as ~1.5 microns at 4 mm off center (1/2 their stated radial field of view). It also excludes a comparison with the mesoSPIM project which at least to me seems to be the most relevant prior to this manuscript. This system is designed for imaging large cleared tissues like the ones shown here. While the original publication in 2019 had a substantially lower lateral resolution, a newer variant, Nikita et al bioRxiv (which is cited in general terms in this manuscript, but not explicitly discussed) also provides 1.5-micron lateral resolution over a comparable field of view. 

      We have updated the table to include the benchtop mesoSPIM from Nikita et al., Nature Communications, 2024. Based on this published version of the manuscript, the lateral resolution is 1.5 µm and axial resolution is 3.3 µm. Assuming the Iris 15 camera sensor, with the stated 2.5 fps, the volumetric rate (megavoxels/sec) is 37.41.

      - The authors state that, "We systematically evaluated dehydration agents, including methanol, ethanol, and tetrahydrofuran (THF), followed by delipidation with commonly used protocols on 1 mm thick brain slices. Slices were expanded and examined for clarity under a macroscope." It would be useful to include some data from this evaluation in the manuscript to make it clear how the authors arrived at their final protocol. 

      Additional details on the expansion protocol may be included in another manuscript.

      General comments: 

      There is a tendency in the manuscript to use negative qualitative terms when describing prior work and positive qualitative terms when describing the work here. Examples include: 

      - "Throughput is limited in part by cumbersome and error-prone microscopy methods". While I agree that performing single neuron reconstructions at a large scale is a difficult challenge, the terms cumbersome and error-prone are qualitative and lacking objective metrics.

      We have revised this statement to be more precise, stating that throughput is limited in part by the speed and image quality of existing microscopy methods.

      - The resolution of the system is described in several places as "near-isotropic" whereas prior methods were described as "highly anisotropic". I agree that the ~1:3 lateral to axial ratio here is more isotropic than the 1:6 ratio of the other cited publications. However, I'm not sure I'd consider 3-fold worse axial resolution than lateral to be considered "near" isotropic.

      We agree that the term near-isotropic is ambiguous. We have modified the text accordingly, removing the term near-isotropic and where appropriate stating that the resolution is more isotropic than that of other cited publications.

      - In the manuscript, the authors describe the photobleaching in their imaging conditions as "negligible". Figure S5 seems to show a loss of 60% fluorescence after 2000 exposures (which in the caption is described as "modest"). I'd suggest removing these qualitative terms and just stating the values.

      We agree and have changed the text accordingly.

      - The results section for Figure 5 is titled "Tracing axons in human neocortex and white matter". Although this section states "larger axons (>1 um) are well separated... allowing for robust automated and manual tracing" there is no data for any tracing in the manuscript. Although I agree that the images are visually impressive, I'm not sure that this claim is backed by data.

      We have now removed the text in this section referring to automated and manual tracing.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper Weber et al. investigate the role of 4 dopaminergic neurons of the Drosophila larva in mediating the association between an aversive high-salt stimulus and a neutral odor. The 4 DANs belong to the DL1 cluster and innervate non-overlapping compartments of the mushroom body, distinct from those involved in appetitive associative learning. Using specific driver lines for individual neurons, the authors show that activation of the DAN-g1 is sufficient to mimic an aversive memory and it is also necessary to form a high-salt memory of full strength, although optogenetic silencing of this neuron has only a partial phenotype. The authors use calcium imaging to show that the DAN-g1 is not the only DAN responding to salt. DAN-c1 and d1 also respond to salt, but they seem to play no role for the associative memory. DAN-f1, which does not respond to salt, is able to lead to the formation of a memory (if optogenetically activated), but it is not necessary for the salt-odor memory formation in normal conditions. However, when silenced together with DAN-g1, it enhances the memory deficit of DAN-g1. Overall, this work brings evidence of a complex interaction between DL1 DANs in both the encoding of salt signals and their teaching role in associative learning, with none of them being individually necessary and sufficient for both functions.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the manuscript contributes interesting results that are useful to understand the organization and function of the dopaminergic system. The behavioral role of the specific DANs is accessed using specific driver lines which allow to test their function individually and in pairs. Moreover, the authors perform calcium imaging to test whether DANs are activated by salt, a prerequisite for inducing a negative association to it. Proper genetic controls are carried across the manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors use two different approaches to silence dopaminergic neurons: optogenetics and induction of apoptosis. The results are not always consistent, but the authors discuss these differences appropriately. In general, the optogenetic approach is more appropriate as developmental compensations are not of major interest for the question investigated.

      The physiological data would suggest the role of a certain subset of DANs in salt-odor association, but a different partially overlapping set is necessary in behavioral assays (with a partial phenotype). No manipulation completely abolishes the salt-odor association, leaving important open questions on the identity of the neural circuits involved in this behavior.

      The EM data analysis reveals a non-trivial organization of sensory inputs into DANs, but it is difficult to extrapolate a link to the functional data presented in the paper.

      We would like to once again thank Reviewer 1 for the positive assessment of our work and for the valuable suggestions provided on the first revision of the manuscript. In this second revision, we have addressed the linguistic issues and most of the minor comments as recommended. We now hope that the current version of our manuscript meets the reviewer’s expectations both in terms of language and content.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this work the authors show that dopaminergic neurons (DANs) from the DL1 cluster in Drosophila larvae are required for the formation of aversive memories. DL1 DANs complement pPAM cluster neurons which are required for the formation of attractive memories. This shows the compartmentalized network organization of how an insect learning center (the mushroom body) encodes memory by integrating olfactory stimuli with aversive or attractive teaching signals. Interestingly, the authors found that the 4 main dopaminergic DL1 neurons act partially redundant, and that single cell ablation did not result in aversive memory defects. However, ablation or silencing of a specific DL1 subset (DAN-f1,g1) resulted in reduced salt aversion learning, which was specific to salt but no other aversive teaching stimuli tested. Importantly, activation of these DANs using an optogenetic approach was also sufficient to induce aversive learning in the presence of high salt. Together with the functional imaging of salt and fructose responses of the individual DANs and the implemented connectome analysis of sensory (and other) inputs to DL1/pPAM DANs this represents a very comprehensive study linking the structural, functional and behavioral role of DL1 DANs. This provides fundamental insight into the function of a simple yet efficiently organized learning center which displays highly conserved features of integrating teaching signals with other sensory cues via dopaminergic signaling.

      Strengths:

      This is a very careful, precise and meticulous study identifying the main larval DANs involved in aversive learning using high salt as a teaching signal. This is highly interesting because it allows to define the cellular substrates and pathways of aversive learning down to the single cell level in a system without much redundancy. It therefore sets the basis to conduct even more sophisticated experiments and together with the neat connectome analysis opens the possibility to unravel different sensory processing pathways within the DL1 cluster and integration with the higher order circuit elements (Kenyon cells and MBONs). The authors' claims are well substantiated by the data and balanced, putting their data in the appropriate context. The authors also implemented neat pathway analyses using the larval connectome data to its full advantage, thus providing network pathways that contribute towards explaining the obtained results.

      Weaknesses:

      Previous comments were fully addressed by the authors.

      We sincerely thank Reviewer 2 for the positive evaluation of our work. We are glad that our responses in the first revision addressed the previous concerns and appreciate the reviewer’s constructive feedback once again.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study of Weber et al. provides a thorough investigation of the roles of four individual dopamine neurons for aversive associative learning in the Drosophila larva. They focus on the neurons of the DL-1 cluster which already have been shown to signal aversive teaching signals. But the authors go beyond the previous publications and test whether each of these dopamine neurons responds to salt or sugar, is necessary for learning about salt, bitter, or sugar, and is sufficient to induce a memory when optogenetically activated. In addition, previously published connectomic data is used to analyze the synaptic input to each of these dopamine neurons. The authors conclude that the aversive teaching signal induced by salt is distributed across the four DL-1 dopamine neurons, with two of them, DAN-f1 and DAN-g1, being particularly important. Overall, the experiments are well designed and performed, support the authors' conclusions, and deepen our understanding of the dopaminergic punishment system.

      Strengths:

      (1) This study provides, at least to my knowledge, the first in vivo imaging of larval dopamine neurons in response to tastants. Although the selection of tastants is limited, the results close an important gap in our understanding of the function of these neurons.

      (2) The authors performed a large number of experiments to probe for the necessity of each individual dopamine neuron, as well as combinations of neurons, for associative learning. This includes two different training regimen (1 or 3 trials), three different tastants (salt, quinine and fructose) and two different effectors, one ablating the neuron, the other one acutely silencing it. This thorough work is highly commendable, and the results prove that it was worth it. The authors find that only one neuron, DAN-g1, is partially necessary for salt learning when acutely silenced, whereas a combination of two neurons, DAN-f1 and DAN-g1, are necessary for salt learning when either being ablated or silenced.

      (3) In addition, the authors probe whether any of the DL-1 neurons is sufficient for inducing an aversive memory. They found this to be the case for two of the neurons, largely confirming previous results obtained by a different learning paradigm, parameters and effector.

      (4) This study also takes into account connectomic data to analyze the sensory input that each of the dopamine neurons receives. This analysis provides a welcome addition to previous studies and helps to gain a more complete understanding. The authors find large differences in inputs that each neuron receives, and little overlap in input that the dopamine neurons of the "aversive" DL-1 cluster and the "appetitive" pPAM cluster seem to receive.

      (5) Finally, the authors try to link all the gathered information in order to describe an updated working model of how aversive teaching signals are carried by dopamine neurons to the larva's memory center. This includes important comparisons both between two different aversive stimuli (salt and nociception) and between the larval and adult stages.

      We would also like to thank Reviewer 3 for the positive assessment of our work. Many of the constructive comments provided were incorporated into the first revision, contributing significantly to the improved clarity and overall quality of the manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Here are some minor comments (and some semantics that could be addressed to improve the manuscript)

      Title: is the title correct given that c1 and d1 do not really signal punishment?

      We think the title is correct and would like to keep it as it is.

      L72 striatum misspelled

      We have corrected the error.

      L74 constitute instead of provide?

      We made the suggested modification in the text.

      L129: "But can these four individual DANs also process other sensory modalities?" other then what? What was used before?

      We have made the required change, which now allows us to contrast somatosensory and chemosensory information.

      L172: (Please refer to the discussion regarding the partial reduction of the memory); would be more natural to explain shortly here, or add a sentence before this parenthesis that point to the effect

      We made the requested change in the manuscript and added a short sentence before the parenthesis.

      L182: "DL1 neurons convey a dopaminergic aversive teaching signal" you cannot make this statement from just TH-GAL4!

      We agree - that's why we have completely revised the sentence and now further restricted it and also refer to further larval and adult published data

      L264: "possible redundancy among" I don't think you are testing a redundancy here, it is more likely a developmental compensation.

      We made the requested change in the sentence and added a potential developmental compensation as an interpretation of our results.

      L296: "to determine if the activation of individual DL1 DANs signals aspects of the natural high salt punishment," - how can the optogenetic activation tell something about aspects of the natural salt punishment? I understand the fact that salt is present, but still I find it inaccurate

      Our approach is based on the framework established by Bertram Gerber and colleagues over the past two decades in larval Drosophila research. According to this logic, memory recall is dependent on the specific properties of the test context, particularly the type and concentration of the stimulus presented on the test plate. Aversive memory retrieval occurs only when the test conditions closely match those of the training stimulus. Consequently, the larva's behavior on the test plate serves as an indicator of the memory content being recalled. We therefore adhere to this established methodology (Gerber & Hendel, 2006; Schleyer et al., 2011; Schleyer et al., 2015).

      L307 "DAN-f1 and DAN-g1 encode aspects of the natural aversive high salt teaching" you cannot conclude that given that f1 does not even respond to salt. I understand the logic of the salt during test, but I think it is still a stretched interpretation

      We agree and thus have deleted the sentence.

      L310 "Individual DL1 DANs are acutely necessary" this is too general, it seems that only one is

      We have changed the title and now clearly state that this is only one DAN of the DL1 cluster.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In Fig.6 the text flow could be optimized as the authors first mention Fig. 6E,F before they follow up with Fig. 6A-D.

      Thanks for bringing this up – we changed it in the revised version of the manuscript. Now 6A-D is mentioned first.

      In Fig.6 the finding that optogenetic inactivation but not ablation of DAN-g1 slightly but significantly reduces aversive salt learning suggests that there is an individual contribution of this DAN in this paradigm. The authors emphasize redundancy of DL1 DANs although the effect size seems comparable between DAN-g1 and DAN-f1,g1 silencing.

      In response to this concern and the one of reviewer 2, we have revised the section title and removed the final sentence of the section before to avoid placing emphasis on the potential redundancy of DL1 DANs within this results section.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors replied to each issue I raised, and revised their manuscript accordingly. In particular, regarding my major concern (the sufficiency of the neurons for salt-"specific" memories), I think the authors found a good solution.

      I have no further comments.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the positive feedback on our revision. We are pleased that the revised manuscript meets the expectations and appreciate the time and effort invested in the review process.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In Causal associations between plasma proteins and prostate cancer: a Proteome-Wide Mendelian Randomization, the authors present a manuscript which seeks to identify novel markers for prostate cancer through analysis of large biobank-based datasets and to extend this analysis to potential therapeutic targets for drugs. This is an area that is already extensively researched, but remains important, due to the high burden and mortality of prostate cancer globally.

      Strengths:

      The main strengths of the manuscript are the identification and use of large biobank data assets, which provide large numbers of cases and controls, essential for achieving statistical power. The databases used (deCODE, FinnGen, and the UK Biobank) allow for robust numbers of cases and controls. The analytical method chosen, Mendelian Randomization, is appropriate to the problem. Another strength is the integration of multi-omic datasets, here using protein data as well as GWAS sources to integrate genomic and proteomic data.

      Thank you for your positive feedback regarding the overall quality of our work and we greatly appreciate you taking time and making effort in reviewing our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      The main weaknesses of the manuscript relate to the following areas:

      (1) The failure of the study to analyse the data in the context of other closely related conditions such as benign prostatic hyperplasia (BPH) or lower urinary tract symptoms (LUTS), which have some pathways and biomarkers in common, such as inflammatory pathways (including complement) and specific markers such as KLK3. As a consequence, it is not possible for readers to know whether the findings are specific to prostate cancer or whether they are generic to prostate dysfunction. Given the prevalence of prostate dysfunction (half of men reaching their sixth decade), the potential for false positives and overtreatment from non-specific biomarkers is a major problem, resulting in the evidence presented in this manuscript being weak. Other researchers have addressed this issue using the same data sources as presented here, for example, in this paper, looking at BPH in the UK Biobank population. https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-018-06920-9

      Thank you for your valuable comment. We fully agree that biomarker development must prioritize specificity to avoid overtreatment. While our study is a foundational step toward identifying potential therapeutic targets or complementary biomarkers for prostate cancer (PCa)—not as a direct endorsement of these proteins for standalone clinical diagnosis. Mendelian randomization (MR) analysis strengthens causal inference by design, and we further ensured robustness through sensitivity analyses (e.g. MR-Egger regression for pleiotropy, Bonferroni correction for multiple testing). These methods distinguish true causal effects from nonspecific associations. Importantly, while PSA’s lack of specificity is widely recognized, its role in reducing PCa mortality underscores the value of biomarker-driven screening. Our findings align with the need to integrate multiple markers (e.g. combining a novel protein with PSA) to improve diagnostic precision. Translating these causal insights into clinical tools remains challenging but represents a necessary next step, and we emphasize that this work provides a rigorous starting point for future validation studies.

      (2) There is no discussion of Gleason scores with regard to either biomarkers or therapies, and a general lack of discussion around indolent disease as compared with more aggressive variants. These are crucial issues with regard to the triage and identification of genomically aggressive localized prostate cancers. See, for example, the work set out in: https://doi.org/10.1038/nature20788

      Thank you for pointing this out. We acknowledge that our original analysis did not directly address this critical issue due to a key data limitation: the publicly available GWAS summary statistics for PCa (from openGWAS and FinnGen) do not provide genetic associations stratified by phenotypic severity or molecular subtypes. This limitation precluded MR analysis of proteins specifically linked to aggressive disease. To partially bridge this gap, we integrate evidence from recent studies in the revised Discussion section to explore the relevance of potential biomarkers to aggressive PCa.

      (3) An additional issue is that the field of PCa research is fast-moving. The manuscript cites ~80 references, but too few of these are from recent studies, and many important and relevant papers are not included. The manuscript would be much stronger if it compared and contrasted its findings with more recent studies of PCa biomarkers and targets, especially those concerned with multi-omics and those including BPH.

      Thank you for your professional comments. We have rigorously updated the manuscript to include more recent publications and we systematically compare and contrast our findings with these recent studies in the revised Discussion section.

      (4) The Methods section provides no information on how the Controls were selected. There is no Table providing cohort data to allow the reader to know whether there were differences in age, BMI, ethnic grouping, social status or deprivation, or smoking status, between the Cases and Controls. These types of data are generally recorded in Biobank data, so this sort of analysis should be possible, or if not, the authors' inability to construct an appropriately matched set of Controls should be discussed as a Limitation.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important methodological concern. We have expanded the Limitations section to state it.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This is potentially interesting work, but the analyses are attempted in a rather scattergun way, with little evident critical thought. The structure of the work (Results before Methods) can work in some manuscripts, but it is not ideal here. The authors discuss results before we know anything about the underlying data that the results come from. It gives the impression that the authors regard data as a resource to be exploited, without really caring where the data comes from. The methods can provide meaningful insights if correctly used, but while I don't have reasons to doubt that the analyses were conducted correctly, findings are presented with little discussion or interpretation. No follow-up analyses are performed.

      In summary, there are likely some gems here, but the whole manuscript is essentially the output from an analytic pipeline.

      We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful evaluation of our work.

      Taking the researchers aims in turn:

      (1) Meta-GWAS - while combining two datasets together can provide additional insights, the contribution of this analysis above existing GWAS is not clear. The PRACTICAL consortium has already reported the GWAS of 70% of these data. What additional value does this analysis provide? (Likely some, but it's not clear from the text.) Also, the presentation of results is unclear - authors state that only 5 gene regions contained variants at p<5x10-8, but Figure 1 shows dozens of hits above 5x10-8. Also, the red line in Figure 1 (supposedly at 5x10-8) is misplaced.

      Thank you very much for your feedback. Although the PRACTICAL consortium constituted the majority of PCa GWAS data, our meta-analysis integrating FinnGen data enhanced statistical power enabling robust detection of low-frequency variants with minor allele frequencies. Moreover, FinnGen's Finnish ancestry (genetic isolate) helps distinguish population-specific effects. The presentation of results showed the top 5 gene regions contained variants at p < 5×10<sup>-8</sup>. We apologize for not noticing that the red line was not displayed correctly in the original figures included in the manuscript. We have updated it in the revised manuscript.

      (2) Cross-phenotype analysis. It is not really clear what this analysis is, or why it is done. What is the iCPAGdb? A database? A statistical method? Why would we want to know cross-phenotype associations? What even are these? It seems that the authors have taken data from an online resource and have written a paragraph based on this existing data with little added value.

      We thank you for raising this issue. The iCPAGdb (interactive Cross-Phenotype Analysis of GWAS database) is an integrative platform that systematically identifies cross-phenotype associations and evaluates genetic pleiotropy by leveraging LD-proxy associations from the NHGRI-EBI GWAS Catalog. The pathogenesis and progression of prostate cancer constitute a complex pathophysiological continuum characterized by dynamic multisystem interactions, extending beyond singular molecular pathway dysregulation to encompass coordinated disruptions across endocrine regulation, immune microenvironment remodeling, and metabolic reprogramming. Therefore, it is indispensable for discriminating primary pathogenic drivers from secondary compensatory responses, ultimately informing the development of precision therapeutic strategies.

      (3) PW-MR. I can see the value of this work, but many details are unclear. Was this a two-sample MR using PRACTICAL + FinnGen data for the outcome? How many variants were used in key analyses? Again, the description of results is sparse and gives little added value.

      We thank you for raising this issue. Two-sample MR refers to an analytical design where genetic instruments for the exposure (plasma proteins) and genetic associations with the outcome (PCa) are derived from non-overlapping populations. This ensures complete sample independence between exposure and outcome datasets to avoid confounding biases, regardless of whether the outcome data originate from single or multiple cohorts. The meta-analysis of PRACTICAL and FinnGen GWAS generates 27,210 quality-controlled variants (p < 5×10<sup>-8</sup>, MAF ≥ 1%, LD-clumped r<sup>2</sup> < 0.1) used in key analyses.

      (4) Colocalization - seems clear to me.

      (5) Additional post-GWAS analyses (pathway + druggability) - again, the analyses seem to be performed appropriately, although little additional insight other than the reporting of output from the methods.

      The post-MR druggability and pathway analyses serve two primary scientific purposes: (1) therapeutic prioritization - systematically evaluating which MR-identified proteins represent tractable drug targets (either through existing FDA-approved agents or compounds in clinical development) with direct relevance to cancer or PCa management, and (2) mechanistic hypothesis generation - mapping these candidate proteins to coherent biological pathways to guide future functional validation studies investigating their causal roles in prostate carcinogenesis.

      Minor points:

      (6) The stated motivation for this work is "early detection". But causality isn't necessary for early detection. If the authors are interested in early detection, other analysis approaches are more appropriate.

      We appreciate your insightful feedback. While early detection is one motivation for this work, our primary goal extends to identifying causally implicated proteins that may serve as intervention targets for PCa prevention or therapy.  Establishing causality is critical for distinguishing biomarkers that drive disease pathogenesis from those that are secondary to disease progression, as the former holds greater specificity for early detection and prioritization of therapeutic targets. While we acknowledge that validation for early detection may require additional methodologies, MR analysis provides a foundational step by prioritizing candidate proteins with causal links to disease. This approach ensures that downstream efforts focus on biomarkers and targets with the greatest potential to alter disease trajectories, rather than merely correlative markers.

      (7) The authors state "193 proteins were associated with PCa risk", but they are looking at MR results - these analyses test for disease associations of genetically-predicted levels of proteins, not proteins themselves.

      In MR, the exposure of interest is the lifelong effect of genetically predicted protein levels. This approach is designed to infer causality while avoiding confounding and reverse causation, as genetic variants are fixed at conception and unaffected by disease processes. When we state “193 proteins were associated with PCa risk,” we specifically refer to proteins whose genetically predicted levels (based on instrument SNPs from protein QTLs) show causal links to PCa. Importantly, MR does not measure the direct association between observed protein concentrations and disease. Instead, it estimates the lifelong causal effect of protein levels predicted by genetics. This distinction is critical for disentangling cause from consequence. For example, a protein elevated due to tumor progression would not be identified as causal in MR if its genetic predictors are unrelated to PCa risk.

      We acknowledge that clinical translation requires further validation of these proteins in observational studies measuring actual protein levels. However, MR provides a robust first step by prioritizing candidates with causal roles, thereby reducing the risk of investing in biomarkers confounded by disease processes.

    1. Author response:

      We thank the reviewing editors, senior editors, and reviewers for their time, efforts, and constructive feedback. We believe the points raised are addressable and we would like to proceed with a revised submission for further review. Specifically, we plan the following revisions:

      Editor’s Comments

      We will clarify study definitions to ensure the meaning of "5-year crude overall survival time" is explicit for readers.

      Reviewer 1 Comments

      - Clarify and supplement the work with detailed sources of study origin (cancer registries or single-center cohorts).

      - Conduct a multi-level hierarchical meta-analysis to address concerns of ecological fallacy in interpreting results.

      - Perform an ecological sensitivity analysis and clarify findings regarding small study effects.

      - Expand the search base significantly by including African local databases; preliminary searches have identified over 50 potentially eligible doctoral theses, dissertations, local journal articles, and gray literature, potentially adding data from five or more additional countries.

      Reviewer 2 Comments

      - Conduct subgroup analyses by sex and assess the influence of the percentage of males in mixed cohorts.

      - Enhance the limited meta-analysis and provide supplementary full forest plots for all analyses.

      - Clarify phrasing in sections identified by the reviewer.

      Additional Planned Clarifications and Analyses

      - Elucidate the role of cumulative meta-analysis in mitigating lead-time bias.

      - Include supplementary cumulative meta-analysis based on the year of investigation (instead of publication year).

      - Perform subgroup analyses by clinical staging, TNM grading, and treatment modalities where data from ≥10 studies is available.

      - Expand discussion on the merits of quality assessment versus risk of bias evaluation in large scale epidemiological and observational studies, in line with other studies of this scale.

      - Condense the comparison with 2018 estimates, as per reviewer suggestions.

      Clarification Regarding SSA vs. AU Classification

      We do not intend to compare survival between "Sub-Saharan Africa" (SSA) and North Africa, as this binary classification is historically rooted and does not reflect current African Union (AU) administrative or policy groupings. Our regional analyses will adhere to the AU’s contemporary regional framework to better reflect political, cultural, and healthcare system realities.

      On Registry Data

      We will clarify that we will not extract raw registry data, as such data is typically unprocessed and does not provide 5-year overall survival metrics. As such extracting raw, individual-level data from registries or vital statistics systems falls outside the methodological scope of a meta-analysis. Meta-analyses are designed to synthesize published survival estimates or those available from reports where survival analyses have already been conducted. Utilizing raw surveillance data would require primary data processing and survival analysis — effectively creating new data, not synthesizing existing results. This would represent a distinct study design, such as a pooled analysis or original cohort study, rather than a meta-analysis. Where registry reports present summary survival estimates (e.g., 5-year overall survival) in a format compatible with meta-analysis, we will certainly include them.

      All planned additional analyses will depend on data quality, consistency, and feasibility for pooling using state-of-the-art statistical techniques. Where pooling is not possible, we will transparently report limitations.

    1. Author response:

      We thank all the reviewers for their thoughtful comments on our submitted manuscript.

      The main points made by all three reviewers were: to discuss the components of the omitted synapses and explore parameter sensitivity and broader physiological variability; to provide deeper physical insights into phase separation; to clarify terminology and provide better presentation and context in relation to previous studies.

      We fully agree with the first point, suggesting that parameter sensitivity and broader physiological variability should be explored. Our model omits scaffold proteins such as GKAP, Shank and Homer, which are present at the bottom of the PSD hierarchy. In addition, there are many other interactions in PSDs whose affinity is altered by phosphorylation, and the phase separation state of the condensate is likely to be affected by ionic concentration and other environmental factors. We will include a more detailed discussion of these environmental factors and a limitation of our study in the Discussion section. Furthermore, regarding to the sensitivity of the parameters, the reviewer's point that the membrane potential parameter is an important value is right since it directly regulates the difference between 3D and 2D systems. We plan to verify this by changing the strength of the membrane potential, and by running simulations again to see how much it affects the morphology of condensates.

      The second point is that we should provide deeper physical insight into phase separation in different dimensions. It would not be straightforward to directly estimate the entropy of the system due to the nature of the model. However, as pointed out, the difference of phase behavior can be elucidated through various simplified theories such as the lattice model. In this context, the reduced coordination number in 2D systems compared to 3D systems, and the decreased pseudo-attractive force due to the depletion effect, can offer rationalizations. We would like to add some theoretical discussion of these aspects with equations.

      Third, we will clarify terminology and provide better explanation in relation to previous studies. In some parts in manuscripts, such as complexes containing receptors, there were some disunity in terminology and lack of annotations in figures. We will improve the wording and visualization in the text for further clarity and add relevant references, as suggested by the reviewers.

      Also, as additionally suggested, scripts for the simulation and analysis together with the initial structure obtained will be deposited to Zenodo or GitHub.

    1. Author response:

      eLife Assessment

      This work presents an important technical advancement with the release of MorphoNet 2.0, a user-friendly, standalone platform for 3D+T segmentation and analysis in biological imaging. The authors provide convincing evidence of the tool's capabilities through illustrative use cases, though broader validation against current state-of-the-art tools would strengthen its position. The software's accessibility and versatility make it a resource that will be of value for the bioimaging community, particularly in specialized subfields.

      We would like to thank the editors and reviewers for their careful and constructive evaluation of our manuscript “MorphoNet 2.0: An innovative approach for qualitative assessment and segmentation curation of large-scale 3D time-lapse imaging datasets”. We are grateful for the positive assessment of MorphoNet 2.0 as a valuable and accessible tool for the bioimaging community, and for the recognition of its technical advancements, particularly in the context of complex 3D+t segmentation tasks.

      The reviewers have highlighted several important points that we will address in the revised manuscript. These include:

      - The need for a clearer demonstration that improvements in unsupervised quality metrics correspond to actual improvements in segmentation quality. In response, we will provide comparisons with gold standard annotations where available and clarify how to interpret metric distributions.<br /> - The potential risk of circular logic when using unsupervised metrics to guide model training. We now explicitly discuss this limitation and emphasize the importance of external validation and expert input.<br /> - The value of comparing MorphoNet 2.0 to other tools such as FIJI and napari. We will include a comparative table to help readers understand MorphoNet’s positioning and complementarity.<br /> - The importance of clearer documentation and terminology. We will overhaul the help pages, standardize plugin naming, and add a glossary-style table to the manuscript.<br /> - Suggestions for future developments, such as mesh export and interoperability with napari, which we will explore for the revision.

      We appreciate the detailed feedback on both scientific and editorial aspects, including corrections to figures and text, and we will integrate all suggested revisions to improve the manuscript’s clarity and impact. We are confident that these changes will strengthen the manuscript and enhance the utility of MorphoNet 2.0 for the community.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors present a substantial improvement to their existing tool, MorphoNet, intended to facilitate assessment of 3D+t cell segmentation and tracking results, and curation of high-quality analysis for scientific discovery and data sharing. These tools are provided through a user-friendly GUI, making them accessible to biologists who are not experienced coders. Further, the authors have re-developed this tool to be a locally installed piece of software instead of a web interface, making the analysis and rendering of large 3D+t datasets more computationally efficient. The authors evidence the value of this tool with a series of use cases, in which they apply different features of the software to existing datasets and show the improvement to the segmentation and tracking achieved.

      While the computational tools packaged in this software are familiar to readers (e.g., cellpose), the novel contribution of this work is the focus on error correction. The MorphoNet 2.0 software helps users identify where their candidate segmentation and/or tracking may be incorrect. The authors then provide existing tools in a single user-friendly package, lowering the threshold of skill required for users to get maximal value from these existing tools. To help users apply these tools effectively, the authors introduce a number of unsupervised quality metrics that can be applied to a segmentation candidate to identify masks and regions where the segmentation results are noticeably different from the majority of the image.

      This work is valuable to researchers who are working with cell microscopy data that requires high-quality segmentation and tracking, particularly if their data are 3D time-lapse and thus challenging to segment and assess. The MorphoNet 2.0 tool that the authors present is intended to make the iterative process of segmentation, quality assessment, and re-processing easier and more streamlined, combining commonly used tools into a single user interface.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for their thorough and encouraging evaluation of our work. We are grateful that they highlighted both the technical improvements of MorphoNet 2.0 and its potential impact for the broader community working with complex 3D+t microscopy datasets. We particularly appreciate the recognition of our efforts to make advanced segmentation and tracking tools accessible to non-expert users through a user-friendly and locally installable interface, and for pointing out the importance of error detection and correction in the iterative analysis workflow. The reviewer’s appreciation of the value of integrating unsupervised quality metrics to support this process is especially meaningful to us, as this was a central motivation behind the development of MorphoNet 2.0. We hope the tool will indeed facilitate more rigorous and reproducible analyses, and we are encouraged by the reviewer’s positive assessment of its utility for the community.

      One of the key contributions of the work is the unsupervised metrics that MorphoNet 2.0 offers for segmentation quality assessment. These metrics are used in the use cases to identify low-quality instances of segmentation in the provided datasets, so that they can be improved with plugins directly in MorphoNet 2.0. However, not enough consideration is given to demonstrating that optimizing these metrics leads to an improvement in segmentation quality. For example, in Use Case 1, the authors report their metrics of interest (Intensity offset, Intensity border variation, and Nuclei volume) for the uncurated silver truth, the partially curated and fully curated datasets, but this does not evidence an improvement in the results. Additional plotting of the distribution of these metrics on the Gold Truth data could help confirm that the distribution of these metrics now better matches the expected distribution.

      Similarly, in Use Case 2, visual inspection leads us to believe that the segmentation generated by the Cellpose + Deli pipeline (shown in Figure 4d) is an improvement, but a direct comparison of agreement between segmented masks and masks in the published data (where the segmentations overlap) would further evidence this.

      We agree that demonstrating the correlation between metric optimization and real segmentation improvement is essential. We will add new analysis comparing the distributions of the unsupervised metrics with the gold truth data before and after curation. Additionally, we will provide overlap scores where ground truth annotations are available, confirming the improvement. We will also explicitly discuss the limitation of relying solely on unsupervised metrics without complementary validation.

      We would appreciate the authors addressing the risk of decreasing the quality of the segmentations by applying circular logic with their tool; MorphoNet 2.0 uses unsupervised metrics to identify masks that do not fit the typical distribution. A model such as StarDist can be trained on the "good" masks to generate more masks that match the most common type. This leads to a more homogeneous segmentation quality, without consideration for whether these metrics actually optimize the segmentation

      We thank the reviewer for this important and insightful comment. It raises a crucial point regarding the risk of circular logic in our segmentation pipeline. Indeed, relying on unsupervised metrics to select “good” masks and using them to train a model like StarDist could lead to reinforcing a particular distribution of shapes or sizes, potentially filtering out biologically relevant variability. This homogenization may improve consistency with the chosen metrics, but not necessarily with the true underlying structures.

      We fully agree that this is a key limitation to be aware of. We will revise the manuscript to explicitly discuss this risk, emphasizing that while our approach may help improve segmentation quality according to specific criteria, it should be complemented with biological validation and, when possible, expert input to ensure that important but rare phenotypes are not excluded.

      In Use case 5, the authors include details that the errors were corrected by "264 MorphoNet plugin actions ... in 8 hours actions [sic]". The work would benefit from explaining whether this is 8 hours of human work, trying plugins and iteratively improving, or 8 hours of compute time to apply the selected plugins.

      We will clarify that the “8 hours” refer to human interaction time, including exploration, testing, and iterative correction using plugins.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This article presents Morphonet 2.0, a software designed to visualise and curate segmentations of 3D and 3D+t data. The authors demonstrate their capabilities on five published datasets, showcasing how even small segmentation errors can be automatically detected, easily assessed, and corrected by the user. This allows for more reliable ground truths, which will in turn be very much valuable for analysis and training deep learning models. Morphonet 2.0 offers intuitive 3D inspection and functionalities accessible to a non-coding audience, thereby broadening its impact.

      Strengths:

      The work proposed in this article is expected to be of great interest to the community by enabling easy visualisation and correction of complex 3D(+t) datasets. Moreover, the article is clear and well written, making MorphoNet more likely to be used. The goals are clearly defined, addressing an undeniable need in the bioimage analysis community. The authors use a diverse range of datasets, successfully demonstrating the versatility of the software.

      We would also like to highlight the great effort that was made to clearly explain which type of computer configurations are necessary to run the different datasets and how to find the appropriate documentation according to your needs. The authors clearly carefully thought about these two important problems and came up with very satisfactory solutions.

      We would like to sincerely thank the reviewer for their positive and thoughtful feedback. We are especially grateful that they acknowledged the clarity of the manuscript and the potential value of MorphoNet 2.0 for the community, particularly in facilitating the visualization and correction of complex 3D(+t) datasets. We also appreciate the reviewer’s recognition of our efforts to provide detailed guidance on hardware requirements and access to documentation—two aspects we consider crucial to ensuring the tool is both usable and widely adopted. Their comments are very encouraging and reinforce our commitment to making MorphoNet 2.0 as accessible and practical as possible for a broad range of users in the bioimage analysis community.

      Weaknesses:

      There is still one concern: the quantification of the improvement of the segmentations in the use cases and, therefore, the quantification of the potential impact of the software. While it appears hard to quantify the quality of the correction, the proposed work would be significantly improved if such metrics could be provided.

      The authors show some distributions of metrics before and after segmentations to highlight the changes. This is a great start, but there seem to be two shortcomings: first, the comparison and interpretation of the different distributions does not appear to be trivial. It is therefore difficult to judge the quality of the improvement from these. Maybe an explanation in the text of how to interpret the differences between the distributions could help. A second shortcoming is that the before/after metrics displayed are the metrics used to guide the correction, so, by design, the scores will improve, but does that accurately represent the improvement of the segmentation? It seems to be the case, but it would be nice to maybe have a better assessment of the improvement of the quality.

      We thank the reviewer for this constructive and important comment. We fully agree that assessing the true quality improvement of segmentation after correction is a central and challenging issue. While we initially focused on changes in the unsupervised quality metrics to illustrate the effect of the correction, we acknowledge that interpreting these distributions may not be straightforward, and that relying solely on the metrics used to guide the correction introduces an inherent bias in the evaluation.

      To address the first point, we will revise the manuscript to provide clearer guidance on how to interpret the changes in metric distributions before and after correction, with additional examples to make this interpretation more intuitive.

      Regarding the second point, we agree that using independent, external validation is necessary to confirm that the segmentation has genuinely improved. To this end, we will include additional assessments using complementary evaluation strategies on selected datasets where ground truth is accessible, to compare pre- and post-correction segmentations with an independent reference. These results reinforce the idea that the corrections guided by unsupervised metrics generally lead to more accurate segmentations, but we also emphasize their limitations and the need for biological validation in real-world cases.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      A very thorough technical report of a new standalone, open-source software for microscopy image processing and analysis (MorphoNet 2.0), with a particular emphasis on automated segmentation and its curation to obtain accurate results even with very complex 3D stacks, including timelapse experiments.

      Strengths:

      The authors did a good job of explaining the advantages of MorphoNet 2.0, as compared to its previous web-based version and to other software with similar capabilities. What I particularly found more useful to actually envisage these claimed advantages is the five examples used to illustrate the power of the software (based on a combination of Python scripting and the 3D game engine Unity). These examples, from published research, are very varied in both types of information and image quality, and all have their complexities, making them inherently difficult to segment. I strongly recommend the readers to carefully watch the accompanying videos, which show (although not thoroughly) how the software is actually used in these examples.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for their thoughtful and encouraging feedback. We are particularly pleased that the reviewer appreciated the comparative analysis of MorphoNet 2.0 with both its earlier version and existing tools, as well as the relevance of the five diverse and complex use cases we selected. Demonstrating the software’s versatility and robustness across a variety of challenging datasets was a key goal of this work, and we are glad that this aspect came through clearly. We also appreciate the reviewer’s recommendation to watch the accompanying videos, which we designed to provide a practical sense of how the tool is used in real-world scenarios. Their positive assessment is highly motivating and reinforces the value of combining scripting flexibility with an interactive 3D interface.

      Weaknesses:

      Being a technical article, the only possible comments are on how methods are presented, which is generally adequate, as mentioned above. In this regard, and in spite of the presented examples (chosen by the authors, who clearly gave them a deep thought before showing them), the only way in which the presented software will prove valuable is through its use by as many researchers as possible. This is not a weakness per se, of course, but just what is usual in this sort of report. Hence, I encourage readers to download the software and give it time to test it on their own data (which I will also do myself).

      We fully agree that the true value of MorphoNet 2.0 will be demonstrated through its practical use by a wide range of researchers working with complex 3D and 3D+t datasets. In this regard, we will improve the user documentation and provide a set of example datasets to help new users quickly familiarize themselves with the platform. We are also committed to maintaining and updating MorphoNet 2.0 based on user feedback to further support its usability and impact.

      In conclusion, I believe that this report is fundamental because it will be the major way of initially promoting the use of MorphoNet 2.0 by the objective public. The software itself holds the promise of being very impactful for the microscopists' community.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer 1:

      Summary:

      Identifying drugs that target specific disease phenotypes remains a persistent challenge. Many current methods are only applicable to well-characterized small molecules, such as those with known structures. In contrast, methods based on transcriptional responses offer broader applicability because they do not require prior information about small molecules. Additionally, they can be rapidly applied to new small molecules. One of the most promising strategies involves the use of “drug response signatures”-specific sets of genes whose differential expression can serve as markers for the response to a small molecule. By comparing drug response signatures with expression profiles characteristic of a disease, it is possible to identify drugs that modulate the disease profile, indicating a potential therapeutic connection.

      This study aims to prioritize potential drug candidates and to forecast novel drug combinations that may be effective in treating triple-negative breast cancer (TNBC). Large consortia, such as the LINCS-L1000 project, offer transcriptional signatures across various time points after exposing numerous cell lines to hundreds of compounds at different concentrations. While this data is highly valuable, its direct applicability to pathophysiological contexts is constrained by the challenges in extracting consistent drug response profiles from these extensive datasets. The authors use their method to create drug response profiles for three different TNBC cell lines from LINCS.

      To create a more precise, cancer-specific disease profile, the authors highlight the use of single-cell RNA sequencing (scRNA-seq) data. They focus on TNBC epithelial cells collected from 26 diseased individuals compared to epithelial cells collected from 10 healthy volunteers. The authors are further leveraging drug response data to develop inhibitor combinations.

      Strengths:

      The authors of this study contribute to an ongoing effort to develop automated, robust approaches that leverage gene expression similarities across various cell lines and different treatment regimens, aiming to predict drug response signatures more accurately. The authors are trying to address the gap that remains in computational methods for inferring drug responses at the cell subpopulation level.

      Weaknesses:

      One weakness is that the authors do not compare their method to previous studies. The authors develop a drug response profile by summarizing the time points, concentrations, and cell lines. The computational challenge of creating a single gene list that represents the transcriptional response to a drug across different cell lines and treatment protocols has been previously addressed. The Prototype Ranked List (PRL) procedure, developed by Iorio and co-authors (PNAS, 2010, doi:10.1073/pnas.1000138107), uses a hierarchical majority-voting scheme to rank genes. This method generates a list of genes that are consistently overexpressed or downregulated across individual conditions, which then hold top positions in the PRL. The PRL methodology was used by Aissa and co-authors (Nature Comm 2021, doi:10.1038/s41467-021-21884-z) to analyze drug effects on selective cell populations using scRNA-seq datasets. They combined PRL with Gene Set Enrichment Analysis (GSEA), a method that compares a ranked list of genes like PRL against a specific set of genes of interest. GSEA calculates a Normalized Enrichment Score (NES), which indicates how well the genes of interest are represented among the top genes in the PRL. Compared to the method described in the current manuscript, the PRL method allows for the identification of both upregulated and downregulated transcriptional signatures relevant to the drug’s effects. It also gives equal weight to each cell line’s contribution to the drug’s overall response signature.

      The authors performed experimental validation of the top two identified drugs; however, the effect was modest. In addition, the effect on TNBC cell lines was cell-line specific as the identified drugs were effective against BT20, whose transcriptional signatures from LINCS were used for drug identification, but not against the other two cell lines analyzed. An incorrect choice of genes for the signature may result in capturing similarities tied to experimental conditions (e.g., the same cell line) rather than the drug’s actual effects. This reflects the challenges faced by drug response signature methods in both selecting the appropriate subset of genes that make up the signature and managing the multiple expression profiles generated by treating different cell lines with the same drug.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful feedback and their suggestion to refer to the Prototype Ranked List (PRL) manuscript. Unfortunately, since this methodology for the PRL isn’t implemented in an open-source package, direct comparison with our approach is challenging. Nonetheless, we investigated whether using ranks would yield similar results for the most likely active drug pairs identified by retriever. To do this, we calculated and compared the rankings of the average effect sizes provided by retriever. Although the Spearman (ρ \= 0.98) correlation coefficient was high, we observed that key genes are disadvantaged when using ranks compared to effect sizes. This difference is particularly evident in the gene set enrichment analysis, where using average ranks identified only one pathway as statistically significantly enriched. The code to replicate these analyses is available at https://github.com/dosorio/L1000-TNBC/blob/main/Code/.

      Author response image 1.

      Given the similarity in purpose between retriever and the PRL approach, we have added the following statement to the introduction: “Previously, this goal was approached using a majority-voting scheme to rank genes across various cell types, concentrations, and time points. This approach generates a prototype ranked list (PRL) that represents the consistent ranks of genes across several cell lines in response to a specific drug.”

      Regarding the experimental validation, we believe there is a misunderstanding about the evidence we provided. We would like to claridy that we used three different TNBC cell lines: CAL120, BT20, and DU4475. It’s important to note that CAL120 and DU4475 were not included in the signature generation process. Despite this, we observed effects that exceeded the additive effects expectations, particularly in the CAL120 cell line (Figure 5, Panel F).

      Reviewer 2:

      Summary:

      In their study, Osorio and colleagues present ‘retriever,’ an innovative computational tool designed to extract disease-specific transcriptional drug response profiles from the LINCS-L1000 project. This tool has been effectively applied to TNBC, leveraging single-cell RNA sequencing data to predict drug combinations that may effectively target the disease. The public review highlights the significant integration of extensive pharmacological data with high-resolution transcriptomic information, which enhances the potential for personalized therapeutic applications.

      Strengths:

      A key finding of the study is the prediction and validation of the drug combination QL-XII-47 and GSK-690693 for the treatment of TNBC. The methodology employed is robust, with a clear pathway from data analysis to experimental confirmation.

      Weaknesses:

      However, several issues need to be addressed. The predictive accuracy of ’retriever’ is contingent upon the quality and comprehensiveness of the LINCS-L1000 and single-cell datasets utilized, which is an important caveat as these datasets may not fully capture the heterogeneity of patient responses to treatment. While the in vitro validation of the drug combinations is promising, further in vivo studies and clinical trials are necessary to establish their efficacy and safety. The applicability of these findings to other cancer types also warrants additional investigation. Expanding the application of ’retriever’ to a broader range of cancer types and integrating it with clinical data will be crucial for realizing its potential in personalized medicine. Furthermore, as the study primarily focuses on kinase inhibitors, it remains to be seen how well these findings translate to other drug classes.

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful and constructive feedback. We appreciate your insights and agree that several important considerations need to be addressed.

      We recognize that the predictive accuracy of retriever depends on the LINCS-L1000 and single-cell datasets. These resources may not fully represent the complete range of transcriptional responses to disease and treatment across different patients. As you mentioned, this is an important limitation. However, we believe that by extrapolating the evaluation of the most likely active compound to each individual patient, we can help address this issue. This approach will provide valuable insights into which patients in the study are most likely to respond positively to treatment.

      On the in-vitro validation of drug combinations, we agree that while promising, these results are not sufficient on their own to establish clinical efficacy. Additional in-vivo studies will be essential in assessing the therapeutic potential and safety of these combinations, and clinical trials will be an important next step to validate the translational impact of our findings.

      Lastly, we appreciate the reviewer’s comment about the focus of our study on kinase inhibitors. This result was unexpected, as we tested the full set of compounds from the LINCS-L1000 project. We agree that exploring other top candidates, including different drug classes, will be important for assessing how broadly retriever approach can be applied.

      Reviewing Editor:

      I appreciate the interesting and potentially impactful nature of your research; the reviewers have some concerns that I believe need to be addressed. While your research addresses an important and timely topic in cancer treatment, the current manuscript does not provide a substantial advance in its present form.

      The significance of your findings is substantial, as you present a novel computational tool, ’retriever,’ which has the potential to revolutionize personalized cancer treatment strategies by predicting effective drug combinations for triple-negative breast cancer (TNBC). The integration of single-cell RNA-seq data with the LINCS-L1000 project’s transcriptional profiles is a powerful approach that could lead to more targeted and effective therapies. However, the manuscript would benefit from a more explicit discussion of how your work advances the field beyond current methodologies, particularly in the context of drug repurposing and combinatorial therapy.

      The strength of the evidence presented is robust, as evidenced by the systematic testing of 152 drug response profiles and 11,476 drug combinations. The identification of QL-XII-47 and GSK-690693 as promising treatment candidates for TNBC is a significant outcome that warrants further exploration. To enhance the manuscript, it would be beneficial to include a more detailed analysis of the biological pathways and mechanisms of action associated with these drugs, as well as a broader experimental validation beyond the cell lines tested.

      Taken together, I encourage you to address the issues raised and consider resubmitting a revised version of your work.

      Following the suggestions of the reviewing editor, we have included a more detailed discussion on how retriever advances the field, especially in the context of drug repurposing and combinatorial therapy in precision medicine, going beyond current methodologies.

      We agree with the suggestion of the editor to offer a more detailed analysis of the biological pathways and mechanisms of action related to these drugs. Consequently, we have expanded our evaluation of these mechanisms. We utilized the Biological Process Gene Ontology to identify changes associated with the mechanisms of each compound individually, as well as the proposed drug combination. Our findings reveal that the statistically significant processes are closely related to cancer deregulation, cross-validating our previous report using the Cancer Hallmarks.

      Author response image 2.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer 1:

      (1) The LINCS-L1000 project is introduced in the manuscript as a resource for published transcriptional profiles of several cell lines. Since the original citation, it has been expanded into a vast resource, and the description probably needs to reflect the recent version of LINCS.

      We agree with the reviewer that the LINCS-L1000 project is introduced in the manuscript as a resource for transcriptional profiles of several cell lines. Since the original citation, the project has grown into a much larger resource.

      To reflect this, we have added a 2022 citation that summarizes efforts to link omics signatures with biological mechanisms using iLINCS: Pilarczyk, Marcin, et al. ”Connecting omics signatures and revealing biological mechanisms with iLINCS.” Nature communications 13.1 (2022): 4678.

      Reviewer 2:

      (1) It would be beneficial for the manuscript if the authors could expand on the potential limitations inherentto the ’retriever’ tool. This discussion could insightfully address how the foundational assumptions of the analysis may influence the predictive accuracy and the extent to which dataset quality could affect the reliability of the outcomes.

      We agree with the reviewer that expanding on the limitations of retriever would help raise awareness of the underlying assumptions in the analysis and how they affect the predictive accuracy and reliability of the results.

      The following paragraph was added to the Discussion section: “Although retriever represents a significant advancement in extracting disease-specific drug response profiles from the LINCS-L1000 dataset. Several limitations must be considered when interpreting its results. One key limitation is the restricted scope of gene expression data in the LINCS-L1000 project, which includes expression profiles for only 1,000 genes. While this gene set provides valuable insights into broad transcriptional changes, it may not fully capture the complexity of cellular responses to drug treatments. A possible solution to this limitation relies on imputation techniques to address the missing quantification in the gene expression matrix. The accuracy of the imputed values is dependent on the quality of the imputation model and the completeness of the available data. Consequently, there is an inherent risk that the imputed values may not accurately represent the true and complete underlying biological response.”

      (2) Enhancing the manuscript with a more detailed exploration of the clinical ramifications of the study’s findings would be valuable. The authors might consider including how these predictions could be strategically incorporated into the design of clinical trials, thereby bridging the gap between computational predictions and clinical application.

      We appreciate the opportunity provided by the reviewer to expand on the potential of retriever for the design of clinical trials and clinical application.

      The following paragraph was added to the discussion section: “Finally, we have shown that the approach implemented in retriever method can predict effective drug combinations for patients with triplenegative breast cancer (TNBC), but its potential goes beyond that. It can also be applied to single-cell RNA sequencing data from individual tumors and other diseases for which a the single-cell transcriptomic profile of a normal control population is available. In line with this, the LINCS project has released datasets for iPSC-derived cardiomyocytes and motor neurons, opening up new possibilities for precision medicine not only in cancer but also in a variety of other diseases. By predicting the most effective drug and combination treatments for each patient, clinical trials can be designed to target the right populations with the responsive transcriptional phenotype, leading to more successful outcomes.”

      (3) It would be insightful if the authors could discuss the potential for drug resistance in the context of thedrug combinations identified by ’retriever’. An analysis of this phenomenon could provide critical insights into the longevity and effectiveness of the proposed treatment strategies.

      We agree with the reviewer that the potential for drug resistance is a critical consideration when evaluating any therapeutic strategy in cancer, especially when using drug combinations. While the current study focuses on identifying effective drug pairings using ‘retriever’, we recognize that the emergence of resistance could limit their long-term utility. We have addressed the topic within the introduction: “Nonetheless, monotherapy in cancer is highly susceptible to the development of resistance following an initial response to treatment. Combination therapy, or the simultaneous administration of multiple drugs to treat a disease, has evolved into the standard pharmacological regimen for treating complex diseases such as cancer. Combination therapy prevent tumor evolution and help inhibit the development of drug resistance in cancer, thereby improving patient survival.”

      (4) Providing details regarding the computational resources necessary for the implementation of ’retriever’,along with any limitations associated with these requirements, could greatly enhance the transparency and reproducibility of the methodology. Such information would be instrumental for other researchers seeking to apply this tool in their own work.

      The following paragraph was added to the data availability section of the manuscript: “The retriever package is available from the Kuijjer Lab repository https://github.com/kuijjerlab/retriever or from the CRAN repositories at https://cran.r-project.org/package=retriever, and it is implemented as an R multiplatform package that can run on standard laptops or desktops with around 16 GB of RAM, making it accessible for most users. It is designed to work on Windows, macOS, and Linux. While the package can function with modest hardware, performance may vary based on dataset size and complexity. For larger datasets, systems with more RAM or cloud-based resources may improve efficiency.”

      (5) A thoughtful discussion on the ethical considerations surrounding the use of patient-derived data in thedevelopment and validation of ’retriever’ would round out the manuscript. Addressing issues of data privacy and the ethical use of such data could set a precedent for responsible research practices in the field of computational biology and personalized medicine.

      We agree with the reviewer on the need of discussing the ethical considerations surrounding the use of patient-derived data in the validation, development and re-purposing of drugs for disease treatment.

      The following paragraph was added to the discussion section: “We want to highlight the important ethical considerations involved in using patient-derived data for drug development and repurposing, particularly around data privacy, informed consent, and the reliability of predictive models. To protect patient privacy, it is crucial to adhere to data protection laws, such as HIPAA and GDPR, and to rigorously de-identify data to minimize the risk of re-identification. Additionally, datasets must be diverse and representative to prevent bias, ensuring that predictive models are applicable to a broad population. Computational models should undergo extensive validation before being used in clinical settings to ensure their accuracy and transparency. Ethical protocols for data sharing must also be established to respect patient autonomy and control over their data. Furthermore, continuous monitoring and validation of drug predictions are necessary to ensure treatment safety, effectiveness, and fairness.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary: 

      The manuscript by Nicoletti et al. presents a minimal model of habituation, a basic form of non-associative learning, addressing both from dynamical and information theory aspects of how habituation can be realized. The authors identify that negative feedback provided with a slow storage mechanism is sufficient to explain habituation.

      Strengths: 

      The authors combine the identification of the dynamical mechanism with information-theoretic measures to determine the onset of habituation and provide a description of how the system can gain maximum information about the environment.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strength of our work and for their comments, which we believe have been instrumental in significantly improving our work and its scope. Below, we address all their concerns.

      Weaknesses: 

      I have several main concerns/questions about the proposed model for habituation and its plausibility. In general, habituation does not only refer to a decrease in the responsiveness upon repeated stimulation but as Thompson and Spencer discussed in Psych. Rev. 73, 16-43 (1966), there are 10 main characteristics of habituation, including (i) spontaneous recovery when the stimulus is withheld after response decrement; dependence on the frequency of stimulation such that (ii) more frequent stimulation results in more rapid and/or more pronounced response decrement and more rapid spontaneous recovery; (iii) within a stimulus modality, the less intense the stimulus, the more rapid and/or more pronounced the behavioral response decrement; (iv) the effects of repeated stimulation may continue to accumulate even after the response has reached an asymptotic level (which may or may not be zero, or no response). This effect of stimulation beyond asymptotic levels can alter subsequent behavior, for example, by delaying the onset of spontaneous recovery. 

      These are only a subset of the conditions that have been experimentally observed and therefore a mechanistic model of habituation, in my understanding, should capture the majority of these features and/or discuss the absence of such features from the proposed model. 

      We are really grateful to the reviewer for pointing out these aspects of habituation that we overlooked in the previous version of our manuscript. Indeed, our model is able to capture most of these 10 observed behaviors, specifically: 1) habituation; 2) spontaneous recovery; 3) potentiation of habituation; 4) frequency sensitivity; 5) intensity sensitivity; 6) subliminal accumulation. Here, we are following the same terminology employed in Eckert et al., Current Biology 34, 5646–5658 (2024), the paper highlighted by the reviewer. We have dedicated a section of the revised version of the manuscript to these hallmarks, substantiating the validity of our framework as a minimal model to have habituation. We remark that these are the sole hallmarks that can be discussed by considering one single external stimulus and that can be identified without ambiguity in a biochemical context. This observation is again in line with Eckert et al., Current Biology 34, 5646–5658 (2024).

      In the revised version, we employ the same strategy of the aforementioned work to determine when the system can be considered “habituated”. Indeed, we introduce a response threshold that is now discussed in the manuscript. We also included a note in the discussions stating that, since any biochemical model will eventually reach a steady state, subliminal accumulation, for example, can only be seen with the use of a threshold. The introduction of different storage mechanisms, ideally more detailed at a molecular level, can shed light on this conceptual gap. This is an interesting direction of research.

      Furthermore, the habituated response in steady-state is approximately 20% less than the initial response, which seems to be achieved already after 3-4 pulses, the subsequent change in response amplitude seems to be negligible, although the authors however state "after a large number of inputs, the system reaches a time-periodic steady-state". How do the authors justify these minimal decreases in the response amplitude? Does this come from the model parametrization and is there a parameter range where more pronounced habituation responses can be observed? 

      The reviewer is correct, but this is solely a consequence of the specific set of parameters we selected. We made this choice solely for visualization purposes in the previous version. In the revised version, in the section discussing the hallmarks of habituation, we also show other parameter choices when the response decrement is more pronounced. Moreover, we remark that the contour plot of \Delta⟨U> clearly shows that the decrement can largely exceed the 20% threshold presented in the previous version.

      In the revised version, also in light of the works highlighted by the reviewer, we decided to move the focus of the manuscript to the information-theoretic advantage of habituation. As such, we modified several parts of the main text. Also, in the region of optimal information gain, habituation is at an intermediate level. For this reason, we decided to keep the same parameter choice as the previous version in Figure 2.

      We stated that the time-periodic steady-state is reached “after a large number of stimuli” from a mathematical perspective. However, by using a habituation threshold, as done in Eckert et al., Current Biology 34, 5646–5658 (2024), we can state that the system is habituated after a few stimuli for each set of parameters. This aspect is highlighted in the revised version of the manuscript (see also the point above).

      The same is true for the information content (Figure 2f) - already at the first pulse, IU, H ~ 0.7 and only negligibly increases afterwards. In my understanding, during learning, the mutual information between the input and the internal state increases over time and the system extracts from these predictions about its responses. In the model presented by the authors, it seems the system already carries information about the environment which hardly changes with repeated stimulus presentation. The complexity of the signal is also limited, and it is very hard to clarify from the presented results, whether the proposed model can actually explain basic features of habituation, as mentioned above. 

      As for the response decrement of the readout, we can certainly choose a set of parameters for which the information gain is higher. In the revised version, we also report the information at the first stimulation and when the system is habituated to give a better idea of the range of these quantities. At any rate, as the referee correctly points out, it is difficult to give an intuitive interpretation of the information in our minimal model.

      It is also important to remark that, since the readout population and the receptor both undergo fast dynamics (with appropriate timescales as discussed in the text), we are not observing the transient gain of information associated with the first stimulus. As such, the mutual information presents a discontinuous behavior that resembles the dynamics of the readout, thereby starting at a non-zero value already at the first stimulus.

      Additionally, there have been two recent models on habituation and I strongly suggest that the authors discuss their work in relation to recent works (bioRxiv 2024.08.04.606534; arXiv:2407.18204).

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out these relevant references. In the revised version, we highlighted that we discuss the information-theoretic aspects of habituation, while the aforementioned references focus on the dynamics of this phenomenon.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I would also like to note here the simplification of the proposed biological model - in particular, that the receptor can be in an active/passive state, as well as proposing the Nf-kB signaling module as a possible molecular realization. Generally, a large number of cell surface receptors including RTKs of GPCRs have much more complex dynamics including autocatalytic activation that generally leads to bistability, and the Nf-kB has been demonstrated to have oscillatory even chaotic dynamics (works of Savas Tsay, Mogens Jensen and others). Considering this, the authors should at least discuss under which conditions these TNF-Alpha signaling could potentially serve as a molecular realisation for habituation. 

      We thank the reviewer for bringing this to our attention. In the previous version, we reported the TNF signaling network only to show a similar coarse-grained modular structure. However, following a suggestion of reviewer #2, we decided to change Figure 1 to include a simplified molecular scheme of chemotaxis rather than TNF signaling, to avoid any source of confusion about this issue.

      Also, a minor point: Figures 2d-e are cited before 2a-c. 

      We apologize for the oversight. The structure of the Figures and their order is now significantly different, and they are now cited in the correct order. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this study, the authors aim to investigate habituation, the phenomenon of increasing reduction in activity following repeated stimuli, in the context of its information-theoretic advantage. To this end, they consider a highly simplified three-species reaction network where habituation is encoded by a slow memory variable that suppresses the receptor and therefore the readout activity. Using analytical and numerical methods, they show that in their model the information gain, the difference between the mutual information between the signal and readout after and before habituation, is maximal for intermediate habituation strength. Furthermore, they demonstrate that the Pareto front corresponds to an optimization strategy that maximizes the mutual information between signal and readout in the steady state, minimizes some form of dissipation, and also exhibits similar intermediate habituation strength. Finally, they briefly compare predictions of their model to whole-brain recordings of zebrafish larvae under visual stimulation. 

      The author's simplified model might serve as a solid starting point for understanding habituation in different biological contexts as the model is simple enough to allow for some analytic understanding but at the same time exhibits all basic properties of habituation in sensory systems. Furthermore, the author's finding of maximal information gain for intermediate habituation strength via an optimization principle is, in general, interesting. However, the following points remain unclear or are weakly explained: 

      We thank the reviewer for deeming our work interesting and for considering it a solid starting point for understanding habituation in biological systems.

      (1) Is it unclear what the meaning of the finding of maximal information gain for intermediate habituation strength is for biological systems? Why is information gain as defined in the paper a relevant quantity for an organism/cell? For instance, why is a system with low mutual information after the first stimulus and intermediate mutual information after habituation better than one with consistently intermediate mutual information? Or, in other words, couldn't the system try to maximize the mutual information acquired over the whole time series, e.g., the time series mutual information between the stimulus and readout?

      This is a delicate aspect to discuss and we thank the referee for the comment. In the revised version, we report information gain, initial and final information, highlighting that both gain and final information are higher in regions where habituation is present. They have qualitatively similar behavior and highlight a clear information-theoretic advantage of this dynamical phenomenon. An important point is that, to determine the optimal Pareto front, we consider a prolonged stimulus and its associated steady-state information. Therefore, from the optimization point of view, there is no notion of “information gain” or “final information”, which are intrinsically dynamical quantities. As a result, the fact that optimal curve lies in the region of optimal information gain is a-priori not expected and hints at the potential crucial role of this feature. In the revised version, we elucidate this aspect with several additional analyses.

      We would like to add that, from a naive perspective, while the first stimulation will necessarily trigger a certain (non-zero) mutual information, multiple observations of the same stimulus have to reflect into accumulated information that consequently drives the onset of observed dynamical behaviors, such as habituation.

      (2) The model is very similar to (or a simplification of previous models) for adaptation in living systems, e.g., for adaptation in chemotaxis via activity-dependent methylation and demethylation. This should be made clearer.

      We apologize for having missed this point. Our choice has been motivated by the fact that we wanted to avoid confusion between the usual definition of (perfect) adaptation and habituation. However, we now believe that this is not the case for the revised manuscript, and we now include chemotaxis as an example in Figure 1.

      (3) It remains unclear why this optimization principle is the most relevant one. While it makes sense to maximize the mutual information between stimulus and readout, there are various choices for what kind of dissipation is minimized. Why was \delta Q_R chosen and not, for instance, \dot{\Sigma}_int or the sum of both? How would the results change in that case? And how different are the results if the mutual information is not calculated for the strong stimulation input statistics but for the background one?

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We agree that a priori, there is no reason to choose \delta Q_R or a function of the internal energy flux J_int (that, in the revised version, we are using in place of \dot\Sigma_int following the suggestion of reviewer #3). The rationale was to minimize \delta Q_R since this dissipation is unavoidable and stems from the presence of the storage inhibiting the receptor through the internal pathway. Indeed, considering the existence of two different pathways implementing sensing and feedback, the presence of any input will result in a dissipation produced by the receptor. This energy consumption is reflected in \delta Q_R.

      In the revised version, we now include in the optimization principle two energy contributions (see Eq. (14) of the revised manuscript): \delta Q_R and E_int, which is the energy consumption associated with the driven storage production per unit energy. All Figures have been updated accordingly. The results remain similar, as \delta Q_R still represents the main contribution, especially at high \beta.

      Furthermore, in the revised version, we include examples of the Pareto optimization for different values of input strength. As detailed both in the main text and the Supplementary Information, changing the value of ⟨H⟩ moves the Pareto frontier in the (\beta, \sigma) space, since the signal needs to be strong enough for the system to distinguish it from the intrinsic thermal noise (controlled by beta). We also show that if the system is able to tune the inhibition strength \kappa, the Pareto frontiers at different ⟨H⟩ collapse into a single curve. This shows that, although the values of, e.g., the mutual information, depend on ⟨H⟩, the qualitative behavior of the system in this regime is effectively independent of it. We also added more details about this in the Supplementary Information.

      (4) The comparison to the experimental data is not too strong of an argument in favor of the model. Is the agreement between the model and the experimental data surprising? What other behavior in the PCA space could one have expected in the data? Shouldn't the 1st PC mostly reflect the "features", by construction, and other variability should be due to progressively reduced activity levels? 

      The agreement between data and model is not surprising - we agree on this - since the data exhibit habituation. However, we believe that the fact that our minimal model is able to capture the features of a complex neural system just by looking at the PCs, without any explicit biological details, is non-trivial. We also stress that the 1st PC only reflects the feature that captures most of the variance of the data and, as such, it is difficult to have a-priori expectations on what it should represent. In the case of the data generated from the model, most of the variance of the activity comes from the switching signal, and similar considerations can be made for the looming stimulations in the data. We updated the manuscript to clarify this point.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The abstract makes it sound like a new finding is that habituation is due to a slow, negative feedback mechanism. But, as mentioned in the introduction, this is a well-known fact. 

      We agree with the reviewer. We have revised the abstract.

      (2) Figure 2c Why does the range of Delta Delta I_f include negative values if the corresponding region is shaded (right-tilted stripes)? 

      The negative values in the range are those attained in the shaded region with right-tilted stripes. We decided to include them in the colorbar for clarity, since Delta Delta I_f is also plotted in the region where it attains negative values.

      (3) What does the Pareto front look like if the optimization is done for input statistics given by ⟨H⟩_min? 

      In the revised version, we include examples of the Pareto optimization for different values of input strength. As detailed both in the main text and the Supplementary Information, changing the value of ⟨H⟩ moves the Pareto frontier in the (\beta, \sigma) space, since the strength of the signal is crucial for the system to discriminate input and thermal noise (see also the answers above).

      In particular, in Figure 4 we explicitly compare the results of the Pareto optimization (which is done with a static input of a given statistics) with the dynamics of the model for different values of ⟨H⟩ in two scenarios, i.e., adaptive and non-adaptive inhibition strength (see answers above for details).

      We also remark that ⟨H⟩_min represents the background signal that the system is not trying to capture, which is why we never used it for optimization.

      (4) From the main text, it is rather difficult to understand how the comparison to the experimental data was performed. How was the PCA done exactly? What are the "features" of the evoked neural response? 

      The PCA on data is performed starting from the single-neuron calcium dynamics. To perform a far comparison, we reconstruct a similar but extremely simplified dynamics using our model as explained in Methods to perform the PCA on analogous simulated data. We added a comment on this in the revised version. While these components capture most of the variance in the data, their specific interpretation is usually out of reach and we believe that it lies beyond the scope of this theoretical work. We also remark that the model does not contain all these biological details - a strong aspect in our opinion - and, as such, it cannot capture specific biological features.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The authors use a generic model framework to study the emergence of habituation and its functional role from information-theoretic and energetic perspectives. Their model features a receptor, readout molecules, and a storage unit, and as such, can be applied to a wide range of biological systems. Through theoretical studies, the authors find that habituation (reduction in average activity) upon exposure to repeated stimuli should occur at intermediate degrees to achieve maximal information gain. Parameter regimes that enable these properties also result in low dissipation, suggesting that intermediate habituation is advantageous both energetically and for the purpose of retaining information about the environment. 

      A major strength of the work is the generality of the studied model. The presence of three units (receptor, readout, storage) operating at different time scales and executing negative feedback can be found in many domains of biology, with representative examples well discussed by the authors (e.g. Figure 1b). A key takeaway demonstrated by the authors that has wide relevance is that large information gain and large habituation cannot be attained simultaneously. When energetic considerations are accounted for, large information gain and intermediate habituation appear to be a favorable combination. 

      We thank the reviewer for this positive assessment of our work and its generality.

      While the generic approach of coarse-graining most biological detail is appealing and the results are of broad relevance, some aspects of the conducted studies, the problem setup, and the writing lack clarity and should be addressed: 

      (1) The abstract can be further sharpened. Specifically, the "functional role" mentioned at the end can be made more explicit, as it was done in the second-to-last paragraph of the Introduction section ("its functional advantages in terms of information gain and energy dissipation"). In addition, the abstract mentions the testing against experimental measurements of neural responses but does not specify the main takeaways. I suggest the authors briefly describe the main conclusions of their experimental study in the abstract.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. In the revised version, we have changed the abstract to reflect the reviewer’s points and the new structure and results of the manuscript.

      (2) Several clarifications are needed on the treatment of energy dissipation. 

      -   When substituting the rates in Eq. (1) into the definition of δQ_R above Eq. (10), "σ" does not appear on the right-hand side. Does this mean that one of the rates in the lower pathway must include σ in its definition? Please clarify.

      We apologize to the reviewer for this typo. Indeed, \sigma sets the energy scale of feedback and, as such, it appears in the energetic driving given by the feedback on the receptor, i.e., in Eq. (1) together with \kappa. This typo has been corrected in the revised manuscript, and all subsequent equations are consistent.

      -   I understand that the production of storage molecules has an associated cost σ and hence contributes to dissipation. The dependence of receptor dissipation on ⟨H⟩, however, is not fully clear. If the environment were static and the memory block was absent, the term with ⟨H⟩ would still contribute to dissipation. What would be the nature of this dissipation?

      In the spirit of building a paradigmatic minimal model with a thermodynamic meaning, we considered H to act as an external thermodynamic driving. Since this driving acts on a different pathway with respect to the one affected by the storage, the receptor is driven out of equilibrium by its presence.

      By eliminating the memory block, we would also be necessarily eliminating the presence of the pathway associated with the storage effect (“internal pathway” in the manuscript), since its presence is solely due to the existence of a storage population. Therefore, in this case, the receptor would be a 2-state, 1-pathway system and, as such, it would always satisfy an effective detailed balance. As a consequence, the definition of \delta Q_R reported in the manuscript would not hold anymore and the receptor would not exhibit any dissipation. Thus, in a static environment and without a memory block, no receptor dissipation would be present. We would also like to stress that our choice to model two different pathways has been motivated by the observation that the negative feedback acts along a different pathway in several biochemical and biological examples. We made some changes to the model description in the revised version and we hope that this aspect has been clarified.

      -   Similarly, in Eq. (9) the authors use the ratio of the rates Γ_{s → s+1} and Γ_{s+1 → s} in their expression for internal dissipation. The first-rate corresponds to the synthesis reaction of memory molecules, while the second corresponds to a degradation reaction. Since the second reaction is not the microscopic reverse of the first, what would be the physical interpretation of the log of their ratio? Since the authors already use σ as the energy cost per storage unit, why not use σ times the rate of producing S as a metric for the dissipation rate? 

      We agree with the referee that the reverse reaction we considered is not the microscopic reverse of the storage production. In the case of a fast readout population, we employed a coarse-grained view to compute this entropy production. To be more precise, we gladly welcomed the referee’s suggestion in the revised version and modified the manuscript accordingly. As suggested, we now employ the energy flux associated with the storage production to estimate the internal dissipation (see new Fig. 3). 

      In the revised version, we also use this quantity in the optimization procedure in combination with \deltaQ_R (see new Fig. 4) to have a complete characterization of the system’s energy consumption. The conclusions are qualitatively identical to before, but we believe that now they are more solid from a theoretical perspective. For this important advance in the robustness and quality of our work, we are profoundly grateful to the referee.

      (3) Impact of the pre-stimulus state. The plots in Figure 2 suggest that the environment was static before the application of repeated stimuli. Can the authors comment on the impact of the pre-stimulus state on the degree of habituation and its optimality properties? Specifically, would the conclusions stay the same if the prior environment had stochastic but aperiodic dynamics? 

      The initial stimulus is indeed stochastic with an average constant in time and mimics the background (small) signal. We apply the (strong) stimulation when the system already reached a stationary state with respect to the background. As it can be appreciated in Fig. 2 of the revised version, the model response depends on the pre-stimulus level, since it sets the storage concentration before the stimulation arrives and, as such, the subsequent habituation dynamics. This dependence is important from a dynamical perspective. The information-theoretic picture has been developed, as said above, by letting the system relax before the first stimulus. This eliminates this arbitrary dependence and provides a clearer idea of the functional advantages of habituation. Moreover, the optimization procedure is performed in a completely different setting, with no pre-stimulus at all, since we only have one prolonged stimulation. We hope that the revised version is clearer on all these points.

      (4) Clarification about the memory requirement for habituation. Figure 4 and the associated section argue for the essential role that the storage mechanism plays in habituation. Indeed, Figure 4a shows that the degree of habituation decreases with decreasing memory. The graph also shows that in the limit of vanishingly small Δ⟨S⟩, the system can still exhibit a finite degree of habituation. Can the authors explain this limiting behavior; specifically, why does habituation not vanish in the limit Δ⟨S⟩ -> 0?

      We apologize for the lack of clarity and we thank the reviewer for spotting this issue. In Figure 4 (now Figure 5 in the revised manuscript) Δ⟨S⟩ is not exactly zero, but equal to 0.15% at the final point. It appeared as 0% in the plot due to an unwanted rounding in the plotting function that we missed. This has been fixed in the revised version, thank you.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Page 2 | "Figure 1b-e" should be "Figure 1b-d" since there is no panel (e) in Figure 1. 

      (2) Figure 1a | In the top schematic, the symbol "k" is used, while in the rest of the text, the proportionality constant is denoted by κ. 

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. Figure 1 has been revised and the panels are now consistent. The proportionality constant (the inhibition strength) has also been fixed.

      (3) Figure 1a | I find the upper part of the schematic for Storage hard to perceive. I understand the lower part stands for the degradation reaction for storage molecules. The upper part stands for the synthesis reaction catalyzed by the readout population. I think the bolded upper arrow would explain it sufficiently well; the left/right arrows, together with the crossed green circle make that part of the figure confusing. Consider simplifying. 

      We decided to remove the left/right arrows, as suggested by the reviewer, as we agree that they were unnecessarily complicating the schematic. We hope that the revised version will be easier to understand.

      (4)Page 3 | It would be helpful to tell what the temporal statistics of the input signal $p_H(h,t)$ is, i.e. <h(t) h(t')>. Looking at the example trajectory in Figure 1a, consecutive signal values do not seem correlated. 

      We agree with the reviewer that this is an important detail and worth mentioning. We now explicitly state that consecutive values are not correlated, for simplicity. 

      (5)Figure 2 | I believe the label "EXTERNAL INPUT" refers to the *average* external input, not one specific realization (similar to panels (d) and (e) that report on average metrics). I suggest you indicate this in the label, or, what may be even better, add one particular realization of the stochastic input to the same graph.

      We thank the reviewer for spotting this. We now write that what we show is the average external signal. We prefer this solution rather than showing a realization of the stochastic input, since it is more consistent with the rest of the plots, where we always show average quantities. We also note that Figure 2 is now Figure 3 in the revised manuscript.

      (6)Figure 2d | The expression of Δ⟨U⟩ is the negative of the definition in Eq. (5). It should be corrected. 

      In the revised version, both the definitions in Figure 2 (now Figure 3) and in the text (now Eq. (11)) are consistent.

      (7) Figure 3(d-e) caption | "where ⟨U⟩ starts to be significantly smaller than zero." There, it should be Δ⟨U⟩ instead of ⟨U⟩. 

      Thanks again, we corrected this typo.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      (1) It is a nice study but lacks some functional data required to determine how useful these alleles will be in practice, especially in comparison with the figure line that stimulated their creation.

      We are grateful for this comment. For the usefulness of these alleles, Figure 3 shows that specific and efficient genetic manipulation of one cell subpopulation can be achieved by mating across the DreER mouse strain to the rox-Cre mouse strain. In addition, Figure 7 shows that R26-loxCre-tdT can effectively ensure Cre-loxP recombination on some gene alleles and for genetic manipulation. The expression of the tdT protein is aligned with the expression of the Cre protein (Alb roxCre-tdT and R26-loxCre-tdT, Figure 2 and Figure 5), which ensures the accuracy of the tracing experiments. We believe more functional data can be shown in future articles that use mice lines mentioned in this manuscript.

      (2) The data in Figure 5 show strong activity at the Confetti locus, but the design of the newly reported R26-loxCre line lacks a WPRE sequence that was included in the iSure-Cre line to drive very robust protein expression.

      Thank you for bringing up this point in the manuscript. In the R26-loxCre-tdT mice knock-in strategy, the WPRE sequence is added behind the loxCre-P2A-tdT sequence, as shown in Supplementary Figure 9.

      (3) The most valuable experiment for such a new tool would be a head-to-head comparison with iSure (or the latest iSure version from the Benedito lab) using the same CreER and target foxed allele. At the very least a comparison of Cre protein expression between the two lines using identical CreER activators is needed.

      Thank you for your valuable and insightful comment. The comparison results of R26-loxCre-tdT with iSuRe-Cre using Alb-CreER and targeting R26-Confetti can be found in Figure 6, according to the reviewer’s suggestion.

      (4) Why did the authors not use the same driver to compare mCre 1, 4, 7, and 10? The study in Figure 2 uses Alb-roxCre for 1 and 7 and Cdh5-roxCre for 4 and 10, with clearly different levels of activity driven by the two alleles in vivo. Thus whether mCre1 is really better than mCre4 or 10 is not clear.

      Response: or two mCre versions that work efficiently. For example, if Alb-mCre1 was competitive with Cdh5-mCre10, we can use them for targeting genes in different cell types, broadening the potential utility of these mice.

      (5) Technical details are lacking. The authors provide little specific information regarding the precise way that the new alleles were generated, i.e. exactly what nucleotide sites were used and what the sequence of the introduced transgenes is. Such valuable information must be gleaned from schematic diagrams that are insufficient to fully explain the approach.

      Response: We appreciate your thoughtful suggestions. The schematic figures, along with the nucleotide sequences for the generation of mice, can be found in the revised Supplementary Figure 9.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      (1) The scenario where the lines would demonstrate their full potential compared to existing models has not been tested.

      Thank you for your thoughtful and constructive comment. The comparative analysis of R26-loxCre-tdT with iSuRe-Cre, employing Alb-CreER to target R26-Confetti, is provided in Figure 6.

      (2) The challenge lies in performing such experiments, as low doses of tamoxifen needed for inducing mosaic gene deletion may not be sufficient to efficiently recombine multiple alleles in individual cells while at the same time accurately reporting gene deletion. Therefore, a demonstration of the efficient deletion of multiple floxed alleles in a mosaic fashion would be a valuable addition.

      Thank you for your constructive comments. Mosaic analysis using sparse labeling and efficient gene deletion would be our future direction using roxCre and loxCre strategies.

      3) When combined with the confetti line, the reporter cassette will continue flipping, potentially leading to misleading lineage tracing results.

      Thank you for your professional comments. Indeed, the confetti used in this study can continue flipping, which would lead to potentially misleading lineage tracing results. Our use of R26-Confetti is to demonstrate the robustness of mCre for recombination. Some multiple-color mice lines that don’t flip have been published, for example, R26-Confetti2(PMID: 30778223) and Rainbow (PMID: 32794408). These reporters could be used for tracing Cre-expressing cells, without concerns of flipping of reporter cassettes.

      (4) Constitutive expression of Cre is also associated with toxicity, as discussed by the authors in the introduction.

      Thank you for your professional comments. The toxicity of constitutive expression of Cre and the toxicity associated with tamoxifen treatment in CreER mice line (PMID: 37692772) are known to the field. This study can’t solve the toxicity of the constitutive expression of Cre in this work. Many mouse lines with constitutive Cre driven by different promoters are present across various fields, representing similar toxicity. To solve this issue, it would be possible to construct a new strategy that enables the removal of Cre after its expression.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      (1) Although leakiness is rather minor according to the original publication and the senior author of the study wrote in a review a few years ago that there is no leakiness(https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbc.2021.100509).

      Thank you so much for your careful check. In this review (PMID: 33676891), the writer’s comments on iSuRe-Cre are on the reader's side, and all summary words are based on the original published paper (PMID: 31118412). Currently, we have tested iSuRe-Cre in our hands. We did detect some leakiness in the heart and muscle, but hardly in other tissues as shown in Author response image 1.

      Author response image 1.

      Leakiness in Alb CreER;iSuRe-Cre mouse line. Pictures are representative results for 5 mice. Scale bars, white 100 µm.

      (2) I would have preferred to see a study, which uses the wonderful new tools to address a major biological question, rather than a primarily technical report, which describes the ongoing efforts to further improve Cre and Dre recombinase-mediated recombination.

      Response: We gratefully appreciate your valuable comment. The roxCre and loxCre mice mentioned in this study provide more effective methods for inducible genetic manipulation in studying gene function. We hope that the application of our new genetic tools could help address some major biological questions in different biomedical fields in the future.

      (3) Very high levels of Cre expression may cause toxic effects as previously reported for the hearts of Myh6-Cre mice. Thus, it seems sensible to test for unspecific toxic effects, which may be done by bulk RNA-seq analysis, cell viability, and cell proliferation assays. It should also be analyzed whether the combination of R26-roxCre-tdT with the Tnni3-Dre allele causes cardiac dysfunction, although such dysfunctions should be apparent from potential changes in gene expression.

      We are sorry that we mistakenly spelled R26-loxCre-tdT into R26-roxCre-tdT in our manuscript. We have not generated the R26-roxCre-tdT mouse line. We also thank the reviewer for concerns about the toxicity of high Cre expression. The toxicity of constitutive expression of Cre and the toxicity of tamoxifen treatment of CreER mice line (PMID: 37692772) are known to the field. This study can’t solve the toxicity of the constitutive expression of Cre in this work. Many mouse lines with constitutive Cre driven by different promoters are present across various fields, representing similar toxicity. To solve this issue, it would be possible to construct a new strategy that enables the removal of Cre after its expression.

      (4) Is there any leakiness when the inducible DreER allele is introduced but no tamoxifen treatment is applied? This should be documented. The same also applies to loxCre mice.

      In this study, we come up with new mice tool lines, including Alb roxCre1-tdT, Cdh5 roxCre4-tdT, Alb roxCre7-GFP, Cdh5 roxCre10-GFP and R26-loxCre-tdT. As the data shown in Supplementary Figure 1, Supplementary Figure 2, and Figure 4D, Alb roxCre1-tdT, Cdh5 roxCre4-tdT, Alb roxCre7-GFP, Cdh5 roxCre10-GFP and R26-loxCre-tdT are not leaky. Therefore, if there is any leakiness driven by the inducible DreER or CreER allele, the leakiness is derived from the DreER or CreER. Additional pertinent experimental data can be referenced in Figure S4C, Figure S7A-B, and Figure S8A.

      (5) It would be very helpful to include a dose-response curve for determining the minimum dosage required in Alb-CreER; R26-loxCre-tdT; Ctnnb1flox/flox mice for efficient recombination.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We value your feedback and have incorporated your suggestion to strengthen our study. Relevant experimental data can be referenced in Figure S8E-G.

      (6) In the liver panel of Figure 4F, tdT signals do not seem to colocalize with the VE-cad signals, which is odd. Is there any compelling explanation?

      The staining in Figure 4F in the revision is intended to deliver optimized and high-resolution images.

      (7) The authors claim that "virtually all tdT+ endothelial cells simultaneously expressed YFP/mCFP" (right panel of Figure 5D). Well, it seems that the abundance of tdT is much lower compared to YFP/mCFP. If the recombination of R26-Confetti was mainly triggered by R26-loxCre-tdT, the expression of tdT and YFP/mCFP should be comparable. This should be clarified.

      Thank you so much for your careful check. We checked these signals carefully and didn't find the “much lower” tdT signal. As the file-loading website has a file size limitation, the compressed image results in some signal unclear. We attached clear high-resolution images here. Author response image 2 shows how we split the tdT signal and compared it with YFP/mCFP.

      Author response image 2.

      (8) In several cases, the authors seem to have mixed up "R26-roxCre-tdT" with "R26-loxCre-tdT". There are errors in #251 and #256.Furthermore, in the passage from line #278 to #301. In the lines #297 and #300 it should probably read "Alb-CreER; R26-loxCretdT; Ctnnb1flox/flox" rather than "Alb-CreER;R26-tdT2;Ctnnb1flox/flox".

      We are grateful for these careful observations. We have corrected these typos accordingly.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) However, for it to be useful to investigators a more direct comparison with the Benedito iSure line (or the latest version) is required as that is the crux of the study.

      Thank you for emphasizing this point, which we have now addressed in the revised manuscript and Figure 6.

      (2) I would like to know how the authors will make these new lines available to outside investigators.

      Please contact the lead author by email to consult about the availability of new mouse lines developed in this study.

      (3) The discussion is overly long and fails to address potential weaknesses. Much of it reiterates what was already said in the results section.

      We are thankful for your critical evaluation, which has helped us improve our discussion.

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) Assessing the efficiency and accuracy of the lines in mosaic deletions of multiple alleles and reporting them in single cells after low-dose tamoxifen exposure would be highly beneficial to demonstrate the full potential of the models.

      We appreciate your careful consideration of this issue. Our future endeavors will focus on mosaic analysis utilizing sparse labeling and efficient gene deletion, employing both roxCre and loxCre strategies.

      (2) Performing FACS analysis to confirm that all targeted (Cre reporter-positive) cells are also tdT-positive would provide more precise data and avoid vague statements like 'virtually all' or 'almost complete' in the results section:

      Line 166: Although mCre efficiently labeled virtually all targeted cells (Figure S3A-E)...

      Line 293: ... and not a single tdT+ hepatocyte 293 expressed Cyp2e1 (Figure 6D)... However, the authors do not provide any quantification. FACS would be ideal here.

      Line 244: ...expression of beta-catenin and GS almost disappeared in the 4W mutant sample... The resolution in the provided PDF is not adequate for assessment.

      Line 296: ... revealed almost complete deletion of Ctnnb1 in the Alb-CreER;R26-tdT2;Ctnnb1flox/flox mice...

      Thank you for suggesting these improvements, which have strengthened the robustness of our conclusions. In the revised version, we have incorporated FACS results that correspond to related sections. Additionally, a quantification statement has been included in the statistical analysis section. We appreciate your meticulous review and comments, which have significantly improved the clarity of our manuscript.

      (3) In the beginning of the results section, it is not clear which results are from this study and which are known background information (like Figure 1A). For example, it is not clear if Figure 1C presents data from R26-iSuRe-Cre. Please revise the text to more clearly present the experimental details and new findings.

      Thank you for this observation. Figure 1C belongs to this study, and the revised version has been modified to the related statement for improved clarity.

      (4) Experimental details regarding the genetic constructs and genotyping of the new knock-in lines are missing. Are R26 constructs driven by the endogenous R26 promoter or were additional enhancers used?

      Thank you for emphasizing this point. The schematic figures and nucleotide sequences for the generation of mice can be found in the revised Supplementary Figure 9, which can help to address this issue.

      (5) The method used to quantify mCre activity in terms of reporter+ target cells is not specified. From images or by FACS?

      Additionally, if images were used for quantification, it would be important to provide details on the number of images analyzed, the number of cells counted per image, and how individual cells were identified.

      Thank you for your comment. We have included the quantification statement in the statistical analysis section. Analyzing R26-Confetti+ target cells using FACS is challenging due to the limitations of the sorting instrument. Consequently, we quantified the related data by images. Each dot on the chart represents one sample, and the quantification for each mouse was conducted by averaging the data from five 10x fields taken from different sections.

      (6) Line 160: These data demonstrate that roxCre was functionally efficient yet non-leaky. Functional efficiency in vivo was not shown in the preceding experiments.

      Functional efficiency in vivo can be referred to in Figures S1-S2 and S4C.

      (7) It would be useful to provide a reference for easy vs low-efficiency recombination of different reporter alleles (lines 56-58).

      We are grateful for this comment, as it has allowed us to improve the clarity of our explanation. Consequently, we have made the necessary modifications.

      (8) Discussion on the potential drawbacks and limitations of the lines would be useful.

      We are thankful for your evaluation, which has significantly contributed to the enhancement of our discourse.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) Wnt3 cue and global PCP. PCP has been described in detail in a previous paper on Clytia (Momose et al, 2012): its orientation along the oral-aboral body axis (ciliary basal body positioning studies), and its function in directional polarity during gastrulation (Stbm-, Fz1-, and Dsh-MO experiments). I wonder if this part could be shortened. What is new, however, are the knockdown and Wnt3-mRNA rescue experiments, which provide a deeper insight into the link between Wnt3 function in the blastopore organiser as a source or cue for axis formation. These experiments demonstrate that the Wnt3 knockdown induces defects equivalent to PCP factor knockdown, but can be rescued by Wnt3-mRNA injection, even at a distance of 200 µm away from the Wnt-positive area. The experimental set-up of these new molecular experiments follows in important aspects those of Freeman's experiments of 1981 (who in turn was motivated to re-examine Teissier's work of 1931/1933 ...). Freeman did not use the term "global polarity" but the concept of an axis-inducing source and a long-range tissue polarity can be traced back to both researchers.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments for evolutionary biology and cnidarian developmental biology.

      Concerning the presentation of the basic PCP structure of Clytia embryo epidermal cells, we prefer to retain this section unless there is a strict limit on manuscript length. These experiments provide background information necessary to establish the biological system for the readers. The structures of cells, notably cell adhesion, cilia, and the cytoskeleton, are essential components of this system.

      We have restored sentences concerning the historical contributions of Freeman and Teissier from a previous version of the manuscript.

      Freeman’s work offered two key insights. The first is the concept that cell polarity spreads and self-organizes over the distances revealed by the tissue orientation of aggregate embryonic cells (Freeman, 1981 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00867804), which was termed “global polarity” in a review by Primus and Freeman (2004 https://doi.org/10.1002/bies.20031). This concept closely resembles the modern understanding of PCP coordination mechanisms mediated by core PCP interactions. Remarkably, Freeman proposed this idea in the early 1980s, at the same time of the first characterization of PCP mutants in Drosophila (Gubb and Garcia-Bellido 1982). The second is the role of egg polarity in defining the axis. Freeman demonstrated that the position of the first cleavage furrow predicts the oral-aboral axis by a series of sophisticated experiments. This was a milestone for the studies of cnidarian body axis development.

      However, some of Freeman’s interpretations were misleading. In the 1981 paper, he stated:

      "Polarity

      Other work that I have done has established that the anterior-posterior axis of the planula is set up at the time of the first cleavage; the site where cleavage is initiated specifies the posterior pole of this axis (Freeman 1980). The experiment reported here in which embryos were cut into halves and each half regulated to form a normal planula with the same polarity properties as the embryo it is from provides evidence that these polarity properties are remarkably stable at all developmental stages tested ranging from 4 cell to postgastrula embryos. "

      Freeman hypothesised that cell polarity at the 2- or 4-cell stage, referred to as the “polarity of first cell cleavage,” is directly inherited as the global polarity observed in later developmental stages.

      In the review by Primus and Freeman (2004), two hypotheses were introduced: (1) maternally localised factors, such as mRNA, determine the axis, and (2) cell polarity of cleavage furrow formation, is inherited to later stages and determines the axis. Freeman described these two hypotheses as mutually exclusive. However, we now know that cell polarity at early cleavage stages does not directly contribute to global polarity/PCP. Instead, Wnt/β-catenin signaling is regionally activated by maternally localised mRNAs distributed along the egg polarity (Momose, 2007; Momose, 2008), which maintain Wnt3 localisation and direct morphological axis patterning. Our study shown in this article unified these hypotheses.

      On the second point, as the reviewer noted, Freeman indeed revisited the work of Georges Teissier (Teissier, 1931), who conducted similar experiments on Amphisbetia embryos. It was Teissier who first described how the egg polarity is preserved in later stages and defines the axis. Teissier, however, carefully avoided asserting continuity between egg and blastula polarities, allowing for the possibility of “rétablissement” (re-establishment). As Teissier stated:

      "…On constate, en second lieu, que la polarité de l’œuf se conserve dans chacun de se fragment et que le maintien ou le rétablissement de cette polarité sont indispensables à un développement normal. Un fragment d’œuf ou de morula n’a aucune partie ni aucun blastomère qui soit rigoureusement déterminé comme endoderme, mais possède, par contre, un pôle antérieur et un pôle postérieur bien définis.…

      Mais cette proposition, qui ne semble pourtant guère dépasser la simple constatation des faits, soulève de grave difficulté. Elle donne en effet à la polarité, propriété encore bien mystérieuse, un rôle morphogénétique de premier ordre et implique des conséquences trop importantes pour qu’on puisse l’accepter sans un très sérieux examen.

      Comme je ne pense pas que les questions relatives à la nature des localisation germinales, à l’existence et au fonctionnement des organisateurs de l’œuf des Cœlentérés, puissant, dans l’état actuel de nos connaissances, être discutées utilement, je ne veux voir dans la proposition précédente qu’une façons commode et tout provisoire de systématiser les faits."

      English translation:

      “We note also that the polarity of the egg is preserved in each fragment and that the maintenance or re-establishment of this polarity is essential for normal development. A fragment of egg or morula has no part or blastomere that is rigorously determined as endoderm, but has, on the other hand, a well-defined anterior and posterior pole....

      But this proposition, which hardly seems to go beyond the simple observation of facts, raises serious difficulties. It gives polarity, still a mysterious property, a morphogenetic role of the first order, and implies consequences too important to be accepted without very serious examination.

      As I do not believe that questions concerning the nature of germinal localisation, or the existence and functioning of the egg organisers in Coelenterates, can, in the present state of our knowledge, be usefully discussed, I prefer only to see in the foregoing proposition a convenient and very provisional way of systematising the facts.”

      Teissier, G. (1931). Étude Expérimentale du Développement de Quelques Hydraires. Ann. Sc. Nat. Zool 14, 5–59.

      Teissier's interpretation and caution were reasonable.

      Our work connects recent molecular research on axis specification mechanisms in cnidarians with the classic experimental studies of Freeman and Teissier. We believe it is essential to present and acknowledge their conceptual contributions.  We have updated the Discussion to include these points.

      (2) PCP propagation and β-catenin. The central but unanswered question in this study focuses on the interaction between Wnt3 and PCP and the propagation of PCP. Wnt3 has been described in cnidarians but also in vertebrates and insects as a canonical Wnt interacting with β-catenin in an autocatalytic loop. The surprising result of this study is that the action of Wnt3 on PCP orientation is not inhibited in the presence of a dominant-negative form of CheTCF (dnTCF) ruling out a potential function of β-catenin in PCP. This was supported by studies with constitutively active β-catenin (CA-β-cat) mRNA which was unable to restore PCP coordination nor elongation of Wnt3-depleted embryos but did restore β-catenin-dependent gastrulation. Based on these data, the authors conclude that Wnt3 has two independent roles: Wnt/β-catenin activation and initial PCP orientation (two-step model for PCP formation). However, the molecular basis for the interaction of Wnt3 with the PCP machinery and how the specificity of Wnt3 for both pathways is regulated at the level of Wnt-receiving cells (Fz-Dsh) remain unresolved. Also, with respect to PCP propagation, there is no answer with respect to the underlying mechanisms. The authors found that PCP components are expressed in the mid-blastula stage, but without any further indication of how the signal might be propagated, e.g., by a wavefront of local cell alignment. Here, it is necessary to address the underlying possible cellular interactions more explicitly.

      The question of how Wnt3 interacts with the core PCP complex remains open for future investigation. An obvious hypothesis is that one of the Frizzled receptors binds Wnt3 ligands. For additional details, please refer to the response to Reviewer 2’s comment. Regarding other non-classic Wnt receptors, studies in the developing mouse limb have demonstrated that a Wnt5a gradient controls PCP polarisation via ROR receptors and graded Strabismus phosphorylation (Gao et al., 2011, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.devcel.2011.01.001). However, in this context, the Wnt5a gradient influences the frequency of polarised cells rather than PCP orientation. In Clytia, we performed gene knockdown experiments targeting ROR and RYK receptors using Morpholinos but did not observe any effect on axial patterning, suggesting that these receptors are unlikely to be involved in Wnt3 interaction.

      Concerning PCP propagation mechanisms, these are well-characterized in both Drosophila and vertebrates and conserved across taxa. The localised Fz-Fmi complex at the apical cortex of a cell interacts with the oppositely localised Stbm-Fmi complex in neighbouring cells, enabling coordination of PCP between directly adjacent cells. This interaction provides a comprehensive explanation for PCP propagation mechanisms. In Drosophila, the “domineering non-autonomy” effect is a well-documented phenomenon where PCP orientation autonomously propagates from core PCP mutant mosaic patches. Overall, PCP propagation is a conserved and robust mechanism across metazoans.

      (3) The proposed two-step model for PCP formation has important evolutionary implications in that it excludes the current alternate model according to which a long-range Wnt3-gradient orients PCP ("Wnt/β-catenin-first"). Nevertheless, the initial PCP orientation by Wnt3 - as proposed in the two-step model - is not explained at all on the molecular level. Another possible, but less well-discussed and studied option for linking Wnt3 with PCP action could be the role of other Wnt pathways. The authors present compelling evidence that Wnt3 is the most highly expressed Wnt in Clytia at all stages of development. The authors convincingly show that Wnt3 is the most highly expressed Wnt in Clytia at all stages of development (Figure S1). However, Wnt7 is also more highly expressed, which makes it a candidate for signal transduction from canonical Wnts to PCP Wnts. An involvement of Wnt7 in PCP regulation has been described in vertebrates (http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2013.12.026). This would challenge the entire discussion and speculation on the evolutionary implications according to which PCP Wnt signaling comes first (PCP-first scenario") and canonical Wnt signaling later in metazoan evolution.

      First of all, we apologise that the expression profile of Wnt7originally provided in Figure S1 was incorrect; Wnt7 is not expressed in the embryonic stage. The error came from the accession number XLOC_034538 assigned to two transcripts, Wnt7 and Ataxin10, in the published genome assembly. Once the expression profile is revised in this light, the data are consistent with the in situ hybridisation data published in Momose et al. (2012, https://doi.org/10.1242/dev.084251). Wnt3 is the only Wnt ligand detectable between egg and gastrula stages. We appreciate the reviewer highlighting this issue and have corrected Figure S1

      If we understand correctly, the reviewer raises the possibility that Wnt3's downstream canonical Wnt/β-catenin pathway activates the expression of other Wnt genes, which in turn orient the PCP. Indeed, we showed that the expression of Wnt1 (previously called WntX2), Wnt2 (WntX1A), Wnt5 and Wnt6 (Wnt9) all becomes undetectable at the planula stage following Wnt3-MO injection (Momose et al., 2012). So, it is a reasonable concern.

      This possibility can be excluded because the canonical pathway activation by CA-β-cat does not restore PCP in Wnt3-MO-injected embryos and Wnt3 can orient PCP without Wnt/β-catenin pathway activity in the presence of dominant negative TCF (dnTCF). Concerning Wnt1b and Wnt11b, these transcripts are maternally stored and even more abundant than Wnt3. However, we can conclude that these do not have any role in axis patterning based on the complete axis loss in Wnt3-MO morphants.

      Lastly, it should of course be remembered that the chronological order of characters appearing in a developmental process does not necessarily reflect their appearance in evolution from ancestral to modern.

      (4) The discussion, including Figure 6, is strongly biased towards the traditional evolutionary scenario postulating a choanzoan-sponge ancestry of metazoans. Chromosome-linkage data of pre-metazoans and metazoans (Schulz et al., 2023; https://doi.org/10(1038/s41586-023-05936-6) now indicate a radically different scenario according to which ctenophores represent the ancestral form and are sister to sponges, cnidarians and bilaterians (the Ctenophora-sister hypothesis). This has also implications for the evolution of Wnt signalling, as discussed in the recent Nature Genetics Review by Holzem et al. (2024) (https://doi.org/10.1038/s41576-024-00699-w). Furthermore, it calls into question the hypothesis of a filter-feeding multicellular gastrula-like ancestor as proposed by Haeckel (Maegele et al., 2023). These papers have not yet been referenced, but they would provide a more robust discussion.

      I overlooked the excellent work of Holzem and colleagues. I appreciate this suggestion. The work, unfortunately, focusses mainly on the Wnt/β-catenin pathway. The PCP pathway consists of not only core PCP (Fmi Stbm, Pk, Dgo, Fz and Dsh) but many other components, such as Rho GTPase, which are all dealt with as "PCP” in this review. While the full set of core PCP is present only in the phylum Cnidaria and bilaterians, Pk and Dgo are present in choanoflagellate and Rho GTPase or ROCK are present even in Fungi (Lapébie et al,  2011 DOI 10.1002/bies.201100023). Holzem et al., described PCP as absent in ctenophores, likely based on the lack of Fmi/Stbm, while claiming its presence in fungi based on Rho GTPase and ROCK. This led to their argument that the Wnt/β-catenin pathway is more ancestral, supported by the absence of PCP components in ctenophores alongside the ctenophore-sister hypothesis.

      This likely reflects the limited attention given to PCP in the metazoan evolutionary biology community. Our work sheds light on the importance of PCP regulation in metazoan evolution. In the revised Discussion, we emphasise this point together with the importance of cell biology studies in basal metazoans and compare them based on functional studies.

      The observation of Aiptasia’s predatory “gastrula-like” larvae is indeed fascinating. Understanding how early metazoan ancestors obtained nutrients is a key to uncovering the origins of metazoans. However, the relevance of this work to metazoan evolution remains unclear. Predatory nutrient uptake is common among cnidarians, and the findings of Maegele et al. could suggest that the predatory gastrula-like state is ancestral, with the symbiotic state being derived, within Cnidaria, but does not notably support it in metazoa. Also, it has to be clarified how predation is defined. Fundamentally, there is little distinction between filter-feeding and predatory feeding regarding heterotrophy; both feeding types require digestive machinery. If active feeding behaviour is the essence of predation, this would be better addressed as an evolutionary neurobiology or neuroscience question. Another mystery is what the metazoan ancestors took as food if they were predatory; there has to be a non-predatorial metazoan, as a food, already present before them.<br /> Overall, Maegele’s work seems premature to be incorporated into the metazoan evo-devo discussion. In either case, the standard approach would involve comparative studies across taxa. It will be interesting to see follow-up works on comparative and functional genomics of predatory/digestive machinery within phylum cnidaria and across metazoan, including sponge and ctenophores.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      We appreciate the reviewer’s expertise and recommendations regarding Wnt and PCP signalling. It would be our great pleasure if our work is seen and referenced by the cell biology community using model animals.

      (1) According to the 2-step model, one would expect that there is a temporal gradient in the spreading of the PCP from oral to aboral. Is there any indication for this?

      The best indication of a spatial and temporal gradient of PCP establishment observed so far is at the blastula stage (Fig.2B). PCP gradually becomes coordinated starting at 9 hpf, when PCP is slightly better organised close to the Wnt3-positive area (oral) compared to distal (aboral) areas. We did live imaging with tagged Poc1 to track the positions of centrioles in each cell (Fig. 2E), but this did not provide any further information about the spreading of the PCP. We hypothesise that there is a delay between PCP polarisation—established through the subcellular localisation of core PCP components—and its structural manifestation as ciliary positioning and orientation. This delay likely varies between cells, preventing the formation of a precise spatial PCP wave. We hope in the future to address this temporal aspect by live-imaging of core PCP proteins labelled with fluorescent proteins.

      (2) PCP is likely to be an all-or-nothing effect, while axial patterning is dose-dependent. is there a critical dose of Wnt3 level required to kick off the PCP pathway?

      We agree that the PCP phenotype is all-or-nothing.  Although we did not perform a quantitative test, we have not seen any intermediate phenotypes in Wnt3-rescue experiments. In our experimental condition (100 ng/µl mRNA), the Wnt3 mRNA injection into a blastomere consistently restores the body axis (via PCP) of Wnt3-MO injected embryos. No axis restoration was observed at 1 ng/µl. At 10 ng/µl, some embryos showed a restored elongated axis, while others showed no axis. The volume of injection is not precisely controllable and can easily vary two-fold, so we assume the limit is somewhere around 10 ng/µl. This contrasts with endoderm rescue via Wnt/β-catenin activation by GSK-β-inhibitors (alsterpaullone) or the constitutively active β-catenin (CA-β-cat), which occurs in a dose-dependent manner (ex. Supplementary Figure S2).

      (3) The key question left unaddressed is whether Wnt3 signals through one or two different Frizzled receptors? Which Frizzled receptors are candidates for this? Could they be knocked down to see which pathway (or both) is affected?

      How Wnt3 orientates the PCP system is an extremely interesting question that needs to be answered, and we plan to address this in the future. In Clytia, four Frizzled genes have been identified in the genome: CheFz1 (vertebrate counterpart of Fz1, 2, 3, 6 and 7), CheFz2 (Fz5 and 8), CheFz3 (Fz9/10) and CheFz4 (Fz4). Knockdown of CheFz1, hereby called Fz1, by Morpholino showed a PCP phenotype (Momose 2012, supplementary data). For a long time, we have suspected that the most likely candidate for PCP mediation is CheFz1. The Wnt3-rescue experiment in CheFz1-blocked background (similar experiment to Figure 3E, F) could potentially have answered this question. No PCP orientation would be expected even near the Wnt3-mRNA injected area if CheFz1 was the Wnt3 receptor for PCP orientation. Unfortunately, no reliable PCP phenotype was observed in this experiment, so this experiment was not included in the manuscript. We initially thought this was due to incomplete suppression of CheFz1 mRNA translation by the Morpholino when used at sub-toxic doses. But we now favour the alternative explanation that Fz1 does not mediate the Wnt3 signal responsible for initiating PCP orientation. We have previously shown that Fz1 is required for the Wnt/ β-catenin pathway (indicated by nuclear β-catenin localisation Momose 2007), which is then required to maintain Wnt3 expression. We cannot rule out that the PCP phenotype obtained previously following Fz1 knockdown (supplementary data in Momose 2012) is an indirect effect of Wnt3 downregulation.

      In future work, we plan to test the PCP involvement of the other Clytia Frizzleds, notably CheFz2 and CheFz4, which are not present as maternal mRNAs but are zygotically expressed in the early gastrula stage. CheFz3 is unlikely to be a candidate because it is aborally localised and acts as a negative receptor for the Wnt/β-catenin pathway (Momose 2007). Lastly, in unpublished experiments, no axial phenotype was obtained with ROR and RYK knockdown by Morpholino (T. Momose unpublished). 

      Based on these considerations, our current working hypothesis is that Wnt3 somehow stabilises or activates one of the Frizzled receptors acting as a core PCP protein in a polarised manner, likely at the oral side of each cell (Stbm is localised at the aboral side), which breaks the PCP symmetry and is propagated across the body axis.

      A few lines have been added to the discussion regarding this point.

      (4) Is there also PCP within the Wnt3 expressing domain? In other words, (and linked to question 2), does PCP require a certain concentration of Wnt3 or a gradient of Wnt3 in order to provide an orientation?

      In the context of a simple Wn3-MO rescue experiment, PCP is coordinated within the Wnt3-positive area. But this could be because PCP can propagate in both orientations, so it does not answer the question. In the Wnt3-rescue experiments in Fmi-MO and Stbm-MO embryos, PCP seemed better oriented close to the boundary between Wnt3-positive and -negative areas, in particular outside the Wnt3-positive area and rather uncoordinated deep in the middle of Wnt3-RNA positive area. 

      If Wnt3 expression is uniform across an embryo, as achieved by Wnt3-mRNA injection into the egg, the axis will be lost entirely (Momose 2008). We interpret these observations as indicating that Wnt3 expression "contrasts" (or steep gradients) act as the PCP orientation cue rather than a permissive manner.

      In normal development, mRNA expression detected by in situ hybridisation has a slight gradient, but we do not have any information about the endogenous protein distribution.

      We greatly appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments. A few sentences addressing points (2) and (4) have been added. The graphical models in Figures 4 and 6A have been updated. While these are relatively minor changes to the manuscript, they significantly impact future perspectives.

      Minor comments:

      (1) Labeling in some of the figures is too small and not legible, e.g. Figures 4E-H. Please check and improve.

      Agreed. Some labelling was way too small (2.5 points). This has been corrected. The minimum font size is now 6-point for most labelling in the revised Figures. 

      (2) Page 13: ...and allow us to novel scenarios for PCP-driven axis symmetry breaking... seems to lack the verb "propose"

      Corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Compelling and clearly described work that combines two elegant cell fate reporter strains with mathematical modelling to describe the kinetics of CD4+ TRM in mice. The aim is to investigate the cell dynamics underlying the maintenance of CD4+TRM.

      The main conclusions are that:

      (1) CD4+ TRM are not intrinsically long-lived.

      (2) Even clonal half-lives are short: 1 month for TRM in skin, and even shorter (12 days) for TRM in lamina propria.

      (3) TRM are maintained by self-renewal and circulating precursors.

      Strengths:

      (1) Very clearly and succinctly written. Though in some places too succinctly! See suggestions below for areas I think could benefit from more detail.

      (2) Powerful combination of mouse strains and modelling to address questions that are hard to answer with other approaches.

      (3) The modelling of different modes of recruitment (quiescent, neutral, division linked) is extremely interesting and often neglected (for simpler neutral recruitment).

      Weaknesses/scope for improvement:

      (1) The authors use the same data set that they later fit for generating their priors. This double use of the same dataset always makes me a bit squeamish as I worry it could lead to an underestimate of errors on the parameters. Could the authors show plots of their priors and posteriors to check that the priors are not overly-influential? Also, how do differences in priors ultimately influence the degree of support a model gets (if at all)? Could differences in priors lead to one model gaining more support than another?

      We now show the priors and posteriors overlaid in Figure S2. The posteriors lie well within the priors, giving us confidence that the priors are not overly influential.

      (2) The authors state (line 81) that cells were "identified as tissue-localised by virtue of their protection from short-term in vivo labelling (Methods; Fig. S1B)". I would like to see more information on this. How short is short term? How long after labelling do cells need to remain unlabelled in order to be designated tissue-localised (presumably label will get to tissue pretty quickly -within hours?). Can the authors provide citations to defend the assumption that all label-negative cells are tissue-localised (no false negatives)?

      And conversely that no label-positive cells can be found in the tissue (no false positives)? I couldn't actually find the relevant section in the methods and Figure S1B didn't contain this information.

      We did describe the in vivo labeling in the first section of Methods (it was for 3 mins before sacrifice). The two aims of Fig S1B were to show the gating strategy (label-positive and negatives from tissue samples were clearly separated) and to address the false-positive issue. Less than 3% of cells in our tissue samples were positive; therefore, at most 3% of truly tissue-resident cells acquired the i.v. label, and likely less. Excluding those (as we did) therefore makes little difference to our analyses in terms of cell numbers. False negative rates are expected to be extremely low; labeling within circulating cells is typically >99% (see refs in Methods).

      (3) Are the target and precursor populations from the same mice? If so is there any way to reflect the between-individual variation in the precursor population (not captured by the simple empirical fit)? I am thinking particularly of the skin and LP CD4+CD69- populations where the fraction of cells that are mTOM+ (and to a lesser extent YFP+) spans virtually the whole range. Would it be nice to capture this information in downstream predictions if possible?

      This is a great point. We do indeed isolate all populations from each mouse. We are very aware of the advantages of using this grouping of information to reduce within-mouse uncertainty – we employ this as often as we can. The issue here was that the label content within the tissue (target) at any time depends on the entire trajectory of the label frequency in the precursor, in that mouse, up to that point. We can’t identify this curve for each animal individually – so we are obliged to use a population average.

      To mitigate this lack of pairing we do take a very conservative approach and fit this empirical function describing the trajectories of YFP and mTom in precursors at the same time as the label kinetics in the target; that is, we account for uncertainty in label influx in our fits and parameter estimates.

      Another issue is that to be sure that we are performing model selection appropriately, we only use the distribution of the likelihood on the target observations when comparing support for different precursors with LOO-IC. If we had been able to pair the precursor and target data in some way, the two would then be entangled and model comparison across precursors would not be possible.

      We’ve added some of this to the discussion.

      (4) In Figure 3, estimates of kinetics for cells in LP appear to be more dependent on the input model (quiescent/neutral/division-linked) than the same parameters in the skin. Can the authors explain intuitively why this is the case?

      This is a nice observation and it has a fairly straightforward explanation. As we pointed out in the paper, estimated rates of self renewal become more sensitive to the mode of recruitment the greater the rate of influx. If immigrants are quiescent, all Ki67 in the tissue has to be explained by self renewal. If all new immigrants are Ki67 high, the estimate of the rate of self renewal within the tissue will be lower. Across the board, the estimated rates of influx into gut were consistently higher than those in skin, and so the sensitivity of parameters to the mode of recruitment was much more obvious at that site.

      The importance of this trade-off for the division linked model can also be seen when you look at the neutral and quiescent models; they give similar parameter estimates because the Ki67 levels within all precursor populations were all less than 25% and so those two modes of recruitment are difficult to distinguish.

      (5) Can the authors include plots of the model fits to data associated with the different strengths of support shown in Figure 4? That is, I would like to know what a difference in the strength of say 0.43 compared with 0.3 looks like in "real terms". I feel strongly that this is important. Are all the fits fantastic, and some marginally better than others? Are they all dreadful and some are just less dreadful? Or are there meaningful differences?

      This is another good point (and from the author recommendations list, is your most important concern).

      We find that a fairly common issue is that models that are clearly distinguished by information criteria or LRTs can often give visually quite similar fits. Our experience is that this is partly due to the fact that models are usually fit on transformed scales (e.g. log for cell counts, logit for fractions) to normalise residuals, and this uncertainty is compressed when one looks at fits on the observed scale (e.g. linear). Another issue in our case is that for each model (precursor, target, and mode of recruitment) we fit 6 time courses simultaneously. Visual comparisons of fits of different models can then be a little difficult or misleading; apparently small differences in each fitted timecourse can add up to quite significant changes in the combined likelihood. We added this to the Discussion.

      The number of models is combinatorial (Fig. 4) so showing them all seems a bit cumbersome. But now in the supporting information (Fig. S3), for each target we show the best, second best, and the worst model fits overlaid, to give a sense of the dynamic range of the models we considered. As you will now see, visual differences among the most strongly supported models were not huge (but refer to our point just above). Measures of out-of-sample prediction error (LOO-IC) discriminated between these models reasonably well, though (weights shown in Fig. 4).

      It’s also worth mentioning here that we have substantially greater confidence in the identity of the precursors than in the precise modes of recruitment - you can see this clearly in the groupings of weights in Figure 4A. We did comment on this in the text but now emphasise it more.

      (6) Figure 4 left me unclear about exactly which combinations of precursors and targets were considered. Figure 3 implies there are 5 precursors but in Figure 4A at most 4 are considered. Also, Figure 4B suggests skin CD69- were considered a target. This doesn't seem to be specified anywhere.

      Thanks for pointing this out. When we were considering CD4+ EM in bulk as target, this population includes CD69- cells; in those fits, therefore, we couldn't use CD69- as a precursor. We now clarify this in the caption. Thanks also for the observation about Figure 4B; we didn’t consider CD69- cells as a target, so we’ve also made that clearer.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This manuscript addresses a fundamental problem of immunology - the persistence mechanisms of tissue-resident memory T cells (TRMs). It introduces a novel quantitative methodology, combining the in vivo tracing of T-cell cohorts with rigorous mathematical modeling and inference. Interestingly, the authors show that immigration plays a key role in maintaining CD4+ TRM populations in both skin and lamina propria (LP), with LP TRMs being more dependent on immigration than skin TRMs. This is an original and potentially impactful manuscript. However, several aspects were not clear and would benefit from being explained better or worked out in more detail.

      (1) The key observations are as follows:

      a) When heritably labeling cells due to CD4 expression, CD4+ TRM labeling frequency declines with time. This implies that CD4+ TRMs are ultimately replenished from a source not labeled, hence not expressing CD4. Most likely, this would be DN thymocytes.

      That’s correct.

      b) After labeling by Ki67 expression, labeled CD4+ TRMs also decline - This is what Figure 1B suggests. Hence they would be replaced by a source that was not in the cell cycle at the time of labeling. However, is this really borne out by the experimental data (Figure 2C, middle row)? Please clarify.

      (2) For potential source populations (Figure 2D): Please discuss these data critically. For example, CD4+ CD69- cells in skin and LP start with a much lower initial labeling frequency than the respective TRM populations. Could the former then be precursors of the latter?

      A similar question applies to LN YFP+ cells. Moreover, is the increase in YFP labeling in naïve T cells a result of their production from proliferative thymocytes? How well does the quantitative interpretation of YFP labeling kinetics in a target population work when populations upstream show opposite trends (e.g., naïve T cells increasing in YFP+ frequency but memory cells in effect decreasing, as, at the time of labeling, non-activated = non-proliferative T cells (and hence YFP-) might later become activated and contribute to memory)?

      These are good (and related) points. We've added some text to the discussion, paragraphs 2 and 3; we reproduce it here, slightly expanded.

      Fig 1B was a schematic but did faithfully reflect the impact of any waning of YFP in precursor on its kinetic in the targets. However, in our experiments, as you noted, the kinetics of YFP in most of the precursor populations were quite flat. This was due in part to memory subsets being sustained by the increasing levels of YFP within naïve cells from the cohort of thymocytes labeled during treatment. There is also likely some residual permanent labeling of lymphocyte progenitor populations. We discussed this in Lukas Front Imm 2023. (The latter is not a problem; all that matters for our analysis is that we generate a reasonable empirical description of the label kinetics in naive cells, however it arises). YFP is therefore not cleanly washed out in the periphery; and so for models with circulating memory as the tissue precursor, the flatness of their YFP curves leads to rather flat curves in the tissues.

      The mTom labelling was more informative as it was clearly diluted out of all peripheral populations by mTom-negative descendants of thymically-derived cells, as you point out in (a).

      Regarding (2), re: interpreting the initial levels of labels in precursors and targets. The important point here is that YFP and mTom were induced quickly in all populations we studied; therefore our inferences regarding precursors and targets aren’t informed by the initial levels of levels in each. (Imagine a slow precursor feeding a rapidly dividing target; YFP levels in the former would start lower than those in the latter). The causal issue that we think you’re referring to would matter if one expects the targets to begin with no label at all; for instance, in our busulfan chimeric mouse model (e.g. Hogan PNAS 2015) new, thymically derived ‘labelled’ (donor) cells progressively infiltrate replete ‘unlabelled’ (host) populations. In that case, one can immediately reject certain differentiation pathways by looking the sequence of accrual of donor cells in different subsets.

      The trends in YFP and mTom frequencies after treatment do matter for pathway inference, though, because precursor kinetics must leave an imprint on the target. For the case you mentioned, with opposite trends in label kinetics, such models would unlikely to be supported strongly; indeed, we never saw strong support for naïve cells (strongly increasing YFP) as a direct precursor of TRM (fairly flat).

      We’ve added a condensed version of this to the Discussion.

      (3) Please add a measure of variation (e.g., suitable credible intervals) to the "best fits" (solid lines in Figure 2).

      Added.

      (4) Could the authors better explain the motivation for basing their model comparisons on the Leave-OneOut (LOO) cross-validation method? Why not use Bayesian evidence instead?

      Bayes factors are very sensitive to priors and are either computationally unstable if calculated with importance sampling methods, or very expensive to calculate, if ones uses the more stable bridge sampling method. (We also note that fitting just a single model here takes a substantial amount of time). Further, using BF can be unreliable unless one of the models is close to the 'true' data generating model; though they seem to work well, we can be sure that none of our models are! For us, a more tractable and real-world selection criterion is based on the usefulness of a model, for which predictive performance is a reasonable proxy. In this case the mean out-of-sample prediction error (which LOO-IC reflects) is a wellestablished and valid means of ascribing support to different models.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      Wang et al. identify Hamlet, a PR-containing transcription factor, as a master regulator of reproductive development in Drosophila. Specifically, the fusion between the gonad and genital disc is necessary for the development of continuous testes and seminal vesicle tissue essential for fertility. To do this, the authors generate novel Hamlet null mutants by CRISPR/Cas9 gene editing and characterize the morphological, physiological, and gene expression changes of the mutants using immunofluorescence, RNA-seq, cut-tag, and in-situ analysis. Thus, Hamlet is discovered to regulate a unique expression program, which includes Wnt2 and Tl, that is necessary for testis development and fertility. 

      Strengths: 

      This is a rigorous and comprehensive study that identifies the Hamlet-dependent gene expression program mediating reproductive development in Drosophila. The Hamlet transcription targets are further characterized by Gal4/UAS-RNAi confirming their role in reproductive development. Finally, the study points to a role for Wnt2 and Tl as well as other Hamlet transcriptionally regulated genes in epithelial tissue fusion. 

      We appreciate that the reviewer thinks our study is rigorous.

      Weaknesses: 

      The image resolution and presentation of figures is a major issue in this study. As a nonexpert, it is nearly impossible to see the morphological changes as described in the results. Quantification of all cell biological phenotypes is also lacking therefore reducing the impact of this study to those familiar with tissue fusion events in Drosophila development. 

      In the revised version, we have improved the image presentation and resolution. For all the images with more than two channels, we included single-channel images, changed the green color to lime and the red to magenta, highlighted the testis (TE) and seminal vescicles to make morphological changes more visible.  

      We had quantification for marker gene expression in the original version, and now also included quantification for cell biological phenotypes which are generally with 100% penetrance.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Strengths: 

      Wang and colleagues successfully uncovered an important function of the Drosophila PRDM16/PRDM3 homolog Hamlet (Ham) - a PR domain-containing transcription factor with known roles in the nervous system in Drosophila. To do so, they generated and analyzed new mutants lacking the PR domain, and also employed diverse preexisting tools. In doing so, they made a fascinating discovery: They found that PR-domain containing isoforms of ham are crucial in the intriguing development of the fly genital tract. Wang and colleagues found three distinct roles of Ham: (1) specifying the position of the testis terminal epithelium within the testis, (2) allowing normal shaping and growth of the anlagen of the seminal vesicles and paragonia and (3) enabling the crucial epithelial fusion between the seminal vesicle and the testis terminal epithelium. The mutant blocks fusion even if the parts are positioned correctly. The last finding is especially important, as there are few models allowing one to dissect the molecular underpinnings of heterotypic epithelial fusion in development. Their data suggest that they found a master regulator of this collective cell behavior. Further, they identified some of the cell biological players downstream of Ham, like for example E-Cadherin and Crumbs. In a holistic approach, they performed RNAseq and intersected them with the CUT&TAG-method, to find a comprehensive list of downstream factors directly regulated by Ham. Their function in the fusion process was validated by a tissue-specific RNAi screen. Meticulously, Wang and colleagues performed multiplexed in situ hybridization and analyzed different mutants, to gain a first understanding of the most important downstream pathways they characterized, which are Wnt2 and Toll. 

      This study pioneers a completely new system. It is a model for exploring a process crucial in morphogenesis across animal species, yet not well understood. Wang and colleagues not only identified a crucial regulator of heterotypic epithelial fusion but took on the considerable effort of meticulously pinning down functionally important downstream effectors by using many state-of-the-art methods. This is especially impressive, as the dissection of pupal genital discs before epithelial fusion is a time-consuming and difficult task. This promising work will be the foundation future studies build on, to further elucidate how this epithelial fusion works, for example on a cell biological and biomechanical level. 

      We appreciate that the reviewer thinks our study is orginal and important.

      Weaknesses: 

      The developing testis-genital disc system has many moving parts. Myotube migration was previously shown to be crucial for testis shape. This means, that there is the potential of non-tissue autonomous defects upon knockdown of genes in the genital disc or the terminal epithelium, affecting myotube behavior which in turn affects fusion, as myotubes might create the first "bridge" bringing the epithelia together. The authors clearly showed that their driver tools do not cause expression in myoblasts/myotubes, but this does not exclude non-tissue autonomous defects in their RNAi screen. Nevertheless, this is outside the scope of this work. 

      We thank the reviewer’s consideration of non-tissue autonomous defects upon gene knockdown. The driver, hamRSGal4, drives reporter gene expression mainly in the RS epithelia, but we did observe weak expression of the reporter in the myoblasts before they differentiate into myotubes. Thus, we could not rule out a non-tissue autonomou effect in the RNAi screen. So we now included a statement in the result, “Given that the hamRSGal4 driver is highly expressed in the TE and SV epithelia, we expect highly effective knockdown occurs only in these epithelial cells. However, hamRSGal4 also drives weak expression in the myoblasts before they differentiated into myotubes (Supplementary Fig. 5B), which may result in a non-tissue autonomous effect when knocking down the candidate genes expressed in myoblasts.”

      However, one point that could be addressed in this study: the RNAseq and CUT&TAG experiments would profit from adding principal component analyses, elucidating similarities and differences of the diverse biological and technical replicates. 

      Thanks for the suggestion. We now have included the PCA analyses in supplementary figure 6A-B and the corresponding description in the text. The PCA graphs validated the consistency between biological replicates of the RNA-seq samples. The Cut&Tag graphs confirm the consistency between the two biological replicates from the GFP samples, but show a higher variability between the w1118 replicates. Importantly, we only considered the overlapped peaks pulled by the GFP antibody from the ham_GFP genotype and the Ham antibody from the wildtype (w1118) sample as true Ham binding sites. 

      Recommendations for the authors:  

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Major Concern: 

      (1) The image resolution and presentation of figures (Figures 2, 5, 6, and 7) is a major issue in this study. As a non-expert, it is nearly impossible to see the morphological changes as described in the results. Images need to be captured at higher resolution and zoomed in with arrows denoting changes as described. Individual channels, particularly for intensity measurement need to be shown in black and white in addition to merged images. Images also need pseudo-colored for color-blind individuals (i.e. no red-green staining). 

      The images were captured at a high resolution, but somehow the resolution was drammaticlly reduced in the BioRxiv PDF. We try to overcome this by directly submitting the PDF in the Elife submission system. In the revised version, we have included single-channel images, changed the green and red colors to lime and magenta for color blindness. We also highlighted the testis (TE) and seminal vescicle structures in the images to make morphological changes more visible.  

      (2) The penetrance of morphological changes observed in RT development is also unclear and needs to be rigorously quantified for data in Figures 2, 5, and 7. 

      We now included quantification for cell biological phenotypes which are generally with 100% penetrance. The percentage of the penetrance and the number of animals used are indicated in each corresponding image.  

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Major Points 

      (1) Lines 193- 220 I would strongly suggest pointing out the obvious shape defects of the testes visible in Figure 2A ("Spheres" instead of "Spirals"). These are probably a direct consequence of a lack in the epithelial connection that myotubes require to migrate onto the testis (in a normal way) as depicted in the cartoons, allowing the testis to adopt a spiral shape through myotube-sculpting (Bischoff et al., 2021), further confirming the authors' findings! 

      Good point. In the revised text, we have added more description of the testis shape defects and pointed out a potential contribution from compromised myotube migration.   

      (2) Line 216: "Often separated from each other". Here it would be important to mention how often. If the authors cannot quantify that from existing data, I suggest carrying it out in adult/pharate adult genital tracts (if there is no strong survivor bias due to the lethality of stronger affected animals), as this is much easier than timing prepupae. This should be a quick and easy experiment. 

      Because it is hard to tell whether the separation of the SV and TE was caused by developmental defects or sometimes could be due to technical issues (bad dissection), we now change the description to, “control animals always showed connected TE and SV, whereas ham mutant TE and SV tissues were either separated from each other, or appeared contacted but with the epithelial tubes being discontinuous (Fig. 2B).” Additionally, we quantified the disconnection phenotype, which is 100% penetrance in 18 mutant animals. This quantification is now included in the figure. 

      (3) Lines 289-305, Figure 3. I could only find how many replicates were analyzed in the RNAseq/CUT&Tag experiments in the Material & Methods section. I would add that at least in the figure legends, and perhaps even in the main text. Most importantly, I would add a Principal Component Analysis (one for RNAseq and one for the CUT&TAG experiment), to demonstrate the similarity of biological replicates (3x RNaseq, 4x Cut&Tag) but also of the technical replicates (RNAseq: wt & wt/dg, ham/ham & ham/df, GD & TE; CUT&TAG: Antibody & GFP-Antibody, TG&TE...). This should be very easy with the existing data, and clearly demonstrate similarities & differences in the different types of replicates and conditions. 

      Principle component analysis and its description are now added to Supplementary Fig 6 and the main text respectively. 

      (4) Line 321; Supplementary Table 1: In the table, I cannot find which genes are down- or upregulated - something that I think is very important. I would add that, and remove the "color" column, which does not add any useful information. 

      In Supplementary table 1, the first sheet includes upregulated genes while the second sheet includes downregulated genes. We removed the column “color” as suggested.  

      (5) Line 409: SCRINSHOT was carried out with candidate genes from the screen. One gene I could not find in that list was the potential microtubule-actin crosslinker shot. If shot knockdown caused a phenotype, then I would clearly mention and show it. If not, I would mention why a shot is important, nonetheless. 

      shot is one of the candidate target genes selected from our RNA-seq and Cut&Tag data. However, in the RNAi screen, knocking down shot with the available RNAi lines did not cause any obvious phenotype. These could be due to inefficient RNAi knockdown or redundancy with other factors. We anyway wanted to examine shot expression pattern in the developing RS, give the important role of shot in epithelial fusion (Lee S., 2002). Using SCRINSHOT, we could detect epithelial-specific expression of shot, implying its potential function in this context. We now revised the text to clarify this point. 

      Minor points 

      (1) Cartoons in Figure 1: The cartoons look like they were inspired by the cartoon from Kozopas et al., 1998 Fig. 10 or Rothenbusch-Fender et al., 2016 Fig 1. I think the manuscript would greatly profit from better cartoons, that are closer to what the tissue really looks like (see Figure 1H, 2G), to allow people to understand the somewhat complicated architecture. The anlagen of the seminal vesicles/paragonia looks like a butterfly with a high columnar epithelium with a visible separation between paragonia/seminal vesicles (upper/lower "wing" of the "butterfly"). Descriptions like "unseparated" paragonia/seminal vesicle anlagen, would be much easier to understand if the cartoons would for example reflect this separation. It would even be better to add cartoons of the phenotypic classes too, and to put them right next to the micrographs. (Another nitpick with the cartoons: pigment cells are drastically larger and fewer in number (See: Bischoff et al., 2021 Figure 1E & MovieM1).) 

      Thanks for the suggestion. We have updated Figure 1 by adding additional illustrations showing the accessory gland and seminal vesicle structures in the pupal stage and changing the size of pigment cells.

      (2) Line 95-121 I would also briefly introduce PR domains, here. 

      We have added a brief descripition of the PR domains.

      (3) Line 152, 158, 160, 162. When first reading it, I was a bit confused by the usage of the word sensory organ. I would at least introduce that bristles are also known as external mechanosensory organs. 

      We have now revised the description to “mechano-sensory organ”.

      eg. Line 184, 194, and many more. Most times, the authors call testis muscle precursors "myoblasts". This is correct sometimes, but only when referring to the stage before myoblast-fusion, which takes place directly before epithelial fusion (28 h APF). Postmyoblast-fusion (eg. during migration onto the testis), these cells should be called myotubes or nascent myotubes, as the fly muscle community defined the term myoblast as the singlenuclei precursors to myotubes. 

      We have now revised the description accordingly.  

      (4) Line 217/Figure 2B. It looks like there is a myotube bridge between the testis and the genital disc. I would point that out if it's true. If the authors have a larger z-stack of this connection, I suggest creating an MIP, and checking if there are little clusters of two/three/four nuclei packed together. This would clearly show that the cells in between are indeed myotubes (granted that loss of ham does not introduce myoblast-fusion-defects). 

      We do not have a Z-stack of this connection, and thus can not confirm whether the cells in this image are myotubes. However, we found that mytubes can migrate onto the testis and form the muscular sheet in the ham mutant despite reduced myotube density. At the junction there are myotubes, suggesting that loss of ham does not introduce myoblast-fusion defects. These results are now included in the revised manuscript, supplementary Fig. 5 C-D.

      (5) Line 231/Supplementary Fig. 3C-G: I would add to the cartoons, where the different markers are expressed. 

      We have added marker gene expression in the cartoons.

      (6) Line 239. I don't see what Figure 1A/1H refers to, here. I would perhaps just remove it. 

      Yes, we have removed it.

      (7) Line 232. I would rephrase the beginning of the sentence to: Our data suggest Ham to be... 

      Yes, we have revised it.

      (8) Line 248-250/Figure 2F. Clonal analyses are great, but I think single channels should be shown in black and white. Also, a version without the white dashed line should be shown, to clearly see the differences between wt and ham-mutant cells. 

      Now single channel images from the green and red images are presented in Supplementary Figures. This particular one is in Supplementary Figure 3B. 

      (9) Line 490. The Toll-9 phenotype was identified on the sterility effect/lack-of-spermphenotype alone, and it was deduced, that this suggests connection defects. By showing the right focus plane in Fig S8B (lower right), it should be easy to directly show whether there is a connection defect or not. Also, one would expect clearer testis-shaping defects, like in ham-mutants, as a loss of connection should also affect myotube migration to shape the testis. This is just a minor point, as it only affects supplementary data with no larger impact on the overall findings, even if Toll-9 is shown not to have a defect, after all. 

      We find that scoring defects at the junction site at the adult stage is difficult and may not be always accurate. Instead, we score the presence of sperms in the SV, which indirectly but firmly suggests successful connection between the TE and SV. We have now included a quantification graph, showing the penetrance of the phentoype in the new Supplementary Fig.14C. There were indeed morphological defects of TE in Toll-9 RNAi animals. We now included the image and quantification in the new Supplementary Fig.14B.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Response to the public reviews:

      We are very pleased to see these positive reviews of our preprint.

      Reviewers 1 and 3 raise issues around PIP-PP1 interactions.

      (1) Role of the “RVxF-ΦΦ-R-W string”

      Most PIPs interact with the globular PP1 catalytic core through short linear interaction motifs (SLiMs) and Choy et al (PNAS 2014) previously showed that many PIPs interact with PP1 through conserved trio of SLiMs, RVxF-ΦΦ-R, which is also present in the Phactrs.

      Previous structural analysis showed the trajectory of the PPP1R15A/B, Neurabin/Spinphilin (PPP1R9A/B), and PNUTS (PPP1R10) PIPs across the PP1 surface encompasses not only the RVxF-ΦΦ-R trio, but also additional sequences C-terminal to it (Chen et al, eLife, 2015). This extended trajectory is maintained in the Phactr1-PP1 complex (Fedoryshchak et al, eLife (2020). Based on structural alignment we proposed the existence of an additional hydrophobic “W” SLiM that interacts with the PP1 residues I133 and Y134.

      The extended “RVxF-ΦΦ-R-W” interaction brings sequences C-terminal to the “W” SLiM into the vicinity of the hydrophobic groove that adjoins the PP1 catalytic centre. In the Phactr1/PP1 complex, these sequences remodel the groove, generating a novel pocket that facilitates sequence-specific substrate recognition.

      This raises the possibility that sequences C-terminal to the extended “RVxF-ΦΦ-R-W string” in the other complexes also confer sequence-specific substrate recognition, and our study aims to test this hypothesis. Indeed, the hydrophobic groove structures of the Neurabin/Spinophilin/PP1 and Phactr1/PP1 complexes differ significantly (Ragusa et al, 2010; see Fedoryshchak et al 2020, Fig2 FigSupp1).

      (2) Orientation of the W side chain

      Reviewer 1 points out that in the substrate-bound PP1/PPP1R15A/Actin/eIF2 pre-dephosphorylation complex the W sidechain is inverted with respect to its orientation in  PP1-PPP1R15B complex (Yan et al, NSMB 2021). The authors proposed that this may reflect the role of actin in assembly of the quaternary complex. This does not necessarily invalidate the notion that sequences C-terminal to the “W” motif might play a role in actin-independent substrate recognition, and we therefore consider our inclusion of the R15A/B fusions in our analysis to be reasonable.

      (3) Conservation of W

      The motif ‘W’ does not mandate tryptophan - Phactrs and PPP1R15A/B indeed have W at this position but Neurabin/spinophilin contain VDP, which makes similar interactions. Similarly the “RVxF” motifs in Phactr1, Neurabin/Spinophilin, PPP1R15A/B and PNUTS are LIRF, KIKF, KV(R/T)F and TVTW respectively.

      In our revision, we will present comparisons of the differentially remodelled/modified PP1 hydrophobic groove in the various complexes, discuss the different orientations of the tryptophan in the previously published PPP1R15A/PP1 and PPP1R15B/PP1 structures. We will also address the other issues raised by the referees.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Comments and suggestions for revisions

      (1) The authors do not provide strong evidence that the interactions of the 'W' of the RVxF- øø -R-W string with the hydrophobic groove of PP1 is conserved in PIPs. Whereas the RVxF motif is well conserved and validated since its discovery in 1997, as are the øø - (an extension of the RVxF motif), and the 'R', the conservation of the Trp residue in the RVxF-øø-R-W string is not conserved.

      We did not mean to imply that the W motif is conserved amongst all PIPs.

      Most PIPs interact with the globular PP1 catalytic core through short linear interaction motifs (SLiMs). Choy et al (PNAS 2014) previously showed that many PIPs interact with PP1 through a conserved trio of SLiMs, RVxF-ΦΦ-R, which is also present in the Phactrs.

      Previous structural analysis showed that the PPP1R15A/B, Neurabin/Spinophilin (PPP1R9A/B), and PNUTS (PPP1R10) PIPs share a trajectory across the PP1 surface that encompasses not only the RVxF-ΦΦ-R SLIMs, but also additional sequences C-terminal to the R SLIM (Chen et al, eLife, 2015). This trajectory is also shared by the Phactr1-PP1 complex (Fedoryshchak et al, eLife, 2020). Based on this structural alignment we proposed the existence of an additional hydrophobic “W” SLiM that interacts with the PP1 residues I133 and Y134 (See Fedoryshchak et al, 2020, Figure 1 figure supplement 2).

      Introduction, paragraph 2 is rewritten to make this clearer.

      The sequence and positions of W differ in amino acid type and position relative to the RVxF-øø-R string.

      The motif ‘W’ does not mandate tryptophan, it is our name for a common structurally aligned motif: although the Phactrs and PPP1R15A/B indeed have W at this position, Neurabin and spinophilin contain VDP, which nevertheless makes similar interactions. Similarly the _“_RVxF” motifs in Phactr1, Neurabin/Spinophilin, PPP1R15A/B and PNUTS are LIRF, KIKF, KV(R/T)F and TVTW respectively.

      In the Discussion the authors state that the hydrophobic groove of PP1 is remodelled by Neurabin. However, details of this are not described or shown in the manuscript.

      The shared trajectory determined by the RVxF-øø-R-W string brings the sequences C-terminal to the W SLIM into the vicinity of the PP1 hydrophobic groove. In the Phactr1/PP1 holoenzyme this generates a novel pocket required for substrate recognition (Fedoryshchak et al, 2020). These observations raised the possibility that sequences C-terminal to the “W” motif in the other RVxF-øø-R-W PIPs also play a role in substrate recognition.

      Introduction paragraph 3 now cites a new Figure 1-S2, which shows how the hydrophobic groove is remodelled in the various different PIP/PP1 complexes. A revised Figure 1A now indicates the hydrophobic residues defining the hydrophobic groove by grey shading.

      (2) To add to the confidence of the structure, the authors should include a 2Fo-Fc simulated annealing omit map, perhaps showing the R and W interactions of the RVxF-øø-R-W string.

      This is now included as new Figure 6 Figure supplement 1. Note that in Neurabin, the W motif is VDP, where the valine and proline sidechains interact similarly to the tryptophan (see also new Figure 1-S2G,H).

      We also add a new supplementary Figure 6-S1 comparing our PBM-liganded Neurabin PDZ domain with the previously published unliganded structure (Ragusa et al 2010).

      (3) Page 16. The authors state that spinophilin remodels the PP1 hydrophobic groove differently from Phactrs. Arguably spinophilin does not remodel the PP1 hydrophobic groove at all. There are no contacts between spinophilin and the PP1 hydrophobic groove in the spinophilin-PP1 structure, correlating with the absence of 'W" in the RVxF-øø-R-W string in spinophilin.

      The VDP sequence corresponding to the W motif in spinophilin and neurabin makes analogous contacts to those made by the W in Phactr1 (see Fedoryshchak et al 2020).

      Remodelling is meant in the sense of altering the structure of the major groove by bringing new sequences into its vicinity rather than necessarily directly interacting with it. The spinophilin/PP1 and Phactr/PP1 hydrophobic grooves are compared in new Figure 1-S2 (see also Fedoryshchak et al 2020, Figure 2 figure supplement 1)

      (4) Page 8. For the cell-based/proteomics-dephosphorylation assay in Figure 2, it isn't clear why there were no dephosphorylation sites detected for the PPP1R15A/B-PP1 fusion (except PPP6R1 S531 for PPP1R15B). One might have expected a correlation with PP1 alone. Does this imply that PPP1R15A/B are inhibiting PP1 catalytic activity? Was the activity tested in vitro?

      The R15A/B data are compared to average abundance of all the phosphosites in the dataset, including those of PP1.

      We have not tested for a general inhibitory effect of R15A/B on PP1 activity. Many PIPs including R15A/B do occlude one or more of the PP1 substrate groove and therefore generally act as inhibitors of PP1 activity against some potential substrates, while enhancing activities against others.

      Other points 

      (4) Figure S1: Colour sequence similarities/identities.

      Done

      (6) Figures: Structure figures lacked labels:

      Figure 1A, label PP1, Phactrs etc.

      Done

      Figure 6, label PP1, Neurabin, previous Neurabin structure (Fig. 6C), hydrophobic groove, PDZ domain, etc.

      Done

      (7) Statistical analysis. p values should be shown for data in:

      Figure 5.

      To avoid cluttering the Figure, a new sheet, “statistical significance” has been added to Supplementary Table 3, summarizing the analysis.

      Figure 1.

      Figure amended (now figure 1-S1).

      (8) Some inconsistency with labels, eg '34-WT' used in Fig. 5C, whereas '34A-WT' (better) in Methods.

      Now changed to 34A etc where used.

      (9) Page 6. PPP1R9A/B is not shown in Figure 1A and Figure S1A.

      PPP1R9A/B are Neurabin and spinophilin - now clarified in Introduction paragraph 2, Results paragraph 1, Discussion paragraph 1.

      (10) Page 7: lines 4, 'site' not 'side'.

      Done

      (11) Page 9: DTL and CAMSAP3 were found to be dephosphorylated in the PP1-Neurabin/spinophilin screen. Are these PDZ-binding proteins?

      Neither DTL nor CAMSAP3 contain C-terminal hydrophobic residues characteristic of classical PBMs. Sentence added in Discussion, paragraph 5

      (12) Page 12 and Figure 5 and S5: The synthetic p4E-BP1 and IRSp53WT peptides with PBM should be given more specific names to indicate the presence of the PBM.

      We have renamed 4E-BP1<sup>WT</sup> and IRSp53<sup>WT</sup> to 4E-BP1<sup>PBM</sup> and  IRSp53<sup>PBM</sup> respectively, emphasising the inclusion of the wildtype or mutated PBM from 4E-BP1 on these peptides.

      Text, Figure 5, and Figure S5 all revised accordingly.

      (13) Give PDB code for spinophilin-PP1 complex coordinates shown in Figure 6C.

      PDB codes for the various PIP/PP1 complexes now given in new Figure 1-S2 and revised Figure 6C.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The work undertaken by the authors is extensive and robust, however, I believe that some improvement in the writing and some detailed explanation of certain results sections would help with the presentation of the work and clarity for the readers.

      (1) The introduction should contain more information about the interaction between PP1 and Neurabin, given that this is the focus of the paper. This would give the reader the necessary background required to follow the paper.

      Introduction paragraph 2 revised to describe the different SLIMs in more detail. New Figure 1-S2 shows detail of the different remodelled hydrophobic grooves in the various PIP/PP1 complexes.

      (2) More information on PP1-IRSp53L460A has to be added before discussing results in S1B.

      Sentence explaining that IRSp53 L460 docks with the remodelled PP1 hydrophobic groove in the Phactr1/PP1 holoenzyme added in Results paragraph 2.

      (3) Page 6: "as expected, the +5 residue L460A mutation, which impairs dephosphorylation by the intact Phactr1/PP1 holoenzyme, impaired sensitivity to all the fusions, indicating that they recognise phosphorylated IRSp53 in a similar way (Figure S1B)". Statistics between IRSp53 and IRSp53L460A across PP1-PIPs need to be conducted before concluding the above. From the graph and the images, the impairment to dephosphorylation is not convincing.

      For each of the four PP1-Phactr fusions, the IRSp53 L460A peptide shows significantly less reactivity than the IRSp53WT peptide (p<0.05 for each fusion).

      Since the proteomics studes in Figure 2 show that the substrate specificity of the four PP1-Phactr1 fusions is virtually identical, we combined the data for the four different fusions. The IRSp53 L460A peptide shows significantly less reactivity than the IRSp53WT peptide in this analysis (p< 0.0001). This result shown in revised Figure S1B and legend.

      (4) mCherry-4E-BP1(118+A), in which an additional C-terminal alanine should still allow TOSmediated phosphorylation, but prevent PDZ interaction. Does 4EBP1 (118+A) actually prevent interaction between PP1-Neurabin? This interaction needs to be validated, especially since spinophilin was shown to bind to multiple regions of PP1.

      It is not clear what the referee is asking for here. The biochemical analysis in Figure 4C shows that the C-terminus of 4E-BP1 constitutes a classical PBM. The X-ray crystallography in Figure 6 confirms this, demonstrating H-bond interactions between the 4E-BP1 C-terminal carboxylate and main chain amides of L514, G515 and I516.

      We consider the possibility that the 4E-BP1(118+A) mutant inhibits the activity of PP1-neurabin via a mechanism other than direct blocking 4E-BP1 / PDZ interaction to be unlikely for the following reasons:

      (1) Addition of a C-terminal alanine will disrupt the PBM interaction because the extra residue sterically blocks access to the PBM-binding groove. This is the most parsimonious explanation, and is based on our solid structural and biochemical evidence that the 4E-BP1 C-terminus is a classical PBM.

      (2) Alphafold3 modelling predicts Neurabin PDZ / 4E-BP1 PBM interaction with high confidence (shown in Figure 6-S2E), but it does not predict any PDZ interaction with 4E-BP1(118+A). Note added in Figure 6-S2 legend.

      (3) Recognition of the 4E-BP1(118+A) mutation without loss of binding affinity would require that the mutant becapable of binding formally equivalent to recognition of an “internal” PDZ-binding peptide. Recognition of such “internal peptides” is dependent on their adopting a specifically constrained conformation, which typically requires reorganisation of the PDZ carboxylate-binding GLGF loop. Such “internal site” recognition typically involves more than one residue C-terminal to the conventional PDZ “0” position (see Penkert et al NSMB 2004, doi:10.1038/nsmb839; Gee et al JBC 1998, DOI: 10.1074/jbc.273.34.21980; Hillier et al 1999, Science PMID: 10221915).

      (5) It is nice to see that the various PP1-Phactr fusions have around 60% substrate overlap between them. Would it be possible to compare these results with previously published mass spec data of Phactr1XXX from the group? There is mention of some substrates being picked up, but a comparison much like in Figure 2E would be more informative about the extent to which the described method captures relevant information.

      This is difficult to do directly as the PP1-Phactr fusion data are from human cells while that in Fedoryshchak et al 2020 is from mouse.

      However, manual curation shows that of the 28 top hits seen in our previous analysis of Phactr1XXX in NIH3T3 cells, 18 were also detectable in the HEK293 system; of these, 13 were also detected as as PP1-Phactr fusion hits. Data summarised in new Figure 2-S1C. Text amended in Results, “Proteomic analysis...”, paragraph 2.

      (6) Figure 3D Why are the levels of pT70, pT37/46 and total protein in vector controls much lower as compared to 0nM Tet in PP1-Neurabin conditions? It is also weird that given total protein is so low, why are the pS65/101 levels high compared to the rest?

      We think it likely these phenomena reflect a low level expression of PP1-Neurabin expression in uninduced cells. Now noted in Figure 3D legend, basal PP1-Neurabin expression shown in new Figure 3-S1C. This alters the relative levels of the different species detected by the total 4E-BP1 antibody in favour of the faster migrating forms, which are less phosphorylated than the slower ones, and the total amount increases about 2-fold (Figure 3D, compare 0nM Tet lanes).

      The altered p65/101-pT70 ratio is also likely to reflect the leaky PP1-Neurabin expression, since the relative intensities of the various phosphorylated species are dependent on both the relative rates of phosphorylation and dephosphorylation. Expression of a phosphatase would therefore be expected to differentially affect the phosphorlyation levels of different sites according to their reactivity.

      (7) Figure 3E: Does inhibiting mTORC further reduce translation when PP1-Neurabin is expressed? If this is the case, this might suggest that they might not necessarily be mTORC inhibitors?

      We have not done this experiment. Since Rapamycin cannot be guaranteed to completely block 4E-BP1 phosphorylation, and PP1-Neurabin cannot be guaranteed to completely dephosphorylate 4E-BP1, any further reduction upon their combination would be hard to interpret.

      (8) Substrate interactions with the remodelled PP1 hydrophobic groove do not affect PP1-Neurabin specificity. Is there evidence that PP1-Neurabin remodels the hydrophobic groove? Is it not possible that Neurabin does not remodel the PP1 groove to begin with and hence there is no effect observed with the various mutants? If this is not the case, it should be explained in a bit more detail.

      Comparison of the Neurabin/PP1 and Phactr1/PP1 structures shows that the hydrophobic groove is remodelled differently in the two complexes. Now shown in new Figure 1-S2B,C,G.

      (9) Figure 5B has a lot of interesting information, which I believe has not been discussed at all in the results section.

      To help interpretation of the enzymology in Figure 5 we have renamed 4E-BP1WT and IRSp53WT to 4E-BP1PBM and IRSp53PBM respectively, emphasising the inclusion of the wildtype or mutated PBM from 4E-BP1 on these peptides. Text in Results, “PDZ domain interaction…”, paragraph 1, and Figures 5 and S5 revised accordingly.

      Why does the 4E-BP1Mut affect catalytic efficiency of PP1 alone when compared with WT, while no difference is observed with IRSp53WT and mutant?

      We do not understand the basis for the differential reactivity of 4E-BP1PBM and 4E-BP1MUT with PP1 alone; we suspect that it reflects the hydrophobicity change resulting from the MDI -> SGS substitution. However this is unlikely to be biologically significant as PP1 is sequestered in PIP-PP1 complexes.

      Importantly, the two PP1 fusion proteins behave consistently in this assay – the presence of the intact PBM increases reactivity with PP1-Neurabin, but has no effect on dephosphorylation by PP1-Phactr1.

      Why does PP1 alone not have a difference between IRSp53WT and mutant, while PP1-Neurabin does have a difference?

      This is due to the presence of the PBM in IRSp53WT (now renamed IRSp53PBM), which affects increases affinity for PP1 Neurabin, but not PP1 alone. Likewise, PP1-Phactr1, which does not possess a PDZ domain, is also unaffected by the integrity of the PBM.

      (7) “Strikingly, alanine substitutions at +1 and +2 in 4E-BP1WT increased catalytic efficiency by both fusions, perhaps reflecting changes at the catalytic site itself (Figure 5E, Figure S5E)”. This could be expanded upon, because this suggests a mechanism that makes the substrate refractory to PDZ/hydrophobic groove remodelling?

      We favour the idea that this reflects a requirement to balance dephosphorylation rates between the multiple 4E-BP1 phosphorylation sites, especially if multiple rounds of dephosphorylation occur for each PBM—PDZ interaction. Additional sentences added in Discussion paragraph 7.

      (8) Typographical errors and minor comments:

      a) PIPs can target PP1 to specific subcellular locations, and control substrate specificity through autonomous substrate-binding domains, occupation or extension of the substrate grooves, or modification of PP1 surface electrostatics.

      b) Phosphophorylation side site abundances within triplicate samples from the same cell line were comparable between replicates (Figure 2B).

      c) While the alanine substitutions had little effect, conversion of +4 to +6 to the IRSp534E-BP1 sequence LLD increased catalytic efficiency some 20-fold (Figure 5C, Figure S5C). 

      d) Figure 3E labels are not clear. The graph can be widened to make the labels of the conditions clearer.

      All corrected

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      This was a very well-written manuscript.

      However, I was looking for a summary mechanistic figure or cartoon to help me navigate the results.

      I noted a few typos in the text.

      New summary Figure 5-S2 added, cited in results, and discussed in Discussion paragraph 6,7.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This article presents a meta-analysis that challenges established abundance-occupancy relationships (AORs) by utilizing the largest known bird observation database. The analysis yields contentious outcomes, raising the question of whether these findings could potentially refute AORs.

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive comments.

      Strengths:

      The study employed an extensive aggregation of datasets to date to scrutinize the abundance-occupancy relationships (AORs).

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive comments.

      Weaknesses:

      While the dataset employed in this research holds promise, a rigorous justification of the core assumptions underpinning the analytical framework is inadequate. The authors should thoroughly address the correlation between checklist data and global range data, ensuring that the foundational assumptions and potential confounding factors are explicitly examined and articulated within the study's context.

      We thank the Reviewer for these comments. We agree that more justification and transparency is needed of the core assumptions that form the foundation of our methods. In our revised version, we have taken the following steps to achieve this:

      - Altered the title to be more explicit about the core assumptions, which now reads: “Local-scale relative abundance is decoupled from global range size”

      - We have added more details on why and how we treat global range size as a measure of ‘occupancy.’

      - We have added a section that discusses the limitations of using eBird relative abundance

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The goal is to ask if common species when studied across their range tend to have larger ranges in total. To do this the authors examined a very large citizen science database which gives estimates of numbers, and correlated that with the total range size, available from Birdlife. The average correlation is positive but close to zero, and the distribution around zero is also narrow, leading to the conclusion that, even if applicable in some cases, there is no evidence for consistent trends in one or other direction.

      We thank the Reviewer for these comments.

      Strengths:

      The study raises a dormant question, with a large dataset.

      We thank the Reviewer for these comments. We intended to take a longstanding question and attempt to apply novel datasets that were not available mere decades ago. While we do not imply that we have ‘solved’ the question, we hope this work highlights the potential for further interrogation using these large datasets.

      Weaknesses:

      This study combines information from across the whole world, with many different habitats, taxa, and observations, which surely leads to a quite heterogeneous collection.

      We agree that there is a heterogeneous collection of data across many habitats, taxa, and observations. However, rather than as a weakness, we see this as a significant strength. Our work assumes we are averaging over this variability to assess for a large-scale pattern in the relationship - something that was potentially a limitation of previous work, as these large datasets were often focused on particular contexts (e.g., much work focused solely on the UK), which we believe could limit some of the generalizability of the previous work. However, the reviewer makes a fair point in regard to the heterogeneity of data collection. We have now added some text in the discussion which is explicit about this - see the new section named “Potential limitations of current work and future work –-although our findings challenge some long-held assumptions about the consistency of the abundance-occupancy relationship, our work only deals with interspecific AORs among birds, synthesizing observations of potentially heterogeneous locations, context and quality”.

      First, scale. Many of the earlier analyses were within smaller areas, and for example, ranges are not obviously bounded by a physical barrier. I assume this study is only looking at breeding ranges; that should be stated, as 40% of all bird species migrate, and winter limitation of populations is important. Also are abundances only breeding abundances or are they measured through the year? Are alien distributions removed?

      Second, consider various reasons why abundance and range size may be correlated (sometimes positively and sometimes negatively) at large scales. Combining studies across such a large diversity of ecological situations seems to create many possibilities to miss interesting patterns. For example:

      (1) Islands are small and often show density release.

      See comment below.

      (2) North temperate regions have large ranges (Rapoport's rule) and higher population sizes than the tropics.

      See comment below.

      (3) Body size correlates with global range size (I am unsure if this has recently been tested but is present in older papers) and with density. For example, cosmopolitan species (barn owl, osprey, peregrine) are relatively large and relatively rare.

      See comment below.

      (4) In the consideration of alien species, it certainly looks to me as if the law is followed, with pigeon, starling, and sparrow both common and widely distributed. I guess one needs to make some sort of statement about anthropogenic influences, given the dramatic changes in both populations and environments over the past 50 years.

      See comment below. We also added a sentence in the methods that highlighted we did not remove alien ranges and provided reasons why. Still, we do acknowledge the dramatic changes in populations and environments over the past 50 years (see the new section  “Potential limitations of current work and futur work”)

      (5) Wing shape correlates with ecological niche and range size (e.g. White, American Naturalist). Aerial foraging species with pointed wings are likely to be easily detected, and several have large ranges reflecting dispersal (e.g. barn swallow).

      We agree that all of the points above are interesting data explorations. As said above, our main purpose was to highlight the potential for further interrogation using these large datasets. However, we have added some additional text in the discussion that explicitly mentions/encourages these additional data explorations. We hope people will pick up on the potential for these data and explore them further.

      Third, biases. I am not conversant with ebird methodology, but the number appearing on checklists seems a very poor estimate of local abundance. As noted in the paper, common species may be underestimated in their abundance. Flocking species must generate large numbers, skulking species few. The survey is often likely to be in areas favorable to some species and not others. The alternative approach in the paper comes from an earlier study, based on ebird but then creating densities within grids and surely comes with similar issues.

      We agree that if we were interested in the absolute abundance of a given species, the local number on an eBird checklist would be a poor representation. However, our study aims not to estimate absolute abundance but to examine relative abundance among species on each checklist. By focusing on relative abundance, we leverage eBird data's strengths in detecting the presence and frequency of species across diverse locations and times, thereby capturing community composition trends that can provide meaningful insights despite individual checklist biases. This approach allows us to assess the comparative prominence of species in the community as reported by the observer, providing a consistent metric of relative abundance. Despite detectability biases, the structure of eBird checklists reflects the observer’s encounter rates with each species under similar conditions, offering a valuable snapshot of relative species composition across sites and times. The key to our assumption is that these biases discussed are not directional and, therefore, random throughout the sampling process, which would translate to no ‘real’ bias in our effect size of interest.

      Range biases are also present. Notably, tropical mountain-occupying species have range sizes overestimated because holes in the range are not generally accounted for (Ocampo-Peñuela et al., Nature Communications). These species are often quite rare, too.

      We thanks the reviewer for pointing to this issue and reference. We included a discussion on these biases in our limitations section and reference Ocampo-Peñuela et al. to emphasize the need for improved spatial resolution in range data for more accurate AOR assessments.”More precise range-size estimates would also improve the accuracy of AOR assessments, since species range data are often overestimated due to the failure to capture gaps in actual distributions ”

      Fourth, random error. Random error in ebird assessments is likely to be large, with differences among observers, seasons, days, and weather (e.g. Callaghan et al. 2021, PNAS). Range sizes also come with many errors, which is why occupancy is usually seen as the more appropriate measure.

      If we consider both range and abundance measurements to be subject to random error in any one species list, then the removal of all these errors will surely increase the correlation for that list (the covariance shouldn't change but the variances will decrease). I think (but am not sure) that this will affect the mean correlation because more of the positive correlations appear 'real' given the overall mean is positive. It will definitely affect the variance of the correlations; the low variance is one of the main points in the paper. A high variance would point to the operation of multiple mechanisms, some perhaps producing negative correlations (Blackburn et al. 2006).

      We agree random errors can affect estimates, but as we wrote above, random errors, regardless of magnitudes, would not bias estimates. After accounting for sampling error (a part of random errors), little variance is left to be explained as we have shown in the MS. This suggests that many of the random errors were part of the sampling errors. And this is where meta-analysis really shines.

      On P.80 it is stated: "Specifically, we can quantify how AOR will change in relation to increases in species richness and sampling duration, both of which are predicted to reduce the magnitude of AORs" I haven't checked the references that make this statement, but intuitively the opposite is expected? More species and longer durations should both increase the accuracy of the estimate, so removing them introduces more error? Perhaps dividing by an uncertain estimate introduces more error anyway. At any rate, the authors should explain the quoted statement in this paper.

      It would be of considerable interest to look at the extreme negative and extreme positive correlations: do they make any biological sense?

      Extremely high correlations would not make any biological sense if these observations were based on large sample sizes. However, as shown in Figure 2, all extreme correlations come from small sample sizes (i.e., low precision), as sampling theory expects (actually our Fig 2 a text-book example of the funnel shape). Therefore, we do not need to invoke any biological explanations here.

      Discussion:

      I can see how publication bias can affect meta-analyses (addressed in the Gaston et al. 2006 paper) but less easily see how confirmation bias can. It seems to me that some of the points made above must explain the difference between this study and Blackburn et al. 2006's strong result.

      We agree. Now, we extended an explanation of why confirmation bias could result in positive AOR. Yet, we point out confirmation bias is a very common phenomena which we cite relevant citations in the original MS. The only way to avoid confirmation bias is to conduct a study blind but this is not often possible in ecological work.

      “Meta-research on behavioural ecology identified 79 studies on nestmate recognition, 23 of which were conducted blind. Non-blind studies confirmed a hypothesis of no aggression towards nestmates nearly three times more often. It is possible that confirmation bias was at play in earlier AOR studies.”

      Certainly, AOR really does seem to be present in at least some cases (e.g. British breeding birds) and a discussion of individual cases would be valuable. Previous studies have also noted that there are at least some negative and some non-significant associations, and understanding the underlying causes is of great interest (e.g. Kotiaho et al. Biology Letters).

      We agree. And yes, we pointed out these in our introduction.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This paper claims to overturn the longstanding abundance occupancy relationship.

      Strengths:

      (1) The above would be important if true.

      (2) The dataset is large.

      We have clarified this point by changing the title to emphasize that we do not suggest overturning AORs entirely but instead provide a refined view of the relationship at a global scale. Our results suggest a weaker and more context-dependent AOR than previously documented. We hope our revised title and additional clarifications in the text convey our intent to contribute to a more nuanced understanding rather than a whole overturning of the AOR framework.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The authors are not really measuring the abundance-occupancy relationship (AOR). They are measuring abundance-range size. The AOR typically measures patches in a metapopulation, i.e. at a local scale. Range size is not an interchangeable notion with local occupancy.

      We have refined this in our revision to be more explicitly focused on global range size. However, we note that the classic paper by Bock and Richlefs (1983, Am Nat) also refers to global (species entire) range size in the context of the AOR. Importantly, Bock and Richlefs pointed out the importance of using species’ entire ranges; without such uses, there will be sampling artifacts creating positive AORs when using arbitrary geographical ranges, which were used in some studies of AORs. So we highlight that our work is well in line with the previous work, allowing us to question the longstanding macroecological work. One of the issues of AOR has been how to define occupancy and global range size, which provides a relatively ambiguous measure, which is why we used this measure.

      (2) Ebird is a poor dataset for this. The sampling unit is non-standard. So abundance can at best be estimated by controlling for sampling effort. Comparisons across space are also likely to be highly heterogenous. They also threw out checklists in which abundances were too high to be estimated (reported as "X"). As evidence of the biases in using eBird for this pattern, the North American Breeding Bird Survey, a very similar taxonomic and geographic scope but with a consistent sampling protocol across space does show clear support for the AOR.

      Yes, we agree the sampling unit is non-standard. However, this is a significant strength in that it samples across much heterogeneity (as discussed in response to Reviewer 2, above). We were interested in relative abundance and not direct absolute abundance per se, which is accurate, especially since we did control for sampling effort.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s attention to our data selection criteria. We excluded checklists containing ‘X’ entries to minimize biases in our abundance estimates. The 'X' notation is often used for the most common species, reflecting the observer's identification of presence without specifying a count. This approach was chosen to avoid disproportionately inflating presence data for these abundant species, which could distort the relative abundance calculations in our analysis. By excluding such checklists, we aimed to retain consistency and ensure that local abundance estimates were representative across all species on each checklist. We have revised our manuscript to clarify this methodological choice and hope this explanation addresses the reviewer’s concern. We modified our text in the methods to make the entries ‘X’ clearer (see the Method section).

      (3) In general, I wonder if a pattern demonstrated in thousands of data sets can be overturned by findings in one data set. It may be a big dataset but any biases in the dataset are repeated across all of those observations.

      Overturning a major conclusion requires careful work. This paper did not rise to this level.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s caution regarding broad conclusions based on a single dataset, even one as large as eBird. Our intention was not to definitively overturn the abundance-occupancy relationship (AOR) but to re-evaluate it with the most extensive and globally representative dataset currently available. We recognise that potential biases in citizen science data, such as observer variation, may influence our findings, and we have taken steps to address these in our methodology and limitations sections. We see this work as a contribution to an ongoing discourse, suggesting that AOR may be less universally consistent than previously believed, mainly when tested with large-scale citizen science data. We hope this study will encourage additional research that tests AORs using other expansive datasets and approaches, further refining our understanding of this classic macroecological relationship. However, we have left our broad message about instigating credible revolution and also re-examining ecological laws.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The investigation focuses solely on interspecific relationships among birds; thus, the extrapolation of these conclusions to broader ecological contexts requires further validation.

      We have now added this point to our new section: “Although our findings challenge some long-held assumptions about the consistency of the abundance-occupancy relationship, our work only deals with interspecific AORs among birds, so we hope this work serves as a foundation for further investigations that utilize such comprehensive datasets.”

      (2) The rationale for combining data from eBird - a platform predominantly representing individual observations from urban North America - with the more globally comprehensive BirdLife International database needs to be substantiated. The potential underrepresentation of global abundance in the eBird checklist data could introduce a sampling bias, undermining the foundational premises of AORs.

      We agree with the limitation of ebird sampling coverage, but it should not bias our results. In statistical definitions, bias is directional, and if not directional, it will become statistical noise, making it difficult to detect the signal. In fact, our meta-analyses adjust what statisticians call sampling bias and it is the strength of meta-analysis.

      (3) In the full mixed-effect model, checklist duration and sampling variance (inversely proportional to sample size N) are treated as fixed effects. However, these variables are likely to be negatively correlated, which could introduce multicollinearity, inflating standard errors and diminishing the statistical significance of other factors, such as the intercept. This calls into question the interpretation of insignificance in the results.

      Multicollinearity is an issue with sample sizes. For example, with small datasets, correlations of 0.5 could be an issue, and such an issue would usually show up as a large SE. We do not have such an issue with ~ 17 million data points. Please refer to this paper.

      Freckleton, Robert P. "Dealing with collinearity in behavioural and ecological data: model averaging and the problems of measurement error." Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 65 (2011): 91-101.

      (4) The observed low heterogeneity may stem from discrepancies in sampling for abundance versus occupancy, compounded by uncertainties in reporting behavior.

      If we assume everybody underreports common species or overreports rare species, this could happen. However, such an assumption is unlikely. If some people report accurately (but not others), we should see high heterogeneity, which we do not observe).  We have touched upon this point in our original MS.

      (5) The contribution and implementation of phylogenetic comparative analysis remain ambiguous and were not sufficiently clarified within the study.

      We need to add more explanation for the global abundance analysis

      “To statistically test whether there was an effect of abundance and occupancy at the macro-scale, we used phylogenetic comparative analysis.  This analysis also addresses the issue of positive interspecific AORs potentially arising from not accounting for phylogenetic relatedness among species examined ”

      (6) The use of large N checklists could skew the perceived rarity or commonality of species, potentially diminishing the positive correlation observed in AORs. A consistent observer effect could lead to a near-zero effect with high precision.

      Regardless of the number of N species in checklists (seen in Fig 2), correlations are distributed around zero. This means there is nothing special about large N checklists. 

      (7) The study should acknowledge and discuss any discrepancies or deviations from previous literature or expected outcomes.

      We felt we had already done this as we discussed the previous meta-analysis and what we expected from this meta-analysis.  Nevertheless, we have added some relevant sentences in the new version of MS.

      In addition to these major points, there are several minor concerns:

      (1) Figure 2B lacks discussion, and the metric for the number of observations is not clarified. Furthermore, the labeling of the y-axis appears to be incorrect.

      Thank you very much for pointing out this shortcoming. Now, the y-axis label has been fixed and we mention 2B in the main text.

      (2) The study should provide a clear, mathematical expression of the multilevel random effect models for greater transparency.

      Many thanks for this point, and now we have added relevant mathematical expressions in Table S6.

      (3) On Line 260, the term "number of species" should be refined to "number of species in a checklist," ideally represented by a formula for precision.

      This ambiguity has been mended as suggested.

      Please provide the data and R code linked to the outputs.

      The referee must have missed the link (https://github.com/itchyshin/AORs) in our original MS. In addition to our GitHub repository link, we now have added a link to our Zenodo repository (https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.14019900).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The authors cite Rabinowitz's 7 forms of rarity paper as a suggestion that previous findings also break the AOR. In fact empirical studies of the 7 forms of rarity typically find that all three forms of rareness vs commonness are heavily correlated (e.g. Yu & Dobson 2000).

      We thank the reviewer for drawing attention to Yu & Dobson (2000) and similar studies that find positive correlations among the axes of rarity. Ref 3 is correct in that Rabinowitz’s (1981) framework does not require that local abundance and geographic range size be uncorrelated for every species; instead, it highlights conceptual scenarios where a species may be common locally yet have a restricted distribution (or vice versa).

      Empirical analyses such as Yu & Dobson (2000) show that, on average, these axes can be correlated, which may align with conventional AOR findings in some taxonomic groups. However, Rabinowitz’s key insight was that exceptions do occur, so these exceptions demonstrate that strong positive AORs may not be universally applicable. Our results do not claim that Rabinowitz’s framework “breaks” the AOR outright; instead, we use it to underscore that local abundance can, in principle, be “decoupled” from global occupancy.  Whether the correlation found by Yu & Dobson (2000) implies a positive AOR, requires a detailed simulation study, which is an interesting avenue for future research. 

      Thus, citing Rabinowitz serves to highlight the potential heterogeneity and complexity of abundance–occupancy relationships rather than to refute every positive correlation reported in the literature. Our findings suggest that when examined at large spatiotemporal scales (with unbiased sampling), the overall AOR signal may be less robust than traditionally believed. This is consistent with Rabinowitz’s view that local abundance and global range can vary along independent axes. Now we added

      “Although studies using her framework found positive correlations between species range and local abundance.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1:

      Summary:

      This manuscript uses a well-validated behavioral estimation task to investigate how optimistic belief updating was attenuated during the 2020 global pandemic. Online participants recruited during and outside of the pandemic estimated how likely different negative life events were to happen to them in the future and were given statistics about these events happening. Belief updating (measured as the degree to which estimations changed after viewing the statistics) was less optimistically biased during the pandemic (compared to outside of it). This resulted from reduced updating from "good news" (better than expected information). Computational models were used to try to unpack how statistics were integrated and used to revise beliefs. Two families of models were compared - an RL set of models where "estimation errors" (analogous to prediction errors in classic RL models) predict belief change and a Bayesian set of models where an implied likelihood ratio was calculated (derived from participants estimations of their own risk and estimation of the base rate risk) and used to predict belief change. The authors found evidence that the former set of models accounted for updating better outside of the pandemic, but the latter accounted for updating during the pandemic. In addition, the RL model provides evidence that learning was asymmetrically positively biased outside of the pandemic but symmetric during it (as a result of reduced learning rates from good news estimation errors).

      Strengths:

      Understanding whether biases in learning are fixed modes of information processing or flexible and adapt in response to environmental shocks (like a global pandemic or economic recession) is an important area of research relevant to a wide range of fields, including cognitive psychology, behavioral economics, and computational psychiatry. The study uses a well-validated task, and the authors conduct a power analysis to show that the sample sizes are appropriate. Furthermore, the authors test that their results hold in both a between-group analysis (the focus of the main paper) and a within-group analysis (mainly in the supplemental).

      The finding that optimistic biases are reduced in response to acute stress, perceived threat, and depression has been shown before using this task both in the lab (social stress manipulation), in the real world (firefighters on duty), and clinical groups (patients with depression). However, the work does extend these findings here in important ways:

      (1) Examining the effect of a new real-world adverse event (the pandemic).<br /> (2) The reduction in optimistic updating here arises due to reduced updating from positive information (previously, in the case of environmental threat, this reduction mainly arose from increased sensitivity to negative information).<br /> (3) Leveraging new RL-inspired computational approaches, demonstrating that the bias - and its attenuation - can be captured using trial-by-trial computational modeling with separate learning rates for positive and negative estimation errors.

      Weaknesses:

      Some interpretation and analysis (the computational modeling in particular) could be improved.

      On the interpretation side, while the pandemic was an adverse experience and stressful for many people (including myself), the absence of any measures of stress/threat levels limits the conclusions one can draw. Past work that has used this task to examine belief updating in response to adverse environmental events took physiological (e.g., SCR, cortisol) and/or self-report (questionnaires) measures of mood. In SI Table 1, the authors possibly had some questionnaire measures along these lines, but this might be for the participants tested during the pandemic.

      Thank you for this review.

      We agree that the lack of physiological and self-report measures of stress, threat, and perceived uncertainty limits the interpretation of findings regarding potential psychological factors. Some self-reported anxiety and perceived risk measures experienced during the lockdowns were collected in a subset of participants (n=40, counting n=21 tested before and during the 1st strict lockdown, and n=19 tested solely during the 1st lockdown). These reports were given retrospectively at the time of release of the 1st lockdown in summer 2020 when the pandemic was still unfolding (SI Table 1).

      Exploratory correlations revealed some noteworthy trends. We found that participants who reported to have perceived a bigger risk of death due to contagion were also those who were less optimistically biased when updating their beliefs about adverse future life risks during the first strict COVID-19-related lockdown (Pearson’s r = -0.36, p = 0.02).

      Moreover, parameter estimates from the computational models of belief updating showed associations with specific survey responses: The rational Bayesian model’s scaling parameter correlated positively with adherence to distancing measures (r = 0.41, p = 0.01) and negatively with the need for social contact (r = -0.37, p = 0.02). This result indicated that participants who were updating their beliefs faster were more likely to follow preventive guidelines and felt less social craving. Meanwhile, the asymmetry parameter correlated negatively with mask wearing (r = -0.41, p = 0.01), positively with physical contact with close others (r = 0.32, p = 0.04) and satisfaction with social interactions (r = 0.33, p = 0.04). This suggests that participants who displayed some asymmetry in belief updating during the COVID-19 pandemic were less likely to comply with mask-wearing rules and more likely to engage in social interactions.

      However, these results did not survive correction for multiple comparisons and the sample size for correlational analyses is in the lower range. The subjective measures of anxiety and fear of contagion did not significantly correlate to the updating bias, or any other variable measured by the belief updating task (e.g. estimation error, updating magnitude).

      We now further discuss on page 12 the limitation, which reads:

      “We did not collect physiological measures of stress or information about the COVID-19 infection status of participants, which precludes a direct exploration of the immediate effects of experiencing the infection on belief-updating behavior and the potential interaction with anxiety and stress levels. Although subjective ratings of the perceived risk of death from COVID-19 correlated negatively to the beliefs updating bias measured during the pandemic, this result was obtained retrospectively in a subset of participants (SI section 4). We thus cannot directly attribute the observed lack of optimistically biased belief updating during the lockdown to psychological causes such as heightened anxiety and stress. This limitation is noteworthy, as the impact of experiencing the pandemic on belief updating about the future could differ between those who directly experienced infection and those who remained uninfected. It is also important to acknowledge that our study was timely and geographically limited to the context of the COVID-19 outbreak in France. Cultural variations and differences in governmental responses to contain the spread of SARS-CoV-2 may have impacted the optimism biases in belief updating differently.”

      On the analysis side, it was unclear what the motivation was for the different sets of models tested. Both families of models test asymmetric vs symmetric learning (which is the main question here) and have similar parameters (scaling and asymmetry parameters) to quantify these different aspects of the learning process. Conceptually, the different behavioral patterns one could expect from the two families of models needed to be clarified.

      Thank you for raising this point. We agree that a clearer conceptual distinction between the two model families can help strengthen the interpretation of our findings. We have added the following considerations to the introduction on pages 2–3, which now reads:

      “The underlying mechanism of optimistically biased belief updating involves an asymmetry in learning from positive and negative belief-disconfirming information[2,3,4], which can unfold in two ways following Reinforcement learning (RL) or Bayes rule[5].

      Conceptually, Reinforcement learning (RL) and Bayesian models of belief updating are complementary but make different assumptions about the hidden process humans may use to adjust their beliefs when faced with information that contradicts them. The RL models assume belief updating is proportional to the estimation error. The key idea of the estimation error expresses the difference between how much someone believes they will experience a future life event and the actual prevalence of the event in the general population. This difference can be positive or negative. A scaling and an asymmetry parameter quantify the propensity to consider the estimation error magnitude and its valence, respectively. These two free parameters form the learning rate, which indicates how fast and biased participants update their beliefs.

      In contrast, Bayesian models assume that following Bayes’ rule the posterior, updated belief is a new hypothesis, formed by pondering prior knowledge with new evidence. The prior knowledge consists in information about the prevalence of life events in the general population. The new evidence comprises various alternative hypotheses. It examines how likely a specific event is to occur or not occur for oneself, compared to the likelihood that it will happen or not happen to others. This probabilistic adjustment of beliefs about future life events can be considered as an approximation of a participant’s confidence in the future. The two free parameters of the Bayesian belief updating model scale how much the initial belief deviates from the updated, posterior belief (i.e., scaling parameter) and the propensity to consider the valence of this deviance (i.e., asymmetry parameter).

      Although RL-like and Bayesian updating models make different assumptions about the updating strategy, they are complementary and powerful formalizations of human reasoning. Both models provide insight into hidden, latent variables of the updating process. Most notably, the learning rate and its components, the scaling and asymmetry parameters, which can vary between individuals and contexts and, through this variance, offer possible explanations for the idiosyncrasy in belief-updating behavior and its cognitive biases. “

      Do the "winning" models produce the main behavioral patterns in Figure 1, and are they in some way uniquely able to do so, for instance? How would updating look different for an optimistic RL learner versus an optimistic Bayesian RL learner?

      We now show that the winning models can reproduce the main behavioral patterns (revised Figure 1b).

      Moreover, we plotted estimated and observed average belief updating for each participant (n=123) using the overall best-fitting asymmetrical RL-like updating model shown in SI Figure 6.

      Would the asymmetry parameter in the former be correlated with the asymmetry parameter in the latter? Moreover, crucially, would one be able to reliably distinguish the models from one another under the model estimation and selection criteria that the authors have used here (presenting robust model recovery could help to show this)?

      The asymmetry parameter estimated with the optimistically biased RL- and Bayesian models did correlate (r = 0.735; p < 0.001).

      However, we argue that while the observed updating behavior and estimated free parameters are similar for RL-like and Bayesian learners, the underlying assumed cognitive processes differed and are identifiable. To test this assumption, we have added a model recovery analysis now reported in the supplement section 2c and main manuscript’s methods section pages 24–25.

      As shown in SI Figure 5 confusion matrix, there is evidence for strong recovery of nearly all models, and importantly for the two winning models: the optimistically biased RL-like model and the rational Bayesian model of belief updating. This analysis thus rules out that the two model families were confused and mitigate concerns about the validity of the model selection.

      Note, one exception was observed. The RL-like and Bayesian updating models that assumed no scaling and asymmetry were best recovered by their respective models that estimated the asymmetry parameter. Many factors could explain this. For example, it could be that the models, which assumed asymmetry, but no scaling, may have captured some bias in updating due to noise generated by the zero parameter models.

      A justification is also needed to focus on the "RL-like updating model with an asymmetry and scaling learning rate component" in Figure 3. As I understand it, this model fits best outside of the pandemic, but another model - the Rational Bayesian Model - does worse (and does the best during the pandemic). What model best combines the groups (outside and inside the pandemic)?

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the need to justify our focus on the biased RL-like updating model in Figure 3. The model chosen for parameter comparison was selected based on a model comparison procedure conducted across all 12 models, including data from all participants (both those tested outside and during the pandemic, n=123). This model comparison revealed that Model 1 — the RL model with both asymmetry and scaling learning rate parameters estimated — provided the best fit across the entire dataset (Ef = 0.40, pxp = 0.99). As such, we focused on this model for parameter comparisons in Figure 3 to ensure consistency with the model comparison results and to interpret the parameters in the context of the overall best-fitting model. We added this information on top of the model parameter comparison results on page 8. Moreover, SI Figure 6 in the supplements shows how this model reproduces the observed belief updating in each of the 123 participants.

      Why do the authors use absolute belief updating (|UPD|) in the first linear mixed effects model (equation iv)? Since an update is calculated differently depending on whether information calls for an update in an upward or downward direction, I do not understand the need to do this (and it means that updates that go in the wrong direction - away from the information - are counted as positive)

      Thank you for driving our attention to this point. The ‘absolute belief updating’ note was incorrect, and we apologize for the confusion. To be precise, we did not use absolute updating values in our analyses. Belief updating was assumed on each trial to go either toward the base rate (e.g., Update = E2 – E1) for negative estimation errors or away from it for positive estimation errors (e.g., Update = E1 – E2). Updates that went in the wrong direction, further away from the base rate, were thus counted and included in the analysis with their negative sign. We have corrected this important point in equation iv of the methods section on page 19.

      Figure 4: The task schema does not show a confidence rating for base rates.

      Thank you for catching this. We have now added the confidence ratings for base rates to the task in Figure 4b in the revised version of the manuscript. We have furthermore corrected a typo in Figure 4a: The sample size for the group 3 tested in Mai 2021 now indicates 31.

      The authors report that base rates are uniformly distributed - this is quite different to other instances of the task where base rates are normally distributed (ideally around the midpoint of the scale). Why this deviation in the design?

      We used life events and base rates like those used in past studies of belief updating (Garrett and Sharot 2017, Sharot et al. 2011, Garrett et al. 2017, Korn et al. 2017), which were normal to uniformly distributed (W = 0.952, p = 0.088, Shapiro-Wilk test). The base rates ranged between 10% and 70%, with a mean of 40%. Participants rated their estimates between 3% and 77%, which ensured that for most likely (base rate = 70%) and most unlikely events (base rate = 10%) there was the same space (7%) to update beliefs toward the base rates. Moreover, all statistical models included the absolute estimation errors as a control for variance potentially explained by different estimation error magnitude[42,43]. We added this extra base rate information to the methods section’s task description on page 16.

      The task is comprised of only negative life events, which arguably this hinders the generalizability of the results. The authors could mention this as a limitation (there has been a significant quantity of debate about this point in relation to this task: see the work from Ulrike Hahn's lab).

      We have added a paragraph to the discussion page 13 to provide a rationale for using only adverse events. This paragraph now reads:

      “In this study we tested how actual adverse experiences affect the updating of negative future outlooks in healthy participants and in analogy to studies conducted in depressed patients[19,20,24] following the cognitive model of depression[37]. One open question is whether findings were specific to the adverse event framing[38,39,40]. We argue that under normal, non-adverse contexts belief updating should also be optimistically biased for positive life events, as shown by previous research[41,42]. However, how context such as experiencing a challenging or favorable situation influence the updating of beliefs about positive and negative outlooks remains an open question.”

      It would be useful to show the parameter recovery for all parameters (not just the learning rates) and the correlation between parameters (both in simulations and in the fitted parameters).

      We apologize for being unclear on this part. The models included two free parameters that were the components of the learning rates: The scaling and the asymmetry parameter. We now have added parameter recovery analyses for the scaling and asymmetry components of the learning rates for (1) the Bayesian model of belief updating during the pandemic, and (2) the RL-like model of belief updating outside the pandemic to the supplement (SI section 2b, SI Figure 4).

      Reviewer #2:

      The authors investigated how experiencing the COVID-19 pandemic affected optimism bias in updating beliefs about the future. They ran a between-subjects design testing for participants on cognitive tasks before, during, and after lifting the sanitary state of emergence during the pandemic. The authors show that optimism bias varied depending on the context in which it was tested. Namely, it disappeared during COVID-19 and re-emerged at the time of lift of sanitary emergency measures. Through advanced computational modeling, they are able to thoroughly characterize the nature of such alternations, pinpointing specific mechanisms underlying the lack of optimistic bias during the pandemic.

      Strengths pertain to the comprehensive assessment of the results via computational modeling and from a theoretical point of view to the notion that environmental factors can affect cognition. However, the relatively small sample size for each group is a limitation.

      Thank you for this review.

      We acknowledge that sample sizes in each group are lower, especially when breaking down the participant sample into four sub-samples tested in the different contexts. To mitigate concerns we checked the power of the observed context by valence interaction on belief updating. To this aim we simulated new belief updates using the parameters from the best fitting optimistic RL-like model of observed belief updating outside the pandemic, and the rational Bayesian model of observed belief updating during the pandemic. At each iteration we performed a linear mixed effects model analysis of the simulated belief updates[44] analogous to equation iv in the main text. The frequency across 1000 iterations with which the LMEs detected a significant interaction of valence by context on simulated belief updating was 75 %. This frequency indicates the power of the valence by context interaction on observed belief updating. In other words, false negatives were 25% likely, which meant type II errors of failing to reject the null hypothesis when the effect was there. We have added these extra analyses to the main manuscript’s results section page 4 and method’s section page 20.

      A major impediment interpreting of the findings is the need for additional measures. While the information on for example, risk perception or the need for social interaction was collected from participants during the pandemic, the fact that these could not be included in the analysis hinders the interpretation of findings, which is now generally based on data collected during the pandemic, for example, reporting increased stress. While authors suggest an interpretation in terms of uncertainty of real-life conditions it is currently difficult to know if that factor drove the effect. Many concurrent elements might have accounted for the findings. This limits understanding of the underlying mechanisms related to changes in optimism bias.

      We agree with the reviewer on the limitation arising from the lack of physiological and self-report measures of stress, threat, and perceived uncertainty. To address this point and a similar point raised by reviewer 1 we have added a section to the supplement (SI section 4) that now reports explorative correlations between questionnaire responses of subjective perceptions of risk and anxiety, behavior (e.g. mask wearing, social distancing) and belief updating measured during the 1st strict lockdown.

      We now also further discuss this limitation on page 12 of the main text’s discussion.

      I recommend that the authors spend more time on explaining the belief-updating task in the presentation of the experiment.

      Thank you for this advice. We now provide a clearer and more detailed description of the belief-updating task in the main manuscript’s methods section and have updated Figure 4b to display the confidence rating event in the task schema.

      The task description now reads:

      “As illustrated in Figure 4b, each of the 40 trials began with presenting an adverse life event. Participants estimated their own risk and the risk of someone else their age and gender. Then the base rate of the event occurring in the general population was displayed on the computer screen. Participants rated their confidence in the accuracy of the presented base rate. Finally, they re-estimated their risk for experiencing the event now informed by the base rate.”

      The experimental task seems to include a self-other dimension, which is completely disregarded in the analysis. It would be interesting to explore whether the effect of diluted optimism bias during the pandemic is specific to information about self vs. Other.

      We appreciate the reviewer's observation regarding the self-versus-other dimension in the belief updating task design. As now shown in SI Figure 2 the participants indeed displayed an optimism bias: They estimated that adverse events are more likely to happen to others than to themselves (ß = 3.02, SE = 0.86, t (232) = 3.53, p = 5.09e-04, 95% CI [1.33 – 4.71]; SI Figure 2; SI Table 18). This effect was observed overall participants. The pandemic context had no significant effect (ß = -1.91, SE = 3.00, t (232) = -0.64, p = 0.52, 95% CI [-7.82 – 4.00]; SI Table 18). Moreover, following previous studies of optimistically biased belief updating we tested the effect of estimation errors (EE) calculated on the difference between the estimate for someone else (eBR) and the base rate (BR), following: EE = eBR – BR[4,5,25,26]. When categorizing trials as good news or bad news based on this alternative EE calculation the context-by-EE valence interaction remained significant (SI Table 6).

      We conclude from these additional analyses that experiencing the pandemic specifically influenced belief updating but did not affect optimism biases in initial beliefs about the future.

      Please provide an English translation of the instructions for the task.

      We now provide an English translation of the task instructions in the Supplement section 5.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study examined the changes in ATL GABA levels induced by cTBS and its relationship with BOLD signal changes and performance in a semantic task. The findings suggest that the increase in ATL GABA levels induced by cTBS is associated with a decrease in BOLD signal. The relationship between ATL GABA levels and semantic task performance is nonlinear, and more specifically, the authors propose that the relationship is an inverted U-shaped relationship.

      Strengths:

      The findings of the research regarding the increase of GABA and decrease of BOLD caused by cTBS, as well as the correlation between the two, appear to be reliable. This should be valuable for understanding the biological effects of cTBS.

      Weakness:

      I am pleased to see the authors' feedback on my previous questions and suggestions, and I believe the additional data analysis they have added is helpful. Here are my reserved concerns and newly discovered issues.

      (1) Regarding the Inverted U-Shaped Curve In the revised manuscript, the authors have accepted some of my suggestions and conducted further analysis, which is now presented in Figure 3B. These results provide partial support for the authors' hypothesis. However, I still believe that the data from this study hardly convincingly support an inverted U-shaped distribution relationship.

      The authors stated in their response, "it is challenging to determine the optimal level of ATL GABA," but I think this is achievable. From Figures 4C and 4D, the ATL GABA levels corresponding to the peak of the inverted U-shaped curve fall between 85 and 90. In my understanding, this can be considered as the optimal level of ATL GABA estimated based on the existing data and the inverted U-shaped curve relationship. However, in the latter half of the inverted U-shaped curve, there are quite few data points, and such a small number of data points hardly provides reliable support for the quantitative relationship in the latter half of the curve. I suggest that the authors should at least explicitly acknowledge this and be cautious in drawing conclusions. I also suggest that the authors consider fitting the data with more types of non-linear relationships, such as a ceiling effect (a combination of a slope and a horizontal line), or a logarithmic curve.

      We appreciate R1’s comments. Inverted U-shaped relationships are well-established in neuroscience, particularly in the context of neurotransmitter concentrations (e.g., dopamine, acetylcholine, noradrenaline) and their influence on cognitive functions such as working memory and cognitive control (Aston-Jones & Cohen., 2005; Cools & D'Esposito., 2011; Vijayraghavan et al., 2007; He & Zempel., 2013). Recently, Ferri et al. (2017) demonstrated an inverted U-shaped relationship between excitation-inhibition balance (EIB: the ratio of Glx and GABA) and multisensory integration, showing that both excessive and insufficient inhibition negatively impact functionality. Given that GABA is the brain’s primary inhibitory neurotransmitter, our findings suggest that ATL GABA may play a similar regulatory role in semantic memory function.

      While our statistical modelling approach demonstrated that the inverted U-shaped function was the best-fitting model for our current data in explaining the relationship between ATL GABA and semantic memory, we acknowledge the limitation of having fewer data points in the latter half (right side) of the curve, where excessive ATL GABA levels are associated with poorer semantic performance. Following R1’s suggestion, we have explicitly acknowledged this limitation in the revised manuscript and exercised caution in our discussion.

      Discussion, p.17, line 408

      "However, our findings should be interpreted with caution due to the limitation of having fewer data points in the latter half (right side) of the inverted U-shaped curve. Future studies incorporating GABA agonists could help further validate and refine these findings."

      Following R1’s latter suggestion, we tested a logarithmic curve model. The results showed significant relationships between ATL GABA and semantic performance (R<sup>2</sup> = 0.544, p < 0.001) and between cTBS-induced changes in ATL GABA and semantic performance (R<sup>2</sup> = 0.202, p < 0.001). However, the quadratic (inverted U-shaped) model explained more variance than the logarithmic model, as indicated by a higher R<sup>2</sup> and lower BIC. Model comparisons further confirmed that the inverted U-shaped model provided the best fit for both ATL GABA in relation to semantic performance (Fig. 4C) and cTBS-induced ATL GABA changes in relation to semantic function (Fig. 4D).

      Author response table 1.

      (2) In Figure 2F, the authors demonstrated a strong practice effect in this study, which to some extent offsets the decrease in behavioral performance caused by cTBS. Therefore, I recommend that the authors give sufficient consideration to the practice effect in the data analysis.

      One issue is the impact of the practice effect on the classification of responders and non-responders. Currently, most participants are classified as non-responders, suggesting that the majority of the population may not respond to the cTBS used in this study. This greatly challenges the generalizability of the experimental conclusions. However, the emergence of so many non-responders is likely due to the prominent practice effect, which offsets part of the experimental effect. If the practice effect is excluded, the number of responders may increase. The authors might estimate the practice effect based on the vertex simulation condition and reclassify participants after excluding the influence of the practice effect.

      Another issue is that considering the significant practice effect, the analysis in Figure 4D, which mixes pre- and post-test data, may not be reliable.

      We appreciate Reviewer 1’s thoughtful comments regarding the practice effect and its potential impact on our findings. Our previous analysis revealed a strong practice effect on reaction time (RT), with participants performing tasks faster in the POST session, regardless of task condition (Fig. S3). Given our hypothesis that inhibitory ATL cTBS would disrupt semantic task performance, we accounted for this by using inverse efficiency (IE), which combines accuracy and RT. This analysis demonstrated that ATL cTBS disrupted semantic task performance compared to both control stimulation (vertex) and control tasks, despite the practice effect (i.e., faster RT in the POST session), thereby supporting our hypothesis. These findings may suggest that the effects of ATL cTBS were more subtly reflected in semantic task accuracy rather than RT.

      Regarding inter-individual variability in response to rTMS/TBS, prior studies have shown that 50–70% of participants are non-responders, either do not respond or respond in an unexpected manner (Goldsworthy et al., 2014; Hamada et al., 2013; Hinder et al., 2014; Lopez-Alonso et al., 2014; Maeda et al., 2000a; Müller-Dahlhaus et al., 2008). Our previous study (Jung et al., 2022) using the same semantic task and cTBS protocol was the first to explore TBS-responsiveness variability in semantic memory, where 12 out of 20 participants (60%) were classified as responders. The proportion of responders and non-responders in the current study aligns with previous findings, suggesting that this variability is expected in TBS research.

      However, we acknowledge R1’s concern that the strong practice effect may have influenced responder classification. To address this, we estimated the practice effect using the vertex stimulation condition and reclassified participants accordingly by adjusting ATL stimulation performance (IE) relative to vertex stimulation performance (IE). This reclassification identified nine responders (an increase of two), aligning with the typical responder proportion (52%) reported in the TBS literature. Overall, we replicated the previous findings with improved statistical robustness.

      A 2×2×2 ANOVA was conducted with task (semantic vs. control) and session (PRE vs. POST) as within-subject factors, and group (responders vs. non-responders) as a between-subject factor. The analysis revealed a significant interaction between the session and group (F<sub>1, 15</sub> = 10.367, p = 0.006), a marginally significant interaction between the session and task (F<sub>1, 15</sub> = 4.370, p = 0.054), and a significant 3-way interaction between the session, task, and group (F<sub>1, 15</sub> = 7.580, p = 0.015). Post hoc t-tests showed a significant group difference in semantic task performance following ATL stimulation (t = 2.349, p = 0.033). Post hoc paired t-test demonstrated that responders exhibited poorer semantic task performance following the ATL cTBS (t = -5.281, p < 0.001), whereas non-responders showed a significant improvement (t = 3.206, p = 0.007) (see Figure. 3A).

      Notably, no differences were observed between responders and non-responders in the control task performance across pre- and post-stimulation sessions, confirming that the practice effect was successfully controlled (Figure. 3B).

      We performed a 2 x 2 ANOVA with session (pre vs. post) as a within subject factor and with group (responders vs. non-responders) as a between subject factor to examine the effects of group in ATL GABA levels. The results revealed a significant main effect of session (F<sub>1, 14</sub> = 39.906, p < 0.001) and group (F<sub>1, 14</sub> = 9.677, p = 0.008). Post hoc paired t-tests on ATL GABA levels showed a significant increase in regional ATL GABA levels following ATL stimulation for both responders (t = -3.885, p = 0.002) and non-responders (t = -4.831, p = 0.001). Furthermore, we replicated our previous finding that baseline GABA levels were significantly higher in responders compared to non-responders (t = 2.816, p = 0.007) (Figure. 3C). This pattern persisted in the post-stimulation session (t = 2.555, p = 0.011) (Figure. 3C).

      Accordingly, we have revised the Methods and Materials (p 26, line 619), Results (p11, line 233-261), and Figure 3.

      (3) The analysis in Figure 3A has a double dipping issue. Suppose we generate 100 pairs of random numbers as pre- and post-test scores, and then group the data based on whether the scores decrease or increase; the pre-test scores of the group with decreased scores will have a very high probability of being higher than those of the group with increased scores. Therefore, the findings in Figure 3A seem to be meaningless.

      Yes, we agreed with R1’s comments. However, Figure 3A illustrates interindividual responsiveness patterns, while Figure 3B demonstrates that these results account for practice effects, incorporating new analyses.

      (4) The authors use IE as a behavioral measure in some analyses and use accuracy in others. I recommend that the authors adopt a consistent behavioral measure.

      We appreciate Reviewer 1’s suggestion. In examining the relationship between ATL GABA and semantic task performance, we have found that only semantic accuracy—not reaction time (RT) or inverse efficiency (IE)—shows a significant positive correlation and regression with ATL GABA levels and semantic task-induced ATL activation, both in our previous study (Jung et al., 2017) and in the current study. ATL GABA levels were not correlated with semantic RT (Jung et al., 2017: r = 0.34, p = 0.14, current study: r = 0.26, p = 0.14). It should be noted that there were no significant correlations between ATL GABA levels and semantic inverse efficiency (IE) in both studies (Jung et al., 2017: r = 0.13, p = 0.62, current study: r = 0.22, p = 0.44). As a result, we found no significant linear and non-linear relationship between ATL GABA levels and RT (linear function R<sup>2</sup> = 0.21, p =0.45, quadratic function: R<sup>2</sup> = 0.17, p = 0.21) and between ATL GABA levels and IE (linear function R<sup>2</sup> = 0.24, p =0.07, quadratic function: R<sup>2</sup> = 2.24, p = 0.12).

      The absence of a meaningful relationship between ATL GABA and semantic RT or IE may be due to the following reasons: 1) RT is primarily associated with premotor and motor activation during semantic processing rather than ATL activation; 2) ATL GABA is likely to play a key role in refining distributed semantic representations through lateral inhibition, which sharpens the activated representation (Jung et al., 2017; Liu et al. 2011; Isaacson & Scanziani., 2011). This sharpening process may contribute to more accurate semantic performance (Jung et al., 2017). In our semantic task, for example, when encountering a camel (Fig. 1B), multiple semantic features (e.g., animal, brown, desert, sand, etc.) are activated. To correctly identify the most relevant concept (cactus), irrelevant associations (tree) must be suppressed—a process that likely relies on inhibitory mechanisms. Given this theoretical framework, we have used accuracy as the primary measure of semantic performance to elucidate the ATL GABA function.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors combined inhibitory neurostimulation (continuous theta-burst stimulation, cTBS) with subsequent MRI measurements to investigate the impact of inhibition of the left anterior temporal lobe (ATL) on task-related activity and performance during a semantic task and link stimulation-induced changes to the neurochemical level by including MR spectroscopy (MRS). cTBS effects in the ATL were compared with a control site in the vertex. The authors found that relative to stimulation of the vertex, cTBS significantly increased the local GABA concentration in the ATL. cTBS also decreased task-related semantic activity in the ATL and potentially delayed semantic task performance by hindering a practice effect from pre to post. Finally, pooled data with their previous MRS study suggest an inverted u-shape between GABA concentration and behavioral performance. These results help to better understand the neuromodulatory effects of non-invasive brain stimulation on task performance.

      Strengths:

      Multimodal assessment of neurostimulation effects on the behavioral, neurochemical, and neural levels. In particular, the link between GABA modulation and behavior is timely and potentially interesting.

      Weaknesses:

      The analyses are not sound. Some of the effects are very weak and not all conclusions are supported by the data since some of the comparisons are not justified. There is some redundancy with a previous paper by the same authors, so the novelty and contribution to the field are overall limited. A network approach might help here.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors used cTBS TMS, magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) as the main methods of investigation. Their data show that cTBS modulates GABA concentration and task-dependent BOLD in the ATL, whereby greater GABA increase following ATL cTBS showed greater reductions in BOLD changes in ATL. This effect was also reflected in the performance of the behavioural task response times, which did not subsume to practice effects after AL cTBS as opposed to the associated control site and control task. This is in line with their first hypothesis. The data further indicates that regional GABA concentrations in the ATL play a crucial role in semantic memory because individuals with higher (but not excessive) GABA concentrations in the ATLs performed better on the semantic task. This is in line with their second prediction. Finally, the authors conducted additional analyses to explore the mechanistic link between ATL inhibitory GABAergic action and semantic task performance. They show that this link is best captured by an inverted U-shaped function as a result of a quadratic linear regression model. Fitting this model to their data indicates that increasing GABA levels led to better task performance as long as they were not excessively low or excessively high. This was first tested as a relationship between GABA levels in the ATL and semantic task performance; then the same analyses were performed on the pre and post-cTBS TMS stimulation data, showing the same pattern. These results are in line with the conclusions of the authors.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have comprehensively addressed my comments from the first round of review, and I consider most of their answers and the steps they have taken satisfactorily. Their insights prompted me to reflect further on my own knowledge and thinking regarding the ATL function.

      I do, however, have an additional and hopefully constructive comment regarding the point made about the study focusing on the left instead of bilateral ATL. I appreciate the methodological complexities and the pragmatic reasons underlying this decision. Nevertheless, briefly incorporating the justification for this decision into the manuscript would have been beneficial for clarity and completeness. The presented argument follows an interesting logic; however, despite strong previous evidence supporting it, the approach remains based on an assumption. Given that the authors now provide the group-level fMRI results captured more comprehensively in Supplementary Figure 2, where the bilateral pattern of fMRI activation can be observed in the current data, the authors could have strengthened their argument by asserting that the activation related to the given semantic association task in this data was bilateral. This would imply that the TMS effects and associated changes in GABA should be similar for both sites. Furthermore, it is worth noting the approach taken by Pobric et al. (2007, PNAS), who stimulated a site located 10 mm posterior to the tip of the left temporal pole along the middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and not the bilateral ATL.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s constructive comment regarding the focus on the left ATL rather than bilateral ATL in our study. Accordingly, we have added the following paragraph in the Supplementary Information.

      “Justification of target site selection and cTBS effects

      Evidence suggests that bilateral ATL systems contribute to semantic representation (for a review, see Lambon Ralph., 2017). Consistent with this, our semantic task induced bilateral ATL activation (Fig. S2). Thus, stimulating both left and right ATL could provide a more comprehensive understanding of cTBS effects and its GABAergic function.

      Previous rTMS studies have applied inhibitory stimulation to the left vs. right ATL, demonstrating that stimulation at either site significantly disrupted semantic task performance (Pobric et al., 2007, PNAS; Pobric et al., 2010, Neuropsychologia; Lambon Ralph et al., 2009, Cerebral Cortex). Importantly, these studies reported no significant difference in rTMS effects between left and right ATL stimulation, suggesting that stimulating either hemisphere produces comparable effects on semantic processing. In the current study, we combined cTBS with multimodal imaging to investigate its effects on the ATL. Given our study design constraints (including the need for a control site, control task, and control stimulation) and limitations in scanning time, we selected the left ATL as the target region. This choice also aligned with the MRS voxel placement used in our previous study (Jung et al., 2017), allowing us to combine datasets and further investigate GABAergic function in the ATL. Accordingly, cTBS was applied to the peak coordinate of the left ventromedial ATL (MNI -36, -15, -30) as identified by previous fMRI studies (Binney et al., 2010; Visser et al., 2012).

      Given that TMS pulses typically penetrate 2–4 cm, we acknowledge the challenge of reaching deeper ventromedial ATL regions. However, our findings indicate that cTBS effectively modulated ATL function, as evidenced by reduced task-induced regional activity, increased ATL GABA concentrations, and poorer semantic performance, confirming that TMS pulses successfully influenced the target region. To further validate these effects, we conducted an ROI analysis centred on the ventromedial ATL (MNI -36, -15, -30), which revealed a significant reduction in ATL activity during semantic processing following ATL stimulation (t = -2.43, p = 0.014) (Fig. S7). This confirms that cTBS successfully modulated ATL activity at the intended target coordinate.”

      We appreciate R3's comment regarding the approach taken by Pobric et al. (2007, PNAS), who stimulated a site 10 mm posterior to the tip of the left temporal pole along the middle temporal gyrus (MTG). This approach has been explicitly discussed in our previous papers and reviews (e.g., Lambon Ralph, 2014, Proc. Royal Society B). Our earlier use of lateral ATL stimulation at this location (Pobric et al. 2007; Lambon Ralph et al. 2009; Pobric et al. 2010) was based on its alignment with the broader ATL region commonly atrophied in semantic dementia (cf. Binney et al., 2010 for a direct comparison of SD atrophy, fMRI data and the TMS region). Since these original ATL TMS investigations, a series of distortion-corrected or distortion-avoiding fMRI studies (e.g., Binney et al 2010; Visser et al, various, Hoffman et al., various; Jackson et al., 2015) have demonstrated graded activation differences across the ATL. While weaker activation is present at the original lateral ATL (MTG) stimulation site, the peak activation is maximal in the ventromedial ATL—a finding that was also observed in the current study. Accordingly, we selected the ventromedial ATL as our target site for stimulation.

      Following these points, we have revised the manuscript in the Methods and Materials.

      Transcranial magnetic stimulation p23, line 525-532,

      “Previous rTMS studies targeted a lateral ATL site 10 mm posterior to the temporal pole on the middle temporal gyrus (MTG) (Pobric et al. 2007; Lambon Ralph et al. 2009; Pobric et al. 2010), aligning with the broader ATL region typically atrophied in semantic dementia  (Binney et al. 2010). However, distortion-corrected fMRI studies (Binney et al. 2010; Visser et al. 2012) have revealed graded activation differences across the ATL, with peak activation in the ventromedial ATL. Based on these findings, we selected the target site in the left ATL (MNI -36, -15, -30) from a prior distortion-corrected fMRI study (Binney et al. 2010; Visser et al. 2012 that employed the same tasks as our study (for further details, see the Supplementary Information).”

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors have responded to all my comments and I found most of the responses reasonable and sufficient. However, I have one remaining point: I pointed out before that the scope of this paper is somehow narrow and asked for a network analysis. I found the response to my question somehow puzzling since the authors write:

      "However, it is important to note that we did not find any significant correlations between ATL GABA changes and cTBS-induced changes in the functional connectivity. Consequently, we are currently preparing another paper that specifically addresses the network-level changes induced by ATL cTBS."

      I don't understand the logic here. Even in the absence of significant correlations between ATL GABA changes and cTBS-induced changes in connectivity, it would be interesting to know how baseline connectivity is correlated with the induced changes. I am not sure if it is adequate to squeeze another paper out of the dataset instead of reporting it here as suggested.

      We apologise that our previous response was not clear. To examine cTBS-induced network-level changes, we conducted ROI analyses targeting key semantic regions, including the bilateral ATL, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), and posterior middle temporal gyrus (pMTG), as well as Psychophysiological Interactions (PPI) using the left ATL as a seed region. The ROI analysis revealed that ATL stimulation significantly decreased task-induced activity in the left ATL (target region) while increasing activity in the right ATL and left IFG. PPI analyses showed that ATL stimulation enhanced connectivity between the left ATL and the right ATL (both ventromedial and lateral ATL), bilateral IFG, and bilateral pMTG, suggesting that ATL stimulation modulates a bilateral semantic network.

      Building on these findings, we conducted Dynamic Causal Modeling (DCM) to estimate and infer interactions among predefined brain regions across different experimental conditions (Friston et al., 2003). The bilateral ventromedial ATL, lateral ATL, IFG, and pMTG were defined as network nodes with mutual connections. Our model examined cTBS effects at the left ATL under both baseline (intrinsic) and semantic task (modulatory) conditions, estimating 56 intrinsic parameters for baseline connectivity and testing 16 different modulatory models to assess cTBS-induced connectivity changes during semantic processing. Here, we briefly summarize the key DCM analysis results: 1) ATL cTBS significantly altered effective connectivity between the left and right lateral and ventromedial ATL in both intrinsic and modulatory conditions; 2) cTBS increased modulatory connectivity from the right to the left ATL compared to vertex stimulation.

      Given the complexity and depth of these findings, we believe that a dedicated paper focusing on the network-level effects of ATL cTBS is necessary to provide a more comprehensive and detailed analysis, which extends beyond the scope of the current study. It should be noted that no significant relationship was found between ATL GABA levels and ATL connectivity in both PPI and DCM analyses.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In response to my comment about the ATL activation being rather medial in the fMRI data and my concern about the TMS pulse perhaps not reaching this site, the authors offer an excellent solution to demonstrate TMS effects to such a medial ATL coordinate. I think that the analyses and figures they provide as a response to this comment and a brief explanation of this result should be incorporated into supplementary materials for methodologically oriented readers. Also, perhaps it would be beneficial to discuss that the effect of TMS on vATL remains a matter of further research to see not just if but also how TMS pulse reaches target coordinates, given the problematic anatomical location of the region.

      We appreciate R3’s suggestion. Please, see our reply above.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) The production of ROS has been measured in a very superficial way.

      The term "ROS" confers a plethora of chemical species which exerts different physiological effects on different cells and situations.

      Mitochondria through one of the source, but not the only source of ROS production. Only measuring ROS with mitosox do not reflect the cellular condition of ROS in a specific condition. I would suggest authors consider doing IF of oxidative stress specific markers , carbonyl group and also, maybe, Amplex red for determining average oxidative stress and ros production in the cells.

      We agree with the reviewer that a detailed analysis of ROS production and its markers would strengthen the manuscript. Accordingly, we will perform the Amplex Red assay for Figure 1.

      (2) 8-OHG signal seems very confusing in Figure 7E. 8-ohg is supposed to be mainly in the nucleus and to some extent in mitochondria. The signal is very diffused in the images. I would suggest a higher magnification and better resolution images for 8-ohg. Also, the VWF signal is pretty weak whereas it should be strong given the staining is in aorta. Authors should redo the experiments.

      The reviewer’s comment is correct regarding the expected signal. We will repeat the assays. However, we would like to note that the flat morphology of the endothelial cell monolayer on the aortic surface may limit the visualization of subcellular signal differentiation when transversely sectioned.

      (3) PCA analysis is quite not clear. Why is there a convergence among the plots? Authors should explain. Also, I would suggest that the authors do the analysis done in Figure 8B again with R based packages. IPA, though being user-friendly, mostly does not yield meaningful results and the statistics carried out is not accurate. Authors should redo the analysis in R or Python whichever is suitable for them.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. We acknowledge the concern regarding the PCA analysis and the convergence observed in the plots. In the revised manuscript, we will revise our interpretation to clarify this observation.

      Additionally, we appreciate your suggestion to use R-based packages for pathway analysis. We will make efforts to regenerate the analysis presented in Figure 8B using R to enhance the statistical robustness and reproducibility of our results.

      (4) The MS analysis part seems pretty vague in methods. Please rewrite.

      We will revise the methods section to improve the legibility.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      All the experiments performed here are in overexpression background therefore, it would be crucial to show that p66Shc is SUMO2ylated at physiological levels.

      To address this concern, we will attempt to assess p66Shc-SUMO2 levels under physiological conditions. However, we would like to highlight a technical limitation: the currently available antibodies do not distinguish p66Shc from other isoforms, nor SUMO2 from SUMO3. Therefore, enriching for the endogenous p66Shc-SUMO2 adduct will require novel tools and techniques, which we are actively exploring.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      One notable weakness is that the link between the observed cellular changes and the ultimate in vivo phenotype remains only partially explored. While the authors successfully show that p66ShcK81R knockin mice are protected from endothelial dysfunction in a hyperlipidemic context, additional experiments characterizing the broader tissue-specific roles, or examining further endothelial assays in vivo, would strengthen the mechanistic conclusions. It would also be beneficial to see more direct evaluations of p66Shc subcellular localization in the protective knockin mice to complement the proteomic findings.

      That is an excellent suggestion. We will determine the tissue specific distribution of endogenous p66ShcK81R.

      Despite these gaps, the data broadly support the authors' main conclusions. The authors lay out a plausible mechanistic pathway for how hyperlipidemia and increased global SUMOylation can converge on the oxidative stress pathway to provoke vascular dysfunction.

      The likely impact of this work on the field is noteworthy. Beyond clarifying how a single post-translational modification event can influence the pathophysiology of endothelial cells, the study provides a model for investigating broader roles of SUMO2 in other cardiovascular conditions and highlights the importance of identifying additional SUMOylation sites and their downstream impact.

      In conclusion, by demonstrating the direct SUMOylation of p66Shc at lysine-81 and linking that modification to endothelial dysfunction in a hyperlipidemic mouse model, this paper offers valuable insights into how broadly acting post-translational modifiers can evoke specific pathological effects.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript assesses the utility of spatial image correlation spectroscopy (ICS) for measuring physiological responses to DNA damage. ICS is a long-established (~1993) method similar to fluorescence correlation spectroscopy, for deriving information about the fluorophore density that underlies the intensity distributions of images. The authors first provide a technical but fairly accessible background to the theory of ICS, then compare it with traditional spot-counting methods for its ability to analyze the characteristics of γH2AX staining. Based on the degree of aggregation (DA) value, the authors then survey other markers of DNA damage and uncover some novel findings, such as that RPA aggregation inversely tracks the sensitivity to PARP inhibitors of different cell lines.

      The need for a more objective and standardized tool for analyzing DNA damage has long been felt in the field and the authors argue convincingly for this. The data in the manuscript are in general well-supported and of high quality, and show promise of being a robust alternative to traditional focus counting. However, there are a number of areas where I would suggest further controls and explanations to strengthen the authors' case for the robustness of their ICS method.

      Strengths:

      The spatial ICS method the authors describe and demonstrate is easy to perform and applicable to a wide variety of images. The DDR was well-chosen as an arena to showcase its utility due to its well-characterized dose-responsiveness and known variability between cell types. Their method should be readily useable by any cell biologist wanting to assess the degree of aggregation of fluorescent tags of interest.

      Weaknesses:

      The spatial ICS method, though of longstanding history, is not as intuitive or well-known as spot-based quantitation. While the Theory section gives a standard mathematical introduction, it is not as accessible as it could be. Additionally, the values of TNoP and DA shown in the Results are not discussed sufficiently with regard to their physical and physiological interpretation.

      We agree that a major limitation in adaption of this approach is a deeper understanding of the theory and results. We have updated the theory section to include further discussion (Page 4 line 132)

      The correlation of TNoP with γH2AX foci is high (Figure 2) and suggestive that the ICS method is suitable for measuring the strength of the DDR. The authors correctly mention that the number of spots found using traditional means can vary based on the parameters used for spot detection. They contrast this with their ICS detection method; however, the actual robustness of spatial ICS is not given equal consideration.

      We found it difficult to give equal consideration of robustness to ICS. The major limitation of traditional approaches is proper selection of an intensity threshold that is necessary to define and separate foci from background intensity. However, ICS does not employ a threshold, therefore we could not test different thresholding applications in ICS as we did with traditional methods. In our view the absence of the need for a threshold is profoundly advantageous. The only inputs we employ in the ICS analysis are used to segment cell nuclei, yet these have no impact on the ICS calculation and are necessary for any analysis of the DDR.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Immunostaining of chromatin-associated proteins and visualization of these factors through fluorescence microscopy is a powerful technique to study molecular processes such as DNA damage and repair, their timing, and their genetic dependencies. Nonetheless, it is well-established that this methodology (sometimes called "foci-ology") is subject to biases introduced during sample preparation, immunostaining, foci visualization, and scoring. This manuscript addresses several of the shortcomings associated with immunostaining by using image correlation spectroscopy (ICS) to quantify the recruitment of several DNA damage response-associated proteins following various types of DNA damage.

      The study compares automated foci counting and fluorescence intensity to image correlation spectroscopy degree of aggregation study the recruitment of DNA repair proteins to chromatin following DNA damage. After validating image correlation spectroscopy as a reliable method to visualize the recruitment of γH2AX to chromatin following DNA damage in two separate cell lines, the study demonstrates that this new method can also be used to quantify RPA1 and Rad51 recruitment to chromatin following DNA damage. The study further shows that RPA1 signal as measured by this method correlates with cell sensitivity to Olaparib, a widely-used PARP inhibitor.

      Strengths:

      Multiple proof-of-concept experiments demonstrate that using image correlation spectroscopy degree of aggregation is typically more sensitive than foci counting or foci intensity as a measure of recruitment of a protein of interest to a site of DNA damage. The sensitivity of the SKOV3 and OVCA429 cell lines to MMS and the PARP inhibitors Olaparib and Veliparib as measured by cell viability in response to increasing amounts of each compound is a valuable correlate to the image correlation spectroscopy degree of aggregation measurements.

      Weaknesses:

      The subjectivity of foci counting has been well-recognized in the DNA repair field, and thus foci counts are usually interpreted relative to a set of technical and biological controls and across a meaningful time period. As such:

      (1) A more detailed description of the numerous prior studies examining the immunostaining of proteins such as γH2AX, RAD51, and RPA is needed to give context to the findings presented herein.

      We apologize for not providing enough detail. We have added further references and discussion. γH2AX foci counting, in particular, has been used in thousands of previous studies. (Pages 18 line 513 and 517)

      (2) The benefits of adopting image correlation spectroscopy should be discussed in comparison to other methods, such as super-resolution microscopy, which may also offer enhanced sensitivity over traditional microscopy.

      Thank you for raising this point. We have added this discussion (page 19 line 553). The limiting factor that ICS addresses is the partition coefficient of signal in a foci or cluster versus outside the cluster. Super-resolution will not necessarily improve this unless it is resolved down to single molecule counting. However, one would still need to evaluate how to define a cluster or foci in the background of non-cluster distribution.

      (3) Additional controls demonstrating the specificity of their antibodies to detection of the proteins of interest should be added, or the appropriate citations validating these antibodies included.

      We have added text stating that we only use validated antibodies (page 6 line 193). One thing to note is that we are measuring differences between treatment conditions, thus, if an antibody has non-specific labeling of proteins of cellular structures that do not change upon treatment, our approach would overcome this limitation.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper described a new tool called "Image Correlation Spectroscopy; ICS) to detect clustering fluorescence signals such as foci in the nucleus (or any other cellular structures). The authors compared ICS DA (degree of aggregation) data with Imaris Spots data (and ImageJ Find Maxima data) and found a comparable result between the two analyses and that the ICS sometimes produced a better quantification than the Imaris. Moreover, the authors extended the application of ICS to detect cell-cycle stages by analyzing the DAPI image of cells. This is a useful tool without the subjective bias of researchers and provides novel quantitative values in cell biology.

      Strengths:

      The authors developed a new tool to detect and quantify the aggregates of immunofluorescent signals, which is a center of modern cell biology, such as the fields of DNA damage responses (DDR), including DNA repair. This new method could detect the "invisible" signal in cells without pre-extraction, which could prevent the effect of extracted materials on the pre-assembled ensembles, a target for the detection. This would be an alternative method for the quantification of fluorescent signals relative to conventional methods.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major comments:

      (1) The ICS theory section is essential and based on an excellent review from one of the authors. It would benefit greatly from a diagram showing where the quantities 𝒈(𝟎, 𝟎), 𝝎𝟎, and 𝒈inf come from in the 2D Gaussian fit, ideally for two cases where these quantities differ (i.e., how they correspond to different DA or TNoP values). In my opinion, this addition would greatly increase the manuscript's accessibility for DDR researchers. The citation of the review at the beginning would also be a plus.

      We have added the review citation at the front of the theory section (page 3 line 87).We have highlighted where g(0,0), the most critical measurement for determination of TNoP and DA, derives from in Figure 2D. However, it is difficult to describe all the curve fit parameters in an image as they have some interdependency on each other and thus labeling one in a single image would not independently capture how they might be observed in a different curve fit.

      (2) The TNoP measured in Figure 2 is a quantity about 2000-3000 times greater than the number of "traditionally detected" foci by both methods and the linear relations have very low Y intercepts. Can the authors comment explicitly on the physical interpretation of this number - are 2 to 3 thousand independent particles present within each "focus" detected by traditional means? If so, then what might one "particle" correspond to? (a single secondary antibody or fluorophore? a nucleosome?). In a similar vein, the X intercepts lie at around 25 foci, meaning that in images with fewer than that number of foci detected by ImageJ or Imaris, the ICS method should detect zero TNoP - is this in line with the authors' predictions? Is it possible that a first-order line fit is not the most appropriate relation between the two methods?

      We apologize for our brevity here. Since DA proved to be a more useful metric we did not spend much effort discussing TNoP. TNoP correlates to the number of clustered particles, or non-diffuse fluorophores. TNoP is the inverse of the number of individual particles per nucleus, but the value is not a direct measure of foci. If a sample had no clustering at all, the number of individual particles would be at a maximum and the TNoP would be at a minimum. However, as fluorophores cluster, the number of individual particles (i.e. non-clustered fluorophores) decreases, which increases the TNoP value. Therefore, TNoP has a correlation to the number of foci detected through traditional measurements, as we found here. Yet, TNoP is a relative measurement and cannot be compared across different conditions. Similar to foci counting, TNoP is unable to factor the size or intensity of each cluster, thus DA is a more appropriate quantification of the DNA damage response.

      The value of TNoP is dependent on the fitted point spread function and the area of the nucleus. The y=0 intercept of TNoP is defined by the optical setup and is not expected to necessarily go through x=0. Intriguingly, other groups have found that some foci identified through traditional measurements are actually clusters of multiple smaller foci, thus the concept of what a foci represents is difficult to interpret. Thus, here we aimed to show a general correlation of TNoP with foci count through traditional methods to reflect how ICS is similar to foci counting, then employed DA to overcome the limitations of defining a foci.

      We have tried to clarify this in the text (page 8, line 266)

      (3) Some suggestions to address the robustness of ICS:

      For a given sample (i.e. one segmented nucleus), the calculation of DA and TNoP should be similar between different images of that same nucleus taken at different times, similar to how the number of traditionally detected foci would be fairly invariant. In particular, it should be shown that these values are not just scaling with the higher normalized intensity seen in stronger DDR responses. In the same vein, the linear relationship between TNoP and "foci" should not change even if the confocal settings are slightly different (i.e., higher/lower illumination intensity) as long as the condition stipulated by the authors in the Discussion holds ("ICS can be implemented on any fluorescence image as long as the square relative fluorescence intensity fluctuations are detectable above noise fluctuations."). To show, as the title states, that spatial ICS is a robust tool, it would be desirable to demonstrate this with a series of images of the same cell at the same or varying excitation intensities.

      Thank you for your suggestions. Indeed, the calculation will be the same over sequential images of the same cell. Observations of dose dependent DA that does not correlate with intensity for RPA1 and RAD51 results (Fig. S5) directly demonstrates that DA does not just scale with intensity.

      We would not expect the TNoP to change with confocal setting, however we show in Figure 1 that the number of foci does indeed change with intensity settings as captured by thresholds. Therefore, any interpretation of TNoP vs. foci count would be very difficult to make at different microscope settings. To ensure we are fairly comparing ICS to existing analysis we keep the settings the same and measure changes between conditions.

      (4) More information is needed on how intensity normalization was performed. The Methods states "Measurements across experiments were normalized by the control in each dataset." The DMSO (0mM drug) plots all appear to have a mean of 1.0, so it appears the values for each set of control nuclei were divided by their own mean, and then the values for each set of experimental nuclei were divided by the mean value of all 3 controls as an aggregate; is this correct?

      We apologize for not being more clear. Thank you for raising this point. We normalized data to a control from each experimental group. Thus, in figures 3,4 and 5 data were collected over multiple experiments with one control per experiment and each treatment condition included in each experiment. Therefore, we normalized each result to the corresponding control from that imaging session. However, in Figure 8 we ran experiments at much higher throughput with multiple controls per experiment, thus the data were normalized to the overall average of the controls, which is why the control averages are not all at a value of 1. We have clarified this in the text. (Page 7 line 218).

      (5) Some more information about the ICS analysis should be given if the full code is not provided - in particular, how the nucleus mask was implemented on the "signal" channel (were the edges abruptly set to zero or was a window function introduced to avoid edge effects in the discrete FFT?

      Thank you for raising this point. We have added the code to GitHub - github.com/ dubachLab/ics. The signal region was established by simply applying the nuclear mask from the DAPI channel to the IF channel. Each region is padded with average intensity value at the edges for 2x the dimensions of the ROI to remove edge effects in the FFT.

      Minor comments:

      (1) Figure 3, 4, 5: I think it would aid figure readability if channels were labeled in the images themselves, not just in the legend.

      Thank you for the suggestion, we tried doing this and struggle to fit a label with the layout of the images. We were also concerned about interpretation of data in each column and the potential to assign data to each figure if they were so prominently labeled.

      (2) Supplemental Figures are mislabeled; the order given in the legends is S1, S2, S3, S2, S3. S4 is called out in the main text where it should be S5.

      Thank you for catching this error. We have made the necessary corrections. S4 contains data on cellular response to the drugs, while S5 contains intensity data in response to MMS.

      (3) It should be stated for each Figure what kind of microscopy was performed - I assume that it is confocal for everything except when widefield is explicitly stated, but for clarity please add this information.

      Indeed, this is correct, we have indicated which microscopy was used for each figure.

      (4) The MATLAB code and full (uncropped) Western blots should be provided as supplemental data if possible.

      We have included a GitHub link for the code and un-cropped western blots.

      (5) The p values from significance tests should indicate whether multiple comparisons correction was necessary (if suggested by Prism) and performed.

      Apologies for a lack of clarity but this was not necessary, significance was calculated vs. the next lower dose (e.g. 10 micromolar vs. 1 micromolar). We have clarified this in the methods (page 7 line 221).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major points:

      In addition to the weaknesses noted above, to encourage widespread adoption of this method, the authors should make the tools that they used for their analysis publicly available. In a few instances (e.g., compare Figures 3J and 3L), other methods outperform DA. It would be meaningful to discuss when especially DA may be a better measure than others (such as intensity or number of foci).

      We have made code available on Github. We expect results, such as those in Figures 3J and 3L where intensity is significantly higher at the highest concentration but DA is not are reflective of the underlying biology and this may be interpreted differently under different experimental conditions. Imaris spots (Fig. 3K) also does not capture a significant increase at the highest dose of olaparib, suggesting that intensity may raise but it doesn’t not generate more foci. These results are likely highly dependent on the mechanism of olaparib at such a high concentration and the DDR response. We are hesitant to draw biological conclusions from these results and instead would like to highlight the capacity of ICS to evaluate the DDR, therefore we don’t want to make any broad comments about different applications.

      Minor points:

      (1) Pg. 12: "We used MMS to induce DNA damage in SKOV3 and OVCA429 cells. As expected, normalized intensity for RPA1 and RAD51 values (Figure S5) did not display a dose dependence on MMS concentration."

      Please provide a citation for the claim that RPA1 and RAD51 normalized intensities do not display a dose dependence on MMS concentration.

      These were data that we generated. We were not expecting an intensity change as that would presumably require increased protein generation in response to MMS, compared to gH2AX where the phospho-specific H2AX is generated in the DDR.

      (2) Pg. 12: "Similar to RPA1, RAD51 does not form distinguishable foci in the nuclei in cells without preextraction (Fig. 5)." Please provide a citation for this claim.

      We did not do pre-extraction and our results don’t produce changes in distinguishable foci. We provided citations discussing how, without pre extraction, foci formation for these proteins is not obvious (REF 38 and 39).

      (3) I noted that the authors cite one paper [38] apparently showing that RPA and Rad51 do not always form foci, however, this is in the C. elegans germline in response to micro irradiation, therefore I am not sure that it is applicable to human cells.

      We apologize for referencing a paper on C elegans. Most papers looking at RPA and RAD51 in the DDR use pre-extraction as it seems necessary to observe foci. Therefore, there are not as many papers, that we could find, that do not use pre-extraction. Reference 39 is in Hela cells.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major points:

      (1) Page 8, the second paragraph: In the Result section, it is better to describe how the authors carried out immuno-staining (without pre-extract subtraction) and ICS briefly, although the method is described in detail in the Method section.

      Thank you for the suggestion, we have added this description (page 8, line 259)

      (2) In Figure 5K-P: The authors analyzed "invisible" RAD51 foci on the image (Fig. 5L, M, O, and P) without pre-extraction. As a control experiment, it is useful to check whether pre-extraction would provide "visible" RAD51 foci and to examine the similar MMS concentration dependency shown in Figure 5R (or 5T). This would strengthen the power of the ICS analysis.

      Thank you for the suggestion. In our hands, pre-extraction is extremely subjective. We have tried performing pre-extraction but find highly variable results depending on conditions. Therefore, we did not include any pre-extraction here. We expect that performing these experiments may or may not agree with results in Figure 5 largely because we are unable to achieve repeatable pre-extraction foci counting.

      (3) Figure 6D (and 6C) looks very interesting. It would be important to show the interpretation of this correlation shown in the graph. Although the authors argued that ICS analysis results shown in the graph could provide new insight into the DDR (page 14, last line 5), as shown in another part, it is important to carry out the same analysis by using Imaris Spots. Moreover, it is interesting to apply the analysis to RAD51 foci (shown in Figure 5), given that the PARPi effect is enhanced in the absence of RAD51mediated recombination.

      We completely agree that this analysis may generate interesting results to help interpret the DDR response to PARP inhibition. These experiments are part of an ongoing follow up study where we extend the use of ICS to other parts of the DDR and investigate protein clustering across several proteins with impact on PARPi response. Therefore, since the focus of this manuscript is introducing ICS as a tool to study the DDR, we believe that omitting those data here does not deter from the central points of the manuscript. We including results in Figure 6 because we wanted to show how ICS could impact DDR research. Furthermore, combined with our advances shown in Figures 7 and 8, we are currently working on adapting ICS to be high-throughput and much simpler than Imaris spots for handling large datasets needed to generate results like those in Figure 6.

      Minor points:

      (1) Figure 1I, blue arrows: These showed an area with a higher background. Because of a low magnification, it is very hard to see the difference from the other areas of the background. It is better to show a magnified image of the representative region with a higher background.

      We hope that readers can see the higher intensity in the diffuse area. We attempted to construct a zoomed in area, but that either blocked a significant portion of the nonzoomed image or added complexity to the figure. We have noted that images in Figure S1 are larger and more obviously capture an increase in background intensity.

      (2) Figure 2 legend, line 5, the same as "A)": This should be "B".

      Here, the number of independent particle clusters is intended to be the same as A, the difference is that the independent particles are clusters in C and individual fluorophores in A.

      (3) Page 9, the first paragraph, last line, foci formation, and foci composition: These should be "focus formation and focus composition".

      We have changed this.

      (4) Page 15, the first paragraph, line 5, palbociclib, camptothecin, or etoposide: please explain what kinds of the drugs are.

      We have added that these drugs cause cells to stall at different cell cycle stages. Explaining the drugs would take considerable room in the text.

      (5) Page 16, the first paragraph, line 1, bleomycin: Please explain what this drug is.

      Similar to above, we have stated that this drug causes DNA damage, going into detail would take several sentences.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Triple-negative breast cancer (TNBC) accounts for approximately 15-20% of all breast cancers. Compared to other types of breast cancer, TNBC exhibits highly aggressive clinical characteristics, a greater likelihood of metastasis, poorer clinical outcomes, and lower survival rates. Immunotherapy is an important treatment option for TNBC, but there is significant heterogeneity in treatment response. Therefore, it is crucial to accurately identify immunosuppressive patients before treatment and actively seek more effective therapeutic approaches for TNBC patients.

      Strengths:

      In this work, the authors collected and integrated data from single cells and large volumes of RNA sequencing and RNA-SEQ to analyze the TME landscape mediated by genes associated with iron death. On this basis, the prediction model of prognosis and treatment response of 131 patients was constructed using a machine learning algorithm, which is beneficial to provide individualized and precise treatment guidance for breast cancer patients.

      Thank you for your appreciation of our work. We are encouraged by your positive feedback and will continue to explore new avenues in personalized medicine for breast cancer.

      Weaknesses:

      However, there are still some issues that need to be clarified:

      (1) The description of the research background is too brief and concise, and it is necessary to add some information about the limitations of existing methods and the differences and advantages of this study compared with other published relevant studies, so as to better highlight the necessity and research value of this study.

      Thank you for your suggestions. We have supplemented the research background and compared the differences between this study and other studies, further highlighting the research value of our study.

      (2) This study is a retrospective analysis of a public data set and lacks experimental validation and prospective experiments to support the results of bioinformatics analysis. This should be added to the acknowledgment of limitations in the study.

      Thank you for the constructive feedback. We also acknowledge that the lack of experimental evidence is one of the limitations of this study. Therefore, we plan to conduct in vivo and in vitro experiments in our future research to support the findings of our bioinformatics analysis, and have already supplemented the relevant content in the limitations of Discussion.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study aims to explore the ferroptosis-related immune landscape of TNBC through the integration of single-cell and bulk RNA sequencing data, followed by the development of a risk prediction model for prognosis and drug response. The authors identified key subpopulations of immune cells within the TME, particularly focusing on T cells and macrophages. Using machine learning algorithms, the authors constructed a ferroptosis-related gene risk score that accurately predicts survival and the potential response to specific drugs in TNBC patients.

      Strengths:

      The study identifies distinct subpopulations of T cells and macrophages with differential expression of ferroptosis-related genes. The clustering of these subpopulations and their correlation with patient prognosis is highly insightful, especially the identification of the TREM2+ and FOLR2+ macrophage subtypes, which are linked to either favorable or poor prognoses. The risk model thus holds potential not only for prognosis but also for guiding treatment selection in personalized oncology.

      Thank you for your thorough review and insightful comments.

      Weaknesses:

      The study has a relatively small sample size, with only 9 samples analyzed by scRNA-seq. Given the typically high heterogeneity of the tumor microenvironment (TME) in cancer patients, this may affect the accuracy of the conclusions. The scRNA-seq analysis focuses on the expression of ferroptosis-related genes in various cells within the TME. In contrast, bulk RNA sequencing uses data from tumor samples, and the results between the two analyses are not consistent. The bulk RNA sequencing results may not accurately capture the changes happening in the microenvironment.

      Thank you for your constructive feedback. Although this study only included 9 samples, given the limited availability of scRNA-seq datasets for untreated TNBC in public databases, we chose to utilize a dataset that contains a relatively larger number of untreated TNBC samples. We are fully aware of the complexity and high heterogeneity of the TME. Despite the limited sample size, we first conducted rigorous quality control on the data and, based on this, preliminarily revealed the landscape of the TME mediated by ferroptosis-related genes. These findings provide a new perspective for understanding the biological mechanisms underlying the onset and progression of breast cancer. To enhance the reliability and generalizability of our research results, we plan to strive to expand the sample size in future work and consider integrating other omics technologies, such as proteomics and metabolomics, with scRNA-seq data for a more in-depth exploration of the complex interactions within the TME.

      We also agree with your viewpoint that scRNA-seq data reveals gene expression within individual cells, while bulk RNA-seq data reveals the average gene expression in tumor tissues, and there are differences in data acquisition and processing methods between the two. However, we believe that there are also some close connections between them in terms of gene expression levels. By comparing the expression specificity of marker genes for specific cell types in breast cancer tissues, we found that they are correlated with patient prognosis, and the results have been validated in both internal and external validation sets. Thank you once again for your valuable suggestions, which will play an important guiding role in our subsequent research.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The breast cancer scRNA-seq dataset files of GSE176078 include 10 TNBC primary tumors (DOI:10.1016/j.compbiomed.2023.107066). However, in this study, only 9 cases were listed, please explain the reason for the data exclusion.

      Thank you for your questions. Although it was clearly stated in the original paper that "To elucidate the cellular architecture of breast cancers, we analyzed 26 primary pre-treatment tumors, including 11 ER+, 5 HER2+ and 10 TNBCs, by scRNA-Seq (Supplementary Table 1)," upon downloading and carefully examining the patient information in Supplementary Table 1, we only included 9 patients explicitly labeled as TNBC in our study (https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC9044823/#SD1).

      (2) The description of the technique in the methods section should be more detailed, such as parameter settings, quality control standards, etc.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestions. We have already supplemented the relevant content in the methods section.

      (3) Please check and correct formatting errors to improve readability, such as lines 176 and 177.

      We were really sorry for our careless mistakes. Thank you for your reminder. We have corrected the “Pseudotime analysis with scRNA-seq data helps to obtain an approximate landscape of gene expression dynamics” into “Pseudotime analysis of scRNA-seq snapshot data helps to provide an approximate landscape of gene expression dynamics”. And we have further checked and revised the formatting errors of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) In multiple sections of the paper, abbreviations are used without being defined when first mentioned.

      We were really sorry for our careless mistakes. Thank you for your reminder. We have already added definitions for the abbreviations in both the abstract and the main text.

      (2) The authors should analyze whether the transcription factors in Figure 2 are correlated with the expression of ferroptosis-related genes.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. Some transcription factors in Figure 2 correlate with the expression of ferroptosis-related genes, which we have supplemented in the Discussion.

      (3) Figures 3d and 4e lack explanations for the axis values, and for Figure 4e, is the unit of the y-axis labeled "survival" in days?

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. We apologize for the lack of explanations for the axis values in Figures 3d and 4e and we have made revisions to both figures accordingly. We have noted that the unit "survival" on the y-axis of Figure 4e is in years, and we have already made the necessary supplement to clarify this. Thank you very much for your reminder.

      (4) The authors conducted their analysis using public databases but did not cite the original literature, nor did they discuss the similarities and differences between their findings and those in the original studies.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestions, and we deeply apologize for our carelessness. We have supplemented the original literature in the references and discussed the differences between this study and the original literature in the Discussion.

      (5) Some figures, particularly those involving heatmaps and t-SNE plots (e.g., Figures 1 and 3), present dense and complex data that may be challenging for readers to interpret. The heatmaps (Figure 1e-f and 3d) include many genes, but it is unclear how these genes were selected, and the scale of gene expression differences is difficult to interpret. Simplifying these figures by focusing on the most differentially expressed and clinically relevant genes (e.g., those with prognostic value) would improve readability.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestions. The t-SNE plots in Figures 1 and 3 primarily serve as a dimensionality reduction technique to visually present the clustering of multiple cells or samples based on gene expression, aiding readers in quickly identifying cell subpopulations. The heatmaps, on the other hand, are mainly used to showcase the differential expression of ferroptosis-related genes across different clinicopathological classifications and cell subpopulations, with varying shades of color helping readers quickly recognize gene expression differences among different cell subpopulations. The genes included in the heatmaps (Figures 1e-f and 3d) are sourced from the FerrDb website. We have uploaded the list of ferroptosis-related genes used in this study as Supplementary Table 1 and added the relevant steps in Method 2.3.

      (6) The study analyzes the expression of ferroptosis-related genes in different immune cells within the TME. The authors should discuss how these changes in gene expression may impact the function and behavior of immune cells.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. We have supplemented the discussion with detailed effects of the main differential genes (FOLR2 and TREM2) on the tumor immune response.

      (7) The authors analyzed the expression of ferroptosis-related genes in immune cells using single-cell sequencing data. However, they subsequently applied the selected genes to perform a risk factor analysis in tumor cells. Is the expression and function of these genes the same in immune cells and tumor cells? This seems questionable.

      Thank you very much for your suggestion. We also believe that there may be differences in the expression and function of genes between immune cells and tumor cells. However, some genes may exhibit similarities in their expression and function in immune cells and tumor cells, especially within the tumor immune microenvironment, due to the complex and tight interactions between immune cells and tumor cells (as shown in Figures 1d and 2h), and their expression levels can be related to the onset, progression, and prognosis of tumors.

      (8) While the risk score model based on ferroptosis-related genes is promising, it lacks experimental validation, which weakens the strength of the conclusions. The authors should consider conducting in vitro or in vivo experiments. These functional studies would provide essential evidence to support the model's predictive capability.

      Thank you for the constructive feedback. We fully recognize the importance of conducting functional studies to substantiate the predictive capability of the model. Therefore, we plan to conduct in vitro and in vivo experiments in our future research to provide the necessary evidence and further validate the model's effectiveness.

      (9) The manuscript predicts sensitivity to 27 drugs based on the risk score, but it lacks mechanistic insight into why patients in the high-risk group might be more responsive to certain drugs. Including a more detailed discussion of the molecular mechanisms underlying this drug sensitivity, particularly linking ferroptosis-related genes to drug metabolism or efficacy, would provide a stronger rationale for the clinical application of these findings.

      Thank you very much for your valuable suggestions. In the discussion, we thoroughly analyzed the mechanism of action of the drugs (ABT-263 and erlotinib) with the greatest difference in sensitivity between high-risk and low-risk groups, as well as their correlation with ferroptosis.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this revised report, Yamanaka and colleagues investigate a proposed mechanism by which testosterone modulates seminal plasma metabolites in mice. The authors identify oleic acid as a particularly important metabolite, derived from seminal vesicle epithelium, that stimulates linear progressive motility in isolated cauda epidydimal sperm in vitro. The authors provide additional experimental evidence of a testosterone dependent mechanism of oleic acid production by the seminal vesicle epithelium.

      Strengths:

      Often, reported epidydimal sperm from mice have lower percent progressive motility compared with sperm retrieved from the uterus or by comparison with human ejaculated sperm. The findings in this report may improve in vitro conditions to overcome this problem, as well as add important physiological context to the role of reproductive tract glandular secretions in modulating sperm behaviors. The strongest observations are related to the sensitivity of seminal vesicle epithelial cells to testosterone. The revisions include addition of methodological detail, modified language to reflect the nuance of some of the measurements, as well as re-performed experiments with more appropriate control groups. The findings are likely to be of general interest to the field by providing context for follow-on studies regarding the relationship between fatty acid beta oxidation and sperm motility pattern.

      Thank you for summarizing and your positive evaluation of our study.

      Weaknesses:

      Support for the proposed mechanism is stronger in this revised report than in the previous report, but there are many challenges in measuring sperm metabolism and its direct relationship with motility patterns. This study is no exception and largely relies on correlations between various experiments in lieu of direct testing. Additionally, the discussion is framed from a human pre-clinical perspective, and it should be noted that the reproductive physiology between mice and humans is very different.

      Thank you for pointing out the challenges in our paper. We appreciate your comment on the limited evidence supporting the direct relationship between sperm metabolism and motility patterns under current experimental conditions. Based on your and reviewer2’s suggestions, we have decided to remove the experiments and discussion on the “effects of OA on sperm metabolism, motility and fertility (Fig. 7, Supplemental Figure 5A and C-F.)” and the corresponding parts in the Discussion section from the paper. (See also Reviewer 2's main comment) These data mainly show correlations, and did not show direct evidence of causality. Instead, we added a new experiment to the manuscript, in which a lipid mixture that mimics the fatty acid profile secreted testosterone-dependently from seminal vesicle epithelial cells was added to the sperm culture medium (New Supplemental Figure 5, Lines 259-268). In this experiment, motility parameters were measured using CASA. This experiment evaluates the direct effects of lipid exposure on sperm motility. With these revisions, we are able to focus on the metabolic changes caused by testosterone in seminal vesicle epithelial cells, which are the central focus of our research. We have added a short statement agreeing the potential importance of OA and our intention to more rigorously investigate the role of OA in sperm function in subsequent studies (Lines 402-407).

      Furthermore, we have revised text, clearly state the limitations of the species difference and clarify that the translational aspects to humans are speculative (Lines 383-384, 395-397, 408-410).

      We appreciate your guidance. We believe that these changes will strengthen our research.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Using a combination of in vivo studies with testosterone-inhibited and aged mice with lower testosterone levels as well as isolated mouse and human seminal vesicle epithelial cells the authors show that testosterone induces an increase in glucose uptake. They find that testosterone induces a difference in gene expression with a focus on metabolic enzymes. Specifically, they identify increased expression of enzymes regulating cholesterol and fatty acid synthesis, leading to increased production of 18:1 oleic acid. The revised version strengthens the role of ACLY as the main regulator of seminal vesicle epithelial cell metabolic programming. 18:1 oleic acid is secreted by seminal vesicle epithelial cells and taken up by sperm, inducing an increase in mitochondrial respiration. The difference in sperm motility and in vivo fertilization in the presence of 18:1 oleic acid and the absence of testosterone, however, is small. Additional experiments should be included to further support that oleic acid positively affects sperm function.

      Thank you very much for carefully reading the manuscript and for your comments. We appreciate your understanding that the role of ACLY in metabolic programming of seminal vesicle epithelial cells has been strengthened in the revised version. On the other hand, we agree with your view that the increase in sperm motility and fertilization rate by oleic acid is minimal under the current experimental conditions. We agree that further evidence is needed to support our conclusion regarding the positive effects of oleic acid on sperm function. Based on your comments and our re-evaluation of the data, we have decided to remove the experiments and discussion on “OA and sperm motility” from the current paper (Fig. 7, Supplemental Figure 5A and C-F). In the revised paper, we have significantly toned down the claims on the previous role of oleic acid and instead focused on the metabolic regulatory mechanisms of seminal vesicle epithelial cells.

      We hope that these revisions address your concerns and improve the overall clarity of the manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Note from the reviewing editor: The reviewers agree that the revised manuscript is significantly improved and view the work as important. Both reviewers agree that the evidence for testosterone effects on seminal vesicle epithelial cells to support fatty acid synthesis is strong and suggest that the authors tone down their conclusion of oleic acid effect on sperm motility as the effect is very small. With this minor changes, the evidence to support the conclusion of the study is viewed as solid.

      Thank you for recognizing the improvements that we have made to our manuscript and for appreciating the importance of our research. We also appreciate your assessment that the evidence for the effect of testosterone on seminal vesicle epithelial cells that support fatty acid synthesis is solid.

      On the other hand, we agree with the two reviewers that the effect of oleic acid on sperm motility is limited and that the relevant data do not measure a direct relationship. Therefore, we have decided to withdraw the data set on the effect of oleic acid on sperm (Fig. 7, Supplemental Figure 5A and C-F) and focus this paper on seminal vesicle epithelial cells (in response to reviewer 2's suggestion). Given that testosterone-induced lipid (Fatty acid) synthesis in seminal vesicle epithelial cells is a key aspect of our study, we have included additional experiments in the revised manuscript to show how lipids affect sperm (New Supplemental Figure5, Lines 259-263).

      With these revisions, the manuscript emphasizes the importance of testosterone-dependent fatty acid synthesis in seminal vesicle epithelial cells and the fact that this includes oleic acid. The title has also been partially revised in line with these revisions.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor Comments:

      (1) The authors indicate in the methods that extracellular flux analysis was normalized to cell count. However, the y-axis units in Figs 4, 8, 9 and SFig 9 are not normalized.

      (2) The OA label appears to be missing from Fig 7A. Additionally, the scale bar is offset in one of the images and the length of the scale bar does not appear to be mentioned in the figure legend.

      Thank you for raising these points. We have corrected.

      Fig. 7 has been withdrawn in response to Reviewer 2's suggestion.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      With the experiments included in their revised version the authors strengthen their conclusions about testosterone-induced metabolic reprogramming in seminal vesicle cells resulting in reduced proliferation. The experiments surrounding ACLY are well-designed and give insights into the underlying molecular mechanisms. For other parts, the manuscript became less clear and it is often hard to follow the author's line of thoughts for their conclusions.

      Based on the experiments shown in the manuscript this reviewer is still not convinced that OA positively affects sperm function. The changes in linear motility are minor, blastocyst levels are lower and the authors do not show that OA alone positively affects cleavage rate during AI. Without additional experiments that show a stronger effect on sperm function, the authors should consider focusing the manuscript exclusively on seminal vesicle epithelial cells.

      Thank you for your constructive comments on our paper. We thank the reviewer for pointing out that the effect of oleic acid (OA) on sperm function is limited in our current experiments. As reviewer 1 also pointed out, we agree that further experiments and improved methodology are needed to reliably demonstrate the functional effects of OA on sperm. Because the strength of the data on the direct relationship between fatty acids in seminal fluid and improved sperm function is currently insufficient, we have removed the data set for oleic acid and sperm motility (Fig. 7, Supplemental Figure 5A and C-F) and focused on the “the mechanism of metabolic regulation of testosterone in seminal vesicle epithelial cells”. We have consistently narrowed the focus of the paper to the theme of “how testosterone changes energy metabolism in seminal vesicle epithelial cells”. In accordance with this change, the structure of the paper has also been partially revised (red text in the manuscript). With these revisions, the main point of the paper focuses on the mechanism by which testosterone regulates metabolic pathways in the seminal vesicle epithelial cells.

      For more detailed revisions, please see the responses to your comments below.

      (1) 45-55 still need major revision. It will not become clear to the reader what the authors mean by epididymal maturation. 'Ability to fertilize in in vitro?' Epididymal sperm are moving linearly in the absence of seminal vesicle fluid. Increased progressive motility, hyperactivation, and the ability to undergo the acrosome reaction are induced upon exposure to seminal vesicle fluid. The authors should introduce the concept of capacitation and that capacitation can be induced in vitro by exposure to bicarbonate and a cholesterol acceptor.

      Thank you for pointing out the ambiguity of epididymal maturation, the need to clarify the concept of capacitation, and the role of seminal plasma in this context. The revised text explains that epididymal maturation only gives sperm their potential ability to fertilize. It also explains that it is the subsequent capacitation process—inducible in vitro by incubation with bicarbonate and cholesterol acceptors—that gives full fertilization potential. On the other hands, we emphasize that in vivo, seminal plasma, which contains both capacitation-promoting and decapacitation factors, plays a key role in fine-tuning the timing of capacitation, ensuring that sperm acquire fertilization competence at the appropriate moment. We hope that these revisions clarify our intended meaning and strengthen the overall message of the paragraph. (lines 42-54)

      “Sperm that have completed spermatogenesis in the testis acquire their potential to fertilize while maturing in the epididymis (5–7). The physiological change of sperm during fertilization process are collectively referred to as “capacitation”. This change includes a large amplitude of flagella (called hyperactivation) and developing the capacity to undergo the acrosome reaction, and can be induced by culturing sperm collected from the epididymis in a medium containing bicarbonate and cholesterol acceptors (8, 9). However, once capacitation is complete, sperm cannot maintain that state for a long time. Therefore, even if epididymal sperm that have not been exposed to seminal plasma are artificially inseminated into the cervix or uterus, the fertilization rate remains low (10–12). That is because, in vivo, during ejaculation, exposure of epididymal sperm to seminal plasma masks the unintended capacitation as they pass through the female reproductive tract and ensures fertilization of sperm that reach the oviduct (13). In other words, seminal plasma plays an important role in fine-tuning the timing of sperm capacitation and in maintaining the sustained sperm motility needed to reach the oviduct.”

      (2) 81: Similar as in their rebuttal the authors should further elute on the connection between fructose, citrate, and testosterone. That still does not become clear. Based on the author's explanation in the rebuttal, why are citrate and fructose levels higher when the animals are castrated?

      We thank you for the opportunity to clarify our statement regarding the relationship between fructose, citrate, and testosterone. Our original explanation was intended to reflect the fact that testosterone from the testes has a stimulating effect on the accessory reproductive glands, and to report that the concentrations of fructose and citric acid were higher in the non-castrated (control) animals than in the castrated animals. In castrated animals, the absence of testosterone leads to decreased activity of these glands and, consequently, lower levels of these metabolites. To make this clear, we have revised the manuscript as follows. (lines 76-82)

      “Several specific factors produced by the male accessory glands that contribute to seminal plasma and impact male fertility have been elucidated. For example, surgical removal of seminal vesicles in male mice and rats was associated with infertility (17, 22, 23). The observations that fructose (24) and citric acid (25) concentrations in seminal plasma of control mice and rats are higher than in castrated animals suggest that the specific metabolism of the accessory glands might be affected by testosterone derived from the testes, which activate intracellular androgen receptors (AR; NR3C4) required for gene regulation of transcription.”

      (3) 111: This reviewer does not understand the author's obsession with reporting linear motility. Sperm are moving linearly when isolated from the epididymis. Again, increase of progressive motility is a well-defined hallmark of capacitation and primarily used in the field when discussing changes in sperm motility during capacitation. This reviewer is assuming that the changes in progressive vs linear motility in Fig. 7 are not significant because the data is more scattered. The % increase seems to be approximately the same. The same is true for Fig. 8. The increase in LIN is so small and not dose-dependent that this reviewer is not comfortable making that one of the main conclusions of the manuscript.

      Our claim is based on the observation that seminal vesicle secretions significantly improve the linear motility (VSL and LIN) of sperm even in an environment that does not contain capacitation-inducing factors such as BSA. We interpret this as a survival strategy for sperm to pass through the female reproductive tract efficiently. Therefore, we believe that this does not mean that the meaning of “progressive motility” in the context of conventional capacitation is the same as that of progressive motility observed in seminal plasma.

      However, the reviewer's point that the current data set does not sufficiently support what the minor increase in linear motility caused by oleic acid means is agreed with. Therefore, we have decided to withdraw the dataset on the effect of oleic acid on sperm motility (Fig. 7, Supplemental Figure 5A and C-F) and have revised the conclusion. (Lines 406-410)

      (4) 128: For the mitochondrial membrane potential measurements the authors should mention that they included antimycin as a control. The manuscript would benefit from including scatter plots with unloaded controls to support their gating strategy. In its current stage, the gating between low and high membrane potential seems arbitrary.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have included an explanation of antimycin as a control in the main text (Lines 920-921). In addition, we have added some reference scatter plots and also added an explanation of the gating strategy between low and high membrane potentials (Supplemental Figure 1C and D, Lines 1101-1104). We hope this change will make the manuscript clearer.

      (5) 190: What do the authors mean by: 'However, there was no difference in the Oligomycin-sensitive ECAR, indicating that testosterone may increase glucose metabolism but does not enhance the expression of a group of enzymes involved in the glycolytic pathway.'

      Our original intention was to state that testosterone probably increases basal glycolytic flux via increased glucose uptake (as supported by the GLUT4 translocation data), but does not increase maximal glycolytic capacity, as indicated by the lack of difference in oligomycin-sensitive ECAR.

      However, as Reviewer 1 previously pointed out, we agree that the assay conditions themselves, such as the use of oligomycin to inhibit oxidative mitochondria, may create non-physiological conditions and not fully reflect the energy distribution in vivo. Under these conditions, there is a possibility that the flow of glycolysis will increase artificially as a compensatory reaction, and parameters such as “maximum glycolytic capacity” should have been interpreted with caution.

      Therefore, we have revised the manuscript to clarify that our data are a single-time point under defined experimental conditions and do not necessarily provide direct insight into changes in expression or activity of individual glycolytic enzymes.

      “These data indicate that testosterone enhances glucose utilization. This leads to the interpretation that testosterone increases the flow of glycolysis by increasing glucose uptake and alters metabolic flux distribution.” (Lines 186-188)

      (6) 205: Could the authors elaborate further on how they came to this conclusion: 'These results suggest that testosterone does not reduce transient enzyme activity in mitochondria but rather weakens the metabolic pathway of the mitochondrial TCA cycle and/or the electron transport chain due to the changes in gene expression patterns in seminal vesicle epithelial cells.' Based on their results at this point the authors have no insights about changes in enzyme activity or gene expression that might explain the phenotype.

      Our statement is based on the following observations. In testosterone-treated cells, the addition of glucose increased ECAR, suggesting an increase in glycolytic flux due to an increase in glucose uptake. On the other hand, mitochondrial respiratory parameters (basal respiration, oligomycin-sensitive respiration, FCCP-uncoupled respiration, and reserve respiratory capacity) were significantly decreased under testosterone treatment.

      From these results, it was speculated that testosterone promotes the redistribution of metabolic flux, directing it away from mitochondrial oxidative phosphorylation and towards the glycolytic pathway and, possibly, lipid synthesis. However, as the reviewers correctly point out, at this point, we have not directly measured changes in the activity or expression of individual enzymes in the TCA cycle or ETC. Therefore, in the next experiment, we extracted mRNA from the cells and performed gene expression analysis using real-time PCR. To make this clear, we have revised the manuscript as follows.

      “Overall, these data indicate that testosterone promotes the redistribution of metabolic flux. In other words, testosterone increased glycolysis in seminal vesicle epithelial cells while decreasing mitochondrial respiration. To determine whether these changes were accompanied by changes in gene expression of specific metabolic-related enzymes, we analyzed gene expression levels.” (Lines 201-205)

      (7) 219: Characterizing ACLY as an enzyme of the ETC is misleading. ACLY is a cytosolic enzyme that connects the TCA cycle with fatty acid synthesis.

      We would like to thank you for pointing out that the description of the function of ACLY could be misunderstood. We agree that characterizing ACLY as an enzyme of the ETC could be misleading. Therefore, we have revised the sentence to clearly indicate that ACLY is a cytosolic enzyme that links the TCA cycle with fatty acid synthesis. The revised text is as follows:

      "Interestingly, testosterone significantly increased the expression of Acly, which encodes a cytoplasmic enzyme that converts citrate transported from the TCA cycle into acetyl-CoA, a substrate that is essential for fatty acid synthesis." (lines216-218)

      (8) 228: Which results support that ETC proteins were upregulated by flutamide?

      We appreciate the reviewer for this point. In preliminary experiments, we analyzed ETC protein expression using real-time qPCR. Our data show that treatment with flutamide significantly upregulates the expression of genes involved in mitochondrial ETC, such as mtND6, while decreasing the expression of the lipogenic genes Acly and Acc. These additional data are now presented in Supplementary Figure S3B. (lines 223-226)

      (9) 245: Aren't the authors showing in Fig. 5 that glut4 expression is reduced in seminal vesicle epithelial cells upon testosterone treatment? How does that fit into the author's hypothesis?

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have already responded to a similar comment from Reviewer 3 in a previous revision. Please refer to our response to Reviewer 3 in a previous version.

      (10) 285: Based on the author's results OA increases the oocyte cleavage rate but then reduces the rate of blastocyst to cleaved oocyte. Doesn't that mean OA affects negatively early development?

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful comment. The one-hour pre-treatment is designed to reflect the transient exposure of sperm to the seminal plasma during ejaculation. In this context, it is unlikely that such a short exposure would impair the overall developmental potential of the embryo. However, although pre-conditioning with oleic acid does not ultimately affect the development of the offspring, it may lead to a decrease in the blastocyst rate at a certain point (approximately 96-120 hours after fertilization). We agree that additional research is needed to demonstrate this.

      Therefore, because the experiments related to the effects of oleic acid on sperm and fertilization are currently incomplete, we have decided to withdraw them for future research.

      (11) 305: What happens to pyruvate and lactate levels when ACLY expression is reduced?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s question regarding the fate of pyruvate and lactate when ACLY expression is reduced. In the absence of testosterone (Ctrl), the expression level of ACLY decreases. At this time, the concentration of pyruvate in the culture medium increased compared to that of testosterone (Testo; Fig. 4D,E). This is probably a reflection of the fact that when the expression of ACLY is suppressed, the rate at which the products of the glycolytic pathway are converted to the fat-producing pathway (i.e., the conversion of citrate to acetyl-CoA) decreases.

      On the other hand, lactate levels did not change significantly. This suggests that the flow of lactate production via lactate dehydrogenase is relatively constant, independent of metabolic reprogramming by ACLY.

      Therefore, our data suggest that a decrease in ACLY expression leads to a decrease in pyruvate demand, while lactate production is maintained. We interpret these findings as supporting the idea that ACLY is important for directing the carbon produced by the glycolytic pathway to lipid synthesis (by transporting citrate from the mitochondria).

      We hope that this explanation clarifies the interpretation of the data.

      Minor revision:

      189: ECAR: extracellular acidification rate. Please correct.

      We have corrected this. (Lines 184-185)

      199: Pyruvate is not synthesized, it is metabolized from PEP. Please correct.

      The following corrections have been made. “pyruvate is metabolized from phosphoenolpyruvic acid through glycolysis”. (Lines 194-195)

      In addition, minor revisions were made to improve the clarity of the overall text.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public reviews:

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) This manuscript introduces a useful curation pipeline of antibody-antigen structures downloaded from the PDB database. The antibody-antigen structures are presented in a new database called AACDB, alongside annotations that were either corrected from those present in the PDB database or added de-novo with a solid methodology. Sequences, structures, and annotations can be very easily downloaded from the AACDB website, speeding up the development of structure-based algorithms and analysis pipelines to characterize antibody-antigen interactions. However, AACDB is missing some key annotations that would greatly enhance its usefulness.

      Here are detailed comments regarding the three strengths above:

      I think potentially the most significant contribution of this database is the manual data curation to fix errors present in the PDB entries, by cross-referencing with the literature. However, as a reviewer, validating the extent and the impact of these corrections is hard, since the authors only provided a few anecdotal examples in their manuscript.

      I have personally verified some of the examples presented by the authors and found that SAbDab appears to fix the mistakes related to the misidentification of antibody chains, but not other annotations.

      (a) "the species of the antibody in 7WRL was incorrectly labeled as "SARS coronavirus B012" in both PDB and SabDab" → I have verified the mistake and fix, and that SAbDab does not fix is, just uses the pdb annotation.

      (b) "1NSN, the resolution should be 2.9 , but it was incorrectly labeled as 2.8" → I have verified the mistake and fix, and that sabdab does not fix it, just uses the PDB annotation.

      (c) "mislabeling of antibody chains as other proteins (e.g. in 3KS0, the light chain of B2B4 antibody was misnamed as heme domain of flavocytochrome b2)" → SAbDab fixes this as well in this case.

      (d) "misidentification of heavy chains as light chains (e.g. both two chains of antibody were labeled as light chain in 5EBW)" → SAbDab fixes this as well in this case.

      I personally believe the authors should make public the corrections made, and describe the procedures - if systematic - to identify and correct the mistakes. For example, what was the exact procedure (e.g. where were sequences found, how were the sequences aligned, etc.) to find mutations? Was the procedure run on every entry?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s valuable feedback. Our correction procedures combined manual curation with systematic sequence analysis. While most metadata discrepancies were resolved through cross-referencing original literature, we implemented a structured approach for identifying mutations in specific cases. For PDB entries labeled as variants (e.g., "Bevacizumab mutant" or "Ipilimumab variant Ipi.106") where the "Mutation(s)" field was annotated as "NO," we retrieved the canonical therapeutic antibody sequence from Thera-SAbDab, then performed pairwise sequence alignment against the PDB entry using BLAST program to identified mutated residues.

      This procedure was not applied to all entries, as mutations are context-dependent. Therapeutic antibodies have well-defined reference sequences, enabling systematic alignment. For antibodies lacking unambiguous wild-type references (e.g., research-grade or non-therapeutic antibodies), mutation annotations were directly inherited from the PDB or literature.

      All corrections have been publicly archived in AACDB. We have added a detailed discussion of this issue in the section “2.3 Metadata” of revised manuscript.

      (2) I believe the splitting of the pdb files is a valuable contribution as it standardizes the distribution of antibody-antigen complexes. Indeed, there is great heterogeneity in how many copies of the same structure are present in the structure uploaded to the PDB, generating potential artifacts for machine learning applications to pick up on. That being said, I have two thoughts both for the authors and the broader community. First, in the case of multiple antibodies binding to different epitopes on the same antigen, one should not ignore the potentially stabilizing effect that the binding of one antibody has on the complex, thereby enabling the binding of the second antibody. In general, I urge the community to think about what is the most appropriate spatial context to consider when modeling the stability of interactions from crystal structure data. Second, and in a similar vein, some antigens occur naturally as homomultimers - e.g. influenza hemagglutinin is a homotrimer. Therefore, to analyze the stability of a full-antigen-antibody structure, I believe it would be necessary to consider the full homo-trimer, whereas, in the current curation of AACDB with the proposed data splitting, only the monomers are present.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments regarding the splitting of PDB files and we appreciate the opportunity to address the reviewer’s thoughtful concerns.

      Firstly, when two antibodies bind to distinct epitopes on the same antigen, we would like to clarify that this scenario can be divided into two cases based on the experimental context: Case1: When two antibodies bind to distinct epitopes on the same antigen, and their complexes are determined in separate structures. For example, SAR650984 (PDB: 4CMH) and daratumumab (PDB: 7DHA) target CD38 at non-overlapping epitopes. These two antibody-antigen complexes were determined independently, and their structures do not influence each other. Case 2 : When the crystal structure contains a ternary complex with two antibodies and an antigen, as in the example of 6OGE discussed in Section 2.2 of our manuscript. After reviewing the original literature, the experiment confirmed that the order of Fab binding does not affect the formation of the ternary complex, and the binding of one antibody does not enhance the binding of the other. This supports the rationale for splitting 6OGE into two separate structures. However, we acknowledge that not all ternary complexes in the PDB provide such detailed experimental descriptions in their original literature. We agree with the reviewer that in some cases, one antibody may stabilize the structure to facilitate the binding of a second antibody. For instance, in 3QUM, the 5D5A5 antibody stabilizes the structure, enabling the binding of the 5D3D11 antibody to human prostate-specific antigen. Such sandwich complexes are indeed valuable for identifying true epitopes and paratopes. Importantly, splitting the structure does not alter the interaction sites.

      Secondly, we fully agree with the reviewer that for antigens that naturally exist as homomultimers (e.g., influenza hemagglutinin as a homotrimer), the full multimeric structure should be considered when analyzing stability. In such cases, users can directly utilize the original PDB structures provided in their multimeric form. Our splitting approach is intended to provide an additional option for cases where monomeric analysis is sufficient or preferred, but it does not preclude the use of the original multimeric structures when necessary.

      (3) I think the manuscript is lacking in justification about the numbers used as cutoffs (1A^2 for change in SASA and 5A for maximum distance for contact) The authors just cite other papers applying these two types of cutoffs, but the underlying physico-chemical reasons are not explicit even in these papers. I think that, if the authors want AACDB to be used globally for benchmarks, they should provide direct sources of explanations of the cutoffs used, or provide multiple cutoffs. Indeed, different cutoffs are often used (e.g. ATOM3D uses 6A instead of 5A to determine contact between a protein and a small molecule https://datasets-benchmarks-proceedings.neurips.cc/paper/2021/hash/c45147dee729311ef5b5c3003946c48f-Abstract-round1.html). I think the authors should provide a figure with statistics pertaining to the interface atoms. I think showing any distribution differences between interface atoms determined according to either strategy (number of atoms, correlation between change in SASA and distance...) would be fundamental to understanding the two strategies. I think other statistics would constitute an enhancement as well (e.g. proportion of heavy vs. light chain residues).

      Some obvious limitations of AACDB in its current form include:

      AACDB only contains entries with protein-based antigens of at most 50 amino acids in length. This excludes non-protein-based antigens, such as carbohydrate- and nucleotide-based, as well as short peptide antigens.

      AACDB does not include annotations of binding affinity, which are present in SAbDab and have been proven useful both for characterizing drivers of antibody-antigen interactions (cite https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0969212624004362?via%3Dihub) and for benchmarking antigen-specific antibody-design algorithms (cite https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2023.12.10.570461v1)).

      We thank the reviewer for raising this critical point about the cutoff values used in AACDB. In the current study, the selection of the threshold value is very objective; the threshold chosen in the manuscript is summarized based on existing literature, and we have provided more literature support in the manuscript. The criteria for defining interacting amino acids in established tools, typically do not set the ΔSASA exceed 1 Å2 and the distance exceed 6 Å. While our manuscript emphasizes widely accepted thresholds for consistency with prior benchmarks, AACDB explicitly provides raw ΔSASA and distance values for all annotated residues. Users can dynamically filter the data from downloaded files by excluding entries exceeding their preferred thresholds (e.g., selecting 5Å instead of 6Å). This ensures adaptability to diverse research needs. In the revised version, we reset the distance threshold to 6 Å and calculated the interacting amino acids in order to give the user a wider range of choices. In the section “3.2 Database browse and search” of revised manuscript, we provide a description of the flexible choice of thresholds for practical use.

      Furthermore, distance and ΔSASA are two distinct metrics for evaluating interactions. Distance directly quantifies spatial proximity between atoms, reflecting physical contacts such as van der Waals interactions or hydrogen bonds, and is ideal for identifying direct spatial adjacency. ΔSASA, on the other hand, measures changes in solvent accessibility of residues during binding, capturing the contribution of buried surfaces to binding free energy. Even for residues not in direct contact, reduced SASA due to conformational changes may indicate indirect functional roles.

      As demonstrated through comparisons on the detailed information pages, the sets of interacting amino acids defined by these two methods differ by only a few residues, with no significant variation in their overall distributions. However, since interaction patterns vary significantly across different complexes, analyzing residue distributions across all structures using both criteria is not feasible.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting these limitations. AACDB currently focuses on protein-based antigens ≤50 amino acids to prioritize structural consistency, which excludes non-protein antigens and shorter peptides. While affinity annotations are critical for benchmarking antibody design tools, these data were not integrated in this release due to insufficient data verification caused by internal team constraints. We acknowledge these gaps and plan to expand antigen diversity and incorporate affinity metrics in future updates.

      Reviewer #2:

      Summary:

      Antibodies, thanks to their high binding affinity and specificity to cognate protein targets, are increasingly used as research and therapeutic tools. In this work, Zhou et al. have created, curated, and made publicly available a new database of antibody-antigen complexes to support research in the field of antibody modelling, development, and engineering.

      Strengths:

      The authors have performed a manual curation of antibody-antigen complexes from the Protein Data Bank, rectifying annotation errors; they have added two methods to estimate paratope-epitope interfaces; they have produced a web interface that is capable of both effective visualisation and of summarising the key useful information in one page. The database is also cross-linked to other databases that contain information relevant to antibody developability and therapeutic applications.

      Weaknesses:

      The database does not import all the experimental information from PDB and contains only complexes with large protein targets.

      Thank you for the valuable feedback. As previously responded to Reviewer 1, due to limitations within our team, comprehensive data integration from PDB has not been achieved in the current version. We acknowledge the significance of expanding the database to encompass a broader range of experimental information and complexes with diverse target sizes. Regrettably, immediate updates to address these limitations are not feasible at this time. Nevertheless, we are committed to enhancing the database in upcoming upgrades to provide users with a more comprehensive and inclusive resource

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) Line 194: "produce" → "produced"

      We thank the reviewer for the feedback. We have checked the grammar and spelling carefully in the revised manuscript.

      (2) As mentioned in the public review, I think adding binding affinity annotations would greatly enhance the use cases for the database.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. As the response in “Public review”. Due to team constraints, these data are not integrated into this release but are being collated. We recognize these gaps and plan to expand antigenic diversity and incorporate affinity metrics in future updates.

      (3) I think adding a visualization of interface atoms and contacts on an entry's webpage would be useful for someone exploring specific entries. It also would be useful if the authors provided a pymol command to select interface residues since that's a procedure any structural biologist is likely to do.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s constructive suggestions. In response to the request for enhanced visualization and accessibility of interface residue information, we have implemented the following improvements: (1) Web Interface Visualization. On the entry-specific webpage, we have added an interactive visualization window that highlights the antigen-antibody interaction interface using distinct colors. The interaction interface visualization has been incorporated into Figure 5 of the revised manuscript, with a detailed description. (2) PyMOL Command Accessibility. The “Help” page now provides step-by-step PyMOL commands to select and visualize interface residues.

      (4) I think the authors should provide headers to the files containing interface residues according to the change-in-SASA criterion, as they do for those computed according to contact. This would avoid unnecessary confusion - however slight - and make parsing easier. I was initially confused by the meaning of the last column, though after a minute I understood it to be the change in SASA.

      We thank the reviewer for providing such detailed feedback. We thank the reviewer for the comment and the suggestion. We have provided headers for the files of the interacting residues defined by ΔSASA.

      (5) Line 233: "AACDB's data processing pipeline supports mmCIF files" → The meaning and implications of this statement are not obvious to me, and are mentioned nowhere else in the paper. Do you mean that in AACDB there are structure entries that the RCSB PDB database only has in mmCIF file format, and not .pdb format? So, effectively, there are some entries in AACDB that are not in any other antibody-specific database?I checked and, as of Dec 3rd, 2024, there are 41 structures in AACDB that are NOT in SAbDab. Manually checking 5 of those 41 structures, none are mmCIF-only structures.

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable comment. Because of the size of the structures within certain entries, representing them in a single PDB format data file is not feasible due to the excessive number of atoms and polymer chains they contain. As a result, PDB stores these structures in “mmcif” format files. In AACDB, 47 entries, such as 7SOF, 7NKT, 7B27, and 6T9D, are only available in the “mmCIF” format from the PDB. The “.pdb” and “.cif” files contain atomic coordinates in distinct text formats, and the segmentation of these structure files is automatically conducted based on manually annotated antibody-antigen chains. To accommodate this, we have incorporated these considerations into our file processing pipeline, thereby enabling a fully automated file segmentation process. Additionally, we employed Naccess to calculate interatomic distances. However, since this software only accepts .pdb format files as input, we also converted all split .cif files into .pdb format within our fully automated pipeline. We apologize for the lack of clarity in the original manuscript and have included a more detailed explanation in the "2.2 PDB Splitting" section of the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) In SabDab and PDB, experimental binding affinities are also reported: could the authors comment on whether they also imported this information and double-checked it against the original paper? If it wasn't imported, that might discourage some users and should be considered as an extension for the future.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment and the suggestion. As the response in “Public review”. Due to current resource constraints, quantitative affinity data has not been incorporated into this release but is undergoing systematic curation. We explicitly recognize these limitations and propose a two-pronged strategy for future iterations: (1) broadening antigen diversity coverage through expanded structural sampling, and (2) integrating quantitative binding affinity measurements. In the Discussion section, we have included description outlining the planned enhancements.

      (2) Line 49-50: the references mentioned in connection to deep learning methods for antibody-antigen predictions seem a bit limited given the amount of articles in this field, with 3 of 4 references on one method only (SEPPA), could the authors expand this list to reflect a bit more the state of the art?

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We agree that more relevant studies should be listed and therefore more references are provided in the revised manuscript.

      When mentioning the limitations of the existing databases, it feels a bit that the criticism is not fully justified. For instance:

      Line 52-53: could the authors elaborate on the reasons why such an identification is challenging? (Isn't it possible to make an efficient database-filtered search? Or rather, should one highlight that a more focussed resource is convenient and why?)

      Thank you for feedback. In this study, the keywords "antibody complex," "antigen complex," and "immunoglobulin complex," were employed during data collection. PDB returned over 30,000 results, of which only one-tenth met our criteria after rigorous filtering. This demonstrates that keyword searches, while useful, inherently limit result precision and introduce substantial redundancy, likely due to the PDB's search mechanism. That’s why we illustrated the significant challenges in identifying antibody-antigen complexes from general protein structures in the PDB.

      Line 55: reading the website http://www.abybank.org/abdb/, it would be fairer to say that the web interface lacks updates, as the database and the code have gone through some updates. Could the authors provide a concrete example of the reason why: 'The AbDb database currently lacks proper organization and management of this valuable data.'?

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting this issue. In our original manuscript, the statement that the AbDb database "lacks proper organization and management" was based on the absence of explicit statement regarding data updates on its official website at the time of submission, even though internal updates to its content may have occurred. We fully respect the long-standing contributions of AbDb to antibody structural research, and our comments were solely directed at the specific state of the database at that time. As the reviewer noted, following the release of our preprint, we have also taken note of AbDb's recent updates. To reflect the latest developments and avoid potential misinterpretation, we have revised the original statement in revised manuscript.

      Also 'this rapid updating process may inadvertently overlook a significant amount of information that requires thorough verification,': it's difficult for me to understand what this means in practice. Could the authors clarify if they simply mean that SabDab collects information from PDB and therefore tends to propagate annotation errors from there? If yes, I think it's enough to state it in these terms, and for sure I agree that the reason is that correcting these annotation errors requires a substantial amount of work.

      We thank the reviewer for providing such detailed feedback on the manuscript. We acknowledge that SabDab represents a highly valuable contribution to the field, and its rapid update mechanism has significantly advanced related research areas. However, as stated by the reviewer, we aim to clarify that SabDab primarily relies on automated metadata extraction from the PDB for annotation, and its rapid update process inherently inherits raw data from upstream sources. According to their paper, manual curation is only applied when the automated pipeline fails to resolve structural ambiguities. This workflow—dependent on PDB annotations with limited manual verification—may propagate errors provided by PDB. Examples include species misannotation and mutation status misinterpretation. We fully agree with the reviewer's observation that correcting errors in such cases necessitates labor-intensive manual curation, which is a core motivation for our study.

      Line 86: why 'Structures that consisted solely of one type of antibody were excluded'? Why exclude complexes with antigens shorter than 50 amino acids? These complexes are genuine antibody-antigen complexes.

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable question. The AACBD database is dedicated to curating structural data of antigen-antibody complexes. Structures featuring only a single antibody type are classified as free antibodies and systematically excluded from the database due to the absence of protein-bound partners. During data screening , we retained sequences shorter than 50 amino acids by categorizing them as peptides rather than eliminating them outright. The current release exclusively encompasses complexes with protein-based antigens. Meanwhile, complexes involving peptide, haptens, and nucleic acid antigens are undergoing systematic curation, with planned inclusion in future updates to broaden antigen category representation.

      Line 96 needs a capital letter at the beginning.

      Line 107: 'this would generate' → 'this generates' (given it is something that has been implemented, correct?).

      Line 124: missing an 'of'.

      Line 163: inspiring by -> inspired by.

      Thank you for feedback. All of the above grammatical or spelling errors have been revised in the manuscript.

      Line 109-111: apart from the example, it would be good to spell out the general rule applied to anti-idiotypic antibodies.

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable feedback. For anti-idiotypic antibodies complex. the partner antibody is treated as a dual-chain antigen, , necessitating individual evaluation of heavy chain and light chain interactions with the anti-idiotypic component. We have given a general rule for anti-idiotypic antibodies in section “2.2 PDB splitting” of revised manuscript.

      Line 155-159: could the authors provide references for the two choices (based on sasa and any-atom distance) that they adopted to define interacting residues?

      We thank the reviewer for the comment and the suggestion. As the same as the response to reviewer #1 in Public review. The interacting residues definition and the threshold chosen in the manuscript is summarized based on existing literature. We have added additional references for support in section “1.Introduction”. Our resource does not provide a fixed amino acid list. Instead, all interacting residues are explicitly documented alongside their corresponding ΔSASA (solvent-accessible surface area changes) and intermolecular distances, allowing researchers to flexibly select residue pairs based on customized thresholds from downloadable datasets. Furthermore, aligning with widely adopted criteria in current literature—where interactions are defined by ΔSASA >1 Ų and atomic distances <6 Å, we have recalibrated our analysis in the revised version. Specifically, we replaced the previous 5 Å distance threshold with a 6 Å cutoff to recalculate interacting residues.

      Line 176-178: could the authors re-phrase this sentence to clarify what they mean by 'change in the distribution'?

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. Our search was conducted with an end date of November 2023. However, Figure 3B includes an entry dated 2024. Upon reviewing this record, we identified that the discrepancy arises from the supersession of the 7SIX database entry (originally released in December 2022) by the 8TM1 version in January 2024. This version update explains the apparent chronological inconsistency. We regret any lack of clarity in our original description and have revised the corresponding section in the manuscript to explicitly clarify this change of database.

      Caption Figure 3: please spell out all the acronyms in the figure. Provide the date when the last search was performed (i.e., the date of the last update of these statistics).

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We have systematically expanded all acronyms and included update dates for statistics in the legend of Figure 3. Corresponding changes have also been made to the statistical pages on the website.

      Finally, it would be advisable to do a general check on the use of the English language (e.g. I noted a few missing articles). In Figure 5 DrugBank contains typos.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's meticulous attention to linguistic precision. We have corrected the typographical error in Figure 5 and conducted a comprehensive review of the entire manuscript to ensure accuracy and clarity.

    1. Author response:

      We are highly appreciative of your constructive criticism and that you found that our findings of interest and significance. Based on your helpful suggestions, we plan to revise the paper as following:

      (1) Although ETFDH is reduced, but not mutated across neoplasia, we appreciate your point pertinent to catalytically activity of ETFDH. To this end, in the revision we are planning to compare the effects of rescues using wild type ETFDH or one of the MADD-associated mutants with compromised catalytic activity.

      (2) We intend to measure steady-state nucleotide levels as a function of ETFDH status in the cell. If time and/or funding allow, we will also perform appropriate labelling experiments.

      (3) We will revise the text of the manuscript to address the minor points raised by the reviewers.

      Again, we would like to thank you for helpful comments, which we aim to address as outlined above and hopefully further improve our report.

    1. Author response:

      We sincerely thank all reviewers for their thoughtful, detailed, and supportive evaluations of our manuscript. We are very pleased that the reviewers appreciated the integrative approach of our study, the quality of the imaging and analyses, and the insights provided into the parallel evolution of biomineralization mechanisms in sponges and corals.

      We are carefully considering all the suggestions made, including those regarding the improvement of figure clarity and the clarification of certain image interpretations. These comments are extremely valuable, and we are preparing a detailed point-by-point reply to accompany our revised manuscript.

      It was also brought to our attention that the links to the Zenodo repository were incorrect. We apologize for this oversight and any inconvenience it may have caused and will updae the links in our revised manuscript. In the meantime, the correct Zenodo repositories can be accessed using the following links:

      https://zenodo.org/records/14755899

      https://zenodo.org/records/13847772

      We again thank the reviewers for their constructive feedback, which will help us to further strengthen the manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      We thank the editors and reviewers for their thoughtful and constructive evaluation of our manuscript, “Krüppel Regulates Cell Cycle Exit and Limits Adult Neurogenesis of Mushroom Body Neural Progenitors in Drosophila.” We are pleased that all reviewers recognised the novelty and significance of identifying Krüppel (Kr) as a key transcription factor promoting timely termination of mushroom body neuroblast (MBNB) proliferation, and the potential antagonistic function of Kr-h1.

      We appreciate the helpful suggestions aimed at improving the mechanistic clarity and presentation of our findings. Below, we outline how we plan to address the major points raised in the full revision.

      (1) Characterisation of the KrIf-1 allele and Kr expression

      We agree that clarifying the nature of the KrIf-1 allele is important. In response to this concern, we will examine Kr expression in KrIf-1 mutant larval, pupal, and adult brains using immunostaining and available reporter lines. These experiments will help determine whether the observed neuroblast retention phenotype correlates with altered Kr expression in MBNBs.

      (2) Regulatory relationships between Kr, Kr-h1, Imp, Syp, Chinmo, and E93

      We are currently performing additional experiments to clarify the interactions among these temporal factors. For instance, we are testing whether Kr-h1 overexpression alters the expression of Imp, Syp, and E93. We have obtained a published E93 antibody from Dr Chris Doe (Syed et al., 2017) and will include E93 expression analysis in our revised manuscript.

      While Chinmo is of interest, its expression is well established to be regulated downstream of Imp/Syp via mRNA stability (Liu et al., 2015; Ren et al., 2017). Given that we currently lack reliable tools to assess Chinmo levels, we will focus primarily on Imp, Syp, and E93 as readouts for Kr/Kr-h1 function. If we succeed in obtaining Chinmo antibodies or reporter lines in time, we will include corresponding data.

      (3) Expression of Kr-h1 in MBNBs

      We fully agree that direct evidence for Kr-h1 expression in MBNBs is important. To address this, we have obtained the Kr-h1::GFP BAC transgenic line (BDSC #96786) and are currently using it to assess Kr-h1 expression in MBNBs. We also tested an anti–Kr-h1 antibody previously reported by Kang et al. (2017), developed in the context of fat body studies, but it did not yield clear signals in larval MBNBs. However, previous work by Shi et al. (2007) clearly demonstrated Kr-h1 expression in the developing MB, including MBNBs, using a custom antibody developed by their lab. We also contacted the Lee lab to request this antibody, but unfortunately, it is no longer available. We will include the results obtained using the GFP BAC line in the revised manuscript and, if needed, pursue RNA in situ hybridisation to further validate Kr-h1 expression in MBNBs.

      (4) Temporal Kr knockdown and MARCM analysis

      We appreciate the suggestion to validate our RNAi-based temporal knockdown results using MARCM. We plan to perform MBNB-specific MARCM analysis following the strategy described by Rossi et al. (2020). However, this approach requires additional time due to the logistics of acquiring the necessary fly stocks, generating appropriate genetic combinations, and conducting clonal analyses. While we will make every effort to include these data, we note that RNAi-based knockdown offers the advantage of temporal reversibility and has been essential for assessing stage-specific requirements in our current study.

      (5) Details of the targeted genetic screen

      Kr was initially identified as part of a broader, ongoing effort to screen for candidate transcription factors and cell cycle regulators involved in neuroblast cell cycle exit and/or quiescence. As this screen is still preliminary and incomplete, we prefer not to include the full dataset at this stage. Instead, we will revise the manuscript to clarify that Kr was prioritised for further investigation based on the striking MBNB-specific phenotype observed upon RNAi-mediated knockdown and in the KrIf-1 mutant, rather than through a completed screening process.

      (6) Clarifying the model (Figure 6D) and interactions

      We will revise the proposed model to distinguish between experimentally supported interactions and speculative ones. As noted above, we will primarily focus on the Imp/Syp and E93 axis in relation to Kr and Kr-h1 activity. Chinmo will be omitted from the model unless further data become available to support its inclusion.

      (7) Clarifications on figures and data presentation

      We appreciate the feedback on figure clarity. We will revise figures such as 1B, 2C, and 3A to improve legibility and presentation. We will also correct typographical errors and figure references, and clarify the activity patterns of the GAL4 drivers. Specifically, while UASmCD8::GFP expression driven by OK107-GAL4 is markedly weaker in MBNBs than in their neuronal progeny (as seen, for example, in Figure S3C), the driver remains active and functionally relevant in MBNBs. We believe the weak expression in MBNBs likely explains the absence of a NB retention phenotype in OK107>KrIR adult brains (see main text, Lines 374–376). As suggested by the reviewer, we will clarify this point earlier in the manuscript and can include additional data showing OK107>GFP expression patterns in pupal MB lineages as supplementary material.

      (8) Analysis of public datasets

      We will include results from our analysis of publicly available datasets such as FlyAtlas2, modENCODE, and a time-course RNA-seq dataset specific to MBNBs (Liu et al., 2015). While the spatial resolution of FlyAtlas2 and modENCODE is limited, the MBNB dataset provides valuable temporal information up to 36 h after puparium formation (APF). From this dataset, we observe that Kr expression remains consistently low throughout development, with only a modest increase at 84 h ALH (mean TPM ~11) and 36 h APF (~7), suggesting it does not undergo strong transcriptional regulation in MBNBs. In contrast, Kr-h1 is highly expressed during early larval stages (24–84 h ALH; mean TPM ~55–60) and shows a marked suppression by 36 h APF (mean TPM ~2), consistent with its proposed role in promoting MBNB proliferation. Importantly, Eip93F (E93) exhibits a reciprocal pattern to Kr-h1—with minimal expression until 84 h ALH (mean TPM ~24), followed by a substantial induction at 36 h APF (mean TPM ~104), aligning with its known role in triggering neuroblast termination. These temporal expression dynamics support our model that Kr-h1 and E93 function in opposition during the transition from proliferative to terminating neuroblast states. We will summarise these findings in the revised manuscript, along with appropriate discussion of dataset limitations.

      We hope this provisional response conveys our strong commitment to thoroughly addressing the reviewers’ concerns and improving the manuscript. We are currently carrying out additional experiments and will submit a revised version with new data and enhanced clarity in due course.

      References:

      Kang et al., 2017. Sci Rep. 7(1):16369. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-16638-1.

      Shi et al., 2007. Dev Neurobiol. 67(11):1614–1626. doi: 10.1002/dneu.20537.

      Rossi et al., 2020. eLife. 9:e58880. doi: 10.7554/eLife.58880.

      Liu et al., 2015. Science. 350(6258):317–320. doi: 10.1126/science.aad1886.

      Ren et al., 2017. Curr Biol. 27(9):1303–1313. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.018. Syed et al., 2017. eLife. 6:e26287. doi: 10.7554/eLife.26287.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Garcia et al. describes how the expression of a respiratory chain alternative oxidase (AOX) from the tunicate Ciona intestinalis, capable of transferring electrons directly from reduced coenzyme Q (CoQ) to oxygen, is able to induce an increase in the mass of Drosophila melanogaster larvae and an accelerated development, especially when the larvae are kept at low temperatures. In order to explain this phenomenon, the paper addresses the modifications in the activity and levels of the 'canonical' electron transfer system (ETS), i.e., complexes I-IV and of the ATP synthase. In addition, the abundance of different metabolites as well as the NAD+/NADH ratios are measured, finding significant differences between the larvae.

      Strengths:

      The observations of differences in growth, body mass and food intake in the wt D. melanogaster larvae vs. those expressing the AOX transgene are solid. The evidence that mild uncoupling of the ETS might accelerate development of the fly larvae is convincing."

      We appreciate the reviewer’s attention to our results and hope we can improve the manuscript to address all criticism appropriately.

      Weaknesses:

      Some of the observations, especially those concerning the origin of the metabolic remodelling in AOX-expressing larvae, are left unexplained, and the argumentation is somewhat speculative. What the authors mean by "reconfiguration" of the mitochondrial electron transfer system is not clear. If this implies that there is an actual change in ETS function and/or structural organisation in the presence of AOX, this conclusion is not supported by the experimental data. In addition, the influence of AOX activity in the mitochondrial ETS system is tested in vitro in the presence of saturating concentrations of substrates. The real degree to which AOX activity is actually influencing ETS activity in vivo remains unknown.

      Indeed, the term “reconfiguration” may seem a little too strong. However, we do have preliminary structural data on larval mitochondria indicating that the term is adequate in this context. We plan to work on obtaining concrete data to sustain our claims that AOX imparts significant functional and structural remodeling of the organelle, which would be consistent with our respirometry and BN-PAGE data. If the data turns out not to be robust enough, we will consider replacing the term with one that better reflects our findings.

      We also realize that the in vivo data we are presenting (body mass, mobility, food intake) are indirect measurements of metabolism and that a more direct approach is necessary to assess the real degree to which AOX influences ETS activity in vivo. To address this issue, we plan to expand our pharmacological treatments of the larval development and to measure whole larval oxygen consumption.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript presents intriguing findings about the role of alternative oxidase (AOX) from the tunicate Ciona intestinalis in accelerating growth and development when expressed in Drosophila melanogaster.

      Strengths:

      The study is overall well-constructed, including appropriate analysis. Likewise, the manuscript is written clearly and supported by high-quality figures. The present study provides valuable insights into AOX's role in Drosophila development. The paper attempts to explore a unique mechanism by which AOX influences Drosophila development, providing insights into mitochondrial respiration and its physiological effects. This is relevant for understanding mitochondrial dysfunction and potential therapeutic applications. The study employs a variety of approaches, including calorimetry, infrared thermography, and genetic analyses, to investigate AOX's impact on metabolism and development.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for recognizing the strengths and acknowledging the novelty of our study.

      Weaknesses:

      There are a number of methodological limitations and substantial gaps in the interpretation of the data presented, which reduces the strength of its conclusions. For instance, there is a misunderstanding of the non-proton motive nature of the AOX - it does not uncouple respiration, merely decouple it as it neither contributes to nor dissipates the proton motive force, in contrast to chemical uncouplers or proton uncouplers such as UCPs. The authors need to reassess their data in light of the above.

      The reviewer is absolutely right about the non-proton motive nature of AOX. We will reassess our data considering that AOX decouples respiration and, if necessary and possible, we will add new experiments to address the methodological limitations raised by the reviewer.

    1. Author response:

      We appreciate the reviewers' positive feedback on our paper. We especially thank them for their evaluation of the genetic analysis, which required a significant amount of timef time. We acknowledge that several aspects of our interpretation and description of the results need correction, as noted by both reviewers. Additionally, we recognize the importance of providing a more comprehensive overview of previous findings, including those conducted in mice, in the manuscript. In the revised version, we will thoroughly address the reviewers' concerns.

      Both reviewers emphasized the need for further validation to ascertain whether the specific requirement of Hox genes in the Hoxba and Hoxbb clusters for pectoral fin bud formation is due to their expression patterns or the functional roles of Hox proteins. This consideration has been on our agenda for some time; however, our submitted paper does not sufficiently address this aspect. In the revised manuscript, we will conduct a comprehensive analysis of the expression patterns of Hox genes in zebrafish to draw informed conclusions on this matter.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      The authors investigate how the viscoelasticity of the fingertip skin can affect the firing of mechanoreceptive afferents and they find a clear effect of recent physical skin state (memory), which is different between afferents. The manuscript is extremely well-written and well-presented. It uses a large dataset of low threshold mechanoreceptive afferents in the fingertip, where it is particularly noteworthy that the SA-2s have been thoroughly analyzed and play an important role here. They point out in the introduction the importance of the non-linear dynamics of the event when an external stimulus contacts the skin, to the point at which this information is picked up by receptors. Although clearly correlated, these are different processes, and it has been very well-explained throughout. I have some comments and ideas that the authors could think about that could further improve their already very interesting paper. Overall, the authors have more than achieved their aims, where their results very much support the conclusions and provoke many further questions. This impact of the previous dynamics of the skin affecting the current state can be explored further in so many ways and may help us to better understand skin aging and the effects of anatomical changes of the skin.

      At the beginning of the Results, it states that FA-2s were not considered as stimuli did not contain mechanical events with frequency components high enough to reliably excite them. Was this really the case, did the authors test any of the FA-2s from the larger dataset? If FA-2s were not at all activated, this is also relevant information for the brain to signal that it is not a relevant Pacinian stimulus (as they respond to everything). Further, afferent receptive fields that were more distant to the stimulus were included, which likely fired very little, like the FA-2s, so why not consider them even if their contribution was low?

      Thank you for bringing this up, we have now clarified in the text that while FA-2s did respond at a low rate during the experiment, their responses were not reliably driven by the force stimuli. In the Methods section we have included the following text:

      “Initially, 10 FA-2 neurons were also included in the analysis. But their responsiveness during the experiment was remarkably low, and unlike the other neuron types, their responses were rarely affected by force stimuli. Specifically, only one of the observed FA-2 neurons responded during the force protraction phases. Due to the lack of clear stimulus-driven responses, FA-2 neurons were subsequently excluded from further analysis.”

      One question that I wondered throughout was whether you have looked at further past history in stimulation, i.e. not just the preceding stimulus, but 2 or 3 stimuli back? It would be interesting to know if there is any ongoing change that can be related back further. I do not think you would see anything as such here, but it would be interesting to test and/or explore in future work (e.g. especially with sticky, forceful, or sharp indentation touch). However, even here, it could be that certain directions gave more effects.

      This is a very interesting question! A discernible effect from the previous stimulus could persist at the end of the current stimulation (see Figure 4C), potentially influencing the next one—a 2-stimuli-back effect. Unfortunately, our experimental design did not allow for rigorous testing of this effect. While all possible pairs of stimulus directions were included in immediately consecutive trials, this was not the case for pairs separated by additional trials. Hence, the combination of a likely weak effect and limited variation in history precluded a thorough analysis of a 2-stimuli-back effect. Future work should delve into the time course of the viscoelastic effect in greater detail.

      Did the authors analyze or take into account the difference between receptive field locations? For example, did afferents more on the sides have lower responses and a lesser effect of history?

      An investigation into the potential impact of the relationship between the receptive field location on the fingertip skin and the primary contact site of the stimulus surface revealed no discernible influence for SA-1 and SA-2 neurons. In contrast, FA-1 neurons, particularly those predominantly sensitive to the previous stimulation or displaying mixed sensitivity, exhibited a tendency to terminate near the primary stimulation site. We have added these observations to the text:

      “We found no straightforward relationship between a neuron's sensitivity to current and previous stimulation and its termination site in fingertip skin. Specifically, there was no statistically significant effect of the distance between a neuron's receptive field center and the primary contact site of the stimulus surface on whether neurons signaled current, prior, or mixed information for SA-1 (Kruskal-Wallis test H(2)=3.86, p= 0.15) or SA-2 neurons (H(2)=0.75, p=0.69). However, a significant difference emerged for FA-1 neurons (H(2)=8.66, p=0.01), indicating that neurons terminating closer to the stimulation site on the flat part of the fingertip were more likely to signal past or mixed information.”

      Was there anything different in the firing patterns between the spontaneous and non-spontaneously active SA-2s? For example, did the non-spontaneous show more dynamic responses?

      The firing patterns of both spontaneously and non-spontaneously active SA-2 neurons shared similarities in terms of adaptation and range of firing rate modulation in response to force stimuli, i.e., ‘dynamic response’. The distinction lay in the pattern of modulation of the firing rate associated with stimulus presentations. For spontaneously active SA-2 neurons, this modulation occurred around a significant background discharge, implying that a force stimulus could either decrease or increase the firing rate, depending on how it deformed the fingertip. This characteristic is well illustrated by the firing pattern of the neuron depicted in the lower panels of Figure 3D. Conversely, in non-spontaneously active SA-2 neurons, a force stimulus could only induce an increase in the firing rate or no change. Although the neuron depicted in the upper panels of Figure 3D exhibited some background activity, it serves to exemplify this characteristic. In the text, we have elucidated the dynamics of the SA-2 neuron response by highlighting that force stimulation can either decrease or increase the firing rate in neurons with spontaneous activity through the following addition/change:

      “This increased variability was most evident during the force protraction phase where most neurons exhibited the most intense responses. Increased variability was also observed in instances where the dynamic response to force stimulation involved a decrease in the firing rate (lower panels of Figure 3D). This phenomenon was observed in SA-2 neurons that maintained an ongoing discharge during intertrial periods (cf. Fig. 2A). In these cases, the response to a force stimulus constituted a modulation of the firing rate around the background discharge, signifying that a force stimulus could either decrease or increase the firing rate depending on the prevailing stimulus direction.”

      Were the spontaneously active SA-2 afferents firing all the time or did they have periods of rest - and did this relate to recent stimulation? Were the spontaneously active SA-2s located in a certain part of the finger (e.g. nail) or were they randomly spread throughout the fingertip? Any distribution differences could indicate a more complicated role in skin sensing.

      SA-2 neurons, in general, are well-known for undergoing significant post-stimulation depression (e.g., Knibestöl and Vallbo, 1970; Chambers et al., 1972; Burgess and Perl, 1973). In our force stimulations, this post-excitatory depression manifested as a reduced or absent response during the latter part of the stimulus retraction period for stimuli in directions that markedly excited the neuron. The excitability recovered when the fingertip relaxed during the subsequent intertrial period, and for "spontaneously active" neurons, the firing resumed (see examples in Figure 7A). Furthermore, some “spontaneously active” neurons could be silenced or exhibit a near-silent period during force stimulation for certain force directions, while the spontaneous firing returned during the upcoming intertrial period when the fingertip shape recovered (for example, see responses to stimulation in the proximal and especially ulnar directions in the top panel in Figure 7A).

      Regarding the location of the receptive field centres of spontaneously active and non-spontaneously active SA-2 neurons on the fingertip we did not observe any obvious spatial segregation. To illustrate this, we have revised Figure 1A by color-marking SA-2 neurons that exhibited ongoing activity in intertrial periods, and the figure caption has been modified accordingly:

      “Figure 1. Experimental setup. A. Receptive field center locations shown on a standardized fingertip for all first-order tactile neurons included in the study, categorized by neuron type. Purple symbols denote spontaneously active SA-2 neurons exhibiting ongoing activity without external stimulation.”

      Did the authors look to see if the spontaneous firing in SA-2s between trials could predict the extent to which the type 1 afferents encode the proceeding stimulus? Basically, does the SA-2 state relate to how the type 1 units fire?

      We found no clear indications that the responses of FA-1 and SA-1 could be readily anticipated based on the firing patterns of SA-2 neurons.

      In the discussion, it is stated that "the viscoelastic memory of the preceding loading would have modulated the pattern of strain changes in the fingertip differently depending on where their receptor organs are situated in the fingertip". Can the authors expand on this or make any predictions about the size of the memory effect and the distance from the point of stimulation?

      We have explored this topic further in the text, referring to recent studies modeling essential aspects of fingertip mechanics. However, in our view, current models lack the capability to predict the specific nature sought by the reviewer. These models should include a detailed understanding of the intricate networks of collagen fibers anchoring the pulp tissue at the distal phalangeal bone and the nail. They should also consider potential inherent directional preferences of the receptor organs, attributed to their microanatomy. The text modifications are as follows:

      “In addition to the receptor organ locations, the variation in sensitivity among neurons to fingertip deformations in response to both previous and current loadings would stem from the fingertip’s geometry and its complex composite material properties. Possible inherent directional preferences of the receptor organs, attributed to their microanatomy, could also be significant. However, mechanical anisotropy, particularly within the viscoelastic subcutaneous tissue of the fingertip induced by intricately oriented collagen fiber strands forming fat columns in the pulp (Hauck et al., 2004), are likely to play a crucial role. This anisotropy would shape the dynamic pattern of strain changes at neurons' receptor sites, intricately influencing a neuron's sensitivity not only to current but also to preceding loadings. Indeed, recent modeling efforts suggest that such mechanical anisotropy strongly influences the spatiotemporal distribution of stresses and strains across the fingertip (Duprez et al., 2024).”

      Relatedly, we have included additional text to provide a more comprehensive explanation of the “bulk deformation” of the fingertip that occurs during the loadings:

      “As pressure increases in the pulp, the pulp tissue bulges at the end and sides of the fingertip. Simultaneously, the tangential force component amplifies the bulging in the direction of the force while stretching the skin on the opposite side.”

      In the discussion, it would be good if the authors could briefly comment more on the diversity of the mechanoreceptive afferent firing and why this may be useful to the system.

      The diversity in responses among neurons is instrumental in enhancing the information transmitted to the brain by averting redundancy in information acquisition. This diversity thereby contributes to an overall increase in information. We've included a brief statement, along with several references, underscoring this concept:

      "The resulting diversity in the sensitivities of neurons might enhance the overall information collected and relayed to the brain by the neuronal population, facilitating the discrimination between tactile stimuli or mechanical states of the fingertip (see Rongala et al., 2024; Corniani et al., 2022; Tummala et al., 2023, for more extensive explorations of this idea)."

      Also, the authors could briefly discuss why this memory (or recency) effect occurs - is it useful, does it serve a purpose, or it is just a by-product of our skin structure? There are examples of memory in the other senses where comparisons could be drawn. Is it like stimulus adaptation effects in the other senses (e.g. aftereffects of visual motion)?

      We have expanded the concluding paragraph of the discussion, specifically delving into the question of whether the mechanical memory effect serves a deliberate purpose or is simply an incidental byproduct of our skin structure:

      “In any case, the viscoelastic deformability of the fingertips plays a pivotal role in supporting the diverse functions of the fingers. For example, it allows for cushioned contact with objects featuring hard surfaces and allows the skin to conform to object shapes, enabling the extraction of tactile information about objects' 3D shapes and fine surface properties. Moreover, deformability is essential for the effective grasping and manipulation of objects. This is achieved, among other benefits, by expanding the contact surface, thereby reducing local pressure on the skin under stronger forces and enabling tactile signaling of friction conditions within the contact surface for control of grasp stability. Throughout, continuous acquisition of information about various aspects of the current state of the fingertip and its skin by tactile neurons is essential for the functional interaction between the brain and the fingers. In light of this, the viscoelastic memory effect on tactile signaling of fingertip forces can be perceived as a by-product of an overall optimization process within prevailing biological constraints.”

      One point that would be nice to add to the discussion is the implications of the work for skin sensing. What would you predict for the time constant of relaxation of fingertip skin, how long could these skin memory effects last? Two main points to address here may be how the hydration of the skin and anatomical skin changes related to aging affect the results. If the skin is less viscoelastic, what would be the implications for the firing of mechanoreceptors?

      It is likely that the time constant depends to some extent on mechanical factors of the skin, which will likely change due to age or environmental factors. However, while these questions are intriguing, they fall outside the scope of the current study and we are not aware of studies that have addressed these issues directly in experiments either.

      How long does it take for the effect to end? Again, this will likely depend on the skin's viscoelasticity. However, could the authors use it in a psychophysical paradigm to predict whether participants would be more or less sensitive to future stimuli? In this way, it would be possible to test whether the direction modifies touch perception.

      Time constants for tissue viscoelasticity have been estimated to extend up to several seconds (see citations in the introduction). While direct perceptual effects could indeed be explored through psychophysical experimental paradigms, we are currently unaware of any studies specifically addressing the type of effect described in this study. In addition to the statement that, concerning manipulation and haptic tasks, "to our knowledge, a possible influence of fingertip viscoelasticity on task performance has not been systematically investigated," we have now also addressed tactile psychophysical tasks conducted during passive touch with the following sentence in the text:

      “Similarly, there is a lack of systematic investigation of potential effects of fingertip viscoelasticity on performance in tactile psychophysical tasks conducted during passive touch.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors sought to identify the impact skin viscoelasticity has on neural signalling of contact forces that are representative of those experienced during normal tactile behaviour. The evidence presented in the analyses indicates there is a clear effect of viscoelasticity on the imposed skin movements from a force-controlled stimulus. Both skin mechanics and evoked afferent firing were affected based on prior stimulation, which has not previously been thoroughly explored. This study outlines that viscoelastic effects have an important impact on encoding in the tactile system, which should be considered in the design and interpretation of future studies. Viscoelasticity was shown to affect the mechanical skin deflections and stresses/strains imposed by previous and current interaction force, and also the resultant neuronal signalling. The result of this was an impaired coding of contact forces based on previous stimulation. The authors may be able to strengthen their findings, by using the existing data to further explore the link between skin mechanics and neural signalling, giving a clearer picture than demonstrating shared variability. This is not a critical addition, but I believe would strengthen the work and make it more generally applicable.

      Strengths:

      - Elegant design of the study. Direct measurements have been made from the tactile sensory neurons to give detailed information on touch encoding. Experiments have been well designed and the forces/displacements have been thoroughly controlled and measured to give accurate measurements of global skin mechanics during a set of controlled mechanical stimuli.

      - Analytical techniques used. Analysis of fundamental information coding and information representation in the sensory afferents reveals dynamic coding properties to develop putative models of the neural representation of force. This advanced analysis method has been applied to a large dataset to study neural encoding of force, the temporal dynamics of this, and the variability in this.

      Weaknesses:

      - Lack of exploration of the variation in neural responses. Although there is a viscoelastic effect that produces variability in the stimulus effects based on prior stimulation, it is a shame that the variability in neural firing and force-induced skin displacements have been presented, and are similarly variable, but there has been no investigation of a link between the two. I believe with these data the authors can go beyond demonstrating shared variability. The force per se is clearly not faithfully represented in the neural signal, being masked by stimulation history, and it is of interest if the underlying resultant contact mechanics are.

      Thank you for this suggestion. We have added a new section investigating the link between skin deformation and neural firing in more depth via a simple neural model. Please see our answer below in the ‘Recommendations’ section for further details.

      Validity of conclusions:

      The authors have succeeded in demonstrating skin viscoelasticity has an impact on skin contact mechanics with a given force and that this impacts the resultant neural coding of force. Their study has been well-designed and the results support their conclusions. The importance and scope of the work is adequately outlined for readers to interpret the results and significance.

      Impact:

      This study will have important implications for future studies performing tactile stimulation and evaluating tactile feedback during motor control tasks. In detailed studies of tactile function, it illustrates the necessity to measure skin contact dynamics to properly understand the effects of a force stimulus on the skin and mechanoreceptors.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (Very) minor comments

      - The authors say at the beginning of the Results that, "The fourth type of tactile neurons in the human glabrous skin, fast adapting type II neurons...". Although generally written that there are four types of afferent in the glabrous skin, it would be better to state that these are low-threshold A-beta myelinated mechanoreceptive afferents, at least one time, as there are other types of afferent in the glabrous skin that respond to mechanical stimulation (e.g. low and high threshold C-fibers).

      This is now clarified at the start of the Results section:

      “We recorded action potentials in the median nerve of individual low-threshold A-beta myelinated first-order human tactile neurons innervating the glabrous skin of the fingertip…”

      - Fig. 3: Could you add '(N)' as the measurement of force for Fig. 3A for Fz, Fy, and Fz? Also, please change 'Data was recorded' to 'Data were recorded' in the legend.

      Fixed.

      - At the beginning of the Methods, you say that your study conforms to the Declaration of Helsinki, which actually requires pre-registration in a database. If you did not pre-register your study, please can you add '... in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, apart from pre-registration in a database'.

      Thanks for making us aware of this. We have added the suggested qualifier to the ethics statement.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The neural representation/encoding of the actual displacement vectors would be a useful addition to the analyses. These vectors have been demonstrated to systematically change with the condition in the irregular series (Figure 2E) and will thus significantly act on the dynamics of induced mechanical changes in the skin with a given interaction force. Thus, it could be examined how the neurons code the magnitude of displacements as well as their direction. An evaluation of the extent to which the imposed displacement magnitudes are encoded in the neural responses would be a useful addition in explaining the signalling of the force events and how the central nervous system decodes these. Evaluating an alternative displacement encoding for comparison to pure force encoding may reveal more about how contact events are represented in the tactile system, which must decode these variable afferent signals to reconstruct a percept of the interaction. It could then be explored how the central nervous system may then scale the dynamic afferent responses based on the background viscoelastic state likely to be present in the SA-II afferent signals (Figure 7) for a context in which to evaluate the dynamic contact forces. This may of course be a complex relationship for the type-I afferents, where the underlying mechanical events evoking the firing (microslips not represented in global forces) have not been measured here. Such a model could be more widely applicable, as the skin viscoelasticity and displacement magnitudes are a straightforward measurement metric and could perhaps be used as a better proxy for neural signalling. This would allow the investigation of a wider variety of forces, and the study of the timing of the viscoelastic effect, both of which have been fixed here. This would give the work a broader impact, rather than just highlighting that this effect produces variability, it could reveal if this mechanical feature is structured in the neural representation. The categorical encoding/decoding tested here is specific to the stimuli used (magnitudes, intervals), but there is the possibility that this may be more generally applicable (within the bounds of forces/speeds) if the underlying basis of the variability in the signalling produced by the viscoelasticity is identified. Since the time course of the viscoelasticity has not been measured here (fixed forces and intervals), further study is required to fully understand the implications this has for a wider variety of situations.

      We agree that a better understanding of how the mechanical deformations are reflected in the resulting spike trains would be valuable. While ultimately a full understanding will need precise measurements of skin deformation across the whole fingertip to account for mechanical propagation to mechanoreceptor locations, relating the deformations at the contact location with neural firing patterns directly can provide useful hints into which aspects of deformation are encoded and how. To this end, we ran a new analysis that aimed to predict the time-varying neural responses directly from the recorded mechanical movements of the contactor.

      Below we have reproduced the new results and methods text along with the additional figures for this analysis. Note that we have also added text in the Discussion to interpret these findings in the context of our other results.

      New section in Results titled Predicting neural responses from contactor movements: “The similarity in the history-dependent variation in neural firing and fingertip deformation at a given force stimulus suggests that neuronal firing is determined by how the fingertip deforms rather than the applied force itself. However, this similarity does not clarify the relationship between fingertip deformation dynamics and neural signaling. To investigate further, we fit cross-validated multiple linear regression models to evaluate how well distinct aspects of contactor movement could predict the time-varying firing rates of individual neurons during the protraction phases of the irregular sequence. The models used predictors based on (1) the three-dimensional position of the contactor, (2) its three-dimensional velocity, (3) a combination of position and velocity signals, and, finally, (4) position and velocity signals along with all possible two-way interactions between them, capturing potentially complex relationship between fingertip deformations and neural signaling.

      Comparing the variance explained (R<sup>2</sup>) by each regression model for each neuron type revealed clear differences between the models (Figure 5A). A two-way mixed design ANOVA, with regression model as within-group effects and neuron type as a between-group effect revealed a main effect of model on variance explained (F(3,462) = 815.5, p < 0.001, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = 0.84). Model prediction accuracy overall increased with the number of predictors, with the two-way interaction model outperforming all others (p < 0.001 for all comparisons, Tukey’s HSD). Additionally, a significant main effect of neuron type (F(2,154) = 29.8, p < 0.001, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = 0.28) and a significant interaction between regression model and neuron type were observed (F(6,462) = 50.8, p < 0.001, η<sub>p</sub><sup>2</sup> = 0.40).

      For neuron type, model predictions were most accurate for SA-2 neurons, followed by SA-1 neurons, with FA-1 neurons showing the lowest accuracy (p < 0.003 for all comparisons, Tukey’s HSD). The interaction between model and neuron type revealed distinct patterns. For SA-1 and SA-2 neurons, position-only and velocity-only models had similar prediction accuracy (p ≥ 0.996, Tukey’s HSD) with no significant differences between these neuron types (p ≥ 0.552, Tukey’s HSD). FA-1 neurons performed poorly with the position-only model but showed higher accuracy with the velocity-only model (p < 0.001, Tukey’s HSD) and better than SA-1 neurons (p = 0.006, Tukey’s HSD). Models combining position and velocity predictors (without interactions) surpassed both position-only and velocity-only models for SA-1 and SA-2 neurons (p < 0.001, Tukey’s HSD). Overall, the differences between neuron types broadly match their tuning to static and dynamic stimulus properties.

      The two-way interaction model, accounting for most variance in neural responses, produced mean R<sup>2</sup> values of 0.75 for FA-1, 0.88 for SA-1, and 0.91 for SA-2 neurons (Figure 5A). To evaluate the contribution of the different predictors, we ranked them using the permutation feature importance method, focusing on the six most important ones. Regression analyses using only these variables explained almost all of the variance explained by the full model, with a median R<sup>2</sup> reduction of just 0.055 across all neurons. Across all neuron types, at least half included all three velocity components (dPx, dPy, dPz) among the top six, with FA-1 neurons showing the highest prevalence (Figure 5B). Interactions between normal position (Pz) and each velocity component were also frequently observed, while interactions involving tangential position and velocity components were less common. Interactions among velocity components were relatively well represented, followed by interactions limited to position components. Position signals were generally less represented, except for normal position (Pz) in slowly adapting neurons, where it appeared in 50% of SA-1 and 68% of SA-2 neurons. Despite these broad trends, important predictors varied widely across ranks even within a given neuron class (see Figure 5-figure supplement 1), and even the most frequent variables appeared in only a subset of cases, suggesting broad variability in sensitivity across neurons.”

      New methods paragraph titled Predicting time-varying firing rates from skin deformations:

      “This analysis was conducted in Python (v3.13) with pandas for data handling, numpy for numerical operations, and scikit-learn for model fitting and evaluation.

      To assess how well individual neurons' time-varying firing rates could be predicted from simultaneous contactor movements, we fitted multiple linear regression models (see Khamis et al., 2015, for a similar approach}. This analysis focused on the force protraction phase of the irregular sequence, where neurons were most responsive and sensitive to stimulation history. Data from 100 ms before to 100 ms after the protraction phase (between -0.100 s and 0.225 s relative to protraction onset) were included for each trial. Neurons were included if they fired at least two action potentials during the force protraction phase and the following 100 ms in at least five of the 25 trials. This ensured sufficient variability in firing rates for meaningful regression analysis, resulting in 68 SA-1, 38 SA-2, and 51 FA-1 neurons being included.

      Contractor position signals digitized at 400 Hz were linearly interpolated to 1000 Hz. Instantaneous firing rates, derived from action potentials sampled at 12.8 kHz, were resampled at 1000 Hz to align with position signals. A Gaussian filter (σ = 10 ms, cutoff ~16 Hz) was applied to the firing rate as well as to the position signals before differentiation. To account for axonal conduction (8–15 ms) and sensory transduction delays (1–5 ms), firing rates were advanced by 15 ms to align approximately with independent variables.

      Regressions were performed using scikit-learn's Ridge and RidgeCV regressors, which apply L2 regularization to mitigate overfitting. Hyperparameter tuning for the regularization parameter (alpha) was performed using GridSearchCV with a predefined range (0.001–1000.0), incorporating five-fold cross-validation to select the best value. To minimize overfitting risks, model performance was further validated with independent five-fold cross-validation (KFold), and R<sup>2</sup> scores were computed using cross_val_score.

      We constructed four linear regression models with increasing complexity: (1) Position-only, using three-dimensional contactor positions (Px, Py, Pz); (2) Velocity-only, using three-dimensional velocities (dPx, dPy, dPz); (3) Combined, including all position and velocity signals (6 predictors); and (4) Interaction, including all signals and their two-way interactions (21 predictors). All features were standardized using StandardScaler to improve regularization and model convergence. PolynomialFeatures generated second-order interaction terms for the interaction model. Feature importance was evaluated with permutation_importance, and simpler models were built using the most important features. These models were validated through cross-validation to assess retained explanatory power.”

      Minor:

      - It would be useful to add a brief description of the material aspects of the contactor tip to the methods (as per Birznieks 2001).

      We have added the following statement:

      “To ensure that friction between the contactor and the skin was sufficiently high to prevent slips, the surface was coated with silicon carbide grains (50–100 μm), approximating the finish of smooth sandpaper.”

      - The axes labelling on Figure 3A and legend description is ambiguous, probably placing the Px, Py, and Pz labels on the far left axes and the Fx, Fy, and Fz on the right side of the far right axes would make this clearer.

      Label placement has been improved along with some other minor fixes.

      - For the quasi-static phase analysis, the phrase "absence of loading" used in reference to the interstimulus period and SA-II afferents does not seem to be a correct description. The finger is still loaded (at least in the normal direction), with a magnitude of imposed displacement that counteracts the viscoelastic force exerted by the skin mechanics of the fingertip. Although there is a zero net-force load, a mechanical stimulus is still being actively applied to the skin.

      We have changed the wording throughout the text and now consistently refer either to the “interstimulus period” directly or to an “absence of externally applied stimulation” to avoid confusion.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The revised paper by Kim et al. reports two disease mutations in proBMP4, S91C and E93G, disrupt the FAM20C phosphorylation site at Ser91, blocking the activation of proBMP4 homodimers, while still allowing BMP4/7 heterodimers to function. Analysis of DMZ explants from Xenopus embryos expressing the proBMP4 S91C or E93G mutants showed reduced expression of pSmad1 and tbxt1. The expert amphibian tissue transplant studies were expanded to in vivo studies in Bmp4S91C/+ and Bmp4E93G/+ mice, highlighting the impact of these mutations on embryonic development, particularly in female mice, consistent with patient studies. Additionally, studies in mouse embryonic fibroblasts (MEFs) demonstrated that the mutations did not affect proBMP4 glycosylation or ER-to-Golgi transport but appeared to inhibit the furin-dependent cleavage of proBMP4 to BMP4. Based on these findings and AI modeling using AlphaFold of proBMP4, the authors speculate that pSer91 influences access of furin to its cleavage site at Arg289AlaLysArg292 in a new "Ideas and Speculation" section. Overall, the authors addressed the reviewers' comments, improving the presentation.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of this work continue to lie in the elegant Xenopus and mouse studies that elucidate the impact of the S91C and E93G disease mutations on BMP signaling and embryonic development. Including an "Ideas and Speculation" subsection for mechanistic ideas reduces some shortcomings regarding the analysis of the underlying mechanisms.

      Weaknesses:

      (1)  (Minor) In Figure S1 and lines 165-174 and 179-180, the authors should consider that, unlike the wild-type protein (Ser), which can be reversibly phosphorylated or dephosphorylated, phosphomimic mutations are locked into mimicking either the phosphorylated state (Asp) or the non-phosphorylated state (Ala). Consequently, if the S91D mutant exhibits lower activity than WT, it could imply that S91D interferes with other regulatory constraints, as the authors suggest. However, it may also be inhibiting activation. Therefore, caution is warranted when comparing S91D with S91C to conclude that Ser91 phosphorylation increases BMP4 activity. While additional experiments are not necessary, further consideration is essential.

      (Minor) In lines 394-399, the authors cleverly speculate that pS91 interacts with Arg289-the essential P4 arginine for furin processing. If so, this interaction could hinder the cleavage of proBMP4, as indicated by the results in Figure S1. The discussion would benefit from considering that, contrary to their favored model, dephosphorylation at Ser91 might actually facilitate cleavage.

      We have added a paragraph raising this possibility but explaining why it is unlikely and inconsistent with our in vivo data. The S91D construct was a simple control that was tested in ectopic expression assays and not in vivo.  We can make no conclusions about whether this construct resembles the phosphorylated state or whether it hinders or facilitates cleavage in vivo. The conclusion that dephosphorylation promotes BMP4 cleavage or activity is not compatible with the finding that two mutations associated with birth defects in humans (p.S91C or p.E93G) that are predicted to prevent FAM20C-mediated phosphorylation of the BMP4 prodomain lead to impaired proteolytic maturation of endogenous BMP4 and reduced BMP activity in vivo. 

      (2)  In Figure 4, panels A, E, and I, the proBMP bands in the mouse embryonic lysates and MEFs expressing the mutations show a clear size shift. Are these shifts a cause or a consequence of the lack of cleavage? Regardless, the size shifts should be explicitly noted.

      These intriguing shifts were observed in some but not all biological replicates.  When present, the shifts were not reversed by treatment with phosphatases or deglycosylases, and the shifts were never observed in epitope tagged wild type controls.  We have added a paragraph noting the shifts and our tests of whether they might be due to glycosylation, phosphorylation or epitope tags. 

      (3)  (Minor) In line 314, the authors should consider modifying the wording to: "is required for modulating proprotein convertase..."

      The original wording (“Collectively, our findings are consistent with a model in which FAM20C-mediated phosphorylation of the BMP4 prodomain is not required for folding or exit of the precursor protein from the ER, but is required for proprotein convertase recognition and/or for trafficking to post-TGN compartment(s) where BMP4 is cleaved”) more accurately reflects the model that is supported by our findings. Stating that “phosphorylation ……is required to modulate proprotein convertase recognition and/or trafficking” is vague and leaves open the possibility that it modulates in either direction, which our data do not support as described in point 1 above.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review): 

      This study investigates the role of microtubules in regulating insulin secretion from pancreatic islet beta cells. This is of great importance considering that controlled secretion of insulin is essential to prevent diabetes. Previously, it has been shown that KIF5B plays an essential role in insulin secretion by transporting insulin granules to the plasma membrane. High glucose activates KIF5B to increase insulin secretion resulting in the cellular uptake of glucose. In order to prevent hypoglycemia, insulin secretion needs to be tightly controlled. Notably, it is known that KIF5B plays a role in microtubule sliding. This is important, as the authors described previously that beta cells establish a peripheral sub-membrane microtubule array, which is critical for the withdrawal of excessive insulin granules from the secretion sites. At high glucose, the sub-membrane microtubule array is destabilized to allow for robust insulin secretion. Here the authors aim to answer the question of how the peripheral array is formed. Based on the previously published data the authors hypothesize that KIF5B organizes the sub-membrane microtubule array via microtubule sliding. 

      General comment: 

      This manuscript provides data that indicate that KIF5B, like in many other cells, mediates microtubule sliding in beta cells. This study is limited to in vitro assays and one cell line. Furthermore, the authors provide no link to insulin secretion and glucose uptake and the overall effects described are moderate. Finally, the overall effect of microtubule sliding upon glucose stimulation is surprisingly low considering the tight regulation of insulin secretion. Moreover, the authors state "the amount of MT polymer on every glucose stimulation changes only slightly, often undetectable…. In fact, we observe a prominent effect of peripheral MT loss only after a long-term kinesin depletion (three-four days)". This challenges the view that a KIF5Bdependent mechanism regulating microtubule sliding plays a major role in controlling insulin secretion. 

      (1) Our initial study was indeed done in a cell line, which is a normal approach to addressing molecular mechanisms of a phenomenon in a challenging cell model: primary pancreatic beta cells are prone to rapidly dedifferentiate outside of the organism and are hard to genetically modify. To address this reviewer’s comment, in the revised manuscript we now confirm the phenotype in beta cells within intact pancreatic islets from a KIF5B KO mouse model (New Figure 2 – Supplemental Figure 1).

      (2) We agree that testing the effect of microtubule sliding on insulin secretion is an important question. Unfortunately, the experimental design needed to accomplish this task is not straighDorward. Importantly, besides microtubule sliding, KIF5B is heavily engaged in insulin granule transport, and GSIS deficiency upon KIF5B inactivation is well documented (e.g. Varadi et al 2002). In this study, we choose not to repeat this GSIS assay because of ample existing data. However, this reported GSIS deficiency could result from a combination of lack of insulin granule delivery to the periphery (previous data) and from the depletion of insulin granules from the periphery due to the loss of the submembrane MT bundle (this study and Bracey et al 2020).  In order to exclusively test the role of MT sliding in secretion, a significant investment in mutant tool development would be needed. Ideally, a new mutant mouse model where insulin granule transport is allowed by MT sliding in blocked must be developed to specifically address this question. To conclude, answering this question will be the subject for another, follow-up study. 

      (3) We respecDully disagree with the reviewer’s opinion that the effect of MT sliding in beta cells is moderate. As MT networks go, even a slight change in MT configuration often has dramatic consequences. For example, in mitotic spindles, a tiny overgrowth of microtubule ends during metaphase, which causes them to attach to both kinetochores rather than just one, is very significant for the efficiency of chromosome segregation, causing aneuploidy and cancer. The changes in beta-cell MT networks that we are reporting are much stronger: the effect on the peripheral MT network accumulated over three days of KIF5B depletion is dramatic (Fig 2 B, C). Short-term gross MT network configurations after a single glucose stimulation are harder to detect, but MTs at the cell periphery are, in fact, destabilized and fragmented, as we and others have previously reported (Ho et al 2020, Mueller et al 2021). Preventing this MT rearrangement completely blocks GSIS (Zhu et al 2015, Ho et al 2020). 

      One of the most fascinating features of insulin secretion regulation is that the amount of generated insulin granules significantly exceeds the normal physiological needs for insulin secretion (~100 times more than needed). At the same time, even slightly facilitated glucose depletion can be devastating. Accordingly, the excessive insulin content of a beta cell resulted in the development of multiple levels of control, preventing excessive secretion. Our previous data suggest that the peripheral MT array provides one of those mechanisms. This study indicates that microtubule sliding is necessary to form the proper peripheral network in the long term. Short-term glucose-induced changes in the peripheral MT array likely need to be subtle to prevent over-secretion. Thus, we are not surprised that a dramatic effect of sliding inhibition is only detectable by our approaches after the changes in the MT network accumulate over time. In the revised paper, we now discuss the potential impact of peripheral MT sliding on positive and negative regulation of secretion and add a schematic model illustrating these processes.

      Specific comments: 

      (1) Notably, the authors have previously reported that high glucose-induced remodeling of microtubule networks facilitates robust glucose-stimulated insulin secretion. This remodeling involves the disassembly of old microtubules and the nucleation of new microtubules. Using real-time imaging of photoconverted microtubules, they report that high levels of glucose induce rapid microtubule disassembly preferentially in the periphery of individual β-cells, and this process is mediated by the phosphorylation of microtubule-associated protein tau. Here, they state that the sub-membrane microtubule array is destabilized via microtubule sliding. What is the relevance of the different processes? 

      In this comment, the summary of our previous conclusions is correct, but the conclusion of this current study is re-stated incorrectly. Indeed, we have previously shown that in high glucose, MTs are destabilized at the cell periphery and nucleated in the cell interior. However, this current paper does not state that “the sub-membrane microtubule array is destabilized via microtubule sliding”. To answer this reviewer’s question, our data support a model where, during glucose stimulation, MT sliding within the peripheral bundle might move fragments of MTs severed by other mechanisms. Importantly, we propose that MT sliding restores the partially destabilized peripheral bundle by delivery of MTs that are nucleated at the cell interior and incorporating them into that bundle. In our overall model, three processes (destabilization, nucleation, and sliding to restore the bundle) are coordinated to maintain beta cell fitness on each GSIS cycle.

      (2) On one hand the authors describe how KIF5B depletion prevents sliding and the transport of microtubules to the plasma membrane to form the sub-membrane microtubule array. This indicates KIF5B is required to form this structure. On the other hand, they describe that at high glucose concentration, KIF5B promotes microtubule sliding to destabilize the sub-membrane microtubule array to allow robust insulin secretion. This appears contradictory. 

      We never intended to make an impression that MT sliding destabilized the sub-membrane bundle. Apologies if there was a reason in our wording that caused this misunderstanding of our model. We propose that while the bundle is destabilized downstream of glucose signaling (e.g. due to tau phosphorylation, please see Ho et al Diabetes 2020), MT sliding remodels the bundle and thereafter rebuilds it to prevent over-secretion. In the revised manuscript, we have doublechecked the whole text to make sure that such misunderstanding is avoided. 

      (3) Previously, it has been shown that KIF5B induces tubulin incorporation along the microtubule shaft in a concentration-dependent manner. Moreover, running KIF5B increases microtubule rescue frequency and unlimited growth of microtubules. Notably, KIF5B regulates microtubule network mass and organization in cells (PMID: 34883065). Consequently, it appears possible that the here observed phenomena of changes in the microtubule network might be due to alterations in these processes. 

      We thank the reviewer for proposing this alternative explanation to the observed change in microtubule networks after KIF5B depletion. We have now directly tested this possibility. Namely, we have re-expressed the kinesin-1 motor domain in MIN6 cells depleted of KIF5B. This motor domain construct by itself is not capable of driving microtubule sliding because it lacks the tail domain. At the same time, it is known to move very efficiently at microtubules and should provide the effects as reported in the article cited by the reviewer. We found that the reexpression of the kinesin motor domain does not rescue microtubule network defects in beta cells (see new Figure 2 – Supplemental Figure 2). Thus, we conclude that the effects of kinesin depletion on the microtubule network in beta cells are due to the lack of microtubule sliding, as reported here.

      (4) The authors provide data that indicate that microtubule sliding is enhanced upon glucose stimulation. They conclude that these data indicate that microtubule sliding is an integral part of glucose-triggered microtubule remodeling. Yet, the authors fail to provide any evidence that this process plays a role in insulin secretion or glucose uptake. 

      We would like to point out that we do not “fail” but rather choose not to overload our study by repeating insulin secretion assays in KIF5B-inactivated cells because this would not have been very informative. It has been found previously that kinesin-1 inactivation or knockout significantly attenuates insulin secretion because kinesin-1 is actively transporting insulin granules and kinesin-1 activity is enhanced under high glucose conditions (e.g. Varadi et al 2002, Cui et al., 2011, Donelan et al, 2002). That said, our current finding is very much in line with these previous data. When kinesin is depleted, two things would be happening at the same time: in the absence of sub-membrane microtubule bundle pre-existing insulin granules would be over-secreted, and new insulin would not be delivered to the periphery, both decreasing GSIS. Unfortunately, we do not have tools yet that would allow us to dissect which part of the insulin secretion defect is due to prior over-secretion (the consequence of deficient MT sliding) and which part is due to the lack of new granule delivery. We plan to develop such tools in the future and elaborate on them in a follow-up study. Here, our goal is to understand microtubule organization principles in beta cells, and we choose not to extend the scope of the current study to metabolic assays.  

      (5) The authors speculate that the sub-membrane microtubule array prevents the over-secretion of insulin. Would one not expect in this case a change in the distribution of insulin granules at the plasma membrane when this array is affected? Or after glucose stimulation? Notably, it has been reported that "the defects of β-cell function in KIF5B mutant mice were not coupled with observable changes in islet morphology, islet cell composition, or β-cell size" and "the subcellular localization of insulin vesicles was found to not be affected significantly by the decreased Kif5b level. The cytoplasm of both wild-type and mutant β-cells was filled with insulin vesicles. Insulin vesicle numbers per square μm were determined by counting all insulin vesicles in randomly photographed β-cells. More insulin granules were found in Kif5b knockout β-cells compared with control cells. This phenomenon is consistent with the observation that insulin secretion by β-cells is affected" whereby "Insulin vesicles (arrowheads) were distributed evenly in both mutant and control cells" (PMID: 20870970).  

      Quantitative analyses in the study cited by the reviewer do not include assays that would be relevant to our study. Particularly, in that study neither the amount of insulin granules at the cell periphery nor the ratio between the number of granules at the periphery and the beta cell interior has been analyzed. In addition, in our preliminary observations not shown here, insulin content in beta cells in KIF5B KO mice is highly heterogeneous, with a subpopulation of cells severely depleted of insulin. This opens a new avenue of investigation into beta cell heterogeneity, which is out of the scope of this current study. Thus, we chose to restrict this current study to microtubule organization data.   

      (6) Does the sub-membrane microtubule array exist in primary beta cells (in vitro and/or in vivo) and how it is affected in KIF5B knockout mice?  

      Yes, it does exist. In fact, we have first reported it in mouse islets (Bracey et al 2020, Ho et al 2020). Now, we report that the sub-membrane bundle is defective, and microtubules are misaligned in KIF5B KO mice (new Figure 2 – Supplemental Figure 1).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review): 

      In this article, Bracey et al. provide insights into the factors contributing to the distinct arrangement observed in sub-membrane microtubules (MTs) within mouse β-cells of the pancreas. Specifically, they propose that in clonal mouse pancreatic β-cells (MIN6), the motor protein KIF5B plays a role in sliding existing MTs towards the cell periphery and aligning them with each other along the plasma membrane. Furthermore, similar to other physiological features of β-cells, this process of MTs sliding is enhanced by a high glucose stimulus. Because a precise alignment of MTs beneath the cell membrane in β-cells is crucial for the regulated secretion of pancreatic enzymes and hormones, KIF5B assumes a significant role in pancreatic activity, both in healthy conditions and during diseases. 

      The authors provide evidence in support of their model by demonstrating that the levels of KIF5B mRNA in MIN6 cells are higher compared to other known KIFs. They further show that when KIF5B is genetically silenced using two different shRNAs, the MT sliding becomes less efficient. Additionally, silencing of KIF5A in the same cells leads to a general reorganization of MTs throughout the cell. Specifically, while control cells exhibit a convoluted and non-radial arrangement of MTs near the cell membrane, KIF5B-depleted cells display a sparse and less dense sub-membrane array of MTs. Based on these findings, the Authors conclude that the loss of KIF5B strongly affects the localization of MTs to the periphery of the cell. Using a dominant-negative approach, the authors also demonstrate that KIF5B facilitates the sliding of MTs by binding to cargo MTs through the kinesin-1 tail binding domain. Additionally, they present evidence suggesting that KIF5B-mediated MT sliding is dependent on glucose, similar to the activity levels of kinesin-1, which increase in the presence of glucose. Notably, when the glucose concentrations in the culturing media of MIN6 cells are reduced from 20 mM to 5 mM, a significant decrease in MT sliding is observed. 

      Strengths:

      This study unveils a previously unexplained mechanism that regulates the specific rearrangement of MTs beneath the cell membrane in pancreatic β-cells. The findings of this research have implications and are of significant interest because the precise regulation of the MT array at the secretion zone plays a critical role in controlling pancreatic function in both healthy and diseased states. In general, the author's conclusions are substantiated by the provided data, and the study demonstrates the utilization of state-of-the-art methodologies including quantification techniques, and elegant dominant-negative experiments. 

      Weaknesses:

      A few relatively minor issues are present and related to data interpretation and the conclusions drawn in the study. Namely, some inconsistencies between what appears to be the overall and sub-membrane MT array in scramble vs. KIF5B-depleted cells, the lack of details about the sub-cellular localization of KIF5B in these cells and the physiological significance of the effect of glucose levels in beta-cells of the pancreas. 

      We thank the reviewer for this insighDul review. In the revised version, we provided re-worded and extended interpretations and conclusions to prevent any issues or misunderstandings.  We trust that while some noted apparent inconsistencies may reflect the intrinsic heterogeneity of the beta cell population, all data presented here indicate the same trend in phenotypes.  In the revised version, we have provided additional cell views and, in places, alternative representative images and videos, to clear out any apparent inconsistencies. We also would like to point out that we in fact reported KIF5B localization: not surprisingly, KIF5B predominantly localized to insulin granules and the punctate staining fills the whole cytoplasm (Figure 2A, bottom panel). However, as pointed out in detail in our response to reviewer 1, we choose to leave out an extensive study of the physiological and metabolic consequences of the reported microtubule network dynamics to a follow-up study. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review): 

      Prior work from the Kaverina lab and others had determined that beta-cells build a microtubule network that differs from the canonical radial organization typical in most mammalian cell types and that this organization facilitates the regulated secretion of insulin-containing secretory granules (IGs). In this manuscript, the authors tested the hypothesis that kinesin-driven microtubule sliding is an underlying mechanism that establishes a sub-membranous microtubule array that regulates IG secretion. They employed knock-down and dominant-negative strategies to convincingly show microtubule sliding does, in fact, drive the assembly of the sub-membranous microtubule band. They also used live cell imaging assays to demonstrate that kinesin-mediated microtubule sliding in beta-cells is triggered by extracellular high glucose. Overall, this is an interesting and important study that relates microtubule dynamics to an important physiological process. The experiments were rigorous and well-controlled. 

      We truly appreciate this reviewer’ opinion. 

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      Figures: 

      (1) Figure 1: 

      a) Why can one not see here, and in most following images, the peripheral sub-membrane microtubule array? One can also not see an accumulation of microtubules in the cell interior. 

      Microtubule pattern in beta cells is variable, and the sub-membrane array is seen in the whole population to a variable extent (see directionality histogram in Figure 2E for statistics). In fact, an array of peripheral MTs parallel to the cell border is present in the example shown in Figure 1 and in all following control images. To make it clearer, we now show the pre-bleach images in Figure 1 D-F at a lower magnification, so that the differences in MT density at the cell periphery and cell center are more clearly seen: MTs lack at the periphery in KF5B-depleted but not the control cells.  

      b) 5 min appears to be a long time and enough time to polymerize a significant number of new microtubules. 

      We interpret this comment as the reviewer’s concern that in FRAP assays, fluorescently-labeled MTs moving into the bleached area might be newly polymerizing MTs rather than preexisting MT relocated into that area. However, this is not the case because newly polymerized MTs contain predominantly quenched “dark” tubulin molecules and only a small percent of fluorescent tubulin. These dim MTs are not included in MT sliding assay analysis, where a threshold for bright MTs is introduced. Now, we added more details for the quantification of these data to Materials and Methods section.

      c) The overall effects appear minor. It is unclear how Fig. 1-Suppl-Fig.1, where no significant difference is shown, is translated into Figure 1 J and K showing a significant difference. 

      With all due respect, we do not agree that the effect is minor. Please see our response to the Public Review where we discuss the major consequences of MT defects in detail. 

      To answer this specific comment, we show that there are significant differences in the number of rapidly moving MTs (5-sec displacement over 0.3 µm) and in the amount of stationary MTs (5sec displacement is below 0.15 µm). There is no significant difference in the amount of slightly displaced MTs (displacements between 0.15 and 0.3 µm; the central part of the histogram). This might indicate that these slight displacements do not depend on kinesin-1 motor but rather are caused by experimental noise, pushing by moving organelles, and/or myosin-dependent forces in the cell. In the revised manuscript, we have this quantification more clearly detailed in Methods and included in Figure legends.

      d) The authors utilize single molecule tracking to further strengthen their conclusion that KIF5B promotes microtubule sliding. The observed effects are weaker than the data obtained from photobleaching experiments. The videos clearly show that there is still significant movement also in KIF5B-depleted cells. If K560RigorE236A binds irreversibly to a microtubule and this microtubule is growing (not only by the addition of tubulin dimers to the plus end; see PMID: 34883065) wouldn't that also result in movement of the tagged K560RigorE236A? As KIF5B is also required in the transport of insulin granules, it should also label "interior microtubules". And in Video 2 it appears that pretty much all "labeled" microtubules are moving. 

      K560RigorE236A forms fiducial marks along the whole MTs lattice, as previously shown in (Tanenbaum et al., 2014). When it is bound to MT lattice, K560RigorE236A moves with the whole MT if it is being relocated. The mechanism described in (PMID: 34883065) appears to be absent or minor in beta cells (see Figure 2- Supplemental Figure 2), thus, even if this mechanism would displace already polymerized MTs, this is not happening in this cell type.

      The reviewer is correct, K560RigorE236A does mark all MTs throughout a beta cell. All MTs are moving slightly in a living cell because they are pushed around by moving organelles, actin contractility, etc. MTs may also be slid by other MT-dependent motors (dynein against the membrane and such). So, it is not surprising that the MT network is “breezing,” and kinesindependent sliding is only a part of MT movement. What we show here is that the KIF5Bdependent MT sliding is responsible for a relatively “long-distance” relocation of MTs manifested in long, directional displacement of fiducial marks.  This does not exclude other movements. This makes extraction of kinesin-dependent MT movements somewhat challenging, of course, that is why we needed to do those extensive analyses. 

      e) Figure 1 G to K is misleading, at least in the context of the provided videos. There are several microtubules that move extensively in shRNA#2-treated cells and overall there appears more movement in this cell as in the control cell. Figure 1I is clearly not representative of the movement shown in Video 2. 

      We apologize if our selection of representative movies/figures for this experiment was imperfect. Indeed, in all depleted cells, SunTag puncta still move to a certain extent, either due to incomplete depletion or to alternative intracellular forces dislocating microtubules. However, there is a clear difference in the fraction of persistently moving puncta (please see Figure 1K and  histogram in Figure 1 - Supplemental Figure 1B). Unfortunately, when the number of SunTag puncta per a cell is variable, it sometimes prevents a good visual perception of the actual distribution of moving versus stationary microtubules. We now show an alternative representative movie for the Figure 1I and the corresponding Video 2, with a goal to compare cells with more consistent numbers of Sun-Tag puncta.

      (2) Figure 2A. 

      a) This is the only image that clearly shows the existence of a sub-membrane microtubule array and the concentration of microtubules in the cell interior. The differences are unclear between the experimental setups including the length of cultivation and knockdown of KIF5B or expression of mutants. 

      We now provide a more detailed description of each image acquisition and processing in Materials and Methods. In brief, while the morphology of MT patterns is intrinsically variable in beta cells, all control cells have populated peripheral MTs that exhibit a more parallel configuration as compared to depletions and mutants.

      b) The authors state "While control cells had convoluted non-radial MTs with a prominent sub-membrane array, typical for beta cells (Fig. 2A), KIF5B-depleted cells featured extra-dense MTs in the cell center and sparse reseeding MTs at the periphery (Fig. 2B, C)". Could that not be explained with the observation that "Kinesin-1 controls microtubule length" (PMID: 34883065)? 

      Thank you for this interesting alternative idea. It does not appear to be the case for beta cells.

      Please see Figure 2-Supplemental Figure 2  and our response to Public Review Comment #3.

      Also, our apologies for the typo in the original manuscript: this is “receding” nor “reseeding”.

      (3) Figure 3: 

      a) This is an elegant way to determine whether KIF5B is involved in microtubule sliding independent of the fact that the effect appears very small. 

      Thank you!            

      b) The assay depends on ectopic expression of a dominant negative mutant. It appears important to show that KIFDNwt is high enough expressed to indeed block the binding of endogenous KIF5B. The authors need to provide a control for this. Furthermore, authors need to provide evidence that other functions of KIF5B are not impaired such as transport of insulin granules and tubulin incorporation or microtubule stability and length.

      Expression of cargo-binding motor domains routinely causes a dominant-negative effect of their cargo transport. This exact construct has been used for the purpose of dominant-negative action previously (Ravindran et al., 2017). It does prevent the membrane cargo binding of KIF5B (Ravindran et al., 2017), thus the transport of insulin granules is also impaired in overexpression cells. Confirming this fact would not influence our study conclusions, so we chose not to repeat these assays for the sake of time.

      c) N-numbers should be similar. The data for KIFDNmut are difficult to interpret with possibly 2 experiments showing little to no displacement and 3 showing displacement. 

      In the revised manuscript, additional data have been added to increase N-numbers.

      (4) Figure 4 and supplements: The morphology of the KIFDNwt cells is greatly affected and this makes it difficult to say whether the effect on microtubules at the cell periphery is a direct or indirect effect. 

      Yes, these cells often have less spread appearance, obscuring visual perception of MT distribution. We have now replaced the image of KIFDNwt cell (Figure 4, Supplemental Figure 1 A) to a more visually representative example.

      Things to do: 

      (1) Notably, the authors have previously reported that high glucose-induced remodeling of microtubule networks facilitates robust glucose-stimulated insulin secretion. This remodeling involves the disassembly of old microtubules and the nucleation of new microtubules. Here, they state that the sub-membrane microtubule array is destabilized via microtubule sliding. What is the relevance of the different processes? Please discuss these in the manuscript. 

      Thank you, we have now extended our discussion of these points and our prior findings. We have also added a schematic model figure for clarity (Figure 7).  

      (2) 5 min appears to be a long time and enough time to polymerize a significant number of new microtubules. Do the authors have any information about the speed of MT formation in MIN6 cells? Can the authors repeat this experiment by preventing MT polymerization? Or repeat the experiment with EB1/EB3 reporter to visualize microtubule growth in the same experimental setting? 

      While some MT polymerization will happen in this timeframe, newly polymerized MTs contain predominantly quenched “dark” tubulin molecules and only a small percent of fluorescent tubulin. These dim MTs are not included in MT sliding assay analysis, where a threshold for bright MTs is introduced. We apologize for initially omitting certain details from the FRAP assay analysis. Now these details have been added.   

      Are the microtubules shown on the cell surface (TIRF microscopy) or do we see here all microtubules? 

      Please see Materials and Methods for microscopy methods and image processing for each figure. Specifically, FRAP assays show a maximum intensity projection of spinning disk confocal stacks over 2.4µm in height (approximately the ventral half of a cell).

      (3) Previously, it has been shown that KIF5B induces tubulin incorporation along the microtubule shaft in a concentration-dependent manner. Moreover, running KIF5B increases microtubule rescue frequency and unlimited growth of microtubules. Notably, KIF5B regulates microtubule network mass and organization in cells (PMID: 34883065). Consequently, it appears possible that the here observed phenomena of changes in the microtubule network might be due to alterations in these processes. Authors need to exclude these possibilities and discuss them. 

      Thank you for this interesting alternative idea. It does not appear to be the case for beta cells. Please see Figure 2-Supplemental Figure 2  and our response to Public Review Comment #3.

      (4) It is important that the authors describe in the text and possibly in the figure legends the differences between the experimental set-ups including the length of cultivation and knock down of KIF5B or expression of mutants. 

      Thank you, please see these details in the text (Materials and Methods section).

      (5) Figure 5: Does KIF5B depletion rescue the kinesore-induced defects 

      Thank you for suggesting this control. We have now conducted corresponding experiments. The answer is yes, it does. Kinesore does not induce detectable changes in MT patterns in KIF5Bdepleted cells (new Figure 5-Supplemental Figure 2). 

      (6) Can the authors block kinesin-1 resulting in microtubule accumulation in the cell center and then release the block, and best inhibiting microtubule formation, to see whether the microtubules accumulated in the cell center will be transported to the periphery? 

      This proposed experiment would have been a nice illustration to the study, however it has proven to be too challenging. Unfortunately we have to leave it for the future studies. However,  the experiments already included in the paper are sufficient to prove our conclusions. 

      Minor comments: 

      (1) The English needs to be improved. Oaen it is unclear what the authors try to convey. The manuscript is difficult to read and contains several overstatements. 

      The revised manuscript has been through several rounds of proof-reading for clarity.

      (2) It is important to describe in more detail in the introduction what is known about KIF5B in beta cells. Previously, it has been demonstrated that silencing, or inactivation by a dominant negative form of KIF5B, blocks the sustained phase of glucose-stimulated insulin secretion (PMID: 9112396, PMID: 12356920, PMID: 20870970). 

      Yes, this is of course very important and have been cited in the original manuscript. Now, we have expanded the discussion on the matter.

      (3) Figure 1B and Fig. 1 Suppl Fig.1: Please provide band sizes and provide information on the size of KIF5B. 

      We have replaced Fig. 1B and Suppl Fig 1A with quantitative analysis of KIF5B depletion, not found in new Fig. 1B and Suppl Fig. 1A-C. 

      (4) It is important to state the used glucose concentrations in Figure 1D (based on the methods section it is probably 25 mM glucose) and all subsequent experiments. Is this correct and comparable to Figure 6A or B? For the non-specialized reader, more information should be provided on why initial glucose starvation is performed.  

      Cell culture models of pancreatic beta cells are routinely maintained at glucose levels that at considered “high”, or stimulatory for secretion. This is needed to prevent the loss of cells’ capacity to respond to glucose stimulation over generations. In order to test GSIS, cells need to be equilibrated at low (fasting, standardly 2.8mM) glucose levels for several hours, so that they are capable of secreting insulin upon glucose addition. 25mM glucose is normally used to stimulate GSIS in cell culture models of beta cells, like MIN6. This is a higher concentration as compared to what is needed to stimulate primary beta cells in islets.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      I have the following specific questions that pertain to data interpretation and the conclusions drawn.

      (1) The morphology of the overall MT array before the bleach treatment in both control cells and KIF5B-KD cells depicted in Figure 1D-F and Figure 2A-C appears to be distinct. In Figure 1, it seems that the absence of KIF5B results in a general augmentation of MT mass, whereas the arrangement presented in Figure 2 indicates the contrary. Even in the sub-membrane areas, this phenomenon appears to hold true. However, the images used in this study, which depict entire cells or a significant portion of cells, may not be ideal for visualizing the sub-membrane regions.

      It would be beneficial if the author could offer some explanations for this apparent inconsistency. 

      While beta cell population is intrinsically heterogeneous, all data presented here indicate the same trend in phenotypes. Possibly, some apparent inconsistency between figure 1 and 2 appeared because in the original manuscript we did not show the pre-bleach whole-cell overview in Figure 1. In the revised version, we now show the whole cells for pre-bleach so that MT organization at the cell periphery can be assessed. Please note that in the control cell, MTs are more or less equally distributed over the cell, while in KIF5B depletions the cell periphery is significantly less populated than the cell center. Furthermore, we did not detect MT mass augmentation or increase in KIF5B depletions. One possible explanation for such reviewer’s impression from Figure 2 is that Figure 2 F-H shows thresholded images where threshold was adjusted to highlight peripheral MTs in each cell. Please note that this is not the same threshold for each cell (see Figure 2 - Supplemental Figure 2 and 3). Thus, KIF5B-depleted cells that have fewer MTs at the periphery appear brighter in these thresholded images. For the true comparison of MT intensity, please see Figure 2 A-C (grayscale image, not the threshold).

      (2) It would be helpful if the author could provide a visual representation or comment on the sub-cellular localization of KIF5B in MIN6 cells. Is it predominantly localized in the submembrane region, or is it more evenly distributed throughout the cytoplasm? 

      Please see Fig 2A, lower panel. KIF5B is seen across the cell as a punctate staining, in agreement with previous findings that it mostly localize at IGs.

      (3) The alteration in microtubule (MT) organization and sliding in the absence of KIF5B seems to initiate in proximity to the apparent microtubule organizing center (MTOC) depicted in Figure 2A, and then "simply" extends towards the sub-membrane region. Although the authors acknowledge it, it would be advantageous for the readers to have a clearer indication that the sub-membrane microtubule (MT) reorganization in the absence of KIF5B is a result of a broader MT reorganization rather than a specific occurrence restricted to the sub-membrane regions. 

      Thank you for this comment. We now extend our discussion to clearer state our conclusions and interpretations of this point. We also have added a schematic Figure 7 as an illustration. 

      (4) Regarding the "glucose experiments," it is common to add 20-25 mM glucose to culture media, but physiological concentrations of glucose typically hover around 5 mM. Therefore, it is somewhat unclear what the implications are when investigating the impact of KIF5B depletion on MT sliding at 2.8 mM of glucose. It would be helpful if the authors could provide some commentary on this matter, particularly in relation to physiological and pathological conditions. 

      2.8 mM glucose is a standard low glucose condition used to model glucose deprivation/fasting. For functional primary beta cells within pancreatic islets, GSIS can be triggered by glucose stimulation as low as 8-12 mM glucose. However, for glucose stimulation of cultured beta cells such as MIN6 used in this paper, 20-25 mM glucose is standardly used because these cell lines have a higher threshold of stimulation compared to primary beta cells and whole islets.

      (5) In supplementary Figure 1A, it would be helpful if the lanes in the WB were marked indicating what is what. In my observation, it appears that Supplementary Figure 1A, particularly lanes #2, 3, and 4, display the GAPDH protein (MW 36 kDa) (or is it alpha-tubulin, as mentioned in the Material and Methods section and indicated in lane #409?) relative to Figure 1A. I am curious about KIF5B (MW 108 kDa). Is it represented by the upper band? Did the author probe the same membrane simultaneously with two different primary antibodies? This should be clarified, and the author should indicate the molecular weight of the ladder. 

      Indeed, in the original WB two antibodies have been used together, due to a challenge in collecting a sufficient number of shRNA-expressing beta cells. It caused a confusion and improper interpretation of the loading control. We thank the reviewer for catching this.  We have now replaced old Fig. 1B and Suppl. Fig. 1A with quantitative analysis of KIF5B depletion based on single-cell immunofluorescent staining. It is now found in new Fig. 1B and Suppl Fig. 1A-C.  

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      In all of the figures that present microtubule orientations (e.g. Figure 2E) the error bars obscure the vertical bins making them difficult to read or interpret. If they were rendered at a larger scale, it would be easier to read and interpret these results. 

      Thank you pointing this out. We now show these histograms with a different format of error bars and without outliers that obscure the view. A variant with outliers is now shown in the supplement. 

      Some of the callouts to the videos in the paper are inaccurate. Perhaps the authors reordered sections of the paper but failed to correctly renumber the video citations? 

      Thank you for this comment, we have corrected all callouts now.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This short report shows that the transcription factor gene mirror is specifically expressed in the posterior region of the butterfly wing imaginal disk, and uses CRISPR mosaic knock-outs to show it is necessary to specify the morphological features (scales, veins, and surface) of this area.

      Strengths:

      The data and figures support the conclusions. The article is swiftly written and makes an interesting evolutionary comparison to the function of this gene in Drosophila. Based on the data presented, it can now be established that mirror likely has a similar selector function for posterior-wing identity in a plethora of insects.

      We thank the reviewer for their feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      This first version has minor terminological issues regarding the use of the terms "domains" and "compartment".

      We acknowledge that the terminologies “domains” and “compartments” might lead to confusion. To avoid confusion we have removed the term “compartment” from the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      This is a short and unpretentious paper. It is an interesting area and therefore, although much of this area of research was pioneered in flies, extending basic findings to butterflies would be worthwhile. Indeed, there is an intriguing observation but it is technically flawed and these flaws are serious.

      The authors show that mirror is expressed at the back of the wing in butterflies (as in flies). They present some evidence that is required for the proper development of the back of the wing in butterflies (a region dubbed the vannus by the ancient guru Snodgrass). But there are problems with that evidence. First, concerning the method, using CRISP they treat embryos and the expectation is that the mirror gene will be damaged in groups of cell lineages, giving a mosaic animal in which some lines of cells are normal for mirror and others are not. We do not know where the clones or patches of cells that are defective for mirror are because they are not marked. Also, we do not know what part of the wing is wild type and what part is mutant for mirror. When the mirror mutant cells colonise the back of the wing and that butterfly survives (many butterflies fail to develop), the back of the wing is altered in some selected butterflies. This raises a second problem: we do not know whether the rear of the wing is missing or transformed. From the images, the appearance of the back of the wing is clearly different from the wild type, but is that due to transformation or not? And then I believe we need to know specifically what the difference is between the rear of the wing and the main part. What we see is a silvery look at the back that is not present in the main part, is it the structure of the scales? We are not told.

      Thank you for this feedback. We appreciate that many readers may not accustomed to looking at mosaic knockouts. As discussed in a previous review article (Zhang & Reed 2017), we rely on a combination of contralateral asymmetry and replicates to infer mutant phenotypes. For many genes (e.g. pigmentation enzymes) mutant clones are obvious, but for other types of genes (e.g. ligands) clone boundaries are sometimes not directly diagnosable. It is simply a limitation of our study system. Nonetheless, you see for yourself that “the back of the wing is altered in some butterflies” – the effects of deleting mirror are clear and repeatable.

      In terms of interpreting mutant phenotypes, we agree that that paper would benefit from a better description of the specific effects. Therefore, we have included an improved, more systematic description of phenotypes, along with better-annotated figures showing changes in wing shape and venation, scale coloration, and color pattern transformation (e.g. posterior elongation of the orange marginal stripes).

      There are other problems. Mirror is only part of a group of genes in flies and in flies both iroquois and mirror are needed to make the back of the wing, the alula (Kehl et al). What is known about iro expression in butterflies?

      In Drosophila mirror, araucan, and caupolican comprise the so-called Iroqouis Complex of genes. As denoted in Figure S4 and in Kerner et al (doi: https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2148-9-74) the divergence of araucan and caupolican into two separate paralogs is restricted to Drosophila. As in most insects, butterflies have only two Iroquois Complex genes: araucan and mirror. We tested the role of araucan in Junonia coenia as shown in our pre-print: https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.11.21.568172. Its expression appears to be restricted to early pupal wings where it is transcribed in all scale-forming cells. Mosaic araucan KOs resulted in a change in scale iridescent coloration associated with changes in the laminar thickness of scale cells.  

      In flies, mirror regulates a late and local expression of dpp that seems to be responsible for making the alula. What happens in butterflies? Would a study of the expression of Dpp in wildtype and mirror compromised wings be useful?

      We thank the reviewer for the proposal and agree that a future study comparing Dpp in wild-type versus mirror KO butterflies would be useful to clarify the mechanism of Dpp signalling in wing development. It is not clear, however, that the results of a Dpp experiment would change the conclusions of our current study therefore we decided not to undertake these additional experiments for our revision.

      Thus, I find the paper to be disappointing for a general journal as it does little more than claim what was discovered in Drosophila is at least partly true in butterflies. 

      We respect that the reviewer does not have a strong interest in the comparative aspects of this study. Fair enough. This report is primarily aimed at biologists interested in the evolutionary history of insect wings.

      Also, it fails to explain what the authors mean by "wing domains" and "domain specification". They are not alone, butterfly workers, in general, appear vague about these concepts, their vagueness allowing too much loose thinking.

      A domain is “a region distinctively marked by some physical feature”. This term is used extensively in the developmental biology literature (e.g. “expression domain”, “embryonic domain”, “tissue domain”, “domain specification”) and is found throughout popular textbooks (e.g. Alberts et al. “The Cell”, Gilbert “Developmental Biology”). We prefer the term “domain” because of its association in the Drosophila literature with transcription factors that define fields of cells. We specifically avoided using the term “compartment” because of its association with cell lineage, which we have not tested. 

      Since these matters are at the heart of the purpose and meaning of the work reported here, we readers need a paper containing more critical thought and information. I would like to have a better and more logical introduction and discussion.

      We would like the very same thing, of course, and we hope the reviewer finds our revised manuscript to be more satisfying to read.

      The authors do define what they mean by the vannus of the wing. In flies the definition of compartments is clear and abundantly demonstrated, with gene expression and requirement being limited precisely to sets of cells that display lineage boundaries. It is true that domains of gene expression in flies, for example of the iroquois complex, which includes mirror, can only be related to patterns with difficulty. Some recap of what is known plus the opinion of the authors on how they interpret papers on possible lineage domains in butterflies might also be useful as the reader, is no wiser about what the authors might mean at the end of it!

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. However, our experiments have little to contribute to the topic of cell lineage compartmentalization. We have therefore opted to avoid speculating on this topic to prevent confusion and to keep the manuscript focused on our experimental results.

      The references are sometimes inappropriate. The discovery of the AP compartments should not be referred to Guillen et al 1995, but to Morata and Lawrence 1975. Proofreading is required.

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting this important reference. We have included it in our revision.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Chatterjee et al. examines the role of the mirror locus in patterning butterfly wings. The authors examine the pattern of mirror expression in the common buckeye butterfly, Junonia coenia, and then employ CRISPR mutagenesis to generate mosaic butterflies carrying clones of mirror mutant cells. They find that mirror is expressed in a well-defined posterior sector of final-instar wing discs from both hindwings and forewings and that CRISPR-injected larvae display a loss of adult wing structures presumably derived from the mirror expressing region of hindwing primordium (the case for forewings is a bit less clear since the mirror domain is narrower than in the hindwing, but there also do seem to be some anomalies in posterior regions of forewings in adults derived from CRISPR injected larvae). The authors conclude that the wings of these butterflies have at least three different fundamental wing compartments, the mirror domain, a posterior domain defined by engrailed expression, and an anterior domain expressing neither mirror nor engrailed. They speculate that this most posterior compartment has been reduced to a rudiment in Drosophila and thus has not been adequately recognized as such a primary regional specialization.

      Critique:

      This is a very straightforward study and the experimental results presented support the key claims that mirror is expressed in a restricted posterior section of the wing primordium and that mosaic wings from CRISPR-injected larvae display loss of adult wing structures presumably derived from cells expressing mirror (or at least nearby). The major issue I have with this paper is the strong interpretation of these findings that lead the authors to conclude that mirror is acting as a high-level gene akin to engrailed in defining a separate extreme posterior wing compartment. To place this claim in context, it is important in my view to consider what is known about engrailed, for which there is ample evidence to support the claim that this gene does play a very ancestral and conserved function in defining posterior compartments of all body segments (including the wing) across arthropods.

      (1) Engrailed is expressed in a broad posterior domain with a sharp anterior border in all segments of virtually all arthropods examined (broad use of a very good panspecies anti-En antibody makes this case very strong).

      (2) In Drosophila, marked clones of wing cells (generated during larval stages) strictly obey a straight anterior-posterior border indicating that cells in these two domains do not normally intermix, thus, supporting the claim that a clear A/P lineage compartment exists.

      In my opinion, mirror does not seem to be in the same category of regulator as engrailed for the following reasons:

      (1) There is no evidence that I am aware of, either from the current experiments, or others that the mirror expression domain corresponds to a clonal lineage compartment. It is also unclear from the data shown in this study whether engrailed is co-expressed with mirror in the posterior-most cells of J. coenia wing discs. If so, it does not seem justified to infer that mirror acts as an independent determinant of the region of the wing where it is expressed.

      (2) Mirror is not only expressed in a posterior region of the wing in flies but also in the ventral region of the eye. In Drosophila, mirror mutants not only lack the alula (derived approximately from cells where mirror is expressed), but also lack tissue derived from the ventral region of the eye disc (although this ventral tissue loss phenotype may extend beyond the cells expressing mirror).

      In summary, it seems most reasonable to me to think of mirror as a transcription factor that provides important development information for a diverse set of cells in which it can be expressed (posterior wing cells and ventral eye cells) but not that it acts as a high-level regulator as engrailed.

      Recommendation:

      While the data provided in this succinct study are solid and interesting, it is not clear to me that these findings support the major claim that mirror defines an extreme posterior compartment akin to that specified by engrailed. Minimally, the authors should address the points outlined above in their discussion section and greatly tone down their conclusion regarding mirror being a conserved selector-like gene dedicated to establishing posterior-most fates of the wing. They also should cite and discuss the original study in Drosophila describing the mirror expression pattern in the embryo and eye and the corresponding eye phenotype of mirror mutants: McNeill et al., Genes & Dev. 1997. 11: 1073-1082; doi:10.1101/gad.11.8.1073.

      We thank the reviewer for their summary, critique, and recommendations. We agree with everything the reviewer says. Honestly, however, we were surprised by these comments because we took great care in the paper to never refer to mirror as a compartmentalization gene or claim it has a function in cell lineage compartmentalization like engrailed. As pointed out, we lack clonal analyses to test for compartmentalization. This is why we used the term “domain” instead of “compartment” in the title and throughout the manuscript. Nevertheless, we have recrafted the discussion in the manuscript, including completely removing the term “compartment”, to better avoid implications that mirror plays a role in cell lineage compartmentalization. 

      We also thank the reviewer for recommending the paper about the role of mirror in eye development. For the sake of keeping the paper focused, however, we decided not to broach the topic of mirror functions outside the context of wing development.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      I have minor comments for improvement.

      The abstract and introductions are terminologically problematic when they refer to the concept of compartment and compartment boundaries. Allegedly this confusion has previously propagated in several articles related to butterfly wing development, which keeps alienating this literature from being taken seriously by fly specialists, for example. So it is important to use the right terms. I will try to explain point by point here, but I would appreciate it if the authors could undertake a significant rewrite taking these comments into account. The authors use the terms compartment and compartment boundary. This has a very specific use in developmental genetics: mitotic clones never cross a boundary (or compartment). I think the authors can keep referring to the equivalent of the A-P boundary, which is situated somewhere between M1-M2 based on unpublished data from the Patel Lab, and is not very well defined (Engrailed expression moves a little bit during development in this area). Domain is a looser term and can be used more liberally to describe genetically defined regions.

      - "Classical morphological work subdivides insect wings into several distinct domains along the antero-posterior (AP) axis, each of which can evolve relatively independently." Yes. This concept of domain and individuation seems important. You could make a proposed link to selector genes here.

      - "There has been little molecular evidence, however, for AP subdivision beyond a single compartment boundary described from Drosophila melanogaster." Incorrect, and this conflates "domain" and "compartment".

      Flies have wing AP domains too, that pattern their veins (see the cited Banerjee et al). 

      - "Our results confirm that insect wings can have more than one posterior developmental domain, and support models of how selector genes may facilitate evolutionarily individuation of distinct AP domains in insect wings". Yes, and I like the second part of the sentence. Still, I would recommend simply deleting "confirm that insect wings can have more than one posterior developmental domain, and" because this is neglecting previous work on AP genetic regionalization in both flies (vein literature) and butterflies (e.g. McKenna and Nijhout, Banerjee et al).

      - "Analyses of wing pattern diversity across butterflies, considering both natural variation and genetic mutants, suggest that wings can be subdivided into at least five AP domains, bounded by the M1, M3, Cu2, and 2A veins respectively, within each of which there are strong correlations in color pattern variation and wing morphology (Figure 1A)". Yes, and I would recommend emphasizing they correspond to welldefined gene expression domains as mentioned in Banerjee et al, or McKenna and Nijhout.

      - "The anterior-most of these domains, bordered by the M1 vein, appears to correspond to an AP compartment boundary originally described by cell lineage tracing in Drosophila melanogaster, and later supported in butterfly wings by expression of the Engrailed transcription factor. Interestingly, however, D. melanogaster work has yet to reveal clear evidence for additional AP domain boundaries in the wing." Confusingly, because the first sentence is about compartments while the second is about AP domains. I also think the claim that Dmel has no other known AP domains is dubious because Spalt is highly regionalized in flies.

      - "Previous authors have proposed the existence of such individuated domains, and speculated that they may be specified by selector genes.5,10 Our data provide experimental support for this model, and now motivate us to identify factors that specify other domain boundaries between the M1 and A2 veins." Yes, I completely agree with this way to emphasize the selector effect, and to link it to the concept of "individuated domain"

      We cannot thank the reviewer enough for the time and thought they devoted to giving helpful suggestions to improve our manuscript. We have applied all of the above recommendations to the revision.

      Fig. S1: the field needs to move away from Red/Green microscopy images, for accessibility reasons.

      The easiest fix here would be to change the red channels to magenta.

      Green/Magenta provides excellent contrast and accessibility in general in 2-channel images.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have improved the color accessibility of Fig. S1.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study addresses how faces and bodies are integrated in two STS face areas revealed by fMRI in the primate brain. It builds upon recordings and analysis of the responses of large populations of neurons to three sets of images, that vary face and body positions. These sets allowed the authors to thoroughly investigate invariance to position on the screen (MC HC), to pose (P1 P2), to rotation (0 45 90 135 180 225 270 315), to inversion, to possible and impossible postures (all vs straight), to the presentation of head and body together or in isolation. By analyzing neuronal responses, they found that different neurons showed preferences for body orientation, head orientation, or the interaction between the two. By using a linear support vector machine classifier, they show that the neuronal population can decode head-body angle presented across orientations, in the anterior aSTS patch (but not middle mSTS patch), except for mirror orientation.

      Strengths:

      These results extend prior work on the role of Anterior STS fundus face area in face-body integration and its invariance to mirror symmetry, with a rigorous set of stimuli revealing the workings of these neuronal populations in processing individuals as a whole, in an important series of carefully designed conditions.

      Minor issues and questions that could be addressed by the authors:

      (1) Methods. While monkeys certainly infer/recognize that individual pictures refer to the same pose with varying orientations based on prior studies (Wang et al.), I am wondering whether in this study monkeys saw a full rotation of each of the monkey poses as a video before seeing the individual pictures of the different orientations, during recordings.

      The monkeys had not been exposed to videos of a rotating monkey pose before the recordings. However, they were reared and housed with other monkeys, providing them with ample experience of monkey poses from different viewpoints.

      (2) Experiment 1. The authors mention that neurons are preselected as face-selective, body-selective, or both-selective. Do the Monkey Sum Index and ANOVA main effects change per Neuron type?

      We have performed a new analysis to assess whether the Monkey Sum Index is related to the response strength for the face versus the body as measured in the Selectivity Test of Experiment 1. To do this we selected face- and body-category selective neurons, as well as neurons responding selectively to both faces and bodies. First, we selected those neurons that responded significantly to either faces, bodies, or the two control object categories, using a split-plot ANOVA for these 40 stimuli. From those neurons, we selected face-selective ones having at least a twofold larger mean net response to faces compared to bodies (faces > 2 * bodies) and the control objects for faces (faces  > 2* objects). Similarly, a body-selective neuron was defined by a twofold larger mean net response to bodies compared to faces and the control objects for bodies. A body-and-face selective neuron was defined as having a twofold larger net response to the faces compared to their control objects, and to bodies compared to their control objects, with the ratio between mean response to bodies and faces being less than twofold. Then, we compared the distribution of the Monkey Sum Index (MSI) for each region (aSTS; mSTS), pose (P1, P2), and centering (head- (HC) or monkey-centered (MC)) condition. Too few body-and-face selective neurons were present in each combination of region, pose, and centering (a maximum of 7) to allow a comparison of their MSI distribution with the other neuron types. The Figure below shows the distribution of the MSI for the different orientation-neuron combinations for the body- and face-selective neurons (same format as in Figure 3a, main text). The number of body-selective neurons, according to the employed criteria, varied from 21 to 29, whereas the number of face-selective neurons ranged from 14 to 24 (pooled across monkeys). The data of the two subjects are shown in a different color and the number of cases for each subject is indicated (n1: number of cases for M1; n2: number of cases for M2). The arrows indicate the medians for the data pooled across the monkey subjects. For the MC condition, the MSI tended to be more negative (i.e. relatively less response to the monkey compared to the sum of the body and face responses) for the face compared to the body cells, but this was significant only for mSTS and P1 (p = 0.043; Wilcoxon rank sum test; tested after averaging the indices per neuron to avoid dependence of indices within a neuron). No consistent, nor significant tendencies were observed for the HC stimuli. This absence of a consistent relationship between MSI and face- versus body-selectivity is in line with the absence of a correlation between the MSI and face- versus body-selectivity using natural images of monkeys in a previous study (Zafirova Y, Bognár A, Vogels R. Configuration-sensitive face-body interactions in primate visual cortex. Prog Neurobiol. 2024 Jan;232:102545).

      We did not perform a similar analysis for the main effects of the two-way ANOVA because the very large majority of neurons showed a significant effect of body orientation and thus no meaningful difference between the two neuron types can be expected.

      Author response image 1.

      (3) I might have missed this information, but the correlation between P1 and P2 seems to not be tested although they carry similar behavioral relevance in terms of where attention is allocated and where the body is facing for each given head-body orientation.

      Indeed, we did not compute this correlation between the responses to the sitting (P1) and standing (P2) pose avatar images. However, as pointed out by the reviewer, one might expect such correlations because of the same head orientations and body-facing directions. Thus, we computed the correlation between the 64 head-body orientation conditions of P1 and P2 for those neurons that were tested with both poses and showed a response for both poses (Split-plot ANOVA). This was performed for the Head-Centered and Monkey-Centered tests of Experiment 1 for each monkey and region. Note that not all neurons were tested with both poses (because of failure to maintain isolation of the single unit in both tests or the monkey stopped working) and not all neurons that were recorded in both tests showed a significant response for both poses, which is not unexpected since these neurons can be pose selective. The distribution of the Pearson correlation coefficients of the neurons with a significant response in both tests is shown in Figure S1. The median correlation coefficient was significantly larger than zero for each region, monkey, and centering condition (outcome of Wilcoxon tests, testing whether the median was different from zero (p1 = p-value for M1; p2: p-value for M2) in Figure), indicating that the effect of head and/or body orientation generalizes across pose. We have noted this now in the Results (page 12) and added the Figure (New Figure S1) in the Suppl. Material.

      (4) Is the invariance for position HC-MC larger in aSTS neurons compared to mSTS neurons, as could be expected from their larger receptive fields?

      Yes, the position tolerance of the interaction of body and head orientation was significantly larger for aSTS compared to mSTS neurons, as we described on pages 11 and 12 of the Results. This is in line with larger receptive fields in aSTS than in mSTS. However, we did not plot receptive fields in the present study.

      (5) L492 "The body-inversion effect likely results from greater exposure to upright than inverted bodies during development". Monkeys display more hanging upside-down behavior than humans, however, does the head appear more tilted in these natural configurations?

      Indeed, infant monkeys do spend some time hanging upside down from their mother's belly. While we lack quantitative data on this behavior, casual observations suggest that even young monkeys spend more time upright. The tilt of the head while hanging upside down can vary, just as it does in standing or sitting monkeys (as when they search for food or orient to other individuals). To our knowledge, no quantitative data exist on the frequency of head tilts in upright versus upside-down monkeys. Therefore, we refrain from further speculation on this interesting point, which warrants more attention.

      (6) Methods in Experiment 1. SVM. How many neurons are sufficient to decode the orientation?

      The number of neurons that are needed to decode the head-body orientation angle depends on which neurons are included, as we show in a novel analysis of the data of Experiment 1. We employed a neuron-dropping analysis, similar to Chiang et al. (Chiang FK, Wallis JD, Rich EL. Cognitive strategies shift information from single neurons to populations in prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2022 Feb 16;110(4):709-721) to assess the positive (or negative) contribution of each neuron to the decoding performance. We performed cross-validated linear SVM decoding N times, each time leaving out a different neuron (using N-1 neurons; 2000 resamplings of pseudo-population vectors). We then ranked decoding accuracies from highest to lowest, identifying the ‘worst’ (rank 1) to ‘best’ (rank N) neurons. Next, we conducted N decodings, incrementally increasing the number of included neurons from 1 to N, starting with the worst-ranked neuron (rank 1) and sequentially adding the next (rank 2, rank 3, etc.). This analysis focused on zero versus straight angle decoding in the aSTS, as it yielded the highest accuracy. We applied it when training on MC and testing on HC for each pose. Plotting accuracy as a function of the number of included neurons suggested that less than half contributed positively to decoding. We show also the ten “best” neurons for each centering condition and pose. These have a variety of tuning patterns for head and body orientation suggesting that the decoding of head-body orientation angle depends on a population code. Notably, the best-ranked (rank N) neuron alone achieved above-chance accuracy. We have added this interesting and novel result to the Results (page 16) and Suppl. Material (new Figure S3).

      (7) Figure 3D 3E. Could the authors please indicate for each of these neurons whether they show a main effect of face, body, or interaction, as well as their median corrected correlation to get a flavor of these numbers for these examples?

      We have indicated these now in Figure 3.

      (8) Methods and Figure 1A. It could be informative to precise whether the recordings are carried in the lateral part of the STS or in the fundus of the STS both for aSTS and mSTS for comparison to other studies that are using these distinctions (AF, AL, MF, ML).

      In experiment 1, the recording locations were not as medial as the fundus. For experiments 2 and 3, the ventral part of the fundus was included, as described in the Methods. We have added this to the Methods now (page 31).

      Wang, G., Obama, S., Yamashita, W. et al. Prior experience of rotation is not required for recognizing objects seen from different angles. Nat Neurosci 8, 1768-1775 (2005). https://doi-org.insb.bib.cnrs.fr/10.1038/nn1600

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper investigates the neuronal encoding of the relationship between head and body orientations in the brain. Specifically, the authors focus on the angular relationship between the head and body by employing virtual avatars. Neuronal responses were recorded electrophysiologically from two fMRI-defined areas in the superior temporal sulcus and analyzed using decoding methods. They found that: (1) anterior STS neurons encode head-body angle configurations; (2) these neurons distinguish aligned and opposite head-body configurations effectively, whereas mirror-symmetric configurations are more difficult to differentiate; and (3) an upside-down inversion diminishes the encoding of head-body angles. These findings advance our understanding of how visual perception of individuals is mediated, providing a fundamental clue as to how the primate brain processes the relationship between head and body - a process that is crucial for social communication.

      Strengths:

      The paper is clearly written, and the experimental design is thoughtfully constructed and detailed. The use of electrophysiological recordings from fMRI-defined areas elucidated the mechanism of head-body angle encoding at the level of local neuronal populations. Multiple experiments, control conditions, and detailed analyses thoroughly examined various factors that could affect the decoding results. The decoding methods effectively and consistently revealed the encoding of head-body angles in the anterior STS neurons. Consequently, this study offers valuable insights into the neuronal mechanisms underlying our capacity to integrate head and body cues for social cognition-a topic that is likely to captivate readers in this field.

      Weaknesses:

      I did not identify any major weaknesses in this paper; I only have a few minor comments and suggestions to enhance clarity and further strengthen the manuscript, as detailed in the Private Recommendations section.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Zafirova et al. investigated the interaction of head and body orientation in the macaque superior temporal sulcus (STS). Combining fMRI and electrophysiology, they recorded responses of visual neurons to a monkey avatar with varying head and body orientations. They found that STS neurons integrate head and body information in a nonlinear way, showing selectivity for specific combinations of head-body orientations. Head-body configuration angles can be reliably decoded, particularly for neurons in the anterior STS. Furthermore, body inversion resulted in reduced decoding of head-body configuration angles. Compared to previous work that examined face or body alone, this study demonstrates how head and body information are integrated to compute a socially meaningful signal.

      Strengths:

      This work presents an elegant design of visual stimuli, with a monkey avatar of varying head and body orientations, making the analysis and interpretation straightforward. Together with several control experiments, the authors systematically investigated different aspects of head-body integration in the macaque STS. The results and analyses of the paper are mostly convincing.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Using ANOVA, the authors demonstrate the existence of nonlinear interactions between head and body orientations. While this is a conventional way of identifying nonlinear interactions, it does not specify the exact type of the interaction. Although the computation of the head-body configuration angle requires some nonlinearity, it's unclear whether these interactions actually contribute. Figure 3 shows some example neurons, but a more detailed analysis is needed to reveal the diversity of the interactions. One suggestion would be to examine the relationship between the presence of an interaction and the neural encoding of the configuration angle.

      This is an excellent suggestion. To do this, one needs to identify the neurons that contribute to the decoding of head-body orientation angles. For that, we employed a neuron-dropping analysis, similar to Chiang et al. (Chiang FK, Wallis JD, Rich EL. Cognitive strategies shift information from single neurons to populations in prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2022 Feb 16;110(4):709-721.) to assess the positive (or negative) contribution of each neuron to the decoding performance. We performed cross-validated linear SVM decoding N times, each time leaving out a different neuron (using N-1 neurons; 2000 resamplings of pseudo-population vectors). We then ranked decoding accuracies from highest to lowest, identifying the ‘worst’ (rank 1) to ‘best’ (rank N) neurons. Next, we conducted N decodings, incrementally increasing the number of included neurons from 1 to N, starting with the worst-ranked neuron (rank 1) and sequentially adding the next (rank 2, rank 3, etc.). This analysis focused on zero versus straight angle decoding in the aSTS, as it yielded the highest accuracy. We applied it when training on MC and testing on HC for each pose. Plotting accuracy as a function of the number of included neurons suggested that less than half contributed positively to decoding (see Figure S3). We examined the tuning for head and body orientation of the 10 “best” neurons (Figure S3). For half or more of those the two-way ANOVA showed a significant interaction. These are indicated by the red color in the Figure. They showed a variety of tuning patterns for head and body orientation, suggesting that the decoding of the head-body orientation angle results from a combination of neurons with different tuning profiles. Based on a suggestion from reviewer 2, we performed for each neuron of experiment 1 a one-way ANOVA with as factor head-body orientation angle. To do that, we combined all 64 trials that had the same head-body orientation angle. The percentage of neurons (required to be responsive in the tested condition) for which this one-way ANOVA was significant was low but larger than the expected 5% (Type 1 error), with a median of 16.5% (range: 3 to 23%) in aSTS and 8% for mSTS (range: 0-19%). However, a higher percentage of the 10 best neurons for each pose (indicated by the star) showed a significant one-way ANOVA for angle (for P1, MC: 50% (95% confidence interval (CI): 19% – 81%); P1, HC: 70% (CI: 35% - 93%); P2, MC: 70% (CI: 35% – 93%); P2: HC: 50% (CI: 19%-81%)). These percentages were significantly higher than expected for a random sample from the population of neurons for each pose-centering combination (expected percentages listed in the same order as above: 16%, 13%, 16%, and 10%; all outside CI). Thus, for at least half of the “best” neurons, the response differed significantly among the head-orientation angles at the single neuron level. Nonetheless, the tuning profiles were diverse, suggesting a populationl code for head-body orientation angle. We have added this interesting and novel result to the Results (page 16) and Suppl. Material (Figure S3).

      (2) Figure 4 of the paper shows a better decoding of the configuration angle in the anterior STS than in the middle STS. This is an interesting result, suggesting a transformation in the neural representation between these two areas. However, some control analyses are needed to further elucidate the nature of this transformation. For example, what about the decoding of head and body orientations - dose absolute orientation information decrease along the hierarchy, accompanying the increase in configuration information?

      We have performed now two additional analyses, one in which we decoded the orientation of the head and another one in which we decoded the orientation of the body. We employed the responses to the avatar of experiment 1, using the same sample of neurons of which we decoded the head-body orientation angle. To decode the head orientation, the trials with identical head orientation, irrespective of their body orientation, were given the same label. For this, we employed only responses in the head-centered condition. To decode the body orientation, the trials with identical body orientation, irrespective of their head orientation, had the same label, and we employed only responses in the body-centered condition. The decoding was performed separately for each pose (P1 and P2) and region. We decoded either the responses of 20 neurons (10 randomly sampled from each monkey for each of the 1000 resamplings), 40 neurons (20 randomly sampled per monkey), or 60 neurons (30 neurons per monkey) since the sample of 60 neurons yielded close to ceiling performance for the body orientation decoding. For each pose, the body orientation decoding was worse for aSTS than for mSTS, although this difference reached significance only for P1 and for the 40 neurons sample of P2 (p < 0.025; two-tailed test; same procedure as employed for testing the significance of the decoding of whole-body orientation for upright versus inverted avatars (Experiment 3))). Face orientation decoding was significantly worse for aSTS compared to mSTS. These results are in line with the previously reported decreased decoding of face orientation in the anterior compared to mid-STS face patches (Meyers EM, Borzello M, Freiwald WA, Tsao D. Intelligent information loss: the coding of facial identity, head pose, and non-face information in the macaque face patch system. J Neurosci. 2015 May 6;35(18):7069-81), and decreased decoding of body orientation in anterior compared to mid-STS body patches (Kumar S, Popivanov ID, Vogels R. Transformation of Visual Representations Across Ventral Stream Body-selective Patches. Cereb Cortex. 2019 Jan 1;29(1):215-229). As mentioned by the reviewer, this contrasts with the decoding of the head-body orientation angle, which increases when moving more anteriorly. We mention this finding now in the Discussion (page 27) and present the new Figure S10 in the Suppl. Material.    

      (3) While this work has characterized the neural integration of head and body information in detail, it's unclear how the neural representation relates to the animal's perception. Behavioural experiments using the same set of stimuli could help address this question, but I agree that these additional experiments may be beyond the scope of the current paper. I think the authors should at least discuss the potential outcomes of such experiments, which can be tested in future studies.

      Unfortunately, we do not have behavioral data. One prediction would be that the discrimination of head-body orientation angle, irrespective of the viewpoint of the avatar, would be more accurate for zero versus straight angles compared to the right versus left angles. We have added this to the Discussion (page 28).

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) P22 L373. It should read Figure S5C instead of S4C.

      Thanks; corrected.

      (2) Figure 7B. All inverted decoding accuracies, although significantly lower than upright decoding accuracies, appear significantly above baseline. Should the title be amended accordingly?

      Thanks for pointing this out. To avoid future misunderstanding we have changed the title to:

      “Integration of head and body orientations in the macaque superior temporal sulcus is stronger for upright bodies”

      (3) Discussion L432-33. "with some neurons being tuned to a particular orientation of both the head and the body". Wouldn't that be visible as a diagonal profile on the normalized net responses in Fig 3D? Or can the Anova evidence such a tuning?

      We meant to say that some neurons were tuned to a particular combination of head and body orientation, like the third aSTS example neuron shown in Figure 3D. We have corrected the sentence.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major comment:

      This paper effectively demonstrates that the angular relationship between the head and body can be decoded from population responses in the anterior STS. In other words, these neurons encode information about the head-body angle. However, how exactly do these neurons encode this information? Given that the study employed electrophysiological recordings from a local population of neurons, it might be possible to provide additional data on the response patterns of individual neurons to shed light on the underlying encoding mechanisms.

      Although the paper already presents example response patterns (Figures 3D, E) and shows that STS neurons encode interactions between head and body orientations (Figure 3B), it remains unclear whether the angle difference between the head and body has a systematic effect on neuronal responses. For instance, a description of whether some neurons preferentially encode specific head-body angle differences (e.g., a "45-degree angle neuron"), or additional population analyses such as a one-way ANOVA with angle difference as the main effect (or two-way ANOVA with angle difference as one of the main effect), would be very informative. Such data could offer valuable insights into how individual neurons contribute to the encoding of head-body angle differences-a detail that may also be reflected in the decoding results. Alternatively, it is possible that the encoding of head-body angle is inherently complex and only discernible via decoding methods applied to population activity. Either scenario would provide interesting and useful information to the field.

      We have performed two additional analyses which are relevant to this comment. First, we attempted to relate the tuning for body and head orientation with the decoding of the head-body orientation angle. To do this, one needs to identify the neurons that contribute to the decoding of head-body orientation angles. For that, we employed a neuron-dropping analysis, similar to Chiang et al. (Chiang FK, Wallis JD, Rich EL. Cognitive strategies shift information from single neurons to populations in prefrontal cortex. Neuron. 2022 Feb 16;110(4):709-721.) to assess the positive (or negative) contribution of each neuron to the decoding performance. We performed cross-validated linear SVM decoding N times, each time leaving out a different neuron (using N-1 neurons; 2000 resamplings of pseudo-population vectors). We then ranked decoding accuracies from highest to lowest, identifying the ‘worst’ (rank 1) to ‘best’ (rank N) neurons. Next, we conducted N decodings, incrementally increasing the number of included neurons from 1 to N, starting with the worst-ranked neuron (rank 1) and sequentially adding the next (rank 2, rank 3, etc.). This analysis focused on zero versus straight angle decoding in the aSTS, as it yielded the highest accuracy. We applied it when training on MC and testing on HC for each pose. Plotting accuracy as a function of the number of included neurons suggested that less than half contributed positively to decoding (see Figure S3). We examined the tuning for head and body orientation of the 10 “best” neurons (Figure S3). For half or more of those the two-way ANOVA showed a significant interaction. These are indicated by the red color in the Figure. They showed a variety of tuning patterns for head and body orientation, suggesting that the decoding of the head-body orientation angle results from a combination of neurons with different tuning profiles.

      Second, we have followed the suggestion of the reviewer to perform for each neuron of experiment 1 a one-way ANOVA with as factor head-body orientation angle. To do that, we combined all 64 trials that had the same head-body orientation angle. The percentage of neurons (required to be responsive in the tested condition) for which this one-way ANOVA was significant is shown in the Tables below for each region, separately for each pose (P1, P2), centering condition (MC = monkey-centered; HC = head-centered) and monkey subject (M1, M2). The percentages were low but larger than the expected 5% (Type 1 error), with a median of 16.5% (range: 3 to 23%) in aSTS and 8% for mSTS (range: 0-19%).

      Author response table 1.

      Interestingly, a higher percentage of the 10 best neurons for each pose (indicated by the star in the Figure above) showed a significant one-way ANOVA for angle (for P1, MC: 50% (95% confidence interval (CI): 19% – 81%); P1, HC: 70% (CI: 35% - 93%); P2, MC: 70% (CI: 35% – 93%); P2: HC: 50% (CI: 19%-81%)). These percentages were significantly higher than expected for a random sample from the population of neurons for each pose-centering combination (expected percentages listed in the same order as above: 16%, 13%, 16%, and 10%; all outside CI). Thus, for at least half of the “best” neurons, the response differed significantly among the head-orientation angles at the single neuron level. Nonetheless, the tuning profiles were quite diverse, suggesting population coding of head-body orientation angle. We have added this interesting and novel result to the Results (page 16) and Suppl. Material (Figure S3).    

      Minor comments:

      (1) Figure 4A, Fourth Row Example (Zero Angle vs. Straight Angle, Bottom of the P2 Examples): The order of the example stimuli might be incorrect- the 0{degree sign} head with 180{degree sign} body stimulus (leftmost) might be swapped with the 180{degree sign} head with 0{degree sign} body stimulus (5th from the left). While this ordering may be acceptable, please double-check whether it reflects the authors' intended arrangement.

      We have changed the order of the two stimuli in Figure 4A, following the suggestion of the reviewer.

      (2) Page 12, Lines 192-194: The text states, "Interestingly, some neurons (e.g. Figure 3D) were tuned to a particular combination of a head and body irrespective of centering." However, Figure 3D displays data for a total of 10 neurons. Could you please specify which of these neurons are being referred to in this context?

      The wording was not optimal. We meant to say that some neurons were tuned to a particular combination of head and body orientation, like the third aSTS example neuron of Figure 3D. We have rephrased the sentence and clarified which example neuron we referred to.

      (3) Page 28, Lines 470-471: The text states, "We observed no difference in response strength between anatomically possible and impossible configurations." Please clarify which data were compared for response strength, as I could not locate the corresponding analyses.

      The anatomically possible and impossible configurations differ in the head-body orientation angle. However, as we reported before in the Results, there was no effect of head-body orientation angle on mean response strength across poses (Friedman ANOVA; all p-values for both poses and centerings > 0.1). We have clarified this now in the Discussion (page 28).

      (4) Pages 40-43, Decoding Analyses: In experiments 2 and 3, were the decoding analyses performed on simultaneously recorded neurons? If so, such analyses might leverage trial-by-trial correlations and thus avoid confounds from trial-to-trial variability. In contrast, experiment 1, which used single-shank electrodes, would lack this temporal information. Please clarify how trial numbers were assigned to neurons in each experiment and how this assignment may have influenced the decoding performance.

      For the decoding analyses of experiments 2 and 3, we combined data from different daily penetrations, with only units from the same penetration being recorded simultaneously. In the decoding analyses of each experiment, the trials were assigned randomly to the pseudo-population vectors, shuffling on each resampling the trial order per neuron. This shuffling abolishes noise correlations in the analysis of each experiment.

      (5) Page 41, Lines 792-802: The authors state that "To assess the significance of the differences in classification scores between pairs of angles ... we computed the difference in classification score between the two pairs for each resampling and the percentile of 0 difference corresponded to the p-value." In a two-sided test under the null hypothesis of no difference between the distributions, the conventional approach would be to compute the p-value as the proportion of resampled differences that are as extreme or more extreme than the observed difference. Since a zero difference might be relatively rare, relying solely on its percentile could potentially misrepresent the tail probabilities relevant to a two-sided test. Could you clarify how their method addresses this issue?

      This test is based on the computation of the distribution of the difference between classification accuracies across resamplings. This is similar to the computation of the confidence interval of a  difference. Thus, we assess whether the theoretical zero value (= no difference; = null hypothesis) is outside the 2.5 and 97.5 percentile interval of the computed distribution of the empirically observed differences. We clarified now in the Methods (page 41) that for a two-tailed test the computed p-value (the percentile of the zero value) should be smaller than 0.025.

      (6) Page 43, Lines 829-834: The manuscript explains: "The mean of 10 classification accuracies (i.e., of 10 resamplings) was employed to obtain a distribution (n=100) of the differences in classification accuracy ... The reported standard deviations of the classification accuracies are computed using also the means of 10 resamplings." I am unfamiliar with this type of analysis and am unclear about the rationale for calculating distributions and standard deviations based on the means of 10 resamplings rather than using the original distribution of classification accuracies. This resampling procedure appears to yield a narrower distribution and smaller standard deviations than the original data. Could you please justify this approach?

      The logic of the analysis is to reduce the noise in the data, by averaging across 10 randomly selected resamplings, but still keeping a sufficient number of data (100 values) for a test.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Some sentences are too long and difficult to parse. For example, in line 177: "the correlations between the responses to the 64 head-body orientation conditions of the two centerings for the neuron and pose combinations showing significant head-body interactions for the two centerings were similar to those observed for the whole population."

      We have modified this sentence: For neuron and pose combinations with significant head-body interactions in both centerings, the correlations between responses to the 64 head-body orientation conditions were similar to those observed in the whole population.

      (2) The authors argue in line 485: "in our study, a search bias cannot explain the body-inversion effect since we selected responsive units using both upright and inverted images." However, the body-selective patches were localized using upright images, correct?

      The monkey-selective patches were localized using upright images indeed. However, we recorded in experiment 3 (and 2) also outside the localized patches (as we noted before in the Methods:  “In experiments 2 and 3 we recorded from a wider region, which overlapped with the two monkey patches and the recording locations of experiment 1”). Furthermore, the preference for upright monkey images is not an all-or-nothing phenomenon: most units still responded to inverted monkeys. Also, we believe it is likely that the mean responses to the inverted bodies in the monkey patches, defined by upright bodies versus objects, would be larger than those to objects and we would be surprised to learn that there is a patch selective for inverted bodies that we would have missed with our localizer.

      (3) Typo: line 447, "this independent"->"is independent"?

      Corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Kv2 subfamily potassium channels contribute to delayed rectifier currents in virtually all mammalian neurons and are encoded by two distinct types of subunits: Kv2 alpha subunits that have the capacity to form homomeric channels (Kv2.1 and Kv2.2), and KvS or silent subunits (Kv5,6,8.9) that can assemble with Kv2.1 or Kv2.2 to form heteromeric channels with novel biophysical properties. Many neurons express both types of subunits and therefore have the capacity to make both homomeric Kv2 channels and heteromeric Kv2/KvS channels. Determining the contributions of each of these channel types to native potassium currents has been very difficult because the differences in biophysical properties are modest and there are no Kv2/KvS-specific pharmacological tools. The authors set out to design a strategy to separate Kv2 and Kv2/KvS currents in native neurons based on their observation that Kv2/KvS channels have little sensitivity to the Kv2 pore blocker RY785 but are blocked by the Kv2 VSD blocker GxTx. They clearly demonstrate that Kv2/KvS currents can be differentiated from Kv2 currents in native neurons using a two-step strategy to first selectively block Kv2 with RY785, and then block both with GxTx. The manuscript is beautifully written; takes a very complex problem and strategy and breaks it down so both channel experts and the broad neuroscience community can understand it.

      Strengths:

      The compounds the authors use are highly selective and unlikely to have significant confounding cross-reactivity to other channel types. The authors provide strong evidence that all Kv2/KvS channels are resistant to RY785. This is a strength of the strategy - it can likely identify Kv2/KvS channels containing any of the 10 mammalian KvS subunits and thus be used as a general reagent on all types of neurons. The limitation then of course is that it can't differentiate the subtypes, but at this stage, the field really just needs to know how much Kv2/KvS channels contribute to native currents and this strategy provides a sound way to do so.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors are very clear about the limitations of their strategy, the most important of which is that they can't differentiate different subunit combinations of Kv2/KvS heteromers. This study is meant to be a start to understanding the roles of Kv2/KvS channels in vivo. As such, this is a minor weakness, far outweighed by the potential of the strategy to move the field through a roadblock that has existed since its inception.

      The study accomplishes exactly what it set out to do: provide a means to determine the relative contributions of homomeric Kv2 and heteromeric Kv2/KvS channels to native delayed rectifier K+ currents in neurons. It also does a fabulous job laying out the case for why this is important to do.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Silent Kv subunits and the channels containing these Kv subunits (Kv2/KvS heteromers) are in the process of discovery. It is believed that these channels fine-tune the voltage-activated K+ currents that repolarize the membrane potential during action potentials, with a direct effect on cell excitability, mostly by determining action potentials firing frequency.

      Strengths:

      What makes silent Kv subunits even more important is that, by being expressed in specific tissues and cell types, different silent Kv subunits may have the ability to fine-tune the delayed rectifying voltage-activated K+ currents that are one of the currents that crucially determine cell excitability in these cells. The present manuscript introduces a pharmacological method to dissect the voltage-activated K+ currents mediated by Kv2/KvS heteromers as a means of starting to unveil their importance, together with Kv2-only channels, to the cells where they are expressed.

      Weaknesses:

      While the method is effective in quantifying these currents in any isolated cell under an electric voltage clamp, it is ineffective as a modulating maneuver to perhaps address these currents in an in vivo experimental setting. This is an important point but is not a claim made by the authors.

      We agree. We have now stated in the introduction that this study does not address the roles of Kv2/KvS currents in an in vivo setting.

      Manuscript revisions:

      While this study does not address the impact of GxTX or RY785 on action potentials or in vivo, the distinct pharmacology of Kv2/KvS heteromers presented here suggests that KvS conductances could be targeted to selectively modulate discrete subsets of cell types.  

      There are other caveats with the methods and data:

      (i) The need for a 'cocktail' of blockers to supposedly isolate Kv2 homomers and Kv2/KvS heteromers' currents from others may introduce errors in the quantification Kv2/KvS heteromers-mediated K+ currents and that is due to possible blockers off targets.

      We now point out that is possible that off target effects of blockers may introduce errors, include references that identify the selectivity of the blockers used in the cocktail, and specifically note that 4-aminopyridine in the cocktail is expected to block 2% of Kv2 homomers yet have a lesser impact Kv2/KvS heteromers. Additionally, to test whether the KvS isolation strategy requires the cocktail in neurons, we performed new experiments on a different subclass of nociceptors without the blocker cocktail and identified a substantial KvS-like component (new Fig 7 Supplement 3).

      Manuscript revisions:

      “After whole-cell voltage clamp was established, non-Kv2/KvS conductances were suppressed by changing to an external solution containing a cocktail of inhibitors: 100 nM alpha-dendrotoxin (Alomone) to block Kv1 (Harvey and Robertson, 2004), 3 μM AmmTX3 (Alomone) to block Kv4 (Maffie et al., 2013; Pathak et al., 2016), 100 μM 4-aminopyridine to block Kv3 (Coetzee et al., 1999; Gutman et al., 2005), 1 μM TTX to block TTX sensitive Nav channels, and 10 μM A803467 (Tocris) to block Nav1.8 (Jarvis et al., 2007). It is possible that off target effects of blockers may introduce errors in the quantification Kv2/KvS heteromer-mediated K<sup>+</sup> currents. For example, 4-aminopyridine is expected to block a small fraction, 2%, of Kv2 homomers and have a lesser impact on Kv2/KvS heteromers (Post et al., 1996; Thorneloe and Nelson, 2003; Stas et al., 2015) which could result in a slight overestimation of the ratio of Kv2/KvS heteromers to Kv2 homomers.”

      “We also tested the other major mouse C-fiber nociceptor population, peptidergic nociceptors, to determine if this subpopulation also has conductances resistant to RY785 yet sensitive to GxTX. We voltage clamped DRG neurons from a CGRP<sup>GFP</sup> mouse line that expresses GFP in peptidergic nociceptors (Gong et al., 2003). Deep sequencing has identified mRNA transcripts for Kv6.2, Kv6.3, Kv8.1 and Kv9.3 present in GFP+ neurons, an overlapping but distinct set of KvS subunits from the Mrgprd<sup>GFP</sup> non-peptidergic population (Zheng et al., 2019). In GFP+ neurons from CGRP<sup>GFP</sup> mice, we found that a fraction of outward current was inhibited by 1 µM RY785 and additional current inhibited by 100 nM GxTX (Fig 7 Supplement 3 A-C). In these experiments, 58 ± 2% (mean ± SEM) was KvS-like (Fig 7 Supplement 3 D) identifying that KvSlike conductances are present in these peptidergic nociceptors. For CGRP<sup>GFP</sup> neurons we did not include the Kv1, Kv3, Kv4, Nav and Cav channel inhibitor cocktail used for other neuron experiments, indicating that the cocktail of inhibitors is not required to identify KvS-like conductances.”

      (ii) During the electrophysiology experiments, the authors use a holding potential that is not as negative as it is needed for the recording of the full population of the Kv2/KvS channels. Depolarized holding potentials lead to a certain level of inactivation of the channels, that vary according to the KvS involved/present in that specific population of channels. As a reminder, some KvS promote inactivation and others prevent inactivation. Therefore, the data must be interpreted as such.

      We agree. We now point out that the physiological holding potentials used are insufficiently negative to relieve inactivation from all Kv2/KvS heteromeric channels. We also note that the ratio of Kv2-like to KvS-like conductance is expected to vary with voltage protocols.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “Neurons were held at a membrane potential of –74 mV to mimic a physiological resting potential. KvS subunits can profoundly shift the voltage-inactivation relation (Salinas et al., 1997a; Kramer et al., 1998; Kerschensteiner and Stocker, 1999) and this potential is likely insufficiently negative to relieve inactivation from all Kv2/KvS heteromeric channels. Also, the activation membrane potential is close to the half-maximal point of Kv2/KvS conductances. Thus the ratio of Kv2-like to KvS-like conductance is expected to vary with voltage protocols.”

      (iii) The analysis of conductance activation by using tail currents is only accurate when dealing with non-inactivating conductances. Also, in dealing with a heterogenous population of Kv2/KvS heteromers, heterogenous K+ conductance deactivation kinetics is a must. Indeed, different KvS may significantly relate to different deactivation kinetics as well.

      We now discuss that the bi-exponential fit of tail currents is likely inadequate to capture the deactivation kinetics of all underlying components of a heterogenous population of Kv2/KvS heteromers.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “We note that the analysis of conductance activation by using tail currents is only accurate when dealing with non-inactivating conductances. We expect that inactivation of Kv2/KvS conductances during the 200 ms pre-pulse is minimal (Salinas et al., 1997a; Kramer et al., 1998; Kerschensteiner and Stocker, 1999) and did not notice inactivation during the activation pulse. Also, deactivation kinetics can vary in a heterogenous population of Kv2/KvS heteromers. While analysis of tail currents could skew the quantification of total Kv2 like and KvS-like conductances, our data supports that mouse nociceptors and human neurons have tail currents that are resistant to RY785 and sensitive to GxTX consistent with the presence of Kv2/KvS heteromers.”

      (iv) Silent Kv subunits may be retained in the ER, in heterologous systems like CHO cells. This aspect may subestimate their expression in these systems. Nevertheless, the authors show similar data in CHO cells and in primary neurons.

      We agree. We now note that in heterologous systems, including CHO cells, transfection of KvS subunits can result in KvS subunits that are retained intracellularly.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “While a fraction of KvS subunits appear to be retained intracellularly, immunofluorescence for Kv5.1, Kv9.3 and Kv2.1 also appeared localized to the perimeter of transfected Kv2.1-CHO cells (Figure 1 Supplement).”

      (v) The hallmark of silent Kv subunits is their effect on the time inactivation of K+ currents. As such, data should be shown throughout, preferably, from this perspective, but it was only done so in Figure 4G.

      Indeed, effects on inactivation are a hallmark of KvS subunits. However, quantifying inactivation of Kv2/KvS channels requires steps to positive voltages for approximately 10 seconds. In neurons steps this long usually resulted in irreversible changes in leak currents/input resistance that degraded the accuracy of RY785/GxTX subtraction currents. Consequently, we did not acquire inactivation data in neurons, and we now explain in the manuscript why such data was not obtained.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “While changes in inactivation are prominent with KvS subunits, we did not investigate inactivation in neurons because the lengthy depolarizations required often resulted in irreversible leak current increases that degraded the accuracy of RY785/GxTX subtraction current quantification.”

      (vi) Functional characterization of currents only, as suggested by the authors as a bona fide of Kv2 and Kv2/KvS currents, should not be solely trusted to classify the currents and their channel mediators.

      We agree, and now state explicitly that functional characterization cannot be trusted to classify their channel mediators of conductances, and we try to be clear about this throughout the manuscript by using soft terms such as "KvS-like" when identity is uncertain.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “As functional characterization alone cannot be trusted to classify their channel mediators of conductances, we define conductances consistent with Kv2/KvS heteromers as 'KvS-like' and conductances consistent with Kv2 homomers as 'Kv2-like'.”

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      There is not a lot to do here - this was a real pleasure to read and very easy to understand, as written. Here are a few minor things to consider:

      (1) The naming of the KvS subunits has always been confusing - it is not clear that Kv5,6,8,9 are members of the Kv2 subfamily from the names. KvS does a good job of differentiating them by assembly phenotype and has been used a lot in the literature, but it doesn't solve the misconception of what subfamily they belong to. This might not matter so much for mammals, where all KvS channels are in the Kv2 subfamily, but it makes it impossible to extend the naming system to other animals where subunits requiring heteromeric assembly are common in most subfamilies. How about trying the name Kv2S? It would have continuity with KvS in the reader's mind, make it clear that they are Kv2 subfamily, and make a naming system that could be extended beyond vertebrates. This is not a problem the authors created - just a completely optional suggestion on how to solve it if so inclined.

      We agree that naming conventions for these subunits are problematic, and agonized quite a bit about nomenclature. In the end we chose to stick with the precedent of KvS.

      (2) Another naming issue they should definitely change is the use of "subfamily" for the different KvS subtypes (Kv5, Kv6, Kv8, and Kv9). This really creates confusion with the higher-order subfamilies that have a very clear functional definition: a subfamily of Kv genes is a group of related genes that have assembly compatibility. Those are Kv1, Kv2, Kv3 and Kv4. KvS genes are assembly compatible with Kv2, evolutionarily derived from the Kv2 lineage, and thus clearly a part of the Kv2 subfamily. Using a subfamily for the next lower level of the naming hierarchy confuses this. The authors should use different terms like sub-type or class or subgroups for the divisions within KvS.

      Thank you. We have standardized to Kv2/KvS as a subfamily; Kv5, Kv6, Kv8, and Kv9 as subtypes; and individual proteins, e.g. Kv8.1, as subunits.

      (3) When you discuss whether the KvS subunit directly disrupts Ry785 binding in the pore or works allosterically and you said you know which KvS residues point into the pore from models, I thought that maybe you could tell from a sequence alignment whether the KvS channels you didn't test look the same in the conduction pathway as the ones you did test. If so, you could mention that if the binding site is the pore, they should all be resistant. Alternatively, if one you didn't test looks fundamentally more similar to the Kv2s in this region, then maybe it could be fingered as a possible exception that needs to be tested later.

      Great ideas. We now assess sequence KvS variability near the proposed RY785 binding site in all KvS subunits. We generated structural models of RY785 docking to Kv2.1 and Kv2.1/Kv8.1 and found that residues near RY785 are different in all KvS subunits.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “We analyzed computational structural models of RY785 docked to a Kv2.1 homomer and a 3:1 Kv2.1:Kv8.1 heteromer (Fig 9) to gain structural insight into how KvS subunits might interfere with RY785 binding. We used Rosetta to dock RY785 to a cryo-EM structure of a Kv2.1 homomer in an apparently open state (Fernández-Mariño et al., 2023). The top-scoring docking pose has RY785 positioned below the selectivity filter and off-axis of the pore (Fig 9 A), similar to a stable pose observed in molecular dynamic simulations (Zhang et al., 2024). In this pose, RY785 contacts a collection of Kv2.1 residues that vary in every KvS subtype (Fig 9 B,D,E). Notably, RY785 bound similarly to a 3:1 model of Kv2.1/Kv8.1, in contact with the three Kv2.1 subunits, yet avoided the Kv8.1 subunit (Fig 9C). This is consistent with RY785 binding less well to Kv2.1/Kv8.1 heteromers, and also suggests that a 3:1 Kv2:KvS channel could retain a RY785 binding site when open.”

      (4) Future suggestion or tip - not for this paper. Your data shows your isolation strategy works really well on Kv6 channels, and these are also the Kv2/KvS channels that have the most pronounced biophysical changes. Working on neurons that have a prominent Kv2/Kv6 component would really show how well the strategy outlined here works to describe the physiology of native neurons. The highest KvS expression I have seen in public data in a wellstudied cell type is Kv6.4 in spinal motor neurons.

      Wonderful tip, thank you. We are indeed very interested in Kv6.4 in spinal motor neurons.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The manuscript makes a good contribution to the identification of Kv2/KvS channels in primary cells. The pharmacological method proposed by the authors to dissect the currents in an experimental setting seems proper. Although meritorious in themselves, the findings are heavily phenomenological in the opinion of this reviewer. The manuscript should be improved with some level of mechanistic data and/or the demonstration of different levels of expression in different cell types.

      Thank you for the suggestions. This manuscript now demonstrates strikingly higher levels of the KvS-like component of Kv2 currents in somatosensory (DRG nonpeptidergic and peptidergic nociceptor) versus autonomic (SCG) neuron types. The mechanistic question of what electrophysiological properties the KvS subunits are providing to the neuronal circuit is an exciting one that we are pursuing separately.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “While we found only RY785-sensitive Kv2-like conductances in SCG neurons, Kv2/KvS heteromer-like conductances were dominant in DRG neurons.”

      At present, the manuscript says that the combination of RY785 and guangxitoxin-1E can be used to define Kv2/KvS-mediated K+ currents. Importantly, this method cannot be used in a way that one can functionally determine the function of Kv2/KvS channels, since it depends on the pre-blocking of Kv2-mediated K+ currents prior. In the opinion of this reviewer, this fact decreases the attention of a potential reader.

      Indeed, our study is focused on revealing KvS heteromers by voltage clamp, and we now clarify in the introduction that we do not determine the function of Kv2/KvS channels in this study, so as not to lead the reader to expect studies of neuronal signaling.

      However, the selective pharmacology we identify suggests RY785 application could reveal the function of Kv2 homomers, and for RY785-insensitive signaling, GxTX application of could reveal the function of Kv2/KvS heteromers. We now mention these possible applications in the Discussion.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “While this study does not address the impact of GxTX or RY785 on action potentials or in vivo, the distinct pharmacology of Kv2/KvS heteromers presented here suggests that KvS conductances could be targeted to selectively modulate discrete subsets of cell types.”

      Please find below suggestions for improving the manuscript:

      (1) The term "Kv2/KvS heteromers" should be used throughout instead of variations such as "Kv2/KvS channels", "Kv2/KvS" and others. Standardization of the term to refer to heteromers would make the manuscript easier to read.

      Thank you. We have standardized terms to consistently refer to Kv2/KvS heteromers.

      (2) Confusing terms like KvS conductances, KvS-like conductances, KvS-like (RY785-resistant, GxTX-sensitive) currents, and KvS channels should be avoided because they disregard the current belief that KvS cannot form functional homomeric channels. The term KvS-containing channels, and Kv2/KvS channels, seem more accurate. Uniformization in this regard will also make the manuscript more easily readable.

      Thank you. We have standardized terms to Kv2/KvS heteromers and KvS-containing channels when channel subunits are known and the use terms KvS-like and Kv2-like for functionally identified endogenous conductances with unknown channel subunits.

      (3) Referring to KvS as a regulatory subunit is inaccurate. It is clear that KvS is part of, and it makes up the alpha pore. KvS therefore is a part of the conductive pathway and not a regulatory (suggesting accessory) subunit. KvS take part in selectivity filter (fully conserved), but they also make up an important part of the conducting pathway with non-conserved amino acid residues.

      We felt it important to include the descriptor “regulatory” to connect our nomenclature with prior use of the descriptor in the literature, and now only use the term at the start of the introduction.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “A potential source of molecular diversity for Kv2 channels are a group of Kv2-related proteins which have been referred to as regulatory, silent, or KvS subunits.”

      (4) The use of a cocktail of channel inhibitors may affect the quantification of Kv2/KvS heteromers-mediated K+ currents because they may interact with RY785 and/or GxTx or they may even interact with the sites for these two drugs on Kv2-containing channels.

      This is an interesting point worth considering, thank you. We now alert readers to this possibility in the discussion when considering the limitations of our approach.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “Also, the cocktail of inhibitors used in most neuron experiments here could potentially alter RY785 or GxTX action against KvS/Kv2 channels.”

      (5) The graphical representation of fractional blocking and other parameters (e.g., Fig 1D), is hard to read in these slim plots. In my opinion, tall bars would be more meaningfully visualized.

      Thank you for pointing out that the graphs were hard to read, we have made the graph easier to read and added tall bars.

      (6) Vehicle control for IHC and electrophysiology. Please state what is the vehicle used in the electrophysiology experiments.

      Thank you. The composition of vehicle has now been stated in the methods.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “All RY785 solutions contained 0.1% DMSO. Vehicle control solutions also contained 0.1% DMSO but lacked RY785.”

      “Sections were incubated in vehicle solution (4% milk, 0.2% triton diluted in PB) for 1 hr at RT.”

      (7) The reference Trapani & Korn, 2003 (?) is not included in the list. This reference is important since it sets what are the Kv2.1-CHO cells. In this regard it is also important to mention, even better to address, the expressing qualities of this system in the face of a co-expression with a plasmid-based expression of silent Kv subunits. Are these two ways of expressing Kv subunits, meant to come together (or not) in heteromers, balanced? This question is critical here. Still, in regard to Kv2.1-CHO cells, it was not clear in the manuscript if the term "transfection" refers only to the plasmids used to temporarily induce the expression of silent Kv subunits and potentially Kv channels accessory subunits.

      We now include the Trapani & Korn, 2003 reference (thank you for pointing out this accidental omission), and better explain expression methods. The benefit of the inducible Kv2.1 expression is control of Kv conductance densities which can otherwise become so large as to be refractory to voltage clamp. The beauty of the expression system is that cells recently transfected with KvS subunits can be induced to express just enough Kv2.1 to get a substantial but not clampoverwhelming RY785-resistant Kv2/KvS conductance. We also discuss that our expression methods are distinct from past studies. We stop short of comparing the expression systems, as this is beyond the scope of what we set out to study.

      Manuscript revisions: See next response

      (8) Kv2.1-CHO cells transfection procedures, induction, and validation are unclear. This validation is important here.

      We have clarified transfection procedures, induction, and validation in the methods section.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “The CHO-K1 cell line transfected with a tetracycline-inducible rat Kv2.1 construct (Kv2.1-CHO) (Trapani and Korn, 2003) was cultured as described previously (Tilley et al., 2014).”

      Transfections were achieved with Lipofectamine 3000 (Life Technologies, L3000001). 1 μl Lipofectamine was diluted, mixed, and incubated in 25 μl of Opti-MEM (Gibco, 31985062).”

      “Concurrently, 0.5 μg of KvS or AMIGO1 or Navβ2, 0.5 μg of pEGFP, 2 μl of P3000 reagent and 25 μl of Opti-MEM were mixed. DNA and Lipofectamine 3000 mixtures were mixed and incubated at room temperature for 15 min. This transfection cocktail was added to 1 ml of culture media in a 24 well cell culture dish containing Kv2.1-CHO cells and incubated at 37 °C in 5% CO2 for 6 h before the media was replaced. Immediately after media was replaced, Kv2.1 expression was induced in Kv2.1-CHO cells with 1 μg/ml minocycline (Enzo Life Sciences, ALX380-109-M050), prepared in 70% ethanol at 2 mg/ml. Voltage clamp recordings were performed 12-24 hours later. We note that the expression method of Kv2/KvS heteromers used here is distinct from previous studies which show that the KvS:Kv2 mRNA ratio can affect the expression of functional Kv2/KvS heteromers (Salinas et al., 1997b; Pisupati et al., 2018). We validated the functional Kv2/KvS heteromer expression using voltage clamp to establish distinct channel kinetics and the presence of RY785-resistant conductance in KvS-transfected cells and using immunohistochemistry to label apparent surface localization of KvS subunits (Figure 4, Figure 1 Supplement, Figure 1 and Figure 5).”

      (9) It is important for readers to add some context to Kv2.1/Kv8.1 channels (and other Kv2/KvS heteromers) used to test the combination of RY785 and GxTx. In my opinion, this enriches the discussion.

      Good idea. We have added context about each of the KvS subunits we test.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “To test the pharmacological response of KvS we began with Kv8.1, a subunit that creates heteromers with biophysical properties distinct from Kv2 homomers (Salinas et al., 1997a), and modulates motor neuron vulnerability to cell death (Huang et al., 2024).

      Each of these KvS subunits create Kv2/KvS heteromers that have distinct biophysical properties (Kramer et al., 1998; Kerschensteiner and Stocker, 1999; Bocksteins et al., 2012). Kv5.1/Kv2.1 heteromers play an important role in controlling the excitability of mouse urinary bladder smooth muscle (Malysz and Petkov, 2020), mutations in Kv6.4 have been shown to influence human labor pain (Lee et al., 2020b), and deficiency of Kv9.3 disrupts parvalbumin interneuron physiology in mouse prefrontal cortex (Miyamae et al., 2021).”

      (10) In general, the membrane potential used to activate Kv2 only channels and Kv2/KvS channels is too close to the activation V1/2. In case the comparing curves are displaced in their relative voltage dependence and voltage sensitivity, using that range of membrane potential may introduce a crucial error in the estimation of the conductance's relative amplitudes.

      We now note that the relative conductances of Kv2-only vs Kv2/KvS channels are expected to vary with voltage protocol, as KvS inclusion results in channels with altered voltage responses.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “…the activation membrane potential is close to the half-maximal point of Kv2/KvS conductances. Thus the ratio of Kv2-like to KvS-like conductance is expected to vary with voltage protocols.”

      (11) The use of tail currents to estimate conductance is problematic if i) lack of current inactivation is not assured, and ii) if the different currents, with possible different deactivation kinetics at the used membrane potential (e.g., mV), are not assured. Why was the activation peak used at times, and at different elapsed times the tail currents were used instead? These aspects of conductance's amplitude estimation methods should be well defined.

      In CHO cells peak currents were analyzed because outward currents seem to offer the best signal/noise. In neurons, we restricted analysis to tail currents at elapsed times to minimize complications from non-Kv2 endogenous voltage-gated channels which deactivate more quickly. We have clarified this analysis in the methods section.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “In CHO cells peak currents were analyzed because outward currents seem to offer the best signal/noise. In neurons, we restricted analysis to tail currents at elapsed times to minimize complications from non-Kv2 endogenous voltage-gated channels which deactivate more quickly. In neurons, voltage gated currents remained in the toxin cocktail + RY785 and GxTX, that were sometimes unstable. To minimize complications from these currents, we restricted analysis of RY785 and GxTX subtraction experiments to tail currents at elapsed times to minimize complications from non-Kv2 endogenous voltage-gated channels which deactivate more quickly. We note that the analysis of conductance activation by using tail currents is only accurate when dealing with non-inactivating conductances. We expect that inactivation of Kv2/KvS conductances during the 200 ms pre-pulse is minimal (Salinas et al., 1997a; Kramer et al., 1998; Kerschensteiner and Stocker, 1999) and did not notice inactivation during the activation pulse. Also, deactivation kinetics can vary in a heterogenous population of Kv2/KvS heteromers. While analysis of tail currents could skew the quantification of total Kv2 like and KvS-like conductances, our data supports that mouse nociceptors and human neurons have tail currents that are resistant to RY785 and sensitive to GxTX consistent with the presence of Kv2/KvS heteromers.”

      (12) Were the experiments including different conditions such as control, RY, and RY+GxTx done pair-wised? This could potentially better the statistics and strengthen the data and the conclusions drawn from them.

      The control, RY, and RY+GxTX in neurons were done pairwise and the statistical tests performed for these experiments were pairwise tests. We have clarified this in the figure legends.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “Wilcoxon rank tests were paired, except the comparison of RY785 to vehicle which was unpaired.”

      (13) The holding potential of the experiments, mostly -89 mV, may be biasing the estimation of Kv2 only channels vs. Kv2/KvS channels conductances. Figure 4I exemplifies this concern.

      We agree. Figure 4I reveals that a holding potential of -89 mV vs -129 mV reduces conductance of Kv2.1/Kv8.1 heteromers vs Kv2.1 homomers in CHO cells by ~20%. We have now alerted readers that the ratio of Kv2 only channels vs. Kv2/KvS conductances can vary with holding voltage.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “Under these conditions, 58 ± 3 % (mean ± SEM) of the delayed rectifier conductance was resistant to RY785 yet sensitive to GxTX (KvS-like) (Fig 7 F). We note that the ratio of KvS- to Kv2-like conductances is expected to vary with holding potential, as KvS subunits can change the degree and voltage-dependence of steady state inactivation (e.g. Fig 4I).”

      (14) It is possible that Figure 6A (control trace) and Figure 6C ("Kv2-like" trace) are the same, by mistake, since their noise pattern looks too similar.

      Indeed the noise pattern of the Figure 6A (control trace) and Figure 6C ("Kv2-like" trace) are related, as they have inputs from the same trace, with Figure 6C ("Kv2-like" trace) being a subtraction of Figure 6A (+RY trace) from Figure 6A (control trace).

      (15) For example, in Figure 7A, what is the identity of the current remaining after the RY+GxTx application? In Figure 7B, a supposed outlier in the group of data referring to "veh" in the right panel is what possibly is making this group different from +RY in the left panel (p=0.02, Wilcoxon rank test). I would recommend parametric tests only since the data is essentially quantitative.

      In Figure 7A, we do not know the identity of the current remaining after the RY+GxTX application, the kinetics of the residual current appeared distinct from the Kv2/KvS-like currents blocked by RY or GxTX, but we did not analyze these.

      The date in Figure 7B, was indeed the positive outlier in the group of data referring to "veh" in the right panel and contributes to the p-value, but we saw no reason to exclude it. We have now replaced the representative trace in 7B with a non-outlier trace. We respectfully disagree with the suggestion to use parametric statistical tests as we do not know the distribution underlying the variance our data.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “Subsequent application of 100 nM GxTX decreased tail currents by 68 ± 5% (mean ± SEM) of their original amplitude before RY785. We do not know the identity of the outward current that remains in the cocktail of inhibitors + RY785 + GxTX.”

      (16) Please state the importance of using nonpeptidergic neurons to study silent Kv5.1 and Kv9.1 subunits. RNA data may not necessarily work to probe function or protein abundance, which is crucial in heteromeric complexes.

      We have now more thoroughly explained our rationale for choosing the nonpeptidergic neurons.

      RNA is not predictive of protein abundance, and we have not yet been successful in measuring KvS protein abundance in these neurons, so we've probed KvS abundance by assessing RY785 resistance.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “Mouse dorsal root ganglion (DRG) somatosensory neurons express Kv2 proteins (Stewart et al., 2024), have GxTX-sensitive conductances (Zheng et al., 2019), and express a variety of KvS transcripts (Bocksteins et al., 2009; Zheng et al., 2019), yet transcript abundance does not necessarily correlate with functional protein abundance. To record from a consistent subpopulation of mouse somatosensory neurons which has been shown to contain GxTXsensitive currents and have abundant expression of KvS mRNA transcripts (Zheng et al., 2019), we used a Mrgprd<sup>GFP</sup> transgenic mouse line which expresses GFP in nonpeptidergic nociceptors (Zylka et al., 2005; Zheng et al., 2019). Deep sequencing identified that mRNA transcripts for Kv5.1, Kv6.2, Kv6.3, and Kv9.1 are present in GFP+ neurons of this mouse line (Zheng et al., 2019) and we confirmed the presence of Kv5.1 and Kv9.1 transcripts in GFP+ neurons from Mrgprd<sup>GFP</sup> mice using RNAscope (Fig 7 Supplement 1).”

      (17) In Figure 8B, were +RY data different from veh data? The figure shows no Wilcoxon (nonparametric) comparison and this is important to be stated. What conductance(s) is the vehicle solution blocking or promoting? What is RY dissolved in, DMSO? What is the DMSO final concentration?

      We now state that in Figure 8B, +RY amplitudes were not statistically different from veh data in this limited data set. However, the RY-subtraction currents always had Kv2-like biophysical properties, whereas vehicle-subtraction currents had variable properties precluding biophysical analysis for Fig 8D.

      In Figure 8B, we do not know what conductance(s) the vehicle solution is affecting, we think the changes observed are likely merely time dependent or due to the solution exchange itself. RY stock is in DMSO. All recording solutions have 0.1% DMSO final concentration, this is now noted in methods.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “Unlike mouse neurons, we did not detect a significant difference in tail currents of RY785 versus vehicle controls. However, RY785-subtracted currents always had Kv2-like biophysical properties whereas vehicle-subtraction currents had variable properties that precluded the same biophysical analysis. Overall, these results show that human DRG neurons can produce endogenous voltage-gated currents with pharmacology and gating consistent with Kv2/KvS heteromeric channels.”

      “All RY785 solutions contained 0.1% DMSO. Vehicle control solutions also contained 0.1% DMSO but lacked RY785.”

      (18) METHODS. The electrophysiology approach should be unified in all aspects as applicable and possible.

      We have unified the mouse dorsal root ganglion and mouse superior cervical ganglion methods sections. We have kept CHO cells and mouse/human neurons section separate because the methods were substantially different.

      (19) DISCUSSION. The discussion section spends half of its space trying to elaborate on possible blocking/inhibiting/modulating mechanisms for RY785. The present manuscript shows no data, at least not that I have noticed, that would evoke such discussion.

      We have shortened this section, and enhance the discussion with structural models (new Fig 9), and our functional data indicating perturbed RY785 interaction with Kv2.1/8.1.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “In this pose, RY785 contacts a collection of Kv2.1 residues that vary in every KvS subtype (Fig 9 B,D,E). Notably, RY785 bound similarly to a 3:1 model of Kv2.1/Kv8.1, in contact with the three Kv2.1 subunits, yet avoided the Kv8.1 subunit (Fig 9C). This is consistent with RY785 binding less well to Kv2.1/Kv8.1 heteromers, and also suggests that a 3:1 Kv2:KvS channel could retain a RY785 binding site when open. However, the RY785 resistance of Kv2/KvS heteromers may primarily arise from perturbed interactions with the constricted central cavity of closed channels. In homomeric Kv2.1, RY785 becomes trapped in closed channels and prevents their voltage sensors from fully activating, indicating that RY785 must interact differently with closed channels (Marquis and Sack, 2022). Here we found that Kv2.1/Kv8.1 current rapidly recovers following washout of RY785, suggesting that Kv2.1/Kv8.1 heteromers do not readily trap RY785 (Figure 2 Supplement). Overall, the structural modeling suggests that KvS subunits sterically interfere with RY785 binding to the central cavity, while functional data suggest KvS subunits disrupt RY785 trapping in closed states.”

      (20) DISCUSSION. Topics like ER retention and release upon certain conditions would be a better enrichment for the manuscript in my opinion.

      ER retention of KvS subunits is indeed an important topic! However, we have opted not to delve into it here.

      (21) DISCUSSION. Speculation about the binding site for RY on Kv2/KvS channels is also not touched by the data shown in the manuscript.

      We have shortened this section of discussion, and now present this with structural models of RY785 docked to a Kv2.1 homomer and 3:1 Kv2.1: Kv8.1 heteromer (new Fig 9) to ground speculations. See manuscript changes noted in response to comment (19) above.

      (22) DISCUSSION. An important reference is missing in regard to stoichiometry: Bocksteins et al., 2017. This work is the only one using a non-optical technique to add knowledge to that question.

      Good point, and an excellent study we didn’t realize we’d not included before. We now include Bocksteins et al. 2017 as a reference in the Introduction.

      (23) In my opinion, allosterism and orthosterism are concepts not yet useful for the discussion of RY binding sites without even a general piece of data.

      We now include structural models of RY785 docked to a Kv2.1 homomer and 3:1 Kv2.1: Kv8.1 heteromer (new Fig 9) to ground blocking speculations. See manuscript changes noted in response to comment (19).

      (24) The term "homogeneously susceptible" associated with a Hill slope close to 1 needs to be more elaborated.

      Thank you, we have elaborated.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “Also, the degree of resistance to RY785 may vary if Kv2:KvS subunit stoichiometry varies. With high doses of RY785, we found that the concentration-response characteristics of Kv2.1/Kv8.1 in CHO cells revealed hallmarks of a homogenous channel population with a Hill slope close to 1 (Fig 2B). However, other KvS subunits might assemble in multiple stoichiometries and result in pharmacologically-distinct heteromer populations.”

      (25) Stating the KvS are resistant to RY785 is not proper in my opinion. This opinion relates to the fact that the RY binding site in the channels is certainly not restricted to a binding site residing only on the Kv subunit.

      Good point. We have now changed phrasing to convey that KvS subunits are a component of a heteromer that imbues RY785 resistance.

      Manuscript revisions:

      “These results show that voltage-gated outward currents in cells transfected with members from each KvS subtype have decreased sensitivity to RY785 but remain sensitive to GxTX. While we did not test every KvS subunit, the ubiquitous resistance suggests that all KvS subunits may provide resistance to 1 μM RY785 yet remain sensitive to GxTX, and that RY785 resistance is a hallmark of KvS-containing channels.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigate the role of the melanocortin system in puberty onset. They conclude that POMC neurons within the arcuate nucleus of the hypothalamus provide important but differing input to kisspeptin neurons in the arcuate or rostral hypothalamus.

      Strengths:

      Innovative and novel

      Technically sound

      Well-designed

      Thorough

      Weaknesses:

      There were no major weaknesses identified.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This interesting manuscript describes a study investigating the role of MC4R signalling on kisspeptin neurons. The initial question is a good one. Infertility associated with MC4 mutations in humans has typically been ascribed to the consequent obesity and impaired metabolic regulation. Whether there is a direct role for MC4 in regulating the HPG axis has not been thoroughly examined. Here, the researchers have assembled an elegant combination of targetted loss of function and gain of function in vivo experiments, specifically targetting MC4 expression in kisspeptin neurons. This excellent experimental design should provide compelling evidence for whether melanocortin signalling dirently affects arcuate kisspeptin neurons to support normal reproductive function. There were definite effects on reproductive function (irregular estrous cycle, reduced magnitude of LH surge induced by exogenous estradiol). However, the magnitude of these responses and the overall effect on fertility were relatively minor. The mice lacking MC4R in kisspeptin neurons remained fertile despite these irregularities. The second part of the manuscript describes a series of electrophysiological studies evaluating the pharmacological effects of melanocortin signalling in kisspeptin cells in ex-vivo brain slides. These studies characterised interesting differential actions of melanocortins in two different populations of kisspeptin neurons. Collectively, the study provides some novel insights into how direct actions of melanocortin signalling via the MC4 receptor in kisspeptin neurons contribute to the metabolic regulation of the reproductive system. Importantly, however, it is clear that other mechanisms are also at play.

      Strengths:

      The loss of function/gain of function experiments provides a conceptually simple but hugely informative experimental design. This is the key strength of the current paper - especially the knock-in study that showed improved reproductive function even in the presence of ongoing obesity. This is a very convincing result that documents that reproductive deficits in MC4R knockout animals (and humans with deleterious MC4R gene variants) can be ascribed to impaired signalling in the hypothalamic kisspeptin neurons and not necessarily caused as a consequence of obesity. As concluded by the authors: "reproductive impairments observed in MC4R deficient mice, which replicate many of the conditions described in humans, are largely mediated by the direct action of melanocortins via MC4R on Kiss1 neurons and not to their obese phenotype." This is important, as it might change how such fertility problems are treated.

      I would like to see the validation experiments for the genetic manipulation studies given greater prominence in the manuscript because they are critical to interpretation. Presently, only single unquantified images are shown, and a much more comprehensive analysis should be provided.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Given that mice lacking MC4R in kisspeptin neurons remained fertile despite some reproductive irregularities, this can be described as a contributing pathway, but other mechanisms must also be involved in conveying metabolic information to the reproductive system. This is now appropriately covered in the discussion.

      (2) The mechanistic studies evaluating melanocortin signalling in kisspeptin neurons were all completed in ovariectomised animals (with and without exogenous hormones) that do not experience cyclical hormone changes. Such cyclical changes are fundamental to how these neurons function in vivo and may dynamically alter how they respond to hormones and neuropeptides. Eliminating this variable makes interpretation difficult, but the authors have justified this as a reductionist approach to evaluate estradiol actions specifically. However, this does not reflect the actual complexity of reproductive function.

      For example, the authors focus on a reduced LH response to exogenous estradiol in ovariectomised mice as evidence that there might be a sub-optimal preovulatory LH surge. However, the preovulatory LH sure (in intact animals) was not measured.

      They have not assessed why some follicles ovulated, but most did not. They have focused on the possibility that the ovulation signal (LH surge) was insufficient rather than asking why some follicles responded and others did not. This suggests some issue with follicular development, likely due to changes in gonadotropin secretion during the cycle and not simply due to an insufficient LH surge.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Talbi R et al. generated transgenic mice to assess the reproduction function of MC4R in Kiss1 neurons in vivo and used electrophysiology to test how MC4R activation regulated Kiss1 neuronal firing in ARH and AVPV/PeN. This timely study is highly significant in neuroendocrinology research for the following reasons.

      (1) The authors' findings are significant in the field of reproductive research. Despite the known presence of MC4R signaling in Kiss1 neurons, the exact mechanisms of how MC4R signaling regulates different Kiss1 neuronal populations in the context of sex hormone fluctuations are not entirely understood. The authors reported that knocking out Mc4r from Kiss1 neurons replicates the reproductive impairment of MC4RKO mice, and Mc4r expression in Kiss1 neurons in the MC4R null background partially restored the reproductive impairment. MC4R activation excites Kiss1 ARH neurons and inhibits Kiss1 AVPV/PeN neurons (except for elevated estradiol).

      (2) Reproduction dysfunction is one of obesity comorbidities. MC4R loss-of-function mutations cause obesity phenotype and impaired reproduction. However, it is hard to determine the causality. The authors carefully measured the body weight of the different mouse models (Figure 1C, Figure 2A, Figure 3B). For example, the Kiss1-MC4RKO females showed no body weight difference at puberty onset. This clearly demonstrated the direct function of MC4R signaling in reproduction but was not a consequence of excessive adiposity.

      (3) Gene expression findings in the "KNDy" system align with the reproduction phenotype.

      (4) The electrophysiology results reported in this manuscript are innovative and provide more details of MC4R activation and Kiss1 neuronal activation.

      Overall, the authors have presented sufficient background in a clear, logical, and organized structure, clearly stated the key question to be addressed, used the appropriate methodology, produced significant and innovative main findings, and made a justified conclusion.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have addressed my comments.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      The reviewers noted that they received comments in response to their concerns, and some improvements have been made to the manuscript. However, as described below, in some cases, a rebuttal was provided, but changes were not made to the manuscript. It is suggested that these issues be addressed to improve the quality of the manuscript.

      We thank the reviewers and editor for the assessment of the manuscript and recommendations for its improvement. We have addressed the remaining comments from reviewer #2 below, and hope that they find our revisions satisfactory.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The manuscript convincingly shows that MC4R in kisspeptin-producing cells can influence reproductive function. This suggests that fertility problems associated with melanocortin mutations are likely due to direct effects on the reproductive systems rather than simply being side effects of the resultant obesity.

      We are pleased that this reviewer finds the data convincing and thank them for the careful review of the manuscript, which has helped to improve its published version.

      The authors have responded to the reviewer's comments and made several improvements to the manuscript.

      The authors are correct in pointing out that the POMC-Cre animals should be fine for studies involving the administration of AAVs to adult animals. I have misinterpreted how these mice were being used, and this concern is fully addressed.

      Unfortunately, in some cases, the authors rebutted the reviewer's comments but did not change the manuscript. I suggest addressing several issues in the manuscript (after all, it is not the reviewer's opinion that counts; this process is about improving the manuscript).

      (1) Validation of the KO is insufficiently reported. From the methods, it appears that this was done thoroughly, but currently, only a single image of the arcuate nucleus is shown, and no image of the AVPV is shown. There is no quantitative information provided. The authors can keep these data as supplementary material, but they should be comprehensive and convincing, as so much depends on the degree of knockout in this model. One cannot assume complete KO based simply on the relevant genetics, as there are examples in this system where different Cre lines produce different outcomes with various floxed genes in the two major populations of kisspeptin neurons. This figure should show the quantitation of the RNAscope analysis from each of the two regions regarding the percentage of kisspeptin cells showing expression of MC4R mRNA. In addition, the lack of MC4 labelling in the arcuate nucleus, outside of kisspeptin neurons, is a concern. One would expect to see AgRP or POMC cells at this level, but are they still showing expression of MC4? A single image is insufficient to be convinced of the model's efficacy.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concerns regarding the validation of the MC4RKO model. Below, we provide clarification and additional justification for our approach.

      (1) Quantification of MC4R in the Arcuate Nucleus (ARC): As noted by the reviewer, we were unable to detect sufficient MC4R signal in the ARC of KO mice to perform meaningful quantification. This is consistent with the expected outcome of a successful MC4R deletion. Given the low endogenous expression levels of MC4R in this region, even in control animals, and the technical limitations of RNAscope in detecting very low-abundance transcripts, especially for receptors, the absence of MC4R signal in the ARC of KO mice strongly supports effective deletion. Moreover, the MC4R loxP mouse has been published and validated by many labs including Brad Lowell’s lab who’s done extensive work using these mice for selective deletion of Mc4r from various neuronal populations such as Sim1 and Vglut2 neurons (Shah et al., 2014, de Souza Cordeiro et al., 2020). To further strengthen our validation, we provide additional images from another animal (Fig_S1) to illustrate the consistency of the MC4R KO in the ARC. These will be included as supplementary material, as suggested.Regarding AgRP and POMC neurons, MC4R is not highly expressed in these neurons (as per previous literature, e.g., Garfield et al., Nat Neurosci. 2015; Padilla SL et al, Endocrinology 2012; Henry et al, Nature, 2015). Instead, MC4R is predominantly found in downstream neurons in the paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and other hypothalamic regions (which is intact in our KO mice as shown in our validation figure). Thus, the absence of MC4R labeling in AgRP or POMC cells in our images aligns with known expression patterns and does not contradict the validity of our model.

      (2) MC4R Expression in the AVPV and OVX Effect on Kiss1 Expression: We acknowledge the reviewer’s request for MC4R expression analysis in the anteroventral periventricular nucleus (AVPV). However, due to the timing of tissue collection after ovariectomy (OVX), Kiss1 expression in the AVPV is significantly suppressed, making it technically unfeasible to perform co-staining of MC4R with Kiss1 in this region. This is a well-documented effect of estrogen depletion following OVX (Smith et al., 2005; Lehman et al., 2010). While we acknowledge that an ideal validation would include AVPV co-labeling, the experimental constraints related to OVX preclude this analysis in our dataset.

      Given these considerations and validations, we are confident that the KO is effective and specific.

      (2) Line 88: "... however, conflicting reports exist". Expand on this sentence to describe what these conflicting reports show. The authors responded to my comment but made no changes to the introduction. As a reader, I dislike being told there are conflicting reports, but then I have to go and look up the reference to see what that actual point of conflict is.

      By conflicting reports we meant that other studies have shown no association between MC4R and reproductive disorders, this has now been included in the revised manuscript (Line 89).

      (3) Could the authors explain how a decrease in AgRP would be interpreted as a "decrease in hypothalamic melanocortin tone" in line 142 and line 364? These overly simplistic interpretations of qPCR data detract from the overall quality of the paper.

      The reference to a decrease in melanocortin tone referred to the decrease in the expression of melanocortin receptor signaling, this has been clarified in the revised manuscript (lines 142 and 360).

      (4) Please show the individual cycle patterns for all animals, as in Figure 2B. This can be a supplemental figure, but the current bar charts are not informative.

      We respectfully disagree that the bar charts are not informative as they include the critical statistical analysis. We have now included all individual estrous cycle data in new separate supplemental figure (Sup. Figure 3). Therefore, we have excluded the representative cycles from the main figures as they are now in the new Supplemental. We have changed the orders of the figures in the text accordingly.

      (5) In their rebuttal, the authors state: "Mice lack true follicular and luteal phases, and therefore, it is impossible to separate estrogen-mediated changes from progesterone-mediated changes (e.g., in a proestrous female). Therefore, we use an ovariectomized female model in which we can generate an LH surge with an E2-replacement regimen [1]. This model enables us to focus on estrogen effects, exclude progesterone effects, and minimize variability. Inclusion of cycling females would make interpretation much more difficult." I disagree, but the authors can take this position if they wish. However, they should not report the responses to exogenous estradiol in an ovariectomised mouse as a "preovulatory LH surge" (line 380). An ovariectomised mouse cannot ovulate, and the estrogen-induced LH surge is significantly different in magnitude and timing from the endogenous preovulatory LH surge (likely due to the actions of progesterone). One goal of these studies is to understand why the ovulation rate appears to be low in the MC4-KO animals. Hence, evaluating whether the preovulatory LH surge is typical is important. This has not been done. The authors have shown that the response to exogenous estradiol is sub-normal. Such an effect might lead to a reduced preovulatory LH surge, but this has not been measured.

      We appreciate this reviewer’s concern about the nature of the preovulatory LH surge. We have clarified this in the revised manuscript and described it as “an induced LH surge” throughout the text (Lines 163, 533, 6560).

      (6) I believe that the ovulation process should be considered "all or none," and I do not quite understand the rebuttal discussion. The authors describe that "numerous follicles mature at the same time....". That is not disputed. My point was that each mature follicle will receive the identical endocrine ovulatory signal (correct? Or do the authors believe something different?). If it were sufficient for one follicle to ovulate, then all of those mature follicles (the number of which will be variable between animals and between cycles) would be expected to undergo ovulation. The fact that they do not raise several possibilities. One that the authors favor is that an insufficient ovulatory signal might approach a threshold where some follicles ovulate and others do not. This possibility is supported by the apparent increase in cystic follicles, which might be preovulatory follicles that did not complete the ovulation process. Such variation might be stochastic, within normal variation for sensitivity to LH. However, it is also possible that the follicles have not matured at the same rate, perhaps influenced by abnormal secretion of LH or FSH during earlier phases of the cycle, and hence are not in the appropriate condition to respond to the ovulation signal when it arrives. Some may even have matured prematurely due to the elevated gonadotropins reported in this study. Given the data and the partial fertility, the most likely explanation is that the genetic manipulation has resulted in fewer follicles being available for ovulation due to changes in follicular development rather than a deficit of the ovulation signal, although the latter mechanism might also contribute. A third possibility is that genetic manipulation has directly affected the ovary. The authors did not answer whether Kiss1 and MC4 are co-expressed in the ovary. I think the authors might want to rule this out by showing no change in MC4R expression in the ovary.

      We thank the reviewer for this thoughtful comment and agree that these are possible outcomes. We have now acknowledged them in the Discussion.

      To answer the reviewer’s question, we have not investigated the co-expression of Kiss1 and Mc4r in the ovary. While MC4R has indeed been documented in the ovary (Chen et al. Reproduction, 2017), the changes in gonadotropin release and supporting in vitro data included in this manuscript clearly document a central effect, however, an additional effect at the level of the ovary cannot be completely ruled out. This has now been added to the discussion (Line 378-387).

      (7) Lines 390, 454 " impaired LH pulse" What was the evidence for impaired LH pulse (see figure 2D)?

      Thank you for pointing this out. This comment referred to augmented LH release. This has been corrected in the revised manuscript (Line 394).

      The paper's strengths remain, as outlined in my original review. The authors have addressed what I perceived to be weaknesses, predominantly by changing the tone of discussion and interpretation of the data. This is appropriate. I consider the focus on the LH surge as the primary mechanism too narrow, and the authors should be considering how other changes during the cycle might influence ovarian function.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful evaluation of our manuscript and their constructive feedback. We are pleased that our revisions have addressed the perceived weaknesses and that the adjustments to the discussion and interpretation were deemed appropriate.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s perspective on broadening the discussion beyond the LH surge to consider additional cycle-dependent influences on ovarian function. While our current study focuses on this specific mechanism, we recognize that ovarian function is influenced by multiple physiological changes throughout the cycle. We have refined our discussion to reflect this broader context and appreciate the suggestion to consider these additional factors in future studies.

      We have addressed all of the reviewer’s comments to the best of our ability and hope they find the revised manuscript satisfactory.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors track the motion of multiple consortia of Multicellular Magnetotactic Bacteria moving through an artificial network of pores and report a discovery of a simple strategy for such consortia to move fast through the network: an optimum drift speed is attained for consortia that swim a distance comparable to the pore size in the time it takes to align the with an external magnetic field. The authors rationalize their observations using dimensional analysis and numerical simulations. Finally, they argue that the proposed strategy could generalize to other species by demonstrating the positive correlation between the swimming speed and alignment time based on parameters derived from literature.

      Strengths:

      The underlying dimensional analysis and model convincingly rationalize the experimental observation of an optimal drift velocity: the optimum balances the competition between the trapping in pores at large magnetic fields and random pore exploration for weak magnetic fields.

      Weaknesses:

      The convex pore geometry studied here creates convex traps for cells, which I expect enhances their trapping. The more natural concave geometries, resulting from random packing of spheres, would create no such traps. In this case, whether a non-monotonic dependence of the drift velocity on the Scattering number would persist is unclear.

      We agree that convex walls increase the time that consortia remain trapped in pores at high magnetic fields. Since the non-monotonic behavior of the drift velocity with the Scattering number arises largely due to these long trapping times, we agree that experiments using concave pores are likely to show a peak drift velocity that is diminished or erased.

      However, we disagree that a random packing of spheres or similar particles provides an appropriate model for natural sediment, which is not composed exclusively of hard particles in a pure fluid. Pore geometry is also influenced by clogging. Biofilms growing within a network of convex pillars in two-dimensional microfluidic devices have been observed to connect neighboring pillars, thereby forming convex pores. Similar pore structures appear in simulations of biofilm growth between spherical particles in three dimensions. Moreover, the salt marsh sediment in which MMB live is more complex than simple sand grains, as cohesive organic particles are abundant. Experiments in microfluidic channels show that cohesive particles clog narrow passageways and form pores similar to those analyzed here. Thus, we expect convex pores to be present and even common in natural sediment where clogging plays a role.

      The concentration of convex pores in the experiments presented here is almost certainly much higher than in nature. Nonetheless, since magnetotactic bacteria continuously swim through the pore space, they are likely to regularly encounter such convexities. Efficient navigation of the pore space thus requires that magnetotactic bacteria be able to escape these traps. In the original version of this manuscript, this reasoning was reduced to only one or two sentences. That was a mistake, and we thank the reviewer for prompting us to expand on this point. As the reviewer notes, this reasoning is central to the analysis and should have been featured more prominently. In the final version, we will devote considerable space to this hypothesis and provide references to support the claims made above.

      The reviewer suggests that the generality of this work depends on our finding a ”positive correlation between the swimming speed and alignment [rate] based on parameters derived from literature.” We wish to emphasize that, in addition to predicting this correlation, our theory also predicts the function that describes it. The black line in Figure 3 is not fitted to the parameters found in the literature review; it is a pure prediction.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The authors have made microfluidic arrays of pores and obstacles with a complex shape and studied the swimming of multicellular magnetotactic bacteria through this system. They provide a comprehensive discussion of the relevant parameters of this system and identify one dimensionless parameter, which they call the scattering number and which depends on the swimming speed and magnetic moment of the bacteria as well as the magnetic field and the size of the pores, as the most relevant. They measure the effective speed through the array of pores and obstacles as a function of that parameter, both in their microfluidic experiments and in simulations, and find an optimal scattering number, which they estimate to reflect the parameters of the studied multicellular bacteria in their natural environment. They finally use this knowledge to compare different species to test the generality of this idea.

      Strengths:

      This is a beautiful experimental approach and the observation of an optimal scattering number (likely reflecting an optimal magnetic moment) is very convincing. The results here improve on similar previous work in two respects: On the one hand, the tracking of bacteria does not have the limitations of previous work, and on the other hand, the effective motility is quantified. Both features are enabled by choices of the experimental system: the use the multicellular bacteria which are larger than the usual single-celled magnetotactic bacteria and the design of the obstacle array which allows the quantification of transition rates due to the regular organization as well as the controlled release of bacteria into this array through a clever mechanism.

      Weaknesses:

      Some of the reported results are not as new as the authors suggest, specifically trapping by obstacles and the detrimental effect of a strong magnetic field have been reported before as has the hypothesis that the magnetic moment may be optimized for swimming in a sediment environment where there is a competition of directed swimming and trapping. Other than that, some of the key experimental choices on which the strength of the approach is based also come at a price and impose some limitations, namely the use of a non-culturable organism and the regular, somewhat unrealistic artificial obstacle array.

      In the “Recommendations for the Authors,” this reviewer drew our attention to a manuscript that absolutely should have been prominently cited. As the reviewer notes, our manuscript meaningfully expands upon this work. We are pleased to learn that the phenomena discussed here are more general than we initially understood. It was an oversight not to have found this paper earlier. The final version will better contextualize our work and give due credit to the authors. We sincerely appreciate the reviewer for bringing this work to our attention.

      We disagree that the use of non-culturable organisms and our unrealistic array should be considered serious weaknesses. While any methodological choice comes with trade-offs, we believe these choices best advance our aims. First, the goal of our research, both within and beyond this manuscript, is to understand the phenotypes of magnetotactic bacteria in nature. While using pure cultures enables many useful techniques, phenotypic traits may drift as strains undergo domestication. We therefore prioritize studying environmental enrichments.

      Clearly, an array of obstacles does not fully represent natural heterogeneity. However, using regular pore shapes allows us to average over enough consortium-wall collisions to enable a parameter-free comparison between theory and experiment. Conducting an analysis like this with randomly arranged obstacles would require averaging over an ensemble of random environments, which is practically challenging given the experimental constraints. Since we find good agreement between theory and experiment in simple geometries, we are now in a position to justify extending our theory to more realistic geometries. Additionally, we note that a microfluidic device composed of a random arrangement of obstacles would also be a poor representation of environmental heterogeneity, as pore shape and network topology differ between two and three dimensions.

      Recommendations for the Authors: 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      My main suggestion is for the authors to describe the limitations of their approach in the case of concave pores.

      As we noted in our public comments, this was a very useful comment to hear from you and one that has been repeated as we have spoken about these results to colleagues. Convexities here represent an experimentally simple way to force bacteria to back track through the maze, as they must through natural sediment. We have greatly expanded this discussion to clarify this reasoning (lines 84–105). We provide references to three types of physical processes that may lead to such traps. First, as in figure 1 of Kurz et al, biofilm (white) can fill the spaces between convex pillars to create covexities. Additionally, clogging by cohesive particles can make narrow passageways between convex particles impassible. An example of clogging is shown in figure 6 of Dressaire & Sauret 2017. Finally, air bubbles trapped in the sediment can create pore-scale dead ends that require bacteria to backtrack. The full references are provided in the main text.

      Small points:

      (1) How many trajectories were used to produce Figures 2 b and c?

      We have modified the caption to note that these data represent the measured transition rates of a total 938 consortia at various Scattering numbers. Each consortium may pass between pores many times.

      (2) Can the authors describe in more detail how Equation (3) is derived? Why doesn’t it depend on the gap size between the pores?

      We have provided a derivation of this equation in Appendix 2 of the new version. This derivation shows that the drift velocity U<sub>drift</sub> is proportional to the pore diameter and difference between the transition rates.

      The proportionality constant α depends on how the pores are connected together in space. In the original version, we wanted to highlight the role of the asymmetry of the transition rates, so we imagined a one dimensional network of pores without gaps. In this case, α \= 1. This reasoning was poorly explained in the previous version and we thank the reviewer for pointing this deficiency out. In the new version, we include the gap size and use the layout of pores in a square lattice with gaps, which is shown in figure 1. The proportionality constant for a square lattice in the absence of gaps√ would be 1/2. The limitations of photolithography require some gap that increase the proportionality constant to α \= 0.8344.

      We have updated the text, equation (3), and the figures to account for the finite gap sizes.

      (3) I found the second part of the abstract, related to the comparison between diverse bacteria, to be slightly misleading. Upon first reading, my expectation was that the authors carried out experiments with different species.

      We have modified the abstract to make clear that we rely on values taken from a literature review.

      (4) More information is needed on how many trajectories were used to produce the probability densities in Figures 1b-d. How were the densities computed?

      The probability distributions give the probability that a pixel in a pore is covered by a consortium. They reflect between 1.2 and 7 million measurements (depending on the panel) of the instantaneous positions of consortia. We have added a section (Lines 453–469) to Materials and Methods that describes exactly how these distributions were calculated.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) As mentioned under Weaknesses in the Public review, some results are less new than claimed here. The existence of an optimal magnetic moment has been shown by Codutti et al eLife eLife13:RP98001 in very similar experiments, where it was also proposed that this may be an evolutionary adaptation to the sediment habitat. The paper here provides additional evidence for this, and with better tracking and quantification, but previous work should be discussed. Likewise, the work by Dekharghani et al. that is mentioned rather suddenly in the Results section appears to be a crucial previous state of the art and could already be mentioned in the introduction.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing this paper, which came out as we were writing this manuscript, to our attention. The hypothesis that there is an optimal phenotype that balances magnetotaxis with obstacle avoidance—and that natural selection could guide organisms to this optimum—goes back to at least 2022. It seems that Codutti et al independently came up with this same hypothesis and provided the first test.

      We have substantively rewritten the introduction (Lines 46–58) to better contextualize our work and give due attention to Dekharghani et al.

      (2) The first paragraph of Results also contains background information and could be moved into the introduction.

      As part of the rewrite to better contextualize our work, we moved the first two paragraphs of results to the introduction.

      (3) I found Figure 1 a bit confusing and it took me some time to understand the geometry. I think the black obstacles are very dominant to the viewer’s eye and draw attention away from the essentially circular shape of the pores. Likewise, I am not sure that cutting the neighboring pores off in a circular fashion in Figures 1b-d was the best choice. The authors should think about whether the presentation can be improved. Likewise, when describing the direction of the field in the text, I would suggest adding that it is along the horizontal direction in Figure 1.

      We have modified the figure and the text as the reviewer suggests.

      (4) That collisions with a pore wall are an important mechanism of changing direction is clear and it is nice to see the paper demonstrate that this mechanism is dominant over rotational diffusion. However, this may not be universal, as (i) rotational diffusion is more important for smaller cells and (ii) interaction with walls can result in all kinds of different behaviors than complete randomization (e.g. swimming along the walls as shown in microfluidic chambers, Ostapenko et al. Phys Rev Lett 2018, Codutti et al. eLife 2022, or reversals, Kuhn et al PNAS 2017). Here, it appears that complete randomization of the direction is an assumption, but this could be tested/quantified by analyzing the trajectories.

      This is an excellent point. We have modified the text to describe qualitatively how these tendencies would shift the Critical Scattering number. We also note in the text that there is evidence of these differences in Fig 3. The Desulfobacterota are shifted upwards in Fig 3 relative to the α-proteobacteria. This shift indicates that Desulfobacterota tend to live at slightly greater scattering numbers of 0.9±0.3 than the α-proteobacteria, which live at scattering number 0.37 ± 0.03. It is likely that this difference reflects taxonomic differences in rotational diffusion and cell-wall interactions.

      It is true that total randomization of the direction is indeed an assumption, and it is stated as such in line 189. We performed all of the numerics to find the solid curves in Fig 2 before we got any experimental data and so, at the time, total randomization seemed like a fair choice. Looking at Fig 2b, it is clear that these numerics systematically overestimate k<sub>−</sub>. We believe that this error is do to the assumption of total randomization.

      As this effect is small and does not change any of the conclusions of the paper and Codutti et al were able to publish their paper in the time that we were writing ours, we feel some urgency to move forward.

      (5) From the manuscript it is not fully clear to what extent experiments and simulations are or can be quantitatively compared. For example: is the curve (“fit”) in Figure 2c based on the simulations? Is there an explicit expression or is this just a spline or something like that? Why does Figure 5 (simulation) show the velocity as a function of Sc<sup>−1</sup>and Figure 2 (experiment) as a function of Sc? It looks to me as if a quantitative comparison could be achieved.

      The original version of Figure 2 shows a quantitative comparison between theory and experiment with no fit parameters. The data points are the result of experiments in which consortia are tracked as they as they move between connected pores. The solid line is a found by interpolating a smooth curve through the data from simulations. As we make clear in the new version (Lines 537–551), this blue curve is the most probable smooth curve that explains the simulations.

      We have added the simulations to figure 2 so that a single panel includes the data, the simulations, and the smooth curve. To further make clear that this comparison is quantitative and parameter free, we have added a panel to Figure 2. This panel directly compares the prediction to observation and is independent of the blue curve.

      As was noted (deep within the methods section) in the original version, our numerics can exactly simulate Sc = ∞. Consequently, it was reasonable to simulate parameters that are uniformly spaced in Sc<sup>−1</sup>.

      (6) While I like the idea behind Figure 3, the data shown here is not as convincing as suggested. If one looks at the data without the black line, I think one gets a weaker dependence. The correlation between U<sub>0</sub> and γ<sub>geo</sub> is likely not as strong as it seems. Calculating a correlation coefficient might be helpful here. In any case, the assumptions going into this figure should be discussed more explicitly and the results should in my opinion be phrased more cautiously (I tend to believe what the authors claim, but I don’t think the evidence for this point is very strong).

      We appreciate the reviewer’s skepticism. However, we believe that the data are stronger than one might understand from the previous text. We have rewritten the text (Lines 219–291) and included new analysis, figures, and explanation to make three points clear.

      (a) It is surprising that speed, magnetic moment, and mobility all vary tremendously(between one and three orders of magnitude) across taxa and environment, however, their dimensionless combination Sc is narrowly distributed. We have added a panel to Fig. 3 to show the measured Scattering numbers.

      It is notable that there are no adjusted parameters in the calculation of the Scattering numbers: it is a simple dimensionless combination of phenotypic and environmental parameters. All but one of these parameters (the pore size) is measured either by us or by other authors. The pore radius is likely narrowly distributed. We measure it at our field site and, when it is not reported, we use a value typical of the geological and fluvial environment. Just as the size of sand grains does not vary greatly between the beaches of Australia, Africa, and California, it is a good assumption that the pore spaces that host these magnetotactic bacteria do not vary tremendously in size.

      (b) In the new version we compare the Scattering number statistics to a parameterfree null model of phenotypic diversity. We argue in the text that it is appropriate to bootstrap over the phenotypic diversity of species. This null model provides the correct method to calculate p-values as the variability in the Scattering numbers is neither identically distributed nor normally distributed.

      We use this null model to show that—given the measured phenotypic diversity across species—the probability that fifteen random species would fall within the measured range of Scattering numbers that is consistent with optimal navigation is ∼ 10<sup>−6</sup>. This result is strong evidence that the phenotypic variables exhibit the correlations that are predicted by our analysis.

      (c) The correlation between U<sub>0</sub>/r and γ<sub>geo</sub> is reasonably strong. I think that our choice of axes in Fig 3, which were chosen to fit the legend, make the data look flatter than then they actually are. Here are the same data plotted without the line with tighter axes:

      Author response image 1.

      With the exception of the very first point and the very last point, the data appear to our eyes to be pretty correlated. This impression is born out by a calculation of the correlation coefficient which gives 0.77. The p-value is 4 × 10<sup>−4</sup>. We have included these values in the main text to clarify that this correlation is both statistically significant and of primary importance.

      (7) There is a comment at the end of the discussion that the evolutionary hypothesis could be tested by transferring the magnetotaxis genes to nonmagnetotactic organisms. This would indeed be highly desirable, but this is very difficult as indicated by the successful efforts in that direction (which often are only moderately magnetic/magnetotactic), see Kolinko et al Nature Nanotech 2014, Dziuba et al Nature Nanotech 2024.

      Thank you for highlighting these references, which we have included. We agree that these experiments will be challenging. Our results make a prediction about the evolution of these strains, so it seems worth mentioning this fact. We feel that this manuscript is not the correct space for a detailed description of challenges that we will encounter should we pursue this direction of study.

      (8) A section on how the bacterial samples were obtained could be added in Methods.

      We have done so.

      Additional Changes

      (1) In the original version, we feared that the consortia in the microfluidic device arepoorly representative of the natural population. Consequently, we used the values from previous experiments, which we performed using consortia taken from the same pond. Since submitting this manuscript we have undertaken new experiments that allowed us to measure the Scattering number of individual consortia. It turns out the effect is smaller than we worried. We have included these measurements in the new version. We find that even as the most common phenotypes vary over the course of time, the Scattering number remains constant. This result is additional evidence that there is strong selective pressure to optimally navigate.

      As a result of these additions, we have added an author, Julia Hernandez, who contributed to these experiments and analysis.

      (2) We have expanded the table of phenotypic variable in Appendix 1 to make it easier forother researchers to reproduce our analysis.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript reports the investigation of PriC activity during DNA replication initiation in Escherichia coli. It is reported that PriC is necessary for the growth and control of DNA replication initiation under diverse conditions where helicase loading is perturbed at the chromosome origin oriC. A model is proposed where PriC loads helicase onto ssDNA at the open complex formed by DnaA at oriC. Reconstituted helicase loading assays in vitro support the model. The manuscript is well-written and has a logical narrative.

      Thank you for understanding this study.

      Major Questions/Comments:

      An important observation here is that a ΔpriC mutant alone displays under-replication, suggesting that this helicase loading pathway is physiologically relevant. Has this PriC phenotype been reported previously? If not, would it be possible to confirm this result using an independent experimental approach (e.g. marker frequency analysis or fluorescent reporter-operator systems)?

      We thank Reviewer 1 for this comment. This study provides the first direct evidence for PriC’s role in initiation of chromosome replication. Given the change of the oriC copy number of ∆priC cells in non-stressed conditions is only slight, resolution of the suggested methods is clearly not high enough to distinguish the differences in the oriC copy number between priC<sup>+</sup> (WT) and ∆priC cells. Thus, to corroborate the ∆priC phenotype, we additionally analyzed using flow cytometry priC<sup>+</sup> and ∆priC cells growing under various nutrition and thermal conditions.

      As shown in Figure 2-figure supplement 1 of the revised version, the fraction of cells with non-2<sup>n</sup> oriC copies was slightly higher in ∆priC cells compared to priC<sup>+</sup> cells. Furthermore, when grown in M9 minimal medium at 37˚C, ∆priC mutant cells exhibited slightly reduced ori/mass values. These are supportive to the idea that inhibition of replication initiation occurs at low frequency even in the WT dnaA and dnaC background, and that PriC function is necessary to ensure normal replication initiation. Related descriptions have been revised accordingly.

      Is PriA necessary for the observed PriC activity at oriC? Is there evidence that PriC functions independently of PriA in vivo?

      As described in Introduction of the original manuscript, PriA is a 3’-to-5’ helicase which specifically binds to the forked DNA with the 3’-end of the nascent DNA strand. Thus, structural specificity of target DNA is essentially different between PriA and PriC. Consistent with this, our in vitro data indicate that PriC alone is sufficient to rescue the abortive helicase loading at oriC (Figure 7), indicating that PriA is principally unnecessary for PriC activity at oriC. Consistently, as described in Introduction, PriC can interact with ssDNA to reload DnaB (Figure 1E). Nevertheless, a possibility that PriA might participate in the PriC-dependent DnaB loading rescue at oriC in vivo can not be completely excluded. However, elucidation of this possibility is clearly beyond the scope of the present study and should be analyzed in the future. An additional explanation has been included in Discussion of the revised version.

      Is PriC helicase loading activity in vivo at the origin direct (the genetic analysis leaves other possibilities tenable)? Could PriC enrichment at oriC be detected using chromatin immunoprecipitation?

      These are advanced questions about genomic dynamics of PriC. Given that PriC facilitates DnaB reloading at stalled replication forks (Figure 1E) (Heller and Marians, Mol Cell., 2005; Wessel et al., J Biol Chem, 2013; Wessel et al., J Biol Chem, 2016), PriC might interact with the whole genome and its localization might not necessarily exhibit a preference for oriC in growing cells. Analysis about these advanced questions is interesting but is beyond the scope of the present study and should be analyzed in the future study.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This is a great paper. Yoshida et al. convincingly show that DnaA does not exclusively do loading of the replicative helicase at the E. coli oriC, but that PriC can also perform this function. Importantly, PriC seems to contribute to helicase loading even in wt cells albeit to a much lesser degree than DnaA. On the other hand, PriC takes a larger role in helicase loading during aberrant initiation, i.e. when the origin sequence is truncated or when the properties of initiation proteins are suboptimal. Here highlighted by mutations in dnaA or dnaC.

      This is a major finding because it clearly demonstrates that the two roles of DnaA in the initiation process can be separated into initially forming an open complex at the DUE region by binding/nucleation onto DnaA-boxes and second by loading of the helicase. Whereas these two functions are normally assumed to be coupled, the present data clearly show that they can be separated and that PriC can perform at least part of the helicase loading provided that an area of duplex opening is formed by DnaA. This puts into question the interpretation of a large body of previous work on mutagenesis of oriC and dnaA to find a minimal oriC/DnaA complex in many bacteria. In other words, mutants in which oriC is truncated/mutated may support the initiation of replication and cell viability only in the presence of PriC. Such mutants are capable of generating single-strand openings but may fail to load the helicase in the absence of PriC. Similarly, dnaA mutants may generate an aberrant complex on oriC that trigger strand opening but are incapable of loading DnaB unless PriC is present.

      We would like to thank Revierwer#2 for the very positive comments about our work.

      In the present work, the sequence of experiments presented is logical and the manuscript is clearly written and easy to follow. The very last part regarding PriC in cSDR replication does not add much to the story and may be omitted.

      Given that the role PriC in stimulating cSDR was unclear, we believe that our finding that PriC has little or no role in cSDR, despite being a negative result, is valuable for the general readership of eLife. To further assess impact of PriC on cSDR and as recommended by Referee #1, we carried out the chromosome loci copy-number analysis by the whole-genome sequencing. As shown in Figure 8-supplement 1 of the revised version, the results support our conclusion from the original version.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      At the abandoned replication fork, loading of DnaB helicase requires assistance from PriABC, repA, and other protein partners, but it does not require replication initiator protein, DnaA. In contrast, nucleotide-dependent DnaA binding at the specific functional elements is fundamental for helicase loading, leading to the DUE region's opening. However, the authors questioned in this study that in case of impeding replication at the bacterial chromosomal origins, oriC, a strategy similar to an abandoned replication fork for loading DnaB via bypassing the DnaA interaction step could be functional. The study by Yoshida et al. suggests that PriC could promote DnaB helicase loading on the chromosomal oriC ssDNA without interacting with the DnaA protein. However, the conclusions drawn from the primarily qualitative data presented in the study could be slightly overwhelming and need supportive evidence.

      Thank you for your understanding and careful comments.

      Strengths:

      Understanding the mechanism of how DNA replication restarts via reloading the replisomes onto abandoned DNA replication forks is crucial. Notably, this knowledge becomes crucial to understanding how bacterial cells maintain DNA replication from a stalled replication fork when challenging or non-permissive conditions prevail. This critical study combines experiments to address a fundamental question of how DnaB helicase loading could occur when replication initiation impedes at the chromosomal origin, leading to replication restart.

      Thank you for your understanding.

      Weaknesses:

      The term colony formation used for a spotting assay could be misleading for apparent reasons. Both assess cell viability and growth; while colony formation is quantitative, spotting is qualitative. Particularly in this study, where differences appear minor but draw significant conclusions, the colony formation assays representing growth versus moderate or severe inhibition are a more precise measure of viability.

      We used serial dilutions of the cell culture for the spotting assay and thus this assay should be referred as semi-quantitative rather than simply qualitative. For more quantitative assessment of viability, we analyzed the growth rates of cells and the chromosome replication activity using flow cytometry.

      Figure 2

      The reduced number of two oriC copies per cell in the dnaA46priC-deficient strain was considered moderate inhibition. When combined with the data suggested by the dnaAC2priC-deficient strain containing two origins in cells with or without PriC (indicating no inhibition)-the conclusion was drawn that PriC rescue blocked replication via assisting DnaC-dependent DnaB loading step at oriC ssDNA.

      The results provided by Saifi B, Ferat JL. PLoS One. 2012;7(3):e33613 suggests the idea that in an asynchronous DnaA46 ts culture, the rate by which dividing cells start accumulating arrested replication forks might differ (indicated by the two subpopulations, one with single oriC and the other with two oriC). DnaA46 protein has significantly reduced ATP binding at 42C, and growing the strain at 42C for 40-80 minutes before releasing them at 30 C for 5 minutes has the probability that the two subpopulations may have differences in the active ATP-DnaA. The above could be why only 50% of cells contain two oriC. Releasing cells for more time before adding rifampicin and cephalexin could increase the number of cells with two oriCs. In contrast, DnaC2 cells have inactive helicase loader at 42 C but intact DnaA-ATP population (WT-DnaA at 42 or 30 C should not differ in ATP-binding). Once released at 30 C, the reduced but active DnaC population could assist in loading DnaB to DnaA, engaged in normal replication initiation, and thus should appear with two oriC in a PriC-independent manner.

      This is a question about dnaA46 Δ_priC_ mutant cells. Inhibition of the replication forks causes inhibition of RIDA (the DNA-clamp complex-dependent DnaA-ATP hydrolysis) system, resulting in the increase of ATP-DnaA molecules (Kurokawa et al. (1999) EMBO J.). Thus, if Δ_priC_ inhibits the replication forks significantly, the ATP-DnaA level should increase and initiation should be stimulated. However, the results of Figure 2BC are opposite, indicating inhibition of initiation by Δ_priC_. Thus, we infer that the inhibition of initiation in the Δ_priC_ cells is not related to possible changes in the ATP-DnaA level. Even if the ATP-DnaA levels are different in subpopulations in dnaA46 cells, Δ_priC_ mutation should not affect the ATP-DnaA levels significantly. Thus, we infer that even in dnaA46 Δ_priC_ mutant cells, Δ_priC_ mutation directly affect initiation mechanisms, rather than indirectly through the ATP-DnaA levels.

      Broadly, the evidence provided by the authors may support the primary hypothesis. Still, it could call for an alternative hypothesis: PriC involvement in stabilizing the DnaA-DnaB complex (this possibility could exist here). To prove that the conclusions made from the set of experiments in Figures 2 and 3, which laid the foundations for supporting the primary hypothesis, require insights using on/off rates of DnaB loading onto DnaA and the stability of the complexes in the presence or absence of PriC, I have a few other reasons to consider the latter arguments.

      This is a very careful consideration. However, we infer that stabilization of the DnaA-DnaB interaction by PriC, even if present, does not always result in stimulation of DnaB loading to oriC. Given that interactions between DnaA and DnaB during DnaB loading to oriC are highly dynamic and complicated with multiple steps, stabilization of the DnaA-DnaB interaction by PriC, even if it occurs, has a considerable risk of inhibiting the DnaB loading by constructing abortive complexes. In addition, DnaA-DiaA binding is very tight and stable (Keyamura et al., 2007, 2009). Even if WT DnaA and WT DnaB are present, PriC can rescue the initiation defects of oriC mutants. Based on these facts and the known characteristics of PriC as explained in Introduction, it is more reasonable to infer that PriC provides a bypass of DnaB loading even at oriC, as proposed for the mechanism at the stalled replication fork. However, we cannot completely rule out the indicated possibility and these explanations are included in the revised version.

      Figure 3

      One should consider the fact that dnA46 is present in these cells. Overexpressing pdnaAFH could produce mixed multimers containing subunits of DnaA46 (reduced ATP binding) and DnaAFH (reduced DnaB binding). Both have intact DnaA-DnaA oligomerization ability. The cooperativity between the two functions by a subpopulation of two DnaA variants may compensate for the individual deficiencies, making a population of an active protein, which in the presence of PriC could lead to the promotion of the stable DnaA: DnaBC complexes, able to initiate replication. In the light of results presented in Hayashi et al. and J Biol Chem. 2020 Aug 7;295(32):11131-11143, where mutant DnaBL160A identified was shown to be impaired in DnaA binding but contained an active helicase function and still inhibited for growth; how one could explain the hypothesis presented in this manuscript. If PriC-assisted helicase loading could bypass DnaA interaction, then how growth inhibition in a strain carrying DnaBL160A should be described. However, seeing the results in light of the alternative possibility that PriC assists in stabilizing the DnaA: DnaBC complex is more compatible with the previously published data.

      Unfortunately, in this comment, there is a crucial misunderstanding in the growth of cells bearing DnaA L160A. Hayashi et al. reported that the dnaB(Ts) cells bearing the dnaB L160A allele grew slowly and formed colonies even at 42°C. This feature is similar to the growth of dnaA46 cells bearing dnaA F46A H136A allele (Figure 2). Thus, the results of dnaB L160A cells are consistent with our model and support the idea that PriC partially rescues the growth inhibition of cells bearing the DnaB L160A allele by bypassing the strict requirement for the DnaA-DnaB interaction. Nevertheless, we have to be careful about a possibility that DnaB L160A could affect interaction with PriC, which we are going to investigate for a future paper.

      As suggested, if mixed complexes of DnaA46 and DnaA F46A H136A proteins are formed, those might retain partial activities in oriC unwinding and DnaB interaction although those cells are inviable at 42°C without PriC. It is noteworthy that in the specific oriC mutants which are impaired in DnaB loading (e.g., Left-oriC), PriC effectively rescues the initiation and cell growth. In these cells, both DnaA and DnaB are intact. Thus, the idea that only mutant DnaA (or DnaB) protein is simulated specifically via PriC interaction is invalid. Even in cells bearing wild-type oriC, DnaA and DnaB, contribution of PriC for initiation is detected.

      In addition, as described in the above response, given that interactions between DnaA and DnaB during DnaB loading to oriC are very dynamic and complicated with multiple steps, stabilization of the DnaA-DnaB interaction by PriC, even if present, would not simply result in stimulation of DnaB loading to oriC; rather we think a probability that it would inhibit the DnaB loading by constructing abortive complexes. Based on the known characteristics of PriC as explained in Introduction, it is more reasonable to infer that PriC provides a bypass of DnaB loading even at oriC, as proposed for the mechanism at the stalled replication fork.

      However, we cannot completely rule out the indicated possibility and this explanation has been described in the revised version as noted in response to the above question.

      Figure 4

      Overexpression of DiaA could contribute to removing a higher number of DnaA populations. This could be more aggravated in the absence of PriC (DiaA could titrate out more DnaA)-the complex formed between DnaA: DnaBC is not stable, therefore reduced DUE opening and replication initiation leading to growth inhibition (Fig. 4A ∆priC-pNA135). Figure 7C: Again, in the absence of PriC, the reduced stability of DnaA: DnaBC complex leaves more DnaA to titrate out by DiaA, and thus less Form I*. However, adding PriC stabilizes the DnaA: DnaBC hetero-complexes, with reduced DnaA titration by DiaA, producing additional Form I*. Adding a panel with DnaBL160A that does not interact with DnaA but contains helicase activity could be helpful. Would the inclusion of PriC increase the ability of mutant helicase to produce additional Form I*?

      Unfortunately, the proposed idea is biased disregarding the fact that DiaA effectively stimulates assembling processes of DnaA molecules at oriC. As oriC contains multiple DnaA boxes and multiple DnaA molecules are recruited there, DiaA will efficiently facilitate assembling of DnaA molecules on oriC. Even DnaA molecules of DnaA-DiaA complexes can efficiently bind to oriC. This is consistent with in vitro experiments showing that higher levels of DiaA stimulate assembly of DnaA molecules and oriC unwinding (i.e., DUE opening) but even excessive levels of DiaA do not inhibit those reactions (Keyamura et al., J. Biol. Chem. (2009) 284, 25038-25050). However, as shown in Figure 9, DiaA tightly binds to the specific site of DnaA which is the same as the DnaB L160-binding site, which causes inhibition of DnaA-DnaB binding (ibid). These are consistent with in vivo experiments, and concordantly consistent with the idea that the excessive DiaA level inhibits interaction and loading of DnaB by the DnaA-oriC complexes, but not oriC unwinding (i.e., DUE opening) in vivo. Also, as mentioned above, we do not consider that stabilization of DnaA-DnaBC complex simply results in stimulation of DnaB loading to oriC. Based on the known characteristics of PriC, it is more reasonable to infer that PriC provides a bypass of DnaB loading even at oriC, as proposed for the mechanism at the stalled replication fork (Figure 1E), as described in the above response.

      As for DnaB L160A, as mentioned above, we are currently investigating interaction modes between DnaB and PriC. While investigating DnaB L160A could further support our model, we believe its contribution to the present manuscript would be incremental. In addition, there is a possibility that DnaA L160A could affect interaction with PriC. Thus, analysis of DnaB mutants in this PriC rescue mechanisms should be addressed in future study.

      Figure 5

      The interpretation is that colony formation of the Left-oriC ∆priC double mutant was markedly compromised at 37˚C (Figure 5B), and 256 the growth defects of the Left-oriC mutant at 25{degree sign}C and 30{degree sign}C were aggravated. However, prima facia, the relative differences in the growth of cells containing and lacking PriC are similar. Quantitative colony-forming data is required to claim these results. Otherwise, it is slightly confusing.

      The indicated concern was raised due to our typing error lacking ∆priC. In the revised manuscript, we have amended as follows: the cell growth of the Left-oriCpriC double mutant was markedly compromised at 37˚C and moderately reduced at 25°C and 30°C (Figure 5B).

      A minor suggestion is to include cells expressing PriC using plasmid DNA to show that adding PriC should reverse the growth defect of dnaA46 and dnaC2 strains at non-permissive temperatures. The same should be added at other appropriate places.

      Even in the presence of PriC, unwinding of oriC and DnaB helicase loading to the wound oriC require DnaA and DnaC activities as indicated by previous studies (see for a review, Windgassen et al., (2018) Nucleic Acids Res. 46, 504-519). Thus, dnaA46 cells and dnaC2 cells bearing pBR322-priC can not grow at 42°C and 37°C (as follows). These are reasonable results. However, at semi-permissive temperatures (37°C for dnaA46 and 35°C for dnaC2), slight stimulation of the cell growth by pBR322-priC might be barely observed (Figure 2-supplement 1 of the revised version). These suggest that the intrinsic level of PriC is functionally nearly sufficient. This explanation has been included in the revised version.

      Author response image 1.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Line 38. "in assembly of the replisome".

      Corrected.

      Line 137. "specifically" rather than specificity.

      Corrected.

      Line 139. "at" rather than by.

      Corrected.

      The DnaA46 protein variant contains two amino acid substitutions (A184V and H252Y) within the AAA+ motif. H136 appears to reside adjacent to A184 in structure. Is A184V mutation causative?

      The DnaA H136A and A184V alleles are responsible for different defects. Indeed, the DnaA A184V variant is thermolabile and defective in ATP binding whereas the H136A variant retains ATP binding but impairs DnaB loading (Carr and Kaguni, Mol. Microbiol., 1996; Sakiyama et al., Front. Microbiol., 2018). These observations strongly support the view that the phenotype of the DnaA H136A allele is independent of that of the DnaA A184V allele.

      Figure 2A. Regarding the dnaA46 allele grown at 37°C.

      Individual colonies cannot be resolved. Is an image from a later time-point available?

      We have replaced the original image with one from another replicate that provides better resolution. Please see Figure 2A in the revised version.

      Figure 2C. Quantification of the number of cells with more than one chromosome equivalent in the dnaC2 ΔpriC strain. The plot from flow cytometry appears to show >20% of cells with only 1 genome. Are these numbers correct?

      Thank you for this careful comment. We quantified the peaks more strictly, but the percentages were noy largely changed. To improve resolution of the DNA profiles, we have changed the range of the x-axis in panels B and C of Figure 2 in the revised version.

      Figure 3. Are both F46A and H136A mutations in the plasmid-encoded dnaA necessary?

      Yes. The related explanation is included in the Discussion section (the third paragraph) of the original manuscript. As described there, dnaA46 cells expressing the DnaA H136A single mutant exhibited severe defects in cell growth even in the presence of PriC (Sakiyama et al., 2018). The His136 residue is located within the weak, secondary DnaB interaction region in DnaA, and is crucial for DnaB loading onto oriC ssDNA. Given domain I in DnaA H136A can stably tether DnaB-DnaC complexes to DnaA complexes on oriC (Sakiyama et al., 2018), we infer that oriC-DnaA complexes including DnaA H136A stably bind DnaB via DnaA domain I as an abortive complex, which inhibits functional interaction between PriC and DnaB as well as DnaB loading to oriC DNA.

      As for DnaA F46A mutant, our previous studies show that DnaA F46A has a limited residual activity in vivo (unlike in vitro), and allows slow growth of cells. As the stable DnaA-DnaB binding is partially impaired in vivo in DnaA F46A, this feature is consistent with the above ideas. Thus, both F46A and H136A mutations are required for severer inhibition of DnaB loading. This is additionally described in the revised Discussion.

      Figure 3. Is the DnaA variant carrying F46A and H136A substitutions stably expressed in vivo?

      We have performed western blotting, demonstrating that the DnaA variant carrying F46A and H136A substitutions is stable in vivo. In the revised version, we have added new data to Figure 3-figure supplement 1 and relevant description to the main text as follows:

      Western blotting demonstrated that the expression levels were comparable between WT DnaA and DnaA F46A H136A double mutant (Figure 3-figure supplement 1).

      Figure 5A. Should the dashed line extending down from I2 reach the R4Tma construct?

      We have amended the indicated line appropriately.

      Figure 6C. It was surprising that the strain combining the subATL mutant with ΔpriC displayed a pronounced under-initiation profile by flow cytometry, and yet there was no growth defect observed (see Figure 6B). This seems to contrast with results using the R4Tma origin, where the ΔpriC mutant produced a relatively modest change to the flow cytometry profile, and yet growth was perturbed (Figure 5C-D). How might these observations be interpreted? Is the absolute frequency of DNA replication initiation critical?

      Please note that, in E. coli, initiation activity corelates closely with the numbers of oriC copies per cell mass (ori/mass), rather than the apparent DNA profiles measured by flow cytometer. When cells were grown in LB at 30˚C, the mean ori/mass values were as follows: 0.34 for R4Tma priC, 0.51 for R4Tma, 0.82 for DATL priC, 0.99 for DATL (Figures 5 & 6 in the original manuscript). These values closely correspond to the cell growth ability shown in Figure 5C in the original manuscript.

      In the revised manuscript, we have cited appropriate references for introduction of the ori/mass values as follows.

      To estimate the number of oriC copies per unit cell mass (ori/mass) as a proxy for initiation activity (Sakiyama et al., 2017, 2022),

      Line 295. Reference for Form I* assay should cite the original publication.

      Done. The following paper is additionally cited.

      Baker, T. A., Sekimizu, K., Funnell, B. E., and Kornberg, A. (1986). Extensive unwinding of the plasmid template during staged enzymatic initiation of DNA replication from the origin of the Escherichia coli chromosome. Cell 45, 53–64.doi: 10.1016/0092-8674(86)90537-4

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The partial complementation of the dnaC2 strain by PriC seems quite straightforward since this particular mutation leads to initiation arrest at the open complex stage and this sets the stage for PriC to load the helicase. The situation is somewhat different for dnaA46. Why is this mutation partly complemented by PriC at 37C? DnaA46 binds neither ATP nor ADP, yet it functions in initiation at permissive temperature. At nonpermissive temperature, it binds oriC as well but does not lead to initiation. Does the present data imply that the true initiation defect of DnaA46 lies in helicase loading? The authors need to comment on this in the text.

      Given the thermolabile propensity of the DnaA46 protein, it is presumable that DnaA46 protein becomes partially denatured at the sub-permissive temperature of 37˚C. This partial denaturation should impair both origin unwinding and helicase loading, though not to the extent that cell viability is lost. The priC deletion should further exacerbate helicase loading defects by inhibiting the bypass mechanism, resulting in the lethality of dnaA46 cells at this temperature. This explanation is included in the revised Discussion section.

      Relating to the above. In Figure 3 it is shown that the pFH plasmid partly complements dnaA46 in a PriC-dependent manner. Again, it would be nice to know the nature of the DnaA46 protein defect. It would be interesting to see how a pING1-dnaA46 plasmid performs in the experiment presented in Figure 3.

      A previous paper showed that multicopy supply of DnaA46 can suppress temperature sensitivity of the dnaA46 cells (Rao and Kuzminov, G3, 2022). This is reasonable in that DnaA46 has a rapid degradation rate unlike wild-type DnaA. As DnaA46 preserves the intact sequences in DnaB binding sites such as G21, F46 and H136, the suppression would not depend on PriC but would be due to the dosage effect.

      Figure 8 B: The authors should either remove the data or show a genome coverage: it is not clear that yapB is a good reference. A genome coverage would be nice, and show whether initiation can occur at oriC even if it is not the major place of initiation in a rnhA mutant.

      As suggested, we carried out the chromosome loci copy-number analysis by whole-genome sequencing to assess impact of PriC on cSDR. The new data are shown in Figure 8-supplement 1 with relevant descriptions of the main text of the revised version as shown below. Briefly, results of the chromosome loci copy-number analysis are consistent with those of real-time qPCR (Figure 8B). Given that the role PriC in stimulating cSDR was unclear, we believe that our finding that PriC has little or no role in cSDR, despite being a negative result, is valuable for the general readership of eLife.

      Line 38-39: .....resulting in replisome assembly.

      Corrected.

      Line 48: Something is wrong with the Michel reference. Also in the reference list.

      Corrected

      Line 156: replace retarded with reduced.

      Corrected.

      Line 171 and elsewhere: WT priC cells is somewhat misleading. Isn't this simply PriC+ cells?

      Yes. We have revised the wording to “priC<sup>+</sup>” for clarity.

      Line 349-350: "the oriC copy number ratio of the dnaA46 DpriC double mutant was lower than that of the dnaA46 single mutant....". This is only provided growth rate of the strains is the same.

      These strains exhibited similar growth rates. This is included in the Result section of the revised manuscript as follows: At the permissive temperature, despite having similar growth rates, the oriC copy number ratio of the dnaA46priC double mutant strain was lower than that of the dnaA46 single mutant.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I would suggest improved or additional experiments, data, or analyses.

      The revised version includes improved or additional experiments, data, or analyses.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      ANALYTICAL

      (1) A key claim made here is that the same relationship (including the same parameter) describes data from pigeons by Gibbon and Balsam (1981; Figure 1) and the rats in this study (Figure 3). The evidence for this claim, as presented here, is not as strong as it could be. This is because the measure used for identifying trials to criterion in Figure 1 appears to differ from any of the criteria used in Figure 3, and the exact measure used for identifying trials to criterion influences the interpretation of Figure 3***. To make the claim that the quantitative relationship is one and the same in the Gibbon-Balsam and present datasets, one would need to use the same measure of learning on both datasets and show that the resultant plots are statistically indistinguishable, rather than simply plotting the dots from both data sets and spotlighting their visual similarity. In terms of their visual characteristics, it is worth noting that the plots are in log-log axis and, as such, slight visual changes can mean a big difference in actual numbers. For instance, between Figure 3B and 3C, the highest information group moves up only "slightly" on the y-axis but the difference is a factor of 5 in the real numbers. Thus, in order to support the strong claim that the quantitative relationships obtained in the Gibbon-Balsam and present datasets are identical, a more rigorous approach is needed for the comparisons.

      ***The measure of acquisition in Figure 3A is based on a previously established metric, whereas the measure in Figure 3B employs the relatively novel nDKL measure that is argued to be a better and theoretically based metric. Surprisingly, when r and r2 values are converted to the same metric across analyses, it appears that this new metric (Figure 3B) does well but not as well as the approach in Figure 3A. This raises questions about why a theoretically derived measure might not be performing as well on this analysis, and whether the more effective measure is either more reliable or tapping into some aspect of the processes that underlie acquisition that is not accounted for by the nDKL metric.

      Figure 3 shows that the relationship between learning rate and informativeness for our rats was very similar to that shown with pigeons by Gibbon and Balsam (1981). We have used multiple criteria to establish the number of trials to learn in our data, with the goal of demonstrating that the correspondence between the data sets was robust. In the revised Figure 3, specifically 3C and 3D, we have plotted trials to acquisition using decision criterion equivalent to those used by Gibbon and Balsam. The criterion they used—at least one peck at the response key on at least 3 out of 4 consecutive trials—cannot be directly applied to our magazine entry data because rats make magazine entries during the inter-trial interval (whereas pigeons do not peck at the response key in the inter-trial interval). Therefore, evidence for conditioning in our paradigm must involve comparison between the response rate during CS and the baseline response rate, rather than just counting responses during the CS. We have used two approaches to adapt the Gibbon and Balsam criterion to our data. One approach, plotted in Figure 3C, uses a non-parametric signed rank test for evidence that the CS response rate exceeds the pre-CS response rate, and adopting a statistical criterion equivalent to Gibbon and Balsam’s 3-out-of-4 consecutive trials (p<.3125). The second method (Figure 3D) estimates the nDkl for the criterion used by Gibbon and Balsam and then applies this criterion to the nDkl for our data. To estimate the nDkl of Gibbon and Balsam’s data, we have assumed there are no responses in the inter-trial interval and the response probability during the CS must be at least 0.75 (their criterion of at least 3 responses out of 4 trials). The nDkl for this difference is 2.2 (odds ratio 27:1). We have then applied this criterion to the nDkl obtained from our data to identify when the distribution of CS response rates has diverged by an equivalent amount from the distribution of pre-CS response rates. These two analyses have been added to the manuscript to replace those previously shown in Figures 3B and 3C.

      (2) Another interesting claim here is that the rates of responding during ITI and the cue are proportional to the corresponding reward rates with the same proportionality constant. This too requires more quantification and conceptual explanation. For quantification, it would be more convincing to calculate the regression slope for the ITI data and the cue data separately and then show that the corresponding slopes are not statistically distinguishable from each other. Conceptually, it is not clear why the data used to test the ITI proportionality came from the last 5 conditioning sessions. What were the decision criteria used to decide on averaging the final 5 sessions as terminal responses for the analyses in Figure 5? Was this based on consistency with previous work, or based on the greatest number of sessions where stable data for all animals could be extracted?

      If the model is that animals produce response rates during the ITI (a period with no possible rewards) based on the overall rate of rewards in the context, wouldn't it be better to test this before the cue learning has occurred? Before cue learning, the animals would presumably only have attributed rewards in the context to the context and thus, produce overall response rates in proportion to the contextual reward rate. After cue learning, the animals could technically know that the rate of rewards during ITI is zero. Why wouldn't it be better to test the plotted relationship for ITI before cue learning has occurred? Further, based on Figure 1, it seems that the overall ITI response rate reduces considerably with cue learning. What is the expected ITI response rate prior to learning based on the authors' conceptual model? Why does this rate differ from pre and post-cue learning? Finally, if the authors' conceptual framework predicts that ITI response rate after cue learning should be proportional to contextual reward rate, why should the cue response rate be proportional to the cue reward rate instead of the cue reward rate plus the contextual reward rate?

      A single regression line, as shown in Figure 5, is the simplest possible model of the relationship between response rate and reinforcement rate and it explains approximately 80% of the variance in response rate. Fixing the log-log slope at 1 yields the maximally simple model. (This regression is done in the logarithmic domain to satisfy the homoscedasticity assumption.) When transformed into the linear domain, this model assumes a truly scalar relation (linear, intercept at the origin) and assumes the same scale factor and the same scalar variability in response rates for both sets of data (ITI and CS). Our plot supports such a model. Its simplicity is its own motivation (Occam’s razor).

      If separate regression lines are fitted to the CS and ITI data, there is a small increase in explained variance (R<sub>2</sub> = 0.82). These regression lines have been added to the plot in the revised manuscript (Figure 5). We leave it to further research to determine whether such a complex model, with 4 parameters, is required. However, we do not think the present data warrant comparing the simplest possible model, with one parameter, to any more complex model for the following reasons:

      · When a brain—or any other machine—maps an observed (input) rate to a rate it produces (output rate), there is always an implicit scalar. In the special case where the produced rate equals the observed rate, the implicit scalar has value 1. Thus, there cannot be a simpler model than the one we propose, which is, in and of itself, interesting.

      · The present case is an intuitively accessible example of why the MDL (Minimum Description Length) approach to model complexity (Barron, Rissanen, & Yu, 1998; Grünwald, Myung, & Pitt, 2005; Rissanen, 1999) can yield a very different conclusion from the conclusion reached using the Bayesian Information Criterion (BIC) approach. The MDL approach measures the complexity of a model when given N data specified with precision of B bits per datum by computing (or approximating) the sum of the maximum-likelihoods of the model’s fits to all possible sets of N data with B precision per datum. The greater the sum over the maximum likelihoods, the more complex the model, that is, the greater its measured wiggle room, it’s capacity to fit data. Recall that von Neuman remarked to Fermi that with 4 parameters he could fit an elephant. His deeper point was that multi-parameter models bring neither insight nor predictive power; they explain only post-hoc, after one has adjusted their parameters in the light of the data. For realistic data sets like ours, the sums of maximum likelihoods are finite but astronomical. However, just as the Sterling approximation allows one to work with astronomical factorials, it has proved possible to develop readily computable approximations to these sums, which can be used to take model complexity into account when comparing models. Proponents of the MDL approach point out that the BIC is inadequate because models with the same number of parameters can have very different amounts of wiggle room. A standard illustration of this point is the contrast between logarithmic model and power-function model. Log regressions must be concave; whereas power function regressions can be concave, linear, or convex—yet they have the same number of parameters (one or two, depending on whether one counts the scale parameter that is always implicit). The MDL approach captures this difference in complexity because it measures wiggle room; the BIC approach does not, because it only counts parameters.

      · In the present case, one is comparing a model with no pivot and no vertical displacement at the boundary between the black dots and the red dots (the 1-parameter unilinear model) to a bilinear model that allows both a change in slope and a vertical displacement for both lines. The 4-parameter model is superior if we use the BIC to take model complexity into account. However, 4-parameter has ludicrously more wiggle room. It will provide excellent fits—high maximum likelihood—to data sets in which the red points have slope > 1, slope 0, or slope < 0 and in which it is also true that the intercept for the red points lies well below or well above the black points (non-overlap in the marginal distribution of the red and black data). The 1-parameter model, on the other hand, will provide terrible fits to all such data (very low maximum likelihoods). Thus, we believe the BIC does not properly capture the immense actual difference in the complexity between the 1-parameter model (unilinear with slope 1) to the 4-parameter model (bilinear with neither the slope nor the intercept fixed in the linear domain).

      · In any event, because the pivot (change in slope between black and red data sets), if any, is small and likewise for the displacement (vertical change), it suffices for now to know that the variance captured by the 1-parameter model is only marginally improved by adding three more parameters. Researchers using the properly corrected measured rate of head poking to measure the rate of reinforcement a subject expects can therefore assume that they have an approximately scalar measure of the subject’s expectation. Given our data, they won’t be far wrong even near the extremes of the values commonly used for rates of reinforcement. That is a major advance in current thinking, with strong implications for formal models of associative learning. It implies that the performance function that maps from the neurobiological realization of the subject’s expectation is not an unknown function. On the contrary, it’s the simplest possible function, the scalar function. That is a powerful constraint on brain-behavior linkage hypotheses, such as the many hypothesized relations between mesolimbic dopamine activity and the expectation that drives responding in Pavlovian conditioning (Berridge, 2012; Jeong et al., 2022; Y.  Niv, Daw, Joel, & Dayan, 2007; Y. Niv & Schoenbaum, 2008).

      The data in Figures 4 and 5 are taken from the last 5 sessions of training. The exact number of sessions was somewhat arbitrary but was chosen to meet two goals: (1) to capture asymptotic responding, which is why we restricted this to the end of the training, and (2) to obtain a sufficiently large sample of data to estimate reliably each rat’s response rate. We have checked what the data look like using the last 10 sessions, and can confirm it makes very little difference to the results. We now note this in the revised manuscript. The data for terminal responding by all rats, averaged over both the last 5 sessions and last 10 sessions, can be downloaded from https://osf.io/vmwzr/

      Finally, as noted by the reviews, the relationship between the contextual rate of reinforcement and ITI responding should also be evident if we had measured context responding prior to introducing the CS. However, there was no period in our experiment when rats were given unsignalled reinforcement (such as is done during “magazine training” in some experiments). Therefore, we could not measure responding based on contextual conditioning prior to the introduction of the CS. This is a question for future experiments that use an extended period of magazine training or “poor positive” protocols in which there are reinforcements during the ITIs as well as during the CSs. The learning rate equation has been shown to predict reinforcements to acquisition in the poor-positive case (Balsam, Fairhurst, & Gallistel, 2006).

      (3) There is a disconnect between the gradual nature of learning shown in Figures 7 and 8 and the information-theoretic model proposed by the authors. To the extent that we understand the model, the animals should simply learn the association once the evidence crosses a threshold (nDKL > threshold) and then produce behavior in proportion to the expected reward rate. If so, why should there be a gradual component of learning as shown in these figures? In terms of the proportional response rule to the rate of rewards, why is it changing as animals go from 10% to 90% of peak response? The manuscript would be greatly strengthened if these results were explained within the authors' conceptual framework. If these results are not anticipated by the authors' conceptual framework, this should be explicitly stated in the manuscript.

      One of us (CRG) has earlier suggested that responding appears abruptly when the accumulated evidence that the CS reinforcement rate is greater than the contextual rate exceeds a decision threshold (C.R.  Gallistel, Balsam, & Fairhurst, 2004). The new more extensive data require a more nuanced view. Evidence about the manner in which responding changes over the course of training is to some extent dependent on the analytic method used to track those changes. We presented two different approaches. The approach shown in Figures 7 and 8 (now 6 and 7), extending on that developed by Harris (2022), assumes a monotonic increase in response rate and uses the slope of the cumulative response rate to identify when responding exceeds particular milestones (percentiles of the asymptotic response rate). This analysis suggests a steady rise in responding over trials. Within our theoretical model, this might reflect an increase in the animal’s certainty about the CS reinforcement rate with accumulated evidence from each trial. While this method should be able to distinguish between a gradual change and a single abrupt change in responding (Harris, 2022) it may not distinguish between a gradual change and multiple step-like changes in responding and cannot account for decreases in response rate.

      The other analytic method we used relies on the information theoretic measure of divergence, the nDkl (Gallistel & Latham, 2023), to identify each point of change (up or down) in the response record. With that method, we discern three trends. First, the onset tends to be abrupt in that the initial step up is often large (an increase in response rate by 50% or more of the difference between its initial value and its terminal value is common and there are instances where the initial step is to the terminal rate or higher). Second, there is marked within-subject variability in the response rate, characterized by large steps up and down in the parsed response rates following the initial step up, but this variability tends to decrease with further training (there tend to be fewer and smaller steps in both the ITI response rates and the CS response rate as training progresses). Third, the overall trend, seen most clearly when one averages across subjects within groups is to a moderately higher rate of responding later in training than after the initial rise. We think that the first tendency reflects an underlying decision process whose latency is controlled by diminishing uncertainty about the two reinforcement rates and hence about their ratio. We think that decreasing uncertainty about the true values of the estimated rates of reinforcement is also likely to be an important part of the explanation for the second tendency (decreasing within-subject variation in response rates). It is less clear whether diminishing uncertainty can explain the trend toward a somewhat greater difference in the two response rates as conditioning progresses. It is perhaps worth noting that the distribution of the estimates of the informativeness ratio is likely to be heavy tailed and have peculiar properties (as witness, for example, the distribution of the ratio of two gamma distributions with arbitrary shape and scale parameters) but we are unable at this time to propound an explanation of the third trend.

      (4) Page 27, Procedure, final sentence: The magazine responding during the ITI is defined as the 20 s period immediately before CS onset. The range of ITI values (Table 1) always starts as low as 15 s in all 14 groups. Even in the case of an ITI on a trial that was exactly 20 s, this would also mean that the start of this period overlaps with the termination of the CS from the previous trial and delivery (and presumably consumption) of a pellet. It should be indicated whether the definition of the ITI period was modified on trials where the preceding ITI was < 20 s, and if any other criteria were used to define the ITI. Were the rats exposed to the reinforcers/pellets in their home cage prior to acquisition?

      There was an error in the description provided in the original text. The pre-CS period used to measure the ITI responding was 10 s rather than 20 s. There was always at least a 5-s gap between the end of the previous trial and the start of the pre-CS period. The statement about the pre-CS measure has been corrected in the revised manuscript.

      (5) For all the analyses, the exact models that were fit and the software used should be provided. For example, it is not necessarily clear to the reader (particularly in the absence of degrees of freedom) that the model discussed in Figure 3 fits on the individual subject data points or the group medians. Similarly, in Figure 6 there is no indication of whether a single regression model was fit to all the plotted data or whether tests of different slopes for each of the conditions were compared. With regards to the statistics in Figure 6, depending on how this was run, it is also a potential problem that the analyses do not correct for the potentially highly correlated multiple measurements from the same subjects, i.e. each rat provides 4 data points which are very unlikely to be independent observations.

      Details about model fitting have been added to the revision. The question about fitting a single model or multiple models to the data in Figure 6 (now 5) is addressed in response 2 above. In Figure 5, each rat provides 2 behavioural data points (ITI response rate and CS response rate) and 2 values for reinforcement rate (1/C and 1/T). There is a weak but significant correlation between the ITI and CS response rates (r = 0.28, p < 0.01; log transformed to correct for heteroscedasticity). By design, there is no correlation between the log reinforcement rates (r = 0.06, p = .404).

      CONCEPTUAL

      (1) We take the point that where traditional theories (e.g., Rescorla-Wagner) and rate estimation theory (RET) both explain some phenomenon, the explanation in terms of RET may be preferred as it will be grounded in aspects of an animal's experience rather than a hypothetical construct. However, like traditional theories, RET does not explain a range of phenomena - notably, those that require some sort of expectancy/representation as part of their explanation. This being said, traditional theories have been incorporated within models that have the representational power to explain a broader array of phenomena, which makes me wonder: Can rate estimation be incorporated in models that have representational power; and, if so, what might this look like? Alternatively, do the authors intend to claim that expectancy and/or representation - which follow from probabilistic theories in the RW mould - are unnecessary for explanations of animal behaviour?***

      It is important for the field to realize that the RW model cannot be used to explain the results of Rescorla’s (Rescorla, 1966; Rescorla, 1968, 1969) contingency-not-pairing experiments, despite what was claimed by Rescorla and Wagner (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972; Wagner & Rescorla, 1972) and has subsequently been claimed in many modelling papers and in most textbooks and reviews (Dayan & Niv, 2008; Y. Niv & Montague, 2008). Rescorla programmed reinforcements with a Poisson process. The defining property of a Poisson process is its flat hazard function; the reinforcements were equally likely at every moment in time when the process was running. This makes it impossible to say when non-reinforcements occurred and, a fortiori, to count them. The non-reinforcements are causal events in RW algorithm and subsequent versions of it. Their effects on associative strength are essential to the explanations proffered by these models. Non-reinforcements—failures to occur, updates when reinforcement is set to 0, hence also the lambda parameter—can have causal efficacy only when the successes may be predicted to occur at specified times (during “trials”). When reinforcements are programmed by a Poisson process, there are no such times. Attempts to apply the RW formula to reinforcement learning soon foundered on this problem (Gibbon, 1981; Gibbon, Berryman, & Thompson, 1974; Hallam, Grahame, & Miller, 1992; L.J. Hammond, 1980; L. J. Hammond & Paynter, 1983; Scott & Platt, 1985). The enduring popularity of the delta-rule updating equation in reinforcement learning depends on “big-concept” papers that don’t fit models to real data and discretize time into states while claiming to be real-time models (Y. Niv, 2009; Y. Niv, Daw, & Dayan, 2005).

      The information-theoretic approach to associative learning, which sometimes historically travels as RET (rate estimation theory), is unabashedly and inescapably representational. It assumes a temporal map and arithmetic machinery capable in principle of implementing any implementable computation. In short, it assumes a Turing-complete brain. It assumes that whatever the material basis of memory may be, it must make sense to ask of it how many bits can be stored in a given volume of material. This question is seldom posed in associative models of learning, nor by neurobiologists committed to the hypothesis that the Hebbian synapse is the material basis of memory. Many—including the new Nobelist, Geoffrey Hinton— would agree that the question makes no sense. When you assume that brains learn by rewiring themselves rather than by acquiring and storing information, it makes no sense.

      When a subject learns a rate of reinforcement, it bases its behavior on that expectation, and it alters its behavior when that expectation is disappointed. Subjects also learn probabilities when they are defined. They base some aspects of their behavior on those expectations, making computationally sophisticated use of their representation of the uncertainties (Balci, Freestone, & Gallistel, 2009; Chan & Harris, 2019; J. A. Harris, 2019; J.A. Harris & Andrew, 2017; J. A. Harris & Bouton, 2020; J. A. Harris, Kwok, & Gottlieb, 2019; Kheifets, Freestone, & Gallistel, 2017; Kheifets & Gallistel, 2012; Mallea, Schulhof, Gallistel, & Balsam, 2024 in press).

      (2) The discussion of Rescorla's (1967) and Kamin's (1968) findings needs some elaboration. These findings are already taken to mean that the target CS in each design is not informative about the occurrence of the US - hence, learning about this CS fails. In the case of blocking, we also know that changes in the rate of reinforcement across the shift from stage 1 to stage 2 of the protocol can produce unblocking. Perhaps more interesting from a rate estimation perspective, unblocking can also be achieved in a protocol that maintains the rate of reinforcement while varying the sensory properties of the US (Wagner). How does rate estimation theory account for these findings and/or the demonstrations of trans-reinforcer blocking (Pearce-Ganesan)? Are there other ways that the rate estimation account can be distinguished from traditional explanations of blocking and contingency effects? If so, these would be worth citing in the discussion. More generally, if one is going to highlight seminal findings (such as those by Rescorla and Kamin) that can be explained by rate estimation, it would be appropriate to acknowledge findings that challenge the theory - even if only to note that the theory, in its present form, is not all-encompassing. For example, it appears to me that the theory should not predict one-trial overshadowing or the overtraining reversal effect - both of which are amenable to discussion in terms of rates.

      I assume that the signature characteristics of latent inhibition and extinction would also pose a challenge to rate estimation theory, just as they pose a challenge to Rescorla-Wagner and other probability-based theories. Is this correct?

      The seemingly contradictory evidence of unblocking and trans-reinforcer blocking by Wagner and by Pearce and Ganesan cited above will be hard for any theory to accommodate. It will likely depend on what features of the US are represented in the conditioned response.

      RET predicts one-trial overshadowing, as anyone may verify in a scientific programming language because it has no free parameters; hence, no wiggle room. Overtraining reversal effects appear to depend on aspects of the subjects’ experience other than the rate of reinforcement. It seems unlikely that it can proffer an explanation.

      Various information-theoretic calculations give pretty good quantitative fits to the relatively few parametric studies of extinction and the partial-reinforcement extinction effect (see Gallistel (2012, Figs 3 & 4); Wilkes & Gallistel (2016, Fig 6) and Gallistel (2025, under review, Fig 6). It has not been applied to latent inhibition, in part for want of parametric data. However, clearly one should not attribute a negative rate to a context in which the subject had never been reinforced. An explanation, if it exists, would have to turn on the effect of that long period on initial rate estimates AND on evidence of a change in rate, as of the first reinforcement.

      Recommendations for authors:

      MINOR POINTS

      (1) It is not clear why Figure 3C is presented but not analyzed, and why the data presented in Figure 4 to clarify the spread of the distribution of the data observed across the plots in Figure 3 uses the data from Figure 3C. This would seem like the least representative data to illustrate the point of Figure 4. It also appears that the data plotted in Figure 4 corresponds to Figure 3A and 3B rather than the odds 10:1 data indicated in the text.

      Figures 3 has changed as already described. The data previously plotted in Figure 4 are now shown in 3B and corresponds to that plotted in Figure 3A.

      (2) Log(T) was not correlated with trials to criterion. If trials to criterion is inversely proportional to log(C/T) and C is uncorrelated with T, shouldn't trials to criterion be correlated with log(T)? Is this merely a matter of low statistical power?

      Yes. There is a small, but statistically non-significant, correlation between log(T) and trials to criterion, r = 0.35, p = .22. That correlation drops to .08 (p = .8) after factoring out log(C/T), which demonstrates that the weak correlation between log(T) and trials to criterion is based on the correlation between log(t) and log(C/T).

      (3) The rationale for the removal of the high information condition samples in the Fig 8 "Slope" plot to be weak. Can the authors justify this choice better? If all data are included, the relationship is clearly different from that shown in the plot.

      We have now reported correlations that include those 3 groups but noted that the correlations are largely driven by the much lower slope values of those 3 groups which is likely an artefact of their smaller number of trials. We use this to justify a second set of correlations that excludes those 3 groups.

      (4) The discussion states that there is at most one free parameter constrained by the data - the constant of proportionality for response rate. However, there is also another free parameter constrained by data-the informativeness at which expected trials to acquisition is 1.

      I think this comment is referring to two different sets of data. The constant of proportionality of the response rate refers to the scalar relationship between reinforcement rate and terminal response rate shown in Figure 5. The other parameter, the informativeness when trials to acquisition equals 1, describes the intercept of the regression line in Figure 1 (and 3).

      (5) The authors state that the measurement of available information is not often clear. Given this, how is contingency measurable based on the authors' framework?

      (6) Based on the variables provided in Supplementary File 3, containing the acquisition data, we were unable to reproduce the values reported in the analysis of Figure 3.

      Figure 3 has changed, using new criteria for trials to acquisition that attempt to match the criterion used by Gibbon and Balsam. The data on which these figures are based has been uploaded into OSF.

      GRAPHICAL AND TYPOGRAPHICAL

      (1) Y-axis labels in Figure 1 are not appropriately placed. 0 is sitting next to 0.1. 0 should sit at the bottom of the y-axis.

      If this comment refers to the 0 sitting above an arrow in the top right corner of the plot, this is not misaligned. The arrow pointing to zero is used to indicate that this axis approaches zero in the upward direction. 0 should not be aligned to a value on the axis since a learning rate of zero would indicate an infinite number of learning trials. The caption has been edited to explain this more clearly.

      (2) Typo, Page 6, Final Paragraph, line 4. "Fourteen groups of rats were trained with for 42 session"

      Corrected. Thank you.

      (3) Figure 3 caption: Typo, should probably be "Number of trials to acquisition"?

      This change has now been made. The axis shows reinforcements to acquisition to be consistent with Gibbon and Balsam, but trials and number of reinforcements are identical in our 100% reinforcement schedule.

      (4) Typo Page 17 Line 1: "Important pieces evidence about".

      Correct. Thank you.

      (5) Consider consistent usage of symbols/terms throughout the manuscript (e.g. Page 22, final paragraph: "iota = 2" is used instead of the corresponding symbol that has been used throughout).

      Changed.

      (6) Typo Page 28, Paragraph 1, Line 9: "We used a one-sample t-test using to identify when this".

      This section of text has been changed to reflect the new analysis used for the data in Figure 3.

      (7) Typo Page 29, Paragraph 1, Line 2: "problematic in cases where one of both rates are undefined" either typo or unclear phrasing.

      “of” has been corrected to “or”

      (8) Typo Page 30: Equation 3 appears to have an error and is not consistent with the initial printing of Equation 3 in the manuscript.

      The typo in initial expression of Eq 3 (page 23) has been corrected.

      (9) Typo Page 33, Line 5: "Figures 12".

      Corrected.

      (10) Typo Page 34, Line 10: "and the 5 the increasingly"? Should this be "the 5 points that"?

      Corrected.

      (11) Typo Page 35, Paragraph 2: "estimate of the onset of conditioned is the trial after which".

      Corrected.

      (12) Clarify: Page 35, final paragraph: it is stated that four-panel figures are included for each subject in the Supplementary files, but each subject has a six-panel figure in the Supplementary file.

      The text now clarifies that the 4-panel figures are included within the 6-panel figures in the Supplementary materials.

      (13) It is hard to identify the different groups in Figure 2 (Plot 15).

      The figure is simply intended to show that responding across seconds within the trial is relatively flat for each group. Individuation of specific groups is not particularly important.

      (14) It appears that the numbering on the y-axis is misaligned in Figure 2 relative to the corresponding points on the scale (unless I have misunderstood these values and the response rate measure to the ITI can drop below 0?).

      The numbers on the Y axes had become misaligned. That has now been corrected.

      (15) Please include the data from Figure 3A in the spreadsheet supplementary file 3. If it has already been included as one of the columns of data, please consider a clearer/consistent description of the relevant column variable in Supplementary File 1.

      The data from Figure 3 are now available from the linked OSF site, referenced in the manuscript.

      (16) Errors in supplementary data spreadsheets such that the C/T values are not consistent with those provided in Table 1 (C/T values of 4.5, 54, 180, and 300 are slightly different values in these spreadsheets). A similar error/mismatch appears to have occurred in the C/T labels for Figures (e.g. Figure 10) and the individual supplementary figures.

      The C/T values on the figures in the supplementary materials have been corrected and are now consistent with those in Table 1.

      (17) Currently the analysis and code provided at https://osf.io/vmwzr/ are not accessible without requesting access from the author. Please consider making these openly available without requiring a request for authorization. As such, a number of recommendations made here may already have been addressed by the data and code deposited on OSF. Apologies for any redundant recommendations.

      Data and code are now available in at the OSF site which has been made public without requiring request.

      (18) Please consider a clearer and more specific reference to supplementary materials. Currently, the reader is required to search through 4 separate supplementary files to identify what is being discussed/referenced in the text (e.g. Page 18, final line: "see Supplementary Materials" could simply be "see Figure S1").

      We have added specific page numbers in references to the Supplementary Materials.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript describes a novel magnetic steering technique to target human adipose derived mesenchymal stem cells (hAMSC) or induce pluripotent stem cells to the TM (iPSC-TM). The authors show that delivery of the stem cells lowered IOP, increased outflow facility, and increased TM cellularity.

      Strengths:

      The technique is novel and shows promise as a novel therapeutic to lower IOP in glaucoma. hAMSC are able to lower IOP below the baseline as well as increase outflow facility above baseline with no tumorigenicity. These data will have a positive impact on the field and will guide further research using hAMSC in glaucoma models.

      Weaknesses:

      The transgenic mouse model of glaucoma the authors used did not show ocular hypertensive phenotypes at 6-7 months of age as previously reported. Therefore, if there is no pathology in these animals the authors did not show a restoration of function, but rather a decrease in pressure below normal IOP.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback and agree with the statement of weakness. Accordingly, we have revised the language to improve clarity. Specifically, all references to "restoration of IOP" or "restoration of conventional outflow function" have been replaced with more precise phrases, in the following locations: 

      • lines 2-3 (title): Magnetically steered cell therapy for reduction of intraocular pressure  as a treatment strategy for open-angle glaucoma

      • lines 36-8 (abstract): We observed a 4.5 [3.1, 6.0] mmHg or 27% reduction in intraocular pressure (IOP) for nine months after a single dose of only 1500 magnetically-steered hAMSCs, explained by increased conventional outflow facility and associated with higher TM cellularity.

      • lines 45-6 (one-sentence summary): A novel magnetic cell therapy provided effective intraocular pressure reduction in mice, motivating future translational studies.

      • lines 123-4 (introduction): Despite the absence of ocular hypertension in our MYOC<sup>Y437H</sup> mice, our data demonstrate sustained IOP lowering and a significant benefit of magnetic cell steering in the eye, particularly for hAMSCs, strongly indicating further translational potential.

      • line 207 (results): The observed reductions in IOP and increases in outflow facility after delivery of both cell types suggested functional changes in the conventional outflow pathway.

      • line 509-10 (discussion): In summary, this work shows the effectiveness of our novel magnetic TM cell therapy approach for long-term IOP reduction through functional changes in the conventional outflow pathway.

      It is very important to note that at the 23rd annual Trabecular Meshwork Study Club meeting (San Diego, December 2024), Dr. Zode, the lead author of reference 26 originally describing the transgenic myocilin mouse model, announced during his talk that this model no longer demonstrates the glaucomatous phenotype in his hands, which incidentally has motivated him to create a new, CRISPR MYOC mouse model. Dr. Zode also stated that he was uncertain of the reason for this loss of phenotype. His observation is consistent with our report. However, other investigators continue to observe the desired phenotype in their colonies of this mouse (Dr. Wei Zhu, personal communication). Continued use of this mouse model should therefore be approached with caution. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This observational study investigates the efficacy of intracameral injected human stem cells as a means to re-functionalize the trabecular meshwork for the restoration of intraocular pressure homeostasis. Using a murine model of glaucoma, human adiposederived mesenchymal stem cells are shown to be biologically safer and functionally superior at eliciting a sustained reduction in intraocular pressure (IOP). The authors conclude that the use of human adipose-derived mesenchymal stem cells has the potential for long-term treatment of ocular hypertension in glaucoma.

      Strengths:

      A noted strength is the use of a magnetic steering technique to direct injected stem cells to the iridocorneal angle. An additional strength is the comparison of efficacy between two distinct sources of stem cells: human adipose-derived mesenchymal vs. induced pluripotent cell derivatives. Utilizing both in vivo and ex vivo methodology coupled with histological evidence of introduced stem cell localization provides a consistent and compelling argument for a sustainable impact exogenous stem cells may have on the refunctionalization of a pathologically compromised TM.

      Weaknesses:

      A noted weakness of the study, as pointed out by the authors, includes the unanticipated failure of the genetic model to develop glaucoma-related pathology (elevated IOP, TM cell changes). While this is most unfortunate, it does temper the conclusion that exogenous human adipose derived mesenchymal stem cells may restore TM cell function. Given that TM cell function was not altered in their genetic model, it is difficult to say with any certainty that the introduced stem cells would be capable of restoring pathologically altered TM function. A restoration effect remains to be seen. 

      We acknowledge that the phrase “restoration of TM function” is not fully supported by our results, given the absence of ocular hypertension in our animal model. Accordingly, we have revised the language to more precisely describe our findings. For specific details regarding these changes, please refer to our response to Reviewer 1’s public comments above.

      Another noted complication to these findings is the observation that sham intracameralinjected saline control animals all showed elevated IOP and reduced outflow facility, compared to WT or Tg untreated animals, which allowed for more robust statistically significant outcomes. Additional comments/concerns that the authors may wish to address are elaborated in the Private Review section.

      We agree that sham-injected animals tended to have higher average IOPs than transgenic animals in our study. However, these differences did not reach statistical significance and therefore remain inconclusive. Further, an increase in IOP following placebo injection has been previously reported (Zhu et al., 2016). 

      Prompted by the Referee’s comments and also a private comment from Referee 1, we further investigated this effect by analyzing IOP in uninjected contralateral eyes at the mid-term time point and comparing the IOPs in these eyes to other cohorts, as now presented as additional data in Supplementary Tables 1 and 2 and Supplementary Figure 4 (see below). In brief, the uninjected contralateral transgenic eyes (10 months old) showed an IOP of 16.5 [15.9, 17.1] mmHg, which was intermediate between the IOP levels of the 6–7-month-old Tg group (15.4 [14.7, 16.1] mmHg) and the sham group (16.9 [15.5, 18.2] mmHg). However, none of these differences reached statistical significance. Additionally, we cannot rule out potential contralateral effects induced by the injections.

      Regarding the best way to assess the effect of cell treatment, we feel very strongly that the most relevant IOP comparison is between cell-injected eyes and control (vehicle)-injected eyes, since this provides the most direct accounting for the effects of injection itself on IOP. Other comparisons, such as WT or untreated Tg eyes vs. cell-treated eyes, are interesting but harder to interpret. However, in response to the referee’s comment, we have added comparisons between cell-treated groups and untreated Tg eyes to Table 2, adjusting the post-hoc corrections accordingly. All hAMSC treated groups show statistically significant decrease in IOP even compared to Tg untreated eyes, while iPSC-TMs fail to reach such significance.

      The following changes were made to the manuscript:

      Lines 326 et seq.: Eyes subjected to saline injection exhibited marginally higher IOPs and lower outflow facilities on average, in comparison to the transgenic animals at baseline. However, due to the lack of statistical significance in these differences and the inherent age difference between the saline-injected animals and the non-injected controls at baseline, no conclusive inference can be drawn regarding the effect of saline injection. To investigate this phenomenon further, we also analyzed IOPs in uninjected contralateral eyes at the midterm time point (Supplementary Tables 1 and 2, Supplementary Figure 4). The uninjected contralateral transgenic eyes (10 months old) showed an IOP of 16.5 [15.9, 17.1] mmHg, which was intermediate between the IOP levels of the 6–7-month-old Tg group (15.4 [14.7, 16.1] mmHg) and the sham-injected group (16.9 [15.5, 18.2] mmHg). However, none of these differences reached statistical significance. Of note, contralateral hypertension has been previously reported after subconjunctival and periocular injection of dexamethasoneloaded nanoparticles (34), and we similarly cannot definitively rule out potential contralateral effects induced by our stem cell injections. Thus, we cannot draw any definite conclusions from these additional IOP comparisons at this time.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The purpose of the current manuscript was to investigate a magnetic cell steering technique for efficiency and tissue-specific targeting, using two types of stem cells, in a mouse model of glaucoma. As the authors point out, trabecular meshwork (TM) cell therapy is an active area of research for treating elevated intraocular pressure as observed in glaucoma. Thus, further studies determining the ideal cell choice for TM cell therapy is warranted. The experimental protocol of the manuscript involved the injection of either human adipose derived mesenchymal stem cells (hAMSCs) or induced pluripotent cell derivatives (iPSC-TM cells) into a previously reported mouse glaucoma model, the transgenic MYOCY437H mice and wild-type littermates followed by the magnetic cell steering. Numerous outcome measures were assessed and quantified including IOP, outflow facility, TM cellularity, retention of stem cells, and the inner wall BM of Schlemm's canal.

      Strengths:

      All of these analyses were carefully carried out and appropriate statistical methods were employed. The study has clearly shown that the hAMSCs are the cells of choice over the iPSC-TM cells, the latter of which caused tumors in the anterior chamber. The hAMSCs were shown to be retained in the anterior segment over time and this resulted in increased cellular density in the TM region and a reduction in IOP and outflow facility. These are all interesting findings and there is substantial data to support it.

      Weaknesses:

      However, where the study falls short is in the MYOCY437H mouse model of glaucoma that was employed. The authors clearly state that a major limitation of the study is that this model, in their hands, did not exhibit glaucomatous features as previously reported, such as a significant increase in IOP, which was part of the overall purpose of the study. The authors state that it is possible that "the transgene was silenced in the original breeders". The authors did not show PCR, western blot, or immuno of angle tissue of the tg to determine transgenic expression (increased expression of MYOC was shown in the angle tissue of the transgenics in the original paper by Zode et al, 2011). This should be investigated given that these mice were rederived. Thus, it is clearly possible that these are not transgenic mice.

      All MYOC mice that were used in this study were genotyped and confirmed to carry the transgene as noted in the original version of the paper (see lines 590-2). However, the transgene seems not to have been active, based on the lack of ocular hypertension as well as the lack of differences in supporting endpoints such as outflow facility and TM cellularity. While it would have been possible to carry out their recommended assays to investigate the root cause of this loss of phenotype this was not an objective of our study. Thus we instead here focus simply on communicating the observed loss of phenotype to readers. We also refer the referee to the final paragraph of our response to Referee 1. 

      If indeed they are transgenics, the authors may want to consider the fact that in the Zode paper, the most significant IOP elevation in the mutant mice was observed at night and thus this could be examined by the authors. 

      This is a good point. However, while the dark-phase IOP does exhibit a distinctly larger elevation (as previously observed in hypertonic saline sclerosis), Zode et al. also reported a notable 3 mmHg IOP increase during the light phase. The complete absence of such daytime (light phase) IOP elevation in our animals diminished our enthusiasm for pursuing darkphase IOP measurements. 

      Other glaucomatous features of these mice could also have been investigated such as loss of RGCs, to further determine their transgenic phenotype. 

      We agree that these other phenotypes could be studied, but in the absence of any detectable IOP elevation (and thus lack of mechanical insult on RGC axons), loss of RGC is extremely unlikely. We also note that the loss of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) in the Myocilin model remains a subject of controversy. For example, despite a significant increase in IOP (>10 mmHg) in this model across four mouse strains, three, including C57BL6/J, did not exhibit any signs of optic nerve damage (McDowell et al., 2012). In contrast, Zhu et al. observed considerable nerve damage in this model, which was reversed following iPSC-TM cell transplantation (Zhu et al., 2016). Given these conflicting findings, we directed our efforts toward outcome measures directly related to aqueous humor dynamics.

      Finally, while increased cellular density in the TM region was observed, proliferative markers could be employed to determine if the transplanted cells are proliferating.

      We agree that identifying the source of the increased trabecular meshwork (TM) cellularity we observed is interesting and we plan to pursue that in future studies. 

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The sham-injected transgenic animals showed elevated IOP 3-4 weeks after the baseline measurements in the transgenic mice. The authors justify this may be due to the increase in age in these animals. However, this seems unlikely due to the short duration of time between measurement of the baseline IOP and the Short time point (3-4 weeks). The authors do not provide IOP data for any WT sham injected eyes or naïve Tg eyes at these time points. These data are essential to determine if the elevation is due to the sham injection, age, or the transgene. Could it be that the IOP in this cohort of Tg mice didn't increase until 7-8 months of age instead of 6-7 months of age? The methods state only unilateral injections of the stem cells were done so it is assumed the contralateral eye was uninjected. What was the IOP in these eyes? These data would clarify the confusion in the data from sham-injected animals compared to baseline (naive) measurements.

      We agree that the average IOP in saline-injected groups is higher than in WT or non-treated Tg mice, although the difference is inconclusive due to a lack of statistical significance. It is important to note, however, that this difference is subtle and not comparable to the 3 mmHg light-phase IOP elevation previously observed in this model (Zode et al., 2011). 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to include IOP data from the contralateral uninjected eyes, and we have now provided this information along with the comparative statistics in the supplementary materials. Additional details can be found in our response to a similar comment from Reviewer 2’s public review. In summary, the IOP difference in contralateral non-injected ten-month-old transgenic eyes was even smaller than in the original Tg group. IOP elevation following saline injection in mice has been reported previously (Zhu et al., 2016). As a potential confounding factor, we highlight possible contralateral effects of the injection itself (which is why we initially did not analyze IOP in the contralateral eyes).

      The hAMSC-treated eyes appear to lower IOP even from baseline (although stats were only provided compared to the sham-injected eyes, which as stated above appear to have increased).

      However, the iPSC-TM-treated eyes had IOPs equal to that of the baseline measurements taken 3 weeks prior. The significance is coming from the "sham-treated" eyes which had elevated IOPs. The controls listed above should be included to make these conclusions.

      The reviewer makes an astute observation. Please refer to our response to a similar observation by Reviewer 2 under public reviews, where we provide and discuss the comparative statistics noted by the reviewer. However, we feel very strongly that the most relevant IOP comparison is between cell-injected eyes and control-injected eyes. 

      If the transgenic mouse model truly did not have a phenotype, then the authors are testing the ability of the stem cells to lower IOP from baseline normal pressures. Therefore, the authors are not "restoring function of the conventional outflow pathway" as there is no damage to begin with. The language in the manuscript should be corrected to reflect this if the transgenics have no phenotype.

      We agree and have adjusted the language accordingly. For further details, please refer to our response to your public review.

      The authors noted in the iPSC-TM-treated eyes there was a high rate of tumorigenicity. If the magnetic steering of these cells is specific and targeted to the TM, why do the tumors form near the central iris?

      While magnetic steering is more specific to the trabecular meshwork (TM) than previouslyused approaches (Bahrani Fard et al., 2023), it is not perfect, and a modest amount of offtarget delivery to the iris, including its central portion, still occurs. Apparently, it took only a few mis-directed iPSC-TM cells to lead to tumors in this work, which is a serious concern for future translational approaches. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) It appears that mice were injected unilaterally (Line 590). I may have missed this, but was the companion un-injected eye analyzed in this study? If not analyzed, was there a confounding concern or limitation that necessitated omitting this possible control option?

      Contralateral effects, such as hypertension in the untreated eye after subconjunctival and periocular injection of dexamethasone-loaded nanoparticles, have previously been reported in the literature (Li et al., 2019) and also reported anecdotally by other leaders in the field to the senior authors, which is why we did not initially analyze contralateral eyes in this study. However, prompted by this comment and others, we have now included the IOP measurements for contralateral uninjected ten-month-old transgenic eyes in the supplementary materials. For further details, please refer to our response to your public review.

      (2) Were all these mice the same gender? Would gender be expected to alter the findings of this study?

      Animals of both sexes were randomly chosen and included in the study. We added the following statement to the Materials and Methods section (line 530): After breeding and genotyping, mice, regardless of sex, were maintained to age 6-7 months, when transgenic animals were expected to have developed a POAG phenotype.

      (3) As noted in the public review, the use of PBS for a control seems to have resulted in a slight elevation in IOP (Figure 2) as well as a reduction in outflow facility (Figure 3B) when compared to WT or Tg mice. Was this difference statistically significant? 

      The differences between the sham (saline)-injected groups at any time point and untreated Tg mice did not reach statistical significance for IOP, facility, or TM cellularity and for facility, did not even show clear trends. For example, WT mice had, on average, 0.2 mmHg higher IOP and 0.6 nl/min/mmHg greater facility than the Tg group. Meanwhile on a similar scale, the long-term sham group exhibited 0.4 nl/min/mmHg higher facility compared to the Tg group. As the statistical tests indicate, these differences should be interpreted more as noise than meaningful signal. 

      If so, then it should be noted as to whether the observed decrease in IOP following stem cell injection remained statistically significant when compared to these un-injected control animals. If significance was lost, then this should be appropriately noted and discussed. It is not apparently obvious why sham controls should have elevated IOP. This is a design and statistical concern.

      Please refer to our response to a similar observation by Reviewer 1. We believe that comparing the treatment (cell suspension in saline) with its age-matched vehicle (saline) is the appropriate approach which maintains rigor by most directly accounting for the effects of injection. 

      (4) The tonicity of the PBS used as a vehicle control was not stated and I did not see within the methods whether the stem cells were suspended using this same PBS vehicle. I assume isotonic phosphate buffered saline was used and that the stem cells were resuspended using the same sterile PBS. 

      Thanks for catching this. We added “sterile PBS (1X, Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA)” to the Methods section of the manuscript (line 567). 

      With regards to using PBS as an injection control, I wonder if a better comparable control might have been to use mesenchymal stem cells that were rendered incapable of proliferating prior to intracameral injection. This, of course, addresses the unexplained mechanism(s) by which mesenchymal stem cells elicit a decrease in IOP.

      This is an interesting idea, and represents another level of control. However, we explicitly chose not to use non-proliferating hAMSCs as a control, for several reasons. Firstly, a saline injection is the simplest control and in this initial study with multiple groups, we did not feel another experimental group should be added. Second, this control would not rule out paracrine effects from injected cells, which our data suggested are an important effect. Third, rendering injected cells truly non-proliferative could introduce unwanted/unknown phenotypes in these cells that would need to be carefully characterized. That being said, if an efficient method could be developed to render an entire population of these cells irreversibly non-proliferating, the reviewer’s suggestion would be worth pursuing to better understand the mechanism of TM cell therapies. 

      (5) As noted in Figure 4C, TM cellular density as quantified was not altered in the sham control, so a loss of cellular density can not explain the elevated IOP with this group. Injecting viable (not determined?) mesenchymal stem cells did show, over the short term, a noted increase in TM cellular density. 

      Thank you for noting this. We agree that changes in cell density do not explain the mild IOP elevation in the sham group. As the referee certainly is aware, there are multiple reasons that IOP can be elevated (changes in trabecular meshwork extracellular matrix, changes in trabecular meshwork stiffness) that are not necessarily related to cell density.  Since we do not know definitively the cause of this mild elevation, we would prefer to not speculate about it in the manuscript. 

      Thanks for pointing out our omission of a statement about injected cell viability. We have now included the following statement in the Materials and Methods section (564-566): “For all the experiments where animals received hAMSC, cell count and >90% viability was verified using a Countess II Automated Cell Counter (Thermo Fisher Scientific, Waltham, MA).”

      I'm confused, as clearly stated (Lines 431-432), mesenchymal stem cells accumulated close to, but not within, the TM. How is it that TM cellular density increased if these stem cells did not enter the TM? The authors may wish to clarify this distinction. Given that mesenchymal stem cells did not increase the risk of tumorigenicity, do the authors have any evidence that these cells actually proliferated post-injection or did they undergo senesce thereby displaying senescence-associated secretory phenotype as a source of paracrine support?

      As the reviewer correctly noted, our observations show that hAMSCs primarily accumulated close to, but outside, the TM (likely caught up in the pectinate ligaments). Based on observations of increased TM cellularity, we think that the most likely explanation of these findings is paracrine signaling, as the reviewer suggests and which was discussed at length in the original version of the manuscript (lines 453-477). 

      We agree that, despite observing little signal from hAMSCs within the TM, labeling with proliferation markers (e.g., Ki-67) and searching for co-localization with exogenous cells, and/or labeling for senescence markers would have provided more mechanistic information. This is an excellent topic for future study, which we plan to pursue, but was outside the scope of this study. 

      (6) As noted in the public review, I think it is a bit of a stretch to even suggest that the findings of this study support stem cell restoration of TM function given that the model apparently did not produce TM cell dysfunction as anticipated. A restoration effect remains to be seen.

      We agree and have adjusted the language accordingly. For further details, please refer to our response to Reviewer 1’s public comment.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Show PCR, western blot, or immuno of angle tissue of the MYOC tg to confirm transgenic expression.

      (2) Examine the IOP of mice at night.

      (3) Investigate other glaucomatous features in the mice to determine if they have any of the transgenic phenotypes previously reported.

      (4) Examine proliferative markers in the TM region of angles injected with stem cells.

      Please see our responses to all four of these comments in the public section.

      Bibliography (for this response letter only)

      Bahrani Fard, M.R., Chan, J., Sanchez Rodriguez, G., Yonk, M., Kuturu, S.R., Read, A.T., Emelianov, S.Y., Kuehn, M.H., Ethier, C.R., 2023. Improved magnetic delivery of cells to the trabecular meshwork in mice. Exp. Eye Res. 234, 109602. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.exer.2023.109602

      Li, G., Lee, C., Agrahari, V., Wang, K., Navarro, I., Sherwood, J.M., Crews, K., Farsiu, S., Gonzalez, P., Lin, C.-W., Mitra, A.K., Ethier, C.R., Stamer, W.D., 2019. In vivo measurement of trabecular meshwork stiffness in a corticosteroid-induced ocular hypertensive mouse model. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 116, 1714–1722.

      https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1814889116

      Zhu, W., Gramlich, O.W., Laboissonniere, L., Jain, A., Sheffield, V.C., Trimarchi, J.M., Tucker, B.A., Kuehn, M.H., 2016. Transplantation of iPSC-derived TM cells rescues glaucoma phenotypes in vivo. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 113, E3492–E3500.

      Zode, G.S., Kuehn, M.H., Nishimura, D.Y., Searby, C.C., Mohan, K., Grozdanic, S.D., Bugge, K., Anderson, M.G., Clark, A.F., Stone, E.M., Sheffield, V.C., 2011. Reduction of ER stress via a chemical chaperone prevents disease phenotypes in a mouse model of primary open angle glaucoma. J. Clin. Invest. 121, 3542–3553. https://doi.org/10.1172/JCI58183

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public reviews:

      Reviewer #1:

      The authors attempted to replicate previous work showing that counterconditioning leads to more persistent reduction of threat responses, relative to extinction. They also aimed to examine the neural mechanisms underlying counterconditioning and extinction. They achieved both of these aims and were able to provide some additional information, such as how counterconditioning impacts memory consolidation. Having a better understanding of which neural networks are engaged during counterconditioning may provide novel pharmacological targets to aid in therapies for traumatic memories. It will be interesting to follow up by examining the impact of varying amounts of time between acquisition and counterconditioning phases, to enhance replicability to real-world therapeutic settings.

      Major strengths

      · This paper is very well written and attempts to comprehensively assess multiple aspects of counterconditioning and extinction processes. For instance, the addition of memory retrieval tests is not core to the primary hypotheses but provides additional mechanistic information on how episodic memory is impacted by counterconditioning. This methodical approach is commonly seen in animal literature, but less so in human studies.

      · The Group x Cs-type x Phase repeated measure statistical tests with 'differentials' as outcome variables are quite complex, however, the authors have generally done a good job of teasing out significant F test findings with post hoc tests and presenting the data well visually. It is reassuring that there is a convergence between self-report data on arousal and valence and the pupil dilation response. Skin conductance is a notoriously challenging modality, so it is not too concerning that this was placed in the supplementary materials. Neural responses also occurred in logical regions with regard to reward learning.

      · Strong methodology with regards to neuroimaging analysis, and physiological measures.

      ·The authors are very clear on documenting where there were discrepancies from their pre-registration and providing valid rationales for why.

      We thank reviewer 1 for the positive feedback and for pointing out the strengths of our work. We agree that future research should investigate varying times between acquisition and counterconditioning to assess its success in real-life applications.

      Major Weaknesses

      (1) The statistics showing that counterconditioning prevents differential spontaneous recovery are the weakest p values of the paper (and using one-tailed tests, although this is valid due to directions being pre-hypothesized). This may be due to a relatively small number of participants and some variability in responses. It is difficult to see how many people were included in the final PDR and neuroimaging analyses, with exclusions not clearly documented. Based on Figure 3, there are relatively small numbers in the PDR analyses (n=14 and n=12 in counterconditioning and extinction, respectively). Of these, each group had 4 people with differential PDR results in the opposing direction to the group mean. This perhaps warrants mention as the reported effects may not hold in a subgroup of individuals, which could have clinical implications.

      General exclusion criteria are described on page 17. We have added more detailed information on the reasons for exclusion (see page 17). All exclusions were in line with pre-registered criteria. For the analysis, the reviewer is referring to (PDR analysis that investigated whether CC can prevent the spontaneous recovery of differential conditioned threat responses), 18 participants were excluded from this analysis: 2 participants did not show evidence for successful threat acquisition as was already indicated on page 17, and 16 participants were excluded due to (partially) missing data. We now explicitly mention the exclusion of the additional 16 participants on page 7 and have updated Figure 3 to improve visibility of the individual data points. Therefore, for this analysis both experimental groups consisted of 15 participants (total N=30).

      It is true that in both groups a few participants show the opposite pattern. Although this may also be due to measurement error, we agree that it is relevant to further investigate this in future studies with larger sample sizes. It will be crucial to identify who will respond to treatments based on the principles of standard extinction or counterconditioning. We have added this point in the discussion on page 14.

      Reviewer #2:

      Summary:

      The present study sets out to examine the impact of counterconditioning (CC) and extinction on conditioned threat responses in humans, particularly looking at neural mechanisms involved in threat memory suppression. By combining behavioral, physiological, and neuroimaging (fMRI) data, the authors aim to provide a clear picture of how CC might engage unique neural circuits and coding dynamics, potentially offering a more robust reduction in threat responses compared to traditional extinction.

      Strengths:

      One major strength of this work lies in its thoughtful and unique design - integrating subjective, physiological, and neuroimaging measures to capture the various aspects of counterconditioning (CC) in humans. Additionally, the study is centered on a well-motivated hypothesis and the findings have the potential to improve the current understanding of pathways associated with emotional and cognitive control. The data presentation is systematic, and the results on behavioral and physiological measures fit well with the hypothesized outcomes. The neuroimaging results also provide strong support for distinct neural mechanisms underlying CC versus extinction.

      We thank reviewer 2 for the feedback and for valuing the thoughtfulness that went into designing the study.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Overall, this study is a well-conducted and thought-provoking investigation into counterconditioning, with strong potential to advance our understanding of threat modulation mechanisms. Two main weaknesses concern the scope and decisions regarding analysis choices. First, while the findings are solid, the topic of counterconditioning is relatively niche and may have limited appeal to a broader audience. Expanding the discussion to connect counterconditioning more explicitly to widely studied frameworks in emotional regulation or cognitive control would enhance the paper's accessibility and relevance to a wider range of readers. This broader framing could also underscore the generalizability and broader significance of the results. In addition, detailed steps in the statistical procedures and analysis parameters seem to be missing. This makes it challenging for readers to interpret the results in light of potential limitations given the data modality and/or analysis choices.

      In this updated version of the manuscript, we included the notion that extinction has been interpreted as a form of implicit emotion regulation. In addition to our discussion on active coping (avoidance), we believe that our discussion has an important link to the more general framework of emotion regulation, while remaining within the scope of relevance. Please see pages 14 and 15 for the changes. In addition to being informative to theories of emotion regulation, our findings are also highly relevant for forms of psychotherapy that build on principles of counterconditioning (e.g. the use of positive reinforcement in cognitive behavioral therapy), as we point out in the introduction. We believe this relevance shows that counterconditioning is more than a niche topic. In line with the recommendation from reviewer 2, we added more details and explanations to the statistical procedures and analyses where needed (see responses to recommendations).

      Reviewer #3:

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Wirz et al use neuroimaging (fMRI) to show that counterconditioning produces a longer lasting reduction in fear conditioning relative to extinction and appears to rely on the nucleus accumbens rather than the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. These important findings are supported by convincing evidence and will be of interest to researchers across multiple subfields, including neuroscientists, cognitive theory researchers, and clinicians.

      In large part, the authors achieved their aims of giving a qualitative assessment of the behavioural mechanisms of counterconditioning versus extinction, as well as investigating the brain mechanisms. The results support their conclusions and give interesting insights into the psychological and neurobiological mechanisms of the processes that underlie the unlearning, or counteracting, of threat conditioning.

      Strengths:

      · Mostly clearly written with interesting psychological insights

      · Excellent behavioural design, well-controlled and tests for a number of different psychological phenomena (e.g. extinction, recovery, reinstatement, etc).

      · Very interesting results regarding the neural mechanisms of each process.

      · Good acknowledgement of the limitations of the study.

      We thank reviewer 3 for the detailed feedback and suggestions.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) I think the acquisition data belongs in the main figure, so the reader can discern whether or not there are directional differences prior to CC and extinction training that could account for the differences observed. This is particularly important for the valence data which appears to differ at baseline (supplemental figure 2C).

      Since our design is quite complex with a lot of results, we left the fear acquisition results as a successful manipulation check in the Supplementary Information to not overload the reader with information that is not the main focus of this manuscript. If the editor would like us to add the figure to the main text, we are happy to do so. During fear acquisition, both experimental groups showed comparable differential conditioned threat responses as measured by PDRs and SCRs. Subjective valence ratings indeed differed depending on CS category. Importantly, however, the groups only differed with respect to their rating to the CS- category, but not the CS+ category, which suggests that the strength of the acquired fear is similar between the groups. To make sure that these baseline differences cannot account for the differences in valence after CC/Ext, we ran an additional group comparison with differential valence ratings after fear acquisition added as a covariate. Results show that despite the baseline difference, the group difference in valence after CC/Ext is still significant (main effect Group: F<sub>(1,43)</sub>=7.364, p=0.010, η<sup>2</sup>=0.146). We have added this analysis to the manuscript (see page 7).

      (2) I was confused in several sections about the chronology of what was done and when. For instance, it appears that individuals went through re-extinction, but this is just called extinction in places.

      We understand that the complexity of the design may require a clearer description. We therefore made some changes throughout the manuscript to improve understanding. Figure 1 is very helpful in understanding the design and we therefore refer to that figure more regularly (see pages 6-7). We also added the time between tasks where appropriate (e.g. see page 7). Re-extinction after reinstatement was indeed mentioned once in the manuscript. Given that the reinstatement procedure was not successful (see page 9), we could not investigate re-extinction and it is therefore indeed not relevant to explicitly mention and may cause confusion. We therefore removed it (see page 12).

      (3) I was also confused about the data in Figure 3. It appears that the CC group maintained differential pupil dilation during CC, whereas extinction participants didn't, and the authors suggest that this is indicative of the anticipation of reward. Do reward-associated cues typically cause pupil dilation? Is this a general arousal response? If so, does this mean that the CSs become equally arousing over time for the CC group whereas the opposite occurs for the extinction group (i.e. Figure 3, bottom graphs)? It is then further confusing as to why the CC group lose differential responding on the spontaneous recovery test. I'm not sure this was adequately addressed.

      Indeed, reward and reward anticipation also evoke an increase in pupil dilation. This was an important reason for including a separate valence-specific response characterization task. Independently from the conditioning task, this task revealed that both threat and reward-anticipation induced strong arousal-related PDRs and SCRs. This was also reflected in the explicit arousal ratings, which were stronger for both the shock-reinforced (negative valence) and reward-reinforced (positive valence) stimuli. Therefore, it is not surprising that reward anticipation leads to stronger PDRs for CS+ (which predict reward) compared to CS- stimuli (which do not predict reward) during CC, but is reduced during extinction due to a decrease in shock anticipation. During the spontaneous recovery test, a return of stronger PDRs for CS+ compared to CS- stimuli in the standard extinction group can only reflect a return of shock anticipation. Importantly, the CC group received no rewards during the spontaneous recovery task and was aware of this, so it is to be expected that the effect is weakened in the CC group. However, CS+ and CS- items were still rated of similar valence and PDRs did not differ between CS+ and CS- items in the CC group, whereas the Ext group rated the CS+ significantly more negative and threat responses to the CS+ did return. It therefore is reasonable to conclude that associating the CS+ with reward helps to prevent a return of threat responses. We have added some clarifications and conclusions to this section on page 8.

      (4) I am not sure that the memories tested were truly episodic

      In line with previous publications from Dunsmoor et al.[1-4], our task allows for the investigation of memory for elements of a specific episode. In the example of our task, retrieval of a picture probes retrieval of the specific episode, in which the picture was presented. In contrast, fear retrieval relies on the retrieval of the category-threat association, which does not rely on retrieval of these specific episodic elements, but could be semantic in nature, as retrieval takes place at a conceptual level. We have added a small note on what we mean with episodic in this context on page 4. We do agree that we cannot investigate other aspects of episodic memories here, such as context, as this was not manipulated in this experiment.

      (5) Twice as many female participants than males

      It is indeed unfortunate that there is no equal distribution between female and male participants. Investigating sex differences was not the goal of this study, but we do hope that future studies with the appropriate sample sizes are able to investigate this specifically. We have added this to the limitations of this study on page 17.

      (6) No explanation as to why shocks were varied in intensity and how (pseudo-randomly?)

      The shock determination procedure is explained on pages 18-19 (Peripheral stimulation). As is common in fear conditioning studies in humans (see references), an ascending staircase procedure was used. The goal of this procedure is to try and equalize the subjective experience of the electrical shocks to be “maximally uncomfortable but not painful”.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1:

      Very well written. No additional comments

      We thank reviewer 1 for valuing our original manuscript version. To further improve the manuscript, we adapted the current version based on the reviewer’s public review (see response to reviewer #1 public review comment 1).

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) I feel that more justification/explanation is needed on why other regions highly relevant to different aspects of counterconditioning (e.g., threat, memory, reward processing) were not included in the analyses.

      We first performed whole-brain analyses to get a general idea of the different neural mechanisms of CC compared to Ext. Clusters revealing significant group differences were then further investigated by means of preregistered ROI analyses. We included regions that have previously been shown to be most relevant for affective processing/threat responding (amygdala), memory (hippocampus), reward processing (NAcc) and regular extinction (vmPFC). We restricted our analyses to these most relevant ROIs as preregistered to prevent inflated or false-positive findings[5]. Beyond these preregistered ROIs, we applied appropriate whole-brain FEW corrections. The activated regions are listed in Supplementary Table 1 and include additional regions that were expected, such as the ACC and insula.

      (2) Were there observed differences across participants in the experiment? Any information on variance in the data such as how individual differences might influence these findings would provide a richer understanding of counterconditioning and increase the depth of interpretation for a broad readership.

      We agree that investigating individual differences is crucial to gain a better understanding of treatment efficacy in the framework of personalized medicine. Specifically, future research should aim to identify factors that help predict which treatment will be most effective for a particular patient. The results of this study provide a good basis for this, as we could show that the vmPFC in contrast to regular extinction, is not required in CC to improve the retention of safety memory. Therefore, this provides a viable option for patients who are not responding to treatments that rely on the vmPFC. In addition, as noted by Reviewer 1, in both groups a few participants show the opposite pattern (see Figure 3). It will be crucial to identify who will respond to treatments based on the principles of standard extinction or counterconditioning. We have added this point in the discussion on page 14.

      (3) While most figures are informative and clear, Figure 3 would benefit from detailed axis labels and a more descriptive caption. Currently, it is challenging to navigate the results presented to support the findings related to differential PDRs. A supplementary figure consolidating key patterns across conditions might also further facilitate understanding of this rather complicated result.

      We have made some changes to the figure to improve readability and understanding. Specifically, we changed the figure caption to “Change from last 2 trials CC/Ext to first 2 trials Spontaneous recovery test”, to give more details on what exactly is shown here. We also simplified the x-axis labels to “counterconditioning”, “recovery test” and “extinction”. With the addition of a clearer figure description, we hope to have improved understanding and do not think that another supplemental figure is needed.

      (4) Additional details on the statistical tests are needed. For example, please clarify whether p-values reported were corrected across all experimental conditions. Also, it would be helpful for the authors to discuss why for example repeated measures ANOVA or mixed-effects conditions were not used in this study. Might those tests not capture variance across participants' PDRs and SCRs over time better?

      We added that significant interactions were followed by Bonferroni-adjusted post-hoc tests where applicable (see page 21). We have used repeated measures ANOVAs to capture early versus late phases of acquisition and CC/extinction, as well as to compare late CC/extinction (last 2 trials) compared to early spontaneous recovery (first 2 trials) as is often done in the literature. A trial-level factor in a small sample would cost too many degrees of freedom and is not expected to provide more information. We have added this information and our reasoning to the methods section on page 21.

      Reviewer #3:

      (1) Suggest putting acquisition data into the main figures. In fact many of the supplemental figures could be integrated into the main figures in my opinion.

      See response to reviewer #3 public review comment 1.

      (2) Include explanations for why shock intensity was varied

      See response to reviewer #3 public review comment 6.

      (3) Include a better explanation for the change in differential responding from training to spontaneous recovery in the CC group (I think the loss of such responding in extinction makes more sense and is supported by the notion of spontaneous recovery, but I'm not sure about the loss in the CC group. There is some evidence from the rodent literature - which I am most familiar with - regarding a loss in contextual gradient across time which could account for some loss in specificity, could it be something like this?).

      See response to reviewer #3 public review comment 3.

      If we understand the reviewer correctly in that the we see a loss of differential responding due to a generalization to the CS-, this would imply an increase in responding to the CS-, which is not what we see. Our data should therefore be correctly interpreted as a loss of the specific response to the CS+ from the CC phase to the recovery test. Therefore, there is no spontaneous recovery in the CC group, and also not a non-specific recovery. To clarify this we relabeled Figure 3 by indicating “recovery test” instead of “spontaneous recovery”.

      (4) Is there a possibility that baseline differences, particularly that in Supplemental Figure 2C, could account for later differences? If differences persist after some transformation (e.g. percentage of baseline responding) this would be convincing to suggest that it doesn't.

      See response to reviewer #3 public review comment 1.

      (5) As I mentioned, I got confused by the chronology as I read through. Maybe mention early on when reporting the spontaneous recovery results that testing occurred the next day and that participants were undergoing re-extinction when talking about it for the second time.

      See response to reviewer #3 public review comment 2.

      (6) Page 8 - I was confused as to why it is surprising that the CC group were more aroused than the extinction group, the latter have not had CSs paired with anything with any valence, so doesn't this make sense? Or perhaps I am misunderstanding the results - here in text the authors refer back to Figure 2B, but I'm not sure if this is showing data from the spontaneous recovery test or from CC/extinction. If it is the latter, as the caption suggests, why are the authors referring to it here?

      Participants in the CC group showed increased differential self-reported arousal after CC, whereas arousal ratings did not differ between CS+ and CS- items after extinction. We interpret this in line with the valence and PDR results as an indication of reward-induced arousal. At the start of the next day, however, participants from the CC and extinction groups gave comparable ratings. It may therefore be surprising why participants in the CC group do not still show stronger ratings since nothing happened between these two ratings besides a night’s sleep (see design overview in Figure 1A). We removed the “suprisingly” to prevent any confusion.

      (7) I suggest that the authors comment on whether there were any gender differences in their results.

      See response to reviewer #3 public review comment 5.

      (8) The study makes several claims about episodic memory, but how can the authors be sure that the memories they are tapping into are episodic? Episodic has a very specific meaning - a biographical, contextually-based memory, whereas the information being encoded here could be semantic. Perhaps a bit of clarification around this issue could be helpful.

      See response to reviewer #3 public review comment 4.

      References

      (1) Dunsmoor, J. E. & Kroes, M. C. W. Episodic memory and Pavlovian conditioning: ships passing in the night. Curr Opin Behav Sci 26, 32-39 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.09.019

      (2) Dunsmoor, J. E. et al. Event segmentation protects emotional memories from competing experiences encoded close in time. Nature Human Behaviour 2, 291-299 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0317-4

      (3) Dunsmoor, J. E., Murty, V. P., Clewett, D., Phelps, E. A. & Davachi, L. Tag and capture: how salient experiences target and rescue nearby events in memory. Trends Cogn Sci 26, 782-795 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2022.06.009

      (4) Dunsmoor, J. E., Murty, V. P., Davachi, L. & Phelps, E. A. Emotional learning selectively and retroactively strengthens memories for related events. Nature 520, 345-348 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14106

      (5) Gentili, C., Cecchetti, L., Handjaras, G., Lettieri, G. & Cristea, I. A. The case for preregistering all region of interest (ROI) analyses in neuroimaging research. Eur J Neurosci 53, 357-361 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14954

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Audio et al. measured cerebral blood volume (CBV) across cortical areas and layers using high-resolution MRI with contrast agents in non-human primates. While the non-invasive CBV MRI methodology is often used to enhance fMRI sensitivity in NHPs, its application for baseline CBV measurement is rare due to the complexities of susceptibility contrast mechanisms. The authors determined the number of large vessels and the areal and laminar variations of CBV in NHP, and compared those with various other metrics.

      Strengths:

      Noninvasive mapping of relative cerebral blood volume is novel for non-human primates. A key finding was the observation of variations in CBV across regions; primary sensory cortices had high CBV, whereas other higher areas had low CBV. The measured CBV values correlated with previously reported neuronal and receptor densities.

      We appreciate your recognition of the novelty of our non-invasive relative cerebral blood volume (CBV) mapping in non-human primates, as well as the observed areal variations and their correlations with neuronal and receptor densities. However, we are concerned that key contributions of our work—such as cortical layer-specific vasculature mapping and benchmarking surface vessel density estimations against anatomical ground truth—are being framed as limitations rather than significant advances in the field pushing the boundaries of current neuroimaging capabilities and providing a valuable foundation for future research. Additionally, we would like to clarify that dynamic susceptibility contrast (DSC) MRI using gadolinium is the gold standard for CBV measurement in clinical settings and the argument that “baseline CBV measurements are rare due to the complexities of susceptibility contrast” is simply not true. The limited use of ferumoxytol for CBV imaging is primarily due to previous FDA regulatory restrictions, rather than inherent methodological shortcomings.

      Changes in text:

      Compared to clinically used gadolinium-based agents, ferumoxytol's substantially longer half-life and stronger R<sub>2</sub>* effect allows for higher-resolution and more sensitive vascular volume measurements (Buch et al., 2022), albeit these methodologies are hampered by confounding factors such as vessel orientation relative to the magnetic field (B<sub>0</sub>) direction (Ogawa et al., 1993).

      Weaknesses:

      A weakness of this manuscript is that the quantification of CBV with postprocessing approaches to remove susceptibility effects from pial and penetrating vessels is not fully validated, especially on a laminar scale. Further specific comments follow.

      (1) Baseline CBV indices were determined using contrast agent-enhanced MRI (deltaR<sub>2</sub>*). Although this approach is suitable for areal comparisons, its application at a laminar scale poses challenges due to significant contributions from large vessels including pial vessels. The primary concern is whether large-vessel contributions can be removed from the measured deltaR<sub>2</sub>* through processing techniques.

      Eliminating the contribution of large vessels completely is unlikely, and we agree with the reviewer that ΔR<sub>2</sub>* results likely reflect a weighted combination of signals from both large vessels and capillaries. However, the distribution of ΔR<sub>2</sub>* more closely aligns with capillary density in areas V1–V5 than with large vessel distributions (Weber et al., 2008), suggesting that our ΔR<sub>2</sub>* results are more weighted toward capillaries. Moreover, we demonstrated that the pial vessel induced signal-intensity drop-outs are clearly limited to the superficial layers and exhibit smaller spatial extent than generally thought (Supp. Figs. 2 and 4).

      (2) High-resolution MRI with a critical sampling frequency estimated from previous studies (Weber 2008, Zheng 1991) was performed to separate penetrating vessels. However, this approach is still insufficient to accurately identify the number of vessels due to the blooming effects of susceptibility and insufficient spatial resolution. The reported number of penetrating vessels is only applicable to the experimental and processing conditions used in this study, which cannot be generalized.

      Our intention was not to suggest that our measurements provide a general estimate of vessel density across the macaque cerebral cortex. At 0.23 mm isotropic resolution, we successfully delineated approximately 30% of the penetrating vessels in V1. Our primary objective was to demonstrate a proof-of-concept quantifiable measurement rather than to establish a generalized vessel density metric for all brain regions. We have consistently emphasized this throughout the manuscript, but if there is a specific point of misunderstanding, we would be happy to consider revisions for clarity.

      (3) Baseline R<sub>2</sub>* is sensitive to baseline R<sub>2</sub>, vascular volume, iron content, and susceptibility gradients. Additionally, it is sensitive to imaging parameters; higher spatial resolution tends to result in lower R<sub>2</sub>* values (closer to the R<sub>2</sub> value). Thus, it is difficult to correlate baseline R<sub>2</sub>* with physiological parameters.

      The observed correlation between R<sub>2</sub>* and neuron density is likely indirect, as R<sub>2</sub>* is strongly influenced by iron, myelin, and deoxyhemoglobin densities. However, the robust correlation between R<sub>2</sub>* and neuron density, peaking in the superficial layers (R = 0.86, p < 10<sup>-10</sup>), is striking and difficult to ignore (revised Supp. Fig. 6D-E). Upon revision, we identified an error in Supp. Fig. 6D-E, where the previous version used single-subject R<sub>2</sub>* and ΔR<sub>2</sub>* maps instead of the group-averaged maps. The revised correlations are slightly stronger than in the earlier version.

      Given that the correlation between neuron density and R<sub>2</sub>* is strongest in the superficial layers, we suggest this relationship reflects an underlying association with tissue cytochrome oxidase (CO) activity and cumulative effect of deoxygenated venous blood drainage toward the pial network. The superficial cortical layers are also less influenced by myelin and iron densities, which are more concentrated in the deeper cortical layers. Additional factors may contribute to this relationship, including the iron dependence of mitochondrial CO activity, as iron is an essential component of CO’s heme groups. Moreover, myelin maintenance depends on iron, which is predominantly stored in oligodendrocytes. The presence of myelinated thin axons and a higher axonal surface density may, in turn, be a prerequisite for high neuron density.

      In this context, it is also valuable to note the absolute range of superficial R<sub>2</sub>* values (≈ 6 s<sup>-1</sup>; Supp. Fig. 6D). This variation in cortical surface R<sub>2</sub>* is about 12-30 times larger compared to the signal changes observed during task-based fMRI (6 vs. 0.2-0.5 s<sup>-1</sup>). This relation seems reasonable because regional increases in absolute blood flow associated with imaging signals, as measured by PET, typically do not exceed 5%–10% of the brain's resting blood flow (Raichle and Mintum 2016; Brain work and brain imaging). The venous oxygenation level is typically 60%, with task-induced activation increasing it by only a few percent. We suggest that this is ~40% oxygen extraction is reflected in the superficial R<sub>2</sub>*. Finally, the large intercept (≈ 14.5 1/s; Supp. Fig. 6D), which is not equivalent to the water R<sub>2</sub>* (≈ 1 1/s), suggests that R<sub>2</sub>* is influenced by substantial non-neuron density factors, such as receptor, myelin, iron, susceptibility gradients and spatial resolution.

      The R<sub>2</sub>* values are well known to be influenced by intra-voxel phase coherence and thus spatial resolution. However, our view is that the proposed methodology of acquiring cortical-layer thickness adjusted high-resolution (spin-echo) R<sub>2</sub> maps poses more methodological limitations and is less practical. Notwithstanding, to further corroborate the relationship between R<sub>2</sub>* and neuron density, we investigated whether a similar correlation exists in non-quantitative T2w SPACE-FLAIR images (0.32 mm isotropic) signal-intensity and neuron density. Using B<sub>1</sub> bias-field and B<sub>0</sub> orientation bias corrected T2w SPACE-FLAIR images (N=7), we parcellated the equivolumetric surface maps using Vanderbilt sections. Our findings showed that signal intensity—where regions with high signal intensity correspond to low R<sub>2</sub> values, and areas with low signal intensity correspond to high R<sub>2</sub> values—was positively correlated with neuron density, particularly in the superficial layers (R = 0.77, p = 10<sup>-11</sup>; Author response image 1).This analysis confirmed the correlation with neuron density and R<sub>2</sub> peaks at superficial layers. However, this correlation was slightly weaker compared to quantitative R<sub>2</sub>* (Supp. Fig. 6D), suggesting the variable flip-angle spin-echo train refocused signal-phase coherence loss from large draining vessels or that non-quantitative T2w-FLAIR images may be confounded by other factors such as B<sub>1</sub> transmission field biases (Glasser et al., 2022). Notwithstanding, this non-quantitative fast spin-echo with variable flip-angles approach, which is in principle less dependent on image resolution and closer to R<sub>2,intrinsic</sub> than R<sub>2</sub>*, yields similar findings in comparison to quantitative gradient-echo.

      Author response image 1.

      (A) T2w-FLAIR SPACE normalized signal-intensity plotted vs neuron density. Note that low signal-intensity corresponds to high R<sub>2</sub> and high neuron density, consistent with findings using ME-GRE. (B) Correlation between T2w-FLAIR SPACE and neuron density across equivolumetric layers. Notably, a similar relationship with neuron density was observed using a variable spin-echo pulse sequence as with quantitative gradient-echo-based imaging.

      Changes in text:

      Results:

      “Because the Julich cortical area atlas covers only a section of the cerebral cortex, and the neuron density estimates are interpolated maps, we extended our analysis using the original Collins sample borders encompassing the entire cerebral cortex (Supp. Fig. 6A-C). This analysis reaffirmed the positive correlation with ΔR<sub>2</sub>* (peak at EL2, R = 0.80, p < 10<sup>-11</sup>) and baseline R<sub>2</sub>* (peak at EL2a, R = 0.86, p < 10<sup>-13</sup>), yielding linear coefficients of ΔR<sub>2</sub>* = 102 × 10<sup>3</sup> neurons/s and R<sub>2</sub>* = 41 × 10<sup>3</sup> neurons/s (Supp. Fig. 6D-G). This suggests that the sensitivity of quantitative layer R<sub>2</sub>* MRI in detecting neuronal loss is relatively weak, and the introduction of the Ferumoxytol contrast agent has the potential to enhance this sensitivity by a factor of 2.5.”

      A new paragraph was added into discussion section 4.3 corroborating the relation between R<sub>2</sub>* and neuron density:

      “Another key finding of this study was the strong correlation between baseline R<sub>2</sub>* and neuron density (Supp. Fig. 6D, E). While R<sub>2</sub>* is well known to be influenced by iron, myelin, and deoxyhemoglobin densities, this correlation peaks in the superficial layers (Supp. Fig. 6E), suggesting a link to CO activity and the accumulation of deoxygenated venous blood draining from all cortical layers toward the pial network. Notably, the absolute range of superficial R<sub>2</sub>* values (max - min ≈ 6 s<sup>-1</sup>; Supp. Fig. 6D) is approximately 12-30 times larger than the ΔR<sub>2</sub>* observed during task-based BOLD fMRI at 3T (0.2-0.5 1/s) (Yablonskiy and Haacke 1994). Since venous oxygenation is around 60% and task-induced changes in blood flow account for only 5%–10% of the brain's resting blood flow (Raichle & Mintun, 2006), these results suggest that superficial R<sub>2</sub>* (Fig. 1D) may serve as a more accurate proxy for total deoxyhemoglobin content (and thus total oxygen consumption), which scales with the neuron density of the underlying cortical gray matter. Importantly, superficial layers may also provide a more specific measure of deoxyhemoglobin, as they are less influenced by myelin and iron, which are more concentrated in deeper cortical layers. Additionally, smaller but direct contributors, such as mitochondrial CO density—an iron-dependent factor—may also play a role in this relationship.”

      References:

      Raichle, M.E., Mintun, M.A., 2006. BRAIN WORK AND BRAIN IMAGING. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 29, 449–476. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.112819

      (4) CBV-weighted deltaR<sub>2</sub>* is correlated with various other metrics (cytoarchitectural parcellation, myelin/receptor density, cortical thickness, CO, cell-type specificity, etc.). While testing the correlation between deltaR<sub>2</sub>* and these other metrics may be acceptable as an exploratory analysis, it is challenging for readers to discern a causal relationship between them. A critical question is whether CBV-weighted deltaR<sub>2</sub>* can provide insights into other metrics in diseased or abnormal brain states.

      We acknowledge that having multivariate analysis using dense histological maps would be valuable to establish causality among these several metrics:

      “To comprehensively understand the factors contributing to the vascular organization of the brain, experimental disentanglement through multivariate analysis of laminar cell types and receptor densities is needed (Hayashi et al., 2021, Froudist-Walsh et al., 2023). Moreover, employing more advanced statistical modeling, including considerations for synapse-neuron interactions, may be important for refined evaluations.”

      We think the primary contributors to the brain's energy budget are neurons and receptors, as shown in several references and stated in the manuscript. To investigate relationship between neuron density and CBV, we estimated the energy budget allocated to neurons and extrapolated the remaining CBV to other contributing factors:

      Changes in text:

      “However, this is a simplified estimation, and a more comprehensive assessment would need to account for an aggregate of biophysical factors such as neuron types, neuron membrane surface area, firing rates, dendritic and synaptic densities (Fig. 6F-G), neurotransmitter recycling, and other cell types (Kageyama 1982; Elston and Rose 1997; Perge et al., 2009; Harris et al., 2012). Indeed, the majority of the mitochondria reside in the dendrites and synaptic transmission is widely acknowledged to drive the majority of the energy consumption and blood flow (Wong-Riley, 1989; Attwell et al., 2001).

      Extrapolating cortical ΔR<sub>2</sub>* to zero neuron density results in a large intercept (~35 1/s), corresponding to 60% of the maximum cortical CBV (57 1/s; Supp. Fig. 6F). This supports the view that the majority of energy consumption occurs in the neuropil—comprising dendrites, synapses, and axons—which accounts for ~80–90% of cortical gray matter volume, whereas neuronal somata constitute only ~10–20% (Wong-Riley, 1989). Although neuronal cell bodies exhibit higher CO activity per unit volume due to their dense mitochondrial content, these results suggest their overall contribution to the total CBV per mm<sup>3</sup> tissue remains lower than that of the neuropil, given the latter's substantially larger volume fraction in cortical tissue.

      Contrary to our initial expectations, we observed a relatively smaller CBV in regions and layers with high receptor density (Fig. 6B, D, F). This relationship extends to other factors, such as number of spines (putative excitatory inputs) and dendrite tree size across the entire cerebral cortex (Supp. Fig. 7) (Froudist-Walsh et al., 2023, Elston 2007). These results align with the work of Weber and colleagues, who reported a similar negative correlation between vascular length density and synaptic density, as well as a positive correlation with neuron density in macaque V1 across cortical layers (Weber et al., 2008).”

      Variations in neurons and receptors are reflected in cytoarchitecture, myelin (axon density likely scales with neuron density and myelin inhibits synaptic connections), and cell-type composition. For example, fast-spiking parvalbumin interneurons, which target the soma or axon hillock, are well-suited for regulating activity in regions with high neuron density, whereas bursting calretinin interneurons, which target distal dendrites, are more adapted to areas with high synaptic density. These factors in turn, gradually change along the cortical hierarchy level (higher levels have thinner cortical layer IV, more complex dendrite trees and more numerous inter-areal connectivity patterns). In our view, these factors are tightly interlinked and explain the strong correlations and metabolic demands observed across different metrics.

      We also agree that cortical layer imaging of vasculature in diseased or abnormal brain states is an intriguing direction for future research; however, it falls beyond the scope of the present study.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript presents a new approach for non-invasive, MRI-based, measurements of cerebral blood volume (CBV). Here, the authors use ferumoxytol, a high-contrast agent and apply specific sequences to infer CBV. The authors then move to statistically compare measured regional CBV with known distribution of different types of neurons, markers of metabolic load and others. While the presented methodology captures and estimated 30% of the vasculature, the authors corroborated previous findings regarding lack of vascular compartmentalization around functional neuronal units in the primary visual cortex.

      Strengths:

      Non invasive methodology geared to map vascular properties in vivo.

      Implementation of a highly sensitive approach for measuring blood volume.

      Ability to map vascular structural and functional vascular metrics to other types of published data.

      Weaknesses:

      The key issue here is the underlying assumption about the appropriate spatial sampling frequency needed to captures the architecture of the brain vasculature. Namely, ~7 penetrating vessels / mm2 as derived from Weber et al 2008 (Cer Cor). The cited work, begins by characterizing the spacing of penetrating arteries and ascending veins using vascular cast of 7 monkeys (Macaca mulatta, same as in the current paper). The ~7 penetrating vessels / mm2 is computed by dividing the total number of identified vessels by the area imaged. The problem here is that all measurements were made in a "non-volumetric" manner and only in V1. Extrapolating from here to the entire brain seems like an over-assumption, particularly given the region-dependent heterogeneity that the current paper reports.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concerns regarding spatial sampling frequency and its implications for characterizing brain vasculature, which we investigated in this study. To clarify, our analysis of surface vessel density was explicitly restricted to V1 precisely due to the limitations of our experimental precision. While we reported the total number of vessels identified in the cortex, we intentionally chose not to present density values across regions in this manuscript. Although these calculations are feasible, we focused on the data directly analyzed and avoided extrapolating density values beyond the scope of our findings. Thus, we are uncertain about the suggestion that we extrapolated vessel density values across the entire brain, as we have taken care to limit our conclusions of our vessel density precision to V1.

      Regarding methodology, we conducted two independent analyses of vessel density specifically in V1. The first involved volumetric analysis using the Frangi filter, while the second used surface-based analysis of local signal-intensity gradients (as illustrated in Fig. 2E and Supp. Figs. 3 and 4), albeit the final surface density analysis is performed using the ultra-high resolution equivolumetric layers. Notably, these two approaches produced consistent and comparable vessel density estimates, supporting the reliability of our findings within the scope of V1 (we found 30% of the vessels relative to the ground-truth).

      Comments on revisions:

      I appreciate the effort made to improve the manuscript. That said, the direct validation of the underlying assumption about spatial resolution sampling remains unaddressed in the final version of this manuscript. With the only intention to further strengthen the methodology presented here, I would encourage again the authors to seek a direct validation of this assumption for other brain areas.

      In their reply, the authors stated "... line scanning or single-plane sequences, at least on first impression, seem inadequate for whole-brain coverage and cortical surface mapping. ". This seems to emanate for a misunderstanding as the method could be used to validate the mapping, not to map per-se.

      We apologize for any misunderstanding in our previous response and appreciate your clarification. We now understand that you were suggesting the use of line-scanning or single-plane sequences as a method to validate, rather than map, our spatial sampling assumptions.

      We agree that single-plane sequences at very high in-plane resolution (e.g., 50 × 50 × 1000 µm) have great potential to detect penetrating vessels and even vessel branching patterns. These techniques could indeed provide valuable insights into region-specific vessel density variations which could then be used to validate whole brain 3D acquisitions. However, as noted above, we have refrained from reporting vessel densities outside V1 precisely due to sampling limitations (we only found 30% of the penetrating vessels in V1, or only 2 mm<sup>2</sup>/30mm<sup>2</sup> ≈ 7% of branching vessel ground-truth, see discussion).

      We acknowledge the merit of incorporating such methods to validate regional vessel densities and agree that this would be an important avenue for future research. Thank you for suggesting this point, we have briefly mentioned the advantage of single-plane EPI at discussion.

      Changes in text:

      “4.1 Methodological considerations - vessel density informed MRI

      …anatomical studies accounting for branching patterns have reported much higher vessel densities up to 30 vessels/mm<sup>2</sup> (Keller et al., 2011; Adams et al., 2015). Further investigations are warranted, taking into account critical sampling frequencies associated with vessel branching patterns (Duverney 1981), and achieving higher SNR through ultra-high B<sub>0</sub> MRI (Bolan et al., 2006; Harel et al., 2010; Kim et al., 2013) and utilize high-resolution single-plane sequences and prospective motion correction schemes to accurately characterize regional vessel densities. Such advancements hold promise for improving vessel quantification, classifications for veins and arteries and constructing detailed cortical surface maps of the vascular networks which may have diagnostic and neurosurgical utilities (Fig. 2A, B) (Iadecola, 2013; Qi and Roper, 2021; Sweeney et al., 2018).”

      During the revision we found a typo and corrected it in Supp. Fig. 8: Dosal -> Dorsal.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      eLife Assessment

      This study provides useful findings about the effects of heterozygosity for Trio variants linked to neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders in mice. However, the strength of the evidence is limited and incomplete mainly because the experimental flow is difficult to follow, raising concerns about the conclusions' robustness. Clearer connections between variables, such as sex, age, behavior, brain regions, and synaptic measures, and more methodological detail on breeding strategies, test timelines, electrophysiology, and analysis, are needed to support their claims.

      We appreciate the opportunity to address the constructive feedback provided by eLife and the reviewers. Below, we respond to the overall assessment and individual reviewers' comments, clarifying our experimental approach, addressing concerns, and providing additional details where necessary.

      We thank the editors for highlighting the significance of our findings regarding the effects of Trio variant heterozygosity in mice. We acknowledge the feedback concerning the experimental flow and agree that clarity is paramount. To address these concerns:

      (1) Connections between variables: The word limit of the initial submission constrained our ability to provide adequate details and connections between variables. We have revised the manuscript to explicitly outline and extend explanations and the relationships between sex, age, behavior, brain regions, and synaptic measures, ensuring that the rationale for each experiment and its relevance to the overall conclusions are improved.

      (2) Methodological details: The Methods section of our initial submission was condensed, with key details provided in the Supplemental Methods section. We have merged all into an extended section to improve clarity. We have expanded our description of breeding strategies, test timelines, electrophysiological protocols, and data analysis methods in the revised Methods section. We believe the additions have enhanced the transparency and reproducibility of our study and ensured full support of our conclusions.

      (3) Experimental flow: We have revised and extended our results, methods, and discussion sections to clarify the rationale and experimental design to guide readers through the experimental sequence and rationale.

      We are confident these revisions address the concerns raised and enhance the robustness and coherence of our findings.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study explores how heterozygosity for specific neurodevelopmental disorder-associated Trio variants affects mouse behavior, brain structure, and synaptic function, revealing distinct impacts on motor, social, and cognitive behaviors linked to clinical phenotypes. Findings demonstrate that Trio variants yield unique changes in synaptic plasticity and glutamate release, highlighting Trio's critical role in presynaptic function and the importance of examining variant heterozygosity in vivo.

      Strengths:

      This study generated multiple mouse lines to model each Trio variant, reflecting point mutations observed in human patients with developmental disorders. The authors employed various approaches to evaluate the resulting behavioral, neuronal morphology, synaptic function, and proteomic phenotypes.

      Weaknesses:

      While the authors present extensive results, the flow of experiments is challenging to follow, raising concerns about the strength of the experimental conclusions. Additionally, the connection between sex, age, behavioral data, brain regions, synaptic transmission, and plasticity lacks clarity, making it difficult to understand the rationale behind each experiment. Clearer explanations of the purpose and connections between experiments are recommended. Furthermore, the methodology requires more detail, particularly regarding mouse breeding strategies, timelines for behavioral tests, electrophysiology conditions, and data analysis procedures.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s recognition of the novelty and comprehensiveness of our approach, particularly the generation of multiple mouse lines and our efforts to model Trio variant effects in vivo.

      Weaknesses

      (1) Experimental flow and rationale and connection between variables: We have expanded on the connections between behavioral data, neuronal morphology, synaptic function, and proteomics in the Results and Discussion sections to clarify how each experiment informs the reasoning and the conclusions and to highlight the relationships between sex, age, behavior, and synaptic measures.

      (2) Methodological details: Our initial Methods section was formatted to be short to fulfill word limits on the submitted version, with additional details provided in the Supplemental Methods section. We have merged our Methods and Supplemental Methods sections and expanded on our breeding strategies, test timelines, electrophysiological protocols, and data analysis. We believe these additions enhance the transparency and reproducibility of our study.

      (3) Recommendations for the authors: We thank Reviewer #1 for providing several recommendations to improve our manuscript. We have addressed their comments in the revision, as detailed below, adding key experiments that bolster our findings.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors generated three mouse lines harboring ASD, Schizophrenia, and Bipolar-associated variants in the TRIO gene. Anatomical, behavioral, physiological, and biochemical assays were deployed to compare and contrast the impact of these mutations in these animals. In this undertaking, the authors sought to identify and characterize the cellular and molecular mechanisms responsible for ASD, Schizophrenia, and Bipolar disorder development.

      Strengths:

      The establishment of TRIO dysfunction in the development of ASD, Schizophrenia, and Bipolar disorder is very recent and of great interest. Disorder-specific variants have been identified in the TRIO gene, and this study is the first to compare and contrast the impact of these variants in vivo in preclinical models. The impact of these mutations was carefully examined using an impressive host of methods. The authors achieved their goal of identifying behavioral, physiological, and molecular alterations that are disorder/variant specific. The impact of this work is extremely high given the growing appreciation of TRIO dysfunction in a large number of brain-related disorders. This work is very interesting in that it begins to identify the unique and subtle ways brain function is altered in ASD, Schizophrenia, and Bipolar disorder.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Most assays were performed in older animals and perhaps only capture alterations that result from homeostatic changes resulting from prodromal pathology that may look very different.

      (2) Identification of upregulated (potentially compensating) genes in response to these disorder-specific Trio variants is extremely interesting. However, a functional demonstration of compensation is not provided.

      (3) There are instances where data is not shown in the manuscript. See "data not shown". All data collected should be provided even if significant differences are not observed.

      I consider weaknesses 1 and 2 minor. While they would be very interesting to explore, these experiments might be more appropriate for a follow-up study. I would recommend that the missing data in 3 should be provided in the supplemental material.

      We are grateful for the reviewer’s recognition of our study’s significance and methodological rigor. The acknowledgment of Trio dysfunction as a novel and impactful area of research is deeply appreciated.

      Weaknesses:

      We agree that focusing on older animals limits insights into early-stage pathophysiology. However, our goal in this study was to examine the functional impacts of Trio heterozygosity at an adolescent stage and to reveal the ultimate impact of these alleles on synaptic function. Our choice of age aligns with our objectives. Future studies of earlier developmental stages will be beneficial and complement these findings.

      Functional compensation:

      We tested functional compensation through rescue experiments in +/K1431M brain slices using a Rac1-specific inhibitor, NSC23766, which prevents Rac1 activation by Trio or Tiam1. Our finding that direct Rac1 inhibition normalizes deficient neurotransmitter release in +/K1431M mice strongly suggests that increased Rac1 activity drives this phenotype.

      Data not shown:

      We will incorporate all previously shown data into the Supplemental Materials, even when results are nonsignificant. We agree that this ensures full transparency and facilitates a more comprehensive evaluation of our findings.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) In Figure 1K-N, the lack of observed differences in +/M2145T mice across all tests raises questions about its validity as a BPD model. Furthermore, the differences in female behavior data compared to males, as shown in the Supplemental section, lack clarification-specifically, whether these variations are due to sex differences or sample size disparities, which is not discussed. Additionally, it's unclear if the same mice were used in tests K through L-N, as the reported numbers differ without explanation; if relevant, any mortality should be reported. Given the observed body weight differences, it is important to display locomotor data, despite the mention of no change in open field results. Lastly, a detailed breeding strategy and timeline for behavioral testing would enhance clarity.

      We thank Reviewer 1 for recognizing these confusing points in our behavioral data and seek to add clarification in our Revision as below:

      (a) We have revised the text to emphasize our goal to evaluate the impact of NDD-related Trio alleles that have discrete and measurable effects on brain development and function, and not to model specific NDDs (e.g. ASD, SCZ, or BPD). The three specific Trio mutations were chosen based on strong evidence of these mutations impairing the biochemical functions of Trio. We reasoned our approach would reveal how impairing Trio in different ways – i.e. altering protein level or GEF1/GEF2 function – and under genetic conditions (heterozygosity) that mimic those found in individuals with Trio-related disorders impacts brain development and function. The lack of behavioral phenotypes in +/M2145T mice is indeed intriguing, especially given the alterations in electrophysiology and biochemistry experiments. It remains possible that further behavioral analyses of these mice will reveal behavioral phenotypes.

      (b) Given that the prevalence and clinical presentation of individuals with various NDDs are influenced by sex, it is possible that the behavioral differences we see in male versus female Trio variant mice reflect human sex difference phenotypes. We have reorganized the Figure panels to clarify these sex differences in behaviors (new Fig. 2, Supp. Fig. 2). We focused on the most significant behavioral phenotypes shared by both sexes in the main text, or in males alone, as our anatomical and electrophysiological experiments were restricted to males to reduce variation due to estrus. The observed behavioral sex differences are not likely due to sample size disparities as power analyses were performed for all experimental results to ensure adequate sample size. A comprehensive study of the mechanisms underlying these behavioral findings merits examination but is outside the scope of this study.

      (c) All mice were subjected to all behavioral tests described. No sudden mortality was observed during the behavioral experiments. Outliers in post-hoc statistical analyses were removed, which explains the apparent sample size differences between behavioral tests. We have revised the Data analysis section in our Methods to include these details (Lines 216-289, 450-457).

      (d) Results of the open field test have been added to the Supplemental Data (new Supp. Fig. 2) and Results (Lines 532-537)

      (e) The Methods section was expanded to include more detail on the breeding strategy (Lines 98-106). A timeline for behavioral testing has also been included in the Figures to enhance clarity (new Fig. 2A).

      (2) In Figure 2A-E, head width and brain weight showed significant differences, but not body weight, how come the ratio does not change? Comparing with female results in Supplementary Figure 2A-E, it does show a difference between males and females. It is essential to clarify which sex authors use in all follow-up experiments, including synapse, transmission, and plasticity. Since the males and females have different phenotypes, why do the authors focus on males only? The E plot has no data points on the bar graph. In Figure 2I, it lacks example images for all four conditions.

      We greatly appreciate this Reviewer’s attention to details in our brain and body weight data and revised the manuscript to address these concerns.

      (a) The ratios of head width/body weight were calculated for each individual mouse. Hence the distribution of the ratio data (old Fig. 2D; new Fig. 3D) differs from the distribution of head width or body weight data alone (old Fig. 2A, 2C, resp.; now Fig. 3A, 3C), and therefore can affect the p-value for statistical significance. The body weight of +/M2145T males is 21.217 ±0.327 g, while for WT males is 21.745 ±0.224 g, a non-significant decrease of 0.528 g (adjusted p=0.3806). These values have been added to the Fig 3. figure legend (Lines 1020-1034) for clarity.

      (b) Similar to the behavioral experiments in comment (1), we observed sex differences in head width, brain weight, and body weight in Trio heterozygous variant mice compared to WT counterparts. The differences in the ratios of head width/body weight or brain weight/body weight were the same for both males and females (i.e. head width/body weight ratio is decreased in +/K1431M mice compared to WT regardless of sex, and brain weight/body weight ratio is decreased in both +/K1431M and +/K1918X mice compared to WT regardless of sex). These findings affirm the impact of Trio mutations on these phenotypes across both sexes. We have modified the text to draw more attention to this key point (Lines 554-566 and 777-801).

      (c) All experiments (excluding behavior and weight data) were performed in males only to minimize the variation in spine and synapse morphology and physiological activity that can occur due to estrus. We have clarified this in the ‘Animal Work’ section of the Methods (Lines 103-106) as well as in the Figure Legends.

      (d) We thank the Reviewer for pointing out Fig. 3E lacks individual data points on the bar graph. Fig. 3E has been modified to now include the brain weight/body weight ratio for each individual mouse rather than across the population, to be consistent with the calculation of head width/body weight ratio (see point 2a).

      On original submission, only a representative WT image was selected due to space constraints. The figure (new Fig. 3H and 3K) and figure legend have been revised to include representative traces for all genotypes examined.

      (3) In lines 315-320, "None of the Trio variant heterozygotes exhibited altered dendritic spine density on M1 L5 pyramidal neurons compared to WT mice on either apical or basal arbors (Supplementary Figure 3L, M). Electron microscopy of cortical area M1 L5 revealed that synapse density was significantly increased in +/K1918X mice compared to WT (Figure 3A, B), possibly due to a net reduction in neuropil resulting from smaller dendritic arbors." The proposed explanation does not adequately address the observed discrepancy between spine density and synapse density reported in these two experiments. A more thorough analysis is needed to reconcile these conflicting findings and clarify how these distinct measurements may relate to each other in the context of the study's conclusions.

      We acknowledge the apparent discrepancy between our dendritic spine density data, which is unchanged from WT for all three Trio variant heterozygotes, and our synapse density data, which showed an increase in +/K1918X M1 L5 compared to WT. We have expanded the explanation for this discrepancy below and added this to the Discussion (Lines 802-811):

      a) Because spine density can vary by dendritic branch order and distance from the soma, only protrusions from secondary dendritic arbors of M1 L5 pyramidal neurons were quantified for consistency in analyses. However, all synapses meeting criteria were quantified in EM images, regardless of where they were located along an individual neuron’s arbors. It is possible that the density and distribution of spines along other arbors are different between genotypes but was not captured in our current data.

      b) +/K1918X L5 pyramidal neurons are smaller and less complex than WT neurons, especially in the basal compartment corresponding to L5 where EM images were obtained, consistent with the smaller brain size and reduced cortical thickness of +/K1918X mice. We posit that due to their smaller dendritic field size, L5 neurons pack more densely contributing to the increased synapse density observed in +/K1918X M1 L5 cortex. Consistent with this hypothesis, we observed a trend toward increased DAPI+ cell density in M1 L5 of +/K1918X neurons (Supp. Fig. 3N).

      (4) In Figure 4, one potential rationale for measuring AMPAR mEPSC frequency is to infer synapse density changes. However, the findings show no frequency change in +/K1431M and +/K1918X, with an increase only in +/M2145T, which contradicts Figure 3 results indicating a trend toward increased density across variants.

      This inconsistency is confusing, especially since the authors claim to follow the methodology from the study "Trio Haploinsufficiency Causes Neurodevelopmental Disease-Associated Deficits"; yet, the observed mEPSC amplitude differs significantly from that study, while the frequency remains unaffected. Additionally, the NMDAR mEPSCs reflect combined AMPAR and NMDAR responses at positive holding potentials, with peak amplitude dominated by AMPAR. This inconsistency between holding potential results is unclear, as frequency should theoretically align across negative and positive potentials. For accurate NMDAR mEPSC measurement, it would be optimal to assess amplitude 50 ms post-initial peak and, if possible, increase the holding potential to enhance the driving force given the typically low signal of NMDAR response.

      We thank the Reviewer for highlighting these important points.

      a) Previous work from our lab and others demonstrate that Trio regulates synaptic AMPA receptor levels, which is why we chose to focus on AMPAR-mediated evoked and miniature EPSC frequencies and amplitudes in the current study. We acknowledge Reviewer 1’s comment on seemingly contradictory results regarding AMPAR mEPSC frequency and synapse density; however, the unchanged AMPAR mEPSC frequency in +/K1431M and +/K1918X mice is consistent with our finding of unaltered dendritic spine density in these mice compared to WT (Supp. Fig. 4L,M). The differences between dendritic spine counts and synapse density is addressed in Response (3) above.

      b) While synapse density changes can be inferred from AMPAR mEPSC frequency, mEPSCs are also measures of spontaneous neurotransmitter release changes especially in the absence of changes in synaptic numbers. Notably, the increased mEPSC frequency in the +/M2145T variant is linked to enhanced spontaneous release, not to spine or synapse density changes. These findings are reinforced by increase in counts of synaptic vesicles, calculated PPR changes, and estimates of the Pr and RRP from HFS train analysis. We have included these points in the Discussion (Lines 861-863).

      c) While it is tempting to compare the current study to our previously published conditional Trio haploinsufficiency model, we highlight key distinctions that may underlie phenotypic differences between these two mouse models. First, our prior model used a NEX-Cre transgene to ablate one Trio allele from excitatory neurons only beginning at embryonic day 11. In contrast, our Trio variants are expressed in all cell types throughout development, akin to the genetic variants found in individuals with TRIO-related disorders. Second, the Trio variant mice in this study are on a C57BL/6 background, while the Trio haploinsufficient mice were on a mixed 129Sv/J X C57BL/6 background. These differences in the current study may explain why some measures, such as mEPSC amplitude, may not align with those from the Trio conditional haploinsufficiency model.

      d) Recordings were performed using specific inhibitors to isolate AMPA and NMDA mEPSCs; these missing methodological details have now been clarified in the updated Methods section (Lines 353-360).

      (5) In Supplementary Figure 4, the sample traces indicate a higher NMDA/AMPA ratio, raising the question of whether the AMPA EPSC amplitude changes, as this could reflect PSD length. In Figure 4B, the increased AMPAR mEPSC amplitude in the +/K1918X condition compared to WT suggests an enhanced postsynaptic response, yet the PSD length is reduced in Figure 3C. Can the authors provide a potential hypothesis to explain this?

      We appreciate the Reviewer’s feedback. Yes, both evoked and miniature recordings indicate increased AMPAR amplitudes in the +/K1918X variants compared to WT. While PSD length is often linked to synaptic strength, the observed reduction in PSD length in EM PSD length reduction in +/K1918X synapses is small (~6% of WT) and clearly does not correlate with significant changes in synaptic strength. We also note that the whole cell recordings of mEPSCs represent input from all active synapses on the neuron, while PSD length is measured only in synapses of the L5.

      (6) In Figure 4, synaptic plasticity appears to decrease to around 50% of baseline; could this reduction be attributed to LTD, or might it result from changes in pipette resistance? Additionally, is the observed potentiation due to changes in presynaptic release probability? Measuring paired-pulse ratio (PPR) before and after induction would clarify this aspect.

      We thank the Reviewer for highlighting these important points.

      a) We used a well-established theta burst stimulation method for LTP induction in M1 L5 pyramidal neurons. This protocol reliably evokes LTP in WT neurons, as shown in Fig. 5J and K. Both +/K1431M and +/K1918X variants exhibit a slight but discernible increase in evoked excitatory postsynaptic currents (eEPSCs), indicative of the initiation of LTP. Although this increase is smaller compared to WT, the presence of potentiation indicates that long-term depression (LTD) is an unlikely explanation for the observed reduction.

      b) To rule out the influence of technical artifacts, pipette resistance was carefully monitored before and after LTP induction. Any cells exhibiting resistance changes exceeding 20% during electrophysiological recordings were excluded from the analysis, ensuring that fluctuations in pipette resistance did not confound LTP measurements. These technical details are denoted in the Methods (Lines 344-346 and 364-366).

      c) The potentiation in the +/M2145T variant may stem from increased release probability (Pr) and greater synaptic vesicle availability, but is beyond the scope of this work. We agree this is an intriguing question, not only for +/M2145T but also for +/K1431M mice. Future studies should address this, ideally using models where the Trio variant is selectively introduced into the presynaptic neuron.

      (7) In lines 377-380, "The +/M2145T PPR curve was unusual, with significantly reduced PPF at short ISIs, yet clearly increased PPF at longer ISI (Figure 5A, B) compared to WT." The unusual PPR observed at the 100 ms ISI appears unexpected. Can the authors provide an explanation for this anomaly? This finding could suggest atypical presynaptic dynamics or modulation at this specific interval, which may differ from typical synaptic behavior. Further insights into possible mechanisms or experimental conditions affecting this result would be valuable.

      "The decreased PPF at initial ISI in +/M2145T mice correlated with increased mEPSC frequency (Fig. 4A-C), suggestive of a possible increase in spontaneous glutamate Pr." If this is the case, it raises the question of why the increased PPR at the initial ISI in +/K1431M does not correspond to the result shown in Figure 4C. This discrepancy suggests that factors beyond initial presynaptic release probability might be influencing the observed synaptic response, or that compensatory mechanisms could be affecting PPR and mEPSC frequency differently in this variant. Further clarification on the interplay between these measurements would help resolve this inconsistency.

      We appreciate the Reviewer’s critical reading and genuine interest on this phenotype in +/M2145T mice.

      a) The unusual shift of the PPR in +/M2145T at ISI 100ms is fascinating and will require significant additional experimentation that lies beyond the scope of this report to address. We propose it results from altered presynaptic regulators, including increased Syt3 and reduced RhoA activity. Notably, Syt3 influences calcium-dependent SV replenishment, which can cause similar PPR defects (Weingarten DJ et al., 2022); this is now included in the Discussion. (Lines 915-918).

      Weingarten DJ, Shrestha A, Juda-Nelson K, Kissiwaa SA, Spruston E, Jackman SL. Fast resupply of synaptic vesicles requires synaptotagmin-3. Nature. 2022 Nov;611(7935):320-325. doi: 10.1038/s41586-022-05337-1. Epub 2022 Oct 19. PMID: 36261524.

      b) Thank you for raising the concern in clarity of this statement "The decreased PPF at initial ISI in +/M2145T mice correlated with increased mEPSC frequency (Fig. 4A-C), suggestive of a possible increase in spontaneous glutamate Pr." We have edited the sentence to be more clear (Lines 701-703). First, the K1431M and M2145T variants impact different TRIO catalytic activities disrupting distinct GTPase pathways and differentially affecting presynaptic regulators, which can lead to non-overlapping phenotypes. Also, we expand our discussion that +/K1431M variant data suggest increased AMPAR numbers and fewer silent synapses (Lines 850-855), potentially increasing AMPAR mEPSC frequency and masking the expected decrease in spontaneous release (Lines 905-910). Further experiments are needed, ideally using mixed cultures with TRIO variants in presynaptic neurons with synapses on WT neurons, as minimal stimulation variance analysis in slices would be inconclusive due to its reflection of both Pr and silent synapse changes, similar to mEPSC frequency.

      (8) In Figure 5, there is no evidence demonstrating that the NSC inhibitor functions specifically in the +/K1431M condition without affecting other conditions. To verify its specificity, the authors should test the NSC inhibitor's effects across other conditions in parallel, including a control group. Additionally, cumulative RRP measurements should be provided for a more comprehensive assessment of the inhibitor's impact on synaptic function.

      We appreciate the Reviewer’s feedback.

      a) Previous studies have shown that Rac1 activity can bidirectionally regulate synchronous release probability (Pr). We used the Rac1-specific inhibitor NSC23766 (NSC) to test how Rac1 inhibition impacted the neurotransmitter release deficits observed in +/K1431M mice. We also added control experiments testing the impact of NSC on WT slices. These new experiments are now presented in new Fig. 8 of the revised manuscript, with expanded details in the Results (Lines 737-750) and Discussion (Lines 892-900).

      b) To estimate Pr and the RRP, we employed the Decay method as described by (Ruiz et al., 2011), which does not rely on cumulative EPSC plots for RRP estimation. This approach was chosen to account for the initial facilitation in these synapses and fits are done using EPSCs plotted against stimulus number. Additional details have been provided in the Methods section  (Lines 367-373).

      Ruiz R, Cano R, Casañas JJ, Gaffield MA, Betz WJ, Tabares L. Active zones and the readily releasable pool of synaptic vesicles at the neuromuscular junction of the mouse. J Neurosci. 2011 Feb 9;31(6):2000-8. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4663-10.2011. PMID: 21307238; PMCID: PMC6633039.

      (9) Given the relevance to NDD, specifying the age window of the mice used is crucial. It is confusing that the synaptic function studies were conducted at P42, while the proteomic analysis was performed at P21. Could the authors clarify the rationale behind using different age points for these analyses? Consistency in age selection, or an explanation for this variation, would help in interpreting the developmental relevance of the findings.

      P42 was chosen as the age as it represents young adulthood, by which time clinical features will have already presented in individuals with neurodevelopmental disorders. Our prior studies of NEX-Cre Trio<sup>-/-</sup> mice found significant measurable differences from WT at this age, after neuronal migration, differentiation, synaptogenesis and pruning have occurred. An earlier developmental timepoint, P21, which coincides with juvenile age in mice, was chosen for proteomics studies to identify earlier changes and potentially targetable and modifiable mechanisms that could influence the phenotypes we observed in older mice. The experiments in P42 versus P21 mice were originally two independent lines of investigation that converged in the current study.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1:

      First, I thank the authors for clarifying some of the confusion I had in the previous comment and I appreciate the efforts the authors put into improving the quality of the manuscript. However, my concerns about the lack of novelty of the key findings are not perfectly addressed and there is no additional analysis done in this revision. Currently in this version of the manuscript, asserting that a p-value of 10-6 is close to genome-wide significance may be considered an overstatement. Further analysis focusing on finding novel and additional discovery is very necessary.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments. Reviewer #2 also made a comment regarding the genomewide threshold, “However, it remains unclear why the authors found it appropriate to apply STEAM to the LAAA model, a joint test for both allele and ancestry effects, which does not benefit from the same reduction in testing burden.” The reviewers’ have correctly identified our oversight - we have amended the manuscript as follows:

      (1) The abstract, “We identified a suggestive association peak (rs3117230, p-value = 5.292 x10-6, OR = 0.437, SE = 0.182) in the HLA-DPB1 gene originating from KhoeSan ancestry.”

      (2) From line 233 to 239: “The R package STEAM (Significance Threshold Estimation for Admixture Mapping) (Grinde et al., 2019) was used to determine the admixture mapping significance threshold given the global ancestral proportions of each individual and the number of generations since admixture (g = 15). For the LA model, a genome-wide significance threshold of pvalue < 2.5 x 10-6 was deemed significant by STEAM. The traditional genome-wide significance threshold of 5 x 10-8 was used for the GA, APA and LAAA models, as recommended by the authors of the LAAA model (Duan et al., 2018).” 

      (3) We excluded the results for the signal on chromosome 20, since this also did not reach the LAAA model genome-wide significance threshold.  

      (4) From line 296 to 308: “LAAA models were successfully applied for all five contributing ancestries (KhoeSan, Bantu-speaking African, European, East Asian and Southeast Asian). However, no variants passed the threshold for statistical significance. Although no variants reached genome-wide significance, a suggestive peak was identified in the HLA-II region of chromosome 6 when using the LAAA model and adjusting for KhoeSan ancestry (Figure 3). The QQ-plot suggested minimal genomic inflation, which was verified by calculating the genomic inflation factor ( = 1.05289) (Supplementary Figure 1). The lead variants identified using the LAAA model whilst adjusting for KhoeSan ancestry in this region on chromosome 6 are summarised in Table 3. The suggestive peak encompasses the HLA-DPA1/B1 (major histocompatibility complex, class II, DP alpha 1/beta 1) genes (Figure 4). It is noteworthy that without the LAAA model, this suggestive peak would not have been observed for this cohort. This highlights the importance of utilising the LAAA model in future association studies when investigating disease susceptibility loci in admixed individuals, such as the SAC population.”

      We acknowledge that our results are not statistically significant. However, our study advances this area of research by identifying suggestive African-specific ancestry associations with TB in the HLA-II region. These findings build upon the work of the ITHGC, which did not identify any significant associations or suggestive peaks in their African-specific analyses. We have included this argument in our manuscript (from lines 425 to 432):

      “The ITHGC did not identify any significant associations or suggestive peaks in their African ancestryspecific analyses.  Notably, the suggestive peak in the HLA-DPB1 region was only captured in our cohort using the LAAA model whilst adjusting for KhoeSan local ancestry. This underscores the importance of incorporating global and local ancestry in association studies investigating complex multi-way admixed individuals, as the genetic heterogeneity present in admixed individuals (produced as a result of admixtureinduced and ancestral LD patterns) may cause association signals to be missed when using traditional association models (Duan et al., 2018; Swart, van Eeden, et al., 2022).”

      We appreciate the comment regarding additional analyses. We acknowledge that we did not validate our SNP peak in the HLA-II region through fine-mapping due to the lack of a suitable reference panel (see lines 490 to 500). Our long-term goal is to develop a HLA-imputation reference panel incorporating KhoeSan ancestry; however, this is beyond the scope and funding allowances of this study.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors we think have done an excellent job with their responses and the manuscript has been substantially improved.

      Thank you for taking the time to help us improve our manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study reveals that TRPV1 signaling plays a key role in tympanic membrane (TM) healing by promoting macrophage recruitment and angiogenesis. Using a mouse TM perforation model, researchers found that blood-derived macrophages accumulated near the wound, driving angiogenesis and repair. TRPV1-expressing nerve fibers triggered neuroinflammatory responses, facilitating macrophage recruitment. Genetic Trpv1 mutation reduced macrophage infiltration, angiogenesis, and delayed healing. These findings suggest that targeting TRPV1 or stimulating sensory nerve fibers could enhance TM repair, improve blood flow, and prevent infections. This offers new therapeutic strategies for TM perforations and otitis media in clinical settings. This is an excellent and high-quality study that provides valuable insights into the mechanisms underlying TM wound healing.

      Strengths:

      The work is particularly important for elucidating the cellular and molecular processes involved in TM repair. However, there are several concerns about the current version.

      We sincerely thank Reviewer #1 for their time and effort in evaluating and improving our study. Below, we are pleased to address the Reviewer's concerns point by point.

      Weaknesses:

      Major concerns

      (1) The method of administration will be a critical factor when considering potential therapeutic strategies to promote TM healing. It would be beneficial if the authors could discuss possible delivery methods, such as topical application, transtympanic injection, or systemic administration, and their respective advantages and limitations for targeting TRPV1 signaling. For example, Dr. Kanemaru and his colleagues have proposed the use of Trafermin and Spongel to regenerate the eardrum.

      We are grateful to the reviewer for raising this important point. While the present study primarily focuses on the mechanistic role of TRPV1 in TM repair, we agree that the mode of therapeutic delivery will be pivotal in translating these findings into clinical practice. In response, we will expand the discussion to explore possible delivery methods—such as topical application, transtympanic injection, and systemic routes—along with their respective benefits and challenges. We will also cite the work by Dr. Kanemaru and colleagues as an example of how local delivery systems may facilitate TM regeneration.

      (2) The authors appear to have used surface imaging techniques to observe the TM. However, the TM consists of three distinct layers: the epithelial layer, the fibrous middle layer, and the inner mucosal layer. The authors should clarify whether the proposed mechanism involving TRPV1-mediated macrophage recruitment and angiogenesis is limited to the epithelial layer or if it extends to the deeper layers of the TM.

      We apologize for any confusion caused by our previous description. In our study, we utilized Z-stack confocal imaging to capture the full thickness of the TM, as illustrated in Author response image 1 (reconstructed from the acquired Z-sections). This imaging technique allowed us to encompass all three layers of the TM entirely. Each sample was imaged using a 10X objective on an Olympus fluorescence microscope. Given the conical shape and size of the TM, we imaged it in four quadrants, acquiring approximately 30 optical sections (with a 3 µm step) per region. Each acquired images were projected and exported using FV10ASW 4.2 Viewer, then stitched together using Photoshop. The resulting Z-stack projections enabled us to visualize the distribution of macrophages, angiogenesis, and the localization of nerve fibers throughout the TM. We will include this detailed methodology in our revision to clarify any potential confusion.

      Author response image 1.

      Representative confocal images showing one quadrant of the TM collected from collected from CSR1F<sup>EGFP</sup> bone marrow transplanted mouse at day 7 post-perforation. (A-B) 3D-rendered views from different angles reveal the close spatial relationship between CSF1R<sup>EGFP</sup> cells (green) and blood vessels (red) within the TM. (C) Cross-sectional view highlights the depth-wise distribution of CSF1R<sup>EGFP</sup> cells (green) and blood vessels (red) across the layered TM architecture. All images were processed using Imaris Viewer x64 (version 10.2.0).

      Minor concerns

      In Figure 8, the schematic illustration presents a coronal section of the TM. However, based on the data provided in the manuscript, it is unclear whether the authors directly obtained coronal images in their study. To enhance the clarity and impact of the schematic, it would be helpful to include representative images of coronal sections showing macrophage infiltration, angiogenesis, and nerve fiber distribution in the TM.

      As noted above, we utilized Z-stack confocal imaging to capture the full thickness of the TM, enabling us to visualize structures across all three layers. This approach ensured that all layers were included in our analysis. Due to the thin and curved nature of the TM, traditional cross-sectional imaging often struggles to clearly depict the spatial relationships between macrophages, blood vessels, and nerve fibers, especially at low magnification as shown in Author response image 2. In response to the reviewer's suggestion, we will include representative coronal images in the revised manuscript to better illustrate the distribution of these structures at higher magnification.

      Author response image 2.

      Confocal images of eardrum cross-sections collected at day 1 (A), 3 (B), and 7 (C) post perforation to demonstrate the wound healing processes.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study examines the role of TRPV1 signaling in the recruitment of monocyte-derived macrophages and the promotion of angiogenesis during tympanic membrane (TM) wound healing. The authors use a combination of genetic mouse models, macrophage depletion, and transcriptomic approaches to suggest that neuronal TRPV1 activity contributes to macrophage-driven vascular responses necessary for tissue repair.

      Strengths:

      (1) The topic of neuroimmune interactions in tissue regeneration is of interest and underexplored in the context of the TM, which presents a unique model due to its anatomical features.

      (2) The use of reporter mice and bone marrow chimeras allows for some dissection of immune cell origin.

      (3) The authors incorporate transcriptomic data to contextualize inflammatory and angiogenic processes during wound healing.

      We sincerely thank Reviewer #2 for their time and effort in improving our study and recognizing its strengths. Below, we are pleased to address the reviewer's concerns point by point.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The primary claims of the manuscript are not convincingly supported by the evidence presented. Most of the data are correlative in nature, and no direct mechanistic experiments are included to establish causality between TRPV1 signaling and macrophage recruitment or function.

      We appreciate Reviewer #2's perspective on the lack of molecular mechanisms linking TRPV1 signaling and macrophages. However, our data demonstrates that TRPV1 mutations significantly affect macrophage recruitment and angiogenesis. This initial study primarily focuses on the intriguing phenomenon of how sensory nerve fibers are involved in eardrum immunity and wound healing, an area that has not been clearly reported in the literature before. We believe that further research is necessary to explore this topic in greater depth.

      (2) Functional validation of key molecular players (such as Tac1 or Spp1) is lacking, and their roles are inferred primarily from gene expression data rather than experimentally tested.

      Although we have identified the TAC1 and SPP1 signals as potentially important for TM wound healing for the first time, we agree with the Reviewer's view regarding the lack of molecular mechanisms explored in this study. We have not yet tested the downstream signaling pathways, but we plan to investigate them in a series of future studies. As this is an early report, we will continue to explore these signals and their potential clinical applications based on our initial findings moving forward.

      (3) The reuse of publicly available scRNA-seq data is not sufficiently integrated or extended to yield new biological insights, and it remains largely descriptive.

      We appreciate Reviewer #2 for highlighting this point. Leveraging publicly available scRNA-seq databases and established analysis pipelines not only saves time and resources—my lab recently collected macrophages from the eardrums of postnatal P15 mice, with each trial requiring 20 eardrums from 10 animals to obtain a sufficient number of cells—but also allows researchers to build on previous work and focus on new biological questions without the need to repeat experiments. A prior study conducted by Dr. Tward and his team utilized scRNA-seq data to make initial discoveries related to eardrum wound healing, primarily focusing on epithelial cells rather than macrophages. We are building on their raw data to uncover new biological insights regarding macrophages, even though we have not yet tested the unidentified signals, which we believe will be valuable to our peers.

      (4) The macrophage depletion model (CX3CR1CreER; iDTR) lacks specificity, and possible off-target or systemic effects are not addressed.

      We agree with reviewer #2, although macrophage depletion model used in our study is a standard and well-used animal model (Shi, Hua et al. 2018), which has been used by many other laboratories, it is important to note that any macrophage depletion model may have potential issues. We will discuss this in our revision.

      (5) Several interpretations of the data appear overstated, particularly regarding the necessity of TRPV1 for monocyte recruitment and wound healing.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We will revise our manuscript where it is overstated accordingly.

      (6) Overall, the study appears to apply known concepts - namely, TRPV1-mediated neurogenic inflammation and macrophage-driven angiogenesis - to a new anatomical site without providing new mechanistic insight or advancing the field substantially.

      Although our study may not seem highly innovative at first glance, it reveals a previously unknown role of the TRPV1 pain signaling pathway in promoting eardrum healing for the first time. This healing process includes the recruitment of monocyte-derived macrophages and the formation of new blood vessels (angiogenesis). While this process has been documented in other organs, most research on macrophage-driven angiogenesis has been conducted using in vitro models, with very few studies demonstrating this process in vivo. Our findings could lead to new translational opportunities, especially considering that tympanic membrane perforation, along with damage-induced otitis media and conductive hearing loss, are common clinical issues affecting millions of people worldwide. Targeting TRPV1 signaling could enhance tympanic membrane immunity, improve blood circulation, promote the repair of damaged tympanic membranes, and ultimately prevent middle ear infections—an idea that has not been previously proposed.

      Overall:

      While the study addresses an interesting topic, the current version does not provide sufficiently strong or novel evidence to support its major conclusions. Additional mechanistic experiments and more rigorous validation would be necessary to substantiate the proposed model and clarify the relevance of the findings beyond this specific tissue context.

      We greatly thank the two reviewers for their helpful critiques to improve our study. We especially thank the Section Editors for their insightful and constructive comments on this initial study.

      References:

      Shi, J., L. Hua, D. Harmer, P. Li and G. Ren (2018). "Cre Driver Mice Targeting Macrophages." Methods Mol Biol 1784: 263-275.

    1. Author response:

      We are grateful to the reviewers for their extensive and constructive feedback. In large the three reviewers noted the following main points:

      (1) The overall evidence for any rhythmicity in this data is not ‘very strong’.

      We do agree and will tone down the conclusions accordingly. However, as one of the reviewers noted, a qualitative interpretation of the specific statistical results remains somewhat vague and speculative by necessity.

      (2) The differences between the results for the individual experiments are generally small. Yet, the same reviewer also asks for speculations as to how differences between experiments can be interpreted.

      We will consider these, but also note that a clear demonstration of the robustness of specific effects requires the replication of individual experiments in a separate experiment.

      (3) A clear-cut interpretation of the current experimental design in the context of continuous listening and true vigilance tasks remains difficult. This makes the interpretation and generalization of the results difficult.

      We do agree in principle, but also note that task designs very widely in previous work, which may be one reason for why there is no clear consensus on the existence or absence of a rhythmic mode of listening. We will consider specific suggestions for future work to be included in the revision.

      (4) The adjustment of task difficulty in the present task design may pose a challenge. Reviewers also suggest analyzing potential rhythmicity in this task difficulty parameter.

      We will consider this for the revision.

      (5) A more clear-cut interpretation of what potential differences in the rhythmicity of sensitivity and bias would mean should be included.

      We will provide this in the revision.

      (6) The study should provide a stronger conceptual framework both for the source of "rhythmic modes" and why one may expect differences between ears.

      In large this has been put forward by many previous studies testing and reporting rhythmicity in auditory tasks.  Rhythmicity is pervasive in neural activity, but whether and how this relates to behavioral data remains less clear. These points will be clarified in a revision.

      (7) Parallels to work in the visual domain by Fiebelkorn, Landau & Fries should be included.

      We will discuss similarities and differences between studies on perceptual rhythmicity in the visual and auditory domains.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      We again thank you for the positive and constructive feedback on our manuscript, and for highlighting its contributions to understanding the role of CARD8 in viral protease-triggered sensing of viral spread, and the potential impact of our findings on chronic inflammation and immune activation. We agree that it will be important for future work to address whether or not HIV-1 protease-triggered CARD8 inflammasome activation contributes to chronic inflammation in PLWH who are receiving ART.

      In response to the question about the baseline level of IL-1β in Fig. 4D, the figure below shows the mock condition for the CD4+ T cell:MDM coculture. We had done this control in parallel with the data presented in the submitted figure. Levels of IL-1β during HIV-1 infection are increased over background (i.e., mock infection). We note that for donor G the IL-1β concentration is below the limit of detection for this assay. Thus, it remains possible that other inflammasomes contribute modestly during cell-to-cell transmission of HIV-1; however, incomplete knockout of CARD8 in a minority of cells may also contribute to the observed levels of IL-1β in response to HIV-1 infection. Nonetheless, collectively, our data strongly supports the role for CARD8 in HIV-1 protease-triggered inflammasome activation.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Joint Public Review:

      Following up on their previous work, the authors investigated whether cell-to-cell transmission of HIV-1 activates the CARD8 inflammasome in macrophages, an important question given that inflammasome activation in myeloid cells triggers proinflammatory cytokine release. The data support the idea that CARD8 is activated by the viral protease and promotes inflammation. However, time-course analyses in primary T cells and macrophages and further information on the specific inflammasome involved would further increase the significance of the study.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is well-written and the data is of good quality. The evidence that CARD8 senses the HIV-1 protease in the context of cell-to-cell transmission is important since cell-to-cell transmission is thought to play a key role in viral spread in vivo, and inflammation is a major driver of disease progression. Clean knockout experiments in primary macrophages are a notable strength and the results clearly support the role of CARD8 in protease-dependent sensing of viral spread and the induction of IL1β release and cell death. The finding that HIV-1 strains are resistant to protease inhibitors differ in CARD8 activation and IL1β production is interesting and underscores the potential clinical relevance of these results.

      Weaknesses:

      One weakness is that the authors used T cell lines which might not faithfully reflect the efficiency of HIV-1 production and cell-cell transfer by primary T cells. To assess whether CARD8 is also activated by protease from incoming viral particles earlier time points should be analyzed. Finally, while the authors exclude the role of NLRP3 in IL-1b and the death of macrophages it would be interesting to know whether the effect is still Gasdermin D dependent.

      Recommendations for the authors

      (1) Co-culture assay should also be done between primary CD4 cells and primary MDMs, because T-cell lines produce much more viruses, and the efficiency of cell-tocell transmission might be dramatically different in primary cells compared to cell lines.

      We have now added data from experiments using infected primary CD4 cells as the donor cells in cell-to-cell HIV-1 transmission to MDMs in new Figure 4. The results largely phenocopy the SUPT1:MDM coculture in that we observe inflammasome activation after co-culture of HIV-infected primary T cells with primary MDMs. We find that this inflammasome activity induced by the CD4:MDM cell-to-cell transmission is abrogated by knockout of CARD8 in the MDMs or treatment of HIV protease inhibitor lopinavir (LPV) or caspase 1 inhibitor VX765, suggesting that this activation is dependent on CARD8, HIV protease, and caspase 1. Additionally, the signal persists in the presence of reverse transcriptase inhibitor nevirapine (NVP), suggesting that the incoming protease is driving activation.

      (2) For all co-culture experiments, supernatants were collected at 48 or 72 hours. Since CARD8 activation is expected to be driven by incoming viral particles without RT, they should measure cytokine production at much earlier time points. 2-3 days co-culture raises concerns. Ideally, the authors can provide a time-course.

      We have now added a time course of the SUPT1:MDM coculture from 3 unique donors taken at 4, 24, 48, and 72 hours post coculture in the presence or absence of reverse transcriptase inhibitor (see new Figure 3B) as well as for the primary CD4 cells to MDM co-culture (see new Figure 4B). We detect IL-1β at the 24hour time point (and later), but not at the 4-hour time point which is slower than what was detected by direct cell-free infection (Kulsuptrakul et al., 2023). However, we still hypothesize that this is driven by active incoming viral protease because the signal is not abrogated by a reverse transcriptase inhibitor, which indicates that de novo protease production is not necessary. We also observed that IL-1β levels do not increase after plateauing 24h after establishing the co-culture, suggesting that secondary infection does not further amplify inflammasome activation. We now speculate on this in the Discussion.

      (3) A potential confounder in the data in Figure 4 is that despite rightly including the cognate adaptations in the Gag cleavage sites with the PI-R protease mutants, some of these viruses still display Gag processing defects. Can the authors disentangle the potency of PR mutant cleavage with either reduced cell entry or reduced protease availability due to processing defects in the incoming virions?

      The reviewer is correct that although the western blot with the p24<sup>gag</sup> antibody suggests that Gag is processed, we cannot rule out that other variables do not contribute to the observed difference in CARD8 inflammasome activation. For example, PI-R clones relative to the LAI strain may have distinct protease substrate specificity, variable efficiency/kinetics in viral assembly, gag dimerization, and other factors may ultimately influence CARD8 inflammasome activation. We have updated the text to reflect these possibilities. Nonetheless, this argument does not change the conclusion that CARD8 inflammasome activation is affected by protease mutations acquired during drug resistance.

      (4) There is considerable donor variation in the macrophages (unsurprising) but can the authors correlate this with CARD8 expression and are there any off-target effects on macrophage permissivity to HIV-1 infection?

      We have now considerably increased the number of primary cell donors from the first submission (see Author response table 1 below). We find that the non-responsive donor presented in the first submission is aberrant since all others do respond to a greater or lesser degree (Figure 3, Figure 4). However, the reviewer may be correct that the particular aberrant donor MDMs were poorly infected. We also note that despite donor variability in the degree of activation (IL-1β secretion) from cocultures with HIV<sub>BaL</sub>-infected SUPT1 cells, HIV-induced activation is comparable to the activation induced by VbP (see new Figure 3–figure supplement 1B). We do not see a notable difference in CARD8 expression between donors. Nonetheless, with the added number of primary cell donors, the data are consistent with a role of primary MDMs from nearly all donors in supporting a CARD8-dependent, HIV-protease dependent inflammasome response after co-culture with infected T cells. We have left in data from all of the donors so that readers can appreciate the variability among primary cells.

      Author response table 1.

      In addition, to address the reviewer concerns about off-target effects of the sgRNAs on macrophage permissivity, we assessed our CD4:MDM cocultures for percent infectivity via intracellular p24<sup>gag</sup> in AAVS1 vs CARD8 KO MDMs and we observed no significant difference in infectivity in AAVS1 vs CARD8 KO MDMs (see Author response image 1 of MDMs after co-culture with T cells that is not affected any potential off-target effects of the sgRNAs.

      Author response image 1.

      Equivalent infection in AAVS1 vs CARD8 KOMDMs. AAVS1 or CARD8 KO from donor 12 were cocultured with mock or HIV infected CD4 T cells as described in Figure 4D for 72 hours then assessed for HIV infection of the MDMs by washing away CD4 T cells, harvesting MDMs, and staining attached MDMs for intracellular p24<sup>gag</sup> for flow cytometry analysis. Datasets represent mean ± SD (n=2 technical replicates from one donor). One-way ANOVA with Dunnett’s test using GraphPad Prism 10. ns = not significant, *p<0.05,**p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001.

      (5) The authors suggest that NLRP3 is unlikely to be the mediator of IL-1b and cell death in the macrophages. Is this death still GSDMDdependent, what other NLRs are expressed in this system and does it make a difference what PAMP you use to prime the response?

      We have now added additional data in support of the conclusion that NLRP3 is not a mediator of the IL-1β secretion in the infected SUPT1 cells to primary MDMs coculture. In addition to using an NLRP3 inhibitor, we have now also made NLRP3 KOs MDMs and used these in the coculture experiments which show that the IL-1β secretion after coculture of infected SUPT1 cells and primary MDMs is mediated by CARD8 and not NLRP3 because the signal is abrogated by CARD8 knockout, but not by NLRP3 knockout. This new data is shown in Figure 3C and D.

      To assess the role of GSDMD, we treated SUPT1:MDM cocultures with disulfiram, a GSDMD inhibitor (Hu et al., 2020). Disulfiram treatment abrogated IL-1β secretion, suggesting that this activation is indeed GSDMD-mediated (see Author response image 2 below). We choose not to include the disulfiram result in the final manuscript since we have not ruled out cytotoxic effects of the drug.

      There are likely other NLRs expressed in primary MDMs; however, since inflammasome activation is completely absent in the CARD8 KO MDMs, we infer that CARD8 is the main inflammasome-forming sensor in this system. However, we cannot rule out the possibility of other innate sensors being activated downstream of CARD8 or under different differentiation conditions.

      To address the concern that alternative priming affects CARD8 activation, we compared pre-treatment of cells with Pam3CSK4 or lipopolysaccharide (LPS) in the presence or absence of HIV protease inhibitor and reverse transcriptase inhibitor. Regardless of the priming agent used, we observed HIV protease-dependent activation that persisted in the presence of reverse transcriptase inhibitor, suggesting that CARD8 is the main sensor under LPS and Pam3CSK4 priming (new Figure 3–figure supplement 1A).

      Author response image 2.

      Inflammasome activation following cell-to-cell HIV infection is mediated by GSDMD. SUPT1-CCR5 cells were either mock-infected or infected with HIV-1<sub>NL4.3BaL</sub> for 20 hours before coculturing with MDMs in either the presence or absence of GSDMD inhibitor disulfarim (25μM). Cocultures were harvested 24 hours later to assess (left) IL-1β secretion via IL-1 reporter assay and (right) cell viability via CellTiter-Glo® assay. Viability was calculated by normalizing to relative luminescence units in the mock untreated control. Dotted line indicates limit of detection (LoD). Dashed line indicates 100% viability as determined by untreated mock control. Datasets represent mean ± SD (n=2 technical replicates for one donor). Two-way ANOVA with Sidak’s test (using GraphPad Prism 10. ns = not significant, *p<0.05,**p<0.01, ***p<0.001, ****p<0.0001.

      Minor points

      (1) In Figure 1, the authors should clarify whether LAI or LAI-VSV-G was used.

      Wild-type virus (LAI strain) was used in Figure 1. This has now been clarified in the figure legend.

      (2) In Figure 1, the fraction of infected cells without DEAE was ~20% in both WT and CARD8 KO THP-1, suggesting somewhat efficient viral entry even in the absence of DEAE. How do the authors reconcile this with the lack of IL-1β production? The increase in infection observed in WT THP-1 +DEAE was overall modest (from ~20% to 25-30%) compared to the dramatic difference in IL-1β production. Can they provide more evidence or discuss how DEAE might be impacting cytokine production? If differences in viral entry are the explanation for differences in inflammasome activation, then they should be able to overcome this by using virus at a higher MOI in the absence of DEAE. Experiments proposed in Figure 1 +/- DEAE should be repeated using a range of MOI for LAI and showing the corresponding percent infection in THP-1 cells (which is not shown in Figure S2 for LAI-VSVG).

      We hypothesize that the lack of IL-1β production without DEAE is likely due to an insufficient amount of incoming viral protease to induce CARD8 activation. Though the increase in infection with DEAE is modest by intracellular p24<sup>gag</sup> at 24 hours post infection, we infer that intracellular p24<sup>gag</sup> may be largely underestimating the actual increase in viral efficiency achieved with DEAE (now in Supplemental Note). We have also updated Figure S2 (now Figure 2–figure supplement 1) legend to include the percent infection for HIV-1<sub>LAI</sub> and HIV-1<sub>LAI-VSVG</sub> infections. We agree that activation in the absence of DEAE could be overcome by infecting with a more concentrated viral stock to increase the MOI. Indeed, our decision to use the cell-to-cell transmission model achieves this in a more physiologic context.

      (3) In Figure S1, the authors point out that RT-activity in the supernatants was similar in the cell-free vs. cell-to-cell model. While in the transwell system THP-1 cells are the only cells capable of producing new virions, how are they able to differentiate viral production from sup-T1 vs. THP-1 in the cell-to-cell system? At a minimum, they should provide some data on the observed RT activity in matching wells containing the same number of infected sup-T1 cells utilized in coculture experiments.

      We think this may have been a misinterpretation. In Figure S1 (now Figure 1B, right), we compare the amount of virus available in the lower chamber of the transwell versus the cell-to-cell condition. We are not comparing cell-free to cell-to-cell infection. We have changed the text and figure title to clarify this point.

      (4) Can the authors provide additional comments on the lack of IL-1β release in donor C in Figure 3? The donor did not produce IL-1β in response to VbP or HIV, although the WB for CARD8 appears similar to the other two donors.

      We have now tested MDMs from additional donors and continue to find a range of IL-1β secretion after the coculture. However, donor C is aberrant since each of the other donors had detectable IL-1β secretion in response to VbP and HIV-1 to greater or lesser extents. Nonetheless, we have included additional donors summarized in the table above corresponding to major comment #4.

      (5) For Figure 3, can the authors provide information on the fraction of MDMs that were infected after coculture with sup-T1 cells? Why didn't the authors measure cell death in MDMs?

      It is difficult to measure the fraction of MDMs infected or dying in the cocultures since it is hard to separate signal from the T cells. Although it would be possible to do so, in this manuscript, we instead prefer to focus on the potential contribution of CARD8 inflammasome activation in exacerbating chronic inflammation in response to HIV rather than the depletion of macrophages.

      (6) In Figure 4, did the authors introduce the mutations associated with PI resistance into the same LAI backbone? If not, this is not a fair comparison, as viral protein expression levels were not at the same level, indicated in Figure 4A. Additionally, such comparison will be further strengthened by using cells other than 293T cells for the coculture assay.

      No, we did not introduce these mutations into LAI, since they were already in an NL4.3 backbone and NL4.3 and LAI differ by only 1 amino acid in protease. We have updated Table S1 to report this amino acid difference. We also note that in our previous manuscript we tested much more diverse proteases such as a clade A HIV-1, HIV-2, and SIVs and find comparable CARD8 cleavage to LAI.

      Additions not requested by Reviewers:

      THP-1 characterization

      In our previous work, we noticed that different “wildtype” THP-1 lines behaved uniquely in response to DEAE-dextran. In particular, we observed inflammasome activation in response to DEAE-dextran alone at the concentration used for spinoculations (20μg/mL), whereas the other THP-1 line did not. Thus, we performed STR profiling on each THP-1 cell line and determined that the THP-1 cells used in our studies (JK THP1s) are distinct from THP-1 cells from ATCC at 3 different loci. This data is now included in the Supplemental Note (Figure A1). Please note that all data in this and the accompanying manuscript were performed in JK THP-1 cells.

      Whole plasmid sequencing of the PI-resistant HIV clones

      Since preprint submission, we have done whole plasmid Oxford Nanopore sequencing on the PI-resistant HIV clones obtained from the NIAID HIV/AIDS Specimen Repository Program. Of note, there were a handful of previously unreported mutations included in these plasmid stocks within protease. We have updated Table S1 to include an additional column titled “Additional amino acid changes in HIV<sup>PR</sup> relative to NL4.3.”

      References

      Hu JJ, Liu X, Xia S, Zhang Z, Zhang Y, Zhao J, Ruan J, Luo X, Lou X, Bai Y, Wang J, Hollingsworth LR, Magupalli VG, Zhao L, Luo HR, Kim J, Lieberman J, Wu H. 2020. FDA-approved disulfiram inhibits pyroptosis by blocking gasdermin D pore formation. Nat Immunol 21:736–745. doi:10.1038/s41590-020-0669-6

      Kulsuptrakul J, Turcotte EA, Emerman M, Mitchell PS. 2023. A human-specific motif facilitates CARD8 inflammasome activation after HIV-1 infection. eLife 12:e84108. doi:10.7554/eLife.84108

    1. Author Response:

      eLife assessment

      This is a valuable initial study of cell type and spatially resolved gene expression in and around the locus coeruleus, the primary source of the neuromodulator norepinephrine in the human brain. The data are generated with cutting-edge techniques, and the work lays the foundation for future descriptive and experimental approaches to understand the contribution of the locus coeruleus to healthy brain function and disease. However, due to small sample size and the need for additional confirmatory data, the data only incompletely support the main conclusions presented here. With the strengthening of the analyses, this paper, and the associated web application, will be of great interest to neuroscientists working on arousal-based behaviors and neurological and neuropsychiatric phenotypes.

      Thank you for the assessment and comments. Overall, the majority of the issues raised by the reviewers relate either directly or indirectly to limitations of the sample size that precluded further optimization of protocols and expansion of the dataset. We fully acknowledge the limited sample size in this dataset and aim to be transparent about the limitations of the study. This is the first report of snRNA-seq and spatially-resolved transcriptomics in the human locus coeruleus (LC). The LC is a very small nucleus, located deep within the brainstem, which is extremely challenging to study due to its small size, difficult to access location, and the very small number of norepinephrine (NE) neurons located within the nucleus, which were of prime interest for this study. We note that this study represents our initial attempt to molecularly and spatially characterize cell types within the human LC. We note that we did not have significant, established funding from extramural sources dedicated to this study, and tissue resources for the LC are difficult to ascertain, contributing to the small sample size in this initial study. We acknowledge that there are limitations in sample size as well as data quality. Findings from this study will be used to inform, improve, and optimize future and ongoing experimental design, as well as technical and analytical workflows for larger-scale studies. As brought up by one of the reviewers, this field is still in its infancy -- pilot experimentation in new brain regions is labor-intensive and these sequencing approaches remain costly. Moreover, due to the small size and difficulties in dissecting, tissue resources from the human brain in this area are a highly limited resource. Hence, notwithstanding limitations, in our view it is important to release the data for community access at this time. Specific responses to the reviewers’ comments are provided point-by-point in the following sections.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Weber et al. collect locus coeruleus (LC) tissue blocks from 5 neurotypical European men, dissect the dorsal pons around the LC and prepare 2-3 tissue sections from each donor on a slide for 10X spatial transcriptomics. […] The authors transparently present limitations of their work in the discussion, but some points discussed below warrant further attention.

      Specific comments:

      1) snRNAseq:

      a. Major concerns with the snRNAseq dataset are A) the low recovery rate of putative LC-neurons in the snRNAseq dataset, B) the fact that the LC neuron cluster is contaminated with mitochondrial RNA, and C) that a large fraction of the nuclei cannot be assigned to a clear cell type (presumably due to contamination or damaged nuclei). The authors chose to enrich for neurons using NeuN antibody staining and FACS. But it is difficult to assess the efficacy of this enrichment without images of the nuclear suspension obtained before FACS, and of the FACS results. As this field is in its infancy, more detail on preliminary experiments would help the reader to understand why the authors processed the tissue the way they did. It would be nice to know whether omitting the FACS procedure might in fact result in higher relative recovery of LC-neurons, or if the authors tried this and discovered other technical issues that prompted them to use FACS.

      Thank you for these comments. We agree these are valid concerns in assessing the data quality and validity of the findings from the snRNA-seq dataset. We will respond to these concerns here to the best of our ability, but in some cases, we do not have definitive answers since comparison data are not yet available for this region. In particular, we were limited in resources for this initial study -- some of the results of the study and issues that we identified in attempting to molecularly profile cells in the human LC were surprising to us, and we intend to generate additional samples and troubleshoot these issues to improve data quality and increase recovery in future work. However, these experiments are (i) expensive, (ii) time- and labor-intensive, and (iii) the tissue for this region is limited and difficult to ascertain. Given the extremely small size of the LC, the tissue resource is quickly depleted. For this study, we had fixed resources and made best-guess decisions on how to proceed with the experimental design, based on our experience with snRNA-seq in other human brain regions (Tran and Maynard et al. 2021). However, the LC is a unique region, and our experiences with this dataset will guide us to make technical adjustments in future studies. Due to the limitations in the tissue resources and the lack of data currently available to the community, we wanted to share these results immediately while acknowledging the limitations of the study as we work to increase our resource availability to expand molecular and spatial profiling studies in this region of the human brain.

      Regarding the reviewer’s concern that our choice to use FANS to enrich for neurons could have potentially led to more damage and contributed to the low recovery rate of LC-NE neurons and the mitochondrial contamination -- we do not have a definitive answer to this question, since we did not perform a direct comparison with non-sorted data. As noted above, our limited tissue resource dictated that we could not do both. We made the decision to enrich for neurons based on our previous experience with identifying relatively rare populations in other brain regions (e.g. nucleus accumbens and amygdala; Tran and Maynard et al. 2021). Based on this previous work, our rationale was that without neuronal enrichment, we could potentially miss the LC-NE population, given the relative scarcity of this neuronal population. The low recovery rate and relatively lower quality / contamination issues may be due to technical issues that lead to LC-NE neurons being more susceptible to damage during nuclear preparation and sorting. We agree that directly comparing to data prepared without NeuN labeling and sorting is reasonable, as the additional perturbations may indeed contribute to cell damage. As mentioned in the discussion, we do not have a definitive answer to the reasons for increased mitochondrial contamination and we suspect that multiple technical factors may contribute -- including the relatively large size and increased fragility of LC-NE neurons. We agree that systematically optimizing the preparation to attempt to increase recovery rate and decrease mitochondrial contamination are important avenues for future work.

      b. It is unclear what percentage of cells that make up each cluster.

      We will add this information in the clustering heatmaps or as a supplementary plot in a revised version of the manuscript.

      c. The number of subjects used in each analysis was not always clear. Only 3 subjects were used for snRNAseq, and one of them only yielded 4 LC-nuclei. This means the results are essentially based on n=2. The authors report these numbers in the corresponding section, but the first sentence of the results section (and Figure 1C specifically!) create the impression that n=5 for all analyses. Even for spatial transcriptomics, if I understood it correctly, 1 sample had to be excluded (n=4).

      This is correct. We will update the figures and text in a revised version of the manuscript to make this limitation (small sample size) more clear, and to further emphasize that the intention of this study is to provide initial data to help determine next steps and best practices for a larger scale and more comprehensive study on this region, especially given the limited availability of tissue resources and currently limited data resources available for this region.

      2) Spatial transcriptomics:

      a. It is not clear to me what the spatial transcriptomics provides beyond what can be shown with snRNAseq, nor how these two sets of results compare to each other. It would be more intuitive to start the story with snRNAseq and then try to provide spatial detail using spatial transcriptomics. The LC is not a homogeneous structure but can be divided into ensembles based on projection specificity. Spatial transcriptomics could - in theory - offer much-needed insights into the spatial variation of mRNA profiles across different ensembles, or as a first step across the spatial (rostral/caudal, ventral/dorsal) extent of the LC. The current analyses, however, cannot address this issue, as the orientation of the LC cannot be deduced from the slices analyzed.

      We understand the point of the reviewer. However, we structured the manuscript in this format due to our aims of creating a data resource for the community as well as being transparent about the limitations of our study. Our experiments began with the spatial experiments on the tissue blocks because this (i) helped orient ourselves to the region, and (ii) provided guidance for how best to score the tissue blocks for the snRNA-seq experiments to maximize recovery of LC-NE neurons. Therefore, we also decided to present the results in this sequence.

      The spatial data also provides more information in that the measurements are from nuclei, cytoplasm, and cell processes (instead of nuclei only). This is one of the main differences / advantages between the platforms at this level of spatial resolution. As noted above, we were also working with a finite tissue resource -- if we ran snRNA-seq first and captured no neurons, the tissue block would be depleted. Due to the logistics / thickness of the required tissue sections for Visium and snRNA-seq respectively, running Visium first allowed us to ensure that we could collect data from both assays.

      Regarding a point raised below on why we only ran snRNA-seq on a subset of the donors -- this was due to resource depletion and not enough available tissue remaining on the tissue blocks to run the assay. We have conducted extensive piloting in other brain regions on the amount (mg) of tissue that is needed from various sized cryosections, and the LC is particularly difficult since these are small tissue blocks and the extent of the structure is small. Hence, in some of the subjects, we did not have sufficient tissue available for the snRNA-seq assay.

      We agree with the reviewer that spatial studies could, in future work, offer needed and important information about expression profiles across the spatial axes (rostral/caudal, ventral/dorsal) of the LC. Our study provides us with insight about optimizing the dissections for spatial assays, as well as bringing to light a number of technical and logistical issues that we had not initially foreseen. For example, during the course of this study and parallel, ongoing work in other small, challenging brain regions, we have now developed a number of specialized technical and logistical strategies for keeping track of orientation and mounting serial sections from the same tissue block onto a single spatial array, which is extremely technically challenging. We are now well-prepared for addressing these issues in future studies with larger numbers of donors and samples, e.g. spaced serial sections across the extent of the LC to make these types of insights. Due to the rarity of the tissue, limited availability of information in this region, and high expense of conducting these studies, we want to share this initial data with the community immediately. We also note that in addition to the 10x Genomics Visium platform, which lacks cellular and sub-cellular resolution, many new and exciting spatial platforms are entering the market, which may be able to address questions in very small regions such as the LC at higher spatial resolution.

      b. Unfortunately, spatial transcriptomics itself is plagued by sampling variability to a point where the RNAscope analyses the authors performed prove more powerful in addressing direct questions about gene expression patterns. Given that the authors compare their results to published datasets from rodent studies, it is surprising that a direct comparison of genes identified with spatial transcriptomics vs snRNAseq is lacking (unless this reviewer missed this comparison). Supplementary Figure 17 seems to be a first step in that direction, but this is not a gene-by-gene comparison of which analysis identifies which LC-enriched genes. Such an analysis should not compare numbers of enriched genes using artificial cutoffs for significance/fold-change, but rather use correlations to get a feeling for which genes appear to be enriched in the LC using both methods. This would result in one list of genes that can serve as a reference point for future work.

      We agree this is a good suggestion, and will add additional computational analyses to address this point in a revised version of the manuscript.

      c. Maybe the spatial transcriptomics could be useful to look at the peri-LC region, which has generated some excitement in rodent work recently, but remains largely unexplored in humans.

      We agree this is an excellent suggestion -- assessing cross-species comparisons related to convergence, especially, of GABAergic cell populations in the human LC is of high interest. We note that these types of extensions are exactly the reason why we have provided the publicly accessible web app (R/Shiny app, which includes the ability to annotate regions). We hope that others will use these apps for specialized topics they are interested in. As discussed above, we note that our initial dissections precluded the ability to keep track of the exact orientation of our tissue sections on the Visium arrays with respect to their location within the brainstem, so definitive localization of this region across subjects is difficult in our current study. However, it is possible, for example, to investigate whether there is a putative peri-LC region that is densely GABAergic that is homologous with the GABAergic peri-LC region in rodents. We also raise attention to a recent preprint by Luskin and Li et al. (2022), who apply snRNA-seq and spatially-resolved transcriptomics to molecularly define both LC and peri-LC cell types in mice -- in a revised version of our manuscript, we will extend our computational analyses of inhibitory neuronal subtypes in our data (Supplementary Figures 13, 16) to directly compare with those identified in this study in more detail. As noted above, we we have now developed a number of specialized technical and logistical strategies for keeping track of orientation of sections from the tissue block onto a single spatial array, and we feel that combined with optimized dissection strategies for this region and the guide of RNAscope for GABAergic markers on serial sections, that annotating the peri-LC region on spatial arrays in future studies will be possible.

      3) The comparison of snRNAseq data to published literature is laudable. Although the authors mention considerable methodological differences between the chosen rodent work and their own analyses, this needs to be further explained. The mouse dataset uses TRAPseq, which looks at translating mRNAs associated with ribosomes, very different from the nuclear RNA pool analyzed in the current work. The rat dataset used single-cell LC laser microdissection followed by microarray analyses, leading to major technical differences in terms of tissue processing and downstream analyses. The authors mention and reference a recent 10x mouse LC dataset (Luskin et al, 2022), however they only pick some neuropeptides from this study for their analysis of interneuron subtypes (Figure S13). Although this is a very interesting part of the manuscript, a more in-depth analysis of these two datasets would be very useful. It would likely allow for a better comparison between mouse and human, given that the technical approach is more similar (albeit without FACS), and Luskin et al have indicated that they are willing to share their data.

      As noted above, we plan to extend our comparisons with the dataset from Luskin and Li et al. (2022) in a revised version of the manuscript, which will provide a more in-depth cross-species comparison. In addition, we also note that there are some additional recent studies using TRAPseq of LC-NE neurons in a functional context, i.e. treatment vs. control experiments or in model systems (e.g. Iannitelli et al. 2023), which provide new opportunities for understanding disease context using in-depth cross-species comparisons. By providing our dataset and reproducible code, we will enable others to adapt and extend these types of comparisons (i.e. TRAPseq of LC-NE neurons or LC snRNA-seq following functional manipulations or in the context of disease or behavioral models) in the future.

      4) Statements in the manuscript about the unexpected identification of a 5-HT (serotonin) cell-cluster seem somewhat contradictory. Figure S14 suggests that 5-HT markers are expressed in the LC-regions just as much as anywhere else, but the RNAscope image in Figure S15 suggests spatial separation between these two populations. And Figure S17 again suggests almost perfect overlap between the LC and 5HT clusters. Maybe I misunderstood, in which case the authors should better clarify/explain these results.

      In our view, the most likely scenario is that the 5-HT neurons come from contamination from the dorsal raphe nucleus based on spatial separation from the RNAscope images, which we agree are more definitive. As mentioned above, since we do not have definitive documentation for the tissue sections in terms of orientation, it is difficult to say with clarity that the regions are the dorsal raphe and which sub-portion of the dorsal raphe they are. This initial study has now allowed us to optimize and improve our dissection strategy and approaches for retaining documentation of the orientation of the tissue sections from their intact position within the brainstem as they move from cryosection to placement on the array, which will enable us to better annotate regions with definitive anatomical information with respect to the rostral/caudal and dorsal/ventral axes in future experiments. Given that there are reports in the rodent that 5-HT markers have been identified in LC-NE neurons (Iijima 1993; Iijima 1989), and taking into account the technical limitations in our study, we felt that it was premature to definitively conclude in the manuscript that we were sure these signals arose from the dorsal raphe. We will update this language in a revised version of the manuscript to ensure that these limitations are clear (referring to Supplementary Figures S14-15, S17).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The data generated for this paper provides an important resource for the neuroscience community. The locus coeruleus (LC) is the known seed of noradrenergic cells in the brain. Due to its location and size, it remains scarcely profiled in humans. Despite the physically minute structure containing these cells, its impact is wide-reaching due to the known neuromodulatory function of norepinephrine (NE) in processes like attention and mood. As such, profiling NE cells has important implications for most neurological and neuropsychiatric disorders. This paper generates transcriptomic profiles that are not only cell-specific but which also maintain their spatial context, providing the field with a map for the cells within the region.

      Strengths:

      Using spatial transcriptomics in a morphologically distinct region is a very attractive way to generate a map. Overlaying macroscopic information, i.e. a region with greater pigmentation, with its corresponding molecular profile in an unbiased manner is an extremely powerful way to understand the specific cellular and molecular composition of that brain structure.

      The technologies were used with an astute awareness of their limitations, as such, multiple technologies were leveraged to paint a more complete and resolved picture of the cellular composition of the region. For example, the lack of resolution in the spatial transcriptomic platform was compensated by complementary snRNA-seq and single molecule FISH.

      This work has been made publicly available and accessible through a user-friendly application such that any interested researcher can investigate the level of expression of their gene of interest within this region.

      Two important implications from this work are 1) the potential that the gene regulatory profiles of these cells are only partially conserved across species, humans, and rodents, and 2) that there may be other neuromodulatory cell types within the region that were otherwise not previously localized to the LC

      Weaknesses:

      Given that the markers used to identify cells are not as specific as they need to be to definitively qualify the desired cell type, the results may be over-interpreted. Specifically, TH is the primary marker used to qualify cells as noradrenergic, however, TH catalyzes the synthesis of L-DOPA, a precursor to dopamine, which in turn is a precursor for epinephrine and norepinephrine suggesting some of the cells in the region may be dopaminergic and not NE cells. Indeed, there are publications to support the presence of dopaminergic cells in the LC (see Kempadoo et al. 2016, Takeuchi et al., 2016, Devoto et al. 2005). This discrepancy is further highlighted by the apparent lack of overlap per given Visium spots with TH, SCL6A2, or DBH. While the single-nucleus FISH confirms that some of the cells in the region are noradrenergic, others very possibly represent a different catecholamine. As such it is suggested that the nomenclature for the cells be reconsidered.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment, and are aware of the reports suggesting the potential presence of dopaminergic cells in the LC. We initially had the same thought as the reviewer when we observed Visium spots in the spatial data with lack of overlap between TH, SLC6A2, and DBH as well as single nuclei in the snRNA-seq data with lack of overlap between TH, SLC6A2, and DBH. This surprising result was exactly why we performed the smFISH/RNAscope experiment with these three marker genes. Given known issues with read depth and coverage in the 10x Genomics assays, we wanted to better understand if this was a technical limitation in the sequencing coverage, or rather a true biological finding. The RNAscope data showed very clearly that nearly every cell body we looked at had co-localization of these three marker genes. We included an image from a single capture array of one tissue section in Supplementary Figure 11, but could, in a revised version of the manuscript, provide additional examples to illustrate how conclusive the images were by visualization. As such, we were quite convinced that the lack of overlap on Visium spots and in single nuclei in the snRNA-seq data was more likely related to technical issues with sequencing coverage, rather than a biological finding. We also note that we checked for the presence of the dopamine transporter, SLC6A3, and as can be appreciated in the iSEE web app for the snRNA-seq data or the R/Shiny web app for the Visium data, there is virtually no expression of SLC6A3 in the dataset, which in our view provides additional evidence against the possibility that there are substantial quantities of dopaminergic cells in this human LC dataset. We will include supplementary plots showing the lack of SLC6A3 expression in a revised version of the manuscript.

      The authors are unable to successfully implement unsupervised clustering with the spatial data, this greatly reduces the impact of the spatial technology as it implies that the transcriptomic data generated in the study did not have enough resolution to identify individual cell types.

      The reviewer is correct -- this is a fundamental limitation of the 10x Genomics Visium platform, i.e. the spatial resolution captures multiple cells per spot (e.g. around 1-10 cells per spot in human brain tissue). We note that new spatial platforms now provide cellular resolution (e.g. Vizgen MERSCOPE, 10x Genomics Xenium, 10x Genomics Visium HD), which will help address this in future work. However, many of these cellular-resolution in situ sequencing platforms have the limitation that they do not quantify genome-wide expression, and instead require users to select a priori gene panels to investigate. This is a problem if no genome-wide reference datasets are available. Hence, despite the limited spatial resolution of the Visium platform, this dataset is useful precisely for helping investigators choose gene panels for higher-resolution platforms or higher-order smFISH multiplexing.

      We also applied spatial clustering (using BayesSpace; Zhao et al. 2021) to attempt to segment the LC regions within the Visium samples in a data-driven manner as an alternative to the manual annotations, which was unsuccessful (and hence we relied on the manually annotated regions for downstream analyses) (Supplementary Figure S5). However, this is a different application of unsupervised clustering, which is separate from the task of identifying cell types.

      The sample contribution to the results is highly unbalanced, which consequently, may result in ungeneralizable findings in terms of regional cellular composition, limiting the usefulness of the publicly available data.

      We acknowledge the limitations of the work due to the small/unbalanced sample sizes. As mentioned above for Reviewer 1, this was an initial study in this region -- results of which will inform our (and hopefully others’) experimental design and approach to molecular profiling in this difficult to access brain region. Overall, this study was executed with finite tissue and financial resources and was intended to uncover limitations and help develop best practices and design workflows for future studies with larger numbers of donors and samples. Given the limited data availability for this brain region, we wanted to make this dataset available for the research community immediately. In addition, we note that making this genome-wide dataset available will help inform targeted gene panel design for higher-resolution platforms (e.g. 10x Genomics Xenium).

      This study aimed to deeply profile the LC in humans and provide a resource to the community. The combination of data types (snRNA-seq, SRT, smFISH) does in fact represent this resource for the community. However, due to the limitations, of which, some were described in the manuscript, we should be cautious in the use of the data for secondary analysis. For example, some of the cellular annotations may lack precision, the cellular composition also may not reflect the general population, and the presence of unexpected cell types may represent the accidental inclusion of adjacent regions, in this case, serotonergic cells from the Raphe nucleus.

      We agree, and have attempted to explain these limitations in the manuscript. We will clarify the language regarding the interpretation of the annotated cell populations and unexpected cell types, and the limited sample sizes, in a revised version of the manuscript.

      Nonetheless having a well-developed app to query and visualize these data will be an enormous asset to the community especially given the lack of information regarding the region in general.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      […] This study has many strengths. It is the first reported comprehensive map of the human LC transcriptome, and uses two independent but complementary approaches (spatial transcriptomics and snRNA-seq). Some of the key findings confirmed what has been described in the rodent LC, as well as some intriguing potential genes and modules identified that may be unique to humans and have the potential to explain LC-related disease states. The main limitations of the study were acknowledged by the authors and include the spatial resolution probably not being at the single cell level and the relatively small number of samples (and questionable quality) for the snRNA-seq data. Overall, the strengths greatly outweigh the limitations. This dataset will be a valuable resource for the neuroscience community, both in terms of methodology development and results that will no doubt enable important comparisons and follow-up studies.

      Major comments:

      Overall, the discovery of some cells in the LC region that express serotonergic markers is intriguing. However, no evidence is presented that these neurons actually produce 5-HT.

      The reviewer is correct that we did not provide any additional evidence to show that these neurons actually produce 5-HT. As noted above in the response to Reviewer 1, in our view, the most likely explanation is that these neurons are from dorsal raphe contamination on the tissue section. However, due to technical and logistical limitations in this study, we could not definitively say this because we did not clearly track the orientation of the tissue sections, and we did not have remaining tissue sections from all donor tissue blocks to repeat RNAscope experiments. For some of the donors, where we had remaining tissue sections to go back to repeat RNAscope experiments after completion of the snRNA-seq and Visium assays, we could see clear separation of the LC region / LC-NE neuron core from where putative 5-HT neurons were located (Supplementary Figure 15). However, we did not have sufficient tissue resources to map this definitively in all donors, and the orientation and anatomy of each tissue block were not fully annotated.

      Due to the lack of clarity, and the fact that there have been reports that LC-NE neurons express serotonergic markers (Iijima 1993; Iijima 1989), we felt that it was premature to definitively declare that these putative 5-HT neurons that we identified were definitively from the raphe. We will clarify the language around this discrepancy in a revised version of the manuscript to ensure that these limitations are clearly described.

      Concerning the snRNA-seq experiments, it is unclear why only 3 of the 5 donors were used, particularly given the low number of LC-NE nuclear transcriptomes obtained, why those 3 were chosen, and how many 100 um sections were used from each donor. It is also unclear if the 295 nuclei obtained truly representative of the LC population or whether they are just the most "resilient" LC nuclei that survive the process.

      As discussed above for Reviewer 1, the reason we included only 3 of the 5 donors for the snRNA-seq assays was due to the tissue availability on the tissue blocks. We will clarify the language in a revised version of the manuscript to make this limitation more clear. We will also include additional details in the Methods section on the number of 100 μm sections used for each donor (which varied between 10-15, approximating 60-80 mg of tissue).

      The LC displays rostral/caudal and dorsal/ventral differences, including where they project, which functions they regulate, and which parts are vulnerable in neurodegenerative disease (e.g. Loughlin et al., Neuroscience 18:291-306, 1986; Dahl et al., Nat Hum Behav 3:1203-14, 2019; Beardmore et al., J Alzheimer's Dis 83:5-22, 2021; Gilvesy et al., Acta Neuropathol 144:651-76, 2022; Madelung et al., Mov Disord 37:479-89, 2022). It was not clear which part(s) of the LC was captured for the SRT and snRNAseq experiments.

      As discussed above for Reviewer 1, a limitation of this study was that we did not record the orientation of the anatomy of the tissue sections, precluding our ability to annotate the tissue sections with the rostral/caudal and dorsal/ventral axis labels. We agree with the reviewer that additional spatial studies, in future work, could offer needed and important information about expression profiles across the spatial axes (rostral/caudal, ventral/dorsal) of the LC. Our study provides us with insight about optimizing the dissections for spatial assays, as well as bringing to light a number of technical and logistical issues that we had not initially foreseen. For example, during the course of this study and parallel, ongoing work in other, small, challenging regions, we have now developed a number of specialized technical and logistical strategies for keeping track of orientation and mounting serial sections from the same tissue block onto a single spatial array, which is extremely technically challenging. We are now well-prepared for addressing these issues in future studies with larger numbers of donors and samples in order to make these types of insights.

      The authors mention that in other human SRT studies, there are typically between 1-10 cells per expression spot. I imagine that this depends heavily on the part of the brain being studied and neuronal density, but it was unclear how many LC cells were contained in each expression spot.

      The reviewer is correct that we did not include this information in the manuscript. We attempted to apply a computational method to count nuclei contained in each gene expression spot based on analyzing the histological H&E images (VistoSeg; Tippani et al. 2022), which we have developed and previously applied in data from the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) (Maynard and Collado-Torres et al. 2021). Based on the segmentation using this workflow we observe that the counts in this region are similar to what we observed in the DLPFC, i.e., typically between 1-10 LC cells per expression spot, with approximately 1-2 LC-NE neurons (which are characterized by their large size) per expression spot. However, these analyses had several technical issues related to the images themselves, the relatively large size and pigmentation of LC-NE neurons, and parameter settings that had been optimized for different brain regions. We are currently optimizing this analysis workflow for these images to provide more accurate estimates of cell counts per spot to give readers additional context on the number of nuclei per spot in the annotated LC regions and outside the LC regions in a revised version of the manuscript.

      Regarding comparison of human LC-associated genes with rat or mouse LC-associated genes (Fig. 2D-F), the authors speculate that the modest degree of overlap may be due to species differences between rodents and human and/or methodological differences (SRT vs microarray vs TRAP). Was there greater overlap between mouse and rat than between mouse/rat and human? If so, that is evidence for the former. If not, that is evidence for the latter. Also would be useful for more in-depth comparison with snRNA-seq data from mouse LC: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2022.06.30.498327v1.

      We will investigate this question and discuss this in updated results in a revised version of the manuscript.

      The finding of ACHE expression in LC neurons is intriguing, especially in light of work from Susan Greenfield suggesting that ACHE has functions independent of ACH metabolism that contributes to cellular vulnerability in neurodegenerative disease.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We were very surprised too by the observed expression of SLC5A7 and ACHE in the LC regions (Visium data) and within the LC-NE neuron cluster (snRNA-seq data), coupled with absence of other typical cholinergic marker genes (e.g. CHAT, SLC18A3), and we do not have a compelling explanation or theory for this. Hence, the work of Susan Greenfield and colleagues suggesting non-cholinergic actions of ACHE, particularly in other catecholaminergic neurons (e.g. dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra) is very interesting. We will include references to this work and how it could inform interpretation of this expression in a revised version of the manuscript (Greenfield 1991; Halliday and Greenfield 2012).

      High mitochondrial reads from snRNA-seq can indicate lower quality. It was not clear why, given the mitochondrial read count, the authors are confident in the snRNA-seq data from presumptive LC-NE neurons.

      We will include additional analyses to further investigate and/or confirm this finding (e.g. comparing sum of UMI counts / number of detected genes and mitochondrial percentage per nucleus for this population to confirm data quality) in additional supplementary figures in a revised version of the manuscript.

      References

      • Greenfield (1991), A noncholinergic action of acetylcholinesterase (AChE) in the brain: from neuronal secretion to the generation of movement, Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology, 11, 1, 55-77.

      • Halliday and Greenfield (2012), From protein to peptides: a spectrum of non-hydrolytic functions of acetylcholinesterase, Protein & Peptide Letters, 19, 2, 165-172.

      • Iannitelli et al. (2023), The neurotoxin DSP-4 dysregulates the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system and recapitulates molecular and behavioral aspects of prodromal neurodegenerative disease, eNeuro, 10, 1, ENEURO.0483-22.2022.

      • Iijima K. (1989), An immunocytochemical study on the GABA-ergic and serotonin-ergic neurons in rat locus ceruleus with special reference to possible existence of the masked indoleamine cells. Acta Histochema, 87, 1, 43-57.

      • Iijima K. (1993), Chemocytoarchitecture of the rat locus ceruleus, Histology and Histopathology, 8, 3, 581-591.

      • Luskin A.T., Li L. et al. (2022), A diverse network of pericoerulear neurons control arousal states, bioRxiv (preprint).

      • Maynard and Collado-Torres et al. (2021), Transcriptome-scale spatial gene expression in the human dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, Nature Neuroscience, 24, 425-436.

      • Tippani et al. (2022), VistoSeg: processing utilities for high-resolution Visium/Visium-IF images for spatial transcriptomics data, bioRxiv (preprint).

      • Tran M.N., Maynard K.R. et al. (2021), Single-nucleus transcriptome analysis reveals cell-type-specific molecular signatures across reward circuitry in the human brain, Neuron, 109, 3088-3103.

      • Zhao E. et al. (2021), Spatial transcriptomics at subspot resolution with BayesSpace, Nature Biotechnology, 39, 1375-1384.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      Reviewer #2:

      Line 295 – was the time post-infection, which varies considerably between groups and across samples, taken into consideration when comparison of response was between ChatCre mice (4-9 weeks post-infection) and WT mice (four to five weeks post-infection)?

      Thank you for your comment. We did not originally assess the effects of time post-injection on DREADD response. Generally, AAV transgene expression has been demonstrated to be long-term and stable in the CNS of mice.[1] However, there is some variation in the reporting time of peak transgene expression[2], and this may potentially impact our results.

      In investigating this issue further, we discovered an error in our reporting as we did have n = 1 wild-type mouse that underwent EMG recordings 62 days (~9 weeks) post-AAV injection. This has been corrected in the manuscript (lines 87-88).

      Addressing this question is challenging due to the uneven distribution of time points within the 4–9-week windows for each group. Essentially, there were two groups per cohort, one studied at 4-5 weeks and one at 8-9 weeks. More specifically:

      - Wild-type cohort: n = 10 animals were studied 28–33 days post-injection, and n = 1 at 62 days.

      - ChAT-Cre cohort: n = 4 animals were studied 28–30 days post-injection, and n = 5 at 56–59 days.

      We performed Pearson correlation analyses between time post-injection and diaphragm EMG response to DREADD activation (peak amplitude and area under the curve, AUC) for both cohorts (Author response image 1):

      - ChAT-Cre: No significant correlations were found (peak amplitude: r<sup>2</sup> = -0.117, r = -0.1492, p = 0.702, Figure 1a-b; AUC:r<sup>2</sup> = -0.0883, r = 0.2184, p = 0.572, Figure 1c-d).

      - Wild type: Initial analysis of all data showed significant correlations (peak amplitude:r<sup>2</sup> = 0.362, r = 0.6523, p = 0.0296, Figure 1a; AUC: r<sup>2</sup> = 0.347, r = 0.6424, p = 0.033, Figure 1c), suggesting a moderate positive correlation between time post-injection and EMG response. However, when the single 8–9-week wild-type mouse was excluded, these correlations were no longer significant (peak amplitude: r<sup>2</sup> = 0.172, r = 0.5142, p = 0.128, Figure 1b; AUC: r<sup>2</sup> = 0.23, r = 0.5614, p = 0.0913, Figure1d).

      Comparing wild-type and ChAT-Cre groups directly was unreliable due to the single wild-type mouse studied at the later time point. We attempted to model time post-injection as a continuous variable (i.e., exact days post-injection) using a restricted maximum likelihood mixed linear model in JMP; however, the analysis could not be performed because there were not sufficient overlapping time points between the two cohorts (i.e., not all days post-injection were represented in both groups). To mitigate this, we binned animals into two groups: 4–5 weeks and 8–9 weeks post-injection. This analysis returned a significant interaction between cohort and time post-injection (p = 0.0391), however there were no significant multiple comparisons upon Tukey post hoc test (i.e., p > 0.05).

      Based on these findings, we feel confident that time post-injection is unlikely to have a significant impact on diaphragm EMG response to DREADD activation in the ChAT-Cre cohort. However, in the wild-type cohort, it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions, as only one animal was studied at the 8–9-week time point. For similar reasons, it remains unclear whether the relationship between time post-AAV transduction and DREADD response differs between cohorts. Given the inconclusive nature of these results, we have elected not to include this analysis in the manuscript. Nevertheless, to ensure transparency, we have provided Author response image 1 below of peak amplitude and AUC plotted against time, allowing readers to evaluate the data independently.

      Author response image 1.

      Plots of diaphragm EMG peak amplitude (a-b) and area under the curve (c-d) vs. days post-AAV injection for wild-type (blue) and ChAT-Cre (orange) mice. Pearson correlation analyses were performed to assess the relationship between time post-AAV injection and diaphragm EMG DREADD response in wild-type and ChAT-Cre mouse cohorts. r<sup>2</sup>, r, and p-values are shown in each panel for both cohorts. Panels a and c display peak amplitude and AUC, respectively, including all animals. Panels b and d present the same variables with the n = 1 wild-type mouse at the 9-week time point excluded; ChAT-Cre data is unchanged between corresponding panels. Scatter points represent data from individual animals. Polynomial trendlines are displayed for each cohort with wild-type in blue and ChAT-Cre in orange.

      REFERENCES

      (1) Kim, J. Y., Grunke, S. D., Levites, Y., Golde, T. E. & Jankowsky, J. L. Intracerebroventricular viral injection of the neonatal mouse brain for persistent and widespread neuronal transduction. J Vis Exp, 51863 (2014). https://doi.org/10.3791/51863

      (2) Hollidge, B. S. et al. Kinetics and durability of transgene expression after intrastriatal injection of AAV9 vectors. Front Neurol 13, 1051559 (2022). https://doi.org/10.3389/fneur.2022.1051559


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Response to reviewer’s public reviews:

      We chose the dose of J60 based on a prior publication that established that off-target effects were possible at relatively high doses[1]. The dose that we used (0.1 mg/kg) was 30-fold less than the dose that was reported in that paper to potentially have off-target responses (3 mg/kg). Further, Author response image 1 shows the results of experiments in which J60 was given to animals that did not have the excitatory DREADD expressed in the spinal cord. This includes a sample of mice (n = 2) and rats (n = 3), recorded from using the same diaphragm EMG procedure described in the manuscript. The figure shows that there was no consistent response to the J60 at 0.1 mg/kg in the “control experiment” in which the DREADD was not expressed in the spinal cord.

      Author response image 1.

      Diaphragm EMG response to J60 administrated to naïve rats and mice. Panel a-b show raw EMG values at baseline, following vehicle (saline) and J60 administration for the left and right hemidiaphragm. Panel c-d shows EMG values normalized to baseline. Neither One-way RM ANOVA (panel a-b) nor paired t-test (panel c-d) returned significant p values (p < 0.05).

      Response to specific reviewer comments:

      Reviewer #1:

      How old were the animals at the time of AAV injection, and in subsequent experiments?

      The wildtype cohort of mice were 7-9 weeks old at time of AAV injection and DREADD experiments took place 4-5 weeks after AAV injection. ChAT-Cre mice were 6-10 weeks old at time of AAV injection and DREADD experiments took place 4-9 weeks after AAV injection. ChAT-Cre rats were 2-5 months old at time of AAV spinal injection. These animals underwent plethysmography recordings 3-4 months post-AAV injection and subsequently phrenic nerve recording 3-8 weeks later. These details have been added to the Method section.

      How many mice were excluded from electrophysiology experiments due to deteriorating electrode contact?

      No mice were excluded from electrophysiology experiments due to deteriorating electrode contact. If you are referring to the n = 1 excluded ChAT-Cre mouse (line 368) this animal was excluded because it showed no histological evidence of DREADD expression (lines 200-206).

      What was the urethane dose?

      The urethane dose for phrenic nerve recordings was 2.1 g/kg. See methods section line 395.

      A graphical timeline of the experimental progression for plethysmography and electrophysiology studies would enhance clarity.

      A graphical timeline has been added. See Figure S6.

      Significance indicators in the figures would greatly enhance clarity. It is a little awkward to have to refer to supplemental tables to figure out statistical differences.

      Significance indicators have been added. See Figures 1, 2, 4, and 5

      In Figures 1, 2, and 5, individual data points should be shown, as in Fig 4.

      Thank you for this suggestion. We agree that, in general, it is best practice to scatter individual data points. However, when we drafted the new figures, it was apparent that including individual scatter points, in this case, created very “cluttered” figures that were very difficult to interpret.

      More detail regarding the plethysmography studies is needed. Was saline/J60 infused via a tail vein catheter? Were animals handled during the infusion? How long is the "IV" period? What volume of fluid was delivered?

      All IV infusions were delivered via a tail vein catheter. Animals were not handled during infusion nor at any point during the recording. An IV catheter was externalized via a port in the plethysmograph allowing for IV infusion without handling of the animal or opening the plethysmograph. The infusion period for both saline and J60 was standardized to 2 minutes. The volume of fluid of both saline and J60 was standardized to 0.6 mL. This information has been added to the methods section (lines 408-410, 415-16, 419-420).

      Reviewer #2:

      The abstract could be improved by briefly highlighting the rationale, scope, and novelty of the study - the intro does a great job of highlighting the scope of the study and the research questions.

      A brief explanation of the rationale, scope, and novelty of the study has been added to the abstract. See lines 2-8.

      Line 18, specifies that this was done under urethane anesthesia.

      This detail has been added to the abstract (line 20).

      The methods section should be moved to the end of the manuscript according to Journal policy.

      The methods section has been moved to the end of the manuscript.

      The authors mention the use of both female and male rats but it is not indicated if they tested for and observed any differences between sexes across experiments.

      We included the use of both male and female animals in this study to improve the generalizability of the results. However, we were not adequately powered for sex comparisons and therefore did not perform any statistical analysis to assess differences between sexes across experiments. Text has been added to the methods section (lines 534-537) to clarify.

      Line 40, since delivery of J60 was performed in both IV and IP, this general statement should be updated.

      This detail has been revised to include both IV and IP. See line 43.

      Line 42. "First, we determined if effective diaphragm activation requires focal DREADD expression targeting phrenic motor neurons, or if non-specific expression in the immediate vicinity of the phrenic motor nucleus would be sufficient...." I don't think that in the experiments with wild-type mice the authors can claim that they selectively targeted the cervical propriospinal network (in isolation from the motoneurons). Given the fact that the histological analysis did not quantify interneurons or motoneurons in the spinal cord, authors should be cautious in proposing which neuronal population is activated in the non-specific approach.

      We agree, and this was a poorly worded statement in our original text. We agree that wild-type DREADD expression was not limited to the cervical propriospinal networks but likely a mix of interneurons and motoneurons. The text has been edited to reflect that (see lines 56-60).

      AAV virus source is not described.

      All AAVs were obtained from the UF Powell Gene Therapy Center. Details of virus source and production have been added to the methods section. See lines 336-347.

      Line 108-125. Because the diaphragm EMG recordings are only described for mice here, I would suggest editing this methods section to clearly state mice instead of vaguely describing "animals" in the procedure.

      “Animals” has been changed to “mice” to avoid ambiguity.

      Line 120, add parenthesis.

      Parenthesis has been added.

      Line 126. Whole body plethysmography protocol. Three hypercapnic hypoxic challenges are a lot for a rat within a 3-hour recording session in freely behaving rats. Did the authors verify with control/ vehicle experiments that repeated challenges in the absence of J60 do not cause potentiation of the response? I understand that it is not possible to invert the order of the injections (due to likely long-term effects of J60) or it is too late to perform vehicle and J60 injections on different days, but controls for repeated challenges should be performed in this type of experiment, especially considering the great variability in the response observed in Figure 4 (in normoxic conditions).

      We did not conduct control experiments to assess the impact of repeated hypercapnic hypoxic challenges on the naïve response (i.e., in the absence of J60). However, our experimental protocol was designed such that each experimental period (i.e., post-vehicle or post-J60 infusion) was normalized to baseline recordings taken immediately prior to the vehicle or J60 infusion. While repeated exposure to hypercapnic hypoxic challenges may have altered respiratory output, we are confident that normalizing each experimental period to its respective baseline effectively captures the impact of DREADD activation on ventilation, independent of any potential potentiation that may have occurred due to gas challenge exposure. We have included raw values for all plethysmography outcomes (see Figure 4, panels a-c) to ensure full data transparency. Still, we believe that the baseline-normalized values more accurately reflect the impact of DREADD activation on the components of ventilation.

      Furthermore, why the response to the hypercapnic hypoxic challenges are not reported? These could be very interesting to determine the effects of DREADD stimulation on chemosensory responses and enhance the significance of the study.

      Response to the hypercapnic hypoxic challenges has been added to the manuscript. See Figure S3 and results section lines 162-167. Briefly, there were no statistically significant (p < 0.05) differences in tidal volume, respiratory rate, or minute ventilation between J60 vs sham condition during hypercapnic-hypoxic ventilatory challenges.

      Line 200 - what is the reason behind performing a qualitative analysis of mCherry in various quadrants? This limits the interpretation of the results. If the authors used Chat-cre rats, the virus should only be in Chat+ MN. Knowing how selective the virus is, and whether its expression was selective for Phrenic MN versus other MN pools, could address several technical questions.

      We agree that detailed quantification of expression by motoneuron pool would be of value in future work.  However, for these initial proof-of-concept experiments, we performed the quadrant-based qualitative analysis of mCherry expression to provide a simple comparison of mCherry expression between groups (i.e., ChAT-Cre vs. wildtype mice). This analysis allowed us to: 1) show the reader that each animal included in the study showed evidence of mCherry expression and 2) give the reader an idea of patterns of mCherry expression throughout the mid-cervical spinal cord. Additionally, it is important to note that while ChAT is a marker of motoneurons some populations of interneurons also express ChAT(2-4).

      Given the increased values of Dia EMG AUC and no changes in respiratory rate, did the authors determine if there was a change in the inspiratory time with J60 administration?

      We did not assess inspiratory time.

      High death rate in DREADD WT mice - was histological analysis performed on these mice? Could it be due to the large volume injected into the spinal cord that affects not only descending pathways but also ascending ones? Or caused by neuronal death due to the large volume of viral solution in injected in mice.

      Histological analysis was performed on these animals to assess mCherry expression only (i.e., no staining for NeuN or other markers was performed). While the reviewer's speculations are reasonable, we feel these reasons are unlikely to explain the death rate in DREADD WT mice as ChAT-Cre mice received the same volume injected into their spine and lived up until and during diaphragm EMG recordings. Additionally, WT mice lived for 4-5 weeks post-injection which would be past the acute phase that a large immune response to the viral dose would have occurred.

      Line 299-304. Can you please clarify whether these rats were tested under anesthesia?

      These rats were assessed under anesthesia. This detail has been added (line 146).

      Given some of the unexpected results on cardiovascular parameters in urethane anesthetized rats, did the authors test the effects of J60 in the absence of AAV construct infection?

      A small cohort (n = 2) of urethane anesthetized naïve wildtype rats were given the J60 ligand (IV, 0.1 mg/kg dose). We did observe a sudden drop in blood pressure after J60 administration that was sustained for the duration of the recording. One animal showed a 12% decrease in mean arterial blood pressure following J60 administration while the other showed a 35% decrease. Thus, it does appear that in this preparation the J60 ligand is producing a drop in arterial blood pressure.

      Line 393. I believe this comment is referred to the intrapleural and diaphragmatic injection. Maybe this should clarified in the sentence.

      This sentence has been revised for clarity (see lines 248-250).

      Figures 1 and 2. It would be informative to show raw traces of the Diaphragm EMG to demonstrate the increase in tonic EMG. It is not possible to determine that from the integrated traces in Figures 1A and B.

      Thank you for bringing up this concern. While the mean data in Figures 1F and 2F do indicate that, on average, animals had tonic diaphragm EMG responses to DREADD activation, the examples given in Figures 1A and 2A show minimal responses. This makes it difficult to fully appreciate the tonic response from those particular traces. However, clear tonic activity can be appreciated from Figures 5A and S2. In these figures, tonic activity is evident from the integrated EMG signals, presenting as a sustained increase in baseline activity between bursts—essentially an upward shift from the zero point.

      References

      (1) Van Savage, J. & Avegno, E. M. High dose administration of DREADD agonist JHU37160 produces increases in anxiety-like behavior in male rats. Behav Brain Res 452, 114553 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2023.114553

      (2) Mesnage, B. et al. Morphological and functional characterization of cholinergic interneurons in the dorsal horn of the mouse spinal cord. J Comp Neurol 519, 3139-3158 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.22668

      (3) Gotts, J., Atkinson, L., Yanagawa, Y., Deuchars, J. & Deuchars, S. A. Co-expression of GAD67 and choline acetyltransferase in neurons in the mouse spinal cord: A focus on lamina X. Brain Res 1646, 570-579 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2016.07.001

      (4) Alkaslasi, M. R. et al. Single nucleus RNA-sequencing defines unexpected diversity of cholinergic neuron types in the adult mouse spinal cord. Nat Commun 12, 2471 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-021-22691-2

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We thank the public reviewers and editors for their insightful comments on the manuscript. We have made the following changes to address their concerns and think the resulting manuscript is stronger as a result. Specifically, we have 1) added RNA FISH data of specific STB-2 and STB-3 RNA markers to confirm their distribution changes between STB<sup>in</sup> and STB<sup>out</sup> TOs, 2) removed language throughout the text that refer to STB-3 as a terminally differentiated nuclear subtype, and 3) generated CRISPR-mediated knock-outs of two genes identified by network analysis and validated their rolse in mediating STB nuclear subtype gene expression.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Strengths: 

      The study offers a comprehensive SC- and SN-based characterization of trophoblast organoid models, providing a thorough validation of these models against human placental tissues. By comparing the older STB<sup>in</sup> and newer STB<sup>out</sup> models, the authors effectively demonstrate the improvements in the latter, particularly in the differentiation and gene expression profiles of STBs. This work serves as a critical resource for researchers, offering a clear delineation of the similarities and differences between TO-derived and primary STBs. The use of multiple advanced techniques, such as high-resolution sequencing and trajectory analysis, further enhances the study's contribution to the field. 

      Thank you for your thoughtful review—we appreciate your recognition of our efforts to comprehensively validate trophoblast organoid models and highlight key advancements in STB differentiation and gene expression.

      Weaknesses: 

      While the study is robust, some areas could benefit from further clarification. 

      (1) The importance of the TO model's orientation and its impact on outcomes could be emphasized more in the introduction. 

      We agree that TO orientation may significantly influence STB nuclear subtype differentiation. As the STB is critical for both barrier formation and molecular transport in vivo, lack of exposure to the surrounding media in STB<sup>in</sup> TOs in vitro could compromise these functions and the associated environmental cues that influence STB nuclear differentiation. We have added text to the introduction to highlight this point (lines 117-120).

      (2) The differences in cluster numbers/names between primary tissue and TO data need a clearer explanation, and consistent annotation could aid in comparison. 

      Thank you for highlighting that the comparisions and cluster annotations need clarification. In Figure 1, we did not aim to directly compare CTB and STB nuclear subtypes between TOs and tissue. Each dataset was analyzed independently, with clusters determined separately and with different resolutions decided via a clustering algorithm (Zappia and Oshlack, 2018). For example, for the STB, this approach identified seven subtypes in tissue but only two in TOs, making direct comparison challenging. To address this challenge, we integrated the SN datasets from TOs and tissue in Figure 6. This integration allowed us to directly compare gene expression between the sample types and examine the proportions within each STB subtype. Similarly, in Figure 2, direct comparison of individual CTB or STB clusters across the separate datasets is challenging (Figures 2A-C) due to differences in clustering. To overcome this, we integrated the datasets to compare cluster gene expression and relative proportions (Figures 2D-E). Nonetheless, to address the reviewers concern we have added text to the results section to clarify that subclusters of CTB and STB between datasets should not be directly compared until the datasets are integrated in Figure 2D-E and Figure 6 (lines 166-167).

      (3) The rationale for using SN sequencing over SC sequencing for TO evaluations should be clarified, especially regarding the potential underrepresentation of certain trophoblast subsets. 

      This is an important point as the challenges of studying a giant syncytial cell are often underappreciated by researchers that study mononucleated cells. We have added text to the introduction to clarify why traditional single cell RNA sequencing techniques were inadequate to collect  and characterize the STB (lines 91-93).

      (4) Additionally, more evidence could be provided to support the claims about STB differentiation in the STB<sup>out</sup> model and to determine whether its differentiation trajectory is unique or simply more advanced than in STB<sup>in</sup>. 

      Our original conclusion that STB<sup>out</sup> nuclei are more terminally differentiated than STB<sup>in</sup> was based on two observations: (1) STB<sup>out</sup> TOs exhibit increased expression of STB-specific pregnancy hormones and many classic STB marker genes and (2) STB<sup>out</sup> nuclei show an enrichment of the STB-3 nuclear subtype, which appears at the end of the slingshot pseudotime trajectory. However, upon consideration of the reviewer comments, we agree that this evidence is not sufficient to definitively distinguish if STB<sup>out</sup> nuclei are more advanced or follow a unique differentiation trajectory dependent on new environmental cues. Pseudotime analyses provided only a predictive framework for lineage tracing, and these predictions must be experimentally validated. Real-time tracking of STB nuclear subtypes in TOs would require a suite of genetic tools beyond the scope of this study. Therefore, to address the reviewers' concerns we have removed language suggesting that STB-3 is a terminally differentiated subtype or that STB<sup>out</sup> nuclei are more differentiated than STB<sup>in</sup> nuclei throughout the text until the discussion. Therein we present both our original hypothesis (that STB nuclei are further differentiated in STB<sup>out</sup>) and alternative explanations like changing trajectories due to local environmental cues (lines 619-625).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Strengths: 

      (1) The use of SN and SC RNA sequencing provides a detailed analysis of STB formation and differentiation. 

      (2) The identification of distinct STB subtypes and novel gene markers such as RYBP offers new insights into STB development. 

      Thank you for highlighting these strengths—we appreciate your recognition of our use of SN and SC RNA sequencing to analyze STB differentiation and the discovery of distinct STB subtypes and novel gene markers like RYBP.

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) Inconsistencies in data presentation. 

      We address the individual comments of reviewer 2 later in this response.

      (2) Questionable interpretation of lncRNA signals: The use of long non-coding RNA (lncRNA) signals as cell type-specific markers may represent sequencing noise rather than true markers. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s attention to detail in noticing the lncRNA signature seen in many STB nuclear subtypes. However, we disagree that these molecules simply represent sequencing noise. In fact, may studies have rigorously demonstrated that lncRNAs have both cell and tissue specific gene expression (e.g., Zhao et al 2022, Isakova et al 2021, Zheng et al 2020). Further, they have been shown to be useful markers of unique cell types during development (e.g., Morales-Vicente et al 2022, Zhou et al 2019, Kim et al 2015) and can enhance clustering interpretability in breast cancer (Malagoli et al 2024). Many lncRNAs have also been demonstrated to play a functional role in the human placenta, including H19, MEG3, and MEG8 (Adu-Gyamfi et al 2023) and differences are even seen in nuclear subtypes in trophoblast stem cells (Khan et al 2021). Therefore, we prefer to keep these lncRNA signatures included and let future researchers test their functional role.

      To improve the study's validity and significance, it is crucial to address the inconsistencies and to provide additional evidence for the claims. Supplementing with immunofluorescence staining for validating the distribution of STB_in, STB_out, and EVT_enrich in the organoid models is recommended to strengthen the results and conclusions. 

      Each general trophoblast cell type (CTB, STB, EVT) has been visualized by immunofluorescence by the Coyne laboratory in their initial papers characterizing the STB<sup>in</sup>, STB<sup>out</sup>, and EVT<sup>enrich</sup> models (Yang et al, 2022 and 2023). We agree that it is important to validate the STB nuclear subtypes found in our genomic study. However, one challenge in studying a syncytia is that immunofluorescence may not be a definitive method when the nuclei share a common cytoplasm. This is because protein products from mRNAs transcribed in one nucleus are translated in the cytoplasm and could diffuse beyond sites of transcription. Therefore, RNA fluorescence in situ hybridization (RNA-FISH) is instead needed. While a systematic characterization of the spatial distribution of the many marker genes found each subtype is outside the scope of this study, we include RNA-FISH of one STB-2 marker (PAPPA2) and one STB-3 marker (ADAMTS6) in Figure 3F-G and Supplemental Figure 3.3. This demonstrates there is an increase in STB-2 marker gene expression in STB<sup>in</sup> TOs and an increase in STB-3 marker gene expression in STB<sup>out</sup> TOs. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):  

      The authors present outstanding progress toward their aim of identifying, "the underlying control of the syncytiotrophoblast". They identify the chromatin remodeler, RYBP, as well as other regulatory networks that they propose are critical to syncytiotrophoblast development. This study is limited in fully addressing the aim, however, as functional evidence for the contributions of the factors/pathways to syncytiotrophoblast cell development is needed. Future experimentation testing the hypotheses generated by this work will define the essentiality of the identified factors to syncytiotrophoblast development and function. 

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful assessment, constructive feedback, and encouraging comments. We acknowledge that the initial manuscript primarily presented analyses suggesting correlations between RYBP and other factors identified in the gene network analysis and STB function. Understanding how gene networks in the STB are formed and regulated is a long-term goal that will require many experiments with collaborative efforts across multiple research groups.

      Nonetheless, to address this concern we have knocked out two key genes, RYBP and AFF1, in TOs using CRISPR-Cas9-mediated gene targeting. Bulk RNA sequencing of STB<sup>in</sup> TOs from both wild-type (WT) and knockout strains revealed that deletion of either gene caused a statistically significant decrease in the expression of the pregnancy hormone human placental lactogen and an increase in the expression of several genes characteristic of the oxygen-sensing STB-2 subtype, including FLT-1, PAPPA2, SPON2, and SFXN3. These findings demonstrate that knocking out RYBP or AFF1 results in an increase in STB-2 marker gene expression and therefore play a role in inhibiting their expression in WT TOs (Figure 5D-E and supplemental Figure 5.2). We also note that this is the first application of CRISPR-mediated gene silencing in a TO model.

      Future work will visualize the distribution of STB nuclear subtypes in these mutants and explore the mechanistic role of RYBP and AFF1 in STB nuclear subtype formation and maintenance. However, these investigations fall outside the scope of the current study.

      Localization and validation of the identified factors within tissue and at the protein level will also provide further contextual evidence to address the hypotheses generated. 

      We agree that visualizing STB nuclear subtype distribution is essential for testing the many hypotheses generated by our analysis. To address this, we have included RNA-FISH experiments for two STB subtype markers (PAPPA2 for STB-2 and ADAMTS6 for STB-3) in TOs. These experiments reveal an increase in PAPPA2 expression in STB<sup>in</sup> TOs and an increase in ADAMTS6 expression in STB<sup>out</sup> TOs (Figure 3F-G and Supplemental Figure 3.3). Genomic studies serve as powerful hypothesis generators, and we look forward to future work—both our own and that of other researchers—to validate the markers and hypotheses presented from our analysis.

      Recommendations for the authors: 

      Reviewing Editor Comments: 

      We strongly encourage the authors to further strengthen the study by addressing all reviewers' comments and recommendations, with particular attention to the following key aspects:

      (1) Clarifying the uniqueness of the STB differentiation trajectory between STB<sup>in</sup> and STB<sup>out</sup>, and determining whether STB<sup>out</sup> represents a more advanced stage of differentiation compared to STB<sup>in</sup>. It is also important to specify which developmental stage of placental villi the STB<sup>out</sup> and STB<sup>in</sup> are simulating. 

      We have revised the manuscript to remove definitive language claiming that STB-3 represents a terminally differentiated subtype or that STB<sup>out</sup> nuclei are more differentiated than STB<sup>in</sup> nuclei. Instead, we now present our hypothesis and alternative explanations in the discussion (lines 619-625), and emphasize the need for experimental validation of pseudotime predictions to test these hypotheses.

      (2) Utilizing immunofluorescence to validate the distribution of cell types in the organoid models. 

      The Coyne lab has previously performed immunofluorescence of CTB and STB markers in STB<sup>in</sup> and STB<sup>out</sup> TOs (Yang et al 2023). The syncytial nature of STBs complicates immunofluorescence-based validation of the STB nuclear subtypes due translating proteins all sharing a single common cytoplasm and therefore being able to diffuse and mix. Instead, we performed RNA-FISH for two STB subtype markers (PAPPA2, STB-2 and ADAMTS6, STB-3), which showed subtype-specific nuclear enrichment in STB<sup>in</sup> and STB<sup>out</sup> TOs, respectively (Figure 3F-G and Supplemental Figure 3.3).  

      (3) Addressing concerns regarding the use of lncRNA as cell marker genes. Employing canonical markers alongside critical TFs involved in differentiation pathways to perform a more robust cell-type analysis and validation is recommended.  

      As discussed in detail above, we maintain that lncRNAs are valuable markers, supported by their demonstrated roles in cell and tissue specificity and placental function. These signatures provide important insights and hypotheses for future research, and we have clarified this rationale in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) The authors have presented an extensive SC- and SN-based characterization of their improved trophoblast TO model, including a comparison to human placental tissues and the previous TO iteration. In this way, the authors' work represents an invaluable resource for investigators by providing thorough validation of the TO model and a clear description of the similarities and differences between primary and TO-derived STBs. I would suggest that the authors reshape the study to further highlight and emphasize this aspect of the study. 

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful recommendation and agree that our datasets will serve as an invaluable resource for comparing in vitro models to in vivo gene expression. However, extensive validation is required to make definitive conclusions about the extent to which these systems mirror one another and where they diverge. For this reason, in this manuscript, we have focused on characterizing STB subtypes to provide a foundational understanding of the model and this poorly characterized subtype.

      (2) Introduction, Paragraph 3: What is the importance of orientation for the trophoblast TO model? The authors may consider removing some of the less important methodologic details from this paragraph and including more emphasis on why their TO model is an improvement. 

      Text has been added to this paragraph to highlight the importance of outward facing STB orientation, which is essential to mirror the STB’s transport function in vitro (lines 118-120).

      (3) Results, Figure 1: In addition to the primary placental tissue plots showing all cell populations, it may be useful to have side-by-side versions of similar plots showing only the trophoblast subsets, so that the primary and TO data could be more easily compared visually. 

      This has been implemented and added to the Supplemental Figure 1.4.

      (4) Results, Figure 1: In simple terms, what is the reason for ending up with different cluster numbers/names from the primary tissue and TO? Would it be possible to apply the same annotation to each (at least for trophoblast types) and thus allow direct comparison between the two? 

      As described above, each dataset was separately analyzed and clusters determined with an algorithm to determine the optimal clustering resolution. Therefore, the number of clusters between each dataset cannot be directly compared until the SN TO and tissue datasets are integrated together in Figure 6. We have added text to the manuscript to make it clear that they should not be compared except for in bulk number until this point (230-232).

      (5) Results, Figure 2: For subsequent evaluation of different in vitro TO conditions, did the authors use only SN sequencing because they wanted to focus on STB? Based on Figure 1, it seems some CTB subsets would be underrepresented if using only SN. Given that the authors look at both STB and CTB in their different TOs, is this an issue? 

      The CTB clusters that showed the greatest divergence between SC and SN datasets were those associated with mitosis and the cell cycle, likely due to nuclear envelope breakdown interfering with capture by the 10x microfluidics pipeline. While cytoplasmic gene expression provides valuable insights into CTB function, our manuscript focuses on the STB starting from Figure 2. Since the STB is captured exclusively by the SN dataset, we concentrated on this approach to streamline our analysis.

      (6) Results, Figure 3: What do the authors consider to be the primary contributing factors for why the STB subsets display differential gene expression between STB<sup>in</sup> and STB<sup>out</sup>? Is this due primarily to the cultural conditions and/or a result of the differing spatial arrangement with CTBs? 

      This is an intriguing question that is challenging to disentangle because the culture conditions are integral to flipping the orientation. The two primary factors that differ between STB<sup>in</sup> and STB<sup>out</sup> TOs are the presence of extracellular matrix in STB<sup>in</sup> and direct exposure to the surrounding media in STB<sup>out</sup>. We believe these environmental cues play a significant role in shaping the gene expression of STB subsets. Fully disentangling this relationship would require a method to alter the TO orientation without changing the culture conditions. While this is an exciting direction for future research, it falls outside the scope of the present study.

      (7) Results, Figure 4: The authors' analysis indicates that the STB nuclei from the STB<sup>out</sup> TO are likely "more differentiated" than those in STB<sup>in</sup> TO. Could the authors provide some qualitative or quantitative support for this? Is the STB<sup>out</sup> differentiated phenotype closer to what would be observed in a fully formed placenta? 

      As discussed earlier, we agree with the reviewers that this claim should be removed from the text outside of the discussion.

      (8) Results, Figure 5: Based on the trajectory analysis, do the authors consider that the STB from STB<sup>out</sup> TO are simply further along the differentiation pathway compared to those from STB<sup>in</sup> TO, or do the STB from STB<sup>out</sup> TO follow a differentiation pathway that is intrinsically distinct from STB<sup>in</sup> TO? 

      We think the idea of an intrinsically distinct pathway is a fascinating alternative hypothesis and have added it into the discussion. We do not find the pseudotime currently allows us to answer this question without additional experiments, so we have removed claims that the STB<sup>out</sup> STB nuclei are further along the differentiation pathway.

      (9) Results, Figure 6: A notable difference between the STB<sup>out</sup> TO and the term tissue is that the CTB subsets are much more prevalent. Is this simply a scale difference, i.e. due to the size of the human placenta compared to the limited STB nuclei available in the STB<sup>out</sup> TO? Or are there other contributing factors? 

      The proportion of CTB to STB nuclei in our term tissue (9:1) aligns with expectations based on stereological estimates. We believe the relatively low number of CTB nuclei in our dataset is due to the need for a larger sample size to capture more of this less abundant cell type. Since the primary focus of this paper is on STB, and we analyzed over 4,000 STB nuclei, we do not view this as a limitation. However, future studies utilizing SN to investigate term tissue should account for the abundance of STB nuclei and plan their sampling carefully to ensure sufficient representation of CTB nuclei if this is a desired focus.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) The color annotations for cell types in Figure 2 are inconsistent between the different panels, and the term "Prolif" in Figure 2E is not explained by the authors. 

      We chose colors to enhance visibility on the UMAP. We do not wish readers to make direct comparisons between the different CTB or STB subtypes of the sample types until the datasets are integrated in Figure 2D. This is because an algorithm for the clustering resolution has been chosen independently for each dataset. Cluster proportions are better compared in the integrated datasets in Figure 2D. We have added text to the results section to make this clear to the reader (lines 166-167).

      (2) In Figure 3 and Supplementary Figures 1.3, the authors frequently present long non-coding RNA (lncRNA) signals as cell type-specific markers in the bubble plots. These signals are likely sequencing noise and may not accurately represent true markers for those cell types. It is recommended to revise this interpretation. 

      As referenced above, there are many examples of lncRNAs that have biological and pathological significance in the placenta (H19, Meg3, Meg8) and lncRNAs often have cell type specific expression that can enhance clustering. We prefer to keep these signatures included and let future researchers determine their biological significance.

      (3) In Figure 3C, the authors performed pathway enrichment analysis on the STB subtypes after integrating STB_in and STB_out organoids. The enrichment of the "transport across the blood-brain barrier" pathway in the STB-3 subtype does not align with the current understanding of STB cell function. Please provide corresponding supporting evidence. Additionally, please verify whether the other functional pathways represent functions specific to the STB subtypes. 

      Interestingly, many of the genes categorized under “transport across the blood-brain barrier” are transporters shared with “vascular transport.” These include genes involved in the transport of amino acids (SLC7A1, SLC38A1, SLC38A3, SLC7A8), molecules essential for lipid metabolism (SLC27A4, SLC44A1), and small molecule exchange (SLC4A4, SLC5A6). Given that the vasculature, the STB, and the blood-brain barrier all perform critical barrier functions, it is unsurprising that molecules associated with these GO terms are enriched in the STB-3 subtype, which expresses numerous transporter proteins. Since the transport of materials across the STB is a well-established function, we have not included additional supporting evidence but have clarified the genes associated with this GO term in the text (lines 392-394 and supplemental Table 9).

      (4) The pseudotime heatmap in Figure 4B is not properly arranged and is inconsistent with the differentiation relationships shown in Figure 4A. It is recommended to revise this. 

      We are uncertain which aspect of the heatmap in Figure 4A is perceived as inconsistent with Figure 4B. One distinction is that pseudotime in Figure 4A is normalized from 0 to 100 to fit the blue-to-yellow-to-red color scale, whereas in Figure 4B, the color scale is not normalized and the color bar ranging from white to red. This difference reflects our intent to simplify Figure 4B-C, as the abundance of color between cell types and gene expression changes required a streamlined representation to ensure the figure remained clear and easy to interpret. This is classically done in the field and consistent with the default code in the slingshot package.

      (5) In Figures 4C and 4D, although RYBP is highly expressed in STB, it is difficult to support the conclusion that RYBP shows the most significant expression changes. It is recommended to provide additional evidence. 

      The claim that RYBP exhibits the most significant expression changes was based on p-value ordering of genes associated with pseudotime via the associationTest function in slingshot and not with immunofluorescence data. The text has been revised to make this distinction clear (lines 390-393).

      (6) In Figure 4E, staining for CTB marker genes is missing, and in Figure 4F, CYTO is difficult to use as a classical STB marker. It is recommended to use the CGBs antibody from Figure 4E as a STB marker for staining to provide evidence.  

      We have revised the Figure 5B-C to use e-Cadherin as a CTB marker gene in TOs and CGB antibody as a marker of STB.

      In tissue, however, obtaining a good STB marker that does not overlap with the RYBP antibody (rabbit) in term tissue is difficult as the STB downregulates hCG expression closer to term to initiate contractions. SDC1 is often used but only labels the plasma membrane so does not help in distinguishing the STB cytoplasm. We have added an image of cytokeratin, e-Cadherin, and the STB marker ENDOU to validate that our current approach with e-Cadherin and cytokeratin allows us to accurately distinguish between CTB and STB cells.

      (7) The velocity results in Figure 5A do not align with the differentiation relationships between cells and contradict the pseudotime results presented in Figure 4 by the authors. 

      The reviewer raises an interesting observation regarding the velocity map in Figure 5A, which appears to show a bifurcation into two STB subtypes. This observation aligns with similar findings reported in tissue by our colleagues (Wang et al., 2024). However, given the low number of CTB cells in our tissue dataset, we were cautious about making definitive conclusions about pseudotime without a larger sample size. Notably, the RNA velocity map closely resembles the pseudotime trajectory in TOs, with CTB transitioning into the CTB-pf subtype and subsequently into the STB. One potential explanation for discrepancies between tissue and TOs is the difference in nuclear age: nuclei in tissue can be up to nine months old, whereas those in TOs are only hours or days old. It is possible that the lineage in TOs could bifurcate if cultured for longer than 48 hours, but our current dataset captures only the early stages of the STB differentiation process. While exploring these hypotheses is fascinating, they are beyond the scope of this current study.  

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Amazing work - I greatly enjoyed reading the manuscript. Here are a few questions and suggestions for consideration: 

      Evidence presented throughout the results sections hints that the organoids may represent an earlier stage of placental development compared to the term. Increased hCG gene expression is observed, but as noted expression is decreased in term STB. STB:CTB ratios are also higher at term compared to the first trimester, etc. It was difficult to conclude definitively based on how data is presented in Fig 6 and discussed. Maybe there is no clear answer. Perhaps the altered cell type ratios in the organoid models (e.g., few STB in EVT enrich conditions) impact recapitulation of the in vivo local microenvironment signaling. As such, can the authors speculate on whether cell ratios could be strategically leveraged to model different gestational time points? 

      Along these same lines, syncytiotrophoblast in early implantation (before proper villi development) is often described as invasive and later at the tertiary villi stage defined by hormone production, barrier function, and nutrient/gas exchange. Do the authors think the different STB subtypes captured in the organoid models represent different stages/functions of syncytiotrophoblast in placental development? 

      Minor Comments 

      (1) Please clarify what the third number represents in the STB:CTB ratio (e.g., 1:3:1 and 2:5:1). EVT? 

      The first number is a decimal point and not a colon (ie 1.3 and 2.5). Therefore these numbers are to be read as the STB:CTB ratio is 1.3 to 1 or 2.5 to 1.

      (2) Could consider co-localizing RYBP in term tissue with a syncytio-specific marker like CGB used for organoids (Fig 4F). 

      We addressed this concern in comment 6 to reviewer 2.

      (3) Recommend defining colors-which colors represent which module in Figure 5C in the legend and main body text. I see the labels surrounding the heatmap in 5B, but defining colors in text (e.g. cyan, magenta, etc.) would be helpful. Do the gray circles represent targets that don't belong to a specific module? Are the bolded factor names based on a certain statistical cutoff/defining criteria or were they manually selected? 

      The text of both the results and figure legends has been revised to clarify these points.

      (4) Data Availability: It would be helpful to provide supplemental table files for analyses (e.g., 5C to list the overlapping relationships in TGs for each TF/CR (5C) and 3E/6F to list DEG genes in comparisons). 

      Supplemental files for each analysis have been added (Supplemental Table 8-14). In addition, the raw and processed data is available on GEO and we have created an interactive Shiny App so people without coding experience can interact with each dataset (lines 917-919).

      (5) “...and found that each sample expressed these markers (Figure 6D), suggesting..." Consider clarifying "these". 

      Text has been added to refer to a few of these marker genes within the text (line 540).

      Citations

      (1) Zappia L, Oshlack A. Clustering trees: a visualization for evaluating clusterings at multiple resolutions. GigaScience. 2018;7(7):giy083. PMCID: PMC6057528

      (2) Zhou J, Xu J, Zhang L, Liu S, Ma Y, Wen X, Hao J, Li Z, Ni Y, Li X, Zhou F, Li Q, Wang F, Wang X, Si Y, Zhang P, Liu C, Bartolomei M, Tang F, Liu B, Yu J, Lan Y. Combined Single-Cell Profiling of lncRNAs and Functional Screening Reveals that H19 Is Pivotal for Embryonic Hematopoietic Stem Cell Development. Cell Stem Cell. 2019;24(2):285-298.e5. PMID: 30639035

      (3) Malagoli G, Valle F, Barillot E, Caselle M, Martignetti L. Identification of Interpretable Clusters and Associated Signatures in Breast Cancer Single-Cell Data: A Topic Modeling Approach. Cancers. 2024;16(7):1350. PMCID: PMC11011054

      (4) Adu-Gyamfi EA, Cheeran EA, Salamah J, Enabulele DB, Tahir A, Lee BK. Long non-coding RNAs: a summary of their roles in placenta development and pathology†. Biol Reprod. 2023;110(3):431–449. PMID: 38134961

      (5) Zheng M, Hu Y, Gou R, Nie X, Li X, Liu J, Lin B. Identification three LncRNA prognostic signature of ovarian cancer based on genome-wide copy number variation. Biomed Pharmacother. 2020;124:109810. PMID: 32000042

      (6) Khan T, Seetharam AS, Zhou J, Bivens NJ, Schust DJ, Ezashi T, Tuteja G, Roberts RM. Single Nucleus RNA Sequence (snRNAseq) Analysis of the Spectrum of Trophoblast Lineages Generated From Human Pluripotent Stem Cells in vitro. Front Cell Dev Biol. 2021;9:695248. PMCID: PMC8334858

      (7) Isakova A, Neff N, Quake SR. Single-cell quantification of a broad RNA spectrum reveals unique noncoding patterns associated with cell types and states. Proc Natl Acad Sci United States Am. 2021;118(51):e2113568118. PMCID: PMC8713755

      (8) Morales-Vicente DA, Zhao L, Silveira GO, Tahira AC, Amaral MS, Collins JJ, Verjovski-Almeida S. Singlecell RNA-seq analyses show that long non-coding RNAs are conspicuously expressed in Schistosoma mansoni gamete and tegument progenitor cell populations. Front Genet. 2022;13:924877. PMCID: PMC9531161

      (9) Kim DH, Marinov GK, Pepke S, Singer ZS, He P, Williams B, Schroth GP, Elowitz MB, Wold BJ. Single-Cell

      Transcriptome Analysis Reveals Dynamic Changes in lncRNA Expression during Reprogramming. Cell Stem Cell. 2015;16(1):88–101. PMCID: PMC4291542

      (10) Yang L, Liang P, Yang H, Coyne CB. Trophoblast organoids with physiological polarity model placental structure and function. bioRxiv. 2023;2023.01.12.523752. PMCID: PMC9882188

    1. Author response:

      General Statements

      In our manuscript, we demonstrate for the first time that RNA Polymerase I (Pol I) can prematurely release nascent transcripts at the 5' end of ribosomal DNA transcription units in vivo. This achievement was made possible by comparing wild-type Pol I with a mutant form of Pol I, hereafter called SuperPol previously isolated in our lab (Darrière at al., 2019). By combining in vivo analysis of rRNA synthesis (using pulse-labelling of nascent transcript and cross-linking of nascent transcript - CRAC) with in vitro analysis, we could show that Superpol reduced premature transcript release due to altered elongation dynamics and reduced RNA cleavage activity. Such premature release could reflect regulatory mechanisms controlling rRNA synthesis. Importantly, This increased processivity of SuperPol is correlated with resistance with BMH-21, a novel anticancer drugs inhibiting Pol I, showing the relevance of targeting Pol I during transcriptional pauses to kill cancer cells. This work offers critical insights into Pol I dynamics, rRNA transcription regulation, and implications for cancer therapeutics.

      We sincerely thank the three reviewers for their insightful comments and recognition of the strengths and weaknesses of our study. Their acknowledgment of our rigorous methodology, the relevance of our findings on rRNA transcription regulation, and the significant enzymatic properties of the SuperPol mutant is highly appreciated. We are particularly grateful for their appreciation of the potential scientific impact of this work. Additionally, we value the reviewer’s suggestion that this article could address a broad scientific community, including in transcription biology and cancer therapy research. These encouraging remarks motivate us to refine and expand upon our findings further.

      All three reviewers acknowledged the increased processivity of SuperPol compared to its wildtype counterpart. However, two out of three questions our claims that premature termination of transcription can regulate ribosomal RNA transcription. This conclusion is based on SuperPol mutant increasing rRNA production. Proving that modulation of early transcription termination is used to regulate rRNA production under physiological conditions is beyond the scope of this study. Therefore, we propose to change the title of this manuscript to focus on what we have unambiguously demonstrated:

      “Ribosomal RNA synthesis by RNA polymerase I is subjected to premature termination of transcription”.

      Reviewer 1 main criticisms centers on the use of the CRAC technique in our study. While we address this point in detail below, we would like to emphasize that, although we agree with the reviewer’s comments regarding its application to Pol II studies, by limiting contamination with mature rRNA, CRAC remains the only suitable method for studying Pol I elongation over the entire transcription units. All other methods are massively contaminated with fragments of mature RNA which prevents any quantitative analysis of read distribution within rDNA.  This perspective is widely accepted within the Pol I research community, as CRAC provides a robust approach to capturing transcriptional dynamics specific to Pol I activity. 

      We hope that these findings will resonate with the readership of your journal and contribute significantly to advancing discussions in transcription biology and related fields.

      (1) Description of the planned revisions

      Despite numerous text modification (see below), we agree that one major point of discussion is the consequence of increased processivity in SuperPol mutant on the “quality” of produced rRNA. Reviewer 3 suggested comparisons with other processive alleles, such as the rpb1-E1103G mutant of the RNAPII subunit (Malagon et al., 2006). This comparison has already been addressed by the Schneider lab (Viktorovskaya OV, Cell Rep., 2013 - PMID: 23994471), which explored Pol II (rpb1-E1103G) and Pol I (rpa190-E1224G). The rpa190-E1224G mutant revealed enhanced pausing in vitro, highlighting key differences between Pol I and Pol II catalytic ratelimiting steps (see David Schneider's review on this topic for further details).

      Reviewer 2 and 3 suggested that a decreased efficiency of cleavage upon backtracking might imply an increased error rate in SuperPol compared to the wild-type enzyme. Pol I mutant with decreased rRNA cleavage have been characterized previously, and resulted in increased errorrate. We already started to address this point. Preliminary results from in vitro experiments suggest that SuperPol mutants exhibit an elevated error rate during transcription. However, these findings remain preliminary and require further experimental validation to confirm their reproducibility and robustness. We propose to consolidate these data and incorporate into the manuscript to address this question comprehensively. This could provide valuable insights into the mechanistic differences between SuperPol and the wild-type enzyme. SuperPol is the first pol I mutant described with an increased processivity in vitro and in vivo, and we agree that this might be at the cost of a decreased fidelity.

      Regulatory aspect of the process:

      To address the reviewer’s remarks, we propose to test our model by performing experiments that would evaluate PTT levels in Pol I mutant’s or under different growth conditions. These experiments would provide crucial data to support our model, which suggests that PTT is a regulatory element of Pol I transcription. By demonstrating how PTT varies with environmental factors, we aim to strengthen the hypothesis that premature termination plays an important role in regulating Pol I activity.

      We propose revising the title and conclusions of the manuscript. The updated version will better reflect the study's focus and temper claims regarding the regulatory aspects of termination events, while maintaining the value of our proposed model.

      (2) Description of the revisions that have already been incorporated in the transferred manuscript

      Some very important modifications have now been incorporated:

      Statistical Analyses and CRAC Replicates:

      Unlike reviewers 2 and 3, reviewer 1 suggests that we did not analyze the results statistically. In fact, the CRAC analyses were conducted in biological triplicate, ensuring robustness and reproducibility. The statistical analyses are presented in Figure 2C, which highlights significant findings supporting the fact WT Pol I and SuperPol distribution profiles are different. We CRAC replicates exhibit a high correlation and we confirmed significant effect in each region of interest (5’ETS, 18S.2, 25S.1 and 3’ ETS, Figure 1) to confirm consistency across experiments. We finally took care not to overinterpret the results, maintaining a rigorous and cautious approach in our analysis to ensure accurate conclusions.

      CRAC vs. Net-seq:

      Reviewer 1 ask to comment differences between CRAC and Net-seq. Both methods complement each other but serve different purposes depending on the biological question on the context of transcription analysis. Net-seq has originally been designed for Pol II analysis. It captures nascent RNAs but does not eliminate mature ribosomal RNAs (rRNAs), leading to high levels of contamination. While this is manageable for Pol II analysis (in silico elimination of reads corresponding to rRNAs), it poses a significant problem for Pol I due to the dominance of rRNAs (60% of total RNAs in yeast), which share sequences with nascent Pol I transcripts. As a result, large Net-seq peaks are observed at mature rRNA extremities (Clarke 2018, Jacobs 2022). This limits the interpretation of the results to the short lived pre-rRNA species. In contrast, CRAC has been specifically adapted by the laboratory of David Tollervey to map Pol I distribution while minimizing contamination from mature rRNAs (The CRAC protocol used exclusively recovers RNAs with 3′ hydroxyl groups that represent endogenous 3′ ends of nascent transcripts, thus removing RNAs with 3’-Phosphate, found in mature rRNAs). This makes CRAC more suitable for studying Pol I transcription, including polymerase pausing and distribution along rDNA, providing quantitative dataset for the entire rDNA gene.

      CRAC vs. Other Methods:

      Reviewer 1 suggests using GRO-seq or TT-seq, but the experiments in Figure 2 aim to assess the distribution profile of Pol I along the rDNA, which requires a method optimized for this specific purpose. While GRO-seq and TT-seq are excellent for measuring RNA synthesis and cotranscriptional processing, they rely on Sarkosyl treatment to permeabilize cellular and nuclear membranes. Sarkosyl is known to artificially induces polymerase pausing and inhibits RNase activities which are involved in the process. To avoid these artifacts, CRAC analysis is a direct and fully in vivo approach. In CRAC experiment, cells are grown exponentially in rich media and arrested via rapid cross-linking, providing precise and artifact-free data on Pol I activity and pausing.

      Pol I ChIP Signal Comparison:

      The ChIP experiments previously published in Darrière et al. lack the statistical depth and resolution offered by our CRAC analyses. The detailed results obtained through CRAC would have been impossible to detect using classical ChIP. The current study provides a more refined and precise understanding of Pol I distribution and dynamics, highlighting the advantages of CRAC over traditional methods in addressing these complex transcriptional processes.

      BMH-21 Effects:

      As highlighted by Reviewer 1, the effects of BMH-21 observed in our study differ slightly from those reported in earlier work (Ref Schneider 2022), likely due to variations in experimental conditions, such as methodologies (CRAC vs. Net-seq), as discussed earlier. We also identified variations in the response to BMH-21 treatment associated with differences in cell growth phases and/or cell density. These factors likely contribute to the observed discrepancies, offering a potential explanation for the variations between our findings and those reported in previous studies. In our approach, we prioritized reproducibility by carefully controlling BMH-21 experimental conditions to mitigate these factors. These variables can significantly influence results, potentially leading to subtle discrepancies. Nevertheless, the overall conclusions regarding BMH-21's effects on WT Pol I are largely consistent across studies, with differences primarily observed at the nucleotide resolution. This is a strength of our CRAC-based analysis, which provides precise insights into Pol I activity.

      We will address these nuances in the revised manuscript to clarify how such differences may impact results and provide context for interpreting our findings in light of previous studies.

      Minor points:

      Reviewer #1:

      •  In general, the writing style is not clear, and there are some word mistakes or poor descriptions of the results, for example: 

      •  On page 14: "SuperPol accumulation is decreased (compared to Pol I)". 

      •  On page 16: "Compared to WT Pol I, the cumulative distribution of SuperPol is indeed shifted on the right of the graph." 

      We clarified and increased the global writing style according to reviewer comment.

      •  There are also issues with the literature, for example: Turowski et al, 2020a and Turowski et al, 2020b are the same article (preprint and peer-reviewed). Is there any reason to include both references? Please, double-check the references.  

      This was corrected in this version of the manuscript.

      •  In the manuscript, 5S rRNA is mentioned as an internal control for TMA normalisation. Why are Figure 1C data normalised to 18S rRNA instead of 5S rRNA? 

      Data are effectively normalized relative to the 5S rRNA, but the value for the 18S rRNA is arbitrarily set to 100%.

      •  Figure 4 should be a supplementary figure, and Figure 7D doesn't have a y-axis labelling. 

      The presence of all Pol I specific subunits (Rpa12, Rpa34 and Rpa49) is crucial for the enzymatic activity we performed. In the absence of these subunits (which can vary depending on the purification batch), Pol I pausing, cleavage and elongation are known to be affected. To strengthen our conclusion, we really wanted to show the subunit composition of the purified enzyme. This important control should be shown, but can indeed be shown in a supplementary figure if desired.

      Y-axis is figure 7D is now correctly labelled

      •  In Figure 7C, BMH-21 treatment causes the accumulation of ~140bp rRNA transcripts only in SuperPol-expressing cells that are Rrp6-sensitive (line 6 vs line 8), suggesting that BHM-21 treatment does affect SuperPol. Could the author comment on the interpretation of this result? 

      The 140 nt product is a degradation fragment resulting from trimming, which explains its lower accumulation in the absence of Rrp6. BMH21 significantly affects WT Pol I transcription but has also a mild effect on SuperPol transcription. As a result, the 140 nt product accumulates under these conditions.

      Reviewer #2:

      •  pp. 14-15: The authors note local differences in peak detection in the 5'-ETS among replicates, preventing a nucleotide-resolution analysis of pausing sites. Still, they report consistent global differences between wild-type and SuperPol CRAC signals in the 5'ETS (and other regions of the rDNA). These global differences are clear in the quantification shown in Figures 2B-C. A simpler statement might be less confusing, avoiding references to a "first and second set of replicates" 

      According to reviewer, statement has been simplified in this version of the manuscript.

      •  Figures 2A and 2C: Based on these data and quantification, it appears that SuperPol signals in the body and 3' end of the rDNA unit are higher than those in the wild type. This finding supports the conclusion that reduced pausing (and termination) in the 5'ETS leads to an increased Pol I signal downstream. Since the average increase in the SuperPol signal is distributed over a larger region, this might also explain why even a relatively modest decrease in 5'ETS pausing results in higher rRNA production. This point merits discussion by the authors. 

      We agree that this is a very important discussion of our results. Transcription is a very dynamic process in which paused polymerase is easily detected using the CRAC assay. Elongated polymerases are distributed over a much larger gene body, and even a small amount of polymerase detected in the gene body can represent a very large rRNA synthesis. This point is of paramount importance and, as suggested by the reviewer, is now discussed in detail.

      •  A decreased efficiency of cleavage upon backtracking might imply an increased error rate in SuperPol compared to the wild-type enzyme. Have the authors observed any evidence supporting this possibility? 

      Reviewer suggested that a decreased efficiency of cleavage upon backtracking might imply an increased error rate in SuperPol compared to the wild-type enzyme. We already started to address this point. Preliminary results from in vitro experiments suggest that SuperPol mutants exhibit an elevated error rate during transcription. However, these findings remain preliminary and require further experimental validation to confirm their reproducibility and robustness. We propose to consolidate these data and incorporate into the manuscript to address this question comprehensively.

      •  pp. 15 and 22: Premature transcription termination as a regulator of gene expression is welldocumented in yeast, with significant contributions from the Corden, Brow, Libri, and Tollervey labs. These studies should be referenced along with relevant bacterial and mammalian research. 

      According to reviewer suggestion, we referenced these studies.

      •  p. 23: "SuperPol and Rpa190-KR have a synergistic effect on BMH-21 resistance." A citation should be added for this statement. 

      This represents some unpublished data from our lab. KR and SuperPol are the only two known mutants resistant to BMH-21. We observed that resistance between both alleles is synergistic, with a much higher resistance to BMH-21 in the double mutant than in each single mutant (data not shown). Comparing their resistance mechanisms is a very important point that we could provide upon request. This was added to the statement.

      •  p. 23: "The released of the premature transcript" - this phrase contains a typo 

      This is now corrected.

      Reviewer #3:

      •  Figure 1B: it would be opportune to separate the technique's schematic representation from the actual data. Concerning the data, would the authors consider adding an experiment with rrp6D cells? Some RNAs could be degraded even in such short period of time, as even stated by the authors, so maybe an exosome depleted background could provide a more complete picture. Could also the authors explain why the increase is only observed at the level of 18S and 25S? To further prove the robustness of the Pol I TMA method could be good to add already characterized mutations or other drugs to show that the technique can readily detect also well-known and expected changes. 

      The precise objective of this experiment is to avoid the use of the Rrp6 mutant. Under these conditions, we prevent the accumulation of transcripts that would result from a maturation defect. While it is possible to conduct the experiment with the Rrp6 mutant, it would be impossible to draw reliable conclusions due to this artificial accumulation of transcripts.

      •  Figure 1C: the NTS1 probe signal is missing (it is referenced in Figure 1A but not listed in the Methods section or the oligo table). If this probe was unused, please correct Figure 1A accordingly. 

      We corrected Figure 1A.  

      •  Figure 2A: the RNAPI occupancy map by CRAC is hard to interpret. The red color (SuperPol) is stacked on top of the blue line, and we are not able to observe the signal of the WT for most of the position along the rDNA unit. It would be preferable to use some kind of opacity that allows to visualize both curves. Moreover, the analysis of the behavior of the polymerase is always restricted to the 5'ETS region in the rest of the manuscript. We are thus not able to observe whether termination events also occur in other regions of the rDNA unit. A Northern blot analysis displaying higher sizes would provide a more complete picture. 

      We addressed this point to make the figure more visually informative. In Northern Blot analysis, we use a TSS (Transcription Start Site) probe, which detects only transcripts containing the 5' extremity. Due to co-transcriptional processing, most of the rRNA undergoing transcription lacks its 5' extremity and is not detectable using this technique. We have the data, but it does not show any difference between Pol I and SuperPol. This information could be included in the supplementary data if asked.

      •  "Importantly, despite some local variations, we could reproducibly observe an increased occupancy of WT Pol I in 5'-ETS compared to SuperPol (Figure 1C)." should be Figure 2C. 

      Thanks for pointing out this mistake. it has been corrected.

      •  Figure 3D: most of the difference in the cumulative proportion of CRAC reads is observed in the region ~750 to 3000. In line with my previous point, I think it would be worth exploring also termination events beyond the 5'-ETS region. 

      We agree that such an analysis would have been interesting. However, with the exception of the pre-rRNA starting at the transcription start site (TSS) studied here, any cleaved rRNA at its 5' end could result from premature termination and/or abnormal processing events. Exploring the production of other abnormal rRNAs produced by premature termination is a project in itself, beyond this initial work aimed at demonstrating the existence of premature termination events in ribosomal RNA production.

      •  Figure 4: should probably be provided as supplementary material. 

      As l mentioned earlier (see comments), the presence of all Pol I specific subunits (Rpa12, Rpa34 and Rpa49) is crucial for the enzymatic activity we performed. This important control should be shown, but can indeed be shown in a supplementary figure if desired.

      •  "While the growth of cells expressing SuperPol appeared unaffected, the fitness of WT cells was severely reduced under the same conditions." I think the growth of cells expressing SuperPol is slightly affected. 

      We agree with this comment and we modified the text accordingly.

      •  Figure 7D: the legend of the y-axis is missing as well as the title of the plot. 

      Legend of the y-axis and title of the plot are now present.

      •  The statements concerning BMH-21, SuperPol and Rpa190-KR in the Discussion section should be removed, or data should be provided.

      This was discussed previously. See comment above.

      •  Some references are missing from the Bibliography, for example Merkl et al., 2020; Pilsl et al., 2016a, 2016b. 

      Bibliography is now fixed

      (3) Description of analyses that authors prefer not to carry out

      Does SuperPol mutant produces more functional rRNAs ?

      As Reviewer 1 requested, we agree that this point requires clarification.. In cells expressing SuperPol, a higher steady state of (pre)-rRNAs is only observed in absence of degradation machinery suggesting that overproduced rRNAs are rapidly eliminated. We know that (pre)rRNas are unable to accumulate in absence of ribosomal proteins and/or Assembly Factors (AF). In consequence, overproducing rRNAs would not be sufficient to increase ribosome content. This specific point is further address in our lab but is beyond the scope of this article.

      Is premature termination coupled with rRNA processing 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments. The suggested experiments regarding the UTP-A complex's regulatory potential are valuable and ongoing in our lab, but they extend beyond the scope of this study and are not suitable for inclusion in the current manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study investigates the hypoxia rescue mechanisms of neurons by non-neuronal cells in the brain from the perspective of exosomal communication between brain cells. Through multi-omics combined analysis, the authors revealed this phenomenon and logically validated this intercellular rescue mechanism under hypoxic conditions through experiments. The study proposed a novel finding that hemoglobin maintains mitochondrial function, expanding the conventional understanding of hemoglobin. This research is highly innovative, providing new insights for the treatment of hypoxic encephalopathy.

      Overall, the manuscript is well organized and written, however, there are some minor/major points that need to be revised before this manuscript is accepted.

      We thank the reviewer for the detailed analysis of our study. Please find our answers to the points raised by the reviewer below.

      Major points:

      (1) Hypoxia can induce endothelial cells to release exosomes carrying hemoglobin, however, how neurons are able to actively take up these exosomes? It is possible for other cells to take up these exosomes also? This point needs to be clarified in this study.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s valuable comments. Regarding the question of how neurons actively uptake extracellular vesicles (EVs) carrying hemoglobin mRNA, existing studies suggest that EVs can enter cells via three main pathways: direct fusion, receptor-mediated endocytosis, and phagocytosis (PMID: 25288114). Our experimental results show that neurons are able to actively uptake EVs from endothelial cells without any treatment, and hypoxic conditions did not significantly increase the uptake of endothelial EVs by neurons (Fig. 5A and I). As for the specific uptake mechanism, there is currently no definitive conclusion. Some studies have found that hypoxic-ischemic injury may induce neurons to upregulate Cav-1, which could enhance the uptake of endothelial-derived EVs via Cav-1-mediated endocytosis (PMID: 31740664), but this mechanism still requires further validation.

      Regarding whether other cell types also take up these EVs, we focused on neurons based on existing literature and our own data, which show that the increased hemoglobin in the brain under hypoxic conditions is primarily found in neurons (Fig. 4H-J, PMID: 19116637). Moreover, we observed that, under hypoxic conditions, almost all non-neuronal supporting cells in the brain transcribe hemoglobin in large amounts and release it via EVs (Fig. 3J). Furthermore, we would like to emphasize that although neurons do not transcribe hemoglobin, we observed substantial expression of hemoglobin within neurons. This suggests that it may serve as an important protective mechanism for the brain. Therefore, the focus of our study is on the protective effect of EVs carrying hemoglobin mRNA on neurons, and the uptake by other cell types was not explored. We greatly appreciate the reviewer’s question, and we believe this is an intriguing avenue for further investigation. This could provide new insights for interventions in hypoxic brain injury, and we plan to delve into this topic in future studies.

      (2) The expression of hemoglobin in neurons is important for mitochondrial homeostasis, but its relationship with mitochondrial homeostasis needs to be further elucidated in the study.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s valuable comments. We fully agree with the importance of hemoglobin expression in neurons for mitochondrial homeostasis. In this study, we have confirmed through in vitro experiments that when neurons are treated with conditioned medium from endothelial cells, they exhibit increased hemoglobin expression. This, in turn, enhances their resistance to hypoxia by restoring mitochondrial membrane potential and increasing mitochondrial numbers, thereby effectively improving neuronal viability. Notably, this protective effect disappears when EVs are removed from the endothelial-conditioned medium or when hemoglobin in endothelial cells is disrupted, further supporting the notion that endothelial cells transfer hemoglobin via EVs, helping neurons express hemoglobin under hypoxic conditions and exert protective effects.

      In summary, hemoglobin primarily helps maintain mitochondrial membrane potential, thereby supporting the restoration of energy metabolism and production under hypoxic conditions, which effectively improves the neuronal resistance to hypoxia. Although we were unable to explore the specific mechanisms of hemoglobin’s role in mitochondrial homeostasis in detail within this study, we recognize the importance of this aspect and plan to further investigate how hemoglobin regulates mitochondrial homeostasis and function in neurons in future research.

      Once again, we greatly appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments. We will continue to optimize our research direction and look forward to further elucidating these important biological mechanisms in future studies.

      Minor points:

      (1) In Figures 1-3, the authors use "Endo" to represent endothelial cells, while in Figures 4-7, the abbreviation "EC" is used. Please standardize the format.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s suggestion. We will use “EC” consistently to refer to endothelial cells throughout the manuscript to ensure uniformity.

      (2) In all qPCR statistical results, please italicize the gene names on the axis.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s valuable suggestion. We will make sure to italicize the gene names on the axis in all qPCR statistical results to adhere to the formatting requirements.

      (3) In the Western blot result of Figure 3C, what type of cell-derived exosomes does the Control group represent, and why can it be used as a control group for brain-derived exosomes?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s insightful question. In Fig. 3C, the control group (Control) represents the cell lysate sample, which serves as a positive control in the EVs Western blot analysis. In this experiment, the positive control is primarily used to validate the specificity of the antibody and the accuracy of the experimental procedure. We used cell lysate as the control to confirm that the antibody can detect EV-associated markers in the cell lysates, thus providing a comparative basis for the identification of brain-derived EVs.

      (4) In Figure 4F, the morphology of hemoglobin in the Con group and the H28d group is not entirely consistent with Figure 4H. Is this difference due to different experimental batches?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s careful observation. The observed difference may indeed be due to variations between different experimental batches. To ensure consistency of the results, we have updated the representative immunofluorescence images, which are now presented in Fig. 4H.

      (5) Supplement the transcription and expression levels of hemoglobin in neurons under different treatment conditions after medium exchange with exosome removal and medium exchange after HBA1 interference.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s valuable suggestions. We have added the experimental data regarding the exchange of culture medium after the removal of EVs. As shown in Fig. S6, the endothelial-derived medium without EVs does not enhance the hemoglobin levels in neurons under hypoxic conditions. Additionally, we have included the detection results of hemoglobin expression in neurons after HBA1 interference, as shown in Fig. S7E-F. The results indicate that the culture medium derived from HBA1-interfered endothelial cells also fails to help neurons increase hemoglobin expression under hypoxic conditions.

      (6) Figure S3 should be split to separately explain the increased exosome release induced by hypoxia, the non-toxic effect of endothelial cell culture medium on neurons, and the successful screening of the HBA1 interference plasmid.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s suggestions. Based on your feedback, we have split the original Fig. S3 into multiple parts to more clearly present the different experimental results. Specifically, the results of hypoxia-induced EVs release increase have been updated in Fig. S4, the non-toxic effects of endothelial cell culture medium on neurons are shown in Fig. S5, and the successful screening of the HBA1 interference plasmid is presented in Fig. S7.

      (7) Regarding the extracellular vesicles/exosomes, it should be expressed consistently in the whole manuscript.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s reminder. We will ensure that the term “extracellular vesicles” is used consistently throughout the manuscript.

      (8) In lines 70 and 80, the O2 should be changed to "O<sub>2</sub>".

      Thank you for the reviewer’s careful observation. We have corrected the formatting of “O2” to “O₂” in lines 70 and 80.

      We would like to thank the Reviewer for taking the time to thoroughly examine our work, for their helpful feedback that has significantly contributed to improving our manuscript, and for their kind and encouraging words.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This is an interesting study with a lot of data. Some of these ideas are intriguing. But a few major points require further consideration.

      We thank the reviewer for the detailed assessment of our study and pinpointing its current weaknesses. Please find our answers to all comments below.

      Major points:

      (1) What disease is this model of whole animal hypoxia supposed to mimic? If one is focused on the brain, can one just use a model of focal or global cerebral ischemia?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s insightful question. The chronic hypoxia model we employed is designed to mimic the multi-organ damage caused by systemic hypoxia, which is relevant to clinical conditions such as high-altitude hypoxia, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, and acute hypoxic brain injury. In contrast to focal or global cerebral ischemia models, the focus of our study is on how the brain, under extreme systemic hypoxia, utilizes endothelial cell-derived extracellular vesicles (EVs) to transfer hemoglobin mRNA, thereby protecting neurons and aiding the brain’s response to hypoxia-induced damage.

      We understand the reviewer’s concern that focal or global ischemia models are typically used to simulate localized brain hypoxia or ischemic injury. However, the core of our research is to explore the brain’s overall adaptive mechanisms under systemic hypoxic conditions. By using a systemic hypoxia model, we can more comprehensively simulate the effects of global hypoxia on the brain and uncover how the brain engages specific molecular mechanisms for self-protection. This approach offers a novel perspective on brain hypoxic-ischemic diseases and holds potential clinical applications, particularly in the study of stroke, vascular cognitive impairment and dementia (VCID), and related conditions.

      Additionally, we have observed that hemoglobin significantly increases in the brain in an animal model of focal ischemia (as shown in Author response image 1 below). This finding further supports the idea that hemoglobin upregulation may be a universal protective mechanism for the brain’s response to hypoxic damage. While this part of the research is still ongoing, preliminary results suggest that both systemic hypoxia and focal ischemia might trigger protective effects through hemoglobin regulation.

      Author response image 1.

      The expression level of Hba-a1 in the brain of VCID mouse.

      Therefore, the core of our study is to elucidate the brain’s self-protection mechanisms under systemic hypoxia, rather than focusing solely on cerebral ischemia models. We believe this approach provides new insights into the prevention and treatment of brain hypoxic-ischemic diseases, with significant clinical application potential.

      In light of this, we have added a related discussion to the manuscript, clearly explaining the rationale for choosing the systemic hypoxia model. The updated content can be found on P11, Line 13-21 as follows: “To investigate this phenomenon, we employed a chronic hypoxia model in which mice were exposed to 7% oxygen for 28 days. This model aims to mimic systemic hypoxia-induced multi-organ damage, a condition observed in diseases such as high-altitude hypoxia, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, and acute hypoxic brain injury. The primary goal of this model is to explore how the brain adapts under extreme low-oxygen conditions and employs specific mechanisms to protect itself from hypoxia-induced damage. This approach provides valuable insight into diseases related to hypoxic-ischemic injury in the brain, including stroke and vascular dementia, offering a novel perspective for potential clinical applications.”

      (2) If this model subjects the entire animal to hypoxia, then other organs will also be hypoxic. Should one also detect endothelial upregulation and release of extracellular vesicles containing hemoglobin mRNA in non-CNS organs? Where do these vesicles go? Into blood?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s valuable feedback. Indeed, in a whole-body hypoxia model, other organs are also affected by hypoxia. Therefore, future research may need to investigate the upregulation of endothelial cells in organs other than the central nervous system, as well as the release of EVs containing hemoglobin mRNA from these organs. However, in this study, we isolated EVs from the brain tissue in situ following perfusion with physiological saline, a method that effectively eliminates the influence of EVs from blood or other organs. As a result, our primary focus was on studying how EVs released by brain endothelial cells are actively taken up by neurons to exert neuroprotective effects. The potential for these EVs to enter the bloodstream and their subsequent fate is indeed a topic worthy of further investigation. Future research could offer new insights into the cross-organ effects of systemic hypoxia.

      (3) What other mRNA are contained in the vesicles released from brain endothelial cells?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s valuable suggestions. We have further analyzed EVs derived from brain endothelial cells, and in addition to hemoglobin mRNA, these EVs also contain a variety of other mRNAs, including Vwf, Hbb-bt, Hba-a1, Hbb-bs, Hba-a2, Acer2, Angpt2, Ldha, Gm42418, Slc16a1, Cxcl12, B2m, Ctla2a, Ccnd1, and Hmgcs2 (Log2FC > 1.2). The biological processes associated with these mRNAs primarily involve: cell-substrate adhesion, regulation of cellular amide metabolic process, negative regulation of cell migration, negative regulation of cell motility, and negative regulation of cellular component movement. These processes may be closely related to the neuroprotective effects of endothelial cell EVs in a hypoxic environment, especially in terms of regulating cell behavior and maintaining cell structure and function. Additionally, these EVs contain multiple key factors associated with intracellular metabolism, movement, and migration, which may collectively influence neuronal function and survival. Notably, our study also found that mRNA of various hemoglobin subunits ranks among the top five in terms of abundance in the mRNA secreted by hypoxic endothelial EVs, further emphasizing the importance of hemoglobin mRNA in endothelial-derived EVs. Therefore, future research may explore the functions of these mRNAs and reveal how they act in concert to protect neurons from hypoxia-induced damage.

      We have updated and added these results in Fig. S4, and have further elaborated on the findings in the revised figure. Once again, we thank the reviewer for the attention and valuable suggestions regarding our work.

      (4) Where do the endothelial vesicles go? Only to neurons? Or to other cells as well?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s important question. As previously mentioned, the focus of this study is to investigate how EVs carrying hemoglobin mRNA influence neuronal function. Through a combined analysis of single-cell transcriptomics and EV transcriptomics from brain tissue, we found that, besides neurons, almost all types of supportive cells in the brain and their secreted EVs contain a significant amount of hemoglobin mRNA (Fig. 3J, 4B). Notably, although neurons do not transcribe hemoglobin mRNA themselves, under hypoxic conditions, neurons significantly increase hemoglobin expression, resulting in a phenomenon where the transcription and expression levels of hemoglobin in neurons are inconsistent. This phenomenon has been observed both in our study and others (Fig. 4H-J, PMID: 19116637). This observation led us to focus on the active uptake of EVs by neurons and the potential neuroprotective effects they might bring.

      Regarding whether other cell types uptake these EVs and their potential functions, although our current research is focused on neurons, this is indeed an important area for further investigation. Given that non-neuronal supportive cells may also transfer hemoglobin mRNA via EVs under hypoxic conditions, future research will further explore the uptake of EVs by different cell types and their roles in hypoxic adaptation.

      We are particularly interested in the hemoglobin expression in neurons under hypoxic conditions and consider neurons to be the primary expressers of hemoglobin, providing a new perspective for exploring the neuroprotective role of hemoglobin. We plan to delve deeper into these issues in future studies.

      (5) Neurons can express endogenous hemoglobin. Is it useful to subject neurons to hypoxia and then see how much the endogenous mRNA goes up? How large is the magnitude of endogenous hemoglobin gene upregulation compared to the hypothesized exogenous mRNA that is supposed to be donated from endothelial vesicles?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s valuable question. We have observed that, in the absence of treatment with endothelial cell-derived conditioned medium, there is no significant change in the transcription and expression levels of endogenous hemoglobin in neurons under hypoxic conditions (Fig. 5I, 6C-D). However, when neurons were treated with endothelial cell-conditioned medium, under the same hypoxic conditions, the transcription levels of hemoglobin increased by approximately 1.2-fold, and the expression levels increased by approximately 3.8-fold (Fig. 6B-D). Additionally, we have added pre-treatment experiments involving EVs depletion from the endothelial culture medium and HBA interference. The results show that, after these two pre-treatments, the conditioned medium lost its ability to enhance the transcription and expression of hemoglobin in neurons under hypoxic conditions (Fig. S6, S7D-F), further emphasizing the important role of endothelial EVs in this process. This finding indicates that endothelial-derived EVs significantly promote hemoglobin expression in neurons, and this effect is far greater than the upregulation of endogenous hemoglobin in neurons. Therefore, while neurons can express endogenous hemoglobin, exogenous hemoglobin significantly enhances its expression, which may help neurons tolerate the hypoxic environment and provide additional protection.

      (6) Finally, it may be useful to provide more information and data to explain how the expression of this exogenous endothelial-derived hemoglobin binds to neuronal mitochondria to alter function.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s valuable suggestion. As we previously mentioned, hemoglobin plays a protective role in neurons by maintaining mitochondrial membrane potential, helping neurons restore energy metabolism and energy production under hypoxic conditions. We fully agree on the importance of this research direction. Several studies have shown that when hemoglobin is expressed in neurons, it predominantly localizes to mitochondria, which aligns with the physiological process of heme synthesis within mitochondria (PMID: 23187133). Furthermore, in the brains of Parkinson’s disease patients, the localization of hemoglobin in neuronal mitochondria is altered compared to normal conditions (PMID: 27181046). Therefore, the interaction between hemoglobin and mitochondria plays a crucial role in neuronal function.

      Although existing research indicates the role of hemoglobin in neuronal mitochondria, studies in this area remain limited. We plan to further investigate how hemoglobin binds to mitochondria and its specific effects on mitochondrial function in our future work. We believe that a deeper understanding of this mechanism will provide essential theoretical insights into the effects of hypoxia on neurons and offer new potential strategies for neuroprotective therapies.

      We would like to thank the Reviewer for taking the time to thoroughly examine our work, for their helpful feedback that has significantly contributed to improving our manuscript, and for their kind and encouraging words.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      In this study, the authors introduced an essential role of AARS2 in maintaining cardiac function. They also investigated the underlying mechanism that through regulating alanine and PKM2 translation are regulated by AARS2. Accordingly, a therapeutic strategy for cardiomyopathy and MI was provided. Several points need to be addressed to make this article more comprehensive:

      Thank this reviewer for the overall supports on our manuscript.

      (1) Include apoptotic caspases in Figure 2B, and Figure 4 B and E as well.

      This is a good point for further investigating the role of apoptosis signaling in cardiac-specific AARS2 knockout hearts. Since we are focusing on cardiomyocyte phenotypes, immunostaining on TUNEL and anti-cTnT directly evaluated the level of cardiomyocyte apoptosis, which was supported by Western blots with anti-Bcl-2 and anti-BAX of control and mutant hearts. TUNEL data accurately represents biochemical and morphological characteristics of apoptotic cells, and is more sensitive than the conventional histochemical and biochemical methods. Future studies are needed to address how apoptosis components including apoptotic caspases are involved in cardiomyocyte apoptosis in AARS2 mutant hearts.

      (2) It would be better to show the change of apoptosis-related proteins upon the knocking down of AARS2 by small interfering RNA (siRNA).

      Since primary culture of neonatal cardiomyocytes also contained non-cardiomyocytes, using Western blots with anti-apoptosis proteins cannot directly assess cardiomyocytes phenotypes. In this work, our data on the elevation of cTnT<sup>+</sup>/TUNEL<sup>+</sup> cardiomyocytes and cardiac fibrosis in AARS2 mutant hearts suggest that AARS2 deficiency induced cardiomyocyte death.

      (3) In Figure 5, the authors performed Mass Spectrometry to assess metabolites of homogenates. I was wondering if the change of other metabolites could be provided in the form of a heatmap.

      Indeed, we assessed other metabolites by mass spectrometry as shown below, we found that overexpression of AARS2 in either transgenic mouse hearts or neonatal cardiomyocytes had no consistent changes on the level of fumarate, succinate, malate, alpha-ketoglutarate (alpha-KG), citrate, oxaloacetate (OAA), ATP, and ADP, thus suggesting that AARS2 overexpression has more specific effect on the level of lactate, pyruvate, and acetyl-CoA.

      Author response image 1.

      (4) The amounts of lactate should be assessed using a lactate assay kit to validate the Mass Spectrometry results.

      We carried out several rounds of mass spectrometry experiments, suggesting that lactate is consistently elevated after AARS2 overexpression in neonatal cardiomyocytes as shown below. We will establish other lactate assays in future studies.

      Author response image 2.

      (5) How about the expression pattern of PKM2 before and after mouse MI. Furtherly, the correlation between AARS2 and PKM2?

      Previous studies have shown that the expression level of PKM2 in mice is significantly increased after cardiac surgery at different time points, which may be related to cardiometabolic changes [1]. Our co-IP experiments showed no direct interactions between AARS2 and PKM2 (Figure 6K), while both AARS2 proteins and mRNA decreased on the 3 days (Figure 1A-B) and 7 days (Author response image 3)after myocardial infarction in mice. Thus, the level of AARS2 is reversely related to PKM2 after myocardial infarction.

      Author response image 3.

      (6) In Figure 5, how about the change of apoptosis-related proteins after administration of PKM2 activator TEPP-46?

      It has been shown that TEPP-46 treatment decreased cardiomyocyte death in different models that induced cardiomyocyte apoptosis [2, 3]. We would like to refer these published works that TEPP-46 treatment improves heart function by inhibiting cardiac injury-induced cardiomyocyte death.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to elucidate the role of AARS2, an alanyl-tRNA synthase, in mouse hearts, specifically its impact on cardiac function, fibrosis, apoptosis, and metabolic pathways under conditions of myocardial infarction (MI). By investigating the effects of both deletion and overexpression of AARS2 in cardiomyocytes, the study aims to determine how AARS2 influences cardiac health and survival during ischemic stress.

      The authors successfully achieved their aims by demonstrating the critical role of AARS2 in maintaining cardiomyocyte function under ischemic conditions. The evidence presented, including genetic manipulation results, functional assays, and mechanistic studies, robustly supports the conclusion that AARS2 facilitates cardiomyocyte survival through PKM2-mediated metabolic reprogramming. The study convincingly links AARS2 overexpression to improved cardiac outcomes post-MI, validating the proposed protective AARS2-PKM2 signaling pathway.

      This work may have a significant impact on the field of cardiac biology and ischemia research. By identifying AARS2 as a key player in cardiomyocyte survival and metabolic regulation, the study opens new avenues for therapeutic interventions targeting this pathway. The methods used, particularly the cardiomyocyte-specific genetic models and ribosome profiling, are valuable tools that can be employed by other researchers to investigate similar questions in cardiac physiology and pathology.

      Understanding the metabolic adaptations in cardiomyocytes during ischemia is crucial for developing effective treatments for MI. This study highlights the importance of metabolic flexibility and the role of specific enzymes like AARS2 in facilitating such adaptations. The identification of the AARS2-PKM2 axis adds a new layer to our understanding of cardiac metabolism, suggesting that enhancing glycolysis can be a viable strategy to protect the heart from ischemic damage.

      We thank this reviewer for his/her supports on our manuscript.

      Strengths:

      (1) Comprehensive Genetic Models: The use of cardiomyocyte-specific AARS2 knockout and overexpression mouse models allowed for precise assessment of AARS2's role in cardiac cells.

      (2) Functional Assays: Detailed phenotypic analyses, including measurements of cardiac function, fibrosis, and apoptosis, provided evidence for the physiological impact of AARS2 manipulation.

      (3) Mechanistic Insights: This study used ribosome profiling (Ribo-Seq) to uncover changes in protein translation, specifically highlighting the role of PKM2 in metabolic reprogramming.

      (4) Therapeutic Relevance: The use of the PKM2 activator TEPP-46 to reverse the effects of AARS2 deficiency presents a potential therapeutic avenue, underscoring the practical implications of the findings.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Species Limitation: The study is limited to mouse and rat models, and while these are highly informative, further validation in human cells or tissues would strengthen the translational relevance.

      We fully agree with this reviewer that this study is limited to mouse and rat models. It would certainly be important to address how AARS2-PKM2 is related myocardial infarction patients in the future.

      (2) Temporal Dynamics: The study does not extensively address the temporal dynamics of AARS2 expression and PKM2 activity during the progression of MI and recovery, which could offer deeper insights into the timing and regulation of these processes.

      Thanks for this critical point. Indeed, we found that both AARS2 proteins and mRNA decreased on 3 days (Figure 1A-B) and 7 days (Author response image 3) after myocardial infarction in mice as shown below. Others have reported PKM2 proteins increased after heart surgery in mice at different time points [1]. Thus, the level of AARS2 is reversely related to PKM2 after myocardial infarction.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      In the present study, the author revealed that cardiomyocyte-specific deletion of mouse AARS2 exhibited evident cardiomyopathy with impaired cardiac function, notable cardiac fibrosis, and cardiomyocyte apoptosis. Cardiomyocyte-specific AARS2 overexpression in mice improved cardiac function and reduced cardiac fibrosis after myocardial infarction (MI), without affecting cardiomyocyte proliferation and coronary angiogenesis. Mechanistically, AARS2 overexpression suppressed cardiomyocyte apoptosis and mitochondrial reactive oxide species production, and changed cellular metabolism from oxidative phosphorylation toward glycolysis in cardiomyocytes, thus leading to cardiomyocyte survival from ischemia and hypoxia stress. Ribo-Seq revealed that AARS2 overexpression increased pyruvate kinase M2 (PKM2) protein translation and the ratio of PKM2 dimers to tetramers that promote glycolysis. Additionally, PKM2 activator TEPP-46 reversed cardiomyocyte apoptosis and cardiac fibrosis caused by AARS2 deficiency. Thus, this study demonstrates that AARS2 plays an essential role in protecting cardiomyocytes from ischemic pressure via fine-tuning PKM2-mediated energy metabolism, and presents a novel cardiac protective AARS2-PKM2 signaling during the pathogenesis of MI. This study provides some new knowledge in the field, and there are still some questions that need to be addressed in order to better support the authors' views.

      We thank this reviewer for his/her overall supports on our manuscript.

      (1) WGA staining showed obvious cardiomyocyte hypertrophy in the AARS2 cKO heart. Whether AARS affects cardiac hypertrophy needs to be further tested.

      WGA staining is widely used to measure the size of cardiomyocytes in the literature. Here, we found that the size of mutant cardiomyocytes increased by ~20% after AARS2 knockout. In addition, we also measured and found that the ratio of heart to body weight increased in AARS2 mutant mice compared with control siblings as shown below.

      Author response image 4.

      (2) The authors observed that AARS2 can improve myocardial infarction, and whether AARS2 has an effect on other heart diseases.

      Thanks for this critical point. We agree with this reviewer that it will be important to address whether overexpression of AARS2 has cardiac protection in other heart diseases such as transverse aortic constriction in the future.

      (3) Studies have shown that hypoxia conditions can lead to mitochondrial dysfunction, including abnormal division and fusion. AARS2 also affects mitochondrial division and fusion and interacts with mitochondrial proteins, including FIS and DRP1, the authors are suggested to verify.

      This is a good point. Mitochondrial dysfunction occurs when cardiomyocytes are subjected to hypoxia conditions such as myocardial infarction. Our ribosome sequencing data suggested that overexpression of AARS2 had no effect on the level of FIS1 and DRP2 as shown below. We agree with this reviewer that future studies are needed to clarify potential interactions between AARS2 and FIS/DRP1 proteins.

      Author response image 5.

      (4) The authors only examined the role of AARS2 in cardiomyocytes, and fibroblasts are also an important cell type in the heart. Authors should examine the expression and function of AARS2 in fibroblasts.

      We fully agree with this reviewer that AARS2 may also function in cardiac fibroblasts since it is expressed in fibroblasts and cardiomyocyte-specific AARS2 knockout led to more fibrosis after myocardial infarction, which certainly warrant future investigations.

      (5) Overexpression of AARS2 can inhibit the production of mtROS, and has a protective effect on myocardial ischemia and H/ R-induced injury, and the occurrence of iron death is also closely related to ROS, whether AARS protects myocardial by regulating the occurrence of iron death?

      Thank this reviewer for his/her critical point. Our current data cannot rule out whether iron-mediated death is involved in AARS2 function in cardiac protection, which warrant future investigations.

      (6) Please revise the English grammar and writing style of the manuscript, spelling and grammatical errors should be excluded.

      Sorry for spelling and grammatical errors. We have carefully revised this manuscript now.

      (7) Recent studies have shown that a decrease in oxygen levels leads to an increase in AARS2, and lactic acid rises rapidly without being oxidized. Both of these factors inhibit oxidative phosphorylation and muscle ATP production by increasing mitochondrial lactate acylation, thereby inhibiting exercise capacity and preventing the accumulation of reactive oxygen species ROS. The key role of protein lactate acylation modification in regulating oxidative phosphorylation of mitochondria, and the importance of metabolites such as lactate regulating cell function through feedback mechanisms, i.e. cells adapt to low oxygen through metabolic regulation to reduce ROS production and oxidative damage, and therefore whether AARS2 in the heart also acts in this way.

      This is an interesting question. Since overexpression of AARS2 in muscles has previously been reported to increase PDHA1 lactylation and decrease its activity [4]. Actually, we initially examined whether overexpression of AARS2 in cardiomyocytes has similar effect on PDHA1 lactylation. However, our results showed that overexpression of AARS2 had no evident increases of lactylated PDHA1 in cardiomyocytes as shown below. However, future studies are needed to explore whether other proteins lactylation by AARS2 are involved in its cardiac protection function.

      Author response image 6.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Suggestions for Improved or Additional Experiments, Data, or Analyses:

      (1) Validation in Human Models: It would be great if, in the future, the authors could conduct experiments with human cardiomyocytes derived from induced pluripotent stem cells (iPSCs) to validate the findings in a human context. This would strengthen the translational relevance of the results.

      We fully agree with this reviewer that this study is limited to mouse and rat models. It would certainly be important to address how AARS2-PKM2 is related myocardial infarction patients and/or human iPSC-derived cardiomyocytes in the future.

      (2) Broader Metabolic Analysis: To perform comprehensive metabolic profiling (e.g., metabolomics) to identify other metabolic pathways influenced by AARS2 overexpression or deficiency. This could provide a more holistic view of the metabolic changes and potential compensatory mechanisms.

      As noted above, we indeed assessed other metabolites by mass spectrometry, we found that overexpression of AARS2 in either transgenic mouse hearts or neonatal cardiomyocytes had no consistent changes on the level of fumarate, succinate, malate, alpha-ketoglutarate (alpha-KG), citrate, oxaloacetic acid (OAA), ATP, and ADP, thus suggesting that AARS2 overexpression has more specific effect on the level of lactate, pyruvate, and acetyl-CoA.

      (3) Temporal Dynamics: Investigate the temporal expression and activity of AARS2 and PKM2 during the progression and recovery phases of myocardial infarction. Time-course studies could elucidate the dynamics and regulatory mechanisms involved.

      As noted above, we found that both AARS2 proteins and mRNA decreased on the third and seventh day after myocardial infarction in mice. Others have reported PKM2 proteins increased after heart surgery in mice at different time points [1]. Thus, the level of AARS2 is reversely related to PKM2 after myocardial infarction.

      (4) Investigate Additional Pathways: Explore the involvement of other signaling pathways and tRNA synthetases that might interact with or complement the AARS2-PKM2 axis. This could uncover broader regulatory networks affecting cardiomyocyte survival and function.

      Thank this reviewer for his/her critical point. This certainly warrants future investigations.

      (5) Mitochondrial Function Assays: Perform detailed mitochondrial function assays, including measurements of mitochondrial respiration and membrane potential, to further elucidate the role of AARS2 in mitochondrial health and function under stress conditions.

      We fully agree with this reviewer that future studies are needed to address how AARS2 is involved in mitochondrial function.

      (6) Single-Cell Analysis: Utilize single-cell RNA sequencing to examine the heterogeneity in cardiomyocyte responses to AARS2 manipulation, providing insights into cell-specific adaptations and potential differential effects within the heart tissue.

      We fully agree with this reviewer that it is important to address how AARS2 (cKO or overexpression) regulate cardiomyocyte heterogeneity and function in the future. 

      Recommendations for Improving the Writing and Presentation:

      (1) Visual Aids: Include more schematic diagrams to illustrate the proposed mechanisms, especially the AARS2-PKM2 signaling pathway and its impact on metabolic reprogramming. This can help readers better understand complex interactions.

      Below is our working hypothesis on the role of AARS2 in cardiac protection. AARS2 deficiency caused mitochondrial dysfunction due to increasing ROS production and apoptosis while decreasing PKM2 function and glycolysis, thus leading to cardiomyopathy in mutant mice.  On the other hand, overexpression of AARS2 in mice activates PKM2 and glycolysis while decreases ROS production and apoptosis, thus improving heart function after myocardial infarction.

      Author response image 7.

      (2) Discussion: Shorten the Discussion and systematically address the significance of the findings, limitations of the study, and potential future directions. This will provide a clearer narrative and context for the results.

      We have now made revisions on the Discussion part to highlight the significance of this work and brief perspective of future direction.

      (3) Minor corrections to the text and figures.

      We have now revised the full text carefully.

      (4) Typographical Errors: Carefully proofread the manuscript to correct any typographical errors and ensure consistent use of terminology and abbreviations throughout the text.

      Thanks. Based on the reviewer’s suggestions, we have carefully revised the manuscript and have done proof-reading on the whole manuscript.

      Availability of data, code, reagents, research ethics, or other issues:

      (1) Data Presentation: Ensure that all graphs and charts are clearly labeled with appropriate units, scales, and legends. Use color schemes that are accessible to color-blind readers.

      We followed these rules to present the data.

      (2) Supplementary Information: Provide detailed supplementary information, including raw data, experimental protocols, and analysis scripts, to enhance the reproducibility of the study.

      We provided the raw data, experimental protocols, and analysis scripts in the manuscript.

      (3) Data and Code Availability. Data Sharing: Authors should ensure that all raw data, processed data, and relevant metadata are deposited in publicly accessible repositories. Provide clear instructions on how to access these data. Code Availability: Make all analysis code available in a public repository, such as GitHub, with adequate documentation to allow other researchers to replicate the analyses.

      We have deposited RNA-Seq data at ArrayExpress (E-MTAB-13767). We have also uploaded the original data in the supplementary file.

      (4) Research Ethics and Compliance. Ethics Statement: Include a detailed statement on the ethical approval obtained for animal experiments, specifying the institution and ethical review board that granted approval. Conflict of Interest: Clearly state any potential conflicts of interest and funding sources that supported the research to ensure transparency.

      Thanks. In the manuscript we made an ethical statement, stating conflicts of interest and sources of funding.

      References:

      (1) Y. Tang, M. Feng, Y. Su, T. Ma, H. Zhang, H. Wu, X. Wang, S. Shi, Y. Zhang, Y. Xu, S. Hu, K. Wei, D. Xu, Jmjd4 Facilitates Pkm2 Degradation in Cardiomyocytes and Is Protective Against Dilated Cardiomyopathy, Circulation, 147 (2023) 1684-1704.

      (2) L. Guo, L. Wang, G. Qin, J. Zhang, J. Peng, L. Li, X. Chen, D. Wang, J. Qiu, E. Wang, M-type pyruvate kinase 2 (PKM2) tetramerization alleviates the progression of right ventricle failure by regulating oxidative stress and mitochondrial dynamics, Journal of translational medicine, 21 (2023) 888.

      (3) B. Saleme, V. Gurtu, Y. Zhang, A. Kinnaird, A.E. Boukouris, K. Gopal, J.R. Ussher, G. Sutendra, Tissue-specific regulation of p53 by PKM2 is redox dependent and provides a therapeutic target for anthracycline-induced cardiotoxicity, Science translational medicine, 11 (2019).

      (4) Y. Mao, J. Zhang, Q. Zhou, X. He, Z. Zheng, Y. Wei, K. Zhou, Y. Lin, H. Yu, H. Zhang, Y. Zhou, P. Lin, B. Wu, Y. Yuan, J. Zhao, W. Xu, S. Zhao, Hypoxia induces mitochondrial protein lactylation to limit oxidative phosphorylation, Cell research, 34 (2024) 13-30.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, Manley and Vaziri investigate whole-brain neural activity underlying behavioural variability in zebrafish larvae. They combine whole brain (single cell level) calcium imaging during the presentation of visual stimuli, triggering either approach or avoidance, and carry out whole brain population analyses to identify whole brain population patterns responsible for behavioural variability. They show that similar visual inputs can trigger large variability in behavioural responses. Though visual neurons are also variable across trials, they demonstrate that this neural variability does not degrade population stimulus decodability. Instead, they find that the neural variability across trials is in orthogonal population dimensions to stimulus encoding and is correlated with motor output (e.g. tail vigor). They then show that behavioural variability across trials is largely captured by a brain-wide population state prior to the trial beginning, which biases choice - especially on ambiguous stimulus trials. This study suggests that parts of stimulus-driven behaviour can be captured by brain-wide population states that bias choice, independently of stimulus encoding.

      Strengths:

      -The strength of the paper principally resides in the whole brain cellular level imaging in a well-known but variable behaviour.

      - The analyses are reasonable and largely answer the questions the authors ask.

      - Overall the conclusions are well warranted.

      Weaknesses:

      A more in-depth exploration of some of the findings could be provided, such as:

      - Given that thousands of neurons are recorded across the brain a more detailed parcelation of where the neurons contribute to different population coding dimensions would be useful to better understand the circuits involved in different computations.

      We thank the reviewer for noting the strengths of our study and agree that these findings have raised a number of additional avenues which we intend to explore in depth in future studies. In response to the reviewer’s comment above, we have added a number of additional figure panels (new Figures S1E, S3F-G, 4I(i), 4K(i), and S5F-G) and updated panels (Figures 4I(ii) and 4K(ii) in the revised manuscript) to show a more detailed parcellation of the visually-evoked neurons, noise modes, turn direction bias population, and responsiveness bias population. To do so. we have aligned our recordings to the Z-Brain atlas (Randlett et al., 2015) as shown in new Figure S1E. In addition, we provided a more detailed parcellation of the neuronal ensembles by providing projections of the full 3D volume along the xy and yz axes, in addition to the unregistered xy projection shown in Figures 4H and 4J in the revised manuscript. We also found that the distribution of neurons across our huc:h2b-gcamp6s recordings is very similar to the distribution of labeling in the huc:h2b-rfp reference image from the Z-Brain atlas (Figure S1E), which further supports our whole-brain imaging results.

      Overall, we find that this more detailed quantification and visualization is consistent with our interpretations. In particular, we show that the optimal visual decoding population (w<sub>opt</sub>) and the largest noise mode (e1) are localized to the midbrain (Figures S3F-G). This is expected, as in Figure 3 we first extracted a low-dimensional subspace of whole-brain neural activity that optimally preserved visual information. Additionally, we provide new evidence that the populations correlated with the turn bias and responsiveness bias are distributed throughout the brain, including a relatively dense localization to the cerebellum, telencephalon, and dorsal diencephalon (habenula, new Figures 4H-K and S5F-G).

      - Given that the behaviour on average can be predicted by stimulus type, how does the stimulus override the brain-wide choice bias on some trials? In other words, a better link between the findings in Figures 2 and 3 would be useful for better understanding how the behaviour ultimately arises.

      We agree with the reviewer that one of the most fundamental questions that this study has raised is how the identified neuronal populations predictive of decision variables (which we describe as an internal “bias”) interact with the well-studied, visually-evoked circuitry. A major limitation of our study is that the slow dynamics of the NL-GCaMP6s prevent clearly distinguishing any potential difference in the onset time of various neurons during the short trials, which might provide clues into which neurons drive versus later reflect the motor output. However, given that these ensembles were also found to be correlated with spontaneous turns, our hypothesis is that these populations reflect brain-wide drives that enable efficient exploration of the local environment (Dunn et al. 2016, doi.org/10.7554/eLife.12741). Further, we suspect that a sufficiently strong stimulus drive (e.g., large, looming stimuli) overrides these ongoing biases, which would explain the higher average pre-stimulus predictability in trials with small to intermediate-sized stimuli. An important follow-up line of experimentation could involve comparing the neuronal dynamics of specific components of the visual circuitry at distinct internal bias states, ideally utilizing emerging voltage indicators to maximize spatiotemporal specificity. For example, what is the difference between trials with a large looming stimulus in the left visual fields when the turn direction bias indicates a leftward versus rightward drive?

      - What other motor outputs do the noise dimensions correlate with?

      To better demonstrate the relationship between neural noise modes and motor activity that we described, we have provided a more detailed correlation analysis in new Figure S4A. We extracted additional features related to the larva’s tail kinematics, including tail vigor, curvature, principal components of curvature, angular velocity, and angular acceleration (S4A(i)). Some of these behavioral features were correlated with one another; for example, in the example traces, PC1 appears to capture nearly the same behavioral feature as tail vigor. The largest noise modes showed stronger correlations with motor output than the smaller noise modes, which is reminiscent recent work in the mouse showing that some of the neural dimensions with highest variance were correlated with various behavioral features (Musall et al. 2019; Stringer et al. 2019; Manley et al. 2024). We anticipate additional motor outputs would exhibit correlations with neural noise modes, such as pectoral fin movements (not possible to capture in our preparation due to immobilization) and eye movements.

      The dataset that the authors have collected is immensely valuable to the field, and the initial insights they have drawn are interesting and provide a good starting ground for a more expanded understanding of why a particular action is determined outside of the parameters experimenters set for their subjects.

      We thank the reviewer for noting the value of our dataset and look forward to future efforts motivated by the observations in our study.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Overview

      In this work, Manley and Vaziri investigate the neural basis for variability in the way an animal responds to visual stimuli evoking prey-capture or predator-avoidance decisions. This is an interesting problem and the authors have generated a potentially rich and relevant data set. To do so, the authors deployed Fourier light field microscopy (Flfm) of larval zebrafish, improving upon prior designs and image processing schemes to enable volumetric imaging of calcium signals in the brain at up to 10 Hz. They then examined associations between neural activity and tail movement to identify populations primarily related to the visual stimulus, responsiveness, or turn direction - moreover, they found that the activity of the latter two populations appears to predict upcoming responsiveness or turn direction even before the stimulus is presented. While these findings may be valuable for future more mechanistic studies, issues with resolution, rigor of analysis, clarity of presentation, and depth of connection to the prior literature significantly dampen enthusiasm.

      Imaging

      - Resolution: It is difficult to tell from the displayed images how good the imaging resolution is in the brain. Given scattering and lensing, it is important for data interpretation to have an understanding of how much PSF degrades with depth.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments and agree that the dependence of the PSF and resolution as a function of depth is an important consideration in light field imaging. To quantify this, we measured the lateral resolution of the fLFM as a function of distance from the native image plane (NIP) using a USAF target. The USAF target was positioned at various depths using an automated z-stage, and the slice of the reconstructed volume corresponding to that depth was analyzed. An element was considered resolved if the modulation transfer function (MTF) was greater than 30%.

      In new Figure S1A, we plot the resolution measurements of the fLFM as compared to the conventional LFM (Prevedel et al., 2014), which shows the increase in resolution across the axial extent of imaging. In particular, the fLFM does not exhibit the dramatic drop in lateral resolution near the NIP which is seen in conventional LFM. In addition, the expanded range of high-resolution imaging motivates our increase from an axial range of 200 microns in previous studies to 280 microns in this study.

      - Depth: In the methods it is indicated that the imaging depth was 280 microns, but from the images of Figure 1 it appears data was collected only up to 150 microns. This suggests regions like the hypothalamus, which may be important for controlling variation in internal states relevant to the behaviors being studied, were not included.

      The full axial range of imaging was 280 microns, i.e. spanning from 140 microns below to 140 microns above the native imaging plane. After aligning our recordings to the Z-Brain dataset, we have compared the 3D distribution of neurons in our data (new Figure S1E(i)) to the labeling of the reference brain (Figure S1E(ii)). This provides evidence that our imaging preparation largely captures the labeling seen in a dense, high-resolution reference image within the indicated 280 microns range.

      - Flfm data processing: It is important for data interpretation that the authors are clearer about how the raw images were processed. The de-noising process specifically needs to be explained in greater detail. What are the characteristics of the noise being removed? How is time-varying signal being distinguished from noise? Please provide a supplemental with images and algorithm specifics for each key step.

      We thank the reviewer for their comment. To address the reviewer’s point regarding the data processing pipeline utilized in our study, in our revised manuscript we have added a number of additional figure panels in Figure S1B-E to quantify and describe the various steps of the pipeline in greater depth.

      First, the raw fLFM images are denoised. The denoising approach utilized in the fLFM data processing pipeline is not novel, but rather a custom-trained variant of Lecoq et al.’s (2021) DeepInterpolation method. In our original manuscript, we also described the specific architecture and parameters utilized to train our specific variation of DeepInterpolation model. To make this procedure clearer, we have added the following details to the methods:

      “DeepInterpolation is a self-supervised approach to denoising, which denoises the data by learning to predict a given frame from a set of frames before and after it. Time-varying signal can be distinguished from shot noise because shot noise is independent across frames, but signal is not. Therefore, only the signal is able to be predicted from adjacent frames. This has been shown to provide a highly effective and efficient denoising method (Lecoq et al., 2021).”

      Therefore, time-varying signal is distinguished from noise based on the correlations of pixel intensity across consecutive imaging frames. To better visualize this process, in new Figure S1B we show example images and fluorescence traces before and after denoising.

      - Merging: It is noted that nearby pixels with a correlation greater than 0.7 were merged. Why was this done? Is this largely due to cross-contamination due to a drop in resolution? How common was this occurrence? What was the distribution of pixel volumes after aggregation? Should we interpret this to mean that a 'neuron' in this data set is really a small cluster of 10-20 neurons? This of course has great bearing on how we think about variability in the response shown later.

      First, to be clear, nearby pixels were not merged; instead neuronal ROIs identified by CNMF-E were merged, as we had described: “the CNMF-E algorithm was applied to each plane in parallel, after which the putative neuronal ROIs from each plane were collated and duplicate neurons across planes were merged.” If this merging was not performed, the number of neurons would be overestimated due to the relatively dense 3D reconstruction with voxels of 4 m axially. Therefore, this merging is a requisite component of the pipeline to avoid double counting of neurons, regardless of the resolution of the data.

      However, we agree with the reviewer that the practical consequences of this merging were not previously described in sufficient detail. Therefore, in our revision we have added additional quantification of the two critical components of the merging procedure: the number of putative neuronal ROIs merged and the volume of the final 3D neuronal ROIs, which demonstrate that a neuron in our data should not be interpreted as a cluster of 10-20 neurons.

      In new Figure S1C(i), we summarize the rate of occurrence of merging by assessing the number of putative 2D ROIs which were merged to form each final 3D neuronal ROI. Across n=10 recordings, approximately 75% of the final 3D neuronal ROIs involved no merging at all, and few instances involved merging more than 5 putative ROIs. Next, in Figure S1C(ii), we quantify the volume of the final 3D ROIs. To do so, we counted the number of voxels contributing to each final 3D neuronal ROI and multiplied that by the volume of a single voxel (2.4 x 2.4 x 4 µm<sup>3</sup>). The majority of neurons had a volume of less than 1000 µm<up>3</sup>, which corresponds to a spherical volume with a radius of roughly 6.2 m. In summary, both the merging statistics and volume distribution demonstrate that few neuronal ROIs could be consistent with “a small cluster of 10-20 neurons”.

      - Bleaching: Please give the time constants used in the fit for assessing bleaching.

      As described in the Methods, the photobleaching correction was performed by fitting a bi-exponential function to the mean fluorescence across all neurons. We have provided the time constants determined by these fits for n=10 recordings in new Figure S1D(i). In addition, we provided an example of raw mean activity, the corresponding bi-exponential fit, and the mean activity after correction in Figure S1D(ii). These data demonstrate that the dominant photobleaching effect is a steep decrease in mean signal at the beginning of the recording (represented by the estimated time constant τ<sub>1</sub>), followed by a slow decay (τ<sub>2</sub>).

      Analysis

      - Slow calcium dynamics: It does not appear that the authors properly account for the slow dynamics of calcium-sensing in their analysis. Nuclear-localized GCaMP6s will likely have a kernel with a multiple-second decay time constant for many of the cells being studied. The value used needs to be given and the authors should account for variability in this kernel time across cell types. Moreover, by not deconvolving their signals, the authors allow for contamination of their signal at any given time with a signal from multiple seconds prior. For example, in Figure 4A (left turns), it appears that much of the activity in the first half of the time-warped stimulus window began before stimulus presentation - without properly accounting for the kernel, we don't know if the stimulus-associated activity reported is really stimulus-associated firing or a mix of stimulus and pre-stimulus firing. This also suggests that in some cases the signals from the prior trial may contaminate the current trial.

      We would like to respond to each of the points raised here by the reviewer individually.

      (1) “It does not appear that the authors properly account for the slow dynamics of calcium-sensing in their analysis. Nuclear-localized GCaMP6s will likely have a kernel with a multiple-second decay time constant for many of the cells being studied. The value used needs to be given…”

      We disagree with the reviewer’s claim that the slow dynamics of the calcium indicator GCaMP were not accounted for. While we did not deconvolve the neuronal traces with the GCaMP response kernel, in every step in which we correlated neural activity with sensory or motor variables, we convolved the stimulus or motor timeseries with the GCaMP kernel, as described in the Methods. Therefore, the expected delay and smoothing effects were accounted for when analyzing the correlation structure between neural and behavioral or stimulus variables, as well as during our various classification approaches. To better describe this, we have added the following description of the kernel to our Methods:

      “The NL-GCaMP6s kernel was estimated empirically by aligning and averaging a number of calcium events. This kernel corresponds to a half-rise time of 400 ms and half-decay time of 4910 ms.”

      This approach accounts for the GCaMP kernel when relating the neuronal dynamics to stimuli and behavior, while avoiding any artifacts that could be introduced from improper deconvolution or other corrections directly to the calcium dynamics. Deconvolution of calcium imaging data, and in particular nuclear-localized (NL) GCaMP6s, is not always a robust procedure. In particular, GCaMP6s has a much more nonlinear response profile than newer GCaMP variants such as jGCaMP8 (Zhang et al. 2023, doi:10.1038/s41586-023-05828-9), as the reviewer notes later in their comments. The nuclear-localized nature of the indicator used in our study also provides an additional nonlinear effect. Accounting for a nonlinear relationship between calcium concentration and fluorescence readout is significantly more difficult because such nonlinearities remove the guarantee that the optimization approaches generally used in deconvolution will converge to global extrema. This means that deconvolution assuming nonlinearities is far less robust than deconvolution using the linear approximation (Vogelstein et al. 2010, doi: 10.1152/jn.01073.2009). Therefore, we argue that we are not currently aware of any appropriate methods for deconvolving our NL-GCaMP6s data, and take a more conservative approach in our study.

      We also argue that the natural smoothness of calcium imaging data is important for the analyses utilized in our study (Shen et al., 2022, doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2021.109431). Even if our data were deconvolved in order to estimate spike trains or more point-like activity patterns, such data are generally smoothed (e.g., by estimating firing rates) before dimensionality reduction, which is a core component of our neuronal population analyses. Further, Wei et al. (2020, doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1008198) showed in detail that deconvolved calcium data resulted in less accurate population decoding, whereas binned electrophysiological data and raw calcium data were equally accurate. When using other techniques, such as clustering of neuronal activity patterns (a method we do not employ in this study), spike and deconvolved calcium data were instead shown to be more accurate than raw calcium data. Therefore, we do not believe deconvolution of the neuronal traces is appropriate in this case without a better understanding of the NL-GCaMP6s response, and do not rely on the properties of deconvolution for our analyses. Still, we agree with the reviewer that one must be mindful of the GCaMP kernel when analyzing and interpreting these data, and therefore have noted the delayed and slow kinematics of the NL-GCaMP within our manuscript, for example: “To visualize the neuronal activity during a given trial while accounting for the delay and kinematics of the nuclear-localized GCaMP (NL-GCaMP) sensor, a duration of approximately 15 seconds is extracted beginning at the onset of the 3-second visual stimulus period.”

      (2) “… and the authors should account for variability in this kernel time across cell types.”

      In addition to the points raised above, we are not aware of any deconvolution procedures which have successfully shown the ability to account for variability in the response kernel across cell types in whole-brain imaging data when cell type is unknown a priori. Pachitariu et al. (2018, doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3339-17.2018) showed that the best deconvolution procedures for calcium imaging data rely on a simple algorithm with a fixed kernel. Further, more complicated approaches either utilize either explicit priors about the calcium kernel or learn implicit priors using supervised learning, neither of which we would be able to confirm are appropriate for our dataset without ground truth electrophysiological spike data.

      However, we agree with the reviewer that we must interpret the data while being mindful that there could be variability in this kernel across neurons, which is not accounted for in our fixed calcium kernel. We have added the following sentence to our revised manuscript to highlight this limitation:

      “The used of a fixed calcium kernel does not account for any variability in the GCaMP response across cells, which could be due to differences such as cell type or expression level. Therefore, this analysis approach may not capture the full set of neurons which exhibit stimulus correlations but exhibit a different GCaMP response.”

      (3) “without properly accounting for the kernel, we don't know if the stimulus-associated activity reported is really stimulus-associated firing or a mix of stimulus and pre-stimulus firing”

      While we agree with the reviewer that the slow dynamics of the indicator will cause a delay and smoothing of the signal over time, we would like to point out that this effect is highly directional. In particular, we can be confident that pre-stimulus activity is not contaminated by the stimulus given the data we describe in the next point regarding the timing of visual stimuli relative to the GCaMP kernel. The reviewer is correct that post-stimulus firing can be mixed with pre-stimulus firing due to the GCaMP kernel. However, our key claims in Figure 4 center around turn direction and responsiveness biases, which are present even before the onset of the stimulus. Still, we have highlighted this delay and smoothing to readers in the updated version of our manuscript.

      (4) “This also suggests that in some cases the signals from the prior trial may contaminate the current trial”

      We have carefully chosen the inter-stimulus interval for maximum efficiency of stimulation, while ensuring that contamination from the previous stimulus is negligible. The inter-stimulus interval was chosen by empirically analyzing preliminary data of visual stimulation with our preparation. New Figure S3C shows the delay and slow kinematics due to our indicator; indeed, visually-evoked activity peaks after the end of the short stimulus period. Importantly, however, the visually-evoked activity is at or near baseline at the start of the next trial.

      Finally, we would like to note that our stimulation protocol is randomized, as described in the Methods. Therefore, the previous stimulus has no correlation with the current stimulus, which would prevent any contamination from providing predictive power that could be identified by our visual decoding methods.

      - Partial Least Squares (PLS) regression: The steps taken to identify stimulus coding and noise dimensions are not sufficiently clear. Please provide a mathematical description.

      We have updated the Results and Methods sections of our revised manuscript to describe in more mathematical detail the approach taken to identify the relevant dimensions of neuronal activity:

      “The comparison of the neural dimensions encoding visual stimuli versus trial-to-trial noise was modeled after Rumyantsev et al. (2020). Partial least squares (PLS) regression was used to find a low-dimensional space that optimally predicted the visual stimuli, which we refer to as the visually-evoked neuronal activity patterns. To perform regression, a visual stimulus kernel was constructed by summing the timeseries of each individual stimulus type, weighted by the stimulus size and negated for trials on the right visual field, thus providing a single response variable encoding both the location, size, and timing of all the stimulus presentations. This stimulus kernel was the convolved with the temporal response kernel of our calcium indicator (NL-GCaMP6s).

      PLS regression identifies the normalized dimensions and that maximize the covariance between paired observations and , respectively. In our case, the visual stimulus is represented by a single variable , simplifying the problem to identifying the subspace of neural activity that optimally preserves information about the visual stimulus (sometimes referred to as PLS1 regression). That is, the N x T neural time series matrix X is reduced to a d x T matrix spanned by a set of orthonormal vectors. PLS1 regression is performed as follows:

      PLS1 algorithm

      Let X<sub>i</sub> = X and . For i = 1…d,

      (1) 

      (2) 

      (3) 

      (4) 

      (5)  (note this is scalar)

      (6) 

      The projections of the neural data {p<sub>i</sub>} thus span a subspace that maximally preserves information about the visual stimulus . Stacking these projections into the N x d matrix P that represents the transform from the whole-brain neural state space to the visually-evoked subspace, the optimal decoding direction is given by the linear least squares solution . The dimensionality d of PLS regression was optimized using 6-fold cross-validation with 3 repeats and choosing the dimensionality between d = 1 and 20 with the lowest cross-validated mean squared error for each larva. Then, was computed using all time points.

      For each stimulus type, the noise covariance matrix  was computed in the low-dimensional PLS space, given that direct estimation of the noise covariances across many thousands of neurons would likely be unreliable. A noise covariance matrix was calculated separately for each stimulus, and then averaged across all stimuli. As before, the mean activity µ<sub>i</sub> for each neuron  was computed over each stimulus presentation period. The noise covariance then describes the correlated fluctuations δ<sub>i</sub> around this mean response for each pair of neurons i and j, where

      The noise modes for α = 1 …d were subsequently identified by eigendecomposition of the mean noise covariance matrix across all stimuli, . The angle between the optimal stimulus decoding direction and the noise modes is thus given by .”

      - No response: It is not clear from the methods description if cases where the animal has no tail response are being lumped with cases where the animal decides to swim forward and thus has a large absolute but small mean tail curvature. These should be treated separately. 

      We thank the reviewer for raising the potential for this confusion and agree that forward-motion trials should not treated the same as motionless trials. While these types of trial were indeed treated separately in our original manuscript, we have updated the Methods section of our revised manuscript to make this clear:

      “Left and right turn trials were extracted as described previously. Response trials included both left and right turn trials (i.e., the absolute value of mean tail curvature > σ<sub>active</sub>), whereas nonresponse trials were motionless (absolute mean tail curvature < σ<sub>active</sub>). In particular, forward-motion trials were excluded from these analyses.”

      While our study has focused specifically on left and right turns, we hypothesize that the responsiveness bias ensemble may also be involved in forward movements and look forward to future work exploring the relationship between whole-brain dynamics and the full range of motor outputs.

      - Behavioral variability: Related to Figure 2, within- and across-subject variability are confounded. Please disambiguate. It may also be informative on a per-fish basis to examine associations between reaction time and body movement.

      The reviewer is correct that our previously reported summary statistics in Figure 2D-F were aggregated across trials from multiple larvae. Following the reviewer’s suggestion to make the magnitudes of across-larvae and within-larva variability clear, in our revised manuscript we have added two additional figure panels to Figure S2.

      New Figure S2A highlights the across-larvae variability in mean head-directed behavioral responses to stimuli of various sizes. Overall, the relationship between stimulus size and the mean tail curvature across trials is largely consistent across larvae; however, the crossing-over point between leftward (positive curvature) and rightward (negative curvature) turns for a given side of the visual field exhibits some variability across larvae.

      New Figure S2B shows examples of within-larva variability by plotting the mean tail curvature during single trials for two example larvae. Consistent with Figure 2G which also demonstrates within-larva variability, responses to a given stimulus are variable across trials in both examples. However, this degree of within-larva variability can appear different across larvae. For example, the larva shown on the left of Figure S2B exhibits greater overlap between responses to stimuli presented on opposite visual fields, whereas the larva shown on the right exhibits greater distinction between responses.

      - Data presentation clarity: All figure panels need scale bars - for example, in Figure 3A there is no indication of timescale (or time of stimulus presentation). Figure 3I should also show the time series of the w_opt projection.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s attention to detail in this regard. We have added scalebars to Figures 3A, 3H-I, S4B(ii), 4H, 4J in the revised manuscript, and all new figure panels where relevant. In addition, the caption of Figure 3A has been updated to include a description of the time period plotted relative to the onset of the visual stimulus.

      Additionally, we appreciate the reviewer’s idea to show w<sub>opt</sub> in Figure 3J of the revised manuscript (previously Figure 3I). This clearly shows that the visual decoding project is inactive during the short baseline period before visual stimulation begins, whereas the noise mode is correlated with motor output throughout the recording.

      - Pixel locations: Given the poor quality of the brain images, it is difficult to tell the location of highlighted pixels relative to brain anatomy. In addition, given that the midbrain consists of much more than the tectum, it is not appropriate to put all highlighted pixels from the midbrain under the category of tectum. To aid in data interpretation and better connect this work with the literature, it is recommended that the authors register their data sets to standard brain atlases and determine if there is any clustering of relevant pixels in regions previously associated with prey-capture or predator-avoidance behavior.

      We agree with the reviewer that registration of our datasets to a standard brain atlas is a highly useful addition. While the dense, pan-neuronal labeling makes the isolation of highly specific circuit components difficult, we have shown in more detail the specific brain regions contributing to these populations by aligning our recordings to the Z-Brain atlas (Randlett et al., 2015) as shown in new Figures S1E, S3F-G, 4I, 4K, and S5F-G. In addition, we provided a more detailed parcellation of the neuronal ensembles by providing projections of the full 3D volume along the xy and yz axes, in addition to the unregistered xy projection shown in new Figures 4H and 4J. We also found that the distribution of neurons in our huc:H2B-GCaMP6s recordings is very similar to the distribution of labeling in the huc:H2B-RFP reference image from the Z-Brain atlas (new Figure S1E), which further supports our whole-brain imaging results.

      Overall, we find that this more detailed quantification and visualization is consistent with the interpretations in the previous version of our manuscript. In particular, we show that optimal visual decoding population (w<sub>opt</sub>) and largest noise mode (e1) are localized to the midbrain (new Figures S3F-G), which is expected since in Figure 3 we first extracted a low-dimensional subspace of whole-brain neural activity that optimally preserved visual information. Additionally, we provide additional evidence that the populations correlated with the turn bias and responsiveness bias are distributed throughout the brain, including a relatively dense localization to the cerebellum, telencephalon, and dorsal diencephalon (habenula, new Figures 4H-K and S5F-G).

      Finally, the reviewer is correct that our original label of “tectum” was a misnomer; the region analyzed corresponded to the midbrain, including the tegmentum, torus longitudinalis, and torus semicicularis in addition to the tectum. We have updated the brain regions shown and labels throughout the manuscript.

      Interpretation

      - W_opt and e_1 orthogonality: The statement that these two vectors, determined from analysis of the fluorescence data, are orthogonal, actually brings into question the idea that true signal and leading noise vectors in firing-rate state-space are orthogonal. First, the current analysis is confounding signals across different time periods - one could assume linearity all the way through the transformations, but this would only work if earlier sources of activation were being accounted for. Second, the transformation between firing rate and fluorescence is most likely not linear for GCaMP6s in most of the cells recorded. Thus, one would expect a change in the relationship between these vectors as one maps from fluorescence to firing rate.

      Unfortunately, we are not entirely sure we have understood the reviewer’s argument. We are assuming that the reviewer’s first sentence is suggesting that the observation of orthogonality in the neural state space measured in calcium imaging precludes the possibility (“actually brings into question”, as the reviewer states) that the same neural ensembles could be orthogonal in firing rate state space measured by electrophysiological data. If this is the reviewer’s conjecture, we respectfully disagree with it. Consider a toy example of a neural network containing N ensembles of neurons, where the neurons within an ensemble all fire simultaneously, and two populations never fire at the same time. As long as the “switching” of firing between ensembles is not fast relative to the resolution of the GCaMP kernel, the largest principal components would represent orthogonal dimensions differentiating the various ensembles, both when observing firing rates or observing timeseries convolved by the GCaMP kernel. This is a simple example where the observed orthogonality would appear similar in both calcium imaging and electrophysical data, demonstrating that we should not allow conclusions from fluorescence data to “bring into question” that the same result could be observed in firing rate data.

      We also disagree with the reviewer’s argument that we are “confounding signals across time periods”. Indeed, we must interpret the data in light of the GCaMP response kernel. However, all of the analyses presented here are performed on instantaneous measurements of population activity patterns. These activity patterns do represent a smoothed, likely nonlinear integration of recent neuronal activity, but unless the variability in the GCaMP response kernel (discussed above) is widely different across these populations (which has not been observed in the literature), we do not expect that the GCaMP transformations would artificially induce orthogonality in our analysis approach. Such smoothing operations tend to instead increase correlations across neurons and population decoding approaches generally benefit from this smoothness, as we have argued above. However, a much more problematic situation would be if we were comparing the activity of two neuronal populations at different points in time (which we do not include in this study), in which case the nonlinearities could overaccentuate orthogonality between non-time-matched activity patterns.

      Finally, we agree with the reviewer that the transformation between firing rate and fluorescence is very likely nonlinear and that these vectors of population activity do not perfectly represent what would be observed if one had access to whole-brain, cellular-resolution electrophysiology spike data. However, similar observations regarding the brain-wide, distributed encoding of behavior have been confirmed across recording modalities in the mouse (Stringer et al., 2019; Steinmetz et al., 2019), where large-scale electrophysiology utilizing highly invasive probes (e.g., Neuropixels) is more feasible than in the larval zebrafish. With the advent of whole-brain voltage imaging in the larval zebrafish, we expect any differences between calcium and voltage dynamics will be better understood, yet such techniques will likely continue to suffer to some extent from the nonlinearities described here.

      - Sources of variability: The authors do not take into account a fairly obvious source of variability in trial-to-trial response - eye position. We know that prey capture responsiveness is dependent on eye position during stimulus (see Figure 4 of PMID: 22203793). We also expect that neurons fairly early in the visual pathway with relatively narrow receptive fields will show variable responses to visual stimuli as the degree of overlap with the receptive field varies with eye movement. There can also be small eye-tracking movements ahead of the decision to engage in prey capture (Figure 1D, PMID: 31591961) that can serve as a drive to initiate movements in a particular direction. Given these possibilities indicating that the behavioral measure of interest is gaze, and the fact that eye movements were apparently monitored, it is surprising that the authors did not include eye movements in the analysis and interpretation of their data.

      We agree with the reviewer that eye movements, such as saccades and convergence, are important motor outputs that are well-known to play a role in the sequence of motor actions during prey capture and other behaviors. Therefore, we have added the following new eye tracking results to our revised manuscript:

      “In order to confirm that the observed neural variability in the visually-evoked populations was not predominantly due to eye movements, such as saccades or convergence, we tracked the angle of each eye. We utilized DeepLabCut, a deep learning tool for animal pose estimation (Mathis et al., 2018), to track keypoints on the eye which are visible in the raw fLFM images, including the retina and pigmentation (Figure S3D(i)). This approach enabled identification of various eye movements, such as convergence and the optokinetic reflex (Figure S3D(ii-iii)). Next, we extracted a number of various eye states, including those based on position (more leftward vs. rightward angles) and speed (high angular velocity vs. low or no motion). Figure S3E(i) provides example stimulus response profiles across trials of the same visual stimulus in each of these eye states, similar to a single column of traces in Figure 3A broken out into more detail. These data demonstrate that the magnitude and temporal dynamics of the stimulus-evoked responses show apparently similar levels of variability across eye states. If neural variability was driven by eye movement during the stimulus presentation, for example, one would expect to see much more variability during the high angular velocity trials than low, which is not apparent. Next, we asked whether the dominant neural noise modes vary across eye states, which would suggest that the geometry of neuronal variability is influenced by eye movements or states. To do so, the dominant noise modes were estimated in each of the individual eye conditions, as well as bootstrapped trials from across all eye conditions. The similarity of these noise modes estimated from different eye conditions (Figure S3E(ii), right)) was not significantly different from the similarity of noise modes estimated from bootstrapped random samples across all eye conditions (Figure S3E(ii), left)). Therefore, while movements of the eye likely contribute to aspects of the observed neural variability, they do not dominate the observed neural variability here, particularly given our observation that the largest noise mode represents a considerable fraction of the observed neural variance (Figure 3E).”

      While these results provide an important control in our study, we anticipate further study of the relationship between eye movements or states, visually-evoked neural activity, and neural noise modes would identify the additional neural ensembles which are correlated with and drive this additional motor output.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Manley and Vaziri designed and built a Fourier light-field microscope (fLFM) inspired by previous implementations but improved and exclusively from commercially available components so others can more easily reproduce the design. They combined this with the design of novel algorithms to efficiently extract whole-brain activity from larval zebrafish brains.

      This new microscope was applied to the question of the origin of behavioral variability. In an assay in which larval zebrafish are exposed to visual dots of various sizes, the fish respond by turning left or right or not responding at all. Neural activity was decomposed into an activity that encodes the stimulus reliably across trials, a 'noise' mode that varies across trials, and a mode that predicts tail movements. A series of analyses showed that trial-to-trial variability was largely orthogonal to activity patterns that encoded the stimulus and that these noise modes were related to the larvae's behavior.

      To identify the origins of behavioral variability, classifiers were fit to the neural data to predict whether the larvae turned left or right or did not respond. A set of neurons that were highly distributed across the brain could be used to classify and predict behavior. These neurons could also predict spontaneous behavior that was not induced by stimuli above chance levels. The work concludes with findings on the distributed nature of single-trial decision-making and behavioral variability.

      Strengths:

      The design of the new fLFM microscope is a significant advance in light-field and computational microscopy, and the open-source design and software are promising to bring this technology into the hands of many neuroscientists.

      The study addresses a series of important questions in systems neuroscience related to sensory coding, trial-to-trial variability in sensory responses, and trial-to-trial variability in behavior. The study combines microscopy, behavior, dynamics, and analysis and produces a well-integrated analysis of brain dynamics for visual processing and behavior. The analyses are generally thoughtful and of high quality. This study also produces many follow-up questions and opportunities, such as using the methods to look at individual brain regions more carefully, applying multiple stimuli, investigating finer tail movements and how these are encoded in the brain, and the connectivity that gives rise to the observed activity. Answering questions about variability in neural activity in the entire brain and its relationship to behavior is important to neuroscience and this study has done that to an interesting and rigorous degree.

      Points of improvement and weaknesses:

      The results on noise modes may be a bit less surprising than they are portrayed. The orthogonality between neural activity patterns encoding the sensory stimulus and the noise modes should be interpreted within the confounds of orthogonality in high-dimensional spaces. In higher dimensional spaces, it becomes more likely that two random vectors are almost orthogonal. Since the neural activity measurements performed in this study are quite high dimensional, a more explicit discussion is warranted about the small chance that the modes are not almost orthogonal.

      We agree with the reviewer that orthogonality is less “surprising” in high-dimensional spaces, and we have added this important point of interpretation to our revised manuscript. Still, it is important to remember that while the full neural state space is very high-dimensional (we record that activity of up to tens of thousands of neurons simultaneously), our analyses regarding the relationship between the trial-to-trial noise modes and decoding dimensions were performed in a low-dimensional subspace (up to 20 dimensions) identified by PLS regression to that optimally preserved visual information. This is a key step in our analysis which serves two purposes: 1. it removes some of the confound described the reviewer regarding the dimensionality of the neural state space analyzed; and 2. it ensures that the noise modes we analyze are even relevant to sensorimotor processing. It would certainly not be surprising or interesting if we identified a neural dimension outside the midbrain which was orthogonal to the optimal visual decoding dimension. 

      Regardless, in order to better control for this confound, we estimated the distribution of angles between random vectors in this subspace. As we describe in the revised manuscript:

      “However, in high-dimensional spaces, it becomes increasingly common that two random vectors could appear orthogonal. While this is particularly a concern when analyzing a neural state space spanned by tens of thousands of neurons, our application of PLS regression to identify a low-dimensional subspace of relevant neuronal activity partially mitigates this concern. In order to control for this confound, we compared the angles between w<sub>opt</sub> and e1 across larvae to that computed with shuffled versions of w<sub>opt,shuff</sub> estimated by randomly shuffling the stimulus labels before identifying the optimal decoding direction. While it is possible to observe shuffled vectors which are nearly orthogonal to e<sub>1</sub>, the shuffled distribution spans a significantly greater range of angles than the observed data, demonstrating that this orthogonality is not simply a consequence of analyzing multi-dimensional activity patterns.”

      The conclusion that sparsely distributed sets of neurons produce behavioral variability needs more investigation because the way the results are shown could lead to some misinterpretations. The prediction of behavior from classifiers applied to neural activity is interesting, but the results are insufficiently presented for two reasons.

      (1) The neurons that contribute to the classifiers (Figures 4H and J) form a sufficient set of neurons that predict behavior, but this does not mean that neurons outside of that set cannot be used to predict behavior. Lasso regularization was used to create the classifiers and this induces sparsity. This means that if many neurons predict behavior but they do so similarly, the classifier may select only a few of them. This is not a problem in itself but it means that the distributions of neurons across the brain (Figures 4H and J) may appear sparser and more distributed than the full set of neurons that contribute to producing the behavior. This ought to be discussed better to avoid misinterpretation of the brain distribution results, and an alternative analysis that avoids the confound could help clarify.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point, which we agree should be discussed in the manuscript. Lasso regularization was a key ingredient in our analysis; l2 regularization alone was not sufficient to prevent overfitting to the training trials, particularly when decoding turn direction and responsiveness. Previous studies have also found that sparse subsets of neurons better predict behavior than single neuron or non-sparse populations, for example Scholz et al. (2018).

      While showing l2 regularization would not be a fair comparison given the poor performance of the l2-regularized classifiers, we opted to identify a potentially “fuller” set of neurons correlated with these biases based on the correlation between each neuron’s activity over the recording and the projection along the turn direction or responsiveness dimension identified using l1 regularization. This procedure has the potential to identify all neurons correlated with the final ensemble dynamics, rather than just a “sufficient set” for lasso regression. In new Figures S5F-G, we show the 3D distribution of all neurons significantly correlated with these biases, which appear similar to those in Figures 4H-K and widely distributed across practically the entire labeled area of the brain.

      (2) The distribution of neurons is shown in an overly coarse manner in only a flattened brain seen from the top, and the brain is divided into four coarse regions (telencephalon, tectum, cerebellum, hindbrain). This makes it difficult to assess where the neurons are and whether those four coarse divisions are representative or whether the neurons are in other non-labeled deeper regions. For these two reasons, some of the statements about the distribution of neurons across the brain would benefit from a more thorough investigation.

      We agree with the reviewer that a more thorough description and visualization of these distributed populations is warranted.

      While the dense, pan-neuronal labeling makes the isolation of highly specific circuit components difficult, we have shown in more detail the specific brain regions contributing to these populations by aligning our recordings to the Z-Brain atlas (Randlett et al., 2015) as shown in new Figures S1E, S3F-G, 4I, 4K, and S5F-G. In addition, we provided a more detailed parcellation of the neuronal ensembles by providing projections of the full 3D volume along the xy and yz axes, in addition to the unregistered xy projection shown in new Figures 4H and 4J. We also found that the distribution of neurons in our huc:H2B-GCaMP6s recordings is very similar to the distribution of labeling in the huc:H2B-RFP reference image from the Z-Brain atlas (new Figure S1E), which further supports our whole-brain imaging results.

      Overall, we find that this more detailed quantification and visualization is consistent with the interpretations in the previous version of our manuscript. In particular, we show that optimal visual decoding population (w<sub>opt</sub>) and largest noise mode (e1) are localized to the midbrain (new Figures S3F-G), which is expected since in Figure 3 we first extracted a low-dimensional subspace of whole-brain neural activity that optimally preserved visual information. Additionally, we provide additional evidence that the populations correlated with the turn bias and responsiveness bias are distributed throughout the brain, including a relatively dense localization to the cerebellum, telencephalon, and dorsal diencephalon (habenula, new Figures 4H-K and S5F-G).

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      In addition to the overall strengths and weaknesses above, I have a few specific comments that I think could improve the study:

      (1) In lines 334-335 you write that 'We proceeded to build various logistic regression classifiers to decode'. Do you mean you tested this with other classifier types as well (e.g. SVM, Naive Bayes) or do you mean various because you trained the classifier described in the methods on each animal? This is not clear. If it is the first, more information is needed about what other classifiers you used.

      We appreciate the reviewer raising this point of clarification. Here, we simply meant that we fit the multiclass logistic regression classifier in the one-vs-rest scheme. In this sense, a single multiclass logistic regression classifier was fit for each larva. We have updated our revised manuscript with this clarification: “The visual stimuli were decoded using a one-versus-rest, multiclass logistic regression classifier with lasso regularization.”

      (2) In Figure 3 you train the decoder on all visually responsive cells identified across the brain. Does this reliability of stimulus decoding also hold for neurons sampled from specific brain regions? For example, does this reliable decoding come from stronger and more reliable responses in the optic tectum, whereas stimulus decodability is not as good in visual encoding neurons identified in other structures?

      In new Figure S5B, we show the performance of stimulus decoding from various brain regions. We find that stimulus classification is possible from the midbrain and cerebellum, very poor from the hindbrain, and not possible from the telencephalon during the period between stimulus onset and the decision.

      (3) In relation to point 2, it would be good to show in which brain areas the visually responsive neurons are located, and maybe the average coefficients per brain area. Plots like Figures 3G, and H would benefit from a quantification into areas. Similarly, a parcellation into more specific brain areas in Figure 4 would also be valuable.

      In addition to providing a more detailed parcellation of the turn direction and responsiveness bias populations in Figure 4, we have provided a similar visualization and quantification of the optimal stimulus decoding population and the dominant noise mode in new Figures S3F-G, respectively.

      (4) In Figure 3f, it is not clear to me how this shows that w<sub>opt</sub> and e1 are orthogonal. They appear correlated.

      The orthogonality we quantify is related to the pattern of coefficients across neurons, not necessarily the timeseries of their projections. The slight shift in the noise mode activations as you move from stimuli on the left visual field to the right actually comes from the motor outputs. Large left stimuli tend to evoke a rightward turn and vice versa, and the example noise mode shown encodes the directionality and vigor of tail movements, resulting in the slight shifts observed.

      (5) I think the wording of this conclusion is too strong for the results and a bit illogical:

      'Thus, our data suggest that the neural dynamics underlying single-trial action selection are the result of a widely-distributed circuit that contains subpopulations encoding internal time-varying biases related to both the larva's responsiveness and turn direction, yet distinct from the sensory encoding circuitry.'

      If that is the case, how is it even possible that the larvae can do a visually guided behaviour?

      Especially given Suppl Fig 4C it would be more appropriate to say something along the lines of: 'When stimuli are highly ambiguous, single trial action selection is dominated by widely-distributed circuit that contains subpopulations encoding internal time-varying biases related to both the larva's responsiveness and turn direction, that encode choice distinctly from the sensory encoding circuitry'.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion and have re-worded this line in the discussion in order to clarify that these time-varying biases are predominant in the case of ambiguous stimuli, as shown in Figure S5C in our revised manuscript (corresponding to Figure S4C in our original submission).

      (6) Line 599: typo: trial-to-trail

      We thank the reviewer for noting this error, which has been corrected in the revised text of the manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:  

      (1) The heatmaps (for example, Figure 3A, B) are challenging to read and interpret due to their size. Is there a way to alter the visualization to improve interpretability? Perhaps coloring the heatmap by general anatomical region could help? We feel that these heatmaps are critical to the utility of the registration strategy, and hence, clear visualization is necessary. 

      We thank the reviewers for this point on aesthetic improvement, and we agree that clearer visualization of our correlation heatmaps is important. To address this point, we have incorporated the capability of grouping “child” subregions in anatomical order by their more general “parent” region into the package function, plot_correlation_heatmaps(). Parent regions will be can now be plotted as smaller sub-facets in the heatmaps. We have also rearranged our figures to fit enlarged heatmaps in Figures 3-5, and Supplementary Figure 10 for easier visualization. 

      (2) Additional context in the Introduction on the use of immediate early genes to label ensembles of neurons that are specifically activated during the various behavioral manipulations would enable the manuscript and methodology to be better appreciated by a broad audience. 

      We thank the reviewers for this suggestion and have revised the first part of our Introduction to reflect the broader use and appeal of immediate early genes (IEGs) for studying neural changes underlying behavior.

      (3) The authors mention that their segmentation strategies are optimized for the particular staining pattern exhibited by each reporter and demonstrate that the manually annotated cell counts match the automated analysis. They mention that alternative strategies are compatible, but don't show this data. 

      We thank the reviewers for this comment. We also appreciate that integration with alternative strategies is a major point of interest to readers, given that others may be interested in compatibility with our analysis and software package, rather than completely revising their own pre-existing pipelines. 

      Generally, we have validated the ability to import datasets generated from completely different workflows for segmentation and registration. We have since released documentation on our package website with step-by-step instructions on how to do so (https://mjin1812.github.io/SMARTTR/articles/Part5.ImportingExternalDatasets). We believe this tutorial is a major entry point to taking advantage of our analysis package, without adopting our entire workflow.

      This specific point on segmentation refers to the import_segmentation_custom()function in the package. As there is currently not a standard cell segmentation export format adopted by the field, this function still requires some data wrangling into an import format saved as a .txt file. However, we chose not to visually demonstrate this capability in the paper for a few reasons.  

      i) A figure showing the broad testing of many different segmentation algorithms, (e.g., Cellpose, Vaa3d, Trainable Weka Segmentation) would better demonstrate the efficacy of segmentation of these alternative approaches, which have already been well-documented. However, demonstrating importation compatibility is more of a demonstration of API interface, which is better shown in website documentation and tutorial notebooks.

      ii) Additionally, showing importation with one well-established segmentation approach is still a demonstration of a single use case. There would be a major burden-of-proof in establishing importation compatibility with all potential alternative platforms, their specific export formats, which may be slightly different depending on post-processing choices, and the needs of the experimenters (e.g., exporting one versus many channels, having different naming conventions, having different export formats). For example, output from Cellpose can take the form of a NumPy file (_seg.npy file), a .png, or Native ImageJ ROI archive output, and users can have chosen up to four channels. Until the field adopts a standardized file format, one flexible enough to account for all the variables of experimental interest, we currently believe it is more efficient to advise external groups on how to transform their specific data to be compatible with our generic import function.  

      (4) The authors provided highly detailed information for their segmentation strategy, but the same level of detail was not provided for the registration algorithms. Additional details would help users achieve optimal alignment.

      We apologize for this lack of detail. The registration strategy depends upon the WholeBrain (Fürth et al., 2018) package for registration to the Allen Mouse Common Coordinate Framework. While this strategy has been published and documented elsewhere, we have substantially revised our methods section on the registration process to better incorporate details of this approach.

      (5) The authors illustrate registration to the Allen atlas. Can they comment on whether the algorithm is compatible with other atlases or with alternative sectioning planes (horizontal/sagittal)? 

      Since the current registration workflow integrates WholeBrain (Fürth et al., 2018), any limitations of WholeBrain apply to our approach, which means limited support for registering non-coronal sectioning planes and reliance on the Allen Mouse Atlas (Dong, 2008). However, network analysis and plotting functions are currently compatible with the Allen Mouse Brain Atlas and the Kim Unified Mouse Brain Atlas version (2019) (Chon et al., 2019). Therefore, current limitations in registration do not preclude the usefulness of the SMARTTR software in generating valuable insights from network analysis of externally imported datasets. 

      There are a number of alternative workflows, such as the QUINT workflow (Yates et al., 2019), that support multiple different mouse atlases, and registration of arbitrarily sectioned angles. We have plans to support and a facilitate an entry point for this workflow in a future iteration of SMARTTR, but believe it is of benefit to the wider community to release and support SMARTTR in its current state.

      (6) Supplemental Figures S10-13 do not have a legend panel to define the bar graphs. 

      We apologize for this omission and have fixed our legends in our resubmission. Our supplement figure orders have changed and the corresponding figures are now Supplemental Figures S11-14.

      (7) When images in a z-stack were collapsed, was this a max intensity projection or average? Assuming this question is in regards to our manual cell counting validation approach, the zstacks were collapsed as a maximum intensity projection.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) While I was able to install the SMARTR package, after trying for the better part of one hour, I could not install the "mjin1812/wholebrain" R package as instructed in OSF. I also could not find a function to load an example dataset to easily test SMARTR. So, unfortunately, I was unable to test out any of the packages for myself. Along with the currently broken "tractatus/wholebrain" package, this is a good example of why I would strongly encourage the authors to publish SMARTR on either Bioconductor or CRAN in the future. The high standards set by Bioc/CRAN will ensure that SMARTR is able to be easily installed and used across major operating systems for the long term. 

      We greatly thank the reviewer for pointing out this weakness; long-term maintenance of this package is certainly a mutual goal. Loading an .RDATA file is accomplished by either doubleclicking directly on the file in a directory window, after specifying this file type should be opened in RStudio or by using the load() function, (e.g., load("directory/example.RData")). We have now explicitly outlined these directions in the online documentation. 

      Moreover, we have recently submitted our package to CRAN and are currently working on revisions following comments. This has required a package rebranding to “SMARTTR”, as there were naming conflicts with a previously archived repository on CRAN. Currently, SMARTTR is not dependent on the WholeBrain package, which remains optional for the registration portion of our workflow. Ultimately, this independence will allow us to maintain the analysis and visualization portion of the package independently.

      In the meantime, we have fully revised our installation instructions (https://mjin1812.github.io/SMARTTR/articles/SMARTTR). SMARTTR is now downloadable from a CRAN-like repository as a bundled .tar.gz file, which should ease the burden of installation significantly. Installation has been verified on a number of different versions of R on different platforms. Again, we hope these changes are sufficient and improve the process of installation. 

      (2) The package is quite large (several thousand lines include comments and space). While impressive, this does inherently make the package more difficult to maintain - and the authors currently have not included any unit tests. The authors should add unit tests to cover a large percentage of the package to ensure code stability. 

      We have added unit testing to improve the reliability of our package. Unit tests now cover over 71% of our source code base and are available for evaluation on our github website (https://github.com/mjin1812/SMARTTR). We focused on coverage of the most front-facing functions. We appreciate this feedback, which has ultimately enhanced the longevity of our software.

      (3) Why do the authors choose to perform image segmentation outside of the SMARTTR package using ImageJ macros? Leading segmentation algorithms such as CellPose and StarMap have well-documented APIs that would be easy to wrap in R. They would likely be faster as well. As noted in the discussion, making SMARTTR a one-stop shop for multi-ensemble analyses would be more appealing to a user. 

      We appreciate this feedback. We believe parts of our response to Reviewer 1, Comment 3, are relevant to this point. Interfaces for CellPose and ClusterMap (which processes in situ transcriptomic approaches, like STARmap) are both in python, and currently there are ways to call python from within R (https://rstudio.github.io/reticulate/index.html). We will certainly explore incorporating these APIs from R. However, we would anticipate this capability is more similar to “translation” between programming languages, but would not currently preclude users from the issue of needing some familiarity with the capabilities of these python packages, and thus with python syntax.

      (4) Given the small number of observations for correlation analyses (n=6 per group), Pearson correlations would be highly susceptible to outliers. The authors chose to deal with potential outliers by dropping any subject per region that was> 2 SDs from the group mean. Another way to get at this would be using Spearman correlation. How do these analyses change if you use Spearman correlation instead of Pearson? It would be a valuable addition for the author to include Spearman correlations as an option in SMARTTR. 

      We thank reviewers for this suggestion and we have updated our code base to include the possibility for using Spearman’s correlation coefficient as opposed to Pearson’s correlation coefficient for heatmaps in the get_correlations() function. Users can now use the `method` parameter, set to either “pearson” or “spearman” and results will propagate throughout the rest of the analysis using these results.

      Below, in Author response image 1 we show a visual comparison of the correlation heat maps for active eYFP<sup>+</sup> ensembles in the CT and IS groups using both Pearson and Spearman correlations. We see a strongly qualitative similarity between the heat maps. Of course, since the statistical assumptions underlying the relationship between variables using Pearson correlation (linear) vs Spearman correlation (monotonic) are different, users should take this into account when interpreting results using different approaches.

      Author response image 1.

      Pearson and Spearmen regional correlations of eYFP+ ensembles activity in the CT and IS groups.

      (5) I see the authors have incorporated the ability to adjust p-values in many of the analysis functions (and recommend the BH procedure) but did not use adjusted p-values for any of the analyses in the manuscript. Why is this? This is particularly relevant for the differential correlation analyses between groups (Figures 3P and 4P). Based on the un-adjusted pvalues, I assume few if any data points will still be significant after adjusting. While it's logical to highlight the regional correlations that strongly change between groups, the authors should caution which correlations are "significant" without adjusting for multiple comparisons. As this package now makes this analysis easily usable for all researchers, the authors should also provide better explanations for when and why to use adjusted p-values in the online documentation for new users. 

      We appreciate the feedback note that our dataset is presented as a more demonstrative and exploratory resource for readers and, as such, we accept a high tolerance for false positives, while decreasing risk of missing possible interesting findings. As noted by Reviewer #2, it is still “logical to highlight the regional correlations that strongly change between groups.” We have clarified in our methods that we chose to present uncorrected p-values when speaking of significance. 

      We have also removed any previous recommendations for preferred methods for multiple comparisons adjustment in our function documentations, as some previous documentation was outdated. Moreover, the standard multiple comparisons adjustment approaches assume complete independence between tests, whereas this assumption is violated in our differential correlational analysis (i.e., a region with one significantly altered connection is more likely than another to have another significantly altered connection).

      Ultimately, the decision to correct for multiple comparisons with standard FDR, and choice of significance threshold, should still be informed by standard statistical theory and user-defined tolerance for inclusion of false-positives and missing of false-negatives. This will be influenced by factors, such as the nature and purpose of the study, and quality of the dataset.  

      (6) The package was developed in R3.6.3. This is several years and one major version behind the current R version (4.4.3). Have the authors tested if this package runs on modern R versions? If not, this could be a significant hurdle for potential users. 

      We thank reviewers for pointing out concerns regarding versioning. We have since updated our installation approach for SMARTTR, which is compatible with versions of R >= 3.6 and has been tested on Mac ARM-based (Apple silicon) architecture (R v4.4.2), and Windows 10 (R v3.6.3, v4.5.0 [devel]). 

      The recommendation for users to install R 3.6.3 is primarily for those interested in using our full workflow, which requires installation of the WholeBrain package, which is currently a suggested package. We anticipate updating and supporting the visualization and network analysis capabilities, whilst maintaining previous versioning for the full workflow presented in this paper.  

      (7) In the methods section: "Networks were constructed using igraph and tidygraph packages." - As this is a core functionality of the package, it would be informative to specify the exact package versions, functions, and parameters for network construction. 

      We thank reviewers for pointing out the necessity for these details for code reproducibility. We have since clarified our language in the manuscript on the exact functions we use in our analysis and package versions, which we also fully document in our online tutorial. Additionally. We have printed our package development and analysis environment online at https://mjin1812.github.io/SMARTTR/articles/Part7.Development.

      (8) On page 11, "Next, we examined the cross-correlations in IEG expression across brain regions, as strong co-activation or opposing activation can signify functional connectivity between two regions" - cross-correlation is a specific analysis in signal processing. To avoid confusion, the authors should simply change this to "correlations". 

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this potentially confusing phrasing. We have changed all instances of “cross-correlation” to “correlation”.

      (9) Panels Q-V are missing in Figure 5 caption. 

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this oversight. We have now fixed this in our revision.

      References

      Chon, U., Vanselow, D. J., Cheng, K. C., & Kim, Y. (2019). Enhanced and unified anatomical labeling for a common mouse brain atlas. Nature Communications, 10(1), 5067. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-019-13057-w

      Dong, H. W. (2008). The Allen reference atlas: A digital color brain atlas of the C57Bl/6J male mouse (pp. ix, 366). John Wiley & Sons Inc.

      Fürth, D., Vaissière, T., Tzortzi, O., Xuan, Y., Märtin, A., Lazaridis, I., Spigolon, G., Fisone, G., Tomer, R., Deisseroth, K., Carlén, M., Miller, C. A., Rumbaugh, G., & Meletis, K. (2018). An interactive framework for whole-brain maps at cellular resolution. Nature Neuroscience, 21(1), 139–149. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-017-0027-7

      Yates, S. C., Groeneboom, N. E., Coello, C., Lichtenthaler, S. F., Kuhn, P.-H., Demuth, H.-U., Hartlage-Rübsamen, M., Roßner, S., Leergaard, T., Kreshuk, A., Puchades, M. A., & Bjaalie, J. G. (2019). QUINT: Workflow for Quantification and Spatial Analysis of Features in Histological Images From Rodent Brain. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 13. https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fninf.2019.00075

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):  

      Summary:  

      This study provides new insights into the role of miR-19b, an oncogenic microRNA, in the developing chicken pallium. Dynamic expression pattern of miR-19b is associated with its role in regulating cell cycle progression in neural progenitor cells. Furthermore, miR-19b is involved in determining neuronal subtypes by regulating Fezf2 expression during pallial development. These findings suggest an important role for miR-19b in the coordinated spatio-temporal regulation of neural progenitor cell dynamics and its evolutionary conservation across vertebrate species.  

      Strengths:  

      The authors identified conserved roles of miR-19 in the regulation of neural progenitor maintenance between mouse and chick, and the latter is mediated by the repression of E2f8 and NeuroD1. Furthermore, the authors found that miR-19b-dependent cell cycle regulation is tightly associated with specification of Fezf1 or Mef2c-positive neurons, in spatio-temporal manners during chicken pallial development. These findings uncovered molecular mechanisms underlying microRNA-mediated neurogenic controls.  

      Weaknesses:  

      Although the authors in this study claimed striking similarities of miR-19a/b in neurogenesis between mouse and chick pallium, a previous study by Bian et al. revealed that miR-19a contributes the expansion of radial glial cells by suppressing PTEN expression in the developing mouse neocortex, while miR-19b maintains apical progenitors via inhibiting E2f2 and NeuroD1 in chicken pallium. Thus, it is still unclear whether the orthologous microRNAs regulate common or species-specific target genes.  

      In this study, we have proposed that miR-19b regulates similar phenomena in both species using different targets, such as regulation of proliferation through PTEN in mouse and through E2f8 in the chicken.

      The spatiotemporal expression patterns of miR-19b and several genes are not convincing. For example, the authors claim that NeuroD1 is initially expressed uniformly in the subventricular zone (SVZ) but disappears in the DVR region by HH29 and becomes detectable by HH35 (Figure 1). However, the in situ hybridization data revealed that NeuroD1 is highly expressed in the SVZ of the DVR at HH29 (Figure 4F). Thus, perhaps due to the problem of immunohistochemistry, the authors have not been able to detect NeuroD1 expression in Figure 1D, and the interpretation of the data may require significant modification.  

      While Fig. 1B may suggest that NeuroD1 expression has disappeared from the DVR region by HH29, this is not true in general because we have observed NeuroD1 to be expressed in the DVR at HH29 in images of other sections. In the revised version, we will include improved images for panels of Fig. 1B which accurately show the expression pattern of NeuroD1 and miR19b at stages HH29 and HH35.  

      It seems that miR-19b is also expressed in neurons (Figure 1), suggesting the role of miR19-b must be different in progenitors and differentiated neurons. The data on the gain- and loss-offunction analysis of miR-19b on the expression of Mef2c should be carefully considered, as it is possible that these experiments disturb the neuronal functions of miR19b rather than in the progenitors.

      As pointed out by the reviewer, it is quite possible that upon manipulation of miR19b its neuronal functions are also perturbed in addition to its function in progenitor cells. After introducing gain-of-function construct in progenitor cells, we have observed changes in the morphology of these cells. These data will be included in the revised version.

      The regions of chicken pallium were not consistent among figures: in Figure 1, they showed caudal parts of the pallium (HH29 and 35), while the data in Figure 4 corresponded to the rostral part of the pallium (Figure 4B).  

      We will address this by providing images from a similar region of the pallium showing Fezf2 and Mef2c expression patterns.

      The neurons expressing Fezf2 and Mef2 in the chicken pallium are not homologous neuronal subtypes to mammalian deep and superficial cortical neurons. The authors must understand that chicken pallial development proceeds in an outside-in manner. Thus, Mef2c-postive neurons in a superficial part are early-born neurons, while FezF2-positive neurons residing in deep areas are later-born neurons. It should be noted that the expression of a single marker gene does not support cell type homology, and the authors' description "the possibility of primitive pallial lamina formation in common ancestors of birds and mammals" is misleading.  

      We appreciate this clarification and will modify or remove this statement regarding the “primitive pallial lamina formation” to avoid any confusion and misinterpretation. 

      Overexpression of CDKN1A or Sponge-19b induced ectopic expression of Fezf2 in the ventricular zone (Figure 3C, E). Do these cells maintain progenitor statement or prematurely differentiate to neurons? In addition, the authors must explain that the induction of Fezf2 is also detected in GFP-negative cells.  

      We propose to follow up on the fate of these cells by extending the observation period post-overexpression of CDKN1A or Sponge-19b to assess whether they retain progenitor characteristics or differentiate. The presence of Fezf2 in GFP-negative cells could be due to the non-cell-autonomous effects, and we will discuss this possibility in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):  

      Summary:  

      This paper investigates the general concept that avian and mammalian pallium specifications share similar mechanisms. To explore that idea, the authors focus their attention on the role of miR-19b as a key controlling factor in the neuronal proliferation/differentiation balance. To do so, the authors checked the expression and protein level of several genes involved in neuronal differentiation, such as NeuroD1 or E2f8, genes also expressed in mammals after conducting their functional gene manipulation experiments. The work also shows a dysregulation in the number of neurons from lower and upper layers when miR-19b expression is altered.  

      To test it, the authors conducted a series of functional experiments of gain and loss of function (G&LoF) and enhancer-reporter assays. The enhancer-reporter assays demonstrate a direct relationship between miR-19b and NeuroD1 and E2f8 which is also validated by the G&LoF experiments. It´s also noteworthy to mention that the way miR-19b acts is maintaining the progenitor cells from the ventricular zone in an undifferentiated stage, thus promoting them into a stage of cellular division.  

      Overall, the paper argues that the expression of miR-19b in the ventricular zone promotes the cells in a proliferative phase and inhibits the expression of differentiation genes such as E2f8 and NeurD1. The authors claim that a decrease in the progenitor cell pool leads to an increase and decrease in neurons in the lower and upper layers, respectively.  

      Strengths:  

      (1) Novelty Contribution  

      The paper offers strong arguments to prove that the neurodevelopmental basis between mammals and birds is quite the same. Moreover, this work contributes to a better understanding of brain evolution along the animal evolutionary tree and will give us a clearer idea about the roots of how our brain has been developed. This stands in contrast to the conventional framing of mammal brain development as an independent subject unlinked to the "less evolved species". The authors also nicely show a concept that was previously restricted to mammals - the role of microRNAs in development.  

      (2) Right experimental approach  

      The authors perform a set of functional experiments correctly adjusted to answer the role of miR-19b in the control of neuronal stem cell proliferation and differentiation. Their histological, functional, and genetic approach gives us a clear idea about the relations between several genes involved in the differentiation of the neurons in the avian pallium. In this idea, they maintain the role of miR-19b as a hub controller, keeping the ventricular zone cells in an undifferentiated stage to perpetuate the cellular pool.  

      (3) Future directions  

      The findings open a door to future experiments, particularly to a better comprehension of the role of microRNAs and pallidal genetic connections. Furthermore, this work also proves the use of avians as a model to study cortical development due to the similarities with mammals.  

      Weaknesses:  

      While there are questions answered, there are still several that remain unsolved. The experiments analyzed here lead us to speculate that the early differentiation of the progenitor cells from the ventricular zone entails a reduction in the cellular pool, affecting thereafter the number of latter-born neurons (upper layers). The authors should explore that option by testing progenitor cell markers in the ventricular zone, such as Pax6. Even so, it remains possible that miR-19b is also changing the expression pattern of neurons that are going to populate the different layers, instead of their numbers, so the authors cannot rule that out or verify it. Since the paper focuses on the role of miR-19b in patterning, I think the authors should check the relationship and expression between progenitors (Pax6) and intermediate (Tbr2) cells when miR-19b is affected. Since neuronal expression markers change so fast within a few days (HH24HH35), I don't understand why the authors stop the functional experiments at different time points.  

      To address this, we will examine the expression of Pax6 and Tbr2 following both gain-of-function and loss-of-function manipulations of miR-19b. We agree with the reviewer that miR-19b may influence not only the number of neurons but also the expression pattern of neuronal markers.  Due to the limitations of our experimental design, we acknowledge that this possibility cannot be ruled out. 

      Regarding time points chosen for the functional experiments: We selected different stages based on the expression dynamics of specific markers. To detect possible ectopic induction, we analyzed developmental stages where the expression of a given marker is normally absent. Conversely, to detect loss of expression we examined stages in which the marker is typically expressed robustly. This approach allowed us to better interpret the functional consequences of miR-19b manipulation within relevant developmental windows. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):  

      Summary:  

      This is a timely article that focuses on the molecular machinery in charge of the proliferation of pallial neural stem cells in chicks, and aims to compare them to what is known in mammals. miR19b is related to controlling the expression of E2f8 and NeuroD1, and this leads to a proper balance of division/differentiation, required for the generation of the right number of neurons and their subtype proportions. In my opinion, many experiments do reflect an interaction between all these genes and transcription factors, which likely supports the role of miR19b in participating in the proliferation/differentiation balance.  

      Strengths:  

      Most of the methodologies employed are suitable for the research question, and present data to support their conclusions.  

      The authors were creative in their experimental design, in order to assess several aspects of pallial development.  

      Weaknesses:  

      However, there are several important issues that I think need to be addressed or clarified in order to provide a clearer main message for the article, as well as to clarify the tools employed. I consider it utterly important to review and reinterpret most of the anatomical concepts presented here. The way the are currently used is confusing and may mislead readers towards an understanding of the bird pallium that is no longer accepted by the community.  

      Major Concerns:  

      (1) Inaccurate use of neuroanatomy throughout the entire article. There are several aspects to it, that I will try to explain in the following paragraphs:  

      Figure 1 shows a dynamic and variable expression pattern of miR19b and its relation to NeuroD1. Regardless of the terms used in this figure, it shows that miR19b may be acting differently in various parts of the pallium and developmental stages. However, all the rest of the experiments in the article (except a few cases) abolish these anatomical differences. It is not clear, but it is very important, where in the pallium the experiments are performed. I refer here, at least, to Figures 2C, E, F, H, I; 3D, E; 4C, D, G, I. Regarding time, all experiments were done at HH22, and the article does not show the native expression at this stage. The sacrifice timing is variable, and this variability is not always justified. But more importantly, we don't know where those images were taken, or what part of the pallium is represented in the images. Is it always the same? Do results reflect differences between DVR and Wulst gene expression modifications? The authors should include low magnification images of the regions where experiments were performed. And they should consider the variable expression of all genes when interpreting results.  

      We agree that precise anatomical context is essential. In the revised version, we propose to: 

      a) Include schematics of the regions of interest where experimental manipulations were performed.

      b) Provide low-magnification panoramic images where appropriate, for anatomical reference.

      c) Show the expression patterns of relevant marker genes to better justify stages and region selection. 

      d) Provide the expression pattern of markers in panoramic view to show differential expression in the DVR and Wulst region and interpret our results accordingly.

      b) SVZ is not a postmitotic zone (as stated in line 123, and wrongly assigned throughout the text and figures). On the contrary, the SVZ is a secondary proliferative zone, organized in a layer, located in a basal position to the VZ. Both (VZ and SVZ) are germinative zones, containing mostly progenitors. The only postmitotic neurons in VZ and SVZ occupy them transiently when moving to the mantle zone, which is closer to the meninges and is the postmitotic territory. Please refer to the original Boulder committee articles to revise the SVZ definition. The authors, however, misinterpret this concept, and label the whole mantle zone as it this would be the SVZ. Indeed, the term "mantle zone" does not appear in the article. Please, revise and change the whole text and figures, as SVZ statements and photographs are nearly always misinterpreted. Indeed, SVZ is only labelled well in Figure 4F.  

      The two articles mentioning the expression of NeuroD1 in the SVZ (line 118) are research in Xenopus. Is there a proliferative SVZ in Xenopus?  

      For the actual existence of the SVZ in the chick pallium, please refer to the recent Rueda-Alaña et al., 2025 article that presents PH3 stainings at different timepoints and pallial areas.  

      We appreciate the correction suggested by the reviewer. In the revised manuscript: a) SVZ will be labeled correctly in all figures and descriptions b) The mantle zone terminology will be incorporated appropriately c) The two Xenopus-based references in line 118 will be removed as they are not directly relevant and d) We will refer to the Rueda-Alaña et al., (2025) to guide accurate anatomical labeling and interpretation of proliferative zones.

      We also acknowledge that while some proliferative cells exist in the SVZ of the chicken, they are relatively few and do not express typical basal progenitor markers such as Tbr2 (Nomura et al., 2016, Development). We will ensure that this nuance is clearly reflected in the text. 

      What is the Wulst, according to the authors of the article? In many figures, the Wulst includes the medial pallium and hippocampus, whereas sometimes it is used as a synonym of the hyperpallium (which excludes the medial pallium and hippocampus). Please make it clear, as the addition or not of the hippocampus definitely changes some interpretations.  

      We propose to modify the text and figures to accurately represent the correct location of the Wulst in the chick pallium.

      d) The authors compare the entirety of the chick pallium - including the hippocampus (see above), hyperpallium, mesopallium, nidopallium - to only the neocortex of mammals. This view - as shown in Suzuki et al., 2012 - forgets the specificity of pallial areas of the pallium and compares it to cortical cells. This is conceptually wrong, and leads to incorrect interpretations (please refer to Luis Puelles' commentaries on Suzuki et al results); there are incorrect conclusions about the existence of upper-layer-like and deep-layer-like neurons in the pallium of birds. The view is not only wrong according to the misinterpreted anatomical comparisons, but also according to novel scRNAseq data (Rueda-Alaña et al., 2025; Zaremba et al., 2025; Hecker et al., 2025). These articles show that many avian glutamatergic neurons of the pallium have highly diversified, and are not comparable to mammalian cortical cells. The authors should therefore avoid this incorrect use of terminology. There are not such upper-layer-like and deeplayer-like neurons in the pallium of birds.  

      We acknowledge this conceptual oversight. In the manuscript: a) We will avoid direct comparisons between the entire chick pallium and the mammalian neocortex b) Terms like “upper-layer-like” and deep-layer-like” neurons will be removed or modified d) We will cite and integrate recent findings from Rueda-Alaña et al. (2025), Zaremba et al. (2025), and Hecker et al. (2025), which provide updated insights from scRNAseq analyses into the complexity of avian pallial neurons. Cell types will be described based on marker gene expression only, without unsupported evolutionary or homology claims.

      (2) From introduction to discussion, the article uses misleading terms and outdated concepts of cell type homology and similarity between chick and pallial territories and cells. The authors must avoid this confusing terminology, as non-expert readers will come to evolutionary conclusions which are not supported by the data in this article; indeed, the article does not deal with those concepts.  

      We agree with the reviewer. In the revised version, we will remove the misleading terms and outdated concepts and avoid speculative evolutionary conclusions.  

      a) Recent articles published in Science (Rueda-Alaña et al., 2025; Zaremba et al., 2025; Hecker et al., 2025) directly contradict some views presented in this article. These articles should be presented in the introduction as they are utterly important for the subject of this article and their results should be discussed in the light of the new findings of this article. Accordingly, the authors should avoid claiming any homology that is not currently supported. The expression of a single gene is not enough anymore to claim the homology of neuronal populations.  

      In the revised version, these above-mentioned articles (Rueda-Alaña et al., 2025; Zaremba et al., 2025; Hecker et al., 2025) will be included in the introduction and discussion.  Our interpretations will be updated to reflect these new insights into neuronal diversity and regionalization in the chick pallium. 

      Auditory cortex is not an appropriate term, as there is no cortex in the pallium of birds. Cortical areas require the existence of neuronal arrangements in laminae that appear parallel to the ventricular surface. It is not the case of either hyperpallium or auditory DVR. The accepted term, according to the Avian Nomenclature forum, is Field L.  

      We will replace all instances of “auditory cortex” with “Field L”, as per the accepted terminology in the Avian Nomenclature Forum.

      c) Forebrain, a term overused in the article, is very unspecific. It includes vast areas of the brain, from the pretectum and thalamus to the olfactory bulb. However, the authors are not researching most of the forebrain here. They should be more specific throughout the text and title.  

      In the revised version, we will replace “forebrain” with “Pallium” throughout the manuscript to more accurately reflect the regions studied.

      (3) In the last part of the results, the authors claim miR19b has a role in patterning the avian pallium. What they see is that modifying its expression induces changes in gene expression in certain neurons. Accordingly, the altered neurons would differentiate into other subtypes, not similar to the wild type example. In this sense, miR19b may have a role in cell specification or neuronal differentiation. However, patterning is a different developmental event, which refers to the determination of broad genetic areas and territories. I don't think miR19b has a role in patterning.  

      We agree with the reviewers that an alteration in one marker for a particular cell type may not indicate a change in patterning. However, including the effect of miR-19b gain- and loss-of-function on Pax6 and Tbr2, may strengthen the idea that it affects patterning as suggested by reviewer #2. 

      (4) Please add a scheme of the molecules described in this article and the suggested interaction between them.  

      In the revised version, we propose to include a diagram to visually summarize the proposed interactions between miR-19b, E2f8, NeuroD1, and other key regulators.  

      (5) The methods section is way too brief to allow for repeatability of the procedures. This may be due to an editorial policy but if possible, please extend the details of the experimental procedures.  

      We will expand the Methods section to provide more detailed protocols and justifications for experimental design, in alignment with journal policy.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors aim to understand the neural basis of implicit causal inference, specifically how people infer causes of illness. They use fMRI to explore whether these inferences rely on content-specific semantic networks or broader, domain-general neurocognitive mechanisms. The study explores two key hypotheses: first, that causal inferences about illness rely on semantic networks specific to living things, such as the 'animacy network,' given that illnesses affect only animate beings; and second, that there might be a common brain network supporting causal inferences across various domains, including illness, mental states, and mechanical failures. By examining these hypotheses, the authors aim to determine whether causal inferences are supported by specialized or generalized neural systems.

      The authors observed that inferring illness causes selectively engaged a portion of the precuneus (PC) associated with the semantic representation of animate entities, such as people and animals. They found no cortical areas that responded to causal inferences across different domains, including illness and mechanical failures. Based on these findings, the authors concluded that implicit causal inferences are supported by content-specific semantic networks, rather than a domain-general neural system, indicating that the neural basis of causal inference is closely tied to the semantic representation of the specific content involved.

      Strengths:

      (1) The inclusion of the four conditions in the design is well thought out, allowing for the examination of the unique contribution of causal inference of illness compared to either a different type of causal inference (mechanical) or non-causal conditions. This design also has the potential to identify regions involved in a shared representation of inference across general domains.

      (2) The presence of the three localizers for language, logic, and mentalizing, along with the selection of specific regions of interest (ROIs), such as the precuneus and anterior ventral occipitotemporal cortex (antVOTC), is a strong feature that supports a hypothesis-driven approach (although see below for a critical point related to the ROI selection).

      (3) The univariate analysis pipeline is solid and well-developed.

      (4) The statistical analyses are a particularly strong aspect of the paper.

      Weaknesses:

      Based on the current analyses, it is not yet possible to rule out the hypothesis that inferring illness causes relies on neurocognitive mechanisms that support causal inferences irrespective of their content, neither in the precuneus nor in other parts of the brain.

      (1) The authors, particularly in the multivariate analyses, do not thoroughly examine the similarity between the two conditions (illness-causal and mechanical-causal), as they are more focused on highlighting the differences between them. For instance, in the searchlight MVPA analysis, an interesting decoding analysis is conducted to identify brain regions that represent illness-causal and mechanical-causal conditions differently, yielding results consistent with the univariate analyses. However, to test for the presence of a shared network, the authors only perform the Causal vs. Non-causal analysis. This analysis is not very informative because it includes all conditions mixed together and does not clarify whether both the illness-causal and mechanical-causal conditions contribute to these results.

      (2) To address this limitation, a useful additional step would be to use as ROIs the different regions that emerged in the Causal vs. Non-causal decoding analysis and to conduct four separate decoding analyses within these specific clusters:

      (a) Illness-Causal vs. Non-causal - Illness First;

      (b) Illness-Causal vs. Non-causal - Mechanical First;

      (c) Mechanical-Causal vs. Non-causal - Illness First;

      (d) Mechanical-Causal vs. Non-causal - Mechanical First.

      This approach would allow the authors to determine whether any of these ROIs can decode both the illness-causal and mechanical-causal conditions against at least one non-causal condition.

      (3) Another possible analysis to investigate the existence of a shared network would be to run the searchlight analysis for the mechanical-causal condition versus the two non-causal conditions, as was done for the illness-causal versus non-causal conditions, and then examine the conjunction between the two. Specifically, the goal would be to identify ROIs that show significant decoding accuracy in both analyses.

      The hypothesis that a neural mechanism supports causal inference across domains predicts higher univariate responses when causal inferences occur than when they do not. This prediction was not generated by us ad hoc but rather has been made by almost all previous cognitive neuroscience papers on this topic (Ferstl & von Cramon, 2001; Satpute et al., 2005; Fugelsang & Dunbar, 2005; Kuperberg et al., 2006; Fenker et al., 2010; Kranjec et al., 2012; Pramod, Chomik-Morales, et al., 2023; Chow et al., 2008; Mason & Just, 2011; Prat et al., 2011). Contrary to this hypothesis, we find that the precuneus (PC) is most activated for illness inferences and most deactivated for mechanical inferences relative to rest, suggesting that the PC does not support domain-general causal inference. To further probe the selectivity of the PC for illness inferences, we created group overlap maps that compare PC responses to illness inferences and mechanical inferences across participants. The PC shows a strong preference for illness inferences and is therefore unlikely to support causal inferences irrespective of their content (Supplementary Figures 6 and 7). We also note that, in whole-cortex analysis, no shared regions responded more to causal inference than noncausal vignettes across domains. Therefore, the prediction made by the ‘domain-general causal engine’ proposal as it has been articulated in the literature is not supported in our data.

      Taking a multivariate approach, the hypothesis that a neural mechanism supports causal inference across domains also predicts that relevant regions can decode between all possible pairs of causal vs. noncausal conditions (e.g., Illness-Causal vs. Noncausal-Illness First, Mechanical-Causal vs. Noncausal-Illness First, etc.). The analysis described by the reviewer in (2), in which the regions that distinguish between causal vs. noncausal conditions in searchlight MVPA are used as ROIs to test various causal vs. noncausal contrasts, is non-independent. Therefore, we cannot perform this analysis. In accordance with the reviewer’s suggestions in (3), now include searchlight MVPA results for the mechanical inference condition compared to the two noncausal conditions (Supplementary Figure 9). No regions are shared across the searchlight analyses comparing all possible pairs of causal and noncausal conditions, providing further evidence that there are no shared neural responses to causal inference in our dataset.

      (4) Along the same lines, for the ROI MVPA analysis, it would be useful not only to include the illness-causal vs. mechanical-causal decoding but also to examine the illness-causal vs. non-causal conditions and the mechanical-causal vs. non-causal conditions. Additionally, it would be beneficial to report these data not just in a table (where only the mean accuracy is shown) but also using dot plots, allowing the readers to see not only the mean values but also the accuracy for each individual subject.

      We have performed these analyses and now include a table of the results as well as figures displaying the dispersion across participants (Supplementary Tables 2 and 3, Supplementary Figures 10 and 11). In the left PC, the illness inference condition was decoded from one of the noncausal conditions, and the mechanical inference condition was decoded from the same noncausal condition. The language network did not decode between any causal/noncausal pairs. In the logic network, the illness inference condition was decoded from one of the noncausal conditions, and the mechanical inference condition was decoded from the other noncausal condition. Thus, no regions showed the predicted ‘domain-general’ pattern, i.e., significant decoding between all causal/noncausal pairs. 

      Importantly, the decoding results must be interpreted in light of significant univariate differences across conditions (e.g., greater responses to illness inferences compared to noncausal vignettes in the PC). Linear classifiers are highly sensitive to univariate differences (Coutanche, 2013; Kragel et al., 2012; Hebart & Baker, 2018; Woolgar et al., 2014; Davis et al., 2014; Pakravan et al., 2022).

      (5) The selection of Regions of Interest (ROIs) is not entirely straightforward:

      In the introduction, the authors mention that recent literature identifies the precuneus (PC) as a region that responds preferentially to images and words related to living things across various tasks. While this may be accurate, we can all agree that other regions within the ventral occipital-temporal cortex also exhibit such preferences, particularly areas like the fusiform face area, the occipital face area, and the extrastriate body area. I believe that at least some parts of this network (e.g., the fusiform gyrus) should be included as ROIs in this study. This inclusion would make sense, especially because a complementary portion of the ventral stream known to prefer non-living items (i.e., anterior medial VOTC) has been selected as a control ROI to process information about the mechanical-causal condition. Given the main hypothesis of the study - that causal inferences about illness might depend on content-specific semantic representations in the 'animacy network' - it would be worthwhile to investigate these ROIs alongside the precuneus, as they may also yield interesting results.

      We thank the reviewer for their suggestion to test the FFA region. We think this provides an interesting comparison to the PC and hypothesized that, in contrast to the PC, the FFA does not encode abstract causal information about animacy-specific processes (i.e., illness). As we mention in the Introduction, although the fusiform face area (FFA) also exhibits a preference for animates, it does so primarily for images in sighted people (Kanwisher et al., 1997; Kanwisher et al., 1997; Grill-Spector et al., 2004; Noppeney et al., 2006; Konkle & Caramazza, 2013; Connolly et al., 2016; Bi et al., 2016).

      We did not select the FFA as a region of interest when preregistering the current study because we did not predict it would show sensitivity to causal knowledge. In accordance with the reviewer’s suggestions, we now include the FFA as an ROI in individual-subject univariate analysis (Supplementary Figure 8, Appendix 4). Because we did not run a separate FFA localizer task when collecting the data, we used FFA search spaces from a previous study investigating responses to face images (Julian et al., 2012). We followed the same analysis procedure that was used to investigate responses to illness inferences in the PC. Neither left nor right FFA exhibited a preference for illness inferences compared to mechanical inferences or to the noncausal conditions. This result is interesting and is now briefly discussed in the Discussion section.

      (6) Visual representation of results:

      In all the figures related to ROI analyses, only mean group values are reported (e.g., Figure 1A, Figure 3, Figure 4A, Supplementary Figure 6, Figure 7, Figure 8). To better capture the complexity of fMRI data and provide readers with a more comprehensive view of the results, it would be beneficial to include a dot plot for a specific time point in each graph. This could be a fixed time point (e.g., a certain number of seconds after stimulus presentation) or the time point showing the maximum difference between the conditions of interest. Adding this would allow for a clearer understanding of how the effect is distributed across the full sample, such as whether it is consistently present in every subject or if there is greater variability across individuals.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We now include scattered box plots displaying the dispersion in average percent signal change across participants in Figures 1, 3, and 4, and Supplementary Figures 8, 12, and 14.

      (7) Task selection:

      (a) To improve the clarity of the paper, it would be helpful to explain the rationale behind the choice of the selected task, specifically addressing: (i) why an implicit inference task was chosen instead of an explicit inference task, and (ii) why the "magic detection" task was used, as it might shift participants' attention more towards coherence, surprise, or unexpected elements rather than the inference process itself.

      (b) Additionally, the choice to include a large number of catch trials is unusual, especially since they are modeled as regressors of non-interest in the GLM. It would be beneficial to provide an explanation for this decision.

      We chose an orthogonal foil detection task, rather than an explicit causal judgment task, to investigate automatic causal inferences during reading and to unconfound such processing as much as possible from explicit decision-making processes (see Kuperberg et al., 2006 for discussion). Analogous foil detection paradigms have been used to study sentence processing and word recognition (Pallier et al., 2011; Dehaene-Lambertz et al., 2018). We now clarify this in the Introduction. The “magical” element occurred both within and across sentences so that participants could not use coherence as a cue to complete the task. Approximately 1/5 (19%) of the trials were magical catch trials to ensure that participants remained attentive throughout the experiment.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors hypothesize that "causal inferences about illness depend on content-specific semantic representations in the animacy network". They test this hypothesis in an fMRI task, by comparing brain activity elicited by participants' exposure to written situations suggesting a plausible cause of illness with brain activity in linguistically equivalent situations suggesting a plausible cause of mechanical failure or damage and non-causal situations. These contrasts identify PC as the main "culprit" in a whole-brain univariate analysis. Then the question arises of whether the content-specificity has to do with inferences about animates in general, or if there are some distinctions between reasoning about people's bodies versus mental states. To answer this question, the authors localize the mentalizing network and study the relation between brain activity elicited by Illness-Causal > Mech-Causal and Mentalizing > Physical stories. They conclude that inferring about the causes of illness partially differentiates from reasoning about people's states of mind. The authors finally test the alternative yet non-mutually exclusive hypothesis that both types of causal inferences (illness and mechanical) depend on shared neural machinery. Good candidates are language and logic, which justifies the use of a language/logic localizer. No evidence of commonalities across causal inferences versus non-causal situations is found.

      Strengths:

      (1) This study introduces a useful paradigm and well-designed set of stimuli to test for implicit causal inferences.

      (2) Another important methodological advance is the addition of physical stories to the original mentalizing protocol.

      (3) With these tools, or a variant of these tools, this study has the potential to pave the way for further investigation of naïve biology and causal inference.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) This study is missing a big-picture question. It is not clear whether the authors investigate the neural correlates of causal reasoning or of naïve biology. If the former, the choice of an orthogonal task, making causal reasoning implicit, is questionable. If the latter, the choice of mechanical and physical controls can be seen as reductive and problematic.

      We have modified the Introduction to clarify that the primary goal of the current study is to test the claim that semantic networks encode causal knowledge – in this case, causal intuitive theories of biology. Most conceptions of intuitive biology, intuitive psychology, and intuitive physics describe them as causal frameworks (e.g., Wellman & Gelman, 1992; Simons & Keil, 1995; Keil et al., 1999; Tenenbaum, Griffiths, & Niyogi, 2007; Gopnik & Wellman, 2012; Gerstenberg & Tenenbaum, 2017). As noted above, we chose an implicit task to investigate automatic causal inferences during reading and to unconfound such processing as much as possible from explicit decision-making processes. We are not sure what the reviewer means when they say that mechanical and physical controls are reductive. This is the standard control condition in neural and behavioral paradigms that investigate intuitive psychology and intuitive biology (e.g., Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Gelman & Wellman, 1991).

      (2) The rationale for focusing mostly on the precuneus is not clear and this choice could almost be seen as a post-hoc hypothesis.

      This study is preregistered (https://osf.io/6pnqg). The preregistration states that the precuneus is a hypothesized area of interest, so this is not a post-hoc hypothesis. Our hypothesis was informed by multiple prior studies implicating the precuneus in the semantic representation of animates (e.g., people, animals) (Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013a, 2013b; Fairhall et al., 2014; Peer et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2016; Silson et al., 2019; Rabini, Ubaldi, & Fairhall, 2021; Deen & Freiwald, 2022; Aglinskas & Fairhall, 2023; Hauptman, Elli, et al., 2025). We also conducted a pilot experiment with separate participants prior to pre-registering the study. We now clarify our rationale for focusing on the precuneus in the Introduction:

      “Illness affects living things (e.g., people and animals) rather than inanimate objects (e.g., rocks, machines, houses). Thinking about living things (animates) as opposed to non-living things (inanimate objects/places) recruits partially distinct neural systems (e.g., Warrington & Shallice, 1984; Hillis & Caramazza, 1991; Caramazza & Shelton, 1998; Farah & Rabinowitz, 2003). The precuneus (PC) is part of the ‘animacy’ semantic network and responds preferentially to living things (i.e., people and animals), whether presented as images or words (Devlin et al., 2002; Fairhall & Caramazza, 2013a, 2013b; Fairhall et al., 2014; Peer et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2016; Silson et al., 2019; Rabini, Ubaldi, & Fairhall, 2021; Deen & Freiwald, 2022; Aglinskas & Fairhall, 2023; Hauptman, Elli, et al., 2025). By contrast, parts of the visual system (e.g., fusiform face area) that respond preferentially to animates do so primarily for images (Kanwisher et al., 1997; Grill-Spector et al., 2004; Noppeney et al., 2006; Mahon et al., 2009; Konkle & Caramazza, 2013; Connolly et al., 2016; see Bi et al., 2016 for a review). We hypothesized that the PC represents causal knowledge relevant to animates and tested the prediction that it would be activated during implicit causal inferences about illness, which rely on such knowledge (preregistration: https://osf.io/6pnqg).”

      (3) The choice of an orthogonal 'magic detection' task has three problematic consequences in this study:

      (a) It differs in nature from the 'mentalizing' task that consists of evaluating a character's beliefs explicitly from the corresponding story, which complicates the study of the relation between both tasks. While the authors do not compare both tasks directly, it is unclear to what extent this intrinsic difference between implicit versus explicit judgments of people's body versus mental states could influence the results.

      (b) The extent to which the failure to find shared neural machinery between both types of inferences (illness and mechanical) can be attributed to the implicit character of the task is not clear.

      (c) The introduction of a category of non-interest that contains only 36 trials compared to 38 trials for all four categories of interest creates a design imbalance.

      We disagree with the reviewer’s argument that our use of an implicit “magic detection” task is problematic. Indeed, we think it is one of the advances of the current study over prior work.

      a) Prior work has shown that implicit mentalizing tasks (e.g., naturalistic movie watching) engages the theory of mind network, suggesting that the implicit/explicit nature of the task does not drive the activation of this network (Jacoby et al., 2016; Richardson et al., 2018). With these data in mind, it is unlikely that the implicit/explicit nature of the causal inference and theory of mind tasks in the present experiment can explain observed differences between them.

      b) Explicit causal inferences introduce a collection of executive processes that potentially confound the results and make it difficult to know whether neural signatures are related to causal inference per se. The current study focuses on the neural basis of implicit causal inference, a type of inference that is made routinely during language comprehension. We do not claim to find neural signatures of all causal inferences, we do not think any study could claim to do so because causal inferences are a highly varied class.

      c) Our findings do not exclude the possibility that content-invariant responses are elicited during explicit causality judgments. We clarify this point in the Results (e.g., “These results leave open the possibility that domain-general systems support the explicit search for causal connections”) and Discussion (e.g., “The discovery of novel causal relationships (e.g., ‘blicket detectors’; Gopnik et al., 2001) and the identification of complex causes, even in the case of illness, may depend in part on domain-general neural mechanisms”).

      d) Because the magic trials are excluded from our analyses, it is unclear how the imbalance in the number of magic trials could influence the results and our interpretation of them. We note that the number of catch trials in standard target detection paradigms are sometimes much lower than the number of target trials in each condition (e.g., Pallier et al., 2011).

      (4) Another imbalance is present in the design of this study: the number of trials per category is not the same in each run of the main task. This imbalance does not seem to be accounted for in the 1st-level GLM and renders a bit problematic the subsequent use of MVPA.

      Each condition is shown either 6 or 7 times per run (maximum difference of 1 trial between conditions), and the number of trials per condition is equal across the whole experiment: each condition is shown 7 times in two of the runs and 6 times four of the runs. This minor design imbalance is typical of fMRI experiments and should not impact our interpretations of the data, particularly because we average responses from each condition within a run before submitting them to MVPA.

      (5) The main claim of the authors, encapsulated by the title of the present manuscript, is not tested directly. While the authors included in their protocol independent localizers for mentalizing, language, and logic, they did not include an independent localizer for "animacy". As such, they cannot provide a within-subject evaluation of their claim, which is entirely based on the presence of a partial overlap in PC (which is also involved in a wide range of tasks) with previous results on animacy.

      We respectfully disagree with this assertion. Our primary analysis uses a within-subject leave-one-run-out approach. This approach allows us to use part of the data itself to localize animacy-relevant causal responses in the PC without engaging in ‘double-dipping’ or statistical non-independence (Vul & Kanwisher, 2011). We also use the mentalizing network localizer as a partial localizer for animacy. This is because the control condition (physical reasoning) does not include references to people or any animate agents (Supplementary Figures 1 and 15). We now clarify this point in Methods section of the paper (see below).

      From the Methods: “To test the relationship between neural responses to inferences about the body and the mind, and to localize animacy regions, we used a localizer task to identify the mentalizing network in each participant (Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Dodell-Feder et al., 2011; http://saxelab.mit.edu/use-our-efficient-false-belief-localizer)...Our physical stories incorporated more vivid descriptions of physical interactions and did not make any references to human agents, enabling us to use the mentalizing localizer as a localizer for animacy.”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study employed an implicit task, showing vignettes to participants while a bold signal was acquired. The aim was to capture automatic causal inferences that emerge during language processing and comprehension. In particular, the authors compared causal inferences about illness with two control conditions, causal inferences about mechanical failures and non-causal phrases related to illnesses. All phrases that were employed described contexts with people, to avoid animacy/inanimate confound in the results. The authors had a specific hypothesis concerning the role of the precuneus (PC) in being sensitive to causal inferences about illnesses.

      These findings indicate that implicit causal inferences are facilitated by semantic networks specialized for encoding causal knowledge.

      Strengths:

      The major strength of the study is the clever design of the stimuli (which are nicely matched for a number of features) which can tease apart the role of the type of causal inference (illness-causal or mechanical-causal) and the use of two localizers (logic/language and mentalizing) to investigate the hypothesis that the language and/or logical reasoning networks preferentially respond to causal inference regardless of the content domain being tested (illnesses or mechanical).

      Weaknesses:

      I have identified the following main weaknesses:

      (1) Precuneus (PC) and Temporo-Parietal junction (TPJ) show very similar patterns of results, and the manuscript is mostly focused on PC (also the abstract). To what extent does the fact that PC and TPJ show similar trends affect the inferences we can derive from the results of the paper? I wonder whether additional analyses (connectivity?) would help provide information about this network.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. While the PC shows the most robust univariate preference for illness inferences compared to both mechanical inferences and noncausal vignettes, the TPJ also shows a preference for illness inferences compared to mechanical inferences in individual-subject fROI analysis. However, as we mention in the Results section, the TPJ does not show a preference for illness inferences compared to noncausal vignettes, suggesting that the TPJ is selective for animacy but may not be as sensitive to causal knowledge about animacy-specific processes. When describing our results, we refer to the ‘animacy network’ (i.e., PC and TPJ) but also highlight that the PC exhibited the most robust responses to illness inferences (from the Results: “Inferring illness causes preferentially recruited the animacy semantic network, particularly the PC”; from the Discussion: “We find that a semantic network previously implicated in thinking about animates, particularly the precuneus (PC), is preferentially engaged when people infer causes of illness…”). We did not collect resting state data that would enable a connectivity analysis, as the reviewer suggests. This is an interesting direction for future work.

      (2) Results are mainly supported by an univariate ROI approach, and the MVPA ROI approach is performed on a subregion of one of the ROI regions (left precuneus). Results could then have a limited impact on our understanding of brain functioning.

      The original and current versions of the paper include results from multiple multivariate analyses, including whole-cortex searchlight MVPA and individual-subject fROI MVPA performed in multiple search spaces (see Supplementary Figures 10 and 11, Supplementary Tables 2 and 3).

      We note that our preregistered predictions focused primarily on univariate differences. This is because the current study investigates neural responses to inferences, and univariate increases in activity is thought to reflect the processing of such inferences. We use multivariate analyses to complement our primary univariate analyses. However, given that we observe significant univariate effects and that multivariate analyses are heavily influenced by significant univariate effects (Coutanche, 2013; Kragel et al., 2012; Hebart & Baker, 2018; Woolgar et al., 2014; Davis et al., 2014; Pakravan et al., 2022), our univariate results constitute the main findings of the paper.

      (3) In all figures: there are no measures of dispersion of the data across participants. The reader can only see aggregated (mean) data. E.g., percentage signal changes (PSC) do not report measures of dispersion of the data, nor do we have bold maps showing the overlap of the response across participants. Only in Figure 2, we see the data of 6 selected participants out of 20.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We now include graphs depicting the dispersion of the data across participants in the following figures: Figures 1, 3, and 4, and Supplementary Figures 8, 12, and 14. We have also created 2 figures that display the overlap of univariate responses across participants (Supplementary Figures 6 and 7). These figures show that there is high overlap across participants in PC responses to illness inferences but not mechanical inferences. In addition, all participants’ results from the analysis depicted in Figure 2 are included in Supplementary Figure 3. 

      (4) Sometimes acronyms are defined in the text after they appear for the first time.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We now define all acronyms before using them.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) I was unable to access the pre-registration on OSF because special permission is required.

      We apologize for this technical error. The preregistration is now publicly available: https://osf.io/6pnqg.

      (2) The length of the MRI session is quite long (around 2 hours). It is generally discouraged to have such extended data acquisition periods, as this can affect the stability and cleanliness of the data. Did you observe any effects of fatigue or attention decline in your data?

      The session was 2 hours long including 1-2 10-minute breaks. Without breaks, the scan would be approximately 1.5 hours. This is a standard length for MRI experiments. The main experiment (causal inference task) was always conducted first and lasted approximately 1 hour. Accuracy did not decrease across the 6 runs of this experiment (repeated measures ANOVA, F<sub>(5,114)</sub> = 1.35, p = .25).

      (3) The last sentence of the results states: "Although MVPA searchlight analysis identified several areas where patterns of activity distinguished between causal and non-causal vignettes, all of these regions showed a preference for non-causal vignettes in univariate analysis (Supplementary Figure 5)." This statement is not entirely accurate. As I previously pointed out, the MVPA searchlight analysis is not very informative and is difficult to interpret. However, as previously suggested, there are additional steps that could be taken to better understand and interpret these results. It is incorrect to conclude that because the brain regions identified in the MVPA analyses show a preference for non-causal vignettes in univariate analyses, the multivariate results lack value. While univariate analyses may show a preference for a specific condition, multivariate analyses can reveal more fine-grained representations of multiple conditions. For a notable example, consider the fusiform face area (FFA) that shows a clear preference for faces at the univariate level but can significantly decode other categories at the multivariate level, even when faces are not included in the analysis.

      The decoding analysis that the reviewer is suggesting for the current study would be analogous to identifying univariate differences between faces and places in the FFA and then decoding between faces and places and claiming that the FFA represents places because the decoding is significant. The decoding analyses enabled by our design are not equivalent to decoding within a condition (e.g., among face identities, among types of illness inferences), as the reviewer suggests above. It is not that such multivariate analyses “lack value” but that they recapitulate established univariate differences. Multivariate analyses are useful for revealing more fine-grained representations when i) significant univariate differences are not observed, or ii) when it is possible to decode among categories within a condition (e.g., among face identities, among types of illness inferences). We are currently collecting data that will enable us to perform within-condition decoding analyses in future work, but the design of the current study does not allow for such a comparison.

      We note that the original quotation from the manuscript has been removed because it is no longer accurate. When including participant response time as a covariate of no interest in the GLM, no regions are shared across the 4 searchlight analyses comparing causal and noncausal conditions, suggesting that there are no shared neural responses to causal inference in our dataset.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Moderating the strength of some claims made to justify the main hypothesis (e.g., "people but not machines transmit diseases to each other through physical contact").

      We changed this wording so that it now reads: “Illness affects living things (e.g., people and animals) rather than inanimate objects (e.g., rocks, machines, houses).” (Introduction)

      (2) Expanding the paragraph introducing the sub-question about inferring people's "body states" vs "mental states". In addition, given the order in which the hypotheses are introduced, and the results are presented, I would suggest switching the order of presentation of both localizers in the methods section and adding a quick reminder of the hypotheses that justify using these localizers.

      We thank the reviewer for these suggestions. In accordance their suggestions, we have expanded the paragraph Introduction that introduces the “body states” vs. “mental states” question (see below). We have also switched the order of the localizer descriptions in the Methods section and added a sentence at the start of each section describing the relevant hypotheses (see below).

      From the Introduction: “We also compared neural responses to causal inferences about the body (i.e., illness) and inferences about the mind (i.e., mental states). Both types of inferences are about animate entities, and some developmental work suggests that children use the same set of causal principles to think about bodies and minds (Carey, 1985, 1988). Other evidence suggests that by early childhood, young children have distinct causal knowledge about the body and the mind (Springer & Keil, 1991; Callanan & Oakes, 1992; Wellman & Gelman, 1992; Inagaki & Hatano, 1993; 2004; Keil, 1994; Hickling & Wellman, 2001; Medin et al., 2010). For instance, preschoolers are more likely to view illness as a consequence of biological causes, such as contagion, rather than psychological causes, such as malicious intent (Springer & Ruckel, 1992; Raman & Winer, 2004; see also Legare & Gelman, 2008). The neural relationship between inferences about bodies and minds has not been fully described. The ‘mentalizing network’, including the PC, is engaged when people reason about agents’ beliefs (Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe et al., 2006; Saxe & Powell, 2006; Dodell-Feder et al., 2011; Dufour et al., 2013). We localized this network in individual participants and measured its neuroanatomical relationship to the network activated by illness inferences.”

      From the Methods, localizer descriptions: “To test the relationship between neural responses to inferences about the body and the mind, and to localize animacy regions, we used a localizer task to identify the mentalizing network in each participant… To test for the presence of domain-general responses to causal inference in the language and logic networks (e.g., Kuperberg et al., 2006; Operskalski & Barbey, 2017), we used an additional localizer task to identify both networks in each participant.”

      (3) Adding a quick analysis of lateralization to support the corresponding claim of left lateralization of responses to causal inferences.

      In accordance with the reviewer’s suggestion, we now include hemisphere as a factor in all ANOVAs comparing univariate responses across conditions.

      From the Results: “In individual-subject fROI analysis (leave-one-run-out), we similarly found that inferring illness causes activated the PC more than inferring causes of mechanical breakdown (repeated measures ANOVA, condition (Illness-Causal, Mechanical-Causal) x hemisphere (left, right): main effect of condition, F<sub>(1,19)</sub> = 19.18, p < .001, main effect of hemisphere, F<sub>(1,19)</sub> = 0.3, p = .59, condition x hemisphere interaction, F<sub>(1,19)</sub> = 27.48, p < .001; Figure 1A). This effect was larger in the left than in the right PC (paired samples t-tests; left PC: t<sub>(19)</sub> = 5.36, p < .001, right PC: t<sub>(19)</sub> = 2.27, p = .04)…In contrast to the animacy-responsive PC, the anterior PPA showed the opposite pattern, responding more to mechanical inferences than illness inferences (leave-one-run-out individual-subject fROI analysis; repeated measures ANOVA, condition (Mechanical-Causal, Illness-Causal) x hemisphere (left, right): main effect of condition, F<sub>(1,19)</sub> = 17.93, p < .001, main effect of hemisphere, F<sub>(1,19)</sub> = 1.33, p = .26, condition x hemisphere interaction, F<sub>(1,19)</sub> = 7.8, p = .01; Figure 4A). This effect was significant only in the left anterior PPA (paired samples t-tests; left anterior PPA: t<sub>(19)</sub> = 4, p < .001, right anterior PPA: t<sub>(19)</sub> = 1.88, p = .08).”

      (4) Making public and accessible the pre-registration OSF link.

      We apologize for this technical error. The preregistration is now publicly available: https://osf.io/6pnqg.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In all figures: there are no measures of dispersion of the data across participants. The reader can only see aggregated (mean) data. E.g., percentage signal changes (PSC) do not report measures of dispersion of the data, nor do we have bold maps showing the overlap of the response across participants. Only in Figure 2, we see the data of 6 selected participants out of 20.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We now include graphs depicting the dispersion of the data across participants in the following figures: Figures 1, 3, and 4, and Supplementary Figures 8, 12, and 14. We have also created 2 figures that display the overlap of univariate responses across participants (Supplementary Figures 6 and 7). In addition, all participants’ results from the analysis depicted in Figure 2 are included in Supplementary Figure 3.

      Minor

      (1) Figure 2: Spatial dissociation between responses to illness inferences and mental state inferences in the precuneus (PC). If the analysis is the result of the MVPA, the figure should report the fact that only the left precuneus was analyzed.

      Figure 2 depicts the spatial dissociation in univariate responses to illness inferences and mental state inferences. We now clarify this in the figure legend.

      (2) VOTC and PSC acronyms are defined in the text after they appear for the first time. TPJ is never defined.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We now define all acronyms before using them.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper addresses the knowledge gap between the representation of goal direction in the central complex and how motor systems stabilize movement toward that goal. The authors focused on two descending neurons, DNa01 and 02, and showed that they play different roles in steering the fly toward a goal. They also explored the connectome data to propose a model to explain how these DNs could mediate response to lateralized sensory inputs. They finally used lateralized optogenetic activation/inactivation experiments to test the roles of these neurons in mediating turnings in freely walking flies.

      Strengths:

      The experiments are well-designed and controlled. The experiment in Figure 4 is elegant, and the authors put a lot of effort into ensuring that ATP puffs do not accidentally activate the DNs. They also have explained complex experiments well. I only have minor comments for the authors.

      We are grateful for this positive feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) I do not fully understand how the authors extracted the correlation functions from the population data in Figure 1. Since the ipsilateral DNs are anti-correlated with the contralateral ones, I expected that the average will drop to zero when they are pooled together (e.g., 1E-G). Of course, this will not be the case if all the data in Figure 1 are collected from the same brain hemisphere. It would be helpful if the authors could explain this.

      We regret that this information was not easy to find in our initial submission. As noted in the Figure 1D legend, Here and elsewhere, ipsi and contra are defined relative to the recorded DN(s). We have now added a sentence to the Results (right after we introduce Figure 1D) that also makes this point.

      (2) What constitutes the goal directions in Figures 1-3 and 8, as the authors could not use EPG activity as a proxy for goal directions? If these experiments were done in the dark, without landmarks, one would expect the fly's heading to drift randomly at times, and they would not engage the DNa01/02 for turning. Do the walking trajectories in these experiments qualify as menotactic bouts?

      Published work (Green et al., 2019) has shown that, even in the dark, flies will often walk for extended periods while holding the bump of EPG activity at a fixed location. During these epochs, the brain is essentially estimating that the fly is walking in a straight line in a fixed direction. (The fact that the fly is actually rotating a bit on the spherical treadmill is not something the fly can know, in the dark.) Thus, epochs where the EPG bump is held fixed are treated as menotactic bouts, even in darkness.

      Our results provide additional support for this interpretation. We find that, when flies are walking in darkness and holding the bump of EPG activity at a fixed location, they will make a corrective behavioral turning maneuver in response to an imposed bump-jump. This result argues that the flies are actually engaging in goal-directed straight-line walking, i.e. menotaxis, and it reproduces the findings of Green et al. (2019).

      To clarify this point, we have adjusted the wording of the Results pertaining to Figure 4.

      (3) In Figure 2B, the authors mentioned that DNa02 overpredicts and 01 underpredicts rapid turning and provided single examples. It would be nice to see more population-level quantification to support this claim.

      In this revision, we have reorganized Figures 1 and 2 (and associated text) to improve clarity. As part of this reorganization, we have removed this passage from the text, as it was a minor point in any event.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The data is largely electrophysiological recordings coupled with behavioral measurements (technically impressive) and some gain-of-function experiments in freely walking flies. Loss-of-function was tested but had minimal effect, which is not surprising in a system with partially redundant control mechanisms. The data is also consistent with/complementary to subsequent manuscripts (Yang 2023, Feng 2024, and Ros 2024) showing additional descending neurons with contributions to steering in walking and flying.

      The experiments are well executed, the results interesting, and the description clear. Some hypotheses based on connectome anatomy are tested: the insights on the pre-synaptic side - how sensory and central complex heading circuits converge onto these DNs are stronger than the suggestions about biomechanical mechanisms for how turning happens on the motor side.

      Of particular interest is the idea that different sensory cues can converge on a common motor program. The turn-toward or turn-away mechanism is initiated by valence rather than whether the stimulus was odor or temperature or memory of heading. The idea that animals choose a direction based on external sensory information and then maintain that direction as a heading through a more internal, goal-based memory mechanism, is interesting but it is hard to separate conclusively.

      To clarify, we mention the role of memory in connection with two places in the manuscript. First, we note that the EPG/head direction system relies on learning and memory to construct a map of directional cues in the environment. These cues are, in principle, inherently neutral, i.e. without valence. Second, we note that specific mushroom body output neurons rely on learning and memory to store the valence associated with an odor. This information is not necessarily associated with an allocentric direction: it is simply the association of odor with value. Both of these ideas are well-attested by previous work.

      The reviewer may be suggesting a sequential scheme whereby the brain initializes an allocentric goal direction based on valence, and then maintains that goal direction in memory, based on that initialization. In other words, memory is used to associate valence with some allocentric direction. This seems plausible, but it is not a claim we make in our manuscript.

      The "see-saw", where left-right symmetry is broken to allow a turn, presumably by excitation on one side and inhibition of the other leg motor modules, is interesting but not well explained here. How hyperpolarization affects motor outputs is not clear.

      We have added several sentences to the Discussion to clarify this point. According to this see-saw model, steering can emerge from right/left asymmetries in excitation, or inhibition, or both. It may be nonintuitive to think that inhibitory input to a DN can produce an action. However, this becomes more plausible given our finding that DNa02 has a relatively high basal firing rate (Fig. 1D), and DNa02 hyperpolarization is associated with contraversive turning (Fig. 5A). It is also relevant to note that there are many inhibitory cell types that form strong unilateral connections onto DNa02 (e.g., AOTU019).

      The statement near Figure 5B that "DNa02 activity was higher on the side ipsilateral to the attractive stimulus, but contralateral to the aversive stimulus" is really important - and only possible to see because of the dual recordings.

      We thank the reviewer for this positive feedback.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Rayshubskiy et al. performed whole-cell recordings from descending neurons (DNs) of fruit flies to characterize their role in steering. Two DNs implicated in "walking control" and "steering control" by previous studies (Namiki et al., 2018, Cande et al., 2018, Chen et al., 2018) were chosen by the authors for further characterization. In-vivo whole-cell recordings from DNa01 and DNa02 showed that their activity predicts spontaneous ipsilateral turning events. The recordings also showed that while DNa02 predicts transient turns DNa01 predicts slow sustained turns. However, optogenetic activation or inactivation showed relatively subtle phenotypes for both neurons (consistent with data in other recent preprints, Yang et al 2023 and Feng et al 2024). The authors also further characterized DNa02 with respect to its inputs and showed a functional connection with olfactory and thermosensory inputs as well as with the head-direction system. DNa01 is not characterized to this extent.

      Strengths:

      (1) In-vivo recordings and especially dual recordings are extremely challenging in Drosophila and provide a much higher resolution DN characterization than other recent studies that have relied on behavior or calcium imaging. Especially impressive are the simultaneous recordings from bilateral DNs (Figure 3). These bilateral recordings show clearly that DNa02 cells not only fire more during ipsilateral turning events but that they get inhibited during contralateral turns. In line with this observation, the difference between left and right DNa02 neuronal activity is a much better predictor of turning events compared to individual DNa02 activity.

      (2) Another technical feat in this work is driving local excitation in the head-direction neuronal ensemble

      (PEN-1 neurons), while simultaneously imaging its activity and performing whole-cell recordings from DNa02

      (Figure 4). This impressive approach provided a way to causally relate changes in the head-direction system to DNa02 activity. Indeed, DNa02 activity could predict the rate at which an artificially triggered bump in the PEN-1 ring attractor returns to its previous stable point.

      (3) The authors also support the above observations with connectomics analysis and provide circuit motifs that can explain how the head direction system (as well as external olfactory/thermal stimuli) communicated with DNa02. All these results unequivocally put DNa02 as an essential DN in steering control, both during exploratory navigation as well as stimulus-directed turns.

      We are grateful for this detailed positive feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) I understand that the first version of this preprint was already on biorxiv in 2020, and some of the "weaknesses" I list are likely a reflection of the fact that I'm tasked to review this manuscript in late 2024 (more than 4 years later). But given this is a 2024 updated version it suffers from laying out the results in contemporary terms. For instance, the manuscript lacks any reference to the DNp09 circuit implicated in object-directed turning and upstream to DNa02 even though the authors cite one of the papers where this was analyzed (Braun et al, 2024). More importantly, these studies (both Braun et al 2024 and Sapkal et al 2024) along with recent work from the authors' lab (Yang et al 2023) and other labs (Feng et al 2024) provide a view that the entire suite of leg kinematics changes required for turning are orchestrated by populations of heterogeneous interconnected DNs. Moreover, these studies also show that this DN-DN network has some degree of hierarchy with some DNs being upstream to other DNs. In this contemporary view of steering control, DNa02 (like DNg13 from Yang et al 2023) is a downstream DN that is recruited by hierarchically upstream DNs like DNa03, DNp09, etc. In this view, DNa02 is likely to be involved in most turning events, but by itself unable to drive all the motor outputs required for the said events. This reasoning could be used while discussing the lack of major phenotypes with DNa02 activation or inactivation observed in the current study, which is in stark contrast to strong phenotypes observed in the case of hierarchically upstream DNs like DNp09 or DNa03. In the section, "Contributions of single descending neuron types to steering behavior": the authors start off by asking if individual DNs can make measurable contributions to steering behavior. Once more, any citations to DNp09 or DNa03 - two DNs that are clearly shown to drive strong turning-on activation (Bidaye et al, 2020, Feng et al 2024) - are lacking. Besides misleading the reader, such statements also digress the results away from contemporary knowledge in the field. I appreciate that the brief discussion in the section titled "Ensemble codes for steering" tries to cover these recent updates. However, I think this would serve a better purpose in the introduction and help guide the results.

      We apologize for these omissions of relevant citations, which we have now fixed. Specifically, in our revised Discussion, we now point out that:

      - Braun et al. (2024) reported that bilateral optogenetic activation of either DNa02 or DNa01 can drive turning (in either direction). 

      - Braun et al. (2024) also identified DNb02 as a steering-related DN.

      - Bidaye et al. (2020), Sapkal et al. (2024), and Braun et al. (2024) all contributed to the identification of DNp09 as a broadcaster DN with the capacity to promote ipsiversive turning.

      We have also revised the beginning of the Results section titled “Contributions of single descending neuron types to steering behavior”, as suggested by the Reviewer.

      Finally, we agree with the Reviewer’s overall point that steering is influenced by multiple DNs. We have not claimed that any DN is solely responsible for steering. As we note in the Discussion: “We found that optogenetically inhibiting DNa01 produced only small defects in steering, and inhibiting DNa02 did not produce statistically significant effects on steering; these results make sense if DNa02 is just one of many steering DNs.”

      (2) The second major weakness is the lack of any immunohistochemistry (IHC) images quantifying the expression of the genetic tools used in these studies. Even though the main split-Gal4 tools for DNa01 and DNa02 were previously reported by Namiki et al, 2018, it is important to document the expression with the effectors used in this work and explicitly mention the expression in any ectopic neurons. Similarly, for any experiments where drivers were combined together (double recordings, functional connectivity) or modified for stochastic expression (Figure 8), IHC images are absolutely necessary. Without this evidence, it is difficult to trust many of the results (especially in the case of behavioral experiments in Figure 8). For example, the DNa01 genetic driver used by the authors is also expressed in some neurons in the nerve cord (as shown on the Flylight webpage of Janelia Research Campus). One wonders if all or part of the results described in Figure 8 are due to DNa01 manipulation or manipulation of the nerve cord neurons. The same applies for optic lobe neurons in the DNa02 driver.

      This is a reasonable request. We used DN split-Gal4 lines to express three types of UAS-linked transgenes:

      (1) GFP

      In these flies, we know that expression in DNs is restricted to the DN types in question, based on published work (Namki et al., 2018), as well as the fact that we see one labeled DN soma per hemisphere. When we label both cells with GFP, we use the spike waveform to identify DNa02 and DNa01, as described in Figure S1

      (2) ReaChR

      In these flies, expression patterns were different in different flies because ReaChR expression was stochastically sparsened using hs-FLP. Expression was validated in each fly after the experiment, as described in the Methods (“Stochastic ReaChR expression”). hs-FLP-mediated sparsening will necessarily produce stochastic patterns of expression in both DNa02 and off-target cells, and this is true of all the flies in this experiment. What makes the “unilateral” flies distinct from the “bilateral” flies is that unilateral flies express ReaChR in one copy of DNa02, whereas bilateral flies express ReaChR in both copies of DNa02. On average, off-target expression will be the same in both groups.

      (3) GtACR1

      In these flies, we initially assumed that GtACR1 expression was the same as GFP expression under control of the same driver. However, we agree with the reviewer’s point that these two expression patterns are not necessarily identical. Therefore, to address the reviewer’s question, we performed immunofluorescence microscopy to characterize GtACR1 patterns in the brain and VNC of both genotypes. These expression patterns are now shown in a new supplemental figure (Figure S8). This figure shows that, as it happens, expression of GtACR1 is indeed indistinguishable from the GFP expression patterns for the same lines (archived on the FlyLight website). Both DN split-Gal4 lines are largely selective for the DNs in question, with limited off-target labeling. We have now drawn attention to this off-target labeling in the last paragraph of the Results, where the GtACR1 results are discussed.

      (3) The paper starts off with a comparative analysis of the roles of DNa01 and DNa02 during steering. Unfortunately, after this initial analysis, DNa01 is largely ignored for further characterization (e.g. with respect to inputs, connectomics, etc.), only to return in the final figure for behavioral characterization where DNa01 seems to have a stronger silencing phenotype compared to DNa02. I couldn't find an explanation for this imbalance in the characterization of DNa01 versus DNa02. Is this due to technical reasons? Or was it an informed decision due to some results? In addition to being a biased characterization, this also results in the manuscript lacking a coherent thread, which in turn makes it a bit inaccessible to the non-specialist.

      Yes, the first portion of the manuscript focuses on DNa01 and DNa02. The latter part of the manuscript transitions to focus mainly on DNa02. 

      Our rationale is noted at the point in the manuscript where we make this transition, with the section titled “Steering toward internal goals”: “Having identified steering-related DNs, we proceeded to investigate the brain circuits that provide input to these DNs. Here we decided to focus on DNa02, as this cell’s activity is predictive of larger steering maneuvers.” When we say that DNa02 is predictive of larger steering maneuvers, we are referring to several specific results:

      - We obtain larger filter amplitudes for DNa02 versus DNa01 (Fig. 2A-C). This means that, just after a unit change in DN firing rate, we see on average a larger change in steering velocity for DNa02 versus DNa01.

      - The linear filter for DNa02 has a higher variance explained, as compared to DNa01 (Fig. 2D). This means that DNa02 is more predictive of steering.

      - The relationship between firing rate and rotational velocity (150 ms later) is steeper for DNa02 than for DNa01 (Fig. 2G). This means that, if we ignore dynamics and we just regress firing rate against subsequent rotational velocity, we see a higher-gain relationship for DNa02.

      Our focus on DNa02 was also driven by connectivity considerations. In the same paragraph (the first paragraph in the section titled “Steering toward internal goals”). We note that “there are strong anatomical pathways from the central complex to DNa02”; the same is not true of DNa01. This point has also been noted by other investigators (Hulse et al. 2021).

      We don’t think this focus on DNa02 makes our work biased or inaccessible. Any study must balance breadth with depth. A useful general way to balance these constraints is to begin a study with a somewhat broader scope, and then narrow the study’s focus to obtain more in-depth information. Here, we began with comparative study of two cell types, and we progressed to the cell type that we found more compelling.

      (4) There seems to be a discrepancy with regard to what is emphasized in the main text and what is shown in Figures S3/S4 in relation to the role of these DNs in backward walking. There are only two sentences in the main text where these figures are cited.

      a) "DNa01 and DNa02 firing rate increases were not consistently followed by large changes in forward velocity

      (Figs. 1G and S3)."

      b) "We found that rotational velocity was consistently related to the difference in right-left firing rates (Fig. 3B). This relationship was essentially linear through its entire dynamic range, and was consistent across paired recordings (Fig. 3C). It was also consistent during backward walking, as well as forward walking (Fig. S4)." These main text sentences imply the role of the difference between left and right DNa02 in turning. However, the actual plots in the Figures S3 and S4 and their respective legends seem to imply a role in "backward walking". For instance, see this sentence from the legend of Figure S3 "When (ΔvoltageDNa02>>ΔvoltageDNa01), the fly is typically moving backward. When (firing rateDNa02>>firing rateDNa01), the fly is also often moving backward, but forward movement is still more common overall, and so the net effect is that forward velocity is small but still positive when (firing rateDNa02>>firing rateDNa01). Note that when we condition our analysis on behavior rather than neural activity, we do see that backward walking is associated with a large firing rate differential (Fig. S4)." This sort of discrepancy in what is emphasized in the text, versus what is emphasized in the figures, ends up confusing the reader. More importantly, I do not agree with any of these conclusions regarding the implication of backward walking. Both Figures S3 and S4 are riddled with caveats, misinterpretations, and small sample sizes. As a result, I actually support the authors' decision to not infer too much from these figures in the "main text". In fact, I would recommend going one step further and removing/modifying these figures to focus on the role of "rotational velocity". Please find my concerns about these two figures below:

      a) In Figures S3 and S4, every heat map has a different scale for the same parameter: forward velocity. S3A is -10 to +10mm/s. S3B is -6 to +6 S4B (left) is -12 to +12 and S4B (right) is -4 to +4. Since the authors are trying to depict results based on the color-coding this is highly problematic.

      b) Figure S3A legend "When (ΔvoltageDNa02>>ΔvoltageDNa01), the fly is typically moving backward." There are also several instances when ΔvoltageDNa02= ΔvoltageDNa01 and both are low (lower left quadrant) when the fly is typically moving backwards. So in my opinion, this figure in fact suggests DNa02 has no role in backward velocity control.

      c) Based on the example traces in S4A, every time the fly walks backwards it is also turning. Based on this it is important to show absolute rotational velocity in Figure S4C. It could be that the fly is turning around the backward peak which would change the interpretation from Figure S4C. Also, it is important to note that the backward velocities in S4A are unprecedentedly high. No previous reports show flies walking backwards at such high velocities (for example see Chen et al 2018, Nat Comm. for backward walking velocities on a similar setup).

      d) In my opinion, Figure S4D showing that right-left DNa02 correlates with rotational velocity, regardless of whether the fly is in a forward or backward walking state, is the only important and conclusive result in Figures S3/S4. These figures should be rearranged to only emphasize this panel.

      We agree that it is difficult to interpret some of the correlations between DN activity and forward velocity, given that forward velocity and rotational velocity are themselves correlated to some degree. This is why we did not make claims based on these results in the main text. In response to these comments, we have taken the Reviewer’s suggestion to preserve Figure S4D (now Figure S3). The other components of these supplemental figures have been removed.

      (5) Figure 3 shows a really nice analysis of the bilateral DNa02 recordings data. While Figure S5 [now Figure S4] shows that authors have a similar dataset for DNa01, a similar level analysis (Figures 3D, E) is not done for DNa01 data. Is there a reason why this is not done?

      The reason we did not do the same analysis for DNa01 is that we only have two paired DNa01-DNa01 recordings. It turned out to be substantially more difficult to perform DNa01-DNa01 recordings, as compared to DNa02-DNa02 recordings. For this reason, we were not able to get more than two of these recordings.

      (6) In Figure 4 since the authors have trials where bump-jump led to turning in the opposite direction to the DNa02 being recorded, I wonder if the authors could quantify hyperpolarization in DNa02 as is predicted from connectomics data in Figure 7.

      We agree this is an interesting question. However, DNa02 firing rate and membrane potential are variable, and stimulus-evoked hyperpolarizations in these DNs tend to be relatively small (on the order of 1 mV, in the case of a contralateral fictive olfactory stimulus, Figure 5A). In the case of our fictive olfactory stimuli, we could look carefully for these hyperpolarizations because we had a very large number of trials, and we could align these trials precisely to stimulus onset. By contrast, for the bump-jump experiments, we have a more limited number of trials, and turning onset is not so tightly time-locked to the chemogenetic stimuli; for these reasons, we are hesitant to make claims about any bump-jump-related hyperpolarization in these trials.

      (7) Figure 6 suggests that DNa02 contains information about latent steering drives. This is really interesting. However, in order to unequivocally claim this, a higher-resolution postural analysis might be needed. Especially given that DNa02 activation does not reliably evoke ipsilateral turning, these "latent" steering events could actually contain significant postural changes driven by DNa02 (making them "not latent"). Without this information, at least the authors need to explicitly mention this caveat.

      This is a good point. We cannot exclude the possibility that DNa02 is driving postural changes when the fly is stopped, and these postural changes are so small we cannot detect them. In this case, however, there would still be an interesting mismatch between the stimulus-evoked change in DNa02 firing rate (which is large) and the stimulus-evoked postural response (which would be very small). We have added language to the relevant Results section in order to make this explicit.

      (8) Figure 7 would really benefit from connectome data with synapse numbers (or weighted arrows) and a corresponding analysis of DNa01.

      In response to this comment, we have added synapses number information (represented by weighted arrows) to Figures 7C, E, and F. We also added information to the Methods to explain how cells were chosen for inclusion in this diagram. (In brief: we thresholded these connections so as to discard connections with small numbers of synapses.)

      We did perform an analogous connectome circuit analysis for DNa01, but if we use the same thresholds as we do for DNa02, we obtain a much sparser connectivity graph. We now show this in a new supplemental figure (Figure S9). MBON32 makes no monosynaptic connections onto DNa01, and it only forms one disynaptic connection, via LAL018, which is relatively weak. PFL3 and PFL2 make no mono- or disynaptic connections onto DNa01 comparable in strength to what we find for DNa02. 

      The sparser connectivity graph for DNa01 is partly due to the fact that fewer cell types converge onto DNa01 as compared to DNa02 (110 cell types, versus 287 cell types). Also, it seems that DNa01 is simply less closely connected to the central complex and mushroom body, as compared to DNa02.

      (9) In Figure 8E, the most obvious neuronal silencing phenotype is decreased sideways velocity in the case of DNa01 optogenetic silencing. In Figure S2, the inverse filter for sideways velocity for DNa01 had a higher amplitude than the rotational velocity filter. Taken together, does this point at some role for DNa01 in sideways velocity specifically?

      No. The forward filters describe the average velocity impulse response, given a brief step change in firing rate.

      Figure 1 and Figure S2 show that the sideways velocity forward filter is actually smaller for DNa01 than for DNa02. This means that a brief step change in DNa01 firing rate is followed by only a very small sideways velocity response. Conversely, the reverse filters describe the average firing rate impulse response, given a brief step change in sideways velocity. Figure S2 shows that the sideways velocity reverse filter is larger for DNa01 than for DNa02, but this means that the relationship between DNa01 activity and sideways velocity is so weak that we would need to see a very large neural response in order to get a brief step change in sideways velocity. In other words, the reverse filter says that DNa01 likely has very little role in determining sideways velocity.

      (10) In Figure 8G, the effect on inner hind leg stance prolongation is very weak, and given the huge sample size, hard to interpret. Also, it is not clear how this fits with the role of DNa01 in slow sustained turning based on recordings.

      Yes, this effect is small in magnitude, which is not too surprising, given that many DNs seem to be involved in the control of steering in walking. To clarify the interpretation of these phenotypes, we have added a paragraph to the end of the Results:

      “All these effects are weak, and so they should be interpreted with caution. Also, both DN split-Gal4 lines drive expression in a few off-target cell types, which is another reason for caution (Fig. S8). However, they suggest that both DNs can lengthen the stance phase of the ipsilateral back leg, which would cause ipsiversive turning. These results are also compatible with a scenario where both DNs decrease the step length in the ipsilateral legs, which would also cause ipsiversive turning. Step frequency does not normally change asymmetrically during turning, so the observed decrease in step frequency during optogenetic inhibition may just be a by-product of increasing step length when these DNs are inhibited.” We have also added caveats and clarifications in a new Discussion paragraph:

      “Our study does not fully answer the question of how these DNs affect leg kinematics, because we were not able to simultaneously measure DN activity and leg movement. However, our optogenetic experiments suggest that both DNs can lengthen the stance phase of the ipsilateral back leg (Fig. 8G), and/or  decrease the step length in the ipsilateral legs (Fig. 8H), either of which would cause ipsiversive turning. If these DNs have similar qualitative effects on leg kinematics, then why does DNa02 precede larger and more rapid steering events? This may be due to the fact that DNa02 receives stronger and more direct input from key steering circuits in the brain (Fig. S9). It may also relate to the fact that DNa02 has more direct connections onto motor neurons (Fig. 1B).”

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) I found the sign conventions for rotational velocity particularly confusing. Figure 3 represents clockwise rotations as +ve values, but Figure 4H represents anticlockwise rotations as positive values. But for EPG bumps, anticlockwise rotations are given negative values. Please make them consistent unless I am missing something obvious.

      Different fields use different conventions for yaw velocity. In aeronautics, a clockwise turn is generally positive. In robotics and engineering of terrestrial vehicles, a counterclockwise turn is generally positive. Historically, most Drosophila studies that quantified rotational (yaw) velocity were focused on the behavior of flying flies, and these studies generally used the convention from aeronautics, where a clockwise turn is defined as a positive turn. When we began working in the field, we adopted this convention, in order to conform to previous literature. It might be argued that walking flies are more like robots than airplanes, but it seemed to us that it was confusing to have different conventions for different behaviors of the same animal. Thus, all of the published studies from our lab define clockwise rotation as having positive rotational velocity.

      Figure 4 focuses on the role of the central complex in steering. As the fly turns clockwise (rightward), the bump of activity in EPG neurons normally moves counterclockwise around the ellipsoid body, as viewed from the posterior side (Turner-Evans et al., 2017). The posterior view is the conventional way to represent these dynamics, because (1) we and others typically image the brain from the posterior side, not the anterior side, and (2) in a posterior view, the animal’s left is on the left side of the image, and vice versa. We have added a sentence to the Figure 4A legend to clarify these points.

      Previous work has shown that, when an experimenter artificially “jumps” the EPG bump, this causes the fly to make a compensatory turn that returns the bump to (approximately) its original location (Green et al., 2019). Our work supports this observation. Specifically, we find that clockwise bump jumps are generally followed by rightward turns (which drive the bump to return to its approximate original location via a counterclockwise path), and vice versa. This is noted in the Figure 4D legend. Note that Figure 4D plots the fly’s rotational velocity during the bump return, plotted against the initial bump jump. 

      Figure 4H shows that clockwise (blue) bump returns were typically preceded by leftward turning, counter-clockwise (green) bump returns were preceded by rightward turning, as expected. This is detailed in the Figure 4H legend, and it is consistent with the coordinate frame described above.

      (2) It would be helpful to have images of the DNa01 and DNa02 split lines used in this paper, considering this paper would most likely be used widely to describe the functions of these neurons. Similarly, images of their reconstructions would be a useful addition.

      High-quality three-dimensional confocal stacks of all the driver lines used in our study are publicly available. We have added this information to the Methods (under “Fly husbandry and genotypes”). Confocal images of the full morphologies of DNa01 and DNa02 have been previously published (Namiki et al., 2018). Figure 1A is a schematic that is intended to provide a quick visual summary of this information.

      EM reconstructions of DNa01 and DNa02 are publicly accessible in a whole-brain dataset (https://codex.flywire.ai/) and a whole-VNC dataset (https://neuprint.janelia.org/). Both datasets are referenced in our study. As these datasets are easy to search and browse via user-friendly web-based tools, we expect that interested readers will have no difficulty accessing the underlying datasets directly.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The description of the activity of the DNs that they "PREDICT steering during walking". This is an interesting word choice. Not causes, not correlates with, not encodes... does that mean the activity always precedes the action? Does that mean when you see activity, you will get behavior? This is important for assessing whether the DN activity is a cause or an effect. It is good to be cautious but it might be worth expanding on exactly what kind of connection is implied to justify the use of the word 'predict'.

      Conventionally, “predict” means “to indicate in advance”. We write that DNs “predict” certain features of behavior. We use this term because (1) these DNs correlate with certain features of behavior, and (2) changes in DN activity precede changes in behavior.

      The notion that neurons can “predict” behavior is not original to our study. Whenever neuroscientists summarize the relationship between neural activity and behavior by fitting a mathematical model (which may be as simple as a linear regression), the fitted model can be said to represent a “prediction” of behavior. These models are evaluated by comparing their predictions with measured behaviors. A good model is predictive, but it also implies that the underlying neural signal is also predictive (Levenstein et al., 2023 Journal of Neuroscience 43: 1074-1088; DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1179-22.2022). Here, prediction simply means correlation, without necessarily implying causation. We also use “prediction” to imply correlation.

      We do not think the term “prediction” implies determinism. Meteorologists are said to predict the weather, but it is understood that their predictions are probabilistic, not deterministic. Certainly, we would not claim that there is a deterministic relationship between DN activity and behavior. Figure 2D shows that neither DN type can explain all the variance in the fly’s rotational or sideways velocity. At the same time, both DNs have significant predictive power.

      We might equally say that these DNs “encode” behavior. We have chosen to use the word “predict” rather than “encode” because we do not think it is necessary to use the framework of symbolic communication in connection with these DNs.

      We agree with the Reviewer that it is helpful to test whether any neuron that “predicts” a behavior might also “cause” this behavior. In Figure 8, we show that directly perturbing these DNs can indeed alter locomotor behavior, which suggests a causal role. Connectome analyses also suggest a causal role for these DNs in locomotor behavior (Figure 1B, see especially also Cheong et al., 2024).

      At the same time, it is clear from our results that these DNs are not “command neurons” for turning: they do not deterministically cause turning. Therefore, to avoid misunderstanding, we have generally been careful to summarize the results of our perturbation experiments by avoiding the statement that “this DN causes this behavior”. Rather, we have generally tried to say that “this DN influences this behavior”, or “this DN promotes this behavior”.

      (2) There is some concern about how the linear filter models were developed and then used to predict the relationship between firing rate and steering behavior: how exactly were the build and test data separated to avoid re-extracting the input? It reads like a self-fulfilling prophecy/tautology.

      We used conventional cross-validation for model fitting and evaluation. We apologize that this was not made explicit in our original submission; this was due to an oversight on our part. To be clear: linear filters were computed using the data from the first 20% of a given experiment. We then convolved each cell’s firing rate estimate with the computed Neuron→Behavior filter (the “forward filter”) using the data from the final 80% of the experiment, in order to generate behavioral predictions. Thus, when a model has high variance explained, this is not attributable to overfitting: rather, it quantifies the bona fide predictive power of the model. We have added this information to the Methods (under “Data analysis - Linear filter analysis”).

      (3) Type-O right above Figure 2 [now Figure 1E]: I assume spike rate fluctuations in DNa02 precede DNa01?

      Fixed. Thank you for reading the manuscript carefully.

      (4) The description of the other manuscripts about neural control of the steering as "follow-up" papers is a bit diminishing. They were likely independent works on a similar theme that happened afterwards, rather than deliberate extensions of this paper, so "subsequent" might be a more accurate description.

      We apologize, as we did not intend this to be diminishing. Given this request, we have revised “follow-up” to “subsequent”.

      (5) The idea that DNa02 is high-gain because it is more directly connected to motor neurons is a hypothesis and this should be made clear. We really don't know the functional consequences of the directness of a path or the number of synapses, and which circuits you compare to would change this. DNa02 may be a higher gain than DNa01, but what about relative to the other DNs that enter pre-motor regions? How do you handle a few synapses and several neurons in a common class? All of these connectivity-based deductions await functional tests - like yours! I think it is better to make this clear so readers don't assume a higher level of certainty than we have.

      The Reviewer asks how we handled few-synapse connections, and how we combined neurons in the same class. We apologize for not making this explicit in our original submission. We have now added this information to the Methods. Briefly, to select cell types for inclusion in Figures 7C, we identified all individual cells postsynaptic to PFL3 and presynaptic to DNa02, discarding any unitary connections with <5 synapses. We then grouped unitary connections by cell type, and then summed all synapse numbers within each connection group (e.g., summing all synapses in all PFL3→LAL126 connections). We then discarded connection groups having <200 synapses or <1% of a cell type’s pre- or postsynaptic total. Reported connection weights are per hemisphere, i.e. half of the total within each connection group. For Figure 7F we did the same, but now discarding connection groups having <70 synapses or <0.4% of a cell type’s pre- or postsynaptic total. In Figure S9, we used the same procedures for analyzing connections onto DNa01. 

      We agree that it is tricky to infer function from connectome data, and this applies to motor neuron connectivity. We bring up DN connectivity onto motor neurons in two places. First, in the Results, we note that “steering filters (i.e., rotational and sideways velocity filters) were larger for DNa02 (Fig. 2A,B). This means that an impulse change in firing rate predicts a larger change in steering for this neuron. In other words, this result suggests that DNa02 operates with higher gain. This may be related to the fact that DNa02 makes more direct output synapses onto motor neurons (Fig. 1B) [emphasis added].” We feel this is a relatively conservative statement.

      Subsequently, in the Discussion, we ask, “why does DNa02 precede larger and more rapid steering events? This may be due to the fact that DNa02 receives stronger and more direct input from key steering circuits in the brain (Fig. S9). It may also relate to the fact that DNa02 has more direct connections onto motor neurons (Fig. 1B) [emphasis added].” Again, we feel this is a relatively conservative statement.

      To be sure, none of the motor neurons postsynaptic to DNa02 actually receive most of their synaptic input from DNa02 (or indeed any DN), and this is typical of motor neurons controlling leg muscles. Rather, leg motor neurons tend to get most of their input from interneurons rather than motor neurons (Cheong et al. 2024). Available data suggests that the walking rhythm originates with intrinsic VNC central pattern generators, and the DNs that influence walking do so, in large part, by acting on VNC interneurons. These points have been detailed in recent connectome analyses (see especially Cheong et al. 2024).

      We are reluctant to broaden the scope of our connectome analyses to include other DNs for comparison, because we think these analyses are most appropriate to full-central-nervous-system-(CNS)-connectomes (brain and VNC together), which are currently under construction. Without a full-CNS-connectome, many of the DN axons in the VNC cannot be identified. In the future, we expect that full-CNS-connectomes will allow a systematic comparison of the input and output connectivity of all DN types, and probably also the tentative identification of new steering DNs. Those future analyses should generate new hypotheses about the specializations of DNa02, DNa01, and other DNs. Our study aims to help lay a conceptual foundation for that future work.

      (6) Given the emphasis on the DNa02 to Motor Neuron connectivity shown (Figure 1B) and multiple text mentions, could you include more analyses of which motor neurons are downstream and how these might be expected to affect leg movements? I would like to see the synapse numbers (Figure 1B) as well as the fraction of total output synapses. These additions would help understand the evidence for the "see-saw" model.

      We agree this is interesting. In follow-up work from our lab (Yang et al., 2023), we describe the detailed VNC connectivity linking DNa02 to motor neurons. We refer the Reviewer specifically to Figure 7 of that study (https://www.cell.com/cell/fulltext/S0092-8674(24)00962-0).

      We regret that the see-saw model was perhaps not clear in our original submission. Briefly, this model proposes that an increase in excitatory synaptic input to one DN (and/or a disinhibition of that DN) is often accompanied by an increase in inhibitory synaptic input to the contralateral DN. This model is motivated by connectome data on the brain inputs to DNa02 (Figure 7), along with our observation that excitation of one DN is often accompanied by inhibition of the contralateral DN (Figure 5). We have now added text to the Results in several places in order to clarify these points. 

      This model specifically pertains to the brain inputs to DNs, comparing the downstream targets of these DNs in the VNC would not be a test of this hypothesis. The Reviewer may be asking to see whether there is any connectivity in the brain from one DN to its contralateral partner. We do not find connections of this sort, aside from multisynaptic connections that rely on very weak links (~10 synapses per connection). Figure 7 depicts a much stronger basis for this hypothesis, involving feedforward see-saw connections from PFL3 and MBON32. 

      (7) The conclusions from the data in Figure 8 could be explained more clearly. These seem like small effect sizes on subtle differences in leg movements - maybe like what was seen in granular control by Moonwalker's circuits? Measuring joint angles or step parameters might help clarify, but a summary description would help the reader.

      We agree that these results were not explained very well in our original submission. 

      In our revised manuscript, we have added a new paragraph to the end of this Results section providing some summary and interpretation:

      “All these effects are weak, and so they should be interpreted with caution. However, they suggest that both DNs can lengthen the stance phase of the ipsilateral back leg, which would promote ipsiversive turning. These results are also compatible with a scenario where both DNs decrease the step length in the ipsilateral legs, which would also promote ipsiversive turning. Step frequency does not normally change asymmetrically during turning, so the observed decrease in step frequency during optogenetic inhibition may just be a by-product of increasing step length when these DNs are inhibited.”

      Moreover, in the Discussion, we have also added a new paragraph that synthesizes these results with other results in our study, while also noting the limitations of our study:

      “Our study does not fully answer the question of how these DNs affect leg kinematics, because we were not able to simultaneously measure DN activity and leg movement. However, our optogenetic experiments suggest that both DNs can lengthen the stance phase of the ipsilateral back leg (Fig. 8G), and/or  decrease the step length in the ipsilateral legs (Fig. 8H), either of which would promote ipsiversive turning. If these DNs have similar qualitative effects on leg kinematics, then why does DNa02 precede larger and more rapid steering events? This may be due to the fact that DNa02 receives stronger and more direct input from key steering circuits in the brain (Fig. S9). It may also relate to the fact that DNa02 has more direct connections onto motor neurons (Fig. 1B).”

      In Figure 8D-H, we measure step parameters in freely walking flies during acute optogenetic inhibition of DNa01 and DNa02. In experiments measuring neural activity in flies walking on a spherical treadmill, we did not have a way to measure step parameters. Subsequently, this methodology was developed by Yang et al. (2023) and results for DNa02 are described in that study. 

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor Points:

      (1) If space allows, actual membrane potential should be mentioned when raw recordings are shown (for example Figure 1D).

      We have now added absolute membrane potential information to Figure 1d.

      (2) Typo in the sentence "To address this issue directly, we looked closely at the timing of each cell's recruitment in our dual recordings, and found that spike rate fluctuations in DNa02 typically preceded the spike rate fluctuations in DNa02 (Fig. 2A)." The final word should be "DNa01".

      Fixed. Thank you for reading the manuscript carefully.

      (3) Figure 2A - although there aren't direct connections between a01 and a02 in the connectome, the authors never rule out functional connectivity between these two. Given a02 precedes a01, shouldn't this be addressed?

      In the full brain FAFB data set, there are two disynaptic connections from DNa02 onto the ipsilateral copy of DNa01. One connection is via CB0556 (which is GABAergic), and the other is via LAL018 (which is cholinergic). The relevant DNa02 output connections are very weak: each DNa02→CB0556 connection consists of 11 synapses, whereas each DNa02→LAL018 connection consists of 10 synapses (on average). Conversely, each CB0556→DNa01 connection consists of 29 synapses, whereas  each LAL018→DNa01 connection consists of 64 synapses. In short, LAL018 is a nontrivial source of excitatory input to DNa01, but DNa02 is not positioned to exert much influence over LAL018, and the two disynaptic connections from DNa02 onto DNa01 also have the opposite sign. Thus, it seems unlikely that DNa02 is a major driver of DNa01 activity. At the same time, it is difficult to completely exclude this possibility, because we do not understand the logic of the very complicated premotor inputs to these DNs in the brain. Thus, we are hesitant to make a strong statement on this point.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Sammons, Masserini et al. examine the connectivity of different types of CA3 pyramidal cells ("thorny" and "athorny"), and how their connectivity putatively contributes to their relative timing in sharp-wave-like activity. First, using patch-clamp recordings, they characterize the degree of connectivity within and between athorny and thorny cells. Based upon these experimental results, they compute a synaptic product matrix, and use this to inform a computational model of CA3 activity. This model finds that this differential connectivity between these populations, augmented by two different types of inhibitory neurons, can account for the relative timing of activity observed in sharp waves in vivo.

      We thank the reviewer for reading our manuscript, as well as for their nice summary and constructive comments

      Strengths:

      The patch-clamp experiments are exceptionally thorough and well done. These are very challenging experiments and the authors should be commended for their in-depth characterization of CA3 connectivity.

      Thank you for the recognition of our efforts.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The computational elements of this study feel underdeveloped. Whereas the authors do a thorough job experimentally characterizing connections between excitatory neurons, the inhibitory neurons used in the model seem to be effectivity "fit neurons" and appear to have been tuned to produce the emergent properties of CA3 sharp wave-like activity. Although I appreciate the goal was to implicate CA3 connectivity contributions to activity timing, a stronger relationship seems like it could be examined. For example, did the authors try to "break" their model? It would be informative if they attempted different synaptic product matrices (say, the juxtaposition of their experimental product matrix) and see whether experimentally-derived sequential activity could not be elicited. It seems as though this spirit of analysis was examined in Figure 4C, but only insofar as individual connectivity parameters were changed in isolation.

      Including the two interneuron types (B and C) in the model is, on the one hand, necessary to align our modeling framework to the state-of-the-art model by Evangelista et al. (2020), which assumes that these populations act as switchers between an SPW and a non-SPW state, and on the other hand, less straightforward because the connectivity involving these interneurons is largely unknown.

      For B cells, the primary criterion to set their connections to and from excitatory cells was to balance the effect of the strong recurrent excitation and to achieve a mid-range firing rate for each population during sharp wave events. Our new simulations (Figure 5B) show that the initial suppression of population T (resulting in the long delay) indeed depends in equal proportions on the outlined excitatory connections and on how strongly each excitatory population is targeted by the B interneurons. However, these simulations demonstrate that there is a broad, clearly distinct, region of the parameter space that supports a long delay between the peaks, rather than a marginal set of finetuned parameters. In addition, the simulations show that B interneurons optimally contribute to the suppression of T when they primarily target T (Fig. 5B, panels 3,7,11,12,13) rather than A (panels 4,8,9,10,11). On the contrary, as reported in the parameter table, and now also displayed graphically in the new Figure 4A (included above, with arrow sizes proportional to the synaptic product between the parameters determining the total strength of each connection), we assume B to target A less weakly than T (to make up for the higher excitability of population A). Therefore, the long delay between the peaks in our model emerges in spite of the interneuron connectivity, rather than because of it, and it is an effect of the asymmetric connectivity between the two excitatory populations, in particular the extremely low connection from A to T.

      (2) Additional explanations of how parameters for interneurons were incorporated in the model would be very helpful. As it stands, it is difficult to understand the degree to which the parameters of these neurons are biologically constrained versus used as fit parameters to produce different time windows of activity in types of CA3 pyramidal cells.

      Response included in point (1).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Sharp wave ripples are transient oscillations occurring in the hippocampus that are thought to play an important role in organising temporal sequences during the reactivation of neuronal activity. This study addresses the mechanism by which these temporal sequences are generated in the CA3 region focusing on two different subtypes of pyramidal neurons, thorny and athorny. Using high-quality electrophysiological recordings from up to 8 pyramidal neurons at a time the authors measure the connectivity rates between these pyramidal cell subtypes in a large dataset of 348 cells. This is a significant achievement and provides important data. The most striking finding is how similar connection characteristics are between cell types. There are no differences in synaptic strength or failure rates and some small differences in connectivity rates and short-term plasticity. Using model simulations, the authors explore the implications of the differences in connectivity rates for the temporal specificity of pyramidal cell firing within sharp-wave ripple events. The simulations show that the experimentally observed connectivity rates may contribute to the previously observed temporal sequence of pyramidal cell firing during sharp wave ripples.

      Thank you very much for your careful review of our manuscript and the overall positive assessment.

      The conclusions drawn from the simulations are not experimentally tested so remain theoretical. In the simple network model, the authors include basket cell and anti-SWR interneurons but the connectivity of these cell types is not measured experimentally and variations in interneuron parameters may also influence temporal specificity of firing.

      As variations in some of these parameters can indeed influence the temporal specificity of firing, we have now performed additional simulations, the results of which are in the new Figures 5 and S5. Please also see response to Reviewer 1, point 1.

      In addition, the influence of short-term plasticity measured in their experiments is not tested in the model.

      We have now included short-term synaptic depression in all the excitatory-to-excitatory synapses and compensated for the weakened recurrent excitation by scaling some of the other parameters. The results of re-running our simulations in this alternative version of the model are reported in Figure S3 and are qualitatively analogous to those in Figure 4.

      Interestingly, the experimental data reveal a large variability in many of the measured parameters. This may strongly influence the firing of pyramidal cells during SWRs but it is not represented within the model which uses the averaged data.

      We have now incorporated variability in the following simulation parameters: the strength and latency of the four excitatory-to-excitatory connections as well as the reversal potential and leak conductance of both types of pyramidal cells, assuming variabilities similar to those observed experimentally (see Materials and Methods for details). Upon a slight re-balancing of some inhibitory connection strengths, in order to achieve comparable firing rates, we found that this version of the model also supports the generation of sharp waves with two pyramidal components (Figure S4B), and is, thus, fully analogous to our basic model. Varying the excitatory connectivities as in the original simulations (cf. Figure 4C and Figure S4C) reveals that increasing the athorny-toathorny or decreasing the athorny-to-thorny connectivity still increases the delay between the peaks, although for some connectivity values the peak of the athorny population appears more spread out in time.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The hippocampal CA3 region is generally considered to be the primary site of initiation of sharp wave ripples-highly synchronous population events involved in learning and memory although the precise mechanism remains elusive. A recent study revealed that CA3 comprises two distinct pyramidal cell populations: thorny cells that receive mossy fiber input from the dentate gyrus, and athorny cells that do not. That study also showed that it is athorny cells in particular that play a key role in sharp wave initiation. In the present work, Sammons, Masserini, and colleagues expand on this by examining the connectivity probabilities among and between thorny and athorny cells. First, using whole-cell patch clamp recordings, they find an asymmetrical connectivity pattern, with athorny cells receiving the most synaptic connections from both athorny and thorny cells, and thorny cells receiving fewer. They then demonstrate in spiking neural network simulations how this asymmetrical connectivity may underlie the preferential role of athorny cells in sharp wave initiation.

      Strengths:

      The authors provide independent validation of some of the findings by Hunt et al. (2018) concerning the distinction between thorny and athorny pyramidal cells in CA3 and advance our understanding of their differential integration in CA3 microcircuits. The properties of excitatory connections among and between thorny and athorny cells described by the authors will be key in understanding CA3 functions including, but not limited to, sharp wave initiation.

      As stated in the paper, the modeling results lend support to the idea that the increased excitatory connectivity towards athorny cells plays a key role in causing them to fire before thorny cells in sharp waves. More generally, the model adds to an expanding pool of models of sharp wave ripples which should prove useful in guiding and interpreting experimental research.

      Thank you very much for your careful review of our manuscript and this positive assessment.

      Weaknesses:

      The mechanism by which athorny cells initiate sharp waves in the model is somewhat confusingly described. As far as I understood, random fluctuations in the activities of A and B neurons provide windows of opportunity for pyramidal cells to fire if they have additionally recovered from adaptive currents. Thorny and athorny pyramidal cells are then set in a winner-takes-all competition which is quickly won by the athorny cells. The main thesis of the paper seems to be that athorny cells win this competition because they receive more inputs both from themselves and from thorny cells, hence, the connectivity "underlies the sequential activation". However, it is also stated that athorny cells activate first due to their lower rheobase and steeper f-I curve, and it is also indicated in the methods that athorny (but not thorny) cells fire in bursts. It seems that it is primarily these features that make them fire first, something which apparently happens even when the A to A connectivity is set to 0albeit with a very small lag. Perhaps the authors could further clarify the differential role of single cell and network parameters in determining the sequential activation of athorny and thorny cells. Is the role of asymmetric excitatory connectivity only to enhance the initial intrinsic advantage of athorny cells? If so, could this advantage also be enhanced in other ways?

      Thank you for the time invested in the review of our manuscript. We especially thank you for pointing out that the description of these dynamics was unclear: we have now improved it in the main text and we provide here an additional summary. As correctly highlighted by Reviewer 3, athorny neurons (A) are more excitable than thorny (T) ones due to single-neuron parameters: therefore, if there is a winner-takes-all competition, they are going to win it. Whether there is a competition in the first place, however, depends on the excitatory (and inhibitory) connections. In particular, we should distinguish two questions: does the activity of populations A and B (PV baskets), without adaptation (so at the beginning of the sharp wave) suppress T? And does the activity of populations T and B suppress A?

      The four possible combinations can be appreciated, for example, in the new Figure 5A5. If A can suppress T, but T cannot suppress A (low A-to-T, high T-to-A, bottom right corner, like in the data), A “wins” and T fires later, after a long delay. If both A and T can suppress each other (both cross-connections are low, bottom left corner), we still get the same outcome: A wins because of its earlier and sharper onset (due to single-neuron parameters). If neither population can suppress the other (high cross-connections, top right corner), then there is no competition and the populations reach the peak approximately at the same time. Only in the case in which T can suppress A, but A cannot suppress T (low T-to-A, high A-to-T, top left corner, opposite to the data), then A “loses” the competition. However, since A neurons nevertheless display some early activity (again, due to the single neuron parameters), this scenario is not as clean as the reversed one: rather, A cells have an initial, small peak, then T neurons quickly take over and grow to their own peak, and then, depending on how strongly T neurons suppress A neurons, there may or may not be a second peak for the A neurons. This is the reason why, in the top left corner of Figure 5B, the statistics show either a long positive or long negative delay, depending on whether the first (small) or second (absent, for some parameters) peak of A is taken into account. In summary, the experimentally measured connectivity does not only enhance the initial intrinsic advantage of A cells, but sets up the competitive dynamics in the first place, which are crucial for the emergence of two distinct peaks, rather than a single peak involving both populations.

      Although a clear effort has been made to constrain the model with biological data, too many degrees of freedom remain that allow the modeler to make arbitrary decisions. This is not a problem in itself, but perhaps the authors could explain more of their reasoning and expand upon the differences between their modeling choices and those of others. For example, what are the conceptual or practical advantages of using adaptation in pyramidal neurons as opposed to short-term synaptic plasticity as in the model by Hunt et al.?

      It should be pointed out that the model by Hunt et al. features adaptation in pyramidal neurons as well, as the neuronal units employed are also adaptive-exponential integrate-and-fire. In an early stage of this project, we obtained from Hunt et al. the code for their model, and ascertained that adaptation is the main mechanism governing the alternations between the sharp-wave and the non-sharp-wave states, to the extent that fully removing short-term plasticity from their model does not have any significant impact on the network dynamics. Therefore, our choices are, in this regard, fully consistent with theirs. In order to confirm that synaptic depression does not significantly impact the dynamics also in our model, we now performed additional simulations (Figure S3), addressed in the main text (lines 149-151) and in the response to Reviewer 1, who expressed similar concerns.

      Relatedly, what experimental observations could validate or falsify the proposed mechanisms?

      As sharp wave generation in this model relies on disinhibitory dynamics (suppression of the anti-sharp-wave interneurons C), the model could be validated/falsified by proving/disproving that a class of interneurons with anti-sharp-wave features exists. In addition, the mechanism we proposed for the long delay between the peaks of the athorny and thorny activity requires at least some connectivity from athorny to basket and from basket to thorny neurons.

      In the data by Hunt et al., thorny cells have a higher baseline (non-SPW) firing rate, and it is claimed that it is actually stochastic correlations in their firing that are amplified by athorny cells to initiate sharp waves. However, in the current model, the firing of both types of pyramidal cells outside of ripples appears to be essentially zero. Can the model handle more realistic firing rates as described by Hunt et al., or as produced by e.g., walking around an environment tiled with place cells, or would that trigger SPWs continuously?

      When building this model, we aimed at having two clearly distinct states the network could alternate between, so we picked a rather polarized connectivity to and from the anti-sharp wave cells (C), resulting in polarized states. As a result, we obtain a low, although non-zero, activity of pyramidal neurons in non-SPW states (0.4 spikes/s for athorny and 0.2 spikes/s for thorny). These assumptions can be partially relaxed, for example in the original model by Evangelista et al. (2020), where the background firing rate of pyramidal cells is ~2 spikes/s. It should also be noted that, when walking in an environment tiled with place cells, the hippocampus is subject to additional extra-hippocampal inputs (e.g. from the medial septum, resulting in theta oscillations) and to neuromodulation, which can alter the network in various ways that we have not included in our model. However, our results are not in contradiction to transient SPW-like activity states initiated at a certain phase of the theta oscillation, when the inhibition is weakest.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The manuscript reads like it was intended as a short-form manuscript for another journal. The introduction and discussion in particular are very brief and would benefit from being expanded and providing a bigger picture for the reader.

      We had originally aimed to submit in the eLife “short report” format. However, also thanks to the suggestion of Reviewer 1, we realized that our text would be better supported by extended introduction and discussion sections, as well as additional figures.

      (2) Graphs would benefit from including all datapoints, where appropriate.

      All datapoints have now been added to boxplots in the main figures and supplement.

      (3) The panels of Figure 4 are laid out strangely, it may be worthwhile to adjust.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have now adjusted the layout of Figure 4 and believe it is now easier to follow.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Useful points to address include:

      (1) Explore within the model the effect of altering interneuron connectivity. Are there other factors that can influence temporal specificity within SWRs?

      The effects of varying the connectivity to and from B interneurons (the ones which are SPWactive and therefore relevant for temporal specificity) have now been investigated in the new Figure 5B, in which such parameters were varied in pairs or combined with the two most relevant excitatoryto-excitatory connections.

      (2) Implement the experimentally observed short-term plasticity in the model to determine how this influences temporal specificity.

      All the findings in Figure 4 have now been replicated in the new Figure S3, in which excitatory-to-excitatory synapses feature synaptic depression.

      (3) Consider if it is possible to incorporate observed experimental variability in the model and explore the implications.

      All the findings in Figure 4 have now been replicated in the new Figure S4, in which heterogeneity has been introduced in multiple neuronal and synaptic parameters of thorny and athorny neurons.

      (4) Include the co-connectivity rates in the data. Ie how many of the recorded neurons are reciprocally connected? Does this change the model simulations?

      We have now added the rates of reciprocal connections that we observed into the main text (lines 8688). We found 2 pairs of reciprocally connected athorny neurons and 2 pairs of reciprocally connected thorny neurons. These rates of reciprocity were not statistically significant. We did not observe reciprocal connections in other paired neuron combinations (i.e. athorny-thorny or vice-versa). Coconnectivity does not have any effect on the model simulations, as the model includes thousands of neurons grouped in populations without specific sub-structures. It might, however, be more relevant if the excitatory populations were further subdivided in assemblies.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Specify which part of CA3 you are recording from.

      We have added this information into our results section - we recorded from 20 cells in CA3a, 274 cells in CA3b and 54 cells in CA3c. This information can now be found in the text on lines 68-69.

      (2) Comment on why you might observe a larger fraction of athorny cells than Hunt et al.

      Hunt et al. cite a broad range for the fraction of athorny cells in their discussion (10-20%). It is unclear where these estimates originate from. In their study, Hunt et al. use the bursting and nonbursting phenotypes as proxies for athorny and thorny cells respectively, and report here numbers of 32 and 70 equating to 31% athorny and 69% thorny. This fraction of athorny cells is more or less in line with our own findings, albeit slightly lower (34% and 66%). However, we believe this difference falls within the range of experimental variability. One caveat is that our electrophysiological recordings likely represent a biased sample of cells. In particular, with multipatch recordings, placement of later electrodes is often restricted to the borders of the pyramidal layer so as not to disturb already patched cells. Thus, our recorded cells do not represent a fully random sample of CA3 pyramidal cells. We believe that, only once a reliable genetic marker for athorny cells has been established can the size of this cell population be properly estimated. Furthermore, the ratio of thorny and athorny cells varies along the proximal distal axis of the CA3 so differences in ratios seen between our study and Hunt et al. may arise from sampling differences along this axis.

      (3) In Figure 3, Aiii (the cell fractions) could also be represented as a vector of two squares stacked one on top of the other, then you could add multiplication signs between Ai, Aii and Aiii, and an equal sign between Aiii and Aiv.

      Thank you! We have implemented this very nice suggestion.

      (4) In Figure 4A, it would be helpful to display the strength of the connections similar to how it is done in Figure 3B.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have now updated Fig 4A to include connection strengths.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Cognitive and brain development during the first two years of life is vast and determinant for later development. However, longitudinal infant studies are complicated and restricted to occidental high-income countries. This study uses fNIRS to investigate the developmental trajectories of functional connectivity networks in infants from a rural community in Gambia. In addition to resting-state data collected from 5 to 24 months, the authors collected growing measures from birth until 24 months and administrated an executive functioning task at 3 or 5 years old.

      The results show left and right frontal-middle and right frontal-posterior negative connections at 5 months that increase with age (i.e., become less negative). Interestingly, contrary to previous findings in high-income countries, there was a decrease in frontal interhemispheric connectivity. Restricted growth during the first months of life was associated with stronger frontal interhemispheric connectivity and weaker right frontal-posterior connectivity at 24 months. Additionally, the study describes that some connectivity patterns related to better cognitive flexibility at pre-school age.

      Strengths:

      - The authors analyze data from 204 infants from a rural area of Gambia, already a big sample for most infant studies. The study might encourage more research on different underrepresented infant populations (i.e., infants not living in occidental high-income countries).

      - The study shows that fNIRS is a feasible instrument to investigate cognitive development when access to fMRI is not possible or outside a lab setting.

      - The fNIRS data preprocessing and analysis are well-planned, implemented, and carefully described. For example, the authors report how the choices in the parameters for the motion artifacts detection algorithm affect data rejection and show how connectivity stability varies with the length of the data segment to justify the threshold of at least 250 seconds free of artifacts for inclusion.

      - The authors use proper statistical methods for analysis, considering the complexity of the dataset.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of this work.

      Weaknesses:

      - No co-registration of the optodes is implemented. The authors checked for correct placement by looking at pictures taken during the testing session. However, head shape and size differences might affect the results, especially considering that the study involves infants from 5 months to 24 months and that the same fNIRS array was used at all ages.

      The fNIRS array used in this work was co-registered onto age-appropriate MNI templates at every time point in a previous published work L. H. Collins-Jones, et al., Longitudinal infant fNIRS channel-space analyses are robust to variability parameters at the group-level: An image reconstruction investigation. Neuroimage 237, 118068 (2021). This is reference No. 68 in the manuscript.

      As we mentioned in the section fNIRS preprocessing and data-analysis: ‘The sections were established via the 17 channels of each hemisphere which were grouped into front, middle and back (for a total of six regions) based on a previous co-registration of the BRIGHT fNIRS arrays onto age-appropriate templates’. The procedure mentioned by the reviewer, involving the examination of pictures showing the placement of headbands on participants, aimed to exclude infants with excessive cap displacement from further analysis.

      - The authors regress the global signal to remove systemic physiological noise. While the authors also report the changes in connectivity without global signal regression, there are some critical differences. In particular, the apparent decrease in frontal inter-hemispheric connections is not present when global signal regression is omitted, even though it is present for deoxy-Hb. The authors use connectivity results obtained after applying global signal regression for further analysis. The choice of regressing the global signal is questionable since it has been shown to introduce anti-correlations in fMRI data (Murphy et al., 2009), and fNIRS in young infants does not seem to be highly affected by physiological noise (Emberson et al., 2016). Systemic physiological noise might change at different ages, which makes its remotion critical to investigate functional network development. However, global signal regression might also affect the data differently. The study would have benefited from having short separation channels to measure the systemic psychological component in the data.

      The work of Emberson et. al (2016) mentioned by the reviewer highlights indeed the challenges of removing systemic changes from the infants’ haemodynamic signal with short-channel separation (SSC). In fact, even a SSC of 1 cm detected changes in the blood in the brain, therefore by regressing this signal from the recorded one, the authors removed both systemic changes AND haemodynamic signal. This paper from Emberson et. al (2016) is taken as a reference in the field to suggest that SSC might not be an ideal tool to remove systemic changes when collecting fNIRS data on young infants, as we did in this work.

      We agree with the reviewer's observation that systemic physiological noise may vary with age and among infants. Therefore, for each infant at each age, we regressed the mean value calculated across all channels. This ensures that the regressed signal is not biased by averaged calculations at group levels.

      We are aware of the criticisms directed towards global signal regression in the fMRI literature, although some other works showed anticorrelations in functional connectivity networks both with and without global signal regression (Chaia, 2012). Furthermore, Murphy himself revised his criticism on the use of global signal regression in functional connectivity analysis in one of his more recent works (Murphy et al, 2017). The fact that the decreased FC is significant in results from data pre-processed without global signal regression gives us confidence that this finding is statistically robust and not solely driven by this preprocessing choice in our pipeline.

      An interesting study by Abdalmalak et al. (2022) demonstrated that failing to correct for systemic changes using any method is inappropriate when estimating FC with fNIRS, as it can lead to a high risk of elevated connectivity across the whole brain (see Figure 4 of the mentioned paper). Consequently, we strongly advocate for the implementation of global signal regression in our analysis pipeline as a fundamental step for accurate functional connectivity estimations.

      References:

      Emberson, L. L., Crosswhite, S. L., Goodwin, J. R., Berger, A. J., & Aslin, R. N. (2016). Isolating the effects of surface vasculature in infant neuroimaging using short-distance optical channels: a combination of local and global effects. Neurophotonics, 3(3), 031406-031406.

      Chaia, X. J., Castañóna, A. N., Öngürb, D., & Whitfield-Gabrielia, S. (2012). Anticorrelations in resting state networks without global signal regression. NeuroImage, 59(2), 1420–1428. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783050076010-014

      Murphy, K., & Fox, M. D. (2017). Towards a consensus regarding global signal regression for resting state functional connectivity MRI. NeuroImage, 154(November 2016), 169–173. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.052

      Abdalmalak, A., Novi, S. L., Kazazian, K., Norton, L., Benaglia, T., Slessarev, M., ... & Owen, A. M. (2022). Effects of systemic physiology on mapping resting-state networks using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Frontiers in neuroscience, 16, 803297.

      - I believe the authors bypass a fundamental point in their framing. When discussing the results, the authors compare the developmental trajectories of the infants tested in a rural area of Gambia with the trajectories reported in previous studies on infants growing in occidental high-income countries (likely in urban contexts) and attribute the differences to adverse effects (i.e., nutritional deficits). Differences in developmental trajectories might also derive from other environmental and cultural differences that do not necessarily lead to poor cognitive development.

      We agree with the reviewer that other factors differing between low- and poor-resource settings might have an impact on FC trajectories. We therefore specified this in the discussion as follows: “We acknowledge that differences in FC could also be attributed to other environmental and cultural disparities between high-resource and low-resource settings, and future studies are needed to investigate this further” (line 238).

      - While the study provides a solid description of the functional connectivity changes in the first two years of life at the group level, the evidence regarding the links between adverse situations, developmental trajectories, and later cognitive capacities is weaker. The authors find that early restricted growth predicts specific connectivity patterns at 24 months and that certain connectivity patterns at specific ages predict cognitive flexibility. However, the link between development trajectories (individual changes in connectivity) with growth and later cognitive capacities is missing. To address this question adequately, the study should have compared infants with different growing profiles or those who suffered or did not from undernutrition. However, as the authors discussed, they lacked statistical power.

      We agree with the reviewer, and indeed we highlighted this as one of the main limitation of our work: “Even given the large sample in our study, we were underpowered to test for group comparisons between sets of infants with distinct undernutrition growth profiles, e.g., infants with early poor growth that later resolved and infants with standard growth early that had a poor growth later. We were also underpowered to test the associations between early growth and FC on clinically undernourished infants (defined as having DWLZ two standard deviations below the mean) (line 311, discussion section).

      We believe this is an important point to consider for the field, as it addresses the sample size required for studies investigating brain development in clinically malnourished infants. We hope this will serve as a valuable reference for future studies in the field. For example, a new study led by Prof. Sophie Moore and other members of the BRIGHT team (INDiGO) is currently recruiting six-hundreds pregnant women with the aim of obtaining a broader distribution of infants’ growth measures (https://www.kcl.ac.uk/research/sophie-moore-research-group).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary and strengths:

      The article pertains to a topic of importance, specifically early life growth faltering, a marker of undernutrition, and how it influences brain functional connectivity and cognitive development. In addition, the data collection was laborious, and data preprocessing was quite rigorous to ensure data quality, utilizing cutting-edge preprocessing methods.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of this work.

      Weaknesses:

      However, the subsequent analysis and explanations were not very thorough, which made some results and conclusions less convincing. For example, corrections for multiple tests need to be consistently maintained; if the results do not survive multiple corrections, they should not be discussed as significant results. Additionally, alternative plans for analysis strategies could be worth exploring, e.g., using ΔFC in addition to FC at a certain age. Lastly, some analysis plans lacked a strong theoretical foundation, such as the relationship between functional connectivity (FC) between certain ROIs and the development of cognitive flexibility.

      Thus, as much as I admire the advanced analysis of connectivity that was conducted and the uniqueness of longitudinal fNIRS data from these samples (even the sheer effort to collect fNIRS longitudinally in a low-income country at such a scale!), I have reservations about the importance of this paper's contribution to the field in its present form. Major revisions are needed, in my opinion, to enhance the paper's quality. 

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding the reporting of results that do not survive multiple comparisons. However, considering the uniqueness of our dataset and the novelty of our work, we believe it is crucial to report all significant findings as well as hypothesis-generating findings that may not pass stringent significance thresholds. We have taken great care to transparently distinguish between results that survived multiple comparisons and those that did not in both the Results and Discussion sections, ensuring that readers are not misled. It is possible that future studies may replicate and further strengthen these associations. Therefore, by sharing these results with the research community, we provide valuable insights for future investigations.

      The relationship between FC and cognitive flexibility (as well as the relationship between growth and FC) has been explored focusing on those FC that showed a significant change with age, as specified in the results sections: ‘To investigate the impact of early nutritional status on FC at 24 months, we used multiple regression with the infant growth trajectory [...] and FC at 24 months [...]. To maximise power, we considered only those FC that showed a statistically significant change with age’ (line 183) and ‘To investigate whether FC early in life predicted cognitive flexibility at preschool age, we used multiple regression of FC across the first two years of life against later cognitive flexibility in preschoolers at three and five years. As per the analysis above, we focused on only those FC that showed a statistically significant change with age’ (line 198).

      We explored the possibility of investigating the relationship between changes in FC and changes in growth. However, the degrees of freedom in these analyses dropped dramatically (~25/30), thereby putting the significance and the meaning of the results at risk. We look forward to future longitudinal studies with less attrition across these time points to maintain the statistical power necessary to run such analyses.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study aimed to investigate whether the development of functional connectivity (FC) is modulated by early physical growth and whether these might impact cognitive development in childhood. This question was investigated by studying a large group of infants (N=204) assessed in Gambia with fNIRS at 5 visits between 5 and 24 months of age. Given the complexity of data acquisition at these ages and following data processing, data could be analyzed for 53 to 97 infants per age group. FC was analyzed considering 6 ensembles of brain regions and thus 21 types of connections. Results suggested that: i) compared to previously studied groups, this group of Gambian infants have different FC trajectory, in particular with a change in frontal inter-hemispheric FC with age from positive to null values; ii) early physical growth, measured through weight-for-length z-scores from birth on, is associated with FC at 24 months. Some relationships were further observed between FC during the first two years and cognitive flexibility at 4-5 years of age, but results did not survive corrections for multiple comparisons.

      Strengths:

      The question investigated in this article is important for understanding the role of early growth and undernutrition on brain and behavioral development in infants and children. The longitudinal approach considered is highly relevant to investigate neurodevelopmental trajectories. Furthermore, this study targets a little-studied population from a low-/middle-income country, which was made possible by the use of fNIRS outside the lab environment. The collected dataset is thus impressive and it opens up a wide range of analytical possibilities.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of this work.

      Weaknesses:

      - Analyzing such a huge amount of collected data at several ages is not an easy task to test developmental relationships between growth, FC, and behavioral capacities. In its present form, this study and the performed analyses lack clarity, unity and perhaps modeling, as it suggests that all possible associations were tested in an exploratory way without clear mechanistic hypotheses. Would it be possible to specify some hypotheses to reduce the number of tests performed? In particular, considering metrics at specific ages or changes in the metrics with age might allow us to test different hypotheses: the authors might clarify what they expect specifically for growth-FC-behaviour associations. Since some FC measures and changes might be related to one another, would it be reasonable to consider a dimensionality reduction approach (e.g., ICA) to select a few components for further correlation analyses?

      We confirm that this work was motivated by a compelling theoretical question: whether neural mechanisms, specifically FC, can be influenced by early adversity, such as growth, and subsequently impact cognitive outcomes, such as cognitive flexibility. This aligns with the overarching goal of the BRIGHT project, established in 2015 (Lloyd-Fox, 2023). We believe this was evident throughout the manuscript in several instances, for example:

      - “The goal of the study was to investigate early physical growth in infancy, developmental trajectories of brain FC across the first two years of life, and cognitive outcome at school age in a longitudinal cohort of infants and children from rural Gambia, an environment with high rates of maternal and child undernutrition. Specifically, we aimed to: (i) investigate whether differences in physical growth through the first two years of life are related to FC at 24 months, and (ii) investigate if trajectories of early FC have an impact on cognitive outcome at pre-school age in these children.” (page 4, introduction)

      - “This study investigated how early adversity via undernutrition drives longitudinal changes in brain functional connectivity at five time points throughout the first two years of life and how these developmental trajectories are associated with cognitive flexibility at preschool age.” (page 6, discussion)

      - We had a clear hypothesis regarding short-range connectivity decreasing with age and long-range connectivity increasing with age, as stated at the end of the introduction: We hypothesized that (i) long-range FC would increase and short-range FC would decrease throughout the first two years of life” (page 4, line 147). However, we were not able to formulate clear hypotheses about the localization of these connections due to the scarcity of previous studies conducted within this age range, particularly in low-resource settings. The ROI approach for analysis was chosen to mitigate this challenge by reducing the number of comparisons while still enabling us to estimate the developmental trajectories of all the connections from which we acquired data.

      Regarding the use of dimensionality reduction approach, we have not considered the use of ICA in our analysis. These methods require selecting a fixed number of components to remove from all participants. However, due to the high variability of infant fNIRS data across the five timepoints, we considered it untenable to precisely determine the number of components to remove at the group level. Such a procedure carries the risk of over-cleaning the data for some participants while leaving noise in for others (Di Lorenzo, 2019). We also felt that using PCA in this initial study would be beyond the scope of the brain-region-specific hypotheses and would be more appropriate in a follow-up analysis of these important data.

      References:

      Lloyd-Fox, S., McCann, S., Milosavljevic, B., Katus, L., Blasi, A., Bulgarelli, C., Crespo-Llado, M., Ghillia, G., Fadera, T., Mbye, E., Mason, L., Njai, F., Njie, O., Perapoch-Amado, M., Rozhko, M., Sosseh, F., Saidykhan, M., Touray, E., Moore, S. E., … Team, and the B. S. (2023). The Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) Study: Cohort Study Protocol. Gates Open Research, 7(126).

      Di Lorenzo, R., Pirazzoli, L., Blasi, A., Bulgarelli, C., Hakuno, Y., Minagawa, Y., & Brigadoi, S. (2019). Recommendations for motion correction of infant fNIRS data applicable to multiple data sets and acquisition systems. NeuroImage, 200(April), 511–527.

      - It seems that neurodevelopmental trajectories over the whole period (5-24 months) are little investigated, and considering more robust statistical analyses would be an important aspect to strengthen the results. The discussion mentions the potential use of structural equation modelling analyses, which would be a relevant way to better describe such complex data.

      We appreciate the complexity of the dataset we are working with, which includes multiple measures and time points. Currently, our focus within the outputs from the BRIGHT project is on examining the relationship between selected measures. While this may not involve statistically advanced modelling at the moment, it is worth noting that most of the results presented in this work have survived correction for multiple comparisons, indicating their statistical robustness. We believe that more advanced statistical analyses are beyond the scope of this rich initial study. In the next phase of the project, known as BRIGHT IMPACT, our team is collaborating with statisticians and experts in statistical modelling to apply more sophisticated and advanced statistical techniques to the data.

      - Given the number of analyses performed, only describing results that survive correction for multiple comparisons is required. Unifying the correction approach (FDR / Bonferroni) is also recommended. For the association between cognitive flexibility and FC, results are not significant, and one might wonder why FC at specific ages was considered rather than the change in FC with age. One of the relevant questions of such a study would be whether early growth and later cognitive flexibility are related through FC development, but testing this would require a mediation analysis that was not performed.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding the reporting of results that do not survive multiple comparisons. However, considering the uniqueness of our dataset and the novelty of our work, we believe it is crucial to report all significant findings. We have taken great care to transparently distinguish between results that survived multiple comparisons and those that did not in both the Results and Discussion sections, ensuring that readers are not misled. It is possible that future studies may replicate and further strengthen these associations. Therefore, by sharing these results with the research community, we provide valuable insights for future investigations.

      We did not perform a mediation analysis as i) ΔWLZ between birth and the subsequent time points positively predicted frontal interhemispheric FC at 24 months, ii) frontal interhemispheric FC at 18 months (and right fronto-posterior connectivity at 24 months) predicted cognitive flexibility at preschool age. Considering that the frontal interhemispheric FC at 24 months that was positively predicted by growth, did not significantly predicted cognitive outcome at preschool age, we did not perform mediation models.

      The reviewer raised concerns about using different methods to correct for multiple comparisons throughout the work. Results showing changes in FC with age were Bonferroni corrected, while we used FDR correction for the regression analyses investigating the relationship between growth and FC, as well as FC and cognitive flexibility. Both methods have good control over Type I errors (false positives), but Bonferroni is very conservative, increasing the likelihood of Type II errors (false negatives). We considered Bonferroni an appropriate method for correcting results showing changes in FC with age, where we had a large sample with strong statistical power (i.e. linear mixed models with 132 participants who had at least 250 seconds of good data for 2 out of 5 visits). However, Bonferroni was too conservative for the regression analyses, with N between 57 and 78) (Acharya, 2014; Félix & Menezes, 2018; Narkevich et al., 2020; Narum, 2006; Olejnik et al., 1997).

      References:

      Acharya, A. (2014). A Complete Review of Controlling the FDR in a Multiple Comparison Problem Framework--The Benjamini-Hochberg Algorithm. ArXiv Preprint ArXiv:1406.7117.

      Félix, V. B., & Menezes, A. F. B. (2018). Comparisons of ten corrections methods for t-test in multiple comparisons via Monte Carlo study. Electronic Journal of Applied Statistical Analysis, 11(1), 74–91.

      Narkevich, A. N., Vinogradov, K. A., & Grjibovski, A. M. (2020). Multiple comparisons in biomedical research: the problem and its solutions. Ekologiya Cheloveka (Human Ecology), 27(10), 55–64.

      Narum, S. R. (2006). Beyond Bonferroni: less conservative analyses for conservation genetics. Conservation Genetics, 7, 783–787.

      Olejnik, S., Li, J., Supattathum, S., & Huberty, C. J. (1997). Multiple testing and statistical power with modified Bonferroni procedures. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 22(4), 389–406.

      - Growth is measured at different ages through different metrics. Justifying the use of weight-for-length z-scores would be welcome since weight-for-age z-scores might be a better marker of growth and possible undernutrition (this impacting potentially both weight and length). Showing the distributions of these z-scores at different ages would allow the reader to estimate the growth variability across infants.

      We consistently used WLZ as the metric to measure growth throughout. Our analysis investigating the relationship between WLZ and growth included HCZ at 7/14 days to correct for head size at birth. When selecting the best growth measure for this paper, we opted for WLZ over WAZ, given extant evidence that infants in our sample are smaller and shorter compared to the reference WHO standard for the same age group (Nabwera et al., 2017). Therefore, using WLZ allows us to adjust each infant's weight for its own length.

      References:

      Nabwera, H. M., Fulford, A. J., Moore, S. E., & Prentice, A. M. (2017). Growth faltering in rural Gambian children after four decades of interventions: a retrospective cohort study. The Lancet Global Health, 5(2), e208–e216.

      - Regarding FC, clarifications about the long-range vs short-range connections would be welcome, as well as drawing a summary of what is expected in terms of FC "typical" trajectory, for the different brain regions and connections, as a marker of typical development. For instance, the authors suggest that an increase in long-range connectivity vs a decrease in short-range is expected based on previous fNIRS studies. However anatomical studies of white matter growth and maturation would suggest the reverse pattern (short-range connections developing mostly after birth, contrarily to long-range connections prenatally).

      We expected an increase in long-range functional connectivity with age, as discussed in the introduction:

      - “Based on data from fMRI, current models hypothesize that FC patterns mature throughout early development (23–27), where in typically developing brains, adult-like networks emerge over the first years of life as long-range functional connections between pre-frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital regions become stronger and more selective (28–31). This maturation in FC has been shown to be related to the cascading maturation of myelination and synaptogenesis (32, 33) - fundamental processes for healthy brain development (34)” (line 93, page 3, introduction);

      - “Importantly, normative developmental patterns may be disrupted and even reversed in clinical conditions that impact development; e.g., increased short-range and reduced long-range FC have been observed in preterm infants (36) and in children with autism spectrum disorder (37, 38)” (line 103, page 3, introduction);

      - “We hypothesized that (i) long-range FC would increase and short-range FC would decrease throughout the first two years of life” (line 147, page 4, introduction).

      Since inferences about FC patterns recorded with fNIRS are highly limited by the number and locations of the optodes, it is challenging to make strong inferences about specific brain regions. Moreover, infant FC fNIRS studies are still limited, which is why we focused our inferences on long-range versus short-range connectivity, without specifically pinpointing particular brain regions.

      Additionally, were unable to locate the works mentioned by the reviewer regarding an increase in short-range white matter connectivity immediately after birth. On the contrary, we found several studies documenting an increase in white-matter long-range connectivity after birth, which is consistent with the hypothesised increase in FC long-range connectivity, such as:

      Yap, P. T., Fan, Y., Chen, Y., Gilmore, J. H., Lin, W., & Shen, D. (2011). Development trends of white matter connectivity in the first years of life. PloS one, 6(9), e24678.

      Dubois, J., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kulikova, S., Poupon, C., Hüppi, P. S., & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2014). The early development of brain white matter: a review of imaging studies in fetuses, newborns and infants. Neuroscience, 276, 48-71.

      Stephens, R. L., Langworthy, B. W., Short, S. J., Girault, J. B., Styner, M. A., & Gilmore, J. H. (2020). White matter development from birth to 6 years of age: a longitudinal study. Cerebral Cortex, 30(12), 6152-6168.

      Hagmann, P., Sporns, O., Madan, N., Cammoun, L., Pienaar, R., Wedeen, V. J., ... & Grant, P. E. (2010). White matter maturation reshapes structural connectivity in the late developing human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(44), 19067-19072.

      Collin G, van den Heuvel MP. The ontogeny of the human connectome: development and dynamic changes of brain connectivity across the life span. Neuroscientist. 2013 Dec;19(6):616-28. doi: 10.1177/1073858413503712.

      The authors test associations between FC and growth, but making sense of such modulation results is difficult without a clearer view of developmental changes per se (e.g., what does an early negative FC mean? Is it an increase in FC when the value gets close to 0? In particular, at 24m, it seems that most FC values are not significantly different from 0, Figure 2B). Observing positive vs negative association effects depending on age is quite puzzling. It is also questionable, for some correlation analyses with cognitive flexibility, to focus on FC that changes with age but to consider FC at a given age.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up this important point and understand that it requires some additional consideration. The negative FC values decreasing with age indicate that these regions go from being anti-correlated to becoming increasingly correlated. Hence, FC of these ROIs increased with age. The trajectory seems to suggest that this will keep increasing with age but of course further data need to be collected to assess this.

      Unfortunately, when considering ΔFC to predict cognitive flexibility, the numbers of participants dropped significantly, with N=~15/20 infants per group of preschoolers, making it very challenging to interpret the results with meaningful statistical power.

      - The manuscript uses inappropriate terms "to predict", "prediction" whereas the conducted analyses are not prediction analyses but correlational.

      We thank the reviewer for giving us to opportunity to thoroughly revise the manuscript about this matter. In this work, we had clear hypotheses regarding which variables predicted which certain measures (such as growth predicting FC and FC predicting cognitive outcomes). Therefore, we performed regression analyses rather than correlational analyses to investigate these associations. Hence, we believe that using the term ‘predict and ‘prediction’ is appropriate

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) In the introduction and discussion, the authors talk about the link between developmental trajectories and cognitive capacities, and undernutrition. However, they did not compare developmental trajectories but connectivity patterns at different ages with ΔWLZ and cognitive flexibility. I recommend that the authors rephrase the introduction and discussion.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out places requiring better clarity in the text. We made edits through the introduction to better match our investigations. In particular we changed:

      - ‘our understanding of the relationships between early undernutrition, developmental trajectories of brain connectivity, and later cognitive outcomes is still very limited,’ to, ‘our understanding of the relationships between early undernutrition, brain connectivity, and later cognitive outcomes is still very limited’ (line 89, introduction);

      - ‘(ii) investigate if trajectories of early FC have an impact on cognitive outcome at pre-school age in these children,’ to, ‘(ii) investigate if early FC has an impact on cognitive outcome at pre-school age in these children’ (line 137, introduction);

      - ‘This study investigated how early adversity via undernutrition drives longitudinal changes in brain functional connectivity at five time points throughout the first two years of life and how these developmental trajectories are associated with cognitive flexibility at preschool age,’ to, ‘This study investigated how early adversity via undernutrition drives brain functional connectivity throughout the first two years of life and how these early functional connections are associated with cognitive flexibility at preschool age’ (line 215, discussion).

      (2) Considering most research is done in occidental high-income countries, and this work is one of the few presenting research in another context, I think the authors should discuss in the manuscript that differences with previous studies might also be due to environmental and cultural differences. Since the study lacks the statistical power to perform a statistical analysis that directly establishes a link between developmental trajectories and restricted growth and cognitive flexibility, the authors cannot disentangle which differences are related to undernutrition and which might result from growing up in a different environment. I recommend that the authors avoid phrases like (lines 57-58): "We observed that early physical growth before the fifth month of life drove optimal developmental trajectories of FC..." or (lines 223-224) "...our cohort of Gambian infants exhibit atypical developmental trajectories of functional connectivity...".

      We thank the reviewer for this observation, and we agree with the reviewer that other factors differing between low- and poor-resource settings might have an impact on FC trajectories. We therefore specified this in the discussion as follows: “We acknowledge that differences in FC could also be attributed to other environmental and cultural disparities between high-resource and low-resource settings, and future studies are needed to explore this further” (line 238). We revised the whole manuscript to reflect similar statements.

      (3) To better interpret the results, it would be interesting to know if poor early growth predicts late cognitive flexibility in the tested sample and if the ΔWLZ distributions differ compared to a population in a high-income country where undernutrition is less frequent.

      We explored the relationship between changes in growth and cognitive flexibility in the two preschooler group, but there were no significant associations.

      Mean and SD values of WLZ are reported in Table 3. The values at every age are negative, indicating that the infants' weight-for-length is below the expected norm at all ages. To our knowledge, no other studies have assessed changes in growth in an infant sample with similar closely spaced age time points in high-income countries, making comparisons on growth changes challenging.

      (4) It is unclear why WLZ at birth and HCZ at 7-14 days are included in the models. I imagine this is to ensure that differences are not due to growing restrictions before birth. It would be nice if the authors could explain this.

      As the reviewer pointed out, HCZ at 7-14 days was included to ensure associations between growth and FC are not due to physical differences at birth. This case be considered as a 'baseline' measure for cerebral development, in the same way that WLZ at birth was used as a baseline for physical development. Therefore, we can more confidently  assume that the associations between growth and FC were specific to the impact of change in WLZ postnatally and not confounded by the size or maturity of the infant at birth. We specified this in the manuscript as follows: “These analyses were adjusted by WLZ at birth and HCZ at 7/14 days, to more confidently assume that the associations between growth and FC were specific to the impact of change in WLZ postnatally and not confounded by the size or maturity of the infant at birth” (line 520, statistical analysis section in the method section).

      (5) Right frontal-posterior connections at 24 months negatively correlate with ΔWLZ. Thus, restricted growth results in stronger frontal-posterior connections at 24 months. However, the same connections at 24 months positively correlate with cognitive flexibility (stronger connections predict better cognitive flexibility). Do the authors have any interpretation of this? How could this relate to previous findings of the authors (Bulgarelli et al. 2020), showing first an increase and then a decrease in functional connectivity between frontal and parietal regions?

      We acknowledge that interpreting the negative relationship between changes in growth and fronto-posterior FC at 24 months, alongside the positive association between the same connection and later cognitive flexibility, is challenging. We refrain from relating these findings to those published by Bulgarelli in 2020 due to differences in optode locations and because in that work the decrease in fronto-posterior FC was observed after 24 months (up to 36 months), whereas the endpoint in this study is right at 24 months.

      (6) With the growth of the head, the frontal channels move to more temporal areas, right? Could this determine the decrease in frontal inter-hemisphere connections?

      As shown in Nabwera (2017) head size does not increase that much in Gambian infants, or at least as expected by the WHO standard measures. We have added HCZ mean and SD values per age in Table 3.

      Minor points

      - HCZ is used in line 184 but not defined.

      We thank the reviewer for spotting this, we have now specified HCZ at line 184 as follows: ‘head-circumference z-score (HCZ)’.

      - Table SI2: NIRS not undertaken = the participant was assessed but did want or could not perform... I imagine there is a missing "not".

      We thank the reviewer for spotting this, we have now modified the legend of Table SI2 as follows: ‘the participant was assessed but did not want or could not perform the NIRS assessments.’

      - The authors should explain what weight-for-length is for those who are not familiar with it.

      We have added an explanation of weight-for-length in the experimental design section, line 339 as follows: ‘We then tested for relationships between brain FC at age 24 months with measures of early growth, as indexed by changes in weight-for-length z-scores (reflecting body weight in proportion to attained growth in length) at one month of age, and at each of the four subsequent visits (details provided below).’

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) I am confused about the authors' interpretation that left and right front-middle and right front-back FC increased with age. It appears in Figure 2 that the negative FC among these ROIs should actually decrease with age. This means that as individuals grow older, the FC values between these regions and zero diminished, albeit starting with negative FC (anticorrelation values) in younger age groups.

      Yes, the reviewer is correct. The negative values of the left and right front-middle and right front-back FC decreasing with age indicate that these regions go from being anti-correlated to becoming increasingly correlated. Hence, FC of these ROIs increased with age.

      (2) Are these negative values mentioned above at 24 months still negative? Have t-tests been run to examine the differences from zero?

      As suggested, we performed t-tests against zero for the mentioned FC at 24 months, and only the left and right fronto-middle FC are significantly different than zero (left fronto-middle FC: t(94) = 1.8, p = 0.036; right fronto-middle FC t(94) = 2.7, p = 0.003).

      (3) With so many correlation analyses, have multiple comparisons been consistently controlled for? While I assume this was done according to the Methods section, could the authors clarify whether FDR adjustment was applied to all the p-values at once or to a group of p-values each time? I found the following way of reporting FDR-adjusted p-values quite informative, such as PFDR, 24 pairs of ROIs < 0.05.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. P-values of regression analyses were FDR corrected per connection investigated, i.e. 21 possible ΔWLZ values per connection. We have specified this in the method section as follows: “To ensure statistical reliability, results from the regression analyses on each FC were corrected for multiple comparisons using false discovery rate (FDR)(Benjamini & Hochberg, 1995) per each connection investigated, i.e. 21 possible ΔWLZ values per each connection,” (page 12, Statistical Analyses section).

      (4) Can early growth trajectories predict changes in FC? Why not use ΔWLZ to predict ΔFC?

      Unfortunately, when considering ΔWLZ to predict ΔFC, the numbers of participants dropped significantly, with N=~30 infants, making it very challenging to interpret the results. We believe this emphasizes the importance of recruiting large samples when conducting longitudinal studies involving infants and employing multiple measures.

      (5) I might have missed the rationale, but why weren't the growth changes after 5 months studied?

      ΔWLZ between all time points were assessed as predictors of FC at 24 months. We have specified this at line 183 as follows: ‘we used multiple regression with the infant growth trajectory (delta weight for length z-score between all time points, DWLZ) and FC at 24 months’. As indicated in Table 2 and 3 the associations between ΔWLZ at all time points and FC at 24 months were tested, but only those with DWLZ calculated between birth and 1 month and the subsequent time points were significant. DWLZ between 5 months and the subsequent time points, DWLZ between 8 months and the subsequent time points, DWLZ between 12 months and the subsequent time points, DWLZ between 18 months and the subsequent time points did not significantly predict FC at 24 months. These are highlighted in Table 2 and Figure 3 in blue and marked as NS (non-significant).

      (6) Once more, the advantage of longitudinal data is that it allows us to tap into developmental changes. Analyzing and predicting cognitive development based solely on FC values at a single age stage (i.e., 24 months) would overlook the benefits of a longitudinal design, which is regrettable. I suggest that the authors attempt to use ΔFC for predictions and observe the outcomes.

      As mentioned to point (4) raised by the reviewer, unfortunately, when considering ΔWLZ to predict ΔFC, the numbers of participants dropped significantly, with N=~30 infants, making it very challenging to interpret the results. We believe this emphasizes the importance of recruiting large samples when conducting longitudinal studies involving infants and employing various measures.

      (7) In the section "Early FC predicts cognitive flexibility at preschool age", the authors pointed out that "...,none of these survived FDR correction for multiple comparisons." However, the paper discussed the association between FC at 24 months of age and cognitive flexibility, as it was supported by the statistical analysis in the following sections. If FDR correction cannot be satisfied, I would rephrase the implication/conclusion of the results to suggest that early FC does not predict cognitive flexibility at preschool age.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding the reporting of results that do not survive multiple comparisons. However, considering the uniqueness of our dataset and the novelty of our work, we believe it is crucial to report all significant findings, even those not passing multiple comparisons corrections, as they may motivate hypothesis-generation for future studies. We have taken great care to transparently distinguish between results that survived multiple comparisons and those that did not in both the Results and Discussion sections, ensuring that readers are not misled. It is possible that future studies may replicate and further support these associations. Therefore, by sharing these results with the research community, we provide valuable insights for future investigations.

      Following the reviewer’ suggestion, we specified that results from regression analysis are significant but they did not survive multiple comparisons in the discussion as follows: ‘While our results are consistent with previous studies, we acknowledge that the significant association between early FC and later cognitive flexibility does not withstand multiple comparisons. Therefore, we encourage future studies that may replicate these findings with a larger sample. (line 290, discussion section).

      (8) Have the authors assessed the impact of growth trajectories on cognitive flexibility?

      We explored the relationship between changes in growth and cognitive flexibility in the two preschooler groups, but there were no significant associations.

      (9) Are there no other cognitive or behavioural measures available? Cognitive flexibility is just one domain of cognitive development, and would the impact of undernutrition on cognitive development be domain-specific? There is a lack of theoretical support here. Why choose cognitive flexibility, and should the impact of undernutrition be domain-specific or domain-general?

      We agree with the reviewer that in this work, we chose to focus on one specific cognitive outcome. While this does not imply that the impact of undernutrition is domain-specific, cognitive flexibility, being a core executive function, has been extensively studied in terms of its neural underpinnings using other neuroimaging modalities, especially fMRI (for example see Dajani, 2015; Uddin, 2021).

      Moreover, other studies looking at the effect of adversity on cognitive outcomes focus on specific cognitive skills, such as working memory (Roberts, 2017), reading and arithmetic skills (Soni, 2021).

      We did assess infants also with Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL), although the cognitive flexibility task within the Early Years Toolbox has been specifically designed for preschoolers (Howard, 2015), and this set of tasks has recently been validated in our team in The Gambia (Milosavljevic, 2023).Future works from the BRIGHT team will investigate performance at the MSEL in relation to other variable of the project.

      References:

      D. R. Dajani, L. Q. Uddin, Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 38, 571–578 (2015).

      L. Q. Uddin, Cognitive and behavioural flexibility: neural mechanisms and clinical considerations. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 167–179 (2021).

      Roberts, S. B., Franceschini, M. A., Krauss, A., Lin, P. Y., de Sa, A. B., Có, R., ... & Muentener, P. (2017). A pilot randomized controlled trial of a new supplementary food designed to enhance cognitive performance during prevention and treatment of malnutrition in childhood. Current developments in nutrition, 1(11), e000885.

      Soni, A., Fahey, N., Bhutta, Z. A., Li, W., Frazier, J. A., Moore Simas, T., ... & Allison, J. J. (2021). Early childhood undernutrition, preadolescent physical growth, and cognitive achievement in India: A population-based cohort study. PLoS Medicine, 18(10), e1003838.

      Howard, S. J., & Melhuish, E. (2015). An Early Years Toolbox (EYT) for assessing early executive function, language, self-regulation, and social development: Validity, reliability, and preliminary norms. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 35(3), 255-275.

      Milosavljevic, B., Cook, C. J., Fadera, T., Ghillia, G., Howard, S. J., Makaula, H., ... & Lloyd‐Fox, S. (2023). Executive functioning skills and their environmental predictors among pre‐school aged children in South Africa and The Gambia. Developmental Science, e13407.

      (10) I would review more previous fNIRS studies on infants if they exist (e.g., the work by S Lloyd-Fox, L Emberson, and others). These studies can help identify brain ROIs likely linked to undernutrition and cognitive flexibility. The current analysis methods lean towards exploratory research. This makes the paper more of a proof-of-concept report rather than a strongly theoretically-driven study.

      We thank the reviewer for this important point. While we have reviewed existing fNIRS infant studies, there are no extant works that showed whether specific brain regions are related undernutrition. However, several fMRI studies assessed regions that do support cognitive flexibility, and we mentioned these in the manuscript (for example see Dajani, 2015; Uddin, 2021).

      Other than the BRIGHT project, we are aware of two other projects that assessed the effect of undernutrition on brain development, assessing cognitive outcomes in poor-resource settings:

      - the BEAN project in Bangladesh in which fNIRS data were recorded from the bilateral temporal cortex (i.e. Pirazzoli, 2022);

      - a project in India in which fNIRS data were recorded from frontal, temporal and parietal cortex bilaterally (i.e. Delgado Reyes, 2020)

      The brain regions recorded in these studies largely overlap with the brain regions we recorded from in this study.

      Another aspect to consider is that infants underwent several fNIRS tasks as part of the BRIGHT project, focusing on social processing, deferred imitation, and habituation responses. Therefore, brain regions for data acquisition were chosen to maximize the likelihood of recording meaningful data for all tasks (Lloyd-Fox, 2023). To clarify the text, we specified this information in the methods section (line 383).

      References:

      D. R. Dajani, L. Q. Uddin, Demystifying cognitive flexibility: Implications for clinical and developmental neuroscience. Trends Neurosci. 38, 571–578 (2015).

      Pirazzoli, L., Sullivan, E., Xie, W., Richards, J. E., Bulgarelli, C., Lloyd-Fox, S., ... & Nelson III, C. A. (2022). Association of psychosocial adversity and social information processing in children raised in a low-resource setting: an fNIRS study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 56, 101125.

      Delgado Reyes, L., Wijeakumar, S., Magnotta, V. A., Forbes, S. H., & Spencer, J. P. (2020). The functional brain networks that underlie visual working memory in the first two years of life. NeuroImage, 219, Article 116971.

      Lloyd-Fox, S., McCann, S., Milosavljevic, B., Katus, L., Blasi, A., Bulgarelli, C., Crespo-Llado, M., Ghillia, G., Fadera, T., Mbye, E., Mason, L., Njai, F., Njie, O., Perapoch-Amado, M., Rozhko, M., Sosseh, F., Saidykhan, M., Touray, E., Moore, S. E., … Team, and the B. S. (2023). The Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) Study: Cohort Study Protocol. Gates Open Research, 7(126).

      (11) Last but not least, in the paper, the authors mentioned that fNIRS offers better spatial resolution and anatomical specificity compared to EEG, thereby providing more precise and reliable localization of brain networks. While I partially agree with this perspective, it remains to be explored whether the current fNIRS analysis strategies indeed yield higher spatial resolution. It is hoped that the authors will delve deeper into this discussion in the paper.

      The brain regions of focus were selected based on coregistration work previously conducted at each time point on the array used in this project (Collins-Jones, 2019). We deliberately avoided making claims about small brain regions, considering that head size might increase slightly less with age in The Gambia compared to Western countries (Nabwera, 2017) . However, we maintain that the conclusions drawn in this study offer higher brain-region specificity than could have been  identified with current common EEG methods alone.

      References:

      L. H. Collins-Jones, et al., Longitudinal infant fNIRS channel-space analyses are robust to variability parameters at the group-level: An image reconstruction investigation. Neuroimage 237, 118068 (2021).

      Nabwera, H. M., Fulford, A. J., Moore, S. E., & Prentice, A. M. (2017). Growth faltering in rural Gambian children after four decades of interventions: a retrospective cohort study. The Lancet Global Health, 5(2), e208–e216.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Introduction

      - Among important developmental mechanisms to mention are the development of exuberant connections and the further selection/stabilization of the relevant ones according to environmental stimulation, vs the pruning of others.

      We agree with the reviewer that the development of exuberant connections and subsequent pruning is a universal process of paramount importance during the first years of life. However, after revising our introduction, given the word limit of the journal, we maintained focus on neurodevelopment and early adversity.

      Results

      - Adding a few more information on the 6 sections and 21 connections would be welcome. In particular for within-section FC: how was this computed?

      The 6 sections were created based on the co-registration of the array used in this study at each age in a previous published work L. H. Collins-Jones, et al., Longitudinal infant fNIRS channel-space analyses are robust to variability parameters at the group-level: An image reconstruction investigation. Neuroimage 237, 118068 (2021). This is reference No. 68 in the manuscript.

      As we mentioned in the section fNIRS preprocessing and data-analysis: ‘The sections were established via the 17 channels of each hemisphere which were grouped into front, middle and back (for a total of six regions) based on a previous co-registration of the BRIGHT fNIRS arrays onto age-appropriate templates’.

      The 21 connections were defined as all the possible links between the 6 regions, specifically: the interhemispheric homotopic connections (in orange in Figure SI1), which connect the same regions between hemispheres (i.e., front left with front right); the intrahemispheric connections (in green in Figure SI1), which correlate channels belonging to the same region; the fronto-posterior connections (in blue in Figure SI1), which link front and middle, middle and back, and front and back regions of the same hemisphere; and the crossing interhemispheric connections (non-homotopic interhemispheric, in yellow in Figure SI1), which link the front, middle, and back areas between left and right hemispheres. We added these specifications also in the legend of Figure SI1 for clarity.

      - The denomination intrahemispheric vs fronto-posterior vs crossed connections is not clear. Maybe prefer intra-hemispheric vs inter-hemispheric homotopic vs inter-hemispheric non-homotopic (also in Figure SI1).

      We appreciate the reviewer's suggestion regarding terminology. However, we believe that the term 'inter-hemispheric non-homotopic' could potentially refer to both connections within the same brain hemisphere from front to back and connections crossing between hemispheres, leading to increased confusion. Therefore, we have chosen not to include the term 'non-homotopic' and instead added 'homotopic' to 'interhemispheric' throughout the manuscript to emphasize that these functional connections occur between corresponding regions of the two hemispheres.

      - with time -> with age.

      We replaced “with time” with “with age” as suggested through the manuscript.

      - The description of both HbO2 and HHb results overloads the main text: would it be relevant to present one of the two in Supplementary Information if the results are coherent?

      We understand the reviewer’s concern regarding overloading the results section with reporting both chromophores. However, reporting results for both HbO and HHb is considered a crucial step for publications in the fNIRS field, as emphasized in recent formal guidance (Yücel et al., 2020). One of the strengths of fNIRS compared to fMRI is its ability to record from both chromophores, enabling a more precise characterization of brain activations and oscillations. Moreover, in FC studies like this one, ensuring that HbO and HHb results overlap is an important check that increases confidence in interpreting the findings.

      References:

      Yücel, M. A., von Lühmann, A., Scholkmann, F., Gervain, J., Dan, I., Ayaz, H., Boas, D., Cooper, R. J., Culver, J., Elwell, C. E., Eggebrecht, A. ., Franceschini, M. A., Grova, C., Homae, F., Lesage, F., Obrig, H., Tachtsidis, I., Tak, S., Tong, Y., … Wolf, M. (2020). Best Practices for fNIRS publications. Neurophotonics, 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.8.1.012101

      - HCZ is not defined when first used.

      We thank the reviewer for spotting this, we have now specified HCZ at line 184 as follows: ‘head-circumference z-score (HCZ)’.

      - Choosing the analyzed measures to "maximize power" could be criticised.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concern. However, correlating all the FC values with all changes in growth would have raised an important issue for multiple comparisons. We therefore we made a priori decision to focus on investigating the relationship between changes in growth and those FC that showed a significant change with age, considering these as the most interesting ones from a developmental perspective in our sample.

      Discussion

      - I would recommend using the same order to synthesize results and further discuss them.

      We agree with the reviewer that the suggested structure is optimal for a clear discussion section. We have indeed followed it, with each paragraph covering specific aspects:

      - Recap of the study aims

      - Results summary and discussion of developmental changes

      - Results summary and discussion of the relationship between changes in growth and FC

      - Results summary and discussion of the relationship between FC and cognitive flexibility

      - Limitations

      - Conclusion

      Given the numerous results presented in this paper, we believe that readers will better digest them by first reading a summary of the results followed by their interpretations, rather than condensing all the interpretations together.

      - Highlighting how "atypical" developmental trajectories are in Gambian infants would be welcome in the Results section. Other interpretations can be found than "The observed decrease in frontal inter-hemispheric FC with increasing age may be due to the exposure to early life undernutrition adversity".

      We agree with the reviewer that other factors that differ between low- and high-resource settings might have an impact on FC trajectories. We therefore specified this in the discussion as follows: “We acknowledge that differences in FC could also be attributed to other environmental and cultural disparities between high-resource and low-resource settings, and future studies are needed to further investigate cultural, environmental, and genetic effects on brain FC” (line 238).

      - Focusing on FC at 24m for the relationship with growth is questionable.

      Correlating the FC values at 5 time points with all changes in growth would have raised an important issue for multiple comparisons. We therefore we made a decision a priori to focus on investigating the relationship between changes in growth and FC at 24 months as our final time point of data collection. We added this information in the methods section as follows: “To investigate the impact of undernutrition on FC development, we used DWLZ as independent variables in regression analyses on HbO2 (as the chromophore with the highest signal-to-noise ratio) FC at 24 months, our final time point of data collection” (line 517, method section).

      - There is too much emphasis on the correlation between FC and cognitive flexibility, whereas results are not significant after correction for multiple comparisons.

      Following the reviewer’ suggestion, we specified that results from regression analysis are significant but they did not survive multiple comparisons in the discussion as follows: While our results are consistent with previous studies, we acknowledge that the significant association between early FC and later cognitive flexibility does not withstand multiple comparisons. Therefore, we encourage future studies that may replicate these findings with a larger sample. (line 290, discussion section).

      Methods

      - I would recommend detailing how z-scores were computed in the paragraph "Anthropometric measures".

      We specified how z-scores were computed in the statistical analysis section as follows: “Anthropometric measures were converted to age and sex adjusted z‐scores that are based on World Health Organization Child Growth Standards (93). Weight‐for‐Length (WLZ) and Head Circumference (HCZ) z-scores were computed” (line 509, method section). As transforming data is the first step of statistical analysis and is not directly related to data collection, we believe it is more appropriate to retain this description in the statistical analysis section.

      - FC computation: the mention of "correlating the first and the last 250s" is not clear.

      We specified this more clearly in the text as follows: We found that correlating the first and the last 250 seconds of valid data after pre-processing provided the highest percentage of infants with strong correlation between the first and the last portion of data (line 467).

      - The manuscript mentions "age 3 years" for the younger preschoolers but ~48months rather corresponds to 4 years.

      We revised the entire manuscript and the supplementary materials, but we could not find any instance in which preschoolers are referred with age in months rather than in years.

      - Specify the number of children evaluated at 4 and 5 years. Is the test of cognitive flexibility normalized for age? If not, how were the 2 groups considered in the analyses? (age as a confounding factor).

      We have added the number of children in the two preschooler groups as follows: younger preschoolers (age mean ± SD=47.96 ± 2.77 months, N=77) and older preschoolers (age mean ± SD=57.58 ± 2.11 months, N=84). (line 484).

      The cognitive flexibility test was not normalized for age, as this task was specifically developed for preschoolers (Howard, 2015). As mentioned in ‘Cognitive flexibility at preschool age’ of the methods section, “data were collected in two ranges of preschool ages”, which guided our decision to perform regression analysis on the impact of FC on cognitive flexibility separately within these two age groups, rather than treating them as a single group of preschoolers.

      References:

      Howard, S. J., & Melhuish, E. (2015). An Early Years Toolbox (EYT) for assessing early executive function, language, self-regulation, and social development: Validity, reliability, and preliminary norms. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 35(3), 255-275.

      Figures and Tables

      - Table 1 could highlight the significant results. It is not clear what the "baseline" results correspond to.

      We have marked in bold the results that are statistically significant in Table 1. In the linear mixed model we performed, the first time point (i.e. 5 months) is chosen as ‘baseline’, i.e. the reference against which the other time points are compared to, and its statistical values refer to its significance against 0 (as it has been performed in Bulgarelli 2020).

      - Figures 2 B and C seem redundant? What is SE vs SD?

      We believe that both figures 2B and 2C are useful for the readers. While the first one shows the mean FC values at the group level, the second one highlights the individual variability of FC values (typical of infant neuroimaging data), which also why it is interesting to relate these measures to other variables of our dataset (i.e. growth and cognitive flexibility). Figure 2C also reports mean FC values per age, but these might be less visible considering that also one dot per infant is also plotted.

      SE stands for standard error, and in the legend of the figure we specified this as follows: ‘Mean and standard error of the mean (SE)’. SD stands for standard deviation, and we have now specified this as follows: ‘mean ± standard deviation (SD)’ .

      - Table 2: I would recommend removing results that don't survive corrections for multiple comparisons.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding the reporting of results that do not survive multiple comparisons. However, considering the uniqueness of our dataset and the novelty of our work, we believe it is crucial to report all significant findings. We have taken great care to transparently distinguish between results that survived multiple comparisons and those that did not in both the Results and Discussion sections, ensuring that readers are not misled. It is possible that future studies may replicate and further strengthen these associations. Therefore, by sharing these results with the research community, we provide valuable insights for future investigations.

      - Figure 3: the top is redundant with Table 2: to be merged? B: the statistical results might be shown in a Table.

      We agree with the reviewer that the top part of Figure 3 and Table 2 report the same results. However, given the richness of these findings, we believe that the top part of Figure 3 serves as a useful summary for readers. Additionally, examining both the top and bottom parts of Figure 3 provides a comprehensive overview of the regression analysis conducted in this study.

      - Figure SI6: Is it really a % in x-axis?

      We thank the reviewer for spotting this typo, the percentage is relevant for the y-axis only. We removed the % symbol from ticks of the x-axis.

      - Table SI1: the presented p-values don't seem to survive Bonferroni correction, contrary to what is written.

      We thank the reviewer for spotting this mistake, we removed the reference to the Bonferroni correction for the p-values.

      - Table SI2: For the proportion of children included in the analysis, maybe be precise that the proportion was computed based on the ones with acquired data. Maybe also add the proportion according to all children, to better show the high drop-out rate at certain ages?

      We thank the reviewer for these useful suggestions. We have specified in the legend of the table how we calculated the proportion of infants included as follows: ‘The proportion of children included in the analysis was computed based on the infants with FC data’. We have also added a column in the table called ‘Inclusion rate (from the 204 infants recruited)’, following the reviewer’s suggestion. This will be a useful reference for future studies.

      - A few typos should be corrected throughout the manuscript.

      We thoroughly revised the main manuscript and the supplementary materials for typos.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Building on previous in vitro synaptic circuit work (Yamawaki et al., eLife 10, 2021), Piña Novo et al. utilize an in vivo optogenetic-electrophysiological approach to characterize sensory-evoked spiking activity in the mouse's forelimb primary somatosensory (S1) and motor (M1) areas. Using a combination of a novel "phototactile" somatosensory stimuli to the mouse's hand and simultaneous high-density linear array recordings in both S1 and M1, the authors report in awake mice that evoked cortical responses follow a triphasic peak-suppression-rebound pattern response. They also find that M1 responses are delayed and attenuated relative to S1. Further analysis revealed a 20-fold difference in subcortical versus corticocortical propagation speeds.

      They also report that PV interneurons in S1 are strongly recruited by hand stimulation. Furthermore, they report that selective activation of PV cells can produce a suppression and rebound response similar to "phototactile" stimuli. Lastly, the authors demonstrate that silencing S1 through local PV cell activation reduces M1 response to hand stimulation, suggesting S1 may directly drive M1 responses.

      Strengths:

      The study was technically well done, with convincing results. The data presented are appropriately analyzed. The author's findings build on a growing body of both in vitro and in vivo work examining the synaptic circuits underlying the interactions between S1 and M1. The paper is well-written and illustrated. Overall, the study will be useful to those interested in forelimb S1-M1 interactions.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the results are clear and convincing, one weakness is that many results are consistent with previous studies in other sensorimotor systems, and thus not all that surprising. For example, the findings that sensory stimulation results in delayed and attenuated responses in M1 relative to S1 and that PV inhibitory cells in S1 are strongly recruited by sensory stimulation are not novel (e.g., Bruno et al., J Neurosci 22, 10966-10975, 2002; Swadlow, Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 357, 1717-1727, 2002; Gabernet et al., Neuron 48, 315-327, 2005; Cruikshank et al., Nat Neurosci 10, 462-468, 2007; Ferezou et al., Neuron 56, 907-923, 2007; Sreenivasan et al., Neuron 92, 1368-1382, 2016; Yu et al., Neuron 104, 412-427 e414, 2019). Furthermore, the observation that sensory processing in M1 depends upon activity in S1 is also not novel (e.g., Ferezou et al., Neuron 56, 907-923, 2007; Sreenivasan et al., Neuron 92, 1368-1382, 2016). The authors do a good job highlighting how their results are consistent with these previous studies.

      We thank the reviewer for the close reading of the manuscript and the many constructive comments and critiques. As the reviewer notes, there have been many prior studies of related circuits in other sensorimotor systems forming an important context for our study and findings, as we have tried to highlight. We appreciate the suggestions for additional relevant articles to cite.

      Perhaps a more significant weakness, in my opinion, was the missing analyses given the rich dataset collected. For example, why lump all responsive units and not break them down based on their depth? Given superficial and deep layers respond at different latencies and have different response magnitudes and durations to sensory stimuli (e.g., L2/3 is much more sparse) (e.g., Constantinople et al., Science 340, 1591-1594, 2013; Manita et al., Neuron 86, 1304-1316, 2015; Petersen, Nat Rev Neurosci 20, 533-546, 2019; Yu et al., Neuron 104, 412-427 e414, 2019), their conclusions could be biased toward more active layers (e.g., L4 and L5). These additional analyses could reveal interesting similarities or important differences, increasing the manuscript's impact. Given the authors use high-density linear arrays, they should have this data.

      We have analyzed the activity patterns as a function of cortical depth, and now include these results in the manuscript as suggested. The key new finding is that the M1 responses are strongest in upper layers, consistent with expectations based on the excitatory corticocortical synaptic connectivity characterized previously. Changes to the manuscript include new figures (Figure 5; Figure 5 - figure supplement 1), which we explain (Methods: page 14, lines 618-621), describe (new Results section: pages 4-5, lines 183-189), comment on (Discussion: page 9, lines 378-391), and summarize the significance of (Abstract: page 1, lines 22-24). In addition, we incorporated the new laminar analysis into a summary schematic (Figure 9). We thank the reviewer for suggesting this analysis.

      Similarly, why not isolate and compare PV versus non-PV units in M1? They did the photostimulation experiments and presumably have the data. Recent in vitro work suggests PV neurons in the upper layers (L2/3) of M1 are strongly recruited by S1 (e.g., Okoro et al., J Neurosci 42, 8095-8112, 2022; Martinetti et al., Cerebral cortex 32, 1932-1949, 2022). Does the author's data support these in vitro observations?

      These experiments were relatively complex and M1 optotagging was not routinely included in the stimulus and acquisition protocol. Therefore, we don’t have sufficient data for this analysis. We plan to address this in future studies.

      It would have also been interesting to suppress M1 while stimulating the hand to determine if any part of the S1 triphasic response depends on M1 feedback.

      We agree that this is of interest but consider this to be outside the scope of the current study.

      I appreciate the control experiment showing that optical hand stimulation did not evoke forelimb movement. However, this appears to be an N=1. How consistent was this result across animals, and how was this monitored in those animals? Can the authors say anything about digit movement?

      We have performed additional experiments to address this point. A constraint with EMG is that it is limited to the muscle(s) one chooses to record from, and it is difficult to implant tiny muscles of the hand. Therefore, for this analysis, we used kilohertz videography as a high-sensitivity method for movement surveillance across the hand. Hand stimulation did not evoke any detectable movements. Changes in the manuscript include: revised Figure 1 - figure supplement 1; supplementary Figure 1 - video 1; and associated text edits in the Methods (page 13, line 557; page 14, lines 626-639) and Results sections (page 2, lines 84-85).

      A light intensity of 5 mW was used to stimulate the hand, but it is unclear how or why the authors chose this intensity. Did S1 and M1 responses (e.g., amplitude and latency) change with lower or higher intensities? Was the triphasic response dependent on the intensity of the "phototactile" stimuli?

      As we now say in the Methods > Optogenetic photostimulation of the hand section (page 13, lines 562-565), “This intensity was chosen based on pilot experiments in which we varied the LED power, which showed that this intensity was reliably above the threshold for evoking robust responses in both S1 and M1 without evoking any visually detectable movements (as subsequently confirmed by videography)”.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Communication between sensory and motor cortices is likely to be important for many aspects of behavior, and in this study, the authors carefully analyse neuronal spiking activity in S1 and M1 evoked by peripheral paw stimulation finding clear evidence for sensory responses in both cortical regions

      Strengths:

      The experiments and data analyses appear to have been carefully carried out and clearly represented.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Some studies have found evidence for excitatory projection neurons expressing PV and in particular some excitatory pyramidal cells can be labelled in PV-Cre mice. The authors might want to check if this is the case in their study, and if so, whether that might impact any conclusions.

      Thank you for pointing this out. The prior studies suggest it is mainly a subset of layer 5B excitatory neurons that may express PV. We checked this in two ways. Anatomically, we did not find double-labeling. An electrophysiology assay showed that, although some evoked excitatory synaptic input could be detected in some neurons, these inputs were very weak. Results from these assays are shown in new Figure 6 - figure supplement 1, with associated text edits in the Methods (page 11, lines 469-471; page 15, lines 657-668) and Results (page 5, lines 198-199) sections.

      (2) I think the analysis shown in Figure S1 apparently reporting the absence of movements evoked by the forepaw stimulation could be strengthened. It is unclear what is shown in the various panels. I would imagine that an average of many stimulus repetitions would be needed to indicate whether there is an evoked movement or not. This could also be state-dependent and perhaps more likely to happen early in a recording session. Videography could also be helpful.

      As noted above, we have performed additional experiments to address this.

      (3) Some similar aspects of the evoked responses, including triphasic dynamics, have been reported in whisker S1 and M1, and the authors might want to cite Sreenivasan et al., 2016.

      Thank you for pointing this out; we now cite this article (page 1, line 46; page 10, line 415).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a solid study of stimulus-evoked neural activity dynamics in the feedforward pathway from mouse hand/forelimb mechanoreceptor afferents to S1 and M1 cortex. The conclusions are generally well supported, and match expectations from previous studies of hand/forelimb circuits by this same group (Yamawaki et al., 2021), from the well-studied whisker tactile pathway to whisker S1 and M1, and from the corresponding pathway in primates. The study uses the novel approach of optogenetic stimulation of PV afferents in the periphery, which provides an impulselike volley of peripheral spikes, which is useful for studying feedforward circuit dynamics. These are primarily proprioceptors, so results could differ for specific mechanoreceptor populations, but this is a reasonable tool to probe basic circuit activation. Mice are awake but not engaged in a somatosensory task, which is sufficient for the study goals.

      The main results are:

      (1) brief peripheral activation drives brief sensory-evoked responses at ~ 15 ms latency in S1 and ~25 ms latency in M1, which is consistent with classical fast propagation on the subcortical pathway to S1, followed by slow propagation on the polysynaptic, non-myelinated pathway from S1 to M1;

      (2) each peripheral impulse evokes a triphasic activation-suppression-rebound response in both S1 and M1;

      (3) PV interneurons carry the major component of spike modulation for each of these phases; (4) activation of PV neurons in each area (M1 or S1) drives suppression and rebound both in the local area and in the other downstream area;

      (5) peripheral-evoked neural activity in M1 is at least partially dependent on transmission through S1.

      All conclusions are well-supported and reasonably interpreted. There are no major new findings that were not expected from standard models of somatosensory pathways or from prior work in the whisker system.

      Strengths:

      This is a well-conducted and analyzed study in which the findings are clearly presented. This will provide important baseline knowledge from which studies of more complex sensorimotor processing can build.

      Weaknesses:

      A few minor issues should be addressed to improve clarity of presentation and interpretation:

      (1) It is critical for interpretation that the stimulus does not evoke a motor response, which could induce reafference-based activity that could drive, or mask, some of the triphasic response. Figure S1 shows that no motor response is evoked for one example session, but this would be stronger if results were analyzed over several mice.

      As noted above, we have performed additional experiments to address this point.

      (2) The recordings combine single and multi-units, which is fine for measures of response modulation, but not for absolute evoked firing rate, which is only interpretable for single units. For example, evoked firing rate in S1 could be higher than M1, if spike sorting were more difficult in S1, resulting in a higher fraction of multi-units relative to M1. Because of this, if reporting of absolute firing rates is an essential component of the paper, Figs 3D and 4E should be recalculated just for single units.

      Thank you for noting this. Although the absolute firing rates are not essential for the main findings or conclusions (which as noted focus on response modulations and relative differences) we agree that analyzing the single-unit response amplitudes is useful. Therefore, changes in the manuscript now include: revised Figure 3, and associated text edits in the Methods (page 12, lines 543-545), Results (page 3, lines 115-119), and Discussion (page 7, lines 305-311) sections.

      (3) In Figure 5B, the average light-evoked firing rate of PV neurons seems to come up before time 0, unlike the single-trial rasters above it. Presumably, this reflects binning for firing rate calculation. This should be corrected to avoid confusion.

      Yes, this reflects the binning. We agree that this is potentially confusing and have removed these average plots below the raster plots, as the rasters alone suffice to demonstrate the result (i.e., that PV units are strongly activated and thus tagged by optogenetic stimulation). Changes are now reflected in revised Figure 6.

      (4) In Figure 6A bottom, please clarify what legends "W. suppression" and "W. rebound" mean.

      In the figure plot legends, the “W.” has been removed. Changes are now reflected in revised Figure 7 and Figure 7 – figure supplement 1.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Did you filter the neural signals during acquisition? If so, please include these details in the results.

      Signals were bandpass-filtered (2.5 Hz to 7.6 KHz) at the hardware level at acquisition (with no additional software filtering applied), as now clarified in the Methods Electrophysiological recordings section as requested (page 12, lines: 525-526).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Some studies have found evidence for excitatory projection neurons expressing PV and in particular some excitatory pyramidal cells can be labelled in PV-Cre mice. The authors might want to check if this is the case in their study, and if so, whether that might impact any conclusions.

      Please see above for our response to this issue.

      (2) I think the analysis shown in Figure S1 apparently reporting the absence of movements evoked by the forepaw stimulation could be strengthened. It is unclear what is shown in the various panels. I would imagine that an average of many stimulus repetitions would be needed to indicate whether there is an evoked movement or not. This could also be state-dependent and perhaps more likely to happen early in a recording session. Videography could also be helpful.

      Please see above for our response to this issue.

      (3) Some similar aspects of the evoked responses, including triphasic dynamics, have been reported in whisker S1 and M1, and the authors might want to cite Sreenivasan et al., 2016.

      As noted above, we now cite this study.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors discovered MYL3 of marine medaka (Oryzias melastigma) as a novel NNV entry receptor, elucidating its facilitation of RGNNV entry into host cells through macropinocytosis, mediated by the IGF1R-Rac1/Cdc42 pathway.

      Strengths:

      In this manuscript, the authors have performed in vitro and in vivo experiments to prove that MnMYL3 may serve as a receptor for NNV via macropinocytosis pathway. These experiments with different methods include Co-IP, RNAi, pulldown, SPR, flow cytometry, immunofluorescence assays, and so on. In general, the results are clearly presented in the manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      For the writing in the introduction and discussion sections, the author Yao et al mainly focus on the viral pathogens and fish in Aquaculture, the meaning and novelty of results provided in this manuscript are limited, and not broad in biology. The authors should improve the likely impact of their work on the viral infection field, maybe also in the evolutionary field with the fish model.

      (1) Myosin is a big family, why did authors choose MYL3 as a candidate receptor for NNV?

      We appreciate your insightful question. We selected MYL3 as a candidate receptor based on a combination of proteomic screening and literature evidence, and functional validation. Increasing evidence indicated that myosins have been implicated in viral infections. For instance, myosin heavy chain 9 plays a role in multiple viral infections (Li et al., 2018), and non-muscle myosin heavy chain IIA has been identified as an entry receptor for herpes simplex virus-1 (Arii et al., 2010). Furthermore, myosin II light chain activation is essential for influenza A virus entry via macropinocytosis (Banerjee et al., 2014). Our previous studies hinted at a potential interaction between MYL3 and CP (Zhang et al., 2020). Huang et al also reported that Epinephelus coioides MYL3 might interact with native NNV CP by proteomic analysis of immunoprecipitation (IP) assay (Huang et al., 2020). Our Co-IP and SPR analyses confirmed a direct interaction between MYL3 and the RGNNV CP. Based on these studies, we selected MYL3 as a candidate receptor for NNV.

      References

      Huang PY, Hsiao HC, Wang SW, Lo SF, Lu MW, Chen LL. 2020. Screening for the Proteins That Can Interact with Grouper Nervous Necrosis Virus Capsid Protein. Viruses 12:1–20.

      Li L, Xue B, Sun W, Gu G, Hou G, Zhang L, Wu C, Zhao Q, Zhang Y, Zhang G, Hiscox JA, Nan Y, Zhou EM. 2018. Recombinant MYH9 protein C-terminal domain blocks porcine reproductive and respiratory syndrome virus internalization by direct interaction with viral glycoprotein 5. Antiviral Res 156:10–20.

      Arii J, Goto H, Suenaga T, Oyama M, Kozuka-Hata H, Imai T, Minowa A, Akashi H, Arase H, Kawaoka Y, Kawaguchi Y. 2010. Non-muscle myosin IIA is a functional entry receptor for herpes simplex virus-1.

      Banerjee I, Miyake Y, Philip Nobs S, Schneider C, Horvath P, Kopf M, Matthias P, Helenius A, Yamauchi Y. 2014. Influenza A virus uses the aggresome processing machinery for host cell entry. Science (80- ) 346:473–477.

      (2) What is the relationship between MmMYL3 and MmHSP90ab1 and other known NNV receptors? Why does NNV have so many receptors? Which one is supposed to serve as the key entry receptor?

      We acknowledge the functional diversity of receptors for NNV. MmHSP90ab1 and MmHSC70 have been identified as receptors involved in NNV entry through clathrin-mediated endocytosis (CME), whereas MYL3 facilitates entry via macropinocytosis. These pathways serve as complementary mechanisms for the virus to enter host cells, potentially enhancing infection efficiency. While HSP90ab1 facilitates CME, MYL3 promotes macropinocytosis, both of which are critical for viral internalization, but through distinct endocytic mechanisms.

      NNV likely utilizes multiple receptors to increase its host range and infection efficiency. The diversity of receptors ensures that the virus can infect a wide variety of host species. By employing HSP90ab1, HSC70, and MYL3, NNV can exploit different cellular pathways for entry, making it more adaptable to various host environments.

      Regarding the identification of a key entry receptor, we agree this is a critical unresolved question. While HSP90ab1/HSC70 appear essential for CME-mediated entry, our data suggest MYL3 plays a distinct role in macropinocytic uptake. To systematically evaluate receptor hierarchy, we initially proposed comparative knockout studies targeting these candidate genes. However, we must acknowledge that current technical limitations in marine fish models – particularly the extended generation time for stable knockout cell lines and challenges in maintaining viable cell cultures post-editing – have delayed these experiments. Nevertheless, we are actively exploring strategies to overcome these obstacles and will continue to refine our approach to address these questions in future research.

      (3) In vivo knockout of MYL3 using CRISPR-Cas9 should be conducted to verify whether the absence of MYL3 really inhibits NNV infection. Although it might be difficult to do it in marine medaka as stated by the authors, the introduction of zebrafish is highly recommended, since it has already been reported that zebrafish could serve as a vertebrate model to study NNV (doi: 10.3389/fimmu.2022.863096).

      As noted in our manuscript from line 374 to 384, marine medaka is a relatively new model for studying viral infections and is not yet optimized for CRISPR-Cas9-mediated gene knockout. The technical challenges related to precise embryo microinjection and off-target effects using CRISPR-Cas9 in marine medaka complicate the establishment of knockout lines. These limitations, including the time required for multiple breeding generations and molecular screening, currently make this approach difficult to implement.

      We fully agree with your suggestion to consider zebrafish as an alternative model. Zebrafish have been well-established as a vertebrate model for studying NNV, and their genetic tractability and well-developed CRISPR-Cas9 protocols provide a more accessible and efficient platform for generating knockout models. In our future studies, we plan to conduct CRISPR-Cas9-mediated knockout experiments targeting multiple NNV receptors in zebrafish. This will allow us to systematically evaluate the role of different receptors in NNV infection and elucidate their potential interactions. The findings from these studies will be included in a future publication, which will provide a more comprehensive understanding of the molecular mechanisms underlying NNV infection in vertebrate models.

      (4) The results shown in Figure 6 are not enough to support the conclusion that "RGNNV triggers macropinocytosis mediated by MmMYL3". Additional electron microscopy of macropinosomes (sizes, morphological characteristics, etc.) will be more direct evidence.

      Previous study has reported that dragon grouper nervous necrosis virus (DGNNV) enters SSN-1 cells primarily through micropinocytosis and macropinocytosis pathways. Electron microscopy observations revealed several kinds of membrane ruffling and large disproportionate macropinosomes were observed in DGNNV infected cells, indicating NNV infection could triggers micropinocytosis (Liu et al., 2005). In our study, the data from inhibitor treatments, co-localization of MmMYL3 with RGNNV CP, and dextran uptake assays also provide compelling evidence for the involvement of macropinocytosis in RGNNV entry via MmMYL3. These methods are well-established in the literature and have been used extensively to study viral entry pathways (Lingemann et al., 2019). Specifically, the dextran uptake assay has been widely utilized as a marker for macropinocytosis and has provided clear evidence of RGNNV internalization via this pathway. The use of macropinocytosis inhibitors, such as EIPA and Rottlerin, significantly reduced RGNNV entry, further supporting our conclusion. Nonetheless, we acknowledge the potential value of additional electron microscopy studies and will consider this approach in our future research.

      References

      Liu W, Hsu CH, Hong YR, Wu SC, Wang CH, Wu YM, Chao CB, Lin CS. 2005. Early endocytosis pathways in SSN-1 cells infected by dragon grouper nervous necrosis virus, J Gen Virol.

      Lingemann M, McCarty T, Liu X, Buchholz UJ, Surman S, Martin SE, Collins PL, Munir S. 2019. The alpha-1 subunit of the Na+,K+-ATPase (ATP1A1) is required for macropinocytic entry of respiratory syncytial virus (RSV) in human respiratory epithelial cells, PLoS Pathogens.

      (5) MYL3 is "predominantly found in muscle tissues, particularly the heart and skeletal muscles". However, NNV is a virus that mainly causes necrosis of nervous tissues (brain and retina). If MYL3 really acts as a receptor for NNV, how does it balance this difference so that nervous tissues, rather than muscle tissues, have the highest viral titers?

      While MYL3 is highly expressed in cardiac and skeletal muscles, studies have shown that MYL3, like other myosin light chains, can also be present in non-muscle tissues. Additionally, proteins involved in viral entry do not always need to be the most highly expressed in the final target tissue, as long as they facilitate the initial infection process. For instance, rabies virus is a rhabdovirus which exhibits a marked neuronotropism in infected animals. Transferrin receptor protein 1 can serve as a receptor for rabies virus through CME pathway, but TfR1 expressed most abundantly in liver tissue not nervous system (Wang et al., 2023).

      Viral tropism is often determined not only by the presence of an entry receptor but also by co-receptors, cellular factors, and post-entry mechanisms. While MYL3 may act as a receptor for NNV, other factors, such as cell-specific proteases, signaling molecules, and intracellular trafficking pathways, likely contribute to NNV’s preferential replication in the brain and retina.

      Reference

      Wang Xinxin, Wen Z, Cao H, Luo J, Shuai L, Wang C, Ge J, Wang Xijun, Bu Z, Wang J. 2023. Transferrin Receptor Protein 1 Is an Entry Factor for Rabies Virus. J Virol 97. doi:10.1128/jvi.01612-22

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript offers an important contribution to the field of virology, especially concerning NNV entry mechanisms. The major strength of the study lies in the identification of MmMYL3 as a functional receptor for RGNNV and its role in macropinocytosis, mediated by the IGF1R-Rac1/Cdc42 signaling axis. This represents a significant advance in understanding NNV entry mechanisms beyond previously known receptors such as HSP90ab1 and HSC70. The data, supported by comprehensive in vitro and in vivo experiments, strongly justify the authors' claims about MYL3's role in NNV infection in marine medaka.

      Strengths:

      (1) The identification of MmMYL3 as a functional receptor for RGNNV is a significant contribution to the field. The study fills a crucial gap in understanding the molecular mechanisms governing NNV entry into host cells.

      (2) The work highlights the involvement of IGF1R in macropinocytosis-mediated NNV entry and downstream Rac1/Cdc42 activation, thus providing a thorough mechanistic understanding of NNV internalization process. This could pave the way for further exploration of antiviral targets.

      Thanks for your review.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript presents a detailed study on the role of MmMYL3 in the viral entry of NNV, focusing on its function as a receptor that mediates viral internalization through the macropinocytosis pathway. The use of both in vitro assays (e.g., Co-IP, SPR, and GST pull-down) and in vivo experiments (such as infection assays in marine medaka) adds robustness to the evidence for MmMYL3 as a novel receptor for RGNNV. The findings have important implications for understanding NNV infection mechanisms, which could pave the way for new antiviral strategies in aquaculture.

      Strengths:

      The authors show that MmMYL3 directly binds the viral capsid protein, facilitates NNV entry via the IGF1R-Rac1/Cdc42 pathway, and can render otherwise resistant cells susceptible to infection. This multifaceted approach effectively demonstrates the central role of MmMYL3 in NNV entry.

      Thanks for your review.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line94: SPR analysis? The full name should be provided when it first shows.

      We have defined SPR when it first appears at line 97 in the revised manuscript.

      (2) Moreover, is it too many for a manuscript to have a total of nine figures in the main text? Some of them might be moved to the supplementary file.

      We have merged the previous Fig 4 and Fig 5 and combined Fig 8 and Fig 9, reducing the number of figures to seven. For the specific details of the figure adjustments, please refer to the corresponding figure legends.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Expand on the potential therapeutic implications of targeting MYL3 or the IGF1R pathway in aquaculture settings. Including a discussion of how inhibitors could be developed or tested in future research would give practical context to the findings.

      Thanks for your valuable suggestion to expand on the therapeutic implications of targeting MYL3 and the IGF1R pathway in aquaculture. In response, we have discussed potential strategies for developing inhibitors, such as small molecules, peptides, or monoclonal antibodies targeting MYL3 to block its interaction with the viral capsid, and IGF1R inhibitors to prevent macropinocytosis-mediated viral entry. We propose using virtual screening platforms to identify these inhibitors, followed by in vivo testing in aquaculture models. Additionally, combining MYL3 and IGF1R inhibitors could provide a synergistic approach to enhance antiviral efficacy. The relevant discussions have been supplemented at lines 358 to 368 in the revised manuscript.

      (2) It is recommended to include the data regarding the lack of interaction between the CMNV CP and MmMYL3 as a supplementary figure.

      We have included supplementary data demonstrating that CMNV CP does not interact with MmMYL3, highlighting the specificity of MYL3 for RGNNV. For detailed information, please refer to Fig. S4.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Consider discussing the broader implications of these findings, particularly whether MYL3 might serve as a receptor for other viruses.

      We appreciate this suggestion. It is important to note that viral receptors typically exhibit specificity for specific types of viruses. Receptor recognition is typically highly specific, and the binding interactions between viral proteins and host receptors often depend on the structural compatibility between the viral capsid/ viral envelope and the host receptor. Our study demonstrates that MYL3 serves as a receptor for NNV based on its direct interaction with the NNV capsid protein (CP). However, when we tested whether MYL3 interacts with CMNV (Covert Mortality Nodavirus), which is phylogenetically closer to NNV, we found that CMNV CP does not bind to MYL3. Given the lack of interaction between MYL3 and CMNV, it is unlikely that MYL3 serves as a receptor for more distantly related viruses. Since MYL3 does not interact with CMNV, a virus more closely related to NNV, it is less likely to function as a receptor for viruses that are more distantly related to NNV. The relevant discussions have been supplemented at lines 306 to 310 in the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Diarrheal diseases represent an important public health issue. Among the many pathogens that contribute to this problem, Salmonella enterica serovar Typhimurium is an important one. Due to the rise in antimicrobial resistance and the problems associated with widespread antibiotic use, the discovery and development of new strategies to combat bacterial infections is urgently needed. The microbiome field is constantly providing us with various health-related properties elicited by the commensals that inhabit their mammalian hosts. Harnessing the potential of these commensals for knowledge about host-microbe interactions as well as useful properties with therapeutic implications will likely remain a fruitful field for decades to come. In this manuscript, Wang et al use various methods, encompassing classic microbiology, genomics, chemical biology, and immunology, to identify a potent probiotic strain that protects nematode and murine hosts from S. enterica infection. Additionally, authors identify gut metabolites that are correlated with protection, and show that a single metabolite can recapitulate the effects of probiotic administration.

      We gratefully appreciate your positive and professional comments.

      Strengths:

      The utilization of varied methods by the authors, together with the impressive amount of data generated, to support the claims and conclusions made in the manuscript is a major strength of the work. Also, the ability to move beyond simple identification of the active probiotic, also identifying compounds that are at least partially responsible for the protective effects, is commendable.

      We gratefully appreciate your positive and professional comments.

      Weaknesses:

      Although there is a sizeable amount of data reported in the manuscript, there seems to be a chronic issue of lack of details of how some experiments were performed. This is particularly true in the figure legends, which for the most part lack enough details to allow comprehension without constant return to the text. Additionally, 2 figures are missing. Figure 6 is a repetition of Figure 5, and Figure S4 is an identical replicate of Figure S3.

      We gratefully appreciate your professional comments. Additional details to perform the related experiments had been added in Materials and methods section and figure legends (e.g., see Line 478-487, Line 996-1001, Line 1010-1012, Line 1019-1020, Line 1031-1033, Line 1041-1042, Line 1051-1053, Line 1082-1083, Line 1087-1088, Line 1093-1094, Line 1105-1107, Line 1113-1114,). Furthermore, we sincerely apologize for the mistakes and the inconvenience in the evaluating process of your review, and we have added the correct Figure 6 (see Line 1043-1053) and Figure S4 (see Line 1084-1088). We will carefully and thoroughly check the whole submitted manuscript along with supplementary information to avoid such mistakes in the future.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this work, the investigators isolated one Lacticaseibacillus rhamnosus strain (P118), and determined this strain worked well against Salmonella Typhimurium infection. Then, further studies were performed to identify the mechanism of bacterial resistance, and a list of confirmatory assays was carried out to test the hypothesis.

      We gratefully appreciate your positive and professional comments.

      Strengths:

      The authors provided details regarding all assays performed in this work, and this reviewer trusted that the conclusion in this manuscript is solid. I appreciate the efforts of the authors to perform different types of in vivo and in vitro studies to confirm the hypothesis.

      We gratefully appreciate your positive and professional comments.

      Weaknesses:

      I have two main questions about this work.

      (1) The authors provided the below information about the sources from which Lacticaseibacillus rhamnosus was isolated. More details are needed. What are the criteria to choose these samples? Where did these samples originate from? How many strains of bacteria were obtained from which types of samples?

      Sorry for the ambiguous and limited information, more details had been added in Materials and methods section (see Line 480-496). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      Lines 486-488: Lactic acid bacteria (LAB) and Enterococcus strains were isolated from the fermented yoghurts collected from families in multiple cities of China and the intestinal contents from healthy piglets without pathogen infection and diarrhoea by our lab.

      Sorry for the ambiguous and limited information, we had carefully revised this section and more details had been added in Materials and methods section (see Line 480-496). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      Lines 129-133: A total of 290 bacterial strains were isolated and identified from 32 samples of the fermented yoghurt and piglet rectal contents collected across diverse regions within China using MRS and BHI medium, which consist s of 63 Streptococcus strains, 158 Lactobacillus/ Lacticaseibacillus Limosilactobacillus strains, and 69 Enterococcus strains.

      Sorry for the ambiguous information, we had carefully revised this section and more details had been added in this section (see Line 129-132). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (2) As a probiotic, Lacticaseibacillus rhamnosus has been widely studied. In fact, there are many commercially available products, and Lacticaseibacillus rhamnosus is the main bacteria in these products. There are also ATCC type strains such as 53103.

      I am sure the authors are also interested to know whether P118 is better as a probiotic candidate than other commercially available strains. Also, would the mechanism described for P118 apply to other Lacticaseibacillus rhamnosus strains?

      It would be ideal if the authors could include one or two Lacticaseibacillus rhamnosus which are currently commercially used, or from the ATCC. Then, the authors can compare the efficacy and antibacterial mechanisms of their P118 with other strains. This would open the windows for future work.

      We gratefully appreciate your professional comments and valuable suggestions. We deeply agree that it will be better and make more sense to include well-known/recognized/commercial probiotics as a positive control to comprehensively evaluate the isolated P118 strain as a probiotic candidate, particularly in comparison to other well-established probiotics, and also help assess whether the mechanisms described for P118 are applicable to other L. rhamnosus strains or lactic acid bacteria in general. Those issues will be fully taken into consideration and included in the further works.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 28 - The sentence "with great probiotic properties" suggests that this strain was already known to have probiotic properties. Is that the case?

      We gratefully appreciate your professional comments. This sentence "with great probiotic properties" in this context was intended as a summary of our findings, emphasizing that L. rhamnosus P118 exerts great probiotic properties after evaluating by traditional and C. elegans-infection screening strategies. We had revised this sentence (see Line27-30).

      (2) Line 30 - What exactly do authors mean by "traditional"? They should add a bit more information here as to what these methods would be.

      We gratefully appreciate your professional comments. By "traditional" methods, we refer to time-consuming and labor-intensive strategies for screening probiotic candidates with heavy works, which include bacterial isolation, culturing, phenotypic characterization, randomized controlled trials, and various in vitro and in vivo tests to assess probiotic properties (Sun et al., 2022). We had indicated this strategy in Line 91-94.

      Reference:

      Sun Y, Li HC, Zheng L, Li JZ, Hong Y, Liang PF, Kwok LY, Zuo YC, Zhang WY, Zhang HP. Iprobiotics: A machine learning platform for rapid identification of probiotic properties from whole-genome primary sequences. Briefings in Bioinformatics 2022;23.

      (3) Line 37 - I believe "harmful microbes" is not the correct term here. I suggest authors use "potentially harmful".

      Done as requested (see Line 36, 209, 212, 217, 381). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (4) Line 75 - What exactly do authors mean by "irregular dietary consumption"?

      "irregular dietary consumption" means "irregular dietary habits" or " eating irregularly " or "abnormal eating behaviors". We had change to "irregular dietary habits" (see Line 76). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (5) Line 85 - What exactly do authors mean by "without residues in raw food products"?

      Here, "without residues in raw food products" means that probiotics barely remain in food animal products (e.g., meat, eggs, dairy) after dietary with probiotics in feeds by livestock and poultry. We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (6) Line 86 - Please, give a specific example of yeast.

      Done as requested (see Line 85-86), “yeast (e.g., Saccharomyces boulardii, S. cerevisiae)”. We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (7) Line 112 - Lactobacillus reuteri should be written out, since this is the first time the species name appears in the main text.

      Done as requested (see Line 112). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (8) Lines 115-118 - Please, rewrite for clarity.

      Done as requested (see Line 115-118). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (9) Line 118 -Lacticaseibacillus rhamnosus should be written out, since this is the first time the species name appears in the main text.

      Done as requested (see Line 118). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (10) Line 119 - Throughout the text authors make it seem like strain P118 was previously known. Is that the case? If yes, how was it isolated again? This should be briefly mentioned in the introduction.

      Sorry for the misunderstand caused by this statement, P118 strain was isolated and its probiotic properties were evaluated by our lab, not previously known, and we have revised this sentence (see Line 118-120). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (11) Line 131 - How were strains identified?

      Matrix-Assisted Laser Desorption Ionization-Time of Flight Mass Spectrometry (MALDI-TOF MS) method was employed to identify of bacterial species (He et al., 2022). This information was indicated in Materials and methods section (see Line 485-489). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      Reference

      He D, Zeng W, Wang Y, Xing Y, Xiong K, Su N, Zhang C, Lu Y, Xing X. Isolation and characterization of novel peptides from fermented products of lactobacillus for ulcerative colitis prevention and treatment. Food Science and Human Wellness 2022;11:1464-74.

      (12) Figure 1 - Legend needs a lot more info. Where are legends to panels PQ? Also, some of the text is too small to read.

      Sorry for the limited info, we have revised Figure 1 legend and added more info (see Line 1000-1019), and we also provide vector graphic of Figure 1. We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (13) Line 136 - All strains were screened and 27 strains were positive, right?

      Yes, all strains were screened and 27 strains were positive. We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (14) Figure 2 - What do authors mean by "spleen index" and "liver index"? This should be described in more detail. Also, p values for 'a', 'b', 'ab' should be given.

      The organ index (spleen index, liver index) were calculated according to the formula: organ index = organ weight (g) / body weight (g) *1000, indicating in Materials and methods section (see Line 587-588). “Different lowercase letters ('a', 'b') indicate a significant difference (P < 0.05)” had been added in Line 1020-1029. We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (15) Line 212-214 - Again, I suggest authors use "potentially harmful" and "potentially beneficial".

      Done as requested (see Line 36, 210, 213, 218, 383). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (16) Figure 3 - Which groups were tested in panels CD? Is this based on color? Legends should be restated in panels or clearly marked in the legend.

      Sorry for this mistake, we have revised and added group info in Figure 3C-D (see Line 1013-1020). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (17) Figure 4 - Lacks details.

      Sorry for the mistakes, we have revised and added group info in Figure 4D-E and legend (see Line 1031-1037). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (18) Figure 6 - This is a repetition of Figure 5.

      Sorry for the mistakes, we have added the correct Figure 6 (see Line 1060-1070). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (19) Lines 329-330 - C. elegans does not "mimic" animal intestinal physiology.

      Sorry for the mistakes, we have revised this statement (see Line 139-142, 324-325). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (20) Lines 358 and 418 - What do authors mean by "metabolic dysfunction" and "metabolic disorder"? I assume they mean changes in fecal metabolites. However, these are terms that may have different interpretations in the field of human metabolism. Therefore, I would suggest that the authors specify that they mean changes in fecal metabolite profiles when using these terms.

      Sorry for the mistakes caused by this statement, we have revised this statement in the revised version (see Line 34-35, 122, 353-354, 413). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (21) Line 475 - What do authors mean by "superficial effects"?

      Sorry for the mistakes, we had change to “beneficial/protective effects” (see Line 469, Line 1074). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (22) Line 486 - Were all yogurts artisanal? Where were piglets from? How were samples collected? Feces, rectal swabs? Does the ethics statement at the end of the manuscript also cover work with piglets?

      Yes, all yogurts were artisanal. The 6 healthy piglet rectal content samples without pathogen infection and diarrhea were from a pig farm of Zhejiang province. Yes, the ethics statement at the end of the manuscript also cover the work with piglets.

      (23) Line 490 - Which MALDI platform was used? The database used can have important implications for strain identification. What was the confidence of ID? This should be included.

      Matrix-Assisted Laser Desorption Ionization-Time of Flight Mass Spectrometry (MALDI-TOF MS, Bruker Daltonik GmbH, Bremen, Germany) was employed to identify of bacterial species with a confidence level > 90%. This information was indicated in Materials and methods section (see Line 487-489). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (24) Line 501 - Is this a widely used method to characterize probiotics? Please, add a reference.

      Done as requested (see Line 498). Many probiotics or microbes can produce milk clotting enzyme to clot milk. It's an important measurement in the dairy industry, especially when making cheese (Zhang et al., 2023; Arbita et al., 2024; Shieh et al., 2009). The milk-clotting activity analysis is usually used for evaluating the potential ability of candidate probiotic isolates in clotting milk into cheeses.

      Reference:

      Zhang Y, Wang J, He J, Liu X, Sun J, Song X, Wu Y. Characteristics and application in cheese making of newly isolated milk-clotting enzyme from bacillus megaterium ly114. Food Res Int 2023;172:113202.

      Arbita AA, Zhao J. Milk clotting enzymes from marine resources and their role in cheese-making: A mini review. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2024;64(27):10036-10047.

      Chwen-Jen Shieh, Lan-Anh Phan Thi, Ing-Lung Shih. Milk-clotting enzymes produced by culture of Bacillus subtilis natto. Biochemical Engineering Journal. 2009;1(43): 85-91.

      (25) Line 713 - How were fecal metabolites extracted?

      Sorry for the missed information, the fecal metabolites extracted information had been added we have revised and added Materials and methods section (see Line 705-706). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (26) Figure 7 - Please correct "macrophages".

      Done as requested (see Figure 7, Line 1072). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (27) Table 1 - Should read "number of strains", not size.

      Done as requested (see Line1084). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (28) Figure S1B - Is this data for P118?

      Sorry for the mistakes, we have revised Figure S1 legend (see Line 1086-1088). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (29) Figure S3 - Legends C, S, PS, P are not specified.

      Sorry for the missed information, we have revised and added group info in Figure S3 legend (see Line 1095-1101). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (30) Figure S3B - What is the "clinical symptom score"? How was this determined?

      Sorry for the lack information, and the detailed information had been added in Materials and methods section (see Line 659-661, Table S7). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (31) Figure S4 - This is an identical copy of Figure S3.

      Sorry for the mistakes, we have added the correct Figure S4 (see Line 1103-1106). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (32) Figure S5 - Legend lacks details.

      Sorry for the missed information, we have revised and added group info in Figure S5 legend (see Line 1107-1112). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (33) Figure S8 - What is "GM"? Since it inhibits growth to a greater extent than the highest metabolite concentration used, I imagine it must be an antibiotic (gentamycin?) as a positive control. This needs to be clearly stated.

      Sorry for the missed information, GM: 100 μg/mL gentamicin (see Line 1134). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      (34) Figure S9 - Labels for panels are missing.

      Sorry for the missed information, labels had been added (see Line 1135-1139). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) This reviewer appreciates the efforts of the authors to provide the details related to this work. In the meantime, the manuscript shall be written in a way that is easy for the readers to follow.

      We had tried our best to revise and make improve the whole manuscript to make it easy for the readers to follow (e.g., see Line 27-30, Line 115-120, Line 129-132, Line 480-496). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

      (2) For example, under the sections of Materials and Methods, there are 19 sub-titles. The authors could consider combining some sections, and/or citing other references for the standard procedures.

      We gratefully appreciate your professional comments and valuable suggestions. Some sections had been combined according to the reviewer’s suggestions (see Line 497-530, Line 637-671).

      (3) Another example: the figures have great resolution, but they are way too busy. Figures 1 and 2 have 14-18 panels. Figure 5 has 21 panels. Please consider separating into more figures, or condensing some panels.

      We deeply agree with you that some submitted figures are way too busy, but it’s not easy to move some results into supplementary information sections, because all of them are essential for fully supporting our hypothesis and conclusions. Nonetheless, some panels had been combined or condensed according to the reviewer’s suggestions (see Line 1000-1020, Line 1052-1071). We gratefully appreciate your professional comments and valuable suggestions.

      (4) Line 30: spell out "C." please.

      Done as requested (see Line 31). We gratefully appreciate your valuable suggestions.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      eLife Assessment

      This valuable work explores how synaptic activity encodes information during memory tasks. All reviewers agree that the quality of the work is high. Although experimental data do support the possibility that phospholipase diacylglycerol signaling and synaptotagmin 7 (Syt7) dynamically regulate the vesicle pool required for presynaptic release, concerns remain that the central finding of paired pulse depression at very short intervals was more likely caused by Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation than pool depletion. Overall, this is a solid study with valuable findings, but the results warrant consideration of alternative interpretations.

      We greatly appreciate invaluable and constructive comments from Editors and Reviewers. We also thank for their time and patience. We are pleased for our manuscript to have been assessed valuable and solid.

      One of the most critical concerns was a possible involvement of Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation in the strong paired pulse depression (PPD). Meanwhile, we have measured total (free plus buffered) calcium increments induced by each of first four APs in 40 Hz trains at axonal boutons of prelimbic layer 2/3 pyramidal cells. We found that first four Ca<sup>2+</sup> increments were not different from one another, arguing against possible contribution of Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation to PPD. Please see our reply to the 2nd issue in the Weakness section of Reviewer #3.

      The second critical issue was on the definition of ‘vesicular probability’. Previously, vesicular probability (p<sub>v</sub>) has been used with reference to the releasable vesicle pool which includes not only tightly docked vesicles but also reluctant vesicles. On the other hand, the meaning of p<sub>v</sub> in the present study is the release probability of tightly docked vesicles. We clarified this point in our replies to the 1st issues in the Weakness sections of Reviewer #2 and Reviewer #3.

      We below described our point-by-point replies to the Reviewers’ comments.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Shin et al. conduct extensive electrophysiological and behavioral experiments to study the mechanisms of short-term synaptic plasticity at excitatory synapses in layer 2/3 of the rat medial prefrontal cortex. The authors interestingly find that short-term facilitation is driven by progressive overfilling of the readily releasable pool, and that this process is mediated by phospholipase C/diacylglycerol signaling and synaptotagmin-7 (Syt7). Specifically, knockdown of Syt7 not only abolishes the refilling rate of vesicles with high fusion probability, but it also impairs the acquisition of trace fear memory. Overall, the authors offer novel insight to the field of synaptic plasticity through well-designed experiments that incorporate a range of techniques.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Shin et al aim to identify in a very extensive piece of work a mechanism that contributes to dynamic regulation of synaptic output in the rat cortex at the second time scale. This mechanism is related to a new powerful model is well versed to test if the pool of SV ready for fusion is dynamically scaled to adjust supply demand aspects. The methods applied are state-of-the-art and both address quantitative aspects with high signal to noise. In addition, the authors examine both excitatory output onto glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons, which provides important information on how general the observed signals are in neural networks, The results are compellingly clear and show that pool regulation may be predominantly responsible. Their results suggests that a regulation of release probability, the alternative contender for regulation, is unlikely to be involved in the observed short term plasticity behavior (but see below). Besides providing a clear analysis pof the underlying physiology, they test two molecular contenders for the observed mechanism by showing that loss of Synaptotagmin7 function and the role of the Ca dependent phospholipase activity seems critical for the short term plasticity behavior. The authors go on to test the in vivo role of the mechanism by modulating Syt7 function and examining working memory tasks as well as overall changes in network activity using immediate early gene activity. Finally, they model their data, providing strong support for their interpretation of TS pool occupancy regulation.

      Strengths:

      This is a very thorough study, addressing the research question from many different angles and the experimental execution is superb. The impact of the work is high, as it applies recent models of short term plasticity behavior to in vivo circuits further providing insights how synapses provide dynamic control to enable working memory related behavior through nonpermanent changes in synaptic output.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) While this work is carefully examined and the results are presented and discussed in a detailed manner, the reviewer is still not fully convinced that regulation of release provability is not a putative contributor to the observed behavior. No additional work is needed but in the moment I am not convinced that changes in release probability are not in play. One solution may be to extend the discussion of changes in release probability as an alternative.

      Quantal content (m) depends on n * p<sub>v</sub>, where n = RRP size and p<sub>v</sub> =vesicular release probability. The value for p<sub>v</sub> critically depends on the definition of RRP size. Recent studies revealed that docked vesicles have differential priming states: loosely or tightly docked state (LS or TS, respectively). Because the RRP size estimated by hypertonic solution or long presynaptic depolarization is larger than that by back extrapolation of a cumulative EPSC plot (Moulder & Mennerick, 2005; Sakaba, 2006) in glutamatergic synapses, the former RRP (denoted as RRP<sub>hyper</sub>) may encompass not only AP-evoked fast-releasing vesicles (TS vesicle) but also reluctant vesicles (LS vesicles). Because we measured p<sub>v</sub> based on AP-evoked EPSCs such as strong paired pulse depression (PPD) and associated failure rates, p<sub>v</sub> in the present study denotes vesicular fusion probability of TS vesicles, not that of LS plus TS vesicles.

      Recent studies suggest that release sites are not fully occupied by TS vesicles in the baseline (Miki et al., 2016; Pulido and Marty, 2018; Malagon et al., 2020; Lin et al., 2022). Instead, the occupancy (p<sub>occ</sub>) by TS vesicles is subject to dynamic regulation by reversible rate constants (denoted by k<sub>1</sub> and b<sub>1</sub>, respectively). The number of TS vesicles (n) can be factored into the number of release sites (N) and p<sub>occ</sub>, among which N is a fixed parameter but p<sub>occ</sub> depends on k<sub>1</sub>/(k<sub>1</sub>+b<sub>1</sub>) under the framework of the simple refilling model (see Methods). Because these refilling rate constants are regulated by Ca<sup>2+</sup> (Hosoi, et al., 2008), p<sub>occ</sub> is not a fixed parameter. Therefore, release probability should be re-defined as p<sub>occ</sub> * p<sub>v</sub>. Given that N is fixed, the increase in release probability is a major player in STF. Our study asserts that STF by 2.3 times can be attributed to an increase in p<sub>occ</sub> rather than p<sub>v</sub>, because p<sub>v</sub> is close to unity (Fig. S8). Moreover, strong PPD was observed not only in the baseline but also at the early and in the middle of a train (Fig. 2 and 7) and during the recovery phase (Fig. 3), arguing against a gradual increase in p<sub>v</sub> of reluctant vesicles.

      We imagine that the Reviewer meant vesicular release or fusion probability (p<sub>v</sub>) by ‘release probability’. If so, p<sub>v</sub> (of TS vesicles) cannot be a major player in STF, because the baseline p<sub>v</sub> is already higher than 0.8 even if it is most parsimoniously estimated (Fig. 2). Moreover, considering very high refilling rate (23/s), the high double failure rate cannot be explained without assuming that p<sub>v</sub> is close to unity (Fig. S8).

      Conventional models for facilitation assume a post-AP residual Ca<sup>2+</sup>-dependent step increase in p<sub>v</sub> of RRP (Dittman et al., 2000) or reluctant vesicles (Turecek et al., 2016). Given that p<sub>v</sub> of TS vesicles is close to one, an increase in p<sub>v</sub> of TS vesicles cannot account for facilitation. The possibility for activity-dependent increase in fusion probability of LS vesicles (denoted as p<sub>v,LS</sub>) should be considered in two ways depending on whether LS and TS vesicles reside in distinct pools or in the same pool. Notably, strong PPD at short ISI implies that p<sub>v,LS</sub> is near zero at the resting state. Whereas LS vesicles do not contribute to baseline transmission, short-term facilitation (STF) may be mediated by cumulative increase in p<sub>v v,LS </sub> that reside in a distinct pool. Because the increase in p<sub>v,LS</sub> during facilitation recruits new release sites (increase in N), the variance of EPSCs should become larger as stimulation frequency increases, resulting in upward deviation from a parabola in the V-M plane, as shown in recent studies (Valera et al., 2012; Kobbersmed et al., 2020). This prediction is not compatible with our results of V-M analysis (Fig. 3), showing that EPSCs during STF fell on the same parabola regardless of stimulation frequencies. Therefore, it is unlikely that an increase in fusion probability of reluctant vesicles residing in a distinct release pool mediates STF in the present study.

      For the latter case, in which LS and TS vesicles occupy in the same release sites, it is hard to distinguish a step increase in fusion probability of LS vesicles from a conversion of LS vesicles to TS. Nevertheless, our results do not support the possibility for gradual increase in p<sub>v,LS</sub> that occurs in parallel with STF. Strong PPD, indicative of high p<sub>v</sub>, was consistently found not only in the baseline (Fig. 2 and Fig. S6) but also during post-tetanic augmentation phase (Fig. 3D) and even during the early development of facilitation (Fig. 2D-E and Fig. 7), arguing against gradual increase in p<sub>v,LS</sub>. One may argue that STF may be mediated by a drastic step increase of p<sub>v,LS</sub> from zero to one, but it is not distinguishable from conversion of LS to TS vesicles.

      To address the reviewer’s concern, we incorporated these perspectives into Discussion and further clarified the reasoning behind our conclusions.

      References

      Moulder KL, Mennerick S (2005) Reluctant vesicles contribute to the total readily releasable pool in glutamatergic hippocampal neurons. J Neurosci 25:3842–3850.

      Sakaba, T (2006) Roles of the fast-releasing and the slowly releasing vesicles in synaptic transmission at the calyx of Held. J Neurosci 26(22): 5863-5871.

      Please note that papers cited in the manuscript are not repeated here.

      (2) Fig 3 I am confused about the interpretation of the Mean Variance analysis outcome. Since the data points follow the curve during induction of short term plasticity, aren't these suggesting that release probability and not the pool size increases? Related, to measure the absolute release probability and failure rate using the optogenetic stimulation technique is not trivial as the experimental paradigm bias the experiment to a given output strength, and therefore a change in release probability cannot be excluded.

      Under the recent definition of release probability, it can be factored into p<sub>v</sub> and p<sub>occ</sub>, which are fusion probability of TS vesicles and the occupancy of release sites by TS vesicles, respectively. With this regard, our interpretation of the Variance-Mean results is consistent with conventional one: different data points along a parabola represent a change in release probability (= p<sub>occ</sub> x p<sub>v</sub>). Our novel finding is that the increase in release probability should be attributed to an increase in p<sub>occ</sub>, not to that in p<sub>v</sub>.

      (3) Fig4B interprets the phorbol ester stimulation to be the result of pool overfilling, however, phorbol ester stimulation has also been shown to increase release probability without changing the size of the readily releasable pool. The high frequency of stimulation may occlude an increased paired pulse depression in presence of OAG, which others have interpreted in mammalian synapses as an increase in release probability.

      To our experience in the calyx of Held synapses, OAG, a DAG analogue, increased the fast releasing vesicle pool (FRP) size (Lee JS et al., 2013), consistent with our interpretation (pool overfilling). Once the release sites are overfilled in the presence of OAG, it is expected that the maximal STF (ratio of facilitated to baseline EPSCs) becomes lower as long as the number of release sites (N) are limited. As aforementioned, the baseline p<sub>v</sub> is already close to one, and thus it cannot be further increased by OAG. Instead, the baseline p<sub>occ</sub> seems to be increased by OAG.

      Reference

      Lee JS, et al., Superpriming of synaptic vesicles after their recruitment to the readily releasable pool. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2013. 110(37): 15079-84.

      (4) The literature on Syt7 function is still quite controversial. An observation in the literature that loss of Syt7 function in the fly synapse leads to an increase of release probability. Thus the observed changes in short term plasticity characteristics in the Syt7 KD experiments may contain a release probability component. Can the authors really exclude this possibility? Figure 5 shows for the Syt7 KD group a very prominent depression of the EPSC/IPSC with the second stimulus, particularly for the short interpulse intervals, usually a strong sign of increased release probability, as lack of pool refilling can unlikely explain the strong drop in synaptic output.

      The reviewer raises an interesting point regarding the potential link between Syt7 KD and increased initial p<sub>v</sub>, particularly in light of observations in Drosophila synapses (Guan et al., 2020; Fujii et al., 2021), in which Syt7 mutants exhibited elevated initial p<sub>v</sub>. However, it is important to note that these findings markedly differ from those in mammalian systems, where the role of Syt7 in regulating initial p<sub>v</sub> has been extensively studied. In rodents, consistent evidence indicates that Syt7 does not significantly affect initial p<sub>v</sub>, as demonstrated in several studies (Jackman et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2017; Turecek and Regehr, 2018). Furthermore, in our study of excitatory synapses in the mPFC layer 2/3, we observed an initial p<sub>v</sub> already near its maximal level, approaching a value of 1. Consequently, it is unlikely that the loss of Syt7 could further elevate the initial p<sub>v</sub>. Instead, such effects are more plausibly explained by alternative mechanisms, such as alterations in vesicle replenishment dynamics, rather than a direct influence on p<sub>v</sub>.

      References

      Chen, C., et al., Triple Function of Synaptotagmin 7 Ensures Efficiency of High-Frequency Transmission at Central GABAergic Synapses. Cell Rep, 2017. 21(8): 2082-2089.

      Fujii, T., et al., Synaptotagmin 7 switches short-term synaptic plasticity from depression to facilitation by suppressing synaptic transmission. Scientific reports, 2021. 11(1): 4059.

      Guan, Z., et al., Drosophila Synaptotagmin 7 negatively regulates synaptic vesicle release and replenishment in a dosage-dependent manner. Elife, 2020. 9: e55443.

      Jackman, S.L., et al., The calcium sensor synaptotagmin 7 is required for synaptic facilitation. Nature, 2016. 529(7584): 88-91.

      Turecek, J. and W.G. Regehr, Synaptotagmin 7 mediates both facilitation and asynchronous release at granule cell synapses. Journal of Neuroscience, 2018. 38(13): 3240-3251.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The report by Shin, Lee, Kim, and Lee entitled "Progressive overfilling of readily releasable pool underlies short-term facilitation at recurrent excitatory synapses in layer 2/3 of the rat prefrontal cortex" describes electrophysiological experiments of short-term synaptic plasticity during repetitive presynaptic stimulation at synapses between layer 2/3 pyramidal neurons and nearby target neurons. Manipulations include pharmacological inhibition of PLC and actin polymerization, activation of DAG receptors, and shRNA knockdown of Syt7. The results are interpreted as support for the hypothesis that synaptic vesicle release sites are vacant most of the time at resting synapses (i.e., p_occ is low) and that facilitation (and augmentation) components of short-term enhancement are caused by an increase in occupancy, presumably because of acceleration of the transition from not-occupied to occupied. The report additionally describes behavioural experiments where trace fear conditioning is degraded by knocking down syt7 in the same synapses.

      Strengths:

      The strength of the study is in the new information about short-term plasticity at local synapses in layer 2/3, and the major disruption of a memory task after eliminating short-term enhancement at only 15% of excitatory synapses in a single layer of a small brain region. The local synapses in layer 2/3 were previously difficult to study, but the authors have overcome a number of challenges by combining channel rhodopsins with in vitro electroporation, which is an impressive technical advance.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The question of whether or not short-term enhancement causes an increase in p_occ (i.e., "readily releasable pool overfilling") is important because it cuts to the heart of the ongoing debate about how to model short term synaptic plasticity in general. However, my opinion is that, in their current form, the results do not constitute strong support for an increase in p_occ, even though this is presented as the main conclusion. Instead, there are at least two alternative explanations for the results that both seem more likely. Neither alternative is acknowledged in the present version of the report.

      The evidence presented to support overfilling is essentially two-fold. The first is strong paired pulse depression of synaptic strength when the interval between action potentials is 20 or 25 ms, but not when the interval is 50 ms. Subsequent stimuli at frequencies between 5 and 40 Hz then drive enhancement. The second is the observation that a slow component of recovery from depression after trains of action potentials is unveiled after eliminating enhancement by knocking down syt7. Of the two, the second is predicted by essentially all models where enhancement mechanisms operate independently of release site depletion - i.e., transient increases in p_occ, p_v, or even N - so isn't the sort of support that would distinguish the hypothesis from alternatives (Garcia-Perez and Wesseling, 2008, https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01348.2007).

      The apparent discrepancy in interpretation of post-tetanic augmentation between the present and previous papers [Sevens Wesseling (1999), Garcia-Perez and Wesseling (2008)] is an important issue that should be clarified. We noted that different meanings of ‘vesicular release probability’ in these papers are responsible for the discrepancy. We added an explanation to Discussion on the difference in the meaning of ‘vesicular release probability’ between the present study and previous studies [Sevens Wesseling (1999), Garcia-Perez and Wesseling (2008)]. In summary, the p<sub>v</sub> in the present study was used for vesicular release probability of TS vesicles, while previous studies used it as vesicular release probability of vesicles in the RRP, which include LS and TS vesicles. Accordingly, p<sub>occ</sub> in the present study is the occupancy of release sites by TS vesicles.

      Not only double failure rate but also other failure rates upon paired pulse stimulation were best fitted at p<sub>v</sub> close to 1 (Fig. S8 and associated text). Moreover, strong PPD, indicating release of vesicles with high p<sub>v</sub>, was observed not only at the beginning of a train but also in the middle of a 5 Hz train (Fig. 2D), during the augmentation phase after a 40 Hz train (Fig 3D), and in the recovery phase after three pulse bursts (Fig. 7). Given that p<sub>v</sub> is close to 1 throughout the EPSC trains and that N does not increase during a train (Fig. 3), synaptic facilitation can be attained only by the increase in p<sub>occ</sub> (occupancy of release sites by TS vesicles). In addition, it should be noted that Fig. 7 demonstrates strong PPD during the recovery phase after depletion of TS vesicles by three pulse bursts, indicating that recovered vesicles after depletion display high p<sub>v</sub> too. Knock-down of Syt7 slowed the recovery of TS vesicles after depletion of TS vesicles, highlighting that Syt7 accelerates the recovery of TS vesicles following their depletion.

      As addressed in our reply to the first issue raised by Reviewer #2 and the third issue raised by Reviewer #3, our results do not support possibilities for recruitment of new release sites (increase in N) having low p<sub>v</sub> or for a gradual increase in p<sub>v</sub> of reluctant vesicles during short-term facilitation.  

      Following statement was added to Discussion in the revised manuscript

      “Previous studies suggested that an increase in p<sub>v</sub> is responsible for post-tetanic augmentation (Stevens and Wesseling, 1999; Garcia-Perez and Wesseling, 2008) by observing invariance of the RRP size after tetanic stimulation. In these studies, the RRP size was estimated by hypertonic sucrose solution or as the sum of EPSCs evoked 20 Hz/60 pulses train (denoted as ‘RRP<sub>hyper</sub>’). Because reluctant vesicles (called LS vesicles) can be quickly converted to TS vesicles (16/s) and are released during a train (Lee et al., 2012), it is likely that the RRP size measured by these methods encompasses both LS and TS vesicles. In contrast, we assert high p<sub>v</sub> based on the observation of strong PPD and failure rates upon paired stimulations at ISI of 20 ms (Fig. 2 and Fig. S8). Given that single AP-induced vesicular release occurs from TS vesicles but not from LS vesicles, p<sub>v</sub> in the present study indicates the fusion probability of TS vesicles. From the same reasons, p<sub>occ</sub> denotes the occupancy of release sites by TS vesicles. Note that our study does not provide direct clue whether release sites are occupied by LS vesicles that are not tapped by a single AP, although an increase in the LS vesicle number may accelerate the recovery of TS vesicles. As suggested in Neher (2024), even if the number of LS plus TS vesicles are kept constant, an increase in p<sub>occ</sub> (occupancy by TS vesicles) would be interpreted as an increase in ‘vesicular release probability’ as in the previous studies (Stevens and Wesseling (1999); Garcia-Perez and Wesseling (2008)) as long as it was measured based on RRP<sub>hyper</sub>.”

      (2) Regarding the paired pulse depression: The authors ascribe this to depletion of a homogeneous population of release sites, all with similar p_v. However, the details fit better with the alternative hypothesis that the depression is instead caused by quickly reversing inactivation of Ca<sup>2+</sup> channels near release sites, as proposed by Dobrunz and Stevens to explain a similar phenomenon at a different type of synapse (1997, PNAS, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.94.26.14843). The details that fit better with Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation include the combination of the sigmoid time course of the recovery from depression (plotted backwards in Fig1G,I) and observations that EGTA (Fig2B) increases the paired-pulse depression seen after 25 ms intervals. That is, the authors ascribe the sigmoid recovery to a delay in the activation of the facilitation mechanism, but the increased paired pulse depression after loading EGTA indicates, instead, that the facilitation mechanism has already caused p_r to double within the first 25 ms (relative to the value if the facilitation mechanism was not active). Meanwhile, Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation would be expected to cause a sigmoidal recovery of synaptic strength because of the sigmoidal relationship between Ca<sup>2+</sup>-influx and exocytosis (Dodge and Rahamimoff, 1967, https://doi.org/10.1113/jphysiol.1967.sp008367).

      The Ca<sup>2+</sup>-channel inactivation hypothesis could probably be ruled in or out with experiments analogous to the 1997 Dobrunz study, except after lowering extracellular Ca<sup>2+</sup> to the point where synaptic transmission failures are frequent. However, a possible complication might be a large increase in facilitation in low Ca<sup>2+</sup> (Fig2B of Stevens and Wesseling, 1999, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80685-6).

      We appreciate the reviewer's thoughtful comment regarding the potential role of Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation in the observed paired-pulse depression (PPD). As noted by the Reviewer, the Dobrunz and Stevens (1997) suggested that the high double failure rate at short ISIs in synapses exhibiting PPD can be attributed to Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation. This interpretation seems to be based on a premise that the number of RRP vesicles are not varied trial-by-trial. The number of TS vesicles, however, can be dynamically regulated depending on the parameters k<sub>1</sub> and b<sub>1</sub>, as shown in Fig. S8, implying that the high double failure rate at short ISIs cannot be solely attributed to Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation. Nevertheless, we acknowledge the possibility that Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation may contribute to PPD, and therefore, we have further investigated this possibility. Specifically, we measured action potential (AP)-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> transients at individual axonal boutons of layer 2/3 pyramidal cells in the mPFC using two-dye ratiometry techniques. Our analysis revealed no evidence for Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation during a 40 Hz train of APs. This finding indicates that voltage-gated Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation is unlikely to contribute to the pronounced PPD.

      Figure 2—figure supplement 2 shows how we measured the total Ca<sup>2+</sup> increments at axonal boutons. First we estimated endogenous Ca<sup>2+</sup>-binding ratio from analyses of single AP-induced Ca<sup>2+</sup> transients at different concentrations of Ca<sup>2+</sup> indicator dye (panels A to E). And then, using the Ca<sup>2+</sup> buffer properties, we converted free [Ca<sup>2+</sup>] amplitudes to total calcium increments for the first four AP-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> transients in a 40 Hz train (panels G-I). We incorporated these results into the revised version of our manuscript to provide evidence against the Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel inactivation.

      (3) On the other hand, even if the paired pulse depression is caused by depletion of release sites rather than Ca<sup>2+</sup>-channel inactivation, there does not seem to be any support for the critical assumption that all of the release sites have similar p_v. And indeed, there seems to be substantial emerging evidence from other studies for multiple types of release sites with 5 to 20-fold differences in p_v at a wide variety of synapse types (Maschi and Klyachko, eLife, 2020, https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.55210; Rodriguez Gotor et al, eLife, 2024, https://doi.org/10.7554/elife.88212 and refs. therein). If so, the paired pulse depression could be caused by depletion of release sites with high p_v, whereas the facilitation could occur at sites with much lower p_v that are still occupied. It might be possible to address this by eliminating assumptions about the distribution of p_v across release sites from the variance-mean analysis, but this seems difficult; simply showing how a few selected distributions wouldn't work - such as in standard multiple probability fluctuation analyses - wouldn't add much.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments regarding the potential increase in p<sub>fusion</sub> of reluctant vesicles. It should be noted, however, that Maschi and Klyachko (2020) showed a distribution of release probability (p<sub>r</sub>) within a single active zone rather than a heterogeneity in p<sub>fusion</sub> of individual docked vesicles. Therefore both p<sub>occ</sub> and p<sub>v</sub> of TS vesicles would contribute to the p<sub>r</sub> distribution shown in Maschi and Klyachko (2020). 

      The Reviewer’s concern aligns closely with the first issue raised by Reviewer #2, to which we addressed in detail. Briefly, new release site may not be recruited during facilitation or post-tetanic augmentation, because variance of EPSCs during and after a train fell on the same parabola (Fig. 3). Secondly, strong PPD was observed not only in the baseline but also during early and late phases of facilitation, indicating that vesicles with very high p<sub>v</sub> contribute to EPSC throughout train stimulations (Fig. 2, 3, and 7). These findings argue against the possibilities for recruitment of new release sites harboring low p<sub>v</sub> vesicles and for a gradual increase in fusion probability of reluctant vesicles.

      To address the reviewers’ concern, we incorporated the perspectives into Discussion and further clarified the reasoning behind our conclusions.

      (4) In any case, the large increase - often 10-fold or more - in enhancement seen after lowering Ca<sup>2+</sup> below 0.25 mM at a broad range of synapses and neuro-muscular junctions noted above is a potent reason to be cautious about the LS/TS model. There is morphological evidence that the transitions from a loose to tight docking state (LS to TS) occur, and even that the timing is accelerated by activity. However, 10-fold enhancement would imply that at least 90 % of vesicles start off in the LS state, and this has not been reported. In addition, my understanding is that the reverse transition (TS to LS) is thought to occur within 10s of ms of the action potential, which is 10-fold too fast to account for the reversal of facilitation seen at the same synapses (Kusick et al, 2020, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-00716-1).

      As the Reviewer suggested, low external Ca<sup>2+</sup> concentration can lower release probability (p<sub>r</sub>). Given that both p<sub>v</sub> and p<sub>occ</sub> are regulated by [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub>, low external [Ca<sup>2+</sup>] may affect not only p<sub>v</sub> but also p<sub>occ</sub>, both of which would contribute to low p<sub>r</sub>. Under such conditions, it would be plausible that the baseline p<sub>r</sub> becomes much lower than 0.1 due to low p<sub>v</sub> and p<sub>occ</sub> (for instance, p<sub>v</sub> decreases from 1 to 0.5, and p<sub>occ</sub> from 0.3 to 0.1, then p<sub>r</sub> = 0.05), and then p<sub>r</sub> (= p<sub>v</sub> x p<sub>occ</sub>) has a room for an increase by a factor of ten (0.5, for example) by short-term facilitation as cytosolic [Ca<sup>2+</sup>] accumulates during a train.

      If p<sub>v</sub> is close to one, p<sub>r</sub> depends p<sub>occ</sub>, and thus facilitation depends on the number of TS vesicles just before arrival of each AP of a train. Thus, post-train recovery from facilitation would depend on restoration of equilibrium between TS and LS vesicles to the baseline. Even if transition between LS and TS vesicles is very fast (tens of ms), the equilibrium involved in de novo priming (reversible transitions between recycling vesicle pool and partially docked LS vesicles) seems to be much slower (13 s in Fig. 5A of Wu and Borst 1999). Thus, we can consider a two-step priming model (recycling pool -> LS -> TS), which is comprised of a slow 1st step (-> LS) and a fast 2nd step (-> TS). Under the framework of the two-step model, the slow 1st step (de novo priming step) is the rate limiting step regulating the development and recovery kinetics of facilitation. Given that on and off rate for Ca<sup>2+</sup> binding to Syt7 is slow, it is plausible that Syt7 may contribute to short-term facilitation (STF) by Ca<sup>2+</sup>-dependent acceleration of the 1st step (as shown in Fig. 9). During train stimulation, the number of LS vesicles would slowly accumulate in a Syt7 and Ca<sup>2+</sup>-dependent manner, and this increase in LS vesicles would shift LS/TS equilibrium towards TS, resulting in STF. After tetanic stimulation, the recovery kinetics from facilitation would be limited by slow recovery of LS vesicles.

      Reference

      Wu, L.-G. and Borst J.G.G. (1999) The reduced release probability of releasable vesicles during recovery from short-term synaptic depression. Neuron, 23(4): 821-832.

      Please note that papers cited in the manuscript are not repeated here.

      Individual points:

      (1) An additional problem with the overfilling hypothesis is that syt7 knockdown increases the estimate of p_occ extracted from the variance-mean analysis, which would imply a faster transition from unoccupied to occupied, and would consequently predict faster recovery from depression. However, recovery from depression seen in experiments was slower, not faster. Meanwhile, the apparent decrease in the estimate of N extracted from the mean-variance analysis is not anticipated by the authors' model, but fits well with alternatives where p_v varies extensively among release sites because release sites with low p_v would essentially be silent in the absence of facilitation.

      Slower recovery from depression observed in the Syt7 knockdown (KD) synapses (Fig. 7) may results from a deficiency in activity-dependent acceleration of TS vesicle recovery. Although basal occupancy was higher in the Syt7 KD synapses, this does not indicate a faster activity-dependent recovery.

      Higher baseline occupancy does not always imply faster recovery of PPR too. Actually PPR recovery was slower in Syt7 KD synapses than WT one (18.5 vs. 23/s). Under the framework of the simple refilling model (Fig. S8Aa), the baseline occupancy and PPR recovery rate are calculated as k<sub>1</sub> / (k<sub>1</sub> + b<sub>1</sub>) and (k<sub>1</sub> + b<sub>1</sub>), respectively. The baseline occupancy depends on k<sub>1</sub>/b<sub>1</sub>, while the PPR recovery on absolute values of k<sub>1</sub> and b<sub>1</sub>. Based on p<sub>occ</sub> and PPR recovery time constant of WT and KD synapses, we expect higher k<sub>1</sub>/b<sub>1</sub> but lower values for (k<sub>1</sub> + b<sub>1</sub>) in Syt7 KD synapses compared to WT ones.

      Lower release sites (N) in Syt7-KD synapses was not anticipated. As you suggested, such low N might be ascribed to little recruitment of release sites during a train in KD synapses. But our results do not support this model. If silent release sites are recruited during a train, the variance should upwardly deviate from the parabola predicted under a fixed N (Valera et al., 2012; Kobbersmed et al. 2020). Our result was not the case (Fig. 3). In the first version of the manuscript, we have argued against this possibility in line 203-208.

      As discussed in both the Results and Discussion sections, the baseline EPSC was unchanged by KD (Fig. S3) because of complementary changes in the number of docking sites and their baseline occupancy (Fig. 6). These findings suggest that Syt7 may be involved in maintaining additional vacant docking sites, which could be overfilled during facilitation. It remains to be determined whether the decrease in docking sites in Syt7 KD synapses is related to its specific localization of Syt7 at the plasma membrane of active zones, as proposed in previous studies (Sugita et al., 2001; Vevea et al., 2021).

      (2) Figure S4A: I like the TTX part of this control, but the 4-AP part needs a positive control to be meaningful (e.g., absence of TTX).

      The reason why we used 4-AP in the presence of TTX was to increase the length constant of axon fibers and to facilitate the conduction of local depolarization in the illumination area to axon terminals. The lack of EPSC in the presence of 4-AP and TTX indicates that illumination area is distant from axon terminals enough for optic stimulation-induced local depolarization not to evoke synaptic transmission. This methodology has been employed in previous studies including the work of Little and Carter (2013).

      Reference

      Little JP and Carter AG (2013) Synaptic mechanisms underlying strong reciprocal connectivity between the medial prefrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala. J Neurosci, 33(39): 15333-15342.

      (3) Line 251: At least some of the previous studies that concluded these drugs affect vesicle dynamics used logic that was based on some of the same assumptions that are problematic for the present study, so the reasoning is a bit circular.

      (4) Line 329 and Line 461: A similar problem with circularity for interpreting earlier syt7 studies.

      (Reply to #3 and #4) We selected the target molecules as candidates based on their well-characterized roles in vesicle dynamics, and aimed to investigate what aspects of STP are affected by these molecules in our experimental context. For example, we could find that the baseline p<sub>occ</sub> and short-term facilitation (STF) are enhanced by the baseline DAG level and train stimulation-induced PLC activation, respectively. Notably, the effect of dynasore informed us that slow site clearing is responsible for the late depression of 40 Hz train EPSC. The knock-down experiments also provided us with information on the critical role of Syt7 in replenishment of TS vesicles. These approaches do not deviate from standard scientific reasoning but rather builds upon prior knowledge to formulate and test hypotheses.

      Importantly, our conclusions do not rely solely on the assumption that altering the target molecule impacts synaptic transmission. Instead, our conclusions are derived from a comprehensive analysis of diverse outcomes obtained through both pharmacological and genetic manipulations. These interpretations align closely with prior literature, further validating our conclusions.

      Therefore, the use of established studies to guide candidate selection and the consistency of our findings with existing knowledge do not represent a logical circularity but rather a reinforcement of the proposed mechanism through converging lines of evidence.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Comments:

      (1) While the authors claim that Syt7-mediated facilitation is connected to the behavioral deficits they observed, this link is still somewhat speculative. This manuscript could benefit from further discussions of other alternative mechanisms to consider.

      We added following statement to Discussion of the revised manuscript:

      “The acquisition of trace fear memory was impaired by inhibition of persistent activity in mPFC during trace period (Gilmartin et al., 2013). The similar deficit observed in Syt7 KD animals is consistent with the hypothesis that STF provides bi-stable ensemble activity in a recurrent network (Mongillo et al., 2012). Nevertheless, alternative mechanisms may be responsible for the behavioral deficit. Not only recurrent network but also long-range loop between the mPFC and the mediodorsal (MD) thalamus play a critical role in maintaining persistent activity within the mPFC especially for a delay period longer than 10 s (Bolkan et al., 2017). Prefrontal L2/3 is heavily innervated by MD thalamus, and L2/3-PCs subsequently relay signals to L5 cortico-thalamic (CT) neurons (Collins et al., 2018). Given that L2/3 is an essential component of the PFC-thalamic loop, loss of STF at recurrent synapses between L2/3 PCs may lead to insufficient L2/3 inputs to L5 CT neurons and failure in the reverberant PFC-MD thalamic feedback loop. Therefore, not only L2/3 recurrent network but also its output to downstream network should be considered as a possible network mechanism underlying behavioral deficit caused by Syt7 KD L2/3.”

      (2) The authors mention that Syt7 contributes to persistent activity during working memory tasks but focus on using only a trace fear conditioning task. However, it would be interesting to see if their results are generalizable to other working memory tasks (i.e. a delayed alternation task).

      We thank to Reviewer for the insightful suggestion. Trace fear conditioning (tFC) shares behavioral properties with working memory (WM) tasks in that tFC is vulnerable to attentional distraction and to the load of WM task. In general WM tasks including delayed alternation tasks such as a T-maze task need persistent activity of ensemble neurons representing target-specific information among multiple choices. Different from such WM tasks, tFC is not appropriate to examine target-specific ensemble activity. Because it is not trivial to examine in vivo recordings in KD animals during delayed alternation tasks, it will be appropriate to study the effect of Syt7 KD in a separate study. 

      (3) The figure legend in Figure 6A and 6B mentions dotted lines and broken lines in the figure. However, this is confusing, and it is unclear as to what these lines are referring to in the figure.

      To avoid the confusion in the figure legend for Figure 6A and 6B, we corrected “dotted line” to " vertical broken line", and “broken lines” to “dashed parabolas”.

      (4) The manuscript can benefit from close reading and editing to catch typos and improve general readability (i.e. line 173: the word "are" is repeated twice).

      We corrected typographical errors throughout the manuscript and carefully read the manuscript to improve readability. A revised version reflecting these corrections has been prepared and will be resubmitted for your consideration.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The points in this section are all minor.

      (1) Line 44: Define release probability (p_r) more clearly. Authors use it to mean p<sub>v</sub>*p<sub>occ</sub>, but others routinely use it to mean p<sub>v</sub>*p<sub>occ</sub>*N.

      We understand that the Reviewer meant “others routinely use it to mean p<sub>v</sub>”. At this statement, we meant conventional definition of release probability, which is release probability among vesicles of RRP. We think that it is not appropriate to re-define release probability as p<sub>v</sub> * p<sub>occ</sub> in this first paragraph of Introduction. Therefore we clarified this issue in Discussion as we mentioned in our reply to the 1st weakness issue raised by Reviewer #3.   

      (2) Line 82: For clarity, define better what recurrent excitatory synapses are. It seems that synapses between L2/3 PCs and local targets may all be recurrent?

      Each of L2/3 and L5 of the prefrontal cortical layers harbors intralaminar recurrent excitatory synapses between pyramidal cells, called a recurrent network. Previous theoretical studies have proposed that a single layer recurrent network model can have bi-stable E/I balanced states (up- and down-states) if recurrent excitatory synapses display short-term facilitation (STF), and thus is able to temporally hold an information once external input shifts the network to the up-state. In this theory, synapses to local targets across layers are not considered and specific roles of L2/3 and L5 in working memory tasks are still elusive. For clarity, we added a statement at the beginning of the paragraph (line 82): “Each of layer 2/3 (L2/3) and layer 5 (L5) of neocortex displays intralaminar excitatory synapses between pyramidal cells comprising a recurrent network (Holmgren et al., 2003; Thomson and Lamy, 2007)”

      (3) Cite earlier studies of short-term synaptic plasticity at synapses between L2/3 pyramidal neurons and local targets in mPFC. If there are none, take more explicit credit for being first.

      As we mentioned in Introduction, previous studies on short-term plasticity (STP) at neocortical excitatory recurrent synapses have focused on synapses between L5 pyramidal cells (PCs) (Hemple et al. 2000; Wang et al. 2006; Morishima et al., 2011; Yoon et al., 2020). The local connectivity between L2/3 PCs in the somatosensory cortex has been elucidated by Homgren et al. (2003) and Ko et al. (2011). Although these study showed STP of EPSPs, it was at a fixed frequency or stimulus pattern at high external [Ca<sup>2+</sup>] (2 mM). There is a study on the frequency-dependence of STP of EPSP between L2/3-PCs (Feldmyer et al., 2006). Different from our study, Feldmyer et al., (2006) observed monotonous STD at all frequencies less than 50 Hz, but this study was done in the somatosensory cortex and at high external [Ca<sup>2+</sup>] (2 mM). To our knowledge, no previous study have investigated STP at recurrent excitatory synapses of L2/3 pyramidal cells of the mPFC especially at physiological external [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]. The present study, therefore, represents the first extensive investigation of STP at recurrent excitatory synapses in L2/3 of the mPFC under physiologically relevant external [Ca<sup>2+</sup>].

      References

      Feldmeyer D, Lubke J, Silver RA, Sakmann B (2002) Synaptic connections between layer 4 spiny neurone-layer 2/3 pyramidal cell pairs in juvenile rat barrel cortex: physiology and anatomy of interlaminar signalling within a cortical column. J Physiol 538:803-822.

      Holmgren C, Harkany T, Svennenfors B, Zilberter Y (2003) Pyramidal cell communication within local networks in layer 2/3 of rat neocortex. J Physiol 551:139-153.

      Ko H, Hofer SB, Pichler B, Buchanan KA, Sjöström PJ, Mrsic-Flogel TD (2011) Functional specificity of local synaptic connections in neocortical networks. Nature 473:87-91.

      Morishima M, Morita K, Kubota Y, Kawaguchi Y (2011) Highly differentiated projection-specific cortical subnetworks. Journal of Neuroscience 31:10380-10391.

      Wang Y, Markram H, Goodman PH, Berger TK, Ma J, Goldman-Rakic PS (2006) Heterogeneity in the pyramidal network of the medial prefrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 9:534-542.

      (4) I couldn't figure out the significance of Figure S3. Perhaps this could be explained better.

      Optical minimal stimulation methods have not been previously documented in detail. This figure illustrates what parameters we should carefully examine in order to attain optical minimal stimulation, which hopefully stimulates a single afferent fiber. A single fiber stimulation by optical minimal stimulation is supported by the similarity of our estimate for the number of release sites (N) as the previous morphological estimate (Holler et al., 2021). For minimal stimulation, we used a collimated DMD-coupled LED was employed to restrict 470 nm illumination to a small and well-defined region within layer 2/3 of the prelimbic mPFC, and carefully adjusted the illumination radius such that one step smaller (by 1 μm) illumination results in failure to evoke EPSCs. Our typical illumination area ranged between 3–4 μm, as shown in Figure S3A. Under this minimal illumination area, we confirmed unimodal distributions for the EPSC parameters (amplitude, rise time, decay time and time to peak; Figure 3B-E). Otherwise, we excluded the recordings from analysis. We hope this explanation provides a clearer understanding of the figure's significance.

      (5) Note that CTZ seems to alter p_r at some synapses.

      We acknowledge that CTZ can increase release probability by blocking presynaptic K<sup>+</sup> currents. Indeed, Ishikawa and Takahashi (2001) reported that CTZ slowed the repolarizing phase of presynaptic action potentials and the frequency of miniature EPSCs in the calyx synapses. Consistently, we observed a slight increase in the baseline EPSC amplitude, from 33.3 pA to 41.9 pA (p=0.045) following the application of 50 µM CTZ. However, given that vesicular release probability (p<sub>v</sub>) is already close to 1 at the synapse of our interest, we believe that the observed effect is more likely attributed to an increase in release sites occupancy (p<sub>occ</sub>), which would be reflected as an increase in miniature EPSC frequency in Ishikawa and Takahashi (2001). Given that PPR depends on p<sub>v</sub> rather than p<sub>occ</sub>, this increase in p<sub>occ</sub> would not critically change our conclusion that AMPA receptor desensitization is not responsible for the strong PPD.

      Reference

      Ishikawa, T., & Takahashi, T. (2001). Mechanisms underlying presynaptic facilitatory effect of cyclothiazide at the calyx of Held of juvenile rats. The Journal of Physiology, 533(2), 423-431.

      (6) Figure 8B. The result in Figure 8C seems important, but I couldn't figure out why behaviour was not altered during the acquisition phase summarized in Figure 8B. Perhaps this could be explained more clearly for non-experts.

      Little difference in freezing behavior during acquisition has been also observed when prelimbic persistent firing was optogenetically inhibited (Gilmartin, 2013). Not only CS (tone) but also other sensory inputs (visual and olfactory etc.) and the spatial context could be a cue predicting US (shock). Moreover, during the acquisition phase, the presence of the electric shock inherently induces a freezing response as a natural defensive behavior, which may obscure specific behavioral changes related to the associative learning process. Therefore, the freezing behavior during acquisition cannot be regarded as a sign for specific association of CS and US. Instead, on the next day, we specifically evaluated the CS-US association of the conditioned animals by measuring freezing behavior in response to CS in a distinct context. We explicitly documented little difference between WT and KD animals during the acquisition phase in the relevant paragraph (line 397).

  2. Apr 2025
    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      It seems as if the main point of the paper is about the new data related to rat fish although your title is describing it as extant cartilaginous fishes and you bounce around between the little skate and ratfish. So here's an opportunity for you to adjust the title to emphasize ratfish is given the fact that leader you describe how this is your significant new data contribution. Either way, the organization of the paper can be adjusted so that the reader can follow along the same order for all sections so that it's very clear for comparative purposes of new data and what they mean. My opinion is that I want to read, for each subheading in the results, about the the ratfish first because this is your most interesting novel data. Then I want to know any confirmation about morphology in little skate. And then I want to know about any gaps you fill with the cat shark. (It is ok if you keep the order of "skate, ratfish, then shark, but I think it undersells the new data).

      The main points of the paper are 1) to define terms for chondrichthyan skeletal features in order to unify research questions in the field, and 2) add novel data on how these features might be distributed among chondrichthyan clades. However, we agree with the reviewer that many readers might be more interested in the ratfish data, so we have adjusted the order of presentation to emphasize ratfish throughout the manuscript.

      Strengths:

      The imagery and new data availability for ratfish are valuable and may help to determine new phylogenetically informative characters for understanding the evolution of cartilaginous fishes. You also allude to the fossil record.

      Thank you for the nice feedback.

      Opportunities:

      I am concerned about the statement of ratfish paedomorphism because stage 32 and 33 were not statistically significantly different from one another (figure and prior sentences). So, these ratfish TMDs overlap the range of both 32 and 33. I think you need more specimens and stages to state this definitely based on TMD. What else leads you to think these are paedomorphic? Right now they are different, but it's unclear why. You need more outgroups.

      Sorry, but we had reported that the TMD of centra from little skate did significantly increase between stage 32 and 33. Supporting our argument that ratfish had features of little skate embryos, TMD of adult ratfish centra was significantly lower than TMD of adult skate centra (Fig1). Also, it was significantly higher than stage 33 skate centra, but it was statistically indistinguishable from that of stage 33 and juvenile stages of skate centra. While we do agree that more samples from these and additional groups would bolster these data, we feel they are sufficiently powered to support our conclusions for this current paper.

      Your headings for the results subsection and figures are nice snapshots of your interpretations of the results and I think they would be better repurposed in your abstract, which needs more depth.

      We have included more data summarized in results sub-heading in the abstract as suggested (lines 32-37).

      Historical literature is more abundant than what you've listed. Your first sentence describes a long fascination and only goes back to 1990. But there are authors that have had this fascination for centuries and so I think you'll benefit from looking back. Especially because several of them have looked into histology and development of these fishes.

      I agree that in the past 15 years or so a lot more work has been done because it can be done using newer technologies and I don't think your list is exhaustive. You need to expand this list and history which will help with your ultimate comparative analysis without you needed to sample too many new data yourself.

      We have added additional recent and older references: Kölliker, 1860; Daniel, 1934; Wurmbach, 1932; Liem, 2001; Arratia et al., 2001.

      I'd like to see modifications to figure 7 so that you can add more continuity between the characters, illustrated in figure 7 and the body of the text.

      We address a similar comment from this reviewer in more detail below, hoping that any concerns about continuity have been addressed with inclusion of a summary of proposed characters in a new Table 1, re-writing of the Discussion, and modified Fig7 and re-written Fig7 legend.

      Generally Holocephalans are the outgroup to elasmobranchs - right now they are presented as sister taxa with no ability to indicate derivation. Why isn't the catshark included in this diagram?

      While a little unclear exactly what was requested, we restructured the branches to indicate that holocephalans diverged earlier from the ancestors that led to elasmobranchs. Also in response to this comment, we added catshark (S. canicula) and little skate (L. erinacea) specifically to the character matrix.

      In the last paragraph of the introduction, you say that "the data argue" and I admit, I am confused. Whose data? Is this a prediction or results or summary of other people's work? Either way, could be clarified to emphasize the contribution you are about to present.

      Sorry for this lack of clarity, and we have changed the wording in this revision to hopefully avoid this misunderstanding.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Further Strengths and Opportunities:

      Your headings for the results subsection and figures are nice snapshots of your interpretations of the results and I think they would be better repurposed in your abstract, which needs more depth. It's a little unusual to try and state an interpretation of results as the heading title in a results section and the figures so it feels out of place. You could also use the headings as the last statement of each section, after you've presented the results. In order I would change these results subheadings to:

      Tissue Mineral Density (TMD)

      Tissue Properties of Neural Arches

      Trabecular mineralization

      Cap zone and Body zone Mineralization Patterns

      Areolar mineralization

      Developmental Variation

      Sorry, but we feel that summary Results sub-headings are the best way to effectively communicate to readers the story that the data tell, and this style has been consistently used in our previous publications. No changes were made.

      You allude to the fossil record and that is great. That said historical literature is more abundant than what you've listed. Your first sentence describes a long fascination and only goes back to 1990. But there are authors that have had this fascination for centuries and so I think you'll benefit from looking back. Especially because several of them have looked into histology of these fishes. You even have one sentence citing Coates et al. 2018, Frey et al., 2019 and ørvig 1951 to talk about the potential that fossils displayed trabecular mineralization. That feels like you are burying the lead and may have actually been part of the story for where you came up with your hypothesis in the beginning... or the next step in future research. I feel like this is really worth spending some more time on in the intro and/or the discussion.

      We’ve added older REFs as pointed out above. Regarding fossil evidence for trabecular mineralization, no, those studies did not lead to our research question. But after we discovered how widespread trabecular mineralization was in extant samples, we consulted these papers, which did not focus on the mineralization patterns per se, but certainly led us to emphasize how those patterns fit in the context of chondrichthyan evolution, which is how we discussed them.

      I agree that in the past 15 years or so a lot more work has been done because it can be done using newer technologies. That said there's a lot more work by Mason Dean's lab starting in 2010 that you should take a look at related to tesserae structure... they're looking at additional taxa than what you did as well. It will be valuable for you to be able to make any sort of phylogenetic inference as part of your discussion and enhance the info your present in figure 7. Go further back in time... For example:

      de Beer, G. R. 1932. On the skeleton of the hyoid arch in rays and skates. Quarterly Journal of Microscopical Science. 75: 307-319, pls. 19-21.

      de Beer, G. R. 1937. The Development of the Vertebrate Skull. The University Press, Oxford.

      Indeed, we have read all of Mason’s work, citing 9 of his papers, and where possible, we have incorporated their data on different species into our Discussion and Fig7. Thanks for the de Beer REFs. While they contain histology of developing chondrichthyan elements, they appear to refer principally to gross anatomical features, so were not included in our Intro/Discussion.

      Most sections within the results, read more like a discussion than a presentation of the new data and you jump directly into using an argument of those data too early. Go back in and remove the references or save those paragraphs for the discussion section. Particularly because this journal has you skip the method section until the end, I think it's important to set up this section with a little bit more brevity and conciseness. For instance, in the first section about tissue mineral density, change that subheading to just say tissue mineral density. Then you can go into the presentation of what you see in the ratfish, and then what you see in the little skate, and then that's it. You save the discussion about what other elasmobranch's or mineralizing their neural arches, etc. for another section.

      We dramatically reduced background-style writing and citations in each Results section (other than the first section of minor points about general features of the ratfish, compared to catshark and little skate), keeping only a few to briefly remind the general reader of the context of these skeletal features.

      I like that your first sentence in the paragraph is describing why you are doing. a particular method and comparison because it shows me (the reader) where you're sampling from. Something else is that maybe as part of the first figure rather than having just each with the graph have a small sketch for little skate and catch shark to show where you sampled from for comparative purposes. That would relate back, then to clarifying other figures as well.

      Done (also adding a phylogenetic tree).

      Second instance is your section on trabecular mineralization. This has so many references in it. It does not read like results at all. It looks like a discussion. However, the trabecular mineralization is one of the most interesting aspect of this paper, and how you are describing it as a unique feature. I really just want a very clear description of what the definition of this trabecular mineralization is going to be.

      In addition to adding Table 1 to define each proposed endoskeletal character state, we have changed the structure of this section and hope it better communicates our novel trabecular mineralization results. We also moved the topic of trabecular mineralization to the first detailed Discussion point (lines 347-363) to better emphasize this specific topic.

      Carry this reformatting through for all subsections of the results.

      As mentioned above, we significantly reduced background-style writing and citations in each Results section.

      I'd like to see modifications to figure 7 so that you can add more continuity between the characters, illustrated in figure 7 and the body of the text. I think you can give the characters a number so that you can actually refer to them in each subsection of the results. They can even be numbered sequentially so that they are presented in a standard character matrix format, that future researchers can add directly to their own character matrices. You could actually turn it into a separate table so it doesn't taking up that entire space of the figure, because there need to be additional taxa referred to on the diagram. Namely, you don't have any out groups in figure 7 so it's hard to describe any state specifically as ancestral and wor derived. Generally Holocephalans are the outgroup to elasmobranchs - right now they are presented as sister taxa with no ability to indicate derivation. Why isn't the catshark included in this diagram?

      The character matrix is a fantastic idea, and we should have included it in the first place! We created Table 1 summarizing the traits and terminology at the end of the Introduction, also adding the character matrix in Fig7 as suggested, including specific fossil and extant species. For the Fig7 branching and catshark inclusion, please see above.

      You can repurpose the figure captions as narrative body text. Use less narrative in the figure captions. These are your results actually, so move that text to the results section as a way to truncate and get to the point faster.

      By figure captions, we assume the reviewer refers to figure legends. We like to explain figures to some degree of sufficiency in the legends, since some people do not read the main text and simply skim a manuscript’s abstract, figures, and figure legends. That said, we did reduce the wording, as requested.

      More specific comments about semantics are listed here:

      The abstract starts negative and doesn't state a question although one is referenced. Potential revision - "Comprehensive examination of mineralized endoskeletal tissues warranted further exploration to understand the diversity of chondrichthyans... Evidence suggests for instance that trabecular structures are not common, however, this may be due to sampling (bring up fossil record.) We expand our understanding by characterizing the skate, cat shark, and ratfish... (Then add your current headings of the results section to the abstract, because those are the relevant takeaways.)"

      We re-wrote much of the abstract, hoping that the points come across more effectively. For example, we started with “Specific character traits of mineralized endoskeletal tissues need to be clearly defined and comprehensively examined among extant chondrichthyans (elasmobranchs, such as sharks and skates, and holocephalans, such as chimaeras) to understand their evolution”. We also stated an objective for the experiments presented in the paper: “To clarify the distribution of specific endoskeletal features among extant chondrichthyans”.

      In the last paragraph of the introduction, you say that "the data argue" and I admit, I am confused. Whose data? Is this a prediction or results or summary of other people's work? Either way, could be clarified to emphasize the contribution you are about to present.

      Sorry for this lack of clarity, and we have changed the wording in this revision to hopefully avoid this misunderstanding.

      In the second paragraph of the TMD section, you mention the synarcual comparison. I'm not sure I follow. These are results, not methods. Tell me what you are comparing directly. The non-centrum part of the synarcual separate from the centrum? They both have both parts... did you mean the comparison of those both to the cat shark? Just be specific about which taxon, which region, and which density. No need to go into reasons why you chose those regions here.. Put into methods and discussion for interpretation.

      We hope that we have now clarified wording of that section.

      Label the spokes somehow either in caption or on figure direction. I think I see it as part of figure 4E, I, and J, but maybe I'm misinterpreting.

      Based upon histological features (e.g., regions of very low cellularity with Trichrome unstained matrix) and hypermineralization, spokes in Fig4 are labelled with * and segmented in blue. We detailed how spokes were identified in main text (lines 241-243; 252-254) and figure legend (lines 597-603).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      General comment:

      This is a very valuable and unique comparative study. An excellent combination of scanning and histological data from three different species is presented. Obtaining the material for such a comparative study is never trivial. The study presents new data and thus provides the basis for an in-depth discussion about chondrichthyan mineralised skeletal tissues.

      Many thanks for the kind words

      I have, however, some comments. Some information is lacking and should be added to the manuscript text. I also suggest changes in the result and the discussion section of the manuscript.

      Introduction:

      The reader gets the impression almost no research on chondrichthyan skeletal tissues was done before the 2010 ("last 15 years", L45). I suggest to correct that and to cite also previous studies on chondrichthyan skeletal tissues, this includes studies from before 1900.

      We have added additional older references, as detailed above.

      Material and Methods:

      Please complete L473-492: Three different Micro-CT scanners were used for three different species? ScyScan 117 for the skate samples. Catshark different scanner, please provide full details. Chimera Scncrotron Scan? Please provide full details for all scanning protocols.

      We clarified exact scanners and settings for each micro-CT experiment in the Methods (lines 476-497).

      TMD is established in the same way in all three scanners? Actually not possible. Or, all specimens were scanned with the same scanner to establish TMD? If so please provide the protocol.

      Indeed, the same scanner was used for TMD comparisons, and we included exact details on how TMD was established and compared with internal controls in the Methods. (lines 486-488)

      Please complete L494 ff: Tissue embedding medium and embedding protocol is missing. Specimens have been decalcified, if yes how? Have specimens been sectioned non-decalcified or decalcified?

      Please complete L506 ff: Tissue embedding medium and embedding protocol is missing. Description of controls are missing.

      Methods were updated to include these details (lines 500-503).

      Results:

      L147: It is valuable and interesting to compare the degree of mineralisation in individuals from the three different species. It appears, however, not possible to provide numerical data for Tissue Mineral Density (TMD). First requirement, all specimens must be scanned with the same scanner and the same calibration values. This in not stated in the M&M section. But even if this was the case, all specimens derive from different sample locations and have, been preserved differently. Type of fixation, extension of fixation time in formalin, frozen, unfrozen, conditions of sample storage, age of the samples, and many more parameters, all influence TMD values. Likewise the relative age of the animals (adult is not the same as adult) influences TMD. One must assume different sampling and storage conditions and different types of progression into adulthood. Thus, the observation of different degrees of mineralisation is very interesting but I suggest not to link this observation to numerical values.

      These are very good points, but for the following reasons we feel that they were not sufficiently relevant to our study, so the quantitative data for TMD remain scientifically valid and critical for the field moving forward. Critically, 1) all of the samples used for TMD calculations underwent the same fixation protocols, and 2) most importantly, all samples for TMD were scanned on the same micro-CT scanner using the same calibration phantoms for each scanning session. Finally, while the exact age of each adult was not specified, we note for Fig1 that clear statistically significant differences in TMD were observed among various skeletal elements from ratfish, shark, and skate. Indeed, ratfish TMD was considerably lower than TMD reported for a variety of fishes and tetrapods (summarized in our paper about icefish skeletons, who actually have similar TMD to ratfish: https://doi.org/10.1111/joa.13537).

      In response, however, we added a caveat to the paper’s Methods (lines 466-469), stating that adult ratfish were frozen within 1 or 2 hours of collection from the wild, staying frozen for several years prior to thawing and immediate fixation.

      Parts of the results are mixed with discussion. Sometimes, a result chapter also needs a few references but this result chapter is full of references.

      As mentioned above, we reduced background-style writing and citations in each Results section.

      Based on different protocols, the staining characteristics of the tissue are analysed. This is very good and provides valuable additional data. The authors should inform the not only about the staining (positive of negative) abut also about the histochemical characters of the staining. L218: "fast green positive" means what? L234: "marked by Trichrome acid fuchsin" means what? And so on, see also L237, L289, L291

      We included more details throughout the Results upon each dye’s first description on what is generally reflected by the specific dyes of the staining protocols. (lines 178, 180, 184, 223, 227, and 243-244)

      Discussion

      Please completely remove figure 7, please adjust and severely downsize the discussion related to figure 7. It is very interesting and valuable to compare three species from three different groups of elasmobranchs. Results of this comparison also validate an interesting discussion about possible phylogenetic aspects. This is, however, not the basis for claims about the skeletal tissue organisation of all extinct and extant members of the groups to which the three species belong. The discussion refers to "selected representatives" (L364), but how representative are the selected species? Can there be a extant species that represents the entire large group, all sharks, rays or chimeras? Are the three selected species basal representatives with a generalist life style?

      These are good points, and yes, we certainly appreciate that the limited sampling in our data might lead to faulty general conclusions about these clades. In fact, we stated this limitation clearly in the Introduction (lines 126-128), and we removed “representative” from this revision. We also replaced general reference to chondrichthyans in the Title by listing the specific species sampled. However, in the Discussion, we also compare our data with previously published additional species evaluated with similar assays, which confirms the trend that we are concluding. We look forward to future papers specifically testing the hypotheses generated by our conclusions in this paper, which serves as a benchmark for identifying shared and derived features of the chondrichthyan endoskeleton.

      Please completely remove the discussion about paedomorphosis in chimeras (already in the result section). This discussion is based on a wrong idea about the definition of paedomorphosis. Paedomorphosis can occur in members of the same group. Humans have paedormorphic characters within the primates, Ambystoma mexicanum is paedormorphic within the urodeals. Paedomorphosis does not extend to members of different vertebrate branches. That elasmobranchs have a developmental stage that resembles chimera vertebra mineralisation does not define chimera vertebra centra as paedomorphic. Teleost have a herocercal caudal fin anlage during development, that does not mean the heterocercal fins in sturgeons or elasmobranchs are paedomorphic characters.

      We agree with the reviewer that discussion of paedomorphosis should apply to members of the same group. In our paper, we are examining paedomorphosis in a holocephalan, relative to elasmobranch fishes in the same group (Chrondrichthyes), so this is an appropriate application of paedomorphosis. In response to this comment, we clarified that our statement of paedomorphosis in ratfish was made with respect to elasmobranchs (lines 37-39; 418-420).

      L432-435: In times of Gadow & Abott (1895) science had completely wrong ideas bout the phylogenic position of chondrichthyans within the gnathostomes. It is curious that Gadow & Abott (1895) are being cited in support of the paedomorphosis claim.

      If paedomorphosis is being examined within Chondrichthyes, such as in our paper and in the Gadow and Abbott paper, then it is an appropriate reference, even if Gadow and Abbott (and many others) got the relative position of Chondrichthyes among other vertebrates incorrect.

      The SCPP part of the discussion is unrelated to the data obtained by this study. Kawaki & WEISS (2003) describe a gene family (called SCPP) that control Ca-binding extracellular phosphoproteins in enamel, in bone and dentine, in saliva and in milk. It evolved by gene duplication and differentiation. They date it back to a first enamel matrix protein in conodonts (Reif 2006). Conodonts, a group of enigmatic invertebrates have mineralised structures but these structure are neither bone nor mineralised cartilage. Cat fish (6 % of all vertebrate species) on the other hand, have bone but do not have SCPP genes (Lui et al. 206). Other calcium binding proteins, such as osteocalcin, were initially believed to be required for mineralisation. It turned out that osteocalcin is rather a mineralisation inhibitor, at best it regulates the arrangement collagen fiber bundles. The osteocalcin -/- mouse has fully mineralised bone. As the function of the SCPP gene product for bone formation is unknown, there is no need to discuss SCPP genes. It would perhaps be better to finish the manuscript with summery that focuses on the subject and the methodology of this nice study.

      We completely agree with the reviewer that many papers claim to associate the functions of SCPP genes with bone formation, or even mineralization generally. The Science paper with the elephant shark genome made it very popular to associate SCPP genes with bone formation, but we feel that this was a false comparison (for many reasons)! In response to the reviewer’s comments, however, we removed the SCPP discussion points, moving the previous general sentence about the genetic basis for reduced skeletal mineralization to the end of the previous paragraph (lines 435-439). We also added another brief Discussion paragraph afterwards, ending as suggested with a summary of our proposed shared and derived chondrichthyan endoskeletal traits (lines 440-453).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Other comments

      L40: remove paedomorphism

      No change; see above

      L53: down tune languish, remove "severely" and "major"

      Done (lines 57-59)

      L86: provide species and endoskeletal elements that are mineralized

      No change; this paragraph was written generally, because the papers cited looked at cap zones of many different skeletal elements and neural arches in many different species

      L130: remove TMD, replace by relative, descriptive, values

      No change; see above

      L135: What are "segmented vertebral neural arches and centra" ?

      Changed to “neural arches and centra of segmented vertebrae” (lines 140-141)

      L166: L168 "compact" vs. "irregular". Partial mineralisation is not necessarily irregular.

      Thanks for pointing out this issue; we changed wording, instead contrasting “non-continuous” and “continuous” mineralization patterns (lines 171-174)

      L192: "several endoskeletal regions". Provide all regions

      All regions provided (lines 198-199)

      L269: "has never been carefully characterized in chimeras". Carefully means what? Here, also only one chimera is analyses, not several species.

      Sentence removed

      302: Can't believe there is no better citation for elasmobranch vertebral centra development than Gadow and Abott (1895)

      Added Arriata and Kolliker REFs here (lines 293-295)

      L318 ff: remove discussion from result chapter

      References to paedomorphism were removed from this Results section

      L342: refer to the species studied, not to the entire group.

      Sorry, the line numbering for the reviewer and our original manuscript have been a little off for some reason, and we were unclear exactly to which line of text this comment referred. Generally in this revision, however, we have tried to restrict our direct analyses to the species analyzed, but in the Discussion we do extrapolate a bit from our data when considering relevant published papers of other species.

      346: "selected representative". Selection criteria are missing

      “selected representative” removed

      L348: down tune, remove "critical"

      Done

      L351: down tune, remove "critical"

      Done

      L 364: "Since stem chondrichthyans did not typically mineralize their centra". Means there are fossil stem chondrichthyans with full mineralised centra?

      Re-worded to “Stem chondrichthyans did not appear to mineralize their centra” (lines 379)

      L379: down tune and change to: "we propose the term "non-tesseral trabecular mineralization. Possibly a plesiomorphic (ancestral) character of chondrichthyans"

      No change; sorry, but we feel this character state needs to be emphasized as we wrote in this paper, so that its evolutionary relationship to other chondrichthyan endoskeletal features, such as tesserae, can be clarified.

      L407: suggests so far palaeontologist have not been "careful" enough?

      Apologies; sentence re-worded, emphasizing that synchrotron imaging might increase details of these descriptions (lines 406-408)

      414: down tune, remove "we propose". Replace by "possibly" or "it can be discussed if"

      Sentence re-worded and “we propose” removed (lines 412-415)

      L420: remove paragraph

      No action; see above

      L436: remove paragraph

      No action; see above

      L450: perhaps add summery of the discussion. A summery that focuses on the subject and the methodology of this nice study.

      Yes, in response to the reviewer’s comment, we finished the discussion with a summary of the current study. (lines 440-453)

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The manuscript investigates the role of the membrane-deforming cytoskeletal regulator protein Abba in cortical development and its potential implications for microcephaly. It is a valuable contribution to the understanding of Abba's role in cortical development. The strengths and weaknesses identified in the manuscript are outlined below:

      Clinical Relevance:

      The authors identified a patient with microcephaly and intellectual disability patient harboring a mutation in the Abba variant (R671W), adding a clinically relevant dimension to the study.

      Mechanistic Insights:

      The study offers valuable mechanistic insights into the development of microcephaly by elucidating the role of Abba in radial glial cell proliferation, radial fiber organization, and the migration of neuronal progenitors. The identification of Abba's involvement in the cleavage furrow during cell division, along with its interaction with Nedd9 and positive influence on RhoA activity, adds depth to our understanding of the molecular processes governing cortical development.

      In Vivo Validation:

      The overexpression of mutant Abba protein (R671W), which results in phenotypic similarities to Abba knockdown effects, supports the significance of Abba in cortical development.

      Weaknesses:

      The findings in the study suggest that heterozygous expression of the R671W variant may exert a dominant-negative effect on ABBA's role, disrupting normal brain development and leading to microcephaly and cognitive delay. However, evidence also points to a possible gain-of-function effect, as the mutation does not decrease RhoA activity or PH3 expression in vivo. Additionally, the impact of ABBA depletion on cell fate is not fully addressed. While abnormal progenitor accumulation in the ventricular and subventricular zones is observed, the transition of progenitors to neuroblasts and their ability to support neuroblast migration remains unclear. Impaired cleavage furrow ingression and disrupted Nedd9 and RhoA signaling could lead to structural abnormalities in radial glial progenitors, affecting their scaffold function and neuroblast progression.  The manuscript lacks an exploration of the loss or decrease in interaction between Abba and NEDD9 in the case of the pathogenic patient-derived mutation in Abba. Furthermore, addressing the changes in localization and ineraction in for NEDD9 following over-expression of the mutant are important to further mehcanistically characterizxe this interaction in future studies. These gaps suggest the need for further exploration of ABBA's role in progenitor cell fate and neuroblast migration to clarify its mechanistic contributions to cortical development.

      (1) Response to statement on dominant-negative vs. gain-of-function effect of R671W variant:

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful analysis of the potential mechanisms underlying the R671W variant. We agree that the heterozygous expression of the human R671W mutation may initially suggest a dominant-negative effect. However, our data indicate that this variant may instead exert a gain-of-function effect. As highlighted in the discussion section, overexpression of ABBA-R671W in cells that also express wild-type ABBA did not result in a dominant-negative decrease in RhoA activation nor affect PH3 expression in vivo. These findings suggest that the R671W mutation does not impair the canonical ABBA-mediated activation of RhoA, and instead, the resulting phenotype may involve post-mitotic processes, such as altered cell migration. This interpretation is further supported by previous clinical studies reporting additional patients with the same mutation and phenotypic outcomes.

      (2) Response to statement on ABBA depletion and progenitor-to-neuroblast transition:

      We agree that the question of how ABBA depletion affects cell fate and the progression of radial glial progenitors (RGPs) to neuroblasts is of significant importance. Our findings suggest that ABBA knockdown disrupts cleavage furrow ingression, which may block radial glial cells prior to abscission. This likely contributes to the observed accumulation of cells in the ventricular and subventricular zones, as seen in Figures 2A and 4D. Additionally, disrupted Nedd9 expression and impaired RhoA signaling appear to alter the structural integrity of RGPs, leading to detachment of apical and basal endfeet (Supplementary Figure 3). These structural abnormalities compromise the ability of RGPs to function as scaffolds for neuroblast migration. Although direct live imaging of neuroblast migration was beyond the scope of the current dataset, we believe our evidence strongly supports a model in which ABBA depletion disrupts progenitor structure and migration. Future studies will address these transitions more directly using live imaging and fate-mapping strategies

      (3) Response to statement on loss of interaction between ABBA and NEDD9 with the R671W mutation:

      We fully agree with the importance of investigating whether the R671W mutation alters ABBA’s interaction with NEDD9. While our study provides evidence for a role of NEDD9 in mediating ABBA function, we acknowledge that we did not directly test whether the R671W mutation disrupts this interaction. We apologize if our manuscript conveyed the impression that this point had been fully addressed. Due to technical limitations, particularly the poor performance of anti-NEDD9 antibodies in slice immunohistochemistry, we were unable to reliably assess the interaction or localization changes in vivo. Nevertheless, this remains a priority for future studies aimed at better understanding the mechanistic underpinnings of the R671W mutation.

      (4) Response to statement on future directions for mechanistic characterization of NEDD9 localization and interaction:

      We agree with the reviewer that further investigation into NEDD9 localization and its interaction with the ABBA R671W mutant is essential to better define the molecular consequences of this mutation. Unfortunately, as mentioned above, the current tools available to us did not permit reliable immunohistochemical detection of NEDD9 in tissue. We fully intend to pursue alternative approaches, such as tagging strategies or the use of more sensitive detection platforms, to determine whether the R671W mutation affects the subcellular localization or stability of the ABBA-NEDD9 interaction. These experiments will be critical to elucidate the pathway through which ABBA regulates progenitor cell behavior and cortical development.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Carabalona and colleagues investigated the role of the membrane-deforming cytoskeletal regulator protein Abba (MTSS1L/MTSS2) in cortical development to better understand the mechanisms of abnormal neural stem cell mitosis. The authors used short hairpin RNA targeting Abba20 with a fluorescent reporter coupled with in utero electroporation of E14 mice to show changes to neural progenitors. They performed flow cytometry for in-depth cell cycle analysis of Abba-shRNA impact to neural progenitors and determined an accumulation in S phase. Using culture rat glioma cells and live imaging from cortical organotypic slides from mice in utero electroporated with Abba-shRNA, the authors found Abba played a prominent role in cytokinesis. They then used a yeast-two-hybrid screen to identify three high confidence interactors: Beta-Trcp2, Nedd9, and Otx2. They used immunoprecipitation experiments from E18 cortical tissue coupled with C6 cells to show Abba requirement for Nedd9 localization to the cleavage furrow/cytokinetic bridge. The authors performed an shRNA knockdown of Nedd9 by in utero electroporation of E14 mice and observed similar results as with the Abba-shRNA. They tested a human variant of Abba using in utero electroporation of cDNA and found disorganized radial glial fibers and misplaced, multipolar neurons, but lacked the impact of cell division seen in the shRNA-Abba model.

      Strengths:

      Fundamental question in biology about the mechanics of neural stem cell division.

      Directly connecting effects in Abba protein to downstream regulation of RhoA via Nedd9.

      Incorporation of human mutation in ABBA gene.

      Use of novel technologies in neurodevelopment and imaging.

      Weaknesses:

      Unexplored components of the pathway (such as what neurogenic populations are impacted by Abba mutation) and unleveraged aspects of their data (such as the live imaging) limit the scope of their findings and left significant questions about the effect of ABBA on radial glia development.

      (1) Claim of disorganized radial glial fibers lacks quantifications.

      - On page 11, the authors claim that knockdown of Abba lead to changes in radial glial morphology observed with vimentin staining. Here they claim misoriented apical processes, detached end feet, and decreased number of RGP cells in the VZ. However, they no not provide quantification of process orientation to better support their first claim. Measurements of radial glia fiber morphology (directionality, length) and of angle of division would be metrics that can be applied to data. Some of these analysis could be done in their time-lapse microscopy images, such as to quantify the number of cell division during their period of analysis (though that is short-15 hours).

      Response to: Lack of quantification of disorganized radial glial fibers and cell divisions in time-lapse data

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment regarding the need for quantification of radial glial (RG) fiber morphology. In the revised manuscript, we have addressed this by providing new quantification of changes in vimentin staining, specifically measuring the dispersion of the signal as a proxy for fiber disorganization (see Supplementary Figure 1). These data support the observed morphological changes, including misoriented apical processes and detachment of endfeet, following Abba knockdown.

      Regarding time-lapse analysis to track cell divisions, we attempted to follow individual cells during the 15-hour imaging window. However, due to the relatively short duration and limited number of cells that could be reliably tracked, the dataset did not allow for statistically meaningful conclusions. As an alternative approach, we performed live-cell imaging using Anillin-GFP, a reliable marker of mitotic progression. The distribution and accumulation of Anillin-GFP were analyzed in ABBA-shRNA3 and control conditions, and the results (now included in Supplementary Figure 3) indicate an increased number of cells arrested in late mitosis upon ABBA knockdown. This supports the notion of disrupted cytokinesis as a consequence of Abba depletion.

      (2) Unclear where effect is:

      - In RG or neuroblasts? Is it in cell cleavage that results in accumulation of cells at VZ (as sometimes indicated by their data like in Fig 2A or 4D)? Interrogation of cell death (such as by cleaved caspase 3) would also help. Given their time lapse, can they identify what is happening to the RG fiber? The authors describe a change in "migration" but do not show evidence for this for either progenitor or neuroblast populations. Given they have nice time-lapse imaging data, could they visualize progenitor versus young neuron migration? Analysis of neuroblasts (such as with doublecortin expression in the tissue) would also help understand any issues in migration (of neurons v stem cells).

      - At cleaveage furrow? In abscission? There is high resolution data that highlights the cleavage furrow as the location of interest (fig 3A), however there is also data (fig 3B) to suggest Abba is expressed elsewhere as well and there is an overall soma decrease. More detail of the localization of Abba during the division process would be helpful-for example, could cleavage furrow proteins, such as Aurora B, co-localization (and potentially co-IP) help delineate subpopulations of Abba protein? Furthermore, the FRET imaging is unique way to connect their mutation with function-could they measure/quantify differences at furrow compared to rest of soma to further corroborate that Abba-associated RhoA effect was furrow-enriched?

      - The data highlights nicely that a furrow doesn't clearly form when ABBA expression and subsequent RhoA activity are decreased (in Fig 3 or 5A). Does this lead to cells that can't divide because of poor abscission, especially since "rounding" still occurs? Or abnormal progenitors (with loss of fiber or inability to support neuroblast migration)? Or abnormal progression of progenitors to neuroblasts?

      Response to: Unclear location of the effect (RG vs. neuroblasts; cleavage furrow/abscission; migration issues)

      We thank the reviewer for this comprehensive and thought-provoking set of questions.

      a) Site of the effect – Radial Glia vs. Neuroblasts:

      Our data suggest that the primary effect of ABBA depletion occurs in radial glial progenitors (RGPs), specifically prior to abscission. We observed accumulation of electroporated cells in the ventricular zone (VZ), which we interpret as a result of cytokinetic failure (e.g., Figure 2A, 4D). We also documented detachment of apical and basal endfeet (see Supplementary Figure 3), further supporting structural disruption of RG fibers.

      b) Cell death analysis:

      We considered using cleaved caspase-3 as a marker for apoptosis, but due to its transient and non-specific activation during development, we opted to assess overall survival via quantification of RGP cell numbers and localization. This approach better reflects the developmental impact of ABBA knockdown (Supplementary Figure 3).

      c) Migration defects:

      We agree that distinguishing between progenitor and neuroblast migration would be highly informative. Although we did not perform doublecortin or similar staining to differentiate these populations in this dataset, the accumulation of electroporated cells in VZ/SVZ strongly suggests a migration deficit. Addressing this in detail will require new experiments using lineage-specific markers and longer time-lapse recordings, which we plan to explore in future studies.

      d) Cleavage furrow and abscission:

      Our high-resolution imaging of Anillin-GFP and FRET-based RhoA activity shows that ABBA localizes predominantly at the cleavage furrow. New quantifications of RhoA activity (now in Figure 5) show that the reduction in signaling is most pronounced at the furrow in ABBA knockdown cells. These findings align with the hypothesis that ABBA, through Nedd9 and RhoA, is essential for proper furrow formation and abscission.

      e) Mechanistic implications:

      As the reviewer notes, ABBA knockdown leads to cells that "round" but do not complete division, likely due to poor cleavage furrow ingression. This could generate abnormal progenitors that are structurally compromised (detached fibers) and thus unable to support neuroblast migration or proper differentiation. The cumulative result is disrupted progression from RGPs to neuroblasts, impaired structural scaffolding, and possibly reduced cell viability.

      (3) Limited to a singular time point of mouse cortical development

      On page 13, the authors outline the results of their Y2H screen with the identification of three high confidence interactors. Notably, they used a E10.5-E12.5 mouse brain embryo library rather than one that includes E14, the age of their in utero electroporation mice. Many of the authors' claims focus on in utero electroporation of shRNA-Abba of E14 mice that are then evaluated at E16-18. Justification for the focus on this age range should be included to support that their findings can then be applied to all of mouse corticogenesis.

      Response to: Use of E10.5–E12.5 library for yeast-two-hybrid (Y2H) screen

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concern regarding the developmental stage of the Y2H library. We chose the E10.5–E12.5 brain embryo library based on prior work demonstrating that ABBA expression is strongest during early cortical development, particularly in radial glia at these stages (see Saarikangas et al., J Cell Sci 2008). The radial glia-specific expression of ABBA was previously validated using RC2 and Tuj1 markers at E12.5. Thus, the library we used is well-suited for identifying interactors relevant to radial glial function, including Nedd9. We have clarified this rationale in the revised manuscript.

      (4) Detail of the effect of the human variant of the ABBA mutation in mouse is lacking.

      Their identification of the R671W mutation is interesting and the IUE model warrants more characterization, as they did with their original KD experiments.

      - Could they show that Abba protein levels are decreased (in either cell lines or electroporated tissue)?

      - While time-lapse morphology might not have been performed, more analysis on cell division phenotype (such as plane of division and radial glia morphology) would be helpful.

      Response to: Lack of detail on R671W human variant effects

      We thank the reviewer for encouraging further characterization of the R671W variant. In the revised manuscript, we now provide additional data on interkinetic nuclear migration (INM) defects resulting from R671W overexpression (see Supplementary Figure 3). These changes are consistent with disrupted radial glial organization and mirror aspects of the ABBA knockdown phenotype.

      a) Protein levels:

      We quantified ABBA expression in cells overexpressing the R671W variant (Supplementary Figure 5) and found no significant reduction compared to wild-type. This argues against a loss-of-function mechanism and supports a gain-of-function or dominant-interfering effect.

      b) Morphological and division phenotyping:

      While time-lapse imaging of R671W-expressing cells was not available in our dataset, we acknowledge that analyses such as division angle or radial glial morphology would be informative. Unfortunately, we were unable to perform these with the current data, but we agree these are important goals for future work.

      Reviewer 2 conclusion:

      The resubmission has addressed many of the questions raised.

      I have a few comments that should be addressed:

      (1) The authors maintain a deficit in "migration of immature neurons" which remains unsubstantiated. In their resonse, they state: "we believe that the data showing the accumulation of migrating electroporated cells in the ventricular (V) and subventricular (SV) zones provide compelling evidence of abnormal migration in ABBA-shRNA electroporated cells. "

      - Firstly, they do not demonstrate that it's immature neurons, not RGs, that are affected. Secondly, accumulation of cells at the V-SVZ could be due to soley the inability for the RGC to undergo mitosis, therefore remaining stuck"

      The commentary of migration, especially of neurons, should be modified.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s careful reading and valid concern regarding our use of the term "migration of immature neurons." We fully agree that the current dataset does not definitively distinguish whether the accumulated cells in the ventricular (V) and subventricular (SV) zones are immature neurons or radial glial progenitors (RGPs) arrested in mitosis.

      To clarify, our observations indicate that electroporated cells accumulate in the VZ/SVZ following ABBA knockdown (Figures 2A and 4D), and this was interpreted as evidence of impaired migration. However, we now recognize that this accumulation may primarily reflect a block in cell cycle progression—specifically, at the stage of cleavage furrow ingression and abscission—rather than a migratory defect per se. This is supported by our new data using Anillin-GFP (Supplementary Figure 3), which show increased accumulation of cells with persistent Anillin expression, consistent with mitotic arrest. Furthermore, the detachment of apical and basal processes (also shown in Supplementary Figure 3) suggests that ABBA knockdown affects the structural integrity of RGPs, potentially compromising their scaffold function.

      In light of these points, we have revised the manuscript to temper our conclusions regarding “migration defects.” Specifically, we now refer to the phenotype as “abnormal accumulation of progenitor cells” and clarify that, while these findings are consistent with impaired cell progression or scaffolding required for migration, we do not directly demonstrate impaired migration of immature neurons. As suggested, addressing this would require additional analyses, such as time-lapse imaging of post-mitotic cells or staining with markers like Doublecortin, which are beyond the scope of the current dataset but will be a focus of future investigations.

      We thank the reviewer again for encouraging a more precise interpretation of our findings

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Supplementary Fig 4B - The figure doesn't show an increase in percentage of PH3 positive cells in the NEDD9-shRNA condition. The control images are also missing for comparison. The figure legend needs to be corrected to match with the figure showing no significant changes.

      Thank you for this comment. This has been amended in the revised manuscript in the form of a new revised Supplementary Fig 4.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor annotations for slice culture assay

      The authors should make note of ages of slice cultures in text and have better annotations of slice cultures (for example, in Fig 4-where is mitosis?)

      We are sorry for the mistake it's not mitosis, it's the cleavage furrow stage.  In addition, a new amended Figure 4 is provided. 

      The effects are hard to see in lower mag slice images in Fig. 6. Would recommend focusing on higher mag to highlight RG differences.

      Thank you for this comment. This has been amended in the revised manuscript in the form of a new revised Figure 6.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) The mechanism by which fenofibrate rescues memory loss in Kallistatin-transgenic mice is unclear. As a PPARalpha agonist, does fenofibrate target the Kallistatin pathway directly or indirectly? Please provide a discussion based on literature supporting either possibility.

      Thank you for your important suggestion. Fenofibrate is indeed acting as a PPARα agonist. Fenofibrate has been shown to protect memory and cognitive function by downregulating α- and β-secretases[1]. Activation of PPARα can reduce Aβ plaques by upregulating ADAM10, thereby protecting memory and cognition[2]. Whereas, Fenofibrate can also act through a PPARα-independent pathway[3]. In our previous study, we proved that Fenofibrate can directly down-regulate the expression of Kallistatin in hepatocytes[4]. Here, our findings showed that Kallistatin induces cognitive memory deterioration by increasing amyloid-β plaques accumulation and tau protein hyperphosphorylation (Fig. 1-3), and Fenofibrate can directly down-regulate the serum level of Kallistatin (Fig. 8G). In addition, the expression of PPARα in the hippocampal tissue of Kallistatin (KAL-TG) mice showed no significant difference compared to the WT group (Author response image 1A-B). Therefore, we think Fenofibrate may improve memory and cognitive function at least in part through a PPARα-independent effect, which provides a new mechanism of Fenofibrate in AD with elevated Kallistatin levels.

      Author response image 1.

      (A-B) Protein levels of PPARα were tested by western blot analysis in hippocampal tissue, then statistically analyzed the above results.

      (2) The current study exclusively investigated the hippocampus. What about other cognitive memory-related regions, such as the prefrontal cortex? Including data from these regions or discussing the possibility of their involvement could provide a more comprehensive understanding of the role of Kallistatin in memory impairment.

      Thank you for your suggestion. In addition to hippocampal tissue analysis, we performed immunohistochemical detection of Aβ and phosphorylated Tau levels in the prefrontal cortex. Our findings revealed that KAL-TG mice exhibited significantly elevated Aβ and phosphorylated Tau levels in the prefrontal cortex compared to WT mice. These observations align with the pathological patterns observed in hippocampal tissues, demonstrating consistent neurodegenerative pathology across both the hippocampus and prefrontal cortex. The data for this part are seen as follows.

      Author response image 2.

      (A-B) Immunofluorescence staining of Aβ and phosphorylated tau (p-tau T231) was carried out in the prefrontal cortex tissue of KAL-TG and WT mice. Error bars represented the Standard Error of Mean (SEM); **p < 0.01. Scale bar, 100 μm.

      (3) Fenofibrate rescued phenotypes in Kallistatin-transgenic mice while rosiglitazone, a PPARgamma agonist, did not. This result contradicts the manuscript's emphasis on a PPARgamma-associated mechanism. Please address this inconsistency.

      Thank you for the reminder. In fact, our results showed a trend towards improved memory and cognitive function in KAL-TG mice treated with Rosiglitazone, although its effect is not as significant as that of Fenofibrate. Several studies have reported that Rosiglitazone has a beneficial effect on memory and cognitive function in mouse models of dementia, while these studies involve treatment periods of 3 to 4 months[5, 6], whereas our treatment period was only one month. Extending the treatment period with Rosiglitazone may result in a more pronounced improvement. In addition, Fenofibrate may have a PPAR-independent pathway by downregulating Kallistatin directly as discussed above and then show stronger effects.

      (4) Most of the immunohistochemistry images are unclear. Inserts have similar magnification to the original representative images, making judgments difficult. Please provide larger inserts with higher resolution.

      According to your suggestion, we provided larger inserts with higher resolution in Fig 3A and Fig 4B, as follows:

      (5) The immunohistochemistry images in different figures were taken from different hippocampal subregions with different magnifications. Please maintain consistency, or explain why CA1, CA3, or DG was analyzed in each experiment.

      Thank you for your advice. The trends of changes in different brain regions(including CA1, CA3, or DG) are consistent. Following your suggestion, we have now selected the DG region replaced the different hippocampal subregions with the DG area, and re-conducted the statistical analysis in Fig 5I & 6C, as follows. Due to the significant deposition of Aβ only in the CA1 region, Fig 2A was not replaced.

      (6) Figure 5B is missing a title. Please add a title to maintain consistency with other graphs.

      Thanks for your suggestion. We have added a title to Figure 5B, as follows:

      (7) Please list statistical methods used in the figure legends, such as t-test or One-way ANOVA with post-hoc tests.

      Thanks for your suggestion. We have listed the statistical methods used in the figure legends.

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) It was suggested that Kallistatin is primarily produced by the liver. The study demonstrates increased Kallistatin levels in the hippocampus tissue of AD mice. It would be valuable to clarify if Kallistatin is also increased in the liver of AD mice, providing a comprehensive understanding of its distribution in disease states.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We extracted liver tissue from APP/PS1 mice, and the Western blot results indicated that the expression of Kallistatin in the liver of APP/PS1 mice was elevated, as follows:

      Author response image 3.

      (A-B) Protein levels of Kallistatin were tested by western blot analysis in the liver tissue, then statistically analyzed the above results. Error bars represented the Standard Error of Mean (SEM); **p < 0.01.

      (2) Does Kallistatin interact directly with Notch1 ligands? Clarifying this interaction mechanism would enhance understanding of how Kallistatin influences Notch1 signaling in AD pathology.

      Thank you for your suggestion. This study reveals that Kallistatin directly binds to Notch1 and contributes to the activation of the Noch1-HES1 signaling pathway. As for whether Kallistatin can bind to the ligands of Notch1, it needs to conduct further investigations in future studies. Our preliminary data showed that Jagged1 was upregulated in the hippocampal tissues of KAL-TG mice by qPCR and Western blot analyses.

      Author response image 4.

      Kallistatin promoted Notch ligand Jagged1 expression to activate Notch1 signaling. (A) QPCR analysis of Notch ligands (Dll1, Dll3, Jagged1, Jagged2) expression in the 9 months hippocampus tissue. (B) Western blotting analysis of Notch ligand Jagged1 expression in the hippocampus tissue. (C) Western blotting analysis of Notch ligand Jagged1 expression in the hippocampus primary neuron. β-actin served as the loading control. Error bars represented the Standard Error of Mean (SEM); *p < 0.05.

      (3) Is there any observed difference in AD phenotype between male and female Kallistatin-transgenic (KAL-TG) mice? Including this information would address potential gender-specific effects on cognitive decline and pathology.

      Thank you for your suggestion. Actually, we have previously used female mice for Morris Water Maze experiments, and the results showed that both male and female KAL-TG mice exhibited a phenotype of decreased memory and cognitive function compared to the gender-matched WT group, while there was no significant difference between male and female KAL-TG mice as follows:

      Author response image 5.

      (A-D) Behavioral performance was assessed through the Morris water maze test. (A) The escape latency time was presented during 1-5 days. (B-D) Cognitive functions were evaluated by spatial probe test on day 6, then analyzing each group of mice crossing platform times(B), time percent in the targeted area (C), and the path traces heatmap (D). Error bars represented the Standard Error of Mean (SEM); F represents Female, M represents Male, and TG refers to KAL-TG; *p < 0.05.

      (4) It is recommended to include molecular size markers in Western blots for clarity and accuracy in protein size determination.

      Thank you for your reminder. We have shown the molecular weight of each bolt.

      (5) The language should be revised for enhanced readability and clarity, ensuring that complex scientific concepts are communicated effectively to a broader audience.

      According to your suggestion, we have polished the article for enhancing readability and clarity.

      Reviewer #3:

      (1) The authors did not illustrate whether the protective effect of fenofibrate against AD depends on Kallistatin.

      Thank you for your important suggestion. Fenofibrate is indeed acting as a PPARα agonist. Fenofibrate has been shown to protect memory and cognitive function by downregulating α- and β-secretases[1]. Activation of PPARα can reduce Aβ plaques by upregulating ADAM10, thereby protecting memory and cognition[2]. Whereas, Fenofibrate can also act through a PPARα-independent pathway[3]. In our previous study,we proved Fenofibrate can directly down-regulate the expression of KAL in hepatocytes[4]. Here, our findings showed that Kallistatin induces cognitive memory deterioration by increasing amyloid-β plaques accumulation and tau protein hyperphosphorylation (Fig. 1-3), and Fenofibrate can directly down-regulate the serum level of Kallistatin (Fig. 8G). In addition, the expression of PPARα in the hippocampal tissue of Kallistatin (KAL-TG) mice showed no significant difference compared to the WT group (Author response image 1-B). Therefore, we think Fenofibrate may improve memory and cognitive function at least in part through downregulatin Kallistatin. To conclusively determine whether fenofibrate’s therapeutic effects depend on Kallistatin, future studies should employ Kallistatin-knockout AD animal models to evaluate fenofibrate’s impact on cognitive and memory functions. These investigations will further clarify the mechanistic underpinnings of fenofibrate in AD therapy.

      (2) The conclusions are supported by the results, but the quality of some results should be improved.

      Thank you for your kind suggestion. We have updated the magnified images in the immunohistochemistry section of the article, ensuring that the fields of view for the immunohistochemistry are within the same brain region, and have shown the molecular weights in each bolt. Additionally, we have conducted a quantitative analysis of the protein levels in the Western blot results presented in Fig6&8.

      (3) Figures 2c, 3c, and 4a present the Western blot results of p-tau from mice of different ages on one membrane, showing age-dependent expression. The authors analyzed the results of mice of different ages in one statistical chart, which will create ambiguity with the results of the representative images. For example, the expression of p-tau 396 in the blot was lower in the WT-12 M group than in the WT-9 M group (Figure 3c), which is contradictory to the statistical analysis.

      Thank you for your reminder. The statistical presentation here does not match the figure. At that time, the WB experiments for the hippocampal tissue at each age group were conducted separately, and it was not appropriate to compare different age groups together. This graph cannot illustrate age dependency. We have replaced the statistical graph in Figure 3B&D, as follows:

      (4) Figure 4b shows that KAL-TG-9 M had greater BACE1 expression than KAL-TG-12 M. Furthermore, the nuclei are not uniformly colored. Please provide more representative figures.

      Thank you for your reminder. Due to the fact that these sets of data were not processed in a single batch, the ages in the graph are not comparable. Regarding the issue of inconsistent nuclear staining, we have provided another representative image from this group, as follows:

      (5) Unclear why the BACE1 and Aβ levels seems less with KAL+shHES1 treatment than GFP+shNC treatment (Fig 6H)? This finding contradicts the conclusion.

      Thank you for your reminder. This experiment was repeated three times, and here, we have represented the representative results along with the corresponding statistical data. There are no difference between KAL+shHES1 treatment and GFP+shNC treatment. We have updated the Fig. 6H.

      (6) The Western blot results in figure 6e-h, 8h-i, and S3-S5 were not quantified.

      Thank you for your reminder. We have added statistical graphs and original images of the pictures in figure 6e-h, 8h-i, and S3-S5.

      (7) The authors did not provide the detection range of the Aβ42 ELISA kit.

      Thank you for your suggestion. The Aβ42 ELISA kit is from the IBL, with the product number 27721. Its standard range is 1.56 - 100 pg/mL, and the sensitivity is 0.05 pg/mL.

      (8)The authors did not specify the sex of the mice. This is important since sex could have had a dramatic impact on the results.

      Thank you for your suggestion. The results we present in the text are all statistically obtained from male mice. Actually, we have previously used female mice for Morris Water Maze experiments, and the results showed that both male and female KAL-TG mice exhibited a phenotype of decreased memory and cognitive function compared to the gender-matched WT group, while there was no significant difference between male and female KAL-TG mice (Author response image 5).

      Minor:

      (1) In Figure 2b, there are no units for the vertical coordinates of the statistical graph.

      Thank you for your reminder. We have added units for the vertical coordinates in Figure 2b.

      (2) In Figure 2c, the left Y-axis title is lacking in the statistic chart.

      Thank you for your reminder. We have added the left Y-axis title in the statistic chart.

      Reference:

      (1) Assaf N, El-Shamarka ME, Salem NA, Khadrawy YA, El Sayed NS. Neuroprotective effect of PPAR alpha and gamma agonists in a mouse model of amyloidogenesis through modulation of the Wnt/beta catenin pathway via targeting alpha- and beta-secretases. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 2020, 97: 109793.

      (2) Rangasamy SB, Jana M, Dasarathi S, Kundu M, Pahan K. Treadmill workout activates PPARα in the hippocampus to upregulate ADAM10, decrease plaques and improve cognitive functions in 5XFAD mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity 2023, 109: 204-218.

      (3) Yuan J, Tan JTM, Rajamani K, Solly EL, King EJ, Lecce L, et al. Fenofibrate Rescues Diabetes-Related Impairment of Ischemia-Mediated Angiogenesis by PPARα-Independent Modulation of Thioredoxin-Interacting Protein. Diabetes 2019, 68(5): 1040-1053.

      (4) Fang Z, Shen G, Wang Y, Hong F, Tang X, Zeng Y, et al. Elevated Kallistatin promotes the occurrence and progression of non-alcoholic fatty liver disease. Signal Transduct Target Ther 2024, 9(1): 66.

      (5) Nelson ML, Pfeifer JA, Hickey JP, Collins AE, Kalisch BE. Exploring Rosiglitazone's Potential to Treat Alzheimer's Disease through the Modulation of Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor. Biology (Basel) 2023, 12(7).

      (6) Pedersen WA, McMillan PJ, Kulstad JJ, Leverenz JB, Craft S, Haynatzki GR. Rosiglitazone attenuates learning and memory deficits in Tg2576 Alzheimer mice. Exp Neurol 2006, 199(2): 265-273.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      The authors investigated the role of the C. elegans Flower protein, FLWR-1, in synaptic transmission, vesicle recycling, and neuronal excitability. They confirmed that FLWR-1 localizes to synaptic vesicles and the plasma membrane and facilitates synaptic vesicle recycling at neuromuscular junctions. They observed that hyperstimulation results in endosome accumulation in flwr-1 mutant synapses, suggesting that FLWR-1 facilitates the breakdown of endocytic endosomes. Using tissue-specific rescue experiments, the authors showed that expressing FLWR-1 in GABAergic neurons restored the aldicarb-resistant phenotype of flwr-1 mutants to wild-type levels. By contrast, cholinergic neuron expression did not rescue aldicarb sensitivity at all. They also showed that FLWR-1 removal leads to increased Ca<sup>2+</sup> signaling in motor neurons upon photo-stimulation. From these findings, the authors conclude that FLWR-1 helps maintain the balance between excitation and inhibition (E/I) by preferentially regulating GABAergic neuronal excitability in a cell-autonomous manner. 

      Overall, the work presents solid data and interesting findings, however the proposed cell-autonomous model of GABAergic FLWR-1 function may be overly simplified in my opinion. 

      Most of my previous comments have been addressed; however, two issues remain. 

      (1) I appreciate the authors' efforts conducting additional aldicarb sensitivity assays that combine muscle-specific rescue with either cholinergic or GABergic neuron-specific expression of FLWR-1. In the revised manuscript, they conclude, "This did not show any additive effects to the pure neuronal rescues, thus FLWR-1 effects on muscle cell responses to cholinergic agonists must be cellautonomous." However, I find this interpretation confusing for the reasons outlined below. 

      Figure 1 - Figure Supplement 3B shows that muscle-specific FLWR-1 expression in flwr-1 mutants significantly restores aldicarb sensitivity. However, when FLWR-1 is co-expressed in both cholinergic neurons and muscle, the worms behave like flwr-1 mutants and no rescue is observed. Similarly, cholinergic FLWR-1 alone fails to restore aldicarb sensitivity (shown in the previous manuscript).

      This data is still shown in the manuscript, Fig. 3D. We interpreted our finding in the muscle/cholinergic co-rescue experiment as meaning, that FLWR-1 in cholinergic neurons over-compensates, so worms should be resistant, and the rescuing effect of muscle FLWR-1 is therefore cancelled. But it is true, if this were the case, why does the pure cholinergic rescue not show over-compensation? We added a sentence to acknowledge this inconsistency and we added a sentence in the discussion (see also below, comment 1) of reviewer #2).

      These observations indicate a non-cell-autonomous interaction between cholinergic neurons and muscle, rather than a strictly muscle cell-autonomous mechanism. In other words, FLWR-1 expressed in cholinergic neurons appears to negate or block the rescue effect of muscle-expressed FLWR-1. Therefore, FLWR-1 could play a more complex role in coordinating physiology across different tissues. This complexity may affect interpretations of Ca<sup>2+</sup> dynamics and/or functional data, particularly in relation to E/I balance, and thus warrants careful discussion or further investigation. 

      For the Ca<sup>2+</sup> dynamics, we think the effects of flwr-1 are likely very immediate, as the imaging assay relies on a sensor expressed directly in the neurons or muscles under study, and not on indirect phenotypes as muscle contraction and behavior, that depend on an interplay of several cell types influencing each other.

      (2) The revised manuscript includes new GCaMP analyses restricted to synaptic puncta. The authors mention that "we compared Ca<sup>2+</sup> signals in synaptic puncta versus axon shafts, and did not find any differences," concluding that "FLWR-1's impact is local, in synaptic boutons." This is puzzling: the similarity of Ca<sup>2+</sup> signals in synaptic regions and axon shafts seems to indicate a more global effect on Ca<sup>2+</sup> dynamics or may simply reflect limited temporal resolution in distinguishing local from global signals due to rapid Ca<sup>2+</sup> diffusion. The authors should clarify how they reached the conclusion that FLWR-1 has a localized impact at synaptic boutons, given that synaptic and axonal signals appear similar. Based on the presented data, the evidence supporting a local effect of FLWR-1 on Ca<sup>2+</sup> dynamics appears limited.

      We apologize, here we simply overlooked this misleading wording in our rebuttal letter. The data we mentioned, showing no obvious difference in axon vs. bouton, are shown below, including time constants for the onset and the offset of the stimulus (data is peak normalized for better visualization):

      Author response image 1.

      One can see that axonal Ca<sup>2+</sup> signals may rise a bit slower than synaptic Ca<sup>2+</sup> signals, as expected for Ca<sup>2+</sup> entering the boutons, and then diffusing out into the axon. The loss of FLWR1 does not affect this. However, the temporal resolution of the used GCaMP6f sensor is ca. 200 ms to reach peak, and the decay time (to t1/2) is ca. 400 ms (PMID: 23868258). Thus, it would be difficult to see effects based on Ca<sup>2+</sup> diffusion using this assay. For the decay, this is similar for both axon and synapse, while flwr-1 mutants do not reduce Ca<sup>2+</sup> as much as wt. In the axon, there is a seemingly slightly slower reduction in flwr-1 mutants, however, given the kinetics of the sensor, this is likely not a meaningful difference. Therefore, we wrote we did not find differences. The interpretation should not have been that the impact of FLWR-1 is local. It may be true if one could image this at faster time scales, i.e. if there is more FLWR-1 localized in boutons (as indicated by our data showing FLWR-1 enrichment in boutons; Fig. 3), and when considering its possible effect on MCA-3 localization (and assuming that MCA-3 is the active player in Ca<sup>2+</sup> removal), i.e. FLWR-1 recruiting MCA-3 to boutons (Fig. 9C, D).  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The Flower protein is expressed in various cell types, including neurons. Previous studies in flies have proposed that Flower plays a role in neuronal endocytosis by functioning as a Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel. However, its precise physiological roles and molecular mechanisms in neurons remain largely unclear. This study employs C. elegans as a model to explore the function and mechanism of FLWR-1, the C. elegans homolog of Flower. This study offers intriguing observations that could potentially challenge or expand our current understanding of the Flower protein. Nevertheless, further clarification or additional experiments are required to substantiate the study's conclusions. 

      Strengths: 

      A range of approaches was employed, including the use of a flwr-1 knockout strain, assessment of cholinergic synaptic activity via analyzing aldicarb (a cholinesterase inhibitor) sensitivity, imaging Ca<sup>2+</sup> dynamics with GCaMP3, analyzing pHluorin fluorescence, examination of presynaptic ultrastructure by EM, and recording postsynaptic currents at the neuromuscular junction. The findings include notable observations on the effects of flwr-1 knockout, such as increased Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels in motor neurons, changes in endosome numbers in motor neurons, altered aldicarb sensitivity, and potential involvement of a Ca<sup>2+</sup>-ATPase and PIP2 binding in FLWR-1's function. 

      The authors have adequately addressed most of my previous concerns, however, I recommend minor revisions to further strengthen the study's rigor and interpretation: 

      Major suggestions 

      (1) This study relies heavily on aldicarb assays to support its conclusions. While these assays are valuable, their results may not fully align with direct assessment of neurotransmitter release from motor neurons. For instance, prior work has shown that two presynaptic modulators identified through aldicarb sensitivity assays exhibited no corresponding electrophysiological defects at the neuromuscular junction (Liu et al., J Neurosci 27: 10404-10413, 2007). Similarly, at least one study from the Kaplan lab has noted discrepancies between aldicarb assays and electrophysiological analyses. The authors should consider adding a few sentences in the Discussion to acknowledge this limitation and the potential caveats of using aldicarb assays, especially since some of the aldicarb assay results in this study are not easily interpretable. 

      Aldicarb assays have been used very successfully in identifying mutants with defects in chemical synaptic transmission, and entire genetic screens have been conducted this way. The reviewer is right, one needs to realize that it is the balance of excitation and inhibition at the NMJ of C. elegans, which underlies the effects on the rate of aldicarb-induced paralysis, not just cholinergic transmission. I.e. if a given mutant affects cholinergic and GABAergic transmission differently, things become difficult to interpret, particularly if also muscle physiology is affected. Therefore, we combined mutant analyses with cell-type specific rescue. We acknowledge that results are nonetheless difficult to interpret. We thus added a sentence in the first paragraph of the discussion.

      (2) The manuscript states, "Elevated Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels were not further enhanced in a flwr-1;mca-3 double mutant." (lines 549-550). However, Figure 7C does not include statistical comparisons between the single and double mutants of flwr-1 and mca-3. Please add the necessary statistical analysis to support this statement. 

      Because we only marked significant differences in that figure, and n.s. was not shown. This was stated in the figure legend.

      (3) The term "Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx" should be avoided, as this study does not provide direct evidence (e.g. voltage-clamp recordings of Ca<sup>2+</sup> inward currents in motor neurons) for an effect of the flwr-1 mutation of Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx. The observed increase in neuronal GCaMP signals in response to optogenetic activation of ChR2 may result from, or be influenced by, Ca<sup>2+</sup> mobilization from of intracellular stores. For example, optogenetic stimulation could trigger ryanodine receptor-mediated Ca<sup>2+</sup> release from the ER via calcium-induced calcium release (CICR) or depolarization-induced calcium release (DICR). It would be more appropriate to describe the observed increase in Ca<sup>2+</sup> signal as "Ca<sup>2+</sup> elevation" rather than increased "Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx". 

      Ok, yes, we can do this, we referred by ‘influx’ to cytosolic Ca<sup>2+</sup>, that fluxes into the cytosol, be it from the internal stores or the extracellular. Extracellular influx, more or less, inevitably will trigger further influx from internal stores, to our understanding. We changed this to “elevated Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels” or “Ca<sup>2+</sup> level rise” or “Ca<sup>2+</sup> level increase”.

      Recommendations for the authors: 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      A thorough discussion on the impact of cell-autonomous versus non-cell-autonomous effects is necessary. 

      Revise and clarify the distinction between local and global Ca²⁺ changes. 

      see above.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Minor suggestions 

      (1) In "Few-Ubi was shown to facilitate recovery of neurons following intense synaptic activity (Yao et al.,....." (lines 283-284), please specify which aspects of neuronal recovery are influenced by the Flower protein. 

      We added “refilling of SV pools”.

      (2) The abbreviation "Few-Ubi" is used for the Drosophila Flower protein (e.g., line 283, Figure 1A, and Figure 8A). Please clarify what "Ubi" stands for and verify whether its inclusion in the protein name is appropriate.

      This is inconsistent across the literature, sometimes Fwe-Ubi is also referred to as FweA. We now added this term. Ubi refers to ubiquitous (“Therefore, we named this isoform fweubi because it is expressed ubiquitously in imaginal discs“) (Rhiner 2010)

      (3) The manuscript uses "pflwr-1" (line 303 and elsewhere) to denote the flwr-1 promoter. This notation could be misleading, as it may be interpreted as a gene name. Please consider using either "flwr-1p" or "Pflwr-1" instead. Additionally, ensure proper italicization of gene names throughout the manuscript. 

      We changed this throughout. We will change to italicized at proof stage, it would be too timeconsuming to spot these incidents now.

      (4) The authors tagged the C-terminus of FLWR-1 by GFP (lines 321). The fusion protein is referred to as "GFP::FLWR-1" throughout the manuscript. Please verify whether "FLWR-1::GFP" would be the more appropriate designation.

      Thank you, yes, we changed this in the text, GFP is indeed N-terminal.

      (5) In "This did not show any additive effects...." (line 363), please clarify what "This" refers to. 

      Altered to “The combined rescues did not show any additive effects…”

      (6) In "..., supporting our previous finding of increased neurotransmitter release in GABAergic neurons" (lines 412-413), please provide a citation for the referenced previous study.

      This refers to our aldicarb data within this paper, just further up in the text. We removed “previous”.

      (7) Figure 4C, D examines the effect of flwr-1 mutation on body length in the genetic background of the unc-29 mutation, which selectively disrupts the levamisole-sensitive acetylcholine receptor. Please comment on the rationale for implicating only the levamisole receptor rather than the nicotinic acetylcholine receptor in muscle cells. 

      This was because we used a behavioral assay. Despite the fact that the homopentameric ACR16/N-AChR mediate about 2/3 of the peak currents in response to acute ACh application to the NMJ (e.g. Almedom et al., EMBO J, 2009), the acr-16 mutant has virtually no behavioral / locomotion phenotype. Likely, this is because the heteropentameric, UNC-29 containing LAChR, while only contributing 1/3 of the peak current, desensitizes much more slowly and thus unc-29 mutants show a severe behavioral phenotype (uncoordinated locomotion, etc.). We thus did not expect a major effect when performing the behavoral assay in acr-16 mutants and thus chose the unc-29 mutant background.

      (8) In "we found no evidence ....insertion into the PM (Yao et al., 2009)", It appears that the cited paper was not authored by any of the current manuscript. Please confirm whether this citation is correctly attributed. 

      This sentence was arranged in a misleading way, we did not mean that we authored this paper. It was change in the text: “While a facilitating role of Flower in endocytosis appears to be conserved in C. elegans, in contrast to previous findings from Drosophila (Yao et al., 2009), we found no evidence that FLWR-1 conducts Ca<sup>2+</sup> upon insertion into the PM.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Xiao et al. classified retroperitoneal liposarcoma (RPLS) patients into two subgroups based on whole transcriptome sequencing of 88 patients. The G1 group was characterized by active metabolism, while the G2 group exhibited high scores in cell cycle regulation and DNA damage repair. The G2 group also displayed more aggressive molecular features and had worse clinical outcomes compared to G1. Using a machine learning model, the authors simplified the classification system, identifying LEP and PTTG1 as the key molecular markers distinguishing the two RPLS subgroups. Finally, they validated these markers in a larger cohort of 241 RPLS patients using immunohistochemistry. Overall, the manuscript is clear and well-organized, with its significance rooted in the large sample size and the development of a classification method.

      Thank you for your positive assessment of our study on classifying RPLS patients based on whole transcriptome sequencing. We appreciate your recognition of the distinct characteristics of the G1 and G2 groups, as well as the significance of our simplified classification system and the identification of LEP and PTTG1 as key molecular markers. Your acknowledgment of the clarity and organization of our manuscript, along with the importance of the large sample size, is greatly appreciated. We will continue to refine our work based on your feedback as we prepare for resubmission.

      Weakness:

      (1) While the authors suggest that LEP and PTTG1 serve as molecular markers for the two RPLS groups, the process through which these genes were selected remains unclear. The authors should provide a detailed explanation of the selection process.

      The selection criteria for identifying LEP and PTTG1 as biomarkers involved selecting prognostic genes that were highly expressed in C1 and C2, respectively, and achieved the highest AUC value in distinguishing the two RPLS groups (Page17 lines 288-290).

      (2) To ensure the broader applicability of LEP and PTTG1 as classification markers, the authors should validate their findings in one or two external datasets.

      We sincerely appreciate your insightful suggestion regarding the external validation of LEP and PTTG1 as classification biomarkers. To address this concern, we performed an independent validation using an external liposarcoma cohort (GSE30929; Page 6, Lines 104-105)), which comprises 140 primary liposarcoma samples with annotated clinicopathological and survival data. This dataset was selected due to its relevance to RPLS (N=63, 45%) and the availability of distant recurrence-free survival (DRFS) outcomes, aligning with the clinical focus of our study. 

      Applying our previously established prognostic model (Risk value = 2.182 × PTTG1 - 2.204 × LEP) to this cohort, we stratified patients into high- and low-risk groups using the median risk score as the cutoff. Consistent with our original findings, the high-risk group exhibited significantly worse DRFS compared to the low-risk group. The ROC curves based on the 1-, 3-, 5-year survival status of patients demonstrated that this model can effectively predict patient DRFS (log-rank P < 0.001, Figure S3A-B). Furthermore, the high-risk group demonstrated a higher proportion of high-grade histology (P < 0.001, Fisher’s exact test, Figure S3C-D).

      These results validate the robustness and generalizability of our risk stratification model across distinct liposarcoma cohorts. The external dataset’s alignment with our findings underscores the potential of LEP and PTTG1 as reproducible biomarkers for prognosis and therapeutic stratification in liposarcoma. We have incorporated these validation results into the revised manuscript (Page 18, Lines 305-315) to strengthen the clinical applicability of our conclusions.

      (3) Since molecular subtyping is often used to guide personalized treatment strategies, it is recommended that the authors evaluate therapeutic responses in the two distinct groups. Additionally, they should validate these predictions using cell lines or primary cells.

      We sincerely appreciate your insightful comments and suggestions regarding the evaluation of therapeutic responses and the validation of our predictions using cell lines or primary cells. We would like to address these points in detail below:

      (1) Purpose of the PTTG1- and LEP-based RPLS Classification Model

      The primary objective of our study was to develop a molecular subtyping model based on PTTG1 and LEP to guide personalized treatment strategies for patients with RPLS, particularly those classified as low-grade by traditional histopathological criteria but exhibiting poor prognosis. This subgroup of patients may benefit from more aggressive surgical resection, which is a potentially curative approach for RPLS. Our model aims to identify these high-risk patients to ensure complete tumor resection, thereby improving their clinical outcomes.

      (2) Therapeutic Response Evaluation in Distinct Groups

      In both our validation cohort and external validation cohort, surgical resection was the primary treatment modality for RPLS. After stratifying patients using our model, we observed significant differences in surgical outcomes between the two groups: the high-risk group exhibited poor prognosis, while the low-risk group showed favorable outcomes (Figure 5D-E and Figure S3A-B). Importantly, our model successfully identified low-grade histopathological cases with poor prognosis, who might otherwise be undertreated (Figure 5G-I and Figure S3C-D). By advocating for more thorough surgical resection in these high-risk patients, we aim to improve their prognosis. This achievement aligns with the primary goal of our study, which is to provide a molecular tool for personalized treatment guidance.

      (3) Future Validation and Functional Exploration of PTTG1 and LEP

      Our study has identified PTTG1 and LEP as key biomarkers for RPLS classification, and we recognize the urgent need to elucidate their molecular functions in RPLS pathogenesis. Here, we are pleased to report that we have already initiated cellular and animal experiments to investigate the roles of PTTG1 and LEP in RPLS. These experiments aim to validate our predictions and explore the underlying mechanisms by which these biomarkers contribute to tumor behavior and treatment response. We anticipate that the results of these studies will provide further mechanistic insights and will be submitted for publication in a suitable journal in the near future.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Surgical resection remains the most effective treatment for retroperitoneal liposarcoma. However, postoperative recurrence is very common and is considered the main cause of disease-related death. Considering the importance and effectiveness of precision medicine, the identification of molecular characteristics is particularly important for the prognosis assessment and individualized treatment of RPLS. In this work, the authors described the gene expression map of RPLS and illustrated an innovative strategy of molecular classification. Through the pathway enrichment of differentially expressed genes, characteristic abnormal biological processes were identified, and RPLS patients were simply categorized based on the two major abnormal biological processes. Subsequently, the classification strategy was further simplified through nonnegative matrix factorization. The authors finally narrowed the classification indicators to two characteristic molecules LEP and PTTG1, and constructed novel molecular prognosis models that presented obviously a great area under the curve. A relatively interpretable logistic regression model was selected to obtain the risk scoring formula, and its clinical relevance and prognostic evaluation efficiency were verified by immunohistochemistry. Recently, prognostic model construction has been a hot topic in the field of oncology. The interesting point of this study is that it effectively screened characteristic molecules and practically simplified the typing strategy on the basis of ensuring high matching clinical relevance. Overall, the study is well-designed and will serve as a valuable resource for RPLS research.

      Thank you for your insightful feedback on our manuscript. We appreciate your recognition of the importance of precision medicine and molecular characteristics in improving prognosis and individualized treatment for RPLS.

      We are pleased that you found our gene expression mapping and innovative molecular classification strategy valuable. Your positive remarks on our pathway enrichment analysis and the categorization of RPLS patients based on abnormal biological processes affirm our approach.

      We are also grateful for your acknowledgment of our focus on the characteristic molecules LEP and PTTG1, as well as the development of novel molecular prognosis models with significant predictive capability.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Joint Public Review:

      Summary

      In this manuscript, Dong et al. study the directed cell migration of tracheal stem cells in Drosophila pupae. The authors study how the directionality of these cells is regulated along the dorsal trunk. They show that inter-organ communication between the tracheal stem cells and the nearby fat body plays a role in posterior migration. They provide compelling evidence that Upd2 production in the fat body and JAK/STAT activation in the tracheal stem cells play a role. Moreover, they show that JAK/STAT signalling might induce the expression of apicobasal and planar cell polarity genes in the tracheal stem cells which appear to be needed to ensure unidirectional migration. Finally, the authors suggest that trafficking and vesicular transport of Upd2 from the fat body towards the tracheal cells might be important.

      Strengths

      The manuscript is well written and presents extensive and varied experimental data to show a link between Upd2-JAK/STAT signaling from the fat body and tracheal progenitor cell migration. The authors provide convincing evidence that the fat body, located near the trachea, secretes vesicles containing the Upd2 cytokine and that affecting JAK-STAT signaling results in aberrant migration of some of the tracheal stem cells towards the anterior. Using ChIP-seq as well as analysis of GFP-protein trap lines of planar cell polarity genes in combination with RNAi experiments, the authors show that STAT92E likely regulates the transcription of planar cell polarity genes and some apicobasal cell polarity genes in tracheal stem cells which appear to be needed for unidirectional migration. The work presented here provides some novel insights into the mechanism that ensures polarized migration of tracheal stem cells, preventing bidirectional migration. This might have important implications for other types of directed cell migration in invertebrates or vertebrates including cancer cell migration. Overall, the authors have substantially improved their manuscript since the first submission but there are still some weaknesses.

      Weaknesses

      Overall, the manuscript lacks insights into the potential significance of the observed phenotypes and of the proposed new signaling model. Most of our concerns could be dealt with by adjusting the text (explaining some parts better and toning down some statements).

      (1) Directional migration of tracheal progenitors is only partially compromised, with some cells migrating anteriorly and others maintaining their posterior migration, a quite discrete phenotype.

      The strongest migration defects quantified in graphs (e.g. 100 μm) are not shown in images, since they would be out of frame, it would be beneficial to see them. In addition, the consequence of defects in polarized migration on tracheal development is not clear and data showing phenotypes on the final trachea morphology in pupae are not explained nor linked to the previous phenotypes.

      We agree with you that it is informative to show strong anterior migration (> 100 μm). Accordingly, we have shown examples in Figure 3B and Figure 7R-S. In addition, we have also discuss on the links between migration defects and the consequential phenotypes of the animal at a later developmental stage in the revised manuscript. The undisciplined migration leads to insufficient regeneration and incomplete remodeling of airway and causes pupal lethality.

      (2) Some important information is lacking, such as the origin of mutant and UAS-RNAi lines, which are not reported in the material and methods. For instance, mutants for components of the JAK-STAT pathway are used but not described. Are they all viable at the pupal stage? Otherwise, pupae would not be homozygous mutants. From the figure legend, it seems that the Stat92EF allele has been used, which is a point mutation, thus not leading to an absence of protein. If the hopTUM allele has been used, as mentioned in the legend, it is a gain-of-function allele. Thus, the authors should not conclude that "The aberrant anterior migration of tracheal progenitors in the absence of JAK/STAT components led to impairment of tracheal integrity and caused melanization in the trachea (Figure 3-figure supplement 1E-I)".

      We apologize for inadequate description of the experimental materials and methods. We have listed the stock number of mutant and RNAi alleles in Key resource table and Materials. The mutant alleles that we chose to examine can survive to pupal stage, which is key to the success of our subsequent characterization of these mutants. According to your suggestion, we modified the statement for accuracy.

      (3) The authors observe that tracheal progenitors display a polarized distribution of Fat that is controlled by JAK-STAT signaling. However, this conclusion is made from a single experiment using only 3 individuals with no statistics. This is insufficient to support the claim that "JAK/STAT signaling promotes the expression of genes involved in planar cell polarity leading to asymmetric localization of Fat in progenitor cells", as mentioned in the abstract, or that "the activated tracheal progenitors establish a disciplined migration through the asymmetrical distribution of polarity proteins which is directed by an Upd2-JAK/STAT signaling stemming from the remote organ of fat body."

      We performed multiple biological replicates for Ft distribution experiments and observed similar trend, although we only showed three representative samples. In the revised text, we have included n number for statistic representation and statistic test.

      (4) The authors demonstrate that Upd2 is transported through vesicles from the fat body to the tracheal progenitors. It remains somewhat unclear in the proposed model how Upd2 activates JAK-STAT signaling. Are vesicles internalized, as it seems to be proposed, and thus how does Upd2 activate JAK-STAT signaling intracellularly? Or is Upd2 released from vesicles to bind Dome extracellularly to activate the JAK-STAT pathway? Moreover, it is not clear nor discussed what would be the advantage of transporting the ligand in vesicles compared to classical ligand diffusion.

      We do not know whether the association between Upd2 and Lbm is inside or outside vesicles. The vesicular trafficking of Upd2 is our observation and supported by various genetic and biochemical experiments. Our research does not imply the message that this vesicular trafficking has advantage over diffusion.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Although the use of antimony has been discontinued in India, the observation that there are Leishmania parasites that are resistant to antimony in circulation has been cited as evidence that these resistant parasites are now a distinct strain with properties that ensure their transmission and persistence. It is of interest to determine what are the properties that favor the retention of their drug resistance phenotype even in the absence of the selective pressure that would otherwise be conferred by the drug. The hypothesis that these authors set out to test is that these parasites have developed a new capacity to acquire and utilize lipids, especially cholesterol which affords them the capacity to grow robustly in infected hosts.

      We sincerely appreciate Reviewer 1's thoughtful and positive evaluation of our manuscript. We acknowledge that the reviewer has a few major concerns, and we would like to address them one by one in the following section.

      Major issues:

      (1) There are several experiments for which they do not provide sufficient details, but proceed to make significant conclusions.

      Experiments in section 5 are poorly described. They supposedly isolated PVs from infected cells. No details of their protocol for the isolation of PVs are provided. They reference a protocol for PV isolation that focused on the isolation of PVs after L. amazonensis infection. In the images of infection that they show, by 24 hrs, infected cells harbor a considerable number of parasites. Is it at the 24 hr time point that they recover PVs? What is the purity of PVs? The authors should provide evidence of the success of this protocol in their hands. Earlier, they mentioned that using imaging techniques, the PVs seem to have fused or interconnected somehow. Does this affect the capacity to recover PVs? If more membranes are recovered in the PV fraction, it may explain the higher cholesterol content.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for correctly pointing out lack of details regarding PV isolation and its purity. There are multiple questions raised by the reviewer and we will answer them one by one in a point wise manner:

      Firstly, “Is it at the 24 hr time point that they recover PVs?”

      In the ‘Methods’ section of the original submission (Line number 606-611), there is a separate section on “Parasitophorous vacuole (PV) Isolation and cholesterol measurement”, where it is clearly mentioned, “24Hrs LD infected KCs were lysed by passing through a 22-gauge syringe needle to release cellular contents. Parasitophorous vacuoles (PV) were then isolated using a previously outlined protocol [Ref: 73].” However, we do acknowledge further details might be useful to enrich this section, and hence we would like to include the following details in the Methods section of the revised manuscript, Line 663-678 “Parasitophorous vacuoles (PV) were isolated using a previously outlined protocol with slight modifications [76]. 107 KCs were seeded in a 100 mm plate and allowed to adhere for 24Hrs. Following this infection was performed with Leishmania donovani (LD) for 24Hrs, the infected KCs were then harvested by gentle scraping and lysed through five successive passages through an insulin needle to ensure membrane disruption while preserving organelle integrity. The lysate was centrifuged at 200 × g for 10mins at 4°C to remove intact cells and large debris. The resulting supernatant was carefully collected and subjected to a discontinuous sucrose density gradient (60%, 40%, and 20%). The gradient was centrifuged at 700 × g for 25mins at 4°C to facilitate organelle separation. The interphase between the 40% and 60% sucrose layers, enriched with PVs, was carefully collected and subjected to a final centrifugation step at 12,000 × g for 25mins at 4°C. The supernatant was discarded, and the resulting pellet was enriched for purified parasitophorous vacuoles, suitable for downstream biochemical and molecular analyses. Cholesterol and protein contents in PV were determined by an Amplex Red assay kit and Bradford assay, respectively. Resulting data were represented as micrograms of cholesterol per microgram of protein.”

      Secondly, What is the purity of PVs? Earlier, they mentioned that using imaging techniques, the PVs seem to have fused or interconnected somehow. Does this affect the capacity to recover PVs? If more membranes are recovered in the PV fraction, it may explain the higher cholesterol content.

      We appreciate the reviewer for pointing this critical lack of data in the submitted manuscript. In the revised manuscript, we have now provided data on the purity of isolated fraction by performing Confocal imaging and Western blot against PV and cytoplasmic fraction in the revised manuscript. We admit, as rightly pointed out by the reviewer we need to access the purity of isolated PV in our experiment. As suggested by the reviewer, we have included the results of this experiment in the Figure 3C i, C ii and C iii. Our results clearly showed an efficient PV isolation with demarcating LAMP-1 positive staining around LD amastigotes, which was further validated by Western Blot showing a significant enrichment of LAMP-1 specifically in the PV fraction. This has been included as (Line 225-234), in the revised manuscript which read as, “Parasitophorous vacuole fractions were isolated from LD-S and LD-R-infected KCs at 24Hrs p.i. using a previously established protocol [35]. Following isolation, PV purity was confirmed through LAMP-1 staining which showed a significant enrichment around isolated PV in Confocal microscopy (Figure 3C i). Purity of isolated PV fractions was further confirmed by Western blot which showed an enhanced enrichment of LAMP-1 for LD-R-PV fraction as compared to LD-S-PV fraction, while PV excluded cellular fraction showed residual LAMP-1 expression confirming the purity of the isolated PV fractions (Figure 3C ii, iii). Following isolation, protein concentration was measured for isolated PV fractions using the Bradford assay, and PV fractions from both LD-S- and LD-R-infected KCs were normalized accordingly.”

      (2) In section 6 they evaluate the mechanism of LDL uptake in macrophages. Several approaches and endocytic pathway inhibitors are employed. The authors must be aware that the role of cytochalasin D in the disruption of fluid phase endocytosis is controversial. Although they reference a study that suggests that cytochalasin D has no effect on fluid-phase endocytosis, other studies have found the opposite (doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058054). It wasn't readily evident what concentrations were used in their study. They should consider testing more than 1 concentration of the drug before they make their conclusions on their findings on fluid phase endocytosis.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment and we apologise for missing out mentioning Cytochalasin-D concentration. To clarify, LDL uptake by LD-R infected KCs is LDL-receptor independent as clearly shown in Section 6, Figure 4A, Figure S4A, Figure S4B i and Figure S4B ii in the  submitted manuscript. In (Figure 4F and Figure S4D) of the  submitted manuscript, as referred by the Reviewer, Cytochalasin-D was used at a concentration of 2.5µg/ml. At this concentration, we did not observe any effect of Cytochalasin-D on LDL-receptor independent fluid phase endocytosis as intracellular LD-R amastigotes was able to uptake LDL successfully and proliferate in infected Kupffer cells, unlike Latranculin-A (5µM) treatment which completely inhibited intracellular proliferation of LD-R amastigotes by blocking only receptor independent Fluid phase endocytosis (Video 2A and 2B and Figure 4E in the  submitted manuscript). In fact, the study referred by the reviewer (doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0058054), used a concentration of 4µg/ml Cytochalasin-D which did affect both LDL-receptor dependent and also receptor independent endocytosis in bone marrow derived macrophages. We would also like to clarify that in this work during our preliminary experiments we have also tested higher concentration Cytochalasin-D (5µg/ml). However, even at this higher concentration there were no significant effect of Cytochalasin-D on LD-R induced LDL-receptor independent fluid phase endocytosis as observed from intracellular LD-R amastigote count. Thus, we strongly believe that Cytochalasin-D does not have any impact on LD-R induced fluid phase endocytosis even at higher concentration. We have now included this data as Figure 4F and Figure S4E in the revised manuscript. Further, to clear out any confusion that readers might have, and also concentration of all the inhibitors used in the study will be mentioned in the Result section (Line 278 and 284), as well as in the revised Figure labels.

      (3) In Figure 5 they present a blot that shows increased Lamp1 expression from as early as 4 hrs after infection with LD-R and by 12 hrs after infection of both LD-S and LD-R. Increased Lamp1 expression after Leishmania infection has not been reported by others. By what mechanism do they suggest is causing such a rapid increase (at 4hrs post-infection) in Lamp-1 protein? As they report, their RNA seq data did not show an increase in LAMP1 transcription (lines 432-434).

      We would like to express our gratitude to the reviewer for highlighting the novelty of this observation. Indeed, to the best of our knowledge, no similar findings (we could not find reference of any quantitative Western blot for LAMP-1) have been reported previously in primary macrophages infected with Leishmania donovani (LD). Firstly, we would like to point out, as stated in the Methods section (Lines 556–566) of the  submitted manuscript: "Flow-sorted metacyclic LD promastigotes were used at a MOI of 1:10 (with variations of 1:5 and 1:20 in some cases) for 4 hours, which was considered the 0th point of infection. Macrophages were subsequently washed to remove any extracellular loosely attached parasites and incubated further as per experimental requirements.” This indicates that our actual study points correspond to approximately the 8th and 28th hours post-infection”. We just wanted to clarify the time point just to prevent any potential confusion.

      Now regarding LAMP1 expression, although we could not find any previous reports of its expression in LD infected primary macrophages, we would like to mention that there is a previous report (doi.org/10.1128/mBio.01464-20), which shows a similar punctuated LAMP-1 upregulation (as observed by us in Figure 5A i of the  submitted manuscript) in response to leishmania infection in nonphagocytic fibroblast. It is tempting to speculate that increased LAMP-1 expression observed in response to LD-R infected macrophages might be due to increased lysosomal biogenesis, required for degrading increased endocytosed-LDL into bioavailable cholesterol. However, since no change in LAMP-1 expression in RNA seq data (Figure 6, of the  submitted manuscript), we can only speculate that this is happening due to some post transcriptional or post translational modifications. But further work will definitely require to investigate this mechanism in details which is beyond the scope of this work. That is why, in the  submitted manuscript, (Line 432-435), we have discussed this, “Although available RNAseq analysis (Figure 6) did not support this increased expression of lamp-1 in the transcript level, it did reflect a notable upregulation of vesicular fusion protein (VSP) vamp8 and stx1a in response to LD-R-infection. LD infection can regulate LAMP-1 expression, and the role of VSPs in LDLvesicle fusion with LD-R-PV is worthy of further investigation.”

      However, we agree with the reviewer that this might not be enough for the clarification. Hence in the revised manuscript this has been updated in the Discussion section (Line 465-472) as follows, “Although available RNAseq analysis (Figure 6) did not support this increased expression of lamp-1 in the transcript level, it did reflect a notable upregulation of vesicular fusion protein (VSP) vamp8 and stx1a in response to LD-R-infection. How, LD infection can regulate LAMP-1 expression, and the role of VSPs in LDL-vesicle fusion with LD-R-PV is worthy of further investigation. It is possible and has been earlier reported that LD infection can regulate host proteins expression through post transcriptional and post translational modifications [61-63]. It is tempting to speculate that LD-R amastigote might be promoting an increased lysosomal biogenesis through any such mechanism to increase supply of bioavailable cholesterol through action of lysosomal acid hydrolases on LDL.”

      (4) In Figure 6, amongst several assays, they reported on studies where SPC-1 is knocked down in PECs. They failed to provide any evidence of the success of the knockdown, but nonetheless showed greater LD-R after NPC-1 was knocked down. They should provide more details of such experiments.

      Although we do understand the concern raised by the reviewer, this statement in question is factually incorrect. We would like to point out that in Figure 6F i, of the  submitted manuscript (Figure 6G ii in the revised manuscript), we have demonstrated decreased NPC-1 staining following transfection with NPC-1-specific siRNA, whereas no such reduction was observed with scrambled RNA. Similar immunofluorescence data confirming LDL-receptor knockdown has also been provided in Figure S4B i of the  submitted manuscript (Figure S4B ii in the revised manuscript). However, we acknowledge that the reviewer may be referring to the lack of quantitative validation of the knockdown via Western blot. We would like to clarify although, we already had this data, but we did not include it to avoid duplication to reduce the data density of the MS. But as suggested by the reviewer, we have included western blot for both NPC-1 and LDL-receptor knock down in the revised manuscript as Figure 6G i and Figure S4B i which again confirms an efficient Knock down of NPC-1 and LDLr as we have observed with IFA.

      Additionally, as suggested by the reviewer, we also noticed lack of details in Methods section of the  submitted manuscript, concerning siRNA mediated Knock down (KD). Therefore, we have included more details in the revised manuscript (Line 821-828), which read as, “For all siRNA transfections, Lipofectamine® RNAiMAX Reagent (Life Technologies, 13778100) specifically designed for knockdown assays in primary cells was used according to the manufacturer's instructions with slight modifications. PECs were seeded into 24-well plates at a density of 1x10<sup>5</sup> per well, and incubated at 37°C with 5% CO2. The transfection complex, comprising (1µl Lipofectamine® RNAiMAX and 50µl Opti MEM) and (1 µl siRNA and 50µl Opti MEM) mixed together directly added to the incubated PECs. Gene silencing was checked by IFA and by Western blot as mentioned previously.”

      Minor issues

      (1) There is an implication that parasite replication occurs well before 24hrs post-infection?

      Studies on Leishmania parasite replication have reported on the commencement of replication after 24hrs post-infection of macrophages (PMCID: PMC9642900). Is this dramatic increase in parasite numbers that they observed due to early parasite replication?

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment and appreciate the opportunity to clarify our findings. Indeed, as rightly assumed by the Reviewer, as our data suggest, and we also believe that this increase intracellular amastigotes number is a consequence of early replication of Leishmania donovani. As already mentioned in response to Point number 3 raised by Reviewer 1, we would again like to highlight that in the Methods section (Lines 562–566), it is clearly stated: "Flow-sorted metacyclic LD promastigotes were used at a MOI of 1:10 (with variations of 1:5 and 1:20 in some cases) for 4 hours, which was considered the 0th point of infection. Macrophages were subsequently washed to remove any extracellular loosely attached parasites and incubated further as per experimental requirements.” This effectively means that our actual study points correspond to approximately the 8th and 28th hours post-infection and we just want to mention it to avoid any confusion regarding experimental time points.

      Now, regarding specific concern related to Leishmania parasite replication, we would like to point out that the study referred by the reviewer on the commencement of replication after 24hrs, was conducted on Leishmania major, which may differ significantly from Leishmania donovani owing to its species and strain-specific characteristics (PMCID: PMC9642900). In fact, doubling time of Leishmania donovani (LD) has been previously reported to be approximately 11.4 hours (doi: 10.1111/j.1550-7408. 1990.tb01147.x). Moreover, multiple studies have indicated an exponential increase in intracellular LD amastigote number (more than two-fold increase) by 24Hrs post infection. (doi:10.1128/AAC.0119607, doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpara.2011.07.013). We also have a similar observation for both infected PEC and KC as depicted in Figure 1C and Figure S1C in the  submitted and revised manuscript) indicating that active replication is happening in this time frame for Leishmania donovani. Hence it was an informed decision from our side to focus on 24Hrs time point to perform the analysis on intracellular LD proliferation.

      (2) Several of the fluorescence images in the paper are difficult to see. It would be helpful if a blown-up (higher magnification image of images in Figure 1 (especially D) for example) is presented.

      We apologise for the inconvenience. Although we have provided Zoomed images for several other Figures in the  submitted manuscript and revised manuscript, like Figure 4, Figure 5, Figure 6 and Figure 8. However, this was not always doable for all the figures (like for Figure 1D), due to lack of space and Figure arrangements requirements. However, to accommodate Reviewer’s request we have provide a blown-up image for Figure 1D iii in the revised manuscript.

      (3) The times at which they choose to evaluate their infections seem arbitrary. It is not clear why they stopped analysis of their KC infections at 24 hrs. As mentioned above, several studies have shown that this is when intracellular amastigotes start replicating. They should consider extending their analyses to 48 or 72 hrs post-infection. Also, they stop in vitro infection of Apoe/- mice at 11 days. Why? No explanation is given for why only 1 point after infection.

      Reviewer has raised two independent concerns and we would like to address them individually.

      Firstly, “The times at which they choose to evaluate their infections seem arbitrary. It is not clear why they stopped analysis of their KC infections at 24 hrs. As mentioned above, several studies have shown that this is when intracellular amastigotes start replicating. They should consider extending their analyses to 48 or 72 hrs post-infection.”

      We have already provided a detail justification for time point selection in our response to Reviewer 1, Minor Comment 1. As mentioned already we observed a significant and sharp rise in the number of intracellular amastigotes between 4Hrs and 24Hrs post-infection in KC, with replication rate appeared to be not increasing proportionally (not doubling) after that (Figure 1C in the revised manuscript). This early stage of rapid replication of LD amastigotes, therefore likely coincides with a critical period of lipid acquisition by intracellular amastigotes (Video 3A and 3B and Figure 4E in the  submitted manuscript and revised manuscript) and thus 24Hrs infected KC was specifically selected. In this regard, we would further like to add that at 72Hrs post-infection, we noticed a notable number of infected Kupffer cells began detaching from the wells with extracellular amastigotes probably egressing out. This phenomenon potentially reflects the severe impact of prolonged infection on Kupffer cell viability and adhesion properties as shown in Video 2 in the revised manuscript and Author response image 1. This observation further influenced our decision to conclude all infection studies in Kupffer cells by the 48Hrs post-infection, which necessitate to complete the infection time point at 24 Hrs, for allowing treatment of Amp-B for another 24 Hrs (Figure 8, and Figure S5, in the  submitted manuscript and revised manuscript). We acknowledge that we should have been possibly clearer on our selection of infection time points and as the Reviewer have suggested we have included this information in the revised manuscript (Line 134-141) for clear understanding of the reader. This read as, “Interestingly, as compared to a significant and sharp rise in the number of intracellular amastigotes between 4Hrs and 24Hrs post infected KC in response to LD-R infection, the number of intracellular amastigotes although increased significantly did not doubled from 24Hrs to 48Hrs p.i. suggesting exponential LD amastigote replication between 4Hrs and 24Hrs time frame and slowing down after that (Figure 1Ci, ii). Moreover, it was also noticed that at 72Hrs p.i. a notable number of infected-KC began detaching from the wells with extracellular amastigotes probably egressing out from the infected-KCs (Video 2). Thus, 24Hrs time point was selected to conduct all further infection studies involving KCs.”

      Author response image 1.

      Representative images of Kupffer cells infected with Leishmania donovani at 72Hrs post-infection showing a significant morphological change. Infected cells exhibit a rounded morphology and progressive detachment. Scale bar 10µm.

      Secondly “Also, they stop in vitro infection of Apoe-/- mice at 11 days. Why? No explanation is given for why only 1 point after infection.”

      We apologize for not providing an explanation regarding the selection of the 11-day time point for  Apoe<sup>-/-</sup> experiments (Figure 2 of the  submitted and revised manuscript). Our rationale for this choice is based on both previous literature and the specific objectives of our study. Previous report suggests that Leishmania donovani infection in hypercholesteraemic Apoe<sup>-/-</sup> mice triggers a heightened inflammatory response at approximately six weeks’ post-infection compared to C57BL/6 mice, leading to more efficient parasite clearance. This is owing to unique membrane composition of Apoe<sup>-/-</sup> which rectifies leishmania mediated defective antigen presentation at a later stage of infection (DOI 10.1194/jlr.M026914). Additionally, previous studies have also indicated that Leishmania donovani infection is well-established in vivo within 6 to 11 days post-infection in murine models (doi: 10.1128/AAC.47.5.1529-1535.2003). Given that in this experiment we particularly aimed to assess the early infection status (parasite load) in diet-induced hypercholesterolemic mice, we would like to argue that the selection of the 11-day time point was rational and well-aligned with our study objectives as this time point within this window are optimal for capturing initial parasite burden depending on initial lipid utilization, before host-driven immune clearance mechanisms could significantly alter infection dynamics. We have included this explanation in the revised manuscript (Line 170-179) as suggested by the Reviewer and this read as, “Previous report has suggested that LD infection in hypercholesteremic Apoe<sup>-/-</sup> mice triggers a heightened inflammatory response at approximately six weeks’ post-infection compared to wild type BL/6 mice, leading to more efficient parasite clearance. This is owing to unique membrane composition of Apoe-/- which rectifies leishmania mediated defective antigen presentation at a later stage of LD infection [20]. Additionally, previous studies have also indicated that LD infection is well-established in mice within 6 to 11 days post-infection in murine models [33]. Thus to evaluate impact of initial lipid utilization on LD amastigote replication in vivo, BL/6 and diet-induced hypercholesterolemic Apoe<sup>-/-</sup> mice were infected with GFP expressing LD-S or LD-R promastigotes and sacrificed 11 days p.i.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study by Pradhan et al. offers critical insights into the mechanisms by which antimonyresistant Leishmania donovani (LD-R) parasites alter host cell lipid metabolism to facilitate their own growth and, in the process, acquire resistance to amphotericin B therapy. The authors illustrate that LD-R parasites enhance LDL uptake via fluid-phase endocytosis, resulting in the accumulation of neutral lipids in the form of lipid droplets that surround the intracellular amastigotes within the parasitophorous vacuoles (PV) that support their development and contribute to amphotericin B treatment resistance. The evidence provided by the authors supporting the main conclusions is compelling, presenting rigorous controls and multiple complementary approaches. The work represents an important advance in understanding how intracellular parasites can modify host metabolism to support their survival and escape drug treatment.

      We would like to sincerely thank the reviewer for appreciating our work and find the evidence compelling to address the issue of emergence of drug resistance in infection with intracellular protozoan pathogens.

      Strengths:

      (1) The study utilizes clinical isolates of antimony-resistant L. donovani and provides interesting mechanistic information regarding the increased LD-R isolate virulence and emerging amphotericin B resistance.

      (2) The authors have used a comprehensive experimental approach to provide a link between antimony-resistant isolates, lipid metabolism, parasite virulence, and amphotericin B resistance. They have combined the following approaches:

      a) In vivo infection models involving BL/6 and Apoe-/- mice.

      b) Ex-vivo infection models using primary Kupffer cells (KC) and peritoneal exudate macrophages (PEC) as physiologically relevant host cells.

      c) Various complementary techniques to ascertain lipid metabolism including GC-MS, Raman spectroscopy, microscopy.

      d) Applications of genetic and pharmacological tools to show the uptake and utilization of host lipids by the infected macrophage resident L. donovani amastigotes.

      (3) The outcome of this study has clear clinical significance. Additionally, the authors have supported their work by including patient data showing a clear clinical significance and correlation between serum lipid profiles and treatment outcomes.

      (4) The present study effectively connects the basic cellular biology of host-pathogen interactions with clinical observations of drug resistance.

      (5) Major findings in the study are well-supported by the data:

      a) Intracellular LD-R parasites induce fluid-phase endocytosis of LDL independent of LDL receptor (LDLr).

      b) Enhanced fusion of LDL-containing vesicles with parasitophorous vacuoles (PV) containing LD-R parasites both within infected KCs and PECs cells.

      c) Intracellular cholesterol transporter NPC1-mediated cholesterol efflux from parasitophorous vacuoles is suppressed by the LD-R parasites within infected cells.

      d) Selective exclusion of inflammatory ox-LDL through MSR1 downregulation.

      e) Accumulation of neutral lipid droplets contributing to amphotericin B resistance.

      Weaknesses:

      The weaknesses are minor:

      (1) The authors do not show how they ascertain that they have a purified fraction of the PV postdensity gradient centrifugation.

      (2) The study could have benefited from a more detailed analysis of how lipid droplets physically interfere with amphotericin B access to parasites.

      We have addressed both these concerns in the revised Version of this work as elaborated in the following section.

      Impact and significance:

      This work makes several fundamental advances:

      (1) The authors were able to show the link between antimony resistance and enhanced parasite proliferation.

      (2) They were also able to reveal how parasites can modify host cell metabolism to support their growth while avoiding inflammation.

      (3) They were able to show a certain mechanistic basis for emerging amphotericin B resistance.

      (4) They suggest therapeutic strategies combining lipid droplet inhibitors with current drugs.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Experimental suggestions:

      a) The authors could have provided a more detailed analysis of lipid droplet composition. This is a critically missing piece in this nice study.

      We completely agree with the Reviewer on this, a more detailed analysis of lipid droplets composition, dynamics of its formation and mechanism of lipid transfer to amastigotes residing within the PV would be worthy of further investigation. To answer the Reviewer, we are already conducting investigation in this direction and have very promising initial results which we are willing to share with the Reviewer as unpublished communication if requested. Since, we plan to address these questions independently, we hope Reviewer will understand our hesitation to include these data into the present work which is already data dense. We sincerely believe existence of lipid droplet contact sites with the PV along with the specific lipid type transfer to amastigotes and its mechanism requires special attention and could stand out as an independent work by itself.

      b) The macrophages (PEC, KC) could have been treated with latex beads as a control, which would indicate that cholesterol and lipids are indeed utilized by the Leishmania parasitophorous vacuole (PV) and essential for its survival and proliferation.

      We thank the reviewer for this nice suggestion, which we believe will further strengthen the conclusion of this work. We have now included this data as Figure 5E in the revised manuscript. Our data showed that infected KC harbouring both LD-R amastigotes and Fluorescent Latex Beads, showed a concentrated staining of Cholesterol around amastigotes, with no positive Cholesterol staining around internalized latex beads similar to LD-S amastigotes. This observation clearly confirmed specific lipid uptake in LD-R-PV, which can not be replicated by phagocytosed Latex Beads.

      c) HMGCoA reductase is an important enzyme for the mevalonate pathway and cholesterol synthesis. The authors have not commented on this enzyme in either host or parasite. Additionally, western blots of these enzymes along with SREBP2 could have been performed.

      We appreciate the concern and do see the point why reviewer is suggesting this. We would like to mention that regarding HMGCoA we already do have real time qPCR data which perfectly aligns with our RNAseq data (Figure 6 A i, in the  submitted and revised manuscript), showing significant downregulation specifically in LD-R infected KC as compared to uninfected control. We are including this data as Author response image 2. However, we did not proceed with checking the level of HMGCoA at the protein level as we noticed several previous reports have suggested that HMGCoA reductase remains under transcriptional control of SERBP2 (doi.org/10.1016/j.cmet.2011.03.005, doi: 10.1194/jlr.C066712, doi:10.1194/jlr.RA119000201), which acts the master regulator of mevalonate pathway and cholesterol synthesis (doi.org/10.1161/ATVBAHA.122.317320) and SERBP2 remains significantly downregulated in response to LD-R infection (Figure 6B i and Figure 6C in the  submitted and revised manuscript). However, as suggested by the Reviewer, we have updated this data in the revised manuscript as Figure 6D. Western blot data further confirmed a significant expected downregulation of HMGCoA in response to LD-R infection.

      Author response image 2.

      qPCR Analysis of HMGCR Expression Following Leishmania donovani Infection: Quantitative PCR analysis showing the relative expression of hmgcr (3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl-CoA reductase) in Kupffer cells after 24 hours of Leishmania donovani (LD) infection compared to uninfected control cells. Gene expression levels are normalized to β-actin as an internal control, and fold change is represented relative to the uninfected condition.

      d) The authors should discuss the expression pattern of any enzyme of the mevalonate pathway that they have found to be dysregulated in the transcript data.

      As per the reviewer’s suggestion, we have looked into the RNA seq data and observed that apart from hmgcr, hmgcs (3-hydroxy-3methylglutaryl-CoA synthase), another key enzyme in the mevalonate pathway, is significantly downregulated in host PECs in response to LD-R infection compared to the LD-S infection. We have Discussed this in the revised manuscript (Line 484-490), which read as “Further RNA sequencing data also revealed a significant downregulation of hmgcs (3-hydroxy-3-methylglutarylCoA synthase) in LD-R infected PECs as compared to LD-S infection. Downregulation of HMGCS which catalyzes the condensation of acetyl-CoA with acetoacetyl-CoA to form 3-hydroxy-3-methylglutaryl-CoA (HMG-CoA), which serves as an intermediate in both cholesterol biosynthesis and ketogenesis further supports our observation that LD-R-infected PECs preferentially rely on endocytosed low-density lipoprotein (LDL)-derived cholesterol rather than de novo synthesized cholesterol to support their metabolic needs.”

      e) The authors have followed a previously published protocol by Real F (reference 73) to enrich for parasitophorous vacuole (PV). However, they do not show how they ascertain that they have a purified fraction of the PV post-density gradient centrifugation. The authors should at least show Western blot data for LAMP1 for different fractions of density gradient from which they enriched the PV.

      As we previously stated in our response to Reviewer 1, in the revised manuscript we have included a detailed analysis of purity for different fractions during PV isolation. We sincerely appreciate the reviewer for highlighting this important concern and for suggesting an approach to conduct the experiment. We have included this data as Figure 3C i, ii, iii) in the revised manuscript. Our Imaging and Western blot data showed a significant enrichment of LAMP-1 in PV fraction, and we believe this result further reinforce the conclusions of our study on increased Cholesterol.

      (2) Presentation improvements:

      a) Add a clear timeline for infection experiments.

      As suggested by the Reviewer, we have included a schematic of Timelines for all the animal infection experiment (Figure 2Ci and Figure 7A,Fi) in the revised manuscript.

      b) Provide more details on patient sample collection and analysis.

      We have included more details on the sample collection in the Method section of the revised manuscript (Line 830-835), “Blood samples were collected from a total of 22 individuals spanning a diverse age range (8 to 70 years) by RMRI, Bihar, India. Among these, nine samples were obtained from healthy individuals residing in endemic regions to serve as controls. Serum was isolated from each blood sample through centrifugation, and the lipid profile was subsequently analysed using a specialized diagnostic kit (Coral Clinical System) following the manufacturer's protocol.”

      c) Consider reorganizing figures to better separate mechanistic and clinical findings.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We felt that a major arrangement altering the sequence of the Figures as presented in the Original Submission will impact smooth flow of the story and hence, we did not disturb that. However, as suggested by the Reviewer we have performed major rearrangement within Figure 2, Figure 5 and Figure 6 and Figure 9 of the revised manuscript for a better representation of the data and convenience of the reader. Also, if the reviewer has specific suggestion regarding rearrangement of any particular figure, we will be happy to consider that.

      (3) Technical clarifications needed:

      a) Specify exact concentrations used for inhibitors.

      We apologise for this unwanted and unnecessary mistake. Please note we have now clearly mentioned the concentration of all the inhibitors used in this study in Result section and in the Figures of the revised manuscript. For easy understanding The revised section (Line 281-287) read as, “Finally, we infected the KCs with GFP expressing LD-R for 4Hrs, washed and allowed the infection to proceed in presence of fluorescent red-LDL and Latrunculin-A (5µM), a compound which specifically inhibits fluid phase endocytosis by inducing actin depolymerization [41]. Real-time fluorescence tracking demonstrated that Latrunculin-A treatment not only prevented the uptake of fluorescent red-LDL but also severely impacted intracellular proliferation of LD-R amastigotes (Video 2A and 2B and Figure 4E). In contrast, treatment with Cytochalasin-D, which alters cellular F-actin organization but does not affect fluid phase endocytosis [41], had no effect on the intracellular proliferation of LD-R irrespective of Cytochalasin-D concentrations (2.5µg/ml and 5µg/ml respectively) (Figure 4F and Figure S4D).”

      b) Include more details on image analysis methods.

      Please note that in specific sections like in Line numbers 574-579, 653-658, 10471049 of the  submitted manuscript, we have put special attention in describing the Image analysis process. However, we agree that in some particular cases more details will be appreciated by the reader. Hence, we have included an additional section of Image Analysis in the Methods section of the revised manuscript. This section (Line 727-739) read as, “Image processing and analysis were conducted using Fiji (ImageJ). For optimal visualization, Giemsa-stained macrophages (MΦs) were represented in grayscale to enhance contrast and structural clarity. To improve the distinction of different fluorescent signals, pseudo-colors were assigned to fluorescence images, ensuring better differentiation between various cellular components. For colocalization analysis (Figures 3, Figure 5, Figure 6, and Figure S2), we utilized the RGB profile plot plugin in ImageJ, which allows for the precise assessment of signal overlap by generating fluorescence intensity profiles across selected regions of interest. This approach provided quantitative insights into the spatial relationship between labelled molecules within infected cells. Additionally, for analyzing the distribution of cofilin in Figure 4, the ImageJ surface plot plugin was employed. This tool enabled three-dimensional visualization of fluorescence intensity variations, facilitating a more detailed examination of cofilin localization and its potential reorganization in response to infection.”

      c) Clarify statistical analysis procedures.

      We have already provided a dedicated section of Statistical Analysis in the Methods section of the Original Submission and also have also shown the groups being compared to determine the statistical analysis in the Figure and in the Figure Legends of the  submitted manuscript. Furthermore, as suggested by the Reviewer we have now also add additional clarification regarding the statistical analysis performed in the revised manuscript (Line 737-749). In the revised manuscript this section read as, “All statistical analyses were performed using GraphPad Prism 8 on raw datasets to ensure robust and reproducible results. For datasets involving comparisons across multiple conditions, one-way or two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted, followed by Tukey’s post hoc test to assess pairwise differences while controlling for multiple comparisons. A 95% confidence interval (CI) was applied to determine the statistical reliability of the observed differences. For non-parametric comparisons across multiple groups, Wilcoxon rank-sum tests were employed, maintaining a 95% confidence interval, which is particularly useful for analysing skewed data distributions. In cases where only two groups were compared, Student’s t-test was used to determine statistical significance, ensuring an accurate assessment of mean differences. All quantitative data are represented as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM) to illustrate variability within experimental replicates. Statistical significance was determined at P ≤ 0.05. Notation for significance levels: *P ≤ 0.05; **P ≤ 0.001; ***P ≤ 0.0001.”

      (4) Minor corrections:

      a) Methods section could benefit from more details on Raman spectroscopy analysis.

      We agree with this suggestion of the Reviewer. For providing more clarity have incorporate additional details in the Methodology for the Raman section of the revised manuscript (Line 638-649). The updated section will read as follows in the revised manuscript. “For confocal Raman spectroscopy, spectral data were acquired from individual cells at 1000× magnification using a 100 × 100 μm scanning area, following previously established specifications. After spectral acquisition, distinct Raman shifts corresponding to specific biomolecular signatures were extracted for further analysis. These included: Cholesterol (535–545 cm¹), Nuclear components (780–790 cm¹), Lipid structures (1262–1272 cm<sup>1</sup>), Fatty acids (1436–1446 cm<sup>1</sup>) Following spectral extraction, pseudo-color mapping was applied to highlight the spatial distribution of each biomolecular component within the cell. These processed spectral images are presented in Figure 3D1, where the first four panels illustrate the individual biomolecular distributions. A merged composite image was then generated to visualize the co-localization of these biomolecules within the cellular microenvironment, with the final panel specifically representing the spatial distribution of key biomolecules.”

      b) In the methods section line 609, page 14, the authors cite Real F protocol as reference 73 for PV enrichment. However, in the very next section on GC-MS analysis (lines 615-616, page 15), they state they have used reference 74 for PV enrichment. Can they explain why a discrepancy in PV isolation references this? Reference 74 does not mention anything related to PV isolation.

      Response: We would like to sincerely apologise for this confusion which probably raised from our writing of this section. We would like to confirm that our PV isolation protocol is based on the published work of Real F protocol (reference 73). However, in the next section of the submitted manuscript, GC-MS analysis was described and that was performed based on protocol referenced in 74. In the revised manuscript, we have avoided this confusion and made correction by putting the references in the proper places. In the revised manuscript, this section (Line 663-678) read as,

      “GC-MS analysis of LD-S and LD-R-PV

      Following a 24Hrs infection period, KCs were harvested, washed with phosphate-buffered saline (PBS), and pelleted. Subsequent to this, PV isolation was carried out using the previously described protocol [35]. After PV isolation Bradford assay was carried out for normalizing the protein concentration. The resulting equal volume of PV pellet was suspended in 20 ml of dichloromethane: methanol (2:1, vol/vol) and incubated at 4°C for 24hours. After centrifugation (11,000 g, 1 hour, 4°C), the supernatant was checked through thin layer chromatography (TLC) and subsequently evaporated under vacuum. The residue and pellet were saponified with 30% potassium hydroxide (KOH) in methanol at 80°C for 2 hours. Sterols were extracted with n-hexane, evaporated, and dissolved in dichloromethane. A portion of the clear yellow sterol solution was treated with N, O-bis(trimethylsilyl)trifluoroacetamide (BSTFA) and heated at 80°C for 1 hour to form trimethylsilyl (TMS) ethers. Gas chromatography/mass spectrometry (GC/MS) analysis was performed using a Varian model 3400 chromatograph equipped with DB5 columns (methyl-phenylsiloxane ratio, 95/5; dimensions, 30 m by 0.25 mm). Helium was used as the gas carrier (1 ml/min). The column temperature was maintained at 270°C, with the injector and detector set at 300°C. A linear gradient from 150 to 180°C at 10°C/min was used for methyl esters, with MS conditions set at 280°C, 70 eV, and 2.2 kV[77].

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Joint Public Review:

      Idiopathic scoliosis (IS) is a common spinal deformity. Various studies have linked genes to IS, but underlying mechanisms are unclear such that we still lack understanding of the causes of IS. The current manuscript analyzes IS patient populations and identifies EPHA4 as a novel associated gene, finding three rare variants in EPHA4 from three patients (one disrupting splicing and two missense variants) as well as a large deletion (encompassing EPHA4) in a Waardenburg syndrome patient with scoliosis. EPHA4 is a member of the Eph receptor family. Drawing on data from zebrafish experiments, the authors argue that EPHA4 loss of function disrupts the central pattern generator (CPG) function necessary for motor coordination.

      The main strength of this manuscript is the human genetic data, which provides convincing evidence linking EPHA4 variants to IS. The loss of function experiments in zebrafish strongly support the conclusion that EPHA4 variants that reduce function lead to IS.

      The conclusion that disruption of CPG function causes spinal curves in the zebrafish model is not well supported. The authors' final model is that a disrupted CPG leads to asymmetric mechanical loading on the spine and, over time, the development of curves. This is a reasonable idea, but currently not strongly backed up by data in the manuscript. Potentially, the impaired larval movements simply coincide with, but do not cause, juvenile-onset scoliosis. Support for the authors' conclusion would require independent methods of disrupting CPG function and determining if this is accompanied by spine curvature. At a minimum, the language of the manuscript could be toned down, with the CPG defects put forward as a potential explanation for scoliosis in the discussion rather than as something this manuscript has "shown". An additional weakness of the manuscript is that the zebrafish genetic tools are not sufficiently validated to provide full confidence in the data and conclusions.

      We highly appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments and the acknowledgment of the main values of our study. We agree with the reviewer that further experiments are needed to fully establish the relationship between CPG and scoliosis. In response, we have revised the conclusion in the manuscript to better reflect this. Additionally, we conducted further analyses on the mutants to provide additional evidence supporting this concept.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Epha4a mutant zebrafish exhibited mild spinal curves, mostly laterally and in the tail. This was 75% of homozyous mutants but also, surprisingly, about 20% of heterozygotes. epha4b mutants also developed some mild scoliosis. If the two zebrafish paralogs can compensate for each other (partial redundancy), we might expect more severe scoliosis in double mutants. Did the authors generate and analyze double mutants? I believe it would be very useful for this study to report the zebrafish phenotype of loss of both paralogs together.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment regarding the potential value of reporting the phenotype of eph4a/eph4b double mutants. While we fully agree that this analysis would be valuable, our attempts to generate double mutants have been unsuccessful. These two genes are closely linked on the chromosome, with less than 100 kb separating them, which makes it challenging to generate double mutants through standard genetic crossing. Establishing a double mutant line would require more than a year due to the technical constraints of the process. Although we are unable to address this question directly at this time, we hypothesize that eph4a/eph4b double mutants may exhibit a higher likelihood of body axis abnormalities based on the phenotypes observed in single mutants and the known functions of these genes.

      We hope this perspective will provide some useful context despite the limitations.

      In Figure 1F, a pCDK5 western blot is performed as a readout of EPH4A signaling after either WT or C849Y mutant EPH4A is transfected into HEK 293T cells. It would be useful to mention in the text, or at least the figure legend, how this experiment was performed/where the protein samples came from. It is included in the methods, but in the main text, it simply says "we conducted western blotting" without mentioning whether the protein samples were from cell lines, patients, or another source.

      Sorry for our ignorance. A detailed description of the western blotting conduction was supplemented at both “results” part (page 8, line 187-190) and the Figure 1 legend.

      Was the relative turn angle biased to the left or right side of the fish? (i.e. is a positive angle a rightward or leftward turn?)

      We are sorry for our unclear description. In Figure 3D, positive angle means turning left, while negative angle means turning right. In wild-type larvae, the average turning angle over a 4-minute period is approximately 0, whereas in mutants, this value deviates from 0, indicating a directional preference (positive for leftward and negative for rightward turns) in swimming behavior during the recording period. We have also made the necessary supplementation in the text and figure legend.

      In Figure 4, morpholinos rather than mutants are used, but it is not clear why. Has it been established that the MO used disrupts gene function specifically? Can the effect of the MO be rescued by expressing a wild-type mRNA of Epha4a? Does MO knockdown induce spinal curves if fish are raised? Indeed, this could be a way to determine whether the spinal curves are caused by early events in development (when MOs are active).

      Thanks for the comments. The efficacy of relevant MOs has been well-documented in numerous previous studies (Addison et al., 2018; Cavodeassi et al., 2013; Letelier et al., 2018; Royet et al., 2017). Following this reviewer’s suggestion, we have raised the epha4a morphants into adults, while no scoliosis were observed, suggesting that the spinal curvature formation may be induced by long-term defects in the absence of Epha4a. Additionally, we reconfirmed the abnormal motor neuron activation frequency phenotype in the mutants background. The corresponding data have replaced the original Figure 4 in the manuscript. 

      References

      (1) Addison, M., Xu, Q., Cayuso, J., and Wilkinson, D.G. (2018). Cell Identity Switching Regulated by Retinoic Acid Signaling Maintains Homogeneous Segments in the Hindbrain. Dev Cell 45, 606-620 e603.

      (2) Cavodeassi, F., Ivanovitch, K., and Wilson, S.W. (2013). Eph/Ephrin signalling maintains eye field segregation from adjacent neural plate territories during forebrain morphogenesis. Development 140, 4193-4202.

      (3) Letelier, J., Terriente, J., Belzunce, I., Voltes, A., Undurraga, C.A., Polvillo, R., Devos, L., Tena, J.J., Maeso, I., Retaux, S., et al. (2018). Evolutionary emergence of the rac3b/rfng/sgca regulatory cluster refined mechanisms for hindbrain boundaries formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115, E3731-E3740.

      (4) Royet, A., Broutier, L., Coissieux, M.M., Malleval, C., Gadot, N., Maillet, D., Gratadou-Hupon, L., Bernet, A., Nony, P., Treilleux, I., et al. (2017). Ephrin-B3 supports glioblastoma growth by inhibiting apoptosis induced by the dependence receptor EphA4. Oncotarget 8, 23750-23759.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Supplementary Table 3 is missing.

      Sorry for any inconvenience caused to the reviewers. Due to the size of the supplementary Table 3, we have separately uploaded an Excel file as supplementary materials. We have also double-checked during the resubmission process of the revised manuscript. Thanks for your thorough review.

      The authors report only a single mutant allele for zebrafish epha4a and epha4b. Additionally, they provide no information about how many generations each allele has been outcrossed. The authors should provide some type of validation that the phenotypes they describe result from loss of function of the targeted gene and not from an off-targeting event.

      Thanks for the comments. For epha4a and epha4b mutants, each homozygous mutant was initially derived from the self-crossing of first filial generation heterozygotes, and subsequent homozygous generations were maintained for fewer than three rounds of in-crossing. Interestingly, we observed a reduction in the incidence of scoliosis across successive generations. This trend may be attributed to potential genetic compensation mechanisms, which could mitigate the phenotypic severity over time. To address concerns about possible off-target effects, we synthesized and injected epha4a mRNA to test for phenotypic rescue. Our data show that epha4a mRNA injection partially restored swimming coordination in the mutants (Fig. S5). Moreover, similar motor coordination defects have been reported in Epha4-deficient mice, as documented in previous studies (Kullander et al., 2003; Borgius et al., 2014). These findings collectively strengthen the hypothesis that Epha4a plays a critical role in regulating motor coordination.

      References

      (1) Borgius, L., Nishimaru, H., Caldeira, V., Kunugise, Y., Low, P., Reig, R., Itohara, S., Iwasato, T., and Kiehn, O. (2014). Spinal glutamatergic neurons defined by EphA4 signaling are essential components of normal locomotor circuits. J Neurosci 34, 3841-3853.

      (2) Kullander, K., Butt, S.J., Lebret, J.M., Lundfald, L., Restrepo, C.E., Rydstrom, A., Klein, R., and Kiehn, O. (2003). Role of EphA4 and EphrinB3 in local neuronal circuits that control walking. Science 299, 1889-1892.

      The authors need to provide allele designations for the mutant alleles following accepted nomenclature guidelines.

      Thank you for your careful review! We have reviewed and made revisions to the genes and mutation symbols throughout the entire text.

      The three antisense morpholino oligonucleotides need to be validated for efficacy and specificity.

      Thanks for the comments. The morpholinos were extensively used and validated in previous studies, and the efficacy of these morpholinos has been thoroughly validated in multiple studies (Addison et al., 2018; Cavodeassi et al., 2013; Letelier et al., 2018; Royet et al., 2017). Furthermore, we also performed swimming behavior analysis in the mutant background, which showed similar results as the morphants. Moreover, we also performed rescue experiments to confirm the specificity of the mutants (Fig. S5). Finally, we reconfirmed the abnormal calcium signaling in the mutants (Fig. 4), which further support our previous knockdown results.

      References

      (1) Addison, M., Xu, Q., Cayuso, J., and Wilkinson, D.G. (2018). Cell Identity Switching Regulated by Retinoic Acid Signaling Maintains Homogeneous Segments in the Hindbrain. Dev Cell 45, 606-620 e603.

      (2) Cavodeassi, F., Ivanovitch, K., and Wilson, S.W. (2013). Eph/Ephrin signalling maintains eye field segregation from adjacent neural plate territories during forebrain morphogenesis. Development 140, 4193-4202.

      (3) Letelier, J., Terriente, J., Belzunce, I., Voltes, A., Undurraga, C.A., Polvillo, R., Devos, L., Tena, J.J., Maeso, I., Retaux, S., et al. (2018). Evolutionary emergence of the rac3b/rfng/sgca regulatory cluster refined mechanisms for hindbrain boundaries formation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 115, E3731-E3740.

      (4) Royet, A., Broutier, L., Coissieux, M.M., Malleval, C., Gadot, N., Maillet, D., Gratadou-Hupon, L., Bernet, A., Nony, P., Treilleux, I., et al. (2017). Ephrin-B3 supports glioblastoma growth by inhibiting apoptosis induced by the dependence receptor EphA4. Oncotarget 8, 23750-23759.

      Line 229. "While in consistent with previous reports, the hindbrain rhombomeric boundaries were found to be defective....". This sentence is not clear. Please describe how it is "inconsistent".

      Thanks for the comments and sorry for the unclear description, we have described this more clearly in our revised manuscript (page 9, line 229-230).

      Animals frequently are described as "heterozygous mutants" or "mutants". Please make clear that the latter are homozygous mutant animals.

      Thanks for the comments. In the manuscript, all references to mutants specifically indicate homozygous mutants. Heterozygous mutants are explicitly identified as such.

      The chromatin interaction portion of the Methods does not include any information on how these experiments were conducted or where the data were obtained. This information needs to be provided.

      Thanks for your advice. The detailed information of chromatin interaction mapping has been provided in “Methods and Materials” (page 18-19, line 450-455). Information about the interacting regions was derived from Hi-C datasets of 21 tissues and cell types provided by GSE87112. The significance of interactions for Hi-C datasets was computed by Fit-Hi-C, with an FDR ≤ 10-6 considered significant.

      The authors present single-cell RNA-seq data in Supplementary Figure 5 for which they cite Cavone et al, 2021. This seems like an odd database to use. Can the authors provide an explanation for choosing it? In any case, the citation should also be made in the Supplementary Figure 5 legend.

      Thank you for your rigorous comment, we have cited this literature in the proper place of the revised manuscript. Cavone et al. used the her4.3:GFP line to label ependymo-radial glia (ERG) progenitor cells and performed single-cell RNA-seq on FACS-isolated fluorescent cells. The isolated cells included not only ERG progenitors but also undifferentiated and differentiated neurons and oligodendrocytes. The authors attributed this to the relative stability of the GFP protein, which remained in the progeny of GFP-expressing her4.3+ ERG progenitor cells, thus effectively acting as a short-term cell lineage tracer. Indeed, clustering analysis of this data successfully identifies neural progenitors and other neural clusters. Therefore, we consider that this scRNA-seq data encompasses a comprehensive range of neural cell types and is suitable for analyzing the expression of genes of interest. Furthermore, we downloaded and analyzed the scRNA-seq data of the zebrafish nervous system reported by Scott et al. in 2021 (Fig. S7B) (Scott et al., 2021). Despite differences in the developmental stages of the larvae analyzed (Cavone et al. examined larvae at 4 dpf, whereas Scott et al. analyzed larvae at 24, 36, and 48 hpf), our findings are consistent. Specifically, epha4a and epha4b are expressed in interneurons, whereas efnb3a and efnb3b are enriched in floor plate cells.

      References

      (1) Scott, K., O'Rourke, R., Winkler, C.C., Kearns, C.A., and Appel, B. (2021). Temporal single-cell transcriptomes of zebrafish spinal cord pMN progenitors reveal distinct neuronal and glial progenitor populations. Dev Biol 479, 37-50.

      In Figure Legend 1, "expressed from the EPHA4-mutant plasmid" is not an accurate description of the experiment.

      Sorry for the previous inaccurate description. The description has been revised to accurately reflect the experiment. “Western blot analysis of EPHA4-c.2546G>A variant showing the protein expression levels of EPHA4 and CDK5 and the amount of phosphorylated CDK5 (pCDK5) in HEK293T cells transfected with EPHA4-mutant or EPHA4-WT plasmid”.

      Figure 3 panels J and K need more explanation. I don't understand what the different colors represent nor do I understand what are wild type and what are mutant data.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. We apologize for the lack of clarity in the original figure legend. To address this, we have revised the legend of Figure 3 to provide a more detailed explanation. In panels J and K, each color-coded curve represents the response of an individual larva from an independent experimental trial to the stimulus. Specifically, panel J depicts the response data for the wild-type larvae, whereas panel K presents the response data for the homozygous epha4a mutants.

      Please provide the genotypes for the images in Figure 5A.

      Thanks for the comments and we are sorry for our unclear description, we have described this more clearly in the Figure 5.

      Figure legend 6B should also note the heterozygote data with the wild type and homozygous mutant data.

      Thanks for the comments, the data are now included in Figure 6B.

      Epha4 and Efnb3 have well-established roles in axon guidance. Although this is noted in the Discussion, I think a more extensive description of prior findings would be helpful.

      Thanks for your valuable feedback. A more detailed description of the roles of Epha4 and Efnb3 in axon guidance was provided in the “Discussion” (page 16, line 388-396).

      The main conclusion of this manuscript is that EPHA4 variants cause IS by disrupting central pattern generator function. I think this is misleading. I think that the more valid conclusion is that EPHA4 loss of function causes axon pathfinding defects that impair locomotion by disrupting CPG activity, thereby leading to IS. I urge the authors to consider this more nuanced interpretation.

      Thank you for your insightful comments. We appreciate your suggestion to refine our main conclusion. We agree that the proposed revision more accurately reflects our findings and will revise the manuscript accordingly to state that “EPHA4 loss of function causes axon pathfinding defects, which impair locomotion by disrupting central pattern generator activity, potentially leading to IS.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Seidenthal et al. investigated the role of the C. elegans Flower protein, FLWR-1, in synaptic transmission, vesicle recycling, and neuronal excitability. They confirmed that FLWR-1 localizes to synaptic vesicles and the plasma membrane and facilitates synaptic vesicle recycling at neuromuscular junctions, albeit in an unexpected manner. The authors observed that hyperstimulation results in endosome accumulation in flwr-1 mutant synapses, suggesting that FLWR-1 facilitates the breakdown of endocytic endosomes, which differs from earlier studies in flies that suggested the Flower protein promotes the formation of bulk endosomes. This is a valuable finding. Using tissue-specific rescue experiments, the authors showed that expressing FLWR-1 in GABAergic neurons restored the aldicarb-resistant phenotype seen in flwr-1 mutants to wild-type levels. In contrast, FLWR-1 expression in cholinergic neurons in flwr-1 mutants did not restore aldicarb sensitivity, yet muscle expression of FLWR-1 partially but significantly recovered the aldicarb-resistant defects. The study also revealed that removing FLWR-1 leads to increased Ca<sup>2+</sup> signaling in motor neurons upon photo-stimulation. Further, the authors conclude that FLWR-1 contributes to the maintenance of the excitation/inhibition (E/I) balance by preferentially regulating the excitability of GABAergic neurons. Finally, SNG-1::pHluorin data imply that FLWR-1 removal enhances synaptic transmission, however, the electrophysiological recordings do not corroborate this finding.

      Strengths:

      This study by Seidenthal et al. offers valuable insights into the role of the Flower protein, FLWR-1, in C. elegans. Their findings suggest that FLWR-1 facilitates the breakdown of endocytic endosomes, which marks a departure from its previously suggested role in forming endosomes through bulk endocytosis. This observation could be important for understanding how Flower proteins function across species. In addition, the study proposes that FLWR-1 plays a role in maintaining the excitation/inhibition balance, which has potential impacts on neuronal activity.

      Weaknesses:

      One issue is the lack of follow-up tests regarding the relative contributions of muscle and GABAergic FLWR-1 to aldicarb sensitivity. The findings that muscle expression of FLWR-1 can significantly rescue aldicarb sensitivity are intriguing and may influence both experimental design and data interpretation. Have the authors examined aldicarb sensitivity when FLWR-1 is expressed in both muscles and GABAergic neurons, or possibly in muscles and cholinergic neurons? Given that muscles could influence neuronal activity through retrograde signaling, a thorough examination of FLWR-1's role in muscle is necessary, in my opinion.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. Indeed, the retrograde inhibition of cholinergic transmission by signals from muscle has been demonstrated by the Kaplan lab in a number of publications. We have now done the experiments that were suggested, see the new Fig. S3B: rescuing FLWR-1 in cholinergic neurons and in muscle did not perform any better in the aldicarb assay, while co-rescue in GABAergic neurons and muscle, like rescue in GABA neurons, led to a complete rescue to wild type levels. Thus, retrograde signaling from muscle to neurons does not contribute to effects on the E/I imbalance caused by the absence of FLWR1. The fact that muscle rescue can partially rescue the flwr-1 phenotype is likely due a cellautonomous effect of FLWR-1 on muscle excitability, facilitating muscle contraction.

      Would the results from electrophysiological recordings and GCaMP measurements be altered with muscle expression of FLWR-1? Most experiments presented in the manuscript compare wild-type and flwr-1 mutant animals. However, without tissue-specific knockout, knockdown, or rescue experiments, it is difficult to separate cell-autonomous roles from non-cell-autonomous effects, in particular in the context of aldicarb assay results. Also, relying solely on levamisole paralysis experiments is not sufficient to rule out changes in muscle AChRs, particularly due to the presence of levamisole-resistant receptors.

      We repeated the Ca<sup>2+</sup> imaging in cholinergic neurons, in response to optogenetic activation, with expression of FLWR-1 in muscle, see Fig. 4E. This did not significantly alter the increased excitability of the flwr-1 mutant. Thus, we conclude that, along with the findings in aldicarb assays, the function of FLWR-1 in muscle is cell-autonomous, and does not indirectly affect its roles in the motor neurons. Also, cholinergic expression of FLWR-1 by itself reduced Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels to those in wild type (Fig. 4E). In addition, we now also assessed the contribution of the N-AChR (ACR-16) to aldicarb-induced paralysis (Fig. S3C), showing that flwr-1 and acr-16 mutations independently mediate aldicarb resistance, and that these effects are additive. Thus, FLWR-1 does not affect the expression level or function of the N-AChR, as otherwise, the flwr1; acr-16 double mutation would not exacerbate the phenotype of the single mutants.

      This issue regarding the muscle role of FLWR-1 also complicates the interpretation of results from coelomocyte uptake experiments, where GFP secreted from muscles and coelomocyte fluorescence were used to estimate endocytosis levels. A decrease in coelomocyte GFP could result from either reduced endocytosis in coelomocytes or decreased secretion from muscles. Therefore, coelomocytespecific rescue experiments seem necessary to distinguish between these possibilities.

      We have performed a rescue of FLWR-1 in coelomocytes to address this, and found that this fully recovered the CC GFP signals to wild type levels. Therefore, the absence of FLWR-1 in muscles does not affect exocytosis of GFP. The data can be found in Fig. 5A, B.

      The manuscript states that GCaMP was used to estimate Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels at presynaptic sites. However, due to the rapid diffusion of both Ca<sup>2+</sup> and GCaMP, it is unclear how this assay distinguishes Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels specifically at presynaptic sites versus those in axons. What are the relative contributions of VGCCs and ER calcium stores here? This raises a question about whether the authors are measuring the local impact of FLWR-1 specifically at presynaptic sites or more general changes in cytoplasmic calcium levels.

      We compared Ca<sup>2+</sup> signals in synaptic puncta versus axon shafts, and did not find any differences. The data previously shown have been replaced by data where the ROIs were restricted to synaptic puncta. The outcome is the same as before. These data are provided in Fig. 4A, B, E, F. We thus conclude that the impact of FLWR-1 is local, in synaptic boutons.

      The experiments showing FLWR-1's presynaptic localization need clarification/improvement. For example, data shown in Fig. 3B represent GFP::FLWR-1 is expressed under its own promoter, and TagRFP::ELKS-1 is expressed exclusively in GABAergic neurons. Given that the pflwr-1 drives expression in both cholinergic and GABAergic neurons, and there are more cholinergic synapses outnumbering GABAergic ones in the nerve cord, it would be expected that many green FLWR-1 puncta do not associate with TagRFP::ELKS-1. However, several images in Figure 3B suggest an almost perfect correlation between FLWR-1 and ELKS-1 puncta. It would be helpful for the readers to understand the exact location in the nerve cord where these images were collected to avoid confusion.

      Thank you for making us aware that the provided images may be misleading. We have now extended this Figure (Fig. 3A-C) and provided more intensity profiles along the nerve cords in Fig. S4A-C. The quantitative analysis of average R<sup>2</sup> for the two fluorescent signals in each neuron type did not show any significant difference between the two, also after choosing slightly smaller ROIs for line scan analysis. We also highlighted the puncta corresponding to FLWR-1 in both neurons types, as well as to ELKS-1 in each specific neuron type, to identify FLWR-1 puncta without co-localized ELKS-1 signal. Also, we indicated the region that was imaged, i.e. the DNC posterior of the vulva, halfway to the posterior end of the nerve cord.

      The SNG-1::pHluorin data in Figure 5C is significant, as they suggest increased synaptic transmission at flwr-1 mutant synapses. However, to draw conclusions, it is necessary to verify whether the total amount of SNG-1::pHluorin present on synaptic vesicles remains the same between flwr-1 mutant and wild-type synapses. Without this comparison, a conclusion on levels of synaptic vesicle release based on changes in fluorescence might be premature, in particular given the results of electrophysiological recordings.

      We appreciate the comment. We now added data and experiments that verify that the basal SNG-1::pHluorin signal in the plasma membrane, measured at synaptic puncta and in adjacent axonal areas, is not different in flwr-1 mutants compared to wild type in the absence of stimulation. This data can be found in Fig. S5A. In addition, we cultured primary neurons from transgenic animals to compare total SNG-1::pHluorin to the vesicular fraction, by adding buffers of defined pH to the external, or buffers that penetrate the cell and fix intracellular pH. These experiments (Fig. S5B, C) showed no difference in the vesicle fraction of the pHluorin signal in wild type vs. flwr-1 mutant cells, demonstrating that flwr-1 mutants do not per se have altered SNG-1::pHluorin in their SV or plasma membranes.

      Finally, the interpretation of the E74Q mutation results needs reconsideration. Figure 8B indicates that the E74Q variant of FLWR-1 partially loses its rescuing ability, which suggests that the E74Q mutation adversely affects the function of FLWR-1. Why did the authors expect that the role of FLWR-1 should have been completely abolished by E74Q? Given that FLWR-1 appears to work in multiple tissues, might FLWR-1's function in neurons requires its calcium channel activity, whereas its role in muscles might be independent of this feature? While I understand there is ongoing debate about whether FLWR1 is a calcium channel, the experiments in this study do not definitively resolve local Ca<sup>2+</sup> dynamics at synapses. Thus, in my opinion, it may be premature to draw firm conclusions about calcium influx through FLWR-1.

      Thank you for bringing this up. We did not expect E74Q to necessarily abolish FLWR-1 function, unless it would be a Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel. Of course the reviewer is right, FLWR-1 might have functions as an ion channel as well as channel-independent functions. Yet, we are quite confident that FLWR-1 is not an ion channel. Instead, we think that E74Q alters stability of the protein (however, in the absence of biochemical data, we removed this conclusion), and that this impairs the function of FLWR-1 as a modulator, or possibly even, accessory subunit of the PMCA MCA-3. This interaction was indicated by a new experiment we added, where we found that FLWR-1 and MCA-3 must be physically very close to each other in the plasma membrane, using bimolecular fluorescence complementation (see new Fig. 9A, B). This provides a reasonable explanation for findings we obtained, i.e. increased Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels in stimulated neurons of the flwr-1 mutant. If FLWR-1 acts as a stimulatory subunit of MCA-3, then its absence may cause reduced MCA-3 function and thus an accumulation of Ca<sup>2+</sup> in the synaptic terminals. In Drosophila, hyperstimulation of neurons led to reduced Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels (Yao et al., 2017, PLoS Biol 15: e2000931), suggesting that Flower is a Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel. Based on our findings, we suggest an alternative explanation. Based on proteomics, the PMCA is a component of SVs (Takamori et al., 2006, Cell 127: 831-846). Increased insertion of PMCA into the plasma membrane during high stimulation, along with impaired endocytosis in flower mutants, would increase the steadystate levels of PMCA in the PM. This could lead to reduced steady state levels of Ca<sup>2+</sup>. This ‘g.o.f.’ in Flower may also impact on Ca<sup>2+</sup> microdomains of the P/Q type VGCC required for SV fusion, which could contribute to the rundown of EPSCs we find during synaptic hyperstimulation (Fig. 5G-J). We acknowledge, though, that Yao et al. (2009, Cell 138: 947– 960), showed increased uptake of Ca<sup>2+</sup> into liposomes reconstituted with purified Flower protein. However, it cannot be ruled out that a protein contaminant could be responsible, as the controls were empty liposomes, not liposomes reconstituted with a mutated Flower protein purified the same way.

      We also tested the E74Q mutant in its ability to rescue the reduced PI(4,5)P<sub>2</sub> levels in coelomocytes (CCs), where we observed no positive effect. While we have not measured Ca<sup>2+</sup> in CCs, we would assume that here a function of FLWR-1 affecting increased PI(4,5)P<sub>2</sub> levels is not linked to a channel function. It was, nevertheless, compromised by E74Q (Fig. 8D).

      Also, the aldicarb data presented in Figures 8B and 8D show notable inconsistencies that require clarification. While Figure 8B indicates that the 50% paralysis time for flwr-1 mutant worms occurs at 3.5-4 hours, Figure 8D shows that 50% paralysis takes approximately 2.5 hours for the same flwr-1 mutants. This discrepancy should be addressed. In addition, the manuscript mentions that the E74Q mutation impairs FLWR-1 folding, which could significantly affect its function. Can the authors show empirical data supporting this claim?

      We performed the aldicarb assays in a consistent manner, but nonetheless note that some variability from day to day can affect such outcomes. Importantly, we always measured each control (wild type, flwr-1) along with each test strain (FLWR-1 point mutants), to ensure the relevant estimate of a point-mutant’s effect. These assays have been repeated, now including the FLWR-1 wild type rescue strain as a comparison. The data are now combined in Fig. 8B. Regarding the assumed instability of the E74Q mutant, as we, indeed, do not have any experimental data supporting this, we removed this sentence.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The Flower protein is expressed in various cell types, including neurons. Previous studies in flies have proposed that Flower plays a role in neuronal endocytosis by functioning as a Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel. However, its precise physiological roles and molecular mechanisms in neurons remain largely unclear. This study employs C. elegans as a model to explore the function and mechanism of FLWR-1, the C. elegans homolog of Flower. This study offers intriguing observations that could potentially challenge or expand our current understanding of the Flower protein. Nevertheless, further clarification or additional experiments are required to substantiate the study's conclusions.

      Strengths:

      A range of approaches was employed, including the use of a flwr-1 knockout strain, assessment of cholinergic synaptic activity via analyzing aldicarb (a cholinesterase inhibitor) sensitivity, imaging Ca<sup>2+</sup> dynamics with GCaMP3, analyzing pHluorin fluorescence, examination of presynaptic ultrastructure by EM, and recording postsynaptic currents at the neuromuscular junction. The findings include notable observations on the effects of flwr-1 knockout, such as increased Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels in motor neurons, changes in endosome numbers in motor neurons, altered aldicarb sensitivity, and potential involvement of a Ca<sup>2+</sup>-ATPase and PIP2 binding in FLWR-1's function.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The observation that flwr-1 knockout increases Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels in motor neurons is notable, especially as it contrasts with prior findings in flies. The authors propose that elevated Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels in flwr-1 knockout motor neurons may stem from "deregulation of MCA-3" (a Ca<sup>2+</sup> ATPase in the plasma membrane) due to FLWR-1 loss. However, this conclusion relies on limited and somewhat inconclusive data (Figure 7). Additional experiments could clarify FLWR-1's role in MCA-3 regulation. For instance, it would be informative to investigate whether mutations in other genes that cause elevated cytosolic Ca<sup>2+</sup> produce similar effects, whether MCA-3 physically interacts with FLWR-1, and whether MCA-3 expression is reduced in the flwr-1 knockout.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up these critical points. As to other mutations that produce elevated cytosolic Ca<sup>2+</sup>: Possible mutations could be g.o.f. mutations of the ryanodine receptor UNC-68, the sarco-endoplasmatic Ca<sup>2+</sup> ATPase, or mutants affecting VGCCs, like the L-type channel EGL-19 or the P/Q-type channel UNC-2. However, any such mutant would affect muscle contractions (as we have shown for r.o.f. mutations in unc-68, egl-19 and unc-2 in Nagel et al. 2005 Curr Biol 15: 2279-84) and thus would affect aldicarb assays (see aldicarb resistance induced by RNAi of these genes in Sieburth et al., 2005, Nature 436: 510). The same should be expected for g.o.f. mutations of any such gene. In neurons, we would expect increased or decreased Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels in response to stimulation.

      Regarding the physical interaction of MCA-3 and FLWR-1, we performed bimolecular fluorescence complementation, with two fragments of mVenus fused to the two proteins. This assay shows mVenus reconstitution (i.e., fluorescence) if the two proteins are found in close vicinity to each other. Testing MCA-3 and FLWR-1 in muscle indeed showed a robust signal, evenly distributed on the plasma membrane. As a control, FLWR-1 did not interact with another plasma membrane protein, the stomatin UNC-1 interacting with gap junction proteins (Chen et al., 2007, Curr Biol 17: 1334-9). FLWR-1 also interacted with the ER chaperone Nicalin (NRA2 in C. elegans), which helps assembling the TM domains of integral membrane proteins in association with the SEC translocon. However, this signal only occurred in the ER membrane, demonstrating the specificity of the BiFC assay. This data is presented in Fig. 9A, B. Additionally, we show that FLWR-1 expression has a function in stabilizing MCA-3 localization at synapses, which is also in line with the idea of a direct interaction (Fig. 9C, D).

      (2) In silico analysis identified residues R27 and K31 as potential PIP2 binding sites in FLWR-1. The authors observed that FLWR-1(R27A/K31A) was less effective than wild-type FLWR-1 in rescuing the aldicarb sensitivity phenotype of the flwr-1 knockout, suggesting that FLWR-1 function may depend on PIP2 binding at these two residues. Given that mutations in various residues can impair protein function non-specifically, additional studies may be needed to confirm the significance of these residues for PIP2 binding and FLWR-1 function. In addition, the authors might consider explicitly discussing how this finding aligns or contrasts with the results of a previous study in flies, where alanine substitutions at K29 and R33 impaired a Flower-related function (Li et al., eLife 2020).

      We further investigated the role of these two residues in an in vivo assay for PIP2 binding and membrane association of a reporter. We used the coelomocytes (CCs), in which a previous publication demonstrated that a GFP variant tagged with a PH domain would be recruited to the CC membrane (Bednarek et al., 2007, Traffic 8: 543-53). This assay was performed in wild type, flwr-1 mutants, and flwr-1 mutants rescued with wild type FLWR-1, the FLWR-1(E74Q) mutant, or the FLWR-1(K27A; R31A) double mutant. The data are shown in Fig. 8C, D. While the wild type FLWR-1 rescued PH-GFP levels at the CC membrane to the wild type control, the FLWR-1(K27A; R31A) double mutant did not rescue the reporter binding, indicating that, at least in CCs, reduced PIP2 levels are associated with non-functional FLWR-1. Mechanistically, this is not clear at present, though we noted a possible mechanism as found for synaptotagmin, that recruits the PIP2 kinase to the plasma membrane via a lysine and arginine containing motif (Bolz et al., 2023, Neuron 111: 3765-3774.e3767). We mention this now in the discussion. We also discussed our data with respect to the findings of Li et al., about the analogous residues K27, R31 (K29, R33) in the discussion section, i.e. lines 667-670, and the differences of our findings in electron microscopy compared to the Drosophila work (more rather than less bulk endosomes) were discussed in lines 713-720.

      (3) A primary conclusion from the EM data was that FLWR-1 participates in the breakdown, rather than the formation, of bulk endosomes (lines 20-22). However, the reasoning behind this conclusion is somewhat unclear. Adding more explicit explanations in the Results section would help clarify and strengthen this interpretation.

      We added a sentence trying to better explain our reasoning. Mainly, the argument is that accumulation of such endosomes of unusually large size is seen in mutants affecting formation of SVs from the endosome (in endophilin and synaptojanin mutants), while mutants affecting mainly endocytosis (dynamin) cause formation of many smaller endocytic structures that stay attached to the plasma membrane (Kittelmann et al., 2013, PNAS 110: E3007-3016). We changed our data analysis in that we collated the data for what we previously termed endosomes and large vesicles. According to the paper by Watanabe, 2013, eLife 2: e00723, endosomes are defined by their location in the synapse, and their size. However, this work used a much shorter stimulus and froze the preparations within a few dozens to hundreds of msec after the stimulus, while we used the protocol of Kittelmann 2013, which uses 30 sec stimulation and freezing after 5 sec. There, endosomes were defined as structures larger than SVs or DCVs, but no larger than 80 nm, with an electron dense lumen, and were very rarely observed. In contrast, large vesicles or ‘100 nm vesicles’, ranged from 50-200 nm diameter, with a clear lumen, were morphologically similar to the bulk endosomes as observed by Li et al., 2021. We thus reordered our data and jointly analyzed these structure as large vesicles / bulk endosomes. The outcome is still the same, i.e. photostimulated flwr-1 mutants showed more LVs than wild type synapses.

      (4) The aldicarb assay results in Figure 3 are intriguing, indicating that reduced GABAergic neuron activity alone accounts for the flwr-1 mutant's hyposensitivity to aldicarb. Given that cholinergic motor neurons also showed increased activity in the flwr-1 mutant, one might expect the flwr-1 mutant to display hypersensitivity to aldicarb in the unc-47 knockout background. However, this was not observed. The authors might consider validating their conclusion with an alternative approach or, at the minimum, providing a plausible explanation for the unexpected result. Since aldicarb-induced paralysis can be influenced by factors beyond acetylcholine release from cholinergic motor neurons, interpreting aldicarb assay results with caution may be advisable. This is especially relevant here, as FLWR-1 function in muscle cells also impacts aldicarb sensitivity (Figure S3B). Previous electrophysiological studies have suggested that aldicarb sensitivity assays may sometimes yield misleading conclusions regarding protein roles in acetylcholine release.

      We tested the unc-47; flwr-1 animals again at a lower concentration of aldicarb, to see if the high concentration may have leveled the differences between unc-47 animals and the double mutant. This experiment is shown in Fig. S3D, demonstrating that the double mutant is significantly less resistant to aldicarb. This verifies that FLWR-1 acts not only in GABAergic neurons, but also in cholinergic neurons (as we saw by electron microscopy and electrophysiology), and that the increased excitability of cholinergic cells leads to more acetylcholine being released. In the double mutant, where GABA release is defective, this conveys hypersensitivity to aldicarb.

      (5) Previous studies have suggested that the Flower protein functions as a Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel, with a conserved glutamate residue at the putative selectivity filter being essential for this role. However, mutating this conserved residue (E74Q) in C. elegans FLWR-1 altered aldicarb sensitivity in a direction opposite to what would be expected for a Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel function. Moreover, the authors observed that E74 of FLWR1 is not located near a potential conduction pathway in the FLWR-1 tetramer, as predicted by Alphafold3. These findings raise the possibility that Flower may not function as a Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel. While this is a potentially significant discovery, further experiments are needed to confirm and expand upon these results.

      As above, we do not exclude that FLWR-1 may constitute a channel, however, based on our findings, AF3 structure predictions and data in the literature, we are considering alternative explanations for the observed effect on Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels of Flower mutants in worms and flies. The observations of increase Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels in stimulated flwr-1 mutant neurons could result from a reduced stimulation of the PMCA, and this was also observed with low stimulation in Drosophila (Yao et al., 2017). This idea is supported by the indications of a direct physical interaction, or proximity, of the two proteins. The reduced Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels after hyperstimulation of Drosophila Flower mutants may have to do with increased levels of non-recycling PMCA in the plasma membrane, indicating that PMCA requires Flower for recycling. This could be underlying the rundown of evoked PSCs we find in worm flwr-1 mutants, and would also be in line with a function of FLWR-1 and MCA-3 in coelomocytes, cells that constantly endocytose, and in which both proteins are required for proper function (our data, Figs. 5A, B; 8D, E) and Bednarek et al., 2007 (Traffic 8: 543-553). CCs need to recycle / endocytose membranes and membrane proteins, and such proteins, likely including FLWR-1 and MCA-3, need to be returned to the PM effectively.

      We thus refrained from testing a putative FLWR-1 channel function in Xenopus oocytes, in part also because we would not be able to acutely trigger possible FLWR-1 gating. A constitutive Ca<sup>2+</sup> current, if it were present, would induce large Cl<sup>-</sup> conductance in oocytes, that would likely be problematic / killing the cells. The demonstration that FLWR-1(E74Q) does not rescue the PI(4,5)P<sub>2</sub> levels in coelomocytes is also more in line with a non-channel function of FLWR-1.

      (6) Phrases like "increased excitability" and "increased Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx" are used throughout the manuscript. However, there is no direct evidence that motor neurons exhibit increased excitability or Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx. The authors appear to interpret the elevated Ca<sup>2+</sup> signal in motor neurons as indicative of both increased excitability and Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx. However, this elevated Ca<sup>2+</sup> signal in the flwr-1 mutant could occur independently of changes in excitability or Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx, such as in cases of reduced MCA-3 activity. The authors may wish to consider alternative terminology that more accurately reflects their findings.

      Thank you, we rephrased the imprecise wording. Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx was meant with respect to the cytosol.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Seidenthal et al. investigated the role of the Flower protein, FLWR-1, in C. elegans and confirmed its involvement in endocytosis within both synaptic and non-neuronal cells, possibly by contributing to the fission of bulk endosomes. They also uncovered that FLWR-1 has a novel inhibitory effect on neuronal excitability at GABAergic and cholinergic synapses in neuromuscular junctions.

      Strengths:

      This study not only reinforces the conserved role of the Flower protein in endocytosis across species but also provides valuable ultrastructural data to support its function in the bulk endosome fission process. Additionally, the discovery of FLWR-1's role in modulating neuronal excitability broadens our understanding of its functions and opens new avenues for research into synaptic regulation.

      Weaknesses:

      The study does not address the ongoing debate about the Flower protein's proposed Ca<sup>2+</sup> channel activity, leaving an important aspect of its function unexplored. Furthermore, the evidence supporting the mechanism by which FLWR-1 inhibits neuronal excitability is limited. The suggested involvement of MCA-3 as a mediator of this inhibition lacks conclusive evidence, and a more detailed exploration of this pathway would strengthen the findings.

      We added new data showing the likely direct interaction of FLWR-1 with the PMCA, possibly upregulating / stimulating its function. This data is shown now in Fig. 9A, B. Also, we show now that FLWR-1 is required to stabilize MCA-3 expression / localization in the pre-synaptic plasma membrane (Fig. 9C, D). These findings are not supporting the putative function of FLWR-1 as an ion channel, but suggest that increased Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels following neuron stimulation in flwr-1 mutants are due to an impairment of MCA-3 and thus reduced Ca<sup>2+</sup> extrusion.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors might consider focusing on one or two key findings from this study and providing robust evidence to substantiate their conclusions.

      We did substantiate the interactions of FLWR-1 and the PMCA, as well as assessing the function of FLWR-1 in the coelomocytes and the function of FLWR-1 in regulating PIP2 levels in the plasma membrane.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Behavioral Analysis of Locomotion

      In Figure 1, the authors are encouraged to examine whether flwr-1 mutants show altered locomotion behaviors, such as velocity, in a solid medium.

      We performed such an analysis for wild type, comparing to flwr-1 mutants and flwr-1 mutants rescued with FLWR-1 expressed from the endogenous promoter. The data are shown in Fig. S1C. There was no difference. We note that we observed differences in swimming assays also only when we strongly stimulated the cholinergic neurons by optogenetic depolarization, but not during unstimulated, normal swimming.

      (2) Validation of FLWR-1 Tagging

      In Figure 2A, it is recommended that the authors confirm the functionality of the C-terminal-tagged FLWR-1.

      We performed such rescue assays during swimming. The data is shown in Fig. S2S, E. While the GFP::FLWR-1 animals were slightly affected right after the photostimulation, they quickly caught up with the wild type controls, while flwr-1 mutants remained affected even after several minutes.

      (3) Explanation of Differential Rescue in GABAergic Neurons and Muscle

      The authors should provide a rationale for why restoring FLWR-1 in GABAergic neurons fully rescues the aldicarb resistance phenotype, while its restoration in muscle also partially rescues it.

      We think that these effects are independent of each other, i.e. loss of FLWR-1 in muscles increases muscular excitability, which becomes apparent in the behavioral assay that depends on locomotion and muscle contraction. To assess this further, we performed combined GABAergic neuron and muscle rescue assays, as shown in Fig. S3B. The double rescue was not different from wild type, and performed better than the muscle rescue alone.

      (4) Rescue Experiments for Swimming Defect in GABAergic Neurons

      Consider adding rescue experiments to determine whether expressing FLWR-1 specifically in GABAergic neurons can restore the swimming defect phenotype.

      We did not perform this assay as swimming is driven by cholinergic neurons, meaning that we would only indirectly probe GABAergic neuron function and a GABAergic FLWR-1 rescue would likely not improve swimming much. Also, given the importance of the correct E/I balance in the motor neurons, it would likely require achieving expression levels that are very precisely matching endogenous expression levels, which is not possible in a cell-specific manner.

      (5) Further Data on GCaMP Assay for mca-3; flwr-1 Additive Effect

      The additive effect of the mca-3 and flwr-1 mutations on GCaMP signals requires further data for substantiation. Additional GCaMP recordings or statistical analysis would provide stronger support for the proposed interaction between MCA-3 and FLWR-1 in calcium signaling.

      Thank you. We increased the number of observations, and could thus improve the outcome of the assay in that it became more conclusive. Meaning, the double mutation was not exacerbating the effect of either single mutant, demonstrating that FLWR-1 and MCA-3 are acting in the same pathway. The data are in Fig. 7B, C.

      (6) Inclusion of Wild-Type FLWR-1 Rescue in Figures 8B and 8D

      Figures 8B and 8D would benefit from the inclusion of wild-type FLWR-1 as a rescue control.

      We included the FLWR-1 wild type rescue as suggested and summarized the data in Fig. 8B.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Responses to final minor critiques following initial revision

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The authors have generally done an excellent job of addressing my and the other reviewers' concerns. I have a few additional concerns that the authors could consider addressing through changes to the text: 

      We thank the Reviewer for this assessment and are glad to have addressed the major points.

      - Regarding the gRNA used for NMR studies, I thank the authors for adding additional rationale for their design of the RNA used. However, I still believe that it is misleading to term this RNA as a "gRNA", given that it is mainly composed of a sequence that is arbitrary (the spacer) and the sections of the gRNA that are constant between all gRNAs are truncated in a way that removes secondary structure that is likely essential for specific contacts with the Rec domains. I do not believe the authors need to make alterations to any of their experiments. However, I do think their description of the "gRNA" should be updated to properly reflect that this RNA lacks any of the secondary structure present in a typical gRNA, much of which is necessary to confer specificity of binding between GeoCas9 and the gRNA. As mentioned in my previous review, this may be best achieved by adding a cartoon of the secondary structure of the full-length gRNA and highlighting the region that was used in the truncated "gRNA". 

      We understand the Reviewer’s point. For any experiment in which the gRNA was truncated (i.e. NMR or some MST studies), we have clarified the text and no longer call it a “gRNA.” We state initially that it is a portion of the gRNA and then call it simply an “RNA.” 

      For experiments using the full-length constructs, we have kept the term “gRNA,” as it remains appropriate.

      We have also added a final Supplementary figure (S12) showing the structures of the truncated and full-length RNAs used, based on the _Geo_Cas9 cryo-EM structure and predicted with RNAfold.

      - Lines 256-257: "The ~3-fold decrease in Kd...". I believe the authors are discussing the Kd's of the mutants relative to WT, in which case the Kd increased. Also, the fold-change appears closer to 2fold than to 3-fold. 

      Yes, the Reviewer makes a good catch. We have corrected this.

      - Lines 407-408: "The mutations also diminished the stability of the full-length GeoCas9 RNP complex." This statement seems at odds with the authors' conclusions in the Results section that the full-length GeoCas9 variants had comparable affinities for the gRNAs (lines 376-382) 

      We agree that this seems contradictory. In the absence of full-length structures for all variants, we can’t definitively state what causes this. It could be that the mutation has an interesting allosteric effect on structure that does not affect RNA binding but induces the Cas9 protein to simply fall apart at lower temperatures, rendering the binding interaction moot. We have added a statement to this section.

      - The authors chose to keep "SpCas9" for consistency with their prior work and the work of many several others, including Doudna et al and Zhang et al. However, I will note that their publications on GeoCas9, the Doudna lab did use SpyCas9 to ensure consistent nomenclature within the publications. 

      We have made the change to “_Spy_Cas9”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The authors clearly answered most of my concerns. I still have some technical questions about the analysis of CPMG-RD data but the numbers provided now seem to make sense. While I still think that crystal structures of the point mutant would make the conclusions more "bullet proof", I do appreciate the work associated with this and consider that the manuscript can be published as is. 

      We agree that additional magnetic fields could allow for additional models of CPMG data fitting and that additional crystal structures of the mutants could add to the conclusions. We appreciate the Reviewer recognizing the balance of the current results and potential future studies in signing off on publication.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary.

      The authors goal was to map the neural circuitry underlying cold sensitive contraction in Drosophila. The circuitry underlying most sensory modalities has been characterized but noxious cold sensory circuitry has not been well studied. The authors achieve their goal and map out sensory and post-sensory neurons involved in this behavior.

      Strengths.

      The manuscript provides convincing evidence for sensory and post sensory neurons involved in noxious cold sensitive behavior. They use both connectivity data and functional data to identify these neurons. This work is a clear advance in our understanding of noxious cold behavior. The experiments are done with a high degree of experimental rigor.

      Positive comments

      - Campari is nicely done to map cold responsive neurons, although it doesn't give data on individual neurons.

      - Chrimson and TNT experiments are nicely done.

      - Cold temperature activates basin neurons, it's a solid and convincing result.

      Weaknesses.

      Among the few weaknesses in this manuscript is the failure to trace the circuit from sensory neuron to motor neuron; and to ignore analysis of the muscles driving, cold induced contraction. Authors also need to elaborate more on the novel aspects of their work in the introduction or abstract.

      We have performed a more thorough em connectivity analysis of the CIII md neuron circuit (Figure 1A, Figure 1 – Figure supplement 1, Figure 10A). We now report all premotor neurons that are connected to CIII md neurons along with two additional projection/commandlike neurons. These additional premotor neurons (A01d3, A02e, A02f, A02g, A27k, and A31k) that are primarily implicated in locomotion were not required for cold nociception (Figure 5 – Figure supplement 2). Collectively, we have tested the requirement in cold nociception for ~94% synapses between CIII md->premotor neurons and all tested premotor with available driver lines. The requirement in cold nociception was also assessed for the two projection/command-like neurons dLIP7 and A02o neurons, which are required for sensory integration and directional avoidance to noxious touch, respectively (Figure 7 – Figure supplement 2) (Hu et al., 2017; Takagi et al., 2017). Silencing dLIP7 neurons resulted in modest reduction in cold-evoked behaviors, meanwhile A02o neurons were not required for cold nociception (Figure 7 – Figure supplement 2). To complete the analysis from thermosensation to evoked behavior, we analyzed cold-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> responses of larval musculature (Figure 10). Premotor neurons, which are connected to CIII md neurons, target multiple muscle groups (DL, DO, LT, VL, and VO) (Figure 10A). Individual larval segments have unique cold-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> responses, where the strongest cold-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> occurs in the central abdominal segments (Figure 10B-D). Inhibiting motor neuron activity or using an anesthetic (ethyl ether), there is a negligible cold-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> response compared to controls (Figure 10 – Figure supplement 1). Analysis of cold-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> in individual muscles reveal unique Ca<sup>2+</sup> dynamics for individual muscle groups (Figure 10E-H).

      Major comments.

      - Class three sensory neuron connectivity is known, and role in cold response is known (turner 16, 18). Need to make it clearer what the novelty of the experiments are.

      In figure 1, we are trying to guide the audience to CIII md neuron circuitry and emphasize the necessity and sufficiency CIII md neurons in cold nociception. Previously, only transient (GCaMP6) cold-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> were reported (Turner et al., 2016, 2018). However, here using CaMPARI, we performed dendritic spatial (sholl) analysis of cold-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> responses (Figure 1B-C). During the revision, we evaluated both CIII- and cold-evoked CT throughout larval development (Figure 1G, H). All in all, the findings from the first figure reiterate and replicate previous findings for the role of CIII md neuron in cold nociception. CIII md connectivity might be known, however, we investigated the functional and physiological roles of individual circuit neurons.

      - Why focus on premotor neurons in mechano nociceptive pathways? Why not focus on PMNs innervating longitudinal muscles, likely involved in longitudinal larval contraction? Especially since chosen premotor neurons have only weak effects on cold induced contraction?

      We assessed requirements for all premotor neurons that are connected to CIII md neurons and for which there are validated driver lines. Only premotor neurons (DnB, mCSI and Chair-1), which were previously initially implicated in mechanosensation, were also required for cold nociception. Premotor neurons previously implicated in locomotion (A01d3, A02e, A02f, A02g, A27k, and A31k) are not required for cold-evoked behaviors (Figure 5 – Figure supplement 2).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Patel et al perform the analysis of neurons in a somatosensory network involved in responses to noxious cold in Drosophila larvae. Using a combination of behavioral experiments, Calcium imaging, optogenetics, and synaptic connectivity analysis in the Drosophila larval they assess the function of circuit elements in the somatosensory network downstream of multimodal somatosensory neurons involved in innocuous and noxious stimuli sensing and probe their function in noxious cold processing, Consistent with their previous findings they find the multidendritic class III neurons, to be the key cold sensing neurons that are both required and sufficient for the CT behaviors response (shown to evoked by noxious cold). They further investigate the downstream neurons identified based on literature and connectivity from EM at different stages of sensory processing characterize the different phenotypes upon activating/silencing those neurons and monitor their responses to noxious cold. The work reveals diverse phenotypes for the different neurons studied and provides the groundwork for understanding how information is processed in the nervous system from sensory input to motor output and how information from different modalities is processed by neuronal networks. However, at times the writing could be clearer and some results interpretations more rigorous.

      Specific comments

      (1) In Figure 1 -supplement 6D-F (Cho co-activation)

      The authors find that Ch neurons are cold sensitive and required for cold nociceptive behavior but do not facilitate behavioral responses induced but CIII neurons

      The authors show that coactivating mdIII and cho inhibits the CT (a typically observed coldinduced behavioral response) in the second part of the stimulation period, while Cho was required for cold-induced CT. Different levels of activation of md III and Cho (different light intensities) could bring some insights into the observed phenotypes upon Cho manipulation as different levels activate different downstream networks that could correspond to different stimuli. Also, it would be interesting to activate chordotonal during exposure to cold to determine how a behavioral response to cold is affected by the activation of chordotonal sensory neurons.

      Modulating both CIII md and Ch activation to assess the contribution of individual sensory neuron’s role in thermosensation would certainly shed unique insights. However, we believe that such analyses are beyond the scope of the current manuscript and better suited to future followup studies.

      (2) Throughout the paper the co-activation experiments investigate whether co-activating the different candidate neurons and md III neurons facilitates the md III-induced CT response. However, the cold noxious stimuli will presumably activate different neurons downstream than optogenetic activation of MdIII and thus can reveal more accurately the role of the different candidate neurons in facilitating cold nociception.

      We agree that the CIII md neuron activation of the downstream circuitry would be different from the cold-evoked activation of neurons downstream of primary sensory neurons. We believe that our current finding lay foundations for future works to evaluate how multiple sensory neurons work in concert for generating stimulus specific behavioral responses.

      (3) Use of blue lights in behavioral and imaging experiments

      Strong Blue and UV have been shown to activate MDIV neurons (Xiang, Y., Yuan, Q., Vogt, N. et al. Light-avoidance-mediating photoreceptors tile the Drosophila larval body wall. Nature 468, 921-926 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09576) and some of the neurons tested receive input from MdIV.

      In their experiments, the authors used blue light to optogenetically activate CDIII neurons and then monitored Calcium responses in Basin neurons, premotor neurons, and ascending neurons and UV light is necessary for photoconversion in Campari Experiments. Therefore, some of the neurons monitored could be activated by blue light and not cdIII activation. Indeed, responses of Basin-4 neurons can be observed in the no ATR condition (Fig 3HI) and quite strong responses of DnB neurons. (Figure 6E) How do authors discern that the effects they see on the different neurons are indeed due to cold nociception and not the synergy of cold and blue light responses could especially be the case for DNB that could have in facilitating the response to cold in a multisensory context (where mdIV are activated by light).

      In addition, the silencing of DNB neurons during cold stimulation does not seem to give very robust phenotypes (no significant CT decrease compared to empty GAL4 control).

      It would be important to for example show that even in the absence of blue light the DNB facilitates the mdIII activation or cold-induced CT by using red light and Chrimson for example or TrpA activation (for coactivation with md III).

      Alternatively, in some other cases, the phenotype upon co-activation could be inhibited by blue light (e.g. chair-1 (Figure 5 H-I)).

      More generally, given the multimodal nature of stimuli activating mdIV , MdIII (and Cho) and their shared downstream circuitry it is important to either control for using the blue light in these stimuli or take into account the presence of the stimulus in interpreting the results as the coactivation of for example Cho and mdIII using blue lights also could activate mdIV (and downstream neurons, alter the state of the network that could inhibit the md III induced CT responses.

      Assessing the differences in behavioral phenotypes in the different conditions could give an idea of the influence of combining different modalities in these assays. For example, did the authors observe any other behaviors upon co-activation of MDIII and Cho (at the expense of CT in the second part of the stimulation) or did the larvae resume crawling? Blue light typically induces reorientation behavior. What about when co-activating mdIII and Basin-4?

      Using Chrimson and red light or TrpA in some key experiments e.g. with Cho, Basin-4, and DNB would clarify the implication of these neurons in cold nociception

      We agree that exposure to a bright light source results in avoidance behaviors in Drosophila larvae, which is primarily mediated by CIV md neurons. However, the light intensities used in our assays is much milder than the ones required to activate sensory neurons. Specifically, based on Xiang et al. 470nm light does not evoke any electrical response at the lowest tested light intensity (0.74mWmm<sup>-2</sup>), whereas our light intensity used in behavioral experiments was much lower at 0.15mWmm<sup>-2</sup>. Additionally, we assessed larval mobility and turning for control conditions ±ATR and also sensory neuron activation. As expected, there is an increase in larval immobility upon CIII md neurons activation (Author response image 1). Only activation of CIV md neurons resulted in light-evoked turning, meanwhile remaining conditions did show stimulus time locked turning response (Author response image 1). Furthermore, we tested whether the intensity of 470nm light used in our behavior experiments was enough to result in light-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> response in CIII md and CIV md neurons. We expressed RCaMP in sensory neurons using a pan-neural driver (GMR51C10<sup>GAL4</sup>). There was no detectable increase in light-evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> response in either CIII md or CIV md neuron (Author response image 1).

      Furthermore, we also tested multiple optogenetic actuators (ChR2, ChR2-H134R, and CsChrimson) and two CIII md driver lines (19-12<sup>Gal4</sup> and R83B04<sup>Gal4</sup>). Regardless of the optogenetic actuator used or the wavelength of the light used, we observe light-evoked CT responses (Figure 1– Figure supplement 6). We found using CsChrimson raises several procedural challenges with our current experimental setup. In our hands, CsChrimson showed extreme sensitivity to any amount ambient white light intensities, whereas others have used infrared imaging to counteract ambient light sensitivity. Our imaging setup is equipped with visible spectrum imaging and cannot be retrofitted record infrared light sources. Thus, we have limited the use of CsChrimson to optogenetic-Ca<sup>2+</sup> imaging experiments, where we are not recording larval behavior.

      The use of TrpA1 would require heat stimulation for activating the channels, which in turn would impact downstream circuit neurons that are shared amongst sensory neurons.

      For CaMPARI experiments, the PC light was delivered using a similar custom filter cube, which was used in the original CaMPARI paper (Fosque et al., 2015). This filter cube delivers 440nm wavelength as the PC light. PC light exposure in absence of cold stimulus does not result in differential CaMPARI conversion between CIII md and CIV md (F<sub>red/green</sub> = 0.086 and 0.097, respectively). For the same condition, Ch neurons have high CaMPARI, but it is expected as they function in proprioception. Therefore, the chances of downstream neurons being solely activated by PC light remain low. The differential baseline CaMPARI F<sub>red/green</sub> ratios of individual circuit neurons could be a result of varying resting state cytosolic Ca<sup>2+</sup> concentrations.

      Lastly, for optogenetic-GCaMP experiments, where we use CIII md>CsChrimson and Basin-2/-4 or DnB>GCaMP to visualize CIII md evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> responses in downstream neuron. Xiang et al. reported that confocal laser excitation for GCaMP does not activate CIV md neurons, which is consistent with what we have observed as well.

      Author response image 1.

      (A) For optogenetic experiments, percent turning was assessed in control conditions and sensory neuron activation. Only CIV md neurons activation results in an increase in bending response. Other conditions do not blue light-evoked turning. (A’) We assessed larval turning based on ellipse fitting using FIJI, the aspect ratio of the radii is indicative of larval bending state. We empirically determined that radii ratio of <2.5 represents a larval turning/bending. This method of ellipse fitting has previously been used to identify C. elegans postures using WrMTrck in FIJI (Nussbaum-Krammer et al., 2015). (B) Percent immobility for all control conditions plus sensory activation driver lines. Only CIII md neuron activation leads to sustained stimulus-locked increase in immobility. There’s also no blue light-evoked reductions in mobility, indicating that there was not increase in larval movement due to blue light. (C) We assessed CIII md (ddaF) and CIV md (ddaC) neurons response to blue light with similar light intensity that was used in behavioral optogenetic experiments. There is no blue light evoked increase in RCaMP fluorescence.

      (4) Basins

      - Page 17 line 442-3 "Neural silencing of all Basin (1-4) neurons, using two independent driver lines (R72F11GAL4 and R57F07<sup>GAL4</sup>).

      Did the authors check the expression profile of the R57F07 line that they use to probe "all basins"? The expression profile published previously (Ohyama et al, 2015, extended data) shows one basin neuron (identified as basin-4 ) and some neurons in the brain lobes. Also, the split GAL4 that labels Basin-4 (SS00740) is the intersection between R72F11 and R57F07 neurons. Thus the R57F07 likely labels Basin-4 and if that is the case the data in Figure 2 9 and supplement) and Figure 3 related to this driver line, should be annotated as Basin-4, and the results and their interpretation modified to take into account the different phenotypes for all basins and Basin-4 neurons.

      Due to the non-specific nature of R57F07<sup>GAL4</sup> in labeling Basin-4 and additional neuron types, we have decided to remove the driver line from our current analysis. We would need to perform further independent investigations to identify the other cell types and validate their role in cold nociception.

      Page 19 l. 521-525 I am confused by these sentences as the authors claim that Basin-4 showed reduced Calcium responses upon repetitive activation of CDIII md neurons but then they say they exhibit sensitization. Looking at the plots in FIG 3 F-I the Basin-4 responses upon repeated activation seem indeed to decrease on the second repetition compared to the first. What is the sensitization the authors refer to?

      We have rephrased this section.

      On Page 47-In this section of the discussion, the authors emit an interesting hypothesis that the Basin-1 neuron could modulate the gain of behavioral responses. While this is an interesting idea, I wonder what would be the explanation for the finding that co-activation of Cho and MDIII does not facilitate cold nociceptive responses. Would activation of Basin-1 facilitate the cold response in different contexts (in addition to CH0-mediated stimuli)?

      Page 48 Thus the implication of the inhibitory network in cold processing should be better contextualized.

      The authors explain the difference in the lower basin-2 Ca- response to Cold/ mdIII activation (compared to Basin-4) despite stronger connectivity, due a stronger inputs from inhibitory neurons to Basin-2 (compared to Basin-4). The previously described inhibitory neurons that synapse onto Basin-2 receive rather a small fraction of inputs from the class III sensory neurons. The differences in response to cold could be potentially assigned to the activation of the inhibitory neurons by the cold-sensing cho- neurons. However, that cannot explain the differences in responses induced by class III neurons. Do the authors refer to additional inhibitory neurons that would receive significant input from MdIII?

      Alternative explanations could exist for this difference in activation: electrical synapses from mdIII onto Basin-4, and by stronger inputs from mdIV (compared to Basin-2 in the case of responses to Cold stimulus (Cold induces responses in md IV sensory neurons). Different subtypes of CD III may differentially respond to cold and the cold-sensing ones could synapse preferentially on basin-4 etc.

      A possible explanation for lack of CT facilitation when Ch and CIII md neurons are both activated are likely the competing sensory inputs going into Basins and yet unknown role of the inhibitory network between sensory and Basin neurons in cold nociception (Jovanic et al., 2016). Mechanical activation of Ch leads to several behavioral responses (hunch, back-up, pause, crawl, and/or bend) and transition between behaviors (Kernan et al., 1994; Tsubouchi et al., 2012; Zhang et al., 2015; Turner et al., 2016, 2018; Jovanic et al., 2019; Masson et al., 2020).

      Meanwhile, primary CIII md-/cold-evoked is CT (Turner et al., 2016, 2018, Patel et al., 2022, Himmel et al., 2023). Certain touch- versus cold- evoked behaviors are mutually exclusive, where co-activation of Ch and CIII md likely leads to competing neural impulses leading to lack of any single behavioral enhancement. Furthermore, the mini circuit motif between Ch and Basins consisting of feedforward, feedback and lateral inhibitory neurons that play a role in behavioral selection and transitions might impact the overall output of Basin neurons. Upon Ch and CIII md neuron co-activation, the cumulative Basin neuronal output may be biased towards increased behavioral transitions instead of sustained singular behavior response.

      While we posited one possible mechanism explaining the differences between cold- or CIII mdevoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> responses in Basin 2 and 4 neurons, where we suggest the differences in evoked Ca<sup>2+</sup> responses may arise due to differential connectivity of TePns and inhibitory network neurons to Basin 2 and/or 4. Furthermore, ascending A00c neurons are connected to descending feedback SEZ neuron, SeIN128, which have connectivity to Basins (1-3 and strongest with Basin 2), A02o, DnB, Chair-1 and A02m/n (Ohyama et al., 2015; Zhu et al., 2024). However, how the 5 different subtypes of CIII md neurons respond to cold is unknown. Electrical recordings of the dorsal CIII md neurons revealed that within & between neuron subtypes there’s variability in temperature sensitivity of individual neurons, where population coding results in fine-tuned central temperature representation (Maksymchuk et al., 2022). Evaluating the role of how individual CIII md subtypes Basin activation could reveal important insights into the precise relationship between CIII md and multisensory integration Basin neurons. However, as of yet there are no known CIII md neuron driver lines that mark a subset of CIII md neurons thus limiting further clarification on how primary sensory information is transduced to integration neurons.

      (5) A00c

      Page 26 Figure 4F-I line While Goro may not be involved in cold nociception the A00c (and A05q) seems to be.

      A00c could convey information to other neurons other than Goro and thus be part of a pathway for cold-induced CT.

      A deeper look into A00c connectivity reveals that there is a reciprocal relationship between A00c and SEZ descending neuron, SeIN128 (Ohyama et al., 2015; Zhu et al., 2024). Additionally, this feedback SEZ descending neuron synapse onto A02o, A05q, Basins (highest connectivity to Basin 2 and weak connectivity to Basin 1 & 3), and select premotor neurons (Chair-1, DnB, and A02m/n) (Ohyama et al., 2015; Zhu et al., 2024). Interestingly, SEZ feedback neuron likely plays a role in the observed cold-/CIII md neuron evoked differential calcium activity and behavioral requirement amongst Basin-2 and -4 in cold nociception. We have added this to our discussion section.

      (6) Page 31 766-768 the conclusion that "premotor function is required for and can facilitate cold nociception" seems odd to stress as one would assume that some premotor neurons would be involved in controlling the behavioral responses to a stimulus. It would be more pertinent in the summary to specify which premotor neurons are involved and what is their function

      We have updated the section regarding premotor neurons’ role in cold nociception and now there’s a more specific concluding statement.

      (7) There are several Split GAL4 used in the study (with transgenes inserted in attP40 et attP2 site). A recent study points to a mutation related to attP40 that can have an effect on muscle function: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9750024/. The controls used in behavioral experiments do not contain the attP40 site. It would be important to check a control genotype bearing an attP40 site and characterize the different parameters of the CT behavior to cold and take this into account in interpreting the results of the experiments using the SplitGAL4 lines

      We have performed control experiments bearing empty attP40;attP2 sites in our neural silencing experiments. The observed muscle phenotypes were present in larvae bearing homozygous copies attP40/attP40 (van der Graaf et al., 2022). However, in our experiments, none of the larvae that we tested behaviorally had homozygous attP40;attP2 insertions. We have updated Table 1 to now include insertion sites.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors follow up on prior studies where they have argued for the existence of cold nociception in Drosophila larvae. In the proposed pathway, mechanosensitive Class III multidendritic neurons are the noxious cold responding sensory cells. The current study attempts to explore the potential roles of second and third order neurons, based on information of the Class III neuron synaptic outputs that have been obtained from the larval connectome.

      Strengths:

      The major strength of the manuscript is the detailed discussion of the second and third order neurons that are downstream of the mechanosensory Class III multidendritic neurons. These will be useful in further studies of gentle touch mechanosensation and mechanonociception both of which rely on sensory input from these cells. Calcium imaging experiments on Class III

      activation with optogenetics support the wiring diagram.

      Weaknesses:

      The scientific premise is that a full body contraction in larvae that are exposed to noxious cold is a sensorimotor behavioral pathway. This premise is, to start with, questionable. A common definition of behavior is a set of "orderly movements with recognizable and repeatable patterns of activity produced by members of a species (Baker et al., 2001)." In the case of nociception behaviors, the patterns of movement are typically thought to play a protective role and to protect from potential tissue damage.

      Does noxious cold elicit a set of orderly movements with a recognizable and repeatable pattern in larvae? Can the patterns of movement that are stimulated by noxious cold allow the larvae to escape harm? Based on the available evidence, the answer to both questions is seemingly no. In response to noxious cold stimulation many, if not all, of the muscles in the larva, simultaneously contract (Turner et al., 2016), and as a result the larva becomes stationary. In response to cold, the larva is literally "frozen" in place and it is incapable of moving away. This incapacitation by cold is the antithesis of what one might expect from a behavior that protects the animals from harm.

      Extensive literature has investigated the physiological responses of insects to cold (reviewed in Overgaard and MacMillan, 2017). In numerous studies of insects across many genera (excluding cold adapted insects such as snow flies), exposure to very cold temperatures quickly incapacitates the animal and induces a state that is known as a chill coma. During a chill coma, the insect becomes immobilized by the cold exposure, but if the exposure to cold is very brief the insect can often be revived without apparent damage. Indeed, it is common practice for many laboratories that use adult Drosophila for studies of behavior to use a brief chilling on ice as a form of anesthesia because chilling is less disruptive to subsequent behaviors than the more commonly used carbon dioxide anesthesia. If flies were to perceive cold as a noxious nociceptive stimulus, then this "chill coma" procedure would likely be disruptive to behavioral studies but is not. Furthermore, there is no evidence to suggest that larval sensation of "noxious cold" is aversive.

      The insect chill coma literature has investigated the effects of extreme cold on the physiology of nerves and muscles and the consensus view of the field is that the paralysis that results from cold is due to complex and combined action of direct effects of cold on muscle and on nerves (Overgaard and MacMillan, 2017). Electrophysiological measurements of muscles and neurons find that they are initially depolarized by cold, and after prolonged cold exposure they are unable to maintain potassium homeostasis and this eventually inhibits the firing of action potentials (Overgaard and MacMillan, 2017). The very small thermal capacitance of a Drosophila larva means that its entire neuromuscular system will be quickly exposed to the effect of cold in the behavioral assays under consideration here. It would seem impossible to disentangle the emergent properties of a complex combination of effects on physiology (including neuronal, glial, and muscle homeostasis) on any proposed sensorimotor transformation pathway.

      Nevertheless, the manuscript before us makes a courageous attempt at attempting this. A number of GAL4 drivers tested in the paper are found to affect parameters of contraction behavior (CT) in cold exposed larvae in silencing experiments. However, notably absent from all of the silencing experiments are measurements of larval mobility following cold exposure. Thus, it is not known from the study if these manipulations are truly protecting the larvae from paralysis following cold exposure, or if they are simply reducing the magnitude of the initial muscle contraction that occurs immediately following cold (ie reducing CT). The strongest effect of silencing occurs with the 19-12-GAL4 driver which targets Class III neurons (but is not completely specific to these cells).

      Optogenetic experiments for Class III neurons relying on the 19-12-GAL4 driver combined with a very strong optogenetic acuator (ChETA) show the CT behavior that was reported in prior studies. It should be noted that this actuator drives very strong activation, and other studies with milder optogenetic stimulation of Class III neurons have shown that these cells produce behavioral responses that resemble gentle touch responses (Tsubouchi et al 2012 and Yan et al 2013). As well, these neurons express mechanoreceptor ion channels such as NompC and Rpk that are required for gentle touch responses. The latter makes the reported Calcium responses to cold difficult to interpret in light of the fact that the strong muscle contractions driven by cold may actually be driving mechanosensory responses in these cells (ie through deformation of the mechanosensitive dendrites). Are the cIII calcium signals still observed in a preparation where cold induced muscle contractions are prevented?

      A major weakness of the study is that none of the second or third order neurons (that are downstream of CIII neurons) are found to trigger the CT behavioral responses even when strongly activated with the ChETA actuator (Figure 2 Supplement 2). These findings raise major concerns for this and prior studies and it does not support the hypothesis that the CIII neurons drive the CT behaviors.

      Later experiments in the paper that investigate strong CIII activation (with ChETA) in combination with other second and third order neurons does support the idea activating those neurons can facilitate body-wide muscle contractions. But many of the co-activated cells in question are either repeated in each abdominal neuromere or they project to cells that are found all along the ventral nerve cord, so it is therefore unsurprising that their activation would contribute to what appears to be a non-specific body-wide activation of muscles along the AP axis. Also, if these neurons are already downstream of the CIII neurons the logic of this coactivation approach is not particularly clear. A more convincing experiment would be to silence the different classes of cells in the context of the optogenetic activation of CIII neurons to test for a block of the effects, a set of experiments that is notably absent from the study.

      The authors argument that the co-activation studies support "a population code" for cold nociception is a very optimistic interpretation of a brute force optogenetics approach that ultimately results in an enhancement of a relatively non-specific body-wide muscle convulsion.

      We have responded extensively to reviewer 3’s comments in our provisional response to address the critiques regarding conceptual merit of this paper.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Nakagawa and colleagues report the observation that YAP is differentially localized, and thus differentially transcriptionally active, in spheroid cultures versus monolayer cultures. YAP is known to play a critical role in the survival of drug-tolerant cancer cells, and as such, the higher levels of basally activated YAP in monolayer cultures lead to higher fractions of surviving drug-tolerant cells relative to spheroid culture (or in vivo culture). The findings of this study, revealed through convincing experiments, are elegantly simple and straightforward, yet they add significantly to the literature in this field by revealing that monolayer cultures may actually be a preferential system for studying residual cell biology simply because the abundance of residual cells in this format is much greater than in spheroid or xenograft models. The potential linkage between matrix density and stiffness and YAP activation, while only speculated upon in this manuscript, is intriguing and a rich starting point for future studies.

      Although this work, like any important study, inspires many interesting follow-on questions, I am limiting my questions to only a few minor ones, which may potentially be explored either in the context of the current study or in separate, follow-on studies.

      We appreciate Reviewer #1's comments that our work is of importance to the field and particularly that it will "...add significantly to the literature in this field by revealing that monolayer cultures may actually be a preferential system for studying residual cell biology..."  We have sought to highlight the importance of how our findings could be applied to study resistance mechanisms at various points in the manuscript.

      Strengths:

      The major strengths of the work are described above.

      Weaknesses:

      Rather than considering the following points as weaknesses, I instead prefer to think of them as areas for future study:

      (1) Given the field's intense interest in the biology and therapeutic vulnerabilities of residual disease cells, I suspect that one major practical implication of this work could be that it inspires scientists interested in working in the residual disease space to model it in monolayer culture. However, this relies upon the assumption that drug-tolerant cells isolated in monolayer culture are at least reasonably similar in nature to drug-tolerant cells isolated from spheroid or xenograft systems. Is this true? An intriguing experiment that could help answer this question would be to perform gene expression profiling on a cell line model in the following conditions: monolayer growth, drug tolerant cells isolated from monolayer growth conditions, spheroid growth, drug tolerant cells isolated from spheroid growth conditions, xenograft tumors, and drug tolerant cells isolated from xenograft tumors. What are the genes and programs shared between drug-tolerant cells cultured in the three conditions above? Which genes and programs differ between these conditions? Data from this exercise could help provide additional, useful context with which to understand the benefits and pitfalls of modeling residual tumor cell growth in monolayer culture.

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting valuable future studies. We agree that the proposed experiments represent important next steps in understanding the role of YAP and other pathways in primary resistance. We believe, however, these experiments are both beyond the scope of the current manuscript and beyond what can reasonably be addressed in a revision. The distinct challenges associated with comparing in vivo and in vitro conditions would require significant optimization of single-cell approaches, especially given the robust cell death driven by afatinib treatment in vivo. Given the complexity of in vivo experimentation, we are concerned that such studies may not guarantee biologically meaningful insights. Nonetheless, we agree that this is a compelling direction for future research. If common gene expression patterns could be identified despite these challenges, such studies could help validate monolayer culture as a relevant model for investigating residual disease.

      (2) In relation to the point above, there is an interesting and established connection between mesenchymal gene expression and YAP/TAZ signaling. For example, analyses of gene expression data from human tumors and cell lines demonstrate an extremely strong correlation between these two gene expression programs. Further, residual persister cancer cells have often been characterized as having undergone an EMT-like transition. From the analysis above, is there evidence that residual tumor cells with increased YAP signaling also exhibit increased mesenchymal gene expression?

      We agree with the reviewer that a connection between YAP/TAZ activity and EMT is likely, given prior studies exploring correlations between these two gene signatures. We believe, however, exploring EMT represents a distinct research direction from the primary focus of the current manuscript.  We are concerned exploration of EMT, especially in the absence of corresponding preclinical models or mechanistic data directly linking EMT to therapy resistance in our models, could distract from the main conclusions of the manuscript. While we plan to stain for EMT-associated markers in the residual cancer tissue from the in vivo studies, it remains unclear whether such data would meaningfully contribute to the revised manuscript, regardless of the outcome.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Nakagawa R, et al describes a mechanism of how NSCLC cells become resistant to EGFR and KRAS G12C inhibition. Here, the authors focus on the initial cellular changes that occur to confer resistance and identify YAP activation as a non-genetic mechanism of acute resistance.

      The authors performed an initial xenograft study to identify YAP nuclear localization as a potential mechanism of resistance to EGFRi. The increase in the stromal component of the tumors upon Afatinib treatment leads the authors to explore the response to these inhibitors in both 2D and 3D culture. The authors extend their findings to both KRAS G12C and BRAF inhibitors, suggesting that the mechanism of resistance may be shared along this pathway.

      The paper would benefit from additional cell lines to determine the generalizability of the findings they presented. While the change in the localization of YAP upon Afatinib treatment was identified in a xenograft model, the authors do not return to animal models to test their potential mechanism, and the effects of the hyperactivated S127A YAP protein on Afatinib sensitivity in culture are modest. Also, combination studies of YAP inhibitors and EGFR/RAS/RAF inhibitors would have strengthened the studies.

      We thank the reviewer for their insightful comments. In this manuscript, we present data from 5 cell lines representing the EGFR/BRAF/KRAS pathway, demonstrating the generalizability of YAP-driven decreased cancer cell sensitivity to targeted inhibitors when cultured in 2D compared to spheroid counterparts. While expanding this analysis to a larger panel of cell lines is beyond the scope of the current study, we believe our findings provide a strong rationale for future investigations, including high-throughput screens conducted by other research groups and pharmaceutical companies, to recognize the value in screening spheroid cell cultures. We hope this work helps shift the field of cancer therapeutics toward screening approaches that better reflect tumor biology into drug discovery pipelines and believe this could be one of the most impactful and enduring contributions of our study.

      Reviewer #2 also mentions that "...combination studies of YAP inhibitors and EGFR/RAS/RAF inhibitors would have strengthened the studies..."  The concept that YAP/TAZ inhibitors (i.e. TEAD inhibitors) could be additive or synergistic in 2D culture is one that is being actively tested across several groups and in pharma. Several recent examples include a publication by Hagenbeek, et al., Nat. Cancer, 2023 (PMID: 37277530) showing that a TEAD inhibitor overcomes KRASG12C inhibitor resistance. Additional, recent work by Pfeifer, et al., Comm. Biol., 2024 (PMID: 38658677) suggests a similar effect between EGFR inhibitors and a different TEAD inhibitor. While neither of these studies extensively probes cell death pathways in the way performed in our studies, they nevertheless provide strong evidence that indeed TEAD + targeted EGFR/RAF/RAS inhibition in 2D have additive, if not synergistic, effects. We feel that these recent published studies affirm our findings and repeating such experiments is unlikely to add much new information. We thus feel they are beyond the scope of our present studies.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs) in the olfactory epithelium detect myriads of environmental odors that signal essential cues for survival. OSNs are born throughout life and thus represent one of the few neurons that undergo life-long neurogenesis. Until recently, it was assumed that OSN neurogenesis is strictly stochastic with respect to subtype (i.e. the receptor the OSN chooses to express).

      However, a recent study showed that olfactory deprivation via naris occlusion selectively reduced birthrates of only a fraction of OSN subtypes and indicated that these subtypes appear to have a special capacity to undergo changes in birthrates in accordance with the level of olfactory stimulation. These previous findings raised the interesting question of what type of stimulation influences neurogenesis, since naris occlusion does not only reduce the exposure to potentially thousands of odors but also to more generalized mechanical stimuli via preventing airflow.

      In this study, the authors set out to identify the stimuli that are required to promote the neurogenesis of specific OSN subtypes. Specifically, they aim to test the hypothesis that discrete odorants selectively stimulate the same OSN subtypes whose birthrates are affected. This would imply a highly specific mechanism in which exposure to certain odors can "amplify" OSN subtypes responsive to those odors suggesting that OE neurogenesis serves, in part, an adaptive function.

      To address this question, the authors focused on a family of OSN subtypes that had previously been identified to respond to musk-related odors and that exhibit higher transcript levels in the olfactory epithelium of mice exposed to males compared to mice isolated from males. First, the authors confirm via a previously established cell birth dating assay in unilateral naris occluded mice that this increase in transcript levels actually reflects a stimulus-dependent birthrate acceleration of this OSN subtype family. In a series of experiments using the same assay, they show that one specific subtype of this OSN family exhibits increased birthrates in response to juvenile male exposure while a different subtype shows increased birthrates to adult mouse exposure. In the core experiment of the study, they finally exposed naris occluded mice to a discrete odor (muscone) to test if this odor specifically accelerates the birth rates of OSN types that are responsive to this odor. This experiment reveals a complex relationship between birth rate acceleration and odor concentrations showing that some muscone concentrations affect birth rates of some members of this family and do not affect two unrelated OSN subtypes.

      In addition to the results nicely summarized by the reviewer, which focus on experiments to examine the effects of odor stimulation on unilateral naris occluded (UNO) mice, an important part of the present study are experiments on non-occluded (i.e., non-UNO-treated) mice. These experiments show: 1) that the exposure of non-occluded mice to odors from adolescent male mice selectively increases quantities of newborn OSNs of the musk-responsive subtype Olfr235 (Figure 3G, H; previously Figure 6), 2) the exposure of non-occluded female mice to 2 different musk odorants (muscone, ambretone) selectively increases quantities of newborn OSNs of 3 musk responsive subtypes: Olfr235, Olfr1440 and Olfr1431 (Figure 4D-F; previously Figure 6), and 3) the exposure of non-occluded adult female mice to a musk odorants selectively increases quantities of newborn OSNs of musk responsive subtypes (Figure 5; previously Fig. S7). We have reorganized the revised manuscript to more prominently and clearly present the experimental design and findings of these experiments. We have also made changes to clarify (via schematics) the experimental conditions used (i.e., UNO, non-UNO, odor exposure) in each experiment.

      Strengths:

      The scientific question is valid and opens an interesting direction. The previously established cell birth dating assay in naris occluded mice is well performed and accompanied by several control experiments addressing potential other interpretations of the data.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The main research question of this study was to test if discrete odors specifically accelerate the birth rate of OSN subtypes they stimulate, i.e. does muscone only accelerate the birth rate of OSNs that express muscone-responsive ORs, or vice versa is the birthrate of muscone-responsive OSNs only accelerated by odors they respond to?

      This question is only addressed in Figure 5 of the manuscript and the results only partially support the above claim. The authors test one specific odor (muscone) and find that this odor (only at certain concentrations) accelerates the birth rate of some musk-responsive OSN subtypes, but not two other unrelated control OSN subtypes. This does not at all show that musk-responsive OSN subtypes are only affected by odors that stimulate them and that muscone only affects the birthrate of musk-responsive OSNs, since first, only the odor muscone was tested and second, only two other OSN subtypes were tested as controls, that, importantly, are shown to be generally stimulus-independent OSN subtypes (see Figure 2 and S2).

      As a minimum the authors should have a) tested if additional odors that do not activate the three musk-responsive subtypes affect their birthrate b) choose 2-3 additional control subtypes that are known to be stimulus-dependent (from their own 2020 study) and test if muscone affects their birthrates.

      We appreciate these suggestions. Within the revised manuscript, we have described and included the results from several new experiments:

      (1) As noted by the reviewer, we had previously tested the effects of exposure to only one exogenous musk odorant, muscone, on quantities of newborn OSNs of the musk-responsive subtypes Olfr235, Olfr1440, and Olfr1431. To test whether the effects observed with muscone exposure occur with other musk odorants, we assessed the effects of exposure to ambretone (5-cyclohexadecenone), a musk odorant previously found to robustly activate musk-responsive OSNs (Sato-Akuhara et al., 2016; Shirasu et al., 2014), on quantities of newborn OSNs of 3 musk-responsive subtypes Olfr235, Olfr1440, and Olfr1431, as well as the SBT-responsive subtype Olfr912, in the OEs of non-occluded female mice. Exposure to ambretone was found to significantly increase quantities of newborn OSNs of all 3 musk-responsive subtypes (Figure 4D-F) but not the SBT-responsive subtype (Figure 4–figure supplement 4C-left), indicating that a variety of musk odorants can accelerate the birthrates of musk responsive subtypes.

      (2) To verify that exogenous non-musk odors do not increase quantities of newborn OSNs of musk responsive OSN subtypes (point a, above), we quantified newborn OSNs of 3 musk-responsive subtypes, Olfr235, Olfr1440, and Olfr1431, in non-occluded female mice that were exposed to the non-musk odorants SBT or IAA. As expected, neither of these odorants significantly affected the birthrates of the subtypes tested (Figure 4D-F).

      (3) To confirm that exogenous musk odors do not accelerate the birthrates of non-musk responsive OSN subtypes that were previously found to undergo stimulation-dependent neurogenesis (point b, above), we quantified newborn OSNs of 2 such subtypes, Olfr827 and Olfr1325, in non-occluded female mice that were exposed to muscone. As expected, exposure to muscone did not significantly affect the birthrates of either of these subtypes (Figure 4–figure supplement 4C-middle, right).

      (4) To provide additional confirmation that only some OSN subtypes have a capacity to exhibit increases in newborn OSN quantities in the presence of odors that activate them, we compared quantities of newborn OSNs of the SBT-responsive subtype Olfr912 in non-occluded females that were either exposed to 0.1% SBT versus unexposed controls. As expected, exposure of SBT caused no significant increase in quantities of newborn Olfr912 OSNs (Figure 4–figure supplement 4C-left).

      (2) The finding that Olfr1440 expressing OSNs do not show any increase in UNO effect size under any muscone concentration (Figure 5D, no significance in line graph for UNO effect sizes, middle) seems to contradict the main claim of this study that certain odors specifically increase birthrates of OSN subtypes they stimulate. It was shown in several studies that olfr1440 is seemingly the most sensitive OR for muscone, yet, in this study, muscone does not further increase birthrates of OSNs expressing olfr1440. The effect size on birthrate under muscone exposure is the same as without muscone exposure (0%).

      In contrast, the supposedly second most sensitive muscone-responsive OR olfr235 shows a significant increase in UNO effect size between no muscone exposure (0%) and 0.1% as well as 1% muscone.

      Findings that quantities of newborn Olfr1440 OSNs do not show a significantly greater UNO effect size in the OEs from mice exposed to muscone compared to control mice was also somewhat surprising to us. We think that there are two potential explanations for this result: 1) Unlike subtype Olfr235, subtype Olfr1440 exhibits a significant open-side bias in newborn OSN quantities in UNO-treated adolescent females even in the absence of exposure to muscone. We speculate that this subtype (as well as subtype Olfr1431) is stimulated by odors that are emitted by female mice at the adolescent stage, and/or by another environmental source. This may limit the influence of muscone exposure on the UNO effect size. 2) There is compelling evidence that odors within the environment can enter the closed side of the OE transnasally [via the nasopharyngeal canal (Kelemen, 1947)] and/or retronasally (via the nasopharynx) in UNO-treated mice [reviewed in (Coppola, 2012)]. Thus, it is conceivable that chronic exposure of UNO-treated mice to muscone results in the eventual entry on the closed side of the OE of muscone at concentrations sufficient to promote neurogenesis. If Olfr1440 is more sensitive to muscone than Olfr235 [e.g., (Sato-Akuhara et al., 2016; Shirasu et al., 2014)], OSNs of this subtype may be especially sensitive to small amounts of odors that enter the closed side of the OE transnasally and/or retronasally. These explanations are supported by the following results:

      - UNO-treated females exposed to 0.1% muscone show higher quantities of newborn Olfr1440 OSNs on both the open and closed sides of the OE in muscone exposed females compared to their unexposed counterparts (Figure 4–figure supplement 1A-middle). Similar results were also observed for newborn Olfr235 OSNs (Figure 4C-middle), albeit to a lesser extent, perhaps due to the lower sensitivity of this subtype to muscone.

      - In non-occluded female mice, exposure to 0.1% muscone was found to significantly increase quantities of newborn Olfr1440 OSNs, as well as newborn Olfr235 and Olfr1431 OSNs (Figure 4D-F in revised manuscript; Figure 6 in original version). Similar results were also observed upon exposure to ambretone, another musk odor (Figure 4D-F). These experiments strongly support the hypothesis that musk odors selectively increase birthrates of OSN subtypes that they stimulate.

      We have addressed these points within the results section of the revised manuscript.

      (3) The authors introduce their choice to study this particular family of OSN subtypes with first, the previous finding that transcripts for one of these musk-responsive subtypes (olfr235) are downregulated in mice that are deprived of male odors. Second, musk-related odors are found in the urine of different species. This gives the misleading impression that it is known that musk-related odors are indeed excreted into male mouse urine at certain concentrations. This should be stated more clearly in the introduction (or cited, if indeed data exist that show musk-related odors in male mouse urine) because this would be a very important point from an ethological and mechanistic point of view.

      In addition, this would also be important information to assess if the chosen muscone concentrations fall at all into the natural range.

      These are important points, which have addressed within the revised manuscript:

      (1) Within the introduction, we have now stated that the emission of musk odors by mice has not been documented. We have also added extensive discussions of what is known about the emission of musk odors by mice in a new subsection within Results, as well as within the Discussion section. Most prominently, we have cited one study (Sato-Akuhara et al., 2016) that noted unpublished evidence for the emission of Olfr1440-activating compounds from male preputial glands: “Indeed, our preliminary experiments suggest that there are unidentified compounds that activate MOR215-1 in mouse preputial gland extracts.” Another study, which used histomorphology, metabolomic and transcriptomic analyses to compare the mouse preputial glands to muskrat scent glands, found that the two glands are similar in many ways, including molecular composition (Han et al., 2022). However, the study did not identify known musk compounds within mouse preputial glands.

      (2) Based on the reviewer’s feedback and our own curiosity, we used GC-MS to analyze both mouse urine and preputial gland extracts for the presence of known musk odorants, particularly those known to activate Olfr235 and Olfr1440 (Sato-Akuhara et al., 2016). Although we were unable to find evidence for known musk odorants in mouse urine extracts (possibly due to insufficient sensitivity of the assay employed), we found that preputial gland extracts contain GC-MS signals that are structurally consistent with known musk odorants. A limitation of this approach, however, is that the conclusive identification of specific musk odorants in extracts derived from mouse urine and tissues requires comparisons to pure standards, many of which we could not readily obtain. For example, we were unable to obtain a pure sample of cycloheptadecanol, a musk molecule with a predicted potential match to a signal identified within preputial gland extracts. Another limitation is that although several known musk odorants have been found to activate Olfr235 and Olfr1440 OSNs, it is conceivable that structurally distinct odorants that have not yet been identified might also activate them. The findings from these experiments have been included in a new figure within the revised manuscript (Appendix 2–figure 1).

      Related: If these are male-specific cues, it is interesting that changes in OR transcripts (Figure 1) can already be seen at the age of P28 where other male-specific cues are just starting to get expressed. This should be discussed.

      We agree that the observed changes in quantities of newborn OSNs of musk-responsive subtypes in mice exposed to juvenile male odors deserves additional discussion. We have included a more extensive discussion of this observation in both the Results and Discussion sections of the revised manuscript.

      (4) Figure 5: Under muscone exposure the number of newborn neurons on the closed sides fluctuates considerably. This doesn't seem to be the case in other experiments and raises some concerns about how reliable the naris occlusion works for strong exposure to monomolecular odors or what other potential mechanisms are at play.

      We agree that the variability in quantities of newborn OSNs of musk-responsive subtypes on the closed side of the OE of UNO-treated mice deserves further discussion. As noted above, we suspect that these fluctuations are due, at least in part, to transnasal and/or retronasal odor transfer via the nasopharyngeal canal (Kelemen, 1947) and nasopharynx, respectively [reviewed in (Coppola, 2012)], which would be expected to result in exposure of the closed OE to odor concentrations that rise with increasing environmental concentrations. In support of this, quantities of newborn Olfr235 and Olfr1440 OSNs increase on both the open and closed sides with increasing muscone concentration (except at the highest concentration, 10%, in the case of Olfr1440) (Figure 4C-middle, Figure 4–figure supplement 1A-middle). It is conceivable that reductions in newborn Olfr1440 OSN quantities observed in the presence of 10% muscone reflect overstimulation-dependent reductions in survival. Our findings from UNO-based experiments are consistent with expectations that naris occlusion does not completely block exposure to odorants on the closed side, particularly at high concentrations. However, they also appear consistent with the hypothesis that exposure to musk odors promotes the neurogenesis of musk-responsive OSN subtypes.

      Considering the limitations of the UNO procedure, it is important to note that the present study also includes experimental exposure of non-occluded animals to both male odors (Figure 3G, H) and exogenous musk odorants (Figures 4D-F). Findings from the latter experiments provide strong evidence that exposure to multiple musk odorants (muscone, ambretone) causes selective increases in the birthrates of multiple musk-responsive OSN subtypes (Olfr235, Olfr1440, Olfr1431).

      We have included within the Results section of the revised manuscript a discussion of how observed effects of muscone exposure of UNO-treated mice may be influenced by transnasal/ retronasal odor transfer to the closed side of the OE.

      (5) In contrast to all other musk-responsive OSN types, the number of newborn OSNs expressing olfr1437 increases on the closed side of the OE relative to the open in UNO-treated male mice (Figure 1). This seems to contradict the presented theory and also does not align with the bulk RNAseq data (Figure S1).

      Subtype Olfr1437 is indeed an outlier among musk-responsive subtypes that were previously found to be more highly represented in the OSN population in 6-month-old sex-separated males compared to females (Appendix 1–figure 1)(C. van der Linden et al., 2018; Vihani et al., 2020). Somewhat unexpectedly, our findings from scRNA-seq experiments show slightly greater quantities of immature Olfr1437 OSNs on the closed side of the OE in juvenile males (Figure 1D, E of the revised manuscript, which now includes data from a second OE). Perhaps more informatively considering the small number of iOSNs of specific subtypes in the scRNA-seq datasets, EdU birthdating experiments show no difference in newborn Orlfr1437 OSN quantities on the 2 sides of the OE from UNO-treated juvenile males (Figure 2G). It is unclear to us why subtype Olfr1437 does not show open-side biases in newborn OSN quantities in juvenile male mice, but potential explanations include:

      - Age: Findings based on bulk RNA-seq that musk responsive OSN subtypes are more highly represented in mice exposed to male odors analyzed mice that were 6 months old (C. van der Linden et al., 2018) or > 9 months old (Vihani et al., 2020) at the time of analysis. By contrast, the present study primarily analyzed mice that were juveniles (PD 28) at the time of scRNA-seq analysis (Figure 1) or EdU labeling (Figure 2G). It is conceivable that different musk-responsive subtypes are selectively responsive to distinct odors that are emitted at different ages. In this scenario, odors that increase the birthrates of Olfr235, Olfr1440, and Olfr1431 OSNs may be emitted starting at the juvenile stage, while those that increase the birthrate of Olfr1437 OSNs may be emitted in adulthood. In potential support of this, juvenile males exposed to their adult parents at the time of EdU labeling showed a slightly greater (although not statistically significantly different) UNO effect size in quantities of newborn Olfr1437 OSNs compared to controls (Figure 3–figure supplement 3).

      - Capacity for stimulation-dependent neurogenesis: It is also conceivable that, unlike other musk-responsive OSN subtypes, Olfr1437 OSNs lack the capacity for stimulation-dependent neurogenesis (like the SBT-responsive subtype Olfr912, for example). If so, this would imply that increased representations of Olfr1437 OSNs observed in mice exposed to male odors for long periods (C. van der Linden et al., 2018; Vihani et al., 2020) may be due to male odor-dependent increases in the lifespans of Olfr1437 OSNs.

      Within the Discussion section of the revised manuscript, we have discussed the findings concerning Olfr1437.

      (6) The authors hypothesize in relation to the accelerated birthrate of musk-responsive OSN subtypes that "the acceleration of the birthrates of specific OSN subtypes could selectively enhance sensitivity to odors detected by those subtypes by increasing their representation within the OE". However, for two other OSN subtypes that detect male-specific odors, they hypothesize the opposite "By contrast, Olfr912 (Or8b48) and Olfr1295 (Or4k45), which detect the male-specific non-musk odors 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole (SBT) and (methylthio)methanethiol (MTMT), respectively, exhibited lower representation and/or transcript levels in mice exposed to male odors, possibly reflecting reduced survival due to overstimulation."

      Without any further explanation, it is hard to comprehend why exposure to male-derived odors should, on one hand, accelerate birthrates in some OSN subtypes to potentially increase sensitivity to male odors, but on the other hand, lower transcript levels and does not accelerate birth rates of other OSN subtypes due to overstimulation.

      We agree that this point deserves further explanation. Within the revised manuscript, we have expanded the Introduction and Results to describe evidence from previous studies that exposure to stimulating odors causes two categories of changes to specific OSN subtypes: elevated representations or reduced representations within the OSN population. In one study (C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020), UNO treatment was found to cause a fraction of OSN subtypes to exhibit lower birthrates and representations on the closed side of the OE relative to the open. By contrast, another fraction of OSN subtypes exhibited higher representations on the closed side of the OEs of UNO-treated mice, but no difference in birthrates between the two sides. The latter subtypes were found to be distinguished by their receipt of extremely high levels of odor stimulation, suggesting that reduced odor stimulation via naris occlusion may lengthen their lifespans. In support of the possibility that Olfr912 (and Olfr1295), which detect SBT and MTMT, respectively (Vihani et al., 2020), which are emitted specifically by male mice (Lin et al., 2005; Schwende et al., 1986), UNO treatment was previously found to increase total Olfr912 OSN quantities on the closed side compared to the open side in sex-separated males (C. van der Linden et al., 2018), a finding confirmed in the present study (Figure 3–figure supplement 1H).

      Taken together, findings from previous studies as well as the current one indicate that olfactory stimulation can accelerate the birthrates and/or reduced the lifespans of OSNs, depending on the specific subtypes and odors within the environment. As we have now indicated in the Discussion, we do not yet know what distinguishes subtypes that undergo stimulation-dependent neurogenesis, but it is conceivable that they detect odors with a particular salience to mice. Thus, observations that some odorants (e.g., musks) cause stimulation-dependent neurogenesis while others do not (e.g., SBT) might reflect an animal’s specific need to adapt its sensitivity to the former. Alternatively, it is conceivable that stimulation-dependent reductions in representations of subtypes such as Olfr912 and Olfr1295 reflect a fundamentally different mode of plasticity that is also adaptive, as has been hypothesized (C. van der Linden et al., 2018; Vihani et al., 2020).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      To support the main claim, several controls are necessary as mentioned under point 1 of the public review.

      As outlined in our responses to the public review, new experiments within the revised manuscript indicate the following:

      (1) Accelerated birthrates of 3 different musk responsive OSN subtypes (Olfr235, Olfr1440, Olfr1431) are observed in non-occluded mice following exposure to multiple exogenous musk odorants (muscone, ambretone) (Figure 4D-F).

      (2) Exposure of non-occluded mice to non-musk odors (SBT, IAA) does not accelerate the birthrates of musk responsive OSN subtypes (Olfr235, Olfr1440, Olfr1431) (Figure 4D-F).

      (3) Exposure of mice to exogenous musk odors (muscone, ambretone) does not accelerate the birthrates of non-musk responsive OSN subtypes (e.g., Olfr912), including those previously found to undergo stimulation-dependent neurogenesis (Olfr827, Olfr1325) (Figure 4–figure supplement 4C).

      (4) Only a fraction of OSN subtypes have a capacity to undergo accelerated neurogenesis in the presence of odors that activate them (e.g., Olfr912 birthrates are not accelerated by SBT exposure) (Figure 4–figure supplement 4C-left).

      In addition, this study could be considerably improved by showing that the proposed mechanism applies beyond a single OSN subtype (olfr235), especially since the most sensitive OR subtype (expressing olfr1440) does not align with the main claim. The introduction states that this is difficult because the ligands for many ORs are unknown including all subtypes previously found to undergo stimulation-dependent neurogenesis referring to your 2020 study. While this reviewer agrees that the lack of deorphanization is a significant hurdle in the field, the 2020 study states that about 4% of all ORs (which should equal >40 ORs) show a stimulus-dependent down-regulation on the closed side, not only the 7 ORs which are closer examined (Figure 1). It would tremendously improve the impact of the current study to show that the proposed effect applies also to one of these other >40 ORs.

      We appreciate this question, as it alerted us to some shortcomings in how our findings were presented within the original manuscript. We respectfully disagree that only findings regarding subtype Olfr235 align with the main hypothesis of this study, which is that discrete odors can selectively promote the neurogenesis of sensory neuron subtypes that they stimulate. Specifically, we would like to draw attention to experiments on non-occluded female mice exposed to exogenous musk odorants (muscone, ambretone; revised Figures 4D-F; previously, Figure 6). Findings from these experiments provide compelling evidence that exposure to musk odorants causes selective increases in the birthrates of three different musk-responsive OSN subtypes: Olfr235, Olfr1440, and Olfr1431. Thus, we would suggest that results from the present study already show that the proposed mechanism applies to more than the just Olfr235 subtype. However, we agree with what we think is the essence of the reviewer’s point: that it is important to determine the extent to which this mechanism applies to OSN subtypes that are responsive to other (i.e., non-musk) odorants. While, as noted by the reviewer, our previous study identified several OSN subtypes that undergo stimulation-dependent neurogenesis (as well as many others that predicted to do so)(C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020), we are not aware of ligands that have been identified with high confidence for those subtypes. Although we are in the process of conducting experiments to identify additional odor/subtype pairs to which the mechanism described in this study applies, the early-stage nature of these experiments precludes their inclusion in the present manuscript.

      The ethological and mechanistic relevance of the current study could be significantly improved by showing that musk-related odors that activate olfr235 are actually found in male mouse urine (and additionally are not found in female mouse urine). Otherwise, the implicated link between the acceleration of OSN birthrates by exposure to male odors and acceleration by specific monomolecular odors does not hold, raising the question of any natural relevance (e.g. the proposed adaptive function to increase sensitivity to certain odors).

      As noted in our responses to the public review, we have addressed this important point within the revised manuscript as follows:

      (1) We have included an extensive discussion of what is known about the emission of musk-like odors by mice.

      (2) We have used GC-MS to analyze both mouse urine and preputial gland extracts for the presence of known musk compounds. Although inconclusive, we report that preputial glands contain signals that are structurally consistent with known musk compounds. The findings of these experiments have been included in the revised manuscript (new Appendix 2–figure 1), along with a discussion of their limitations.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      In their paper entitled "In mice, discrete odors can selectively promote the neurogenesis of sensory neuron subtypes that they stimulate" Hossain et al. address lifelong neurogenesis in the mouse main olfactory epithelium. The authors hypothesize that specific odorants act as neurogenic stimuli that selectively promote biased OR gene choice (and thus olfactory sensory neuron (OSN) identity). Hossain et al. employ RNA-seq and scRNA-seq analyses for subtype-specific OSN birthdating. The authors find that exposure to male and musk odors accelerates the birthrates of the respective responsive OSNs. Therefore, Hossain et al. suggest that odor experience promotes selective neurogenesis and, accordingly, OSN neurogenesis may act as a mechanism for long-term olfactory adaptation.

      We appreciate this summary but would like to underscore that a mechanism involving biased OR gene choice is just one of two possibilities proposed in the Discussion section to explain how odorant stimulation of specific subtypes accelerates the birthrates of those subtypes.

      The authors follow a clear experimental logic, based on sensory deprivation by unilateral naris occlusion, EdU labeling of newborn neurons, and histological analysis via OR-specific RNA-FISH. The results reveal robust effects of deprivation on newborn OSN identity. However, the major weakness of the approach is that the results could, in (possibly large) parts, depend on "downregulation" of OR subtype-specific neurogenesis, rather than (only) "upregulation" based on odor exposure. While, in Figure 6, the authors show that the observed effects are, in part, mediated by odor stimulation, it remains unclear whether deprivation plays an "active" role as well. Moreover, as shown in Figure 1C, unilateral naris occlusion has both positive and negative effects in a random subtype sample.

      In our view, the present study involves two distinct and complementary experimental designs: 1) odor exposure of UNO-treated animals and 2) odor exposure of non-occluded animals. Here we address this comment with respect to each of these designs:

      (1) For experiments performed on UNO-treated animals, we agree that observed differences in birthrates on the open and closed sides of the OE reflect, largely, a deceleration (i.e., downregulation) of the birthrates of these subtypes on the closed side relative to the open (as opposed to an acceleration of birthrates on the open side). Our objective in using this design was to test the extent to which specific OSN subtypes undergo stimulation-dependent neurogenesis under various odor exposure conditions. According to the main hypothesis of this study, a lower birthrate of a specific OSN subtype on the closed side of the OE compared to the open is predicted to reflect a lower level of odor stimulation on the closed side received by OSNs of that subtype. However (and as described in our responses to reviewer #1), a limitation of this design is that environmental odorants, especially at high concentrations, are likely to stimulate responsive OSNs on the closed side of the OE in addition to the open side due to transnasal and/or retronasal air flow.

      (2) Experiments performed on non-occluded animals were designed to provide critical complementary evidence that specific OSN subtypes undergo accelerated neurogenesis in the presence of specific odors. Using this design, we have found compelling evidence that:

      - Exposure of non-occluded mice to male odors causes the selective acceleration of the birthrate of Olfr235 OSNs (Figure 3G, H).

      - Exposure of non-occluded female mice to two different musk odorants (muscone and ambretone) selectively accelerates the birthrates three different musk responsive subtypes: Olfr235, Olfr1440, and Olf1431 (Figure 4D-F and Figure 4–figure supplement 4C).

      We have reorganized the revised manuscript to more clearly present the most important experimental findings using these two experimental designs. We have also highlighted (via schematics) the experimental conditions (e.g., UNO, non-occlusion, odor exposure) used for each experiment.

      Another weakness is that the authors build their model (Figure 8), specifically the concept of selectivity, on a receptor-ligand pair (Olfr912 that has been shown to respond, among other odors, to the male-specific non-musk odors 2-sec-butyl-4,5-dihydrothiazole (SBT)) that would require at least some independent experimental corroboration. At least, a control experiment that uses SBT instead of muscone exposure should be performed.

      We agree that this important concern deserves additional control experiments and discussion. We have addressed this concern within the revised manuscript as follows:

      - Within the Results section, we have added multiple new control experiments (detailed in response to Reviewer #1), including the one recommended above. As suggested, we quantified newborn OSNs of the SBT-responsive subtype Olfr912 in non-occluded females that were either exposed to 0.1% SBT or unexposed controls. Exposure of SBT was found to cause no significant increase in quantities of newborn Olfr912 OSNs (newly added Figure 4–figure supplement 4C-left). These findings further support the model in Figure 7 (previously Figure 8) that only a fraction of OSN subtypes have a capacity to undergo accelerated neurogenesis in the presence of odors that activate them.

      - Also within the Results section, we have made efforts to better highlight relevant control experiments that were included in the original version, particularly those showing that quantities of newborn Olfr912 OSNs are not affected by UNO in mice exposed to male odors (Figure 2H and Figure 3–figure supplement 1G; previously Figure 2F and Figure 3H) or by exposure of non-occluded females to male odors (Figure 3H; previously Figure 6E). Since Olfr235 is responsive to component(s) of male odors (C. van der Linden et al., 2018; Vihani et al., 2020), these results indicate that this subtype does not have the capacity of stimulation-dependent neurogenesis, which is consistent with our previous findings that only a fraction of subtypes have this capacity (C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020).

      In this context, it is somewhat concerning that some results, which appear counterintuitive (e.g., lower representation and/or transcript levels of Olfr912 and Olfr1295 in mice exposed to male odors) are brushed off as "reflecting reduced survival due to overstimulation." The notion of "reduced survival" could be tested by, for example, a caspase3 assay.

      This is a point that we agree deserves further discussion. Please see the explanation that we have outlined above in response to Reviewer #1.

      Within the revised manuscript, we have expanded the Introduction to describe evidence from previous studies that exposure to stimulating odors causes two categories of changes to specific OSN subtypes: elevated representations or reduced representations within the OSN population. We outline evidence from previous studies that Olfr912 and Olfr1295 belong to the latter category, and that the representations of these subtypes are likely reduced by male odor overstimulation-dependent shortening of OSN lifespan.

      Important analyses that need to be done to better be able to interpret the findings are to present (i) the OR+/EdU+ population of olfactory sensory neurons not just as a count per hemisection, but rather as the ratio of OR+/EdU+ cells among all EdU+ cells; and (ii) to the ratio of EdU+ cells among all nuclei (UNO versus open naris). This way, data would be normalized to (i) the overall rate of neurogenesis and (ii) any broad deprivation-dependent epithelial degeneration.

      We have addressed this concern in two ways within the revised manuscript:

      (1) We have noted within the Methods section that the approach of using half-sections for normalization has been used in multiple previous studies for quantifying newborn (OR+/EdU+) and total (OR+) OSN abundances (Hossain et al., 2023; Ibarra-Soria et al., 2017; C. van der Linden et al., 2018; C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020). Additionally, within the figure legends and Methods, we have more thoroughly described the approach used, including that it relies on averaging the quantifications from at least 5 high-quality coronal OE tissue sections that are evenly distributed throughout the anterior-posterior length of each OE and thereby mitigates the effects of section size and cell number variation among sections. In the case of UNO treated mice, the open and closed sides within the same section are paired, which further reduces the effects of section-to section variation. We have found that this approach yields reproducible quantities of newborn and total OSNs among biological replicate mice and enables accurate assessment of how quantities of OSNs of specific subtypes change as a result of altered olfactory experience, a key objective of this study.

      (2) To assess whether the use of alternative approaches for normalizing newborn OSN quantities suggested by the reviewers would affect the present study’s findings, we compared three methods for normalizing the effects of exposure to male odors or muscone on quantities of newborn Olfr235 OSNs in the OEs of both UNO-treated and non-occluded mice: 1) OR+/EdU+ OSNs per half-section (used in this study), 2) OR+/EdU+ OSNs per total number of EdU+ cells (reviewer suggestion (i)), and 3) OR+/EdU+ OSNs per unit of DAPI+ area (an approximate measure of nuclei number; reviewer suggestion (ii)). The three normalization methods yielded statistically indistinguishable differences in assessing the effects of exposure of either UNO-treated or non-occluded mice to male odors (newly added Figure 2–figure supplement 2 and Figure 3–figure supplement 2), or of exposure of non-occluded mice to muscone (newly added Figure 4–figure supplement 3). Based on these findings, and the considerable time that would be required to renormalize all data in the manuscript, we have chosen to maintain the use of normalization per half-section.

      Finally, the paper will benefit from improved data presentation and adequate statistical testing. Images in Figures 2 - 7, showing both EdU labeling of newborn neurons and OR-specific RNA-FISH, are hard to interpret. Moreover, t-tests should not be employed when data is not normally distributed (as is the case for most of their samples).

      We have made extensive changes within the revised manuscript to increase the clarity and interpretability of the figures, including:

      (1) Addition of a split-channel, high-magnification view of a representative image that shows the overlap of FISH and EdU signals (Figure 2D).

      (2) Addition of experimental schematics and timelines corresponding to each set of experiments.

      In the revised manuscript, several changes to the statistical tests have been made, as follows:

      (1) To assess deviation from normality of the histological quantifications of newborn and total OSNs of specific subtypes in this study, all datasets were tested using the Shapiro-Wilk test for non-normality and the P values obtained are included in Supplementary file 1 (figure source data). Of the 274 datasets tested, 253 were found to have Shapiro-Wilk P values > 0.05, indicating that the vast majority (92%) do not show evidence of significant deviation from a normal distribution.

      (2) A general lack of deviation of the datasets in this study from a normal distribution is further supported by quantile-quantile (QQ) plots, which compare actual data to a theoretically normal distribution (Appendix 4–figure 1). The datasets analyzed were separated into the following categories:

      a. Quantities of newborn OSNs in UNO treated mice (Appendix 4-figure 1A)

      b. Quantities of total OSNs in UNO treated mice (Appendix 4-figure 1B)

      c. Quantities of newborn OSNs in non-occluded mice (Appendix 4-figure 1C)

      d. UNO effect sizes for newborn or total OSNs (Appendix 4-figure 1D)

      (3) Results of both parametric and non-parametric statistical tests of comparisons in this study have been included in Supplementary file 2 (statistical analyses). In general, the results from parametric and non-parametric tests are in good agreement.

      (4) Statistical analyses of differences in OSN quantities in the OEs of non-occluded mice or UNO effect sizes in UNO-treated mice subjected more than two different experimental conditions have now been performed using one-way ANOVA tests, FDR-adjusted using the 2-stage linear step-up procedure of Benjamini, Krieger and Yekutieli.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the Authors):

      The manuscript by Hossain et al. would benefit from a thorough revision. Here, we outline several points that should be addressed:

      Figure 3E - I & Figure 4E&F: Red lines that connect mean values are misleading.

      Within the revised manuscript, the UNO effect size graphs have been modified for clarity, including removal of the lines between mean values except for those comparing changes over time post EdU injection (Figure 6 and Figure 6-figure supplement 1). For these latter graphs, we think that lines help to illustrate changes in effect sizes over time.

      Figure 3E - I: UNO effect sizes (right) should be tested via ANOVA.

      In the revised manuscript, statistical analyses of UNO effect sizes in UNO-treated mice subjected more than two different experimental conditions were done using one-way ANOVA tests, FDR-adjusted using the 2-stage linear step-up procedure of Benjamini, Krieger and Yekutieli (Figure 2-figure supplement 2; Figure 3; Figure 3-figure supplement 1; Figure 4; Figure 4-figure supplements 1, 2). The same tests were used for analysis of differences in OSN quantities in the OEs of non-occluded mice subjected more than two different experimental conditions (Figure 3; Figure 3-figure supplement 2; Figure 4; Figure 4-figure supplements 3, 4). For comparisons of differences in quantities of newborn OSNs of musk-responsive subtypes at 4 and 7 days post-EdU between non-occluded mice exposed and unexposed to muscone, a two sample ANOVA - fixed-test, using F distribution (right-tailed) was used (Figure 6; Figure 6-figure supplement 1).

      Images in Figures 2 - 7, showing both EdU labeling of newborn neurons and OR-specific RNA-FISH: Colabeling is hard / often impossible to discern. Show zoom-ins and better explain the criteria for "colabeling" in the methods.

      In the revised manuscript an enlarged and split-channel view of an image showing multiple newborn Olfr235 OSNs (OR+/EdU+) has been added (Figure 2D). A detailed description of the criteria for OR+/EdU+ OSNs is provided in Methods under the section “Histological quantification of newborn and total OSNs of specific subtypes.”

      Figure 1C: add Olfr912.

      As a control group for iOSN quantities of musk-responsive subtypes in Figure 1, we selected random subtypes that are expressed in the same zones: 2 and 3. Olfr912 OSNs were not included because this subtype was not randomly chosen, nor is it expressed the same zones (Olfr912 is expressed in zone 4). We also note that the scRNA-seq analysis was done to allow an initial exploration of the hypothesis that some OSN subtypes with that are more highly represented in mice exposed to male odors show stimulation-dependent neurogenesis. Considering that the scRNA-seq datasets contain only small numbers of iOSNs of specific subtypes, we think they are more useful for analyzing changes in birthrates within groups of subtypes (e.g., musk responsive, random) rather than individual subtypes.

      The time of OE dissection is different for data shown in Figure 1 (P28) as compared to other figures (P35). Please comment/discuss.

      Within the Results section of the revised manuscript, we have now clarified that the PD 28 timepoint chosen for EdU birthdating in the histological quantification of newborn OSNs of specific subtypes is analogous to the PD 28 timepoint chosen for identification of immature (Gap43-expressing) OSNs in the scRNA-seq samples. In the case of EdU birthdating, it is necessary to provide a chase period of sufficient length to enable robust and stable expression of an OR, which defines the subtype. A chase period of 7 days was chosen based on a previous study (C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020). Hence, a dissection date of PD 35 was chosen.

      Figure 3F&G: please discuss the female à female effects

      In the Results and Discussion sections of the revised manuscript, we discuss our observation that the Olfr1440 and Olfr1431 subtypes show significantly higher quantities of newborn OSNs on the open side compared to closed sides in UNO-treated females. We speculate that these subtypes may receive some odor stimulation in juvenile females, perhaps via musk or related odors emitted by females themselves or from elsewhere within the environment.

      Figure 4E (and other examples): male à male displays two populations (no effect versus effect); please explain/speculate.

      For some UNO effect sizes, there appears to be high degree of variation among mice, and, in some cases, this diversity appears to cause the data to separate into groups. We assessed whether this diversity might reflect mice that came from different litters, but this is not the case. Rather, we speculate that the observed diversity most likely reflects low representations of newborn OSNs of some subtypes and/or under specific conditions. The data referred to by the reviewer (now Figure 3–figure supplement 3D), for example, shows UNO effect sizes for quantities of newborn Olfr1431 OSNs, which has the lowest representation among the musk-responsive subtypes analyzed in this study.

      Figure 5C-E: It is unclear why strong muscone concentrations (10%) have no effect, whereas no muscone sometimes (D&E) has an effect.

      As discussed in response to comments from Reviewer #1, we speculate that fluctuations in UNO effect sizes in muscone-exposed mice, particularly at high muscone concentrations, may be due, at least in part, to transnasal and/or retronasal air flow [reviewed in (Coppola, 2012)], which would be expected to result in exposure of the closed side of the OE to muscone concentrations that increase with increasing environmental concentrations. In support of this, quantities of newborn Olfr235 (Figure 4C-middle) and Olfr1440 (Figure 4–figure supplement 1A-middle) OSNs increase on both the open and closed sides with increasing muscone concentration (except at the highest concentration, 10%, in the case of Olfr1440). We speculate that reductions in newborn Olfr1440 OSN quantities observed in the presence of 10% muscone may reflect overstimulation-dependent reductions in survival.

      As emphasized above, our study also includes experiments on non-occluded animals (Figures 3, 4, 5). Findings from these experiments provide additional evidence that exposure to multiple musk odorants (muscone, ambretone) causes selective increases in the birthrates of multiple musk-responsive OSN subtypes (Olfr235, Olfr1440, Olfr1431).

      We have included an extensive interpretation of UNO-based experiments, including their limitations, within the Results section of the revised manuscript.

      Figure S1: please explain the large error bars regarding "Transcript level".

      We have clarified that the error bars in this figure, which is now Appendix 1–figure 1, correspond to 95% confidence intervals.

      The figure captions could be improved for ease of reading.

      Figure captions have been revised for increased clarity.

      Figure 4: Include Olfr235 data for consistency.

      All OSN subtypes analyzed for the effects of exposure to adult mice on UNO-induced open-side biases in quantities of newborn OSNs have been included in a single figure, which is now Figure 3–figure supplement 3.

      Figure S6F&G: Do not run statistics on n = 2 (G) or 3 (F) samples.

      We have removed statistical test results from comparisons involving fewer than 4 observations.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Neurogenesis in the mammalian olfactory epithelium persists throughout the life of the animal. The process replaces damaged or dying olfactory sensory neurons. It has been tacitly that replacement of the OR subtypes is stochastic, although anecdotal evidence has suggested that this may not be the case. In this study, Santoro and colleagues systematically test this hypothesis by answering three questions: is there enrichment of specific OR subtypes associated with neurogenesis? Is the enrichment dependent on sensory stimulus? Is the enrichment the result of differential generation of the OR type or from differential cell death regulated by neural activity? The authors provide some solid evidence indicating that musk odor stimulus selectively promotes the OR types expressing the musk receptors. The evidence argues against a random selection of ORs in the regenerating neurons.

      Strengths:

      The strength of the study is a thorough and systematic investigation of the expression of multiple musk receptors with unilateral naris occlusion or under different stimulus conditions. The controls are properly performed. This study is the first to formulate the selective promotion hypothesis and the first systematic investigation to test it. The bulk of the study uses in situ hybridization and immunofluorescent staining to estimate the number of OR types. These results convincingly demonstrate the increased expression of musk receptors in response to male odor or muscone stimulation.

      Weaknesses:

      A major weakness of the current study is the single-cell RNASeq result. The authors use this piece of data as a broad survey of receptor expression in response to unilateral nasal occlusion. However, several issues with this data raise serious concerns about the quality of the experiment and the conclusions. First, the proportion of OSNs, including both the immature and mature types, constitutes only a small fraction of the total cells. In previous studies of the OSNs using the scRNASeq approach, OSNs constitute the largest cell population. It is curious why this is the case. Second, the authors did not annotate the cell types, making it difficult to assess the potential cause of this discrepancy. Third, given the small number of OSNs, it is surprising to have multiple musk receptors detected in the open side of the olfactory epithelium whereas almost none in the closed side. Since each OR type only constitutes ~0.1% of OSNs on average, the number of detected musk receptors is too high to be consistent with our current understanding and the rest of the data in the manuscript. Finally, unlike the other experiments, the authors did not describe any method details, nor was there any description of quality controls associated with the experiment. The concerns over the scRNASeq data do not diminish the value of the data presented in the bulk of the study but could be used for further analysis.

      We are grateful to the reviewer for raising these important questions.

      In the revised manuscript, we have clarified that the scRNA-seq dataset presented in the original version of the manuscript (now called dataset OE 1) was published and described in detail in a previous study (C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020). The reviewer is correct that the proportion of OSNs within that dataset was lower in that dataset than in other datasets that have been published more recently (using updated methods). We think this is likely because of the way that the cells were processed (e.g., from cryopreserved single cells followed by live/dead selection). However, because the open and closed sides were processed identically, we do not expect the ratios of OSNs of specific subtypes to be greatly affected. Hence, the differences observed for specific OSN subtypes on the open versus closed sides are expected to be valid.

      As the reviewer notes, there is a surprisingly large difference between the number of OSNs of musk-responsive subtypes on the open and closed sides within the OE 1 dataset. This difference is a key piece of information that led us to formulate the hypothesis in the study: that musk responsive subtypes are born at a higher rate in the presence of male/musk odor stimulation. And while it is true that, on average, each subtype represents ~0.1% of the population, it is known that there is wide variance in representations among different subtypes [e.g., (Ibarra-Soria et al., 2017)]. The frequencies of the musk responsive subtypes among all OSNs on the open side of OE 1 (0.3% for Olfr235, 0.4% for olfr1440, 0.06% for Olfr1434, 0% for olfr1431, and 1% for Olfr1437) are in line with previous findings.

      To confirm that the scRNA-seq findings from dataset OE 1 are not an artifact of the cell preparation methods used, we generated a second scRNA-seq dataset, OE 2, which has been added to the revised manuscript (Figure 1). The OE 2 dataset was prepared according to the same experimental timeline as OE 1, but the cells were captured immediately after dissociation and live/dead sorting via FACS. As expected, most cells within OE 2 dataset are OSNs (77% on the open side, 66% on the closed). Importantly, like the OE 1 dataset, the OE 2 dataset shows higher quantities of iOSNs of musk responsive subtypes on the open side of the OE compared to the closed (normalized for either total cells or total OSNs) (Figure 1–figure supplement 1D, E).

      A weakness of the experiment assessing musk receptor expression is that the authors do not distinguish immature from mature OSNs. Immature OSNs express multiple receptor types before they commit to the expression of a single type. The experiments do not reveal whether mature OSNs maintain an elevated expression level of musk receptors.

      While it is established that multiple ORs are coexpressed at a low level during OSN differentiation (Bashkirova et al., 2023; Fletcher et al., 2017; Hanchate et al., 2015; Pourmorady et al., 2024; Saraiva et al., 2015; Scholz et al., 2016; Tan et al., 2015), this has been found to occur primarily at the immediate neuronal precursor 3 (INP3) stage (Bashkirova et al., 2023; Fletcher et al., 2017), which is characterized by expression of Tex15 (Fletcher et al., 2017; Pourmorady et al., 2024) and precedes the immature OSN (iOSN) stage, which is characterized by expression of Gap43 (Fletcher et al., 2017; McIntyre et al., 2010; Verhaagen et al., 1989). Within the scRNA-seq datasets in the present study, iOSNs of specific subtypes are identified based on robust expression of Gap43 (Log<sup>2</sup> UMI > 1) and a specific OR gene (Log<sup>2</sup> UMI > 2), as described in the figures and methods. Thus, the cells defined as iOSNs are expected to express a single OR gene and this expression should be maintained as iOSNs transition to mOSNs. To confirm these predictions, we carried out a detailed analysis of OR expression at three different stages of OSN differentiation: INP3, iOSN, and mOSN (Figure 1–figure supplement 2). The cells chosen for analysis express the musk-responsive ORs Olfr235 or Olfr1440 or a randomly chosen OR Olfr701, in addition to markers that define INP3, iOSN, or mOSN cells. As expected, individual iOSNs and mOSNs of musk-responsive subtypes were found to exhibit robust and singular OR expression on the open and closed sides of OEs from UNO-treated mice. Moreover, and as observed previously, INP3 cells coexpress multiple OR transcripts at low levels. A detailed description of how the analysis was performed is included in the Methods section under Quantification and statistical analysis.

      Within the histology-based quantifications, newborn OSNs are identified based on their robust RNA-FISH signals corresponding to a specific OR transcript and an EdU label. Considering the EdU chase time of 7 days, most EdU-positive cells are expected to have passed the INP3 stage and be iOSNs or mOSNs. Moreover, considering the low level of OR expression within INP3 cells, it is unlikely OR transcripts are expressed at a high enough level to be detectable and/or counted at this stage and thereby affect newborn OSN quantifications.

      There are also two conceptual issues that are of concern. The first is the concept of selective neurogenesis. The data show an increased expression of musk receptors in response to male odor stimulation. The authors argue that this indicates selective neurogenesis of the musk receptor types. However, it is not clear what the distinction is between elevated receptor expression and a commitment to a specific fate at an early stage of development. As immature OSNs express multiple receptors, a likely scenario is that some newly differentiated immature OSNs have elevated expression of not only the musk receptors but also other receptors. The current experiments do not distinguish the two alternatives. Moreover, as pointed out above, it is not clear whether mature OSNs maintain the increased expression. Although a scRNASeq experiment can clarify it, the authors, unfortunately, did not perform an in-depth analysis to determine at which point of neurogenesis the cells commit to a specific musk receptor type. The quality of the scRNASeq data unfortunately also does not lend confidence for this type of analysis.

      The addition of a second scRNA-seq dataset within the revised manuscript (Figure 1), combined with the new scRNA-seq-based analyses of OR expression in INP3, iOSN, and mOSN cells (Figure 1-figure supplement 2), provide strong evidence that iOSNs and mOSNs robustly express a single OR gene and that cellular expression is stable from the iOSN to the mOSN stage. These analyses do not support a scenario in which odor stimulation causes upregulated expression of multiple ORs and thereby causes apparent increases in quantities of newly generated OSNs that express musk-responsive ORs. Rather, the data firmly support a mechanism in which odor stimulation increases quantities of newly generated OSNs that have stably committed to the robust expression of a single musk-responsive OR.

      A second conceptual issue, the idea of homeostasis in regeneration, which the authors presented in the Introduction, needs clarification. In its current form, it is confusing. It could mean that a maintenance of the distribution of receptor types, or it could mean the proper replacement of a specific OR type upon the loss of this type. The authors seem to refer to the latter and should define it properly.

      We have revised the Introduction section to clarify our use of the term homeostatic in one instance (paragraph 4) and replace it with more specific language in a second instance (paragraph 5).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Concerns over scRNASeq data. It appears that the samples may have included non-OE tissues, which reduced the representation of the OSNs. This experiment may need to be repeated to increase the number of OSNs.

      As outlined in the response to the public comments, we think that the low proportion of OSNs in the OE 1 data set reflects how the cells were prepared and processed. We have now included a second scRNA-seq dataset to address this concern.

      Cell types should be identified in the scRNASeq analysis, and the number of cells documented for each cell type, at least for the OSNs. The data should be made available for general access.

      We have now clarified that the OE 1 dataset was published as part of a previous study (C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020) and was made publicly available as part of that study (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE157119). All cell types in the newly generated OE 2 dataset have been annotated (Figure 1) and this dataset has also been made publicly available (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE278693). The numbers and percentages of OSNs within OE 1 and OE 2 datasets have been added to the legend of Figure 1-figure supplement 1.

      The specific OR types should be segregated for mature and immature OSNs. The percentage of a specific OR type should be normalized to the total number of OSNs, rather than the total cells. The current quantification is misleading because it gives the false sense that the muscone receptors represent ~0.1% of cells when the proportion is much higher if only OSNs are considered.

      In the revised manuscript, quantities of iOSNs (Gap43+ cells) of specific subtypes within the OE 1 and OE 2 scRNA-seq datasets are graphed as percentages of both all OSNs (Figure 1E, Figure 1–figure supplement 1D) and all cells (Figure 1–figure supplement 1E). As a percentage of all OSNs, average quantities of iOSNs of musk responsive subtypes on the open side of the OE range from 0.005% (for Olfr1431) to 0.14% (for Olfr1440) (Figure 1E).

      Within the feature plots for the two datasets, the differentiation stages of indicated OSNs have been clearly defined within the figures and figure legends. For the OE 1 dataset, iOSNs are differentiated from mOSNs by arrows (Figure 1–figure supplement 1C). For the OE 2 dataset (Figure 1D), only immature OSNs are shown for simplicity.

      Technical details of the scRNASeq should be documented. In the feature plot of musk-response receptors (Figure. 1D), it is better to use the actual quantity of expression rather than binarized representation (with or without an OR). If one needs to use on/off to determine the number of cells for a given OR type, then the criteria of selection should be given.

      Technical details of generation of the scRNA-seq datasets have been documented in the “Method details” section (for the OE 2 dataset) and in the method section of our previous publication of the OE 1 dataset (C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020). Details of the scRNA-seq analyses, including the criteria used to define immature OSNs of specific subtypes, are documented within the “Quantification and statistical analysis” section.

      Within the feature plots, we have decided to show OSNs of a given subtype in a binary fashion using specific colors for the sake of simplicity (Figure 1D, Figure 1-figure supplement 1C). To address the reviewer’s cooncern, we have added a new figure that provides detailed information about OR transcript expression (levels and genes) within iOSNs and mOSNs of two different musk responsive subtypes and a randomly chosen subtype (Figure 1-figure supplement 2).

      An in-depth analysis of the onset of OR expression in the GBC, INP, immature, and mature OSNs should be performed. It is also important to determine how many other receptors are detected in the cells that express the musk receptors. The current scRNASeq data may not be of sufficiently high quality and the experiment needs to be repeated. It is also important for the authors to take measures to eliminate ambient RNA contamination.

      The revised manuscript includes a second scRNA-seq dataset (OE 2; Figure 1). Details of how both the original (OE 1) and new datasets were generated have been documented within the Methods sections of the corresponding publications [(C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020); present study]. For both datasets, live/dead selection of cells was performed, which was expected to reduce ambient RNA.

      The revised manuscript also includes a new figure that provides detailed information about OR transcript expression within INP3, iOSN and mOSN cells that express one of two different musk responsive ORs or a randomly chosen OR (Figure 1-figure supplement 2). These data reveal, as reported previously (Bashkirova et al., 2023; Fletcher et al., 2017; Pourmorady et al., 2024), that low levels of multiple OR transcripts are detected in INP3 (Tex15+) cells. By contrast, iOSN (Gap43+) and mOSN (Omp+) cells robustly express a single OR, with little or no expression of other ORs.

      Quantification of cells for Figure 2-7 should be changed. Instead of using cell number per 1/2 section, the data should be calculated using density (using the area of the epithelium or normalized to the total number of cells (based on DAPI staining). This is because multiple sections are taken from the same mouse along the A-P axis. These sections have different sizes and numbers of cells.

      As noted in response to a similar concern of Reviewer #2, this has been addressed in two ways within the revised manuscript:

      (1) We have noted within the Methods section that the approach of using half-sections for normalization has been used in multiple previous studies for quantifying newborn (OR+/EdU+) and total (OR+) OSN abundances (Hossain et al., 2023; Ibarra-Soria et al., 2017; C. van der Linden et al., 2018; C. J. van der Linden et al., 2020). Additionally, within the figure legends and Methods, we have more thoroughly described the approach used, including that it relies on averaging the quantifications from at least 5 high-quality coronal OE tissue sections that are evenly distributed throughout the anterior-posterior length of each OE and thereby mitigates the effects of section size and cell number variation among sections. In the case of UNO treated mice, the open and closed sides within the same section are paired, which further reduces the effects of section-to section variation. We have found that this approach yields reproducible quantities of newborn and total OSNs among biological replicate mice and enables accurate assessment of how quantities of OSNs of specific subtypes change as a result of altered olfactory experience, a key objective of this study.

      (2) To assess whether the use of alternative approaches for normalizing newborn OSN quantities suggested by the reviewers would affect the present study’s findings, we compared three methods for normalizing the effects of exposure to male odors or muscone on quantities of newborn Olfr235 OSNs in the OEs of both UNO-treated and non-occluded mice: 1) OR+/EdU+ OSNs per half-section (used in this study), 2) OR+/EdU+ OSNs per total number of EdU+ cells (reviewer suggestion (i)), and 3) OR+/EdU+ OSNs per unit of DAPI+ area (an approximate measure of nuclei number; reviewer suggestion (ii)). The three normalization methods yielded statistically indistinguishable differences in assessing the effects of exposure of either UNO-treated or non-occluded mice to male odors (newly added Figure 2–figure supplement 2 and Figure 3–figure supplement 2), or of exposure of non-occluded mice to muscone (newly added Figure 4–figure supplement 3). Based on these findings, and the considerable time that would be required to renormalize all data in the manuscript, we have chosen to maintain the use of normalization per half-section.

      References

      Bashkirova, E. V., Klimpert, N., Monahan, K., Campbell, C. E., Osinski, J., Tan, L., Schieren, I., Pourmorady, A., Stecky, B., Barnea, G., Xie, X. S., Abdus-Saboor, I., Shykind, B. M., Marlin, B. J., Gronostajski, R. M., Fleischmann, A., & Lomvardas, S. (2023). Opposing, spatially-determined epigenetic forces impose restrictions on stochastic olfactory receptor choice. eLife, 12, RP87445. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.87445

      Coppola, D. M. (2012). Studies of olfactory system neural plasticity: The contribution of the unilateral naris occlusion technique. Neural Plasticity, 2012, 351752. https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/351752

      Fletcher, R. B., Das, D., Gadye, L., Street, K. N., Baudhuin, A., Wagner, A., Cole, M. B., Flores, Q., Choi, Y. G., Yosef, N., Purdom, E., Dudoit, S., Risso, D., & Ngai, J. (2017). Deconstructing Olfactory Stem Cell Trajectories at Single-Cell Resolution. Cell Stem Cell, 20(6), 817-830.e8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.stem.2017.04.003

      Han, X., Jiang, Y., Feng, N., Yang, P., Zhang, M., Jin, W., Zhang, T., Huang, Z., Zhao, H., Zhang, K., Liu, S., & Hu, D. (2022). Comparison of the Homology Between Muskrat Scented Gland and Mouse Preputial Gland. Journal of Mammalian Evolution, 29(2), 435–446. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10914-022-09604-w

      Hanchate, N. K., Kondoh, K., Lu, Z., Kuang, D., Ye, X., Qiu, X., Pachter, L., Trapnell, C., & Buck, L. B. (2015). Single-cell transcriptomics reveals receptor transformations during olfactory neurogenesis. Science (New York, N.Y.), 350(6265), 1251–1255. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aad2456

      Hossain, K., Smith, M., & Santoro, S. W. (2023). A histological protocol for quantifying the birthrates of specific subtypes of olfactory sensory neurons in mice. STAR Protocols, 4(3), 102432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.xpro.2023.102432

      Ibarra-Soria, X., Nakahara, T. S., Lilue, J., Jiang, Y., Trimmer, C., Souza, M. A., Netto, P. H., Ikegami, K., Murphy, N. R., Kusma, M., Kirton, A., Saraiva, L. R., Keane, T. M., Matsunami, H., Mainland, J., Papes, F., & Logan, D. W. (2017). Variation in olfactory neuron repertoires is genetically controlled and environmentally modulated. eLife, 6. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.21476

      Kelemen, G. (1947). The junction of the nasal cavity and the pharyngeal tube in the rat. Archives of Otolaryngology, 45(2), 159–168. https://doi.org/10.1001/archotol.1947.00690010168002

      Lin, D. Y., Zhang, S.-Z., Block, E., & Katz, L. C. (2005). Encoding social signals in the mouse main olfactory bulb. Nature, 434(7032), 470–477. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature03414

      McIntyre, J. C., Titlow, W. B., & McClintock, T. S. (2010). Axon growth and guidance genes identify nascent, immature, and mature olfactory sensory neurons. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 88(15), 3243–3256. https://doi.org/10.1002/jnr.22497

      Pourmorady, A. D., Bashkirova, E. V., Chiariello, A. M., Belagzhal, H., Kodra, A., Duffié, R., Kahiapo, J., Monahan, K., Pulupa, J., Schieren, I., Osterhoudt, A., Dekker, J., Nicodemi, M., & Lomvardas, S. (2024). RNA-mediated symmetry breaking enables singular olfactory receptor choice. Nature, 625(7993), 181–188. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-023-06845-4

      Saraiva, L. R., Ibarra-Soria, X., Khan, M., Omura, M., Scialdone, A., Mombaerts, P., Marioni, J. C., & Logan, D. W. (2015). Hierarchical deconstruction of mouse olfactory sensory neurons: From whole mucosa to single-cell RNA-seq. Scientific Reports, 5, 18178. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep18178

      Sato-Akuhara, N., Horio, N., Kato-Namba, A., Yoshikawa, K., Niimura, Y., Ihara, S., Shirasu, M., & Touhara, K. (2016). Ligand Specificity and Evolution of Mammalian Musk Odor Receptors: Effect of Single Receptor Deletion on Odor Detection. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 36(16), 4482–4491. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3259-15.2016

      Scholz, P., Kalbe, B., Jansen, F., Altmueller, J., Becker, C., Mohrhardt, J., Schreiner, B., Gisselmann, G., Hatt, H., & Osterloh, S. (2016). Transcriptome Analysis of Murine Olfactory Sensory Neurons during Development Using Single Cell RNA-Seq. Chemical Senses, 41(4), 313–323. https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjw003

      Schwende, F. J., Wiesler, D., Jorgenson, J. W., Carmack, M., & Novotny, M. (1986). Urinary volatile constituents of the house mouse,Mus musculus, and their endocrine dependency. Journal of Chemical Ecology, 12(1), 277–296. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01045611

      Shirasu, M., Yoshikawa, K., Takai, Y., Nakashima, A., Takeuchi, H., Sakano, H., & Touhara, K. (2014). Olfactory receptor and neural pathway responsible for highly selective sensing of musk odors. Neuron, 81(1), 165–178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.021

      Tan, L., Li, Q., & Xie, X. S. (2015). Olfactory sensory neurons transiently express multiple olfactory receptors during development. Molecular Systems Biology, 11(12), 844. https://doi.org/10.15252/msb.20156639

      van der Linden, C. J., Gupta, P., Bhuiya, A. I., Riddick, K. R., Hossain, K., & Santoro, S. W. (2020). Olfactory Stimulation Regulates the Birth of Neurons That Express Specific Odorant Receptors. Cell Reports, 33(1), 108210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108210

      van der Linden, C., Jakob, S., Gupta, P., Dulac, C., & Santoro, S. W. (2018). Sex separation induces differences in the olfactory sensory receptor repertoires of male and female mice. Nature Communications, 9(1), 5081. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-07120-1

      Verhaagen, J., Oestreicher, A. B., Gispen, W. H., & Margolis, F. L. (1989). The expression of the growth associated protein B50/GAP43 in the olfactory system of neonatal and adult rats. The Journal of Neuroscience: The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 9(2), 683–691.

      Vihani, A., Hu, X. S., Gundala, S., Koyama, S., Block, E., & Matsunami, H. (2020). Semiochemical responsive olfactory sensory neurons are sexually dimorphic and plastic. eLife, 9, e54501. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54501

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Le et al.. aimed to explore whether AAV-mediated overexpression of Oct4 could induce neurogenic competence in adult murine Müller glia, a cell type that, unlike its counterparts in cold-blooded vertebrates, lacks regenerative potential in mammals. The primary goal was to determine whether Oct4 alone, or in combination with Notch signaling inhibition, could drive Müller glia to transdifferentiate into bipolar neurons, offering a potential strategy for retinal regeneration.

      The authors demonstrated that Oct4 overexpression alone resulted in the conversion of 5.1% of Müller glia into Otx2+ bipolar-like neurons by five weeks post-injury, compared to 1.1% at two weeks. To further enhance the efficiency of this conversion, they investigated the synergistic effect of Notch signaling inhibition by genetically disrupting Rbpj, a key Notch effector. Under these conditions, the percentage of Müller gliaderived bipolar cells increased significantly to 24.3%, compared to 4.5% in Rbpjdeficient controls without Oct4 overexpression. Similarly, in Notch1/2 double-knockout Müller glia, Oct4 overexpression increased the proportion of GFP+ bipolar cells from 6.6% to 15.8%.

      To elucidate the molecular mechanisms driving this reprogramming, the authors performed single-cell RNA sequencing (scRNA-seq) and ATAC-seq, revealing that Oct4 overexpression significantly altered gene regulatory networks. They identified Rfx4, Sox2, and Klf4 as potential mediators of Oct4-induced neurogenic competence, suggesting that Oct4 cooperates with endogenously expressed neurogenic factors to reshape Müller glia identity.

      Overall, this study aimed to establish Oct4 overexpression as a novel and efficient strategy to reprogram mammalian Müller glia into retinal neurons, demonstrating both its independent and synergistic effects with Notch pathway inhibition. The findings have important implications for regenerative therapies as they suggest that manipulating pluripotency factors in vivo could unlock the neurogenic potential of Müller glia for treating retinal degenerative diseases.

      Strengths:

      (1) Novelty: The study provides compelling evidence that Oct4 overexpression alone can induce Müller glia-to-bipolar neuron conversion, challenging the conventional view that mammalian Müller glia lacks neurogenic potential.

      (2) Technological Advances: The combination of Muller glia-specific labeling and modifying mouse line, AAV-GFAP promoter-mediated gene expression, single-cell RNA-seq, and ATAC-seq provides a comprehensive mechanistic dissection of glial reprogramming.

      (3) Synergistic Effects: The finding that Oct4 overexpression enhances neurogenesis in the absence of Notch signaling introduces a new avenue for retinal repair strategies.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) In this study, the authors did not perform a comprehensive functional assessment of the bipolar cells derived from Müller glia to confirm their neuronal identity and functionality.

      (2) Demonstrating visual recovery in a bipolar cell-deficiency disease model would significantly enhance the translational impact of this work and further validate its therapeutic potential.

      Response: We thank the Reviewer for their evaluation. We agree that functional analysis of Müller glia-derived bipolar cells is indeed important, but is beyond the current scope of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors harness single-cell RNAseq data from zebrafish and mice to identify Oct4 as a candidate driver of neurogenesis. They then use adeno-associated virus vectors to show that while Oct4 overexpression alone converts rare adult Müller glia (MG) to bipolar cells, it synergizes with Notch pathway inhibition to cause this neurogenesis (achieved by Cre-mediated knockout of Rbpj floxed allele). Importantly, they genetically lineage-mark adult MG using a GLAST-CreER transgene and a Sun-GFP reporter, so that any non-MG cells that convert can be identified unambiguously. This is crucial because several high-profile papers made erroneous claims using short promoters in the viral delivery vector itself to mark MG, but those promoters are leaky and mark other non-MG cell types, making it impossible to definitively state whether manipulations studied were actually causing neurogenesis, or were merely the result of expression in pre-existing neurons. Once the authors establish Oct4 + RbpjKO synergy they use snRNAseq/ATACseq to identify known and novel transcription factors that could play a role in driving neurogenesis.

      Strengths:

      The system to mark MG is stringent, so the authors are studying transdifferentiation, not artifactual effects due to leaky viral promoters. The synergy between Oct4 and Notch pathway blockade is notable. The single-cell results add the potential involvement of new players such as Rfx4 in adult-MG-neurogenesis.

      Weaknesses:

      The existing version is difficult to read due to an unusually high number of text errors (e.g. references to the wrong figure panels etc.). A fuller explanation for the fraction of non-MG cells seen in control scRNAseq assays is required, particularly because the neurogenic trajectory which is enhanced in the Oct4/Rbpj-KO context is also evident in the control retina. Claims regarding the involvement of transcription factors in adult neurogenesis (such as Rfx4) need to be toned down unless they are backed up with functional data. It is possible that such factors are important, but equally, they may have no role or a redundant role, and without functional tests, it's impossible to say one way or the other.

      Overall, the authors achieved what they set out to do, and have made new insights into how neurogenesis can be stimulated in MG. Ultimately, a major long-term goal in the field is to replace lost photoreceptors as this is most relevant to many human visual disorders, and while this paper (like all others before it) does not generate rods or cones, it opens new strategies to coax MG to form a related neuronal cell type. Their approach underscores the benefits of using a gold-standard approach for lineage tracing.

      We thank the Reviewer for their evaluation. We have made extensive changes to the manuscript to correct errors and modify discussion as recommended. These are detailed below in our point-by-point responses to specific recommendations to the authors.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor corrections:

      (1) In Figure 1C top GFAP-mCherry panel, two dim GFP + cells have colocalized with Otx2, is it caused by optic imaging thickness or some muller glia cells having the Otx2 expression?

      This indeed reflects the effects of optic imaging thickness. Colocalization of Sun1-GFP and Otx2 is not observed when Z-stack images are examined in GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP retinas. This can also be appreciated by the fact that, in cases of apparent overlap of nuclear envelope-targeted Sun1 and Otx2, the sizes of the labeled areas differ. In cases of true expression overlap, such as is seen following Oct4 overexpression, the labeled areas are the same size, or very nearly so.

      Whether the Glast-CreERT2 x Rosa26-LSL-Sun1-GFP mouse line has cross-labeling with the Otx2+ bipolar cells, the author should image the mCherry ctrl sample with a thin optical imaging layer with a small pinhole for Z-stack to verify the co-labeling the GFP and Otx2 in mCherry ctrl sample.

      Please see above. Since we first described this line (de Melo, et al. 2012), we have examined thousands of sections of GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP retinas, and have yet to see a single GFP-positive neuron. To avoid confusion, however, we have replaced these images with an additional image from a control mCherry-infected GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP retina processed for the same study.

      In the middle upper panel, Oct4-mCherry group, the white arrows indicate the GFP colocalized with Otx2 signal, but seems not mCherry positive, by contrast, the neighbor cells have significant mCherry expression but no colocalization with Otx2. The GFAP promoter-Oct4-mCherry may have stopped expression after the Müller Glia cells were converted into Otx2+ bipolar cells, but is there any middle stage in which the Oct4mCherry and Otx2 co-expression? And after Müller glia to Bipolar conversion, why have Glast-CreERT2 driven GFP expressions not suppressed as GFAP promoter driven Oct4-mCherry? Could the author discuss this point?

      We observed a significant number of Muller glia-derived cells expressing both Otx2 and weak mCherry signal. GFP expression is driven by the ubiquitous CAG promoter following Cre-dependent excision of a transcriptional stop cassette. We have modified the text to make this point explicit.

      (2) In Figure S2b, the mouse is labeled with wild type; I assume it should be the same mouse line as Fig.1. Otherwise, the author should describe the source of the GFP signal.

      “Wildtype” in this case refers to GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP controls, which as the Reviewer correctly points out, are not truly wildtype. The genotype of these animals is specified in all figure legends, and is now referred to as “control” rather than “wildtype” in the figures and main text throughout.

      In Figure S2k and l, mCherry ctrl panel, the GFP+ cells looked co-labeling with Otx2, so again, is it the thicker optical imaging layer that caused overlapping vertically or the low specific of Müller Glia of the mouse line? Please describe the stars' meaning in Figure S2i,j in the figure legend. There are 2 figures labeled "n" of the quantification data.

      This is, again, an example of the thicker optical imaging layer causing apparent overlap. We have previously demonstrated that the Sun1-GFP+ cells do not co-label with Otx2 in GFAP-mCherry AAV-injected control retinas (Le et al., 2022; Fig. 2C). The asterisks (*) indicate mouse-on-mouse vascular staining, which is now clarified in the figure legend. The 2 figures labeled ‘n’ have been relabeled as ‘m’ and ‘n’.

      (3) In Figure 2c in the top panel, the Otx2 image was wrong; please replace it with the correct one.

      We thank the Reviewer for spotting this error. This is an inadvertent duplication of the single-channel Otx2 staining for mCherry control sample. We have replaced this with the correct image.

      (4) In Figure 3a, the Rbpj-cKO mouse line was used, but where was the GFP signal from? Please verify the mouse line you used in your work. The same question is also asked in Figure S3, S4b.

      GlastCreER;Rpbj<sup>lox/lox</sup>;Sun1-GFP were used in Figure 3a. As now specified in the Methods and all figure legends, all mice used in this study carry both the GlastCreER and Sun1-GFP transgenes.

      (5) In Figure S4c,d, and 5 wks time point, if the authors quantify the GFP+/Sox2- cells changing, it will be more helpful to understand the percentage of the Müller glia cells conversion to Bipolar cells compared to the Figure 2D, and can be as a supplement to the conclusion Müller to Bipolar conversion rather the Müller proliferation.

      Sox2-/GFP+ cells are a measure of Müller glia to bipolar cell conversion that complements that of GFP+/Otx2+ cells. This is now clarified in the text. We also include quantification of Sox2-/GFP+ neurons at 5 weeks post-injury in Fig. S5b.

      (6) In Figure S1b,c, there is a large portion of cells that are activated Müller glia after NMDA injury. Did the activated Müller glial cells lose their Müller glial identity? Between the loss of Müller glial identity and neuronal reprogramming, are there any markers that can be used to assess whether Müller glial cells are truly transdifferentiating into neurons rather than remaining in a reactive glial state or an intermediate phase?

      Wildtype Müller glia progressively revert to resting state, and by 72 hours post-injury have already lost expression of Klf4 and Myc (Hoang, et al. 2020), a point which is now specifically mentioned in the text. In GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP;Nfia/b/x<sup>lox/lox</sup>;Rbpj<sup>lox/lox</sup> Müller glia, reactive MG appear to largely convert to bipolar and amacrine-like cells, and it remains unclear if they eventually revert to a resting state (Le, et al. 2024).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      This work demonstrates that Oct4 (Pou5f3) can induce neurogenesis in murine Müller glia (MG). Le et al start by showing that murine and zebrafish MG lack expression of Oct4 (Pou5f3) and its target Nanog. To assess the effect of Oct4 they first label adult MG with Sun1-GFP using tamoxifen-treated GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP mice, then later transduce in vivo with AAV vectors expressing mCherry alone or Oct4 + mCherry. Subsequently, they damage the retina with NMDA and assess the effects several weeks later. In Oct4+ cells at 2 weeks there is rare induction of the neural determinant Ascl1, down-regulation of the MG marker Sox2, induction of bipolar markers (Otx2, Scgn,Cabp5) but not amacrine (HuC/D) or rod (Nrl) markers. Combining Oct4 with

      Notch inhibition (deleting floxed Rbpj) synergistically increases bipolar cell induction, with Otx2 staining rising to >20% of GFP-marked cells, and cells losing MG identify (loss of Sox2/9). EdU labeling was negligible suggesting direct trans-differentiation. Similar synergy was seen upon combining Oct4 expression with Notch1/2 double gene knockout. Attempts to combine Oct4 with Nfia, Nfib, and Nfix loss were unsuccessful as the GFAP promoter driving Oct4 in MG seems to require these three related transcription factors. scRNAseq confirmed the Oct4-overexpression/Rbpj-KO-driven increase in bipolar cells and decrease in MG cells and revealed that these manipulations may enhance bipolar cell genesis by repressing genes that define quiescent MG and enhancing expression of genes that define reactive MG and neurogenic cells. Finally, multiomic snRNA/scATAC-seq data was performed to assess the effect of Oct2 in wt or Rbpj null MG. This approach revealed that, as anticipated, more genes were up and down-regulated in the context of both manipulations vs Oct4 OE alone. Moreover, Oct4 and Rbpj KO reduced chromatin accessibility at target motifs for transcription factors involved in MG identify/quiescence, while MGPCs showed elevated accessibility for neurogenic factors. The combination of Oct4 OE and Rbpj KO induces accessibility at various interesting TF sites that may contribute to the synergistic neurogenesis, including Rfx4, Klf4, Insm1, and others.

      This is an interesting paper that adds to the growing literature on how neurogenesis can be induced in mammalian MG. The focus on Oct4 is interesting and the synergistic effects are striking and analyzed in some detail with scRNAseq and multiomic snRNA/scATACseq. The latter results provide useful new insight into transcriptional programs that may be critical in driving neurogenesis. Functional insight into these new candidates is not explored in this manuscript, but that's beyond the scope of the current work and forms the basis for new studies. There are some overreaching statements in the Discussion that need to be toned down, but apart from that and a long list of textual errors that need to be fixed, this paper is a valuable contribution to the field.

      Major comments

      There are numerous textual errors (some, but not all, examples are detailed in minor comments). It was difficult to follow this paper given the unusually high number of textual errors and the abbreviated legends. Greater attention should be paid to harmonizing the text with the figures and ensuring that the legends are correct and complete.

      The manuscript has been proofread carefully and errors corrected.

      The opening section of the scRNAseq data should outline briefly why sorting for GFP labeled cells purifies a significant fraction of non-MG cell types, despite the earlier claim, (which agrees with other publications), that GLAST-CreER transgene expression is highly specific to MG. Presumably, it mainly/totally reflects the co-purification of cells, cell fragments, and/or cell-free mRNA from other lineages. Is it also possible that a fraction (however small) of these cells reflect low-level spurious/temporary activation of GLAST-CreER expression in non-MG? The "contamination" is present despite the addition of the GFP sequence to the reference genome (as explained in Methods). They mention: "a clear differentiation trajectory connecting Muller glia, neurogenic Muller gliaderived progenitor cells (MGPCs), and differentiating amacrine and bipolar cells (Fig. 3b)". However, the same trajectory is evident in control mCherry samples, so one could argue that this trajectory is active in normal retina at some low rate, but that would/should equate to rare sun-GFP+ non-MG in controls. Are there any such cells, even extremely rarely, or is it truly 0%? At any rate, the authors need to raise these concerns and offer some explanation(s) at the start of their scRNAseq Results section. If there are really no such sun-GFP+ cells, the authors should comment on the presence of the apparent inactive trajectory in the Discussion.

      Since we first described this line (de Melo, et al. 2012), we have examined thousands of sections of GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP retinas, and have yet to see a single GFP-positive neuron. We have also previously shown (Hoang, et al. 2020) that FACSbased isolation of GFP-positive cells from GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP yields a roughly thirty-fold enrichment of Muller glia, implying the presence of small numbers of contaminating neurons. We thereby conclude that the presence of small numbers of neurons (rods, cones, bipolar, and amacrine cells) in the control GlastCreER;Sun1-GFP represents contamination rather than low levels of glia-to-neuron conversion, particularly since we are unable to detect the expression of genes such as neurogenic bHLH factors or immature photoreceptor precursor-specific factors such as Prdm1 that indicate the presence of intermediate cell states. This is now addressed in the Results section related to both Figures 3 and 4.

      Discussion:

      In reference to other strategies to induce neurogenesis the authors make the claim that Oct4 is fundamentally different: "In these cases, Müller glia broadly upregulate proneural genes and/or downregulate Notch signaling. Oct4 instead induces expression of the neurogenic transcription factor Rfx4, which is not expressed in developing retina. It is likely that activation of this parallel pathway to neurogenic competence in part accounts for synergistic induction of neurogenesis seen in Rbpj-deficient Müller glia". First, all these strategies, including Oct4, seem to activate bHLH factors, so they have that in common and the authors should note that overlap. More seriously, without functional tests (e.g. KO Rfx4) the authors need to dial back the over-reaching statement that Rfx4 is the fundamental mechanism driving the Oct4 effect. They can certainly suggest that this is one possibility, but equally, Rfx4 may have very little or no effect on neurogenesis, or it could act redundantly with some of the other factors the authors uncovered. It's impossible to know without functional data, so they either need to add the functional data, or hold back on the strong one-sided and overreaching claim.

      Since both Rfx4 expression and motif accessibility are selectively observed following Oct4 overexpression, and Rfx4 also has known neurogenic activity, we stand by our conclusion that it is a particularly strong candidate for mediating the neurogenic effects of Oct4 overexpression. However, the Reviewer is correct that in the absence of functional data, speculation about its function should be qualified. We have done this in the revised manuscript.

      Minor comments

      This sentence in the Results is confusing: "While expression of neurogenic bHLH factors driven by the Gfap promoter was rapidly silenced in Muller glia and activated in amacrine and retinal ganglion cells, Gfap-Oct4-mCherry remained selectively expressed in Muller glia but did not induce detectable levels of Muller glia-derived neurogenesis in the uninjured retina (Le et al., 2022)". The cited reference is at the end so it sounds like the Oct4 assay was performed in Le et al 2022, and there is no reference to a Figure for the Oct4 data in the current paper.

      As stated here, in Le, et al. 2022, we did not observe any conversion of Sun1-GFP-positive Muller glia to neurons in the absence of injury. In the current study, we instead test whether NMDA-induced excitotoxicity induced glia to neuron conversion in Muller glia overexpressing Oct4. This is now made clear in the revised text.

      There are many errors and omissions regarding Figure S2:

      Figure S2a, b legend, and panels do not match. 2a should be a schematic of the strategy to label MG with Sun1-GFP using GLAST-Cre and a floxed Sun1-GFP allele, but that's missing and instead, the current 2a is a schematic of AAV vectors. It seems that the current 2b legend may describe the combination of the current 2a and 2b panels.

      This has been corrected.

      Figure S2: Asterisks label certain stained elements in the Oct4 labeled panels, but there is no explanation in the legend. Are these meant to indicate non-specific staining? If so, what is the evidence that the signal is non-specific?

      These asterisks represent non-specific mouse-on-mouse vascular staining observed with the mouse monoclonal anti-Oct4 used in this study. This is now indicated in the figure legend.

      The text refers to Ascl1 staining in Figure S2e,f, but it's S2g,h.

      This has been corrected.

      Re this: "While Sun1-GFP-positive cells infected with Oct4-mCherry mostly express the Muller glial marker Sox2 (Fig. S2a,b), from 2 weeks post-injury onwards a subset of GFP positive cells did not show detectable Sox2 expression (Fig. S2b, yellow arrows)". Figure S2a, b are schematic diagrams, not immunofluorescence. They probably mean Figure S2c, d.

      This has been corrected.

      Fig S2m is mislabeled "n".

      This has been corrected.

      There are probably other errors with this figure, but I mostly gave up at this point. The authors should go through the paper to find and correct any additional mistakes/omissions in the text and legends.

      The manuscript has been carefully proofread and errors corrected.

      The figure panels are not always mentioned in the order that they appear. There are many examples.

      Figure panels are now mentioned in the order that they appear.

      Several schematics use "d-18-14" to indicate "day -18 to -14". The former is at first uninterpretable or at best unclear (could mean day -18 to day 14), perhaps d -18 to -14, or d -18:-14 would be clearer.

      This has been corrected.

      Re: "AAV-infected wildtype Muller glia could be readily identified by selective expression of Oct4 (Fig. 4e). Wildtype Oct4-expressing Muller glia give rise to both small numbers of neurogenic MGPCs (Fig. 4b),". Figure 4E is labeled Pou5f1, but it would be helpful to avoid confusion by also indicating on the figure that Pou5f1 = Oct4; and Fig 4b does not indicate neurogenic MGPCs (perhaps they mean 4c).

      This has been corrected.

      Some parts of the Results are written in the present tense and should be in the past tense (for guidance: https://www.nature.com/scitable/topicpage/effective-writing13815989/).

      Past tense is now used throughout.

      Pit1 (Pou1f1) is referred to as a "close variant" of Oct4/Pou4f5, but this is unclear (e.g. variant could mean a splice variant from the same locus) and the term "paralogue" should be used.

      “Paralogue” is now used in this context.

      Re: "Infection with Oct4-mCherry vector induced both Oct4 (Fig. S5e) and Ascl1 (Fig. S5d) expression in Notch1/2-deficient Müller glia." Supplementary image 5d is the one depicting Oct4 and 5e is the one showing Ascl1. However, the reference is reversed.

      This has been corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      We deeply appreciate the reviewer’s careful review and critiques. These are excellent critiques that we are working on and probably require a few more years of work. Published together, we believe these critiques add value to our manuscript.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Yu and coworkers investigates the potential role of Secretory leukocyte protease inhibitor (SLPI) in Lyme arthritis. They show that, after needle inoculation of the Lyme disease (LD) agent, B. burgdorferi, compared to wild type mice, a SLPI-deficient mouse suffers elevated bacterial burden, joint swelling and inflammation, pro-inflammatory cytokines in the joint, and levels of serum neutrophil elastase (NE). They suggest that SLPI levels of Lyme disease patients are diminished relative to healthy controls. Finally, they find that SLPI may interact directly the B. burgdorferi.

      Strengths:

      Many of these observations are interesting and the use of SLPI-deficient mice is useful (and has not previously been done).

      Weaknesses:

      (a) The known role of SLPI in dampening inflammation and inflammatory damage by inhibition of NE makes the enhanced inflammation in the joint of B. burgdorferi-infected mice a predicted result; (b) The potential contribution of the greater bacterial burden to the enhanced inflammation is acknowledged but not experimentally addressed; (c) The relationship of SLPI binding by B. burgdorferi to the enhanced disease of SLPI-deficient mice is not addressed in this study, making the inclusion of this observation in this manuscript incomplete; and (d) assessment of SLPI levels in healthy controls vs. Lyme disease patients is inadequate.

      We greatly appreciate the critiques, and we do agree. Even though the observation of NE level is predictable, we believe that it is important to actually demonstrate it in the context of murine Lyme arthritis. The function of SLPI goes beyond inhibiting NE level.  As an ongoing project in our lab, we believe that the current study serves as a good starting point to explore the pleiotropic effects SLPI in the pathogenesis of murine Lyme arthritis and in patients. And, the critiques here are of great value to our research.

      Comments on revised version:

      Several of the points were addressed in the revised manuscript, but the following issues remain:

      Previous point that the relationship of SLPI binding to B. burgdorferi to the enhanced disease of SLPI-deficient mice is not investigated: The authors indicate that such investigations are ongoing. In the absence of any findings, I recommend that their interesting BASEHIT and subsequent studies be presented in a future study, which would have high impact.

      We thank the reviewer for the critique. We do agree that this part of the story is not complete. However, we would like to keep the BASEHIT and binding data in the paper, as we believe that it is an important finding. We confirmed the binding using ELISA, flow cytometry, and immunofluorescent microscopy. We showed that the binding is specific to infectious strain of B. burgdorferi, thus likely to contribute to the pathogenesis of murine Lyme arthritis. Our data suggest that SLPI can directly interact with a B. burgdorferi protein. We are exploring the biological significance of the binding. And this finding can be further explored by other labs too.

      Previous recommendation 1: (The authors added lines 267-68, not 287-68). This ambiguity is acknowledged but remains. In addition, in the revised manuscript, the authors state "However, these data also emphasize the importance of SLPI in controlling the development of inflammation in periarticular tissues of B. burgdorferi-infected mice." Given acknowledged limitations of interpretation, "suggest" would be more appropriate than "emphasize".

      We thank the reviewer for the careful reading, and we apologize for the mistake. The change has been made accordingly (line 268).

      Previous recommendation 5: The lack of clinical samples can be a challenge. Nevertheless, 4 of the 7 samples from LD patients are from individuals suffering from EM rather than arthritis (i.e., the manifestation that is the topic of the study) and some who are sampled multiple times, make an objective statistical comparison difficult. I don't have a suggestion as to how to address the difference in number of samples from a given subject. However, the authors could consider segregating EM vs. LA in their analysis (although it appears that limiting the comparison between HC and LA patients would not reveal a statistical difference).

      We thank the reviewer for the critique. And we agree with the reviewer that the patient’s data presented are not ideal. We believe that at this point the combination of the samples is most logical, as the number of samples we have from patients with Lyme arthritis is fairly limited. We stated the limitation in the discussion. We do believe that the finding of the correlation is important. It suggests the potential function of SLPI in patients, beyond murine infection.

      What’s more, various groups with large number of different samples can elucidate the relationship further.

      Previous recommendation 6: Given that binding of SLPI to the bacterial surface is an essential aspect of the authors' model, and that the ELISA assay to indicate SLPI binding used cell lysates rather than intact bacteria, a control PI staining to validate the integrity of bacteria seems reasonable.

      We appreciate the suggestion and has provided the propidium iodide staining in Supplemental Figure 5 (line 539-542, 568-569, 718-722).

      Previous recommendation 8: The inclusion of a no serum control (that presumably shows 100% viability) would validate the authors' assertion that 20% serum has bactericidal activity.

      We appreciate the suggestion. As stated in the manuscript (line 583-584), the percent viability was normalized to the control spirochetes culture without any treatment. Thus, the control spirochetes culture, without serum and SLPI treatment, showed 100% viability. We have revised Supplemental Figure 3 accordingly.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The paper proposes an interesting perspective on the spatio-temporal relationship between FC in fMRI and electrophysiology. The study found that while similar networks configurations are found in both modalities, there is a tendency for the networks to spatially converge more commonly at synchronous than asynchronous timepoints. However, my confidence in the findings and their interpretation is undermined by an incomplete justification for the expected outcomes for each of the proposed scenarios.

      As detailed below, the reviewer’s comment motivated us to conduct simulations to establish the relationship between the scenarios that we seek to adjudicate and the empirical outcomes.

      Main Concern

      Fig 1 makes sense to me conceptually, including the schematics of the trajectories, i.e.:

      - Scenario1. Temporally convergent, same trajectories through connectome state space

      - Scenario2. Temporally divergent, different trajectories through connectome state space

      However, based on my understanding (and apologies if I am mistaken), I am concerned that these scenarios do not necessarily translate into the schematic CRP plots shown in fig 2C, or the statements in the main text, i.e.:

      - For scenario1, "epochs of cross-modal spatial similarity should occur more frequently at on-diagonal (synchronous) than off-diagonal (asynchronous) entries, resulting in an on-/off-diagonal ratio larger than unity"

      - For scenario2, "epochs of spatial similarity could occur equally likely at on-diagonal and off-diagonal entries (ratio≈1)"

      Where do the authors get these statements and the schematics in fig2C from? They do not seem to be fully justified via previous literature, theory, or simulations?

      In particular, I am not convinced based on the evidence currently in the paper, that the ratio of off- to on-diagonal entries (and under what assumptions) is a definitive way to discriminate between scenarios 1 and 2.

      For example, what about the case where the same network configuration reoccurs in both modalities at multiple time points. It seems to me that you would get a CRP with entries occurring equally on the on-diagonal as on the off-diagonal, regardless of whether the dynamics are matched between the two modalities or not (i.e. regardless of scenario 1 or 2 being true).

      This thought experiment example might have a flaw in it, and the authors might ultimately be correct, but nonetheless a systematic justification needs to be provided for using the ratio of off- to on-diagonal entries to discriminate between scenario 1 and 2 (and under what assumptions it is valid).

      Thank you for raising this important point. In response, we have now included simulation results to complement our earlier authors’ response, which provided literature references and a theoretical explanation of the on-/off-diagonal ratio metric.

      In the absence of theory, the authors could use surrogate data for scenario 1 and 2. For example:

      a. For scenario 1, run the CRP using a single modality. E.g. feed in the EEG into the analysis as both modality 1 AND modality 2. This should provide at least one example of CRP under scenario 1 (although it does not ensure that all CRPs under this scenario will look like this, it is at least a useful sanity check).

      Note: This simulation was included in the previous round of author’s responses.

      b. For scenario 2, run the CRP using a single modality plus a shuffled version. E.g. feed in the EEG into the analysis as both modality 1 AND a temporally shuffled version of the EEG as modality 2. The temporal shuffling of the EEG could be done by simple splitting the data into blocks of say ~10s and then shuffling them into a new order. This should provide a version of the CRP under scenario 2 (although it does not ensure that all CRPs under this scenario will look like this, it is at least a useful sanity check)

      The authors have provided CRP plots for option a. It shows a CRP, as expected, consistent with scenario 1. This is a useful sanity check. However, as mentioned above, it does not ensure that all CRPs under this scenario will look like this.

      However, the authors have not shown a CRP as per option b. As such, there is an incomplete justification for the expected outcomes of the scenarios.

      Note that another option, which has not been carried out, is to use full simulations, with clearly specified assumptions, for scenario1 and 2. One way of doing this is to use a simplified (state-space) setup where you randomly simulate N spatially fixed networks that are independently switching on and off over time (i.e. "activation" is 0 or 1). Note that this would result in a N-dimensional connectome state space.

      Using this, you can simulate and compute the CRPs for the two scenarios:

      a. Scenario 1: where the simulated activation timecourses are set to be the same between both modalities

      b. Scenario 2: where the simulated activation timecourses are simulated separately for each of the modalities

      We followed the reviewer’s suggestion and have now included full simulations to address the concerns regarding the theory of the on-/off-diagonal ratio metric. As recommended, we defined a random quantized signal with N levels to represent the recurrent manifestation of N fixed connectome states. This setup was used to demonstrate the relationship between the two scenarios and the CRP observations used to adjudicate between the scenarios in our paper.

      The CRP matrices in Fig. S10 provide an example illustration of this simulation. In the case where the two state timeseries are identical, there are more co-occurrences of the same state (white entries) on the diagonal than off the diagonal (left subplot). This is in line with Scenario 1, where both spatial and temporal convergence are present. Conversely, in Scenario 2, where state time courses are shuffled, co-occurrences of the same states are more dispersed, and the diagonal prominence vanishes (right subplot). This difference illustrates how the CRP reflects the presence or absence of temporal alignment, dissociating scenarios 1 and 2.

      To quantitively validate this observation, we calculated the on-/off-diagonal ratio across simulations with varying N values. For Scenario 2 (shuffled version), the ratio consistently remained close to 1, indicating the absence of temporal synchronization. In contrast, Scenario 1 (non-shuffled version) produced significantly higher ratios, exceeding 1, confirming the metric's ability to capture meaningful synchrony. These results demonstrate that the simulations successfully replicate the expected relationship between the two scenarios and the CRPs, and validate the theoretical foundation of the ratio metric under the defined assumptions.

      Minor Concern

      Leakage correction. The paper states: "To mitigate this issue, we provide results from source-localized data both with and without leakage correction (supplementary and main text, respectively)." It is great that the authors provide both. However, given that FC in EEG is almost totally dominated by spatial leakage (see Hipp paper), the main results/figures for the scalp EEG should be done using spatial leakage corrected EEG data.

      Thank you. We agree that source leakage is an important consideration, which is why the current work investigated the intracranial EEG-fMRI data as a primary approach and subsequently added the scalp EEG-fMRI approach. While source leakage correction is essential for addressing spurious connectivity, it can also risk removing genuine functional connectivity that includes zero-lag relationships. We are reassured by the observation that the scalp data both without and with leakage correction confirmed the findings of the intracranial data, i.e., the presence of spatial and a lack of temporal cross-modal convergence. As such we do not believe that source leakage had a considerable impact on the specific question at hand.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study investigates the brain's functional connectivity (FC) dynamics across different timescales using simultaneous recordings of intracranial EEG/source-localized EEG and fMRI. The primary research goal was to determine which of three convergence/divergence scenarios is the most likely to occur.

      The results indicate that despite similar FC patterns found in different data modalities, the timepoints were not aligned, indicating spatial convergence but temporal divergence.

      The researchers also found that FC patterns in different frequencies do not overlap significantly, emphasizing the multi-frequency nature of brain connectivity. Such asynchronous activity across frequency bands supports the idea of multiple connectivity states that operate independently and are organized into a multiplex system.

      Strengths:

      The data supporting the authors' claims are convincing and come from simultaneous recordings of fMRI and iEEG/EEG, which has been recently developed and adapted.

      The analysis methods are solid and involved a novel approach to analyzing the co-occurrence of FC patterns across modalities (cross-modal recurrence plot, CRP) and robust statistics, including replication of the main results using multiple operationalizations of the functional connectome (e.g., amplitude, orthogonalized, and phase-based coupling).

      In addition, the authors provided a detailed interpretation of the results, placing them in the context of recent advances and understanding of the relationships between functional connectivity and cognitive states.

      The authors also did a control analysis and verified the effect of temporal window size or different functional connecvitity operationalizations. I also applaud their effort to make the analysis code open-sourced.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors answer my concerns and they are resolved.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This study investigates alterations in the autophagic-lysosomal pathway in the Q175 HD knock-in model crossed with the TRGL autophagy reporter mouse. The findings provide valuable insights into autophagy dynamics in HD and the potential therapeutic benefits of modulating this pathway. The study suggests that autophagy stimulation may offer therapeutic benefits in the early stages of HD progression, with mTOR inhibition showing promise in ameliorating lysosomal pathology and reducing mutant huntingtin accumulation.

      However, the data raises concerns regarding the strength of the evidence. The observed changes in autophagic markers, such as autolysosome and lysosome numbers, are relatively modest, and the Western blot results do not fully match the quantitative results. These discrepancies highlight the need for further validation and more pronounced effects to strengthen the conclusions. While the study suggests the potential of autophagy regulation as a long-term therapeutic strategy, additional experiments and more reliable data are necessary to confirm the broader applicability of the TRGL/Q175 mouse model.

      Furthermore, the 2004 publication by Ravikumar et al. demonstrated that inhibition of mTOR by rapamycin or the rapamycin ester CCI-779 induces autophagy and reduces the toxicity of polyglutamine expansions in fly and mouse models of Huntington's disease. mTOR is a key regulator of autophagy, and its inhibition has been explored as a therapeutic strategy for various neurodegenerative diseases, including HD. Studies suggest that inhibiting mTOR enhances autophagy, leading to the clearance of mHTT aggregates. Given that dysfunction of the autophagic-lysosomal pathway and lysosomal function in HD is already well-established, and that mTOR inhibition as a therapeutic approach for HD is also known, this study does not present entirely novel findings.

      Major Concerns:

      (1) In Figure 3A1 and A2, delayed and/or deficient acidification of AL causes deficits in the reformation of LY to replenish the LY pool. However, in Figure S2D, there is no difference in AL formation or substrate degradation, as shown by the Western blotting results for CTSD and CTSB. How can these discrepancies be explained?

      We appreciate the reviewer raising this point, and we agree with the concern. Please note that the material used for our immunoblotting was hemibrain homogenates, containing not only neurons but also glial cells, so the results for any protein, e.g., CTSD or CTSB in Fig. S2D, represented combined signals from neurons and glial cells. Our longstanding experience with western blot analysis of autophagy pathway markers is that signals from glial cells significantly interfere with/dilute the signals from neurons. By contrast, the immunofluorescence (IF) results in Fig. 3A, obtained with the assistance of tfLC3 probe and hue angle-based AV/LY subtype analysis, revealed the in situ conditions of the AL and LY within neurons selectively, which reflects the advantage of using the in vivo neuron-specific expression of the LC3 probe combined with IF with a LY marker in this study and our other related studies (Lee, Rao et al. 2019, Lee, Yang et al. 2022) as explained in the Introduction of this paper. Please also refer to a similar discussion regarding the WB-detected protein levels of p-ATG14 in L542-547. 

      (2) The results demonstrate that in the brain sections of 17-month-old TRGL/Q175 mice, there was an increase in the number of acidic autolysosomes (AL), including poorly acidified autolysosomes (pa-AL), alongside a decrease in lysosome (LY) numbers. These AL/pa-AL changes were not significant in 2-month-old or 7-month-old TRGL/Q175 mice, where only a reduction in lysosome numbers was observed. This indicates that these changes, representing damage to the autophagy-lysosome pathway (ALP), manifest only at later stages of the disease. Considering that the ALP is affected predominantly in the advanced stages of the disease (e.g., at 17 months), why were 6-month-old TRGL/Q175 mice selected for oral mTORi INK treatment, and why was the treatment duration restricted to just 3 weeks?

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. A key outcome measure in our evaluation of mTORi treatment was amelioration of mHTT pathology, i.e., mHTT aggregates/IBs. Before conducting the mTORi treatment experiments, we had learned from our assessments of age-associated progression of mHTT aggresomes/IBs in mice of different ages (e.g., 2-, 6-, 10- and 17-mo) that there were already severe mHTT accumulations in Q175 at 10-mo-old (e.g., Fig. 2A). This is consistent with a previous report (Carty, Berson et al. 2015) showing that striatal mHTT inclusions dynamically increase from 4 to 8 months. From a therapeutic point of view, more aggregates in the mouse brain would make it more difficult for the autophagy machinery to clear these aggregates. Thus, the high degree of aggregates in 10- or 17-mo may not be modifiable by the mTORi and/or prevent reliable/sensitive measurements on mTORi-induced phenotype changes. We then preferred to apply the treatment to younger (i.e., 6-mo-old) mice when the mHTT pathology was not so severe, with detectable, albeit mild, ALP abnormality.  Additionally, due to the 2-year funding limit for this project, there was insufficient time to generate a large set of old mice (e.g., ~18-mo) for another drug treatment experiment.  In future studies, it might be worthy to conduct the treatment “in the advanced stages of the disease (e.g., ~18-mo)” to further examine the modification potential of the mTORi on the ALP as well as the HTT aggregations. As for the treatment duration, we were interested in an acute treatment schedule given that, in our dosing tests, we observed rapid responses to the treatment (e.g., target engagement) in a few days even with one dose, and that the 14-15-day treatments produced consistent responses (e.g., Fig. S3A). Long-term treatment, however, would be worthy testing in the future although our current study informs a therapeutic approach that has been suggested by others involving intermittent/pulsatile administration of mTOR inhibitors to minimize side effects of chronic long-term administration.

      (3) Is the extent of motor dysfunction in TRGL/Q175 mice comparable to that in Q175 mice? Does the administration of mTORi INK improve these symptoms?

      Unfortunately, we were unable to investigate motor functions experimentally with specific assays such as open field or rotarod tests in this study (partially affected by the falling of the funded research period within the COVID-19 pandemic peak periods in 2020). Based on our experience in handling the mice, we did not notice any obvious differences between Q175 and TRGL/Q175, and any improvements after the acute mTORi INK treatment.  

      (4) Why is eGFP expression not visible in Fig. 6A in TRGL-Veh mice? Additionally, why do normal (non-poly-Q) mice have fewer lysosomes (LY) than TRGL/Q175-INK mice? IHC results also show that CTSD levels are lower in TRGL mice compared to TRGL/Q175-INK mice. Does this suggest lysosome dysfunction in TRGL-Veh mice?

      We appreciate the reviewer raising this point, which has been corrected (through slightly increasing the eGFP signal in the green channel and the merged channels equally for all genotypes), and the revised Fig. 6A is showing better eGFP signals. Regarding higher LY numbers/CTSD levels in TRGL/Q175-INK compared to the control TRGL-Veh mice, it does not necessarily imply LY dysfunction in TRGL mice, rather, it likely suggests mTORi treatment inducing LY biogenesis. Our original characterization of the TRGL mouse of varying ages, where low expression of the tgLC3 construct, produces only a very small increment of total LC3, resulting in no discernable functional changes in the autophagy pathway (Lee, Rao et al. 2019). The underlying mechanism, e.g., TFEB activation following mTOR inhibition, remains to be investigated in future studies. 

      (5) In Figure 5A, the phosphorylation of ATG14 (S29) shows minimal differences in Western blotting, which appears inconsistent with the quantitative results. A similar issue is observed in the quantification of Endo-LC3.

      We welcome the reviewer’s point, and therefore bands showing bigger differences of p-ATG14 (S29) have been used in the revised Fig. 5A, making the images and the quantitative results more consistent and representative. Similar changes have also been made to the Endo-LC3 data at the bottom of Fig. 5A.

      (6) In Figure S2A and Figure S2B, 17-month-old TRGL/Q175 mice show a decrease in pp70S6K and the p-ULK1/ULK1 ratio, but no changes are observed in autophagy-related markers. Do these results indicate only a slight change in autophagy at this stage in TRGL/Q175 mice? Since the mTOR pathway regulates multiple cellular mechanisms, could mTOR also influence other processes? Is it possible that additional mechanisms are involved?

      We completely agree with the reviewer. As mentioned in the text at multiple locations, LAP alterations in Q175 and TRGL/Q175 mice are mild even at a relatively old age (e.g., 17-mo), especially at the protein levels detected by immunoblotting. We agree that even if the mild alterations in the levels of pp70S6K (T389) and p-ULK1/ULK1 ratio may indicate “a slight change in autophagy”, it may also imply that other cell processes are involved given that mTOR signaling regulates multiple cellular functions. In particular, the p70S6K/p-p70S6K – a mTOR substrate used as a readout for mTOR activity in this study – is a key component of the protein synthesis pathway (Wang and Proud 2006, Magnuson, Ekim et al. 2012) , so its changes may serve as readouts for alterations in not only the autophagy pathway, but also the protein synthesis pathway. [A related discussion about mTOR/protein synthesis pathways, in response to a comment from Reviewer 2, has been incorporated into the text under Discussion, L633-640]

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors have explored the beneficial effect of autophagy upregulation in the context of HD pathology in a disease stage-specific manner. The authors have observed functional autophagy lysosomal pathway (ALP) and its machineries at the early stage in the HD mouse model, whereas impairment of ALP has been documented at the later stages of the disease progression. Eventually, the authors took advantage of the operational ALP pathway at the early stage of HD pathology, in order to upregulate ALP and autophagy flux by inhibiting mTORC1 in vivo, which ultimately reverted back to multiple ALP-related abnormalities and phenotypes. Therefore, this manuscript is a promising effort to shed light on the therapeutic interventions with which HD pathology can be treated at the patient level in the future.

      Strengths:

      The study has shown the alteration of ALP in the HD mouse model in a very detailed manner. Such stage-dependent in vivo study will be informative and has not been done before. Also, this research provides possible therapeutic interventions for patients in the future.

      Weaknesses:

      Some constructive comments and suggestions in order to reflect the key aspects and concepts better in the manuscript :

      (1) The authors have observed lysosome number alteration in a temporally regulated disease stage-specific manner. In this scenario investigation of regulation, localization, and level of TFEB, the transcription factor required for lysosome biogenesis, would be interesting and informative.

      We thank the reviewer for this point and completely agree that exploring TFEBrelated aspects would be interesting which will be investigated in future studies. 

      (2) For the general scientific community better clarification of the short forms will be useful. For example, in line 97, page 4, AP full form would be useful. Also 'metabolized via autophagy' can be replaced by 'degraded via autophagy'.

      We appreciate the reviewer for raising this point. We introduced each abbreviation at the location where the full term first appears and, for the case of “AP”, it was introduced in (previous) Line 69 when “autophagosome” first appears. We agree with the reviewer about easy reading for the general scientific community and thus we have added an Abbreviation section after the Key Words section, listing abbreviations used in this manuscript.

      Also, the word “metabolized” has been replaced with “degraded” as suggested. 

      (3) The nuclear vs cytosolic localization of HTT aggregates shown in Figure 2, are very interesting. The increase in cytosolic HTT aggregate formation at 10 months compared to 6 months probably suggests spatio-temporal regulation of aggregate formation. The authors could comment in a more elaborate manner, on the reason and impact of this kind of regulation of aggregate formation in the context of HD pathology.

      We value the reviewer’s important point. Previous studies have well documented that mHTT aggregates exist in both intranuclear and extranuclear locations in the brains of both human HD and mouse models (DiFiglia, Sapp et al. 1997, Li, Li et al. 1999, Carty, Berson et al. 2015, Peng, Wu et al. 2016, Berg, Veeranna et al. 2024). HTT can travel between the nucleus and cytoplasm and the default location for HTT is cytoplasmic, and thus the occurrence of nuclear mHTT aggregates is considered as a result of dysfunction in the nuclear exporting system for proteins (DiFiglia, Sapp et al. 1995, Gutekunst, Levey et al. 1995, Sharp, Loev et al. 1995, Cornett, Cao et al. 2005) while other factors such as phosphorylation of HTT may also affect nuclear targeting (DeGuire, Ruggeri et al. 2018). Extranuclear aggregates of mHTT usually appear later than nuclear aggregates and develop more aggressively in terms of numbers and pace after their appearance (Li, Li et al. 1999, Carty, Berson et al. 2015, Landles, Milton et al. 2020). The fact that there are neurons containing extranuclear aggregates without having nuclear aggregates within the same cells (Carty, Berson et al. 2015) does not support a nuclear-cytoplasmic sequence for aggregate formation, implying different mechanisms controlling the formation of these two types of aggregates. It was reported that there were no significant differences in toxicity associated with the presence of nuclear compared with extranuclear aggregates (Hackam, Singaraja et al. 1999), while other studies have proposed that nuclear aggregates correlate with transcriptional dysfunction while extranuclear aggregates may impair neuronal communication and can track disease progression (Li, Li et al. 1999, Benn, Landles et al. 2005, Landles, Milton et al. 2020). Thus, the observation of a higher level of extranuclear mHTT aggregates at 10-mo compared to 6-mo from the present study is consistent with previous findings mentioned above. In addition, our EM observations of homogenous granular/short fine fibril ultrastructure of both nuclear and extranuclear aggregates are consistent with findings from mouse model studies (Davies, Turmaine et al. 1997, Scherzinger, Lurz et al. 1997), which, interestingly, is different from in vitro studies where nuclear aggregates exhibited a core and shell structure but extranuclear aggregates did not possess the shell (Riguet, Mahul-Mellier et al. 2021), reflecting differences between in vivo and in vitro conditions. Taken together, even if efforts have been made in this and previous studies in trying to understand the differences between nuclear and extranuclear aggregates, the mechanisms regarding the spatial-temporal regulation of aggregate formation have so far not been fully revealed which will require additional investigations.

      (4) In this manuscript, the authors have convincingly shown that mTOR inhibition is inducing autophagy in the HD mouse model in vivo. On the other hand, mTOR inhibition would also reduce overall cellular protein translation. This aspect of mTOR inhibition can also potentially contribute to the alleviation of disease phenotype and disease symptoms by reducing protein overload in HD pathology. The authors' comments regarding this aspect would be appreciated.

      We recognize the value of the reviewer’s point which we completely agree with. Lowering mHTT via interfering protein translation (e.g., through RNAi, antisense oligonucleotides) has been an attractive strategy in HD therapeutic development (Kordasiewicz, Stanek et al. 2012, Tabrizi, Ghosh et al. 2019).  As mentioned above, mTOR regulates multiple cellular pathways including protein synthesis, and inhibition of mTOR as what was done in the present study is potentially affect protein synthesis as well. While our results of decreases in mHTT signals (Fig. 7) can be interpreted as a result of autophagymediated clearance of mHTT, certainly, a possibility cannot be excluded that mTOR inhibition may result in a reduction in HTT production which may also contribute to the observed results – future studies should determine how significant of such a contribution is. [The above description has been incorporated into the text under Discussion, L633-640] 

      (5) The authors have shown nuclear inclusion formation and aggregation of mHTT and also commented on its potential removal with the UPS system (proteasomal degradation) in vivo. As there is also a reciprocal relationship present between autophagy and proteasomal machineries, upon upregulation of autophagy machinery by mTOR inhibition proteasomal activity may decrease. How nuclear proteasomal activity increases to tackle nuclear mHTT IBs, would be interesting to understand in the context of HD pathology. Comments from the authors in this aspect would clarify the role of multiple degradation pathways in handling mutant HTT protein in HD pathology.

      We appreciate the reviewer raising this point. We agree that there are reciprocal relationships between autophagy and the UPS (Korolchuk, Menzies et al. 2010, Park and Cuervo 2013). In general, failure in one pathway would lead to compensatory upregulation of the other pathway, and vice versa (Lee, Park et al. 2019). So, as the reviewer pointed out, “upon upregulation of autophagy machinery by mTOR inhibition proteasomal activity may decrease”. However, we proposed in the Discussion that “It is possible that stimulation of autophagy is reducing the mHTT in the cytoplasm and thereby partially relieves the burden of the proteasome both in the cytoplasm and in the nucleus so that the nuclear proteasome operates more effectively”, which is inconsistent with the general expectation for a decreased UPS activity. However, please note that there are also instances where two pathways may act in the same direction, e.g., autophagy inhibition disturbs UPS degradative function (Korolchuk, Mansilla et al. 2009, Park and Cuervo 2013). Anyhow, our statement is just speculation, requiring verifications with additional experiments in the future. One of the observations reported here which may support the above speculation is the reductions of AV-non-associated form of mHTT/p62/Ub (Fig. 7B3), given that some of them might exist within the nucleus, whose reduced levels may reflect increased intranuclear UPS activity, besides the other possibility that they may travel from the nucleus to the cytosol for clearance as already discussed inside the text. [The last sentence has been incorporated into the text under Discussion, L628-632]

      (6) For the treatment of neurodegenerative disorders taking the temporal regulation into consideration is extremely important, as that will determine the success rate of the treatments in patients. The authors in this manuscript have clearly discussed this scenario. However, for neurodegenerative disordered patients, in most cases, the symptom manifestation is a late onset scenario. In that case, it will be complicated to initiate an early treatment regime in HD patients. If the authors can comment on and discuss the practicality of the early treatment regime for therapeutic purposes that would be impactful.

      We appreciate the reviewer raising this point and we agree with the main concern that “for neurodegenerative disordered patients, in most cases, the symptom manifestation is a late onset scenario.” This is really a common challenge in the therapeutic fields for neurodegeneration diseases. It should be first noted that the current study is an experimental therapeutical attempt in a mouse model which is consistent with previous reports (Ravikumar, Vacher et al. 2004) as a proof of concept for manipulating autophagy (i.e., via inhibiting mTOR in the current setting) as a potential therapeutic, whose clinical practicality requires further verifications. Moreover, in our opinion, early diagnosis (e.g., genetic testing in individuals with higher risk for HD) may be a key in overcoming the above challenges, i.e., if early diagnosis is enabled, it would become possible for earlier interventions. [The above description has been incorporated into the text under Discussion, L654-659] 

      Recommendations for the authors: 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor concerns:

      (1) Figures 1 and 2 should indicate the number of sections and mice/genotypes.

      Thanks for the suggestion, and the info has been added in the figure legends. 

      (2) Figure 3A2 should explain how AP, AL, pa-AL, and LY are quantified.

      Thanks for raising this point. Please note that the quantitation of AP, AL, pa-AL and LY was performed by the hue angle-based analysis which was described under “Confocal image collection and hue angle-based quantitative analysis for AV/LY subtypes” within the Materials and Methods. A phrase “(see the Materials and Methods)” has been added after the existing description “Hue angle-based analysis was performed for AV/LY subtype determination using the methods described in Lee et al., 2019” in the figure legend.

      References

      Benn, C. L., C. Landles, H. Li, A. D. Strand, B. Woodman, K. Sathasivam, S. H. Li, S. Ghazi-Noori, E. Hockly, S. M. Faruque, J. H. Cha, P. T. Sharpe, J. M. Olson, X. J. Li and G. P. Bates (2005). "Contribution of nuclear and extranuclear polyQ to neurological phenotypes in mouse models of Huntington's disease." Hum Mol Genet 14(20): 3065-3078.

      Berg, M. J., Veeranna, C. M. Rosa, A. Kumar, P. S. Mohan, P. Stavrides, D. M. Marchionini, D.S. Yang and R. A. Nixon (2024). "Pathobiology of the autophagy-lysosomal pathway in the Huntington’s disease brain." bioRxiv: 2024.2005.2029.596470.

      Carty, N., N. Berson, K. Tillack, C. Thiede, D. Scholz, K. Kottig, Y. Sedaghat, C. Gabrysiak, G. Yohrling, H. von der Kammer, A. Ebneth, V. Mack, I. Munoz-Sanjuan and S. Kwak (2015). "Characterization of HTT inclusion size, location, and timing in the zQ175 mouse model of Huntington's disease: an in vivo high-content imaging study." PLoS One 10(4): e0123527.

      Cornett, J., F. Cao, C. E. Wang, C. A. Ross, G. P. Bates, S. H. Li and X. J. Li (2005). "Polyglutamine expansion of huntingtin impairs its nuclear export." Nat Genet 37(2): 198204.

      Davies, S. W., M. Turmaine, B. A. Cozens, M. DiFiglia, A. H. Sharp, C. A. Ross, E. Scherzinger, E. E. Wanker, L. Mangiarini and G. P. Bates (1997). "Formation of neuronal intranuclear inclusions underlies the neurological dysfunction in mice transgenic for the HD mutation." Cell 90(3): 537-548.

      DeGuire, S. M., F. S. Ruggeri, M. B. Fares, A. Chiki, U. Cendrowska, G. Dietler and H. A. Lashuel (2018). "N-terminal Huntingtin (Htt) phosphorylation is a molecular switch regulating Htt aggregation, helical conformation, internalization, and nuclear targeting." J Biol Chem 293(48): 18540-18558.

      DiFiglia, M., E. Sapp, K. Chase, C. Schwarz, A. Meloni, C. Young, E. Martin, J. P. Vonsattel, R. Carraway, S. A. Reeves and et al. (1995). "Huntingtin is a cytoplasmic protein associated with vesicles in human and rat brain neurons." Neuron 14(5): 1075-1081.

      DiFiglia, M., E. Sapp, K. O. Chase, S. W. Davies, G. P. Bates, J. P. Vonsattel and N. Aronin (1997). "Aggregation of huntingtin in neuronal intranuclear inclusions and dystrophic neurites in brain." Science 277(5334): 1990-1993.

      Gutekunst, C. A., A. I. Levey, C. J. Heilman, W. L. Whaley, H. Yi, N. R. Nash, H. D. Rees, J. J. Madden and S. M. Hersch (1995). "Identification and localization of huntingtin in brain and human lymphoblastoid cell lines with anti-fusion protein antibodies." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 92(19): 8710-8714.

      Hackam, A. S., R. Singaraja, T. Zhang, L. Gan and M. R. Hayden (1999). "In vitro evidence for both the nucleus and cytoplasm as subcellular sites of pathogenesis in Huntington's disease." Hum Mol Genet 8(1): 25-33.

      Kordasiewicz, H. B., L. M. Stanek, E. V. Wancewicz, C. Mazur, M. M. McAlonis, K. A. Pytel, J. W. Artates, A. Weiss, S. H. Cheng, L. S. Shihabuddin, G. Hung, C. F. Bennett and D. W. Cleveland (2012). "Sustained therapeutic reversal of Huntington's disease by transient repression of huntingtin synthesis." Neuron 74(6): 1031-1044.

      Korolchuk, V. I., A. Mansilla, F. M. Menzies and D. C. Rubinsztein (2009). "Autophagy inhibition compromises degradation of ubiquitin-proteasome pathway substrates." Mol Cell 33(4): 517-527.

      Korolchuk, V. I., F. M. Menzies and D. C. Rubinsztein (2010). "Mechanisms of cross-talk between the ubiquitin-proteasome and autophagy-lysosome systems." FEBS Lett 584(7): 1393-1398.

      Landles, C., R. E. Milton, N. Ali, R. Flomen, M. Flower, F. Schindler, C. Gomez-Paredes, M. K. Bondulich, G. F. Osborne, D. Goodwin, G. Salsbury, C. L. Benn, K. Sathasivam, E. J. Smith, S. J. Tabrizi, E. E. Wanker and G. P. Bates (2020). "Subcellular Localization And Formation Of Huntingtin Aggregates Correlates With Symptom Onset And Progression In A Huntington'S Disease Model." Brain Commun 2(2): fcaa066.

      Lee, J. H., S. Park, E. Kim and M. J. Lee (2019). "Negative-feedback coordination between proteasomal activity and autophagic flux." Autophagy 15(4): 726-728.

      Lee, J. H., M. V. Rao, D. S. Yang, P. Stavrides, E. Im, A. Pensalfini, C. Huo, P. Sarkar, T. Yoshimori and R. A. Nixon (2019). "Transgenic expression of a ratiometric autophagy probe specifically in neurons enables the interrogation of brain autophagy in vivo." Autophagy 15(3): 543-557.

      Lee, J. H., D. S. Yang, C. N. Goulbourne, E. Im, P. Stavrides, A. Pensalfini, H. Chan, C. Bouchet-Marquis, C. Bleiwas, M. J. Berg, C. Huo, J. Peddy, M. Pawlik, E. Levy, M. Rao, M. Staufenbiel and R. A. Nixon (2022). "Faulty autolysosome acidification in Alzheimer's disease mouse models induces autophagic build-up of Abeta in neurons, yielding senile plaques." Nat Neurosci 25(6): 688-701.

      Li, H., S. H. Li, A. L. Cheng, L. Mangiarini, G. P. Bates and X. J. Li (1999). "Ultrastructural localization and progressive formation of neuropil aggregates in Huntington's disease transgenic mice." Hum Mol Genet 8(7): 1227-1236.

      Magnuson, B., B. Ekim and D. C. Fingar (2012). "Regulation and function of ribosomal protein S6 kinase (S6K) within mTOR signalling networks." Biochem J 441(1): 1-21.

      Park, C. and A. M. Cuervo (2013). "Selective autophagy: talking with the UPS." Cell Biochem Biophys 67(1): 3-13.

      Peng, Q., B. Wu, M. Jiang, J. Jin, Z. Hou, J. Zheng, J. Zhang and W. Duan (2016). "Characterization of Behavioral, Neuropathological, Brain Metabolic and Key Molecular Changes in zQ175 Knock-In Mouse Model of Huntington's Disease." PLoS One 11(2): e0148839.

      Ravikumar, B., C. Vacher, Z. Berger, J. E. Davies, S. Luo, L. G. Oroz, F. Scaravilli, D. F. Easton, R. Duden, C. J. O'Kane and D. C. Rubinsztein (2004). "Inhibition of mTOR induces autophagy and reduces toxicity of polyglutamine expansions in fly and mouse models of Huntington disease." Nat Genet 36(6): 585-595.

      Riguet, N., A. L. Mahul-Mellier, N. Maharjan, J. Burtscher, M. Croisier, G. Knott, J. Hastings, A. Patin, V. Reiterer, H. Farhan, S. Nasarov and H. A. Lashuel (2021). "Nuclear and cytoplasmic huntingtin inclusions exhibit distinct biochemical composition, interactome and ultrastructural properties." Nat Commun 12(1): 6579.

      Scherzinger, E., R. Lurz, M. Turmaine, L. Mangiarini, B. Hollenbach, R. Hasenbank, G. P. Bates, S. W. Davies, H. Lehrach and E. E. Wanker (1997). "Huntingtin-encoded polyglutamine expansions form amyloid-like protein aggregates in vitro and in vivo." Cell 90(3): 549-558.

      Sharp, A. H., S. J. Loev, G. Schilling, S. H. Li, X. J. Li, J. Bao, M. V. Wagster, J. A. Kotzuk, J. P. Steiner, A. Lo and et al. (1995). "Widespread expression of Huntington's disease gene (IT15) protein product." Neuron 14(5): 1065-1074.

      Tabrizi, S. J., R. Ghosh and B. R. Leavitt (2019). "Huntingtin Lowering Strategies for Disease Modification in Huntington's Disease." Neuron 101(5): 801-819.

      Wang, X. and C. G. Proud (2006). "The mTOR pathway in the control of protein synthesis." Physiology (Bethesda) 21: 362-369.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This study offers a valuable investigation into the role of cholecystokinin (CCK) in thalamocortical plasticity during early development and adulthood, employing a range of experimental techniques. The authors demonstrate that tetanic stimulation of the auditory thalamus induces cortical long-term potentiation (LTP), which can be evoked through either electrical or optical stimulation of the thalamus or by noise bursts. They further show that thalamocortical LTP is abolished when thalamic CCK is knocked down or when cortical CCK receptors are blocked. Interestingly, in 18-month-old mice, thalamocortical LTP was largely absent but could be restored through the cortical application of CCK. The authors conclude that CCK contributes to thalamocortical plasticity and may enhance thalamocortical plasticity in aged subjects.

      While the study presents compelling evidence, I would like to offer several suggestions for the authors' consideration:

      (1) Thalamocortical LTP and NMDA-Dependence:

      It is well established that thalamocortical LTP is NMDA receptor-dependent, and blocking cortical NMDA receptors can abolish LTP. This raises the question of why thalamocortical LTP is eliminated when thalamic CCK is knocked down or when cortical CCK receptors are blocked. If I correctly understand the authors' hypothesis - that CCK promotes LTP through CCKR-intracellular Ca2+-AMPAR. This pathway should not directly interfere with the NMDA-dependent mechanism. A clearer explanation of this interaction would be beneficial.

      Thank you for your question regarding the role of CCK and NMDA receptors (NMDARs) in thalamocortical LTP. We propose that CCK receptor (CCKR) activation enhances intracellular calcium levels, which are crucial for thalamocortical LTP induction. Calcium influx through NMDARs is also essential to reach the threshold required for activating downstream signaling pathways that promote LTP (Heynen and Bear, 2001). Thus, CCKRs and NMDARs may function in a complementary manner to facilitate LTP, with both contributing to the elevation of intracellular calcium.

      However, it is important to note that the postsynaptic mechanisms of thalamocortical LTP in the auditory cortex (ACx) differ from those in other sensory cortices. Studies have shown that thalamocortical LTP in the ACx appears to be less dependent on NMDARs (Chun et al., 2013), which is distinct from somatosensory or visual cortices. Our previous studies also found that while NMDAR antagonists can block HFS-induced LTP in the inner ACx, LTP can still be induced in the presence of CCK even after the NMDARs blockade (Chen et al. 2019). These findings suggest that CCK may act through an alternative mechanism involving CCKR-mediated calcium signaling and AMPAR modulation, which partially compensates for the loss of NMDAR signaling. This distinction may reflect functional differences between the ACx and other sensory cortices, as highlighted in previous studies (King and Nelken, 2009).

      While our current study focuses on the role of CCKR-mediated plasticity in the auditory system, further investigations are needed to elucidate how CCKRs and NMDARs interact within the broader framework of thalamocortical neuroplasticity across different cortical regions. Understanding whether similar mechanisms operate in other sensory systems, such as the visual cortex, will be an important direction for future research.

      Heynen, A.J., and Bear, M.F. (2001). Long-term potentiation of thalamocortical transmission in the adult visual cortex in vivo. J Neurosci 21, 9801-9813. 10.1523/jneurosci.21-24-09801.2001.

      Chun, S., Bayazitov, I.T., Blundon, J.A., and Zakharenko, S.S. (2013). Thalamocortical Long-Term Potentiation Becomes Gated after the Early Critical Period in the Auditory Cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience 33, 7345-7357. 10.1523/jneurosci.4500-12.2013.

      Chen, X., Li, X., Wong, Y.T., Zheng, X., Wang, H., Peng, Y., Feng, H., Feng, J., Baibado, J.T., Jesky, R., et al. (2019). Cholecystokinin release triggered by NMDA receptors produces LTP and sound-sound associative memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116, 6397-6406. 10.1073/pnas.1816833116.

      King, A. J., & Nelken, I. (2009). Unraveling the principles of auditory cortical processing: can we learn from the visual system? Nature neuroscience, 12(6), 698-701.

      (2) Complexity of the Thalamocortical System:

      The thalamocortical system is intricate, with different cortical and thalamic subdivisions serving distinct functions. In this study, it is not fully clear which subdivisions were targeted for stimulation and recording, which could significantly influence the interpretation of the findings. Clarifying this aspect would enhance the study's robustness.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. We would like to clarify that stimulation was conducted in the medial geniculate nucleus ventral (MGv), and recording was performed in layer IV of the ACx. Targeting the MGv allows us to investigate the influence of thalamic inputs on auditory cortical responses. Layer IV of the ACx is known to receive direct thalamic projections, making it an ideal site for assessing how thalamic activity influences cortical processing. We will incorporate this clarification into the revised manuscript to enhance the robustness of our study.

      Results section:

      “Stimulation electrodes were placed in the MGB (specifically in the medial geniculate nucleus ventral subdivision, MGv), and recording electrodes were inserted into layer IV of ACx”

      “The recording electrodes were lowered into layer IV of ACx, while the stimulation electrodes were lowered into MGB (MGv subdivision). The final stimulating and recording positions were determined by maximizing the cortical fEPSP amplitude triggered by the ES in the MGB. The accuracy of electrode placement was verified through post-hoc histological examination and electrophysiological responses.”

      (3) Statistical Variability:

      Biological data, including field excitatory postsynaptic potentials (fEPSPs) and LTP, often exhibit significant variability between samples, sometimes resulting in a standard deviation that exceeds 50% of the mean value. The reported standard deviation of LTP in this study, however, appears unusually small, particularly given the relatively limited sample size. Further discussion of this observation might be warranted.

      Thank you for your question. In our experiments, the sample size N represents the number of animals used, while n refers to the number of recordings, with each recording corresponding to a distinct stimulation and recording sites. To adhere to ethical guidelines and minimize animal usage, we often perform multiple recordings within a single animal, such as from different hemispheres of the brain. Although N may appear small, our statistical analyses are based on n, ensuring sufficient data points for reliable conclusions.

      Furthermore, as our experiments are conducted in vivo, we observe lower variability in the increase of fEPSP slopes following LTP induction compared to brain slice preparations, where standard deviations exceeding 50% of the mean are common. This reduced variability likely reflects the robustness of the physiologically intact conditions in the in vivo setup.

      (4) EYFP Expression and Virus Targeting:

      The authors indicate that AAV9-EFIa-ChETA-EYFP was injected into the medial geniculate body (MGB) and subsequently expressed in both the MGB and cortex. If I understand correctly, the authors assume that cortical expression represents thalamocortical terminals rather than cortical neurons. However, co-expression of CCK receptors does not necessarily imply that the virus selectively infected thalamocortical terminals. The physiological data regarding cortical activation of thalamocortical terminals could be questioned if the cortical expression represents cortical neurons or both cortical neurons and thalamocortical terminals.

      Thank you for your question. In Figure 2A, EYFP expression indicates thalamocortical projections, while the co-expression of EYFP with PSD95 confirms the identity of thalamocortical terminals. The CCK-B receptors (CCKBR) are located on postsynaptic cortical neurons. The observed co-labeling of thalamocortical terminals and postsynaptic CCKBR suggests that CCK-expressing neurons in the medial geniculate body (MGB) can release CCK, which subsequently acts on the postsynaptic CCKBR. This evidence supports our interpretation of the functional role of CCK modulating neural plasticity between thalamocortical inputs and cortical neurons. As shown in Figure 2A, we aim to demonstrate that the co-labeling of thalamocortical terminals with CCK receptors accounts for a substantial proportion of the thalamocortical terminals. We will ensure that this clarification is emphasized in the revised manuscript to address your concerns.

      Results section:

      “Cre-dependent AAV9-EFIa-DIO-ChETA-EYFP was injected into the MGB of CCK-Cre mice. EYFP labeling marked CCK-positive neurons in the MGB. The co-expression of EYFP thalamocortical projections with PSD95 confirms the identity of thalamocortical terminals (yellow), which primarily targeted layer IV of the ACx (Figure 2A, upper panel). Immunohistochemistry revealed that a substantial proportion (15 out of 19, Figure 2A lower right panel) of thalamocortical terminals (arrows) colocalize with CCK receptors (CCKBR) on postsynaptic cortical neurons in the ACx (Figure 2A lower panel), supporting the functional role of CCK in modulating thalamocortical plasticity.”

      (5) Consideration of Previous Literature:

      A number of studies have thoroughly characterized auditory thalamocortical LTP during early development and adulthood. It may be beneficial for the authors to integrate insights from this body of work, as reliance on data from the somatosensory thalamocortical system might not fully capture the nuances of the auditory pathway. A more comprehensive discussion of the relevant literature could enhance the study's context and impact.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. We will enhance our discussion on auditory thalamocortical LTP during early development and adulthood to provide a more comprehensive context for our study.

      (6) Therapeutic Implications:

      While the authors suggest potential therapeutic applications of their findings, it may be somewhat premature to draw such conclusions based on the current evidence. Although speculative discussion is not harmful, it may not significantly add to the study's conclusions at this stage.

      Thank you for your thoughtful feedback. We agree that the therapeutic applications mentioned in our study are speculative at this stage and should be regarded as a forward-looking perspective rather than definitive conclusions. Our intention was to highlight the broader potential of our findings to inspire further research, rather than to propose immediate clinical applications.

      In light of your feedback, we have adjusted the language in the manuscript to reflect a more cautious interpretation. Speculative discussions are now explicitly framed as hypotheses or possibilities for future exploration. We emphasize that our findings provide a foundation for further investigations into CCK-based plasticity and its implications.

      We believe that appropriately framed forward-thinking discussions are valuable in guiding the direction of future research. We sincerely hope that our current and future work will contribute to a deeper understanding of thalamocortical plasticity and, over time, potentially lead to advancements in human health.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work used multiple approaches to show that CCK is critical for long-term potentiation (LTP) in the auditory thalamocortical pathway. They also showed that the CCK mediation of LTP is age-dependent and supports frequency discrimination. This work is important because it opens up a new avenue of investigation of the roles of neuropeptides in sensory plasticity.

      Strengths:

      The main strength is the multiple approaches used to comprehensively examine the role of CCK in auditory thalamocortical LTP. Thus, the authors do provide a compelling set of data that CCK mediates thalamocortical LTP in an age-dependent manner.

      Weaknesses:

      The behavioral assessment is relatively limited but may be fleshed out in future work.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Cholecystokinin (CCK) is highly expressed in auditory thalamocortical (MGB) neurons and CCK has been found to shape cortical plasticity dynamics. In order to understand how CCK shapes synaptic plasticity in the auditory thalamocortical pathway, they assessed the role of CCK signaling across multiple mechanisms of LTP induction with the auditory thalamocortical (MGB - layer IV Auditory Cortex) circuit in mice. In these physiology experiments that leverage multiple mechanisms of LTP induction and a rigorous manipulation of CCK and CCK-dependent signaling, they establish an essential role of auditory thalamocortical LTP on the co-release of CCK from auditory thalamic neurons. By carefully assessing the development of this plasticity over time and CCK expression, they go on to identify a window of time that CCK is produced throughout early and middle adulthood in auditory thalamocortical neurons to establish a window for plasticity from 3 weeks to 1.5 years in mice, with limited LTP occurring outside of this window. The authors go on to show that CCK signaling and its effect on LTP in the auditory cortex is also capable of modifying frequency discrimination accuracy in an auditory PPI task. In evaluating the impact of CCK on modulating PPI task performance, it also seems that in mice <1.5 years old CCK-dependent effects on cortical plasticity are almost saturated. While exogenous CCK can modestly improve discrimination of only very similar tones, exogenous focal delivery of CCK in older mice can significantly improve learning in a PPI task to bring their discrimination ability in line with those from young adult mice.

      Strengths:

      (1) The clarity of the results along with the rigor multi-angled approach provide significant support for the claim that CCK is essential for auditory thalamocortical synaptic LTP. This approach uses a combination of electrical, acoustic, and optogenetic pathway stimulation alongside conditional expression approaches, germline knockout, viral RNA downregulation, and pharmacological blockade. Through the combination of these experimental configures the authors demonstrate that high-frequency stimulation-induced LTP is reliant on co-release of CCK from glutamatergic MGB terminals projecting to the auditory cortex.

      (2) The careful analysis of the CCK, CCKB receptor, and LTP expression is also a strength that puts the finding into the context of mechanistic causes and potential therapies for age-dependent sensory/auditory processing changes. Similarly, not only do these data identify a fundamental biological mechanism, but they also provide support for the idea that exogenous asynchronous stimulation of the CCKBR is capable of restoring an age-dependent loss in plasticity.

      (3) Although experiments to simultaneously relate LTP and behavioral change or identify a causal relationship between LTP and frequency discrimination are not made, there is still convincing evidence that CCK signaling in the auditory cortex (known to determine synaptic LTP) is important for auditory processing/frequency discrimination. These experiments are key for establishing the relevance of this mechanism.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Given the magnitude of the evoked responses, one expects that pyramidal neurons in layer IV are primarily those that undergo CCK-dependent plasticity, but the degree to which PV-interneurons and pyramidal neurons participate in this process differently is unclear.

      Thank you for this insightful comment. We agree that the differential roles of PV-interneurons and pyramidal neurons in CCK-dependent thalamocortical plasticity remain unclear and acknowledge this as an important limitation of our study. Our primary focus was on pyramidal neurons, as our in vivo electrophysiological recordings measured the fEPSP slope in layer IV of the auditory cortex, which primarily reflects excitatory synaptic activity. However, we recognize the critical role of the excitatory-inhibitory balance in cortical function and the potential contribution of PV-interneurons to this process. In future studies, we plan to utilize techniques such as optogenetics, two-photon calcium imaging and cell-type-specific recordings to investigate the distinct contributions of PV-interneurons and pyramidal neurons to CCK-dependent thalamocortical plasticity, thereby providing a more comprehensive understanding of how CCK modulates thalamocortical circuits.

      (2) While these data support an important role for CCK in synaptic LTP in the auditory thalamocortical pathway, perhaps temporal processing of acoustic stimuli is as or more important than frequency discrimination. Given the enhanced responsivity of the system, it is unclear whether this mechanism would improve or reduce the fidelity of temporal processing in this circuit. Understanding this dynamic may also require consideration of cell type as raised in weakness #1.

      Thank you for this thoughtful comment. We acknowledge that our study did not directly address the fidelity of temporal processing, which is indeed a critical aspect of auditory function. Our behavioral experiments primarily focused on linking frequency discrimination to the role of CCK in synaptic strengthening within the auditory thalamocortical pathway. However, we agree that enhanced responsivity of the system could also impact temporal processing dynamics, such as the precise timing of auditory responses. Whether this modulation improves or reduces the fidelity of temporal processing remains an open and important question.

      As you noted, understanding these dynamics will require a deeper investigation into the interactions between different cell types, particularly the balance between excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Exploring how CCK modulation affects both the circuit and cellular levels in temporal processing is an important direction for future research, which we plan to pursue. Thank you again for raising this important point.

      Disscusion section:

      “While we focused on homosynaptic plasticity at thalamocortical synapses by recording only fEPSPs in layer IV of ACx, it is essential to further explore heterosynaptic effects of CCK released from thalamocortical synapses on intracortical circuits, particularly its role in modulating the excitatory-inhibitory balance. PV-interneurons, as key regulators of cortical inhibition, may contribute to the temporal fidelity of sensory processing, which is critical for auditory perception (Nocon et al., 2023; Cai et al., 2018). Additionally, CCK may facilitate cross-modal plasticity by modulating heterosynaptic plasticity in interconnected cortical areas. Future studies would provide valuable insights into the broader role of CCK in shaping sensory processing and cortical network dynamics.”

      Nocon, J.C., Gritton, H.J., James, N.M., Mount, R.A., Qu, Z., Han, X., and Sen, K. (2023). Parvalbumin neurons enhance temporal coding and reduce cortical noise in complex auditory scenes. Communications Biology 6, 751. 10.1038/s42003-023-05126-0.

      Cai, D., Han, R., Liu, M., Xie, F., You, L., Zheng, Y., Zhao, L., Yao, J., Wang, Y., Yue, Y., et al. (2018). A Critical Role of Inhibition in Temporal Processing Maturation in the Primary Auditory Cortex. Cereb Cortex 28, 1610-1624. 10.1093/cercor/bhx057.

      (3) In Figure 1, an example of increased spontaneous and evoked firing activity of single neurons after HFS is provided. Yet it is surprising that the group data are analyzed only for the fEPSP. It seems that single-neuron data would also be useful at this point to provide insight into how CCK and HFS affect temporal processing and spontaneous activity/excitability, especially given the example in 1F.

      Thank you for your insightful comment. In our in vivo electrophysiological experiments on LTP induction, we recorded neural activity for over 1.5 hours to assess changes in neuronal responses over time, both prior to and following the induction. While single neuron firing data can provide valuable insights, such measurements are inherently more variable due to factors like cortical state fluctuations and the condition of nearby neurons, which makes them less reliable for long-term analysis. For this reason, we focused on fEPSP, as it offers a more stable and robust readout of synaptic activity over extended periods.

      We appreciate your suggestion and recognize the value of single-neuron data in understanding how CCK and HFS affect temporal processing and excitability. In future studies, we will consider to incorporate single-neuron analyses to complement our synaptic-level findings and provide a more comprehensive understanding of these mechanisms.

      (4) The authors mention that CCK mRNA was absent in CCK-KO mice, but the data are not provided.

      Thank you for your comment. Data from the CCK-KO mice are presented in Figure 3A (far right) and in the upper panel of Figure 3B (far right). In the lower panel of Figure 3B, data from the CCK-KO group are not shown because the normalized values for this group were essentially zero, as expected due to the absence of CCK mRNA.

      (5) The circuitry that determines PPI requires multiple brain areas, including the auditory cortex. Given the complicated dynamics of this process, it may be helpful to consider what, if anything, is known specifically about how layer IV synaptic plasticity in the auditory cortex may shape this behavior.

      Thank you for raising this important point. Pre-pulse inhibition (PPI) of the acoustic startle response indeed involves multiple brain regions, with the ascending auditory pathway playing a key role (Gómez-Nieto et al., 2020). Within the auditory cortex, layer IV neurons receive tonotopically organized inputs from the medial geniculate nucleus and are critical for integrating thalamic inputs and shaping auditory processing.

      In our behavioral experiments, mice were required to discriminate pre-pulses of varying frequencies against a continuous background sound. Given the role of auditory cortical neurons in integrating thalamic inputs and shaping auditory processing, it is likely that synaptic plasticity in these neurons contributes to the enhanced discrimination of pre-pulses. Supporting this idea, our previous work demonstrated that local infusion of CCK, paired with weak acoustic stimuli, significantly increased auditory responses in the auditory cortex (Li et al., 2014). In the current study, we further showed that CCK release during high-frequency stimulation of the thalamocortical pathway induced LTP in layer IV of the auditory cortex. Together, these findings suggest that CCK-dependent synaptic plasticity in layer IV may amplify the cortical representation of weak auditory inputs, thereby improving pre-pulses detection and enhancing PPI performance.

      It is also worth noting that aged mice with hearing loss typically exhibit PPI deficits due to impaired auditory processing (Ouagazzal et al., 2006 and Young et al., 2010). We propose that enhanced plasticity in the thalamocortical pathway, mediated by CCK, might partially compensate for these deficits by amplifying residual auditory signals in aged mice. However, the precise mechanisms by which layer IV synaptic plasticity modulates PPI behavior remain to be fully understood. Given the complex dynamics of sensory processing, future studies could explore how layer IV neurons interact with other cortical and subcortical circuits involved in PPI, as well as the specific contributions of excitatory and inhibitory cell types. These investigations will help provide a more comprehensive understanding of the role of CCK in modulating sensory gating and auditory processing.

      Gómez-Nieto, R., Hormigo, S., & López, D. E. (2020). Prepulse inhibition of the auditory startle reflex assessment as a hallmark of brainstem sensorimotor gating mechanisms. Brain sciences, 10(9), 639.

      Li, X., Yu, K., Zhang, Z., Sun, W., Yang, Z., Feng, J., Chen, X., Liu, C.-H., Wang, H., Guo, Y.P., and He, J. (2014). Cholecystokinin from the entorhinal cortex enables neural plasticity in the auditory cortex. Cell Research 24, 307-330. 10.1038/cr.2013.164.

      Ouagazzal, A. M., Reiss, D., & Romand, R. (2006). Effects of age-related hearing loss on startle reflex and prepulse inhibition in mice on pure and mixed C57BL and 129 genetic background. Behavioural brain research, 172(2), 307-315.

      Young, J. W., Wallace, C. K., Geyer, M. A., & Risbrough, V. B. (2010). Age-associated improvements in cross-modal prepulse inhibition in mice. Behavioral neuroscience, 124(1), 133.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major concerns:

      (1) In Figure 1, the authors used different metrics for fEPSP strength. In Figure 1D, the authors used the slope, while they used the amplitude in Figure 1G. It is known that the two metrics are different from each other. While the slope is calculated from the linear regression between the voltage change per time of the rising phase of the fEPSP, the amplitude represents the voltage value of the fEPSP's peak. Please clarify here and in the method what metric you used, because the two terms are not interchangeable.

      Thank you for pointing out this oversight in our manuscript. We confirm that we used the slope of the fEPSP as the metric for assessing synaptic strength throughout the study, including both Figure 1D and Figure 1G. We will make the necessary corrections to ensure clarity and consistency. Thank you for bringing this to our attention.

      (2) It is not mentioned in the details of the methods about the CCK-KO mice. Please give such details. Although the authors used the CCK-KO mouse model as a control, I think that it is not a good choice to test the hypothesis mentioned in lines 165 and 166. The experiment was supposed to monitor the CCK-BR activity after HFS of the MGB and answer whether the CCK-BR will get activated by thalamic stimulation, but the CCK-KO mouse does not have CCK to be released after the optogenetic activation of the Chrimson probe. Therefore, it is expected to give nothing as if the experimenter runs an experiment without intervention. I think that the appropriate way to examine the hypothesis is to compare mice that were either injected with AAV9-Syn-FLEX-ChrimsonR-tdTomato or AAV9-Syn-FLEX-tdTomato. However, CCK-OK would be a perfect model to confirm that LTP can be only generated dependently on CCK, by simply running the HFS of the MGB that would be associated with the cortical recording of the fEPSP. This also will rule out the assumption that the authors mentioned in lines 191 and 192.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. The rationale behind our experimental design was to validate the newly developed CCK sensor and confirm its specificity. We aimed to verify CCK release post-HFS by comparing the responses of the CCK sensor in CCK-KO mice and CCK-Cre mice. This comparison allowed us to determine that the observed increase in fluorescence intensity post-HFS was specifically due to CCK release, rather than other neurotransmitters induced by HFS.

      We appreciate your suggestion to compare mice injected with AAV9-Syn-FLEX-ChrimsonR-tdTomato and AAV9-Syn-FLEX-tdTomato, as it is indeed a valuable approach for directly testing the hypothesis regarding CCK-BR activation. However, we prioritized using the CCK-KO model to validate the CCK sensor's efficacy and specificity. The validation can be inferred by comparing the CCK sensor activity before and after HFS.

      Regarding concerns mentioned in lines 191 and 192 about potential CCK release from other projections via indirect polysynaptic activation, CCK-KO mice were not suitable for this aspect due to their global knockout of CCK. To address this limitation, we utilized shRNA to specifically down-regulate Cck expression in MGB neurons. This approach focused on the necessity of CCK released from thalamocortical projections for the observed LTP and effectively ruled out the possibility of indirect polysynaptic activation.

      We also acknowledge that the methods section lacked sufficient details about the CCK-KO mice, which may have caused confusion. In the revised methods section, we will add the following details:

      (1) The genotype of the CCK-KO mice used in this study (CCK-ires-CreERT2, Jax#012710).

      (2) A brief description of the CCK-KO validation, emphasizing the absence of CCK mRNA in these mice (as shown in Figure 3A and 3B).

      (3) The experimental purpose of using CCK-KO mice to validate the specificity of the CCK sensor.

      We believe these additions will clarify the rationale for using CCK-KO mice and their role in this study. Thank you again for highlighting these important points.

      (3) Figure 3C: The authors should examine if there is a difference in the baseline of fEPSPs across different age groups as the dependence on the normalization in the analysis within each group would hide if there were any difference of the baseline slope of fEPSP between groups which could be related to any misleading difference after HFS. Also, I wonder about the absence of LTP in P20, which is a closer age to the critical period. Could the authors discuss that, please?

      Thank you for your insightful feedback. To address your concern regarding baseline differences in fEPSP slopes across age groups, we conducted additional analysis. Baseline fEPSP across the three groups (P20, 8w, 18m), normalized to the 8w group, were 64.8± 13.1%, 100.0 ± 20.4%, and 58.8± 10.3%, respectively. While there was a trend suggesting smaller fEPSP slopes in the P20 and 18m groups compared to the young adult group, these differences were not statistically significant due to data variability (P20 vs. 8w, P = 0.319; 8w vs. 18m, P=0.147; P20 vs. 18m, P = 1.0, one-way ANOVA). These results suggest that baseline variability is unlikely to confound the observed differences in LTP after HFS. Furthermore, we ensured that normalization minimized any potential baseline effects.

      Regarding the absence of LTP in P20, this likely reflects developmental regulation of CCKBR expression in the auditory cortex (ACx). The HFS-induced thalamocortical LTP observed in our study is CCK-dependent and mechanistically distinct from the NMDA-dependent thalamocortical LTP during the critical period. Specifically, correlated pre- and postsynaptic activity can induce NMDA-dependent thalamocortical LTP only during an early critical period corresponding to the first several postnatal days, after which this pairing becomes ineffective starting from the second postnatal week (Crair and Malenka, 1995; Isaac et al., 1997; Chun et al., 2013). In contrast, the CCK-dependent Thalamocortical LTP induced by HFS is robust in adult mice but appears absent in P20, likely due to the lack of postsynaptic CCKBR expression in the ACx at this developmental stage.

      We will include these clarifications in the revised manuscript, particularly in the Discussion section, to provide a more comprehensive explanation of our findings. Thank you for your valuable comments and suggestions.

      Crair, M.C., and Malenka, R.C. (1995). A critical period for long-term potentiation at thalamocortical synapses. Nature 375, 325-328. 10.1038/375325a0.

      Isaac, J.T.R., Crair, M.C., Nicoll, R.A., and Malenka, R.C. (1997). Silent Synapses during Development of Thalamocortical Inputs. Neuron 18, 269-280. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80267-6.

      Chun, S., Bayazitov, I.T., Blundon, J.A., and Zakharenko, S.S. (2013). Thalamocortical Long-Term Potentiation Becomes Gated after the Early Critical Period in the Auditory Cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience 33, 7345-7357. 10.1523/jneurosci.4500-12.2013.

      (4) Figure 4F: It is noticed that the baseline fEPSP of the CCK group and ACSF groups were different, which raises a concern about the baseline differences between treatment groups.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback and for pointing out this important detail. We apologize for any confusion caused by the presentation of the data. As noted in the figure legend, the scale bars for the fEPSPs were different between the left (0.1 mV) and right panels (20 µV). This difference in scale may have created the perception of baseline differences between the CCK and ACSF groups. To enhance clarity and avoid potential misunderstanding, we will unify the scale bar values in the revised figure. This adjustment will provide a clearer and more accurate comparison of fEPSPs between groups. Thank you again for bringing this issue to our attention.

      (5) From Figure S2D, it seems that different animals were injected with the drug and ACSF. Therefore, how the authors validate the position of the recording electrode to the cortical area of certain CF and relative EF. Also, there is not enough information about the basis of the selection of the EF. Should it be lower than the CF with a certain value? Was the EF determined after the initial tuning curve in each case? To mitigate this difference, it would be appropriate if the authors examined the presence of a significant difference in the tuning width and CFs between animals exposed to ACSF and CCK-4. This will give some validation of a balanced experiment between ACSF and CCK-4. I wonder also why the authors used rats here not mice, as it will be easier to interpret the results came from the same species.

      Thank you for your thoughtful comments. The effective frequency (EF) was determined after measuring the initial tuning curve for each case. The EF was selected to elicit a clear sound response while maintaining a sufficient distance from the characteristic frequency (CF) to allow measurable increases in response intensity. Specifically, EF was selected based on the starting point of the tuning peak, which corresponds to the onset of its fastest rising phase. From this point, EF was determined by moving 0.2 or 0.4 octaves toward the CF. While there were individual differences in EF selection among animals, the methodology for determining EF was standardized and applied consistently across both the ACSF and CCK-4 groups.

      Regarding the use of rats in these experiments, these studies were conducted prior to our current work with mice. The findings in rat provide valuable insights that support our current results in mice. Since the rat data are supplementary to the primary findings, we included them as supplementary material to provide additional context and validation. Furthermore, in consideration of animal welfare, we chose not to replicate these experiments in mice, as the findings from rats were sufficient to support our conclusions.

      Methods section:

      “The tuning curve was determined by plotting the lowest intensity at which the neuron responded to different tones. The characteristic frequency (CF) is defined as the frequency corresponding to the lowest point on this curve. The effective frequency (EF) was determined to elicit a clear sound response while maintaining a sufficient distance from the CF to allow measurable increases in response intensity. Specifically, EF was selected based on the starting point of the tuning peak, which corresponds to the onset of its fastest rising phase. From this point, EF was determined by moving 0.2 or 0.4 octaves toward the CF.”

      (6) Lines 384-386: There are no figures named 5H and I.

      Thank you for pointing this out. The references to Figures 5H and 5I were incorrect and should have referred to Figures 5C and 5D. We sincerely apologize for this oversight and will correct these errors in the revised manuscript to ensure clarity and accuracy. Thank you again for bringing this to our attention.

      (7) The authors should mention the sex of the animals used.

      Thank you for your comment and for highlighting this important detail. The sex of the animals used in this study is specified in the Animals section of the Methods: "In the present study, male mice and rats were used to investigate thalamocortical LTP." We appreciate your careful attention to this point and will ensure that this detail remains clearly stated in the manuscript.

      (8) Lines 534 and 648: These coordinates are difficult to understand. Since the experiment was done on both mice and rats, we need a clear description of the coordinates in both. Also, I think that you should mention the lateral distance from the sagittal suture as the ventral coordinates should be calculated from the surface of the skull above the AC and not from the sagittal suture.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback and for pointing out this important issue. We apologize for any confusion caused by our description of the coordinates. The term “ventral” was deliberately used because the auditory cortex is located on the lateral side of the skull, which may have caused some misunderstanding.

      To provide a clearer and more accurate descriptions of the coordinates, we will revise the text in the manuscript as follows: “A craniotomy was performed at the temporal bone (-2 to -4 mm posterior and -1.5 to -3 mm ventral to bregma for mice; -3.0 to -5.0 mm posterior and -2.5 to -6.5 mm ventral to bregma for rats) to access the auditory cortex.'

      We appreciate your attention to these details and will ensure that the revised manuscript includes this clarification to improve accuracy and eliminate potential confusion. Thank you again for bringing this to our attention.

      (9) Line 536: The author should specify that these coordinates are for the experiment done on mice.

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. We will revise the manuscript to explicitly specify that these coordinates refer to the experiments conducted on mice. This clarification will help improve the clarity and precision of the manuscript. We greatly appreciate your attention to this point and your effort to enhance the quality of our work.

      Methods section:

      “and a hole was drilled in the skull according to the coordinates of the ventral division of the MGB (MGv, AP: -3.2 mm, ML: 2.1 mm, DV: 3.0 mm) for experiments conducted on mice.”

      (10) Line 590: Please add the specifications of the stimulating electrode. Is it unipolar or bipolar? What is the cat.# provided by FHC?

      Thank you for your valuable feedback. The electrodes used in the experiments are unipolar. We will include the catalog number provided by FHC in the revised manuscript for clarity. The revised text will be updated as follows:

      “In HFS-induced thalamocortical LTP experiments, two customized microelectrode arrays with four tungsten unipolar electrodes each, impedance: 0.5-1.0 MΩ (recording: CAT.# UEWSFGSECNND, FHC, U.S.), and 200-500 kΩ (stimulating: CAT.# UEWSDGSEBNND, FHC, U.S.), were used for the auditory cortical neuronal activity recording and MGB ES, respectively.”

      We appreciate your attention to this detail, and we will ensure that the revised manuscript reflects this clarification accurately.

      (11) Lines 612-614: There are no details of how the optic fiber was inserted or post-examined. If there is a word limitation, the authors may reference another study showing these procedures.

      Thank you for your insightful comment and for highlighting this important aspect of the methodology. To address this, we will reference the study by Sun et al. (2024) in the revised manuscript, which provides detailed procedures for optic fiber insertion and post-examination. We believe that this reference will help enhance the clarity and completeness of the methods section.

      Sun, W., Wu, H., Peng, Y., Zheng, X., Li, J., Zeng, D., Tang, P., Zhao, M., Feng, H., Li, H., et al. (2024). Heterosynaptic plasticity of the visuo-auditory projection requires cholecystokinin released from entorhinal cortex afferents. eLife 13, e83356. 10.7554/eLife.83356.

      We appreciate your valuable suggestion, which will contribute to improving the quality of the manuscript.

      Minor concerns:

      (1) The definition of HFS was repeated many times throughout the manuscript. Please mention the defined name for the first time in the manuscript only followed by its abbreviation (HFS).

      Thank you for your suggestion and for pointing out this important detail. We will revise the manuscript to ensure that all abbreviations are defined only upon their first mention in the manuscript, with subsequent mentions using the abbreviations consistently. We appreciate your careful attention to detail and your effort to help improve the manuscript.

      (2) Line 173: There is a difference between here and the methods section (620 nm here and 635 nm there) please correct which wavelength the authors used.

      Thank you for your careful review and for bringing this discrepancy to our attention. We have corrected the inconsistency, and the wavelength has been unified throughout the manuscript to ensure accuracy and clarity. The revised text now reads as follows:

      “The fluorescent signal was monitored for 25s before and 60s after the HFLS (5~10 mW, 620 nm) or HFS application.”

      We appreciate your valuable feedback, which has helped us improve the precision and consistency of the manuscript.

      (3) Line 185: I think the authors should refer to Figure 2G before mentioning the statistical results.

      Thank you for your careful review and for pointing out this oversight. We have now added a reference to Figure 2G at the appropriate location to ensure clarity and logical flow in the manuscript, as recommended..

      (4) Line 202: I think the authors should refer to Figure 2J before mentioning the statistical results.

      Thank you again for your careful review and for highlighting this point. We have revised the manuscript to include a reference to Figure 2J before mentioning the statistical results.

      We appreciate your valuable feedback, which has helped us improve the accuracy and presentation of the results.

      (5) Line 260: Please add appropriate references at the end of the sentence to support the argument.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. To address this, we have add appropriate references to support the statement regarding the multiple steps involved between mRNA expression and neuropeptide release. Additionally, we have revised the statement to adopt a more cautious interpretation. The revised text is as follows:

      “It is widely recognized that mRNA levels do not always directly correlate with peptide levels due to multiple steps involved in peptide synthesis and processing, including translation, post-translational modifications, packaging, transportation, and proteolytic cleavage, all of which require various enzymes and regulatory mechanisms (38-41). A disruption at any stage in this process could lead to impaired CCK release, even when Cck mRNA is present.”

      We have included the following references to support this statement:

      38. Mierke, C.T. (2020). Translation and Post-translational Modifications in Protein Biosynthesis. In Cellular Mechanics and Biophysics: Structure and Function of Basic Cellular Components Regulating Cell Mechanics, C.T. Mierke, ed. (Springer International Publishing), pp. 595-665. 10.1007/978-3-030-58532-7_14.

      39. Gualillo, O., Lago, F., Casanueva, F.F., and Dieguez, C. (2006). One ancestor, several peptides post-translational modifications of preproghrelin generate several peptides with antithetical effects. Mol Cell Endocrinol 256, 1-8. 10.1016/j.mce.2006.05.007.

      40. Sossin, W.S., Fisher, J.M., and Scheller, R.H. (1989). Cellular and molecular biology of neuropeptide processing and packaging. Neuron 2, 1407-1417. https://doi.org/10.1016/0896-6273(89)90186-4.

      41. Hook, V., Funkelstein, L., Lu, D., Bark, S., Wegrzyn, J., and Hwang, S.R. (2008). Proteases for processing proneuropeptides into peptide neurotransmitters and hormones. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol 48, 393-423. 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.48.113006.094812.

      We greatly appreciate your helpful feedback, which has allowed us to improve both the accuracy and the depth of discussion in the manuscript.

      (6) Line 278: The authors mentioned "due to the absence of CCK in aged animals", which was not an appropriate description. It should be a reduction of CCK gene expression or a possible deficient CCK release.

      Thank you for your careful review and for pointing out the inaccuracy in our description. We agree with your suggestion and have revised the statement to more appropriately reflect the findings.

      “Our findings revealed that thalamocortical LTP cannot be induced in aged mice, likely due to insufficient CCK release, despite intact CCKBR expression.”

      This revision ensures a more accurate and precise description of the potential mechanisms underlying the observed phenomenon. We greatly appreciate your valuable feedback, which has helped us improve the clarity and accuracy of the manuscript.

      (7) Line 291: The authors mentioned that "without MGB stimulation", which is confusing. The MGB was stimulated with a single electrical pulse to evoke cortical fEPSPs. Therefore it should be "without HFS of MGB".

      Thank you for pointing this out and for highlighting the potential confusion caused by our original phrasing. Upon review, we recognize that our original phrasing "without MGB stimulation" may have been unclear and could have led to misinterpretation. To clarify, our intention was to describe the period during which CCK was present without any stimulation of the MGB.

      It is important to note that, in the presence of CCK, LTP can be induced even with low-frequency stimulation, including in aged mice. This observation underscores the potent effect of CCK in facilitating thalamocortical LTP, regardless of the specific stimulation protocol used.

      To address this issue, we have revised the sentence for improved clarity as follows::

      " To investigate whether CCK alone is sufficient to induce thalamocortical LTP without activating thalamocortical projections, we infused CCK-4 into the ACx of young adult mice immediately after baseline fEPSPs recording. Stimulation was then paused for 15 min to allow for CCK degradation, after which recording resumed."

      We believe this revision resolves the misunderstanding and provides a clearer and more accurate description of the experimental context. We greatly appreciate your insightful feedback, which has helped us refine the manuscript for clarity and precision.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor comments:

      (1) Line 99, 134, possibly other locations: "site" to "sites".

      Thank you for your careful review. We appreciate your attention to detail and have made the necessary corrections in the manuscript.

      (2) Throughout the manuscript there are some minor issues with language choice and subtle phrasing errors and I suggest English language editing.

      Thank you for your suggestion. In response, we have thoroughly reviewed the manuscript and addressed issues related to language choice and phrasing. The text has been carefully edited to ensure clarity, precision, and consistency. We believe these revisions have significantly enhanced the overall quality of the manuscript. We greatly appreciate your feedback, which has been invaluable in improving the presentation of our work.

      (3) Based on the experimental configurations, I do not think it is a problematic caveat, but authors should be aware of the high likelihood of AAV9 jumping synapses relative to other AAV serotypes.

      Thank you for bringing up the potential of AAV9 crossing synapses, a recognized characteristic of this serotype. We appreciate your observation regarding its relevance to our experimental design. In our study, we carefully considered the possibility of trans-synaptic transfer during both the experimental design and data interpretation phases. To minimize the likelihood of significant trans-synaptic spread, we implemented several measures, including controlling the injection volume, using a slow injection rate, and limiting the viral expression time. Post-hoc histological analyses confirmed that the expression of AAV9 was largely confined to the intended regions, with limited evidence of synaptic jumping under our experimental conditions.

      While we acknowledge the inherent potential for AAV9 to cross synapses, we believe this effect does not substantially confound the interpretation of our findings in the current study. To address this concern, we have added a brief discussion on this point in the revised manuscript to enhance clarity. We greatly appreciate your insightful comment, which has helped us further refine our work.

      Discussion section:

      “ One potential limitation of our study is the trans-synaptic transfer property of AAV9. To mitigate this, we carefully controlled the injection volume, rate, and viral expression time, and conducted post-hoc histological analyses to minimize off-target effects, thereby reducing the likelihood of trans-synaptic transfer confounding the interpretation of our findings.”

      (4) The trace identifiers (1-4) do not seem correctly placed/colored in Figure S1D. Please check others carefully.

      Thank you for your careful review and for bringing this issue to our attention. We have corrected the trace identifiers in Figure S1D. Additionally, we have carefully reviewed all other figures to ensure their accuracy and consistency. We greatly appreciate your attention to detail, which has helped improve the overall quality of the manuscript.

      (5) Please provide a value of the laser power range based on calibrated values.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have included the calibrated laser power range in the revised manuscript as follows:

      “The laser stimulation was produced by a laser generator (5-20 mW(30), Wavelength: 473 nm, 620 nm; CNI laser, China) controlled by an RX6 system and delivered to the brain via an optic fiber (Thorlabs, U.S.) connected to the generator.”

      We appreciate your feedback, which has helped improve the clarity and precision of our methodological description.

      (6) It would be useful to annotate figures in a way that identifies in which transgenic mice experiments are being performed.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. We will add annotations to the figures to explicitly identify the type of mice used in each experiment. We believe this enhancement will improve the clarity and accessibility of our results. We greatly appreciate your input in making our manuscript more informative.

      (7) Please comment on the rigor you use to address the accuracy of viral injections. How often did they spread outside of the MGB/AC?

      Thank you for raising this important question regarding the accuracy of viral injections and the potential spread outside the MGB or AC. Below, we provide details for each set of experiments:

      shRNA Experiments:

      For the shRNA experiments targeting the MGB, our primary goal was to achieve comprehensive coverage of the entire MGB. To this end, we used larger injection volumes and multiple injection sites, which inevitably resulted in some viral spread beyond the MGB. However, this approach was necessary to ensure robust knockdown effects that were representative of the entire MGB. While strict confinement to specific subregions could not be guaranteed, this strategy allowed us to prioritize the effectiveness of the knockdown within the target region.

      Fiber photometry Experiments:

      For the fiber photometry experiments targeting the auditory cortex (AC), we used larger injection volumes and multiple injection sites to cover its relatively large size. Although this approach might have resulted in some CCK-sensor virus spread outside the AC, the placement of the optic fiber was guided by the location of the auditory cortex. Consequently, any minor viral expression outside the AC would not affect the experimental results, as recordings were confined to the intended area through precise fiber placement.  

      Optogenetic Experiments:

      For the optogenetic experiments targeting the MGB, we specifically injected virus into the MGv subregion. To minimize viral spread, we employed several strategies, including the used fine injection needles, waiting for tissue stabilization (7 minutes post-needle insertion), delivering small volumes at a slow rate to prevent backflow, aspirating 5 nL of the solution post-injection, and raising the needle by 100 μm before waiting an additional 5 minutes prior to full retraction. These measures significantly reduced the risk of viral leakage to adjacent regions.

      Histological Validation:

      After the electrophysiological experiments, we systematically verified the accuracy of viral expression by examining histological sections to ensure that the expression was primarily localized within the intended regions.

      Terminology in the Manuscript:

      In the manuscript, we deliberately used the term "MGB" in the manuscript rather than specifically "MGv" to transparently acknowledge the potential for viral spread in some experiments.

      We hope this explanation clarifies the strategies we employed to address the accuracy of viral injections, as well as how we managed potential viral spread. We have also added a brief information in the revised manuscript to reflect these points and acknowledge the inherent variability in viral delivery.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We thank the reviewers for their constructive and helpful comments, which led us to make major changes in the model and manuscript, including adding the results of new experiments and analyses. We believe that the revised manuscript is much better than the previous version and that it addresses all issued raised by the reviewers. 

      Summary of changes made in the revised manuscript:

      (1) We increased the training set size from 39 video clips to 97 video clips and the testing set size from 25 video clips to 60 video clips. The increase in training set size improved the overall accuracy from a mean F1 score of 0.81 in the previous version to a mean F1 score of 0.891 (see Figure 2 and Figure 3) in the current version. Specifically, the F1 score for urine detection was improved from 0.79 to 0.88.

      (2) We further evaluated the accuracy of the DeePosit algorithm in comparison to a second human annotator and found that the algorithm accuracy is comparable to human-level accuracy.

      (3) The additional test videos allowed us to test the consistency of the algorithm performance across gender, space, time, and experiment type (SP, SxP, and ESPs). We found consistent levels of performance across all categories (see Figure 3), suggesting that errors made by the algorithm are uniform across conditions, hence should not create any bias of the results.

      (4) In addition, we tested the algorithm performance on a second strain of mice (male C57BL/6) in a different environmental condition (white arena instead of a black one) and found that the algorithm achieves comparable accuracy, even though C57BL/6 mice and white arena were not included in the training set. Thus, the algorithm seems to be robust and efficient across various experimental conditions.

      (5) Analyzing urination and defecation dynamics in an additional strain of mice revealed interesting strain-specific features, as discussed in the revised manuscript.

      (6) Overall, we found DeePosit accuracy to be stable with no significant bias across stages of the experiment, types of the experiment, gender of the mice, strain of mice, and across experimental conditions.

      (7) We also compared the performance of DeePosit to a classic object detection algorithm: YOLOv8. We trained YOLOv8 both on a single image input (YOLOv8 Gray) and on 3 image inputs representing a sequence of three time points around the ground truth event (t): t+0, t+10, and t+30 seconds (YOLOv8 RGB). DeePosit achieved significantly better accuracy over both YOLOv8 alternatives. YOLOv8 RGB achieved better accuracy than YOLOv8 Gray, suggesting that temporal information is important for this task. It's worth mentioning that while YOLOv8 requires the annotator to draw rectangles surrounding each urine spot or feces as part of the training set, our algorithm training set used just a single click inside each spot, allowing faster generation of training sets. 

      (8) As for the algorithm parameters, we tested the effect of the main parameter of the preliminary detection (the temperature threshold for the detection of a new blob) and found that a threshold of 1.6°C gave the best accuracy and used this parameter for all of the experiments instead of 1.1°C which was used in the original manuscript. It's worth mentioning that the performance is quite stable (mean F1 score of 0.88-0.89) for the thresholds between 1.1°C and 3°C (Figure 3—Figure Supplement 2).

      (9) We also checked if changing the input length of the video clip that is fed to the classifier affects the accuracy by training the classifier with -11..30 seconds video clips (41 seconds in total) instead of -11..60 seconds (71 seconds in total) and found no difference in accuracy. 

      (10) In the revised paper, we report recall, precision, and F1 scores in the caption of the relevant figures and also supply Excel files with the full statistics for each of the figures.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript provides a novel method for the automated detection of scent marks from urine and feces in rodents. Given the importance of scent communication in these animals and their role as model organisms, this is a welcome tool.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of our tool

      Strengths:

      The method uses a single video stream (thermal video) to allow for the distinction between urine and feces. It is automated.

      Weaknesses:

      The accuracy level shown is lower than may be practically useful for many studies. The accuracy of urine is 80%. 

      We have trained the model better, using a larger number of video clips. The increase in training set size improved the overall accuracy from a mean F1 score of 0.81 in the previous version to a mean F1 score of 0.891 (see Figure 2 and Figure 3) in the current version. Specifically, the F1 score for urine detection was improved from 0.79 to 0.88. 

      This is understandable given the variability of urine in its deposition, but makes it challenging to know if the data is accurate. If the same kinds of mistakes are maintained across many conditions it may be reasonable to use the software (i.e., if everyone is under/over counted to the same extent). Differences in deposition on the scale of 20% would be challenging to be confident in with the current method, though differences of the magnitude may be of biological interest. Understanding how well the data maintain the same relative ranking of individuals across various timing and spatial deposition metrics may help provide further evidence for the utility of the method.

      The additional test videos allowed us to test the consistency of the algorithm performance across gender, space, time and experiment type (SP, SxP, and ESP). We found consistent levels of performance across all categories (see Figure 3), suggesting that errors made by the algorithm are uniform across conditions, hence should not create any bias of the results.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors built a tool to extract the timing and location of mouse urine and fecal deposits in their laboratory set up. They indicate that they are happy with the results they achieved in this effort.

      Yes, we are.

      The authors note urine is thought to be an important piece of an animal's behavioral repertoire and communication toolkit so methods that make studying these dynamics easier would be impactful.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of our work.

      Strengths:

      With the proposed method, the authors are able to detect 79% of the urine that is present and 84% of the feces that is present in a mostly automated way.

      Weaknesses:

      The method proposed has a large number of design choices across two detection steps that aren't investigated. I.e. do other design choices make the performance better, worse, or the same? 

      We chose to use a heuristic preliminary detection algorithm for the detection of warm blobs, since warm blobs can be robustly detected with heuristic algorithms without the need for a training set. This design selection might allow easier adaptation of our algorithm for different types of arenas. Another advantage of using a heuristic preliminary detection is the easy control of the preliminary detection parameters such as the minimum temperature difference for detecting a blob, size limits of the detected blob, cooldown rate and so on that may help in adopting it to new conditions. As for the classifier, we chose to feed it with a relatively small window surrounding each preliminary detection, and hence it is not affected by the arena’s appearance outside of its region of interest. This should allow lower sensitivity to the arena’s appearance.  

      As for the algorithm parameters, we tested the effect of the main parameter of the preliminary detection (the temperature threshold for the detection of a new blob) and found that a threshold of 1.6°C gave the best accuracy and used this parameter for all of the experiments instead of 1.1°C which was used in the original manuscript. It's worth mentioning that the performance is quite stable (mean F1 score of 0.88-0.89) for the thresholds between 1.1°C and 3°.

      We also checked if changing the input length of the video clip fed to the classifier affects the accuracy by training the classifier with -11..30 seconds video clips (41 seconds in total) instead of -11..60 seconds (71 seconds in total) and found no difference in accuracy. 

      Overall, the algorithm's accuracy seems to be rather stable across various choices of parameters.

      Are these choices robust across a range of laboratory environments?

      We tested the algorithm performance on a second strain of mice (male C57BL/6) in a different environmental condition (white arena instead of a black one) and found that the algorithm achieves comparable accuracy, even though C57BL/6 mice and white arena were not included in the training set. Thus, the algorithm seems to be robust and efficient across various experimental conditions.

      How much better are the demonstrated results compared to a simple object detection pipeline (i.e. FasterRCNN or YOLO on the raw heat images)?

      We compared the performance of DeePosit to a classic object detection algorithm: YOLOv8. We trained YOLOv8 both on a single image input (YOLOv8 Gray) and on 3 image inputs representing a sequence of three time points around the ground truth event (t): t+0, t+10, and t+30 seconds (YOLOv8 RGB). DeePosit achieved significantly better accuracy over both YOLOv8 alternatives. YOLOv8 RGB achieved better accuracy than YOLOv8 Gray, suggesting that temporal information is important for this task. It's worth mentioning that while YOLOv8 requires annotator to draw rectangles surrounding each urine spot or feces as part of the training set, our algorithm training set used just a single click inside each spot, allowing faster generation of a training sets. 

      The method is implemented with a mix of MATLAB and Python.

      That is right.

      One proposed reason why this method is better than a human annotator is that it "is not biased." While they may mean it isn't influenced by what the researcher wants to see, the model they present is still statistically biased since each object class has a different recall score. This wasn't investigated. In general, there was little discussion of the quality of the model. 

      We tested the consistency of the algorithm performance across gender, space, time and experiment type (SP, SxP, and ESP). We found consistent levels of performance across all categories (see Figure 3), suggesting that errors made by the algorithm are uniform across conditions, hence should ne create any bias of the results. Specifically, the detection accuracy is similar between urine and feces, hence should not impose a bias between the various object classes.

      Precision scores were not reported.

      In the revised paper we report recall, precision, and F1 scores in the caption of the relevant figures and also supply Excel files with the full statistics for each of the figures.

      Is a recall value of 78.6% good for the types of studies they and others want to carry out? What are the implications of using the resulting data in a study?

      We have trained the model better, using a larger number of video clips. The increase in training set size improved the overall accuracy from a mean F1 score of 0.81 in the previous version to a mean F1 score of 0.891 (see Figure 2 and Figure 3) in the current version. Specifically, the F1 score for urine detection was improved from 0.79 to 0.88. 

      How do these results compare to the data that would be generated by a "biased human?"

      We further evaluated the accuracy of the DeePosit algorithm in comparison to a second human annotator and found that the algorithm accuracy is comparable to human-level accuracy (Figure 3).

      5 out of the 6 figures in the paper relate not to the method but to results from a study whose data was generated from the method. This makes a paper, which, based on the title, is about the method, much longer and more complicated than if it focused on the method.

      We appreciate the reviewer's comment, but the analysis of this new dataset by DeePosit demonstrates how the algorithm may be used to reveal novel and distinguishable dynamics of urination and defecation activities during social interactions, which were not yet reported. 

      Also, even in the context of the experiments, there is no discussion of the implications of analyzing data that was generated from a method with precision and recall values of only 7080%. Surely this noise has an effect on how to correctly calculate p-values etc. Instead, the authors seem to proceed like the generated data is simply correct.

      As mentioned above, the increase in training set size improved the overall accuracy from a mean F1 score of 0.81 in the previous version to a mean F1 score of 0.891 (see Figure 2 and Figure 3) in the current version. Specifically, the F1 score for urine detection was improved from 0.79 to 0.88.  

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors introduce a tool that employs thermal cameras to automatically detect urine and feces deposits in rodents. The detection process involves a heuristic to identify potential thermal regions of interest, followed by a transformer network-based classifier to differentiate between urine, feces, and background noise. The tool's effectiveness is demonstrated through experiments analyzing social preference, stress response, and temporal dynamics of deposits, revealing differences between male and female mice.

      Strengths:

      The method effectively automates the identification of deposits

      The application of the tool in various behavioral tests demonstrates its robustness and versatility.

      The results highlight notable differences in behavior between male and female mice

      We thank the reviewer for the positive assessment of our work.

      Weaknesses:

      The definition of 'start' and 'end' periods for statistical analysis is arbitrary. A robustness check with varying time windows would strengthen the conclusions.

      In all the statistical tests conducted in the revised manuscript, we have used a time period of 4 minutes for the analysis. We did not used the last minute of each stage for the analysis since the input of DeePosit requires 1 minute of video after the event. Nevertheless, we also conducted the same tests using a 5-minute period and found similar results (Figure 5—Figure Supplement 1).

      The paper could better address the generalizability of the tool to different experimental setups, environments, and potentially other species.

      As mentioned above, we tested the algorithm performance on a second strain of mice (male C57BL/6) in a different environmental condition (white arena instead of a black one) and found that the algorithm achieves comparable accuracy, even though C57BL/6 mice and white arena were not included in the training set. Thus, the algorithm seems to be robust and efficient across various experimental conditions.

      The results are based on tests of individual animals, and there is no discussion of how this method could be generalized to experiments tracking multiple animals simultaneously in the same arena (e.g., pair or collective behavior tests, where multiple animals may deposit urine or feces).

      At the moment, the algorithm cannot be applied for multiple animals freely moving in the same arena. However, in the revised manuscript we explicitly discussed what is needed for adapting the algorithm to perform such analyses.

      Recommendations for the authors: 

      -  Add a note and/or perform additional calculations to show that the results do not depend on the specific definitions of 'start' and 'end' periods. For instance, vary the time window thresholds and recalculate the statistics using different windows (e.g., 1-5 minutes instead of 1-4 minutes).

      In all the statistical tests conducted in the revised manuscript, we have used a time period of 4 minutes for the analysis. We did not use the last minute of each stage for the analysis since the input of DeePosit requires 1 minute of video after the event. Nevertheless, we also conducted the same tests using a 5-minute period and found similar results (Figure 5—Figure Supplement 1).

      - Condense Figures 4, 5, and 6 to simplify the presentation. Focus on demonstrating the effectiveness of the tool rather than detailed experimental outcomes, as the primary contribution of this paper is methodological.

      We have added to the revised manuscript one technical figure (Figure 3) comparing the accuracy of the algorithm performance across gender, space, time, and experiment type (SP, SxP, and ESP) as well as comparing its performance to a second human annotator and to YOLOv8. One more partially technical figure (Figure 5) compares the results of the algorithm between white ICR mice in the black arena and black C57BL/6 mice in the white arena. Thus, only Figures 4 and 6 show detailed experimental outcomes.

      - Provide more detail on how the preliminary detection procedure and parameters might need adjustment for different experimental setups or conditions. Discuss potential adaptations for field settings or more complex environments.

      As for the algorithm parameters, we tested the effect of the main parameter of the preliminary detection (the temperature threshold for the detection of a new blob) and found that a threshold of 1.6°C gave the best accuracy and used this parameter for all of the experiments instead of 1.1°C which was used in the original manuscript. It's worth mentioning that the performance is quite stable (mean F1 score of 0.88-0.89) for the thresholds between 1.1°C and 3°.

      We also checked if changing the input length of the video clip that is fed to the classifier affects the accuracy by training the classifier with -11..30 seconds video clips (41 seconds in total) instead of -11..60 seconds (71 seconds in total) and found no difference in accuracy. 

      Overall, the algorithm's accuracy seems to be rather stable across various choices of parameters.

      Editor's note:

      Should you choose to revise your manuscript, please ensure your manuscript includes full statistical reporting including exact p-values wherever possible alongside the summary statistics (test statistic and df) and 95% confidence intervals. These should be reported for all key questions and not only when the p-value is less than 0.05 in the main manuscript.

      We have deposited the detailed statistics of each figure in https://github.com/davidpl2/DeePosit/tree/main/FigStat/PostRevision

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      eLife Assessment

      This valuable study investigates how hearing impairment affects neural encoding of speech, in particular the encoding of hierarchical linguistic information. The current analysis provides incomplete evidence that hearing impairment affects speech processing at multiple levels, since the novel analysis based on HM-LSTM needs further justification. The advantage of this method should also be further explained. The study can also benefit from building a stronger link between neural and behavioral data.

      We sincerely thank the editors and reviewers for their detailed and constructive feedback.

      We have revised the manuscript to address all of the reviewers’ comments and suggestions. The primary strength of our methods lies in the use of the HM-LSTM model, which simultaneously captures linguistic information at multiple levels, ranging from phonemes to sentences. As such, this model can be applied to other questions regarding hierarchical linguistic processing. We acknowledge that our current behavioral results from the intelligibility test may not fully differentiate between the perception of lower-level acoustic/phonetic information and higher-level meaning comprehension. However, it remains unclear what type of behavioral test would effectively address this distinction. We aim to xplore this connection further in future studies.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      The authors are attempting to use the internal workings of a language hierarchy model, comprising phonemes, syllables, words, phrases, and sentences, as regressors to predict EEG recorded during listening to speech. They also use standard acoustic features as regressors, such as the overall envelope and the envelopes in log-spaced frequency bands. This is valuable and timely research, including the attempt to show differences between normal-hearing and hearing-impaired people in these regards. I will start with a couple of broader questions/points, and then focus my comments on three aspects of this study: The HM-LSTM language model and its usage, the time windows of relevant EEG analysis, and the usage of ridge regression.

      Firstly, as far as I can tell, the OSF repository of code, data, and stimuli is not accessible without requesting access. This needs to be changed so that reviewers and anybody who wants or needs to can access these materials. 

      It is my understanding that keeping the repository private during the review process and making them public after acceptance is standard practice. As far as I understand, although the OSF repository was private, anyone with the link should be able to access it. I have now made the repository public.

      What is the quantification of model fit? Does it mean that you generate predicted EEG time series from deconvolved TRFs, and then give the R2 coefficient of determination between the actual EEG and predicted EEG constructed from the convolution of TRFs and regressors? Whether or not this is exactly right, it should be made more explicit.

      Model fit was measured by spatiotemporal cluster permutation tests (Maris & Oostenveld, 2007) on the contrasts of the timecourses of the z-transformed coefficient of determination (R<sup>2</sup>). For instance, to assess whether words from the attended stimuli better predict EEG signals during the mixed speech compared to words from the unattended stimuli, we used the 150dimensional vectors corresponding to the word layer from our LSTM model for the attended and unattended stimuli as regressors. We then fit these regressors to the EEG signals at 9 time points (spanning -100 ms to 300 ms around the sentence offsets, with 50 ms intervals). We then conducted one-tailed two-sample t-tests to determine whether the differences in the contrasts of the R<sup>2</sup> timecourses were statistically significant. Note that we did not perform TRF analyses. We have clarified this description in the “Spatiotemporal clustering analysis” section of the “Methods and Materials” on p.10 of the manuscript.

      About the HM-LSTM:

      • In the Methods paragraph about the HM-LSTM, a lot more detail is necessary to understand how you are using this model. Firstly, what do you mean that you "extended" it, and what was that procedure? 

      The original HM-LSTM model developed by Chung et al. (2017) consists of only two levels: the word level and the phrase level (Figure 1b from their paper). By “extending” the model, we mean that we expanded its architecture to include five levels: phoneme, syllable, word, phrase, and sentence. Since our input consists of phoneme embeddings, we cannot directly apply their model, so we trained our model on the WenetSpeech corpus (Zhang et al., 2021), which provides phoneme-level transcripts. We have added this clarification on p.4 of the manuscript.

      • And generally, this is the model that produces most of the "features", or regressors, whichever word we like, for the TRF deconvolution and EEG prediction, correct? 

      Yes, we extracted the 2048-dimensional hidden layer activity from the model to represent features for each sentence in our speech stimuli at the phoneme, syllable, word, phrase and sentence levels. But we did not perform any TRF deconvolution, we fit these features (downsampled to 150-dimension using PCA) to the EEG signals at 9 timepoints around the offset of each sentence using ridge regression. We have now added a multivariate TRF (mTRF) analysis following Reviewer 3’s suggestions, and the results showed similar patterns to the current results (see Figure S2). We have added the clarification in the “Ridge regression at different time latencies” section of the “Methods and Materials” on p.10 of the manuscript.

      Resutls from the mTRF analyses were added on p.7 of the manuscript.

      • A lot more detail is necessary then, about what form these regressors take, and some example plots of the regressors alongside the sentences.

      The linguistic regressors are just 5 150-dimensional vectors, each corresponding to one linguistic level, as shown in Figure 1B.

      • Generally, it is necessary to know what these regressors look like compared to other similar language-related TRF and EEG/MEG prediction studies. Usually, in the case of e.g. Lalor lab papers or Simon lab papers, these regressors take the form of single-sample event markers, surrounded by zeros elsewhere. For example, a phoneme regressor might have a sample up at the onset of each phoneme, and a word onset regressor might have a sample up at the onset of each word, with zeros elsewhere in the regressor. A phoneme surprisal regressor might have a sample up at each phoneme onset, with the value of that sample corresponding to the rarity of that phoneme in common speech. Etc. Are these regressors like that? Or do they code for these 5 linguistic levels in some other way? Either way, much more description and plotting is necessary in order to compare the results here to others in the literature.

      No, these regressors were not like that. They were 150-dimensional vectors (after PCA dimension reduction) extracted from the hidden layers of the HM-LSTM model. After training the model on the WenetSpeech corpus, we ran it on our speech stimuli and extracted representations from the five hidden layers to correspond to the five linguistic levels. As mentioned earlier, we did not perform TRF analyses; instead, we used ridge regression to predict EEG signals around the offset of each sentence, a method commonly employed in the literature (e.g., Caucheteux & King, 2022; Goldstein et al., 2022; Schmitt et al., 2021; Schrimpf et al., 2021). For instance, Goldstein et al. (2022) used word embeddings from GPT-2 to predict ECoG activity surrounding the onset of each word during naturalistic listening. We have included these literatures on p.3 in the manuscript, and the method is illustrated in Figure 1B.

      • You say that the 5 regressors that are taken from the trained model's hidden layers do not have much correlation with each other. However, the highest correlations are between syllable and sentence (0.22), and syllable and word (0.17). It is necessary to give some reason and interpretation of these numbers. One would think the highest correlation might be between syllable and phoneme, but this one is almost zero. Why would the syllable and sentence regressors have such a relatively high correlation with each other, and what form do those regressors take such that this is the case?

      All the regressors are represented as 2048-dimensional vectors derived from the hidden layers of the trained HM-LSTM model. We applied the trained model to all 284 sentences in our stimulus text, generating a set of 284 × 2048-dimensional vectors. Next, we performed Principal Component Analysis (PCA) on the 2048 dimensions and extracted the first 100 principal components (PCs), resulting in 284 × 100-dimensional vectors for each regressor. These 284 × 100 matrices were then flattened into 28,400-dimensional vectors. Subsequently, we computed the correlation matrix for the z-transformed 28,400-dimensional vectors of our five linguistic regressors. The code for this analysis, lstm_corr.py, can be found in our OSF repository. We have added a section “Correlation among linguistic features” in “Materials and Methods” on p.10 of the manuscript.

      We consider the observed coefficients of 0.17 and 0.22 to be relatively low compared to prior model-brain alignment studies which report correlation coefficients above 0.5 for linguistic regressors (e.g., Gao et al., 2024; Sugimoto et al., 2024). In Chinese, a single syllable can also function as a word, potentially leading to higher correlations between regressors for syllables and words. However, we refrained from overinterpreting the results to suggest a higher correlation between syllable and sentence compared to syllable and word. A paired ttest of the syllable-word coefficients versus syllable-sentence coefficients across the 284 sentences revealed no significant difference (t(28399)=-3.96, p=1). We have incorporated this information into p.5 of the manuscript.

      • If these regressors are something like the time series of zeros along with single sample event markers as described above, with the event marker samples indicating the onset of the relevant thing, then one would think e.g. the syllable regressor would be a subset of the phoneme regressor because the onset of every syllable is a phoneme. And the onset of every word is a syllable, etc.

      All the regressors are aligned to 9 time points surrounding sentence offsets (-100 ms to 300 ms with a 50 ms interval). This is because all our regressors are taken from the HM-LSTM model, where the input is the phoneme representation of a sentence (e.g., “zh ə_4 y ie_3 j iəu_4 x iaŋ_4 sh uei_3 y ii_2 y aŋ_4”). For each unit in the sentence, the model generates five 2048dimensional vectors, each corresponding to the five linguistic levels of the entire sentence. We have added the clarification on p.11 of the manuscript.

      For the time windows of analysis:

      • I am very confused, because sometimes the times are relative to "sentence onset", which would mean the beginning of sentences, and sometimes they are relative to "sentence offset", which would mean the end of sentences. It seems to vary which is mentioned. Did you use sentence onsets, offsets, or both, and what is the motivation?

      • If you used onsets, then the results at negative times would not seem to mean anything, because that would be during silence unless the stimulus sentences were all back to back with no gaps, which would also make that difficult to interpret.

      • If you used offsets, then the results at positive times would not seem to mean anything, because that would be during silence after the sentence is done. Unless you want to interpret those as important brain activity after the stimuli are done, in which case a detailed discussion of this is warranted.

      Thank you very much for pointing this out. All instances of “sentence onset” were typos and should be corrected to “sentence offset.” We chose offset because the regressors are derived from the hidden layer activity of our HM-LSTM model, which processes the entire sentence before generating outputs. We have now corrected all the typos. In continuous speech, there is no distinct silence period following sentence offsets. Additionally, lexical or phrasal processing typically occurs 200 ms after stimulus offsets (Bemis & Pylkkanen, 2011; Goldstein et al., 2022; Li et al., 2024; Li & Pylkkänen, 2021). Therefore, we included a 300 ms interval after sentence offsets in our analysis, as our regressors encompass linguistic levels up to the sentence level. We have added this motivation on p.11 of the manuscript.

      • For the plots in the figures where the time windows and their regression outcomes are shown, it needs to be explicitly stated every time whether those time windows are relative to sentence onset, offset, or something else.

      Completely agree and thank you very much for the suggestion. We have now added this information on Figure 4-6.

      • Whether the running correlations are relative to sentence onset or offset, the fact that you can have numbers outside of the time of the sentence (negative times for onset, or positive times for offset) is highly confusing. Why would the regressors have values outside of the sentence, meaning before or after the sentence/utterance? In order to get the running correlations, you presumably had the regressor convolved with the TRF/impulse response to get the predicted EEG first. In order to get running correlation values outside the sentence to correlate with the EEG, you would have to have regressor values at those time points, correct? How does this work?

      As mentioned earlier, we did not perform TRF analyses or convolve the regressors. Instead, we conducted regression analyses at each of the 9 time points surrounding the sentence offsets, following standard methods commonly used in model-brain alignment studies (e.g., Gao et al., 2024; Goldstein et al., 2022). The time window of -100 to 300 ms was selected based on prior findings that lexical and phrasal processing typically occurs 200–300 ms after word offsets (Bemis & Pylkkanen, 2011; Goldstein et al., 2022; Li et al., 2024; Li & Pylkkänen, 2021). Additionally, we included the -100 to 200 ms time period in our analysis to examine phoneme and syllable level processing (cf. Gwilliams et al., 2022). We have added the clarification on p. of the manuscript.

      • In general, it seems arbitrary to choose sentence onset or offset, especially if the comparison is the correlation between predicted and actual EEG over the course of a sentence, with each regressor. What is going on with these correlations during the middle of the sentences, for example? In ridge regression TRF techniques for EEG/MEG, the relevant measure is often the overall correlation between the predicted and actual, calculated over a longer period of time, maybe the entire experiment. Here, you have calculated a running comparison between predicted and actual, and thus the time windows you choose to actually analyze can seem highly cherry-picked, because this means that most of the data is not actually analyzed.

      The rationale for choosing sentence offsets instead of onsets is that we are aligning the HM-LSTM model’s activity with EEG responses, and the input to the model consists of phoneme representations of the entire sentence at one time. In other words, the model needs to process the whole sentence before generating representations at each linguistic level. Therefore, the corresponding EEG responses should also align with the sentence offsets, occurring after participants have seen the complete sentence. The ridge regression followed the common practice in model-brain alignment studies (e.g., Gao et al., 2024; Goldstein et al., 2022; Huth et al., 2016; Schmitt et al., 2021; Schrimpf et al., 2021), and the time window is not cherrypicked but based on prior literature reporting lexical and sublexical processing at these time period (e.g., Bemis & Pylkkanen, 2011; Goldstein et al., 2022; Gwilliams et al., 2022; Li et al., 2024; Li & Pylkkänen, 2021).

      • In figures 5 and 6, some of the time window portions that are highlighted as significant between the two lines have the lines intersecting. This looks like, even though you have found that the two lines are significantly different during that period of time, the difference between those lines is not of a constant sign, even during that short period. For instance, in figure 5, for the syllable feature, the period of 0 - 200 ms is significantly different between the two populations, correct? But between 0 and 50, normal-hearing are higher, between 50 and 150, hearing-impaired are higher, and between 150 and 200, normal-hearing are higher again, correct? But somehow they still end up significantly different overall between 0 and 200 ms. More explanation of occurrences like these is needed.

      The intersecting lines in Figures 5 and represent the significant time windows for withingroup comparisons (i.e., significant model fit compared to 0). They do not depict betweengroup comparisons, as no significant contrasts were found between the groups. For example, in Figure 1, the significant time windows for the acoustic models are shown separately for the hearing-impaired and normal-hearing groups. No significant differences were observed, as indicated by the sensor topography. We have now clarified this point in the captions for Figures 5 and 6.

      Using ridge regression:

      • What software package(s) and procedure(s) were specifically done to accomplish this? If this is ridge regression and not just ordinary least squares, then there was at least one non-zero regularization parameter in the process. What was it, how did it figure in the modeling and analysis, etc.?

      The ridge regression was performed using customary python codes, making heavy use of the sklearn (v1.12.0) package. We used ridge regression instead of ordinary least squares regression because all our linguistic regressors are 150-dimensional dense vectors, and our acoustic regressors are 130-dimension vectors (see “Acoustic features of the speech stimuli” in “Materials and Methods”). We kept the default regularization parameter (i.e., 1). This ridge regression methods is commonly used in model-brain alignment studies, where the regressors are high-dimensional vectors taken from language models (e.g., Gao et al., 2024; Goldstein et al., 2022; Huth et al., 2016; Schmitt et al., 2021; Schrimpf et al., 2021). The code ridge_lstm.py can be found in our OSF repository, and we have added the more detailed description on p.11 of the manuscript.

      • It sounds like the regressors are the hidden layer activations, which you reduced from 2,048 to 150 non-acoustic, or linguistic, regressors, per linguistic level, correct? So you have 150 regressors, for each of 5 linguistic levels. These regressors collectively contribute to the deconvolution and EEG prediction from the resulting TRFs, correct? This sounds like a lot of overfitting. How much correlation is there from one of these 150 regressors to the next? Elsewhere, it sounds like you end up with only one regressor for each of the 5 linguistic levels. So these aspects need to be clarified.

      • For these regressors, you are comparing the "regression outcomes" for different conditions; "regression outcomes" are the R2 between predicted and actual EEG, which is the coefficient of determination, correct? If this is R2, how is it that you have some negative numbers in some of the plots? R2 should be only positive, between 0 and 1.

      Yes we reduced 2048-dimensional vectors for each of the 5 linguistic levels to 150 using PCA, mainly for saving computational resources. We used ridge regression, following the standard practice in the field (e.g., Gao et al., 2024; Goldstein et al., 2022; Huth et al., 2016; Schmitt et al., 2021; Schrimpf et al., 2021). 

      Yes, the regression outcomes are the R<sup>2</sup> values representing the fit between the predicted and actual EEG data. However, we reported normalized R<sup>2</sup> values which are ztransformed in the plots. All our spatiotemporal cluster permutation analyses were conducted using the z-transformed R<sup>2</sup> values. We have added this clarification both in the figure captions and on p.11 of the manuscript. As a side note, R<sup>2</sup> values can be negative because they are not the square of a correlation coefficient. Rather, R<sup>2</sup> compares the fit of the chosen model to that of a horizontal straight line (the null hypothesis). If the chosen model fits the data worse than the horizontal line, then R<sup>2</sup> value becomes negative: https://www.graphpad.com/support/faq/how-can-rsup2sup-be-negative 

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      This study compares neural responses to speech in normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners, investigating how different levels of the linguistic hierarchy are impacted across the two cohorts, both in a single-talker and multi-talker listening scenario. It finds that, while normal-hearing listeners have a comparable cortical encoding of speech-in-quiet and attended speech from a multi-talker mixture, participants with hearing impairment instead show a reduced cortical encoding of speech when it is presented in a competing listening scenario. When looking across the different levels of the speech processing hierarchy in the multi-talker condition, normal-hearing participants show a greater cortical encoding of the attended compared to the unattended stream in all speech processing layers - from acoustics to sentencelevel information. Hearing-impaired listeners, on the other hand, only have increased cortical responses to the attended stream for the word and phrase levels, while all other levels do not differ between attended and unattended streams.

      The methods for modelling the hierarchy of speech features (HM-LSTM) and the relationship between brain responses and specific speech features (ridge-regression) are appropriate for the research question, with some caveats on the experimental procedure. This work offers an interesting insight into the neural encoding of multi-talker speech in listeners with hearing impairment, and it represents a useful contribution towards understanding speech perception in cocktail-party scenarios across different hearing abilities. While the conclusions are overall supported by the data, there are limitations and certain aspects that require further clarification.

      (1) In the multi-talker section of the experiment, participants were instructed to selectively attend to the male or the female talker, and to rate the intelligibility, but they did not have to perform any behavioural task (e.g., comprehension questions, word detection or repetition), which could have demonstrated at least an attempt to comply with the task instructions. As such, it is difficult to determine whether the lack of increased cortical encoding of Attended vs. Unattended speech across many speech features in hearing-impaired listeners is due to a different attentional strategy, which might be more oriented at "getting the gist" of the story (as the increased tracking of only word and phrase levels might suggest), or instead it is due to hearing-impaired listeners completely disengaging from the task and tuning back in for selected key-words or word combinations. Especially the lack of Attended vs. Unattended cortical benefit at the level of acoustics is puzzling and might indicate difficulties in performing the task. I think this caveat is important and should be highlighted in the Discussion section. RE: Thank you very much for the suggestion. We admit that the hearing-impaired listeners might adopt different attentional strategies or potentially disengage from the task due to comprehension difficulties. However, we would like to emphasize that our hearing-impaired participants have extended high-frequency (EHF) hearing loss, with impairment only at frequencies above 8 kHz. Their condition is likely not severe enough to cause them to adopt a markedly different attentional strategy for this task. Moreover, it is possible that our normalhearing listeners may also adopt varying attentional strategies, yet the comparison still revealed notable differences.We have added the caveat in the Discussion section on p.8 of the manuscript.

      (2) In the EEG recording and preprocessing section, you state that the EEG was filtered between 0.1Hz and 45Hz. Why did you choose this very broadband frequency range? In the literature, speech responses are robustly identified between 0.5Hz/1Hz and 8Hz. Would these results emerge using a narrower and lower frequency band? Considering the goal of your study, it might also be interesting to run your analysis pipeline on conventional frequency bands, such as Delta and Theta, since you are looking into the processing of information at different temporal scales.

      Indeed, we have decomposed the epoched EEG time series for each section into six classic frequency bands components (delta 1–3 Hz, theta 4–7 Hz, alpha 8–12 Hz, beta 12–20 Hz, gamma 30–45 Hz) by convolving the data with complex Morlet wavelets as implemented in MNE-Python (version 0.24.0). The number of cycles in the Morlet wavelets was set to frequency/4 for each frequency bin. The power values for each time point and frequency bin were obtained by taking the square root of the resulting time-frequency coefficients. These power values were normalized to reflect relative changes (expressed in dB) with respect to the 500 ms pre-stimulus baseline. This yielded a power value for each time point and frequency bin for each section. We specifically examined the delta and theta bands, and computed the correlation between the regression outcome (R<sup>2</sup> in the shape of number of subject * sensor * time were flattened for computing correlation) for the five linguistic predictors from these bands and those obtained using data from all frequency bands. The results showed high correlation coefficients (see the correlation matrix in Supplementary Figures S2 for the attended and unattended speech). Therefore, we opted to use the epoched EEG data from all frequency bands for our analyses. We have added this clarification in the Results section on p.5 and the “EEG recording and preprocessing” section in “Materials and Methods” on p.11 of the manuscript.

      (3) A paragraph with more information on the HM-LSTM would be useful to understand the model used without relying on the Chung et al. (2017) paper. In particular, I think the updating mechanism of the model should be clarified. It would also be interesting to modify the updating factor of the model, along the lines of Schmitt et al. (2021), to assess whether a HM-LSTM with faster or slower updates can better describe the neural activity of hearing-impaired listeners. That is, perhaps the difference between hearing-impaired and normal-hearing participants lies in the temporal dynamics, and not necessarily in a completely different attentional strategy (or disengagement from the stimuli, as I mentioned above).

      Thank you for the suggestion. We have added more details on our HM-LSTM model on p.10 “Hierarchical multiscale LSTM model” in “Materials and Methods”: Our HM-LSTM model consists of 4 layers, at each layer, the model implements a COPY or UPDATE operation at each time step t. The COPY operation maintains the current cell state of without any changes until it receives a summarized input from the lower layer. The UPDATE operation occurs when a linguistic boundary is detected in the layer below, but no boundary was detected at the previous time step t-1. In this case, the cell updates its summary representation, similar to standard RNNs. We agree that exploring modifications to the model’s updating factor would be an interesting direction. However, since we have already observed contrasts between normal-hearing and hearing-impaired listeners using the current model’s update parameters, we believe discussing additional hypotheses would overextend the scope of this paper.

      (4) When explaining how you extracted phoneme information, you mention that "the inputs to the model were the vector representations of the phonemes". It is not clear to me whether you extracted specific phonetic features (e.g., "p" sound vs. "b" sound), or simply the phoneme onsets. Could you clarify this point in the text, please?

      The model inputs were individual phonemes from two sentences, each transformed into a 1024-dimensional vector using a simple lookup table. This lookup table stores embeddings for a fixed dictionary of all unique phonemes in Chinese. This approach is a foundational technique in many advanced NLP models, enabling the representation of discrete input symbols in a continuous vector space. We have added this clarification on p.10 of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to investigate how the brain processes different linguistic units (from phonemes to sentences) in challenging listening conditions, such as multi-talker environments, and how this processing differs between individuals with normal hearing and those with hearing impairments. Using a hierarchical language model and EEG data, they sought to understand the neural underpinnings of speech comprehension at various temporal scales and identify specific challenges that hearing-impaired listeners face in noisy settings.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the combination of computational modeling, detailed EEG analysis, and comprehensive experimental design thoroughly investigates the neural mechanisms underlying speech comprehension in complex auditory environments.

      The use of a hierarchical language model (HM-LSTM) offers a data-driven approach to dissect and analyze linguistic information at multiple temporal scales (phoneme, syllable, word, phrase, and sentence). This model allows for a comprehensive neural encoding examination of how different levels of linguistic processing are represented in the brain.

      The study includes both single-talker and multi-talker conditions, as well as participants with normal hearing and those with hearing impairments. This design provides a robust framework for comparing neural processing across different listening scenarios and groups.

      Weaknesses:

      The analyses heavily rely on one specific computational model, which limits the robustness of the findings. The use of a single DNN-based hierarchical model to represent linguistic information, while innovative, may not capture the full range of neural coding present in different populations. A low-accuracy regression model-fit does not necessarily indicate the absence of neural coding for a specific type of information. The DNN model represents information in a manner constrained by its architecture and training objectives, which might fit one population better than another without proving the non-existence of such information in the other group. To address this limitation, the authors should consider evaluating alternative models and methods. For example, directly using spectrograms, discrete phoneme/syllable/word coding as features, and performing feature-based temporal response function (TRF) analysis could serve as valuable baseline models. This approach would provide a more comprehensive evaluation of the neural encoding of linguistic information.

      Our acoustic features are indeed direct the broadband envelopes and the log-mel spectrograms of the speech streams. The amplitude envelope of the speech signal was extracted using the Hilbert transform. The 129-dimension spectrogram and 1-dimension envelope were concatenated to form a 130-dimension acoustic feature at every 10 ms of the speech stimuli. Given the duration of our EEG recordings, which span over 10 minutes, conducting multivariate TRF (mTRF) analysis with such high-dimensional predictors was not feasible. Instead, we used ridge regression to predict EEG responses across 9 temporal latencies, ranging from -100 ms to +300 ms, with additional 50 ms latencies surrounding sentence offsets. To evaluate the model's performance, we extracted the R<sup>2</sup> values at each latency, providing a temporal profile of regression performance over the analyzed time period. This approach is conceptually similar to TRF analysis.

      We agree that including baseline models for the linguistic features is important, and we have now added results from mTRF analysis using phoneme, syllable, word, phrase, and sentence rates as discrete predictors (i.e., marking a value of 1 at each unit boundary offset). Our EEG data spans the entire 10-minute duration for each condition, sampled at 10-ms intervals. The TRF results for our main comparison—attended versus unattended conditions— showed similar patterns to those observed using features from our HM-LSTM model. At the phoneme and syllable levels, normal-hearing listeners showed marginally significantly higher TRF weights for attended speech compared to unattended speech at approximately -80 to 150 ms after phoneme offsets (t=2.75, Cohen’s d=0.87, p=0.057), and 120 to 210 ms after syllable offsets (t=3.96, Cohen’s d=0.73d = 0.73, p=0.083). At the word and phrase levels, normalhearing listeners exhibited significantly higher TRF weights for attended speech compared to unattended speech at 190 to 290 ms after word offsets (t=4, Cohen’s d=1.13, p=0.049), and around 120 to 290 ms after phrase offsets (t=5.27, Cohen’s d=1.09, p=0.045). For hearing-impaired listeners, marginally significant effects were observed at 190 to 290 ms after word offsets (t=1.54, Cohen’s d=0.6, p=0.059), and 180 to 290 ms after phrase offsets (t=3.63, Cohen’s d=0.89, p=0.09). These results have been added on p.7 of the manuscript, and the corresponding figure is included as Supplementary F2.

      It is not entirely clear if the DNN model used in this study effectively serves the authors' goal of capturing different linguistic information at various layers. Specifically, the results presented in Figure 3C are somewhat confusing. While the phonemes are labeled, the syllables, words, phrases, and sentences are not, making it difficult to interpret how the model distinguishes between these levels of linguistic information. The claim that "Hidden-layer activity for samevowel sentences exhibited much more similar distributions at the phoneme and syllable levels compared to those at the word, phrase and sentence levels" is not convincingly supported by the provided visualizations. To strengthen their argument, the authors should use more quantified metrics to demonstrate that the model indeed captures phrase, word, syllable, and phoneme information at different layers. This is a crucial prerequisite for the subsequent analyses and claims about the hierarchical processing of linguistic information in the brain.

      Quantitative measures such as mutual information, clustering metrics, or decoding accuracy for each linguistic level could provide clearer evidence of the model's effectiveness in this regard.

      In Figure 3C, we used color-coding to represent the activity of five hidden layers after dimensionality reduction. Each dot on the plot corresponds to one test sentence. Only phonemes are labeled because each syllable in our test sentences contains the same vowels (see Table S1). The results demonstrate that the phoneme layer effectively distinguishes different phonemes, while the higher linguistic layers do not. We believe these findings provide evidence that different layers capture distinct linguistic information. Additionally, we computed the correlation coefficients between each pair of linguistic predictors, as shown in Figure 3B. We think this analysis serves a similar purpose to computing the mutual information between pairs of hidden-layer activities for our constructed sentences. Furthermore, the mTRF results based on rate models of the linguistic features we presented earlier align closely with the regression results using the hidden-layer activity from our HM-LSTM model. This further supports the conclusion that our model successfully captures relevant information across these linguistic levels. We have added the clarification on p.5 of the manuscript.

      The formulation of the regression analysis is somewhat unclear. The choice of sentence offsets as the anchor point for the temporal analysis, and the focus on the [-100ms, +300ms] interval, needs further justification. Since EEG measures underlying neural activity in near real-time, it is expected that lower-level acoustic information, which is relatively transient, such as phonemes and syllables, would be distributed throughout the time course of the entire sentence. It is not evident if this limited time window effectively captures the neural responses to the entire sentence, especially for lower-level linguistic features. A more comprehensive analysis covering the entire time course of the sentence, or at least a longer temporal window, would provide a clearer understanding of how different linguistic units are processed over time. Additionally, explaining the rationale behind choosing this specific time window and how it aligns with the temporal dynamics of speech processing would enhance the clarity and validity of the regression analysis.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We chose this time window as lexical or phrasal processing typically occurs 200 ms after stimulus offsets (Bemis & Pylkkanen, 2011; Goldstein et al., 2022; Li et al., 2024; Li & Pylkkänen, 2021). Additionally, we included the -100 to 200 ms time period in our analysis to examine phoneme and syllable level processing (e.g., Gwilliams et al., 2022). Using the entire sentence duration was not feasible, as the sentences in the stimuli vary in length, making statistical analysis challenging. Additionally, since the stimuli consist of continuous speech, extending the time window would risk including linguistic units from subsequent sentences. This would introduce ambiguity as to whether the EEG responses correspond to the current or the following sentence. We have added this clarification on p.12 of the manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      As I mentioned, I think the OSF repo needs to be changed to give anyone access. I would recommend pursuing the lines of thought I mentioned in the public review to make this study complete and to allow it to fit into the already existing literature to facilitate comparisons.

      Yes the OSF folder is now public. We have made revisions following all reviewers’ suggestions.

      There are some typos in figure labels, e.g. 2B.

      Thank you for pointing it out! We have now revised the typo in Figure 2B.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) I was able to access all of the audio files and code for the study, but no EEG data was shared in the OSF repository. Unless there is some ethical and/or legal constraint, my understanding of eLife's policy is that the neural data should be made publicly available as well.

      The preprocessed EEG data in .npy format in the OSF repository. 

      (2) The line-plots in Figures 4B,5B, and 6B have very similar colours. They would be easier to interpret if you changed the line appearance as well as the colours. E.g., dotted line for hearingimpaired listeners, thick line for normal-hearing.

      Thank you for the suggestion! We have now used thicker lines for normal-impaired listeners in all our line plots.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The authors may consider presenting raw event-related potentials (ERPs) or spatiotemporal response profiles before delving into the more complex regression encoding analysis. This would provide a clearer foundational understanding of the neural activity patterns. For example, it is not clear if the main claims, such as the neural activity in the normal-hearing group encoding phonetic information in attended speech better than in unattended speech, are directly observable. Showing ERP differences or spatiotemporal response pattern differences could support these claims more straightforwardly. Additionally, training pattern classifiers to test if different levels of information can be decoded from EEG activity in specific groups could provide further validation of the findings.

      We have now included results from more traditional mTRF analyses using phoneme, syllable, word, phrase, and sentence rates as baseline models (see p.7 of the manuscript and Figure S3). The results show similar patterns to those observed in our current analyses. While we agree that classification analyses would be very interesting, our regression analyses have already demonstrated distinct EEG patterns for each linguistic level. Consequently, classification analyses would likely yield similar results unless a different method for representing linguistic information at these levels is employed. To the best of our knowledge, no other computational model currently exists that can simultaneously represent these linguistic levels.

      (2) Is there any behavioral metric suggesting that these hearing-impaired participants do have deficits in comprehending long sentences? The self-rated intelligibility is useful, but cannot fully distinguish between perceiving lower-level phonetic information vs longer sentence comprehension.

      In the current study, we included only self-rated intelligibility tests. We acknowledge that this approach might not fully distinguish between the perception of lower-level phonetic information and higher-level sentence comprehension. However, it remains unclear what type of behavioral test would effectively address this distinction. Furthermore, our primary aim was to use the behavioral results to demonstrate that our hearing-impaired listeners experienced speech comprehension difficulties in multi-talker environments, while relying on the EEG data to investigate comprehension challenges at various linguistic levels.

      Minor:

      (1) Page 2, second line in Introduction, "Phonemes occur over ..." should be lowercase.

      According to APA format, the first word after the colon is capitalized if it begins a complete sentence (https://blog.apastyle.org/apastyle/2011/06/capitalization-after-colons.html). Here

      the sentence is a complete sentence so we used uppercase for “phonemes”.

      (2) Page 8, second paragraph "...-100ms to 100ms relative to sentence onsets", should it be onsets or offsets?

      This is typo and it should be offsets. We have now revised it.

      References

      Bemis, D. K., & Pylkkanen, L. (2011). Simple composition: An MEG investigation into the comprehension of minimal linguistic phrases. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(8), 2801– 2814.

      Gao, C., Li, J., Chen, J., & Huang, S. (2024). Measuring meaning composition in the human brain with composition scores from large language models. In L.-W. Ku, A. Martins, & V. Srikumar (Eds.), Proceedings of the 62nd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics (Volume 1: Long Papers) (pp. 11295–11308). Association for Computational Linguistics.

      Goldstein, A., Zada, Z., Buchnik, E., Schain, M., Price, A., Aubrey, B., Nastase, S. A., Feder, A., Emanuel, D., Cohen, A., Jansen, A., Gazula, H., Choe, G., Rao, A., Kim, C., Casto, C., Fanda, L., Doyle, W., Friedman, D., … Hasson, U. (2022). Shared computational principles for language processing in humans and deep language models. Nature Neuroscience, 25(3), Article 3.

      Gwilliams, L., King, J.-R., Marantz, A., & Poeppel, D. (2022). Neural dynamics of phoneme sequences reveal position-invariant code for content and order. Nature Communications, 13(1), Article 1.

      Huth, A. G., de Heer, W. A., Griffiths, T. L., Theunissen, F. E., & Gallant, J. L. (2016). Natural speech reveals the semantic maps that tile human cerebral cortex. Nature, 532(7600), 453–458.

      Li, J., Lai, M., & Pylkkänen, L. (2024). Semantic composition in experimental and naturalistic paradigms. Imaging Neuroscience, 2, 1–17.

      Li, J., & Pylkkänen, L. (2021). Disentangling semantic composition and semantic association in the left temporal lobe. Journal of Neuroscience, 41(30), 6526–6538.

      Maris, E., & Oostenveld, R. (2007). Nonparametric statistical testing of EEG- and MEG-data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 164(1), 177–190.

      Schmitt, L.-M., Erb, J., Tune, S., Rysop, A. U., Hartwigsen, G., & Obleser, J. (2021). Predicting speech from a cortical hierarchy of event-based time scales. Science Advances, 7(49), eabi6070.

      Schrimpf, M., Blank, I. A., Tuckute, G., Kauf, C., Hosseini, E. A., Kanwisher, N., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Fedorenko, E. (2021). The neural architecture of language: Integrative modeling converges on predictive processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(45), e2105646118.

      Sugimoto, Y., Yoshida, R., Jeong, H., Koizumi, M., Brennan, J. R., & Oseki, Y. (2024). Localizing Syntactic Composition with Left-Corner Recurrent Neural Network Grammars. Neurobiology of Language, 5(1), 201–224.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      Reviewer 1 (Public Review):

      I want to reiterate my comment from the first round of reviews: that I am insufficiently familiar with the intricacies of Maxwell’s equations to assess the validity of the assumptions and the equations being used by WETCOW. The work ideally needs assessing by someone more versed in that area, especially given the potential impact of this method if valid.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s candor. Unfortunately, familiarity with Maxwell’s equations is an essential prerequisite for assessing the veracity of our approach and our claims.

      Effort has been made in these revisions to improve explanations of the proposed approach (a lot of new text has been added) and to add new simulations. However, the authors have still not compared their method on real data with existing standard approaches for reconstructing data from sensor to physical space. Refusing to do so because existing approaches are deemed inappropriate (i.e. they “are solving a different problem”) is illogical.

      Without understanding the importance of our model for brain wave activity (cited in the paper) derived from Maxwell’s equations in inhomogeneous and anisotropic brain tissue, it is not possible to critically evaluate the fundamental difference between our method and the standard so-called “source localization” method which the Reviewer feels it is important to compare our results with. Our method is not “source localization” which is a class of techniques based on an inappropriate model for static brain activity (static dipoles sprinkled sparsely in user-defined areas of interest). Just because a method is “standard” does not make it correct. Rather, we are reconstructing a whole brain, time dependent electric field potential based upon a model for brain wave activity derived from first principles. It is comparing two methods that are “solving different problems” that is, by definition, illogical.

      Similarly, refusing to compare their method with existing standard approaches for spatio-temporally describing brain activity, just because existing approaches are deemed inappropriate, is illogical.

      Contrary to the Reviewer’s assertion, we do compare our results with three existing methods for describing spatiotemporal variations of brain activity.

      First, Figures 1, 2, and 6 compare the spatiotemporal variations in brain activity between our method and fMRI, the recognized standard for spatiotemporal localization of brain activity. The statistical comparison in Fig 3 is a quantitative demonstration of the similarity of the activation patterns. It is important to note that these data are simultaneous EEG/fMRI in order to eliminate a variety of potential confounds related to differences in experimental conditions.

      Second, Fig 4 (A-D) compares our method with the most reasonable “standard” spatiotemporal localization method for EEG: mapping of fields in the outer cortical regions of the brain detected at the surface electrodes to the surface of the skull. The consistency of both the location and sign of the activity changes detected by both methods in a “standard” attention paradigm is clearly evident. Further confirmation is provided by comparison of our results with simultaneous EEG/fMRI spatial reconstructions (E-F) where the consistency of our reconstructions between subjects is shown in Fig 5.

      Third, measurements from intra-cranial electrodes, the most direct method for validation, are compared with spatiotemporal estimates derived from surface electrodes and shown to be highly correlated.

      For example, the authors say that “it’s not even clear what one would compare [between the new method and standard approaches]”. How about:

      (1) Qualitatively: compare EEG activation maps. I.e. compare what you would report to a researcher about the brain activity found in a standard experimental task dataset (e.g. their gambling task). People simply want to be able to judge, at least qualitatively on the same data, what the most equivalent output would be from the two approaches. Note, both approaches do not need to be done at the same spatial resolution if there are constraints on this for the comparison to be useful.

      (2) Quantitatively: compare the correlation scores between EEG activation maps and fMRI activation maps

      These comparison were performed and already in the paper.

      (1) Fig 4 compares the results with a standard attention paradigm (data and interpretation from Co-author Dr Martinez, who is an expert in both EEG and attention). Additionally, Fig 12 shows detected regions of increased activity in a well-known brain circuit from an experimental task (’reward’) with data provided by Co-author Dr Krigolson, an expert in reward circuitry.

      (2) Correlation scores between EEG and fMRI are shown in Fig 3.

      (3) Very high correlation between the directly measured field from intra-cranial electrodes in an epilepsy patient and those estimated from only the surface electrodes is shown in Fig 9.

      There are an awful lot of typos in the new text in the paper. I would expect a paper to have been proof read before submitting.

      We have cleaned up the typos.

      The abstract claims that there is a “direct comparison with standard state-of-the-art EEG analysis in a well-established attention paradigm”, but no actual comparison appears to have been completed in the paper.

      On the contrary, as mentioned above, Fig 4 compares the results of our method with the state-of-the-art surface spatial mapping analysis, with the state-of-the-art time-frequency analysis, and with the state-of-the-art fMRI analysis

      Reviewer 2 (Public Review):

      This is a major rewrite of the paper. The authors have improved the discourse vastly.

      There is now a lot of didactics included but they are not always relevant to the paper.

      The technique described in the paper does in fact leverage several novel methods we have developed over the years for analyzing multimodal space-time imaging data. Each of these techniques has been described in detail in separate publications cited in the current paper. However, the Reviewers’ criticisms stated that the methods were non-standard and they were unfamiliar with them. In lieu of the Reviewers’ reading the original publications, we added a significant amount of text indeed intended to be didactic. However, we can assume the Reviewer that nothing presented was irrelevant to the paper. We certainly had no desire to make the paper any longer than it needed to be.

      The section on Maxwell’s equation does a disservice to the literature in prior work in bioelectromagnetism and does not even address the issues raised in classic text books by Plonsey et al. There is no logical “backwardness” in the literature. They are based on the relative values of constants in biological tissues.

      This criticism highlights the crux of our paper. Contrary to the assertion that we have ignored the work of Plonsey, we have referenced it in the new additional text detailing how we have constructed Maxwell’s Equations appropriate for brain tissue, based on the model suggested by Plonsey that allows the magnetic field temporal variations to be ignored but not the time-dependence electric fields.

      However, the assumption ubiquitous in the vast prior literature of bioelectricity in the brain that the electric field dynamics can be “based on the relative values of constants in biological tissues”, as the Reviewer correctly summarizes, is precisely the problem. Using relative average tissue properties does not take into account the tissue anisotropy necessary to properly account for correct expressions for the electric fields. As our prior publications have demonstrated in detail, taking into account the inhomogeneity and anisotropy of brain tissue in the solution to Maxwell’s Equations is necessary for properly characterizing brain electrical fields, and serves as the foundation of our brain wave theory. This led to the discovery of a new class of brain waves (weakly evanescent transverse cortical waves, WETCOW).

      It is this brain wave model that is used to estimate the dynamic electric field potential from the measurements made by the EEG electrode array. The standard model that ignores these tissue details leads to the ubiquitous “quasi-static approximation” that leads to the conclusion that the EEG signal cannot be spatial reconstructed. It is indeed this critical gap in the existing literature that is the central new idea in the paper.

      There are reinventions of many standard ideas in terms of physics discourses, like Bayesian theory or PCA etc.

      The discussion of Bayesian theory and PCA is in response to the Reviewer complaint that they were unfamiliar with our entropy field decomposition (EFD) method and the request that we compare it with other “standard” methods. Again, we have published extensively on this method (as referenced in the manuscript) and therefore felt that extensive elaboration was unnecessary. Having been asked to provide such elaboration and then being pilloried for it therefore feels somewhat inappropriate in our view. This is particularly disappointing as the Reviewer claims we are presenting “standard” ideas when in fact the EFD is new general framework we developed to overcome the deficiencies in standard “statistical” and probabilistic data analysis methods that are insufficient for characterizing non-linear, nonperiodic, interacting fields that are the rule, rather than the exception, in complex dynamical systems, such as brain electric fields (or weather, or oceans, or ....).

      The EFD is indeed a Bayesian framework, as this is the fundamental starting point for probability theory, but it is developed in a unique and more general fashion than previous data analysis methods. (Again, this is detailed in several references in the papers bibliography. The Reviewer’s requested that an explanation be included in the present paper, however, so we did so). First, Bayes Theorem is expressed in terms of a field theory that allows an arbitrary number of field orders and coupling terms. This generality comes with a penalty, which is that it’s unclear how to assess the significance of the essentially infinite number of terms. The second feature is the introduction of a method by which to determine the significant number of terms automatically from the data itself, via the our theory of entropy spectrum pathways (ESP), which is also detailed in a cited publication, and which produces ranked spatiotemporal modes from the data. Rather than being “reinventions of many standard ideas” these are novel theoretical and computational methods that are central to the EEG reconstruction method presented in the paper.

      I think that the paper remains quite opaque and many of the original criticisms remain, especially as they relate to multimodal datasets. The overall algorithm still remains poorly described. benchmarks.

      It’s not clear how to assess the criticisms that the algorithm is poorly described yet there is too much detail provided that is mistakenly assessed as “standard”. Certainly the central wave equations that are estimated from the data are precisely described, so it’s not clear exactly what the Reviewer is referring to.

      The comparisons to benchmark remain unaddressed and the authors state that they couldn’t get Loreta to work and so aborted that. The figures are largely unaltered, although they have added a few more, and do not clearly depict the ideas. Again, no benchmark comparisons are provided to evaluate the results and the performance in comparison to other benchmarks.

      As we have tried to emphasize in the paper, and in the Response to Reviewers, the standard so-called “source localization” methods are NOT a benchmark, as they are solving an inappropriate model for brain activity. Once again, static dipole “sources” arbitrarily sprinkled on pre-defined regions of interest bear little resemblance to observed brain waves, nor to the dynamic electric field wave equations produced by our brain wave theory derived from a proper solution to Maxwell’s equations in the anisotropic and inhomogeneous complex morphology of the brain.

      The comparison with Loreta was not abandoned because we couldn’t get it to work, but because we could not get it to run under conditions that were remotely similar to whole brain activity described by our theory, or, more importantly, by an rationale theory of dynamic brain activity that might reproduce the exceedingly complex electric field activity observed in numerous neuroscience experiments.

      We take issue with the rather dismissive mention of “a few more” figures that “do not clearly depict the idea” when in fact the figures that have been added have demonstrated additional quantitative validation of the method.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer 1 (Public Review):

      The paper proposes a new source reconstruction method for electroencephalography (EEG) data and claims that it can provide far superior spatial resolution than existing approaches and also superior spatial resolution to fMRI. This primarily stems from abandoning the established quasi-static approximation to Maxwell’s equations.<br /> The proposed method brings together some very interesting ideas, and the potential impact is high. However, the work does not provide the evaluations expected when validating a new source reconstruction approach. I cannot judge the success or impact of the approach based on the current set of results. This is very important to rectify, especially given that the work is challenging some long- standing and fundamental assumptions made in the field.

      We appreciate the Reviewer’s efforts in reviewing this paper and have included a significant amount of new text to address their concerns.

      I also find that the clarity of the description of the methods, and how they link to what is shown in the main results hard to follow.

      We have added significantly more detail on the methods, including more accessible explanations of the technical details, and schematic diagrams to visualize the key processing components.

      I am insufficiently familiar with the intricacies of Maxwell’s equations to assess the validity of the assumptions and the equations being used by WETCOW. The work therefore needs assessing by someone more versed in that area. That said, how do we know that the new terms in Maxwell’s equations, i.e. the time-dependent terms that are normally missing from established quasi-static-based approaches, are large enough to need to be considered? Where is the evidence for this?

      The fact that the time-dependent terms are large enough to be considered is essentially the entire focus of the original papers [7,8]. Time-dependent terms in Maxwell’s equations are generally not important for brain electrodynamics at physiological frequencies for homogeneous tissues, but this is not true for areas with stroung inhomogeneity and ansisotropy.

      I have not come across EFD, and I am not sure many in the EEG field will have. To require the reader to appreciate the contributions of WETCOW only through the lens of the unfamiliar (and far from trivial) approach of EFD is frustrating. In particular, what impact do the assumptions of WETCOW make compared to the assumptions of EFD on the overall performance of SPECTRE?

      We have added an entire new section in the Appendix that provides a very basic introduction to EFD and relates it to more commonly known methods, such as Fourier and Independent Components Analyses.

      The paper needs to provide results showing the improvements obtained when WETCOW or EFD are combined with more established and familiar approaches. For example, EFD can be replaced by a first-order vector autoregressive (VAR) model, i.e. y<sub>t</sub> = Ay<sub>t−1</sub> + e<sub>t</sub> (where y<sub>t</sub> is [num<sub>gridpoints</sub> ∗ 1] and A is [num<sub>gridpoints</sub> ∗ num<sub>gridpoints</sub>] of autoregressive parameters).

      The development of EFD, which is independent of WETCOW, stemmed from the necessity of developing a general method for the probabilistic analysis of finitely sampled non-linear interacting fields, which are ubiquitous in measurements of physical systems, of which functional neuroimaging data (fMRI, EEG) are excellent examples. Standard methods (such as VAR) are inadequate in such cases, as discussed in great detail in our EFD publications (e.g., [12,37]). The new appendix on EFD reviews these arguments. It does not make sense to compare EFD with methods which are inappropriate for the data.

      The authors’ decision not to include any comparisons with established source reconstruction approaches does not make sense to me. They attempt to justify this by saying that the spatial resolution of LORETA would need to be very low compared to the resolution being used in SPECTRE, to avoid compute problems. But how does this stop them from using a spatial resolution typically used by the field that has no compute problems, and comparing with that? This would be very informative. There are also more computationally efficient methods than LORETA that are very popular, such as beamforming or minimum norm.

      he primary reason for not comparing with ’source reconstruction’ (SR) methods is that we are are not doing source reconstruction. Our view of brain activity is that it involves continuous dynamical non-linear interacting fields througout the entire brain. Formulating EEG analysis in terms of reconstructing sources is, in our view, like asking ’what are the point sources of a sea of ocean waves’. It’s just not an appropriate physical model. A pre-chosen limited distribution of static dipoles is just a very bad model for brain activity, so much so that it’s not even clear what one would compare. Because in our view, as manifest in our computational implementation, one needs to have a very high density of computational locations throughout the entire brain, including white matter, and the reconstructed modes are waves whose extent can be across the entire brain. Our comments about the low resolution of computational methods for SR techniques really is expressing the more overarching concern that they are not capable of, or even designed for, detecting time-dependent fields of non-linear interacting waves that exist everywhere througout the brain. Moreover, the SR methods always give some answer, but in our view the initial conditions upon which those methods are based (pre-selected regions of activity with a pre-selected number of ’sources’) is a highly influential but artificial set of strong computational constraints that will almost always provide an answer consist with (i.e., biased toward) the expectations of the person formlating the problem, and is therefore potentially misleading.

      In short, something like the following methods needs to be compared:

      (1) Full SPECTRE (EFD plus WETCOW)

      (2) WETCOW + VAR or standard (“simple regression”) techniques

      (3) Beamformer/min norm plus EFD

      (4) Beamformer/min norm plus VAR or standard (“simple regression”) techniques

      The reason that no one has previously ever been able to solve the EEG inverse problem is due to the ubiquitous use of methods that are too ’simple’, i.e., are poor physical models of brain activity. We have spent a decade carefully elucidating the details of this statement in numerous highly technical and careful publications. It therefore serves no purpose to return to the use of these ’simple’ methods for comparison. We do agree, however, that a clearer overview of the advantages of our methods is warranted and have added significant additional text in this revision towards that purpose.

      This would also allow for more illuminating and quantitative comparisons of the real data. For example, a metric of similarity between EEG maps and fMRI can be computed to compare the performance of these methods. At the moment, the fMRI-EEG analysis amounts to just showing fairly similar maps.

      We disagree with this assessment. The correlation coefficient between the spatially localized activation maps is a conservative sufficient statistic for the measure of statistically significant similarity. These numbers were/are reported in the caption to Figure 5, and have now also been moved to, and highlighted in, the main text.

      There are no results provided on simulated data. Simulations are needed to provide quantitative comparisons of the different methods, to show face validity, and to demonstrate unequivocally the new information that SPECTRE can ’potentially’ provide on real data compared to established methods. The paper ideally needs at least 3 types of simulations, where one thing is changed at a time, e.g.:

      (1) Data simulated using WETCOW plus EFD assumptions

      (2) Data simulated using WETCOW plus e.g. VAR assumptions

      (3) Data simulated using standard lead fields (based on the quasi-static Maxwell solutions) plus e.g. VAR assumptions

      These should be assessed with the multiple methods specified earlier. Crucially the assessment should be quantitative showing the ability to recover the ground truth over multiple realisations of realistic noise. This type of assessment of a new source reconstruction method is the expected standard

      We have now provided results on simulated data, along with a discussion on what entails a meaningful simulation comparison. In short, our original paper on the WETCOW theory included a significant number of simulations of predicted results on several spatial and temporal scales. The most relevant simulation data to compare with the SPECTRE imaging results are the cortical wave loop predicted by WETCOW theory and demonstrated via numerical simulation in a realistic brain model derived from high resolution anatomical (HRA) MRI data. The most relevant data with which to compare these simulations are the SPECTRE recontruction from the data that provides the closest approximation to a “Gold Standard” - reconstructions from intra-cranial EEG (iEEG). We have now included results (new Fig 8) that demonstrate the ability of SPECTRE to reconstruct dynamically evolving cortical wave loops in iEEG data acquired in an epilepsy patient that match with the predicted loop predicted theoretically by WETCOW and demonstrated in realistic numerical simulations.

      The suggested comparison with simple regression techniques serves no purpose, as stated above, since that class of analysis techniques was not designed for non-linear, non-Gaussian, coupled interacting fields predicted by the WETCOW model. The explication of this statement is provided in great detail in our publications on the EFD approach and in the new appendix material provided in this revision. The suggested simulation of the dipole (i.e., quasi-static) model of brain activity also serves no purpose, as our WETCOW papers have demonstrated in great detail that is is not a reasonable model for dynamic brain activity.

      Reviewer 2 (Public Review):

      Strengths:

      If true and convincing, the proposed theoretical framework and reconstruction algorithm can revolutionize the use of EEG source reconstructions.

      Weaknesses:

      There is very little actual information in the paper about either the forward model or the novel method of reconstruction. Only citations to prior work by the authors are cited with absolutely no benchmark comparisons, making the manuscript difficult to read and interpret in isolation from their prior body of work.

      We have now added a significant amount of material detailing the forward model, our solution to the inverse problem, and the method of reconstruction, in order to remedy this deficit in the previous version of the paper.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer 1 (Recommendations):

      It is not at all clear from the main text (section 3.1) and the caption, what is being shown in the activity patterns in Figures 1 and 2. What frequency bands and time points etc? How are the values shown in the figures calculated from the equations in the methods?

      We have added detailed information on the frequency bands reconstructed and the activity pattern generation and meaning. Additional information on the simultaneous EEG/fMRI acquisition details has been added to the Appendix.

      How have the activity maps been thresholded? Where are the color bars in Figures 1 and 2?

      We have now included that information in new versions of the figures. In addition, the quantitative comparison between fMRI and EEG are presented is now presented in a new Figure 2 (now Figure 3).

      P30 “This term is ignored in the current paper”. Why is this term ignored, but other (time-dependent) terms are not?

      These terms are ignored because they represent higher order terms that complicate the processing (and intepretation) but do not substatially change the main results. A note to this effect has been added to the text.

      The concepts and equations in the EFD section are not very accessible (e.g. to someone unfamiliar with IFT).

      We have added a lengthy general and more accessible description of the EFD method in the Appendix.

      Variables in equation 1, and the following equation, are not always defined in a clear, accessible manner. What is ?

      We have added additional information on how Eqn 1 (now Eqn 3) is derived, and the variables therein.

      In the EFD section, what do you mean conceptually by α, i.e. “the coupled parameters α”?

      This sentence has been eliminated, as it was superfluous and confusing.

      How are the EFD and WETCOW sections linked mathematically? What is ψ (in eqn 2) linked to in the WETCOW section (presumably ϕ<sub>ω</sub>?) ?

      We have added more introductory detail at the beginning of the Results to describe the WETCOW theory and how this is related to the inverse problem for EEG.

      What is the difference between data d and signal s in section 6.1.3? How are they related?

      We have added a much more detailed Appendix A where this (and other) details are provided.

      What assumptions have been made to get the form for the information Hamiltonian in eqn3?

      Eq 3 (now Eqn A.5) is actually very general. The approximations come in when constructing the interaction Hamiltonian H<sub>i</sub>.

      P33 “using coupling between different spatio-temporal points that is available from the data itself” I do not understand what is meant by this.

      This was a poorly worded sentence, but this section has now been replaced by Appendix A, which now contains the sentence that prior information “is contained within the data itself”. This refers to the fact that the prior information consists of correlations in the data, rather than some other measurements independent of the original data. This point is emphasized because in many Bayesian application, prior information consists of knowledge of some quantity that were acquired independently from the data at hand (e.g., mean values from previous experiments)

      Reviewer 2 (Recommendations):

      Abstract

      The first part presents validation from simultaneous EEG/fMRI data, iEEG data, and comparisons with standard EEG analyses of an attention paradigm. Exactly what constitutes adequate validation or what metrics were used to assess performance is surprisingly absent.

      Subsequently, the manuscript examines a large cohort of subjects performing a gambling task and engaging in reward circuits. The claim is that this method offers an alternative to fMRI.

      Introduction

      Provocative statements require strong backing and evidence. In the first paragraph, the “quasi-static” assumption which is dominant in the field of EEG and MEG imaging is questioned with some classic citations that support this assumption. Instead of delving into why exactly the assumption cannot be relaxed, the authors claim that because the assumption was proved with average tissue properties rather than exact, it is wrong. This does not make sense. Citations to the WETCOW papers are insufficient to question the quasi-static assumption.

      The introduction purports to validate a novel theory and inverse modeling method but poorly outlines the exact foundations of both the theory (WETCOW) and the inverse modeling (SPECTRE) work.

      We have added a new introductory subsection (“A physical theory of brain waves”) to the Results section that provides a brief overview of the foundations of the WETCOW theory and an explicit description of why the quasi-static approximation can be abandoned. We have expanded the subsequent subsection (“Solution to the inverse EEG problem”) to more clearly detail the inverse modeling (SPECTRE) method.

      Section 3.2 Validation with fMRI

      Figure 1 supposedly is a validation of this promising novel theoretical approach that defies the existing body of literature in this field. Shockingly, a single subject data is shown in a qualitative manner with absolutely no quantitative comparison anywhere to be found in the manuscript. While there are similarities, there are also differences in reconstructions. What to make out of these discrepancies? Are there distortions that may occur with SPECTRE reconstructions? What are its tradeoffs? How does it deal with noise in the data?

      It is certainly not the case that there are no quantitative comparisons. Correlation coefficients, which are the sufficient statistics for comparison of activation regions, are given in Figure 5 for very specific activation regions. Figure 9 (now Figure 11) shows a t-statistic demonstrating the very high significance of the comparison between multiple subjects. And we have now added a new Figure 7 demonstrating the strongly correlated estimates for full vs surface intra-cranial EEG reconstructions. To make this more clear, we have added a new section “Statistical Significance of the Results”.

      We note that a discussion of the discrepancies between fMRI and EEG was already presented in the Supplementary Material. Therein we discuss the main point that fMRI and EEG are measuring different physical quantities and so should not be expected to be identical. We also highlight the fact that fMRI is prone to significant geometrical distortions for magnetic field inhomogeities, and to physiological noise. To provide more visibility for this important issue, we have moved this text into the Discussion section.

      We do note that geometric distortions in fMRI data due to suboptimal acquisitions and corrections is all too common. This, coupled with the paucity of open source simultaneous fMRI-EEG data, made it difficult to find good data for comparison. The data on which we performed the quantitative statistical comparison between fMRI and EEG (Fig 5) was collected by co-author Dr Martinez, and was of the highest quality and therefore sufficient for comparison. The data used in Fig 1 and 2 was a well publicized open source dataset but had significant fMRI distortions that made quantitative comparison (i.e., correlation coefficents between subregions in the Harvard-Oxford atlas) suboptimal. Nevertheless, we wanted to demonstrate the method in more than one source, and feel that visual similarity is a reasonble measure for this data.

      Section 3.2 Validation with fMRI

      Figure 2 Are the sample slices being shown? How to address discrepancies? How to assume that these are validations when there are such a level of discrepancies?

      It’s not clear what “sample slices” means. The issue of discrepancies is addressed in the response to the previous query.

      Section 3.2 Validation with fMRI

      Figure 3 Similar arguments can be made for Figure 3. Here too, a comparison with source localization benchmarks is warranted because many papers have examined similar attention data.

      Regarding the fMRI/EEG comparison, these data are compared quantitatively in the text and in Figure 5.

      Regarding the suggestion to perform standard ’source localization’ analysis, see responses to Reviewer 1.

      Section 3.2 Validation with fMRI

      Figure 4 While there is consistency across 5 subjects, there are also subtle and not-so-subtle differences.

      What to make out of them?

      Discrepancies in activations patterns between individuals is a complex neuroscience question that we feel is well beyond the scope of this paper.

      Section 3.2 Validation with fMRI

      Figures 5 & 6 Figure 5 is also a qualitative figure from two subjects with no appropriate quantification of results across subjects. The same is true for Figure 6.

      On the contrary, Figure 5 contains a quantitative comparison, which is now also described in the text. A quantitative comparison for the epilepsy data in Fig 6 (and C.4-C.6) is now shown in Fig 7.

      Section 3.2 Validation with fMRI

      Given the absence of appropriate “validation” of the proposed model and method, it is unclear how much one can trust results in Section 4.

      We believe that the quantitative comparisons extant in the original text (and apparently missed by the Reviewer) along with the additional quantitative comparisons are sufficient to merit trust in Section 4.

      Section 3.2 Validation with fMRI

      What are the thresholds used in maps for Figure 7? Was correction for multiple comparisons performed? The final arguments at the end of section 4 do not make sense. Is the claim that all results of reconstructions from SPECTRE shown here are significant with no reason for multiple comparison corrections to control for false positives? Why so?

      We agree that the last line in Section 4 is misleading and have removed it.

      Section 3.2 Validation with fMRI

      Discussion is woefully inadequate in addition to the inconclusive findings presented here.

      We have added a significant amount of text to the Discussion to address the points brought up by the Reviewer. And, contrary to the comments of this Reviewer, we believe the statistically significant results presented are not “inconclusive”.

      Supplementary Materials

      This reviewer had an incredibly difficult time understanding the inverse model solution. Even though this has been described in a prior publication by the authors, it is important and imperative that all details be provided here to make the current manuscript complete. The notation itself is so nonstandard. What is Σ<sup>ij</sup>, δ<sup>ij</sup>? Where is the reference for equation (1)? What about the equation for <sup>ˆ</sup>(R)? There are very few details provided on the exact implementation details for the Fourier-space pseudo-spectral approach. What are the dimensions of the problem involved? How were different tissue compartments etc. handled? Equation 1 holds for the entire volume but the measurements are only made on the surface. How was this handled? What is the WETCOW brain wave model? I don’t see any entropy term defined anywhere - where is it?

      We have added more detail on the theoretical and numerical aspects of the inverse problem in two new subsections “Theory” and “Numerical Implementation” in the new section “Solution to the inverse EEG problem”.

      Supplementary Materials

      So, how can one understand even at a high conceptual level what is being done with SPECTRE?

      We have added a new subsection “Summary of SPECTRE” that provides a high conceptual level overview of the SPECTRE method outlined in the preceding sections.

      Supplementary Materials

      In order to understand what was being presented here, it required the reader to go on a tour of the many publications by the authors where the difficulty in understanding what they actually did in terms of inverse modeling remains highly obscure and presents a huge problem for replicability or reproducibility of the current work.

      We have now included more basic material from our previous papers, and simplified the presentation to be more accessible. In particular, we have now moved the key aspects of the theoretic and numerical methods, in a more readable form, from the Supplementary Material to the main text, and added a new Appendix that provides a more intuitive and accessible overview of our estimation procedures.

      Supplementary Materials

      How were conductivity values for different tissue types assigned? Is there an assumption that the conductivity tensor is the same as the diffusion tensor? What does it mean that “in the present study only HRA data were used in the estimation procedure?” Does that mean that diffusion MRI data was not used? What is SYMREG? If this refers to the MRM paper from the authors in 2018, that paper does not include EEG data at all. So, things are unclear here.

      The conductivity tensor is not exactly the same as the diffusion tensor in brain tissues, but they are closely related. While both tensors describe transport properties in brain tissue, they represent different physical processes. The conductivity tensor is often assumed to share the same eigenvectors as the diffusion tensor. There is a strong linear relationship between the conductivity and diffusion tensor eigenvalues, as supported by theoretical models and experimental measurements. For the current study we only used the anatomical data for estimatition and assignment of different tissue types and no diffusion MRI data was used. To register between different modalities, including MNI, HRA, function MRI, etc., and to transform the tissue assignment into an appropriate space we used the SYMREG registration method. A comment to the effect has been added to the text.

      Supplementary Materials

      How can reconstructed volumetric time-series of potential be thought of as the EM equivalent of an fMRI dataset? This sentence doesn’t make sense.

      This sentence indeed did not make sense and has been removed.

      Supplementary Materials

      Typical Bayesian inference does not include entropy terms, and entropy estimation doesn’t always lend to computing full posterior distributions. What is an “entropy spectrum pathway”? What is µ∗? Why can’t things be made clear to the reader, instead of incredible jargon used here? How does section 6.1.2 relate back to the previous section?

      That is correct that Bayesian inference typically does not include entropy terms. We believe that their introduction via the theory of entropy spectrum pathways (ESP) is a significant advance in Bayesian estimation as it provides highly relevent prior information from within the data itself (and therefore always available in spatiotemporal data) that facilitates a practical methodology for the analysis of complex non-linear dynamical system, as contained in the entropy field decomposition (EFD).

      Section 6.1.3 has now been replaced by a new Appendix A that discusses ESP in a much more intuitive and conceptual manner.

      Supplementary Materials

      Section 6.1.3 describes entropy field decomposition in very general terms. What is “non-period”? This section is incomprehensible. Without reference to exactly where in the process this procedure is deployed it is extremely difficult to follow. There seems to be an abuse of notation of using ϕ for eigenvectors in equation (5) and potentials earlier. How do equations 9-11 relate back to the original problem being solved in section 6.1.1? What are multiple modalities being described here that require JESTER?

      Section 6.1.3 has now been replaced by a new Appendix A that covers this material in a much more intuitive and conceptual manner.

      Supplementary Materials

      Section 6.3 discusses source localization methods. While most forward lead-field models assume quasistatic approximations to Maxwell’s equations, these are perfectly valid for the frequency content of brain activity being measured with EEG or MEG. Even with quasi-static lead fields, the solutions can have frequency dependence due to the data having frequency dependence. Solutions do not have to be insensitive to detailed spatially variable electrical properties of the tissues. For instance, if a FEM model was used to compute the forward model, this model will indeed be sensitive to the spatially variable and anisotropic electrical properties. This issue is not even acknowledged.

      The frequency dependence of the tissue properties is not the issue. Our theoretical work demonstrates that taking into account the anisotropy and inhomogeneity of the tissue is necessary in order to derive the existence of the weakly evanescent transverse cortical waves (WETCOW) that SPECTRE is detecting. We have added more details about the WETCOW model in the new Section “A physical theory of brain wave” to emphasize this point.

      Supplementary Materials

      Arguments to disambiguate deep vs shallow sources can be achieved with some but not all source localization algorithms and do not require a non-quasi-static formulation. LORETA is not even the main standard algorithm for comparison. It is disappointing that there are no comparisons to source localization and that this is dismissed away due to some coding issues.

      Again, we are not doing ’source localization’. The concept of localized dipole sources is anathema to our brain wave model, and so in our view comparing SPECTRE to such methods only propagates the misleading idea that they are doing the same thing. So they are definitely not dismissed due to coding issues. However, because of repeated requests to do compare SPECTRE with such methods, we attempted to run a standard source localization method with parameters that would at least provide the closest approximation to what we were doing. This attempt highlighted a serious computational issue in source localization methods that is a direct consequence of the fact that they are not attempting to do what SPECTRE is doing - describing a time-varying wave field, in the technical definition of a ’field’ as an object that has a value at every point in space-time.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      eLife Assessment

      The study presents some useful findings on Mendelian randomization-phenome-wide association, with BMI associated with health outcomes, and there is a focus on sex differences. Although there are some solid phenotype and genotype data, some of the data are incomplete and could be better presented, perhaps benefiting from more rigorous approaches. Confirmation and further assessment of the observed sex differences will add further value.

      Thank you for your positive comments. We have revised the analysis based on your feedback and that from the two reviewers. Specifically, we implemented a stricter multiple testing correction approach, improved the figures, included additional figures in the Supplementary Materials, considered the sex differences more rigorously and reported them in more detail. A comprehensive description of the revisions is provided below.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study uses information from the UK Biobank and aims to investigate the role of BMI on various health outcomes, with a focus on differences by sex. They confirm the relevance of many of the well-known associations between BMI and health outcomes for males and females and suggest that associations for some endpoints may differ by sex. Overall their conclusions appear supported by the data. The significance of the observed sex variations will require confirmation and further assessment.

      Strengths:

      This is one of the first systematic evaluations of sex differences between BMI and health outcomes. The hypothesis that BMI may be associated with health differentially based on sex is relevant and even expected. As muscle is heavier than adipose tissue, and as men typically have more muscle than women, as a body composition measure BMI is sometimes prone to classifying even normal weight/muscular men as obese, while this measure is more lenient when used in women. Confirmation of the many well-known associations is as expected and attests to the validity of their approach. Demonstration of the possible sex differences is interesting, with this work raising the need for further study.

      Thank you for your valuable comments. We are grateful for the time and effort you have devoted to reviewing our manuscript. We have strengthened our paper by adding your insightful comment about the rationale for sex-specific analysis to the introduction:

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Many of the statistical decisions appeared to target power at the expense of quality/accuracy. For example, they chose to use self-reported information rather than doctor diagnoses for disease outcomes for which both types of data were available.

      Thank you for your valuable comments. We apologize for the lack of clarity in our original description of the phenotypes. Information about health in the UK Biobank was obtained at baseline from tests, measurements and self reports. Subsequently comprehensive data linkage to hospital admissions, death registries and cancer registries was implemented. However, data linkage to primary care data, such as doctor diagnoses, has not been comprehensively implemented for the UK Biobank, possibly for logistic reasons. Doctor diagnoses are only available for about half the cohort, (https://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/enable-your-research/about-our-data/health-related-outcomes-data). So, we used self-reported diagnoses because they are substantially more comprehensive than the doctor diagnoses. We have explained this point by making the following change to the Methods:

      “Where attributes were available from both self-report and doctor diagnosis, we used self-reports. This is because comprehensive record linkage to doctor diagnoses has not yet been fully implemented for the UK Biobank, so information from doctor diagnoses may not fully represent the broader UK Biobank cohort.”

      (2) Despite known problems and bias arising from the use of one sample approach, they chose to use instruments from the UK Biobank instead of those available from the independent GIANT GWAS, despite the difference in sample size being only marginally greater for UKB for the context. With the way the data is presented, it is difficult to assess the extent to which results are compatible across approaches.

      Thank you for your comments. We agree completely about the issues with a one sample approach, please accept our apologies for not explaining our rationale. The sex-specific GIANT GWAS study is similar in size to the UK Biobank GWAS. However, the sex-specific GIANT GWAS is much less densely genotyped (~2,5 million variants) than the sex-specific UK Biobank GWAS (~10 million variants), so has less power, hence our use of the UK Biobank. To make this clear, we have added the number of variants in each study to the method section. Nevertheless, we also repeated analysis using sex-specific GIANT, as now given in the methods by making the following change

      We amended the description in the first paragraph of the results section:

      “Initial analysis using sex-specific BMI from GIANT yielded similar estimates as when using sex-specific BMI from the UK Biobank but had fewer SNPs resulting in wider confidence intervals (S Table 1) and fewer significant associations (S Table 1). Analysis using sex-combined GIANT yielded more significant associations but lacks granularity, so we presented the results obtained using sex-specific BMI from the UK Biobank.”

      In the discussion we also made the following changes:

      “Tenth, although this study primarily utilized sex-specific BMI, we also conducted analyses using overall BMI from GIANT including the UK Biobank, which gave a generally similar interpretation (S Table 1). Using sex-specific BMI from the UK Biobank and GIANT may lead to lower statistical power than using overall population BMI but allows for the detection of traits that are affected differently by BMI by sex. Including findings from the overall population BMI from sex-combined GIANT (S Table 1) makes the results more comparable to previous similar studies.”

      (3) The approach to multiple testing correction appears very lenient, although the lack of accuracy in the reporting makes it difficult to know what was done exactly. The way it reads, FDR correction was done separately for men, and then for women (assuming that the duplication in tests following stratification does not affect the number of tests). In the second stage, they compared differences by sex using Z-test, apparently without accounting for multiple testing.

      Thank you, we have accounted for multiple comparisons when considering differences by sex and have made corresponding changes. Specifically, in the methods, we changed:

      “We obtained differences by sex using a z-test (Paternoster et al., 1998), which as recommended was on a linear scale for dichotomous outcomes (Knol et al., 2007; Rothman, 2008), then we identified which ones remained after allowing for false discovery”

      We have made the following changes to the results section:

      “We found significant differences by sex in the associations of BMI with 105 health-related attributes (p-value<0.05); 46 phenotypes remained after allowing for false discovery (Table 1). Of these 46 differences most (35) were in magnitude but not direction, such as for SHBG, ischemic heart disease, heart attack, and facial aging, while 11 were directionally different.

      Notably, BMI was more strongly positively associated with myocardial infarction, major coronary heart disease events, ischemic heart disease, heart attack, and facial aging in men than in women. BMI was more strongly positively associated with diastolic blood pressure, and hypothyroidism/myxoedema in women than men. BMI was more strongly inversely associated with LDL-c, hay fever and allergic rhinitis in men than women. BMI was more strongly inversely associated with SHBG in women than men.

      BMI was inversely associated with ApoB, iron deficiency anemia, hernia, and total testosterone in men, while positively associated with these traits in women (Table 1). BMI was inversely associated with sensitivity/hurt feelings, and ever seeking medical advice for nerves, anxiety, tension, or depression in men. However, BMI was positively associated with sensitivity/hurt feelings and ever seeking medical advice for these same issues in women. BMI was positively associated with muscle or soft tissue injuries and haemorrhage from respiratory passages in men, whilst inversely associated with these traits in women.”

      We have correspondingly amended the discussion to reflect these changes by adding:

      “Whether the difference in ischemic heart disease rates between men and women that emerged in the US and the UK the late 19th century (Nikiforov & Mamaev, 1998) is explained by rising BMI remains to be determined.”

      (4) Presentation lacks accuracy in a few places, hence assessment of the accuracy of the statements made by the authors is difficult.

      Thank you, we have revised the whole manuscript in order to improve clarity.

      (5) Conclusion (Abstract) "These findings highlight the importance of retaining a healthy BMI" is rather uninformative, especially as they claim that for some attributes the effects of BMI may be opposite depending on sex/gender.

      Thank you for your comments. We have changed the conclusion of the abstract, as given below:

      “Our study revealed that BMI might affect a wide range of health-related attributes and also highlights notable sex differences in its impact, including opposite associations for certain attributes, such as ApoB; and stronger effects in men, such as for cardiovascular diseases. Our findings underscore the need for nuanced, sex-specific policy related to BMI to address inequities in health.”.

      We have changed the Impact statement, as given below:

      “BMI may affect a wide range of health-related attributes and there are notable sex differences in its impact, including opposite associations for certain attributes, such as ApoB; and stronger effects in men, such as for cardiovascular diseases. Our findings underscore the need for nuanced, sex-specific policy related to BMI.”

      We have changed the conclusion of the paper, as given below:

      “Our contemporary systematic examination found BMI associated with a broad range of health-related attributes. We also found significant sex differences in many traits, such as for cardiovascular diseases, underscoring the importance of addressing higher BMI in both men and women possibly as means of redressing differences in life expectancy. Ultimately, our study emphasizes the harmful effects of obesity and the importance of nuanced, sex-specific policy related to BMI to address inequities.in health.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this present Mendelian randomization-phenome-wide association study, the authors found BMI to be positively associated with many health-related conditions, such as heart disease, heart failure, and hypertensive heart disease. They also found sex differences in some traits such as cancer, psychological disorders, and ApoB.

      Strengths:

      The use of the UK-biobank study with detailed phenotype and genotype information.

      Thank you for your valuable comments. We are grateful for the time and effort you have devoted to reviewing our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Previous studies have performed this analysis using the same cohort, with in-depth analysis. See this paper: Searching for the causal effects of body mass index in over 300,000 participants in UK Biobank, using Mendelian randomization. https://journals.plos.org/plosgenetics/article?id=10.1371/journal.pgen.10079i51

      Thank you for your valuable comments. We checked the paper carefully. It gives sex-specific estimates when the outcome was assessed in different ways in men and women, for example the question about number of children was asked in terms of live births in women and number of children fathered in men. In addition, for some significant findings, the authors investigated differences by sex. However, the paper did not use sex-specific BMI or sex-specific outcomes systematically. We have added this paper to our introduction and amended the text to explain the novelty of our study compared to previous studies.

      “Previous phenome-wide association studies using MR (MR-PheWASs) have identified impacts of sex-combined BMI on endocrine disorders, circulatory diseases, inflammatory and dermatological conditions, some biomarkers and feelings of nervousness (Hyppönen et al., 2019; Millard et al., 2015; Millard et al. 2019), but did not systematically use sex-specific BMI for the exposure or sex-specific outcomes.”

      (2) I believe that the authors' claim, "To our knowledge, no sex-specific PheWAS has investigated the effects of BMI on health outcomes," is not well supported. They have not cited a relevant paper that conducted both overall and sex-stratified PheWAS using UK Biobank data with a detailed analysis. Given the prior study linked above, I am uncertain about the additional contributions of the present research.

      Thank you for your valuable comments, please accept our apologies for this oversight. As explained above, we have checked very carefully. There are three previous PheWAS for BMI, Hyppönen et al., 2019, Millard et al., 2015 and Millard et al. 2019. Hyppönen et al., 2019 and Millard et al., 2015 are not sex-specific. Millard et al. 2019 used sex-combined instruments, but some sex-specific outcomes, when the questions were asked sex-specifically, such as age at puberty asked as “age when periods started (menarche)” in women and “relative age of first facial hair” and “relative age voice broke” in men. When they found a factor significantly associated with BMI, they sometimes analyze it further including sex-specific analysis, but they did not do the analysis systematically for men and women with sex-specific BMI and sex-specific outcomes. We have amended the introduction to clarify this point.

      “To our knowledge, no sex-specific PheWAS has investigated the effects of BMI on health outcomes (Hyppönen et al., 2019; Millard et al., 2015; Millard et al. 2009). To address this gap, we conducted a sex-specific PheWAS, using the largest available sex-specific GWAS of BMI, to explore the impact of sex-specific BMI on sex-specific health-related attributes”

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Presentation, accuracy, and referencing:

      (1) The quality of the English language needs to be checked, including that all sentences carry all components required (including verbs).

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. The manuscript has undergone language editing by a native English-speaker, with particular attention to grammatical completeness (including verb consistency and sentence structure). We have also clarified ambiguities and inconsistencies in terms pointed out by the native English speakers. All revisions have been implemented in the updated manuscript.

      (2) The accuracy of statements needs to be checked. For example, in lines 82-83 it is not true that 2015/2019 was 'before the advent of large-scale GWAs studies". In the context of the above in lines 83-85, how can reference be made to a study published in 2020 calling that 'previous' MR studies and how a trial published in 2016 is 'recent'? Please revise, and please also check the manuscript for any other issues with accuracy of this kind.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have checked the manuscript and revised these sentences to be clearer, by making the following change.

      “Previous phenome-wide association studies using MR (MR-PheWASs) have identified impacts of sex-combined BMI on endocrine disorders, circulatory diseases, inflammatory and dermatological conditions, some biomarkers and feelings of nervousness (Hyppönen et al., 2019; Millard et al., 2015; Millard et al. 2019), but did not systematically use sex-specific BMI for the exposure or sex-specific outcomes. Previous MR studies and trials of incretins have expanded our knowledge about a broad range of effects of BMI (Larsson et al., 2020; Marso et al., 2016).”

      (3) The adequacy of referencing will need to be checked, e.g. line 136 "as recommended by UK biobank" is vague and needs to be referenced.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have added citations.

      “We categorized attributes as age at recruitment, physical measures, lifestyle and environmental, medical conditions, operations, physiological factors, cognitive function, health and medical history, sex-specific factors, blood assays and urine assays, based on the UK Biobank categories (https://biobank.ndph.ox.ac.uk/ukb/cats.cgi).”

      (4) The accurate use of terminology needs to be checked. For example, BMI is a measure of adiposity, while high BMI (typically >30) is used to index obesity.

      We thank you for your comments. We have changed the descriptions into “overweight/obesity” throughout.

      (5) Figure 1, Please check that complete information is given for 'selection criteria' and that the rationale for all information included is clear. For example, it is currently unclear what is the distinction between the bottom two sections which both present a number of features included in the analyses? Also, the Box detailing exclusion of 3585 variables does not give the criteria for these exclusions. Please add.

      Thank you for your comments. We have represented and revised Figure 1. Specifically, we have revised the bottom two sections to give each reason for exclusion and the number excluded for that reason. The updated “Excluded: 3,572 phenotypes, for the reason listed below:” box now contains bullet-points giving each reason for exclusion in the box (e.g. age of certain diseases/disorders onset: 26, alcohol: 56).

      (6) Figure 4, does not look to be of typical publication quality.

      We thank you for your comments. We have used different colors to make it smaller and more readable. Please see Table 1.

      Analyses:

      (1) As it stands, it is very difficult for a reader to confirm the conclusion that similar findings are obtained both when using instruments from the UKB and GIANT based on data presented (Stable 1 and 2). I suggested two things.

      a) Organise stable 1 and 2 by significance and category, with separation by highlighting for those which are significant under correction. I would consider merging these two tables, such that it would be easy for the reader to make the comparisons side by side. Consider presenting separate tables for the analyses for women and men.

      We thank you for your comments. We have followed your helpful advice and merged S Table 1 and S Table 2 into S Table 1. Furthermore, we have also merged S Table 5 to S Table 1.

      b) In Stable 3, please add information from related comparisons using the GIANT instruments. To support the authors' claim that associations are similar, but only the precision of estimation differed, you could consider adding information for numbers of associations for those that are directionally consistent and which have an association at least under nominal significance'. For associations where this does not hold, I would refrain from making a claim that the results are not affected by the choice of instrument (or biases relating to the analysis conducted).

      We thank you for your comments. Among 42 significant sex-specific associations identified in both the UK Biobank and the sex-specific GIANT consortium for men, all showed consistent directions of effect. Similarly, for women, all of the 45 significant associations exhibited consistent directions for UK Biobank compared with GIANT instruments.

      In the sex-specific UK Biobank, there are 203 significant associations in men, and 232 significant associations in women. We have added: in the sex-specific GIANT, there are 46 significant associations in men, and 84 significant associations in women. In the sex-combined GIANT, there are 246 significant associations in men, and 276 significant associations in women. We have provided all this information in S Table 2.

      We added the following descriptions at the end of the results section:

      “Of the 42 significant sex-specific associations identified in both the UK Biobank and the sex-specific GIANT consortium for men, all were directionally consistent. Similarly, for women, all 45 such significant associations were directionally consistent.

      We amended the following descriptions in the first paragraph of the results section:

      “Initial analysis using sex-specific BMI from the GIANT yielded similar estimates as when using sex-specific BMI from the UK Biobank but had fewer SNPs resulting in wider confidence intervals (S Table 1) and fewer significant associations (S Table 2). Analysis using sex-combined GIANT yielded more significant associations but lacks granularity, so we presented the results obtained using sex-specific BMI from the UK Biobank.”

      In the methods, we changed:

      “We obtained differences by sex using a z-test (Paternoster et al., 1998), which as recommended was on a linear scale for dichotomous outcomes (Knol et al., 2007; Rothman, 2008), then we identified which ones remained after allowing for false discovery.”

      We have made the following changes to the results section:

      “We found significant differences by sex in the associations of BMI with 105 health-related attributes (p-value<0.05); 46 phenotypes remained after allowing for false discovery (Table 1). Of these 46 differences most (35) were in magnitude but not direction, such as for SHBG, ischemic heart disease, heart attack, and facial aging, while 11 were directionally different.

      Notably, BMI was more strongly positively associated with myocardial infarction, major coronary heart disease events, ischemic heart disease, heart attack, and facial aging in men than in women. BMI was more strongly positively associated with diastolic blood pressure, and hypothyroidism/myxoedema in women than men. BMI was more strongly inversely associated with LDL-c, hay fever and allergic rhinitis in men than women. BMI was more strongly inversely associated with SHBG in women than men.

      BMI was inversely associated with ApoB, iron deficiency anemia, hernia, and total testosterone in men, while positively associated with these traits in women (Table 1). BMI was inversely associated with sensitivity/hurt feelings, and ever seeking medical advice for nerves, anxiety, tension, or depression in men. However, BMI was positively associated with sensitivity/hurt feelings and ever seeking medical advice for these same issues in women. BMI was positively associated with muscle or soft tissue injuries and haemorrhage from respiratory passages in men, whilst inversely associated with these traits in women.”

      (2) It is not clear what statistical criteria were used to determine sex differences, and the strategy/presentation should be clarified. In lines 229-231, it is implied that the 'significance' in one gender, but not in the other is used to indicate a difference. However, 'comparison of p-values' is not a valid statistical approach, and a more formal test (accounting for multiple testing would be warranted). It may be that a systematic approach has been implemented, but please check that it is adequately and accurately described to the reader.

      Please accept our apologies for being unclear. Multiple comparisons are for independent phenotypes however, here, some phenotypes cannot be independent, therefore, using multiple comparisons in men and women separately is quite strict. We added multiple comparisons for the assessment of sex-differences, which is now given in Table 1. Initially, there were 105 significant associations (p value for sex-difference<0.05) (Table 1), and 46 associations remained after FDR correction (Table 1).  

      Furthermore, we have made additional minor changes to clarify the wording.

      Knol, M. J., van der Tweel, I., Grobbee, D. E., Numans, M. F., & Geerlings, M. I. (2007). Estimating interaction on an additive scale between continuous determinants in a logistic regression model. Int J Epidemiol, 36(5), 1111-1118.

      Nikiforov, S. V., & Mamaev, V. B. (1998). The development of sex differences in cardiovascular disease mortality: a historical perspective. Am J Public Health, 88(9), 1348-1353. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.88.9.1348

      Paternoster, R., Brame, R., Mazerolle, P., & Piquero, A. (1998). Using the correct statistical test for the equality of regression coefficients. Criminology, 36(4), 859-866.

      Rothman, K. (2008). Greenland S, Lash TL (ed.). Modern Epidemiology. In: Philadelphia: Lippincott Wolliams & Wilkins.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary:

      The study identifies two types of activation: one that is cue-triggered and nonspecific to motion directions, and another that is specific to the exposed motion directions but occurs in a reversed manner. The finding that activity in the medial temporal lobe (MTL) preceded that in the visual cortex suggests that the visual cortex may serve as a platform for the manifestation of replay events, which potentially enhance visual sequence learning.

      Evaluations:

      Identifying the two types of activation after exposure to a sequence of motion directions is very interesting. The experimental design, procedures and analyses are solid. The findings are interesting and novel.

      In the original submission, it was not immediately clear to me why the second type of activation was suggested to occur spontaneously. The procedural differences in the analyses that distinguished between the two types of activation need to be a little better clarified. However, this concern has been satisfactorily addressed in the revision.

      We thank the reviewer for his/her positive evaluation and thoughtful comments. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This paper shows and analyzes an interesting phenomenon. It shows that when people are exposed to sequences of moving dots (That is moving dots in one direction, followed by another direction etc.), that showing either the starting movement direction, or ending movement direction causes a coarsegrained brain response that is similar to that elicited by the complete sequence of 4 directions. However, they show by decoding the sensor responses that this brain activity actually does not carry information about the actual sequence and the motion directions, at least not on the time scale of the initial sequence. They also show a reverse reply on a highly-compressed time scale, which is elicited during the period of elevated activity, and activated by the first and last elements of the sequence, but not others. Additionally, these replays seem to occur during periods of cortical ripples, similar to what is found in animal studies.

      These results are intriguing. They are based on MEG recordings in humans, and finding such replays in humans is novel. Also, this is based on what seems to be sophisticated statistical analysis. The statistical methodology seems valid, but due to its complexity it is not easy to understand. The methods especially those described in figures 3 and 4 should be explained better.  

      We thank the reviewer’s detailed evaluation. As suggested, we have further revised the Methods and Results sections, particularly the descriptions related to Figures 3 and 4, to enhance clarity. Please see the revisions highlighted in red in the revised manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The most important results here are in Figure 4, and they rely on methods explained in Figure 3. Figure 4 and the results in the figure are confusing.

      What is the red bar in 4B,E. What are the units of the Y axis in figure 4B,E?

      Does sequenceness have units? How do we interpret these magnitudes apart from the line of statistical significance? Shouldn't there be two lines, one for forward replay and the other for backward replay rather than a single line with positive and negative values? The term sequnceness is defined in figure 3, and is key. The replayed sequence in figure 4A,D seems to last about 120 ms.

      What is the meaning of having significance only within a window of 28-36 ms?

      We thank the reviewer’s careful reading and insightful comments. We apologize for the lack of clarity regarding these details in the previous version. As mentioned above, we have revised the Methods and Results sections to enhance clarity throughout the manuscript. For convenience, we provide detailed explanations addressing the specific points raised by the reviewer below.

      First, the red bars in Figures 4B and 4E indicate the lags when the evidence of sequenceness surpassed the statistical significance threshold, as determined by permutation testing. We have now explicitly clarified this in the revised figure captions.

      Second, sequenceness doesn’t have units. It corresponds to the regression coefficient (β) obtained from the second-level GLM in the TDLM framework. Specifically, in the first step of TDLM, we constructed an empirical transition matrix that quantifies the evidence for all possible transitions (e.g., 0° → 90°) at each time lag (Δt). In the second step, we evaluated the extent to which each model transition matrix (e.g., forward or backward transitions) predicts the empirical transition matrix at each Δt, yielding second-level β values. Sequenceness is defined as the difference between the β values for the forward and backward transition models, reflecting the relative strength and directionality of sequential replay. As it is derived from regression coefficients, sequenceness is inherently a unitless measure.

      Regarding the interpretation of sequenceness magnitudes beyond statistical significance, the β values reflect the extent to which the model transition matrix explains variance in the empirical transition matrix. While larger β values suggest stronger sequenceness, absolute magnitudes are influenced by various factors, such as between-participant noise. Therefore, the key criterion for interpreting these values is whether they surpass permutationbased significance thresholds, which indicate that the observed sequenceness is unlikely to have occurred by chance.

      Third, as the reviewer correctly pointed out, we initially computed two separate regression lines, one for forward replay and the other for backward replay. We then defined sequenceness as the contrast between the forward and backward replay (forward minus backward). This contrast approach is commonly used in previous studies to remove between-participant variance in the sequential replay per se, which may arise due to variability in task engagement or measurement sensitivity (Liu et al., 2021; Nour et al., 2021).

      Finally, regarding the duration of replay events, the example sequences shown in Figures 4A and 4D indeed span about 120 ms in total. However, the time lag (Δt) between successive reactivation peaks within these sequences is about 30 ms. This is in line with the findings shown in Figures 4B and 4E, where statistical significance is observed at a time lag window of 28 – 36 ms on the x-axis. It is important to note that the x-axis in these plots represents the time lag (Δt) between sequential reactivations, rather than absolute time.

      We hope these clarifications address the reviewer’s concerns, and we have revised the manuscript accordingly to make these points clearer to readers.

      The methods here are not simple and not simple to explain. The new version is easier to understand. From the new version it seems that the methodology is sound. It should be still clarified and better explained.

      We have carefully revised the manuscript to better explain the methodology. We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback, which is valuable in improving the clarity of our work.

      Now that I understand what they mean by decoding probability, I think that this term is confusing or even misleading. The decoding accuracy is the probability that the direction of motion classification was correct. It seems the so-called decoding probability is value of the logistic regression after normalizing the sum to 1. If this is a standard term it can probably be kept, if not another term would be better.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We agree that the term decoding probability may initially seem confusing. However, decoding probability is a commonly used term in the neural decoding literature, particularly in human studies (e.g., Liu et al., 2019; Nour et al., 2021; Turner et al., 2023). To maintain consistency with previous work, we have kept this term in the manuscript. We appreciate the opportunity to clarify this point.

      References

      Liu, Y., Dolan, R. J., Higgins, C., Penagos, H., Woolrich, M. W., Ólafsdóttir, H. F., Barry, C., Kurth-Nelson, Z., & Behrens, T. E. (2021). Temporally delayed linear modelling (TDLM) measures replay in both animals and humans. eLife, 10, e66917. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.66917

      Liu, Y., Dolan, R. J., Kurth-Nelson, Z., & Behrens, T. E. J. (2019). Human Replay Spontaneously Reorganizes Experience. Cell, 178(3), 640-652.e14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2019.06.012

      Nour, M. M., Liu, Y., Arumuham, A., Kurth-Nelson, Z., & Dolan, R. J. (2021). Impaired neural replay of inferred relationships in schizophrenia. Cell, 184(16), 4315-4328.e17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2021.06.012

      Turner, W., Blom, T., & Hogendoorn, H. (2023). Visual Information Is Predictively Encoded in Occipital Alpha/Low-Beta Oscillations. Journal of Neuroscience, 43(30), 5537–5545. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0135-23.2023

    1. Author response:

      We thank the editors and the reviewers for their valuable comments and for taking the time to evaluate our manuscript.

      Answers to Reviewer 1:

      (1) The core contribution of our method is that it learns meaningful spatiotemporal embeddings directly from image data without requiring pose estimation or eigenworm-based features as input. The learned embedding space can serve as a foundation for downstream tasks such as behavioral classification, clustering, or anomaly detection, further supporting its utility beyond visualization through eigenworm-derived features. Here we use the Tierpsy-derived features for latent space interpretation and for validation that our approach does indeed encode meaningful postural information. Additionally, without any Tierpsy-calculated features users can still color embeddings by known metadata like mutation or age and compare different strains to each other. 

      (2) The numbers shown in Fig. 2.3 are illustrative placeholders intended to conceptually represent a vector of behavioral features. They do not correspond to any specific measurements or carry intrinsic meaning. We agree that this may lead to confusion, and we will clarify this in the revised manuscript.

      (3) The visualizations in Figs. 4 (b) and (c) show the embeddings of sequences of behavior, rather than individual poses. Therefore, motion-related features such as speed are related to temporal patterns in those sequences rather than static postures. The color overlays reflect average motion characteristics (e.g., speed) of short behavior clips projected into the embedding space, rather than being directly linked to any single frame or pose.

      Answers to Reviewer 2:

      (1) In the abstract, our use of the term "unbiased" refers specifically to the avoidance of human-generated bias through feature engineering—i.e., the model does not rely on handcrafted features or predefined pose representations – the representations are based on data only. However, we agree that the model is still subject to dataset biases and will rectify this in the revised manuscript.

      (2) The worm images are rotated to a common vertical orientation to remove orientation as a source of variability in the input. This ensures that the model focuses on learning pose and behavioral dynamics rather than arbitrary head-tail or angular positioning. While data augmentation could in theory account for this variability, we found in our preliminary experiments that applying this preprocessing step led to more stable and interpretable embeddings.

      (3) We agree that simplifying the technical explanations would enhance the manuscript’s accessibility. In the revised version, we will briefly introduce contrastive learning in a less technical language.

      (4) The gray points in Fig. 3a represent frames that Tierpsy could not resolve, primarily due to coiled, self-intersecting, or overlapping worm postures as Tierpsy uses skeletonization to estimate the centerline. This approach can fail if kind of challenging elements are part of the image.

      (5) We appreciate this suggestion and consider it for a revised version of the manuscript.

      (6) Although it may seem intuitive for highly bent (red) poses to lie near coiled (gray) ones in the embedding space, the clustering pattern observed reflects how the network organizes pose information. The red/orange cluster consists of distinguishable bent poses that are visually distinct and consistently separable from other postures. In contrast, the greenish and blueish poses are less strongly bent and may share more visual overlap with the unresolved (gray) images.

      (7) The overlap occurs because some highly bent or coiled worms can still be (partially) resolved by Tierpsy, depending on specific pose conditions (e.g., head and tail not touching, not self-overlapping). However, Tierpsy fails to consistently resolve such frames. We will describe these cases in more detail in the revised manuscript.

      (8) Thank you, we agree this claim needs to be better supported and will develop it in the revision.

      (9) To support this statement we mainly visualized the respective sequences embedded in this area of the embedding space and found that it mostly consists of common behaviors such as forward locomotion. 

      (10) We agree that interpretability is important and plan to include additional figures quantifications of the embedding space using more basic Tierpsy features.

      (11) Fig. 5a is indeed based solely on N2 animals. In the revised manuscript we will include quantitative measures of behavioral variability and its change with age.

      (12) We appreciate this suggestion and consider it for a revised version

      (13) We agree this would be a valuable analysis. However, our current dataset primarily includes aging data for N2 animals. We acknowledge this limitation and consider adding more strains for future work.

      (14) We will include links to our source code in the revised manuscript

      Answers to Reviewer 3:

      (1-2) Our current method is agnostic to head-tail orientation, which indeed restricts the ability to distinguish behaviors that rely on directional cues. We made this design choice as we believe that correctly identifying head/tail orientation can be a challenging task that may introduce additional biases or fail in difficult imaging conditions. However, we fully agree that integrating directional information would improve behavioral resolution, and this is a natural extension of our current framework. In future work, we aim to incorporate head-tail disambiguation.

      (3) We explicitly designed our preprocessing and training pipeline to encourage size invariance, for example by resizing individuals to a consistent scale, as the focus of our work is to encode posture and movement only. However, we acknowledge that absolute size information is lost in this process, which can be informative for distinguishing genotypes or age-related changes.

      (4) We agree that a direct quantitative comparison between our embedding-based representations and skeleton-based feature sets would strengthen the paper. Our current focus was to assess whether meaningful behavioral features could be learned from a skeleton-free representation.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer 1:

      (1) In general, the representation of target and distractor processing is a bit of a reach. Target processing is represented by SSVEP amplitude, which is most likely going to be related to the contrast of the dots, as opposed to representing coherent motion energy, which is the actual target. These may well be linked (e.g., greater attention to the coherent motion task might increase SSVEP amplitude), but I would call it a limitation of the interpretation. Decoding accuracy of emotional content makes sense as a measure of distractor processing, and the supplementary analysis comparing target SSVEP amplitude to distractor decoding accuracy is duly noted.

      We agree with the reviewer. This is certainly a limitation and will be acknowledged as such in the revised manuscript.

      (2) Comparing SSVEP amplitude to emotional category decoding accuracy feels a bit like comparing apples with oranges. They have different units and scales and probably reflect different neural processes. Is the result the authors find not a little surprising in this context? This relationship does predict performance and is thus intriguing, but I think this methodological aspect needs to be discussed further. For example, is the phase relationship with behaviour a result of a complex interaction between different levels of processing (fundamental contrast vs higher order emotional processing)?

      Traditionally, the SSVEP amplitude at the distractor frequency is used to quantify distractor processing. Given that the target SSVEP amplitude is stronger than that for the distractor, it is possible that the distractor SSVEP amplitude is contaminated by the target SSVEP amplitude due to spectral power leakage; see Figure S4 for a demonstration of this. Because of this issue we therefore introduce the use of decoding accuracy as an index of distractor processing. This has not been done in the SSVEP literature. The lack of correlation between the distractor SSVEP amplitude and the distractor decoding accuracy, although it is kind of like comparing apples with oranges as pointed out by the reviewer, serves the purpose of showing that these two measures are not co-varying, and the use of decoding accuracy is free from the influence of the distractor SSVEP amplitude and thereby free from the influence by the target SSVEP amplitude. This is an important point. We will provide a more thorough discussion of this point in the revised manuscript. 

      Reviewer 2:

      (1) Incomplete Evidence for Rhythmicity at 1 Hz: The central claim of 1 Hz rhythmic sampling is insufficiently validated. The windowing procedure (0.5s windows with 0.25s step) inherently restricts frequency resolution, potentially biasing toward low-frequency components like 1 Hz. Testing different window durations or providing controls would significantly strengthen this claim.

      This is an important point. We plan to follow the reviewer’s suggestion and repeat our analysis using different window sizes to test the robustness of the observed 1Hz rhythmicity. In addition, we plan to also apply the Hilbert transform to extract time-point-by-time-point amplitude envelopes, which will provide a window-free estimation of the distractor strength and further validate the presence of the low-frequency 1Hz dynamics.

      (2) No-Distractor Control Condition: The study lacks a baseline or control condition without distractors. This makes it difficult to determine whether the distractor-related decoding signals or the 1 Hz effect reflect genuine distractor processing or more general task dynamics.

      We agree with the reviewer. This is certainly a limitation and will be acknowledged as such in the revised manuscript.

      (3) Decoding Near Chance Levels: The pairwise decoding accuracies for distractor categories hover close to chance (~55%), raising concerns about robustness. While statistically above chance, the small effect sizes need careful interpretation, particularly when linked to behavior.

      This is a good point. In addition to acknowledging this in the revised manuscript, we will carry out two additional analyses to test this issue further. First, we will implement a random permutation procedure, in which the trial labels are randomly shuffled and the null-hypothesis distribution for decoding accuracy is built, and compare the decoding accuracy from the actual data to this distribution. Second, we will perform a temporal generalization analysis to examine whether the neural representations of the distractor drift over the course of an entire trial, which is 11 seconds long. Recent studies suggest that even when the stimulus stays the same, their neural representations may drift over time.

      (4) No Clear Correlation Between SSVEP and Behavior: Neither target nor distractor signal strength (SSVEP amplitude) correlates with behavioral accuracy. The study instead relies heavily on relative phase, which - while interesting - may benefit from additional converging evidence.

      We felt that what the reviewer pointed out is actually the main point of our study, namely, it is not the overall target or distractor strength that matters for behavior, it is their temporal relationship that matters for behavior. This reveals a novel neuroscience principle that has not been reported in the past. We will stress this point further in the revised manuscript.

      (5) Phase-analysis: phase analysis is performed between different types of signals hindering their interpretability (time-resolved SSVEP amplitude and time-resolved decoding accuracy).

      The time-resolved SSVEP amplitude is used to index the temporal dynamics of target processing whereas the time-resolved decoding accuracy is used to index the temporal dynamics of distractor processing. As such, they can be compared, using relative phase for example, to examine how temporal relations between the two types of processes impact behavior. This said, we do recognize the reviewer’s concern that these two processes are indexed by two different types of signals. We plan to normalize each time course, make them dimensionless, and then compute the temporal relations between them.   

      Appraisal of Aims and Conclusions:

      The authors largely achieved their stated goal of assessing rhythmic sampling of distractors. However, the conclusions drawn - particularly regarding the presence of 1 Hz rhythmicity - rest on analytical choices that should be scrutinized further. While the observed phase-performance relationship is interesting and potentially impactful, the lack of stronger and convergent evidence on the frequency component itself reduces confidence in the broader conclusions.

      Impact and Utility to the Field:

      If validated, the findings will advance our understanding of attentional dynamics and competition in complex visual environments. Demonstrating that ignored distractors can be rhythmically sampled at similar frequencies to targets has implications for models of attention and cognitive control. However, the methodological limitations currently constrain the paper's impact.

      Thanks for these comments and positive assessment of our work’s potential implications and impact. We will try our best in the revision process to address the concerns.

      Additional Context and Considerations:

      (1) The use of EEG-fMRI is mentioned but not leveraged. If BOLD data were collected, even exploratory fMRI analyses (e.g., distractor modulation in visual cortex) could provide valuable converging evidence.

      Indeed, leveraging fMRI data in EEG studies would be very beneficial, as having been demonstrated in our previous work. However, given that this study concerns the temporal relationship between target and distractor processing, it is felt that fMRI, given its well-known limitation in temporal resolution, has limited potential to contribute. We will be exploring this rich dataset in other ways where the two modalities are integrated to gain more insights not possible with either modality used alone.

      (2) In turn, removal of fMRI artifacts might introduce biases or alter the data. For instance, the authors might consider investigating potential fMRI artifact harmonics around 1 Hz to address concerns regarding induced spectral components.

      We have done extensive work in the area of simultaneous EEG-fMRI and have not encountered artifacts with a 1Hz rhythmicity. Also, the fact that the temporal relations between target processing and distractor processing at 1Hz predict behavior is another indication that the 1Hz rhythmicity is a neuroscientific effect not an artifact. However, we will be looking into this carefully and address this in the revision process.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This computational modeling study builds on multiple previous lines of experimental and theoretical research to investigate how a single neuron can solve a nonlinear pattern classification task. The authors construct a detailed biophysical and morphological model of a single striatal medium spiny neuron, and endow excitatory and inhibitory synapses with dynamic synaptic plasticity mechanisms that are sensitive to (1) the presence or absence of a dopamine reward signal, and (2) spatiotemporal coincidence of synaptic activity in single dendritic branches. The latter coincidence is detected by voltage-dependent NMDA-type glutamate receptors, which can generate a type of dendritic spike referred to as a "plateau potential." The proposed mechanisms result in moderate performance on a nonlinear classification task when specific input features are segregated and clustered onto individual branches, but reduced performance when input features are randomly distributed across branches. Given the high level of complexity of all components of the model, it is not clear which features of which components are most important for its performance. There is also room for improvement in the narrative structure of the manuscript and the organization of concepts and data.

      Strengths:

      The integrative aspect of this study is its major strength. It is challenging to relate low-level details such as electrical spine compartmentalization, extrasynaptic neurotransmitter concentrations, dendritic nonlinearities, spatial clustering of correlated inputs, and plasticity of excitatory and inhibitory synapses to high-level computations such as nonlinear feature classification. Due to high simulation costs, it is rare to see highly biophysical and morphological models used for learning studies that require repeated stimulus presentations over the course of a training procedure. The study aspires to prove the principle that experimentally-supported biological mechanisms can explain complex learning.

      Weaknesses:

      The high level of complexity of each component of the model makes it difficult to gain an intuition for which aspects of the model are essential for its performance, or responsible for its poor performance under certain conditions. Stripping down some of the biophysical detail and comparing it to a simpler model may help better understand each component in isolation. That said, the fundamental concepts behind nonlinear feature binding in neurons with compartmentalized dendrites have been explored in previous work, so it is not clear how this study represents a significant conceptual advance. Finally, the presentation of the model, the motivation and justification of each design choice, and the interpretation of each result could be restructured for clarity to be better received by a wider audience.

      Thank you for the feedback! We agree that the complexity of our model can make it challenging to intuitively understand the underlying mechanisms. To address this, we have revised the manuscript to include additional simulations and clearer explanations of the mechanisms at play.

      In the revised introduction, we now explicitly state our primary aim: to assess to what extent a biophysically detailed neuron model can support the theory proposed by Tran-Van-Minh et al. and explore whether such computations can be learned by a single neuron, specifically a projection neuron in the striatum. To achieve this, we focus on several key mechanisms:

      (1) A local learning rule: We develop a learning rule driven by local calcium dynamics in the synapse and by reward signals from the neuromodulator dopamine. This plasticity rule is based on the known synaptic machinery for triggering LTP or LTD in the corticostriatal synapse onto dSPNs (Shen et al., 2008). Importantly, the rule does not rely on supervised learning paradigms and neither is a separate training and testing phase needed.

      (2) Robust dendritic nonlinearities: According to Tran-Van-Minh et al., (2015) sufficient supralinear integration is needed to ensure that e.g. two inputs (i.e. one feature combination in the NFBP, Figure 1A) on the same dendrite generate greater somatic depolarization than if those inputs were distributed across different dendrites. To accomplish this we generate sufficiently robust dendritic plateau potentials using the approach in Trpevski et al., (2023). 

      (3) Metaplasticity: Although not discussed much in more theoretical work, our study demonstrates the necessity of metaplasticity for achieving stable and physiologically realistic synaptic weights. This mechanism ensures that synaptic strengths remain within biologically plausible ranges during training, regardless of initial synaptic weights.

      We have also clarified our design choices and the rationale behind them, as well as restructured the interpretation of our results for greater accessibility. We hope these revisions make our approach and findings more transparent and easier to engage with for a broader audience.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      This study extends three previous lines of work:  

      (1) Prior computational/phenomenological work has shown that the presence of dendritic nonlinearities can enable single neurons to perform linearly non-separable tasks like XOR and feature binding (e.g. Tran-Van-Minh et al., Front. Cell. Neurosci., 2015).

      Prior computational and phenomenological work, such as Tran-Van-Minh et al. (Front. Cell. Neurosci., 2015), directly inspired our study, as we now explicitly state in the introduction (page 4, lines 19-22). While Tran-Van-Minh theoretically demonstrated that these principles could solve the NFBP, it remains untested to what extent this can be achieved quantitatively in biophysically detailed neuron models using biologically plausible learning rules - which is what we test here.

      (2) This study and a previous biophysical modeling study (Trpevski et al., Front. Cell. Neurosci., 2023) rely heavily on the finding from Chalifoux & Carter, J. Neurosci., 2011 that blocking glutamate transporters with TBOA increases dendritic calcium signals. The proposed model thus depends on a specific biophysical mechanism for dendritic plateau potential generation, where spatiotemporally clustered inputs must be co-activated on a single branch, and the voltage compartmentalization of the branch and the voltage-dependence of NMDARs is not enough, but additionally glutamate spillover from neighboring synapses must activate extrasynaptic NMDARs. If this specific biophysical implementation of dendritic plateau potentials is essential to the findings in this study, the authors have not made that connection clear. If it is a simple threshold nonlinearity in dendrites that is important for the model, and not the specific underlying biophysical mechanisms, then the study does not appear to provide a conceptual advance over previous studies demonstrating nonlinear feature binding with simpler implementations of dendritic nonlinearities.

      We appreciate the feedback on the hypothesized role of glutamate spillover in our model. While the current manuscript and Trpevski et al. (2023) emphasize glutamate spillover as a plausible biophysical mechanism to provide sufficiently robust and supralinear plateau potentials, we acknowledge, however, that the mechanisms of supralinearity of dendritic integration, might not depend solely on this specific mechanism in other types of neurons. In Trpevski et al (2023) we, however, realized that if we allow too ‘graded’ dendritic plateaus, using the quite shallow Mg-block reported in experiments, it was difficult to solve the NFBP. The conceptual advance of our study lies in demonstrating that sufficiently nonlinear dendritic integration is needed and that this can be accounted for by assuming spillover in SPNs—but regardless of its biophysical source (e.g. NMDA spillover, steeper NMDA Mg block activation curves or other voltage dependent conductances that cause supralinear dendritic integration)—it enables biophysically detailed neurons to solve the nonlinear feature binding problem. To address this point and clarify the generality of our conclusions, we have revised the relevant sections in the manuscript to state this explicitly.

      (3) Prior work has utilized "sliding-threshold," BCM-like plasticity rules to achieve neuronal selectivity and stability in synaptic weights. Other work has shown coordinated excitatory and inhibitory plasticity. The current manuscript combines "metaplasticity" at excitatory synapses with suppression of inhibitory strength onto strongly activated branches. This resembles the lateral inhibition scheme proposed by Olshausen (Christopher J. Rozell, Don H. Johnson, Richard G. Baraniuk, Bruno A. Olshausen; Sparse Coding via Thresholding and Local Competition in Neural Circuits. Neural Comput 2008; 20 (10): 2526-2563. doi: https://doi.org/10.1162/neco.2008.03-07-486). However, the complexity of the biophysical model makes it difficult to evaluate the relative importance of the additional complexity of the learning scheme.

      We initially tried solving the NFBP with only excitatory plasticity, which worked reasonably well, especially if we assume a small population of neurons collaborates under physiological conditions. However, we observed that plateau potentials from distally located inputs were less effective, and we now explain this limitation in the revised manuscript (page 14, lines 23-37).

      To address this, we added inhibitory plasticity inspired by mechanisms discussed in Castillo et al. (2011) , Ravasenga et al., and Chapman et al. (2022) , as now explicitly stated in the text (page 32, lines 23-26). While our GABA plasticity rule is speculative, it demonstrates that distal GABAergic plasticity can enhance nonlinear computations. These results are particularly encouraging, as it shows that implementing these mechanisms at the single-neuron level produces behavior consistent with network-level models like BCM-like plasticity rules and those proposed by Rozell et al. We hope this will inspire further experimental work on inhibitory plasticity mechanisms.

      P2, paragraph 2: Grammar: "multiple dendritic regions, preferentially responsive to different input values or features, are known to form with close dendritic proximity." The meaning is not clear. "Dendritic regions" do not "form with close dendritic proximity."

      Rewritten (current page 2, line 35)

      P5, paragraph 3: Grammar: I think you mean "strengthened synapses" not "synapses strengthened".

      Rewritten (current page 14, line 36)

      P8, paragraph 1: Grammar: "equally often" not "equally much".

      Updated (current page 10, line 2)

      P8, paragraph 2: "This is because of the learning rule that successively slides the LTP NMDA Ca-dependent plasticity kernel over training." It is not clear what is meant by "sliding," either here or in the Methods. Please clarify.

      We have updated the text and removed the word “sliding” throughout the manuscript to clarify that the calcium dependence of the kernels are in fact updated

      P10, Figure 3C (left): After reading the accompanying text on P8, para 2, I am left not understanding what makes the difference between the two groups of synapses that both encode "yellow," on the same dendritic branch (d1) (so both see the same plateau potentials and dopamine) but one potentiates and one depresses. Please clarify.

      Some "yellow" and "banana" synapses are initialized with weak conductances, limiting their ability to learn due to the relatively slow dynamics of the LTP kernel. These weak synapses fail to reach the calcium thresholds necessary for potentiation during a dopamine peak, yet they remain susceptible to depression under LTD conditions. Initially, the dynamics of the LTP kernel does not allow significant potentiation, even in the presence of appropriate signals such as plateau potentials and dopamine (page 10, lines 22–26). We have added a more detailed explanation of how the learning rule operates in the section “Characterization of the Synaptic Plasticity Rule” on page 9 and have clarified the specific reason why the weaker yellow synapses undergo LTD (page 11, lines 1–7).

      As shown in Supplementary Figure 6, during subthreshold learning, the initial conductance is also low, which similarly hinders the synapses' ability to potentiate. However, with sufficient dopamine, the LTP kernel adapts by shifting closer to the observed calcium levels, allowing these synapses to eventually strengthen. This dynamic highlights how the model enables initially weak synapses to "catch up" under consistent activation and favorable dopaminergic conditions.

      P9, paragraph 1: The phrase "the metaplasticity kernel" is introduced here without prior explanation or motivation for including this level of complexity in the model. Please set it up before you use it.

      A sentence introducing metaplasticity has been added to the introduction (page 3, lines 36-42) as well as on page 9, where the kernel is introduced (page 9, lines 26-35)

      P10, Figure 3D: "kernel midline" is not explained.

      We have replotted fig 3 to make it easier to understand what is shown. Also, an explanation of the Kernel midpoint is added to the legend (current page 12, line 19)

      P11, paragraph 1; P13, Fig. 4C: My interpretation of these data is that clustered connectivity with specific branches is essential for the performance of the model. Randomly distributing input features onto branches (allowing all 4 features to innervate single branches) results in poor performance. This is bad, right? The model can't learn unless a specific pre-wiring is assumed. There is not much interpretation provided at this stage of the manuscript, just a flat description of the result. Tell the reader what you think the implications of this are here.

      Thanks for the suggestion - we have updated this section of the manuscript, adding an interpretation of the results that the model often fails to learn both relevant stimuli if all four features are clustered onto the same dendrite (page 13, lines 31-42). 

      In summary, when multiple feature combinations are encoded in the same dendrite with similar conductances, the ability to determine which combination to store depends on the dynamics of the other dendrite. Small variations in conductance, training order, or other stochastic factors can influence the outcome. This challenge, known as the symmetry-breaking problem, has been previously acknowledged in abstract neuron models (Legenstein and Maass, 2011). To address this, additional mechanisms such as branch plasticity—amplifying or attenuating the plateau potential as it propagates from the dendrite to the soma—can be employed (Legenstein and Maass, 2011). 

      P12, paragraph 2; P13, Figure 4E: This result seems suboptimal, that only synapses at a very specific distance from the soma can be used to effectively learn to solve a NFBP. It is not clear to what extent details of the biophysical and morphological model are contributing to this narrow distance-dependence, or whether it matches physiological data.

      We have added Figure 5—figure supplement 1A to clarify why distal synapses may not optimally contribute to learning. This figure illustrates how inhibitory plasticity improves performance by reducing excessive LTD at distal dendrites, thereby enhancing stimulus discrimination. Relevant explanations have been integrated into Page 18, Lines 25-39 in the revised manuscript.

      P14, paragraph 2: Now the authors are assuming that inhibitory synapses are highly tuned to stimulus features. The tuning of inhibitory cells in the hippocampus and cortex is controversial but seems generally weaker than excitatory cells, commensurate with their reduced number relative to excitatory cells. The model has accumulated a lot of assumptions at this point, many without strong experimental support, which again might make more sense when proposing a new theory, but this stitching together of complex mechanisms does not provide a strong intuition for whether the scheme is either biologically plausible or performant for a general class of problem.

      We acknowledge that it is not currently known whether inhibitory synapses in the striatum are tuned to stimulus features. However, given that the striatum is a purely inhibitory structure, it is plausible that lateral inhibition from other projection neurons could be tuned to features, even if feedforward inhibition from interneurons is not. Therefore, we believe this assumption is reasonable in the context of our model. As noted earlier, the GABA plasticity rule in our study is speculative. However, we hope that our work will encourage further experimental investigations, as we demonstrate that if GABAergic inputs are sufficiently specific, they can significantly enhance computations (This is discussed on page 17, lines 8-15.).

      P16, Figure 5E legend: The explanation of the meaning of T_max and T_min in the legend and text needs clarification.

      The abbreviations  T<sub>min</sub> and  T<sub>max</sub> have been updated to CTL and CTH to better reflect their role in calcium threshold tracking. The Figure 5E legend and relevant text have been revised for clarity. Additionally, the Methods section has been reorganized for better readability.

      P16, Figure 5B, C: When the reader reaches this paper, the conundrums presented in Figure 4 are resolved. The "winner-takes-all" inhibitory plasticity both increases the performance when all features are presented to a single branch and increases the range of somatodendritic distances where synapses can effectively be used for stimulus discrimination. The problem, then, is in the narrative. A lot more setup needs to be provided for the question related to whether or not dendritic nonlinearity and synaptic inhibition can be used to perform the NFBP. The authors may consider consolidating the results of Fig. 4 and 5 so that the comparison is made directly, rather than presenting them serially without much foreshadowing.

      In order to facilitate readability, we have updated the following sections of the manuscript to clarify how inhibitory plasticity resolves challenges from Figure 4:

      Figure 5B and Figure 5–figure supplement 1B: Two new panels illustrate the role of inhibitory plasticity in addressing symmetry problems.

      Figure 5–figure supplement 1A: Shows how inhibitory plasticity extends the effective range of somatodendritic distances.

      P18, Figure 6: This should be the most important figure, finally tying in all the previous complexity to show that NFBP can be partially solved with E and I plasticity even when features are distributed randomly across branches without clustering. However, now bringing in the comparison across spillover models is distracting and not necessary. Just show us the same plateau generation model used throughout the paper, with and without inhibition.

      Figure updated. Accumulative spillover and no-spillover conditions have been removed.

      P18, paragraph 2: "In Fig. 6C, we report that a subset of neurons (5 out of 31) successfully solved the NFBP." This study could be significantly strengthened if this phenomenon could (perhaps in parallel) be shown to occur in a simpler model with a simpler plateau generation mechanism. Furthermore, it could be significantly strengthened if the authors could show that, even if features are randomly distributed at initialization, a pruning mechanism could gradually transition the neuron into the state where fewer features are present on each branch, and the performance could approach the results presented in Figure 5 through dynamic connectivity.

      To model structural plasticity is a good suggestion that should be investigated in later work, however, we feel that it goes beyond what we can do in the current manuscript.  We now acknowledge that structural plasticity might play a role. For example we show that if we can assume ‘branch-specific’ spillover, that leads to sufficiently development of local dendritic non-linearities, also one can learn with distributed inputs. In reality, structural plasticity is likely important here, as we now state (current page 22, line 35-42). 

      P17, paragraph 2: "As shown in Fig. 6B, adding the hypothetical nonlinearities to the model increases the performance towards solving part of the NFBP, i.e. learning to respond to one relevant feature combination only. The performance increases with the amount of nonlinearity." This is not shown in Figure 6B.

      Sentence removed. We have added a Figure 6 - figure supplement 1 to better explain the limitations.

      P22, paragraph 1: The "w" parameter here is used to determine whether spatially localized synapses are co-active enough to generate a plateau potential. However, this is the same w learned through synaptic plasticity. Typically LTP and LTD are thought of as changing the number of postsynaptic AMPARs. Does this "w" also change the AMPAR weight in the model? Do the authors envision this as a presynaptic release probability quantity? If so, please state that and provide experimental justification. If not, please justify modifying the activation of postsynaptic NMDARs through plasticity.

      This is an important remark. Our plasticity model differs from classical LTP models as it depends on the link between LTP and increased spillover as described by Henneberger et al., (2020).

      We have updated the method section (page 27, lines 6-11), and we acknowledge, however, that in a real cell, learning might first strengthen the AMPA component, but after learning the ratio of NMDA/AMPA is unchanged ( Watt et al., 2004). This re-balancing between NMDA and AMPA might perhaps be a slower process.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The study explores how single striatal projection neurons (SPNs) utilize dendritic nonlinearities to solve complex integration tasks. It introduces a calcium-based synaptic learning rule that incorporates local calcium dynamics and dopaminergic signals, along with metaplasticity to ensure stability for synaptic weights. Results show SPNs can solve the nonlinear feature binding problem and enhance computational efficiency through inhibitory plasticity in dendrites, emphasizing the significant computational potential of individual neurons. In summary, the study provides a more biologically plausible solution to single-neuron learning and gives further mechanical insights into complex computations at the single-neuron level.

      Strengths:

      The paper introduces a novel learning rule for training a single multicompartmental neuron model to perform nonlinear feature binding tasks (NFBP), highlighting two main strengths: the learning rule is local, calcium-based, and requires only sparse reward signals, making it highly biologically plausible, and it applies to detailed neuron models that effectively preserve dendritic nonlinearities, contrasting with many previous studies that use simplified models.

      Weaknesses:

      I am concerned that the manuscript was submitted too hastily, as evidenced by the quality and logic of the writing and the presentation of the figures. These issues may compromise the integrity of the work. I would recommend a substantial revision of the manuscript to improve the clarity of the writing, incorporate more experiments, and better define the goals of the study.

      Thanks for the valuable feedback. We have now gone through the whole manuscript updating the text, and also improved figures and added some supplementary figures to better explain model mechanisms. In particular, we state more clearly our goal already in the introduction.

      Major Points:

      (1) Quality of Scientific Writing: The current draft does not meet the expected standards. Key issues include:

      i. Mathematical and Implementation Details: The manuscript lacks comprehensive mathematical descriptions and implementation details for the plasticity models (LTP/LTD/Meta) and the SPN model. Given the complexity of the biophysically detailed multicompartment model and the associated learning rules, the inclusion of only nine abstract equations (Eq. 1-9) in the Methods section is insufficient. I was surprised to find no supplementary material providing these crucial details. What parameters were used for the SPN model? What are the mathematical specifics for the extra-synaptic NMDA receptors utilized in this study? For instance, Eq. 3 references [Ca2+]-does this refer to calcium ions influenced by extra-synaptic NMDARs, or does it apply to other standard NMDARs? I also suggest the authors provide pseudocodes for the entire learning process to further clarify the learning rules.

      The model is quite detailed but builds on previous work. For this reason, for model components used in earlier published work (and where models are already available via model repositories, such as ModelDB), we refer the reader to these resources in order to improve readability and to highlight what is novel in this paper - the learning rules itself. The learning rule is now explained in detail. For modelers that want to run the model, we have also provided a GitHub link to the simulation code. We hope this is a reasonable compromise to all readers, i.e, those that only want to understand what is new here (learning rule) and those that also want to test the model code. We explain this to the readers at the beginning of the Methods section.

      ii. Figure quality. The authors seem not to carefully typeset the images, resulting in overcrowding and varying font sizes in the figures. Some of the fonts are too small and hard to read. The text in many of the diagrams is confusing. For example, in Panel A of Figure 3, two flattened images are combined, leading to small, distorted font sizes. In Panels C and D of Figure 7, the inconsistent use of terminology such as "kernels" further complicates the clarity of the presentation. I recommend that the authors thoroughly review all figures and accompanying text to ensure they meet the expected standards of clarity and quality.

      Thanks for directing our attention to these oversights. We have gone through the entire manuscript, updating the figures where needed, and we are making sure that the text and the figure descriptions are clear and adequate and use consistent terminology for all quantities.

      iii. Writing clarity. The manuscript often includes excessive and irrelevant details, particularly in the mathematical discussions. On page 24, within the "Metaplasticity" section, the authors introduce the biological background to support the proposed metaplasticity equation (Eq. 5). However, much of this biological detail is hypothesized rather than experimentally verified. For instance, the claim that "a pause in dopamine triggers a shift towards higher calcium concentrations while a peak in dopamine pushes the LTP kernel in the opposite direction" lacks cited experimental evidence. If evidence exists, it should be clearly referenced; otherwise, these assertions should be presented as theoretical hypotheses. Generally, Eq. 5 and related discussions should be described more concisely, with only a loose connection to dopamine effects until more experimental findings are available.

      The “Metaplasticity” section (pages 30-32) has been updated to be more concise, and the abundant references to dopamine have been removed.

      (2) Goals of the Study: The authors need to clearly define the primary objective of their research. Is it to showcase the computational advantages of the local learning rule, or to elucidate biological functions?

      We have explicitly stated our goal in the introduction (page 4, lines 19-22). Please also see the response to reviewer 1.

      i. Computational Advantage: If the intent is to demonstrate computational advantages, the current experimental results appear inadequate. The learning rule introduced in this work can only solve for four features, whereas previous research (e.g., Bicknell and Hausser, 2021) has shown capability with over 100 features. It is crucial for the authors to extend their demonstrations to prove that their learning rule can handle more than just three features. Furthermore, the requirement to fine-tune the midpoint of the synapse function indicates that the rule modifies the "activation function" of the synapses, as opposed to merely adjusting synaptic weights. In machine learning, modifying weights directly is typically more efficient than altering activation functions during learning tasks. This might account for why the current learning rule is restricted to a limited number of tasks. The authors should critically evaluate whether the proposed local learning rule, including meta-plasticity, actually offers any computational advantage. This evaluation is essential to understand the practical implications and effectiveness of the proposed learning rule.

      Thank you for your feedback. To address the concern regarding feature complexity, we extended our simulations to include learning with 9 and 25 features, achieving accuracies of 80% and 75%, respectively (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A). While our results demonstrate effective performance, the absence of external stabilizers—such as error-modulated functions used in prior studies like Bicknell and Hausser (2021)—means that the model's performance can be more sensitive to occasional incorrect outcomes. For instance, while accuracy might reach 90%, a few errors can significantly affect overall performance due to the lack of mechanisms to stabilize learning.

      In order to clarify the setup of the rule, we have added pseudocode in the revised manuscript (Pages 31-32) detailing how the learning rule and metaplasticity update synaptic weights based on calcium and dopamine signals. Additionally, we have included pseudocode for the inhibitory learning rule on Pages 34-35. In future work, we also aim to incorporate biologically plausible mechanisms, such as dopamine desensitization, to enhance stability.

      ii. Biological Significance: If the goal is to interpret biological functions, the authors should dig deeper into the model behaviors to uncover their biological significance. This exploration should aim to link the observed computational features of the model more directly with biological mechanisms and outcomes.

      As now clearly stated in the introduction, the goal of the study is to see whether and to what quantitative extent the theoretical solution of the NFBP proposed in Tran-Van-Minh et al. (2015) can be achieved with biophysically detailed neuron models and with a biologically inspired learning rule. The problem has so far been solved with abstract and phenomenological neuron models (Schiess et al., 2014; Legenstein and Maass, 2011) and also with a detailed neuron model but with a precalculated voltage-dependent learning rule (Bicknell and Häusser, 2021).

      We have also tried to better explain the model mechanisms by adding supplementary figures.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Minor:

      (1) The [Ca]NMDA in Figure 2A and 2C can have large values even when very few synapses are activated. Why is that? Is this setting biologically realistic?

      The elevated [Ca²⁺]NMDA with minimal synaptic activation arises from high spine input resistance, small spine volume, and NMDA receptor conductance, which scales calcium influx with synaptic strength. Physiological studies report spine calcium transients typically up to ~1 μM (Franks and Sejnowski 2002, DOI: 10.1002/bies.10193), while our model shows ~7 μM for 0.625 nS and around ~3 μM for 0.5 nS, exceeding this range. The calcium levels of the model might therefore be somewhat high compared to biologically measured levels - however, this does not impact the learning rule, as the functional dynamics of the rule remain robust across calcium variations.

      (2) In the distributed synapses session, the study introduces two new mechanisms "Threshold spillover" and "Accumulative spillover". Both mechanisms are not basic concepts but quantitative descriptions of them are missing.

      Thank you for your feedback. Based on the recommendations from Reviewer 1, we have simplified the paper by removing the "Accumulative spillover" and focusing solely on the "Thresholded spillover" mechanism. In the updated version of the paper, we refer to it only as glutamate spillover. However, we acknowledge (page 22, lines 40-42) that to create sufficient non-linearities, other mechanisms, like structural plasticity, might also be involved (although testing this in the model will have to be postponed to future work).

      (3) The learning rule achieves moderate performance when feature-relevant synapses are organized in pre-designed clusters, but for more general distributed synaptic inputs, the model fails to faithfully solve the simple task (with its performance of ~ 75%). Performance results indicate the learning rule proposed, despite its delicate design, is still inefficient when the spatial distribution of synapses grows complex, which is often the case on biological neurons. Moreover, this inefficiency is not carefully analyzed in this paper (e.g. why the performance drops significantly and the possible computation mechanism underlying it).

      The drop in performance when using distributed inputs (to a mean performance of 80%) is similar to the mean performance in the same situation in Bicknell and Hausser (2021), see their Fig. 3C. The drop in performance is due to that: i) the relevant feature combinations are not often colocalized on the same dendrite so that they can be strengthened together, and ii) even if they are, there may not be enough synapses to trigger the supralinear response from the branch spillover mechanism, i.e. the inputs are not summated in a supralinear way (Fig. 6B, most input configurations only reach 75%).

      Because of this, at most one relevant feature combination can be learned. In the several cases when the random distribution of synapses is favorable for both relevant feature combinations to be learned, the NFBP is solved (Figs. 6B, some performance lines reach 100 % and 6C, example of such a case). We have extended the relevant sections of the paper trying to highlight the above mentioned mechanisms.

      Further, the theoretical results in Tran-Van-Minh et al. 2015 already show that to solve the NFBP with supralinear dendrites requires features to be pre-clustered in order to evoke the supralinear dendritic response, which would activate the soma. The same number of synapses distributed across the dendrites i) would not excite the soma as strongly, and ii) would summate in the soma as in a point neuron, i.e. no supralinear events can be activated, which are necessary to solve the NFBP. Hence, one doesn’t expect distributed synaptic inputs to solve the NFBP with any kind of learning rule. 

      (4) Figure 5B demonstrates that on average adding inhibitory synapses can enhance the learning capabilities to solve the NFBP for different pattern configurations (2, 3, or 4 features), but since the performance for excitatory-only setup varies greatly between different configurations (Figure 4B, using 2 or 3 features can solve while 4 cannot), can the results be more precise about whether adding inhibitory synapses can help improve the learning with 4 features?

      In response to the question, we added a panel to Figure 5B showing that without inhibitory synapses, 5 out of 13 configurations with four features successfully learn, while with inhibitory synapses, this improves to 7 out of 13. Figure 5—figure supplement 1B provides an explanation for this improvement: page 18 line 10-24

      (5) Also, in terms of the possible role of inhibitory plasticity in learning, as only on-site inhibition is studied here, can other types of inhibition be considered, like on-path or off-path? Do they have similar or different effects?

      This is an interesting suggestion for future work. We observed relevant dynamics in Figure 6A, where inhibitory synapses increased their weights on-site when randomly distributed. Previous work by Gidon and Segev (2012) examined the effects of different inhibitory types on NMDA clusters, highlighting the role of on-site and off-path inhibition in shunting. In our context, on-site inhibition in the same branch, appears more relevant for maintaining compartmentalized dendritic processing.

      (6) Figure 6A is mentioned in the context of excitatory-only setup, but it depicts the setup when both excitatory and inhibitory synapses are included, which is discussed later in the paper. A correction should be made to ensure consistency.

      We have updated the figure and the text in order to make it more clear that simulations are run both with and without inhibition in this context (page 21 line 4-13)

      (7) In the "Ca and kernel dynamics" plots (Fig 3,5), some of the kernel midlines (solid line) are overlapped by dots, e.g. the yellow line in Fig 3D, and some kernel midlines look like dots, which leads to confusion. Suggest to separate plots of Ca and kernel dynamics for clarity. 

      The design of the figures has been updated to improve the visibility of the calcium and kernel dynamics during training.

      (8) The formulations of the learning rule are not well-organized, and the naming of parameters is kind of confusing, e.g. T_min, T_max, which by default represent time, means "Ca concentration threshold" here.

      The abbreviations of the thresholds  ( T<sub>min</sub>,  T<sub>max</sub> in the initial version) have been updated to CTL and CTH, respectively, to better reflect their role in tracking calcium levels. The mathematical formulations have further been reorganized for better readability. The revised Methods section now follows a more structured flow, first explaining the learning mechanisms, followed by the equations and their dependencies.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer 1 (Public Review):

      Thank you for the helpful comments. Below, we have quoted the relevant sections from the revised manuscript as we respond to the reviewer’s comments item-by-item.

      Weaknesses:

      While the task design in this study is intentionally stimulus-rich and places a minimal constraint on the animal to preserve naturalistic behavior, this is, unfortunately, a double-edged sword, as it also introduces additional variables that confound some of the neural analysis. Because of this, a general weakness of the study is a lack of clear interpretability of the task variable neural correlates. This is a limitation of the task, which includes many naturally correlated variables - however, I think with some additional analyses, the authors could strengthen some of their core arguments and significantly improve clarity.

      We acknowledge the weakness and have included additional analyses to compensate for it. The details are as follows in our reply to the subsequent comments.  

      For example, the authors argue, based on an ANN decoding analysis (Figure 2b), that PFC neurons encode spatial information - but the spatial coordinate that they decode (the distance to the active foraging zone) is itself confounded by the fact that animals exhibit different behavior in different sections of the arena. From the way the data are presented, it is difficult to tell whether the decoder performance reflects a true neural correlate of distance, or whether it is driven by behavior-associated activity that is evoked by different behaviors in different parts of the arena. The author's claim that PFC neurons encode spatial information could be substantiated with a more careful analysis of single-neuron responses to supplement the decoder analysis. For example, 1) They could show examples of single neurons that are active at some constant distance away from the foraging site, regardless of animal behavior, and 2) They could quantify how many neurons are significantly spatially modulated, controlling for correlates of behavior events. One possible approach to disambiguate this confound could be to use regression-based models of neuron spiking to quantify variance in neuron activity that is explained by spatial features, behavioral features, or both.

      First of all, we would like to point out that while the recording was made during naturalistic foraging with minimal constraints behaviorally, a well-trained rat displayed an almost fixed sequence of actions within each zone. The behavioral repertoire performed in each zone was very different from each other: exploratory behaviors in the N-zone, navigating back and forth in the F-zone, and licking sucrose while avoiding attacks in the E-zone. Therefore, the entire arena is not only divided by the geographical features but also by the distinct set of behaviors performed in each zone. This is evident in the data showing a higher decoding accuracy of spatial distance in the F-zone than in the N- or E-zone. In this sense, the heterogeneous encoding reflects heterogenous distribution of dominant behaviors (navigation in the F-zone and attack avoidance while foraging in the E-zone) and hence corroborate the reviewer’s comment at a macroscopic scale encompassing the entire arena.

      Having said that, the more critical question is whether the neural activity is more correlated with microscopic behaviors at every moment rather than the location decoded in the F-zone. As the reviewer suggested, the first-step is to analyze single-neuron activity to identify whether direct neural correlates of location exist. To this end, traditional place maps were constructed for individual neurons. Most neurons did not show cohesive place fields across different regions, indicating little-to-no direct place coding by individual neurons. Only a few neurons displayed recognizable place fields in a consistent manner. However, even these place fields were irregular and patchy, and therefore, nothing comparable to the place cells or grid cells found in the hippocampus or entorhinal cortex. Some examples firing maps have been added to Figure 2 and characterized in the text as below.

      “To determine whether location-specific neural activity exists at the single-cell level in our mPFC data, a traditional place map was constructed for individual neurons. Although most neurons did not show cohesive place fields across different regions in the arena, a few neurons modulated their firing rates based on the rat’s current location. However, even these neurons were not comparable to place cells in the hippocampus (O’Keefe & Dostrovsky, 1971) or grid cells in the entorhinal cortex (Hafting et al., 2005) as the place fields were patchy and irregular in some cases (Figure 2B; Units 66 and 125) or too large, spanning the entire zone rather than a discrete location within it (Units 26 and 56). The latter type of neuron has been identified in other studies (e.g., Kaefer et al., 2020).”

      Next, to verify whether the location decoding reflects neuronal activity due to external features or particular type of action, predicted location was compared between the opposite directions within the F-zone, inbound and outbound in reference to the goal area (Lobsterbot). If the encoding were specifically tied to a particular action or environmental stimuli, there should be a discrepancy when the ANN decoder trained with outbound trajectory is tested for predictions on the inbound path, and vice versa. However, the results showed no significant difference between the two trajectories, suggesting that the decoded distance was not simply reflecting neural responses to location-specific activities or environmental cues during navigation.

      “To determine whether the accuracy of the regressor varied depending on the direction of movement, we compared the decoding accuracy of the regressor for outbound (from the N- to E-zone) vs. inbound (from the E- to N- zone) navigation within the F-zone. There was no significant difference in decoding accuracy between outbound vs. inbound trips (paired t-test; t(39) = 1.52, p =.136), indicating that the stability of spatial encoding was maintained regardless of the moving direction or perceived context (Figure 2E).”

      Additionally, we applied the same regression analysis on a subset of data that were recorded while the door to the robot compartment was closed during the Lobsterbot sessions. This way, it is possible to test the decoding accuracy when the most salient spatial feature, the Lobsterbot, is blocked out of sight. The subset represents an average of 38.92% of the entire session. Interestingly, the decoding accuracy with the subset of data was higher accuracy than that with the entire dataset, indicating that the neural activities were not driven by a single salient landmark. This finding supports our conclusion that the location information can be decoded from a population of neurons rather than from individual neurons that are associated with environmental or proprioceptive cues. We have added the following description of results in the manuscript.

      “Previous analyses indicated that the distance regressor performed robustly regardless of movement direction, but there is a possibility that the decoder detects visual cues or behaviors specific to the E-zone. For example, neural activity related to Lobsterbot confrontation or licking behavior might be used by the regressor to decode distance. To rule out this possibility, we analyzed a subset of data collected when the compartment door was closed, preventing visual access to the Lobsterbot and sucrose port and limiting active foraging behavior. The regressor trained on this subset still decoded distance with a MAE of 12.14 (± 3.046) cm (paired t-test; t(39) = 12.17, p <.001). Notably, the regressor's performance was significantly higher with this subset than with the full dataset (paired t-test; t(39) = 9.895, p <.001).”

      As for the comment on “using regression-based models of neuron spiking to quantify variance in neuron activity that is explained by spatial features, behavioral features, or both”, it is difficult to separate a particular behavioral event let alone timestamping it since the rat’s location was being monitored in the constantly-moving, naturalistic stream of behaviors. However, as mentioned above, a new section entitled “Overlapping populations of mPFC neurons adaptively encode spatial information and defensive decision” argues against single-neuron based account by performing the feature importance analysis. The results showed that even when the top 20% of the most informative neurons were excluded, the remaining neural population could still decode both distance and events.  This analysis supports the idea of a population-wide mode shift rather than distinct subgroups of neurons specialized in processing different sensory or motor events. This idea is also expressed in the schematic diagrams featured in Figure 8 of the revision.

      To substantiate the claim that PFC neurons really switch between different coding "modes," the authors could include a version of this analysis where they have regressed out, or otherwise controlled for, these confounds. Otherwise, the claim that the authors have identified "distinctively different states of ensemble activity," as opposed to simple coding of salient task features, seems premature.

      A key argument in our study is that the mPFC neurons encode different abstract internal representations (distance and avoidance decision) at the level of population. This has been emphasized in the revision with additional analyses and discussions. Most of all, we performed single neuron-based analysis for both spatial encoding (place fields for individual neurons) and avoidance decision (PETHs for head entry and head withdrawal) and contrasted the results with the population analysis. Although some individual neurons displayed a fractured “place cell-like” activity, and some others showed modulated firing at the head-entry and the head-withdrawal events, the ensemble decoding extracted distance information for the current location of the animal at a much higher accuracy. Furthermore, the PCA analysis identified abstract feature dimensions especially regarding the activity in the E-zone that cannot be attributable to a small number of sensory- or motor-related neurons. 

      To mitigate the possibility that the PCA is driven primarily by a small subset of units responsive to salient behavioral events, we also applied PCA to the dataset excluding the activity in the 2-second time window surrounding the head entry and withdrawal. While this approach does not eliminate all cue- or behavior-related activity within the E-zone, it does remove the neural activity associated with emotionally significant events, such as entry into the E-zone, the first drop of sucrose, head withdrawal, and the attack. Even without these events, the PC identified in the E-zone was still separated from those in the F-zone and N-zone. This result again argues in support of distinct states of ensemble activity formed in accordance with different categories of behaviors performed in different zones. Finally, the Naïve Bayesian classifier trained with ensemble activity in the E-zone was able to predict the success and failure of avoidance that occur a few seconds later, indicating that the same population of neurons are encoding the avoidance decision rather than the location of the animal.

      Reviewer 1 (Recommendations):

      The authors include an analysis (Figure 4) of population responses using PCA on session-wide data, which they use to support the claim that PFC neurons encode distinctive neural states, particularly between the encounter zone and nesting/foraging zones. However, because the encounter zone contains unique stimulus and task events (sucrose, threat, etc.), and the samples for PCA are drawn from the entire dataset (including during these events), it seems likely that the Euclidean distance measures analyzed in Figure 4b are driven mostly by the neural correlates of these events rather than some more general change in "state" of PFC dynamics. This does not invalidate this analysis but renders it potentially redundant with the single neuron results shown in Figure 5 - and I think the interpretation of this as supporting a state transition in the coding scheme is somewhat misleading. The authors may consider performing a PCA/population vector analysis on the subset of timepoints that do not contain unique behavior events, rather than on session-wide data, or otherwise equalizing samples that correspond to behavioral events in different zones. Observing a difference in PC-projected population vectors drawn from samples that are not contaminated by unique encounter-related events would substantiate the idea that there is a general shift in neural activity that is more related to the change in context or goal state, and less directly to the distinguishing events themselves.

      Thank you for the comments. Indeed, this is a recurring theme where the reviewers expressed concerns and doubts about heterogenous encoding of different functional modes. Besides the systematic presentation of the results in the manuscript, from PETH to ANN and to Bayesian classifier, we argue, however, that the activity of the mPFC neurons is better represented by the population rather than loose collection of stimulus- or event-related neurons.

      The PCA results that we included as the evidence of distinct functional separation, might reflect activities driven by a small number of event-coding neurons in different zones. As mentioned in the public review, we conducted the same analysis on a subset of data that excluded neural activity potentially influenced by significant events in the E-zone. The critical times are defined as ± 1 second from these events and excluded from the neural data. Despite these exclusions, the results continued to show populational differences between zones, reinforcing the notion that neurons encode abstract behavioral states (decision to avoid or stay) without the sensory- or motor-related activity. Although this analysis does not completely eliminate all possible confounding factors emerging in different external and internal contexts, it provides extra support for the population-level switch occurring in different zones.

      In Figure 7, the authors include a schematic that suggests that the number of neurons representing spatial information increases in the foraging zone, and that they overlap substantially with neurons representing behaviors in the encounter zone, such as withdrawal. They show in Figure 3 that location decoding is better in the foraging zone, but I could not find any explicit analysis of single-neuron correlates of spatial information as suggested in the schematic. Is there a formal analysis that lends support to this idea? It would be simple, and informative, to include a quantification of the fraction of spatial- and behavior-modulated neurons in each zone to see if changes in location coding are really driven by "larger" population representations. Also, the authors could quantify the overlap between spatial- and behavior-modulated neurons in the encounter zone to explicitly test whether neurons "switch" their coding scheme.

      The Figure 7 (now Figure 8) is now completely revised. The schematic diagram is modified to show spatial and avoidance decision encoding by the overlapping population of mPFC neurons (Figure 8a). Most notably, there are very few neurons that encode location but not the avoidance decision or vice versa. This is indicated by the differently colored units in F-zone vs. E-zone. The model also included units that are “not” engaged in any type of encoding or engaged in only one-type of encoding although they are not the majority.

      We have also added a schematic for hypothetical switching mechanisms (Figure 8b) to describe the conceptual scheme for the initiation of encoding-mode switching (sensory-driven vs. arbitrator-driven process)

      “Two main hypotheses could explain this switch. A bottom-up hypothesis suggests sensory inputs or upstream signals dictate encoding priorities, while a top-down hypothesis proposes that an internal or external “arbitrator” selects the encoding mode and coordinates the relevant information (Figure 8B). Although the current study is only a first step toward finding the regulatory mechanism behind this switch, our control experiment, where rats reverted to a simple shuttling task, provide evidence that might favor the top-down hypothesis. The absence of the Lobsterbot degraded spatial encoding rather than enhancing it, indicating that simply reducing the task demand is not sufficient to activate one particular type of encoding mode over another.  The arbitrator hypothesis asserts that the mPFC neurons are called on to encode heterogenous information when the task demand is high and requires behavioral coordination beyond automatic, stimulus-driven execution. Future studies incorporating multiple simultaneous tasks and carefully controlling contextual variables could help determine whether these functional shifts are governed by top-down processes involving specific neural arbitrators or by bottom-up signals.”

      Related to this difference in location coding throughout the environment, the authors suggest in Figure 3a-b that location coding is better in the foraging zone compared to the nest or encounter zones, evidenced by better decoder performance (smaller error) in the foraging zone (Figure 3b). The authors use the same proportion of data from the three zones for setting up training/test sets for cross-validation, but it seems likely that overall, there are substantially more samples from the foraging zone compared to the other two zones, as the animal traverses this section frequently, and whenever it moves from the next into the encounter zone (based on the video). What does the actual heatmap of animal location look like? And, if the data are down-sampled such that each section contributes the same proportion of samples to decoder training, does the error landscape still show better performance in the foraging zone? It is important to disambiguate the effects of uneven sampling from true biological differences in neural activity.

      Thank you for the comment. We agree with the concern regarding uneven data size from different sections of the arena. Indeed, as the heatmap below indicates, the rats spent most of their time in two critical locations, one being a transition area between N-and F-zone and the other near the sucrose port. This imbalance needs to be corrected. In fact we have included methodology to correct this biased sampling. In the result section “Non-navigational behavior reduces the accuracy of decoded location” we have the following results.

      Author response image 1.

      Heatmap of the animal’s position during one example session. (Left) Unprocessed occupancy plot. Each dot represents 0.2 seconds. Right) Smoothed occupancy plot using a Gaussian filter (sigma: 10 pixels, filter size: 1001 pixels). The white line indicates a 10 cm length.

      “To correct for the unequal distribution of location visits (more visits to the F- than to other zones), the regressor was trained using a subset of the original data, which was equalized for the data size per distance range (see Materials and Methods). Despite the correction, there was a significant main effect of the zone (F(1.16, 45.43) = 119.2, p <.001) and the post hoc results showed that the MAEs in the N-zone (19.52 ± 4.46 cm; t(39) = 10.45; p <.001) and the E-zone (26.13 ± 7.57 cm; t(39) = 11.40; p <.001) had a significantly higher errors when compared to the F-zone (14.10 ± 1.64 cm).”

      Also in the method section, we have stated that:

      “In the dataset adjusted for uneven location visits, we divided distance values into five equally sized bins. Then, a sub-dataset was created that contains an equal number of data points for each of these bins.”

      Why do the authors choose to use a multi-layer neural network (Figure 2b-c) to decode the animal's distance to the encounter zone?(…) The authors may consider also showing an analysis using simple regression, or maybe something like an SVM, in addition to the ANN approach.

      We began with a simple linear regression model and progressed to more advanced methods, including SVM and multi-layer neural networks. As shown below, simpler methods could decode distance to some extent, but neural networks and random forest regressors outperformed others (Neural Network: 16.61 cm ± 3.673; Linear Regression: 19.85 cm ± 2.528; Quadratic Regression: 18.68 cm ± 4.674; SVM: 18.88 cm ± 2.676; Random Forest: 13.59 cm ± 3.174).

      We chose the neural network model for two main reasons: (1) previous studies demonstrated its superior performance compared to Bayesian regressors commonly used for decoding neural ensembles, and (2) its generalizability and robustness against noisy data. Although the random forest regressor achieved the lowest decoding error, we avoided using it due to its tendency to overfit and its limited generalization to unseen data.

      Overall, we expect similar results with other regressors but with different statistical power for decoding accuracy. Instead, we speculate that neural network’s use of multiple nodes contributes to robustness against noise from single-unit recordings and enables the network to capture distributed processing within neural ensembles.

      In Figure 6c, the authors show a prediction of withdrawal behavior based on neural activity seconds before the behavior occurs. This is potentially very interesting, as it suggests that something about the state of neural dynamics in PFC is potentially related to the propensity to withdraw, or to the preparation of this behavior. However, another possibility is that the behaves differently, in more subtle ways, while it is anticipating threat and preparing withdrawal behavior - since PFC neurons are correlated with behavior, this could explain decoder performance before the withdrawal behavior occurs. To rule out this possibility, it would be useful to analyze how well, and how early, withdrawal success can be decoded only on the basis of behavioral features from the video, and then to compare this with the time course of the neural decoder. Another approach might be to decode the behavior on the basis of video data as well as neural data, and using a model comparison, measure whether inclusion of neural features significantly increases decoder performance.

      We appreciate this important point, as mPFC activity might indeed reflect motor preparation preceding withdrawal behavior. Another reviewer raised a similar concern regarding potential micro-behavioral influences on mPFC activity prior to withdrawal responses. However, our behavioral analysis suggests that highly trained rats engage in sucrose licking which has little variability regardless of the subsequent behavioral decision. To support, 95% of inter-lick intervals were less than 0.25 seconds, which is not enough time to perform any additional behavior during encounters.

      Author response image 2.

      To further clarify this, we included additional video showing both avoidance and escape withdrawals at close range. This video was recorded during the development of the behavioral paradigm, though we did not routinely collect this view, as animals consistently exhibited stable licking behavior in the E-zone. As demonstrated in the video, the rat remains highly focused on the lick port with minimal body movement during encounters. Therefore, we believe that the neural ensemble dynamics observed in the mPFC are unlikely to be driven by micro-behavioral changes.

      Reviewer 2 (Public Review):

      Thank you for the positive comment on our behavior paradigm and constructive suggestions on additional analysis. We came to think that the role of mPFC could be better portrayed as representing and switching between different encoding targets under different contexts, which in part, was more clearly manifested by the naturalistic behavioral paradigm. In the revision we tried to convey this message more explicitly and provide a new perspective for this important aspect of mPFC function.

      It is not clear what proportion of each of the ensembles recorded is necessary for decoding distance from the threat, and whether it is these same neurons that directly 'switch' to responding to head entry or withdrawal in the encounter phase within the total population. The PCA gets closest to answering this question by demonstrating that activity during the encounter is different from activity in the nesting or foraging zones, but in principle this could be achieved by neurons or ensembles that did not encode spatial parameters. The population analyses are focused on neurons sensitive to behaviours relating to the threat encounter, but even before dividing into subtypes etc., this is at most half of the recorded population.

      In our study, the key idea we aim to convey is that mPFC neurons adapt their encoding schemes based on the context or functional needs of the ongoing task. Other reviewers also suggested strengthening the evidence that the same neurons directly switch between encoding two different tasks. The counteracting hypothesis to "switching functions within the same neurons" posits that there are dedicated subsets of neurons that modulate behavior—either by driving decisions/behaviors themselves or being driven by computations from other brain regions.

      To test this idea, we included an additional analysis chapter in the results section titled Overlapping populations of mPFC neurons adaptively encode spatial information and defensive decision. In this section, we directly tested this hypothesis by examining each neuron's contribution to the distance regressor and the event classifier. The results showed that the histogram of feature importance—the contribution to each task—is highly skewed towards zero for both decoders, and removing neurons with high feature importance does not impair the decoder’s performance. These findings suggest that 1) there is no direct division among neurons involved in the two tasks, and 2) information about spatial/defensive behavior is distributed across neurons.

      Furthermore, we tested whether there is a negative correlation between the feature importance of spatial encoding and avoidance encoding. Even if there were no “key neurons” that transmit a significant amount of information about either spatial or defensive behavior, it is still possible that neurons with higher information in the navigation context might carry less information in the active-foraging context, or vice versa. However, we did not observe such a trend, suggesting that mPFC neurons do not exhibit a preference for encoding one type of information over the other.

      Lastly, another reviewer raised the concern that the PCA results, which we used as evidence of functional separation of different ensemble functions, might be driven by a small number of event-coding neurons. To address this, we conducted the same analysis on a subset of data that excluded neural activity potentially influenced by significant events in the E-zone. In the Peri-Event Time Histogram (PETH) analysis, we observed that some neurons exhibit highly-modulated activity upon arrival at the E-zone (head entry; HE) and immediately following voluntary departure or attack (head withdrawal; HW). We defined 'critical event times' as ± one second from these events and excluded neural data from these periods to determine if PCA could still differentiate neural activities across zones. Despite these exclusions, the results continued to show populational differences between zones, reinforcing the notion that neurons adapt their activity according to the context. We acknowledge that this analysis still cannot eliminate all of the confounding factors due to the context change, but we confirmed that excluding two significant events (delivery onset of sucrose and withdrawal movement) does not alter our result.

      To summarize, these additional results further support the conclusion that spatial and avoidance information is distributed across the neural population rather than being handled by distinct subsets. The analyses revealed no negative correlation between spatial and avoidance encoding, and excluding event-driven neural activity did not alter the observed functional separation, confirming that mPFC neurons dynamically adjust their activity to meet contextual demands.

      A second concern is also illustrated by Fig. 7: in the data presented, separate reward and threat encoding neurons were not shown - in the current study design, it is not possible to dissociate reward and threat responses as the data without the threat present were only used to study spatial encoding integrity.

      Thank you for this valuable feedback. Other reviewers have also noted that Figure 7 (now Figure 8) is misleading and contains assertions not supported by our experiments. In response, we have revised the model to more accurately reflect our findings. We have eliminated the distinction between reward coding and threat coding neurons, simplifying it to focus on spatial encoding and avoidance encoding neurons. The updated figure will more appropriately align with our findings and claims. A. Distinct functional states (spatial vs. avoidance decision) encoded by the same population neurons are separable by the region (F- vs. E zone). B. Hypothetical control models by which mPFC neurons assume different functional states.

      Thirdly, the findings of this work are not mechanistic or functional but are purely correlational. For example, it is claimed that analyzing activity around the withdrawal period allows for ascertaining their functional contributions to decisions. But without a direct manipulation of this activity, it is difficult to make such a claim. The authors later discuss whether the elevated response of Type 2 neurons might simply represent fear or anxiety motivation or threat level, or whether they directly contribute to the decision-making process. As is implicit in the discussion, the current study cannot differentiate between these possibilities. However, the language used throughout does not reflect this. 

      We acknowledge that our experiments only involve correlational study and this serves as weakness. Although we carefully managed to select word to not to be deterministic, we agree that some of the language might mislead readers as if we found direct functional contribution. Thus, we changed expressions as below.

      “We then further analyzed the (functional contribution ->)correlation between neural activity and success and failure of avoidance behavior. If the mPFC neurons (encode ->)participate in the avoidance decisions, avoidance withdrawal (AW; withdrawal before the attack) and escape withdrawal (EW; withdrawal after the attack) may be distinguishable from decoded population activity even prior to motor execution.”

      Also, we added part below in discussion section to clarify the limitations of the study.

      “Despite this interesting conjecture, any analysis based on recording data is only correlational, mandating further studies with direct manipulation of the subpopulation to confirm its functional specificity.”

      Fourthly, the authors mention the representation of different functions in 'distinct spatiotemporal regions' but the bulk of the analyses, particularly in terms of response to the threat, do not compare recordings from PL and IL although - as the authors mention in the introduction - there is prior evidence of functional separation between these regions.

      Thank you for bringing this part to our attention. As we mentioned in the introduction, we acknowledge the functional differences between the PL and IL regions. Although differences in spatial encoding between these two areas were not deeply explored, we anticipated finding differences in event encoding, given the distinct roles of the PL and IL in fear and threat processing. However, our initial analysis revealed no significant differences in event encoding between the regions, and as a result, we did not emphasize these differences in the manuscript. To address this point, we have reanalyzed the data separately and included the following findings in the manuscript.

      “However, we did not observe a difference in decoding accuracy between the PL and IL ensembles, and there were no significant interactions between regressor type (shuffled vs. original) and regions (mixed-effects model; regions: p=.996; interaction: p=.782). These results indicate that the population activity in both the PL and IL contains spatial information (Figure 2D, Video 3).

      […]

      Furthermore, we analyzed whether there is a difference in prediction accuracy between sessions with different recorded regions, the PL and the IL. A repeated two-way ANOVA revealed no significant difference between recorded regions, nor any interaction (regions: F(1, 38) = 0.1828, p = 0.671; interaction: F(1, 38) = 0.1614, p = 0.690).

      […]

      We also examined whether there is a significant difference between the PL and IL in the proportion of Type 1 and Type 2 neurons. In the PL, among 379 recorded units, 143 units (37.73%) were labeled as Type 1, and 75 units (19.79%) were labeled as Type 2. In contrast, in the IL, 156 units (61.66%) and 19 units (7.51%) of 253 recorded units were labeled as Type 1 and Type 2, respectively. A Chi-square analysis revealed that the PL contains a significantly higher proportion of Type 2 neurons (χ²(1, 632) = 34.85, p < .001), while the IL contains a significantly higher proportion of Type 1 neurons compared to the other region (χ²(1, 632) = 18.07, p < .001).”

      To summarize our additional results, we did not observe performance differences in distance decoding or event decoding. The only difference we observed was the proportional variation of Type 1 and Type 2 neurons when we separated the analysis by brain region. These results are somewhat counterintuitive, considering the distinct roles of the two regions—particularly the PL in fear expression and the IL in extinction learning. However, since the studies mentioned in the introduction primarily used lesion and infusion methods, this discrepancy may be due to the different approach taken in this study. Considering this, we have added the following section to the discussion.

      “Interestingly, we found no difference between the PL and IL in the decoding accuracy of distance or avoidance decision. This somewhat surprising considering distinct roles of these regions in the long line of fear conditioning and extinction studies, where the PL has been linked to fear expression and the IL to fear extinction learning (Burgos-Robles et al., 2009; Dejean et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2013; Quirk et al., 2006; Sierra-Mercado et al., 2011; Vidal-Gonzalez et al., 2006). On the other hand, more Type 2 neurons were found in the PL and more Type 1 neurons were found in the IL. To recap, typical Type 1 neurons increased the activity briefly after the head entry and then remained inhibited, while Type 2 neurons showed a burst of activity during head entry and sustained increased activity. One study employing context-dependent fear discrimination task (Kim et al., 2013) also identified two distinct types of PL units: short-latency CS-responsive units, which increased firing during the initial 150 ms of tone presentation, and persistently firing units, which maintained firing for up to 30 seconds. Given the temporal dynamics of Type 2 neurons, it is possible that our unsupervised clustering method may have merged the two types of neurons found in Kim et al.’s study.

      While we did not observe decreased IL activity during dynamic foraging, prior studies have shown that IL excitability decreases after fear conditioning (Santini et al., 2008), and increased IL activity is necessary for fear extinction learning. In our paradigm, extinction learning was unlikely, as the threat persisted throughout the experiment. Future studies with direct manipulation of these subpopulations, particularly examining head withdrawal timing after such interventions, could provide insight into how these subpopulations guide behavior.”

      Additionally, we made some changes in the introduction, mainly replacing the PL/IL with mPFC to be consistent with the main body of results and conclusion and also specifying the correlational nature of the recording study.

      “Machine learning-based populational decoding methods, alongside single-cell analyses, were employed to investigate the correlations between neuronal activity and a range of behavioral indices across different sections within the foraging arena.”

      Reviewer 2 (Recommendations):

      The authors consistently use parametric statistical tests throughout the manuscript. Can they please provide evidence that they have checked whether the data are normally distributed? Otherwise, non-parametric alternatives are more appropriate.

      Thank you for mentioning this important issue in the analysis. We re-ran the test of normality for all our data using the Shapiro-Wilk test with a p-value of .05 and found that the following data sets require non-parametric tests, as summarized in Author response table 1 below. For those analyses which did not pass the normality test, we used a non-parametric alternative test instead. We also updated the methods section. For instance, repeated measures ANOVA for supplementary figure S1 and PCA results were changed to the Friedman test with Dunn’s multiple comparison test.

      Author response table 1.

      Line 107: it is not clear here or in the methods whether a single drop of sucrose solution is delivered per lick or at some rate during the encounter, both during the habituation or in the final task. This is important information in order to understand how animals might make decisions about whether to stay or leave and how to interpret neural responses during this time period. Or is it a large drop, such that it takes multiple licks to consume? Please clarify.

      The apparatus we used incorporated an IR-beam sensor-controlled solenoid valve. As the beam sensor was located right in front of the pipe, the rat’s tongue activated the sensor. As a result, each lick opened the valve for a brief period, releasing a small amount of liquid, and the rat had to continuously lick to gain access to the sucrose. We carefully regulated the flow of the liquid and installed a small sink connected to a vacuum pump, so any remaining sucrose not consumed by the rat was instantly removed from the port. We clarified how sucrose was delivered in the methods section and also in the results section.

      Method:

      “The sucrose port has an IR sensor which was activated by a single lick. The rat usually stays in front of the lick port and continuously lick up to a rate of 6.3 times per second to obtain sucrose. Any sucrose droplets dropped in the bottom sink were immediately removed by negative pressure so that the rat’s behavior was focused on the licking.”

      Result:

      “The lick port was activated by an IR-beam sensor, triggering the solenoid valve when the beam was interrupted. The rat gradually learned to obtain rewards by continuously licking the port.”

      However, I'm not sure I understand the authors' logic in the interpretation: does the S-phase not also consist of goal-directed behaviour? To me, the core difference is that one is mediated by threat and the other by reward. In addition, it would be helpful to visualize the behaviour in the S-phase, particularly the number of approaches. This difference in the amount of 'experience' so to speak might drive some of the decrease in spatial decoding accuracy, even if travel distance is similar (it is also not clear how travel distance is calculated - is this total distance?) Ideally, this would also be included as a predictor in the GLM.

      We agree that the behaviors observed during the shuttling phase can also be considered goal-directed, as the rat moves purposefully toward explicit goals (the sucrose port and the N-zone during the return trip). However, we argue that there is a significant difference in the level of complexity of these goals.

      During the L-phase, the rat not only has to successfully navigate to the E-zone for sucrose but also pay attention to the robots, either to avoid an attack from the robot's forehead or escape the fast-striking motion of the claw. When the rat runs toward the E-zone, it typically takes a side-approaching path, similar to Kim and Choi (2018), and exhibits defensive behaviors such as a stretched posture, which were not observed in the S-phase. This behavioral characteristic differs from the S-phase, where the rat adopted a highly stereotyped navigation pattern fairly quickly (within 3 sessions), evidenced by more than 50 shuttling trajectories per session. In this phase, the rat exhibited more stimulus-response behavior, simply repeating the same actions over time without deliberate optimization.

      In our additional experiment with two different levels of goal complexity (reward-only vs. reward/threat conflict), we used a between-subject design in which both groups experienced both the S-phase and L-phase before surgery and underwent only one type of session afterward. This approach ruled out the possibility of differences in contextual experience. Additionally, since we initially designed the S-phase as extended training, behaviors in the apparatus tended to stabilize after rats completed both the S-phase and L-phase before surgery. As a result, we compared the post-surgery Lobsterbot phase to the post-surgery shuttling phase to investigate how different levels of goal complexity shape spatial encoding strength.

      To clarify our claim, we edited the paragraph below.

      “This absence of spatial correlates may result from a lack of complex goal-oriented navigation behavior, which requires deliberate planning to acquire more rewards and avoid potential threats.

      […]

      After the surgery, unlike the Lob-Exp group, the Ctrl-Exp group returned to the shuttling phase, during which the Lobsterbot was removed. With this protocol, both groups experienced sessions with the Lobsterbot, but the Ctrl-Exp group's task became less complex, as it was reduced to mere reward collection.

      . Given these observations, along with the mPFC’s lack of consistency in spatial encoding, it is plausible that the mPFC operates in multiple functional modes, and the spatial encoding mode is preempted when the complexity of the task requires deliberate spatial navigation.”

      Additionally, we added behavior data during initial S-phase into Supplementary Figure 1.

      It is good point that the amount of experience might drive decrease in spatial decoding accuracy. To test this hypothesis, we added a new variable, the number of Lobsterbot sessions after surgery, to the previous GLM analysis. The updated model predicted the outcome variable with significant accuracy (F(4,44) = 10.31, p < .001), and with the R-squared value at 0.4838. The regression coefficients were as follows: presence of the Lobsterbot (2.76, standard error [SE] = 1.11, t = 2.42, p = .020), number of recorded cells (-0.43, SE = .08, t = -5.22, p < .001), recording location (0.90, SE = 1.11, p = .424), and number of L sessions (0.002, SE = 0.11, p = .981). These results indicate that the number of exposures to the Lobsterbot sessions, as a measure of experience, did not affect spatial decoding accuracy.

      For minor edit, we edited the term as “total travel distance”.

      Relating to the previous point, it should be emphasized in both sections on removing the Lobsterbot and on non-navigational behaviours that the spatial decoding is all in reference to distance from the threat (or reward location). The language in these sections differs from the previous section where 'distance from the goal' is mentioned. If the authors wish to discuss spatial decoding per se, it would be helpful to perform the same analysis but relative to the animals' own location which might have equal accuracy across locations in the arena. Otherwise, it is worth altering the language in e.g. line 258 onwards to state the fact that distance to the goal is only decodable when animals are actively engaged in the task.

      Thank you for this comment, we changed the term as “distance from the conflict zone” or “distance of the rat to the center of the E-zone” to clarify our experiment setup.

      In Fig. 5, why is the number of neurons shown in the PETHs less than the numbers shown in the pie charts?

      The difference in the number of neurons between the PETHs and the pie charts in Figure 5 is because PETHs are drawn only for 'event-responsive' units. For visualizing the neurons, we selectively included those that met certain criteria described in Method section (Behavior-responsive unit analysis). We have updated the caption for Figure 5 as follows to minimize confusion.

      “Multiple subpopulations in the mPFC react differently to head entry and head withdrawal.

      (A) Top: The PETH of head entry-responsive units is color-coded based on the Z-score of activity.

      (C) The PETH of head withdrawal-responsive units is color-coded based on the Z-score of activity.”

      I appreciate the amount of relatively unprocessed data plotted in Figure 5, but it would be great to visualize something similar for AW vs. EW responses within the HW2 population. In other words, what is there that's discernably different within these responses that results in the findings of Fig. 6?

      To visualize the difference in neural activity between AW and EW, we included an additional supplementary figure (Supplementary Figure 5). We divided the neurons into Type 1 and Type 2 and plotted PETH during Avoidance Withdrawal (AW) and Escape Withdrawal (EW). Consistent with the results shown in Figure 6d, we could visually observe increased activity in Type 2 neurons before the execution of AW compared to EW. However, we couldn’t find a similar pattern in Type 1 neurons.

      On a related note, it would add explanatory power if the authors were able to more tightly link the prediction accuracy of the ensemble (particularly the Type 2 neurons) to the timing of the behaviour. Earlier in the manuscript it would be helpful to show latency to withdraw in AW trials; are animals leaving many seconds before the attack happens, or are they just about anticipating the timing of the attack? And therefore when using ensemble activity to predict the success of the AW, is the degree to which this can be done in advance (as the authors say, up to 6 seconds before withdrawal) also related to how long the animal has been engaged with the threat?

      We agree that the timing of head withdrawal, particularly in AW trials, is a critical factor in describing the rat's strategy toward the task. To test whether the rat uses a precise timing strategy—for instance, leaving several seconds before the attack or exploiting the discrete 3- and 6-second attack durations—we plotted all head withdrawal timepoints during the 6-second trials. The distribution was more even, without distinguishable peaks (e.g., at the very initial period or at the 3- or 6-second mark). This indicates a lack of precise temporal strategy by the rat. We included additional data in the supplementary figure (Supplementary Figure 6) and added the following to the results section.

      “We monitored all head withdrawal timepoints to assess whether rats developed a temporal strategy to differentiate between the 3-second and 6-second attacks. We found no evidence of such a strategy, as the timings of premature head withdrawals during the 6-second attack trials were evenly distributed (see Supplementary Figure S1).”

      As depicted in the new supplementary figure, head withdrawal times during avoidance behavior vary from sub-seconds to the 3- or 6-second attack timepoints. After receiving the reviewer’s comment, we became curious whether there is a decoding accuracy difference depending on how long the animal engaged with the threat. We selected all 6-second attack and avoidance withdrawal trials and checked if correctly classified trials (AW trials classified as AW) had different head withdrawal times—perhaps shorter durations—compared to misclassified trials (AW trials classified as EW). As shown in Author response image 3 below, there was no significant difference between these two types, indicating that the latency of head withdrawal does not affect prediction accuracy.

      Author response image 3.

      Finally, there remain some open questions. One is how much encoding strength - of either space or the decision to leave during the encounter - relates to individual differences in animal performance or behaviour, particularly because this seems so variable at baseline. A second is how stable this encoding is. The authors mention that the distance encoding must be stable to an extent for their regressor to work; I am curious whether this stability is also found during the encounter coding, and also whether it is stable across experience. For example, in a session when an individual has a high proportion of anticipatory withdrawals, is the proportion of Type 2 neurons higher?

      Thank you for these questions. To recap the number of animals that we used, we used five rats during Lobsterbot experiments, and three rats for control experiment that we removed Lobsterbot after training. Indeed, there were individual differences in performance (i.e. avoidance success rate), number of recorded units (related to the recording quality), and baseline behaviors. To clarify these differences, see author response image 4 below.

      Author response image 4.

      We used a GLM to measure how much of the decoder’s accuracy was explained by individual differences. The result showed that 38.96% of distance regressor’s performance, and 12.14% of the event classifier’s performance was explained by the individual difference. Since recording quality was highly dependent on the animals, the high subject variability detected in the distance regression might be attributed to the number of recorded cells. Rat00 which had the lowest average mean absolute error had the highest number of recorded cells at average of 18. Compared to the distance regression, there was less subject variability in event classification. Indeed, the GLM results showed that the variability explained by the number of cells was only 0.62% in event classification.

      The reason we mentioned that "distance encoding must be stable for our regressor to work" is entirely based on the population-level analysis. Because we used neural data and behaviors from entire trials within a session, the regressor or classifier would have low accuracy if encoding dynamics changed within the session. In other words, if the way neurons encode avoidance/escape predictive patterns changed within a training set, the classifier would fail to generate an optimized separation function that works well across all datasets.

      To further investigate whether changes in experience affect event classification results over time, we plotted an additional graph below. Although there are individual and daily fluctuations in decoding accuracy, there was no observable trend throughout the experiments.

      Author response image 5.

      Regarding the correlation between the ratio of avoidance withdrawal and the proportion of Type 2 neurons, we were also curious and analyzed the data. Across 40 sessions, the correlation was -0.0716. For Type 1 neurons, it was slightly higher at 0.1459. We believe this indicates no significant relationship between the two variables.

      Minor points:

      I struggled with the overuse of acronyms in the paper. Some might be helpful but F-zone/N-zone, for example, or HE/HW, AW/EW are a bit of a struggle. After reading the paper a few times I learned them but a naive reader might need to often refer back to when they were first defined (as I frequently had to).

      To increase readability, we removed acronyms that are not often used and changed HE/HW to head-entry/head-withdrawal.

      I have a few questions about Figure 1F: in the text (line 150) it says that 'surgery was performed after three L sessions when the rats displayed a range of 30% to 60% AW'. This doesn't seem consistent with what is plotted, which shows greater variability in the proportion of AW behaviours both before and after surgery. It also appears that several rats only experienced two days of the L1 phase; please make clear if so. And finally, what is the line at 50% indicating? Neither the text nor the legend discuss any sort of thresholding at 50%. Instead, it would be best to make the distinction between pre- and post-surgery behaviour visually clearer.

      Thank you for pointing out this issue. We acknowledge there was an error in the text description. As noted in the Methods section, we proceeded with surgery after three Lobsterbot sessions. We have removed the incorrect part from the Results section and revised the Methods section for clarity.

      “After three days of Lobsterbot sessions, the rats underwent microdrive implant surgery, and recording data were collected from subsequent sessions, either Lobsterbot or shuttling sessions, depending on the experiment. For all post-surgery sessions, those with fewer than 20 approaches in 30 minutes were excluded from further analysis.”

      Among the five rats, Rat2 and Rat3 did not approach the robot during the entire Lob2 session, which is why these two rats do not have Lob2 data points. We updated the caption for regarding issue.

      Initially, we added a 50% reference line, but we agree it is unnecessary as we do not discuss this reference. We have updated the figure to include the surgery point, as shown in Supplementary Figure 1.

      Fig. 2C: each dot is an ensemble of simultaneously recorded neurons, i.e. a subset of the total 800-odd units if I understand correctly. How many ensembles does each rat contribute? Similarly, is this evenly distributed across PL and IL?

      Yes, each dot represents a single session, with a total of 40 sessions. Five rats contributed 11, 9, 8, 7, and 5 sessions, respectively. Although each rat initially had more than 10 sessions, we discarded some sessions with a low unit count (fewer than 10 sessions; as detailed in Materials and Methods - Data Collection). We collected 25 sessions from the PL and 15 sessions from the IL. Our goal was to collect more than 200 units per each region.

      Please show individual data points for Fig. 2D.

      We update the figure with individual data points.

      Is there a reason why the section on removing the Lobsterbot (lines 200 - 215) does not have associated MAE plots? Particularly the critical comparison between Lob-Exp and Ctl-Exp.

      We intentionally removed some graphs to create a more compact figure, but we appreciate your suggestion and have included the graph in Figure 2.

      Some references to supplementary materials are not working, e.g. line 333.

      Our submitted version of manuscript had reference error. For the current version, we used plane text, and the references are fixed.

      The legend for Supp. Fig. 2B is incorrect.

      We greatly appreciate this point. We changed the caption to match the figure.

      Reviewer 3 (Public Review):

      Thank you for recognizing our efforts in designing an ethologically relevant foraging task to uncover the multiple roles of the mPFC. While we acknowledge certain limitations in our methodology—particularly that we only observed correlations between neural activity and behavior without direct manipulation—we have conducted additional analyses to further strengthen our findings.

      Weakness:

      The primary concern with this study is the absence of direct evidence regarding the role of the mPFC in the foraging behavior of the rats. The ability to predict heterogeneous variables from the population activity of a specific brain area does not necessarily imply that this brain area is computing or using this information. In light of recent reports revealing the distributed nature of neural coding, conducting direct causal experiments would be essential to draw conclusions about the role of the mPFC in spatial encoding and/or threat evaluation. Alternatively, a comparison with the activity from a different brain region could provide valuable insights (or at the very least, a comparison between PL and IL within the mPFC).

      Thank you for the comment. Indeed, the fundamental limitation of the recording study is that it is only correlational, and any causal relationship between neural activity and behavioral indices is only speculative. We made it clearer in the revision and refrained from expressing any speculative ideas suggesting causality throughout the revision. While we did not provide direct evidence that the mPFC is computing or utilizing spatial/foraging information, we based our assertion on previous studies that have directly demonstrated the mPFC's role in complex decision-making tasks (Martin-Fernandez et al., 2023; Orsini et al., 2018; Zeeb et al., 2015) and in certain types of spatial tasks (De Bruin et al., 1994; Sapiurka et al., 2016) . We would like to emphasize that, to the best of our knowledge, there was no previous study which investigated the mPFC function while animal is solving multiple heterogenous problems in semi-naturalistic environment. Therefore, although our recording study only provides speculative causal inference, it certainly provides a foundation for investigating the mPFC function. Future study employing more sophisticated, cell-type specific manipulations would confirm the hypotheses from the current study.

      One of the key questions of this manuscript is how multiple pieces of information are represented in the recorded population of neurons. Most of the studies mentioned above use highly structured experimental designs, which allow researchers to study only one function of the mPFC. In the current study, the semi-naturalistic environment allows rats to freely switch between multiple behavioral sets, and our decoding analysis quantitatively assesses the extent to which spatial/foraging information is embedded during these sets. Our goal is to demonstrate that two different task hyperspaces are co-expressed in the same region and that the degree of this expression varies according to the rat’s current behavior (See Figure 8(b) in the revised manuscript).

      Alternatively, we added multiple analyses. First, we included a single unit-level analysis looking at the place cell-like property to contrast with the ensemble decoding. Most neurons did not show well-defined place fields although there were some indications for place cell-like property. For example, some neurons displayed fragmented place fields or unusually large place fields only at particular spots in the arena (mostly around the gates). The accuracy from this place information at the single-neuron level is much lower than that acquired from population decoding. Likewise, although there were neurons with modulated firing around the time of particular behavior (head entry and withdrawal), overall prediction accuracy of avoidance decision was much higher when the ensemble-based classifier was applied.

      Moreover, given that high-dimensional movement has been shown to be reflected in the neural activity across the entire dorsal cortex, more thorough comparisons between the neural encoding of task variables and movement would help rule out the possibility that the heterogeneous encoding observed in the mPFC is merely a reflection of the rats' movements in different behavioral modes.

      Thanks for the comment. We acknowledge that the neural activity may reflect various movement components across different zones in the arena. We performed several analyses to test this idea. First, we want to recap our run-and-stop event analysis may provide an insight regarding whether the mPFC neurons are encoding locations despite the significant motor events. The rats typically move across the F-zone fairly routinely and swiftly (as if they are “running”) to reach the E-zone at which they reduce the moving speed to almost a halt (“stopping”). The PETHs around these critical motor events, however, did not show any significant modulation of neural activity indicating that most neurons we recorded from mPFC did not respond to movement.

      We added this analysis to demonstrate that these sudden stops did not evoke the characteristic activation of Type 1 and Type 2 neurons observed during head entry into the E-zone. When we isolated these sudden stops outside the E-zone, we did not observe this neural signature (Supplementary Figure 2).

      Second, our PCA results showed that population activity in the E-zone during dynamic foraging behavior was distinct from the activity observed in the N- and F-zones during navigation. However, there is a possibility that the two behaviorally significant events—entry into the E-zone and voluntary or sudden exit—might be driving the differences observed in the PCA results. To account for this, we designated ±1 second from head entry and head withdrawal as "critical event times," excluded the corresponding neural data, and reanalyzed the data. This method removed neural activity associated with sudden movements in specific zones. Despite this exclusion, the PCA still revealed distinct population activity in the E-zone, different from the other zones (Supplementary Figure 4). This result reduces the likelihood that the observed heterogeneous neural activity is merely a reflection of zone-specific movements.

      Lastly, the main claim of the paper is that the mPFC population switches between different functional modes depending on the context. However, no dynamic analysis or switching model has been employed to directly support this hypothesis.

      Thank you for this comment. Since we did not conduct a manipulation experiment, there is a clear limitation in uncovering how switching occurs between the two task contexts. To make the most of our population recording data, we added an additional results section that examines how individual neurons contribute to both the distance regressor and the event classifier. Our findings support the idea that distance and dynamic foraging information are distributed across neurons, with no distinct subpopulations dedicated to each context. This suggests that mPFC neurons adjust their coding schemes based on the current task context, aligning with Duncan’s (2001) adaptive coding model, which posits that mPFC neurons adapt their coding to meet the task's current demands.

      Reviewer 3 (Recommendations):

      The evidence for spatial encoding is relatively weak. In the F-zone (50 x 48 cm), the average error was approximately 17 cm, constituting about a third of the box's width and likely not significantly smaller than the size of a rat's body. The errors in the shuffled data are also not substantially greater than those in the original data. An essential test indicates that spatial decoding accuracy decreases when the Losterbot is removed. However, assessing the validity of the results is difficult in the current state. There is no figure illustrating the results, and no statistics are provided regarding the test for matching the number of neurons.

      We acknowledge that the average error (~ 17 cm ) measured in our study is relatively large, even though the error is significantly smaller than that by the shuffled control model (22.6 cm). Previous studies reported smaller prediction errors but in different experimental conditions: 16 cm in Kaefer et al. (2020) and less than 10 cm in Ma et al. (2023) and Mashhoori et al. (2018). Most notably, the average number of units used in our study (15.8 units per session) is significantly smaller compared to the previous works, which used 63, 49, and 40 units, respectively. As our GLM results demonstrated, the number of recorded cells significantly influenced decoding accuracy (β = -0.43 cm/neuron). With a similar number of recorded cells, we would have achieved comparable decoding accuracy. In addition, unlike other studies that have employed a dedicated maze such as the virtual track or the 8-shaped maze, we exposed rats to a semi-naturalistic environment where they exhibited a variety of behaviors beyond simple navigation. As argued throughout the manuscript, we believe that the spatial information represented in the mPFC is susceptible to disruption when the animal engages in other activities. A similar phenomenon was reported by Mashhoori et al. (2018), where the decoder, which typically showed a median error of less than 10 cm, exhibited a much higher error—nearly 100 cm—near the feeder location.

      As for the reviewer’s request for comparing spatial decoding without the Lobsterbot, we added a new figure to illustrate the spatial decoding results, including statistical details. We also applied a Generalized Linear Model to regress out the effect of the number of recorded neurons and statistically assess the impact of Lobsterbot removal. This adjustment directly addresses the reviewer's request for a clearer presentation of the results and helps contextualize the decoding performance in relation to the number of recorded neurons.

      As indicated in the public review, drawing conclusions about the role of the mPFC in navigation and avoidance behavior during the foraging task is challenging due to the exclusively correlational nature of the results. The accuracy in AW/EW discrimination increases a few seconds before the response, implying that changes in mPFC activity precede the avoidance/escape response. However, one must question whether this truly reflects the case. Could this phenomenon be attributed to rats modifying their "micro-behavior" (as evidenced by changes in movement observed in the video) before executing the escape response, and subsequently influencing mPFC activity?

      We appreciate the reviewer's thoughtful observation regarding the correlational nature of our results and the potential influence of pre-escape micro-behaviors on mPFC activity. We acknowledge that the increased accuracy in AW/EW discrimination preceding the response could also be correlated with micro-behaviors. However, there is very little room for extraneous behavior other than licking the sucrose delivery port within the E-zone, as the rats are highly trained to perform this stereotypical behavior. To support this, we measured the time delays between licking events (inter-lick intervals). The results show a sharp distribution, with 95% of the intervals falling within a quarter second, indicating that the rats were stable in the E-zone, consistently licking without altering their posture.

      To complement the data presented in Author response image 2, a video clip showing a rat engaged in licking behavior was included. We carefully designed the robot compartment and adjusted the distance between the Lobsterbot and the sucrose port to ensure that rats could exhibit only limited behaviors inside the E-zone. The video confirms that no significant micro-behaviors were observed during the rat’s activity in the E-zone.

      If mPFC activity indeed switches mode, the results do not clearly indicate whether individual cells are specifically dedicated to spatial representation and avoidance or if they adapt their function based on the current goal. Figure 7, presented as a schematic illustration, suggests the latter option. However, the proportion of cells in the HE and HW categories that also encode spatial location has not been demonstrated. It has also not been shown how the switch is manifested at the level of the population.

      Thank you for this comment. As the reviewer pointed out, we suggest that mPFC neurons do not diverge based on their functions, but rather adapt their roles according to the current goal. To support this assertion, we added an additional results section that calculates the feature importance of decoders. This analysis allows us to quantitatively measure each neuron’s contribution to both the distance regressor and the event decoder. Our results indicate that distance and defensive behavior are not encoded by a small subset of neurons; instead, the information is distributed across the population. Shuffling the neural data of a single neuron resulted in a median increase in decoding error of 0.73 cm for the distance regressor and 0.01% for the event decoder, demonstrating that the decoders do not rely on a specific subset of neurons that exclusively encode spatial and/or defensive behavior

      Although we found supporting evidence that mPFC neurons encode two different types of information depending on the current context, we acknowledge that we could not go further in answering how this switch is manifested. One simple explanation is that the function is driven by current contextual information and goals—in other words, a bottom-up mechanism. However, in our control experiment, simplifying the navigation task worsened the encoding of spatial information in the mPFC. Therefore, we speculate that an external or internal arbitrator circuit determines what information to encode. A precise temporal analysis of the timepoint when the switch occurs in more controlled experiments might answer these questions. We have added this discussion to the discussion section.

      PL and IL are two distinct regions; however, there is no comparison between the two areas regarding their functional properties or the representations of the cells. Are the proportions of cell categories (HE vs HW or HE1 vs HE2, spatial encoding vs no spatial encoding) different in IL and PL? Are areas differentially active during the different behaviors?

      Thank you for bringing up this issue. As mentioned in our response to the public review, we included a comparison between the PL and IL regions. While we did not observe any differences in spatial encoding (feature importance scores), the only distinction was in the proportion of Type 1 and Type 2 neurons, as the reviewer suggested. We have incorporated our interpretation of these results into the discussion section.

      The results and interpretations of the cluster analysis appear to be highly dependent on the parameters used to define a cluster. For example, the HE2 category includes cells with activity that precedes events and gradually decreases afterward, as well as cells with activity that only follows the events.

      We strongly agree that dependency on hyperparameters is a crucial point when using unsupervised clustering methods. To eliminate any subjective criteria in defining clusters, we carefully selected our clustering approach, which requires only two hyperparameters: the number of initial clusters (set to 8) and the minimum number of cells required to be considered a valid cluster (cutoff limit, 50). The rationale behind these choices was: 1) a higher number of initial clusters would fail to generalize neural activity, 2) clusters with fewer than 50 neurons would be difficult to analyze, and 3) to prevent the separation of clusters that show noisy responses to the event.

      Author response table 2 shows the differences in the number of cell clusters when we varied these two parameters. As demonstrated, changing these two variables does result in different numbers of clusters. However, when we plotted each cluster type’s activity around head entry (HE) and head withdrawal (HW), an increased number of clusters resulted in the addition of small subsets with low variation in activity around the event, without affecting the general activity patterns of the major clusters.

      The example mentioned by the reviewer—possible separation of HE2—appears when using a hyperparameter set those results in 4 clusters, not 3. In this result, 83 units, which were labeled as HE2 in the 3-cluster hyperparameter set, form a new group, HE3 (Group 3). This group of units shows increased activity after head entry and exhibited characteristics similar to HE2, with most of the units classified as HW2, maintaining high activity until head withdrawal. Among the 83 HE3 units, 36 were further classified as HW2, 44 as non-significant, and 3 as HW1. Therefore, we believe this does not affect our analysis, as we observed the separation of two major groups, Type 1 (HE1-HW1) and Type 2 (HE2-HW2), and focused our analysis on these groups afterward.

      Despite this validation, there remains a strong possibility that our method might not fully capture small yet significant subpopulations of mPFC units. As a result, we have included a sentence in the methods section addressing the rationale and stability of our approach.

      “(Materials and Methods) To compensate for the limited number of neurons recorded per session, the hyperparameter set was chosen to generalize their activity and categorize them into major types, allowing us to focus on neurons that appeared across multiple recording sessions. Although changes in the hyperparameter sets resulted in different numbers of clusters, the major activity types remained consistent (Supplementary Figure S8). However, there is a chance that this method may not differentiate smaller subsets of neurons, particularly those with fewer than 50 recorded neurons.”

      Author response table 2.

      Minor points:

      Line 333: Error! Reference source not found. This was probably the place for citing Figure S2?

      Lines 339, 343: Error! Reference source not found.

      Thank you for mentioning these comments. In the new version, all reference functions from Word have been replaced with plain text.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In this manuscript, the authors use a large dataset of neuroscience publications to elucidate the nature of self-citation within the neuroscience literature. The authors initially present descriptive measures of self-citation across time and author characteristics; they then produce an inclusive model to tease apart the potential role of various article and author features in shaping self-citation behavior. This is a valuable area of study, and the authors approach it with a rich dataset and solid methodology.

      The revisions made by the authors in this version have greatly improved the validity and clarity of the statistical techniques, and as a result the paper's findings are more convincing.

      This paper's primary strengths are: 1) its comprehensive dataset that allows for a snapshot of the dynamics of several related fields; 2) its thorough exploration of how self-citation behavior relates to characteristics of research and researchers.

      Thank you for your positive view of our paper and for your previous comments.

      Its primary weakness is that the study stops short of digging into potential mechanisms in areas where it is potentially feasible to do so - for example, studying international dynamics by identifying and studying researchers who move between countries, or quantifying more or less 'appropriate' self-citations via measures of abstract text similarity.

      We agree that these are limitations of the existing study. We updated the limitations section as follows (page 15, line 539):

      “Similarly, this study falls short in several potential mechanistic insights, such as by investigating citation appropriateness via text similarity or international dynamics in authors who move between countries.”

      Yet while these types of questions were not determined to be in scope for this paper, the study is quite effective at laying the important groundwork for further study of mechanisms and motivations, and will be a highly valuable resource for both scientists within the field and those studying it.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The study presents valuable findings on self-citation rates in the field of Neuroscience, shedding light on potential strategic manipulation of citation metrics by first authors, regional variations in citation practices across continents, gender differences in early-career self-citation rates, and the influence of research specialization on self-citation rates in different subfields of Neuroscience. While some of the evidence supporting the claims of the authors is solid, some of the analysis seems incomplete and would benefit from more rigorous approaches.

      Thank you for your comments. We have addressed your suggestions presented in the “Recommendations for the authors” section by performing your recommended sensitivity analysis that specifically identifies authors who could be considered neurologists, neuroscientists, and psychiatrists (as opposed to just papers that are published in these fields). Please see the “Recommendations for the authors” section for more details.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This paper analyses self-citation rates in the field of Neuroscience, comprising in this case, Neurology, Neuroscience and Psychiatry. Based on data from Scopus, the authors identify self-citations, that is, whether references from a paper by some authors cite work that is written by one of the same authors. They separately analyse this in terms of first-author self-citations and last-author self-citations. The analysis is well-executed and the analysis and results are written down clearly. The interpretation of some of the results might prove more challenging. That is, it is not always clear what is being estimated.

      This issue of interpretability was already raised in my review of the previous revision, where I argued that the authors should take a more explicit causal framework. The authors have now revised some of the language in this revision, in order to downplay causal language. Although this is perfectly fine, this misses the broader point, namely that it is not clear what is being estimated. Perhaps it is best to refer to Lundberg et al. (2021) and ask the authors to clarify "What is your Estimand?" In my view, the theoretical estimands the authors are interested in are causal in nature. Perhaps the authors would argue that their estimands are descriptive. In either case, it would be good if the authors could clarify that theoretical estimand.

      Thank you for your comment and for highlighting this insightful paper. After reading this paper, we believe that our theoretical estimand is descriptive in nature. For example, in the abstract of our paper, we state: “This work characterizes self-citation rates in basic, translational, and clinical Neuroscience literature by collating 100,347 articles from 63 journals between the years 2000-2020.” This goal seems consistent with the idea of a descriptive estimand, as we are not interested in any particular intervention or counterfactual at this stage. Instead, we seek to provide a broad characterization of subgroup differences in self-citations such that future work can ask more focused questions with causal estimands.

      Our analysis included subgroup means and generalized additive models, both of which were described as empirical estimands for a theoretical descriptive estimand in Lundberg et al. We added the following text to the paper (page 3, line 112):

      “Throughout this work, we characterized self-citation rates with descriptive, not causal, analyses. Our analyses included several theoretical estimands that are descriptive 17, such as the mean self-citation rates among published articles as a function of field, year, seniority, country, and gender. We adopted two forms of empirical estimands. First, we showed subgroup means in self-citation rates. We then developed smooth curves with generalized additive models (GAMs) to describe trends in self-citation rates across several variables.”

      In addition, we added to the limitations section as follows (page 15, line 539):

      “Yet, this study may lay the groundwork for future works to explore causal estimands.”

      Finally, in my previous review, I raised the issue of when self-citations become "problematic". The authors have addressed this issue satisfactorily, I believe, and now formulate their conclusions more carefully.

      Thank you for your previous comments. We agree that they improved the paper.

      Lundberg, I., Johnson, R., & Stewart, B. M. (2021). What Is Your Estimand? Defining the Target Quantity Connects Statistical Evidence to Theory. American Sociological Review, 86(3), 532-565. https://doi.org/10.1177/00031224211004187

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Thank you for your thorough revisions and responses to the reviews

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I appreciate the authors' responses and am satisfied with all their replies except for my second comment. I still find the message conveyed slightly misleading, as the results seem to be generalized to neurologists, neuroscientists, and psychiatrists. It is important to refine the analysis to focus specifically on neuroscientists, identified as first or last authors based on their publication history. This approach is common in the science of science literature and would provide a more accurate representation of the findings specific to neuroscientists, avoiding the conflation with other related fields. This refinement could serve as a robustness check in the supplementary. I think adding this sub-analysis is essential to the validity of the results claimed in this paper.

      Thank you for your comment. We added a sensitivity analysis where fields are defined by an author’s publication history, not by the journal of each article.

      In the main text, we added the following:

      (Page 3, line 129) “When determining fields by each author’s publication history instead of the journal of each article, we observed similar rates of self-citation (Table S7). The 95% confidence intervals for each field definition overlapped in most cases, except for Last Author self-citation rates in Neuroscience (7.54% defined by journal vs. 8.32% defined by author) and Psychiatry (8.41% defined by journal vs. 7.92% defined by author).”

      Further details are provided in the methods section (page 21, line 801):

      “4.11 Journal-based vs. author-based field sensitivity analyses

      We refined our field-based analysis to focus only on authors who could be considered neuroscientists, neurologists, and psychiatrists. For each author, we looked at the number of articles they had in each subfield, as defined by Scopus. We considered 12 subfields that fell within Neurology, Neuroscience, and Psychiatry. These subfields are presented in Table S12. For each First Author and Last Author, we excluded them if any of their three most frequently published subfields did not include one of the 12 subfields of interest. If an author’s top three subfields included multiple broader fields (e.g., both Neuroscience and Psychiatry), then that author was categorized according to the field in which they published the most articles. Among First Authors, there were 86,220 remaining papers, split between 33,054 (38.33%) in Neurology, 23,216 (26.93%) in Neuroscience, and 29,950 (34.73%) in Psychiatry. Among Last Authors, there were 85,954 remaining papers, split between 31,793 (36.98%) in Neurology, 25,438 (29.59%) in Neuroscience, and 28,723 (33.42%) in Psychiatry.”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I would like to thank the authors for their responses the points that I raised, I do not have any new comments or further responses.

    1. Author response:

      We appreciate that the reviewers recognize the conceptual novelty of our work and find our work interesting.

      Reviewer #1:

      We thank Reviewer #1 for making us aware that the image presentation of some of what we see as very clear phenotypes in our work might not have been optimal in the reviewed pdf file, presumably due to the relatively low resolution and lack of appropriately magnified images in the merged pdf file. This issue– if not caught and corrected now– might have caused future readers to similarly not appreciate these clear phenotypes. We will carefully revise the figures and ensure maintenance of appropriate pdf resolution in the merged file so that image presentation is optimal and our findings are appropriately represented.

      We appreciate that Reviewer #1 carefully and critically assessed the growth cone transcriptomic data. We agree that future additional validation is warranted, and this will be clearly stated in our revised paper. Because we judge that these data – even in their current form – will be of potential interest to other investigators sooner rather than later, we respectfully offer and request that we should share them in this paper as our attempt so far to identify elements of the relevant growth cone biology, rather than waiting for years before completing additional validation.

      Even upon repeated reflection, we judge and respectfully submit that our CRISPR in utero electroporation experiments are, indeed, conducted with appropriate controls. We thought through the potential controls deeply prior to completing these complex experiments. We will describe our reasoning in detail in our point-by-point response.

      Reviewer #2:

      We thank Reviewer #2 for encouraging us to elaborate on the direction and cross- repressive interplay between Bcl11a and Bcl11b, which we previously identified (Woodworth*, Greig* et al., Cell Rep, 2016). We omitted deep discussion because we had already published this result, cited that work, and did not want to seem overly self- referential, as well as for reasons of length. Though we know and have reported that Bcl11a and Bcl11b are cross-repressive in SCPN development, we currently do not know whether increased Bcl11a expression in Bcl11b-null SCPN contributes to reduced Cdh13 expression. Also, we do not know if there is a similar Bcl11a-Bcl11b cross repression in striatal medium spiny neurons. This will be clarified in our revised paper.

      We agree fully with the reviewer that “the common practice of picking from a list of differentially expressed genes the most likely ones” has been useful for and has substantially contributed to the elucidation of molecular mechanisms in many systems, including in CNS development. Indeed, the current paper identifies Cdh13 as a newly recognized functional molecule in SCPN axon development by in part using this approach. Cdh13 belongs to a well-known gene family, and its expression by SCPN was already reported by us (Arlotta*, Molyneauz* et al., Neuron, 2005). Despite these two facts, we newly identify its function in SCPN development, which has never been investigated or reported. We appreciate the reviewer encouraging us to elaborate on this here.

      Recent technical advancement allows functional screening of a larger list of genes in vivo (Jin et al., Science, 2020; Ramani et al., bioRxiv, 2024; Zheng et al., Cell, 2024). That said, it is still a challenge to specifically access SCPN in vivo and apply such a high-throughput screening assay for axon development. We agree and predict that future work of this type might likely lead to identification of other new and unknown molecular regulators. We respectfully submit that our work reported here will provide useful foundation for many such future studies.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript reports that expression of the E. coli operon topAI/yjhQ/yjhP is controlled by the translation status of a small open reading frame, that authors have discovered and named toiL, located in the leader region of the operon. The authors propose the following model for topAI activation: Under normal conditions, toiL is translated but topAI is not expressed because of Rho-dependent transcription termination within the topAI ORF and because its ribosome binding site and start codon are trapped in an mRNA hairpin. Ribosome stalling at various codons of the toiL ORF, caused by the presence of some ribosome-targeting antibiotics, triggers an mRNA conformational switch which allows translation of topAI and, in addition, activation of the operon's transcription because the presence of translating ribosomes at the topAI ORF blocks Rho from terminating transcription. Even though the model is appealing and several of the experimental data support some aspects of it, several inconsistencies remain to be solved. In addition, even though TopAI was shown to be an inhibitor of topoisomerase I (Yamaguchi & Inouye, 2015, NAR 43:10387), the authors suggest, without offering any experimental support, that, because ribosome-targeting antibiotics act as inducers, expression of the topAI/yjhQ/yjhP operon may confer resistance to these drugs.

      Strengths:

      - There is good experimental support of the transcriptional repression/activation switch aspect of the model, derived from well-designed transcriptional reporters and ChIP-qPCR approaches.

      - There is a clever use of the topAI-lacZ reporter to find the 23S rRNA mutants where expression topAI was upregulated. This eventually led the authors to identify that translation events occurring at toiL are important to regulate the topAI/yjhQ/yjhP operon. Is there any published evidence that ribosomes with the identified mutations translate slowly (decreased fidelity does not necessarily mean slow translation, does it?)?

      G2253 is in helix 80 of the 23S rRNA, which has been proposed to be involved in correct positioning of the tRNA. Mutations in helix 80 have been reported to cause defects in peptidyl transferase center activity, which could reduce the rate of ribosome movement along the mRNA. If ribosomes are sufficiently slowed when translating toiL, this could induce expression of topAI. G1911 and Ψ1917 are in helix 69 of the 23S rRNA, which is involved in forming the inter-subunit bridge, as well as interactions with release factors. Mutations in helix 69 cause a decrease in the processivity of translation, suggesting that the mutations we identified may increase the occupancy of ribosomes within toiL, thereby inducing expression of topAI. We have added text to the Discussion section to include this speculation.

      - Authors incorporate relevant links to the antibiotic-mediated expression regulation of bacterial resistance genes. Authors can also mention the tryptophan-mediated ribosome stalling at the tnaC leader ORF that activates the expression of tryptophan metabolism genes through blockage of Rho-mediated transcriptional attenuation.

      We have added a citation to a recent structural study of ribosomes translating the tnaC uORF. Specifically, we speculate in the Discussion that toiL may have evolved to sense a ribosome-targeting antibiotic, or another ribosome-targeting small molecule such as an amino acid.

      Weaknesses:

      The main weaknesses of the work are related to several experimental results that are not consistent with the model, or related to a lack of data that needs to be included to support the model.

      The following are a few examples:

      - It is surprising that authors do not mention that several published Ribo-seq data from E. coli cells show active translation of toiL (for example Li et al., 2014, Cell 157: 624). Therefore, it is hard to reconcile with the model that starts codon/Shine-Dalgarno mutations in the toiL-lux reporter have no effect on luciferase expression (Figure 2C, bar graphs of the no antibiotic control samples).

      These data are for a topAI-lux reporter construct rather than toiL-lux. In our model, ribosome stalling within toiL is required to induce expression of the downstream genes; preventing translation of toiL by mutating the start codon or Shine-Dalgarno sequence would not cause ribosome stalling, consistent with the lack of an effect on topAI expression.

      - The SHAPE reactivity data shown in Figure 5A are not consistent with the toiL ORF being translated. In addition, it is difficult to visualize the effect of tetracycline on mRNA conformation with the representation used in Figure 5B. It would be better to show SHAPE reactivity without/with Tet (as shown in panel A of the figure).

      We have modified this figure (now Figure 6) so that we no longer show the SHAPE-seq data +/- tetracycline overlayed on the predicted RNA structure, since at best, the predicted structure likely only represents uninduced state. We have included the predicted structure together with the SHAPE-seq data for untreated cells as a separate panel because it is part of the basis for our model. We have also added a supplementary figure showing a similar RNA structure prediction based on conservation of the topAI upstream region across species (Figure 6 – figure supplement 1), and we describe this in the text.

      - The "increased coverage" of topAI/yjhP/yjhQ in the presence of tetracycline from the Ribo-seq data shown in Figure 6A can be due to activation of translation, transcription, or both. For readers to know which of these possibilities apply, authors need to provide RNA-seq data and show the profiles of the topAI/yjhQ/yjhP genes in control/Tet-treated cells.

      A previous study (Li et al., 2014, PMID 24766808) compared RNA-seq and Ribo-seq data for E. coli to measure normalized ribosome occupancy for each gene. However, sequence coverage for topAI was too low to confidently quantify either the RNA-seq or the Ribo-seq data. Presumably RNA levels were low because of Rho termination. Hence, we were not confident that RNA-seq would provide information on the regulation of topAI-yjhQP. Other data in our study provide strong evidence that regulation is primarily at the level of translation. And the key conclusion from Figure 6 (now Figure 7) is that tetracycline stalls ribosomes on start codons.

      - Similarly, to support the data of increased ribosomal footprints at the toiL start codon in the presence of Tet (Figure 6B), authors should show the profile of the toiL gene from control and Tet-treated cells.

      Figure 6B shows data for both treated and untreated cells. The overall ribosome occupancy is much lower for untreated cells, making it difficult to draw strong conclusions about the relative distribution of ribosomes across toiL.

      - Representation of the mRNA structures in the model shown in Figure 5, does not help with visualizing 1) how ribosomes translate toiL since the ORF is trapped in double-stranded mRNA, and 2) how ribosome stalling on toiL would lead to the release of the initiation region of topAI to achieve expression activation.

      We now show the predicted structure with only SHAPE-seq data for untreated cells. The comparison of SHAPE-seq +/- tetracycline is shown without reference to the predicted structure.

      - The authors speculate that, because ribosome-targeting antibiotics act as expression inducers [by the way, authors should mention and comment that, more than a decade ago, it had been reported that kanamycin (PMID: 12736533) and gentamycin (PMID: 19013277) are inducers of topAI and yjhQ], the genes of the topAI/yjhQ/yjhP operon may confer resistance to these antibiotics. Such a suggestion can be experimentally checked by simply testing whether strains lacking these genes have increased sensitivity to the antibiotic inducers.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out these references, which we now cite. The fact that another group found that gentamycin induces topAI expression – it is one of the most highly induced genes in that paper – strongly suggests that we missed the key inducing concentrations for one or more antibiotics, meaning that topAI is induced by even more ribosome-targeting antibiotics than we realized.

      We did some preliminary experiments to look for effects of TopAI, YjhQ, and/or YjhP on antibiotic sensitivity, but generated only negative results. Since these experiments were preliminary and far from exhaustive, we have chosen not to include them in the manuscript. Other studies of genes regulated by ribosome stalling in a uORF have looked at genes whose functions in responding to translation stress were already known, so the environmental triggers were more obvious. With so many possible triggers for topAI-yjhQP, it will likely require considerable effort to find the relevant trigger(s). Hence, we consider this an important question, but beyond the scope of this manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this important study, Baniulyte and Wade describe how the translation of an 8-codon uORF denoted toiL upstream of the topAI-yjhQP operon is responsive to different ribosome-targeting antibiotics, consequently controlling translation of the TopAI toxin as well as Rho-dependent termination with the gene.

      Strengths:

      I appreciate that the authors used multiple different approaches such as a genetic screen to identify factors such as 23S rRNA mutations that affect topA1 expression and ribosome profiling to examine the consequences of various antibiotics on toiL-mediated regulation. The results are convincing and clearly described.

      Weaknesses:

      I have relatively minor suggestions for improving the manuscript. These mainly relate to the figures.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors nicely show that the translation and ribosome stalling within the ToiL uORF upstream of the co-transcribed topAI-yjhQ toxin-antitoxin genes unmask the topAI translational initiation site, thereby allowing ribosome loading and preventing premature Rho-dependent transcription termination in the topAI region. Although similar translational/transcriptional attenuation has been reported in other systems, the base pairing between the leader sequence and the repressed region by the long RNA looping is somehow unique in toiL-topAI-yjhQP. The experiments are solidly executed, and the manuscript is clear in most parts with areas that could be improved or better explained. The real impact of such a study is not easy to appreciate due to a lack of investigation on the physiological consequences of topAI-yjhQP activation upon antibiotic exposure (see details below).

      Strengths:

      Conclusion/model is supported by the integrated approaches consisting of genetics, in vivo SHAPE-seq and Ribo-Seq.

      Provide an elegant example of cis-acting regulatory peptides to a growing list of functional small proteins in bacterial proteomes.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewing Editor Comments:

      (1) Examine the consequences of mutations impeding translation of the topAI/yjhQ/yjhP operon on cell growth in the presence and absence of antibiotics.

      See response to Reviewer 1’s comment.

      (2) Resolve discrepancies between the SHAPE data indicating constitutive sequestration of the toiL Shine Dalgarno sequence with antibiotic-regulated translation of the toiL ORF.

      See response to Reviewer 1’s comment.

      (3) Reconcile published Ribo-Seq data with the model that start codon/Shine-Dalgarno mutations in the toiL-lux reporter have no effect on luciferase expression in the absence of antibiotics.

      See response to Reviewer 1’s comment.

      (4) Clarify whether antibiotic MIC values were employed to select antibiotic concentrations for different experiments.

      The antibiotic concentrations we used are in line with reported MICs for E. coli. We now list the reported ECOFFs/MICs and include relevant citations.

      (5) Provide RNA-seq data to complement the Ribo-Seq data for the topAI/yjhQ/yjhP genes in control vs. Tet-treated cells.

      See response to Reviewer 1’s comment.

      (6) Revise the text to address as many of the reviewers' suggestions as reasonably possible.

      Changes to the text have been made as indicated in the responses to the reviewers’ comments.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the Authors):

      (1) Page 6: I would have liked to have more information about the 39 suppressor mutations in rho. Do any of the cis-acting mutations give support for the model proposed in Figure 8?

      We only know the specific mutation for some of the strains, and we now list those mutations in the Methods section. For other mutants, we mapped the mutation to either the rho gene or to Rho activity, but we did not sequence the rho gene. Most of the specific mutations we did identify fall within the primary RNA-binding site of Rho and hence should be considered partial-loss-of-function mutations (complete loss of function would be lethal).

      We identified cis-acting mutations by re-transforming the lacZ reporter plasmid into a wild-type strain. We did not sequence any of these plasmids.

      (2) Page 12-13, Section entitled "Mapping ribosome stalling sites induced by different antibiotics": This section should start with a better transition regarding the logic of why the experiments were carried out and should end with an interpretation of the results.

      We have added a few sentences at the start of this section to explain the rationale. We have also added two sentences at the end of this section to summarize the interpretation of the data.

      (3) Page 15: The authors should discuss under what conditions the expression of TopAI (and YjhQ/YjhP might be induced? Is expression also elevated upon amino acid starvation?

      We have looked through public RNA-seq data but have not identified growth conditions other than antibiotic treatment that induce expression of topAI, yjhQ or yjhP.

      (4) References: The authors should be consistent about capitalization, italics, and abbreviations in the references.

      These formatting errors will be fixed in the proofing stage.

      (5) All graph figures: There should be more uniformity in the sizes of individual data points (some are almost impossible to see) and error bars across the figures.

      We have tried to make the data points and error bars more visible for figures where they were smaller.

      (6) Figure 1B: I do not think the left arrow labeling is very intuitive and suggest renaming these constructs.

      We have removed the arrows to improve clarity.

      (7) Figure 2A: toiL should be introduced at the first mention of Figure 2A.

      We have added a schematic of the topAI-yjhQ-yjhP region as Figure 1A, including the toiL ORF, which we briefly mention in the text. We have opted to split Figure 2C into two panels. In Figure 2C we now only show data for the wild-type construct. Data for the mutant constructs are now shown in a new figure (Figure 5), alongside data for the wild-type constructs. We have simplified Figure 2A, since the mutations are not relevant to this revised figure, and we now show the schematic with the mutations as Figure 5A.

      (8) Figure 3C and 3D: I suggest giving these graphs headings (or changing the color of the bars in Figure 3D) to make it more obvious that different things are measured in the two panels.

      We have added headers to panels B-D make it clear that which graphs show ChIP-qPCR data which graph shows qRT-PCR data.

      (9) Figure 6: It might be nice to show the topAI-yjhPQ operon here.

      We now show the operon in Figure 1A.

      (10) Figure 8: This figure could be optimized by adding 5' and 3' end labels and having more similarity with the model in Figure 7.

      The constructs shown in Figure 7 lack most of the topAI upstream region, so they aren’t readily comparable to the schematic in Figure 8. However, we have changed the color of the ribosome in Figure 7 to match that in Figure 8. We also indicate the 5’ end of the RNA in Figure 8.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the Authors):

      Areas to improve:

      (1) While it's important to learn about ToiL-dependent regulation of the downstream topAI-yjhQ toxin-antitoxin genes, the physiological consequence of topAI-yjhQ activation seems to be lost in the manuscript. Everything was done with a reporter lacZ/lux. In the absence of toiL translation (i.e. SD mutant) and/or ribosome stalling, does premature transcription termination result in non-stochiometric synthesis of toxin vs. antitoxin, leading to growth arrest or other measurable phenotype? Knowing the impact of ToiL in the native topAI-yjhQ context will be valuable.

      See response to Reviewer 1’s comment.

      (2) It was indicated in Figure 4-figure supplement 1 that toiL homologs are found in many other proteobacteria, are the UR sequences in those species also form a similar inhibitory RNA loop?? The nt sequence identity of toiL is likely to be constrained by the base pairing of the topAI 5' region.

      We have added a supplementary figure panel showing an RNA structure prediction for the topAI upstream region based on sequence alignment of homologous regions from other species (Figure 6 – figure supplement 1).

      What is the frequency of the MLENVII hepta-peptide in the E. coli genome-wide. Is the sequence disfavored to avoid spurious multi-antibiotic sensing?

      LENVII is not found in any annotated E. coli K-12 protein. However, this is a sufficiently long sequence that we would expect few to no instances in the E. coli proteome.

      (3) Figure 1A, it would be helpful to indicate the location of the toiL (red arrow as in Figure 2A) relative to the putative rut site early in the beginning of the results. Does TSS mark the transcription start site? There is no annotation of TSS in the figure legend. Was TSS previously mapped experimentally? Please include relevant citations.

      We now indicate the position of the TSS relative to the topAI start codon. Similarly, we indicate the position of the start of toiL relative to the topAI start codon in Figure 2A. We now explain “TSS” in the figure legend. There is a reference in the text for the TSS (Thomason et al., 2015).

      (4) Please consider rearranging the results section, perhaps more helpful to introduce the toiL in Figure 1 or earlier. The current format requires readers to switch back-and-forth between Figure 4 and Figure 2.

      We have added a schematic of the topAI upstream region as Figure 1A, and we have separated Figure 2C as described in a response to a comment from Reviewer 2.

      (5) Figure 2A and Figure 2-Figure Suppl 1A, for clarity, please mark the rut site upstream of the red arrow.

      Rather than mark the rut on Figure 2A, which would make for a busy schematic, readers can compare the positions of the rut to those of toiL, which we have now added to Figures 1B (formerly Figure 1A) and 2A.

      (6) The following conclusion seems speculative: "...but does not trigger termination until RNAP ..., >180 nt further downstream…". Shouldn't the authors already know where the termination site is based on their previous Term-seq data (see Ref 1, Adams PP et al 2021)?

      Sites of Rho-dependent transcription termination cannot be mapped precisely from Term-seq data because exoribonucleases rapidly process the unstructured RNA 3’ ends.

      (7) Genetic screen: Please discuss why the 23S rRNA mutations that cause translational infidelity could promote topAI translation. Wouldn't the mutant ribosome be affected in translating toiL?

      See response to Reviewer 1’s comment.

      (8) Although antibiotic concentrations were provided in Figure 2 legend, please provide the MIC values of each antibiotic, e.g., in Table S2, for the tested E. coli strain, to inform readers how specific subinhibitory concentrations were chosen.

      See response to Reviewing Editor.

      (9) Please clarify the calculation of luciferase units in the y-axis of Figure 2A, why the scale is drastically higher than that of Figure 7C using the same antibiotics?

      These reporter assays use different constructs. The reporter construct used for experiments in Figure 7 includes a portion of the ermCL gene and associated downstream sequence. We have enlarged Figure 7A to highlight the difference in reporter constructs.

      (10) Table S4 needs a few more details. It is unclear how those numbers in columns G-H were generated. Do those numbers correspond to ribosome density per nt/ORF?

      We have added footnotes to Table S4 to indicate that the numbers in columns G and H represent sequence read coverage normalized by region length and by the upper quartile of gene expression.

      (11) Figure 5, if the SHAPE results were true, the Shine Dalgarno sequence of toiL is sequestered in the hairpin structure with and without tetracycline treatment. It is inconceivable that translational initiation will occur efficiently, please discuss.

      Our representation of the SHAPE-seq data was confusing since we overlayed the SHAPE-seq changes on a predicted structure that likely corresponds to the uninduced state. We hope that the new version of Figure 5 is clearer.

      We presume the reviewer is referring to the Shine-Dalgarno sequence of topAI rather than toiL, since the Shine-Dalgarno sequence of toiL is predicted to be unstructured even in the absence of tetracycline treatment. The ribosome-binding site of topAI is more accessible in cells treated with tetracycline, although the SHAPE-seq data suggest that this is a transient event. The binding of the initiating ribosome may also reduce reactivity in this region under inducing conditions. We now discuss this briefly in the text.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews: 

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      The manuscript consists of two separate but interlinked investigations: genomic epidemiology and virulence assessment of Salmonella Dublin. ST10 dominates the epidemiological landscape of S. Dublin, while ST74 was uncommonly isolated. Detailed genomic epidemiology of ST10 unfolded the evolutionary history of this common genotype, highlighting clonal expansions linked to each distinct geography. Notably, North American ST10 was associated with more antimicrobial resistance compared to others. The authors also performed long read sequencing on a subset of isolates (ST10 and ST74), and uncovered a novel recombinant virulence plasmid in ST10 (IncX1/IncFII/IncN). Separately, the authors performed cell invasion and cytotoxicity assays on the two S. Dublin genotypes, showing differential responses between the two STs. ST74 replicates better intracellularly in macrophage compared to ST10, but both STs induced comparable cytotoxicity levels. Comparative genomic analyses between the two genotypes showed certain genetic content unique to each genotype, but no further analyses were conducted to investigate which genetic factors likely associated with the observed differences. The study provides a comprehensive and novel understanding on the evolution and adaptation of two S. Dublin genotypes, which can inform public health measures. The methodology included in both approaches were sound and written in sufficient detail, and data analysis were performed with rigour. Source data were fully presented and accessible to readers. 

      Comments on revised version: 

      The authors have addressed all the points raised by the reviewer. The manuscript is now much enhanced in clarity and accuracy. The re-written Discussion is more relevant and brings in comparison with other invasive Salmonella serotypes. 

      Comments: 

      In light of the metadata supplied in this revision, for Australian isolates, all human cases of ST74 (n=7) were from faeces (assuming from gastroenteritis) while 18/40 of ST10 were from invasive specimen (blood and abscess). This may contradict with the manuscript's finding and discussion on different experiment phenotypes of the two STs, with ST74 showing more replication in macrophages and potentially more invasive. Thus, the reviewer suggests the authors to mention this disparity in the Discussion, and discuss possible reasons underlying this disparity. This can strengthen the author's rationale for further in vivo studies. 

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this important observation. We have amended the text in the Discussion to address the differences in source of human cases as suggested by the Reviewer (lines 392-430). We have also included text highlighting the important knowledge gaps in understanding the drivers for emerging iNTS with broad host ranges and identify future avenues of research that could be explored to better understand the observed differences in the host-pathogen interactions.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      This is a comprehensive analysis of Salmonella Dublin genomes that offers insights into the global spread of this pathogen and region-specific traits that are important to understand its evolution. The phenotyping of isolates of ST10 and ST74 also offer insights into the variability that can be seen in S. Dublin, which is also seen in other Salmonella serovars, and reminds the field that it is important to look beyond lab-adapted strains to truly understand these pathogens. This is a valuable contribution to the field. The only limitation, which the authors also acknowledge, is the bias towards S. Dublin genomes from high income settings. However, there is no selection bias; this is simply a consequence of publicly available sequences. 

      We thank the reviewer for their comments and acknowledge the limitations of this study.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) The authors repeatedly assert that an individual's behavior in the foraging assay depends on its prior history (particularly cultivation conditions). While this seems like a reasonable expectation, it is not fully fleshed out. The work would benefit from studies in which animals are raised on more or less abundant food before the behavioral task.

      Cultivation density: While we agree with the reviewer that testing the effects of varying bacterial density during animal development (cultivation) is an interesting experiment, it is not feasible at this time. We previously attempted this experiment but found it nontrivial to maintain stable bacterial density conditions over long timescales as this requires matching the rate of bacterial growth with the rate of bacterial consumption. Despite our best efforts, we have not been able to identify conditions that satisfy these requirements. Thus, we focused our revised manuscript to include only assertions about the effects of recent experiences and added this inquiry as a future direction (lines 618-624).

      (2) The authors convincingly show that the probability of particular behavioral outcomes occurring upon patch encounter depends on time-associated parameters (time since last patch encounter, time since last patch exploitation). There are two concerns here. First, it is not clear how these values are initialized - i.e., what values are used for the first occurrence of each behavioral state? More importantly, the authors don't seem to consider the simplest time parameter, the time since the start of the assay (or time since worm transfer). Transferring animals to a new environment can be associated with significant mechanical stimulus, and it seems quite possible that transferring animals causes them to enter a state of arousal. This arousal, which certainly could alter sensory function or decision-making, would likely decay with time. It would be interesting to know how well the model performs using time since assay starts as the only time-dependent parameter.

      Parameter Initialization: We thank the reviewer for pointing out an oversight in our methods section regarding the model parameter values used for the first encounter. We clarified the initialization of parameters in the manuscript (lines 1162-1179). In short, for the first patch encounter where k = 1:

      ρ<sub>k</sub> is the relative density of the first patch.

      τ<sub>s</sub> is the duration of time spent off food since the beginning of the recorded experiment. For the first patch, this is equivalent to the total time elapsed.

      ρ<sub>h</sub> is the approximated relative density of the bacterial patch on the acclimation plates (see Assay preparation and recording in Methods). Acclimation plates contained one large 200 µL patch seeded with OD<sub>600</sub> = 1 and grown for a total of ~48 hours. As with all patches, the relative density was estimated from experiments using fluorescent bacteria OP50-GFP as described in Bacterial patch density estimation in Methods.

      ρ<sub>e</sub> is equivalent to ρ<sub>h</sub>.

      Transfer Method: We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful comment on how the stress of transferring animals to a new plate may have resulted in an increased arousal state and thus a greater probability of rejecting patches. We anticipated this possibility and, in order to mitigate the stress of moving, we used an agar plug method where animals were transferred using the flat surface of small cylinders of agar. Importantly, the use of agar as a medium to transfer animals provides minimal disruption to their environment as all physical properties (e.g. temperature, humidity, surface tension) are maintained. Qualitatively, we observed no marked change in behavior from before to after transfer with the agar plug method, especially as compared to the often drastic changes observed when using a metal or eyelash pick. We added these additional methodological details to the methods (lines 791-796).

      Time Parameter: However, the reviewer’s concern that the simplest time parameter (time since start of the assay) might better predict animal behavior is valid. We thank the reviewer for pointing out the need to specifically test whether the time-dependent change in explore-exploit decision-making corresponds better with satiety (time off patch) or arousal (time since transfer/start of assay) state. To test this hypothesis, we ran our model with varying combinations of the satiety term τ<sub>s</sub> and a transfer term τ<sub>t</sub>. We found that when both terms were included in the model, the coefficient of the transfer term was non-significant. This result suggests that the relevant time-dependent term is more likely related to satiety than transfer-induced stress (lines 343-358; Figure 4 - supplement 4D).

      (3) Similarly, Figures 2L and M clearly show that the probability of a search event occurring upon a patch encounter decreases markedly with time. Because search events are interpreted as a failure to detect a patch, this implies that the detection of (dilute) patches becomes more efficient with time. It would be useful for the authors to consider this possibility as well as potential explanations, which might be related to the point above.

      Time-dependent changes in sensing: We agree with the reviewer that we observe increased responsiveness to dilute patches with time. Although this is interesting, our primary focus was on what decision an animal made given that they clearly sensed the presence of the bacterial patch. Nonetheless, we added this observation to the discussion as an area of future work to investigate the sensory mechanisms behind this effect (lines 563-568).

      (4) Based on their results with mec-4 and osm-6 mutants, the authors assert that chemosensation, rather than mechanosensation, likely accounts for animals' ability to measure patch density. This argument is not well-supported: mec-4 is required only for the function of the six non-ciliated light-touch neurons (AVM, PVM, ALML/R, PLML/R). In contrast, osm-6 is expected to disrupt the function of the ciliated dopaminergic mechanosensory neurons CEP, ADE, and PDE, which have previously been shown to detect the presence of bacteria (Sawin et al 2000). Thus, the paper's results are entirely consistent with an important role of mechanosensation in detecting bacterial abundance. Along these lines, it would be useful for the authors to speculate on why osm-6 mutants are more, rather than less, likely to "accept" when encountering a patch.

      Sensory mutant behavior: We thank the reviewer for pointing out the error in our interpretation of the behavior of osm-6 and mec-4 animals. We further elaborated on our findings and edited the text to better reflect that osm-6 mutants lack both chemosensory and mechanosensory ciliated sensory neurons (lines 406-448; lines 567-577). Specifically, we provided some commentary on the finding that osm-6 mutants show an augmented ability to detect the presence of bacterial patches but a reduced ability to assess their bacterial density. While this finding seems contradictory, it suggests that in the absence of the ability to assess bacterial density, animals must prioritize exploiting food resources when available.

      (5) While the evidence for the accept-reject framework is strong, it would be useful for the authors to provide a bit more discussion about the null hypothesis and associated expectations. In other words, what would worm behavior in this assay look like if animals were not able to make accept-reject decisions, relying only on exploit-explore decisions that depend on modulation of food-leaving probability?

      Accept-reject vs. stay-switch: We thank the reviewer for alerting us to this gap in our discussion. We have revised the text to further extrapolate upon our point of view on this somewhat philosophical distinction and what it predicts about C. elegans behavior (lines 507-533).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      (1) Sensing vs. non-sensing

      The authors claim that when animals encounter dilute food patches, they do not sense them, as evidenced by the shallow deceleration that occurs when animals encounter these patches. This seems ethologically inaccurate. There is a critical difference between not sensing a stimulus, and not reacting to it. Animals sense numerous stimuli from their environment, but often only behaviorally respond to a fraction of them, depending on their attention and arousal state. With regard to C. elegans, it is well-established that their amphid chemosensory neurons are capable of detecting very dilute concentrations of odors. In addition, the authors provide evidence that osm-6 animals have altered exploit behaviors, further supporting the importance of amphid chemosensory neurons in this behavior.

      Interpretation of “non-sensing” encounters: We thank the reviewer for their comment and agree that we do not know for certain whether the animals sensed these patches or were merely non-responsive to them. We are, however, confident that these encounters lack evidence of sensing. Specifically, we note that our analyses used to classify events as sensing or non-sensing examined whether an animal’s slow-down upon patch entry could be distinguished from either that of events where animals exploited or that of encounters with patches lacking bacteria. We found that  “non-sensing” encounters are indeed indistinguishable from encounters with bacteria-free patches where there are no bacteria to be sensed (see Figure 2 - Supplement 8A-C and Patch encounter classification as sensing or non-responding in Methods). Regardless, we agree with the reviewer that all that can be asserted about these events is that animals do not appear to respond to the bacterial patch in any way that we measured. Therefore, we have replaced the term “non-sensing” with “non-responding” to better indicate the ethological interpretation of these events and clarified the text to reflect this change (lines 193-200; lines 211-212).

      (2) Search vs. sample & sensing vs. non-sensing

      In Figures 2H and 2I, the authors claim that there are three behavioral states based on quantifying average velocity, encounter duration, and acceleration, but I only see three. Based on density distributions alone, there really only seem to be 2 distributions, not 3. The authors claim there are three, but to come to this conclusion, they used a QDA, which inherently is based on the authors training the model to detect three states based on prior annotations. Did the authors perform a model test, such as the Bayesian Information Criterion, to confirm whether 2 vs. 3 Gaussians is statistically significant? It seems like the authors are trying to impose two states on a phenomenon with a broad distribution. This seems very similar to the results observed for roaming vs. dwelling experiments, which again, are essentially two behavioral states.

      Validation of sensing clusters: We are grateful to the reviewer for pointing out the difficulty in visualizing the clusters and the need for additional clarity in explaining the semi-supervised QDA approach. We added additional visualizations and methods to validate the clusters we have discovered. Specifically, we used Silverman’s test to show that the sensing vs. non-responding data were bi-modal (i.e. a two-cluster classification method fits best) and accompanied this statistical test with heat maps which better illustrate the clusters (lines 171-173; lines 190-191; lines 948-972; lines 1003-1005; Figure 2 - supplement 6A-C; Figure 2 - supplement 7C-F).

      Further, it seems that there may be some confusion as to how we arrived at 3 encounter types (i.e. search, sample, exploit). It’s important to note that two methods were used on two different (albeit related) sets of parameters. We first used a two-cluster GMM to classify encounters as explore or exploit. We then used a two-cluster semi-supervised QDA to classify encounters as sensing or non-sensing (now changed to “non-responding”, see above response) using a different set of parameters. We thus separated the explore cluster into two (sensing and non-responding exploratory events) resulting in three total encounter types: exploit, sample (explore/sensing), and search (explore/non-sensing).

      (4) History-dependence of the GLM

      The logistic GLM seems like a logical way to model a binary choice, and I think the parameters you chose are certainly important. However, the framing of them seems odd to me. I do not doubt the animals are assessing the current state of the patch with an assessment of past experience; that makes perfect logical sense. However, it seems odd to reduce past experience to the categories of recently exploited patch, recently encountered patch, and time since last exploitation. This implies the animals have some way of discriminating these past patch experiences and committing them to memory. Also, it seems logical that the time on these patches, not just their density, should also matter, just as the time without food matters. Time is inherent to memory. This model also imposes a prior categorization in trying to distinguish between sensed vs. not-sensed patches, which I criticized earlier. Only "sensed" patches are used in the model, but it is questionable whether worms genuinely do not "sense" these patches.

      Model design: We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful comments on the model. We completed a number of analyses involving model selection including model selection criteria (AIC, BIC) and optimization with regularization techniques (LASSO and elastic nets) and found that the problem of model selection was compounded by the enormous array of highly-correlated variables we had to choose from. Additionally, we found that both interaction terms and non-linear terms of our task variables could be predictive of accept-reject decisions but that the precise set of terms selected depended sensitively on which model selection technique was used and generally made rather small contributions to prediction. The diverse array of results and combinatorial number of predictors to possibly include failed to add anything of interpretable value. We therefore chose to take a different approach to this problem. Rather than trying to determine what the “best” model was we instead asked whether a minimal model could be used to answer a set of core questions. Indeed, our goal was not maximal predictive performance but rather to distinguish between the effects of different influences enough to determine if encounter history had a significant, independent effect on decision making. We thus chose to only include task variables that spanned the most basic components of behavioral mechanisms to ask very specific questions. For example, we selected a time variable that we thought best encapsulated satiety. While we could have included many additional terms, or made different choices about which terms to include, based on our analyses these choices would not have qualitatively changed our results. Further, we sought to validate the parameters we chose with additional studies (i.e. food-deprived and sensory mutant animals). We regard our study as an initial foray into demonstrating accept-reject decision-making in nematodes. The exact mechanisms and, consequently, the best model design are therefore beyond the scope of this study.

      Lastly, in regards to the use of only sensed patches in the model; while we acknowledge that we are not certain as to whether the “non-responding” encounters are truly not sensed, we find qualitatively similar results when including all exploratory patches in our analyses. However, we take the position that sensation is necessary for decision-making and thus believe that while our model’s predictive performance may be better using all encounters, the interpretation of our findings is stronger when we only include sensing events. We have added additional commentary about our model to the discussion section (lines 667-695).

      (5) osm-6

      The osm-6 results are interesting. This seems to indicate that the worms are still sensing the food, but are unable to assess quality, therefore the default response is to exploit. How do you think the worms are sensing the food? Clearly, they sense it, but without the amphid sensory neurons, and not mechanosensation. Perhaps feeding is important? Could you speculate on this?

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful remarks. We have added additional commentary about the result of our sensory mutant experiments as described above in response to Reviewer #1 under Sensory mutant behavior.

      (7) Impact:

      I think this work will have a solid impact on the field, as it provides tangible variables to test how animals assess their environment and decide to exploit resources. I think the strength of this research could be strengthened by a reassessment of their model that would both simplify it and provide testable timescales of satiety/starvation memory.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors title the work as an "ethological study" and emphasize the theme of "foraging in naturalistic environments" in contrast to typical laboratory conditions. The only difference in this study relative to typical laboratory conditions is that the food bacteria is distributed in many small patches as compared to one large patch. First, it is not clear to the reviewer that the size of the food patches in these experiments is more relevant to C. elegans in its natural context than the standard sizes of food patches. Furthermore, all the other highly unnatural conditions typical of laboratory cultivation still apply: the use of a 2D agar substrate, a single food bacteria that is not a component of a naturalistic diet, and the use of a laboratory-adapted strain of C. elegans with behavior quite distinct from that of natural isolates. The reviewer is not suggesting that the authors need to make their experiments more naturalistic, only that the experiments as described here should not be described as naturalistic or ethological as there is no support for such claims.

      Ethological interpretation: We thank the reviewer for their comments about the use of the term ethological to describe this study. We chose to develop a patchy bacterial assay to mimic the naturalistic “boom-or-bust” environment. While we agree with the reviewer that we do not know if the size and distribution of the food patches in these experiments is more relevant to C. elegans, we maintain that these experiments were ecologically-inspired and revealed behavior that is difficult to observe in environments with large, densely-seeded bacterial patches. We have updated our text to better reflect that this study was “ecologically-inspired” rather than truly “ethological” in nature (lines 94, 693).

      The main finding of the paper is that worms explore and then exploit, i.e. they frequently reject several bacterial patches before accepting one. This result requires additional scrutiny to reject other possible interpretations. In particular, when worms are transferred to a new plate we would expect some period of increased arousal due to the stressful handling process. A high arousal state might cause rejection of food patches. Could the measured accept/reject decisions be influenced by this effect? One approach to addressing this concern would be to allow the animals to acclimate to the new plate on a bare region before encountering the new food patches.

      We thank the reviewer for their comment on how the stress of transferring animals to a new plate may have resulted in an increased arousal state and thus a greater probability of rejecting patches. We addressed this above in response to Reviewer #1 under Transfer Method and Time Parameter. In brief, we used a worm picking method that mitigated stress and added additional analyses showing that a transfer-related term was less predictive than a satiety-related term.

      Related to the above, in what circumstances exactly are the authors claiming that worms first explore and then exploit? After being briefly deprived of food? After being handled?

      Explore-then-exploit: All animals were well-fed and handled gently as described above under Transfer Method (lines 787-795). Our results suggest that the appearance of an explore-then-exploit strategy is a byproduct of being transferred from an environment with high bacterial density to an environment with low bacterial density as described in the manuscript (lines 461-466).

      The authors emphasize their analysis of the accept/reject decision as a critical innovation. However, the accept/reject decision does not strike me as substantially different from the previously described stay/switch decision. When a worm encounters a new patch of bacteria, accepting this bacteria is equivalent to staying on it and rejecting (leaving) it is equivalent to switching away from it. The authors should explain how these concepts are significantly distinct.

      Accept-reject vs. stay-switch: We thank the reviewer for alerting us to this gap in our discussion. We have revised the text to further extrapolate upon our point of view on this somewhat philosophical distinction and what it predicts about C. elegans behavior (lines 507-533).

      During patch encounter classification, the authors computed three of the animals' behavioral metrics (Line 801-804) and claimed that the combination of these three metrics reveals two non-Gaussian clusters representing encounters where animals sensed the patch or did not appear to sense the patch. The authors also refer to a video to demonstrate the two clusters by rotating the 3-dimension scatter plot. However, the supposed clusters, if any, are difficult to see in a 3D (Video 5) or in a 2D scatter plot (Figure 3I). The authors need to clearly demonstrate the distinct clustering as claimed in the paper as this feature is fundamental and necessary for the model implementation and interpretation of results.

      We are grateful to the reviewer for pointing out the difficulty in visualizing the clusters. We added additional visualizations and methods to validate the clusters we have discovered as described in our above response to Reviewer #3 under Validation of sensing clusters.

      When selecting parameters (covariates) for their model, it is critical to avoid overfitting. Therefore, the authors used AIC and BIC (Figure 4- supplement 1) to demonstrate that the full GLM model has a better model performance than the other models which contain only a subset of the full covariates (in a total of 5). However, the authors compare the full set with only 4 other models whereas the total number of models that need to be compared with is 2^5-2. The authors at least need to include the AIC and BIC scores of all possible models in order to draw the conclusion about the performance of the full model.

      Model selection criterion: We thank the reviewer for pointing out this gap in our methodology. We have now run the model with all combinations of subsets of model parameters and have confirmed that the model with all 5 covariates outperforms all other models even when using BIC, the strictest criterion for overfitting (Figure 1 - supplement 1A). The only other model that performs well (though not as often as the 5-term model) is the 4-term model lacking ρ<sub>h</sub>. This result is not surprising as ρ<sub>h</sub> only changes substantially once in an animal’s encounter history for the single-density, multi-patch data that this model was fit to. For example, for an animal foraging on patches of density 10, on the first encounter ρ<sub>h</sub> = ~200 (see Parameter initialization above), but on every subsequent encounter ρ<sub>h</sub> = ~10. Resultantly, the effect of ρ<sub>h</sub> on the probability of exploiting is somewhat binary on the single-density, multi-patch data set. Nevertheless, we see significantly improved prediction of behavior in the novel multi-density, multi-patch data (Figure 4F) as we observe an effect of the most recently encountered patch. Additionally, we observe a similar impact (i.e., significant coefficient of negative sign) of the ρ<sub>h</sub> term when the model is fit to the multi-density, multi-patch data set (Figure 4 - supplement 4D).

      In any bacterial patch, the edges have a higher density of bacteria than the patch center. Thus, it is possible that a worm scans the patch edge density, on the basis of which it decides to accept or reject the patch whose average density is smaller. This could potentially cause an underestimate of the bacteria density used in the model. Furthermore, the potential inhomogeneity of the patch may further complicate the worm's decision-making, and the discrepancy between the reality and the model assumption will reduce the validity of the model. The authors need to estimate the inhomogeneity of the bacterial patches used in their assays and discuss how the edge effects may affect their results and conclusions.

      Bacterial patch inhomogeneity: We extensively tested the landscape of the bacterial patches by imaging fluorescently-labeled bacteria OP50-GFP (Bacterial Patch Density in Methods; Figure 2 - supplement 1-3). As the reviewer mentions, we observe significantly greater bacterial density at the patch edge. This within-patch spatial inhomogeneity results from areas of active proliferation of bacteria and likely complicates an animal’s ability to accurately assess the quantity of bacteria within a patch and, consequently, our ability to accurately compute a metric related to our assumptions of what the animal is sensing. In our study, we used the relative density of the patch edge where bacterial density is highest as a proxy for an animal’s assessment of bacterial patch density (Figure 2 – supplement 1). This decision was based on a previous finding that the time spent on the edge of a bacterial patch affected the dynamics of subsequent area-restricted search. While within-patch spatial inhomogeneity likely affects an animal’s ability to assess patch density, we do not believe that this qualitatively affects the results of our study. Both the patch densities tested (Figure 2 – supplement 3A) as well as our observations of time-dependent changes in exploitation (Figure 2E,N-O; Figure 3H-I) maintained a monotonic relationship. Therefore, alternative methods of patch density estimation should yield similar results. We have added additional discussion on this topic to our manuscript (lines 578-593).

      The authors claim that their methods (GMM and semi-supervised QDA) are unbiased. This seems unlikely as the QDA involves supervision. The authors need to provide additional explanation on this point.

      Semi-supervised QDA labelling: We have removed the term “unbiased” to avoid any misinterpretation of the methodology and clarified our method of labelling used for “supervising” QDA. Specifically, we made two simple assumptions: 1) animals must have sensed the patch if they exploited it and 2) animals must not have sensed the patch if there was no bacteria to sense. Thus, we labeled encounters as sensing if they were found to be exploitatory as we assume that sensation is prerequisite to exploitation; and we labeled encounters as non-sensing for events where animals encountered patches lacking bacteria (OD<sub>600</sub> = 0). All other points were non-labeled prior to learning the model. In this way, our labels were based on the experimental design and results of the GMM, an unsupervised method; rather than any expectations we had about what sensing should look like. The semi-supervised QDA method then used these initial labels to iteratively fit a paraboloid that best separated these clusters, by minimizing the posterior variance of classification (lines 1012-1021). See Figure 2 - supplement 8A-B for a visualization showing the labelled data.

      Based on the authors' result, worms behaviorally exhibit their preferences toward food abundance (density), which results in a preference scale for a range of densities. Does this scale vary with the worms' initial cultivation states? The author partially verified that by observing starved worms. This hypothesis could be better tested if the authors could analyze the decision-making of the worms that were initially cultivated with different densities of bacterial food.

      While we agree with the reviewer that testing the effects of varying bacterial density during animal development (cultivation) is a very interesting experiment, it is not feasible at this time. We focused our revised manuscript to include only assertions about the effects of recent experiences and added this inquiry as a future direction as described above in our response to Reviewer #1 under Cultivation density.

      It would be helpful to elaborate more on how the framework developed in this paper can be applied more broadly to other behaviors and/or organisms and how it may influence our understanding of decision-making across species.

      We thank the reviewer for alerting us to this gap in our discussion. We have added additional commentary about our model and its utility to the discussion section (lines 667-695).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Sensing vs. non-sensing

      Perhaps a more ethologically accurate term to describe this behavior would be "ignoring" rather than "not sensing". If the authors feel strongly about using the term "not sensing", then they should provide experimental evidence supporting this claim. However, I think simply changing the terminology negates these experiments.

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful comments. While we agree with the reviewer that the term “non-sensing” may not be ethologically accurate (see response to Public Review above under Interpretation of “non-sensing” encounters), we interpret the term “ignoring” to mean that the animal sensed the patches but decided not to react. We have chosen to replace the term “non-sensing” with “non-responding” to best indicate the ethological interpretation of our observation. Nonetheless, we believe that it remains possible that animals are truly not sensing the bacterial patches as our method of classification compared the behavior against encounters with patches lacking bacteria (as described above in response to Reviewer #2 under Semi-supervised QDA labelling).

      History-dependence of the GLM

      Perhaps a simpler approach would be to say the worm senses everything, and this accumulative memory affects the decision to exploit. For example, the animal essentially experiences two feeding states: feeding on patches, and starvation off of patches.

      The level of satiety could be modeled linearly:

      Satiety(t_enter:t_leave) = k_feed*patch_density*delta_t

      Where k_feed is some model parameter for rate of satiety signal accumulation, t_enter is the time the animal entered the patch, t_leave is the time the animal left the patch, and delta_t is the difference between the two. Perhaps you could add a saturation limit to this, but given your data, I doubt that is the case.

      Starvation could be modeled as simply a decay from the last satiety signal:

      Starvation(t_leave:t_enter) = Satiety(t_leave)*exp(-k_starve*delta_t).

      Where starvation is the rate constant for the decay of the satiety signal.

      For the logistic model, the logistic parameter is simply the difference between the current patch density and the current satiety signal.

      A nice thing about this approach is that it negates the need to categorize your patches. All patch encounters matter. Brief patch encounters (categorized as non-sensing and not used in the prior GLM) naturally produce a very small satiety signal and contribute very little to the exploit decision. Another nice thing about this approach is that it gives you memory timescales, that are testable. There is a rate of satiety accumulation and a rate of satiety loss. You should be able to predict behavior with lower patch density, assuming the rate constants hold. (I am not advocating you do more experiments here, just pointing out a nice feature of this approach).

      You could possibly apply this to a GLM for velocity on a non-exploited patch as well, though I assume this would be a linear GLM, given the velocity distributions you provided.

      We thank the reviewer for their time and thoughtfulness in thinking about our model. The reviewer’s proposed model seems entirely reasonable and could aid in elucidating the time component of how prior experience affects decision-making. However, we decided to keep our paper focused on using a minimal model to answer a set of core questions (e.g., Does encounter history or satiety influence decision-making?) (see above under Model design for a more detailed response). Future studies investigating the mechanisms of these foraging decisions should open the door for more mechanistically accurate models. We have expanded our discussion of the model to include this assertion (lines 667-695).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Sample size: If the sample size of the study is increased, more confidence and new insights can be inferred about myometrial enhancer-mediated gene regulation in term pregnancy. Such a small sample size (N = 3) limits the statistical power of the study. As mentioned in the manuscript they failed to identify chromatin loops in the second subject's biopsy is observed due to a limited sample.

      We agree with the reviewer’s comment about the sample size. We sincerely hope the result of this study would increase the interest of stakeholders to fund future projects in a larger scale.

      (2) Figure quality: There is a lack of good representations of the results (e.g., screenshots of tables as figure panels!) as well as missing interpretations that might add value to the manuscript.

      Figure 1B and 2B have been converted to the pie chart format.

      (3) Definition of super-enhancer: The definition of super-enhancer is not clear. Also, the computational merging of enhancers to define super-enhancers should be described better.

      Added more details about tool and parameter setting in the Method section of “Identification of super enhancers”:

      “Identification of super enhancers

      H3K27ac-positive enhancers were defined as regions of H3K27ac ChIP-seq peaks in each sample. The enhancers within 12.5Kb were merged by using bedtools merge function with parameter “-d 12500”. The combined enhancer regions were called super enhancers if they were larger than 15Kb. The common super enhancers from multiple samples were used for downstream analysis.”

      Reference:

      Whyte WA, Orlando DA, Hnisz D, Abraham BJ, Lin CY, Kagey MH, Rahl PB, Lee TI, Young RA. Master transcription factors and mediator establish super-enhancers at key cell identity genes. Cell. 2013 Apr 11;153(2):307-19. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.03.035. PMID: 23582322; PMCID: PMC3653129.

      (4) Assay-Specific Limitations: Each assay employed in the study, such as ChIP-Seq and CRISPRa-based Perturb-Seq, has its limitations, including potential biases, sensitivity issues, and technical challenges, which could impact the accuracy and reliability of the results. These limitations should be addressed properly to avoid false-positive results and improve the interpretability of the results.

      The major limitations of the CRISPRa-based Perturb-Seq protocol in this study are the use of the hTERT-HM cells and the two-vector system for transduction. While hTERT-HM cells are a much easier platform in terms of technical operation, primary human myometrial cells are generally considered retaining a molecular context that is closer to the in vivo tissues. Due to the limitation on the efficiency of having two vectors simultaneously present in the same cell, hTERT-HM cells are much more affordable and operationally feasible to conduct the experiment. Future advancements on the increase of viral vector payload capacity may overcome this challenge and open the venue to perform the assay on primary human myometrial cells.

      (5) Sample collection and comparison: There is mention of matched gravid term and non-gravid samples whereas no description or use of control samples was found in the results. Also, the comparison of non-labor samples with labor samples would provide a better understanding of epigenomic and transcriptomic events of myometrium leading to laboring events.

      The description has been updated:

      “Collection of myometrial specimens

      Permission to collect human tissue specimens was prospectively obtained from individuals undergoing hysterectomy or cesarean section for benign clinical indications (H-33461). Gravid myometrial tissue was obtained from the margin of the hysterotomy in women undergoing term cesarean sections (>38 weeks estimated gestational age) without evidence of labor. Non-gravid myometrial tissue was collected from pre-menopausal women undergoing hysterectomy for benign conditions. Specimens from gravid women receiving treatment for pre-eclampsia, eclampsia, pregnancy-related hypertension, or pre-term labor were excluded.”

      (6) Lack of clarity:

      (6a) It is written as 'Chromatin Conformation Capture (Hi-C)'. I think Hi-C is Histone Capture and 3C is Chromosome Conformation Capture! This needs clear writing.

      As the reviewer suggested, to make it clear, we have changed the text “A high throughput chromatin conformation capture (Hi-C) assay” to “A High-throughput Chromosome Conformation Capture (Hi-C) assay”.

      (6b) In multiple places, 'PLCL2' gene is written as 'PCLC2'.

      Corrected as suggested.

      (6c) What is the biological relevance of considering 'active' genes with FPKM {greater than or equal to} 1? This needs clarification.

      In RNA-seq analysis, the gene expression levels are often quantified using FPKM (Fragments Per Kilobase of transcript per Million mapped reads). Setting a threshold of FPKM for defining "active" genes in RNA-seq analysis is biologically relevant, because it helps to distinguish between genuinely expressed genes and background noise. It helps researchers focus on genes, which are more likely to have a significant biological impact. A common threshold for defining "active" genes is FPKM ≥ 1. Genes with FPKM values below this threshold may be transcribed at very low levels or could be background noise.

      (6d) The understanding of differentially methylated genes at promoters is underrated as per the authors. But, why leaving DNA methylation apart, they selected histone modification as the basis of epigenetic reprogramming in terms of myometrium is unclear.

      DNA methylation indeed plays a crucial role in evaluating the impact of cis-acting elements on gene regulation. Large-scale studies, such as the comprehensive analysis of the myometrial methylome landscape in human biopsies (Paul et al., JCI Insight, 2022, PMID: 36066972), have provided valuable insights. When integrated with histone modification and chromatin looping data, contributed by our group and collaborators, future secondary analyses leveraging machine learning are poised to further elucidate the mechanisms underlying myometrial transcriptional regulation.

      (6e) How does the identification of PGR as an upstream regulator of PLCL2 gene expression in human myometrial cells contribute to our understanding of progesterone signaling in myometrial function?

      In a previous study, we demonstrated a positive correlation between PLCL2 and PGR expression in a mouse model and identified PLCL2's role in negatively modulating oxytocin-induced myometrial cell contraction (Peavy et al., PNAS, 2021, PMID: 33707208). The present study builds on this by providing evidence for a direct regulatory mechanism in which PGR influences PLCL2 transcription, likely through a cis-acting element located 35 kb upstream. These findings suggest that PLCL2 acts as a mediator of PGR-dependent myometrial quiescence prior to labor, rather than merely participating in a parallel pathway. Further in vivo studies are necessary to delineate the extent to which PLCL2 mediates PGR activity, particularly the contraction-dampening function of the PGR-B isoform.

      (7) Grammatical error: The manuscript has numerous grammatical errors. Please correct them.

      Corrections have been made as suggested.

      (8) Use of single-cell data: Though from the Methods section, it can be understood that single-cell RNA-seq was done to identify CRISPRa gRNA expressing cells to characterize the effect of gene activation, some results from single-cell data e.g., cell clustering, cell types, gRNA expression across clusters could be added for better elucidation.

      As reviewer suggested, we have prepared a file “PerturbSeq_summary.xlsx” (Dataset S9) to provide additional results of perturb-seq data analysis. It includes 2 spreadsheets, “Cell_per_gRNA” for clustering and “Protospacer_calls_per_cell” for gRNA expression across clusters.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The following are a number of grammatical issues in the abstract. I suggest having a careful read of the entire manuscript to identify additional grammatical issues as I may not be able to highlight all of these issues.

      (1a) "The myometrium plays a critical component during pregnancy." change component to role.

      (1b) "It is responsible for the uterus' structural integrity and force generation at term," à replace "," with "."

      (1c) Also, I suggest rephrasing the first 2 sentences to: The myometrium plays a critical role during pregnancy as it is responsible for both the structural integrity of the uterus and force generation at term.

      (1d) "Here we investigated the human term pregnant nonlabor myometrial biopsies for transcriptome, enhancer histone mark cistrome, and chromatin conformation pattern mapping." Remove "the", and modify to "Here we investigated human term pregnant".

      (1e) Missing period and sentence fragment, "PGR overexpression facilitated PLCL2 gene expression in myometrial cells Using CRISPR activation the functionality of a PGR putative enhancer 35-kilobases upstream of the contractile-restrictive gene PLCL2.

      Corrections have been made as suggested.

      (2) Sentence fragment: Studies on the role of steroid hormone receptors in myometrial remodeling have provided evidence that the withdrawal of functional progesterone signaling at term is due to a stoichiometric increase of progesterone receptor (PGR) A to B isoform-related estrogen receptor (ESR) alpha expression activation at term. (Mesiano, Chan et al. 2002) (Merlino, Welsh et al. 2007) (Nadeem, Shynlova et al. 2016).

      The statement has been updated:

      “Studies on the role of steroid hormone receptors in myometrial remodeling suggest that the withdrawal of functional progesterone signaling at term results from a stoichiometric shift favoring the PGR-A isoform over PGR-B. This shift is associated with increased activation of estrogen receptor alpha (ESR1) expression at term (Mesiano, Chan et al. 2002) (Merlino, Welsh et al. 2007) (Nadeem, Shynlova et al. 2016).”

      (3) FOS:JUN heterodimers are implicated to be critical for the initiation of labor through transcriptional regulation of gap junction proteins such as Cx43 (Nadeem, Farine et al. 2018) (Balducci, Risek et al. 1993).

      Use Gja1 (Gap junction alpha 1) as the current correct gene, not Cx43.

      Also, several references predate Nadeem, Farine et al. 2018 and are more appropriate to use as references for the role of Ap-1 proteins in regulating Gja1; PMID: 15618352 and PMID: 12064606 were the first to show this relationship in myometrial cells.

      The statement has been updated as suggested:

      “FOS:JUN heterodimers are implicated to be critical for the initiation of labor through transcriptional regulation of gap junction proteins such as GJA1 (Nadeem, Farine et al. 2018) (Balducci, Risek et al. 1993)”

      (4) Define PLCL2 on first use.

      Updated as suggested.

      (5) There are a number of issues with this section, "Matched sSpecimens of gravid myometrium were collected at the margin of hysterotomy from women undergoing clinically indicated cesarean section at term (>38 weeks estimated gestation age) without evidence of labor. Specimens of healthy, non-gravid myometrium were also pecimens were collected from uteri removed from pre-menopausal women undergoing hysterectomy for benign clinical indications."

      The description has been updated:

      “Collection of myometrial specimens

      Permission to collect human tissue specimens was prospectively obtained from individuals undergoing hysterectomy or cesarean section for benign clinical indications (H-33461). Gravid myometrial tissue was obtained from the margin of the hysterotomy in women undergoing term cesarean sections (>38 weeks estimated gestational age) without evidence of labor. Non-gravid myometrial tissue was collected from pre-menopausal women undergoing hysterectomy for benign conditions. Specimens from gravid women receiving treatment for pre-eclampsia, eclampsia, pregnancy-related hypertension, or pre-term labor were excluded.”

      (6) Enriched motifs were identified by HOMER (Hypergeometric Optimization of Motif EnRichment) v4.11 (Heinz, Benner et al. 2010).

      Please clarify what background is used for motif enrichment.

      We used the default background sequences generated by HOMER from a set of random genomic sequences matching the input sequences in terms of basic properties, such as GC content and length. We have added more details in the Method section:

      “DNA-binding factor motif enrichment analysis

      Enriched motifs were identified by HOMER (Hypergeometric Optimization of Motif EnRichment) v4.11 with default background sequences matching the input sequences (Heinz, Benner et al. 2010).”

      (7) "Six of the seven regions are also co-localized with previously published genome occupancy of transcription regulators curated by the ReMap Atlas"

      Please clarify if this Atlas includes myometrial tissues or not and clarify the cell types included in the atlas.

      According to the UCSC Genome Browser and the reference by Hammal et al. (2022), the current ReMap database includes PGR ChIP-seq data from human myometrial biopsies, available under NCBI GEO accession number GSE137550, alongside data from various other cell and tissue types. ReMap provides valuable insights into potential functional cis-acting elements in the genome from a systems biology perspective. However, tissue specificity requires independent validation.

      (8) "Notably, 76% of the putative super-enhancers are co-localized with known PGR-occupied regions in the human myometrial tissue (Figure S2). This is significantly higher than the 20% co-localization in the regular enhancer group (Figure S2)."

      Because there is a huge difference in the size of the putative super enhancer regions and the isolated enhancers this comparison is not appropriate as conducted. The comparison needs to account for the difference in size of the regions. Please provide P values for significance statements.

      We acknowledge the reviewer's concern that our initial statement was overstated and potentially misleading, given the substantial difference in size between putative super-enhancer regions and regular enhancers. Rather than emphasizing the enrichment, it would be more accurate to simply describe our observation that super-enhancers encompass more PGR-occupied regions.

      Here is the updated version:

      “Notably, 76% of the putative super-enhancers co-localize with known PGR-occupied regions in human myometrial tissue, compared to 20% co-localization observed in regular enhancers (Figure S2).”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Title is extremely misleading, as here we do not get a view of the epigenomic landscape, but rather sparce data related to H3K27ac and H3K4me (focusing on enhancers) and chromatin conformation associated with the PLCL2 transcription start site (TSS).

      As suggested, the title is modified to “Assessment of the Histone Mark-based Epigenomic Landscape in Human Myometrium at Term Pregnancy”.

      (2) Improve the first result paragraph by providing a clear rationale for the experiments and their objectives, as well as introducing the samples used. Rather than simply listing approaches and end results in Table 1, offer concise explanations for the experiments alongside the supporting data presented in detailed figures. Using appropriate figures/graphs to effectively contextualize these datasets would be greatly appreciated by readers and would add more value to this research. Currently, it is difficult for us to assess and appreciate the quality of the data.

      The following statement is included in the beginning of the Result section:

      "To better understand the regulatory network shaping the myometrial transcriptome before labor, we analyzed transcriptome and putative enhancers in individual human myometrial specimens. Using RNA-seq, we identified actively expressed RNAs, while ChIP-seq for H3K27ac and H3K4me1 was used to map putative enhancers. Active genes were associated with nearby putative enhancers based on their genomic proximity. Additionally, chromatin looping patterns were mapped using Hi-C to further link active genes and putative enhancers within the same chromatin loops."

      (3) The statistics for every sequencing approach need to be provided for each sample (e.g., RNA-seq: number of total reads, number of mapped reads, % of mapped reads; ChIP-Seq: number of mapped reads, % of mapped reads, % of duplicates).

      We have generated the summary table of each dataset included in this study (Dataset S7) [NGS-summary.xls].

      (4) Figure S1: The rationale behind comparing the Dotts study and yours regarding H3K27ac-positive regions needs to be better defined. Why is this performed if the data will not be used afterwards? What are the conserved regions associated with vs the ones that are variable? Is this biologically relevant? Why not use only the regions conserved between the 6 samples, to have more robust conclusions?

      The purpose of comparing our data with the Dotts dataset is to highlight the degree of variation across studies. In this study, we focused on addressing specific biological questions using our own dataset rather than developing methodologies for meta-analysis. Future advancements in meta-analysis techniques could leverage the combined power of multiple datasets to provide deeper insights.

      (5) Perhaps due to a lack of details, I am unable to ascertain how the putative myometrial enhancers were defined. In Dataset S1, it is stated, "we define the regions that have overlapping H3K27ac and H3K4me1 marks as putative myometrial enhancers at the term pregnant nonlabor stage (Dataset S1)". Within Dataset S1, for subjects 1, 2, and 3, H3K27ac and H3K4me1 double-positive enhancers are shown in term pregnant, non-labor human myometrial specimens, with approximately 100 regions corresponding to 131 (sample 1), 127 (sample 2), and 140 (sample 3) common peaks. However, in Figure 1a, reference is made to the 13114 putative enhancers commonly present across the three specimens. Is Dataset S1 intended to represent only a small fraction of the 13114 putative enhancers? Detailed analyses need to be conducted and better showcased.

      Dataset S1 has been updated to list all 13,114 putative enhancers.

      (6) For the gene expression analyses of RNA-seq data, FPKM values were utilized. However, it is unclear why the gene expression count matrix was normalized based on the ratio of total mapped read pairs in each sample to 56.5 million for the term myometrial specimens. I would recommend exercising caution regarding the use of FPKM expression units, as samples are normalized only within themselves, lacking cross-sample normalization. Consequently, due to external factors unaccounted for by this normalization method, a value of 10 in one sample may not equate to 10 in another.

      We value the reviewer’s input. This question will be addressed in future secondary data analyses with suitable methodologies, as it is beyond the scope of this study.

      (7) In Figure 1b, the authors have categorized their 12157 active genes into 3 bins based on FPKM values: >5 FPKM >1, >15 FPKM >5, and >15 FPKM. However, in the text, they describe these as 'actively high-expressing genes (FPKM >= 15)'. I would advise caution regarding the interpretation of these values, as an FPKM of 15 is not typically associated with highly expressed genes. According to literature and resources such as the Expression Atlas, an FPKM of 15 is generally considered to represent a low to medium expression level.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback. This question will be revisited during secondary data analyses using appropriate methodologies, as it falls outside the scope of the present study.

      To increase readability and clarity, we modified the sentence as following: More than 40% of the 540 putative super enhancers are located within a 100-kilobase distance to high-expressing genes (FPKM >= 15), while only 7.3% of putative myometrial super enhancers are found near low-expressing genes (5 > FPKM >=1) (Figure 2B).

      (8) Out of the 12157 active genes, approximately two-thirds have an FPKM >15. Was this expected? How does this correspond to what is observed in the literature, particularly in other similar studies (https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30988671/ ; https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/35260533/ ) .

      This is indeed an intriguing question that merits further exploration in future secondary analyses.

      (9) It is also surprising to see that for the motif enrichment analysis (Fig. 1C), the P-values are small. This is probably because the percentage of target sequences with the motif is very similar to the percentage of background sequences with the motif. For instance, for selected genes in Figure 1C: AP-1 (50.68% vs. 46.50%), STAT5 (28.08% vs. 25.04%), PGR (17.90% vs. 16.12%), etc. Can one really say that you have a biologically relevant enrichment for values that are so close between target sequences and background sequences?

      Reviewer’s comment is noted. Biological relevance shall be experimentally examined though wet-lab assays in future studies.

      (10) For Figure 2, again not convinced that FPKM >= 15 can be used to say: Compared with the regular putative enhancers, the putative myometrial super-enhancers are found more frequently near active genes that are expressed at relatively higher levels (Figure 1B and Figure 2B). A higher threshold should be used if they want to say this.

      To compare the association of putative enhancers with active genes expressed at different levels, we categorized the active genes into three groups based on their FPKM (Fragments Per Kilobase of transcript per Million mapped reads) values. These groups are defined as follows: the top third active genes (FPKM ≥ 15), the middle third active genes (5 ≤ FPKM < 15), and the bottom third active genes (1 ≤ FPKM < 5). By "active genes expressed at relatively higher levels," we refer specifically to the top third active genes with FPKM values of 15 or higher, indicating their relatively higher expression levels compared to the other groups of active genes.

      (11) More detailed explanations and methods are needed regarding how the data for Figure S2 was obtained.

      The following details were added to the methods section:

      “Colocalization of super enhancers and PGR genome occupancy was compared by calling peaks from previously published PGR ChIP-seq data (GSM4081683 and GSM4081684). The percentages of enhancers and super enhancers that manifest PGR occupancy were calculated by overlapping the genomic regions in each category with PGR occupancy regions.”

      (12) In Figure 2C, there is no information provided on the genes used to obtain the results. It would be helpful to include examples of these genes, along with their expression values, for instance.

      The expression levels of the 346 active genes that are associated with myometrial super enhancers are included in Dataset S4, along with results of the updated gene ontology enrichment analysis using the Database for Annotation, Visualization, and Integrated Discovery (DAVID) of Knowledgebase v2024q4. Selected pathways of interest are listed in updated Figure 2C.

      (13) The linking of PLCL2-related data to the first part of the story is lacking, and the rationale behind it is missing. This entire section should be more detailed, and the data should be expanded to better reflect the context.

      As suggested, we included the following statement at the beginning of the section “Cis-acting elements for the control of the contractile gene PLCL2”:

      “We previously demonstrated the positive correlation of PLCL2 and PGR expression in a mouse model and PLCL2’s function on negatively modulating oxytocin-induced myometrial cell contraction (Peavy et al., 2021). However, the mechanism underlies the PGR regulation of PLCL2 remains unclear. Taking advantage of the mapped myometrial cis-acting elements, we aimed to identify the cis-acting elements that may contribute to the PLCL2 transcriptional regulation with a special interest on the PGR-related enhancers.”

      The context is that our results provide additional evidence to support a direct regulation mechanism of PGR on the PLCL2 transcription, likely though the 35-kb upstream cis-acting element. This finding suggests that PLCL2 likely plays a mediator’s role of PGR dependent myometrial quiescence before laboring rather than a mere passenger on a parallel pathway. Further studies using in vivo models are needed to determine the extent of PLCL2 in mediating PGR, especially PGR-B isoform’s contraction-dampening function.

      (14) The entire Hi-C data should be presented to allow for the assessment of its quality and further value.

      The revised manuscript has included the Hi-C quality control summary in Dataset S8 [HiC-QC-Summary.xlsx].

      (15) The authors state: "For the purpose of functional screening, we focus on H3K27ac signals instead of using H3K27ac/H3K4me1 double positive criterium to cast a wider net." However, it is unclear how many of the targeted regions contained H3K27ac/H3K4me1 peaks. Were enhancers or super-enhancers targeted, and if so, how did they compare to H3K27ac sites?

      The numbers of H3K27ac/H3K4me1 double positive peaks are recorded in Figure 1A. Compared to the numbers of H3K27ac intervals (Table 1), the H3K27ac/H3K4me1 double positive peaks are 62.9%, 70.7%, and 61.2% of corresponding H3K27ac intervals in each individual specimen.

      (16) For the first set of data (Table 1), the authors state, "Together, these results reveal an epigenomic landscape in the human term pregnant myometrial tissue before the onset of labor, which we use as a resource to investigate the molecular mechanisms that prepare the myometrium for subsequent parturition." While it is acknowledged that an epigenetic landscape exists in all tissues, there is a lack of clarity regarding this landscape in the current manuscript, as we are only presented with a table containing numbers.

      This sentence has been revised to: “Together, these results delineate a map of H3K27ac and H3K4me1 positive signals in the human term pregnant myometrial tissue before the onset of labor, which we use as a resource to investigate the molecular mechanisms that prepare the myometrium for subsequent parturition.”

      (17) For S1, the authors conclude: These data together highlight the degree of variation in mapping the epigenome among specimens and datasets. This conclusion seems somewhat perplexing, and I find myself in partial disagreement. Firstly, providing a clear rationale for this section would strengthen the conclusions. It's important to consider what factors may contribute to this variability. It could simply be attributed to differences in experimental settings, such as variations in samples, protocols used, antibodies, sequencing departments, or overall data quality. Deeper analyses of the data could have provided more information.

      We agree with the reviewer that deeper analyses are needed in order to extract more information among studies. However, appropriate methods for meta-analyses should be carefully evaluated and employed for this purpose. We humbly believe that such a task should belong to future studies that may combine available datasets for secondary analyses, leveraging the collective contribution of the reproductive biology community.

      (18) In the methods section, please include an explanation of how enhancers and super-enhancers were defined or add appropriate citations for reference.

      Added more details about tool and parameter setting in the Method section of “Identification of super enhancers”.

      “Identification of super enhancers

      H3K27ac-positive enhancers were defined as regions of H3K27ac ChIP-seq peaks in each sample. The enhancers within 12.5Kb were merged by using bedtools merge function with parameter “-d 12500”. The combined enhancer regions were called super enhancers if they were larger than 15Kb. The common super enhancers from multiple samples were used for downstream analysis.”

      Reference:

      Whyte WA, Orlando DA, Hnisz D, Abraham BJ, Lin CY, Kagey MH, Rahl PB, Lee TI, Young RA. Master transcription factors and mediator establish super-enhancers at key cell identity genes. Cell. 2013 Apr 11;153(2):307-19. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2013.03.035. PMID: 23582322; PMCID: PMC3653129.

      (19) Additional description on the "Inferred myometrial PGR activities and the correlation analysis "method section should be included to enhance clarity and understanding.

      The description has been updated:

      “The inferred PGR activities were represented by the T-score, which was derived by inputting the mouse myometrial Pgr gene signature, based on the differentially expressed genes between control and myometrial Pgr knockout groups at mid-pregnancy (Wu, Wang et al., 2022), into the SEMIPs application (Li, Bushel et al., 2021). The T-scores were computed using this signature alongside the normalized gene expression counts (FPKM) from 43 human myometrial biopsy specimens.”

      (20) How was the qPCR analysis performed? Was the ddCT method utilized, and was a reference gene used for control? Additional information would be beneficial.

      Quantifying relative mRNA levels was performed via the standard curve method.

      The following details were added: “Relative levels of genes of interest were normalized to the 18S rRNA.”

      (21) Regarding the RNA-Seq analysis of Provera-treated human Myometrial Specimens, the continued use of FPKM is not ideal due to potential differences in RNA composition between libraries. Additionally, clarification is needed on why Cufflinks 2.0.2 was used, considering it is no longer supported.

      FPKM (Fragments Per Kilobase of transcript per Million mapped reads) is used in RNA-Seq analysis, because it allows for the normalization of gene expression data, accounting for differences in gene length and sequencing depth, and facilitates comparability across different genes and libraries. This makes it one of the essential tools for accurately measuring and comparing gene expression levels in various biological and clinical research contexts.

      CuffLinks was once a popular tool for analyzing RNA-seq data, transcriptome assembly, and DEG identification. Its usage has declined in recent years due to the emergence of newer and more advanced tools. The main reason is that it was used for RNA-seq analysis at early stage of this study a few years ago. For the purpose of comparison and consistency, we continued using this tool for later RNA-seq analysis. If we start a new project now, we will choose newer tools, such as HISAT2, Salmon, and DEseq2.

      (22) Overall, sentence structure and typos need to be corrected across the text. Here are some examples:

      Line 17: at term, emerging studies.

      Line 20-22: Here we investigated the human term pregnant nonlabor myometrial biopsies for transcriptome, enhancer histone mark cistrome, and chromatin conformation pattern mapping.

      Line 30-32: PGR overexpression facilitated PLCL2 gene expression in myometrial cells Using CRISPR activation the functionality of a PGR putative enhancer 35-kilobases upstream of the contractile-restrictive gene PLCL2.

      Line 66-70: However, the role of differential myometrial DNA methylation at contractility-driving gene promoter CpG islands in preterm birth is not thought to be major (Mitsuya, Singh et al. 2014), but given that DNA methylation-mediated gene regulation often occurs outside of CpG islands (Irizarry, Ladd-Acosta et al. 2009), there is still work to be done at this interface.

      Line 80-83: Putative enhancers upstream of the PLCL2, a gene encoding for the protein PLCL2 which has been implicated in the modulation of calcium signaling (Uji, Matsuda et al. 2002) and maintenance of myometrial quiescence (Peavey, Wu et al. 2021), transcriptional start site were subject to functional assessment using CRISPR activation based assays.

      Line 290 : sSpecimens

      We appreciate the reviewer’s kind efforts and have made changes accordingly.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major comments

      (1) The section on page 20 describing the proteomic analysis of EVs is poorly written and confusing, with a lot of data in the supplement. It is not clear what the proteomics data actually means.

      We appreciate your feedback on the clarity of the proteomic analysis section. We have rewritten the section on page 20 with more detained information to provide a clearer explanation of the proteomics data and its biological significance. Additionally, we have incorporated a comparative analysis of the EV and total cell lysate proteomes (Fig. 8E, Supplementary Fig. S7A, Supplementary Tables 3 and 4) for supplemental data interpretation.

      (2) The order of the data could be improved.

      We appreciate your feedback regarding the data organization. We have reorganized the order and position of some data in a more structured and coherent manner, as suggested by the reviewers.

      - Reorganization of the qPCR data (previously Fig. 1C) as Fig. 3A

      - Removal of the data on the growth analysis on raffinose media (previously Fig. 7H).

      -Reorganization of the spotting data of the double mutant (previously Fig 3B) to Supplementary Fig. S3B

      - Reorganization of the subcellular localization data (previously Fig 3E) to Supplementary Fig. S4A

      (3) The discussion is repetitive with the introduction and merely summarizes the results and speculates on the mechanism of how the absence of UGGT, leading to ERQC defects, results in defective EV biogenesis/cargo loading in C. neoformans.

      We removed several repetitive sentences in the discussion and provided additional information on proteome analysis.

      Other questions and comments

      (1) Instead of comprehensively analyzing EVs from the UGG1 mutant, a more informative approach to better understanding how defects in N-linked glycosylation impact secretion, would be to do a proteomic analysis on the total secretions (including beta glucanase-treated cells to release classically secreted proteins from the cell wall) and EVs.

      We agree that a comprehensive proteomic analysis of total secretions and classically secreted proteins would provide deeper insights into how defects in N-glycosylation impact secretion in C. neoformans. To address this concern, we performed an additional set of proteomic analyses, the proteome profiles of total cell lysates and the secretome of C. neoformans cultivated in SD broth and presented the results as Supplementary Table S5 and Supplementary Fig. S7B. These additional analyses provide further insights into the impact of UGG1 deletion on both conventional and unconventional secretion pathways, supporting a more pronounced effect of the UGG1 defect on EV-mediated trafficking. The discussion has been updated accordingly (Page 22, lines 509-514).

      (2) The melanization defect in Ugg1 mutant is not strong. Could the reduction be due to partially compromised Ugg1 mutant growth at 30{degree sign}C as indicated in the spot tests. Were photos of the spot dilution assays taken at 1 and 2 days to investigate slower growth? Or alternatively were growth curves taken in a liquid culture?

      For accuracy of melanin synthesis defect, in addition to analysis on L-DOPA plates, we had assessed melanin production in liquid L-DOPA medium following a 3-day incubation, and the melanin production in liquid media was normalized by cell density (OD<sub>600</sub>). The data on normalized melanin production is now included as Fig. 4B in the revised manuscript. The defective laccase activity in the _ugg1_Δ mutant (Fig. 7C) further corroborates our melanization assay results, which is additionally mentioned in the text (Page 18, lines 393-395).

      (3) Is it accurate to say that some virulence factors (i.e. melanin, capsule and phosphatases) are predominantly trafficked through EV's in C. neoformans? Have studies been done to determine the proportion of virulence factors trafficked via EV's versus traditional secretion?

      We thank you for the thoughtful comments. Some virulence factors, such as urease, melanin and capsule polysaccharides, lack a signal peptide required for targeting for the conventional ER/Golgi secretion pathway. It is generally assumed that the trafficking of these factors in C. neoformans is predominantly mediated by non-conventional secretion via EVs. Additionally, even some virulence factors with signal peptides, such as laccase and phosphatases, are also transported via EVs besides the conventional secretion. The quantitative analysis to compare the proportion of virulence factors secretion via EVs versus the conventional pathway has not been yet reported, despite that genetic evidence suggests that conventional secretion also plays a significant role in the export of capsule polysaccharides. Thus, we were also careful not to highlight EV as the main route of virulence factors in the manuscript.

      (4) There is insufficient background in the introduction linking what is known about the ERQC process to secretion in general. The topic changes from the ERQC process to fungal virulence factor, with a primary focus on non-classical (EV-based) secretion. Classical secretion should also be discussed without assuming that non classical (EV) secretion is the major pathway contributing to fungal virulence.

      We appreciate your insightful comments highlighting the need for more background on the ERQC process and its relationship with secretion. To address the reviewer’s concerns, we have added sentences to describe the key roles of ERQC in conventional protein secretion in the Introduction (Page 5, lines 102-106).

      (5) Figure 1A. What does the blue filled circle with the red outline signify? Fig 1 A legend is not well explained. A summary using material provided in the intro/discussion should be included to briefly explain the process and the differences between fungal species. Please also be aware that the intro starts describing the human ERQC process and then switches to what happens in S. cerevisiae.

      We have revised Figure 1A by removing the red circle and updated the figure legend in the revised manuscript to include more detailed information about the ERQC differences across higher eukaryotes and fungal species.

      (6) Figure 2A. There are no units on the Y-axis. Presumably, the scale is the same for all 3 strains.

      Thank you for your comments. The Y-axis is the same for all three strains and, as in Fig. 2C, and represents the relative fluorescence intensity obtained from the HPLC analysis. We added the units on the Y-axis in Fig. 2A.

      (7) If Mnl1 and 2 have proposed roles in proteasomal degradation, wouldn't they be expected to have ER retention signals, like Ugg1?

      We appreciate your valuable insights regarding the absence of ER retention signals in Mnl1 and Mnl2. Previous studies have shown that Saccharomyces cerevisiae Mnl1/Htm1 does not possess canonical KDEL/HDEL-like ER retention signals. Instead, its retention in the ER lumen is facilitated through its interaction with protein disulfide isomerase Pdi1, which contains an HDEL sequence (Gauss et al. 2011). Thus, it is expected that non-canonical retention mechanisms—such as interactions with other ER proteins—could contribute to the retention of Mnl1 and Mnl2 within the ER. We added this information to the revised manuscript (Page 8, lines 154-159).

      (8) Figure 1 C qPCR showing change in mRNA in response to ER stress should not be grouped in this figure. It could be standalone or discussed when the spot dilution assays are performed. Anyway, spots tests are more convincing of a role in stress response than qPCR as the ugg1 mutant is sensitive to tunicamycin, DTT and cell wall stressing agents.

      As suggested by the reviewer, we have reorganized the qPCR data as a part of Figure 3 (Figure 3A) in the revised manuscript.

      (9) It is odd that mns1/101 mutants are not sensitive to ER and CW stress given their proposed differing location/function in the pathway (Figure 1) determined from the N-linked profiling. Any explanation? Could there be redundancy?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s observation regarding the lack of ER and CW stress sensitivity in the mns1_Δ and _mns101_Δ mutants, despite their proposed roles in _N-glycan processing. We had previously reported that the C. neoformans alg3_Δ mutant, lacking a critical enzyme responsible for the synthesis of Dol-PP-Man<sub>6</sub>GlcNAc<sub>2</sub> in the _N-glycosylation pathway, exhibited clearly impaired N-glycan elongation, but showed no detectable growth defects even under stress conditions in vitro. However, alg3_Δ is avirulent in _in vivo pathogenicity (Thak et al., 2020). Similarly, the mns1_Δ_101_Δ double mutant shows glycan-processing defects that do not compromise cellular fitness under stress conditions but result in attenuated virulence in animal models. These findings suggest that some glycosylation-related defects may impact more severely _in vivo pathogenicity rather than in vitro stress sensitivity.

      (10) Although the Silver-stained gels of the ugg1 mutant are not particularly informative, why weren't they (and Con A blots) performed for the other mutants?

      The overall decrease of hypermannosylated glycans observed in the ugg1_Δ mutant allowed us to detect clear alterations in protein glycosylation patterns in the lectin blot using _Galanthus nivalis agglutinin, which recognizes terminal α1,2-, α1,3-, and α1,6-linked mannose residues. In contrast, the limited changes of a few glycan species in other mutants, including mns1_Δ, _mns101_Δ, and _mns1_Δ_101_Δ, are relatively subtle to be detected in the lectin blot, due to only minor differences in the average lengths of their _N-glycans compared to the WT. Therefore, we presented the lectin blotting data only for the _ugg1_Δ mutant.

      (11) If there is ER stress under normal conditions in the Ugg1 mutant then technically this mutant should be growing more slowly under normal conditions. This is difficult to predict in a spot dilution assay where growth is only visualized at day three when any growth defect may have been corrected. The slower growth rather than the reduced secretion of GXM specifically is therefore more likely to be responsible for the reduced virulence.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment regarding the interplay between ER stress, growth defects, and virulence attenuation in the ugg1_Δ mutant. While retarded growth in _C. neoformans is often associated with reduced virulence, there are a few exceptions. For instance, disruptions in cell cycle progression in C. neoformans have been reported to result in larger capsule sizes, which rather enhance in vivo virulence when analyzed in Galleria mellonella infection models (García-Rodas et al., 2014). This highlights that growth defect alone is not sufficient for virulence attenuation. In the case of the _ugg1_Δ mutant, we speculate that the almost complete loss of virulence is attributed not only to its growth retardation but also to its impaired secretion of key virulence factors, including the polysaccharide capsule.

      (12) The rationale for using leucine analogue 5',5',5'-trifluoroleucine (TFL), in a growth assay (Fig. 3C) to determine whether the defective ugg1Δ phenotypes are induced by ER stress caused by misfolded protein accumulation is not explained.

      The leucine analogue 5',5',5'-trifluoroleucine (TFL) can be incorporated into newly synthesized proteins, disrupting normal folding and thus leading to the generation of misfolded proteins (Trotter et al., 2002; Cowie et al., 1959). In the context of a defective ERQC pathway, these misfolded proteins cannot be adequately repaired, resulting in their accumulation and triggering ER stress. Excessive ER stress may ultimately inhibit cell growth in the presence of TFL. This explanation has been incorporated into the revised manuscript (Page 11, lines 236–241).

      (13) I would argue that only the Ugg1 and double Mns mutant were defective in virulence. For the single mutants, it looks like no difference was found relative to WT. The longer median survival of these mutants (if significant) is most likely due to poor infection technique.

      We agree with the reviewer’s opinion that the mns1_Δ and _mns101_Δ single mutants have no significant difference in _in vivo virulence compared to the WT strain, unlike the _mns1_Δ_101_Δ double mutant which showed significant attenuated virulence. We had previously addressed that in the manuscript (Page 13, lines 267-269).

      (14) The authors conclude that the ugg1Δ strain specifically is impaired in extracellular secretion of capsular polysaccharides but is this via classical (SAV1) secretion or EVs?

      In addition to EV-mediated transport, capsular polysaccharide secretion can occur via the Sav1 (Sec4p)-mediated classical secretion pathway. However, our proteome data of total cell lysates indicated that the protein levels of Sav1 were comparable between the WT and _ugg1_Δ strains, suggesting that Sav1p function itself might not be impaired. Given that the _ugg1_Δ mutant exhibits altered vesicular structures (Supplementary Fig. S6) and loss of microvesicles (Fig. 8A), we speculate that a defect might occur at a post-Sav1p step, such as vesicle fusion with the plasma membrane, likely contributing to the complete defect in secretion of capsular polysaccharides in the _ugg1_Δ strain, in which EV biogenesis and defective cargo loading are severely impaired, producing EVs that lack capsular polysaccharides (Figure 8F). However, further studies should be carried out to define the contribution of SAV1 to the secretion of capsular polysaccharides in in the _ugg1_Δ strain.

      (15) The rationale for doing 7 H is very confusing.

      The experiment assessing raffinose utilization as a carbon source was inspired by the previous work of Garcia-Rivera et al., reporting that the _cap59_Δ mutant is unable to utilize raffinose due to a defect in the secretion of raffinose-hydrolyzing enzymes. As another way to investigate potential defects in the conventional secretion pathway, we investigated the growth of the _ugg1_Δ mutant in the presence of raffinose. Due to our extensive data length, we have decided to remove this complementary data from the manuscript.

      (16) It is speculated in the discussion that ER stress impacts lipid/sterol synthesis and that LDs (lipid droplets?) aid the UPR and ERAD in degrading misfolded proteins during ER stress in S. cerevisiae. The authors mention that they observed a drastic increase in LDs in the ugg1Δ mutant. Where is this data? Even with the data, this is all speculation. The authors also speculate that increased numbers of vacuoles in ugg1 (where is the data?) could be the cause of the altered vesicular structures observed in the mutants, which may indicate abnormal lipid homeostasis caused by the ERQC defects, which could, in turn, affect EV biogenesis. Again, this is speculative.

      The data on lipid droplets (LDs) and vacuole staining are presented in Supplementary Figure S6, showing a drastic increase in LDs and an increased in vacuolar size in the _ugg1_Δ mutant compared to the wild-type strain, especially in capsule-inducing conditions. In addition to such changes in vesicular structures, our preliminary data on sphingolipids and sterol analysis in the surface lipid fraction of the _ugg1_Δ mutant led us to propose the hypothesis that ERQC defects may impact lipid metabolism, which in turn could influence EV biogenesis and membrane properties. It is expected that these findings would provide a strong foundation for future studies exploring the link between ERQC, lipid homeostasis, and EV biogenesis. We have revised our speculation on the association of abnormal lipid homeostasis, caused by ERQC, with EV biogenesis more appropriately by adding the information on our preliminary data of lipid profiles and mentioning that the _ugg1_Δ mutant lacks microvesicles, which are derived from the plasma membrane (Page 24, lines 554-559).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) My suggestions for the authors are the same as those presented in the public review: (1) reducing the text in certain sections of the paper to improve readability for the audience, and (2) reconsidering the figures to reduce the amount of information in each one, moving some of the content to the supplementary material.

      We thank the reviewer for their constructive suggestions regarding the organization and readability of the manuscript. As suggested, we addressed your concerns as follows:

      (1) Reducing the text in the Introduction, Results, and Discussion sections by removing repetitive statements and simplifying complex descriptions where possible.

      (2) Changing the presentation of figures: we have also reorganized the presentation of some data by moving non-essential data to the supplementary material. The updated figures and supplementary materials have been clearly referenced in the text to guide readers.

      (3) Reorganization of materials and methods: some parts of methods were moved to Supplementary Information

      (4) Removal of Figure 7H and the sentences describing the result

      More detailed explanations on the reduction and reorganization are also described in the response to the major comments (2) and (3) made by Reviewer #1.

      (2) Figure 3, for example, shows no difference in fungal growth under different cultivation conditions. This information is valuable but could be mentioned in the text, with the image provided as supplementary material, focusing the figure only on images that show significant growth differences among the strains. I suggest a similar approach for other figures so that the authors can include only the most relevant results in the main body of the article and move some figures to the supplementary materials.

      For Fig. 3, the spotting data of the double mutant (previously Fig. 3B) is now presented in the supplementary information (Supplementary Fig. S3B). Additionally, the subcellular localization data (previously Fig 3E) was also moved to the supplementary material (Supplementary Fig. S4A).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 43 "EV-mediated transport of virulence bags" doesn't make sense. EVs have been described as "virulence bags" (and are in this work later in the introduction) but this should here be "transport of virulence factors" or "compounds associated with virulence" but only if you have confirmed that the "cargo" is consistent with this- which is not evident in the abstract.

      Thank you for your insightful comment. We have revised this to "EV-mediated transport of virulence factors" in line with your suggestion.

      (2) Line 49 "secretory pathway" - is there not more than one secretion pathway?

      Thank you for pointing this out. The term "secretory pathway" has been updated to "secretory pathways" to acknowledge the presence of both conventional and unconventional secretion mechanisms.

      (3) Line 53 "recognizes folding defects, repairs them, and ensures the translocation of irreparable misfolded proteins" should be "recognizes folding defects and repairs them or ensures the translocation of irreparable misfolded proteins.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have revised the sentence as you suggested.

      (4) Lines 88-90 ALG needs to be written out the first time - Asn-linked glycans. Also, consider adding that ALG genes are present in most eukaryotes as it is unclear what you are comparing C. neoformans to.

      Thank you for your helpful comment. We have revised the text to write out "ALG" as "Asn-linked glycosylation" and added the sentence “ALG genes are evolutionary conserved in most eukaryotes” in the revised manuscript (Page 4, line 84).

      (5) Line 99 Cryptococcus has already been abbreviated to C. so don't write it out again.

      We have corrected "Cryptococcus" to “C.” throughout the manuscript after its first mention.

      (6) Line 152- tunicamycin and DTT are not described yet, which may make it challenging for some readers to understand what these drugs are doing/why they were used. What is on lines 156 and 157 for these drugs should go up with the first mention of these drugs.

      Thank you for your helpful suggestion. We have revised the manuscript to include the descriptions and purpose of using tunicamycin (TM) and dithiothreitol (DTT) immediately following their first mention, as recommended (Page 10, lines 208-210).

      (7) The text for Figure 1 C is inaccurate. High temperature also induced KAR2, as noted above, but inaccurately stated in line 160. There is no comment on the significant UGG1 increase with tunicamycin or that KAR2 was highest in this condition.

      Thank you for your thoughtful comment. We have better clarified the significant increase of UGG1 expression following tunicamycin treatment and KAR2 induction upon heat stress in the revised manuscript (Page 10, lines 216-217). Please note that Fig. 1C was revised and is now referred to as Fig. 3A.

      (8) Figure 2B is not well explored/explained. There appears to be more protein in the mutant, including of higher weight in the intracellular compartment. It is difficult to ascertain if there is more too in the secretion phase with this gel. The methods do not specifically describe the concentration of protein added - just volume. Is what we are seeing a loading issue vs real differences?

      Thank you for your insightful comments regarding Figure 2B. We added information on amounts of protein (30 µg per lane) in the legend of Figure 2B.

      The main purpose of Fig. 2B is to examine the altered glycosylation pattern of ERQC by detecting glycoproteins using the Galanthus nivalis agglutinin, which specifically bind terminal α1,2-, α1,3-, and α1,6-linked mannose residues. The result of lectin blotting indicated that glycoproteins are more abundantly detected in the secretion fraction compared to in the soluble intracellular fraction, consistent with the general notion that more than 50% of secretory proteins are glycoproteins. Also, the more abundant proteins with decreased molecular weight in the secretion fraction of ugg1_Δ mutant supported the _N-glycan profiles with decreased hypermannosylation in _ugg1_Δ mutant. We added the purpose and more detailed interpretation on Figure 2B in the revised manuscript (Page 9, lines 174-179).

      (9) Line 242 "melanin pigment" is redundant as melanin is a pigment.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the redundancy in the phrase. We revised the text to simply state "melanin".

      (10) Line 250 drops "completely" especially as the mutant did colonize the lungs of mice.

      To avoid any possible misleading, we removed the term "completely" in the revised manuscript.

      (11) Line 275- need to reference 18B7 as it is first introduced here.

      We added the reference on the antibody 18B7 in the revised manuscript.

      (12) Line 308- there are specific techniques to measure GXM size that could validate or refute the statement on "incomplete" polysaccharides. For example, DOI:10.1128/EC.00268-09.

      We appreciated the valuable suggestion on specific techniques to measure GXM size, which will be one of key experiments in our future study. In the revised manuscript we cited the suggested reference to indicate the need for validation of our statement (Page 14, lines 316-318).

      (13) Line 496 "mammals" - why is this used when the study is on a fungus, not a mammal? The structure of the first 2 paragraphs can be clearer to focus more on fungal biology.

      We have compared both mammals and fungi to emphasize that the ERQC system is conserved among eukaryotes but diverged with a few species-specific features. This comparison is relevant in the context of understanding the evolutionary unique features of ERQC pathways in C. neoformans. We modified the first 2 paragraphs to clarify the main issue of our present study (Page 21, lines 472-483).

      (14) Line 525- the ugg mutant was not avirulent as CFU was present and histopathology in the supplementary figures shows the tissue with ugg1 deletion was not normal (although the images are not especially easy to review). Yes, the mutant did not kill under your test conditions, but it was not avirulent (incapable of causing disease). Significantly attenuated or other descriptors should be utilized. Line 548 is also thus incorrect "complete loss of virulence").

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concern regarding the description of the _ugg1_Δ mutant as avirulent. We agree that the use of merely “avirulent" may not fully capture the observed phenotypes in the CFU and histopathological data, since we cannot exclude the possibility that the _ugg1_Δ mutant retains the ability to establish an infection. Thus, we have revised the text by describing the _ugg1_Δ mutant as "almost avirulent".

      (15) Line 597- the study by Fukuoka used kidney cells. It is misleading to not clearly state that this finding of ER stress was NOT done in fungi as the way it is presented makes it read as if this work was performed in C. neoformans. This should be clarified. This should also be double-checked and clarified for other statements, such as the reference to Harada in line 606, as this study used melanoma cells. These cell types are very different from cryptococcus- though I absolutely concur that lessons can be learned from comparative assessments.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the need to clarify the experimental context of the cited studies. We explicitly stated the host cell types used in the referenced studies by Fukuoka et al. and by Harada et al., respectively, in the revised manuscript (Page 25, lines 560 and 568).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      eLife assessment 

      This valuable study aims to present a mathematical theory for why the periodicity of the hexagonal pattern of grid cell firing would be helpful for encoding 2D spatial trajectories. The idea is supported by solid evidence, but some of the comparisons of theory to the experimental data seem incomplete, and the reasoning supporting some of the assumptions made should be strengthened. The work would be of interest to neuroscientists studying neural mechanisms of spatial navigation. 

      We thank the reviewers for this assessment. We have addressed the comments made by reviewers and believe that the revised manuscript has theoretical and practical implications beyond the subfield of neuroscience concerned with mechanisms underpinning spatial memory and spatial navigation. Specifically, the demonstration that four simple axioms beget the spatial firing pattern of grid cells is highly relevant for the field of artificial intelligence and neuromorphic computing. This relevance stems from the fact that the four axioms define a set of four simple computational algorithms that can be implemented in future work in grid cell-inspired computational algorithms. Such algorithms will be impactful because they can perform path integration, a function that is independent of an animal’s or agent’s location and therefore generalizable. Moreover, because of the functional organization of grid cells into modules, the algorithm is also scalable. Generalizability and scalability are two highly sought-after properties of brain-inspired computational frameworks. We also believe that the question why grid cells emerge in the brain is a fundamental one. This manuscript is, to our knowledge, the first one that provides an interpretable and intuitive answer to why grid cells are observed in the brain. 

      Before addressing each comment, we would like to point out that the first sentence of the assessment appears misphrased. The study does not aim to present a theory for why the periodicity in grid cell firing would be helpful for encoding 2D spatial trajectories. To present a theory “for why grid cell firing would be helpful for encoding 2D trajectories”, one assumes the existence of grid cells a priori. Instead of assuming the existence of grid cells and deriving a computational function from grid cells, our study derives grid cells from a computational function, as correctly summarized by reviewers #1 and #3 in their individual statements. In contrast to previous normative models, we prove mathematically that spatial periodicity in grid cell firing is implied by a sequence code of trajectories. If the brain uses cell sequences to code for trajectories, spatially periodic firing must emerge. As correctly pointed out by reviewer #1, the underlying assumptions of this study are that the brain codes for trajectories and that it does so using cell sequences. In response to comments by reviewer #1, we now discuss these two assumptions more rigorously.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review): 

      Rebecca R.G. et al. set to determine the function of grid cells. They present an interesting case claiming that the spatial periodicity seen in the grid pattern provides a parsimonious solution to the task of coding 2D trajectories using sequential cell activation. Thus, this work defines a probable function grid cells may serve (here, the function is coding 2D trajectories), and proves that the grid pattern is a solution to that function. This approach is somewhat reminiscent in concept to previous works that defined a probable function of grid cells (e.g., path integration) and constructed normative models for that function that yield a grid pattern. However, the model presented here gives clear geometric reasoning to its case. 

      Stemming from 4 axioms, the authors present a concise demonstration of the mathematical reasoning underlying their case. The argument is interesting and the reasoning is valid, and this work is a valuable addition to the ongoing body of work discussing the function of grid cells. 

      However, the case uses several assumptions that need to be clearly stated as assumptions, clarified, and elaborated on: Most importantly, the choice of grid function is grounded in two assumptions: 

      (1) that the grid function relies on the activation of cell sequences, and 

      (2) that the grid function is related to the coding of trajectories. While these are interesting and valid suggestions, since they are used as the basis of the argument, the current justification could be strengthened (references 28-30 deal with the hippocampus, reference 31 is interesting but cannot hold the whole case). 

      We thank this reviewer for the overall positive and constructive criticism. We agree with this reviewer that our study rests on two premises, namely that 1) a code for trajectories exist, and 2) this code is implemented by cell sequences. We now discuss and elaborate on the data in the literature supporting the two premises.

      In addition to the work by Zutshi et al. (reference 31 in the original manuscript), we have now cited additional work presenting experimental evidence for sequential activity of neurons in the medial entorhinal cortex, including sequential activity of grid cells.

      We have added the following paragraph to the Discussion section:

      “Recent studies provided compelling evidence for sequential activity of neurons representing spatial trajectories. In particular, Gardner et al. (2022) demonstrated that the sequential activity of hundreds of simultaneously recorded grid cells in freely foraging rats represented spatial trajectories. Complementary preliminary results indicate that grid cells exhibit left-rightalternating “theta sweeps,” characterized by temporally compressed sequences of spiking activity that encode outwardly oriented trajectories from the current location (Vollan et al., 2024).

      The concept of sequential grid cell activity extends beyond spatial coding. In various experimental contexts, grid cells have been shown to encode non-spatial variables. For instance, in a stationary auditory task, grid cells fired at specific sounds along a continuous frequency axis (Aronov et al., 2017). Further studies revealed that grid cell sequences also represent elapsed time and distance traversed, such as during a delay period in a spatial alternation task (Kraus et al., 2015). Similar findings were reported for elapsed time encoded by grid cell sequences in mice performing a virtual “Door Stop” task (Heys and Dombeck, 2018).

      Additionally, spatial trajectories represented by temporally compressed grid cell sequences have been observed during sleep as replay events (Ólafsdóttir et al., 2016; O’Neill et al., 2017). Collectively, these studies demonstrate that sequential activity of neurons within the MEC, particularly grid cells, consistently encodes ordered experiences, suggesting a fundamental role for temporal structure in neuronal representations.

      The theoretical underpinnings of grid cell activity coding for ordered experiences have been explored previously by Rueckemann et al. (2021) who argued that the temporal order in grid cell activation allows for the construction of topologically meaningful representations, or neural codes, grounded in the sequential experience of events or spatial locations. However, while Rueckemann et al. argue that the MEC supports temporally ordered representations through grid cell activity, our findings suggest an inverse relationship: namely, that grid cell activity emerges from temporally ordered spatial experiences. Additional studies demonstrate that hippocampal place cells may derive their spatial coding properties from higher-order sequence learning that integrates sensory and motor inputs (Raju et al., 2024) and that hexagonal grids, if assumed a priori, optimally encode transitions in spatiotemporal sequences (Waniek, 2018).

      Together, experimental and theoretical evidence demonstrate the significance of sequential neuronal activity within the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex as a core mechanism for representing both spatial and temporal information and experiences.”

      The work further leans on the assumption that sequences in the same direction should be similar regardless of their position in space, it is not clear why that should necessarily be the case, and how the position is extracted for similar sequences in different positions. 

      We thank this reviewer for giving us the opportunity to clarify this point. We define a trajectory as a path taken in space (Definition 6). By this definition, a code for trajectories is independent of the animal’s spatial location. This is consistent with the definition of path integration, which is also independent of an animal’s spatial location. If the number of neurons is finite (Axiom #4) and the space is large, sequences must eventually repeat in different locations. This results in neural sequences coding for the same directions being identical at different locations. We have clarified this point under new Remark 6.1. in the Results section of the revised:

      “Remark 6.1. Note that a code for trajectories is independent of the animal’s spatial location, consistent with the definition of path integration. This implies that, if the number of neurons is finite (Axiom #4) and the space is large, sequences must eventually repeat in different location, resulting in neural sequences coding for the same trajectories at different locations.”

      The formal proof was already included in the original manuscript: “Generally speaking, starting in a firing field of element i and going along any set of firing fields, some element must eventually become active again since the total number of elements is finite by axiom 4. Once there is a repeat of one element’s firing field, the whole sequence of firing fields of all elements must repeat by axiom 1. More specifically, if we had a sequence 1,2, … , k, 1, t of elements, then 1,2 and 1, t both would code for traveling in the same direction from element 1, contradicting axiom 1.”

      Further: “More explicitly, assuming axioms 1 and 4, the firing fields of trajectory-coding elements must be spatially periodic, in the sense that starting at any point and continuing in a single direction, the initial sequence of locally active elements must eventually repeat with a repeat length of at least 3”.

      Regarding the question how an animal’s position is extracted for similar sequences in different positions, we agree with this reviewer that this is an important question when investigating the contributions of grid cells to the coding of space. However, since a code for trajectories is independent of spatial location, the question of how to extract an animal’s position from a trajectory code is irrelevant for this study.

      While a trajectory code by neural sequences begets grid cells, a spatial code by neural sequences does not. Nevertheless, grid cells could contribute to the coding of space (in addition to providing a trajectory code). However, while experimental evidence from studies with rodents and human subjects and theoretical work demonstrated the importance of grid cells for path integration (Fuhs and Touretzky, 2006; McNaughton et al., 2006; Moser et al., 2017), experimental studies have shown that grid cells contribute little to the coding of space by place cells (Hales et al., 2014). Yet, theoretical work (Mathis et al., 2012) showed that coherent activity of grid cells across different modules can provide a code for spatial location that is more accurate than spatial coding by place cells in the hippocampus. Importantly, such a spatial code by coherent activity across grid cell modules does not require location-dependent differences in neural sequences.

      The authors also strengthen their model with the requirement that grid cells should code for infinite space. However, the grid pattern anchors to borders and might be used to code navigated areas locally. Finally, referencing ref. 14, the authors claim that no existing theory for the emergence of grid cell firing that unifies the experimental observations on periodic firing patterns and their distortions under a single framework. However, that same reference presents exactly that - a mathematical model of pairwise interactions that unifies experimental observations. The authors should clarify this point. 

      We thank this reviewer for this valuable feedback. We agree that grid cells anchor to borders and may be used to code navigated areas locally. In fact, the trajectory code performs a local function, namely path integration, and the global grid pattern can only emerge from performing this local computation if the activity of at least one grid unit or element (we changed the wording from unit to element based on feedback from reviewer #3) is anchored to either a spatial location or a border. Yet, the trajectory code itself does not require anchoring to a reference frame to perform local path integration. Because of the local nature of the trajectory code, path integration can be performed locally without the emergence of a global grid pattern. This has been shown experimentally in mice performing a path integration task where changes in the location of a task-relevant object resulted in translations of grid patterns in single trials. Although no global grid pattern was observed, grid cells performed path integration locally within the multiple reference frames defined by the task-relevant object, and grid patterns were visible when the changes in the references frames were accounted for in computing the rate maps (Peng et al., 2023). The data by Peng et al. (2023) confirm that the anchoring of the grid pattern to borders and the emergence of the global pattern are not required for local coding of trajectories. The global pattern emerges only when the reference frame does not change. However, this global pattern itself might not serve any function. According to the trajectory code model, the beguiling grid pattern is merely a byproduct of a local path integration function that is independent of the animal’s current location (which makes the code generalizable across space). The reviewer is correct that, if the reference frame used to anchor the grid pattern did not change in infinite space, the trajectory code model of grid cell firing would predict an infinite global pattern. But does the proof implicitly assume that space is infinite? The trajectory code model makes the quantitative prediction that the field size increases linearly with an increase in grid spacing (the distance between two fields). If the field size remains fixed, periodicity will emerge in finite spaces that are larger than the grid spacing. We have clarified these points in the revised manuscript:

      “Notably, the trajectory code itself does not require anchoring to a reference frame to perform local path integration. Because of the local nature of the trajectory code, path integration can be performed locally without the emergence of a global grid pattern. This has been shown experimentally in mice performing a path integration task where changes in the location of a task-relevant object resulted in translations of grid patterns in single trials (Peng et al., 2023). Although no global grid pattern was observed because the reference frame was not fixed in space, grid cells performed path integration locally within the reference frame defined by the moving task-relevant object, and grid patterns were visible when the changes in the references frames were accounted for in computing the rate maps”.

      Regarding how the emergence of grid cells from a trajectory code relates to the theory of a local code by grid cells brought forward by Ginosar et al. (ref. 14), we argue that the local computational function suggested by Ginosar et al. is to provide a code for trajectories. The perspective article by Ginosar et al. provides an excellent review of the experimental data on grid cells that point to grid cells performing a local function (see also Kate Jeffery’s excellent review article (Jeffery, 2024) on the mosaic structure of the mammalian cognitive map.) Assuming the existence of grid cells a priori, Ginosar et al. then propose three possible functions of grid cells, all of which are consistent with the trajectory code model of grid cell firing. Yet, the perspective article remains agnostic, in our opinion, on the exact nature of the local computation that is carried out by grid cells. But without knowing the local computation underlying grid cell function, a unifying theory explaining the emergence of grid cells cannot be considered complete. In contrast, our manuscript identifies the local computational function as a trajectory code by cell sequences. We have clarified these points in the revised manuscript:

      “The influential hypothesis that grid cells provide a universal map for space is challenged by experimental data suggesting a yet to be identified local computational function of grid cells (Ginosar et al., 2023; Jeffery, 2024). Here, we identify this local computational function as a trajectory code.”

      The mathematical model of pairwise interactions described by Ginosar et al. is fundamentally different from the mathematical framework developed in our manuscript. The mathematical model by Ginosar et al. describes how pairwise interactions between already existent grid fields can explain distortions in the grid pattern caused by the environment’s geometry, reward zones, and dimensionality. However, the model does not explain why there is a grid pattern in the first place. In contrast, our trajectory model provides an explanation for why grid cells may exist by demonstrating that a grid pattern emerges from a trajectory code by cell sequences. We stand by our assessment that a unifying theory of grid cells is not complete if it takes the existence of the grid pattern for granted.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      In this work, the authors consider why grid cells might exhibit hexagonal symmetry - i.e., for what behavioral function might this hexagonal pattern be uniquely suited? The authors propose that this function is the encoding of spatial trajectories in 2D space. To support their argument, the authors first introduce a set of definitions and axioms, which then lead to their conclusion that a hexagonal pattern is the most efficient or parsimonious pattern one could use to uniquely label different 2D trajectories using sequences of cells. The authors then go through a set of classic experimental results in the grid cell literature - e.g. that the grid modules exhibit a multiplicative scaling, that the grid pattern expands with novelty or is warped by reward, etc. - and describe how these results are either consistent with or predicted by their theory. Overall, this paper asks a very interesting question and provides an intriguing answer. However, the theory appears to be extremely flexible and very similar to ideas that have been previously proposed regarding grid cell function. 

      We thank this reviewer for carefully reading the manuscript and their valuable feedback which helps us clarify major points of the study. One major clarification is that the theoretical/axiomatic framework we put forward does not assume grid cells a priori. In contrast, we start by hypothesizing a computational function that a brain region shown to be important for path integration likely needs to solve, namely coding for spatial trajectories. We go on to show that this computational function begets spatially periodic firing (grid maps). By doing so, we provide mathematical proof that grid maps emerge from solving a local computational function, namely spatial coding of trajectories. Showing the emergence of grid maps from solving a local computational function is fundamentally different from many previous studies on grid cell function, which assign potential functions to the existing grid pattern. As we discuss in the manuscript, our work is similar to using normative models of grid cell function. However, in contrast to normative models, we provide a rigorous and interpretable mathematical framework which provides geometric reasoning to its case.

      Major strengths: 

      The general idea behind the paper is very interesting - why *does* the grid pattern take the form of a hexagonal grid? This is a question that has been raised many times; finding a truly satisfying answer is difficult but of great interest to many in the field. The authors' main assertion that the answer to this question has to do with the ability of a hexagonal arrangement of neurons to uniquely encode 2D trajectories is an intriguing suggestion. It is also impressive that the authors considered such a wide range of experimental results in relation to their theory.  

      We thank this reviewer for pointing out the significance of the question addressed by our manuscript.

      Major weaknesses: 

      One major weakness I perceive is that the paper overstates what it delivers, to an extent that I think it can be a bit confusing to determine what the contributions of the paper are. In the introduction, the authors claim to provide "mathematical proof that ... the nature of the problem being solved by grid cells is coding of trajectories in 2-D space using cell sequences. By doing so, we offer a specific answer to the question of why grid cell firing patterns are observed in the mammalian brain." This paper does not provide proof of what grid cells are doing to support behavior or provide the true answer as to why grid patterns are found in the brain. The authors offer some intriguing suggestions or proposals as to why this might be based on what hexagonal patterns could be good for, but I believe that the language should be clarified to be more in line with what the authors present and what the strength of their evidence is. 

      We thank this reviewer for this assessment. While there is ample experimental evidence demonstrating the importance of grid cells for path integration, we agree with this reviewer that there may be other computational functions that may require or largely benefit from the existence of grid cells. We now acknowledge the fact that we have provided a likely teleological cause for the emergence of grid cells and that there might be other causes for the emergence of grid cells. We have changed the wording in the abstract and discussion sections to acknowledge that our study does provide a likely teleological cause. We choose “likely” because the computational function – trajectory coding – from which grid maps emerge is very closely associated to path integration, which numerous experimental and theoretical studies associate with grid cell function.

      Relatedly, the authors claim that they find a teleological reason for the existence of grid cells - that is, discover the function that they are used for. However, in the paper, they seem to instead assume a function based on what is known and generally predicted for grid cells (encode position), and then show that for this specific function, grid cells have several attractive properties. 

      We agree with this reviewer that we leveraged what is known about grid cells, in particular their importance for path integration, in finding a likely teleological cause. However, the major significance of our work is that we demonstrate that coding for spatial trajectories requires spatially periodic firing (grid cells).This is very different from assuming the existence of grid cells a priori and then showing that grid cells have attractive, if not optimal, properties for this function. If we had shown that grid cells optimized a code for trajectories, this reviewer would be correct: we would have suggested just another potential function of grid cells. Instead, we provide both proof and intuition that trajectory coding by cell sequences begets grid cells (not the other way around), thereby providing a likely teleological cause for the emergence of grid cells. As stated above, we clarified in the revised manuscript that we provide a likely teleological cause which requires additional experimental verification.

      There is also some other work that seems very relevant, as it discusses specific computational advantages of a grid cell code but was not cited here: https://www.nature.com/articles/nn.2901

      We thank this reviewer for pointing us toward this article by (Sreenivasan and Fiete, 2011). The revised manuscript now cites this article in the Introduction and Discussion sections. We agree that the article by (Sreenivasan and Fiete, 2011) discusses a specific computational advantage of a population code by grid cells, namely unprecedented robustness to noise in estimating the location from the spiking information of noisy neurons. However, the work by (Sreenivasan and Fiete, 2011) differs from our work in that the authors assume the existence of grid cells a priori.

      In addition, we now discuss other relevant work, namely work on the conformal isometry hypothesis  by (Schøyen et al., 2024) and (Xu et al., 2024), published as pre-prints after publication of the first version of our manuscript, as well as work on transition scale- spaces by Nicolai Waniek. (Xu et al., 2024) and (Schøyen et al., 2024) investigate conformal isometry in the coding of space by grid cells. Conformal isometry means that trajectories in neural space map trajectories in physical space. (Xu et al., 2024) show that the conformal isometry hypothesis can explain the spatially periodic firing pattern of grid cells. (Schøyen et al., 2024) further show that a module of seven grid cells emerges if space is encoded as a conformal isometry, ensuring equal representation in all directions. While the work by (Xu et al., 2024) and (Schøyen et al., 2024) arrive at very similar conclusions as stated in the current manuscript, the conformal isometry hypothesis provides only a partial answer to why grid cells exist because it doesn’t explain why conformal isometry is important or required. In contrast, a sequence code of trajectories provides an intuitive answer to why such a code is important for animal behavior. Furthermore, we included the work by Nicolai Waniek, (2018, 2020) in the Discussion, who demonstrated that the hexagonal arrangement of grid fields is optimal for coding transitions in space. 

      The paragraph added to the Discussion reads as follows:

      “As part of the proof that a trajectory code by cell sequences begets spatially periodic firing fields, we proved that the centers of the firing fields must be arranged in a hexagonal lattice. This arrangement implies that the neural space is a conformally isometric embedding of physical space, so that local displacements in neural space are proportional to local displacements of an animal or agent in physical space, as illustrated in Figure 5. This property has recently been introduced in the grid cell literature as the conformal isometry hypothesis(Schøyen et al., 2024; Xu et al., 2024). Strikingly, Schøyen et al.(Schøyen et al., 2024) arrive at similar if not identical conclusions regarding the geometric principles in the neural representations of space by grid cells.”

      A second major weakness was that some of the claims in the section in which they compared their theory to data seemed either confusing or a bit weak. I am not a mathematician, so I was not able to follow all of the logic of the various axioms, remarks, or definitions to understand how the authors got to their final conclusion, so perhaps that is part of the problem. But below I list some specific examples where I could not follow why their theory predicted the experimental result, or how their theory ultimately operated any differently from the conventional understanding of grid cell coding. In some cases, it also seemed that the general idea was so flexible that it perhaps didn't hold much predictive power, as extra details seemed to be added as necessary to make the theory fit with the data. 

      I don't quite follow how, for at least some of their model predictions, the 'sequence code of trajectories' theory differs from the general attractor network theory. It seems from the introduction that these theories are meant to serve different purposes, but the section of the paper in which the authors claim that various experimental results are predicted by their theory makes this comparison difficult for me to understand. For example, in the section describing the effect of environmental manipulations in a familiar environment, the authors state that the experimental results make sense if one assumes that sequences are anchored to landmarks. But this sounds just like the classic attractornetwork interpretation of grid cell activity - that it's a spatial metric that becomes anchored to landmarks. 

      We thank this reviewer for giving us the opportunity to clarify in what aspects the ‘sequence code of trajectories’ theory of grid cell firing differs from the classic attractor network models, in particular the continuous attractor network (CAN) model. First of all, the CAN model is a mechanistic model of grid cell firing that is specifically designed to simulate spatially periodic firing of grid cells in response to velocity inputs. In contrast, the sequence code of trajectories theory of grid cell firing resembles a normative model showing that grid cells emerge from performing a specific function. However, in contrast to previous normative models, the sequence code of trajectories model grounds the emergence of grid cell firing in a mathematical proof and both geometric reasoning and intuition. The proof demonstrates that the emergence of grid cells is the only solution to coding for trajectories using cell sequences. The sequence code of trajectories model of grid cell firing is agnostic about the neural mechanisms that implements the sequence code in a population of neurons. One plausible implementation of the sequence code of trajectories is in fact a CAN. In fact, the sequence code of trajectories theory predicts conformal isometry in the CAN, i.e., a trajectory in neural space is proportional to a trajectory of an animal in physical space. However, other mechanistic implementations are possible. We have clarified how the sequence code of trajectories theory of grid cells relates to the mechanistic CAN models of grid cells. 

      We added the following text to the Discussion section:

      “While the sequence code of trajectories-model of grid cell firing is agnostic about the neural mechanisms that implements the sequence code, one plausible implementation is a continuous attractor network (McNaughton et al., 2006; Burak and Fiete, 2009). Interestingly, a sequence code of trajectories begets conformal isometry in the attractor network, i.e., a trajectory in neural space is proportional to a trajectory of an animal in physical space.”

      It was not clear to me why their theory predicted the field size/spacing ratio or the orientation of the grid pattern to the wall. 

      We thank this reviewer for bringing to our attention that we lacked a proper explanation for why the sequence code of trajectories theory predicts the field size/spacing ration in grid maps. We have modified/added the following text to the Results section of the manuscript to clarify this point:

      “Because the sequence code of trajectories model of grid cell firing implies a dense packing of firing fields, the spacing between two adjacent grid fields must change linearly with a change in field size. It follows that the ratio between grid spacing and field size is fixed. When using the distance between the centers of two adjacent grid fields to measure grid spacing and a diameter-like metric to measure grid field size, we can compute the ratio of grid spacing to grid field size as √7≈2.65 (see Methods).”

      We are also grateful for this reviewer’s correctly pointing out that the explanation as to why the sequence code of trajectories predicts a rotation of the grid pattern relative to a set of parallel walls in a rectangular environment. We have now made explicit the underlying premise that a sequence of firing fields from multiple grid cells are aligned in parallel to a nearby wall of the environment. We cite additional experimental evidence supporting this premise. Concretely, we quote Stensola and Moser summarizing results reported in (Stensola et al. 2015): “A surprising observation, however, was that modules typically assumed one of only four distinct orientation configurations relative to the environment” (Stensola and Moser, 2016). Importantly, all of the four distinct orientations show the characteristic angular rotation. Intriguingly, this is predicted by the sequence code of trajectories-model under the premise that a sequence of firing fields aligns with one of the geometric boundaries of the environment, as shown in Author response image 1 below.

      Author response image 1.

      Under the premise that a sequence of firing fields aligns with one of the geometric boundaries (walls) of a square arena, there are precisely four possible distinct configurations of orientations. This is precisely what has been observed in experiments (Stensola et al., 2015; Stensola and Moser, 2016).

      We added clarifying language to the Results section: “Under the premise that a sequence of firing fields aligns with one of the geometric boundaries of the environment, the sequence code model explains that the grid pattern typically assume one of only four distinct orientation configurations relative to the environment41,46. Concretely, the four orientation configurations arise when one row of grid fields aligns with one of the two sets of parallel walls in a rectangular environment, and each arrangement can result in two distinct orientations (Figure 3B).”

      I don't understand how repeated advancement of one unit to the next, as shown in Figure 4E, would cause the change in grid spacing near a reward. 

      In familiar environments, spatial firing fields of place cells in hippocampal CA1 and CA3 tend to shift backwards with experience (Mehta et al., 2000; Lee et al., 2004; Roth et al., 2012; Geiller et al., 2017; Dong et al., 2021). This implies that the center of place fields move closer to each other. A potential mechanism has been suggested, namely NMDA receptor-dependent longterm synaptic plasticity (Ekstrom et al., 2001). When we apply the same principle observed for place fields on a linear track to grid fields anchored to a reward zone, grid fields will “gravitate” towards the reward side. A similar idea has been presented by (Ginosar et al., 2023) who use the analogy of reward locations as “black holes”. In contrast to (Ginosar et al., 2023), who we cite multiple times, our idea unifies observations on place cells and grid cells in 1-D and 2-D environments and suggests a potential mechanism. We changed the wording in the revised manuscript and clarified the underlying premises.

      I don't follow how this theory predicts the finding that the grid pattern expands with novelty. The authors propose that this occurs because the animals are not paying attention to fine spatial details, and thus only need a low-resolution spatial map that eventually turns into a higher-resolution one. But it's not clear to me why one needs to invoke the sequence coding hypothesis to make this point. 

      We agree with this reviewer that this point needs clarification. The sequence code model adds explanatory power to the hypothesis that the grid pattern in a novel environment reflects a lowresolution mapping of space or spatial trajectories because it directly links spatial resolution to both field size and spacing of a grid map. Concretely, the spatial resolution of the trajectory code is equivalent to the spacing between two adjacent spatial fields, and the spatial resolution is directly proportional to the grid spacing and field size. If one did not evoke the sequence coding hypothesis, one would need to explain how and why both spacing and field size are related to the spatial resolution of the grid map. Lastly, as written in the manuscript text, we point out that, while the experimentally observed expansion of grid maps is consistent with the sequence code of trajectory, it is not predicted by the theory without making further assumption. 

      The last section, which describes that the grid spacing of different modules is scaled by the square root of 2, says that this is predicted if the resolution is doubled or halved. I am not sure if this is specifically a prediction of the sequence coding theory the authors put forth though since it's unclear why the resolution should be doubled or halved across modules (as opposed to changed by another factor). 

      We agree with reviewer #2 that the exact value of the scaling factor is not predicted by the sequence coding theory. E.g., the sequence code theory does not explain why the spatial resolution doesn’t change by a factor 3 or 1.5 (resulting in changes in grid spacing by square root of 3 or square root of 1.5, respectively). We have changed the wording to reflect this important point. We further clarified in the revised manuscript that future work on multiscale representations using modules of grid cells needs to show why changing the spatial resolution across modules by a factor of 2 is optimal. Interestingly, a scale ratio of 2 is commonly used in computer vision, specifically in the context of mipmapping and Gaussian pyramids, to render images across different scales. Literature in the computer vision field describes why a scaling factor of 2 and the use of Gaussian filter kernels (compare with Gaussian firing fields) is useful in allowing a smooth and balanced transition between successive levels of an image pyramid (Burt and Adelson, 1983; Lindeberg, 2008). Briefly, larger factors (like 3) could result in excessive loss of detail between levels, while smaller factors (like 1.5) would not reduce the image size enough to justify additional levels of computation (that would come with the structural cost of having more grid cell modules in the brain). We have clarified these points in the Discussion section.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review): 

      The manuscript presents an intriguing explanation for why grid cell firing fields do not lie on a lattice whose axes aligned to the walls of a square arena. This observation, by itself, merits the manuscript's dissemination to the eLife's audience. 

      We thank this reviewer for their positive assessment.

      The presentation is quirky (but keep the quirkiness!). 

      We kept the quirkiness.

      But let me recast the problem presented by the authors as one of combinatorics. Given repeating, spatially separated firing fields across cells, one obtains temporal sequences of grid cells firing. Label these cells by integers from $[n]$. Any two cells firing in succession should uniquely identify one of six directions (from the hexagonal lattice) in which the agent is currently moving. 

      Now, take the symmetric group $\Sigma$ of cyclic permutations on $n$ elements.  We ask whether there are cyclic permutations of $[n]$ such that 

      \left(\pi_{i+1} - \pi_i \right) \mod n \neq \pm 1 \mod n, \; \forall i. 

      So, for instance, $(4,2,3,1)$ would not be counted as a valid permutation of $(1,2,3,4)$, as $(2,3)$ and $(1,4)$ are adjacent. 

      Furthermore, given $[n]$, are there two distinct cyclic permutations such that {\em no} adjacencies are preserved when considering any pair of permutations (among the triple of the original ordered sequence and the two permutations)? In other words, if we consider the permutation required to take the first permutation into the second, that permutation should not preserve any adjacencies. 

      {\bf Key question}: is there any difference between the solution to the combinatorics problem sketched above and the result in the manuscript? Specifically, the text argues that for $n=7$ there is only {\em one} solution. 

      Ideally, one would strive to obtain a closed-form solution for the number of such permutations as a function of $n$.  

      This is a great question! We currently have a student working on describing all possible arrangements of firing fields (essentially labelings of the hexagonal lattice) that satisfy the axioms in 2D, and we expect that results on the number of such arrangements will come out of his work. We plan to publish those results separately, possibly targeting a more mathematical audience.   

      The argument above appears to only apply in the case that every row (and every diagonal) contains all of the elements 1,...,n. However, when n is not prime, there are often arrangements where rows and/or diagonals do not contain every element from 1,...,n. For example, some admissible patterns with 9 neurons have a repeat length of 3 in all directions (horizontally and both diagonals). As a result the construction listed here will not give a full count of all possible arrangements. 

      Recommendations for the authors:  

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      I think the concise style of mathematical proof is both a curse and a blessing. While it delivers the message, I think the fluency and readability of the mathematical proof could be improved with longer paragraphs and some more editing. 

      We have added some clarifications in the text that we hope improve the readability.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      A minor qualm I have with the nomenclature: 

      On page 7: 

      “To prove this statement, suppose that row A consists of units $1, \dots , k$ repeating in this order. Then any row that contains any unit from $1, \dots, k$ must contain the full repeat $1, \dots , k$ by axiom 1. So any row containing any unit from $1,\dots , k$ is a translation of row A, and any unit that does not contain them is disjoint from row A.”

      The last use of `unit' at the end of this paragraph instead of `row' is confusing. Technically, the authors have given themselves license to use this term by defining a unit to be “either to a single cell or a cell assembly”. Yet modern algebra tends to use `unit' as meaning a ring element that has an inverse.  

      We have renamed “unit” to “element” to avoid confusion with the terminology in modern algebra.

    1. Author response:

      Joint Public Review:

      Summary:

      In this study, Daniel et al. used three cognitive tasks to investigate behavioral signatures of cerebellar degeneration. In the first two tasks, the authors found that if an equation was incorrect, reaction times slowed significantly more for cerebellar patients than for healthy controls. In comparison, the slowing in the reaction times when the task required more operations was comparable to normal controls. In the third task, the authors show increased errors in cerebellar patients when they had to judge whether a letter string corresponded to an artificial grammar.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the work is methodologically sound and the manuscript well written. The data do show some evidence for specific cognitive deficits in cerebellar degeneration patients.

      Thank you for the thoughtful summary and constructive feedback. We are pleased that the methodological rigor and clarity of the manuscript were appreciated, and that the data were recognized as providing meaningful evidence regarding cognitive deficits in cerebellar degeneration.

      Weaknesses:

      The current version has some weaknesses in the visual presentation of results. Overall, the study lacks a more precise discussion on how the patterns of deficits relate to the hypothesized cerebellar function. The reviewers and the editor agreed that the data are interesting and point to a specific cognitive deficit in cerebellar patients. However, in the discussion, we were somewhat confused about the interpretation of the result: If the cerebellum (as proposed in the introduction) is involved in forming expectations in a cognitive task, should they not show problems both in the expected (1+3 =4) and unexpected (1+3=2) conditions? Without having formed the correct expectation, how can you correctly say "yes" in the expected condition? No increase in error rate is observed - just slowing in the unexpected condition. But this increase in error rate was not observed. If the patients make up for the lack of prediction by using some other strategy, why are they only slowing in the unexpected case? If the cerebellum is NOT involved in making the prediction, but only involved in detecting the mismatch between predicted and real outcome, why would the patients not show specifically more errors in the unexpected condition?

      Thank you for asking these important questions and initiating an interesting discussion. While decision errors and processing efficiency are not fully orthogonal and are likely related, they are not necessarily the same internal construct. The data from Experiments 1 and 2 suggest impaired processing efficiency rather than increased decision error. Reaction time slowing without increased error rates suggests that the CA group can form expectations but respond more slowly, possibly due to reduced processing efficiency. Thus, this analysis of our data can indicate that the cerebellum is not essential for forming expectations, but it plays a critical role in processing their violations.

      Relatedly, two important questions remain open in the literature concerning the cerebellum’s role in expectation-related processes. The first is whether the cerebellum contributes to the formation of expectations or the processing of their violations. In Experiments 1 and 2, the CA group did not show impairments in the complexity manipulation. As mentioned by the editors, solving these problems requires the formation of expectations during the reasoning process. Given the intact performance of the CA group, these results suggest that they are not impaired in forming expectations. However, in both Experiments 1 and 2, patients exhibited selective impairments in solving incorrect problems compared to correct problems. Since expectation formation is required in both conditions, but only incorrect problems involve a violation of expectation (VE), we hypothesize that the cerebellum is involved in VE processes. We suggest that the CA group can form expectations in familiar tasks, but are impaired in processing unexpected compared to expected outcomes. This supports the notion that the cerebellum contributes to VE, rather than to forming expectations.

      Importantly, while previous experimental manipulations(1–6) have provided important insights, some may have confounded these two internal constructs due to task design limitations (e.g., lack of baseline conditions). Notably, some of these previous studies did not include control conditions (e.g., correct trials) where there was no VE. In addition, other studies did not include a control measure (e.g., complexity effect), which limits their ability to infer the specific cerebellar role in expectation manipulation.

      In addition to the editors’ question, we would like to raise a second important question regarding cerebellar contributions to expectations-related processes. While our findings point to a both unique and consistent cerebellar role in VE processes in sequential tasks, we do not aim to generalize this role to all forms of expectations(2,7,8). Another interesting process is how expectations are formed. Expectations can be formed by different processes(2,7,8), and this should be taken into account when defining cerebellar function. For instance, previous experimental paradigms(1–6), aiming to assess VE, utilized tasks that manipulated rule-based errors or probability-based errors, but did not fully dissociate these constructs. In our Experiments 1 and 2, we specifically manipulated error signals derived from previous top-down effects. However, in Experiment 3, the participant’s VE was derived from within-task processes. In Experiment 3, expectations were formed either by statistical learning or by rule-based learning. During the test stage, when evaluating sensitivity to correct and incorrect problems, the CA group showed deficits only when expectations were formed based on rules. These findings suggest that cerebellar patients may retain a general ability to form expectations. However, their deficit appears to be specific to processing rule-based VE, but not statistically derived VE. This pattern of results aligns with the results of Experiments 1 and 2 where the rules are known and based on pre-task knowledge.

      We suggest that these two key questions are relevant to both motor and non-motor domains and were not fully addressed even in the previous, well-studied motor domain. Thus, the current experimental design used in three different experiments provides a valuable novel experimental perspective, allowing us to distinguish between some, but not all, of the processes involved in the formation of expectations and their violations. For instance, to our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate a selective impairment in rule-based VE processing in cerebellar patients across both numerical reasoning and artificial grammar tasks.

      If feasible, we propose that future studies should disentangle different forms of VE by operationalizing them in experimental tasks in an orthogonal manner. This will allow us, as a scientific community, to achieve a more detailed, well-defined cerebellar motor and non-motor mechanistic account.

      References

      (1) Butcher, P. A. et al. The cerebellum does more than sensory prediction error-based learning in sensorimotor adaptation tasks. J. Neurophysiol. 118, 1622–1636 (2017).

      (2) Moberget, T., Gullesen, E. H., Andersson, S., Ivry, R. B. & Endestad, T. Generalized role for the cerebellum in encoding internal models: Evidence from semantic processing. J. Neurosci. 34, 2871–2878 (2014).

      (3) Riva, D. The cerebellar contribution to language and sequential functions: evidence from a child with cerebellitis. Cortex. 34, 279–287 (1998).

      (4) Sokolov, A. A., Miall, R. C. & Ivry, R. B. The Cerebellum: Adaptive Prediction for Movement and Cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 313–332 (2017).

      (5) Fiez, J. A., Petersen, S. E., Cheney, M. K. & Raichle, M. E. Impaired non-motor learning and error detection associated with cerebellar damage. A single case study. Brain 115 Pt 1, 155–178 (1992).

      (6) Taylor, J. A., Krakauer, J. W. & Ivry, R. B. Explicit and Implicit Contributions to Learning in a Sensorimotor Adaptation Task. J. Neurosci. 34, 3023–3032 (2014).

      (7) Sokolov, A. A., Miall, R. C. & Ivry, R. B. The Cerebellum: Adaptive Prediction for Movement and Cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 313–332 (2017).

      (8) Fiez, J. A., Petersen, S. E., Cheney, M. K. & Raichle, M. E. IMPAIRED NON-MOTOR LEARNING AND ERROR DETECTION ASSOCIATED WITH CEREBELLAR DAMAGEA SINGLE CASE STUDY. Brain 115, 155–178 (1992).

      (9) Picciotto, Y. De, Algon, A. L., Amit, I., Vakil, E. & Saban, W. Large-scale evidence for the validity of remote MoCA administration among people with cerebellar ataxia administration among people with cerebellar ataxia. Clin. Neuropsychol. 0, 1–17 (2024).

      (10) Binoy, S., Monstaser-Kouhsari, L., Ponger, P. & Saban, W. Remote Assessment of Cognition in Parkinsons Disease and Cerebellar Ataxia: The MoCA Test in English and Hebrew. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 17, (2023).

      (11) Saban, W. & Ivry, R. B. Pont: A protocol for online neuropsychological testing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 33, 2413–2425 (2021).

      (12) Algon, A. L. et al. Scale for the assessment and rating of ataxia : a live e ‑ version. J. Neurol. (2025). doi:10.1007/s00415-025-13071-7

      (13) McDougle, S. D. et al. Continuous manipulation of mental representations is compromised in cerebellar degeneration. Brain 145, 4246–4263 (2022).

    1. Author response:

      eLife Assessment

      This important study uses an innovative task design combined with eye tracking and fMRI to distinguish brain regions that encode the value of individual items from those that accumulate those values for value-based choices. It shows that distinct brain regions carry signals for currently evaluated and previously accumulated evidence. The study provides solid evidence in support of most of its claims, albeit with current minor weaknesses concerning the evidence in favour of gaze-modulation of the fMRI signal. The work will be of interest to neuroscientists working on attention and decision-making.

      We thank the Editor and Reviewers for their summary of the strengths of our study, and for their thoughtful review and feedback on our manuscript. We plan to undertake some additional analyses suggested by the Reviewers to bolster the evidence in favor of gaze-modulation of the fMRI signal.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study builds upon a major theoretical account of value-based choice, the 'attentional drift diffusion model' (aDDM), and examines whether and how this might be implemented in the human brain using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). The aDDM states that the process of internal evidence accumulation across time should be weighted by the decision maker's gaze, with more weight being assigned to the currently fixated item. The present study aims to test whether there are (a) regions of the brain where signals related to the currently presented value are affected by the participant's gaze; (b) regions of the brain where previously accumulated information is weighted by gaze.

      To examine this, the authors developed a novel paradigm that allowed them to dissociate currently and previously presented evidence, at a timescale amenable to measuring neural responses with fMRI. They asked participants to choose between bundles or 'lotteries' of food times, which they revealed sequentially and slowly to the participant across time. This allowed modelling of the haemodynamic response to each new observation in the lottery, separately for previously accumulated and currently presented evidence.

      Using this approach, they find that regions of the brain supporting valuation (vmPFC and ventral striatum) have responses reflecting gaze-weighted valuation of the currently presented item, whereas regions previously associated with evidence accumulation (preSMA and IPS) have responses reflecting gaze-weighted modulation of previously accumulated evidence.

      Strengths:

      A major strength of the current paper is the design of the task, nicely allowing the researchers to examine evidence accumulation across time despite using a technique with poor temporal resolution. The dissociation between currently presented and previously accumulated evidence in different brain regions in GLM1 (before gaze-weighting), as presented in Figure 5, is already compelling. The result that regions such as preSMA respond positively to |AV| (absolute difference in accumulated value) is particularly interesting, as it would seem that the 'decision conflict' account of this region's activity might predict the exact opposite result. Additionally, the behaviour has been well modelled at the end of the paper when examining temporal weighting functions across the multiple samples.

      Thank you!

      Weaknesses:

      The results relating to gaze-weighting in the fMRI signal could do with some further explication to become more complete. A major concern with GLM2, which looks at the same effects as GLM1 but now with gaze-weighting, is that these gaze-weighted regressors may be (at least partially) correlated with their non-gaze-weighted counterparts (e.g., SVgaze will correlate with SV). But the non-gaze-weighted regressors have been excluded from this model. In other words, the authors are not testing for effects of gaze-weighting of value signals *over and above* the base effects of value in this model. In my mind, this means that the GLM2 results could simply be a replication of the findings from GLM1 at present. GLM3 is potentially a stronger test, as it includes the value signals and the interaction with gaze in the same model. But here, while the link to the currently attended item is quite clear (and a replication of Lim et al, 2011), the link to previously accumulated evidence is a bit contorted, depending upon the interpretation of a behavioural regression to interpret the fMRI evidence. The results from GLM3 are also, by the authors' own admission, marginal in places.

      We thank the Reviewer for their thoughtful critique. We acknowledge that our formulation of GLM2 does not test for the effects of gaze-weighted value signals beyond the base effects of value, only in place of the base effects of value. In our revision, we plan to examine alternative ways of quantifying the relative importance of gaze in these results.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, the authors seek to disentangle brain areas that encode the subjective value of individual stimuli/items (input regions) from those that accumulate those values into decision variables (integrators) for value-based choice. The authors used a novel task in which stimulus presentation was slowed down to ensure that such a dissociation was possible using fMRI despite its relatively low temporal resolution. In addition, the authors leveraged the fact that gaze increases item value, providing a means of distinguishing brain regions that encode decision variables from those that encode other quantities such as conflict or time-on-task. The authors adopt a region-of-interest approach based on an extensive previous literature and found that the ventral striatum and vmPFC correlated with the item values and not their accumulation, whereas the pre-SMA, IPS, and dlPFC correlated more strongly with their accumulation. Further analysis revealed that the pre-SMA was the only one of the three integrator regions to also exhibit gaze modulation.

      Strengths:

      The study uses a highly innovative design and addresses an important and timely topic. The manuscript is well-written and engaging, while the data analysis appears highly rigorous.

      Weaknesses:

      With 23 subjects, the study has relatively low statistical power for fMRI.

      We thank the Reviewer for their comments on the strengths of the manuscript, and for highlighting an important limitation. We agree that the number of participants in the study, after exclusions, was lower than your typical fMRI study. However, it is important to note that we do have a lot of data for each subject. Due to our relatively fast, event-related design, we have on average 65 trials per subject (SD = 18) and 5.95 samples per trial (SD \= 4.03), for an average of 387 observations per subject (SD = 18). Our model-based analysis looks for very specific neural time courses across these ~387 observations, giving us substantial power to detect our effects of interest. Still, we acknowledge that our small number of subjects does still limit our power and our ability to generalize to other subjects. We plan to add the following disclaimer to the Discussion section:

      “Together with our limited sample size (n = 23), we may not have had adequate statistical power required to observe consistent effects. Additional research with larger sample sizes is needed to resolve this issue.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer 1 (Public Review):

      Summary

      The manuscript uses state-of-the-art analysis technology to document the spatio-temporal dynamics of brain activity during the processing of threats. The authors offer convincing evidence that complex spatio-temporal aspects of brain dynamics are essential to describe brain operations during threat processing.

      Strengths

      Rigorous complex analyses well suited to the data.

      Weaknesses

      Lack of a simple take-home message about discovery of a new brain operation.

      We have addressed the concern under response to item 1 in Recommendations for the authors of Reviewer 2 below.

      Reviewer 1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The paper presents sophisticated analyses of how the spatiotemporal activity of the brain processes threats. While the study is elegant and relevant to the threat processing literature, it could be improved by better clarification of novelty, scope, assumptions and implications. Suggestions are reported below.

      (1) Introduction: It is difficult to understand what is unsatisfactory in the present literature and why we need this study. For example, lines 57-64 report what works well in the work of Anderson and Fincham but do not really describe what this approach lacks, either in failing to explain real data in conceptual terms.

      We have edited the corresponding lines to better describe what such approaches generally lack:

      Introduction; Lines 63-66: However, the mapping between brain signals and putative mental states (e.g., “encoding”) remained speculative. More generally, state-based modeling of fMRI data would benefit from evaluation in contexts where the experimental paradigm affords a clearer mapping between discovered states and experimental manipulation.

      (2) Also, based on the introduction it is unclear if the focus is on understanding the processing of threat or in the methodological development of experimental design and analysis paradigms for more ecologically valid situations.

      In our present work, we tried to focus on understanding dynamics of threat processing while also contributing to methodological development of analysis of dynamic/ecologically inspired experiments. To that end, we have added a new paragraph at the end of Introduction to clarify the principal focus of our work:

      Introduction; Lines 111-118: Is the present contribution focused on threat processing or methodological developments for the analysis of more continuous/ecologically valid paradigms? Our answer is “both”. One goal was to contribute to the development of a framework that considers brain processing to be inherently dynamic and multivariate. In particular, our goal was to provide the formal basis for conceptualizing threat processing as a dynamic process (see (Fanselow and Lester, 1987)) subject to endogenous and exogenous contributions. At the same time, our study revealed how regions studied individually in the past (e.g., anterior insula, cingulate cortex) contribute to brain states with multi-region dynamics.

      (3) The repeated statement, based on the Fiete paper, that most analyses or models of brain activity do not include an exogenous drive seems an overstatement. There is plenty of literature that not only includes exogenous drives but also studies and documents them in detail. There are many examples, but a prominent one is the study of auditory processing. Essentially all human brain areas related to hearing (not only the activity of individual areas but also their communication) are entrained by the exogenous drive of speech (e.g. J. Gross et al, PLoS Biology 11 e1001752, 2013).

      We have altered the original phrasing, which now reads as:

      Introduction; Lines 93-95: Importantly, we estimated both endogenous and exogenous components of the dynamics, whereas some past work has not modeled both contributions (see discussion in (Khona and Fiete, 2022)).

      Discussion; Lines 454-455: Work on dynamics of neural circuits in systems neuroscience at times assumes that the target circuit is driven only by endogenous processes (Khona and Fiete, 2022).

      (4) Attractor dynamics is used as a prominent descriptor of fMRI activity, yet the discussion of how this may emerge from the interaction between areas is limited. Is it related to the way attractors emerge from physical systems or neural networks (e.g. Hopfield?).

      This is an important question that we believe will benefit from computational and mathematical modeling, but we consider it beyond the scope of the present paper.

      (5) Fig 4 shows activity of 4 regions, not 2 s stated in lines 201-202. Correct?

      Fig. 4 shows activity of two regions and also the average activity of regions belonging to two resting-state networks engaged during threat processing (discussed shortly after lines 201-202). To clarify the above concern, we have changed the following line:

      Results; Lines 228-230: In Fig. 4, we probed the average signals from two resting-state networks engaged during threat-related processing, the salience network which is particularly engaged during higher threat, and the default network which is engaged during conditions of relative safety.

      (6) It would be useful to state more clearly how Fig 7B, C differs from Fig 2A, B (my understanding it is that in the former they are isolating the stimulus-driven processes)

      We have clarified this by adding the following line in the Results:

      Results; Lines 290-292: Note that in Fig. 7B/C we evaluated exogenous contributions only for stimuli associated with each state/state transition reported in Fig. 2A/B (see also Methods).

      Reviewer 2 (Public Review):

      Summary

      This paper by Misra and Pessoa uses switching linear dynamical systems (SLDS) to investigate the neural network dynamics underlying threat processing at varying levels of proximity. Using an existing dataset from a threat-of-shock paradigm in which threat proximity is manipulated in a continuous fashion, the authors first show that they can identify states that each has their own linear dynamical system and are consistently associated with distinct phases of the threat-of-shock task (e.g., “peri-shock”, “not near”, etc). They then show how activity maps associated with these states are in agreement with existing literature on neural mechanisms of threat processing, and how activity in underlying brain regions alters around state transitions. The central novelty of the paper lies in its analyses of how intrinsic and extrinsic factors contribute to within-state trajectories and betweenstate transitions. A final set of analyses shows how the findings generalize to another (related) threat paradigm.

      Strengths

      The analyses for this study are conducted at a very high level of mathematical and theoretical sophistication. The paper is very well written and effectively communicates complex concepts from dynamical systems. I am enthusiastic about this paper, but I think the authors have not yet exploited the full potential of their analyses in making this work meaningful toward increasing our neuroscientific understanding of threat processing, as explained below.

      Weaknesses

      (1) I appreciate the sophistication of the analyses applied and/or developed by the authors. These methods have many potential use cases for investigating the network dynamics underlying various cognitive and affective processes. However, I am somewhat disappointed by the level of inferences made by the authors based on these analyses at the level of systems neuroscience. As an illustration consider the following citations from the abstract: “The results revealed that threat processing benefits from being viewed in terms of dynamic multivariate patterns whose trajectories are a combination of intrinsic and extrinsic factors that jointly determine how the brain temporally evolves during dynamic threat” and “We propose that viewing threat processing through the lens of dynamical systems offers important avenues to uncover properties of the dynamics of threat that are not unveiled with standard experimental designs and analyses”. I can agree to the claim that we may be able to better describe the intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics of threat processing using this method, but what is now the contribution that this makes toward understanding these processes?

      We have addressed the concern under response to item 1 in Recommendations for the authors below.

      (2) How sure can we be that it is possible to separate extrinsically and intrinsically driven dynamics?

      We have addressed the concern under response to item 2 in Recommendations for the authors below.

      Reviewer 2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) To address the first point under weaknesses above: I would challenge the authors to make their results more biologically/neuroscientifically meaningful, in particular in the sections (in results and/or discussion) on how intrinsic and extrinsic factors contribute to within-state trajectories and between-state transitions, and make those explicit in both the abstract and the discussion (what exactly are the properties of the dynamics of threat that are uncovered?). The authors may also argue that the current approach lies the groundwork for such efforts, but does not currently provide such insights. If they would take this position, that should be made explicit throughout (which would make it more of a methodological paper).

      The SLDS approach provides, we believe, a powerful framework to describe system-level dynamics (of threat processing in the the present case). A complementary type of information can be obtained by studying the contribution of individual components (brain regions) within the larger system (brain), an approach that helps connect our approach to studies that typically focus on the contributions of individual regions, and contributes to providing more neurobiological interpretability to the results. Accordingly, we developed a new measure of region importance that captured the extent to which individual brain regions contributed to driving system dynamics during a given state.

      Abstract; Lines 22-25: Furthermore, we developed a measure of region importance that quantifies the contributions of an individual brain region to system dynamics, which complements the system-level characterization that is obtained with the state-space SLDS formalism.

      Introduction; Lines 95-99: A considerable challenge in state-based modeling, including SLDS, is linking estimated states and dynamics to interpretable processes. Here, we developed a measure of region importance that provides a biologically meaningful way to bridge this gap, as it quantifies how individual brain regions contribute to steering state trajectories.

      Results; Lines 302-321: Region importance and steering of dynamics: Based on time series data and input information, the SLDS approach identifies a set of states and their dynamics. While these states are determined in the latent space, they can be readily mapped back to the brain, allowing for the characterization of spatiotemporal properties across the entire brain. Since not all regions contribute equally to state properties, we propose that a region’s impact on state dynamics serves as a measure of its importance.

      We illustrate the concept for STATE 5 (“near miss”) in Fig. 8 (see Fig. S17 for all states). Fig. 8A shows importance in the top row and activity below as a function of time from state entry.The dynamics of importance and activity can be further visualized (Fig. 8B), where some regions of particularly high importance are illustrated together with the ventromedial PFC, a region that is typically not engaged during high-threat conditions. Notably, the importance of the dorsal anterior insula increased quickly in the first time points, and later decreased. In contrast, the importance of the periaqueductal gray was relatively high from the beginning of the state and decreased moderately later.

      Fig. 8C depicts the correlation between these measures as a function of time. For all but STATE 1, the correlation increased over time. Interestingly, for STATES 4-5, the correlation was low at the first and second time points of the state (and for STATE 2 at the first time point), and for STATE 3 the measures were actually anticorrelated; both cases indicate a dissociation between activity and importance. In summary, our results illustrate that univariate region activity can differ from multivariate importance, providing a fruitful path to understand how individual brain regions contribute to collective dynamic properties.

      Discussion; Lines 466-487: In the Introduction, we motivated our study in terms of determining multivariate and distributed patterns of activity with shared dynamics. At one end of the spectrum, it is possible to conceptualize the whole brain as dynamically evolving during a state; at the other end, we could focus on just a few “key” regions, or possibly a single one (at which point the description would be univariate). Here, we addressed this gap by studying the importance of regions to state dynamics: To what extent does a region steer the trajectory of the system? From a mathematical standpoint, our proposed measure is not merely a function of activity of a region but also of the coefficients of the dynamics matrix capturing its effect on across-region dynamics (Eichler, 2005; Smith et al., 2010).

      How distributed should the dynamics of threat be considered? One answer to this question is to consider the distribution of importance values for all states. For STATE 1 (“post shock”), a few regions displayed the highest importance values for a few time points. However, for the other states the distribution of importance values tended to be more uniform at each time point. Thus, based on our proposed importance measure, we conclude that threat-related processing is profitably viewed as substantially distributed. Furthermore, we found that while activity and importance were relatively correlated, they could also diverge substantially. Together, we believe that the proposed importance measure provides a valuable tool for understanding the rich dynamics of threat processing. For example, we discovered that the dorsal anterior insula is important not only during high-anxiety states (such as STATE 5; “near miss”) but also, surprisingly, for a state that followed the aversive shock event (STATE 1; “post shock”). Additionally, we noted that posterior cingulate cortex, widely known to play a central role in the default mode network, to have the highest importance among all other regions in driving dynamics of low-anxiety states (such as STATE 3 and STATE 4; “not near”).

      Methods; Lines 840-866: Region importance We performed a “lesion study”, where we quantified how brain regions contribute to state dynamics by eliminating (zeroing) model parameters corresponding to a given region, and observing the resulting changes in system dynamics. According to our approach, the most important regions are those that cause the greatest change in system dynamics when eliminated.

      The SLDS model represents dynamics in a low dimensional latent space and model parameters are not readily available at the level of individual regions. Thus, the first step was to project the dynamics equation onto the brain data prior to computing importance values. Thus, the linear dynamics equation in the latent space (Eq. 2) was mapped to the original data space of N = 85 ROIs using the emissions model (Eq. 1):

      where C<sup>†</sup> represents the Moore-Penrose pseudoinverse of C, and and denote the corresponding dynamics matrix, input matrix, and bias terms in the original data space.

      Based on the above, we defined the importance of the i<sup>th</sup> ROI at time t based on quantifying the impact of “lesioning” the i<sup>th</sup> ROI, i.e., by setting the i<sup>th</sup> column of , the i<sup>th</sup> row of ,   and the i<sup>th</sup> element of to 0, denoted , , and respectively. Formally, the importance of the i<sup>th</sup> ROI was defined as:

      where ‘∗’ indicates element-wise multiplication of a scalar with a vector, is the activity of i<sup>th</sup> ROI at time corresponds to the i<sup>th</sup> column of is the inner product between i<sup>th</sup> row of and input corresponds to the i<sup>th</sup> element of and represents an indicator vector corresponding to the i<sup>th</sup> ROI. Note that the term is a function of both the i<sup>th</sup> ROI’s activity as well as the coefficients of the dynamics matrix capturing the effect of region i on the one-step dynamics of the entire system (Eichler, 2005; Smith et al., 2010); the remaining terms capture the effect of the external inputs and the bias term on the one-step dynamics of the i<sup>th</sup> ROI.

      After computing for a given run, the resultant importance time series was normalized to zero mean and unit variance.

      (2) To address the second point under the weaknesses above: Given that the distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics appears central to the novelty of the paper, I would suggest the authors explicitly address this issue in the introduction and/or discussion sections.

      The distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics is a modeling assumption of SLDS. We used such an assumption because in experimental designs with experimenter manipulated inputs one can profitably investigate both types of contribution to dynamics. While we should not reify the model’s assumption, we can gain confidence in our separation of extrinsically and intrinsically driven dynamics through controlled experiments where we can manipulate external inputs, or by demonstrating time-scale separation of intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics and that they operate at different frequencies. This is an important question that requires additional computational/mathematical modeling, but we consider it beyond the scope of the current paper. We have added the following lines in the discussion section:

      Discussion; Lines 521-528: A further issue that we wish to discuss is related to the distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics, which is explicitly modeled in our SLDS approach (see Methods, equation 2). We believe this is a powerful approach because in experimental designs with experimenter manipulated inputs, one can profitably investigate both types of contribution to dynamics. However, complete separation between intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics is challenging to ascertain. More generally, one can gain confidence in their separation through controlled experiments where external inputs are manipulated, or by demonstrating timescale separation of intrinsic and extrinsic dynamics.

      (3) In the abstract, the statement “.. studies in systems neuroscience that frequently assume that systems are decoupled from external inputs” sounds paradoxical after first introducing how threat processing is almost exclusively studied using blocked and event-related task designs (which obviously rely on external inputs only). Please clarify this.

      In this work, we wished to state that the SLDS framework characterizes both endogenous and exogenous contributions to dynamics, whereas some past work has not modeled both contributions. To clarify, we have changed the corresponding line:

      Abstract; Lines 19-20: Importantly, we characterized both endogenous and exogenous contributions to dynamics.

      (4) In the abstract, the first mention of circles comes out of the blue; the paradigm needs to be introduced first to make this understandable.

      We have rephrased the corresponding text:

      Abstract; Lines 14-17: First, we demonstrated that the SLDS model learned the regularities of the experimental paradigm, such that states and state transitions estimated from fMRI time series data from 85 regions of interest reflected threat proximity and threat approach vs. retreat.

      (5 In Figure 3, the legend shows z-scores representing BOLD changes associated with states. However, the z-scores are extremely low (ranging between -.4 and .4). Can this be correct, given that maps are thresholded at p < ._001 (i.e., _z > 3_._09)? A similar small range of z-scores is shown in the legend of Fig 5. Please check the z-score ranges.

      The p-value threshold used in Fig. 3 is based on the voxelwise t-test conducted between the participantbased bootstrapped maps and null maps (see Methods : State spatial maps : “To identify statistically significant voxels, we performed a paired t-test between the participant-based boostrapped maps and the null maps.”). Thus, the p-value threshold in the figure does not correspond to the z-scores of the groupaveraged state-activation maps. Similarly in Fig. 5, we only visualized the state-wise attractors on a brain surface map without any thresholding. The purpose of using a z-score color bar was to provide a scale comparable to that of BOLD activity.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      A cortico-centric view is dominant in the study of the neural mechanisms of consciousness. This investigation represents the growing interest in understanding how subcortical regions are involved in conscious perception. To achieve this, the authors engaged in an ambitious and rare procedure in humans of directly recording from neurons in the subthalamic nucleus and thalamus. While participants were in surgery for the placement of deep brain stimulation devices for the treatment of essential tremor and Parkinson's disease, they were awakened and completed a perceptual-threshold tactile detection task. The authors identified individual neurons and analyzed single-unit activity corresponding with the task phases and tactile detection/perception. Among the neurons that were perception-responsive, the authors report changes in firing rate beginning ~150 milliseconds from the onset of the tactile stimulation. Curiously, the majority of the perception-responsive neurons had a higher firing rate for missed/not perceived trials. In summary, this investigation is a valuable addition to the growing literature on the role of subcortical regions in conscious perception.

      Strengths:

      The authors achieved the challenging task of recording human single-unit activity while participants performed a tactile perception task. The methods and statistics are clearly explained and rigorous, particularly for managing false positives and non-normal distributions. The results offer new detail at the level of individual neurons in the emerging recognition of the role of subcortical regions in conscious perception.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive comments.

      Weaknesses:

      "Nonetheless, it remains unknown how the firing rate of subcortical neurons changes when a stimulus is consciously perceived." (lines 76-77) The authors could be more specific about what exactly single-unit recordings offer for interrogating the role of subcortical regions in conscious perception that is unique from alternative neural activity recordings (e.g., local field potential) or recordings that are used as proxies of neural activity (e.g., fMRI).

      We agree with the reviewer that the contribution of micro-electrode recordings was not sufficiently put forward in our manuscript. We added the following sentences to the discussion, when discussing the multiple types of neurons we found:

      Single-unit recordings provide a much higher temporal resolution than functional imaging, which helps assess how the neural correlates of consciousness unfold over time. Contrary to local field potentials, single-unit recordings can expose the variety of functional roles of neurons within subcortical regions, thereby offering a potential for a better mechanistic understanding of perceptual consciousness.

      Related comment for the following excerpts:

      "After a random delay ranging from 0.5 to 1 s, a "respond" cue was played, prompting participants to verbally report whether they felt a vibration or not. Therefore, none of the reported analyses are confounded by motor responses." (lines 97-99).

      "These results show that subthalamic and thalamic neurons are modulated by stimulus onset, irrespective of whether it was reported or not, even though no immediate motor response was required." (lines 188190).

      "By imposing a delay between the end of the tactile stimulation window and the subjective report, we ensured that neuronal responses reflected stimulus detection and not mere motor responses." (lines 245247).

      It is a valuable feature of the paradigm that the reporting period was initiated hundreds of milliseconds after the stimulus presentation so that the neural responses should not represent "mere motor responses". However, verbal report of having perceived or not perceived a stimulus is a motor response and because the participants anticipate having to make these reports before the onset of the response period, there may be motor preparatory activity from the time of the perceived stimulus that is absent for the not perceived stimulus. The authors show sensitivity to this issue by identifying task-selective neurons and their discussion of the results that refer to the confound of post-perceptual processing. Still, direct treatment of this possible confound would help the rigor of the interpretation of the results.

      We agree with the reviewer that direct treatment would have provided the best control. One way to avoid motor preparation is to only provide the stimulus-effector mapping after the stimulus presentation (Bennur & Gold, 2011; Twomey et al., 2016; Fang et al., 2024). Other controls to avoid post-perceptual processing used in consciousness research consist of using no-report paradigms (Tsuchiya et al., 2015) as we did in previous studies (Pereira et al., 2021; Stockart et al., 2024). Unfortunately, neither of these procedures was feasible during the 10 minutes allotted for the research task in an intraoperative setting with auditory cues and vocal responses. We would like to highlight nonetheless that the effects we report are shortlived and incompatible with sustained motor preparation activity.

      We added the following sentence to the discussion:

      Future studies ruling out the presence of motor preparation triggered by perceived stimuli (Bennur & Gold, 2011; Fang et al., 2024; Twomey et al., 2016) and verifying that similar neuronal activity occurs in the absence of task-demands (no-reports; Tsuchiya et al., 2015) or attention (Wyart & Tallon-Baudry, 2008) will be useful to support that subcortical neurons contribute specifically to perceptual consciousness.

      "When analyzing tactile perception, we ensured that our results were not contaminated with spurious behavior (e.g. fluctuation of attention and arousal due to the surgical procedure)." (lines 118-117).

      Confidence in the results would be improved if the authors clarified exactly what behaviors were considered as contaminating the results (e.g., eye closure, saccades, and bodily movements) and how they were determined.

      This sentence was indeed unclear. It introduced the trial selection procedure we used to compensate for drifts in the perceptual threshold, which can result from fluctuations in attention or arousal. We modified the sentence, which now reads:

      When analyzing tactile perception, we ensured that our results were not contaminated by fluctuating attention and arousal due to the surgical procedure. Based on objective criteria, we excluded specific series of trials from analyses and focused on time windows for which hits and misses occurred in commensurate proportions (see methods).

      During the recordings, the experimenter stood next to the patients and monitored their bodily movements, ensuring they did not close their eyes or produce any other bodily movements synchronous with stimulus presentation.

      The authors' discussion of the thalamic neurons could be more precise. The authors show that only certain areas of the thalamus were recorded (in or near the ventral lateral nucleus, according to Figure S3C). The ventral lateral nucleus has a unique relationship to tactile and motor systems, so do the authors hypothesize these same perception-selective neurons would be active in the same way for visual, auditory, olfactory, and taste perception? Moreover, the authors minimally interpret the location of the task, sensory, and perception-responsive neurons. Figure S3 suggests these neurons are overlapping. Did the authors expect this overlap and what does it mean for the functional organization of the ventral lateral nucleus and subthalamic nucleus in conscious perception?

      These are excellent questions, the answers to which we can only speculate. In rodents, the LT is known as a hub for multisensory processing, as over 90% of LT neurons respond to at least two sensory modalities (for a review, see Yang et al., 2024). Yet, no study has compared how LT neurons in rodents encode perceived and nonperceived stimuli across modalities. Evidence in humans is scarce, with only a few studies documenting supramodal neural correlates of consciousness at the cortical level with noninvsasive methods (Noel et al., 2018; Sanchez et al., 2020; Filimonov et al., 2022). We now refer to these studies in the revised discussion: Moreover, given the prominent role of the thalamus in multisensory processing, it will be interesting to assess if it is specifically involved in tactile consciousness or if it has a supramodal contribution, akin to what is found in the cortex (Noel et al., 2018; Sanchez et al., 2020; Filimonov et al., 2022).

      Concerning the anatomical overlap of neurons, we could not reconstruct the exact locations of the DBS tracts for all participants. Because of the limited number of recorded neurons, we preferred to refrain from drawing strong conclusions about the functional organization of the ventral lateral nucleus.

      "We note that, 6 out of 8 neurons had higher firing rates for missed trials than hit trials, although this proportion was not significant (binomial test: p = 0.145)." (lines 215-216).

      It appears that in the three example neurons shown in Figure 4, 2 out of 3 (#001 and #068) show a change in firing rate predominantly for the missed stimulations. Meanwhile, #034 shows a clear hit response (although there is an early missed response - decreased firing rate - around 150 ms that is not statistically significant). This is a counterintuitive finding when compared to previous results from the thalamus (e.g., local field potentials and fMRI) that show the opposite response profile (i.e., missed/not perceived trials display no change or reduced response relative to hit/perceived trials). The discussion of the results should address this, including if these seemingly competing findings can be rectified.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this limitation of the discussion. We avoided putting too much emphasis on these aspects due to the limited number of perception-selective neurons. Although subcortical connectivity models would predict that neurons in the thalamus should increase their firing rate for perceived stimuli, we were not surprised to see this heterogeneity as we had previously found neurons decreasing their firing rates for missed stimuli in the posterior parietal cortex (Pereira et al., 2021). We answer these points in response to the reviewer’s last comment below on the latencies of the effects.

      The authors report 8 perception-responsive neurons, but there are only 5 recording sites highlighted (i.e., filled-in squares and circles) in Figures S3C and 4D. Was this an omission or were three neurons removed from the perception-responsive analysis?

      Unfortunately, we could not obtain anatomical images for all participants. This information was present in the methods section, although not clearly enough:

      For 34 / 50 neurons, preoperative MRI and postoperative CT scans (co-registered in patient native space using CranialSuite) were available to precisely reconstruct surgical trajectories and recording locations (for the remaining 16 neurons, localizations were based on neurosurgical planning and confirmed by electrophysiological recordings at various depths).

      Therefore, we added the following sentence in Figures 2, 3, 4 and S3.

      [...] for patients for which we could obtain anatomical images.

      Could the authors speak to the timing of the responses reported in Figure 4? The statistically significant intervals suggested both early (~160-200ms) to late responses (~300ms). Some have hypothesized that subcortical regions are early - ahead of cortical activation that may be linked with conscious perception. Do these results say anything about this temporal model for when subcortical regions are active in conscious perception?

      We agree that response timing could have been better described. We performed a new analysis of the latencies at which our main effects were observed. This analysis revealed the existence of the two clusters mentioned by the reviewer very clearly. We now include this analysis in a new Figure 5 in the revised manuscript.

      We also performed a new analysis to support the existence of bimodal distributions and quantified the latencies. We added this text to the result section:

      We note that the timings of sensory and perception effects in Figures 3 and 4 showed a bimodal distribution with an early cluster (149 ms for sensory neurons; 121 ms for perception neurons; c.f. methods) and a later cluster (330 ms for sensory neurons; 315 ms for perception neurons; Figure 5). and this section to the methods:

      To measure bimodal timings of effect latencies, we fitted a two-component Gaussian mixture distribution to the data in Figure 5 by minimizing the mean square error with an interior-point method. We took the best of 20 runs with random initialization points and verified that the resulting mean square error was markedly (> 4 times) better than using a single component.

      We updated the discussion, including the points made in the comment about higher activity for missed stimuli (above):

      The early cluster’s average timing around 150 ms post-stimulus corresponds to the onset of a putative cortical correlate of tactile consciousness, the somatosensory awareness negativity (Dembski et al., 2021). Similar electroencephalographic markers are found in the visual and auditory modality. It is unclear, however, whether these markers are related to perceptual consciousness or selective attention (Dembski et al., 2021). The later cluster is centered around 300 ms and could correspond to a well known electroencephalographic marker, the P3b (Polich, 2007) whose association with perceptual consciousness has been questioned (Pitts et al., 2014; Dembski et al., 2021) although brain activity related to consciousness has been observed at similar timing even in the absence of report demands (Sergent et al., 2021; Stockart et al., 2024). It is also important to note that these clusters contain neurons with both increased and decreased firing rates following stimulus onset, similar to what was observed previously in the posterior parietal cortex (Pereira et al., 2021).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The authors have studied subpopulations of individual neurons recorded in the thalamus and subthalamic nucleus (STN) of awake humans performing a simple cognitive task. They have carefully designed their task structure to eliminate motor components that could confound their analyses in these subcortical structures, given that the data was recorded in patients with Parkinson's Disease (PD) and diagnosed with an Essential Tremor (ET). The recorded data represents a promising addition to the field. The analyses that the authors have applied can serve as a strong starting point for exploring the kinds of complex signals that can emerge within a single neuron's activity. Pereira et. al conclude that their results from single neurons indicate that task-related activity occurs, purportedly separate from previously identified sensory signals. These conclusions are a promising and novel perspective for how the field thinks about the emergence of decisions and sensory perception across the entire brain as a unit.

      We thank the reviewer for these positive comments.

      Despite the strength of the data that was obtained and the relevant nature of the conclusions that were drawn, there are certain limitations that must be taken into consideration:

      (1) The authors make several claims that their findings are direct representations of consciousnessidentifiable in subcortical structures. The current context for consciousness does not sufficiently define how the consciousness is related to the perceptual task.

      This is indeed a complex issue in all studies concerned with perceptual consciousness and we were careful not to make such “direct” claims. Instead, we used the state-of-the-art tools available to study consciousness (see below) and only interpreted our findings with respect to consciousness in the discussion. For example, in the abstract, our claim is that “Our results provide direct neurophysiological evidence of the involvement of the subthalamic nucleus and the thalamus for the detection of vibrotactile stimuli, thereby calling for a less cortico-centric view of the neural correlates of consciousness.”

      In brief, first, we used near-threshold stimuli which allowed us to contrast reported vs. unreported trials while keeping the physical properties of the stimulus comparable. Second, we used subjective reports without incentive for participants to be more conservative or liberal in their response (e.g. through reward). Third, we introduced a random delay before the responses to limit confounding effects due to the report. We also acknowledged that “... it will be important in future studies to examine if similar subcortical responses are obtained when stimuli are unattended (Wyart & Tallon-Baudry, 2008), task-irrelevant (Shafto & Pitts, 2015), or when participants passively experience stimuli without the instruction to report them (i.e., no-report paradigms) (Tsuchyia et al., 2015)”. This last sentence now reads (to address a point made by Reviewer 1 about motor preparation):

      Future studies ruling out the presence of motor preparation triggered by perceived stimuli (Bennur & Gold, 2011; Fang et al., 2024; Twomey et al., 2016) and verifying that similar neuronal activity occurs in the absence of task-demands (no-reports; Tsuchiya et al., 2015) or attention (Wyart & Tallon-Baudry, 2008) will be useful to support that subcortical neurons contribute specifically to perceptual consciousness.

      (2) The current work would benefit greatly from a description and clarification of what all the neurons thathave been recorded are doing. The authors' criteria for selecting subpopulations with task-relevant activity are appropriate, but understanding the heterogeneity in a population of single neurons is important for broader considerations that are being studied within the field.

      We followed the reviewer’s suggestions and added new results regarding the latencies of the reported effects (new Figure 5). We also now show firing rates for hits, misses and overall sensory activity (hits and misses combined) for all perception-selective or sensory-selective (when behavior was good enough; Figure S5). Although a more detailed characterization of the heterogeneity of the neurons identified would have been relevant, it seems beyond the scope of the present study, especially given the relatively small number of neurons we identified, as well as the relative simplicity of the paradigm imposed by the clinical context in which we worked.

      (3) The authors have omitted a proper set of controls for comparison against the active trials, forexample, where a response was not necessary. Please explain why this choice was made and what implications are necessary to consider.

      We had mentioned this limitation in the discussion: Nevertheless, it will be important in future studies to examine if similar subcortical responses are obtained when stimuli are unattended (Wyart & TallonBaudry, 2008), task-irrelevant (Shafto & Pitts, 2015), or when participants passively experience stimuli without the instruction to report them (i.e., no-report paradigms) (Tsuchyia et al., 2015). We agree that such a control would have been relevant, but this was not feasible during the 10 minutes allotted for the research task in an intraoperative setting. These constraints are both clinical, to minimize discomfort for patients and practical, as is difficult to track neurons in an intraoperative setting for more than 10 minutes.

      We added a sentence to this effect in the discussion.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This important study relies on a rare dataset: intracranial recordings within the thalamus and the subthalamic nucleus in awake humans, while they were performing a tactile detection task. This procedure allowed the authors to identify a small but significant proportion of individual neurons, in both structures, whose activity correlated with the task (e.g. their firing rate changed following the audio cue signalling the start of a trial) and/or with the stimulus presentation (change in firing rate around 200 ms following tactile stimulation) and/or with participant's reported subjective perception of the stimulus (difference between hits and misses around 200 ms following tactile stimulation). Whereas most studies interested in the neural underpinnings of conscious perception focus on cortical areas, these results suggest that subcortical structures might also play a role in conscious perception, notably tactile detection.

      Strengths:

      There are two strongly valuable aspects in this study that make the evidence convincing and even compelling. First, these types of data are exceptional, the authors could have access to subcortical recordings in awake and behaving humans during surgery. Additionally, the methods are solid. The behavioral study meets the best standards of the domain, with a careful calibration of the stimulation levels (staircase) to maintain them around the detection threshold, and an additional selection of time intervals where the behavior was stable. The authors also checked that stimulus intensity was the same on average for hits and misses within these selected periods, which warrants that the effects of detection that are observed here are not confounded by stimulus intensity. The neural data analysis is also very sound and well-conducted. The statistical approach complies with current best practices, although I found that, in some instances, it was not entirely clear which type of permutations had been performed, and I would advocate for more clarity in these instances. Globally the figures are nice, clear, and well presented. I appreciated the fact that the precise anatomical location of the neurons was directly shown in each figure.

      We thank the reviewer for this positive evaluation.

      Weaknesses:

      Some clarification is needed for interpreting Figure 3, top rows: in my understanding the black curve is already the result of a subtraction between stimulus present trials and catch trials, to remove potential drifts; if so, it does not make sense to compare it with the firing rate recorded for catch trials.

      The black curve represents the firing rate without any subtraction. We only subtracted the firing rates of catch trials in the statistical procedure, as the reviewer noted, to remove potential drift. We added (before baseline correction) to the legend of Figure 3.

      I also think that the article could benefit from a more thorough presentation of the data and that this could help refine the interpretation which seems to be a bit incomplete in the current version. There are 8 stimulus-responsive neurons and 8 perception-selective neurons, with only one showing both effects, resulting in a total of 15 individual neurons being in either category or 13 neurons if we exclude those in which the behavior is not good enough for the hit versus miss analysis (Figure S4A). In my opinion, it should be feasible to show the data for all of them (either in a main figure, or at least in supplementary), but in the present version, we get to see the data for only 3 neurons for each analysis. This very small selection includes the only neuron that shows both effects (neuron #001; which is also cue selective), but this is not highlighted in the text. It would be interesting to see both the stimulus-response data and the hit versus miss data for all 13 neurons as it could help develop the interpretation of exactly how these neurons might be involved in stimulus processing and conscious perception. This should give rise to distinct interpretations for the three possible categories. Neurons that are stimulus-responsive but not perception-selective should show the same response for both hits and misses and hence carry out indifferently conscious and unconscious responses. The fact that some neurons show the opposite pattern is particularly intriguing and might give rise to a very specific interpretation: if the neuron really doesn't tend to respond to the stimulus when hits and misses are put together, it might be a neuron that does not directly respond to the stimulus, but whose spontaneous fluctuations across trials affect how the stimulus is perceived when they occur in a specific time window after the stimulus. Finally, neuron #001 responds with what looks like a real burst of evoked activity to stimulation and also shows a difference between hits and misses, but intriguingly, the response is strongest for misses. In the discussion, the interesting interpretation in terms of a specific gating of information by subcortical structures seems to apply well to this last example, but not necessarily to the other categories.

      We now provide a supplementary Figure showing firing rates for hits, misses and the combination of both. The reviewer’s analysis about whether a perception-selective neuron also has to respond to the stimulus to be involved in gating is interesting. With more data, a finer characterization of these neurons would have been possible. In our study, it is possible that more neurons have similar characteristics as #001 (e.g. #032, #062, #068) but do not show a significant difference with respect to baseline when both hits and misses are considered. We now avoid interpreting null effects, especially considering the low number of trials with near-threshold detection behavior we could collect in 10 minutes. 

      We also realized that we had not updated Figure S7 after the last revision in which we had corrected for possible drifts to obtain sensory-selective neurons. The corrected panel A is provided below.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      It appears that the correct rejection was low for most participants. It would improve interpretation of the behavioral results if correct rejection was shown as a rate (i.e., # of correct rejection trials / total number of no stimulus/blank trials) rather than or in addition to reporting the number of correct rejection trials (Figure 1C).

      We added the following figure to the supplementary information.

      The axis tick marks in Figure 5A late versus early are incorrect (appears the axis was duplicated).

      Thank you for spotting this, it has been corrected.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      We would like to congratulate the authors on this strongly supported contribution to the field. The manuscript is well-written, although a little bit too concise in sections. See the following comments for the methods that could benefit the present conclusions:

      Thank you for these suggestions that we believe improved our interpretations.

      Major Points

      (1) The subpopulations of neurons that are considered are small, but it is not a confounding issue for the conclusions drawn. However, the behavior of the neurons that were excluded should be considered by calculating the percentage of neurons that are selective for the distinct parameters, as a function of time. This would greatly strengthen the understanding of what can be observed in the two subcortical structures.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We performed a new analysis of the latencies at which our main effects were observed. This analysis revealed the existence of two clusters, as shown in the new Figure 5 copied below

      We also performed a new analysis to support the existence of bimodal distributions and quantified the latencies. We added this text to the result section:

      We note that the timings of sensory and perception effects in Figures 3 and 4 showed a bimodal distribution with an early cluster (149 ms for sensory neurons; 121 ms for perception neurons; c.f. methods) and a later cluster (330 ms for sensory neurons; 315 ms for perception neurons; Figure 5). and this section to the methods:

      To measure bimodal timings of effect latencies, we fitted a two-component Gaussian mixture distribution to the data in Figure 5 by minimizing the mean square error with an interior-point method. We took the best of 20 runs with random initialization points and verified that the resulting mean square error was markedly (> 4 times) better than using a single component.

      We also updated the discussion:

      The early cluster’s average timing around 150 ms post-stimulus corresponds to the onset of a putative cortical correlate of tactile consciousness, the somatosensory awareness negativity (Dembski et al., 2021). Similar electroencephalographic markers are found in the visual and auditory modality. It is unclear, however, whether these markers are related to perceptual consciousness or selective attention (Dembski et al., 2021). The later cluster is centered around 300 ms and could correspond to a well known electroencephalographic marker, the P3b (Polich, 2007) whose association with perceptual consciousness has been questioned (Pitts et al., 2014; Dembski et al., 2021) although brain activity related to consciousness has been observed at similar timing even in the absence of report demands (Sergent et al., 2021; Stockart et al., 2024). It is also important to note that these clusters contain neurons with both increased and decreased firing rates following stimulus onset, similar to what was observed previously in the posterior parietal cortex (Pereira et al., 2021).

      (2) We highly recommend that the authors consider employing some analysis that decodes therepresentations observable in the activity of individual neurons as a function of time (e.g. Shannon's Mutual Information). This would reinforce and emphasize the most relevant conclusions.

      We thank the reviewers for this suggestion. Unfortunately, such methods would require many more trials than what we were able to collect in the 10-minute slots available in the operating room.

      (3) Although there are small populations recorded in each of the two subcortical structures, they aresufficient to attempt a study using population dynamics (primarily, PCA can still work with smaller populations). Given the broad range of dynamics that are observed in a population of single units typically involved in decision-making, it would be interesting to consider whether heterogeneity is a hallmark of decision-making, and trying to summarize the variance in the activity of the entire population should provide a certain understanding of the cue-selective versus the perception-selective qualities, as an example.

      We now present all 13 neurons that were sensory- or perception-selective for which we had good enough behavior to show hit vs. miss differences in Supplementary Figure S5. Although population-level analyses would be relevant, they are not compatible with the number of neurons we identified.

      (4) A stronger presentation of what the expectations are for the results would also benefit theinterpretability of the manuscript when added to the introduction and discussion sections.

      Due to the scarcity of single-neuron data related to perceptual consciousness, especially in the subcortical structures we explored, our prior expectations did not exceed finding perception-selective neurons. We would prefer to avoid refining these expectations post-hoc. 

      Minor Comments

      (1) Add the shared overlap between differently selective neurons explicitly in the manuscript.

      We added this information at the end of the results section.

      (2) Add a consideration in the methods of why the Wilcoxon test or permutation test was selected forseparate uses. How do the results compare?

      Sorry for this misunderstanding. We clarified this in revised methods:

      To deal with possibly non-parametric distributions, we used Wilcoxon rank sum test or sign test instead of t-tests to test differences between distributions. We used permutation tests instead of Binomial tests to test whether a reported number of neurons could have been obtained by chance.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Suggestions for improved or additional experiments, data or analysis:

      As suggested already in the public review, it might be worth showing all 13 neurons with either stimulusresponsive or perception-selective behaviour and, based on that, deepen the potential interpretation of the results for the different categories.

      We agree that this information improves the understanding of the underlying data and this addition was also proposed by reviewer 2. We added it in a new supplementary Figure S5.

      Recommendations for improving the writing and presentation

      As mentioned in the public review, I think Figure 3 needs clarification. I found that, in some instances, it was not entirely clear which type of analyses or permutation tests had been performed, and I would advocate for more clarity in these instances. For example:

      Page 6 line 146 "permuting trial labels 1000 times": do you mean randomly attributing a trial to aneuron? Or something else?

      We agree that this was somewhat unclear. We modified the sentence to:

      permuting the sign of the trial-wise differences

      We now define a sign permutation test for paired tests and a trial permutation test for two-sample tests in the methods and specify which test was used in the maintext.

      Page 7, neurons which have their firing rate modulated by the stimulus: I think you ought to be moreexplicit about the analysis so that we grasp it on the first read. To understand what is shown in Figure 3 I had to go back and forth between the main text and the method, and I am still not sure I completely understood. You compare the firing rate in sliding windows following stimulus onset with the mean firing rate during the 300ms baseline. Sliding windows are between 0 and 400 ms post-stim (according to methods ?) and a neuron is deemed responsive if you find at least one temporal cluster that shows a significant difference with baseline activity (using cluster permutation). Is that correct? Either way, I would recommend being a bit more precise about the analysis that was carried out in the main text, so that we only need to refer to methods when we need specialized information.

      We agree that the methods section was unclear. We re-wrote the following two paragraphs:

      To identify sensory-selective neurons, we assumed that subcortical signatures of stimulus detection ought to be found early following its onset and looked for differences in the firing rates during the first 400 ms post-stimulus onset compared to a 300 ms pre-stimulus baseline. To correct for possible drifts occurring during the trial, we subtracted the average cue-locked activity from catch trials to the cuelocked activity of each stimulus-present trials before realigning to stimulus onset. We defined a cluster as a set of adjacent time points for which the firing rates were significantly different between hits and misses, as assessed by a non-parametric sign rank test. A putative neuron was considered sensory-selective when the length of a cluster was above 80 ms, corresponding to twice the standard deviation of the smoothing kernel used to compute the firing rate. Whether for the shuffled data or the observed data, if more than one cluster was obtained, we discarded all but the longest cluster. This permutation test allowed us to control for multiple comparisons across time and participants.

      For perception-selective neurons, we looked for differences in the firing rates between hit and miss trials during the first 400 ms post-stimulus onset. We defined a cluster as a set of adjacent time points for which the firing rates were significantly different between hits and misses as assessed by a nonparametric Wilcoxon rank sum test. As for sensory-selective neurons, a putative neuron was considered perception-selective when the length of a cluster was above 80 ms, corresponding to twice the standard deviation of the smoothing kernel used to compute the firing rate and we discarded all but the longest cluster.

      Minor points:

      Figure 3: inset showing action potentials, please also provide the time scale (in the legend for example), so that it's clear that it is not commensurate with the firing rate curve below, but rather corresponds to the dots of the raster plot.

      We added the text ”[...], duration: 2.5 ms” in Figures 2, 3, and 4.

      Line 210: I recommend: “we found 8 neurons [...] showing a significant difference *between hits and misses* after stimulus onset."

      We made the change.

      Top of page 9, the following sentence is misleading “This result suggests that neurons in these two subcortical structures have mostly different functional roles ; this could read as meaning that functional roles are different between the two structures. Probably what you mean is rather something along this line : “these two subcortical structures both contain neurons displaying several different functional roles”

      Changed.

      Line 329: remove double “when”

      We made the change, thank you for spotting this.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      We would like to thank you for your valuable comments and suggestions, which have greatly contributed to improving our manuscript.

      We have carefully addressed all the reviewers' suggestions, and detailed responses for each Reviewer are provided at the end of this letter. In summary:

      • The Introduction has been revised to provide a more focused discussion on results, toning down the speculative discussion on seasonal host shifts.

      • The methodology section has been clarified, particularly the power analysis, which now includes a clearer explanation. The random effects in the models have been better described to ensure transparency.

      • The Results section was reorganized to highlight the key findings more effectively.

      • The Discussion has been restructured for clarity and conciseness, ensuring the interpretation of the results is clearer and better aligned with the study objectives.

      • Minor edits throughout the manuscript were made to improve readability and accuracy.

      We hope you find this revised version of the manuscript satisfactory.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study examines the role of host blood meal source, temperature, and photoperiod on the reproductive traits of Cx. quinquefasciatus, an important vector of numerous pathogens of medical importance. The host use pattern of Cx. quinquefasciatus is interesting in that it feeds on birds during spring and shifts to feeding on mammals towards fall. Various hypotheses have been proposed to explain the seasonal shift in host use in this species but have provided limited evidence. This study examines whether the shifting of host classes from birds to mammals towards autumn offers any reproductive advantages to Cx.

      quinquefasciatus in terms of enhanced fecundity, fertility, and hatchability of the offspring. The authors found no evidence of this, suggesting that alternate mechanisms may drive the seasonal shift in host use in Cx. quinquefasciatus.

      Strengths:

      Host blood meal source, temperature, and photoperiod were all examined together.

      Weaknesses:

      The study was conducted in laboratory conditions with a local population of Cx. quinquefasciatus from Argentina. I'm not sure if there is any evidence for a seasonal shift in the host use pattern in Cx. quinquefasciatus populations from the southern latitudes.

      Comments on the revision:

      Overall, the manuscript is much improved. However, the introduction and parts of the discussion that talk about addressing the question of seasonal shift in host use pattern of Cx. quin are still way too strong and must be toned down. There is no strong evidence to show this host shift in Argentinian mosquito populations. Therefore, it is just misleading. I suggest removing all this and sticking to discussing only the effects of blood meal source and seasonality on the reproductive outcomes of Cx. quin.

      Introduction and discussion have been modified, toned down and sticked to discuss the results as suggested.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Some more minor comments are mentioned below.

      Line 51: Because 'of' this,

      Changed as suggested.

      Line 56: specialists 'or' generalists

      Changed as suggested.

      Line 56: primarily

      Changed as suggested.

      Line 98: Because 'of' this,

      Changed as suggested.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Conceptually, this study is interesting and is the first attempt to account for the potentially interactive effects of seasonality and blood source on mosquito fitness, which the authors frame as a possible explanation for previously observed hostswitching of Culex quinquefasciatus from birds to mammals in the fall. The authors hypothesize that if changes in fitness by blood source change between seasons, higher fitness on birds in the summer and on mammals in the autumn could drive observed host switching. To test this, the authors fed individuals from a colony of Cx. quinquefasciatus on chickens (bird model) and mice (mammal model) and subjected each of these two groups to two different environmental conditions reflecting the high and low temperatures and photoperiod experienced in summer and autumn in Córdoba, Argentina (aka seasonality). They measured fecundity, fertility, and hatchability over two gonotrophic cycles. The authors then used generalized linear mixed models to evaluate the impact of host species, seasonality, and gonotrophic cycle on fecundity, fertility, and hatchability. The authors were trying to test their hypothesis by determining whether there was an interactive effect of season and host species on mosquito fitness. This is an interesting hypothesis; if it had been supported, it would provide support for a new mechanism driving host switching. While the authors did report an interactive impact of seasonality and host species, the directionality of the effect was the opposite from that hypothesized. The authors have done a very good job of addressing many of the reviewer's concerns, especially by adding two additional replicates. Several minor concerns remain, especially regarding unclear statements in the discussion.

      Strengths:

      (1) Using a combination of laboratory feedings and incubators to simulate seasonal environmental conditions is a good, controlled way to assess the potentially interactive impact of host species and seasonality on the fitness of Culex quinquefasciatus in the lab.

      (2) The driving hypothesis is an interesting and creative way to think about a potential driver of host switching observed in the field.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The methods would be improved by some additional details. For example, clarifying the number of generations for which mosquitoes were maintained in colony (which was changed from 20 to several) and whether replicates were conducted at different time points.

      Changed as suggested.

      (2) The statistical analysis requires some additional explanation. For example, you suggest that the power analysis was conducted a priori, but this was not mentioned in your first two drafts, so I wonder if it was actually conducted after the first replicate. It would be helpful to include further detail, such as how the parameters were estimated. Also, it would be helpful to clarify why replicate was included as a random effect for fecundity and fertility but as a fixed effect for hatchability. This might explain why there were no significant differences for hatchability given that you were estimating for more parameters.

      The power analysis was conducted a posteriori, as you correctly inferred. While I did not indicate that it was performed a priori, you are right in noting that this was not explicitly mentioned. As you suggested, the methodology for the power analysis has been revised to clarify any potential doubts.

      Regarding the model for hatchability, a model without a random effect variable was used, as all attempts to fit models with random effects resulted in poor validation. These points have now been clarified and explained in the corresponding section.

      (3) A number of statements in the discussion are not clear. For example, what do you mean by a mixed perspective in the first paragraph? Also, why is the expectation mentioned in the second paragraph different from the hypothesis you described in your introduction?

      Changed as suggested.

      (4) According to eLife policy, data must be made freely available (not just upon request).

      Data and code will be publicly available. The corresponding section was modified.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Your manuscript is much improved by the inclusion of two additional replicates! The results are much more robust when we can see that the trends that you report are replicable across 3 iterations of the experiment. Congratulations on a greatly improved study and paper! I have several minor concerns and suggestions, listed below:

      38-39: I think it is clearer to say "no statistically significant effect of season on hatchability of eggs" ... or specify if you are referring to blood or the interaction of blood and season. It isn't clear which treatment you are referring to here.

      Changed as suggested.

      54-57: This could be stated more succinctly. Instead of citing papers that deal with specific examples of patterns, I would suggest citing a review paper that defines these terms.

      Changed as suggested.

      83-84: What if another migratory bird is the preferred host in Argentina? I would state this more cautiously (e.g. "may not be applicable...").

      Changed as suggested.

      95-96: I don't understand what you mean by this. These hypotheses are specifically meant to understand mosquitoes that DO have a distinct seasonal phenology, so I'm not sure why this caveat is relevant. And naturally this hypothesis is host dependent, since it is based on specific host reproductive investments. I think that the strongest caveat to this hypothesis is simply that it hasn't been proven.

      Changed as suggested.

      97-115: This is a great paragraph! Very clear and compelling.

      Thanks for your words!

      118: Do you have an exact or estimated number of rafts collected?

      Sorry, I have not the exact number of rafts, but it was at leas more than 20-30.

      135: "over twenty" was changed to "several"; several would imply about 3 generations, so this is misleading. If the colony was actually maintained for over twenty generations, then you should keep that wording.

      Changed as suggested.

      163-164: Can you please clarify whether the replicates were conducted a separate time points?

      Changed as suggested.

      Note: the track changes did not capture all of the changes made; e.g. 163-164 should show as new text but does not.

      You are absolutely right; when I uploaded the last version, I unfortunately deleted all tracked changes and cannot recover them. In this new version, I will ensure that all minimal changes are included as tracked changes.

      186 - 189: the terms should be "fixed effect" and "random effect"

      Changed as suggested.

      191: Edit: linear

      Changed as suggested.

      194: why was replicate not included as a random effect here when it was above? Also, can you please clarify "interaction effects"? Which interactions did you include?

      Changed as suggested. Explained above and in methodology. Hatchability models with random effect variable were poor fitted and validated. The interactions for hatchability were a four-way (season, blood source, cycle and replicate)

      207-208: I'm not sure what you mean by "aimed to achieve"? Weren't you doing this after you conducted the experiments, so wouldn't this be determining the power of your model (post-hoc power analysis)? Also, I think you should provide the parameter estimates that were used (e.g. effect size - did you use the effect size you estimated across the 3 replicates?).

      Changed as suggested.

      214-215: this should be reworded to acknowledge that this is estimated for the given effect size; for example, something like "This sample size was sufficient to detect the observed effect with a statistical power of 0.8" or something along those lines (unless I am misunderstanding how you conducted this test).

      Changed as suggested.

      246. Abbreviate Culex

      Changed as suggested.

      253-255: This sentence isn't clear. What do you mean by mixed? Also, the season really seemed to mainly impact the fitness of mosquitoes fed on mouse blood and here the way it is phrased seems to indicate that season has an impact on the fitness of those fed with chicken blood.

      Changed as suggested.

      258-260: You stated your hypothesis as the relative fitness shifting between seasons, but this statement about the expectation is different from your hypothesis stated earlier. Please clarify.

      You are right. Thank you for noting this. It was changed as suggested.  

      263-266: I also don't understand this sentence; what does the first half of the sentence have to do with the second?

      Changed as suggested.

      269-270: This doesn't align with your observation exactly; you say first AND second are generally most productive, but you observed a drop in the second. Please clarify this.

      Changed as suggested.

      280: I suggest removing "as same as other studies"; your caveats are distinct because your experimental design was unique

      Changed as suggested.

      287: you shouldn't be looking for a "desired" effect; I suggest removing this word

      Changed as suggested.

      288: It wasn't really a priori though, since you conducted it after your first replicate (unless you didn't use the results from the first replicate you reported in the original drafts?)

      It was a posteriori. Changed as suggested.

      290: Why is 290 written here?

      It was a mistype. Deleted as suggested.

      291-298: The meaning of this section of your paragraph is not clear.

      Improve as suggested.

      304-313: This list of 3 explanations are directed at different underlying questions. Explanations 1 and 2 are alternative explanations for why host switching occurs if not due to differences in fitness. This isn't really an explanation of your results so much as alternative explanations for a previously reported phenomenon. And the third is an explanation for why you may not have observed the expected effect. I suggest restructuring this to include the fact that Argentinian quinqs may not host switch as part of your previous list of caveats. Then you can include your two alternative explanations for host switching as a possible future direction (although I would say that it is really just one explanation because "vector biology" is too broad of a statement to be testable). Also, you haven't discussed possible explanations for your actual result, which showed that mosquito fitness decreased when feeding on mouse blood in autumn conditions and in the second gonotrophic, while those that fed on chicken did not experience these changes. Why might that be?

      The discussion was restructured to include all these suggested changes. Additionally, it was also discussed some possible explanations of our results.

      315-317: This statement is vague without a direct explanation of how this will provide insight. I suggest removing or providing an explanation of how this provides insight to transmission and forecasting.

      Changed as suggested.

      319-320: According to eLife policy, all data should be publicly available. From guidelines: "Media Policy FAQs Data Availability Purpose and General Principles To maintain high standards of research reproducibility, and to promote the reuse of new findings, eLife requires all data associated with an article to be made freely and widely available. These must be in the most useful formats and according to the relevant reporting standards, unless there are compelling legal or ethical reasons to restrict access. The provision of data should comply with FAIR principles (Findable, Accessible, Interoperable, Reusable). Specifically, authors must make all original data used to support the claims of the paper, or that is required to reproduce them, available in the manuscript text, tables, figures or supplementary materials, or at a trusted digital repository (the latter is recommended). This must include all variables, treatment conditions, and observations described in the manuscript. The authors must also provide a full account of the materials and procedures used to collect, pre-process, clean, generate and analyze the data that would enable it to be independently reproduced by other researchers."

      - so you need to make your data available online; I also understand the last sentence to indicate that code should be made available.  

      Data and code will be publicly available.

      Table 1: it is notable that in replicate 2, the autumn:mouse:gonotrophic cycle II fecundity and fertility are actually higher than in the summer, which is the opposite of reps 1 and 3 and the overall effect you reported from the model. This might be worth mentioning in the discussion.

      Mentioned in the discussion as suggested.

      Tables 1 and 2: shouldn't this just be 8 treatments? You included replicate as a random effect, so it isn't really a separate set of treatments.

      This table reflects the output of the whole experiment, that is why it is present the 24 expetiments.

      Figure 3: Can you please clarify if this is showing raw data?

      Changed as suggested.

      Note: grammatical copy editing would be beneficial throughout

      Grammar was improved as suggested.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In this study, Tian et al. explore the role of ubiquitination of non-structural protein 16 (nsp16) in the SARS-CoV-2 life cycle. nsp16, in conjunction with nsp10, performs the final step of viral mRNA capping through its 2'-O-methylase activity. This modification allows the virus to evade host immune responses and protects its mRNA from degradation. The authors demonstrate that nsp16 undergoes ubiquitination and subsequent degradation by the host E3 ubiquitin ligases UBR5 and MARCHF7 via the ubiquitin-proteasome system (UPS). Specifically, UBR5 and MARCHF7 mediate nsp16 degradation through K48- and K27-linked ubiquitination, respectively. Notably, degradation of nsp16 by either UBR5 or MARCHF7 operates independently, with both mechanisms effectively inhibiting SARS-CoV-2 replication in vitro and in vivo. Furthermore, UBR5 and MARCHF7 exhibit broad-spectrum antiviral activity by targeting nsp16 variants from various SARS-CoV-2 strains. This research advances our understanding of how nsp16 ubiquitination impacts viral replication and highlights potential targets for developing broadly effective antiviral therapies.

      Strengths:

      The proposed study is of significant interest to the virology community because it aims to elucidate the biological role of ubiquitination in coronavirus proteins and its impact on the viral life cycle. Understanding these mechanisms will address broadly applicable questions about coronavirus biology and enhance our overall knowledge of ubiquitination's diverse functions in cell biology. Employing in vivo studies is a strength.

      Weaknesses:

      Minor comments:

      Figure 5A- The authors should ensure that the figure is properly labeled to clearly distinguish between the IP (Immunoprecipitation) panel and the input panel.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have exchanged Figure 5 in this version.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript "SARS-CoV-2 nsp16 is regulated by host E3 ubiquitin ligases, UBR5 and MARCHF7" is an interesting work by Tian et al. describing the degradation/ stability of NSP16 of SARS CoV2 via K48 and K27-linked Ubiquitination and proteasomal degradation. The authors have demonstrated that UBR5 and MARCHF7, an E3 ubiquitin ligase bring about the ubiquitination of NSP16. The concept, and experimental approach to prove the hypothesis looks ok. The in vivo data looks ok with the controls. Overall, the manuscript is good.

      Strengths:

      The study identified important E3 ligases (MARCHF7 and UBR5) that can ubiquitinate NSP16, an important viral factor.

      Comments on revisions:

      I had gone through the revised form of the manuscript thoroughly. The authors have addressed all of my concerns. To me, the experimental approach looks convincing that the host E3 ubiquitin ligases (UBR5 and MARCHF7) ubiquitinate NSP16 and mark it for proteasomal degradation via K48- and K27- linkage. The authors have represented the final figure (Fig.8) in a convincing manner, opening a new window to explore the mechanism of capping the vRNA bu NSP16.

      Thank you for your recognition.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors investigated the effect of chronic activation of dopamine neurons using chemogenetics. Using Gq-DREADDs, the authors chronically activated midbrain dopamine neurons and observed that these neurons, particularly their axons, exhibit increased vulnerability and degeneration, resembling the pathological symptoms of Parkinson's disease. Baseline calcium levels in midbrain dopamine neurons were also significantly elevated following the chronic activation. Lastly, to identify cellular and circuit-level changes in response to dopaminergic neuronal degeneration caused by chronic activation, the authors employed spatial genomics (Visium) and revealed comprehensive changes in gene expression in the mouse model subjected to chronic activation. In conclusion, this study presents novel data on the consequences of chronic hyperactivation of midbrain dopamine neurons.

      Strengths:

      This study provides direct evidence that the chronic activation of dopamine neurons is toxic and gives rise to neurodegeneration. In addition, the authors achieved the chronic activation of dopamine neurons using water application of clozapine-N-oxide (CNO), a method not commonly employed by researchers. This approach may offer new insights into pathophysiological alterations of dopamine neurons in Parkinson's disease. The authors also utilized state-of-the-art spatial gene expression analysis, which can provide valuable information for other researchers studying dopamine neurons. Although the authors did not elucidate the mechanisms underlying dopaminergic neuronal and axonal death, they presented a substantial number of intriguing ideas in their discussion, which are worth further investigation.

      We thank the reviewer for these positive comments.

      Weaknesses:

      Many claims raised in this paper are only partially supported by the experimental results. So, additional data are necessary to strengthen the claims. The effects of chronic activation of dopamine neurons are intriguing; however, this paper does not go beyond reporting phenomena. It lacks a comprehensive explanation for the degeneration of dopamine neurons and their axons. While the authors proposed possible mechanisms for the degeneration in their discussion, such as differentially expressed genes, these remain experimentally unexplored.

      We thank the reviewer for this review. We do believe that the manuscript has a substantial mechanistic component, as the central experiments involve direct manipulation of neuronal activity, and we show an increase in calcium levels and gene expression changes in dopamine neurons that coincide with the degeneration. However, we agree that deeper mechanistic investigation would strengthen the conclusions of the paper. We have executed several important revisions, including the addition of CNO behavioral controls, manipulation of intracellular calcium using isradipine, additional transcriptomics experiments and further validation of findings. We believe that these additions significantly bolster the conclusions of the paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Rademacher et al. present a paper showing that chronic chemogenetic excitation of dopaminergic neurons in the mouse midbrain results in differential degeneration of axons and somas across distinct regions (SNc vs VTA). These findings are important. This mouse model also has the advantage of showing a axon-first degeneration over an experimentally-useful time course (2-4 weeks). 2. The findings that direct excitation of dopaminergic neurons causes differential degeneration sheds light on the mechanisms of dopaminergic neuron selective vulnerability. The evidence that activation of dopaminergic neurons causes degeneration and alters mRNA expression is convincing, as the authors use both vehicle and CNO control groups, but the evidence that chronic dopaminergic activation alters circadian rhythm and motor behavior is incomplete as the authors did not run a CNO-control condition in these experiments.

      Strengths:

      This is an exciting and important paper.

      The paper compares mouse transcriptomics with human patient data.

      It shows that selective degeneration can occur across the midbrain dopaminergic neurons even in the absence of a genetic, prion, or toxin neurodegeneration mechanism.

      We thank the reviewer for these comments.

      Weaknesses:

      Major concerns:

      (1) The lack of a CNO-positive, DREADD-negative control group in the behavioral experiments is the main limitation in interpreting the behavioral data. Without knowing whether CNO on its own has an impact on circadian rhythm or motor activity, the certainty that dopaminergic hyperactivity is causing these effects is lacking.

      We thank the reviewer for this important recommendation. Although the initial version showed that CNO does not produce degeneration of DA neuron terminals, it did not exclude a contribution to the behavioral changes. To address this, we now include a cohort of DREADD free non-injected mice treated with either vehicle or CNO (Figure S1C). We found that on its own, CNO did not significantly impact either light cycle or dark cycle running. Together these results along with the lack of degeneration observed with CNO treatment in non-DREADD mice (Figure 2D) support that our behavioral and histological results are the result of dopamine neuron activation.

      (2) One of the most exciting things about this paper is that the SNc degenerates more strongly than the VTA when both regions are, in theory, excited to the same extent. However, it is not perfectly clear that both regions respond to CNO to the same extent. The electrophysiological data showing CNO responsiveness is only conducted in the SNc. If the VTA response is significantly reduced vs the SNc response, then the selectivity of the SNc degeneration could just be because the SNc was more hyperactive than the VTA. Electrophysiology experiments comparing the VTA and SNc response to CNO could support the idea that the SNc has substantial intrinsic vulnerability factors compared to the VTA.

      We agree that additional electrophysiology conducted in the VTA dopamine neurons would meaningfully add to our understanding of the selective vulnerability in this model, and have completed these experiments in the revision (Figure 1, Figure S2). We now show that in vivo treatment with CNO causes some of the same physiological changes in VTA dopamine neurons as we found in SNc dopamine neurons, including an increased spontaneous firing rate, and a similar decrease in responsiveness to CNO in the slice recordings. Together these observations support the conclusion that SNc axons are intrinsically more vulnerable to increased activity than VTA dopamine axons. 

      (3) The mice have access to a running wheel for the circadian rhythm experiments. Running has been shown to alter the dopaminergic system (Bastioli et al., 2022) and so the authors should clarify whether the histology, electrophysiology, fiber photometry, and transcriptomics data are conducted on mice that have been running or sedentary.

      We have clarified which mice had access to a running wheel in the methods of our revision. Briefly, mice for histology, electrophysiology, and transcriptomics all had access to a running wheel during their treatment. The mice used for photometry underwent about 7 days of running wheel access approximately 3 weeks prior to the beginning of the experiment. The photometry headcaps prevented mice from having access to a running wheel in their home cage. Mice used for non-responder and non-hM3Dq (CNO alone) experiments also had access to a running wheel during their treatment. Mice used for the isradipine experiment did not have access to a running wheel, as the number of mice was too large and while unilateral hM3Dq expression allows for within-animal controls, it does not lend to clear interpretation of running wheel data.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Rademacher and colleagues examined the effect on the integrity of the dopamine system in mice of chronically stimulating dopamine neurons using a chemogenetic approach. They find that one to two weeks of constant exposure to the chemogenetic activator CNO leads to a decrease in the density of tyrosine hydroxylase staining in striatal brain sections and to a small reduction of the global population of tyrosine hydroxylase positive neurons in the ventral midbrain. They also report alterations in gene expression in both regions using a spatial transcriptomics approach. Globally, the work is well done and valuable and some of the conclusions are interesting. However, the conceptual advance is perhaps a bit limited in the sense that there is extensive previous work in the literature showing that excessive depolarization of multiple types of neurons associated with intracellular calcium elevations promotes neuronal degeneration. The present work adds to this by showing evidence of a similar phenomenon in dopamine neurons.

      We thank the reviewer for the careful and thoughtful review of our manuscript.

      While extensive depolarization and associated intracellular calcium elevations promote degeneration generally, we emphasize that the process we describe is novel. Indeed, prior studies delivering chronic DREADDs to vulnerable neurons in models of Alzheimer’s disease did not detect an increase in neurodegeneration, despite seeing changes in protein aggregation (e.g. Yuan and Grutzendler, J Neurosci 2016, PMID: 26758850; Hussaini et al., PLOS Bio 2020, PMID: 32822389). Further, a critical finding from our study is that in our paradigm, this stressor does not impact all dopamine neurons equally, as the SNc DA neurons are more vulnerable than VTA DA neurons, mirroring selective vulnerability characteristic of Parkinson’s disease. This is consistent with a large body of literature that SNc dopamine neurons are less capable of handling large energetic and calcium loads compared to neighboring VTA neurons, and the finding that chronically altered activity is sufficient to drive this preferential loss is novel. In addition, we are not aware of prior studies that have chronically activated DREADDs over several weeks to produce neurodegeneration.

      In terms of the mechanisms explaining the neuronal loss observed after 2 to 4 weeks of chemogenetic activation, it would be important to consider that dopamine neurons are known from a lot of previous literature to undergo a decrease in firing through a depolarization-block mechanism when chronically depolarized. Is it possible that such a phenomenon explains much of the results observed in the present study? It would be important to consider this in the manuscript.

      Thank you for this comment. As discussed in greater detail in the “comments on results section” below, our data suggests this isn’t a prominent feature in our model. However, we cannot rule out a contribution of depolarization block, and have expanded on the discussion of this possibility in the revised manuscript.

      The relevance to Parkinson's disease (PD) is also not totally clear because there is not a lot of previous solid evidence showing that the firing of dopamine neurons is increased in PD, either in human subjects or in mouse models of the disease. As such, it is not clear if the present work is really modelling something that could happen in PD in humans.

      We completely agree that evidence of increased dopamine neuron activity from human PD patients is lacking, and the little data that exists is difficult to interpret without human controls. However, as we outline in the manuscript, multiple lines of evidence suggest that the activity level of dopamine neurons almost certainly does change in PD. Therefore, it is very important that we understand how changes in the level of neural activity influence the degeneration of DA neurons. In this paper we examine the impact of increased activity. Increased activity may be compensatory after initial dopamine neuron loss, or may be an initial driver of death (Rademacher & Nakamura, Exp Neurol 2024, PMID: 38092187). In addition to the human and rodent data already discussed in the manuscript, additional support for increased activity in PD models include:

      • Elevated firing rates in asymptomatic MitoPark mice (Good et al., FASEB J 2011, PMID: 21233488)

      • Increased frequency of spontaneous firing in patient-derived iPSC dopamine neurons and primary mouse dopamine neurons that overexpress synuclein (Lin et al., Acta Neuropath Comm 2021, PMID: 34099060)

      • Increased spontaneous firing in dopamine neurons of rats injected with synuclein preformed fibrils compared to sham (Tozzi et al., Brain 2021, PMID: 34297092)

      We have included citation of these important examples in our revision. In our model, we have found that chronic hyperactivity causes a substantial loss of nigral DA terminals while mesolimbic terminals are relatively spared (Figure 2), and that striatal DA levels are markedly decreased (Figure S6), phenomena that are hallmarks of Parkinson’s disease.

      There are additional levels of complexity to accurately model changes in PD, which may differ between subtypes of the disease, the disease stage, and the subtype of dopamine neuron. Our study models a form of increased intrinsic activity, and interpretation of our results will be facilitated as we learn more about how the activity of DA neurons changes in humans in PD. Similarly, in future studies, it will also be important to study the impact of decreasing DA neuron activity.

      Comments on the introduction:

      The introduction cites a 1990 paper from the lab of Anthony Grace as support of the fact that DA neurons increase their firing rate in PD models. However, in this 1990 paper, the authors stated that: "With respect to DA cell activity, depletions of up to 96% of striatal DA did not result in substantial alterations in the proportion of DA neurons active, their mean firing rate, or their firing pattern. Increases in these parameters only occurred when striatal DA depletions exceeded 96%." Such results argue that an increase in firing rate is most likely to be a consequence of the almost complete loss of dopamine neurons rather than an initial driver of neuronal loss. The present introduction would thus benefit from being revised to clarify the overriding hypothesis and rationale in relation to PD and better represent the findings of the paper by Hollerman and Grace.

      We agree that the findings of Hollerman and Grace support compensatory changes in dopamine neuron activity in response to loss of dopamine neurons, rather than informing whether dopamine neuron loss can also be an initial driver of activity. Importantly, while significant changes to burst firing were not seen until almost complete loss of dopamine neurons, these recordings were made in anesthetized rats which may not be representative of neural activity in awake animals. We adjusted the text so that this is no longer referred to as ‘partial’ loss. At the same time, we point out that the results of other studies on this point are mixed: a 50% reduction in dopamine neurons didn’t alter firing rate or bursting (Harden and Grace, J Neurosci 1995, PMID: 7666198; Bilbao et al., Brain Res 2006, PMID: 16574080), while a 40% loss was found to increase firing rate and bursting (Chen et al., Brain Res 2009. PMID: 19545547) and larger reductions alter burst firing (Hollerman & Grace, Brain Res 1990, PMID: 2126975; Stachowiak et al., J Neurosci 1987, PMID: 3110381). Importantly, even if compensatory, such late-stage increases in dopamine neuron activity may contribute to disease progression and drive a vicious cycle of degeneration in surviving neurons. In addition, we also don’t know how the threshold of dopamine neuron loss and altered activity may differ between mice and humans, and PD patients do not present with clinical symptoms until ~30-60% of nigral neurons are lost (Burke & O’Malley, Exp Neurol 2013, PMID: 22285449; Shulman et al., Annu Rev Pathol 2011, PMID: 21034221).   

      Other lines of evidence support the potential role of hyperactivity in disease initiation, including increased activity before dopamine neuron loss in MitoPark mice (Good et al., FASEB J 2011, PMID: 21233488), increased spontaneous firing in patient-derived iPSC dopamine neurons (Lin et al., Acta Neuropath Comm 2021, PMID: 34099060), and increased activity observed in genetic models of PD (Bishop et al., J Neurophysiol 2010, PMID: 20926611; Regoni et al., Cell Death Dis 2020, PMID: 33173027).

      It would be good that the introduction refers to some of the literature on the links between excessive neuronal activity, calcium, and neurodegeneration. There is a large literature on this and referring to it would help frame the work and its novelty in a broader context.

      We agree that a discussion of hyperactivity, calcium, and neurodegeneration would benefit the introduction. Accordingly, we have expanded on our citation of this literature in both the introduction and discussion sections. However, we believe that the novelty of our study lies in: 1) a chronic chemogenetic activation paradigm via drinking water, 2) demonstrating selective vulnerability of dopamine neurons as a result of altering their activity/excitability alone, and 3) comparing mouse and human spatial transcriptomics.

      Comments on the results section:

      The running wheel results of Figure 1 suggest that the CNO treatment caused a brief increase in running on the first day after which there was a strong decrease during the subsequent days in the active phase. This observation is also in line with the appearance of a depolarization block.

      The authors examined many basic electrophysiological parameters of recorded dopamine neurons in acute brain slices. However, it is surprising that they did not report the resting membrane potential, or the input resistance. It would be important that this be added because these two parameters provide key information on the basal excitability of the recorded neurons. They would also allow us to obtain insight into the possibility that the neurons are chronically depolarized and thus in depolarization block.

      We do report the input resistance in Figure S1C (now Figure S2A, S2B), which was unchanged in CNO-treated animals compared to controls. We did not previously report the resting membrane potential because many of the DA neurons were spontaneously firing. In the revision, we now report the initial membrane potential on first breaking into the cell for the whole cell recordings, which did not vary between groups (Figure S2). This is still influenced by action potential activity, but is the timepoint in the recording least impacted by dialyzing the neuron with the internal solution, which might alter the intracellular concentrations of ions. We observed increased spontaneous action potential activity ex vivo in slices from CNO-treated mice (Figure 1D), thus at least under these conditions these dopamine neurons are not in depolarization block. We also did not see strong evidence of changes in other intrinsic properties of the neurons with whole cell recordings (e.g. Figure S2). Overall, our electrophysiology experiments are not consistent with the depolarization block model, at least not due to changes in the intrinsic properties of the neurons. Although our ex vivo findings cannot exclude a contribution of depolarization block in vivo, we do show that CNO-treated mice removed from their cages for open field testing continue to have a strong trend for increased activity for approximately 10 days (Figure S4B). This finding is also consistent with increased activity of the DA neurons. We have added discussion of these important considerations in the revision.

      It is great that the authors quantified not only TH levels but also the levels of mCherry, coexpressed with the chemogenetic receptor. This could in principle help to distinguish between TH downregulation and true loss of dopamine neuron cell bodies. However, the approach used here has a major caveat in that the number of mCherry-positive dopamine neurons depends on the proportion of dopamine neurons that were infected and expressed the DREADD and this could very well vary between different mice. It is very unlikely that the virus injection allowed to infect 100% of the neurons in the VTA and SNc. This could for example explain in part the mismatch between the number of VTA dopamine neurons counted in panel 2G when comparing TH and mCherry counts. Also, I see that the mCherry counts were not provided at the 2-week time point. If the mCherry had been expressed genetically by crossing the DAT-Cre mice with a floxed fluorescent reported mice, the interpretation would have been simpler. In this context, I am not convinced of the benefit of the mCherry quantifications. The authors should consider either removing these results from the final manuscript or discussing this important limitation.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment, and we agree that this is a caveat of our mCherry quantification. Quantitation of the number of mCherry+ DA neurons specifically informs the impact on transduced DA neurons, and mCherry appears to be less susceptible to downregulation versus TH. As the reviewer points out, it carries the caveat that there is some variability between injections. Our control animals give us an indicator of injection variability, which is likely substantial and prevents us from detecting more subtle changes. Nonetheless, we believe that it conveys useful complementary data. We discuss this caveat in our revision. Note that mCherry was not quantified at the two-week timepoint because there is no loss of TH+ cells at that time.

      Although the authors conclude that there is a global decrease in the number of dopamine neurons after 4 weeks of CNO treatment, the post-hoc tests failed to confirm that the decrease in dopamine number was significant in the SNc, the region most relevant to Parkinson's. This could be due to the fact that only a small number of mice were tested. A "n" of just 4 or 5 mice is very small for a stereological counting experiment. As such, this experiment was clearly underpowered at the statistical level. Also, the choice of the image used to illustrate this in panel 2G should be reconsidered: the image suggests that a very large loss of dopamine

      neurons occurred in the SNc and this is not what the numbers show. A more representative image should be used.

      We agree that the stereology experiments were performed on relatively small numbers of animals, such that only robust effects would be detected. Combined with the small effect size, this may have contributed to the post-hoc tests showing a trend of p=0.1 for both the TH and mCherry dopamine cell counts in the SN at 4 weeks. Given this small effect size, we would indeed need much larger groups to better discern these changes. Stereology is an intensive technique, and we have therefore elected to focus on terminal loss. We have also replaced panel 2G with a more representative CNO image.

      In Figure 3, the authors attempt to compare intracellular calcium levels in dopamine neurons using GCaMP6 fluorescence. Because this calcium indicator is not quantitative (unlike ratiometric sensors such as Fura2), it is usually used to quantify relative changes in intracellular calcium. The present use of this probe to compare absolute values is unusual and the validity of this approach is unclear. This limitation needs to be discussed. The authors also need to refer in the text to the difference between panels D and E of this figure. It is surprising that the fluctuations in calcium levels were not quantified. I guess the hypothesis was that there should be more or larger fluctuations in the mice treated with CNO if the CNO treatment led to increased firing. This needs to be clarified.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We understand that this method of comparing absolute values is unconventional. However, these animals were tested concurrently on the same system, and a clear effect on the absolute baseline was observed. We have included a caveat of this in our discussion. Panel D of this figure shows the raw, uncorrected photometry traces, whereas panel E shows the isosbestic corrected traces for the same recording. In panel E, the traces follow time in ascending order. We have also included frequency and amplitude data for these recordings (Figure S4A), along with discussion of the significance of these findings.

      Although the spatial transcriptomic results are intriguing and certainly a great way to start thinking about how the CNO treatment could lead to the loss of dopamine neurons, the presented results, the focusing of some broad classes of differentially expressed genes and on some specific examples, do not really suggest any clear mechanism of neurodegeneration. It would perhaps be useful for the authors to use the obtained data to validate that a state of chronic depolarization was indeed induced by the chronic CNO treatment. Were genes classically linked to increased activity like cfos or bdnf elevated in the SNc or VTA dopamine neurons? In the striatum, the authors report that the levels of DARP32, a gene whose levels are linked to dopamine levels, are unchanged. Does this mean that there were no major changes in dopamine levels in the striatum of these mice?

      While levels of DARPP32 mRNA were unchanged, our additional HPLC data show strong decreases in striatal dopamine in hyperactivated mice. We do not see strong changes in classic activity-related genes (data not shown), however these genes may behave differently in the context of chronic hyperactivity and ongoing degeneration. Instead, we employed NEUROeSTIMator (Bahl et al., Nature Comm. 2024, PMID: 38278804), a deep learning method to predict neural activation based on transcriptomic data. We found that predicted activity scores were significantly higher in GqCNO dopaminergic regions compared to controls (Figure X). Indeed, some of the genes used within the model to predict activity are immediate early genes eg. c-fos.

      The usefulness of comparing the transcriptome of human PD SNc or VTA sections to that of the present mouse model should be better explained. In the human tissues, the transcriptome reflects the state of the tissue many years after extensive loss of dopamine neurons. It is expected that there will be few if any SNc neurons left in such sections. In comparison, the mice after 7 days of CNO treatment do not appear to have lost any dopamine neurons. As such, how can the two extremely different conditions be reasonably compared? Our mouse model and human PD progress over distinct timescales, as is the case with essentially all mouse models of neurodegenerative diseases. Nonetheless, in our view there is still great value in comparing gene expression changes in mouse models with those in human disease. It seems very likely that the same pathologic processes that drive degeneration early in the disease continue to drive degeneration later in the disease. Note that we have tried to address the discrepancy in time scales in part by comparing our mouse model to early PD samples when there is more limited SNc DA neuron loss (see the proportion of DA neurons within the areas of human tissues we selected for sampling in Author response image 1). Therefore, we can indeed use spatial transcriptomics to compare dopamine neurons from mice with initial degeneration to those in patients where degeneration is ongoing.    

      Author response image 1.

      Violin plot of DA neuron proportions sampled within the vulnerable SNV (deconvoluted RCTD method used in unmasked tissue sections of the SNV). Control and early PD subjects.

      Comments on the discussion:

      In the discussion, the authors state that their calcium photometry results support a central role of calcium in activity-induced neurodegeneration. This conclusion, although plausible because of the very broad pre-existing literature linking calcium elevation (such as in excitotoxicity) to neuronal loss, should be toned down a bit as no causal relationship was established in the experiments that were carried out in the present study.

      Our model utilizes hM3Dq-DREADDs that function by activating Gq pathways that are classically expected to increase intracellular calcium to increase neuronal excitability. Indeed in slices from mice that were not treated with CNO, acute CNO application caused depolarizations (Figure 1E) that can be due to an increase in intracellular calcium and also cause increases in intracellular calcium. Additionally, our results show increased calcium by fiber photometry and changes to calcium-related genes, suggesting a causal relation and crucial role of calcium in the mechanism of degeneration. However, we agree that we have not experimentally proven this point. Indeed, a small preliminary experiment with chronic isradipine failed to show protection, although it lacked power to detect a partial effect. We have acknowledged this in the text, and also briefly consider other mechanisms such as increased dopamine levels that could also mediate the toxicity.

      In the discussion, the authors discuss some of the parallel changes in gene expression detected in the mouse model and in the human tissues. Because few if any dopamine neurons are expected to remain in the SNc of the human tissues used, this sort of comparison has important conceptual limitations and these need to be clearly addressed.

      As discussed, we sampled SN DA neurons in early PD (see Author response image 1), and in our view there is great value for such comparisons.

      A major limitation of the present discussion is that it does not discuss the possibility that the observed phenotypes are caused by the induction of a chronic state of depolarization block by the chronic CNO treatment. I encourage the authors to consider and discuss this hypothesis.

      As discussed above, our analyses of DA neuron firing in slices and open field testing to date do not support a prominent contribution of depolarization block with chronic CNO treatment. However, we cannot rule out this hypothesis, therefore we have included additional electrophysiology experiments and have added discussion of this important consideration.  

      Also, the authors need to discuss the fact that previous work was only able to detect an increase in the firing rate of dopamine neurons after more than 95% loss of dopamine neurons. As such, the authors need to clearly discuss the relevance of the present model to PD. Are changes in firing rate a driver of neuronal loss in PD, as the authors try to make the case here, or are such changes only a secondary consequence of extensive neuronal loss (for example because a major loss of dopamine would lead to reduced D2 autoreceptor activation in the remaining neurons, and to reduced autoreceptor-mediated negative feedback on firing). This needs to be discussed.

      As discussed above, while increases in dopamine neuron activity may be compensatory after loss of neurons, the precise percentage required to induce such compensatory changes is not defined in mice and varies between paradigms, and the threshold level is not known in humans. We also reiterate that a compensatory increase in activity could still promote the degeneration of critical surviving DA neurons, whose loss underlies the substantial decline in motor function that typically occurs over the course of PD. Moreover, there are also multiple lines of evidence to suggest that changes in activity can initiate and drive dopamine neuron degeneration (Rademacher & Nakamura, Exp Neurol 2024). For example, overexpression of synuclein can increase firing in cultured dopamine neurons (Dagra et al., NPJ Parkinsons Dis 2021, PMID: 34408150), while mice expressing mutant Parkin have higher mean firing rates (Regoni et al., Cell Death Dis 2020, PMID: 33173027). Similarly, an increased firing rate has been reported in the MitoPark mouse model of PD at a time preceding DA neuron degeneration (Good et al., FASEB J 2011, PMID: 21233488). We also acknowledge that alterations to dopamine neuron activity are likely complex in PD, and that dopamine neuron health and function can be impacted not just by simple increases in activity, but also by changes in activity patterns and regularity. We have amended our discussion to include the important caveat of changes in activity occurring as compensation, as well as further evidence of changes in activity preceding dopamine neuron death.

      There is a very large, multi-decade literature on calcium elevation and its effects on neuronal loss in many different types of neurons. The authors should discuss their findings in this context and refer to some of this previous work. In a nutshell, the observations of the present manuscript could be summarized by stating that the chronic membrane depolarization induced by the CNO treatment is likely to induce a chronic elevation of intracellular calcium and this is then likely to activate some of the well-known calcium-dependent cell death mechanisms. Whether such cell death is linked in any way to PD is not really demonstrated by the present results. The authors are encouraged to perform a thorough revision of the discussion to address all of these issues, discuss the major limitations of the present model, and refer to the broad pre-existing literature linking membrane depolarization, calcium, and neuronal loss in many neuronal cell types.

      While our model demonstrates classic excitotoxic cell death pathways, we would like to emphasize both the chronic nature of our manipulation and the progressive changes observed, with increasing degeneration seen at 1, 2, and 4 weeks of hyperactivity in an axon-first manner. This is a unique aspect of our study, in contrast to much of the previous literature which has focused on shorter timescales. Thus, while we have revised the discussion to more comprehensively acknowledge previous studies of calcium-dependent neuron cell death, we believe we have made several new contributions that are not predicted by existing literature. We have shown that this chronic manipulation is specifically toxic to nigral dopamine neurons, and the data that VTA dopamine neurons continue to be resilient even at 4 weeks is interesting and disease-relevant. We therefore do not want to use findings from other neuron types to draw assumptions about DA neurons, which are a unique and very diverse population. We acknowledge that as with all preclinical models of PD, we cannot draw definitive conclusions about PD with this data. However, we reiterate that we strongly believe that drawing connections to human disease is important, as dopamine neuron activity is very likely altered in PD and a clearer understanding of how dopamine neuron survival is impacted by activity will provide insight into the mechanisms of PD.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The temporal design of the experiments is quite confusing. For instance, Figures 1 and 3 illustrate the daily changes of the mice and suggest some critical time points within 2 weeks of CNO administration, whereas Figure 2 presents data at 2 and 4 weeks, which are much later than the proposed critical time points. Furthermore, Figure 4 includes only 1 week data, and lacks subsequent data from 2 and 4 weeks, at which significant changes such as calcium levels and neuronal/axonal degeneration are observed.

      While interesting behavior and calcium phenotypes were detected within 2 and 4 weeks of CNO administration (Figures 1 and 3), we only collected tissues for histology at the 2 and 4 week time points (Figure 2). Observing degeneration of DA neuron axons but not cell bodies at 2 weeks served as a rationale to extend to the 4 week time point to determine whether degeneration was progressive. At the same time, our primary focus is on identifying early changes that may drive or contribute to the degeneration. As such, we recorded calcium changes over a 2-week treatment period, capturing the period during which almost all of the dopamine axons are lost. Similarly, we had the capacity to perform spatial transcriptomics at only one time point, and the 1 week time point was selected to capture transcriptomic changes that precede and potentially contribute to the mild and severe degeneration that occurs at 2 and 4 weeks, respectively. We have added text clarifying the rationale for the time points chosen.

      (2) The authors showed the changes in neuronal firing in dopamine neurons by the administration of CNO. However, one of the most important features of dopaminergic neuronal activity is dopamine release at its axon terminals in the striatum. Thus, the claims raised in this paper would be better supported if the authors further show any alterations in dopamine release (by FSCV or fluorescent dopamine sensors) at some critical time points during or after CNO application.

      While we are confident that DA release is altered due to the significant changes in behavior when hM3Dq DREADDs are activated specifically in DA neurons, the current manuscript does not quantify this, or distinguish between axonal and somatodendritic DA release. Interestingly, we did find significantly decreased striatal dopamine by HPLC after chronic activation (Figure S6). We believe that resolving these questions is beyond the scope of this manuscript, but have added text indicating the importance of these experiments.

      (3) The authors used 2% sucrose as a vehicle via drinking water. Please explain the rationale behind this choice.

      We used 2% sucrose as the vehicle because it is also added to the CNO water to counteract the bitterness of CNO (Kumar et al., J Neurotrauma 2024, PMID: 37905504). We have clarified this in the manuscript.

      (4) As we know, mRNA levels of some genes do not always predict their protein levels; there is sometimes a huge discrepancy between mRNA and protein abundance. In this paper, the mechanistic interpretation of the results by the authors heavily relies on the spatial transcriptomics of the midbrain and striatum. Thus, the authors need to provide additional data proving that the gene expression of some genes in the CNO group is also changed at the level of protein.

      We agree that validating hits at the protein level is valuable, however we were limited in our ability to assess these changes for the revision. However, we have done additional transcriptomics with the high resolution Xenium platform to increase confidence in a subset of hits of interest for follow up in future work, and we included data on genes related to DA metabolism and markers of DA neurons.

      (5) The authors provided spatial transcriptomics data only for mice with one week of chronic activation. However, other data also indicate significant differences when the activation period extends beyond 10 to 12 days (Figure 1C, Figure 3D-F). While a 7-day chronic activation time point might be crucial, additional transcriptomics data from later time points would be beneficial to confirm the persistence of these changes in gene expression. Furthermore, differential gene expression (DEG) analysis at these later time points could identify novel pathways or genes influenced by the chronic activation of dopamine neurons.

      This is an interesting point and would provide valuable data as to how chronic activity influences gene expression, however additional transcriptomics at later timepoints is beyond the scope of this paper. In future studies we will assess changes observed in this manuscript at other time points.

      (6) Figure 1D, Figure S1C:

      The authors should present the sample recording traces to demonstrate that the electrophysiological recordings were appropriately made.

      These data have been provided in Figure S2.

      (7) Figure S1C:

      AP thresholds in SNc dopamine neurons from both groups look quite high. In addition, considering the data from the previous reports, AP peak amplitudes in SNc dopamine neurons from both groups seem to be very low. Are these values correct? 

      The thresholds and peaks are correct, including the AP (threshold to peak), which is typical in our (Dr. Margolis’s) experience. AP thresholds are measured from an average of at least 10 APs, as the voltage at which the derivative of the trace first exceeds 10 V/s. As mentioned in the methods section, junction potentials were not corrected, which can result in values that are a bit depolarized from ground truth. This junction potential would be consistent across all recordings, thus not impede detection of a difference in AP thresholds between groups of animals.

      (8) Figure 1E:

      It would be better if the statistical significance is depicted in the graph.

      We don’t perform repeated measures statistics across data like these, as the data are continuous, collected at 10 kHz. For ease of displaying the data, the data for each neuron is binned and then these traces are averaged together. We display SEM to give a sense of the variance across neurons. We have provided sample traces of individual neurons to better demonstrate the variability and significance of this data (Figure S2).

      (9) Figure 2C:

      The representative staining images appear to be taken from coronal slices at anatomically different positions along the rostral-to-caudal axis. Although the total numbers of TH+ cells are comparable between vehicle and CNO groups in the graph, the sample images do not reflect this result. The authors should replace the current images with the better ones.

      We have replaced this image in the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Minor concerns:

      (1) The authors claim that their transcriptomics experiments are conducted 'before any degeneration has occurred'. And they do not see significant differences in the TH expression in the striatum. However, the n for these mice at 1 week is lower than the n use at 2 weeks (n=5 vs n=8-9) and the images used to show 'no degeneration' really look like there is some degeneration going on. Also, throughout the paper, there is a stronger effect when degeneration is measured with mCherry compared to when it is measured with TH. The 'no change' claim is made only with the TH comparison. It seems possible (and almost likely) that there would be significant axonal degeneration at one week with either a higher sample size or using the mCherry comparison. The authors should simply claim that their transcriptomics data is collected before any 'somatic' degeneration occurs.

      Thank you, we have included data that shows partial terminal loss after one week of activation (Figure S3B, Figure S5A) and have corrected this language in the manuscript to reflect transcriptomics occurring before somatic degeneration.

      (2) While selective degeneration is one of the most interesting findings in the paper, that finding is not emphasized and why it would be interesting to compare the VTA vs SNc is not discussed in the introduction.

      Emphasis for comparing the VTA vs the SNc has been added to the introduction, along with additional electrophysiology data in VTA dopamine neurons in Figure 1 and Figure S2.

      (3) In a similar direction, the vulnerability of dopaminergic neurons has been shown to be differential even within the SNc, with the ventral tier neurons degenerating more severely and the dorsal tier neurons remaining resilient. Is there any evidence for a ventral-dorsal degeneration gradient in the SNc in these experiments?

      This is a really interesting point and changes to dopamine neuron subtypes along the ventraldorsal axis may be occurring in this model, particularly as there is more selective loss of SNc neurons. However, the cell type involved would be difficult to determine at this stage, since single cell transcriptomic resolution is necessary across the entire SNc to identify cell subtypes. Transcriptomic identification is further complicated given that transcriptome change has recently been shown with genetic manipulation (Gaertner et al., bioRxiv 2024, PMID: 38895448), and we would think could similarly change with increased activity. Assessing these issues are beyond the scope of this paper.

      (4) The running data is very interesting and the circadian rhythm alterations are compelling.

      However, it is unclear whether the CNO mice run more total compared with the vehicle mice.

      The authors should show the combined total running data to evaluate this. We now show total running data in Figure 1C.

      (5) The finding that acute CNO has no effect on the membrane potential of SNc neurons after chronic CNO exposure is very peculiar! Especially because the fiber photometry data suggests that CNO continues to have an effect in vivo. Is there any explanation for this?

      While there is no acute electrophysiological response to CNO detected in this group, there may be intracellular pathways activated by the DREADD that do not acutely impact membrane potential in current clamp (I = 0 pA) mode.

      (6) The terminology of chronic CNO is sometimes confusing as it refers to both 2-week and 4week administration. Using additional terminology such as 'early' and 'late' might help with clarity.

      We have decreased usage of ‘chronic,’ and increased usage of more specific treatment times in order to increase clarity throughout the manuscript.

      (7) In Figure 2C, the SNc image looks binarized.

      This image has been updated.

      (8) Also in Figure 2, why are TH and mCherry measured for the 4-week time point, but only TH measured for the 2-week time point?

      mCherry quantification was performed to further support the finding of DA neuron death, and was therefore not assessed at 2 weeks given that there was no change in the TH stereology.

      (9) Additional scale bars and labeling is needed in Figure 3. In addition, there is such a strong reduction in noise after chronic CNO in the fiber photometry recordings, and the noise does not return upon CNO washout. What is the explanation for this?

      Additional scale bars were added to Figure 3. Traces are not getting less noisy with chronic CNO treatment, rather, there is less bursting activity in the dopamine cells. Our interpretation is that the baseline activity is rescued during washout but this bursting activity is not.

      (10) While not necessary to support the claims in this paper, it would be very interesting to see if chronic inhibition of dopaminergic neurons had a similar or different effect, as too little dopaminergic activity may also cause degeneration in some cases.

      We agree that assessing chronic inhibition is valuable, and this is an important area for future research.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      All the mice used in the study are not listed in the methods section. For example, the GCaMP6f floxed mice discussed in the results section are not listed in the methods. Also, the breeding scheme used for the different mouse lines needs to be described. For example, did the DAT-Cre mice carry one or two alleles?

      Both the DAT<sup>IRES</sup>Cre and GCaMP6f floxed (Ai148) Jax mouse line numbers and RRIDs are included in the methods. DAT<sup>IRES</sup>Cre mice carried two alleles.

      In the methods section, the amount of virus injected needs to be mentioned.

      This information has been added to the methods section.

      In all result graphs, please include the individual data points so that the readers can see the distribution of the data and quickly see the sample size.

      Graphs have been updated to include all individual data points. For line graphs, the distribution is communicated by the error bars, while the n is in the legends.

      The authors provide running wheel data in supplementary figure 1A to validate that chemogenetic activation of dopamine neurons leads to increased locomotor activity. The results shown in the figure appear to be qualitative as no average data is presented. The authors should provide average data from all mice tested.

      Average IP response data for all mice assessed for running wheel activity has been included in Figure S1.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary, and Strengths:

      The authors and their team have investigated the role of Vimentin Cysteine 328 in epithelial-mesenchymal transition (EMT) and tumorigenesis. Vimentin is a type III intermediate filament, and cysteine 328 is a crucial site for interactions between vimentin and actin. These interactions can significantly influence cell movement, proliferation, and invasion. The team has specifically examined how Vimentin Cysteine 328 affects cancer cell proliferation, the acquisition of stemness markers, and the upregulation of the non-coding RNA XIST. Additionally, functional assays were conducted using both wild-type (WT) and Vimentin Cysteine 328 mutant cells to demonstrate their effects on invasion, EMT, and cancer progression. Overall, the data supports the essential role of Vimentin Cysteine 328 in regulating EMT, cancer stemness, and tumor progression. Overall, the data and its interpretation are on point and support the hypothesis. I believe the manuscript has great potential.

      The authors are thankful to the reviewers for carefully reading the manuscript and evaluating the data to make positive comments and supporting our conclusions.

      Weaknesses:

      Minor issues are related to the visibility and data representation in Figures 2E and 3 A-F

      We have revised the figures (Figure 2E and Figure 3A-F) to increase the data visibility.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The aim of the investigation was to find out more about the mechanism(s) by which the structural protein vimentin can facilitate the epithelial-mesenchymal transition in breast cancer cells.

      The authors focussed on a key amino acid of vimentin, C238, its role in the interaction between vimentin and actin microfilaments, and the downstream molecular and cellular consequences. They model the binding between vimentin and actin in silico to demonstrate the potential involvement of C238, but the outcome is described vaguely.

      We have expanded the discussion of these results in the manuscript to more explicitly describe the critical role of C238 in the vimentin-actin interaction. Specifically, we highlight that C238 lies within a region of the vimentin rod domain known to mediate key protein-protein interactions. Our modeling shows that the thiol group of C238 enables specific hydrogen bonding and potential disulfide-mediated interactions with actin, which are disrupted upon mutation to serine. These findings provide mechanistic insight into the functional importance of this residue.

      The phenotype of a non-metastatic breast cancer cell line MCF7, which doesn't express vimentin, could be changed to a metastatic phenotype when mutant C238S vimentin, but not wild-type vimentin, was expressed in the cells. Expression of vimentin was confirmed at the level of mRNA, protein, and microscopically. Patterns of expression of vimentin and actin reflected the distinct morphology of the two cell lines. Phenotypic changes were assessed through assay of cell adhesion, proliferation, migration, and morphology and were consistent with greater metastatic potential in the C238S MCF7 cells. Changes in the transcriptome of MCF7 cells expressing wild-type and C238S vimentins were compared and expression of Xist long ncRNA was found to be the transcript most markedly increased in the metastatic cells expressing C238S vimentin. Moreover changes in expression of many other genes in the C238S cells are consistent with an epithelial mesenchymal transition. Tumourigenic potential of MCF7 cells carrying C238S but not wild-type, vimentin was confirmed by inoculation of cells into nude mice. This assay is a measure of the stem-cell quality of the cells and not a measure of metastasis. It does demonstrate phenotypic changes that could be linked to metastasis.

      shRNA was used to down-regulate vimentin or Xist in the MCF7 C238S cells. The description of the data is limited in parts and data sets require careful scrutiny to understand the full picture. Down-regulation of vimentin reversed the morphological changes to some degree, but down-regulation of Xist didn't.

      This is understandable given the fact that vimentin interacts with actin which is known to determine cell shape. XIST being a non-coding RNA will not have the same effect.

      Conversely, down-regulation of XIST inhibited cell growth, a sign of reversing metastatic potential, but down-regulation of vimentin had no effect on growth.

      XIST is known to get induced in a number of cancers (see Figure 3E) which is consistent with our observation that its downregulation will inhibit cell growth. However, downregulation of vimentin had no effect on growth which is consistent with our previously published observation that ectopic expression of wildtype vimentin in MCF-7 cells did not influence cell growth (Usman et al Cells 2022, 11(24), 4035; https://doi.org/10.3390/cells11244035).

      Down-regulation of either did inhibit cell migration, another sign of metastatic reversal.

      We have previously shown that ectopic expression of wildtype vimentin in MCF-7 stimulate cell migration due to downregulation of CDH5 (endothelial cadherins) (Usman et al Cells 2022, 11(24), 4035). Therefore, downregulation of vimentin is expected to inhibit cell migration which is what we observed in this study. Why downregulation of XIST inhibited cell migration is not clear. It is conceivable that XIST downregulation affects Lamin expression which may suppress intercellular interactions to increase cell migration. This hypothesis is supported by the fact that vimentin expression in MCF-7 affects Lamin expression (Usman et al Cells 2022, 11(24), 4035).

      The interpretation of this type of experiment is handicapped when full reversal of expression is not achieved, as was the case in this study.

      Full reversal of any biological effect is almost impossible to achieve which is because the shRNAs by nature are not 100% effective. This can however be tested using crispr Cas 9 gene editing to completely knockdown a protein (can’t be used for XIST as it is a non-coding RNA). In that case one has to assume that it will have no off-target effect.

      Overall the study describes an intriguing model of metastasis that is worthy of further investigation, especially at the molecular level to unravel the connection between vimentin and metastasis. The identification of a potential role for Xist in metastasis, beyond its normal role in female cells to inactivate one of the X chromosomes, corroborates the work of others demonstrating increased levels in a variety of tumours in women and even in some tumours in men. It would be of great interest to see where in metastatic cells Xist is expressed and what it binds to.

      The authors fully agree that it is an interesting model of metastasis/oncogenesis that requires further investigation.