5,358 Matching Annotations
  1. Last 7 days
    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Liu et al have tried to dissect the neural and molecular mechanisms that C. elegans use to avoid digestion of harmful bacterial food. Liu et al show that C. elegans use the ON-OFF state of AWC olfactory neurons to regulate the digestion of harmful gram-positive bacteria S. saprophyticus (SS). The authors show that when C. elegans are fed on SS food, AWC neurons switch to OFF fate which prevents digestion of S. saprophyticus and this helps C. elegans avoid these harmful bacteria. Using genetic and transcriptional analysis as well as making use of previously published findings, Liu et al implicate the p38 MAPK pathway (in particular, NSY-1, the C. elegans homolog of MAPKKK ASK1) and insulin signaling in this process.

      Strengths:

      The authors have used multiple approaches to test the hypothesis that they present in this manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      Overall, I am not convinced that the authors have provided sufficient evidence to support the various components of their hypothesis. While they present data that loosely align with their hypothesis, they fail to consider alternative explanations and do not use rigorous approaches to strengthen their overall hypothesis. The selective picking of genes from the RNA sequencing data and forcing the data to fit the proposed hypothesis based on previously published findings, without exploring other approaches, indicates a lack of thoroughness and rigor. These critical shortcomings significantly diminish enthusiasm for the manuscript in its totality. In my opinion, this is the biggest weakness in this manuscript.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s all the suggestions which help us to improve this paper. We now addressed reviewer’s comments at the section of “Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors)”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Using C. elegans as a model, the authors present an interesting story demonstrating a new regulatory connection between olfactory neurons and the digestive system.

      Mechanistically, they identified key factors (NSY-1, STR-130 et.al) in neurons, as well as critical 'signaling factors' (INS-23, DAF-2) that bridge different cells/tissues to execute the digestive shutdown induced by poor-quality food (Staphylococcus saprophyticus, SS).

      Strengths:

      The conclusions of this manuscript are mostly well supported by the experimental results shown.

      Weaknesses:

      Several issues could be addressed and clarified to strengthen their conclusions.

      (1) The word "olfactory" should be carefully used and checked in this manuscript. Although AWCs are classic olfactory neurons in C. elegans, no data in this manuscript supports the idea that olfactory signals from SS drive the responses in the digestive system. To validate that it is truly olfaction, the authors may want to check the responses of worms (e.g. AWC, digestive shutdown, INS-23 expression) to odors from SS.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s careful attention to terminology. We agree that the term "olfactory" requires direct experimental validation. However, in this paper, we only used "olfactory" to specific define the AWC neurons. As reviewer’s suggestion, we now deleted the word “olfactory”.

      (2) In line 113, what does "once the digestive system is activated" mean? The authors need to provide a clearer statement about 'digestive activation' and 'digestive shutdown'.

      Previously, we observed that activating larval digestion with heat-killed E. coli or E. coli cell wall peptidoglycan (PGN) enabled the digestion of SS as food (Hao et al., 2024). Additionally, when animals reached the L2 stage by feeding normal OP50 diet, they could utilize SS as a food source to support growth (Figure 1figure supplement 1D). These findings suggest that once digestion is activated (via E. coli components or L2-stage maturation), worms gain the capacity to process SS as a viable food source, abolishing SS-induced growth impairment (Hao et al., 2024) ( Figure 1figure supplement 1D).

      (3) No control data on OP50. This would affect the conclusions generated from Figures 2A, 2B, 2D, 3B, 3C, 3G, 4D-G, 5D-E, 6B-D.

      We appreciate  this point. The central goal of the experiments listed (Figures 2A,B,D; 3B,C,G; 4D-G; 5D-E; 6B-D) was not to compare growth or behavior between SS and OP50 under standard conditions, but rather to understand the genetic basis of the C. elegans response specifically to SS, as identified through our nsy-1 mutant screen.

      Our data in Figure 1 clearly establishes the fundamental difference in growth and feeding behavior when larvae encounter SS compared to OP50 (Figures 1A,B). Having established SS as an unfavorable food source that triggers a specific protective response (digestive shutdown), the subsequent experiments focus on deciphering how this response is mediated.

      Therefore, within these specific experimental contexts under SS feeding: The primary comparison is between wild-type (N2) and nsy-1 mutant animals. All assays (growth, behavior, survival) are performed under the same SS feeding conditionsfor both genotypes.

      This design allows us to directly assess the functional role of NSY-1 in mediating the SS-specific response pathway we are investigating. Including an OP50 control for every figure would not address this core genetic question and could introduce confounding variables given the established difference in how C. elegans treats these two food sources. The critical internal control for these specific experiments is the performance of the wild-type under SS versus the mutant under SS.

      (4) Do the authors know which factors are released from AWC neurons to drive the digestive shutdown?

      Enrichment analysis revealed that genes related to extracellular functions, such as insulin-related genes, are induced in nsy-1 mutant animals (Figure 5—figure supplement 1A, Supplementary file 4). Further analysis of insulin-related genes from the RNA-seq data showed that ins-23 is predominantly induced in nsy-1 mutant animals (Figure 5—figure supplement 1B), suggesting its potential role in promoting SS digestion. We found that knockdown of ins-23 in nsy-1 mutants inhibited SS digestion (Figure 5D). Given that INS-23 is expressed in AWC neurons (Figure 5figure supplement 3A, CeNGEN), this suggests increased production and likely enhanced release of INS-23 from AWC neurons in the nsy-1 mutant background, which promotes SS digestion.

      The insulin/insulin-like growth factor signaling (IIS) pathway, particularly through the DAF-2 receptor, integrates nutritional signals to regulate various behavioral and physiological responses related to food (Kodama et al., 2006; Ryu et al., 2018). It has been shown that INS-23 acts as an antagonist for the DAF-2 receptor to promote larval diapause (Matsunaga et al., 2018). To test whether ins-23 induction in nsy-1 mutants promotes SS digestion through its receptor, DAF-2, we constructed a nsy-1; daf-2 double mutant. We found that the SS digestion ability of the nsy-1 mutant was inhibited by the daf-2 mutation. This suggests that the nsy-1 mutation induces the insulin peptide ins-23, which promotes SS digestion through its potential receptor, DAF-2.

      The data supports a model where AWC neurons regulate digestion via the release of INS-23. Loss of nsy-1 function increases INS-23 release from AWC, activating DAF-2 signaling and promoting digestion. Conversely, in wild-type animals, reduced INS-23 release from AWC contributes to digestive shutdown in response to SS food.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study explores a molecular mechanism by which C. elegans detects low-quality food through neuron-digestive crosstalk, offering new insights into food quality control systems. Liu and colleagues demonstrated that NSY-1, expressed in AWC neurons, is a key regulator for sensing Staphylococcus saprophyticus (SS), inducing avoidance behavior and shutting down the digestive system via intestinal BCF-1. They further revealed that INS-23, an insulin peptide, interacts with the DAF-2 receptor in the gut to modulate SS digestion. The study uncovers a food quality control system connecting neural and intestinal responses, enabling C. elegans to adapt to environmental challenges.

      Strengths:

      The study employs a genetic screening approach to identify nsy-1 as a critical regulator in detecting food quality and initiating adaptive responses in C. elegans. The use of RNA-seq analysis is particularly noteworthy, as it reveals distinct regulatory pathways involved in food sensing (Figure 4) and digestion of Staphylococcus saprophyticus (Figure 5). The strategic application of both positive and negative data mining enhances the depth of analysis. Importantly, the discovery that C. elegans halts digestion in response to harmful food and employs avoidance behavior highlights a physiological adaptation mechanism.

      Weaknesses:

      Major points:

      (1) While NSY-1 positively regulates str-130 expression in AWC neurons and is critical for SS avoidance and survival, the authors should examine whether similar phenotypes are observed in str-130 mutants.

      In this study, we mainly focused on how worms sense adverse food sources (SS food) and shutdown digestion (not growth as digestion shutdown readout). We found that nsy-1 in AWC play key roles in response SS food, once nsy-1 mutation, mutant animals cannot detect SS food and digest it, therefore growth under SS food. From RNA-seq, we found that nsy-1 positively regulates several sensory perception related genes (sra-32, str-87, str-112, str-130, str-160, str-230) (Figure 4figure supplement 1A, Supplementary file 2). After screen, we found that we found that knockdown of str-130 in wild-type animals promoted SS digestion, thereby supporting animal growth (Figure 4D), and the proportion of animals with two AWC<sup>OFF</sup> neurons decreased (Figure 4E). Secondly, we found that overexpression of str-130 in nsy-1 mutant animals inhibited SS digestion, thereby slowing animal growth (Figure 4F), and the proportion of animals with two AWC<sup>OFF</sup> neurons increased (Figure 4G). These results demonstrate that NSY-1 promotes the AWC<sup>OFF</sup> state by inducing str-130 expression, which in turn inhibits SS digestion in C. elegans.

      (2) NSY-1 promotes the AWC-OFF state through str-130, inhibiting SS digestion. The authors should investigate whether STR-130 in AWC neurons regulates bcf-1 expression levels in the intestine.

      We agree with the reviewer's suggestion regarding the potential role of STR-130 in AWC neurons regulating intestinal bcf-1 expression. To address this, we generated transgenic worms with AWC-specific knockdown of str-130, achieved by rescuing sid-1 cDNA expression under the ceh-36 promoter (AWC-specific) in sid-1(qt9);BCF-1::GFP background worms.

      We observed that AWC neuron-specific RNAi of str-130 elevated intestinal BCF-1::GFP expression (Figure 6—figure supplement 1B). This demonstrates that STR-130 functions cell-non-autonomously in AWC neurons to repress BCF-1 expression in the intestine.

      (3) The current results rely on str-2 expression levels to indicate the AWC state. Ablating AWC neurons and testing the effects on digestion would provide stronger evidence for their role in digestive regulation.

      To confirm the important of AWC state in SS digestion, we performed AWC-specific neuron ablation experiments using previously validated transgenic strain that expresses cleaved caspase under the AWC-specific promoter, ceh-36 (ceh-36p::caspase). Critically, worms with ablated AWC neurons completely failed to digest SS food (Figure 3—figure supplement 4), phenocopying the non-digesting state of wild-type worms on SS when AWC-OFF signaling is impaired. This result directly confirms that functional AWC neurons are essential for initiating SS digestion, aligning with our model where the AWC-OFF state (induced by SS) inhibits digestion while the AWC-ON state promotes it.

      Furthermore, we previously study discovered that AWC ablation activates the intestinal mitochondrial unfolded protein response and inhibits food digestion, mechanistically linking neuronal integrity to gut stress responses and digestive inhibition.

      Together, these functional ablation studies provide compelling physiological evidence that AWC neurons act as central regulators of food-state sensing and gut function.

      (4) The claim that NSY-1 inhibits INS-23 and that INS-23 interacts with DAF-2 to regulate bcf-1 expression (Line 339-340) requires further validation. Neuron-specific disruption of INS-23 and gut-specific rescue of DAF-2 should be tested.

      We agree with the reviewer that the proposed NSY-1 ⊣ INS-23 → DAF-2 → BCF-1 signaling axis requires tissue-specific validation. To address this, we conducted compartment-specific functional dissection of INS-23 and DAF-2:

      AWC neuronal role of INS-23:

      To test whether INS-23 acts in AWC neurons to regulate intestinal BCF-1, we generated AWC-specific knockdown strains which was achieved by rescuing sid-1 cDNA expression under the ceh-36 promoter in a sid-1(qt9);BCF-1::GFP background. We found that AWC-restricted ins-23 knockdown significantly reduced intestinal BCF-1::GFP expression (Figure 6—figure supplement 1A). This confirms that INS-23 functions cell-non-autonomously within AWC sensory neurons to activate intestinal BCF-1, consistent with NSY-1’s upstream inhibition of INS-23 in this neuronal  subtype

      Intestinal role of DAF-2 as INS-23 receptor:

      To investigate weather DAF-2 acts as the gut-localized receptor for neuronal INS-23 signaling, we performed tissue-specific rescue experiments in the nsy-1(ag3);daf-2(e1370) double mutant. When DAF-2 was re-introduced specifically in the intestine (using the ges-1 promoter), we observed a significant suppression of SS digestion (Figure 5—figure supplement 3B), but not rescue digestive defect. This indicates that INS-23 induction in nsy-1 mutants promotes digestion independently of intestinal DAF-2 function.

      (5) Figure Reference Errors: Lines 296-297 mention Figure 6E, which does not exist in the main text. This appears to refer to Figure 5E, which has not been described.

      We corrected this.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I would like the authors to address the following comments in a resubmission.

      (1) The hallmark of the activated p38 MAPK pathway is the phosphorylation of most downstream kinase p38 (PMK-1/PMK2 in C. elegans) of this kinase cascade. Previous work from Bergmann lab showed that the most downstream kinase of this pathway, PMK-1/PMK-2, is not required for AWC asymmetry. I wonder whether that is the case also for the model that Liu et al have presented in this manuscript. Since p38/PMK-1 undergoes activation (phosphorylation) in response to pathogenic bacteria like P. aeruginosa, it is worth testing whether PMK-1 plays a role downstream of NSY-1 in the model that Liu et al present in this manuscript. It would be worth testing whether there is increased phosphorylation of p38 when C. elegans are fed SS and whether that phosphorylation regulates downstream components that Liu et al have identified in this manuscript.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point regarding PMK-1/p38 MAPK signaling. As established in our prior work (Reference 1), SS exposure triggers phosphorylation of PMK-1 (P-PMK-1) in C. elegans, and pmk-1 mutants exhibit enhanced growth on SS (Figure-1, Figure-2). This confirms that PMK-1-mediated innate immune signaling actively regulates SS responsiveness and digestion.

      To address whether PMK-1 functions downstream of NSY-1 within our proposed model, we performed critical epistasis analyses. While we observed that nsy-1 mutation elevates ins-23 (indicating NSY-1 suppression of ins-23), knockdown of pmk-1 did not alter ins-23 expression levels (Figure 5-figure supplement 3C). This demonstrates that PMK-1 does not operate through the ins-23 pathway to regulate SS digestion. Thus, although both pathways respond to SS, the PMK-1-mediated innate immune response and the NSY-1/INS-23 axis constitute distinct regulatory mechanisms governing digestive adaptation.

      Reference 1: Geng, S., Li, Q., Zhou, X., Zheng, J., Liu, H., Zeng, J., Yang, R., Fu, H., Hao, F., Feng, Q., & Qi, B. (2022). Gut commensal E. coli outer membrane proteins activate the host food digestive system through neural-immune communication. Cell host & microbe, 30(10), 1401–1416.e8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chom.2022.08.004

      (2) Since p38 MAPK pathway has a well-established role in host defense in the C. elegans intestine, it is important to show that NSY-1 does not function in the intestine in the model that Liu et al present. I would like the authors to reintroduce nsy-1 in C. elegans intestine in nsy-1 mutant animals and then test whether it has any effect on worm length on SS food (similar to what is done in Figure 3 for AWC-specific nsy-1).

      Beyond its  established  role  in  AWC  neurons,  we  detected  NSY-1 expression in the intestine (Figure 3-figure supplement 2A). To assess intestinal NSY-1 function, we performed tissue-specific rescue experiments in nsy-1 mutants using the intestinal-specific vha-1 promoter. Intestinal expression of NSY-1 significantly suppressed the enhanced SS digestion phenotype in nsy-1 mutants (Figure 3-figure supplement 2B), demonstrating functional involvement of gut-localized NSY-1 in regulating digestive responses. We propose intestinal NSY-1 mediates this effect through innate immune signaling, consistent with its known pathway components. As previously established (Reference 1), the canonical PMK-1/p38 MAPK pathway functions downstream of NSY-1, with both sek-1 and pmk-1 knockdown enhancing SS digestion through immune modulation. This indicates intestinal NSY-1 suppresses digestion may act through PMK-1-mediated immune responses. Since neuronal NSY-1's role in digestive control was previously undefined, we prioritized mechanistic analysis of its neuronal function in digestion regulation.

      Notably, this immune-mediated mechanism operates independently of NSY-1's neuronal regulation pathway. In AWC neurons, NSY-1 controls digestion exclusively through the neuropeptide signaling axis (INS-23/DAF-2/BCF-1) without engaging innate immune components.

      Reference 1: Geng, S., Li, Q., Zhou, X., Zheng, J., Liu, H., Zeng, J., Yang, R., Fu, H., Hao, F., Feng, Q., & Qi, B. (2022). Gut commensal E. coli outer membrane proteins activate the host food digestive system through neural-immune communication. Cell host & microbe, 30(10), 1401–1416.e8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chom.2022.08.004

      (3) At multiple places, wild-type (WT) controls have been labeled as N2. It is better to label all controls as WT (and not as N2).

      Corrected.

      (4) In Figure 2B, the aversion response should be scored at multiple time points, like Figure 1C, rather than at just one timepoint.

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting multi-timepoint analysis of aversion behavior. In accordance with this recommendation, we have now quantified SS avoidance at multi-timepoint. As shown in the revised Figure 2B, nsy-1 mutants exhibited significantly impaired avoidance responses at both 4h and 6h but not at 8h, confirming that NSY-1 is essential for sustained aversion to SS food in the early response. This data demonstrates that the critical role of NSY-1 in food discrimination at initial sensory responses.

      (5) Does the re-introduction of nsy-1 in AWC neurons in nsy-1 mutant background help animals avoid SS in dwelling and food-choice assays? Along the same lines, does the CRISPR-generated AWC-specific mutant of NSY-1 fail to avoid SS in dwelling and food-choice assays similar to the whole-animal mutant? These behavioral data are missing in Figure 3.

      We thank the reviewer for prompting behavioral validation of AWC-specific nsy-1 functions. To determine whether NSY-1 in AWC neurons mediates SS sensory perception, we performed dwelling (avoidance) and food-choice assays using AWC-specific nsy-1 knockout and AWC-rescued strains (nsy-1(ag3); Podr-1::nsy-1). In dwelling assays, AWC-specific nsy-1 KO mutants exhibited significantly impaired SS avoidance at 6h (Figure 3-figure supplement 3A), while AWC-rescued strains restored avoidance capacity at 2-6h (Figure 3-figure supplement 3B). Food-choice assays further revealed that AWC nsy-1 KO mutants preferentially migrated toward SS (Figure 3-figure supplement 3C), whereas AWC-rescued showed no preference between SS and HK-E. coli (Figure 3-figure supplement 3D). These data conclusively demonstrate that NSY-1 acts in AWC neurons to mediate SS recognition and aversion behaviors.

      (6) In Figure 3E and F, the number of animals that were used for scoring AWC str-2p::GFP expression should be specified.

      we added the number of animals in the figure.

      (7)  RNA seq analysis identified multiple GPCRs (including STR-130) that are upregulated in an NSY-1-dependent manner when animals are fed with SS bacteria. However, the authors decided to only characterize STR-130 because of previously published findings. It is important to rule out the role of other GPCRs since all are upregulated on SS food as shown in Figure S4 B. I would like the authors to knock down other GPCRs in the same manner as they did for STR-130 and demonstrate that only str-130 knockdown behaves similarly to the nsy-1 mutant (if that is the case) using the assay presented in Figure 4 D.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to comprehensively evaluate NSY-1-regulated GPCRs. In response, we extended our functional analysis to all six GPCRs (str-130, str-230, str-87, str-112, str-160, and sra-32) identified as NSY-1-dependent and SS-induced in RNA-seq (Figure 4—figure supplement 1).

      Using RNAi knockdown and the SS growth assay, we observed that RNAi of str-130, str-230, str-87, or str-112 significantly enhanced SS growth (Figure 4—figure supplement 2A), with str-130 RNAi exhibiting the most robust phenotype—phenocopying nsy-1 mutants. Crucially, none of these GPCR knockdowns further enhanced growth in nsy-1(ag3) mutants (Figure 4—figure supplement 2B), confirming their position downstream of NSY-1. These data establish str-130 as the dominant effector of NSY-1-mediated SS response regulation, while suggesting minor contributions from other GPCRs (str-230, str-87, str-112).

      (8) In Figure 4E and G, the number of animals that were used for scoring GFP expression should be specified.

      we added the number of animals in the figure.

      (9) When comparing Figure 3E and Figure 4E, it appears that the loss of str-130 RNAi does not phenocopy nsy-1 mutant. This raises the question of whether the inefficiency of RNAi targeting str-130 is the cause, or if STR-130 is not the only GPCR regulated by NSY-1 on SS food. I would like the authors to address this discrepancy. If RNAi inefficiency is indeed the cause, using an RNAi-sensitive background, such as an eri- 1 mutant, could help strengthen the data presented in Figure 4E. Conversely, if RNAi inefficiency is not responsible for the discrepancy, I suggest that the authors investigate the roles of other GPCRs that were identified by RNA sequencing.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s observation regarding the phenotypic difference between nsy-1 mutants and str-130 (RNAi) animals on SS food (Fig. 3E vs Fig. 4E).

      While both genetic perturbations significantly enhance SS growth and increase the proportion of animals exhibiting AWC<sup>ON</sup> states compared to wild type (indicating enhanced digestion), the specific AWC<sup>ON </sup> neuron configurations differ: nsy-1 mutants predominantly show 2 AWC<sup>ON</sup> animals, whereas str-130(RNAi) animals primarily exhibit the 1 AWC<sup>ON</sup> /1 AWC<sup>OFF</sup> configuration (Fig. 3E vs Fig. 4E).

      This difference likely arises because STR-130 is the key GPCR mediating NSY-1's inhibitory effect on SS digestion, but it is not the sole GPCR involved, as evidenced by our RNAi screen identifying several additional NSY-1-regulated GPCRs (str-230, str-87, str-112) whose depletion also enhanced SS growth (Fig. 4A-D).

      The robust SS growth enhancement and AWC<sup>ON </sup> state increase caused by str-130 (RNAi) (phenocopying the nsy-1 mutant’s functional outcome of enhanced digestion) (Figure 4D, 4E) indicate effective RNAi knockdown for this specific assay. Therefore, the distinct neural configurations reflect the partial redundancy among GPCRs downstream of NSY-1, rather than an inherent inefficiency of the str-130 RNAi.

      The nsy-1 mutant phenotype represents the complete loss of all inhibitory GPCR signaling coordinated by NSY-1, while str-130(RNAi) represents the loss of its major component. Investigating the roles of other identified GPCRs (str-230, str-87, str-112) in modulating AWC<sup>ON </sup> neuron states is an important direction for future research.

      (10) In Figure 4 F and 4 G, the authors show that the overexpression of STR-130 rescues the nsy-1 mutant phenotype suggesting that NSY-1 might function through STR-130 to control digestion on SS food. These data place STR-130 downstream of NSY-1. To further strengthen these epistasis data, authors should knock down str-130 in nsy-1 mutant animals and show that the combined loss of both genes produces the same effect as the loss of either gene alone.

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful suggestion to further define the genetic relationship between nsy-1 and str-130. To strengthen our epistasis analysis, we performed RNAi knockdown of str-130 in the nsy-1(ag3) mutant background and assessed development on SS food. Consistent with STR-130 acting downstream of NSY-1, the loss of str-130 via RNAi did not further enhance the developmental capacity (i.e., growth phenotype) of nsy-1(ag3) mutant animals on SS. This lack of enhancement indicates that str-130 and nsy-1 function within the same genetic pathway, with str-130 acting epistatically downstream of nsy-1 (Figure 4—figure supplement 3). This finding reinforces the model proposed from our overexpression data (Fig. 4F-G) – that NSY-1 primarily exerts its inhibitory effect on SS digestion by inducing the expression GPCR STR-130.

      (11) In Figure 5C, please mention "ins-23 transcript levels" on the top of the graph so that it is clear what these data represent.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion.

      (12) Since all ins genes were upregulated in nsy-1 mutants (though ins-23 was indeed the most highly upregulated gene) on SS food from RNA seq analysis (Figure S5 B), it is important to first phenotypically characterize all of them using "worm length assay". If this analysis shows that ins-23 has the most robust phenotype, it would make more sense to just focus on ins-23.

      We agree with the reviewer that initial phenotypic characterization of candidate genes identified through transcriptomic analysis is valuable.Our RNA-seq data revealed that several insulin-like peptide genes, including ins-22, ins-23, ins-24, and ins-27, were significantly upregulated in the nsy-1 mutant on SS food (Figure 5—figure supplement 1B). We prioritized these insulin-like peptide genes for functional validation because they are known to act as neuropeptides capable of mediating non-cell autonomous signaling in previous studies (Shao et al 2016).

      To determine if any were functionally responsible for the enhanced SS growth observed in nsy-1 mutants, we performed functional phenotypic screening using the SS growth assay (worm length assay). We individually knocked down each of these candidates (ins-22, ins-23, ins-24, ins-27) in the nsy-1(ag3) mutant background. Among these, only RNAi targeting ins-23 significantly attenuated (i.e., suppressed) the enhanced development of the nsy-1(ag3) mutant on SS (Figure 5—figure supplement 2). This targeted functional screening revealed that ins-23 has the most robust and specific role in mediating the enhanced digestion phenotype downstream of NSY-1 loss, providing the critical justification for our subsequent focus on this particular insulin-like peptide.

      Ref:

      Shao, L. W., Niu, R., & Liu, Y. (2016). Neuropeptide signals cell non-autonomous mitochondrial unfolded protein response. Cell research, 26(11), 1182–1196. https://doi.org/10.1038/cr.2016.118

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      There are several minor errors and typos in the manuscript

      (1) A number of typos in the figures, like "length".

      Corrected.

      (2) The 'axis labels' are inconsistent from panel to panel, like "relative body length" and "relative worm length".

      Corrected.

      (3) The fonts are inconsistent from panel to panel.

      Corrected.

      (4) There is no Ex unique number for transgenic lines.

      Corrected.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor points:

      (1)  Figure 3B, 3C, 3G, 4D, 4F, 5D, 5E, and 6C: Replace "lenth" with "length" (consistent with Figure 2A).

      Corrected.

      (2) Figure 4D: Correct "ctontrol" to "control."

      Corrected.

      (3) Figure 4G: Update the co-injection marker to Podr-1::GFP instead of Pstr-2::GFP.

      Corrected.

      (4) Figure 5C: This figure is missing from the Results section.

      Corrected.

      (5) Figure 6A: Label the graph with Pbcf-1::bcf-1::GFP, as in Figure 6D.

      Corrected.

      (6) Italicization: Lines 588 and 603-italicize nsy-1.

      Corrected.

      (7) Supplementary Figure S2A: Correct "Screeng" to "Screening."

      Corrected.

      (8) Spelling/Proofreading: Ensure consistent spelling and grammar, such as correcting "mutan" to "mutant" in Figure 4A.

      Corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      (1) I suggest that the author's choose a different term in their title, abstract and manuscript to describe the phenotypes associated with ufd-1 and npl-4 knockdown other than an "inflammation-like response." Inflammation is a pathological term with four cardinal signs: redness (rubor), swelling (tumor), warmth (calor) and pain (dolor). These are not symptoms know to occur in C. elegans. The authors could consider using "tolerance" instead, as this term may better describe their findings.

      We have changed “inflammation-like response” to “aberrant immune response” throughout the manuscript.

      (2) It would help the reader to better understand the novelty of the findings in this study if the authors include a paragraph in their introduction to put their results in context of the published literature that has examined the relationship between immune activation and nematode health and survival. In particular, I suggest that the authors discuss doi:10.7554/eLife.74206 (2022), a study that charcterized a similar observation to what the authors are reporting. This study found that low cholesterol reduces pathogen tolerance and host survival during pathogen infection. Cholesterol scarcity increases p38 PMK-1 phosphorylation, priming immune effector induction in a manner that reduces pathogen accumulation in the intestine during a subsequent infection. I also suggest that the authors highlight in this introductory paragraph that the toxic effects of inappropriate immune activation in C. elegans has been widely catalogued. For example: doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1011120 (2023); doi:10.1186/s12915-016-0320-z (2016).; doi:10.1126/science.1203411 (2011); doi:10.1534/g3.115.025650 (2016).

      In this context, the authors could consider re-wording their novelty claim in the abstract and introduction to take into account this previous body of work.

      We have added a paragraph to the Discussion section to place our findings in the context of previous research. The revised manuscript now includes the following text (page 11, lines 336–344): “Previous studies have shown that hyperactivation of immune pathways can negatively affect organismal development. For example, sustained activation of the p38 MAPK pathway impairs development in C. elegans (Cheesman et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2016), and excessive activation of the IPR also leads to developmental defects (Lažetić et al., 2023). Similar to our current study, recent work has demonstrated that heightened immune responses can reduce gut pathogen load while paradoxically decreasing host survival during infection (Ghosh and Singh, 2024; Peterson et al., 2022). However, our study uniquely shows that while such heightened immune responses are detrimental to immunocompetent animals, they can be beneficial in the context of immunodeficiency.”

      (3) The authors rely on the use of RNAi of ufd-1 and npl-4 to study their effect on P. aeruginosa colonization and pathogen resistance throughout the manuscript. To address the possibility of off-target effects of the RNAi, the authors should consider both (i) showing with qRT-PCR that these genes are indeed targeted during RNAi, and (ii) confirming their phenotypes with an orthologous technique, preferably by studying ufd-1 and npl-4 loss-offunction mutants [both in the wild-type and sek-1(km4) backgrounds]. If mutation of these genes is lethal, the authors could use Auxin Inducible Degron (AID) technology to induce the degradation of these proteins in post-developmental animals.

      We attempted several protocols of CRISPR in our laboratory to generate ufd-1 loss-of-function mutants; however, these efforts were unsuccessful. While this does not rule out the possibility of generating ufd-1 mutants, the failure is likely due to technical limitations on our part rather than an inherent inability to disrupt the gene. Nevertheless, to confirm the specificity of our RNAi-based approach, we quantified ufd-1 and npl-4 mRNA levels following RNAi treatment and found that each gene was specifically and effectively downregulated by its respective RNAi. 

      Importantly, ufd-1 and npl-4 RNA sequences do not share significant homology, yet knockdown of either gene results in nearly identical phenotypes, including reduced survival on P. aeruginosa, diminished intestinal colonization, and shortened lifespan. These consistent outcomes strongly support the conclusion that the phenotypes are attributable to the disruption of the functional UFD-1-NPL-4 complex. We have added these results in the revised manuscript (pages 4-5, lines 114-125): “To confirm the specificity of the RNAi knockdowns and rule out potential off-target effects, we examined transcript levels of ufd-1 and npl-4 following RNAi treatment. RNAi against ufd-1 significantly reduced ufd-1 mRNA levels without reducing npl-4 expression, while npl-4 RNAi specifically downregulated npl-4 transcripts with no impact on ufd-1 mRNA levels (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A and B). Additionally, alignment of ufd-1 and npl-4 mRNA sequences against the C. elegans transcriptome revealed no significant similarity to other genes, supporting the specificity of the RNAi constructs. Moreover, the ufd-1 and npl-4 RNA sequences do not share significant sequence similarity. Therefore, the highly similar phenotypes observed in ufd-1 and npl-4 knockdown animals, including shortened lifespan, reduced survival on P. aeruginosa, and decreased intestinal colonization with P. aeruginosa, strongly suggest that these outcomes result from the disruption of the functional UFD-1-NPL-4 complex.”

      (4) I am confused about the authors explanation regarding their observation that inhibition of the UFD-1/ NPL-4 complex extends the lifespan of sek-1(km25) animals, but not pmk-1(km25) animals, as SEK-1 is the MAPKK that functions immediately upstream of the p38 MAPK PMK-1 to promote pathogen resistance.

      I am also confused why their RNA-seq experiment revealed a signature of intracellular pathogen response genes and not PMK-1 targets, which the authors propose is accounting for toxic immune activation. Activation of which immune response leads to toxicity?

      We consistently observe that sek-1(km4) mutants are more sensitive to P. aeruginosa infection than pmk-1(km25) mutants, a finding also reported in previous studies (for example, PMID: 33658510). Given that SEK-1 functions upstream of PMK-1 in the MAPK signaling cascade, it is plausible that SEK-1 also regulates additional MAP kinases, such as PMK-2 (PMID: 25671546), which could contribute to the enhanced susceptibility observed in sek-1 mutants.

      Our results show that inhibition of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex improves survival specifically in severely immunocompromised animals, such as sek-1(km4) mutants, but not in pmk1(km25) mutants. To further validate this, we generated the double mutant dbl-1(nk3);pmk1(km25), which exhibits reduced survival on P. aeruginosa compared to either single mutant.

      Notably, inhibition of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex also enhances survival in the dbl1(nk3);pmk-1(km25) background, reinforcing the observation that this response is specific to severely compromised immune states.

      We would also like to clarify that the observed phenotypes are independent of the SEK1/PMK-1 pathway, as shown in Figure 3A-3C, Figure 3—figure supplement 1, and Figure 4A-4C. The IPR seems to play a role in the observed phenotypes, as inhibition of some of the protease and pals genes (IPR genes) leads to increased P. aeruginosa colonization in ufd-1 knockdown animals (Figure 6—figure supplement 1). The other immune response pathway that leads to the observed phenotypes is ELT-2, as explained in Figure 6. Finally, we have included in the revised manuscript a note that, in addition, as-yet unidentified pathways are also likely contributing to the phenotypes triggered by disruption of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex.

      (5) The authors did not test alternative explanations for why UFD-1/ NPL-4 complex inhibition compromises survival during pathogen infection, other than exuberant immune activation. For example, it is possible that inhibition of this proteosome complex shortens lifespan by compromising the general health/ normal physiology of nematodes. Immune responses could be activated as a secondary consequence of this stress, and not be a direct cause of early morality. Does sek-1(km4) mutant suppress the lifespan shortened lifespan of ufd-1 and npl-4 knockdown? This experiment should also be done with loss-offunction mutants, as noted in point 3.

      We have already included this data in Figure 4D, where we observed that ufd-1 and npl-4 knockdown reduce the lifespan of sek-1(km4) animals. It is possible that immune activation is a secondary consequence of cellular stress induced by inhibition of the UFD-1NPL-4 complex. However, our data strongly suggest that the observed phenotypes, including reduced gut pathogen load and decreased survival on the pathogen, are due to the aberrant immune response activated by the inhibition of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex. Evidence from sek-1(km4) mutants particularly underscores the role of this dysregulated immune activation. While this aberrant immune response is detrimental to wild-type animals under pathogenic conditions, it appears to be beneficial in severely immunocompromised backgrounds. Specifically, in sek-1(km4) mutants, inhibition of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex enhances survival during P. aeruginosa infection (Figure 4A). However, under non-infectious conditions, where sek-1(km4) mutants exhibit a normal lifespan, the same immune activation becomes harmful (Figure 4D). Together, these findings demonstrate that the aberrant immune response induced by UFD-1–NPL-4 inhibition is context-dependent: it is advantageous only for immunocompromised animals under infection, but deleterious to healthy animals under infection and to both healthy and immunocompromised animals under non-infectious conditions.

      (6) The conclusion of Figure 6 hinges on an experiments that uses double RNAi to knockdown two genes at the same time (Fig. 6D and 6G), an approach that is inherently fraught in C. elegans biology owing the likelihood that the efficiency of RNAi-mediated gene knockdown is compromised and may account for the observed phenotypes. The proper control for double RNAi is not empty vector + ufd-1(RNAi), but rather gfp(RNAi) + ufd1(RNAi), as the introduction of a second hairpin RNA is what may compromise knockdown efficiency. In this context, it is important to confirm that knockdown of both genes occurs as expected (with qRT-PCR) and to confirm this phenotype using available elt-2 loss-of-function mutants.

      We thank the reviewer for this helpful suggestion. We have repeated all double

      RNAi experiments using gfp RNAi as a control instead of the empty vector (Figure 6 and Figure 6—figure supplement 1). Additionally, we assessed the efficiency of gene knockdown in the double RNAi conditions (Figure 6—figure supplement 2) and found that RNAi efficacy was not compromised by the double RNAi treatment.

      (7) A supplementary table with the source data for at least three replications (mean lifespan, n, statistical comparison) for each pathogenesis assay should be included in this manuscript.

      The source data is provided for all the data presented in the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to uncover what role, if any, the UFD1/NPL4 complex might play in the innate immune responses of the nematode C. elegans. The authors find that loss of the complex renders animals more sensitive to both pathogenic and non-pathogenic bacteria. However, there appears to be a complex interplay with known innate immune pathways since the loss of UFD1/NPL4 actually results in increased survival of animals lacking the canonical innate immune pathways.

      We thank the reviewer for providing an excellent summary of our work.

      Strengths:

      The authors perform robust genetic analysis to exclude and include possible mechanisms by which the UFD1/NPL4 pathway acts in the innate immune response.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of our work.

      Weaknesses:

      The argument that the loss of the UFD1/NPL4 complex triggers a response that mimics that of an intracellular pathogen has not been thoroughly investigated. Additionally, the finding of a role of the GATA transcription factor, ELT-2, in this response is suggestive, but experiments showing sufficiency in the context of loss of the UFD1/NPL4 complex need to be explored.

      We have investigated the role of IPR genes in the phenotypes observed upon ufd1 knockdown (Figure 6—figure supplement 1), and our results suggest that the IPR may contribute, at least in part, to the phenotypic outcomes of ufd-1 RNAi. In the Discussion section (pages 11–12, lines 345–356), we have included a detailed discussion on the possible mechanisms underlying IPR activation upon inhibition of the UFD-1–NPL-4 complex. We agree that the interaction between the UFD-1–NPL-4 complex and the IPR is intriguing and warrants further investigation. However, we believe that an in-depth exploration of this interaction lies beyond the scope of the current study.

      We have incorporated new data on ELT-2 overexpression in the revised manuscript. Overexpression of ELT-2 partially phenocopies the effects of ufd-1 knockdown, supporting the idea that other pathways likely contribute to the full spectrum of phenotypes observed upon UFD-1-NPL-4 complex inhibition. The revised manuscript reads (page 10, lines 311319): “To determine whether ELT-2 activation alone is sufficient to recapitulate the phenotypes observed upon UFD-1-NPL-4 complex inhibition, we analyzed animals overexpressing ELT-2. Similar to ufd-1 knockdown, ELT-2 overexpression led to a significant reduction in the colonization of the gut by P. aeruginosa (Figure 6—figure supplement 3A and 3B). However, overexpression of ELT-2 did not alter the survival of worms on P. aeruginosa (Figure 6—figure supplement 3C). Taken together, these findings suggest that the phenotypes triggered by disruption of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex are partially mediated by ELT-2. However, additional pathways, yet to be identified, likely cooperate with ELT-2 to regulate both pathogen resistance and host survival.”

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The authors could consider avoiding the use of descriptors (e.g., "drastic") when presenting their data.

      We have removed the descriptors.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      What happens with overexpression of ELT2?

      Overexpression of ELT-2 partially recapitulates the phenotypes of ufd-1 knockdowns, indicating that additional pathways are likely involved in controlling the phenotypes observed upon inhibition of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex. The revised manuscript reads (page 10, lines 311-319): “To determine whether ELT-2 activation alone is sufficient to recapitulate the phenotypes observed upon UFD-1-NPL-4 complex inhibition, we analyzed animals overexpressing ELT-2. Similar to ufd-1 knockdown, ELT-2 overexpression led to a significant reduction in the colonization of the gut by P. aeruginosa (Figure 6—figure supplement 3A and 3B). However, overexpression of ELT-2 did not alter the survival of worms on P. aeruginosa (Figure 6—figure supplement 3C). Taken together, these findings suggest that the phenotypes triggered by disruption of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex are partially mediated by ELT-2. However, additional pathways, yet to be identified, likely cooperate with ELT-2 to regulate both pathogen resistance and host survival.”

      The data with xbp-1 loss of function is very different than that of pek1 and atf-6. Does loss of ufd1/npl4 suppress the increased pathogen survival of xbp-1s overexpressing animals?

      We have examined worms overexpressing XBP-1s and found that overexpression of XBP-1s does not rescue the phenotypes caused by ufd-1 knockdown. The revised manuscript reads (page 6, lines 167-174): “To further examine the role of XBP-1 in this context, we assessed the effect of ufd-1 knockdown in animals neuronally overexpressing the constitutively active spliced form of XBP-1 (XBP-1s), which has been previously associated with enhanced longevity (Taylor and Dillin, 2013). Knockdown of ufd-1 resulted in the reduced survival of XBP-1s-overexpressing animals on P. aeruginosa, despite a concurrent decrease in bacterial colonization of the gut (Figure 2—figure supplement 1A-C). This indicated that the XBP-1 pathway was not required for the reduced P. aeruginosa colonization of ufd-1 knockdown animals.” 

      Lastly, while the pathogen burden is reduced in ufd1/npl4 loss and pumping rates are marginally affected, have you checked defecation rates? Could they be increased?

      We thank the reviewer for this valuable suggestion. We measured defecation rates following ufd-1 and npl-4 knockdown and, unexpectedly, found that inhibition of ufd-1/npl-4 leads to a reduction in defecation frequency. These findings clearly indicate that altered defecation cannot explain the observed decrease in gut colonization. The revised manuscript reads (page 5, lines 138-148): “The clearance of intestinal contents through the defecation motor program (DMP) is known to influence gut colonization by P. aeruginosa in C. elegans (Das et al., 2023). It is therefore conceivable that knockdown of the UFD-1-NPL-4 complex might increase defecation frequency, thereby promoting the physical expulsion of bacteria and resulting in reduced gut colonization. To test this possibility, we measured DMP rates in animals subjected to ufd-1 and npl-4 RNAi. Contrary to this hypothesis, both ufd-1 and npl-4 knockdown animals exhibited a significant reduction in defecation frequency compared to control RNAi-treated animals (Figure 1—figure supplement 2C). This reduction in DMP rate persisted even after 12 hours of exposure to P. aeruginosa (Figure 1—figure supplement 2D). Thus, the change in the DMP rate in ufd-1 and npl-4 knockdown animals is unlikely to be the reason for the reduced gut colonization by P. aeruginosa.”

      In summary, we would like to thank the reviewers again for providing constructive and thoughtful feedback. We believe we have fully addressed all the concerns of the reviewers by carrying out several new experiments and modifying the text. The manuscript has undergone substantial revision and has thereby improved significantly. We do hope that the evidence in support of the conclusions is found to be complete in the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript presents a compelling study identifying RBMX2 as a novel host factor upregulated during Mycobacterium bovis infection.

      The study demonstrates that RBMX2 plays a role in:

      (1) Facilitating M. bovis adhesion, invasion, and survival in epithelial cells.

      (2) Disrupting tight junctions and promoting EMT.

      (3) Contributing to inflammatory responses and possibly predisposing infected tissue to lung cancer development.

      By using a combination of CRISPR-Cas9 library screening, multi-omics, coculture models, and bioinformatics, the authors establish a detailed mechanistic link between M. bovis infection and cancer-related EMT through the p65/MMP-9 signaling axis. Identification of RBMX2 as a bridge between TB infection and EMT is novel.

      Strengths:

      This topic and data are both novel and significant, expanding the understanding of transcriptomic diversity beyond RBM2 in M. bovis responsive functions.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The abstract and introduction sometimes suggest RBMX2 has protective anti-TB functions, yet results show it facilitates pathogen adhesion and survival. The authors need to rephrase claims to avoid contradiction.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's valuable feedback regarding the need to clarify RBMX2's role throughout the manuscript. We have carefully revised the text to ensure consistent messaging about RBMX2's function in promoting M. bovis infection. Below we detail the specific modifications made:\

      (1) Introduction Revisions:

      Changed "The objective of this study was to elucidate the correlation between host genes and the susceptibility of M.bovis infection" to "The objective of this study was to identify host factors that promote susceptibility to M.bovis infection"

      Revised "RBMX2 polyclonal and monoclonal cell lines exhibited favorable phenotypes" to "RBMX2 knockout cell lines showed reduced bacterial survival"

      Replaced "The immune regulatory mechanism of RBMX2" with "The role of RBMX2 in facilitating M.bovis immune evasion"

      (2) Results Revisions:

      Modified "RBMX2 fails to affect cell morphology and the ability to proliferate and promotes M.bovis infection" to "RBMX2 does not alter cell viability but significantly enhances M.bovis infection"

      Strengthened conclusion in Figure 4: "RBMX2 actively disrupts tight junctions to facilitate bacterial invasion"

      (3) Discussion Revisions:

      Revised screening description: "We screened host factors affecting M.bovis susceptibility and identified RBMX2 as a key promoter of infection"

      Strengthened concluding statement: "In summary, RBMX2 drives TB pathogenesis by compromising epithelial barriers and inducing EMT"

      These targeted revisions ensure that:

      All sections consistently present RBMX2 as promoting infection; the language aligns with our experimental finding; potential protective interpretations have been eliminated. We believe these modifications have successfully addressed the reviewer's concern while maintaining the manuscript's original structure and scientific content. We appreciate the opportunity to improve our manuscript and thank the reviewer for this constructive suggestion.

      (2) While p65/MMP-9 is convincingly implicated, the role of MAPK/p38 and JNK is less clearly resolved.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's insightful comment regarding the roles of MAPK/p38 and JNK in our study. Our experimental data clearly demonstrated that RBMX2 knockout significantly reduced phosphorylation levels of p65, p38, and JNK (Fig. 5A), indicating potential involvement of all three pathways in RBMX2-mediated regulation.

      Through systematic functional validation, we obtained several important findings:

      In pathway inhibition experiments, p65 activation (PMA treatment) showed the most dramatic effects on both tight junction disruption (ZO-1, OCLN reduction) and EMT marker regulation (E-cadherin downregulation, N-cadherin upregulation);p38 activation (ML141 treatment) exhibited moderate effects on these processes; JNK activation (Anisomycin treatment) displayed minimal impact.

      Most conclusively, siRNA-mediated silencing of p65 alone was sufficient to:

      Restore epithelial barrier function

      Reverse EMT marker expression

      Reduce bacterial adhesion and invasion

      These results establish a clear hierarchy in pathway importance: p65 serves as the primary mediator of RBMX2's effects, while p38 plays a secondary role and JNK appears non-essential under our experimental conditions. We have now clarified this relationship in the revised Discussion section to strengthen this conclusion.

      This refined understanding of pathway hierarchy provides important mechanistic insights while maintaining consistency with all our experimental data. We thank the reviewer for this valuable suggestion that helped improve our manuscript.

      (3) Metabolomics results are interesting but not integrated deeply into the main EMT narrative.

      Thank you for this constructive suggestion. In this article, we detected the metabolome of RBMX2 knockout and wild-type cells after Mycobacterium bovis infection, which mainly served as supporting evidence for our EMT model. However, we did not conduct an in-depth discussion of these findings. We have now added a detailed discussion of this section to further support our EMT model.

      ADD:Meanwhile, metabolic pathways enriched after RBMX2 deletion, such as nucleotide metabolism, nucleotide sugar synthesis, and pentose interconversion, primarily support cell proliferation and migration during EMT by providing energy precursors, regulating glycosylation modifications, and maintaining redox balance; cofactor synthesis and amino sugar metabolism participate in EMT regulation through influencing metabolic remodeling and extracellular matrix interactions; chemokine and cGMP-PKG signaling pathways may further mediate inflammatory responses and cytoskeletal rearrangements, collectively promoting the EMT process.

      (4) A key finding and starting point of this study is the upregulation of RBMX2 upon M. bovis infection. However, the authors have only assessed RBMX2 expression at the mRNA level following infection with M. bovis and BCG. To strengthen this conclusion, it is essential to validate RBMX2 expression at the protein level through techniques such as Western blotting or immunofluorescence. This would significantly enhance the credibility and impact of the study's foundational observation.

      Thank you for your comment. We have supplemented the experiments in this part and found that Mycobacterium bovis infection can significantly enhance the expression level of RBMX2 protein.

      (5) The manuscript would benefit from a more in-depth discussion of the relationship between tuberculosis (TB) and lung cancer. While the study provides experimental evidence suggesting a link via EMT induction, integrating current literature on the epidemiological and mechanistic connections between chronic TB infection and lung tumorigenesis would provide important context and reinforce the translational relevance of the findings.

      We sincerely appreciate the valuable comments from the reviewer. We fully agree with your suggestion to further explore the relationship between tuberculosis (TB) and lung cancer. In the revised manuscript, we will add a new paragraph in the Discussion section to systematically integrate the current literature on the epidemiological and mechanistic links between chronic tuberculosis infection and lung cancer development, including the potential bridging roles of chronic inflammation, tissue damage repair, immune microenvironment remodeling, and the epithelial-mesenchymal transition (EMT) pathway. This addition will help more comprehensively interpret the clinical implications of the observed EMT activation in the context of our study, thereby enhancing the biological plausibility and clinical translational value of our findings.

      ADD:There is growing epidemiological evidence suggesting that chronic TB infection represents a potential risk factor for the development of lung cancer. Studies have shown that individuals with a history of TB exhibit a significantly increased risk of lung cancer, particularly in areas of the lung with pre-existing fibrotic scars, indicating that chronic inflammation, tissue repair, and immune microenvironment remodeling may collectively contribute to malignant transformation 74. Moreover, EMT not only endows epithelial cells with mesenchymal features that enhance migratory and invasive capacity but is also associated with the acquisition of cancer stem cell-like properties and therapeutic resistance 75. Therefore, EMT may serve as a crucial molecular link connecting chronic TB infection with the malignant transformation of lung epithelial cells, warranting further investigation in the intersection of infection and tumorigenesis.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      I am not familiar with cancer biology, so my review mainly focuses on the infection part of the manuscript. Wang et al identified an RNA-binding protein RBMX2 that links the Mycobacterium bovis infection to the epithelial-Mesenchymal transition and lung cancer progression. Upon mycobacterium infection, the expression of RBMX2 was moderately increased in multiple bovine and human cell lines, as well as bovine lung and liver tissues. Using global approaches, including RNA-seq and proteomics, the authors identified differential gene expression caused by the RBMX2 knockout during M. bovis infection. Knockout of RBMX2 led to significant upregulations of tight-junction related genes such as CLDN-5, OCLN, ZO-1, whereas M. bovis infection affects the integrity of epithelial cell tight junctions and inflammatory responses. This study establishes that RBMX2 is an important host factor that modulates the infection process of M. bovis.

      Strengths:

      (1) This study tested multiple types of bovine and human cells, including macrophages, epithelial cells, and clinical tissues at multiple timepoints, and firmly confirmed the induced expression of RBMX2 upon M. bovis infection.

      (2) The authors have generated the monoclonal RBMX2 knockout cell lines and comprehensively characterized the RBMX2-dependent gene expression changes using a combination of global omics approaches. The study has validated the impact of RBMX2 knockout on the tight-junction pathway and on the M. bovis infection, establishing RBMX2 as a crucial host factor.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The RBMX2 was only moderately induced (less than 2-fold) upon M. bovis infection, arguing its contribution may be small. Its value as a therapeutic target is not justified. How RBMX2 was activated by M. bovis infection was unclear.

      Thank you for your valuable and constructive comments. In this study, we primarily utilized the CRISPR whole-genome screening approach to identify key factors involved in bovine tuberculosis infection. Through four rounds of screening using a whole-genome knockout cell line of bovine lung epithelial cells infected with Mycobacterium bovis, we identified RBMX2 as a critical factor.

      Although the transcriptional level change of RBMX2 was less than two-fold, following the suggestion of Reviewer 1, we examined its expression at the protein level, where the change was more pronounced, and we have added these results to the manuscript.

      Regarding the mechanism by which RBMX2 is activated upon M. bovis infection, we previously screened for interacting proteins using a Mycobacterium tuberculosis secreted and membrane protein library, but unfortunately, we did not identify any direct interacting proteins from M. tuberculosis (https://doi.org/10.1093/nar/gkx1173).

      (2) Although multiple time points have been included in the study, most analyses lack temporal resolution. It is difficult to appreciate the impact/consequence of M. bovis infection on the analyzed pathways and processes.

      We appreciate the valuable comments from the reviewers. Although our study included multiple time points post-infection, in our experimental design we focused on different biological processes and phenotypes at distinct time points:

      During the early phase (e.g., 2 hours post-infection), we focused on barrier phenotypes during the intermediate phase (e.g., 24 hours post-infection), we concentrated more on pathway activation and EMT phenotypes;

      And during the later phase (e.g., 48–72 hours post-infection), we focused more on cell death phenotypes, which were validated in another FII article (https://doi.org/10.3389/fimmu.2024.1431207).

      We also examined the impact of varying infection durations on RBMX2 knockout EBL cellular lines via GO analysis. At 0 hpi, genes were primarily related to the pathways of cell junctions, extracellular regions, and cell junction organization. At 24 hpi, genes were mainly associated with pathways of the basement membrane, cell adhesion, integrin binding and cell migration By 48 hpi, genes were annotated into epithelial cell differentiation and were negatively regulated during epithelial cell proliferation. This indicated that RBMX2 can regulate cellular connectivity throughout the stages of M. bovis infection.

      For KEGG analysis, genes linked to the MAPK signaling pathway, chemical carcinogen-DNA adducts, and chemical carcinogen-receptor activation were observed at 0 hpi. At 24 hpi, significant enrichment was found in the ECM-receptor interaction, PI3K-Akt signaling pathway, and focal adhesion. Upon enrichment analysis at 48 hpi, significant enrichment was noted in the TGF-beta signaling pathway, transcriptional misregulation in cancer, microRNAs in cancer, small cell lung cancer, and p53 signaling pathway.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study investigates the role of the host protein RBMX2 in regulating the response to Mycobacterium bovis infection and its connection to epithelial-mesenchymal transition (EMT), a key pathway in cancer progression. Using bovine and human cell models, the authors have wisely shown that RBMX2 expression is upregulated following M. bovis infection and promotes bacterial adhesion, invasion, and survival by disrupting epithelial tight junctions via the p65/MMP-9 signaling pathway. They also demonstrate that RBMX2 facilitates EMT and is overexpressed in human lung cancers, suggesting a potential link between chronic infection and tumor progression. The study highlights RBMX2 as a novel host factor that could serve as a therapeutic target for both TB pathogenesis and infection-related cancer risk.

      Strengths:

      The major strengths lie in its multi-omics integration (transcriptomics, proteomics, metabolomics) to map RBMX2's impact on host pathways, combined with rigorous functional assays (knockout/knockdown, adhesion/invasion, barrier tests) that establish causality through the p65/MMP-9 axis. Validation across bovine and human cell models and in clinical tissue samples enhances translational relevance. Finally, identifying RBMX2 as a novel regulator linking mycobacterial infection to EMT and cancer progression opens exciting therapeutic avenues.

      Weaknesses:

      Although it's a solid study, there are a few weaknesses noted below.

      (1) In the transcriptomics analysis, the authors performed (GO/KEGG) to explore biological functions. Did they perform the search locally or globally? If the search was performed with a global reference, then I would recommend doing a local search. That would give more relevant results. What is the logic behind highlighting some of the enriched pathways (in red), and how are they relevant to the current study?

      We appreciate the reviewer's thoughtful questions regarding our transcriptomic analysis. In this study, we employed a localized enrichment approach focusing specifically on gene expression profiles from our bovine lung epithelial cell system. This cell-type-specific analysis provides more biologically relevant results than global database searches alone.

      Regarding the highlighted pathways, these represent:

      Temporally significant pathways showing strongest enrichment at each stage:

      (1) 0h: Cell junction organization (immediate barrier response)

      (2) 24h: ECM-receptor interaction (early EMT initiation)

      (3) 48h: TGF-β signaling (chronic remodeling)

      Mechanistically linked to our core findings about RBMX2's role in:

      (1) Epithelial barrier disruption

      (2) Mesenchymal transition

      (3) Chronic infection outcomes

      We selected these particular pathways because they:

      (1) Showed the most statistically significant changes (FDR <0.001)

      (2) Formed a coherent biological narrative across infection stages

      (3) Were independently validated in our functional assays

      This targeted approach allows us to focus on the most infection-relevant pathways while maintaining statistical rigor.

      (2) While the authors show that RBMX2 expression correlates with EMT-related gene expression and barrier dysfunction, the evidence for direct association remains limited in this study. How does RBMX2 activate p65? Does it bind directly to p65 or modulate any upstream kinases? Could ChIP-seq or CLIP-seq provide further evidence for direct RNA or DNA targets of RBMX2 that drive EMT or NF-κB signaling?

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's in-depth questions regarding the mechanisms by which RBMX2 activates p65 and its association with EMT. Although the molecular mechanism remains to be fully elucidated, our study has provided experimental evidence supporting a direct regulatory relationship between RBMX2 and the p65 subunit of the NF-κB pathway. Specifically, we investigated whether the transcription factor p65 could directly bind to the promoter region of RBMX2 using CHIP experiments. The results demonstrated that the transcription factor p65 can physically bind to the RBMX2 region.

      Furthermore, dual-luciferase reporter assays were conducted, showing that p65 significantly enhances the transcriptional activity of the RBMX2 promoter, indicating a direct regulatory effect of RBMX2 on p65 expression.

      These findings support our hypothesis that RBMX2 activates the NF-κB signaling pathway through direct interaction with the p65 protein, thereby participating in the regulation of EMT progression and barrier function.

      In our subsequent work papers, we will also employ experiments such as CLIP to further investigate the specific mechanisms through which RBMX2 exerts its regulatory functions.

      ADD and Revise in Results:

      To thoroughly verify the regulatory mechanism between RBMX2 and p65, we initiated our investigation by conducting an in-depth analysis of the RBMX2 promoter region to identify potential interactions with the transcription factor p65. Initially, we performed molecular docking simulations to predict the binding affinity and interaction patterns between RBMX2 and p65 proteins. These simulations revealed multiple amino acid residues within the RBMX2 protein that formed strong, stable interactions with p65. The docking analysis yielded a high docking score of 1978.643 (Fig. 7K), indicating a significant likelihood of a direct physical interaction between these two proteins.

      To complement the protein-protein interaction analysis, we next investigated whether p65 could directly bind to the promoter region of the RBMX2 gene at the transcriptional level. Using the JASPAR database, a comprehensive resource for transcription factor binding profiles, we queried the RBMX2 promoter sequence for potential p65 binding sites. This analysis identified several putative binding motifs, suggesting that p65 may act as a transcriptional regulator of RBMX2 expression.

      To experimentally validate this transcriptional regulatory relationship, we employed a dual-luciferase reporter assay. We cloned the RBMX2 promoter region containing the predicted p65 binding sites into a luciferase reporter plasmid. This construct was then co-transfected into cultured cells along with a plasmid expressing p65. The luciferase activity was significantly increased in cells expressing p65 compared to control groups, providing functional evidence that p65 enhances the transcriptional activity of the RBMX2 promoter (Fig. 7I).

      Furthermore, to confirm the direct binding of p65 to the RBMX2 promoter in a chromatin context, we performed chromatin immunoprecipitation followed by quantitative PCR (ChIP-qPCR). In this assay, we used specific antibodies against p65 to immunoprecipitate chromatin fragments containing p65-bound DNA. The enriched DNA fragments were then analyzed using primers targeting the RBMX2 promoter region. Our results demonstrated a significant enrichment of the RBMX2 promoter in the p65 immunoprecipitated samples compared to the IgG control, thereby confirming that p65 physically associates with the RBMX2 promoter in vivo (Fig. 7J). Collectively, these findings-ranging from computational docking predictions to transcriptional reporter assays and ChIP validation-provide strong evidence supporting a direct regulatory interaction between p65 and RBMX2. This regulatory mechanism may play a critical role in the biological pathways involving these two molecules, particularly in contexts such as inflammation, immune response, or cellular stress, where p65 (a subunit of NF-κB) is known to be prominently involved.

      (3) The manuscript suggests that RBMX2 enhances adhesion/invasion of several bacterial species (e.g., E. coli, Salmonella), not just M. bovis. This raises questions about the specificity of RBMX2's role in Mycobacterium-specific pathogenesis. Is RBMX2 a general epithelial barrier regulator or does it exhibit preferential effects in mycobacterial infection contexts? How does this generality affect its potential as a TB-specific therapeutic target?

      Thank you for your valuable comments. When we initially designed this experiment, we were interested in whether the RBMX2 knockout cell line could confer effective resistance not only against Mycobacterium bovis but also against Gram-negative and Gram-positive bacteria. Surprisingly, we indeed observed resistance to the invasion of these pathogens, albeit weaker compared to that against Mycobacterium bovis.

      Nevertheless, we believe these findings merit publication in eLife. Moreover, RBMX2 knockout does not affect the phenotype of epithelial barrier disruption under normal conditions; its significant regulatory effect on barrier function is only evident upon infection with Mycobacterium bovis.

      Importantly, during our genome-wide knockout library screening, RBMX2 was not identified in the screening models for Salmonella or Escherichia coli, but was consistently detected across multiple rounds of screening in the Mycobacterium bovis model.

      (4) The quality of the figures is very poor. High-resolution images should be provided.

      Thank you for your feedback; we provided higher-resolution images.

      (5) The methods are not very descriptive, particularly the omics section.

      Thank you for your comments; we have revised the description of the sequencing section.

      (6) The manuscript is too dense, with extensive multi-omics data (transcriptomics, proteomics, metabolomics) but relatively little mechanistic integration. The authors should have focused on the key mechanistic pathways in the figures. Improving the narratives in the Results and Discussion section could help readers follow the logic of the experimental design and conclusions.

      Thank you for your valuable comments. We have streamlined the figures and revised the description of the results section accordingly.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The first part of the results and the major conclusions largely overlap with the previous paper by the same authors (Frontiers in Immunology, https://doi.org/10.3389/fimmu.2024.1431207). The previous paper has already established that RBMX2 is induced upon infection as a host factor, and its knockout led to cell proliferation. Thus, the current paper should focus more on the mechanisms rather than repeating the previous story.

      We appreciate the reviewer's careful reading and constructive feedback. We fully acknowledge the foundational work published in our Frontiers in Immunology paper (doi:10.3389/fimmu.2024.1431207), which established RBMX2 as an infection-induced host factor affecting cell proliferation. The current study represents a significant mechanistic extension of these initial findings, with the following key advances:

      (1) Novel Mechanistic Insights (Current Study Focus):

      Discovery of the p65/MMP-9 pathway as the central mechanism mediating RBMX2's effects on EMT (Figs. 4-6)

      First demonstration of RBMX2's role in epithelial barrier disruption (Figs. 2-3)

      Identification of temporal regulation patterns during infection progression (Fig. 7)

      (2) Expanded Biological Scope:

      Demonstration of RBMX2's function in both bovine and human cell systems (vs. previous bovine-only data)

      Clinical correlation with TB lesions

      Therapeutic potential assessment through pathway inhibition

      (3) Technical Advancements:

      CRISPR-based mechanistic validation (vs. previous siRNA approach)

      Multi-omics integration (transcriptomics + metabolomics)

      Advanced live-cell imaging

      We have now:

      Removed redundant proliferation data from Results

      Sharpened the Introduction to highlight mechanistic questions

      Added explicit discussion comparing both studies

      The current work provides the first comprehensive mechanistic framework for RBMX2's role in TB pathogenesis, moving substantially beyond the initial observational findings. We believe these new insights into the molecular pathways and therapeutic implications represent an important advance for the field..

      (2) Line 107-110: The CRISPR screening results are not provided. Has it been published, or is it an unpublished dataset? RBMX2 knockout cells exhibited 'significant' resistance to the infection. How significant? Data?

      Thank you for your valuable comments. The library mentioned, along with data on another host factor, TOP1, is being submitted by another researcher from our laboratory to a journal, and we will cite each other in the future. RBMX2 ranked second in terms of enrichment among all the identified genes, and its knockout cell line exhibited the second highest anti-infective capacity among all the host factors.

      (3) Line 152: The RNA-seq analysis has already been performed/reported in the previous Frontiers paper. Therein, 173 genes were found to be differentially expressed. In the current paper, 42 genes were differentially expressed in all three time points. If the addition of new time points were the highlight of this paper, why would the authors focus on differentially expressed genes from all three time points?

      Thank you for your valuable comments.

      In the newly added data, we aimed to investigate the temporal changes during Mycobacterium bovis infection of host cells.

      Previous study (Frontiers): Single 24h timepoint → 173 DEGs

      Current study: Three timepoints (0h, 24h, 48h) with 42 consistently regulated genes → Reveals temporally stable core regulators of infection response

      On one hand, we briefly described in the manuscript those important genes that exhibited changes across all time points.

      On the other hand, in the supplementary materials, we also focused on the enriched genes at each individual time point, to better understand the temporal dynamics regulated by RBMX2.

      (4) Line 153: The '0 h' time point is in fact 2 h post-infection. Why did the authors skip the real 0h time point? All the analysis and data should be relative to the 0h pi, rather than relative to the WT at each time point.

      We appreciate the reviewer's important question regarding our timepoint nomenclature. The experimental timeline was designed as follows:

      (1) Infection Protocol:

      2h to 0h: Bacterial co-culture (MOI 20:1)

      0h: Gentamicin (100 μg/ml) added to kill extracellular bacteria

      0h+: Monitored intracellular survival

      (2) Rationale for "0h" Designation:

      This marks the onset of intracellular infection phase when Extracellular bacteria are eliminated (validated by plating)Host cell responses to intracellular pathogens begin All subsequent measurements reflect genuine infection (not attachment)

      (3)Technical Validation:

      Confirmed complete extracellular killing by:

      Culture supernatant plating (0 CFU after gentamycin)

      Microscopy ( no surface-associated bacteria)

      (4) Comparative Analysis:

      All data are presented as:

      Fold-change relative to uninfected controls at each timepoint

      We have now:

      Clarified the timeline in Methods

      Specified "0h = post-gentamicin" in all figure legends

      This standardized approach aligns with established intracellular pathogen studies (e.g., Cell Microbiol. 2018;20:e12840). We're happy to adjust terminology if "0hpi (post-invasion)" would be clearer.

      (5) Figure 2F: The data should be compared to the 0h pi, and show the temporal changes of gene expression.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have added additional information to this section. At the same time, we also aim to focus on the changes in gene expression between RBMX2 knockout and wild-type (WT) samples.

      We have now:

      Added temporal expression profiles relative to 0hpi baseline (SFig.4C).

      Clarified the dual normalization approach in Methods

      Maintained original between-group comparisons for phenotypic correlation

      (6) Line 207. Not all the proteins were down-regulated post-infection.

      Thank you for your comment. The overall level of the Tight junction related protein is downregulated, although it may not show a significant change at a specific time point.

      We have revised our description, changing the keyword from "All" to "Most."

      (7) Line 278, the introduction of the H1299 cell line should appear earlier when it was mentioned for the first time in the manuscript.

      Thank you for your comment. We have provided a description in the abstract and Result1.

      ADD:

      Abstrat: Meanwhile, we also validated the EMT process in human lung epithelial cancer cells H1299.

      Result 1: Furthermore, RBMX2-silenced H1299 cells exhibited a higher survival rate compared to H1299 ShNc cells after M. bovis infection (Fig. 1H).

      (8) Figure 4 is huge and almost illegible, which may be divided into two figures.

      Thank you for your valuable comments. We have streamlined the figures and revised the description of the results section accordingly.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I encountered frequent grammatical and syntactic issues. Thoroughly revising the manuscript for English language and clarity, preferably with professional editing assistance, could increase the quality of the paper.

      Thank you for your valuable comments; we will invite a professional editor to polish the language.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In the manuscript, Aldridge and colleagues investigate the role of IL-27 in regulating hematopoiesis during T. gondii infection. Using loss-of-function approaches, reporter mice, and the generation of serial chimeric mice, they elegantly demonstrate that IL-27 induction plays a critical role in modulating bone marrow myelopoiesis and monocyte generation to the infection site. The study is well-designed, with clear experimental approaches that effectively adddress the mechanisms by which IL-27 regulates bone marrow myelopoiesis and prevents HSC exhaustion.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Aldridge et al. aim to demonstrate the role of IL27 in limiting emergency myelopoiesis in response to Toxoplasma gondii infection by acting directly at the level of early haematopoietic progenitors.

      They used different mouse genetic models, such as HSC lineage tracing, IL27 and IL27R-deficient mice, to show that:

      (1) HSCs actively participate in emergency myelopoiesis during Toxoplasma gondii infection.

      (2) The absence of IL27 and IL27R increases monocyte progenitors and monocytes, mainly inflammatory monocytes CCR2hi.

      (3) At steady state, loss of IL27 impairs HSC fitness as competitive transplantation shows long-term engraftment deficiency of IL27 BM cells. This impairment is exacerbated after infection.

      (4) IL27 is produced by various BM and other tissue cells at steady state, and its expression increases with infection, mainly by increasing the number of monocytes producing it.

      Although it is indisputable that IL27 has a role in emergency myelopoiesis by limiting the number of proinflammatory monocytes in response to infection, the authors' claim that it acts only on HSCs and not on more committed progenitors (CMP, GMP, MP) is not supported by the quality of the data presented here, as described below in the weakness section. In addition, this study highlights a role for IL27 during infection, but does not focus on trained immunity, which is the focus of the targeted elife issue.

      We thank the reviewer for these comments. We did try (and perhaps failed) to highlight that all cells within the HSPC category, which includes HSCs and MPPs, have the potential to contribute. The lack of IRGM1-RFP reporter expression in CMPs (Supp Fig5C) suggests that only HSCs and MPPs are progenitors that respond to IL-27 within the bone marrow, and thus that IL-27 signaling on these contributes to the effects observed on monopoiesis and peripheral monocyte populations. We have emphasized this in the revised manuscript, particularly in the introduction (line 82) and discussion (lines 469-472). While this manuscript does not focus solely on trained immunity, the impacts of infection regulating HSC differentiation and having a long-term impact on this compartment are a central theme of trained immunity. For example, Figure 6 and the supporting supplemental figures almost exclusively focus on the differentiation potential that is programed into LTHSCs by infection and the role of IL-27 in regulating this programing. Additionally, Figure 7 shows the long-term consequences of such training. The introduction      and discussion have been modified  to emphasize these connections to trained immunity.         

      Weakness

      (1) In Figure 4, MFI quantification is required. This figure also shows the expression level (FACS and RNA) in progenitors (GMP and CMP, GP, MP), which is quite similar to that of HSC at this level, so it is really surprising that CMP does not respond at all to IL27 (S5C).

      As requested, we have included the MFIs, calculated as a fold change over control FMOs, in the revised manuscript. While HSPCs and CMPs show relatively similar RNA expression of Il27ra (Supp. Fig. 5 A), the levels of surface IL-27R expression by CMPs is lower than HSPCs (Fig. 4C, revised). Additional downstream progenitors (including GMPs) show highly reduced RNA expression and a corresponding low expression of the receptor protein. This is now more apparent with the quantified MFIs (Fig 4-5).

      (2) Total BM was used to test the direct effect of IL27 on HSC. There could be an indirect effect from other more mature BM cells, even if they show lower receptor expression than HSC. This should be done on a different sorted population to prove the direct effect of IL27 on HSC. The authors need to look more closely at some stat-dependent genes or stat itself in different sorted cell populations, not just irgm1. It is also known that Stat is associated with increased HSC proliferation in response to IFN, which is the opposite of what is observed here.

      We thank the reviewer for this question. We have found that the methanol fixation required to detect pSTAT disrupted the ability to stain for HSPCs by flow cytometry. Thus, we used the IRGM1 reporter, which we have found to be a sensitive and high-fidelity reporter of STAT1 activity while preserving epitope markers of HSPCs.

      We agree that the use of bulk bone marrow in the in vitro stimulations could allow for the activation of non-HSPC cell types that are IL-27R+. This is now emphasized in the text. However, there are advantages to this bulk approach as it allows simultaneous analysis of all HSPC populations and downstream progenitors in the same cultures, allowing the ability to assess how the small numbers of IL-27R expressing lymphocytes present in these cultures respond (data that are now included, Supp. Fig. 5C). These cultures also allow a direct comparison of our IL-27R expression analysis with responsiveness to IL-27. Only a selection of the populations analyzed are shown in these data; however, all populations in Figure 4A were also analyzed in Supp. Fig. 5C. These data sets directly correlate receptor expression with sensitivity to IL-27. If this effect was indirect (i.e the ability of IL-27 to induce IFN-γ) then we would expect more robust expression of the IRGM1 reporter across other cell populations. However, while IFN-γ stimulates broad expression of IRGM1, the effects of IL-27 are restricted to HSPC and mature lymphocytes (Supp. Fig. 5C). In other words, the cells that express the highest levels of the IL-27R are most responsive to IL-27.

      While we do not directly measure HSPC proliferation in these cultures, we agree with the reviewer that the decreased proportions of proliferating HSPCs seen in the absence of IL-27 during infection (Fig. 7A) is a complex data set. The reviewer is also correct that interferons can promote HSC proliferations; however, they can also promote cell stress, DNA damage, and even cell death of HSCs during chronic exposure (reviewed extensively in Demerdash, Y., et al. Exp Hematol. 2021. PMID: 33571568). Thus IFNs, much like IL-27, appear to regulate HSPCs with contextual importance, inducing their proliferation but also death. The activation of STAT1 and STAT3 by IL-27 may be at the core of some of these effects observed in our data, and we point out that IL-10, another activator of STAT1+3, has been shown to limit HSC responses to inflammation (lined 58-62), but we have also presented other possibilities in the discussion.

      (3) The decrease in HSC fitness in IL27R KO at steady state could be an indirect effect of the increase in proinflammatory monocytes contributing to high levels of inflammatory cytokines in the BM and thus chronic HSC activation that is enhanced in response to infection. What is the pro-Inflammatory cytokine profile of the BM of IL27 OR IL27R deficient mice and of mixed chimera mice.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. This was part of our stated rationale in generating the mixed WT:IL-27R-/- BM chimeras presented in Figure 2. In this mixed setting, there remained differences between the ability of the IL-27R sufficient and deficient stem cells to generate inflammatory macrophages. These results suggest that differences in the inflammatory environment do not account for the differences observed. This conclusion is further supported by the observation that the infection-induced levels of IFN-γ in the bone marrow are equivalent in the presence or absence of IL-27 (now included in the revised manuscript, Supp. Fig. 1F).

      (4) Furthermore, the FACS profile of KI67/brdu of Figure 7 is doubtful, as it is shown in different literature that KSL are not predominantly quiescent as shown here, but about 50% are KI67-. This is also inconsistent with the increase of HSC observed in Figure 1. Quantification of total BruDU+ HSC and other progenitors is also important to quantify all cells that have proliferated during infection. As the repopulation of IL27-deficient BM is also lower in the absence of infection the proliation  of HSC in IL27R KO mice in the absence of infection is also important.

      The comment indicates that the reviewer is concerned that our staining for Ki67 is on the low end of reported literature (~10-50% of LSKs, depending on age of the mice and simulation (Thapa R, et al. Stem Cell Res Ther. 2023. PMID: 37280691; Nies KPH, et al. Cytometry A. 2018. PMID: 30176186)). Our stains were performed on cells from infected mice, which does alter the classic markers used to identify HSPCs. For this reason, we are stringent with our gating strategy and may be excluding more HSPCs than are included in other reports. We have included our FMO control in the revised manuscript to indicate our gating approach (Supp. Fig. 9A). While the population of Ki67+ HSPCs is low, these results were consistent between our experiments and provide data sets that are interpretable.

      (5) The immunofluorescence in Figure 3 shows a high level of background and it is difficult to see the GFP and tomato positive cells. In this sense, the number of HSCs quantified as Procr+ (more than 8000 on a single BM section) is inconsistent with the total number of HSCs that a BM can contain (i.e., around 6000 per BM as quantified in Figure 1).

      We agree with the reviewer and have found that there is a high level of background in these stains. We have thresholded these images, as described in our methods, to minimize this. Additionally, the increased numbers of Procr+ cells in the imaging vs our flow data is expected, and has been reported by others (Steinert, EM, et al. Cell. 2015. PMID: 25957682).

      (6) The addition of arrows to the figure will help to visualise positive cells. It is also not clear why the author normalised the GFP+ cells to the tomato+ cells in Figure 3D.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and have added the suggested arrows. We have also included a more detailed explanation for our normalization strategy.

      (7) Furthermore, even if monocytes represent a high proportion of IL27-producing cells, they are only 50% of the cells at 5dpi, as shown in Figure 3 and S4. Without other monocyte markers, line 307 is incorrect.

      We thank the reviewer for this clarification and have adjusted the text accordingly.

      (8) How do the authors explain that in Figure 1, 5-10% of labelled precursors and monocytes can give 100% of monocytes? This would mean that only labelled HSC can differentiate into PEC monocytes. 5

      We thank the reviewer for their interest in this result. Monocytes and macrophages are some

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I have two minor comments that could enhance the conceptual framework of this study:

      (1) The authors indirectly show that IL-27R expression on HSPCs is necessary for regulating HSC proliferation and preventing exhaustion. However, given that they have access to IL-27RFlox mice, they could cross these with Fgd5Cre mice to specifically delete IL-27R on long-term HSCs. This would provide direct evidence for the role of IL-27 signaling in LTHSCs during infection.

      We appreciate this comment and did attempt this experiment with several HSPC specific Cres, including the Procr-cre (used elsewhere in the manuscript) and the MDS1-cre-ERT2 (Jackson Laboratory Strain #:032863). Unfortunately, validation revealed that deletion efficiency of the IL-27R with these HSCspecific Cre lines was inefficient, and so experiments are ongoing to enhance efficiency of the deletion and test alternative Cre lines (such as the Fgd5-cre).

      (2) Since memory T and B cells often home to the bone marrow, it would be interesting to consider the potential cross-talk between these cells, HSPCs, and IL-27 signaling during secondary T. gondii infection. A brief discussion of this possibility would strengthen the study's broader implications.

      We thank the reviewer for this opportunity. We have previously investigated the interplay between immune cells in the bone marrow (Glatman Zaretsky A, et al. Cell Rep. 2017. PMID: 28228257) and now include these possibilities in the discussion (line 465-470).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor points:

      (1) Figures 6F and 7B: should be shown as % of donor and not total number to clarify the lineage potency of LTHSC. The fact that the results of transplantation are separated into different figures makes it not easy to follow. To see if the increase in monocyte production by IL27 KO BM is specific, the percent of donorderived cells for other populations, such as lymphoid, but also in MP, and inflammatory monocytes, is necessary to confirm Figure 2.

      Perhaps there has been a misunderstanding? In these plots, we are not analyzing mixed chimeras but single transfer chimeras into lethally irradiated hosts. Thus, the % of donor reaches ~80- 90%. However, to measure the actual output of the HSPCs, the cell number was necessary to compare amongst groups. Additional description is provided in the figure legends and in the text of the manuscript (lines 391-392, 434-436, 651-653, and 680-682).

      (2) The heavy UMAP description is unnecessary. Responses As requested, we have reduced this description of how the UMAPs were derived.

      As requested, we have reduced this description of how the UMAPs were derived

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors were attempting to describe whether trained innate immunity would modulate antibody-dependent cellular phagocytosis (ADCP) and/or efferocytosis.

      Strengths:

      The use of primary murine macrophages, and not a cell line, is considered a strength. The trained immunity-mediated changes to phagocytosis affected both melanoma and breast cancer cells. The broad effect is consistent with trained immunity.

      Weaknesses:

      The most significant weakness, also noted by the authors in the discussion, is the lack of in vivo data. Without these data, it is not possible to put the in vitro data in context. It is unknown if the described effects on efferocytosis will be relevant to the in vivo progression of cancer.

      We thank the reviewer for these comments. To examine the role of trained immunity on the modulation of macrophage efferocytosis in vivo, we performed immunostaining analysis in sections from B16F10 tumour samples.

      Importantly, we found that macrophage efferocytosis of apoptotic tumour cells was significantly decreased in the tumour tissue that was excised from mice treated with β-glucan 7 days prior to tumour inoculation (supplementary Figure 3). These data are consistent with our findings using co-culture assays further strengthening the impact of our key findings in this report.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors follow up their preclinical work on beta-glucan-induced trained immunity in murine tumor models that they published in Cell in 2020. In particular, they focus on the role of trained immunity and efferocytosis of cancer cells

      Strengths:

      While properly conducted, the work is underwhelming and fully depends on in vitro observations performed with co-cultures of bone marrow derived macrophages from beta-glucantreated mice and tumor cell lines. From these in vitro studies, the authors conclude that trained immunity induction has no effect on antibody-dependent cellular phagocytosis, while it decreases efferocytosis.

      Weaknesses:

      It would be important to study these phenomena in tumor mouse models in vivo. The authors clearly have the expertise as they have shown in previous studies. Especially because the in vitro observation appears to conflict with the in vivo anti-tumor found in mice prophylactically treated with beta-glucan. Clearly, trained immunity is associated with diverse cellular responses and mechanisms, some of which may promote tumor growth, as the current manuscript suggests, but in the absence of in vivo studies, it is merely a mechanistic exercise of which the relevance is difficult to determine.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important comment. We have followed reviewer’s suggestion and examined the role of trained immunity on the modulation of macrophage efferocytosis in vivo. As mentioned in our response to Reviewer 1, we demonstrate that efferocytosis of apoptotic melanoma cells in situ was attenuated in tumour samples from ‘trained’ mice as compared to those from controltreated mice.

      Efferocytosis displays a pro-tumour and immunosuppressive role, therefore both our in vitro co-culture (Figure 1) and in vivo (supplementary Figure 3) findings are consistent with our previously published in vivo data supporting the tumour-suppressive role of prophylactic treatment with β-glucan (Kalafati, Kourtzelis et al, PMID: 33125892). 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Chatzis et al showed that β-glucan trained macrophages have decreased phagocytic activity of apoptotic tumor cells and that is accompanied by lower levels of secreted IL-1β using a mouse model. Strengths: This finding has a potential impact on designing new cancer immunotherapeutic approaches by targeting macrophage efferocytosis.

      Weaknesses:

      Whether this finding could be applied to other scenarios is underdetermined.

      (1)  Does the decrease of efferocytosis also occur in human monocytes/macrophages after training?

      (2)  Both β-glucan and BCG are well-trained innate immunity agents, the authors showed that β-glucan decreased efferocytosis via IL-1 β, so it is interesting to know whether BCG has a similar effect.

      We thank the reviewer for these comments. Our data suggest that induction of trained immunity with β-glucan contributes to decreased macrophage efferocytosis of tumour cells based on co-culture and in vivo approaches in a mouse setting.  

      We agree with the reviewer that utilisation of a human setting would be important to provide additional validation of our findings.

      Induction of trained immunity entails epigenetic and metabolic reprogramming of hematopoietic stem and progenitor cells (HSPCs). As such, the elucidation of mechanisms that modulate trained immunity in human cells would require the establishment of a macrophage differentiation model based on the use of HSPCs rather than the stimulation of monocytes or macrophages with β-glucan.

      Additionally, the investigation of the impact of BCG in trained immunity-dependent phagocytosis would require the assessment of all different types of phagocytic cargos (apoptotic melanoma and breast cancer cells, apoptotic neutrophils, microbial bioparticles) as we did in the case of the β-glucan.  The capacity of different molecules to induce trained immunity in the efferocytosis setting requires further investigation that would be beyond the scope of this study. Therefore, we plan to address these very interesting points in a future study.

      Additional text was added in the Discussion section to clarify the reviewer's points. In addition, we provide a more specific title that reflects better the specificity of our findings.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary:

      This fundamental work employed multidisciplinary approaches and conducted rigorous experiments to study how a specific subset of neurons in the dorsal striatum (i.e., "patchy" striatal neurons) modulates locomotion speed depending on the valence of the naturalistic context. 

      Strengths: 

      The scientific findings are novel and original and significantly advance our understanding of how the striatal circuit regulates spontaneous movement in various contexts.  Response: We appreciate the reviewer’s positive evaluation.

      Weaknesses: 

      This is extensive research involving various circuit manipulation approaches. Some of these circuit manipulations are not physiological. A balanced discussion of the technical strengths and limitations of the present work would be helpful and beneficial to the field. Minor issues in data presentation were also noted. 

      We have incorporated the recommended discussion of technical limitations and addressed the physiological plausibility of our manipulations on Page 33 of the revised Discussion section. Specifically, we wrote: 

      “Judicious interpretation of the present data must consider the technical limitations of the various methods and circuit-level manipulations applied. Patchy neurons are distributed unevenly across the extensive structure of the striatum, and their targeted manipulation is constrained by viral spread in the dorsal striatum. Somatic calcium imaging using single-photon microscopy captures activity from only a subset of patchy neurons within a narrow focal plane beneath each implanted GRIN lens. Similarly, limitations in light diffusion from optical fibers may reduce the effective population of targeted fibers in both photometry and optogenetic experiments. For example, the more modest locomotor slowing observed with optogenetic activation of striatonigral fibers in the SNr compared to the stronger effects seen with Gq-DREADD activation across the dorsal striatum could reflect limited fiber optic coverage in the SNr.Alternatively, it may suggest that non-striatonigral mechanisms also contribute to generalized slowing. Our photometry data do not support a role for striatopallidal projections from patchy neurons in movement suppression. The potential contribution of intrastriatal mechanisms, discussed earlier, remains to be empirically tested. Although the behavioral assays used were naturalistic, many of the circuit-level interventions were not. Broad ablation or widespread activation of patchy neurons and their efferent projections represent non-physiological manipulations. Nonetheless, these perturbation results are interpreted alongside more naturalistic observations, such as in vivo imaging of patchy neuron somata and axon terminals, to form a coherent understanding of their functional role”.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Hawes et al. investigated the role of striatal neurons in the patch compartment of the dorsal striatum. Using Sepw1-Cre line, the authors combined a modified version of the light/dark transition box test that allows them to examine locomotor activity in different environmental valence with a variety of approaches, including cell-type-specific ablation, miniscope calcium imaging, fiber photometry, and opto-/chemogenetics. First, they found ablation of patchy striatal neurons resulted in an increase in movement vigor when mice stayed in a safe area or when they moved back from more anxiogenic to safe environments. The following miniscope imaging experiment revealed that a larger fraction of striatal patchy neurons was negatively correlated with movement speed, particularly in an anxiogenic area. Next, the authors investigated differential activity patterns of patchy neurons' axon terminals, focusing on those in GPe, GPi, and SNr, showing that the patchy axons in SNr reflect movement speed/vigor. Chemogenetic and optogenetic activation of these patchy striatal neurons suppressed the locomotor vigor, thus demonstrating their causal role in the modulation of locomotor vigor when exposed to valence differentials. Unlike the activation of striatal patches, such a suppressive effect on locomotion was absent when optogenetically activating matrix neurons by using the Calb1-Cre line, indicating distinctive roles in the control of locomotor vigor by striatal patch and matrix neurons. Together, they have concluded that nigrostriatal neurons within striatal patches negatively regulate movement vigor, dependent on behavioral contexts where motivational valence differs.

      We are grateful for the reviewer’s thorough summary of our main findings.

      In my view, this study will add to the important literature by demonstrating how patch (striosomal) neurons in the striatum control movement vigor. This study has applied multiple approaches to investigate their functionality in locomotor behavior, and the obtained data largely support their conclusions. Nevertheless, I have some suggestions for improvements in the manuscript and figures regarding their data interpretation, accuracy, and efficacy of data presentation

      We appreciate the reviewer’s overall positive assessment and have made substantial improvements to the revised manuscript in response to reviewers’ constructive suggestions.

      (1) The authors found that the activation of the striatonigral pathway in the patch compartment suppresses locomotor speed, which contradicts with canonical roles of the direct pathway. It would be great if the authors could provide mechanistic explanations in the Discussion section. One possibility is that striatal D1R patch neurons directly inhibit dopaminergic cells that regulate movement vigor (Nadal et al., Sci. Rep., 2021; Okunomiya et al., J Neurosci., 2025). Providing plausible explanations will help readers infer possible physiological processes and give them ideas for future follow-up studies.

      We have added the recommended data interpretation and future perspectives on Page 30 of the revised Discussion section. Specifically, we wrote:

      “Potential mechanisms by which striatal patchy neurons reduce locomotion involve the supression of dopamine availability within the striatum. Dopamine, primarily supplied by neurons in the SNc and VTA,broadly facilitates locomotion (Gerfen and Surmeier 2011, Dudman and Krakauer 2016). Recent studies have shown that direct activation of patchy neurons leads to a reduction in striatal dopamine levels, accompanied by decreased walking speed (Nadel, Pawelko et al. 2021, Dong, Wang et al. 2025, Okunomiya, Watanabe et al. 2025). Patchy neuron projections terminate in structures known as “dendron bouquets”, which enwrap SNc dendrites within the SNr and can pause tonic dopamine neuron firing (Crittenden, Tillberg et al. 2016, Evans, Twedell et al. 2020). The present work highlights a role for patchy striatonigral inputs within the SN in decelerating movement, potentially through GABAergic dendron bouquets that limit dopamine release back to the striatum (Dong, Wang et al. 2025). Additionally, intrastriatal collaterals of patch spiny projection neurons (SPNs) have been shown to suppress dopamine release and associated synaptic plasticity via dynorphin-mediated activation of kappa opioid receptors on dopamine terminals (Hawes, Salinas et al. 2017). This intrastriatal mechanism may further contribute to the reduction in striatal dopamine levels and the observed decrease in locomotor speed, representing a compelling avenue for future investigation.”

      (2) On page 14, Line 301, the authors stated that "Cre-dependent mCheery signals were colocalized with the patch marker (MOR1) in the dorsal striatum (Fig. 1B)". But I could not find any mCherry on that panel, so please modify it.

      We have included representative images of mCherry and MOR1 staining in Supplementary Fig. S1 of the revised manuscript.

      (3) From data shown in Figure 1, I've got the impression that mice ablated with striatal patch neurons were generally hyperactive, but this is probably not the case, as two separate experiments using LLbox and DDbox showed no difference in locomotor vigor between control and ablated mice. For the sake of better interpretation, it may be good to add a statement in Lines 365-366 that these experiments suggest the absence of hyperactive locomotion in general by ablating these specific neurons.

      As suggested by the reviewer, we have added the following statement on Page 17 of the revised manuscript: “These data also indicate that PA elevates valence-specific speed without inducing general hyperactivity”.

      (4) In Line 536, where Figure 5A was cited, the author mentioned that they used inhibitory DREADDs (AAV-DIO-hM4Di-mCherrry), but I could not find associated data on Figure 5. Please cite Figure S3, accordingly.

      We have added the citation for the now Fig. S4 on Page 25 of the revised manuscript.

      (5) Personally, the Figure panel labels of "Hi" and "ii" were confusing at first glance. It would be better to have alternatives.

      As suggested by the reviewer, we have now labeled each figure panel with a distinct single alphabetical letter.

      (6) There is a typo on Figure 4A: tdTomata → tdTomato

      We have made the correction on the figure.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Hawes et al. combined behavioral, optical imaging, and activity manipulation techniques to investigate the role of striatal patch SPNs in locomotion regulation. Using Sepw1-Cre transgenic mice, they found that patch SPNs encode locomotion deceleration in a light-dark box procedure through optical imaging techniques. Moreover, genetic ablation of patch SPNs increased locomotion speed, while chemogenetic activation of these neurons decreased it. The authors concluded that a subtype of patch striatonigral neurons modulates locomotion speed based on external environmental cues. Below are some major concerns:

      The study concludes that patch striatonigral neurons regulate locomotion speed. However, unless I missed something, very little evidence is presented to support the idea that it is specifically striatonigral neurons, rather than striatopallidal neurons, that mediate these effects. In fact, the optogenetic experiments shown in Fig. 6 suggest otherwise. What about the behavioral effects of optogenetic stimulation of striatonigral versus striatopallidal neuron somas in Sepw1-Cre mice?

      Our photometry data implicate striatonigral neurons in locomotor slowing, as evidenced by a negative cross-correlation with acceleration and a negative lag, indicating that their activity reliably precedes—and may therefore contribute to—deceleration. In contrast, photometry results from striatopallidal neurons showed no clear correlation with speed or acceleration.

      Figure 6 demonstrates that optogenetic manipulation within the SNr of Sepw1-Cre<sup>+</sup> striatonigral axons recapitulated context-dependent locomotor changes seen with Gq-DREADD activation of both striatonigral and striatopallidal Sepw1-Cre<sup>+</sup> cells in the dorsal striatum but failed to produce the broader locomotor speed change observed when targeting all Sepw1-Cre<sup>+</sup> cells in the dorsal striatum using either ablation or Gq-DREADD activation. The more subtle speed-restrictive phenotype resulting from ChR activation in the SNr could, as the reviewer suggests, implicate striatopallidal neurons in broad locomotor speed regulation. However, our photometry data indicate that this scenario is unlikely, as activity of striatopallidal Sepw1-Cre<sup>+</sup> fibers is not correlated with locomotor speed. Another plausible explanation is that the optogenetic approach may have affected fewer striatonigral fibers, potentially due to the limited spatial spread of light from the optical fiber within the SNr. Broad locomotor speed change in LDbox might require the recruitment of a larger number of striatonigral fibers than we were able to manipulate with optogenetics. We have added discussion of these technical limitations to the revised manuscript. Additionally, we now discuss the possibility that intrastriatal collaterals may contribute to reduced local dopamine levels by releasing dynorphin, which acts on kappa opioid receptors located on dopamine fibers (Hawes, Salinas et al. 2017), thereby suppressing dopamine release.

      The reviewer also suggests an interesting experiment involving optogenetic stimulation of striatonigral versus striatopallidal somata in Sepw1-Cre mice. While we agree that this approach would yield valuable insights, we have thus far been unable to achieve reliable results using retroviral vectors. Moreover, selectively targeting striatopallidal terminals optogenetically remains technically challenging, as striatonigral fibers also traverse the pallidum, and the broad anatomical distribution of the pallidum complicates precise targeting. This proposed work will need to be pursued in a future study, either with improved retrograde viral tools or the development of additional mouse lines that offer more selective access to these neuronal populations as we documented recently (Dong, Wang et al. 2025).

      In the abstract, the authors state that patch SPNs control speed without affecting valence. This claim seems to lack sufficient data to support it. Additionally, speed, velocity, and acceleration are very distinct qualities. It is necessary to clarify precisely what patch neurons encode and control in the current study.

      We believe the reviewer’s interpretation pertains to a statement in the Introduction rather than the Abstract: “Our findings reveal that patchy SPNs control the speed at which mice navigate the valence differential between high- and low-anxiety zones, without affecting valence perception itself.” Throughout our study, mice consistently preferred the dark zone in the Light/Dark box, indicating intact perception of the valence differential between illuminated areas. While our manipulations altered locomotor speed, they did not affect time spent in the dark zone, supporting the conclusion that valence perception remained unaltered. We appreciate the reviewer’s insight and agree it is an intriguing possibility that locomotor responses could, over time, influence internal states such as anxiety. We addressed this in the Discussion, noting that while dark preference was robust to our manipulations, future studies are warranted to explore the relationship between anxious locomotor vigor and anxiety itself. We report changes in scalar measures of animal speed across Light/Dark box conditions and under various experimental manipulations. Separately, we show that activity in both patchy neuron somata and striatonigral fibers is negatively correlated with acceleration—indicating a positive correlation with deceleration. Notably, the direction of the cross-correlational lag between striatonigral fiber activity and acceleration suggests that this activity precedes and may causally contribute to mouse deceleration, thereby influencing reductions in speed. To clarify this, we revised a sentence in the Results section:

      “Moreover, patchy neuron efferent activity at the SNr may causally contribute to deceleration, asindicated by the negative cross-correlational lag, thereby reducing animal speed.”. We also updated the Discussion to read: “Together, these data specifically implicate patchy striatonigral neurons in slowing locomotion by acting within the SNr to drive deceleration.”

      One of the major results relies on chemogenetic manipulation (Figure 5). It would be helpful to demonstrate through slice electrophysiology that hM3Dq and hM4Di indeed cause changes in the activity of dorsal striatal SPNs, as intended by the DREADD system. This would support both the positive (Gq) and negative (Gi) findings, where no effects on behavior were observed.

      We were unable to perform this experiment; however, hM3Dq has previously been shown to be effective in striatal neurons (Alcacer, Andreoli et al. 2017). The lack of effect observed in GiDREADD mice serves as an unintended but valuable control, helping to rule out off-target effects of the DREADD agonist JHU37160 and thereby reinforcing the specificity of hM3Dq-mediated activation in our study. We have now included an important caveat regarding the Gi-DREADD results, acknowledging the possibility that they may not have worked effectively in our target cells:

      “Potential explanations for the negative results in Gi-DREADD mice include inherently low basal activity among patchy neurons or insufficient expression of GIRK channels in striatal neurons, which may limit the effectiveness of Gicoupling in suppressing neuronal activity (Shan, Fang et al. 2022).”

      Finally, could the behavioral effects observed in the current study, resulting from various manipulations of patch SPNs, be due to alterations in nigrostriatal dopamine release within the dorsal striatum?

      We agree that this is an important potential implication of our work, especially given that we and others have shown that patchy striatonigral neurons provide strong inhibitory input to dopaminergic neurons involved in locomotor control (Nadel, Pawelko et al. 2021, Lazaridis, Crittenden et al. 2024, Dong, Wang et al. 2025, Okunomiya, Watanabe et al. 2025). Accordingly, we have expanded the discussion section to include potential mechanistic explanations that support and contextualize our main findings.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Here are some minor issues for the authors' reference:

      (1) This work supports the motor-suppressing effect of patchy SPNs, and >80% of them are direct pathway SPNs. This conclusion is not expected from the traditional basal ganglia direct/indirect pathway model. Most experiments were performed using nonphysiological approaches to suppress (i.e., ablation) or activate (i.e., continuous chemo-optogenetic stimulation). It remains uncertain if the reported observations are relevant to the normal biological function of patchy SPNs under physiological conditions. Particularly, under what circumstances an imbalanced patch/matrix activity may be induced, as proposed in the sections related to the data presented in Figure 6. A thorough discussion and clarification remain needed. Or it should be discussed as a limitation of the present work.

      We have added discussion and clarification of physiological limitations in response to reviewer feedback. Additionally, we revised the opening sentence of an original paragraph in the discussion section to emphasize that it interprets our findings in the context of more physiological studies reporting natural shifts in patchy SPN activity due to cognitive conflict, stress, or training. The revised opening sentence now reads: “Together with previous studies of naturally occurring shifts in patchy neuron activation, these data illustrate ethologically relevant roles for a subgroup of genetically defined patchy neurons in behavior.”

      (2) Lines 499-500: How striato-nigral cells encode speed and deceleration deserves a thorough discussion and clarification. These striatonigral cells can target both SNr GABAergic neurons and dendrites of the dopaminergic neurons. A discussion of microcircuits formed by the patchy SPNs axons in the SNr GABAergic and SNC DAergic neurons should be presented.

      We have added this point at lines 499–500, including a reference to a relevant review of microcircuitry. Additionally, we expanded the discussion section to address microcircuit mechanisms that may underlie our main findings.

      (3) Line 70: "BNST" should be spelled out at the first time it is mentioned.

      This has been done.

      (4) Line 133: only GCaMP6 was listed in the method, but GCaMP8 was also used (Figure 4). Clarification or details are needed.

      Thank you for your careful attention to detail. We have corrected the typographical errors in the Methods section. Specifically, in the Stereotaxic Injections section, we corrected “GCaMP83” to “GCaMP8s.” In the Fiber Implant section, we removed the incorrect reference to “GCaMP6s” and clarified that GCaMP8s was used for photometry, and hChR2 was used for optogenetics.

      (5) Line 183: Can the authors describe more precisely what "a moment" means in terms of seconds or minutes?

      This has been done.

      (6) Line 288: typo: missing / in ΔF

      Thank you this has been fixed

      (7) Line 301-302: the statement of "mCherry and MOR1 colocalization" does not match the images in Figure 1B.

      This has been corrected by proving a new Supplementary Figure S1.

      (8) Related to the statement between Lines 303-304: Figure 1c data may reflect changes in MOR1 protein or cell loss. Quantification of NeuN+ neurons within the MOR1 area would strengthen the conclusion of 60% of patchy cell loss in Figure 1C

      Since the efficacy of AAV-FLEX-taCasp3 in cell ablation has been well established in our previous publications and those of others (Yang, Chiang et al. 2013, Wu, Kung et al. 2019), we do not believe the observed loss of MOR1 staining in Fig. 1C merely reflects reduced MOR1 expression. Moreover, a general neuronal marker such as NeuN may not reliably detect the specific loss of patchy neurons in our ablation model, given the technical limitations of conventional cell-counting methods like MBF’s StereoInvestigator, which typically exhibit a variability margin of 15–20%.

      (9) Lines 313-314: "Similarly, PA mice demonstrated greater stay-time in the dark zone (Figure 1E)." Revision is needed to better reflect what is shown in Figure 1E and avoid misunderstandings.

      Thank you this has been addressed.

      (10) The color code in Figure 2Gi seems inconsistent with the others? Clarifications are needed

      Color coding in Figure 2Gi differs from that in 2Eii out of necessity. For example, the "Light" cells depicted in light blue in 2Eii are represented by both light gray and light red dots in 2Gi. Importantly, Figure 2G does not encode specific speed relationships; instead, any association with speed is indicated by a red hue.

      (11) Lines 538-539: the statement of "Over half of the patch was covered" was not supported by Figure 5C. Clarification is needed.

      Thank you. For clarity, we updated the x-axis labels in Figures 1C and 5C from “% area covered” to “% DS area covered,” and defined “DS” as “dorsal striatal” in the corresponding figure legends. Additionally, we revised the sentence in question to read: “As with ablation, histological examination indicated that a substantial fraction of dorsal patch territories, identified through MOR1 staining, were impacted (Fig. 5C).”

      (12) Figure 3: statistical significance in Figure 3 should be labeled in various panels.

      We believe the reviewer's concern pertains to the scatter plot in panel F—specifically, whether the data points are significantly different from zero. In panel 3F, the 95% confidence interval clearly overlaps with zero, indicating that the results are not statistically significant.

      (13) Figures 6D-E: no difference in the speed of control mice and ChR2 mice under continuous optical stimulation was not expected. It was different from Gq-DRADDS study in Figure 5E-F. Clarifications are needed.

      For mice undergoing constant ChR2 activation of Sepw1-Cre+ SNr efferents, overall locomotor speed does not differ from controls. However, the BIL (bright-to-illuminated) effect on zone transitions isdisrupted: activating Sepw1-Cre<sup>+ </sup> fibers in the SNr blunts the typical increase in speed observed when mice flee from the light zone toward the dark zone. This impaired BIL-related speed increase upon exiting the light was similarly observed in the Gq-DREADD cohort. The reviewer is correct that this optogenetic manipulation within the SNr did not produce the more generalized speed reductions seen with broader Gq-DREADD activation of all Sepw1-Cre<sup>+ </sup> cells in the dorsal striatum. A likely explanation is the difference in targeting—ChR2 specifically activates SNr-bound terminals, whereas Gq-DREADD broadly activates entire Sepw1-Cre<sup>+ </sup> cells. Notably, many of the generalized speed profile changes observed with chemogenetic activation are opposite to those resulting from broad ablation of Sepw1-Cre<sup>+ </sup> cells. The more subtle speed-restrictive phenotype observed with ChR2 activation targeted to the SNr may suggest that fewer striatonigral fibers were affected by this technique, possibly due to the limited spread of light from the fiber optic. Broad locomotor speed change in LDbox might require the recruitment of a larger number of striatonigral fibers than we were able to manipulate with an optogenetic approach. Alternatively, it could indicate that non-striatonigral Sepw1-Cre<sup>+ </sup> projections—such as striatopallidal or intrastriatal pathways—play a role in more generalized slowing. If striatopallidal fibers contributed to locomotor slowing, we would expect to see non-zero cross-correlations between neural activity and speed or acceleration, along with negative lag indicating that neural activity precedes the behavioral change. However, our fiber photometry data do not support such a role for Sepw1-Cre<sup>+ </sup> striatopallidal fibers. We have also referenced the possibility that intrastriatal collaterals could suppress striatal dopamine levels, potentially explaining the stronger slowing phenotype observed when the entire striatal population is affected, as opposed to selectively targeting striatonigral terminals. These technical considerations and interpretive nuances have been incorporated and clarified in the revised discussion section.

      (14) Lines 632: "compliment": a typo?

      Yes, it should be “complement”.

      (15) Figure 4 legend: descriptions of panels A and B were swapped

      Thank you. This has been corrected.

      (16) Friedman (2020) was listed twice in the bibliography (Lines 920-929).

      Thank you. This has been corrected.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      It will be helpful to label and add figure legends below each figure.

      Thank you for the suggestion.

      Editor's note:

      Should you choose to revise your manuscript, if you have not already done so, please include full statistical reporting including exact p-values wherever possible alongside the summary statistics (test statistic and df) and, where appropriate, 95% confidence intervals. These should be reported for all key questions and not only when the p-value is less than 0.05 in the main manuscript. We noted some instances where only p values are reported.

      Readers would also benefit from coding individual data points by sex and noting N/sex

      We have included detailed statistical information in the revised manuscript. Both male and female mice were used in all experiments in approximately equal numbers. Since no sex-related differences were observed, we did not report the number of animals by sex.

      References

      Alcacer, C., L. Andreoli, I. Sebastianutto, J. Jakobsson, T. Fieblinger and M. A. Cenci (2017). "Chemogenetic stimulation of striatal projection neurons modulates responses to Parkinson's disease therapy." J Clin Invest 127(2): 720-734.

      Crittenden, J. R., P. W. Tillberg, M. H. Riad, Y. Shima, C. R. Gerfen, J. Curry, D. E. Housman, S. B. Nelson, E. S. Boyden and A. M. Graybiel (2016). "Striosome-dendron bouquets highlight a unique striatonigral circuit targeting dopamine-containing neurons." Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 113(40): 1131811323.

      Dong, J., L. Wang, B. T. Sullivan, L. Sun, V. M. Martinez Smith, L. Chang, J. Ding, W. Le, C. R. Gerfen and H. Cai (2025). "Molecularly distinct striatonigral neuron subtypes differentially regulate locomotion." Nat Commun 16(1): 2710.

      Dudman, J. T. and J. W. Krakauer (2016). "The basal ganglia: from motor commands to the control of vigor." Curr Opin Neurobiol 37: 158-166.

      Evans, R. C., E. L. Twedell, M. Zhu, J. Ascencio, R. Zhang and Z. M. Khaliq (2020). "Functional Dissection of Basal Ganglia Inhibitory Inputs onto Substantia Nigra Dopaminergic Neurons." Cell Rep 32(11): 108156.

      Gerfen, C. R. and D. J. Surmeier (2011). "Modulation of striatal projection systems by dopamine." Annual review of neuroscience 34: 441-466.

      Hawes, S. L., A. G. Salinas, D. M. Lovinger and K. T. Blackwell (2017). "Long-term plasticity of corticostriatal synapses is modulated by pathway-specific co-release of opioids through kappa-opioid receptors." J Physiol 595(16): 5637-5652.

      Lazaridis, I., J. R. Crittenden, G. Ahn, K. Hirokane, T. Yoshida, A. Mahar, V. Skara, K. Meletis, K.Parvataneni, J. T. Ting, E. Hueske, A. Matsushima and A. M. Graybiel (2024). "Striosomes Target Nigral Dopamine-Containing Neurons via Direct-D1 and Indirect-D2 Pathways Paralleling Classic DirectIndirect Basal Ganglia Systems." bioRxiv.

      Nadel, J. A., S. S. Pawelko, J. R. Scott, R. McLaughlin, M. Fox, M. Ghanem, R. van der Merwe, N. G. Hollon, E. S. Ramsson and C. D. Howard (2021). "Optogenetic stimulation of striatal patches modifies habit formation and inhibits dopamine release." Sci Rep 11(1): 19847.

      Okunomiya, T., D. Watanabe, H. Banno, T. Kondo, K. Imamura, R. Takahashi and H. Inoue (2025).

      "Striosome Circuitry Stimulation Inhibits Striatal Dopamine Release and Locomotion." J Neurosci 45(4).

      Shan, Q., Q. Fang and Y. Tian (2022). "Evidence that GIRK Channels Mediate the DREADD-hM4Di Receptor Activation-Induced Reduction in Membrane Excitability of Striatal Medium Spiny Neurons." ACS Chem Neurosci 13(14): 2084-2091.

      Wu, J., J. Kung, J. Dong, L. Chang, C. Xie, A. Habib, S. Hawes, N. Yang, V. Chen, Z. Liu, R. Evans, B. Liang, L. Sun, J. Ding, J. Yu, S. Saez-Atienzar, B. Tang, Z. Khaliq, D. T. Lin, W. Le and H. Cai (2019). "Distinct Connectivity and Functionality of Aldehyde Dehydrogenase 1a1-Positive Nigrostriatal Dopaminergic Neurons in Motor Learning." Cell Rep 28(5): 1167-1181 e1167.

      Wu, J., J. Kung, J. Dong, L. Chang, C. Xie, A. Habib, S. Hawes, N. Yang, V. Chen, Z. Liu, R. Evans, B. Liang, L. Sun, J. Ding, J. Yu, S. Saez-Atienzar, B. Tang, Z. Khaliq, D. T. Lin, W. Le and H. Cai (2019). "Distinct Connectivity and Functionality of Aldehyde Dehydrogenase 1a1-Positive Nigrostriatal Dopaminergic Neurons in Motor Learning." Cell Rep 28(5): 1167-1181 e1167.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have investigated the role of GAT3 in the visual system. First, they have developed a CRISPR/Cas9-based approach to locally knock out this transporter in the visual cortex. They then demonstrated electrophysiologically that this manipulation increases inhibitory synaptic input into layer 2/3 pyramidal cells. They further examined the functional consequences by imaging neuronal activity in the visual cortex in vivo. They found that the absence of GAT3 leads to reduced spontaneous neuronal activity and attenuated neuronal responses and reliability to visual stimuli, but without an effect on orientation selectivity. Further analysis of this data suggests that Gat3 removal leads to less coordinated activity between individual neurons and in population activity patterns, thereby impairing information encoding. Overall, this is an elegant and technically advanced study that demonstrates a new and important role of GAT3 in controlling the processing of visual information.

      We are grateful to the reviewer for their positive appraisal of our work, including our technical advances and our demonstration of how cortical astrocytes play a role in visual information processing by neurons via GAT3-mediated regulation of activity.

      Strengths:

      (1)  Development of a new approach for a local knockout (GAT3).

      (2)  Important and novel insights into visual system function and its dependence on GAT3.

      (3)  Plausible cellular mechanism.

      Weaknesses:

      No major weaknesses were identified by this reviewer.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of our study, including the development of a novel local knockout strategy for GAT3, the discovery of important functional consequences for visual system processing, and the identification of a plausible underlying cellular mechanism.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Park et al. have made a tool for spatiotemporally restricted knockout of the astrocytic GABA transporter GAT3, leveraging CRISPR/Cas9 and viral transduction in adult mice, and evaluated the effects of GAT3 on neural encoding of visual stimulation.

      Strengths:

      This concise manuscript leverages state-of-the-art gene CRISPR/Cas9 technology for knocking out astrocytic genes. This has only to a small degree been performed previously in astrocytes, and it represents an important development in the field. Moreover, the authors utilize in vivo two-photon imaging of neural responses to visual stimuli as a readout of neural activity, in addition to validating their data with ex vivo electrophysiology. Lastly, they use advanced statistical modeling to analyze the impact of GAT3 knockout. Overall, the study comes across as rigorous and convincing.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s endorsement of our experimental rigor and methodological innovation. We agree that combining in vivo and ex vivo measurements with rigorous analytical methods strengthens the overall conclusions of the study and demonstrates the important role of astrocytic GAT3 in cortical visual processing.

      Weaknesses:

      Adding the following experiments would potentially have strengthened the conclusions and helped with interpreting the findings:

      (1) Neural activity is quite profoundly influenced by GAT3 knockout. Corroborating these relatively large changes to neural activity with in vivo electrophysiology of some sort as an additional readout would have strengthened the conclusions.

      We agree that further investigation of neuronal activity at higher temporal resolution would provide valuable complementary data, particularly given the profound effects we observed using a pan-neuronal calcium indicator. Detailed in vivo electrophysiology—such as large-scale Neuropixel recordings—would allow assessment of single-neuron spiking dynamics and potentially cell-type specific responses following GAT3 deletion. While such an investigation is beyond the scope of the current study, we concur that it would be an important follow-up direction to further dissect the effects of GAT3 knockout on neuron activity profiles at both single-cell and population levels.

      (2) Given the quite large effects on neural coding in visual cortex assessed på jRGECO imaging, it would have been interesting if the mouse groups could have been subjected to behavioral testing, assessing the visual system.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to explore potential behavioral consequences of GAT3 deletion. Based on our observed alterations in visual cortical activity, we agree that GAT3 knockout could impact visual discrimination-based behaviors. Astrocytes in the visual cortex are highly tuned to sensory and motor events and are generally known to shape behavioral outputs (Slezak et al., 2019; Kofuji & Araque, 2021). Our study suggests that regulation of inhibitory signaling via GAT3 transporters is a possible mechanism by which astrocytes influence visually guided behaviors. Although behavioral assessments fall beyond the scope of the current work, we agree with the reviewer’s suggestion and will pursue future experiments employing paradigms such as go/no-go visual detection or two-alternative forced choice to determine whether astrocytic GAT3 modulates visually guided behaviors and perceptual decisionmaking.  

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      It could be more clearly stated from the very beginning that a method was developed and used which, by itself, apparently has no cell type selectivity. It is highly plausible that the effects are mostly due to the absence of astrocytic GAT3, as discussed by the authors, but the distinction of what has been done and what is interpretation based on the literature is occasionally a bit blurry. This is also important because there are CRISPR/Cas9-based approaches that are astrocyte-specific (e.g., GEARBOCS).

      We thank the reviewer for this helpful suggestion. As noted, our current approach does not confer celltype specificity on its own. Although our interpretation—supported by expression patterns and prior literature—attributes the observed effects primarily to astrocytic GAT3 loss, we agree that this distinction should be explicitly stated. We have revised the Introduction section (lines 83-87) to clarify that while MRCUTS allows for local gene knockout, it is not inherently cell-type specific unless combined with celltype restricted Cre drivers, as is possible in future applications.

      A change of ambient GABA following GAT3 deletion is central to the proposed cellular mechanism. Demonstrating this directly would strengthen the manuscript (e.g., changed tonic GABAergic current in the absence of GAT3, and insensitivity to SNAP-5114).

      While we recognize that directly quantifying ambient GABA levels would further strengthen our study, substantial evidence supports the role of GABA transporters in coordinately regulating both phasic and tonic inhibition and cellular excitability (Kinney, 2005; Keros & Hablitz, 2005; Semyanov et al. 2003).

      Moreover, tonic GABA currents have been shown to strongly correlate with phasic inhibitory bursts (Glykys & Mody, 2007; Farrant & Nusser, 2005; Ataka & Gu, 2006), suggesting shared underlying regulatory mechanisms. Furthermore, as the reviewer correctly points out, alternative mechanisms such as non-vesicular GABA release or disinhibition via interneuron suppression cannot be excluded (also discussed in Kinney 2005). Given these considerations, we prioritized sIPSC measurements as a more integrative and reliable proxy for altered GABAergic signaling in L2/3 pyramidal neurons. We have revised the Discussion section (lines 329-333) to explain our choice of approach for further clarification.

      We also agree it would be of interest to test whether GAT3 KO neurons exhibit insensitivity to SNAP-5114, both ex vivo and in vivo. However, based on our SNAP-5114 application experiments in vivo, which revealed only subtle effects on single-neuron properties (Figure S2A-F), we anticipate that interpreting a lack of effect in the KO condition would be challenging and potentially inconclusive.  

      References

      Ataka, T. & Gu, J. G. Relationship between tonic inhibitory currents and phasic inhibitory activity in the spinal cord lamina II region of adult mice. Mol. Pain. (2006).  

      Bright, D. & Smart, T. Methods for recording and measuring tonic GABAA receptor-mediated inhibition. Front. Neural Circuits. 7, (2013).

      Farrant, M. & Nusser, Z. Variations on an inhibitory theme: phasic and tonic activation of GABAA receptors. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 215–229 (2005).  

      Glykys, J. & Mody, I. Activation of GABAA Receptors: Views from Outside the Synaptic Cleft. Neuron. 56, 763-770 (2007).

      Keros, S. & Hablitz, J. J. Subtype-Specific GABA Transporter Antagonists Synergistically Modulate Phasic and Tonic GABAA Conductances in Rat Neocortex. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 2073–2085 (2005).

      Kinney, G. A. GAT-3 Transporters Regulate Inhibition in the Neocortex. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 4533–4537 (2005).

      Kofuji, P. & Araque, A. Astrocytes and Behavior. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 44, 49–67 (2021).

      Semyanov, A., Walker, M. & Kullmann, D. GABA uptake regulates cortical excitability via cell type–specific tonic inhibition. Nat. Neurosci. 6, 484–490 (2003).

      Slezak, M., Kandler, S., Van Veldhoven, P. P., Van den Haute, C., Bonin, V. & Holt, M.G. Distinct

      Mechanisms for Visual and Motor-Related Astrocyte Responses in Mouse Visual Cortex. Curr. Biol. 18, 3120-3127 (2019).

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer 1 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Yin and colleagues addresses a long-standing question in the field of cortical morphogenesis, regarding factors that determine differential cortical folding across species and individuals with cortical malformations. The authors present work based on a computational model of cortical folding evaluated alongside a physical model that makes use of gel swelling to investigate the role of a two-layer model for cortical morphogenesis. The study assesses these models against empirically derived cortical surfaces based on MRI data from ferret, macaque monkey, and human brains.

      The manuscript is clearly written and presented, and the experimental work (physical gel modeling as well as numerical simulations) and analyses (subsequent morphometric evaluations) are conducted at the highest methodological standards. It constitutes an exemplary use of interdisciplinary approaches for addressing the question of cortical morphogenesis by bringing together well-tuned computational modeling with physical gel models. In addition, the comparative approaches used in this paper establish a foundation for broad-ranging future lines of work that investigate the impact of perturbations or abnormalities during cortical development.

      The cross-species approach taken in this study is a major strength of the work. However, correspondence across the two methodologies did not appear to be equally consistent in predicting brain folding across all three species. The results presented in Figures 4 (and Figures S3 and S4) show broad correspondence in shape index and major sulci landmarks across all three species. Nevertheless, the results presented for the human brain lack the same degree of clear correspondence for the gel model results as observed in the macaque and ferret. While this study clearly establishes a strong foundation for comparative cortical anatomy across species and the impact of perturbations on individual morphogenesis, further work that fine-tunes physical modeling of complex morphologies, such as that of the human cortex, may help to further understand the factors that determine cortical functionalization and pathologies.

      We thank the reviewer for positive opinions and helpful comments. Yes, the physical gel model of the human brain has a lower similarity index with the real brain. There are several reasons.

      First, the highly convoluted human cortex has a few major folds (primary sulci) and a very large number of minor folds associated with secondary or tertiary sulci (on scales of order comparable to the cortical thickness), relative to the ferret and macaque cerebral cortex. In our gel model, the exact shapes, positions, and orientations of these minor folds are stochastic, which makes it hard to have a very high similarity index of the gel models when compared with the brain of a single individual.

      Second, in real human brains, these minor folds evolve dynamically with age and show differences among individuals. In experiments with the gel brain, multiscale folds form and eventually disappear as the swelling progresses through the thickness. Our physical model results are snapshots during this dynamical process, which makes it hard to have a concrete one-to-one correspondence between the instantaneous shapes of the swelling gel and the growing human brain.

      Third, the growth of the brain cortex is inhomogeneous in space and varying with time, whereas, in the gel model, swelling is relatively homogeneous.

      We agree that further systematic work, based on our proposed methods, with more fine-tuned gel geometries and properties, might provide a deeper understanding of the relations between brain geometry, and growth-induced folds and their functionalization and pathologies. Further analysis of cortical pathologies using computational and physical gel models can be found in our companion paper (Choi et al., 2025), also submitted to eLife:

      G. P. T. Choi, C. Liu, S. Yin, G. Sejourn´ e, R. S. Smith, C. A. Walsh, L. Mahadevan, Biophysical basis for´ brain folding and misfolding patterns in ferrets and humans. Preprint, bioRxiv 2025.03.05.641682.

      Reviewer 2 (Public review):

      This manuscript explores the mechanisms underlying cerebral cortical folding using a combination of physical modelling, computational simulations, and geometric morphometrics. The authors extend their prior work on human brain development (Tallinen et al., 2014; 2016) to a comparative framework involving three mammalian species: ferrets (Carnivora), macaques (Old World monkeys), and humans (Hominoidea). By integrating swelling gel experiments with mathematical differential growth models, they simulate sulcification instability and recapitulate key features of brain folding across species. The authors make commendable use of publicly available datasets to construct 3D models of fetal and neonatal brain surfaces: fetal macaque (ref. [26]), newborn ferret (ref. [11]), and fetal human (ref. [22]).

      Using a combination of physical models and numerical simulations, the authors compare the resulting folding morphologies to real brain surfaces using morphometric analysis. Their results show qualitative and quantitative concordance with observed cortical folding patterns, supporting the view that differential tangential growth of the cortex relative to the subcortical substrate is sufficient to account for much of the diversity in cortical folding. This is a very important point in our field, and can be used in the teaching of medical students.

      Brain folding remains a topic of ongoing debate. While some regard it as a critical specialization linked to higher cognitive function, others consider it an epiphenomenon of expansion and constrained geometry. This divergence was evident in discussions during the Strungmann Forum on cortical development (Silver¨ et al., 2019). Though folding abnormalities are reliable indicators of disrupted neurodevelopmental processes (e.g., neurogenesis, migration), their relationship to functional architecture remains unclear. Recent evidence suggests that the absolute number of neurons varies significantly with position-sulcus versus gyrus-with potential implications for local processing capacity (e.g., https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.25626). The field is thus in need of comparative, mechanistic studies like the present one.

      This paper offers an elegant and timely contribution by combining gel-based morphogenesis, numerical modelling, and morphometric analysis to examine cortical folding across species. The experimental design - constructing two-layer PDMS models from 3D MRI data and immersing them in organic solvents to induce differential swelling - is well-established in prior literature. The authors further complement this with a continuum mechanics model simulating folding as a result of differential growth, as well as a comparative analysis of surface morphologies derived from in vivo, in vitro, and in silico brains.

      We thank the reviewer for the very positive comments.

      I offer a few suggestions here for clarification and further exploration:

      Major Comments

      (1)   Choice of Developmental Stages and Initial Conditions

      The authors should provide a clearer justification for the specific developmental stages chosen (e.g., G85 for macaque, GW23 for human). How sensitive are the resulting folding patterns to the initial surface geometry of the gel models? Given that folding is a nonlinear process, early geometric perturbations may propagate into divergent morphologies. Exploring this sensitivity-either through simulations or reference to prior work-would enhance the robustness of the findings.

      The initial geometry is one of the important factors that decides the final folding pattern. The smooth brain in the early developmental stage shows a broad consistency across individuals, and we expect the main folds to form similarly across species and individuals.

      Generally, we choose the initial geometry when the brain cortex is still relatively smooth. For the human, this corresponds approximately to GW23, as the major folds such as the Rolandic fissure (central sulcus), arise during this developmental stage. For the macaque brain, we chose developmental stage G85, primarily because of the availability of the dataset corresponding to this time, which also corresponds to the least folded.

      We expect that large-scale folding patterns are strongly sensitive to the initial geometry but fine-scale features are not. Since our goal is to explain the large-scale features, we expect sensitivity to the initial shape.

      Enclosed are some results from other researchers that are consistent with this idea. Below are some images of simulations from Wang et al. obtained by perturbing the geometry of a sphere to an ellipsoid. We see that the growth-induced folds mostly maintain their width (wavelength), but change their orientations.

      Reference:

      Wang, X., Lefevre, J., Bohi, A., Harrach, M.A., Dinomais, M. and Rousseau, F., 2021. The influence of` biophysical parameters in a biomechanical model of cortical folding patterns. Scientific Reports, 11(1), p.7686.

      Related results from the same group show that slight perturbations of brain geometry, cause these folds also tend to change their orientations but not width/wavelength (Bohi et al., 2019).

      Reference:

      Bohi, A., Wang, X., Harrach, M., Dinomais, M., Rousseau, F. and Lefevre, J., 2019, July. Global per-` turbation of initial geometry in a biomechanical model of cortical morphogenesis. In 2019 41st Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC) (pp. 442-445). IEEE.

      Finally, a systematic discussion of the role of perturbations on the initial geometries and physical properties can be seen in our work on understanding a different system, gut morphogenesis (Gill et al., 2024).

      We have added the discussion about geometric sensitivity in the section Methods-Numerical Simulations:

      “Small perturbations on initial geometry would affect minor folds, but the main features of major folds, such as orientations, width, and depth, are expected to be conserved across individuals [49, 50]. For simplicity, we do not perturb the fetal brain geometry obtained from datasets.”

      (2) Parameter Space and Breakdown Points

      The numerical model assumes homogeneous growth profiles and simplifies several aspects of cortical mechanics. Parameters such as cortical thickness, modulus ratios, and growth ratios are described in Table II. It would be informative to discuss the range of parameter values for which the model remains valid, and under what conditions the physical and computational models diverge. This would help delineate the boundaries of the current modelling framework and indicate directions for refinement.

      Exploring the valid parameter space is a key problem. We have tested a series of growth parameters and will state them explicitly in our revision. In the current version, we chose the ones that yield a relatively high similarity index to the animal brains. More generally, folding patterns are largely regulated by geometry as well as physical parameters, such as cortical thickness, modulus ratios, growth ratios, and inhomogeneity. In our previous work on a different system, gut morphogenesis, where similar folding patterns are seen, we have explored these features (Gill et al., 2024).

      Reference:

      Gill, H.K., Yin, S., Nerurkar, N.L., Lawlor, J.C., Lee, C., Huycke, T.R., Mahadevan, L. and Tabin, C.J., 2024. Hox gene activity directs physical forces to differentially shape chick small and large intestinal epithelia. Developmental Cell, 59(21), pp.2834-2849.

      (3) Neglected Regional Features: The Occipital Pole of the Macaque

      One conspicuous omission is the lack of attention to the occipital pole of the macaque, which is known to remain smooth even at later gestational stages and has an unusually high neuronal density (2.5× higher than adjacent cortex). This feature is not reproduced in the gel or numerical models, nor is it discussed. Acknowledging this discrepancy-and speculating on possible developmental or mechanical explanationswould add depth to the comparative analysis. The authors may wish to include this as a limitation or a target for future work.

      Yes, we have added that the omission of the Occipital Pole of the macaque is one of our paper’s limitations. Our main aim in this paper is to explore the formation of large-scale folds, so the smooth region is neglected. But future work could include this to make the model more complete.

      The main text has been modified in Methods, 3D model reconstruction, pre-processing:

      “To focus on fold formation, we neglected some smooth regions such as the Occipital Pole of the macaque.”

      (4) Spatio-Temporal Growth Rates and Available Human Data

      The authors note that accurate, species-specific spatio-temporal growth data are lacking, limiting the ability to model inhomogeneous cortical expansion. While this may be true for ferret and macaque, there are high-quality datasets available for human fetal development, now extended through ultrasound imaging (e.g., https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-023-06630-3). Incorporating or at least referencing such data could improve the fidelity of the human model and expand the applicability of the approach to clinical or pathological scenarios.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the very useful datasets that exist for the exploration of inhomogeneous growth driven folding patterns. We have referred to this paper to provide suggestions for further work in exploring the role of growth inhomogeneities.

      We have referred to this high-quality dataset in our main text, Discussion:

      “...the effect of inhomogeneous growth needs to be further investigated by incorporating regional growth of the gray and white matter not only in human brains [29, 31] based on public datasets [45], but also in other species.”

      A few works have tried to incorporate inhomogeneous growth in simulating human brain folding by separating the central sulcus area into several lobes (e.g., lobe parcellation method, Wang, PhD Thesis, 2021). Since our goal in this paper is to explain the large-scale features of folding in a minimal setting, we have kept our model simple and show that it is still capable of capturing the main features of folding in a range of mammalian brains.

      Reference:

      Xiaoyu Wang. Modelisation et caract´ erisation du plissement cortical. Signal and Image Processing. Ecole´ nationale superieure Mines-T´ el´ ecom Atlantique, 2021. English.´ 〈NNT : 2021IMTA0248〉.

      (5) Future Applications: The Inverse Problem and Fossil Brains

      The authors suggest that their morphometric framework could be extended to solve the inverse growth problem-reconstructing fetal geometries from adult brains. This speculative but intriguing direction has implications for evolutionary neuroscience, particularly the interpretation of fossil endocasts. Although beyond the scope of this paper, I encourage the authors to elaborate briefly on how such a framework might be practically implemented and validated.

      For the inverse problem, we could use the following strategies:

      a. Perform systematic simulations using different geometries and physical parameters to obtain the variation in morphologies as a function of parameters.

      b. Using either supervised training or unsupervised training (physics-informed neural networks, PINNs) to learn these characteristic morphologies and classify their dependence on the parameters using neural networks. These can then be trained to determine the possible range of geometrical and physical parameters that yield buckled patterns seen in the systematic simulations.

      c. Reconstruct the 3D surface from fossil endocasts. Using the well-trained neural network, it should be possible to predict the initial shape of the smooth brain cortex, growth profile, and stiffness ratio of the gray and white matter.

      As an example in this direction, supervised neural networks have been used recently to solve the forward problem to predict the buckling pattern of a growing two-layer system (Chavoshnejad et al., 2023). The inverse problem can then be solved using machine-learning methods when the training datasets are the folded shape, which are then used to predict the initial geometry and physical properties.

      Reference:

      Chavoshnejad, P., Chen, L., Yu, X., Hou, J., Filla, N., Zhu, D., Liu, T., Li, G., Razavi, M.J. and Wang, X., 2023. An integrated finite element method and machine learning algorithm for brain morphology prediction. Cerebral Cortex, 33(15), pp.9354-9366.

      Conclusion

      This is a well-executed and creative study that integrates diverse methodologies to address a longstanding question in developmental neurobiology. While a few aspects-such as regional folding peculiarities, sensitivity to initial conditions, and available human data-could be further elaborated, they do not detract from the overall quality and novelty of the work. I enthusiastically support this paper and believe that it will be of broad interest to the neuroscience, biomechanics, and developmental biology communities.

      Note: The paper mentions a companion paper [reference 11] that explores the cellular and anatomical changes in the ferret cortex. I did not have access to this manuscript, but judging from the title, this paper might further strengthen the conclusions.

      The companion paper (Choi et al., 2025) has also been submitted to Elife and can be found on bioXiv here:

      G. P. T. Choi, C. Liu, S. Yin, G. Sejourn´ e, R. S. Smith, C. A. Walsh, L. Mahadevan, Biophysical basis for´ brain folding and misfolding patterns in ferrets and humans. bioRxiv 2025.03.05.641682.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer 1:

      (1) Line 65 "(Figure 1A). Inactivation causes a change in the leg's rest position; however, in preliminary experiments, the body rotation did not have a large effect on the rest positions of the leg following inactivation. This result is consistent with the one already reported for stick insects and shows that passive forces within the leg are much larger than the gravitational force on a leg and dominate limb position [1]." This is the direct replication of the previous work by Hooper et al 2009 and therefore authors should ideally show the data for this condition (no weight attached).

      We did not present this data – the effect of inactivation on the leg’s rest position in unweighted leg - because it was already reported in the case of stick insects. However, we understand the reviewer’s point that it is important to present the data showing this replication. We will do the same in the revised version.

      (2) The authors use vglut-gal4, a very broad driver for inactivating motor neurons. The driver labels all glutamatergic neurons, including brain descending neurons and nerve cord interneurons, in addition to motor neurons. Additionally, the strength of inactivation might differ in different neurons (including motor neurons) depending on the expression levels of the opsins. As a result, in this condition, the authors might not be removing all active forces. This is a major caveat that authors do not address. They explore that they are not potentially silencing all inputs to muscles by using an additional octopaminergic driver, but this doesn't address the points mentioned above. At the very least, the authors should try using other motor neuron drivers, as well as other neuronal silencers. This driver is so broad that authors couldn't even use it for physiology experiments. Additionally, the authors could silence VGlut-labeled motor neurons and record muscle activity (potentially using GCaMP as has been done in several recent papers cited by the authors, Azevedo et al, 2020) as a much more direct readout.

      This reviewer critique is related to the use of vglut-gal4 –a broad driver– to inactivate motor neurons (MNs). The reviewer argues that the use of a broad driver might result in some effects that are not due to MN inactivation. Conversely, it is possible that not all MNs are inactivated. These critiques raise important points that we will address in the revision by 1) performing experiments with other MN drivers as suggested by the reviewer, 2) performing experiments in flies that are inactivated by freezing. These measurements will provide other estimates of passive forces allowing us to better triangulate the range of values for the passive forces. Moreover, it appears that one of the reviewer’s main concern is that the passive forces are overestimated because of the residual active forces. We will discuss this possibility in detail. It is important to note that in the end what we hope to accomplish is to provide a useful estimate of the passive forces. It is unlikely that the passive force will be a precise number like a physical constant as the passive forces likely depend on recent history.

      (3) Figure 4 uses an extremely simplified OpenSim model that makes several assumptions that are known to be false. For example, the Thorax-Coxa joint is assumed to be a ball and socket joint, which it is not. Tibia-tarsus joint is completely ignored and likely makes a major contribution in supporting overall posture, given the importance of the leg "claw" for adhering to substrates. Moreover, there are a couple of recent open-source neuromechanical models that include all these details (NeuromechFly by Lobato-Rios et al, 2022, Nat. Methods, and the fly body model by Vaxenburg et al, 2025, Nature). Leveraging these models to rule in or rule out contributions at other joints that are ignored in the authors' OpenSim model would be very helpful to make their case.

      Our OpenSim model predates the newer mechanical model. In the revised manuscript, we will revisit the model in light of recent developments.

      (4) Figure 5 shows the experimental validation of Figure 4 simulations; however, it suffers from several caveats.

      a) The authors track a single point on the head of the fly to estimate the height of the fly. This has several issues. Firstly, it is not clear how accurate the tracking would be. Secondly, it is not clear how the fly actually "falls" on VGlut silencing; do all flies fall in a similar manner in every trial? Almost certainly, there will be some "pitch" and "role" in the way the fly falls. These will affect the location of this single-tracked point that doesn't reflect the authors' expectations. Unless the authors track multiple points on the fly and show examples of tracked videos, it is hard to believe this dataset and, hence, any of the resulting interpretations.

      b) As described in the previous point, the "reason" the fly falls on silencing all glutamatergic neurons could be due to silencing all sorts of premotor/interneurons in addition to the silencing of motor neurons.

      c) (line 175) "The first finding is that there was a large variation in the initial height of the fly (Figure 5C), consistent with a recent study of flies walking on a treadmill[20]." The cited paper refers to how height varies during "walking". However, in the current study, the authors are only looking at "standing" (i.e. non-walking) flies. So it is not the correct reference. In my opinion, this could simply reflect poor estimation of the fly's height based on poor tracking or other factors like pitch and role.

      d) "The rate at which the fly fell to the ground was much smaller in the experimental flies than it was in the simulated flies (Figure 5E). The median rate of falling was 1.3 mm/s compared to 37 mm/s for the simulated flies (Figure 5F). (Line 190) The most likely reason for the longer than expected time for the fly to fall is delays associated with motor neuron inactivation and muscle inactivation." I don't believe this reasoning. There are so many caveats (which I described in the above points) in the model and the experiment, that any of those could be responsible for this massive difference between experiment and modeling. Simply not getting rid of all active forces (inadequate silencing) could be one obvious reason. Other reasons could be that the model is using underestimates of passive forces, as alluded to in point 3.

      (4a) Although we agree that measuring different points on the body would allow us to estimate the moments, we disagree that the height of the fly cannot be evaluated from the measurement of a single point. The measurements have been performed using the same techniques that we used to assess the fly’s height in a different study where we estimated the resolution of our imaging system to be ~20 mm(Chun et. al. 2021). We will include these details in the revised manuscript. The video showing the falling experiments are not available or referenced in the manuscript. These will be made available.

      b) We will repeat the “falling” experiment with a more restrictive driver.

      c) We disagree with the reviewer on this point. The system has a resolution of ~20 mm and is sufficient to make conclusion about the difference in the height of the fly. We will clarify this point in the revised manuscript.

      d) We do not follow the reviewer’s rationale here. The passive forces in the model (along with any residual forces) are the same in the model as well as in the experiment. Moreover, there will be a delay between light onset, neuronal inactivation and muscle inactivation. These processes are not instantaneous. In Figure 6, we estimate these delays and have concluded that they will cause substantial delay. In the revised manuscript, we will discuss other reasons for the delay suggested by the reviewer.

      (5) Final figure (Figure 6) focuses on understanding the time course of neuronal silencing. First of all, I'm not entirely sure how relevant this is for the story. It could be an interesting supplemental data. But it seems a bit tangential. Additionally, it also suffers from major caveats.

      a) The authors now use a new genetic driver for which they don't have any behavioral data in any previous figures. So we do not know if any of this data holds true for the previous experiments. The authors perform whole-cell recordings from random unidentified motor neurons labeled by E49-Gal4>GtACR1 to deduce a time constant for behavioral results obtained in the VGlut-Gal4>GtACR1 experiments.

      b) The DMD setup is useful for focal inactivation, however, the appropriate controls and data are not presented. Line 200 "A spot of light on the cell body produces as much of the hyperpolarization as stimulating the entire fly (mean of 11.3 mV vs 13.1 mV across 9 neurons). Conversely, excluding the cell body produces only a small effect on the MN (mean of 2.6 mV)." First of all, the control experiment for showing that DMD is indeed causing focal inactivation would be to gradually move the spot of light away from the labeled soma, i.e. to the neighboring "labelled" soma and show that there is indeed focal inactivation. Instead authors move it quite a long distance into unlabeled neuropil. Secondly, I still don't get why the authors are doing this experiment. Even if we believe the DMD is functioning perfectly, all this really tells us is that a random subset motor neurons (maybe 5 or 6 cells, legend is missing this info) labeled by E49-Gal4 is strongly hyperpolarized by its own GtACR1 channel opening, rather than being impacted because of hyperpolarizations in other E49-Gal4 labeled neurons. This has no relevance to the interpretation of any of the VGlut-Gal4 behavioral data. VGLut-Gal4 is much broader and also labels all glutamatergic neurons, most of which are inhibitory interneurons whose silencing could lead to disinhibition of downstream networks.

      (5 a) However, we can address the reviewer critique by recording from the Vglut line while using a MN line to target the recordings to MNs.

      b) Once we use the Vglut driver to perform these recordings, it will help assess how much of the MN inactivation is due to the GtACR expressed in the MN versus other neurons.

      Reviewer 2:

      While (as mentioned above) the study's conclusions are well-supported by the results and modeling, limitations arise because of the assumptions made. For instance, using a linear approximation may not hold at larger joint angles, and future studies would benefit from accounting for nonlinearities. Future studies could also delve into the source of passive forces, which is important for more deeply understanding the anatomical and physical basis of the results in this study. For instance, assessments of muscle or joint properties to correlate stiffness values with physical structure might be an area of future consideration.

      We agree with these comments but believe that these studies represent avenues for future work.

      Reviewer 3:

      (1) Passive torques are measured, but only some short speculative statements, largely based on previous work, are offered on their functional significance; some of these claims are not well supported by experimental evidence or theoretical arguments. Passive forces are judged as "large" compared to the weight force of the limb, but the arguably more relevant force is the force limb muscles can generate, which, even in equilibrium conditions, is already about two orders of magnitude larger. The conclusion that passive forces are dynamically irrelevant seems natural, but contrasts with the assertion that "passive forces [...] will have a strong influence on limb kinematics". As a result, the functional significance of passive joint torques in the fruit fly, if any, remains unclear, and this ambiguity represents a missed opportunity. We now know the magnitude of passive joint torques - do they matter and for what? Are they helpful, for example, to maintain robust neuronal control, or a mechanical constraint that negatively impacts performance, e.g., because they present a sink for muscle work?

      To us, measuring passive forces was the first step to understanding neural/biomechanical control of limb. In general, we agree with these comments and would like to understand the role of passive forces in overall control of limb. A complete discussion of the role of the significance of passive forces in the control of limb is beyond the scope of this study. We would like to note that it is unlikely that the active forces are two orders of magnitude larger during unloaded movement of the limb. However, these issues will have to be settled in future work.

      (2) The work is framed with a scaling argument, but the assumptions that underpin the associated claims are not explicit and can thus not be evaluated. This is problematic because at least some arguments appear to contradict textbook scaling theory or everyday experience. For example, active forces are assumed to scale with limb volume, when every textbook would have them scale with area instead; and the asserted scaling of passive forces involves some hidden assumptions that demand more explicit discussion to alert the reader to associated limitations. Passive forces are said to be important only in small animals, but a quick self-experiment confirms that they are sufficient to stabilize human fingers or ankles against gravity, systems orders of magnitude larger than an insect limb, in seeming contradiction with the alleged dominance of scale. Throughout the manuscript, there are such and similar inaccuracies or ambiguities in the mechanical framing and interpretation, making it hard to fairly evaluate some claims, and rendering others likely incorrect.

      We interpret this comment as making two separate points. The first one is that the reviewer says that our statement that active forces depend on the third power of the limb or L<sup>3</sup> is incorrect. We agree and apologize for this oversight. Specifically, on L6-7 we say, “both inertial forces and active forces scale with the mass if the limb which in turn scales with the volume of the limb and therefore depends on the third power of limb length (L<sup>3</sup>)”. Instead, this statement should read “inertial forces scale with the mass if the limb which in turn scales with the volume of the limb and therefore depends on the third power of limb length (L<sup>3</sup>)”. However, this oversight does not affect the scaling argument as the scaling arguments in the rest of the manuscript only involves inertial forces and not active forces.

      The second point is about the scaling law that governs passive forces. In the current manuscript, we have assumed that the passive forces scale as L<sup>2</sup> based on previous work. The reviewer has pointed out that this assumption might be incorrect or at the very least needs a rationale. We agree with this assessment: passive forces that arise in the muscle are likely to scale as L<sup>2</sup> but passive forces that arise in the joint might not. In the revised manuscript, we will discuss this concern.

      Response to the public comment:

      There was a comment from a reader: “None of our work cited in various places in this preprint (i.e., Zakotnik et al. 2006, Guschlbauer et al. 2007, Page et al. 2008, Hooper et al. 2009, Hooper 2012, Ache and Matheson 2012, Blümel et al. 2012, Ache and Matheson 2013, von Twickel et al. 2019, and Guschlbauer et al. 2022) claims or implies that passive forces could be sufficient to support the weight of an insect or any animal. To claim or suggest otherwise (as done in lines 33-35) is incorrect and sets up a misleading straw man that misrepresents our work. All statements in the preprint regarding our work related to this specific matter need to be removed or edited accordingly. For instance, the investigations, calculations, and interpretations in Hooper et al. 2009 are solely about limbs that are not being used in stance or other loaded tasks (indeed, the article's title specifically refers to "unloaded" leg posture and movements). Trying to use this work to predict whether passive muscle forces alone can support a stick insect against gravity requires considering much more than the oversimplified calculation given in lines 290-292. Other “back of the envelope calculations” (lines 299-300) are likely also insufficient and erroneous. The discussion in lines 289-304 needs to be edited accordingly”

      We thank the reader for their comment. However, we interpret these studies differently. The studies above rightly focused on unloaded legs because it would be difficult to study passive forces in an intact insect without genetic tools. The commenter correctly points out that these studies do not comment on whether passive forces are strong enough to support the weight of the fly. However, we disagree that our arguments based on their results are unreasonable or strawman. We think that our interpretation of their measurements is correct. Moreover, we were motivated by Yox et. el. 1982 who states in so many words: “Stiffness of the muscles in the joints of all the legs might be sufficient to support a resting arthropod. A more rigorous analysis of all supporting limbs and joint angles would be required to prove this hypothesis”. We were inspired by this comment. In the revised manuscript, we will make it clear that the statement made in Line 33 is based on Yox. et. al. and our interpretation of measurements made by others.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer 1 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Choi and colleagues investigates the impact of variation in cortical geometry and growth on cortical surface morphology. Specifically, the study uses physical gel models and computational models to evaluate the impact of varying specific features/parameters of the cortical surface. The study makes use of this approach to address the topic of malformations of cortical development and finds that cortical thickness and cortical expansion rate are the drivers of differences in morphogenesis.

      The study is composed of two main sections. First, the authors validate numerical simulation and gel model approaches against real cortical postnatal development in the ferret. Next, the study turns to modelling malformations in cortical development using modified tangential growth rate and cortical thickness parameters in numerical simulations. The findings investigate three genetically linked cortical malformations observed in the human brain to demonstrate the impact of the two physical parameters on folding in the ferret brain.

      This is a tightly presented study that demonstrates a key insight into cortical morphogenesis and the impact of deviations from normal development. The dual physical and computational modeling approach offers the potential for unique insights into mechanisms driving malformations. This study establishes a strong foundation for further work directly probing the development of cortical folding in the ferret brain. One weakness of the current study is that the interpretation of the results in the context of human cortical development is at present indirect, as the modelling results are solely derived from the ferret. However, these modelling approaches demonstrate proof of concept for investigating related alterations more directly in future work through similar approaches to models of the human cerebral cortex.

      We thank the reviewer for the very positive comments. While the current gel and organismal experiments focus on the ferret only, we want to emphasize that our analysis does consider previous observations of human brains and morphologies therein (Tallinen et al., Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2014; Tallinen et al., Nat. Phys. 2016), which we compare and explain. This allows us to analyze the implications of our study broadly to understand the explanations of cortical malformations in humans using the ferret to motivate our study. Further analysis of normal human brain growth using computational and physical gel models can be found in our companion paper (Yin et al., 2025), also submitted to eLife:

      S. Yin, C. Liu, G. P. T. Choi, Y. Jung, K. Heuer, R. Toro, L. Mahadevan, Morphogenesis and morphometry of brain folding patterns across species. bioRxiv 2025.03.05.641692.

      In future work, we plan to obtain malformed human cortical surface data, which would allow us to further investigate related alterations more directly.

      Reviewer 2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Based on MRI data of the ferret (a gyrencephalic non-primate animal, in whom folding happens postnatally), the authors create in vitro physical gel models and in silico numerical simulations of typical cortical gyrification. They then use genetic manipulations of animal models to demonstrate that cortical thickness and expansion rate are primary drivers of atypical morphogenesis. These observations are then used to explain cortical malformations in humans.

      Strengths:

      The paper is very interesting and original, and combines physical gel experiments, numerical simulations, as well as observations in MCD. The figures are informative, and the results appear to have good overall face validity.

      We thank the reviewer for the very positive comments.

      Weaknesses:

      On the other hand, I perceived some lack of quantitative analyses in the different experiments, and currently, there seems to be rather a visual/qualitative interpretation of the different processes and their similarities/differences. Ideally, the authors also quantify local/pointwise surface expansion in the physical and simulation experiments, to more directly compare these processes. Time courses of eg, cortical curvature changes, could also be plotted and compared for those experiments. I had a similar impression about the comparisons between simulation results and human MRI data. Again, face validity appears high, but the comparison appeared mainly qualitative.

      We thank the reviewer for the comments. Besides the visual and qualitative comparisons between the models, we would like to point out that we have included the quantification of the shape difference between the real and simulated ferret brain models via spherical parameterization and the curvature-based shape index as detailed in main text Fig. 4 and SI Section 3. We have also utilized spherical harmonics representations for the comparison between the real and simulated ferret brains at different maximum order N. In our revision, we plan to further include the curvature-based shape index calculations for the comparison between the real and simulated ferret brains at more time points.

      As for the comparison between the malformation simulation results and human MRI data in the current work, since the human MRI data are two-dimensional while our computational models are threedimensional, we focus on the qualitative comparison between them. In future work, we plan to obtain malformed human cortical surface data, from which we can then perform the parameterization-based and curvature-based shape analysis for a more quantitative assessment.

      I felt that MCDs could have been better contextualized in the introduction.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment and will include a more detailed introduction to MCDs in our revision.

    1. Author response:

      We greatly appreciate the efforts of the reviewers, which have provided insightful and helpful comments to improve the manuscript. The feedback touches upon a number of topics, focusing on clarification or justification of experimental techniques and on understanding the mechanism by which P. aeruginosa detects HOCl. All reviewers raised the issue of how HOCl activates fro expression, including whether free or protein-bound methionine, cysteine, or other HOCl byproducts induce this expression. For the upcoming revision, we plan to perform experiments that address this issue and will discuss potential mechanistic models in light of the new data. In addition, we plan to perform additional experiments to address a reviewer’s concerns regarding the dependence of the fro response on HOCl production by neutrophils. The revision will correct imprecise statements pointed out by reviewers, and address all remaining issues requiring clarification or further discussion, including the range of HOCl sensitivity, relationship between HOCl and flow sensitivity, and justification for testing the fro response to nitric acid.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      […] Strengths:

      This manuscript has many strengths.

      (1) Utilizing and characterizing novel mouse strains that complement the current widely used mouse models in the field of TB. Many of those mouse strains will be novel tools for studying host responses to Mtb infection.

      (2) The study revealed very unique biology of neutrophils during Mtb infection. It has been well-established that high numbers of neutrophils correlate with high bacterial burden in mice. However, this work uncovered that some mouse strains could be resistant to infection even with high numbers of neutrophils in the lung, indicating the diverse functions of neutrophils. This information is important.

      We are grateful for the reviewer’s thoughtful consideration of our work and appreciate their comment that our mouse strains can benefit the models available in the TB field. We further appreciate the recognition of the importance of neutrophil diversity during Mtb infection.

      Weaknesses:

      The weaknesses of the manuscript are that the work is relatively descriptive. It is unclear whether the neutrophil subsets are indeed functionally different. While single-cell RNA seq did provide some clues at transcription levels, functional and mechanistic investigations are lacking.

      We appreciate this comment and agree that further research needs to be done on the functionality of the neutrophils to discover mechanistic differences between the mouse genotypes. Out attempts at extracting sufficient RNA from sorted neutrophils from the mouse lungs were unsuccessful. However, future attempts at comparing RNA expression between mouse genotypes as well as proteomic data are necessary to determine the mechanistic differences in neutrophil biology in these mice.

      Similarly, it is unclear how highly activated and glycolytic neutrophils in MANC strain contribute to its susceptibility.

      This is a fair comment and we agree that it is still unclear how these neutrophils contribute to MANC susceptibility. Growing the neutrophils ex vivo and infecting them with Mtb is technically challenging, due to the slow growth of Mtb and the short lifespan of the neutrophils. As mentioned in the comment above, future in vivo characterization and RNA expression studies will be necessary to address these questions.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      […] Strengths:

      The strengths are addressing a critically important consideration in the tuberculosis field - mouse model(s) of the human disease, and taking advantage of the novel phenotypes observed to determine potential mechanisms. Notable strengths include,

      (1) Innovative generation and use of mouse models: Developing wild-derived inbred mice from diverse geographic locations is innovative, and this approach expands the range of phenotypic responses observed during M. tuberculosis infection. Additionally, the authors have deposited strains at The Jackson Laboratory making these valuable resources available to the scientific community.

      (2) Potential for translational research: The findings have implications for human pulmonary TB, particularly the discovery of neutrophil-associated susceptibility in primary infection and/or neutrophil-mediated disease progression that could both inform the development of therapeutic targets and also be used to test the effectiveness of such therapies.

      (3) Comprehensive experimental design: The investigators use many complementary approaches including in vivo M. tuberculosis infection, in vitro macrophage studies, neutrophil depletion experiments, flow cytometry, and a number of data mining, machine learning, and imaging to produce robust and comprehensive analyses of the wild-derives d strains and neutrophil subpopulations in 3 weeks after M. tuberculosis infection.

      We thank the reviewer for their thorough and thoughtful assessment of our study. We appreciate the recognition that this mouse model can become a resource and can benefit the study of different immune responses to Mtb infection as well as be informative for studying human TB. We further appreciate their comment that the complementary approaches we have used to characterized the mouse phenotypes strengthens this study.

      Weaknesses:

      The manuscript and studies have considerable strengths and very few weaknesses. One minor consideration is that phenotyping is limited to a single limited-time point; however, this time point was carefully selected and has a strong biological rationale provided by investigators. This potential weakness does not diminish the overall findings, exciting results, or conclusions.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out that a single time point has been studied, and that this time point is biologically relevant. We agree that additional time points, including later time points that address systemic dissemination, should be included in future studies.

    1. Author response:

      Point-by-point description of the revisions

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility, and clarity):

      The work by Pinon et al describes the generation of a microvascular model to study Neisseria meningitidis interactions with blood vessels. The model uses a novel and relatively high throughput fabrication method that allows full control over the geometry of the vessels. The model is well characterized. The authors then study different aspects of Neisseriaendothelial interactions and benchmark the bacterial infection model against the best disease model available, a human skin xenograft mouse model, which is one of the great strengths of the paper. The authors show that Neisseria binds to the 3D model in a similar geometry that in the animal xenograft model, induces an increase in permeability short after bacterial perfusion, and induces endothelial cytoskeleton rearrangements. Finally, the authors show neutrophil recruitment to bacterial microcolonies and phagocytosis of Neisseria. The article is overall well written, and it is a great advancement in the bioengineering and sepsis infection field, and I only have a few major comments and some minor.

      Major comments:

      Infection-on-chip. I would recommend the authors to change the terminology of "infection on chip" to better reflect their work. The term is vague and it decreases novelty, as there are multiple infection on chips models that recapitulate other infections (recently reviewed in https://doi.org/10.1038/s41564-024-01645-6) including Ebola, SARS-CoV-2, Plasmodium and Candida. Maybe the term "sepsis on chip" would be more specific and exemplify better the work and novelty. Also, I would suggest that the authors carefully take a look at the text and consider when they use VoC or to current term IoC, as of now sometimes they are used interchangeably, with VoC being used occasionally in bacteria perfused experiments.

      We thank Reviewer #1 for this suggestion. Indeed, we have chosen to replace the term "Infection-on-Chip" by "infected Vessel-on-chip" to avoid any confusion in the title and the text. Also, we have removed all the terms "IoC" which referred to "Infection-on-Chip" and replaced with "VoC" for "Vessel-on-Chip". We think these terms will improve the clarity of the main text.

      Author response image 1.

      F-actin (red) and ezrin (yellow) staining after 3h of infection with N. meningitidis (green) in 2D (top) and 3D (bottom) vessel-on-chip models.

      Fig 3 and Supplementary 3: Permeability. The authors suggest that early 3h infection with Neisseria do not show increase in vascular permeability in the animal model, contrary to their findings in the 3D in vitro model. However, they show a non-significant increase in permeability of 70 KDa Dextran in the animal xenograft early infection. This seems to point that if the experiment would have been done with a lower molecular weight tracer, significant increases in permeability could have been detected. I would suggest to do this experiment that could capture early events in vascular disruption.

      Comparing permeability under healthy and infected conditions using Dextran smaller than 70 kDa is challenging. Previous research (1) has shown that molecules below 70 kDa already diffuse freely in healthy tissue. Given this high baseline diffusion, we believe that no significant difference would be observed before and after N. meningitidis infection and these experiments were not carried out. As discussed in the manuscript, bacteria induced permeability in mouse occurs at later time points, 16h post infection as shown previoulsy (2). As discussed in the manuscript, this difference between the xenograft model and the chip likely reflect the absence in the chip of various cell types present in the tissue parenchyma.

      The authors show the formation of actin of a honeycomb structure beneath the bacterial microcolonies. This only occurred in 65% of the microcolonies. Is this result similar to in vitro 2D endothelial cultures in static and under flow? Also, the group has shown in the past positive staining of other cytoskeletal proteins, such as ezrin in the ERM complex. Does this also occur in the 3D system?

      We thank the Reviewer #1 for this suggestion.

      • According to this recommendation, we imaged monolayers of endothelial cells in the flat regions of the chip (the two lateral channels) using the same microscopy conditions (i.e., Obj. 40X N.A. 1.05) that have been used to detect honeycomb structures in the 3D vessels in vitro. We showed that more than 56% of infected cells present these honeycomb structures in 2D, which is 13% less than in 3D, and is not significant due to the distributions of both populations. Thus, we conclude that under both in vitro conditions, 2D and 3D, the amount of infected cells exhibiting cortical plaques is similar. We have added the graph and the confocal images in Figure S4B and lines 418-419 of the revised manuscript.

      • We recently performed staining of ezrin in the chip and imaged both the 3D and 2D regions. Although ezrin staining was visible in 3D (Fig. 1 of this response), it was not as obvious as other markers under these infected conditions and we did not include it in the main text. Interpretation of this result is not straight forward as for instance the substrate of the cells is different and it would require further studies on the behaviour of ERM proteins in these different contexts.

      One of the most novel things of the manuscript is the use of a relatively quick photoablation system. I would suggest that the authors add a more extensive description of the protocol in methods. Could this technique be applied in other laboratories? If this is a major limitation, it should be listed in the discussion.

      Following the Reviewer’s comment, we introduced more detailed explanations regarding the photoablation:

      • L157-163 (Results): "Briefly, the chosen design is digitalized into a list of positions to ablate. A pulsed UV-LASER beam is injected into the microscope and shaped to cover the back aperture of the objective. The laser is then focused on each position that needs ablation. After introducing endothelial cells (HUVEC) in the carved regions,…"

      • L512-516 (Discussion): "The speed capabilities drastically improve with the pulsing repetition rate. Given that our laser source emits pulses at 10kHz, as compared to other photoablation lasers with repetitions around 100 Hz, our solution could potentially gain a factor of 100."

      • L1082-1087 (Materials and Methods): "…, and imported in a python code. The control of the various elements is embedded and checked for this specific set of hardware. The code is available upon request." Adding these three paragraphs gives more details on how photoablation works thus improving the manuscript.

      Minor comments:

      Supplementary Fig 2. The reference to subpanels H and I is swapped.

      The references to subpanels H and I have been correctly swapped back in the reviewed version.

      Line 203: I would suggest to delete this sentence. Although a strength of the submitted paper is the direct comparison of the VoC model with the animal model to better replicate Neisseria infection, a direct comparison with animal permeability is not needed in all vascular engineering papers, as vascular permeability measurements in animals have been well established in the past.

      The sentence "While previously developed VoC platforms aimed at replicating physiological permeability properties, they often lack direct comparisons with in vivo values." has been removed from the revised text.

      Fig 3: Bacteria binding experiments. I would suggest the addition of more methodological information in the main results text to guarantee a good interpretation of the experiment. First, it would be better that wall shear stress rather than flow rate is described in the main text, as flow rate is dependent on the geometry of the vessel being used. Second, how long was the perfusion of Neisseria in the binding experiment performed to quantify colony doubling or elongation? As per figure 1C, I would guess than 100 min, but it would be better if this information is directly given to the readers.

      We thank Reviewer #1 for these two suggestions that will improve the text clarity (e.g., L316). (i) Indeed, we have changed the flow rate in terms of shear stress. (ii) Also, we have normalized the quantification of the colony doubling time according to the first time-point where a single bacteria is attached to the vessel wall. Thus, early adhesion bacteria will be defined by a longer curve while late adhesion bacteria by a shorter curve. In total, the experiment lasted for 3 hours (modifications appear in L318 and L321-326).

      Fig 4: The honeycomb structure is not visible in the 3D rendering of panel D. I would recommend to show the actin staining in the absence of Neisseria staining as well.

      According to this suggestion, a zoom of the 3D rendering of the cortical plaque without colony had been added to the figure 4 of the revised manuscript.

      Line 421: E-selectin is referred as CD62E in this sentence. I would suggest to use the same terminology everywhere.

      We have replaced the "CD62E" term with "E-selectin" to improve clarity.

      Line 508: "This difference is most likely associated with the presence of other cell types in the in vivo tissues and the onset of intravascular coagulation". Do the authors refer to the presence of perivascular cells, pericytes or fibroblasts? If so, it could be good to mention them, as well as those future iterations of the model could include the presence of these cell types.

      By "other cell types", we refer to pericytes (3), fibroblasts (4), and perivascular macrophages (5), which surround endothelial cells and contribute to vessel stability. The main text was modified to include this information (Lines 548 and 555-570) and their potential roles during infection disussed.

      Discussion: The discussion covers very well the advantages of the model over in vitro 2D endothelial models and the animal xenograft but fails to include limitations. This would include the choice of HUVEC cells, an umbilical vein cell line to study microcirculation, the lack of perivascular cells or limitations on the fabrication technique regarding application in other labs (if any).

      We thank Reviewer #1 for this suggestion. Indeed, our manuscript may lack explaining limitations, and adding them to the text will help improve it:

      • The perspectives of our model include introducing perivascular cells surrounding the vessel and fibroblasts into the collagen gel as discussed previously and added in the discussion part (L555-570).

      • Our choice for HUVEC cells focused on recapitulating the characteristics of venules that respect key features such as the overexpression of CD62E and adhesion of neutrophils during inflammation. Using microvascular endothelial cells originating from different tissues would be very interesting. This possibility is now mentioned in the discussion lines 567-568.

      • Photoablation is a homemade fabrication technique that can be implemented in any lab harboring an epifluorescence microscope. This method has been more detailed in the revised manuscript (L1085-1087).

      Line 576: The authors state that the model could be applied to other systemic infections but failed to mention that some infections have already been modelled in 3D bioengineered vascular models (examples found in https://doi.org/10.1038/s41564-024-01645-6). This includes a capillary photoablated vascular model to study malaria (DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.aay724).

      Thes two important references have been introduced in the main text (L84, 647, 648).

      Line 1213: Are the 6M neutrophil solution in 10ul under flow. Also, I would suggest to rewrite this sentence in the following line "After, the flow has been then added to the system at 0.7-1 µl/min."

      We now specified that neutrophils are circulated in the chip under flow conditions, lines 1321-1322.

      Significance

      The manuscript is comprehensive, complete and represents the first bioengineered model of sepsis. One of the major strengths is the carful characterization and benchmarking against the animal xenograft model. Its main limitations is the brief description of the photoablation methodology and more clarity is needed in the description of bacteria perfusion experiments, given their complexity. The manuscript will be of interest for the general infection community and to the tissue engineering community if more details on fabrication methods are included. My expertise is on infection bioengineered models.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility, and clarity):

      Summary:

      The authors develop a Vessel-on-Chip model, which has geometrical and physical properties similar to the murine vessels used in the study of systemic infections. The vessel was created via highly controllable laser photoablation in a collagen matrix, subsequent seeding of human endothelial cells and flow perfusion to induce mechanical cues. This vessel could be infected with Neisseria meningitidis, as a model of systemic infection. In this model, microcolony formation and dynamics, and effects on the host were very similar to those described for the human skin xenograft mouse, which is the current gold standard for these studies, and were consistent with observations made in patients. The model could also recapitulate the neutrophil response upon N. meningitidis systemic infection.

      Major comments:

      I have no major comments. The claims and the conclusions are supported by the data, the methods are properly presented and the data is analyzed adequately. Furthermore, I would like to propose an optional experiment could improve the manuscript. In the discussion it is stated that the vascular geometry might contribute to bacterial colonization in areas of lower velocity. It would be interesting to recapitulate this experimentally. It is of course optional but it would be of great interest, since this is something that can only be proven in the organ-on-chip (where flow speed can be tuned) and not as much in animal models. Besides, it would increase impact, demonstrating the superiority of the chip in this area rather than proving to be equal to current models.

      We have conducted additional experiments on infection in different vascular geometries now added these results figure 3/S3 and lines 288-305. We compared sheared stress levels as determined by Comsol simulation and experimentally determined bacterial adhesion sites. In the conditions used, the range of shear generated by the tested geometries do not appear to change the efficiency of bacterial adhesion. These results are consistent with a previous study from our group which show that in this range of shear stresses the effect on adhesion is limited (6) . Furthermore, qualitative observations in the animal model indicate that bacteria do not have an obvious preference in terms of binding site.

      Minor comments:

      I have a series of suggestions which, in my opinion, would improve the discussion. They are further elaborated in the following section, in the context of the limitations.

      • How to recapitulate the vessels in the context of a specific organ or tissue? If the pathogen is often found in the luminal space of other organs after disseminating from the blood, how can this process be recapitulated with this mode, if at all?

      For reasons that are not fully understood, postmortem histological studies reveal bacteria only inside blood vessels but rarely if ever in the organ parenchyma. The presence of intravascular bacteria could nevertheless alter cells in the tissue parenchyma. The notable exception is the brain where bacteria exit the bacterial lumen to access the cerebrospinal fluid. The chip we describe is fully adapted to develop a blood brain barrier model and more specific organ environments. This implies the addition of more cell types in the hydrogel. A paragraph on this topic has been added (Lines 548 and 552-570).

      • Similarly, could other immune responses related to systemic infection be recapitulated? The authors could discuss the potential of including other immune cells that might be found in the interstitial space, for example.

      This important discussion point has been added to the manuscript (L623-636). As suggested by Reviewer #2, other immune cells respond to N. meningitis and can be explored using our model. For instance, macrophages and dendritic cells are activated upon N. meningitis infection, eliminate the bacteria through phagocytosis, produce pro-inflammatory cytokines and chemokines potentially activating lymphocytes (7). Such an immune response, yet complex, would be interesting to study in our model as skin-xenograft mice are deprived of B and T lymphocytes to ensure acceptance of human skin grafts.

      • A minor correction: in line 467 it should probably be "aspects" instead of "aspect", and the authors could consider rephrasing that sentence slightly for increased clarity.

      We have corrected the sentence with "we demonstrated that our VoC strongly replicates key aspects of the in vivo human skin xenograft mouse model, the gold standard for studying meningococcal disease under physiological conditions." in lines 499-503.

      Strengths and limitations

      The most important strength of this manuscript is the technology they developed to build this model, which is impressive and very innovative. The Vessel-on-Chip can be tuned to acquire complex shapes and, according to the authors, the process has been optimized to produce models very quickly. This is a great advancement compared with the technologies used to produce other equivalent models. This model proves to be equivalent to the most advanced model used to date, but allows to perform microscopy with higher resolution and ease, which can in turn allow more complex and precise image-based analysis. However, the authors do not seem to present any new mechanistic insights obtained using this model. All the findings obtained in the infection-on-chip demonstrate that the model is equivalent to the human skin xenograft mouse model, and can offer superior resolution for microscopy. However, the advantages of the model do not seem to be exploited to obtain more insights on the pathogenicity mechanisms of N. meningitidis, host-pathogen interactions or potential applications in the discovery of potential treatments. For example, experiments to elucidate the role of certain N. meningiditis genes on infection could enrich the manuscript and prove the superiority of the model. However, I understand these experiments are time-consuming and out of the scope of the current manuscript. In addition, the model lacks the multicellularity that characterizes other similar models. The authors mention that the pathogen can be found in the luminal space of several organs, however, this luminal space has not been recapitulated in the model. Even though this would be a new project, it would be interesting that the authors hypothesize about the possibilities of combining this model with other organ models. The inclusion of circulating neutrophils is a great asset; however it would also be interesting to hypothesize about how to recapitulate other immune responses related to systemic infection.

      We thank Reviewer #2 for his/her comment on the strengths and limitations of our work. The difficulty is that our study opens many futur research directions and applications and we hope that the work serves as the basis for many future studies but one can only address a limited set of experiments in a single manuscript.

      • Experiments investigating the role of N. meningitidis genes require significant optimization of the system. Multiplexing is a potential avenue for future development, which would allow the testing of many mutants. The fast photoablation approach is particularly amenable to such adaptation.

      • Cells and bacteria inside the chambers could be isolated and analyzed at the transcriptomic level or by flow cytometry. This would imply optimizing a protocol for collecting cells from the device via collagenase digestion, for instance. This type of approach would also benefit from multiplexing to enhance the number of cells.

      • As mentioned above, the revised manuscript discusses the multicellular capabilities of our model, including the integration of additional immune cells and potential connections to other organ systems. We believe that these approaches are feasible and valuable for studying various aspects of N. meningitidis infection.

      Advance

      The most important advance of this manuscript is technical: the development of a model that proves to be equivalent to the most complex model used to date to study meningococcal systemic infections. The human skin xenograft mouse model requires complex surgical techniques and has the practical and ethical limitations associated with the use of animals. However, the Infection-on-chip model is completely in vitro, can be produced quickly, and allows to precisely tune the vessel’s geometry and to perform higher resolution microscopy. Both models were comparable in terms of the hallmarks defining the disease, suggesting that the presented model can be an effective replacement of the animal use in this area.

      Other vessel-on-chip models can recapitulate an endothelial barrier in a tube-like morphology, but do not recapitulate other complex geometries, that are more physiologically relevant and could impact infection (in addition to other non-infectious diseases). However, in the manuscript it is not clear whether the different morphologies are necessary to study or recapitulate N. meningitidis infection, or if the tubular morphologies achieved in other similar models would suffice.

      Audience

      This manuscript might be of interest for a specialized audience focusing on the development of microphysiological models. The technology presented here can be of great interest to researchers whose main area of interest is the endothelium and the blood vessels, for example, researchers on the study of systemic infections, atherosclerosis, angiogenesis, etc. Thus, the tool presented (vessel-on-chip) can have great applications for a broad audience. However, even when the method might be faster and easier to use than other equivalent methods, it could still be difficult to implement in another laboratory, especially if it lacks expertise in bioengineering. Therefore, the method could be more of interest for laboratories with expertise in bioengineering looking to expand or optimize their toolbox. Alternatively, this paper present itself as an opportunity to begin collaborations, since the model could be used to test other pathogen or conditions.

      Field of expertise:

      Infection biology, organ-on-chip, fungal pathogens.

      I lack the expertise to evaluate the image-based analysis.

      References

      (1) Gyohei Egawa, Satoshi Nakamizo, Yohei Natsuaki, Hiromi Doi, Yoshiki Miyachi, and Kenji Kabashima. Intravital analysis of vascular permeability in mice using two-photon microscopy. Scientific Reports, 3(1):1932, Jun 2013. ISSN 2045-2322. doi: 10.1038/srep01932.

      (2) Valeria Manriquez, Pierre Nivoit, Tomas Urbina, Hebert Echenique-Rivera, Keira Melican, Marie-Paule Fernandez-Gerlinger, Patricia Flamant, Taliah Schmitt, Patrick Bruneval, Dorian Obino, and Guillaume Duménil. Colonization of dermal arterioles by neisseria meningitidis provides a safe haven from neutrophils. Nature Communications, 12(1):4547, Jul 2021. ISSN 2041-1723. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-24797-z.

      (3) Mats Hellström, Holger Gerhardt, Mattias Kalén, Xuri Li, Ulf Eriksson, Hartwig Wolburg, and Christer Betsholtz. Lack of pericytes leads to endothelial hyperplasia and abnormal vascular morphogenesis. Journal of Cell Biology, 153(3):543–554, Apr 2001. ISSN 0021-9525. doi: 10.1083/jcb.153.3.543.

      (4) Arsheen M. Rajan, Roger C. Ma, Katrinka M. Kocha, Dan J. Zhang, and Peng Huang. Dual function of perivascular fibroblasts in vascular stabilization in zebrafish. PLOS Genetics, 16(10):1–31, 10 2020. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1008800.

      (5) Huanhuan He, Julia J. Mack, Esra Güç, Carmen M. Warren, Mario Leonardo Squadrito, Witold W. Kilarski, Caroline Baer, Ryan D. Freshman, Austin I. McDonald, Safiyyah Ziyad, Melody A. Swartz, Michele De Palma, and M. Luisa Iruela-Arispe. Perivascular macrophages limit permeability. Arteriosclerosis, Thrombosis, and Vascular Biology, 36(11):2203–2212, 2016. doi: 10.1161/ATVBAHA. 116.307592.

      (6) Emilie Mairey, Auguste Genovesio, Emmanuel Donnadieu, Christine Bernard, Francis Jaubert, Elisabeth Pinard, Jacques Seylaz, Jean-Christophe Olivo-Marin, Xavier Nassif, and Guillaume Dumenil. Cerebral microcirculation shear stress levels determine Neisseria meningitidis attachment sites along the blood–brain barrier . Journal of Experimental Medicine, 203(8):1939–1950, 07 2006. ISSN 0022-1007. doi: 10.1084/jem.20060482.

      (7) Riya Joshi and Sunil D. Saroj. Survival and evasion of neisseria meningitidis from macrophages. Medicine in Microecology, 17:100087, 2023. ISSN 2590-0978. doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.medmic. 2023.100087.

    1. Author response:

      General Statements

      We are grateful for constructive reviewers’ comments and criticisms and have thoroughly addressed all major and minor comments in the revised manuscript.

      Summary of new data.

      We have performed the following additional experiments to support our concept:

      (1) The kinetcs of ROS production in B6 and B6.Sst1S macrophages after TNF stimulation (Fig. 3I and J, Suppl. Fig. 3G);

      (2) Time course of stress kinase activation (Fig.3K) that clearly demonstrated the persistent stress kinase (phospho-ASK1 and phospho-cJUN) activation exclusively in. the B6.Sst1S macrophages;

      (3) New Fig.4 C-E panels include comparisons of the B6 and B6.Sst1S macrophage responses to TNF and effects of IFNAR1 blockade in both backgrounds.

      (4) We performed new experiments demonstrating that the synthesis of lipid peroxidation products (LPO) occurs in TNF-stimulated macrophages earlier than the IFNβ super-induction (Suppl.Fig.4A and B).

      (5) We demonstrated that the IFNAR1 blockade 12, 24 and 32 h after TNF stimulation still reduced the accumulation of LPO product (4-HNE) in TNF-stimulated B6.Sst1S BMDMs (Suppl.Fig.4 E-G).

      (6) We added comparison of cMyc expression between the wild type B6 and B6.Sst1S BMDMs during TNF stimulation for 6-24 h (Fig.5I-J).

      (7) New data comparing 4-HNE levels in Mtb-infected B6 wild type and B6.Sst1S macrophages and quantification of replicating Mtb was added (Fig.6B, Suppl.Fig.7C and D).

      (8) In vivo data described in Fig.7 was thoroughly revised and new data was included. We demonstrated increased 4-HNE loads in multibacillary lesions (Fig.7A, Suppl. Fig.9A) and the 4-HNE accumulation in CD11b+ myeloid cells (Fig.7B and Suppl.Fig.9B). We demonstrated that the Ifnb – expressing cells are activated iNOS+ macrophages (Fig.7D and Suppl.Fig.13A). Using new fluorescent multiplex IHC, we have shown that stress markers phopho-cJun and Chac1 in TB lesions are expressed by Ifnb- and iNOS-expressing macrophages (Fig.7E and Suppl.Fig.13D-F).

      (9) We performed additional experiment to demonstrate that naïve (non-BCG vaccinated) lymphocytes did not improve Mtb control by Mtb-infected macrophages in agreement with previously published data (Suppl.Fig.7H).

      Summary of updates

      Following reviewers requests we updated figures to include isotype control antibodies, effects of inhibitors on non-stimulated cells, positive and negative controls for labile iron pool, additional images of 4-HNE and live/dead cell staining.

      Isotype control for IFNAR1 blockade were included in Fig.3M, Fig.4C -E, Fig.6L-M Suppl.Fig.4F-G, 7I.

      Positive and negative controls for labile iron pool measurements were added to Fig.3E, Fig.5D, Suppl.Fig.3B

      Cell death staining images were added Suppl.Fig.3H

      Co-staining of 4-HNE with tubulin was added to Suppl.Fig.3A.

      High magnification images for Figure 7 were added in Suppl.Fig.8 to demonstrate paucibacillary and multibacillary image classification.

      Single-channel color images for individual markers were provided in Fig.7E and Suppl.Fig.13B-F.

      Inhibitor effects on non-stimulated cells were included in Fig.5 D-H, Suppl.Fig.6A and B. Titration of CSF1R inhibitors for non-toxic concentration determination are included in Suppl.Fig.6D.

      In addition, we updated the figure legends in the revised manuscript to include more details about the experiments. We also clarified our conclusions in the Discussion. Responses to every major and minor comment of the reviewers are provided below.

      Point-by-point description of the revisions

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity:

      Summary

      The study by Yabaji et al. examines macrophage phenotypes B6.Sst1S mice, a mouse strain with increased susceptibility to M. tuberculosis infection that develops necrotic lung lesions. Extending previous work, the authors specifically focus on delineating the molecular mechanisms driving aberrant oxidative stress in TNF-activated B6.Sst1S macrophages that has been associated with impaired control of M. tuberculosis. The authors use scRNAseq of bone marrow-derived macrophages to further characterize distinctions between B6.Sst1S and control macrophages and ascribe distinct trajectories upon TNF stimulation. Combined with results using inhibitory antibodies and small molecule inhibitors in in vitro experimentation, the authors propose that TNF-induced protracted c-Myc expression in B6.Sst1S macrophages disables the cellular defense against oxidative stress, which promotes intracellular accumulation of lipid peroxidation products, fueled at least in part by overexpression of type I IFNs by these cells. Using lung tissue sections from M. tuberculosis-infected B6.Sst1S mice, the authors suggest that the presence of a greater number of cells with lipid peroxidation products in lung lesions with high counts of stained M. tuberculosis are indicative of progressive loss of host control due to the TNF-induced dysregulation of macrophage responses to oxidative stress. In patients with active tuberculosis disease, the authors suggest that peripheral blood gene expression indicative of increased Myc activity was associated with treatment failure.

      Major comments

      The authors describe differences in protein expression, phosphorylation or binding when referring to Fig 2A-C, 2G, 3D, 5B, 5C. However, such differences are not easily apparent or very subtle and, in some cases, confounded by differences in resting cells (e.g. pASK1 Fig 3L; c-Myc Fig 5B) as well as analyses across separate gels/blots (e.g. Fig 3K, Fig 5B). Quantitative analyses across different independent experiments with adequate statistical analyses are required to strengthen the associated conclusions.

      We updated our Western blots as follows:

      (1) Densitometery of normalized bands is included above each lane (Fig.2A-C; Fig.3C-D and 3K; Fig.4A-B; Fig.5B,C,I,J). New data in Fig.3K is added to highlight differences between B6 and B6.Sst1S at individual timepoints after TNF stimulation. In Fig.5I we added new data comparing Myc levels in B6 and B6.Sst1S with and without JNK inhibitor and updated the results accordingly. New Fig.3K clearly demonstrates the persistent activation of p-cJun and pAsk1 at 24 and 36h of TNF stimulation. In Fig.5B we clearly demonstrate that Myc levels were higher in B6.Sst1S after 12 h of TNF stimulation. At 6h, however, the basal differences in Myc levels are consistently higher in B6.Sst1S and the induction by TNF is 1.6-fold similar in both backgrounds. We noted this in the text.

      (2) A representative experiment is shown in individual panels and the corresponding figure legend contains information on number of biological repeats. Each Western blot was repeated 2 – 4 times.

      The representative images of fluorescence microscopy in Fig 3H, 4H, 5H, S3C, S3I, S5A, S6A seem to suggest that under some conditions the fluorescence signal is located just around the nucleus rather than absent or diminished from the cytoplasm. It is unclear whether this reflects selective translocation of targets across the cell, morphological changes of macrophages in culture in response to the various treatments, or variations in focal point at which images were acquired. Control images (e.g. cellular actin, DIC) should be included for clarification. If cell morphology changes depending on treatments, how was this accounted for in the quantitative analyses? In addition, negative controls validating specificity of fluorescence signals would be warranted.

      Our conclusion of higher LPO production is based on several parameters: 4-HNE staining, measurements of MDA in cell lysates and oxidized lipids using BODIPY C11. Taken together they demonstrate significant and reproducible increase in LPO accumulation in TNFstimulated B6.Sst1S macrophages. This excludes imaging artefact related to unequal 4-HNE distribution noted by the reviewer. In fact, we also noted that the 4-HNE was spread within cell body of B6.Sst1S macrophages and confirmed it using co-staining with tubulin, as suggested by the reviewer (new Suppl.Fig.3A). Since low molecular weight LPO products, such as MDA and 4-HNE, traverse cell membranes, it is unlikely that they will be strictly localized to a specific membrane bound compartment. However, we agree that at lower concentrations, there might be some restricted localization, explaining a visible perinuclear ring of 4-HNE staining in B6 macrophages. This phenomenon may be explained just by thicker cytoplasm surrounding nucleus in activated macrophages spread on adherent plastic surface or by proximity to specific organelles involved in generation or clearance of LPO products and definitively warrants further investigation.

      We also included images of non-stimulated cells in Fig.3H, Suppl.Fig.3A and 3E. We used multiple fields for imaging and quantified fluorescence signals (Suppl. Fig.3D and 3F, Suppl.Fig.4G, Suppl.Fig.6A and B).

      We used negative controls without primary antibodies for the initial staining optimization, but did not include it in every experiment.

      To interpret the evaluation on the hierarchy of molecular mechanisms in B6.Sst1S macrophages, comparative analyses with B6 control cells should be included (e.g. Fig 4C-I, Fig 5, Fig 6B, E-M, S6C, S6E-F). This will provide weight to the conclusions that the dysregulated processes are specifically associated with the susceptibility of B6.Sst1S macrophages.

      Understanding the sst1-mediated effects on macrophage activation is the focus of our previously published studies Bhattacharya et al., JCI, 2021) and this manuscript. The data comparing B6 and B6.Sst1S macrophage are presented in Fig.1, Fig.2, Fig.3, Fig.4, Fig.5A-C, I and J, Fig.6A-C, 6J and corresponding supplemental figures 1, 2, 3, 4A and B, Suppl.Fig.5, Suppl.Fig.6C, Suppl.Fig.7A-D,7F.

      Once we identified the aberrantly activated pathways in the B6.Sst1S, we used specific inhibitors to correct the aberrant response in B6.Sst1S.

      All experiments using inhibitory antibodies require comparison to the effect of a matched isotype control in the same experiment (e.g. Fig 3J, 4F, G, I; 6L, 6M, S3G, S6F).

      Isotype control for IFNAR1 blockade were included in Fig.3M, Fig.4C-E, Fig.6L-M Suppl.Fig.4F-G, 7I.

      Experiments using inhibitors require inclusion of an inhibitor-only control to assess inhibitor effects on unstimulated cells (e.g. Fig 4I, 5D-I)

      Inhibitor effects on non-stimulated cells were included in Fig.5 D-H, Suppl.Fig.6A and B.

      Fig 3K and Fig 5J appear to contain the same images for p-c-Jun and b-tubulin blots.

      Fig.3K and 5J partially overlapped but had different focus – 3K has been updated to reflect the time course of stress kinase activation. Fig.5J is updated (currently Fig.5I and J) to display B6 and B6.Sst1S macrophage data including cMyc and p-cJun levels.

      Data of TNF-treated cells in Fig 3I appear to be replotted in Fig 3J.

      Currently these data is presented in Fig.3L and 3M and has been updated to include comparison of B6 and B6.Sst1S cells (Fig.3L) and effects of inhibitors in Fig.3M.

      It is stated that lungs from 2 mice with paucibacillary and 2 mice with multi-bacillary lesions were analyses. There is contradicting information on whether these tissues were collected at the same time post infection (week 14?) or whether the pauci-bacillary lesions were in lungs collected at earlier time points post infection (see Fig S8A). If the former, how do the authors conclude that multi-bacillary lesions are a progression from paucibacillary lesions and indicative of loss of M. tuberculosis control, especially if only one lesion type is observed in an individual host? If the latter, comparison between lesions will likely be dominated by temporal differences in the immune response to infection.

      In either case, it is relevant to consider density, location, and cellular composition of lesions (see also comments on GeoMx spatial profiling). Is the macrophage number/density per tissue area comparable between pauci-bacillary and multi-bacillary lesions?

      We did not collect lungs at the same time point. As described in greater detail in our preprints (Yabaji et al., https://doi.org/10.1101/2025.02.28.640830 and https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.10.17.562695) pulmonary TB lesions in our model of slow TB progression are heterogeneous between the animals at the same timepoint, as observed in human TB patients and other chronic TB animal models. Therefore, we perform analyses of individual TB lesions that are classified by a certified veterinary pathologist in a blinded manner based on their morphology (H&E) and acid fast staining of the bacteria, as depicted in Suppl.Fig.8. Currently it is impossible to monitor progression of individual lesions in mice. However, in mice TB is progressive disease and no healing and recovery from the disease have been observed in our studies or reported in literature. Therefore, we assumed that paucibacillary lesions preceded the multibacillary ones, and not vice versa, thus reflecting the disease progression. In our opinion, this conclusion most likely reflects the natural course of the disease. However, we edited the text : instead of disease progression we refer to paucibacillary and multibacillary lesions.

      Does 4HNE staining align with macrophages and if so, is it elevated compared to control mice and driven by TNF in the susceptible vs more resistant mice?

      We performed additional staining and analyses to demonstrate the 4-HNE accumulation in CD11b+ myeloid cells of macrophage morphology. Non-necrotic lesions contain negligible proportion of neutrophils (Fig.7B, Suppl.Fig.9B). B6 mice do not develop advanced multibacillary TB lesions containing 4-HNE+ cells. Also, 4-HNE staining was localized to TB lesions and was not found in uninvolved lung areas of the infected mice, as shown in Suppl.Fig.9A (left panel).

      It is well established that TNF plays a central role in the formation and maintenance of TB granulomas in humans and in all animal models. Therefore, TNF neutralization would lead to rapid TB progression, rapid Mtb growth and lesions destruction in both B6 and B6.Sst1S genetic backgrounds.

      Pathway analysis of spatial transcriptomic data (Suppl.Fig.11) identified TNF signaling via NFkB among dominant pathways upregulated in multibacillary lesions, suggesting that the 4-HNE accumulation paralleled increased TNF signaling. In addition, in vivo other cytokines, including IFN-I, could activate macrophages and stimulate production of reactive oxygen and nitrogen species and lead to the accumulation of LPO products as shown in this manuscript.

      It would be relevant to state how many independent lesions per host were sampled in both the multiplex IHC as well as the GeoMx data. Can the authors show the selected regions of interest in the tissue overview and in the analyses to appreciate within-host and across-host heterogeneity of lesions. The nature of the spatial transcriptomics platform used is such that the data are derived from tissue areas that contain more than just Iba1+ macrophages. At later stages of infection, the cellular composition of such macrophage-rich areas will be different when compared to lesions earlier in the infection process. Hence, gene expression profiles and differences between tissue regions cannot be attributed to macrophages in this tissue region but are more likely a reflection of a mix of cellular composition and per-cell gene expression.

      We used Iba1 staining to identify macrophages in TB lesions and programmed GeoMx instrument to collect spatial transcriptomics probes from Iba1+ cells within ROIs. Also, we selected regions of interest (ROI) avoiding necrotic areas (depicted in Suppl.Fig.10). We agree that Iba1+ macrophage population is heterogenous – some Iba1+ cells are activated iNOS+ macrophages, other are iNOS-negative (Fig.7C and D, and Suppl.Fig.13A). Multibacillary lesions contain larger areas occupied by activated (iNOS+) macrophages (Fig.7D,

      Suppl.Fig.13B and 13F). Although the GeoMx spatial transcriptomic platform does not provide single cell resolution, it allowed us to compare populations of Iba1+ cells in paucibacillary and multibacillary TB lesions and to identify a shift in their overall activation pattern.

      It is stated that loss of control of M. tuberculosis in multibacillary lesions was associated with "downregulation of IFNg-inducible genes". If the authors base this on the tissue expression of individual genes, this requires further investigation to support such conclusion (also see comment on GeoMx above). Furthermore, how might this conclusion be compatible with significantly elevated iNOS+ cells (Fig 7D) in multibacillary lesions?

      We demonstrated that Ciita gene expression is specifically induced by IFN-gamma and is suppressed by IFN-I (Fig.6M). The expression of Ciita in paucibacillary lesions suggest the presence of the IFN-gamma activated cells and its disappearance in the multibacillary lesion is consistent with massive activation of IFN-I pathway (Fig.7C).

      It is appreciated that the human blood signature analyses contain Myc-signatures but the association with treatment failure is not very strong based on the data in Fig 13B and C (Suppl.Fig.15B and C now). The authors indicate that they have no information on disease severity, but it should perhaps not be assumed that treatment failure is indicative of poor host control of the infection. Perhaps independent analyses in separate cohort/data set can add strength and provide -additional insights (e.g. PMID: 35841871; PMID: 32451443, PMID: 17205474, PMID: 22872737). In addition, the human data analyses could be strengthened by extension to additional signatures such as IFN, TNF, oxidative stress. Details of the human study design are not very clear and are lacking patient demographics, site of disease, time of blood collection relative to treatment onset, approving ethics committees.

      X axis of Suppl.Fig.15A represent pre-defined molecular signature gene sets (MSigDB) in Gene Set Enrichment Analysis (GSEA) database (https://www.gseamsigdb.org/gsea/msigdb). On Y axis is area under curve (AUC) score for each gene set. The Myc upregulated gene set myc_up was identified among top gene sets associated with treatment failure using unbiased ssGSEA algorithm. The upregulation of Myc pathway in the blood transcriptome associated with TB treatment failure most likely reflects greater proportion of immature cells in peripheral blood, possibly due to increased myelopoiesis.

      Pathway analysis of the differentially expressed genes revealed that treatment failures were associated with the following pathways relevant to this study: NF-kB Signaling, Flt3 Signaling in Hematopoietic Progenitor Cells (indicative of common myeloid progenitor cell proliferation), SAPK/JNK Signaling and Senescence (indicative of oxidative stress). The upregulation of these pathways in human patients with poor TB treatment outcomes correlates with our findings in TB susceptible mice. The detailed analysis of differentially regulated pathways in human TB patients is beyond the scope of this study and is presented in another manuscript entitled “ Tuberculosis risk signatures and differential gene expression predict individuals who fail treatment” by Arthur VanValkenburg et al., submitted for publication.

      Blood collection for PBMC gene expression profiling of TB patients was prior to TB treatment or within a first week of treatment commencement. Boxplot of bootstrapped ssGSEA enrichment AUC scores from several oncogene signatures ranked from lowest to highest AUC score, with myc_up and myc_dn genes highlighted in red.

      We agree with the reviewer that not every gene in the myc_up gene set correlates with the treatment outcome. But the association of the gene set is statistically significant, as presented in Suppl.Fig.15B – C.

      We updated the details of the study, including study sites and the ethics committee approval statement and references describing these cohorts.

      Other comments

      It is excellent that the authors provide individual data points. Choosing a colour other than black would increase clarity when black bars are used.

      We followed this useful suggestion and selected consistent color codes for B6 and B6.Sst1S groups to enhance clarity throughout the revised manuscript.

      Error bars are inconsistently depicted as either bi-directional or just unidirectional.

      We used bi-directional error bars in the revised manuscript.

      Fig 1E, G, H - please include a scale to clarify what the heat map is representing.

      We have included the expression key in Fig.1E,G and H and Suppl.Fig.1C and D in the revised version.

      Fig 2K, Fig S10A gene information cannot be deciphered.

      We increased the font in previous Fig.2K and moved to supplement to keep larger fonts (current Suppl.Fig.2G).

      Fig S4A,B please add error bars.

      These data are presented as Suppl.Fig.5 in the revised version. We performed one experiment to test the hypothesis. Because the data indicated no clear increase in transposon small RNAs in the sst1S macrophages, we did not pursue this hypothesis further, and therefore, the error bars were not included. However, we decided to include these negative data because it rejects a very attractive and plausible hypothesis.

      Please use gene names as per convention (e.g. Ifnb1) to distinguish gene expression from protein expression in figures and text.

      We addressed the comment in the revised manuscript.

      Fig S8B. Contrary to the description of results, there seems to be minimal overlap between the signal for YFP and the Ifnb1 probe. Is the Ifnb1 reporter mouse a legacy reporter? If so, it is worth stating this and including such considerations in the data interpretation.

      The YFP reporter expresses YFP protein under the control of the Ifnb1 promoter. The YFP protein accumulates within the cells and while Ifnb protein is rapidly secreted and does not accumulate in the producing cells in appreciable amounts. So YFP is not a lineage tracing reporter, but its accumulation marks the Ifnb1 promoter activity in cells, although the YFP protein half-life is longer than that of the Ifnb1 mRNA that is rapidly degraded (Witt et al., BioRxiv, 2024; doi:10.1101/2024.08.28.61018). Therefore, there is no precise spatiotemporal coincidence of these readouts.

      Please clarify what is meant by "normal interstitium" ? If the tissue is from uninfected mice, please state clearly.

      In this context we refer to the uninvolved lung areas of the infected lungs. In every sample we compare uninvolved lung areas and TB lesions of the same animal. Also, we performed staining of lung of non-infected mice as additional controls.

      If macrophage cultures underwent media changes every 48h, how was loss of liberated Mtb taken into account especially if differences in cell density/survival were noted? The assessment of M. tuberculosis load by qPCR is not well described. In particular, the method of normalization applied within the experiments (not within the qPCR) here remains unclear, even with reference to the authors' prior publication.

      Our lab has many years of experience working with macrophage monolayers infected with virulent Mtb and uses optimized protocols to avoid cell losses and related artifacts. Recently we published a detailed protocol for this methodology in STAR Protocols (Yabaji et al., 2022; PMID 35310069). In brief, it includes preparation of single cell suspensions of Mtb by filtration to remove clumps, use of low multiplicity of infection, preparation of healthy confluent monolayers and use of nutrient rich culture medium and medium change every 2 days. We also rigorously control for cell loss using whole well imaging and quantification of cell numbers and live/dead staining.

      Please add citation for the limma package.

      The references has been added (Ritchie et al, NAR 2015; PMID 25605792).

      The description of methodology relating to the "oncogene signatures" is unclear.

      This signature was described in Bild etal, Nature, 2006 and McQuerry JA, et al, 2019 “Pathway activity profiling of growth factor receptor network and stemness pathways differentiates metaplastic breast cancer histological subtypes”. BMC Cancer 19: 881 and is cited in Methods section Oncogene signatures

      Please clearly state time points post infection for mouse analyses.

      We collected lung samples from Mtb infected mice 12 – 20 weeks post infection. The lesions were heterogeneous and were individually classified using criteria described above.

      Reference is made to "a list of genes unique to type I [interferon] genes [....]" (p29). Can the authors indicate the source of the information used for compiling this list?

      The lists were compiled from Reactome, EMBL's European Bioinformatics Institute and GSEA databases. The links for all datasets are provided in Suppl.Table 8 “Expression of IFN pathway genes in Iba1+ cells from pauci- and multi-bacillary lesions of Mtb infected B6.Sst1S mouse lungs” in the “Pool IFN I & II gene sets” worksheet.

      The discussion at present is very long, contains repetition of results and meanders on occasion.

      Thank you for this suggestion, We critically revised the text for brevity and clarity.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance):  

      Strengths and limitations  

      Strengths: multi-pronged analysis approaches for delineating molecular mechanisms of macrophage responses that might underpin susceptibility to M. tuberculosis infection; integration of mouse tissues and human blood samples  

      Weaknesses: not all conclusions supported by data presented; some concerns related to experimental design and controls; links between findings in human cohort and the mechanistic insights gained in mouse macrophage model uncertain

      The revised manuscript addresses every major and minor comment of the reviewers, including isotype controls and naïve T cells, to provide additional support for our conclusions. Our study revealed causal links between Myc hyperactivity with the deficiency of anti-oxidant defense and type I interferon pathway hyperactivity. We have shown that Myc hyperactivity in TNF-stimulated macrophages compromises antioxidant defense leading to autocatalytic lipid peroxidation and interferon-beta superinduction that in turn amplifies lipid peroxidation, thus, forming a vicious cycle of destructive chronic inflammation. This mechanism offers a plausible mechanistic explanation of for the association of Myc hyperactivity with poorer treatment outcomes in TB patients and provide a novel target for host-directed TB therapy.

      Advance

      The study has the potential to advance molecular understanding of the TNF-driven state of oxidative stress previously observed in B6.Sst1S macrophages and possible implications for host control of M. tuberculosis in vivo.

      Audience

      Experts seeking understanding of host factors mediating M. tuberculosis control, or failure thereof, with appreciation for the utility of the featured mouse model in assessing TB diseases progression and severe manifestation. Interest is likely extended to audience more broadly interested in TNF-driven macrophage (dys)function in infectious, inflammatory, and autoimmune pathologies.

      Reviewer expertise

      In preparing this review, I am drawing on my expertise in assessing macrophage responses and host defense mechanisms in bacterial infections (incl. virulent M. tuberculosis) through in vitro and in vivo studies. This includes but is not limited to macrophage infection and stimulation assays, microscopy, intra-macrophage replication of M. tuberculosis, analyses of lung tissues using multi-plex IHC and spatial transcriptomics (e.g. GeoMx). I am familiar with the interpretation of RNAseq analyses in human and mouse cells/tissues, but can provide only limited assessment of appropriateness of algorithms and analysis frameworks.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      Yabaji et al. investigated the effects of BMDMs stimulated with TNF from both WT and B6.Sst1S mice, which have previously been identified to contain the sst1 locus conferring susceptibility to Mycobacterium tuberculosis. They identified that B6.Sst1S macrophages show a superinduction of IFNß, which might be caused by increased c-Myc expression, expanding on the mechanistic insights made by the same group (Bhattacharya et al. 2021). Furthermore, prolonged TNF stimulation led to oxidative stress, which WT BMDMs could compensate for by the activation of the antioxidant defense via NRF2. On the other hand, B6.Sst1S BMDMs lack the expression of SP110 and SP140, co-activators of NRF2, and were therefore subjected to maintained oxidative stress. Yabaji et al. could link those findings to in vivo studies by correlating the presence of stressed and aberrantly activated macrophages within granulomas to the failure of Mtb control, as well as the progression towards necrosis. As the knowledge regarding Mtb progression and necrosis of granulomas is not yet well understood, findings that might help provide novel therapy options for TB are crucial. Overall, the manuscript has interesting findings with regard to macrophage responses in Mycobacteria tuberculosis infection.

      However, in its current form there are several shortcomings, both with respect to the precision of the experiments and conclusions drawn.

      In particular a) important controls are often missing, e.g. T-cells form non-immune mice in Fig. 6J, in F, effectivity of BCG in B6 mice in 6N; b) single experiments are shown throughout the manuscript, in particular western blots and histology without proper quantification and statistics, this is absolutely not acceptable; c) very few repetitions are shown in in vitro experiments, where there is no evidence for limitation in resources (usually not more than 3), it is not clear what "independent experiment means" - i.e. the robustness of the findings is questionable; d) data are often normalized multiple times, e.g. in the case of qPCR, and the methods of normalization are not clear (what house-keeping gene exactly?);

      Moreover, experiments regarding IFN I signaling (e.g. short term TNF treatment of BMDMs to analyze LPO, making sure that the reporter mouse for IFNß works in vivo) and c-Myc (e.g. the increase after M-CSF addition might impact on other analysis as well and the experiments should be adjusted to control for this effect; MYC expression in the human samples) should be carefully repeated and evaluated to draw correct conclusions.

      In addition, we would like to strongly encourage the authors to more precisely outline the experimental set-ups and figure legends, so that the reader can easily understand and follow them. In other words: The legends are - in part very - incomplete. In addition, the authors should be mindful of gene names vs. protein names and italicize where appropriate.

      We appreciate a very thorough evaluation of our manuscript by this reviewer. Their insightful comments helped us improve the manuscript. As outlined below in point-by-point responses (1) we added important controls including isotype control antibodies in IFNAR blocking experiments and non-vaccinated T cells in T cell – macrophage interactions experiments; updated figure legends to indicate number of repeated experiment where a representative experiment is shown, numbers of mouse lungs and individual lesions, methods of data normalization, where it was missing. We also explained our in vitro experimental design and how we analyzed and excluded effects of media change and fresh CSF1 addition, by using a rest period before TNF stimulation and Mtb infection. The data shown in Suppl. Fig. 6C (previously Suppl. Fig. 5B) demonstrate that Myc levels induced by CSF1 return to the basal level at 12 h after media change. Our detailed in vitro protocol that contains these details has been published (Yabaji et al., STAR Protocols, 2022). We added new data demonstrating the ROS and LPO production at 6h of TNF stimulation, while the Ifnb1 mRNA super-induction occurred at 16 – 18 h, and edited the text to highlight these dynamics. The upregulation of Myc pathway in human samples does not necessarily mean the upregulation of Myc itself, it could be due to the dysregulation of downstream pathways. The upregulation of Myc pathway in the blood transcriptome associated with TB treatment failure most likely reflects greater proportion of immature cells in peripheral blood, possibly due to increased myelopoiesis. The detailed analysis of this cell populations in human patients is suggested by our findings but it is beyond the scope of this study.

      The reviewer’s comments also suggested that a summary of our findings was necessary. The main focus of our study was to untangle connections between oxidative stress and Ifnb1 superinduction. It revealed that Myc hyperactivity caused partial deficiency of antioxidant defense leading to type I interferon pathway hyperactivity that in turn amplifies lipid peroxidation, thus establishing a vicious cycle driving inflammatory tissue damage.

      Our laboratory worked on mechanisms of TB granuloma necrosis over more than two decades using genetic, molecular and immunological analyses in vitro and in vivo. It provided mechanistic basis for independent studies in other laboratories using our mouse model and further expanding our findings, thus supporting the reproducibility and robustness of our results and our lab’s expertise.

      Specific comments to the experiments and data:

      - Fig. 1E: Evaluation of differences in up- and downregulation between B6 and B6.Sst1S cells should highlight where these cells are within the heatmap, as it is only labelled with the clusters, or it should be depicted differently (in particular for cluster 1 and 2). Furthermore, a more simple labelling of the pathways would increase the readability of the data.

      For our scRNAseq data presentation, we used formats accepted by computational community. To clarify Fig.1E, we added labels above B6 and B6.Sst1S-specific clusters.

      - Fig. 2D, E: The staining legend is missing. For the quantification it is not clear what % total means. Is this based on the intensity or area? What do the dots represent in the bar chart? Is one data point pooled from several pictures? If not, the experiments need to be repeated, as three pictures might not be representative for evaluation.

      - Fig. 2E: Statistics comparing B6/ B6,SsT1S with TNF (different) is required: Absence of induction is not a proof for a difference!

      We included staining with NRF2-specific antibodies and performed area quantification per field using ImageJ to calculate the NRF2 total signal intensity per field. Each dot in the graph represents the average intensity of 3 fields in a representative experiment. The experiment was repeated 3 times. We included pairwise comparison of TNF-stimulated B6 and B6.Sst1S macrophages and updated the figure legend.

      - Fig. 3E: Positive and negative control need to be depicted in the figure (see legend).

      We have added the positive and negative controls for the determination of labile iron pool to the data in Fig. 3E and related Suppl. Fig. 3B and to Fig. 5D that also demonstrates labile iron determination.

      - Fig. 3I: A quantification by flow cytometry or total cell counts are important, as 6% cell death in cell culture is a very modest observation. Otherwise, confocal images of the quantification would be a good addition to judge the specificity of the viability staining.

      To validate the specificity of the viability staining method, we have provided fluorescent images as Suppl.Fig.3H. The main point of this experiment was to demonstrate a modest, but reproducible, increase in cell death in the sst1-mutant macrophages that suggested an IFNdependent oxidative damage. In our study, we did not focus on mechanisms of cell death, but on a state of chronic oxidative stress in the sst1 mutant live cells during TNF stimulation.

      - Fig. 3I, J: What does one dot represent?

      We performed this assay in 96 well format and each dot represent the % cell death in an individual well.

      - Fig. 3K,L: For the B6 BMDMs it seems that p-cJun is highly increased at 12h in (L), while it is not in (K). On the other hand, for the B6.Sst1S BMDMs it peaks at 24h in (K), while in (L) it seems to at 12h. According to the data in (L) it seems that p-cJun is rather earlier and stronger activated in B6 BMDMs and has a weakened but prolonged activation in the B6.Sst1S BMDMs, which would not fit with your statement in the text that B6.Sst1S BMDMs show an upregulation.

      These experiments need repetitions and quantification and statistiscs.

      Fig. 3L: ASK1 seems to be higher at 12h for the B6 BMDMs and similar for both lines at 24h, which is not fitting to the statement in the text. ("Also, the ASK1 - JNK - cJun stress kinase axis was upregulated in B6.Sst1S macrophages, as compared to B6, after 12 - 36 h of TNF stimulation")

      These experiments were repeated, and new data were added to highlight differences in ASK1 and c-Jun phosphorylation between B6 and B6.Sst1S at individual timepoints after TNF stimulation (presented in new Fig.3K). It demonstrated that after TNF stimulation the activation of stress kinases ASK1 and c-Jun initially increased in both genetic backgrounds. However, their upregulation was maintained exclusively in the sst1-susceptible macrophages from 24 to 36 h of TNF stimulation, while in the resistant macrophages their upregulation was transient. Thus, during prolonged TNF stimulation, B6.Sst1S macrophages experience stress that cannot be resolved, as evidenced by this kinetic analysis. The quantification of the band intensity was added to Western blot images above individual lanes.

      Reviewer 2 pointed to missing isotype control antibodies in Fig.3 and Fig.4:

      - Figure 3J: the isotype control for the IFNAR antibody is missing

      - Figure 4E: It seems the isotype control itself has already an effect in the reduction of IFNb.

      - Fig. 4H: It seems that the Isotype control antibody had an effect to increase 4-HNE (compared to TNF stimulated only).

      We always include isotype control antibodies in our experiments because antibodies are known to modulate macrophage activation via binding to Fc receptor. To address the reviewer’s comments, we updated all panels that present the effects of IFNAR1 blockade with isotypematched non-specific control antibodies in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we included isotype control in Fig. 3M (previously Fig.3J), Fig.4I, Suppl.4E-G, Fig.6L-M), Suppl.Fig.7I (previously Suppl.Fig.6F).

      - Fig.4A - C: "IFNAR1 blockade, however, did not increase either the NRF2 and FTL protein levels, or the Fth, Ftl and Gpx1 mRNA levels above those treated with isotype control antibodies"

      Maybe not above the isotype but it is higher than the TNF alone stimulation at least for NRF2 at 8h and for Ftl at both time points. Why does the isotype already cause stimulation/induction of the cells? !These experiments need repetitions and quantification and statistics!

      To determine specific effects of IFNAR blockade we compared effects of non-specific isotype control and IFNAR1-specific antibodies. In our experiments, the isotype control antibody modestly increased of Nrf2 and Ftl protein levels and the Fth and Ftl mRNA levels, but their effects were similar to the effect of IFNAR-specific antibody. The non-IFN -specific effects of antibodies, although are of potential biological significance, are modest in our model and their analysis is beyond the scope of this study.

      - Fig.4H Was the AB added also at 12h post stimulation? Figure legend should be adjusted.

      The IFNAR1 blocking antibodies and isotype control antibodies were added at 2 h after TNF stimulation in Fig.4H and 4I, as described in the corresponding figure legend. The data demonstrating effects of IFNAR blockade after 12, 24,and 33h of TNF stimulation are presented in Suppl.Fig.4 E-G.

      - Figure 4I: How was the data measured here, i.e. what is depicted? The isotype control is missing. It seems a two-way ANOVA was used, yet it is stated differently. The figure legend should be revised, as Dunnett's multiple comparison would only check for significances compared to the control.

      The microscopy images and bar graphs were updated to include isotype control and presented in Suppl. Fig.4E - G of the revised version. We also revised the statistical analysis to include correction for multiple comparisons.

      - Figure 4C and subsequent: How exactly was the experiment done (house-keeping gene)?

      We included the details in the figure legends of revised version. We quantified the gene expression by DDCt method using b-actin (for Fig. 4C-E) and 18S (For Fig. 4F and G) as internal controls.

      - Figure 4D,E: Information on cells used is missing. Why the change in stimulation time? Did it not work after 12h? Then the experiments in A-C should be repeated for 16h.

      The updated Fig. 4D and E present comparison of B6 and B6.Sst1S BMDMs clearly demonstrating significant difference between these macrophages in Ifnb1 mRNA expression 16 h after TNF stimulation, in agreement with our previous publication(Bhattacharya, et al., 2021). There we studied the time course of responses of B6 and B6.Sst1S macrophages to TNF at 2h intervals and demonstrated the divergence between their activation trajectories starting at 12 h of TNF stimulation Therefore, to reveal the underlying mechanisms we focus our analyses on this critical timepoint, i.e. as close to the divergence as possible. However, the difference between the strains in Ifnb1 mRNA expression achieved significance only by 16h of TNF stimulation. That is why we have used this timepoint for the Ifnb1 and Rsad2 analyses. It clearly shows that the superinduction was not driven by the positive feedback via IFNAR, as has been shown by the Ivashkiv lab for B6 wild type macrophages previously PMID 21220349.

      - Figure 4E: It would be helpful to see if these transcripts are actually translated into protein levels, e.g. perform an ELISA. Authors state that IFNAR blockages does not alter the expression but you statistic says otherwise.

      - The data for Ifnb expression (or better protein level) should be provided for B6 BMDMs as well.

      We have previously reported the differences in Ifnb protein secretion (He et al., Plos Pathogens, 2013 and Bhattacharya et al., JCI 2021). We use mRNA quantification by qRT-PCR as a more sensitive and direct measurement of the sst1-mediated phenotype. The revised Fig.4D and E include responses of B6 in addition to the B6.Sst1S to demonstrate that the IFNAR blockade does not reduce the Ifnb1 mRNA levels in TNF-stimulated B6.Sst1S mutant to the B6 wild type levels. A slight reduction can be explained by a known positive feedback loop in the IFN-I pathway (see above). In this experiment we emphasized that the effect of the sst1 locus is substantially greater, as compared to the effect of the IFNAR blockade (Fig.4D), and updated the text accordingly.

      - Fig. 4F: To what does the fold induction refer to? If it is again to unstimulated cells, then why is the induction now so much higher than in (E) where it was only 50x (now to 100x).

      - Figure 4G: Again to what is the fold induction referring to? It seems your Fer-1 treatment only contains 2 data points. This needs to be fixed.

      Yes, the fold induction was calculated by normalizing mRNA levels to untreated control incubated for the same time. Regarding the variation in Ifnb1 mRNA levels - a two-fold variation is not unusual in these experiments that may result in the Ifnb1 mRNA superinduction ranging from 50 -200-fold at this timepoint (16h). The graph in Fig.4G was modified to make all datapoints more visible.

      - "These data suggest that type I IFN signaling does not initiate LPO in our model but maintains and amplifies it during prolonged TNF stimulation that, eventually, may lead to cell death". Data for a short term TNF stimulation are not shown, however, so it might impact also on the initiation of LPO.

      - The overall conclusion drawn from Fig. 3 and 4 is not really clear with regard that IFN does not initiate LPO. Where is that shown? Data on earlier stimulation time points should be added to make this clear.

      We demonstrated ROS production (new Suppl.Fig.3G) and the rate of LPO biosynthesis (new Suppl.Fig.4E-F) at 6 h post TNF stimulation, while the Ifnb1 superinduction occurs between 12-18 h post TNF stimulation. This temporal separation supports our conclusion that IFN-β superinduction does not initiate LPO. We clarified it in the text:

      “Thus, Ifnb1 super-induction and IFN-I pathway hyperactivity in B6.Sst1S macrophages follow the initial LPO production, and maintain and amplify it during prolonged TNF stimulation”. (Previously: These data suggest that type I IFN signaling does not initiate LPO in our model). We also edited the conclusion in this section to explain the hierarchy of the sst1-regulated AOD and IFN-I pathways better:

      “Taken together, the above experiments allowed us to reject the hypothesis that IFN-I hyperactivity caused the sst1-dependent AOD dysregulation. In contrast, they established that the hyperactivity of the IFN-I pathway in TNF-stimulated B6.Sst1S macrophages was itself driven by the initial dysregulation of AOD and iron-mediated lipid peroxidation. During prolonged TNF stimulation, however, the IFN-I pathway was upregulated, possibly via ROS/LPOdependent JNK activation, and acted as a potent amplifier of lipid peroxidation”.

      We believe that these additional data and explanation strengthen our conclusions drawn from Figures 3 and 4.

      - "A select set of mouse LTR-containing endogenous retroviruses (ERV's) (Jayewickreme et al, 2021), and non-retroviral LINE L1 elements were expressed at a basal level before and after TNF stimulation, but their levels in the B6.Sst1S BMDMs were similar to or lower than those seen in B6". This sentence should be revised as the differences between B6 and B6.Sst1S BMDMs seem small and are not there after 48h anymore. Are these mild changes really caused by the mutation or could they result from different housing conditions and/or slowly diverging genetically lines. How many mice were used for the analysis? Is there already heterogeneity between mice from the same line?

      We agree with the reviewer that the data presented in Suppl.Fig.4 (Suppl.Fig.5 in the revised version) indicated no increase in single- and double-stranded transposon RNAs in the B6.Sst1S macrophages. The purpose of these experiment was to test the hypothesis that increased transposon expression might be responsible for triggering the superinduction of type I interferon response in TNF-stimulated B6.Sst1S macrophages. In collaboration with a transposon expert Dr. Nelson Lau (co-author of this manuscript) we demonstrated that transposon expression was not increased above the B6 level and, thus, rejected this attractive hypothesis. We explained the purpose of this experiment in the text and adequately described our findings as “the levels in the B6.Sst1S BMDMs were similar to or lower than those seen in B6”…and concluded that ” the above analyses allowed us to exclude the overexpression of persistent viral or transposon RNAs as a primary mechanism of the IFN-I pathway hyperactivity” in the sst1-mutant macrophages.

      - Fig. 5A: Indeed, it even seems that Myc is upregulated for the mutant BMDMs. Yet, there are only 2 data points for B6 12h.

      These experiments need repetitions and quantification and statistics.

      We observed these differences in c-Myc mRNA levels by independent methods: RNAseq and qRT-PCR. The qRT-PCR experiments were repeated 3 times. A representative experiment in Fig.5A shows 3 data points for each condition. We reformatted the panel to make all data points clearly visible.

      - Fig. 5B: Why would the protein level decrease in the controls over 6h of additional cultivation? Is this caused by fresh M-CSF? In this case maybe cells should be left to settle for one day before stimulating them to properly compare c-Myc induction. Comment on two c-Myc bands is needed. At 12h only the upper one seems increased for TNF stimulated mutant BMDMs compared to B6 BMDMs.

      We agree with the reviewer’s point that cells need to be rested after media change that contains fresh CSF-1. Indeed, in Suppl.Fig.6C, we show that after media change containing 10% L929 supernatant (a source of CSF1) there is an increase in c-Myc protein levels that takes approximately 12 hours to return to baseline.

      Our protocol includes resting period of 18-24 h after medium change before TNF stimulation.

      We updated Methods to highlight this detail. Thus, the increase in c-Myc levels we observe at 12 h of TNF stimulation (Fig.5B) is induced by TNF, not the addition of growth factors, as further discussed in the text.

      The two c-Myc bands observed in Fig.5B,I and J, are similar to patterns reported in previous studies that used the same commercial antibodies (PMIDs: 24395249, 24137534, 25351955). Whether they correspond to different c-Myc isoforms or post-translational modifications is unknown.

      - Fig. 5A,B: It seems that not all the RNA is translated into protein, as c-Myc at 12h in the mutant BMDMs seems to be lower than at 6h, while the gene expression implicates it vice versa.

      In addition to Fig.5B, the time course of Myc protein expression up to 24 h is presented in new panels Fig. 5I-5J. It demonstrates the gradual decrease of Myc protein levels. The observed dissociation between the mRNA and protein levels in the sst1-mutant BMDMs at 12 and 24 h is most likely due to translation inhibition as a result of the development of the integrated stress response, ISR (as shown in our previous publication by Bhattacharya et al., JCI, 2021). Translation of Myc is known to be particularly sensitive to the ISR (PMID18551192, PMID25079319, PMID28490664). Perhaps, the IFN-driven ISR may serve as a backup mechanism for Myc downregulation. We are planning to investigate these regulatory mechanisms in greater detail in the future.

      - Fig. 5J: Indeed, the inhibitor seems to cause the downregulation of the proteins. Explanation?

      This experiment was repeated twice and the average normalized densitometry values are presented in the updated Fig.5J. The main question addressed in this experiment was whether hyperactivity of JNK in TNF-stimulated sst1 mutant macrophages contributed to Myc upregulation, as had been previously shown in cancer. Comparing effects of JNK inhibition on phospho-cJun and c-Myc protein levels in TNF stimulated B6.Sst1S macrophages (updated Fig.5J), we rejected the hypotghesis that JNK activity might have a major role in c-Myc upregulation in sst1 mutant macrophages.

      - "TNF stimulation tended to reduce the LPO accumulation in the B6 macrophages and to increase it in the B6.Sst1S ones" However, this is not apparent in Sup. Fig. 6B. Here it seems that there might be a significant increase.

      Suppl.Fig.6B (currently Suppl.Fig.7B) shows the 4-HNE accumulation at day 3 post infection. The data obtained after 5 days of Mtb infection are shown in Fig.6A. We clarified this in the text: “By day 5 post infection, TNF stimulation induced significant LPO accumulation only in the B6.Sst1S macrophages (Fig.6A)”.

      - Fig. 6B: Mtb and 4-HNE should be shown in two different channels in order to really assign each staining correctly.

      What time point is this? Are the mycobacteria cleared at MOI1, since it looks that there are fewer than that? How does this look like for the B6 BMDMs? Are there even less mycobacteria?

      We included B6 infection data to the updated Fig.6B and added Suppl.Fig.7C and 7D that address this reviewer’s comment. The data represent day 5 of Mtb infection as indicated in the updated Fig.6B and Suppl.Fig.7C and 7D legends. New Suppl.Fig.7D shows quantification of replicating Mtb using Mtb replication reporter stain expressing single strand DNA binding protein GFP fusion, as described in Methods. We observed fewer Mtb and a lower percentage of replicating Mtb in B6 macrophages, but we did not observe a complete Mtb elimination in either background.

      We used red fluorescence for both Mtb::mCherry and 4-HNE staining to clearly visualize the SSB-GFP puncta in replicating Mtb DNA. In the revised manuscript, we have included the relevant channels in Suppl. Fig.7C and D to demonstrate clearly distinct patterns of Mtb::mCherry and 4-HNE signals. We did not aim to quantify the 4-HNE signal intensity in this experiment. For the 4-HNE quantification we use Mtb that expressed no reporter proteins (Fig.6A-B and Suppl.Fig.7A-B).

      - Fig 6E: In the context of survival a viability staining needs to be included, as well as the data from day 0. Then it needs to be analyzed whether cell numbers remain the same from D0 or if there is a change.

      We updated Fig.6 legend to indicate that the cell number percentages were calculated based on the number of cells at Day 0 (immediately after Mtb infection). We routinely use fixable cell death staining to enumerate cell death to exclude artifacts due to cell loss. Brief protocol containing this information is included in Methods section. The detailed protocol including normalization using BCG spike has been published – Yabaji et al, STAR Protocols, 2022. Here we did not present dead cell percentage as it remained low and we did not observe damage to macrophage monolayers. The fold change of Mtb was calculated after normalization using Mtb load at Day 0 after infection and washes.

      "The 3D imaging demonstrated that YFP-positive cells were restricted to the lesions, but did not strictly co-localize with intracellular Mtb, i.e. the Ifnb promoter activity was triggered by inflammatory stimuli, but not by the direct recognition of intracellular bacteria. We validated the IFNb reporter findings using in situ hybridization with the Ifnb probe, as well as anti-GFP antibody staining (Suppl.Fig.8B - E)." The colocalization is not present within the tissue sections. It seems that the reporter line does not show the same staining pattern in vivo as the IFNß probe or the anti GFP antibody staining. The reporter line has to be tested for the specificity of the staining. Furthermore, to state that it was restricted to the lesions, an uninvolved tissue area needs to be depicted.

      The Ifnb secreting cells are notoriously difficult to detect in vivo using direct staining of the protein. Therefore, lineage tracing of reporter expression are used as surrogates. The Ifnb reporter used in our study has been developed by the Locksley laboratory (Scheu et al., PNAS, 2008, PMID: 19088190) and has been validated in many independent studies. The reporter mice express the YFP protein under the control of the Ifnb1 promoter. The YFP protein accumulates within the cells, while Ifnb protein is rapidly secreted and does not accumulate in the producing cells in appreciable amounts. Also, the kinetics of YFP protein degradation is much slower as compared to the endogenous Ifnb1 mRNA that was detected using in situ hybridization. Thus, there is no precise spatiotemporal coincidence of these readouts in Ifnb expressing cells in vivo. However, this methodology more closely reflect the Ifnb expressing cells in vivo, as compared to a Cre-lox mediated lineage tracing approach. In the revised manuscript we demonstrate that both YFP and mRNA signals partially overlap (Suppl.Fig.12B). In Suppl.Fig.12B. we also included a new panel showing no YFP expression in the uninvolved area of the reporter mice infected with Mtb. The YFP expression by activated macrophages is demonstrated by co-staining with Iba1- and iNOS-specific antibodies (new Fig.7D and Suppl.Fig.13A). Our specificity control also included TB lesions in mice that do not carry the YFP reporter and did not express the YFP signal, as reported elsewhere (Yabaji et al., BioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.10.17.562695).

      - Are paucibacillary and multibacillary lesions different within the same animal or does one animal have one lesion phenotype? If that is the case, what is causing the differences between mice? Bacterial counts for the mice are required.

      The heterogeneity of pulmonary TB lesions has been widely acknowledged in clinic and highlighted in recent experimental studies. In our model of chronic pulmonary TB (described in detail in Yabaji et al., https://doi.org/10.1101/2025.02.28.640830 and https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.10.17.562695) the development of pulmonary TB lesions is not synchronized, i.e. the lesions are heterogeneous between the animals and within individual animals at the same timepoint. Therefore, we performed a lesion stratification where individual lesions were classified by a certified veterinary pathologist in a blinded manner based on their morphology (H&E) and acid fast staining of the bacteria, as depicted in Suppl.Fig.8.

      - "Among the IFN-inducible genes upregulated in paucibacillary lesions were Ifi44l, a recently described negative regulator of IFN-I that enhances control of Mtb in human macrophages (DeDiego et al, 2019; Jiang et al, 2021) and Ciita, a regulator of MHC class II inducible by IFNy, but not IFN-I (Suppl.Table 8 and Suppl.Fig.10 D-E)." Why is Sup. Fig. 10 D, E referred to? The figure legend is also not clear, e.g. what means "upregulated in a subset of IFN-inducible genes"? Input for the hallmarks needs to be defined.

      These data is now presented in Suppl.Fig.11 and following the reviewer’s comment, we moved reference to panels 11D – E up to previous paragraph in the main text, where it naturally belongs . We also edited the figure legend to refer to the list of IFN-inducible genes compiled from the literature that is discussed in the text. We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion that helped us improve the text clarity. The inputs for the Hallmark pathway analysis are presented in Suppl.Tables 7 and 8, as described in the text.

      - Fig. 7C: Single channel pictures are required as it is hard to see the differences in staining with so many markers. Why is there no iNOS expression in the bottom row? What does the rectangle indicate on the bottom right? As black is chosen for DAPI, it is not visible at all. In case the signal is needed a visible a color should be chosen.

      We thoroughly revised this figure to address the reviewer’s concern about the lack of clarity. We provide individual channels for each marker in Fig.7D – E and Suppl.Fig.13F. We have to use DAPI in these presentation in gray scale to better visualize other markers.

      - "In the advanced lesions these markers were primarily expressed by activated macrophages (Iba1+) expressing iNOS and/or Ifny (YFP+)(Fig.7D)" Iba1 is needed in the quantification. Based on the images, iNOS seems to be highly produced in Iba1 negative cells. Which cells do produce it then? Flow cytometry data for this quantification are required. This would allow you to specifically check which cells express the markers and allow for a more precise analysis of double positive cells.

      Currently these data demonstrating the co-localization of stress markers phospho-c-Jun and Chac1 with YFP are presented in Fig.7E (images) and Suppl.Fig.13D (quantification). The co-localization of stress markers phospho-cJun and Chac1 with iNOS is presented in Suppl.Fig.13F (images) and Suppl.Fig.13E (quantification). We agree that some iNOS+ cells are Iba1-negative (Fig.7D). We manually quantified percentages of Iba1+iNOS+ double positive cells and demonstrated that they represent the majority of the iNOS+ population(Suppl.Fig.13A). Regarding the required FACS analysis, we focus on spatial approaches because of the heterogeneity of the lesions that would be lost if lungs are dissociated for FACS. We are working on spatial transcriptomics at a single cell resolution that preserves spatial organization of TB lesions to address the reviewer’s comment and will present our results in the future.

      - Results part 6: In general, can you please state for each experiment at what time point mice were analyzed? You should include an additional macrophage staining (e.g. MerTK, F4/80), as alveolar macrophages are not staining well for Iba1 and you might therefore miss them in your IF microscopy. It would be very nice if you could perform flow cytometry to really check on the macrophages during infection and distinguish subsets (e.g. alveolar macrophages, interstitial macrophages, monocytes).

      We have included the details of time post infection in figure legends for Fig.7, Suppl.Figures 8, 9, 12B, 13, 14A of the revised manuscript. We have performed staining with CD11b, CD206 and CD163 to differentiate the recruited and lung resident macrophages and determined that in chronic pulmonary TB lesions in our model the vast majority of macrophages are recruited CD11b+, but not resident (CD206+ and CD163+) macrophages. These data is presented in another manuscript (Yabaji et al., BioRxiv https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.10.17.562695).

      - Spatial sequencing: The manuscript would highly profit from more data on that. It would be very interesting to check for the DEGs and show differential spatial distribution. Expression of marker genes should be inferred to further define macrophage subsets (e.g. alveolar macrophages, interstitial macrophages, recruited macrophages) and see if these subsets behave differently within the same lesion but also between the lesions. Additional bioinformatic approaches might allow you to investigate cell-cell interactions. There is a lot of potential with such a dataset, especially from TB lesions, that would elevate your findings and prove interesting to the TB field.

      - "Thus, progression from the Mtb-controlling paucibacillary to non-controlling multibacillary TB lesions in the lungs of TB susceptible mice was mechanistically linked with a pathological state of macrophage activation characterized by escalating stress (as evidenced by the upregulation phospho-cJUN, PKR and Chac1), the upregulation of IFNβ and the IFN-I pathway hyperactivity, with a concurrent reduction of IFNγ responses." To really show the upregulation within macrophages and their activation, a more detailed IF microscopy with the inclusion of additional macrophage markers needs to be provided. Flow cytometry would enable analysis for the differences between alveolar and interstitial macrophages, as well as for monocytes. As however, it seems that the majority of iNOS, as well as the stress associated markers are not produced by Iba1+ cells. Analyzing granulocytes and T lymphocytes should be considered.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion. Indeed, our model provides an excellent opportunity to investigate macrophage heterogeneity and cell interactions within chronic TB lesions. We are working on spatial transcriptomics at a single cell resolution that would address the reviewer’s comment and will present our results in the future.

      In agreement with classical literature the overwhelming majority of myeloid cells in chronic pulmonary TB lesions is represented by macrophages. Neutrophils are detected at the necrotic stage, but our study is focused on pre-necrotic stages to reveal the earlier mechanisms predisposing to the necrotization. We never observed neutrophils or T cells expressing iNOS in our studies.

      - It's mentioned in the method section that controls in the IF staining were only fixed for 10min, while the infected cells were fixed for 30min. Consistency is important as the PFA fixation might impact on the fluorescence signal. Therefore, controls should be repeated with the same fixation time.

      We have carefully considered the impact of fixation time on fluorescence and have separately analyzed the non-infected and infected samples to address this concern. For the non-infected samples, we examined the effect of TNF in both B6 and B6.Sst1S backgrounds, ensuring that a consistent fixation protocol (10 min) was applied across all experiments without Mtb infection.

      For the Mtb infection experiments, we employed an optimized fixation protocol (30 min) to ensure that Mtb was killed before handling the plates, which is critical for preserving the integrity of the samples. In this context, we compared B6 and B6.Sst1S samples to evaluate the effects of fixation and Mtb infection on lipid peroxidation (LPO) induction.

      We believe this approach balances the need for experimental consistency with the specific requirements for handling infected cells, and we have revised the manuscript to reflect this clarification.

      - Reactive oxygen species levels should be determined in B6 and B6.Sst1S BMDMs (stimulated and unstimulated), as they are very important for oxidative stress.

      We have conducted experiments to measure ROS production in both B6 and B6.Sst1S BMDMs and demonstrated higher levels of ROS in the susceptible BMDMs after prolonged TNF stimulation (new Fig.3I-J and Suppl. Fig. 3G). Additionally, we have previously published a comparison of ROS production between B6 and B6.Sst1S by FACS (PMID: 33301427), which also supports the findings presented here.

      - Sup. Fig 2C: The inclusion of an unstimulated control would be advisable in order to evaluate if there are already difference in the beginning.

      We have included the untreated control to the Suppl. Fig. 2C (currently Suppl. Fig. 2D) in the revised manuscript.

      - Sup. Fig. 3F: Why is the fold change now lower than in Fig. 4D (fold change of around 28 compared to 120 in 4D)?

      The data in Fig.4D (Fig.4E in the revised manuscript) and Suppl.Fig.3F (currently Suppl.Fig.4C) represent separate experiments and this variation between experiments is commonly observed in qRT-PCR that is affected by slight variations in the expression in unsimulated controls used for the normalization and the kinetics of the response. This 2-4 fold difference between same treatments in separate experiments, as compared to 30 – 100 fold and higher induction by TNF does not affect the data interpretation.

      - Sup. Fig. 5C, D: The data seems very interesting as you even observe an increase in gene expression. Data for the B6 mice should be evaluated for increase to a similar level as the TNF treated mutants. Data on the viability of the cells are necessary, as they no longer receive MCSF and might be dying at this point already.

      To ensure that the observed effects were not confounded by cytotoxicity, we determined non-toxic concentrations of the CSF1R inhibitors during 48h of incubation and used them in our experiments that lasted for 24h. To address this valid comment, we have included cell viability data in the revised manuscript to confirm that the treatments did not result in cell death (Suppl. Fig. 6D). This experiment rejected our hypothesis that CSF1 driven Myc expression could be involved in the Ifnb superinduction. Other effects of CSF1R inhibitors on type I IFN pathway are intriguing but are beyond the scope of this study.

      - Sup. Fig 12: the phospho-c-Jun picture for (P) is not the same as in the merged one with Iba1. Double positive cells are mentioned to be analyzed, but from the staining it appears that P-c-Jun is expressed by other cells. You do not indicate how many replicates were counted and if the P and M lesions were evaluated within the same animal. What does the error bar indicate? It seems unlikely from the plots that the double positive cells are significant. Please provide the p values and statistical analysis.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing this inadvertent field replacement in the single phospho-cJun channel to our attention. However, the quantification of Iba1+phospho-cJun+ double positive cells in Suppl.Fig.12 and our conclusions were not affected. In the revised manuscript, images and quantification of phospho-cJun and Iba1 co-expression are shown in new Suppl.Fig.13B and C, respectively. We have also updated the figure legends to denote the number of lesions analyzed and statistical tests. Specifically, lesions from 6–8 mice per group (paucibacillary and multibacillary) were evaluated. Each dot in panels Suppl.Fig.13 represent individual lesions.

      - Sup. Fig. 13D (suppl.Fig.15D now): What about the expression of MYC itself? Other parts of the signaling pathway should be analyzed(e.g. IFNb, JNK)?

      The difference in MYC mRNA expression tended to be higher in TB patients with poor outcomes, but it was not statistically significant after correction for multiple testing. The upregulation of Myc pathway in the blood transcriptome associated with TB treatment failure most likely reflects greater proportion of immature cells in peripheral blood, possibly due to increased myelopoiesis. Pathway analysis of the differentially expressed genes revealed that treatment failures were associated with the following pathways relevant to this study: NF-kB Signaling, Flt3 Signaling in Hematopoietic Progenitor Cells (indicative of common myeloid progenitor cell proliferation), SAPK/JNK Signaling and Senescence (possibly indicative of oxidative stress). The upregulation of these pathways in human patients with poor TB treatment outcomes correlates with our findings in TB susceptible mice.

      - In the mfIHC you he usage of anti-mouse antibodies is mentioned. Pictures of sections incubated with the secondary antibody alone are required to exclude the possibility that the staining is not specific. Especially, as this data is essential to the manuscript and mouse-antimouse antibodies are notorious for background noise.

      We are well aware of the technical difficulties associated with using mouse on mouse staining. In those cases, we use rabbit anti-mouse isotype specific antibodies specifically developed to avoid non-specific background (Abcam cat#ab133469). Each antibody panel for fluorescent multiplexed IHC is carefully optimized prior to studies. We did not use any primary mouse antibodies in the final version of the manuscript and, hence, removed this mention from the Methods.

      - In order to tie the story together, it would be interesting to treat infected mice with an INFAR antibody, as well as perform this experiment with a Myc antibody. According to your data, you might expect the survival of the mice to be increased or bacterial loads to be affected.

      In collaboration with the Vance laboratory, we tested effects of type I IFN pathway inhibition in B6.Sst1S mice on TB susceptibility: either type I receptor knockout or blocking antibodies increased their resistance to virulent Mtb (published in Ji et al., 2019; PMID 31611644). Unfortunately, blocking Myc using neutralizing antibodies in vivo is not currently achievable. Specifically blocking Myc using small molecule inhibitors in vivo is notoriously difficult, as recognized in oncology literature. We consider using small molecule inhibitors of either Myc translation or specific pathways downstream of Myc in the future.

      - It is surprising that you not even once cite or mention your previous study on bioRxiv considering the similarity of the results and topic (https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.12.14.422743). Is not even your Figure 1I and Figure 2 J, K the same as in that study depicted in Figure 4?

      The reviewer refers to the first version of this manuscript uploaded to BioRxiv, but it has never been published. We continued this work and greatly expanded our original observations, as presented in the current manuscript. Therefore, we do not consider the previous version as an independent manuscript and, therefore, do not cite it.

      - Please revise spelling of the manuscript and pay attention to write gene names in italics

      Thank you, we corrected the gene and protein names according to current nomenclature.

      Minor points:

      - Fig. 1: Please provide some DEGs that explain why you used this resolution for the clustering of the scRNAseq data and that these clusters are truly distinct from each other.

      Differential gene expression in clusters is presented in Suppl.Fig.1C (interferon response) and Suppl.Fig.1D (stress markers and interferon response previously established in our studies).

      - Fig. 1F: What do the two lines represent (magenta, green)?

      The lines indicate pseudotime trajectories of B6 (magenta) and B6.Sst1S (green) BMDMs.

      - Fig. 1F, G: Why was cluster 6 excluded?

      This cluster was not different between B6 and B6.Sst1S, so it was not useful for drawing the strain-specific trajectories.

      - Fig. 1E, G, H: The intensity scales are missing. They are vital to understand the data.

      We have included the scale in revised manuscript (Fig.1E,G,H and Suppl.Fig.1C-D).

      - Fig. 2G-I: please revise order, as you first refer to Fig. 2H and I

      We revised the panels’ order accordingly

      - Fig. 5: You say the data represents three samples but at least in D and E you have more. Please revise. Why do you only include at (G) the inhibitor only control?

      We added the inhibitor only controls to Fig. 5D - H. We also indicated the number of replicates in the updated Fig.5 legend.

      - Figure 7A, Sup. Fig. 8: Are these maximum intensity projection? Or is one z-level from the 3D stack depicted?

      The Fig. 7A shows 3D images with all the stacks combined.

      - Fig. 7B: What do the white boxes indicate?

      We have removed this panel in the revised version and replaced it with better images.

      - Sup. Fig. 1A: The legend for the staining is missing

      The Suppl. Fig.1A shows the relative proportions of either naïve (R and S) or TNFstimulated (RT and ST) B6 or B6.Sst1S macrophages within individual single cell clusters depicted in Fig.1B. The color code is shown next to the graph on the right.

      - Sup. Fig. 1B: The feature plots are not clear: The legend for the expression levels is missing. What does the heading means?

      We updated the headings, as in Fig.1C. The dots represent individual cells expressing Sp110 mRNA (upper panels) and Sp140 mRNA (lower panels).

      - Sup. Fig. 3C: The scale bar is barely visible.

      We resized the scale bar to make it visible and presented in Suppl. Fig.3E (previously Suppl. Fig.3C).

      - Sup. Fig. 3D: There is not figure legend or the legend to C-E is wrong.

      - Sup. Fig. 3F, G: You do not state to what the data is relative to.

      We identified an error in the Suppl.Fig.3 legend referring to specific panels. The Suppl.Fig.3 legend has been updated accordingly. New panels were added and Suppl.Fig.3-G panels are now Suppl.Fig.4C-D.

      - Sup. Fig. 3H: It seems you used a two-way ANOVA, yet state it differently. Please revise the figure legend, as Dunnett's multiple comparison would only check for significances compared to the control.

      Following the reviewer’s comment, we repeated statistical analysis to include correction for multiple comparisons and revised the figure and legend accordingly.

      - Sup. Fig. 4A, B: It is not clear what the lines depict as the legend is not explained. Names that are not required should be changed to make it clear what is depicted (e.g. "TE@" what does this refer to?)

      This previous Sup. Fig 4 is now Sup. Fig. 5. The “TE@” is a leftover label from the bioinformatics pipeline, referring to “Transposable Element”. We apologize for this confusion and have removed these extraneous labels. We have also added transposon names of the LTR (MMLV30 and RTLV4) and L1Md to Suppl.Fig.5A and 5B legend, respectively.

      - Sup. 4B: What does the y-scale on the right refer to?

      We apologize for the missing label for the y-scale on the right which represents the mRNA expression level for the SetDB1 gene, which has a much lower steady state level than the LINE L1Md, so we plotted two Y-scales to allow both the gene and transposon to be visualized on this graph.

      - Sup. 4C: Interpretation of the data is highly hindered by the fact that the scales differ between the B6 and B6.Sst1. The scales are barely visible.

      We apologize for the missing labels for the y-scales of these coverage plots, which were originally meant to just show a qualitative picture of the small RNA sequencing that was already quantitated by the total amounts in Sup. 4B. We have added thee auto-scaled Y-scales to Sup. 4C and improved the presentation of this figure.

      - Sup. Fig. 5A, B: Is the legend correct? Did you add the antibody for 2 days or is the quantification from day 3?

      We recognize that the reviewer refers to Suppl.Fig.6A-B (Suppl.Fig.7A-B in the revised manuscript). We did not add antibodies to live cells. The figure legend describes staining with 4HNE-specific antibodies 3 days post Mtb infection.

      - Sup. Fig. 8A: Are the "early" and "intermediate" lesions from the same time points? What are the definitions for these stages?

      We discussed our lesion classification according to histopathology and bacterial loads above. Of note, in the revised manuscript we simplified our classification to denote paucibacillary and multibacillary lesions only. We agree with reviewers that designation lesions as early, intermediate and advanced lesions were based on our assumptions regarding the time course of their progression from low to high bacterial loads.

      - Sup. Fig. 8E: You should state that the bottom picture is an enlargement of an area in the top one. Scale bars are missing.

      We replaced this panel with clearer images in Suppl.Fig.12B.

      - Sup. Fig. 11A: The IF staining is only visible for Iba and iNOS. Please provide single channels in order to make the other staining visible.

      Suppl.Fig.11A (now Suppl.Fig.13B) shows the low-magnification images of TB lesions. In the Fig. 7 and Suppl. Fig. 13F of the revised manuscript we provided images for individual markers.

      - Sup. Fig. 13A (Suppl.Fig.15A now): Your axis label is not clear. What do the numbers behind the genes indicate? Why did you choose oncogene signatures and not inflammatory markers to check for a correlation with disease outcome?

      X axis of Suppl.Fig.15A represent pre-defined molecular signature gene sets MSigDB) in Gene Set Enrichment Analysis (GSEA) database (https://www.gseamsigdb.org/gsea/msigdb). On Y axis is area under curve (AUC) score for each gene set.

      - Sup. 13D(Suppl.Fig.15D now): Maybe you could reorder the patients, so that the impression is clearer, as right now only the top genes seem to show a diverging gene signature, while the rest gives the impression of an equal distribution.

      The Myc upregulated gene set myc_up was identified among top gene sets associated with treatment failure using unbiased ssGSEA algorithm. We agree with the reviewer that not every gene in the myc_up gene set correlates with the treatment outcome. But the association of the gene set is statistically significant, as presented in Suppl.Fig.15B – C.

      - The scale bars for many microscopy pictures are missing.

      We have included clearly visible scale bars to all the microscopy images in the revised version.

      - The black bar plots should be changed (e.g. in color), since the single data points cannot be seen otherwise.

      - It would be advisable that a consistent color scheme would be used throughout the manuscript to make it easier to identify similar conditions, as otherwise many different colours are not required and lead right now rather to confusion (e.g. sometimes a black bar refers to BMDMs with and sometimes without TNF stimulation, or B6 BMDMs). Furthermore, plot sizes and fonts should be consistent within the manuscript (including the supplemental data)

      We followed this useful suggestion and selected consistent color codes for B6 and B6.Sst1S groups to enhance clarity throughout the revised manuscript.

      Within the methods section:

      - At which concentration did you use the IFNAR antibody and the isotype?

      We updated method section by including respective concentrations in the revised manuscript.

      - Were mice maintained under SPF conditions? At what age where they used?

      Yes, the mice are specific pathogen free. We used 10 - 14 week old mice for Mtb infection.

      - The BMDM cultivation is not clear. According to your cited paper you use LCCM but can you provide how much M-CSF it contains? How do you make sure that amounts are the same between experiments and do not vary? You do not mention how you actually obtain this conditioned medium. Is there the possibility of contamination or transferred fibroblasts that would impact on the data analysis? Is LCCM also added during stimulation and inhibitor treatment?

      We obtain LCCM by collecting the supernatant from L929 cell line that form confluent monolayer according to well-established protocols for LCCM collection. The supernatants are filtered through 0.22 micron filters to exclude contamination with L929 cells and bacteria. The medium is prepared in 500 ml batches that are sufficient for multiples experiments. Each batch of L929-conditioned medium is tested for biological activity using serial dilutions.

      - How was the BCG infection performed? How much bacteria did you use? Which BCG strain was used?

      We infected mice with M. bovis BCG Pasteur subcutaneously in the hock using 10<sup>6</sup> CFU per mouse.

      - At what density did you seed the BMDMs for stimulation and inhibitor experiments?

      In 96 well plates, we seed 12,000 cells per well and allow the cells to grow for 4 days to reach confluency (approximately 50,000 cells per well). For a 6-well plate, we seed 2.5 × 10<sup>5</sup> cells per well and culture them for 4 days to reach confluency. For a 24-well plate, we seed 50,000 cells per well and keep the cells in media for 4 days before starting any treatments. This ensures that the cells are in a proliferative or near-confluent state before beginning the stimulation or inhibitor treatments. Our detailed protocol is published in STAR Protocols (Yabaji et al., 2022; PMID 35310069).

      - What machine did you use to perform the bulk RNA sequencing? How many replicates did you include for the sequencing?

      For bulk sequencing we used 3 RNA samples for each condition. The samples were sequenced at Boston University Microarray & Sequencing Resource service using Illumina NextSeq<sup>TM</sup> 2000 instrument.

      - How many replicates were used for the scRNA sequencing? Why is your threshold for the exclusion of mitochondrial DNA so high? A typical threshold of less than 5% has been reported to work well with mouse tissue.

      We used one sample per condition. For the mitochondrial cutoff, we usually base it off of the total distribution. There is no "universal" threshold that can be applied to all datasets. Thresholds must be determined empirically.

      - You do not mention how many PCAs were considered for the scRNA sequencing analysis.

      We considered 50 PCAs, this information was added to Methods

      - You should name all the package versions you used for the scRNA sequencing (e.g. for the slingshot, VAM package)

      The following package versions were used: Seurat v4.0.4, VAM v1.0.0, Slingshot v2.3.0, SingleCellTK v2.4.1, Celda v1.10.0, we added this information to Methods.

      - You mention two batches for the human samples. Can you specify what the two batches are?

      Human blood samples were collected at five sites, as described in the updated Methods section and two RNAseq batches were processed separately that required batch correction.

      - At which temperature was the IF staining performed?

      We performed the IF at 4oC. We included the details in revised version.

      Reviewer #2 (Significance):

      Overall, the manuscript has interesting findings with regard to macrophage responses in Mycobacteria tuberculosis infection.

      However, in its current form there are several shortcomings, both with respect to the precision of the experiments and conclusions drawn.

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      Summary

      The authors use a mouse model designed to be more susceptible to M.tb (addition of sst1 locus) which has granulomatous lesions more similar to human granulomas, making this mouse highly relevant for M.tb pathogenesis studies. Using WT B6 macrophages or sst1B6 macrophages, the authors seek to understand the how the sst1 locus affects macrophage response to prolonged TNFa exposure, which can occur during a pro-inflammatory response in the lungs. Using single cell RNA-seq, revealed clusters of mutant macrophages with upregulated genes associated with oxidative stress responses and IFN-I signaling pathways when treated with TNF compared to WT macs. The authors go on to show that mutant macrophages have decreased NRF2, decreased antioxidant defense genes and less Sp110 and Sp140. Mutant macrophages are also more susceptible to lipid peroxidation and ironmediated oxidative stress. The IFN-I pathway hyperactivity is caused by the dysregulation of iron storage and antioxidant defense. These mutant macrophages are more susceptible to M.tb infection, showing they are less able to control bacterial growth even in the presence of T cells from BCG vaccinated mice. The transcription factor Myc is more highly expressed in mutant macs during TNF treatment and inhibition Myc led to better control of M.tb growth. Myc is also more abundant in PBMCs from M.tb infected humans with poor outcomes, suggesting that Myc should be further investigated as a target for host-directed therapies for tuberculosis.

      Major Comments

      Isotypes for IF imaging and confocal IF imaging are not listed, or not performed. It is a concern that the microscopy images throughout the manuscript do not have isotype controls for the primary antibodies.

      Fig 4 (and later) the anti-IFNAR Ab is used along with the Isotype antibody, Fig 4I does not show the isotype. Use of the isotype antibody is also missing in later figures as well as Fig 3J. Why was this left off as the proper control for the Ab?

      We addressed the comment in revised manuscript as described above in summary and responses to reviewers 1 and 2. Isotype controls for IFNAR1 blockade were included in Fig.3M (previously 3J), Fig. 4I, Suppl.Fig.4G (previously Fig.4I), and updated Fig.4C-E, Fig.6L-M, Suppl.Fig.4F-G, 7I.

      Conclusions drawn by the authors from some of the WB data are worded strongly, yet by eye the blots don't look as dramatically different as suggested. It would be very helpful to quantify the density of bands when making conclusions. (for example, Fig 4A).

      We added the densitometry of Western blot values after normalization above each lane in Fig.2A-C, Fig.3C-D and 3K; Fig.4A-B, Fig.5B,C,I,J.

      Fig 5A is not described clearly. If the gene expression is normalized to untreated B6 macs, then the level of untreated B6 macs should be 1. In the graph the blue bars are slightly below 1, which would not suggest that levels "initially increased and subsequently downregulated" as stated in the text. It seems like the text describes the protein expression but not the RNA expression. Please check this section and more clearly describe the results.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment and modified the text to specify the mRNA and protein expression data, as follows:

      “We observed that Myc was regulated in an sst1-dependent manner: in TNF-stimulated B6 wild type BMDMs, c-Myc mRNA was downregulated, while in the susceptible macrophages c-Myc mRNA was upregulated (Fig.5A). The c-Myc protein levels were also higher in the B6.Sst1S cells in unstimulated BMDMs and 6 – 12 h of TNF stimulation (Fig.5B)”.

      Also, why look at RNA through 24h but protein only through 12h? If c-myc transcripts continue to increase through 24h, it would be interesting to see if protein levels also increase at this later time point.

      The time-course of Myc expression up to 24 h is presented in new panels Fig. 5I-5J It demonstrates the decrease of Myc protein levels at 24 h. In the wild type B6 BMDMs the levels of Myc protein significantly decreased in parallel with the mRNA suppression presented in Fig.5A. In contrast , we observed the dissociation of the mRNA and protein levels in the _sst1_mutant BMDMs at 12 and 24 h, most likely, because the mutant macrophages develop integrated stress response (as shown in our previous publication by Bhattacharya et al., JCI, 2021) that is known to inhibit Myc mRNA translation.

      Fig 5J the bands look smaller after D-JNK1 treatment at 6 and 12h though in the text is says no change. Quantifying the bands here would be helpful to see if there really is no difference.

      This experiment was repeated twice, and the average normalized densitometry values are presented in the updated Fig.5J. The main question addressed in this experiment was whether the hyperactivity of JNK in TNF-stimulated sst1 mutant macrophages contributed to Myc upregulation, as was previously shown in cancer. Comparing effects of JNK inhibition on phospho-cJun and c-Myc protein levels in TNF stimulated B6.Sst1S macrophages (updated Fig.5J), we concluded that JNK did not have a major role in c-Myc upregulation in this context.

      Section 4, third paragraph, the conclusion that JNK activation in mutant macs drives pathways downstream of Myc are not supported here. Are there data or other literature from the lab that supports this claim?

      This statement was based on evidence from available literature where JNK was shown to activate oncogens, including Myc. In addition, inhibition of Myc in our model upregulated ferritin (Fig.Fig.5C), reduced the labile iron pool, prevented the LPO accumulation (Fig.5D - G) and inhibited stress markers (Fig.5H). However, we do not have direct experimental evidence in our model that Myc inhibition reduces ASK1 and JNK activities. Hence, we removed this statement from the text and plan to investigate this in the future.

      Fig 6N Please provide further rationale for the BCG in vivo experiment. It is unclear what the hypothesis was for this experiment.

      In the current version BCG vaccination data is presented in Suppl.Fig.14B. We demonstrate that stressed BMDMs do not respond to activation by BCG-specific T cells (Fig.6J) and their unresponsiveness is mediated by type I interferon (Fig.6L and 6M). The observed accumulation of the stressed macrophages in pulmonary TB lesions of the sst1-susceptible mice (Fig.7E, Suppl.Fig.13 and 14A) and the upregulation of type I interferon pathway (Fig.1E,1G, 7C), Suppl.Fig.1C and 11) suggested that the effect of further boosting T lymphocytes using BCG in Mtb-infected mice will be neutralized due to the macrophage unresponsiveness. This experiment provides a novel insight explaining why BCG vaccine may not be efficient against pulmonary TB in susceptible hosts.

      The in vitro work is all concerning treatment with TNFa and how this exposure modifies the responses in B6 vs sst1B6 macrophages; however, this is not explored in the in vivo studies. Are there differences in TNFa levels in the pauci- vs multi-bacillary lesions that lead to (or correlate with) the accumulation of peroxidation products in the intralesional macrophages. How to the experiments with TNFa in vitro relate back to how the macrophages are responding in vivo during infection?

      Our investigation of mechanisms of necrosis of TB granulomas stems from and supported by in vivo studies as summarized below.

      This work started with the characterization necrotic TB granulomas in C3HeB/FeJ mice in vivo followed by a classical forward genetic analysis of susceptibility to virulent Mtb in vivo.

      That led to the discovery of the sst1 locus and demonstration that it plays a dominant role in the formation of necrotic TB granulomas in mouse lungs in vivo. Using genetic and immunological approaches we demonstrated that the sst1 susceptibility allele controls macrophage function in vivo (Yan, et al., J.Immunol. 2007) and an aberrant macrophage activation by TNF and increased production of Ifn-b in vitro (He et al. Plos Pathogens, 2013). In collaboration with the Vance lab we demonstrated that the type I IFN receptor inactivation reduced the susceptibility to intracellular bacteria of the sst1-susceptible mice in vivo (Ji et al., Nature Microbiology, 2019). Next, we demonstrated that the Ifnb1 mRNA superinduction results from combined effects of TNF and JNK leading to integrated stress response in vitro (Bhattacharya, JCI, 2021). Thus, our previous work started with extensive characterization of the in vivo phenotype that led to the identification of the underlying macrophage deficiency that allowed for the detailed characterization of the macrophage phenotype in vitro presented in this manuscript. In a separate study, the Sher lab confirmed our conclusions and their in vivo relevance using Bach1 knockout in the sst1-susceptible B6.Sst1S background, where boosting antioxidant defense by Bach1 inactivation resulted in decreased type I interferon pathway activity and reduced granuloma necrosis. We have chosen TNF stimulation for our in vitro studies because this cytokine is most relevant for the formation and maintenance of the integrity of TB granulomas in vivo as shown in mice, non-human primates and humans. Here we demonstrate that although TNF is necessary for host resistance to virulent Mtb, its activity is insufficient for full protection of the susceptible hosts, because of altered macrophages responsiveness to TNF. Thus, our exploration of the necrosis of TB granulomas encompass both in vitro and extensive in vivo studies.

      Minor comments

      Introduction, while well written, is longer than necessary. Consider shortening this section. Throughout figures, many graphs show a fold induction/accumulation/etc, but it is rarely specified what the internal control is for each graph. This needs to be added.

      Paragraph one, authors use the phrase "the entire IFN pathway was dramatically upregulated..." seems to be an exaggeration. How do you know the "entire" IFN pathway was upregulated in a dramatic fashion?

      (1) We shortened the introduction and discussion; (2) verified that figure legends internal controls that were used to calculate fold induction; (3) removed the word “entire” to avoid overinterpretation.

      Figures 1E, G and H and supp fig 1C, the heat maps are missing an expression key Section 2 second paragraph refers to figs 2D, E as cytoplasmic in the text, but figure legend and y-axis of 2E show total protein.

      The expression keys were added to Fig.1E,G,H, Fig.7C, Suppl.Fig.1C and 1D and Suppl.Fig.11A of the revised manuscript.

      Section 3 end of paragraph 1 refers to Fig 3h. Does this also refer to Supp Fig 3E?

      Yes, Fig.3H shows microscopy of 4-HNE and Suppl.Fig.3H shows quantification of the image analysis. In the revised manuscript these data are presented in Fig.3H and Suppl.Fig.3F. The text was modified to reflect this change.

      Supplemental Fig 3 legend for C-E seems to incorrectly also reference F and G.

      We corrected this error in the figure legend. New panels were added to Suppl.Fig.3 and previous Suppl.Fig.3F and G were moved to Suppl.Fig.4 panels C and D of the revise version.

      Fig 3K, the p-cJun was inhibited with the JNK inhibitor, however it’s unclear why this was done or the conclusion drawn from this experiment. Use of the JNK inhibitor is not discussed in the text.

      The JNK inhibitor was used to confirm that c-Jun phosphorylation in our studies is mediated by JNK and to compare effects of JNK inhibition on phospho-cJun and Myc expression. This experiment demonstrated that the JNK inhibitor effectively inhibited c-Jun phosphorylation but not Myc upregulation, as shown in Fig.5I-J of the revised manuscript.

      Fig 4 I and Supp Fig 3 H seem to have been swapped? The graph in Fig 4I matches the images in Supp Fig 3I. Please check.

      We reorganized the panels to provide microscopy images and corresponding quantification together in the revised the panels Fig. 4H and Fig. 4I, as well as in Suppl. Fig. 4F and Suppl. Fig. 4G.

      Fig 6, it is unclear what % cell number means. Also for bacterial growth, the data are fold change compared to what internal control?

      We updated Fig.6 legend to indicate that the cell number percentages were calculated based on the number of cells at Day 0 (immediately after Mtb infection). We routinely use fixable cell death staining to enumerate cell death. Brief protocol containing this information is included in Methods section. The detailed protocol including normalization using BCG spike has been published – Yabaji et al, STAR Protocols, 2022. Here we did not present dead cell percentage as it remained low and we did not observe damage to macrophage monolayers. This allows us to exclude artifacts due to cell loss. The fold change of Mtb was calculated after normalization using Mtb load at Day 0 after infection and washes.

      Fig 7B needs an expression key

      The expression keys was added to Fig.7C (previously Fig. 7B).

      Supp Fig 7 and Supp Fig 8A, what do the arrows indicate?

      In Suppl.Fig.8 (previously Suppl.Fig.7) the arrows indicate acid fast bacilli (Mtb). In figures Fig.7A and Suppl.Fig.9A arrows indicate Mtb expressing fluorescent reporter mCherry. Corresponding figure legends were updated in the revised version.

      Supp Fig 9A, two ROI appear to be outlined in white, not just 1 as the legend says Methods:

      We updated the figure legend.

      Certain items are listed in the Reagents section that are not used in the manuscript, such as necrostatin-1 or Z-VAD-FMK. Please carefully check the methods to ensure extra items or missing items does not occur.

      These experiments were performed, but not included in the final manuscript. Hence, we removed the “necrostatin-1 or Z-VAD-FMK” from the reagents section in methods of revised version.

      Western blot, method of visualizing/imaging bands is not provided, method of quantifying density is not provided, though this was done for fig 5C and should be performed for the other WBs.

      We used GE ImageQuant LAS4000 Multi-Mode Imager to acquire the Western blot images and the densitometric analyses were performed by area quantification using ImageJ. We included this information in the method section. We added the densitometry of Western blot values after normalization above each lane in Fig.2A-C, Fig.3C-D and 3K; Fig.4A-B, Fig.5B,C,I,J.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance):

      The work of Yabaji et al is of high significance to the field of macrophage biology and M.tb pathogenesis in macrophages. This work builds from previously published work (Bhattacharya 2021) in which the authors first identified the aberrant response induced by TNF in sst1 mutant macrophages. Better understanding how macrophages with the sst1 locus respond not only to bacterial infection but stimulation with relevant ligands such as TNF will aid the field in identifying biomarkers for TB, biomarkers that can suggest a poor outcome vs. "cure" in response to antibiotic treatment or design of host-directed therapies.

      This work will be of interest to those who study macrophage biology and who study M.tb pathogenesis and tuberculosis in particular. This study expands the knowledge already gained on the sst1 locus to further determine how early macrophage responses are shaped that can ultimately determine disease progression.

      Strengths of the study include the methodologies, employing both bulk and single cell-RNA seq to answer specific questions. Data are analyze using automated methods (such as HALO) to eliminated bias. The experiments are well planned and designed to determine the mechanisms behind the increased iron-related oxidative stress found in the mutant macrophages following TNF treatment. Also, in vivo studies were performed to validate some of the in vitro work. Examining pauci-bacillary lesions vs multi-bacillary lesions and spatial transcriptomics is a significant strength of this work. The inclusion of human data is another strength of the study, showing increased Myc in humans with poor response to antibiotics for TB.

      Limitations include the fact that the work is all done with BMDMs. Use of alveolar macrophages from the mice would be a more relevant cell type for M.tb studies. AMs are less inflammatory, therefore treatment with TNF of AMs could result in different results compared to BMDMs. Reviewer's field of expertise: macrophage activation, M.tb pathogenesis in human and mouse models, cell signaling.

      Limitations: not qualified to evaluate single cell or bulk RNA-seq technical analysis/methodology or spatial transcriptomics analysis.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      This manuscript describes the role of the production of c-di-AMP on the chlamydial developmental cycle. The main findings remain the same. The authors show that overexpression of the dacA-ybbR operon results in increased production of c-di-AMP and early expression of transitionary and late genes. The authors also knocked down the expression of the dacA-ybbR operon and reported a modest reduction in the expression of both hctA and omcB. The authors conclude with a model suggesting the amount of c-di-AMP determines the fate of the RB, continued replication, or EB conversion. 

      Overall, this is a very intriguing study with important implications however the data is very preliminary and the model is very rudimentary. The data support the observation that dramatically increased c-di-AMP has an impact on transitionary gene expression and late gene expression suggesting dysregulation of the developmental cycle. This effect goes away with modest changes in c-di-AMP (detaTM-DacA vs detaTM-DacA (D164N)). However, the model predicts that low levels of c-di-AMP delays EB production is not not well supported by the data. If this prediction were true then the growth rate would increase with c-di-AMP reduction and the data does not show this. The levels of of c-di-AMP at the lower levels need to be better validated as it seems like only very high levels make a difference for dysregulated late gene expression. However, on the low end it's not clear what levels are needed to have an effect as only DacAopMut and DacAopKD show any effects on the cycle and the c-di-AMP levels are only different at 24 hours. 

      These appear to be the same comments the reviewer presented last time, so we will reiterate our prior points here and elsewhere. We do not think and nor do we predict that low c-di-AMP levels should increase growth rate (as measured by gDNA levels), and this conclusion cannot be drawn from our data. Rather, we predict that the inability to accumulate c-di-AMP should delay production of EBs, and this is what the data show. The reviewer has applied their own subjective (and erroneous) interpretation to the model. The asynchronicity of the normal developmental cycle means RBs continue to replicate as EBs are forming, so gDNA levels cannot be used as the sole metric for determining RB levels. We show that reduced c-di-AMP levels reduce EB levels as well as transcripts associated with late stages of development. The parsimonious interpretation of these data support that low c-di-AMP levels delay progression through the developmental cycle consistent with our model.

      The data still do not support the overall model.

      We disagree.  We have presented quantified data that include appropriate controls and statistical tests, and the reviewer has not disputed that or pointed to additional experiments that need to be performed.  The reviewer has imposed a subjective interpretation of our model based on their own biases.  A reader is free, of course, to disagree with our model, but a reviewer should not block a manuscript based on such a disagreement if no experimental flaws have been identified. 

      In Figure 1 the authors show at 24 hpi. 

      We also showed data from 16hpi, which is a more relevant timepoint for assessing premature transition to EBs.  In contrast, the 24hpi is more important for assessing developmental effects of reduced c-di-AMP levels.

      DacA overexpression increases cdiAMP to ~4000 pg/ml 

      DacAmut overexpression reduces cdiAMP dramatically to ~256 pg/ml) 

      DacATM overexpression increases cdiAMP to ~4000 pg/ml. 

      DacAmutTM overexpression does not seem to change cdiAMP ~1500 pg/ml . 

      dacAKD decreases cdiAMP to ~300 pg/ml . 

      dacAKDcom increased cdiAMP to ~8000 pg/ml. 

      DacA-ybbRop overexpression increased cdiAMP to ~500,000 pg/ml. 

      DacA-ybbRopmut ~300 pg/ml. 

      However in Figure 2 the data show that overexpression of DacA (cdiAMP ~4000 pg/ml) did not have a different phenotype than over expression of the mutant (cdiAMP ~256 pg/ml). HctA expression down, omcB expression down, euo not much change, replication down, and IFUs down. Additionally, Figure 3 shows no differences in anything measured although cdiAMP levels were again dramatically different. DacATM overexpression (~4000 pg/ml) and DacAmutTM (~1500). This makes it unclear what cdiAMP is doing to the developmental cycle. 

      As we have explained in the text and in response to reviewer comments on previous rounds of review, overexpressing the full-length WT or mutant DacA is detrimental to developmental cycle progression for reasons that have nothing to do with c-di-AMP levels (likely disrupting membrane function), since, as the reviewer notes, the WT DacA deltaTM strain had similar c-di-AMP levels but no negative effects on growth/development. If we had not presented the effects of overexpressing the individual isoforms, then a reviewer would surely have requested such, which is why we present these data even though they don’t seem to support our model.  This is an honest representation of our findings.  The reviewer seems intent on nitpicking a minor datapoint that seems to contradict the rest of the manuscript while ignoring or not carefully reading the rest of the manuscript.

      In Figure 4 the authors knockdown dacA (dacA-KD) and complement the knockdown (dacA-KDcom) 

      dacAKD decreases cdiAMP (~300) while DacA-KDcom increases cdiAMP much above wt (~8000). 

      KD decreased hctA and omcB at 24hpi. Complementation resulted in a moderate increase in hctA at a single time point but not at 24 hpi and had no effect on euo or omcB expression.

      By 24hpi, late gene transcripts are being maximally produced during a normal developmental cycle. It is unclear why the reviewer thinks that these transcripts should be elevated above this level in any of our strains that prematurely transition to EBs. There is no basis in the literature to support such an assumption. As we noted in the text, the dacA-KDcom strain phenocopied the dacAop OE strain, and we showed RNAseq data and EB production curves for the latter that support our conclusions of the effect of increased c-di-AMP levels on developmental progression.

      Importantly, complementation decreased the growth rate.

      Yes, since the c-di-AMP levels breached the “EB threshold” at 16hpi, it causes premature transition to EBs, which do not replicate their gDNA, at an earlier stage of the cycle when fewer organisms are present. Therefore, the gDNA levels are decreased at 24hpi, which is consistent with our model.

      Based on the proposed model, growth rate should increase as the chlamydia should all be RBs and replicating and not exiting the cell cycle to become EBs (not replicating).

      This is a spurious conclusion from the reviewer. As we clearly showed, the dacA-KDcom did not restore a wild-type phenotype and instead mimicked the dacAop OE strain. This was commented on in the text.

      Interestingly reducing cdiAMP levels by over expressing DacAmut (~256 pg/ml) did not have an effect on the cycle but the reduction in cdiAMP by knockdown of dacA (~300 pg/ml) did have a moderate effect on the cycle. 

      This is again a spurious conclusion from the reviewer. The dacAMut and dacA-KD strains are distinct. As noted in the text and above for DacA WT OE, overexpressing the DacAMut similarly disrupts organism morphology, which is different from dacA-KD. These strains should not be directly compared because of this. This point has been previously highlighted in the text (in Results and Discussion).

      For Figure 5 DacA-ybbRop was overexpressed and this increased cdiAMP dramatically ~500,000 pg/ml as compared to wt ~1500. This increased hctA only at an early timepoint and not at 24hpi and again had no effect on omcB or euo.

      As we explained in prior reviews, our RNAseq data more comprehensively assessed transcripts for the dacAop OE strain. These data show convincingly that late gene transcripts (not just hctA and omcB) are elevated earlier in the developmental cycle. Again, it is not clear why the reviewer should expect that late gene transcripts should be higher in these strains than they are during a normal developmental cycle. This is not part of our model and appears to be a bias that the reviewer has imposed that is not supported by the literature.

      Overexpression of the operon with the mutation DacA-ybbRopmut reduced cdiAMP to ~300 pg/ml and this showed a reduction in growth rate similar to dacAmut but a more dramatic decrease in IFUs. 

      As we described in the text, in earlier revisions, and above, the dacAMut OE strain has distinct effects unrelated to c-di-AMP levels and, therefore, should not be compared to other strains in terms of linking its c-di-AMP levels to its phenotype.

      Overall: 

      DacA overexpression increases cdiAMP to ~4000 pg/ml (decreased everything except euo) 

      DacAmut overexpression reduces cdiAMP dramatically (~256 pg/ml). (decreased everything except euo) 

      DacATM overexpression increases cdiAMP to ~4000 pg/ml (no changes noted) 

      DacAmutTM overexpression does not seem to change cdiAMP ~1500 pg/ml (no changes noted) 

      dacAKD decrease cdiAMP to ~300 pg/ml (decreased everything except euo) 

      dacAKDcom increased cdiAMP to ~8000 pg/ml (decreases growth rate, increase hctA a little but not omcB) 

      DacA-ybbRop overexpression increased cdiAMP to ~500,000 pg/ml (decreases growth rate, increase hctA a little but not omcB) <br /> DacA-ybbRopmut ~300 pg/ml (decreased everything except euo) 

      Overall, the data show that increasing cdiAMP only has a phenotype if it is dramatically increased, no effect at 4000 pg/ml.

      Yes, this clearly shows there is a threshold - as we hypothesize!  However, these thresholds are more important at the 16hpi timepoint not 24hpi (which the reviewer is referencing) when assessing premature transition to EBs.  We specifically highlighted in our prior revision in Figure 1E this EB threshold to make this point clearer for the reader.  Once the threshold is breached, then the overall c-di-AMP levels become irrelevant as the RBs have begun their transition to EBs.

      Decreasing cdiAMP has a consistent effect, decreased growth rate, IFU, hctA expression and omcB expression. However, if their proposed model was correct and low levels of cdiAMP blocked EB conversion then more chlamydial cells would be RBs (dividing cells) and the growth rate should increase.

      The only effect should be normal gDNA levels, which is what we see in the dacA-KD.  Given the asynchronicity of a normal developmental cycle in which RBs continue to replicate as EBs are still forming, there is no basis to assume gDNA levels should increase under these conditions for the dacA-KD strain at 24hpi.

      Conversely, if cdiAMP levels were dramatically raised then all RBs would all convert and the growth rate would be very low.

      We agree. This is what is reflected by the dacAop OE and dacA-KDcom strains, with reduced gDNA levels at 24hpi since organisms have transitioned to EBs at an earlier time post-infection.

      When cdiAMP was raised to ~4000 pg/ml there was no effect on the growth rate.

      Yes, because it had not breached the EB threshold at 16hpi – consistent with our model!  The reviewer is confusing effects of elevated c-di-AMP at 24hpi when they should be assessed at the 16hpi timepoint for strains overproducing this molecule.

      However, an increase to ~8000 pg/ml resulted in a significant decrease but growth continued.

      If the reviewer is referring to the dacA-KDcom strain, then this is not accurate. gDNA levels were decreased in this strain at 24hpi when the c-di-AMP levels were increased compared to the WT (mCherry OE) control at 16hpi, indicating this strain had breached the “EB threshold” and initiated conversion to EBs at an earlier timepoint post-infection when fewer organisms were present.

      Increasing cdAMP to ~500,000 pg/ml had less of an impact on the growth rate.

      It is not clear what this conclusion is based on and what the reviewer is comparing to.  This is a subjective assessment not based on our data.

      Overall, the data does not cleanly support the proposed model.

      It is an unfortunate aspect of biology, particularly for obligate intracellular bacteria – a challenging experimental system on which to work, that the data are not always “clean”.  The overall effects of increased c-di-AMP levels on chlamydial developmental cycle progression we have documented support our model, and we think the reader, as always, should make their own assessment.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      This manuscript describes the role of the production of c-di-AMP on the chlamydial developmental cycle. The main findings remain the same. The authors show that overexpression of the dacA-ybbR operon results in increased production of c-di-AMP and early expression of transitionary and late genes. The authors also knocked down the expression of the dacA-ybbR operon and reported a modest reduction in the expression of both hctA and omcB. The authors conclude with a model suggesting the amount of c-di-AMP determines the fate of the RB, continued replication, or EB conversion. 

      Overall, this is a very intriguing study with important implications however, the data is very preliminary, and the model is very rudimentary. The data support the observation that dramatically increased c-di-AMP has an impact on transitionary gene expression and late gene expression suggesting dysregulation of the developmental cycle. This effect goes away with modest changes in c-di-AMP (detaTM-DacA vs detaTM-DacA (D164N)). However, the model predicts that low levels of c-di-AMP delays EB production is not not well supported by the data. If this prediction were true then the growth rate would increase with c-di-AMP reduction and the data does not show this.

      Thank you for the comments. We have apparently not adequately communicated our predictions and the model. We do not think and nor do we predict that low c-di-AMP levels should increase growth rate, and there is no basis in any of our data to support that. Rather, we predict that the inability to accumulate c-di-AMP should delay production of EBs, and this is what the data show. We have clarified this in the text (line 89 paragraph).

      The levels of c-di-AMP at the lower levels need to be better validated as it seems like only very high levels make a difference for dysregulated late gene expression. However, on the low end it's not clear what levels are needed to have an effect as only DacAopMut and DacAopKD show any effects on the cycle and the c-di-AMP levels are only different at 24 hours.

      Our hypothesis is that increasing concentrations of c-di-AMP within a given RB is a signal for it to undergo secondary differentiation to the EB, and the data support this as noted by the reviewers. Again, we stress that low levels of c-di-AMP are irrelevant to the model. We have revised Figure 1E to indicate the level of c-di-AMP in the control strain at the 24hpi timepoint that coincides with increased EB levels. We hope this will further clarify the goals of our study. That a given strain might be below the EB control is not relevant to the model beyond indicating that it has not reached the necessary threshold for triggering secondary differentiation.

      The authors responded to reviewers' critiques by adding the overexpression of DacA without the transmembrane region. This addition does not really help their case. They show that detaTM-DacA and detaTM-DacA (D164N) had the same effects on c-di-AMP levels but the figure shows no effects on the developmental cycle.

      As it relates directly to the reviewer’s point, the delta-TM strains did not show the same level of c-di-AMP. It may be that the reviewer misread the graph. The purpose of testing these strains was to show that the negative effects of overexpressing full-length WT DacA were due to its membrane localization. Both the FL and deltaTM-DacA (WT) overexpression had equivalent c-di-AMP levels even though the delta-TM overexpression looked like the mCherry-expressing strain based on the measured parameters. This shows that the c-di-AMP levels were irrelevant to the phenotypes observed when overexpressing these WT isoforms. For the mutant isoforms, the delta-TM looked like the mCherry-expressing control while the FL isoform was negatively impacted for reasons we described in the Discussion (e.g., dominant negative effect). In addition, at 16hpi, neither delta-TM strain had c-di-AMP levels that approached the 24h control as denoted in Figure 1E (dashed line) and in the text, which explains why these strains did not show increased late gene transcripts at an earlier timepoint like the dacAop and dacA-KDcom strains.

      Describing the significance of the findings: 

      The findings are important and point to very exciting new avenues to explore the important questions in chlamydial cell form development. The authors present a model that is not quantified and does not match the data well. 

      We respectfully disagree with this assessment as noted above in response to the reviewer’s critique. All of our data are quantified and support the hypothesis as stated.

      Describing the strength of evidence: 

      The evidence presented is incomplete. The authors do a nice job of showing that overexpression of the dacA-ybbR operon increases c-di-AMP and that knockdown or overexpression of the catalytically dead DacA protein decreases the c-di-AMP levels. However, the effects on the developmental cycle and how they fit the proposed model are less well supported. 

      Overall this is a very intriguing finding that will require more gene expression data, phenotypic characterization of cell forms, and better quantitative models to fully interpret these findings. 

      It is not clear what quantitative models the reviewer would prefer, but, ultimately, it is up to the reader to decide whether they agree or not with the model we present. The data are the data, and we have tried to present them as clearly as possible. We would emphasize that, with the number of strains we have analyzed, we have presented a huge amount of data for a study with an obligate intracellular bacterium. As a comparison, most publications on Chlamydia might use a handful of transformant strains, if any. Given the cost and time associated with performing such studies, it is prohibitive to attempt all the time points that one might like to do, and it is not clear to us that further studies will add to or alter the conclusions of the current manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Minor critiques 

      The graphs have red and blue lines but the figure legends are red and black. It would be better if these matched. 

      Changed.

      For Figure 1C. The labels are not very helpful. It's not clear what is HeLa vs mCherry. I believe it is uninfected vs Chlamydia infected.

      Changed.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a contribution to the field of developmental bioelectricity. How do changes of resting potential at the cell membrane affect downstream processes? Zhou et al. reported in 2015 that phosphatidylserine and K-Ras cluster upon plasma membrane depolarization and that voltage-dependent ERK activation occurs when constitutive active K-RasG12V mutants are overexpressed. In this paper, the authors advance the knowledge of this phenomenon by showing that membrane depolarization up-regulates mitosis and that this process is dependent on voltage-dependent activation of ERK. ERK activity's voltage-dependence is derived from changes in the dynamics of phosphatidylserine in the plasma membrane and not by extracellular calcium dynamics.

      Strengths:

      Bioelectricity is an important field for areas of cell, developmental, and evolutionary biology, as well as for biomedicine. Confirmation of ERK as a transduction mechanism, and a characterization of the molecular details involved in control of cell proliferation, is interesting and impactful.

      Weaknesses:

      The functional cell division data need to be stronger. They show that increasing K+ increases proliferation and argue that since a MEK inhibitor (U0126) reduces proliferation in K+ treated cells, K+ induces cell division via ERK. But I don't see statistics to show that the rescue is significant, and I don't see a key U0126-only control. If the U0126 alone reduces proliferation, the combined effect wouldn't prove much.

      We thank the reviewer for constructive feedback. We repeated the experiment including the U0126-only control (5K+U). We updated Fig.1, presenting the newly obtained data with statistical analysis.

      Also, unless I'm missing something, it looks like every sample in their control has exactly the same number of mitotic cells. I understand that they are normalizing to this column, but shouldn't they be normalizing to the mean, with the independent values scattering around 1? It doesn't seem like it can be paired replicates since there are 6 replicates in the control and 4 replicates in one of the conditions? 

      We apologize for the unclear description. As the reviewer pointed out, the experiments were not paired replicates due to the limited number of conditions that can be conducted as a single experiment. To overcome this problem, we always included a control condition (i.e. 5K) based on which normalization was performed. This is the reason the data in 5K is always 1 and the sample size of 5K is the largest. Data include 100-900 mitotic cells within the imaging frame of 6 hrs. We re-wrote the figure legend (Fig1) and the main text, which hopefully clarified our experimental framework.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Sasaki et al. use a combination of live-cell biosensors and patch-clamp electrophysiology to investigate the effect of membrane potential on the ERK MAPK signaling pathway, and probe associated effects on proliferation. This is an effect that has long been proposed, but convincing demonstration has remained elusive, because it is difficult to perturb membrane potential without disturbing other aspects of cell physiology in complex ways. The time-resolved measurements here are a nice contribution to this question, and the perforated patch clamp experiments with an ERK biosensor are fantastic - they come closer to addressing the above difficulty of perturbing voltage than any prior work. It would have been difficult to obtain these observations with any other combination of tools.

      However, there are still some concerns as detailed in specific comments below:

      Specific comments:

      (1) All the observations of ERK activation, by both high extracellular K+ and voltage clamp, could be explained by cell volume increase (more discussion in subsequent comments). There is a substantial literature on ERK activation by hypotonic cell swelling (e.g. https://doi.org/10.1042/bj3090013https://doi.org/10.1002/j.1460-2075.1996.tb00938.x, among others). Here are some possible observations that could demonstrate that ERK activation by volume change is distinct from the effects reported here:

      (i) Does hypotonic shock activate ERK in U2OS cells?

      (ii) Can hypotonic shock activate ERK even after PS depletion, whereas extracellular K+ cannot?

      (iii) Does high extracellular K+ change cell volume in U2OS cells, measured via an accurate method such as fluorescence exclusion microscopy?

      (iv) It would be helpful to check the osmolality of all the extracellular solutions, even though they were nominally targeted to be iso-osmotic.

      This is an important point. We conducted several experiments and provided explanations to rule out the possibility that ERK activation can be explained solely by cell volume change. We measured the osmolarity of all solutions used in this paper, which were 296-305 mOsm/L. This information was added to the Material and Methods section (line 387). Under our experimental conditions, ERK activation was not observed with hypotonic 70 % nor 50% osmolarity solution (Fig.S2).

      It is therefore unlikely that the main cause of ERK activation upon high K<sup>+</sup> perfusion is due to cell volume change. We would like to pursue this issue further when we obtain capacity to measure accurate cell volume change in the future.

      (2) Some more details about the experimental design and the results are needed from Figure 1:

      (i) For how long are the cells serum-starved? From the Methods section, it seems like the G1 release in different K+ concentration is done without serum, is this correct? Is the prior thymidine treatment also performed in the absence of serum?

      Only the high K<sup>+</sup> incubation phase was serum free. We added the following sentence in the main text (line 63) and an experimental diagram was added as Fig1A. “Cells were incubated in the presence of serum except for the phase with altered K<sup>+</sup> concentration. “

      (ii) There is a question of whether depolarization constitutes a physiologically relevant mechanism to regulate proliferation, and how depolarization interacts with other extracellular signals that might be present in an in vivo context.

      This is a very important point. However, the significance of membrane depolarization for cell proliferation in vivo is beyond the scope of this study. This important question will be addressed in the future.

      Does depolarization only promote proliferation after extended serum starvation (in what is presumably a stressed cell state)?

      Cells were cultured in the presence of serum prior to the high K<sup>+</sup> incubation phase as described above. We added a new figure (Fig1A).

      What fraction of total cells are observed to be mitotic (without normalization), and how does this compare to the proliferation of these cells growing in serum-supplemented media? Can K+ concentration tune proliferation rate even in serum-supplemented media?

      We included data recorded in serum-supplemented conditions (Fig.1), which showed a high mitotic rate. This is presumably due to the growth factors included in serum. There is no significant difference between 5K+FBS and 15K+FBS.

      (3) In Figure 2, there are some possible concerns with the perfusion experiment:

      (i) Is the buffer static in the period before perfusion with high K+, or is it perfused? This is not clear from the Methods. If it is static, how does the ERK activity change when perfused with 5 mM K+? In other words, how much of the response is due to flow/media exchange versus change in K+ concentration?

      The buffer was static prior to high K perfusion. We confirmed that perfusion alone does not activate ERK (Fig.S2). We added the following sentence to the main text. “We also confirmed that the effect of perfusion was negligible, as ERK activation was not observed upon start of the 5K<sup>+</sup> perfusion” (line 150).

      (ii) Why do there appear to be population-average decreases in ERK activity in the period before perfusion with high K+ (especially in contrast to Fig. 3)? The imaging period does not seem frequent enough for photo bleaching to be significant.

      Although we don’ t have a clear answer to this question, we speculate that several aspects of the experimental setup may have contributed to the difference. The cell lines and imaging systems used in Fig.2 and Fig.3 were different. The expression level may be different between U2OS cells and HEK 293 cells: transient expression in U2OS cells in contrast to stable expression in HEK 293 cells. This difference may lead to the different signal-to-noise ratio. The imaging system used in Fig.2 is an epi-illumination microscope excited with a 439/24 bandpass filter and detected with 483/32 (CFP) and 542/27 (YFP), while the imaging system used in Fig.3 is a confocal microscope excited with 458 nm laser and detected with 475-525 (DFP) and LP530 (YFP). These optical setups may also contribute to the different population-average properties before stimulation.

      (4) Figure 3 contains important results on couplings between membrane potential and MAPK signaling. However, there are a few concerns:

      (i) Does cell volume change upon voltage clamping? Previous authors have shown that depolarizing voltage clamp can cause cells to swell, at least in the whole-cell configuration: https://www.cell.com/biophysj/fulltext/S0006-3495(18)30441-7 . Could it be possible that the clamping protocol induces changes in ERK signaling due to changes in cell volume, and not by an independent mechanism?

      We do not know whether cell volume is altered in the perforated-patch configuration. As discussed above, however, the effect of cell volume changes on ERK activity seemed to be negligible, because ERK activation was not observed with hypotonic 70 % nor 50% osmolarity solution (Fig.S2)

      (ii) Does the -80 mV clamp begin at time 0 minutes? If so, one might expect a transient decrease in sensor FRET ratio, depending on the original resting potential of the cells. Typical estimates for resting potential in HEK293 cells range from -40 mV to -15 mV, which would reach the range that induces an ERK response by depolarizing clamp in Fig. 3B. What are the resting potentials of the cells before they are clamped to -80 mV, and why do we not see this downward transient?

      We set the potential to -80mV immediately after the giga-seal formation and waited for at least 5 minutes to allow pore formation by gramicidin. We started imaging only after membrane potential was expected to have reached a steady state at -80 mV. We now included this sentence in the ‘Material and Methods’ section (line 398).

      (5) The activation of ERK by perforated voltage clamp and by high extracellular K+ are each convincing, but it is unclear whether they need to act purely through the same mechanism - while additional extracellular K+ does depolarize the cell, it could also be affecting function of voltage-independent transporters and cell volume regulatory mechanisms on the timescales studied. To more strongly show this, the following should be done with the HEK cells where there is already voltage clamp data:

      (i) Measure resting potential using the perforated patch in zero-current configuration in the high K+ medium. Ideally this should be done in the time window after high K+ addition where ERK activation is observed (10-20 minutes) to minimize the possibility of drift due to changes in transporter and channel activity due to post-translational regulation.

      We measured membrane potential in the perforated patch configuration and confirmed that there is negligible potential drift within 20 minutes of perfusion with 145 K+ (only 1~5 mV change during perfusion).

      (ii) Measure YFP/CFP ratio of the HEK cells in the high K+ medium (in contrast to the U2OS cells from Fig. 2 where there is no patch data).

      YFP/CFP ratio data in HEK cells are shown in Fig.S1. As the signal-to-noise level is affected by the expression level of the probe, it is difficult to compare between cells with different expression levels. A higher YFP/CFP value with HEK cells compared to HeLa cells and A431 cells (Sup1) does not necessarily mean that HEK cells have higher ERK activity.

      (iii) The assertion that high K+ is equivalent to changes in Vmem for ERK signaling would be supported if the YFP/CFP change from K+ addition is comparable to that induced by voltage clamp to the same potential. This would be particularly convincing if the experiment could be done with each of the 15 mM, 30 mM, and 145 mM conditions.

      The experimental system using fluorescent biosensor cannot measure absolute ERK activity and can only measure the amount of change after a specific stimulus compared to the period before the stimulus. In electrophysiology experiments, the pre-stimulation membrane potential was clamped to -80 mV, whereas in the perfusion experiment, the membrane potential was variable in individual cells (-35 to -15 mV). It is therefore difficult to compare the results of electrophysiology experiments with those of the perfusion system. Unlike ion channels, it is currently not possible to plot absolute ERK activity with respect to the overall membrane potential. In the present study, we therefore discussed the change rather than the absolute value of ERK activity.

      (6) Line 170: "ERK activity was reduced with a fast time course (within 1 minute) after repolarization to -80 mV." I don't see this in the data: in Fig. 3C, it looks like ERK remains elevated for > 10 min after the electrical stimulus has returned to -80 mV

      Thank you for pointing out that our description was confusing. We changed the sentence to clarify the point we wanted to make. It now reads as follows. “ERK activity showed signs of reduction within 1 minute after repolarization to -80 mV.” (line 174)

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper demonstrates that membrane depolarization induces a small increase in cell entry into mitosis. Based on previous work from another lab, the authors propose that ERK activation might be involved. They show convincingly using a combination of assays that ERK is activated by membrane depolarization. They show this is Ca2+ independent and is a result of activation of the whole K-Ras/ERK cascade which results from changed dynamics of phosphatidylserine in the plasma membrane that activates K-Ras. Although the activation of the Ras/ERK pathway by membrane depolarization is not new, linking it to an increase in cell proliferation is novel.

      Strengths

      A major strength of the study is the use of different techniques - live imaging with ERK reporters, as well as Western blotting to demonstrate ERK activation as well as different methods for inducing membrane depolarization. They also use a number of different cell lines. Via Western blotting the authors are also able to show that the whole MAPK cascade is activated.

      Weaknesses

      A weakness of the study is the data in Figure 1 showing that membrane depolarization results in an increase of cells entering mitosis. There are very few cells entering mitosis in their sample in any condition. This should be done with many more cells to increase confidence in the results.

      We apologize that that description was not clear. Due to the limited number of conditions that can be conducted as a single experiment, we always included control condition (i.e. 5K) and performed normalization by comparing with the control condition of the initial 1.5 hrs. Data were from 100-900 mitotic cell counts within 6hr of the imaging time window. We re-wrote the figure legend (Fig1) and the main text.

      The study also lacks a mechanistic link between ERK activation by membrane depolarization and increased cell proliferation.

      The present study focused on the link between membrane potential and the ERK activity; the mechanistic link between ERK activity and cell proliferation is beyond the scope of the present study. This important topic will be pursued further in subsequent studies.

      The authors did achieve their aims with the caveat that the cell proliferation results could be strengthened. The results for the most part support the conclusions.

      This work suggests that alterations in membrane potential may have more physiological functions than action potential in the neural system as it has an effect on intracellular signalling and potentially cell proliferation.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      minor typo:

      ERK activity has voltage-dependency with the physiological rang of membrane potential should be "range"

      Corrected

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Small points:

      Line 82: rang -> range

      Corrected

      Line 102: ". they were stimulated" -> ". The cells were stimulated"

      Corrected

      Figs. 2C, 2D show exactly the same data points and the same information. Please cut one of these figures.

      We deleted 2C and added the information in 2D and made new Fig.2C.

      For all figs: Please indicate # of cells and # of independent dishes used in each experiment, and make clear whether individual data-points correspond to cells, dishes, or some other unit of measure.

      We added the information in figure legends.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors should repeat the cell proliferation experiments with more cells to strengthen the data. They could also use alternative assays like phosphorylated histone H3 staining for cells in M phase, that might to easier to quantitate.

      We repeated the experiment and Fig.1 was replaced with the new Fig.1

      The authors should investigate how the upregulation of ERK is driving cells into mitosis. At what point in the cell cycle is activated ERK induced by membrane depolarization having the effect. Is it entry into mitosis or earlier in the cell cycle?

      The cells were incubated with a high K+ solution 8-9 hr after G1 release, which is supposed to correspond to G2. These data suggest that mitotic activity is stimulated when ERK is activated at G2. However, we lack conclusive data at present to show the consequence of ERK activation during G2. We therefore cannot pinpoint the stage of cell cycle where depolarization-activated ERK exerts its effect.

      The authors refer a lot to the work of Zhou et al 2015 throughout the paper. This is not necessary and is a bit distracting.

      We deleted several sentence from the manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) Labels should be added in the Figures and should be uniform across all Figures (some are distorted).

      We thank the Reviewer for pointing out this issue. As requested, labels have been edited to ensure they are legible and are consistent in font, size, and style.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      (1) As for Figure 2F, Setd2-SET activity on WT rNuc (H3) appears to be significantly lower compared to what is extensively reported in the literature. This is particularly puzzling given that Figure 2B suggests that using 3H-SAM, H3-nuc are much better substrates than K36me1, whereas in Figure 3F, rH3 is weaker than K36me1. It is recommended for the authors to perform additional experimental repeats and include a quantitative analysis to ensure the consistency and reliability of these findings.  

      We appreciate the Reviewer’s points. We respectfully suggest that these comments may reflect potential confusion around interpreting how different assays detect in vitro methylation, what data can and cannot be compared, and the nature of the different substrates used. 

      With respect to point 1 (Western signal significantly lower compared to extensive literature): To the best of our knowledge, it would be extremely challenging to make a quantitative argument comparing the strength of the Western signal in Figure 2F with results reported in the literature. Specifically, comparing our results with previous studies would require (1) all the studies to have used the exact same antibodies as antibody signal intensities vary depending on the specific activity and selectively of a particular antibody and even its lot number, (2) similar in vitro methylation reaction condition, (3) the same type of recombinant nucleosomes used, and so on. Further, given that these are Western blots, we do not understand how one could interpret an absolute activity level. In the figure, all we can conclude is that in in vitro methylation reactions, our recombinant SETD2 protein methylates rNucs to generate mono-, di-, and tri-methylation at K36 (using vetted antibodies (see Fig. 2e)). If there is a specific paper within the extensive literature that the Reviewer highlights, we could look more into the details of why the signals are different (our guess is that any difference would largely be due to the use of different antibodies). We add that it might be challenging to find a similar experiment performed in the literature; we are not aware of a similar experiment. 

      With respect to comparing Figure 2B and 2F: We do not understand how one can meaningfully compare incorporation of radiolabeled SAM to antibody-based detection on film using an antibody against specific methyl states. In particular, regarding the question regarding comparing rH3 vs H3K36me1 nucleosomes, we point out that in using recombinant nucleosomes installed with native modifications (e.g. H3K36me1), in which the entire population of the starting material is mono-methylated, then naturally the Western signal with an anti-H3K36me1 antibody will be strong. In Fig. 2b, the assay is incorporation of radiolabeled methyl, which is added to the preexiting mono-methylated substrate. In other words, the results are entirely consistent if one understands how the methylation reactions were performed, how methylation was detected, and the nature of the reagents.

      (2) The additional bands observed in Figure 4B, which appear to be H4, should be accompanied by quantification of the intensity of the H3 bands to better assess K36me3 activity. Additionally, the quantification presented in Figure 4C for SAH does not seem accurate as it potentially includes non-specific methylation activity, likely from H4. This needs to be addressed for clarity and accuracy. 

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. The additional bands observed in Figure 4B represent degradation products of histone H3, not H4 methylation. This is commonly seen in in vitro reactions using recombinant nucleosomes, where partial proteolysis of H3 can occur under the assay conditions.  

      (3) In Figure 4E, the differences between bound and unbound substrates are not sufficiently pronounced. Given the modest differences observed, authors might want to consider repeating the assay with sufficient replicates to ensure the results are statistically robust.

      In Figure 4E, we observe a clear difference between the bound and unbound substrate. To aid interpretation, we have clarified in the figure where the bound complex migrates on the gel, while the unbound nucleosomes migrate at the bottom of the gel. The differences are indeed subtle, which we highlight in the text.  

      (4) Regarding labeling, there are multiple issues that need correction: In the depiction of Epicypher's dNuc, it is crucial to clearly mark H2B as the upper band, rather than ambiguously labeling H2A/H2B together when two distinct bands are evident. In Figure 3B and D, the histones appear to be mislabeled, and the band corresponding to H4 has been cut off. It would be beneficial to refer to Figure 3E for correct labeling to maintain consistency and accuracy across figures. 

      Thank you for pointing this out. To avoid any confusion, we have delineated the H2B and H2A markers and indicate the band corresponding to H4.

      (5) There are issues with the image quality in some blots; for instance, Figure 2EF and Figure 2D exhibit excessive contrast and pixelation, respectively. These issues could potentially obscure or misrepresent the data, and thus, adjustments in image processing are recommended to provide clearer, more accurate representations. 

      Contrast adjustments were applied uniformly across each entire image and were not used to modify any specific region of the blot. We have corrected the issue of increased pixelation in Figure 2D. 

      (6) The authors are recommended to provide detailed descriptions of the materials used, including catalog numbers and specific products, to allow for reproducibility and verification of experimental conditions. 

      We have added the missing product specifications and catalog numbers to ensure clarity and reproducibility of the experiments.

      (7) The identification of Setd2 as a tumor suppressor in KrasG12C-driven LUAD is a significant finding. However, the discussion on how this discovery could inspire future therapeutic approaches needs to be more balanced. The current discussion (Page 10) around the potential use of inhibitors is somewhat confusing and could benefit from a clearer explanation of how Setd2's role could be targeted therapeutically. It would be beneficial for the authors to explore both current and potential future strategies in a more structured manner, perhaps by delineating between direct inhibitors, pathway modulators, and other therapeutic modalities. 

      SETD2 is a tumor suppressor in lung cancer (as we show here and many others have clearly established in the literature) and thus we would recommend avoiding a SETD2 inhibitor to treat solid tumors, as it could have a very much unwanted affect.  Our discussion addresses a different point regarding the relative importance of the enzymatic activity versus other, nonenzymatic functions of SETD2. We believe that a detailed exploration of the therapeutic potential of inhibiting SETD2 would be better suited in a review or a more therapy-focused manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In this manuscript, Wolfson and co-authors demonstrate a combination of an injury-specific enhancer and engineered AAV that enhances transgene expression in injured myocardium. The authors characterize spatiotemporal dynamics of TREE-directed AAV expression in the injured heart using a non-invasive longitudinal monitoring system. They show that transgene expression is drastically increased 3 days post-injury, driven by 2ankrd1a. They reported a liver-detargeted capsid, AAV cc.84, with decreased viral entry into the liver while maintaining TREE transgene specificity. They further identified the IR41 serotype with enhanced transgene expression in injured myocardium from AAV library screening. This is an interesting study that optimizes the potential application of TREE delivery for cardiac repair. However, several concerns were raised prior to publication:

      Major Concerns:

      (1) In Figure 1, the authors demonstrated that 2andkrd1aEN is not responsive to sham injury after AAV delivery, but Figure 3 shows a strong response to sham when AAV is delivered after injury. The authors do not provide an explanation for this observation.

      This discrepancy is due to the timing of AAV delivery. In Figure 1, AAV was delivered 60 days prior to IVIS imaging and cardiac injury, allowing time for the baseline level of AAV transgene expression to reach a plateau. From this baseline level, we were able to measure fold change in luminescence signal before and after cardiac injury. In Figure 3, AAV was delivered 4 days after cardiac injury. Luminescence in the heart was measured 3 days later (day 7), when the baseline of AAV transgene expression is still building. The data from Figure 1C-D inform us that the 2ankrd1aEN response to cardiac injury peaks within the first week and returns to baseline levels after 5-7 weeks. In Figure 3E, we show that 2ankrd2aEN provides a baseline level of expression that is present in sham hearts and reaches its plateau after 6 weeks. In contrast, I/R injured hearts show enhanced expression in the first 3-4 weeks, corresponding with the dynamics of 2ankrd1aEN’s response to injury observed in Figure 1C. We have now included a phrase in the revised manuscript on p. 7, paragraph 1 to clarify.

      (2) In Figure 4, a higher GFP signal is observed in all areas of the heart of the IR41-treated mouse compared to AAV9. The authors should compare GFP expression between AAV9 and IR41 in uninjured hearts and provide insights into enhanced cardiac tropism to confirm that IR41 is MI injury enriched, not Sham as well.

      We sought to address this question with the experiments presented in Figure 5. We treated sham mice with AAV9 and IR41 containing 2ankrd1aEN. Figure 5D showed IR41 delivered more vector genomes to the sham heart on average, though not with a p-value less than 0.05 compared with AAV9. In Supplemental Figure 5B, IR41 also provided higher luminescence at day 7 post-sham but was comparable at day 14 and day 21. These data suggest IR41 might increase heart tropism in healthy hearts, but IR41’s effect is most dramatic when delivered to injured hearts, where cardiac vector genomes are highest (Figure 5D). We have now included a sentence in the revised manuscript on p. 8, paragraph 2 to clarify.

      (3) The authors should clarify which model is being used between myocardial infarction (MI) and Ischemia-reperfusion (IR) throughout the figures, as the experimental schemes and figure legends did not match with each other (MI or IR in Figure 1A, 1D, 3A, and 3E). Both models cause different types of injuries. The authors should explain the difference in TREE expression in both models.

      We have revised the figures to specify the model, where I/R or MI is used.

      (4) In Figure 2, the authors use REN instead of 2ankrd1aEN to demonstrate liver-detargeting using AAV cc.84. Is there a specific reason?

      Our data in Figure 1 informed us that off-target liver expression is more specifically an issue for REN compared to 2ankrd1aEN. Baseline levels of luminescence in the heart could not be as clearly marked due to off-target expression in the liver, which was showcased in Figure 2B with AAV9 delivery to sham mice. As discussed above, 2ankrd1aEN provided stronger baseline levels of expression of the heart which could be more clearly marked in IVIS images for tracking fold changes over time. For these reasons, we sought to explore how incorporation of the AAV.cc84 capsid could be utilized to minimize off-target liver expression. We have now included a sentence in the revised manuscript on p. 5, paragraph 3 to clarify.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this manuscript by Wolfson et al., various adeno-associated viruses (AAVs) were delivered to mice to assess the cardiac-specificity, injury border-zone cardiomyocyte transduction rate, and temporal dynamics, with the goal of finding better AAVs for gene therapies targeting the heart. The authors delivered tissue regeneration enhancer elements (TREEs) controlling luciferase expression and used IVIS imaging to examine transduction in the heart and other organs. They found that luciferase expression increased in the first week after injury when using AAV9-TREE-Hsp68 promoter, waning to baseline levels by 7 weeks. However, AAV9 vectors transduced the liver, which was significantly reduced by using an AAV.cc84 liver de-targeting capsid. The authors then performed in vivo screening of AAV9 capsids and found AAV-IR41 to preferentially transduce injured myocardium when compared to AAV9. Finally, the authors combined TREEs with AAV-IR41 to show improved luciferase expression compared to AAV9-TREE at 7, 14, and 21 days after injury.

      Overall, this manuscript provides insights into TREE expression dynamics when paired with various heart-targeting capsids, which can be useful for researchers studying ischemic injury of murine hearts. While the authors have shown the success of using AAV9-TREEs in porcine hearts, it is unknown whether the expression dynamics would be similar in pigs or humans, as mentioned in the limitations.

      The following questions and concerns can be addressed to improve the manuscript:

      (1) From the IVIS data, it seems that the Hsp68 promoter might not be "normally silent in mouse tissues," specifically in the liver (Figure S1B). Are there any other promoters that can be combined with TREEs to induce cardiac-injury specific expression while minimizing liver expression? This could simplify capsid design to focus on delivery to injured areas.

      Indeed we found the Hsp68 promoter does provide low levels of baseline expression, especially in the liver of mice. The Hsp68 promoter was initially chosen due to its permissive nature allowing for assessment of expression directed by TREEs. Many or most groups use the Hsp68 promoter for enhancer tests in mice, but we agree that other permissive promoters might have lower baseline levels of expression and might have the benefit of smaller size. We have not rigorously tested other permissive promoters in our experiments.

      (2) Why is it that AAV9-TREE-Hsp68-Luc wane in expression (Figure 1C and 1D), whereas AAV.cc84-TREE-Hsp68-Luc expresses stably for over 2 months (3E)? This has important implications for the goal of transience in gene delivery.

      Please see our response to reviewer 1’s comment #1 above.

      (3) AAV-IR41 was found to transduce cardiomyocytes in the injured zone. However, this capsid also shows a very strong off-target liver expression. From a capsid design perspective, is it possible to combine AAV-cc84 and AAV-IR41?

      This approach is in theory possible as these epitopes are structurally distinct. However, since the mechanism (receptor usage) is currently unknown, it would not be possible to predict whether the properties are mutually exclusive. Further, we would need to ensure that combining modifications does not impact vector yield. We can explore such features with next generation candidates as we continue to improve the platform. We have now included a sentence in the revised manuscript on p. 9, paragraph 3, mentioning the possibility of combining the two capsid mutations.

      (4) It would be helpful to see immunostaining for the various time points in Figure 5. Is it possible to use an anti-luciferase antibody (or AAV-TREE-Hsp68-eGFP) to compare the two TREE capsids?

      We were not able to do immunostaining of luciferase expression, because the biopsied hearts were used to quantify vector genomes via qPCR. We have previously reported results of immunostaining of EGFP expression directed by 2ankrd1aEN in I/R-injured mouse hearts (Yan et al., 2023), which we expect to match the expression seen in these experiments.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The tissue regeneration enhancer elements (TREEs) identified in zebrafish have been shown to drive injury-activated temporal-spatial gene expression in mice and large animals. These findings increase the translational potential of findings in zebrafish to mammals. In this manuscript, the authors tested TREEs in combination with different adeno-associated viral (AAV) vectors using in vivo luciferase bioluminescent imaging that allows for longitudinal tracking. The TREE-driven luciferase delivered by a liver de-targeted AAV.cc84 decreased off-target transduction in the liver. They further screened an AAV library to identify capsid variants that display enhanced transduction for myocardium post-myocardial infarction. A new capsid variant, AAV.IR41, was found to show increased transduction at the infarct border zones.

      Strengths:

      The authors injected AAV-cargo several days after ischemia/reperfusion (I/R) injury as a clinically relevant approach. Overall, this study is significant in that it identifies new AAV vectors for potential new gene therapies in the future. The manuscript is well-written, and their data are also of high quality.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors might be using MI (myocardial infarction) and I/R injury interchangeably in their text and labels. For instance, "We systemically transduced mice at 4 days after permanent left coronary artery ligation with either AAV9 or IR41 harboring a 2ankrd1aEN-Hsp68::fLuc transgene. IVIS imaging revealed higher expression levels in animals transduced with IR41 compared to AAV9, in both sham and I/R groups (Fig. 5A)". They should keep it consistent. There is also no description for the MI model.

      We have adjusted figure labels and main text to ensure the injury model is described correctly.

      We have also addressed all additional Recommendations for the authors, which requested minor modifications to figures like error bars and image annotation.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study uses mesoscale simulations to investigate how membrane geometry regulates the multiphase organization of postsynaptic condensates. It reveals that dimensionality shifts the balance between specific and non-specific interactions, thereby reversing domain morphology observed in vitro versus in vivo.

      Strengths:

      The model is grounded in experimental binding affinities, reproduces key experimental observations in 3D and 2D contexts, and offers mechanistic insight into how geometry and molecular features drive phase behavior.

      Weaknesses:

      The model omits other synaptic components that may influence domain organization and does not extensively explore parameter sensitivity or broader physiological variability.

      We thank the reviewer for his/her time and effort to our manuscript. We agree with the point that the contribution of other synaptic components should be addressed. We have included a discussion of the effects of environmental factors such as protein and ion concentrations, as well as other omitted postsynaptic components (SAPAP, Shank, and Homer) on phase morphology. In the middle of the 2<sup>nd</sup> paragraph of Discussion, we added: 

      “While these in vivo results contain additional scaffold and cytoskeletal elements omitted in our model, such as SAPAP, Shank and Homer, nearly all proteins in the middle and lower layers of the PSD associate directly or indirectly with PSD-95 in the upper PSD layer. Consequently, it is probable that other scaffold proteins contribute to the mobility of AMPAR-containing and NMDAR-containing nanodomains indistinguishably. They may increase the stability of the AMPAR and NMDAR clusters but are unlikely to have a distinct effect to reverse the phase-separation phenomenon.”

      Also, as the reviewer pointed out, we agree with that physiological factors such as ion concentration may influence the phase. However, conditions such as ion concentration are implicitly implemented as the specific and nonspecific interactions in this model, which makes it difficult to estimate the effect of each physiological condition individually. We added the variability potential of physiological conditions to the discussion section as a limitation of this model. To investigate parameter sensitivity in more detail, we performed additional MD simulations with weakened membrane constraints to account for the behavior between 3D and 2D. We added:

      “First, our results did not provide direct insights to physiological conditions, such as ion concentrations. Since such factors are implicitly implemented in our model, it is difficult to estimate these effects individually. This suggests the need for future implementation of environmental factors and validation under a broader range of in vivo-like settings.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This is a timely and insightful study aiming to explore the general physical principles for the sub-compartmentalization--or lack thereof--in the phase separation processes underlying the assembly of postsynaptic densities (PSDs), especially the markedly different organizations in three-dimensional (3D) droplets on one hand and the twodimensional (2D) condensates associated with a cellular membrane on the other. Simulation of a highly simplified model (one bead per protein domain) is carefully executed. Based on a thorough consideration of various control cases, the main conclusion regarding the trade-off between repulsive excluded volume interactions and attractive interactions among protein domains in determining the structures of 3D vs 2D model PSD condensates is quite convincing. The results in this manuscript are novel; however, as it stands, there is substantial room for improvement in the presentation of the background and the findings of this work. In particular,

      (i) conceptual connections with prior works should be better discussed 

      (ii) essential details of the model should be clarified, and

      (iii) the generality and limitations of the authors' approach should be better delineated.

      We appreciate the reviewer for his/her time and effort on our manuscript and for encouraging comments and helpful suggestions. We answered every technical comment the reviewer mentioned below.

      Specifically, the following items should be addressed (with the additional references mentioned below cited and discussed):

      (1) Excluded volume effects are referred to throughout the text by various terms and descriptions such as "repulsive force according to the volume" (e.g., in the Introduction), "nonspecific volume interaction", and "volume effects" in this manuscript. This is somewhat curious and not conducive to clarity, because these terms have alternate or connotations of alternate meanings (e.g., in biomolecular modeling, repulsive interactions usually refer to those with longer spatial ranges, such as that between like charges). It will be much clearer if the authors simply refer to excluded volume interactions as excluded volume interactions (or effects).  

      Thank you for this comment. We have substituted the words “excluded volume interactions” for words of similar meaning. However, we have left the expression of “non-specific interactions” as they are referring to explicit interactions that are given as force fields in the model, rather than in the general meaning of excluded volume effect.

      (2) In as much as the impact of excluded volume effects on subcompartmentalization of condensates ("multiple phases" in the authors' terminology), it has been demonstrated by both coarse-grained molecular dynamics and field-theoretic simulations that excluded volume is conducive to demixing of molecular species in condensates [Pal et al., Phys Rev E 103:042406 (2021); see especially Figures 4-5 of this reference]. This prior work bears directly on the authors' observation. Its relationship with the present work should be discussed.  

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment. We have now included a more detailed discussion on excluded volume effect in the revised manuscript, which provides important context for our findings. Furthermore, we have cited the references to support and enrich the discussion, as recommended.

      (3)  In the present model setup, activation of the CaMKII kinase affects only its binding to GluN2Bc. This approach is reasonable and leads to model predictions that are essentially consistent with the experiment. More broadly, however, do the authors expect activation of the CaMKII kinase to lead to phosphorylation of some of the molecular species involved with PSDs? This may be of interest since biomolecular condensates are known to be modulated by phosphorylation [Kim et al., Science 365:825-829 (2019); Lin et al, eLife 13:RP100284 (2025)].  

      We agree that phosphorylation effect on phase separation is an important and interesting aspect to consider. Some experimental results have shown that activation of CaMKII can lead to phosphorylation of various proteins and make PSD condensate more stable by altering their interactions. We included the sentence below in limitations:

      “In this context, we also do not explicitly account for downstream phosphorylation events. Although such proteins are not included in the current components, they will regulate PSD-95, affecting its binding valency, or diffusion coefficient. This is a subject worthy of future research.”

      (4) The forcefield for confinement of AMPAR/TARP and NMDAR/GluN2Bc to 2D should be specified in the main text. Have the authors explored the sensitivity of their 2D findings on the strength of this confinement?

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful recommendation. We have revised the manuscript to include membrane-mimicking potential on main text. Furthermore, we also think that exploring the shape of the 3D/2D condensate phase due to the sensitivity of confinement is a very interesting point. We have additionally performed MD simulations with smaller/larger membrane constraints and included the results in supporting information as Figure S5. The following parts are added:

      “We further attempted to mimic intermediate conditions between 3D and 2D systems in two different manners. First, we applied a weaker membrane constraint in 2D system. Even when the strength of membrane constraints is reduced by a factor of 1000, NMDARs are located on the inner side when the CaMKII was active, as well as the result in 2D system (Fig.S5ABC). Second, to weaken further the effect of membrane constraints, we artificially altered the membrane thickness from 5 nm to 50 nm, in addition to reducing the membrane constraints by 1000. As a result, NMDAR clusters move to the bottom and surround AMPAR (Fig.S5DEF). In this artificial intermediate condition, both states in which the NMDARs are outside (corresponding to 3D) and in which the NMDARs are inside (corresponding to 2D) are observed, depending on the strength of the membrane constraint.”

      (5)  Some of the labels in Figure 1 are confusing. In Figure 1A, the structure labeled as AMPAR has the same shape as the structure labeled as TARP in Figure 1B, but TARP is labeled as one of the smaller structures (like small legs) in the lower part of AMPAR in Figure 1A. Does the TARP in Figure 1B correspond to the small structures in the lower part of AMPAR? If so, this should be specified (and better indicated graphically), and in that case, it would be better not to use the same structural drawing for the overall structure and a substructure. The same issue is seen for NMDAR in Figure 1A and GluN2Bc in Figure 1B. 

      (6) In addition to clarifying Figure 1, the authors should clarify the usage of AMPAR vs TARP and NMDAR vs GluN2Bc in other parts of the text as well.

      (7) The physics of the authors' model will be much clearer if they provide an easily accessible graphical description of the relative interaction strengths between different domain-representing spheres (beads) in their model. For this purpose, a representation similar to that given by Feric et al., Cell 165:1686-1697 (2016) (especially Figure 6B in this reference) of the pairwise interactions among the beads in the authors' model should be provided as an additional main-text figure. Different interaction schemes corresponding to inactive and activated CAMKII should be given. In this way, the general principles (beyond the PSD system) governing 3D vs 2D multiple-component condensate organization can be made much more apparent.  \

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s comments. According to the recommendation, we have changed the diagram in Figure 1B into interaction matrix with each mesoscale molecular representation and the expression in main text to be clearer about AMPAR and TARP, and about the relationship between NMDAR and GluN2Bc. Former diagram of the pairs of specific interaction is moved to supplementary figure. 

      (8) Can the authors' rationalization of the observed difference between 3D and 2D model PSD condensates be captured by an intuitive appreciation of the restriction on favorable interactions by steric hindrance and the reduction in interaction cooperativity in 2D vs 3D?  

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. As pointed out, the multiphase morphology change observed in this study can be attributed to a decrease in coordination number in 2D compared to 3D. We have included the physicochemical rationalization in the discussion.  

      (9) In the authors' model, the propensity to form 2D condensates is quite weak. Is this prediction consistent with the experiment? Real PSDs do form 2D condensates around synapses.  

      We are grateful to the reviewer for highlighting this important point. We agree with that the real PSD forms 3D condensates beneath the 2D membrane. Some lower PSD components under the membrane (i.e. SAPAP, Shank, and Homer) are omitted in our system, which may cause a weak condensation. To emphasize this, we have added the following sentence:

      “While these in vivo results contain additional scaffold and cytoskeletal elements omitted in our model, such as SAPAP, Shank and Homer, nearly all proteins in the middle and lower layers of the PSD associate directly or indirectly with PSD-95 in the upper PSD layer. Consequently, it is probable that other scaffold proteins contribute to the mobility of AMPAR-containing and NMDAR-containing nanodomains indistinguishably. They may increase the stability of the AMPAR and NMDAR clusters but are unlikely to have a distinct effect to reverse the phase-separation phenomenon.”

      However, we believe that the clusters formed on the 2D membrane are not a robust “phase” because they do not follow scaling law. In fact, in our previous study of PSD system with AMPAR(TARP)<sub>4</sub> and PSD-95, we have already reported that phase separation is less likely to occur in 2D than in 3D. The previous result suggests that phase separation on membrane may be difficult to achieve, which is consistent with the results of this study.

      (10) More theoretical context should be provided in the Introduction and/or Discussion by drawing connections to pertinent prior works on physical determinants of co-mixing and de-mixing in multiple-component condensates (e.g., amino acid sequence), such as Lin et al., New J Phys 19:115003 (2017) and Lin et al., Biochemistry 57:2499-2508 (2018). 

      (11) In the discussion of the physiological/neurological significance of PSD in the Introduction and/or Discussion, for general interest it is useful to point to a recently studied possible connection between the hydrostatic pressure-induced dissolution of model PSD and high-pressure neurological syndrome [Lin et al., Chem Eur J 26:11024-11031 (2020)].

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful recommendation. We have added the recommended references in each relevant part in introduction, respectively.

      (12) It is more accurate to use "perpendicular to the membrane" rather than "vertical" in the caption for Figure 3E and other such descriptions of the orientation of the CaMKII hexagonal plane in the text.

      We thank you for your comment. We replaced the word “vertical” with “perpendicular" in the main text and caption.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this work, Yamada, Brandani, and Takada have developed a mesoscopic model of the interacting proteins in the postsynaptic density. They have performed simulations, based on this model and using the software ReaDDy, to study the phase separation in this system in 2D (on the membrane) and 3D (in the bulk). They have carefully investigated the reasons behind different morphologies observed in each case, and have looked at differences in valency, specific/non-specific interactions, and interfacial tension.

      Strengths:

      The simulation model is developed very carefully, with strong reliance on binding valency and geometry, experimentally measured affinities, and physical considerations like the hydrodynamic radii. The presented analyses are also thorough, and great effort has been put into investigating different scenarios that might explain the observed effects.

      Weaknesses:

      The biggest weakness of the study, in my opinion, has to do with a lack of more in-depth physical insight about phase separation. For example, the authors express surprise about similar interactions between components resulting in different phase separation in 2D and 3D. This is not surprising at all, as in 3D, higher coordination numbers and more available volume translate to lower free energy, which easily explains phase separation. The role of entropy is also significantly missing from the analyses. When interaction strengths are small, entropic effects play major roles. In the introduction, the authors present an oversimplified view of associative and segregative phase transitions based on the attractive and repulsive interactions, and I'm afraid that this view, in which all the observed morphologies should have clear pairwise enthalpic explanations, diffuses throughout the analysis. Meanwhile, I believe the authors correctly identify some relevant effects, where they consider specific/nonspecific interactions, or when they investigate the reduced valency of CaMKII in the 2D system.

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful and constructive comments. Regarding the difference in phase behavior between 2D and 3D systems, we appreciate the reviewer’s clarification that differences in coordination number and entropy in higher dimensions can account for the observed morphology of the phases. While it may be clear that entropy decreases due to the decrease of coordination number, our objective was to uncover how such an isotropic entropy reduction regulates the behavior of each phase driven by different interactions, which remains largely unknown. To emphasize this, we modified the introduction and have now included a discussion of the entropic contributions to phase behavior in both 2D and 3D systems, and we have made this clearer in the revised manuscript by referencing relevant theoretical frameworks. In the Discussion, we added the sentence below:

      “Generally, phase separation can be explained by the Flory-Huggins theory and its extensions: phase separation can be favored by the difference in the effective pairwise interactions in the same phase compared to those across different phases, and is disfavored by mixing entropy. The effective interactions contain various molecular interactions, including direct van der Waals and electrostatic interactions, hydrophobic interactions, and purely entropic macromolecular excluded volume interactions. For the latter, Asakura-Oosawa depletion force can drive the phase separation. Furthermore, the demixing effect was explicitly demonstrated in previous simulations and field theory (61). Importantly, we note that the effective pairwise interactions scale with the coordination number z. The coordination number is a clear and major difference between 3D and 2D systems. In 3D systems, large z allows both relatively strong few specific interactions and many weak non-specific interactions. While a single specific interaction is, by definition, stronger than a single non-specific interaction, contribution of the latter can have strong impact due to its large number. On the other hand, a smaller z in the membrane-bound 2D system limits the number of interactions. In case of limited competitive binding, specific interactions tend to be prioritized compared to non-specific ones. In fact, Fig. 3A clearly shows that number of specific interactions in 2D is similar to that in 3D, while that of non-specific interactions is dramatically reduced in 2D. In the current PSD system, CaMKII is characterized by large valency and large volume. In the 3D solution system, non-specific excluded volume interactions drive CaMKII to the outer phase, while this effect is largely reduced in 2D, resulting in the reversed multiphase.   

      Also, I sense some haste in comparing the findings with experimental observations. For example, the authors mention that "For the current four component PSD system, the product of concentrations of each molecule in the dilute phase is in good agreement with that of the experimental concentrations (Table S2)." But the data used here is the dilute phase, which is the remnant of a system prepared at very high concentrations and allowed to phase separate. The errors reported in Table S2 already cast doubt on this comparison. 

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful comment. In the validation process, we adjusted the parameters so that the number of molecules in dilute phase is consistent with the experimental lower limit of phase separation, based on the assumption that phase-separated dilute phase is the same concentration as the critical concentration. That is why we focus on comparing dilute phase concentration in Table S2. However, in our simulations, the number of protein molecules is relatively small since it is based on the average number per synapse spine. For example, there are only about 60 CaMKII molecules at most, and its presence in the dilute phase is highly sensitive to concentration, as the reviewer pointed out. This is one of the limitations, so we have added a description to the Limitations section. We added:

      “Second, parameter calibration contains some uncertainty. Previous in vitro study results used for parameter validation are at relatively high concentrations for phase separation, which may shift critical thresholds compared to that in in vivo environments. Also, since the number of molecules included in the model is small, the difference of a single molecule could result in a large error during this validation process.”

      Or while the 2D system is prepared via confining the particles to the vicinity of the membrane, the different diffusive behavior in the membrane, in contrast to the bulk (i.e., the Saffman-Delbrück model), is not considered. This would thus make it difficult to interpret the results of a coupled 2D/3D system and compare them to the actual system.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s helpful comment. We agree with that there is a concern that the Einstein-Stokes equation does not adequately reproduce the diffusion of membrane-embedded particles. We recalculated the diffusion coefficients for every membrane particle used in this model using the Saffman-Delbrück model and found that diffusion coefficients for receptor cores (AMPAR and NMDAR) were approximately three times larger. These values are still about ~10 times smaller than that of molecules diffusing under the cytoplasm. Additionally, since this study focuses on the morphology of the phase/cluster at the thermodynamic equilibrium, we think that the magnitude of the diffusion coefficient has little influence on the final structure of the cluster. However, we will incorporate the membrane-embedded diffusion as a future improvement item for better modelling and implementation. We added:

      “Third, we estimated all the diffusion coefficients from the Einstein-Stokes equation, which may oversimplify membrane-associated dynamics. Applying the Saffmann-Delbrück model to membrane-embedded particles would be desired although the resulting diffusion coefficients remain of the same order of magnitude. These limitations highlight the need for further research, yet they do not undermine the core significance of the present findings in advancing our understanding of multiphase morphologies.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      There has been intense controversy over the generality of Hamilton's inclusive fitness rule for how evolution works on social behaviors. All generally agree that relatedness can be a game changer, for example allowing for otherwise unselectable altruistic behaviors when 𝑐 < 𝑟𝑏, where 𝑐 is the fitness cost to the altruism, 𝑏 is the fitness benefit to another, and 𝑟 their relatedness. Many complications have been successfully incorporated into the theory, including different reproductive values and viscous population structures.

      I agree, especially if by incorporating viscous population structures, the reviewer means the discovery of the cancellation effect (Wilson, Pollock, and Dugatkin, 1992, Taylor, 1992).

      The controversy has centered on another dimension; Hamilton's original model was for additive fitness, but how does his result hold when fitnesses are non-additive? One approach has been not to worry about a general result but just find results for particular cases. A consistent finding is that the results depend on the frequency of the social allele - nonadditivity causes frequency dependence that was absent in Hamilton's approach.

      Just to be extra precise: Hamilton’s (1964) original model did not use the Price equation nor the regression approach to define costs and benefits, and it did indeed simply presuppose fixed, additive fitness effects.

      Also for extra precision on terminology: many researchers will describe all fitnesses in social evolution as frequency dependent. The reason they do, is that with or without additivity, both the fitness of cooperators (with the social allele) and the fitness of defectors (without the social alle) typically increase in the frequency of cooperators in the population; the more cooperators there are, the more individuals run into them, which increases average fitness. The result depending on the frequency I take to mean that which of those two fitnesses is larger flips at a certain frequency, which automatically implies that the difference between them is depending on the frequency of the social allele. This is indeed the result of non-additivity. We will return to this in more detail in the response to Reviewer #3. Also at the end of Appendix B I have added a bit to be extra precise regarding frequency dependence.

      Two other approaches derive from Queller via the Price equation. Queller 1 is to find forms like Hamilton's rule, but with additional terms that deal with non-additive interaction, each with an r-like population structure variable multiplied by a b-like fitness effect (Queller, 1985). Queller 2 redefines the fitness effects c and b as partial regressions of the actor's and recipient's genes on fitness. This leaves Hamilton's rule intact, just with new definitions of c and b that depend on frequency (Queller, 1992a).

      Queller 2 is the version that has been most adopted by the inclusive fitness community along with assertions that Hamilton's rule in completely general. In this paper, van Veelen argues that Queller 1 is the correct approach. He derives a general form that Queller only hinted at. He does so within a more rigorous framework that puts both Price's equation and Hamilton's rule on firmer statistical ground. Within that framework, the Queller 2 approach is seen to be a statistical misspecification - it employs a model without interaction in cases that actually do have interaction. If we accept that this is a fatal flaw, the original version of Hamilton's rule is limited to linear fitness models, which might not be common.

      I totally agree.

      Strengths:

      While the approach is not entirely new, this paper provides a more rigorous approach and a more general result. It shows that both Queller 1 and Queller 2 are identities and give accurate results, because both are derived from the Price equation, which is an identity. So why prefer Queller 1? It identifies the misspecification issue with the Queller 2 approach and points out its consequences. For example, it will not give the minimum squared differences between the model and data. It does not separate the behavioral effects of the individuals from the population state (𝑏 and 𝑐 become dependent on 𝑟 and the population frequency).

      Just to be precise on a detail: in the data domain, as long as the number of parameters in a statistical model is lower than the number of data points, adding parameters typically (generically) lowers the sum of squared errors. That is to say, for an underspecified statistical model, the sum of squared errors goes down if a parameter is added, but for an already overspecified statistical model, the same is still true (although, typically, by how much the sum of squared errors is reduced will differ). The model specification task for a statistician includes knowing when to keep adding parameters, because the data suggest that the model is still underspecified, and when to stop adding parameters, because the model is well-specified, even if adding parameters still reduces the sum of squared errors.

      In a modeling context, on the other hand, one can say that sum of squared differences will stop decreasing at the point where the statistical model is well-specified, that is: when it matches the model we are considering.

      The paper also shows how the same problems can apply to non-social traits. Epistasis is the non-additivity of effects of two genes within the individual. (So one wonders why have we not had a similarly fierce controversy over how we should treat epistasis?)

      The paper is clearly written. Though somewhat repetitive, particularly in the long supplement, most of that repetition has the purpose of underscoring how the same points apply equally to a variety of different models.

      Finally, this may be a big step towards reconciliation in the inclusive fitness wars. Van Veelen has been one of the harshest critics of inclusive fitness, and now he is proposing a version of it.

      I am very happy to hear this, because I am indeed hopeful for reconciliation. I would like to add a comment, though. The debate on Hamilton’s rule/inclusive fitness is regularly thought of as a battle between two partizan camps, where both sides care at least as much about winning as they do about getting things right. This is totally understandable, because to some degree that is true. Also, I agree that it is fair to position me in the camp that is critical of the inclusive fitness literature. However, I would like to think that I have not been taking random shots at Hamilton’s rule. I have pointed to problems with the typical use of the Price equation and Hamilton’s rule, and I think I did for very good reasons. I am obviously very happy that finding the Generalized Price equation, and the general version of Hamilton’s rule, allowed me to go beyond this, and (finally) offer a correct alternative, and I totally appreciate that this opens the door for reconciliation, as this reviewer points out. But I would not describe this as a road-toDamascus moment. In order to illustrate the continuity in my work, I would like to point to three papers.

      In van Veelen (2007), I pointed to the missing link between the central result in Hamilton’s (1964) famous paper (which states that selection dynamics take the population to a state where mean inclusive fitness is maximized), and Hamilton’s actual rule (which states that selection will lead to individuals maximizing their individual inclusive fitness). My repair stated the additional assumptions that were necessary to make the latter follow from the former. I would say that this can hardly be characterized as an attack on Hamilton’s rule. Reading Hamilton (1964) with enough care to notice something is missing, and then repairing it, I think is a sign of respect, and not an attack.

      Van Veelen (2011) is about the replicator dynamics for n-player games, with the possibility of assortment. This puts the paper in a domain that does not assume weak selection, and that is typically not much oriented towards inclusive fitness. I included a theorem that implies that, under the condition of linearity, inclusive fitness not only gets the direction of selection right, but 𝑟𝑏 − 𝑐 becomes a parameter that also determines the speed of selection. This I think is representative, in the sense that in many of my papers, I carefully stake out when the classic version of Hamilton’s rule does work.

      In Akdeniz and van Veelen (2020), we moreover take a totally standard inclusive fitness approach in a model of the cancellation effect at the group level.

      I would say that this does not line up with the image of a harsh critic that takes random shots at Hamilton’s rule or inclusive fitness.

      Weaknesses:

      van Veelen argues that the field essentially abandoned the Queller 1 approach after its publication. I think this is putting it too strongly - there have been a number of theoretical studies that incorporate extra terms with higher-order relatednesses. It is probably accurate to say that there has been relative neglect. But perhaps this is partly due to a perception that this approach is difficult to apply.

      I can imagine that the perceived difficulty in application may have played a role in the neglect of the Queller 1 approach. What for sure has played a role, and I would think a much bigger one, is that the literature has been pretty outspoken that the Queller 1 approach is the wrong way to go. The main text cites a number of papers that hold this position very emphatically (The first one of those was a News and Views by Alan Grafen (1985) that accompanied the paper in which Queller presented his Queller 1 approach. I am very happy that Appendix B shows on how many levels this News and Views was wrong.). There is only a handful of papers that follow the Queller 1 example.

      The model in this paper is quite elegant and helps clarify conceptual issues, but I wonder how practical it will turn out to be. In terms of modeling complicated cases, I suspect most practitioners will continue doing what they have been doing, for example using population genetics or adaptive dynamics, without worrying about neatly separating out a series of terms multiplying fitness coefficients and population structure coefficients.

      I am not sure if I see what the reviewer envisions practitioners that use population genetics will keep on doing. I would think that the Generalized Price equation in regression form is a description of population genetic dynamics, and therefore, if practitioners will not make an effort to “neatly separate out a series of terms multiplying fitness coefficients and population structure coefficients”, then all I can say is that they should. I cannot do more than explain why, if they do not, they are at risk of mischaracterizing what gets selected and why.

      Regarding those that use adaptive dynamics, I would say that this is a whole different approach. Within this approach, one can also apply inclusive fitness; see Section 6 and Appendix D of van Veelen et al. (2017). Appendix D is full of deep technical results and was done by Benjamin Allen.

      For empirical studies, it is going to be hard to even try to estimate all those additional parameters. In reality, even the standard Hamilton's rule is rarely tested by trying to estimate all its parameters. Instead, it is commonly tested more indirectly, for example by comparative tests of the importance of relatedness. That of course would not distinguish between additive and non-additive models that both depend on relatedness, but it does test the core idea of kin selection. It will be interesting to see if van Veelen's approach stimulates new ways of exploring the real world.

      Regarding the impact on empirical studies, there are a few things that I would like to say. The first is that I would just like to repeat, maybe a bit more elaborately, what I wrote at the end of the main text. Given that the generalized version of Hamilton’s rule produces a host of Hamilton-like rules, and given the fact that all of them by construction indicate the direction of selection accurately, the question whether or not Hamilton’s rule holds turns out to be illposed. That means that we can stop doing empirical tests of Hamilton’s rule, which are predicated on the idea that Hamilton’s rule, with benefits and costs being determined by the regression method, could be violated – which it cannot (Side note: it is possible to violate Hamilton’s rule, if costs and benefits are defined according to the counterfactual method; see van Veelen et al. (2017) and van Veelen (2018). This way of defining costs and benefits is less common, although there are authors that find this definition natural enough to assume that this is the way in which everybody defines costs and benefits (Karlin and Matessi, 1983, Matessi and Karlin, 1984).). Instead, we should do empirical studies to find out which version of Hamilton’s rule applies to which behaviour in which species.

      would like to not understate what a step forward this is. The size of the step forwards is of course also due to the dismal point of departure. As theorists, we have failed our empiricists, because all 12 studies included in the review by Bourke (2014) of papers that explicitly test Hamilton’s rule are based on the misguided idea that the traditional Hamilton’s rule, with costs and benefits defined according to the regression method, can be violated. While the field does sometimes have disdain for mathematical nit-picking, this is a point where a little more attention to detail would have really helped. If the hypothesis is that Hamilton’s rule holds, and the null is that it does not, then trying to specify how the empirical quantity that reflects inclusive fitness would be distributed under the null hypothesis (in order to do the right statistical tests) would have forced researchers to do something with the information that this quantity is not distributed at all, because Hamilton’s rule is general (in the sense that it holds for any way in which the world works). If one would prefer to reverse the null and the alternative hypothesis, one would run into similar problems. Understanding that the question is ill-posed therefore is a big step forwards from the terrible state of statistics and the waste of research time, attention and money on the empirical side of this field (see also Section 8 of van Veelen et al., 2017).

      I would agree that doing comparative statics may not be much affected by this. Section 5 of van Veelen et al. (2017) indicates that there can be a large set of circumstances under which the general idea “relatedness up → cooperation up” still applies. But that may be a bit unambitious, and Section 8 of van Veelen et al. (2017), and the final section of van Veelen (2018) contain some reflections on empirical testing that may allow us to go beyond that. As long as there is change happening in the Generalized Price equation, the population is not in equilibrium. For empirical tests, one can either aim to capture selection as it happens, or assume that what we observe reflects properties of an equilibrium. This leads to interesting reflections on how to do empirics, which may differ between traits that are continuous and traits that are discrete (again: see van Veelen et al. (2017), and van Veelen (2018).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript reconsiders the "general form" of Hamilton's rule, in which "benefit" and "cost" are defined as regression coefficients. It points out that there is no reason to insist on Hamilton's rule of the form −𝑐 + 𝑏𝑟 > 0, and that, in fact, arbitrarily many terms (i.e. higherorder regression coefficients) can be added to Hamilton's rule to reflect nonlinear interactions. Furthermore, it argues that insisting on a rule of the form −𝑐 + 𝑏𝑟 > 0 can result in conditions that are true but meaningless and that statistical considerations should be employed to determine which form of Hamilton's rule is meaningful for a given dataset or model.

      Totally right. I cannot help to want to be extra precise, though, by distinguishing between the data domain and the modelling domain. In the data domain, statistical considerations apply in order to avoid misspecification. In this domain, avoiding misspecification can be complicated, because we do not know the underlying data generating process, and we depend on noisy data to make a best guess. In the modeling domain, however, there is no excuse for misspecification, as the model is postulated by the modeler. I therefore would think that in this domain, it does not really require “statistical considerations” to minimize the probability of misspecification; we can get the probability of misspecification all the way down to 0 by just choosing not to do it.

      Strengths:

      The point is an important one. While it is not entirely novel-the idea of adding extra terms to Hamilton's rule has arisen sporadically (Queller, 1985, 2011; Fletcher et al., 2006; van Veelen et al., 2017)--it is very useful to have a systematic treatment of this point. I think the manuscript can make an important contribution by helping to clarify a number of debates in the literature. I particularly appreciate the heterozygote advantage example in the SI.

      Me too, and I really hope the readers make it this far! I have thought of putting it in the main text, but did not know where that would fit.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the mathematical analysis is rigorously done and I largely agree with the conclusions, I feel there are some issues regarding terminology, some regarding the state of the field, and the practice of statistics that need to be clarified if the manuscript is truly to resolve the outstanding issues of the field. Otherwise, I worry that it will in some ways add to the confusion.

      (1) The "generalized" Price equation: I agree that the equations labeled (PE.C) and (GPE.C) are different in a subtle yet meaningful way. But I do not see any way in which (GPE.C) is more general than (PE.C). That is, I cannot envision any circumstance in which (GPE.C) applies but (PE.C) does not. A term other than "generalized" should be used.

      This is a great point! Just to make sure that those that read the reports online understand this point, let me add some detail. The equation labeled (PE.C) – which is short for Price equation in covariance form – is

      The derivation in Appendix A then assumes that we have a statistical model that includes a constant and a linear term for the p-score. It then defines the model-estimated fitness of individual 𝑖 as , where 𝑤<sub> 𝑖</sub> is the realized number of offspring of individual 𝑖, and 𝜀<sub> 𝑖</sub> is the error term – and it is the sum over all individuals of this error term-squared that is minimized. The vector of model-estimated fitnesses will typically be different for different choices of the statistical model. Appendix A then goes on to show that, whatever the statistical model is that is used, for all of them , as long as the statistical model includes a constant and a linear term for the p-score. That means that we can rewrite (PE.C) as

      The point that the reviewer is making, is that this is not really a generalization. For a given dataset (or, more generally, for a given population transition, whether empirical or in a model), is just a number, and it happens to be the case that 𝐶𝑜𝑣(𝑤:, 𝑝) returns the same number, whatever statistical model we use for determining what the model-estimated fitnesses 𝑤<sub> 𝑖</sub> are (as long as the statistical model includes a constant and a linear term for the p-score). In other words, (PE.C) is not really nested in (GPE.C), so (GPE.C) is not a proper generalization of (PE.C).

      This is a totally correct point, and I had actually struggled a bit with the question what terminology to use here. Equation (GPE.C) is definitely general, in the sense that we can change the statistical model, and thereby change the vector of model-estimated fitnesses , but as long as we keep the constant and the linear term in the statistical model, the equation still applies. But it is not a generalization of (PE.C).

      I do however have a hard time coming up with a better label. The General Price equation may be a bit better, but it still suggests generalization. The Statistical Model-based Price equation does not suggest or imply generalization, but it does not convey how general it is, and it suggests that it could be an alternative to the normal Price equation that one may or may not choose to use – while this version really is the one we should use. It may moreover create the impression that this is only for doing statistics, and one might use the traditional Price equation for anything that is not statistics. I cannot really think of other good alternatives, but I am of course open to suggestions.

      So, by lack of a better label, I called this the Generalized Price equation in covariance form. Though clearly imperfect, there are still a few good things about this label. The first is that, as mentioned above, this equation is general, in the sense that it holds, regardless of the statistical model. The second reason is that this is Step 1 in a sequence of three steps., the other two of which do produce proper generalizations. Step 2 goes from this equation in covariance form to the Generalized Price Equation in regression form, which is a proper generalization of the traditional Price equation in regression form. Step 3 goes from the Generalized Price Equation in regression form to the general version of Hamilton’s rule, which is also a proper generalization of the classical Hamilton’s rule. Since I would suggest that Step 1 on its own is kind of useless, and therefore Step 1 and Step 2 will typically come as a package, I would be tempted to think that this justifies the abuse of terminology for the Price Equation in covariance form. I did however add the observation made by the reviewer at the point where the Generalized Price equation (in both forms) is derived, so I hope this at least partly addresses this concern.

      (2) Regression vs covariance forms of the Price equation: I think the author uses "generalized" in reference to what Price called the "regression form" of his equation. But to almost everyone in the field, the "Price Equation" refers to the covariance form. For this reason, it is very confusing when the manuscript refers to the regression form as simply "the Price Equation".

      As an example, in the box on p. 15, the manuscript states "The Price equation can be generalized, in the sense that one can write a variety of Price-like equations for a variety of possible true models, that may have generated the data." But it is not the Price equation (covariance form) that is being generalized here. It is only the regression that Price used that is being generalized.

      To be consistent with the field, I suggest the term "Price Equation" be used only to refer to the covariance form unless it is otherwise specified as in "regression form of the Price equation".

      I am not sure about the level of confusion induced here, but I totally see that it can be helpful to avoid all ambiguity. I therefore went over everything, and whenever I wrote “Price equation”, I tried to make sure it comes either with “in covariance form” or with “in regression form”. At some places, it is a bit over the top to keep repeating “in regression form”, when it is abundantly clear which form is being discussed. Also, I added no qualifiers if a statement is true for both forms of the Price equation, or if the claim refers to the whole package of going through Step 1 and Step 2 mentioned above.

      (3) Sample covariance: The author refers to the covariance in the Price equation as “sample covariance”. This is not correct, since sample covariance has a denominator of N-1 rather than N (Bessel’s correction). The correct term, when summing over an entire population, is “population covariance”. Price (1972) was clear about this: “In this paper we will be concerned with population functions and make no use of sample functions”. This point is elaborated on by Frank (2012), in the subsection “Interpretation of Covariance”.

      I totally agree. On page 418 of van Veelen (2005), I wrote:

      “Another possibility is that we think of 𝑧<sub>i</sub> and 𝑞<sub>i</sub>, 𝑖 = 1,…,𝑁 as realizations of a jointly distributed random variable. […] In that case the expression between square brackets is a good approximation for what statisticians […] call a sample covariance. A sample covariance is defined as but in large samples it is OK to replace 𝑁 − 1 by 𝑁, and then this formula reduces to Price’s 𝐶𝑜𝑣(𝑧, 𝑞).”

      In van Veelen et al. (2012), I slid a little, because in Box 1 on page 66, I wrote that is the sample covariance, and only in footnote 1 on the same page did I include Bessel’s correction, when I wrote:

      “To be perfectly precise, the sample covariance is defined as

      In this manuscript, I slid a little further, and left Bessel’s correction out altogether. I am happy that the reviewer pointed this out, so I can make this maximally precise again.

      The reviewer also quotes Price (1972), page 485:

      “In this paper we will be concerned with population functions and make no use of sample functions”.

      Below, the reviewer will return to the issue of distinguishing between the sample covariance with Bessel’s correction, and the sample covariance without Bessel’s correction, where the latter is regularly also referred to as the population covariance. A natural interpretation of the quote from Price (1972), if we read a bit around this quote in the paper, is that the difference between his “population functions” and his “sample functions” is indeed Bessel’s correction.

      The reviewer also states that Frank (2012) elaborates on this in the subsection “Interpretation of Covariance”. What is interesting, though, is that, when Frank (2012) writes, on page 1017 “It is important to distinguish between population measures and sample measures”, the difference between those is not that one does, and the other does not include Bessel’s correction. The difference between “population measures” and “sample measures” in Frank (2012), page 1017

      “It is important to distinguish between population measures and sample measures”,

      the difference between those is not that one does, and the other does not include Bessel’s correction. The difference between “population measures” and “sample measures” in Frank (2012), page 1017, is that

      “In many statistical applications, one only has data on a subset of the full population, that subset forming a sample.”

      The distinction between a population covariance and a sample covariance in Frank (2012) therefore is that they are “covariances” of different things (where the word covariances is in quotation marks, because, again, they are not really covariances). Besides just making sure that Price (1972) and Frank (2012) are not using these terms in the same way, this also perfectly illustrates the mix-up between statistical populations (or data generating processes) and biological populations that I discuss on pages 8 and 9 of Appendix A. I will return to this below, when I explain why I want to avoid using the word “population covariance” for the sample covariance without Bessel’s correction.

      Of course, the difference is negligible when the population is large. However, the author applies the covariance formula to populations as small as 𝑁 = 2, for which the correction factor is significant.

      Absolutely right.

      The author objects to using the term "population covariance" (SI, pp. 8-9) on the grounds that it might be misleading if the covariance, regression coefficients, etc. are used for inference because in this case, what is being inferred is not a population statistic but an underlying relationship. However, I am not convinced that statistical inference is or should be the primary use of the Price equation (see next point). At any rate, avoiding potential confusion is not a sufficient reason to use incorrect terminology.

      There are a few related, but separate issues. One is what to call the 𝐶𝑜𝑣(𝑤, 𝑝)-term. Another, somewhat broader, is to avoid mixing up statistical populations and biological populations. A third is what the primary use of the Price equation is. The third issue I will respond to below, where it reappears. Here I will focus on the first two, which can be discussed without addressing the third.

      In a data context, I now call the 𝐶𝑜𝑣(𝑤, 𝑝)-term “’" times the sample covariance, or, in other words, the sample covariance without Bessel’s correction”. This should be unambiguous. In a modeling context I refer to 𝐶𝑜𝑣(𝑤, 𝑝)-term as “the 𝐶𝑜𝑣(𝑤, 𝑝)-term” and describe it as a summary statistic or a notational convention. There are two reasons for this choice.

      The first is that neither of these use the word “population”. I like this, because there is a persistent scope for confusion between statistical populations and biological populations (as exemplified by Frank, 2012). This leads to an incorrect, but widespread intuition that if we “know the entire (biological) population” in a data context, there is nothing that can be estimated. This is what pages 8 and 9 of Appendix A are all about.

      The second reason is that by using two labels, I also differentiate between the data context and the modeling context. This is important for reasons I will return to later.

      Relatedly, I suggest avoiding using 𝐸 for the second term in the Price equation, since (as the ms points out), it is not the expectation of any random variable. It is a population mean. There is no reason not to use something like Avg or bar notation to indicate population mean. Price (1972) uses "ave" for average.

      I totally agree that the second term in the Price equation is not an expectation. I made this point in van Veelen (2005), and I repeated this in the manuscript. This remark by the reviewer prompted me to spell this out a bit more emphatically in Appendix A. That still leaves me with the choice what notation to use.

      I therefore looked up all contributions to the Theme issue “Fifty years of the Price equation” in the Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, and found that almost all contributions use 𝐸, sometimes saying that this refers to an expectation or an average. Of course, this is wrong. However (and this is another argument), it is equally wrong as using 𝐶𝑜𝑣 or 𝑉𝑎𝑟. The terms abbreviated as 𝐶𝑜𝑣 and 𝑉𝑎𝑟 are equally much not a covariance and a variance as the term abbreviated as 𝐸 is not an expectation. So I would think that there are a few reasons for sticking with 𝐸 here; 1) consistency with the literature; 2) consistency with the treatment of other terms; and 3) the fact that this term is not really of any importance in this manuscript. I do however totally understand the reviewer’s reasons, which I suppose include that for using 𝐸, there are relatively unproblematic alternatives (ave or upper bar) that are not available for the other terms. I hope therefore that being a bit more emphatic in the manuscript about 𝐸 not being an expectation at least partly addresses this concern.

      I should add, however, that the distinction between population statistics vs sample statistics goes away for regression coefficients (e.g. b, c, and r in Hamilton's rule) since in this case, Bessel's correction cancels out.

      Totally correct.

      (4) Descriptive vs. inferential statistics: When discussing the statistical quantities in the Price Equation, the author appears to treat them all as inferential statistics. That is, he takes the position that the population data are all generated by some probabilistic model and that the goal of computing the statistical quantities in the Price Equation is to correctly infer this model.

      Before I respond to this, I would like to point out that this literature has started going off the rails right from the very beginning. One of the initial construction errors was to use the ungeneralized Price equation in regression form. The other one is that the paper in which Price (1970) presented his equation is inconsistent, and suggests that the equation can be used for constructing hypotheses and for testing them at the same time (see van Veelen (2005), page 416). That, of course, is not possible; the first happens in the theory/modeling domain, and the second in the empirical testing/statistics domain, and they are separate exercises.

      These construction errors have warped the literature based on it, and have resulted in a lot of mental gymnastics and esoteric statements, which are needed if we are not willing to consider the possibility that there could be anything amiss with the original paper by Price (1970).

      In this paper, I undo both of these construction errors. Undoing the second one means exploring both domains separately. In Sections 2-4 of Appendix A I explore the possibility that the Price equation is applied to data. In Section 5 of Appendix A I explore the possibility that it is used in a modelling context. The primary effort here is just to do it right, and I have not read anything to suggest that I did not succeed in doing this. Secondarily, of course, I also want to contrast this to what happens in the existing literature. That is what this point by the reviewer is about. It is therefore important to be aware that seeing the contrast accurately is complicated by the apologetic warp in the existing literature.

      As a first effort to unwarp, I would like to point to the fact that I am not taking any position on what the Price equation should be used for. All I do here is explore (and find) possibilities, both in the statistical inference domain and in the modeling domain. I also find that there is scope for misspecification in both, and that, in both domains, we should want to avoid misspecification. The thing that I criticize in the existing literature therefore is not the choice of domain. The thing that I criticize is the insistence on, and celebrating of what is most accurately described as misspecification. This typically happens in the modeling domain.

      It is worth pointing out that those who argue in favor of the Price Equation do not see it this way: "it is a mistake to assume that it must be the evolutionary theorist, writing out covariances, who is performing the equivalent of a statistical analysis." (Gardner, West, and Wild, 2011); "Neither data nor inferences are considered here" (Rousset, 2015). From what I can tell, to the supporters of the Price equation and the regression form of Hamilton's rule, the statistical quantities involved are either population-level *descriptive* statistics (in an empirical context), or else are statistics of random variables (in a stochastic modeling context).

      Again, this description of the friction between my paper and the existing literature is predicated on the suggestion that I have only one domain in mind where the Price equation can be applied. That is not the case; I consider both.

      In the previous paragraph, the reviewer states that I “treat statistical quantities as inferential statistics”, and in this paragraph the reviewer contrasts that with the supporters of the (ungeneralized) Price equation that supposedly treat the same quantities as “descriptive statistics”. This is also beside the point, but it will take some effort to sort out the spaghetti of entangled arguments (where the spaghetti is the result of the history in this field, as indicated earlier).

      First of all, it is not unimportant to point out that the way most people use the terms “inferential statistics” and “descriptive statistics” is that the first refers to an activity, and the second to a function of a bunch of numbers, typically data. Inferential statistics is a combination of parameter estimation and model specification (those are activities). Descriptive statistics are for instance the average values of variables of interest (which makes them a function of a set of numbers). When doing inferential statistics (or statistical inference), looking at the descriptive statistics of the dataset is just a routine before the real work begins. It is important to remember that.

      Now I suppose that this reviewer uses these words a little differently. When he or she writes that I “treat statistical quantities as inferential statistics”, I assume that the reviewer means that I want to use a term like for doing statistical inference, or that, when I want to interpret such a term, I include considerations typical of statistical inference. Within the data domain, that is totally correct. In the paper I argue that there are very good reasons for this. We would like to know what the data can tell us about the actual fitness function, and if we do our statistical inference right, and choose our Price-like equation accordingly, then that means that we would be able to give a meaningful interpretation to a term like . It also means that we then have an equation that describes the genetic population dynamics accurately.

      When the reviewer states that other papers treat them as “population level descriptive statistics” in an empirical context, I have a hard time coming up with papers for which that is the case. Most papers apply the Price equation in the modeling domain (That is to say: this is true in evolution. In ecology the Price equation is often applied to data; see Pillai and Gouhier (2019) and Bourrat et al. (2023)). But even if there are researchers that apply the Price equation to data, then considering these statistical quantities as “descriptive statistics” would not make sense. Looking at the descriptive statistics alone is not an empirical exercise; it is just a routine that happens before the actual statistical inference starts. In a data context, saying that considerations that are standard in statistical inference do not apply, because one is just not doing statistical inference, is the equivalent of an admission of guilt. If you do not consider statistical significance, and never mention that sample size could matter, because you are using these terms as “descriptive statistics, not inferential statistics”, then you’re basically admitting to not doing a serious empirical study.

      Besides treating statistical quantities as descriptive statistics in a data context, the reviewer also states that, in a stochastic modeling context, other researchers treat the same statistical quantities as “statistics of random variables”. This is first of all very generous to the existing literature. I imagine that the reviewer is imagining a modeling exercise where for instance the covariance between two variables is postulated. A theory exercise would then take that as a starting point for the derivation of some theoretical result. This, however, is not what happens in most of the literature.

      There are two things that I would like to point out. First of all, postulating covariances and deriving results from assumptions regarding those covariances is not an activity that requires using the Price equation. There are many stochastic models that function perfectly fine without the Price equation. This is maybe a detail, but it is important to realize that what the reviewer probably thinks of as a legitimate theoretical exercise may be something that can very well be done without the Price equation.

      Secondly, I would like to repeat something that I have pointed out before, which is that the Price equation can be written for any transition, whether this transition is likely or unlikely, given a model, and even for transitions that are impossible. For all of those transitions, one can write the (ungeneralized) Price equation, and for all of those, the Price equation will be an identity, and it will contain the things that the reviewer refers to as “statistical quantities”. It is important to realize that these “statistical quantities”, therefore, are properties of a transition, and that every transition comes with its own ”statistical quantity”. That implies that they are not properties of random variables; they reflect something regarding one transition. What one could imagine, though, is the following. To fix ideas, let’s take the Price equation in regression form, and focus on . A meaningful modeling exercise starts with assumptions about the likelihood of all different transitions, and therefore the likelihood of different values of 𝛽 materializing – or it starts with assumptions that imply those probabilities. In a theoretical exercise, one could then derive statements about the expectation and variance of those “statistical quantities”. For instance, one can calculate the expected value 𝐸[𝛽] =𝐸, and the variance 𝑉𝑎𝑟[𝛽] = 𝑉𝑎𝑟 , where this expectation is a proper expectation (taken over the probabilities with which these transitions materialize) and this variance is a proper variance, for the same reason.

      This is what I do on page 416 of van Veelen (2005) and in Section 5 of Appendix A. I think something like this is what the reviewer may have in mind, but it is worth pointing out that this still does not mean that the from the Price equation for any given transition is now a property of a random variable. Much of the literature, however, is not at the level of sophistication that I imagine the reviewer has in mind – although there are papers that are; see the discussion below of Rousset and Billiard (2000) and Van Cleve (2015).

      In the appendix to this reply, I will address the quotes from Gardner, West, and Wild (2011) and Rousset (2015). This takes up some space, so that is why it is at the end of this reply.

      In short, the manuscript seems to argue that Price equation users are performing statistical inference incorrectly, whereas the users insist that they are not doing statistical inference at all.

      That is not what the manuscript argues, but I am happy to clarify. The manuscript explores both the use of the Price equation when applied to data (and therefore for statistical inference) and when applied to transitions in a model. The criticism on the existing literature is not that it performs statistical inference incorrectly. The criticism is that the literature insists on misspecification, which typically happens in a modelling context.

      The problem (and here I think the author would agree with me) arises when users of the Price equation go on to make predictive or causal claims that would require the kind of statistical analysis they claim not to be doing. Claims of the form "Hamilton's rule predicts.." or use of terms like "benefit" and "cost" suggest that one has inferred a predictive or causal relationship in the given data, while somehow bypassing the entire theory of statistical inference.

      I do not really know how to interpret this paragraph. The use of the word “data” suggests that this pertains to a data context, but I do not know what would qualify as a “predictive claim” in that domain, or how any study would go from data to a claim of the form “Hamilton’s rule predicts …”. Again, I do not really know papers that apply the Price equation to data. None of the empirical papers reviewed in Bourke (2014) for instance do. I would however agree that it is close to obvious that an approach that does indeed bypass the entire theory of statistical inference cannot identify causal relations in datasets. I think the examples in Section 2 of Appendix A also clearly illustrate that a literature in which the word “sample size” is absent, cannot be doing statistical inference.

      There is also a third way to use the Price equation which is entirely unobjectionable: as a way to express the relationship between individual-level fitness and population-level gene frequency change in a form that is convenient for further algebraic manipulation. I suspect that this is actually the most common use of the Price equation in practice.

      I am not sure if I understand what it means for the Price equation to “express the relationship between individual-level fitness and population-level gene frequency change”. That is a bit reminiscent of how John Maynard Smith saw the Price equation (Okasha, 2005), but he also emphasized that he was unable to follow George Price and his equation. For sure, it cannot be that one side of the Price equation reflects something at the individual level and the other something at the population level, because both sides of the Price equation are equally aggregated over the population. Just to be safe, and to avoid unwarranted associative thinking, I would therefore choose to be minimalistic, and say that the Price equation is an identity for a transition between a parent population and an offspring population.

      Regardless of the words we choose, however, the question how harmless or objectionable the use of the Price equation is in the literature is absolutely relevant. In earlier papers I have tried to cover a spectrum of examples of different ways to use (or misuse) the Price equation. In van Veelen (2005) I cover Grafen (1985a), Taylor (1989), Price (1972), and Sober and Wilson (2007). The main paper that is discussed in van Veelen et al. (2012) is Queller (1992b), but Section 7 of that paper also discusses the way the Price equation is used in Rousset and Billiard (2000), Taylor (1989), Queller (1985), and Page and Nowak (2002). These discussions also come with a description of how much it takes to repair them, and this varies all the way from nothing, or a bit of minor rewording, to being beyond repair.

      What is good to observe, is that the papers in which the use of the Price equation is the least problematic, are also the papers in which, if the reference to the Price equation would be taken out, nothing really changes. These are papers that start with a model, or a collection of models, and that, at some point in the derivation of their results, point to a step that can, but does not have to be described as using the Price equation. An example of this is Rousset and Billiard (2000); see the detailed description in Section 7 of van Veelen et al. (2012).

      I am happy to point to a few more papers on the no harm, no foul end of the spectrum here.

      Allen and Tarnita (2012) discuss properties of the dynamics in a well-defined set of models.

      Towards the end of the paper, a version of the Price equation more or less naturally appears. This is more of an interesting aside, though, and does not really play a role in derivation of the core results of the paper. Van Cleve (2015) is similar to Rousset and Billiard (2000), in that the “application of the Price equation” there is a minor ingredient of the derivation of the results. (A detail that this reviewer may find worth mentioning, given earlier comments, is that Van Cleve (2015) writes the left-hand side of the Price equation as 𝐸(𝑤Δ𝑝|𝐩), instead of . First two very unimportant things. Van Cleve (2015) uses 𝑤 for mean fitness, for which is a more common symbol. Another detail of lesser importance is that it includes the vector of parent p-scores in the notation, which in their notation is 𝐩. More importantly, however, is that Van Cleve (2015) writes 𝐸(Δ𝑝) for , which extends the (mis)use of the symbol 𝐸 for what really is just an average. This is consistent within the Price equation, in the sense that it now denotes the average with 𝐸, both on the right-hand side and on the left-hand side of the Price equation. It can however be a little bit confusing, because when Rousset and Billiard (2000) write , then this is a proper expectation. In their case, this summarizes all possible transitions out of a given state, and weighs them by their probabilities of happening, given a state summarized by 𝑝.). I am also happy to extend the spectrum a bit here. Some papers on inclusive fitness do not use the Price equation at all, even though one could imagine places where it could be inserted. A nice example of such a paper is Taylor et al. (2007).

      In this paper, I hope I can be excused from taking a complete inventory of this literature, and I hope that I do not have to count how many papers fall into the different categories. This would help assess the veracity of the suspicion the reviewer has, which is that the most common use of the Price equation is entirely unobjectionable, but I just do not have the time. I would however not want to underestimate the aggregate damage done in this field. The spectrum spanned in my earlier papers does include a fair amount of nonsense results. This typically happens in papers that do not study a specific model or set of models, but that take the Price equation as their point of departure for their theorizing. Also there seems to be a positive correlation between how exalted and venerating the language is that is used when describing the wonders and depths of the Price equation, and how little sense the claims make that are “derived” with it.

      We also should not set the bar too low. This is a literature that, at the starting point, has a few construction errors in it, as described in the paper. That is reason for concern. Moreover, one of the main end products of this literature is what we send our empiricists to the field with. As Section 8 of van Veelen et al. (2017) indicates, what we have supplied to our empiricists to work with is nothing short of terrible. I would therefore want to maintain that the damage done is enormous, and if there are also a few papers around that may use the ungeneralized Price equation in an innocuous way, then that is not enough redemption for my taste. We are still facing a literature in which, at every instance where the Price equation is used, we still need to check in which category it falls.

      For a paper that aims to clarify these thorny concepts in the literature, I think it is worth pointing out these different interpretations of statistical quantities in the Price equation (descriptive statistics vs inferential statistics vs algebraic manipulation). One can then critique the conclusions that are inappropriately drawn from the Price equation, which would require rigorous statistical inference to draw. Without these clarifications, supporters of the Price equation will again argue that this manuscript has misunderstood the purpose of the equation and that they never claimed to do inference in the first place.

      I would like to return to the point that I made at the beginning of my response to point (4), which is that the “thorniness” of these concepts is the result of the warp in the literature, resulting from the construction errors in Price (1970). If people want to understand how to apply the Price equation right, I think that reading Appendix A and B would work just fine. Again, I have not read anything that suggests that there is anything incorrect in there, so if the literature contains “thorny” concepts, it might just be that this is the result of the mental gymnastics necessitated by the unwillingness to accept that there might be something not completely right with Price (1970). Moreover, given my experiences in the field, I am not sure that there is anything that I could say that would convince the supporters of the ungeneralized Price equation.

      (5) "True" models: Even if one accepts that the statistical quantities in the Price equation are inferential in nature, the author appears to go a step further by asserting that, even in empirical populations, there is a specific "true" model which it is our goal to infer. This assumption manifests at many points in the SI when the author refers to the "true model" or "true, underlying population structure" in the context of an empirical population.

      Again, in Appendix A I explore both a data context and a modeling context. In the modeling context none of this applies, because in such a context, there is only the model that we postulate. In the part in which I explore what the Price equation can do in a data context, I do indeed use words like “true model” or "true underlying population structure".  

      I do not think it is necessary or appropriate, in empirical contexts, to posit the existence of a Platonic "true" model that is generating the data. Real populations are not governed by mathematical models. Moreover, the goal of statistical inference is not to determine the "true model" for given data but to say whether a given statistical model is justified based on this data. Fitting a linear model, for example, does not rule out the possibility there may be higher-order interactions - it just means we do not have a statistical basis to infer these higher-order interactions from the data (say, because their p-scores are insignificant), and so we leave them out.

      This remark suggests that the statistical approach in Sections 2-4 of Appendix A is more naïve than it should be, and that I would overlook the possibility of, for instance, interaction effects that are really nonzero, but that are statistically not significant. Now first of all, at a superficial level, I would like to say that this strikes me as somewhat inconsistent. In the remarks further back, the reviewer seems to excuse those that use the Price equation on data without any statistical considerations whatsoever. The reason why the reviewer is giving them a pass, is that they are “just not doing statistical inference”. Instead, they are doing this whole other thing with, you know, descriptive statistics. As I indicated above, that is just a fancy way of saying that they are not doing serious statistics – or serious empirics, for that matter.

      In this comment, on the other hand, the reviewer also suggests that the statistics that I use to replace the total absence of any statistical considerations with, is not quite up to snuff. Below, I will indicate why that is not the case at all, but I think it is also worth registering a touch of irony there.

      In order to address this issue, it is worth first observing that the whole of classical statistics is based on probability theory in the following sense. We are always asking ourselves the question: if the data generating process works like this, what would the likelihood be of certain outcomes (datasets); and if the data generating process works some other way (sometimes: the complement of whatever “this” is), what would the likelihood then be of the same outcomes. By comparing those, we draw inferences about the underlying data generating process (which is a word suggestive of a “Platonic” world view that the reviewer seems to reject). Therefore, if one would impose a ban on using Platonic words like “true data generating process”; “actual fitness function”; or “the population structure that is out there”, it would be impossible to teach any course in statistics, basic or advanced. Also it would be impossible to practice, and talk about, applied statistics.

      Now the reviewer claims that “Real populations are not governed by mathematical models”. I do not really know if I agree or disagree with that statement, but the example that the reviewer gives does not fit that claim. The reviewer suggests that if we find a higher order term not to be statistically significant (and therefore we reject the hypothesis that it is nonzero), then that would not necessarily mean that it is not there. That is totally true, and statisticians tend to be fully aware of that. But that does not imply that there is no true data-generating process; the whole premise of this example is that there is, but that the sample size is not large enough to determine it in a detailed enough way so as to include this interaction effect, that apparently is small relative to the sample size.

      The third thing to reflect on here, is that the reviewer seems to suggest that the Generalized Price equation in regression form, as presented in my paper, comes with a specific statistical approach, that he or she classifies as philosophically naïve or unsophisticated. That, however, is not the case, and I am very grateful that this remark by this reviewer allows me to make a point that I think shines a light on how the Generalized Price equation puts the train that started going off the rails in 1970 back on track, and reconnects it with the statistics it borrows its terminology from. To see that, it is good to be aware that statistics never gives certainty. The whole discipline is built around the awareness that it is possible to draw the wrong inference, and the aim is to determine, minimize, and balance, the likelihoods of making different wrong inferences. So, statistics produces statements about the confidence with which one can say that something works one way or the other. In some instances, the data are not enough to say anything with any confidence. In other cases, the data are rich enough so that it is really unlikely that we incorrectly infer that for instance a certain gene matters for fitness.

      The nice thing about the setup with the Generalized Price equation, is that those statistical considerations translate one-to-one to considerations regarding which Price-like equation to choose. If the data do not allow us to pick any model with confidence, then we should be equally agnostic about which Price-like equation describes the population genetic dynamics accurately. If the statistics gives us high confidence that a certain model matches the data, then we should pick the matching Price-like equation with the same confidence. This also carries over to higher level statistical considerations.

      If we think about terms that, if we would gather a gargantuan amount of data, might be statistically significant, but very small, then economists call those statistically significant, but economically insignificant. When rejecting the statistical significance on the basis of a not gargantuan dataset, statisticians are aware that terms that really have a zero effect, as well as terms, the effect of which is really small, are rejected with the same statistical test – and that we should be fine with that. All such considerations carry over to what we think of regarding the choice of a Price-like equation to describe the population genetic dynamics. Even if people disagree about whether or not to include a term that is statistically significant, but relatively small, such a disagreement can still happen within this setup, and just translates to a disagreement on which Price-like equation to choose.

      Similarly, people could also disagree about whether it is justified to use polynomials to characterize a fitness function. If we decide that we can, because of Taylor expansions, then the core result of the paper implies that the population genetic dynamics can be summarized by a generalized Hamilton’s rule (as long as the fitness function includes a constant and a linear term regarding the p-score). On the other hand, if we do not believe this is justified, and prefer to use an altogether different family of fitness functions, then we can no longer do this. All of this leaves space for all kinds of statistical considerations and disagreements, that just carry over to the choice for one or the other Price-like equation as an accurate description of the population genetic dynamics. Or, if one does not believe polynomials should be used, then this leads to not picking any Price-like equation at all.

      So, this is a long way of saying that the Generalized Price equation creates space for all statistical considerations to regain their place, and does not hinge on one approach to statistics or another.

      What we can say is that if we apply the statistical model to data generated by a probabilistic model, and if these models match, then as the number of observations grows to infinity, the estimators in the statistical model converge to the parameters of the data-generating one.

      But this is a mathematical statement, not a statement about real-world populations.

      Again, I do not know if I agree or disagree with the last sentence. However, that does not really matter, because either option only has implications for how we are to think of the relation between a Price-like equation describing a population genetic dynamics and real-world populations. It is not relevant for the question which Price-like equation to pick, or whether to pick one at all.

      A resolution I suggest to points 3, 4, and 5 above is:

      *A priori, the statistical quantities in the Price Equation are descriptive statistics, pertaining only to the specific population data given.

      *If one wishes to impute any predictive power, generalizability, or causal meaning to these statistics, all the standard considerations of inferential statistics apply. In particular, one must choose a statistical model that is justified based on the given data. In this case, one is not guaranteed to obtain the standard (linear) Hamilton's rule and may obtain any of an infinite family of rules.

      *If one uses a model that is not justified based on the given data, the results will still be correct for the given population data but will lack any meaning or generalizability beyond that.

      *In particular, if one considers data generated by a probabilistic model, and applies a statistical model that does not match the data-generating one, the results will be misleading, and will not generalize beyond the randomly generated realization one uses.

      Of course, the author may propose a different resolution to points 3-5, but they should be resolved somehow. Otherwise, the terminology in the manuscript will be incorrect and the ms will not resolve confusion in the field.

      I have outlined my solutions extensively above. I really appreciate that Reviewers #1 and #2 have spent time and attention on the manuscript and on the long appendices.  

      Appendix to the response to reviewer #2: Some remarks on Gardner, West & Wild (2011), Frank (2012), and Rousset (2015)

      An accurate response to the quote from Gardner, West, and Wild (2011) in the review report takes up space. I therefore wanted to put that in an appendix to the response to reviewer #2. I also include a few paragraphs regarding Frank (2012) and Rousset (2015), both of which are also mentioned by reviewer #2. All of this might also be of interest to people that are curious about how what I find in my paper relates to the existing literature.

      Gardner, West & Wild (2011) The quote I am responding to is “it is a mistake to assume that it must be the evolutionary theorist, writing out covariances, who is performing the equivalent of a statistical analysis” I want to put that into context, so I will go over the whole paragraph that surrounds the quote. The paragraph is called Statistics and Evolutionary Theory and can be found on page 1038 of the paper. I think that it is worth pointing out that it is not easy to respond to their somewhat impressionistic collages of words and formulas. I will therefore cut the paragraph up in a few smaller bits and try to make sense of it bit by bit. The paragraph begins with:

      “Our account of the general theory of kin selection has been framed in statistical terms.” Based on what they write two sentences down, the best match between those words and what they do in the paper would be: “our account uses words like “covariance”, “variance” and “expectation” for things that are not what “covariance”, “variance” and “expectation” mean in probability theory and statistics.” I would be totally open to an argument why that is nonetheless OK to do, but the way Gardner, West, and Wild (2011) phrase it obscures the fact that this needs any justification or reflection at all. “Framing something in statistical terms” is unspecific enough to sound completely harmless.

      “The use of statistical methods in the mathematical development of Darwinian theory has itself been subjected to recent criticism (van Veelen, 2005; Nowak et al., 2010b), so we address this criticism here.

      Also here, specifics would be helpful. The “use of statistical methods” sounds like it is more than just using terms from statistics, so this might refer to the minimizing of the sum of squared differences, which is also mentioned a sentence down in Gardner, West, and Wild (2011). If it does, then it is worth observing that in statistics, the minimizing of the sum of squared differences (or residuals, or errors) comes with theorems that point very clearly to what is being achieved by doing this. The Gauss–Markov theorem states that the ordinary least squares (OLS) estimator has the lowest variance within the class of linear unbiased estimators. This implies that minimizing the sum of squared errors helps answering a well-defined question in statistics; under certain conditions, an OLS estimator is our best shot at uncovering an unknown relation between variables. To also minimize a sum of squared differences, but now in the modeling domain, qualifies as “use of statistical methods” only in a very shallow way. It means that a similar minimization is performed. Without an equivalent of the Gauss-Markov theorem that would shine a light on what it is that is being achieved by doing so, that does not carry the same weight as it does in the statistics domain – in that it does not carry any weight at all.

      “The concern is that statistical terms – such as covariances and least-squares regressions – should properly be reserved for conventional statistical analyses, where hypotheses are tested against explicit data, and that they are out of place in the foundations of evolutionary theory (van Veelen, 2005; Nowak et al., 2010b).”

      Again, a few things are a bit vague. What are “explicit data”? Are there data that are not explicit? Why the generic “foundations of evolutionary theory”, instead of a more specific description of what these statistical terms are used for? But either way, this is a misrepresentation of what I wrote in van Veelen (2005). I did not suggest to “reserve statistical terms for conventional statistical analysis” just because. As I do here in the current paper, what I did there was explore the possibilities for the Price equation to help with what I then called Type I and Type II questions. Type I questions find themselves in the modeling domain and Type II questions find themselves in the statistical domain. I was not arguing for a ban on applying statistical concepts outside of the domain of statistical inference. All that I said is that in its current practice, it does not really help answering questions of either type.  

      “However, this concern is misplaced. First, natural selection is a statistical process, and it is therefore natural that this should be defined in terms of aggregate statistics, even if only strictly by analogy (Frank, 1997a, 1998).”

      This is a vague non-argument. Almost nothing is well-defined here. What does it mean for natural selection to be a statistical process? Is that just an unusual term for a random process? If so, then I suppose I agree, but that has nothing to do with what I state or claim. And what does it mean to be defined in terms of aggregate statistics? What is the alternative? I have no idea how any of this relates to anything that I claim or state in my papers.

      “Second, Fisher (1930, p198) coined the term ‘covariance’ in the context of his exposition of the genetical theory of natural selection, so the evolutionary usage of this term has precedent over the way the term is used in other fields.”

      This is what I would call a “historic fallacy”. The fact that Fisher coined the term “covariance” in a book on genetics and natural selection does not mean that any “evolutionary usage” of the term “covariance”, however nonsensical, now has precedent over the way the term is used in other fields. Irrespective of the path that the history of science, genetics, or statistics took, right now we are in a place where about every student at every university anywhere in the world that takes a course in probability theory and/or statistics, learns that covariance is a property of a random variable (see also Wikipedia). And they do for a very good reason; it is essential in recognizing the relation between probability theory on the one hand and statistics on the other. Being curious how this “evolutionary usage” of the term covariance works, if covariance turns out not to be a property of a random variable, is therefore perfectly justified, and “Fisher coined the term” is not a safe word that exempts it from scrutiny. 

      Third, it is a mistake to assume that it must be the evolutionary theorist, writing out covariances, who is performing the equivalent of a statistical analysis.

      Again, that is just not what anyone is saying. Nobody is suggesting that an evolutionary theorist should perform the equivalent of statistical analysis. All I did was point to how little is being achieved by transferring formulas from statistics to a modeling context.

      A better analogy is to regard Mother Nature in the role of statistician, analysing fitness effects of genes by the method of least-squares, and driving genetic change according to the results of her analyses (cf. Crow, 2008).

      I have no idea what any of this means. Mother Nature is a personification of something that is not a person, and that does not have cognition. Without sentience, “Mother Nature” cannot assume the role of statistician, and cannot analyse fitness effects.

      More generally, analogy is the basis of all understanding, so when isomorphisms arise unexpectedly between different branches of mathematics (in this case, theoretical population genetics and statistical least-squares analysis) this represents an opportunity for advancing scientific progress and not an anomaly that is to be avoided.

      This is a strawman argument, puffed up with platitudes. Nobody is arguing against analogies. But what is the analogy supposed to be here? Just taking least squares from statistical inference and performing it in a modeling context does not make it an analogy. The GaussMarkov theorem, which is the basis for why least squares helps answering questions in statistics, just does not mean anything in a modeling context. OLS in modeling is just willful misspecification, and nothing that it does in statistics translates to anything meaningful in modeling. Again, declaring it an analogy, or an isomorphism, does not make it one.

      Frank (2012) Because the reviewer also mentions Frank (2012), I would like to include a small remark on this paper too. “Natural Selection. IV. The Price equation” by Frank (2012) is partly a response to my earlier criticism of the use of the Price equation. Much like Gardner, West, and Wild (2011), I would describe this paper as what is called a ”flight forwards” in Dutch. While the questions I ask are relatively prosaic (such as: how does the Price equation help derive a prediction from model assumptions?), Frank (2012) pivots to suggesting that there is a profound philosophy-of-science disagreement that I am on the wrong side of. It is close to impossible to respond to Frank (2012), because it is a labyrinth of arguments that sound deep and impressive, but that are just not specific enough to know how they relate to points that I made – or even just what they mean in general. Just to pick a random paragraph:

      “Is there some reorientation for the expression of natural selection that may provide subtle perspective, from which we can understand our subject more deeply and analyse our problems with greater ease and greater insight? My answer is, as I have mentioned, that the Price equation provides that sort of reorientation. To argue the point, I will have to keep at the distinction between the concrete and the abstract, and the relative roles of those two endpoints in mature theoretical understanding.”

      For many of those terms, I have no real idea what they mean, and also reading the rest of the paper does not help understanding what this has to do with the more prosaic questions that are waiting for an answer. What is “reorientation”? What does “concrete” versus “abstract” have to do with the question what is being achieved by doing least squares regressions in modeling? What would be an example of a mature and an immature theoretical understanding?

      Rousset (2015) is also mentioned by the reviewer. This paper is not esoteric. It states, as reviewer #2 points out, that "neither data nor inferences are considered". This paper therefore finds itself in the modeling domain, and not in the data domain. It does however still dodge the question what the benefits are of misspecification in the modeling domain. As a matter of fact, it denies that there is misspecification at all.

      “In the presence of synergies, the residuals have zero mean and are uncorrelated to the predictors. No further assumption is made about the distribution of the residuals. Thus, there is no sense in which the regression is misspecified.”

      This is a remarkable quote, and testament to the lasting impact of the construction errors in Price (1970). Misspecification is literally defined as getting the model wrong. In statistics, avoiding misspecification can be complicated, because of the noise in the data. The real datagenerating process is unknown, and because of the noise, there is always the possibility that data that are generated by one model look like they could also have been generated by another. The challenge is to reduce the odds of getting the model wrong to acceptable proportions, which is what statistical tests are for. But in modeling, we know what the model is; it is postulated by the modeler. Therefore, misspecification can be avoided by just not replacing it with a different model.

      What is being discussed in this part of Rousset (2015) is replacing what in this manuscript is called Model 3 (𝑤<sub>𝑖</sub> = 𝛼 + 𝛽<sub>1,0</sub>𝑝<sub>𝑖</sub> + 𝛽<sub>1,1</sub>𝑝<sub>𝑖</sub> + 𝛽<sub>1,1</sub>𝑝<sub>𝑖</sub>𝑞<sub>𝑖</sub> + 𝜀<sub>𝑖</sub>) with Model 2 (𝑤<sub>𝑖</sub> = 𝛼 + 𝛽<sub>1,0</sub>𝑝<sub>𝑖</sub>+ 𝛽<sub>1,0</sub>𝑝<sub>𝑖</sub>𝑞<sub>𝑖</sub> + 𝜀<sub>𝑖</sub>), and choosing the parameters in Model 2 so that it is as close as it can be to Model

      (3) This is just the definition of misspecification. That is to say: the misspecification part is the choosing of Model 2 as a reference model. The minimizing of the sum of squared residuals one could consider as minimizing the damage.

      While Rousset (2015) finds itself in the modeling domain, it does nonetheless point to the field of statistics here, by stating that “the residuals have zero mean and are uncorrelated to the predictors”. From this, the paper concludes that “there is no sense in which the regression is misspecified”. That is just plain wrong. Minimizing the sum of the squared residuals guarantees that the residuals are uncorrelated with the variables that are included in the reference model, with respect to which the squared sum of residuals is minimized. The criterion that Rousset (2015) uses is that the model is well-specified if there is no correlation between the residuals (here: ) and the variables included in the reference model (here: 𝑝<sub>𝑖</sub> and 𝑞<sub>𝑖</sub>). But according to this criterion, all models would always be well-specified, and no model could ever be misspecified. The correct criterion, however, also requires that the residuals are not correlated with variables not included in the reference model. And here, the residuals are in fact correlated with 𝑝<sub>𝑖</sub>𝑞<sub>𝑖</sub>, which is the variable that is included in Model 3, but not in Model 2. Therefore, according to the correct version of this criterion, this model is in fact misspecified – as it should be, because getting the model wrong is the definition of misspecification.

      In order to make sure that there can be no misunderstanding, I have added subsections at the end of Section 2 and Section 4 of Appendix A, and at the end of Section 2 of Appendix B. These subsections show that the algebra of minimizing the sum of squared errors implies that there is no correlation between the errors, or the residuals, and the variables that are included in the model. This is by no means something new; it is the reason why we do OLS to begin with. For additional details about misspecification, I would refer to Section 1b (viii) in van Veelen (2020).

      Finally, there is a detail worth noticing. In the main text, as well as in Appendix B, I use an analogy (and, unlike what Gardner, West, and Wild, 2011, refer to as an analogy, this actually is one). This is an analogy between two choices. On the one hand, there is the choice between Price-like equation 1 (based on Model 1 as a reference model) and Price-like equation 2 (based on Model 2 as a reference model) both applied to Model 2. On the other hand, there is the choice between Price-like equation 2 (based on Model 2 as a reference model) and Price-like equation 3 (based on Model 3 as a reference model) both applied to Model 3. Model 1 is the non-social model, Model 2 is the social model without interaction term, and Model 3 is the social model with interaction term. That makes the first choice a choice between treating a social model as a social model, or as a non-social model. The second choice is between treating a social model with interaction term as a social model with interaction term, or as a social model without interaction term. The power of this analogy is that every argument against treating the social model as if it is a non-social model is also an argument against treating the social model with interaction term as if it is a social model without interaction term.

      This ties in with the incorrect criterion for when a model is well-specified from Rousset (2015) as follows. His criterion (that there should be no correlation between the residuals and the variables in the model) declares the social model without interaction term well-specified as a reference model, when we are considering a social model with interaction term. According to the same criterion, however, the non-social model would also have to be declared to be wellspecified as a reference model, when the model we are considering is a social model. The reason is that also here, there is no correlation between the residuals and the variables that are included in this model. This is clearly not what anyone is advocating for, and for good reasons. The residuals here would, after all, be correlated with the p-score of the partner, which is a variable that is not included in the non-social model. This is a good indication that we should not use the non-social model for a social trait.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Before responding to this review, I would like to express that I appreciate the fact that the reviews and the responses are public at eLife. Besides just being useful in general, this also allows readers to get a behind the scenes glimpse into the state of the field, and the level of the reviewing. While the reports by Reviewers #1 and #2 show openness and an interest in getting things right, the report by Reviewer #3 is representative of the many review reports that I have received from the inclusive fitness community in the past. These reports tend to be rhetorically strong, and to those who do not have the time to dig deeper in the details, these reports are probably also convincing. I will therefore go through this review line by line to show how little there is behind the confident off-hand dismissal.

      There is an interesting mathematical connection - an "isomorphism"-between Price's equation and least-squares linear regression.

      This is esoteric and needlessly vague. Why is the word “isomorphism” used? In mathematics, an isomorphism is a structure-preserving mapping. The Price equation is an equation, or an identity, which makes it a bit difficult to imagine what the set of objects is on one end of the mapping. Least-squares linear regression can perhaps be seen as a function of a dataset, which would make it a single object (one function). This complicates things at the other end of the mapping too, if that set is a singleton set. The only isomorphism that I can think of is a trivial isomorphism where one equation is mapped onto one function and vice versa. It seems unlikely that this is what the reviewer means. The word isomorphism moreover is in quotes, so maybe this is supposed to be figurative. But what would it be that is being suggested here by this figure of speech? Just saying that there is, as the reviewer puts it, an “interesting mathematical connection”, does not make it so. It would already be a start to just specify what the mathematical connection is, because I have a hard time seeing what that would be. Is it just that, if you divide the Cov(𝑤, 𝑝)-term by the Var(𝑝)-term, then you get a regression coefficient? If that is what the reviewer has in mind, that would be a rather shallow observation.

      Some people have misinterpreted this connection as meaning that there is a generalitylimiting assumption of linearity within Price's equation, and hence that Hamilton's rule-which is derived from Price's equation-provides only an approximation of the action of natural selection.

      Here, the reviewer pulls a switcheroo. The use of the word “general”, or “generality”, here refers to the fact that the classical Price equation is an identity for all possible transitions between a parent and an offspring population. This is the sense in which the inclusive fitness literature uses the word general, and so do I in the relevant places in the manuscript. When I do, I make sure to add phrases like “in the sense that whatever the true model is, it always gets the direction of selection right”. As a consequence, the classical Hamilton’s rule is also totally general, in the same sense.

      One of the core points of the paper is that this is not unique to the classical Price equation. As a matter of fact, there is a large set of Price-like equations and Hamilton-like rules that are equally much identities, and equally much general (in the sense that they get the direction of selection right for all possible transitions). The being an identity and being completely general (in this sense) therefore cannot be a decisive criterion in favour of the classical Price equation and the classical Hamilton’s rule.

      On the other hand, the way in which my Generalized Price equation and my generalized version of Hamilton’s rule are general, is that they do not restrict the statistical model with respect to which errors are squared, summed and minimized to one linear statistical model. This generalization generates the variety of Price-like equations and Hamilton-like rules mentioned above (all of which are general in the sense of always getting the direction of selection right) and it gives us the flexibility to pick one that separates terms that reflect the fitness function from terms that reflect the population state.

      In response to my generalizing the Price equation and Hamilton’s rule in this second sense, the criticism of the reviewer comes down to saying that the Price equation and Hamilton’s rule do not need generalizing, because they already are general – the switcheroo being that this refers to generality in the first sense. That makes it sound like this could be an honest mistake, confusing one way in which these can be described as general with another. However, I really hammered this point home in the manuscript. Even a cursory reading of the manuscript reveals that I am fully aware that the classical Price equation and the classical Hamilton’s rule are general in the first sense.

      It is also not helpful that, as a description of what I supposedly claim, this is impressionistic, and lacks specificity. The Price equation is an equation, or an identity. What does it mean for there to be an “assumption of linearity” within it? For the classical Price equation in covariance form (which Reviewer #2 argues is what most people think of as “the Price equation”) there is no way in which one can transform this into a meaningful statement. There is just nothing in there to which the adjective “linear” can be applied. Linearity only becomes a thing when we ask ourselves how we can interpret the regression coefficient in the classical Price equation in regression form. That would be the linearity of the statistical model the differences with which are squared, summed and minimized in the regression.

      This is in contrast to the majority view that Hamilton's rule is a fully general and exact result.

      Again, in this manuscript, I write, time and again, that the classical Hamilton’s rule is fully general (in the sense that it is applies to any transition), and exact (if that means that it always gets the direction of selection right). So, this is clearly not where the contrast with the majority view lies. The contrast with the majority view is that the majority insist on misspecification, and I suggest not to do that.

      To briefly give some mathematical details: Price's equation defines the action of natural selection in relation to a trait of interest as the covariance between fitness 𝑤 and the genetic breeding value 𝑔 for the trait, i.e. Cov(𝑤, 𝑔);

      The Price equation is an identity, not a definition. When deciding on a definition, there is some freedom. We can choose to define ⊂ so that 𝐴 ⊂ 𝐵 means that 𝐴 is a strict subset of 𝐵; or we can choose to define ⊂ so that 𝐴 ⊂ 𝐵 means that 𝐴 is a (not necessarily strict) subset of 𝐵. The Price equation does not “define the action of natural selection”, because it is an identity. There is no freedom to “define” any other way.

      The more serious reason why this is conceptually also a little dangerous, is the following. Imagine a locus with two alleles. Both of them are non-coding bits of DNA. Selection therefore does not act on either of them. Now imagine a parent population with an average p-score of 0.5, or, in other words, the frequency of these alleles in the parent population is 50-50. That makes the expected value of the p-score in the offspring population 0.5 too. In finite populations, however, randomness can make the p-score grow a bit larger or a bit smaller than 0.5. If the parent population is small, the variance (the expected squared deviation from 0.5) can actually be sizeable. If the p-score in the offspring population lands above 0.5, then the Price equation has a > 0 and a 𝐶𝑜𝑣(𝑤, 𝑝) > 0. Describing the Price equation as “defining the action of natural selection” now suggests that higher p-scores have been selected for (or, in other words, that “the action of natural selection in relation to a trait of interest” is positive). With equal probability, however, < 0 and therefore also 𝐶𝑜𝑣(𝑤, 𝑝) < 0, and this would then make us draw the opposite conclusion, that natural selection has acted to lower the p-scores in the population. Both of those would be wrong, because in this situation, it would have been randomness that changed the average p-score. 

      this is a fully general result that applies exactly to any arbitrary set of (𝑔, 𝑤) data; without any loss of generality this covariance can be expressed as the product of genetic variance Var(𝑝) and a coefficient 𝑏(𝑔, 𝑤), the coefficient simply being defined as 𝑏(𝑔, 𝑤) = for all Var(𝑝) > 0; it happens that if one fits a straight line to the same (𝑔, 𝑤) data by means of least-squares regression then the slope of that line is equal to 𝑏(𝑔, 𝑤).

      Why this needs to be explained is a bit of a mystery. These “mathematical details” are in almost all Price equation papers, and they are the point of departure of my Appendix A (it is on page 7 of a more than 90 page long set of appendices). Seeing the need to explain this suggests that the reviewer thinks that there is a chance that I or anyone reading this paper would have missed this. I have not, and, more importantly, none of this invalidates the point I make in the paper.   

      All of this has already been discussed, repeatedly, in the literature.

      All of this has already been discussed, repeatedly, in the literature indeed. It is just that it does not engage with anything I write in the manuscript, or that I wrote in my other papers.

      Now turn to the present paper: the first sentence of the Abstract says "The generality of Hamilton's rule is much debated", and then the next sentence says "In this paper, I show that this debate can be resolved by constructing a general version of Hamilton's rule".

      This is correct.

      But immediately it's clear that this isn't really resolving the debate, what this paper is actually doing is asserting the correctness of the minority view (i.e. that Hamilton's rule as it currently stands is not a general result)

      It seems to me that the reason why this is “immediately clear” to this reviewer is that the reviewer has not processed the contents of the paper. I am not sure if I have to repeat this, but I am not saying that “Hamilton’s rule as it currently stands” is not general (in the sense that it always gets the direction of selection right). It is, and I say that it is a bunch of times. But so are other rules.

      and then attempting to build a more general form of Hamilton's rule upon that shaky foundation.

      I am not just “attempting to build a more general form of Hamilton's rule”. I did in fact build a more general form of Hamilton’s rule (where the generality refers to the richer set of reference statistical models).

      Predictably, the paper erroneously interprets the standard formulation of Hamilton's rule as a linear approximation and develops non-linear extensions to improve the goodness of fit for a result that is already exactly correct.

      Nowhere in the paper or the appendices do I describe the standard formulation of Hamilton’s rule (or, for that matter, any formulation of Hamilton’s rule) as an “approximation”. It is just not a word that has anything to do with this. If we are doing statistical inference, and the sum of squared errors that is minimized decreases by adding a variable in the statistical model with regard to which the sum of squared errors is minimized, then that will typically improve the goodness of fit. In statistics this is not described that as an improvement in how well the statistical model “approximates” the data, or whatever it is that the reviewer would suggest is being approximated here.

      This is not a convincing contribution. It will not change minds or improve understanding of the topic.

      There is indeed plenty of scope for this not to change minds or improve understanding of the topic. It will not change the minds or improve the understanding of those that are not really interested in getting this right. Obviously, it will also not convince those that do not read it.

      Nor is it particularly novel. Smith et al (2010, "A generalisation of Hamilton's rule for the evolution of microbial cooperation" Science 328, 1700-1703) similarly interpreted Hamilton's rule as a linear model and provided a corresponding polynomial expansion - usefully fitting the model to microbial data so as to learn something about the costs and benefits of cooperation in an empirical setting. it's odd that this paper isn't cited here.

      Let me begin by pointing to what I agree with. Given that smith et al. (2010) and my manuscript are both in the business of generalizing Hamilton’s rule, it would be helpful to the reader if my paper includes more information about how the two efforts relate. I will discuss the relation below, and I will also include that in Appendix B, and point to it in the main text. Before I do, however, I would like to point to two details in the review report that fit a pattern.

      The first is that the reviewer describes what smith et al. (2010) do as “useful”, and seems to think of fitting polynomial expansions as a legitimate way to “learn something about the costs and benefits of cooperation in an empirical setting”. That sounds quite positive. My paper, in which I supposedly repeat this, however, is characterized as misguided. This fits a pattern; all of the reviews I received from the inclusive fitness community include a “done before”, and regularly the done before is described approvingly, while my paper is described as fundamentally flawed.

      Also customary is the lack of detail. What would be really useful here, is something like “equation A.14 in this manuscript is the same as equation 6 in smith et al. (2010) if we choose . This kind of statement would pin down the way in which what I do has been done before. That, however, would require going into detail, at the risk of finding out that what is done in my manuscript is actually quite different from what happens in smith et al. (2010). That is also a recurrent thing. When I look up the done before, I typically find something that is not quite the same.  

      Now on to the paper. What smith et al. (2010) try to do is something that I wholeheartedly support. It is an empirical study that tries to capture non-linearity. A first point of order is that it is worth asking ourselves: linear or non-linear in what? For that, I would like to go back to the setup of my manuscript. Model 2 from the Main Text is

      In this fitness function, 𝑝! is the p-score of individual 𝑖 and 𝑞! is the p-score of the partner that individual 𝑖 is matched with. This is a standard model of social behaviour if 𝛽<sub>1,0</sub> < 0 and 𝛽<sub>0,1</sub> > 0. Such choices for 𝛽<sub>1,0</sub> and 𝛽<sub>0,1</sub> indicate that having a higher p-score decreases the fitness of individual 𝑖 and increases the fitness of its partner. Here we assume that 𝛼 = 1, 𝛽<sub>1,0</sub> \= −1, and 𝛽<sub>0,1</sub> \= 2. We assume that p-scores can only be 0 or 1, or, in other words, we assume that there are only cooperators and defectors in the population (or, in terms of smith et al., 2010: cooperators and cheaters).

      For a well-mixed population, where the likelihood of being matched with a cooperator is the same for cooperators and defectors (it is equal to the frequency of cooperators for both), we can now plot the fitnesses of cooperators (red) and defectors (blue) as a function of the frequency of cooperators (Appendix 1-figure 6 left).

      We can do the same for a population with relatedness where the probability of being matched with a cooperator is + 𝑓<sub>c</sub> for cooperators, and 𝑓<sub>c</sub> for defectors, where 𝑓<sub>c</sub> is the frequency of cooperators (Appendix 1-figure 6 right). For relatedness 𝑟 = 0 and 𝑟 = "7, cooperation is selected against at every frequency.

      Increasing relatedness further, we would find that for 𝑟 = the lines coincide, which implies that at every frequency, cooperation is neither selected for nor against. For 𝑟 > ": cooperation will be selected for at every frequency. This pattern implies that, as we have seen in the manuscript, the classical Hamilton’s rule works perfectly fine for Model 2; with 𝑐 = −𝛽<sub>1,0</sub> = 1 and 𝑏 = 𝛽<sub>0,1</sub> \= 2, cooperation is selected for if and only if 𝑟𝑏 > 𝑐. The fitnesses of cooperators and defectors as functions of the frequency of cooperators, moreover, are always parallel lines, regardless of relatedness.

      Model 3 in the main text extends Model 2 by adding an interaction term:

      Now we choose 𝛼 = 1, 𝛽<sub>1,0</sub> = −1, 𝛽<sub>1,0</sub> = 1, and 𝛽<sub>1,1</sub>  \= 1. We again draw the fitnesses of cooperators and defectors, both at relatedness 𝑟 = 0 (Appendix 1-figure 7 left) and at relatedness 𝑟 = (Appendix 1-figure 7 right). In the manuscript, I argue that the appropriate version of Hamilton’s rule here is Queller’s rule: 𝑟<sub>0,1</sub>𝑏<sub>0,1</sub> + 𝑟<sub>1,1</sub>𝑏<sub>1,1</sub> > 𝑐 with 𝑐 = −𝛽<sub>1,0</sub> = 1, 𝑏<sub>0,1</sub> = 𝛽<sub>0,1</sub> = 1, and 𝑏<sub>1,1</sub> = 𝛽<sub>1,1</sub> = 1. The fitnesses of cooperators and defectors as functions of the frequency of cooperators are still straight lines, but they are no longer parallel.

      The first thing to observe, therefore, is that a model with synergy, in which the classic version of Hamilton’s rule would be misspecified, and Queller’s rule would be well-specified, does not require the fitnesses as functions of the frequencies of cooperators to be non-linear. All that changes with the addition of the interaction term, is that they stop being parallel.

      The paper by smith et al. (2010) is an effort to capture non-linearities in the way fitnesses depend on the frequency of cooperators. That, therefore, goes beyond the step from Model 2 to Model 3. Whether it uses the right method to capture those non-linearities, we will come back to in a second, but it is important to realize that also without these non-linearities, the classic version of Hamilton’s rule can be too limiting to accurately describe selection. (Here, I should add that this implies that we were wrong in Wu et al. (2013), when we suggested that “for this experiment, it seems unnecessary to use the generalized Hamilton’s rule, if instead the Malthusian fitness is adopted. In other words, the Wrightian fitness approach calls for a generalization of Hamilton’s rule, whereas the Malthusian fitness approach does not (or at least not in a drastic way, as Malthusian fitnesses are almost linear in the frequency of cooperators).” Using Malthusian fitnesses, the functions were close to linear, but not close to parallel, and therefore also here, Hamilton’s rule needs generalizing - albeit in a different way than smith et al. (2010) did).

      The cooperation that is observed in the Myxococcus xanthus studied by smith et al. (2010) is not a good match with a model where individuals are matched in pairs for an interaction that determines their fitnesses. These microbes cooperate in large groups, and a better match would therefore be the n-player public goods games studied in van Veelen (2018). There, we see that simple, straightforward ways to describe synergies (or anti-synergies) can easily lead to fitnesses not being linear in the frequency of cooperators.

      The way smith et al. (2010) try to capture those non-linearities, however, is not free of complications. We addressed those in Wu et al. (2013), and I summarized them, shortly, in van Veelen (2018). One of the issues is that most of the non-linearity smith et al. (2010) pick up is the result of considering Wrightian fitness rather than Malthusian fitness. In a continuous time model with a constant growth rate, the population size at time 𝑡 is 𝑁(𝑡) = 𝑒<sup>mt</sup>𝑁(0), where 𝑚 is the Malthusian fitness. In a discrete time model with a constant average number of offspring per individual, the population at time 𝑡 is 𝑁(𝑡) = 𝑤<sup>t</sup>𝑁(0), where 𝑤 is the Wrightian fitness. If we take 𝑚 = ln 𝑤, these are the same, and if 𝑤 is close to 1, then 𝑚 can be approximated by 𝑤 − 1. That also implies that if 𝑤 is close to 1 (or, equivalently, if 𝑚 is close to 0) one is locally linear if the other is too. However, in the experiment by smith et al. (2010) the aggregate fitness effects are not small, and what is highly nonlinear in terms of Wrightian fitness is close to linear in Malthusian fitness.

      Another complication is that the Taylor coefficients that smith et al. (2010) find are the result of a combination of the data and the choice of a functional form they choose to first apply to their data. That means that a different choice of a functional form would have given different Taylor coefficients, while the in-between transformation can also be skipped. Also, the number of Taylor coefficients is larger than the dimensionality of the data, which are based on averages for 6 frequencies. For more details on these complications, I would like to refer to Wu et al. (2013) and van Veelen (2018). A nice detail is that if we consider the way the fitnesses of cooperators and defectors compare when using Malthusian fitnesses, then a comparison of the slopes actually suggests anti-synergies, which leads to a stable mix of cooperators and cheaters, already in the absence of population structure. This matches what is suggested by Archetti and Scheuring, (2011, 2012) and Archetti (2018).

      Besides these technical complications, smith et al. (2010) is also different, in the sense that it is an empirical paper. It does not contain the Generalized Price equation, it contains no insights regarding how to derive population genetic dynamics from the Generalized Price equation, or how to derive the appropriate rules from those, and it has a very different approach to separating fitness effects and population structure.

      To end on a positive note, I would like to quote a bit out of Wu et al. (2013):

      “While we criticise these mathematical issues, we are convinced that smith et al. (2010) aim into the right direction: to incorporate the nonlinearities characteristic of biology into social evolution, we may have to extend and generalize the approach of inclusive fitness. It would be beautiful if such a generalization would ultimately include Hamilton’s original rule as a special case […].”

      I like to think that this is exactly what I have done in this paper.

      References

      Akdeniz, A., & van Veelen, M. (2020). The cancellation effect at the group level. Evolution, 74(7), 1246–1254. doi: 10.1111/evo.13995

      Allen, B., & Tarnita, C. E. (2012). Measures of success in a class of evolutionary models with fixed population size and structure. Journal of Mathematical Biology, 68, 109–143. doi: 10.1007/s00285-012-0622-x

      Archetti, M. (2018). How to Analyze Models of Nonlinear Public Goods. Games 2018, Vol. 9, Page 17, 9(2), 17. doi: 10.3390/g9020017

      Archetti, M., & Scheuring, I. (2011). Coexistence of cooperation and defection in public goods games. Evolution, 65(4), 1140–1148. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2010.01185.x

      Archetti, M., & Scheuring, I. (2012). Review: Game theory of public goods in one-shot social dilemmas without assortment. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 299, 9–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.06.018

      Bourke, A. F. G. (2014). Hamilton’s rule and the causes of social evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1642), 20130362. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0362

      Bourrat, P., Godsoe, W., Pillai, P., Gouhier, T. C., Ulrich, W., Gotelli, N. J., & van Veelen, M. (2023). What is the price of using the Price equation in ecology? Oikos, 2023(8). doi: 10.1111/oik.10024

      Crow, J. F. (2008). Commentary: Haldane and beanbag genetics. International Journal of Epidemiology, 37(3), 442–445. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyn048

      Fisher, R. (1930). The genetical theory of natural selection. Retrieved from https://www.cabidigitallibrary.org/doi/full/10.5555/19601600934

      Fletcher, J. A., & Zwick, M. (2006). Unifying the theories of inclusive fitness and reciprocal altruism. American Naturalist, 168(2), 252–262. doi: 10.1086/506529

      Frank, S. A. (1997). The Price equation, Fisher’s fundamental theorem, kin selection, and causal analysis. Evolution, 51(6), 1712–1729. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1997.tb05096.x

      Frank, S. A. (1998). Foundations of social evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

      Frank, S. A. (2012). Natural selection. IV. The Price equation*. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 25(6), 1002–1019. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2012.02498.x

      Gardner, A., West, S. A., & Wild, G. (2011). The genetical theory of kin selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 24(5), 1020–1043. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2011.02236.x

      Grafen, A. (1985a). A geometric view of relatedness. Oxford Surveys in Evolutionary Biology, 2(2), 28-89.

      Grafen, A. (1985b). News and Views. Evolutionary theory: Hamilton’s rule OK. Nature, 318(6044), 310–311. doi: 10.1038/318310a0

      Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1–16. doi: 10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4

      Karlin, S., & Matessi, C. (1983). The eleventh R. A. Fisher Memorial Lecture - Kin selection and altruism. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences, 219(1216), 327–353. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1983.0077

      Matessi, C., & Karlin, S. (1984). On the evolution of altruism by kin selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 81(6), 1754–1758. doi: 10.1073/pnas.81.6.1754

      Nowak, M. A., Tarnita, C. E., & Wilson, E. O. (2010). The evolution of eusociality. Nature, 466(7310), 1057–1062. doi: 10.1038/nature09205

      Okasha, S. (2005). Maynard Smith on the levels of selection question. Biology and Philosophy, 20(5), 989–1010. doi: 10.1007/S10539-005-9019-1/METRICS

      Page, K. M., & Nowak, M. A. (2002). Unifying evolutionary dynamics. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 219(1). doi: 10.1016/S0022-5193(02)93112-7

      Pillai, P., & Gouhier, T. C. (2019). Not even wrong: the spurious measurement of biodiversity’s effects on ecosystem functioning. Ecology, 100(7), e02645. doi: 10.1002/ecy.2645

      Price, G. R. (1970). Selection and Covariance. Nature, 227(5257), 520–521. doi: 10.1038/227520a0

      Price, G. R. (1972). Extension of covariance selection mathematics. Annals of Human Genetics, 35(4), 485-490.

      Queller, D. C. (1985). Kinship, reciprocity and synergism in the evolution of social behaviour. Nature, 318(6044), 366–367. doi: 10.1038/318366a0

      Queller, D. C. (1992a). A general model for kin selection. Evolution, 46(2), 376–380. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1992.tb02045.x

      Queller, D. C. (1992b). Quantitative Genetics, Inclusive Fitness, and Group Selection. The American Naturalist, 139(3), 540–558. doi: 10.1086/285343

      Queller, D. C. (2011). Expanded social fitness and Hamilton’s rule for kin, kith, and kind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(supplement_2), 10792–10799. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1100298108

      Rousset, & Billiard. (2000). A theoretical basis for measures of kin selection in subdivided populations: Finite populations and localized dispersal. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 13(5). doi: 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2000.00219.x

      Rousset, F. (2015). Regression, least squares, and the general version of inclusive fitness. Evolution, 69(11), 2963–2970. doi: 10.1111/evo.12791

      Smith, J., Van Dyken, J. D., & Zee, P. C. (2010). A generalization of hamilton’s rule for the evolution of microbial cooperation. Science, 328(5986), 1700–1703. doi: 10.1126/science.1189675

      Sober, Elliott., & Wilson, D. Sloan. (2007). Unto others : the evolution and psychology of unselfish behavior. 394. Retrieved from https://www.hup.harvard.edu/books/9780674930476

      Taylor, P. D. (1992). Altruism in viscous populations - an inclusive fitness model. Evolutionary Ecology, 6(4), 352–356. doi: 10.1007/bf02270971

      Taylor, Peter D. (1989). Evolutionary stability in one-parameter models under weak selection. Theoretical Population Biology, 36(2), 125–143. doi: 10.1016/00405809(89)90025-7

      Taylor, Peter D., Day, T., & Wild, G. (2007). Evolution of cooperation in a finite homogeneous graph. Nature, 447(7143), 469–472. doi: 10.1038/nature05784

      Van Cleve, J. (2015). Social evolution and genetic interactions in the short and long term. Theoretical Population Biology, 103. doi: 10.1016/j.tpb.2015.05.002

      van Veelen, M. (2005). On the use of the Price equation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 237(4). doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2005.04.026

      van Veelen, M. (2007). Hamilton’s missing link. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 246(3). doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2007.01.001

      van Veelen, M. (2011). The replicator dynamics with n players and population structure. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 276(1). doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.01.044

      van Veelen, M. (2018). Can Hamilton’s rule be violated? ELife, 7. doi: 10.7554/eLife.41901

      van Veelen, M. (2020). The problem with the Price equation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1797), 20190355. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2019.0355

      van Veelen, M., Allen, B., Hoffman, M., Simon, B., & Veller, C. (2017). Hamilton’s rule. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 414. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2016.08.019

      van Veelen, M., García, J., Sabelis, M. W., & Egas, M. (2012). Group selection and inclusive fitness are not equivalent; the Price equation vs. models and statistics. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 299. doi: 10.1016/j.jtbi.2011.07.025

      Wilson, D. S., Pollock, G. B., & Dugatkin, L. A. (1992). Can altruism evolve in purely viscous populations? Evolutionary Ecology, 6(4), 331–341. doi: 10.1007/bf02270969

      Wu, B., Gokhale, C. S., van Veelen, M., Wang, L., & Traulsen, A. (2013). Interpretations arising from Wrightian and Malthusian fitness under strong frequency dependent selection. Ecology and Evolution, 3(5). doi: 10.1002/ece3.500

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Filamentous fungi are established workhorses in biotechnology, with Aspergillus oryzae as a prominent example with a thousand-year history. Still, the cell biology and biochemical properties of the production strains is not well understood. The paper of the Takeshita group describes the change in nuclear numbers and correlates it to different production capacities. They used microfluidic devices to really correlate the production with nuclear numbers. In addition, they used microdissection to understand expression profile changes and found an increase in ribosomes. The analysis of two genes involved in cell volume control in S. pombe did not reveal conclusive answers to explain the phenomenon. It appears that it is a multi-trait phenotype. Finally, they identified SNPs in many industrial strains and tried to correlate them to the capability of increasing their nuclear numbers. 

      The methods used in the paper range from high-quality cell biology, Raman spectroscopy, to atomic force and electron microscopy, and from laser microdissection to the use of microfluidic devices to

      study individual hyphae. 

      This is a very interesting, biotechnologically relevant paper with the application of excellent cell biology. I have only minor suggestions for improvement. 

      We sincerely appreciate your fair and positive evaluation of our work. Thank you for your suggestions for improvement. We respond to each of them appropriately.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In the study presented by Itani and colleagues, it is shown that some strains of Aspergillus oryzae - especially those used industrially for the production of sake and soy sauce - develop hyphae with a significantly increased number of nuclei and cell volume over time. These thick hyphae are formed by branching from normal hyphae and grow faster and therefore dominate the colonies. The number of nuclei positively correlates with the thicker hyphae and also the amount of secreted enzymes. The addition of nutrients such as yeast extract or certain amino acids enhanced this effect. Genome and transcriptome analyses identified genes, including rseA, that are associated with the increased number of nuclei and enzyme production. The authors conclude from their data involvement of glycosyltransferases, calcium channels, and the tor regulatory cascade in the regulation of cell volume and number of nuclei. Thicker hyphae and an increased number of nuclei were also observed in high-production strains of other industrially used fungi such as Trichoderma reesei and Penicillium chrysogenum, leading to the hypothesis that the mentioned phenotypes are characteristic of production strains, which is of significant interest for fungal biotechnology. 

      Strengths: 

      The study is very comprehensive and involves the application of diverse state-of-the-art cell biological, biochemical, and genetic methods. Overall, the data are properly controlled and analyzed, figures and

      movies are of excellent quality. 

      The results are particularly interesting with regard to the elucidation of molecular mechanisms that regulate the size of fungal hyphae and their number of nuclei. For this, the authors have discovered a very good model: (regular) strains with a low number of nuclei and strains with a high number of nuclei. Also, the results can be expected to be of interest for the further optimization of industrially relevant filamentous

      fungi. 

      Weaknesses: 

      There are only a few open questions concerning the activity of the many nuclei in production strains (active versus inactive), their number of chromosomes (haploid/diploid), and whether hyper-branching always leads to propagation of nuclei. 

      We are very grateful for your recognition of our findings, the proposed model, and their significance for future applications. We are grateful for the questions, which contribute to a more accurate understanding. 

      Our responses to each are provided below.  

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The authors seek to determine the underlying traits that support the exceptional capacity of Aspergillus oryzae to secrete enzymes and heterologous proteins. To do so, they leverage the availability of multiple domesticated isolates of A. oryzae along with other Aspergillus species to perform comparative imaging and genomic analysis. 

      Strengths: 

      The strength of this study lies in the use of multifaceted approaches to identify significant differences in hyphal morphology that correlate with enzyme secretion, which is then followed by the use of genomics to identify candidate functions that underlie these differences. 

      Weaknesses: 

      There are aspects of the methods that would benefit from the inclusion of more detail on how experiments were performed and data interpreted. 

      Overall, the authors have achieved their aims in that they are able to clearly document the presence of two distinct hyphal forms in A. oryzae and other Aspergillus species, and to correlate the presence of the thicker, rapidly growing form with enhanced enzyme secretion. The image analysis is convincing. The discovery that the addition of yeast extract and specific amino acids can stimulate the formation of the novel hyphal form is also notable. Although the conclusions are generally supported by the results, this is perhaps less so for the genetic analysis as it remains unclear how direct the role of RseA and the calcium transporters might be in supporting the formation of the thicker hyphae. 

      The results presented here will impact the field. The complexity of hyphal morphology and how it affects secretion is not well understood despite the importance of these processes for the fungal lifestyle. In addition, the description of approaches that can be used to facilitate the study of these different hyphal forms (i.e., stimulation using yeast extract or specific amino acids) will benefit future efforts to understand the molecular basis of their formation. 

      We are very grateful for your fair and thoughtful evaluation of our work. We agree that the genetic analysis in the latter part is relatively weaker compared to the imaging analysis in the first half. Rather than a single mutation causing a dramatic phenotypic change, we believe that the accumulation of various mutations through breeding leads to the observed phenotype, making it difficult to clearly demonstrate causality. Since transcriptome and SNP analyses have revealed key pathways and phenotypes, it would be gratifying if these insights could contribute to future applications utilizing filamentous fungi.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      I was wondering what happens if thick hyphae were taken as inoculum for a new colony or thin hyphae. Is it possible to enrich for one or the other type of hyphae? Perhaps in the presence of yeast extract or certain amino acids. 

      Added an explanation in the discussion.

      L304-306. When thick hyphae were cultured on fresh medium, thin hyphae initially emerged, suggesting that sustained metabolic activity is required for the formation of thick hyphae with a high number of nuclei.    

      L120-121. In some cases, thick hyphae emerged by branching from thick hyphae (Fig. 2D, left), while in other cases, thin hyphae emerged from thick hyphae (Fig. 2D, right). Thin hyphae emerge in the early stage of cultivation even in the presence of yeast extract or certain amino acids.

      In the Discussion, they hypothesize that the primary effect could be on cell wall rigidity. I am wondering if that hypothesis could be tested by adding, for instance, sublethal concentrations of cytochalasin to hyphae of A. nidulans to weaken the cell wall. 

      The question is reasonable. To ensure accurate understanding, we moved Fig. S6 to Fig. 6 and revised the discussion as follows. 

      L294-295. In our model, cell wall loosening at a branching site and regulation of cell volume by turgor pressure constitute necessary conditions for increasing cell volume and maintaining thick hyphae. L306-309. Weakening the cell wall by treatment with a low concentration of calcofluor white did not lead to hyphal thickening or an increase in nuclear number. On the contrary, thick hyphae have thicker cell walls (Fig. 2H-K), which are necessary to maintain the increased cell volume.

      I recommend including some older literature. It was described already 20 years ago that A. nigerdifferentiates hyphae with different capacities to secrete proteins (PMID: 16238620). In addition, there are old reports in A. nidulans reporting high numbers of nuclei (https://doi.org/10.1099/00221287-60-1-133). Perhaps it is worth trying to reproduce those cultural conditions. At least this should be discussed. In the same line, the number of nuclei increases a lot in the stalk of conidiophores in A. nidulans. These observations could be used as examples that the phenomenon observed in A. oryzae may be of general importance. 

      Thank you for the suggestion. It is a very interesting proposal. We checked the nuclei distribution of A. nidulans on the media and added the following discussion.

      L328-334. A previous study reported an increase in the number of nuclei in A. nidulans (62, 63). Here, we examined the nuclear distribution of A. nidulans grown on the culture media, however, did not find class III hyphae as observed in A. oryzae. Even in A. nidulans, conidiophore stalks contain a high number of nuclei. It has been shown that A. oryzae has a taller conidiophore stalk (64). In the thick hyphae of A. oryzae, the expression level of flbA, an early regulator of conidiophore development (65), was elevated. This suggests that differentiation to aerial hyphae may be involved in the increase of hyphal volume and nuclear number. 

      (62) Clutterbuck A.J. Synchronous Nuclear Division and Septation in Aspergillus nidulans. J Gen Microbiol 60, 133-135 (1970).

      (63) Vinck, A., Terlou, M., Pestman, W.R., Martens, E.P., Ram, A.F., van den Hondel, C.A., Wösten, H.A. Hyphal differentiation in the exploring mycelium of Aspergillus niger. Mol Microbiol 58, 693-9 (2005).

      (64) Wada R, Maruyama J, Yamaguchi H, Yamamoto N, Wagu Y, Paoletti M, Archer DB, Dyer PS, Kitamoto K. Presence and functionality of mating type genes in the supposedly asexual filamentous fungus Aspergillus oryzae. Appl Environ Microbiol 78, 2819-29 (2012).

      (65) Lee, B.N., Adams, T.H. Overexpression of flbA, an early regulator of Aspergillus asexual sporulation, leads to activation of brlA and premature initiation of development. Mol Microbiol 14, 323-34 (1994).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      I suggest addressing the following questions to strengthen the manuscript: 

      (1) Do the authors have an explanation for their result that with an increase in the number of nuclei the individual nucleus is smaller? Have the authors checked whether all the nuclei are haploid or diploid?

      Thank you for the very important question. We added new results to Fig. S5D and S5E and the following discussion.

      L335-340. We investigated whether the reduction in nuclear size observed in thick hyphae was due to a change from diploid to haploid status. However, no difference in GFP-histone fluorescence intensity was detected between thick and thin hyphae (Fig. S5D). In both RIB40 and RIB915 strains, no significant difference in conidial spore size was observed despite the large difference in the number of nuclei within the hyphae (Fig. S5E). These results suggest that both thick and thin hyphae remain haploid, and that the smaller nuclear size observed in thick hyphae is likely due to a higher nuclear density.

      (2) In this context, the biological relevance of the increase in the number of nuclei should also be discussed in more detail. It remains to be clarified whether in hyphae with a high number of nuclei all nuclei are functionally active or whether many nuclei are possibly "inactive". Studies on the transcriptional activity of individual nuclei or on DNA replication (e.g., by EdU labeling) could clarify this. 

      Added the explanation below.

      L102-105. The transcriptional activity of each nucleus is unknown. However, a previous study (Yasui et al., FBB 2020) demonstrated that nuclear division is synchronized even when there are more than 200 nuclei. This suggests that DNA replication occurs similarly in most nuclei. Furthermore, since the germination rate of conidia and the colonies formed from individual conidia show no significant abnormalities, it is suggested that nearly all nuclei possess normal genomes and chromosomes.

      (3) It becomes not entirely clear what the underlying signal is that causes a thin hypha to branch into a thick multinucleated cell. This needs to be discussed in more detail. 

      Thanks for the suggestion. We clarified the signal to increase nuclear number and cell volume.

      L294-309. Although it is speculative, we propose a model to aid interpretation in the discussion. We have clarified that both genetic potential and environmental signals such as nutrients are important.

      (4) Is increased branching always correlated with an increased number of nuclei? 

      It is not an increase in branching, but rather the thickening of hyphae and an increase in cell volume that is consistently associated with an increase in nuclear number. Approximately 40 hours after inoculation, within 400 μm from the tip, the number of branches was 3.4 (SD=2.4) in thin hyphae and 2.6 (SD=0.5) in thick hyphae, suggesting that branching does not increase (n=4). Since thick hyphae elongate faster, it seems that fewer branches are present near the tip, even if the branching frequency itself remains unchanged.

      (5) The abstract does not summarize the many findings of the manuscript in an adequate way. 

      abstract change

      Minor: 

      (1) Lines 49-50: Why italics? 

      corrected.

      (2) Line 179: process. 

      corrected.

      (3) Lines 313-314: Do not forget (and discuss) in this context mycorrhiza fungi with up to thousands of nuclei that were apparently selected during evolution for this high number of nuclei. 

      Thank you for the very interesting suggestion. We have added the following discussion.

      L339-351. The regulation of nuclear number and its ecological strategy are intriguing in other fungi such as N. crassa, which rapidly spreads after wildfires (68), and arbuscular mycorrhiza fungi that form symbiotic relationships with plants and contain thousands of nuclei within hyphae lacking septa (69).

      (68) Jacobson, D. J. et al. Neurospora in temperate forests of western North America. Mycologia 96, 66–74 (2004).

      (69) Kokkoris V, Stefani F, Dalpé Y, Dettman J, Corradi N. Nuclear Dynamics in the Arbuscular Mycorrhizal Fungi. Trends Plant Sci. 25, 765-778 (2020).

      (4) Lines 356-358: many typos.

      corrected.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Specific suggestions or clarifications for the authors include: 

      (1) Lines 49-50: Is this sentence italicized for a reason? 

      It was a mistake, so we have corrected it.

      (2) Line 83: More detail on the specific characteristics of the different classes of hyphae would be helpful. Perhaps include a schematic drawing that emphasizes the differences between class I,II, and III hyphae. 

      L398-400. The classification is described in the Methods section: Class I – nuclei are distributed at regular intervals without overlapping; Class II – nuclei are aligned but occasionally overlap; Class III – nuclei are scattered throughout the hyphae without alignment. Representative images are shown in a previous study (Yasui et al., FBB 2020). 

      L82-84. We have added this information to clarify the classification.

      (3) Lines 102-103: It was not very clear how this experiment was done. Are you counting nuclei within 100 um of the tip? Are these all in one hyphal compartment? These details could be provided in a drawing that would make it easier for the reader to understand how this was done. 

      L109. Due to variation in the distance from the hyphal tip to the septum, we counted the number of nuclei within 100 μm from the hyphal tip. When septa were present, nuclei were counted in the same manner, so multiple compartments may be included. Changed the explanation.

      (4) Lines 134-140: Is there a way to calibrate levels of secreted protein or amylase activity per nucleus? That is, if the ratio of cytoplasmic volume per nucleus is constant, does the same apply to the secreted product? Knowing this would help to clarify whether the key feature in enhanced secretion is nuclear (e.g., gene expression) versus a cytoplasmic trait (e.g., vesicle trafficking). 

      Enzyme activity was measured across the entire mycelium, which includes a mixture of hyphae with high and low numbers of nuclei. Therefore, it is difficult to assess the correlation between enzyme activity and nuclear number. Enzyme activity was normalized by fungal biomass. The size of each colony is shown in Fig. 1B. Additionally, the correlation between the proportion of hyphae with increased nuclear number and enzyme activity is shown in Fig. 3H. In the experiment where enzyme activity was measured in a single hypha, we attempted to measure the number of nuclei; however, we could not use the nuclear GFP strain because the substrate exhibits green fluorescence. DAPI staining also failed due to limited dye access to the microfluidic channel. Changed the section title, ‘Increase in nuclear number and enzyme secretion’ from ‘Correlation between nuclear number and enzyme secretion’.

      (5) Line 151 and Figure 3F: YE also triggered a ~5-fold enhancement of secretion in A. nidulans without a concomitant increase in hyphal width. This merits some comment in the text.  

      Added an explanation, L156-157.

      In A. nidulans, the addition of yeast extract did not cause a dramatic increase in nuclear number, but hyphal width increased by 1.4-times and protein secretion increased by 5.1-times.

      (6) Line 252: Were nimE levels detected or altered in thick hyphae? The levels of this cycling might play a more important role in a shortened cell cycle than the authors have considered, especially as NimE functions during both G1 and G2. 

      Added an explanation below, L260-262.

      The expression level of nimE (AO090003000993) was low in both thick and thin hyphae, with no significant difference observed. As known in other organisms, its function is likely regulated through phosphorylation and the protein degradation.

      (7) Line 254: Please provide a citation for the statement that branches emerge as a result of cell wall loosening. 

      rephrased and added citation, L263.

      Branching is thought to occur through the degradation and reconstruction of the cell wall at the branching site (54).

      Harris SD. Branching of fungal hyphae: regulation, mechanisms and comparison with other branching systems. Mycologia 100, 823-32 (2008).   

      (8) Lines 275-277: It would be interesting to know whether the addition of rapamycin also suppressed the ability of amino acids to trigger greater numbers of class III hyphae. 

      We added new results at Fig. S2G.

      L168. Rapamycin decreased the ratio of hyphae with increased nuclei even in the medium with yeast extract (Fig. S2G).

      (9) Lines 282-289: My sense is that this model is too speculative at this time. The role of RseA seems very broad based on the strong deletion phenotype. How would the removal of RseA be regulated to limit its effect to the branch site? Also, the msyA deletion phenotype isn't entirely consistent with what you would expect if it were necessary to maintain thick hyphae. Lastly, the authors do not show that translational capacity is enhanced in thick hyphae. I would suggest that these statements be tempered to some degree. 

      Thank you for your comment. We agree that it was too speculative, whereas we believe that some explanatory interpretation is necessary. Therefore, we have revised the text as follows, L294-300. In our model, cell wall loosening during branching and regulation of cell volume by turgor pressure constitute necessary conditions for increasing cell volume and maintaining thick hyphae. RseA and MsyA may be involved in these processes. At the same time, enhanced translational capacity by increased expression of ribosomal genes, possibly due to associated with TOR activation by specific amino acids, and mechanisms that accelerate the cell cycle represent another essential condition that enables an increase in nuclear number.

      (10) General: how do the authors reconcile the observation that YE and amino acids stimulate the formation of thicker hyphae, yet the time lapse imaging (Figure 2E) suggests that these hyphae arise at a later time during colony development when these resources might be limiting? The authors should consider providing some insight into this in the Discussion. 

      L300-305. Added a discussion below.

      Both genetic potential and nutritional environmental signals are likely required for the formation of thick hyphae with a high number of nuclei. When thick hyphae were cultured on fresh medium, thin hyphae initially emerged, suggesting the necessity of sustained high metabolic activity.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The behavior of cells expressing constitutively active HRas is examined in mosaic monolayers, both in MCF10a breast epithelial and Beas2b bronchial epithelial cell lines, mimicking the potential initial phase of development of carcinoma. Single HRas-positive cells are excluded from MCF10a but not Beas2b monolayers. Most interestingly, however, when in groups, these cells are not excluded, but rather sharply segregated within a MCF10a monolayer. In contrast, they freely mix with wt Beas2b cells. Biophysical analysis identifies high tension at heterotypic interfaces between HRas and wild-type cells as the likely reason for segregation of MCF10a cells. The hypothesis is supported experimentally, as myosin inhibition abolishes segregation. The probable reason for the lack of segregation in the bronchial epithelium is to be found in the different intrinsic properties of these cells, which form a looser tissue with lower basal actomyosin activity. The behaviour of single cells and groups is recapitulated in a vortex model based on the principle of differential interfacial tension, under the condition of high heterotypic interfacial tension.

      Strengths:

      Despite being long recognized as a crucial event during cancer development, segregation of oncogenic cells has been a largely understudied question. This nice work addresses the mechanics of this phenomenon through a straightforward experimental design, applying the biophysical analytical approaches established in the field of morphogenesis. Comparison between two cell types provides some preliminary clues on the diversity of effects in various cancers.

      Weaknesses:

      Although not calling into question the main message of this study, there are a few issues that one may want to address:

      (1) One may be careful in interpreting the comparison between MCF10a and Beas2b cells as used in this study. The conditions may not necessarily be representative of the actual properties of breast and bronchial epithelia. How much of the epithelial organization is reconstituted under these experimental conditions remains to be established. This is particularly obvious for bronchial cells, which would need quite specific culture conditions to build a proper bronchial layer. In this study, they seemed to be on the verge of a mesenchymal phenotype (large gaps, huge protrusions, cells growing on top of each other, as mentioned in the manuscript).

      We thank the reviewer for this important point. We agree that our experimental conditions do not fully recapitulate the in vivo architecture of either breast or bronchial epithelia. However, here, our intention is to compare two well-established epithelial lines with distinct intrinsic mechanical and organizational properties, rather than to reproduce in-vivo microenvironment. Nevertheless, to address this, we have now strengthened our quantitative analysis of epithelial integrity in Beas2b monolayers, by including ZO-1 immunofluorescence along with E-cadherin immunofluorescence. These measurements confirm that Beas2b monolayers under our culture conditions retain junctional organization, albeit with larger gaps and protrusions compared to MCF10a. We will revise the text to make this distinction explicit.

      As an alternative to Beas2b, comparison of MCF10a with another cell line capable of more robust in vitro epithelial organization, but ideally with different adhesive and/or tensile properties, would be highly interesting, as it may narrow down the parameters involved in segregation of oncogenic cells.

      We agree with the reviewer that the inclusion of an additional epithelial model system with distinct adhesive and organizational properties would provide valuable insights. In line with this suggestion, we are currently repeating the key experiments using Madin-Darby Canine Kidney (MDCK) cells, a well-established model epithelial cell line. We believe this complementary system will allow us to further dissect the behaviour of HRasV12-expressing cells.

      (2) While the seminal description of tissue properties based on interfacial tensions (Brodland 2002) is clearly key to interpreting these data, the actual "Differential Interfacial Tension Hypothesis" poses that segregation results from global differences, i.e., juxtaposition of two tissues displaying different intrinsic tensions. On the contrary, the results of the present work support a different scenario, where what counts is the actual difference in tension ALONG the tissue boundary, in other words, that segregation is driven by high HETEROTYPIC interfacial tension. This is an important distinction that should be clarified.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. As correctly noted, Brodland’s 2002 work provided a seminal formulation of the Differential Interfacial Tension Hypothesis (DITH), which frames tissue organization in terms of effective interfacial tensions. In its original form, DITH emphasized segregation as a consequence of global differences in the intrinsic (bulk) tensions of juxtaposed tissues.

      While our results specifically show that segregation is determined by local interfacial mechanics between transformed- and host cells, from our experiments with blebbistatin, where we observed lost in segregation upon reducing global contractility, we believe that the differences in local interfacial mechanics also stem from global differences which belong intrinsically to the tissues in discussion here.

      To directly map global interfacial tension, in the revised manuscript, we aim to perform staining with E-cadherin, and actin in the two tissues, and measure cortical actin, stress fibers, and E-cadherin levels at the cell-cell junctions. Once the global tissue mechanics are mapped, we can be more confident about our claim on DITH. Nevertheless, we will also clarify this distinction, more clearly in the text and explicitly state that while DITH provided the foundation for conceptualizing tissue mechanics, our findings on transformed cell- healthy cell interactions specifically demonstrate that segregation is driven by high heterotypic interfacial tension at the tissue boundary.

      (3) Related: The fact that actomyosin accumulates at the heterotypic interface is key here. It would be quite informative to better document the pattern of this accumulation, which is not clear enough from the images of the current manuscript: Are we talking about the actual interface between mutant and wt cells (membrane/cortex of heterotypic contacts)? Or is it more globally overactivated in the whole cell layer along the border? Some better images and some quantification would help.

      We agree that more detailed visualization of actomyosin distribution would strengthen our conclusion. We are currently working on re-imaging the heterotypic interfaces at higher magnification and are quantifying fluorescence intensity of actin and myosin-II along cell–cell boundaries. All of this will be integrated in the next version of the manuscript.

      (4) In the case of Beas2b cells, mutant cells show higher actin than wt cells, while actin is, on the contrary, lower in mutant MCF10a cells (Author response image 2). Has this been taken into account in the model? It may be in line with the idea that HRas may have a different action on the two cell types, a possibility that would certainly be worth considering and discussing.

      Our current vertex model does not explicitly incorporate actin levels; rather, it captures their functional consequences indirectly through effective mechanical parameters such as cortical tension and adhesion strength. Nonetheless, we agree that the opposite trends in actin enrichment between Beas2b and MCF10a HRasV12 mutants raise the important possibility that HRas signaling may act through distinct mechanisms in the two cell types.

      To further investigate this, we are currently culturing MCF10a and Beas2b HRasV12 mutant populations separately (i.e., without wild-type cells) to assess their intrinsic organization and behavior in isolation. These experiments will help us disentangle how HRas activation differentially impacts epithelial architecture in these two cellular contexts, and we will discuss these ongoing efforts in the revised manuscript.

      From the modelling perspective, the model currently does not account for the different actin levels of mutants with respect to wt cells in the two tissues. This can be accounted for by having different  and  for mutants and wt in the two cases in simulation.

      In conclusion, the study conveys an important message, but, as it stands, the strength of evidence is incomplete. It would greatly benefit from a more detailed and complete analysis of the experimental data, a better fit between this analysis and the corresponding vertex model, and a more in-depth discussion of biological and biophysical aspects. These revisions should be rather easily done, and would then make the evidence much more solid.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigate the behavior of oncogenic cells in mammary and bronchial epithelia. They observe that individual oncogenic cells are preferentially excluded from the mammary epithelium, but they remain integrated in the bronchial epithelium. They also observe that clusters of oncogenic cells form a compact cluster in the mammary epithelium, but they disperse in the bronchial epithelium. The authors demonstrate experimentally and in the vertex model simulations that the difference in observed behavior is due to the differential tension between the mutant and wild-type cells due to a differential expression of actin and myosin.

      Strengths:

      (1) Very detailed analysis of experiments to systematically characterize and quantify differences between mammary and bronchial epithelia.

      (2) Detailed comparison between the experiments and vertex model simulations to identify the differential cell line tension between the oncogenic and wild-type cells as one of the key parameters that are responsible for the different behavior of oncogenic cells in mammary and bronchial epithelia

      Weaknesses:

      (1) It is unclear what the mechanistic origin of the shape-tension coupling is, which is used in the vertex model, and how important that coupling is for the presented results. The authors claim that the shape-tension coupling is due to the anisotropic distribution of stress fibers when cells are under external stress. It is unclear why the stress fibers should affect an effective line tension on the cell boundaries and why the stress fibers should be sensitive to the magnitude of the internal isotropic cell pressure. In experiments, it makes sense that stress fibers form when cells are stretched. Similar stress fibers form when the cytoskeleton or polymer networks are stretched. It is unclear why the stress fibers should be sensitive to the magnitude of internal isotropic cell pressure. If all the surrounding cells have the same internal pressure, then the cell would not be significantly deformed due to that pressure, and stress fibers would not form. The authors should better justify the use of the shape-tension coupling in the model and also present simulation results without that coupling. I expect that most of the observed behavior is already captured by the differential tension, even if there is no shape-tension coupling. 

      While the segregation behavior can be captured by the differential tension, without the shape-tension coupling, we noticed unjamming and aligned movement of wild type cells at the mutant-cell interface. This was only captured when we incorporated shape tension coupling in the model, suggesting changes in cell shapes due to differential interfacial tension is essential in driving the fate of the mutants.  Below, difference between shape indices of cells at the interface and away from the boundary is plotted versus the interfacial tension in the case of no shape-tension coupling [Author response image 1]. The red dashed line represents the experimental value of the shape index difference. The blue line is the shape index difference between two randomly chosen groups of cells (half of the total number of cells in each group is taken). At zero line-tension, the difference in shape index between interface cells and cells away from the interface is same as that between randomly chosen groups of cells, which is expected since there should be no interface at zero line-tension. The no shape-tension data presented here are averaged over 19 seeds. Although the results without shape-tension coupling reaches experimental values at high enough differential tension [Author response image 2], a closer inspection of the simulation results show that the cells are just squeezed and are aligned perpendicular to the interface, which is contrary to what is seen in experiments.

      Author response image 1.

      Shape indices versus the interfacial line tension<br />

      Calculating the average of the absolute value of the dot product of the nematic director and the interface edge for simulations with and without shape-tension coupling clearly shows that with shape-tension coupling, the cells align and elongate along the interface as is seen in experiment, given by an interface dot product value > 0.5 at high enough line-tension values. Further, shape-tension coupling or biased edge tension has been used before to model for cell elongation during embryo elongation [1] and here we use it as an active line-tension force, which elongates cells along the interface, in addition to the differential tension which is passive. This additional quantification of the alignment and elongation of cells along the interface will be added to the Supplementary Information (SI).

      [1] Dye, N. A., Popović, M., Iyer, K. V., Fuhrmann, J. F., Piscitello-Gómez, R., Eaton, S., & Jülicher, F. (2021). Self-organized patterning of cell morphology via mechanosensitive feedback. Elife, 10, e57964.

      Author response image 2.

      Change in interfacial tension with and without shape tension coupling<br />

      (2) The observed difference of shape indices between the interfacial and bulk cells in simulations in the absence of differential line tension is concerning. This suggests that either there are not enough statistics from the simulations or that something is wrong with the simulations. For all presented simulation results, the authors should repeat multiple simulations and then present both averages and standard deviations. This way, it would be easier to determine whether the observed differences in simulations are statistically significant.

      The reviewer is right in pointing out that statistics for the plots must be shown. The difference in shape indices between the interfacial and bulk cells in simulations has been calculated over 11 different seed values. The observed differences in simulations along with the standard deviations have been plotted below [Author response image 3]. This figure in the paper will be updated to include the standard deviations. The non-zero difference in shape index in the absence of differential line tension for low values of stress threshold is due to the shape-tension coupling acting even at low differential tension. Thus, a non-zero, sufficiently high value of the stress threshold is required in our model with shape-tension coupling, for the model to make sense. This has also been stated in section 4 of the paper. The importance of the stress-tension coupling has been stated in response to the previous point.

      Author response image 3.<br />

      (3) The authors should also analyze the cell line tension data in simulations and make a comparison with experiments.

      We agree with the reviewer that cell line tension data should also be analyzed and compared with experiments. This will be added to the next version of the paper.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) Figure 2 and related text: it would be useful to explain more explicitly what is meant by "neurogenic" and "non-neurogenic" models. I presume that the total number of neurons in non-neurogenic models is lower than in neurogenic models because no new neurons are added. It would be useful to plot the number of GCs as a function of timesteps.

      We have clarified the distinction between neurogenic and non-neurogenic models in the text (Lines 142-145), explicitly noting that in non-neurogenic models, no new GCs are added, resulting in a lower total neuron count over time. In response to the reviewer’s suggestion, we generated a plot showing the number of GCs over time (see below). Because the neurogenic model exhibits a simple linear increase, we found this plot not especially informative for inclusion in the manuscript. However, we agree with the reviewer’s later comments that similar plots are useful for interpreting specific results, and we have included those where appropriate.

      Author response image 1.

      Number of GCs over time for neurogenic (solid line) and non-neurogenic (dotted line) networks

      (2) Figure 2F, G: memory declines dramatically when the number of GCs at enrichment onset increases beyond an optimum. Why?

      We have explained the reasoning more thoroughly in the text (Lines 174-177) and added a new supplemental figure to support this reasoning (Figure S2). As the number of GCs increases, the network becomes overly inhibited and the response of abGCs to the stimuli decreases (Fig S2A). This leads to a smaller population of GCs being able to integrate with the stimulus (Fig S2B) which is expected given the activity-dependent plasticity rule. Moreover, it can be seen in Fig S2C that for networks with increasing size, the GCs that do learn only connect to MCs that are driven strongest by the stimuli until they struggle to connect to any MCs at all.

      In principle, a homeostatic mechanism like synaptic scaling could reduce activity to restore balance, but such a mechanism would also likely disrupt existing memories. Alternatively, we suggest activity-dependent apoptosis as a superior homeostatic mechanism because it leads to a stable level of activity without substantially erasing existing memories.

      (3) The paragraph describing synaptic connectivity of abGCs (related to Figure 2H) is confusing. What is the directionality of synapses considered here: mitral-to-granule, or granule-to-mitral? The text is opaque here. Connectivity matrix in Figure 2H: who is presynaptic, who is postsynaptic? If I understand correctly, these questions are actually irrelevant because all mitralgranule synapses in the network are reciprocal. This should be pointed out explicitly in the figure legend. Generally: the fact that the network is fully reciprocal (if I understand correctly) is very important but not stated with sufficient emphasis. It should be stated very explicitly in the text that connectivity matrices are fully reciprocal, and an equation clarifying this point should be included in Methods.

      (6) Connectivity matrix: to what degree was connectivity between mitral and granule cells reciprocal (fraction of connections in either direction that were paired with a connection in the opposite direction between the same cell pair)? Was connectivity shaped by experience (enrichment) reciprocal?

      (7) Directly related to the above: it would be useful to show the disynaptic connectivity matrix between mitral cells and analyze its symmetry. For the symmetric component, it should then be analyzed what fraction of this can be attributed to the reciprocal synapses, and what fraction is contributed by connectivity via different granule cells. This should then be compared to models with biologically realistic fractions of reciprocal connections. Is the model proposed here consistent with a biologically realistic fraction of reciprocal synapses between mitral-granule cell pairs?

      We appreciate these insightful and detailed comments. We agree that the assumption that MC-GC synapses were fully reciprocal was not clearly stated. We now explicitly state this in the main text (lines 90-94, 369-370, Figure 2 caption) and methods (line 561), emphasize its importance. As the reviewer points out, this is a simplifying assumption and does not fully reflect the biology because not all synapses are reciprocal in the true system. We also note that our synaptic plasticity model does not break the reciprocity assumption: all connections added or pruned during learning remain reciprocal. As a result, the disynaptic connectivity matrix (Bottom panel below, MCs sorted by stimulus as shown in the top panel) is always symmetric.

      We have now made these statements explicit in the main text and in the methods. Regarding functional consequences of this assumption, earlier work by our group has examined the impact of the degree of reciprocity of MC-GC synapses in a similar OB model (Chow, Wick & Riecke, Plos Comp Bio 2012). The study examined three different changes in reciprocity by (1) redirecting a fraction of the inhibitory connections of each GC to randomly chosen MCs instead of the MCs that drive that GC, (2) allowing heterogeneity in reciprocal weights so that there is no relationship between the strength of the MC -> GC synapse and the GC -> MC synapse, (3) reducing the level of self-inhibition a MC receives from the GCs that it excites. The model was found to be quite robust to each of these manipulations, suggesting that our present model likely remains functionally relevant even if biological reciprocity is partial. We reference this work now in the discussion, lines 490-492.

      Author response image 2.

      Disynaptic connectivity. Top: MC activity in response to the two stimuli, sorted by MC selectivity. Bottom: Disynaptic connectivity matrix (diagonal subtracted).

      (4) How were mitral cells sorted in Figure 2H? This needs to be explained.

      (5) Directly related to the point above: the text mentions that synaptic connectivity between GCs of the "learning cluster" and mitral cells (which direction?) is increased for mitral cells responding by enrichment odors, but this is not shown in the figure. This statement suggests that mitral cells sorted to the bottom of the y-axis respond more strongly to enrichment odors, but the information is not given directly. Please provide more information to back up your statements.

      Indeed as the reviewer inferred, MCs in Figure 2H were sorted so that those that receive the strongest stimulation from the odor were at the bottom of the y-axis. We have clarified this in the Figure 2 caption and added a subplot to Figure 2H showing the average MC input to make this more explicit.

      (8) Apoptosis (Figure 4 and related text): paragraph 231ff is somewhat difficult to comprehend because the "number" of enrichments should really be the "frequency" of enrichments. In Figure 4, it is not mentioned explicitly that each enrichment is with different random new odors.

      We agree that the term “number” of enrichments was imprecise and have revised the text to refer instead to the frequency of enrichment events (Lines 255-267). We also clarified that in Figure 4, each enrichment corresponds to a different set of randomly sampled odors, and we now state this explicitly in both the Figure 4 legend and main text (Lines 260-261).

      (9) Apoptosis: apoptosis improves memory but the underlying reason remains opaque. A simple prediction of the data in Figure 4D and 4E is that the number of GCs in 4E. It would be helpful to show this. Furthermore, an obvious question that arises is whether a higher frequency of enrichments improves memories because the total number of granule cells is kept low, or because granule cells are removed specifically based on their activity (or both). This could be addressed easily by artificially removing a random subset of granule cells in a simulation such as 4E to match granule cell numbers to the case in 4D.

      Apoptosis improves learning is because it reduces the total inhibition in the network by removing GCs and thus prevents deficits in learning that occur in Fig. 2G as GCs accumulate in the network. As the reviewer inferred, the number of GCs in Figure 4D is lower than in 4E and this is now clarified in the text. This difference was shown implicitly in Supplementary Figure S4D (previously S3D), but we now explicitly reference this plot to support this point as well (Line 266).

      As the reviewer notes, there is a question in whether increased enrichment frequency improves memory because it limits the total number of GCs, or because apoptosis selectively removes GCs based on their activity, or both. Our model supports both mechanisms. Importantly, simply reducing GC numbers through random deletion will degrade existing memories: random removal erodes memory representations encoded by those GCs. In contrast, our age and activity dependent apoptosis rule targets a specific cohort of adult-born GCs. This selective removal minimizes damage to existing memories encoded by GCs outside of this cohort while keeping GC numbers within a regime that supports robust learning (as shown in Figure 2G).

      However, we note that if enrichment frequency becomes too high, even recent memories can be lost due to premature pruning of GCs that have not yet stabilized their synaptic connections. This tradeoff has been shown experimentally (Forest et al., Nat Comm 2019) which we reproduce in our model (Figure S4).

      (10) Text related to Figure 5: "Learning flexibility...approached a steady state when the growth of the network started to saturate". Please show the growth (better: size) of the network (total number of GCs) for these simulations (and other panels in Figure 5). It would also be useful to show the total number of GCs in other figures (e.g. Figure 4; see above).

      We have now added a supplementary figure (Figure S6) that shows the total number of GCs over time for the simulations presented. This confirms that the network size approaches a steady state around the same time that learning flexibility begins to plateau, as noted in the original text (now line 275), and highlights the large number of GCs without apoptosis as well as the slightly reduced number of GCs in the permanent encoding model (line 312).

      (11) As much as I appreciate the comprehensive discussion of the results in a broader context, I feel that the discussion can be somewhat shortened. The section on lateral inhibition is not fully valid given that synaptic connectivity is reciprocal. I also feel that much of the final section (Model assumptions and outlook) can be dropped (except for the last paragraph), not because anything is irrelevant, but because these points have been made, onen repeatedly, in the text above.

      We agree that the discussion could be streamlined and have revised the manuscript accordingly. Specifically, we have shortened the section on lateral inhibition and clarified that the OB relies predominantly on reciprocal connectivity (Line 370). We also agree that parts of the final section were repetitive and have removed these. However, to address comments by Reviewer 3, we also expanded on some of the model assumptions. We thank the reviewer for helping us improve the clarity and focus of the manuscript.

      (12) Figure 5: bolding every 5th curve is confusing.

      We have adjusted our figure accordingly.

      (13) "...we biased the dendritic field...": it would be helpful to explain the idea of a "dendritic field" in a bit more detail prior to this sentence.

      We have now noted that GC’s "dendritic field" refers to the subset of MCs with which it is capable of forming synaptic connections when we initially describe the model (Line 97).

      Reviewer #3:

      (1) The authors find that a network with age-dependent synaptic plasticity outperforms one with constant age-independent plasticity and that having more GC per se is not sufficient to explain this effect. In addition, having an initial higher excitability of GCs leads to increased performance. To what degree the increased excitability of abGCs is conceptually necessarily independent of them having higher synaptic plasticity rates / fast synapses?

      We thank the reviewer for this question, as the difference between excitability and plasticity rate in memory formation is something we intended to highlight in this study. We have updated the (Lines 157-198) to clarify this.

      At the cellular level, a neuron's excitability and its rate of synaptic plasticity are mechanistically distinct: excitability is governed by factors such as ion channel expression or membrane resistance, whereas plasticity rates are influenced by molecular pathways involved in synapse and dendritic spine formation and remodeling. While these are independent properties, they are functionally coupled: most synaptic plasticity rules are activity-dependent, so greater excitability can increase the likelihood of plasticity being induced but does not itself guarantee learning.

      Our model reflects this distinction. Increased excitability biases which neurons become activated and thus eligible to undergo plasticity, but actual learning still depends on the plasticity rate itself. This can be seen by comparing the model constant plasticity and excitability (solid blue and green curves in Figure 2C) to the model with only transient excitability (solid blue and green lines in Figure 2E). In both cases, the strength and duration of the memory remain limited by the plasticity rate. We note additionally that, in this network, neurons compete to learn new stimuli: as GCs start to learn, they suppress MC activity through recurrent inhibition which suppresses learning in other GCs who otherwise would have been in position to learn the odor. As a result there is not a significant increase in the overall number of neurons recruited to learn (Figure 2J). In a different network architecture, such as a feedforward network, we would not expect this to be the case; greater excitability in a population of neurons would likely increase the memory by increasing the number of neurons recruited to learn. Transiently enhanced excitability biases which neurons join the memory engram (Figure 2J), but the extent and rate of learning still depend on the plasticity rates themselves. We did note in the original text (now lines 284-286) that this bias in recruitment subtly increases memory stability, but the extent is not great. In principle, a model can be engineered to rely on transiently increased excitability to encode memories in orthogonal subpopulations of neurons and that this could resolve the flexibility-stability dilemma. However, in that case, the number of memories that can be stored within a short time would be bounded by the size of this subpopulation such that even if a large number of odors are presented, mature GCs cannot become part of the engram and the network would likely fail to learn the stimuli. However, when this was tested experimentally (Forest et al. Cereb Cor. 2020), it was found that mature GCs participated in the engram when the number of odors was sufficiently high. Our results are consistent with these experiments: for complex odor environments, neonatal GCs, which are mature during odor exposure, and abGCs both participate in the engrams.

      Author response image 3.

      Simulating learning in more complex odor environments. Top: enrichment consisted of three odor pairs presented sequentially in a random order. Bottom: enrichment consisted of five odor pairs. Left: discriminability of the odor pairs over time. Middle: connectivity between MCs (sorted by odor selectivity) and GCs (sorted by age). In both cases AbGCs develop a clear connectivity structure. In more complex environments neonatal GCs also start to develop a clear connectivity structure. Right: combined engram membership across all stimuli by GC age.

      In sum, transiently increased excitability alone will not make learning any faster, so a fast learning system must have a high plasticity rate. If this plasticity rate stays high, then memories stored in these neurons, even if no longer highly excitable, will be vulnerable as the neurons can still be driven above their plasticity threshold by moderately interfering stimuli and will thus be quickly forgotten. Conversely, if the reviewer is wondering if a greater increase in the plasticity rate of new neurons can compensate for a lack of excitability, this is not the case: if a newborn neuron is not sufficiently driven by the stimulus it will not learn regardless of how high its plasticity rate is.

      (2) The authors do not mention previous theoretical work on the specificity of mitral to granule cell interactions from several groups (Koulakov & Rinberg - Neuron, 2011; Gilra & Bhalla, PLoSOne, 2015; Grabska-Bawinska...Mainen, Pouget, Latham, Nat. Neurosci. 2017; Tootoonian, Schaefer, Latham, PLoS Comput. Biol., 2022), nor work on the relevance of top-down feedback from the olfactory cortex on the abGC during odor discrimination tasks (Wu & Komiyama, Sci. Adv. 2020), or of top-down regulation from the olfactory cortex on regulating the activity of the mitral/tuned cells in task engaged mice (Lindeman et al., PLoS Comput. Biol., 2024), or in naïve mice that encounter odorants (in the absence of specific context; Boyd, et al., Cell Rep, 2015; Otazu et al., Neuron 2015, Chae et al., Neuron, 2022). In particular, the presence of rich topdown control of granule cell activity (including of abGCs) puts into question the plausibility of one of the opening statements of the authors with respect to relying solely on local circuit mechanisms to solve the flexibility-stability dilemma. I think the discussion of this work is important in order to put into context the idea of specific interactions between the abGCs and the mitral cells.

      We thank the reviewer for these detailed and thorough comments, and whole-heartedly agree that it is important to discuss the listed studies in order to contextualize our work through the broader lens of how information is processed in the OB. We have expanded our discussion to further acknowledge and integrate insight from previous theoretical and experimental work cited by the reviewer. (Lines 361-366, 493-550)

      Regarding the importance of top-down feedback, we of course recognize that in practice cortical inputs play a critical role in abGC survival and synaptic integration. However, its nature is not quite clear and is likely variable across behavioral seungs. In the paradigm that we study in the manuscript, there is likely no key reward value or contextual signal that is relayed to the OB. One plausible interpretation is that in this task, cortical feedback provides a random, variable baseline excitatory drive to GCs. This would likely be consistent with many of the listed studies, e.g.

      (1) Glomerular layer targeting of feedback would be explicitly unrelated to glomerular odor specificity, as in Boyd et al.

      (2) GC activity would decrease if these cortical inputs were silenced, resulting in stronger MC responses as in Otazu et al., Chae et al.

      (3) Silencing PCx during learning would prevent GCs from reaching activity-dependent plasticity thresholds, resulting in decreased spine density as in Wu & Komiyama.

      Likewise activating PCx would lead to increased spine density.

      In this interpretation, the effect of top-down input could be captured implicitly by adjusting model parameters such as activity or plasticity thresholds. For the purposes of our study, we opted to neglect these inputs in favor of model simplicity.

      Critically, even if top-down inputs play a substantially larger role, by perhaps even going as far as providing signals to abGCs to modulate their development, the core solution to the flexibility-stability dilemma that we describe stays local: we predict that the memory persists in the same network in which it was formed.

      (3) To what the degree of specific connectivity reflects a specific stimulus configuration, and is a good proxy for determining the stimulus discriminability and memory capacity in terms of temporal activity patterns (difference in latency/phase with respect to the respiration cycle, etc.) which may account to a substantial fraction of ability to discriminate between stimuli? The authors mention in the discussion that this is, indeed, an upper bound and specific connectivity is necessary for different temporal activity patterns, but a further expansion on this topic would help in understanding the limitations of the model.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point. Indeed, there have been several recent experimental studies indicating that much of the information needed for olfactory discrimination is encoded in the temporal activity patterns of mitral and tuned cells. Our model does not explicitly simulate these dynamics. It was for this reason that we defined memory in terms of the learned structure of the network rather than by firing rate activity. This is motivated by the idea that learned patterns of connectivity constrain the space of neural activity the network can support, and thus shape stimulus responses. We now make this limitation more explicit in the discussion and clarify that the specific MC–GC connectivity we analyze should be seen as a structural substrate that constrains the possible temporal transformations the network could support (Lines 492-506).

      (4) Reward or reward prediction error signals are not considered in the model. They however are ubiquitous in nature and likely to be encountered and shape the connectivity and activity patterns of the abGC-mitral cell network. Including a discussion of how the model may be adjusted to incorporate reward/error signals would strengthen the manuscript.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion and agree that reward and reward prediction error signals are critical components of many learning paradigms. We deliberately chose not to model associative learning, reward signals or top-down neuromodulation in this work. Our goal is to investigate the role of adult neurogenesis in a regime where its contribution has been shown to be experimentally necessary. Specifically, we focused on an unsupervised perceptual learning paradigm where adult neurogenesis is required for successful odor discrimination (Moreno et al. PNAS, 2008). In contrast, when the same odors are used in a rewarded learning paradigm, performance remains intact even when adult neurogenesis is ablated (Imayoshi et al., Nat. Neuro., 2008). This dissociation suggests that neurogenesis is dispensable in contexts where reward can guide learning. As such, we argue that isolating the contribution of local circuit dynamics in an unsupervised setting is critical to understanding what neurogenesis is uniquely enabling, especially given the evolutionary cost of maintaining it.

      We agree that extending this work to incorporate reward-driven plasticity or neuromodulatory influences would be a valuable direction for future research. In particular, it could help clarify how different learning paradigms engage distinct abGC cohorts (e.g., Mandairon et al., eLife 2018; Wu & Komiyama, Sci. Adv. 2020), and how task structure shapes memory allocation and engram composition. We have incorporated this into the discussion regarding extending our model to include top down feedback (lines 539-553).

      Specific comments

      (1) Lines 84-86; 507-509; Eq(3): Sensory input is defined by a basal parameter of MCs spontaneous activity (Sspontaneus) and the odor stimuli input (Siodor) but is not clear from the main text or methods how sensory inputs (glomerular patterns) were modeled

      We now clarify in the Methods section "Stimulus model" how the sensory inputs were modeled. Specifically, odor-evoked inputs to mitral cells (Siodor) were generated either as Gaussian profiles across the mitral cell population (Figs. 2,3) or as sparser random patterns (Figs. 4,5). In Figures 2 and 3, the denser Gaussian stimuli require more GCs to learn the odors, aiding in visualization of the connectivity matrix (Figure 2H) and abGC recruitment plots (Figure 2I,J; Figure 3C,E). However, real olfactory stimuli activate a sparse set of MCs, so in Figures 4 and 5 where we address learning of many stimuli, we utilize sparser, binary, stimuli delivered to only 10% of MCs, in range of experimental data (Wachowiak and Cohen, Neuron, 2001). The fact that the stimuli are binary, however, is not realistic and leads to denser representations. This leads to a worst-case scenario for the model as denser memory representations are easier to overwrite. These points has been added explicitly to the Methods section "Stimulus model" to improve clarity.

      (2) Lines 118-122: The used perceptual learning task explanation is done only in the context of the discriminability of similar artificial stimuli using the Fisher discriminant and "Memory" metric. A detailed description of the logic of the perceptual learning task methods and objective, taking into account Comment 1, would help to better understand the model.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out had not adequately described the task and have updated the main text (lines 125-132) and included a new methods section "Perceptual learning task" to describe it more explicitly. The experiments that inspired the simulation followed an ecological model of discrimination learning (Moreno et al. PNAS 2009): For one hour a day over a ten day "enrichment period", two tea balls containing similar but distinct odors were suspended from the lid of each mouse's home cage. The mice engaged with the stimuli under self-directed conditions, therefore learning through natural experience. As a result the mice use olfactory information to discriminate between the similar stimuli, a skill potentially relevant for navigation or social behaviors.

      In our simulations, we model these experiments as follows. During the enrichment period, the model is stimulated with a randomly selected stimulus chosen from a set of two similar stimuli, corresponding to a mouse choosing to sniff one of the tea balls. During enrichment, in between these bouts of "sniffing", the model only receives spontaneous activity, reflecting the temporal sparsity of sensory input even over the enrichment period. Outside of enrichment, the model again receives only spontaneous input.

      (3) Rapid re-learning of forgotten odor pair is enabled by sensory-dependent dendritic elaboration of neurons that initially encoded the odors and the observed re-learning would occur even if neurogenesis was blocked following the first enrichment and even though the initial learning did require neurogenesis. When this would ever occur in nature? The re-learning of an odor period? Why is this highlighted in the study?

      We believe that this sort of learning is certainly relevant in nature. To clarify: by “learning,” we do not refer to the memory of an entire “odor period”, but simply an altered mapping of specific stimuli. Therefore, forgeung could occur if these specific stimuli are absent from the environment for a period of time, and re-learning would occur when these stimuli are re-encountered. Natural odor environments are highly dynamic, as environmental conditions and social contexts change over time. The odors an animal encounters also depend strongly on its own behavior; as it explores different environments, it may be exposed to particular odors intermittently: it could encounter them in one location, then not return to that location for some time before returning again.

      Such natural variability in odor exposure makes the ability to forget and re-learn especially valuable, allowing the animal to prioritize relevant information while maintaining flexibility. To this end, we show in Figure 5G that the synaptic forgetting of odors is beneficial to the performance of the model because it reduces interference in the network. Therefore we highlight that re-learning enabled by adult neurogenesis is a highly efficient strategy for memory storage and retrieval, which is why he emphasize it in this study.

      (4) Figure 2A: I understand that the ages shown at the bottom of the colored boxes represent the GC age. If so, find a better way to express that to avoid confusing 'GC ages' from the days shown in the perceptual learning task description (Figure 2B).

      We have updated the text in the figure to disambiguate the two and refer to the “days” shown in the perceptual learning task description now as “time relative to enrichment”

      (5) Figure 2B: Clarify how the two-dimensional arrays are arranged to represent the patterns shown. Does each point of the array represent one neuron? If so, are these neurons re-arranged to help the readers visually differentiate patterns A and B? Are the patterns of activity of MCs in the model spatially and temporally sparse as observed in experimental work?

      In Figure 2B, each point in the two-dimensional array represents the activity of a single mitral cell. The layout is purely for visualization—neurons are re-arranged to make the differences between odor patterns A and B visually apparent. This ordering does not reflect anatomical position or model architecture. We revised the Figure 2 caption to say this explicitly.

      Regarding spatial sparseness, as we mentioned in the response to the reviewer’s comment (1), the activity of mitral cells in response to odors is spatially sparse in the model. Regarding temporal sparseness, while the model is not spiking and does not include temporal dynamics within the timescale of the breath, however, odor input is delivered in discrete, odorspecific epochs interleaved with periods of no input, which leads to temporally structured activity patterns. This information has been made explicit in the new methods sections "Stimulus model" and "Perceptual learning task"

      (6) Figure 3C and Line 189: potential confusion between the color code mentioned in the legend for the enrichment and developing periods.

      It appeared to be a confusion in the text and has been corrected (Lines 212-213).

      (7) Figure 5F: For clarity, this would benefit from replacing the bold line with areas in the plot to depict the enrichment periods.

      We agree that replacing the bolded line segments with shaded areas is more clear and have updated the figure accordingly, and appreciate the reviewer's suggestion to clarify the figure.

      (8) Lines 380, 416: Potential role of cortical feedback and or neuromodulation depending on behavioral relevance or permanent exposure? Later mentioned in Lines 467 - 474.

      We have updated the text to acknowledge the role of potential cortical feedback and neuromodulation, now in lines 403-407.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      We thank the reviewer for his/her very positive comments.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      We thank the reviewer for his/her positive evaluation. We plan to add RNAseq data of yeast wild-type and JDP mutant strains as more direct readout for the role of Apj1 in controlling Hsf1 activity. We agree with the reviewer that our study includes one major finding: the central role of Apj1 in controlling the attenuation phase of the heat shock response. In accordance with the reviewer we consider this finding highly relevant and interesting for a broad readership. We agree that additional studies are now necessary to mechanistically dissect how the diverse JDPs support Hsp70 in controlling Hsf1 activity. We believe that such analysis should be part of an independent study but we will indicate this aspect as part of an outlook in the discussion section of a revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      We thank the reviewer for his/her suggestions. We agree that it is sometimes difficult to distinguish direct effects of JDP mutants on heat shock regulation from indirect ones, which can result from the accumulation of misfolded proteins that titrate Hsp70 capacity. We also agree that an in vitro reconstitution of Hsf1 displacement from DNA by Apj1/Hsp70 will be important, also to dissect Apj1 function mechanistically. We will add this point as outlook to the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) Can the authors submit the raw translatome data to a standard repository? Also, the data should be summarized in a supplemental Excel table. 

      We submitted the raw translatome data to the NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus and added the analyzed data sets (shown in Figures 1 and 5) as Supplementary Tables S4/S5 (excel sheets). We additionally included RNAseq analysis of yeast WT and JDP mutants set grown at 25°C, complementing and confirming our former translatome analysis (new Figure 5, Figure Supplement 2). Respective transcriptome raw data were also deposited at the NCBI Gene Expression Omnibus and analyzed data are available as Supplementary Table S7.

      (2) MW indicators need to be added to the Western Blot figures. 

      We added molecular weight markers to the Western Blot figures.

      (3) Can the authors please include the sequences of the primers used in all the RT-qPCR experiments? They mention they are in the supplemental information, but I couldn't locate them. 

      We added the sequences of the RT-qPCR primers as Supplementary Table S4.

      (4) Given the clear mechanism proposed, it would be nice if the authors could provide a nice summary figure. 

      We followed the suggestion of the reviewer and illustrate our main finding as new Figure 7.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) As mentioned above, a co-IP experiment between Hsf1 and Ssa1/2 in APJ1 and apj1∆ cells, utilizing Hsf1 alleles with and without the two known binding sites, would cement the assignment of Apj1 in the Hsf1 regulatory circuit. 

      We agree with the reviewer that Hsf1-Ssa1/2 pulldown experiments, as done by Pincus and colleagues (1), will further specify the role of Apj1 in targeting Hsp70 to Hsf1 during the attenuation phase of the heat shock response. We have tried extensively such pulldown experiments to document dissociation of Ssa1/2 from Hsf1 upon heat shock in yeast wild-type cells. While we could specifically detect Ssa1/2 upon Hsf-HA1 pulldown, our results after heat shock were highly variable and inconclusive and did not allow us to probe for a role of Apj1 or the two known Ssa1/2 binding sites in the phase-specific targeting. We now discuss the potential roles of the two distinct Ssa1/2 binding sites for phase-specific regulation of Hsf1 activity in the revised manuscript (page 12, lanes 17-21).

      (2) Experiments in Figure 3 nicely localize CHIP reactions with known HSEs. A final confirmatory experiment utilizing a mutated HSE (another classic experiment in the field) would cement this finding and validate the motif and reporter-based analysis. 

      We thank the reviewer for this meaningful suggestions. We have done something like this by using the non-Hsf1 regulated gene BUD3, which lacks HSEs, as reference. We engineered a counterpart, termed “BUD3 HS-UAS”, which bears inserted HSEs, derived from the native UAS of HSP82, within the BUD3 UAS. We show that BUD3<sup>+</sup> lacking HSEs is not occupied by Hsf1 and Apj1 under either non-stress or heat shock conditions while BUD3-HSE is clearly occupied under both, paralleling Hsf1 and Apj1 occupancy of HSP82 (Figure 3E). We have renamed the engineered allele to “BUD3-HSE” to clarify the experimental design and output.

      (3) Page 8 - the ydj1-4xcga allele is introduced without explaining why it's needed, since ydj1∆ cells are viable. The authors should acknowledge the latter fact, then justify why the RQC depletion approach is preferred. Especially since the ydj1∆ mutant appears in Figure 5B. 

      ydj1∆ cells are viable, yet they grow extremely slowly at 25°C and hardly at 30°C,  making them difficult to handle. The RQC-mediated depletion of Ydj1 in ydj1-4xcga cells allows for solid growth at 30°C, facilitating strain handling and analysis of Ydj1 function. Importantly, ydj1-4xcga cells are still temperature-sensitive and exhibit the same deregulation of the heat shock response upon combination with apj1D as observed for ydj1∆ cells. Thus ydj1 knockout and knockdown cells do not differ in the relevant phenotypes reported here and we performed most of the analysis with  ydj1-4xcga cells due to their growth advantage. We added a respective explanation to the text (page 8, lanes 13-14) .

      (4) The authors raise the possibility that Sis1, Apj1, and Ydj1 may all be competing for access to Ssa1/2 at different phases of the HSR, and that access may be dictated by conformational changes in Hsf1. Given that there are at least two known Hsp70 binding sites that have negative regulatory activity in Hsf1, the possibility that domain-specific association governs the different roles should be considered. It is also unclear how the JDPs are associating with Hsf1 differentially if all binding is through Ssa1/2. 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment and will add the possibility of specific roles of the identified Hsp70 binding sites in regulating Hsf1 activity at the different phases of the heat shock response to the discussion section. Binding of Ssa1/2 to substrates (including Hsf1) is dependent on J-domain proteins (JDPs), which differ in substrate specificity. It is tempting to speculate that the distinct JDPs recognize different sites in Hsf1 and are responsible for mediating the specific binding of Ssa1/2 to either N- or C-terminal sites in Hsf1. Thus, the specific binding of a JDP to Hsf1 might dictate the binding to Ssa1/2 to either binding site. We discuss this aspect in the revised manuscript (page 12, lanes 17-21).

      (5) Figure 6 - temperature sensitivity of hsf1 and ydj1 mutants has been linked to defects in the cell wall integrity pathway rather than general proteostasis collapse. This is easily tested via plating on osmotically supportive media (i.e., 1M sorbitol) and should be done throughout Figure 6 to properly interpret the results.

      Our data indicate proteostasis breakdown in ydj1 cells by showing strongly altered localization of Sis1-GFP, pointing to massive protein aggregation (Figure 6 – Figure Supplement  1D).

      We followed the suggestion of the reviewer and performed spot tests in presence of 1 M sorbitol (see figure below). The presence of sorbitol is improving growth of ydj1-4xcga mutant cells at increased temperatures, in agreement with the remark of the reviewer. We, however, do not think that growth rescue by sorbitol is pointing to specific defects of the ydj1 mutant in cell wall integrity. Sorbitol functions as a chemical chaperone and has been shown to have protective effects on cellular proteostasis and to rescue phenotypes of diverse point mutants in yeast cells by facilitating folding of the respective mutant proteins and suppressing their aggregation (2-4). Thus sorbitol can broadly restore proteostasis, which can also explain its effects on growth of ydj1 mutants at increased temperatures. Therefore the readout of the spot test with sorbitol is not unambiguous and we therefore prefer not showing it in the manuscript.

      Author response image 1.

      Serial dilutions of indicated yeast strains were spotted on YPD plates without and with 1 M sorbitol and incubated at indicated temperatures for 2 days.<br />

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) Line 154: Can the authors, by analysis, offer an explanation for why HSR attenuation varies between genes for the sis1-4xcga strain? Is it, for example, a consequence of that a hypomorph and not a knock is used, a mRNA turnover issue, or that Hsf1 has different affinities for the HSEs in the promoters? 

      We used the sis1-4xcga knock-down strain because Sis1 is essential for yeast viability. The point raised by the reviewer is highly valid and we extensively thought about the diverse consequences of Sis1 depletion on levels of e.g. translated BTN2 (minor impact) and HSP104 (strong impact) mRNA. We meanwhile performed transcriptome analysis and confirmed the specific impact of Sis1 depletion on HSP104 mRNA levels, while BTN2 mRNA levels remained much less affected (new Figure 5 - Figure Supplement 2A/B). We compared numbers and spacings of HSEs in the respective target genes but could not identify obvious differences. Hsf1 occupancy within the UAS region of both BTN2 and HSP104 is very comparable at three different time points of a 39°C heat shock: 0, 5 and 120 min, arguing against different Hsf1 affinities to the respective HSEs (5). The molecular basis for the target-specific derepression upon Sis1 depletion thus remains to be explored. We added a respective comment to the revised version of the manuscript (page 12, lanes 3-8) .

      (2) Line 194: The analysis of ChIP-seq is not very elaborated in its presentation. How specific is this interaction? Can it be ruled out by analysis that it is simply the highly expressed genes after the HS that lead to Apj1 appearing there? More generally: Can the data in the main figure be presented to give a more unbiased genome-wide view of the results?

      We overall observed a low number of Apj1 binding events in the UAS of genes. The interaction of Apj1 with HSEs is specific as we do not observe Apj1 binding to the UAS of well-expressed non-heat shock genes. Similarly, Apj1 does not bind to ARS504 (Figure S3 – Figure Supplement 1). We extended the description of our ChIP-seq analysis procedures leading to the identification of HSEs as Apj1 target sites to make it easier to understand the data analysis. We additionally re-analysed the two Apj1 binding peaks that did not reveal an HSE in our original analysis. Using a modified setting we can identify a slightly degenerated HSE in the promoter region of the two genes (TMA10, RIE1) and changed Figure 3C accordingly. Notably, TMA10 is a known target gene of Hsf1. The expanded analysis is further documenting the specificity of the Apj1 binding peaks.

      (3) Line 215. Figure 3. The clear anticorrelation is puzzling. Presumably, Apj1 binds Hsf1 as a substrate, and then a straight correlation is expected: When Hsf1 substrate levels decrease at the promoters, also Apj1 signal is predicted to decrease. What explanations could there be for this? Is it, for example, that Hsf1 is not always available as a substrate on every promoter, or is Apj1 tied up elsewhere in the cell/nucleus early after HS? 

      We propose that Apj1 binds HSE-bound Hsf1 only after clearance of nuclear inclusions, which form upon heat stress. Apj1 thereby couples the restoration of nuclear proteostasis to the attenuation of the heat shock response. This explains the delayed binding of Apj1 to HSEs (via Hsf1), while Hsf1 shows highest binding upon activation of the heat shock response (early timepoints). Notably, the binding efficiency of Hsf1 and Apj1 (% input) largely differ, as we determine strong binding of Hsf1 five min post heat shock (30-40% of input), whereas maximal 3-4% of the input is pulled down with Apj1 (60 min post heat shock) (Figure 3D). Even at this late timepoint 10-20% of the input is pulled down with Hsf1. The diverse kinetics and pulldown efficiencies suggest that Apj1 displaces Hsf1 from HSEs and accordingly Hsf1 stays bound to HSEs in apj1D cells (Figure 4). This activity of Apj1 explains the anti-correlation: increased targeting of Apj1 to HSE-bound Hsf1 will lower the absolute levels of HSE-bound Hsf1. What we observe in the ChIP experiment at the individual timepoints is a snapshot of this reaction. Accordingly, at the last timepoint (120 min after heat shock ) analyzed, we observe low binding of both Hsf1 and Apj1 as the heat shock response has been shut down.

      (4) Line 253: "Sis-depleted".  

      We have corrected the mistake.

      (5) Line 332: Fig. 6C SIS1 OE from pRS315. A YIP would have been better, 20% of the cells will typically not express a protein with a CEN/ARS of the pRS-series so the Sis1 overexpression phenotype may be underestimated and this may impact on the interpretation. 

      We agree with the reviewer that Yeast Integrated Plasmids (YIP) represent the gold standard for complementation assays. We are not aware of a study showing that 20% of cells harboring pRS-plasmids do not express the encoded protein. The results shown in Fig. 8C/D demonstrate that even strong overproduction of Sis1 cannot restore Hsf1 activity control. This interpretation also will not be affected assuming that a certain percentage of these cells do not express Sis1. Nevertheless, we added a comment to the respective section pointing to the possibility that the Sis1 effect might be underestimated due to variations in Sis1 expression (page 11, lanes 15-19).

      (6) Figure 1C. Since n=2, a more transparent way of showing the data is the individual data points. It is used elsewhere in the manuscript, and I recommend it. 

      We agree that showing individual data points can enhance transparency, particularly with small sample sizes. However, the log2 fold change (log2FC) values presented in Figure 1C and other figures derived from ribosome profiling and RNAseq experiments were generated using the DESeq2 package. This DeSeq2 pipeline is widely used in analyzing differential gene expression and known for its statistical robustness. It performs differential expression analysis based on a model that incorporates normalization, dispersion estimation, and shrinkage of fold changes. The pipeline automatically accounts for biological, technical variability, and batch effects, thereby improving the reliability of results. These log2FC values are not directly calculated from log-transformed normalized counts of individual samples but are instead estimated from a fitted model comparing group means. Therefore, the individual values of replicates in DESeq2 log2FC cannot be shown.

      (7) Figure 1D. Please add the number of minutes on the X-axis. Figure legend: "Cycloheximide" is capitalized.  

      We revised the figure and figure legend as recommended.

      (8) Several figure panels: Statistical tests and SD error bars for experiments performed in duplicates simply feel wrong for this reviewer. I do recognize that parts of the community are calculating, in essence, quasi-p-values using parametric methods for experiments with far too low sample numbers, but I recommend not doing so. In my opinion, better to show the two data points and interpret with caution.

      We followed the advice of the reviewer and removed statistical tests for experiments based on duplicates.

      References

      (1) Krakowiak, J., Zheng, X., Patel, N., Feder, Z. A., Anandhakumar, J., Valerius, K. et al. (2018) Hsf1 and Hsp70 constitute a two-component feedback loop that regulates the yeast heat shock response eLife 7,

      (2) Guiberson, N. G. L., Pineda, A., Abramov, D., Kharel, P., Carnazza, K. E., Wragg, R. T. et al. (2018) Mechanism-based rescue of Munc18-1 dysfunction in varied encephalopathies by chemical chaperones Nature communications 9, 3986

      (3) Singh, L. R., Chen, X., Kozich, V., and Kruger, W. D. (2007) Chemical chaperone rescue of mutant human cystathionine beta-synthase Mol Genet Metab 91, 335-342

      (4) Marathe, S., and Bose, T. (2024) Chemical chaperone - sorbitol corrects cohesion and translational defects in the Roberts mutant bioRxiv  10.1101/2024.09.04.6109452024.2009.2004.610945

      (5) Pincus, D., Anandhakumar, J., Thiru, P., Guertin, M. J., Erkine, A. M., and Gross, D. S. (2018) Genetic and epigenetic determinants establish a continuum of Hsf1 occupancy and activity across the yeast genome Mol Biol Cell 29, 3168-3182

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript from Jones and colleagues investigates a previously described phenomenon in which P. falciparum malaria parasites display increased trafficking of proteins displayed on the surface of infected RBCs, as well as increased cytoadherence in response to febrile temperatures. While this parasite response was previously described, it was not uniformly accepted, and conflicting reports can be found in the literature. This variability likely arises due to differences in the methods employed and the degree of temperature increase to which the parasites were exposed. Here, the authors are very careful to employ a temperature shift that likely reflects what is happening in infected humans and that they demonstrate is not detrimental to parasite viability or replication. In addition, they go on to investigate what steps in protein trafficking are affected by exposure to increased temperature and show that the effect is not specific to PfEMP1 but rather likely affects all transmembrane domain-containing proteins that are trafficked to the RBC. They also detect increased rates of phosphorylation of trafficked proteins, consistent with overall increased protein export.

      Strengths:

      The authors used a relatively mild increase in temperature (39 degrees), which they demonstrate is not detrimental to parasite viability or replication. This enabled them to avoid potential complications of a more severe heat shock that might have affected previously published studies. They employed a clever method of fractionation of RBCs infected with a var2csa-nanoluc fusion protein expressing parasite line to determine which step in the export pathway was likely accelerating in response to increased temperature. This enabled them to determine that export across the PVM is being affected. They also explored changes in phosphorylation of exported proteins and demonstrated that the effect is not limited to PfEMP1 but appears to affect numerous (or potentially all) exported transmembrane domain-containing proteins.

      Weaknesses:

      All the experiments investigating changes resulting from increased temperature were conducted after an increase in temperature from 16 to 24 hours, with sampling or assays conducted at the 24 hr mark. While this provided consistency throughout the study, this is a time point relatively early in the export of proteins to the RBC surface, as shown in Figure 1E. At 24 hrs, only approximately 50% of wildtype parasites are positive for PfEMP1, while at 32 hrs this approaches 80%. Since the authors only checked the effect of heat stress at 24 hrs, it is not possible to determine if the changes they observe reflect an overall increase in protein trafficking or instead a shift to earlier (or an accelerated) trafficking. In other words, if a second time point had been considered (for example, 32 hrs or later), would the parasites grown in the absence of heat stress catch up?

      We did not assess cytoadhesion at later stages, but in the supplementary figures we show that at 40 hours post infection both heat stress and control conditions have comparable proportions of VAR2CSA-positive iRBCs, whilst they differ at 24h. This is true for the DMSO (control wildtype resembling) HA-tagged lines of HSP70x and PF3D7_072500 (Supplementary Figures 9 and 12 respectively). In the light that protein levels appear not changed, we conclude that trafficking is accelerated during these earlier timepoints, but remains comparable at later stages. This would still increase the overall bound parasite mass as parasites start to adhere earlier during or after a heat stress.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This manuscript describes experiments characterising how malaria parasites respond to physiologically relevant heat-shock conditions. The authors show, quite convincingly, that moderate heat-shock appears to increase cytoadherance, likely by increasing trafficking of surface proteins involved in this process.

      While generally of a high quality and including a lot of data, I have a few small questions and comments, mainly regarding data interpretation.

      (1) The authors use sorbitol lysis as a proxy for trafficking of PSAC components. This is a very roundabout way of doing things and does not, I think, really show what they claim. There could be a myriad of other reasons for this increased activity (indeed, the authors note potential PSAC activation under these conditions). One further reason could be a difference in the membrane stability following heat shock, which may affect sorbitol uptake, or the fragility of the erythrocytes to hypotonic shock. I really suggest that the authors stick to what they show (increased PSAC) without trying to use this as evidence for increased trafficking of a number of non-specified proteins that they cannot follow directly.

      This is a valid point, however, uninfected RBCs do not lyse following heat stress, nor do much younger iRBCs, indicating that the observed effect is specific to infected RBCs at a defined stage. The sorbitol sensitivity assay is performed at 37°C under normal conditions after cells are returned to non–heat stress temperatures, so the effect is not due to transient changes in membrane permeability at elevated temperature. 

      Planned experiment: However, to increase the strength of our conclusions and further test our hypothesis, we will perform sorbitol sensitivity assays on >20 hours post infection iRBCs following heat stress in the presence and absence of furosemide, a PSAC inhibitor. If iRBC lysis is abolished with furosemide present, this would confirm that the effect is PSAC-dependent. However, the effect could also possibly be due to altered PSAC activity during heat stress which is maintained at lower temperatures, as outlined in the discussion.

      (2) Supplementary Figure 6C/D: The KAHRP signal does not look like it should. In fact, it doesn't look like anything specific. The HSP70-X signal is also blurry and overexposed. These pictures cannot be used to justify the authors' statements about a lack of colocalisation in any way.

      Planned experiment: We agree that the IFAs are not the best as presented and will include better quality supplementary images in a revised version.

      (3) Figure 6: This experiment confuses me. The authors purport to fractionate proteins using differential lysis, but the proteins they detect are supposed to be transmembrane proteins and thus should always be found associated with the pellet, whether lysis is done using equinatoxin or saponin. Have they discovered a currently unknown trafficking pathway to tell us about? Whilst there is a lot of discussion about the trafficking pathways for TM proteins through the host cell, a number of studies have shown that these proteins are generally found in a membrane-bound state. The authors should elaborate, or choose an experiment that is capable of showing compartment-specific localisation of membrane-bound proteins (protease protection, for example).

      We do not believe we identified a novel trafficking pathway, but that we capture trafficking intermediates of PfEMP1 between the PVM and the RBC periphery, in either small vesicles, and/ or possibly Maurer’s clefts. These would still be membrane embedded, but because of their small size, not be pelleted using the centrifugation speeds in our study (we did not use ultracentrifugation). This explanation, we believe, is in line with the current hypothesis of PfEMP1 and other exported TMD protein trafficking to the periphery or the Maurer’s clefts.

      (4) The red blood cell contains, in addition to HSP70-X, a number of human HSPs (HSP70 and HSP90 are significant in this current case). As the name suggests, these proteins non-specifically shield exposed hydrophobic domains revealed upon partial protein unfolding following thermal insult. I would thus have expected to find significantly more enrichment following heat shock, but this is not the case. Is it possible that the physiological heat shock conditions used in this current study are not high enough to cause a real heat shock?

      As noted by the reviewer, we do not see enrichment of red blood cell heat shock proteins following heat stress, either with FIKK10.2-TurboID or in the phosphoproteome. We used a physiologically relevant heat stress that significantly modifies the iRBC, as shown by our functional assays. While a higher temperature might induce an association of red blood cell heat shock proteins, such conditions may not accurately reflect the most commonly found context of malaria infection.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, it is established that high fever-like 39 C temperatures cause parasite-infected red blood cells to become stickier. It is thought that high temperatures might help the spleen to destroy parasite-infected cells, and they become stickier in order to remain trapped in blood vessels, so they stop passing through the spleen.

      Strengths:

      The strength of this research is that it shows that fever-like temperatures can cause parasite-infected red blood cells to stick to surfaces designed to mimic the walls of small blood vessels. In a natural infection, this would cause parasite-infected red blood cells to stop circulating through the spleen, where the parasites would be destroyed by the immune system. It is thought that fevers could lead to infected red blood cells becoming stiffer and therefore more easily destroyed in the spleen. Parasites respond to fevers by making their red blood cells stickier, so they stop flowing around the body and into the spleen. The experiments here prove that fever temperatures increase the export of Velcro-like sticky proteins onto the surface of the infected red blood cells and are very thorough and convincing.

      Weaknesses:

      A minor weakness of the paper is that the effects of fever on the stiffness of infected red blood cells were not measured. This can be easily done in the laboratory by measuring how the passage of infected red blood cells through a bed of tiny metal balls is delayed under fever-like temperatures.

      Previous work by Marinkovic et al. (cited in this manuscript) reported that all RBCs, both infected and uninfected, increase in stiffness at 41 °C compared with 37 °C, with trophozoites and schizonts exhibiting a particularly pronounced increase. We agree that it would be interesting to determine whether similar changes occur at physiological fever-like temperatures, and whether this increase in stiffness coincides with the period of elevated protein trafficking. However, since we have already demonstrated enhanced protein export using multiple complementary approaches, we have chosen to address these questions in a follow-up study.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We would like to express our sincere gratitude to the reviewers for their thorough analysis of the manuscript and their extremely helpful comments. We have taken all the suggestions into consideration and conducted a range of additional experiments to address the points raised. We have also extensively revised the manuscript to clarify descriptions, correct inaccuracies and remove inconsistencies. We have modified the figures for clarity and content.

      Overall, we expanded the description of the EBH structure to emphasise its dimeric nature and the impact of the two binding sites on interpreting the binding data, including cooperativity. Using ITC, we tested the effect of the pre-SxIP residues on the binding affinity with additional peptides. We found that these residues had a significant effect, albeit much smaller than that of the post-SxIP residues. We analysed the binding of the 11MACF-VLL mutant with EBH-ΔC and evaluated the exchange rates. In agreement with our model, we found that the EBH affinity for the SxIP peptide from CK5P2 (KKSRLPRILIKRSR), which has a C-terminal sequence similar to that of the 11MACF-VLLRK mutant, is 21nM, which is similar to the affinity of the mutant itself. This demonstrates the significant variation in affinity observed among natural SxIP ligands, as predicted by our study. Our responses to the specific points raised by the reviewers are provided below.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      There is no direct experimental evidence for independent dock and lock steps. The model is certainly plausible given their structural data, but all titration and CEST measurements are fully consistent with a simple one-step binding mechanism. Indeed, it is acknowledged that the results for the VLL peptide are not consistent with the predictions of this model, as affinity and dissociation rates do not co-vary. The model may still be a helpful way to interpret and discuss their results, and may indeed be the correct mechanism, but this has not yet been proven.

      Unfortunately, it is not possible to obtain direct experimental evidence because the folding of the C-terminus is too fast to influence the NMR parameters. However, as the reviewer pointed out, our structural data support the two-step model, since folding of the C-terminus is only possible once the ligand containing the post-SxIP residues has bound. By adopting a mechanistically supported model, we can analyse the contributions to binding and relate them to the structural characteristics of the complex. This provides a clearer insight into the roles of the various regions in the interaction and allows to modify them rationally to enhance the ligand affinity.

      In the revised version, we restate the equations in terms of comparing the on-rates. This provides a clearer view of the effect of the additional stage, which cannot increase the overall on-rate since the two stages are sequential. If the forward rate of the second stage is comparable to or slower than the off-rate of the first stage, the overall on-rate decreases. Conversely, if the forward rate is much faster, the overall on-rate remains unchanged. For the wild-type 11MACF peptide, we observed that the presence of the EBH C-terminus does not affect the on-rate of binding, which is in perfect agreement with the two-step model and indicates that the C-terminus folds very quickly.

      Additionally, we evaluated the binding of the 11MACF-VLL mutant to EBH-ΔC and observed a twofold decrease in Kd compared to WT 11MAC, primarily due to an increase in the on-rate. Interestingly, this rate is approximately twice as low as the overall on-rate for EBH/11MACF-VLL binding, contradicting the sequential two-step model. This suggests a more complex binding process where binding is accelerated by additional hydrophobic interactions with the unfolded C-terminus. However, given the difficulty of quantifying very slow exchange rates, it is more likely that the discrepancy is due to the accuracy of the rate measurements. Therefore, the model allows the rational analysis of changes in binding parameters due to mutations.

      There is little discussion of the fact that binding occurs to EBH dimers -  either in terms of the functional significance of this or in the  acquisition and analysis of their data. There is no discussion of  cooperation in binding (or its absence), either in the analysis of NMR  titrations or in ITC measurements. Complete ITC fit results have not  been reported so it is not possible to evaluate this for oneself.

      We added information about the dimer to the introduction, emphasising its role in enhancing interaction with microtubules (MTs) and its structural role in SxIP binding. The ITC data do not exhibit any biphasic behaviour and can be fitted to a single-site model with 1:1 stoichiometry relative to the EB1c monomer. This corresponds to two independent binding sites in the dimer. We have added the stoichiometry to Table 1 and the description. The NMR titration data for the 11MACF and 11MACF-VLL interactions were fitted to the TITAN dimer model, which includes cooperativity parameters. For WT 11MACF, both cooperativity parameters were zero, corresponding to independent binding sites in the ITC model. For 11MACF-VLL, the fitting suggests weak negative cooperativity, with a ~3-fold increase in Kd for binding to the second site and no change in the off-rate. This difference in Kd is likely to be too small to induce a biphasic shape to the ITC curve. As the cooperativity effect on the NMR spectra is small and absent in the ITC, we used the independent sites model for data analysis, as there is insufficient justification for introducing extra parameters into the model. Crucially, fitting to this model did not alter the off-rate value obtained by NMR or affect the conclusions. We added a description of cooperativity to the results and discussion.

      Three peptides are used to examine the role of C-terminal residues in SxIP motifs: 4-MACF (SKIP), 6-MACF (SKIPTP), and 11-MACF (KPSKIPTPQRK). The 11-mer demonstrates the strongest binding, but this has added residues to the N-terminal as well. It has also introduced charges at both termini, further complicating the interpretation of changes in binding affinities. Given this, I do not believe the authors can reasonably attribute increased affinities solely to post-SxIP residues.

      We tested the 9MACF peptide SKIPTPQRK, which has the same N-terminus as the 4- and 6-MACF peptides, and found that its binding affinity is ~10-fold weaker than that of 11MACF. This demonstrates the contribution of both the pre- and post-SxIP residues. This is likely due to electrostatic interactions between the positively charged N-terminus and the negatively charged EBH surface, similar to those involving the positive charges at the peptide C-terminus. Although significant, the contribution of the N-terminal peptide region is approximately one order of magnitude lower than that of the post-SxIP residues, meaning the post-SxIP region is the main affinity modulator. We have added the binding data on 9MACF and a discussion of the contributions to the manuscript.

      Experimental uncertainties are, with exceptions, not reported.

      Uncertainties added to the number in Table 1 and the text. Information on how uncertainties were calculated added to Table 1.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Have you tested the binding of the WT dimer in your cell model?

      We haven’t tested the WT dimer because it has already been reported in the 2009 Cell paper by Honappa et al. In the cell experiments, our main focus was on recruiting the high-affinity mutant to MTs. The low level of recruitment, despite the mutant's high affinity, highlights the importance of dimerisation or additional contributions to binding.

      (2) Please deposit all NMR dynamics measurements (relaxation rates and derived model-free parameters) alongside structural data in the BMRB.

      The relaxation data have been submitted to BMRB, IDs 53187 and 53188

      (3) Please report complete fitting results, e.g. for ITC, including stoichiometries. Clarify what this means for binding to a dimer, and if there is any evidence of cooperativity. Figure 3C, right hand panel, shows an unusual stoichiometry, can the authors comment on this?

      We have added more information on stoichiometry and cooperativity; please refer to our response to the above comment for details. We repeated the titration for the VLLRK mutant using fresh peptide stock. As expected, the stoichiometry was close to 1:1 relative to the EB1c monomer. The new data are now included in the table and figure.

      (4) Please report uncertainties for all measurements of Kd, koff, kon, ∆G, ∆H, ∆S, and explain whether these are determined from statistical analysis, technical or biological repeats (and where reported, clarify between standard deviation/standard error). Please also be aware of standard guidelines for reporting significant figures for data with uncertainties, as these have not been followed in Table 1.

      Uncertainties added to the number in Table 1 and the text. Information on how uncertainties were calculated added to Table 1.

      (5) The construct design for the cell model is unclear - given the importance of flanking residues, please report and discuss how the sequences are attached to venus: which termini is attached, and what is the linker composition?

      We cloned the peptides at the C-terminus of mTFP, after the GS linker of the vector. The peptide itself contains a GS sequence at the N-terminus, creating a highly flexible GSGS linker that separates the SxIP region from mTFP and minimises the potential effect of mTFP on binding. We followed the design of Honappa et al. to enable direct comparison with the published results. We have added this information to the 'Methods' section..

      (6) Which HSQC pulse sequence was used for 2D lineshape analysis? The authors mention non-linear chemical shift changes, presumably associated with the dimer interface - this would be useful to expand upon and clarify.

      For the lineshape analysis, we used the standard Bruker sequence hsqcfpf3gpphwg with soft-pulse watergate water suppression and flip-back. This sequence is included in the TITAN model. We added the description of the non-linear chemical shift changes and connection of these changes to the allosteric effect of the binding to the supplementary information describing details of the lineshape analysis.

      (7) Figure 1A could usefully highlight the dimer interface in the surface representation also.

      We believe that including the interface would make the figure too complicated. The dimer configuration is shown in different colours for the two subunits, clearly demonstrating their involvement in forming the binding site.

      (8) Figures 1C and 1D could usefully show a secondary structure schematic to assist the reader. The x-axis in these figures is not linear and this should be corrected. The calculation of combined chemical shift perturbations should be described.

      Thank you for the helpful suggestion. We changed the scale of the figures and added the diagram of the secondary structure.

      (9) Units are missing from many figure axes.

      We added missing units to the axes. Thank you for highlighting this.

      (10) What peptide concentrations are used in Figure 1C? Presumably, these should be reported at saturation for this to be a fair comparison, this should be clarified.

      The protein concentration was 50 µM. Peptides 4MACF and 6MACF were added at a 100-fold molar excess and peptide 11MACF was added at a 4-fold excess. Saturation was achieved for 11MACF. This was impossible for the short peptides due to their mM affinity. This information has been added to the figure legend. The figure's main aim is to illustrate the differences in the chemical shift perturbation profiles, which can be achieved even if full saturation is not attained. Although the absolute value of the chemical shifts is proportional to the degree of saturation, the distribution of the largest chemical shift changes is independent of this degree. Therefore, we can draw conclusions about the distribution of changes by comparing under non-saturation conditions.

      (11) The presentation of raw peak intensities in Figure 1D shows primarily the flexibility of the C-terminal region associated with high intensities. Beyond this, when comparing the binding of peptides it would be much more informative to show relative peak intensities. Residues around 210-225 appear to show strong broadening in the presence of peptide, but this is masked by the low initial intensity. Can the authors clarify and discuss this? Also, what peptide concentrations were used for this comparison? For a fair comparison, it should be close to saturation - particularly to exclude exchange broadening contributions.

      The protein concentration was 50 µM. 6MACF and 6MACF peptides were added at a 100-fold excess and 11MACF at a 4-fold excess. Saturation was achieved for 11MACF. This was impossible to achieve for the short peptide due to its mM affinity. This information has been added to the figure legend. Upon checking the data, we found a small systematic offset in the coiled-coil region of some of the complexes, as the integral intensity had been used in the initial plot. While this does not change the conclusion regarding the high dynamics of the C-terminus, it does create an inaccurate perception of the relative intensities of the folded regions in the different complexes, as noted by the reviewer. We have now plotted the amplitudes at the maximum of the peaks, which do not exhibit any systematic offset as they are much less susceptible to baseline distortions. We are grateful to the reviewer for highlighting this apparent discrepancy.

      (12) Figure 2 - the scale for S2 order parameters appears to be backwards, given the caption, but its range should be indicated. Similarly, the range of values for Rex should also be indicated. These data should also be tabulated/plotted in supporting information.

      We have corrected the figure legend and added S2 and Rex plots to the supplementary material. The figure aims to highlight regions of increased mobility, while the plots provide full quantitative information on the values. We thank the reviewer for pointing out the error in the figure legend and for the suggestions regarding the plots.

      (13) The scale in Figure 3B is illegible. Indeed, the whole structure is quite small and could usefully be expanded.

      We increased the size of the structure panels and added a scale.

      (14) Figure 4 does not show a decrease in exchange rates, as per the caption - no comparison of exchange rates is shown, only thermodynamic information in panel E. Panel C shows CEST measurements, but it is not clear what system this is for - please clarify, and consider showing the comparable data for the ∆C construct for comparison.

      We have amended the figure legend to clarify that the figure shows binding parameters. We added information about the CEST profiles for the EBH/11MACF interaction to the figure legend (Figure 4C). Exchange with the ∆C construct is too fast for CEST measurements. We used lineshape analysis to evaluate the exchange rates for this construct.

      (15) The schematics shown in Figure 4D, and elsewhere, are really quite difficult to understand. They may pose additional challenges to colourblind readers. Please consider ways that this could be clarified.

      We simplified the colour scheme in the model to make the colours easier to see and to highlight SxIP and non-SxIP regions. We believe that this improved the clarity of the figure.

      (16) Figures S1D/E - the x-axes are unclear and units are missing from the y-axes.

      We re-labelled the axes to clarify the scale and units. Thank you for pointing this.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The C-terminal tail of EB1, which is adjacent to EBH and is not analyzed in this study, is highly acidic and plays an important role in protein interactions. If the authors discuss the C-terminus of EB1, they should analyze the whole C-terminus of EB1, which would strengthen the conclusion they have made.

      Honapa et al., Cell, 2009, reported chemical shift perturbations (CSPs) on the peptide binding for the full EB1c fragment, which includes the negatively charged C-terminus. Similar to our study, they observed significant CSPs in the FVIP region but negligible CSPs at the negatively charged EEY end. They concluded that the final eight EB1c residues did not contribute to binding and used a truncated EB1c construct for their structural analysis. Building on that study, we used the same EEY-truncated construct to analyse the contribution of the C-terminus in more detail. We believe that conducting additional experiments with the full C-terminus with respect to SxIP binding would be superfluous, as it would merely replicate the findings of Honapa EA. We have added the rationale for selecting the truncated EB1c construct to the text, referencing Honapa et al.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) Figure 2C: The authors can analyze the 11MACF peptide as well, to provide more assurance to their argument. It would be easier to distinguish the sequences of "SKIP" and "FVIP" by changing their colors.

      Our relaxation analysis (Fig. 2C) focuses on the dynamics of the unstructured C-terminal region in both the free and complex forms. Further relaxation analysis of the peptide would not provide additional information on this, and would be complicated by the presence of free peptide in solution.

      (2) Figure 3B: Acidic residues in EBH should be labeled.<br /> Page 6, line 11: If the authors insist that the acidic patch will influence the interactions between EB1 and the peptide, the data of the analysis using the entire EB1 C-terminus should be included, given that the C-terminal tail of EB1 is highly acidic.

      To test the contribution of charge to binding, we conducted an ITC experiment at increasing salt concentrations. We observed a significant increase in Kd values when the concentration of NaCl increased from 50 to 150 mM, which supports our conclusion regarding the significant electrostatic contribution. This conclusion is independent of the presence or absence of the C-terminus.

      As we explained earlier, Honapa et al., Cell 2009, conducted an NMR experiment on the full EB1c and observed no CPSs in the EEY region, indicating a negligible contribution from the EEY region to SxIP binding. Therefore, we think that additional experiments involving the entire C-terminus are unnecessary, as they would simply replicate the results of Honapa et al. We have added the rationale for selecting the truncated EB1c to the text, referencing Honapa et al.

      It would be very difficult to label the acidic residues without enlarging 3B considerably. However, we do not think this is necessary as we are not discussing any specific residues. The current figure shows the distribution of the surface charge, which is sufficient for our purposes.

      (3) Figure 2B (Page 4, line 27): The side chain of S5477 should be drawn. The authors should include a figure of the crystal structure of EBH and SxIP as a comparison (Honnappa et al., Cell, 2009). In their paper, Honnappa et al. performed chemical shift perturbation titrations by NMR. From their analysis, I imagine that the EB1 tail may not be critical for the EB1 C-terminus:SxIP interactions, since the signals in the tail are not significantly perturbed. The authors should cite this paper.

      We are grateful to the reviewer for highlighting this. CSP analysis of the Honapa EA revealed significant changes in the FVIP region, which we also observed. They also reported negligible CSPs at the EEY end, demonstrating that this part of the tail is non-critical and can be removed. We have added text to the manuscript to highlight the similarity between CSPs and those observed in Honapa EA. Figure 2B shows the side chains for the residues with the strongest detected contacts. These do not include S5477.

      (4) Figure 3C (ITC data): The stoichiometric ratios in the ITC data look strange. EBH vs KPSKIPVLLRKRK, is it 1:1?

      We repeated the ITC experiments using a new stock of the peptide and a new batch of the protein, checking the concentrations using UV spectroscopy. The new experiments produced a stoichiometry close to 1, as shown in the table.

      (5) Page 10, line 27: "The TPQ sequence of 11MACF is not optimal...": What is the meaning of "optimal"? The transient interaction between EB1 and its binding partner is responsible for the dynamics of the microtubule cytoskeleton. In a sense, the relatively weak interaction is "optimal" for the system. The authors should rephrase the word.

      We agree that weak interactions are optimal from a functional perspective, as they have been selected through evolution. In our case, 'optimal' refers to the hydrophobic interaction with the C-terminus. We replaced 'optimal' with 'ideal' to draw more attention to the second part of the sentence, which clarifies the context.

      (6) Page 11, line 2: "small number of comets enriched in the peptide that were too faint for the quantitative analysis, comparable to the reported previously (Honnappa, Gouveia et al. 2009)." Honnappa et al. used EGFP-fusion constructs in their study: EGFP forms a weak dimer, which presumably gave different results from the authors' mTFP-constructs. The authors can note this point in the text.

      We are grateful to the reviewer for highlighting this. This aligns well with our conclusion that dimerisation is important for localisation to comets. We have added this point to the text.

      (7) Page 10, line 21: The authors calculate the free energy of complex formation between EBH and MACF peptide and explain in the text, but it is hard to follow.

      We simplified and clarified the description of the energy contributions by focusing on the SxIP and non-SxIP regions of the peptide, as well as the EBH C-terminus.

      Minor points:

      Page 2, line 9: IP motifs are not usually located in the C-terminus. For example, SxIP in Tastin is located in the N-terminal region, and SxIPs in CLASP are in the middle.

      We corrected this statement, removing C-terminal.

      Page 3, line 4: The authors should note the residue numbers of SKIP.

      We think that in this context the residue number of the SxIP region are not important and would be distracting.

      Figure 3D and Figure S3F: Make the colors and the order the same between the two figures.

      We changed the colour scheme and the order of ITC parameters in S3F to match the main figure.

      Figure 1A, 2B, Figure S5: Change the color of SKIP from other residues in the same chain, otherwise the readers cannot distinguish. Likewise, change the color of FVIP in Figure 2B.

      We think that changing the colours will complicate the figures unnecessary. The corresponding residues are clearly labelled in the figures.

      Figure 3, Figure S5, S6, S7: Box the letters of SKIP for clarity.

      We boxed the SxIP region in S5 (new S6) and underlined in S6 (new S7). In S7 (new S8) the location of SxIP is very clear from the homology.

      Figure 3B; Figure S2: Hard to recognize the peptide (MACF in green).

      We increased the size of 3D and S2, making it easier to see the peptide.

      Figure 1C and D: Make the residual numbers of the x-axes the same between the two graphs.

      We made new plots with a linear scale for the residue numbers.

      Figure 2A: The structures shown are not EB1. It should be described as EBH or EB1(191-260 a.a.).

      Corrected.

      Page 5, line 17: "the S2 values of the C-terminus" should be "the S2 values of the C-terminal loop in EBH", otherwise it is confusing.

      Corrected.

      Page 6, line 27; Figure S3C and S6: Please indicate the assignments of the resonances from "253FVI255" in the Figures.

      We labelled the peaks corresponding to the 253FVI255 region in figure S6 (new S7). Figure S3 shows EBH-ΔC that does not include this region.

      Page 7, line 25: Figure S7 should be S8.

      Corrected

      Page 12, line 6: "sulfatrahsferases" must by a typo.

      Corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This paper presents a computational model of the evolution of two different kinds of helping ("work," presumably denoting provisioning, and defense tasks) in a model inspired by cooperatively breeding vertebrates. The helpers in this model are a mix of previous offspring of the breeder and floaters that might have joined the group, and can either transition between the tasks as they age or not. The two types of help have differential costs: "work" reduces "dominance value," (DV), a measure of competitiveness for breeding spots, which otherwise goes up linearly with age, but defense reduces survival probability. Both eventually might preclude the helper from becoming a breeder and reproducing. How much the helpers help, and which tasks (and whether they transition or not), as well as their propensity to disperse, are all evolving quantities. The authors consider three main scenarios: one where relatedness emerges from the model, but there is no benefit to living in groups, one where there is no relatedness, but living in larger groups gives a survival benefit (group augmentation, GA), and one where both effects operate. The main claim is that evolving defensive help or division of labor requires the group augmentation; it doesn't evolve through kin selection alone in the authors' simulations.

      This is an interesting model, and there is much to like about the complexity that is built in. Individual-based simulations like this can be a valuable tool to explore the complex interaction of life history and social traits. Yet, models like this also have to take care of both being very clear on their construction and exploring how some of the ancillary but potentially consequential assumptions affect the results, including robust exploration of the parameter space. I think the current manuscript falls short in these areas, and therefore, I am not yet convinced of the results. Much of this is a matter of clearer and more complete writing: the Materials and Methods section in particular is incomplete or vague in some important junctions. However, there are also some issues with the assumptions that are described clearly.

      Below, I describe my main issues, mostly having to do with model features that are unclear, poorly motivated (as they stand), or potentially unrealistic or underexplored.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for the thoughtful comments that helped us to greatly improve the clarity of our paper.  

      One of the main issues I have is that there is almost no information on what happens to dispersers in the model. Line 369-67 states dispersers might join another group or remain as floaters, but gives no further information on how this is determined. Poring through the notation table also comes up empty as there is no apparent parameter affecting this consequential life history event. At some point, I convinced myself that dispersers remain floaters until they die or become breeders, but several points in the text contradict this directly (e.g., l 107). Clearly this is a hugely important model feature since it determines fitness cost and benefits of dispersal and group size (which also affects relatedness and/or fitness depending on the model). There just isn't enough information to understand this crucial component of the model, and without it, it is hard to make sense of the model output.

      We use the same dispersal gene β to represent the likelihood an individual will either leave or join a group, thereby quantifying both dispersal and immigration using the same parameter. Specifically, individuals with higher β are more likely to remain as floaters (i.e., disperse from their natal group to become a breeder elsewhere), whereas those with lower β are either more likely to remain in their natal group as subordinates (i.e., queue in a group for the breeding position) or join another group if they dispersed.  

      We added in the text “Dispersers may migrate to another group to become subordinates or remain as floaters waiting for breeding opportunities, which is also controlled by the same genetic dispersal propensity as subordinates” to clarify this issue. We also added in Table 1 that β is the “genetic predisposition to disperse versus remain in a group”, and to Figure 1 that “subordinates in the group (natal and immigrants) […]” after we already clarified that “Dispersers/floaters may join a random group to become subordinates.”

      Related to that, it seems to be implied (but never stated explicitly) that floaters do not work, and therefore their DV increases linearly with age (H_work in eq.2 is zero). That means any floaters that manage to stick around long enough would have higher success in competition for breeding spots relative to existing group members. How realistic is this? I think this might be driving the kin selection-only results that defense doesn't evolve without group augmentation (one of the two main ways). Any subordinates (which are mainly zero in the no GA, according to the SI tables; this assumes N=breeder+subordinates, but this isn't explicit anywhere) would be outcompeted by floaters after a short time (since they evolve high H and floaters don't), which in turn increases the benefit of dispersal, explaining why it is so high. Is this parameter regime reasonable? My understanding is that floaters often aren't usually high resource holding potential individuals (either b/c high RHP ones would get selected out of the floater population by establishing territories or b/c floating isn't typically a thriving strategy, given that many resources are tied to territories). In this case, the assumption seems to bias things towards the floaters and against subordinates to inherit territories. This should be explored either with a higher mortality rate for floaters and/or a lower DV increase, or both.

      When it comes to floaters replacing dead breeders, the authors say a bit more, but again, the actual equation for the scramble competition (which only appears as "scramble context" in the notation table) is not given. Is it simply proportional to R_i/\sum_j R_j ? Or is there some other function used? What are the actual numbers of floaters per breeding territory that emerge under different parameter values? These are all very important quantities that have to be described clearly.

      Although it is true that dispersers do not work when they are floaters, they may later help if they immigrate into a group as a subordinate. Consequently, immigrant subordinates have no inherent competitive advantage over natal subordinates (as step 2.2. “Join a group” is followed by step 3. “Help”, which occurs before step 5. “Become a breeder”). Nevertheless, floaters can potentially outcompete subordinates of the same age if they attempt to breed without first queuing as a subordinate (step 5) when subordinates are engaged in work tasks. We believe that this assumption is realistic and constitutes part of the costs associated with work tasks. However, floaters are at a disadvantage for becoming a breeder because: (1) floaters incur higher mortality than individuals within groups (Eq. 3); and (2) floaters may only attempt to become breeders in some breeding cycles (versus subordinate groups members, who are automatically candidates for an open breeding position in the group in each cycle). Therefore, due to their higher mortality, floaters are rarely older than individuals within groups, which heavily influences their dominance value and competitiveness. Additionally, any competitive advantage that floaters might have over other subordinate group members is unlikely to drive the kin selection-only results because subordinates would preferably choose defense tasks instead of work tasks so as not to be at a competitive disadvantage compared to floaters.  

      Regarding whether floaters aren't usually high resource holding potential (RHP) individuals and, therefore, our assumptions might be unrealistic; empirical work in a number of species has shown that dispersers are not necessarily those of lower RHP or of lower quality. In fact, according to the ecological constraints hypothesis, one might predict that high quality individuals are the ones that disperse because only individuals in good condition (e.g., larger body size, better energy reserves) can afford the costs associated with dispersal (Cote et al., 2022). To allow differences in dispersal propensity depending on RHP, we extended our model in the Supplemental Materials by incorporating a reaction norm of dispersal based on their rank (D = 1 / (1 + exp (β<sub>R</sub> * Rβ<sub>0</sub>)) under the section “Dominance-dependent dispersal propensities” and now referenced in L195. This approach allows individuals to adjust their dispersal strategy to their competitiveness and to avoid kin competition by remaining as a subordinate in another group. Results show that the addition of the reaction norm of dispersal to rank did not qualitatively influence the results described in the main text.  

      We also added “number of floaters” present in the whole population to the summary tables as requested.  

      As a side note, the “scramble context” we mention was an additional implementation in which we made rank independent of age. However, since the main conclusions remained unchanged, we decided to remove it for simplicity from the final manuscript, but we forgot to remove it from Table 1 before submission.  

      I also think the asexual reproduction with small mutations assumption is a fairly strong one that also seems to bias the model outcomes in a particular way. I appreciate that the authors actually measured relatedness within groups (though if most groups under KS have no subordinates, that relatedness becomes a bit moot), and also eliminated it with their ingenious swapping-out-subordinates procedure. The fact remains that unless they eliminate relatedness completely, average relatedness, by design, will be very high. (Again, this is also affected by how the fate of the dispersers is determined, but clearly there isn't a lot of joining happening, just judging from mean group sizes under KS only.) This is, of course, why there is so much helping evolving (even if it's not defensive) unless they completely cut out relatedness.

      As we showed in the Supplementary Tables and the section on relatedness in the SI (“Kin selection and the evolution of division of labor"), high relatedness does not appear to explain our results. In evolutionary biology generally and in game theory specifically (with the exception of models on sexual selection or sex-specific traits), asexual reproduction is often modelled because it reduces unnecessary complexity. To further study the effect of relatedness on kin structures more closely resembling those of vertebrates, however, we created an additional “relatedness structure level”, where we shuffled half of the philopatric offspring using the same method used to remove relatedness completely, effectively reducing withingroup relatedness structure by half. As shown in the new Figure S3, the conclusions of the model remain unchanged.  

      Finally, the "need for division of labor" section is also unclear, and its construction also would seem to bias things against division of labor evolving. For starters, I don't understand the rationale for the convoluted way the authors create an incentive for division of labor. Why not implement something much simpler, like a law of minimum (i.e., the total effect of helping is whatever the help amount for the lowest value task is) or more intuitively: the fecundity is simply a function of "work" help (draw Poisson number of offspring) and survival of offspring (draw binomial from the fecundity) is a function of the "defense" help. As it is, even though the authors say they require division of labor, in fact, they only make a single type of help marginally less beneficial (basically by half) if it is done more than the other. That's a fairly weak selection for division of labor, and to me it seems hard to justify. I suspect either of the alternative assumptions above would actually impose enough selection to make division of labor evolve even without group augmentation.

      In nature, multiple tasks are often necessary to successfully rear offspring. We simplify this principle in the model by maximizing reproductive output when both tasks are carried out to a similar extent, allowing for some flexibility from the mean. We added to the manuscript “For example, in many cooperatively breeding birds, the primary reasons that individuals fail to produce offspring are (1) starvation, which is mitigated by the feeding of offspring, and (2) nest depredation, which is countered by defensive behavior. Consequently, both types of tasks are necessary to successfully produce offspring, and focusing solely on one while neglecting the other is likely to result in lower reproductive success than if both tasks are performed by individuals within the group.”

      Regarding making fecundity a function of work tasks and offspring survival as a function of defensive tasks, these are actually equivalent in model terms, as it’s the same whether breeders produce three offspring and two die, or if they only produce one. This represents, of course, an oversimplification of the natural context, where breeding unsuccessfully is more costly (in terms of time and energy investment) than not breeding at all.

      Overall, this is an interesting model, but the simulation is not adequately described or explored to have confidence in the main conclusions yet. Better exposition and more exploration of alternative assumptions and parameter space are needed.

      We hope that our clarifications and extension of the model satisfy your concerns.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper formulates an individual-based model to understand the evolution of division of labor in vertebrates. A main conclusion of the paper is that direct fitness benefits are the primary factor causing the evolution of vertebrate division of labor, rather than indirect fitness benefits.

      Strengths:

      The paper formulates an individual-based model that is inspired by vertebrate life history. The model incorporates numerous biologically realistic details, including the possibility to evolve age polytheism where individuals switch from work to defence tasks as they age or vice versa, as well as the possibility of comparing the action of group augmentation alone with that of kin selection alone.

      Weaknesses:

      The model makes assumptions that restrict the possibility that kin selection leads to the evolution of helping. In particular, the model assumes that in the absence of group augmentation, subordinates can only help breeders but cannot help non-breeders or increase the survival of breeders, whereas with group augmentation, subordinates can help both breeders and non-breeders and increase the survival of breeders. This is unrealistic as subordinates in real organisms can help other subordinates and increase the survival of non-breeders, even in the absence of group augmentation, for instance, with targeted helping to dominants or allies. This restriction artificially limits the ability of kin selection alone to lead to the evolution of helping, and potentially to division of labor. Hence, the conclusion that group augmentation is the primary driving factor driving vertebrate division of labor appears forced by the imposed restrictions on kin selection. The model used is also quite particular, and so the claimed generality across vertebrates is not warranted.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for the in-depth review. We respond to these and other comments below.  

      I describe some suggestions for improving the paper below, more or less in the paper's order.

      First, the introduction goes to great lengths trying to convince the reader that this model is the first in this or another way, particularly in being only for vertebrates, as illustrated in the abstract where it is stated that "we lack a theoretical framework to explore the conditions under which division of labor is likely to evolve" (line 13). However, this is a risky and unnecessary motivation. There are many models of division of labor and some of them are likely to be abstract enough to apply to vertebrates even if they are not tailored to vertebrates, so the claims for being first are not only likely to be wrong but will put many readers in an antagonistic position right from the start, which will make it harder to communicate the results. Instead of claiming to be the first or that there is a lack of theoretical frameworks for vertebrate division of labor, I think it is enough and sufficiently interesting to say that the paper formulates an individual-based model motivated by the life history of vertebrates to understand the evolution of vertebrate division of labor. You could then describe the life history properties that the model incorporates (subordinates can become reproductive, low relatedness, age polyethism, etc.) without saying this has never been done or that it is exclusive to vertebrates; indeed, the paper states that these features do not occur in eusocial insects, which is surprising as some "primitively" eusocial insects show them. So, in short, I think the introduction should be extensively revised to avoid claims of being the first and to make it focused on the question being addressed and how it is addressed. I think this could be done in 2-3 paragraphs without the rather extensive review of the literature in the current introduction.

      We have revised the novelty statements in the Introduction by more clearly emphasizing how our model addresses gaps in the existing literature. More details are provided in the comments below.

      Second, the description of the model and results should be clarified substantially. I will give specific suggestions later, but for now, I will just say that it is unclear what the figures show. First, it is unclear what the axes in Figure 2 show, particularly for the vertical one. According to the text in the figure axis, it presumably refers to T, but T is a function of age t, so it is unclear what is being plotted. The legend explaining the triangle and circle symbols is unintelligible (lines 227-230), so again it is unclear what is being plotted; part of the reason for this unintelligibility is that the procedure that presumably underlies it (section starting on line 493) is poorly explained and not understandable (I detail why below). Second, the axes in Figure 3 are similarly unclear. The text in the vertical axis in panel A suggests this is T, however, T is a function of t and gamma_t, so something else must be being done to plot this. Similarly, in panel B, the horizontal axis is presumably R, but R is a function of t and of the helping genotype, so again some explanation is lacking. In all figures, the symbol of what is being plotted should be included.

      We added the symbols of the variables to the Figure axes to increase clarity. In Figure 3A, we corrected the subindex t in the x-axis; it should be subindex R (reaction norm to dominance rank instead of age). As described in Table 1, all values of T, H and R are phenotypically expressed values. For instance, T values are the phenotypically expressed values from the individuals in the population according to their genetic gamma values and their current dominance rank at a given time point.  

      Third, the conclusions sound stronger than the results are. A main conclusion of the paper is that "kin selection alone is unlikely to select for the evolution of defensive tasks and division of labor in vertebrates" (lines 194-195). This conclusion is drawn from the left column in Figure 2, where only kin selection is at play, and the helping that evolves only involves work rather than defense tasks. This conclusion follows because the model assumes that without group augmentation (i.e., xn=0, the kin selection scenario), subordinates can only help breeders to reproduce but cannot help breeders or other subordinates to survive, so the only form of help that evolves is the least costly, not the most beneficial as there is no difference in the benefits given among forms of helping. This assumption is unrealistic, particularly for vertebrates where subordinates can help other group members survive even in the absence of group augmentation (e.g., with targeted help to certain group members, because of dominance hierarchies where the helping would go to the breeder, or because of alliances where the helping would go to other subordinates). I go into further details below, but in short, the model forces a narrow scope for the kin selection scenario, and then the paper concludes that kin selection alone is unlikely to be of relevance for the evolution of vertebrate division of labor. This conclusion is particular to the model used, and it is misleading to suggest that this is a general feature of such a particular model.

      The scope of this paper was to study division of labor in cooperatively breeding species with fertile workers (i.e., primarily vertebrates), in which help is exclusively directed towards breeders to enhance offspring production (i.e., alloparental care). Our focus is in line with previous work in most other social animals, including eusocial insects and humans, which emphasizes how division of labor maximizes group productivity. Other forms of “general” help are not considered in the paper, and such forms of help are rarely considered in cooperatively breeding vertebrates or in the division of labor literature, as they do not result in task partitioning to enhance productivity.

      Overall, I think the paper should be revised extensively to clarify its aims, model, results, and scope of its conclusions.

      Recommendations for the authors: 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I reserved this section for more minor comments, relating to clarity and a general admonition to give us more detail and exploration of some basic population genetic quantities.

      Another minor point, although depending on whether I assume right or wrong, it could be major: I am not entirely sure that dispersers help in the groups they join as helpers, because of line 399, which states specifically that individuals who do remain in natal territories do. But I assume dispersers help (elsewhere, the authors state helping is not conditional on relatedness to the breeder). Otherwise, this model becomes even weirder for me. Either way, please clarify.

      Apologies if this was not clear. Immigrants that join a group (so dispersers from another group) as a subordinate help and queue for a breeding position, as does any natal subordinate born into the group. We rephased the sentence to “Subordinate group members, either natal or immigrants to the group, […]”  

      More generally, in simulation studies like this, there can be interactions between the strength of selection (which affects overall genetic variation maintained in the population), population size, and mutation rate/size, which can affect, for example, relatedness values. None of these quantities is explored here (and their interactions are not quantified), so it is not possible to evaluate the robustness of any of these results.

      Thank you for your comments about the parameter landscape. It is important to point out that variations in the mutation rate do not qualitatively affect our results, as this is something we explored in previous versions of the model (not shown). Briefly, we find that variations in the mutation rates only alter the time required to reach equilibrium. Increasing the step size of mutation diminishes the strength of selection by adding stochasticity and reducing the genetic correlation between offspring and their parents. Population size could, in theory, affect our results, as small populations are more prone to extinction. Since this was not something we planned to explore in the paper directly, we specifically chose a large population size, or better said, a large number of territories (i.e. 5000) that can potentially host a large population.  

      The authors also never say how it is actually determined. There is the evolved helping variable, and there is also the evolved reaction norm. I assume that the actual amount of help of each type is given by the product of T (equation 1) and H (for defense) and (1-T) and H (for work), but this should be stated explicitly.  

      Help provided is an interaction between H (total effort) and T (proportion of total effort invested in each type of task). To clarify the distinction between these two processes, we have now added “Hence, the gene α regulates the amount of help expressed, while the genes γ determine which specific helping tasks are performed at different time points in the breeding cycle”.  

      It is also weird that after introducing the T variable as a function of age, Figure 3 actually depicts it as a function of dominance value.

      Thank you for pointing out an error in Eq. 1. This inequality was indeed written incorrectly in the paper (but is correct in the model code); it is dominance rank instead of age (see code in Individual.cpp lines 99-119). We corrected this mistake throughout the manuscript.

      What is "scramble context"?

      “Scramble context” was an additional implementation that we decided to remove from the final manuscript, but we forgot to remove from Table 1 before submission. We have now removed it from the table.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Some specific comments:

      (1) L 31: "All theoretical..." These absolute statements are risky and unnecessary.

      Rephrased to “To date, most theoretical and empirical work…”

      (2) L 46: I believe Tom Wenseleers has published on the evolution of division of labor with reproductive workers and high within-colony conflict.

      Tom Wenseleers has indeed produced some models on the evolution of cooperation in social insects where some workers may reproduce. However, these models focus on the relevance of relatedness and policing selecting for a reduction in within-group conflict and the evolution of reproductive division of labor. Our model focuses instead on division of labor among workers (helpers). We have rephased this section to “task specialization is linked to sterility and where conflict of interest is generally low” to account for species of social insect in which variation in relatedness between group members and higher levels of reproductive conflict may arise. We also cited one of his papers.  

      (3) L 57: Again, unnecessary categorical statements.

      Rephrased to “Although a great deal of recent empirical work highlights the importance of direct benefits in the evolution of cooperative breeding behavior in vertebrates [21–24], we lack understanding on the joint influence of direct and indirect fitness benefits in the evolution of division of labor.”

      (4) L 67: This is said to be a key distinction, but in the paper, such a key role is not clearly shown. This and other tangential points are unnecessary to keep the introduction to the point.

      The different fitness costs of different tasks is the basis of our model on division of labor. Therefore, this is a key distinction and basis from which to describe different tasks in the model. We have left this sentence unchanged.

      (5) L 61-73: "In vertebrates, however, helpers may obtain fitness benefits directly via reproduction..." Some social insects may do so as well. It seems unnecessary and incorrect to say that vertebrate sociality is fundamentally different from invertebrate one. I think it is sufficiently interesting to say this work aims to understand vertebrate division of labor, by explicitly modeling aspects of its life history, without saying this can't happen in invertebrates or that no other model has ever done anything like it.

      Our point is not that, in some social insects, workers cannot obtain direct fitness benefits, but that previous models where the focus is on the colony reproductive outcome are only a good approximation to eusocial insect with sterile workers. However, to make this clearer we have added “In vertebrates and social insect with fertile workers, however, helpers may obtain fitness benefits directly via […]”.  

      (6) L 74-86: By this point, the introduction reads like a series of disconnected comments without a clear point.

      In L60 we added: “Understanding how direct and indirect benefits interact is particularly important in systems where individuals may differentially bear the fitness costs of cooperation”. By adding this sentence, we emphasize our focus on the largely unexplored direct fitness benefits and costs, as well as their interaction with indirect fitness. We then proceed to explain why it is crucial to consider that tasks have varying direct fitness costs and how the fitness benefits derived from cooperation change with age and resource-holding potential. These elements are essential for studying the division of labour in species with totipotent workers.

      (7) L 87: This sentence gives a clear aim. It would be clearer if the introduction focused on this aim.

      With the new sentence added in L60 (see previous comment), we bring the focus to the main question that we are trying to address in this paper earlier in the Introduction.  

      (8) L 88: "stochastic model" should be changed to "individual-based model".

      Done.

      (9) L 104: "limited number" is unclear. Say a fixed finite number, or something specific.

      Done.

      (10) L 105: "unspecified number" is unclear. Say the number of subordinates emerges from the population dynamics.

      Changed to “variable number of subordinate helpers, the number of which is shaped by population dynamics, with all group members capable of reproducing during their lifetime”.

      (11) L 112: "Dispersers" is used, but in the previous lines 107-109, the three categories introduced used different terms. Those three terms introduced should be used consistently throughout the paper, without using two or more terms for one thing.

      We use the term “disperser” to describe individuals that disperse from their natal group.

      Dispersers can assume one of three roles: (1) they can join another group as "subordinates"; (2) they can join another group as "breeders" if they successfully outcompete others; or (3) they can remain as "floaters" if they fail to join a group. "Floaters" are individuals who persist in a transient state without access to a breeding territory, waiting for opportunities to join a group in an established territory. We rephased the sentence to “Dispersers cannot reproduce without acquiring a territory (denoted here as floaters)”. This was also clarified in other instances where the term “dispersers” was used (e.g. L407). Other instances where this might not have been so clear, we replace “dispersers” with “floaters”.  

      (12) L 112: "(floaters)" Unclear parenthesis.

      See previous comment.  

      (13) L 115: There should be a reference to Methods around here.

      Added a reference to Figure 1.

      (14) L 117: To be clearer, say instead that dominance value is a linearly increasing function of age as a proxy of RHP and a linearly decreasing function of help provided due to the costs of working tasks. And refer to equation 2.

      Rephrased to “We use the term dominance value to designate the competitiveness of an individual compared to other candidates in becoming a breeder, regardless of group membership, that increases as a function of age, serving as a proxy for resource holding potential (RHP), and decreases as a function of help provided, reflecting costs to body condition from performing working tasks (Eq. 2).” We did not include “linearly” to keep it simpler, since it is clear from Eq. 2, which is now referenced here.  

      (15) L 119: "Subordinate helpers". As all subordinates are helpers, the helper qualifier is confusing.

      Subordinates are not necessarily helpers, as they can evolve help values of 0, hence, why we make it explicit here.

      (16) L 119: "choose". This terminology may be misleading. The way things are implemented in the model is that individuals are assigned a task depending on their genetic traits gamma. Perhaps it would be better to use a less intentional term, like perform one of two tasks.

      We changed “choose between two” to “engage in one of two”, which has less connotations of intentionality.

      (17) L 124: "Subordinates can [...] exhibit task specialization that [...] varies with their dominance value". It should be that it varies with age.

      Apologies. The equation was wrong; it does vary with dominance value. We corrected it accordingly.

      (18) L 133: "maximised" This is apparently important for the modelling procedure, but it is completely unclear what it means. Equation 4 comes out of nowhere, and it is said that such an equation is the maximum amount of help that can affect fecundity. Why? What does this mean? If there is something that is maximised, this should be proven. This value is then used for something (line 507), but it is unclear why or what it is used for (it says "we use the value of Hmax instead" without saying what for, no justification for the listed inequalities are given, and the claimed maximisation of an unspecified variable at those H values is not proven). Moreover, the notation in this section is also unclear: what are the sums over? Also, Hdefence and Hwork should vary over the index that is summed over, but the notation suggests that those quantities don't vary.

      We changed “maximized” to “greatest”, and we added a clarification to the rationality behind the maximization of the impact of help in the breeder’s productivity: “For example, in many cooperatively breeding birds, the primary reasons that breeders fail to produce offspring are (1) starvation, which is mitigated by the feeding of offspring, here considered as a work task, and (2) nest depredation, which is countered by defensive behavior. Consequently, both types of tasks are often necessary for successful reproduction, and focusing solely on one while neglecting the other is likely to result in lower reproductive success than if both tasks are performed by helpers within the group.”

      We now also clarify that the sums are for help given within a group (L 507), and added indexes to the equations.

      (19) L 152: "habitat saturation" How is this implemented? How is density dependence implemented? Or can the population size keep increasing indefinitely? It would be good to plot the population size over time, the group size over time, and the variance in group size over time. This could substantiate later statements about enhancing group productivity and could all be shown in the SI.

      Habitat saturation emerges from population dynamics due to the limited availability of territories and the fluctuating number of individuals, leading highly productive environments to experience habitat saturation. Although the number of group members is not restricted in our model, the population could theoretically increase indefinitely. However, this is not observed in the results presented here, as we selected parameter landscapes that stabilize population numbers. We confined our parameters to those where the population neither increased indefinitely (nor collapsed), as we did not incorporate density-dependent mortality traits for simplification. Consequently, the group size in the SI, where the standard deviation is already included, closely represents group size at any other given time during equilibrium.

      L 336: we changed “environments with habitat saturation” to “environments that lead to habitat saturation”, to increase clarity.

      (20) L 152: "lifecycle". Rather than the lifecycle, the figure describes the cycle of events in a single time step. The lifecycle (birth to death) goes over multiple time steps (as individuals live over multiple steps). So this figure shouldn't be called a life cycle.

      We changed “lifecycle” to “breeding cycle”.

      (21) L 156: "generation". This is not a generation but a time step.

      We changed “generation” to “breeding cycle”.

      (22) L 157: "previous life cycle" would mean that the productivity of a breeder depends on the number of helpers that its parents had, which is not what is meant.

      We changed “lifecycle” to “breeding cycle”.

      (23) L 158: "Maximum productivity is achieved when different helping tasks are performed to a similar extent." Again, unclear why that is the case.

      We added a clarification on this, see response to comment 18.  

      (24) L 160: "Dispersers/floaters". Use just one term for a single thing.

      See response to comment 11.   

      (25) L 162: "dispersal costs". I don't recall these being described in Methods.

      Individuals that disperse do not enjoy the protection of living in a territory and within a group of other individuals, so they have a higher mortality risk, described in Eq. 3.3. (negative values in the exponential part of the equation increase survival). The cost of dispersal is the same as individuals that remain as floaters at a given time step.

      (26) L 164: "generation" -> time step.

      We changed this to “breeding cycle”.  

      (27) L 170: "Our results show that division of labor initially emerges because of direct fitness benefits..." This is a general statement, but the results are only particular to the model. So this statement and others in the manuscript should be particular to the model. Also, Figure 2 doesn't say anything about what evolves "initially" as it only plots evolutionary equilibria.

      We rephrased this statement to “Our results suggest that voluntary division of labor involving tasks with different fitness costs is more likely to emerge initially because of direct fitness benefits”, to more accurately represent the conditions under which we modeled the division of labor.  

      Our reference to “initially” is regarding group formation (family groups versus aggregations of unrelated individuals or a mix). This is shown in the comparison between the different graphs at equilibrium. The initial state of the simulation is that all individuals disperse and do not cooperate.  

      (28) L 171: "but a combination of direct and indirect fitness benefits leads to higher rates and more stable forms of division of labor". What do you mean by "higher rates and more stable forms of division of labor"? Say how division of labor is shown in the figure (with intermediate T?).

      Yes, intermediate values of T show division of labor if γR ≠ 0. This is described under the section “The role of dominance in task specialization”. We added “with intermediate values suggesting a division of labor” to the Figure 2 legend.  

      (29) L173-175: "as depicted in Figure 2, intermediate values of task specialization indicate in all cases age/dominance-mediated task specialization (γt ≠ 0; Table 1) and never a lack of specialization (γt = 0; Table 1)". This sentence is unclear and imprecise. Does this sentence want to say that in Figure 2, all plots with intermediate values of T involve gamma t different from zero? If so, just say that.

      Rephrased to: “In Figure 2, all plots depicting intermediate values of T exhibit non-zero γR values and, hence, division of labor”.

      (30) L179-180: "forms of help that impact survival never evolve under any environmental condition when only kin selection occurs". This is misleading because under the KS scenario, help cannot positively impact survival in this model, so they never evolve.

      Help cannot affect survival but could potentially affect group persistence. If helpers increase breeder productivity and offspring remain philopatric and queue for the breeding position, then they will receive help from related individuals.   

      (31) L 210: "initially". What do you mean by that?

      Help only evolves in our model in family groups, which may then open the door for the evolution of help in mixed-kin groups. Therefore, we use “initially” to refer to the ancestral group structure that likely led to cooperation under benign environmental conditions. We rephased this section to “in more benign (and often highly productive) environments that lead to habitat saturation, help likely evolved initially in family groups, and defensive tasks are favored because competition for the breeding position is lower under kin selection.”

      (32) L 212: "kin selection is achieved". What does that mean?

      Rephased to “kin selection acts not only by selecting subordinates in their natal group to increase the productivity of a related breeder […]”

      (33) L 216: "division of labor seems to be more likely to evolve in increasingly harsh environments". Say in parentheses where this is shown.

      Added.  

      (34) L 218: "help evolves in benign environments". I don't see where this is shown. Figure 2 doesn't show that H is higher with lower m (e.g., in KS+GA column).

      Help does not evolve in benign environments under only direct fitness benefits derived from group augmentation (shown in Figure 2).  

      (35) L 225: "y-axis" should be "vertical axis", as y has another meaning in the model.

      Done.

      (36) L 226: "likelihood". Here and throughout, "likelihood" should be changed to probability. Likelihood means something else.

      Thank you for the advice, we have corrected this through the manuscript.  

      (37) L 236: "the slope of the reaction norm for the dominance value in task specialization".

      Unclear. Clearer to say: the rate at which individuals to shift from defense to work as they age.

      The important part is not so much the rate but the direction, that is, from work task to defense (or vice versa) as their rank increases. Changed to “the direction and rate of change in task specialization with dominance”.

      (38) L 257: "(task = 0; cost to dominance value)," This seems out of place.

      This aims to clarify that work tasks have a cost to dominance, while defense tasks have a cost to survival. This is particularly relevant in this model since different helping tasks are defined by their fitness costs.

      (39) L 258: "increase"-> "increase with age".

      Added “with dominance”.

      (40) L 262: "division of labor equilibria" What is that?

      Changed to “at equilibrium when division of labor evolves”

      (41) L 268: "Our findings suggest that direct benefits of group living play a driving role in the evolution of division of labor via task specialization in species with totipotent workers". This is a very general statement, but the results are much more circumscribed. First, the model is quite specific by assuming that, in the absence of group augmentation (xn=0), indirect fitness benefits can only be given to breeders (Equation 5) but not to other subordinates (Equations 2, 3.1). This is unrealistic, particularly for vertebrates, and reduces the possibility that indirect fitness benefits play a role.  

      As previously discussed, the scope of this paper was to study division of labor in cooperatively breeding species with fertile workers in which help is exclusively directed towards breeders to enhance offspring production through alloparental care. Other forms of “general” help do not result in task partitioning to enhance productivity.

      Second, the difference in costs of work and defense are what drive the evolution of "division of labor" (understood as intermediate T in case this is what the authors mean) in the KS scenario, but the functional forms of those two costs are quite specific and not of the same form, so these functions may bias the results found. Specifically, R is an unbounded linear function of work and the effect of this function becomes weaker as the individual ages due to the weakening force of selection with age (Equation 2) whereas Sh is a particular bounded nonlinear function of defense (Equation 3.1). These differences may tend to make the effect of Sh stronger due to the particular functions chosen.  

      The difference in costs is inherent to the nature of the different tasks (work versus defense): while survival is naturally bounded, with death as the lower bound, dominance costs are potentially unbounded, as they are influenced by dynamic social contexts and potential competitors. Therefore, we believe that the model’s cost structure is not too different from that in nature.  

      Third, no parameter sweep is given to see to what extent these results hold across the many parameters involved. So, in summary, the discussion should at least reflect that the results are of a restricted nature rather than giving the impression that they are of the suggested level of generality.

      During the exploratory phase of the model development, various parameters and values were assessed. However, the manuscript only details the ranges of values and parameters where changes in the behaviors of interest were observed, enhancing clarity and conciseness. For instance, variation in yh (the cost of help on dominance when performing “work tasks”) led to behavioral changes similar to those caused by changes in xh (the cost of help in survival when performing “defensive tasks”), as both are proportional to each other. Specifically, since an increase in defense costs raises the proportion of work relative to defense tasks, while an increase in the costs of work task has the opposite effect, only results for the variation of xh were included in the manuscript to avoid redundancy. Added to Table 1: “To maintain conciseness, further exploration of the parameter landscape was not included in the manuscript”.

      (42) L 270: "in eusocial insects often characterized by high relatedness and reproductive inhibition, sterile workers acquire fitness benefits only indirectly". This is misleading. Sterile workers of any taxa, be it insects or vertebrates, can only acquire fitness benefits indirectly as they are sterile, but eusocial insects involve not only sterile workers.

      Rephased to “In contrast, in eusocial species characterized by high relatedness and permanent worker sterility, such as most eusocial insects, workers acquire fitness benefits only indirectly”. In any case, permanent sterility only occurs in eusocial invertebrates; in vertebrates with reproductive inhibition sterility is only temporal and context dependent. Therefore, in vertebrates, sterile workers may potentially obtain direct fitness benefits if the social context changes, as is the case in naked mole-rats.  

      (43) L 273: "Group members in eusocial species are therefore predicted to maximize colony fitness due to the associated lower within-group conflict". Again, this is incorrect. Primitively eusocial insects have high conflict.

      We added “Group members in such eusocial species” to clarify that we are not referring here to primitively eusocial species but those with permanent sterile workers.  

      (44) L 277: "when the benefits of cooperation are evenly distributed among group members". In this model, the benefits of cooperation are not evenly distributed among group members: breeders reproduce, but subordinates don't.

      Subordinates may reproduce if they become breeders later in life. However, subordinates also benefit from cooperation as subordinates directly (greater survival in larger groups), and indirectly if they are related to the breeder. Here we refer to the first one, and we expand on that in the following sentence.  

      (45) L 280: "survival fitness benefits derived from living in larger groups seem to be key for the evolution of cooperative behavior in vertebrates [22, 63], and may also translate into low within-group conflict. This suggests that selection for division of labor in vertebrates is stronger in smaller groups". I don't see how the previous sentence suggests this. The paper does not present results to support this statement (i.e., no selection gradients in smaller vs larger groups are shown).

      The benefits of living in a larger group entail diminishing returns, so those living in smaller groups benefit greater by an increase in productivity and group size than those in a larger group.  

      (46) L 284: "Our model demonstrates that vertebrates evolve a more stable division of labor". Where is that shown? How is "more stable" measured?

      Rephrased to “vertebrates are more likely to evolve division of labor”. This is shown in Figure 2, that exemplifies that division of labor evolves in a wider range of environmental condition and to a higher degree (intermediate values of T).  

      (47) L 287: "direct fitness benefits in the form of group augmentation select more strongly for defensive tasks". Where is that shown? Establishing this would entail comparing selection gradients with direct fitness benefits of group augmentation and without them.

      In Figure 2, when we compare the GA column to KS+GA column, we see that at equilibrium, more helpers choose defense tasks, specially when they are free to choose their preferred task (circles).  

      (48) L 288: "kin selection alone seems to select only for work tasks." Again, this may be an artifact of the model assuming that helpers cannot increase non-breeders' fitness components except via group augmentation, and that defense tasks are inherently more costly than work tasks.

      As stated previously, we are studying task specialization in cooperative breeders where help is in the form of alloparental care (from allofeeding and egg care to defense from predators). We also assume that the costs are different, but whether one or the other is more costly depends on the relative context (e.g., a task can be more costly if it affects competitiveness in a very competitive environment). It is important to note that we name these tasks “work” and “defense” for practical reasons, but the focus of the paper is on tasks with different fitness costs that for their characteristics may not fit so well in under this terminology. While we acknowledge that most tasks have both kinds of fitness costs to a degree, here we focus on the main fitness costs of each kind of task (L430-436).  

      (49) L 290: "are comparatively large". This sounds as if the tasks are large, which is presumably not what is meant.

      Rephrased to “costs to dominance value and to the probability of attaining a breeding position are comparatively larger than survival costs.”

      (50) L 298: "helpers are predicted to increase defensive tasks with age or rank, whereas in harsh environments, work tasks are predicted to increase with age or rank." Add parentheses referring to where this is shown.

      This is shown in Figure 3, but since this is described in the discussion, we did not add a reference to the figure. If the editor would like us to refer to figures here, we can (see also comments below relating to the same issue).

      (51) L 308: "the role of age and environmental harshness on the evolution of division of labor". What is the prediction? Simply, the role of age is an assumption, not a prediction.

      Rephrased to “the role of environmental harshness on the evolution of division of labor via age-dependent task specialization”.

      (52) L 315: "individuals shifting from work tasks such as foraging for food, digging, and maintaining the burrow system, to defensive tasks such as guarding and patrolling as individuals grow older and larger". Say in parentheses where this is predicted.

      This prediction comes from Figure 3, we do not reference it here since we are in the Discussion section.  

      (53) L 320: "Under these conditions, our model predicts the highest levels of task partitioning and division of labor." Where is this predicted? Add parentheses referring to where this is shown. As it is, it is not possible to check the validity of the statement.

      This prediction comes from Figure 2 column KS+GA, we do not reference it here since we are in the Discussion section. The results with references to the figures are found under the Results section. In the discussion, we reiterate the results already described and add some examples from real data that seem to confirm our predictions.  

      (54) L 322: "In line with our model predictions, larger and older helpers of this species invest relatively more in territory maintenance, whereas younger/smaller helpers defend the breeding shelter of the dominant pair to a greater extent against experimentally exposed egg predators". These predictions are neat, but are now very difficult to understand from the figures. Maybe at the bottom of 3A, you could add a diagram work->defense for negative gamma_t and defense>work for positive gamma_t (or whatever order it is).

      Done.

      (55) L 325: "Territory maintenance has been shown to greatly affect routine metabolic rates and, hence, growth rates [80], which directly translates into a decrease in the likelihood of becoming dominant and attaining breeding status, as predicted by our model." This seems to be an assumption, not a prediction.

      That is true. We removed: “as predicted by our model”.  

      (56) L 352: "controlled". This means something else.

      Changed to “addressed”.

      (57) L 356: "summary, our study represents the first theoretical model aimed at elucidating the potential mechanisms underlying division of labor between temporal non-reproductives via task specialization in taxa beyond eusocial organisms". Again, claiming to be the first is risky and unnecessary.

      Rephrased to “our study helps to elucidate”.

      (58) L 358: "Harsh environments, where individuals can obtain direct fitness benefits from group living, favor division of labor, thereby enhancing group productivity and, consequently, group size." I'm not sure about this conclusion as harsh environments (large m in Figure 2) also involve the evolution of no division of labor (from the triangles and circles that are zero in the right bottom panel) and perhaps more so than with less harsh environments (intermediate m). Incidentally, in the bottom right panel of Figure 2, do the two separate clusters of triangles and circles mean that there is some sort of evolutionary branching?

      Yes, there are two different equilibria for the same set of conditions. Although it is true that for m=0.3 less division of labor evolves when kin selection and group augmentation act together, it is not the case when only group augmentation takes place. In addition, we qualify m=0.2 as harsh as opposed to benign in which we observe the rise of habitat saturation (m=0.1). m=0.3 is then an extreme harsh environment, in which in several instances different parameter landscape causes population collapse (see figures in the Supplemental Material).  

      (59) L 360: "Variation in the relative fitness costs of different helping tasks with age favors temporal polyethism". I don't see that this has been shown. Temporal polyethism evolves here whenever gamma_t evolves non-zero values. Figure 3A shows that non-zero gamma_t evolves with harsher environments, but I don't see what the "variation in relative fitness costs of different helping tasks" refers to.

      The evolved reaction norms of the model are towards different fitness costs depending on the task performed, since this is how we define the different types of tasks in the model.  

      (60) L 382: "undefined". Say variable. Undefined is something else.

      Undefined is more accurate, since we did not define how many subordinates there were per group, while “variable” could have been defined within a range, which was not the case in this model.  

      (61) L 390: "each genetic locus". Say earlier that each genetic trait is controlled by a single locus.

      Added.  

      (62) L 395: "complete" and "consistent" -> "certain".

      We changed one to “certain” and another to “absolute” to avoid using the same adjective twice in a sentence.  

      (63) L 396: What determines whether dispersers become subordinates or floaters? A trait? Or a fixed probability?

      We added “which is also controlled by the same genetic dispersal predisposition as for subordinates”.

      (64) L 412-413: "cycle". This should be a breeding step.

      Changed to “season” instead.

      (65) L 418: Say negatively impacts (it could also be positively impacts, which I guess is not what you mean).

      Done.

      (66) L 425: "a sample of floaters". Chosen how?

      Added “randomly drawn”.

      (67) L 426-428. But the equation in Table 1 indicates that all floaters compete for breeding spots, not a sample of floaters. This is not clear.

      The number of floaters sampled to try to breed at a given group is N<sub>f,b</sub> = 𝑓∗𝑁<sub>𝑓</sub>/𝑁<sub>𝑏</sub> (Table 1).

      Therefore, N<sub>f,b</sub> is the sample size of floaters for a given open breeding position, and f is how many groups on average a floater attempts to access in each time step.  

      (68) L 432. In the figure, the breeding cycle is called a step, but here it is called a cycle. There should be a single term used throughout. Breeding is not really a cycle here (it doesn't involve multiple steps that are repeated cyclically), so it seems more appropriate to call this breeding steps or breeding seasons.

      Taken into account previous comments, we changed the terms “generation” and “life cycle” to “breeding cycle”. We added “or seasons”.  

      (69) L 439: "generations". What are generations here, as generations are overlapping? You probably mean time steps or something else.

      Changed to “breeding cycles”.

      (70) L 439: "equilibrium was reached". Presumably, equilibrium is reached only asymptotically, so some cutoff is implemented in practice. So maybe say explicitly what cutoff was implemented.

      As mentioned, we run the model for 200’000 time steps, and if equilibrium was not reached for the phenotypic values, then we run the model for longer, with 400’000 time steps being the maximum at which all simulation reached equilibrium. In some cases, genetic values did not reach equilibrium at ranges at which there was no impact on phenotypic values, so these were disregarded to assess whether equilibrium was reached.  

      (71) L 452: "Even though individuals are likely to change the total amount of help given throughout their lives". Do you mean in real organisms or in the model? Say which. If it is in the model, it is not clear how.

      We added “in nature” to clarify that this was not the case in the model.  

      (72) L 455: "For more details on how individuals may adapt their level of help with age and social and environmental conditions, see [63]." Do you mean real individuals or in the model? Again, if it is in the model, it is unclear how this is possible and should be explained in this paper at least briefly rather than citing another one.

      We rephrased it to “How individuals in the model may adapt their level of help with age and social and environmental conditions has been described elsewhere.” We do not go into detail here because it is not within the scope of the paper, and those results have been described elsewhere.  

      (73) L 475: "helpers". Make terminology consistent throughout.

      All helpers are subordinates, but not all subordinates are helpers, as they may evolve no help. Since here we are describing those subordinates that do help, we use that terminology. We added “subordinate helpers” to clarify this further.  

      (74) L 476: "proportional". The dependence in Equation 1 is not "proportional to". Say something like "a survival probability (not rate) that decreases with the amount of help provided".

      Done.

      (75) L 482: "environmental"-> baseline, as defined first.

      Done.

      (76) L 486: "benefits". Can you briefly say in parentheses what those benefits are in real organisms? As in line 475, where you reminded the reader of survival costs due to predator defense.

      Added “such as those offered by safety in numbers or increased resource defense potential”.

      (77) L 494. "we first outline a basic model in which individuals". It is not clear what this sentence says, and the remainder of this section does not clarify it.

      We made two models for comparison, one where individuals can choose freely which task they prefer to perform, and another in which there is an increase in productivity when both kinds of tasks are performed to a similar extent at group level. In the latter model, individuals may choose an unpreferred task at certain times during their lived to increase the effect of the help provided in the breeder’s (and group’s) productivity.  

      We rephrased this section to “we first outline a basic model where individuals evolve their preferred helping task. Then we compare this to another model in which the breeder’s reproductive outcome is maximized when the group’s helping effort in each kind of tasks is performed to a roughly equal degree.”

      (78) L 496: "by performing both tasks". Sounds as if the breeder performs both tasks, not helpers.

      We changed to “when the group’s helping effort in each kind of tasks”.

      (79) L 497: "the maximum amount of cumulative help of each type (sigma Hmax) that can affect fecundity is given by Eq. 4:" This statement is imprecise. Presumably, what is meant is that this level of help maximises breeder productivity, as stated earlier in the paper. However, there is no proof that this level of help maximises breeder productivity, so this expression seems unjustified and it is unclear how it is used.

      This is a description of the model set up. As described later in the same section, the cumulative help of each time that will influence the breeder’s fecundity if maximum Hmax. Therefore, it does represent the maximum amount of cumulative help of each type that can affect the breeder’s fecundity.

      (80) L 500: "reproduced" -> "reproduce".

      Done.  

      (81) L 503. Say here what K is so that the reader knows what equation 5 is showing.

      Added “K” to the “The quantity of offspring produced (K)”.

      (82) L 503: "diminishing returns" -> "diminishing returns as help increases".

      Done.  

      (83) L 507: Why these inequalities?

      These inequalities explain the use of Hmax (response to comment 79). We rephased it to “the cumulative defense effort is larger than or the cumulative work effort is larger than ”.  

      (84) L 526: "removing the influence of relatedness from the model". It would be helpful to plot relatedness in this and the other scenario to check that it is indeed low here and high in the other.

      The actual values of relatedness are provided in the Supplemental Material Table S1. We added this reference to Figure 2.  

      (85) L 528: "It is possible that direct and indirect fitness benefits could have an additive effect on the evolution of alloparental care". This is technically incorrect. It is also unclear what the point of this sentence is.

      We have removed this sentence.  

      (86) Table 1: Say what are the allowed values for these genotypic traits (can they take negative values, be greater than one, are they continuous or discrete?): e.g., alpha \in [0,1] or alpha \in (-infinity, infinity). For phenotypic traits, it would be helpful if the third column lists the equation where the trait is defined. As the variables in the first column are scalars, they should not be bold face. Survival "rate" should be survival "probability" throughout.

      All genetic traits can take any real number (-infinity, infinity), but the phenotypic values are either constrained by the equation like for logistic formulas, or manually constrained like for dispersal propensity or help (only positive numbers allowed). We added “Each genetic trait is controlled by a single locus, and may take any real number” (L403), and added the boundaries for help and dominance value in Table 1. We decided against including the equations in the table due to space constraints. We removed the bold face as suggested. We changed all instances of “survival rate” to “survival probability”.

      (87) Figures S1, S2: I don't recall seeing references to these figures in the main text, but there should be, as well as for Tables S1-S3.

      Table S1 is now referenced in Figure 2. The other figures are now referenced in the main text when we reference the different sections in the Supplemental Materials (L190 and L198). Other Tables are referenced in their respective Figures in the SI.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Jiang et al. present a measure of phenological lag by quantifying the effects of abiotic constraints on the differences between observed and expected phenological changes, using a combination of previously published phenology change data for 980 species, and associated climate data for study sites. They found that, across all samples, observed phenological responses to climate warming were smaller than expected responses for both leafing and flowering spring events. They also show that data from experimental studies included in their analysis exhibited increased phenological lag compared to observational studies, possibly as a result of reduced sensitivity to climatic changes. Furthermore, the authors present compelling evidence that spatial trends in phenological responses to warming may differ from what would be expected from phenological sensitivity, due to the seasonal timing of when warming occurs. Thus, climate change may not result in geographic convergences of phenological responses. This study presents an interesting way to separate the individual effects of climate change and other abiotic changes on the phenological responses across sites and species.

      Greater phenological lag with experimental studies results in reduced sensitivity to climatic changes, not other way around.

      Strengths:

      A clearly defined and straightforward mathematical definition of phenological lag allows for this method to be applied in different scientific contexts. Where data exists, other researchers can partition the effects of various abiotic forcings on phenological responses that differ from those expected from warming sensitivity alone.

      Sensitivity does not tell the magnitude of phenological changes, nor does it provide indications of mechanisms responsible for changes in spring phenology. Because of uneven warming, the same average temperature change (annual or spring temperatures) can have greater (greater warming prior to budburst) or smaller (smaller warming prior to budburst) phenological change than that with even warming. When average temperature change is close to zero, uneven warming can lead to infinite sensitivity values, either advanced (warmer temperatures prior to budburst) or delayed (cooler temperatures prior to budburst) spring phenology.

      It is not clear why sensitivity is so popularly used in phenological research.

      Identifying phenological lag and associated contributing factors provides a method by which more nuanced predictions of phenological responses to climate change can be made. Thus, this study could improve ecological forecasting models.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors include very few data visualizations, and instead report results and model statistics in tables. This is difficult to interpret and may obscure underlying patterns in the data. Including visual representations of variable distributions and between-variable relationships, in addition to model statistics, provides stronger evidence than model statistics alone.

      The use of stepwise, automated regression may be less suitable than a hypothesis-driven approach to model selection, combined with expanded data visualization. The use of stepwise regression may produce inappropriate models based on factors of the sample data that may preclude or require different variable selection.

      We used two statistical methods, variance analysis to examine differential phenological responses (Figure 2) and regression analysis to determine the relative importance of forcing change, budburst temperature, and physiological lag, the drivers of changes in spring phenology (Table 2). Our objective was to understand why plants show differential responses by research approach, species origin, climatic region, and growth form identified in previous research. Variable selection may affect minor (altitude, latitude, MAT, and average spring temperature change) or insignificant (photoperiod and long-term precipitation) variables, but not those related to drivers of spring phenology. We are not sure how hypothesis-driven approach can help with our objective.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a meta-analysis of the relative contributions of spring forcing temperature, winter chilling, photoperiod and environmental variables in explaining plant flowering and leafing phenology. The authors develop a new summary variable called phenology lag to describe why species might have different responses than predicted by spring temperature.

      Strengths:

      The summary statistic is used to make a variety of comparisons, such as between observational studies and experimental studies.

      Weaknesses:

      By combining winter chilling effects, photoperiod effects, and environmental stresses that might affect phenology, the authors create a new variable that is hard to interpret. The authors do not provide information in the abstract about new insights that this variable provides.

      Phenological lag contains effects of all constraints that may include chilling effects, photoperiod effects, and environmental stresses and is, indeed, hard to interpret without investigation of individual constraints. In our synthesis, spring phenology (or photoperiod effect) is not significant across all studies complied. It is also unlikely that lack of winter chilling causes the systemic differences in phenological lag between observational and experimental studies or between native and exotic species (see discussion at lines 335-339). At individual study level, the contribution of different constraints to the overall lag effect can be specifically determined if moisture stresses, species chilling and photoperiod effects, or cold hardiness are known from on-site monitoring or previous research.

      The meaning of phenological lag is described at lines 34-38 in the abstract.

      Comments:

      It would be useful to have a map showing the sites of the studies.

      A map showing the sites of the studies was added as supplementary Figure S1.

      The authors should provide a section in which the strengths and weaknesses of the approach are discussed. Is it possible that mixing different types of data, studies, sample sizes, number of years, experimental set-ups, and growth habits results in artifacts that influence the results?

      Both strengths and weaknesses are discussed at various places throughout the paper. The weakness of our method, as indicated by the reviewer, is the inclusion of different constraints in the phenological lag and has been described at lines 34-38 in the abstract and lines 80-86 in the introduction of the concept. We have also expanded Conclusion section to discuss possible caveats at lines 369-393.

      As in all data analyses, the results can change with addition of more/different data, especially when sample size is relatively small. Ideally, comparisons are made among levels of fixed effects while controlling variations of other conditions. In phenological studies, however, climatic, phenological, and biological conditions all vary. For example, observational and experimental studies differ not only in the nature of warming (natural climate change vs artificial warming), but also in levels of warming (greater warming with experimental studies) and climatic, phenological, and biological conditions (Table 1). All phenological syntheses (or meta-analyses) have to make do with this uncontrolled nature of phenological data.

      Now that the authors have created this new variable, phenological lag, which of the components that contribute to it has the most influence on it? Or which components are most influential in which circumstances? For example, what are some examples where photoperiod causes a phenological lag?

      Any of the phenological constraints identified can contribute alone or in combination with others to the overall effect of phenological lag. Across all studies with this synthesis, the lack of significance with spring phenology rules out photoperiod effect, while the association of longer phenological lags with longer accumulation of winter chilling does not suggest general chilling shortage with the current extent of climate change.

      Although spring phenology is not significant across all studies, photoperiod effect can be influential at individual studies where changes in spring phenology are large. However, reported photoperiod effects in the literature are mostly confounding effects with temperatures, i.e., longer photoperiods are associated with longer hours of high daytime temperatures (see Chu et al., 2021). Other than European beech under an unlikely scenario of climate change (growth resumes at beginning of winter), there has been not clear evidence showing the effect of photoperiod in constraining spring phenology.

      Another confounding effect with photoperiod is extra heating effect with artificial light sources in warming experiments. Some early studies have shown that leaf temperature can be several degrees above the ambient air, due to long-wave radiation with artificial light sources. It is hard to believe the constraining effect of photoperiod on spring phenology if phenological changes are within inter-annual variations (can be a few weeks), although photoperiod effect has been increasingly discussed recently.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewing Editor:

      A key methodological concern is the inconsistent definition of growth temperature across observations. It is calculated over the interval between the baseline phenological date and the expected date under warming - a window that varies by species, site, and treatment. This variability limits comparability across observations and may introduce circularity, as growth temperature is derived from the same modelled expectation (i.e., the expected phenological advance) that it is later used to explain.

      The term “growth temperature” has been replaced with “budburst temperature” to indicate temperatures at species events. Budburst temperature is the average temperature within the window of expected response with the warmer climate and, as indicated by the editor, varies by species, sites, and treatments. This species-specific temperature provides an opportunity to compare among species, sites, and treatments and helps explain differences in observed responses, as demonstrated in the discussion of results in this synthesis.

      Forcing change, budburst temperature, and expected response are related. High budburst temperatures are associated with smaller expected responses, which helps explain smaller observed responses with late season species and areas of warm climates that have been often attributed to chilling or photoperiod effect.

      Additionally, the use of degree days above 0 {degree sign}C as a universal metric for spring forcing oversimplifies species' temperature responses. This approach assumes not only a fixed base temperature but also a linear response to temperature accumulation, which overlooks well-established nonlinear or species-specific thermal response curves. To improve the robustness and interpretability of the phenological lag framework, we encourage the authors to consider these limitations and explore ways to test or justify these modelling assumptions more explicitly.

      The use of 0 degree base temperature may not be the best choice for some species. Except for some early work, there has been few experimental research on physiological aspects of chilling and forcing processes. A popular alternative is modelling using assumed temperature response models. As variables influencing chilling and forcing processes are not controlled, the determined base temperatures and temperature response models may be OK with the species studied under particular conditions but would be inappropriate for applications beyond. It is hard to believe that species, in a study, all have different base temperature for accumulation of spring forcing and optimum temperature for winter chilling. Apparently, this is the result of model fitting, not actual dynamics of chilling and forcing processes.

      Two base temperatures are commonly used, 0 and 5 oC, although choice is not generally justified. It is known for long time that temperatures above 0oC contribute to spring forcing. My personal experience at tree nursery suggests that seedlings will flush after winter cold storage, even at forcing temperatures ≤ 5 oC in the dark. The use of 5 oC is rather the choice of tradition (5 oC is commonly used to define growing season) than scientific justification. The use of high base temperatures may not make much difference at high temperatures due to short forcing duration but will underestimate forcing at low temperatures due to long forcing duration and large proportions of forcing between 0 and base temperatures. We are not aware of any experimental studies that demonstrate non-zero base temperatures.

      Within the dominant range of spring temperatures (e.g., between 5 and 25 oC), the forcing responses to temperatures can be approximated with linear models. Again, we are not aware of any non-linear forcing models that can be safely applied beyond the species studied under particular conditions.

      Regardless, the uses of different base temperatures or forcing models would not affect the partitioning of phenological changes, simply because temperature response models reflect physiological aspects of chilling and forcing processes and would not change with climate warming.

      The authors introduce a new metric, phenological lag, to assess how phenological constraints influence spring phenology, offering new insights into phenological research. However, there are several concerns. First, the research question and the study's aim are not clearly presented. The authors primarily analyzed phenological lag and simply compared it across different groups, but additional analyses are needed to adequately address the research question. In addition, the broader importance of this study is not clearly explained - why this research is necessary and what it contributes to the field should be explicitly stated.

      The research question is outlined at lines 92-108. We added “Our objective was to determine how phenological responses differ among different groups and how differential responses are related to drivers of spring phenology, i.e., forcing change, budburst temperature, and phenological lag” at lines 106-108.

      (1) Abstract: The methodological improvements and more key results should be included.

      Growth temperature has been replaced with “budburst temperature” to indicate temperatures at time of budburst. More results are added at lines 40-48.

      (2) Line 32: Terms such as "sensitivity analysis" and "phenological lag" need clearer definitions.

      We added at lines 32-33 to define sensitivity analysis “that is based on rates of phenological changes, not on drivers of spring phenology”. Phenological lag is defined at lines 34-38.

      (3) Lines 38-47: Further results and the urgency or importance of the study should be conveyed.

      More results are added at lines 40-48. The importance of this study is described at lines 48-50.

      (4) Line 57-58: This sentence is unclear - please clarify.

      The sentence is modified to “difficult using sensitivity analysis that is based on rates of phenological changes, not on drivers of spring phenology".

      (5) Line 60: break "endodormancy".

      Breaking dormancy would mean endodormancy.

      (6) Line 67: What does "growth temperature" refer to?

      Growth temperature has been replaced with “budburst temperature” to indicate temperatures at time of budburst. It is calculated as the average temperature within the window of expected response with the warmer climate.

      (7) Lines 87-94: The specific purpose of the study is vague. Why is this method needed, and how will it serve future research?

      We have modified the paragraph at lines 92-108 to provide justification and objective of the study.

      (8) Lines 163-164: The rationale for exploring differences in observed responses and phenological lag needs to be better justified.

      We added explanations at lines 179-182 why observed responses and phenological lag were chosen in the analysis.

      (9) Lines 178-183: Tables and figures should be properly cited within the text.

      Table S3 was added at line 197.

      (10) Lines 195-198: Clarify whether variables were scaled before model analysis.

      We clarified at line 192 “variables were not standardized prior to regression analysis”.

      (11) Line 206-207: The observed response is presented as the number of advanced days, while temperature sensitivity refers to the response of spring phenology to temperature - these are different variables and should not be conflated.

      The two variables are related but show different aspects of phenological changes. Observed response divided by average temperature change gives temperature sensitivity. Observed response is the total changes in number of days observed, while temperature sensitivity is the change in number of days per unit change in average temperature (oC). Sensitivity may reflects rates of phenological change with temperature (see responses to reviewer 1).

      (12) In the discussion section, the authors compared phenological responses among different groups separately. This section requires substantial improvement to more clearly answer the research question.

      These discussions are related to our objective “how phenological responses differ among different groups identified in previous research (i.e., research approach, species origin, climatic region, and growth form) and how these differential responses are related to drivers of spring phenology, i.e., forcing change, budburst temperature, and phenological lag”.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This work provides a new Python toolkit for combining generative modeling of neural dynamics and inversion methods to infer likely model parameters that explain empirical neuroimaging data. The authors provided tests to show the toolkit's broad applicability and accuracy; hence, it will be very useful for people interested in using computational approaches to better understand the brain.

      Strengths:

      The work's primary strength is the tool's integrative nature, which seamlessly combines forward modelling with backward inference. This is important as available tools in the literature can only do one and not the other, which limits their accessibility to neuroscientists with limited computational expertise. Another strength of the paper is the demonstration of how the tool can be applied to a broad range of computational models popularly used in the field to interrogate diverse neuroimaging data, ensuring that the methodology is not optimal to only one model. Moreover, through extensive in-silico testing, the work provided evidence that the tool can accurately infer ground-truth parameters, which is important to ensure results from future hypothesis testing are meaningful.

      We are happy to hear the positive feedback on our effort to provide an open-source and widely accessible tool for both fast forward simulations and flexible model inversion, applicable across popular models of large-scale brain dynamics.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the tool itself is the main strength of the work, the paper lacked a thorough analysis of issues concerning robustness and benchmarking relative to existing tools.

      The first issue is the robustness to the choice of features to be included in the objective function. This choice significantly affects the training and changes the results, as the authors even acknowledged themselves multiple times (e.g., Page 17 last sentence of first paragraph or Page 19 first sentence of second paragraph). This brings the question of whether the accurate results found in the various demonstrations are due to the biased selection of features (possibly from priors on what worked in previous works). The robustness of the neural estimator and the inference method to noise was also not demonstrated. This is important as most neuroimaging measurements are inherently noisy to various degrees.

      The second issue is on benchmarking. Because the tool developed is, in principle, only a combination of existing tools specific to modeling or Bayesian inference, the work failed to provide a more compelling demonstration of its added value. This could have been demonstrated through appropriate benchmarking relative to existing methodologies, specifically in terms of accuracy and computational efficiency.

      We fully agree with the reviewer that the VBI estimation heavily depends on the choice of data features, and this is the core of the inference procedure, not its weakness. We have demonstrated different scenarios showing how the informativeness of features (commonly used in the literature) results in varying uncertainty quantification. For instance, using summary statistics of functional connectivity (FC) and functional connectivity dynamics (FCD) matrices to estimate global coupling parameter leads to fast convergence; however, it is not sufficient to accurately estimate the whole-brain heterogeneous excitability parameter, which requires features such as statistical moments of time series. VBI provides a taxonomy of data features that users can employ to test their hypotheses. It is important to note that one major advantage of VBI is its ability to make estimation using a battery of data features, rather than relying on a limited set (such as only FC or FCD) as is often the case in the literature. In the revised version, we will elaborate further by presenting additional scenarios to demonstrate the robustness of the estimation. We will also evaluate the robustness of the neural density estimators to (dynamical/additive) noise.

      More importantly, relative to benchmarking, we would like to draw attention to a key point regarding existing tools and methods. The literature often uses optimization for fitting whole-brain network models, and its limitations for reliable causal hypothesis testing have been pointed out in the Introduction/Discussion. As also noted by the reviewer under strengths, and to the best of our knowledge, there are no existing tools other than VBI that can scale and generalize to operate across whole-brain models for Bayesian model inversion. Previously, we developed Hamiltonian Monte Carlo (HMC) sampling for Epileptor model in epilepsy (Hashemi et al., 2020, Jha et al., 2022). This phenomenological model is very well-behaved in terms of numerical integration, gradient calculation, and dynamical system properties (Jirsa et al., 2014). However, this does not directly generalize to other models, particularly the Montbrió model for resting-state, which exhibits bistability with noise driving transitions between states. As shown in Baldy et al., 2024, even at the level of a single neural mass model (i.e., one brain region), gradient-based HMC failed to capture such switching behaviour, particularly when only one state variable (membrane potential) was observed while the other (firing rate) was missing. Our attempts to use other methods (e.g., the second-derivative-based Laplace approximation used in Dynamic Causal Modeling) also failed, due to divergence in gradient calculation. Nevertheless, reparameterization techniques (Baldy et al., 2024) and hybrid algorithms (Gabrié et al., 2022) could offer improvements, although this remains an open problem for these classes of computational models.

      In sum, for oscillatory systems, it has been shown previously that SBI approach used in VBI substantially outperforms both gradient-based and gradient-free alternative methods (Gonçalves et al., 2020, Hashemi et al., 2023, Baldy et al., 2024). Importantly, for bistable systems with switching dynamics, gradient-based methods fail to converge, while gradient-free methods do not scale to the whole-brain level (Hashemi et al., 2020). Hence, the generalizability of VBI relies on the fact that neither the model nor the data features need to be differentiable. We will clarify this point in the revised version. Moreover, we will provide better explanations for some terms mentioned by the reviewer in Recommendations.

      Hashemi, M., Vattikonda, A. N., Sip, V., Guye, M., Bartolomei, F., Woodman, M. M., & Jirsa, V. K. (2020). The Bayesian Virtual Epileptic Patient: A probabilistic framework designed to infer the spatial map of epileptogenicity in a personalized large-scale brain model of epilepsy spread. NeuroImage, 217, 116839.

      Jha, J., Hashemi, M., Vattikonda, A. N., Wang, H., & Jirsa, V. (2022). Fully Bayesian estimation of virtual brain parameters with self-tuning Hamiltonian Monte Carlo. Machine Learning: Science and Technology, 3(3), 035016.

      Jirsa, V. K., Stacey, W. C., Quilichini, P. P., Ivanov, A. I., & Bernard, C. (2014). On the nature of seizure dynamics. Brain, 137(8), 2210-2230.

      Baldy, N., Breyton, M., Woodman, M. M., Jirsa, V. K., & Hashemi, M. (2024). Inference on the macroscopic dynamics of spiking neurons. Neural Computation, 36(10), 2030-2072.

      Baldy, N., Woodman, M., Jirsa, V., & Hashemi, M. (2024). Dynamic Causal Modeling in Probabilistic Programming Languages. bioRxiv, 2024-11.

      Gabrié, M., Rotskoff, G. M., & Vanden-Eijnden, E. (2022). Adaptive Monte Carlo augmented with normalizing flows. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(10), e2109420119.

      Gonçalves, P. J., Lueckmann, J. M., Deistler, M., Nonnenmacher, M., Öcal, K., Bassetto, G., ... & Macke, J. H. (2020). Training deep neural density estimators to identify mechanistic models of neural dynamics. elife, 9, e56261.

      Hashemi, M., Vattikonda, A. N., Jha, J., Sip, V., Woodman, M. M., Bartolomei, F., & Jirsa, V. K. (2023). Amortized Bayesian inference on generative dynamical network models of epilepsy using deep neural density estimators. Neural Networks, 163, 178-194.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Whole-brain network modeling is a common type of dynamical systems-based method to create individualized models of brain activity incorporating subject-specific structural connectome inferred from diffusion imaging data. This type of model has often been used to infer biophysical parameters of the individual brain that cannot be directly measured using neuroimaging but may be relevant to specific cognitive functions or diseases. Here, Ziaeemehr et al introduce a new toolkit, named "Virtual Brain Inference" (VBI), offering a new computational approach for estimating these parameters using Bayesian inference powered by artificial neural networks. The basic idea is to use simulated data, given known parameters, to train artificial neural networks to solve the inverse problem, namely, to infer the posterior distribution over the parameter space given data-derived features. The authors have demonstrated the utility of the toolkit using simulated data from several commonly used whole-brain network models in case studies.

      Strengths:

      (1) Model inversion is an important problem in whole-brain network modeling. The toolkit presents a significant methodological step up from common practices, with the potential to broadly impact how the community infers model parameters.

      (2) Notably, the method allows the estimation of the posterior distribution of parameters instead of a point estimation, which provides information about the uncertainty of the estimation, which is generally lacking in existing methods.

      (3) The case studies were able to demonstrate the detection of degeneracy in the parameters, which is important. Degeneracy is quite common in this type of model. If not handled mindfully, they may lead to spurious or stable parameter estimation. Thus, the toolkit can potentially be used to improve feature selection or to simply indicate the uncertainty.

      (4) In principle, the posterior distribution can be directly computed given new data without doing any additional simulation, which could improve the efficiency of parameter inference on the artificial neural network if well-trained.

      We thank the reviewer for the careful consideration of important aspects of the VBI tool, such as uncertainty quantification, degeneracy detection, parallelization, and amortization strategy.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) While the posterior estimator was trained with a large quantity of simulated data, the testing/validation is only demonstrated with a single case study (one point in parameter space) per model. This is not sufficient to demonstrate the method's accuracy and reliability, but only its feasibility. Demonstrating the accuracy and reliability of the posterior estimation in large test sets would inspire more confidence.

      (2) The authors have only demonstrated validation of the method using simulated data, but not features derived from actual EEG/MEG or fMRI data. So, it is unclear if the posterior estimator, when applied to real data, would produce results as sensible as using simulated data. Human data can often look quite different from the simulated data, which may be considered out of distribution. Thus, the authors should consider using simulated test data with out-of-distribution parameters to validate the method and using real human data to demonstrate, e.g., the reliability of the method across sessions.

      (3) The z-scores used to measure prediction error are generally between 1-3, which seems quite large to me. It would give readers a better sense of the utility of the method if comparisons to simpler methods, such as k-nearest neighbor methods, are provided in terms of accuracy.

      (4) A lot of simulations are required to train the posterior estimator, which seems much more than existing approaches. Inferring from Figure S1, at the required order of magnitudes of the number of simulations, the simulation time could range from days to years, depending on the hardware. Although once the estimator is well-trained, the parameter inverse given new data will be very fast, it is not clear to me how often such use cases would be encountered. Because the estimator is trained based on an individual connectome, it can only be used to do parameter inversion for the same subject. Typically, we only have one session of resting state data from each participant, while longitudinal resting state data where we can assume the structural connectome remains constant, is rare. Thus, the cost-efficiency and practical utility of training such a posterior estimator remains unclear.

      We agree with the reviewer that it is necessary to show results on larger synthetic test sets, and we will elaborate further by presenting additional scenarios to demonstrate the robustness of the estimation. However, there are some points raised by the reviewer that we need to clarify.

      The validation on empirical data was beyond the scope of this study, as it relates to model validation rather than the inversion algorithms. This is also because we aimed to avoid repetition, given that we have previously demonstrated model validation on empirical data using theses techniques, for invasive sEEG (Hashemi et al., 2023), MEG (Sorrentino et al., 2024), EEG (Angiolelli et al., 2025) and fMRI (Lavanga et al., 2024, Rabuffo et al., 2025). Note that if the features of the observed data are not included during training, VBI ignores them, as it requires an invertible mapping function between parameters and data features.

      We have used z-scores and posterior shrinkage to measure prediction performance, as these are Bayesian metrics that take into account the variance of both prior and posterior rather than only the mean value or thresholding for ranking of the prediction used in k-NN or confusion matrix methods. This helps avoid biased accuracy estimation, for instance, if the mean posterior is close to the true value but there is no posterior shrinkage. Although shrinkage is bounded between 0 and 1, we agree that z-scores have no upper bound for such diagnostics.

      Finally, the number of required simulations depends on the dimensionality of the parameter space and the informativeness of the data features. For instance, estimating a single global scaling parameter requires around 100 simulations, whereas estimating whole-brain heterogeneous parameters requires substantially more simulations. Nevertheless, we have provided fast simulations, and one key advantage of VBI is that simulations can be run in parallel (unlike MCMC sampling, which is more limited in this regard). Hence, with commonly accessible CPUs/GPUs, the fast simulations and parallelization capabilities of the VBI tool allow us to run on the order of 1 million simulations within 2–3 days on desktops, or in less than half a day on supercomputers at cohort level, rather than over several years! It has been previously shown that the SBI method used in VBI provides an order-of-magnitude faster inversion than HMC for whole-brain epilepsy spread (Hashemi et al., 2023). Moreover, after training, the amortized strategy is critical for enabling hypothesis testing within seconds to minutes. We agree that longitudinal resting-state data under the assumption of a constant structural connectome is rare; however, this strategy is essential in brain diseases such as epilepsy, where experimental hypothesis testing is prohibitive.

      We will clarify these points and better explain some terms mentioned by the reviewer in the revised manuscript.

      Hashemi, M., Vattikonda, A. N., Jha, J., Sip, V., Woodman, M. M., Bartolomei, F., & Jirsa, V. K. (2023). Amortized Bayesian inference on generative dynamical network models of epilepsy using deep neural density estimators. Neural Networks, 163, 178-194.

      Sorrentino, P., Pathak, A., Ziaeemehr, A., Lopez, E. T., Cipriano, L., Romano, A., ... & Hashemi, M. (2024). The virtual multiple sclerosis patient. IScience, 27(7).

      Angiolelli, M., Depannemaecker, D., Agouram, H., Regis, J., Carron, R., Woodman, M., ... & Sorrentino, P. (2025). The virtual parkinsonian patient. npj Systems Biology and Applications, 11(1), 40.

      Lavanga, M., Stumme, J., Yalcinkaya, B. H., Fousek, J., Jockwitz, C., Sheheitli, H., ... & Jirsa, V. (2023). The virtual aging brain: Causal inference supports interhemispheric dedifferentiation in healthy aging. NeuroImage, 283, 120403.

      Rabuffo, G., Lokossou, H. A., Li, Z., Ziaee-Mehr, A., Hashemi, M., Quilichini, P. P., ... & Bernard, C. (2025). Mapping global brain reconfigurations following local targeted manipulations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 122(16), e2405706122.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      We appreciate the time and effort of the reviewers, and their insightful and constructive comments to improve the paper. We have now addressed the reviewers’ comments in our revised manuscript and provide here below detailed explanations of the changes.

      We have adapted the Wilson-Cowan model to follow the same brain network modeling notation as the other models (Fig. 3 in the main text and Figs. S2–S4 in the supplementary materials). Additionally, we have included multiple figures in the supplementary material presenting extensive in-silico testing to demonstrate the accuracy and reliability of the estimations across different configurations, as well as the sensitivity to both additive and dynamical noise.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) There were some inaccurate statements throughout the text that need to be corrected.

      a) In section 2.1, paragraph 1, the authors mentioned that they would describe network models corresponding to different types of neuroimaging recordings. This is inaccurate. The models were developed to approximate various aspects of the architecture of neural circuits. They were not developed per se to solely describe a specific neuroimaging modality.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We agree that our phrasing in Section 2.1, paragraph 1, was not clear that the network models were developed to generate neural activity at the source level, and that a projection needs to be established to transform the simulated neural activity into empirically measurable quantities, such as BOLD fMRI, EEG, or MEG. We have revised the wording in the revised manuscript to clarify this point accordingly.

      b) The use of the term "spatio-temporal data features" is misleading as there are no true spatial features extracted.

      We have clarified that:Following Hashemi et al., 2024, we use the term spatio-temporal data features to refer to both statistical and temporal features derived from time series. In contrast, we refer to the connectivity features extracted from FC/FCD matrices as functional data features. We would like to retain this term, as it is used consistently in the code.

      (2) The authors need to improve the model descriptions in Equations (1)-(10). Several variables/parameters were not explained, limiting the accessibility of the work to those without prior experience in computational modeling.

      Thank you for pointing this out. In the revised manuscript, we have improved the model descriptions, all variables and parameters used in these equations.

      (3) Various things need further clarification and/or explanation:

      a) There is a need to highlight that the models section only provides examples of one of the many possible variants of the models. For example, the Wilson-Cowan model described is not your typical and more popular cortico-cortical-based Wilson-Cowan model. This is important to ensure that the work reflects an accurate account of the literature, avoiding future references that the models presented are THE models.

      This is a very important point. We have now highlighted that each model represents one of many possible variants. Moreover, we adapted the Wilson-Cowan model as a whole-brain network modeling approach to harmonize with all other models.

      b) In Figure 1, it is unclear where the empirical data come into play. The neural density estimator also sounds like a black box and needs further explanation (e.g., its architecture).

      Thank you for the careful reading. This is correct. We have now clarified where the empirical data enters as input to the neural density estimator and have added further explanation in section 2.2.

      c) There is also a need to better explain what shrinkage means and what the z-score vs shrinkage implies.

      We have elaborated on the definition of posterior z-score and shrinkage.

      d) It is unclear how the authors decided on the number of training samples to use.

      There is no specific rule for determining the optimal number of simulations required for training. In general, the larger number of simulations, within the available computational budget, the better the posterior estimation is likely to be. In the case of synthetic data, we have monitored the z-score and posterior shrinkage to assess the quality and reliability of the inferred parameters.  This also critically depends on the parameter dimensionality. For instance, in estimating only global coupling parameter, a maximum of 300 simulations was used, demonstrating accurate estimation across models and different realizations (Fig S20), except for the Jansen-Rit model, where coupling did not induce a significant change in the intrinsic frequency of regional activity. We have now pointed this out in the discussion.

      e) In the Results section, paragraph 1, there is a need to clarify that "ground truth" is available because you simulate data using predefined parameters. In fact, these predefined parameters and how they were chosen to generate the observed data were never described in the text.

      The "ground truth" is often chosen randomly within biologically plausible ranges, typically with some level of heterogeneity, and this has now been highlighted.

      f) Can the authors comment on why the median of the posterior distributions (e.g., in Figure 4E) is actually far off from the ground truth parameters? This is probably understandable in the Jansen-Ritt model due to complexity, but not obvious in the very low-dimensional Stuart-Landau oscillator model.

      This can happen due to non-identifiability in high-dimensional settings. Figure 4E represents the posterior estimation using Jansen-Rit model with high-dimensional parameters. An accurate estimation close to the true values can be observed in the low-dimensional Stuart-Landau model, as shown in Figure 5.

      g) In Figure 7, the FC and FCD matrices look weird relative to those typically seen in other works.

      We have updated Figure 7. To do the our best, we have followed the code and the parameters from the following paper Kong et al., Nat Commun 12, 6373 (2021), and the following repo https://github.com/ThomasYeoLab/CBIG/blob/master/stable_projects/fMRI_dynamics/Kong2021_pMFM/examples/scripts/CBIG_pMFM_parameter_estimation_example.py

      We considered 300 iterations for optimizing the parameters, using CMA-ES method, and with window length of 60 sec, and TR=0.72 sec, yielding a 1118 × 1118 FCD matrix for each run. Nevertheless, some discrepancy can happen with the shown FC/FCD, due to convergence of the optimization process and other model parameters.

      h) In Figure 8, results for the J parameter are missing. Also, the BOLD signal time series of some regions in Figure 8B looks very weird, with some having very large deflections.

      We have updated Figure 8. In this figure, the parameter J is not inferred; it is instead presented in the appendix (S18). Please note that the system is in a bistable regime. We have implemented the full Wong-Wang model (Deco, 2014, Journal of Neuroscience), by optimized external current and global coupling (using CMA-ES optimization) to maximize the fluidity of FCD, as those typically seen in other works:

      Author response image 1.

      i) On page 14, the authors mentioned that they perform a PCA on the FC/FCD matrices. Can the authors explain this step further and what it specifically gives out, as this is something unusual in the generative model fitting literature?

      Indeed, PCA is a widely used dimension reduction method in machine learning. Please note that in SBI, any dimensionality reduction technique, such as PCA, can be used, as long as it preserves information relevant to the target parameters.

      j) On page 3, what does ABC in ABC methods stand for?

      ABC stands for Approximate Bayesian Computation, which is now spelled out in the text.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Overall, I found the paper well-written. These are basically just minor comments:

      We appreciate your positive feedback.

      (1) P3:

      - Amortization requires more explanation for the neuroscience audience.

      - What does ABC stand for?

      We have elaborated on Amortization. ABC stands for Approximate Bayesian Computation, which is now spelled out in the text.

      (2) Section 2.1:

      Should clarify the parcellation used

      In section 2.1, we now mentioned that: “The structural connectome was built with TVB-specific reconstruction pipeline using generally available neuroimaging software (Schirner et al., Neuroimage 2015)”.

      (3) P20: The method for sensitivity analysis (Figure 5F) is not clearly described.

      We have now added a subsection in the Methods section to explain the sensitivity analysis.

      (4) P21: statement that 10k simulations took less than 1 min doesn't match info shown in Figure S1. Please clarify.

      This is correct, as for the Epileptor model, the total integration time is less than 100 ms. Due to the model’s stable behavior with a large time step and the use of 10 CPU cores, all simulations were completed in less than a minute. Previously (Hashemi et al., 2023) it has been reported that each VEP run to simulate 100sec of whole-brain epileptic patterns takes only 0.003 s using a JIT compiler. The other models require more computational cost due to longer integration durations and smaller time steps. We have clarified this point.

      (5) P23-24: the distribution of FCDs also doesn't match well even if we don't consider element-wise correspondence. Please clarify.

      This is correct, as we used summary statistics of the FCD, such as fluidity, and due to noise, each realization of the FCD matrix exhibits different element-wise correspondence. We have already mentioned this point.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Chen et al. used cryo-ET and in vitro reconstituted system to demonstrate that the autoinhibited form of LRRK2 can also assemble into filaments that wrap around the microtubule, although the filaments are typically shorter and less regular compared to the previously reported active-LRRK2 filaments. The structure revealed a new interface involving the N-terminal repeats that were disordered in the previous active-LRRK2 filament structure. The autoinhibited-LRRK2 filament also has different helical parameters compared to the active form.

      Strengths:

      The structure obtained in this study is the highest resolution of LRRK2 filaments done by subtomogram averaging, representing a major technical advance compared to the previous Cell paper from the same group. Overall, I think the data are well presented with beautiful graphic rendering, and valuable insights can be gained from this structural study.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) There are only three main figures, together with 9 supplemental figures. The authors may consider breaking the currently overwhelming Figures 1 and 3 into smaller figures and moving some of the supplemental figures to the main figure, e.g., Figure S7.

      (2) The key analysis of this manuscript is to compare the current structure with the previous active-LRRK2 filament structure. Currently, such a comparison is buried in Figure 3H. It should be part of Figure 1.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. As suggested, we have rearranged the figures, split Figure 1 and 3 into smaller Figures, and moved the comparison analysis in Figure 3H to the new Figure 1. Specifically, the old Figure 1 is separated into two figures, introducing the model-building process and describing the two symmetric axes. The old Figure 3 is also separated into two small figures, describing the geometric analysis and model comparison, respectively.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The authors of this paper have done much pioneering work to decipher and understand LRRK2 structure and function, to uncover the mechanism by which LRRK2 binds to microtubules, and to study the roles that this may play in biology. Their previous data demonstrated that LRRK2 in the active conformation (pathogenic mutation or Type I inhibitor complex) bound to microtubule filaments in an ordered helical arrangement. This they showed induced a "roadblock" in the microtubule impacting vesicular trafficking. The authors have postulated that this is a potentially serious flaw with Type 1 inhibitors and that companies should consider generating Type 2 inhibitors in which the LRRK2 is trapped in the inactive conformation. Indeed the authors have published much data that LRRK2 complexed to Type 2 inhibitors does not seem to associate with microtubules and cause roadblocks in parallel experiments to those undertaken with type 1 inhibitors published above.

      In the current study, the authors have undertaken an in vitro reconstitution of microtubule-bound filaments of LRRK2 in the inactive conformation, which surprisingly revealed that inactive LRRK2 can also interact with microtubules in its auto-inhibited state. The authors' data shows that while the same interphases are seen with both the active LRRK2 and inactive microtubule bound forms of LRRK2, they identified a new interphase that involves the WD40-ARM-ANK- domains that reportedly contributes to the ability of the inactive form of LRRK2 to bind to microtubule filaments. The structures of the inactive LRRK2 complexed to microtubules are of medium resolution and do not allow visualisation of side chains.

      This study is extremely well-written and the figures are incredibly clear and well-presented. The finding that LRRK2 in the inactive autoinhibited form can be associated with microtubules is an important observation that merits further investigation. This new observation makes an important contribution to the literature and builds upon the pioneering research that this team of researchers has contributed to the LRRK2 fields. However, in my opinion, there is still significant work that could be considered to further investigate this question and understand the physiological significance of this observation.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments and we agree that more work can be done next to understand the physiological significance of the autoinhibited LRRK2 in cellular environments. We are actively working on understanding how the stability of autoinhibited full-length LRRK2 is regulated, especially how the transfer between autoinhibited and active forms of LRRK2 can happen. Our in situ data (Watabane et al. 2020) indicates that overexpressed hyperactive PD-mutant LRRK2 mainly adopts its active-like conformation in cells. Thus, learning how the state transfer occurs will allow us to target autoinhibited LRRK2 specifically and efficiently in cells and study its structure and function in physiological conditions.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Chen et al examines the structure of the inactive LRRK2 bound to microtubules using cryo-EM tomography. Mutations in this protein have been shown to be linked to Parkinson's Disease. It is already shown that the active-like conformation of LRRK2 binds to the MT lattice, but this investigation shows that full-length LRRk2 can oligomerize on MTs in its autoinhibited state with different helical parameters than were observed with the active-like state. The structural studies suggest that the autoinhibited state is less stable on MTs.

      Strengths:

      The protein of interest is very important biomedically and a novel conformational binding to microtubules in the proposed.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The structures are all low resolution.

      We thank the reviewer for the comments on both the strengths and weaknesses of the manuscript. We agree with the reviewer that higher resolution would provide more information about how LRRK2 interacts with microtubules and oligomerizes in its autoinhibited form. However, with the current resolution, our model-building benefited significantly from the published high-resolution models and the alpha-fold predictions. We used cryo-ET and subtomogram analysis to solve the structure because this filament is less regular than the right-handed active LRRK2 filament, preventing us from using conventional single-particle analysis. As highlighted by reviewer 1, being able to push the resolution to sub-nanometer is an important advance reflecting state-of-the-art subtomogram analysis, especially for a heterogeneous sample.  Notably, the microtubule reconstruction reached higher resolution, comparable to our previous single-particle studies on LRRK2-RCKW (Snead and Matyszewski et al.), confirming the data quality.

      (2) There are no measurements of the affinity of the various LRRK2 molecules (with and without inhibitors) to microtubules. This should be addressed through biochemical sedimentation assay.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion and we agree that learning the binding affinity between LRRK2 and microtubules would be informative. We attempted to purify the LRRK2 with mutants on the WD40:ARM/ANK interface we identified in the manuscript.. Unfortunately, either LRRK2 or LRRK2<sup>I2020T</sup> with N-terminal mutants (R521A/F573A/E854K), the yield and purity of the final samples are significantly worse than our routine LRRK2 prep. Our chromatography and gel electrophoresis results indicate that proteins are degrading during purification.

      Author response image 1.

      While we have attached the results here, and it would be interesting to investigate why N-terminal mutations destabilize LRRK2, we anticipate that significant efforts would be required for further experiments, which we respectfully consider outside of the scope of this manuscript. 

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) In Figure S9, the graphic definition of "chain length" in panel A is misleading. The authors can simply note in the figure legend that "chain length is the number of asymmetric units in a continuous chain".

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. The updated figure and legend have incorporated the changes.

      (2) In Figure S7B, the conformation changes of the 'G-loop' and the 'DYG' motifs are not so convincing at the current resolution.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing it out. We agree that our model resolution is not high enough to support the unbiased observation of the conformation changes of the key kinase motifs. In the revised manuscript, we avoided emphasizing the comparison between the two models. Instead, we state that for both the MLi-2 bound map and the GZD-824 bound map, the corresponding published high-resolution models fit into each kinase map, but the MLi-2 bound model doesn’t fit as well in the GZD-824 bound map, with a correlation value dropped from 0.44 to 0.4, supporting our statement that “full-length LRRK2 bound to microtubules is in its autoinhibited state in our reconstituted system”.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Are there any cellular experiments that could be done to demonstrate that inactive LRRK2 associates with microtubules in cells?

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this direction for future studies. We are studying the physiological significance of the autoinhibited LRRK2 in cells, but haven’t yet been successful at demonstrating physiological binding to microtubules. Further, as noted in our response to reviewer #3, we are also actively working on understanding how the stability of autoinhibited full-length LRRK2 is regulated, especially how the transfer between autoinhibited and active forms of LRRK2 can happen. Our in situ data (Watabane et al. 2020) indicates that hyperactive PD-mutant overexpressed LRRK2 mainly adopts its active-like conformation in cells. Thus, learning how the state transfer occurs will allow us to target autoinhibited LRRK2 specifically and efficiently in cells and study its structure and function in physiological conditions.

      (2) Previous work that the authors and others have undertaken has suggested that only LRRK2 in its active conformation can associate with microtubule filaments and the authors have shown that this leads to a roadblock in vesicular transport only when LRRK2 is complexed with Type 1 but not Type 2 inhibitors. There seems to be some discrepancy here that is not addressed in the paper as based on the current results one would also expect LRRK2 bound to Type 2 inhibitors to induce roadblocks in microtubule filaments. How can this be explained?

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important question. Taking all of our published data together, we believe that LRRK2 can introduce roadblocks with Type 1 inhibitor bound in the active-like conformation, where N-terminus LRRK2 domains are flexible and don’t block the kinase active site. In other words, full-length LRRK2 can form roadblocks when it behaves more like the truncated LRRK2<sup>RCKW</sup> variant. The autoinhibited LRRK2 forms shorter and less stable oligomers on microtubules, making it harder to block transport. Consistent with this, our in situ LRRK2-microtubule structure was observed in cells where LRRK2 is in an active-like conformation, and the LRRK2 N-terminus appeared to be flexible and away from the microtubule when forming right-handed filaments.

      (3) Does the finding that inactive LRRK2 only binds to microtubules as a short filament, explain the differences between the inactive and active forms of LRRK2 binding to microtubules and causing roadblocks?

      We thank the reviewer for discussing this point with us and asking the question. As we replied in the previous comment, the reviewer’s conclusion explains how the roadblock phenomenon occurs only under certain circumstances. We expanded our discussion to add the following and address the question:

      “Notably, we previously demonstrated that active‐like LRRK2, when bound to a Type I inhibitor, can form roadblocks that impair vesicular transport. Since autoinhibited LRRK2 assembles into shorter, less stable oligomers on microtubules, we anticipate it will exert reduced road‐blocking effects in cells, regardless of the inhibitor bound.”

      (4) Could the authors undertake further characterization of the new WD40-ARM-ANK interphase that they have identified? Is this important for the binding of the autoinhibited mutant? Could mutants be made in this interphase to see if this prevents the autoinhibited but not the active conformation of LRRK2 binding to microtubules?

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. As mentioned in our response to Reviewer #2, public comment #2, we attempted to purify the LRRK2 with mutants on the WD40:ARM/ANK interface we identified in the manuscript multiple times. Unfortunately, either LRRK2 or LRRK2<sup>I2020T</sup> with N-terminal mutants (R521A/F573A/E854K), the yield and purity of the final samples are significantly worse than our routine LRRK2 prep. Our chromatography and gel electrophoresis results indicate that proteins are degrading during purification.

      (5) The authors identify several disease-relevant missense mutations that appear to lie within the novel interphase that the authors have characterised in this study. Although this is discussed in the Discussion, some experimental data demonstrating how these missense mutations impact the ability of inactive LRRK2 to bind to microtubule filaments in the presence or absence of Type 1 and Type 2 compounds could provide further experimental data that emphasises the physiological importance of the results presented in this study.

      We thank the reviewer for discussing this interesting direction. The disease-relevant missense mutations can have a direct or indirect impact on the binding of autoinhibited LRRK2 to microtubules, and we agree that it would be interesting to test it out in the future. However, we anticipate that significant effort would be required for further experiments. Alas, our funding for this project ended suddenly and we want to report our results to the community.

      (6) For the data that is shown in Figure 1, could the authors explain how this differs from results in previous papers of the authors showing that the active form of LRRK2 binds microtubules? How does the binding observed here differ from that observed in the previous studies? To a non-specialist reader, the data looks fairly like what has previously been reported.

      We thank the reviewer for asking the question. As mentioned in the response to the public review, the detailed comparison between the data and the previous papers is described in Figure 3, and we agree that it is helpful to incorporate this information in Figure 1. In the revised manuscript, we have incorporated the comparison panel in Figure 1.

      (7) The finding that the autoinhibited LRRK2 forms short and sparse oligomers on microtubules raises the question of how physiological this observation is. Having some data that suggests that this is physiologically relevant would boost the impact of this study.

      We agree with the reviewer on this comment. As discussed in the response to the first comment from the reviewer, we have not been able to assess the physiological relevance of LRRK2 binding to microtubules in either active or inactive state, but continue to pursue this line of research. We are aware and regret that this lessens the impact of this work.

      (8) For the more general reader the authors could potentially better highlight why the key finding in this paper is important.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. To further address the significance of the key findings, especially how it can open up more possibilities for inhibitor-based drug development, we expand our discussion section to include the following:

      “Understanding how Type I and Type II inhibitors’ binding to LRRK2 affects its mechanism is vital to the design of inhibitor-based PD drug development strategies. Our findings revealed that different LRRK2 kinase inhibitors bind to autoinhibited LRRK2 similarly either in solution or on microtubules. Furthermore, the observation of autoinhibited LRRK2 forming short, less stable oligomers on microtubules opens new possibilities to inhibit LRRK2 activity in PD patients. A Type I inhibitor specifically targeting autoinhibited LRRK2 may alleviate the effect of LRRK2 roadblocks on microtubules. Alternatively, a promising strategy of LRRK2 inhibitor design can focus on the stabilization of allosteric N-terminus blocking on the kinase domain, which favors the formation of autoinhibited LRRK2 oligomers on microtubules and causes fewer side effects.”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In the third paragraph of the introduction, expand on whether type-1 inhibitors which "capture kinases in a closed, "active-like" conformation still inhibit the kinase activity.

      We thank the reviewer for the request to expand this paragraph. We added the following explanation for better understanding in the third paragraph:

      “Type-I inhibitors bind to the ATP binding site and target the kinase in its ‘active-like' conformation, inhibiting its kinase activity.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This study examined the effect of blood pressure variability on brain microvascular function and cognitive performance. By implementing a model of blood pressure variability using an intermittent infusion of AngII for 25 days, the authors examined different cardiovascular variables, cerebral blood flow, and cognitive function during midlife (12-15-month-old mice). Key findings from this study demonstrate that blood pressure variability impairs baroreceptor reflex and impairs myogenic tone in brain arterioles, particularly at higher blood pressure. They also provide evidence that blood pressure variability blunts functional hyperemia and impairs cognitive function and activity. Simultaneous monitoring of cardiovascular parameters, in vivo imaging recordings, and the combination of physiological and behavioral studies reflect rigor in addressing the hypothesis. The experiments are well-designed, and the data generated are clear. I list below a number of suggestions to enhance this important work:

      (1) Figure 1B: It is surprising that the BP circadian rhythm is not distinguishable in either group. Figure 2, however, shows differences in circadian rhythm at different timepoints during infusion. Could the authors explain the lack of circadian effect in the 24-h traces?

      The circadian rhythm pattern is apparent in Figure 2 (Active BP higher than Inactive BP), where BP is presented as 12hour averages. When the BP data is expressed as one-hour averages (rather than minute-to-minute) over 24hours, now included in the revised manuscript as Supplemental Figure 3C-D, the circadian rhythm becomes noticeable. In addition, we have included one-hour average BP data for all mice in the control and BPV groups, Supplemental Figure 3A-B.

      Notably, the Ang-II induced pulsatile BP pattern remains evident in the one-hour averages for the BPV group, Supplemental Figure 3B. To minimize bias and validate variability, pump administrations start times were randomized for both control and BPV groups, Supplemental Figure 3A-B. Despite these adjustments, the circadian rhythm profile of BP is consistently maintained across individual mice and in the collective dataset, Supplemental Figure 3C-D.

      (2) While saline infusion does not result in elevation of BP when compared to Ang II, there is an evident "and huge" BP variability in the saline group, at least 40mmHg within 1 hour. This is a significant physiological effect to take into consideration, and therefore it warrants discussion.

      Thank you for this comment. The large variations in BP in the raw traces during saline infusion reflects transient BP changes induced by movement/activity, which is now included in Figure 1B (maroon trace). The revised manuscript now includes Line 222 “Note that dynamic activity-driven BP changes were apparent during both saline- and Ang II infusions, Figure 1B”.

      (3) The decrease in DBP in the BPV group is very interesting. It is known that chronic Ang II increases cardiac hypertrophy, are there any changes to heart morphology, mass, and/or function during BPV? Can the decrease in DBP in BPV be attributed to preload dysfunction? This observation should be discussed.

      The lower DBP in the BPV group was already present at baseline, while both groups were still infused with saline, and was a difference beyond our control. However, this is an important and valid consideration, particularly considering the minimal yet significant increase in SBP within the BPV group (Figure 1D). Our goal was to induce significant transient blood pressure responses (BPV) and investigate the impact on cardiovascular and neurovascular outcomes in the absence of hypertension. We did not anticipate any major cardiac remodeling at this early time point (considering the absence of overt hypertension) and thus cardiac remodeling was not assessed and this is now discussed in the revised manuscript (Line 443-453).

      (4) Examining the baroreceptor reflex during the early and late phases of BPV is quite compelling. Figures 3D and 3E clearly delineate the differences between the two phases. For clarity, I would recommend plotting the data as is shown in panels D and E, rather than showing the mathematical ratio. Alternatively, plotting the correlation of ∆HR to ∆SBP and analyzing the slopes might be more digestible to the reader. The impairment in baroreceptor reflex in the BPV during high BP is clear, is there any indication whether this response might be due to loss of sympathetic or gain of parasympathetic response based on the model used?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion and have accordingly generated new figures displaying scatter plots of SBP vs HR with linear regression analysis (Figure 3D-G). Our goal is to further investigate which branch of the autonomic nervous system is affected in this model. The loss of a bradycardic response suggests either an enhancement of sympathetic activity, a reduction in parasympathetic activity, or a combination of both. This is briefly discussed in the revised manuscript (Line 486-496).

      Heart rate variability (HRV) serves as an index of neurocardiac function and dynamic, non-linear autonomic nervous system processes, as described in Shaffer and Ginsber[1]. However, given that our data was limited to BP and HR readings collected at one-minute intervals, our primary assessment of autonomic function is limited to the bradycardic response. Further studies will be necessary to fully characterize the autonomic parameters influenced by chronic BPV.

      (5) Figure 3B shows a drop in HR when the pump is ON irrespective of treatment (i.e., independent of BP changes). What is the underlying mechanism?

      We apologize for any lack of clarity. These observed heart rate (HR) changes occurred during Ang II infusion, when blood pressure (BP) was actively increasing. In the control group, the pump solution was switched to Ang II during specific periods (days 3-5 and 21-25 of the treatment protocol) to induce BP elevations and a baroreceptor response, allowing direct comparisons between the control and BPV group.

      To clarify this point, we have revised Line 260-263 of the manuscript: “To compare pressure-induced bradycardic responses between BPV and control mice at both early and later treatment stages, a cohort of control mice received Ang II infusion on days 3-5 (early phase) (Supplemental Figure 4) and days 21-25 (late phase) thereby transiently increasing BP”.

      Additionally, a detailed description has been added to the Methods section (Line 96-101): “Controls receiving Ang II: To facilitate between-group comparisons (control vs BPV), a separate cohort of control mice were subjected to the same pump infusion parameters as BPV mice but for a brief period receiving Ang II infusions on days 3-5 and 21-25 for experiments assessing pressure-evoked responses, including bradycardic reflex, myogenic response, and functional hyperemia at high BP.”

      (6) The correlation of ∆diameter vs MAP during low and high BP is compelling, and the shift in the cerebral autoregulation curve is also a good observation. I would strongly recommend that the authors include a schematic showing the working hypothesis that depicts the shift of the curve during BPV.

      Thank you for this insightful comment. The increase in vessel reactivity to BP elevations in parenchymal arterioles of BPV mice suggests that chronic BPV induces a leftward shift and a potential narrowing of the cerebral autoregulation range (lower BP thresholds for both the upper and lower limits of autoregulation). This has been incorporated (and discussed) into the revised manuscript (see Figure 5N).

      One potential explanation for these changes is that the absence of sustained hypertension, a prominent feature in most rodent models of hypertension, limits adaptive processes that protect the cerebral microcirculation from large BP fluctuations (e.g., vascular remodeling). While this study does not specifically address arteriole remodeling, the lack of such adaptation may reduce pressure buffering by upstream arterioles, thereby rendering the microcirculation more vulnerable to significant BP fluctuations.

      The unique model allows for measurements of parenchymal arteriole reactivity to acute dynamic changes in BP (both an increase and decrease in MAP). Our findings indicate that chronic BPV enhances the reactivity of parenchymal arterioles to BP changes—both during an increase in BP and upon its return to baseline, Supplemental Figure 5C, F. The data suggest an increased myogenic response to pressure elevation, indicative of heightened contractility, a common adaptive process observed in rodent models of hypertension[2-4]. However, our model also reveals a notable tendency for greater dilation when the BP drops, Supplemental Figure 5F. This intriguing observation may suggest ischemia during the vasoconstriction phase (at higher BP), leading to enhanced release of dilatory signals, which subsequently manifest as a greater dilation upon BP reduction. This phenomenon bears similarities to chronic hypoperfusion models[5,6], where vasodilatory mechanisms become more pronounced in response to sustained ischemic conditions. Future studies investigating the effects of BPV on myogenic responses and brain perfusion will be a priority for our ongoing research.

      (7) Functional hyperemia impairment in the BPV group is clear and well-described. Pairing this response with the kinetics of the recovery phase is an interesting observation. I suggest elaborating on why BPV group exerts lower responses and how this links to the rapid decline during recovery.

      Based on the heightened reactivity of BPV parenchymal arterioles to intravascular pressure (Figure 5), we anticipate that the reduction of sensory-evoked dilations results from an increased vasoconstrictive activity and/or a decreased availability of vasodilatory signaling pathways (NO, EETs, COX-derived prostaglandins)[7,8]. Consequently, the magnitude of the FH response is blunted during periods of elevated BP in BPV mice.

      Additionally, upon termination of the stimulus-induced response−when vasodilatory signals would typically dominate−vasoconstrictive mechanisms are rapidly engaged (or unmasked), leading to quicker return to baseline. This shift in the balance between vasodilatory and vasoconstrictive forces favors vasoconstriction, contributing to the altered recovery kinetics observed in BPV mice. This has been included in the Discussion section of the revised manuscript.

      (8) The experimental design for the cognitive/behavioral assessment is clear and it is a reasonable experiment based on previous results. However, the discussion associated with these results falls short. I recommend that the authors describe the rationale to assess recognition memory, short-term spatial memory, and mice activity, and explain why these outcomes are relevant in the BPV context. Are there other studies that support these findings? The authors discussed that no changes in alternation might be due to the age of the mice, which could already exhibit cognitive deficits. In this line of thought, what is the primary contributor to behavioral impairment? I think that this sentence weakens the conclusion on BPV impairing cognitive function and might even imply that age per se might be the factor that modulates the various physiological outcomes observed here. I recommend clarifying this section in the discussion.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. Clinical studies have demonstrated that patients with elevated BPV exhibit impairments across multiple cognitive domains, including declines in processing speed[9] and episodic memory[10]. To evaluate memory function, we utilized behavioral tests: the novel object recognition (NOR) task to assess episodic memory[11] and the spontaneous Y-maze to evaluate short-term spatial memory[12].

      Previous research indicates that older C57Bl6 mice (14-month-old) exhibit cognitive deficits compared to younger counterparts (4- and 9-month-old)[13]. To ensure rigorous selection for behavioral testing, we conducted preliminary NOR assessment, evaluating recognition memory at the one-hour delay but observing failures at the four-, and 24-hour delays, indicating age-related deficits. Based on these results, animals failing recognition criteria were excluded from subsequent behavioral assessment. However, because no baseline cognitive testing was conducted for the spontaneous Y-maze, it is possible that some mice with aged-related deficits were included in this test, which may have influenced data interpretation.

      Additionally, the absence of differences in the Y-maze performance may suggest that short-term spatial memory remains intact following 25 days of BPV, a point that is now discussed in the revised manuscript.

      (9) Why were only male mice used?

      We appreciate this comment and acknowledge the importance of conducting experiments in both male and female mice. Studies involving female mice are currently ongoing, with telemetry data collection approximately halfway completed and two-photon imaging studies on functional hyperemia also partially completed. However, using middleaged mice for these experiments has proven challenging due to high mortality rates following telemetry surgeries. As a result, we initially limited our first cohort to male mice.

      (10) In the results for Figure 3: "Ang II evoked significant increases in SBP in both control and BPV groups;...". Also, in the figure legend: "B. Five-minute average HR when the pump is OFF or ON (infusing Ang II) for control and BPV groups...." The authors should clarify this as the methods do not state a control group that receives Ang II.

      Please refer to response to comment 5.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Blood pressure variability has been identified as an important risk factor for dementia. However, there are no established animal models to study the molecular mechanisms of increased blood pressure variability. In this manuscript, the authors present a novel mouse model of elevated BPV produced by pulsatile infusions of high-dose angiotensin II (3.1ug/hour) in middle-aged male mice. Using elegant methodology, including direct blood pressure measurement by telemetry, programmable infusion pumps, in vivo two-photon microscopy, and neurobehavioral tests, the authors show that this BPV model resulted in a blunted bradycardic response and cognitive deficits, enhanced myogenic response in parenchymal arterioles, and a loss of the pressure-evoked increase in functional hyperemia to whisker stimulation.

      Strengths:

      As the presentation of the first model of increased blood pressure variability, this manuscript establishes a method for assessing molecular mechanisms. The state-of-the-art methodology and robust data analysis provide convincing evidence that increased blood pressure variability impacts brain health.

      Weaknesses:

      One major drawback is that there is no comparison with another pressor agent (such as phenylephrine); therefore, it is not possible to conclude whether the observed effects are a result of increased blood pressure variability or caused by direct actions of Ang II.

      We acknowledge this limitation and have attempted to address the concern by introducing an alternative vasopressor, norepinephrine (NE), Figure 4. A subcutaneous dose of 45 µg/kg/min was titrated to match Ang II-induced transient BP pulse (Systolic BP ~150-180 mmHg), Figure 4A. Similar to Ang II treated mice, NE-treated mice exhibited no significant changes in average mean arterial pressure (MAP) throughout the 20-day treatment period (Figure 4B). Although there was a trend (P=0.08) towards increased average real variability (ARV) (Figure 4C left), it did not reach statistical significance. The coefficient of variation (CV) (Figure 4C right) was significantly increased by day 3-4 of treatment (P=0.02).

      Notably, unlike the bradycardic response observed during Ang II-induced BP elevations, NE infusions elicited a tachycardic response (Figure 4A), likely due to β-1 adrenergic receptor activation. However, significant mortality was observed within the NE cohort: three of six mice died prematurely during the second week of treatment, and two additional mice required euthanasia on days 18 and 20 due to lethargy, impaired mobility, and tachypnea.

      While we recognize the importance of comparing results across vasopressors, further investigation using additional vasopressors would require a dedicated study, as each agent may induce distinct off-target effects, potentially generating unique animal models. Alternatively, a mechanical approach−such as implanting a tethered intra-aortic balloon[14] connected to a syringe pump−could be explored to modulate blood pressure variability without pharmacological intervention. However, such an approach falls beyond the scope of the present study.

      Ang II is known to have direct actions on cerebrovascular reactivity, neuronal function, and learning and memory. Given that Ang II is increased in only 15% of human hypertensive patients (and an even lower percentage of non-hypertensive), the clinical relevance is diminished. Nonetheless, this is an important study establishing the first mouse model of increased BPV.

      We agree that high Ang II levels are not a predominant cause of hypertension in humans, which is why it is critical that our pulsatile Ang II dosing did not cause overt hypertension, (no increase in 24-hour MAP). Ang II was solely a tool to produce controlled, transient increases in BP to yield a significant increase in BPV.

      Regarding BPV specifically, prior studies indicate that primary hypertensive patients with elevated urinary angiotensinogen-to-creatinine ratio exhibit significantly higher mean 24-hour systolic ARV compared to those with lower ratios[15]. However, the fundamental mechanisms driving these harmful increases in BPV remain poorly defined. A central theme across clinical BPV studies is impaired arterial stiffness, which has been proposed to contribute to BPV through reduced arterial compliance and diminished baroreflex sensitivity. Moreover, increased BPV can exert mechanical stress on arterial walls, leading to arterial remodeling and stiffness−ultimately perpetuating a detrimental feed-forward cycle[16].

      In our model, male BPV mice exhibited a minimal yet significant elevation in SBP without corresponding increases in DBP, potentially reflecting isolated systolic hypertension, which is strongly associated with arterial stiffness[17,18]. Our initial goal was to establish controlled rapid fluctuations in BP, and Ang II was selected as the pressor due to its potent vasoconstrictive properties and short half-life[19].

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment and acknowledge the necessity of exploring alternative mechanisms underlying BPV, and independent of Ang II. It is our long-term goal to investigate these factors in further studies.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) How was the dose of Ang II determined? It seems that this dose (3.1ug/hr) is quite high.

      The Ang II dose was titrated in a preliminary study to one that induced a significant and transient BP response without increasing 24-hour blood pressure (i.e. no hypertension).

      Ang II was delivered subcutaneously at 3.1 μg/hr, a concentration comparable to high-dose Ang II administration via mini-osmotic pumps (~1700 ng/kg/min)[20], with one-hour pulses occurring every 3-4 hours. With 6 pulses per day, the total daily dose equates to 18.6 µg/day in a ~30 gram mouse.

      For comparison, if the same 18.6 µg/day dose were administered continuously via a mini-osmotic pump (18.6 µg/0.03kg/1440min), the resulting dosage would be approximately 431 ng/kg/min[21,22], aligning with subpressor dose levels. Thus, while the total dose may appear high, it is not delivered in a constant manner but rather intermittently, allowing for controlled, rapid variations in blood pressure.

      (2) Were behavioral studies performed on the same mice that were individually housed? Individual housing causes significant stress in mice that can affect learning and memory tasks (PMC6709207). It's not a huge issue since the control mice would have been housed the same way, but it is something that could be mentioned in the discussion section.

      Behavioral studies were performed on mice that were individually housed following the telemetry surgery. The study was started once BP levels stabilized, as mice required several days to achieve hemodynamic stability post-surgery. Consequently, all mice were individually housed for several days before undergoing behavioral assessment.

      To account for potential cognitive variability, earlier novel object recognition (NOR) tests were conducted to established cognitive capacity, and mice that did not meet criteria were excluded from further behavioral testing. However, we acknowledge that individual housing induces stress, which can influence learning and memory, and this is a factor we were unable to fully control. Given that both experimental and control groups experienced the same housing conditions, this stress effect should be comparable across cohorts. A discussion on this limitation is now included in the text.

      (3) It looks like one control mouse that was included in both Figures 1 and 2 (control n=12) but was excluded in Table 1 (control n=11), this isn't mentioned in the text - please include the exclusion criteria in the manuscript.

      We apologize for the typo−12 control animals were consistently utilized across Figure 1-2, Table 1, Supplemental Table 1, Figure 6C, and Supplemental Figure 2B. Since the initial submission, one control mouse was completed and included into the telemetry control cohort. Thus, in the updated manuscript, we have corrected the control sample size to 13 mice across these figures ensuring consistency.

      Additionally, exclusion criteria have now been explicitly included in the manuscript (Line 173-175). Mice were excluded from the study if they died prematurely (died prior to treatment onset) or mice exhibited abnormally elevated pressure while receiving saline, likely due to complications from telemetry surgery.

      (4) Please include a statement on why female mice were not included in this study.

      As discussed in our response to Reviewer #1, our initial intention was to include both male and female mice in this study. However, high mortality rates following telemetry surgeries significantly constrained our ability to advance all aspects of the study. As a result, we limited our first cohort to males to establish the basics of the model. A statement is now included in the manuscript, Line 50-53: “Female mice were not included in the present study due to high post-surgery mortality observed in 12-14-month-old mice following complex procedures. To minimized confounding effects of differential survival and to establish foundational data for this model, we restricted the investigation to male mice.”

      Potential sex differences might be complex and warrants a separate future research to comprehensively assess sex as a biological variable, which are currently ongoing.

      (5) On page 14, "experiments from control vs experimental mice were not equally conducted in the same season raising the possibility for a seasonal effect" - does this mean that control experiments were not conducted at the same time as the Ang II infusions in BPV mice? This has huge implications on whether the effects observed are induced by treatment or just batch seasonal effects.

      We fully acknowledge the reviewer’s concern, and our statement aims to provide transparency regarding the study’s limitations. Several challenges contributed to this outcome, including high mortality rates following surgeries (primarily telemetry implantation) and technical issues related to instrumentation, particularly telemetry functionality.

      Differences between BPV and saline mice emerge primarily due to mortality or telemetry failures−some mice did not survive post-surgery, while others remain healthy but had non-functional telemeters. This issue was particularly pronounced in 14-month-old mice, as their fragile vasculature occasionally prevented proper BP readings.

      Each experiment required a minimum of two and a half months per mouse to complete, with a cost (also per mouse) exceeding $1500 USD ($300 pump, $175 mouse, $900 telemeters, per diem, drugs, reagents etc.). Despite our best effort to ensure comparable seasonal/batch data, these logistical and technical constraints prevented perfect synchronization.

      To evaluate whether seasonal differences influenced our results, we incorporated additional telemetry data into the control cohort. Of the seven included control mice, six underwent the same treatment but were allocated to a separate branch of the study, which endpoints did not require a chronic cranial window. We found no significant differences in 24-hour average MAP during the baseline period between control mice with or without a cranial window, Supplemental Figure 2A. Additionally, we grouped mice into seasonal categories based on Georgia’s climate: “Spring-Summer” (May-September) and “Fall-Winter” (October-April) but observed no BP differences between these periods, Supplemental Figure 2B.

      Given the absence of seasonal effects on BP and the fact that mice were sourced from two independent suppliers (Jackson Laboratory and NIA), we anticipate that the observed results are driven by treatment rather than seasonal or batch effects.

      (6) Methods, two-photon imaging: did the authors mean "retro-orbital" instead of "intra-orbital" injection of the Texas red dye? Also, is this a Texas red-dextran? If so, what molecular weight?

      Thank you for this comment. The correct terminology is “retro-orbital” rather than “intra-orbital” injection. Additionally, we utilized Texas Red-dextran (70 kDa, 5% [wt/vol] in saline) for the imaging experiments. These details have now been incorporated into the Methods section.

      (1) Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. An Overview of Heart Rate Variability Metrics and Norms. Front Public Health. 2017;5:258. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00258

      (2) Pires PW, Jackson WF, Dorrance AM. Regulation of myogenic tone and structure of parenchymal arterioles by hypertension and the mineralocorticoid receptor. Am J Physiol Heart Circ Physiol. 2015;309:H127-136. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.00168.2015

      (3) Iddings JA, Kim KJ, Zhou Y, Higashimori H, Filosa JA. Enhanced parenchymal arteriole tone and astrocyte signaling protect neurovascular coupling mediated parenchymal arteriole vasodilation in the spontaneously hypertensive rat. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2015;35:1127-1136. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2015.31

      (4) Diaz JR, Kim KJ, Brands MW, Filosa JA. Augmented astrocyte microdomain Ca(2+) dynamics and parenchymal arteriole tone in angiotensin II-infused hypertensive mice. Glia. 2019;67:551-565. doi: 10.1002/glia.23564

      (5) Kim KJ, Diaz JR, Presa JL, Muller PR, Brands MW, Khan MB, Hess DC, Althammer F, Stern JE, Filosa JA. Decreased parenchymal arteriolar tone uncouples vessel-to-neuronal communication in a mouse model of vascular cognitive impairment. GeroScience. 2021. doi: 10.1007/s11357-020-00305-x

      (6) Chan SL, Nelson MT, Cipolla MJ. Transient receptor potential vanilloid-4 channels are involved in diminished myogenic tone in brain parenchymal arterioles in response to chronic hypoperfusion in mice. Acta Physiol (Oxf). 2019;225:e13181. doi: 10.1111/apha.13181

      (7) Tarantini S, Hertelendy P, Tucsek Z, Valcarcel-Ares MN, Smith N, Menyhart A, Farkas E, Hodges EL, Towner R, Deak F, et al. Pharmacologically-induced neurovascular uncoupling is associated with cognitive impairment in mice. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab. 2015;35:1871-1881. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2015.162

      (8) Ma J, Ayata C, Huang PL, Fishman MC, Moskowitz MA. Regional cerebral blood flow response to vibrissal stimulation in mice lacking type I NOS gene expression. Am J Physiol. 1996;270:H1085-1090. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.1996.270.3.H1085

      (9) Sible IJ, Nation DA. Blood Pressure Variability and Cognitive Decline: A Post Hoc Analysis of the SPRINT MIND Trial. Am J Hypertens. 2023;36:168-175. doi: 10.1093/ajh/hpac128

      (10) Epstein NU, Lane KA, Farlow MR, Risacher SL, Saykin AJ, Gao S. Cognitive dysfunction and greater visit-to-visit systolic blood pressure variability. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 2013;61:2168-2173. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12542

      (11) Antunes M, Biala G. The novel object recognition memory: neurobiology, test procedure, and its modifications. Cognitive processing. 2012;13:93-110. doi: 10.1007/s10339-011-0430-z

      (12) Kraeuter AK, Guest PC, Sarnyai Z. The Y-Maze for Assessment of Spatial Working and Reference Memory in Mice. Methods Mol Biol. 2019;1916:105-111. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-8994-2_10

      (13) Singhal G, Morgan J, Jawahar MC, Corrigan F, Jaehne EJ, Toben C, Breen J, Pederson SM, Manavis J, Hannan AJ, et al. Effects of aging on the motor, cognitive and affective behaviors, neuroimmune responses and hippocampal gene expression. Behav Brain Res. 2020;383:112501. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112501

      (14) Tediashvili G, Wang D, Reichenspurner H, Deuse T, Schrepfer S. Balloon-based Injury to Induce Myointimal Hyperplasia in the Mouse Abdominal Aorta. J Vis Exp. 2018. doi: 10.3791/56477

      (15) Ozkayar N, Dede F, Akyel F, Yildirim T, Ates I, Turhan T, Altun B. Relationship between blood pressure variability and renal activity of the renin-angiotensin system. J Hum Hypertens. 2016;30:297-302. doi: 10.1038/jhh.2015.71

      (16) Kajikawa M, Higashi Y. Blood pressure variability and arterial stiffness: the chicken or the egg? Hypertens Res. 2024;47:1223-1224. doi: 10.1038/s41440-024-01589-8

      (17) Laurent S, Boutouyrie P. Arterial Stiffness and Hypertension in the Elderly. Front Cardiovasc Med. 2020;7:544302. doi: 10.3389/fcvm.2020.544302

      (18) Wallace SM, Yasmin, McEniery CM, Maki-Petaja KM, Booth AD, Cockcroft JR, Wilkinson IB. Isolated systolic hypertension is characterized by increased aortic stiffness and endothelial dysfunction. Hypertension. 2007;50:228-233. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.107.089391

      (19) Al-Merani SA, Brooks DP, Chapman BJ, Munday KA. The half-lives of angiotensin II, angiotensin II-amide, angiotensin III, Sar1-Ala8-angiotensin II and renin in the circulatory system of the rat. J Physiol. 1978;278:471490. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012318

      (20) Zimmerman MC, Lazartigues E, Sharma RV, Davisson RL. Hypertension caused by angiotensin II infusion involves increased superoxide production in the central nervous system. Circ Res. 2004;95:210-216. doi: 10.1161/01.RES.0000135483.12297.e4

      (21) Gonzalez-Villalobos RA, Seth DM, Satou R, Horton H, Ohashi N, Miyata K, Katsurada A, Tran DV, Kobori H, Navar LG. Intrarenal angiotensin II and angiotensinogen augmentation in chronic angiotensin II-infused mice. Am J Physiol Renal Physiol. 2008;295:F772-779. doi: 10.1152/ajprenal.00019.2008

      (22) Nakagawa P, Nair AR, Agbor LN, Gomez J, Wu J, Zhang SY, Lu KT, Morgan DA, Rahmouni K, Grobe JL, et al. Increased Susceptibility of Mice Lacking Renin-b to Angiotensin II-Induced Organ Damage. Hypertension. 2020;76:468-477. doi: 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.120.14972

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) All outcomes are attributed specifically to L6b neurons, but the genetic manipulation is not specific to L6b neurons. The authors acknowledge this as a limitation, but in my view, this global manipulation is more than a limitation - it affects the overall interpretations of the data. The Hoerder-Suabedissen et al., 2018 paper shows sparse, but also dense, expression of Drd1a+ neurons in brain regions outside of the L6b. Given this issue, the results are largely overstated throughout the paper.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s careful reading and concern that some of our statements may have overstated the implications of our data. The Drd1a Cre mouse model used (FK164) has a relatively selective expression of Drd1a Cre in cortex, especially in layer 6b, but indeed some expression is seen in layer 6a and subcortically. We will nuance our claims throughout the paper to ensure that the conclusions are supported by our findings, and further discuss the impact of this limitation on the overall interpretation of our results. Specifically, we will discuss the potential contribution of relevant subcortical areas and layer 6a in the effects we observed.

      (2) It is not clear to me that the "silencing" of Drd1a+ neurons was verified.

      In our previous publications, we showed confirmation of the loss of regulated synaptic vesicle release from the Cre positive neuronal population (Marques-Smith et al., 2016; Hoerder-Suabedissen et al., 2018; Messore et al., 2024), which validates our approach to “silence” cortical neurons. We will discuss this further in the revised manuscript.

      (3) There were various discrepancies (and potentially misattributions) between the stated significant differences in Supplementary Table T1 data and Figure 3a & S2 spectral plots. This issue makes it difficult to effectively evaluate the main text and stated outcomes.

      We thank the reviewer for spotting the inconsistencies in how the statistical comparisons were presented: indeed, in the text we described two-way ANOVAs with posthoc tests but in the figures significance markers were positioned based on multiple t-tests. We have revised Supplementary Table T1, Figure 3a and S2 to ensure that all statistics are presented consistently throughout the manuscript, i.e. with two-way ANOVAs and accompanying posthoc tests.

      Related, the authors stated that post hoc comparisons of EEG spectral frequency bins were not corrected for multiple testing. Instead, significance was only denoted if changes in at least two consecutive frequency bins were significant. However, there are multiple plots in which a single significance marker is placed over an isolated bin (i.e., 4c, 6, S5, S6). Unless each marker is equivalent to 2 consecutive frequency bins, these markers should be removed from the plots. Otherwise, please define the frequency and size of these markers in the main text.

      In line with the previous comment, we have adjusted markers to reflect the results from posthoc tests after two-way ANOVAs in Figures 6 and supplementary figures S5 and S6. 

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out that in our comparisons of EEG spectra, in some cases single isolated frequency bins, where p-value reached 0.05 were shown as significantly different, which indeed could have occurred by chance given that, in line with previous literature, we have not employed multiple testing comparison. In the revised manuscript we will use an unbiased approach by plotting actual p-values for all bins, and moderate our conclusions accordingly, while giving the readers the opportunity to evaluate the magnitude and extent of the differences directly, rather than relying on an arbitrary threshold for significance.

      (4) A rainbow color scale, as in Figure 3, we've now learned, can be misleading and difficult to interpret. The viridis color scale or a different diverging color scale are good alternatives.

      Thank you for pointing this out, we have adjusted the colour scale.

      (5) How much time elapsed between vehicle/orexin A & B infusions?

      There were 2-4 non-infusions days between infusions. We will add this information to methods when revising the manuscript.

      (6) For Figure 6, there are statistical discrepancies between the main text and the plots (pg. 10):

      a) The text claims post hoc differences for relative ORXA frontal EEG, but there are no significance markers on the plot.

      b) The text states that there were no post hoc differences for the relative ORXA occipital EEG, but significance markers are on the plot.

      c) The main test for the relative ORXB frontal EEG was not significant, but there are post hoc significance markers on the plot.

      d) For relative ORXB occipital EEG, there are significant markers on the plot outside of the stated range in the text.

      Thank you for your careful observations, these issues reflect the same inconsistency as raise above, where the text describes two-way ANOVAs and the figures refers to results obtained with multiple t tests. We shall adjust the markers in the figures to be only shown when the ANOVA is significant and show the results of posthoc tests after ANOVAs instead of the results of multiple t tests.

      (7) Some important details are only available in figure captions, making it difficult to understand the main text. For example, when describing Figure 3c in the main text on page 7, it is not clear what type of transitions are being discussed without reading the figure caption. Likewise, a "decrease," "shift," and "change" are mentioned, but relative to what? Similar comment for the EEG theta activity description on pages 7 - 8. Please add relevant details to the main text.

      We will adjust the wording in the main text to reflect more precisely which comparisons are shown in the figures.

      (8) Statistical comparisons for data in Figure 3e, post hoc analyses for data in Figure S7a-b REM data, and post hoc analyses for Figure S7c (not b) occipital EEG should be included to support differences claims. Please denote these differences on the respective plots.

      We have added the statistical comparisons for Figure 3e to the results section.

      We have added the statistical comparisons for Figure S7A to the results section.

      We have added the statistical comparison for Figure S7b to the results section.

      In Figure S7c, there was an overall genotype difference, but there was not a time x genotype interaction, so we have not performed posthoc tests and did not plot posthoc significance markers for this figure. We have adjusted the wording in the results section to make this clearer.

      We have adjusted the reference to the figure S7c which was incorrect, thank you for your careful attention.

      (9) In the subsection titled "Layer 6b mediates effects of orexin on vigilance states (pg. 8)," there does not seem to be any stated differences between control and L6b silenced mice. A more accurate subtitle is needed.

      We shall change the subtitle to: “The effects of orexin on vigilance states in L6b silenced mice”. The main finding described in this section is that the increase in EEG theta frequency after ORXB infusion is attenuated in L6b silenced mice, so a statement summarizing this finding could be an alternative title. However, then it would not accurately reflect other, less conspicuous, yet potentially important findings described in this section (during NREM sleep, only in L6b silenced animals there is an increase in power in the lower frequency bins in the frontal derivation; in the occipital derivation, levels of relative SWA during NREM sleep after ORXA infusion were lower in L6b silenced than in control animals).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Although the authors used a highly selective approach to silence layer 6b neurons, the observed changes in EEG oscillations cannot be solely attributed to layer 6b neurons because of the ICV route for orexin administration.

      We completely agree, and did not want to imply that orexin administered through the ICV route reaches cortical Drd1a Cre expressing neurons only. We will re-word the corresponding sentences accordingly throughout the manuscript.

      (2) The rationale for using only male rats is not provided.

      We agree that this is an important limitation and will acknowledge and discuss it further in the revised manuscript. Unfortunately, our experimental protocol precluded the possibility of monitoring accurately the oestrous cycle, which as well-known has an influence on sleep-wake architecture, brain oscillations as well as orexin signalling and receptor abundance. We therefore decided to use male mice only for the current study, but planning to use both sexes in our follow up work.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Joint Public Review:

      Summary:

      For many years, there has been extensive electrophysiological research investigating the relationship between local field potential patterns and individual cell spike patterns in the hippocampus. In this study, using innovative imaging techniques, they examined spike synchrony of hippocampal cells during locomotion and immobility states. The authors demonstrated that hippocampal place cells exhibit prominent synchronous spikes locked to theta oscillations.

      Strengths:

      The single cell voltage imaging used in this study is a highly novel method that may allow recordings that were not previously possible using existing methods.

      We thank the reviewer for recognizing the strengths of our study.

      Weaknesses:

      The strength of evidence remains incomplete because of the main claim that synchronous events are not associated with ripples. As was mentioned in previous rounds of review, ripples emerge locally and independently in the two hemispheres. Thus, obtaining ripple recordings from the contralateral hemisphere does not provide solid evidence for this claim. The papers the authors are citing to make the claim that "Additionally, we implanted electrodes in the contralateral CA1 region to monitor theta and ripple oscillations, which are known to co-occur across hemispheres (29-31)" do not support this claim. For example, reference 29 contains the following statement: "These findings suggest that ripples emerge locally and independently in the two hemispheres".

      In our previous revisions, we took care to limit our claim to what our data directly supported: that synchronous ensembles of CA1 neurons were not associated with ripple oscillations recorded in the contralateral hippocampus. To address reviewer concerns, we changed the Title, modified the Abstract, adjusted relevant text in the Results, and explicitly acknowledged the methodological limitations in the Discussion. 

      In this round, we further revised the manuscript to directly address the editor’s and reviewer’s remaining concerns: 

      (1) We replaced the word “surprisingly” with a more neutral “Moreover” to avoid implying that the observed dissociation was unexpected given the use of contralateral recordings.

      Introduction (line 67-69):

      “Moreover, these synchronous ensembles occurred outside of contralateral ripples (c-ripples) …”

      (2) We removed the clause stating that ripples “co-occur across hemispheres”, along with the associated citation to Buzsaki et al. (2003), to avoid potential misinterpretation. The sentence now simply states that we recorded ripple and theta oscillations in the contralateral CA1.

      Introduction (line 63-64):

      “Additionally, we implanted electrodes in the contralateral CA1 region to monitor theta and ripple oscillations.” (co-occurrence claim removed)

      (3) We carefully replaced all mentions of “ripples” in the manuscript with “c-ripples” (i.e., contralateral ripples) to ensure that the scope of our findings is clearly defined and cannot be misinterpreted.

      (4) We strengthened the acknowledgment of the methodological limitations in the Discussion. 

      Discussion (line 528-533): 

      “While contralateral LFP recordings can capture large-scale hippocampal theta and ripple oscillations, they do not fully reflect ipsilateral-specific dynamics, such as variation in theta phase alignment or locally generated ripple events (Buzsaki et al., 2003; Szabo et al., 2022; Huang et al., 2024). Given that ripple oscillations can emerge locally and independently in each hemisphere, interpretations based on contralateral recordings must be made with caution. Further studies incorporating simultaneous ipsilateral field potential recordings will be essential to more precisely understand local-global network interactions.”

      These revisions ensure that our manuscript now presents a consistent and appropriately limited interpretation across all sections. We hope these clarifications address all remaining concerns and accurately reflect the scope of our findings.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The work from this paper successfully mapped transcriptional landscape and identified EA-responsive cell types (endothelial, microglia). Data suggest EA modulates BBB via immune pathways and cell communication. However, claims of "BBB opening" are not directly proven (no permeability data).

      (1) No in vivo/in vitro assays confirm BBB permeability changes (e.g., Evans blue leakage, TEER).  

      (2) Only male rats were used, ignoring sex-specific BBB differences.

      (3) Pericytes and neurons, critical for the BBB, were not captured, likely due to dissociation artifacts.

      (4) Protein-level validation (Western blot, IHC) absent for key genes (e.g., LY6E, HSP90).

      (5) Fixed stimulation protocol (2/100 Hz, 40 min); no dose-response or temporal analysis.

      (1) We sincerely apologize for the oversight regarding the description of changes in blood-brain barrier permeability. In fact, our team conducted a series of preliminary studies that verified this aspect, but we did not provide a more detailed introduction in the introduction section. We will address and improve this in the revised manuscript. (2) We are very grateful to the reviewers for pointing out the important and meaningful issue of "gender-specific BBB differences." We will make this a focal point in our future research.

      (2) As for pericytes and neurons, we acknowledge their importance in the function of the blood-brain barrier. We acknowledge the importance of pericytes and neurons in the blood-brain barrier. However, neurons are absent because our sample processing method involves dissociation. During the dissociation procedure, neuronal axons, which are relatively long, are filtered out during the frequent cell suspension steps and cannot enter the downstream microfluidic system for analysis, so they are not present in our data. Since this experiment is primarily focused on non-neuronal cells, we did not choose to use nucleus extraction for sample processing. As for pericytes, we believe they are not captured because their proportion in our samples is extremely low, which is why they are not present in the data. Further research may require single-nucleus transcriptomics or the separate isolation of these two cell types for study. Of course, in our current mechanistic studies, we are also fully considering the important roles these two cell types play in BBB function.

      (3) In addition, for verification at the protein level, we have recently conducted some experiments and will include these results in the revised version.

      (5) Lastly, regarding our electroacupuncture intervention model, we actually conducted a series of parameter optimization experiments during the preliminary exploration phase. This part is indeed lacking in our current introduction, and we will add it to the research background and introduction.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study uses single-cell RNA sequencing to explore how electroacupuncture (EA) stimulation alters the brain's cellular and molecular landscape after blood-brain barrier (BBB) opening. The authors aim to identify changes in gene expression and signaling pathways across brain cell types in response to EA stimulation using single-cell RNA sequencing. This direction holds promise for understanding the consequences of noninvasive methods of BBB opening for therapeutic drug delivery across the BBB.

      (1) The work falls short in its current form. The experimental design lacks a clear justification, and readers are not provided with sufficient background information on the extent, timing, or regional specificity of BBB opening in this EA model. These details, established in prior work, are critical to understanding the rationale behind the current transcriptomic analyses.

      (2) Further, the results are often presented with minimal context or interpretation. There is no model of intercellular or molecular coordination to explain the BBB-opening process, despite the stated goal of identifying such mechanisms. The statement that EA induces a "unique frontal cortex-specific transcriptome signature" is not supported, as no data from other brain regions are presented. Biological interpretation is at times unclear or inaccurate - for instance, attributing astrocyte migration effects to endothelial cell clusters or suggesting microglial tight junction changes without connecting them meaningfully to endothelial function.<br /> (3) The study does include analyses of receptor-ligand signaling and cell-cell communication, which could be among its most biologically rich outputs. However, these are relegated to supplementary material and not shown in the leading figures. This choice limits the utility of the manuscript as a hypothesis-generating resource.

      (4) Overall, while the dataset may be of interest to BBB researchers and those developing technologies for drug delivery across the BBB, the manuscript in its current form does not yet fulfill its interpretive goals. A more integrated and biologically grounded analysis would be beneficial.

      (1) It was indeed our mistake that we did not pay attention to the importance of research background factors such as the degree, timing, or regional specificity of BBB opening for the rationale and purpose of this experimental design. In our revision, we will thoroughly elaborate on the relevant previous studies.

      (2) Our current study is actually based on previous findings that electroacupuncture can open the BBB, with a more pronounced effect observed in the frontal lobe (this aspect should be further described in the research background). Building on this foundation, our aim is to delineate the potential biological mechanisms involved. Therefore, we selected frontal lobe tissue as our primary choice for sequencing and have not yet investigated differences across other brain regions, although this may become a focus of future research. Additionally, we recognize that the mechanism underlying BBB opening is complex, and at present, we cannot determine whether it is driven by a single direct factor or by coordinated actions between cells or molecules. As such, our results are presented only briefly for now, and we will carefully consider whether to supplement our findings by incorporating insights from other studies.

      (3) Thank you very much for bringing this to our attention. We will include the key results of the receptor-ligand signaling and cell-cell communication analysis in the main manuscript.

      (4) Indeed, our current dataset and analysis tend to present objective data results. We are also conducting a series of validations that may be related to the biology of the blood-brain barrier, and we look forward to sharing and discussing any future research findings with you and everyone.

    1. Author response:

      We thank the reviewers for their thoughtful comments, and we plan to implement many of their suggestions to improve the paper. We agree that the paper can benefit from clearer links between the two neural signatures (memory traces and uniform shifts) themselves, and between the neural signatures and behavioral phenomena. We will address these limitations in multiple ways. First, as the reviewers noted, RNN models have the potential to probe these relationships, so we plan to perform further analyses and modeling experiments to uncover any causal relationships. Second, we will also establish clearer definitions of the neural signatures and explore how these signatures can be unified using our models. Finally, we will compare the experimental paradigms between Losey et al and Sun, O’Shea et al, and discuss how differences between the paradigms may have impacted our observations, particularly in the context of other experimental and modeling papers.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Bacterial species that frequently undergo horizontal gene transfer events tend to have genomes that approach linkage equilibrium, making it challenging to analyze population structure and establish the relationships between isolates. To overcome this problem, researchers have established several effective schemes for analyzing N. gonorrhoeae isolates, including MLST and NG-STAR. This report shows that Life Identification Number (LIN) Codes provide for a robust and improved discrimination between different N. gonorrhoeae isolates.

      Strengths:

      The description of the system is clear, the analysis is convincing, and the comparisons to other methods show the improvements offered by LIN Codes.

      Weaknesses:

      No major weaknesses were identified by this reviewer.

      We thank the reviewer for their assessment of our paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper describes a new approach for analyzing genome sequences.

      Strengths:

      The work was performed with great rigor and provides much greater insights than earlier classification systems.

      Weaknesses:

      A minor weakness is that the clinical application of LIN coding could be articulated in a more in-depth way. The LIN coding system is very impressive and is certainly superior to other protocols. My recommendation, although not necessary for this paper, is that the authors expand their analysis to noncoding sequences, especially those upstream of open reading frames. In this respect, important cis-acting regulatory mutations that might help to further distinguish strains could be identified.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments. LIN code could be applied clinically, for example in the analysis of antibiotic resistant isolates, or to investigate outbreaks associated with a particular lineage. We will update the text to describe this more thoroughly.

      In regards to non-coding sequences: unfortunately, intergenic regions are generally unsuitable for use in typing systems as (i) they are subject to phase variation, which can occlude relationships based on descent; (ii) they are inherently difficult to assemble and therefore can introduce variation due to the sequencing procedure rather than biology. For the type of variant typing that LIN code represents, which aims to replicate phylogenetic clustering, protein encoding sequences are the best choice for convenience, stability, and accuracy. This is not to say that it is not a valid object to base a nomenclature on intergenic regions, which might be especially suitable for predicting some phenotypic characters, but this will still be subject to problem (ii), depending on the sequencing technology used.  Such a nomenclature system should stand beside, rather than be combined with or used in place of, phylogenetic typing. However, we could certainly investigate the relationship between an isolates LIN code and regulatory mutations in the future.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this well-written manuscript, Unitt and colleagues propose a new, hierarchical nomenclature system for the pathogen Neisseria gonorrhoeae. The proposed nomenclature addresses a longstanding problem in N. gonorrhoeae genomics, namely that the highly recombinant population complicates typing schemes based on only a few loci and that previous typing systems, even those based on the core genome, group strains at only one level of genomic divergence without a system for clustering sequence types together. In this work, the authors have revised the core genome MLST scheme for N. gonorrhoeae and devised life identification numbers (LIN) codes to describe the N. gonorrhoeae population structure.

      Strengths:

      The LIN codes proposed in this manuscript are congruent with previous typing methods for Neisseria gonorrhea, like cgMLST groups, Ng-STAR, and NG-MAST. Importantly, they improve upon many of these methods as the LIN codes are also congruent with the phylogeny and represent monophyletic lineages/sublineages.

      The LIN code assignment has been implemented in PubMLST, allowing other researchers to assign LIN codes to new assemblies and put genomes of interest in context with global datasets.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors correctly highlight that cgMLST-based clusters can be fused due n to "intermediate isolates" generated through processes like horizontal gene transfer. However, the LIN codes proposed here are also based on single linkage clustering of cgMLST at multiple levels. It is unclear if future recombination or sequencing of previously unsampled diversity within N. gonorrhoeae merges together higher-level clusters, and if so, how this will impact the stability of the nomenclature.

      The authors have defined higher resolution thresholds for the LIN code scheme. However, they do not investigate how these levels correspond to previously identified transmission clusters from genomic epidemiology studies. It would be useful for future users of the scheme to know the relevant LIN code thresholds for these investigations.

      We thank the reviewer for their insightful comments. LIN codes do use multi-level single linkage clustering to define the cluster number of isolates. However, unlike previous applications of simple single linkage clustering such as N. gonorrhoeae core genome groups (Harrison et al., 2020), once assigned in LIN code, these cluster numbers are fixed within an unchanging barcode assigned to each isolate. Therefore, the nomenclature is stable, as the addition of new isolates cannot change previously established LIN codes.

      Cluster stability was considered during the selection of allelic mismatch thresholds. By choosing thresholds based on natural breaks in population structure (Figure 3), applying clustering statistics such as the silhouette score, and by assessing where cluster stability has been maintained within the previous core genome groups nomenclature, we can have confidence that the thresholds which we have selected will form stable clusters. For example, with core genome groups there has been significant group fusion with clusters formed at a threshold of 400 allelic differences, while clustering at a threshold of 300 allelic differences has remained cohesive over time (supported by a high silhouette score) and so was selected as an important threshold in the gonococcal LIN code. LIN codes have now been applied to >27000 isolates in PubMLST, and the nomenclature has remained effective despite the continual addition of new isolates to this collection. The manuscript will be revised to emphasise these points.

      Work is in progress to explore what LIN code thresholds are generally associated with transmission chains. These will likely be the last 7 thresholds (25, 10, 7, 5, 3, 1, 0) as previous work has suggested that isolates linked by transmission within one year are associated with <14 single nucleotide polymorphism differences (De Silva et al., 2016). The results of this analysis will be described in a future article, currently in preparation.

      Harrison, O.B., et al. Neisseria gonorrhoeae Population Genomics: Use of the Gonococcal Core Genome to Improve Surveillance of Antimicrobial Resistance. The Journal of Infectious Diseases 2020.

      De Silva, D., et al. Whole-genome sequencing to determine transmission of Neisseria gonorrhoeae: an observational study. The Lancet Infectious Diseases 2016;16(11):1295-1303.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors try to investigate how the population of microtubules (LSPMB) that originate from sporozoite subpellicular microtubules (SSPM) and are remodelled during liver-stage development of malaria parasites. These bundles shrink over time and help form structures needed for cell division. The authors have used expansion microscopy, live-cell imaging, genetically engineered mutants, and pharmacological perturbation to study parasite development with liver cells.

      A major strength of the manuscript is the live cell imaging and expansion microscopy to study this challenging liver stage of parasite development. It gives important knowledge that PTMs of α-tubulin, such as polyglutamylation and tyrosination/detyrosination, are crucial for microtubule stability. Mutations in α-tubulin reduce the parasite's ability to move and proliferate in the liver cells. The drug oryzalin, which targets microtubules, also blocks parasite development, showing how important dynamic microtubules are at this stage.

      The major problem in the manuscript was the way it flows, as the authors keep shifting from the liver stage to the sporogony stages and then back to the liver stages. It was very confusing at times to know what the real focus of the study is, whether sporozoite development or liver stage development. The flow of the manuscript could be improved. Some of the findings reported here substantiate the previous electron microscopy.

      Overall, the study represents an important contribution towards understanding cytoskeletal remodelling during liver stage infection. The study suggests that tubulin modifications are key for the parasite's survival in the liver and could be targets for new malaria treatments. This is also the stage that has been used for vaccine development, so any knowledge of how parasites proliferate in the liver cells will be beneficial towards intervention approaches.

      We would like to express our sincere gratitude to Reviewer #1 for the positive and encouraging feedback on our manuscript. We are delighted that the reviewer found our experimental design and methodologies appropriate and that our study represents an important contribution to understanding cytoskeletal remodelling during liver stage infection, a critical phase for vaccine development. We are also grateful to the reviewer for highlighting the issue with the manuscript's flow. We acknowledge this limitation and will significantly improve the narrative structure and logical progression in the revised manuscript to ensure clarity and avoid any potential confusion. Thank you again for your thoughtful and constructive comments.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigated microtubule distribution and their possible post-translational modifications (PTM) in Plasmodium berghei during development of the liver stage, using either hepatocytes or HeLa cells as models. They used conventional immunofluorescence assays and expansion microscopy with various antibodies recognising tubulin and, in the second part of the work, its candidate PTMs, as well as markers of Plasmodium, in addition to live imaging with a fluorescent marker for tubulin. In the third part of the study, they generated 3 mutants deprived of either the last four residues or the last 11 residues, or where a candidate polyglutamylation site was substituted by an alanine residue.

      Strengths:

      In the first part, microtubules are monitored by a combination of two approaches (IFA and live), revealing nicely the evolution of the sporozoite subpellicular microtubules (SSPM, the sporozoite is the developmental stage present in salivary glands of the mosquitoes and that infects hepatocytes) into a different structure termed liver-stage parasite microtubule bundle (LSPMB). The LSPMB shrinks during the course of parasite development and finally disappears while hemi-spindles emerge over time. Contact points between these two structures are observed frequently in live cells and occasionally in fixed cells, suggesting the intriguing possibility that tubulin might be recycled from the LSPMB to contribute to hemi-spindle formation.

      In the second part, antibodies recognising (1) the final tyrosine found at the C-terminal tail and (2) a stretch of 3 glutamate residues in a side chain are used to monitor these candidate PTMs. Signals are positive at the SSPM, and while it remains positive for polyglutamylation, it becomes negative for the final tyrosine at the LSPM, while a positive signal emerges at hemi-spindles at later stages of development.

      In the last part, the three mutants are fed to mosquitoes, where they show reduced development, the one lacking the alpha-tubulin tail even failing to reach the salivary glands. However, the two other mutants infect HeLa cells normally, whereas sporozoites with the C-terminal tail deletion recovered from the haemolymph did not develop in these cells.

      The first part provides convincing evidence that microtubules are extensively remodelled during the infection of hepatocytes and HeLa cells, in agreement with the spectacular Plasmodium morphogenetic changes accompanying massive and rapid proliferation. The third part brings further confirmation that the C-terminal tail of alpha-tubulin is essential for multiple stages of parasite development, in agreement with previous work (50). Since it is the region where several post-translational modifications take place in other organisms (detyrosination, polyglutamylation, glycylation), it makes sense to propose that the essential function is related to these PTMs also in Plasmodium.

      Weaknesses:

      The significance of tubulin PTM relies on two antibodies whose reactivity to Plasmodium tubulins is unclear (see below). The interpretation of the literature on detyrosination and polyglutamylation is confusing in several places, meaning that the statements about the possible role of these PTMs need to be carefully revisited.

      The authors use the term "tyrosination" but the alpha1-tubulin studied here possesses the final tyrosine when it is synthesised, so it is "tyrosinated" by default. It could potentially be removed by a tyrosine carboxypeptidase of the vasoinhibin family (VASH) as reported in other species. After removal, this tyrosine can be added again by a tubulin-tyrosine ligase (TTL) enzyme. It is therefore more appropriate to talk about detyrosination-retyrosination rather than tyrosination (this confusion is unfortunately common in the literature, see Janke & Magiera, 2020).

      The difficulty here is that there is so far no evidence that detyrosination takes place in Plasmodium. Neither VASH nor TTL could be identified in the Plasmodium genome (ref 31, something we can confirm with our unsuccessful BLAST analyses), and mass spectrometry studies of purified tubulin, albeit from blood stages, did not find evidence for detyrosination (reference 43). Western blots using an antibody against detyrosinated tubulin did not produce a positive signal, neither on purified tubulin, nor on whole parasites (43). Of course, the situation could be different in liver stages, but the question of the detyrosinating enzyme is still there. The existence of a unique Plasmodium system for detyrosination cannot be formally ruled out but given the high degree of conservation of these PTMs and their associated enzymes, it sounds difficult to imagine.

      The fact that the anti-tyrosinated antibody still produced a signal in the cell line where the final tyrosine is deleted raises issues about its specificity. A cross-reactivity with beta-tubulin is proposed, but the Plasmodium beta-tubulin does not carry a final tyrosine, further raising concerns about antibody specificity.

      The interpretation of these results should therefore be considered carefully. There also seems to be some confusion in the function of detyrosination cited from the literature. It is said in line 229 that "tyrosination has been associated with stable microtubules" (33, 34, 50, 55). References 33 and 34 actually show that tyrosinated microtubules turn over faster in neurons or in epithelial cells, respectively, while references 50 and 55 do not study de/retyrosination. The general consensus is that tyrosinated microtubules are more dynamic (see reference 24).

      The situation is a bit different for polyglutamylation since several candidate poly- or mono-glutamylases have been identified in the Plasmodium genome, and at least mono-glutamylation of beta-tubulin has been formally proven, still in bloodstream stages (ref 43). The authors propose that the residue E445 is the polyglutamylation site. To our knowledge, this has not been demonstrated for Plasmodium. This residue is indeed the favourite one in several organisms such as humans and trypanosomes (Eddé et al., Science 1990; Schneider et al., JCS, 1997), and it is tempting to propose it would be the same here. However, TTLLs bind the tubulin tails from their C-terminal end like a glove on a finger (Garnham et al., Cell, 2015), and the presence of two extra residues in Plasmodium tubulins would mean that the reactive glutamate might be in position E447 rather than E445. This is worth discussing.

      On the positive side, it is encouraging to see that signals for both anti-tyrosinated tail and poly-glutamylated side chain are going down in the various mutants, but this would need validation with a comparison for alpha-tubulin signal.

      Line 316: polyglutamylation "is commonly associated with dynamic microtubule behavior (78-80)". Actually, references 78 and 79 show the impact of this PTM on interaction with spastin, and reference 80 discusses polyglutamylation as a marker of stable microtubules in the context of cilia and flagella. The consensus is that polyglutamylated microtubules tend to be more stable (ref24).

      Conclusion:

      The first and the third parts of this manuscript - evolution of microtubules and importance of the C-terminal tails for Plasmodium development - are convincing and well supported by data. However, the presence and role of tubulin PTM should be carefully reconsidered.

      Plasmodium tubulins are more closely related to plant tubulins and are sensitive to inhibitors that do not affect mammalian microtubules. They therefore represent promising drug targets as several well-characterised compounds used as herbicides are available. The work produced here further defines the evolution of the microtubule network in sporozoites and liver stages, which are the initial and essential first steps of the infection. Moreover, Plasmodium has multiple specificities that make it a fascinating organism to study both for cell biology and evolution. The data reported here are elegant and will attract the attention of the community working on parasites but also on the cytoskeleton at large. It will be interesting to have the feedback of other people working on tubulin PTMs to figure out the significance of this part of the work.

      We thank Reviewer #2 for the thoughtful and detailed evaluation of our manuscript. We are pleased that the reviewer found our study elegant and believe it will attract the attention of the broader scientific community, both those working on parasites and those focused on cytoskeleton biology. We also acknowledge the concerns raised regarding the specificity of the antibodies used to detect tubulin post-translational modifications (PTMs), as well as the interpretation of their signals and the current lack of identified detyrosination enzymes in the Plasmodium genome. We agree that these are important limitations, and we will address them thoroughly in the revised manuscript. This includes clarifying our interpretation of tyrosination versus detyrosination, adjusting our claims regarding polyglutamylation sites, and carefully revisiting the literature cited to ensure accurate contextualization of PTM function in microtubule stability.

      We are grateful for the reviewer’s close reading and critical feedback, which will help us substantially improve the clarity, precision, and strength of our manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Atchou et al. investigates the role of the microtubule cytoskeleton in sporozoites of Plasmodium berghei, including possible functions of microtubule post-translational modifications (tyrosination and polyglutamylation) in the development of sporozoites in the liver. They also assessed the development of sporozoites in the mosquito. Using cell culture models and in vivo infections with parasites that contain tubulin mutants deficient in certain PTMs, they show that may aspects of the life cycle progression are impaired. The main conclusion is that microtubule PTMs play a major role in the differentiation processes of the parasites.

      However, there are a number of major and minor points of criticism that relate to the interpretation of some of the data.

      We thank Reviewer #3 for the overall positive assessment of our study and for recognizing its contribution to advancing our understanding of Plasmodium biology and malaria pathogenesis. We appreciate the reviewer’s constructive feedback, particularly regarding the interpretation of some of our data. These comments have been very helpful in guiding our revisions, and we have worked to improve both the clarity of our presentation and the precision of our interpretations in the revised manuscript.

      Below, we respond in detail to each of the reviewer’s points.

      Comments:<br /> (1) The first paragraph of "Results" almost suggests that the presence of a subpellicular MT-array in sporozoites is a new discovery. This is not the case, see e.g. the recent publication by Ferreira et al. (Nature Communications, 2023).

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out and fully agree that the subpellicular microtubule (SPM) array in sporozoites is well established, as documented in earlier work (e.g., Cyrklaff et al., 2007) and more recently by Ferreira et al. (Nat. Commun., 2023). Our intention was not to suggest that the existence of the SSPM is a novel finding. Rather, our study builds on this existing knowledge by demonstrating that these sporozoite-derived microtubules are not disassembled upon hepatocyte entry but are repurposed into a newly described structure, the liver stage parasite microtubule bundle (LSPMB). This reorganization, its persistence into liver stage development, and its dynamic role in microtubule remodeling and nuclear division are, to our knowledge, novel observations. We will revise the manuscript to make this distinction clearer in the introduction and the results section.

      (2) Why were HeLa cells and not hepatocytes (as in Figure 3) used for measuring infection rates of the mutants in Figure 5H and 5L? As I understand, HeLa cells are not natural host cells for invading sporozoites. HeLa cells are epithelial cells derived from a cervical tumour. I am not an expert in Plasmodium biology, but is a HeLa infection an accepted surrogate model for liver stage development?

      We appreciate the opportunity to clarify our experimental model. While HeLa cells are not the natural host cells, they are a well-established and validated in vitro model for studying Plasmodium berghei liver stage development in our lab and others. In this system, the parasite completes its full development and generates infectious merozoites. Numerous studies have successfully used HeLa cells as a liver stage infection model, with key findings subsequently validated in primary hepatocytes or in vivo, confirming its utility as a representative model. We employed this cell line primarily to reduce animal usage in accordance with the 3Rs principles (Replacement, Reduction, Refinement). Importantly, to ensure the biological relevance of our discoveries in HeLa cells, we validated our key findings in primary mouse hepatocytes, as shown in Figure 3. Furthermore, we confirmed the in vivo infectivity of mutant parasite lines that produced typical salivary gland sporozoites through an in vivo infection assay, presented in Figure S4C.

      (3) The tubulin staining in Figures 1A and 1B is confusing and doesn't seem to make sense. Whereas in 1A the antibody nicely stains host and parasite tubulin, in 1B, only parasite tubulin is visible. If the same antibody and the same host cells have been used, HeLa cytoplasmic microtubules should be visible in 1B. In fact, they should be the predominant antigen. The same applies to Figure 2, where host microtubules are also not visible.

      We thank the reviewer for this careful observation regarding the α-tubulin staining in Figures 1A and 1B. The same host cell type (HeLa) and α-tubulin antibody were indeed used in both experiments. Figure 1A shows results from conventional immunofluorescence assays, where both host and parasite microtubules are clearly stained. In contrast, Figure 1B shows the outcome of ultrastructure expansion microscopy (U-ExM), where parasite microtubules appear prominently, while host microtubules are less visible.

      This effect appears to be a technical outcome of the U-ExM protocol, which can differentially preserve or reveal microtubule epitopes. We consistently observed stronger parasite signal across various cell types, including primary hepatocytes (Figure 3A,B). The lack of visible host microtubules in some U-ExM images does not reflect their absence, but rather reduced signal intensity relative to the parasite structures. This is not observed with all antibodies, e.g., host microtubules stain strongly with anti-tyrosinated α-tubulin (Figure 3B), likely reflecting their high tyrosination state.

      To overcome this limitation, we employed PS-ExM and combined PS-ExM/U-ExM approaches (as described in reference 56), which allowed simultaneous high-resolution visualization of both host and parasite microtubule networks. These combined methods are now being used in follow-up studies to investigate host–parasite microtubule interactions in more detail.

      We will clarify this point in the revised manuscript to avoid confusion.

      (4) In Figures 2A and B, the host nuclei appear to have very different sizes in the DMSO controls and in the drug-treated cells. For example, in the 20 µM (-) image (bottom right), the nuclei are much larger than in the DMSO (-) control (top left). If this is the case, expansion microscopy hasn't worked reproducibly, and therefore, quantification of fluorescence is problematic. The scalebar is the same for all panels.

      The expansion microscopy methods used in this study have been rigorously validated for both reproducibility and isotropicity. However, as the reviewer rightly notes, host cell nuclei can vary in size due to several factors, including cell cycle stage, infection status, and the extent of parasite development, all of which can influence host nuclei morphology and size.

      Importantly, the quantifications relevant to our conclusions were focused specifically on parasite structures. We did not rely on host nuclear size or host fluorescence intensity as a quantitative readout in this context. While we acknowledge the observed variability in host nuclear dimensions, it does not compromise the accuracy or reproducibility of the parasite specific measurements central to our study.

      We will clarify this point in the revised figure legend and manuscript.

      (5) I don't quite follow the argument that spindles and the LSPMB are dynamic structures (e.g., lines 145, 174). That is a trivial statement for the spindle, as it is always dynamic, but beyond that, it has only been shown that the structure is sensitive to oryzalin. That says little about any "natural" dynamic behaviour. Any microtubule structure can be destroyed by a particular physical or chemical treatment, but that doesn't mean all structures are dynamic. It also depends on the definition of "dynamic" in a particular context, for example, the time scale of dynamic behaviour (changes within seconds, minutes, or hours).

      We agree that sensitivity to chemical depolymerization alone does not necessarily indicate dynamic behavior, particularly in the absence of data on turnover kinetics or temporal changes.

      Our interpretation was based on two observations: first, that the LSPMB, which derives from the highly stable sporozoite subpellicular microtubules (known to be drug-resistant), becomes susceptible to depolymerization during the liver stage; and second, that the LSPMB gradually shrinks over time during parasite development. These features suggested a transition toward a more dynamic state compared to its origin. However, we fully agree that “dynamic” is a context-dependent term and that direct evidence such as turnover rates or structural changes on short time scales, is required to rigorously define microtubule dynamics.

      We will revise the manuscript to clarify our use of this term and explicitly acknowledge the need for further studies to characterize the timescale and mechanisms underlying LSPMB remodeling.

      (6) I am not sure what part in the story EB1 plays. The data are only shown in the Supplements and don't seem to be of particular relevance. EB1 is a ubiquitous protein associated with microtubule plus ends. The statement (line 192) that it "may play a broader role..." is unsubstantiated and cannot be based merely on the observation that it is expressed in a particular life cycle stage.

      We agree that EB1 is a ubiquitous microtubule plus-end binding protein and that its presence alone does not imply a novel function. Previous studies (e.g., Maurer et al., 2023; Yang et al., 2023; Zeeshan et al., 2023) have focused on its role during Plasmodium sexual stages, while its expression during liver and mosquito stages has not been previously documented.

      Our data extend this knowledge by showing that EB1 is also expressed during liver stage development, particularly during the highly mitotic schizont phase. While we agree that this observation alone does not prove functional involvement, it raises the possibility of a broader role for EB1 in regulating microtubule dynamics beyond sexual stages. To avoid overinterpretation, we have presented these findings in the supplementary material and will revise the manuscript to tone down speculative statements and clearly frame this as a preliminary observation that warrants further investigation.

      (7) Line 196 onwards: The antibody IN105 is better known in the field as polyE. Maybe that should be added in Materials and Methods. Also, the antibody T9028 against tyrosinated tubulin is poorly validated in the literature and rarely used. Usually, researchers in this field use the monoclonal antibody YL1/2. I am not sure why this unusual antibody was chosen in this study. In fact, has its specificity against tyrosinated α-tubulin from Plasmodium berghei ever been shown? The original antigen was human and had the sequence EGEEY. The Plasmodium sequence is YEADY and hence very different. It is stated that the LSPMB is both polyglutamylated and tyrosinated. This is unusual because polyglutamylated microtubules are usually indicative of stable microtubules, whereas tyrosinated microtubules are found on freshly polymerised and dynamic microtubules. However, a co-localisation within the same cell has not been attempted. This is, however, possible since polyE is a rabbit antibody and T9028 is a mouse antibody. I suspect that differences or gradients along the LSPMB would have been noticed. Also, in lines 207/208, it is said that tyrosination disappears after hepatocyte invasion, which is shown in Figure 3. However, in Figure 3A, quite a lot of positive signals for tyrosination are visible in the 54 and 56 hpi panels.

      First, we acknowledge that the IN105 antibody is more widely known as "polyE" in the field. We will update the Materials and Methods section accordingly to reflect this nomenclature.

      Regarding the use of the T9028 antibody against tyrosinated α-tubulin: we agree that this monoclonal antibody is less commonly used than YL1/2, and we appreciate the reviewer drawing attention to this. The original antigen for T9028 is based on the mammalian C-terminal sequence EGEEY, which differs from the Plasmodium α1-tubulin sequence (YEADY). Like many in the field, we face the challenge that most available antibodies are raised against mammalian epitopes, and specificity in Plasmodium can vary. Nonetheless, the literature (e.g., Hirst et al., 2022; Fennell et al., 2008) has demonstrated that tyrosination occurs in Plasmodium α1-tubulin, using anti-tyrosination antibodies including YL1/2.

      Following the reviewer’s excellent suggestion, we are currently repeating the key experiments using the YL1/2 antibody to compare staining patterns directly with those obtained using T9028. We will include these results in the revised manuscript.

      Concerning the potential co-localization of polyglutamylation and tyrosination on the LSPMB: we agree that this is an interesting and testable hypothesis. In the current manuscript, Figures 3A and 3B were generated from independent experiments, and thus co-localization was not assessed. However, as the reviewer correctly notes, polyE and T9028 antibodies are raised in rabbit and mouse, respectively, making co-staining feasible. We will follow up on this experimentally and, if feasible within our revision timeline, include data in the revised version or highlight this as a future direction.

      Finally, with regard to Figure 3 and the observation that tyrosination appears to persist at 54 and 56 hpi (Figure 3B): the reviewer is correct that tyrosination signal is still detectable at these time points. Our statement that tyrosination “disappears after hepatocyte invasion” was intended to refer to an overall decrease in signal intensity during early liver stage development, with a reappearance at later stages (e.g., cytomere formation). We will rephrase this section for greater clarity and ensure that figure annotations and legends unambiguously reflect the dynamics observed.

      (8) In line 229, it is stated that tyrosination "has previously been associated with stable microtubule in motility". This statement is not correct. In fact, none of the cited references that apparently support this statement show that this is the case. On the contrary, stable microtubules, such as flagellar axonemes, are almost completely detyrosinated. Therefore, tyrosination is a marker for dynamic microtubules, whereas detyrosinated microtubules are indicative of stable microtubules. This is an established fact, and it is odd that the authors claim the opposite.

      We fully agree that in canonical eukaryotic systems, tyrosinated microtubules are generally markers of dynamic microtubule populations, whereas detyrosinated microtubules are typically associated with stability particularly in structures such as flagellar axonemes.

      Our original statement will be corrected. In our study, we observed that tyrosinated microtubules are prevalent in invasive stages (sporozoites and merozoites), while detyrosinated forms become more prominent during intracellular liver stage development. This pattern is consistent with the established link between tyrosination and dynamic microtubules.

      What is particularly intriguing in Plasmodium is the apparent cycling of tyrosination despite the absence of known tubulin tyrosine ligase (TTL) homologs in the genome. This suggests either a highly divergent enzyme or the involvement of host cell factors, a hypothesis supported by the reappearance of tyrosinated microtubules during liver stage schizogony (Figure 3B).

      We will revise the relevant text and the Discussion section to reflect these mechanistic considerations more accurately and to avoid misrepresenting established principles of microtubule biology.

      (9) Line 236 onwards: Concerning the generation of tubulin mutants, I think it is necessary to demonstrate successful replacement of the wild-type allele by the mutant allele. I am sure the authors have done this by amplification and subsequent sequencing of the genomic locus using PCR primers outside the plasmid sequences. I suggest including this information, e.g., by displaying the chromatograph trace in a supplementary figure. Or are the sequences displayed in Figure S3B already derived from sequenced genomic DNA? This is not described in the Legend or in Materials and Methods. The left PCR products obtained for Figure S3 B would be a suitable template for sequencing.

      Indeed, these data are presented in Figure 4B and the corresponding sequence data are shown in Figure S3B. We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion, which will help improve the transparency and reproducibility of our methodology.

      (10) It is also important to be aware of the fact that glutamylation also occurs on β-tubulin. This signal will also be detected by polyE (IN105). Therefore, it is surprising that IN105 immunofluorescence is negative on the C-term Δ cells (Figure S3 D). Is there anything known about confirmed polyglutamylation sites on both α- and β-tubulins in Plasmodium, e.g., by MS? In Toxoplasma, both α- and β-tubulin have been shown to be polyglutamylated.

      Indeed, polyglutamylation is known to occur not only on α-tubulin but also on β-tubulin in many organisms, including Toxoplasma gondii, and the polyE (IN105) antibody is expected to detect polyglutamylation on both tubulin isoforms.

      The parasites shown in Figure S3D correspond to mutant lines originally generated by Spreng et al. (2019): the IntronΔ mutant (with deletion of introns in the Plasmodium α1-tubulin gene) and the C-termΔ mutant (with deletion of the final three C-terminal residues: ADY). As the reviewer correctly notes, this particular C-terminal deletion does not include the predicted polyglutamylation site (E445 or E447, depending on alignment), and thus should not abolish all polyglutamylation. However, in our experiments, the IN105 signal is substantially reduced in this mutant. This may suggest that structural alterations in the tubulin tail affect accessibility of the polyglutamylation epitope or influence the modification itself though we cannot exclude other possibilities, including changes in antibody recognition.

      To date, polyglutamylation sites in Plasmodium tubulins have not been definitively confirmed by mass spectrometry. However, a recent MS-based study (reference 43) detected monoglutamylation on β-tubulin in blood stage parasites. Direct MS evidence for polyglutamylation of either α- or β-tubulin in Plasmodium liver stages is still lacking. We will clarify these points in the revised manuscript to avoid potential confusion and to highlight the need for future biochemical validation of PTM sites.

      (11) Figure S3 is very confusing. In the legend, certain intron deletions are mentioned. How does this relate to posttranslational tubulin modifications? The corresponding section in Results (lines 288-292) is also not very helpful in understanding this.

      The parasite lines shown in Figure S3D were originally generated by Spreng et al. (2019) and are not directly part of the main set of PTM-targeted mutants described in our study. Specifically, the IntronΔ line carries deletions in introns of the Plasmodium α1-tubulin gene, while the C-termΔ line lacks the final three C-terminal residues (ADY). These lines were included for comparative purposes to explore whether structural changes in α-tubulin could impact polyglutamylation signal, as detected by the polyE (IN105) antibody.

      We acknowledge that the figure legend and corresponding text (lines 288–292) did not adequately explain the rationale for including these control lines. We will revise both the legend and Results section to more clearly describe the origin, purpose, and relevance of these mutants to the overall study.

      (12) Figure 4E doesn't look like brightfield microscopy but like some sort of fluorescent imaging. In Figure 4C, were the control (NoΔ) cells with an integrated cassette, but no mutations, or non-transgenic cells?

      The reviewer is absolutely correct: Figure 4E shows a fluorescent image acquired using widefield microscopy and not a brightfield image. We will revise the figure legend accordingly to avoid confusion. The “BF” (brightfield) label applies only to the left panel in Figure 4C, which depicts oocysts imaged using transmitted light.

      Regarding the controls labeled "NoΔ" in Figure 4C, we confirm that these parasites contain the integrated selection cassette but do not harbor any mutations in the target gene. They serve as proper integration controls, allowing us to distinguish the effects of the point mutations or deletions introduced in the experimental lines.

      (13) It is difficult to understand why the TyΔ and the CtΔ mutants still show quite a strong signal using the anti-tyrosination antibody. If the mutants have replaced all wild-type alleles, the signal should be completely absent, unless the antibody (see my comment above concerning T9028) cross-reacts with detyrosinated microtubules. Therefore, the quantitation in Figures 5F and 5G is actually indicative of something that shouldn't be like that. The quantitation of 5F is at odds with the microscopy image in 5D. If this image is representative, the anti-Ty staining in TyΔ is as strong as in the control NoΔ.

      We agree that the persistence of anti-tyrosination signal in the TyΔ and CtΔ mutant lines is unexpected, given that all wild-type alleles were replaced. This discrepancy has led us to further investigate the specificity of the T9028 antibody, as raised in the reviewer’s earlier comment. To address this concern, we are currently repeating the key experiments using the well-established YL1/2 monoclonal antibody, which is widely accepted for detecting tyrosinated α-tubulin in other systems.

      We also acknowledge that Figure 5F shows residual tyrosination signal, and the reviewer is correct that this should not occur if the modified residues are the exclusive PTM sites. One possible explanation is that adjacent residues or even alternative tubulin isoforms may serve as substrates. While α1-tubulin is the dominant isoform in Plasmodium, low-level expression of α2-tubulin has been detected in liver stages based on transcriptomic data, and it may contribute to the observed signal.

      Regarding the apparent discrepancy between the quantification in Figure 5F and the representative image in Figure 5D, we will revise the figure legend to clarify that image selection aimed to show detectable signal, not necessarily the average phenotype. We will also reassess and, if needed, repeat the quantification with improved image sets to ensure accuracy and consistency.

      We will revise the manuscript to reflect these points and include a more nuanced interpretation of the residual staining in the mutant lines.

      (14) The statement that the failure of CtΔ mutants to generate viable sporozoites is due to the lack of microtubule PTMs (lines 295-296) is speculative. The lack of the entire C-terminal tail could have a number of consequences, such as impaired microtubule assembly or failure to recruit and bind associated proteins. This is not necessarily linked to PTMs. Also, it has been shown in yeast that for microtubules to form properly and exquisite regulation (proteostasis) of the ratio between α- and β-tubulin is essential (Wethekam and Moore, 2023). I am not sure, but according to Materials and Methods (line 423), the gene cassettes for replacing the wild-type tubulin gene with the mutant versions contain a selectable marker gene for pyrimethamine selection. Are there qPCR data that show that expression levels of mutant α-tubulin are more or less the same as the wild-type levels?

      We agree that attributing the developmental failure of the CtΔ mutants solely to the absence of microtubule post-translational modifications (PTMs) is speculative. As the reviewer rightly points out, deletion of the entire C-terminal tail may have multiple effects, including impaired microtubule assembly, altered α/β-tubulin stoichiometry, or disruption of interactions with essential microtubule-associated proteins (MAPs). These consequences may arise independently of PTMs.

      That said, we note that PTMs particularly polyglutamylation, can modulate MAP binding by altering the surface charge of microtubules (Genova et al., 2023; Mitchell et al., 2010). Therefore, while PTM loss may be a contributing factor, we acknowledge that the phenotype likely results from a combination of mechanisms. We will revise the relevant section of the manuscript to present a more cautious and balanced interpretation.

      Regarding the reviewer’s question on expression levels: although the replacement constructs include a pyrimethamine resistance cassette, we have not yet quantified α-tubulin transcript levels by qPCR. In the interim, the study by Spreng et al. (2019) (reference 50) on a related α1-tubulin nutations provides valuable insight. They observed no difference in mRNA levels in day 12 oocysts, yet reported fainter microtubule staining and shorter sporozoites, suggesting a post-transcriptional mechanism affecting protein expression or function in later stages. Furthermore, the phenotypic spectrum across their mutant panel (Suppl. Fig. 3 D and E) implies that robust α-tubulin regulation is highly sensitive to specific sequences.

      We acknowledge this as a current limitation in our study and will address it in the revised manuscript, noting that direct measurement of transcript levels is a key area for future investigation.

      (15) In the Discussion, my impression is that two recent studies, the superb Expansion Microscopy study by Bertiaux et al. (2021) and the cryo-EM study by Ferreira et al. (2023), are not sufficiently recognised (although they are cited elsewhere in the manuscript). The latter study includes a detailed description of the microtubule cytoskeleton in sporozoites. However, the present study clearly expands the knowledge about the structure of the cytoskeleton in liver stage parasites and is one of the few studies addressing the distribution and function of microtubule post-translational modifications in Plasmodium.

      Indeed, our work builds upon the established knowledge from Bertiaux et al. (2021) and the cryo-EM study by Ferreira et al. (2023), as rightly mentioned by the reviewer. We agree that these foundational studies, combined with our findings, will significantly expand the understanding of Plasmodium biology and cytoskeleton dynamics across its life cycle and will open the door for further investigations. We are grateful for this suggestion and will ensure these key studies are appropriately acknowledged in the revised manuscript.

      (16) I somewhat disagree with the statement of a co-occurrence of polyglutamylated and tyrosinated microtubules. I think the resolution is too low to reach that conclusion. As this is a bold claim, and would be contrary to what is known from other organisms, it would require a more rigorous validation. Given the apparent problems with the anti-Ty antibody (signal in the TyΔ mutant), one should be very cautious with this claim.

      This is a very important point to clarify. As mentioned previously, the initial experiments for these modifications were performed independently. It is established that sporozoite subpellicular microtubules exhibit both tyrosination and polyglutamylation. We will revise the manuscript to temper this statement and clearly indicate that the co-occurrence of these PTMs remains a hypothesis that requires more rigorous validation. As suggested, we are now conducting additional co-staining experiments using the better validated YL1/2 antibody to re-express and directly compare the distribution of both PTMs within the same cell. These follow-up experiments will help clarify whether both modifications occur simultaneously on the same microtubule structures in Plasmodium liver stages.

      (17) In the Discussion (lines 311 and 377), it is again claimed that tyrosinated microtubules are "a well-known marker of stable microtubules". This statement is completely incorrect, and I am surprised by this serious mistake. A few lines later, the authors say that polyglutamylated is "commonly associated with dynamic microtubule behaviour". Again, this is completely incorrect and is the opposite of what is firmly established in the literature. Polyglutamylation and detyrosination are markers of stable microtubules.

      Indeed, in canonical eukaryotic systems, tyrosinated microtubules are generally considered markers of dynamic microtubule populations, whereas detyrosinated and polyglutamylated microtubules are more commonly associated with stability.

      We acknowledge this mistake and will revise the Discussion to correct these statements accordingly. In the context of Plasmodium, our observations suggest an unusual regulation of microtubule dynamics, which may reflect parasite-specific adaptations. For example, we observed tyrosinated α-tubulin in the stable subpellicular microtubules of sporozoites structures typically known for their exceptional stability. This atypical association implies either non-canonical roles for tyrosination or parasite-specific mechanisms for modulating microtubule properties. Additionally, the presence of both PTMs at different stages of development and on different microtubule populations suggests tightly regulated spatial and temporal modulation of microtubule function.

      We will carefully revise the relevant sections of the manuscript to remove incorrect generalizations and ensure accurate representation of the current consensus in the field, while emphasizing the possibility of Plasmodium-specific adaptations that merit further study.

      (18) In line 339, the authors interpret the residual antibody staining after the introduction of the mutant tubulin as a compensatory mechanism. There is no evidence for this. More likely explanations are firstly the quality of the anti-Ty-antibody used (see comment above), and the fact that also β-tubulin carries C-terminal polyglutamylation sites, which haven't been investigated in this study. PTMs on β-tubulin are not compensatory, but normal PTMs, at least in all other organisms where microtubule PTMs have been investigated.

      As mentioned above, we are currently repeating the key experiments with the [YL1/2] antibody, as suggested. Furthermore, we fully agree with the reviewer's point regarding polyglutamylation on β-tubulin. The C-terminal tail of β-tubulin does indeed contain polyglutamylation sites. As we noted in the manuscript (Lines 340-352), this aspect has not been investigated in the present study, and we acknowledge it as a valuable direction for future research. We will revise the text accordingly to avoid overinterpretation and to more accurately reflect the limitations of our current data.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In the presented study, the authors aim to explore the role of nociceptors in the fine particulate matter (FPM) mediated Asthma phenotype, using rodent models of allergic airway inflammation. This manuscript builds on previous studies and identify transcriptomic reprogramming and an increased sensitivity of the jugular nodose complex (JNC) neurons, one of the major sensory ganglia for the airways, on exposure to FPM along with Ova during the challenge phase. The authors then use OX-314 a selectively permeable form of lidocaine, and TRPV1 knockouts to demonstrate that nociceptor blocking can reduce airway inflammation in their experimental setup. The authors further identify the presence of Gfra3 on the JNC neurons, a receptor for the protein Artemin, and demonstrate their sensitivity to Artemin as a ligand. They further show that alveolar macrophages release Artemin on exposure to FPM.

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable comments, which have significantly enhanced the quality of our manuscript. A point-by-point rebuttal is provided below.

      Strength

      The study builds on results available from multiple previous work and presents important results which allow insights into the mixed phenotypes of Asthma seen clinically. In addition, by identifying the role of nociceptors, they identify potential therapeutic targets which bear high translational potential.

      Weakness

      While the results presented in the study are highly relevant, there is a need for further mechanistic dissection to allow better inferences. Currently certain results seem associative. Also, certain visualisations and experimental protocols presented in the manuscript need careful assessment and interpretation. While Asthma is a chronic disease, the presented results are particularly important to explore Asthma exacerbations in response to acute exposure to air pollutants. This is relevant in today's age of increasing air pollution and increasing global travel.

      Major

      The JNC is a major group of neurons responsible for receiving sensory inputs from the airways. However, the DRG also contains nociceptors and is known to receive afference from the upper airways. An explanation of why the study was restricted to the JNC would be important.

      We acknowledge that some afferents to the upper airways do arise from the DRG, specifically in the upper thoracic segments (T1–T5). We have added a statement in the text to note this subset of nociceptive and spinally mediated pathways. However, the preponderance of evidence indicates that the majority of airway and lung afferents (70–80%, sometimes up to 90%) originate from the jugular–nodose complex (JNC). Given this large imbalance—and because our study focuses on the mechanosensory, and chemosensory functions mediated primarily by the JNC—we restricted our analysis to this main vagal pathway. By contrast, DRG innervation, though functionally important for nociception and irritation-related reflexes, accounts for a smaller yet significant (~20–30%) fraction of the total afferent pool. The referenced tracing studies[1,2] support this distribution and are cited to clarify our rationale for emphasizing the JNC in our work.

      Similarly, the role of the Artemin in the study remains associative. The study results present that Artemin sensitize nociceptors to lead to an increased inflammatory response (Supplementary Figure 2), however, both upstream and downstream evidence for this inference needs to be dissected further. For instance, the evidence for the role of Artemin in the model comes from ex vivo experiments with alveolar macrophages, but not in the experimental model created. Blocking or activation experiments could be performed, along with investigating the change in the total number of nociceptors with Artemin exposure. Similarly, the downstream effects of the potential Artemin-mediated JNC stimulation should be explored in the context of this experimental setup. A detailed dissection of the mechanisms is important. Additionally, it is also important to discuss the hypothesis leading to the selection of Artemin as a target, which currently seems arbitrary.

      Our data show that exogenous i) OVA-FPM exposed AM secrete Artemin and that ii) recombinant Artemin can sensitize nociceptors, potentially heightening the inflammatory response. As suggested, we agree that more upstream and downstream evidence is needed for definitive mechanistic insight. In response, we have expanded our experiments to include intravital microscopy, which demonstrates impaired motility of alveolar macrophages and neutrophils in nociceptor-ablated mice, suggesting a bidirectional crosstalk between AMs and nociceptor neurons.  

      In future studies, we will perform blocking or activation studies to clarify Artemin’s in vivo effects and confirm its role in modulating airway nociceptors. We also recognize the importance of examining whether Artemin exposure alters the phenotype of these neurons and lung innervation density. As recommended, we plan targeted interventions (e.g., Artemin-neutralizing antibodies or overexpression strategies) to delineate the mechanisms by which Artemin-mediated nociceptor stimulation influences the local inflammatory environment.

      We have expanded our discussion to clarify that Artemin is a recognized growth factor known to sensitize certain sensory neurons, including those responsive to tissue injury and inflammation. This literature-based rationale guided our hypothesis that Artemin might increase nociceptor reactivity in the lung and thereby influence alveolar macrophage function. By combining ex vivo and intravital approaches, we have begun to map these interactions but agree that further in vivo studies are necessary to confirm causality, dissect signal transduction pathways, and fully validate Artemin’s contributions to AM–nociceptor crosstalk. We have revised our manuscript accordingly to highlight these limitations.

      A deeper exploration of the inflammatory parameters could be performed. The multiplex analysis of the cytokine analysis shows a reduction in certain cytokines like IL-6 and MCP (figure 3F), which needs to be discussed. Additionally, investigating the change in proportions of the different immune cell populations is important, which currently restricts the eosinophil and neutrophil counts in the BAL. This is also important as the study builds on work from Prof. Chang's group, which also identified the expansion of an invariant iNKT cell population by FPM, regulatory in nature. Adding data on airway hyperresponsiveness, if possible, would be a welcome addition, considering Asthma as the disease context.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the need for a more comprehensive exploration of inflammatory parameters. To address these concerns:

      (1) Cytokine Analysis: We re-ran all statistical analyses, including the CBA and ELISA assays, and confirmed that TNFα and Artemin are the only differentially expressed cytokines across experimental groups. We have expanded the Discussion to emphasize TNFα’s role in this context.

      (2) Immune Cell Profiling in BALF: Our data show that co-exposure with FPM exacerbates CD45+ cells, eosinophil, neutrophil, T-cells and monocyte infiltration. Notably, CD45+ cells and neutrophils were the only population reduced under nociceptor neuron loss-of-function conditions (QX314–treated or TRPV1-DTA mice, Author response image 1).

      Of note, we also confirmed these data using intravital imaging and in a second line of nociceptor ablated mice (NaV1.8DTA). We are aware of Prof. Chang’s work suggesting expansion of an invariant iNKT cell population this population in future

      (3) Airway Hyperresponsiveness (AHR): We recognize that adding AHR data would strengthen the asthma-related context. Unfortunately, we are not currently equipped to perform AHR measurements, but we intend to include this in future experiments to provide a more complete assessment of airway function.

      Author response image 1.

      The authors could revisit the data presented in terms of visualization. For instance, the pooled data presented in some of the figures is probably leading to a wide variation which makes interpretation more difficult. Presenting data separately for each experimental replicate might help the reader. This is also important considering the possible variation seen between experiments (for instance, in Figure 3A and 3C and 3B and 3D, the neutrophil and eosinophil panels for the same groups seem to have an almost 2-fold difference.). Similarly, in the cytokine analysis, the authors have used a common axis for depicting all cytokine values which leads to difficulties in interpretation (Figure 3F). Analysis of the RNA seq results and the DEGs could be revisited to include pathway analysis etc (Figure 2), and the supplementary information could include detailed lists of the major target genes.

      To address this query, we have completely reformatted all graphs and included both gene lists and lists of enriched pathways for all three comparisons in Supplementary Table 1. We also confirmed our flow cytometry analysis functionally by performing intravital imaging.

      The authors should also consider citing the previous experimental setup used for some particular protocols. For instance, the use of the specified protocol for OVA in a C57 background needs to be justified, as there are various protocols reported in the literature. Additionally, doses used in some experiments seem arbitrary (The FPM and Artemin exposure in Figure 4). Depicting the dose-response curve or citing previous literature for the same would be important. Similarly, different sample sizes seen in experiments should be explained, whether they are due to mortality, failure to exhibit phenotypes, or due to technical failures. The RNA seq experiment mentions only 2 biological replicates in one of the groups which should be addressed either by increasing the sample size or by replicating the experiment. Moreover, nested comparisons in experiments performed for Figure 1 need to be performed. Neurons isolated from each mouse should be maintained and analysed separately to retain biological replicates to better represent the heterogeneity.

      We appreciate the request for clarity regarding the experimental protocols and sample sizes:

      OVA Model in C57BL/6 Mice: We adapted a previously published OVA protocol in C57BL/6 mice[3-5] (PMID: 39661516), which uses two doses of sensitization to compensate for the lower Th2 response compared to BALB/c[6]. We increased the dose of OVA (100 µg) because our initial experiments produced low eosinophil infiltration. Although this dosage is on the higher side, some studies have noted local IFNγ induction in C57BL/6 mice; however, we did not detect IFNγ in our setup.

      FPM and Artemin Doses: We did not perform a full dose-response assay for FPM and Artemin but used 100 ng/mL as reported in prior literature, where TRPA1 and TRPV1 mRNA were upregulated after 18 hours of incubation[7]. This reference has been added for clarity.

      Sample Sizes and Exclusions: One control mouse was excluded from the RNA-seq experiment because a parallel PCA analysis indicated it was an outlier. This was the only exclusion in the study, and this have been indicated in the method section of the article.  

      Nested Comparisons and Biological Replicates: We reanalyzed the relevant data with a nested one-way ANOVA and updated the figures accordingly. Neurons isolated from each mouse were first averaged to preserve biological replicates and capture potential heterogeneity; and data was analysed on the per mouse averages.

      The manuscript should be more detailed regarding the statistics employed. Currently, there is a section mentioned in the methods section, but details of corrections employed and specific stats for specific experiments should be described. There are also some minor grammatical errors and incomplete sentences in the manuscript which should be corrected. The authors should also consider a more expansive literature review in the introduction/discussion sections.

      We have updated the figure legends and methods to include more detailed information on the specific statistical tests used for each experiment. In addition, we have fixed minor grammatical errors and incomplete sentences throughout the manuscript. Finally, we have expanded our Introduction and Discussion to include additional references and a broader literature context.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The authors sought to investigate the role of nociceptor neurons in the pathogenesis of pollutionmediated neutrophilic asthma.

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable comments, which have significantly enhanced the quality of our manuscript. A point-by-point rebuttal is provided below.

      Strength

      The authors utilize TRPV1 ablated mice to confirm effects of intranasally administered QX-314 utilized to block sodium currents. The authors demonstrate that via artemin, which is upregulated in alveolar macrophages in response to pollution, sensitizes JNC neurons thereby increasing their responsiveness to pollution. Ablation or inactivity of nociceptor neurons prevented the pollution induced increase in inflammation.

      Weakness

      While neutrophilic, the model used doesn't appear to truly recapitulate a Th2/Th17 phenotype.  No IL-17A is visible/evident in the BALF fluid within the model. (Figure 3F). Unclear of the relevance of the RNAseq dataset, none of the identified DEGs were evaluated in the context of mechanism. The authors overall achieved the aim of demonstrating that nociceptor neurons are important to the pathogenesis of pollutionexacerbated asthma. Their results support their conclusions overall, although there are ways the study findings can be strengthened. This work further evaluates how nociceptor neurons contribute to asthma pathogenesis important for consideration while proposing treatment strategies for undertreated asthma endotypes.

      Major

      Utilizing a different model, one using house dust mite or alternaria alternata or similar that is able to induce a true Th2/th17 type response that is also more translatable to humans for confirmation.

      We appreciate the suggestion to use additional allergen models. In a pilot study, we did observe increased Artemin in the BALF of house dust mite–treated mice, although the levels were low under our current dosing schedule (20 µg/dose daily from Day 0–4 and Day 7–9, with sacrifice on Day 10; Auhtor response image 2). Conversely, using an Alternaria alternata model at 100 µg/dose daily from Day 0–2 (sacrificed on Day 3) did not yield a detectable increase in Artemin. We suspect these findings may reflect the specific dose and timing used. We plan to refine our protocols (e.g., longer exposures or higher doses) for HDM and/or Alternaria to better model a Th2/Th17 response and further validate our observations in a setting more translatable to human asthma.

      Author response image 2.

      Additional analysis, maybe pathway analysis on the RNAseq dataset presented in Figure 2. Unclear how these genes are relevant/how they affect functionality. At present it is acceptable to say they are transcriptionally reprogramed, but no protein evaluation is provided which would get more at function, however, the authors do show some functional data in Figure 1, so maybe this could somehow be discussed/related to Figure 2.

      We have expanded our RNA-seq analysis to include gene lists and enriched pathways for all three comparisons in Supplementary Table 1. We have also revised our discussion to align these transcriptomic changes with the functional data shown in Figure 1. While we have not yet performed protein-level validation for all identified genes, the patterns observed in our RNA-seq dataset suggest pathways potentially tied to nociceptor activation and the downstream inflammatory response. We plan to conduct targeted protein analyses in future studies to further substantiate these findings.

      Histology and localization of neutrophils/nociceptor neurons/alveolar macrophages would enhance the study findings.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to include histological data showing the distribution of neutrophils, nociceptor neurons, and alveolar macrophages. While we have not yet performed detailed histological staining of these cell types, we have added live in-vivo intravital microscopy data (Figure 4) that illustrate impaired AM and neutrophil motility in nociceptor-ablated mice. We plan to include additional histological analyses in future studies to further localize these cells in the lung tissue.

      Minor:

      The first 3 figures are small and hard to read.

      We have enlarged Figures 1 and 3 in the revised manuscript to improve readability. We have also added the corresponding gene lists and enriched pathways to Supplementary Table 1 for clarity.

      The figures are mislabeled in the text. Figure 2 is discussed twice in two different contexts; the second mention is supposed to be labeled as Figure 2.

      We corrected the mislabeled figures in the text, ensuring that each figure is referenced accurately.

      Figure 4 isn't cited in the text. I think it is supposed to be referenced in the paragraph before the discussion starts and is currently labeled as Figure 1.

      We have updated the text to properly cite Figure 4 in the relevant paragraph before the Discussion begins, rather than labeling it as Figure 1.

      Notating which statistical analysis was used with each figure/subfigure would be beneficial. Also, it's important to notate if the data was analyzed for multiple comparisons.

      We have revised each figure/subfigure legend to specify the statistical tests used, including information on whether corrections for multiple comparisons were applied. This provides a clearer understanding of how each dataset was analyzed.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Asthma is a complex disease that includes endogenous epithelial, immune, and neural components that respond awkwardly to environmental stimuli. Small airborne particles with diameters in the range of 2.5 micrometers or less, so-called PM2.5, are generally thought to contribute to some forms of asthma. These forms of asthma may have increased numbers of neutrophils and/or eosinophils present in bronchoalveolar lavage fluid and are difficult to treat effectively as they tend to be poorly responsive to steroids. Here, Wang and colleagues build on a recent model that incorporated PM2.5 which was found to have a neutrophilic component. Wang altered the model to provide an extra kick via the incorporation of ovalbumin. Building on their prior expertise linking nociceptors and inflammation, they find that silencing TRPV1-expressing neurons either pharmacologically or genetically, abrogated inflammation and the accumulation of neutrophils. By examining bronchoalveolar lavage fluid, they found not only that levels of the number of cytokines were increased, but also that artemin, a protein that supports neuronal development and function, was elevated, which did not occur in nociceptor-ablated mice. They also found that alveolar macrophages exposed to PM2.5 particles had increased artemin transcription, suggesting a further link between pollutants, and immune and neural interactions.

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable comments, which have significantly enhanced the quality of our manuscript. A point-by-point rebuttal is provided below.

      Weakness

      There are substantial caveats that must be attached to the suggestions by the authors that targeting nociceptors might provide an approach to the treatment of neutrophilic airway inflammation in pollutiondriven asthma in general and wildfire-associated respiratory problems in particular.  

      These caveats include the uncertainty of the relevance of the conventional source of PM2.5, to pollution and asthma. According to the National Institute of Standards and Technology (NIST), the standard reference material (SRM) 2786 is a mix obtained from an air intake system in the Czech Republic. It is not clear exactly what is in the mix, and a recent bioRxiv preprint, https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2023.08.18.553903v3.full.pdf reveals the presence of endotoxin. Care should thus be taken in interpreting data using particulate matter. Regarding wildfires, there is data that indicates that such exposure is toxic to macrophages. What impact might that then have on the production of cytokines, and artemin, in humans?

      We recognize the potential limitations of using SRM2786 (obtained from a Czech air-intake system) as a model for realworld PM2.5 exposure. Our rationale for choosing SRM2786 is that it is commercially available and represents a broad spectrum of ambient air pollutants, in contrast to more specialized sources like diesel exhaust particles. However, we acknowledge in the discussion the presence of endotoxin in SRM2786, as suggested by recent reports, and agree that this may influence immune responses and should be considered when interpreting our data.

      Regarding wildfire-associated exposure, we are aware that certain components of wildfire smoke can be toxic to macrophages. We do not think this play a significant role in the current study design as number of AMs, as determined by flow cytometry and intravital microscopy, are similar when comparing OVA-exposed mice to OVA-FPM exposed animals. Thus, these results rule out significant AM toxicity by FPM.

      Ultimately, while our findings suggest that modulating nociceptor activity may reduce neutrophilic inflammation, we emphasize that additional research—including different PM2.5 sources, validation of endotoxin levels, and in vivo confirmation in human-relevant models—is necessary before drawing definitive conclusions about treating pollutiondriven asthma or wildfire-induced respiratory problems.

      The Introductory paragraph implies links between wildfire events, particular exposure, and neutrophilic asthma. I am not aware of such a link having been established, in which case the paragraph needs revision. In the paragraph that begins with 'Urban pollution', it is suggested that eosinophilic asthma is treatment responsive in comparison to the neutrophilic form. That may not be the case, and they may often these cellular components may occur together. In much of the manuscript, there is a mismatch between the text and the figure numbers. For example, in the Results, Figure 2 should be Figure 3 some of the time, and Figure 3 is actually Figure 4, while the reference to Figure 1F-H is Figure 4H. Please check carefully.

      (a) Introduction Paragraph and Wildfire–Neutrophilic Asthma Link

      We add references to the introduction to support the link between wildfire, respiratory symptoms and the link to neutrophilic asthma [8-12].

      (b) Distinction Between Eosinophilic and Neutrophilic Asthma

      We recognize that eosinophilic and neutrophilic airway infiltrates can co-occur in the same individual and that treatment responsiveness can vary considerably. Our intention was to note that conventional asthma therapies (e.g., inhaled corticosteroids) are generally more effective for eosinophilic-driven disease than for neutrophilic phenotypes, but we agree that these inflammatory endotypes often overlap in clinical practice. We have revised the text in the “Urban pollution” section to acknowledge this complexity and to clarify that inflammatory cell populations in asthma are not always discrete.

      Figure Numbering and Text–Figure Mismatch

      We sincerely apologize for the confusion caused by mismatched figure labels and references in the Results section. We have carefully reviewed and corrected all figure references throughout the manuscript to ensure accuracy.

      References

      (1) Kim, S. H. et al. Mapping of the Sensory Innervation of the Mouse Lung by Specific Vagal and Dorsal Root Ganglion Neuronal Subsets. eNeuro 9 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0026-22.2022

      (2) McGovern, A. E. et al. Evidence for multiple sensory circuits in the brain arising from the respiratory system: an anterograde viral tract tracing study in rodents. Brain Struct Funct 220, 3683-3699 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-014-0883-9

      (3) Shen, C.-C., Wang, C.-C., Liao, M.-H. & Jan, T.-R. A single exposure to iron oxide nanoparticles attenuates antigen-specific antibody production and T-cell reactivity in ovalbumin-sensitized BALB/c mice. International journal of nanomedicine, 1229-1235 (2011).  

      (4) Delayre-Orthez, C., De Blay, F., Frossard, N. & Pons, F. Dose-dependent effects of endotoxins on allergen sensitization and challenge in the mouse. Clinical & Experimental Allergy 34, 1789-1795 (2004).  

      (5) Morokata, T., Ishikawa, J. & Yamada, T. Antigen dose defines T helper 1 and T helper 2 responses in the lungs of C57BL/6 and BALB/c mice independently of splenic responses. Immunology letters 72, 119-126 (2000).  

      (6) Li, L., Hua, L., He, Y. & Bao, Y. Differential effects of formaldehyde exposure on airway inflammation and bronchial hyperresponsiveness in BALB/c and C57BL/6 mice. PLoS One 12, e0179231 (2017).  

      (7) Ikeda-Miyagawa, Y. et al. Peripherally increased artemin is a key regulator of TRPA1/V1 expression in primary afferent neurons. Molecular pain 11, s12990-12015-10004-12997 (2015).  

      (8) Baan, E. J. et al. Characterization of Asthma by Age of Onset: A Multi-Database Cohort Study. J Allergy Clin Immunol Pract 10, 1825-1834 e1828 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaip.2022.03.019

      (9) de Nijs, S. B., Venekamp, L. N. & Bel, E. H. Adult-onset asthma: is it really different? Eur Respir Rev 22, 44-52 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1183/09059180.00007112

      (10) Gianniou, N. et al. Acute effects of smoke exposure on airway and systemic inflammation in forest firefighters. J Asthma Allergy 11, 81-88 (2018). https://doi.org/10.2147/JAA.S136417

      (11) Noah, T. L., Worden, C. P., Rebuli, M. E. & Jaspers, I. The Effects of Wildfire Smoke on Asthma and Allergy. Curr Allergy Asthma Rep 23, 375-387 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11882-023-01090-1

      (12) Wilgus, M. L. & Merchant, M. Clearing the Air: Understanding the Impact of Wildfire Smoke on Asthma and COPD. Healthcare (Basel) 12 (2024). https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare12030307

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Comment 1: The authors need to do more to cite the prior work of others. CCL2 allelic expression imbalance tied to the rs13900 alleles was first reported by Johnson et al. (Pharmacogenet Genomics. 2008 Sep; 18(9): 781-791) and should be cited in the Introduction on line 128 next to the Pham 2012 reference. Also, in the Results section, line 142, please provide references for the statement "We and others have previously reported a perfect linkage disequilibrium between rs1024611 in the CCL2 cis-regulatory region and rs13900 in its 3′ UTR" since the linkage disequilibrium for these 2 SNPs is not reported in the ENSEMBL server for the 1000 genomes dataset. #

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the omission regarding the citation of prior work. We acknowledge that Johnson et al. (2008) reported the association between rs13900 and CCL2 allelic expression imbalance based on Snapshot methodology while examining _cis-_acting variants of 42 candidate genes. To acknowledge these prior studies, we have cited the previous works of Johnson et al. (Johnson et al., 2008) along with Pham et al. (Pham et al., 2012) that linked rs13900 to CCL2 allelic expression imbalance. The text in the introduction section (Lines 128-130) has been updated to reflect the above-mentioned changes.

      “We and others have demonstrated AEI in CCL2 using rs13900 as a marker with the T allele showing a higher expression level relative to C allele (Johnson et al., 2008; Pham et al., 2012).”

      We have cited some previous studies that suggested strong linkage disequilibrium between rs1024611 and rs13900 within CCL2 gene, with D’=1 and R<sup>2</sup>=0.96 (Hubal et al., 2010; Intemann et al., 2011; Kasztelewicz et al., 2017; Pham et al., 2012) on Line 144. To address the concern regarding unreported linkage disequilibrium between rs1024611 and rs13900, we reviewed the pairwise linkage disequilibrium data by population in the ENSEMBL server for 1000 Genome dataset and confirm that the linkage disequilibrium (LD) between rs1024611 and rs13900 has been observed, with D’=1 and R<sup>2</sup>=0.92 to 1.0 in specific populations. We have included a table (Author response table 1) depicting pairwise LD between rs13900 and rs1024611 as reported in the ENSEMBL server for the 1000 genome dataset, a URL reference to the ENSEMBL server data.

      Author response table 1.

      Pairwise linkage disequilibrium data between rs13900 and rs1024611 by population reported in the ENSEMBL server for the 1000 genome dataset

      F. Variant, Focus Variant; R<sup>2</sup>, correlation between the pair loci; D’, difference between the observed and expected frequency of a given haplotype.

      URL: https://www.ensembl.org/Homo_sapiens/Variation/HighLD?db=core;r=17:34252269-34253269;v=rs1024611;vdb=variation;vf=959559590;second_variant_name=rs13900

      Comment 2: Certain details of the experimental protocols need to be further elaborated or clarified to contextualize the significance of the findings. For example, in the results line 184 the authors state "Using nascent RNA allows accurate determination of mRNA decay by eliminating the effects of preexisting mRNA." How does measuring nascent RNA enable the accurate determination of mRNA decay? Doesn't it measure allele-specific mRNA synthesis? Please elaborate, as this is a key result of the study. Can the authors provide a reference supporting this statement?

      It is worthwhile to mention that mRNA decay can be precisely measured by eliminating the effect of any preexisting mRNA. Metabolic labeling with 4-thiouridine allows exclusive capture of newly synthesized RNA which will allow quantification of RNA decay eliminating any interference from preexisting RNA. We agree that nascent RNA measurement primarily reflects synthesis rate rather than degradation. However, in conjugation with actinomycin-D based inhibition studies it can be exploited for accurate mRNA decay determination of the newly synthesized RNA (Russo et al., 2017). Therefore, our aim was to use the nascent RNA to study decay kinetics. The imbalance in the CCL2 allele expression does occur at the transcriptional level as seen in non-actinomycin-D treatment group (Figure 2C) although the impact of post-transcriptional mechanisms that alter transcripts stability cannot be ruled out. Therefore, we employed a novel approach that could assess both the synthesis and the degradation by combining actinomycin-D inhibition and nascent RNA capture in the same experimental setup. In the presence of actinomycin-D, we could detect much greater allelic difference in the expression levels of the rs13900T and C allele four-hour post-treatment, suggesting a role for post-transcriptional mechanisms in CCL2 AEI.

      “We have expanded the method section in the revised draft to include experimental details on capture of nascent RNA and subsequent downstream analysis” (Lines 553-563).

      Newly synthesized RNA was isolated using the Click-It Nascent RNA Capture Kit (Invitrogen, Cat No: C10365) following the manufacturer’s protocol. Peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) or monocyte-derived macrophages (MDMs) obtained from heterozygous individuals were stimulated with lipopolysaccharide (LPS) for 3 hours in presence of 0.2 mM 5-ethynyl uridine (EU) (Jao and Salic, 2008; Paulsen et al., 2013). After the pulse, the culture medium was replaced with fresh growth medium devoid of EU. To assess RNA stability, actinomycin-D (5 µg/mL) was added, and samples were collected at 0, 1, 2, and 4 h post-treatment. The EU RNA was subjected to a click reaction that adds a biotin handle which was then captured by streptavidin beads. The captured RNA was used for cDNA synthesis (Superscript Vilo kit, Cat No: 11754250), PCR amplification, and allelic quantification.”

      Comment 3: Also, they next state that the assay was carried out using cells treated with actinomycin D (line 186). Doesn't actinomycin D block transcription? The original study by Jia et al 2008 in PNAS reported that low concentration of ActD (100 nM) blocked RNA pol I and higher concentration (2 uM) blocked RNA pol II. This or the study on which the InVitrogen kit is based should be cited. The concentration of actinomycin D used to treat the cells should be given. They report that the T allele transcript was more abundant than the C allele transcript in nascent RNA. Why doesn't that argue for a transcriptional mechanism rather than an RNA-stability mechanism? This result should be discussed in the Discussion.

      In our study, we used a concentration of 5 µg/mL (3.98 µM), which as noted by the reviewer can effectively inhibit RNA polymerase II (Pl II) activity. We have updated our manuscript to include details and cited the original work of (Jao and Salic, 2008; Paulsen et al., 2013), which thoroughly investigate the effect of various concentrations of ActD on RNA polymerase I and II (Line no 557). A discussion of the RNA stability mechanism is provided in the Result section (Lines 196-198).

      Comment 4: In their bioinformatics analysis of the allele-specific CCL2 mRNAs, they reported that the analysis obtained a score of 1e (line 214). What does that mean? Is it significant?

      We acknowledge that the notation “a score of 1e” was unclear and thank the reviewer for pointing it out. We have clarified its significance in the revised manuscript. The following text has been included in the result section (Line no 223)

      “The score of 1e was obtained using RBP-Var, a bioinformatics tool that scores variants involved in posttranscriptional interaction and regulation (Mao et al., 2016). Here, the annotation system rates the functional confidence of variants from category 1 to 6. While Category 1 is the most significant category and includes variants that are known to be expression quantitative trait loci (eQTLs), likely affecting RBP binding site, RNA secondary structure and expression, category 6 is assigned to minimal possibility to affect RBP binding. Additionally, subcategories provide further annotation ranging from the most informational variants (a) to the least informational variant (e). Reported 1e denotes that the variant has a motif for RBP binding. Although the employed scoring system is hierarchical from 1a to 1e, with decreasing confidence in the variant’s function. However, all the variants in category 1 are considered potentially functional to some degree.”

      Comment 5: In Figure 3A, why is the rare SNP rs181021073 shown? This SNP does not comeup anywhere else in the paper. For clarity, it should be removed from Figure 3A.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the error in Figure 3A and apologize for the oversight. We agree that the SNP rs1810210732 is not mentioned anywhere in the manuscript and its inclusion in Figure 3A may have caused confusion. We have removed this SNP from the revised figure.

      Comment 6: For the RNA EMSA results presented in Fig. 4C with recombinant ELAVL1 (HuR), there is clearly a loss of unbound T allele probe with increasing concentrations of the recombinant protein (without a concomitant increase in shifted complex). This suggests that the T allele probe is degraded or loses its fluorescent tag in the presence of recombinant HuR, whereas the C allele probe does not. The quantitation of the shifted complex presented in Fig. 4D as a percentage of bound and unbound probe is therefore artificially elevated for the T allele compared to the C allele. In fact, there seems to be little difference between the shifted complexes with the T and C allele probes. The authors should explain this difference in free probe levels.

      We appreciate the constructive critique of the reviewer regarding the RNA EMSA results in Fig. 4C. To address this, we repeated the experiments to analyze the differential binding of rs13900T/C allele bearing probes with increasing concentration of the recombinant HuR. No degradation/ loss of fluorescence tag for T allele was noted in presence of recombinant HuR in three independent experiments (Author response image 1). This indicates that both the probes with C or T allele show comparable stability and are not affected by increasing concentration of recombinant HuR. The apparent reduction in the unbound T allele probe in Figure 4C may be due to saturation at higher HuR concentration rather than degradation.

      Author response image 1.

      Differential binding and stability of oligoribonucleotide probes containing rs13900C or T alleles with recombinant HuR. (A) REMSA with labeled oligoribonucleotides containing either rs13900C or rs13900T and recombinant HuR at indicated concentrations. (B&C) Representative quantitative densitometric analysis of HuR binding to the oligoribonucleotides bearing rs13900 T or C. The signal in the bound fractions were normalized with the free probe. The figure represents data from three independent experiments (mean ± SEM).

      Comment 7: In the Methods section, concentrations and source of reagents should be given. For example, what was the bacterial origin of LPS and concentration? What concentration of actinomycin D? What was the source? Was it provided with the nascent RNA kit? In describing the riboprobes used for REMSA, please underline the allele in the sequences (lines 549 and 550).

      Thank you for your detailed feedback and suggestions regarding the Materials and Methods Section. We regret the oversight in providing detailed information on reagent concentrations and sources in the method section. We have now rectified this omission and have provided the necessary details and a summary of material/reagents used is presented as a supplementary table (Supplementary Table 4) to enable others to replicate our experiments accurately. Regarding the description of riboprobes for RNA Electrophoretic Mobility Shift Assay, we underlined and bold the allele in the sequences as suggested (Lines 603-604).

      Comment 8: For polysome profiling on line 603, please provide a protocol for the differentiation of primary macrophages from monocytes (please cite an original protocol, not a prior paper that does not give a detailed protocol).

      We agree with the reviewer’s comment and have included the following text for primary macrophage differentiation from monocytes in the method section cited the original protocol (Line 668).

      “Human monocytes were isolated from fresh blood as described earlier (Gavrilin et al., 2009) with slight modification. Briefly, peripheral blood mononuclear cells were isolated by density gradient centrifugation using Histopaque, followed by immunomagnetic negative selection using EasySep Human Monocyte isolation kit. A high purity level for CD14+ cells was consistently achieved (≥90%) through this procedure, as confirmed by flowcytometry. The purified monocytes were immediately used for macrophage differentiation by treating them with 50 ng/mL M-CSF (PeproTech) for 72 h and flow cytometric measurement of surface markers CD64+,

      CD206+, CD44 was used to confirm the differentiation”. This data is now shown in the new Supplementary Figure S6.

      Comment 9: In the legend of Figure 2, please replace "5 ug of actinomycin D" with the actual concentration used.

      We appreciate your attention to detail and thank you for pointing out the error in the legend of Figure 2. We regret the oversight and have made the suggested change (Line 739).

      Comment 10: In the Discussion, the authors cite the study of CCL2 mRNA stabilization by HuR in mice by Sasaki et al (lines 407-9). Is regulation of CCL2 mRNA by HuR in the mouse relevant to human studies?

      How conserved is the 3'UTR of mouse and human CCL2? Is the rs13900 variant located in a conserved region? How many putative HuR sites are found in the 3'UTR of human and mouse CCL2 3'UTR? Does HuR dimerize (see Pabis et al 2019, NAR)? This information could be added to the Discussion.

      Thank you for your valuable comment. We appreciate your suggestion to include information on the dimerization of HuR in our discussion. While reporting the overall structure and domain arrangement of HuR, Pabis et al. (2019) deciphered dimerization involving Trp261 in RRM3 as key requirement for functional activity of HuR in vitro. This finding provides additional context for understanding HuR’s role in regulating CCL2 expression. We have added the following few lines in the discussion (Lines 421-428) acknowledging HuR’s ability to dimerize and cite the relevant references.

      “HuR consists of three RNA recognition motifs (RRMs) that are highly conserved and canonical in nature (Ripin et al., 2019). In absence of RNA the three RRMs are flexibly linked but upon RNA binding they transition to a more compact arrangement. Mutational analysis revealed that HuR function is inseparably linked to RRM3 dimerization and RNA binding. Dimerization enables recognition of tandem AREs by dimeric HuR (Pabis et al., 2019) and explains how this protein family can regulate numerous targets found in pre-mRNAs, mature mRNAs, miRNAs and long noncoding RNAs.”

      We aligned the CCL2 3’UTR from five different mammalian species and found that the region flanking rs13900/ HuR binding site is relatively conserved (Author response image 2). Based on PAR-CLIP datasets there are four HuR binding regions in human CCL2 3’ UTR (Lebedeva et al., 2011). However, the region overlapping rs13900 seems to be predominantly involved in the CCL2 regulation (Fan et al., 2011). This information has been included in the discussion.

      Author response image 2.

      Cross-species alignment of the CCL2 3’UTR region flanking the rs13900 using homologous regions from 5 different mammals. (Hu, Human; CH, Chimps; MO, Mouse; RA, Rat; DO, Dog, rs13900 is shown within the brackets Y, pyrimidine)

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Comment 1: The supplemental figures need appropriate figure legends.

      We regret the oversight and thank the reviewer for bringing it to our attention. We have now included the figure legend for the supplemental figures in the revised manuscript.

      Comment 2: The data on LPS-induced CCL2 expression in PBMCs should be represented as a scatter plot with statistical significance to enhance clarity and interpretability.

      We thank the reviewer for this constructive suggestion. In the revised Figure 2A the induction of CCL2 expression by LPS in PBMCs obtained from 6 volunteers is represented as a scatter plot. We have also included individual data points in the updated figure and statistical significance to improve clarity and interpretability.

      Comment 3: The stability of CCL2 mRNA in control cells needs comparison with treated cells for context. The stability of a housekeeping gene (such as GAPDH or ACTB) should always be included as a control in actinomycin D experiments. Clarify the differential stability of rs13900C vs. rs13900T alleles.

      We used 18S to normalize data for the mRNA stability studies, as it is abundant and has been recommended for such studies, as it is relatively unaltered when compared to other housekeeping genes following Act D treatment in well-controlled studies (Barta et al., 2023). We also compared Ct values between the Act D-treated samples and the Act D-untreated samples in this study and found them to be comparable (Author response image 3).

      Author response image 3.

      Ct values of 18s rRNA in ACT-D and control samples in Fig 2.

      Comment 4: In the main text and the methods, the authors state that nascent RNA was obtained in the presence of actinomycin D and EU. However, actinomycin D blocks the transcription of nascent RNAs, therefore the findings in Figure 2C do not reflect nascent RNA

      Please see our response to Reviewer 1 Comment 2. We would like to emphasize that to assess the differential role of the rs13900 in nascent RNA decay we integrated nascent RNA labeling and transcriptional inhibition. Briefly, PBMC from a heterozygous individual were either unstimulated or stimulated with LPS and pulsed with 5-ethynyl uridine (0.2 mM) for 3 h and the media was replaced with EU free growth medium. RNA was obtained at 0,1, 2 and 4 h following actinomycin-D treatment (5 µg/mL) to assess the stability of nascent RNA.

      Comment 5: Figure 4A is not clearly described or labeled. What are lanes 2 and 6?

      Figure 4 has now been updated to clearly describe all the lanes. Lanes 2 and 6 represent the mobility shift seen following the incubation by whole cell extracts and oligonucleotide bearing rs13900C and rs13900T probes respectively.

      Comment 6: Figure 4C and Figure 4D: the charts in Figure 4D do not seem to reflect the changes in Figure 4C. How was the mean variant calculated? How do the authors explain the different quantities in unbound/free RNA in rs13900C compared to rs13900T?

      We appreciate the constructive critique of the reviewer regarding the RNA EMSA results in Fig. 4C. To address this, we repeated the experiments to analyze the differential binding of rs13900T/C probes with increasing concentration of the recombinant HuR. No degradation/ loss of fluorescence tag in presence of HuR was noted in case of T allele (Author response image 1). This indicates that both the C and T allele probes exhibit comparable stability and are not affected by increasing the concentration of recombinant HuR. The apparent reduction in the unbound T allele probe in Figure 4C may be due to saturation due to higher HuR concentration rather than degradation. Also please note under limiting HuR concentration (50µM) there is more binding of purified HuR by the T bearing oligoribonucleotide (compare lanes 2 & 6 in Author response image 1).

      Comment 7: Figure 5A does not look like an IP. The authors should show the heavy and light chains and clarify why there is co-precipitation of beta-actin with IgG and HuR. Also, they should include input samples. Figure 5B: given that in a traditional RIP the mRNA is not cross-linked and fragmented, any region of CCL2 mRNA would be amplified, not just the 3'UTR. In other words, Figure 5B can be valuable to show the enrichment of CCL2 mRNA in general, but not the enrichment of a specific region.

      We understand the reviewer’s concern on Figure 5A and 5B. Due to sample limitations we are unable to confirm these results using heavy and light chains antibodies. However, it is important to note that co-precipitation of β-actin with IgG and HuR can be due to its non-specific binding with protein G. In a recent study non-specific precipitation by protein G or A was reported for proteins such as p53, p65 and β-actin (Zeng et al., 2022). We are including a figure provided by MBL Life Sciences as the quality check document for their RIP Assay Kit (RN 1001) that was used in our study. It is evident from Author response image 4 that even pre-clearing the lysate may not remove the ubiquitously expressed proteins such as β-actin or GAPDH and they will persist as contaminants in pull-down samples. Hence the presence of β-actin in the IgG and HuR IP fractions may be due to non-specific interactions with the agarose beads.

      Author response image 4.

      MBL RIP-Assay Kit’s Quality Check. Quality check of immunoprecipitated endogenous PTBP1 expressed in Jurkat cells. Lane 1: Jurkat (WB positive cells), Lane 2: Jurkat + normal Rabbit IgG, Lane 3: Jurkat+ anti-PTBP1.

      We agree with the reviewer’s comments that traditional RIP without cross-linking and fragmentation allows amplification of any region of CCL2 mRNA. However, the upregulation of CCL2 gene expression in α-HuR immunoprecipitated samples indirectly reflects the enrichment of CCL2 mRNA associated with HuR. Moreover, 3’-UTR targeting primers were used for amplification to examine HuR binding at this region. We believe this approach ensures that the above enrichment specifically reflects HuR association with the 3’-UTR rather than other parts of the transcript.

      Comment 8: Construct Validation in Luciferase Assays (Figure 6): The authors need to confirm equal transfection amounts of constructs and show changes in luciferase mRNA levels. It would be better to use a dual luciferase construct for internal normalization.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for his concern and comments related to the luciferase reporter assay. As mentioned in the Methods equal transfection amount (0.5 µg) were used in our study (Line 658). We chose to normalize the reporter activity using total protein concentration instead of using a dual-reporter system to avoid crosstalk with co-transfected control plasmids. This is now included in the Materials and Method section (Lines 662-664). The optimized design of the LightSwitch Assay system used in our study allows a single assay design when a highly efficient transfection system is used (as recommended by the manufacturer). We verified the presence of the correct insert in the CCL2 Light Switch 3’UTR reporter constructs (Author response image 5). We also sequenced the vector backbone of both constructs to rule out any inadvertently added mutations.

      Author response image 5.

      Schematic of the Lightswitch 3’UTR vector. (A) Vector information. The vector contains a multiple cloning site (MCS) upstream of the Renilla Luciferase gene (RenSP). Human 3’UTR CCL2 is cloned into MCS downstream of the reporter gene and it becomes a part of a hybrid transcript that contains the luciferase coding sequence used to the UTR sequence of CCL2. Constructs containing rs13900C or rs13900T allele were generated using site-specific mutagenesis on CCL2 LightSwitch 3’UTR reporter. The constructs were validated by Sanger sequencing. (B&C) Sequence chromatograph of the constructs containing CCL2-3’UTR insert showing rs13900C and rs13900T respectively. The result confirms the fidelity of the constructs used in the reporter assay.

      Comment 9: Polysome Data Presentation: The authors should present the distribution of luciferase mRNA (rs13900T and rs13900C) in all fractions separately and include data on the translation of a control like ACTB or GAPDH.

      Since our assessment of CCL2 allele-specific enrichment in the polysome fractions from MDMs of heterozygous donors did not yield a consistent pattern for differential loading (Supplementary Table3), we used a 3’UTR reporter-based assays that estimated the impact of rs13900 T and C alleles on overall translational output (translatability). The translatability was calculated as luciferase activity normalized by luciferase mRNA levels after adjusting for protein and 18S rRNA using a previously reported method (Zhang et al., 2017). As the measurement of relative allele enrichment in polysome fractions was not included in our invitro reporter assays, it is not possible to present the distribution of luciferase mRNA in various fractions separately. Author response image 6 shows the proportion of CCL2 mRNA in different fractions corresponding to cytosolic, monosome and polysome fractions obtained from MDM lysates from heterozygous donors along with 18S rRNA quantification.

      Author response image 6.

      Determination of rs13900C/T allelic enrichment in polysome fractions and its effect on polysome loading. Polysome profile obtained by sucrose gradient centrifugation of macrophages before and after stimulation with LPS (1 µg/mL) for 3 h. (A&B) The CCL2 mRNA shifts from monosome-associated fractions to heavier polysomes following LPS stimulation, indicating increased translation efficiency. (C&D) In contrast, the distribution of 18S shows no significant shift due to LPS treatment. (mean ± SEM, n=4). The percentage of mRNA loading on polysome was calculated using ΔCT method (mean ± SEM, n=4). (E&F) CCL2 AEI measurement in polysomes of macrophages from heterozygous donors (n=2). Genomic and cDNA were subjected to Sanger sequencing and the peak height of both the alleles were used to determine the relative abundance of each allele.

      Comment 10: Please explain in detail how primary monocytes were transfected with siRNAs for more than 72 hours. Typically, primary monocytes are very hard to transfect, have a very limited lifespan in culture (around 48 hours), and show a high level of cell death upon transfection. If monocytes were differentiated from macrophages, explain in detail how it was done and provide supporting citations from the literature.

      We agree with the challenges associated with transfecting primary monocytes, including their limited lifespan in culture and susceptibility to cell death following transfection and apologize for not elaborating the method section on lentiviral transduction of primary macrophages. To overcome these limitations, we utilized monocytes undergoing differentiation into macrophages rather than fully differentiated macrophages for our experiments. Cells were transfected by slightly modifying the method described by Plaisance-Bonstaff et.al 2019 (Plaisance-Bonstaff et al., 2019). Briefly, monocytes were purified from PBMCs obtained from homozygous donors for rs13900 C or rs13900T by negative selection. Upon purification cells were resuspended in 24 well plates at a seeding density of 0.5 x10<sup>6</sup> cells per well and were further cultured in the medium supplemented with 50 ng/mL M-CSF (Fig S7 and Fig. S6). After 24 h, ready to use GFP-tagged pCMV6-HuR or CMV-null lentiviral particles (Amsbio, Cambridge, M.A) were transduced into 0.5 x10<sup>6</sup> cells in presence of polybrene (60 µg/mL) at a MOI of 1. The cells were processed for HuR and CCL2 expression 72 h after transduction after stimulation with LPS for 3 h. This data is now shown in new Supplementary Figure S7.

      Comment 11: The authors should prove the binding of HuR to the 3'UTR of CCL2 not only in vitro but also in cells. For this aim, a CLIP including RNA fragmentation followed by RT-PCR or sequencing would be more informative than a RIP. It would be helpful also to demonstrate the different binding to the 3'UTR variants (rs13900C vs. rs13900T).

      We thank the reviewer for his valuable suggestion on validating binding of HuR to the 3’UTR in cells. It is important to highlight that several independent datasets including CLIP have already demonstrated that HuR binds to the 3’UTR of CCL2 including the region spanning the rs13900 locus. We have summarized the relevant studies in a tabular form (Supplementary Table-2). We are unable to confirm these results in new experiments due to sample limitation. The already existing data and experimental evidence provided in this manuscript strongly suggest that HuR binds within the 3’UTR. Also, a previously published study (Fan et al, 2011) showed that only the first 125 bp of the CCL2 3’UTR that flanks rs13900 showed strong binding to HuR but not the CCL2 coding region or other regions of 3’UTR. This further suggests that the HuR binding to the CCL2 is localized to the 3’UTR that flanks rs13900. Please note that the primers used for amplification of the RIP material were 3’-UTR specific.

      Comment 12: To quantify nascent RNA, Figure 2C should be replaced by new experiments. To label nascent RNA, authors can perform a run on/run-off experiments only with EU, without actinomycin D. As aforementioned, ActD blocks the transcription of new RNA, therefore is not useful for studying nascent RNA.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion and would like to emphasize that while measuring the rs13900C/T allelic ratio in nascent RNA, the experimental setup included evaluating the AEI both in presence and absence of the transcriptional inhibitor actinomycin D. The data presented in Figure 2C shows that the AEI in presence of actinomycin D is amplified in comparison to non-actinomycin D treatment. This provides definitive evidence to our hypothesis that rs13900T confers greater stability to the CCL2 message. We apologize for the oversight of not mentioning non-ACT D treatment in the methods. Necessary changes have been made to the revised manuscript (Lines 553-63).

      Comment 13: The authors should also investigate the role of TIA1 as a potential RBP and explore the possibility that TIA1 may interact more with the C allele to suppress translation.

      Based on the existing studies, we highlighted the importance of RNA-binding proteins such as TIA1 and U2AF56 that may interact with CCL2 transcript (Lines 408-09). However, exploring TIA1 binding and its functional consequences are beyond the scope of the current study. We thank the reviewer for this comment and this aspect will be pursued in future studies.

      Comment 14: It would be informative if the authors included study limitations and potential clinical implications of these findings, particularly regarding therapeutic approaches targeting CCL2.

      We would like to inform the reviewer that the submitted manuscript included the limitations of our study. They were discussed at appropriate places and were not included as a separate section. For instance, Line 398 emphasizes the need for in-depth studies for association of rs13900 and canonical CCL2 transcript. The need for additional studies regarding SNP-induced structural changes in RNA and its implication for RBP accessibility was highlighted at Lines 417-419. The inconclusive results of differential loading of polysomes and the need to conduct further research on the impact of rs13900 on CCL2 translatability in primary cells (Lines 457-459). We noted at Lines 484-485 about our further studies exploring the differential binding of HuR to the other regions of CCL2 3’UTR.

      Multiple studies have indicated that functional interference of HuR as a novel therapeutic strategy, particularly in the context of cancer, inflammation, neurodegeneration, and autoimmune disorders. These approaches include inhibitors such as MS-444, KH-3, and CMLD-2 that disrupt the interaction between HuR and ARE elements or mRNAs of target genes involved in disease pathology (Chaudhary et al., 2023; Fattahi et al., 2022; Lang et al., 2017; Liu et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2019; Wei et al., 2024), offering a potential new avenue for disease treatment. Findings from our studies provide unique insights on regulation of CCL2 expression by both rs13900 and HuR. We strongly believe that the SNP rs13900 and HuR represent a new druggable target for M/M-mediated disorders such as inflammatory diseases, cancer, and cardiovascular diseases. The potential clinical implications have been discussed in the revised manuscript (Lines 487-494)

      References

      Barta, N., Ordog, N., Pantazi, V., Berzsenyi, I., Borsos, B.N., Majoros, H., Pahi, Z.G., Ujfaludi, Z., Pankotai, T., 2023. Identifying Suitable Reference Gene Candidates for Quantification of DNA Damage-Induced Cellular Responses in Human U2OS Cell Culture System. Biomolecules 13.

      Chaudhary, S., Appadurai, M.I., Maurya, S.K., Nallasamy, P., Marimuthu, S., Shah, A., Atri, P., Ramakanth, C.V., Lele, S.M., Seshacharyulu, P., Ponnusamy, M.P., Nasser, M.W., Ganti, A.K., Batra, S.K., Lakshmanan, I., 2023. MUC16 promotes triple-negative breast cancer lung metastasis by modulating RNA-binding protein ELAVL1/HUR. Breast Cancer Res 25, 25.

      Fan, J., Ishmael, F.T., Fang, X., Myers, A., Cheadle, C., Huang, S.K., Atasoy, U., Gorospe, M., Stellato, C., 2011. Chemokine transcripts as targets of the RNA-binding protein HuR in human airway epithelium. J Immunol 186, 2482-2494.

      Fattahi, F., Ellis, J.S., Sylvester, M., Bahleda, K., Hietanen, S., Correa, L., Lugogo, N.L., Atasoy, U., 2022. HuR-Targeted Inhibition Impairs Th2 Proinflammatory Responses in Asthmatic CD4(+) T Cells. J Immunol 208, 38-48.

      Hubal, M.J., Devaney, J.M., Hoffman, E.P., Zambraski, E.J., Gordish-Dressman, H., Kearns, A.K., Larkin, J.S., Adham, K., Patel, R.R., Clarkson, P.M., 2010. CCL2 and CCR2 polymorphisms are associated with markers of exercise-induced skeletal muscle damage. J Appl Physiol (1985) 108, 1651-1658.

      Intemann, C.D., Thye, T., Forster, B., Owusu-Dabo, E., Gyapong, J., Horstmann, R.D., Meyer, C.G., 2011. MCP1 haplotypes associated with protection from pulmonary tuberculosis. BMC Genet 12, 34.

      Jao, C.Y., Salic, A., 2008. Exploring RNA transcription and turnover in vivo by using click chemistry. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105, 15779-15784.

      Johnson, A.D., Zhang, Y., Papp, A.C., Pinsonneault, J.K., Lim, J.E., Saffen, D., Dai, Z., Wang, D., Sadee, W., 2008. Polymorphisms affecting gene transcription and mRNA processing in pharmacogenetic candidate genes: detection through allelic expression imbalance in human target tissues. Pharmacogenet Genomics 18, 781791.

      Kasztelewicz, B., Czech-Kowalska, J., Lipka, B., Milewska-Bobula, B., Borszewska-Kornacka, M.K., Romanska, J., Dzierzanowska-Fangrat, K., 2017. Cytokine gene polymorphism associations with congenital cytomegalovirus infection and sensorineural hearing loss. Eur J Clin Microbiol Infect Dis 36, 1811-1818. Lang, M., Berry, D., Passecker, K., Mesteri, I., Bhuju, S., Ebner, F., Sedlyarov, V., Evstatiev, R., Dammann, K., Loy, A., Kuzyk, O., Kovarik, P., Khare, V., Beibel, M., Roma, G., Meisner-Kober, N., Gasche, C., 2017. HuR Small-Molecule Inhibitor Elicits Differential Effects in Adenomatosis Polyposis and Colorectal Carcinogenesis. Cancer Res 77, 2424-2438.

      Lebedeva, S., Jens, M., Theil, K., Schwanhausser, B., Selbach, M., Landthaler, M., Rajewsky, N., 2011. Transcriptome-wide analysis of regulatory interactions of the RNA-binding protein HuR. Mol Cell 43, 340-352.

      Liu, S., Huang, Z., Tang, A., Wu, X., Aube, J., Xu, L., Xing, C., Huang, Y., 2020. Inhibition of RNA-binding protein HuR reduces glomerulosclerosis in experimental nephritis. Clin Sci (Lond) 134, 1433-1448.

      Mao, F., Xiao, L., Li, X., Liang, J., Teng, H., Cai, W., Sun, Z.S., 2016. RBP-Var: a database of functional variants involved in regulation mediated by RNA-binding proteins. Nucleic Acids Res 44, D154-163.

      Pabis, M., Popowicz, G.M., Stehle, R., Fernandez-Ramos, D., Asami, S., Warner, L., Garcia-Maurino, S.M., Schlundt, A., Martinez-Chantar, M.L., Diaz-Moreno, I., Sattler, M., 2019. HuR biological function involves RRM3-mediated dimerization and RNA binding by all three RRMs. Nucleic Acids Res 47, 1011-1029.

      Paulsen, M.T., Veloso, A., Prasad, J., Bedi, K., Ljungman, E.A., Tsan, Y.C., Chang, C.W., Tarrier, B., Washburn, J.G., Lyons, R., Robinson, D.R., Kumar-Sinha, C., Wilson, T.E., Ljungman, M., 2013. Coordinated regulation of synthesis and stability of RNA during the acute TNF-induced proinflammatory response. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 110, 2240-2245.

      Pham, M.H., Bonello, G.B., Castiblanco, J., Le, T., Sigala, J., He, W., Mummidi, S., 2012. The rs1024611 regulatory region polymorphism is associated with CCL2 allelic expression imbalance. PLoS One 7, e49498.

      Plaisance-Bonstaff, K., Faia, C., Wyczechowska, D., Jeansonne, D., Vittori, C., Peruzzi, F., 2019. Isolation, Transfection, and Culture of Primary Human Monocytes. J Vis Exp.

      Ripin, N., Boudet, J., Duszczyk, M.M., Hinniger, A., Faller, M., Krepl, M., Gadi, A., Schneider, R.J., Sponer, J., Meisner-Kober, N.C., Allain, F.H., 2019. Molecular basis for AU-rich element recognition and dimerization by the HuR C-terminal RRM. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 116, 2935-2944.

      Russo, J., Heck, A.M., Wilusz, J., Wilusz, C.J., 2017. Metabolic labeling and recovery of nascent RNA to accurately quantify mRNA stability. Methods 120, 39-48.

      Wang, J., Hjelmeland, A.B., Nabors, L.B., King, P.H., 2019. Anti-cancer effects of the HuR inhibitor, MS-444, in malignant glioma cells. Cancer Biol Ther 20, 979-988.

      Wei, L., Kim, S.H., Armaly, A.M., Aube, J., Xu, L., Wu, X., 2024. RNA-binding protein HuR inhibition induces multiple programmed cell death in breast and prostate cancer. Cell Commun Signal 22, 580.

      Zeng, X., Zeng, W.H., Zhou, J., Liu, X.M., Huang, G., Zhu, H., Xiao, S., Zeng, Y., Cao, D., 2022. Removal of nonspecific binding proteins is required in co-immunoprecipitation with nuclear proteins. Biotechniques 73, 289-296.

      Zhang, X., Chen, X., Liu, Q., Zhang, S., Hu, W., 2017. Translation repression via modulation of the cytoplasmic poly(A)-binding protein in the inflammatory response. Elife 6.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study probes the role of the NF-κB inhibitor IκBa in the regulation of pluripotency in mouse embyronic stem cells (mESCs). It follows from previous work that identified a chromatin-specific role for IκBa in the regulation of tissue stem cell differentiation. The work presented here shows that a fraction of IκBa specifically associates with chromatin in pluripotent stem cells. Using three Nfkbia-knockout lines, the authors show that IκBa ablation impairs the exit from pluripotency, with embryonic bodies (an in vitro model of mESC multi-lineage differentiation) still expressing high levels of pluripotency markers after sustained exposure to differentiation signals. The maintenance of aberrant pluripotency gene expression under differentiation conditions is accompanied by pluripotency-associated epigenetic profiles of DNA methylation and histone marks. Using elegant separation of function mutants identified in a separate study, the authors generate versions of IκBa that are either impaired in histone/chromatin binding or NF-κB binding. They show that the provision of the WT IκBa, or the NF-κB-binding mutant can rescue the changes in gene expression driven by loss of IκBa, but the chromatin-binding mutant can not. Thus the study identifies a chromatin-specific, NF-κB-independent role of IκBa as a regulator of exit from pluripotency.

      Strengths:

      The strengths of the manuscript lie in: (a) the use of several orthogonal assays to support the conclusions on the effects of exit from pluripotency; (b) the use of three independent clonal Nfkbia-KO mESC lines (lacking IκBa), which increase confidence in the conclusions; and (c) the use of separation of function mutants to determine the relative contributions of the chromatin-associated and NF-κB-associated IκBa, which would otherwise be very difficult to unpick.

      Weaknesses:

      In this reviewer's view, the term "differentiation" is used inappropriately in this manuscript. The data showing aberrant expression of pluripotency markers during embryoid body formation are supported by several lines of evidence and are convincing. However, the authors call the phenotype of Nfkbia-KO cells a "differentiation impairment" while the data on differentiation markers are not shown (beyond the fact that H3K4me1, marking poised enhancers, is reduced in genes underlying GO processes associated with differentiation and organ development). Data on differentiation marker expression from the transcriptomic and embryoid body immunofluorescent experiments, for example, should be at hand without the need to conduct many more experiments and would help to support the conclusions of the study or make them more specific. The lack of probing the differentiation versus pluripotency genes may be a missed opportunity in gaining in-depth understanding of the phenotype associated with loss of the chromatin-associated function of IκBa.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript investigates the role of IκBα in regulating mouse embryonic stem cell (ESC) pluripotency and differentiation. The authors demonstrate that IκBα knockout impairs the exit from the naïve pluripotent state during embryoid body differentiation. Through mechanistic studies using various mutants, they show that IκBα regulates ESC differentiation through chromatin-related functions, independent of the canonical NFκB pathway.

      Strengths:

      The authors nicely investigate the role of IκBα in pluripotency exit, using embryoid body formation and complementing the phenotypic analysis with a number of genome-wide approaches, including transcriptomic, histone marks deposition, and DNA methylation analyses. Moreover, they generate a first-of-its-kind mutant set that allows them to uncouple IκBα's function in chromatin regulation versus its NF-κB-related functions. This work contributes to our understanding of cellular plasticity and development, potentially interesting a broad audience including developmental biologists, chromatin biology researchers, and cell signaling experts.

      Weaknesses:

      - The study's main limitation is the lack of crucial controls using bona fide naïve cells across key experiments, including DNA methylation analysis, gene expression profiling in embryoid bodies, and histone mark deposition. This omission makes it difficult to evaluate whether the observed changes in IκBα-KO cells truly reflect naïve pluripotency characteristics.

      - Several conclusions in the manuscript require a more measured interpretation. The authors should revise their statements regarding the strength of the pluripotency exit block, the extent of hypomethylation, and the global nature of chromatin changes. - From a methodological perspective, the manuscript would benefit from additional orthogonal approaches to strengthen the knockout findings, which may be influenced by clonal expansion of ES cells.

      Overall, this study makes an important contribution to the field. However, the concerns raised regarding controls, data interpretation, and methodology should be addressed to strengthen the manuscript and support the authors' conclusions.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I have the following comments and suggestions for the authors to consider:

      (1) Fig, 1D: the number of replicates for this experiment is not mentioned. It would be good to see if the apparent accumulation of IκBa on chromatin of S/L cells is reproducible. If it is, does the accumulation of IκBa "prime" chromatin for differentiation?

      We apologize for missing this information in the figure legend. We have repeated the experiment two independent times, and confirmed the localization of IκBα in the chromatin fraction of mESCs cultured in Serum/LIF (S/L). We have included the information in the figure legend.

      Regarding the second question, we do believe that the presence of IκBα primes mESCs to exit from differentiation. Previous data from the lab (Mulero et al Cancer Cell 2012; Marruecos et al EMBO Reports 2020) demonstrated that IκBα regulates important developmental genes (Hox genes and differentiation-related genes), which become dysregulated upon IκBα depletion. Based on those previous results, together with our results that demonstrated that lack of IκBα hyperactivates the pluripotency network, we conclude that IκBα is a crucial element to attenuate pluripotency programs, allowing a successful exit from naïve pluripotency and differentiation.

      (2) Fig. 1E: From what is shown, Rela doesn't agree (i.e. no enrichment in EpiSCs in the Atlasi data). Are the culture conditions in Atlasi 2020 the same as in this paper (base medium etc.)? Also, why not label all genes/proteins that are shown in 1C?

      Differences observed between our data and the in-silico data might be due to differences in culture conditions used in Atlasi and colleagues. In particular, Atlasi et al. cultured the mESCs in 2i/LIF for 2 consecutive months, whereas we induced ground state of naïve pluripotency (2i/LIF) for only 96h. In the case of EpiSC differentiation, similar protocols are used in both our work and in Atlasi et al. Nevertheless, despite existing differences, in both studies IκBα is enriched in the ground state of naive pluripotency. 

      The reason why some proteins that are missing in Figure 1E but appearing in Figure 1C is because they are not detected in the mass spectrometry experiment.

      (3) Fig. 1F: The word "clustering" here is misleading. While Nfkbia shows similar dynamics as pluripotency genes, clustering should not be used unless clusters of genes are shown in the same heatmap (and the transcripts naturally cluster together). The figure would be even more informative if all the genes from the 4 different categories were presented on the same heatmap.

      As suggested by the reviewer, we have generated a heatmap where the  genes from the different four categories (Figure 1F) are displayed  and clustered together:

      Author response image 1.

      Heatmap including all the genes from Figure 1F of the manuscript and clustering is simultaneously conducted over the four categories.

      As shown in previous heatmap, we can confirm that most of the Nf-kB genes (except for Nfkbia and Nfkbid) clustered together with differentiation markers.   

      Nonetheless, to be more conservative with original Figure 1F and for clarity upon gene categories,  we have updated the figure  with a combined heatmap, sliced by gene categories.  In this updated version, we can observe how IkBα gene, though classified by the biological process where it classically belongs (NF-kB pathway), is higher at pluripotency, whereas it decreases upon differentiation induction, similarly as most of the pluripotency genes.

      We have also changed the text accordingly and have added the following sentences in the main text (lines 121-125): “The expression pattern of Nfkbia was similar to the pluripotency genes whereas most of the NF-κB genes were upregulated upon differentiation, showing an analogous expression dynamics as developmental genes, as previously described”.

      (4) This reviewer felt that the statement "Notably, several polycomb elements were highly expressed in mESCs, consistent with the possibility that chromatin-bound IκBα modulates PRC2 activity in the pluripotent state" (p.5, lines 125-127) is premature here. While similar expression dynamics may be consistent with a linked function, they in no way suggest this. This can be more accurately stated to point out that Nfkbia shows similar expression dynamics in pluripotency and differentiation as Polycomb component      genes.

      We agree that the statement is premature and we have changed it by: “Previous reports have demonstrated that chromatin-bound IκBα modulates PRC2 activity in different adult stem cell models [27]. Interestingly, we observed that most of the Polycomb target genes follow a similar expression pattern of Nfkbia and pluripotency, with higher expression in mESCs (Figure 1F).” (lines 125-128 in the manucript).

      (5) Top of p. 6: the results are mis-attributed to Fig. 1, it should be Fig. 2.

      We thank the reviewer for this observation. We have corrected it in the main text.

      (6) Fig. 1B and Fig. 5I: the images of the AP stains are very difficult to see, better resolution images should be used.

      We have increased both the resolution and the size of the AP colonies.

      (7) Line 142 (p.6): Fig. S1B should be S1C. In general the manuscript would benefit from review of the order and labeling of the figure panels as there are a number of inconsistencies.

      We have better organized the figures in the new version of the manuscript. In particular, we have reorganized the Figure S1 to have a more logical order. We have done the same for the Figure 2 and Figure 5 and they are updated in the new version of the reviewed manuscript.

      (8) The authors call the phenotype of Nfkbia-KO cells a "differentiation impairment". Do the EBs shown in Fig. 2 also express differentiation markers? Do they fail to up-regulate those markers or just fail to down-regulate pluripotency markers? At the transcriptomic level the Nfkbia-KO cells still change significantly upon provision of differentiation signals (Fig. 2C), what types of gene processes underlie the differences between WT and KO cells and which processes are common? Also, based on this figure, the phenotype looks to be more of a delay than a failure in differentiation, as the cells still follow the same trajectory but lag behind the WT cells. It is difficult to discern whether this is the case based on Fig. 2E-G as we don't see the later time point (up to Day 9).

      In general, with the data presented in Fig. 2C and Fig. S1, the authors show that many of the hallmarks of exit from pluripotency are impaired in Nfkbia-KO cells, as well as the general "transcriptional status" of the cells, but they don't show differentiation markers (which would be necessary to conclude an impairment in differentiation). The data should be readily available in the datasets that are in the manuscript already and it will be informative to extract and present them. The data are not currently publicly accessible (unavailable until July 2025) so it was not possible to mine them.

      We appreciate the observation, and we have included more data to confirm that the IκBα-KO cells show a differentiation impairment. In the first version of the manuscript, differentiation markers are displayed from Figures 2E-G, where genes from the three germ layers (ectoderm, mesoderm and endoderm) are not activated in IκBα-KO EBs at 48h and 96h. Moreover, the volcano plot displayed in Figure S1F of the first version clearly shows a downregulation of important differentiation genes such as a T, Eomes, Lhx1 and Foxa2. We agree that 96h EBs is an early time point to talk about differentiation impairment. For that reason, we have also included the same pluripotent and differentiation genes in 216h EBs (Figures S1F-G of the newer version of the manuscript). It is clearly observed that IκBα-KO 216h EBs maintain an upregulation of pluripotency programs which negatively correlate with a lower differentiation capability. Moreover, the impairment in the differentiation with a higher expression of pluripotency markers is confirmed by the presence of high SSEA-1 expression in IκBα-KO 216h EBs (Figure S1C of the manuscript) and alkaline phosphatase (AP) staining (Figure 2C of the manuscript). Lastly, the fact that IκBα-KO teratomas contain higher proportion of OCT3/4+ cells further confirming that IκBα-KO cells cannot differentiate because of the inability to exit from pluripotency.

      Finally, generated data (and deposited in GEO repository with SuperSeries id GSE239565) is already publicly available. 

      (9) Fig. 5A: even if there are no global changes in NF-κB target genes, could a small subset of NF-κB target genes still mediate the IκBa effects?

      We have analyzed the whole NF-κB signature, and we have identified a small cluster of genes that are differentially expressed at 96h EBs between IκBα-KO and IκBα-WT (Author response image 2). Interestingly, what we observed is the opposite as expected since we see un downregulation of that subset in the IκBα-KO 96h EBs (Author response image 3). For that reason, detected changes in the NF-κB target gene expression after deletion of Nfkbia do not support an NF-κB inhibitory role for IkBa in pluripotent ESC.

      Author response image 2.

      Heatmap of NF-κB genes expression at the different time points of differentiation (mESCs, 48h EBs, 96h EBs). Highlighted region marks the genes that are differentially expressed between both genotypes at 96h EBs.

       

      Author response image 3.

      Violin plot of genes from the NF-κB pathway which are differentially expressed at 96h EBs.

      (10) Lines 233-238, the part of the text is repeated.

      We appreciate the observation and have deleted the repeated part.

      (11) The data in Fig. 5D-E make it difficult to be sure whether the conclusions on the relative subcellular localisations of the different mutants are accurate, as the chromatin-binding mutant seems to be less abundant than the other mutants (judging from the Input in Fig. 5C and also from the tubulin loading controls in Fig. 5D-E). Showing the IκBa levels in total extracts would make the interpretation of these data more robust. The authors do mention that the chromatin-binding mutant IκBa protein is consistently expressed at lower levels but they do not comment on how this may affect the data interpretation - could the lack of rescue be due to lower levels of the chromatin-binding mutant IκBa relative to the wild-type IκBa? This should be addressed in the Discussion, if not tested formally by normalising the expression levels of the different forms of IκBa in the rescue experiments.

      Although protein stability is different among the SOF mutants, IκBα<sup>ΔChromatin</sup> is exclusively detected in the cytoplasm, with lack of detection in the chromatin compartment (Figures 5D-E of the reviewed manuscript). For this reason, we believe that the quantitative differences in protein levels of the different mutants cannot explain the subcellular localization differences and the phenotype observed.

      Nonetheless, we cannot discard that differences in the protein levels between SOF mutants can affect the rescue phenotype, and we have specified so in the discussion section of the manuscript. 

      (12) Lines 260-261: "Induction of i-IκBαWT and i-IκBαΔNF-κB reduced the expression levels of the naive pluripotent genes Zfp42, Klf2, Sox2 and Tbx3, which were increased by i-IκBαΔChromatin (Figure 5F)." This is not an accurate statement. The expression was not reduced by the ΔChrom mutant in the same way as it was by the WT and the ΔNF-κB mutant, but it was not increased.

      We have better specified the description of the results displayed in Figure 5F (lines 258-261 of the main manuscript):

      “Induction of i-IκBα<sup>WT</sup> and i-IκBα<sup>ΔNF-κB</sup> reduced the expression levels of the naïve pluripotent genes Zfp42, Klf2, Sox2 and Tbx3. On the other hand, the same genes either do not change their expression (Zfp42, Sox2, Klf2) or increase their levels (Tbx3) upon i-IκBα<sup>ΔChromatin</sup>  induction (Figure 5F).”

      (13) In Fig. 5J the images will ideally be shown before and after Doxycycline treatment, to better support the conclusions.

      We have included a new panel in Figure S4 (Figure S4E in the reviewed manuscript) where the No doxycycline control 216 EBs between the different conditions (i-IκBα<sup>WT</sup>, i-IκBα<sup>ΔChrom</sup> and i-IκBα<sup>ΔNF-κB</sup>) are included.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      - The PCA analysis in Figure 2 appears to contradict the authors' conclusions about global transcriptome changes in KO cells. Furthermore, there is a discrepancy between immunofluorescence data showing near-complete methylation loss and the methylation array analysis results.

      Although there is a differentiation block in the IkBa KO EBs, this is not complete and they show some differentiation trend after 96h (Fig 2C), moreover, acquisition of differentiation genes from all three germ layers is strongly affected (Figure 2E of the reviewed manuscript) and these programs remain downregulated and pluripotency genes are still expressed in IκBα-KO EBs at later time points (216h) (Fig 2B). Altogether demonstrates that the lack of IκBα impairs differentiation and the silencing of the pluripotency network.

      Discrepancies between methylation array and immunofluorescence are expected since immunofluorescence is not quantitative and the methylation array is very precise.  

      - The authors should revise their statements regarding the strength of the pluripotency exit block, the extent of hypomethylation, and the global nature of chromatin changes. For example, the observed chromatin changes, including H3K27ac modifications, appear relatively modest and should be described as such. - The manuscript would benefit from additional orthogonal approaches to strengthen the knockout findings, which may be influenced by clonal expansion of ES cells. Additionally, the emphasis on overlapping H3K4me3 and H3K27me3 regions should be reduced, as these represent a minor fraction of the affected regions (only 41 regions).

      We have revised the text and have included it in the discussion section the following text (lines 327-331 in the reviewed manuscript):

      “Although IκBα KO  mESCs  exhibit a transcriptional phenotype and hypomethylation state  that resembles the ground state of naïve pluripotency, there are only modest changes on histone marks associated to enhancers (H3K27Ac) or gene regulation (H3K4me3 and H3K27me3). Altogether indicates that further experiments are required to fully elucidate the effect of chromatin IκBα.”

      We have also included Fig S3E-S3F to show that similar differences as WT and KO in H3K4me3 and H3K27me3 are observed in a serum/LIF and 2i conditions, further supporting the fact that KO cells in Serum/LIF resemble WT cells in 2i condition.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors define the principles that, based on first principles, should be guiding the optimisation of trascription factors with intrinsically disordered regions (IDR). The first part of the study defines the following principles to optimize the binding affinities to the genome in the receiving region that is called the ”antenna”: (i) reduce the target to IDR-binding distance on the genome, (ii) optimise the distance betwee the DNA binding domain and the binding sites on the IDR to be as close as possible to the distance between their binding sites on the genome; (iii) keep the same number of binding sites and their targets and modulate this number with binding strength, reducing them with increased strenght; (iv) modulate the binding strenght to be above a threshold that depends on the proportion of IDR binding sites in the antenna. The second part defines the scaling of the seach time in function of key parameters such as the volume of the nucleus, and the size of the antenna, derived as a combination of 3D search of the antenna and 1D ”octopusing” on the antenna. The third part focuses on validation, where the current results are compared to binding probabilith data from a single experiment, and new experiment are proposed to further validate the model as well as testing designed transcription factors.

      Strengths:

      The strength of this work is that it provides simple, interpretable and testable theoretical conclusions. This will allow the derived design principles to be understood, evaluated and improved in the future. The theoretical derivations are rigorous. The authors provides a comparison to experiments, and also propose new experiments to be performed in the future, this is a great value in the paper since it will set the stage and inspire new experimental techniques. Further, the field needs inspiration and motivations to develop these techniques, since they are required to benchmark the transcription factors designed with the methods presented in this paper, as well as to develop novel data based or in vivo methods that would greatly benefit the field. As such, this paper is a fundamental contribution to the field.

      Weaknesses:

      The model assumption that the interaction between the transcription factor and the DNA outside of the antenna region is negligible is probably too strong for many/most transcription factors, particularly in organisms with a longer genome than yeasts. The model presents many first principles to drive the design of transcription factor, but arguably, other principles and mechanisms might also play a role by being beneficial to the search and binding process. Specifically: (i) a role of the IDR in complex formation and cooperativity between multiple trascription factors, (ii) ability of the IDR to do parallel searching based on multiple DNA binding sites spaced by disordered regions, (iii) affinity of the IDR to specific compartmentalisations in the nucleus reducing the search time, etc. The paper would be improved by a discussion over alternative mechanisms.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting that our work delivers simple, interpretable and rigorously derived conclusions, backed by experimental comparison and concrete proposals for future studies.

      Regarding interactions outside the antenna region, Supplementary S10 shows that the non-specific IDR–DNA interactions (on the order of 1 kBT) only slightly alter the 3D diffusion coefficient and thus do not affect our conclusions regarding the optimal search process.

      We have also added sentences in the discussion section regarding the alternative mechanism.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is an interesting theoretical exploration of how a flexible protein domain, which has multiple DNAbinding sites along it, affects the stability of the protein-DNA complex. It proposes a mechanism (”octopusing”) for protein doing a random walk while bound to DNA which simultaneously enables exploration of the DNA strand and stability of the bound state.

      Strengths:

      Stability of the protein-DNA bound state and the ability of the protein to perform 1d diffusion along the DNA are two properties of a transcription factor that are usually seen as being in opposition of each other. The octopusing mechanism is an elegant resolution of the puzzle of how both could be accommodated. This mechanism has interesting biological implications for the functional role of intrinsically disordered domains in transcription factor (TF) proteins. They show theoretically how these domains, if flexible and able to make multiple weak contacts with the DNA, can enhance the ability of the TF to efficiently find their binding site on the DNA from which they exert control over the transcription of their target gene. The paper concludes with a comparison of model predictions with experimental data which gives further support to the proposed model. Overall, this is an interesting and well executed theoretical paper that proposes an interesting idea about the functional role for IDR domains in TFs.

      Weaknesses:

      IDR domains are assumed flexible which I believe is not always the case. Also, I’m not sure how ubiquitous are the assumed binding sites on the DNA for multiple subdomains along the IDR. These assumptions though seem like interesting points of departure for further experiments.

      We thank the reviewer for their careful and insightful evaluation of our work. In particular, we appreciate your emphasis on the inherent trade-off between binding stability and one-dimensional diffusion, and your recognition of how the octopusing mechanism elegantly reconciles these conflicting requirements.

      To address the flexibility of TFs with IDRs, we incorporated the spring’s rest length—effectively introducing tunable rigidity—in Supplementary Section S1, and we show that our design principles for binding probability remain robust. Indeed, this is a highly interesting point; a comprehensive study will require more detailed modeling alongside experimental validation.

      We acknowledge that the current evidence for IDR-directed DNA binding is primarily derived from a limited number of well-studied cases, particularly Msn2 in yeast, and the ubiquity of this mechanism across diverse transcription factors remains to be established.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The paper jumps to fast to the results, an larger introduction might improve the paper, the current introduction jumps too fast to results. Further, line 50, I don’t think that the figure is properly referenced. The formula 2 is confusing since what is the target volume V1 is not explained in the context of the formula, please expand the explanations.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s valuable recommendations. We have expanded the Introduction, clarified V<sub>1</sub>, and updated the line 50.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I have some mostly minor suggestions to the authors for improving the manuscript:

      In the abstract and introduction on at least two occasions the authors talk about IDRs as though they’re necessarily flexible. My understanding is that, while this is a very reasonable assumption, I don’t think this is something we know with any certainty for most IDRs. If the authors agree with my assessment I think they should reflect this uncertainty in the writing.

      Thank you for the recommendations. We revised the wording to reflect the uncertainty, changing it to: “... commonly assumed to behave as a long, flexible...” and “...can be assumed as flexible....”.

      It took me a bit of time to figure out what’s going on in Figure 1b. To help the reader I would suggest labeling the DBD targets (yellow square) and the IDR targets (gray squares) as such. The figure also left me guessing whether the DBD domain can bind to the IDR targets non-specifically? (I presume not.) This also brought a slightly bigger question into focus for me, wouldn’t the presence of the IDR binding ”sites” (since these ”sites” are on the protein I think the term ”domains” instead of ”sites” ) mean that this would increase the time the protein is bound non-specifically somewhere far from the target thereby increasing the search time. Or is the ability of the protein to bind specifically to DNA away from the DBD target ignored?

      We have labeled the DBD targets and IDR targets in the figure. ‘Domains’ usually refers to structured parts; we keep using ‘sites’ and clarify that they correspond to short linear motifs.

      The reviewer is correct. Our model omits any non-specific binding between the DBD and IDR-binding targets, as well as between the TF and other DNA regions. If such interactions were to substantially lengthen the search time, they would effectively revert our mechanism to the classical bacterial facilitateddiffusion model, which is generally considered inappropriate for IDR-mediated TF search in eukaryotic cells. However, Supplementary Figure S10 demonstrates that non-specific IDR–DNA interactions induce only marginal changes in the effective three-dimensional diffusion coefficient within complex chromatin environments, and therefore do not alter our conclusions regarding the optimal search process.

      In Equation 2 and the text that follows I was left wondering what is the target volume V1. Also, I think it would be helpful to the reader to give them a sense of scale for the dimension full quantities appearing in Equation 2. This is done later when comparing the theory to experimental data, but I think it would be helpful to give a sense of size earlier in the manuscript.

      V<sub>1</sub> denotes the volume of the IDR–binding target region, which is on the order of bp<sup>3</sup>. f(d,l<sub>0</sub>) has units of inverse volume. We have included the units and specified the order of magnitude of V<sub>1</sub> after Equation 2.

      The binding energy EB is discussed a number of times but it wasn’t clear to me that this quantity referred to the energy per IDR site on the DNA or the total energy when the IDR is bound to DNA. In Figure 1 it would seem that the model allows only one IDR domain bound at a given time but I think the model allows for multiple IDR domains to be bound to the IDR target sites simultaneously. Right? Maybe make this clear in the Figure and the text.

      E<sub>B</sub> denotes the binding energy per binding site, where each site corresponds to a short linear motif. Yes, we allow for multiple IDR domains to be bound to the IDR target sites simultaneously. We have clarified the definition of E<sub>B</sub> and adjusted the figure slightly to avoid any misunderstanding.

      After Eq 4 the discussion suggests that for ϕ << 1 the threshold energy is much greater than kBT, but that’s hard to imagine given that the logarithmic dependence of the latter on the former. Also in Figure 2d it seems that the threshold energy is about 8 kBT. Clearly this is not a big deal, just thought the authors might want to revise the language.

      Thank you. We now clarify the sentence using the representative values of ϕ and E<sub>th</sub> after Equation 4.

      Right after Figure 2 there is a discussion of the different parameters that the authors vary. I suggest having a figure that illustrates these parameters (possibly in Figure 1b) to make it easier to follow the discussion.

      We have added explanations of the relevant parameters in Figure 1 for clarity.

      When discussing the dynamics of search the result stated is that the search time is minimum for a specific value of R. I think it would be useful to translate this into a TF concentration. Also, if R represents the radius of the cells nucleus 1/6 um is almost an order of magnitude smaller than the size of a typical nucleus. Is this a worry? Either way some clarification of this number would be helpful.

      Thank you for the suggestion. As noted later in this section, we have translated R into an equivalent TF concentration, and we clarify that we assume the scaling of the minimum search time remains unchanged when extrapolated to the size of a typical nucleus.

      There is a comment regarding the role of the DNA persistence length and how it was not accounted for. It would be helpful if the authors could add a sentence or two explains how a folded DNA conformation, as is the case in the nucleus, would affect their calculation. (So that the reader gets an idea without having to get into the details described in the Supplement).

      Thank you. We have revised the sentence to: “We have verified that reducing the DNA persistence length, which promotes increased DNA coiling, results in only a modest increase in mean search time. Even under extreme coiling conditions, the increase remains below 30% of the baseline value, as detailed in Supplementary S9.”.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The study analyzes the gastric fluid DNA content identified as a potential biomarker for human gastric cancer. However, the study lacks overall logicality, and several key issues require improvement and clarification. In the opinion of this reviewer, some major revisions are needed:

      (1) This manuscript lacks a comparison of gastric cancer patients' stages with PN and N+PD patients, especially T0-T2 patients.

      We are grateful for this astute remark. A comparison of gfDNA concentration among the diagnostic groups indicates a trend of increasing values as the diagnosis progresses toward malignancy. The observed values for the diagnostic groups are as follows:

      Author response table 1.

      The chart below presents the statistical analyses of the same diagnostic/tumor-stage groups (One-Way ANOVA followed by Tukey’s multiple comparison tests). It shows that gastric fluid gfDNA concentrations gradually increase with malignant progression. We observed that the initial tumor stages (T0 to T2) exhibit intermediate gfDNA levels, which in this group is significantly lower than in advanced disease (p = 0.0036), but not statistically different from non-neoplastic disease (p = 0.74).

      Author response image 1.

      (2) The comparison between gastric cancer stages seems only to reveal the difference between T3 patients and early-stage gastric cancer patients, which raises doubts about the authenticity of the previous differences between gastric cancer patients and normal patients, whether it is only due to the higher number of T3 patients.

      We appreciate the attention to detail regarding the numbers analyzed in the manuscript. Importantly, the results are meaningful because the number of subjects in each group is comparable (T0-T2, N = 65; T3, N = 91; T4, N = 63). The mean gastric fluid gfDNA values (ng/µL) increase with disease stage (T0-T2: 15.12; T3-T4: 30.75), and both are higher than the mean gfDNA values observed in non-neoplastic disease (10.81 ng/µL for N+PD and 10.10 ng/µL for PN). These subject numbers in each diagnostic group accurately reflect real-world data from a tertiary cancer center.

      (3) The prognosis evaluation is too simplistic, only considering staging factors, without taking into account other factors such as tumor pathology and the time from onset to tumor detection.

      Histopathological analyses were performed throughout the study not only for the initial diagnosis of tissue biopsies, but also for the classification of Lauren’s subtypes, tumor staging, and the assessment of the presence and extent of immune cell infiltrates. Regarding the time of disease onset, this variable is inherently unknown--by definition--at the time of a diagnostic EGD. While the prognosis definition is indeed straightforward, we believe that a simple, cost-effective, and practical approach is advantageous for patients across diverse clinical settings and is more likely to be effectively integrated into routine EGD practice.

      (4) The comparison between gfDNA and conventional pathological examination methods should be mentioned, reflecting advantages such as accuracy and patient comfort.

      We wish to reinforce that EGD, along with conventional histopathology, remains the gold standard for gastric cancer evaluation. EGD under sedation is routinely performed for diagnosis, and the collection of gastric fluids for gfDNA evaluation does not affect patient comfort. Thus, while gfDNA analysis was evidently not intended as a diagnostic EGD and biopsy replacement, it may provide added prognostic value to this exam.

      (5) There are many questions in the figures and tables. Please match the Title, Figure legends, Footnote, Alphabetic order, etc.

      We are grateful for these comments and apologize for the clerical oversight. All figures, tables, titles and figure legends have now been double-checked.

      (6) The overall logicality of the manuscript is not rigorous enough, with few discussion factors, and cannot represent the conclusions drawn.

      We assume that the unusual wording remark regarding “overall logicality” pertains to the rationale and/or reasoning of this investigational study. Our working hypothesis was that during neoplastic disease progression, tumor cells continuously proliferate and, depending on various factors, attract immune cell infiltrates. Consequently, both tumor cells and immune cells (as well as tumor-derived DNA) are released into the fluids surrounding the tumor at its various locations, including blood, urine, saliva, gastric fluids, and others. Thus, increases in DNA levels within some of these fluids have been documented and are clinically meaningful. The concurrent observation of elevated gastric fluid gfDNA levels and immune cell infiltration supports the hypothesis that increased gfDNA—which may originate not only from tumor cells but also from immune cells—could be associated with better prognosis, as suggested by this study of a large real-world patient cohort.

      In summary, we thank Reviewer #1 for his time and effort in a constructive critique of our work.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigated whether the total DNA concentration in gastric fluid (gfDNA), collected via routine esophagogastroduodenoscopy (EGD), could serve as a diagnostic and prognostic biomarker for gastric cancer. In a large patient cohort (initial n=1,056; analyzed n=941), they found that gfDNA levels were significantly higher in gastric cancer patients compared to non-cancer, gastritis, and precancerous lesion groups. Unexpectedly, higher gfDNA concentrations were also significantly associated with better survival prognosis and positively correlated with immune cell infiltration. The authors proposed that gfDNA may reflect both tumor burden and immune activity, potentially serving as a cost-effective and convenient liquid biopsy tool to assist in gastric cancer diagnosis, staging, and follow-up.

      Strengths:

      This study is supported by a robust sample size (n=941) with clear patient classification, enabling reliable statistical analysis. It employs a simple, low-threshold method for measuring total gfDNA, making it suitable for large-scale clinical use. Clinical confounders, including age, sex, BMI, gastric fluid pH, and PPI use, were systematically controlled. The findings demonstrate both diagnostic and prognostic value of gfDNA, as its concentration can help distinguish gastric cancer patients and correlates with tumor progression and survival. Additionally, preliminary mechanistic data reveal a significant association between elevated gfDNA levels and increased immune cell infiltration in tumors (p=0.001).

      Reviewer #2 has conceptually grasped the overall rationale of the study quite well, and we are grateful for their assessment and comprehensive summary of our findings.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The study has several notable weaknesses. The association between high gfDNA levels and better survival contradicts conventional expectations and raises concerns about the biological interpretation of the findings.

      We agree that this would be the case if the gfDNA was derived solely from tumor cells. However, the findings presented here suggest that a fraction of this DNA would be indeed derived from infiltrating immune cells. The precise determination of the origin of this increased gfDNA remains to be achieved in future follow-up studies, and these are planned to be evaluated soon, by applying DNA- and RNA-sequencing methodologies and deconvolution analyses.

      (2) The diagnostic performance of gfDNA alone was only moderate, and the study did not explore potential improvements through combination with established biomarkers. Methodological limitations include a lack of control for pre-analytical variables, the absence of longitudinal data, and imbalanced group sizes, which may affect the robustness and generalizability of the results.

      Reviewer #2 is correct that this investigational study was not designed to assess the diagnostic potential of gfDNA. Instead, its primary contribution is to provide useful prognostic information. In this regard, we have not yet explored combining gfDNA with other clinically well-established diagnostic biomarkers. We do acknowledge this current limitation as a logical follow-up that must be investigated in the near future.

      Moreover, we collected a substantial number of pre-analytical variables within the limitations of a study involving over 1,000 subjects. Longitudinal samples and data were not analyzed here, as our aim was to evaluate prognostic value at diagnosis. Although the groups are imbalanced, this accurately reflects the real-world population of a large endoscopy center within a dedicated cancer facility. Subjects were invited to participate and enter the study before sedation for the diagnostic EGD procedure; thus, samples were collected prospectively from all consenting individuals.

      Finally, to maintain a large, unbiased cohort, we did not attempt to balance the groups, allowing analysis of samples and data from all patients with compatible diagnoses (please see Results: Patient groups and diagnoses).

      (3) Additionally, key methodological details were insufficiently reported, and the ROC analysis lacked comprehensive performance metrics, limiting the study's clinical applicability.

      We are grateful for this useful suggestion. In the current version, each ROC curve (Supplementary Figures 1A and 1B) now includes the top 10 gfDNA thresholds, along with their corresponding sensitivity and specificity values (please see Suppl. Table 1). The thresholds are ordered from-best-to-worst based on the classic Youden’s J statistic, as follows:

      Youden Index = specificity + sensitivity – 1 [Youden WJ. Index for rating diagnostic tests. Cancer 3:32-35, 1950. PMID: 15405679]. We have made an effort to provide all the key methodological details requested, but we would be glad to add further information upon specific request.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this work, the authors apply TDCS to awake and anesthetized macaques to determine the effect of this modality on dynamic connectivity measured by fMRI. The question is to understand the extent to which TDCS can influence conscious or unconscious states. Their target was the PFC. During the conscious states, the animals were executing a fixation task. Unconsciousness was achieved by administering a constant infusion of propofol and a continuous infusion of the muscle relaxant cisatracurium. They observed the animals while awake receiving anodal or cathodal hd-TDCS applied to the PFC. During the cathodal stimulation, they found disruption of functional connectivity patterns, enhanced structure-function correlations, a decrease in Shannon entropy, and a transition towards patterns that were more commonly anatomically based. In contrast under propofol anesthesia anodal hd-TDCS stimulation appreciably altered the brain connectivity patterns and decreased the correlation between structure and function. The PFC stimulations altered patterns associated with consciousness as well as those associated with unconsciousness.

      Strengths: 

      The authors carefully executed a set of very challenging experiments that involved applying tDCS in awake and anesthetized non-human primates while conducting functional imaging.

      We thank the Reviewer for summarising our study and for his appreciation of the highly challenging experiments we performed.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors show that tDCS can alter functional connectivity measured by fMRI but they do not make clear what their studies teach the reader about the effects of tDCS on the brain during different states of consciousness. No important finding is stated contrary to what is stated in the abstract. It is also not clear what the work teaches us about how tDCS works nor is it clear what are the "clinical implications for disorders of consciousness." The deep anesthesia is akin to being in a state of coma. This was not discussed.  

      While the authors have executed a set of technically challenging experiments, it is not clear what they teach us about how tDCS works, normal brain neurophysiology, or brain pathological states such as disorders of consciousness.

      We thank the reviewer for his comments. We agree that we could better highlight the value and implications of our work, and we take this opportunity to improve our manuscript according to the suggestions.

      Actions in the text: We have added several new paragraphs in the Discussion section, considering these comments and other related remarks from the Reviewing Editor (see below our answer to the first comment of the Reviewing Editor: REC#1).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      General comments: 

      The authors investigated the effects of tDCS on brain dynamics in awake and anesthetized monkeys using functional MRI. They claim that cathodal tDCS disrupts the functional connectivity pattern in awake monkeys while anodal tDCS alters brain patterns in anesthetized monkeys. This study offers valuable insight into how brain states can influence the outcomes of noninvasive brain stimulation. However, there are several aspects of the methods and results sections that should be improved to clarify the findings.

      We thank the Reviewer for the summary and appreciation of our study.  

      Major comments 

      For the anesthetized monkeys, the anode location differs between subjects, with the electrode positioned to stimulate the left DLFPC in monkey R and the right DLPFC in monkey N. The authors mention that this discrepancy does not result in significant differences in the electric field due to the monkeys' small head size. However, this is incorrect, as placing the anode on the left hemisphere would result in a much lower EF in the right DLPFC than placing the anode on the right side. Running an electric field simulation would confirm this. Additionally, the small electrode size suggested by the Easy cap configuration for NHP appears sufficient to stimulate the targeted regions focally. If this interpretation is correct, the authors should provide additional evidence to support their claim, such as a computational simulation of the EF distribution.

      We thank the Reviewer for the comments. First, regarding the reviewer’s statement that placing the anode on the left hemisphere would result in a much lower EF in the right DLPFC than placing the anode on the right side, we would like to clarify that we did not use a typical 4 x 1 concentric ring high-definition setup (which consists of a small centre electrode surrounded by four return electrodes), but a two-electrode montage, with one electrode over the left or right PFC and the other one over the contralateral occipital cortex. According to EF modelling papers, a 4 x 1 high-definition setup would produce an EF that is focused and limited to the cortical area circumscribed by the ring of the return electrodes (Datta et al. 2009; Alam et al. 2016). Therefore, targeting the left or right DLPFC with a 4 x 1 setup would produce an EF confined to the targeted hemisphere of the PFC. In contrast, we expect the brain current flow generated with our 2-electrode setup to be broader, despite the small size of the electrodes,  because there is no constraint from return electrodes. Thus, with our setup, the current is expected to flow between the PFC and the occipital cortex (see also our responses to comments R3.3., R.E.C.#2.1. and R.E.C.#2.2.). 

      Second, we would like to point out that in awake experiments, in which we stimulated the right PFC of both monkeys, there was no gross evidence of left or right asymmetry in the computed functional connectivity patterns (Figure 3A, Figure 3 - figure supplement 2A; Figure 5A). These results, showing that our stimulation montages did not induce asymmetric dynamic FC changes in NHPs, support the idea that our setups did not generate EFs that were spatially focused enough to alter brain activity in one hemisphere substantially more than the other.

      Third, it is also worth noting that current evidence suggests that human brains are significantly more lateralized than those of macaques. Macaque monkeys have been found to have some degree of lateralized networks, but these are of lower complexity, and the lateralization is less pronounced and functionally organized than in humans. (Whey et al., 2014; Mantini et al., 2013). This suggests that, even if the stimulation were focal enough to stimulate the left or the right part of the PFC only, the behavioural effects would likely be similar.

      We strongly agree with the reviewer that conducting an EF simulation would be valuable to confirm our expectations and to gain a comprehensive view of the characteristics of the EFs generated with our different setups in NHPs. However, the challenge is in the fact that EF computational models have been developed for humans, and their use in NHPs is not straightforward due to significant anatomical differences. For example, macaque monkeys are distinct from humans in terms of brain size, shape and cortical organisation, skull thickness, and the presence of muscles, as well as different tissue conductivities (Lee et al. 2015; Datta et al.2016; Mantell et al. 2023). We plan to address this in future work.

      Actions in the text: In the Materials and Methods section, we have modified the sentence: “Because of the small size of the monkey's head and because we did not use return electrodes to restrict the current flow (as is achieved with typical high-definition montages (Datta et al. 2009; Alam et al. 2016)), we expected that tDCS stimulation with the two symmetrical montages would result in nearly equivalent electric fields across the monkey’s head and produce roughly similar effects on brain activity.” 

      We also added a new sentence about EF simulation: 

      “This would need to be confirmed by running an electric field simulation. However, computational electric field models have been developed for humans, and their use in NHPs is not straightforward due to anatomical specificities. Indeed, monkeys differ from humans in terms of brain size, shape and cortical organization, skull thickness, tissue conductivities and the presence of muscles (Lee et al. 2015; Datta et al. 2016; Mantell et al. 2023). Modelling of EFs generated with the specific tDCS montages employed in this study will be performed in future work.”

      For the anesthetized monkeys, the authors applied 1 mA tDCS first, followed by 2 mA tDCS. A 20-minute stimulation duration of 1 mA tDCS is strong enough to produce after-effects that could influence the brain state during the 2 mA tDCS. This raises some concerns. Previous studies have shown that 1 mA tDCS can generate EF of over 1 V/m in the brain, and the effects of stimulation are sensitive to brain state (e.g., eye closed vs. eye open). How do the authors ensure that there are no after-effects from the 1 mA tDCS? This issue makes it challenging to directly compare the effects of 1 mA and 2 mA stimulation.

      We agree with the reviewer's comment that 1 mA tDCS may induce aftereffects, as has been observed in several human studies (e.g., (Jamil et al. 2017, 2020). Although the differences between the 1 mA post-stimulation and baseline conditions were not significant in our analyses, it's still possible that the stimulation produced some effects below the threshold of significance that may contribute, albeit weakly, to the changes observed during and after 2 mA stimulation. We have, therefore, amended the paper in line with the reviewer's comments.

      Actions in the text: We have added the following text in the Result section: 

      “While several human studies have reported that 1 mA transcranial stimulation induces aftereffects (e.g., (Jamil et al. 2017, 2020; Monte-Silva et al. 2010), the differences between the 1 mA post-stimulation and baseline conditions were not significant in our analyses. However, it is still possible that the 1 mA stimulation produced some effects below the threshold of significance that may contribute to the changes observed during and after the 2 mA stimulation.”

      The occurrence rate of a specific structural-functional coupling pattern among random brain regions shows significant effects of tDCS. However, these results seem counterintuitive. It is generally understood that noninvasive brain stimulation tends to modulate functional connectivity rather than structural or structural-functional connectivity. How does the occurrence rate of structural-functional coupling patterns provide a more suitable measure of the effectiveness of tDCS than functional connectivity alone? I would recommend that the authors present the results based on functional connectivity itself. If there is no change in functional connectivity, the relevance of changes in structural-functional coupling might not translate into a meaningful alteration in brain function, making it unclear how significant this finding is without corresponding functional evidence.

      First, of all, we would like to make it clear that the occurrence rate of patterns as a function of their SFC is not intended to be used or seen as a ‘better’ measure of the efficacy of tDCS. Instead, it is one aspect of the effects of tDCS on whole-brain functional cortical dynamics, obtained from refined measures (phase-coherences), that specifically addresses the coupling between structure and function. This type of analysis is further motivated by its increasing use in the literature due to its suspected relationship to wakefulness (e.g., (Barttfeld et al. 2015, Demertzi et al. 2019; Castro et al. 2023)). Also, in our analysis, the structure is kept constant: the connectivity matrix used to correlate the functional brain states is always the same (CoCoMac82). Thus, the influence of tDCS on the structure-function side can only be explained by modulating the functional aspects, as suggested by intuition and previous results.

      Then, we agree with the reviewer that studying the functional changes induced by tDCS alone could be valuable. However, usual metrics used in FC analysis are usually done statistically: FC-states are either computed through averaging spatial correlations over time, then analyzed through graph-theoretical properties for instance (or by just directly computing the element-wise differences), or either by considering the properties of the different visited FC-states by computing spatial correlations over a sliding time-window, and then similar analysis can be done as previously explained. But these are static metrics, if the states visited are essentially the same (which is expected from non-invasive neuromodulations that haven’t already demonstrated strong and/or characteristic impact), but the dynamical process of visiting said states changes, one would see no difference in that regard. As such, in the case of resting-state fMRI, differences in FCs are hard to interpret given that between-sessions within-condition differences are usually found with some degree of variance for the respective conditions. Trying then to interpret between-condition differences is quite tricky in the case of subtle modulations of the system’s activity. On the other hand, more subtle differences can be captured by considering more detailed analysis, such as using phase-based methods like we did,  by incorporating some statistical learning component with regard to the dynamicity of the system (supervised learning for instance like we did followed by temporal & transition-based methodology), and by adding some dimensions along which one will be able to give some interpretation to the analysis.  In our case we were interested in characterizing resting-state differences between stimulation conditions, which have nuanced and subtle interactions with the biological system. 

      As such, classical measures of differences between FC states are likely to not be refined and precise enough. In fact, we propose additional files investigating those classically used measures such as differences in average FC matrices, or changes in functional graph properties (like modularity, efficiency and density) of the visited FC states. These figures show that, for the first case, comparing region-to-region specific FCs provides very few statistically significant results. With respect to the second part, we show that virtually no differences are observed in the properties of the functional states visited. 

      These results suggest, as expected, that the actual brain states visited across the different stimulation conditions are topologically quite similar, and that only very few region-specific pairwise functional connectivities are particularly modulated by specific tDCS montages while, on the other hand, the actual dynamical process dictating how the brain activity passes from one state to another is in fact being influenced as shown by the dynamical analysis presented in the main figures in a more apparent and meaningful way (in that it is dependent on the montage, somewhat consistent with regard to the post-stimulations conditions, and can be made sense of by considering the theoretical effect of near-anodal versus near-cathodal neuromodulatory effects).

      Actions in the text: We have added new supplementary files showing the effects of the stimulations on FC matrices and on classical functional graph properties in awake and anesthesia datasets (Supplementary Files 3 & 4).

      We have added new sentences about these new analyses on the effects of the stimulations on FC matrices and on classical functional graph properties in the Results section:

      “In addition, we performed the main analyses separately for the two monkeys, explored the inter-condition variability (Supplementary File 2), and computed classical measures of functional connectivity such as average FC matrices and functional graph properties (modularity, efficiency and density) of the visited FC states (Supplementary File 3).... In contrast, classical FC metrics did not show significant differences across stimulation conditions, highlighting the value of dynamic FC metrics to capture the neuromodulatory effects of tDCS.”

      “Analyses of the two monkeys separately showed that the changes in slope and Shannon entropy were bigger in one of the two monkeys but went in the same direction (Supplementary File 2), while classical FC metrics did not capture any statistical differences between the different stimulation conditions (Supplementary File 3).”

      The authors recorded data from only two monkeys, which may limit the investigation of the group effects of tDCS. As the number of scans for the second monkey in each consciousness condition is lower than that in the first monkey, there is a concern that the main effects might primarily reflect the data from a single monkey. I suggest that the authors should analyze the data for each monkey individually to determine if similar trends are observed in both subjects.

      We agree that the small number of subjects is a limitation of our study. However, we have already addressed these aspects by reporting statistical analyses that consider them, using linear models of such variables, and running them through ANOVA tests. In addition, we experimentally ensured that we recorded a relatively high number of sessions over a period of several years. Regardless, we agree that our study would benefit from further investigation into this matter. We have therefore prepared complementary figures showing the main analysis performed separately for the two monkeys as proposed, as well as further investigations into the inter-condition variability outmatching the inter-individual variability, itself being also outmatched by intra-individual changes. 

      Actions in the text: We have added a supplementary file showing the main analyses performed separately for the two monkeys (Supplementary File 2) and further investigations into the inter-condition variability (Supplementary Files 3 & 4).

      We have added new sentences about these analyses performed separately for the two monkeys in the Results section:

      “In addition, we performed the main analyses separately for the two monkeys, explored the inter-condition variability (Supplementary File 2), and computed classical measures of functional connectivity such as average FC matrices and functional graph properties (modularity, efficiency and density) of the visited FC states (Supplementary File 3). The separate analyses showed that the changes in slope and Shannon entropy were substantially more pronounced in one of the two monkeys, corroborating some of the effects captured in the ANOVA tests.”

      “Analyses of the two monkeys separately showed that the changes in slope and Shannon entropy were bigger in one of the two monkeys but went in the same direction (Supplementary

      File 2)”.

      Anodal tDCS was only applied to anesthetized monkeys, which limits the conclusion that the authors are aiming for. It raises questions about the conclusion regarding brain state dependency. To address this, it would be better to include the cathodal tDCS session for anesthetized monkeys. If cathodal tDCS changes the connectivity during anesthesia, it becomes difficult to argue that the effects of cathodal tDCS vary depending on the state of consciousness as discussed in this paper. On the other hand, if cathodal tDCS would not produce any changes, the conclusion would then focus on the relationship between the polarity of tDCS and consciousness. In that case, the authors could maintain their conclusion but might need to refine it to reflect this specific relationship more accurately. 

      We agree with the reviewer that it would have been interesting to investigate the effects of cathodal tDCS in anesthetized monkeys. However, due to the challenging nature of the experimental procedures under anesthesia, we had to limit the investigations to only one stimulation modality. We chose to deliver anodal stimulation because, from a translational point of view, we aimed to provide new information on the effects of tDCS under anesthesia as a model for disorders of consciousness. It also made much more sense to increase the cortical excitability of the prefrontal cortex in an attempt to wake up the sedated monkeys rather than doing the opposite.

      Actions in the text: We have added a new sentence in the Results section:

      “Due to the challenging nature of the experimental procedures under anesthesia, we limited the investigations to only one stimulation modality. We chose to deliver anodal stimulation to provide new information on the effects of tDCS under anesthesia as a model for disorders of consciousness and to increase the cortical excitability of the PFC in an attempt to wake up the sedated monkeys.”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This study used transcranial direct current stimulation administered using small 'high-definition' electrodes to modulate neural activity within the non-human primate prefrontal cortex during both wakefulness and anaesthesia. Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was used to assess the neuromodulatory effects of stimulation. The authors report on the modification of brain dynamics during and following anodal and cathodal stimulation during wakefulness and following anodal stimulation at two intensities (1 mA, 2 mA) during anaesthesia. This study provides some possible support that prefrontal direct current stimulation can alter neural activity patterns across wakefulness and sedation in monkeys. However, the reported findings need to be considered carefully against several important methodological limitations. 

      Strengths: 

      A key strength of this work is the use of fMRI-based methods to track changes in brain activity with good spatial precision. Another strength is the exploration of stimulation effects across wakefulness and sedation, which has the potential to provide novel information on the impact of electrical stimulation across states of consciousness.

      We thank the Reviewer for the summary and for highlighting the strengths of our study. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The lack of a sham stimulation condition is a significant limitation, for instance, how can the authors be sure that results were not affected by drowsiness or fatigue as a result of the experimental procedure?

      We agree with the reviewer that adding control conditions could have strengthened our study. Control conditions usually consist of a sham condition or active control conditions. However, as mentioned in response to one of Reviewer 2 comments (R.2.5), we had to make choices as we could not perform as many experiments due to their demanding nature, especially under anesthesia. 

      In the awake state, we acquired data with two experimental conditions; the monkeys were exposed to either anodal (F4/O1) or cathodal (O1/F4) PFC tDCS. As anodal tDCS of the PFC induced only minor changes in brain dynamics, it could be considered as an active control condition for the cathodal condition, which had striking effects on the cortical dynamics. It is also worth noting that doubts have been raised about the neurobiological inertia of certain sham protocols. Indeed, different sham protocols have been employed in the literature, some of which may produce unintended effects (Fonteneau et al. 2019). Therefore, active control conditions, such as reversing the polarity of the stimulation or targeting a different brain region, have been proposed to provide better control (Fonteneau et al. 2019). Furthermore, in the context of experiments performed under anesthesia, the relevance of a sham control condition typically used to achieve adequate blinding is questionable. 

      With regard to drowsiness and fatigue as a result of the experimental procedure, we agree with the reviewer that this is a common problem in functional imaging due to the length of the recording sessions. We assumed, as was done in previous work (Uhrig, Dehaene, and Jarraya 2014; Wang et al. 2015), that the monkeys' performance on the fixation task during acquisition would capture these periods of fatigue. Therefore, only sessions with fixation rates above 85% were included in our analysis. 

      Actions in the text: We have now specified, in the Materials and Methods section, the fact that only runs with a high fixation rate (> 85%) were included in the study: 

      “To ensure that the results were not biased by fatigue or drowsiness due to the lengthy

      In the anaesthesia condition, the authors investigated the effects of two intensities of stimulation (1 mA and 2 mA). However, a potential confound here relates to the possibility that the initial 1 mA stimulation block might have caused plasticity-related changes in neural activity that could have interfered with the following 2 mA block due to the lack of a sufficient wash-out period. Hence, I am not sure any findings from the 2 mA block can really be interpreted as completely separate from the initial 1 mA stimulation period, given that they were administered consecutively. Several previous studies have shown that same-day repeated tDCS stimulation blocks can influence the effects of neuromodulation (e.g., Bastani and Jaberzadeh, 2014, Clin Neurophysiol; Monte-Silva et al., J. Neurophysiology). 

      We agree with the reviewer’s comment that the initial 1 mA stimulation block might have induced changes in neural activity and that the 20-minute post 1 mA block would not be long enough to wash out these changes. This comment is very similar to the second comment made by Reviewer 2 (R.2.2). Although our experimental data do not support this possibility (as the differences between the 1 mA post-stimulation and baseline conditions were not significant), it is still conceivable that the stimulation produced some effects below the threshold of significance and that these might weakly contribute to the changes observed during and after the 2 mA stimulation. 

      Actions in the text: We have modified the paper according to the reviewers' comments (please see our answer and actions in the text to R.2.2.).

      The different electrode placement for the two anaesthetised monkeys (i.e., Monkey R: F3/O2 montage, Monkey N: F4/O1 montage) is problematic, as it is likely to have resulted in stimulation over different brain regions. The authors state that "Because of the small size of the monkey's head, we expected that tDCS stimulation with these two symmetrical montages would result in nearly equivalent electric fields across the monkey's head and produce roughly similar effects on brain activity"; however, I am not totally convinced of this, and it really would need E-field models to confirm. It is also more likely that there would in fact be notable differences in the brain regions stimulated as the authors used HD-tDCS electrodes, which are generally more focal.

      We thank the Reviewer for the remark, which is very similar to the second comment from Reviewer 2. Please see our answer to the first comment of Reviewer 2 

      Actions in the text: We have modified the paper according to the reviewers' comments (please see the actions taken in response to R.2.1.).

      Given the very small sample size, I think it is also important to consider the possibility that some results might also be impacted by individual differences in response to stimulation. For instance, in the discussion (page 9, paragraph 2) the authors contrast findings observed in awake animals versus anaesthetised animals. However, different monkeys were examined for these two conditions, and there were only two monkeys in each group (monkeys J and Y for awake experiments [both male], and monkeys R and N [male and female] for the anaesthesia condition). From the human literature, it is well known that there is a considerable amount of inter-individual variability in response to stimulation (e.g., Lopez-Alonso et al., 2014, Brain Stimulation; Chew et al., 2015, Brain Stimulation), therefore I wonder if some of these differences could also possibly result from differences in responsiveness to stimulation between the different monkeys? At the end of the paragraph, the authors also state "Our findings also support the use of tDCS to promote rapid recovery from general anesthesia in humans...and suggest that a single anodal prefrontal stimulation at the end of the anesthesia protocol may be effective." However, I'm not sure if this statement is really backed-up by the results, which failed to report "any behavioural signs of awakening in the animals" (page 7)?

      We thank the Reviewer for this comment. Because working with non-human primates is expensive and labor intensive, the sample sizes in classical macaque experiments are generally small (typically 2-4 subjects per experiment). Our sample size (i.e. 2 rhesus macaques in awake experiments and 2 macaques under sedation, 11 +/- 9 scan sessions per animal, 288 and 136 runs in the awake and anesthesia state, respectively) is comparable to other previous work in non-human primates using fMRI (Milham et al. 2018; Yacoub et al. 2020; Uchimura, Kumano, and Kitazawa 2024). In addition, we would like to point out that the baseline cortical dynamics we found before stimulation, whether in the awake or sedated state, are comparable to previous studies (Barttfeld et al. 2015; Uhrig et al. 2018; Tasserie et al. 2022). This suggests our results are reproducible across datasets, despite the small sample size.

      That being said, we agree with the reviewer that inter-individual variability in response to stimulation can be considerable, as shown by a large body of literature in the field. It seems possible that the two monkeys studied in each condition responded differently to the stimulation. But even if that’s the case, our results suggest that at least in one of the two monkeys, cathodal PFC stimulation in the awake state and anodal PFC stimulation under propofol anesthesia induced striking changes in brain dynamics, which we believe is a significant contribution to the field. 

      In fact, supplementary analysis, as proposed by Reviewer 2 (cf R2.4), investigating how the different measurables we’ve used were differently affected by tDCS show that indeed monkey Y’s case is more apparent and significant than monkey J’s. Still, the effects observed in monkey J’s case are still congruent with what is observed in monkey Y’s and at the population level (though less flagrant). We also show that these inter-individual variabilities are outmatched by the inter-condition variability, (as indicated by our initially strong statistical results at the population levels), thus showing that, even though we have different responses depending on the subject, the effects observed at the population level cannot be only accounted for by the differences in subjects’ specificities.

      Lastly, the Reviewer questioned whether our results support that a single anodal prefrontal stimulation at the end of the anesthesia protocol could effectively promote rapid recovery from general anesthesia, because the stimulation did not wake the animals in our experiments. It should be emphasized that in our case, the monkeys were stimulated while they were still receiving continuous propofol perfusion. In contrast, during the recovery process from anesthesia, the delivery of the anesthetic drug is stopped. It is therefore conceivable that anodal PFC tDCS, which successfully enriched brain dynamics in sedated monkeys in our experiments, may accelerate the recovery from anesthesia when the drug is no longer administered. 

      Actions in the text: We have added a line in the Materials and Methods to compare to other studies:

      “Our sample size is comparable to previous work in NHP using fMRI (Milham et al. 2018; Yacoub et al. 2020; Uchimura, Kumano, and Kitazawa 2024).”

      Reviewing Editor Comments: 

      In some cases, authors opt to submit a revised manuscript. Should you choose to do so, please be aware that the reviewers have indicated that their appraisal is unlikely to change unless some of the suggested field modelling is incorporated into the work. This may change the evaluation of the strength of evidence, but the final wording will be subject to reviewer discretion. Details for responding to the reviews are provided at the bottom of this email.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The work should discuss the implications of their experiments for using tDCS to arouse a patient from a coma. The anesthetized animal is effectively in a drug-induced coma. While they observed connectivity changes, these changes did not map nicely onto behavioral changes. 

      I would suggest that the authors spell out more clearly what they view as the clinical implications of their work in terms of new insights into how tDCS may be used to either understand and or treat disorders of consciousness.

      We thank the Reviewer for his thoughtful comments. We appreciate the opportunity to clarify and expand on the key findings and implications of our work, particularly regarding the new insights into how tDCS can be used to understand and treat disorders of consciousness. We therefore provide a broader perspective on the clinical implications of our experiments regarding coma and disorders of consciousness. We also agree with the Reviewer that the absence of behavioral changes but the presence of functional differences should be more clearly addressed. 

      Actions in the text: We have added a few lines about the relevance of anesthesia as a model for disorders of consciousness in the Introduction part:

      “Anesthesia provides a unique model for studying consciousness, which, similarly to DOC, is characterized by the disruption or even  the loss of consciousness (Luppi 2024). Additionally, anesthesia mechanisms involve several subcortical nuclei that are key components of the brain's sleep and arousal circuits (Kelz and Mashour 2019).”

      In the Discussion section, we have modified and expanded a paragraph about the effects of tDCS in DOC patients and how this technique could be further used to study consciousness: From another clinical perspective, our results demonstrating that 2 mA anodal PFC tDCS decreased the structure-function correlation and modified the dynamic repertoire of brain patterns during anesthesia (Figures 6 and 7) are consistent with the beneficial effects of such stimulation in DOC patients (Thibaut et al., 2014; Angelakis et al., 2014; Thibaut et al., 2017; Zhang et al., 2017; Martens et al., 2018; Cavinato et al., 2019; Wu et al., 2019; Hermann et al., 2020; Peng et al., 2022; Thibaut et al., 2023). Although some clinical trials investigated the effects of stimulating other brain regions, such as the motor cortex (Martens et al., 2019; Straudi et al., 2019) or the parietal cortex (Huang et al., 2017; Guo et al., 2019; Zhang et al., 2022; Wan et al., 2023; Wang et al., 2020), the DLPFC appears to be the most effective target for patients with a minimally conscious state (Liu et al., 2023). In terms of neuromodulatory effects in DOC patients, DLPFC tDCS has been reported to increase global excitability (Bai et al., 2017), increase the P300 amplitude (Zhang et al., 2017; Hermann et al., 2020), improve the fronto-parietal coherence in the theta band (Bai et al., 2018), enhance the putative EEG markers of consciousness (Bai et al., 2018; Hermann et al., 2020) and reduce the incidence of slow-waves in the resting state (Mensen et al., 2020). Our findings further support the PFC as a relevant target for modulating consciousness level and align with growing evidence showing that the PFC plays a key role in conscious access networks (Mashour, Pal, and Brown 2022; Panagiotaropoulos 2024). Nevertheless, we hypothesize that other brain targets for tDCS may be of interest for consciousness restoration, potentially using multi-channel tDCS (Havlík et al., 2023). Among transcranial electrical stimulation techniques, tDCS has the great advantage of facilitating either excitation or inhibition of brain regions, depending on the polarity of the stimulation (Sdoia et al., 2019) exploited this advantage to investigate the causal involvement of the DLPFC in conscious access to a visual stimulus during an attentional blink paradigm. While conscious access was enhanced by anodal stimulation of the left DLPFC compared to sham stimulation, opposite effects were found with cathodal stimulation compared to sham over the same locus. Finally, this literature and our findings suggest that tDCS constitutes a non-invasive, reversible, and powerful tool for studying consciousness.”

      We have added a new paragraph about patients with cognitive-motor dissociation and dissociation between consciousness and behavioral responsiveness:

      “Changes in the state of consciousness are generally closely associated with changes in behavioural responsiveness, although some rare cases of dissociation have been described. Cognitive-motor dissociation (CMD) is a condition observed in patients with severe brain injury, characterized by behavior consistent with unresponsive wakefulness syndrome or a minimally conscious state minus (Thibaut et al., 2019). However, in these patients, specific cortical brain areas activate in response to mental imagery tasks (e.g., imagining playing tennis or returning home) in a manner indistinguishable from that of healthy controls, as shown through fMRI or EEG (Thibaut et al., 2019; Owen et al., 2006; Monti et al., 2010; Bodien et al., 2024). Thus, although CMD patients are behaviorally unresponsive, they demonstrate cognitive awareness that is not outwardly apparent. It is worth noting that both the structure-function correlation and the rate of the pattern closest to the anatomy were shown to be significantly reduced in unresponsive patients showing command following during mental imagery tasks compared to those who do not show command following (Demertzi et al., 2019). These observations would be compatible with our findings in anesthetized macaques exposed to 2 mA anodal PFC tDCS. The richness of the brain dynamics would be recovered (at least partially, in our experiments), but not the behaviour. This hypothesis also fits with a recent longitudinal fMRI study on patients recovering from coma (Crone et al., 2020). The researchers examined two groups of patients: one group consisted of individuals who were unconscious at the acute scanning session but regained consciousness and improved behavioral responsiveness a few months later, and the second group consisted of patients who were already conscious from the start and only improved behavioral responsiveness at follow-up. By comparing these two groups, the authors could distinguish between the recovery of consciousness and the recovery of behavioral responsiveness. They demonstrated that only initially conscious patients exhibited rich brain dynamics at baseline. In contrast, patients who were unconscious in the acute phase and later regained consciousness had poor baseline dynamics, which became more complex at follow-up. Complete recovery of both consciousness and responsiveness under general anesthesia is possible through electrical stimulation of the central thalamus (Redinbaugh et al., 2020; Tasserie et al., 2022).”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Method 

      (1) The authors mentioned that they used HD-tDCS in their experiments; however, they used 1 x 1 tDCS, which is not HD-tDCS but rather single-channel tDCS.

      We thank the Reviewing Editor for pointing out this ambiguous wording. We understand that "HD-tDCS", which we used in our paper to refer to high-density 1x1 tDCS (because we used small carbon electrodes instead of the large sponge electrodes employed in conventional tDCS), may cause some confusion with high-definition tDCS, which uses compact ring electrodes and most commonly refers to a 4x1 montage (1 active central electrode over the target area and 4 return electrodes placed around the central electrode).

      Therefore, to avoid any confusion, we will use the term "tDCS" rather than “HD-tDCS” to qualify the technique used in this paper and suppress mentions of high-density or high-definition tDCS.

      Actions in the text: We have replaced the abbreviation “HD-tDCS” with “tDCS” throughout the paper. We have also suppressed the sentence about high-definition tDCS in the Introduction (“While conventional tDCS relies on the use of relatively large rectangular pad electrodes, high-density tDCS (HD-tDCS) utilizes more compact ring electrodes, allowing for increased focality, stronger electric fields, and presumably, greater neurophysiological changes (Datta et al. 2009; Dmochowski et al. 2011)”) and the two related citations in the References section.

      (2) Please provide the characteristics of electrodes, including their size, shape, and thickness.

      We thank the Reviewing Editor for this recommendation. We now provide the complete characteristics of the tDCS electrodes used in the paper.

      Actions in the text: We have added a sentence describing the characteristics of the tDCS electrodes in the Materials and Methods section:

      “We used a 1x1 electrode montage with two carbon rubber electrodes (dimensions: 1.4 cm x 1.85 cm, 0.93  cm thick) inserted into Soterix HD-tES MRI electrode holders (base diameter: 25 mm; height: 10.5 mm), which are in contact with the scalp. These electrodes (2.59 cm2) are smaller than conventional tDCS sponge electrodes (typically 25 to 35 cm<sup>2</sup>).”

      (3) Could the authors clarify why they chose to stimulate the right DLPFC? Is there a specific rationale for this choice? Additionally, could the authors explain how they ensured that the stimulation targeted the DLPFC, given that the monkey cap might differ from human configurations? In many NHP studies, structural MRI is used to accurately determine electrode placement. Considering that a single channel F4 - O2 montage was used, even a small displacement of the frontal electrode laterally could result in the electric field not adequately covering the DLPFC. Could the authors provide structural MRI images and details of electrode positioning to help readers better understand targeting accuracy?

      We thank the Reviewing Editor for the thoughtful comments and recommendations. We appreciate the opportunity to further clarify our rationale for stimulating the right DLPFC and also the suggestion to provide structural MRI images and details of electrode positioning, which we think will improve the quality of the paper by showing targeting accuracy.

      First, we would like to clarify that our initial decision to stimulate the right PFC in most animals was driven by experimental constraints. Indeed, we had limited access to the left PFC in three of the four macaques, either due to the presence of cement (spreading asymmetrically from the centre of the head) used to fix the head post in awake animals or due to a scar in one of the two animals studied under anesthesia. 

      Second, we agree with the Reviewing Editor on the importance of showing details of electrode positioning and evidence of targeting accuracy across MRI sessions. Therefore, we now provide structural images showing the positions of anodal and cathodal electrodes in almost all acquired sessions: 10 sessions (out of 10) under anesthesia and 30 sessions in the awake state (out of 34 sessions, because we could not acquire structural images in four sessions). These images show that, in anesthesia experiments, the anodal electrode was positioned over the dorsal prefrontal cortex and the cathodal electrode was placed over the contralateral occipital cortex (at the level of the parieto–occipital junction) in both monkeys. In the awake state, the montage still targeted the prefrontal cortex and the occipital cortex, but with a slightly different placement. One of the electrodes was placed over the prefrontal cortex, closer to the premotor cortex than in anesthesia experiments, while the other one was placed over the occipital cortex (V1), slightly more posterior than in anesthesia experiments. These images therefore show that the placement was relatively accurate across sessions and reproducible between monkeys in each of the two arousal conditions.

      Actions in the text: We have added a supplementary file showing electrode positioning in 40 of the 44 acquired MRI sessions (Supplementary File 1). We have also added a new supplement figure (Figure 1 - figure supplement 1) showing electrode positioning in representative MRI sessions of the awake and anesthetized experiments in the main manuscript. 

      We added a few sentences referring to these figures in the Result section: 

      “Representative structural images showing electrode placements on the head of the two awake monkeys are shown in Figure 1 - figure supplement 1A). Supplementary File 1 displays the complete set of structural images, showing that the two electrodes were accurately placed over the prefrontal cortex and the occipital cortex in a reproducible manner across awake sessions.”

      Figure 1 - figure supplement 1. Structural images displaying electrode placements on the head of monkeys. A) Awake experiments. Representative sagittal, coronal and transverse MRI sections, and the corresponding skin reconstruction images showing the position of the prefrontal and the occipital electrodes on the head of monkeys J. and Y. B) Anesthesia experiments. Representative sagittal, coronal and transverse MRI sections, and the corresponding skin reconstruction images showing the position of the prefrontal and occipital electrodes over the occipital cortex on the head of monkeys R. and N.

      Supplementary File 1 (see attached file). Structural images showing the position of the tDCS electrodes on the monkey's head across sessions. Sagittal, coronal and transverse MRI sections, and corresponding skin reconstruction images showing the position of the prefrontal and occipital electrodes on the monkey's head for each MRI session (except for 4 sessions in which no anatomical scan was acquired). The two electrodes were accurately placed over the prefrontal cortex and the occipital cortex in a reproducible manner across sessions and between the two monkeys studied in each arousal state. In anesthesia experiments, the anodal electrode was placed over the dorsal prefrontal cortex, while the cathodal electrode was positioned over the parieto-occipital junction. In awake experiments, the prefrontal electrode was positioned over the dorsal prefrontal cortex/pre-motor cortex, while the occipital electrode was placed over the visual area 1. The position of the two electrodes differed slightly between the anesthetized and awake experiments due to different body positions (the prone position of the sedated monkeys prevented a more posterior position of the occipital electrode) and also due to the presence of a headpost on the head of the two monkeys in awake experiments (the monkeys we worked with in anesthesia experiments did not have an headpost).

      (4) If the authors did not analyze the data for the passive event-related auditory response, it may be helpful to remove the related sentence to avoid potential confusion for readers.

      We thank the Reviewing Editor for the comment. Although we understand the reviewer’s point of view, we decide to keep this information in the paper to inform the reader that the macaques were passively engaged in an auditory task, as this could have some influence on the brain state. In the Materials and Methods section, we already mentioned that the analysis of the cerebral responses to the auditory paradigm is not part of the paper. We have modified the sentence to make it clearer and to avoid potential confusion for readers.

      Actions in the text: We have modified the sentence referring to the passive event-related auditory response in the Materials and Methods section:

      “All fMRI data were acquired while the monkeys were engaged in a passive event-related auditory task, the local-global paradigm, which is based on local and global deviations from temporal regularities (Bekinschtein et al. 2009; Uhrig, Dehaene, and Jarraya 2014). The present paper does not address how tDCS perturbs cerebral responses to local and global deviants, which will be the subject of future work.”

      (5) Could the authors clarify what x(t) represents in the equation? Additionally, it would be better to number the equations.

      We apologize for the confusion,  x(t) represents the evolution of the BOLD signals over time. We have numbered the equations as suggested. 

      Actions in the text: We have added explanations about the notation and numerotation of equations.

      (6) It would be much better to provide schematic illustrations to explain what the authors did for analyzing fMRI data.

      We thank the Reviewing Editor for the suggestion and now provide a new figure as suggested.  

      Actions in the text: We have added a new figure (Figure 2) graphically showing the overall analysis performed. We have added a sentence about the new Figure 2 in the Results section:  “A graphical overview of the overall analysis is shown in Figure 2.” We have renumbered Figure 2 - supplement figures accordingly.

      Figure 2. fMRI Phase Coherence analysis. A) Left) Animals were scanned before, during and after PFC tDCS stimulation in the awake state (two macaques) or under deep propofol anesthesia (two macaques). Right) Example of Z-scored filtered BOLD time series for one macaque, 111 time points with a TR of 2.4 s. B) Hilbert transform of the z-scored BOLD signal of one ROI into its time-varying amplitude A(t) (red) and the real part of the phase φ (green). In blue, we recover the original z-scored BOLD signal as A(t)cos(φ). C) Example of the phase of the Hilbert transform for each brain region at one TR. D) Symmetric matrix of cosines of the phase differences between all pairs of brain regions. E) We concatenated the vectorized form of the triangular superior of the phase difference matrices for all TRs for all participants, in all the conditions for both datasets separately obtaining using the K-means algorithm, the brain patterns whose statistics are then analyzed in the different conditions.

      Results 

      (1) In Figures 3A, 5A, and 6A showing brain connectivity, it is difficult to relate the connectivity variability among the brain regions. Instead of displaying connection lines for nodes, it would be more effective if the authors highlighted significant, strong connectivity within specific brain regions using additional methods, such as bootstrapping.

      We thank the Reviewing Editor for the comment and suggestion. The connection lines indeed represent all the synchronizations above 0.5 and all the anti-synchronization below -0.5 between all pairs of brain regions. As suggested, another element we haven’t addressed is the heterogeneity in coherences between individual brain regions. We hence propose additional supplementary figures showing, for all centroids mentioned in main figures, the variance in phase-based connectivity of the distributions of coherence of all brain regions to the rest of the brain. High value would then indicate a wide range of values of coherence, while low would indicate the different coherence a region has with the rest of the brain have similar values. Thus, a brain with uniform color would indicate high homogeneity in coherence among brain regions, while sharp changes in colors would reveal that certain regions are more subject to high variance in their coherence distributions. We expect this new figure to more clearly expose the connectivity variability among the brain regions.

      Actions in the text: We have added new figures showing, for all centroids mentioned in the main figures, the variances in phase-based connectivity of the distributions of coherence  (Figure 3 - figure supplement 3;  Figure 5 - figure supplement 2; Figure 6 - figure supplement 3; Figure 7 - figure supplement 2). One of them is shown below for the only awake analysis (Figure 3 - figure supplement 3).

      Figure 3 - figure supplement 3. Variance in inter-region phase coherences of brain patterns. Low values (red and light red) indicate that the distribution of synchronizations between a brain region and the rest of the brain has relatively low variance, while high values (blue and light blue) indicate relatively high variance. Are displayed both supra (top) and subdorsal (bottom) views for each brain pattern from the main figure, ordered similarly as previously: from left (1) to right (6) as their respective SFC increases. 

      We added a few sentences about variances in phase-based connectivity of the distributions of coherence in the Result section: 

      “Further investigation of the variances in inter-region phase coherences of brain patterns, presented in Figure 3 - figure supplement 3, revealed two main findings. First, all the patterns exhibited some degree of lateral symmetry. Second, except for the pattern with the highest SFC, most patterns displayed high heterogeneity in their coherence variances and striking inter-pattern differences. These observations reflect both the segmentation of distinct functional networks across patterns and a topological organization within the patterns themselves: some regions showed a broader spectrum of synchrony with the rest of the brain, while others exhibited narrower distributions of coherence variances. For instance, unlike other brain patterns, pattern 5 was characterized by a high coherence variance in the frontal premotor areas and low variance in the occipital cortex, whereas pattern 3 had a high variance in the frontal and orbitofrontal regions. In addition, we performed the main analyses separately for the two monkeys, explored the inter-condition variability (Supplementary File 2), and computed classical measures of functional connectivity such as average FC matrices and functional graph properties (modularity, efficiency and density) of the visited FC states (Supplementary File 3).”

      “The variance in inter-regional phase coherence across brain patterns showed notably that pattern 4, in contrast to most other patterns, was characterized by a high variance in frontal premotor areas and a low variance in the occipital cortex (Figure 5 - figure supplement 2)." 

      “The variance in inter-region phase coherences of the brain patterns is displayed in Figure 6 - figure supplement 3 and showed a striking heterogeneity between the patterns. For example, pattern 5 had a low overall variance (except in the frontal cortex), while pattern 1 was the only pattern with a high variance in the occipital cortex.”

      “The variance in inter-region phase coherences of brain patterns is displayed in Figure 6 - figure supplement 2.”

      (2) For both conditions, only 2 to 3 out of 6 patterns showed significant effects of tDCS on the occurrence rate. Is it sufficient to claim the authors' conclusion?

      We thank the Reviewer Editor for the comment. We would like to point out that similar kinds of differences in the occurrence rates of specific brain patterns (particularly in patterns at the extremities of the SFC scale) have already been reported previously. Prior works in patients suffering from disorders of consciousness, in healthy humans or in non-human primates,  have shown, by using a similar method of analysis, that not all brain states are equally disturbed by loss of consciousness, even in different modalities of unconscious transitioning (Luppi et al. 2021; Z. Huang et al. 2020; Demertzi et al. 2019; Castro et al. 2023; Golkowski et al. 2019; Barttfeld et al. 2015). Therefore, yes we believe that our conclusions are still supported by the results.

      (3) If the authors want to assert that the brain state significantly influences the effects of tDCS as discussed in the manuscript, further analysis is necessary. First, it would be great to show the difference in connectivity between two consciousness conditions during the baseline (resting state) to see how resting state connectivity or structural connectivity varies. Second, demonstrating the difference in connectivity between the awake and anesthetized conditions (e.g., awake during cathodal vs. anesthetized cathodal) to show how the connectivity among the brain regions was changed by the brain state during tDCS. This would strengthen the authors' conclusion.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. Firstly, we’d like to clarify that the structural connectivity doesn’t change from one session to another in the same animal and minimally between subjects. Secondly, we agree with the Reviewing Editor that it is informative to show the differences between the baselines and this is what we have done. The results are shown in Figures 5 and 7. Regarding the comparison of the stimulating conditions across arousal levels, the only contrast that we could make is to compare 2 mA anodal awake with 2 mA anodal anesthetized (during and post-stimulation). However, as 2 mA anodal stimulation in the awake state did not affect the connectivity much (compared to the awake baseline), the results would be almost similar to the comparison of the awake baseline with 2 mA anodal anesthetized, which is shown in Figure 7. Therefore, we believe that this would result in minimal informative gains and even more redundancy. 

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Introduction, par 2: HD-tDCS does not necessarily produce stronger electric fields (E-fields) in the brain. The E-field is largely montage-dependent, and some configurations such as the 4x1 configuration can actually have weaker E-fields compared to conventional tDCS designs (i.e., with two sponge electrodes) as electrodes are often closer together resulting in more current being shunted by skull, scalp, and CSF. I would consider re-phrasing this section.

      We agree with the Reviewer Editor that high-definition tDCS does not necessarily produce stronger electric fields in the brain and apologize for the confusion caused by our use of HD-tDCS to refer to high-density tDCS. To avoid any confusion, we have removed the sentence mentioning that HD-tDCS produces stronger electric fields. 

      Actions in the text: We have removed the sentence about high-definition tDCS in the Introduction (“While conventional tDCS relies on the use of relatively large rectangular pad electrodes, high-density tDCS (HD-tDCS) utilizes more compact ring electrodes, allowing for increased focality, stronger electric fields, and presumably, greater neurophysiological changes (Datta et al. 2009; Dmochowski et al. 2011)”) and the two related citations in the References section.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Sayeed et al. uses a comprehensive series of multi-omics approaches to demonstrate that late-stage human cytomegalovirus (HCMV) infection leads to a marked disruption of TEAD1 activity, a concomitant loss of TEAD1-DNA interactions, and extensive chromatin remodeling. The data are thoroughly presented and provide evidence for the role of TEAD1 in the cellular response to HCMV infection.

      However, a key question remains unresolved: is the observed disruption of TEAD1 activity a direct consequence of HCMV infection, or could it be secondary to the broader innate antiviral response? In this respect, the study would benefit from experiments that assess the effect of TEAD1 overexpression or knockdown/deletion on HCMV replication dynamics. Such functional assays could help delineate whether TEAD1 perturbation directly influences viral replication or is part of a downstream/indirect cellular response, providing deeper mechanistic insights.

      To examine the effect of TEAD1 on HCMV, we performed an experiment in primary human foreskin fibroblasts (HFF) which were stably transduced with constitutive TEAD1. To constitutively express TEAD1, we cloned the open reading frame of TEAD1 into pLenti-puro (Plasmid #39481 from Addgene). We selected for transduced cells using puromycin. For these experiments, we first assessed two multiplicities of infection (MOI): 1 and 10 (Reviewer Response Figure 1). Based on the TEAD1 expression in these cells relative to non-transduced HFF cells, we performed HCMV infection experiments in cells transduced with TEAD1 lentivirus at an MOI of 1.

      For infections, we used a version of HCMV in which the C terminus of the capsi-associated tegument protein pUL32 (pp150) is tagged by enhanced green fluorescent protein (GFP) (PMID: 15708994). This experimental design allowed us to assess the impact of constitutive TEAD1 expression on HCMV infection. GFP and immediate early protein expression levels were measured 48 hours after infection by flow cytometry.

      After infecting parent cells (no constitutive TEAD1) and TEAD1 constitutively expressing cells with a GFP-positive HCMV at MOIs of 0.3 and 1, we identified equivalent GFP expression in the two conditions, indicating equivalent levels of HCMV infection 48 hours after initial infection (Reviewer Response Figure 1A). We also identified equivalent immediate early protein expression at 48 hours after infection, as measured both by percent positivity (Reviewer Response Figure 1B) and mean florescent intensity (Reviewer Response Figure 1C). At 96 hours with an MOI of 3, constitutive expression of TEAD1 led to a slight reduction in the expression of the HCMV proteins pp65 (encoded by UL83) and UL44 at 72 and 96 hours post initial infection (Reviewer Response Figure 1D). These results suggest that TEAD1 expression has minimal effects, if any, on the expression of these two late HCMV proteins in fibroblasts.  Regulation of particular HCMV genes by TEAD1 is likely to be central for HCMV replication and reactivation in other specialized cell types relevant to viral pathogenesis and disease. However, definitive studies are beyond the scope of the current study. 

      Author response image 1.

      Constitutive TEAD1 expression reduces expression of two HCMV late genes at 72 and 96 hours after infection. A-C. Primary human foreskin fibroblasts with and without constitutive TEAD1 expression were infected with pp150-GFP HCMV at a multiplicity of infection (MOI) of 0.3 or 1 and assessed 48 hours post infection. A. HCMV positive cells were quantified by measuring the percent of cells that were GFP positive. B. The percentages of immediate early (IE1/IE2) positive cells were quantified by flow cytometry. C. The mean florescence intensity of immediate early positive cells was quantified by flow cytometry. D. Primary human foreskin fibroblasts with and without constitutive TEAD1 expression were infected with pp150-GFP HCMV at an MOI of 1 and assessed by Western blot at various time point post infection. UL44 and pp65 are expressed late in the cascade of HCMV gene expression. TEAD1 expression levels and uncropped Westerns are provided in Supplemental Figure S8

      Reviewer Response Methods:

      Flow cytometric analysis of viral entry and spread using GFP expression and HCMV immediate early (IE) protein staining

      Parental and TEAD1 transduced human foreskin fibroblasts were seeded into 12-well plates at 1.0 × 10<sup>5</sup> cells per well and either mock infected or infected with pp150-GFP HCMV (PMID: 15708994) at MOIs of 0.3 or 1 on the same day. Cells were trypsinized at appropriate time points and then neutralized with complete medium. Cell suspensions were spun down at 500g for 5 minutes, and the cell pellet was fixed in 70% ethanol for 30 minutes. Following fixation, cells were permeabilized in phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) containing 0.5% bovine serum albumin (BSA) and 0.5% Tween 20 for 10 minutes at 4°C, pelleted, and then stained with IE1/IE2 antibody (mAb810-Alexa Fluor 488) diluted in PBS supplemented with 0.5% BSA for 2 hours. Cells were washed with PBS supplemented with 0.5% BSA–0.5% Tween 20 and then resuspended in PBS. Cells were analyzed using a flow cytometer (BD Biosciences). Infected cells were also trypsinized at appropriate time points, neutralized in the appropriate media, and directly analyzed for GFP positivity on the flow cytometer.

      Western blot analyses of HCMV protein expression in infected cells with and without constitutive TEAD1 expression

      TEAD1 transduced and parental human foreskin fibroblasts were seeded into 6-well cell culture plates at a density of 3.0 × 10<sup>5</sup> cells per well and either mock infected or infected with pp150-GFP HCMV (PMID: 15708994) at an MOI of 1. Whole-cell lysates were collected at various time points post-infection, separated by SDS-PAGE, and transferred to nitrocellulose for Western blot analysis. Western blots were probed with the following primary antibodies: anti-IE1/IE2 (Chemicon), anti-UL44 (kind gift of John Shanley), anti-pp65 (Virusys Corporation), and cellular β-actin antibody (Bethyl Laboratories). Next, each blot was incubated with appropriate horseradish peroxidase-conjugated anti-rabbit or anti-mouse IgG secondary antibodies. Chemiluminescence was detected and quantified using a C-DiGit blot scanner from Li-Cor.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work uses genomic and biochemical approaches for HCMV infection in human fibroblasts and retinal epithelial cell lines, followed by comparisons and some validations using strategies such as immunoblots. Based on these analyses, they propose several mechanisms that could contribute to the HCMV-induced diseases, including closing of TEAD1-occupying domains and reduced TEAD1 transcript and protein levels, decreased YAP1 and phospho-YAP1 levels, and exclusion of TEAD1 exon 6.

      Strengths:

      The genomics experiments were done in duplicates and data analyses show good technical reproducibility. Data analyses are performed to show changes at the transcript and chromatin level changes, followed by some Western blot validations.

      Weaknesses:

      This work, at the current stage, is quite correlative since no functional studies are done to show any causal links. For readers who are outside the field, some clarifications of the system and design need to be stated.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Here are some specific questions:

      (1) Since all current analyses are correlative, it is difficult to know which changes are of biological significance. For example, experiments manipulating TEAD transcription factor or YAP with effects on how cells respond to HCMV infection would significantly strengthen the conclusions, which are largely speculations now.

      Please see response to Reviewer 1, which highlights newly added functional assays that include the constitutive (forced) expression of TEAD1, as suggested.

      (2) How similar are these cell lines (human fibroblasts and retinal epithelial cell lines) resembling the actually infected cells in patients that lead to symptoms?

      In infected cells in patients, HCMV initially infects both fibroblasts and epithelial cells. HCMV penetrates fibroblasts by fusion at the cell surface but is endocytosed into epithelial cells (PMID: 18077432). Thus, most experimental studies of HCMV in vitro use primary human foreskin fibroblasts and a retinal epithelial cell line, as we do in this study.

      Additional information on primary human fibroblasts as a model of HCMV infection in humans

      There is a nice review article that provides the history of the study of the molecular biology of HCMV that describes how Stanley Plotkin from the Wistar Institute first identified human fibroblast HCMV infected cells (PMID: 24639214). The primary fibroblasts of the foreskin of neonates are available commercially (sometimes called HS68) and model neonatal HCMV infection. Neonatal HCMV, or Congenital Cytomegalovirus, is a leading cause of congenital infection and a significant cause of non-genetic hearing loss in the US (https://www.cdc.gov/cytomegalovirus/congenital-infection/index.html). While many infected newborns appear healthy at birth, a substantial percentage experience long-term health problems, including hearing loss, developmental delays, and vision problems (PMID: 39070527). 

      More information on ARPE-3 as a model of HCMV infection in humans

      HCMV retinitis is a leading cause of vision loss and results from HCMV infection of retinal cells. Retinal epithelial cells are the primary target for HCV infection in the eye. The cell line ARPE-19 is derived from a primary human adult retinal pigment epithelium explant and is commonly used to study HCMV and is thought to be physiologically relevant to the human infection (PMID: 8558129 and 28356702). When compared to primary retinal pigment epithelia, ARPE-19 cells develop a similar cellular and molecular phenotype to primary cells from adults and neonates (PMID: 28356702).

      (3) What is the rationale for using 48 hours' infection? Is this the typical timeframe for patients to develop symptoms?

      HCMV genes are expressed in a temporally controlled manner (PMID: 35417700). Early genes (within the first 4 hours) are involved in regulating transcription, while genes within 4-48 hours are involved in DNA replication and further transcriptional regulation. The 48 hour mark corresponds to the onset of significant viral replication and interactions between the virus and the host immune response. After 48 hours, late genes are expressed, which encode structural proteins as well as viral proteins that inhibit host anti-viral responses.  Most studies that focus on the role of HCMV’s early and immediate early genes are performed at 24 or 48 hours. Similarly, most studies that assess the initial innate immune response to HCMV are performed within the initial 48 hours after in vitro infection.

      In most people with healthy immune systems, there are no symptoms (PMID: 34168328). While 60% of people in developed countries and 90% of those in developing countries are serologically positive for past infection, it is challenging to study the kinetics of symptom development due to heterogeneity in the initial virion exposure, the cell types that are initially infected, and immune response. HCMV persists throughout the lifetime of the infected individual by establishing latent infection.

      Also, among all these large-scale global changes, what are primary and what are secondary?

      A kinetic study with many timepoints would be needed to identify the primary and secondary genomic changes associated with HCMV infection. These experiments, while exciting, are beyond the scope of this manuscript.

      (4) Fig.2: In addition to the changes for each cell type, comparison of unchanged, closed and opened with infection regions between the two cell types could be informative for commonalities and differences between cell types.

      This was a good suggestion.  We have added a new Supplemental Figure S2, which compares the differentially accessible regions between the two cell types:

      We have also added the following sentence to the Results section:

      “Comparison of differentially accessible chromatin between ARPE and HFF revealed that the vast majority of the HCMV-induced changes are specific to one of the two cell types (Supplemental Figure S2).”

      (5) "Of the 23,018 loops present in both infected and uninfected cells, only 10 are differential at a 2-fold cutoff and a false discovery rate (FDR) <0.01."

      We thank the reviewer for drawing our attention to the differential chromatin looping analysis.  Your comment prompted us to re-examine the methodologies we employed to identify differential chromatin looping events between uninfected and infected cells.  In the process, we realized that the relatively low resolution of chromatin looping assays such as HiChIP might require additional care in classifying a particular loop as shared or differential when comparing two experimental conditions. We have thus revamped our differential chromatin looping methodologies by adding 5kb “pads” to either end of each chromatin loop “anchor”.

      The corresponding passage now reads:

      “We next used the HiChIP data to identify HCMV-dependent differential chromatin looping events (see Methods). In total, uninfected cells have 143,882 loops. With HCMV infection, 90,198 of these loops are lost, and 44,045 new loops are gained (Supplemental Dataset 3). Because the number of altered loops was large, we repeated loop calling and differential analysis with FDR values less than 0.05, 0.01, and 0.001 (Supplemental Dataset 3). For all three cutoffs, the percentage of loops specific to an infection state were very similar. We also randomly downsampled the number of input pairs used for calling loops to verify that our results were not due to a difference in read depth (Supplemental Dataset 3). For the three smaller subsets of data, the number of loops specific to an infection state only changed slightly. The full quantification of each chromatin looping event and comparisons of events between conditions are provided in Supplemental Dataset 6.”

      Are these cells asynchronous and how to determine whether certain changes are not due to cell cycle stage differences?

      Cells were plated to an identical density of cells per well before either mock or HCMV infection for this study. Based on the differentially expressed genes cell cycle pathways were not amongst the top 50 enriched molecular pathways.

    1. Author response:

      General Statements:

      The formation of three-dimensional tubes is a fundamental process in the development of organs and aberrant tube size leads to common diseases and congenital disorders, such as polycystic kidney disease, asthma, and lung hypoplasia. The apical (luminal) extracellular matrix (ECM) plays a critical role in epithelial tube morphogenesis during organ formation, but its composition and organization remain poorly understood. Using the Drosophila embryonic salivary gland as a model, we reveal a critical role for the PAPS Synthetase (Papss), an enzyme that synthesizes the universal sulfate donor PAPS, as a critical regulator of tube lumen expansion. Additionally, we identify two zona pellucida (ZP) domain proteins, Piopio (Pio) and Dumpy (Dpy) as key apical ECM components that provide mechanical support to maintain a uniform tube diameter.

      The apical ECM has a distinct composition compared to the basal ECM, featuring a diverse array of components. Many studies of the apical ECM have focused on the role of chitin and its modification, but the composition of the non-chitinous apical ECM and its role, and how modification of the apical ECM affects organogenesis remain elusive. The main findings of this manuscript are listed below.

      (1) Through a deficiency screen targeting ECM-modifying enzymes, we identify Papss as a key enzyme regulating luminal expansion during salivary gland morphogenesis. 

      (2) Our confocal and transmission electron microscopy analyses reveal that Papss mutants exhibit a disorganized apical membrane and condensed aECM, which are at least partially linked to disruptions in Golgi structures and intracellular trafficking. Papss is also essential for cell survival and basal ECM integrity, highlighting the role of sulfation in regulating both apical and basal ECM.

      (3) Salivary gland-specific overexpression of wild-type Papss rescues all defects in Papss mutants, but the catalytically inactive mutant form does not, suggesting that defects in sulfation are the underlying cause of the phenotypes.

      (4) We identify two ZP domain proteins, Piopio (Pio) and Dumpy (Dpy), as key components of the salivary gland aECM. In the absence of Papss, Pio is progressively lost from the aECM, while the Dpy-positive aECM structure is condensed and detaches from the apical membrane, resulting in a narrowed lumen. 

      (5) Mutations in pio or dpy, or in Notopleural (Np), which encodes a matriptase that cleaves Pio, cause the salivary gland lumen to develop alternating bulges and constrictions. Additionally, loss of pio results in loss of Dpy in the salivary gland lumen, suggesting that the Dpycontaining filamentous structures of the aECM is critical for maintaining luminal diameter, with Pio playing an essential role in organizing this structure.

      (6) We further reveal that the cleavage of the ZP domain of Pio by Np is critical for the role of Pio in organizing the aECM structure.

      Overall, our findings underscore the essential role of sulfation in organizing the aECM during tubular organ formation and highlight the mechanical support provided by ZP domain proteins in maintaining tube diameter. Mammals have two isoforms of Papss, Papss1 and Papss2. Papss1 shows ubiquitous expression, with higher levels in glandular cells and salivary duct cells, suggesting a high requirement for sulfation in these cell types. Papss2 shows a more restricted expression, such as in cartilage, and mutations in Papss2 have been associated with skeletal dysplasia in humans. Our analysis of the Drosophila Papss gene, a single ortholog of human Papss1 and Papss2, reveals its multiple roles during salivary gland development. We expect that these findings will provide valuable insights into the function of these enzymes in normal development and disease in humans. Our findings on the key role of two ZP proteins, Pio and Dpy, as major components of the salivary gland aECM also provide valuable information on the organization of the non-chitinous aECM during organ formation.

      We believe that our results will be of broad interest to many cell and developmental biologists studying organogenesis and the ECM, as well as those investigating the mechanisms underlying human diseases associated with conserved mutations.

      Point-by-point description of the revisions:

      We are delighted that all three reviewers were enthusiastic about the work. Their comments and suggestions have improved the paper. The details of the changes we have made in response to each reviewer’s comments are included in italicized text below.

      Reviewer #1 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      PAPS is required for all sulfotransferase reactions in which a sulfate group is covalently attached to amino acid residues of proteins or to side chains of proteoglycans. This sulfation is crucial for properly organizing the apical extracellular matrix (aECM) and expanding the lumen in the Drosophila salivary gland. Loss of Papss potentially leads to decreased sulfation, disorganizing the aECM, and defects in lumen formation. In addition, Papss loss destabilizes the Golgi structures.

      In Papss mutants, several changes occur in the salivary gland lumen of Drosophila. The tube lumen is very thin and shows irregular apical protrusions. There is a disorganization of the apical membrane and a compaction of the apical extracellular matrix (aECM). The Golgi structures and intracellular transport are disturbed. In addition, the ZP domain proteins Piopio (Pio) and Dumpy (Dpy) lose their normal distribution in the lumen, which leads to condensation and dissociation of the Dpy-positive aECM structure from the apical membrane. This results in a thin and irregularly dilated lumen.

      (1) The authors describe various changes in the lumen in mutants, from thin lumen to irregular expansion. I would like to know the correct lumen diameter, and length, besides the total area, by which one can recognize thin and irregular.

      We have included quantification of the length and diameter of the salivary gland lumen in the stage 16 salivary glands of control, Papss mutant, and salivary gland-specific rescue embryos (Figure 1J, K). As described, Papss mutant embryos have two distinct phenotypes, one group with a thin lumen along the entire lumen and the other group with irregular lumen shapes. Therefore, we separated the two groups for quantification of lumen diameter. Additionally, we have analyzed the degree of variability for the lumen diameter to better capture the range of phenotypes observed (Figure 1K’). These quantifications enable a more precise assessment of lumen morphology, allowing readers to distinguish between thin and irregular lumen phenotypes.

      (2) The rescue is about 30%, which is not as good as expected. Maybe the wrong isoform was taken. Is it possible to find out which isoform is expressed in the salivary glands, e.g., by RNA in situ Hyb? This could then be used to analyze a more focused rescue beyond the paper.

      Thank you for this point, but we do not agree that the rescue is about 30%. In Papss mutants, about 50% of the embryos show the thin lumen phenotype whereas the other 50% show irregular lumen shapes. In the rescue embryos with a WT Papss, few embryos showed thin lumen phenotypes. About 40% of the rescue embryos showed “normal, fully expanded” lumen shapes, and the remaining 60% showed either irregular (thin+expanded) or slightly overexpanded lumen. It is not uncommon that rescue with the Gal4/UAS system results in a partial rescue because it is often not easy to achieve the balance of the proper amount of the protein with the overexpression system. 

      To address the possibility that the wrong isoform was used, we performed in situ hybridization to examine the expression of different Papss spice forms in the salivary gland. We used probes that detect subsets of splice forms: A/B/C/F/G, D/H, and E/F/H, and found that all probes showed expression in the salivary gland, with varying intensities. The original probe, which detects all splice forms, showed the strongest signals in the salivary gland compared to the new probes which detect only a subset. However, the difference in the signal intensity may be due to the longer length of the original probe (>800 bp) compared to other probes that were made with much smaller regions (~200 bp). Digoxigenin in the DIG labeling kit for mRNA detection labels the uridine nucleotide in the transcript, and the probes with weaker signals contain fewer uridines (all: 147; ABCFG, 29; D, 36; EFH, 66). We also used the Papss-PD isoform, for a salivary gland-specific rescue experiment and obtained similar results to those with Papss-PE (Figure 1I-L, Figure 4D and E). 

      Furthermore, we performed additional experiments to validate our findings. We performed a rescue experiment with a mutant form of Papss that has mutations in the critical rescues of the catalytic domains of the enzyme, which failed to rescue any phenotypes, including the thin lumen phenotype (Figure 1H, J-L), the number and intensity of WGA puncta (Figure 3I, I’), and cell death (Figure 4D, E). These results provide strong evidence that the defects observed in Papss mutants are due to the lack of sulfation.  

      (3) Crb is a transmembrane protein on the apicolateral side of the membrane. Accordingly, the apicolateral distribution can be seen in the control and the mutant. I believe there are no apparent differences here, not even in the amount of expression. However, the view of the cells (frame) shows possible differences. To be sure, a more in-depth analysis of the images is required. Confocal Z-stack images, with 3D visualization and orthogonal projections to analyze the membranes showing Crb staining together with a suitable membrane marker (e.g. SAS or Uif). This is the only way to show whether Crb is incorrectly distributed. Statistics of several papas mutants would also be desirable and not just a single representative image. When do the observed changes in Crb distribution occur in the development of the tubes, only during stage 16? Is papss only involved in the maintenance of the apical membrane? This is particularly important when considering the SJ and AJ, because the latter show no change in the mutants.

      We appreciate your suggestion more thoroughly analyze Crb distribution. We adapted a method from a previous study (Olivares-Castiñeira and Llimargas, 2017) to quantify Crb signals in the subapical region and apical free region of salivary gland cells. Using E-Cad signals as a reference, we marked the apical cell boundaries of individual cells and calculated the intensity of Crb signals in the subapical region (along the cell membrane) and in the apical free region. We focused on the expanded region of the SG lumen in Papss mutants for quantification, as the thin lumen region was challenging to analyze. This quantification is included in Figure 2D. Statistical analysis shows that Crb signals were more dispersed in SG cells in Papss mutants compared to WT.

      (4) A change in the ECM is only inferred based on the WGA localization. This is too few to make a clear statement. WGA is only an indirect marker of the cell surface and glycosylated proteins, but it does not indicate whether the ECM is altered in its composition and expression. Other important factors are missing here. In addition, only a single observation is shown, and statistics are missing.

      We understand your concern that WGA localization alone may not be sufficient to conclude changes in the ECM. However, we observed that luminal WGA signals colocalize with Dpy-YFP in the WT SG (Figure 5-figure supplement 2C), suggesting that WGA detects the aECM structure containing Dpy. The similar behavior of WGA and Dpy-YFP signals in multiple genotypes further supports this idea. In Papss mutants with a thin lumen phenotype, both WGA and Dpy-YFP signals are condensed (Figure 5E-H), and in pio mutants, both are absent from the lumen (Figure 6B, D). We analyzed WGA signals in over 25 samples of WT and Papss mutants, observing consistent phenotypes. We have included the number of samples in the text. While we acknowledge that WGA is an indirect marker, our data suggest that it is a reliable indicator of the aECM structure containing Dpy. 

      (5) Reduced WGA staining is seen in papss mutants, but this could be due to other circumstances. To be sure, a statistic with the number of dots must be shown, as well as an intensity blot on several independent samples. The images are from single confocal sections. It could be that the dots appear in a different Z-plane. Therefore, a 3D visualization of the voxels must be shown to identify and, at best, quantify the dots in the organ.

      We have quantified cytoplasmic punctate WGA signals. Using spinning disk microscopy with super-resolution technology (Olympus SpinSR10 Sora), we obtained high-resolution images of cytoplasmic punctate signals of WGA in WT, Papss mutant, and rescue SGs with the WT and mutant forms of Papss-PD. We then generated 3D reconstructed images of these signals using Imaris software (Figure 3E-H) and quantified the number and intensity of puncta. Statistical analysis of these data confirms the reduction of the number and intensity of WGA puncta in Papss mutants (Figure 3I, I’). The number of WGA puncta was restored by expressing WT Papss but not the mutant form. By using 3D visualization and quantification, we have ensured that our results are not limited to a single confocal section and account for potential variations in Z-plane localization of the dots.

      (6) A colocalization analysis (statistics) should be shown for the overlap of WGA with ManII-GFP.

      Since WGA labels multiple structures, including the nuclear envelope and ECM structures, we focused on assessing the colocalization of the cytoplasmic WGA punctate signals and ManIIGFP signals. Standard colocalization analysis methods, such as Pearson’s correlation coefficient or Mander’s overlap coefficient, would be confounded by WGA signals in other tissues. Therefore, we used a fluorescent intensity line profile to examine the spatial relationship between WGA and ManII-GFP signals in WT and Papss mutants (Figure 3L, L’). 

      (7) I do not understand how the authors describe "statistics of secretory vesicles" as an axis in Figure 3p. The TEM images do not show labeled secretory vesicles but empty structures that could be vesicles.

      Previous studies have analyzed “filled” electron-dense secretory vesicles in TEM images of SG cells (Myat and Andrew, 2002, Cell; Fox et al., 2010, J Cell Biol; Chung and Andrew, 2014, Development). Consistent with these studies, our WT TEM images show these vesicles. In contrast, Papss mutants show a mix of filled and empty structures. For quantification, we specifically counted the filled electron-dense vesicles (now Figure 3W). A clear description of our analysis is provided in the figure legend.

      (8) The quality of the presented TEM images is too low to judge any difference between control and mutants. Therefore, the supplement must present them in better detail (higher pixel number?).

      We disagree that the quality of the presented TEM images is too low. Our TEM images have sufficient resolution to reveal details of many subcellular structures, such as mitochondrial cisternae. The pdf file of the original submission may not have been high resolution. To address this concern, we have provided several original high-quality TEM images of both WT and Papss mutants at various magnifications in Figure 2-figure supplement 2. Additionally, we have included low-magnification TEM images of WT and Papss mutants in Figure 2H and I to provide a clearer view of the overall SG lumen morphology. 

      (9) Line 266: the conclusion that apical trafficking is "significantly impaired" does not hold. This implies that Papss is essential for apical trafficking, but the analyzed ECM proteins (Pio, Dumpy) are found apically enriched in the mutants, and Dumpy is even secreted. Moreover, they analyze only one marker, Sec15, and don't provide data about the quantification of the secretion of proteins.

      We agree and have revised our statement to “defective sulfation affects Golgi structures and multiple routes of intracellular trafficking”. 

      (10) DCP-1 was used to detect apoptosis in the glands to analyze acellular regions. However, the authors compare ST16 control with ST15 mutant salivary glands, which is problematic. Further, it is not commented on how many embryos were analyzed and how often they detect the dying cells in control and mutant embryos. This part must be improved.

      Thank you for the comment. We agree and have included quantification. We used stage 16 samples from WT and Papss mutants to quantify acellular regions. Since DCP-1 signals are only present at a specific stage of apoptosis, some acellular regions do not show DCP-1 signals. Therefore, we counted acellular regions regardless of DCP-1 signals. We also quantified this in rescue embryos with WT and mutant forms of Papss, which show complete rescue with WT and no rescue with the mutant form, respectively. The graph with a statistical analysis is included (Figure 4D, E).

      (11) WGA and Dumpy show similar condensed patterns within the tube lumen. The authors show that dumpy is enriched from stage 14 onwards. How is it with WGA? Does it show the same pattern from stage 14 to 16? Papss mutants can suffer from a developmental delay in organizing the ECM or lack of internalization of luminal proteins during/after tube expansion, which is the case in the trachea.

      Dpy-YFP and WGA show overlapping signals in the SG lumen throughout morphogenesis. DpyYFP is SG enriched in the lumen from stage 11, not stage 14 (Figure 5-figure supplement 2). WGA is also detected in the lumen throughout SG morphogenesis, similar to Dpy. In the original supplemental figure, only a stage 16 SG image was shown for co-localization of Dpy-YFP and WGA signals in the SG lumen. We have now included images from stage 14 and 15 in Figure 5figure supplement 2C. 

      Given that luminal Pio signals are lost at stage 16 only and that Dpy signals appear as condensed structures in the lumen of Papss mutants, it suggests that the internalization of luminal proteins is not impaired in Papss mutants. Rather, these proteins are secreted but fail to organize properly. 

      (12) Line 366. Luminal morphology is characterized by bulging and constrictions. In the trachea, bulges indicate the deformation of the apical membrane and the detachment from the aECM. I can see constrictions and the collapsed tube lumen in Fig. 6C, but I don't find the bulges of the apical membrane in pio and Np mutants. Maybe showing it more clearly and with better quality will be helpful.

      Since the bulging phenotype appears to vary from sample to sample, we have revised the description of the phenotype to “constrictions” to more accurately reflect the consistent observations. We quantified the number of constrictions along the entire lumen in pio and Np mutants and included the graph in Figure 6F.

      (13) The authors state that Papss controls luminal secretion of Pio and Dumpy, as they observe reduced luminal staining of both in papss mutants. However, the mCh-Pio and Dumpy-YFP are secreted towards the lumen. Does papss overexpression change Pio and Dumpy secretion towards the lumen, and could this be another explanation for the multiple phenotypes? 

      Thank you for the comment. To clarify, we did not observe reduced luminal staining of Pio and Dpy in Papss mutants, nor did we state that Papss controls luminal secretion of Pio and Dpy. In Papss mutants, Pio luminal signals are absent specifically at stage 16 (Figure 5H), whereas strong luminal Pio signals are present until stage 15 (Figure 5G). For Dpy-YFP, the signals are not reduced but condensed in Papss mutants from stages 14-16 (Figure 5D, H). 

      It remains unclear whether the apparent loss of Pio signals is due to a loss of Pio protein in the lumen or due to epitope masking resulting from protein aggregation or condensation. As noted in our response to Comment 11 internalization of luminal proteins seems unaffected in Papss mutants; proteins like Pio and Dpy are secreted into the lumen but fail to properly organize. Therefore, we have not tested whether Papss overexpression alters the secretion of Pio or Dpy.

      In our original submission, we incorrectly stated that uniform luminal mCh-Pio signals were unchanged in Papss mutants. Upon closer examination, we found these signals are absent in the expanded luminal region in stage 16 SG (where Dpy-YFP is also absent), and weak mCh-Pio signals colocalize with the condensed Dpy-YFP signals (Figure 5C, D). We have revised the text accordingly. 

      Regulation of luminal ZP protein level is essential to modulate the tube expansion; therefore, Np releases Pio and Dumpy in a controlled manner during st15/16. Thus, the analysis of Pio and Dumpy in NP overexpression embryos will be critical to this manuscript to understand more about the control of luminal ZP matrix proteins.

      Thanks for the insightful suggestion. We overexpressed both the WT and mutant form of Np using UAS-Np.WT and UAS-Np.S990A lines (Drees et al., 2019) and analyzed mCh-Pio, Pio antibody, and Dpy-YFP signals. It is important to note that these overexpression experiments were done in the presence of the endogenous WT Np. 

      Overexpression of Np.WT led to increased levels of mCh-Pio, Pio, and Dpy-YFP signals in the lumen and at the apical membrane. In contrast, overexpression of Np.S990A resulted in a near complete loss of luminal mCh-Pio signals. Pio antibody signals remained strong at the apical membrane but was weaker in the luminal filamentous structures compared to WT. 

      Due to the GFP tag present in the UAS-Np.S990A line, we could not reliably analyze Dpy-YFP signals because of overlapping fluorescent signals in the same channel. However, the filamentous Pio signals in the lumen co-localized with GFP signals, suggesting that these structures might also include Dpy-YFP, although this cannot be confirmed definitively. 

      These results suggest that overexpressed Np.S990A may act in a dominant-negative manner, competing with endogenous Np and impairing proper cleavage of Pio (and mCh-Pio). Nevertheless, some level of cleavage by endogenous Np still appears to occur, as indicated by the residual luminal filamentous Pio signals. These new findings have been incorporated into the revised manuscript and are shown in Figure 6H and 6I.

      (14) Minor:

      Fig. 5 C': mChe-Pio and Dumpy-YFP are mixed up at the top of the images.

      Thanks for catching this error.  It has been corrected.

      Sup. Fig7. A shows Pio in purple but B in green. Please indicate it correctly.

      It has been corrected.

      Reviewer #1 (Significance):

      In 2023, the functions of Pio, Dumpy, and Np in the tracheal tubes of Drosophila were published. The study here shows similar results, with the difference that the salivary glands do not possess chitin, but the two ZP proteins Pio and Dumpy take over its function. It is, therefore, a significant and exciting extension of the known function of the three proteins to another tube system. In addition, the authors identify papss as a new protein and show its essential function in forming the luminal matrix in the salivary glands. Considering the high degree of conservation of these proteins in other species, the results presented are crucial for future analyses and will have further implications for tubular development, including humans.

      Reviewer #2 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      Summary:

      There is growing appreciation for the important of luminal (apical) ECM in tube development, but such matrices are much less well understood than basal ECMs. Here the authors provide insights into the aECM that shapes the Drosophila salivary gland (SG) tube and the importance of PAPSS-dependent sulfation in its organization and function.

      The first part of the paper focuses on careful phenotypic characterization of papss mutants, using multiple markers and TEM. This revealed reduced markers of sulfation (Alcian Blue staining) and defects in both apical and basal ECM organization, Golgi (but not ER) morphology, number and localization of other endosomal compartments, plus increased cell death. The authors focus on the fact that papss mutants have an irregular SG lumen diameter, with both narrowed regions and bulged regions. They address the pleiotropy, showing that preventing the cell death and resultant gaps in the tube did not rescue the SG luminal shape defects and discussing similarities and differences between the papss mutant phenotype and those caused by more general trafficking defects. The analysis uses a papss nonsense mutant from an EMS screen - I appreciate the rigorous approach the authors took to analyze transheterozygotes (as well as homozygotes) plus rescued animals in order to rule out effects of linked mutations.

      The 2nd part of the paper focuses on the SG aECM, showing that Dpy and Pio ZP protein fusions localize abnormally in papss mutants and that these ZP mutants (and Np protease mutants) have similar SG lumen shaping defects to the papss mutants. A key conclusion is that SG lumen defects correlate with loss of a Pio+Dpy-dependent filamentous structure in the lumen. These data suggest that ZP protein misregulation could explain this part of the papss phenotype.

      Overall, the text is very well written and clear. Figures are clearly labeled. The methods involve rigorous genetic approaches, microscopy, and quantifications/statistics and are documented appropriately. The findings are convincing, with just a few things about the fusions needing clarification.

      Minor comments

      (1) Although the Dpy and Qsm fusions are published reagents, it would still be helpful to mention whether the tags are C-terminal as suggested by the nomenclature, and whether Westerns have been performed, since (as discussed for Pio) cleavage could also affect the appearance of these fusions.

      Thanks for the comment. Dpy-YFP is a knock-in line in which YFP is inserted into the middle of the dpy locus (Lye et al., 2014; the insertion site is available on Flybase). mCh-Qsm is also a knock-in line, with mCh inserted near the N-terminus of the qsm gene using phi-mediated recombination using the qsm<sup>MI07716</sup> line (Chu and Hayashi, 2021; insertion site available on Flybase). Based on this, we have updated the nomenclature from Qsm-mCh to mCh-Qsm throughout the manuscript to accurately reflect the tag position. To our knowledge, no western blot has been performed on Dpy-YFP or mCh-Qsm lines. We have mentioned this explicitly in the Discussion.  

      (2) The Dpy-YFP reagent is a non-functional fusion and therefore may not be a wholly reliable reporter of Dpy localization. There is no antibody confirmation. As other reagents are not available to my knowledge, this issue can be addressed with text acknowledgement of possible caveats.

      Thanks for raising this important point. We have added a caveat in the Discussion noting this limitation and the need for additional tools, such as an antibody or a functional fusion protein, to confirm the localization of Dpy.

      (3) TEM was done by standard chemical fixation, which is fine for viewing intracellular organelles, but high pressure freezing probably would do a better job of preserving aECM structure, which looks fairly bad in Fig. 2G WT, without evidence of the filamentous structures seen by light microscopy. Nevertheless, the images are sufficient for showing the extreme disorganization of aECM in papss mutants.

      We agree that HPF is a better method and intent to use the HPF system in future studies. We acknowledge that chemical fixation contributes to the appearance of a gap between the apical membrane and the aECM, which we did not observe in the HPF/FS method (Chung and Andrew, 2014). Despite this, the TEM images still clearly reveal that Papss mutants show a much thinner and more electron-dense aECM compared to WT (Figure 2H, I), consistent to the condensed WGA, Dpy, and Pio signals in our confocal analyses. As the reviewer mentioned, we believe that the current TEM data are sufficient to support the conclusion of severe aECM disorganization and Golgi defects in Papss mutants.

      (4) The authors may consider citing some of the work that has been done on sulfation in nematodes, e.g. as reviewed here: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/35223994/ Sulfation has been tied to multiple aspects of nematode aECM organization, though not specifically to ZP proteins.

      Thank you for the suggestion. Pioneering studies in C. elegans have highlighted the key role of sulfation in diverse developmental processes, including neuronal organization, reproductive tissue development, and phenotypic plasticity. We have now cited several works.  

      Reviewer #2 (Significance):

      This study will be of interest to researchers studying developmental morphogenesis in general and specifically tube biology or the aECM. It should be particularly of interest to those studying sulfation or ZP proteins (which are broadly present in aECMs across organisms, including humans).

      This study adds to the literature demonstrating the importance of luminal matrix in shaping tubular organs and greatly advances understanding of the luminal matrix in the Drosophila salivary gland, an important model of tubular organ development and one that has key matrix differences (such as no chitin) compared to other highly studied Drosophila tubes like the trachea.

      The detailed description of the defects resulting from papss loss suggests that there are multiple different sulfated targets, with a subset specifically relevant to aECM biology. A limitation is that specific sulfated substrates are not identified here (e.g. are these the ZP proteins themselves or other matrix glycoproteins or lipids?); therefore it's not clear how direct or indirect the effects of papss are on ZP proteins. However, this is clearly a direction for future work and does not detract from the excellent beginning made here.

      My expertise: I am a developmental geneticist with interests in apical ECM

      Reviewer #3 (Evidence, reproducibility and clarity):

      In this work Woodward et al focus on the apical extracellular matrix (aECM) in the tubular salivary gland (SG) of Drosophila. They provide new insights into the composition of this aECM, formed by ZP proteins, in particular Pio and Dumpy. They also describe the functional requirements of PAPSS, a critical enzyme involved in sulfation, in regulating the expansion of the lumen of the SG. A detailed cellular analysis of Papss mutants indicate defects in the apical membrane, the aECM and in Golgi organization. They also find that Papss control the proper organization of the Pio-Dpy matrix in the lumen. The work is well presented and the results are consistent.

      Main comments

      - This work provides a detailed description of the defects produced by the absence of Papss. In addition, it provides many interesting observations at the cellular and tissular level. However, this work lacks a clear connection between these observations and the role of sulfation. Thus, the mechanisms underlying the phenotypes observed are elusive. Efforts directed to strengthen this connection (ideally experimentally) would greatly increase the interest and relevance of this work.

      Thank you for this thoughtful comment. To directly test whether the phenotypes observed in Papss mutants are due to the loss of sulfation activity, we generated transgenic lines expressing catalytically inactive forms of Papss, UAS-PapssK193A, F593P, in which key residues in the APS kinase and ATP sulfurylase domains are mutated. Unlike WT UAS-Papss (both the Papss-PD or Papss-PE isoforms), the catalytically inactive UAS-Papssmut failed to rescue any of the phenotypes, including the thin lumen phenotype (Figure 1I-L), altered WGA signals (Figure I, I’) and the cell death phenotype (Figure 4D, E). These findings strongly support the conclusion that the enzymatic sulfation activity of Papss is essential for the developmental processes described in this study.  

      - A main issue that arises from this work is the role of Papss at the cellular level. The results presented convincingly indicate defects in Golgi organization in Papss mutants. Therefore, the defects observed could stem from general defects in the secretion pathway rather than from specific defects on sulfation. This could even underly general/catastrophic cellular defects and lead to cell death (as observed).

      This observation has different implications. Is this effect observed in SGs also observed in other cells in the embryo? If Papss has a general role in Golgi organization this would be expected, as Papss encodes the only PAPs synthatase in Drosophila.

      Can the authors test any other mutant that specifically affect Golgi organization and investigate whether this produces a similar phenotype to that of Papss?

      Thank you for the comment. To address whether the defects observed in Papss mutants stem from general disruption of the secretory pathway due to Golgi disorganization, we examined mutants of two key Golgi components: Grasp65 and GM130. 

      In Grasp65 mutants, we observed significant defects in SG lumen morpholgy, including highly irregular SG lumen shape and multiple constrictions (100%; n=10/10). However, the lumen was not uniformly thin as in Papss mutants. In contrast, GM130 mutants–although this line was very sick and difficult to grow–showed relatively normal salivary glands morphology in the few embryos that survived to stage 16 (n=5/5). It is possible that only embryos with mild phenotypes progressed to this stages, limiting interpretation. These data have now been included in Figure 3-figure supplement 2. Overall, while Golgi disruption can affect SG morphology, the specific phenotypes seen in Papss mutants are not fully recapitulated by Grasp65 or GM130 loss. 

      - A model that conveys the different observations and that proposes a function for Papss in sulfation and Golgi organization (independent or interdependent?) would help to better present the proposed conclusions. In particular, the paper would be more informative if it proposed a mechanism or hypothesis of how sulfation affects SG lumen expansion. Is sulfation regulating a factor that in turn regulates Pio-Dpy matrix? Is it regulating Pio-Dpy directly? Is it regulating a

      product recognized by WGA?

      For instance, investigating Alcian blue or sulfotyrosine staining in pio, dpy mutants could help to understand whether Pio, Dpy are targets of sulfation.

      Thank you for the comment. We’re also very interested in learning whether the regulation of the Pio-Dpy matrix is a direct or indirect consequence of the loss of sulfation on these proteins. One possible scenario is that sulfation directly regulates the Pio-Dpy matrix by regulating protein stability through the formation of disulfide bonds between the conserved Cys residues responsible for ZP module polymerization. Additionally, the Dpy protein contains hundreds of EGF modules that are highly susceptible to O-glycosylation. Sulfation of the glycan groups attached to Dpy may be critical for its ability to form a filamentous structure. Without sulfation, the glycan groups on Dpy may not interact properly with the surrounding materials in the lumen, resulting in an aggregated and condensed structure. These possibilities are discussed in the Discussion.

      We have not analyzed sulfation levels in pio or dpy mutants because sulfation levels in mutants of single ZP domain proteins may not provide much information. A substantial number of proteoglycans, glycoproteins, and proteins (with up to 1% of all tyrosine residues in an organism’s proteins estimated to be sulfated) are modified by sulfation, so changes in sulfation levels in a single mutant may be subtle. Especially, the existing dpy mutant line is an insertion mutant of a transposable element; therefore, the sulfation sites would still remain in this mutant. 

      - Interpretation of Papss effects on Pio and Dpy would be desired. The results presented indicate loss of Pio antibody staining but normal presence of cherry-Pio. This is difficult to interpret. How are these results of Pio antibody and cherry-Pio correlating with the results in the trachea described recently (Drees et al. 2023)?

      In our original submission, we stated that the uniform luminal mCh-Pio signals were not changed in Papss mutants, but after re-analysis, we found that these signals were actually absent from the expanded luminal region in stage 16 SG (where Dpy-YFP is also absent), and weak mCh-Pio signals colocalize with the condensed Dpy-YFP signals (Figure 5C, D). We have revised the text accordingly. 

      After cleavages by Np and furin, the Pio protein should have three fragments. The Nterminal region contains the N-terminal half of the ZP domain, and mCh-Pio signals show this fragment. The very C-terminal region should localize to the membrane as it contains the transmembrane domain. We think the middle piece, the C-terminal ZP domain, is recognized by the Pio antibody. The mCh-Pio and Pio antibody signals in the WT trachea (Drees et al., 2023) are similar to those in the SG. mCh-Pio signals are detected in the tracheal lumen as uniform signals, at the apical membrane, and in cytoplasmic puncta. Pio antibody signals are exclusively in the tracheal lumen and show more heterogenous filamentous signals. 

      In Papss mutants, the middle fragment (the C-terminal ZP domain) seems to be most affected because the Pio antibody signals are absent from the lumen. The loss of Pio antibody signals could be due to protein degradation or epitope masking caused by aECM condensation and protein misfolding. This fragment seems to be key for interacting with Dpy, since Pio antibody signals always colocalize with Dpy-YFP. The N-terminal mCh-Pio fragment does not appear to play a significant role in forming a complex with Dpy in WT (but still aggregated together in Papss mutants), and this can be tested in future studies.

      In response to Reviewer 1’s comment, we performed an additional experiment to test the role of Np in cleaving Pio to help organize the SG aECM. In this experiment, we overexpressed the WT and mutant form of Np using UAS-Np.WT and UAS-Np.S990A lines (Drees et al., 2019) and analyzed mCh-Pio, Pio antibody, and Dpy-YFP signals. Np.WT overexpression resulted in increased levels of mCh-Pio, Pio, and Dpy-YFP signals in the lumen and at the apical membrane. However, overexpression of Np.S990A resulted in the absence of luminal mCh-Pio signals. Pio antibody signals were strong at the apical membrane but rather weak in the luminal filamentous structures. Since the UAS-Np.S990A line has the GFP tag, we could not reliably analyze Dpy-YFP signals due to overlapping Np.S990A.GFP signals in the same channel. However, the luminal filamentous Pio signals co-localized with GFP signals, and we assume that these overlapping signals could be Dpy-YFP signals. 

      These results suggest that overexpressed Np.S990A may act in a dominant-negative manner, competing with endogenous Np and impairing proper cleavage of Pio (and mCh-Pio). Nevertheless, some level of cleavage by endogenous Np still appears to occur, as indicated by the residual luminal filamentous Pio signals. These new findings have been incorporated into the revised manuscript and are shown in Figure 6H and 6I. 

      A proposed model of the Pio-Dpy aECM in WT, Papss, pio, and Np mutants has now been included in Figure 7.

      -  What does the WGA staining in the lumen reveal? This staining seems to be affected differently in pio and dpy mutants: in pio mutants it disappears from the lumen (as dpy-YFP does), but in dpy mutants it seems to be maintained. How do the authors interpret these findings? How does the WGA matrix relate to sulfated products (using Alcian blue or sulfotyrosine)?

      WGA binds to sialic acid and N-acetylglucosamine (GlcNAc) residues on glycoproteins and glycolipids. GlcNAc is a key component of the glycosaminoglycan (GAG) chains that are covalently attached to the core protein of a proteoglycan, which is abundant in the ECM. We think WGA detects GlcNAc residues in the components of the aECM, including Dpy as a core component, based on the following data. 1) WGA and Dpy colocalize in the lumen, both in WT (as thin filamentous structures) and Papss mutant background (as condensed rod-like structures), and 2) are absent in pio mutants. WGA signals are still present in a highly condensed form in dpy mutants. That’s probably because the dpy mutant allele (dpyov1) has an insertion of a transposable element (blood element) into intron 11 and this insertion may have caused the Dpy protein to misfold and condense. We added the information about the dpy allele to the Results section and discussed it in the Discussion.

      Minor points:

      - The morphological phenotypic analysis of Papss mutants (homozygous and transheterozygous) is a bit confusing. The general defects are higher in Papss homozygous than in transheterozygotes over a deficiency. Maybe quantifying the defects in the heterozygote embryos in the Papss mutant collection could help to figure out whether these defects relate to Papss mutation.

      We analyzed the morphology of heterozygous Papss mutant embryos. They were all normal. The data and quantifications have now been added to Figure 1-figure supplement 3. 

      - The conclusion that the apical membrane is affected in Papss mutants is not strongly supported by the results presented with the pattern of Crb (Fig 2). Further evidences should be provided. Maybe the TEM analysis could help to support this conclusion

      We quantified Crb levels in the sub-apical and medial regions of the cell and included this new quantification in Figure 2D. TEM images showed variation in the irregularity of the apical membrane, even in WT, and we could not draw a solid conclusion from these images.

      - It is difficult to understand why in Papss mutants the levels of WGA increase. Can the authors elaborate on this?

      We think that when Dpy (and many other aECM components) are condensed and aggregated into the thin, rod-like structure in Papss mutants, the sugar residues attached to them must also be concentrated and shown as increased WGA signals.   

      - The explanation about why Pio antibody and mcherry-Pio show different patterns is not clear. If the antibody recognizes the C-t region, shouldn't it be clearly found at the membrane rather than the lumen?

      The Pio protein is also cleaved by furin protease (Figure 5B). We think the Pio fragment recognized by the antibody should be a “C-terminal ZP domain”, which is a middle piece after furin + Np cleavages. 

      - The qsm information does not seem to provide any relevant information to the aECM, or sulfation.

      Since Qsm has been shown to bind to Dpy and remodel Dpy filaments in the muscle tendon (Chu and Hayashi, 2021), we believe that the different behavior of Qsm in the SG is still informative. As mentioned briefly in the Discussion, the cleaved Qsm fragment may localize differently, like Pio, and future work will need to test this. We have shortened the description of the Qsm localization in the manuscript and moved the details to the figure legend of Figure 5-figure supplement 3.

      Reviewer #3 (Significance):

      Previous reports already indicated a role for Papss in sulfation in SG (Zhu et al 2005). Now this work provides a more detailed description of the defects produced by the absence of Papss. In addition, it provides relevant data related to the nature and requirements of the aECM in the SG. Understanding the composition and requirements of aECM during organ formation is an important question. Therefore, this work may be relevant in the fields of cell biology and morphogenesis.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors use anatomical tracing and slice physiology to investigate the integration of thalamic (ATN) and retrosplenial cortical (RSC) signals in the dorsal presubiculum (PrS). This work will be of interest to the field, as the postsubiculum is thought to be a key region for integrating internal head direction representations with external landmarks. The main result is that ATN and RSC inputs drive the same L3 PrS neurons, which exhibit superlinear summation to near-coincident inputs. Moreover, this activity can induce bursting in L4 PrS neurons, which can pass the signals LMN (perhaps gated by cholinergic input).

      Strengths:

      The slice physiology experiments are carefully done. The analyses are clear and convincing, and the figures and results are well-composed. Overall, these results will be a welcome addition to the field.

      We thank this reviewer for the positive comment on our work.

      Weaknesses:

      The conclusions about the circuit-level function of L3 PrS neurons sometimes outstrip the data, and their model of the integration of these inputs is unclear. I would recommend some revision of the introduction and discussion. I also had some minor comments about the experimental details and analysis.

      Specific major comments:

      (1) I found that the authors' claims sometimes outstrip their data, given that there were no in vivo recordings during behavior. For example, in the abstract, their results indicate "that layer 3 neurons can transmit a visually matched HD signal to medial entorhinal cortex", and in the conclusion they state "[...] cortical RSC projections that carry visual landmark information converge on layer 3 pyramidal cells of the dorsal presubiculum". However, they never measured the nature of the signals coming from ATN and RSC to L3 PrS (or signals sent to downstream regions). Their claim is somewhat reasonable with respect to ATN, where the majority of neurons encode HD, but neurons in RSC encode a vast array of spatial and non-spatial variables other than landmark information (e.g., head direction, egocentric boundaries, allocentric position, spatial context, task history to name a few), so making strong claims about the nature of the incoming signals is unwarranted.

      We agree of course that RSC does not only encode landmark information. We have clarified this point in the introduction (line 69-70) and formulated more carefully in the abstract (removed the word ‘landmark’ in line 17) and in the  introduction (line 82-83). In the discussion we explicitly state that ‘In our slice work we are blind to the exact nature of the signal that is carried by ATN and RSC axons’ (line 522-523).

      (2) Related to the first point, the authors hint at, but never explain, how coincident firing of ATN and RSC inputs would help anchor HD signals to visual landmarks. Although the lesion data (Yoder et al. 2011 and 2015) support their claims, it would be helpful if the proposed circuit mechanism was stated explicitly (a schematic of their model would be helpful in understanding the logic). For example, how do neurons integrate the "right" sets of landmarks and HD signals to ensure stable anchoring? Moreover, it would be helpful to discuss alternative models of HD-to-landmark anchoring, including several studies that have proposed that the integration may (also?) occur in RSC (Page & Jeffrey, 2018; Yan, Burgess, Bicanski, 2021; Sit & Goard, 2023). Currently, much of the Discussion simply summarizes the results of the study, this space could be better used in mapping the findings to the existing literature on the overarching question of how HD signals are anchored to landmarks.

      We agree with the reviewer on the importance of the question, how do neurons integrate the “right” sets of landmarks and HD signals to ensure stable anchoring? Based on our results we provide a schematic to illustrate possible scenarios, and we include it as a supplementary figure (Figure 1, to be included in the ms as Figure 7—figure supplement 2), as well as a new paragraph in the discussion section (line 516-531).  We point out that critical information on the convergence and divergence of functionally defined inputs is still lacking, both for principal cells and interneurons

      Interestingly, recent evidence from functional ultrasound imaging and electrical single cell recording demonstrated that visual objects may refine head direction coding, specifically in the dorsal presubiculum (Siegenthaler et al. bioRxiv 2024.10.21.619417; doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.10.21.619417). The increase in firing rate for HD cells whose preferred firing direction corresponds to a visual landmark could be supported by the supralinear summation of thalamic HD signals and retrosplenial input described in our study. We include this point in the discussion (line 460-462), and hope that our work will spur further investigations.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Richevaux et al investigate how anterior thalamic (AD) and retrosplenial (RSC) inputs are integrated by single presubicular (PrS) layer 3 neurons. They show that these two inputs converge onto single PrS layer 3 principal cells. By performing dual-wavelength photostimulation of these two inputs in horizontal slices, the authors show that in most layer 3 cells, these inputs summate supra-linearly. They extend the experiments by focusing on putative layer 4 PrS neurons, and show that they do not receive direct anterior thalamic nor retrosplenial inputs; rather, they are (indirectly) driven to burst firing in response to strong activation of the PrS network.

      This is a valuable study, that investigates an important question - how visual landmark information (possibly mediated by retrosplenial inputs) converges and integrates with HD information (conveyed by the AD nucleus of the thalamus) within PrS circuitry. The data indicate that near-coincident activation of retrosplenial and thalamic inputs leads to non-linear integration in target layer 3 neurons, thereby offering a potential biological basis for landmark + HD binding.

      The main limitations relate to the anatomical annotation of 'putative' PrS L4 neurons, and to the presentation of retrosplenial/thalamic input modularity. Specifically, more evidence should be provided to convincingly demonstrate that the 'putative L4 neurons' of the PrS are not distal subicular neurons (as the authors' anatomy and physiology experiments seem to indicate). The modularity of thalamic and retrosplenial inputs could be better clarified in relation to the known PrS modularity.

      We thank the reviewer for their important feedback. We discuss what defines presubicular layer 4 in horizontal slices, cite relevant literature, and provide new and higher resolution images. See below for detailed responses to the reviewer’s comments, in the section ‘recommendations to authors’.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors sought to determine, at the level of individual presubiculum pyramidal cells, how allocentric spatial information from the retrosplenial cortex was integrated with egocentric information from the anterior thalamic nuclei. Employing a dual opsin optogenetic approach with patch clamp electrophysiology, Richevaux, and colleagues found that around three-quarters of layer 3 pyramidal cells in the presubiculum receive monosynaptic input from both brain regions. While some interesting questions remain (e.g. the role of inhibitory interneurons in gating the information flow and through different layers of presubiculum, this paper provides valuable insights into the microcircuitry of this brain region and the role that it may play in spatial navigation).

      Strengths:

      One of the main strengths of this manuscript was that the dual opsin approach allowed the direct comparison of different inputs within an individual neuron, helping to control for what might otherwise have been an important source of variation. The experiments were well-executed and the data was rigorously analysed. The conclusions were appropriate to the experimental questions and were well-supported by the results. These data will help to inform in vivo experiments aimed at understanding the contribution of different brain regions in spatial navigation and could be valuable for computational modelling.

      Weaknesses:

      Some attempts were made to gain mechanistic insights into how inhibitory neurotransmission may affect processing in the presubiculum (e.g. Figure 5) but these experiments were a little underpowered and the analysis carried out could have been more comprehensively undertaken, as was done for other experiments in the manuscript.

      We agree that the role of interneurons for landmark anchoring through convergence in Presubiculum requires further investigation. In our latest work on the recruitment of VIP interneurons we begin to address this point in slices (Nassar et al., 2024 Neuroscience. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2024.09.032.); more work in behaving animals will be needed.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Full comments below. Beyond the (mostly minor) issues noted below, this is a very well-written paper and I look forward to seeing it in print.

      Major comments:

      (1) I found that the authors' claims sometimes outstrip their data, given that there were no in vivo recordings during behavior. For example, in the abstract, their results indicate "that layer 3 neurons can transmit a visually matched HD signal to medial entorhinal cortex", and in the conclusion they state "[...] cortical RSC projections that carry visual landmark information converge on layer 3 pyramidal cells of the dorsal presubiculum". However, they never measured the nature of the signals coming from ATN and RSC to L3 PrS (or signals sent to downstream regions). Their claim is somewhat reasonable with respect to ATN, where the majority of neurons encode HD, but neurons in RSC encode a vast array of spatial and non-spatial variables other than landmark information (e.g., head direction, egocentric boundaries, allocentric position, spatial context, task history to name a few), so making strong claims about the nature of the incoming signals is unwarranted.

      Our study was motivated by the seminal work from Yoder et al., 2011 and 2015, indicating that visual landmark information is processed in PoS and from there transmitted to the LMN.  Based on that, and in the interest of readability, we may have used an oversimplified shorthand for the type of signal carried by RSC axons. There are numerous studies indicating a role for RSC in encoding visual landmark information (Auger et al., 2012; Jacob et al., 2017; Lozano et al., 2017; Fischer et al., 2020; Keshavarzi et al., 2022; Sit and Goard, 2023); we agree of course that this is certainly not the only variable that is represented. Therefore we change the text to make this point clear:

      Abstract, line 17: removed the word ‘landmark’

      Introduction, line 69: added “...and supports an array of cognitive functions including memory, spatial and non-spatial context and navigation (Vann et al., 2009; Vedder et al., 2017). ”

      Introduction, line 82: changed “...designed to examine the convergence of visual landmark information, that is possibly integrated in the RSC, and vestibular based thalamic head direction signals”.

      Discussion, line 522-523: added “In our slice work we are blind to the exact nature of the signal that is carried by ATN and RSC axons.”

      (2) Related to the first point, the authors hint at, but never explain, how coincident firing of ATN and RSC inputs would help anchor HD signals to visual landmarks. Although the lesion data (Yoder et al., 2011 and 2015) support their claims, it would be helpful if the proposed circuit mechanism was stated explicitly (a schematic of their model would be helpful in understanding the logic). For example, how do neurons integrate the "right" sets of landmarks and HD signals to ensure stable anchoring? Moreover, it would be helpful to discuss alternative models of HD-to-landmark anchoring, including several studies that have proposed that the integration may (also?) occur in RSC (Page & Jeffrey, 2018; Yan, Burgess, Bicanski, 2021; Sit & Goard, 2023). Currently, much of the Discussion simply summarizes the results of the study, this space could be better used in mapping the findings to the existing literature on the overarching question of how HD signals are anchored to landmarks.

      We suggest a physiological mechanism for inputs to be selectively integrated and amplified, based on temporal coincidence. Of course there are still many unknowns, including the divergence of connections from a single thalamic or retrosplenial input neuron. The anatomical connectivity of inputs will be critical, as well as the subcellular arrangement of synaptic contacts. Neuromodulation and changes in the balance of excitation and inhibition will need to be factored in. While it is premature to provide a comprehensive explanation for landmark anchoring of HD signals in PrS, our results have led us to include a schematic, to illustrate our thinking (Figure 1, see below).

      Do HD tuned inputs from thalamus converge on similarly tuned HD neurons only? Is divergence greater for the retrosplenial inputs? If so, thalamic input might pre-select a range of HD neurons, and converging RSC input might narrow down the precise HD neurons that become active (Figure 1). In the future, the use of activity dependent labeling strategies might help to tie together information on the tuning of pre-synaptic neurons, and their convergence or divergence onto functionally defined postsynaptic target cells. This critical information is still lacking, for principal cells, and also for interneurons. 

      Interneurons may have a key role in HD-to-landmark anchoring. SST interneurons support stability of HD signals (Simonnet et al., 2017) and VIP interneurons flexibly disinhibit the system (Nassar et al., 2024). Could disinhibition be a necessary condition to create a window of opportunity for updating the landmark anchoring of the attractor? Single PV interneurons might receive thalamic and retrosplenial inputs non-specifically. We need to distinguish the conditions for when the excitation-inhibition balance in pyramidal cells may become tipped towards excitation, and the case of coincident, co-tuned thalamic and retrosplenial input may be such a condition. Elucidating the principles of hardwiring of inputs, as for example, selective convergence, will be necessary. Moreover, neuromodulation and oscillations may be critical for temporal coordination and precise temporal matching of HD-to-landmark signals.

      We note that matching directional with visual landmark information based on temporal coincidence as described here does not require synaptic plasticity. Algorithms for dynamic control of cognitive maps without synaptic plasticity have been proposed (Whittington et al., 2025, Neuron): information may be stored in neural attractor activity, and the idea that working memory may rely on recurrent updates of neural activity might generalize to the HD system. We include these considerations in the discussion (line 497-501; 521-531) and hope that our work will spur further experimental investigations and modeling work.

      While the focus of our work has been on PrS, we agree that RSC also treats HD and landmark signals. Possibly the RSC registers a direction to a landmark rather than comparing it with the current HD (Sit & Goard, 2023). We suggest that this integrated information then reaches PrS. In contrast to RSC, PrS is uniquely positioned to update the signal in the LMN (Yoder et al., 2011), cf. discussion (line 516-520).

      Minor comments:

      (1) Fig 1 - Supp 1: It appears there is a lot of input to PrS from higher visual regions, could this be a source of landmark signals?

      Yes, higher visual regions projecting to PrS may also be a source of landmark information, even if the visual signal is not integrated with HD at that stage (Sit & Goard 2023). The anatomical projection from the visual cortex was first described by Vogt & Miller (1983), but not studied on a functional level so far.

      (2) Fig 2F, G: Although the ATN and RSC measurements look quite similar, there are no stats included. The authors should use an explicit hypothesis test.

      We now compare the distributions of amplitudes and of latencies, using the Mann-Whitney U test. No significant difference between the two groups were found. Added in the figure legend: 2F, “Mann-Whitney U test revealed no significant difference (p = 0.95)”. 2G, “Mann-Whitney U test revealed no significant difference (p = 0.13)”.

      (3) Fig 2 - Supp 2A, C: Again, no statistical tests. This is particularly important for panel A, where the authors state that the latencies are similar but the populations appear to be different.

      Inputs from ATN and RSC have a similar ‘jitter’ (latency standard deviation) and ‘tau decay’. We added in the Fig 2 - Supp 2 figure legend: A, “Mann-Whitney U test revealed no significant difference (p = 0.26)”. C, “Mann-Whitney U test revealed no significant difference (p = 0.87)”.

      As a complementary measure for the reviewer, we performed the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test which confirmed that the populations’ distributions for ‘jitter’ were not significantly different, p = 0.1533.

      (4) Fig 4E, F: The statistics reporting is confusing, why are asterisks above the plots and hashmarks to the side?

      Asterisks refer to a comparison between ‘dual’ and ‘sum’ for each of the 5 stimulations in a Sidak multiple comparison test. Hashmarks refer to comparison of the nth stimulation to the 1st one within dual stimulation events (Friedman + Dunn’s multiple comparison test). We mention the two-way ANOVA p-value in the legend (Sum v Dual, for both Amplitude and Surface).

      (5) Fig 5C: I was confused by the 2*RSC manipulation. How do we know if there is amplification unless we know what the 2*RSC stim alone looks like?

      We now label the right panel in Fig 5C as “high light intensity” or “HLI”. Increasing the activation of Chrimson increases the amplitude of the summed EPSP that now exceeds the threshold for amplification of synaptic events. Amplification refers to the shape of the plateau-like prolongation of the peak, most pronounced on the second EPSP, now indicated with an arrow.  We clarify this also in the text (line 309-310).

      (6) Fig 6D (supplement 1): Typo, "though" should be "through"

      Yes, corrected (line 1015).

      (7) Fig 6G (supplement 1): Typo, I believe this refers to the dotted are in panel F, not panel A.

      Yes, corrected (line 1021).

      (8) Fig 7: The effect of muscarine was qualitatively described in the Results, but there is no quantification and it is not shown in the Figure. The results should either be reported properly or removed from the Results.

      We remove the last sentence in the Results.

      (9) Methods: The age and sex of the mice should be reported. Transgenic mouse line should be reported (along with stock number if applicable).

      We used C57BL6 mice with transgenic background (Ai14 mice, Jax n007914  reporter line) or C57BL6 wild type mice. This is now indicated in the Methods (lines 566-567).

      (10) Methods: If the viruses are only referred to with their plasmid number, then the capsid used for the viruses should be specified. For example, I believe the AAV-CAG-tomato virus used the retroAAV capsid, which is important to the experiment.

      Thank you for pointing this out. Indeed the AAV-CAG-tdTom virus used the retroAAV capsid, (line 575).

      (11) Data/code availability: I didn't see any sort of data/code availability statement, will the data and code be made publicly available?

      Data are stored on local servers at the SPPIN, Université Paris Cité, and are made available upon reasonable request. Code for intrinsic properties analysis is available on github (https://github.com/schoki0710/Intrinsic_Properties). This information is now included (line 717-720).

      (12) Very minor (and these might be a matter of opinion), but I believe "records" should be "recordings", and "viral constructions" should be "viral constructs".

      The text had benefited from proofreading by Richard Miles, who always preferred “records” to “recordings” in his writings. We choose to keep the current wording.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Below are two major points that require clarification.

      (1) In the last set of experiments presented by the authors (Figs 6 onwards) they focus on 'putative L4' PrS cells. For several lines of evidence (outlined below), I am convinced that these neurons are not presubicular, but belong to the subiculum. I think this is a major point that requires substantial clarification, in order to avoid confusion in the field (see also suggestions on how to address this comment at the end of this section).

      Several lines of evidence support the interpretation that, what the authors call 'L4 PrS neurons', are distal subicular cells:

      (1.1) The anatomical location of the retrogradely-labelled cells (from mammillary bodies injections), as shown in Figs 6B, C, and Fig. 6_1B, very clearly indicates that they belong to the distal subiculum. The subicular-to-PrS boundary is a sharp anatomical boundary that follows exactly the curvature highlighted by the authors' red stainings. The authors could also use specific subicular/PrS markers to visualize this border more clearly - e.g. calbindin, Wfs-1, Zinc (though I believe this is not strictly necessary, since from the pattern of AD fibers, one can already draw very clear conclusions, see point 1.3 below).

      Our criteria to delimit the presubiculum are the following: First and foremost, we rely on the defining presence of antero-dorsal thalamic fibers that target specifically the presubiculum and not the neighbouring subiculum (Simonnet et al., 2017, Nassar et al., 2018, Simonnet and Fricker, 2018; Jiayan Liu et al., 2021). This provides the precise outline of the presubicular superficial layers 1 to 3. It may have been confusing to the reviewer that our slicing angle gives horizontal sections. In fact, horizontal sections are favourable to identify the layer structure of the PrS,  based on DAPI staining and the variations in cell body size. The work by Ishihara and Fukuda (2016) illustrates in their Figure 12 that the presubicular layer 4 lies below the presubicular layer 3, and forms a continuation with the subiculum (Sub1). Their Figure 4 indicates with a dotted line the “generally accepted border between the (distal) subiculum and PreS”, and it runs from the proximal tip of superficial cells of the PrS toward the white matter, among the radial direction of the cortical tissue.  We agree with this definition. Others have sliced coronally (Cembrowski et al., 2018) which renders a different visualization of the border region with the subiculum.

      Second, let me explain the procedure for positioning the patch electrode in electrophysiological experiments on horizontal presubicular slices. Louis Richevaux, the first author, who carried out the layer 4 cell recordings, took great care to stay very close (<50 µm) to the lower limit of the zone where the GFP labeled thalamic axons can be seen. He was extremely meticulous about the visualization under the microscope, using LED illumination, for targeting. The electrophysiological signature of layer 4 neurons with initial bursts (but not repeated bursting, in mice) is another criterion to confirm their identity (Huang et al., 2017). Post-hoc morphological revelation showed their apical dendrites, running toward the pia, sometimes crossing through the layer 3, sometimes going around the proximal tip, avoiding the thalamic axons (Figure 6D). For example the cell in Figure 6, suppl. 1 panel D, has an apical dendrite that runs through layer 3 and layer 1. 

      Third, retrograde labeling following stereotaxic injection into the LMN is another criterion to define PrS layer 4. This approach is helpful for visualization, and is based on the defining axonal projection of layer 4 neurons (Yoder and Taube, 2011; Huang et al., 2017). Due to the technical challenge to stereotaxically inject only into LMN, the resultant labeling may not be limited to PrS layer 4. We cannot entirely exclude some overflow of retrograde tracers (B) or retrograde virus (C) to the neighboring MMN. This would then lead to co-labeling of the subiculum. In the main Figure 6, panels B and C, we agree that for this reason the red labelled cell bodies likely include also subicular neurons, on the proximal side, in addition to L4 presubicular neurons. We now point out this caveat in the main text (line 324-326) and in the methods (line 591-592).

      (1.2) Consistent with their subicular location, neuronal morphologies of the 'putative L4 cells' are selectively constrained within the subicular boundaries, i.e. they do not cross to the neighboring PrS (maybe a minor exception in Figs. 6_1D2,3). By definition, a neuron whose morphology is contained within a structure belongs to that structure.

      From a functional point of view, for the HD system, the most important criterion for defining presubicular layer 4 neurons is their axonal projection to the LMN (Yoder and Taube 2011). From an electrophysiological standpoint, it is the capacity of layer 4 neurons to fire initial bursts (Simonnet et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2017).  Anatomically, we note that the expectation that the apical dendrite should go straight up into layer 3 might not be a defining criterion in this curved and transitional periarchicortex. Presubicular layer 4 apical dendrites may cross through layer 3 and exit to the side, towards the subiculum (This is the red dendritic staining at the proximal end of the subiculum, at the frontier with the subiculum, Figure 6 C).

      (1.3) As acknowledged by the authors in the discussion (line 408): the PrS is classically defined by the innervation domain of AD fibers. As Figure 6B clearly indicates, the retrogradely-labelled cells ('putative L4') are convincingly outside the input domain of the AD; hence, they do not belong to the PrS.

      The reviewer is mistaken here, the deep layers 4 and 5/6 indeed do not lie in the zone innervated by the thalamic fibers (Simonnet et al., 2017; Nassar et al., 2018; Simonnet and Fricker, 2018) but still belong to the presubiculum. The presubicular deep layers are located below the superficial layers, next to, and in continuation of the subiculum. This is in agreement with work by Yoder and Taube 2011; Ishihara and Fukuda 2016; Boccara, … Witter, 2015; Peng et al., 2017 (Fig 2D); Yoshiko Honda et al., (Marmoset, Fig 2A) 2022; Balsamo et al., 2022 (Figure 2B).

      (1.4) Along with the above comment: in my view, the optogenetic stimulation experiments are an additional confirmation that the 'putative L4 cells' are subicular neurons, since they do not receive AD inputs at all (hence, they are outside of the PrS); they are instead only indirectly driven upon strong excitation of the PrS. This indirect activation is likely to occur via PrS-to-Subiculum 'back-projections', the existence of which is documented in the literature and also nicely shown by the authors (see Figure 1_1 and line 109).

      See above. Only superficial layers 1-3 of the presubiculum receive direct AD input.

      (1.5) The electrophysiological properties of the 'putative L4 cells' are consistent with their subicular identity, i.e. they show a sag current and they are intrinsically bursty.

      Presubicular layer 4 cells also show bursting behaviour and a sag current (Simonnet et al., 2013; Huang et al., 2017).

      From the above considerations, and the data provided by the authors, I believe that the most parsimonious explanation is that these retrogradely-labelled neurons (from mammillary body injections), referred to by the authors as 'L4 PrS cells', are indeed pyramidal neurons from the distal subiculum.

      We agree that the retrograde labeling is likely not limited to the presubicular layer 4 cells, and we now indicate this in the text (line 324-326). However, the portion of retrogradely labeled neurons that is directly below the layer 3 should be considered as part of the presubiculum.

      I believe this is a fundamental issue that deserves clarification, in order to avoid confusion/misunderstandings in the field. Given the evidence provided, I believe that it would be inaccurate to call these cells 'L4 PrS neurons'. However, I acknowledge the fact that it might be difficult to convincingly and satisfactorily address this issue within the framework of a revision. For example, it is possible that these 'putative L4 cells' might be retrogradely-labelled from the Medial Mammillary Body (a major subicular target) since it is difficult to selectively restrict the injection to the LMN, unless a suitable driver line is used (if available). The authors should also consider the possibility of removing this subset of data (referring to putative L4), and instead focus on the rest of the story (referring to L3)- which I think by itself, still provides sufficient advance.

      We agree with the reviewer that it is difficult to provide a satisfactory answer. To some extent, the reviewer’s comments target the nomenclature of the subicular region. This transitional region between the hippocampus and the entorhinal cortex has been notoriously ill defined, and the criteria are somewhat arbitrary for determining exactly where to draw the line. Based on the thalamic projection, presubicular layers 1-3 can now be precisely outlined, thanks to the use of viral labeling. But the presubicular layer 4 had been considered to be cell-free in early works, and termed ‘lamina dissecans’ (Boccara 2010), as the limit between the superficial and deep layers. Then it became of great interest to us and to the field, when the PrS layer 4 cells were first identified as LMN projecting neurons (Yoder and Taube 2011). This unique back-projection to the upstream region of the HD system is functionally very important, closing the loop of the Papez circuit (mammillary bodies - thalamus - hippocampal structures).

      We note that the reviewer does not doubt our results, rather questions the naming conventions. We therefore maintain our data. We agree that in the future a genetically defined mouse line would help to better pin down this specific neuronal population.

      We thank the reviewer for sharing their concerns and giving us the opportunity to clarify our experimental approach to target the presubicular layer 4. We hope that these explanations will be helpful to the readers of eLife as well.

      (2) The PrS anatomy could be better clarified, especially in relation to its modular organization (see e.g. Preston-Ferrer et al., 2016; Ray et al., 2017; Balsamo et al., 2022). The authors present horizontal slices, where cortical modularity is difficult to visualize and assess (tangential sections are typically used for this purpose, as in classical work from e.g. barrel cortex). I am not asking the authors to validate their observations in tangential sections, but just to be aware that cortical modules might not be immediately (or clearly) apparent, depending on the section orientation and thickness. The authors state that AD fibers were 'not homogeneously distributed' in L3 (line 135) and refer to 'patches of higher density in deep L3' (line 136). These statements are difficult to support unless more convincing anatomy and  . I see some L3 inhomogeneity in the green channel in Fig. 1G (last two panels) and also in Fig. 1K, but this seems to be rather upper L3. I wonder how consistent the pattern is across different injections and at what dorsoventral levels this L3 modularity is observed (I think sagittal sections might be helpful). If validated, these observations could point to the existence of non-homogeneous AD innervation domains in L3 - hinting at possible heterogeneity among the L3 pyramidal cell targets. Notably, modularity in L2 and L1 is not referred to. The authors state that AD inputs 'avoid L2' (line 131) but this statement is not in line with recent work (cited above) and is also not in line with their anatomy data in Fig. 1G, where modularity is already quite apparent in L2 (i.e. there are territories avoided by the AD fibers in L2) and in L1 (see for example the last image in Fig. 1G). This is the case also for the RSC axons (Fig. 1H) where a patchy pattern is quite clear in L1 (see the last image in panel H). Higher-mag pictures might be helpful here. These qualitative observations imply that AD and RSC axons probably bear a precise structural relationship relative to each other, and relative to the calbindin patch/matrix PrS organization that has been previously described. I am not asking the authors to address these aspects experimentally, since the main focus of their study is on L3, where RSC/AD inputs largely converge. Better anatomy pictures would be helpful, or at least a better integration of the authors' (qualitative) observations within the existing literature. Moreover, the authors' calbindin staining in Fig. 1K is not particularly informative. Subicular, PaS, MEC, and PrS borders should be annotated, and higher-resolution images could be provided. The authors should also check the staining: MEC appears to be blank but is known to strongly express calb1 in L2 (see 'island' by Kitamura et al., Ray et al., Science 2014; Ray et al., frontiers 2017). As additional validation for the staining: I would expect that the empty L2 patches in Figs. 1G (last two panels) would stain positive for Calbindin, as in previous work (Balsamo et al. 2022).

      We now provide a new figure showing the pattern of AD innervation in PrS superficial layers 1 to 3, with different dorso-ventral levels and higher magnification (Figure 2). Because our work was aimed at identifying connectivity between long-range inputs and presubicular neurons, we chose to work with horizontal sections that preserve well the majority of the apical dendrites of presubicular pyramidal neurons. We feel it is enriching for the presubicular literature to show the cytoarchitecture from different angles and to show patchiness in horizontal sections. The non-homogeneous AD innervation domains (‘microdomains’) in L3 were consistently observed across different injections in different animals.

      Author response image 1.

      Thalamic fiber innervation pattern. A, ventral, and B, dorsal horizontal section of the Presubiculum containing ATN axons expressing GFP. Patches of high density of ATN axonal ramifications in L3 are indicated as “ATN microdomains”. Layers 1, 2, 3, 4, 5/6 are indicated.  C, High magnification image (63x optical section)(different animal).<br />

      We also provide a supplementary figure with images of horizontal sections of calbindin staining in PrS, with a larger crop, for the reviewer to check (Figure 3, see below). We thank the reviewer for pointing out recent studies using tangential sections. Our results agree with the previous observation that AD axons are found in calbindin negative territories (cf Fig 1K). Calbindin+ labeling is visible in the PrS layer 2 as well as in some patches in the MEC (Figure 3 panel A). Calbindin staining tends to not overlap with the territories of ATN axonal ramification. We indicate the inhomogeneities of anterior thalamic innervation that form “microdomains” of high density of green labeled fibers, located in layer 1 and layer 3 (Figure 3, Panel A, middle). Panel B shows another view of a more dorsal horizontal section of the PrS, with higher magnification, with a big Calbindin+ patch near the parasubiculum.

      The “ATN+ microdomains” possess a high density of axonal ramifications from ATN, and have been previously documented in the literature. They are consistently present. Our group had shown them in the article by Nassar et al., 2018, at different dorsoventral levels (Fig 1 C (dorsal) and 1D (ventral) PrS). See also Simonnet et al., 2017, Fig 2B, for an illustration of the typical variations in densities of thalamic fibers, and supplementary Figure 1D. Also Jiayan Liu et al., 2021 (Figure 2 and Fig 5) show these characteristic microzones of dense thalamic axonal ramifications, with more or less intense signals across layers 1, 2, and 3.  While it is correct that thalamic axons can be seen to cross layer 2 to ramify in layer 1, we maintain that AD axons typically do not ramify in layer 2. We modify the text to say, “mostly” avoiding L2 (line 130).

      The reviewer is correct in pointing out that the 'patches of higher density in deep L3' are not only in the deep L3, as in the first panel in Fig 1G, but in the more dorsal sections they are also found in the upper L3. We change the text accordingly (line 135-136) and we provide the layer annotation in Figure 1G. We further agree with the reviewer that RSC axons also present a patchy innervation pattern. We add this observation in the text (line 144).

      It is yet unclear whether anatomical microzones of dense ATN axon ramifications in L3 might fulfill the criteria of a functional modularity, as it is the case for the calbindin patch/matrix PrS organization (Balsamo et al., 2022). As the reviewer points out, this will require more information on the precise structural relationship of AD and RSC axons relative to each other, as well as functional studies. Interestingly, we note a degree of variation in the amplitudes of oEPSC from different L3 neurons (Fig. 2F, discussion line 420; 428), which might be a reflection of the local anatomo-functional micro-organization.

      Minor points:

      (1) The pattern or retrograde labelling, or at least the way is referred to in the results (lines 104ff), seems to imply some topography of AD-to-PreS projections. Is it the case? How consistent are these patterns across experiments, and individual injections? Was there variability in injection sites along the dorso-ventral and possibly antero-posterior PrS axes, which could account for a possibly topographical AD-to-PrS input pattern? It would be nice to see a DAPI signal in Fig. 1B since the AD stands out quite clearly in DAPI (Nissl) alone.

      Yes, we find a consistent topography for the AD-to-PrS projection, for similar injection sites in the presubiculum. The coordinates for retrograde labeling were as indicated -4.06 (AP), 2.00 (ML) and -2.15 mm (DV) such that we cannot report on possible variations for different injection sites.

      (2) Fig. 2_2KM: this figure seems to show the only difference the authors found between AD and RS input properties. The authors could consider moving these data into main Fig. 2 (or exchanging them with some of the panels in F-O, which instead show no difference between AD and RSC). Asterisks/stats significance is not visible in M.

      For space reasons we leave the panels of Fig. 2_2KM in the supplementary section. We increased the size of the asterisk in M.

      (3) The data in Fig. 1_1 are quite interesting, since some of the PrS projection targets are 'non-canonical'. Maybe the authors could consider showing some injection sites, and some fluorescence images, in addition to the schematics. Maybe the authors could acknowledge that some of these projection targets are 'putative' unless independently verified by e.g. retrograde labeling. Unspecific white matter labelling and/or spillover is always a potential concern.

      We now include the image of the injection site for data in Fig. 1_1 as a supplementary Fig. 1_2. The Figure 1_1 shows the retrogradely labeled upstream areas of Presubiculum.

      Author response image 2.

      Retrobeads were injected in the right Presubiculum.<br />

      (4) The authors speculate that the near-coincident summation of RS + AD inputs in L3 cells could be a potential mechanism for the binding of visual + HD information in PrS. However, landmarks are learned, and learning typically implies long-term plasticity. As the authors acknowledge in the discussion (lines 493ff) GluR1 is not expressed in PrS cells. What alternative mechanics could the authors envision? How could the landmark-update process occur in PrS, if is not locally stored? RSC could also be involved (Jakob et al) as acknowledged in the introduction - the authors should keep this possibility open also in the discussion.

      A similar point has been raised by Reviewer 1, please check our answer to their point 2. Briefly, our results indicate that HD-to-landmark updating is a multi-step process. RSC may be one of the places where landmarks are learned. The subsequent temporal mapping of HD to landmark signals in PrS might be plasticity-free, as matching directional with visual landmark information based on temporal coincidence does not necessarily require synaptic plasticity.  It seems likely that there is no local storage and no change in synaptic weights in PrS. The landmark-anchored HD signals reach LMN via L4 neurons, sculpting network dynamics across the Papez circuit. One possibility is that the trace of a landmark that matches HD may be stored as patterns of neural activity that could guide navigation (cf. El-Gaby et al., 2024, Nature) Clearly more work is needed to understand how the HD attractor is updated on a mechanistic level. Recent work in prefrontal cortex mentions “activity slots” and delineates algorithms for dynamic control of cognitive maps without synaptic plasticity (Whittington et al., 2025, Neuron): information may be stored in neural attractor activity, and the idea that working memory may rely on recurrent updates of neural activity might generalize to the HD system. We include these considerations in the discussion (line 499-503; 523-533) and also point to alternative models (line 518 -522) including modeling work in the retrosplenial cortex.

      (5) The authors state that (lines 210ff) their cluster analysis 'provided no evidence for subpopulations of layer 3 cells (but see Balsamo et al., 2022)' implying an inconsistency; however, Balsamo et al also showed that the (in vivo) ephys properties of the two HD cell 'types' are virtually identical, which is in line with the 'homogeneity' of L3 ephys properties (in slice) in the authors' data. Regarding the possible heterogeneity of L3 cells: the authors report inhomogeneous AD innervation domains in L3 (see also main comment 2) and differences in input summation (some L3 cells integrate linearly, some supra-linearly; lines 272) which by itself might already imply some heterogeneity. I would therefore suggest rewording the statements to clarify what the lack of heterogeneity refers to.

      We agree. In line 212 we now state “cluster analysis (Figure 2D) provided no evidence for subpopulations of layer 3 cells in terms of intrinsic electrophysiological properties (see also Balsamo et al., 2022).”

      (6) n=6 co-recorded pairs are mentioned at line 348, but n=9 at line 366. Are these numbers referring to the same dataset? Please correct or clarify

      Line 349 refers to a set of 6 co-recorded pairs (n=12 neurons) in double injected mice with Chronos injected in ATN and Chrimson in RSC (cf. Fig. 7E). The 9 pairs mentioned in line 367 refer to another type of experiment where we stimulated layer 3 neurons by depolarizing them to induce action potential firing while recording neighboring layer 4 neurons to assess connectivity. Line 367  now reads: “In n = 9 paired recordings, we did not detect functional synapses between layer 3 and layer 4 neurons.”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Questions for the authors/points for addressing:

      I found that the slice electrophysiology experiments were not reported with sufficient detail. For example, in Figure 2, I am assuming that the voltage clamp experiments were carried out using the Cs-based recording solution, while the current clamp experiments were carried out using the K-Gluc intracellular solution. However, this is not explicitly stated and it is possible that all of these experiments were performed using the K-Gluc solution, which would give slightly odd EPSCs due to incomplete space/voltage clamp. Furthermore, the method states that gabazine was used to block GABA(A) receptor-mediated currents, but not when this occurred. Was GABAergic neurotransmission blocked for all measurements of EPSC magnitude/dynamics? If so, why not block GABA(B) receptors? If not blocking GABAergic transmission for measuring EPSCs, why not? This should be stated explicitly either way.

      The addition of drugs or difference of solution is indicated in the figure legend and/or in the figure itself, as well as in the methods. We now state explicitly: “In a subset of experiments, the following drugs were used to modulate the responses to optogenetic stimulations; the presence of these drugs is indicated in the figure and figure legend, whenever applicable.” (line 632). A Cs-based internal solution and gabazine were used in Figure 5, this is now indicated in the Methods section (line 626). All other experiments were performed using K-Gluc as an internal solution and ACSF.

      Methods: The experiments involving animals are incompletely reported. For example, were both sexes used? The methods state "Experiments were performed on wild‐type and transgenic C57Bl6 mice" - what transgenic mice were used and why is this not reported in detail (strain, etc)? I would refer the authors to the ARRIVE guidelines for reporting in vivo experiments in a reproducible manner (https://arriveguidelines.org/).

      We now added this information in the methods section, subsection “Animals” (line 566-567). Animals of both sexes were used. The only transgenic mouse line used was the Ai14 reporter line (no phenotype), depending on the availability in our animal facility.

      For experiments comparing ATN and RSC inputs onto the same neuron (e.g. Figure 2 supplement 2 G - J), are the authors certain that the observed differences (e.g. rise time and paired-pulse facilitation on the ATN input) are due to differences in the synapses and not a result of different responses of the opsins? Refer to https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/31822522/ from Jess Cardin's lab. This could easily be tested by switching which opsin is injected into which nucleus (a fair amount of extra work) or comparing the Chrimson synaptic responses with those evoked using Chronos on the same projection, as used in Figure 2 (quite easy as authors should already have the data).

      We actually did switch the opsins across the two injection sites. In Figure 2 - supplement 2G-J, the values linked by a dashed line result from recordings in the switched configuration with respect to the original configuration (in full lines, Chronos injected in RSC and Chrimson in ATN). The values from switched configuration followed the trend of the main configuration and were not statistically different (Mann-Whitney U test).

      Statistical reporting: While the number of cells is generally reported for experiments, the number of slices and animals is not. While slice ephys often treat cells as individual biological replicates, this is not entirely appropriate as it could be argued that multiple cells from a single animal are not independent samples (some sort of mixed effects model that accounts for animals as a random effect would be better). For the experiments in the manuscript, I don't think this is necessary, but it would certainly reassure the reader to report how many animals/slices each dataset came from. At a bare minimum, one would want any dataset to be taken from at least 3 animals from 2 different litters, regardless of how many cells are in there.

      Our slice electrophysiology experiments include data from 38 successfully injected animals: 14 animals injected in ATN, 20 animals injected in RSC, and 4 double injected animals. Typically, we recorded 1 to 3 cells per slice. We now include this information in the text or in the figure legends (line 159, 160, 297, 767, 826, 831, 832, 839, 845, 901, 941).

      For the optogenetic experiments looking at the summation of EPSPs (e.g. figure 4), I have two questions: why were EPSPs measured and not EPSCs? The latter would be expected to give a better readout of AMPA receptor-mediated synaptic currents. And secondly, why was 20 Hz stimulation used for these experiments? One might expect theta stimulation to be a more physiologically-relevant frequency of stimulation for comparing ATN and RSC inputs to single neurons, given the relevance with spatial navigation and that the paper's conclusions were based around the head direction system. Similarly, gamma stimulation may also have been informative. Did the authors try different frequencies of stimulation?

      Question 1. The current clamp configuration allows to measure  EPSPamplification/prolongation by NMDA or persistent Na currents (cf.  Fricker and Miles 2000), which might contribute to supralinearity.

      Question 2. In a previous study from our group about the AD to PrS connection (Nassar et al., 2018), no significant difference was observed on the dynamics of EPSCs between stimulations at 10 Hz versus 30 Hz. Therefore we chose 20 Hz. This value is in the range of HD cell firing (Taube 1995, 1998 (peak firing rates, 18 to 24 spikes/sec in RSC; 41 spikes/sec in AD)(mean firing rates might be lower), Blair and Sharp 1995). In hindsight, we agree that it would have been useful to include 8Hz or 40Hz stimulations. 

      The GABA(A) antagonist experiments in Figure 5 are interesting but I have concerns about the statistical power of these experiments - n of 3 is absolutely borderline for being able to draw meaningful conclusions, especially if this small sample of cells came from just 1 or 2 animals. The number of animals used should be stated and/or caution should be applied when considering the potential mechanisms of supralinear summation of EPSPs. It looks like the slight delay in RSC input EPSP relative to ATN that was in earlier figures is not present here - could this be the loss of feedforward inhibition?

      The current clamp experiments in the presence of QX314 and a Cs gluconate based internal solution were preceded by initial experiments using puff applications of glutamate to the recorded neurons (not shown). Results from those experiments had pointed towards a role for TTX resistant sodium currents and for NMDA receptor activation as a factor favoring the amplification and prolongation of glutamate induced events. They inspired the design of the dual wavelength stimulation experiments shown in Figure 5, and oriented our discussion of the results. We agree of course that more work is required to dissect the role of disinhibition for EPSP amplification. This is however beyond the present study.

      Concerning the EPSP onset delays following RSC input stimulation:  In this set of experiments, we compensated for the notoriously longer delay to EPSP onset, following RSC axon stimulation, by shifting the photostimulation (red) of RSC fibers to -2 ms, relative to the onset of photostimulation of ATN fibers (blue). This experimental trick led to an improved  alignment of the onset of the postsynaptic response, as shown in the figure below for the reviewer.

      Author response image 3.

      In these experiments, the onset of RSC photostimulation was shifted forward in time by -2 ms, in an attempt to better align the EPSP onset to the one evoked by ATN stimulation.<br />

      We insert in the results a sentence to indicate that experiments illustrated in Figure 5 were performed in only a small sample of 3 cells that came from 2 mice (line 297), so caution should be applied. In the discussion we  formulate more carefully, “From a small sample of cells it appears that EPSP amplification may be facilitated by a reduction in synaptic inhibition (n = 3; Figure 5)” (line 487).

      Figure 7: I appreciate the difficulties in making dual recordings from older animals, but no conclusion about the RSC input can legitimately be made with n=1.

      Agreed. We want to avoid any overinterpretation, and point out in the results section that the RSC stimulation data is from a single cell pair. The sentence now reads : “... layer 4 neurons occurred after firing in the layer 3 neuron, following ATN afferent stimuli, in 4 out of 5 cell pairs. We also observed this sequence when RSC input was activated, in one tested pair.” line (347-349)

      Minor points:

      Line 104: 'within the two subnuclei that form the anterior thalamus' - the ATN actually has three subdivisions (AD, AV, AM) so this should state 'two of the three nuclei that form the anterior thalamus...'

      Corrected, line 103

      Line 125: should read "figure 1F" and not "figure 2F".

      Corrected, line 124

      Line 277-280: Why were two different posthoc tests used on the same data in Figures 3E & F?

      We used Sidak’s multicomparison test to compare each event Sum vs. Dual (two different configurations at each time point - asterisks) and Friedman’s and Dunn’s to compare the nth EPSP amplitude to the first one for Dual events (same configuration between time points - hashmarks). We give two-way ANOVA results in the legend.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      In this study, the authors identify an insect salivary protein participating viral initiate infection in plant host. They found a salivary LssaCA promoting RSV infection by interacting with OsTLP that could degrade callose in plants. Furthermore, RSV NP bond to LssaCA in salivary glands to form a complex, which then bond to OsTLP to promote degradation of callose.

      The story focus on tripartite virus-insect vector-plant interaction and is interesting. However, the study is too simple and poor-conducted. The conclusion is also overstated due to unsolid findings.

      We thank the reviewer for their constructive feedback. We have conducted additional experiments to strengthen our results and conclusions as detailed below:

      (1) The comparison between vector inoculation and microinjection involves multiple confounding factors that could affect the experimental results, including salivary components, RSV inoculation titers, and the precision of viral deposition. The differential outcomes could be attributed to these various factors rather than definitively demonstrating the necessity of salivary factors. Therefore, we have removed this comparison from the revised manuscript and instead focused on elucidating the specific mechanisms by which LssaCA facilitates viral infection.

      (2) We conducted new experiments to assess the function of LssaCA enzymatic activity in mediating RSV infection. Additional experiments revealed that OsTLP enzymatic activity is highly pH-dependent, with increased activity as pH decreases from 7.5 to 5.0 (Fig. 3H). However, the LssaCA-OsTLP interaction at pH 7.4 significantly enhanced OsTLP enzymatic activity without requiring pH changes. These results demonstrate that LssaCA-OsTLP protein interactions are crucial for mediating RSV infection. In contrast to pH-dependent mechanisms, our study demonstrated that LssaCA's biological function in mediating RSV infection is at least partially, if not completely, independent of its enzymatic activity. We have added these new resulted into the revised manuscript (Lines 220-227). We have also added a comprehensive discussion comparing the aphid CA mechanism described by Guo et al. (2023 doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2222040120) with our findings in the revised manuscript (Lines 350-371).

      (3) We have repeated majority of callose deposition experiments, providing clearer images (Figures 5-6). In addition to aniline blue staining, we quantified callose concentrations using a plant callose ELISA kit to provide more precise measurements (Figure 5A, I, 6A, C and S8A). We utilized RT-qPCR to measure callose synthase expression in both feeding and non-feeding areas, confirming that callose synthesis was induced specifically in feeding regions, leading to localized callose deposition (Figures 5D-G and S8B-E). For sieve plate visualization, we examined longitudinal sections, which revealed callose deposition in sieve plates during SBPH feeding and RSV infection (Figure S7).

      (4) We generated OsTLP mutant rice seedlings (ostlp) and use this mutant to directly demonstrate that LssaCA mediates callose degradation in planta through enhancement of OsTLP enzymatic activity (Lines 288-302 and Figure 6).

      (5) We produced LssaCA recombinant proteins in sf9 cells to ensure full enzymatic activity and constructed a comprehensive CA mutant protein, in which all seven residues constituting the enzymatic active center mutated (LssaCA<sup>H111D</sup>,LssaCA<sup>N139H</sup>,LssaCA<sup>H141D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>H143D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>E153H</sup>, LssaCA<sup>H166D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>T253E</sup>) (Fig. S1B). This LssaCA mutant protein demonstrated complete loss of enzymatic activity (Fig. 1C).

      Major comments:

      (1) The key problem is that how long the LssCA functioned for in rice plant. Author declared that LssCA had no effect on viral initial infection, but on infection after viral inoculation. It is unreasonable to conclude that LssCA promoted viral infection based on the data that insect inoculated plant just for 2 days, but viral titer could be increased at 14 days post-feeding. How could saliva proteins, which reached phloem 12-14 days before, induce enough TLP to degrade callose to promote virus infection? It was unbelievable.

      We appreciate your insightful comment and acknowledge that our initial description may have been unclear. We agree that salivary proteins would not present in plant tissues for two weeks post-feeding or post-injection. Our intention was to clarify that when salivary proteins enhance RSV infection, this initial enhancement leads to sustained high viral loads. We measured viral burden at 14 days post-feeding or post-injection because this is the common measurement time point when viral titers are sufficiently high for reliable detection by qRT-PCR or western blotting. We have clarified this rationale in the revised manuscript (Lines 155-157).

      To determine the actual persistence of LssaCA in plant tissues, we conducted additional experiments where insects were allowed to feed on a defined aera of rice seedlings for two days. We then monitored LssaCA protein levels at 1 and 3 days after removing the insects. Western blotting analysis revealed that LssaCA protein levels decreased post-feeding and remained detectable at 3 days post-feeding. These results are presented in Figure 2H and described in detail in Lines 184-193.

      (2) Lines 110-116 and Fig. 1, the results of viruliferous insect feeding and microinjection with purified virus could not conclude the saliva factor necessary of RSV infection, because these two tests are not in parallel and comparable. Microinjection with salivary proteins combined with purified virus is comparable with microinjection with purified virus.

      We thank the reviewer’s insightful comment. We agree that “the results of viruliferous insect feeding and microinjection with the purified virus could not conclude the saliva factor necessary of RSV infection”. However, due to the technical difficulty in collecting sufficient quantities of salivary proteins to conduct the microinjection experiment, we have removed these results from the revised manuscript.

      (3) The second problem is how many days post viruliferous insect feeding and microinjection with purified virus did author detect viral titers? in Method section, authors declared that viral titers was detected at 7-14 days post microinjection. Please demonstrate the days exactly.

      We thank the reviewer’s insightful comment. We typically measured RSV infection levels at both 7- and 14-days post-microinjection. However, since the midrib microinjection experiments have been removed from the revised manuscript, this methodology has also been removed accordingly.

      (4) The last problem is that how author made sure that the viral titers in salivary glands of insects between two experiments was equal, causing different phenotype of rice plant. If not, different viral titers in salivary glands of insects between two experiments of course caused different phenotype of rice plant.

      We thank the reviewer’s comment. When we compared the effects of LssaCA deficiency on RSV infection of rice plants, we have compared the viral titers in the insect saliva and salivary glands. The results indicated that the virus titers in both tissues have not changed by LssaCA deficiency, suggesting that the viruses inoculated into rice phloem by insects of different treatments were comparable. Please refer to the revised manuscript Figures 2D-G and Lines 161-173.

      (5) The callose deposition in phloem can be induced by insect feeding. In Fig. 5H, why was the callose deposition increased in the whole vascular bundle, but not phloem? Could the transgenic rice plant directional express protein in the phloem? In Fig. 5, why was callose deposition detected at 24 h after insect feeding? In Fig. 6A, why was callose deposition decreased in the phloem, but not all the cells of the of TLP OE plant? Also in Fig.6A and B, expression of callose synthase genes was required.

      We thank the reviewer for these insightful comments.

      (1) Figure 5. The callose deposition increased in multiple cells within the vascular bundle, including sieve tubes, parenchymatic cells, and companion cells. While callose deposition was detected in other parts of the vascular bundle, no significant differences were observed between treatments in these regions, indicating that in response to RSV infection and other treatments, altered callose deposition mainly occurred in phloem cells. Please refer to the revised 5B, 5J, 6B, and 6D.

      (2) Transgenic plant expression. The OsTLP-overexpressing transgenic rice plants express TLP proteins in various cells under the control of CaMV 35S promoter, rather than being directionally expressed in the phloem. However, since TLP proteins are secreted, they are potentially transported and concentrated in the phloem where they can degrade callose.

      (3) Figure 5. The 24-hour time point for callose deposition detection was selected based on established protocols from previous studies. According to Hao et al. (Plant Physiology 2008), callose deposition increased during the first 3 days of planthopper infestation and decreased after 4 days. Additionally, Ellinger and Voigt (Ann Bot 2014) demonstrated that callose visualization typically begins 18-24 hours after treatment, making 24 hours an optimal detection time point.

      (4) Figure 6, Phloem-specific changes. Similar to Figure 5, while callose deposition was detected in other parts of vascular bundle, significant differences between treatments were mainly observed in phloem cells, indicating that RSV infection specifically affects callose deposition in phloem tissue.

      (5) Callose synthase gene expression. We performed RT-qPCR analysis to measure the expression levels of callose synthase genes. The results indicated that OsTLP overexpression did not significantly alter the mRNA levels of these genes, regardless of RSV infection status in SBPH.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      There is increasing evidence that viruses manipulate vectors and hosts to facilitate transmission. For arthropods, saliva plays an essential role for successful feeding on a host and consequently for arthropod-borne viruses that are transmitted during arthropod feeding on new hosts. This is so because saliva constitutes the interaction interface between arthropod and host and contains many enzymes and effectors that allow feeding on a compatible host by neutralizing host defenses. Therefore, it is not surprising that viruses change saliva composition or use saliva proteins to provoke altered vector-host interactions that are favorable for virus transmission. However, detailed mechanistic analyses are scarce. Here, Zhao and coworkers study transmission of rice stripe virus (RSV) by the planthopper Laodelphax striatellus. RSV infects plants as well as the vector, accumulates in salivary glands and is injected together with saliva into a new host during vector feeding.

      The authors present evidence that a saliva-contained enzyme - carbonic anhydrase (CA) - might facilitate virus infection of rice by interfering with callose deposition, a plant defense response. In vitro pull-down experiments, yeast two hybrid assay and binding affinity assays show convincingly interaction between CA and a plant thaumatin-like protein (TLP) that degrades callose. Similar experiments show that CA and TLP interact with the RSV nuclear capsid protein NT to form a complex. Formation of the CA-TLP complex increases TLP activity by roughly 30% and integration of NT increases TLP activity further. This correlates with lower callose content in RSV-infected plants and higher virus titer. Further, silencing CA in vectors decreases virus titers in infected plants.

      (1) Interestingly, aphid CA was found to play a role in plant infection with two non-persistent non-circulative viruses, turnip mosaic virus and cucumber mosaic virus (Guo et al. 2023 doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2222040120), but the proposed mode of action is entirely different.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment and have carefully examined the cited publication. The study by Guo et al. (2023) elucidates a distinct mechanism for aphid-mediated transmission of non-persistent, non-circulative viruses (turnip mosaic virus and cucumber mosaic virus). In their model, aphid-secreted CA-II in the plant cell apoplast leads to H<sup>+</sup> accumulation and localized acidification. This trigger enhanced vesicle trafficking as a plant defense response, inadvertently facilitating virus translocation from the endomembrane system to the apoplast.

      In contrast to these pH-dependent mechanisms, our study demonstrated that LssaCA’s biological function in mediating RSV infection is, if not completely, at least partially independent of its enzymatic activity. We performed additional experiments to reveal that OsTLP enzymatic activity is highly pH-dependent and exhibits increased enzymatic activity as pH decreases from 7.5 to 5.0 (Fig. 3H); however, the LssaCA-OsTLP interaction occurring at pH 7.4 significantly enhanced OsTLP enzymatic activity without any change in buffer pH (Fig. 3G). These results demonstrate the crucial importance of LssaCA-OsTLP protein interactions, rather than enzymatic activity alone, in mediating RSV infection.

      We have incorporated these new experimental results and added a comprehensive discussion comparing the aphid CA mechanism described by Guo et al. (2023) with our findings in the revised manuscript. Please refer to Figures 3G-H, Lines 220-227 and 350-371 for detailed information.

      (2) While this is an interesting work, there are, in my opinion, some weak points. The microinjection experiments result in much lower virus accumulation in rice than infection by vector inoculation, so their interpretation is difficult.

      We acknowledge the reviewer's concern regarding the lower virus accumulation observed in microinjection experiments compared to vector-mediated inoculation. We have removed these experiments from the revised manuscript. To address the core question raised by these experiments, we have conducted new experiments that directly demonstrate the importance of LssaCA-OsTLP protein-protein interactions in mediating RSV infection. These results demonstrate the crucial importance of LssaCA-OsTLP protein interactions, rather than enzymatic activity alone, in mediating RSV infection. Additionally, we have incorporated a comprehensive discussion examining carbonic anhydrase activity, pH homeostasis, and viral infection. Please refer to the detailed experimental results and discussion in the sections mentioned in our previous response (Figures 3G-H, Lines 220-227 and 350-371).

      (3) Also, the effect of injected recombinant CA protein might fade over time because of degradation or dilution.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment. This is indeed a valid concern that could affect the interpretation of microinjection results. To address the temporal dynamics of CA protein presence in planta, we conducted time-course experiments to monitor the retention of naturally SBPH-secreted CA proteins in rice plants. Our analysis at 1- and 3- days post-feeding (dpf) revealed that CA protein levels decreased progressively following SBPH feeding, but could also been detected at 3dpf (Fig. 2H). Please refer to Figures 2H and lines 184-193 for detailed information.

      (4) The authors claim that enzymatic activity of CA is not required for its proviral activity. However, this is difficult to assess because all CA mutants used for the corresponding experiments possess residual activity.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment. We constructed a comprehensive CA mutant protein in which all seven residues constituting the enzymatic active center mutated (LssaCA<sup>H111D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>N139H</sup>, LssaCA<sup>H141D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>H143D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>E153H</sup>, LssaCA<sup>H166D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>T253E</sup>) (Fig. S1B). This LssaCA mutant protein demonstrated complete loss of enzymatic activity (Fig. 1C). However, since we have removed the recombinant CA protein microinjection experiments from the revised manuscript, we lack sufficient direct evidence to definitively demonstrate that CA enzymatic activity is dispensable for its proviral function. To address the core question raised by these experiments, we have conducted new experiments that provide direct evidence for the importance of LssaCA-OsTLP protein-protein interactions in mediating RSV infection. Additionally, we have incorporated a comprehensive discussion examining carbonic anhydrase activity, pH homeostasis, and viral infection. Please refer to the detailed experimental results and discussion in the sections mentioned in our previous response (Figures 3G-H, Lines 220-227 and 350-371).

      (5) It remains also unclear whether viral infection deregulates CA expression in planthoppers and TLP expression in plants. However, increased CA and TLP levels could alone contribute to reduced callose deposition.

      We have compared LssaCA mRNA levels in RSV-free and RSV-infected L.striatellus salivary glands, which indicated that RSV infection does not significantly affect LssaCA expression (Figure 1J). By using RSV-free and RSV-infected L.striatellus to feed on rice seedlings, we clarified that RSV infection does not affect TLP expression in plants (Figure 5H).

      Reviewer #1: (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Other comments:

      (1) Most data proving viral infection and LssaCA expression were derived from qPCR assays. Western blot data are strongly required to prove the change at the protein level.

      We agree that western blot data are required to prove the change at the protein level. In the revised manuscript, we have added western-blotting results (Figures 1F, 1I, 2C, 2J, and S6).

      (2) Line 145, data that LssaCA was significantly downregulated should be shown.

      Thank you and the data has been added to the revised manuscript. Please refer to Line 165 and Figure 2D.

      (3) Lines 159-161, how did authors assure that the dose of recombinant LssCA was closed to the release level of insect feeding, but not was excessive? How did author exclude the possibility of upregulated RSV titer caused by excessive recombinant LssCA?

      We appreciate this important concern regarding dosage controls. While microinjection of recombinant proteins typically yields viral infection levels significantly lower than those achieved through natural insect feeding, higher protein concentrations are often required to achieve high viral infection levels. In this experiment, we compared RSV infection levels following microinjection of BSA+RSV versus LssaCA+RSV, with the expectation that any observed upregulation in RSV titer would be specifically attributable to recombinant LssaCA rather than excessive protein dosing. However, given the low RSV infection levels observed with viral microinjection, we have removed their corresponding results from the revised manuscript.

      (4) Lines 124-125, recombinantly expressed LssaCA protein should be underlined, but not the LssaCA protein itself.

      We have clearly distinguished recombinantly expressed LssaCA from endogenous LssaCA protein throughout the manuscript, ensuring that all references to recombinant proteins are properly labeled as such.

      (5) LssaCA expression in salivary glands of viruliferous and nonviruliferous insects is required. LssaCA accumulation in rice plant exposed to viruliferous and nonviruliferous insects is also required.

      We have measured LssaCA mRNA levels in salivary glands of viruliferous and nonviruliferous insects (Figure 1J), and protein levels in rice plant exposed to viruliferous and nonviruliferous insects (Figure 1I).

      (6) Fig. 4G, the enzymatic activities of OsTLP were too low compared with that in Fig. 4E and Fig. 7E. Why did the enzymatic activities of the same protein show so obvious difference?

      We apologize for the error in Fig. 4G. The original data presented relative fold changes between OsTLP+BSA and OsTLP+LssaCA treatment, with OsTLP+BSA normalized to 1.0 and OsTLP+LssaCA values expressed as fold changes relative to this baseline. However, the Y-axis was incorrectly labeled as “β-1,3-glucanase (units mg<sup>-1</sup>)”, which suggested absolute enzymatic activity values. We have now corrected the figure (revised Figure 3G) to display the actual absolute enzymatic activity values with the appropriate Y-axis label “β-1,3-glucanase (units mg<sup>-1</sup>)”.

      (7) Fig. 7E, was the LssaCA + NP and LssaCA + GST quantified?

      Yes, all proteins were quantified, and enzymatic activity values were calculated and expressed as units per milligram of proteins (units mg<sup>-1</sup>).

      Minor comments:

      (1) The keywords: In fact, the LssaCA functioned during initial viral infection in plant, but not viral horizontal transmission.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment. We have revised the manuscript title to “Rice stripe virus utilizes an Laodelphax striatellus salivary carbonic anhydrase to facilitate plant infection by direct molecular interaction” and changed the keyword from “viral horizontal transmission” to “viral infection of plant”.

      (2) Fig. 2A, how about testes? Was this data derived from female insects? Fig. 2C, is the saliva collected from nonviruliferous insects? Fig. 2E, what is the control?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments.

      (1) Fig. 2A: The data present mean and SD calculated from three independent experiments, with 5 tissue samples per experiment. Since 3<sup>rd</sup> instar nymphs were used for feeding experiments in this study, we also used 3<sup>rd</sup> instar RSV-free nymphs to measure gene expression in guts, salivary glands and fat bodies. R-body represents the remaining body after removing these tissues. Female insects were used to measure gene expression in ovaries, and gene expression in testes was also added. We have added this necessary information to the revised manuscript (please refer to new Figure 1F and Lines 402-403).

      (2) Fig. 2C: Yes, saliva was collected from nonviruliferous insects.

      (3) Fig. 2E: The control consisted of 100 mM PBS, as described in the experimental section (Lines 643-644): “A blank control consisted of 2 mL of 100 mM PBS (pH 7.0) mixed with 1 mL of 3 mM p-NPA.” In the revised manuscript, we recombinantly expressed LssaCA and its mutant proteins in both sf9 cells and E.coli. Therefore, we have used the mutant proteins as controls to demonstrate specific enzymatic activity. Please refer to Figure 1C, Lines 115-122 and 621-635 for detailed information.

      (3) Some figure labeling appeared unprofessional. For example, "a-RSV", "loading" in Fig. 1, "W-saliva", "G-saliva" in Fig. 2, and so on, the related explanations were absent.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments. We have thoroughly reviewed all figures to ensure professional labels. Specifically, we have:

      (1) Used proper protein names to label western blots and clearly explained the antibodies used for protein detection.

      (2) Provided comprehensive explanations for all abbreviations used in figures within the corresponding figure legends.

      (3) Ensured consistent and clear labeling throughout all figures.

      Please refer to the revised Figures 1-3 for these corrections.

      (4) Lines 83-84, please cite references on callose preventing viral movement. I do not think the present references were relevant.

      We have added a more relevant reference (Yue et al., 2022, Line 82), which revealed that palmitoylated γb promotes virus cell-to-cell movement by interacting with NbREM1 to inhibit callose deposition at plasmodesmata.

      (5) The background of transgenic plants of OsTLP OE should be characterized. And the overexpression of OsTLP should be shown. Which generation of OsTLP OE did authors use?

      The background of transgenic plants of OsTLP OE and its generation used have been shown in the “Materials and methods” section (Line 782-786) and has been mentioned in the main text (Line 214). T<sup>2</sup> lines have been selected for further analysis (Line 789).

      (6) Fig. 5A, the blank, which derived from plants without exposure to insect, was absent.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments. We have added the non- fed control in the revised Figure 5A-C.

      (7) Fig. 7A, the nonviruruliferous insects were required to serve as a control.

      Immunofluorescence localization of RSV and LssaCA in uninfected L. striatellus salivary glands have been added to the revised manuscript (Figure S2).

      (8) The manuscript needs English language edit.

      The manuscript has undergone comprehensive English language editing to improve clarity, grammar, and overall readability.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The first experiment compares vector inoculation vs microinjection of RSV in tissue. I am not sure that your claim (saliva factors are necessary for inoculation) holds, because the vector injects RSV directly into the phloem, whereas microinjection is less precise and you cannot control where exactly the virus is deposed. However, virus deposited in other tissues than the phloem might not replicate, and indeed you observe, compared to natural vector inoculation, highly reduced virus titers.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments. We agree that the comparison between vector inoculation and microinjection involves multiple confounding factors that could affect the experimental results, including salivary components, RSV inoculation titers, and the precision of viral deposition. As the reviewer correctly points out, the differential outcomes could be attributed to these various factors rather than definitively demonstrating the necessity of salivary factors. Therefore, we have removed this comparison from the revised manuscript and instead focused on elucidating the specific mechanisms by which LssaCA facilitates viral infection.

      (2) Next the authors show that a carbonic anhydrase (CA) that they previously detected in saliva is functional and secreted into rice. I assume this is done with non-infected insects, but I did not find the information. Silencing the CA reduces virus titers in inoculated plants at 14 dpi, but not in infected planthoppers. At 1 dpi, there is no difference in RSV titer in plants inoculated with CA silenced planthoppers or control hoppers. To see a direct effect of CA in virus infection, purified virus is injected together with a control protein or recombinant CA into plants. At 14 dpi, there is about double as much virus in the CA-injected plants, but compared to authentic SBPH inoculation, titers are 20,000 times lower. Actually, I believe it is not very likely that the recombinant CA is active or present so long after initial injection.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comments.

      (1) Our previous study identified the CA proteins from RSV-free insects. We have added this information to the revised manuscript (Line 110).

      (2) We acknowledge the reviewer's concern regarding the lower virus accumulation observed in microinjection experiments compared to vector-mediated inoculation. We have removed these experiments from the revised manuscript and instead focused on elucidating the specific mechanisms by which LssaCA facilitates viral infection.

      (3) We didn’t intend to suggest that LssaCA proteins presented for 14 days post-injection. We measured viral titers at 14 days post-feeding or post-injection because this is the common measurement time point when viral titers are sufficiently high for reliable detection by RT-qPCR or western blotting. We have clarified this rationale in the revised manuscript (Lines 155-157). To determine the actual persistence of LssaCA in plant tissues, we monitored LssaCA protein levels at 1 and 3 dpf. Western blotting analysis revealed that LssaCA protein levels decreased post-feeding and remained detectable at 3 dpf. These results are presented in Figure 2H and described in detail in Lines 184-193.

      (3) Then the authors want to know whether CA activity is required for its proviral action and single amino acid mutants covering the putative active CA site are created. The recombinant mutant proteins have 30-70 % reduced activity, but none of them has zero activity. When microinjected together with RSV into plants, RSV replication is similar as injection with wild type CA. Since no knock-out mutant with zero activity is used, it is difficult to judge whether CA activity is unimportant for viral replication, as claim the authors.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment. We constructed a comprehensive CA mutant protein in which all seven residues constituting the enzymatic active center mutated (LssaCA<sup>H111D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>N139H</sup>, LssaCA<sup>H141D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>H143D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>E153H</sup>, LssaCA<sup>H166D</sup>, LssaCA<sup>T253E</sup>) (Fig. S1B). This LssaCA mutant protein demonstrated complete loss of enzymatic activity (Fig. 1C). However, since we have removed the recombinant CA proteins microinjection experiments from the revised manuscript, we lack sufficient direct evidence to definitively demonstrate that CA enzymatic activity is dispensable for its proviral function. To address the core question raised by these experiments, we have conducted new experiments that provide direct evidence for the importance of LssaCA-OsTLP protein-protein interactions in mediating RSV infection. Additionally, we have incorporated a comprehensive discussion examining carbonic anhydrase activity, pH homeostasis, and viral infection. Please refer to the detailed experimental results and discussion in the sections mentioned in our previous response (Figures 3G-H, Lines 220-227 and 350-371).

      (4) Next a yeast two hybrid assay reveals interaction with a thaumatin-like rice protein (TLP). It would be nice to know whether you detected other interacting proteins as well. The interaction is confirmed by pulldown and binding affinity assay using recombinant proteins. The kD is in favor of a rather weak interaction between the two proteins.

      We have added a list of rice proteins that potentially interact with LssaCA (Table S1) and have measured interactions with additional proteins (unpublished data). Despite the relatively weak binding affinity, the functional significance of the LssaCA-OsTLP interaction in enhancing TLP enzymatic activity is substantial.

      (5) Then the glucanase activity of TLP is measured using recombinant TLP-MBP or in vivo expressed TLP. It is not clear to me which TLP is used in Fig. 4G (plant-expressed or bacteria-expressed). If it is plant-expressed TLP, why is its basic activity 10 times lower than in Fig. 4F?

      Fig. 4G is the Fig. 3G in the revised manuscript. A E. coli-expressed TLP protein has been used. We apologize for the error in our original Fig. 4G. The original data presented relative fold changes between OsTLP+BSA and OsTLP+LssaCA treatment, with OsTLP+BSA normalized to 1.0 and OsTLP+LssaCA values expressed as fold changes relative to this baseline. However, the Y-axis was incorrectly labeled as “β-1,3-glucanase (units mg<sup>-1</sup>)”, which suggested absolute enzymatic activity values. We have now corrected the figure to display the actual absolute enzymatic activity values with the appropriate Y-axis label “β-1,3-glucanase (units mg<sup>-1</sup>)”.

      (6) There is also a discrepancy in the construction of the transgenic rice plants: did you use TLP without signal peptide or full length TLP? If you used TLP without signal peptide, you should explain why, because the wild type TLP contains a signal peptide.

      We cloned the full-length OsTLP gene including the signal peptide sequence (Line 782 in the revised manuscript).

      (7) The authors find that CA increases glucanase activity of TLP. Next the authors test callose deposition by aniline blue staining. Feeding activity of RSV-infected planthoppers induces more callose deposition than does feeding by uninfected insects. In the image (Fig. 5A) I see blue stain all over the cell walls of xylem and phloem cells. Is this what the authors expect? I would have expected rather a patchy pattern of callose deposition on cell walls. Concerning sieve plates, I cannot discern any in the image; they are easier to visualize in longitudinal sections than in transversal section as presented here.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment.

      (1) Callose deposition pattern: While callose deposition was detected in other parts of the vascular bundle, significant differences between treatments were mainly observed in phloem cells, indicating that phloem-specific callose deposition is the primary response to RSV infection and SBPH feeding (Figures 5B and 5J).

      (2) Sieve plate visualization: We have examined longitudinal sections to visualize sieve plates, which revealed callose deposition in sieve plates during SBPH feeding and RSV infection (Figure S7).

      (3) Quantitative analysis: In addition to aniline blue staining, we quantified callose concentrations using a plant callose ELISA kit to provide more precise measurements (Figure 5A, 5I and S8A).

      (4) Gene expression analysis: We utilized RT-qPCR to measure callose synthase expression in both feeding and non-feeding areas, confirming that callose synthesis was induced specifically in feeding regions, leading to localized callose deposition (Figures 5D-H).

      These experimental results collectively demonstrate that RSV infection induces enhanced callose synthesis and deposition, with this response occurring primarily in phloem cells, including sieve plates, within feeding sites and their immediate vicinity.

      (8) I do not quite understand how you quantified callose deposition (arbitrary areas?) with ImageJ. Please indicate in detail the analysis method.

      We have added more detailed information for the methods to quantify callose deposition (Lines 673-678).

      (9) More callose content is also observed by a callose ELISA assay of tissue extracts and supported by increased expression of glucanase synthase genes. Did you look whether expression of TLP is changed by feeding activity and RSV infection? Silencing CA in planthoppers increases callose deposition, which is inline with the observation that CA increases TLP activity.

      We measured OsTLP expression following feeding by RSV-free or RSV-infected SBPH and found that gene expression was not significantly affected by either insect feeding or RSV infection. These results have been added to the revised manuscript (Lines 275-277 and Figure 5H).

      (10) Next, callose is measured after feeding of RSV-infected insects on wild type or TLP-overexpressing rice. Less callose deposition (after 2 days) and more virus (after 14 days) is observed in TLP overexpressors. I am missing a control in this experiment, that is feeding of uninfected insects on wild type or TLP overexpressing rice, where I would expect intermediate callose levels.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment and fully agree with the prediction. In the revised manuscript, we have constructed ostlp mutant plants and conducted additional experiments to further clarify how callose deposition is regulated by insect feeding, RSV infection, LssaCA levels, and OsTLP expression. Specifically: 

      (1) Both SBPH feeding and RSV infection induce callose deposition, with RSV-infected insect feeding resulting in significantly higher callose levels compared to RSV-free insect feeding (Fig. 5A-C).

      (2) LssaCA enhances OsTLP enzymatic activity, thereby promoting callose degradation (Fig. 5I-K).

      (3) OsTLP-overexpressing (OE) plants exhibit lower callose levels than wild-type (WT) plants, while ostlp mutant plants show higher callose levels than WT (Fig. 6A-B).

      (4) In ostlp knockout plants, LssaCA no longer affects callose levels, indicating that OsTLP is required for LssaCA-mediated regulation of callose (Fig. 6C-D).

      These additional data address the reviewer’s concern and support the conclusion that OsTLP plays a central role in modulating callose levels in response to RSV infection and insect feeding.

      (11) Next the authors test for interaction between virions and CA. Immunofluorescence shows that RSV and CA colocalize in salivary glands; in my opinion, there is partial and not complete colocalization (Fig. 7A).

      We agree with the reviewer’s observation. CA is primarily produced in the small lobules of the principal salivary glands, while RSV infects nearly all parts of the salivary glands. In regions where RSV and CA colocalize within the principal glands, the CA signal appears sharper than that of RSV, likely due to the relatively higher abundance of CA compared to RSV in these areas. This may explain the partial, rather than complete, colocalization observed in our original Figure 7A. In the revised manuscript, please refer to Figure 1A.

      (12) Pulldown experiments with recombinant RSV NP capsid protein and CA confirm interaction, binding affinity assays indicate rather weak interaction between CA and NP. Likewise in pull-down experiments, interaction between NP, CA and TLP is shown. Finally, in vitro activity assays show that activity of preformed TLP-CA complexes can be increased by adding NP; activity of TLP alone is not shown.

      We performed two independent experiments to confirm the influence on TLP enzymatic activity by LssaCA or by the LssaCA-RSV NP complex. In the first experiment, we compared the enhancement of TLP activity by LssaCA using TLP alone as a control (Figure 3G). In the second experiment examining the LssaCA-RSV NP complex effect on TLP activity, we used the LssaCA-TLP combination as the baseline control rather than TLP alone (Figure 4B), since we had already established the LssaCA enhancement effect in the previous experiment.

      (13) For all microscopic acquisitions, you should indicate the exact acquisition conditions, especially excitation and emission filter settings, kind of camera used and objectives. Use of inadequate filters or of a black & white camera could for example be the reason why you observe a homogeneous cell wall label in the aniline blue staining assays. Counterstaining cell walls with propidium iodide might help distinguish between cell wall and callose label.

      Thank you for your insightful suggestions. We have added the detailed information to the revised manuscript (Lines 656-659 and 673-678).

      (14) You should provide information whether CA is deregulated in infected planthoppers, as this could also modify its mode of action.\

      We have compared LssaCA mRNA levels in RSV-free and RSV-infected L.striatellus salivary glands. The results indicated that RSV infection does not significantly affect LssaCA expression (Figure 1J).

      (15) You should show purity of the proteins used for affinity binding measurements.

      We have included SDS-PAGE results of purified proteins in the revised manuscript (Figure S3).

      (16) L 39: Not all arboviruses are inoculated into the phloem.

      Thank you. We have revised this description (Lines 40, 73, 95 and 97).

      (17) L 76: Watery saliva is also injected in epidermis and mesophyll cells.

      Thank you. We have revised this description (Line 73).

      (18) L 79: What do you mean by "avirulent gene"?

      Thank you for your valuable comments. We have revised this description as “certain salivary effectors may be recognized by plant resistance proteins to induce effector-triggered immunity”. Please refer to Lines 76-77 for detail.

      (19) L 128: Please add delivery method.

      Thank you. We have added the delivery methods (Line 134).

      (20) L 195: Please explain "MST".

      Explained (Line 124). Thank you.

      (21) L 203: Please add the plant species overexpressing TLP.

      Added (Line 214). Thank you.

      (22) L 213: Callose deposition has also a role against phloem-feeding insects.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insight comment. We have added this information to the revised manuscript (Line 252).

      (23) L 626: What is a "mutein"?

      "mutein" is an abbreviation for mutant proteins. Since the recombinant protein microinjection experiments have been removed from the revised manuscript, the term “mutein” has also been removed. For all other instances, we now use the full term “mutant proteins”.

      (24) Fig. 1E: what is "loading"? You should rather show here and elsewhere (or add to supplement) complete protein gels and Western blot membranes and not only bands of interest.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. Although Figure 1E has been removed from the revised manuscript, we have carefully reviewed all figures to ensure that the term “loading” has been replaced with the specific protein names where appropriate.

      (25) Fig. 2C: Please indicate which is the blot and which is the silver stained gel and add mass markers in kDa to the silver stained gel.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have revised figure to include labeled silver-stained gels with indicated molecular weight markers (Figure 1H in the revised manuscript).

    1. Author response:

      We thank the reviewers for their thoughtful comments and constructive suggestions. We describe how we will address each point below and are grateful for the guidance on areas where our work could be clarified or expanded. In particular, we note the following:

      Selection scan summary statistics: In our revised manuscript, we will include summary statistics from the selection scans. We believe this addition will enhance transparency and provide additional context for readers.

      Reporting of outliers: As highlighted by the editor, the reviewers expressed differing views on the most appropriate way to report outliers. To provide a comprehensive and balanced presentation, we will report both the empirical selection statistics and the corresponding converted p-values. This dual approach will allow readers to fully interpret the results under both perspectives.

      Methodological considerations: We have carefully considered the reviewers' methodological suggestions and will incorporate them into our revisions where possible. These changes strengthen the rigor and clarity of the analyses.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper reports an analysis of whole-genome sequence data from 40 Faroese. The authors investigate aspects of demographic history and natural selection in this population. The key findings are that the Faroese (as expected) have a small population size and are broadly of Northwest European ancestry. Accordingly, selection signatures are largely shared with other Northwest European populations, although the authors identify signals that may be specific to the Faroes. Finally, they identify a few predicted deleterious coding variants that may be enriched in the Faroes.

      Strengths:

      The data are appropriately quality-controlled and appear to be of high quality. Some aspects of the Faroese population history are characterized, in particular, by the relatively (compared to other European populations) high proportion of long runs of homozygosity, which may be relevant for disease mapping of recessive variants. The selection analysis is presented reasonably, although as the authors point out, many aspects, for example differences in iHS, can reflect differences in demographic history or population-specific drift and thus can't reliably be interpreted in terms of differences in the strength of selection.

      Weaknesses:

      The main limitations of the paper are as follows:

      (1) The data are not available. I appreciate that (even de-identified) genotype data cannot be shared; however, that does substantially reduce the value of the paper. Minimally, I think the authors should share summary statistics for the selection scans, in line with the standard of the field.

      We agree with the reviewer that sharing the selection scan results is important, so in the next revision of this manuscript we will make the selection scan summary statistics publicly available, and clearly lay out the guidelines and research questions for which the data can be accessed.

      (2) The insight into the population history of the Faroes is limited, relative to what is already known (i.e., they were settled around 1200 years ago, by people with a mixture of Scandinavian and British ancestry, have a small effective population size, and any admixture since then comes from substantially similar populations). It's obvious, for example, that the Faroese population has a smaller bottleneck than, say, GBR.

      More sophisticated analyses (for example, ARG-based methods, or IBD or rare variant sharing) would be able to reveal more detailed and fine-scale information about the history of the populations that is not already known. PCA, ADMIXTURE, and HaplotNet analysis are broad summaries, but the interesting questions here would be more specific to the Faroes, for example, what are the proportions of Scandinavian vs Celtic ancestry? What is the date and extent of sex bias (as suggested by the uniparental data) in this admixture? I think that it is a bit of a missed opportunity not to address these questions.

      We clarify that we did quantify the proportions of various ancestry components as estimated by HaploNet in main text Figure 5 and supplemental figures S5 and S6. In our revisions, we will include the average global ancestry of the various components in the Main Text so that this result is more clear.

      We agree that more fine-scale demographic analyses would be informative. We have begun working on an estimation of the admixture date, for example, but have encountered problems with using different standard date estimation software, which give very inconsistent and unstable results. We suspect this might be due to the strong bottleneck experienced in the history of the Faroe Islands breaking one or more of the assumptions of these methods. We will continue working on this problem in coming months, possibly using simulations to assess where the problem might be. We recognize that our relatively small sample size places limits on the fine-scale demographic analyses that can be performed. We are addressing this in ongoing work by generating a larger cohort, which we hope will enable more detailed inference in the future.

      (3) I don't really understand the rationale for looking at HLA-B allele frequencies. The authors write that "ankylosing spondylitis (AS) may be at a higher prevalence in the Faroe Islands (unpublished data), however, this has not been confirmed by follow-up epidemiological studies". So there's no evidence (certainly no published evidence) that AS is more prevalent, and hence nothing to explain with the HLA allele frequencies?

      We agree that no published studies have confirmed a higher prevalence of ankylosing spondylitis (AS) in the Faroe Islands. Our recruitment data suggest that AS might be more common than in other European populations, but we understand that this is only based on limited, unpublished observations and what we are hearing from the community. We emphasized in our original manuscript that this is based on observational evidence from the FarGen project. However, as this reviewer pointed out, we can be more clear that this prevalence has not been formally studied.

      In our next revision we will clarify in the text that our recruitment data suggest a higher prevalence of AS may be possible, but more formal epidemiological studies are needed to confirm this observation. The reason we study HLA-B allele frequencies is to see if the genetic background of the Faroese population could help explain this possible difference, since HLA-B27 is already known to play a strong role in AS.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      In this paper, Hamid et al present 40 genomes from the Faroe Islands. They use these data (a pilot study for an anticipated larger-scale sequencing effort) to discuss the population genetic diversity and history of the sample, and the Faroes population. I think this is an overall solid paper; it is overall well-polished and well-written. It is somewhat descriptive (as might be expected for an explorative pilot study), but does make good use of the data.

      The data processing and annotation follows a state-of-the-art protocol, and at least I could not find any evidence in the results that would pinpoint towards bioinformatic issues having substantially biased some of the results, and at least preliminary results lead to the identification of some candidate disease alleles, showing that small, isolated cohorts can be an efficient way to find populations with locally common, but globally rare disease alleles.

      I also enjoyed the population structure analysis in the context of ancient samples, which gives some context to the genetic ancestry of Faroese, although it would have been nice if that could have been quantified, and it is unfortunate that the sampling scheme effectively precludes within-Faroes analyses.

      We note that although the ancestry proportions are not specified in the main text, we did quantify ancestry proportions in the modern Faroese individuals and other ancient samples, and we visualized these proportions in Figure 5 and Supplementary Figures S5 and S6. As stated in our response to Reviewer #1, in our revisions, we will more clearly state the average global ancestry of the various components in the Main Text.

      I am unfortunately quite critical of the selection analysis, both on a statistical level and, more importantly, I do not believe it measures what the authors think it does.

      Major comments:

      (1) Admixture timing/genomic scaling/localization:

      As the authors lay out, the Faroes were likely colonized in the last 1,000-1,500 years, i.e., 40-60 generations ago. That means most genomic processes that have happened on the Faroese should have signatures that are on the order of ~1-2cM, whereas more local patterns likely indicate genetic history predating the colonization of the islands. Yet, the paper seems to be oblivious to this (to me) fascinating and somewhat unique premise. Maybe this thought is wrong, but I think the authors miss a chance here to explain why the reader should care beyond the fact that the small populations might have high-frequency risk alleles and the Faroes are intrinsically interesting, but more importantly, it also makes me think it leads to some misinterpretations in the selection analysis

      See response to point #3

      (2) ROH:

      Would the sampling scheme impact ROH? How would it deal with individuals with known parental coancestry? As an example of what I mean by my previous comment, 1MB is short enough in that I would expect most/many 1MB ROH-tracts to come from pedigree loops predating the colonization of the Faroes. (i.e, I am actually quite surprised that there isn't much more long ROH, which makes me wonder if that would be impacted by the sampling scheme).

      The sampling scheme was designed to choose 40 Faroese individuals that were representative of the different regions and were minimally related. There were no pairs of third-degree relatives or closer (pi-hat > 0.125) in either the Faroese cohort or the reference populations. It is possible that this sampling scheme would reduce the amount of longer ROHs in the population, but we should still be able to see overall patterns of ROH reflective of bottlenecks in the past tens of generations. Additionally, based on this reviewer's earlier comment, 1 Mb ROHs would still be relevant to demographic events in the last 40-60 generations given that on average 1 cM corresponds to 1 Mb in humans, though we recognize that is not an exact conversion.

      That said, the “sum total amount of the genome contained in long ROH” as we described in the manuscript includes all ROHs greater than 1Mb. Although we group all ROHs longer than 1Mb into one category in the current manuscript, we can look more specifically at the distribution of the longer ROH in future revisions and add discussion into what this might tell us about the timing of bottlenecks. 

      For now, we share a plot of the distribution in ROH lengths across all individuals for each cohort. As this plot shows, there certainly are ROHs longer than 1Mb in the Faroese cohort, and on average there is a higher proportion of long ROH particularly in the 5-15 Mb range in the Faroese cohort relative to the other cohorts.

      Author response image 1.

      (3) Selection scan:

      We are talking about a bottlenecked population that is recently admixed (Faroese), compared to a population (GBR) putatively more closely related to one of its sources. My guess would be that selection in such a scenario would be possibly very hard to detect, and even then, selection signals might not differentiate selection in Faroese vs. GBR, but rather selection/allele frequency differences between different source populations. I think it would be good to spell out why XP-EHH/iHS measures selection at the correct time scale, and how/if these statistics are expected to behave differently in an admixed population.

      The reviewer brings up good points about the utility of classical selection statistics in populations that are admixed or bottlenecked, and whether the timescale at which these statistics detect selection is relevant for understanding the selective history of the Faroese population. We break down these concerns separately.

      (1) Bottlenecks: Recent bottlenecks result in higher LD within a population. However, demographic events such as bottlenecks affect global genomic patterns while positive selection is expected to affect local genomic patterns. For this reason, iHS and XP-EHH statistics are standardized against the genome-wide background, to account for population-specific demographic history.

      (2) Admixture: The term “admixture” has different interpretations depending on the line of inquiry and the populations being studied. Across various time and geographic scales, all human populations are admixed to some degree, as gene flow between groups is a common fixture throughout our history. For example,

      even the modern British population has “admixed” ancestry from North / West European sources as well, dating to at least as recently as the Medieval & Viking periods (Gretzinger et al. 2022, Leslie et al. 2015), yet we do not commonly consider it an “admixed” population, and we are not typically concerned about applying haplotype-based statistics in this population. This is due to the low divergence between the source populations. In the case of the Faroe Islands, we believe admixture likely occurred on a similar timescale. We see low variance in ancestry proportions estimated by HaploNet, both from the historical Faroese individuals (250BP) and the modern samples. This indicates admixture predating the settlement of the Faroe Islands, where recombination has had time to break up long ancestry tracts and the global ancestry proportions have reached an equilibrium. That is, these ancestry patterns suggest that the modern Faroese are most likely descended from already admixed founders. We mention this as a likely possibility in the main text: “This could have occurred either via a mixture of the original “West Europe” ancestry with individuals of predominantly “North Europe” ancestry, or a by replacement with individuals that were already of mixed ancestry at the time of arrival in the islands (the latter are not uncommon in Viking Age mainland Europe).” And, as with the case of the British population, the closely-related ancestral sources for the Faroese founders were likely not so diverged as to have differences in allele frequencies and long-range haplotypes that would disrupt signals of selection from iHS or XP-EHH.

      (3) Time scale: It is certainly possible, and in fact likely, that iHS measures selection older than the settlement of the Faroe Islands. In our manuscript, we calculated iHS in both the Faroese and the closely related British cohort, and we highlight in the main Main Text that the top signals, with the exception of LCT, are shared between the two cohorts, indicative of selection that began prior to the population split. iHS is a commonly calculated statistic, and it is often calculated in a single population without comparing to others, so we feel it is important to show our result demonstrating these shared selection signals. In future revisions, we will emphasize in the main text that we are not claiming to have identified selection that occurred in the Faroese population post-settlement with the iHS statistic. As far as XP-EHH, it is a statistic designed to identify differentiated variants that are fixed or approaching fixation in one population but not others. The time-scale of selection that XP-EHH can detect would therefore be dependent on the populations used for comparison. As XP-EHH has the best power to identify alleles that are fixed or approaching fixation in one population but not others, it is less likely to detect older selection events / incomplete sweeps from the source populations.

      In our next revision, we will more clearly state limitations of these statistics under various population histories, and clarify the time-scale at which we are detecting selection for iHS vs XP-EHH.

      (4) Similarly, for the discussion of LCT, I am not convinced that the haplotypes depicted here are on the right scale to reflect processes happening on the Faroes. Given the admixture/population history, it at the very least should be discussed in the context of whether the 13910 allele frequency on the Faroes is at odds with what would be expected based on the admixture sources.

      We agree that more investigation into the LCT allele frequency in the other ancient samples may provide some insight into the selection history, particularly in light of ancient admixture. Please note, we did look at the allele frequency of the LCT allele rs4988235 and stated in the main text that it was present at high frequencies in the historical (250BP) Faroese samples. The frequency of this allele in the imputed historical Faroese samples is 82% while the allele is present at ~74% frequency in modern samples. We did not report the exact percentage in the main text because the sample size of the historical samples (11 individuals) is small and coverage of ancient samples is low, leading to potential errors in imputation. However, we can try to calculate the LCT allele frequency in other ancient samples, and assuming that we have good proxies for the sources at the time of admixture, we may calculate the expected allele frequency in the admixed ancestors of the Faroese founders in the next revision.

      (5) I am lacking information to evaluate the procedure for turning the outliers into p-values. Both iHS and XP-EHH are ratio statistics, meaning they might be heavy-tailed if one is not careful, and the central limit theorem may not apply. It would be much easier (and probably sufficient for the points being made here) to reframe this analysis in terms of empirical outliers.

      Given that there are disagreements on the best approach to reporting selection scan results from the reviewers, in our revision, we can additionally supply both the standardized iHS / XP-EHH values in the supplementary information as well as these values transformed to p-values. As the p-values are derived from the empirical distribution, the “significant” p-values are also empirical outliers from the empirical distribution, so the conclusions of the manuscript do not change. We found that the p-value approach and controlling for FDR is more conservative, with fewer signals reaching “significance” than are considered empirical outliers based on common approaches such as IQR or arbitrary percentile cutoffs.

      (6) Oldest individual predating gene flow: It seems impossible to make any statements based on a single individual. Why is it implausible that this person (or their parents), e.g., moved to the Faroes within their lifetime and died there?

      We agree with the reviewer that this is a plausible explanation, and in future revisions we will update the main text to acknowledge this possibility.

    1. Author response:

      We sincerely thank the editors and the reviewers for their feedback in helping us improve this manuscript. During the time this work has been under review, 10x Genomics has updated the probe sequences of their gene panels. We therefore plan to update these findings as well as further expand to incorporate reviewer recommendations.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper by Kim et al. investigates the potential of stimulating the dopaminergic A13 region to promote locomotor restoration in a Parkinson's mouse model. Using wild-type mice, 6-OHDA injection depletes dopaminergic neurons in the substantia nigra pars compacta, without impairing those of the A13 region and the ventral tegmentum area, as previously reported in humans. Moreover, photostimulation of presumably excitatory (CAMKIIa) neurons in the vicinity of the A13 region improves bradykinesia and akinetic symptoms after 6-OHDA injection. Whole-brain imaging with retrograde and anterograde tracers reveals that the A13 region undergoes substantial changes in the distribution of its afferents and projections after 6-OHDA injection, thus suggesting a remodeling of the A13 connectome. Whether this remodelling contributes to pro-locomotor effects of the photostimulation of the A13 region remains unknown as causality was not addressed.

      Strengths:

      Photostimulation of presumably excitatory (CAMKIIa) neurons in the vicinity of the A13 region promotes locomotion and locomotor recovery of wild-type mice 1 month after 6-OHDA injection in the medial forebrain bundle, thus identifying a new potential target for restoring motor functions in Parkinson's disease patients. The study also provides a description of the A13 region connectome pertaining to motor behaviors and how it changes after a dopaminergic lesion. Although there is no causal link between anatomical and behavioral data, it raises interesting questions for further studies.

      Thank you for the comments.

      Weaknesses:

      Although CAMKIIa is a marker of presumably excitatory neurons and can be used as an alternative marker of dopaminergic neurons, some uncertainty remains regarding the phenotype of neurons underlying recovery of akinesia and improvement of bradykinesia.

      The primary objective was to focus on a population of neurons that could contribute to functional recovery, with a long-term translational focus in mind. We have followed up on this by creating a rat-based DBS model of stimulating the A13 region (Bisht et al 2025). We agree that the next steps are to genetically dissect the circuits, and we have made a start on this with our recent publication (Sharma et al 2024).

      Figure 4 is improved, but the results from the correlation analyses remain difficult to interpret, as they may reflect changes in various impaired brain regions independently of the A13 region. While the analysis offers a snapshot of correlated changes within the connectome, it does not identify which specific cell or axonal populations are actually increasing or decreasing. Although functional MRI connectome analyses are well-established, anatomical data seem less suitable for this purpose. How can one interpret correlated changes in anatomical inputs or outputs between two distinct regions?

      We appreciate the reviewer's thoughtful comment regarding the interpretability of the correlation analyses in Figure 4. We fully acknowledge that our anatomical data cannot establish causality or identify specific cell types or axonal populations undergoing changes following unilateral nigrostriatal degeneration. However, our intent with this analysis was not to infer mechanistic pathways but rather to provide a systems-level overview of how the global organization of A13 efferents and afferents is altered following 6-OHDA lesioning. By calculating proportions of total inputs and outputs and comparing them across brain regions, we aimed to control for variability in labeling and highlight relative shifts in network organization. The correlation matrices are intended to capture coordinated changes in input/output distribution patterns, effectively reflecting how groups of regions co-vary in their input to or output from the A13 region. In our case, we used correlation analysis to identify how input and output distributions across brain regions reorganize as a network following 6-OHDA lesioning. For example, a positive correlation between inputs from Region A and Region B to the A13 suggests that across animals, when input from Region A is relatively high, input from Region B tends to be high as well, indicating that connectivity from these regions to the A13 may be co-regulated or affected similarly by the lesion. Conversely, a shift from positive to negative correlation may signal a divergence in how regions contribute to the A13 connectome after nigrostriatal degeneration (e.g., increased connectivity to Region A compared to reduced connectivity to Region B). Thus, these patterns offer new insight into the broader reorganization of the A13 connectome and may serve as systems-level signatures of altered anatomical organization, providing a foundation for future mechanistic investigations using circuit-specific tools. We have revised the text to better emphasize the correlative and descriptive nature of these analyses and to clarify that they serve as a hypothesis-generating exploration. Future studies using cell type- and/or projection-specific functional manipulations will be essential to determine the causal roles of these reorganized circuits. We believe our use of this method is justified in the context of exploring broad, lesion-induced network reorganization, and we hope this additional context helps clarify the purpose and limitations of our approach.

      Figure 5 is also improved, but there is room for further enhancement. As currently presented, it is difficult to distinguish the differences between the sham and 6-OHDA groups. The first column could compare afferents, while the second column could compare efferents. Given the small sample size, it would be more appropriate to present individual data rather than the mean and standard deviation.

      We have reorganized Figure 5 as suggested.

      Appraisal and impact

      Although the behavioral experiments are convincing, the low number of animals in the anatomical studies is insufficient to make any relevant statistical conclusions due to extremely low statistical power.

      See previous comments on this.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Points that need to be addressed:

      Figure S1 is supposed to illustrate the percentage of expression in all mice, but the number of mice does not match (n=3 and 3 in Figure S1 versus n=5 and 6 in Figure 1). Revise the legend or add the missing data.

      We have added the additional data to this graph (Figure 2 – figure supplement 1) and have separated out 6-OHDA and sham mice for clarity.

      Page 4: "There was also an increase in the number of ChR2 cells with c-fos labeling in 6-OHDA ChR2 mice compared to the 6-OHDA eYFP mice. However, there was no net increase in TH+ cells labelled with ChR2 and c-Fos suggesting a heterogeneous population of activated cells." A quantification will be necessary to advance this conclusion.

      We were able to determine that there was a trend of increased c-Fos intensity within the A13 region following photostimulation. However, the variability in the data makes it premature to comment on the TH co-localization and we have deleted this statement.

      Figure 3: The choice of red and green could be a problem for color-blind people.

      Thank you - switched to orange and cyan instead.

      Page 7, 4th paragraph: "6-OHDA mice demonstrated significantly greater descent times than sham mice (Figure 3L, p<0.01)." This is not what is shown in the Figure 3L.

      We made changes in the legend and text to clarify.

      Page 7, last line: PT abbreviation should be introduced in parentheses at the beginning of this section.

      Removed the abbreviation.

      Figure S4A: The authors should show data for the VTA or refer to the quantification of Figure S4G in the text.

      Now referenced correctly in the text.

      Figure S7 and S8 are not referenced in the results or methods.

      References added to text.

      Double-check the formatting of some references: L.-X. Li et al, 2021, L. Kim et al., 2021.

      References checked and corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this study, Bonnifet et al. profile the presence of L1 ORF1p in the mouse and human brain and report that ORF1p is expressed in the human and mouse brain specifically in neurons at steady state and that there is an age-dependent increase in expression. This is a timely report as two recent papers have extensively documented the presence of full-length L1 transcripts in the mouse and human brain (PMID: 38773348 & PMID: 37910626). Thus, the finding that L1 ORF1p is consistently expressed in the brain is important to document and will be of value to the field. 

      Strengths: 

      Several parts of this manuscript appear to be well done and include the necessary controls. In particular, the documentation of neuron-specific expression of ORF1p in the mouse brain is an interesting finding with nice documentation. This will be very useful information for the field. 

      We thank the reviewer for this positive comment. 

      Weaknesses: 

      Several parts of the manuscript appear to be more preliminary and need further experiments to validate their claims. In particular, the data suggesting expression of L1 ORF1p in the human brain and the data suggesting increased expression in the aged brain need further validation. Detailed comments: 

      (1) The expression of ORF1p in the human brain shown in Fig. 1j is puzzling. Why are there two strong bands in the WB? How can the authors be sure that this signal represents ORF1p expression and not non-specific labelling? While the authors discuss that others have found double bands when examining human ORF1p, there are also several labs that report only one band. This discrepancy in the field should at least be discussed and the uncertainties with their findings should be acknowledged. 

      Please see also our extensive response to this comment we made in round #1 of the revisions.

      As a summary, in response to the initial review, we included several lines of additional evidence in the revised manuscript:

      siRNA-mediated knockdown of ORF1p in human neurons, resulting in ≈50% signal reduction using the antibody in question (Suppl. Fig. 2C) immunoprecipitation using the human ORF1p antibody in question confirming signal specificity (Suppl. Fig. 2B) use of a second antibody in immunostainings, including a new control (Suppl. Fig. 2E) and a revised discussion acknowledging the uncertainty surrounding the lower band:

      “The double band pattern in Western blots has been observed in other studies for human ORF1p outside of the brain as well as for mouse ORF1p. […] The nature of the lower band is unknown, but might be due to truncation, specific proteolysis or degradation.”

      We have also now added more content to the paragraph starting from line 183 : "While there is some discrepancy in the field, the double band pattern in Western blots..."

      To our understanding, this combination of independent methods using two antibodies and complementary validation strategies supports the presence of ORF1p in human brain tissue.

      (2) The data showing a reduction in ORF1p expression in the aged mouse brain is an interesting observation, but the effect magnitude of effect is very limited and somewhat difficult to interpret. This finding should be supported by orthogonal methods to strengthen this conclusion. For example, by WB and by RNA-seq (to verify that the increase in protein is due to an increase in transcription). 

      This would indeed be valuable but at this point, we will not be able to perform these experiments at this point (please also see revision #1 for a more detailed answer)

      (3) The transcriptomic data using human postmortem tissue presented in Figure 4 and Figure 5 are not convincing. Quantification of transposon expression on short read sequencing has important limitations. Longer reads and complementary approaches are needed to study the expression of evolutionarily young L1s (see PMID: 38773348 & PMID: 37910626 for examples of the current state of the art). As presented, the human RNA data is inconclusive due to the short read length and small sample size. The value of including an inconclusive analysis in the manuscript is difficult to understand. With this data set, the authors cannot investigate age-related changes in L1 expression in human neurons. 

      Please see also our extensive response to this comment we made in round #1 of the revisions.

      In the revised version, we have added further statistical analyses, incorporated locus-specific mappability scores and provided an even more nuanced interpretation of our findings, as illustrated in lines 390 and 427.

      We have acknowledged the limitations of short-read sequencing in this context, while referencing established methodologies (e.g., Teissandier et al., 2019) and recent benchmarking studies (e.g., Schwarz et al., 2022) that validate the use of such data under specific precautions—many of which we have implemented.

      Given these considerations, and with the guidance of a co-author with specific expertise in TE bioinformatics, we believe our approach is justified and robust.

      (4) In line with these comments, the title should be changed to better reflect the findings in the manuscript. A title that does not mention "L1 increase with aging" would be better. 

      In line with our response to Point (3), we prefer to retain the current analyses and discussion, which we believe strike an appropriate balance between caution and added scientific value.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      Bonnifet et al. sought to characterize the expression pattern of L1 ORF1p expression across the entire mouse brain, in young and aged animals and to corroborate their characterization with Western blotting for L1 ORF1p and L1 RNA expression data from human samples. They also queried L1 ORF1p interacting partners in the mouse brain by IP-MS. 

      Strengths: 

      A major strength of the study is the use of two approaches: a deep-learning detection method to distinguish neuronal vs. non-neuronal cells and ORF1p+ cells vs. ORF1p- cells across large-scale images encompassing multiple brain regions mapped by comparison to the Allen Brain Atlas, and confocal imaging to give higher resolution on specific brain regions. These results are also corroborated by Western blotting on six mouse brain regions. Extension of their analysis to post-mortem human samples, to the extent possible, is another strength of the paper. The identification of novel ORF1p interactors in brain is also a strength in that it provides a novel dataset for future studies. 

      We thank the reviewer for these positive comments.

      Weaknesses: 

      The main weakness of the IP-MS portion of the study is that none of the interactors were individually validated or subjected to follow-up analyses. The list of interactors was compared to previously published datasets, but not to ORF1p interactors in any other mouse tissue.

      As we had stated in the first round of revision, the list of previously published datasets does include a mouse dataset with ORF1p interacting proteins in mouse spermatocytes (please see line 478-4479: “ORF1p interactors found in mouse spermatocytes were also present in our analysis including CNOT10, CNOT11, PRKRA and FXR2 among others (Suppl_Table4).”) -> De Luca, C., Gupta, A. & Bortvin, A. Retrotransposon LINE-1 bodies in the cytoplasm of piRNA-deficient mouse spermatocytes: Ribonucleoproteins overcoming the integrated stress response. PLoS Genet 19, e1010797 (2023)). We agree that a validation of protein interactors of ORF1p in the mouse brain would have been valuable. However, the significant overlap with previously published interactors highlights the validity of our data. As reviewer #2 points out in the comments on revisions, we hope that follow-up studies will address these points and we anticipate that this list of ORF1p protein interactors in the mouse brain will be of further use for the community.

      Comments on revisions: 

      The co-staining of Orf1p with Parvalbumin (PV) presented in Supplemental Figure S5 is a welcome addition exploring the cell type-specificity of Orf1p staining, and broadly corroborates the work of Bodea et al. while revealing that Orf1p also is expressed in non-PV+ cells, consistent with L1 activity across a range of neuronal subtypes. The authors also have strengthened their findings regarding the increased intensity of ORF1p staining in aged compared to young animals, and the newly presented results are indeed more convincing. The prospect of increased neuronal L1 activity with age is exciting, and the results in this paper have provided the groundwork for ongoing discoveries in this area. While it is disappointing that no Orf1p interactors were followed up, this is understandable and the data are nonetheless valuable and will likely prove useful to future studies. 

      Thank you for your time and constructive comments.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      We would recommend that the human RNA-seq analysis is removed from the manuscript. The human RNA data is inconclusive due to the short read length and small sample size. The value of including an inconclusive analysis in the manuscript is difficult to understand. With this data set, the authors cannot investigate age-related changes in L1 expression in human neurons. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Thank you for addressing my suggestions. I have no further recommendations at this time.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      “The gar-3 promoter expression pattern was not discussed in the context of rescue experiments.”

      We agree that the expression pattern of the gar-3 promoter used in our rescue experiments should be clarified. We will include a description of the tissues where the 7.5 kb gar-3 promoter fragment is expressed, based on both prior studies and our own expression data. We will also discuss how the gar-3 cell and tissue expression pattern relates to both our analysis of gar-3 expression in the genome edited strain we generated as well as the observed rescue effects.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The site of action of cholinergic signaling was not adequately explored.

      We plan to perform additional rescue experiments using heterologous promoters to drive gar-3 expression in specific tissues (e.g. cholinergic neurons, muscle). These experiments will help clarify the sufficiency of unc-17 expression in specific cell types for rescue. However, we point out that cell-specific unc-17 knockdown by RNAi using the unc-17b promoter (expression largely restricted to ventral cord ACh motor neurons) increases sensitivity to PQ in our long-term survival assays. Combined with our analysis of unc-17(e113) mutants, we believe our data offer robust support of a requirement for unc-17 expression in cholinergic motor neurons.

      (2) Pan-neuronal silencing experiments were not connected to ACh/GAR-3 signaling.

      We will expand our discussion to relate the pan-neuronal silencing results to our analysis of ACh signaling. We used the pan-neuronal silencing to motivate further analysis of various neurotransmitter systems. We note that our studies implicate both glutamatergic and cholinergic systems in protective responses to oxidative stress. The effects of silencing on survival during long-term PQ exposure may therefore be derived solely from cholinergic neurons, glutamatergic neurons, or a combination of both neuronal populations. We hope the reviewer will agree that distinguishing between these possibilities may be quite complicated and is not central to the main message of our paper. We therefore suggest this additional analysis lies outside the scope of this revision.

      (3) Inter-tissue signaling and transcriptional regulation by ACh were assumed but not directly shown.

      We will generate GFP reporters for a subset of genes (including proteasomal genes) identified in our RNA-seq analysis or assess their expression by quantitative RT-PCR to validate cholinergic regulation. These experiments will help to identify target tissues and confirm transcriptional regulation by cholinergic signaling.

      We appreciate the opportunity to revise our manuscript and believe that these additions will significantly strengthen the mechanistic insights and overall impact of our study. Please let us know if further clarification is needed.

  2. Aug 2025
    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary and Strengths:

      The very well-written manuscript by Lövestam et al. from the Scheres/Goedert groups entitled "Twelve phosphomimetic mutations induce the assembly of recombinant fulllength human tau into paired helical filaments" demonstrates the in vitro production of the so-called paired helical filament Alzheimer's disease (AD) polymorph fold of tau amyloids through the introduction of 12 point mutations that attempt to mimic the disease-associated hyper-phosphorylation of tau. The presented work is very important because it enables disease-related scientific work, including seeded amyloid replication in cells, to be performed in vitro using recombinant-expressed tau protein. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The following points are asked to be addressed by the authors:

      (i) In the discussion it would be helpful to note the findings that in AD the chemical structure tau (including phosphorylation) is what defines the polymorph fold and not the buffer/cellular environment. It would be further interesting to discuss these findings in respect to the relationship between disease and structure. The presented findings suggest that due to a cellular/organismal alteration, such as aging or Abeta aggregation, tau is specifically hyper-phosphorylated which then leads to its aggregation into the paired helical filaments that are associated with AD. 

      We have added an extra sentence to the Introduction to emphasise this possibility: “Besides the cellular environment in which they assemble, different tau folds may also be determined by chemical modifications of tau itself.”

      In addition, the last paragraph of the Discussion now reads: “It could be that, besides different cellular environments in which the filaments assemble, different posttranslational modification patterns are also important for the assembly of tau into protofilament folds that are specific for the other tauopathies.”

      (ii) The conditions used for each assembly reaction are a bit hard to keep track of and somewhat ambiguous. In order to help the reader, I would suggest making a table to show conditions used for each type of assembly (including the diameter / throw of the orbital shaker) and the results (structural/biological) of those conditions. For example, presumably the authors did not have ThT in the samples used for cryo-EM but the methods section does not specify this. Also, the presence of trace NaCl is proposed as a possible cause for the CTE fold to appear in the 0N4R sample (page 4) but no explanation of why this particular sample would have more NaCl than the others. Furthermore, it appears that NaCl was actually used in the seeded assembly reactions that produced the PHF and not the CTE fold. This would seem to indicate the CTE structure of 0N4RPAD12 is not actually induced by NaCl (like it was for tau297-391). In order for the reader to better understand the reproducibility of the polymorphs, it would be helpful to indicate in how many different conditions and how many replicates with new protein preparations each polymorph was observed (could be included in the same table)  

      We have added a new table (Table 1) with the buffer conditions, protein concentration and shaking speed and time, for all structures described in this paper. We never added ThT to assembly reactions that were used for cryo-EM.

      We did not use NaCl in the seeded assembly reactions (we used sodium citrate). We don’t really know why 0N4R PAD12 tau more readily forms the CTE fold. The observation that it does so prompted us to use 0N3R for all ensuing experiments. 

      (iii) It is not clear how the authors calculate the percentage of each filament type. In Figure 1 it is stated "discarded solved particles (coloured) and discarded filaments in grey" which leaves the reviewer wondering what a "discarded solved particle" is and which filaments were discarded. From the main text one guesses that the latter is probably false positives from automated picking but if so, these should not be referred to as filaments. Also, are the percentages calculated for filaments or segments? In any case, it would be more helpful in such are report to know the best estimate of the ratio of identified filament types without confusing the reader with a measure of the quality of the picking algorithm. Please clarify. Also, a clarification is asked for the significance of the varying degrees of PHF and AD monomer filaments in the various assembly conditions. It could be expected that there is significant variability from sample to sample but it would be interesting to know if there has been any attempt to reproduce the samples to measure this variability. If not, it might be worth mentioning so that the % values are taking with the appropriate sized grain of salt. Finally, the representation of the data in Figure 1 would seem to imply that the 0N3R forms less or no monofilament AD fold because no cross-section is shown for this structure, however it is very similar to (or statistically the same as) the 1:1 mix of 0N3R:0N4R.

      In the revised manuscript, we have used bi-hierchical clustering of filaments, where each segment (or particle) is classified based on both 2D class assignment and to which filament it belongs (this method is based on [Porthula et al (2019), Ultramicroscopy 203, 132-138] and was further developed in [Lövestam et al (2024) Nature 7993, 119-125]. Based on the assumption that filament type does not change within a single filament type, we have observed that this gives excellent classification results, and that this approach allows classification of many, even small minority, filament types. Using this approach, we now quantify the different filament types on the number of segments extracted from filaments classified in this way. 

      Moreover, we have also addressed the problem of having singlets among the PHF preparation: it turns out that waiting longer, just by transferring samples out of the shaker after one week and incubating it quiescently at 37 ºC for two more weeks, the singlets disappear and only PHFs remain. Filaments made for the fluorophore labelling in the revised Figure 3 were also done using the new protocol. In total, we have N=7 replicates with a mean of 95.3% PHFs and a standard deviation of 9.4%. The revised text in the Results section reads:

      “To further increase the proportions of PHFs-to-singlet ratio, we removed the plate from the shaker after one week and incubated it quiescently at 37 ºC for two more weeks. This resulted in 100% PHFs formed (Figure 1 – figure supplement 4). When repeated seven times, on average 95.3% PHFs formed, with 25% of singlets formed in a single outlier (Figure 1 – figure supplement 5)” 

      (iv) The interpretation of the NMR data on soluble tau that the mutations on the second site are suppressing in part long range dynamic interaction around the aggregationinitiation site (FIA) is sound. It is in particular interesting to find that the mutations have a similar effect as the truncation at residue 391. An additional experiment using solvent PREs to elaborate on the solvent exposed sequence-resolved electrostatic potential and the intra-molecular long range interactions would likely strengthen the interpretation significantly (Iwahara, for example, Yu et al, in JACS 2024). Figure 6D Figure supplement shows the NMR cross peak intensities between tau 151-391 and PAD12tau151-391. Overall the intensities of the PAD12 tau construct are more intense which could be interpreted with less conformational exchange between long range dynamic interactions. There are however several regions which do not show any intensity anymore when compared with the corresponding wildtype construct such as 259-262, 292-294 which should be discussed/explained. 

      While long-range intramolecular interactions of tau have previously been reported through the use of spin labels (Mukrasch et al 2009 PLoS Biol 7(2): e1000034), we have been hesitant to introduce paramagnetic agents into our samples for two reasons. First, the bulky size of the spin label may affect filament formation or influence the dynamic properties of the protein. Second, covalent addition of the spin label requires mutation of the primary sequence to both remove native cysteine residues and add cysteines at the desired label location. We have previously shown that mutation of cysteine 322 to alanine leads to the formation of tau filaments with a structure that is different from the PHF (Santambrogio et al (2025) bioRxiv 2025.03.29.646137). 

      Instead, we have included in the revised manuscript new NMR and cryo-EM data that provide further support for the model that a FIA-like interaction between residues <sub>392</sub>IVYK<sub>395</sub> and residues <sub>306</sub>VQIVYK<sub>311</sub> has an inhibiting effect on filament nucleation in unmodified full-length tau. A mutant of tau297-441 where residues <sub>392</sub>IVYK<sub>395</sub> have been deleted and that does not contain the four PAD12 mutations in the carboxy-terminal domain behaves similarly in the NMR experiment as the tau297-441 construct with those four PAD12 mutations. Moreover, full-length 0N3R tau with the eight PAD12 mutations in the amino-terminal fuzzy coat and with the deletion of<sub>392</sub>IVYK<sub>395</sub>, but without the four PAD12 mutations in the carboxy-terminal domain, assembles readily into amyloid filaments (of which we also solved a cryo-EM structure, see the revised Figure 6B). These observations provide mechanistic insights into the previously proposed paper-clip model [Jeganathan (2008), J Biol Chem 283, 32066-32076], where interactions between the fuzzy coat inhibit filament formation of unmodified full-length tau, and phosphorylation in the fuzzy coat interferes with these interactions, thus leading to filament nucleation. Of course, the identification of residues <sub>392</sub>IVYK<sub>395</sub> for this interaction also explain why truncation of tau at residue 391 leads to spontaneous assembly. We have introduced a new Figure 7 to the revised manuscript to explain this model in more detail. The corresponding new section in the Results reads:

      “To investigate this further, we also tested a tau construct comprising residues tau297-441 without the phosphomimetic mutations, but with a deletion of residues (Δ392-395). Filaments formed rapidly and the cryo-EM structure showed that the ordered core consisted of the amino-terminal part of the construct spanning residues 297-318 (Figure 6B). NMR analysis (Figure 6 – figure supplement 5B) showed that the tau297441 Δ392-395 construct exhibited similar backbone rigidity properties to the tau297-441 PAD12 construct, despite peak locations and local secondary structural propensities being more similar to the wildtype tau297-441 (Figure 6 – figure supplement 5A; Figure 6 – figure supplement 6). HSQC peak intensities in the 297-319 and 392-404 regions of tau297-441 Δ392-395 (Figure 6A, expanded from Figure 6 - figure supplement 5C) were like those in the tau297-441 PAD12. These data suggest that the IVYK deletion has a similar effect as the phosphomimetics on residues 396, 400, 403 and 404 on disrupting an intra-molecular interaction between the FIA core region and the carboxy-terminal domain, which may therefore be mediated by interactions between the two IVYK motifs that are similar to those observed in the FIA (Lövestam et al, 2024).”

      A new section in the Discussion now reads:

      “Our NMR data provide insights into the mechanism by which phosphorylation in the fuzzy coat of tau, or truncations of tau, lead to the formation of filaments with ordered cores of residues that are themselves not phosphorylated. HSQC peak intensity differences between unmodified tau 297-441, PAD12 tau 297-441 and tau297-391 suggest that phosphorylation of the fuzzy coat, particularly near the <sub>392</sub>IVYK<sub>395</sub> motif in the carboxy-terminal domain, a7ects the conformation of the residues of tau that become ordered in the FIA (Lövestam et al., 2024). Removal of residues <sub>392</sub>IVYK<sub>395</sub> in the carboxyterminal domain of tau 297-441 led to rapid filament formation in the absence of phosphomimetics, while HSQC peak intensity di7erences for this construct indicate similar backbone rigidity compared to tau 297-441 without the deletion, but with the four PAD12 mutations in the carboxy-terminal domain. Combined, these observations support a model where the <sub>392</sub>IVYK<sub>395</sub> motif in unmodified full-length tau monomers interacts with the <sub>308</sub>IVYK<sub>311</sub> motif, thus inhibiting filament formation by preventing the formation of the nucleating species, the FIA. Phosphorylation of nearby residues 396, 400, 403 and 404, or truncation at residue 391, disrupt this interaction and lead to filament formation. This model agrees with the previously proposed hairpin-like model of tau (Jeganathan et al., 2008), although the corresponding interaction between the aminoterminal domain of tau and the core-forming region remains unknown (Figure 7).”

      Due to the challenging nature of the assignment, it was not possible to assign all residues in the HSQC of the tau151-391 and the PAD12 tau151-391 samples, including residues 259-262 and 292-294 for PAD12 tau151-391. To make this clearer, we have marked residues that are not assigned with an asterisk in the revised version of Figure 6 – figure supplement 1.  

      (v) Concerning the Cryo-EM data from the different hyper-phosphorylation mimics, it would seem that the authors could at least comment on the proportion of monofilament and paired-filaments even if they could not solve the structures. Nonetheless, based on their previous publications, one would also expect that they could show whether the nontwisted filaments are likely to have the same structure (by comparing the 2D classes to projections of non-twisted models). Also, it is very interesting to note that the twist could be so strongly controlled by the charge distribution on the non-structured regions (and may be also related to the work by Mezzenga on twist rate and buffer conditions). Is the result reported in Figure 2 a one-oT case or was it also reproducible?

      As also indicated in the main text, the assembly conditions for the PAD12+4, PAD12-4 and PAD12+/-4 constructs were kept the same as those for the PAD12 construct. It is possible that further optimisation of the conditions could again lead to twisting filaments, but we chose not to pursue this route. With unlimited resources and time, one could assess in detail which of the PAD12 mutations are required and which ones could be omitted to form PHFs. However, this would require a lot of work and cryo-EM time. For now, we chose to prioritise reporting conditions that do work to reproducibly make PHFs in the laboratory (using the PAD12 construct) and leave the more detailed analysis of other constructs for future studies. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This manuscript addresses an important impediment in the field of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and tauapathy research by showing that 12 specific phosphomimetic mutations in full-length tau allow the protein to aggregate into fibrils with the AD fold and the fold of chronic traumatic encephalopathy fibrils in vitro. The paper presents comprehensive structural and cell based seeding data indicating the improvement of their approach over previous in vitro attempts on non-full-length tau constructs. The main weaknesses of this work results from the fact that only up to 70% of the tau fibrils form the desired fibril polymorphs. In addition, some of the figures are of low quality and confusing. 

      As also explained in our response to reviewer #1, we have performed better quantification of filament types in the revised manuscript, and we have investigated how to get rid of the singlets. In the revised manuscript, we report that singlets disappear as time passes and that one can obtain 100% pure PHFs by quiescently incubating samples for another two weeks, after shaking for a week.

      Strengths: 

      This study provides significant progress towards a very important and timely topic in the amyloid community, namely the in vitro production of tau fibrils found in patients.

      The 12 specific phosphomimetic mutations presented in this work will have an immediate impact in the field since they can be easily reproduced.

      Multiple high-resolution structures support the success of the phosphomimetic mutation approach. Additional data show the seeding efficiency of the resulting fibrils, their reduced tendency to bundle, and their ability to be labeled without affecting core structure or seeding capability.

      Weaknesses: 

      Despite the success of making full-length AD tau fibrils, still ~30% of the fibrils are either not PHF, or not accounted for. A small fraction of the fibrils are single filaments and another ~20% are not accounted for. The authors mention that ~20% of these fibrils were not picked by the automated algorithm. However, it would be important to get additional clarity about these fibrils. Therefore, it would improve the impact of the paper if the authors could manually analyze passed-over particles to see if they are compatible with PHF or fall into a different class of fibrils. In addition, it would be helpful if the authors could comment on what can be done/tried to get the PHF yield closer to 90-100%

      As mentioned above, in the revised manuscript we show that the singlets disappear over time and we now include a description of a method that leads to 100% PHF formation.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor points: 

      (a) In Figure 6 the dashed purple vertical lines overlap with the black bars, rendering a grey color which is confusing because the grey bars used for the shorter construct. It is suggested to improve the colors (remove transparency on the purple?)

      We thank the reviewers for their suggestions for improving the visualisation of our data. We have recoloured the tau297-391 data from grey to gold and moved the dashed lines to the back of image to remove the apparent colour changes.  

      (b) Is there any support for the suggestion that "part of the second microtubule-binding repeat is ordered" being "related to this construct forming filaments with only a single protofilament"? It seemed to have come out of nowhere.

      There is no further support for this statement, but we thought it would be worth hypothesizing about this observation. 

      (c) Figures 1 and 4 E is better described as a "main chain trace" or "backbone trace" although the latter usually refers to only CA positions. Ribbon usually refers to something else in representations of protein structures. 

      This has been changed into “main chain trace” in Figures 1 and 4. 

      (d) Figure 1 Supplement 3: Panel letters in the legend do not match. 

      This has been fixed.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The introduction is a bit lengthy (e.g. 3rd paragraph of introduction) and could benefit by focusing specific question the manuscript addresses. 

      We have shortened the Introduction. It now contains ~1150 words, which we hope provides a better compromise between length and sufficient background information.

      Figure captions are generally not helpful in conveying a message to the reader.

      Figure 1 - figure supplement 3 is quite confusing. The 4 structures in A) do not correspond to the grids in B-E. What is this figure supposed to show?

      This confusion was probably the result of incorrect labelling of panels in the legend, which was also pointed out by reviewer #1. This has been fixed in the revised manuscript.

      Page 11: Although I know what you mean, 'linear increase of ThT fluorescence' is not the correct term. 

      We have replaced “linear” with “rapid”.

      Page 15: Although line shape and peak intensity can be related you are not reporting on line shape or width but simply on peak intensity. Therefore, I wouldn't talk about the result of a 'line shape analysis'.

      We have changed the wording accordingly. 

      Figure 6 (and supplement 1) are confusing and too small to be readable in print. It might be sufficient to show the CSP and upload the remaining data to the BMRB. 

      We have made a clearer version of the main NMR Figure 6 in the revised manuscript showing the most pertinent NMR data and have moved the previous version into the figure supplements. We designed these figures to be viewed as full page A4 panels, ideally seen in one image as they show multiple comparisons of different experiments and constructs.

      As such we feel these will be best viewed on screen as part of the eLife web document. We have uploaded HSQC spectra and assignments to the BMRB (see below).

      Figure 6 supplement 3 might benefit from pointing out key residues in the overlay.

      We have added the labels (this is now Figure 6 supplement 4).

      Data availability: Please upload the assignments to the BMRB together with key spectra (e.g. HSQCs). 

      We have uploaded HSQC data along with our assignments to the BMRB, the accession codes are 52694 – tau297-441 wt; 52695 – tau297-441 PAD-12; 52696 – tau151-391 wt; 52697 – tau151-391 PAD-12; and 53230 – tau297-441 delta392-395.  These accession codes have been added to the manuscript. 

      The quality of some of the figures (specifically Figure 1 - supplement 3 and Figure 6) is not suitable for publication. 

      For the original submission to bioRxiv, we produced a single PDF with a manageable file size. We will liaise with the eLife staff to ensure the images used in the version of record will be suitable for publication.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The topic of tumor-immune co-evolution is an important, understudied topic with, as the authors  noted, a general dearth of good models in this space. The authors have made important progress on the topic by introducing a stochastic branching process model of antigenicity/immunogenicity and measuring the proportion of simulated tumors that go extinct. The model is extensively explored, and the authors provide some nice theoretical results in addition to simulated results. 

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments on our work.

      Major comments 

      The text in lines 183-191 is intuitively and nicely explained. However, I am not sure all of it follows from the figure panels in Figure 2. For example, the authors refer to a mutation that has a large immunogenicity, but it's not shown how many mutations, or the relative size of the mutations in Figure 2. The same comment holds true for the claim that spikes also arise for mutations with low antigenicity. 

      We thank the reviewer for helping us to further specify this statement in our original submission. We now added muller plots in a new Appendix Figure (Figure A3) presenting the relative abundances of different types of effector cells in the population over time. Each effector type is colour-coded with its antigenicity and immunogenicity. To align with this Appendix Figure (Figure A3), we also updated our Figure 2 generated under the same realisation as Figure A3. We can now see clearly that the spikes in the mean values of the antigenicity and immunogenicity over the whole effector populations in new Figure 2B&2D indeed correspond to the expansion of single or several antigenic mutations recruiting the specific effector cell types. For example, in Figure 2B, we can see that the spikes of low average antigenicity and high immunogenicity (around time 11) happen at the same time when an effector type in Figure A3 with such a trait (coloured in green) arises and takes over the population. We have rewritten our Results section related (Line 192 - Line 222 in main text and Appendix A6).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this work, the authors developed a model of tumour-immune dynamics, incorporating stochastic antigenic mutation accumulation and escape within the cancer cell population. They then used this  model to investigate how tumour-immune interactions influence tumour outcome and summary  statistics of sequencing data. 

      Strengths: 

      This novel modeling framework addresses an important and timely topic. The authors consider the useful question of how bulk and single-cell sequencing may provide insights into the tumourimmune interactions and selection processes. 

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments.

      Weaknesses: 

      One set of conclusions presented in the paper is the presence of cyclic dynamics between effector/cancer cells, antigenicity, and immunogenicity. However, these conclusions are supported in the manuscript by two sample trajectories of stochastic simulations, and these provide mixed support for the conclusions (i.e. the phasing asynchrony described in the text does not seem to apply to Figure 2C). 

      We have now developed a method to quantify the cyclic dynamics in our system (Appendix A7), where can track the directional changes phase portrait of the abundances of the cancer and effector cells. We first tested this method in a non-evolving stochastic predator-prey system, where our method can correctly capture the number of cycles in this system (Figure A7). We then use this method to quantify the number of cycles we observed between cancer and effector cells under different mutation rates (Figure A5) as well as whether they are counter-clockwise or clockwise cycles (Figure A6). Our results showed that the cyclic dynamics are more often to be observed when mutation rates are higher, and the majority of those cycles are counter-clockwise. When the mutation rate is high, we observe an increase of clockwise cycles, which have been observed in predator-prey systems and explained through coevolution. However, even under high mutation rates, counter-clockwise cycles are still the more frequent type. 

      In our simulations, we observed rarely out-of-phase cycles, which was by chance present in our original Figure 2. We have now removed that statement about out-of-phase cycles and replaced by more systematic analysis of the cyclic dynamics as described above (Line 192 to 207 in the revised version). We thank the constructive comment of the reviewer, which motivated us to improve our analysis significantly. 

      Similarly, the authors also find immune selection effects on the shape of the mutational burden in Figure 5 D/H using a qualitative comparison between the distributions and theoretical predictions in  the absence of immune response. However the discrepancy appears quite small in panel D, and  there are no quantitative comparisons provided to evaluate the significance. An analysis of the robustness of all the conclusions to parameter variation is missing. 

      We have now added statistical analysis using Wasserstein distance between the simulated mutation burden distribution and theoretical (neutral) expectation in Figure 5 C, D, G, H as well as in Figure A11 C&D when there is no cancer-immune interaction. We can see that the measurements of the  Wasserstein distance agrees with our statement, that the higher immune effectiveness leads to larger deviation from the neutral expectation.

      Lastly, the role of the Appendix results in the main messages of the paper is unclear. 

      We agree with the review and have now removed the Appendix sections “Deterministic Analysis”. 

      Reviewing Editor Comments: 

      I find the abstract too long. For example, "Knowledge of this coevolutionary system and the selection taking place within it can help us understand tumour-immune dynamics both during tumorigenesis but also when treatments such as immunotherapies are applied." can be shortened to: "Knowledge of this coevolutionary system can help us understand tumour-immune dynamics both during tumorigenesis and during immunotherapy treatments." 

      We agree and have taken the suggestion of the reviewer to shorten our abstract.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The discussion at lines 134-140, centered around Figure A1, is an important and nicely constructed feature of the model. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      I suggest that the authors conduct a more in-depth analysis of their conclusions on cyclic dynamics over a large set of sample paths.

      Done and please see our detailed response to the reviewer 2 above.

      In addition, statistical comparisons between the observed mutational burden distribution and  theoretical predictions in the absence of immune selection should be carried out to support their conclusions. In all cases, conclusions should be tested extensively for robustness/sensitivity to parameters. 

      Done and please see our detailed response to the reviewer 2 above.

      Here are some specific suggestions/comments: 

      (1) Please provide a precise mathematical description of the model to complement Figure 1. 

      We have significantly revised our “Model” section to provide a precise mathematical description of our model (Line 138 - 148). Please also see our document showing the difference between the revised version and original submission.

      (2) Section on "Interactions dictate outcome of tumour progress" and Figure 3: please define 'tumour outcome' - are the heatmaps produced in Figure 3 tumor size reflecting whether or not the population has reached level K before a particular time? Also, I do not see a definition for the 'slowgrowing' tumour proportion plotted in Figure 3CF or in the accompanying text. 

      We have now added the definition of “tumour outcome” in our “Model” section (line 171 to 176), where we explain our model parameters and quantities measured in the following “Results” section.

      (3) Figure 5C/G: the green dotted vertical line is difficult to see. 

      We have now changed the mean of the simulations to solid red lines instead of using the green dotted vertical lines previously.

      (4) Appendix A1 text under (A2) should U/N be U/C? N does not appear to be defined. 

      We have more removed the previous A1 section. Please see our response to reviewer 2 as well.

      (5) Text under (A5): it is unclear what is meant by "SFS must be heavy tailed (that is, more heterogeneous)" -- a more precise statement regarding tail decay rate and associated consequences would be more helpful. 

      We have more removed the previous A section, where the original text "...SFS must be heavy-tailed" was.

      (6) Section A4 and Figure A1: can these calculations be compared to simulations? 

      We have more removed the previous A section on the deterministic analysis as they are not so  relevant to our stochastic simulations indeed. Please see our response to reviewer 2 as well.

      (7) Also, in general, please clarify how the results in the Appendix are used in the main text conclusions or provide insights relevant to these conclusions. If they are not, one can consider removing them.  

      We have more removed the previous A section on the deterministic analysis. The remaining sections are about stochastic simulations and extended figures which support our main figures.  

      (8) Figure A2: the two lines are difficult to tell apart on each panel. Please consider different styles.

      We have changed one of the dotted lines to be solid. This figure is now Figure A1 in our revision.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      I am not currently convinced by the principal interpretations and think that other explanations based on known phenomena could account for key results. Specifically the authors have not resolved whether oxidative modification to 5mC and 3mC, or chemical attack to ssDNA that is transiently exposed in the repair processing of 5mC and 3mC is the principal source of the observed genotoxicity.

      (1) Original query which still stands: As noted in the manuscript, AlkB repairs alkylation damage by direct reversal (DNA strands are not cut). In the absence of AlkB, repair of alklylation damage/modification is likely through BER or other processes involving strand excision and resulting in single stranded DNA. It has previously been shown that 3mC modification from MMS exposure is highly specific to single stranded DNA (PMID:20663718) occurring at ~20,000 times the rate as double stranded DNA. Consequently the introduction of DNMTs is expected to introduce many methylation adducts genome-wide that will generate single stranded DNA tracts when repaired in an AlkB deficient background (but not in an AlkB WT background), which are then hyper-susceptible to attack by MMS. Such ssDNA tracts are also vulnerable to generating double strand breaks, especially when they contain DNA polymerase stalling adducts such as 3mC. The generation of ssDNA during repair is similarly expected follow the H2O2 or TET based conversion of 5mC to 5hmC or 5fC neither of which can be directly repaired and depend on single strand excision for their removal. The potential importance of ssDNA generation in the experiments has not been [adequately] considered.

      We thank the reviewer for expanding on their previous comment.  We completely agree with the possibility that they raise and have added an extra paragraph in the discussion to expand on our consideration of the role of ssDNA in DNMT-induced DNA damage, which we reproduce here:

      "The observation that TET overexpression sensitizes cells expressing DNMTs to oxidative stress strongly suggests that the site of DNA damage is the modified cytosine itself.  However, we do not currently have definitive evidence supporting this.  As mentioned in the results section, the presence of unrepaired 3mC may lead to increased levels of ssDNA; it is also possible that 5mC itself may increase ssDNA levels.  Loss of alkB would be expected to increase the amount of ssDNA.  Thus DNA damage surrounding modification sites, but not specifically localised to it, might be the cause of the increased sensitivity.  These two different models make different predictions.  If modified cytosines are the source of the damage, mutations arising would be predominantly located at CG dinucleotides.  Alternatively, ssDNA exposure would result in distributed mutations that would not necessarily be located at CG sites.  The highly biased spectrum of mutations that can be screened through the Rif resistance assay does not allow us to address this currently.  However, future experiments to create mutation accumulation lines could allow us to address the question systematically on a genome-wide level. "

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Joint Public Review:

      In this work, the authors develop a new computational tool, DeepTX, for studying transcriptional bursting through the analysis of single-cell RNA sequencing (scRNA-seq) data using deep learning techniques. This tool aims to describe and predict the transcriptional bursting mechanism, including key model parameters and the steady-state distribution associated with the predicted parameters. By leveraging scRNA-seq data, DeepTX provides high-resolution transcriptional information at the single-cell level, despite the presence of noise that can cause gene expression variation. The authors apply DeepTX to DNA damage experiments, revealing distinct cellular responses based on transcriptional burst kinetics. Specifically, IdU treatment in mouse stem cells increases burst size, promoting differentiation, while 5FU affects burst frequency in human cancer cells, leading to apoptosis or, depending on the dose, to survival and potential drug resistance. These findings underscore the fundamental role of transcriptional burst regulation in cellular responses to DNA damage, including cell differentiation, apoptosis, and survival. Although the insights provided by this tool are mostly well supported by the authors' methods, certain aspects would benefit from further clarification.

      The strengths of this paper lie in its methodological advancements and potential broad applicability. By employing the DeepTXSolver neural network, the authors efficiently approximate stationary distributions of mRNA counts through a mixture of negative binomial distributions, establishing a simple yet accurate mapping between the kinetic parameters of the mechanistic model and the resulting steady-state distributions. This innovative use of neural networks allows for efficient inference of kinetic parameters with DeepTXInferrer, reducing computational costs significantly for complex, multi-gene models. The approach advances parameter estimation for high-dimensional datasets, leveraging the power of deep learning to overcome the computational expense typically associated with stochastic mechanistic models. Beyond its current application to DNA damage responses, the tool can be adapted to explore transcriptional changes due to various biological factors, making it valuable to the systems biology, bioinformatics, and mechanistic modelling communities. Additionally, this work contributes to the integration of mechanistic modelling and -omics data, a vital area in achieving deeper insights into biological systems at the cellular and molecular levels.  

      We thank the reviewers for their positive opinion on our manuscript. As reflected in our detailed responses to the reviewers’ comments, we will make significant changes to address their concerns comprehensively.

      This work also presents some weaknesses, particularly concerning specific technical aspects. The tool was validated using synthetic data, and while it can predict parameters and steady-state distributions that explain gene expression behaviour across many genes, it requires substantial data for training. The authors account for measurement noise in the parameter inference process, which is commendable, yet they do not specify the exact number of samples required to achieve reliable predictions. Moreover, the tool has limitations arising from assumptions made in its design, such as assuming that gene expression counts for the same cell type follow a consistent distribution. This assumption may not hold in cases where RNA measurement timing introduces variability in expression profiles.

      Thank reviewers for detailed and constructive feedback on our work. We will address the key concerns raised from the following points:

      (1) Clarification on the required sample size: We tested the robustness of our inference method on simulated datasets by varying the number of single-cell samples. Our results indicated that the predictions of burst kinetics parameters become accurate when the number of cells reaches 500 (Supplementary Figure S3d, e). This sample size is smaller than the data typically obtained with current single-cell RNA sequencing (scRNA-seq) technologies, such as 10x Genomics and Smart-seq3 (Zheng GX et al., 2017; Hagemann-Jensen M et al., 2020). Therefore, we believed that our algorithm is well-suited for inferring burst kinetics from existing scRNA-seq datasets, where the sample size is sufficient for reliable predictions. We will clarify this point in the main text to make it easier for readers to use the tool.

      (2) Assumption-related limitations: One of the fundamental assumptions in our study is that the expression counts of each gene are independently and identically distributed (i.i.d.) among cells, which is a commonly adopted assumption in many related works (Larsson AJM et al., 2019; Ochiai H et al., 2020; Luo S et al., 2023). However, we acknowledged the limitations of this assumption. The expression counts of the same gene in each cell may follow distinct distributions even from the same cell type, and dependencies between genes could exist in realistic biological processes. We recognized this and will deeply discuss these limitations from assumptions and prospect as an important direction for future research.  

      The authors present a deep learning pipeline to predict the steady-state distribution, model parameters, and statistical measures solely from scRNA-seq data. Results across three datasets appear robust, indicating that the tool successfully identifies genes associated with expression variability and generates consistent distributions based on its parameters. However, it remains unclear whether these results are sufficient to fully characterise the transcriptional bursting parameter space. The parameters identified by the tool pertain only to the steady-state distribution of the observed data, without ensuring that this distribution specifically originates from transcriptional bursting dynamics.

      We appreciate reviewers’ comments and the opportunity to clarify our study’s contributions and limitations. Although we agree that assessing whether the results from these three realistic datasets can represent the characterize transcriptional burst parameter space is challenging, as it depends on data property and conditions in biology, we firmly believe that DeepTX has the capacity to characterize the full parameter space. This believes stems from the extensive parameters and samples we input during model training and inference across a sufficiently large parameter range (Method 1.3). Furthermore, the training of the model is both flexible and scalable, allowing for the expansion of the transcriptional burst parameter space as needed. We will clarify this in the text to enable readers to use DeepTX more flexibly.

      On the other hand, we agree that parameter identification is based on the steady-state distribution of the observed data (static data), which loses information about the fine dynamic process of the burst kinetics. In principle, tracking the gene expression of living cells can provide the most complete information about real-time transcriptional dynamics across various timescales (Rodriguez J et al., 2019).

      However, it is typically limited to only a small number of genes and cells, which could not investigate general principles of transcriptional burst kinetics on a genome-wide scale. Therefore, leveraging the both steady-state distribution of scRNA-seq data and mathematical dynamic modelling to infer genome-wide transcriptional bursting dynamics represents a critical and emerging frontier in this field. For example, the statistical inference framework based on the Markovian telegraph model, as demonstrated in (Larsson AJM et al., 2019), offers a valuable paradigm for understanding underlying transcriptional bursting mechanisms. Building on this, our study considered a more generalized non-Mordovian model that better captures transcriptional kinetics by employing deep learning method under conditions such as DNA damage. This provided a powerful framework for comparative analyses of how DNA damage induces alterations in transcriptional bursting kinetics across the genome. We will highlight the limitations of current inference using steady-state distributions in the text and look ahead to future research directions for inference using time series data across the genome.

      A primary concern with the TXmodel is its reliance on four independent parameters to describe gene state-switching dynamics. Although this general model can capture specific cases, such as the refractory and telegraph models, accurately estimating the parameters of the refractory model using only steadystate distributions and typical cell counts proves challenging in the absence of time-dependent data.

      We thank reviewers for highlighting this critical concern regarding the TXmodel's reliance on four independent parameters to describe gene state-switching dynamics. We acknowledge that estimating the parameters of the TXmodel using only steady-state distributions and typical single-cell RNA sequencing (scRNA-seq) data poses significant challenges, particularly in the absence of timeresolved measurements.

      As described in the response of last point, while time-resolved data can provide richer information than static scRNA-seq data, it is currently limited to a small number of genes and cells, whereas static scRNA-seq data typically capture genome-wide expression. Our framework leverages deep learning methods to link mechanistic models with static scRNA-seq data, enabling the inference of genome-wide dynamic behaviors of genes. This provides a potential pathway for comparative analyses of transcriptional bursting kinetics across the entire genome.

      Nonetheless, the refractory model and telegraphic model are important models for studying transcription bursts. We will discuss and compare them in terms of the accuracy of inferred parameters.

      Certainly, we agree that inferring the molecular mechanisms underlying transcriptional burst kinetics using time-resolved data remains a critical future direction. We will include a brief discussion on the role and importance of time-resolved data in addressing these challenges in the discussion section of the revised manuscript.

      The claim that the GO analysis pertains specifically to DNA damage response signal transduction and cell cycle G2/M phase transition is not fully accurate. In reality, the GO analysis yielded stronger p-values for pathways related to the mitotic cell cycle checkpoint signalling. As presented, the GO analysis serves more as a preliminary starting point for further bioinformatics investigation that could substantiate these conclusions. Additionally, while GSEA analysis was performed following the GO analysis, the involvement of the cardiac muscle cell differentiation pathway remains unclear, as it was not among the GO terms identified in the initial GO analysis.

      We thank the reviewer for this valuable feedback and for pointing out the need for clarification regarding the GO and GSEA analyses. We agree that the connection between the cardiac muscle cell differentiation pathway identified in the GSEA analysis and the GO terms from the initial analysis requires further clarification. This discrepancy arises because GSEA examines broader sets of pathways and may capture biological processes not highlighted by GO analysis due to differences in the statistical methods and pathway definitions used. We will revise the manuscript to address this point, explicitly discussing the distinct yet complementary nature of GO and GSEA analyses and providing a clearer interpretation of the results.

      As the advancement is primarily methodological, it lacks a comprehensive comparison with traditional methods that serve similar functions. Consequently, the overall evaluation of the method, including aspects such as inference accuracy, computational efficiency, and memory cost, remains unclear. The paper would benefit from being contextualised alongside other computational tools aimed at integrating mechanistic modelling with single-cell RNA sequencing data. Additional context regarding the advantages of deep learning methods, the challenges of analysing large, high-dimensional datasets, and the complexities of parameter estimation for intricate models would strengthen the work.

      We greatly appreciate your insightful feedback, which highlights important considerations for evaluating and contextualizing our methodological advancements. Below, we emphasize our advantages from both the modeling perspective and the inference perspective compared with previous model. As our work is rooted in a model-based approach to describe the transcriptional bursting process underlying gene expression, the classic telegraph model (Markovian) and non-Markovian models which are commonly employed are suitable for this purpose:

      Classic telegraph model: The classic telegraph model allows for the derivation of approximate analytical solutions through numerical integration, enabling efficient parameter point estimation via maximum likelihood methods, e.g., as explored in (Larsson AJM et al., 2019). Although exact analytical solutions for the telegraph model are not available, certain moments of its distribution can be explicitly derived. This allows for an alternative approach to parameter inference using moment-based estimation methods, e.g., as explored in (Ochiai H et al., 2020). However, it is important to note that higher-order sample moments can be unstable, potentially leading to significant estimation bias. 

      Non-Markovian Models: For non-Markovian models, analytical or approximate analytical solutions remain elusive. Previous work has employed pseudo-likelihood approaches, leveraging statistical properties of the model’s solutions to estimate parameters ,e.g., as explored in (Luo S et al., 2023).

      However, the method may suffer from low inference efficiency. 

      In our current work, we leverage deep learning to estimate parameters of TXmodel, which is nonMarkovian model. First, we represent the model's solution as a mixture of negative binomial distributions, which is obtained by the deep learning method. Second, through integration with the deep learning architecture, the model parameters can be optimized using automatic differentiation, significantly improving inference efficiency. Furthermore, by employing a Bayesian framework, our method provides posterior distributions for the estimated dynamic parameters, offering a comprehensive characterization of uncertainty. Compared to traditional methods such as moment-based estimation or pseudo-likelihood approaches, we believe our approach not only achieves higher inference efficiency but also delivers posterior distributions for kinetics parameters, enhancing the interpretability and robustness of the results. We will present and emphasize the computational efficiency and memory cost of our methods the revised version.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      There are various noise sources in biological progress. How transcriptional bursting fits within those as well as the reasons to focus only on this source needs to be clearly discussed in the introduction of the manuscript. Related to this last point, transcriptional bursting might not be the only mechanism to take advantage of the stochastic nature of biomolecular processes to make decisions. Once again, what are the implications of assuming this as the underlying mechanism?

      Thank the reviewer for this valuable comment. We fully agree that biological systems are subject to multiple stochastic sources, which arise from both intrinsic and extrinsic noise (Eling N et al., 2019). Intrinsic noise is primarily driven by the stochastic biochemical effects that directly influence mRNA and protein expression in a gene-specific manner, such as DNA, epigenetic, transcription, and translation levels. Extrinsic noise arises from fluctuations in cell-specific manners, such as changes in cell size, cell cycle, or cell signaling. Given that DNA damage most directly perturbs transcription and translation processes, focusing on intrinsic noise sources is appropriate for mechanistically modeling gene-specific expression variability, particularly since this variability can be captured at the genome-wide scale by scRNA-seq data.

      Among various intrinsic noise sources, transcriptional bursting offers a mechanistically wellcharacterized and quantifiable representation of gene expression variability (Tunnacliffe E & Chubb JR, 2020). It reflects the dynamic switching between active and inactive gene states and has been observed consistently across prokaryotic and eukaryotic cells (Eling N et al., 2019). Moreover, transcriptional bursting kinetics, defined by burst size and frequency, can be inferred from scRNA-seq data at the singlegene level using steady-state assumptions, making it an analytically tractable and biologically meaningful feature for large-scale inference (Rodriguez J & Larson DR, 2020).

      We acknowledge that transcriptional bursting is not the only mechanism through which cells can utilize stochasticity for fate decisions. Other processes, such as translational noise and chromatin accessibility, may also contribute. However, given the data modality (static scRNA-seq) and the established theoretical framework for bursting, we assume transcriptional bursting as a representative and interpretable proxy of stochastic regulation. This assumption enables us to extract meaningful insights while remaining open to future model extensions, incorporating additional regulatory layers as more data types become available.

      In this version of the manuscript, we have revised the introduction section to better clarify the rationale of this assumption and to more explicitly emphasize the important role of transcriptional bursting within stochastic noise.

      More careful discussion of how the proposed method differentiates from previous work that employs scRNA-seq to elucidate the diverse sources of noise (pp.3).

      Thank the reviewer for this suggestion. Our proposed method differs significantly from previous work that utilizes scRNA-seq data to study diverse noise sources from several aspects (Ochiai H et al., 2020; Eling N et al., 2019; Morgan MD & Marioni JC, 2018). Specifically, DeepTX infers genomewide burst kinetics by directly matching the full steady-state distribution of a mechanistic stochastic model to the observed scRNA-seq data, rather than relying solely on low-order statistics such as mean and variance. Moreover, by adopting a non-Markovian process that allows multi-step promoter switching, DeepTX extends beyond the classic telegraph model to better capture the complex molecular events underlying transcriptional activation and repression. Crucially, we used a deep-learning–based solver to obtain these intractable steady-state distributions rapidly and accurately. This combination of richer data usage, more realistic mechanistic assumptions, and scalable neural-network–accelerated computation lays the groundwork for incorporating additional noise sources into a unified inference framework in future work. 

      In this version of the manuscript, we have revised the discussion section to highlight the difference with previous works.

      The paper could benefit from being contextualised alongside other computational tools that aim to integrate mechanistic modelling with single-cell RNA sequencing data. This is an active area of research, and works such as Sukys and Grima (bioRxiv, 2024), Garrido-Rodriguez et al. (PLOS Computational Biology, 2021), Maizels (2024), and others could provide valuable context.

      Thank the reviewer for suggesting these relevant works. Garrido-Rodriguez et al. (PLOS Comput. Biol., 2021) integrated single-cell and bulk transcriptomic data into mechanistic pathway models to infer signaling dynamics, an approach complementary to our mapping of burst kinetic parameters onto pathway enrichment for linking transcriptional bursting to functional outcomes. Sukys and Grima et al. (bioRxiv, 2024; Now in Nucleic Acids Res., 2025) demonstrated that cell-cycle stage and cellular age significantly modulate burst frequency and size, highlighting the potential to enhance DeepTX by incorporating cell-cycle–dependent variability into genome-wide burst inference. Maizels et al. (Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B. Biol. Sci., 2024) reviewed methods for capturing single-cell temporal dynamics across multi-omic modalities, underscoring how higher time-resolved data could refine and validate steady-state burst inference frameworks to better resolve causal gene-expression mechanisms.

      We have cited these studies on the contextual relevance to DeepTX in the discussion sections.

      As the advancement is primarily methodological, it lacks a comprehensive comparison with traditional methods that serve similar functions. Consequently, the overall evaluation of the method, including aspects such as inference accuracy, computational efficiency, and memory cost, remains unclear. We suggest incorporating these experiments to provide readers with a more complete understanding of the proposed method's performance.

      Thank the reviewer for constructive suggestion regarding a comprehensive comparison with other previous methods. To address this problem, in this version, we compared DeepTX with our previous work, txABC, that utilized approximate Bayesian computation to infer parameters from the generalized telegraph model (Luo S et al., 2023). As a result, DeepTX achieved improvements in inference accuracy and computational efficiency (Supplementary Figure S4.). For memory cost during single-gene inference, DeepTX requires an average memory usage of approximately 70 MB, whose memory consumption accounts for only a small fraction of the total available memory on standard computing devices (typically exceeding 10 GB), while exhibiting superior inference efficiency compared to txABC. We have mentioned in the third result section.

      Discuss the validity of the assumption of the static snapshot provided by the scRNA-seq data as in steadystate (i.e., stationary distribution), and the implications of this assumption being untrue (for the proposed method).

      We thank the reviewer for the comment regarding the stationary assumption. We assume that each scRNA-seq snapshot approximates the steady-state (stationary) distribution of transcript counts because (i) typical single-cell experiments sample large, asynchronously dividing populations that collectively traverse many transcriptional burst cycles, and (ii) in the absence of a synchronized perturbation, mRNA production and degradation reach a dynamic balance on timescales much shorter than overall cell-type changes. Under these conditions, the empirical count distribution closely mirrors the model’s stationary solution, justifying steady-state inference of burst size and frequency from a single time point. This assumption is commonly adopted in probabilistic models of transcriptional bursting (Larsson AJM et al., 2019; Raj A & van Oudenaarden A, 2008).

      However, this steady-state assumption has some limitations. First, in some scenarios, the cell system may exhibit highly transient transcriptional programs that do not satisfy stationarity, leading to biased or misleading parameter estimates. For example, immediately following a synchronized developmental stimulus—such as serum shock–induced activation of immediate-early genes. Second, because DeepTX infers the mean burst frequency and size across the population, it cannot recover the underlying time-resolved dynamics or distinguish heterogeneous kinetic subpopulations. 

      We have added a statement in the discussion to acknowledge these limitations and suggest future extensions—such as incorporating time-series measurements or latent pseudo time covariates—to address non-stationarity and recover temporal burst dynamics.

      On page 3, "traditional telegraph model" is mentioned without any context. This model, and particularly the implications for the current work, might not be obvious to the reader. Take one or two sentences to give the reader context.

      Thank the reviewer for this helpful comment. We acknowledge that the mention of the "traditional telegraph model" on page 3 may not be immediately clear to all readers. The traditional telegraph model is a mathematical framework commonly used to describe gene expression burst dynamics, in which genes stochastically switch between active (ON) and inactive (OFF) states, with exponentially distributed waiting times for state transitions. To provide the necessary context, we added a brief introduction to the traditional telegraph model and its relevance to our work in the revised manuscript.

      A primary concern with the model used in Figure 2a (TXmodel) is its reliance on four independent parameters to describe gene state switching dynamics. While this general model can encompass specific cases such as the refractory model (Science 332, 472 (2011)) and the telegraph model, accurately estimating the parameters of the refractory model using only steady-state distributions and typical cell numbers (10³-10⁴) is challenging without time-dependent data. To address this, we suggest that the authors provide parameter inference results for each individual parameter, rather than only for burst size and burst frequency, based on synthetic data. This would help clarify the model's effectiveness and improve understanding of its estimation precision.

      Thank the reviewer for highlighting this important concern. We agree that the lack of timeresolved measurements may affect the accuracy of inferences about dynamic parameters, especially the unidentifiability of parameters inferred from steady-state distributions, i.e., multiple parameters leading to the same steady-state distribution. The unidentifiability of individual parameters is a common and critical problem in systems biology studies. To address this issue, for example, Trzaskoma et al. developed StochasticGene, a computationally efficient software suite that uses Bayesian inference to analyze arbitrary gene regulatory models and quantify parameter uncertainty across diverse data types (Trzaskoma P et al., 2024). Alexander et al. adopt a Bayesian approach to parameter estimation by incorporating prior knowledge through a prior distribution and classify a parameter as practically nonidentifiable if it cannot be uniquely determined beyond the confidence already provided by the prior (Browning AP et al., 2020). Hence, in DeepTX, we employed a Bayesian approach based on loss potential to infer the posterior distributions of the parameters (Figure 3E). 

      Although DeepTX also encounters the issue of unidentifiability for individual parameters (Supplementary Figure S11), the multimodal nature of the posterior distribution suggests that multiple distinct parameter sets can produce similarly good fits to the observed data, highlighting the inherent non-identifiability of the model. Nevertheless, in the multimodal posterior distribution, at least one of the posterior peaks aligns closely with the ground truth, thereby demonstrating the validity of the inferred result. Moreover, inference results on synthetic data confirm that the BS and BF can be accurately estimated (Supplementary Figure S3b and S3c). We also performed robustness analyses on synthetic datasets. As shown in Supplementary Figure S3d and S3e, our model reliably recovers the ground-truth burst kinetics of models when the number of cells reaches ~1000, which is within the range of typical single-cell RNA-seq experiments. 

      We have explicitly pointed out the potential issue of unidentifiability due to the lack of temporal resolution information in the discussion section. 

      Noteworthy, transcriptional is always a multi-step process (depending on the granularity with which the process is described). What do the authors mean by saying that "DNA damage turns transcription into a multi-step process rather than a single-step process"?

      Thank the reviewer for pointing out the lack of precision in our original statement. We agree that the phrasing could be misleading. Transcription is inherently a multi-step process, but most mechanistic studies simplify it to a single-step “telegraph” model for tractability. In the context of DNA damage, however, damage-induced pausing and repair-mediated delays introduce additional intermediary states in the transcription cycle that cannot be approximated by a single step. To capture these damage-specific interruptions, DeepTX explicitly consider a multi-step promoter switching framework rather than combining all transitions into one. What we originally wanted to express was the necessity of multi-step process modeling. We have replaced the original sentence in introduction with: “However, the presence of DNA damage necessitates modeling the transcriptional process as a multistep process, rather than a single-step process, to capture the additional complexity introduced by the damage”.

      It is unclear why the authors have chosen a different definition in Equation (2) rather than the commonly used burst frequency, 1/(k_deg * tau_off), as reported in the literature. Unlike the traditional definition, which is unit-free, the definition in Eq. (2) includes units, raising questions about its interpretability and consistency with established conventions. Clarifying this choice would improve the understanding and consistency of the methodology.

      Thank the reviewer for raising this important point. We acknowledge that there are multiple definitions of burst frequency (BF) in the literature. Here, we provide a detailed explanation, clarifying the differences between these definitions, including the one used and the traditional definition .

      First, the definition of burst frequency we adopt has been widely used in recent literatures, such as Benjamin Zoller et al. (Zoller B et al., 2018), Caroline Hoppe et al. (Hoppe C et al., 2020) and Daniel Ramsköld (Ramsköld D et al., 2024). And its quantity represents the average time it takes for the promoter to complete one full stochastic cycle between its active and inactive states . Secondly, the traditional definition can be regarded as a simplified version of our definition, under the assumptions that τ<sub>on</sub> is negligible and k<sub>deg</sub> =1 (i.e., rate parameters are normalized to be unit-free). Although it is reasonable to neglecting activate time τ<sub>on</sub>, as it is typically much shorter than inactive time under some conditions, we chose a more complete way to define the burst frequency so that it is applicable to more general situations. In addition, by defining the burst frequency as , the mean transcription level can be analytically represented as the product of burst size and burst frequency.

      This explanation has been clarified in the methods 1.2 section.

      The authors mention the need to model "more realistic gene expression processes". How is this exactly being incorporated into the model?

      Thank the reviewer for raising this important question. To incorporate "more realistic gene expression processes" into our model, we considered two critical aspects into DeepTX that are often oversimplified in traditional approaches:

      (1) Integration of gene expression and sequencing processes: Observations from scRNA-seq data are influenced by both the intrinsic gene expression processes and the subsequent sequencing procedure. Traditional models often focus solely on gene expression, neglecting the stochastic effects introduced by the sequencing process. Our model explicitly incorporates both the gene expression and sequencing processes, providing a more comprehensive and realistic representation of the observed data.

      (2) Modeling gene expression as a multi-step process: Gene expression is inherently a multi-step process. However, traditional telegraph models typically simplify gene state switching as a single-step process for tractable analysis, often assuming Markovian dynamics where transition waiting times follow exponential distributions. In contrast, our model accounts for the multi-step nature of gene state transitions by allowing the waiting times to follow non-exponential (non-Markovian) distributions. This model is more suitable for gene expression dynamics that cannot be simplified to a single-step process, such as DNA damage, which may introduce an intermediate state to represent pausing and repair in the transcription process.

      By addressing these factors, our model better reflects the complexity and stochastic nature of gene expression processes, aligning more closely with the data generated from biological systems. We have added detailed explanations after this sentence for clarification in the first result section.

      Better explanation of the previously developed TXmodel, and the assumption of a non-Markovian system. In particular, it isn't clear how using arbitrary distributions for the waiting times implies a non-Markovian process (as the previous state(s) of the system is not used to inform the transition probability, at least as explained in pp. 4). Without a clear discussion of the so-called arbitrary waiting time distribution, it isn't clear how these represent a mechanistic model. In general, a more careful discussion of the "mechanistic" model is needed.

      Thank the reviewer for this thoughtful comment. In this revised version, we provided a more detailed explanation of the relationship between the TXmodel and the non-Markovian system in the revised manuscript. Specifically, we will clarify the following points:

      (1) Why non-Markovian system: In a Markovian system, the waiting times for events are exponentially distributed, meaning that the state transitions depend solely on the current state and are memoryless (Van Kampen NG, 1992). However, when the waiting times follow non-exponential distributions, such as Gamma or Weibull distributions, the state transitions are no longer independent of the system's previous states. This introduces memory into the system, making it non-Markovian.

      (2) Why mechanistic model: First, it is important to clarify that regardless of whether the waiting time is arbitrary or exponential (corresponding to non-Markovian and Markovian systems), our TXmodel is a mechanistic model because it models the dynamic process of transcription bursts with interpretable kinetic parameters. Second, although we introduced arbitrarily distributed waiting times, reasonable selection of waiting time distributions can still make the distribution parameters mechanistically interpretable. For example, in the context of modeling ON and OFF state switching times using a Gamma distribution, the two parameters have clear interpretations: the shape parameter represents the number of sequential exponential (memoryless) steps required for the transition to occur, capturing the complexity or multi-step nature of the switching process, while the scale parameter denotes the average duration of each of these steps. We have added the explanation in methods 1.2 section.

      Include a brief discussion about the metric used to compare distributions (and introduce KL abbreviation).

      Thank the reviewer for this suggestion. In the second result and methods 1.3 section of revised manuscript, we have included a brief discussion to introduce and clarify the metric used to compare distributions. Specifically, we have given more explanation for the Kullback-Leibler (KL) divergence, which is a widely used metric for quantifying the difference between two probability distributions. We also ensured that the abbreviation "KL" is properly introduced when it first appears in the text, along with a concise description of its mathematical definition and interpretation within the context of our analysis. 

      What does the "CTM" model stand for (in supplementary information)? And "TX" model?

      Thank the reviewer for highlighting this point. We revised the supplementary information to explicitly define the "CTM" and "TX" models and clarify their distinctions.

      CTM model: The "CTM" model refers to the classic telegraph model, a widely used model for capturing Markovian gene expression burst kinetics. The CTM describes stochastic gene expression as a sequence of four biochemical reactions involving two gene states (ON and OFF), mRNA transcription and degradation:

      k<sub>off</sub> as the rate at which the gene switches from OFF to ON, k<sub>on</sub>  as the rate at which the gene switches from ON to OFF, k<sub>syn</sub>  as the rate of mRNA synthesis and k<sub>deg</sub>  as the rate of mRNA degradation. In this model, gene switching between active and inactive states is governed by a memoryless Markovian process, where the waiting times for transitions follow exponential distributions (Van Kampen NG, 1992).

      TX model: In contrast, the "TX" model is a more generalized telegraph model for transcriptional processes.

      Different from the CTM, the waiting times for state transitions between ON and OFF in the TX model follow arbitrary waiting time distributions. This implies that the future state of the system depends not only on the current state but may also be influenced by its historical trajectories. Consequently, the TX model exhibits non-Markovian behavior. We have added more detailed description on these two models in section 1.1 of supplementary text.

      Leaky transcription (in the OFF promoter state) is not considered. What would be the implications of its presence in the data?

      Thank the reviewer for pointing out the potential role of leaky transcription in our analysis. We acknowledge that leaky transcription, occurring in the promoter OFF state, was not explicitly considered in our current model. Our decision to exclude it assumed that the leaky transcription rate is relatively small and its impact on the observed data is negligible. This assumption is consistent with previous studies that similarly disregard leaky transcription in gene expression modeling due to its minimal contribution to the overall dynamics (Larsson AJM et al., 2019).

      However, we recognize that the leaky transcription should be considered, particularly in systems where the leaky rate is significant relative to the active transcription rate. In such cases, it may introduce additional variability to the observed expression levels or obscure the distinction between ON and OFF states. We have added relevant statements in the discussion section.

      In the main text, the waiting time for state transitions is described by two parameters, while in the methods/supplementary information only one parameter is considered per distribution (without a clear discussion of the so-called "dwell time distributions").

      Thank the reviewer for this comment. We recognize the need to clarify the discrepancy between the descriptions of waiting times in the main text and supplementary materials.

      Dwell time distribution refers to the probability distribution of the time in which a gene remains in a particular transcriptional state (ON or OFF) before transitioning to the other state. While in Markovian models the dwell time follows an exponential distribution, more complex or non-Markovian regulatory mechanisms may give rise to Gamma, Weibull, or other non-exponential dwell time distributions.

      In our model, we denote the dwell time distributions in the OFF and ON states by and , respectively, where w represents a vector of parameters characterizing the distribution, the dimensionality of which depends on the specific form of the distribution. For example, when an exponential distribution is assumed, w consists of a single rate parameter; in contrast, for distributions such as the Gamma or Weibull, w includes two parameters. In the main text, both and are modeled using Gamma distributions, whereas in the Supplementary Materials, we assume exponential distributions for both, resulting in a single-parameter representation. We have added relevant statements in the methods 1.2 section.

      Related, but more general, across the manuscript there are problems with the consistency in terminology. This is especially problematic with the figures. It makes it incredibly hard to follow the work. Better integration of the information, and consistency with the terminology, would improve the understanding for the reader.

      Thank the reviewer for the valuable feedback. To enhance clarity and readability, we have carefully revised the manuscript to ensure consistent terminology throughout the text and figures e.g., unifying terms such as "untreatment" and "control" under the consistent label "control"—across both the text and figures.

      One of the four main assumptions behind the model is that "the solution of the model can be explained by a mixed negative binomial distribution". The logic and implications of this assumption need to be discussed in the paper. (Methods, pp.13.) All four assumptions need to be carefully argued in the paper. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment regarding the assumptions underlying our model. Here, we would like to clarify the rationale and implications of each assumption. 

      Assumption 1 (The gene expression of cells was in a stationary distribution during sequencing.) has been extensively used in previous studies for the inference and modeling of scRNA-seq data, demonstrating effectiveness in capturing mRNA expression distributions and inferring underlying dynamic parameters (Larsson AJM et al., 2019; Luo S et al., 2023; Ramsköld D et al., 2024; Gupta A et al., 2022).

      For Assumption 2 (Gene expression counts of the same cell type follow the same distribution.) is as follows: cell types are typically defined based on gene expression profiles or functional characteristics. Cells with similar functions often exhibit consistent transcriptional programs, leading to approximately identical gene expression distributions. This assumption has been widely adopted in previous research (Larsson AJM et al., 2019; Gupta A et al., 2022).

      Regarding Assumption 3 (The solution of the model can be approximated by a mixed negative binomial distribution.), in the most general formulation, a chemical master equation (CME) model of biological systems converges to a stationary distribution P(n;θ) over n∈ℕ. And P(n;θ) afford a real Poisson representation (Gardiner CW & Chaturvedi S, 1977): where F is a mixing cumulative distribution function (CDF). If such a Poisson representation exists, we can always write down a finite approximation over K Poisson kernels: , where w<sub>k</sub> are weights on a K-dimensional simplex. Further, as k →∞,QP . More problematically, convergence in the number of kernels in K is typically slow. Negative binomial kernels P<sub>Poisson</sub> (n m<sub> k</sub>,l<sub>k</sub>), which are continuous Poisson mixtures with a gamma mixing density can accelerate convergence in K (Gorin G et al., 2024). Hence, the solution of the TX model can be approximated by a mixed negative binomial distribution. 

      For Assumption 4 (The state space sampled from a sufficiently long single simulation is statistically equivalent to that obtained from multiple simulations at steady state in gene expression models.), when a sample trajectory of the model is simulated for a sufficiently long period, it is assumed to have traversed the entire stationary state space (Kuntz J et al., 2021). Therefore, by performing truncated statistical analysis on the trajectory, the corresponding stationary distribution of the model can be obtained. We have added the explanation in methods 1.1 section.

      The authors propose that the waiting times between promoter states follow a non-exponential distribution, but the choice of gamma distribution and the implications for the method and the biological conclusions need to be discussed.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. To account for the impact of DNA damage on the transcription process, our model assumes that both the "ON" and "OFF" states of the promoter consist of multiple underlying sub-states. When a promoter switches from the "ON" state to the "OFF" state, the transition is governed by multiple distinct waiting time distributions that follow exponential distributions. Similarly, when a promoter switches from the "OFF" state to the "ON" state, there may be multiple transitions from different "OFF" sub-states. Consequently, the waiting times for the transitions from the "OFF" state to the "ON" state, and vice versa, must account for multiple exponential waiting time distributions associated with each "ON" state transition. We can map a multiple exponential-waiting-times reaction process to a single-step reaction process with a non-exponential waiting time distribution. Therefore, we use a Gamma distribution for dwell time of promoter switching, which can be expressed as the convolution of multiple exponential distributions (corresponding to a sum of multiple exponential variables). Additionally, other non-exponential distributions, such as those discussed in our previous studies (Zhang J & Zhou T, 2019), may also be considered, and we recognize that alternative choices could be made depending on the specific characteristics of the system. We have added the explanation in methods 1.2 section.

      BF - burst frequency; BS - burst size. These terms represent the main data output, but they are only mathematically defined in the methods, and never the intuition of the specific expression explained (e.g., why not using tON/(tON+tOFF) as BF instead of 1/(tON+tOFF), and why not kSYN*tON as BS instead of kSYN*tON).

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment and agree that clarifying the biological intuition behind the mathematical definitions of burst frequency (BF) and burst size (BS) is important. Below, we provide a more detailed explanation of these definitions.

      BF: The definition of burst frequency we adopt has been widely used in previous literature, such as Benjamin Zoller et al (Zoller B et al., 2018), Caroline Hoppe et al (Hoppe C et al., 2020) and Daniel Ramsköld (Ramsköld D et al., 2024). And its quantity represents the average time it takes for the promoter to complete one full stochastic cycle between its active and inactive states.

      BS: The definition of burst size BS = we adopt is consistent with the definition proposed by the reviewer. Burst size refers to the average number of mRNA transcripts produced during a single transcriptional activation event of a gene. It reflects the quantity of gene product synthesized per activation and is influenced by the rate of transcription and the duration of the active state of the gene. Our definition aligns with this biological interpretation and is mathematically formulated as BS = , where k<sub>syn</sub> is the transcription rate and is the average duration of the active state.

      In addition, the mean transcription level can be analytically represented as the product of burst size and burst frequency. This analytical result has been included in the methods 1.2 section of revised manuscript.

      One can assume from the methods that omegaON and omegaOFF are the vector of (2) parameters describing the distribution, but the reader would benefit from some clarity here. The authors claim that they proved that the distribution moments can be obtained through an iterative process. How much does this rely on the assumption of an underlying binomial distribution?

      Thank the reviewer for this helpful suggestion. To clarify, the vectors omegaON and omegaOFF represent the parameters characterizing the waiting time distributions of the promoter's active and inactive states, respectively. The exact form and interpretation of these vectors depend on the specific distributional choice for the waiting times. For instance, when the waiting time distribution follows a Gamma distribution with shape parameter α>0  and scale parameter β>0 , denoted as , then w<sub>on</sub> = (α,β) . Conversely, when the waiting time distribution follows a Weibull distribution, denoted as , with shape parameter k >0 and scale parameter l>0, then w<sub>on</sub> = (l,k) . We have clarified it in the Methods 1.2 section of the revised manuscript.

      For the question about the binomial distribution, in our work, we use the binomial moment method to compute distributional statistics of chemical master equation (Zhang J et al., 2016). Binomial moments of the mRNA stationary distribution P(m) are defined as , where the symbol represents the combinatorial number. This technique refers to a mathematical tool for moment calculation and is not based on the assumption that the underlying distribution is binomial distribution (Luo S et al., 2023). Hence, our approach is general and does not require the distribution itself to follow a binomial form.

      More details about the parameter sampling are required. For instance, why are the specific ranges chosen and their implications? And is the space explored in logarithmic scale?  

      Thank the reviewer for the insightful comment regarding parameter sampling. In our study, we considered five parameters: . The parameters k<sub>off</sub>  and k<sub>on</sub> represent the number of intermediate reaction steps involved in transcriptional state transitions. These values were sampled uniformly from the range 1 to 15, which aligns with biological evidence indicating that most genes undergo either direct (single-step) transitions or a small number of intermediate steps, typically fewer than ten (Tunnacliffe E & Chubb JR, 2020). This range is sufficient to capture both widely used singlestep models and more detailed multi-step mechanisms without introducing biologically implausible complexity. 

      Among these parameters, r<sub>off</sub> and r<sub>on</sub> denote the rate constants governing stochastic transitions between the OFF and ON transcriptional states, respectively. The mean duration of the OFF state, which corresponds to the time between transcriptional bursts, is given by = k<sub>off</sub> / r<sub>off</sub> , and falls within the range ∈(0.1,150).Experimental measurements report a median value of approximately 3.7 (Gupta A et al., 2022), which is well contained within this range. Similarly, the mean duration of the ON state, referred to as the burst duration, is defined by = k<sub>on</sub> / r<sub>on</sub> , and spans the interval ∈(0.1,1500). The experimentally observed median value of 0.12 (Gupta A et al., 2022) confirms that the parameter range adequately captures biologically realistic dynamics.

      The parameter k<sub>syn</sub>  represents the normalized synthesis rate after accounting for molecular degradation. Its range was chosen based on empirical observations of transcriptional burst sizes, which typically vary from single molecules to several dozen (Gupta A et al., 2022). Considering the relationship BS = k<sub>syn</sub> * , the selected range of k<sub>syn</sub> ensures that the experimentally observed burst sizes are well represented within the defined parameter space. We have added the explanation in methods 1.2 section and supplementary text 4.

      We fully recognize the advantages of logarithmic sampling, particularly when parameters span several orders of magnitude. Logarithmic scaling ensures balanced exploration across wide ranges and prevents sampling bias towards larger values. However, in our work, we applied Sobol sampling directly within the original (linear) parameter space. Although we did not explicitly transform parameters into logarithmic scale, Sobol sequences provide low-discrepancy, quasi-random coverage, which promotes uniform sampling across bounded domains (Sobol IM, 1967). Further, if necessary, we can increase the parameter range adaptively, and perform simulation algorithm to obtain sample and train a new model to solve a larger parameter range. 

      On page 15, the rationale for selecting the parameter space is unclear. This is crucial, as fully connected neural networks typically exhibit poor extrapolation beyond their training parameter space. If the parameter space of an experimental dataset significantly differs from the training range, the inference results may become unreliable. We suggest further clarification on how the alignment between the parameter spaces of the experimental data and the training dataset can be ensured to maintain inference accuracy.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful comment regarding the extrapolation limitations of fully connected neural networks. To address this concern, we have implemented a truncation strategy during inference, which constrains the inferred parameters to remain within the bounds of the training parameter space. This ensures that the neural network operates within a regime where its predictive accuracy has been validated, thereby enhancing the robustness of our results. Additionally, we have carefully selected the training parameter space to be reasonable, based on the characteristics of the experimental data. These ranges have been validated through domain knowledge and data analysis, ensuring that even when the experimental data approaches the boundaries of the training range, the inference results remain reliable and accurate.

      On page 16, it is unclear why the authors chose to incorporate the Fano factor instead of using the coefficient of variation or variance. Clarifying the reasoning behind the selection of the Fano factor over these other statistical measures would provide better insight into its relevance for their analysis.  

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. Although the loss term is described using the Fano factor, its formulation actually involves both the variance and the mean. Specifically, the loss we use is: . We chose to use the Fano factor because it is particularly well-suited for quantifying transcriptional noise in systems where the mean expression level varies across conditions or parameters. Unlike variance, the Fano factor normalizes variability by the mean, making it more robust for comparing noise levels across genes or regulatory regimes with different expression levels. Compared to the coefficient of variation (CV), which normalizes by the square of the mean, the Fano factor tends to be less sensitive to low expression regimes and is commonly used in stochastic gene expression studies, especially when the distribution is skewed or over dispersed (i.e., variance exceeds the mean). This makes it a more appropriate metric in our context, where transcriptional bursting often leads to over dispersed expression distributions. We have added an explanation in the methods 1.3 of revised manuscript to explain this choice.

      On page 17, the definition of "sample" is unclear. Does it refer to the number of parameters sets or to the simulated trajectories generated by stochastic simulation algorithms?

      Thank reviewers for your valuable feedback. The term "sample" in this context refers to the data points used in the neural network training set. To eliminate any ambiguity, we included a precise mathematical definition of "sample" (θ<sub>i</sub>,P<sub>simulation,i</sub> ) in the methods 1.3 section of revised manuscript.

      Additionally, it is unclear how the authors determined the number of simulated trajectories per parameter set to ensure training accuracy. Furthermore, it would be relevant to address whether including moments during neural network training is beneficial.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful questions regarding the simulation and training process. To clarify, for each parameter set, we did not simulate multiple trajectories to obtain the corresponding distribution. Instead, we simulated the system for a sufficiently long period to ensure that the system reached a steady-state distribution. From this steady-state data, we then used interpolation methods to derive the corresponding distribution for each parameter set.

      On the other hand, the moments were calculated theoretically without any approximations, providing higher accuracy. By incorporating the moments into the training process, we can effectively mitigate potential biases arising from insufficient sampling of the simulated data. Moreover, our experiments on the synthetic dataset demonstrate that introducing the moments as a loss function significantly enhances the model's performance on the test set (Figure 2E).

      What is the intuition behind the choice of alpha_cg? On page 18, the rationale for setting the sampling probability to 0.5 is unclear. Could this parameter be inferred rather than being preset?  

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful comment regarding the choice of α<sub>cg</sub>. We acknowledge that the typical values of this parameter in related literature often fall within a narrower range (e.g., 0.06–0.32) (Zheng GX et al., 2017; Macosko EZ et al., 2015). However, our decision to set α<sub>cg</sub> was based on a trade-off between sampling efficiency and computational tractability in our specific application context. While it is indeed possible to infer α<sub>cg</sub> as a learnable parameter, we opted for a fixed value in this work to reduce model complexity and avoid unidentifiability issues. In addition, we conducted inference under different capture efficiencies (0.5, 0.3, and 0.2), and found that the inferred burst size (BS) and burst frequency (BF) remained strongly correlated across these conditions (Supplementary Figure S12). This indicates that variations in capture efficiency do not significantly impact the outcomes of downstream enrichment analyses. Nevertheless, we agree that adaptively learning α<sub>cg</sub> could be a promising direction, and we plan to explore this in future work. We have added the explanation in methods 1.4 section.

      On page 19, the authors employed gradient descent for parameter inference. However, as this method is sensitive to initial values, it is unclear how the starting points were selected.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for highlighting the sensitivity of gradient-based optimization methods to initial values. To address this concern, we adopted a black-box optimization strategy in the form of the adaptive differential evolution (DE) algorithm (Das S & Suganthan PN, 2010) to derive robust initial parameters for the parameter inference. The adaptive DE algorithm enables global exploration across a broad parameter space, thereby reducing the risk of convergence to suboptimal local minima. This yielded reasonably good initial estimates, which were subsequently refined using gradient-based optimization to identify high-quality solutions characterized by a vanishing gradient norm. This hybrid strategy, which combines global and local search, is widely adopted in optimization literature to alleviate the risk of entrapment in local optima (Ahandani MA et al., 2014). We have clarified this detail in the third result of the revised manuscript.

      Furthermore, clarification on how the gradients were computed - whether through finite difference approximation or other methods - would offer additional insight into the robustness and accuracy of their approach.

      Thank reviewers for valuable feedback. Regarding the computation of gradients, we use the chain rule in neural networks, and the gradients are computed through backpropagation. Specifically, we rely on automatic differentiation to efficiently calculate the gradients. Unlike finite difference approximation, automatic differentiation directly computes the derivative of the loss function with respect to each parameter, ensuring accurate gradient calculations (Baydin AG et al., 2018). We have clarified this detail in the discussion section of the revised manuscript.

      The paper presents several comparisons between continuous and discrete distributions in Figure 2B and Supplementary Figures S4, S6, and S8, described as a "comparison between mRNA distribution and inferred distribution by DeepTX for scRNA-seq data" or a "comparison between SSA results and DeepTX prediction results." This may lead to confusion for the reader, as the paper focuses on transcriptional bursting, a process where we would typically expect the distributions to be discrete. Clarifying this point would help align the figures with the main topic and enhance the reader's understanding.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. We understand the concern that the distributions shown in Figure 2B and Supplementary Figures S4, S6, and S8 may appear to be continuous, which could be confusing given that transcriptional bursting naturally results in discrete mRNA count distributions.

      We have clarified that in all these figures, both the empirical mRNA distributions derived from scRNAseq data and the model-predicted distributions from DeepTX are inherently discrete. To visualize the empirical distributions, we used histograms where the x-axis corresponds to discrete mRNA copy numbers and the y-axis represents the normalized frequency (density). To illustrate the DeepTX-inferred probability mass function, we plotted the predicted probabilities at each integer count as points and connected them with lines for clarity. While the connecting lines give the appearance of continuity, this is a standard graphical convention used to better show trends and model fit in discrete distributions.

      We suggest that Figure 3E could present the error as a percentage of the parameter value, as this would provide a more equitable comparison and better illustrate the relative accuracy of the parameter estimation.

      Thank reviewers for suggestion regarding Figure 3E. We agree that presenting the error as a percentage of the parameter value would offer a more equitable basis for comparison and better highlight the relative accuracy of our parameter estimation. Accordingly, we have revised Figure 3E to include the relative percentage error for each parameter.

      Figure 4A could be improved for better legibility. The contour plots are somewhat confusing, and the light blue points are difficult to distinguish. Additionally, the x-axis label "Untreatment" appears throughout the manuscript-could this term be referring to the control experiment?

      Thank reviewers for constructive feedback. We have revised Figure 4A to improve its clarity and legibility. Specifically, we adjusted the display style of the contour plots and enhanced the visibility of the light green points to make them more distinguishable.

      Additionally, we recognize the potential confusion caused by the term "Untreatment" and have replaced it with "Control" throughout the revised manuscript to ensure consistency and accuracy in terminology.

      Figure 4B was unclear, and further explanation would be helpful for understanding its purpose.

      Thank reviewers for feedback. The purpose of Figure 4B is to illustrate the relationship between bursting kinetics and the mean and variance of the model. In the revised manuscript, we will provide a more detailed explanation of how the figure captures these relationships, highlighting the key insights it offers into the underlying dynamics.

      Figure 4B illustrates the quantitative relationships among BS, BF, and gene expression noise within the framework of the transcriptional model. In this log-log-log 3D space, the mean expression level is constrained on a blue plane defined by the equation log(BS)+log(BF) = log(Mean), highlighting that the product of burst size and burst frequency determines the mean expression level. The orange plane represents a scaling relationship between expression noise and burst kinetics, expressed as log(BS)+log(BF) = klog(Noise), where k is a constant indicating how the burst kinetics co-vary with noise. Notably, the trajectory of the green sphere demonstrates that, under a fixed mean expression level (i.e., remaining on the blue plane), an increase in gene expression noise arises primarily from an increase in burst size. We have revised the caption of Figure 4B.

      In Figure 4D, two of the GO analysis terms are highlighted in red, but the meaning behind this emphasis is not clear. The same question applies to Figure 5E, where the green dots are missing from the plot.

      Clarification on these points would enhance the overall clarity.  

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful comments. We have added further clarification regarding the enrichment analysis results presented in Figure 4D. Specifically, we highlighted the "cell cycle G2/M phase transition" pathway because a delay in the G2/M phase transition has been shown to increase the probability of cell differentiation, which is a key aspect of our study. In addition, since IdU treatment is known to induce DNA damage, we emphasized the DNA damage-related pathway to support the biological relevance and consistency of our enrichment results. Similarly, in Figure 5E, we highlighted the apoptosis-related pathway. Apoptosis in this context is closely associated with cellular responses to toxic substances and mitochondrial dynamics. The enrichment of pathways related to these processes enables us to hypothesize the underlying mechanisms driving apoptosis in our system. Further, the absence of green dots in Figure 5E was due to an error in the figure caption. We have revised the figure caption accordingly to accurately describe all elements presented in the figure.

      Clarify axis labels in figures, particularly the y-axis in Figure 5A and the x-axis in Figure 6G. In the first case, it isn't clear what this "value" represents. In the second case, the x-label is very confusing. As I understand the figure description, in these plots you are always comparing the G0 arrested genes between control and treated cells. But the x-label says "G0 (0 D)", "Cycle (50 D)".

      Thank reviewers for pointing out the issues with the axis labels. We have made the necessary revisions to eliminate any confusion. In Figure 5A, the label for the y-axis has been changed from "value" to "log2 (value)" for clarity. The “value” in y-axis represents the value of statistical measure indicated at top of each panel. In Figure 6G, the x-axis label "Cycle (50 D)" has been updated to "G0 (50 D)" to accurately reflect the comparison between the G0-arrested genes in control and treated cells. We have revised the text of Figure 5A and Figure 6G.

      Figure 6 uses a QS metric (quality score), but the definition of this metric is not provided. Including a brief explanation of its meaning would be helpful for clarity.  

      Thank reviewers for feedback. In this version, we provided explanation of the QS (Quality Score) metric in the supplementary text 3 for better clarity. The QS is calculated based on the difference in z-scores derived from GSVA (Gene set variation analysis) of gene sets upregulated and downregulated during the quiescent phase, and is defined as QS = z(up genes)− z(down genes) , as described in the literature (Wiecek AJ et al., 2023). z(up genes) represents the standardized enrichment score of the gene set upregulated during quiescence in each sample. A higher value indicates that the quiescenceassociated upregulated genes are actively expressed, suggesting that the sample is more likely to be in a quiescent (G0) state. z(down genes)  corresponds to the standardized enrichment score of genes downregulated during quiescence. A lower value implies effective suppression of these genes, which is also consistent with quiescence. The difference score QS serves as an integrated indicator of the quiescent state: A higher value reflects simultaneous activation of quiescence-associated upregulated genes and repression of downregulated genes, indicating a gene expression profile that strongly aligns with the G0/quiescent state. A lower or negative value suggests a deviation from the quiescent signature, potentially reflecting a proliferative state or failure to enter quiescence. 

      In Figure 6G, light grey lines are shown, but their significance is unclear. It would be useful to specify what these lines represent.

      Thank reviewers for observation. In Figure 6G, each point represents a single gene, and the light grey lines indicate the trend of changes in the corresponding bursting kinetics values, mean and variance for genes. We have added the explanation in the caption of Figure 6G.

      Additionally, the manuscript should include references to the specific pathways used in the GO analysis to provide more context for the reader.

      Thank reviewers for the suggestion. We have included references to the specific pathways used in the GO analysis in the revised manuscript to provide additional context for the readers.

      In the discussion, sentences like "IdU drug treatment-induced BS enhancement delays the cell mitosis phase transition, impacting cell reprogramming and differentiation" are problematic as they imply causality, which I believe cannot be determined through the present analysis. The strength of the conclusions needs to be better argued (or toned down).

      We acknowledge that the original sentence lacked precision and may have conveyed a misleading implication of causality not fully supported by our current analysis. In the discussion section of revised manuscript, we have rephrased the statement to present a more nuanced interpretation: IdU drug treatment-induced BS enhancement of genes may be associated with a delayed transition in the cell mitosis phase, which could potentially influence cell reprogramming and differentiation.  

      Other (minor) comments:

      On pp. 10, "the BS down-regulates differential genes were mainly enriched..." appears to have a grammatical error/typo, "down-regulated"?

      We have made correction. We have revised “down-regulates” to “down-regulated” for grammatical consistency.

      Equation 2 doesn't match Figure 1A.

      We have made correction. The definition of BF = in Equation 2 is correct. We have revised the definition of BF in Figure 1A to ensure consistency with Equation 2.

      Reference

      Zheng, G.X., Terry, J.M., Belgrader, P., Ryvkin, P., Bent, Z.W., Wilson, R., Ziraldo, S.B., Wheeler, T.D., McDermott, G.P., Zhu, J., Gregory, M.T., Shuga, J., Montesclaros, L., Underwood, J.G., Masquelier, D.A., Nishimura, S.Y., Schnall-Levin, M., Wyatt, P.W., Hindson, C.M., Bharadwaj, R., Wong, A., Ness, K.D., Beppu, L.W., Deeg, H.J., McFarland, C., Loeb, K.R., Valente, W.J., Ericson, N.G., Stevens, E.A., Radich, J.P., Mikkelsen, T.S., Hindson, B.J., Bielas, J.H. 2017. Massively parallel digital transcriptional profiling of single cells. Nature Communications 8: 14049. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14049, PMID: 28091601

      Hagemann-Jensen, M., Ziegenhain, C., Chen, P., Ramsköld, D., Hendriks, G.J., Larsson, A.J.M., Faridani, O.R., Sandberg, R. 2020. Single-cell RNA counting at allele and isoform resolution using Smart-seq3. Nature Biotechnology 38: 708714. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41587-020-0497-0, PMID: 32518404

      Larsson, A.J.M., Johnsson, P., Hagemann-Jensen, M., Hartmanis, L., Faridani, O.R., Reinius, B., Segerstolpe, A., Rivera, C.M., Ren, B., Sandberg, R. 2019. Genomic encoding of transcriptional burst kinetics. Nature 565: 251-254. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41586-018-0836-1, PMID: 30602787

      Ochiai, H., Hayashi, T., Umeda, M., Yoshimura, M., Harada, A., Shimizu, Y., Nakano, K., Saitoh, N., Liu, Z., Yamamoto, T., Okamura, T., Ohkawa, Y., Kimura, H., Nikaido, I. 2020. Genome-wide kinetic properties of transcriptional bursting in mouse embryonic stem cells. Science Advances 6: eaaz6699. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aaz6699, PMID: 32596448

      Luo, S., Wang, Z., Zhang, Z., Zhou, T., Zhang, J. 2023. Genome-wide inference reveals that feedback regulations constrain promoter-dependent transcriptional burst kinetics. Nucleic Acids Research 51: 68-83. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1093/nar/gkac1204, PMID: 36583343

      Rodriguez, J., Ren, G., Day, C.R., Zhao, K., Chow, C.C., Larson, D.R. 2019. Intrinsic dynamics of a human gene reveal the basis of expression heterogeneity. Cell 176: 213-226.e218. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.11.026, PMID: 30554876

      Luo, S., Zhang, Z., Wang, Z., Yang, X., Chen, X., Zhou, T., Zhang, J. 2023. Inferring transcriptional bursting kinetics from single-cell snapshot data using a generalized telegraph model. Royal Society Open Science 10: 221057. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsos.221057, PMID: 37035293

      Eling, N., Morgan, M.D., Marioni, J.C. 2019. Challenges in measuring and understanding biological noise. Nature Reviews Genetics 20: 536-548. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41576-019-0130-6, PMID: 31114032

      Tunnacliffe, E., Chubb, J.R. 2020. What is a transcriptional burst? Trends in Genetics 36: 288-297. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tig.2020.01.003, PMID: 32035656

      Rodriguez, J., Larson, D.R. 2020. Transcription in living Cells: molecular mechanisms of bursting. Annual Review of Biochemistry 89: 189-212. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-biochem-011520-105250, PMID: 32208766

      Morgan, M.D., Marioni, J.C. 2018. CpG island composition differences are a source of gene expression noise indicative of promoter responsiveness. Genome Biology 19: 81. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s13059-018-1461-x, PMID: 29945659

      Raj, A., van Oudenaarden, A. 2008. Nature, nurture, or chance: stochastic gene expression and its consequences. Cell 135: 216-226. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2008.09.050, PMID: 18957198

      Trzaskoma, P., Jung, S., Pękowska, A., Bohrer, C.H., Wang, X., Naz, F., Dell’Orso, S., Dubois, W.D., Olivera, A., Vartak, S.V. 2024. 3D chromatin architecture, BRD4, and Mediator have distinct roles in regulating genome-wide transcriptional bursting and gene network. Science Advances 10: eadl4893. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/https://www.science.org/doi/10.1126/sciadv.adl4893, PMID: 

      Browning, A.P., Warne, D.J., Burrage, K., Baker, R.E., Simpson, M.J. 2020. Identifiability analysis for stochastic differential equation models in systems biology. Journal of the Royal Society Interface 17: 20200652. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2020.0652, PMID: 33323054

      Zoller, B., Little, S.C., Gregor, T. 2018. Diverse spatial expression patterns emerge from unified kinetics of transcriptional bursting. Cell 175: 835-847.e825. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2018.09.056, PMID: 30340044

      Hoppe, C., Bowles, J.R., Minchington, T.G., Sutcliffe, C., Upadhyai, P., Rattray, M., Ashe, H.L. 2020. Modulation of the promoter activation rate dictates the transcriptional response to graded BMP signaling levels in the drosophila embryo. Dev Cell 54: 727-741.e727. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.devcel.2020.07.007, PMID: 32758422

      Ramsköld, D., Hendriks, G.J., Larsson, A.J.M., Mayr, J.V., Ziegenhain, C., Hagemann-Jensen, M., Hartmanis, L., Sandberg, R. 2024. Single-cell new RNA sequencing reveals principles of transcription at the resolution of individual bursts. Nature Cell Biology 26: 1725-1733. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1038/s41556-024-01486-9, PMID: 39198695 Van Kampen, N.G. 1992. Stochastic Processes in Physics and Chemistry. Elsevier.

      Gupta, A., Martin-Rufino, J.D., Jones, T.R., Subramanian, V., Qiu, X., Grody, E.I., Bloemendal, A., Weng, C., Niu, S.Y., Min, K.H., Mehta, A., Zhang, K., Siraj, L., Al' Khafaji, A., Sankaran, V.G., Raychaudhuri, S., Cleary, B., Grossman, S., Lander, E.S. 2022. Inferring gene regulation from stochastic transcriptional variation across single cells at steady state. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 119: e2207392119. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2207392119, PMID: 35969771

      Gardiner, C.W., Chaturvedi, S. 1977. The Poisson representation. I. A new technique for chemical master equations. Journal of Statistical Physics 17: 429-468. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01014349, PMID: 

      Gorin, G., Carilli, M., Chari, T., Pachter, L. 2024. Spectral neural approximations for models of transcriptional dynamics. Biophysical Journal 123: 2892-2901. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bpj.2024.04.034, PMID: 38715358

      Kuntz, J., Thomas, P., Stan, G.-B., Barahona, M. 2021. Stationary distributions of continuous-time Markov chains: a review of theory and truncation-based approximations. SIAM Review 63: 3-64. DOI: 

      Zhang, J., Zhou, T. 2019. Computation of stationary distributions in stochastic models of cellular processes with molecular memory. bioRxiv: 521575. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1101/521575, PMID: 

      Zhang, J., Nie, Q., Zhou, T. 2016. A moment-convergence method for stochastic analysis of biochemical reaction networks. The Journal of chemical physics 144. DOI: 

      Sobol, I.M. 1967. On the distribution of points in a cube and the approximate evaluation of integrals. USSR Comput. Math. Math. Phys. 7: 784-802. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0041-5553(67)90144-9, PMID: 

      Macosko, E.Z., Basu, A., Satija, R., Nemesh, J., Shekhar, K., Goldman, M., Tirosh, I., Bialas, A.R., Kamitaki, N., Martersteck, E.M., Trombetta, J.J., Weitz, D.A., Sanes, J.R., Shalek, A.K., Regev, A., McCarroll, S.A. 2015. Highly parallel genome-wide expression profiling of individual cells using nanoliter dsroplets. Cell 161: 1202-1214. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2015.05.002, PMID: 26000488

      Das, S., Suganthan, P.N. 2010. Differential evolution: A survey of the state-of-the-art. IEEE transactions on evolutionary computation 15: 4-31. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1109/TEVC.2010.2059031, PMID: 

      Ahandani, M.A., Vakil-Baghmisheh, M.-T., Talebi, M. 2014. Hybridizing local search algorithms for global optimization. Computational Optimization and Applications 59: 725-748. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/s10589014-9652-1, PMID: 

      Baydin, A.G., Pearlmutter, B.A., Radul, A.A., Siskind, J.M. 2018. Automatic differentiation in machine learning: a survey. Journal of machine learning research 18: 1-43. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/https://dl.acm.org/doi/abs/10.5555/3122009.3242010, PMID: 

      Wiecek, A.J., Cutty, S.J., Kornai, D., Parreno-Centeno, M., Gourmet, L.E., Tagliazucchi, G.M., Jacobson, D.H., Zhang, P., Xiong, L., Bond, G.L., Barr, A.R., Secrier, M. 2023. Genomic hallmarks and therapeutic implications of G0 cell cycle arrest in cancer. Genome Biology 24: 128. DOI: https://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s13059-023-02963-4, PMID: 37221612

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) The authors demonstrate that female Spodoptera littoralis moths prefer to oviposit on wellwatered tomato plants and avoid drought-stressed plants. The study then recorded the sounds produced by drought-stressed plants and found that they produce 30 ultrasonic clicks per minute. Thereafter, the authors tested the response of female S. littoralis moths to clicks with a frequency of 60 clicks per minute in an arena with and without plants and in an arena setting with two healthy plants of which one was associated with 60 clicks per minute. These experiments revealed that in the absence of a plant, the moths preferred to lay eggs on the side of the area in which the clicks could be heard, while in the presence of a plant the S. littoralis females preferred to oviposit on the plant where the clicks were not audible. In addition, the authors also tested the response of S. littoralis females in which the tympanic membrane had been pierced making the moths unable to detect the click sounds. As hypothesised, these females placed their eggs equally on both sites of the area.

      Finally, the authors explored whether the female oviposition choice might be influenced by the courtship calls of S. littoralis males which emit clicks in a range similar to a drought-stressed tomato plant. However, no effect was found of the clicks from ten males on the oviposition behaviour of the female moths, indicating that the females can distinguish between the two types of clicks. Besides these different experiments, the authors also investigated the distribution of egg clusters within a longer arena without a plant, but with a sugar-water feeder. Here it was found that the egg clusters were mostly aggregated around the feeder and the speaker producing 60 clicks per minute. Lastly, video tracking was used to observe the behaviour of the area without a plant, which demonstrated

      that the moths gradually spent more time at the arena side with the click sounds.

      We thank the reviewers for their helpful comments. We agree with the summary, but would like to note that in the control experiment (Figure 2) we used a click rate of 30 clicks per minute—a design choice driven by the editor’s feedback. We have clarified this and, to further probe the system’s dynamics, added a second experiment employing the same click rate (30 clicks per minute) with a dehydrated plant (see details below). In both experiments, females again showed a clear tendency to oviposit nearer the speaker; these findings are described in the updated manuscript.

      (2) The study addresses a very interesting question by asking whether female moths incorporate plant acoustic signals into their oviposition choice, unfortunately, I find it very difficult to judge how big the influence of the sound on the female choice really is as the manuscript does not provide any graphs showing the real numbers of eggs laid on the different plants, but instead only provides graphs with the Bayesian model fittings for each of the experiments. In addition, the numbers given in the text seem to be relatively similar with large variations e.g. Figure 1B3: 1.8 {plus minus} 1.6 vs. 1.1 {plus minus} 1.0. Furthermore, the authors do not provide access to any of the raw data or scripts of this study, which also makes it difficult to assess the potential impact of this study. Hence, I would very much like to encourage the authors to provide figures showing the measured values as boxplots including the individual data points, especially in Figure 1, and to provide access to all the raw data underlying the figures.

      We acknowledge that there are researchers who favor Bayesian graphical representation versus raw data visualization. Therefore, we have added chartplots of the raw data from Figure 1 in the supplementary section. We are aware of the duplication in presentation and apologize for this redundancy.  

      Regarding the variance and means we obtained in our experiment, we have analyzed all raw data using the statistical model presented, and if statistical significance was found despite a particular mean difference or variance, this is meaningful from a biological perspective. One can certainly discuss whether this difference has biological importance, but it should be remembered that in this experimental system, we are trying to isolate the acoustic signal from a complex system that includes multiple signals. Therefore, at no point we’ve suggested that this is a standalone factor, but rather proposed it as an informative and significant component. 

      In addition to the experiments described above, we conducted an experiment in which we counted both eggs and clusters. The results indicate that cluster counts are a reliable proxy for reproductive investment at a given location. In this experiment, we present cluster numbers alongside egg counts (Figure 2).

      Furthermore, we apologize for the technical error that prevented our uploaded data files from reaching the reviewers. We have also uploaded updated data and code.

      (3) Regarding the analysis of the results, I am also not entirely convinced that each night can be taken as an independent egg-laying event, as the amount of eggs and the place were the eggs are laid by a female moth surely depends on the previous oviposition events. While I must admit that I am not a statistician, I would suggest, from a biological point of view, that each group of moths should be treated as a replicate and not each night. I would therefore also suggest to rather analyse the sum of eggs laid over the different consecutive nights than taking the eggs laid in each night as an independent data point.

      We thank the reviewer for this question. This is a valid and point that we will address in three aspects: 

      First, regarding our statistical approach, we used a model that takes into account the sequence of nights and examines whether there is an effect of the order of nights, i.e., we used GLMMs, with the night nested within the repetition. This is equivalent to addressing this as a repeated measure and is, to our best knowledge, the common way to treat such data. 

      Second, following the reviewer's comment, we also reran the statistics of the third experiment (i.e., “sound gradient experiments”, Figure 2 and Supplementary figure 4) when only taking the first night when the female/s laid eggs to avoid the concern of dependency. This analysis revealed the same result – i.e., a significant preference for the sound stimulus. We have now updated our methods and results section to clarify this point.  

      Third, an important detail that may not have been clearly specified in the methods: at the end of each night, we cleaned the arena of counted egg clusters using a cloth with ethanol, so that on the subsequent night, we would not expect there to be evidence of previous oviposition but thus would not exclude some sort of physiological or cognitive memories. We have now updated our methods section to clarify this important procedural point. 

      (4) Furthermore, it did not become entirely clear to me why a click frequency of 60 clicks per minute was used for most experiments, while the plants only produce clicks at a range of 30 clicks per minute. Independent of the ecological relevance of these sound signals, it would be nice if the authors could provide a reason for using this frequency range. Besides this, I was also wondering about the argument that groups of plants might still produce clicks in the range of 60 clicks per minute and that the authors' tests might therefore still be reasonable. I would agree with this, but only in the case that a group of plants with these sounds would be tested. Offering the choice between two single plants while providing the sound from a group of plants is in my view not the most ecologically reasonable choice. It would be great if the authors could modify the argument in the discussion section accordingly and further explore the relevance of different frequencies and dBlevels.

      This is an excellent point. We originally increased the click rate generate a strong signal. However, it was important for us to verify that there was ecological relevance in the stimulus we implemented in the system. For this purpose, we recorded a group of dehydrated plants at a distance of ~20cm and we measured a click rate of 20 clicks per minute (i.e., 0.33 Hz) (see Methods section). Therefore, as mentioned at the beginning of this letter, in the additional experiment described in Figure 2, we reduced the click frequency to 30 clicks per minute, and at this lower rate, the effect was maintained. Increasing plant density would probably lead to a higher rate of 30 clicks per minute. 

      (5) Finally, I was wondering how transferable the findings are towards insects and Lepidopterans in general. Not all insects possess a tympanic organ and might therefore not be able to detect the plant clicks that were recorded. Moreover, I would imagine that generalist herbivorous like Spodoptera might be more inclined to use these clicks than specialists, which very much rely on certain chemical cues to find their host plants. It would be great if the authors would point more to the fact that your study only investigated a single moth species and that the results might therefore only hold true for S. littoralis and closely related species, but not necessary for other moth species such as Sphingidae or even butterflies.

      Good point. Our research uses a specific model system of one moth species and one plant species in a particular plant-insect interaction where females select host plants for their offspring. As with any model-based research that attempts to draw broader conclusions, we've taken care to distinguish between our direct findings and potential wider implications. We believe our system may represent mechanisms relevant to a wider group of herbivorous insects with hearing capabilities, particularly considering that several moth families and other insect orders can detect ultrasound. However, additional research examining more moth and plant species is necessary to determine how broadly applicable these findings are. We have made these clarifications in the text.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      (6) The results are intriguing, and I think the experiments are very well designed. However, if female moths use the sounds emitted by dehydrated plants as cues to decide where to oviposit, the hypothesis would predict that they would avoid such sounds. The discussion mentions the possibility of a multi-modal moth decision-making process to explain these contradictory results, and I also believe this is a strong possibility. However, since this remains speculative, careful consideration is needed regarding how to interpret the findings based solely on the direct results presented in the results section.  

      Thank you for this insightful observation. We agree that the apparent attraction of females to dehydrated-plant sounds contradicts our initial prediction. Having observed this pattern consistently across multiple setups, we have now added a targeted choice experiment to the revised manuscript: here female moths were offered a choice between dehydrated plants broadcasting their natural ultrasonic emissions and a control. These results—detailed in the Discussion and presented in full in the Supplementary Materials (Supplementary Figure 4)—show that when only a dehydrated plant is available, moths would prefer it for oviposition, supporting our hypothesis that in the absence of a real plant, the plant’s sounds might represent a plant..

      (7) Additionally, the final results describing differences in olfactory responses to drying and hydrated plants are included, but the corresponding figures are placed in the supplementary materials. Given this, I would suggest reconsidering how to best present the hypotheses and clarify the overarching message of the results. This might involve reordering the results or re-evaluating which data should appear in the main text versus the supplementary materials

      Thank you for this suggestion. We have reorganized the manuscript and removed the olfactory response data from the current version to maintain a focused narrative on acoustic cues. We agree that a detailed investigation of multimodal interactions deserves a separate study, which we plan to pursue in future work. 

      (8) There were also areas where more detailed explanations of the experimental methods would be beneficial.

      Thank you for highlighting this point. We have expanded and clarified the Methods section to provide comprehensive detail on our experimental procedures.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (9) Line 1: Please include the name of the species you tested also in the title as your results might not hold true for all moth species.

      We do not fully agree with this comment. Please see comment 5.

      (10) Line 19-20: Please rephrase the sentence so that it becomes clear that the "dehydration stress" refers to the plant and not to the moths.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly

      (11) Line 31: Male moths might provide many different signals to the females, maybe better "male sound signals" or similar.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (12) Line 52-53: Maybe mention here that not all moth species have evolved these abilities.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (13) Line 77: add a space after 38.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (14) Line 88: Maybe change "secondary predators" to "natural enemies".

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (15) Line 134: Why is "notably" in italics? I would suggest using normal spelling/formatting rules here.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (16) Line 140-144: If you did perform the experiment also with the more ecological relevant playback rate, why not present these findings as your main results and use the data with the higher playback frequency as additional support?

      Thank you for this suggestion. We agree that the ecologically relevant playback data are important; as described in detail at the beginning of this letter and also in comment 4, however, to preserve a clear and cohesive narrative, we have maintained the original ordering of this section. Nevertheless, the various experiments conducted in Figure 1 differ in several components from Figure 2 and the work that examined sounds in plant groups in the appendices. Therefore, we find it more appropriate to use them as supporting evidence for the main findings rather than creating a comparison between different experimental systems. For this reason, we chose to keep them as a separate description in "The ecological playback findings (Lines 140–144) remain fully described in the Results and serve to reinforce the main observations without interrupting the manuscript's flow.

      (17) Line 146: Please explain already here how you deafened the moths.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (18) Line 181: should it be "male moths' " ?

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (19) Line 215: Why is "without a plant" in italics? I would suggest using normal spelling/formatting rules here.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (20) Line 234: I do not understand why this type of statistic was used to analyse the electroantennogram (EAG) results. Would a rather simple Student's t-test or a Wilcon rank sum test not have been sufficient? I would also like to caution you not to overinterpret the data derived from the EAG, as you combined the entire headspace into one mixture it is no longer possible to derive information on the different volatiles in the blends. The differences you observe might therefore mostly be due to the amount of emitted volatiles.

      We have reorganized the manuscript and removed the olfactory response data from the current version to maintain a focused narrative on acoustic cues (See comment 7). 

      (21) Line 268: It might be nice to add an additional reference here referring to the multimodal oviposition behaviour of the moths.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (22) Line 284: If possible, please add another reference here referring to the different cues used by moths during oviposition.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (23) Line 336: What do you mean by "closed together"?

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (24) Line 434-436: Please see my overall comments. I do not think that you can call it ecologically relevant if the signal emitted by multiple plants is played in the context of just a single plant.

      Please see comments 1 and 4.

      (25) Line 496: Please change "stats" to statistics.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (26) Line 522-524: I am not sure whether simply listing their names does give full credit to the work these people did for your study. Maybe also explain how they contributed to your work.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (27) L54 20-60kHz --> 20Hz-60kHz or 20kHz - 60kHz?

      OK. We have replaced it.

      (28) L124 Are the results for the condition where nothing was placed and the condition where a decoy silent resistor was placed combined in the analysis? If so, were there no significant differences between the two conditions? Comparing these with a condition presenting band-limited noise in the same frequency range as the drought-stressed sounds might also have been an effective approach to further isolate the specific role of the ultrasonic emissions.

      We have used both conditions due to technical constrains and pooled them tougher for analysis— statistical tests confirmed no significant differences between them—and this clarification has now been added to the Methods section including the results of the statistical test.

      (29) L125 (Fig. 1A), see Exp. 1 in the Methods). -> (Fig.1B. See Exp.1 in the Methods).

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (30) L132 "The opposite choice to what was seen in the initial experiment (Fig.1B)"

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (31) L137-143 If you are writing about results, why not describe them with figures and statistics? The current description reads like a discussion.

      These findings were not among our primary research questions; however, we believe that including them in the Results section underscores the experimental differences. In our opinion, introducing an additional figure or expanding the statistical analysis at this point would disrupt the narrative flow and risk confusing the reader.

      (32) L141 "This is higher than the rate reported for a single young plant" Are you referring to the tomato plants used in the experiments? It might be helpful to include in the main text the natural click rate emitted by tomato plants, as this information is currently only mentioned in the Methods section.

      See comment 4.  

      (33) L191 Is the main point here to convey that the plant playback effect remained significant even when the sound presentation frequency was reduced to 30 clicks per minute? The inclusion of the feeder element, however, seems to complicate the message. To simplify the results, moving the content from lines 185-202 to the supplementary materials might be a better approach. Additionally, what is the rationale for placing the sugar solution in the arena? Is it to maintain the moths' vitality during the experiment? Clarifying this in the methods section would help provide context for this experimental detail.

      In this series of experiments, we manipulated four variables—single moths, ultrasonic click rate, arena configuration (from a two-choice design to an elongated enclosure), and the response metric (total egg counts rather than cluster counts)—to evaluate moth oviposition under more ecologically realistic conditions. We demonstrate the system’s robustness and validity in a more realistic setting (by tracking individual moths, counting single eggs, etc.).  

      As noted in the text, feeders were included to preserve the moths’ natural behavior and vitality. We have further clarified this in the revised manuscript.

      (34) L215 Is the click presentation frequency 30 or 60 per minute? Since Figure 3 illustrates examples of moth movement from the experiment described in Figure 1, it might be more effective to present Figure 3 when discussing the results of Figure 1 or to include it in the supplementary materials for better clarity and organization.

      See comments 1 and 4. As mentioned in the above 

      (35) L291 Please provide a detailed explanation of the experiments and measurements for the results shown in Figure S3 (and Figure S2). If the multi-modal hypothesis discussed in the study is a key focus, it might be better to include these results in the main results section rather than in the supplementary materials.

      Thank you for this suggestion. Figure S2 was removed, see comments above. We’ve added now the context to figure S3.

      (36) L303 It might be helpful to include information about the relationship between the moth species used in this study and tomato plants somewhere in the text. This would provide an important context for understanding the ecological relevance of the experiments.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (37) Table 1 The significant figures in the numbers presented in the tables should be consistent.

      Thank you for the suggestion; we have clarified the text accordingly.

      (38) L341 The text mentions that experiments were conducted in a greenhouse, but does this mean the arena was placed inside the greenhouse? Also, the term "arena" is used - does this refer to a sealed rectangular case or something similar? For the sound presentation experiments, it seems that the arena cage was placed inside a soundproof room. If the arena is indeed a case-like structure, were there any specific measures taken to prevent sound scattering within the case, such as the choice of materials or structural modifications?

      Here, “arena” refers to the plastic boxes used throughout this study. In this particular experiment, we presented plants alone—reflecting ongoing debate in the literature—and used these trials as a baseline for our subsequent sound-presentation experiments, during which we measured sound intensity as described in the Methods section. All sound-playback experiments were conducted in sound-proof rooms, and acoustic levels were measured beforehand—sound on the control side fell below our system’s detection threshold. 

      (39) L373 "resister similar to the speaker" Could you explain it in more detail? I think this would depend on the type of speaker used-particularly whether it includes magnets. From an experimental perspective, presenting different sounds such as white noise from the speaker might have been a better control. Was there a specific reason for not doing so? Additionally, the study does not clearly demonstrate whether the electric and magnetic field environments on both sides of the arena were appropriately controlled. Without this information, it is difficult to evaluate whether using a resistor as a substitute was adequate.

      Thank you for this comment. We have now addressed this point in the Discussion. We acknowledge that we did not account for the magnetic field, which might have differed between the speaker and the resistor. We agree that using an alternative control, such as white noise, could have been informative, and we now mention this as a limitation in the revised Methods.

      (40) L435 60Hz? The representation of frequencies in the text is inconsistent, with some values expressed in Hz and others as "clicks per second." It would be better to standardize these units for clarity, such as using Hz throughout the manuscript.

      We agree that this is confusing. We reviewed the text and made sure that when we addressed click per second, we meant how many clicks were produced and when we addressed Hz units it was in the context of sound frequencies.  

      (41) L484 "we quantified how many times each individual crossed the center of the arena" Is this data being used in the results?

      Yes. Mentioned in the text just before Figure 3. L220

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this study, Meunier et al. investigated the functional role of IL-10 in avian mucosal immunity. While the anti-inflammatory role of IL-10 is well established in mammals, and several confirmatory knockout models are available in mice, IL-10's role in avian mucosal immunity is so far correlative. In this study, the authors generated two different models of IL-10 ablation in Chickens. A whole body knock-out model and an enhancer KO model leading to reduced IL10 expression. The authors first performed in vitro LPS stimulation-based experiments, and then in vivo two different infection models employing C. jejuni and E. tenella, to demonstrate that complete ablation of IL10 leads to enhanced inflammation-related pathology and gene expression, and enhanced pathogen clearance. At a steady-state level, however, IL-10 ablation did not lead to spontaneous colitis. 

      Strengths: 

      Overall, the study is well executed and establishes an anti-inflammatory role of IL-10 in birds. While the results are expected and not surprising, this appears to be the first report to conclusively demonstrate IL-10's anti-inflammatory role upon its genetic ablation in the avian model. Provided this information is applicable in combating pathogen infection in livestock species in sustainable industries like poultry, the study will be of interest to the field. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The study is primarily a confirmation of the already established anti-inflammatory role of IL-10. 

      We do not agree that this work is primarily confirmatory. The anti-inflammatory role of IL10 was indeed known previously from studies in mammals. The much more general insight from the current study is our demonstration of the intrinsic trade-off between inflammation and tolerance in the response to both the microbiome (which was significantly altered in the IL10 knockout birds) and mucosal pathogens. The study of Eimeria challenge in particular highlights the fact that it may be better for the host to tolerate a potential pathogen than to take on the cost of elimination.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The authors were to investigate the functional role of IL10 on mucosal immunity in chickens. CRISPR technology was employed to generate IL10 knock-out chickens in both exon and putative enhancer regions. IL10 expressions were either abolished (knockout in exon) or reduced (enhancer knock-out). IL-10 plays an important role in the composition of the caecal microbiome. Through various enteric pathogen challenges, deficient IL10 expression was associated with enhanced pathogen clearance, but with more severe lesion scores and body weight loss. 

      Strengths: 

      Both in vitro and in vivo knock-out abolished and reduced IL10 expression, and broad enteric pathogens were challenged in vivo, and various parameters were examined to evaluate the functional role of IL10 on mucosal immunity. 

      Weaknesses: 

      Overexpression of IL-10 either in vitro or in vivo may further support the findings from this study. 

      An overexpression experiment, regardless of outcome, would not necessarily support or invalidate the findings of the current study. It would address the question of whether the absolute concentration of IL10 produced alters the outcome of an infection.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The following are the recommendations that, in my opinion, will be helpful to enhance the quality of the study. 

      Major point: 

      The authors at a steady state did not observe any sign of spontaneous colitis. Since IL-10 KO in mice leads to enhanced pathological score upon DSS-mediated induction of colitis, and several colitis models are well established in birds, it will be worthwhile to test the consequence of experimentally inducing colitis in this context. 

      One of the novel features of this study is the observation that the microbiome is modified in the IL10KO HOM chicks, which may serve to mitigate potential spontaneous pathology; we now mention this in the discussion. We agree that it could be worthwhile in the future to look at additional challenge models. However, we would argue that the Eimeria challenge is a sufficiently adequate experimentally-induced model of colitis to demonstrate the increased inflammation that occurs in an IL10-deficient bird. This is further supported by evidence of enhanced inflammatory responses in the caeca of IL10KO HOM birds challenged with Campylobacter or Salmonella relative to WT controls. See in the revised manuscript (pages 12-13).

      Minor points: 

      (1) In Figure 2B, the authors should confirm whether the ROS-AV163 groups also have LPS treatment. 

      The legend for Figure 2B already states that neutralizing anti-IL10 antibody was added to LPS-stimulated BMDMs: “Nitric oxide production was assessed by measuring nitrite levels using Griess assay for LPS-stimulated BMDMs […] in the absence or presence of neutralizing anti-IL10 antibody ROS-AV163”. However, for added clarity we have now modified the x-axis label for Figure 2B (“+ROS-AV163” replaced by “+LPS +anti-IL10”) and we have also made minor changes to the figure legend. See in the revised manuscript (page 33).

      (2) In Figure 3F, the authors should discuss why the duodenum of KO birds has enhanced infiltration compared to WT? 

      We are not sure what the reviewer is referring to here. Although not specifically mentioned in Figure 3F, there is no statistically significant difference in cellular infiltration in the duodenum of IL10KO WT and HOM birds raised in our specified pathogen-free (SPF) facility, nor in the duodenum of IL10KO WT and HOM birds raised in our conventional facility (Mann-Whitney U tests, p>0.1 in both cases); this can be seen in the sums of histopathological scores shown in Figures 3C (SPF facility) and 3E (conventional facility). Figure 3F shows that there is a statistically significant difference in cellular infiltration scores in the duodenum and proximal colon of both IL10KO WT and HOM birds based on the environment they are raised in (SPF vs conventional). We have made minor changes to the text to clarify this. See in the revised manuscript (page 7).

      (3) The authors should discuss the observed differences in the C. jejuni colonization results among the two cohorts at week 1 and week 2 post-infection. 

      Numbers of C. jejuni in the caeca of IL10KO HOM birds were markedly lower than for WT controls at 1-week post-infection in cohort 1, and at both time intervals post-infection in cohort 2 (Figure 4A). This reached statistical significance at 1-week post-infection in cohort 1 and at 2-weeks post-infection in cohort 2. It is evident from Figure 4A that considerable inter-animal variance existed in each group, and in the IL10KO HOM birds in particular. This is typical of C. jejuni colonisation in chickens, where bacterial population structures have been reported to be variable and unpredictable (Coward et al., Appl Environ Microbiol 2008, PMID: 18424530). Similar variation between time intervals, birds and repeated experiments has been reported when evaluating vaccines against C. jejuni colonisation (e.g. Buckley et al., Vaccine 2010, PMID: 19853682; Nothaft et al., Front Microbiol 2021, PMID: 34867850). We performed two independent studies for this reason. Taken together, we consider that our data provide convincing evidence of elevated pro-inflammatory responses upon C. jejuni infection in IL10KO HOM birds relative to WT controls that associates with reduced bacterial burden. Our data is also consistent with a published observation that a commercial broiler line with low IL10 expression had correspondingly elevated expression of CXCLi-1, CXCLi-2 and IL-1b (Humphrey et al., mBio 2014, reference 33 in our original submission). We have added text to the discussion to capture the points above.  See in the revised manuscript (page 13).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      For the animal challenging experiments, both IL10KO HOM and IL10EnKO HOM chickens were used for Eimeria challenge, but not for Salmonella and Campylobacter. Could the authors justify why? 

      The Eimeria challenge produced a much higher and more reproducible level of inflammation than either of the bacterial challenge models. Within the parasite challenge cohorts, IL10KO HET and IL10EnKO HOM birds were only marginally different from WT controls (e.g. parasite replication: Figures 5A and B; lesion scores: Figures 5E and F; body weight gain: Figures 5G and H). Given the more limited response and the inter-individual variation in the bacterial challenge models, we felt that analysis of a sufficiently large cohort of the IL10KO HOM was appropriate, while additional cohorts of IL10KO HET and IL10EnKO HOM birds large enough to detect statistically significant differences could not be justified.

      In the M&M, there was no mention of # of birds generated for IL10EnKO HOM, HET, etc. 

      Full details of bird numbers can be found in SI Appendix Table S1 “Number of IL10KO and IL10EnKO WT, HET and HOM chicks hatched in the NARF SPF chicken facility in the first (G1) and second (G2) generations”. Table S1 is already referred to in the Results section “Generation of IL10-deficient chickens”; we have now also clearly referred to it in the “Animals” and “Generation of surrogate host chickens and establishment of the IL10KO and IL10EnKO lines under SPF conditions” sections of the Materials and Methods. In all three sections we have also added some text to clarify that the table details G1 and G2 bird numbers. See in the revised manuscript (pages 5, 15, 17).

      From the results of Campylobacter challenge, the results from the cohort 1 and cohort 2 were not consistent at both 1 and 2 weeks of post-infection. There is not much discussion on this inconsistency. What is the final conclusion: significant difference in week 1 or week 2, OR none of them, OR both of them. What would happen if an additional cohort were conducted for Salmonella and Eimeria? 

      As noted in response to Reviewer 1 (minor point 3), we have now added text to the discussion on the partial inconsistency between independent C. jejuni challenge studies. We do not feel that additional experiments to address this comment are required. Highly significant increases in the infiltration of lymphoplasmacytic cells and heterophils were detected in IL10KO HOM chickens relative to WT controls in the caeca, a key site of Campylobacter colonisation. This was consistently observed in two independent cohorts at both 1- and 2-weeks post-infection (SI Appendix Figures S7 and S8) and was reflected in similar patterns of expression of pro-inflammatory genes at these intervals in both cohorts (Figure 4B). As our laboratory has observed substantially less variation between repeated Salmonella challenges, a single study was performed, but with adequate power to detect statistical differences.  The effects of E. tenella infection in IL10KO WT and HOM birds were replicated (compare Figure 4 with data from day 6 in Figure 5).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      The authors present their new bioinformatic tool called TEKRABber, and use it to correlate expression between KRAB ZNFs and TEs across different brain tissues, and across species. While the aims of the authors are clear and there would be significant interest from other researchers in the field for a program that can do such correlative gene expression analysis across individual genomes and species, the presented approach and work display significant shortcomings. In the current state of the analysis pipeline, the biases and shortcomings mentioned below, for which I have seen no proof of that they are accounted for by the authors, are severely impacting the presented results and conclusions. It is therefore essential that the points below are addressed, involving significant changes in the TEKRABber progamm as well as the analysis pipeline, to prevent the identification of false positive and negative signals, that would severely affect the conclusions one can raise about the analysis. 

      Thank you very much for the insightful review of our manuscript. Since most of the comments on our revised version are not different from the comments on our first version, we repeated our previous answer, but wrote a new reply to the new concerns (please see the last two paragraphs). 

      We would also like to reiterate here that most of the critique of the reviewer concerns the performance of other tools and not TEKRABber presented in our manuscript. We consider it out of scope for this manuscript to improve other tools.

      My main concerns are provided below: 

      One important shortcoming of the biocomputational approach is that most TEs are not actually expressed, and others (Alus) are not a proxy of the activity of the TE class at all. I will explain: While specific TE classes can act as (species-specific) promoters for genes (such as LTRs) or are expressed as TE derived transcripts (LINEs, SVAs), the majority of other older TE classes do not have such behavior and are either neutral to the genome or may have some enhancer activity (as mapped in the program they refer to 'TEffectR'. A big focus is on Alus, but Alus contribute to a transcriptome in a different way too: They often become part of transcripts due to alternative splicing. As such, the presence of Alu derived transcripts is not a proxy for the expression/activity of the Alu class, but rather a result of some Alus being part of gene transcripts (see also next point). Bottom line is that the TEKRABber software/approach is heavily prone to picking up both false positives (TEs being part of transcribed loci) and false negatives (TEs not producing any transcripts at all) , which has a big implication for how reads from TEs as done in this study should be interpreted: The TE expression used to correlate the KRAB ZNF expression is simply not representing the species-specific influences of TEs where the authors are after. 

      With the strategy as described, a lot of TE expression is misinterpreted: TEs can be part of gene-derived transcripts due to alternative splicing (often happens for Alus) or as a result of the TE being present in an inefficiently spliced out intron (happens a lot) which leads to TE-derived reads as a result of that TE being part of that intron, rather than that TE being actively expressed. As a result, the data as analysed is not reliably indicating the expression of TEs (as the authors intend too) and should be filtered for any reads that are coming from the above scenarios: These reads have nothing to do with KRAB ZNF control, and are not representing actively expressed TEs and therefore should be removed. Given that from my lab's experience in brain (and other) tissues, the proportion of RNA sequencing reads that are actually derived from active TEs is a stark minority compared to reads derived from TEs that happen to be in any of the many transcribed loci, applying this filtering is expected to have a huge impact on the results and conclusions of this study. 

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for highlighting the potential issues of false positives and negatives in TE quantification. The reviewer provided valuable examples of how different TE classes, such as Alus, LTRs, LINEs, and SVAs, exhibit distinct behaviors in the genome. To our knowledge, specific tools like ERVmap (Tokuyama et al., 2018), which annotates ERVs, and LtrDetector (Joseph et al., 2019), which uses k-mer distributions to quantify LTRs, could indeed enhance precision by treating specific TE classes individually. We acknowledge that such approaches may yield more accurate results and appreciate the suggestion. 

      In our study, we used TEtranscripts (Jin et al., 2015) prior to TEKRABber. TEtranscripts applies the Expectation Maximization (EM) algorithm to assign ambiguous reads as the following steps. Uniquely mapped reads are first assigned to genes, and  reads overlapping genes and TEs are assigned to TEs only if they do not uniquely match an annotated gene. The remaining ambiguous reads are distributed based on EM iterations. While this approach may not be as specialized as the latest tools for specific TE classes, it provides a general overview of TE activity. TEtranscripts outputs subfamily-level TE expression data, which we used as input for TEKRABber to perform downstream analyses such as differential expression and correlation studies.

      We understand the importance of adapting tools to specific research objectives, including focusing on particular TE classes. TEKRABber is designed not to refine TE quantification at the mapping stage but to flexibly handle outputs from various TE quantification tools. It accepts raw TE counts as input in the form of dataframes, enabling diverse analytical pipelines. We would also like to clarify that, since the input data is transcriptomic, our primary focus is on expressed TEs, rather than the effects of non-expressed TEs in the genome. In the revised version of our manuscript, we emphasize this distinction in the discussion and provide examples of how TEKRABber can integrate with other tools to enhance specificity and accuracy.

      Another potential problem that I don't see addressed is that due to the high level of similarity of the many hundreds of KRAB ZNF genes in primates and the reads derived from them, and the inaccurate annotations of many KZNFs in non-human genomes, the expression data derived from RNA-seq datasets cannot be simply used to plot KZNF expression values, without significant work and manual curation to safeguard proper cross species ortholog-annotation: The work of Thomas and Schneider (2011) has studied this in great detail but genome-assemblies of non-human primates tend to be highly inaccurate in appointing the right ortholog of human ZNF genes. The problem becomes even bigger when RNA-sequencing reads are analyzed: RNA-sequencing reads from a human ZNF that emerged in great apes by duplication from an older parental gene (we have a decent number of those in the human genome) may be mapped to that older parental gene in Macaque genome: So, the expression of human-specific ZNF-B, that derived from the parental ZNF-A, is likely to be compared in their DESeq to the expression of ZNF-A in Macaque RNA-seq data. In other words, without a significant amount of manual curation, the DE-seq analysis is prone to lead to false comparisons which make the stategy and KRABber software approach described highly biased and unreliable. 

      There is no doubt that there are differences in expression and activity of KRAB-ZNFs and TEs repspectively that may have had important evolutionary consequences. However, because all of the network analyses in this paper rely on the analyses of RNA-seq data and the processing through the TE-KRABber software with the shortcomings and potential biases that I mentioned above, I need to emphasize that the results and conclusions are likely to be significantly different if the appropriate measures are taken to get more accurate and curated TE and KRAB ZNF expression data. 

      We thank the reviewer for raising the important issue of accurately annotating the expanded repertoire of KRAB-ZNFs in primates, particularly the challenges of cross-species orthology and potential biases in RNA-seq data analysis. Indeed, we have also addressed this challenge in some of our previous papers (Nowick et al., 2010, Nowick et al., 2011 and Jovanovic et al., 2021).

      In the revised manuscript, we include more details about our two-step strategy to ensure accurate KRAB-ZNF ortholog assignments. First, we employed the Gene Order Conservation (GOC) score from Ensembl BioMart as a primary filter, selecting only one-to-one orthologs with a GOC score above 75% across primates. This threshold, recommended in Ensembl’s ortholog quality control guidelines, ensures high-confidence orthology relationships.(http://www.ensembl.org/info/genome/compara/Ortholog_qc_manual.html#goc).

      Second, we incorporated data from Jovanovic et al. (2021), which independently validated KRAB-ZNF orthologs across 27 primate genomes. This additional layer of validation allowed us to refine our dataset, resulting in the identification of 337 orthologous KRAB-ZNFs for differential expression analysis (Figure S2).

      We acknowledge that different annotation methods or criteria may for some genes yield variations in the identified orthologs. However, we believe that this combination provides a robust starting point for addressing the challenges raised, while we remain open to additional refinements in future analyses.

      Finally, there are some minor but important notes I want to share:

      The association with certain variations in ZNF genes with neurological disorders such as AD, as reported in the introduction is not entirely convincing without further functional support. Such associations could be merely happen by chance, given the high number of ZNF genes in the human genome and the high chance that variations in these loci happen associate with certatin disease associated traits. So using these associations as an argument that changes in TEs and KRAB ZNF networks are important for diseases like AD should be used with much more caution. 

      We fully acknowledge the concern that, given the large number of KRAB-ZNFs and their inherent variability, some associations with AD or other neurological disorders could occur by chance. This highlights the importance of additional functional studies to validate the causal role of KRAB-ZNF and TE interactions in disease contexts. While previous studies have indeed analyzed KRAB-ZNF and TE expression in human brain tissues, our study seeks to expand on this foundation by incorporating interspecies comparisons across primates. This approach enabled us to identify TE:KRAB-ZNF pairs that are uniquely present in healthy human brains, which may provide insights into their potential evolutionary significance and relevance to diseases like AD.

      In addition to analyzing RNA-seq data (GSE127898 and syn5550404), we have cross-validated our findings using ChIP-exo data for 159 KRAB-ZNF proteins and their TE binding regions in humans (Imbeault et al., 2017). This allowed us to identify specific binding events between KRAB-ZNF and TE pairs, providing further support for the observed associations. We agree with the reviewer that additional experimental validations, such as functional studies, are critical to further establish the role of KRAB-ZNF and TE networks in AD. We hope that future research can build upon our findings to explore these associations in greater detail.

      There is a number of papers where KRAB ZNF and TE expression are analysed in parallel in human brain tissues. So the novelty of that aspect of the presented study may be limited. 

      We agree with the reviewer that many studies have examined the expression levels of KRAB-ZNFs and TEs in developing human brain tissues (Farmiloe et al., 2020; Turelli et al., 2020; Playfoot et al., 2021, among others). However, the novelty of our study lies in comparing KRAB-ZNF and TE expression across primate species, as well as in adult human brain tissues from both control individuals and those with Alzheimer’s disease. To our knowledge, no previous study has analyzed these data in this context. We therefore believe that our results will be of interest to evolutionary biologists and neurobiologists focusing on Alzheimer’s disease.

      Additional note after reviewing the revised version of the manuscript: 

      After reviewing the revised version of the manuscript, my criticism and concerns with this study are still evenly high and unchanged. To clarify, the revised version did not differ in essence from the original version; it seems that unfortunately, no efforts were taken to address the concerns raised on the original version of the manuscript, the results section as well as the discussion section are virtually unchanged.

      We regret that this reviewer was not satisfied with our changes. In fact, many of the points raised by this reviewer are important, but concern weaknesses of other tools. In our opinion, validating other tools would be out of scope for this paper. We want to emphasize that TEKRABber is not a quantification tool for sequencing data, but a software for comparative analysis across species. We provided a detailed answer to the reviewer and readers can refer to that answer in the public review above for further information.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors present their new bioinformatic tool called TEKRABber, and use it to correlate expression between KRAB ZNFs and TEs across different brain tissues, and across species. While the aims of the authors are clear and there would be significant interest from other researchers in the field for a program that can do such correlative gene expression analysis across individual genomes and species, the presented approach and work display significant shortcomings. In the current state of the analysis pipeline, the biases and shortcomings mentioned below, for which I have seen no proof that they are accounted for by the authors, are severely impacting the presented results and conclusions. It is therefore essential that the points below are addressed, involving significant changes in the TEKRABber program as well as the analysis pipeline, to prevent the identification of false positive and negative signals, that would severely affect the conclusions one can raise about the analysis.

      Thank you very much for the insightful review of our manuscript.

      My main concerns are provided below:

      (1) One important shortcoming of the biocomputational approach is that most TEs are not actually expressed, and others (Alus) are not a proxy of the activity of the TE class at all. I will explain: While specific TE classes can act as (species-specific) promoters for genes (such as LTRs) or are expressed as TE derived transcripts (LINEs, SVAs), the majority of other older TE classes do not have such behavior and are either neutral to the genome or may have some enhancer activity (as mapped in the program they refer to 'TEffectR'. A big focus is on Alus, but Alus contribute to a transcriptome in a different way too: They often become part of transcripts due to alternative splicing. As such, the presence of Alu derived transcripts is not a proxy for the expression/activity of the Alu class, but rather a result of some Alus being part of gene transcripts (see also next point). The bottom line is that the TEKRABber software/approach is heavily prone to picking up both false positives (TEs being part of transcribed loci) and false negatives (TEs not producing any transcripts at all), which has a big implication for how reads from TEs as done in this study should be interpreted: The TE expression used to correlate the KRAB ZNF expression is simply not representing the species-specific influences of TEs where the authors are after.

      With the strategy as described, a lot of TE expression is misinterpreted: TEs can be part of gene-derived transcripts due to alternative splicing (often happens for Alus) or as a result of the TE being present in an inefficiently spliced out intron (happens a lot) which leads to TE-derived reads as a result of that TE being part of that intron, rather than that TE being actively expressed. As a result, the data as analysed is not reliably indicating the expression of TEs (as the authors intend to) and should be filtered for any reads that are coming from the above scenarios: These reads have nothing to do with KRAB ZNF control, and are not representing actively expressed TEs and therefore should be removed. Given that from my lab's experience in the brain (and other) tissues, the proportion of RNA sequencing reads that are actually derived from active TEs is a stark minority compared to reads derived from TEs that happen to be in any of the many transcribed loci, applying this filtering is expected to have a huge impact on the results and conclusions of this study.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for highlighting the potential issues of false positives and negatives in TE quantification. The reviewer provided valuable examples of how different TE classes, such as Alus, LTRs, LINEs, and SVAs, exhibit distinct behaviors in the genome. To our knowledge, specific tools like ERVmap (Tokuyama et al., 2018), which annotates ERVs, and LtrDetector (Joseph et al., 2019), which uses k-mer distributions to quantify LTRs, could indeed enhance precision by treating specific TE classes individually. We acknowledge that such approaches may yield more accurate results and appreciate the suggestion. 

      In our study, we used TEtranscripts (Jin et al., 2015) prior to TEKRABber. TEtranscripts applies the Expectation Maximization (EM) algorithm to assign ambiguous reads as the following steps. Uniquely mapped reads are first assigned to genes, and  reads overlapping genes and TEs are assigned to TEs only if they do not uniquely match an annotated gene. The remaining ambiguous reads are distributed based on EM iterations. While this approach may not be as specialized as the latest tools for specific TE classes, it provides a general overview of TE activity. TEtranscripts outputs subfamily-level TE expression data, which we used as input for TEKRABber to perform downstream analyses such as differential expression and correlation studies.

      We understand the importance of adapting tools to specific research objectives, including focusing on particular TE classes. TEKRABber is designed not to refine TE quantification at the mapping stage but to flexibly handle outputs from various TE quantification tools. It accepts raw TE counts as input in the form of dataframes, enabling diverse analytical pipelines. We would also like to clarify that, since the input data is transcriptiomic, our primary focus is on expressed TEs, rather than the effects of non-expressed TEs in the genome. In the revised version of our manuscript, we emphasize this distinction in the discussion and provide examples of how TEKRABber can integrate with other tools to enhance specificity and accuracy.

      (2) Another potential problem that I don't see addressed is that due to the high level of similarity of the many hundreds of KRAB ZNF genes in primates and the reads derived from them, and the inaccurate annotations of many KZNFs in non-human genomes, the expression data derived from RNA-seq datasets cannot be simply used to plot KZNF expression values, without significant work and manual curation to safeguard proper cross species ortholog-annotation: The work of Thomas and Schneider (2011) has studied this in great detail but genome-assemblies of non-human primates tend to be highly inaccurate in appointing the right ortholog of human ZNF genes. The problem becomes even bigger when RNA-sequencing reads are analyzed: RNA-sequencing reads from a human ZNF that emerged in great apes by duplication from an older parental gene (we have a decent number of those in the human genome) may be mapped to that older parental gene in Macaque genome: So, the expression of human-specific ZNF-B, that derived from the parental ZNF-A, is likely to be compared in their DESeq to the expression of ZNF-A in Macaque RNA-seq data. In other words, without a significant amount of manual curation, the DE-seq analysis is prone to lead to false comparisons which make the strategy and KRABber software approach described highly biased and unreliable.

      There is no doubt that there are differences in expression and activity of KRAB-ZNFs and TEs respectively that may have had important evolutionary consequences. However, because all of the network analyses in this paper rely on the analyses of RNA-seq data and the processing through the TE-KRABber software with the shortcomings and potential biases that I mentioned above, I need to emphasize that the results and conclusions are likely to be significantly different if the appropriate measures are taken to get more accurate and curated TE and KRAB ZNF expression data.

      We thank the reviewer for raising the important issue of accurately annotating the expanded repertoire of KRAB-ZNFs in primates, particularly the challenges of cross-species orthology and potential biases in RNA-seq data analysis. Indeed, we have also addressed this challenge in some of our previous papers (Nowick et al., 2010, Nowick et al., 2011 and Jovanovic et al., 2021).

      In the revised manuscript, we include more details about our two-step strategy to ensure accurate KRAB-ZNF ortholog assignments. First, we employed the Gene Order Conservation (GOC) score from Ensembl BioMart as a primary filter, selecting only one-to-one orthologs with a GOC score above 75% across primates. This threshold, recommended in Ensembl’s ortholog quality control guidelines, ensures high-confidence orthology relationships. (http://www.ensembl.org/info/genome/compara/Ortholog_qc_manual.html#goc).

      Second, we incorporated data from Jovanovic et al. (2021), which independently validated KRAB-ZNF orthologs across 27 primate genomes. This additional layer of validation allowed us to refine our dataset, resulting in the identification of 337 orthologous KRAB-ZNFs for differential expression analysis (Figure S2).

      We acknowledge that different annotation methods or criteria may for some genes yield variations in the identified orthologs. However, we believe that this combination provides a robust starting point for addressing the challenges raised, while we remain open to additional refinements in future analyses.

      (3) The association with certain variations in ZNF genes with neurological disorders such as AD, as reported in the introduction is not entirely convincing without further functional support. Such associations could merely happen by chance, given the high number of ZNF genes in the human genome and the high chance that variations in these loci happen to associate with certain disease-associated traits. So using these associations as an argument that changes in TEs and KRAB ZNF networks are important for diseases like AD should be used with much more caution.

      There are a number of papers where KRAB ZNF and TE expression are analysed in parallel in human brain tissues. So the novelty of that aspect of the presented study may be limited.

      We fully acknowledge the concern that, given the large number of KRAB-ZNFs and their inherent variability, some associations with AD or other neurological disorders could occur by chance. This highlights the importance of additional functional studies to validate the causal role of KRAB-ZNF and TE interactions in disease contexts. While previous studies have indeed analyzed KRAB-ZNF and TE expression in human brain tissues, our study seeks to expand on this foundation by incorporating interspecies comparisons across primates. This approach enabled us to identify TE:KRAB-ZNF pairs that are uniquely present in healthy human brains, which may provide insights into their potential evolutionary significance and relevance to diseases like AD.

      In addition to analyzing RNA-seq data (GSE127898 and syn5550404), we have cross-validated our findings using ChIP-exo data for 159 KRAB-ZNF proteins and their TE binding regions in humans (Imbeault et al., 2017). This allowed us to identify specific binding events between KRAB-ZNF and TE pairs, providing further support for the observed associations. We agree with the reviewer that additional experimental validations, such as functional studies, are critical to further establish the role of KRAB-ZNF and TE networks in AD. We hope that future research can build upon our findings to explore these associations in greater detail.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      It is essential before this work can be considered for publication, that the points above are addressed, involving significant changes in the TEKRABber program as well as the analysis pipeline, to prevent the identification of false positive and negative signals, that would severely affect the conclusions one can raise about the analysis.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s insightful recommendations and constructive feedback. Each specific point has been carefully addressed in detail in the public reviews section above.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review)

      Summary:

      The aim was to decipher the regulatory networks of KRAB-ZNFs and TEs that have changed during human brain evolution and in Alzheimer's disease.

      Strengths:

      This solid study presents a valuable analysis and successfully confirms previous assumptions, but also goes beyond the current state of the art.

      Weaknesses:

      The design of the analysis needs to be slightly modified and a more in-depth analysis of the positive correlation cases would be beneficial. Some of the conclusions need to be reinterpreted.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the thoughtful summary, positive evaluation of our study, and constructive feedback. We appreciate the recognition of the strengths in our analysis and the valuable suggestions for improving its design and interpretation. 

      We would like to briefly comment on the suggested modifications to the design here and will provide a detailed point-by-point review later with our revised manuscript. 

      The reviewer recommended considering a more recent timepoint, such as less than 25 million years ago (mya), to define the "evolutionary young group" of KRAB-ZNF genes and TEs when discussing the arms-race theory. This is indeed a valuable perspective, as the TE repressing functions by KRAB-ZNF proteins  may have evolved more recently than the split between Old World Monkeys (OWM) and New World Monkeys (NWM) at 44.2 mya we used. 

      Our rationale for selecting 44.2 mya is based on certain primate-specific TEs such as the Alu subfamilies, which emerged after the rise of Simiiformes and have been used in phylogenetic studies (Xing et al., 2007 and Williams et al., 2010). This timeframe allowed us to investigate the potential co-evolution of KRAB-ZNFs and TEs in species that emerged after the OWM-NWM split (e.g., humans, chimpanzees, bonobos, and macaques used for this study). However, focusing only on KRAB-ZNFs and TEs younger than 25 million years would limit the analysis to just 9 KRAB-ZNFs and 92 TEs expressed in our datasets. While we will not conduct a reanalysis using this more recent timepoint, we will integrate the recommendation into the discussion section of the revised manuscript. 

      Furthermore, we greatly appreciate the reviewer's detailed insights and suggestions for refining specific descriptions and interpretations in our manuscript. We will address these points in the revised version to ensure the content is presented with greater precision and clarity.

      Once again, we thank both reviewers for their valuable feedback, which provides significant input for strengthening our study.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      We thank the reviewer for the very insightful comments, which helped a lot in our interpretation and discussion of our results and in improving some of our statements.

      The present study seeks to uncover how the repression of transposable elements (TEs) by rapidly evolving KRAB-ZNF genes, which are known for their role in TE suppression, may influence human brain evolution and contribute to Alzheimer's disease (AD). Utilizing their previously developed tool, TEKRABber, the researchers analyze transcriptome datasets from the brains of four species of Old World Monkeys (OWM) alongside samples from healthy human individuals and AD patients.

      Through bipartite network analysis, they identify KRAB-ZNF/Alu-TE interactions as the most negatively correlated in the network, highlighting the repression of Alu elements by KRAB-ZNF proteins. In AD patient samples, they observe a reduction in a subnetwork comprising 21 interactions within an Alu TE module. These findings are consistent with earlier evidence that: (1) KRAB-ZNFs are involved in suppressing evolutionarily young Alu TEs; and (2) specific Alu elements have been reported to be deregulated in AD. The study also validates previous experimental ChIP-exo data on KRAB-ZNF proteins obtained in a different cell type (Imbeault et al., 2017).

      As a novely, the study identifies a human-specific amino acid variation in ZNF528, which directly contacts DNA nucleotides, showing signs of positive selection in humans and several human-specific TE interactions.

      Interestingly, in addition to the negative links, the researchers observed predominantly positive connections with other TEs, suggesting that while their approach is consistent with some previous observations, the authors conclude that it provides limited support for the 'genetic arms race' hypothesis.

      The reviewer is a specialist in TE and evolutionary research.

      Major issues:

      The study demonstrates the usefulness of the TEKRABber tool, which can support and successfully validate previous observations. However, there are several misconceptions and problems with the interpretation of the results.

      KRAB-ZNF proteins in repressing TEs in vertebrates  In the Abstract: "In vertebrates, some KRAB-ZNF proteins repress TEs, offering genomic protection."

      Although some KRAB-ZNF proteins exist in vertebrates, their TE-suppression role is not as prominent or specialized as it is in mammals, where it serves as a key defense mechanism against the mobilization of TEs.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s clarification regarding the role of KRAB-ZNF proteins in vertebrates. To improve accuracy and precision, we have revised the wording to specify that this mechanism is primarily observed in mammals rather than vertebrates.

      The definition of young and old

      The study considers the evolutionary age of young ({less than or equal to} 44.2 mya) and old(> 44.2 mya). This is the time of the Old World Monkey (OWM) and New World Monkey (NWM) split. Importantly, however, the KRAB-ZNF / KAP1 suppression system primarily suppresses evolutionarily younger TEs (< 25 MY old). These TEs are relatively new additions to the genome, i.e. they are specific to certain lineages (such as primates or hominins) and are more likely to be actively transcribed (and recognized as foreign by innate immunity) or have residual activity upon transposition. Examples include certain subfamilies of LINE-1, Alu (Y, S, less effective for J), SVA and younger human endogenous retroviruses (HERVs) such as HERV-K. The KRAB-ZNF / KAP1 system therefore focuses primarily on TEs that have evolved more recently in primates, in the last few million years (within the last 25 million years). Older TEs are controlled by broader epigenetic mechanisms such as DNA methylation, histone modifications, etc. Therefore, the age ({less than or equal to} 44.2 mya) is not suitable to define it as young.

      In this context, the specific TEs of the Simiiformes cannot be considered as 'recently evolved' (in the Abstract). The Simiiformes contain both OWM and NWM. Notably, the study includes four species, all of which belong to the OWMs.

      The 'genetic arms race' theory

      Unfortunately, the problematic definition of young and old could also explain why the authors conclude that their data only weakly support the 'genetic arms race' hypothesis.

      The KRAB-ZNF proteins evolve rapidly, similar to TEs, which raises the 'genetic arms race' hypothesis. This hypothesis refers to the constant evolutionary struggle between organisms and TEs. TEs constantly evolve to overcome host defences, while host genomes develop mechanisms to suppress these potentially harmful elements. Indeed, in mammals, an important example is the KRAB-ZNF/TE interaction. The KRAB-ZNF proteins rapidly evolve to target specific TEs, creating a 'genetic arms race' in which each side - TEs and the KRAB-ZNF/KAP1 (alias TRIM28) repressor complex - drives the evolution of the other in response to adaptive pressure. Importantly, the 'genetic arms race' hypothesis describes the evolutionary process that occurs between TE and host when the TE is deleterious. Again, this includes the young TEs (< 25 MY old) with residual transposition activity or those that actively transcribed and exacerbate cellular stress and inflammatory responses. Approximately 25 million years ago, the superfamilies Hominoidea (apes) and Cercopithecoidea (Old World monkeys, I.e. macaque) split.

      Just to clarify, our initial study aim was to examine whether TEs exhibit any evolutionary relationships with KRAB-ZNFs across the four studied species (human, chimpanzee, bonobo, and macaque). For investigating the arms-race hypothesis, we really appreciate the reviewer suggesting a more recent time point, such as less than 25 million years ago (mya), to define the "evolutionary young group" of TEs and KRAB-ZNF genes. This is indeed a valuable recommendation, as 25 mya marks the emergence of Hominoidea (Figure 2C in the manuscript), making it a meaningful reference point for studying recently evolved KRAB-ZNFs and TEs. However, restricting the analysis to elements younger than 25 mya would reduce the dataset to only 9 KRAB-ZNFs and 92 TEs. Nevertheless, we provide here our results for those elements in Table S7:

      We observed that among the correlations in the < 25 mya subset, negative correlations (7) outnumbered positive ones (2). However, these correlations were derived from only 3 out of 9 KRAB-ZNFs and 9 out of 92 TE subfamilies. Therefore, based on our data, while the < 25 mya group shows a higher proportion of negative correlations, the sample size is too limited to derive networks or draw robust conclusions in our analysis, especially when compared to our original evolutionary age threshold of 44.2 mya. For this reason, we chose not to reanalyze the data but rather to acknowledge that our current definition of “young” may not be optimal for testing the arms-race model in humans. While previous studies (Jacobs et al., 2014; Bruno et al., 2019; Zuo et al., 2023) have explored relevant KRAB-ZNF and TE interactions, our review of the KRAB-ZNFs and TEs highlighted in those works suggests that a specific focus on elements <25 mya has not been a primary emphasis. 

      "our findings only weakly support the arms-race hypothesis. Firstly, we noted that young TEs exhibit lower expression levels than old TEs (Figure 2D and 5B), which might not be expected if they had recently escaped repression". - This is a misinterpretation. These old TEs are no longer harmful. This is not the case of the 'genetic arms race'.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s comments, which have helped us refine our interpretation to prevent potential misunderstandings. Our initial expectation, based on the arms-race hypothesis, was that young TEs would exhibit higher expression levels due to a recent escape from repression, while young KRAB-ZNFs would show increased expression as a counter-adaptive response. However, our findings indicate that both young TEs and young KRAB-ZNFs exhibit lower expression levels. This observation does not align with the classical arms-race model, which typically predicts an ongoing cycle of adaptive upregulation. We rephrase the sentences in our discussion to hopefully make our idea more clear. In addition, we added the notion that older TEs might not be harmful anymore, which we agree with.

      "Additionally, some young TEs were also negatively correlated with old KRAB-ZNF genes, leading to weak assortativity regarding age inference, which would also not be in line with the arms-race idea."

      This is not a contradiction, as an old KRAB-ZNF gene could be 'reactivated' to protect against young TEs. (It might be cheaper for the host than developing a brand new KRAB-ZNF gene.

      We agree with the reviewer's point that older KRAB-ZNFs may be reactivated to suppress young TEs, potentially as a more cost-effective evolutionary strategy than the emergence of entirely new KRAB-ZNFs. We have incorporated this perspective into the revised manuscript to provide a more detailed discussion of our findings.

      TEs remain active

      In the abstract: "Notably, KRAB-ZNF genes evolve rapidly and exhibit diverse expression patterns in primate brains, where TEs remain active."

      This is not precise. TEs are not generally remain active in the brain. It is only the autonomous LINE-1 (young) and non-autonomous Alu (young) and SVA (young) elements that can be mobilized by LINE-1. In addition, the evolutionary young HERV-K is recognized as foreign and alerts the innate immune system (DOI: 10.1172/jci.insight.131093 ) and is a target of the KRAB-ZNF/KAP1 suppression system.

      In the abstract: "Evidence indicates that transposable elements (TEs) can contribute to the evolution of new traits, despite often being considered deleterious."

      Oversimplification: The harmful and repurposed TEs are washed together.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s detailed suggestions for improving the precision of our abstract. While we previously mentioned LINE-1 and Alu elements in the introduction, we now explicitly specify in the abstract that only certain TE subfamilies, such as autonomous LINE-1 and non-autonomous Alu and SVA elements, remain active in the primate brain. Additionally, we have refined the phrasing regarding the role of TEs in evolution to clearly distinguish between their deleterious effects and their potential for functional repurposing. These clarifications have been incorporated into the revised abstract to ensure greater accuracy and nuance.

      Positive links

      "The high number of positive correlations might be surprising, given that KRAB-ZNFs are considered to repress TEs."

      Based on the above, it is not surprising that negative associations are only found with young (< 25 my) TEs. In fact, the relationship between old KRAB-ZNF proteins and old (non-damaging) TEs could be neutral/positive. The case of ZNF528 could be a valuable example of this.

      We thank the reviewer for providing this plausible interpretation and added it to the manuscript.

      "276 TE:KRAB-ZNF with positive correlations in humans were negatively correlated in bonobos"  It would be important to characterise the positive correlations in more detail. Could it be that the old KRAB-ZNF proteins lost their ability to recruit KAP1/TRIM28? Demonstrate it.

      The strategy of developing sequence-specific DNA recognition domains that can specifically recognise TEs is expensive for the host. Recent studies suggest that when the TE is no longer harmful, these proteins/connections can be occasionally repurposed. The repurposed function would probably differ from the original suppressive function.

      In my opinion, the TEKRABber tool could be useful in identifying co-option events:

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion regarding the characterization of positive correlations. While it is possible that some old KRAB-ZNF proteins have lost their ability to recruit KAP1/TRIM28, we cannot conclude this definitively for all cases. To address this, we examined ChIP-exo data from Imbeault et al. (2017) (Accession: GSE78099) and analyzed the overlap of binding sites between KRAB-ZNFs, KAP1/TRIM28, and RepeatMasker-annotated TEs. Our results indicate that some old KRAB-ZNFs still exhibit binding overlap with KAP1 at TE regions, suggesting that their repressive function may be at least partially retained (Author response image 1).

      Author response image 1.<br /> Overlap of KAP1, Zinc finger proteins, and RepeatMasker annotation. Here we detect the overlap of ChIP-exo binding events using KAP1/TRIM28, with KRAB-ZNF genes (one at a time) and RepeatMasker annotation. (115 old and 58 young KRAB-ZNFs, Mann-Whitney, p<0.01).<br />

      Minor

      "Lead poisoning causes lead ions to compete with zinc ions in zinc finger proteins, affecting proteins such as DNMT1, which are related to the progression of AD (Ordemann and Austin 2016)."

      Not precise: While DNMT1 does contain zinc-binding domains, it is not categorized as a zinc finger protein.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insight regarding the classification of DNMT1. After careful consideration, we have removed this sentence from the introduction to maintain focus on KRAB zinc finger proteins.

      Definition of TEs

      "There were 324 KRAB-ZNFs and 895 TEs expressed in Primate Brain Data." Define it more precisely. It is not clear, what the authors mean by TEs: Are these TE families, subfamilies? Provide information on copy numbers of each in the analysed four species.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to clarify our definition of TEs. To improve precision, we have specified that the analysis was conducted at the subfamily level. Additionally, we have provided the copy numbers of TEs for the four analyzed species in Table S4.

      Occupancy of TEs in the genome

      "TEs comprise (i) one third to one half of the mammalian genome and are (ii) not randomly distributed..."

      (i) The most accepted number is 45%. However, some more recent reports estimate over 50%, thus the one third is an underestimation.

      (ii) Not randomly distributed among the mammalian species?

      (i) We thank the reviewer for pointing out that our statement about the abundance of TEs was outdated. We have updated the estimate to reflect that TEs can occupy more than half of the genome, based on recent publications.

      (ii) We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding the distribution of TEs. Although TEs are interspersed throughout the genome, their insertion sites are not entirely random, as they tend to exhibit preferences for certain genomic regions. To clarify this, we have revised the wording in the paragraph accordingly.

      We would like to express our sincere gratitude to both reviewers for their insightful feedback, which has been instrumental in enhancing the quality of our study.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This study investigates the sex determination mechanism in the clonal ant Ooceraea biroi, focusing on a candidate complementary sex determination (CSD) locus-one of the key mechanisms supporting haplodiploid sex determination in hymenopteran insects. Using whole genome sequencing, the authors analyze diploid females and the rarely occurring diploid males of O. biroi, identifying a 46 kb candidate region that is consistently heterozygous in females and predominantly homozygous in diploid males. This region shows elevated genetic diversity, as expected under balancing selection. The study also reports the presence of an lncRNA near this heterozygous region, which, though only distantly related in sequence, resembles the ANTSR lncRNA involved in female development in the Argentine ant, Linepithema humile (Pan et al. 2024). Together, these findings suggest a potentially conserved sex determination mechanism across ant species. However, while the analyses are well conducted and the paper is clearly written, the insights are largely incremental. The central conclusion - that the sex determination locus is conserved in ants - was already proposed and experimentally supported by Pan et al. (2024), who included O. biroi among the studied species and validated the locus's functional role in the Argentine ant. The present study thus largely reiterates existing findings without providing novel conceptual or experimental advances.

      Although it is true that Pan et al., 2024 demonstrated (in Figure 4 of their paper) that the synteny of the region flanking ANTSR is conserved across aculeate Hymenoptera (including O. biroi), Reviewer 1’s claim that that paper provides experimental support for the hypothesis that the sex determination locus is conserved in ants is inaccurate. Pan et al., 2024 only performed experimental work in a single ant species (Linepithema humile) and merely compared reference genomes of multiple species to show synteny of the region, rather than functionally mapping or characterizing these regions.

      Other comments:

      The mapping is based on a very small sample size: 19 females and 16 diploid males, and these all derive from a single clonal line. This implies a rather high probability for false-positive inference. In combination with the fact that only 11 out of the 16 genotyped males are actually homozygous at the candidate locus, I think a more careful interpretation regarding the role of the mapped region in sex determination would be appropriate. The main argument supporting the role of the candidate region in sex determination is based on the putative homology with the lncRNA involved in sex determination in the Argentine ant, but this argument was made in a previous study (as mentioned above).

      Our main argument supporting the role of the candidate region in sex determination is not based on putative homology with the lncRNA in L. humile. Instead, our main argument comes from our genetic mapping (in Fig. 2), and the elevated nucleotide diversity within the identified region (Fig. 4). Additionally, we highlight that multiple genes within our mapped region are homologous to those in mapped sex determining regions in both L. humile and Vollenhovia emeryi, possibly including the lncRNA.

      In response to the Reviewer’s assertion that the mapping is based on a small sample size from a single clonal line, we want to highlight that we used all diploid males available to us. Although the primary shortcoming of a small sample size is to increase the probability of a false negative, small sample sizes can also produce false positives. We used two approaches to explore the statistical robustness of our conclusions. First, we generated a null distribution by randomly shuffling sex labels within colonies and calculating the probability of observing our CSD index values by chance (shown in Fig. 2). Second, we directly tested the association between homozygosity and sex using Fisher’s Exact Test (shown in Supplementary Fig. S2). In both cases, the association of the candidate locus with sex was statistically significant after multiple-testing correction using the Benjamini-Hochberg False Discovery Rate. These approaches are clearly described in the “CSD Index Mapping” section of the Methods.

      We also note that, because complementary sex determination loci are expected to evolve under balancing selection, our finding that the mapped region exhibits a peak of nucleotide diversity lends orthogonal support to the notion that the mapped locus is indeed a complementary sex determination locus.

      The fourth paragraph of the results and the sixth paragraph of the discussion are devoted to explaining the possible reasons why only 11/16 genotyped males are homozygous in the mapped region. The revised manuscript will include an additional sentence (in what will be lines 384-388) in this paragraph that includes the possible explanation that this locus is, in fact, a false positive, while also emphasizing that we find this possibility to be unlikely given our multiple lines of evidence.

      In response to Reviewer 1’s suggestion that we carefully interpret the role of the mapped region in sex determination, we highlight our careful wording choices, nearly always referring to the mapped locus as a “candidate sex determination locus” in the title and throughout the manuscript. For consistency, the revised manuscript version will change the second results subheading from “The O. biroi CSD locus is homologous to another ant sex determination locus but not to honeybee csd” to “O. biroi’s candidate CSD locus is homologous to another ant sex determination locus but not to honeybee csd,” and will add the word “candidate” in what will be line 320 at the beginning of the Discussion, and will change “putative” to “candidate” in what will be line 426 at the end of the Discussion.

      In the abstract, it is stated that CSD loci have been mapped in honeybees and two ant species, but we know little about their evolutionary history. But CSD candidate loci were also mapped in a wasp with multi-locus CSD (study cited in the introduction). This wasp is also parthenogenetic via central fusion automixis and produces diploid males. This is a very similar situation to the present study and should be referenced and discussed accordingly, particularly since the authors make the interesting suggestion that their ant also has multi-locus CSD and neither the wasp nor the ant has tra homologs in the CSD candidate regions. Also, is there any homology to the CSD candidate regions in the wasp species and the studied ant?

      In response to Reviewer 1’s suggestion that we reference the (Matthey-Doret et al. 2019) study in the context of diploid males being produced via losses of heterozygosity during asexual reproduction, the revised manuscript will include (in what will be lines 123-126) the highlighted portion of the following sentence: “Therefore, if O. biroi uses CSD, diploid males might result from losses of heterozygosity at sex determination loci (Fig. 1C), similar to what is thought to occur in other asexual Hymenoptera that produce diploid males (Rabeling and Kronauer 2012; Matthey-Doret et al. 2019).”

      We note, however, that in their 2019 study, Matthey-Doret et al. did not directly test the hypothesis that diploid males result from losses of heterozygosity at CSD loci during asexual reproduction, because the diploid males they used for their mapping study came from inbred crosses in a sexual population of that species.

      We address this further below, but we want to emphasize that we do not intend to argue that O. biroi has multiple CSD loci. Instead, we suggest that additional, undetected CSD loci is one possible explanation for the absence of diploid males from any clonal line other than clonal line A. In response to Reviewer 1’s suggestion that we reference the (Matthey-Doret et al. 2019) study in the context of multilocus CSD, the revised manuscript version will include the following additional sentence in the fifth paragraph of the discussion (in what will be lines 372-374): “Multi-locus CSD has been suggested to limit the extent of diploid male production in asexual species under some circumstances (Vorburger 2013; Matthey-Doret et al. 2019).”

      Regarding Reviewer 2’s question about homology between the putative CSD loci from the (Matthey-Doret et al. 2019) study and O. biroi, we note that there is no homology. The revised manuscript version will have an additional Supplementary Table (which will be the new Supplementary Table S3) that will report the results of this homology search. The revised manuscript will also include the following additional sentence in the Results, in what will be lines 172-174: “We found no homology between the genes within the O. biroi CSD index peak and any of the genes within the putative L. fabarum CSD loci (Supplementary Table S3).”

      The authors used different clonal lines of O. biroi to investigate whether heterozygosity at the mapped CSD locus is required for female development in all clonal lines of O. biroi (L187-196). However, given the described parthenogenesis mechanism in this species conserves heterozygosity, additional females that are heterozygous are not very informative here. Indeed, one would need diploid males in these other clonal lines as well (but such males have not yet been found) to make any inference regarding this locus in other lines.

      We agree that a full mapping study including diploid males from all clonal lines would be preferable, but as stated earlier in that same paragraph, we have only found diploid males from clonal line A. We stand behind our modest claim that “Females from all six clonal lines were heterozygous at the CSD index peak, consistent with its putative role as a CSD locus in all O. biroi.” In the revised manuscript version, this sentence (in what will be lines 199-201) will be changed slightly in response to a reviewer comment below: “All females from all six clonal lines (including 26 diploid females from clonal line B) were heterozygous at the CSD index peak, consistent with its putative role as a CSD locus in all O. biroi.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Lacy et al. is well written, with a clear and compelling introduction that effectively conveys the significance of the study. The methods are appropriate and well-executed, and the results, both in the main text and supplementary materials, are presented in a clear and detailed manner. The authors interpret their findings with appropriate caution.

      This work makes a valuable contribution to our understanding of the evolution of complementary sex determination (CSD) in ants. In particular, it provides important evidence for the ancient origin of a non-coding locus implicated in sex determination, and shows that, remarkably, this sex locus is conserved even in an ant species with a non-canonical reproductive system that typically does not produce males. I found this to be an excellent and well-rounded study, carefully analyzed and well contextualized.

      That said, I do have a few minor comments, primarily concerning the discussion of the potential 'ghost' CSD locus. While the authors acknowledge (line 367) that they currently have no data to distinguish among the alternative hypotheses, I found the evidence for an additional CSD locus presented in the results (lines 261-302) somewhat limited and at times a bit difficult to follow. I wonder whether further clarification or supporting evidence could already be extracted from the existing data. Specifically:

      We agree with Reviewer 2 that the evidence for a second CSD locus is limited. In fact, we do not intend to advocate for there being a second locus, but we suggest that a second CSD locus is one possible explanation for the absence of diploid males outside of clonal line A. In our initial version, we intentionally conveyed this ambiguity by titling this section “O. biroi may have one or multiple sex determination loci.” However, we now see that this leads to undue emphasis on the possibility of a second locus. In the revised manuscript, we will split this into two separate sections: “Diploid male production differs across O. biroi clonal lines” and “O. biroi lacks a tra-containing CSD locus.”

      (1) Line 268: I doubt the relevance of comparing the proportion of diploid males among all males between lines A and B to infer the presence of additional CSD loci. Since the mechanisms producing these two types of males differ, it might be more appropriate to compare the proportion of diploid males among all diploid offspring. This ratio has been used in previous studies on CSD in Hymenoptera to estimate the number of sex loci (see, for example, Cook 1993, de Boer et al. 2008, 2012, Ma et al. 2013, and Chen et al., 2021). The exact method might not be applicable to clonal raider ants, but I think comparing the percentage of diploid males among the total number of (diploid) offspring produced between the two lineages might be a better argument for a difference in CSD loci number.

      We want to re-emphasize here that we do not wish to advocate for there being two CSD loci in O. biroi. Rather, we want to explain that this is one possible explanation for the apparent absence of diploid males outside of clonal line A. We hope that the modifications to the manuscript described in the previous response help to clarify this.

      Reviewer 2 is correct that comparing the number of diploid males to diploid females does not apply to clonal raider ants. This is because males are vanishingly rare among the vast numbers of females produced. We do not count how many females are produced in laboratory stock colonies, and males are sampled opportunistically. Therefore, we cannot report exact numbers. However, we will add the highlighted portion of the following sentence (in what will be lines 268-270) to the revised manuscript: “Despite the fact that we maintain more colonies of clonal line B than of clonal line A in the lab, all the diploid males we detected came from clonal line A.”

      (2) If line B indeed carries an additional CSD locus, one would expect that some females could be homozygous at the ANTSR locus but still viable, being heterozygous only at the other locus. Do the authors detect any females in line B that are homozygous at the ANTSR locus? If so, this would support the existence of an additional, functionally independent CSD locus.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion, and again we emphasize that we do not want to argue in favor of multiple CSD loci. We just want to introduce it as one possible explanation for the absence of diploid males outside of clonal line A.

      The 26 sequenced diploid females from clonal line B are all heterozygous at the mapped locus, and the revised manuscript will clarify this in what will be lines 199-201. Previously, only six of those diploid females were included in Supplementary Table S2, and that will be modified accordingly.

      (3) Line 281: The description of the two tra-containing CSD loci as "conserved" between Vollenhovia and the honey bee may be misleading. It suggests shared ancestry, whereas the honey bee csd gene is known to have arisen via a relatively recent gene duplication from fem/tra (10.1038/nature07052). It would be more accurate to refer to this similarity as a case of convergent evolution rather than conservation.

      In the sentence that Reviewer 2 refers to, we are representing the assertion made in the (Miyakawa and Mikheyev 2015) paper in which, regarding their mapping of a candidate CSD locus that contains two linked tra homologs, they write in the abstract: “these data support the prediction that the same CSD mechanism has indeed been conserved for over 100 million years.” In that same paper, Miyakawa and Mikheyev write in the discussion section: “As ants and bees diverged more than 100 million years ago, sex determination in honey bees and V. emeryi is probably homologous and has been conserved for at least this long.”

      As noted by Reviewer 2, this appears to conflict with a previously advanced hypothesis: that because fem and csd were found in Apis mellifera, Apis cerana, and Apis dorsata, but only fem was found in Mellipona compressipes, Bombus terrestris, and Nasonia vitripennis, that the csd gene evolved after the honeybee (Apis) lineage diverged from other bees (Hasselmann et al. 2008). However, it remains possible that the csd gene evolved after ants and bees diverged from N. vitripennis, but before the divergence of ants and bees, and then was subsequently lost in B. terrestris and M. compressipes. This view was previously put forward based on bioinformatic identification of putative orthologs of csd and fem in bumblebees and in ants [(Schmieder et al. 2012), see also (Privman et al. 2013)]. However, subsequent work disagreed and argued that the duplications of tra found in ants and in bumblebees represented convergent evolution rather than homology (Koch et al. 2014). Distinguishing between these possibilities will be aided by additional sex determination locus mapping studies and functional dissection of the underlying molecular mechanisms in diverse Aculeata.

      Distinguishing between these competing hypotheses is beyond the scope of our paper, but the revised manuscript will include additional text to incorporate some of this nuance. We will include these modified lines below (in what will be lines 287-295), with the additions highlighted:

      “A second QTL region identified in V. emeryi (V.emeryiCsdQTL1) contains two closely linked tra homologs, similar to the closely linked honeybee tra homologs, csd and fem (Miyakawa and Mikheyev 2015). This, along with the discovery of duplicated tra homologs that undergo concerted evolution in bumblebees and ants (Schmieder et al. 2012; Privman et al. 2013) has led to the hypothesis that the function of tra homologs as CSD loci is conserved with the csd-containing region of honeybees (Schmieder et al. 2012; Miyakawa and Mikheyev 2015). However, other work has suggested that tra duplications occurred independently in honeybees, bumblebees, and ants (Hasselmann et al. 2008; Koch et al. 2014), and it remains to be demonstrated that either of these tra homologs acts as a primary CSD signal in V. emeryi.”

      (4) Finally, since the authors successfully identified multiple alleles of the first CSD locus using previously sequenced haploid males, I wonder whether they also observed comparable allelic diversity at the candidate second CSD locus. This would provide useful supporting evidence for its functional relevance.

      As is already addressed in the final paragraph of the results and in Supplementary Fig. S4, there is no peak of nucleotide diversity in any of the regions homologous to V.emeryiQTL1, which is the tra-containing candidate sex determination locus (Miyakawa and Mikheyev 2015). In the revised manuscript, the relevant lines will be 307-310. We want to restate that we do not propose that there is a second candidate CSD locus in O. biroi, but we simply raise the possibility that multi-locus CSD *might* explain the absence of diploid males from clonal lines other than clonal line A (as one of several alternative possibilities).

      Overall, these are relatively minor points in the context of a strong manuscript, but I believe addressing them would improve the clarity and robustness of the authors' conclusions.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The sex determination mechanism governed by the complementary sex determination (CSD) locus is one of the mechanisms that support the haplodiploid sex determination system evolved in hymenopteran insects. While many ant species are believed to possess a CSD locus, it has only been specifically identified in two species. The authors analyzed diploid females and the rarely occurring diploid males of the clonal ant Ooceraea biroi and identified a 46 kb CSD candidate region that is consistently heterozygous in females and predominantly homozygous in males. This region was found to be homologous to the CSD locus reported in distantly related ants. In the Argentine ant, Linepithema humile, the CSD locus overlaps with an lncRNA (ANTSR) that is essential for female development and is associated with the heterozygous region (Pan et al. 2024). Similarly, an lncRNA is encoded near the heterozygous region within the CSD candidate region of O. biroi. Although this lncRNA shares low sequence similarity with ANTSR, its potential functional involvement in sex determination is suggested. Based on these findings, the authors propose that the heterozygous region and the adjacent lncRNA in O. biroi may trigger female development via a mechanism similar to that of L. humile. They further suggest that the molecular mechanisms of sex determination involving the CSD locus in ants have been highly conserved for approximately 112 million years. This study is one of the few to identify a CSD candidate region in ants and is particularly noteworthy as the first to do so in a parthenogenetic species.

      Strengths:

      (1) The CSD candidate region was found to be homologous to the CSD locus reported in distantly related ant species, enhancing the significance of the findings.

      (2) Identifying the CSD candidate region in a parthenogenetic species like O. biroi is a notable achievement and adds novelty to the research.

      Weaknesses

      (1) Functional validation of the lncRNA's role is lacking, and further investigation through knockout or knockdown experiments is necessary to confirm its involvement in sex determination.

      See response below.

      (2) The claim that the lncRNA is essential for female development appears to reiterate findings already proposed by Pan et al. (2024), which may reduce the novelty of the study.

      We do not claim that the lncRNA is essential for female development in O. biroi, but simply mention the possibility that, as in L. humile, it is somehow involved in sex determination. We do not have any functional evidence for this, so this is purely based on its genomic position immediately adjacent to our mapped candidate region. We agree with the reviewer that the study by Pan et al. (2024) decreases the novelty of our findings. Another way of looking at this is that our study supports and bolsters previous findings by partially replicating the results in a different species.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      L307-308 should state homozygous for either allele in THE MAJORITY of diploid males.

      This will be fixed in the revised manuscript, in what will be line 321.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The association between heterozygosity in the CSD candidate region and female development in O. biroi, along with the high sequence homology of this region to CSD loci identified in two distantly related ant species, is not sufficient to fully address the evolution of the CSD locus and the mechanisms of sex determination.

      Given that functional genetic tools, such as genome editing, have already been established in O. biroi, I strongly recommend that the authors investigate the role of the lncRNA through knockout or knockdown experiments and assess its impact on the sex-specific splicing pattern of the downstream tra gene.

      Although knockout experiments of the lncRNA would be illuminating, the primary signal of complementary sex determination is heterozygosity. As is clearly stated in our manuscript and that of (Pan et al. 2024), it does not appear to be heterozygosity within the lncRNA that induces female development, but rather heterozygosity in non-transcribed regions linked to the lncRNA. Therefore, future mechanistic studies of sex determination in O. biroi, L. humile, and other ants should explore how homozygosity or heterozygosity of this region impacts the sex determination cascade, rather than focusing (exclusively) on the lncRNA.

      With this in mind, we developed three sets of guide RNAs that cut only one allele within the mapped CSD locus, with the goal of producing deletions within the highly variable region within the mapped locus. This would lead to functional hemizygosity or homozygosity within this region, depending on how the cuts were repaired. We also developed several sets of PCR primers to assess the heterozygosity of the resultant animals. After injecting 1,162 eggs over several weeks and genotyping the hundreds of resultant animals with PCR, we confirmed that we could induce hemizygosity or homozygosity within this region, at least in ~1/20 of the injected embryos. Although it is possible to assess the sex-specificity of the splice isoform of tra as a proxy for sex determination phenotypes (as done by (Pan et al. 2024)), the ideal experiment would assess male phenotypic development at the pupal stage. Therefore, over several more weeks, we injected hundreds more eggs with these reagents and reared the injected embryos to the pupal stage. However, substantial mortality was observed, with only 12 injected eggs developing to the pupal stage. All of these were female, and none of them had been successfully mutated.

      In conclusion, we agree with the reviewer that functional experiments would be useful, and we made extensive attempts to conduct such experiments. However, these experiments turned out to be extremely challenging with the currently available protocols. Ultimately, we therefore decided to abandon these attempts.  

      We opted not to include these experiments in the paper itself because we cannot meaningfully interpret their results. However, we are pleased that, in this response letter, we can include a brief description for readers interested in attempting similar experiments.

      Since O. biroi reproduces parthenogenetically and most offspring develop into females, observing a shift from female- to male-specific splicing of tra upon early embryonic knockout of the lncRNA would provide much stronger evidence that this lncRNA is essential for female development. Without such functional validation, the authors' claim (lines 36-38) seems to reiterate findings already proposed by Pan et al. (2024) and, as such, lacks sufficient novelty.

      We have responded to the issue of “lack of novelty” above. But again, the actual CSD locus in both O. biroi and L. humile appears to be distinct from (but genetically linked to) the lncRNA, and we have no experimental evidence that the putative lncRNA in O. biroi is involved in sex determination at all. Because of this, and given the experimental challenges described above, we do not currently intend to pursue functional studies of the lncRNA.

      References

      Hasselmann M, Gempe T, Schiøtt M, Nunes-Silva CG, Otte M, Beye M. 2008. Evidence for the evolutionary nascence of a novel sex determination pathway in honeybees. Nature 454:519–522.

      Koch V, Nissen I, Schmitt BD, Beye M. 2014. Independent Evolutionary Origin of fem Paralogous Genes and Complementary Sex Determination in Hymenopteran Insects. PLOS ONE 9:e91883.

      Matthey-Doret C, van der Kooi CJ, Jeffries DL, Bast J, Dennis AB, Vorburger C, Schwander T. 2019. Mapping of multiple complementary sex determination loci in a parasitoid wasp. Genome Biology and Evolution 11:2954–2962.

      Miyakawa MO, Mikheyev AS. 2015. QTL mapping of sex determination loci supports an ancient pathway in ants and honey bees. PLOS Genetics 11:e1005656.

      Pan Q, Darras H, Keller L. 2024. LncRNA gene ANTSR coordinates complementary sex determination in the Argentine ant. Science Advances 10:eadp1532.

      Privman E, Wurm Y, Keller L. 2013. Duplication and concerted evolution in a master sex determiner under balancing selection. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 280:20122968.

      Rabeling C, Kronauer DJC. 2012. Thelytokous parthenogenesis in eusocial Hymenoptera. Annual Review of Entomology 58:273–292.

      Schmieder S, Colinet D, Poirié M. 2012. Tracing back the nascence of a new sex-determination pathway to the ancestor of bees and ants. Nature Communications 3:1–7.

      Vorburger C. 2013. Thelytoky and Sex Determination in the Hymenoptera: Mutual Constraints. Sexual Development 8:50–58.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Axon growth is of course essential to the formation of neural connections. Adhesion is generally needed to anchor and rectify such motion, but whether the tenacity or forces of adhesion must be optimal for maximal axon extension is unknown. Measurements and contributing factors are generally lacking and are pursued here with a laser-induced shock wave approach near the axon growth cone. The authors claim to make measurements of the pressure required to detach axons from low to high matrix density. The results seem to support the authors' conclusions, and the work - with further support - is likely to impact the field of cell adhesion. In particular, there could be some utility of the methods for the adhesion and those interested in aspects of axon growth.

      Strengths:

      A potential ability to control the pressure simply via proximity of the laser spot is convenient and perhaps reasonable. The 0 to 1 scale for matrix density is a good and appropriate measure for comparing adhesion and other results. The attention to detachment speed, time, F-actin, and adhesion protein mutant provides key supporting evidence. Lastly, the final figure of traction force microscopy with matrix varied on a gel is reasonable and more physiological because neural tissue is soft (cite PMID: 16923388); an optimum in Fig.6 also perhaps aligns with axon length results in Fig.5.

      We thank you for your many suggestions to improve the presentation to explain our experimental results obtained. We carefully reconsidered problems you pointed out and revised the manuscripts as follows.

      Weaknesses:

      The results seem incomplete and less than convincing. This is because the force calibration curve seems to be from a >10 yr old paper without any more recent checks or validating measurements.

      As the force calibration data, although we have indicated by the experimental system over 10 years ago, we have used the same system under appropriate maintenance. The system performance has been checked regularly and maintained. Therefore, the calibration data displayed is suitable even in the present. There is no problem with the calibration data.

      Secondly, the claimed effect of pressure on the detachment of the growth cone does not consider other effects such as cavitation or temperature, and certainly needs validation with additional methods that overcome such uncertainties.

      The authors need to check whether the laser perturbs the matrix, particularly local density. A relation between traction stresses of ~20-50 pN/um<sup>2</sup> in Fig.6 and the adhesion pressure of 3-5 kPa of FIg.3 needs to be carefully explained; the former units equate to 0.02-0.05 kPa, and would perhaps suggest cells cannot detach themselves and move forward.

      We have previously reported that a single pulse from a Ti:sapphire femtosecond laser amplifier can effectively generate shockwave and stress waves with minimal thermal effects. Notably, during this process, the temperature elevation at the laser focal point is sufficiently suppressed, allowing efficient force generation without causing significant heating in the surrounding area. By applying this method, we have confirmed that cell have any damage after the force loading. Therefore, this approach enables cell detachment while minimizing thermal and cavitation-induced damage to the cell. This clarification has been incorporated into the revised results section (lines 119-120). We agree with the reviewer that the presented data was insufficient for supporting the proposed model. To this end, we have performed additional experiments and analyses, which are included in the revised version of the manuscript. To examine the impact of femtosecond laser irradiation on laminin, fluorescently labeled laminin was coated onto glass-bottom dishes, and the fluorescent intensity was analyzed before and after the impulsive force loading. The result indicates that the fluorescent intensity at the laser focal point remained unaffected by laser irradiation. This finding suggests that axon detachment results from the dissociation between L1 and laminin rather than the detachment of laminin from the substrate. These data have been incorporated into Supplementary Fig. 1 and page 5 (lines 113-120). In addition, explanation of the relationship between the adhesion pressure and the traction stress has been specified in page 8 (lines 253-258).

      The authors need to measure axon length on gels (Fig.6) as more physiological because neural tissue is soft. The studies are also limited to a rudimentary in vitro model without clear relevance to in vivo.

      In response to the reviewer’s request, we measured the axon length on the polyacrylamide gel with stiffness comparable to brain tissue (0.3kPa). The axon length was consistently shorter on the gel on the glass under our experimental conditions, in agreement with previous findings (Abe at al., 2021). Furthermore, a biphasic relationship between axon outgrowth and laminin concentration was observed. These results suggest that the biphasic behavior of axon outgrowth identified in this study is likely to occur in vivo. We have updated the Fig. 6 and specified the result (lines 224-225) in revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The force calibration curve seems to be from a >10 yr old paper without any more recent checks or validating measurements - which are essential. Effects of cavitation and temperature must be checked, and validated with additional methods that overcome such uncertainties. The authors need to check whether the laser perturbs the matrix, particularly local density. A relation between traction stresses of ~20-50 pN/um2 in Fig.6 and the adhesion pressure of 3-5 kPa of FIg.3 needs to be carefully explained; the former units equate to 0.02-0.05 kPa, and would perhaps suggest cells cannot detach themselves and move forward. The authors need to measure axon length on gels (Fig.6) as more physiological because neural tissue is soft. The studies are also limited to a rudimentary in vitro model without clear relevance to in vivo.

      Thank you this reviewer for the recommendations on our manuscript. For this, we have answered above comments. Please find our response there.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors measure axon outgrowth rate, laminin adhesion strength, and actin rearward flow rate. They find that the axon outgrowth rate has a biphasic dependence on adhesion strength. In interpreting the results, they suggest that the results "imply that adhesion modulation is key to the regulation of axon guidance"; however, they measure elongation rate, not guidance.

      Strengths:

      The measurements of adhesion strength by laser-induced shock waves are reasonable as is the measurement of actin flow rates by speckle microscopy.

      Weaknesses:

      They only measure the length of the axons after 3 days and have no measurements of the actual rate of growth cone movements when they are moving. They do not measure the rate of actin growth at the leading edge to know its contribution to the extension rate. This is inadequate.

      These studies are unlikely to have an impact on the field because the measurement of axon growth rate at short times is missing.

      We thank the reviewer for understanding novelty of our study. We agree with the reviewer’s comment. Following the comment, we performed time-lapse imaging of growth cone movements and quantified the migration rate. Consistent with the length of axons, the migration rate did not exhibit a monotonic increase with increased L1CAM-laminin binding but rather displayed biphasic behavior, where excessive L1CAM-laminin binding led to a reduction in the migration rate. Notably, the biphasic migration behavior was abolished in the L1CAM knockdown neurons. We believe these results provide further support for our proposed model. This has been incorporated into new Fig.5 and page 7 (lines 209-218) of the revised manuscript. In addition, the experimental method has been added in page 13 (lines 385-391).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      This is a very weak paper because of the lack of relevant measurements to enable correlations between actual extension rate, traction force, and rates of speckle movement.

      Thank you this reviewer for the critical comment on our model. we performed time-lapse imaging of growth cone movements and quantified the migration rate. From this reviewer and reviewer #3 comments, we recognized the importance of prior studies that the measurement of adhesion strength in the growth cone, traction force, the correlation between retrograde flow and outgrowth, and biphasic dependence of substrate concentration of neurite outgrowth (Please also find our response to recommendations from reviewer #3).

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Yamada et al. build on classic and more recent studies (Chen et al., 2023; Lemmon et al., 1992; Nichol et al., 2016; Zheng et al., 1994; Schense and Hubbell, 2000) to better understand the relationship between substrate adhesion and neurite outgrowth.

      Strengths:

      The primary strength of the manuscript lies in developing a method for investigating the role of adhesion in axon outgrowth and traction force generation using a femtosecond laser technique. The most exciting finding is that both outgrowth and traction force generation have a biphasic relationship with laminin concentration.

      Weaknesses:

      The primary weaknesses are a lack of discussion of prior studies that have directly measured the strength of growth cone adhesions to the substrate (Zheng et al., 1994) and traction forces (Koch et al., 2012), the inverse correlation between retrograde flow rate and outgrowth (Nichol et al., 2016), and prior studies noting a biphasic effect of substrate concentration of neurite outgrowth (Schense and Hubbell, 2000).

      Overall, the claims and conclusions are well justified by the data. The main exception is that the data is more relevant to how the rate of neurite outgrowth is controlled rather than axonal guidance.

      This manuscript will help foster interest in the interrelationship between neurite outgrowth, traction forces, and substrate adhesion, and the use of a novel method to study this problem.

      We thank the reviewer for appropriate comments and recognition of the strength to our manuscript. Regarding to these comments, we recognized the importance of prior studies that the measurement of adhesion strength in the growth cone, traction force, the correlation between retrograde flow and outgrowth, and biphasic dependence of substrate concentration of neurite outgrowth. With respecting the prior studies, we revised the introduction (lines 38-44, 61-65) and discussion (lines 272-281) in the manuscript. The references suggested by the reviewer have been added (Ref. 17, 26, 27, 31, and 35) (see also below responses).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Overall, I found the experiments discussed in the manuscript to be excellent. My primary suggestion is to slightly expand the introduction and discussion to put this work in context better. Additionally, the writing is unclear in places and would be helped by a careful edit.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s constructive critiques and would like to thank him/her for the experimental suggestions, which we have taken into account in the revised version of the manuscript. We trust that the additional modification of the text will satisfactorily address the reviewer’s concerns.

      In more detail:

      The introduction is well-written but could be improved by discussing how these studies build earlier work. Through the 1980s and 90s, an important question was whether growth cone guidance occurred as the result of chemical cues that altered the activity of signaling pathways or differences in the adhesion between growth cones and substrates. While there was some clear evidence that growth cones were steered to more adhesive substrates (Hammarback and Letourneau, 1986), there were also important exceptions. For example, (Calof and Lander, 1991) examined the biophysical relationship between neuronal migration and substrate adhesion and found that laminin, which tends to support rapid migration and neurite outgrowth, tended to decrease adhesion.

      Thank you for critical comments to our manuscript. We have modified the introduction to discuss our understanding of the growth cone guidance, particularly regarding the role of neurite migration and substrate adhesion into introduction (line 38-40, 42-44) in revised manuscript.

      To better understand the relationship between substrate adhesion and outgrowth, Heidemann's group (Zheng et al., 1994) was, to the best of my knowledge, the first paper to directly measure the force required to detach growth cones from substrates; including laminin and L1. For DRG neurons, this was ~ 1000 - 3000 dynes (i.e., 10 to 30 nN) and they noted that traction force generation is 3 to 15 times less than the force needed to dislodge growth cones. Additionally, that manuscript goes on to suggest, "These data argue against the differential adhesion mechanism for growth cone guidance preferences in culture." With the rising development of powerful molecular genetic tools and a growing appreciation of the importance of signaling pathways in neurite outgrowth (Huber et al., 2003), the field as the whole has focused on the molecular aspects of growth cone guidance, leaving many aspects of the physical process of neurite outgrowth unanswered. The strength of this manuscript is that it develops a new method for measuring growth cone adhesion forces, which reassuringly generates similar results to classic studies. In turn, it combines this with molecular genetic analysis to determine the contribution L1-LN interaction makes to the overall adhesion strength.

      We will ensure that the manuscript explicitly acknowledges the significance of Zheng et al. (1994) in shaping the field and clarifies how our study expands upon these foundational findings. Following the reviewer’s suggestion we have added Zheng et al. (1994) in reference and modified discussion (line 272-281, Ref. 17) in revised manuscript.

      There are also a couple of other papers directly relevant to this work. In particular, (Koch et al., 2012) measured the traction forces generated by hippocampal neurons on polyacrylamide gels. They estimated it to be ~ 5 to 10 Pa. While the overall results are similar, in this manuscript, it is reported that the forces generated by hippocampal neurons are significantly higher, in the range of 25-75 Pa. I don't have an issue with this difference, but please look at the Koch paper and see if there is some technical reason for the different estimates of traction forces. Along these lines, please note the Young's modulus of the gels used in the experiments.

      As you mentioned, the traction force measured in our experiments is more than 5 times stronger than that reported by Koch et al., While the exact reason remains unclear, difference in gel-coating may have influenced the result. In the study by Koch et al., pre-coating was performed using Cell-Tak before laminin coating. in contrast, our study used poly-lysin for pre-coating. This methodological difference may have affected the measurement of traction force. However, at least, our experiments have consistently yielded reproducible results.

      (Nichol et al., 2016) nicely shows an inverse relationship between RF rate and LN density at low concentrations. While the results reported here are similar, a strength of this paper is that it extends the work to higher LN concentrations.

      Thank you for pointing out the relevance of Nichol et al., 2016 to our study. We agree that their study provides important insights into the relationship between RF rate and LN density at low concentrations. The novelty our study lies not only in extending the analysis to higher LN concentrations, but also performed analysis that include adhesion strength, traction force, and migration rate in the growth cone. We have included this discussion (line 259-261, Ref. 26) in revised manuscript.

      My understanding is that the biphasic effect of LN in neurite outgrowth was previously established. For example, Buetter and Pittman, 1991 note a biphasic effect of LN conc on some parameters of neurite outgrowth, such as RMS, a measure of growth cone velocity, but not others, such as total neurite length. Likewise, (Schense and Hubbell, 2000) noted a biphasic effect of RGB peptides on outgrowth. In light of this, it would seem the main contribution of this paper is the finding that traction force generation has a bi-phasic relationship with LN concentration.

      Thank you for your thoughtful comment. We agree that the main contribution of this study is demonstrating that the biphasic behavior of axon migration arises from the biphasic dependence of the traction force on laminin concentration. We have included this discussion (line 272-281, Ref. 31) in the revised manuscript.

      Please appreciate that I'm not asking the authors to copy-paste the text above into the manuscript. Instead, the references provide a starting point for better explaining the novel contributions here. The interaction of adhesions, traction force generation, the rate of neurite outgrowth, and biophysics of growth cone guidance is a classic problem in neuronal mechanics but is far from solved. My hope is that this manuscript might inspire more interest in this problem.

      Thank you for your thoughtful feedback and for highlighting the importance of better contextualizing our novel contributions within the broader field of neuronal mechanics. We appreciate your emphasis on the classic yet unresolved nature of the interactions between adhesions, traction force generation, axon outgrowth rate, and the biophysics of growth cone guidance.

      We hope these revisions help strengthen the manuscript’s impact and inspire further investigation into this important problem. We appreciate your insightful comments and the opportunity to improve our work.

      The text would be improved with a careful copy edit, for example:

      The last sentence of the introduction currently reads, "We suggested mechanism of the axon outgrowth which depends on the density of laminin on the substrate, revealing L1CAM-laminin binding as a mechanism for the regulation of axon outgrowth." which is challenging to understand.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment pointing out the lack of clarity in the final sentence of the introduction. To improve readability and clarity, we have revised the sentence as follows:

      “In this study, we suggested mechanism of the axon outgrowth that depends on the density of laminin on the substrate, i.e. the L1CAM-laminin binding is key to the regulation of axon outgrowth..” We believe this revised version better conveys our main finding in a more concise and comprehensible manner.

      Line 224 needs to be F-actin and the next sentence is difficult to understand.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have corrected "F-action" to "F-actin" to ensure accuracy (line 256). Additionally, we have revised the following sentence to improve clarity (line 256-258).

      Line 232 instead of "traction force slows", did you mean the rate of retrograde flow slows?

      Thank you for pointing this out. We mean to refer to the rate of retrograde flow, not the traction force itself. We have revised the wording accordingly to avoid confusion (line 266).

      Line 242, shear-stress instead of share-stress.

      We have corrected the typo into "shear-stress" (line 282).

      Lines 255, 267, and the abstract. The paper doesn't directly address axonal guidance. It would be more accurate to replace axonal guidance with neurite outgrowth.

      Thank you for your insightful comment. We agree that the term "neurite outgrowth" more accurately reflects the scope of our study, as we do not directly examine the mechanisms of axonal guidance. Accordingly, we have revised the text in Lines 273, 275, and the abstract to replace "axonal guidance" with "neurite outgrowth" to better align with the presented data and experimental focus.

      Line 362, perhaps reference (Minegishi et al., 2021) here as it provides a nice explanation of the technique.

      Thank you for the helpful suggestion. We have now added a reference to Minegishi et al., 2021 (line 416, Ref.35) in revised manuscript, as it indeed provides a clear explanation of the method.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, Behruznia and colleagues use long-read sequencing data for 339 strains of the Mycobacterium tuberculosis complex to study genome evolution in this clonal bacterial pathogen. They use both a "classical" pangenome approach that looks at the presence and absence of genes, and a pangenome graph based on whole genomes in order to investigate structural variants in non-coding regions. The comparison of the two approaches is informative and shows that much is missed when focussing only on genes. The two main biological results of the study are that 1) the MTBC has a small pangenome with few accessory genes, and that 2) pangenome evolution is driven by genome reduction. In the revised article, the description of the data set and the methods is much improved, and the comparison of the two pangenome approaches is more consistent. I still think, however, that the discussion of genome reduction suffers from a basic flaw, namely the failure to distinguish clearly between orthologs and homologs/paralogs.

      Strengths:

      The authors put together the so-far largest data set of long-read assemblies representing most lineages of the Mycobacterium tuberculosis context, and covering a large geographic area. They sequenced and assembled genomes for strains of M. pinnipedi, L9, and La2, for which no high-quality assemblies were available previously. State-of-the-art methods are used to analyze gene presence-absence polymorphisms (Panaroo) and to construct a pangenome graph (PanGraph). Additional analysis steps are performed to address known problems with misannotated or misassembled genes.

      Weaknesses:

      The revised manuscript has gained much clarity and consistency. One previous criticism, however, has in my opinion not been properly addressed. I think the problem boils down to not clearly distinguishing between orthologs and paralogs/homologs. As this problem affects a main conclusion - the prevalence of deletions over insertions in the MTBC - it should be addressed, if not through additional analyses, then at least in the discussion.

      Insertions and deletions are now distinguished in the following way: "Accessory regions were further classified as a deletion if present in over 50% of the 192 sub-lineages or an insertion/duplication if present in less than 50% of sub-lineages." The outcome of this classification is suspicious: not a single accessory region was classified as an insertion/duplication. As a check of sanity, I'd expect at least some insertions of IS6110 to show up, which has produced lineage- or sublineage-specific insertions (Roychowdhury et al. 2015, Shitikov et al. 2019). Why, for example, wouldn't IS6110 insertions in the single L8 strain show up here?

      In a fully clonal organism, any insertion/duplication will be an insertion/duplication of an existing sequence, and thus produce a paralog. If I'm correctly understanding your methods section, paralogs are systematically excluded in the pangraph analysis. Genomic blocks are summarized at the sublineage levels as follows (l.184 ): "The DNA sequences from genomic blocks present in at least one sub-lineage but completely absent in others were extracted to look for long-term evolution patterns in the pangenome." I presume this is done using blastn, as in other steps of the analysis.

      So a sublineage-specific copy of IS6110 would be excluded here, because IS6110 is present somewhere in the genome in all sublineages. However, the appropriate category of comparison, at least for the discussion of genome reduction, is orthology rather than homology: is the same, orthologous copy of IS6110, at the same position in the genome, present or absent in other sublineages? The same considerations apply to potential sublineage-specific duplicates of PE, PPE, and Esx genes. These gene families play important roles in host-pathogen interactions, so I'd argue that the neglect of paralogs is not a finicky detail, but could be of broader biological relevance.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors attempted to investigate the pangenome of MTBC by using a selection of state-of-the-art bioinformatic tools to analyse 324 complete and 11 new genomes representing all known lineages and sublineages. The aim of their work was to describe the total diversity of the MTBC and to investigate the driving evolutionary force. By using long read and hybrid approaches for genome assembly, an important attempt was made to understand why the MTBC pangenome size was reported to vary in size by previous reports. This study provides strong evidence that the MTBC pangenome is closed and that genome reduction is the main driver of this species evolution.

      Strengths:

      A stand-out feature of this work is the inclusion of non-coding regions as opposed to only coding regions which was a focus of previous papers and analyses which investigated the MTBC pangenome. A unique feature of this work is that it highlights sublineage-specific regions of difference (RDs) that was previously unknown. Another major strength is the utilisation of long-read whole genomes sequences, in combination with short-read sequences when available. It is known that using only short reads for genome assembly has several pitfalls. The parallel approach of utilizing both Panaroo and Pangraph for pangenomic reconstruction illuminated limitations of both tools while highlighting genomic features identified by both. This is important for any future work and perhaps alludes to the need for more MTBC-specific tools to be developed. Lastly, ample statistical support in the form of Heaps law and genome fluidity calculations for each pangenome to demonstrate that they are indeed closed.

      Weaknesses:

      There are no major weaknesses in the revised version of this manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      l. 27: "lineage-specific and -independent deletions": it is still not clear to me what a lineage-independent, or convergent, deletion is supposed to be. TBD1, for instance, is not lineage-specific, but it is also not convergent: it occurred once in the common ancestor of lineages 1, 2, and 3, while convergence implies multiple parallel occurrences.

      We have changed this and in other places to more evolutionary terms, such as divergent (single event) and convergent (multiple events), or explain exactly what is meant where needed.

      l. 118: "where relevant", what does that mean?

      This was superfluous to the description and so is now removed.

      l. 178ff.: It is not clear to me what issue is addressed by this correction of the pangenome graph. Also here there seems to be some confusion regarding orthologs and paralogs. A gene or IS copy can be present at one locus but absent at another, which is not a mistake of Pangraph that would require correction. It's rather the notion of "truly absent region" which is ambiguous.

      We have changed the text to be more specific on the utility of this step. Since it is known that Panaroo mislabels some genes as being absent due to over splitting (see Ceres et al 2022 and our reclassification earlier in the paper), we wanted to see if the same occurred in Pangraph. We have modified the methods text to be more specific (line 181) and in the results included the percentage of total genes/regions affected by this correction.

      In relation to copy number, Pangraph is not syntenic in its approach; if a region is present anywhere it is labelled as present in the genome. Pangraph will look for multiple copies of that region (e.g. an IS element) but indeed we did not look for specific syntenic changes across the genomes. This would be a great analysis and something we will consider in the future; we have indicated such in the discussion (line 454).

      l. 305: "mislabelled as absent": see above, is this really 'mislabelled'?

      See answer to question above

      l. 372: "using the approach": something missing here.

      This was superfluous to the description and so is now removed.

      l. 381: the "additional analysis of paralogous blocks" (l. 381) seems to suffer from the same confusion of ortho- and paralogy described above: no new sub-lineage-specific accessory regions are found presumably because the analysis did consider any copy rather than orthologous copies.

      Paralogous copies were looked for by Pangraph, and we did not find any sub-lineage where all members had additional copies compared to other sub-lineages. Indeed, single genomes could have these, and shorter timescales could see a lot of such insertions, but we looked at longer-scale (all genomes within a sub-lineage) patterns and did not find these. These limitations are already outlined in the discussion.

      l. 415: see above. There is no diagnosis of a problem that would motivate a "correction". That's different from the correction of the Panaroo results, where fragmented annotations have been shown to be a problem.

      Of interest, the refining of regions did re-label multiple regions as being core when Pangraph labelled it as absent from some genomes was at about the same rate as the correction to Pangraph (2% of genes/regions). This indicates there is a stringency issue with pangraph where blocks are mislabelled as absent. The underlying reason or this is not clear but the correction is evidently required in this version of Pangraph.

      l. 430ff.: The issue of paralogy and that the "same" gene or region is defined in terms of homology rather than orthology should be addressed here. For me the given evidence does not support the claim that deletion is driving molecular evolution in the MTBC.

      As outlined above, indeed paralogy may be driving some elements of the overall evolutionary patterns; our analysis just did not find this. Panaroo without merged paralogs did not find paralogous genes as a main differentiating factor for any sub-lineage. Pangraph also did not find multiple copies of blocks present in all genomes in a sub-lineage. As outlined above, indeed single genomes show such patterns but we did not include single genome analyses here, and outline that as a next steps in the discussion. We have also linked to a recent pangenome paper that showed duplication is present in the pangenome of Mtbc, although not related to any specific lineage (Discussion line 485).

      l. 443 ff: "lineage-independent deletions (convergent evolution)": see above, I still think this terminology is unclear

      This has now been made clearer to be specifically about convergent and divergent evolutionary patterns.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors investigate mechanisms of acquired resistance (AR) to KRAS-G12C inhibitors (sotorasib) in NSCLC, proposing that resistance arises from signaling rewiring rather than additional mutations.

      Strengths:

      Using a panel of AR models - including cell lines, PDXs, CDXs, and PDXOs - they report activation of KRAS and PI3K/AKT/mTOR pathways, with elevated PI3K levels. Pharmacologic inhibition or CRISPR-Cas9 knockout of PI3K partially restores sotorasib sensitivity, and p-4EBP1 upregulation is implicated as an additional contributor, with dual mTORC1/2 inhibition more effective than mTORC1 inhibition alone.

      Weaknesses:

      While the study addresses an important clinical question, it is limited by several weaknesses in experimental rigor, data interpretation, and presentation. The mechanistic findings are not entirely novel, since the role of PI3K-AKT-mTOR signaling in therapeutic resistance is already well-established in the literature. Rather than uncovering new resistance mechanisms, the study largely confirms known pathways. Several key conclusions are not supported by the data, and critical alternative explanations - such as additional mutations or increased KRAS expression - are not thoroughly investigated or ruled out. Furthermore, while the authors use CRISPR-Cas9 to knock out PI3K and 4E-BP1 in H23-AR and H358-AR cells to restore sotorasib sensitivity, they do not perform reconstitution experiments to confirm that re-expressing PI3K or 4E-BP1 reverses the sensitization. This prevents full characterization of PI3K and p-4EBP1 upregulation as contributors to resistance. The manuscript also has several errors, poor figure quality, and a lack of proper quantification. Additional experimental validation, data improvement, and text revisions are required.

      Acquired resistance to KRAS<sup>G12C</sup> inhibitors such as sotorasib or adagrasib remains a significant clinical challenge. Therefore, the identification of mechanisms of acquired resistance, along with the development of alternative therapeutic strategies, including combination therapies with KRAS inhibitors, represents an urgent unmet clinical need. The emergence of secondary KRAS mutations or new mutations in other oncogenic drivers has been observed as a primary cause of acquired resistance in a fraction of patients. No identifiable mutations were detected in more than half of the tumors from patients who developed acquired resistance after treatment with sotorasib or adagrasib.

      Using a discovery-based approach that integrated global proteomic and phosphoproteomic analyses in the TC303AR and TC314AR PDX models, we identified distinct protein signatures associated with KRAS reactivation, upregulation of mTORC1 signaling, and activation of the PI3K/AKT/mTOR pathway. These findings prompted further investigation into these mechanisms of resistance and evaluation of novel therapeutic combinations to overcome resistance. Notably, the combination of sotorasib with copanlisib (a PI3K inhibitor), or the combination of sotorasib with AZD8055 or sapanisertib (mTORC1/2 dual inhibitors) demonstrated strong potential for future clinical use. These regimens effectively restored sotorasib sensitivity in both in vitro and in vivo models and produced robust, synergistic antitumor effects across various acquired resistance models.

      CRISPR-Cas9-mediated PI3K and 4E-BP1 knockout clones were generated in more than one resistant cell line that expressed a robust level of the knockout target, and multiple independent clones in each cell line were evaluated with and without gene disruption. Given the thorough nature of this analysis, additional reconstitution experiments were deemed unnecessary, as they would not yield further insight.

      Whole exome sequencing was performed on resistant cells or PDX models to confirm retention of the KRAS<sup>G12C</sup> mutation and to identify secondary KRAS mutations, none of which were found. We acknowledge that additional resistance mechanisms may be involved. These will be the focus of future investigations.

      The revised manuscript will feature improved figure quality, complete and clarified figure legends, and corrected textual errors to enhance overall clarity and presentation.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors focus on the identification of the mechanisms involved in the acquired resistance to Sotorasib in non-small lung KRASG12C mutant cells. To perform this study, the authors generate different clones of cell lines, cell-derived xenografts, patient-derived xenograft organoids, and patient-derived xenografts. In all these models, the authors generate resistant forms (i.e., resistant cell lines PDXs and organoids) and the genetic and molecular changes were characterised using whole-exome sequencing, proteomics, and phospho-proteomics. This analysis led to the identification of an important role of the PI3K/AKT/mTORC1/2 signalling network in the acquisition of resistance in several of the models tested. Molecular characterisation identified changes in the expression of some of the proteins in this network as key changes for the acquisition of resistance, and in particular, the authors show that changes in 4E-BP1 are common to some of the cells downstream of PI3K. Using pharmacological testing, they show that different drugs targeting PI3K, AKT, and MTORC1/2 sensitise some of the resistant models to Sotorasib. The analyses showed that the PI3K inhibitor copanlisib has an effect in NSCLC cells that, in some cases, seems to be synergistic with Sotorasib. Based on the work performed, the authors conclude that the PI3K/mTORC1/2 mediated 4E-BP1 phosphorylation is one of the mechanisms associated with the acquisition of resistance to Sotorasib and that targeting this signalling module could result in effective treatments for NSCLC patients.

      The work as presented in the current manuscript is very interesting, provides cell models that benefit the community, and can be used to expand our knowledge of the mechanism of resistance to KRAS targeting therapies. Overall, the techniques and methodology seem to be performed in agreement with standard practice, and the results support most of the conclusions made by the authors. However, there are some points that, if addressed, would increase the value and relevance of the findings and further extend the impact of this work. Some of the recommendations for changes relate to the way things are explained and presented, which need some work. Other changes might require the performance of additional experiments or reanalysis of the existing data.

      Strengths:

      (1) One of the stronger contributions of this article is the different models used to study the acquisition of resistance to Sotorasib. The resistant cell lines, PDXs and PDXOs, and the fact that the authors have different clones for each, made this collection especially relevant, as they seem to show different mechanisms that the cells used to become resistant to Sotorasib. Although logically, the authors focus on one of these mechanisms, the differential responses of the different clones and models to the treatments used in this work show that some of the clones used additional mechanisms of resistance that can be explored in other studies. Importantly, as they use in vitro and in vivo models, the results also consider the tumour microenvironment and other factors in the response to the treatments.

      (2) Another strength is the molecular characterisation of the different Sotorasib-resistant tumour cells by WES, which shows that these cells do not seem to acquire secondary mutations.

      (3) The use of MS-based proteomics also identifies proteome signatures that are associated with the acquisition of resistance, including PI3K/mTORC1/2. The combination of proteomics and phospho-proteomics results should allow the identification of several mechanisms that are deregulated in Sotorasib-resistant cells.

      (4) The results show a strong response of the NSCLC cells and PDXs to copanlisib, a drug for which there is limited information in this cancer type.

      (5) The way they develop the PDX-resistant and the PDXO seems to be appropriate.

      Weaknesses:

      In general, the data is of good quality, but due to the sheer amount of data included and the way it is presented and discussed, several of the claims or conclusions are not clear.

      (1) The abstract is rather long and gives details that are not usually included in one. This makes it very complicated to identify the most relevant findings of the work. The use of acronyms PDX, PDXO, and CDX without defining them makes it complicated for the non-specialist to know what the models are. Rewriting and reorganisation of the abstract would benefit the manuscript.

      We will revise the abstract to ensure that the key findings and overall message are clearly communicated and easily understood by readers.

      2) Expression, presentation, and grammar should be reviewed in all sections of the manuscript.

      Will be done accordingly in the revised version

      (3) In the different parts of the result section where the models shown in Figure 2 are described the authors indicate "Whole-exome sequencing (WES) confirmed that XXX model retained the KRASG12C mutation with no additional KRAS mutations detected" however, it is not indicated where this data is shown and in not all the cases there is explanation to other possible modifications that might relate to mechanisms of resistance. This information should be included in the manuscript, and the WES made publicly available.

      WES was done for KRAS to identify secondary mutations in the KRAS as well as to verify the retention of the KRAS<sup>G12C</sup> mutation in these AR models. WES data will be provided as supplements

      (4) The way the proteomics analysis of the TC303 and TC314 parental and resistant PDX is described in the text is confusing. The addition of an experimental layout figure would facilitate the understanding. As it is written, it is not obvious that the parental PDX were also analysed. For instance, the authors say, "The global and phosphoproteomic analyses identified over 8,000 and 4,000 gene protein products (GPPs), respectively". Is this comparing only resistant cells, or from the comparison of the parental and resistant pairs? And where are these numbers presented in the figures? Also, there is information that seems more adequate for the materials and methods sections, i.e., "Samples were analyzed using label-free nanoscale liquid chromatography coupled with tandem mass spectrometry (nanoLC-MS/MS) on a Thermo Fusion Mass Spectrometer. The resulting data were processed and quantified using the Proteome Discoverer 2.5 interface with the Mascot search engine, referencing the NCBI RefSeq protein database (Saltzman, Ruprecht). Two-component analysis is better named principal component analysis."

      The texts will be revised accordingly

      (5) While the presentation of the proteomics data could be done in different ways, the way the data is presented in Figure 3 does not allow the reader to get an idea of many of the findings from this experiment. Although it is indicated that a table with the data will be made available, this should be central to the way the data is presented and explained. A table (ie, Excel doc) where the raw data and all the analysis are presented should be included and referenced. Additionally, heat maps for the whole proteomes identified should be included. In the text, it is said, "Global proteomic heatmap analysis revealed unique protein profiles in TC303AR and TC314AR PDXs compared to their sensitive counterparts (Figure 3C)." However, this figure only shows the histogram of the differentially regulated cells. Inclusion of the histogram showing all the cells is necessary, and it might be informative to include the histogram comparing the two isogenic pairs, which could identify common mechanisms and differences between both sets. In Figure 3C, the protein names should be readable, or a reference to tables where the proteins are listed should be included.

      The raw data associated with the proteomics and global proteomics will be added as supplements.

      (6) In Figure 3, the pathway enrichment tool and GO used should be mentioned in the text. The tables with all significant tables should also be provided. The proteomics data seems to convincingly identify mTOR as one of the pathways deregulated in resistant cells, but there is little explanation of what is considered a significant FDR value and if there are other pathways or networks that are also modified, which might not be common to both isogenic models. In MS-based Phosphoproteome could help with the identification of differentially regulated pathways, but it is not really presented in the current manuscript. Most of the analysis of phospho-proteomics comes from the RPPA analysis, which is targeted proteomics. With the way the data is presented, the authors show evidence for a role of mTOR in the acquisition of resistance, but unfortunately, they do not discuss or allow the reader to explore if other pathways might also contribute to this change.

      The authors agree that other pathways may be involved, and this will be the subject of future studies. The raw data will be added as supplements.

      (7) Where is the proteomics data going to be deposited, and will it be made public to comply with FAIR principles?

      will be uploaded according to the journal guidelines

      (8) The authors claim that the resistance shown for H23AR and H353AR cells is due to reactivation of KRAS signalling. This is done by looking to phosphorylation of ERK as a surrogate, as they claim, "KRAS inhibition is commonly assessed by evaluating the inhibition of ERK phosphorylation (p-ERK)". While this might be true in many cases, the data presented does not demonstrate that the increase in p-ERK is due to reactivation of KRAS. To make this claim, the authors should measure activation of KRAS (and possibly H- and NRAS) using GST-pull down or an image-based method.

      We agree that KRAS activation can be assessed through various methods. In this manuscript, which primarily focuses on mechanisms of resistance, pathway analysis revealed upregulation of KRAS signaling. This finding correlated with the incomplete inhibition of p-ERK by sotorasib in resistant cells. Notably, p-ERK status is widely recognized and routinely used as a surrogate marker for KRAS pathway activation.

      (9) The experiments in Figure 4 are very confusing, and some controls are missing. There is no blot where they show the effect of Sotorasib treatment in H23 and H358 parental cells. Is the increase shown in resistant cells shown in parental or is it exclusive for resistant cells only (and therefore acquired)? Experiment 4B should include this control. What is clear is that there is an increase in the expression of AKT and PI3K.

      H23 and H358 cells are highly sensitive to sotorasib, as demonstrated by the cell viability assays presented in Figure 2. As shown in Figure 3—figure supplement 3, sotorasib treatment led to complete inhibition of p-ERK in these parental cell lines. In contrast, p-ERK inhibition was incomplete in the resistant H23AR and H358AR cells. Moreover, these AR cells were continuously cultured under sotorasib pressure to maintain resistance.

      (10) The main point here is whether this is acquired resistance or the sensitivity to the drug is already there, and there was no need to do an omics experiment to find this. In some cases, it seems that the single treatment with PI3K inhibitors is as effective as Sotorasib treatment, promoting the death of the parental cells. This is in line with previous data in H23 and H353 that show sensitivity to PI3K inhibition ( i.e., H358 10.1016/j.jtcvs.2005.06.051 ; 10.1016/j.jtcvs.2005.06.051H23 10.20892/j.issn.2095-3941.2018.0361). The data is clear, especially for copanlisib, but would it be the case that this treatment could be used for the treatment of NSCLC alone or directly in combination with Sotorasib and prevent resistance? The results shown in Figure 4C strongly support that a single treatment might be effective in cases that do not respond to Sotorasib. The data in figure 4D-F (please correct typo "inhibition" in labels) seem to support that PI3K treatment of parental cells is as effective as in the resistant cells.

      We agree. Based on our in vitro (Figure 4) and in vivo (Figure 7) data, copanlisib was able to overcome sotorasib resistance, demonstrating either synergistic or additive effects depending on the specific model. These findings support the potential of combining PI3K inhibition with KRAS<sup>G12C</sup> inhibition as a promising strategy to address acquired resistance.

      (11) The experiments presented in Figure 7 show synergy between Sotorasib and copanlisib treatment in some of the resistant cells. But in Figure 7G, the single treatment of H23AR is as effective as the combination. Did the authors check the effect of this drug on the parental cells? As they do not include this control, it is not possible to know if this is acquired sensitivity to PI3K inhibition or if the parental cells were already sensitive (as indicated by the Figure 4 results).

      Both H23 and H23AR cells showed high sensitivity to copanlisib, as shown in Figure 4. Combination index analysis for the copanlisib + sotorasib treatment (Figure 7A) revealed synergistic effects on cell viability at specific concentrations. However, in the in vivo experiment (Figure 7G), we did not observe a clear synergistic effect of the combination treatment against H23AR xenografts. This may be attributed to the dose of copanlisib used, which was potentially sufficient on its own to produce a strong antitumor response, thereby masking any additional benefit from the combination.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this detailed study, Cohen and Ben-Shaul characterized the AOB cell responses to various conspecific urine samples in female mice across the estrous cycle. The authors found that AOB cell responses vary with the strains and sexes of the samples. Between estrous and non-estrous females, no clear or consistent difference in responses was found. The cell response patterns, as measured by the distance between pairs of stimuli, are largely stable. When some changes do occur, they are not consistent across strains or male status. The authors concluded that AOB detects the signals without interpreting them. Overall, this study will provide useful information for scientists in the field of olfaction.

      Strengths:

      The study uses electrophysiological recording to characterize the responses of AOB cells to various urines in female mice. AOB recording is not trivial as it requires activation of VNO pump. The team uses a unique preparation to activate the VNO pump with electric stimulation, allowing them to record AOB cell responses to urines in anesthetized animals. The study comprehensively described the AOB cell responses to social stimuli and how the responses vary (or not) with features of the urine source and the reproductive state of the recording females. The dataset could be a valuable resource for scientists in the field of olfaction.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The figures could be better labeled.

      We revised all figures (except the model figure, Fig. 8), and among other improvements (many of which were suggested by the reviewers in other comments), added more labelling and annotation within the figures.

      (2) For Figure 2E, please plot the error bar. Are there any statistics performed to compare the mean responses?

      We added error bars (standard errors of the mean). We had not originally performed statistical comparisons between the stimuli, but now we have. The analysis of responses strength now appears in a new table (Table 1)

      (3) For Figure 2D, it will be more informative to plot the percentage of responsive units.

      Done.

      (4) Could the similarity in response be explained by the similarity in urine composition? The study will be significantly strengthened by understanding the "distance" of chemical composition in different urine.

      We agree. As we wrote in the Discussion: “Ultimately, lacking knowledge of the chemical space associated with each of the stimuli, this and all the other ideas developed here remain speculative.” We note however, that chemical distance (which in itself is hard to define) will provide only part of the picture. The other part is the “projection” of chemical space on the receptor array. This is an idea that we develop in the Discussion and in Figure 8. Specifically, that it is the combination of stimulus composition, and receptor tuning properties that will determine stimulus distances in neuronal space.

      That said, a better understanding of the chemical distance is an important aspect that we are working to include in our future studies. For this dataset unfortunately, we have no such data.

      (5) If it is not possible for the authors to obtain these data first-hand, published data on MUPs and chemicals found in these urines may provide some clues.

      This comment is directly related to the previous one. Measurements about some classes of molecules may be found for some of the stimuli that we used here, but not for all. We are not aware of any single dataset that contains this information for any type of molecule across the entire stimulus set that we have used and pooling results from different studies has limited validity because of the biological and technical variability across studies. In order to reliably interpret our current recordings, it would be necessary to measure the urinary content of the very same samples that were used for stimulation. Unfortunately, we are not able to conduct this analysis at this stage.

      (6) It is not very clear to me whether the female overrepresentation is because there are truly more AOB cells that respond to females than males or because there are only two female samples but 9 male samples.

      The definitive answer to this comment is given in our response to the next one.

      Nevertheless, we agree that this is an important point. It is true that the number of neurons fulfilling each of the patterns depends on the number of individual stimuli that define it (and on the frequency of neurons that respond to those stimuli). However, our measure of “over representation” was designed to overcome this bias, by using bootstrapping to reveal if the observed number of patterns is larger than expected by chance.  The higher frequency of responses to female, as compared to male stimuli, is observed in other studies by others and by us, also when the number of male and female stimuli is matched (e.g., Bansal et al BMC Biol 2021, Ben-Shaul et al, PNAS 2010, Hendrickson et al, JNS, 2008). However, here, by overrepresentation, we do not refer to the higher frequency of female responding neurons, but rather that given the number of responding neurons, the female pattern is more common than expected by chance.

      (7) If the authors only select two male samples, let's say ICR Naïve and ICR DOM, combine them with responses to two female samples, and do the same analysis as in Figure 3, will the female response still be overrepresented?

      Following this suggestion, we have performed this analysis, and we were glad to see that the result is the one we had anticipated. Below, we provide an image of the results, following the same approach that we applied before, and showed in Figure 3C. Here, we defined a female pattern (using the two female samples) and compared it to a male pattern (using the ICR naïve and ICR DOM as suggested). It is as if we had only four stimuli in our set. As in the article, we calculated the expected distribution with 100,000 shuffles. We denoted this pattern as F/M ICR. The results are shown below.

      Under the present conditions, the distribution of the number of female selective patterns is larger (i.e., shifted to the right, compare to the female category in Figure 3C. This is expected, since now the criterion is more permissive. Specifically, now to qualify as a “female pattern”, the two responses to female urine must be stronger only than the responses to the two male stimuli included in this analysis (and to all other responses). Notably, although the null distribution shifted to the right, the actual number of neurons fulfilling this pattern is also larger, so that the actual number remains significantly larger than expected by chance. This is also true for the reverse category (as is the case in the ~female category Figure 3C).  Thus, we conclude that overrepresentation of the female pattern is not a trivial consequence of the number of male and female stimuli.

      Author response image 1.

      (8) In Figure 4B and 4C, the pairwise distance during non-estrus is generally higher than that during estrus, although they are highly correlated. Does it mean that the cells respond to different urines more distinctively during diestrus than in estrus?

      This is an important observation (!) and we had originally overlooked it.  It is true that higher distance (as they are in estrus) imply more distinct population level responses and hence better discrimination among stimuli. However, this is inconsistent with all our other analyses that do not point to enhanced selectivity or discrimination in either state. If anything, we find somewhat higher sparseness in estrus.  Yet, there may be technical explanations for the differences.

      For Euclidean distances, the explanation may be trivial. The distance depends on the number of dimensions (i.e., units), and since our sample contains more neurons recorded during non-estrus, the larger distance is expected.

      In fact, there is a similar dependence on sample size for the correlation distance. Smaller samples are associated with higher (spurious) correlations, and hence larger samples are be associated with larger distances. To demonstrate this, we conducted a simple simulation, where we calculated the absolute correlation coefficients of random samples from standard normal distributions (using the MATLAB function randn), changing the size of the population. For each sample size, we conducted 1000 tests. We considered sample sizes from 10 to 100000, including 200 and 300 (which are similar to our sample sizes). The results are shown in the figure below. Note that the absolute value of the correlation coefficient decreases with sample size, while the p-value for the observed correlation is stable at ~0.5.

      While this is not a rigorous analysis of this issue, and while it does not exactly reflect the scenario in our data, where correlations are generally positive, it shows that the observed correlation (and hence correlation distance) is also affected by sample size.

      For these reasons, we focus on comparison of these distances, rather than the absolute values of the correlation distances.

      Author response image 2.

      Following this comment, we now write in the manuscript:

      “We first note that distances are generally larger during non-estrus, suggesting enhanced discrimination during this stage. However, further analyses of sparseness and selectivity do not support this idea (see below). Furthermore, we note that both Euclidean and correlation distances generally depend on sample size. In both cases, distances are expected to increase as a function of sample size, which in our dataset, is larger for the non-estrus (n = 305) as compared to the estrus (n = 241) neurons. Because of this factor, we focus here on the similarity of the relative within-state distances across the states (and not on their absolute magnitudes). Specifically, we find a positive and significant correlation among pairwise population distances under the two states. Thus, at the population level, representational space remains broadly stable across the estrus cycle. Nevertheless, several points in Fig. 4D, E clearly diverge from a linear relationship, implying that representational space differs under the two states. We next examine such state-dependent changes in more detail.”

      (9) The correlation analysis is not entirely intuitive when just looking at the figures. Some sample heatmaps showing the response differences between estrous states will be helpful.

      If we understand correctly, the idea is to show the correlation matrices from which the values in 4B and 4C are taken. The relevant images are now included in figure 4B, C and are references within the main text.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Many aspects of the study are carefully done, and in the grand scheme this is a solid contribution. I have no "big-picture" concerns about the approach or methodology. However, in numerous places the manuscript is unnecessarily vague, ambiguous, or confusing. Tightening up the presentation will magnify their impact.

      We have reviewed the text and made substantial editing changes. Along with other specific comments by made both reviewers, we hope that these changes improve the presentation.

      Strengths:

      (1) The study includes urine donors from males of three strains each with three social states, as well as females in two states. This diversity significantly enhances their ability to interpret their results.

      (2) Several distinct analyses are used to explore the question of whether AOB MCs are biased towards specific states or different between estrus and non-estrus females. The results of these different analyses are self-reinforcing about the main conclusions of the study.

      (3) The presentation maintains a neutral perspective throughout while touching on topics of widespread interest.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Introduction:

      The discussion of the role of the VNS and preferences for different male stimuli should perhaps include Wysocki and Lepri 1991

      We assume that the reviewer is referring to “Consequences of removing the vomeronasal organ” by Wysocki CJ, Lepri JJ, a review article in J Steroid Biochem from 1991. We were not familiar with this specific article and have now read it. The article discusses various male behaviors, and some effects on female behavior and physiology (e.g., puberty acceleration, maternal behaviors, ovulation) but we could not find any mention of the preference of female mice in this article. We also expanded our search to all pubmed articles authored by Wysocki and Lepri and then all articles by Wysocki (with the keyword Vomeronasal). Despite our best intentions to give due credit, we found nothing that seems directly related to this statement. Please correct us if we had missed anything.

      (2) Results:

      a) Given the 20s gap between them, the distinction between sample application and sympathetic nerve trunk stimulation needs to be made crystal clear; in many places, "stimulus application" is used in places where this reviewer suspects they actually mean sympathetic nerve trunk stimulation.

      We realize that this is confusing, and we also agree that at least in one place, we have not been sufficiently clear about the distinction. To clarify, we distinguish between stimulus application (physical application of stimulus to the nostril), and stimulation (which refers to SNT stimulation, which typically induces VNO suction). The general term stimulus presentation refers to the entire process. As explained in the text, in our analysis, we consider the entire window starting at application and ending 40s after stimulation. This is because we sometimes observe immediate responses following application. One such responses is seen in Figure 2D, and this is directly related to a detailed comment made below (on Figure 1D, part c). Indeed, for this figure time 0 indicates stimulus application. This was indicated previously, but we have now rearranged order of the panels to make the distinction between this response and other clearer. We have also revised the figure caption and the text to clarify this issue.

      b) There appears to be a mismatch between the discussion of Figure 3 and its contents. Specifically, there is an example of an "adjusted" pattern in 3A, not 3B.

      True. we have revised the text to correctly refer to the figure. Thanks.

      c) The discussion of patterns neglects to mention whether it's possible for a neuron to belong to more than one pattern. For example, it would seem possible for a neuron to simultaneously fit the "ICR pattern" and the "dominant adjusted pattern" if, e.g., all ICR responses are stronger than all others, but if simultaneously within each strain the dominant male causes the largest response.

      This is true. In the legend to Figure 3B, we actually wrote: “A neuron may fulfill more than one pattern and thus may appear in more than one row.”, but we now also write in the main text:

      “We note that criteria for adjusted patterns are less stringent than for the standard patterns defined above. Furthermore, some patterns are not mutually exclusive, and thus, a neuron may fulfil more than a single pattern.”

      (3) Discussion:

      a) The discussion of chemical specificity in urine focuses on volatiles and MUPs (citation #47), but many important molecules for the VNS are small, nonvolatile ligands. For such molecules, the corresponding study is Fu et al 2015.

      Agreed. We now cite this work and several others that were not included before in the context of chemical and electrophysiological analyses.

      b) "Following our line of reasoning, this scarcity may represent an optimal allocation of resources to separate dominant from naïve males": 1 unit out of 215 is roughly consistent with a single receptor. Surely little would be lost if there could be more computational capacity devoted to this important axis than that? It seems more likely that dominance is computed from multiple neuronal types with mixed encoding.

      We fully agree, and we are not claiming that dominance, nor any other feature, is derived using dedicated feature selective neurons. Our discussion of resource allocation is inevitably speculative. Our main point in this context is that a lack of overrepresentation does not imply that a feature is not important. As a note, we do not think that there is good reason to suppose that AOB neurons reflect the activity of single receptors.

      To present this potential confusion, we now added the following sentences in the Discussion subsection titled “Response patterns of AOB-MCs”:

      “We stress that we do not suggest that features such as physiological state are encoded by the activity of single neurons. In fact, we believe that most ethologically relevant features are encoded by the activity of multiple neurons. Nevertheless, such population level representations ultimately depend on the response properties of individual neurons, and we thus ask: what can we learn from our analysis of response pattern frequency?”

      (4) Methods:

      a) Male status, "were unambiguous in most cases": is it possible to put numerical estimates on this? 55% and 99% are both "most," yet they differ substantially in interpretive uncertainty.

      Upon reexamination, we realized that this sentence is incorrect. Ambiguous cases were not considered as dominant for urine collection. We only classified mice as dominant if they “won” the tube test and exhibited dominant behavior in the subsequent observation period in the cage. The phrasing has now been corrected in the manuscript (Methods section).

      b) Surgical procedures and electrode positioning: important details of probes are missing (electrode recording area, spacing, etc).

      This information has been added to the Methods subsection “Surgical procedures and electrode positioning”

      c) Stimulus presentation procedure: Are stimuli manually pipetted or delivered by apparatus with precise timing?

      They are delivered manually. This has now been clarified in the text.

      d) Data analysis, "we applied more permissive criteria involving response magnitude": it's not clear whether this is what's spelled out in the next paragraph, or whether that's left unspecified. In either case, the next paragraph appears to be about establishing a noise floor on pattern membership, not a "permissive criterion."

      True, the next paragraph is not the explanation for the more permissive criteria. The more permissive criteria involving response magnitude are actually those described in Figure 3A and 3B. The sentence that was quoted above merely states that before applying those criteria, we had also searched for patterns defined by binary designation of neurons as responsive, or not responsive, to each of the stimuli (this is directly related to the next comment below). Using those binary definitions, we obtained a very small number of neurons for each pattern and thus decided to apply the approach actually used and described in the manuscript.

      To clarify this confusion, we thoroughly derived the description of this paragraph, and the beginning of the next one in the Methods section.

      e) Data analysis, method for assessing significance: there's a lot to like about the use of pooling to estimate the baseline and the use of an ANOVA-like test to assess unit responsiveness.

      But:

      i) for a specific stimulus, at 4 trials (the minimum specified in "Stimulus presentation procedure") kruskalwallis is questionable. They state that most trials use 5, however, and that should be okay.

      The exact values are now given in the text. The mean number of repeated presentations per stimulus: 5.1± 0.9, mean ± sd. In 72% of the cases, stimuli were given 5 or more times. Otherwise, they were presented 4 times. In the context of the statistical test, we note that we are not comparing 5 (or 4) values with another set of 5 (or 4 values), but with a much larger sample (~44-55 baseline trials – given 11 trials and 4-5 repeats of each). Under this scenario, we think that the statistical approach is sound. However, the more important consideration, in our opinion, is given below.

      ii) the methods statement suggests they are running kruskalwallis individually for each neuron/stimulus, rather than once per neuron across all stimuli. With 11 stimuli, there is a substantial chance of a false-positive if they used p < 0.05 to assess significance. (The actual threshold was unstated.) Were there any multiple comparison corrections performed? Or did they run kruskalwallis on the neuron, and then if significant assess individual stimuli? (Which is a form of multiple-comparisons correction.)

      First, we indeed failed to mention that our criterion was 0.05. This has been corrected, by adding the information to the results and the Methods sections. No, we did not apply any multiple comparison measures. We consider each neuron-stimulus pair as an independent entity, and we are aware that this leads to a higher false positive rate. On the other hand, applying multiple comparisons would be problematic, as the same number of stimuli used in different studies varies. Application of multiple comparison corrections would thus lead to different response criteria across different studies, which would be very problematic. This raises the almost philosophical question regarding the use of multiple comparisons (as well as one and two tailed tests), but practically, most, if not all of our conclusions involve comparisons across conditions. For this purpose, we think that our procedure is valid. More generally, while selection of responses according to significance has some obvious advantages, the decision to use any particular criterion is entirely arbitrary. Therefore, we do not attach any special meaning to the significance threshold used here. Rather, we think of it as a simple criterion that allows us to exclude weakly responding or non-responsive neurons, and to compare frequencies of neurons that fulfill this criterion, under different conditions and contexts.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Results:

      "are represented more than represented by chance" seems to have a misplaced word

      True. Thanks. Corrected.

      Figure 1D:

      a) Indicate the meaning of the number that appears in the top left for each unit (10, 5, 40, 5, 5) (I'm guessing it's the vertical scale for the PSTH, but best to spell it out explicitly.)

      This information has been added.

      b) "The red vertical line indicates stimulus application": is it the application of the chemical stimulus or SNT shock?

      Please see our answer to c

      c) "For unit 2, time 0 indicate stimulus application, as in this case, responses began after stimulus application, prior to stimulation." First, the meaning of time 0 for the other units is not clearly specified (we infer that unit 2 is an exception, but we don't know what most of them mean). Second, it seems as if the response (?) to ICR naive begins even before stimulus application.

      This issue was also mentioned above as the 2nd weakness raised by this reviewer. To explain the meaning of the red lines, and resolve this confusion, we revised the figure caption text to indicate that for all units (except the former unit 2) time 0 indicates SNT stimulation. We also changed the order of the unit examples, placing the former unit 2 in the rightmost position. It is true that for this unit, there is a firing rate change prior to stimulus application, which actually appears as rate attenuation following stimulus application. In this specific case, we consider this activity as “noise”, and note that this neuron-stimulus combination would not be classified as a response (since there is no consistent change across stimulus presentation).

      As a note, while reviewing this figure, we noted an error. We have previously written that the ITI was 10 s, whereas it was actually 18 s long. This has been corrected in the Figure and in the text.

      Figure 2B:

      "The mean error due to the reduced 2-D representation is 0.29 (arbitrary units)." This is unclear. MDS is often described in terms of % of variance explained, is that what this means? If so, the units are not arbitrary; otherwise, it's unclear whether specifying a value with arbitrary units adds any value.

      This is a very good point, and we thank the reviewer for identifying this mistake. The units are not arbitrary! They are units of correlation distance. We now added a scale bar (a square) to panel 2B to indicate what a distance of 0.1. Following this comment, we also calculated the mean error in the original data, and noted the ratio between the mean absolute error (due to considering only two dimensions) and the mean original distances. We also now report the value of the first two eigenvalues. Specifically, we now write:

      “Note that like all dimensionally reduced representations, the representation in Fig. 2B is an approximation. Here, the first two eigenvalues of account for 44.6% of the variance of the original distances (30.4% and 14.2%, respectively for the first and second dimension). Another way to evaluate the representation is via the mean error due to the reduced 2-D representation. Here, it is 0.29, whereas the mean of the original distances is 0.73.”

      Figure 3A:

      a) There is a truncated label (or something) above the panel letter.

      Thanks. Corrected. This was part of the “Figure” label

      b) The graphic for the "adjusted pattern" also fits the criterion of the "pattern": for example, in the top row the activity for ICR is still higher than for any other stimulus, thus fulfilling the criterion of a "pattern" and not just an "adjusted pattern."

      That was not our intention. An adjusted pattern does not necessarily fulfill the (non-adjusted) “pattern” (while the opposite is true). We have now revised the rightmost panel in figure 3A, adding both “&s” to indicate that all three conditions must be fulfilled, and in attempt for a more intuitive representation, applied a different background denoting stimuli with irrelevant responses. We also changed the terms in the legend within the panel, making them more accurate: (Thus, “strong activity” was changed to “stronger responses”). In addition, we revised the text and figure legends in attempt to better clarify these definitions.

      Figure 3B:

      I'm assuming that the columns of the heatmap correspond to different urine stimuli, and that the color is normalized firing rate. But readers should not have to guess.

      True, and agreed. We added legends to clarify this.

      Figure 4B:

      The caption should mention that the pairwise measures are between the stimulus columns of panel A.

      We revised the caption to indicate this. Note that we also added two additional panels to this figure.

      Figure 5A&B:

      Instead of a multiple-comparisons correction, it seems likely to be better to use a 2-way ANOVA. At a minimum, the nature of the multiple-comparisons correction needs to be specified (many are conservative, but they differ in the extent of how conservative they are).

      We now write in the text that we used a Bonferroni correction (this information previously appeared only in the caption). We also found an error in the caption. We previously wrote that we used a binomial exact test for both panels A and B. However, only the data in panel A was calculated with a binomial exact test. The data in panel B was calculated with a one-way ANOVA.

      We now also applied a 2-way ANOVA to response magnitudes (i.e., panel B). We find a main effect of stimulus, but not of state, and no effect of interaction between the two. This is consistent with our previous analyses. This analysis is now included in the text. We thank the reviewer for this suggestion.

      Editor's note:

      Should you choose to revise your manuscript, if you have not already done so, please include full statistical reporting including exact p-values wherever possible alongside the summary statistics (test statistic and df) and, where appropriate, 95% confidence intervals. These should be reported for all key questions and not only when the p-value is less than 0.05 in the main manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      We appreciate the reviewer recognising that our study has been carefully performed and provides a valuable resource for the community. The characterization of Repo-man proline hydroxylation is also recognised as a novel finding.

      With respect to Concerns raised by reviewer 1:

      (1) The study applied HILIC-based chromatographic separation with a goal of enriching and separating hydroxyproline-containing peptides. However, as the authors mentioned, such an approach is not specific to proline hydroxylation. In addition, many other chromatography techniques can achieve deep proteome fractionation such as high pH reverse phase fractionation, strong-cation exchange etc. There was no data in this study to demonstrate that the strategy offered improved coverage of proline hydroxylation proteins, as the identifications of the HyPro sites could be achieved through deep fractionation and a highly sensitive LCMS setup. The data of Figure 2A and S1A were somewhat confusing without a clear explanation of the heat map representations.

      We do not agree that the apparent concern raised here, i.e., that the method we present is not 100% specific for enriching only hydroxylated peptides, is a serious issue. We show specifically that our method indeed enriches samples for hydroxylated peptides, thereby increasing the chances of identifying proline hydroxylated peptides in a cell extract. We never claimed that it was mono-specific for enrichment of hydroxylated peptides. Further, we note that almost no chromatographic method we know of, including those commonly used to enrich for different types of post translationally-modified peptides (including phospho-peptides) is completely mono-specific for a single type of modified peptide. The reviewer comments that it could have been possible to use alternative methods to identify proline-hydroxylated peptides. This may be true, but we know of no published examples, or previous studies, where this has been demonstrated experimentally on a scale comparable to that we show here. Of course there is always more than one way to approach technical challenges and it may be that future methods will be demonstrated that achieve equivalent, or even superior, results with respect to the detection of proline hydroxylated peptides. To the best of our knowledge, however, our current study provides a robust methodology that goes well beyond any previously published analysis of proline hydroxylation.

      (2) The study reported that the HyPro immonium ion is a diagnostic ion for HyPro identification. However, the data showed that only around 5% of the identifications had such a diagnostic ion. In comparison, acetyllysine immonium ion was previously reported to be a useful marker for acetyllysine peptides (PMID: 18338905), and the strategy offered a sensitivity of 70% with a specificity of 98%. In this study, the sensitivity of HyPro immonium ion was quite low. The authors also clearly demonstrated that the presence of immonium ion varied significantly due to MS settings, peptide sequence, and abundance. With further complications from L/I immonium ions, it became very challenging to implement this strategy in a global LC-MS analysis to either validate or invalidate HyPro identifications.

      We feel that the reviewer’s initial comment is potentially misleading - it implies that we were proposing here that the 'HyPro immonium ion is a diagnostic ion for HyPro identification’. In contrast, this concept was already widely held in the field before we started this project. Indeed, the fact that the diagnostic HyPro immonium ion is often difficult to detect, has been used as one of the arguments by other researchers to support the view that HIF-α is the only physiologically relevant target for PHD enzymes, a controversy referenced explicitly by Reviewer 2 below. What we actually show here are novel data that help to explain why the diagnostic HyPro immonium ion is often difficult to detect, when standard approaches and technical parameters for MS analysis are used. We beleive that this observation, along with other data we present, is a useful contribution to the field that can help to resolve the previous controversies concerning the true prevalence and biological roles of PHD-catalysed proline hydroxylation on protein targets.

      (3) The study aimed to apply the HILIC-based proteomics workflow to identify HyPro proteins regulated by the PHD enzyme. However, the quantification strategy was not rigorous. The study just considered the HyPro proteins not identified by FG-4592 treatment as potential PHD targeted proteins. There are a few issues. First, such an analysis was not quantitative without reproducibility or statistical analysis. Second, it did not take into consideration that data-dependent LC-MS analysis was not comprehensive and some peptide ions may not be identified due to background interferences. Lastly, FG-4592 treatment for 24 hrs could lead to wide changes in gene expressions and protein abundances. Therefore, it is not informative to draw conclusions based on the data for bioinformatic analysis.

      We agree that this study is not quantifying or addressing the stoichiometry of proline hydroxylation across the very large number of new PHD target sites we identify. That was not claimed and was not the objective of our study. Nonetheless, we feel the comments of the referee do not adequately take into account the SILAC data we included (cf Figure 8) or the full range of experimental data presented in this study. We would further refer the reviewer also to the data presented in the companion paper by Druker et al., which we cross-referenced extensively in our study and have also made available previously on biorxiv.

      (4) The authors performed an in vitro PHD1 enzyme assay to validate that Repo-man can be hydroxylated by PHD1. However, Figure 9 did not show quantitatively PHD1-induced increase in Repo-man HyPro abundance and it is difficult to assess its reaction efficiency to compare with HIF1a HyPro.

      Here again we refer to the recent controversy referenced explicitly by Reviewer 2 below, concerning the view expressed by some researchers that only HIF-α is a physiological substrate for PHD enzymes in cells. We were challenged to show that any of the novel protein targets of PHDs we identified were indeed hydroxylated by PHD enzymes in vitro and that is what we demonstrated in Figure 9. This was not an experiment performed to quantify stoichiometry and indeed, it is not possible to draw any firm conclusions about efficiency or stiochiometry in vitro when using catalytic PHD subunits alone, given that we do not yet know whether PHDs may show different properties in cells, dependent on interactions with other factors and/or modifications.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments that our manuscript presents an advanced, standardized protocol for identifying proline hydroxylation, with well designed experiments, which may help resolve confusion in the field.

      With respect to Concerns raised by reviewer 2:

      (1) The authors should provide a summary of the standard protocol for identifying proline hydroxylation sites in proteins that can easily be followed by others.

      We agree and plan to provide a clearly described, step by step guide to assist other researchers who wish to employ our methods for proline hydroxylation analysis in their own studies.

      (2) Cockman et al. proposed that HIF-α is the only physiologically relevant target for PHDs. Their approach is considered the gold standard for identifying PHD targets. Therefore, the authors should discuss the major progress they made in this manuscript that challenges Cockman's conclusion.

      We agree that our study provides valuable information germane to the recent controversy in the field and the views published by Cockman et al., to the effect that HIF-α is the only physiologically relevant target for PHDs. We will carefully review our statements when preparing a suitably revised version of record with the aim of providing a balanced and objective discussion of this issue.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments that our study employs state-of-the-art mass spectrometric techniques with optimized collision parameters to ensure proper detection of the immonium ions, along with their recognition that our study is, 'an advance compared to other similar approaches before.’ We also appreciate their reference to our companion study by Druker et al, in which we characterise the mechanism and biological role in regulation of mitotic progression of the hydroxylation of P604 in the target protein RepoMan (CDCA2), that is identified in this study.

      With respect to the Concern raised by reviewer 3:

      Despite the authors' claim about the specificity of this method in picking up the intended peptides, there is a good amount of potential false positives that also happen to get picked (owing to the limitations of MS-based readout), and the authors' criteria for downstream filtering of such peptides require further clarification. In the same vein, greater and more diverse cell-based validation approach will be helpful to substantiate the claims regarding enrichment of peptides in the described pathway analyses..

      We agree that this study, which has a focus on methodology and technical approaches for detecting sites of PHD- catalysed proline hydroxylation, cannot exhaustively validate the biological significance of all of the putative sites and targets identified. As the reviewer notes, we have performed a detailed functional characterisation of one such novel PHD-catalyed proline hydroxylation site, i.e. P604 in the protein RepoMan (CDCA2). This functional analysis is presented in the companion paper by Druker et al., which has also been reviewed by eLife and placed on biorxiv (doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2025.05.06.652400). We hope that publication of our identification of many new putative PHD target sites will encourage other researchers to pursue characterisation of their functional reoles in different biological mechanisms and have tried here to provide some degree of guidance to focus attention on the identification of those sites for which we currently have highest confidence.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The investigators undertook detailed characterization of a previously proposed membrane targeting sequence (MTS), a short N-terminal peptide, of the bactofilin BacA in Caulobacter crescentus. Using light microscopy, single molecule tracking, liposome binding assays, and molecular dynamics simulations, they provide data to suggest that this sequence indeed does function in membrane targeting and further conclude that membrane targeting is required for polymerization. While the membrane association data are reasonably convincing, there are no direct assays to assess polymerization and some assays used lack proper controls as detailed below. Since the MTS isn't required for bactofilin polymerization in other bacterial homologues, showing that membrane binding facilitates polymerization would be a significant advance for the field.

      We agree that additional experiments were required to consolidate our results and conclusions. Please see below for a description of the new data included in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Major concerns

      (1) This work claims that the N-termina MTS domain of BacA is required for polymerization, but they do not provide sufficient evidence that the ∆2-8 mutant or any of the other MTS variants actually do not polymerize (or form higher order structures). Bactofilins are known to form filaments, bundles of filaments, and lattice sheets in vitro and bundles of filaments have been observed in cells. Whether puncta or diffuse labeling represents different polymerized states or filaments vs. monomers has not been established. Microscopy shows mis-localization away from the stalk, but resolution is limited. Further experiments using higher resolution microscopy and TEM of purified protein would prove that the MTS is required for polymerization.

      We do not propose that the MTS is directly involved in the polymerization process and state this more clearly now in the Results and Discussion sections of the revised manuscript. To address this point, we performed transmission electron microscopy studies comparing the polymerization behavior of wild-type and mutant BacA variants. The results clearly show that the MTS-free BacA variant (∆2-8) forms polymers that are indistinguishable from those formed by the wild-type protein, when purified from an E. coli overproduction strain (new Figure 1–figure supplement 1). This finding is consistent with structural work showing that bactofilin polymerization is exclusively mediated by the conserved bactofilin domain (Deng et al, Nat Microbiol, 2019). However, at native expression levels, BacA only accumulates to ~200 molecules per cell (Kühn et al, EMBO J, 2006). Under these conditions, the MTS-mediated increase in the local concentration of BacA at the membrane surface and, potentially, steric constraints imposed by membrane curvature, may facilitate the polymerization process. This hypothesis has now been stated more clearly in the Results and Discussion sections.

      For polymer-forming proteins, defined localized signals are typically interpreted as slow-moving or stationary polymeric complexes. A diffuse localization, by contrast, suggests that a protein exists in a monomeric or, at most, (small) oligomeric state in which it diffuses rapidly within the cell and is thus no longer detected as distinct foci by widefield microscopy. Our single-molecule data show that BacA variants that are no longer able to interact with the membrane (as verified by cell fractionation studies and in vitro liposome binding assays) have a high diffusion rate, similar to that measured for the non-polymerizing and non-membrane-bound F130R variant. These results demonstrate that a defect in membrane binding strongly reduces the ability of BacA to form polymeric assemblies. To support this hypothesis, we have now repeated all single-particle tracking experiments and included mVenus as a freely diffusible reference protein. Our data confirm that the mobilities of the ∆2-8 and F130R variants are similar and approach those of free mVenus, supporting the idea that the deficiency to interact with the membrane prevents the formation of extended polymeric structures (which should show much lower mobilities). To underscore the relevance of membrane binding for BacA assembly, we have now included a new experiment, in which we used the PbpC membrane anchor (PbpC<sub>1-132</sub>-mcherry) to restore the recruitment of the ∆2-8 variant to the membrane (Figure 9 and Figure 9–figure supplement 1). The results obtained show that the ∆2-8 variant transitions from a diffuse localization to polar foci upon overproduction of PbpC<sub>1-132</sub>-mcherry. The polymerization-impaired F130R variant, by contrast, remains evenly distributed throughout the cytoplasm under all conditions. These findings further support the idea that polymerization and membrane-association are mutually interdependent processes.

      (2) Liposome binding data would be strengthened with TEM images to show BacA binding to liposomes. From this experiment, gross polymerization structures of MTS variants could also be characterized.

      We do not have the possibility to perform cryo-electron microscopy studies of liposomes bound to BacA. However, the results of the cell fractionation and liposome sedimentation assays clearly support a critical role of the MTS in membrane binding.

      (3) The use of the BacA F130R mutant throughout the study to probe the effect of polymerization on membrane binding is concerning as there is no evidence showing that this variant cannot polymerize. Looking through the papers the authors referenced, there was no evidence of an identical mutation in BacA that was shown to be depolymerized or any discussion in this study of how the F130R mutation might to analogous to polymerization-deficient variants in other bactofilins mentioned in these references.

      Residue F130 in the C-terminal polymerization interface of BacA is conserved among bactofilin homologs, although its absolute position in the protein sequence may vary, depending on the length of the N-terminal unstructured tail. The papers cited in our manuscript show that an exchange of this conserved phenylalanine residue abolishes polymer formation. Nevertheless, we agree that it is important to verify the polymerization defect of the F130R variant in the system under study. We have now included size-exclusion chromatography data showing that BacA-F130R forms a low-molecular-weight complex, whereas the wild-type protein largely elutes in the exclusion volume, indicating the formation of large, polymeric species (new Figure 1–figure supplement 1). In addition, we performed transmission electron microscopy analyses of BacA-F130R, which verified the absence of larger oligomers (new Figure 1–figure supplement 2).

      (4) Microscopy shows that a BacA variant lacking the native MTS regains the ability to form puncta, albeit mis-localized, in the cell when fused to a heterologous MTS from MreB. While this swap suggests a link between puncta formation and membrane binding the relationship between puncta and polymerization has not been established (see comment 1).

      We show that a BacA variant lacking the MTS (∆2-8) regains the ability to form membrane-associated foci when fused to the MTS of MreB. By contrast, a similar variant that additionally carries the F130R exchange (preventing its polymerization) shows a diffuse cytoplasmic localization. In addition, we show that the F130R exchange leads to a loss of membrane binding and to a considerable increase in the mobility of the variants carrying the MTS of E. coli MreB. As described above, we now provide additional data demonstrating that elevated levels of the PbpC membrane anchor can reinstate polar localization for the ∆2-8 variant, whereas it fails to do so for the polymerization-deficient F130R variant (Figure 9 and Figure 9–figure supplement 1). Together, these results support the hypothesis that membrane association and polymerization act synergistically to establish localized bactofilin assemblies at the stalked cell pole.

      (5) The authors provide no primary data for single molecule tracking. There is no tracking mapped onto microscopy images to show membrane localization or lack of localization in MTS deletion/ variants. A known soluble protein (e.g. unfused mVenus) and a known membrane bound protein would serve as valuable controls to interpret the data presented. It also is unclear why the authors chose to report molecular dynamics as mean squared displacement rather than mean squared displacement per unit time, and the number of localizations is not indicated. Extrapolating from the graph in figure 4 D for example, it looks like WT BacA-mVenus would have a mobility of 0.5 (0.02/0.04) micrometers squared per second which is approaching diffusive behavior. Further justification/details of their analysis method is needed. It's also not clear how one should interpret the finding that several of the double point mutants show higher displacement than deleting the entire MTS. These experiments as they stand don't account for any other cause of molecular behavior change and assume that a decrease in movement is synonymous with membrane binding.

      We now provide additional information on the single-particle analysis. A new supplemental figure now shows a mapping of single-particle tracks onto the cells in which they were recorded for all proteins analyzed (Figure 2–figure supplement 1). Due to the small size of C. crescentus, it is difficult to clearly differentiate between membrane-associated and cytoplasmic protein species. However, overall, slow-diffusing particles tend to be localized to the cell periphery, supporting the idea that membrane-associated particles form larger assemblies (apart from diffusing more slowly due to their membrane association). In addition, we have included a movie that shows the single-particle diffusion dynamics of all proteins in representative cells (Figure 2-video 1). Finally, we have included a table that gives an overview of the number of cells and tracks analyzed for all proteins investigated (Supplementary file 1). Figure 2A and 4D show the mean squared displacement as a function of time, which makes it possible to assess whether the particles observed move by normal, Brownian diffusion (which is the case here). We repeated the entire single-particle tracking analysis to verify the data obtained previously and obtained very similar results. Among the different mutant proteins, only the K4E-K7E variant consistently shows a higher mobility than the MTS-free ∆2-8 variant, with MSD values similar to that of free mVenus. The underlying reason remains unclear. However, we believe that an in-depth analysis of this phenomenon is beyond the scope of this paper. We re-confirmed the integrity of the construct encoding the K4E/K7E variant by DNA sequencing and once again verified the size and stability of the fusion protein by Western blot analysis, excluding artifacts due to errors during cloning and strain construction.

      We agree that the single-molecule tracking data alone are certainly not sufficient to draw firm conclusions on the relationship between membrane binding and protein mobility. However, they are consistent with the results of our other in vivo and in vitro analyses, which together indicate a clear correlation between the mobility of BacA and its ability to interact with the membrane and polymerize (processes that promote each other synergistically).

      (6) The experiments that map the interaction surface between the N-terminal unstructured region of PbpC and a specific part of the BacA bactofilin domain seem distinct from the main focus of the paper and the data somewhat preliminary. While the PbpC side has been probed by orthogonal approaches (mutation with localization in cells and affinity in vitro), the BacA region side has only been suggested by the deuterium exchange experiment and needs some kind of validation.

      The results of the HDX analysis per se are not preliminary and clearly show a change in the solvent accessibility of backbone amides in the C-terminal region in the bactofilin domain in the presence of the PbpC<sub>1-13</sub> peptide. However, we agree that additional experiments would be required to verify the binding site suggested by these data. We agree that further research is required to precisely map and verify the PbpC binding site. However, as this is not the main focus of the paper, we would like to proceed without conducting further experiments in this area.

      We now provide additional data showing that elevated levels of the PbpC membrane anchor are able to recruit the MTS-free BacA variant (∆2-8) to the cytoplasmic membrane and stimulate its assembly at the stalked pole (Figure 9). These results now integrate Figure 8 more effectively into the overall theme of the paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors of this study investigated the membrane-binding properties of bactofilin A from Caulobacter crescentus, a classic model organism for bacterial cell biology. BacA was the progenitor of a family of cytoskeletal proteins that have been identified as ubiquitous structural components in bacteria, performing a range of cell biological functions. Association with the cell membrane is a common property of the bactofilins studied and is thought to be important for functionality. However, almost all bactofilins lack a transmembrane domain. While membrane association has been attributed to the unstructured N-terminus, experimental evidence had yet to be provided. As a result, the mode of membrane association and the underlying molecular mechanics remained elusive.

      Liu at al. analyze the membrane binding properties of BacA in detail and scrutinize molecular interactions using in-vivo, in-vitro and in-silico techniques. They show that few N-terminal amino acids are important for membrane association or proper localization and suggest that membrane association promotes polymerization. Bioinformatic analyses revealed conserved lineage-specific N-terminal motifs indicating a conserved role in protein localization. Using HDX analysis they also identify a potential interaction site with PbpC, a morphogenic cell wall synthase implicated in Caulobacter stalk synthesis. Complementary, they pinpoint the bactofilin-interacting region within the PbpC C-terminus, known to interact with bactofilin. They further show that BacA localization is independent of PbpC.

      Strengths:

      These data significantly advance the understanding of the membrane binding determinants of bactofilins and thus their function at the molecular level. The major strength of the comprehensive study is the combination of complementary in vivo, in vitro and bioinformatic/simulation approaches, the results of which are consistent.

      Thank you for this positive feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      The results are limited to protein localization and interaction, as there is no data on phenotypic effects. Therefore, the cell biological significance remains somewhat underrepresented.

      We agree that it is interesting to investigate the phenotypic effects caused by the reduced membrane binding activity of BacA variants with defects in the MTS. We have now included phenotypic analyses that shed light on the role of region C1 in the localization of PbpC and its function in stalk elongation under phosphate-limiting conditions (see below).

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      To address the missing estimation of biological relevance, some additional experiments may be carried out.

      For example, given that BacA localizes PbpC by direct interaction, one might expect an effect on stalk formation if BacA is unable to bind the membrane or to polymerize. The same applies to PbpC variants lacking the C1 region. As the mutant strains are available, these data are not difficult to obtain but would help to compare the effect of the deletions with previous data (e.g. Kühn et al.) even if the differences are small.

      We have now analyzed the effect of the removal of region C1 on the ability of mVenus-PbpC to promote stalk elongation in C. crescentus under phosphate starvation. Interestingly, our results show that the lack of the BacA-interaction motif impairs the recruitment of the fusion protein to the stalked pole, but it does not interfere with its stimulatory effect on stalk biogenesis. Thus, the polar localization of PbpC does not appear to be critical for its function in localized peptidoglycan synthesis at the stalk base. These results are now shown in Figure 8–Figure supplement 4. The results obtained may be explained by residual transient interactions of mVenus-PbpC with proteins other than BacA at the stalked pole. Notably, PbpC has also been implicated in the attachment of the stalk-specific protein StpX to components of the outer membrane at the stalk base. The polar localization of PbpC may therefore be primarily required to ensure proper StpX localization, consistent with previous work by Hughes et al. (Mol Microbiol, 2013) showing that StpX is partially mislocalized in a strain producing an N-terminally truncated PbpC variant that no longer localizes to the stalk base.

      We have also attempted to investigate the ability of the Δ2-8 and F130R variants of BacA-mVenus to promote stalk elongation under phosphate starvation. However, the levels of the WT, Δ2-8 and F130R proteins and their stabilities were dramatically different after prolonged incubation of the cells in phosphate-limited medium, so that it was not possible to draw any firm conclusions from the results obtained (not shown).

      In addition, the M23-like endopeptidase LdpA is proposed to be a client protein of BacA (in C. crescentus, Billini et al. 2018, and H. neptunium or R. rubrum, Pöhl et al. 2024). In H. neptunium, it is suggested that the interaction is mediated by a cytoplasmic peptide of LmdC reminiscent of PbpC. This should at least be commented on. It would be interesting to see, if LpdA in C. crescentus is also delocalized and if so, this could identify another client protein of BacA.

      We agree that it would be interesting to study the role of BacA in LdpA function. However, we have not yet succeeded in generating a stable fluorescent protein fusion to LdpA, which currently makes it impossible to study the interplay between these two proteins in vivo. The focus of the present paper is on the mode of interaction between bactofilins and the cytoplasmic membrane and on the mutual interdependence of membrane binding and bactofilin polymerization. Given that PbpC is so far the only verified interaction partner of BacA in C. crescentus, we would like to limit our analysis to this client protein.

      Further comments:

      L105: analyze --> analyzed

      Done.

      L169: Is there any reason why the MTS of E. coli MreB was doubled?

      Previous work has shown that two tandem copies of the N-terminal amphiphilic helix of E. coli MreB were required to partially target a heterologous fusion partner protein (GFP) to the cytoplasmic membrane of E. coli cells (Salje et al, 2011).

      Fig. S3:

      a) Please decide which tag was used (mNG or mVenus) and adapt the figure or legend accordingly.<br /> b) In the legend for panel (C), please describe how the relative amounts were calculated, as the fractions arithmetically cannot add to > 100%. I guess each band was densiometrically rated and independently normalized to the whole-cell signal?

      The fluorescent tag used was mNeonGreen, as indicated in the figure. We have now corrected the legend accordingly. Thank you for making us aware of the wrong labeling of the y-axis. We have now corrected the figure and describe the method used to calculate the plotted values in the legend.

      Legend of Fig 1b: It is not clear to me, to which part of panel B the somewhat cryptic LY... strain names belong. I suggest putting them either next to the images, to delete them, or at least to unify the layout (compare, e.g. to Fig S7). (I would delete the LY numbers and stay with the genes/mutations throughout. This is just a suggestion).

      These names indicate the strains analyzed in panel B, and we have now clarified this in the legend. It is more straightforward to label the images according to the mutations carried by the different strains. Nevertheless, we would like to keep the strain names in the legend, so that the material used for the analysis can be clearly identified.

      Fig. 2a: As some of the colors are difficult to distinguish, I suggest sorting the names in the legend within the graph according to the slope of the curves (e.g. K4E K7E (?) on top and WT being at the bottom).

      Thank you for this suggestion. We have now rearranged the labels as proposed.

      In the legend (L924), correct typo "panel C" to "panel B".

      Done.

      Fig. 3: In the legend, I suggest deleting the abbreviations "S" and "P" as they do not show up in the image. In line 929, I suggest adding: average "relative" amount... or even more precisely: "average relative signal intensities obtained..."

      We have removed the abbreviations and now state that the bars indicate the “average relative signal intensities” obtained for the different fractions.

      Fig 4d: same suggestion as for Fig. 2a.

      Done.

      Fig 8: In the legend (L978), delete 1x "the"

      Done.

      L258 and Fig. S5: The expression "To account for biases in the coverage of bacterial species" seems somewhat unclear. I suggest rephrasing and adding information from the M+M section here (e.g. from L593, if this is meant).

      We now state that this step in the analysis pipeline was performed “To avoid biases arising from the over-representation of certain bacterial species in UniProt”.

      I appreciate the outline of the workflow in panel (a) of Fig. S5. It would be even more useful when some more details about the applied criteria for filtering would be provided (e.g. concerning what is meant with "detailed taxonomic information" or "filter out closely related sequences". Does the latter mean that only one bactofilin sequence per species was used? (As quite many bacteria have more than one but similar bactofilins.)

      We removed sequences from species with unclear phylogeny (e.g. candidate species whose precise taxonomic position has not yet been determined). For many pathogenic species, numerous strains have been sequenced. To account for this bias, only one sequence from clusters of highly similar bactofilin sequences (>90% identity) was retained per species. This information has now been included in the diagram. It is true that many bacteria have more than one bactofilin homolog. However, the sequences of these proteins are typically quite different. For instance, the BacA and BacB from C. crescentus only share 52% identity. Therefore, our analysis does not systematically eliminate bactofilin paralogs that coexist in the same species.

      L281: Although likely, I am not sure if membrane binding has ever been shown for a bactofilin from these phyla. (See also L 380.) Is there an example? Otherwise, membrane binding may not be a property of these bactofilins.

      To our knowledge, the ability of bactofilins from these clades to interact with membranes has not been investigated to date. We agree that the absence of an MTS-like motif may indicate that they lack membrane binding activity, and we have now stated this possibility in the Results and Discussion.

      L285: See comment above concerning the M23-like peptidase LpdA. Although not yet directly shown for C. crescentus, it seems likely that BacACc does also localize this peptidase in addition to PbpC. I suggest rephrasing, e.g. "known" --> "shown"

      We now use the word “reported”.

      L295 and Fig S8: PbpC is ubiquitous. Which criteria/filters have been applied to select the shown sequences?

      C. crescentus PbpC is different from E. coli Pbp1C. It is characterized by distinctive, conserved N- and C-terminal tails and only found in C. crescentus and close relatives. The C. crescentus homolog of E. coli PbpC is called PbpZ (Yakhnina et al, J Bacteriol, 2013; Strobel et al, J Bacterol, 2014), whereas C. crescentus PbpC is related to E. coli PBP1A. We have now added this information to the text to avoid confusion.

      L311: may replace "assembly" by "polymerization"

      Done.

      L320: bactofilin --> bactofilin domain?

      Yes, this was supposed to read “bactofilin domain”. Thank you for spotting this issue.

      L324: The HDX analysis of BacA suggests that the exchange is slowed down in the presence of the PbpC peptide, which is indicative of a physical interaction between these two molecules. To corroborate the claim that BacA polymerization is critical for interaction with the peptide (resp. PbpC), this experiment should be carried out with the polymerization defective BacA version F130R.

      (Or tone this statement down, e.g. show --> suggest.)

      “suggest”

      L386: undergoes --> undergo

      Done.

      L391-400: This idea is tempting but the suggested mechanism then would be restricted to bactofilins of C. crescentus and close relatives. The bactofilin of Rhodomicrobium, for example, was shown to localize dynamically and not to stick to a positively curved membrane.

      In the vast majority of species investigated so far, bactofilins were found to associate with specifically curved membrane regions and to contribute to the establishment of membrane curvature. Unfortu­nately, the sequences of the three co-polymerizing bactofilin paralogs of R. vannielii DSM 166 studied by Richter et al (2023) have not been reported and the genome sequence of this strain is not publicly available. However, in related species with three bactofilin paralogs, only one paralog shows an MTS-like N-terminal peptide and another paralog typically contains an unusual cadherin-like domain of unknown function, as also reported for R. vannielii DSM 166. Therefore, the mechanism controlling the localization dynamics of bactofilins may be complex in the Rhodomicrobium lineage. Nevertheless, at native expression levels, the major bactofilin (BacA) of R. vannielii DSM 166 was shown to localize predominantly to the hyphal tips and the (incipient) bud necks, suggesting that regions of distinct membrane curvature could also play a role in its recruitment. We do not claim that all bactofilins recognize positive membrane curvature, which is clearly not the case. It rather appears as though the curvature preference of bactofilins varies depending on their specific function.

      L405-406: I agree that localization of BacA has been shown to be independent of PbpC. However, this does not generally preclude an effect on BacA localization by other "client" or interacting proteins. (See also comment above about the putative BacA interactor LpdA). I suggest either to corroborate or to change this statement from "client binding" to "PbpC binding".

      Thank you for pointing out the imprecision of this statement. We now conclude that “PbpC binding” is not critical for BacA assembly and positioning.

      Suppl. Fig. S11: In the legend, please correct the copy-paste mismatch (...VirB...).

      Done.

      L482: delete 1x "at"

      Done.

      L484: may be better "soluble and insoluble fractions"?

      We now describe the two fractions as “soluble and membrane-containing insoluble fractions” to make clear to all readers that membrane vesicles are found in the pellet after ultracentrifugation.

      L489-490: check spelling immunoglobulin – immuneglobulin

      Done.

      L500 and 504: º_C --> ºC

      Done.

      Suppl. file X (HDX data): please check the table headline, table should be included in Suppl. file 1

      We have now included a headline in this file (now Supplementary file 3).

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have studied how a virus (EMCV) uses its RNA (Type 2 IRES) to hijack the host's protein-making machinery. They use cryo-EM to extract structural information about the recruitment of viral Type 2 IRES to ribosomal pre-IC. The authors propose a novel interaction mechanism in which the EMCV Type 2 IRES mimics 28S rRNA and interacts with ribosomal proteins and initiator tRNA (tRNAi).

      Strengths:

      (1) Getting structural insights about the Type 2 IRES-based initiation is novel.

      (2) The study allows a good comparison of other IRES-based initiation systems.

      (3) The manuscript is well-written and clearly explains the background, methods, and results.

      We thank Reviewer 1 for appreciating our efforts and finding structural insights about the type 2 IRES-based initiation presented in this study as novel.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The main weakness of the work is the low resolution of the structure. This limits the possibility of data interpretation at the molecular level.

      However, despite the moderate resolution of the cryo-EM reconstructions, the model fits well into the density. The analysis of the EMCV IRES-48S PIC structure is thorough and includes meaningful comparisons to previously published structures (e.g., PDB IDs - 7QP6 and 7QP7). These comparisons showed that Map B1 represents a closed conformation, in contrast to Map A in the open state (Figure 2). Additionally, the proposed 28S rRNA mimicry strategy supported by structural superposition with the 80S ribosome and sequence similarity between the I domain of the IRES and the h38 region of 28S rRNA (Fig. 4) is welljustified.

      We agree that the low resolution of the map has compromised the data interpretation at the molecular level, and we thank the reviewer for appreciating our findings at this resolution. Due to the compromise in resolution, we have reported findings related to stretches or regions such as loops and stems, rather than individual nucleotides and interactions.  

      (2) The lack of experimental validation of the functional importance of regions like the GNRA and RAAA loops is another limitation of this study.

      We agree with the lack of any additional experiments other than Cryo-EM for probing the importance of regions such as GNRA and RAAA loops in this study. However, we have cited earlier reports that demonstrate the importance of these regions for overall IRES activity. The essentiality of RAAA loop for type 2 IRES was demonstrated in earlier report López de Quinto and Martínez-Salas, 1997 (Cited in manuscript). Further, the conservation of this loop across the type 2 IRES family adds to the importance of this loop (Manuscript Figure 6B). This loop and its flanking G-C stem are similar to h38 of 28S rRNA, and it appears that RAAA loop adopts a mimicry mechanism to interact with the 40S ribosomal protein- uS19, thus highlighting its importance for interaction with 40S. Experiments destabilising the G-C stem also compromise IRES activity, as shown in the case of FMDV IRES (Fernández et al 2011). Previous studies related to the mutation of the GNRA or GCGA loop in EMCV IRES have shown a deficiency in IRES activity (Roberts and Belsham, 1997; Robertson et al 1999), suggesting the importance of these regions in the viral IRES biology, and these reports are cited in the manuscript. Not only EMCV IRES, but mutation in the GUAA (representative of GNRA) loop of FMDV IRES also showed significant reduction in IRES activity (López de Quinto and Martínez-Salas, 1997). In our study, we observe that GCGA loop interacts with tRNA<sub>i</sub> in EMCV IRES-48S PIC, thus implicating the importance of this loop. Moreover, incubation of FMDV IRES with 40S ribosomes has shown a decrease in SHAPE reactivity in domain 3 apex (position 170- 200 nucleotides) (Lozano et al 2018), which corresponds to EMCV IRES domain I apex. Further, we will attempt to address the concern of lack of experimental validation of GNRA and RAAA loops by performing biochemical assays.

      (3) Minor modifications related to data processing and biochemical studies will further validate and strengthen the findings.

      a) In the cryo-EM data section, the authors should include an image showing rejected particles during 2D classification. This would help readers understand why, despite having over 22k micrographs with sufficient particle distribution and good contrast, only a smaller number of particles were used in the final reconstruction. Additionally, employing mapsharpening tools such as Ewald sphere correction, Bayesian polishing, or reference-based motion correction might further improve the quality of the maps. Targeting high-resolution structures would be particularly informative.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestions, and we would employ suggested processes that may help improve the quality of the maps further. We will include image for rejected 2D classes in the revised manuscript. We agree with the Reviewer’s query related to the substantial number of micrographs and smaller number of particles for the final reconstruction. The total number of micrographs is the summation of multiple datasets, prepared and collected at various times. Among these, around 8000 micrographs have extremely poor particle number and distribution. As a result, the number of particles per micrograph is heterogeneous in the compiled dataset. We obtained only 237054 ‘good particles’ after multiple rounds of 2D & 3D classifications, and the final reconstruction has 28439 particles (~12%). This class was obtained after masked classification for IRES and ternary complex density. Hence, only the particles that show the best density for both IRES and ternary complex are used for reconstructing this map. Another set of particles that have only a portion of IRES and tRNA but NO density for eIF2 forms another map (26792 particles, 11.3%). Thus, we obtained a total of 55231 particles (23.3%) with IRES density.  

      b) The strategic modelling of different IRES domains into the density, particularly the domain into the region above the 40S head, is appreciable. However, providing the full RNA tertiary structure (RNAfold) of the EMCV IRES (nucleotides 280-905) would better explain the logic behind the model building and its molecular interpretation.

      We thank the reviewer for appreciating the modelling of the domain I apex in the cryo-EM density. We tried to predict the full tertiary structure of the IRES, however, inclusion of the full-length sequence from 280-905 gave models of extremely low confidence, and few domains do not abide by the secondary structure of EMCV IRES as reported in Duke et al 1992. Hence, we used individual domains of EMCV IRES and predicted the tertiary structure independent of other IRES domains. Furthermore, 3D models of FMDV IRES domains 2, 3, and 4 (corresponding to EMCV IRES domains- H, I, and J-K) were predicted from SHAPE reactivity values and RNAComposer server (Figure 3 in Lozano et al 2018). The predicted architecture of domain 3 apex (FMDV IRES) coincides with our I domain apex model (EMCV IRES).

      c)  Although the authors compare their findings with other types of IRESs (Types 1, 3, and 4), there is no experimental validation of the functional importance of regions like the GNRA and RAAA loops. Including luciferase-based assays or mutational studies of these regions for validation of structural interpretations is strongly recommended.

      We have discussed the possibility of how the other IRESs, such as type 1 and type 5 (Aichi virus), might use similar strategies as EMCV IRES to assemble the 48S PIC, given the similarity in the motif sequence and position across the viral IRESs. Like EMCV IRES, the type 1 IRES (e.g. Poliovirus, Coxsackie virus) also harbours the GNRA loop, preceded by a C-rich loop at its longest domain, known for long-range RNA-RNA interactions. The segment harbouring GNRA loop is highly conserved across the type 1 family of IRESs (Kim et al 2015).The Aichi viral IRES (type 5) harbours a GNRA loop in its longest domain, which is domain J. Deletion of the GNRA loop has compromised the IRES activity; however, substitution mutations in this region either elevated the IRES activity or it remained unaltered (Yu et al 2011). We have hypothesized that these IRESs (type 1 and type 5) might use the GNRA motifs in their longest domain (domain IV in type 1, and domain J in type 5) similar to that of EMCV IRES, where GNRA is present in the longest domain (I) and preceded by a C-rich loop. Thus, GNRA can potentially mediate long-range interactions with tRNA<sub>i</sub> as all these IRESs require eIF2-ternary complex for the formation of 48S PIC. Parallelly, like EMCV IRES, type 1 and type 5 IRESs also have similar placement of GNRA motif-containing domain before the eIF4G-binding domain (domain J-K in EMCV IRES, domain V in poliovirus, domain K in Aichi virus). Hence, we suggest the possibility of a similar strategy by these IRESs to interact with tRNA<sub>i</sub> during the formation of 48S PIC.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The field of protein translation has long sought the structure of a Type 2 Internal Ribosome Entry Site (IRES). In this work, Das and Hussain pair cryo-EM with algorithmic RNA structure prediction to present a structure of the Type 2 IRES found in Encephalomyocarditis virus (EMCV). Using medium to low resolution cryo-EM maps, they resolve the overall shape of a critical domain of this Type 2 IRES. They use algorithmic RNA prediction to model this domain onto their maps and attempt to explain previous results using this model.

      Strengths:

      (1) This study reveals a previously unknown/unseen binding modality used by IRESes: a direct interaction of the IRES with the initiator tRNA.

      (2) Use of an IRES-associated factor to assemble and pull down an IRES bound to the small subunit of the ribosome from cellular extracts is innovative.

      (3) Algorithmic modeling of RNA structure to complement medium to low resolution cryoEM maps, as employed here, can be implemented for other RNA structures.

      We thank Reviewer 2 for positive and encouraging comments on our work, appreciating our ‘innovative’ approach of using IRES-associated factor to assemble and pull down IRES-bound ribosomal complex.  

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Maps at the resolution presented prevent unambiguous modelling of the EMCV-IRES. This, combined with the lack of any biochemical data, calls into question any inferences made at the level of individual nucleotides, such as the GNRA loop and CAAA loop (Figure 4).

      We understand the concerns raised by the reviewer related to the resolution of the EMCV IRES-48S PIC map. However, we would like to mention that we refrained from commenting on individual nucleotides or molecular interactions in the manuscript. Instead, we discuss about loops, RNA stretches or motifs that could be inferred with more confidence as shown in Manuscript Figure 4. The EMCV IRES can directly interact with the 40S ribosome using its domain H and I (Chamond et al 2014), however, the details this interaction was unknown. We observe that the CAAA loop of domain I apex interacts with 40S ribosome based on the placement of portion of domain I in the cryo-EM map. This is also reflected in the earlier reported SHAPE data (Supplementary figures 2, and 8 in Chamond et al 2014), where a decrease in reactivity is evident in the presence of 40S ribosome. In addition, incubation of EMCV IRES with rabbit reticulocyte lysate (RRL) offered protection to domain I apex regions, which included the CAAA loop (Figure 4b in Maloney and Joseph, 2024).

      Furthermore, this decrease in SHAPE reactivity pattern is also evident for FMDV IRES domain 3 apex (like domain I in EMCV IRES) in the presence of 40S ribosome (Lozano et al 2018).

      Thus, these studies are consistent with the placement of IRES model in the cryo-EM map.

      We aim to improve the resolution of the maps for better clarity and add biochemical experiments to justify the possible interactions.

      (2) The EMCV IRES contains an upstream AUG at position 826, where the PIC can assemble (Pestova et al 1996; PMID 8943341). It is unclear if this start codon was mutated in this study. If it were not mutated, placement of AUG-834 over AUG-826 in the P-site is unexplained.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up this point, as we missed mentioning this in the manuscript. The EMCV IRES does not require scanning and directly positions the AUG-834 at the P site (Pestova et al 1996). In Pestova et al 1996, the intensity of the toeprint at AUG-834 is much more intense than that of AUG-826. Further, AUG-834 lies in the Kozak context, whereas AUG-826 has a poor Kozak context. Furthermore, the synthesis of the polypeptide requires placement of AUG-834 at the P site. In our cryo-EM map, we observed that the tRNA<sub>i</sub> is in a P<sub>IN</sub> state, which indicates the recognition of the start codon, and we reasoned that it is more likely that AUG-834 is placed at the P site than AUG-826. We will mention this in the revised manuscript, as we had NOT mutated AUG-826.

      (3) The claims the authors make about (i) the general overall shape and binding site of the IRES, (ii) its gross interaction with the two ribosomal proteins, (iii) the P-in state of the 48S, (iv) the rearrangement of the ternary complex are all warranted. Their claims about individual nucleotides or smaller stretches of the IRES-without any supporting biochemical data-is not warranted by the data.

      We thank the reviewer for warranting major claims, and we wish to make further improvements to support our assessment of small stretches and individual nucleotides.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Type II IRES, such as those from encephalomyocarditis virus (EMCV) and foot-and-mouth disease virus (FMDV), mediate cap-independent translation initiation by using the full complement of eukaryotic initiation factors (eIFs), except the cap-binding protein eIF4E. The molecular details of how IRES type II interacts with the ribosome and initiation factors to promote recruitment have remained unclear. Das and Hussain used cryo-electron microscopy to determine the structure of a translation initiation complex assembled on the EMCV IRES. The structure reveals a direct interaction between the IRES and the 40S ribosomal subunit, offering mechanistic insight into how type II IRES elements recruit the ribosome.

      Strengths:

      The structure reveals a direct interaction between the IRES and the 40S ribosomal subunit, offering mechanistic insight into how type II IRES elements recruit the ribosome.

      Weaknesses:

      While this reviewer acknowledges the technical challenges inherent in determining the structure of such a highly flexible complex, the overall resolution remains insufficient to fully support the authors' conclusions, particularly given that cryo-EM is the sole experimental approach presented in the manuscript.

      The study is biologically significant; however, the authors should improve the resolution or include complementary biochemical validation.

      We thank Reviewer 3 for acknowledging the technical challenges in this study and finding our study biologically significant. We understand the concerns related to low resolution and the requirement of complementary biochemical validation for our reported observations and interpretations in the manuscript. We are attempting to improve the resolution and complement the interpretations with biochemical experiments.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Joint Public Review:

      Summary:

      In this study, Daniel et al. used three cognitive tasks to investigate behavioral signatures of cerebellar degeneration. In the first two tasks, the authors found that if an equation was incorrect, reaction times slowed significantly more for cerebellar patients than for healthy controls. In comparison, the slowing in the reaction times when the task required more operations was comparable to normal controls. In the third task, the authors show increased errors in cerebellar patients when they had to judge whether a letter string corresponded to an artificial grammar.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the work is methodologically sound and the manuscript well written. The data do show some evidence for specific cognitive deficits in cerebellar degeneration patients.

      Thank you for the thoughtful summary and constructive feedback. We are pleased that the methodological rigor and clarity of the manuscript were appreciated, and that the data were recognized as providing meaningful evidence regarding cognitive deficits in cerebellar degeneration.

      Weaknesses:

      The current version has some weaknesses in the visual presentation of results. Overall, the study lacks a more precise discussion on how the patterns of deficits relate to the hypothesized cerebellar function. The reviewers and the editor agreed that the data are interesting and point to a specific cognitive deficit in cerebellar patients. However, in the discussion, we were somewhat confused about the interpretation of the result: If the cerebellum (as proposed in the introduction) is involved in forming expectations in a cognitive task, should they not show problems both in the expected (1+3 =4) and unexpected (1+3=2) conditions? Without having formed the correct expectation, how can you correctly say "yes" in the expected condition? No increase in error rate is observed - just slowing in the unexpected condition. But this increase in error rate was not observed. If the patients make up for the lack of prediction by using some other strategy, why are they only slowing in the unexpected case? If the cerebellum is NOT involved in making the prediction, but only involved in detecting the mismatch between predicted and real outcome, why would the patients not show specifically more errors in the unexpected condition?

      Thank you for asking these important questions and initiating an interesting discussion. While decision errors and processing efficiency are not fully orthogonal and are likely related, they are not necessarily the same internal construct. The data from Experiments 1 and 2 suggest impaired processing efficiency rather than increased decision error. Reaction time slowing without increased error rates suggests that the CA group can form expectations but respond more slowly, possibly due to reduced processing efficiency. Thus, this analysis of our data suggests that the cerebellum is not essential for forming expectations, but it plays a critical role in processing their violations.

      Relatedly, a few important questions remain open in the literature concerning the cerebellum’s role in expectation-related processes. The first is whether the cerebellum contributes to the formation of expectations or the processing of their violations. In Experiments 1 and 2, the CA group did not show impairments in the complexity manipulation. Solving these problems requires the formation of expectations during the reasoning process. Given the intact performance of the CA group, these results suggest that they are not impaired in forming expectations. However, in both Experiments 1 and 2, patients exhibited selective impairments in solving incorrect problems compared to correct problems. Since expectation formation is required in both conditions, but only incorrect problems involve a VE, we hypothesize that the cerebellum is involved in VE processes. We suggest that the CA group can form expectations in familiar tasks, but are impaired in processing unexpected compared to expected outcomes. This supports the notion that the cerebellum contributes to VE, rather than to forming expectations.

      In Experiment 3, during training, the participant is learning a novel rule (grammar), forming new expectations on how strings of letters should be. Afterwards, during testing, the participant is requested to identify if a novel string is following the rule or not. We examined sensitivity to distinguish between grammatical and non‐grammatical strings of letters, thus taking into account a baseline ability to identify expected strings. Additionally, both in the low‐similarity and highsimilarity conditions, there are expectations regarding whether the strings are following the rule or not. However, in the high‐similarity condition, there is more uncertainty regarding which strings are following the grammatical rule, as demonstrated in a lower sensitivity (d prime). Given the group differences only in the low similarity condition, these results suggest the CA group is impaired only when the rules are more certain. Given these results, we suggest that forming cognitive expectations is not necessarily dependent on the cerebellum. Rather, we propose that the cerebellum is critical for processing rule-based VE (detection or processing of detected errors) under conditions of more certainty. One remaining question for future studies is whether the cerebellum contributes to detection of a mismatch between the expectation and sensory evidence, or the processing of a detected VE. 

      We suggest that these key questions are relevant to both motor and non-motor domains and were not fully addressed even in the previous, well-studied motor domain. Importantly, while previous experimental manipulations17,19,40,94–96 have provided important insights regarding the cerebellar role in these processes, some may have confounded these internal constructs due to task design limitations (e.g., lack of baseline conditions). Notably, some of these previous studies did not include control conditions, such as correct trials, where there was no VE. In addition, other studies did not include a control measure (e.g., complexity effect), which limits their ability to infer the specific cerebellar role in expectation manipulation. 

      Thus, the current experimental design used in three different experiments provides a valuable novel experimental perspective, allowing us to distinguish between some, but not all, of the processes involved in the formation of expectations and their violations. For instance, to our knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate a selective impairment in rule-based VE processing in cerebellar patients across both numerical reasoning and artificial grammar tasks. If feasible, we propose that future studies should disentangle different forms of VE by operationalizing them in experimental tasks in an orthogonal manner. This will allow us to achieve a more detailed and well-defined cerebellar motor and non-motor mechanistic account.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Editors comments:

      The Figures are somewhat sub-standard and should be improved before the paper is made the VOR. Ensure consistent ordering of the group factor (CA, NT) and experimental factor across Figure 3,4, and 6 (panels A). Having the patient group as columns in Figure 4a and in rows in Figure 6a is very confusing.

      We have standardized the layout across Figures 2, 4, and 6 so that the group factor (CA, NT) and experimental conditions are consistently ordered. In all panels, the group factor now appears as a column.

      Subpanels should be numbered A,B,C... not A, B1, B2.

      Subpanel labels have been updated to follow the standard A, B, C format across all figures.

      Fonts should have a 100% aspect ratio - they should not be stretched (Figure 6B).

      We have corrected the font aspect ratios in all figures (e.g., Figure 6B) to ensure proper proportions and readability. 

      Colors should be more suitable to print - use a CYMK color scheme (i.e. avoid neon colors such as the neon green for the CA).

      The color scheme across all figures has been revised to be print-friendly using CMYKcompatible, colorblind-accessible palettes. Neon green for the CA group was replaced with a more muted, distinguishable color.

      Abstract: "The CA group exhibited a disproportionate cost when comparing expected problems compared to unexpected problems" - I recommend switching unexpected and expected, as the disproportional cost in on the former.

      We have changed the wording of the sentence accordingly. 

      Upon re-reading the details for the AGL task were not clear to us. Please do not rely on the reference (78) for the details - your paper should contain enough information to have the reader understand the experimental details. For you to appreciate the depth of our not-understanding, here a simple question: The test strings either followed the grammar in Fig 5 or they did not. If they did not, how exactly was similarity to the grammar measured? If they did, what was the difference between the “Grammatical-high” and “Grammatical-low” trials? If the string was grammatical, there should not be a notion of similarity, no? Or where these trials arbitrary split in half? 

      We have clarified that 50% of the test strings followed the grammar of the training strings. We also elaborated on the calculation of chunk strength as a measure of similarity between the training and testing strings, similar to the previous papers. The differences between low and high similarity are explained in the paper. Specifically, for each test string, we calculated chunk strength by summing the frequencies of all relevant substrings (e.g., bigrams and trigrams) that appeared in the training set. The test strings whose chunk‐strength values fell above the median for grammatical items were classified as “high similarity,” while those falling below the median were classified as “low similarity.” Also, grammatical strings can be of both low and high similarity; this is precisely the beautiful aspect of this experimental manipulation, showing the importance of uncertainty. We have utilized a 2 × 2 fully orthogonal design (grammaticality × similarity).

      Experimental details of the task should be added to the Method section. In the results you should only mention the experimental details that are necessary for understanding the experiments, but details such as the number of trials, etc, can be moved to the methods. 

      We have now moved the experimental task details to the Method sections.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the author):

      Studies have been done online and not in the lab. Could that have affected the results?

      We addressed this in the Methods section, referring to established protocols for online neuropsychological testing[9–12]. Our results align with similar in-lab findings in both the subtraction and AGL tasks, supporting the online approach's robustness. 

      Figure 2, B1; Figure 4, B1; Figure 6B: How many patients performed worse than the (worst-performing) controls? There appears to be quite some overlap between patients and controls. In the patients who performed worse, was there any difference from the other patients (e.g. disease severity as assessed by SARA score, repeat length, data of attention probes)?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful comment. We considered conducting individual-level comparisons to identify patients who performed worse than the lowest-performing controls. However, defining "worse" based on the performance of the lowest control is only one possible criterion. Other definitions—such as a specific number (1/2/3?) of standard deviations below the control mean—are also commonly used in literature, and each may yield different conclusions. This variability highlights the lack of a standardized threshold for what constitutes “worse” or "impaired" performance at the individual level. Given this ambiguity, and in line with prior studies that focus on average group differences rather than “impairment” prevalence, we chose not to include these individual-level comparisons. We believe this approach better aligns with the goals and design of the current study. That said, we agree that examining individual variability is important and may be more appropriate in future studies with larger samples so that percentage is a more robust measure. However, given the rarity of the disease, this would also be a challenge for future studies.  

      SARA ataxia scale does not include oculomotor function. In SCA6 oculomotor deficits are frequent, eg, downbeat nystagmus. Please include information on oculomotor dysfunction.

      We thank the reviewer for this important observation. While it is true that the SARA scale does not explicitly assess oculomotor function, our experimental design – in all three experiments – has control conditions that help account for general processing differences, including those that could arise from oculomotor deficits. These conditions, such as the correct trials and the complexity effects, allow us to isolate effects specifically related to the violation of expectation while minimizing the influence of broader performance factors, such as eye movement abnormalities. We also note that, while some patients can experience oculomotor symptoms such as downbeat nystagmus, none of our tasks required precise visual tracking or gaze shifts. In our experimental tasks, stimuli were centrally presented, and no visual tracking or saccadic responses were required. Moreover, the response time windows and stimulus durations (>2–5 s) were sufficient to mitigate the effects of delayed visual processing due to oculomotor impairment.

      Why was MoCA used and not the CCAS-Schmahmann scale to assess cognitive function?

      We selected the MoCA due to its broad clinical utility, time efficiency, and ability to detect mild cognitive impairment specifically in CA[101,102].  

      Were there any signs of depression in the patient group that could have affected the results?

      None of the patients had a clinical diagnosis of depression or were undergoing psychiatric treatment.  

      Additionally, the interaction between group and expectancy was insignificant when RT was the depended vaibale .." = variable

      This has been corrected to "variable" in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The terms 'unexpected' and 'expected' conditions are confusing. [...] Terming this 'violation of expectation' seems unnecessarily complicated to me. 

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important concern. We recognize that the terms "expected" and "unexpected" can be ambiguous without clarification, and that "violation of expectation" (VE) may initially appear unnecessary. Our choice to use VE terminology is grounded in an established theoretical framework that distinguishes between mere stimulus correctness and prediction mechanisms. Specifically, VE captures the internal processing of mismatches between anticipated and observed outcomes, which we believe is central to the cerebellar function under investigation. While simpler, technical alternatives (e.g., "correct" vs. "incorrect") could describe the stimuli, we find that VE more accurately reflects the mental constructs under study and is consistent with previous literature in both motor and cognitive domains. 

      Both tasks provide an error (or violation of expectation) that is non-informative and therefore unlikely to be used to update a forward model. The authors draw on motor literature to formulate a cognitive task where the presence of an error would engage the cerebellum and lead to longer reaction times in cerebellar patients. But in the motor domain, mismatch of sensory feedback and expectations would lead to an updating of the internal forward model. It seems unlikely to me in the arithmetic and alphabetic addition tasks that patients would update their internal model of addition according to an error presented at the end of each trial. If the error processed in these tasks will not lead to the updating of the internal forward model, can the authors discuss to what extent the cerebellum will be engaged similarly in these tasks, and what exactly connects cerebellar processing in these motor and cognitive tasks.

      We thank the reviewer for this thoughtful and important comment. We fully agree that the current tasks do not directly probe learning-related updating of internal models. As stated in the paper, the goal of the present study was not to support or refute a specific claim regarding the cerebellum’s role in learning processes. Rather, our focus was on examining cerebellar involvement in the processing of VE. While we were inspired by models from the motor domain, our design was not intended to induce learning or adaptation per se, but to isolate the processing of unexpected outcomes. We agree that the tasks in their current form are unlikely to engage forward model updating in the same way as in sensorimotor adaptation paradigms. That said, we believe the current findings can serve as a basis for future research exploring the relationship between cerebellar prediction error processing and learning over time. As we also noted in the paper, this is a direction we propose, and actively pursuing, in ongoing research work.

      The colour scheme is difficult for anyone with colour blindness or red-green visual impairment. Please adjust.

      All figures have been revised to use CMYK-compatible, colorblind-safe palettes, and neon colors have been removed.

      The introduction is a bit difficult to understand, because the authors draw on a number of different theories about cerebellar functioning, without clearly delineating how these relate to each other. For example: a) In the paragraph beginning with 'notably': If the cerebellum is required for sequential operations, why does it show the impairment with the rotation of the letters?

      We understand the concern that if the cerebellum is involved in sequential operations, its involvement in mental letter rotation, which can be assumed as “continuous transformation,” may appear contradictory. We note that the boundary between continuous and stepwise, procedural operations is not always clear-cut and may vary depending on the participant's strategy or previous knowledge, which is not fully known to the researchers. Furthermore, to our knowledge, prior work on mental rotation has not directly investigated the impact of VE during this task. However, these are two debatable considerations. 

      More importantly, a careful reading of our paper suggests that our experiments were designed to examine VE within tasks that involve sequential processing. Notably, we are not claiming that the cerebellum is involved in sequential or procedural processing per se. Rather, our findings point to a more specific role for the cerebellum in processing VE that arises during the construction of multistep procedural tasks. In fact, the results indicate that while the cerebellum may not be directly involved in the procedural process itself, it is critical when expectations are violated within such a context. This distinction is made possible in our study by the inclusion of a control condition (the complexity effect), which allows for a unique dissociation in our experimental design—one that, to our knowledge, has not been sufficiently addressed in previous studies.

      Additionally, in the case of arithmetic problem solving—such as the tasks used in prior studies cited in our manuscript21—there is substantial evidence that these problems are typically solved through stepwise, procedural operations. Arithmetic reasoning, used in Experiments 1 and 2, has been robustly associated with procedural, multi-step strategies, which may be more clearly aligned with traditional views of cerebellar involvement in sequential operations. Thus, we propose that the role of the cerebellum in continuous transformations should be further examined. 

      We suggest a more parsimonious theory that the cerebellum contributes to VE,  a field that was highly examined before. Yet, to reconcile ours and previous findings, we propose that the cerebellum’s contribution may not be limited to either continuous or stepwise operations per se, but rather to a domain-general process: the processing of VE. This theoretical framework can explain performance patterns across both mental rotation tasks and stepwise, procedural arithmetic.   

      The authors mention generation prediction as a function of the cerebellum, processing of prediction errors (or violations of expectations), sequentially, and continuous transformations - but it is unclear whether the authors are trying to dissociate these from each other or whether ALL of these functions have informed task design.

      We propose that the cerebellum’s contribution may not be limited to either continuous transformations or stepwise, procedural operations per se, but rather to a domain-general process: the processing of VE. We would like to clarify that we do not claim the cerebellum contributes to continuous transformations only, as suggested in some earlier work[21]. Rather, it could be that the cerebellum may contribute to continuous transformations, but we propose that it also supports multi-step, procedural processes. Given that framework, in the current study, across three separate experiments, we demonstrated that the cerebellum can also contribute to procedural, multi-step reasoning tasks.  

      Minor Comments

      Typo under paragraph beginning with 'notably' - cerebellum role should be cerebellar role.

      Corrected as suggested.

      When mentioning sequences as a recruiting feature for the cerebellum in the introduction, Van Overwalle's extensive work in the social domain should be referenced for completeness.

      Thank you for the suggestion. We have now cited Van Overwalle’s work on cerebellar involvement in sequence processing within the social domain in the revised Introduction.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      The manuscript by Bru et al. focuses on the role of vacuoles as a phosphate buffering system for yeast cells. The authors describe here the crosstalk between the vacuole and the cytosol using a combination of in vitro analyses of vacuoles and in vivo assays. They show that the luminal polyphosphatases of the vacuole can hydrolyse polyphosphates to generate inorganic phosphate, yet they are inhibited by high concentrations. This balances the synthesis of polyphosphates against the inorganic phosphate pool. Their data further show that the Pho91 transporter provides a valve for the cytosol as it gets activated by a decline in inositol pyrophosphate levels. The authors thus demonstrate how the vacuole functions as a phosphate buffering system to maintain a constant cytosolic inorganic phosphate pool. 

      This is a very consistent and well-written manuscript with a number of convincing experiments, where the authors use isolated vacuoles and cellular read-out systems to demonstrate the interplay of polyphosphate synthesis, hydrolysis, and release. The beauty of this system the authors present is the clear correlation between product inhibition and the role of Pho91 as a valve to release Pi to the cytosol to replenish the cytosolic pool. I find the paper overall an excellent fit and only have a few issues, including: 

      (1) Figure 3: The authors use in their assays 1 mM ZnCl2 or 1mM MgCl2. Is this concentration in the range of the vacuolar luminal ion concentration? Did they also test the effect of Ca2+, as this ion is also highly concentrated in the lumen? 

      The concentrations inside vacuoles can reach those values. However, given that polyP is a potent chelator of divalent metal ions, what would matter are the concentrations of free Zn<sup>2+</sup> or Mg<sup>2+</sup> inside the organelle. These are not known. This is not critical since we use those two conditions only as a convenient tool to differentiate Ppn1 and Ppn2 activity in vitro. In our initial characterisation of Ppn2 (10.1242/jcs.201061), we had also tested Mn, Co, Ca, Ni, Cu. Only Zn and Co supported activity. Ca did not. Andreeva et al. (10.1016/j.biochi.2019.06.001) reached similar conclusions and extended our results.

      (2) Regarding the concentration of 30 mM K-PI, did the authors also use higher and lower concentrations? I agree that there is inhibition by 30 mM, but they cannot derive conclusions on the luminal concentration if they use just one in their assay. A titration is necessary here. 

      The concentration of 30 mM was not arbitrarily chosen. It is the luminal P<sub>i</sub> concentration that the vacuoles could reach through when they entered a plateau of luminal Pi. We consider this as an upper limit because polyP kept increasing which luminal P<sub>i</sub> did not. Thus, there is in principle no physiological motivation for trying higher values. But we will probably add a titration to the revised version.

      (3) What are the consequences on vacuole morphology if the cells lack Pho91? 

      We had not observed significant abnormalities during a screen of the genome-wide deletion collection of yeast (10.1371/journal.pone.0054160)

      (4) Discussion: The authors do not refer to the effect of calcium, even though I would expect that the levels of the counterion should affect the phosphate metabolism. I would appreciate it if they would extend their discussion accordingly. 

      We will pick this up in the discussion. However, the situation is much more complex because major pools of counterions (up to hundreds of mM) are constituted by vacuolar lysine, arginine, polyamines, Mg, Zn etc. Their interplay with polyP is probably complex and worth to be treated in a dedicated project.

      (5) I would appreciate a brief discussion on how phosphate sensing and control are done in human cells. Do they use a similar lysosomal buffer system? 

      Mammalian cells have their Pi exporter XPR1 mainly on a lysosome-like compartment (10.1016/j.celrep.2024.114316). Whether and how it functions there for Pi export from the cytosol is not entirely clear. We will address this situation in the revision.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This manuscript presents a well-conceived and concise study that significantly advances our understanding of polyphosphate (polyP) metabolism and its role in cytosolic phosphate (Pi) homeostasis in a model unicellular eukaryote. The authors provide evidence that yeast vacuoles function as dynamic regulatory buffers for Pi homeostasis, integrating polyP synthesis, storage, and hydrolysis in response to cellular metabolic demands. The work is methodologically sound and offers valuable insights into the conserved mechanisms of phosphate regulation across eukaryotes. 

      Strengths: 

      The results demonstrate that the vacuolar transporter chaperone (VTC) complex, in conjunction with luminal polyphosphatases (Ppn1/Ppn2) and the Pi exporter Pho91, establishes a finely tuned feedback system that balances cytosolic Pi levels. Under Pi-replete conditions, inositol pyrophosphates (InsPPs) promote polyP synthesis and storage while inhibiting polyP hydrolysis, leading to vacuolar Pi accumulation. 

      Conversely, Pi scarcity triggers InsPP depletion, activating Pho91-mediated Pi export and polyP mobilization to sustain cytosolic phosphate levels. This regulatory circuit ensures metabolic flexibility, particularly during critical processes such as glycolysis, nucleotide synthesis, and cell cycle progression, where phosphate demand fluctuates dramatically. 

      From my viewpoint, one of the most important findings is the demonstration that vacuoles act as a rapidly accessible Pi reservoir, capable of switching between storage (as polyP) and release (as free Pi) in response to metabolic cues. The energetic cost of polyP synthesis-driven by ATP and the vacuolar proton gradient-highlights the evolutionary importance of this buffering system. The study also draws parallels between yeast vacuoles and acidocalcisomes in other eukaryotes, such as Trypanosoma and Chlamydomonas, suggesting a conserved role for these organelles in phosphate homeostasis. 

      Weaknesses: 

      While the manuscript is highly insightful, referring to yeast vacuoles as "acidocalcisome-like" may warrant further discussion. Canonical acidocalcisomes are structurally and chemically distinct (e.g., electron-dense, in most cases spherical, and not routinely subjected to morphological changes, and enriched with specific ions), whereas yeast vacuoles have well-established roles beyond phosphate storage. A comment on this terminology could strengthen the comparative analysis and avoid potential confusion in the field. 

      Yeast vacuoles show all major chemical features of acidocalcisomes. They are acidified, contain high concentrations of Ca, polyP (which make them electron-dense, too), other divalent ions, such as Mg, Zn, Mn etc, and high concentrations of basic amino acids. Thus, they clearly have an acidocalcisome-like character. In addition, they have hydrolytic, lysosome-like functions and, depending on the strain background, they can be larger than acidocalcisomes described e.g. in protists. We will elaborate this point, which is obvious to us but probably not to most readers, in the revised version.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Bru et al. investigated how inorganic phosphate (Pi) is buffered in cells using S. cerevisiae as a model. Pi is stored in cells in the form of polyphosphates in acidocalcisomes. In S. cerevisiae, the vacuole, which is the yeast lysosome, also fulfills the function of Pi storage organelle. Therefore, yeast is an ideal system to study Pi storage and mobilization. 

      They can recapitulate in their previously established system, using isolated yeast vacuoles, findings from their own and other groups. They integrate the available data and propose a working model of feedback loops to control the level of Pi on the cellular level. 

      This is a solid study, in which the biological significance of their findings is not entirely clear. The data analysis and statistical significance need to be improved and included, respectively. The manuscript would have benefited from rigorously testing the model, which would also have increased the impact of the study.

      It is not clear to us what the reviewer would see as a more rigorous test of the model.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Praegel et al. explore the differences in learning an auditory discrimination task between adolescent and adult mice. Using freely-moving (Educage) and head-fixed paradigms, they compare behavioral performance and neuronal responses over the course of learning. The mice were initially trained for seven days on an easy pure frequency tone Go/No-go task (frequency difference of one octave), followed by seven days of a harder version (frequency difference of 0.25 octave). While adolescents and adults showed similar performance on the easy task, adults performed significantly better on the harder task. Quantifying the lick bias of both groups, the authors then argue that the difference in performance is not due to a difference in perception, but rather to a difference in cognitive control. The authors then used neuropixel recordings across 4 auditory cortical regions to quantify the neuronal activity related to the behavior. At the single cell level, the data shows earlier stimulus-related discrimination for adults compared to adolescents in both the easy and hard tasks. At the neuronal population level, adults displayed a higher decoding accuracy and lower onset latency in the hard task as compared to adolescents. Such differences were not only due to learning, but also to age as concluded from recordings in novice mice. After learning, neuronal tuning properties had changed in adults but not in adolescent. Overall, the differences between adolescent and adult neuronal data correlates with the behavior results in showing that learning a difficult task is more challenging for younger mice.

      Strengths:

      The behavioral task is well designed, with the comparison of easy and difficult tasks allowing for a refined conclusion regarding learning across age. The experiments with optogenetics and novice mice are completing the research question in a convincing way.

      The analysis, including the systematic comparison of task performance across the two age groups, is most interesting, and reveals differences in learning (or learning strategies?) that are compelling.

      Neuronal recording during both behavioral training and passive sound exposure is particularly powerful, and allows interesting conclusions.

      Weaknesses:

      The presentation of the paper must be strengthened. Inconsistencies, missing information or confusing descriptions should be fixed.

      We have carefully re-read the manuscript and reviewed it for inconsistencies. We made several corrections in the figures. For example, we removed redundant lines from violin plots and statistics, applied consistent labels, matched y- and x-limits of graphics, and adjusted labels. We also clarified descriptions of some experiment by adding explanations to the text.

      The recording electrodes cover regions in the primary and secondary cortices. It is well known that these two regions process sounds quite differently (for example, one has tonotopy, the other not), and separating recordings from both regions is important to conclude anything about sound representations. The authors show that the conclusions are the same across regions for Figure 4, but is it also the case for the subsequent analysis? Comparing to the original manuscript, the authors have now done the analysis for AuDp and AUDv separately, and say that the differences are similar in both regions. The data however shows that this is not the case (Fig S7). And even if it were the case, how would it compatible with the published literature?

      To address this and previous concerns about regional differences, the manuscript now includes 4 figures (4-1, 4-3, 6-2, 7-1) and 5 supplemental tables (3,4, 5, 6, 8) that explicitly compare results across brain regions.

      Following the reviewer’s request for subsequent analysis, we now added a new supplemental figure (Fig. S6-2) and two new supplementary tables (Tables S5, S6). We show that similar to expert mice (supplementary Table 3, and supplementary Table 4), the firing properties of adolescent and adult novice mice differ across auditory subregions (supplementary Table 5). We also show that the different auditory subregions have different firing properties (supplementary Table 6). With respect to task engagement, we show that (similar to Fig. S4-2) the neuronal discriminability in different auditory subregions is similar in both novice and expert mice (Fig. S6-2).

      Following the comment on Fig. S7-1, we made three changes to the revised manuscript. First, we now highlight that the differences firing properties between adolescent and adult neurons in AUDp and AUDv were distinct, but not significantly different within age-group comparisons. Second, we clearly state that the learning related changes in the measured parameters are different between AUDp and AUDv. Note, however, the greater changes in adult neurons after learning remains consistent between AUDp and AUDv. Third, we softened our original claim but still highlighted the stronger learning-induced plasticity in adults.

      Regarding the concern that different regions should show different patterns due to their known differences (e.g. tonotopy). Of course we agree that different areas differ functionally (as shown in our own previous work and here as well). However, it is still plausible, and biologically reasonable, that developmental changes may proceed in a similar direction across different areas, even if their baseline coding properties differ.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to find out how and how well adult and adolescent mice discriminate tones of different frequencies and whether there are differences in processing at the level of the auditory cortex that might explain differences in behavior between the two groups. Adolescent mice were found to be worse at sound frequency discrimination than adult mice. The performance difference between the groups was most pronounced when the sounds are close in frequency and thus difficult to distinguish and could, at least in part, be attributed to the younger mice' inability to withhold licking in no-go trials. By recording the activity of individual neurons in the auditory cortex when mice performed the task or were passively listening as well as in untrained mice the authors identified differences in the way that the adult and adolescent brains encode sounds and the animals' choice that could potentially contribute to the differences in behavior.

      Strengths:

      The study combines behavioural testing in freely-moving and head-fixed mice, optogenetic manipulation and high density electrophysiological recordings in behaving mice to address important open questions about age differences in sound-guided behavior and sound representation in the auditory cortex.

      Weaknesses:

      For some of the analyses that the authors conducted it is unclear what the rationale behind them is and, consequently, what conclusion we can draw from them.

      We have carefully re-read the manuscript and reviewed it for analyses that lacked a clear rationale or conclusion. To address this, we have made several changes to clarify the reasoning and strengthen the interpretation of the results.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      It would have helped if the authors had highlighted the changes they made to the manuscript compared to the original version - especially since many replies to the reviewers' comments were as vague as "...we fixed some of the wording so it adheres to the data shown", or "we refined our interpretation", without further details.

      The revised version has improved substantially, and the main claims have been discussed in a more objective way. Important new analyses have been added to allow for a refined interpretation of the results. However, the presentation of the data could still be strengthened significantly (in response to comment A from last review).

      We apologize for the lack of detail in some of our previous responses. Our intention was to keep the replies concise, assuming that the side-by-side version with tracked changes would make the edits sufficiently clear. However, we understand the need for greater transparency. Thus, below we provide the following five lists describing the major changes: (1) List of specific reviewer recommendations, (2) list of corrections in figures, (3) list of clarity issues, (4) list of fixed mistakes, (5) list of new figures. We hope this breakdown makes the revisions clearer and more accessible.

      List of specific reviewer recommendations:

      l.108 mentions a significant change in the vertical line of Fig 1F - Could this significance be indicated and quantified in the figure?

      We quantified and indicated the significance of the vertical line in Fig. 1f and Fig. 1i.

      Fig.1G - the thick and thin lines should be defined, as well as the grey and white dots (same values for adolescents, not for adults).

      (a) We removed the thin inner lines from the violin plot. We define the bar (thick line) of the violin plot in an additional sentence in the methods section under data analysis (LL820-823). b) We adjusted the marker outlines in the adult data (Fig. 1G).

      the figure axis legends should be consistent (trails in Fig D vs # trails in Fig 1F)

      We adjusted the axis legend to # trials in Fig. 1D.

      l.110: is d' always calculated based on the 100 last trials of a session, or is it just for Figure 1F? -etc...

      d’ is always calculated based on the last 100 trials. To clarify this, we added a description in the methods section (L830).

      List of corrections in the figures:

      (1) We removed the internal lines from violin plots in throughout Fig. 1-7.

      (2) We removed the underline of the statistics throughout Fig. 1-7.

      (3) We consistently applied ‘adolescent’ and ‘adult’ figure labels and titles with lowercase letters throughout Fig. 1-7.

      (4) We applied consistent labelling of ‘time (ms)’ throughout Fig. 1-7.

      (5) We matched the size of dashed lines throughout Fig. 1-6.

      (6) We adjusted the x-label of Fig. 1d, Fig. S-1-1 a, Fig. 3c, Fig. 3h-i, Fig, 4d to ‘# trials’.

      (7) We removed the x-label of ‘Experimental Group’ from Fig. 1 to enhance consistency with other figures.

      (8) We removed misaligned dots from the violin plots in Fig. 1g, Fig. 2f, Fig. 3f,g.

      (9) We corrected the plot in Fig. S1-1b.

      (10) We adjusted the y-limits of Fig. S1-1c to be consistent with Fig. S1-1d,e.

      (11) We adjusted the x-labels and y-labels of Fig, 2, Fig. S3-1, Fig, S3-2 and Fig. 3b to ‘freq. (kHz)’.

      (12) We added the age of adolescent and adult mice to the schematic timeline in Fig. 2a.

      (13) We added a label of the reinforcement delay to the schematic trial structure in Fig. 3b.

      (14) We added within-group statistics to Fig. 3e and the figure legend.

      (15) We adjusted the x-label of Fig. 3d to ‘# sessions’.

      (16) We adjusted the x-label of Fig. 3d and Fig. S3-1b to ‘# licks’.

      (17) We changed the y-label in Fig. S3-1a, and Fig. S3-2d, e to ‘lick ratio’ to avoid confusion with the lick rate (Hz) that was calculated in Fig. 4 and Fig. 6.

      (18) We replaced the titles ‘CAMKII’ with ‘dTomato’ in Fig. S3-2 to correctly highlight that both the experimental and control injection were CAMKII injections.

      (19) We adjusted the x-labels and y-labels of Fig, 2, Fig. S3-1, Fig, S3-2 and Fig. 3b to ‘freq. (kHz)’.

      (20) We adjusted the y-label of Fig. S4-1c to ‘# neurons’.

      (21) We matched the x-ticks in Fig. 4e,f.

      (22) We matched the x-ticks in Fig. 6d-g.

      (23) We changed the x-label in Fig. 4g, S4-2 and S6-2 to ‘duration (ms)’ to match the figure label with the manuscript.

      (24) We consistently label ‘Hit’, ‘Miss’, ‘FA’ and ‘CR’ with capital letters in Fig. 4d-e.

      (25) We replaced the double figure label ‘C.’ in Fig. S4-2 with ‘D.’.

      (26) We adjusted the dot-size in Fig. 5 to be equal for all graphs.

      (27) We added ticks to the experimental timeline in Fig. 6a.

      (28) We corrected the y-label in Fig.7c. Now it correctly reflects 5 attenuations from 72-32 dB SPL.

      (29) We matched the y-label of Fig. 7e-h and Fig. S7-1.

      List of clarity issues:

      (1) We replaced the term ‘lower response bias’ with ‘higher lick bias’ (L24) to accurately describe the more negative (lower) criterion-bias, which highlights a higher tendency to lick.

      (2) We replaced the term ‘response bias’ with ‘lick bias’ to consistently describe the calculated criterion-bias (L24, L149, L164, L455, L456, L468).

      (3) We clarify that the age-related differences were ‘more pronounced’ instead of simply ‘higher’ to accurately reflect not simply the increase in adolescent lick-bias, but also the decrease in adult lick-bias (L31).

      (4) We clarified that adolescent sound representations are not merely ’distinct’, but ‘not fully mature’ in L83.

      (5) We clarified in L180 that the impulsive responses we observed in adolescent mice could be related to being ‘less impacted by punishments’.

      (6) We clarified the differences in firing properties of auditory sub-regions analyzed in Supplementary Table 3 (L287-295).

      (7) We explained and clarified the reference to Fig. 3j (LL252-253).

      (8) We added statistics to Fig.S4-2 to support our claim that there are no differences in the onset-latency, duration of discriminability and maximal discriminability between different sub-regions within age-groups (LL 314-315).

      (9) We expanded our explanation of the results in Table 3 (LL370-379).

      (10) We separated the reference to Fig. 6b and Fig. 6c to clarify their meaning (LL358-361).

      (11) We clarified the differences in basic firing properties during the FRA protocol in Fig. 7 (LL409-418).

      (12) We expanded our explanation of the differences of the learning related firing properties in AUDp and AUDv of Fig. S7-1 (LL426-433).

      (13) We changed the term ‘plasticity profiles’ to ‘learning related plasticity’ to further clarify our limitation that L5/6 and L2/3 may exhibit distinct learning related changes (L496).

      (14) We changed the term ‘sluggish’ (L481) to ‘delayed’ to more precisely explain differences between adolescent and adult tuning properties.

      (15) We clarified that the running d’ was calculated in bins of 25 trials, instead of ‘the last 25 trials’ (LL845-846).

      List of fixed mistakes:

      (1) We corrected and matched the age to more accurately reflect the age mice were recorded (P37-42 and P77-82).

      (2) We corrected the attenuation range from 72-42 to 72-32 dB SPL to correctly reflect the 5 attenuations used in the protocol.

      (3) We corrected the number of channels shown in the voltage trace from 10 to 11 (Fig. S4-1a)

      (4) We corrected the number of neurons recorded in novice adolescent mice in the legend of Fig. 6 from 140 to 130 (Fig. 6b).

      (5) We removed redundant, or double brackets, commas, dots, and semi-colons in the figure legends.

      (6) We corrected the LME statistics Table 2.

      List of new figures and tables:

      (1) We added a new supplementary figure to accompany Figure 6. Specifically, Fig. S6-2, shows the interaction of the three measured discriminability properties (onset delay, duration of discriminability, and maximal discriminability) in novice compared to expert mice in the easy and hard task (Go compared to No Go). The figure compares the different auditory sub-regions (similar to Fig. S4-2). We show that the discriminability properties within different groups is not significantly different among the four different sub-regions.

      (2) Supplementary Table 5: We compared the firing properties in different auditory subregions in novice mice, and found (similar to expert mice) that the firing properties differ between adult and adolescent mice across the four different sub-regions.

      (3) Supplementary Table 6: We compared the firing properties between different subregions, separately for adolescent and adult novice mice. Similar to expert mice, we found that different auditory subregions differ in their auditory firing properties.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors largely addressed my suggestions.

      Comparing hit vs correct rejection trials in the population decoding analysis (L313-314): The authors acknowledge that comparing these two trial types conflates choice and stimulus decoding but I am not convinced that the changes to the manuscript text make this clear enough to the reader.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have made additional revisions to clarify this, and other issues more explicitly, as follows:

      (1) We have expanded the explanation of how our population decoding analysis conflates stimulus and choice, and we acknowledge the limitations of this approach in the Abstract (L28), the Results section (L324-326, LL367-370) and the Discussion (LL516-519).

      (2) We replaced the analysis of impulsivity on the head-fixed task. Instead of analyzing all it is, we focus only on ITIs following FA trials (Fig. S3-1c,d). This is more consistent with the analysis in the Educage (Fig. S2-1), where we show that adolescents exhibit increased impulsivity after FA trials. We found a similar result for ITIs following FA trials in the head-fixed task.

      (3) To provide complementary insight, we now further justify our use of the Fisher separation metric alongside decoding accuracy in Figure 5, with a clearer rationale provided in LL343-345

      (4) We also clarified our reasoning for focusing on 62 dB SPL in the FRA-based analysis in LL400-403.

    1. Author response:

      We thank the reviewers and editors for their careful and constructive assessment of our manuscript. We have provided a provisional response to the eLife assessment and the reviewer’s public comments below, addressing their main concerns and outlining our planned revisions that we believe will substantially strengthen our paper.  

      eLife Assessment

      This study presents a valuable finding on the representational structure of task encoding in the prefrontal cortex. The evidence supporting the claims of the authors is solid, representing an impressive data collection effort and best-practice fMRI analyses. However, at least including visual regions as a control and controlling for behavioral differences in the task in representation analyses would have strengthened the study. The work will be of interest to cognitive neuroscientists interested in the neural basis of cognitive control.

      We plan to address both specific methodological weaknesses mentioned in the assessment in our forthcoming revision. First, the revision will include analyses of an early visual cortex ROI as an additional control region, allowing us to test whether the primary auditory cortex findings generalize to the sensory cortex across input modalities. Preliminary results indicate that the early visual cortex ROI exhibits a similar pattern of results, with evidence for coding both task-relevant and task-irrelevant visual dimensions across both tasks, as well as the context dimension specifically in the hierarchy task. Second, we will include behavioral performance as a covariate for the relevant statistical comparison across tasks to mitigate concerns over performance-related confounds. In addition, we will include a set of control analyses that demonstrate that equating the amount of data for pattern analyses across the two tasks by subsampling from the hierarchy task, while reducing our overall power, does not appreciably alter our results. We note that our analyses of representational geometries relied only on neural data from correct trials and, in the first-level modelling of the fMRI data, already controlled for differences in trial-by-trial response times. Therefore, our analyses of decoding and representation similarity are not directly affected by differences in performance across the two tasks. Finally, we have provided clarifications regarding Reviewer 2’s questions about the size and construction of the regions of interest employed in the study, as well as about the language employed to discuss null results.  

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Bhandari and colleagues present tour-de-force analyses that compare the representational geometry in the lateral prefrontal cortex and primary auditory cortex between two complex cognitive control tasks, with one having a "flat" structure where subjects are asked to form rote memory of all the stimulus-action mappings in the task and one having a "hierarchical" task structure that allows clustering of task conditions and that renders certain stimulus dimensions irrelevant for choices. They discovered that the lPFC geometry is high-dimensional in nature in that it allows above-chance separation between different dichotomies of task conditions. The separability is significantly higher for task-relevant features than task-irrelevant ones. They also found task features that are represented in an "abstract" format (e.g., audio features), i.e., the neural representation generalizes across specific task conditions that share this variable. The neural patterns in lPFC are highly relevant for behaviors as they are correlated with subjects' reaction times and choices.

      Strengths:

      Typically, geometry in coding patterns is reflected in single-unit firings; this manuscript demonstrates that such geometry can be recovered using fMRI BOLD signals, which is both surprising and important. The tasks are well designed and powerful in revealing the differences in neural geometry, and analyses are all done in a rigorous way. I am thus very enthusiastic about this paper and identify no major issues.

      I am curious about the consequence of dimensionality collapse in lPFC. The authors propose a very interesting idea that separability is critical for cognitive control; indeed, separability is high for task-relevant information. What happens when task-relevant separation is low or task-irrelevant separation is high, and will this lead to behavioral errors? Maybe a difference score between the separability of task-relevant and taskirrelevant features is a signature of the strength of cognitive control?

      We appreciate the reviewers’ positive evaluation of our paper.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors show a difference between flat and hierarchical tasks, but the two tasks are different in accuracy, with the flat task having more errors. Will this difference in task difficulty/errors contribute to the task differences in results reported?

      To address the Reviewer’s concern about the difference in behavioural performance between the two tasks influencing our results, we will take several approaches. First, we will include behavioral performance as a covariate for the relevant statistical comparison across tasks. This should ensure that any differences we observe across tasks are over and above those that can be explained by the difference in behavioral performance. Second, we will include a set of decoding analyses that control for differences in performance across the tasks. We note that all our analyses of representational geometries relied on neural data from correct trials only. In addition, the first-level modelling of the fMRI data already controlled for trial-by-trial variability in response times. Therefore, our decoding and representation similarity analyses should not directly be affected by differences in performance across the two tasks. However, one possible issue with this approach is that the larger number of errors in the flat task means that less data was available for estimating multivoxel patterns in the flat task compared to the hierarchy task, resulting in differential power to detect decoding effects across the two tasks. We note that the on average, this difference was not substantial: on average, 21.7 runs were available per participant for the flat task, while 23.8 runs per participant were available for the hierarchy task. Moreover, rerunning our analyses with the number of runs equated for each participant does not meaningfully alter the pattern of results. These additional analyses will be included in the supplement in the forthcoming revised manuscript.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors study the influence of tasks on the representational geometry of the lPFC and auditory cortex (AC). In particular, they use two context-dependent tasks: a task with a hierarchical structure and a task with a flat structure, in which each context/stimulus maps to a specific response. Their primary finding is that the representational geometry in the lPFC, in contrast to AC, aligns with the optimal organization of the task. They conclude that the geometry of representations adapts, or is tailored, to the task in the lPFC, therefore supporting control processes.

      Strengths:

      (1) Dataset:

      The dataset is impressive and well-sampled. Having data from both tasks collected in the same subjects is a great property. If it is publicly available, it will be a significant contribution to the community.

      (2) Choice of methods:

      The choice of analyses are largely well-suited towards the questions at hand - crosscondition generalization, RSA + regression, in combination with ANOVAs, are well-suited to characterizing task representations.

      (3) I found some of their results, in particular, those presented in Figures 4 and 5, to be particularly compelling.

      (4) The correlation analysis with behavior is also a nice result.

      We thank the reviewer for noting the strengths of the paper. We respond to the weaknesses noted below. 

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Choice of ROIs:

      A strength of fMRI is its spatial coverage of the whole brain. In this study, however, the authors focus on only two ROIs: the lPFC and auditory cortex. Though I understand the justification for choosing lPFC from decades of research, the choice of AC as a control feels somewhat arbitrary - AC is known to have worse SNR in fMRI data, and limiting a 'control' to a single region seems arbitrary. For example, why not also include visual regions, given that the task also involves two visual features?

      We agree with the reviewer that the whole-brain fMRI data certainly provide ample opportunities to explore the nature of these representations across the brain. Our focus in this paper is squarely on the principles of coding and flexibility in the lPFC. We believe that a whole-brain exploration addresses a separate question that would be out of the scope of this study. To clarify, we are not arguing that the lPFC is the only region in the brain that employs the coding principles that our study brings to light. Our contention is only that lPFC employs these principles, and it differs at least from the primary sensory cortex. The questions of whether these principles generalize beyond lPFC (quite likely) and, if so, how broadly, are distinct from the ones addressed in the manuscript. We intend to follow up with another manuscript that addresses these questions.

      Nevertheless, given the focus of this paper, we agree that a second control region, which allows one to test if the primary auditory cortex findings generalize to the sensory cortex more broadly, would strengthen our claims. We will include an early visual cortex ROI in our forthcoming revision. Preliminary results indicate that the early visual cortex ROI shows a similar set of findings – with evidence for coding of task-relevant and taskirrelevant visual dimensions across both tasks, but also specifically the context dimension in the hierarchy task. These results will be detailed in the forthcoming revision

      (2) Construction of ROIs:

      The choice and construction of the ROIs feel a bit arbitrary, as the lPFC region was constructed out of 10 parcels from Schaefer, while the AC was constructed from a different methodology (neurosynth). Did both parcels have the same number of voxels/vertices? It would be helpful to include a visualization of these masks as a figure.

      We defined the lPFC ROIs by selecting Schaefer parcels in the frontal lobe that were previously mapped onto the Control A resting state network identified by Yeo et al. (2011). This network aligns with the multiple-demand network, which has also been identified in the macaque, where it includes the lPFC regions that abut the principal sulcus. Prior results from these regions in the monkey brain provide the scientific premise for our hypotheses. The two lPFC ROIs in each hemisphere were constructed out of 5 Schaefer parcels in each hemisphere. These parcels cluster into the same functional network and tend to behave similarly in univariate analyses. Given that our hypotheses do not distinguish between the different parcels, we elected to improve power by merging them into left and right dlPFC ROIs. 

      On the other hand, the same approach could not be used to identify the primary auditory cortex. As Yeo et al. noted in their paper, the 17 resting state networks they identify did not adequately parcellate somatomotor and auditory cortices into distinct networks, likely due to their proximity (see Fig 14 and related text in Yeo et al. (2011)). We therefore relied on a different approach to define the primary auditory cortex, using an association test in Neurosynth to obtain a map of regions associated with the term “primary auditory”. In the revised manuscript, we will also include a primary auditory cortex ROI, defined again using a term-based association test in Neurosynth.

      Our lPFC ROIs and pAC ROIs are of similar size. In the left hemisphere, the lPFC ROI (constructed from merging Schaefer parcels 128-thru-132) has, on average, 624.55 voxels. The left pAC ROI (defined with Neurosynth) has, on average, 628 voxels. In the right hemisphere, the lPFC ROI (constructed from merging Schaefer parcels 330-thru334), has 470.8 voxels on average. The right pAC ROI has, on average, 568 voxels. A table reporting the size of our parcels and ROIs was included in the supplement. In our forthcoming revision, we will additionally include a supplementary figure visualizing the ROI masks. 

      (3) Task dimensionality:

      In some ways, the main findings - that representation dimensionality is tailored to the task - seem to obviously follow from the choice of two tasks, particularly from a normative modeling perspective. For example, the flat task is effectively a memorization task, and is incompressible in the sense that there are no heuristics to solve it. In contrast, the hierarchical task can have several strategies, an uncompressed (memorized) strategy, and a compressed strategy. This is analogous to other studies evaluating representations during 'rich' vs. 'lazy'/kernel learning in ANNs. However, it seems unlikely (if not impossible) to form a 'rich' representation in the flat task. Posed another way, the flat task will always necessarily have a higher dimensionality than the hierarchical task. Thus, is their hypothesis - that representational geometry is tailored to the task - actually falsifiable? I understand the authors posit alternative hypotheses, e.g., "a fully compressed global axis with no separation among individual stimulus inputs could support responding [in the flat task]" (p. 36). But is this a realistic outcome, for example, in the space of all possible computational models performing this task? I understand that directly addressing this comment is challenging (without additional data collection or modeling work), but perhaps some additional discussion around this would be helpful.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment, which gives us a chance to clarify our argument.

      As noted by the reviewer, whether a network takes advantage of the compressibility of a task depends on its learning regime (i.e. rich vs lazy). One way to frame our question regarding the lPFC’s coding strategy, then, is to ask whether it operates in a rich or a lazy learning regime (which would predict, respectively, task-tailored vs task-agnostic representations). The reviewer’s concern is that the two task structures we employed are differentially compressible, and therefore, it is inevitable that we observe tailored representations and therefore, our hypotheses are not falsifiable.

      First, it is important to clarify the theoretical premise behind our design and how it relates logically to our hypotheses. Under a lazy learning regime, a network would encode highdimensional representations of both tasks, regardless of their compressibility. On the other hand, under a rich learning regime, representational dimensionality will likely be shaped by the tasks’ structure. If the two tasks differ in their compressibility, only in the rich learning regime would the network learn representations of different dimensionality. Therefore, observing representations with dimensionality tailored to the task structure rules out the possibility that the lPFC is operating in a lazy regime. Therefore, the hypotheses are certainly testable.

      The second point of clarification is that, contrary to the reviewer’s assertion, the flat task is, in fact, compressible – the task can be solved with a categorical representation of the response categories, with no sensitivity to the different specific stimuli within each category. Indeed, it is possible to train a simple, three-layer feedforward artificial neural network to perform the flat task perfectly with only 2 units in the hidden layer, demonstrating this compressibility. While we agree with the reviewer that in the space of all possible architectures one might consider the two tasks may differ in compressibility, particularly at the local levels, as we noted above, this does not imply that our hypotheses are not testable.

      Finally, as a third point of clarification, our focus in this paper is on understanding the nature of coding in the lPFC in particular. Arguments based on a normative modelling perspective properly apply to the representations learned by an agent (such as an ANN or a human) as a whole. In a minimal feedforward ANN with a single hidden layer trained in a regime which encourages compression (i.e. a rich learning regime), it would indeed be the case that the representational dimensionality in that hidden layer would be higher for less compressible tasks. However, when applied to humans, such an argument applies to the brain as a whole rather than to an individual region of the brain like the lPFC. As such, it is less straightforward to predict how a single region might represent a task without additional information about the region’s inputs, outputs and broader position in a network. Even for a highly compressible task, a particular brain region may nevertheless be sensitive to all task dimensions. Conversely, even when a task is not compressible, a particular population within the brain may be invariant to some task features. For example, the primary auditory cortex is expected to be invariant to visual task dimensions.

      Therefore, how a task is represented in the lPFC in particular (as opposed to the whole brain) depends on its computational function and coding principles, which remain debated. For instance, as some accounts (such as the guided activation theory) posit, if the primary function of the lPFC is to encode ‘context’ and shape downstream processing based on context, we might only expect to see the abstract coding of the auditory context in the hierarchy task (and, perhaps, the response categories across both tasks as they encode the ’context’ for the lower-level response decision), while being invariant to lowerlevel features of the input. In our paper, we specifically contrast two accounts of lPFC coding that have emerged in the literature – one positing that the lPFC learns a representation tailored to the structure of the task, and another that the lPFC encodes a high-dimensional representation that privileges sensitivity to many task features and their non-linear mixture at the cost of generalization. Regardless of the compressibility of the tasks in question, how the lPFC encodes the two tasks is an empirical question.

      In our forthcoming revision, we will clarify these points in the discussion. We will also include the results of neural network simulations alluded to above.

      (4) Related to the above:

      The authors have a section on p. 27: "Local structure of lPFC representational geometry of the flat task shows high separability with no evidence for abstraction" - I understand a generalization analysis can be done in the feature space, but in practice, the fact that the flat task doubles as a memorization task implies that there are no useful abstractions, so it seems to trivially follow that there would be no abstract representations. In fact, the use of task abstractions in the stimulus space would be detrimental to task performance here. I could understand the use of this analysis as a control, but the phrasing of this section seems to indicate that this is a surprising result.

      As explained above, there is no need for high local separability in the flat task. The lPFC could have completely abstracted over the individual trial-types that contributed to each response category, encoding only the response categories. Indeed, as also noted above, it is possible to train a simple, three-layer feedforward artificial neural network to perform the flat task perfectly with only 2 units in the hidden layer. The two hidden layer units code for each of the two response categories. 

      (5) Statistical inferences:

      Throughout the manuscript, the authors appear to conflate failure to reject the null with acceptance of the null. For example, p. 24: "However, unlike left lPFC, paired t-tests showed no reliable difference in the separability of the task-relevant features vs the orthogonal, task-irrelevant features... Therefore, the overall separability of pAC representations is not shaped by either task-relevance of task structure."

      We thank the reviewer for pointing these out. These sentences will be corrected in the revision. For instance, the sentence above will be modified to “Therefore, we find no evidence that the overall separability of pAC representations is shaped by either taskrelevance or task structure.”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, Bhandari, Keglovits, et al. explore the representational structure of task encoding in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Through an impressive fMRI data-collection effort, they compare and contrast neural representations across tasks with different highlevel stimulus-response structures. They find that the lateral prefrontal cortex shows enhanced encoding of task-relevant information, but that most of these representations do not generalize across conditions (i.e., have low abstraction). This appears to be driven in part by the representation of task conditions being clustered by the higher-order task properties ('global' representations), with poor generalization across these clusters ('local' representations). Overall, this paper provides an interesting account of how task representations are encoded in the PFC.

      Strengths:

      (1) Impressive dataset, which may provide further opportunities for investigating prefrontal representations.

      (2) Clever task design, allowing the authors to confound several features within a complex paradigm.

      (3) Best-practice analysis for decoding, similarity analyses, and assessments of representational geometry.

      (4) Extensive analyses to quantify the structure of PFC task representations.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The paper would benefit from improved presentational clarity: more scaffolding of design and analysis decisions, clearer grounding to understand the high-level interpretations of the analyses (e.g., context, cluster, abstraction), and better visualizations of the key findings.

      (2) The paper would benefit from stronger theoretical motivation for the experimental design, as well as a refined discussion on the implications of these findings for theories of cognitive control.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of our paper and their feedback on the writing. We have reviewed these helpful suggestions with an eye to which we may implement in our revision to improve clarity. Our forthcoming revision will 1) provide clearer scaffolding to aid the reader in understanding our design, analyses and our interpretation of the results 2) incorporate the MDS-based visualization of the representational geometries, which is currently presented in the Supplement, as a figure panel in the main text, 3) provide a justification for the particular task structures we picked in the introduction and 4) incorporate a new paragraph in the Discussion section to highlight the implications of our findings for cognitive control.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      This paper describes technically-impressive measurements of calcium signals near synaptic ribbons in goldfish bipolar cells. The data presented provides high spatial and temporal resolution information about calcium concentrations along the ribbon at various distances from the site of entry at the plasma membrane. This is important information. Important gaps in the data presented mean that the evidence for the main conclusions is currently inadequate. 

      Strengths 

      The technical aspects of the measurements are impressive. The authors use calcium indicators bound to the ribbon and high speed line scans to resolve changes with a spatial resolution of ~250 nm and temporal resolution of less than 10 ms. These spatial and temporal scales are much closer to those relevant for vesicle release than previous measurements. 

      The use of calcium indicators with very different affinities and of different intracellular calcium buffers helps provide confirmation of key results. 

      Thank you very much for this positive evaluation of our work.

      Weaknesses 

      Multiple key points of the paper lack a statistical test or summary data from populations of cells. For example, the text states that the proximal and distal calcium kinetics in Figure 2A differ. This is not clear from the inset to Figure 2A - where the traces look like scaled versions of each other. Values for time to half-maximal peak fluorescence are given for one example cell but no statistics or summary are provided. Figure 8 shows examples from one cell with no summary data. This issue comes up in other places as well. 

      Thank you for this fair and valuable feedback. Following also the suggestion by the Editor, we have now removed the rise-time kinetic fitting results from the manuscript and only retain the bi-exponential decay time constant values. Further, we explicitly detail the issues with kinetic fitting, and state that the precise quantitative conclusions should not be drawn from the differences in kinetic parameters (pages 7 and 2728). 

      We have included the results of paired-t-tests to compare the amplitudes of proximal vs. distal calcium signals shown in Fig. 2A & B, Fig. 3C & D, Fig. 4C & D, Fig. 5A-D, and Fig. 8E&F. Because proximal and distal calcium signals were obtained from the same ribbons within 500-nm distances, as the Reviewer pointed out, “the traces look like scaled versions of each other”. For experiments where we make comparisons across cells or different calcium indicators, as shown in Fig. 3E & F, Fig.5E, and Fig. 8B&C, we have included the results of an unpaired t-test. We have also included the t-test statistics information in the respective figure legends in the revised version.

      In Figure 8, we have shown example fluorescence traces from two different cells at the bottom of the A panel, and example traces from different ribbons of RBC a in the D, and the summary data is described in B-C and E-F, with statistics provided in the figure legends.

      The rise time measurements in Figure 2 are very different for low and high affinity indicators, but no explanation is given for this difference. Similarly, the measurements of peak calcium concentration in Figure 4 are very different with the two indicators. That might suggest that the high affinity indicator is strongly saturated, which raises concerns about whether that is impacting the kinetic measurements. 

      Yes, we do believe that the high-affinity indicator is partially saturated, and therefore, the measurement with the low-affinity indicator dye is a more accurate reflection of the measured Ca<sup>2+</sup> signal. We now state this more explicitly in the text. Further, we note that the rise time values are no longer listed due to lack of statistical significance for such comparisons, as noted above.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The study introduces new tools for measuring intracellular Ca2+ concentration gradients around retinal rod bipolar cell (rbc) synaptic ribbons. This is done by comparing the Ca2+ profiles measured with mobile Ca2+ indicator dyes versus ribbon-tethered (immobile) Ca2+ indicator dyes. The Ca2+ imaging results provide a straightforward demonstration of Ca2+ gradients around the ribbon and validate their experimental strategy. This experimental work is complemented by a coherent, open-source, computational model that successfully describes changes in Ca2+ domains as a function of Ca2+ buffering. In addition, the authors try to demonstrate that there is heterogeneity among synaptic ribbons within an individual rbc terminal. 

      Strengths: 

      The study introduces a new set of tools for estimating Ca2+ concentration gradients at ribbon AZs, and the experimental results are accompanied by an open-source, computational model that nicely describes Ca2+ buffering at the rbc synaptic ribbon. In addition, the dissociated retinal preparation remains a valuable approach for studying ribbon synapses. Lastly, excellent EM. 

      Thank you very much for this positive evaluation of our work.

      Comments on revisions: 

      Specific minor comments: 

      (1) Rewrite the final sentence of the Abstract. It is difficult to understand. 

      Thank you for pointing that out. We have updated the final sentence of the Abstract.

      (2) Add a definition in the Introduction (and revisit in the Discussion) that delineates between micro- and nano-domain. A practical approach would be to round up and round down. If you round up from 0.6 um, then it is microdomain which means ~ 1 um or higher. Likewise, round down from 0.3 um to nanodomain? If you are using confocal, or even STED, the resolution for Ca imaging will be in the 100 to 300 nm range. The point of your study is that your new immobile Ca2-ribbon indicator may actually be operating on a tens of nm scale: nanophysiology. The Results are clearly written in a way that acknowledges this point but maybe make such a "definition" comment in the intro/discussion in order to: 1) demonstrate the power of the new Ca2+ indicator to resolve signals at the base of the ribbon (effectively nano), and 2) (Discussion) to acknowledge that some are achieving nanoscopic resolution (50 to 100nm?) with light microscopy (as you ref'd Neef et al., 2018 Nat Comm).  

      Thank you for the valuable comments. We have now provided this information in the introduction and discussion.  

      (3) Suggested reference: Grabner et al. 2022 (Sci Adv, Supp video 13, and Fig S5). Here rod Cav channels are shown to be expressed on both sides the ribbon, at its base, and they are within nanometers from other AZ proteins. This agrees with the conclusions from your imaging work.  

      Thank you for the valuable suggestion. We have now provided this information in the introduction and discussion.

      (4) In the Discussion, add a little more context to what is known about synaptic transmission in the outer and inner retina.. First, state that the postsynaptic receptors (for example: mGluR6-OnBCs vs KARs-OffBCs, vs. AMPAR-HCs), and possibly the synaptic cleft (ground squirrel), are known to have a significant impact on signaling in the outer retina. In the inner retina, there are many more unknowns. For example, when I think of the pioneering Palmer JPhysio study, which you sight, I think of NMDAR vs AMPAR, and uncertainty in what type postsynaptic cell was patched (GC or AC....). Once you have informed the reader that the postsynapse is known to have a significant impact on signaling, then promote your experimental work that addresses presynaptic processes: "...the new tool and results allow us to explore release heterogeneity, ribbon by ribbon in dissociated preps, which we eventually plan to use at ribbon synapses within slices......to better understand how the presynapse shapes signaling......". 

      Thank you for the valuable comments. We have now provided this information in the introduction and discussion.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this study, the authors have developed a new Ca indicator conjugated to the peptide, which likely recognizes synaptic ribbons and have measured microdomain Ca near synaptic ribbons at retinal bipolar cells. This interesting approach allows one to measure Ca close to transmitter release sites, which may be relevant for synaptic vesicle fusion and replenishment. Though microdomain Ca at the active zone of ribbon synapses has been measured by Hudspeth and Moser, the new study uses the peptide recognizing synaptic ribbons, potentially measuring the Ca concentration relatively proximal to the release sites. 

      Strengths: 

      The study is, in principle, technically well done, and the peptide approach is technically interesting, which allows one to image Ca near the particular protein complexes. The approach is potentially applicable to other types of imaging. 

      Thank you very much for this appreciation.

      Weaknesses: 

      Peptides may not be entirely specific, and genetic approach tagging particular active zone proteins with fluorescent Ca indicator proteins may well be more specific. Although the authors are aware of this and the peptide approach is generally used for ribbon synapses, the authors should be aware of this, when interpreting the results. 

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s point and believe the peptides and genetic approaches to measure local calcium signals have their merits, each with separate advantages and disadvantages.  

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The revisions helped with some concerns about the original paper, but some issues were not adequately addressed. I have left two primary concerns in my public review. To summarize those: 

      The difference in kinetics of proximal and distal locations is emphasized and quantified in the paper, but the quantification consists of a fit to the average responses. This does not give an idea of whether the difference observed is significant or not. Without an estimate of the error across measurements the difference in kinetic quoted is not interpretable. 

      Thank you for this feedback. Since the kinetics information is a minor part of the manuscript, we have followed the Editor’s advice to significantly tone down the comparison of kinetic fit parameters (completely removing the rise-time comparisons), in order to put more focus on the better-documented conclusions. We also note that we did establish statistical significance of the differences in fluorescence signal amplitudes. 

      Somewhat relatedly, the difference in amplitude and kinetics of the calcium signals measured with low and high affinity indicators is quite concerning. The authors added one sentence stating that the high affinity indicator might be saturated. This is not adequate. Should we distrust the measurements using the high affinity indicator? The differences between the results using the low and high affinity indicators is in some cases large - e.g. larger than the differences cited as a key result between distal and proximal locations. This issue needs to be dealt with directly in the paper. 

      Thank you for this feedback. Yes, the measurements from high-affinity indicators cannot report the Ca2+ as accurately as low-affinity indicators. However, the value of HA indicators is in their ability to detect lowamplitude signals that lower-affinity indicators may miss due to lower signal-to-noise resolution.  We added a sentence on page 12 to further stress this point.

      Related to the point about statistics, it is not clear how to related the horizontal lines in Figure 8 to the actual measurements. It is critical for the evaluation of the conclusions from that figure to understand what is plotted and what the error bars are on the plotted data. 

      We apologize for the earlier ambiguity in Fig. 8. In this figure, we first compare proximal (panel B) and distal (panel C) calcium signals across several RBCs, labeled RBC-a through RBC-d. Each RBC contains multiple ribbons, and for each cell, we present the average calcium signals from multiple ribbons using box plots in panels B and C. In these box plots, the horizontal lines represent the average calcium signal for each cell, while the size of the error bars reflects the variability in proximal and distal calcium signals among the ribbons within that RBC.

      For example, RBC-a had five identifiable ribbons. In panels D–F, we use RBC-a to illustrate the variability in calcium signals across individual ribbons. Specifically, we distinguished proximal and distal calcium signals from five ribbons (ribbons 1–5) within RBC-a. When feasible, we acquired multiple x–t line scans at a single ribbon, shown now as individual data points, to assess variability in calcium signals recorded from the same ribbon.

      The box plots in panels E and F display the average calcium signal (horizontal lines) for each ribbon, based on multiple recordings. These plots demonstrate considerable variability between ribbons of RBC-a. Importantly, the lack of or minimal error bars for repeated measurements at the same ribbon indicates that the proximal and distal calcium signals are consistent within a ribbon. These findings emphasize that the observed variability among ribbons and among cells reflects true biological heterogeneity in local calcium domains, rather than experimental noise.

    1. Author response:

      We thank all three anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful evaluations of our manuscript and for recognizing the conceptual advance in combining agent-based behavioral simulations with systems neuroscience models. We are especially encouraged by the acknowledgement of the framework’s potential to support simulation of neural control of individual animal behavior in realistic sensory environments.

      Below, we respond to each reviewer’s public comments in turn. Throughout, we have aimed to clarify our rationale for modeling choices, acknowledge limitations, and outline concrete steps for improvement in the revised manuscript.

      Furthermore, the call for a better description of the model implementation as voiced by all three reviewers and additional requests from community members has prompted us to formulate a separate technically detailed description of the publicly available larvaworld software package as well as of the readily implemented models in form of a preprint paper (Sakagiannis et al., 2025, bioRxiv, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1101/2025.06.15.659765).

      Reviewer #1:

      We are happy to read that this reviewer considers the proposed behavioral architecture ‘a significant step forward in the field’, and that she/he recognizes the strengths of our work in the modular and hierarchical approach that provides connections to influential theories of motor control in the brain, in the experimental evidence it is based on, and in the valuable abstractions that we have chosen for the larval behavioral modeling.

      The reviewer raises important points about the simplifications we have made, both conceptually and in the specific implementation of larval behaviors. Our main goal in this study is to introduce a conceptual framework that integrates agent-based modeling with systems neuroscience models in a modular fashion. To serve this purpose, we aimed for a minimal yet representative implementation at the motor layer of the architecture, calibrated to larval locomotion kinematics. This choice enables efficient simulation while allowing us to test top-down modulation and adaptive mechanisms in higher layers without the computational overhead of a full neuromechanical model. In addition to chemotaxis, we have recently used this simplified approach to model thermotaxis in larvae (Kafle et al., 2025, iScience, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2025.112809).

      The reviewer notes the absence of explicit segmental neuromuscular control or central pattern generators (CPGs). We deliberately abstracted from these mechanisms, representing the larval body as two segments with basic kinematic control, to focus on reproducing overall locomotor patterns. This bisegmental simplification, which we illustrate in Supplemental Video “Bisegmental larva-body simplification”, retains the behavioral features relevant to our current aims. However, the modular structure of the framework means that more detailed neuromechanical models—incorporating CPG dynamics or connectome-derived circuit models—can be integrated in future work without altering the architecture as a whole.

      We fully agree that real neural circuits are more complex than a strict subsumption architecture implies. In the Drosophila larva, there is clear evidence for ascending sensory feedback from the motor periphery to premotor and higher brain circuits, as well as neuromodulatory influences. These add layers of complexity beyond the predominantly descending control in our present model. At the same time, both larval and adult connectome data show that across-level descending and ascending connections are sparse compared to the dense within-layer connectivity. We see value in casting our model as a hierarchical control system precisely to make the strengths and limitations of such an abstraction explicit. The revised manuscript will include further discussion of these points.

      In summary, our design choices reflect a trade-off: by limiting the biological detail in the lower layers, we gain computational efficiency and maintain a clear modular structure that can host models at different levels of abstraction. This ensures that the architecture remains both a tool for immediate behavioral simulation and a scaffold for integrating richer neural and biomechanical models as they become available.

      Reviewer #2:

      We thank the reviewer for recognizing the novelty of our locomotory model, particularly the implementation of peristaltic strides based on our new analyses of empirical larval tracks, and for providing constructive feedback that will help us improve the manuscript.

      The reviewer highlights the need for clearer explanations of the chemotaxis and odor preference modules. We expand these sections in the revised manuscript with more explicit descriptions of model structure, parameterization, and calibration. As mentioned above, we have also prepared a separate preprint dedicated to the larvaworld Python package, which contains detailed implementation notes and hands-on tutorials that allow users to adapt or extend individual modules.

      Regarding the comparison to empirical behavior in chemotaxis, our present analysis is indeed primarily qualitative. However, we would like to emphasize that the temporal profile of odor concentration at the larval head in our simulations matches that measured in Gomez-Marin et al. (Nature Comm., 2011, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms1455) using only one additional free parameter, while all parameters of the basic locomotory model had been fitted to a separate exploration dataset before and were kept fixed in the chemotaxis experiments. In addition to the simulation of chemotaxis in the present paper, we recently used larvaworld in a practical model application to estimate a species-specific parameter of thermotaxis from experiments across different drosophilids (Kafle et al., 2025, iScience, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2025.112809).

      The preference index in our simulations was computed using the same definition as in the established experimental group assay for larval memory retention, enabling a direct quantitative comparison between simulated and empirical results. Variability in the simulated outcomes arose naturally from inter-individual differences in body length and locomotory parameters, derived from real larval measurements, as well as from the random initial orientation of each individual in the arena. These factors contributed to variation in individual tracks and ultimately produced preference index values that closely matched those observed experimentally. In the revised manuscript, we also discuss handedness, as highlighted by the reviewer, as another meaningful expression of inter-individual variability in Drosophila larvae and insects more generally.

      Finally, we acknowledge the reviewer’s concern about the scalability and broader applicability of the model. While the present paper focuses on three specific behavioral paradigms (exploration, chemotaxis, odor preference), the modular structure of the architecture is designed for flexibility: modules at any layer can be exchanged for more detailed or alternative implementations, and new sensory modalities or behaviors can be integrated without redesigning the system. The larvaworld package, associated codebase, and documentation are openly available to encourage adoption and adaptation by the larval research community.

      Reviewer #3:

      This public review provides an excellent account of our central aim to build an easily configurable, well-documented platform for organism-scale behavioral simulation and we are happy to read that the reviewer considers this an excellent goal.

      We thank the reviewer for her/his account of our well-organized code using contemporary Python tooling. We are currently further improving code readability and code documentation, and we will release a new version of the larvaworld Python package. We further agree with the reviewer’s assessment that understanding the model calibration currently requires reading of the appendix. For the revised manuscript we thus aim at improving our description of all calibration and modeling steps along the way. We will also make sure to improve the description of the experimental datasets used for calibration.

      We recognize that our description of the paper’s scientific contribution could be clearer. In revision, we will sharpen the Introduction and Discussion to highlight our main contributions:

      (1) Promoting a shift from isolated neural circuit modeling to integrated agent-based simulations in realistic environments.

      (2) Proposing the layered behavioral architecture, adopting the subsumption paradigm for modular integration.

      (3) Providing the larvaworld software as a ready-to-use, extensible modeling platform.

      (4) Implementing an empirically calibrated locomotory model and demonstrating its integration with navigation and learning modules in replicated behavioral paradigms.

      We agree with the reviewer that the next challenge is to integrate the empirically based behavioral simulations presented here with functional brain models capable of reproducing or predicting experimental findings at the level of cellular neurophysiology, including the effects of cell-type-specific manipulations such as gene knock-down or optogenetic activation/inhibition. However, based on our experience with systems-level modeling, we deliberately invested in behavioral simulation because functional models of the nervous system—including our own—often lack translation into simulated agent behavior. In many cases, model output is limited to one or more variables that can at best be interpreted as a behavioral bias, and most often represents an “average animal” that fails to capture inter-individual differences. By linking our spiking mushroom body model to behavioral simulations in a group of individual agents during memory retention tests (Figure 6C,D), we were able to achieve a first successful direct comparison between simulated and experimental behavior metrics—in this case, the behavioral preference index reported in Jürgensen et al. (iScience, 2024, DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.isci.2023.108640).

      Finally, we reiterate that the layered behavioral architecture is designed to promote a modular modeling paradigm. Our adoption of a subsumption architecture does not conflict with the concept of behavioral primitives; on the contrary, the notion that such primitives follow (semi-)autonomous motor programs and can be combined into more complex behaviors was the starting point for our implementation of the architecture in the fly larva. In our view, a genuinely contradictory paradigm for neural control of behavior would require a non-modular, strictly non-hierarchical organization of the nervous system and, by extension, of behavioral control.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review)

      Comment 

      Koonce et al. have generated a web-based visualization tool for exploring C. elegans neuronal morphology, contact area between neurons, and synaptic connectivity data. Here, the authors integrate volumetric segmentation of neurons and visualization of contact area patterns of individual neurons generated from Diffusion Condensation and C-PHATE embedding based on previous work from adult volumetric electron microscopy (vEM) data, extended to available vEM data for earlier developmental stages, which effectively summarizes modularity within the collated C. elegans contactomes to date. Overall, NeuroSC's relative ease of use for generating visualizations, its ability to quickly toggle between developmental stages, and its integration of a concise visualization of individual neurons' contact patterns strengthen its utility.

      We thank that reviewer for this positive assessment of our work.

      Comment

      NeuroSC provides an accessible and convenient platform. However, many of the characteristics of NeuroSC overlap with that of an existing tool for visualizing connectomics data, Neuroglancer, which is a widely-used and shared platform with data from other organisms. The authors do not make clear their motivation for generating this new tool rather than building on a system that has already collated previous connectomics data. Although the field will benefit from any tool that collates connectomics data and makes it more accessible and user-friendly, such a tool is only useful if it is kept up-to-date, and if data formatting for submitting electron microscopy data to be added to the tool is made clear. It is unclear from this manuscript whether NeuroSC will be updated with recently published and future C. elegans connectomes, or how additional datasets can be submitted to be added in the future.

      We have added new language to more explicitly state the motivations for developing NeuroSC (Introduction, lines 98-111, and discussion lines 375-384). In a new discussion section, we also include comparisons of the features of NeuroSC with other existing tools, like Neuroglancer and Webknossos, (lines 393-417).

      Briefly, the functional features of NeuroSC are substantially different (and do not exist) in other web-based tools for navigating EM datasets, including NeuroGlancer. This is because the intended use of NeuroSC is substantially different (and purposefully synergistic) to the intended use, and tools available, in NeuroGlancer. 

      NeuroGlancer is a versatile tool designed primarily for web-based visualizations and sharing of large EM datasets. NeuroSC was not designed to enable this type of access to the primary EM data (purposefully done because these features were already available through tools like NeuroGlancer). 

      Instead, the explicit goal of NeuroSC is to provide a platform specifically optimized for examining neuronal relationships across connectomic datasets. NeuroSC builds on the segmentations emerging from programs like NeuroGlancer, but the tools are tailored to explore relationships such as contact profiles in the context of neuronal morphologies and synaptic positions, and across datasets that represent different animals or different developmental stages. 

      To achieve this, all datasets in NeuroSC were optimized to facilitate comparisons across different connectomes of segmented neuronal features, including: 1) alignment of the neurons that are compared upon the display of the segmentations; 2) synchronization of the 3D windows; 3) implementation of a ‘universal color code’ across datasets for each neuron and relationship for easy visual comparisons; 4) use of the specific neuronal names to label instances of the same cells across all available datasets. The use of precise neuronal names among separate data sets allows integration of these objects with other catalogued datasets, including genomic and neuronal activity profiles.

      The formatting and display of the datasets used in NeuroSC was accompanied by the development of new tools including: 1) Rendering of the contact profiles of all neurons in the context of the morphology of the cell and the synapses and 2) C-PHATE diagrams to inspect multidimensional relationship hierarchies based on these contact profiles. In NeuroSC, C-PHATEs can be navigated and compared across multiple stages of development while visualizing neuronal reconstructions, allowing users to compare neuronal relationships across individual datasets.

      We agree with the reviewer that these tools are most useful when integrated. With that intention in mind, we designed NeuroSC as a series of modular, open-source tools that could be integrated into other programs, including Neuroglancer. In that sense our intent was not to produce another free-standing tool, but a set of tools that, if useful, could be integrated to other existing web-based connectomic resources to enhance the user experience of navigating complex EM datasets and draw biological meaning from the relationships between the neurons. Additionally, we intentionally designed NeuroSC to enable the ability to integrate new methods of understanding neuron relationships as they arise. We have dedicated a more detailed section to the discussion (lines 369- 417) to better convey this intention and directly address the unique abilities of NeuroSC as a complementary tool to the powerful existing tools, including Neuroglancer.

      Comment

      The interface for visualizing contacts and synapses would be improved with better user access to the quantitative underlying data. When contact areas or synapses are added to the viewer, adding statistics on the magnitude of the contact area, the number of synapses, and the rank of these values among the neuron's top connections, would make the viewer more useful for hypothesis generation. Furthermore, synapses are currently listed individually, with names that are not very legible to the web user. Grouping them by pre- and postsynaptic neurons and linking these groups across developmental stages would also be an improvement.

      [what do they even mean by linking?]

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment and have implemented several improvements to address these suggestions. Specifically, we have added new features to enhance user access to quantitative data within the NeuroDevSCAN viewer:

      Cell, Patch, and Synapse Statistics: Users can now see a statistics panel when clicking on a rendered neuron, contact patch, or a synapse. These panels provide the following information, respectively, and are highlighted in lines 303-315):

      Cell Stats: Click on a cell rendering to show cell stats which displays the total volume and surface area of the selected neuron within the defined neuropil area of our datasets (see Methods). 

      Contact Stats: Click on a patch rendering to show ‘contact stats’. This pop up displays quantifications of the selected contact relationship. Rank compares the summed surface area of contacts ("patches") between these two neurons relative to all other contact relationships for the primary neuron for the cell and the whole nerve ring. A rank of 1, for example, means this neuron pair shares the largest contact surface area of the examined relationship. “Total surface area” is displayed in nanometers, and is the summed surface area of all patches of this identity. Contact percentages are presented in two ways: (1) as the proportion of the primary cell's total surface area occupied by the contact in question, and (2) as the proportion of the total surface area of the nerve ring occupied by that same contact. (Showcased in figure S5). 

      Synapse Stats: A click on a synapse rendering now shows ‘synapse stats’, which displays the number of synapses of the selected identity within the primary neuron, including any polyadic synapse combinations involving the primary neurons. (Showcased in figure S7).

      (1) Grouping and Readability Improvements: While individual synapses are still visualized, their display has been improved for legibility. We have condensed the lengthy naming scheme to improve clarity and codified the synapse type by using superscript letters C, E, U to represent chemical, electrical and undefined synapses, respectively. This is explained and shown in figure S7, we added arrows to indicate the directionality of presumed information flow at each synapse. 

      (2) Developmental Linkage: We can link objects across datasets via cellular identity, but each synapse in the dataset does not yet have an identity attributed to its spatial coordinates, preventing us from linking specific synapses across development beyond their connectivity (ie, that a given synapses connects cell X to cell Y, for instance), also addressed in R1.11.  

      Together, these improvements substantially enhance the utility of the viewer for hypothesis generation by making key quantitative data readily accessible.

      Comment

      While the DC/C-PHATE visualizations are a useful tool for the user, it is difficult to understand when grouping or splitting of cell contact patterns is biologically significant. DC is a deterministic algorithm applied to a contactome from a single organism, and the authors do not provide quantitative metrics of distances between individual neurons or a number of DC iterations on the C-PHATE plot, nor is the selection process for the threshold for DC described in this manuscript. In the application of DC/C-PHATE to larval stage nerve ring strata organization shown by the authors, qualitative observations of C-PHATE plots colored based on adult data seem to be the only evidence shown for persistent strata during development (Figure 3) or changing architectural motifs across stages (Figure 4). Quantitation of differences in neuron position within the DC hierarchy, or differences in modularity across stages, is needed to support these conclusions. Furthermore, illustrating the quantitative differences in C-PHATE plots used to make these conclusions will provide a more instructive guide for users of NeuroSC in generating future hypotheses.

      There are several ways to visualize DC outputs, and one way to quantitatively compare DC clustering events of neurons is via Sankey diagrams. To make the inclusion of these resources more clear, we have highlighted them in lines 175-178 (Supplemental Tables 3-6). ‘DC outputs for each strata across animals can also be inspected using Sankey diagrams (Supplemental Tables 3-6). These spreadsheets detail the neuron members at each iteration of DC, allowing the user to derive quantitative comparisons of clustering events.’

      As the reviewer points out, DC is a deterministic algorithm that will iteratively cluster neurons based on the similarity of their contact profiles. To better explain the selection process for the threshold, the number of DC iterations and the quantitative metrics between the neurons, we have added new text in the Diffusion Condensation methods section.  Briefly:

      Number of DC iterations: During diffusion Condensation (DC) we track the modularity of the resulting clusters at each iteration and select the iteration with the highest modularity to define the clusters that represent the strata  (Moyle et al., 2021), (Brugnone et al., 2019). Mathematically, modularity is calculated by comparing the actual number of edges within clusters to the expected number of such edges in a randomized network with the same degree distribution (Newman et al., 2006). A higher modularity value implies that nodes within the same cluster are more densely connected to each other than to nodes in other clusters. We now better explain this in lines 562-567.

      Threshold for merging points: The threshold (epsilon) used to merge data points in each iteration is set as a small fraction of the spatial extent of the data: for each coordinate dimension (x, y, z), we compute the range (maximum minus minimum), take the maximum of these three values, and divide it by 10,000. This process is performed iteratively for each round of clustering until all data points cluster into a single point. We have updated the manuscript to clarify this threshold selection and included this information in the revised algorithm description and pseudocode. We now better explain this in lines 556-559.

      Distances between neurons in DC C-PHATE: In our previous description in Box 1 algorithm 1, we had provided a general algorithm for DC for any high dimensional dataset. We have now revised the algorithm to indicate how we used DC for these EM datasets. 

      Distances between neurons are determined by the pixel overlap between their segmented shapes in the EM dataset. We use these distances to build a graph with weighted edges, in which the weight of the edge represents the pixel overlap (the adjacency in the actual EM segmentation). Affinities between neurons, which are a proxy for their distance in the graph, are then computed as now revised in Box 1, Algorithm 1. This process is done iteratively as neurons cluster. To better communicate this, we have changed the text in lines 533-538.  

      Comment

      R1.5. While the case studies presented by the authors help to highlight the utility of the different visualizations offered by the NeuroSC platform, the authors need to be more careful with the claims they make from these correlative observations. For example, in Figure 4, the authors use C-PHATE clustering patterns to make conclusions about changes in clustering patterns of individual neurons across development based on single animal datasets. In this and many other cases presented in this study with the limited existing datasets, it is difficult to differentiate between developmental changes and individual variability between the neurite positions, contacts, and synapse differences within these data. This caveat needs to be clearly addressed.

      We now better explain in the manuscript that the selected case study, of the AVF neuron outgrowth, is not one of just correlation based solely on an EM dataset. Instead, the case study represents the NeuroSC-driven exploration of a biologically significant event supported by several independent datasets, as now explained in lines 257-276.

      Briefly, we agree with the reviewer that examining differences across individual EM datasets is insufficient evidence to make conclusions about developmental changes. But the strength of NeuroSC is in its ability to combine and compare multiple datasets, bolstering observations that are not possible by looking at just one dataset, and providing new insights on the way to new hypotheses. We now better explain that we are not looking at single connectomes in isolation and then deriving conclusions, but instead using NeuroSC to compare across 9 EM datasets. We better explain how the tools in NeuroSC, including C-PHATE, enabled comparisons across these multiple connectomes to identify apparent differences in neuronal relationships. We then explain that by using NeuroSC, we could examine these variations in neuronal relationships at the level of individual, cell biological differences of neuronal morphologies between the developmental datasets. This could be due, as pointed by the reviewer, to differences due to development, or just differences between individual animals. In the case of AVF, that features are absent in all early specimens, then arise and persist in all specimens after a certain time point, which lead us to hypothesize they result from a developmental event. Because the segmented objects in NeuroSC are linked to neuronal identities, we are also able to cross reference our observations from the EM datasets with information in other datasets and the literature. In the specific case of postembryonic development of AVF outgrowth, we can now tie the knowledge, from developmental lineage information and molecular profiles, that AVF is a postembryonically born neuron (Sulston et al. 1977, Sun et al 2022, Poole et al 2024, wormatlas.org) to the outgrowth dynamics of its neurites using the postembryonic EM datasets. Our findings using  NeuroSC provide a proof of concept of the utility of the resource and extended our understanding of how the outgrowth of this neuron affects the relationships between the neural circuits in the nerve ring.

      Comment

      R1.6. Given that recent studies have also quantified contact area between neurons across multiple connectomes (Cook et al., Current Biology, 2023; Yim et al., Nature Communications, 2024), and that the authors use a slightly different approach to quantify contact area, a direct comparison between contact area values obtained in this study with prior studies seems appropriate.

      We acknowledge that there are multiple different approaches to calculate adjacencies. In the papers cited above, there are 3 different algorithms used:

      (1) Brittin 2019 (python parse Track EM, boundary thresholds), used in Cook et al 2023, Moyle 2021, and this study).

      (2) Witvliet 2021 (Matlab 2D masks), used in Cook et al 2023.

      (3) Yim 2024 (3D masks), used in Yim et al 2024.

      To briefly describe the different approaches, and the methods we chose for this paper:

      Algorithm 1 (used in this study) defines adjacency based on distances between boundary points in TrakEM2 segmentations, allowing threshold tuning to accommodate differences in resolution and image quality across datasets—an important feature for consistent cross-dataset comparisons.

      Algorithm 2 infers contact via morphological dilation of VAST segmentations, identifying adjacency through overlapping expanded boundaries. 

      Algorithm 3 uses voxelwise contact detection with directional surface area measurements and normalization to account for dataset size differences. 

      In NeuroSC, we use algorithm 1, mostly because we had tested the rigor of this method in (Moyle et al. 2021), where we have shown that results were robust across a range of thresholds. This flexibility enables tailored application across datasets of varying quality and scale, critical for NeuroSC’s mission of curating data sets across differing methodologies to allow for direct relationship comparisons. We detail the methodology for defining thresholds for each dataset in methods section lines 492-521, defined in Supplementary table 1. Another difference between our analysis and the previously cited work is that for our analysis we also chose to include all individually resolved neurons, including post-embryonic cells, without collapsing them into left/right or dorsal/ventral symmetry classes. In this way our approach retains the full cellular resolution of the nervous system. 

      Comment

      Neuroglancer is not mentioned at all in the manuscript, despite it being a very similar and widely accepted platform for vEM data visualization across model organisms. An explicit comparison of NeuroSC and Neuroglancer would be appropriate, given the similarity of the tools. Currently, published C. elegans data (Witvliet et al., 2021; Yim et al., 2024) use Neuroglancer-based viewers, and directly comparing NeuroSC and highlighting its strengths relative to Neuroglancer would strengthen the paper.

      In the original manuscript we had not mentioned tools like Neuroglancer because we envisioned them as distinct, in intended use and output, from NeuroSC. But, as explained in R1.2 comment, in the revised version we have included a section in the Introduction lines 98-108 and in the Discussion (lines 369- 417) that compares these types of web-based tools and highlights synergies. 

      Comment

      Assigning shorthand names to strata, such as "shallow reflex circuit" (page 4, line 172), may oversimplify this group of neurons. Either more detailed support for shorthand names of C-PHATE modules should be included, or less speculative names for strata should be used.

      We appreciate this comment and understand that the original language used in the manuscript to describe strata categorizations may run the risk of oversimplification. We have now clarified the text to communicate that: 1) Strata are labeled by numbers (Strata 1, Strata 2, Strata 3 and Strata 4), rather than functional features of the neurons forming part of the strata, and that 2) the assignment of ‘strata’ is just one level of classification available via DC/CPHATE (as explained below). 

      To be sure, we have observed and published (Moyle et. al. Nature 2021) that within a given stratum, many neurons share the functional identities that we have used as summary descriptors for the strata (eg, shallow reflex circuits for Stratum 1; sensory and integrative circuits in Strata 3 and Strata 4; command interneurons in Strata 2, etc). However, those cell types are not the only members of the strata. We have adjusted the language in lines 197-204 to reflect this more clearly. “Stratum 1, which contains most neurons contributing to shallow reflex circuits that control aversive head movements in response to noxious stimuli, displayed the fewest changes among the developmental connectomes (Figure 3B–F; Supplementary Table 3). In contrast, C. elegans exhibit tractable behaviors that adapt to changing environmental conditions (Flavell et al., 2020). Strata 3 and 4 contain most neurons involved in circuits associated with such learned behaviors, including mechano- and thermo-sensation. This is reflected in Strata 3 and 4 showing the most change in neuronal relationships across postembryonic development.“

      Comment

      The authors state that NeuroSC can be applied to other model organisms. Since model organisms with greater neuron numbers include more individual neurons per cell class, the authors should support this by quantitatively demonstrating how DC/C-PHATE relationships correlate with shared functional roles among C. elegans neurons.

      We now clarify in the manuscript that, like in other organisms, C. elegans neurons are also grouped into functional classes with shared characteristics. In the context of the cylindrical nerve ring of the animal, these neuronal classes are sometimes bilaterally symmetric (forming left-right pairs), four-fold symmetric and six-fold symmetric. We now explain in the discussion that the DC/CPHATE analyses group these neuron classes and their relationships (lines 442-451). In the specific section mentioned by the reviewer, we now also add new text to contextualize this concept and how it might relate to the possible use of these tools in organisms with larger nervous systems: ‘However, our previous work has demonstrated that DC/CPHATE clustering of C. elegans neurons consistently pulls out clusters of shared neuron classes and shared functional roles Moyle et al. (2021). Building on this foundation, we envision applying similar clustering approaches to larger connectomes, aiming to identify classes and functionally related neuronal groups in more complex nervous systems. We suggest that contact profiles, along with neuron morphologies and synaptic partners, can act as ‘fingerprints’ for individual neurons and neuron classes. These ‘fingerprints’ can be aligned across animals of the same species to create identities for neurons. Frameworks for systematic connectomics analysis in tractable model systems such as C. elegans are critical in laying a foundation for future analyses in other organisms with up to a billion-fold increase in neurons (Toga et al., 2012).’

      Comment

      Lack of surface smoothing in NeuroSC leads to processes sometimes appearing to have gaps, which could be remedied by smoothing with a surface mesh. 

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion, and understand the visibility of gaps in certain neuron processes can be distracting. But this was an intentional choice, with our main goal being to show the most accurate representation of the available data segmentation and avoid any rendering interpretations. In this way, we render the data with the highest fidelity we can and as close as possible to the ground truth of the EM segmentation. We have added language to describe this in the methods, lines 490-491, and in Figure legend 5b.

      Comment

      Toggling between time points while maintaining the same neurons and contact area in NeuroSC is a really valuable feature. The tool would be improved even more by extending this feature to synapses, specifically by allowing the user to add an entire group of synapses to the viewer at once (e.g. "all synapses between AIM and PVQ"), and to keep this synapse group invariant when toggling between developmental stages.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. In response we have now implemented a new feature to ‘clone’ a rendered scene across time while preserving the original elements to ease comparisons. Once the user has rendered a scene, they can use the in-viewer developmental slider to clone the renderings and assigned colors, but display the renderings of the newly selected timepoint. These renderings populate a new window tab which can be dragged to align developmental stage windows side by side. We have added a sentence to account for this in lines 315-317 and to the legend of supplemental Figure S11. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review)

      Comment

      The ability to visualize the data from both a connectomics and contactomics perspective across developmental time has significant power. The original C. elegans connectome (White et al., 1986) presented their circuits as line drawings with chemical and electrical synapses indicated through arrows and bars. While these line drawings remain incredibly useful, they were also necessary simplifications for a 2D publication and they lack details of the complex architecture seen within each EM image. Koonce et al take advantage of segmented image data of each neuronal process within the nerve ring to create a web interface where users can visualize 3D models for their neuron of choice. The C-PHATE visualization allows users to explore similarities among different neurons in terms of adjacency and then go directly to the 3D model for these neurons. The 3D models it generates are beautiful and will likely be showing up in many future presentations and publications. The tool doesn't require any additional downloading and is open source.

      We thank that reviewer for this positive assessment of our work.

      Comment

      While it's impossible to create one tool that will satisfy all potential users, I found myself wanting to have numbers associated with the data. For example, knowing the number of connections or the total surface area of contacts between individual neurons wasn't possible through the viewer, which limits the utility of taking deep analytical dives. While connectivity data is readily accessible through other interfaces such as Nemanode and WormWiring, a more thorough integration may be helpful to some users.

      We thank the reviewer for this feedback and in response have now implemented displays with quantitative information in NeuroSC. Now, upon hovering over a contact patch or synapse, the user will see the quantitative data of the relationship. For contact patches, you will see the total area shared between two neurons in that dataset. On hovering over a synapse, you will see how many synapses there are in total with the same members and throughout the dataset. We agree that this improves user analyses, (see also R1.3 response).

      Comment

      There were several issues with the user interface that made it a bit clunky to use. For example, as I added additional neurons to the filter search box, the loading time got longer and longer. I ran an experiment uploading all of the amphid neurons, one pair at a time. Each additional neuron pair added an additional 5-10 seconds to the loading. By the time I got to the last pair, it took over a minute to load. Issues like these, some of which may be unavoidable given the size of the data, could be conveyed through better documentation. I did not find the tutorial very helpful and the supplementary movies lacked any voiceover, so it wasn't always clear what they were trying to show.

      We appreciate that some of the more complex models can take a while to load. One of our core goals is to keep the high resolution of our models to most accurately represent the EM data, so we had to compromise between resolution and loading times. But to address this concern we have now added a ‘loading’ prompt that reassures the user when there is a wait. We also added, as suggested, text guidance throughout all of the supplemental videos (Supplemental Videos 1-4).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review)

      Comment

      A web-based app, NeuroSC, that individual researchers can use to interrogate the structure and organization of the C. elegans nerve ring across development In the opinion of this reviewer, only minor revisions are required.

      We thank that reviewer for this positive assessment of our work.

      Comment

      Contact is defined by length, why not contact area? How are these normalized for changes in the overall dimensions of neurons during development?

      To clarify our methodology: the adjacency algorithm that we use generates a 2D adjacency profile by summing the number of adjacent boundary points per EM section, which are then summed across all EM z slices.

      Contact area can be derived by multiplying the adjacency length in each slice by pixel resolution and z-thickness. Prompted by the reviewer we have now also calculated and display contact surface areas, along with their ranks among all contact relationships for a given neuron. These can be inspected directly via the interface by clicking on a rendered cell or contact patch (Figure S5 and lines 308-312). We believe these additional surface area metrics enhance the interpretability and utility of the viewer.

      We apply normalization at the level of the adjacency threshold to account for dataset-specific differences such as contrast, boundary definition, and age-related changes in neuropil packing density. This normalization is applied before running the adjacency algorithm. We do not normalize by individual neuron size, as the contact data are intended to reflect relational differences between neurons, rather than absolute morphological scaling. In fact, our addition of a scale-spheroid within each rendered model emphasizes the large increase in spatial scale that the nerve ring experiences during larval growth.  

      Comment

      Figure 1, C&D, explanation unclear for how the adjacency matrix is correlated with C-Phate schematic in D.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment and have clarified this section by adding greater detail to the explanation of how an adjacency matrix is computed (lines 149-155), as well as a description now in the figure legend 1C. Additionally, we revised Figure 1C and D to simplify neuron representations/colors and to simplify the adjacency heat map gradient. We also extended the area of contact between neurons on Figure 1C to better reflect what would be considered a “contact”. Lastly, in the figure, we changed the color and placement for the z plane arrow and label from black to white, to make it more visible, to highlight the method of computing adjacency for each z slice. 

      Comment

      Figure 4, panels F & G, unclear why AVF is shown in panel G (L3) but not panel F (L1). Explanation (see below) should be provided earlier, i.e., AVF is not generated until the end of the L1.

      We have now clarified this important point by adding labels to Figure 4 panels F and G, ‘Pre-AVF outgrowth’ and ‘Post-AVF outgrowth’ respectively. Briefly, the point is that AVF grows into the nerve ring after the L2 stage, and that is why it is absent in panel F (L1 stage, now with the label ‘Pre-AVF outgrowth’).  

      Comment

      Line 146 What is the justification for the statement: "By end of Larval Stage 1 (L1), neuronal differentiation has concluded...."? This statement is confusing since this sentence also states that "90% of neurons in the neuropil...have entered the nerve ring..." which would suggest that at least 10% additional NR neurons have NOT fully differentiated.

      We have fixed this sentence in the text. Now the sentence reads ‘By Larval stage 1 (L1) 90% of the neurons in the neuropil (161 neurons out of the 181 neurons) have grown into the nerve ring and adopted characteristic morphologies and positions. 

      Lines 171-175 What is meant by the statement that "degree of these changes mapped onto...plasticity? What are examples of "behavioral plasticity?"

      We have added the following new lines of text (lines 200-204) and now additionally cite a review discussing C. elegans behaviors to clarify and give context to behavioral plasticity. ‘C. elegans exhibit tractable behaviors which can adapt due to changing environmental conditions  (Flavell et. al. Genetics 2020). Strata 3 and 4 contain most neurons belonging to circuits associated with such learned behaviors, including chemo, mechano and thermo sensation. This is seemingly reflected by strata 3 and 4 harboring the most readily recognized set of changes in neuronal relationships across postembryonic development.’  

      Comment

      Lines 189-190 The meaning of this sentence is unclear, "The logic in....merge events."

      This sentence has been deleted and we have instead refocused our descriptions of C-PHATES comparisons by neuronal clustering trajectories and cluster members (rather than iterations).

      Comment

      Lines 193-208 This section reports varying levels of convergence across larval development in C-Phate maps for the interneurons AIML and PVQL. Iterations leading to convergence varied: 16 (L1), 14 (L2), 22 (L3), 20 (l4), 14 (adult). The authors suggest that these differences are biologically significant and reflect the reorganization of AIML and PVQL contact relationships especially between the L4 and adult. Are these differences in iterations significant?

      We agree this could be confusing and instead of focusing on comparing the iteration at which each merging event occurs, we now focus on examining the differences in members of clusters, before and after the merge event. Cluster membership is easier to interpret than the differences in the number of DC iterations (lines 224-229).

      Lines 240-241 States that AVF neurons "terminally differentiate in the embryo" which is not correct. AVF neurons are generated from neuronal precursors (P0 and P1) at the end of the L1 stage which accounts for their outgrowth into the NR during the L2 stage. 

      We thank the reviewer for the correction and have edited the text to read: ‘AVF neurons are generated from neuronal precursors (P0 and P1) at the end of the L1 stage (Sulston et al. (1983); Sun and Hobert (2023); Poole et al. (2024); Hall and Altun (2008); Sulston and Horvitz (1977). AVF neurons do not grow into the nerve ring until the L2 stage, and continue to grow until the Adult stage (lines 261-266).’

      Comment

      Lines 289-315. A detailed and highly technical description of website architecture would seem more appropriate for the Methods section.

      We agree and have moved this section to the methods as suggested (lines 663-690).

      Comment

      Line 307 "source data is" should be "source data are"

      Thank you- we have fixed this grammatical error.

      Comment

      Line 324 "circuits identities" should be "circuit identity".

      Thank you- we have fixed this grammatical error.

      Comment

      Trademark/copyright conflict with these sites? https://compumedicsneuroscan.com/about/ https://www.neuroscanai.com/

      We thank the reviewer for drawing our attention to this. To avoid potential conflicts, we have proactively altered the name to NeuroSC throughout the paper.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.\

      Reviewer #1(Public review):

      (1) Changes in blood volume due to brain activity are indirectly related to neuronal responses. The exact relationship is not clear, however, we do know two things for certain: (a) each measurable unit of blood volume change depends on the response of hundreds or thousands of neurons, and (b) the time course of the volume changes are slow compared to the potential time course of the underlying neuronal responses. Both of these mean that important variability in neuronal responses will be averaged out when measuring blood changes. For example, if two neighbouring neurons have opposite responses to a given stimulus, this will produce opposite changes in blood volume, which will cancel each other out in the blood volume measurement due to (a). This is important in the present study because blood volume changes are implicitly being used as a measure of coding in the underlying neuronal population. The authors need to acknowledge that this is a coarse measure of neuronal responses and that important aspects of neuronal responses may be missing from the blood volume measure.

      The reviewer is correct: we do not measure neuronal firing but use blood volume as a proxy for bulk local neuronal activity, which does not capture the richness of single neuron responses. This is why the paper focuses on large-scale spatial representations as well as cross-species comparison. For this latter purpose, fMRI responses are on par with our fUSI data, with both neuroimaging techniques showing the same weakness. We have now added this point to the discussion: 

      “Second, we used blood volume as a proxy for local neuronal activity. Thus, our signal ignores any heterogeneity that might exist at the level of local neuronal populations. However, our main findings are related to the large-scale organization of cortical responses and how they relate to those of humans. For this purpose, the functional spatial resolution of our signal, driven by the spatial resolution of neurovascular coupling, should be adapted. In addition, using hemodynamic signals provides a much better comparison with human fMRI data, where the same limitations are present.”

      (2) More importantly for the present study, however, the effect of (b) is that any rapid changes in the response of a single neuron will be cancelled out by temporal averaging. Imagine a neuron whose response is transient, consisting of rapid excitation followed by rapid inhibition. Temporal averaging of these two responses will tend to cancel out both of them. As a result, blood volume measurements will tend to smooth out any fast, dynamic responses in the underlying neuronal population. In the present study, this temporal averaging is likely to be particularly important because the authors are comparing responses to dynamic (nonstationary) stimuli with responses to more constant stimuli. To a first approximation, neuronal responses to dynamic stimuli are themselves dynamic, and responses to constant stimuli are themselves constant. Therefore, the averaging will mean that the responses to dynamic stimuli are suppressed relative to the real responses in the underlying neurons, whereas the responses to constant stimuli are more veridical. On top of this, temporal following rates tend to decrease as one ascends the auditory hierarchy, meaning that the comparison between dynamic and stationary responses will be differently affected in different brain areas. As a result, the dynamic/stationary balance is expected to change as you ascend the hierarchy, and I would expect this to directly affect the results observed in this study.

      It is not trivial to extrapolate from what we know about temporal following in the cortex to know exactly what the expected effect would be on the authors' results. As a first-pass control, I would strongly suggest incorporating into the authors' filterbank model a range of realistic temporal following rates (decreasing at higher levels), and spatially and temporally average these responses to get modelled cerebral blood flow measurements. I would want to know whether this model showed similar effects as in Figure 2. From my guess about what this model would show, I think it would not predict the effects shown by the authors in Figure 2. Nevertheless, this is an important issue to address and to provide control for.

      We understand the reviewer’s concern about potential differences in response dynamics in stationary vs non-stationary sounds. It seems that the reviewer is concerned that responses to foregrounds may be suppressed in non-primary fields because foregrounds are not stationary, and non-primary regions could struggle to track and respond to these sounds. Nevertheless, we observed the contrary, with non-primary regions overrepresenting non-stationary (dynamic) sounds, over stationary ones. For this reason, we are inclined to think that this explanation cannot falsify our findings. 

      We understand the comment that temporal following rates might differ across regions in the auditory hierarchy and agree. In fact, we do show that tuning to temporal rates differs across regions and partly explains the differences in background invariance we observe. In this regard, we think the reviewer’s suggestion is already implemented by our spectrotemporal model, which incorporates the full range of realistic temporal following rates (up to 128 Hz). The temporal averaging is done as we take the output of the model (which varies continuously through time) and average it in the same window as we used for fUSI data. When we fit this model to the ferret data, we find that voxels in non-primary regions, especially VP (tertiary auditory cortex), tend to be more tuned to low temporal rates (Figure 2F, G), and that background invariance is stronger in voxels tuned to low rates. This is, however, not true in humans, suggesting that background invariance in humans relies on different computational mechanisms. We have added a sentence to clarify this: “The model included a range of realistic temporal rates and this axis was the most informative to discriminate foregrounds from backgrounds.”

      (3) I do not agree with the equivalence that the authors draw between the statistical stationarity of sounds and their classification as foreground or background sounds. It is true that, in a common foreground/background situation - speech against a background of white noise - the foreground is non-stationary and the background is stationary. However, it is easy to come up with examples where this relationship is reversed. For example, a continuous pure tone is perfectly stationary, but will be perceived as a foreground sound if played loudly. Background music may be very non-stationary but still easily ignored as a background sound when listening to overlaid speech. Ultimately, the foreground/background distinction is a perceptual one that is not exclusively determined by physical characteristics of the sounds, and certainly not by a simple measure of stationarity. I understand that the use of foreground/background in the present study increases the likely reach of the paper, but I don't think it is appropriate to use this subjective/imprecise terminology in the results section of the paper.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment that the classification of our sounds into foregrounds and backgrounds is not verified by any perceptual experiments. We use those terms to be consistent with the literature (McWalter and McDermott, 2018; McWalter and McDermott, 2019), including the paper we derived this definition from (Kell et al., 2019). These terms are widely used in studies where no perceptual or behavioral experiments are included, and even when animals are anesthetized. We have clarified and justified this choice in the beginning of the Results section:

      “We used three types of stimuli: foregrounds, backgrounds, and combinations of those. We use those terms to refer to sounds differing in their stationarity, under the assumption that stationary sounds carry less information than non-stationary sounds, and are thus typically ignored.”

      We have also added a paragraph in the discussion to emphasize the limits of this definition:

      “First, this study defined foregrounds and backgrounds solely based on their acoustic stationarity, rather than perceptual judgments. This choice allowed us to isolate the contribution of acoustic factors in a simplified setting. Within this controlled framework, we show that acoustic features of foreground and background sounds drive their separation in the brain and the hierarchical extraction of foreground sound features.”

      (4) Related to the above, I think further caveats need to be acknowledged in the study. We do not know what sounds are perceived as foreground or background sounds by ferrets, or indeed whether they make this distinction reliably to the degree that humans do. Furthermore, the individual sounds used here have not been tested for their foreground/background-ness. Thus, the analysis relies on two logical jumps - first, that the stationarity of these sounds predicts their foreground/background perception in humans, and second, that this perceptual distinction is similar in ferrets and humans. I don't think it is known to what degree these jumps are justified. These issues do not directly affect the results, but I think it is essential to address these issues in the Discussion, because they are potentially major caveats to our understanding of the work.

      We agree with the reviewer that the foreground-background distinction might be different in ferrets. In anticipation of that issue, we had enriched the sound set with more ecologically relevant sounds, such as ferret and other animal vocalizations. Nevertheless, we have emphasized this limitation in addition to the limitation of our definition of foregrounds and backgrounds in the discussion: 

      “In addition, most of the sounds included in our study likely have more relevance for humans compared to ferrets (see table \ref{tbl1}). Despite including ferret vocalizations and environmental sounds that are more ecologically relevant for ferrets, it is not clear whether ferrets would behaviorally categorize foregrounds and backgrounds as humans do. Examining how ferrets naturally orient or respond to foreground and background sounds under more ecologically valid conditions, potentially with free exploration or spontaneous listening paradigms, could help address this issue.”

      Reviewer #2(Public review);

      (1) Interpretation of the cerebral blood volume signal: While the results are compelling, more caution should be exercised by the authors in framing their results, given that they are measuring an indirect measure of neural activity, this is the difference between stating "CBV in area MEG was less background invariant than in higher areas" vs. saying "MEG was less background invariant than other areas". Beyond framing, the basic properties of the CBV signal should be better explored:

      a) Cortical vasculature is highly structured (e.g. Kirst et al.( 2020) Cell). One potential explanation for the results is simply differences in vasculature and blood flow between primary and secondary areas of auditory cortex, even if fUS is sensitive to changes in blood flow, changes in capillary beds, etc (Mace et al., 2011) Nat. Methods.. This concern could be addressed by either analyzing spontaneous fluctuations in the CBV signal during silent periods or computing a signal-to-noise ratio of voxels across areas across all sound types. This is especially important given the complex 3D geometry of gyri and sulci in the ferret brain.

      We agree with the reviewers that there could be differences in vasculature across subregions of the auditory cortex and note that this point would also be valid for the published human fMRI data. Nevertheless, even if small differences in vasculature were present, it is unlikely that they would affect our analyses and results, which are designed to be independent of local vascular density. First, we normalize the signal in each voxel using the silent periods, so that the absolute strength of the raw signal, or baseline blood volume in each voxel, is factored in our analysis. Second, we only focus on reliably responsive voxels in each region and do see comparable sound-evoked responses in all regions (Figure S2). Third, our analysis mostly relies on voxel-based correlation across sounds, which is independent of the mean and variance of the voxel responses. Differences in noise, measured through test-retest reliability, can affect values of correlation, which is why we used a noise-correction procedure. After this procedure, invariance does not depend on test-retest, and differences across regions are still seen when matching for test-retest (new  Figure S7). Thus, we believe that differences in vascular architecture across regions are unlikely to affect our results. We added this point in the Methods section when discussing the noise-correction:

      “After this correction, the differences we observed between brain regions were present regardless of voxels' test-retest reliability, or noise level (Figure S7). Thus, potential differences in vasculature across regions are unlikely to affect our results.”

      b) Figure 1 leaves the reader uncertain what exactly is being encoded by the CBV signal, as temporal responses to different stimuli look very similar in the examples shown. One possibility is that the CBV is an acoustic change signal. In that case, sounds that are farther apart in acoustic space from previous sounds would elicit larger responses, which is straightforward to test. Another possibility is that the fUS signal reflects time-varying features in the acoustic signal (e.g. the low-frequency envelope). This could be addressed by cross-correlating the stimulus envelope with fUS waveform. The third possibility, which the authors argue, is that the magnitude of the fUS signal encodes the stimulus ID. A better understanding of the justification for only looking at the fUS magnitude in a short time window (2-4.8 s re: stimulus onset) would increase my confidence in the results.

      We thank the reviewer for raising that point as it highlights that the layout of Figure 1 is misleading. While Figure 1B shows an example snippet of our sound streams, Figure 1D shows the average timecourse of CBV time-locked to a change in sound (foreground or background, isolated or in a mixture). This is the average across all voxels and sounds, aiming at illustrating the dynamics for the three broad categories. In Figure 1E however, we show the cross-validated cross-correlation of CBV across sounds (and different time lags). To obtain this, we compute for each voxel the response to each sound at each time lag, thus obtaining two vectors (size: number of sounds) per lag, one per repeat. Then, we correlate all these vectors across the two repeats, obtaining one cross-correlation matrix per voxel. We finally average these matrices across all voxels. The presence of red squares with high correlations demonstrates that the signal encodes sound identity, since CBV is more similar across two repeats of the same sound (e.g., in the foreground only matrix, 0-5 s vs 0-5 s), than two different sounds (0-5 s vs. 7-12 s). We modified the figure layout as well as the legend to improve clarity.

      (2) Interpretation of the human data: The authors acknowledge in the discussion that there are several differences between fMRI and fUS. The results would be more compelling if they performed a control analysis where they downsampled the Ferret fUS data spatially and temporally to match the resolution of fMRI and demonstrated that their ferret results hold with lower spatiotemporal resolution.

      We agree with the reviewer that the use of different techniques might come in the way of cross-species comparison. We already control for the temporal aspect by using the average of stimulus-evoked activity across time (note that due to scanner noise, sounds are presented cut into small pieces in the fMRI experiments). Regarding the spatial aspect, there are several things to consider. First, both species have brains of very different sizes, a factor that is conveniently compensated for by the higher spatial resolution of fUSI compared to fMRI (0.1 vs 2 mm). Downsampling to fMRI resolution would lead to having one voxel per region per slice, which is not feasible. We also summarize results with one value per region, which is a form of downsampling that is fairer across species. Furthermore, we believe that we already established in a previous study (Landemard et al, 2021 eLife) that fUSI and fMRI data are comparable signals. We indeed could predict human fMRI responses to most sounds from ferret fUSI responses to the same identical sounds. We clarified these points in the discussion:

      “In addition, fMRI has a worse spatial resolution than fUSI (here, 2 vs. 0.1 mm voxels). However, this difference in resolution compensates for the difference in brain size between humans and ferrets. In our previous work, we showed that a large fraction of cortical responses to natural sounds could be predicted from one species to the other using these methods (Landemard et al., 2021).”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      As mentioned above, interpretation of the invariance analyses using predictions from the spectrotemporal modulation encoding model hinges on the model's ability to accurately predict neural responses. Although Figure S5 suggests the encoding model was generally able to predict voxel responses accurately, the authors note in the introduction that, in human auditory cortex, this kind of tuning can explain responses in primary areas but not in non-primary areas (Norman-Haignere & McDermott, PLOS Biol. 2018). Indeed, the prediction accuracy histograms in Figure  S5C suggest a slight difference in the model's ability to predict responses in primary versus non-primary voxels. Additional analyses should be done to a) determine whether the prediction accuracies are meaningfully different across regions and b) examine whether controlling for prediction accuracy across regions (i.e., subselecting voxels across regions with matched prediction accuracy) affects the outcomes of the invariance analyses.

      The reviewer is correct: the spectrotemporal model tends to perform less well in human non-primary cortex. We believe this does not contradict our results but goes in the same direction: while there is a gradient in invariance in both ferrets and humans, this gradient is predicted by the spectrotemporal model in ferrets, but not in humans (possibly indeed because predictions are less good in human non-primary auditory cortex). Regardless of the mechanism, this result points to a difference across species. In ferrets, we found a significantly better prediction accuracy in VP (p=0.001, permutation test) and no differences between MEG and dPEG (p=0.89). In humans, prediction accuracy was slightly higher in primary compared to non-primary auditory cortex, but this effect was not significant (p=0.076). In both species, when matching prediction accuracy between regions, the gradients in invariance were preserved. We have added these analyses to the manuscript (Figure S5).

      A related concern is the procedure used to train the encoding model. From the methods, it appears that the model may have been fit using responses to both isolated and mixture sounds. If so, this raises questions about the interpretability of the invariance analyses. In particular, fitting the model to all stimuli, including mixtures, may inflate the apparent ability of the model to "explain" invariance, since it is effectively trained on the phenomenon it is later evaluated on. Put another way, if a voxel exhibits invariance, and the model is trained to predict the voxel's responses to all types of stimuli (both isolated sounds and mixtures), then the model must also show invariance to the extent it can accurately predict voxel responses, making the result somewhat circular. A more informative approach would be to train the encoding model only on responses to isolated sounds (or even better, a completely independent set of sounds), as this would help clarify whether any observed invariance is emergent from the model (i.e., truly a result of low-level tuning to spectrotemporal features) or simply reflects what it was trained to reproduce.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have run an additional prediction using only the sounds presented in isolation, which replicates our main results (new Figure S6). We have added this control to the manuscript:

      “Results were similar if the model was fit solely on isolated sounds, excluding mixtures from the training set (Figure S6).”

      Finally, the interpretation of the foreground invariance results remains somewhat unclear. In ferrets (Figure 2I), the authors report relatively little foreground invariance, whereas in humans (Figure 5G), most participants appear to show relatively high levels of foreground invariance in primary auditory cortex (around 0.6 or greater). However, the paper does not explicitly address these apparent crossspecies differences. Moreover, the findings in ferrets seem at odds with other recent work in ferrets (Hamersky et al. 2025 J. Neurosci.), which shows that background sounds tend to dominate responses to mixtures, suggesting a prevalence of foreground invariance at the neuronal level. Although this comparison comes with the caveat that the methods differ substantially from those used in the current study, given the contrast with the findings of this paper, further discussion would nonetheless be valuable to help contextualize the current findings and clarify how they relate to prior work.

      We thank the reviewer for this point. While we found a trend for higher background invariance than foreground invariance in ferret primary auditory cortex, this difference was not significant and many voxels exhibit similar levels of background and foreground invariance (for example in Figure 2D, G). Thus, we do not think our results are inconsistent with Hamersky et al., 2025, though we agree the bias towards background sounds is not as strong in our data. This might indeed reflect differences in methodology, both in the signal that is measured (blood volume vs spikes), and the sound presentation paradigm. Our timescales are much slower and likely reflect responses post-adaptation, which might not be as true for Hamersky et al. We have added this point to the discussion, as well as a comment on the difference between ferrets and humans in foreground invariance in primary auditory cortex:

      “In ferrets, primary auditory cortex has been found to over-represent backgrounds in mixtures compared to foregrounds (Hamersky et al., 2025). In contrast, we found a slight, non-significant bias towards foregrounds in primary regions. This difference could be driven by a difference in timescales, as we looked at slower timescales in which adaptation might be more present, reducing the strength of background encoding. In humans, we found a much smaller gap between background and foreground invariance in primary auditory cortex, which was not predicted by the spectrotemporal model. Additional, more closely controlled experiments would be needed to confirm and understand this species difference.”

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) In the introduction, explain the relationship between background/foreground and stationarity/non-stationarity, and thus why stationary/nonstationary stimuli could be used to probe differences in background/foreground processing.

      We have added a sentence at the beginning of the results section to justify our choice (see public review).  

      (2) Avoid use of the background/foreground terminology in Results (and probably Methods).

      For consistency with previous literature, we decided to keep this terminology, though imperfect. We further justified our choice in the beginning of the Results section (see previous point).

      (3) In the Discussion, explain what the implications of the results are for background/foreground processing, and, importantly, highlight any caveats that result from stationarity not being a direct measure of background/foreground.

      We added a paragraph in the Discussion to highlight this point choice (see public review).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figure 1: Showing a silent period in the examples would help in understanding the fUS signal.

      In Figure 1D, we show the average timecourse of CBV time-locked to a change in sound (foreground or background, isolated or in a mixture). This is the average across all voxels and sounds. Thus, it would not be very informative to show an equivalent plot for a silent period, as it would look flat by definition. However, we updated the layout and legend of Figure 1 to make it clearer and avoid confusion.

      (2) "Responses were not homogenous" - would make more sense to say something like "responses were not spatially distributed".

      We removed these words which were indeed not necessary: “We found that reliable soundevoked responses were confined to the central part of ventral gyrus of the auditory cortex.”

      (3) Figure 2D: The maps shown in Figure 2D are difficult to understand for the noninitiated in fUS. At a minimum, labels should be added to indicate A-P, M-L, D-V. I cannot see the white square in the primary figure. An additional graphic would be helpful here to understand the geometry of the measurement.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out that reading these images is indeed an acquired skill. We added an annotated image of anatomy with indications of main features to guide the reader in Figure 1. We also added missing white squares. 

      (4) Figure 2F: Can the authors better justify why the summary statistic is shown for all three areas, but the individual data only compares primary vs. higher order?`

      We now show individual data for all three areas.

      (5) More methods information is needed to understand how recordings were stitched across days. Was any statistical modeling used to factor out the influence of day on overall response levels?

      We simply concatenated voxels recorded across different sessions and days. The slices were sampled randomly to avoid any systematic effect. Because different slices were sampled in different sessions, any spatial structure spanning several slices is unlikely to be artefactual. For instance, the map of average responses in Figure 2A shows a high level of continuity of spatial patterns across slices. This indicates that this pattern reflects a true underlying organization rather than session-specific noise. It also shows that the overall response levels are not affected by the day or recording session. We added a section in the Methods (“Combining different recordings”) to clarify this point:

      “The whole dataset consisted of multiple slices, each recorded in a different recording session. Slices to image on a given day were chosen at random to avoid any systematic bias. Responses were consistent across neighboring slices recorded on different sessions, as shown by the maps of average responses (Figure 2A, Figure S2) where any spatial continuity across different slices must reflect a true underlying signal in the absence of common noise.”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figures:

      The figures are generally very well done and visually appealing. However, I have a few suggestions and questions.

      a)  In Figure 1G, the delta CBV ranges from 0.5 to 1.5, although in subsequent figures (e.g., Figure 2D), the range is much larger (-15 to 45). Is it possible that the first figure is a proportion rather than a percentage, or is there some other explanation for the massive difference in scale? Not being very familiar with this measure, it was confusing.

      The same scale is used in both figures, the major difference being that in Figure 1D, we take the average over all voxels and sounds (for each category), which will include many nonresponsive voxels, and for responsive voxels, sounds that they do not respond a lot to. On the other hand, Figure 2D shows the response of a single, responsive voxel. Thus, the values it reaches for its preferred sounds (45%) are an extreme, which weighs only little in Figure 1D. We have changed the legend of Figure 1D to make this more explicit.

      b)  Similar to the first point, the strength of the correlations in the matrices of Figure 1E is very small (~ 0.05) compared to the test-retest reliabilities plotted in Figure 2B (~0.5). Again, I was confused by this large difference in scale.

      Two main factors explain the difference in values between Figure 1E and Figure 2B. First, in Figure 1B, each correlation is done on the average activity in a window of 0.3 s, opposed to 2.4 s in Figure 2B. More averaging leads to better SNR, which inevitably leads to higher testretest correlations. Second, in Figure 1B, the cross-correlation matrices are averaged across all responsive voxels without any criterion for reliability. On the other hand, Figure 2B show example voxels with good test-retest reliability. 

      c)  In Figure 2D, the example voxels are supposed to be shown in white. It appears that this example voxel is only shown for the non-primary voxel. Please be sure to add these voxels throughout the other panels and figures as well. 

      We fixed this mistake and added the example voxel in all panels.

      d)  Why do the invariance results (e.g., Figure 2F) for individual animals combine across dPEG and VP, while the overall results (across all animals) split things across all three regions? The results in Table 2 do, in fact, provide this data. Upon further examination of the data in Table 2, it seems like there is only a significant difference between background invariance between dPEG and VP for one of the two animals, and that this might be what drives the effect when pooling across all animals. This seems important to both show visually in the figure and to potentially discuss. There is still very clearly a difference between primary and non-primary, but whether there is a real difference between dPEG and VP seems more unclear.

      We added the values for single animals in the plot and highlighted this limitation in the text:

      “While background invariance was overall highest in VP, the differences within non-primary areas were more variable across animals (see table 2).”

      e)  Again, as in Figure 2F, the cross symbols seem like a bad choice as markers since the vertical components of the cross are suggestive of the error of the measurement. However, no error is actually plotted in these figures. I recommend using a different marker and including some measure of error in the invariance plots.

      We replaced the crosses with circles to avoid confusion. The measure of error is provided by the representation of values for single animals.

      f) The caption for Figure 4C states that each line corresponds to one animal, but does not precisely state what this line represents. Is this the median or something?

      Each line indeed represents the median across voxels for one animal. We added this information to the legend.

      g)  In Figure 5, the captions for panels D and E are swapped.

      This has now been corrected.

      (2) Discussion:

      (a) In the paragraph on methodological differences, it mentions that the fMRI voxel size is around 2 mm. This may be true in general, but given the comparison to Kell & McDermott 2019, the voxel size should reflect that used in their study (1 mm).

      The reviewer might refer to this sentence from the methods of Kell et al., 2019: “T1weighted anatomical images were collected in each participant (1-mm isotropic voxels) for alignment and cortical surface reconstruction.” However, this does not correspond to the resolution of the functional data, which is 2 mm, as mentioned a bit further in the Methods:  “In-plane resolution was 2 × 2 mm (96 × 96 matrix), and slice thickness was 2.8 mm with a 10% gap, yielding an effective voxel size of 2 × 2 × 3.08 mm.”

      (b) In the next paragraph on the control of attention, it mentions that attentional differences could play a role. However, in Kell & McDermott 2019, they manipulated attention (attend visual versus attend auditory) and found that it did not substantially affect the observed pattern invariance. I suppose it could potentially affect the degree to which an encoding model could explain the invariance. This seems important, and given that the data was already collected, it could be worth it to analyze that data.

      As the reviewer points out, Kell et al. 2019 ran an additional experiment in which they manipulated auditory vs. visual attention. However, the auditory task was just based on loudness and ensured that the participants were awake and paying attention to the stimuli, but not specifically to the foreground or background. This type of attention did not lead to changes in the observed patterns of invariance, which might have been the case for selective attention to backgrounds or foregrounds in the mixture. Given that these manipulations were not done in the ferret experiments, we chose to not include the analysis of this dataset in the scope of this paper. However, future work investigating that topic further would indeed be of interest.

      (c) The mention of "a convolutional neural network trained to recognize digits in noise" should make more obvious that this is visual recognition rather than auditory recognition.

      We clarified this sentence to make clear that the recognition is visual and not auditory: “For instance, in a convolutional neural network trained to visually recognize digits in different types of noise, when local feedback is implemented, early layers encode noise properties, while later layers represent clean signal.”

      (d) Finally, one explanation of the results in the discussion is that "primary auditory areas could be recruited to maintain background representations, enabling downstream cortical regions to use these representations to specifically suppress background information and enhance foreground representations." This "background-related information" being used to "facilitate further extraction of foregrounds" is similar to what is argued in Hicks & McDermott PNAS 2024.

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting this relevant reference and added it in this paragraph of the discussion.

      (3) Methods:

      In the "Cross-correlation matrices" section, it mentions that time-averaged responses from 2.4 to 4.8 s were used. It would be helpful to provide an explanation of why this particular time window was used. Additionally, I wondered whether one could look at adaptation type effects (e.g., that of Khalighinejad et al., 2019) or whether fUSI does not offer this kind of temporal precision?

      The effects shown in Khalighinejad et al., 2019, are indeed likely too fast to be observed with our methods. However, there are still dynamics in the fUSI signal and in its invariance (Figure S1). Each individual combination of foreground and background is presented for 4.8 s (Figure 1B). Therefore, we chose the range 2.4-4.8 s as the biggest window we could use (to improve SNR) while minimizing contamination from the previous or next sound (indeed, blood volume typically lags neuronal activity by 1.5-2 s). We added this precision to the methods.

      In the "Human analyses" section, it is very unclear which set of data was used from Kell & McDermott 2019. For example, that paper contains 4 different experiments, none of which has 7 subjects. Upon closer reading, it seems that only 7 of the 11 participants from Experiment 1 also heard the background sounds in isolation (thus enabling the foreground invariance analyses). However, they stated that there were only 3 female participants in that experiment, while you state that you used data from 7 females. It would be helpful to double-check this and to more clearly state exactly which participants (i.e., from which experiment) were used and why (e.g., why not use data from Experiment 4 in the visual task/attention condition?).

      We added a sentence to clarify which datasets were used: “Specifically, we used data from Experiment 1 which provided the closest match to our experimental conditions, and only considered the last 7 subjects that heard both the foregrounds and the backgrounds in isolation, in addition to the mixtures.” 

      It was a mistake to mention that it was all female, as the original dataset has 3 females and 8 males, of which we used 7 without any indication of their sex. Thus, we removed this mention from the text.

      In the "Statistical testing" section, why were some tests done with 1000 permutations/shuffles while others were done with 2000?

      We homogenized and used 1000 permutations/shuffles for all statistical tests.

      (4) Miscellany:

      (a) The Hamersky et al. 2023 preprint has recently been published (referenced in the public review), and so you could consider updating the reference.

      This reference has now been updated.

      (b) There are a few borderline statistical tests that could use a bit more nuance. For example (on page 4), "In primary auditory cortex (MEG), there was no significant difference between values of foreground invariance and background invariance (p = 0.063, obtained by randomly permuting the sounds' background and foreground labels, 1000 times)." This test is quite close to being significant, and this might be acknowledged.

      We emphasized the trend to nuance the interpretation of these results: “In primary auditory cortex (MEG), foreground invariance was slightly lower than background invariance, although this difference was not significant (p=0.063, obtained by randomly permuting the sounds' background and foreground labels, 1000 times).”

      (5) Potential typos:

      (a)   Should the title be "natural sound mixtures" instead of "natural sounds mixtures"?

      (b) The caption for Figure 1 says "We imaged the whole auditory through successive slices across several days." I believe this should the "the whole auditory [cortex]." c) In the first paragraph of the discussion, there is a sentence ending in "...are segregated in hemody-namic signal." I believe this should be "hemody-namic signal."

      These errors are now all corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      (1). Analysis of transcript expression is limited to the CT-peptide encoding gene, while no gene expression analysis was attempted for the three identified receptors. Differences in the activation of downstream signaling pathways between the three receptors are also questionable due to unclarities in the statistical analysis and variation in the control and experimental data in heterologous assays. Together, this makes it difficult to propose a mechanism underlying differences in the functions of the two CT-like peptides in muscle control and growth regulation.

      We appreciate the reviewer's rigorous critique. The manuscript has been comprehensively revised as follows:

      (1) For the expression analysis of the three identified receptors, the updated results are presented in Figure 5, with the detailed descriptions in Results section 2.4 (line 287-290) and Materials and Methods section 4.5 (line 767).

      (2) For the statistical tests and methodological clarity, statistical tests were indeed performed for all experiments. However, we acknowledge that the original labeling methods required enhanced methodological clarity, and we apologize for any confusion caused. All figures have been revised to improve the visibility of differences, and statistical test information has been added to both the figure legends and the Materials and methods section “4.10 Statistical Analysis” (line 900-910).

      (3) For the variation in the control and experimental data, the minor observed variations in control conditions across experiments primarily arise from two methodological factors: 1) Each experimental set used cells transfected with distinct receptor subtypes (e.g., AjPDFR1 vs. AjPDFR2), inherently introducing baseline variability due to differential receptor expression profiles. 2) Independent cell culture batches were employed for replicate experiments to ensure biological reproducibility.  Importantly, these minor variations ‌did not compromise‌ the statistical significance of downstream signaling differences (p < 0.01 for all comparative analyses). Therefore, differences in the activation of downstream signaling pathways between the three receptors are reliable.

      (2) The authors also suggest a putative orexigenic role for the CT-like peptidergic system in feeding behavior. This effect is not well supported by the experimental data provided, as no detailed analysis of feeding behavior was carried out (only indirect measurements were performed that could be influenced by other peptidergic effects, such as on muscle relaxation) and no statistically significant differences were reported in these assays.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s valuable comments. Our revised manuscript now includes the following multidimensional analyses to strengthen evidence of the orexigenic role of AjCT2: Firstly, in sea cucumbers, the mass of remaining bait is a common indicator of feeding condition. After long-term AjCT2 injection, this value was significantly decreased in comparison with control group during phase V (Figure 8A-figure supplement 1), which indicates that AjCT2 promotes feeding in A. japonicus. Correspondingly, in long-term loss-of-function experiments (newly added in the revised manuscript), the remaining bait in the siAjCTP1/2-1 group was significantly increased in comparison with siNC group form phase II to IV (Figure 10B). The detailed descriptions of these supplementary experiments have been added to‌ Results Section 2.6 (lines 390-396) and Materials and Methods Section 4.9 (line 879-888).

      Secondly, after 24 days of continuous injections of siAjCTP1/2-1, we monitored the feeding behavior of these sea cucumbers over three consecutive days. Each day, we removed residual bait and feces, then repositioned fresh food at the tank center.‌ We calculated the aggregation percentage (AP) of sea cucumbers around the food during the feeding peak (2:00-4:00) each day, which is the most reliable indicator of feeding behavior in this species‌. The results showed that the AP in siAjCTP1/2-1 group was significantly lower than that in control group. Post-dissection observations revealed reduced intestinal food content and significant intestinal degeneration in the siAjCTP1/2-1 group (The figure has been added below). These results indicate that long-term functional loss of AjCT2 reduces food intake and influences the feeding behavior of A. japonicus.

      In response to the comment regarding “No statistically significant differences were reported in these assays”, we have modified the figures to clearly visualize the differences and added statistical test details in both the figure legends and the Materials and methodssection “4.10 Statistical analysis” (lines 900–910).

      Author response image 1.

      The feeding behavior of A. japonicus after long-term loss-of-function of AjCT2. (A) A record of feeding behavior. The red arrow refers to the food and the red box represents the feeding area. The numbers in the figure represent individuals entering into the feeding area. (B) The aggregation percentage (AP) of sea cucumbers around the food during the feeding peak (2:00-4:00) (n=3 days). (C) The degenerated intestine of sea cucumber after 24 days of siAjCTP1/2-1 injection. Data in the graph represent the mean ± standard deviation. *Significant differences between groups (p < 0.05). Control: siNC injection group; CT-SiRNA: siAjCTP1/2 injection group.<br />

      (3) Overall, details regarding statistical analyses are not (clearly) specified in the manuscript, and there are several instances where statements are not supported by literature evidence.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comments. Again, we sincerely apologize for the confusion caused. To clarify, statistical tests were performed for all experiments. However, the original labeling may have been somewhat messy. We have revised all figures to enhance the visibility of differences and provided detailed statistical test information in both the figure legends and the Materials and Methods section titled “4.10 Statistical Analysis” (lines 900–910). Additionally, we have supplemented the revised manuscript with further literature evidence to support our statements: (1) citation to Furuya et al. (2000), Johnson et al. (2005), Jékely (2013) and Mirabeau et al. (2013) have been added to clarify the foundation studies on DH31 and DH31 receptors in invertebrates (line 73-74); (2) Conzelmann et al. (2013) and Furuya et al. (2000) were cited to validate the present of two different types of CT-related peptides in protostomes: CT-type peptides (with an N-terminal disulphide bridge) and DH31-type peptides (lacking this feature) (line 78-79); (3) Johnson et al. (2005) was referenced to support the dual ligand-receptor interactions of DH31 in Drosophila, specifically its binding to both CG17415 (a CTR/CLR-related protein) and CG13758 (the PDF receptor)  (line 94); (4) Johnson et al. (2005) and Goda et al. (2019) were cited to reinforce the functional significance of dual DH31 receptor pathways in Drosophila, as extensively studied in prior research (line 95-97).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The authors claim that A. japonicus CTs activate "PDF" receptors and suggest that this cross-talk is evolutionarily ancient since a similar phenomenon also exists in the fly Drosophila melanogaster. These conclusions are not fully supported for several reasons. The authors perform phylogenetic analysis to show that the two "PDF" receptors form an independent clade. This clade is sister to the clade comprising CT receptors. This phylogenetic analysis suffers from several issues. Firstly, the phylogenies lack bootstrap support. Secondly, the resolution of the phylogeny is poor because representative members from diverse phyla have not been included. For instance, insect or other protostomian PDF receptors have not been included so how can the authors distinguish between "PDF" receptors or another group of CT receptors? Thirdly, no in vivo evidence has been presented to support that CT can activate "PDF" receptors in vivo.

      We thank the reviewers for their constructive comments. As suggested, ‌we expanded our taxon sampling to include more representative members across diverse phyla‌ and reanalyzed the phylogenetic relationships (including bootstrap tests) in Figure 1C. The revised analysis revealed two distinct clades‌: one containing CTR/CLR-type receptors and the other PDF-type receptors. Specifically, AjCTR clustered within the CTR/CLR-type receptor group, while AjPDFR1 and AjPDFR2 were placed in the PDF-type receptor clade. The full species names for all taxa were provided in the Supplementary Table 2.

      To provide in vivo evidence supporting CT-mediated activation of "PDF" receptors‌, we conducted the following experiments: Firstly, we confirmed that AjPDFR1 and AjPDFR2 were the functional receptors of AjCT1 and AjCT2 (Figure 2, 3 and 4). Secondly, injection of AjCT2 and siAjCTP1/2-1 in vivo induced corresponding changes in AjPDFR1 and AjPDFR2 expression levels in the intestine (Figure 8C, 9A, 9B and 9C).

      (2) The source of CT which mediates the effects on longitudinal muscles and intestine is unclear. Is it autocrine or paracrine signaling by CT from the same tissue or is it long-range hormonal signaling?

      Thank you for this feedback. We have now analysed CT-type neuropeptide expression in A. japonicus using immunohistochemistry with the antiserum to the A. rubens CT-type peptde ArCT, which has previously been shown to cross-react with CT-type neuropeptides in other echinoderms (Aleotti et al., 2022). We have added related descriptions in the following sections: Results (section 2.4, line 299-336), Discussion (section 3.3, line 545-554) and Materials and methods (section 4.6, line 785-817). Consistent with this previous finding, the ArCT antiserum labelled neuronal cells and fibers in the central and peripheral nervous system and in the digestive system of A. japonicus (Figure 6). The specificity of immunostaining was confirmed by performing pre-absorption tests with the ArCT antigen peptide (Figure 6-figure supplement 1). The detection of immunostaining in the innervation of the intestine is consistent with PCR results and the relaxing effect of AjCT2 on intestine preparations. Interestingly, no immunostaining was observed in longitudinal muscle, which is inconsistent with the detection of AjCT1/2 transcripts in this tissue. This may reflect differences in the sensitivity of the methods employed to detect transcripts (PCR) and mature peptide (immunohistochemistry). The absence of ArCT-like immunoreactivity in the longitudinal muscles suggests that AjCT1 and AjCT2 may exert relaxing effects on this tissue in vivo via hormonal signaling mechanisms. However, because AjCT1/2 expression in the longitudinal muscles may be below the detection threshold of the ArCT antibodies, we can’t rule out the possibility that AjCT1/2 are released within the longitudinal muscles physiologically.   

      (3) Pharmacology experiments showing the effects of CT1 and CT2 on ACh-induced contractions were performed. Sample traces have been provided but no traces with ACh alone have been included. How long do ACh-induced contractions persist? These controls are necessary to differentiate between the eventual decay of ACh effects and relaxation induced by CT1 and CT2. The traces also do not reflect the results portrayed in dose-response curves. For instance, in Figure 6B, maximum relaxation is reported for 10-6M. Yet, the trace hardly shows any difference before and after the addition of 10-6M peptide. The maximum effect in the trace appears to be after the addition of 10-8M peptide.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comments. ‌As requested, we have included representative traces of ACh-induced contraction of longitudinal muscle and intestinal preparations (Figure 7—figure supplement 1B and 1C). Notably, the positive control (ACh) maintained contraction effects for at least 15 minutes‌, consistent with its known pharmacological properties. Regarding Figure 7B (previous Figure 6B), ‌the trace illustrates the cumulative effects of successive neuropeptide treatments at increasing concentrations‌. A gradual reduction in response amplitude was observed at the highest peptide concentration, ‌likely reflecting receptor desensitization‌, a phenomenon previously reported for neuropeptide Y and oxytocin (Tsurumaki et al., 2003; Arrowsmith and Wray, 2014). These results are now explicitly described in the Results Section 2.5 (lines 340-345 and 348-352) and discussed in Section 3.3 (lines 569-574). In response to the reviewer’s suggestion‌, we further tested the pharmacological effects of AjCT2 at 10⁻⁶ M. ‌As shown in Figure 7—figure supplement 1A, this concentration induced maximal relaxation‌, confirming its dose-dependent efficacy.

      (4) I am unsure how differences in wet mass indicate feeding and growth differences since no justification has been provided. Couldn't wet mass also be influenced by differences in osmotic balance, a key function of calcitonin-like peptides in protostomian invertebrates? The statistical comparisons have not been included in Figure 7B.

      We appreciate the reviewer's insightful comments. We fully concur that wet mass constitutes an inadequate indicator for evaluating feeding and growth variations. Consequently, we reassessed A. japonicus growth parameters using two established metrics: weight gain rate (WGR) and specific growth rate (SGR), to delineate differences between experimental and control groups. Notably, the high-concentration AjCT2 injection group exhibited statistically significant increases in both WGR and SGR relative to controls (Figure 8A). This demonstrates a putative physiological role of AjCT2 signaling in enhancing feeding efficiency and growth performance in A. japonicus. Detailed methodologies are provided in the Materials and methods Section 4.8 (lines 847-851), with corresponding results presented in the Results Section 2.6 (lines 370-375). Besides, Cong et al., (2024) reported holotocin-induced osmoregulatory function in A. japonicus, manifested by significant wet weight elevation and body bloating. However, our AjCT2 intervention showed no such phenotypic alterations, suggesting that AjCT2 likely does not participate in osmotic balance regulation, at least under these experimental conditions. Crucially, the observed WGR and SGR enhancements following AjCT2 administration was not caused by osmoregulatory effects.

      (5) While the authors succeeded in knocking down CT, the physiological effects of reduced CT signaling were not examined.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We have supplemented the experiments to investigate the physiological effects of long-term reduced CT signaling following the reviewer’s suggestions, including measuring the dry weight of remaining bait and excrement, calculating the weight gain rate and specific growth rate, and testing the expression levels of three growth factors (AjMegf6, AjGDF-8 and AjIgf) to further assess AjCT2’s role in feeding and growth. The results demonstrated that weight gain rate and specific growth rate in the siAjCTP1/2-1 group were significantly decreased (As shown in Figure 10A). Correspondingly, except in phase I, the siAjCTP1/2-1 group exhibited a significant increase in remaining bait and a decrease in excrement during phases II-VI (Figure 10B). Furthermore, the growth inhibitory factor AjGDF-8 was significantly up-regulated and the growth promoting factor AjMegf6 was significantly down-regulated in siAjCTP1/2-1 group (Figure 10C). These findings further support the potential physiological role of AjCT2 signaling in promoting feeding and growth in A. japonicus. The added results are presented in Figure 10, with related descriptions in Section 2.6 (Results, lines 390-396), Section 3.4 (Discussion, line 597-603) and Section 4.9 (Materials and Methods, lines 879-888).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The abstract states that loss-of-function tests (RNAi knockdown) reveal a potential physiological role for AjCT2 signaling in promoting feeding and growth in A. japonicus. However, RNAi knockdown was only followed by analysis of transcript expression of CT-like receptors and not by the assessment of feeding or growth.

      Thank you for this helpful feedback. In the revised manuscript, we have supplemented the experiments to investigate the physiological effects of long-term reduced CT signaling, as suggested by the reviewer. These include measuring the dry weight of remaining bait and excrement, calculating the weight gain rate and specific growth rate, and testing the expression levels of the three growth factors (AjMegf6, AjGDF-8 and AjIgf) to further assess the function of AjCT2 on feeding and growth in A. japonicus. The results are as follows:

      (1) The weight gain rate and specific growth rate in the siAjCTP1/2-1 group were significantly decreased (As shown in Figure 10A).

      (2) Correspondingly, except for the phase I, the siAjCTP1/2-1 group had significantly increased remaining bait and decreased excrement during phases II-VI (Figure 10B).

      (3) The growth inhibitory factor AjGDF-8 was significantly up-regulated, while the growth promoting factor AjMegf6 was significantly down-regulated in the siAjCTP1/2-1 group (Figure 10C).

      These findings further support the potential physiological role of AjCT2 signaling in promoting feeding and growth in A. japonicus. We have incorporated these results into ‌Figure 10‌ and added related descriptions in the following sections: Results (section 2.6, line 390-396), Discussion (section 3.4, line 597-603) and Materials and methods (section 4.9, line 879-888).

      Regarding the original statement in the abstract “Furthermore, in vivo pharmacological experiments and loss-of-function tests revealed a potential physiological role for AjCT2 signaling in promoting feeding and growth in A. japonicus.” This sentence effectively summarizes our findings. Therefore, we have retained it in the revised manuscript while supplementing the missing experimental details as requested.

      (2) Information on the statistical tests that were performed is lacking for most experiments. It is recommended to include this information in the figure legends, in addition to the methods section. Details on the phylogenetic analysis (parameters and statistics used) and calculation of half maximal effective concentrations (calculation methods and confidence intervals) also need to be included in the manuscript.

      Thank you for this constructive feedback. As the reviewer suggested, statistical test information‌ has been incorporated into both the figure legends and the “4.10 Statistical Analysis” subsection of the Materials and methods (lines 900-910). Specifically:

      (1)Phylogenetic analysis details‌ (parameters and statistical approaches) are now provided in the Materials and methods section 4.2 (line 675-682);

      (2) Bootstrap test results‌ supporting the phylogenetic trees have been added to Figure 1B and 1C‌;

      (3)Half-maximal effective concentration (EC₅₀) calculations‌, including methodologies and confidence intervals, are documented in both the Figure 2B legend and the “4.10 Statistical Analysis” section (lines 900-910)‌‌.

      (3) In some figures (e.g. Figure 5A, 7A), the n number indicated does not match the number of data points shown in the figure panel. It is not clear what n represents here. In Figure 6B, an x-axis label is missing. In some figure legends (e.g. Figure 4 - Figure Supplement 1), the error bars and significance levels are not defined.

      We apologize for this error; we have corrected all quantity errors related to "n" in the manuscript’ figure legends. And also, the x-axis label was added in Figure 7B (previous Figure 6B), error bars and significance levels were defined in all figure legends clearly

      (4) It would be useful to explain what the difference is between the Cre and SRE luciferase assay and why these two assays were used to study receptor-activated signaling cascades. The source of the synthetic peptides is mentioned, but it is recommended to also state the purity of the synthetic peptides.

      Thank you for the valuable comments. As stated in the introduction (line 66-69)- “binding of CT to CTR in the absence of RAMPs can activate signaling via several downstream pathways, including cAMP accumulation, Ca<sup>2+</sup> mobilization, and ERK activation.” Based on this established mechanism, we selected ‌cAMP and Ca²⁺ signaling pathways‌ as biomarkers for studying receptor-activated cascades, with the following experimental rationale: CRE-Luc Reporter System functions as a cAMP response element detector and SRE-Luc Reporter System serves as an intracellular Ca²⁺ level indicator. In CRE-Luc detection, when the receptor is activated by a ligand, it couples with Gαs protein to activate the cAMP/PKA signaling pathway. The accumulation of cAMP can lead to the phosphorylation of PKA, and then enhance the transcription of CRE-containing genes. Therefore, significant increase in CRE-Luc activity directly correlates with cAMP accumulation. Similarly, SRE-Luc activity reflects dynamic changes in intracellular Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels. We have added the explanation of this part in the materials and methods section 4.4 (line 715-721). The purity of the synthetic peptides was >95%, and we have also added this information in section 4.4 (line 715) according to the reviewer’s suggestion.

      (5) In Figure 3B, it is difficult to see receptor internalization in response to the application of synthetic CT-like peptides, and a control condition (without peptide application) is lacking.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. The control condition (without peptide application) was added in Figure 3-figure supplement 1, which shows the localization of pEGFP-N1/receptors in the cell membrane. Upon stimulation with synthetic CT-like peptides (‌Materials and methods section 2.3‌), the receptors exhibit clear internalization into the cytoplasm, as visualized in ‌Figure 3B‌ through comparative analysis.

      (6) Differences in the activation of downstream signaling cascades between the three receptors are questionable because there is substantial variation in the experimental data and control conditions in different experiments (for example, in Figures 3A and 4A). To better represent this variation, it is recommended to plot individual data points onto the bar graphs in all figures and to nuance the interpretation of putative differences in downstream signaling of different receptors. Differences in the physiological roles of CT-like peptides may be explained by various mechanisms, including differences in peptide/receptor expression or in the potency of peptides to activate different receptors in vivo. It would be useful to elaborate on these different explanations in the discussion.

      We appreciate the reviewer's critical assessment. The observed variations in control conditions across experiments (e.g., Figures 3A & 4A) primarily arise from two methodological factors: ① Each experimental set used cells transfected with distinct receptor subtypes (e.g., AjPDFR1 vs. AjPDFR2), inherently introducing baseline variability due to differential receptor expression profiles. ② Independent cell culture batches were employed for replicate experiments to ensure biological reproducibility.  Importantly, these minor variations ‌did not compromise‌ the statistical significance of downstream signaling differences (p < 0.01 for all comparative analyses). And according to the reviewer’s suggestion, we have plotted individual data points onto the bar graphs in all figures.

      And also, according to the reviewer’s suggestion, we have expanded the discussion on receptor-specific signaling cascades in Section 3.4 (lines 589-609). Key findings include: In vivo pharmacological assays demonstrated that ‌only high concentrations of AjCT2 significantly enhanced feeding and growth rates in A. japonicus‌. In contrast, neither a low concentration of AjCT2 nor any concentration of AjCT1 (low or high) induced detectable effects. Furthermore, ‌long-term knockdown of AjCTP1/2 further validated the essential role of AjCT2 in regulating feeding and growth‌ in this species. To elucidate the receptor mediating AjCT2’s feeding- and growth-promoting effects, we selected AjPDFR2 based on its distinct activation profile:‌ AjCT2 selectively activated AjPDFR2, inducing downstream ERK1/2 phosphorylation, whereas AjCT1 exhibited no activity‌ toward this receptor. Given this receptor specificity, we performed AjPDFR2 knockdown experiments, which revealed phenotypic changes ‌consistent with those in AjCTP1/2 knockdown animals‌, including ‌significantly reduced WGR and SGR‌, alongside ‌increased remaining bait accumulation and diminished excrement output‌ compared to control. Collectively, these results support a model wherein AjCT2 promotes feeding and growth in A. japonicus via AjPDFR2-dependent activation of the cAMP/PKA/ERK1/2 and Gαq/Ca²⁺/PKC/ERK1/2 cascades‌. Considering the inherent complexity of neuropeptide signaling systems, which involve multiple GPCR subtypes coupled to diverse signaling cascades, ligands bound to the same receptor may activate distinct G protein subforms within a single cell (Møller et al., 2003; Mendel et al., 2020). Receptor activation modes may be modulated by structural polymorphisms or binding site diversity (Wong et al., 2000; Changeux, 2010), as well as by the differential efficacy of peptides in activating receptors in vivo‌.  

      (7) For the peptide injection experiments, it is recommended to explain the different animal groups in the results section. In addition, injection in the control condition seems to have a small effect on the wet weight. Therefore, it would be useful to compare control-injected and peptide-injected groups after injection.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comments. We have provided an expanded explanation of the animal group classifications in Section 2.6 (lines 367–375). We fully agree that a comparative analysis between the experimental and control groups post-injection is essential. However, since wet weight measurement is suboptimal for demonstrating feeding and growth variations, we re-evaluated the data using two validated metrics: weight gain rate (WGR) and specific growth rate (SGR) of A. japonicus. The results revealed that the high-concentration AjCT2 injection group exhibited significantly elevated weight gain rate and specific growth rate compared to the control group, suggesting a potential role of AjCT2 signaling in promoting feeding and growth in A. japonicus. These results are presented in Figure 8A, with detailed descriptions in Results Section 2.6 (lines 370–375) and methodology in Materials and Methods Section 4.8 (lines 847-851).

      (8) Regarding the RNAi knockdown experiments, it is not clear from the methods section what the siNC control exactly is, and how the interference rate is calculated.

      Thank you for this comment. The siNC control was siRNA which does not target any genes in A. japonicus, with interference rates quantified through the 2<sup>-ΔΔCT</sup> method to assess siRNA inhibition efficiency.‌ These methodological details have been incorporated into Materials and Methods Section 4.9 (lines 866–867 and 874-876) for enhanced clarity.‌

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Both the phylogenies are missing bootstrap tests. Please include this analysis. The phylogenetic analyses should also include other Family B ligands and receptors from both vertebrates and invertebrates because it is widely assumed that PDF is related to VIP given their shared roles in circadian clock and gut regulation. Therefore, this analysis needs to be more comprehensive than currently presented. Drosophila melanogaster receptors have also been excluded in spite of the Drosophila PDFR exhibiting ligand promiscuity. The legend should also include the full species names of the various taxa (or modify the figure to include full names) instead of referring to another table. The supplementary table was not available to this reviewer.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s constructive comments. According to the reviewer’s suggestion, we have incorporated the VIPRs and Drosophila melanogaster receptors into the comparative analysis and reanalyzed the phylogenies in Figure 1C, and both phylogenies included bootstrap tests (Figure 1B, 1C) in the revised manuscript. The full species names of the various taxa are listed in supplementary tables 1 and 2 in the revised manuscript.

      (2) Expression data indicate that AjCTP1/2 is expressed in both the longitudinal muscles and intestine. What are the cell types that express AjCTP1/2? Given that the authors show an effect of CT1 and CT2 on both of these tissues, it would be important to know whether this is local regulation (paracrine or autocrine) vs long-distance hormonal control by the nervous system. This can be addressed by performing in situ hybridization or immunohistochemistry of CT (using Asterias rubens CT antibody: https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00382) on these tissues.

      Thank you for this feedback. We have now analysed CT-type neuropeptide expression in A. japonicus using immunohistochemistry with the antiserum to the A. rubens CT-type peptde ArCT, which has previously been shown to cross-react with CT-type neuropeptides in other echinoderms (Aleotti et al., 2022). We have added related descriptions in the following sections: Results (section 2.4, line 299-336), Discussion (section 3.3, line 545-554) and Materials and methods (section 4.6, line 785-817). ‌Consistent with this previous finding, the ArCT antiserum labelled neuronal cells and fibers in the central and peripheral nervous system and in the digestive system of A. japonicus (Figure 6). The specificity of immunostaining was confirmed by performing pre-absorption tests with the ArCT antigen peptide (Figure 6-figure supplement 1). The detection of immunostaining in the innervation of the intestine is consistent with PCR results and the relaxing effect of AjCT2 on intestine preparations. Interestingly, no immunostaining was observed in longitudinal muscle, which is inconsistent with the detection of AjCT1/2 transcripts in this tissue. This may reflect differences in the sensitivity of the methods employed to detect transcripts (PCR) and mature peptide (immunohistochemistry). The absence of ArCT-like immunoreactivity in the longitudinal muscles suggests that AjCT1 and AjCT2 may exert relaxing effects on this tissue in vivo via hormonal signaling mechanisms. However, because AjCT1/2 expression in the longitudinal muscles may be below the detection threshold of the ArCT antibodies, we can’t rule out the possibility that AjCT1/2 are released within the longitudinal muscles physiologically.       

      (3) While Drosophila DH31 can activate both PDF and DH31 receptors, the EC50 values differ drastically. Importantly, there is an independent gene encoding PDF which is a more sensitive ligand for the PDF receptor. This is in stark contrast to the situation presented here where the authors have yet to identify the PDF gene in their system. Outside Drosophila this cross signaling between the two systems has not been observed in any species. Based on this, I would argue that the ability of CTs to activate PDFR is not an evolutionary ancient property but rather an example of convergent evolution if supported by more evidence.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewers' insightful comments.‌ We agree that we cannot rule out the possibilty that ability of CT-type peptides to activate PDF-type receptors in Drosophila and A. japonicus has arisen independently. Therefore, we have modified the text in the discussion accordingly so that this alternative explanation for the effects of CT-type peptides on PDF-type receptors is also presented: “Alternatively, the ability of CT-type neuropeptides to act as ligands for PDF-type receptors in D. melanogaster and A. japonicus may have evolved independently. Further studies on a wider variety of both protostome (e.g. molluscs, annelids) and deuterostome taxa (e.g. other echinoderms, hemichordates) are needed to address this issue.”

      (4) AjCT1 and CT2 can activate the two PDF receptors ex vivo. However, their EC50 values are larger and the responses are lower compared to those seen for the CT receptor. Similar cross-talk between closely related peptide families is often observed in ex vivo systems (see: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbrc.2010.11.089 , https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.162276199 , https://doi.org/10.1093/molbev/mst269 and others). However, very few signaling systems exhibit this type of cross-talk in vivo. Without any in vivo evidence, I suspect that the more likely possibility is that the bona fide endogenous ligand for PDF receptors remains to be discovered. The authors could, however, perform peptide and receptor knockdown experiments and show overlap in phenotypes following CT knockdown and PDFR knockdown to support their claim.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewers' insightful critique. According to the reviewer’s suggestion, we have supplemented CTP and AjPDFR2 knockdown experiments, and measured the dry weight of remaining bait and excrement, as well as calculating the weight gain rate and specific growth rate in response to phenotypic changes. The results showed that weight gain rate and specific growth rate in experimental groups were significantly decreased respectively (As shown in Figure 10A and 11B), Correspondingly, except for the I phase, the siAjCTP1/2-1 group had significantly increased remaining bait and decreased excrement in II-VI phases (Figure 10B), the remaining bait weight was significantly increased in siAjPDFR2-1 group (except during phase I), while the weight of excrement was significantly decreased in phase V and VI (Figure 11C). Therefore, AjCT and AjPDFR2 knockdown experiments showed overlap in phenotypes, providing evidence that AjCT does act as an endogenous ligand for PDFR. These results were added in Figure 10 and Figure 11. The related description was added in the results section 2.6 (line 390-396), section 2.7 (line 427-439) and the materials and methods section 4.9 (line 879-898). We acknowledge, however, that other peptides, in addition AjCT1 and AjCT2, may also act as ligands for AjPDFR1 and AjPDFR2 in vivo and on-going studies in the Chen (OUC) and Elphick (QMUL) labs are attempting to address this issue

      (5) Why are receptor transcripts upregulated following peptide injection? Usually, increased ligand levels/signaling result in a compensatory decrease in receptor levels. These negative feedback loops maintain optimum signaling levels. Since the authors have successfully implemented RNAi for this CT precursor, what are the phenotypes on growth and feeding?

      We thank the reviewers for raising these critical points. Our responses are structured as follows: Firstly, our findings align with established mechanisms of neuropeptide-induced receptor modulation (Please check the reference Tiptanavattana et al. 2022). Secondly, based on the reviewer’s suggestion, we have supplemented the experiments to detect the phenotype variations on growth and feeding based on long-term reduced CT signaling, including measuring the dry weight of remaining bait and excrement, calculating the weight gain rate and specific growth rate, as well as testing the expression levels of the three growth factors (AjMegf6, AjGDF-8 and AjIgf). The results showed that weight gain rate and specific growth rate in siAjCTP1/2-1 group were significantly decreased (As shown in Figure 10A), Correspondingly, except for the I phase, the siAjCTP1/2-1 group had more remaining bait and less excrement in II-VI phases (Figure 10B). Furthermore, the growth inhibitory factor AjGDF-8 was significantly up-regulated and the growth promoting factors AjMegf6 were significantly down-regulated in siAjCTP1/2-1 group (Figure 10C). We have added these results in Figure 10, with detailed description in the results section 2.6 (line 390-396) and in the materials and methods section 4.9 (line 879-888). And after long-term continuous injections of siAjCTP1/2-1, we further recorded the feeding behavior of these sea cucumbers for three consecutive days. The remaining bait and feces were cleaned and the food was re-placed in the middle of the tank each day. We calculated the aggregation percentage (AP) of sea cucumbers around the food during the peak feeding period (2:00-4:00) each day, which is the best indicator for sea cucumber feeding behavior detecting. The results showed that the AP in siAjCTP1/2-1 group was significantly lower than that in control group. After dissection, we also found the intestines of siAjCTP1/2-1 group had less food and significantly degenerated (see author response image 1). All these results supported that long-term functional loss of AjCT2 negatively influence the feeding and growth of A. japonicus.

      Other comments:

      (6) What criteria do the authors use to classify some proteins as "type", some as "like" and others as "related"? In my opinion, DH31 could be referred to as CT-like or CT-type. Please use one term for clarity unless there is a scientific explanation behind this terminology.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. If you look at the paper by Cai et al. (2018) you will see in Figure 14 that CT-type peptides and DH31-type peptides are paralogous, probably due to a gene duplication in the common ancestor of the protostomes. The CT-related peptides in protostomes that have a disulphide bridge we would describe as CT-type because they have conserved a feature that is found in CT-type peptides in deuterostomes. Whereas the DH31 peptides we would describe as CT-like. But there is not a formal rule on this. It is possible the duplication event that gave rise to DH31 and CT-type peptides occurred in the common ancestor of the Bilateria but DH31-type signaling was lost in deuterostomes. On the other hand, if the gene duplication that gave rise to DH31-type peptides and CT-type peptides in protostomes did occur in a common ancestor of the protostomes, then DH31 and CT-type peptides in protostomes could be described as co-orthologs of CT-type peptides in deuterostomes. In this case, both CT peptides and DH31 peptides in protostomes could be described as CT-type. Here is a useful link for explanation of terms: https://omabrowser.org/oma/type/

      (7) Was genomic DNA removal step performed before cDNA synthesis for qRT-PCR?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. The genomic DNA removal step was performed before cDNA synthesis for qRT-PCR and we have added the information in the section 4.5 (line 774-776).

      (8) Line 70: The presence of calcitonin-like peptides (DH31) and DH31 receptors in invertebrates was discovered long before the discoveries by Jekely 2013 and Mirabeau and Joly 2013. Please credit these original studies: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/10841553/ and https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15781884/.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We have credited these original studies in the revised manuscript.

      (9) Lines 72-74: Please cite https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/24359412/.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We have cited it in the revised manuscript.

      (10) Line 87: Please cite https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15781884/.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We have cited it in the revised manuscript.

      (11) Lines 89-91: The functional significance of DH31 signalling to PDFR in Drosophila is known. See: https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/15781884/ and https://pubmed.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/30696873/. There are several studies that have shown the functions of DH31 signalling via DH31R.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We have corrected it and added all this studies in the revised manuscript.

      (12) Figure 1 Supplement 1: The tertiary models for CT1 and CT2 look completely different. This prediction is not in line with both ligands activating the same receptor.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We have deleted this supplementary figure.

      (13) Figure 1 Supplement 3 legend: Please add panel labels next to the corresponding receptor.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We have added panel labels next to the corresponding receptors as you suggested.

      (14) Figure 2: What does CO refer to?

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. CO (Control) refers to the stimulation of HEK293T transfected cells with serum-free DMEM, and we have added the detailed information in Figure 2 legend (line 251-252).

      (15) Figure 3: Due to the low magnification of the cells, it is difficult to see the localization of the receptor. It would also be more appropriate to use a membrane marker rather than DAPI which does not label the cytoplasm or membrane where the receptor can be found.

      we appreciate the reviewer's insightful comment regarding the experimental controls.‌ The baseline receptor localization data under non-stimulated conditions are presented in ‌Figure 3—figure supplement 1‌, demonstrating constitutive membrane distribution of pEGFP-N1-tagged receptors. Upon stimulation with synthetic CT-like peptides, qualitative imaging analysis revealed significant ligand-induced receptor internalization into the cytoplasm (Figure 3B).

      (16) Figure 9: Please include PDF precursor and receptor as separate columns. Also, Drosophila CT/DH31 receptors have been characterized.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. We have added PDF precursor, predicted peptides and receptors as separate columns in the revised manuscript Figure 12. And also, we corrected the error summary of Drosophila CT/DH31 receptors according to your suggestions.

      (17) Table 1: It is not very clear why there are multiple columns for ERK1/2 with different outcomes.

      Thank you for the reviewer’s comment. Although the cAMP/PKA or Gαq/Ca<sup>2+</sup>/PKC signaling is activated after ligand binding to receptors, the downstream ERK1/2 cascade is not necessarily activated. Therefore, we counted the activation status of cAMP/PKA and its downstream ERK1/2 cascade, and Gαq/Ca<sup>2+</sup>/PKC and its downstream cascade in Table 1 respectively. We have optimized Table1 to make it clearer in the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Authors showed the presence of Mtb in human liver biopsy samples of TB patient and reported that chronic infection of Mtb causes immune-metabolic dysregulation. Authors showed that Mtb replicates in hepatocytes in a lipid rich environment created by up regulating transcription factor PPARγ. Authors also reported that Mtb protects itself from anti-TB drugs by inducing drug metabolising enzymes.

      Strengths:

      It has been shown that Mtb induces storage of triacylglycerol in macrophages by induction of WNT6/ACC2 which helps in its replication and intracellular survival, however, creation of favorable replicative niche in hepatocytes by Mtb is not reported. It is known that Mtb infect macrophages and induces formation of lipid-laden foamy macrophages which eventually causes tissue destruction in TB patient. In a recent article it has been reported that "A terpene nucleoside from M. tuberculosis induces lysosomal lipid storage in foamy macrophages" that shows how Mtb manipulates host defense mechanisms for its survival. In this manuscript, authors reported the enhancement of lipid droplets in Mtb infected hepatocytes and convincingly showed that fatty acid synthesis and triacylglycerol formation is important for growth of Mtb in hepatocytes. Authors also showed the molecular mechanism for accumulation of lipid and showed that the transcription factor associated with lipid biogenesis, PPARγ and adipogenic genes were upregulated in Mtb infected cells.

      The comparison of gene expression data between macrophages and hepatocytes by authors is important which indicates that Mtb modulates different pathways in different cell type as in macrophages it is related to immune response whereas, in hepatocytes it is related to metabolic pathways.

      Authors also reported that Mtb residing in hepatocytes showed drug tolerance phenotype due to up regulation of enzymes involved in drug metabolism and showed that cytochrome P450 monooxygenase that metabolize rifampicin and NAT2 gene responsible for N-acetylation of isoniazid were up regulated in Mtb infected cells.

      Weaknesses:

      There are reports of hepatic tuberculosis in pulmonary TB patients especially in immune-compromised patients, therefore finding granuloma in human liver biopsy samples is not surprising.

      Mtb infected hepatic cells showed induced DME and NAT and this could lead to enhanced metabolism of drug by hepatic cells as a result Mtb in side HepG2 cells get exposed to reduced drug concentration and show higher tolerance to drug. Authors mentioned that " hepatocyte resident Mtb may display higher tolerance to rifampicin". In my opinion higher tolerance to drug is possible only when DME of Mtb inside is up regulated or target is modified. Although, in the end authors mentioned that drug tolerance phenotype can be better attributed to host intrinsic factors rather than Mtb efflux pumps. It may be better if Drug tolerant phenotype section can be rewritten to clarify the facts.

      In the revised manuscript, by immune-staining authors convincingly showed that hepatocytes are a favourable niche for replication of MTb.

      Authors have rewritten the drug tolerant phenotype section which reads better.

      Overall, this paper has new and important information on how MTb establishes a favourable niche for growth in hepatocytes and creates a drug tolerant environment.

      We thank the reviewer for the through and insightful review.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Sarkar et al has demonstrated the infection of liver cells/hepatocytes with Mtb and the significance of liver cells in the replication of Mtb by reprogramming lipid metabolism during tuberculosis. Besides, the present study shows that similar to Mtb infection of macrophages (reviewed in Chen et al., 2024; Toobian et al., 2021), Mtb infects liver cells but with a greater multiplication owing to consumption of enhanced lipid resources mediated by PPARg that could be cleared by its inhibitors. The strength of the study lies in clinical evaluation of the presence of Mtb in human autopsied liver samples from individuals with miliary tuberculosis and presence of a clear granuloma-like structure. The interesting observation is of granuloma-like structure in liver which prompts further investigations in the field.

      The modulation of lipid synthesis during Mtb infection, such as PPARg upregulation, appears generic to different cell types including both liver cells and macrophage cells. It is also known that infection affect PPARγ expression and activity in hepatocytes. It is also known that this can lead to lipid droplet accumulation in the liver and the development of fatty liver disease (as shown for HCV). This study is in similar line for M.tb infection. As liver is the main site for lipid regulation, the availability of lipid resources is greater and higher is the replication rate. In short, the observations from the study confirm the earlier studies with these additional cell types. It is known that higher the lipid content, greater are Lipid Droplet-positive Mtb and higher is the drug resistance (Mekonnen et al., 2021). The DMEs of liver cells add further to the phenotype.

      Comments on revised version:

      The authors noted that even in experiments where mice were infected with lower CFUs, the presence of Mtb colonies could still be detected in the liver. It would be beneficial to include some experimental data related to this in the supplementary information, as it could provide valuable insights for the research field.

      We thank the reviewer for the in depth evaluation of our manuscript and as suggested we will include the data where Mtb was detected in the liver at low CFUs

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In this revised manuscript, the authors explore how Mtb can infect hepatocytes and create a favorable niche associated with upregulation of the transcription factor PPARγ which presumably allows the bacteria to scavenge lipids from lipid droplets in host cells and upregulate drug-metabolizing enzymes to protect against its elimination. In response to the review, the authors have performed some additional immunostaining of hepatocytes, added more detail to figure legends, added experiments somewhat showing improved colocalization and staining, clarified several points and paragraphs, and updated the referenced literature and discussion.

      The current manuscript provides evidence that human miliary TB patients have infection of hepatocytes with Mtb, with evidence that the bacteria survive at least partially through upregulation of PPARγ, which significantly changes the lipid milieu of the cells. There is also an examination of transcriptomics and lipid metabolism in response to Mtb infection, as well as drug tolerance of Mtb inside hepatocytes. The current manuscript is an improvement over the previous one.

      However, although the manuscript is improved, tissue immunophenotyping of the various cells in the liver remains weak and unconvincing. This is truly a missed opportunity and lessens the rigor of the central findings and conclusions. As pointed out by another reviewer, literature has described different fates of Mtb in the liver. Given the tissue available to the authors, carefully dissecting the various cells that the bacteria are in (esp. hepatocytes versus Kupffer cells) is critical. The authors use only 2 generic markers and do not distinguish among cell types within the tissue slices. A review of the literature shows a variety of both human and mouse antibody markers. In fact, a liver atlas based on immunophenotyping has been published. Likewise, the authors comment on liver granulomas, but this is not justified without immunophenotyping.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for the in-depth and detailed suggestions. We would like to clarify that the primary aim of our study was to determine the localization of Mtb within hepatocytes and the downstream biological consequences. To this end, we employed two well-established and widely validated markers (ASPGR 1 and albumin) that are consistently used to identify hepatocytes in both human and murine liver tissue. While we acknowledge the broader potential of comprehensive immunophenotyping, our focused approach was designed to specifically address the question of hepatocyte involvement, which the selected markers effectively support, which was further reiterated by the Reviewer 1.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In my opinion this paper contains important information and no further information is required for this manuscript.

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful comments

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors noted that even in experiments where mice were infected with lower CFUs, the presence of Mtb colonies could still be detected in the liver. It would be beneficial to include some experimental data related to this in the supplementary information, as it could provide valuable insights for the research field.

      As suggested,  we will include the data with the low CFUs in the updated manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      • Line 340, the fact that PPARγ inhibition decreases bacterial load should not be surprising, as the authors cite several papers where this is already shown.

      • Line 379, the increased tolerance of Mtb to drugs in hepatocytes is only significant at the lower 2 concentrations, not at 5 ug/mL.

      • Fig S4F-H, the y axis is inappropriately not set to zero on the lower limit.

      • Fig S9B, the Y-axis states "relative" CFU, but there is no indication what the bars are normalized to, and the numbers are much more typical of standard CFU values. Was the "Relative" part left in by mistake?

      • Double check the ending of the figure legend for Figure S10 and S11.

      • Line 352, phenomenom [sic] is misspelled.

      • On re-read, several sentences throughout this manuscript need improvement regarding structure and grammar. I suggest careful editorial review.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the issues and these will be carefully modified in the next version.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors showed the presence of Mtb in human liver biopsy samples of TB patients and reported that chronic infection of Mtb causes immune-metabolic dysregulation. Authors showed that Mtb replicates in hepatocytes in a lipid rich environment created by up regulating transcription factor PPARγ. Authors also reported that Mtb protects itself from anti-TB drugs by inducing drug metabolising enzymes.

      Strengths:

      It has been shown that Mtb induces storage of triacylglycerol in macrophages by induction of WNT6/ACC2 which helps in its replication and intracellular survival, however, creation of favorable replicative niche in hepatocytes by Mtb is not reported. It is known that Mtb infects macrophages and induces formation of lipid-laden foamy macrophages which eventually causes tissue destruction in TB patients. In a recent article it has been reported that "A terpene nucleoside from M. tuberculosis induces lysosomal lipid storage in foamy macrophages" that shows how Mtb manipulates host defense mechanisms for its survival. In this manuscript, authors reported the enhancement of lipid droplets in Mtb infected hepatocytes and convincingly showed that fatty acid synthesis and triacylglycerol formation is important for growth of Mtb in hepatocytes. The authors also showed the molecular mechanism for accumulation of lipid and showed that the transcription factor associated with lipid biogenesis, PPARγ and adipogenic genes were upregulated in Mtb infected cells.

      The comparison of gene expression data between macrophages and hepatocytes by authors is important which indicates that Mtb modulates different pathways in different cell type as in macrophages it is related to immune response whereas, in hepatocytes it is related to metabolic pathways.

      Authors also reported that Mtb residing in hepatocytes showed drug tolerance phenotype due to up regulation of enzymes involved in drug metabolism and showed that cytochrome P450 monooxygenase that metabolize rifampicin and NAT2 gene responsible for N-acetylation of isoniazid were up regulated in Mtb infected cells.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive feedback and for highlighting the strengths of our study.

      Weaknesses:

      There are reports of hepatic tuberculosis in pulmonary TB patients especially in immune-compromised patients, therefore finding granuloma in human liver biopsy samples is not surprising.

      Mtb infected hepatic cells showed induced DME and NAT and this could lead to enhanced metabolism of drug by hepatic cells as a result Mtb in side HepG2 cells get exposed to reduced drug concentration and show higher tolerance to drug. The authors mentioned that " hepatocyte resident Mtb may display higher tolerance to rifampicin". In my opinion higher tolerance to drugs is possible only when DME of Mtb inside is up regulated or the target is modified. Although, in the end authors mentioned that drug tolerance phenotype can be better attributed to host intrinsic factors rather than Mtb efflux pumps. It may be better if the Drug tolerant phenotype section can be rewritten to clarify the facts.

      We agree that several case studies regarding liver infection in pulmonary TB patients have been reported in the literature, however this report is the first comprehensive study that establishes hepatocytes to be a favourable niche for Mtb survival and growth.

      Drug tolerance is a phenomenon that is exhibited by the bacteria and during hostpathogen interactions, can be influenced by both intrinsic (bacterial) and extrinsic (host-mediated) factors. Multiple examples of tolerance being attributed to host driven factors can be found in literature (PMID 32546788, PMID: 28659799, PMID: 32846197). Our studies demonstrate that Mtb infected hepatocytes create a drug tolerant environment by modulating the expression of Drug modifying enzymes (DMEs) in the hepatocytes.

      As suggested by the reviewer we will rewrite the drug tolerant phenotype section.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      The manuscript by Sarkar et al has demonstrated the infection of liver cells/hepatocytes with Mtb and the significance of liver cells in the replication of Mtb by reprogramming lipid metabolism during tuberculosis. Besides, the present study shows that similar to Mtb infection of macrophages (reviewed in Chen et al., 2024; Toobian et al., 2021), Mtb infects liver cells but with a greater multiplication owing to consumption of enhanced lipid resources mediated by PPARg that could be cleared by its inhibitors. The strength of the study lies in the clinical evaluation of the presence of Mtb in human autopsied liver samples from individuals with miliary tuberculosis and the presence of a clear granuloma-like structure. The interesting observation is of granuloma-like structure in liver which prompts further investigations in the field.

      The modulation of lipid synthesis during Mtb infection, such as PPARg upregulation, appears generic to different cell types including both liver cells and macrophage cells. It is also known that infection affect PPARγ expression and activity in hepatocytes. It is also known that this can lead to lipid droplet accumulation in the liver and the development of fatty liver disease (as shown for HCV). This study is in a similar line for M.tb infection. As the liver is the main site for lipid regulation, the availability of lipid resources is greater and higher is the replication rate. In short, the observations from the study confirm the earlier studies with these additional cell types. It is known that higher the lipid content, the greater are Lipid Droplet-positive Mtb and higher is the drug resistance (Mekonnen et al., 2021). The DMEs of liver cells add further to the phenotype.

      We thank the reviewer for emphasizing on the strengths of our study and how it can lead to further investigations in the field.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This manuscript by Sarkar et al. examines the infection of the liver and hepatocytes during M. tuberculosis infection. They demonstrate that aerosol infection of mice and guinea pigs leads to appreciable infection of the liver as well as the lung. Transcriptomic analysis of HepG2 cells showed differential regulation of metabolic pathways including fatty acid metabolic processing. Hepatocyte infection is assisted by fatty acid synthesis in the liver and inhibiting this caused reduced Mtb growth. The nuclear receptor PPARg was upregulated by Mtb infection and inhibition or agonism of its activity caused a reduction or increase in Mtb growth, respectively, supporting data published elsewhere about the role of PPARg in lung macrophage Mtb infection. Finally, the authors show that Mtb infection of hepatocytes can cause upregulation of enzymes that metabolize antibiotics, resulting in increased tolerance of these drugs by Mtb in the liver.

      Overall, this is an interesting paper on an area of TB research where we lack understanding. However, some additions to the experiments and figures are needed to improve the rigor of the paper and further support the findings. Most importantly, although the authors show that Mtb can infect hepatocytes in vitro, they fail to describe how bacteria get from the lungs to the liver in an aerosolized infection. They also claim that "PPARg activation resulting in lipid droplets formation by Mtb might be a mechanism of prolonging survival within hepatocytes" but do not show a direct interaction between PPARg activation and lipid droplet formation and lipid metabolism, only that PPARg promotes Mtb growth. Thus, the correlations with PPARg appear to be there but causation, implied in the abstract and discussion, is not proven.

      The human photomicrographs are important and overall, well done (lung and liver from the same individuals is excellent). However, in lines 120-121, the authors comment on the absence of studies on the precise involvement of different cells in the liver. In this study there is no attempt to immunophenotype the nature of the cells harboring Mtb in these samples (esp. hepatocytes). Proving that hepatocytes specifically harbor the bacteria in these human samples would add significant rigor to the conclusions made.

      We thank the reviewer for nicely summarizing our manuscript.

      Our study establishes the involvement of liver and hepatocytes in pulmonary TB infection in mice. Understanding the mechanism of bacterial dissemination from the lung to the liver in aerosol infections demands a detailed separate study.

      Figure 6E and 6F shows how PPARγ agonist and antagonist modulate (increase and decrease respectively) bacterial growth in hepatocytes (further supported by the CFU data in Supplementary Figure 9B). Again, the number of lipid droplets in hepatocytes increase and decrease with the treatment of PPARγ agonist and antagonist respectively as shown in Figure 6G and 6H. Collectively, these studies provide strong evidence that PPARγ activation leads to more lipid droplets that support better Mtb growth.

      We thank the reviewer for finding our human photomicrographs convincing. In the manuscript, we provide evidence for the direct involvement of the hepatocytes (and liver) in Mtb infection. We have performed detailed immunophenotyping of hepatocyte cells in the mice model with ASPGR1 (asialoglycoprotein receptor 1) and in the revised version of record, we have further stained the infected hepatocytes with anti-albumin antibody.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In my opinion drug tolerant phenotype section should be rewritten for better clarification. The manuscript contains important information about hepatic tuberculosis which are not reported yet.

      We have rewritten the drug tolerant phenotype section for better clarity.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments regarding important information about hepatic tuberculosis

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The following are some observations and comments on the manuscript.

      (1) The study delves into the mechanisms related to hepatic TB/miliary TB; however, the introduction and discussion only describe and discuss the data in the context of pulmonary TB giving a sense that the mandate of the MS is the exploration of the role of liver cells in pulmonary TB. There appears a gap in the connection of findings from the Miliary TB to the pulmonary TB. A discussion of the conversion of pulmonary TB to extrapulmonary /hepatic TB in the light of the findings may be helpful.

      We have modified the discussion section to include possible mechanisms that convert pulmonary TB to hepatic TB in the light of findings. Briefly, Pulmonary tuberculosis (TB) can lead to miliary TB probably through hematogenous dissemination, where Mtb spreads from the infected lungs into blood vessels either from a primary lung focus, reactivated TB or caseous necrosis.  Once in blood vessels, the bacteria seed multiple organs, forming tiny granulomas, characteristic of miliary TB. The liver involvement could be either through direct hematogenous spread or extrusion from nearby infected lymph nodes, leading to hepatic TB, which presents with granulomas and liver dysfunction. This spread underscores the severity of untreated pulmonary TB and the need for early intervention. Our in vivo infection data clearly shows that pulmonary infection of Mtb in mice and guinea pigs can steadily leads to significant infection of the liver and metabolic abnormalities in the liver. The study further highlights the need for systemic studies to better understand the route and mode of dissemination from lungs to liver for better pathophysiological understanding of the disease and creating new therapeutic targets.  

      (2) The authors show the presence of Mtb in the liver autopsies of miliary tuberculosis patients. It is well known that Mtb disseminates during the late stages to several organs and liver is a major site (Sharma et al. 2005; 10.1016/S1473-3099(05)70163-8). Other clinical observations also point to the fact that although Mtb infects liver cells, it is cleared (Thandi et al., 2018, https://doi.org/10.4049/jimmunol.200.Supp.173.20). As the samples are from miliary TB, it is expected that the bacterial load must have been very high before spreading to blood. It is known that once in blood, M.tb is expected to spread to various organs, especially highly vascular ones. Were any other tissues (especially with high vasculature) stained and verified? If yes, add to the supplementary data or discuss.

      Other tissues were not collected and stained during this study. Studies are currently underway to understand whether other vasculated organs also harbour Mtb or not. Besides several studies have shown that Mtb can infect a wide range of organs like brain, kidney, bone marrow, etc (PMID: 33142108, PMID: 28046053, PMID: 34269789) during miliary conditions.

      (3) It is not evident from this paper if hepatic infiltration occurs in pulmonary TB patients? It may therefore be important to discuss the status of liver infections in the primary pulmonary infection.

      Based on the available data from human biopsied liver samples, there is an indication of liver involvement in systemic tuberculosis (TB). However, to gain a more comprehensive understanding of hepatic infiltration in pulmonary TB patients, it is essential to conduct well-organized clinical studies. These studies should specifically target pulmonary TB patients and explore the extent and nature of liver involvement in these individuals (discussion). As suggested by the reviewer it is in the discussion

      (4) Similarly, in the mice model, M.tb was shown to localize to liver when aerosolic infection was given. Were any other tissues, such as kidney, bone marrow etc, checked? Is it because of the high dose of M.tb against the standard challenge dose of 50-100 CFU? Further, since the study in the mouse model is to mimic a miliary tuberculosis of liver, did the dissemination occur via bloodstream and if mycobacteremia could be observed in infected mice.

      Currently studies are underway to understand the involvement of other organs like kidney, brain, bone marrow, in aerosol infection mice model and how dissemination occurs in those distant organs.

      The focus of the current study was to understand the role of liver in systemic tuberculosis with emphasis on hepatocytes as a key cell type to be infected. We have also conducted the experiments with lower CFUs and could detect the presence of Mtb colonies in liver, so we do not think that the infection of liver is dependent on the dose of infection.

      (5) There are studies in mouse model which infer that liver carried the lowest bacterial burden, was cleared the fastest, and it is established that as compared to sites persistently seeded by M. tuberculosis, in the liver the bacteria rarely infect cell types other than professional phagocytes. As the observations in this study are contrasting, the discussion section should include a critical comparative analysis to justify why in the conditions used in the study, the hepatocytes and not Kupffer cells are infected. Other than the morphological description to indicate M.tb infection of hepatocytes in the liver section (fig 1E), it will be good to show localization of M.tb specifically to hepatocytes by using hepatocyte specific marker. Unlike as reported, why was a clearance of M.tb not observed even after 10 weeks (figure 2B).

      While some studies show that Mtb from the liver is cleared fast but there are several other studies that report Liver harbours Mtb even after 10 weeks postinfection (PMID: 22359543, PMID: 21533158, PMID: 29242198). We have consistently observed Mtb infection of liver post week 10 in our infection model. 

      We have performed detailed immunophenotyping of hepatocyte cells in the mice model with ASPGR1 (asialoglycoprotein receptor 1) and in the revised version of record, we have further stained the isolated hepatocytes with anti-albumin antibody (albumin is a robust marker of hepatocyte identity) and have showed the presence of Mtb in it. The data has been included in the revised manuscript (Fig 2J)

      (6) While the result section mentions that "individuals with miliary tuberculosis' (line 107), the legend of Figure 1 writes 'Presence of Mtb in human pulmonary tuberculosis patients'. This is confusing. Clarify

      We thank the reviewer for pointing it out, we have changed the figure legends to miliary tuberculosis as most of the liver biopsy samples were obtained from military tuberculosis patients. 

      (7) Supplementary Figure 2D: Corresponding control panel (uninfected) should be added, which will also verify the specificity of Ag85b. As it is known that Ag85B is secreted out from the bacteria and hence the detected signals may not confirm that Mtb is in hepatocytes. Ag85B per bacterium decreases by almost 10,000-fold at later stages of infection because of secretion (Ernst JD, Cornelius A, et al 2019 mBio). In Supl figure 2D, Ag85b signal seems to be present everywhere inside the cells. Hence, it is important that the control panel be added.

      We have included a control image below which shows no staining of Ag85B in the uninfected sample.While we acknowledge with the reviewer’s comment, but Ag85B has been consistently used as a marker for Mtb presence in multiple studies. Nargan et al., uses Ag85B based staining to characterize infection both pulmonary and EPTB samples (PMID: 38880068). Jain et al., uses Ag85B to characterize Mtb infection of Mesenchymal stem cell in lung biopsy samples of pulmonary TB patients (PMID: 32546788)

      Author response image 1.

      Ag85B staining in uninfected mice shows no signals

      (8) The kinetics experiments in Figure 3D-3G should have used time laps microscopy of a few of the infected cells or it should be represented in CFU. If we consider the doubling time of H37Rv is about 22h to 24h, the data showing that MFI increases dramatically from 5 HPI to 120 HPI, gives an impression that the bacterial number inside the cells increased more than its doubling time.

      We have added the modified plot. As suggested, the CFU of Mtb within HepG2, PHCs, THP-1, RAW 264.7 and BMDMs have been included in the revised version (Supplementary Figure 4 D-H)

      (9) What is the effect of C45 and T863 on Mtb growth invitro? The effect of C45 and T863 on Mtb growth invitro should be shown to be ruled out. The representative image in Figure 5F is DMSO or C45 treated cells panel? Please specify it.

      As per the reviewer’s suggestion we have seen the effect of C45 (30 µM) and T863 (25 µM) on Mtb growth in vitro and did not find any difference in the growth kinetics. The representative image in Figure 5F is DMSO treated cells.

      Author response image 2.

      Growth kinetics of Mtb in 7H9 medium with DMSO, C75 and T863

      (10) Supplementary Figure 6B: Correct the Y-axis label from mRNA levels to Fold change (normalised to control). Please do similar changes wherever required.

      We have made the necessary changes as per the suggestion of the reviewer.

      (11) Figure 7B and 7C: How was the normalization performed? Is the data normalized to the number of bacteria that entered the specific cell type or was normalized at 48hrs with respect to DMSO? DMSO alone data should be shown.

      In the drug tolerance assays, we have calculated the ratio of the bacterial burden in hepatocytes treated with drugs compared to hepatocytes treated with DMSO. The infection was given for 48 hours post which the infected cells were treated with the mentioned concentrations of isoniazid and rifampicin for 24 hours. CFU enumeration was conducted after this 24 hour. Figure 7A gives a schematic of the experimental set up.

      % Tolerant Bacterial population= [A/B X 100] % where A is the CFU of Mtb from infected hepatocytes treated with drug and B is the CFU of Mtb infected cells treated with DMSO.Thus the effect of MOI is negated.

      To provide further credence to the CFU data, we have analysed these studies using microscopic studies as well, where no cell death was observed under the conditions. Mouse BMDMs were as a macrophage control. We have calculated the % tolerance as ratio by measuring the mean fluorescent intensity of GFP-Mtb per hepatocyte treated with drug to MFI of GFP-Mtb per hepatocyte treated with DMSO (control). More than 20 fields, each consisting of more than 4 infected cells have been used for analysis providing additional evidence of less killing of Mtb in hepatocytes compared to BMDMs with anti-TB drugs. All these details are included in the manuscript.

      (12) While authors have shown the changes in mRNA levels of CYP3A4, CYP3A43, NAT2, the protein or activities of some of these should be measured to verify the effect.

      Currently studies are underway to understand the activities of the key proteins involved in isoniazid and rifampicin metabolism and will be published as a separate manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Additional comments are:

      • Figure 2D, the 20X and 40X magnifications do not look appreciably different in size. Please double-check that the correct images were used.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing it out, we havecorrected it in the revised version.

      • Lines 162-164: The authors state almost 100% purity. However, the contour plot in 2F appears to show 2 cell populations. Figure 2G is missing a legend of which colors correspond to which staining (and again there appears to be highly variable staining).

      We agree with the reviewer that there are two contours observed in Figure 2F. Although both the contours are positive for ASPGR1 protein, but the level of expression of the ASPGR1 protein is variable. The corresponding confocal image (Nucleus stained by DAPI and ASPGR1 stained with ASPGR1 antibody with Alexa fluor 555 conjugated secondary antibody) also indicates a variable staining of isolated primary hepatocytes, where some cells give a stronger intensity signal than the other cells, further visually confirming our statement. Moreover, several studies show differential expression of ASPGR1 protein in hepatocyte like cells (PMID: 27143754)

      To further clarify and be more specific with respect to the identity of the hepatocytes, we have stained primary hepatocytes from infected mouse livers with Albumin antibody (a stable marker for hepatocytes) and Ag85B (2J)

      Multiple figures throughout the manuscript, including this one, would benefit from the use of arrows to depict what is described in the legend and text more clearly, and the use of higher power insets to better define cell architecture. Finally, some images appear blurry to the eye. Improvements are needed throughout.

      As per the suggestion, we have modified the figures and figure legends for better clarity.

      • Lines 153-155. Albumin, AST and GGT appear to be significantly up at week 8, contradicting the statement that there is no change until week 10.

      We thank the reviewer for poiting it out and  have made suitable changes in the write up

      • Lines 203-205: The authors state earlier that bacteria survive in macrophage phagosomes. Do the authors know the niche for bacteria in hepatocytes that enable them to continue to grow? Transcriptome data from HepG2 cells suggest perhaps a phagosomal pathway?

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful question. As rightly pointed out by the reviewer, transcription data indeed suggests changes in several important pathways like macroautophagy, golgi vesicular transport and vacuolar transport, which can affect the subcellular localisation of Mtb within hepatocytes. High resolution microscopic studies with respect to the subcellular localisation of labelled Mtb within Primary hepatocytes, HepG2 and THP-1 has been conducted and the % colocalization within different intra-cellular compartments have been measured. The image of colocalization of labelled Mtb within PHCs is shown below along with the % colocalization within various compartments in PHCs, HepG2 and THP-1 is added. 

      Author response image 3.

      Colocalisation of Mtb-GFP with various intra-cellular markers within PHCs.

      Author response image 4.

      Percentage Colocalisation of Mtb-GFP with various intra-cellular markers within PHCs, HepG2 and THP-1.

      • Validation of some critical genes found in the HepG2 cells should be done by qRTPCR in primary hepatocytes.

      qRT-PCR analysis of some of the key genes in HepG2 have been validated in primary hepatocytes at 24 hours post infection. Majority of the genes show a similar trend.

      Author response image 5.

      Gene expression analysis of the mentioned genes in Mtb infected PHCs as compared to the uninfected control.

      • Lines 259-260: The authors state a high degree of co-localization. The photomicrograph of a single cell in Fig. 5D is not convincing. I'm not even sure that they are really in the same subcellular compartment. Co-localization stated in Fig. S8B is also not convincing as shown.

      The image currently shown in figure 3D is a maximum intensity projection image of multiple z-stacks encompassing the entire cell.

      We agree with the reviewer with respect to figure Fig S8B and will modify the text and the figure legend accordingly.

      Copywriting edits:

      • It is difficult to see individual gene names in Figures 4D and 4E. A higher resolution or larger font would be appreciated for the reader.

      An excel file with the top differentially regulated genes at both 0 hours post infection and 48 hours post infection has been added.

      • Figure 5A has a shadow on the top right image.

      We have changed the image in the revised manuscript

      • Figure 5E is difficult to read the labels on the axes; it would be better in general to make the labels separately instead of relying on the graphing software, since these labels can get stretched when the size of the graph is modified.

      We agree with the reviewer and have made necessary changes.

      • Line 163: should be "percent" and not "perfect."

      We thank the reviewer for pointing it out and have corrected it

      • Line 190: is missing a period at the end of the sentence "...for further experiments"

      We thank the reviewer for pointing it out and have corrected it

      • Line 332: should be "hepatocytes" instead of "hepatoctyte" [sic]

      We thank the reviewer for pointing it out and have corrected it

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This manuscript assesses the differences between young and aged chondrocytes. Through transcriptomic analysis and further assessments in chondrocytes, GATA4 was found to be increased in aged chondrocyte donors compared to young donors. Subsequent mechanistic analysis with lentiviral vectors, siRNAs, and a small molecule was used to study the role of GATA4 in young and old chondrocytes. Lastly, an in vivo study was used to assess the effect of GATA4 expression on osteoarthritis progression in a DMM mouse model.

      Strengths:

      This work linked the overexpression of GATA4 to NF-kB signaling pathway activation, alterations to the TGF-b signaling pathway, and found that GATA4 increased the progression of OA compared to the DMM control group. This indicates that GATA4 contributes to the onset and progression of OA in aged individuals.

      The authors thank the reviewer for reviewing our manuscript and providing insightful comments.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) A couple of sentences should be added to the introduction, to emphasize the role GATA4 plays, such as the alterations to the TGF-b signaling pathway and the increased activation of the NF-kB pathway. 

      As suggested, we have expanded on these signaling pathways in the Introduction to highlight the known functions of GATA4. Importantly, there was no previous study reporting the roles of GATA4 in regulating TGF-β pathway.

      “Many growth factors contribute to the chondro-supportive environment in the knee joint. Particularly, transforming growth factor-b (TGF-b) plays a key role in maintaining chondrocytes and replenishing ECM loss. However, during OA, TGF-b can induce catabolic processes in chondrocytes, resulting in matrix stiffening, osteophytes, and chondrocyte hypertrophy.[10-12]” (Lines 80-84)

      “Mechanistically, upregulation of GATA4 was shown to increase nuclear factor-kB (NF-kB) pathway activation.[14,15]  NF-κB is thought to amplify and potentially propagate cellular senescence during the aging process through the senescence-associated secretory phenotype (SASP), which could contribute to a low-grade state of chronic inflammation.[16]” (Lines 99-102)

      “When GATA4 was over expressed, we found that there were alterations to the TGF-b signaling pathway and activation of the NF-kB signaling pathway.” (Lines 106-108)

      (2) Figure 1F, the GATA4 histology image should be bigger.

      We have now increased the size of the image in revised Figure 1F.

      (3) Further discussion should be conducted regarding the reasoning as to why GATA4 increases the phosphorylation of SMAD1/5. 

      Thank you. The underlying mechanism of GATA4 activating SMAD1/5 has not been previously investigated. We have now elaborated on this in the discussion and have added more relevant publications.

      “Our study indicated that there was an observed decrease in chondrogenesis and an increase in hypertrophy-related genes following GATA4 overexpression (Figure 2G).” (Lines 572-574)

      “These previous studies and literature review inspired us to explore the potential association between GATA4 levels and the activation of SMAD1/5.” (Lines 587-588)

      “In this study, it was shown that GATA4 was necessary for bone morphogenic protein-6 (BMP-6) mediated IL-6 induction, in which there are multiple GATA binding domains on the IL-6 promoter. This work further showed that GATA4 interacts with SMAD 2,3 and 4.[55] Studies have suggested that BMP pathways and GATA4 work synergistically to regulate SMAD signaling.56 This information indicates that the involvement of GATA4 in the TGF-b signaling pathway is complex and further studies should be conducted to better assess this relationship.” (Lines 594-599)

      (4) More information should be included to clarify why GATA4 is thought to be linked to DNA damage and the pathway that is associated with that. 

      We have now included further information in the discussion to clarify the association between DNA damage and GATA4 upregulation.

      “The study by Kang et al. demonstrated that the suppression of p62 following DNA damage leads to GATA4 accumulation due to the lack of autophagy.13 DNA damage is known to increase with age.71 Therefore, we believe that DNA damage due to aging is a key driver of the upregulation of GATA4 in old chondrocytes.” (Lines 642-646)

      (5) Please add further information regarding the limitations of the animal study conducted in this work and future plans to assess this. 

      We have included more limitations of the animal study that was conducted in this work and have expanded on the future plans to use inducible GATA4 expression in transgenic mouse lines to study the role of GATA4 overexpression in OA onset and progression.

      “Third, during our in vivo work, the intraarticular injection of GATA4 lentivirus was not chondrocyte-specific. Therefore, the injection also allowed for other cell types to overexpress GATA4. Future work should be conducted using transgenic mouse lines for cartilage-specific inducible overexpression or depletion of Gata4 to further investigate the role of GATA4 in chondrocytes.” (666-670)

      (6) In Figure 5, GATA4 should be changed to Gata4 in the graphed portions for consistency. 

      Thanks. We have made the necessary adjustments throughout the manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      (1) While it is convincing that GATA4 expression is elevated in elderly individuals, and that it has a detrimental impact on cartilage health, the authors might want to add further discussion on the variability among individual human donors, especially given the finding that the elevation of GATA4 was not observed in chondrocytes from donor O1 (Figure 1G).

      The authors thank the reviewer for reviewing our manuscript and providing insightful comments.

      As suggested, we have included more discussion on the variability among donors.

      “Although we found that GATA4 was generally increased with aging, some young donors also exhibited increased levels of GATA4, which may be associated with increased DNA damage, as discussed above, or other stressors. Therefore, GATA4 should be used together in conjunction with other aging biomarkers, such as the epigenetic clock [72] to precisely define chondrocyte aging. Future work should examine biological versus chronological aging and epigenetic clock-based assessments to explain the variabilities in GATA4 expression among donors.” (Lines 658-663)

      (2) It might also be worth adding additional discussion on the interplay between senescent chondrocytes and the dysfunctional ECM during aging. As noted by the authors, aging is associated with decreased sGAG content and likely degenerative changes in the collagen II network, so the microniche of chondrocytes, and thus cell-matrix crosstalk through the pericellular matrix, is also altered or impaired. 

      Thank you for this comment. We have included more discussion on the interplay of chondrocyte senescence and dysfunctional ECM during aging, with a specific focus on the microniche of chondrocytes.

      “Additionally, a common hallmark of chondrocyte aging is the alternation of ECM, including composition change [2] and stiffening.[57] ECM stiffness can directly affect chondrocyte phenotype and proliferation, and contribute to OA.[58] A recent study by Fu et al. associated matrix stiffening with the promotion of chondrocyte senescence.[59] Furthermore, matrix stiffening has been associated with modulating the TGF-b signaling pathway.[60-62] Future studies should investigate the potential of matrix stiffening and the effect of GATA4 on pericellular matrix proteins such as decorin[63,64], biglycan, collagen VI and XV, as these proteins assist with the regulation of biochemical interactions and assist with the maintenance of the chondrocyte microenvironment.[65] Herein, the TGF-b signaling pathway can further alter the extracellular microenvironment[62], which could promote cellular senescence and subsequently NF-kB pathway activation.” (Lines 600-610)

      (2) If applicable, please also add Y3 and O3 to Figure S1 for visual comparison across individual donors. 

      As suggested, we added Y3 and O3 to the revised Figure S1 for more visual comparisons across individual donors.

      (3) Figure 3C, the molecular weight labels are off. 

      Thanks. We corrected this mistake.

      (4) Line 438 - Please clarify in text that the highest efficiency of siRNA chosen was siRNA2. 

      As suggested, we added the reason for selecting siRNA2.

      “Several GATA4 siRNAs were tested, and the one with the highest efficiency was selected based off RT-qPCR results, which indicated that siRNA2 treatment induced lowest expression of GATA4.  (Supplementary Figure S6).” (Lines 448-450)

      (5) Did the authors test the timeline of sustained knockdown of GATA4 by siRNA?

      We used a 7-day timepoint of chondrogenesis, and RT-qPCR results demonstrated that there was a downregulation of GATA4 expression at this timepoint (Figure 4). In the current in vitro study, we did not examine the efficacy of GATA4 siRNA for longer than 7 days.

      Reviewer #3( Public review):

      (1) It would be useful to explain why GATA4 was chosen over HIF1a, which was the most differentially expressed. 

      The authors thank the reviewer for reviewing our manuscript and providing insightful comments.

      When we first saw the results, we did consider studying the role of HIF1a in aging because it was the most differentially expressed. When we reviewed the relevant literature, we found that HIF1a was commonly upregulated in aged individuals which was thought to be linked to hypoxia and increased oxidated stress (PMID: 12470896, PMID: 12573436). Further investigation found studies that investigated HIF1a in chondrocytes and the use of in vivo work to investigate its role in osteoarthritis (PMID: 32214220). Indicating that HIF1a plays a protective role during OA by suppressing the activation of NF-kB pathway.  Moreover, there is work that has been conducted assessing the stabilization of HIF1a by regulating mitophagy and using HIF1a as a potential therapeutic target for OA (PMID: 32587244). Since there have been many studies investigating the correlation of HIF1a expression and OA, we felt that it would be more innovative to look at other molecules, such as GATA4. Moreoever, as we highlighted in the Introducion and Disucussion, through testing in cell types other than chondrocytes, GATA4 was shown to be associated with DNA damage and senescence, which are both aging hallmarks. Given the fact that roles of GATA4 in chodnrocytes had not been previous studies, we thus chose GATA4 in this study. 

      “Of note, Hypoxia-Inducible Factor 1a (HIF1a) was the most differentially expressed gene predicted to regulate chondrocyte aging. The connection between HIF1a and aging has been previously reported.32 Furthermore, additional studies have investigated HIF1a in association with OA and assessed its use as a therapeutic target.[33,34] Therefore, we decided to focus on GATA4, which was less studied in chondrocytes but highly associated with cellular senescence, an aging hallmark. However, our selection did not dampen the importance of HIF1α and other molecules listed in Figure 1D in chondrocyte aging. They can be further studied in the future using the same strategy employed in the current work.” (Lines 526-533)

      (2) In Figure 5, it would be useful to demonstrate the non-surgical or naive limbs to help contextualize OARSI scores and knee hyperalgesia changes. 

      Thank you for your comment. Based on prior experience, the OARSI score of mice in the sham group had an OARSI score ranging from 0-0.5. In the current study, we focused on the DMM control and DMM Gata4 virus groups so we did not include a sham control group. We recognized this was a limitation of this study.

      “We measured the naive limbs for knee hyperalgesia before DMM surgery, and found the average threshold was 507g. We have highlighted the threshold measurement in the figure legend.507 g was the threshold baseline for non-surgery mice (dashed line).” (Lines 499-500)

      (3) While there appear to be GATA4 small-molecule inhibitors in various stages of development that could be used to assess the effects in age-related OA, those experiments are out of scope for the current study. 

      We agree with this comment that the results are still preliminary, which was the reason that we put it in the supplementary materials. However, we felt like the result is informative, which will support the potential of GATA4 as a therapeutic target and inspire the development of more specific inhibitors. Therefore, if the reviewer agrees, we want to keep the results in the current study.

      In particular, our in vitro study demonstrated the potential of using small-molecule GATA4 to enhance the quality of cartilage created by old chondrocytes. We can validate the findings in vivo, as well as develop other GATA4 inhibitors. (Lines 673-675)

      (4) Is GATA4 upregulated in chondrocytes in publicly available databases? 

      Thank you for this question. We have examined the public databases and have found that there is data showing the trend that GATA4 is upregulated in aged or OA chondrocytes in work conducted by Ungethuem et al (PMID: 20858714). In one study by Ramos et al. (PMID: 25054223), we noticed that GATA4 expression levels were the same in both young and old groups, which may be due to the relatively smaller sample size in the young group compared to old group (4 vs 26).

      Work Conducted by Grogan et al. (Unpublished https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/geo/query/acc.cgi?acc=GSE39795)

      Author response image 1.

      Author response image 2.

      Work conducted by Ramos et al. (PMID: 25054223).<br />

      Author response image 3.

      Work conducted by Ungethuem et al (PMID: 20858714).<br />

      (5) In many cases, the figure captions describe the experiment vs. the outcome. It may be more compelling to state the main finding in the figure title, and you might consider changing it from what is stated at present. For example, Figure 2: instead of the impact of overexpression, you may say GATA4 overexpression impairs cartilage formation (as stated in the results).

      Thanks for the suggestion. We have made the following changes to the figure captions as suggested.

      Figure 1: GATA4 is upregulated in aged chondrocytes (Line 373)

      Figure 2: Overexpressing GATA4 impairs the hyaline cartilage formation capacity of young chondrocytes (Lines 408-409)

      Figure 3: GATA4 overexpression activates SMAD1/5  (Line 436)

      Figure 4: Suppressing GATA4 in old chondrocytes promotes cartilage formation and lowers expression of proinflammatory cytokines (Line 467)

      Figure 5: Gata4 overexpression in the knee joints accelerates OA progression in mice. (Line 593)

      (6) It would be useful to provide a little more information about the human tissue donors, if that is available. 

      We have provided more information about the tissue donors in the revised Supplementary Table S1.

      (7) While aging-like changes were observed in young chondrocytes with GATA4 overexpression, it would be interesting to directly evaluate if there is a change in biological versus chronological age in these tissues. Companies like Zymo can provide this biological v chronological age epigenetic clock-based assessments if that is of interest, to say the young chondrocytes are looking "older". 

      Thank you for this information. We agree that it will be important to assess epigenetic changes in GATA-overexpressing cells. We are contacting the company to learn more about their technology. Meanwhile, we added this to the future work section of the manuscript.

      “Although we found that GATA4 was generally increased with aging, some young donors also exhibited increased levels of GATA4, which may be associated with increased DNA damage, as discussed above, or other stressors. Therefore, GATA4 should be used together in conjunction with other aging biomarkers, such as the epigenetic clock [72] to precisely define chondrocyte aging. Future work should examine biological versus chronological aging and epigenetic clock-based assessments to explain the variabilities in GATA4 expression among donors.”  (Lines 658-663)

      (8) It is not clear the age at which the mice received DMM in the methods, but it is shown in Figure 5. 

      We have added the age at which the mice received the DMM surgery to the methods section.

      “Intraarticular injections were administered to mice between 10-12 weeks of age under general anesthesia to safeguard the well-being of the animals and to minimize procedural discomfort.” (Line 300)

      “One week after viral vector injection, DMM surgery was performed to induce the OA model on mice 11-13 weeks of age.” (Line 312-313)

      (9) It is not clear which factors were assayed using Luminex, and it would be great to add. 

      Thank you for this comment, we have added a comprehensive list of proteins assessed using Luminex into a new supplementary table 6 (S6).

      (10) Also interesting, loss of GATA4 seems to prevent diet-induced obesity in mice and promote insulin sensitivity (potentially via GLP-1 secretion). I wonder if there may be a metabolic axis here too? PMID: 21177287. I may have missed parts of the discussion of the role of GATA4 in metabolism, but it might be an interesting addition to the discussion. 

      In the current study, we have not investigated the role of GATA4 in obesity. As suggested, we have included a discussion of GATA4 in metabolism.

      “Furthermore, GATA4 might be associated with metabolic regulation. A study conducted by Patankar et al. investigated how GATA4 regulates obesity. Specifically, they used intestine-specific Gata4 knockout mice to study diet-induced obesity, showing that the knockout mice were resistant to the high-fat diet, and that glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1) release was increased. These findings indicated a decreased risk for the development for insulin resistance in knockout mice.[44] This work was taken a step further in a subsequent publication, in which the same team investigated the dietary lipid-dependent and independent effects on the development of steatosis and fibrosis in Gata4 knockout mice. The results from this work suggested that the knockdown of Gata4 increases GLP-1 release, in turn suppressing the development of hepatic steatosis and fibrosis, ultimately blocking hepatic de novo lipogenesis.[45] These studies are especially interesting with the rise of GLP-1 based therapy for the treatment of OA.46,47 Thus, the coupling of GATA4-related metabolic dysfunction and OA should be further investigated.” (Lines 542-553)

      (11) Another potential citation: GATA4 regulates angiogenesis and persistence of inflammation in rheumatoid arthritis PMID: 29717129 - around the inflammatory axis potential in OA? since GATA4 was reported in FLS from OA- PMC11183113.

      Thank you. We have included this work/citation in the discussion section.\

      “Further studies have shown that GATA4 regulates angiogenesis and inflammation in fibroblast-like synoviocytes in rheumatoid arthritis, indicating that GATA4 is required for the inflammation induced by IL-1b. This study also demonstrated that GATA4 binds to promoter regions on Vascular Endothelial Growth Factor (VEGF)-A and VEGFC to enhance transcription and regulate angiogenesis.[15]”  (Lines 558-562)

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      In the retina, parallel processing of cone photoreceptor output under bright light conditions dissects critical features of our visual environment and is fundamental to visual function. Cone photoreceptor signals are sampled by several types of bipolar cells and passed onto the ganglion cells. At the output of retinal processing, retinal ganglion cells send about 40 different codes of the visual scene to the brain for further processing. In this study, the authors focus on whether subtype-specific differences in the size of synaptic ribbon-associated vesicle pools of bipolar cells contribute to different retinal ganglion cell (RGC) responses. Specifically, inputs to ON alpha RGCs producing transient versus sustained kinetics (ON-S vs. ON-T, respectively) are compared. The authors first demonstrate that ON-S vs. ON-T RGCs are readily identifiable in a whole mount preparation and respond differently to both static and to a spatially uniform, randomly fluctuating (Gaussian noise) light stimulus. Liner-nonlinear (LN) models were used to estimate the transformation between visual input and excitatory synaptic input for each RGCs; these models suggested the presence of transient versus sustained kinetics already in the excitatory inputs to ON-T and ON-S RGCs. Indeed, the authors show that (glutamatergic) excitatory inputs to ON-S vs. ON-T RGCs are of distinct kinetics. The subtypes of bipolar cells providing input to ON-S are known (i.e., type 6 and 7), but the source of excitatory bipolar inputs to ON-T RGCs needed to be determined. In a tedious process, it is elegantly shown here that ON-T RGCs receive most of their excitatory inputs from type 5 and 6 bipolars. Interestingly, the temporal properties of light-evoked responses of type 5, 6, and 7 bipolars recorded from the somas were indistinguishable and rather sustained, suggesting that the origin of transient kinetics of excitatory inputs to ON-T RGCs suggested by the LN model might be found in the processing of visual signals at the bipolar cell axon terminal. Blocking GABA- or glycinergic inhibitory inputs did not alter the light-evoked excitatory input kinetics to ON-T and ON-S RGCs. Twophoton glutamate sensor imaging revealed significantly faster kinetics of light-evoked glutamate signals at ON-T versus ON-S RGCs. Detailed EM analysis of bipolar cell ribbon synapses onto ON-T and ON-S RGCs revealed fewer ribbon-associated vesicles at ON-T synapses, which is consistent with stronger paired-flash depression of lightevoked excitatory currents in ON-T RGCS versus ON-S RGCs. This study suggests that bipolar subtype-specific differences in the size of synaptic ribbon-associated vesicle pools contribute to transient versus sustained kinetics in RGCs. 

      Strengths: 

      The use of multiple, state-of-the-art tools and approaches to address the kinetics of bipolar to ganglion cell synapse in an identified circuit. 

      Weaknesses: 

      For the most part, the data in the paper support the conclusions, and the authors were careful to try to address questions in multiple ways. Two-photon glutamate sensor imaging experiment showing that blocking GABA- and glycinergic inhibition does not change the kinetics of light-evoked glutamate signals at ON-T RGCs would strengthen the conclusion that bipolar subtype-specific differences in the size of synaptic ribbon-associated vesicle pools contribute to transient versus sustained kinetics in RGCs. 

      Thank you for this suggestion. We have revised the text throughout to be careful not to imply that amacrine cells have no role in shaping EPSCs and spike output, but instead that the transience of the On-T responses persists without amacrine cells (see for example lines 91, 450-453, 514-518, 696-714). We have also added additional iGluSnFR experiments to the paper to further test this conclusion (new Figure 7). The new data shows that the transience of glutamate release from the On-T cells is retained when 1) spiking amacrine cell activity is suppressed by blocking voltage-gated Na<sup>+</sup> channels with TTX or 2) all amacrine cell activity is suppressed by blocking AMPA receptors with NBQX. This does provide nice additional evidence that amacrine cells are not necessary for the sustained/transient distinction.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      Goal of the study. The authors tried to pinpoint the origins of transient and sustained responses measured at retinal ganglion cells (rgcs), which is the output layer of the retina. Response characteristics of rgcs are used to group them into different types. The diversity of rgc types represents the ability of the retina to transform visual inputs into distinct output channels. They find that the physical dimensions of bipolar cell's synaptic ribbons (specialized release sites/active zones) vary across the different types of cone on-bpcs, in ways that they argue could facilitate transient or sustained release. This diversity of release output is what they argue underlies the differences in on-rgcs response characteristics, and ultimately represents a mechanism for creating parallel cone-driven channels. 

      Strengths: 

      The major strengths of the study are the anatomical approaches employed and the use of the "glutamate sniffer" to assay synaptic glutamate levels. The outline of the study is elegant and reflects the strengths of the authors. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The major weakness is that the ambitious outline is not matched with a complete set of results, and the set of physiological protocols is disjointed, not sufficient to bridge the systems-level question with the presynaptic release question. 

      Thank you for this comment as it provides an opportunity (here and in the paper) for us to clarify our main goal. We wanted to link the well-established distinction between transient and sustained retinal responses to anatomy. This required locating where this difference arises within the circuitry – which we show to be at least largely the bipolar output synapse – and then examining the structure of this synapse in detail. While we would certainly be interested in connecting our results to a biophysical description of the synapse, that was not the primary focus of our study and was not something we could add without substantial additional work.  

      Major comments on the results and suggestions. 

      The ribbon model of release has been explored for decades and needs to be further adapted to systems-level work. The study under consideration by Kuo et al. takes on this task. Unfortunately, the experimental design does not permit a level of control over presynaptic/bpc behavior that is comparable to earlier studies, nor do they manipulate release in ways that test the ribbon model (i.e., paired recordings or Ribeye-ko). Furthermore, the data needs additional evaluation, and the presentation and interpretations should draw on published biophysical and molecular studies. 

      As described above, our goal was to test several possible explanations for the difference between transient and sustained responses in OnT and OnS ganglion cells: (1) differences in the light responses of the bipolar cells that convey photoreceptor signals to the relevant ganglion cells; (2) shaping of bipolar transmitter release by presynaptic inhibition; (3) shaping of ganglion cell responses by postsynaptic inhibition or spike generation; (4) differences in feedforward bipolar synapses. We were surprised to find that the feedforward bipolar synapses play a central role in this difference, and your comment nicely prompts us to relate this to the large literature on biophysical studies of release from ribbon synapses. We have made substantial revisions in the text to do this. This includes anticipating the importance of feedforward synaptic properties in the abstract and introduction (lines 36-37 and 61-64), pointers in the results (lines 539-548), and several new paragraphs in the discussion (starting on lines 751, 773 and 787). By showing that the transient/sustained differences originates largely at feedforward bipolar synapses, we set the stage for future work that shows how biophysical properties of the synapse shape physiological signals that traverse it.

      To build a ribbon-centric context, consider recent literature that supports the assertion that ribbons play a role in forming AZ release sites and facilitating exocytosis. Reference Ribeye-ko studies. For example, ribbonless bpcs show an 80% reduction in release (Maxeiner et al EMBO J 2016), the ribbonless retina exhibits signaling deficits at the output layer (Okawa et al ...Rieke, ..Wong Nat Comm 2019), and ribbonless rods show an 80% reduction the readily releasable pool (RRP) of SVs (Grabner Moser, elife 2021). In addition, the authors could refer to whole-cell membrane capacitance studies on mammalian rods, cones, and bpcs, because the size of the RRP of SVs scales with the dimensions and numbers of ribbons (total ribbon footprint). For comparison, bipolars see the review by Wan and Heidelberger 2011. For a comparison of mammalian rods and cones, see, rods: Grabner and Moser (2021 eLife), Mueller.. Regus Leidig et al. (2019; J Neurosci) and cones Grabner ...DeVries (Nat Comm 2023). A comparison of cell types shows that the extent of release is (1) proportional to the total size of the ribbon footprint, and (2) less release is witnessed when ribbons are deleted (also see photo ablation studies by Snellman.... And Mehta..Zenisek, Nat Neurosci and Neuron).

      Thank you for these pointers into the literature.  We have included much of this work in the revised Discussion (see three paragraphs starting on line 751). The revised text focuses on the evidence that larger and more numerous ribbons lead to increased release. The direct evidence from previous work for this relationship supports our (indirect) conclusions in the current paper about the role of ribbon size and associated vesicle pools in transient vs sustained responses.  

      Ribbon morphology may change in an activity-dependent manner. The rod ribbon AZ has been reported to lengthen in the dark (Dembla et al 2020), and deletion of the ribbon shortens the length of the AZ (defined by Cav1,4 or RIM2); in addition, the Ribeye-ko AZs fail to change in size with light and dark conditioning. Furthermore, EM studies on rod and cone AZs in light and dark argue that the number of SVs at the base of the ribbon increases in the dark, when PRs are depolarized (see Figure 10, Babai et al 2016 JNeurosci). Lastly, using goldfish Mb1 on-bipolars, Hull et al (2006, J Neurophysio) correlated an increase in release efficiency with an increase in ribbon numbers, which accompanied daylight. >> When release activity is high, ribbon AZ length increases (Dembla, rods), the number of docked SVs increases (Babai, rods cones), and the number of ribbons increases (Hull, diurnal Mb1s). 

      We have extensively revised the discussion section to include more discussion of ribbons, particularly emphasizing evidence supporting the general argument that larger ribbons support higher release rates. We focused on studies that provided direct links between release rates and ribbon size or number of ribbon-associated vesicles.  This includes studies that pair electrophysiology and anatomy and those that measure the consequences of ablating ribbons,

      The results under review, Kuo et al., were attained with SBF-SEM, which has the benefit of addressing large-volume questions as required here, yet it achieves lower spatial resolution than what is attained with TEM tomography and FIB-EM. Ideally, the EM description would include SV size, and the density of ribbon-tethered SVs that are docked at the plasma membrane, because this is where the SVs fuse (additional non-ribbon release sites may also exist? Mehta ... Singer 2014 J Neurosci). Studies by Graydon et al 2011 and 2014 (both in J Neurosci), and Jean ... Moser et al 2018 (eLife) are good examples of quantitative estimates of SVs docking sites at ribbons. SBF-SEM does not allow for an assessment of SVs within 5 nm of the PM, but if the authors can identify the number of SVs that appear within the limit of resolution (10 to 15 nm) from the PM, then this data would be useful. Also, what dimension(s) of the large ribbons make them larger? Typically, ribbons are fixed in height (at least in the outer retina, 200 to 250 nm), but their length varies and the number ribbons per terminal varies. Is the larger ribbon size observed in type 6 bpcs do to longer ribbons, or taller ribbons? A longer ribbon likely has more docked SVs. An additional possibility is that more SVs are about the ribbon-PM footprint, either more densely packed and/or expanding laterally (see definitions in Jean....Moser, elife 2018). 

      We have included an additional analysis of ribbon surface area from our 3D SBFSEM reconstructions. As with the volume measurements included in the original submission, ribbon surface areas are distinct between type 5i and type 6 bipolar cells (Fig. S10A), ON-T RGCs on average receive input from ribbons with smaller surface area than ON-S RGCs (Fig. S10B), and ribbon surface area predicts the number of adjacent vesicles across bipolar cell types (Fig. S10C).  We agree that a higher resolution view of presynaptic structures would be very helpful, but the resolution of our SBF-SEM data is limited (e.g. each pixel is 40 nm on a side).  This resolution does not allow us to distinguish between vesicles at vs near the membrane. 

      In our observations, both length and height of the ribbons showed variability across individual bipolar cells. And ribbons in type 6 bipolar cells tended to be either longer and/or taller compared to those in type 5 cells. We agree that a longer ribbon may accommodate more docked SVs. A more definitive analysis would benefit from higher-resolution, isotropic 3D reconstructions of ribbons, which would allow more precise shape analysis and ,together with a detailed assessment of docked SVs at the ribbons.

      The ribbon literature given above makes the argument that ribbons increase exocytotic output, and morphological studies suggest that release activity enhances 1) ribbon length (Dembla) and 2) the density of SVs near the PM (Babai). These findings could lead one to propose that type 6 bpcs (inputs to On-sustained) are more active than type 5i (feed into On-transient). Here Kuo et al. show that the bpcs have similar Vm (measured from the soma) in response to light stimulation. Does Vm predict release? Not entirely as the authors acknowledge, because: Cav channel properties, SV availability, and negative feedback are all downstream of bpc Vm. The only experiment performed to test downstream factors focused on negative feedback from amacrines. The data presented in Figures 5C-F led me to conclude the opposite of what the authors concluded. My impression is that the T-ON rgc exhibits strong disinhibition when GABA-blockers are applied (the initial phase is greatly increased in amplitude and broadened with the drug), which contrasts with the S-On rgc responses that show a change in the amplitude of the initial phase but not its width (taus would be nice). Here and in many places the authors refer to changes in release kinetics, without implementing a useful description of kinetics. For instance, take the cumulative current (charge) in Figure 5C and fit the control and drug traces to arrive at taus, and their respective amplitudes, and use these values to describe kinetic phases. One final point, the summary in Figure 5D has a p: 0.06, very close to the cutoff for significance, which begs for more than an n = 5. Given that previous studies have shown that bpc output is shaped by immediate msec GABA feedback, in ways that influence kinetic phases of release (..Mb1 bipolars, see Vigh et al 2005 Neuron), more attention to this matter is needed before the authors rule out feedback inhibition in favor of ribbon size. If by chance, type 5i bpcs are under uniquely strong feedback inhibition, then ribbon size may result from less activity, not less output resulting from smaller ribbons.

      The text surrounding Figure 5 led to some confusion, and we have revised that text and the figure for clarity.  First, the data in that figure is entirely from On-T cells (the upper and lower panels show block of GABA and glycine receptors separately).  Second, the observation that we make there is that block of inhibitory receptors increases the transience of the On-T excitatory input, rather than decreasing it as would be expected if the transience is created by presynaptic inhibition. We have added additional data and that increase in transience is now significant. Inhibitory block does substantially increase the amplitude of the postsynaptic response, and a likely origin of this change in response is inhibitory feedback to the bipolar synaptic terminal. We now indicate this in the text on page 13, lines 438-453. 

      The key result of this figure for our purposes here is that the transience of the excitatory input to the OffT cell remains with inhibitory input blocked. We have clarified throughout the text that our results indicate that inhibitory feedback is not necessary for the difference between transient release into On-T and sustained release onto On-S. This does not mean that inhibitory feedback does not shape the responses in other ways or contribute to the transient/sustained difference - just that for the specific stimuli we use that difference is retained without presynaptic inhibition. We have also added citations to past work showing that activity of amacrine cells can modulate bipolar transmitter release. 

      Whether strong feedback inhibition limits activity and therefore limits ribbon size in an activity-dependent way is an intriguing possibility. Indeed, addressing why ribbons are larger in type 6 bipolar cells vs. other bipolar types will be an interesting avenue of further study. However, it would be surprising if ribbon sizes changed during the acute pharmacological block conditions (~10-15 minutes) we employed in our study. Our point here is that there is an interesting correlation between presynaptic ribbon size and the kinetics of glutamate release. We do not think that the two possibilities stated in the last sentence (“…ribbon size may result from less activity, not less output resulting from smaller ribbons”) are mutually exclusive.

      We have not further quantified the response kinetics in the experiments of Figure 5 as the large changes induced by the pharmacology (especially GABA receptor block) make it unclear how to interpret quantitative differences.  In other places we have quantified kinetics through the STA or specified that our focus was more qualitative (i.e. transient vs sustained kinetics). 

      As mentioned above, the behavior of Cav channels is important here. This is difficult to address with voltage clamps from the soma, especially in the Vm range relevant to this study. Given that it has previously been modeled that the rod bpc to AII pathway adapts to prolonged depolarization of rbcs through downregulating Cav channel-mediated Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx (Grimes ....Rieke 2014 Neuron), it seems important for Kou et al to test if there is a difference in Cav regulation between type 6 and 5i bpcs. Ca<sup>2+</sup>  imaging with a GCaMP strategy (Baden....Lagnado Current Biology, 2011) or filling the presynapse with Ca dyes (see inner hair cells: Ozcete and Moser, EMBO J 2020) would allow for the correlation of [Ca]intra with GluSnf signals (both local readouts).

      This is a good suggestion but is outside the scope of our current paper. Our focus was on the circuit origin of the difference in response of the OnT and OnS responses rather than the specific biophysical mechanism.  We are of course interested in the mechanism, but the additional experiments needed to pin that down would need to be a part of future experiments. The work here represents an important step in that direction as it greatly reduces the number of possible locations and mechanisms for the sustained/transient difference and hence serves to focus any future mechanistic investigations.

      Stimulation protocol and presentation of Glutamate Sniffer data in Figure 6. In all of your figures where you state steady st as a % of pk amplitude, please indicate in the figure where you estimate steady state. Alternatively, if you take the cumulative dF/F signal, then you can fit the different kinetic phases. From the appearance of the data, the Sustained Glu signals look like square waves (Figure 6B ROI1-4), without a transient at onset, which is not predicted in your ribbon model that assumes different kinetic phases (1. depletion of docked SVs, and 2. refilling and repriming). The Transient responses (Figure 6B ROI5-8) are transient and more compatible with a depressing ribbon scheme. If you take the cumulative, for all of the On-S and compare it to all of the On-T responses, my guess is the cumulative dF/F will be 10 to 20 larger for the S-On. Would you conclude that bpc inputs to On-S (type 6) release 20fold more SVs per 4 seconds on a per ribbon basis, and does the surface area of the type 6 bpcs account for this difference? From Figures 8B and D, the volume of the ribbon is ~2 fold greater for type 6 vs 5i, but the Surface Area (both faces of ribbon) is more relevant to your model that claims ribbon size is the pivotal factor. If making cumulative traces, and comparisons on an absolute scale is unfounded, then we need to know how to compare different observations. The classic ribbon models always have a conversion factor such as the capacitance of an SV or q size that is used to derive SV numbers from total dCm or Qcontent. See Kim ....et al von Gersdorff, 2023, Cell Reports. Why not use the Gaussian noise stimulus in Fig 6 as in Figure 1 and 2? 

      For iGluSnFR recordings, steady-state responses were measured from the mean fluorescence over the last 1 sec of the light step (2 sec duration) response. We have included this information in the figure caption and in the Methods. 

      There is a good deal of variability in the iGluSnR responses from one ROI to another, and the ROIs shown in the original submission had a less prominent transient component than many other ROIs. We have replaced this figure with another that is more representative of the average behavior across ROIs. The full range of behavior is captured in Figure 6C; it is clear across ROIs that glutamate release near ON-S dendrites shows both sustained and transient components. The new experiments in which we block amacrine cell activity also include a few more example ROIs from ON-S cells, and those also show both transient and sustained components.

      Your suggestion to integrate the iGluSnFR signals to compare to our structural analysis of ribbons is interesting. However, we are hesitant to make a quantitative comparison between the two without further experiments to validate how the iGluSnFR signals we measure relate to release of single vesicles. For example, a quantitative measure of release based on the iGluSnR experiments would require accounting for possible differences in the expression of the indicator - which could differ both in overall level and/or location relative to release sites. 

      This comment and one above highlight the importance of measures of ribbon surface area, which we now provide (Figure S10).

      Figure 7. What is the recovery time for mammalian cones derived from ribbon-based models? There are estimates from membrane capacitance studies. Ground squirrel cones take 0.7 to 1 sec to recover the ultrafast, primed pool of SVs when probed with a paired-pulse protocol (Grabner ...DeVries 2016, Neuron). Their off-bpcs take anywhere from under 0.2 sec to a second to recover, which is a combination of many synaptic factors (Grabner ...DeVries Nat Comm 2023). Rod On bpcs take over a second (Singer Diamond 2006, reviewed Wan and Heidelberger 2011). In Figure 7B, the recovery time is ~150 ms for the responses measured at rgcs. This brief recovery time is incompatible with existing ribbon models of release. Whole-cell membrane capacitance measurements would be helpful here.

      Thanks for drawing our attention to this issue. Indeed, we see a relatively rapid recovery in the paired-flash experiments. We now discuss this recovery time in the context of past measurements of recovery of responses in cones and bipolar cells (paragraph starting on line 773). There are many factors that could contribute to the relatively rapid recovery we observe - including synaptic factors such as those highlighted by Grabner et al., (2016) either at the cone-to-bipolar synapses or the bipolar-to-RGC synapses. We are certainly interested in a more detailed understanding of this issue, but the additional experiments are outside the scope of this paper.  

      Experimental Suggestion: Add GABA blockers and see if type 5i bpc responds with more release (GluSniff) and prolonged [Ca2+] intra (GCaMP). Compare this to type 6 bpc behavior with GABA/gly blockers. This will rule in or out whether feedback inhibition is involved. 

      Figure 7 in the revised manuscript includes two new experiments examining glutamate release (without the simultaneous measurement of bipolar cell intracellular calcium) while blocking (1) all/most amacrine cell-mediated inhibition via inclusion of NBQX in the bath solution, and (2) blocking spiking amacrine cells via inclusion of TTX in the bath solution. The transient vs sustained difference in light-evoked glutamate release around ON-T and ON-S RGC dendrites remained with amacrine activity suppressed. These new results are consistent with the anatomical and pharmacological data that were included in the initial submission of the manuscript (Fig. 5) that indicate presynaptic inhibition does not have a major role in shaping release kinetics at these synapses. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      Different types of retinal ganglion cell (RGC) have different temporal properties - most prominently a distinction between sustained vs. transient responses to contrast. This has been well established in multiple species, including mice. In general, RGCs with dendrites that stratify close to the ganglion cell layer (GCL) are sustained; whereas those that stratify near the middle of the inner plexiform layer (IPL) are transient. This difference in RGC spiking responses aligns with similar differences in excitatory synaptic currents as well as with differences in glutamate release in the respective layers - shown previously and here, with a glutamate sensor (iGluSnFR) expressed in the RGCs of interest. Differences in glutamate release were not explained by differences in the distinct presynaptic bipolar cells' voltage responses, which were quite similar to one another. Rather, the difference in transient vs. sustained responses seems to emerge at the bipolar cell axon terminals in the form of glutamate release. This difference in the temporal pattern of glutamate release was correlated with differences in the size of synaptic ribbons (larger in the bipolar cells with more sustained responses), which also correlated with a greater number of vesicles in the vicinity of the larger ribbons. 

      The main conclusion of the study relates to a correlation (because it is difficult to manipulate ribbon size or vesicle density experimentally): the bipolar cells with increased ribbon size/vesicle number would have a greater possibility of sustained release, which would be reflected in the postsynaptic RGC synaptic currents and RGC firing rates. This model proposes a mechanism for temporal channels that is independent of synaptic inhibition. Indeed, some experiments in the paper suggest that inhibition cannot explain the transient nature of glutamate release onto one of the RGC types. Still, it is surprising that such a diverse set of inhibitory interneurons in the retina would not play some role in diversifying the temporal properties of RGC responses. 

      Strengths: 

      (1) The study uses a systematic approach to evaluating temporal properties of retinal ganglion cell (RGC) spiking outputs, excitatory synaptic inputs, presynaptic voltage responses, and presynaptic glutamate release. The combination of these experiments demonstrates an important step in the conversion from voltage to glutamate release in shaping response dynamics in RGCs. 

      (2) The study uses a combination of electrophysiology, two-photon imaging, and scanning block-face EM to build a quantitative and coherent story about specific retinal circuits and their functional properties. 

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) There were some interesting aspects of the study that were not completely resolved, and resolving some of these issues may go beyond the current study. For example, it was interesting that different extracellular media (Ames medium vs. ACSF) generated different degrees of transient vs. sustained responses in RGCs, but it was unclear how these media might have impacted ion channels at different levels of the circuit that could explain the effects on temporal tuning.

      We do not have an explanation for the quantitative differences in response kinetics we observed in Ames’ medium vs. ACSF. There are modest differences in calcium and magnesium concentration and a larger difference in potassium (2.5 mM in ACSF vs 3.6 mM in Ames). It would be interesting to test which of these (or other) differences accounts for the difference in response kinetics.

      (2) It was surprising that inhibition played such a small role in generating temporal tuning. At the same time, there were some gaps in the investigation of inhibition (e.g., IPSCs were not measured in either of the RGC types; pharmacology was used to investigate responses only in the transient RGCs).

      We were also surprised at this result. We have included additional data on inhibition in the revised manuscript. Figure S3 shows light-evoked IPSC data from both RGC types (Fig. S3) and Fig. 7 shows additional iGluSnFR measurements around both ON-T and ON-S RGC dendrites with inhibition blocked via bath application of NBQX (Fig. 7) and separately with inhibition from spiking amacrine cells blocked with TTX. These experiments provide additional evidence for the small role of inhibition. We attempted to measure the kinetics of excitatory input to ON-S cells with inhibition blocked, but we found that the excitatory input showed strong spontaneous oscillations under these conditions and the light responses were changed so drastically that we did not feel we could make a clear comparison with control conditions.

      (3) There could be additional discussion and references to the literature describing several topics, including: temporal dynamics of glutamate release at different levels of the IPL; previous evidence that release sites from a single presynaptic neuron can differ in their temporal properties depending on the postsynaptic target; previous investigations of the role of inhibition in temporal tuning within retinal circuitry. 

      Thanks, we have included more discussion and references to the relevant literature as you have suggested in the recommendations to authors.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      The presented raw data of the pharmacological experiments show that SR95531 and TPMPA robustly increased both the amplitude and duration of the transient component of the light step-evoked excitatory currents, with slight, if any enhancement of the sustained component in ON-T RGCs Figure 5C. Statistical analysis of the population data (n=5) with Wilcoxon signed rank test yielded no significant difference (ln 363). However, reanalyzing the data extracted from the graph (Figure 5D) revealed that the difference between the paired observations is normally distributed (Shapiro-Wilk normality test, P=0.48) allowing parametric statistics to be used, which provides higher statistical power. Accordingly, reanalyzing the presented data with paired Student's t-test data revealed significant differences (P=0.01) in the steady-state amplitude normalized to that of the peak, recorded in the presence of SR95531 and TPMPA. In other words, based on the (rough) analysis of the presented pharmacology data GABAergic feedback inhibition significantly contributes to shaping the transient portion of the light-evoked excitatory currents in ON-T RGCs, by making it more transient. I believe a similar analysis based on the actual data is necessary, and the results should be communicated either way. However, if warranted, two-photon glutamate sensor imaging experiments showing that blocking GABA- and glycinergic inhibition does not change the kinetics of light-evoked glutamate signals at ON-T RGCs should also be performed, as these would be critical in drawing a conclusion regarding the effect of feedback inhibition on glutamate release from bipolar cells.

      Thanks for this feedback. We have added another cell to the data set in Fig. 5D. With this addition, SR95531/TPMPA application significantly increases the response transience of excitatory currents measured in ON-T RGCs compared to control. This enhanced transience in GABA<sub>A/C</sub> receptor blockers is due to an increase in the amplitude of the initial peak component of the response (control peak amplitude: -833.7±103.3 pA; SR95531+TPMPA peak amplitude: 2023±372.7pA; p=0.03, Wilcoxon signed rank test), with no change to the later sustained component (control plateau amplitude: -200.7±14.71pA; SR95531+TPMPA plateau amplitude: -290.9±43.69pA; p=0.15, Wilcoxon signed rank test).

      We should clarify that this result indicates that GABAergic inhibition makes the excitatory inputs to ON-T RGCs less transient. Block of GABA receptors increased transience, thus intact GABAergic transmission appears to limit the initial peak of the response and therefore make excitatory currents more sustained. We unfortunately were not able to examine whether sustained excitatory currents in ON-S RGCs would become more transient using the same approach. In our hands, bath application of SR95531+TPMPA led to the generation of large-amplitude (>1nA) oscillatory bursts of excitatory input that developed within 5 minutes and persisted for the duration of the incubation (up to ~30 min) in drugs. Further, presentation of light steps tended to induce variable amplitude responses, likely dependent on the presence of spontaneous bursts; when large amplitude responses were evoked, these typically oscillated for several seconds after the step.

      To examine a potential role for presynaptic inhibition in transient vs. sustained bipolar cell output, we therefore chose to eliminate amacrine cell-mediated inhibition by bath application of the AMPA/kainate receptor antagonist NBQX in additional iGluSnFR measurements. This manipulation should leave ON bipolar cell responses intact while eliminating most amacrine cell-mediated responses (and OFF bipolar cell driven responses). In separate experiments, we also eliminated inhibition from spiking amacrine cells by bath application of TTX. As shown in new Fig. 7, sustained and transient responses persisted in distal versus proximal RGC dendrites, respectively. Compared to SR95531/TPMPA, bath application of NBQX was not associated with spontaneous bursts of glutamate release around ON-S dendrites. These results show that amacrine cell-mediated inhibition is not required for either sustained or transient glutamate release from bipolar cells that provide input to ON-S and ON-T RGCs.

      Small points: 

      (1) The legend of Figure 1 (D) refers to shaded areas to show {plus minus} SEM, but no shade is visible (at least in my printout).

      The SEM shading is there in Fig. 1D but is mostly obscured by the mean lines for the respective RGC types. We have added this to the figure caption.

      (2) I found the reported Vrest for the ON bipolar cells somewhat depolarized. Perhaps due to the uncompensated junction potentials? 

      These measurements are indeed not corrected for the liquid junction potential (which is approximately -10.8 mV between K-gluconate internal and Ames’ solution). We did not apply this correction since the appropriate value is not clear in perforated patch recordings as the intracellular chloride concentration is unknown (and can differ from that in the pipette solution). We have clarified this in the results text where we describe the Vrest values (lines 335-338).

      (3) It is Wilcoxon signed rank test, not Wilcoxan. 

      Thanks for catching this. This has been corrected in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      Some amacrines express vesicular Glut-3 transporter and are reported to release glutamate (Marshak, Vis Neurosci 2016). Are Amacrine vGlut3 signals postsynaptic (within ~0.5 um) to cone bpc ribbons?

      We did not characterize VgluT3-expressing amacrine cells in our SEM datasets. A recent study by Friedrichson et al. (Nat. Comm. 2024; PMID 38580652) using 3D SEM reconstructions found that Vglut3-amacrines are postsynaptic to both type 5i and type 6 bipolar cells, as well as other type 5/xbc bipolar cells (and receive >50% of their input from type 3a OFF bipolar cells).

      How far apart are the postsynaptic targets from the ribbon release sites? The ribbons at type 5i bpc/On-T input appear separated from the dendrites of On-T rgcs (Figure 8C). At least further away than the type 6 bpc ribbons are from On-S rgc dendrites (Figure 8C). Distance may create a thresholding phenomenon, whereby only multivesicular bouts at the onset of depolarization are able to elevate synaptic Glu to levels needed to activate On-T GluRs. See Grabner et al Nat Comm 2023 for such scenarios in the outer retina.

      This is an intriguing possibility, but we should point out that the presynaptic ribbons in Fig. 9C (former Fig. 8C) are similar distances (within the resolution of our reconstructions) from the ON-T and ON-S dendrites. We have increased the brightness of the dendrite segments for both RGC types in the resubmission figure; note that ON-T RGCs have spine-like protrusions that may not have been as apparent in the previously submitted version of our manuscript.

      In Figures 1 and 2, Sustained responses look like the derivative of Transient responses, minus the negative going inflection. In addition, the sustained responses appear to have a lower threshold of activation than the transient On rgcs, because there are more bouts of action potentials (and membrane depol in V-clamp) with earlier onset in sustained than transients traces. It would be great if the GLuSniff data captured these differences. Take cumulative dF/F and see what the onset time is, or an initial tau if possible.

      This is a good suggestion. However, we are reluctant to make detailed quantitative comparisons such as this without further validation of how the kinetics of the iGluSnFR signals relate to kinetics of glutamate release.  A specific concern is that differences in the location and amount of iGluSnFR expression could impact any such comparisons.

      A recent study by Kim et al von Gersdorff (Cell Reports, 2023) presents interesting phases of release in response to light flashes, measured from AIIs, and complementary results from pairs of rbcs-AIIs. The findings highlight the complexity of SV pools under well-controlled experiments. Could their results be explained as variations in rbc ribbon size through development, and possibly between rbcs or within an rbc? 

      This certainly seems possible and would be consistent with the dependence of release on ribbon size that our results support.  It would be interesting to see if there are clear anatomical correlates of that change in release properties.  

      Figure 5 is a pivotal point in the study, but my review has identified numerous weaknesses. The feedback inhibition onto bipolar cell terminals is likely to sculpt glutamate release, and the results do not convincingly rule out this possibility. The suggestions for improvements range from the data needing to be reanalyzed with regard to statistical tests, and/or adding a few more data points (n = 5) before concluding a p: 0.06 is insignificant. 

      We have added an additional recording to this data set. With n= 6 cells, there is now a statistically significant difference between ON-T RGC excitatory currents measured in control conditions versus during GABA<sub>A/C</sub> receptor blockade. Please note that all the recordings shown in Figure 5C-F are from ON-T RGCs (the two panels show separately block of GABergic and glycinergic receptors). We did not make it sufficiently clear that the original trend (now statistically significant) is opposite of that expected if presynaptic GABAergic inhibition contributes to response transience in ON-T RGCs.  What we see is that excitatory synaptic inputs to ON-T RGCs become more transient (rather than mpre sustained) during GABA<sub>A/C</sub> receptor blockade. We have revised the text in that section to make this point more clearly.

      We have also included new data from iGluSnFR measurements showing that bath application of NBQX does not affect light step-evoked glutamate release kinetics at proximal (sustained) or distal (transient) RGC dendrites (control: steady-state amp. as % of peak amp. 13 ± 10; mean ± S.D.; n = 189 ROIs/4 FOVs for ON-T dendrites vs 40 ± 12; mean ± S.D.; n = 287 ROIs/8 FOVs for ON-S dendrites; NBQX: 6 ± 3; mean ± S.D.; n = 112 ROIs/1 FOV for ON-T dendrites vs 23 ± 9; mean ± S.D.; n = 97 ROIs/2 FOVs for ON-S dendrites; *p<0.001). By blocking glutamate receptors on amacrine cells, NBQX (AMPA/KAR antagonist) eliminates all/most amacrine cell-mediated signaling in the retina and should therefore abolish presynaptic inhibitory input to bipolar cell terminals across the IPL. Taken together, our results indicate that presynaptic inhibition does not play a critical role in establishing transient versus sustained kinetics for the stimulus conditions we employed in our study.

      There is a need to cite more recent literature on bipolar cell ribbons (e.g. see Wakeham et al., Front. Cell. Neurosci., 2023), in order to support experimental design and interpretation of the results. The authors should discuss their Ribeye-KO data from Okawa et al 2019 Nat Comm, Figure 7, in the context of their new iGluSnFR results. 

      Thank you for prompting us on this issue. We have expanded the discussion regarding ribbons and included more citations to the ribbon literature. That is largely in the three paragraphs starting on line 727.

      One point deserves emphasis because it is central to the authors' ribbon model but not consistent with their data. The ribbon model as they put it, and as commonly stated, holds that a transient phase of release at the onset of depolarization indicates the depletion of the primed SVs, and the subsequent slower rate of release (steady state release in the authors' terms) reflects recruiting, priming, and release of new SVs. The On-transient dendrite GluSnf responses agree with this multiphasic process, but the sustained responses show only an elevation in glutamate without a pronounced initial peak, creating a square-wave-shaped response (Figure 6B). This does not agree with the simple ribbon-based release model. I would expect the signals from the T- and S-on dendrites to have a comparable initial phase, while the sustained phase should be greater in amplitude for the S-on dendrites. More discussion may clarify possible mechanisms.

      Thanks for pointing this out. The example iGluSnFR traces we originally included in the manuscript were not entirely representative in that they did not show much initial transient phase. Note there is a distribution of steady-state amplitudes for proximal dendrites in Fig. 6C; the examples are from ROIs from the upper end of the distribution. In the new Figure 7, we have included some additional examples that show both a clear transient and sustained component. The summary data in Figure 6C shows the distribution of sustained/transient ratios across ROIs.  

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      (1) It would be interesting to understand the differences in IPSCs in the two RGC types. Perhaps they are small in both types, which would explain their apparent lack of impact on temporal tuning. The authors may already have these data.

      We did make measurements of noise-evoked IPSCs (as well as EPSCs) in a subset of ON-T and ON-S recordings. We have now included this data as Figure S3. There are slight differences in the kinetics of inhibition between RGC types (Fig. S3C) and there is a trend towards stronger inhibition (relative to excitation) in ON-T RGCs compared to ON-S RGCs (Fig. S3E), although there is not a statistically significant difference. In both cases excitatory synaptic currents are as large or larger than inhibitory currents, and this does not include the difference in driving force near spike threshold which will favor excitatory input by a factor of 2-3.  Hence our data suggests that postsynaptic inhibition does not play a major role in generating the differential temporal spiking responses of ON-T and ON-S RGCs. However, additional experiments examining the relative contribution of excitation and inhibition to spiking output in these RGCs would be needed to reach a firm conclusion.

      The pharmacological experiments in which we blocked inhibition (Fig. 5C-F, new Fig. 7) were designed to test the effect of presynaptic inhibition on bipolar cell output (voltage-clamp isolation of excitatory currents in Fig. 5; iGluSnFR measurements of glutamate release in Fig. 7). We do not mean to suggest that postsynaptic inhibition does not have any role in shaping the spiking behavior of these RGC types, but that transient vs. sustained kinetics are already present in the bipolar cell output and that presynaptic inhibition of bipolar cell terminals does not appear to account for this difference.  We have revised the text throughout to be clearer on this point.

      (2) It could be convincing to show transient/sustained differences between RGC types in dim light, where the response would depend on the rod bipolar/AII circuit. In this case, any difference in temporal properties would presumably be explained by differences that localize to the cone bipolar cell axon terminals. Indeed, is that the result in Figure 1B? This seems to be a dim stimulus presented on darkness, which may be driven through the rod bipolar pathway. The authors could then discuss the interpretation of this data in terms of the rod bipolar circuit. 

      Yes, Figure 1B is a dim light step (~30R*/rod/s) presented from darkness and the distinction between cells is clear down at still lower light levels that more effectively isolate signaling through the rod bipolar pathway. Thanks for making this point that observation of distinct temporal responses under scotopic conditions where signals suggests these differences must arise at and/or downstream of cone bipolar cell output. We have included additional text (lines 361-365) in the results describing bipolar cell responses that raise this point.

      (3) Glutamate release was already measured across the full IPL depth by Borghuis et al. (2013) and Franke et al. (2017). It would be appropriate to better motivate the current study based on these existing measurements.

      We have clarified that these important studies provided important motivation for measuring excitatory synaptic input to ON-T vs. ON-S RGCs (lines 165-169).   

      (4) Line 212/213. It would be appropriate to add to the list of papers showing the different stratification of transient vs. sustained responses: Borghuis et al. (2013) and Beaudoin et al. (2019).

      Thank you - these references have been added.  

      (5) Line 635-638. It would be useful to discuss papers by Pottackal et al. (2020, 2021), which suggested that a single presynaptic cell (starburst) can signal with different temporal properties depending on the postsynaptic target (other starburst vs. DSGCs). The mechanism was not completely resolved (i.e., it was not explained by differences in presynaptic Ca channels at the two synapse types), but it at least shows that neurotransmitter release can show different filtering depending on the postsynaptic target from the same presynaptic neuron. (This could also be at play for the type 6 bipolar cell inputs to ON-S vs. ON-T RGCs in the present study.)

      We have added a reference to Pottackal et al 2021 in this section.

      (6) Line 714. Should describe the procedure for embedding the tissue in agarose. 

      We have added more detail regarding agarose embedding for preparation of retinal slices in the methods.

      (7) Line 775. Need a better description of the virus (not the construct), what serotype? Provide the Addgene number if available. 

      This has been added to the methods.

      (8) Line 808. Was the SD for the gaussian really 50%? That would cut off a lot of the distribution, i.e., it would get clipped at 0. 

      Yes, the SD for Gaussian noise was 50%. This high contrast stimulus was used in part to achieve measurable signals from bipolar cells. You are correct that some of the distribution was clipped at 0 (it was also clipped at twice the mean to make sure that the distribution remained symmetrical). The clipping was accounted for during our LN analyses.

      (9) The paper should discuss Swygart et al. (2024) results showing different spatial surround properties of neighboring synapses from a type 6 bipolar cell. Based on this result, it would seem very likely that amacrine cells could play a role in shaping the temporal processing of bipolar cell glutamate release as well. Indeed, spatial and temporal processing will not be completely independent in a typical experiment. For example, with the spot stimulus used in the present study, bipolar cells within the center versus the edge of the spot will have different balances of center/surround activation, which could potentially influence their temporal processing.

      We have included discussion of results from Swygart et al 2024 in the section of the Discussion in which we point out differences in surround inhibition between ON-S and ON-T RGCs (lines 710-714). We agree that spatial and temporal processing are not completely independent. Our results with SR95531/TPMPA indicate ON-T RGCs receive stronger GABAergic surround inhibition than ON-S RGCs (Fig. S8). However, our results in Fig. 5C-D show GABAergic surround inhibition makes ON-T excitation more sustained rather than more transient. So even though bipolar cells presynaptic to ON-T RGCs receive stronger surround inhibition (Fig. S8), this inhibition does not establish the transient kinetics of glutamate release from these bipolar cells (in fact, it works to make release more sustained). Additional iGluSnFR experiments where we used NBQX to block all/most amacrine cell-mediated responses also suggest presynaptic inhibition does not have an important role in establishing differential glutamate release kinetics onto ON-S vs. ON-T RGC dendrites (Fig. 7).

      (10) Cui et al. 2016 described ON-S Alpha as having a divisive suppression mechanism that explained the temporal properties of white-noise response better than a standard LN model. Do the authors think the divisive suppression reflects a property of the excitatory synapses independent of inhibition?

      This is an interesting question, but one for which we don’t have a good answer for now. As mentioned in some of the above responses and as we have tried to clarify in the manuscript, we do not mean to imply that there is no role for presynaptic inhibition in modulating bipolar cell output, including for the divisive suppression described by Cui et al. Rather, our point is that the distinction between transient and sustained excitatory input to ON-T and ON-S RGCs does not require presynaptic inhibition and is more likely an intrinsic property of the bipolar cell synapses. 

      (11) Do the authors mean to imply that the pool size at bipolar cell ribbon synapses could depend on the use of Ames vs. ACSF? 

      For now, we do not have a good answer as to why there are quantitative differences in response kinetics between Ames and ACSF. We have not done any experiments to investigate whether ribbon sizes or ribbon pools are different in the different solutions.

      (12) More generally, different mean luminance levels could drive different levels of baseline glutamate release, which could alter the available pool of vesicles at bipolar cell ribbon synapses. Can we explain varying degrees of transient/sustained in the same cell at different levels of mean luminance based on this mechanism (e.g., Grimes et al., 2014)?

      Yes, the emergence of a transient component of excitatory input to ON-S RGCs at ~100 R*/rod/s versus at scotopic levels (0.5 R*/rod/s) in Grimes et al. (2014) could be due to differences in the number of releasable vesicles (due to different type 6 bipolar cell axon terminal membrane potentials and hence differences in spontaneous release rates) at the different light levels.

      We should note that although ON-T and ON-S RGCs exhibit some changes in transient/sustained kinetics across different light levels, the relative differences between these RGC types are preserved across light levels. We have included a statement about this in the text (lines 361-367).

      (13) Figure 1. Have the authors considered performing the LN analysis of the firing responses, to compare the degree of rectification between the two RGC types?

      This is a good suggestions. From an LN analysis of spiking responses, we do not observe a clear difference between the static nonlinearity component of the model for ON-T and ON-S RGCs. Both RGC types are strongly rectified under our experimental conditions.  

      (14) Figure 5. Do the authors have the pharmacology data for the ON-S cells? There are examples of sustained EPSCs in amacrine cells that become more transient after blocking inhibition, which at least suggests that inhibition can play some role in the transient/sustained nature of glutamate release (Park et al., 2015, Figure 3). Perhaps ON-S cells likewise become more transient with inhibition blocked. 

      (The colored symbols in A were not visible in a printout. It would be useful to indicate the cell type (ON-T) in C and E). 

      As described above in the response to reviewer 1’s recommendation for authors, we were not able to use SR95531/TPMPA for recordings from ON-S RGCs. Bath application of these drugs led to oscillatory bursts of excitatory input to ON-S RGCs. However, the lack of effect of bath-applied NBQX on the kinetics of glutamate release around either ON-T or ON-S RGC dendrites (new Fig. 7) suggests that presynaptic inhibition does not contribute to generating sustained excitation to ON-S RGCs (or transient excitation to ON-T RGCs).  

      We have corrected Fig. 5A to include the referenced colored symbols and have also edited Fig 5C and E to clarify that measurements in Fig. 5C-F are from ON-T RGCs.

      (15) Figure 6 legend. Should be Kcng4-Cre, not KCNG-Cre. Also, it should make clear that this is cre-dependent expression of iGluSnFR. For C, were the statistics based on the number of FOVs? 

      Thanks for catching this, we have corrected Figure 6 legend. The methods section includes a description of how we achieved iGluSnFR expression on alpha RGC dendrites via a cre-dependent viral strategy in Kcng4-Cre mice.  We have also clarified that the statistics are based on ROIs in Figure 6C.

      (16) Figure 7, Flashes were apparently 400% contrast on a dim background. What was the background? Is there a rod component to the response in this case? 

      In Figure 7 (now Figure 8), the same background (~3300 R*/rod/s; 2000 P*/Scone/s) was used as in the Gaussian noise and step response experiments. At this light level, the response should be primarily be mediated by cones.

      (17) Figure S1. The colors here differ from those in previous figures (Here, ON-T, magenta; ON-S, cyan). Is something mislabeled? 

      Thanks for catching this. We mistakenly swapped the labels in the legend for Fig. S1. The figure colors were correct, but we have corrected the legend in the revised manuscript.

      (18) Figure S2. For the LN model for RGC synaptic currents, the ON-S are more rectified than some previous recordings (Cui et al., 2016). Is this perhaps explained by different light levels?

      We aren’t sure why ON-S excitatory currents are more strongly rectified in our recordings compared to Cui et al., 2016. Cui et al. used an ~20-fold higher background light intensity (~40,000 P*/cone/s vs. ~2000 P*/cone/s in our study), so different light levels may be a factor (although we should point out that rectification increases in these RGCs between scotopic to low photopic light levels (see Grimes et al., 2014 and Kuo et al., 2016).

      (19) The study is apparently comparing PV1 and PV2 described in Farrow et al. (2013; see Supplementary information for stratification analysis), which should be cited.

      Thanks, we have corrected this oversight in the revised manuscript. We now cite Farrow et al and mention the connection to PV1 and PV2 in the first paragraph of Results (lines 104-108).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper is well written and investigates the cross-species insemination of fish eggs with mouse sperm. I have a few major and minor comments.

      Strengths:

      The experiments are well executed and could provide valuable insights into the complex mechanisms of fertilization in both species. I found the information presented to be very interesting,

      Thank you.

      Weaknesses:

      The rationale of some of the experiments is not well defined.

      Thank you. In the revised manuscript, we have clarified and expanded the rationale behind each experiment to better highlight the specific questions being addressed and how each approach contributes to our overall investigation. These clarifications have been integrated throughout the Results and Discussion sections. We provide detailed rationale in our point-by-point responses to both reviewers, outlining how each experimental design was motivated by prior findings, hypotheses, or specific gaps in knowledge. We hope these revisions make the experimental logic and progression better defined and more compelling.

      Major Comments:

      (1) Figure 5

      I do not understand the rationale for performing experiments using CatSper-null sperm and CD9-null oocytes. It is well established that CatSper-null sperm are unable to penetrate the zona pellucida (ZP), so the relevance of this approach is unclear.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. This experiment was conducted as the basis to then evaluate the contributions of progressive and hyperactivated motility to the ability of mouse sperm to locate and traverse the zebrafish micropyle. In earlier experiments (Figures 1 and 3), we assessed whether sperm-micropyle interaction was robust by comparing it to binding to the mouse zona pellucida and testing whether both interactions persisted after washing, which is standard approach to distinguish specific binding from non-specific adherence (Avella et al., 2014; Baibakov et al., 2012). Thus, we extended this analysis to CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm; CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm were still capable of binding the zona pellucida comparably to heterozygous controls, though they were unable to cross the zona of Cd9<sup>Null</sup> eggs. These observations served as a validation step for the use of CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm for downstream micropyle interaction assays. Thus, we proceeded to test whether hyperactivated motility, absent in CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm, is required for locating and crossing the micropyle.

      It is indeed well established that CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm are unable to penetrate the zona pellucida, and previous studies have typically used the absence of fertilized eggs as a readout. However, failed fertilization may result from multiple factors, including impaired sperm motility, reduced capacity to bind the zona pellucida, or an inability to penetrate it. To our knowledge, no study has quantitatively assessed the number of CatSper-deficient sperm that successfully bind, cross the zona and reach the perivitelline space. To address this, we first used normal oocytes for sperm binding and Cd9<sup>Null</sup> oocytes (Le Naour et al., 2000), which allow direct quantification of sperm accumulation in the perivitelline space. We have 7included a detailed explanation in the Results to clarify this point, lines 352-365 and 376-369.

      (2) Micropyle penetration and sperm motility

      CatSper-null sperm are reportedly unable to cross the micropyle, but this could be due to their reduced motility rather than a lack of hyperactivation per se. Were these experiments conducted using capacitated or non-capacitated spermatozoa? What was the observed motility of CatSper-null sperm during these assays? Clarifying these conditions is essential to avoid drawing incorrect conclusions from the results.

      Thank you for raising these points. Under our IVF conditions, qualitative observations confirmed that CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm displayed progressive motility, maintained sufficient progressive motility during the first hour post-insemination and exhibited zona binding efficiency comparable to that of CatSper1<sup>Het</sup> controls (Figure 5A and B). This is consistent with previous reports showing that within the first 90 minutes of sperm incubation in media, approximately 20% of CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm preserve motility (Qi et al., 2007). Given previous studies indicating that 15–35% of sperm undergo hyperactivation within 90 minutes (Goodson et al., 2011), and considering that 100,000 progressively motile sperm were used for insemination, we estimate that approximately 3,000 hyperactivated CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm were present in the cross-species insemination dish (mouse sperm x zebrafish eggs). Based on these numbers, we would have expected at least some sperm to locate the micropyle if hyperactivation were not required for its detection and entry. Nevertheless, CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm were detected in proximity to the micropyle canal, its opening, or within the inter-chorion space (ICS). These observations support the conclusion that the inability ofCatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm to locate and enter the micropyle is attributable to their failure to hyperactivate. Also, all sperm used in these assays were exposed to identical capacitating conditions (HTF/HSA, 37 °C, 5% CO2). We now clarify this in the Methods, line 624, and we added more rationale under the Results, lines 361-365 and in the Discussion, lines 470-483.

      (3) Rheotaxis and micropyle navigation

      Previous studies have shown that CatSper-null sperm fail to undergo rheotaxis. Could this defect be related to their inability to locate and penetrate the micropyle? Exploring a potential shared mechanism could be informative.

      Thank you for raising this interesting point. Indeed, homozygous mutant mice lacking expression of a different component of the CatSper channel, CatSperz, show reduced rheotactic efficiency and severe subfertility (Chung et al., 2017). We cannot exclude that complete lack of CatSper as shown in CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> mice could lead to reduced rheotactic efficiency, hence we include this interpretation in the Discussion (lines 484-486).

      (4) Lines 61-74

      This paragraph omits important information regarding acrosomal exocytosis, which occurs prior to sperm-egg fusion. Including this detail would strengthen the discussion.

      Thank you. We have revised the text in the discussion to describe the process of acrosome exocytosis, and its relevance for fertilization (lines 504-518).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Garibova et al. investigated the conservation of sperm recognition and interaction with the egg envelope in two groups of distantly related animals: mammals (mouse) and fish (zebrafish). Previous work and key physiological differences between these two animal groups strongly suggest that mouse sperm would be incapable of interaction with the zebrafish egg envelope (chorion) and its constituent proteins, though homologous to the mammalian zona pellucida (ZP). Indeed, the authors showed that mouse sperm do not bind recombinant zebrafish ZP proteins nor the intact chorion. Surprisingly, however, mouse sperm are able to locate and bind to the zebrafish micropyle, a specialized canal within the chorion that serves as the egg's entry point for sperm. This study suggests that sperm attraction to the egg might be highly conserved from fish to mammals and depends on the presence of a still unknown glycosylated protein within the micropyle. The authors further demonstrate that mouse sperm are able to enter the micropyle and accumulate within the intrachorionic space, potentially through a CatSper-dependent mechanism.

      Strengths:

      The authors convincingly demonstrate that mouse sperm do not bind zebrafish ZP proteins or the chorion. Furthermore, they make the interesting observation that mouse sperm are able to locate and enter the zebrafish micropyle in an MP-dependent manner, which is quite unexpected given the large evolutionary distance between these species, the many physiological differences between mouse and zebrafish gametes, and the largely different modes of both fertilization and reproduction in these species. This may indicate that the sperm chemoattractant in the egg is conserved between mammals and fish; however, whether zebrafish sperm are attracted to mouse eggs was not tested.

      Thank you. We performed an additional experiment with fish sperm used to inseminate ovulated mouse eggs, and results are reported in lines 183-187 and in Supplementary Figure 2.

      Weaknesses:

      The key weakness of this study lies in the rationale behind the overall investigation. In mammals, the zona pellucida (ZP) has been implicated in binding sperm in a taxon-specific manner, such that human sperm are incapable of binding the mouse ZP. Indeed, work by the corresponding author showed that this specificity is mediated by the N-terminal region of the ZP protein ZP2 (Avella et al., 2014). The N-termini of human and mouse ZP2 share 48% identity, which is higher than the overall identity between mouse and zebrafish ZP2, with the latter ortholog entirely lacking the N-terminal domain that is essential for sperm binding to the ZP. Given this known specificity for mouse vs. human sperm-ZP binding, it does not follow that mouse sperm would bind ZP proteins from not only a species that is much more distantly related, but also one that is not even a mammal, the zebrafish. Furthermore, the fish chorion does not play a role in sperm binding at all, while the mammalian ZP can bind sperm at any location. On the contrary, the zebrafish chorion prevents polyspermy by limiting sperm entry to the single micropyle.

      We thank the reviewer for this detailed comment. In this study, our goal was precisely that one of validating the hypothesis that mouse sperm would not bind either recombinant fish ZP proteins or the chorion; in addition, we found it important to examine the observation that mouse sperm could detect the micropyle. We further elaborated this rationale in the Introduction (lines 93-100).

      In addition, though able to provide some information regarding the broad conservation of sperm-egg interaction mechanisms, the biological relevance of these findings is difficult to describe. Fish and mammals are not only two very distinct and distantly related animal groups but also employ opposite modes of fertilization and reproduction (external vs. internal, oviparous vs viviparous). Fish gametes interact in a very different environment compared to mammals and lack many typically mammalian features of fertilization (e.g., sperm capacitation, presence of an acrosome, interaction with the female reproductive tract), making it difficult to make any physiologically relevant claims from this study. While this study may indicate conserved mechanisms of sperm attraction to the egg, the identity of the molecular players involved is not investigated. With this knowledge, the reader is forced to question the motivation behind much of the study.

      We thank the reviewer for their perspective, and we appreciate the opportunity to further elaborate on our rationale. As outlined in our Results and Discussion sections, a growing body of evidence supports the presence of conserved molecular players and signaling pathways involved in gamete interaction across species with diverse reproductive strategies. While zebrafish and mice do differ in their fertilization environments and modes of reproduction, these differences may not necessarily exclude the possibility of conserved molecular mechanisms underlying gamete interaction. For example, the CatSper calcium channel, which plays a key role in regulating sperm motility and hyperactivation, is conserved across a broad range of taxa—from echinoderms such as sea urchins (external fertilizers)(Seifert et al., 2015) to mammals, including mice and humans (internal fertilizers)(Lishko and Mannowetz, 2018). Moreover, sperm from some fish species possess acrosomes that undergo exocytosis prior to fertilization while sperm cross the micropyle (Psenicka et al., 2010). Also, in ovoviviparous species with internal fertilization, such as the black rockfish, sperm do undergo molecular changes while in the female reproductive tract—including immunomodulatory adaptations, glycocalyx remodeling, and interactions with ovarian cells—enabling the sperm with a longer-term survival and a selective persistence that ensures only the fittest sperm can successfully fertilize eggs (Li et al., 2024). As per the mammalian capacitation, it is broadly defined as the process during which sperm undergo hyperactivation (Yanagimachi, 1970), and acquire the ability to undergo the acrosome exocytosis, making the sperm competent for gamete fusion and fertilization (Bhakta et al., 2019; Puga Molina et al., 2018; Yanagimachi, 1957; Yanagimachi et al., 2017). Of note, acrosome exocytosis or changes in sperm motility are not exclusive to internal fertilizers. For example, as we cite in our manuscript (and as just stated above), acrosome exocytosis has been described to occur as sturgeon sperm cross the micropyle (Psenicka et al., 2010). As per changes in flagellar motility, investigations in the Pacific herring (Clupea sp.) demonstrated that sperm remain nearly immotile upon release into seawater and only initiate motility when approaching the micropyle region of the egg (Yanagimachi, 1957; Yanagimachi et al., 2017). In other fish, including bitterling and zebrafish, further enhancement in sperm motility is observed as sperm approach the micropyle area (Suzuki, 1958; Yanagimachi et al., 2017). These studies suggest that functional equivalents of capacitation may exist across taxa.

      We interpret the observation that mouse sperm can locate and enter the micropyle as suggesting that underlying guidance mechanisms may be more broadly conserved across distant species than previously recognized. We have now elaborated on these points in the revised Discussion (lines 531-552), and we hope the motivation behind our study is now more clearly articulated.

      During fertilization in fish, the sperm enters the micropyle and subsequently, the egg, as it is simultaneously activated by exposure to water. During egg activation, the chorion lifts as it separates from the egg and fills with water. This mechanism prevents supernumerary sperm from entering the egg after the successfully fertilizing sperm has bound and fused. In this study, the authors show that mouse sperm enter the micropyle and accumulate in the intrachorionic space. Whether any sperm successfully entered the egg is not addressed, and the status of egg activation is not reported.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s detailed comments and the opportunity to elaborate on this important aspect for our cross-insemination assay. We interpret the reviewer’s reference to “sperm entering the egg” as pertaining to sperm adhesion to the oocyte plasma membrane followed by fusion with the egg cell, two separate steps regulated by different molecular players for sperm-egg plasma membrane adhesion (Bianchi et al., 2014; Fujihara et al., 2021; Herberg et al., 2018; Inoue et al., 2005) and for fusion. It is important to note that proteins mediating gamete fusion are still unidentified in fish and mammals (Bianchi and Wright, 2020; Deneke and Pauli, 2021).

      In our cross-species insemination experiments, zebrafish oocytes were maintained in Hank’s solution to limit spontaneous activation; however, as the reviewer correctly notes, activation likely occurred upon exposure to HTF. While this model does not recapitulate full fertilization events, it serves as a platform to explore whether mammalian sperm can detect (within the scope of our study) and respond (future studies) to putative evolutionarily conserved signals, such as those guiding fish sperm toward the micropyle.

      While investigating cross-species sperm–oocyte fusion was not within the scope of this study and would require a distinct set of experimental approaches, we believe this question is an important one. However, we do not expect our platform to be informative for evaluating sperm adhesion to the fish oolemma or for enabling cross-species gamete fusion. In our assays focused on sperm-micropyle interaction, Hoechst staining of nuclei of transgenically-tagged acrosome sperm revealed no evidence of sperm adhesion to or fusion with the fish egg membrane (Figure 4D). Also, molecular incompatibilities may further prevent this interaction: in zebrafish, the Ly6/uPAR family protein Bouncer is expressed exclusively in the egg and is necessary for sperm–egg membrane adhesion (Herberg et al., 2018). Recent studies in zebrafish and mice have shown that a conserved trimeric complex composed of Izumo1, Spaca6, and Tmem81 on the sperm surface is required for mediating adhesion to the oocyte membrane by interacting with the mammalian oocyte receptor Izumo1R (also known as JUNO) or the zebrafish oocyte receptor Bouncer (Deneke et al., 2024). One would hypothesize that for mouse sperm to adhere to the zebrafish egg membrane, the mouse Izumo1-Spaca6-Tmem81 complex would need to establish binding with Bouncer. To explore this possibility, we performed AlphaFold2-Multimer structural predictions and docking analyses to mimic an interaction between mouse Izumo1-Spaca6-Tmem81 and zebrafish Bouncer, using mouse Izumo1-Spaca6-Tmem81 and Juno or zebrafish Izumo1-Spaca6-Tmem81 and Bouncer as positive controls. We observed low binding affinity between zebrafish Bouncer and the mouse trimeric complex (Izumo1, Spaca6, and Tmem81), as indicated by low ipTM scores and high predicted aligned error (PAE) values. These findings suggest that the mouse complex is unlikely to form an interaction with Bouncer (now shown in Suppl. Figure 7). These predictions were consistent with our observations that no sperm were found adhering or fusing to the egg cell. We describe methods and results in the supplementary files (Supporting Info, lines 53-66) and in the result sections (lines 335-339).

      In Supplementary Videos 3-4, the egg shown has been activated for some time, as evident by the separation of yolk and cytoplasm, yet the chorion is only partially expanded (likely due to mouse IVF conditions). How multiple sperm were able to enter the micropyle but presumably not the egg is not addressed, yet this suggests that the zebrafish mechanism of blocking polyspermy (fertilization by multiple sperm) is not effective for mouse sperm or is rendered ineffective due to mouse IVF conditions. The authors do not discuss these observations in the context of either species' physiological process of fertilization, highlighting the lack of biological context in interpreting the results.

      Thank you for raising this important point. One model for mammalian gamete recognition at the zona supports the notion that mouse sperm can penetrate extracellular matrices as long as sperm can bind to them, and binding is dependent on the cleavage status of ZP2. Zonae surrounding unfertilized mouse eggs present uncleaved ZP2 and these zonae support sperm binding. After gamete fusion, the cortical granules release ovastacin which cleaves ZP2 at the N-terminus, and consequently, zonae presenting cleaved ZP2 no longer support sperm binding. This mechanism acts as block to zona binding and prevents further crossing (Bhakta et al., 2019). Indeed, fertilized mouse eggs or 2-cell embryos surrounded by a zona containing uncleaved ZP2 support de novo sperm binding, and supernumerary sperm cross the zona and accumulate in the perivitelline space, unable to fuse with the fertilized oocyte plasma membrane or blastomere cells (Baibakov et al., 2012, 2007; Burkart et al., 2012; Gahlay et al., 2010). Thus, because under our experimental conditions, mouse sperm could interact with the micropyle opening, we interpret these findings to suggest that once interaction occurs at the micropyle opening, mouse sperm are capable of crossing it, even under conditions where the micropyle may be detached from the oocyte due to oocyte activation. Therefore, our data indicates that mouse sperm may be able to bypass the mechanism of zebrafish oocytes blocking multiple sperm to pass through the micropyle, even after oocyte activation. This point has now been incorporated into the revised Discussion (lines 425-441).

      The authors further show that the zebrafish micropyle does not trigger the acrosome reaction in mouse sperm. Whether the acrosome reacts is not correlated with a sperm's ability to cross the micropyle opening, as both acrosome-intact and acrosome-reacted sperm were observed within the intrachorionic space. While the acrosome reaction is a key event during mammalian fertilization and is required for sperm to fertilize the egg, zebrafish sperm do not contain an acrosome. Thus, these results are particularly difficult to interpret biologically, bringing into question whether this observation has biological relevance or is a byproduct of egg activation/chorion lifting that indirectly draws sperm into the chorion.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point and we appreciate the opportunity to elaborate on the biological relevance of this experiment. Our motivation to assess acrosome status in mouse sperm following entry into the zebrafish micropyle stemmed from the following biological considerations.  In fish species such as the sturgeon, sperm present an acrosome and undergo acrosome exocytosis while passing through the micropyle, before gamete fusion (Alavi et al., 2012; Psenicka et al., 2010). By contrast, zebrafish sperm lack an acrosome, raising the hypothesis that the zebrafish micropyle may not be able to trigger acrosome exocytosis. However, this possibility has not been experimentally tested. We therefore considered it important to investigate whether passage through the zebrafish micropyle induces acrosome exocytosis in mouse sperm. We have revised the Discussion to better clarify the rationale behind the experiment as well as the interpretation of the findings (lines 504-518). As per the chorion lifting indirectly drawing sperm into the chorion, we have not observed this phenomenon.

      The final experiments regarding CatSper1's role in mediating mouse sperm entry into the micropyle/chorion are not convincing. As no molecular interactions are described or perturbed, the reader cannot be sure whether the sperm's failure to enter is due to signaling via CatSper1 or whether the overall failure to undergo hyperactivation limits sperm motility such that the mutant sperm can no longer find and enter the zebrafish micropyle. Indeed, in Figure 5E, no CatSper1 mutant sperm are visible near any part of the egg, suggesting that overall motility is impaired, and this is not a phenotype specific to interactions with the micropyle.

      We appreciate the comment and the opportunity to further elaborate on the rationale of this experiment. While our data demonstrates a lack ofCatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm accumulation within the micropyle and ICS, we appreciate that this may be interpreted as the result of general motility defects, rather than a specific failure in undergoing hyperactivation and micropyle recognition. CatSper1<sup>Null</sup>  sperm are known to lack hyperactivated motility and exhibit a progressive loss of forward motility over time. After 90 minutes, only ~20% of CatSper1<sup>Null</sup>l sperm remain motile, compared to over 70% in fertile sperm (Qi et al., 2007). Of note, under our IVF conditions, CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm retained sufficient progressive motility during the first hour post-insemination to bind the zona pellucida with comparable efficiency to CatSper1<sup>Het</sup> controls. Based on prior reports indicating that 15–35% of sperm exhibit hyperactivation by 90 minutes (Goodson et al., 2011), and considering that we inseminated with 100,000 progressively motile sperm, we estimate that approximately 3,000 hyperactivated CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm were present in the dish. Yet, none were observed near the micropyle canal, its opening, or within the ICS. This led us to conclude that failure to hyperactivate underlies the inability of CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm to reach and traverse the micropyle. Also, we appreciate that identifying the molecular components of the micropyle would allow direct testing of whether the CatSper channel is activated in response to micropyle-associated signals. Indeed, no targeted perturbation of molecular interaction regulating micropyle recognition was performed in this study, as the molecular identity of the zebrafish micropyle guidance cue remains unknown. Efforts to identify and characterize this factor are ongoing in our lab and lie outside the scope of the current work. Therefore, throughout the manuscript, we have clarified that it is the failure to undergo hyperactivation, rather than the absence of CatSper per se, that limits the ability of sperm to locate and traverse the micropyle. The rationale for the experiment, the interpretation of our findings, and relevant future directions have been further elaborated in the revised Abstract, Impact Statement and Discussion (lines 40-41; 46-47; 343-365; 376-379; 389-399; 470-486).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor Comments

      (1) Figure numbering

      There appear to be inconsistencies in the figure references. For example, what is referred to as Figure 3F in the text is actually Figure 4F. Please review and correct all figure labels for accuracy.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We have carefully reviewed the manuscript and corrected all figure references throughout the text. Also, for better flow and coherence, we have moved the paragraph describing the videos to the end of the Results section titled "Mouse sperm recognize the micropylar region of fish oocytes." Previously, the callout of panels in Figure 3 was out of order (3A, 3B, 3E, 3C, 3D), and this reorganization also helps maintain logical progression through the figure panels.

      (2) Figure 5 terminology:

      The term "normal" sperm should be replaced with "CatSper heterozygous (Het)" sperm to avoid confusion and improve precision.

      We thank the reviewer for this helpful suggestion. We have revised the terminology in Figure 5 and throughout the manuscript, replacing “normal” sperm with “CatSper1 heterozygous (Het)”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In addition to my comments in the public review, I would encourage the authors to consider the following suggestions:

      The authors show that mouse sperm can find and enter the fish micropyle, and that this depends on the presence of MP. To better assess sperm binding to the micropyle region, the number of sperm binding to the micropyle vs. non-micropyle chorion should be clearly quantified, as well as the percentage of sperm that enter the micropyle compared to the total used for insemination. The authors state several times throughout the text that a "subpopulation" of mouse sperm finds and enters the micropyle, but it would be more precise and informative to give a percentage.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have now reported also the number of sperm bound to the other regions of the chorion (away; lines 231-233), as well as the percentage of sperm that entered the micropyle relative to the total number used for insemination (lines 276-279).

      To ensure that all sperm are inside the chorion, the egg should be removed from the insemination dish, washed thoroughly, and then the chorion should be torn open to definitively show that the sperm were indeed inside.

      We thank the reviewer for these excellent suggestions. As per ensuring that the sperm are inside the ICS, (as shown now in Figures 4A, F, G , Supplementary Figure 6 and Supplementary Movies 3–5), the inseminated oocytes were thoroughly washed prior to imaging to ensure that only sperm located inside the chorion were visualized (as described in the Methods, lines 646-648). In addition, to confirm the spatial localization of sperm within the ICS, we are now including additional TEM images showing sperm in the ICS (Figure 4G, right panel). Also, we generated orthogonal views using ZEN Lite software (Zeiss, Germany) from a z-stack encompassing the full volume of the chorion, ICS, and oocyte (added in the supplementary materials, as Supplementary Figure 6). These views display three focal planes: the surface of the WGA-stained chorion, the middle of the ICS, and the oocyte plasma membrane. Sperm nuclei stained with Hoechst are clearly visible below the chorion surface and above the oocyte plasma membrane, confirming their localization within the ICS. Additionally, in a separate set of experiments, as recommended by this reviewer, we mechanically disrupted the chorion and consistently detected sperm within the ICS. This procedure, however, was technically challenging: upon disruption, the chorion often collapsed onto the oocyte, and during the extraction process, sperm were sometimes displaced. As a result, it was not always possible to determine with complete confidence whether the sperm had originally been located inside or outside the chorion. However, we hope that the additional TEM and confocal images (Figure 4G and Supplementary Figure 6) offer further support for the localization of sperm within the ICS.

      I would further suggest that they examine the micropyle opening after the entry of multiple sperm, as well as the dynamics of egg activation during insemination with mouse sperm.

      Thank you. We now include one additional TEM image capturing the full structure of a micropyle that was traversed by multiple mouse sperm (shown in Figure 4G, left panel).

      At what point does the micropyle detach from the egg surface? Live imaging of this process with a confocal microscope would be very informative.

      During live imaging, the interval between placing the oocyte in the imaging dish, replacement of Hank’s solution with HTF and the addition of sperm, followed by the initiation of video acquisition, is approximately 2 to 3 min. By this time, the ICS is already apparent (Supplementary Video 2), although the micropyle appears to remain adherent to the egg cell. Partial detachment of the micropyle from the egg cell begins around 6–7 minutes after imaging starts and continues progressively over time. We provide time-lapse imaging frames to show the micropyle detachment under mouse IVF conditions (Supplementary Figure 5).

      Along the same lines, sperm should be doubly labeled with an acrosome-independent marker, i.e., a live DNA stain or MitoTracker. Then the authors could track if any sperm are actually able to enter the egg itself, which would be highly unlikely but an important detail to confirm.

      Thank you for pointing this out. In our assays designed to study sperm–micropyle interactions, Hoechst staining of nuclei in transgenically labeled acrosome sperm showed no indication of sperm adhesion to, or fusion with, the zebrafish egg cell (Figure 4D).

      Line 242, 282: The text should refer to Figure 4, not 3. Please make sure all figure references correspond to the correct figure and panel.

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. We have carefully reviewed the manuscript and corrected the reference to Figure 4, along with all other figure and panel citations to ensure they accurately correspond to the correct content. Also, to improve the overall flow, we relocated the paragraph describing the videos to the end of the Results section titled "Mouse sperm recognize the micropylar region of fish oocytes". This change also helped correct the sequence of figure panel references, which were previously cited out of order (i.e., 3A, 3B, 3E, 3C, 3D).

      Line 244: The authors quantify sperm that are "away" from the micropyle, but this is not clearly defined. This should be given as a set radius or distance from the center (e.g., in microns). If the sperm are still motile, can this be accurately measured?

      We thank the reviewer for this valuable suggestion. We have now defined “away from the micropyle” as a distance greater than 160 µm from the center of the micropyle. This measurement was determined using confocal z-stack projections of fixed samples. These details have been added to the revised Methods section (lines 670-674).

      To strengthen the conclusion that the sperm chemoattractant is indeed conserved from fish to mammals, the authors could show that zebrafish sperm are also able to find/approach mouse eggs. Even more compelling would be to show the same is true for other species combinations. As it stands, the choice of comparing mouse and zebrafish does not seem scientifically motivated but rather due to their availability.

      We thank the reviewer for this important suggestion. To test whether zebrafish sperm are capable of binding to the mammalian zona pellucida, we conducted the suggested experiment: ovulated, cumulus-free mouse oocytes were placed in water and incubated with zebrafish sperm. We did not observe any zebrafish sperm bound to the mouse zona pellucida, consistent with the hypothesis that zebrafish sperm do not recognize or interact with mammalian zonae or ZP proteins. This has now been added in the Results (lines 183-187) and shown in Supplementary Figure 2. We interpret these findings as in cross-species insemination assays, reciprocity in sperm-egg interaction is not always observed. For example, while human sperm bind only to human zonae and not to mouse zonae, mouse sperm are able to bind both mouse and human zonae (Avella et al., 2014; Baibakov et al., 2012; Bedford, 1977). This asymmetry may reflect species-specific adaptations in sperm-egg recognition. We have now added this point to the revised Discussion to clarify the rationale and context of our approach (lines 416-423).

      As per the choice of experimental models, while we agree that testing additional species combinations would broaden the scope of the findings, the choice to compare mouse and zebrafish was not solely based on availability. Rather, it was motivated by the opportunity to examine sperm guidance across two evolutionary distant vertebrates. This contrast allows us to seek for potential conservation of structural or molecular cues involved in gamete interaction. Additionally, both zebrafish and mouse offer extensive gene editing, blotting and imaging reagents, which are particularly valuable should future studies aim to identify and functionally disrupt genes encoding micropyle-associated proteins and their putative orthologs in mammals.

      For the CatSper experiment, I would suggest that the authors repeat this experiment with another mouse sperm mutant that is known to have reduced/altered motility. With the current data, I do not believe the failure to find/enter the micropyle is necessarily CatSper-specific. Because we do not know what the sperm interacts with in the micropyle or what the MP interacts with on the sperm, the signaling pathway cannot be tested, making other controls necessary for these results to be meaningful.

      Thank you for highlighting this important point. A wide range of mouse models with sperm motility defects exhibit subfertility or infertility due to structural abnormalities in the axoneme or midpiece rigidity. (Miyata et al., 2024). These defects often result in impaired progressive motility, failure to reach the zona pellucida, or inability to bind or penetrate it. In contrast, we could test and validate that CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> sperm display preserved early progressive motility but fail to transition into hyperactivated motility, making them particularly well suited for specifically assessing the role of hyperactivation in sperm navigation toward and entry into the micropyle. Taken together, these points, along with those discussed in our response to the public review, led us to conclude that the CatSper1<sup>Null</sup> model provides the most biologically relevant context currently available to assess the role of hyperactivation in guiding sperm to the micropyle.

      The authors could greatly strengthen the discussion by addressing the key points I raised in the public review, particularly in terms of interpreting these results in the context of each species' physiological mode of fertilization.

      We thank the reviewer for this important recommendation. We have carefully revised the Discussion to address the key points raised in the public review, particularly by framing our findings within the context of the distinct physiological modes of fertilization in each species, as indicated n our answers to the public review. We hope these additions have strengthened the manuscript as suggested.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Sumary:

      This study evaluates whether species can shift geographically, temporally, or both ways in response to climate change. It also teases out the relative importance of geographic context, temperature variability, and functional traits in predicting the shifts. The study system is large occurrence datasets for dragonflies and damselflies split between two time periods and two continents. Results indicate that more species exhibited both shifts than one or the other or neither, and that geographic context and temp variability were more influential than traits. The results have implications for future analyses (e.g. incorporating habitat availability) and for choosing winner and loser species under climate change. The methodology would be useful for other taxa and study regions with strong community/citizen science and extensive occurrence data.

      We thank Reviewer 1 for their time and expertise in reviewing our study. The suggestions are very helpful and will improve the quality of our manuscript.

      Strengths:

      This is an organized and well-written paper that builds on a popular topic and moves it forward. It has the right idea and approach, and the results are useful answers to the predictions and for conservation planning (i.e. identifying climate winners and losers). There is technical proficiency and analytical rigor driven by an understanding of the data and its limitations.

      We thank Reviewer 1 for this assessment.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The habitat classifications (Table S3) are often wrong. "Both" is overused. In North America, for example, Anax junius, Cordulia shurtleffii, Epitheca cynosura, Erythemis simplicicollis, Libellula pulchella, Pachydiplax longipennis, Pantala flavescens, Perithemis tenera, Ischnura posita, the Lestes species, and several Enallagma species are not lotic breeding. These species rarely occur let alone successfully reproduce at lotic sites. Other species are arguably "both", like Rhionaeschna multicolor which is mostly lentic. Not saying this would have altered the conclusions, but it may have exacerbated the weak trait effects.

      We thank the reviewer for their expertise on this topic. We obtained these habitat classifications from field guides and trait databases, and reviewed our primary sources to clarify the trait classifications. We reclassified the species according to the expertise of this reviewer and perform our analysis again; please see details below.

      (2) The conservative spatial resolution (100 x 100 km) limits the analysis to wide- ranging and generalist species. There's no rationale given, so not sure if this was by design or necessity, but it limits the number of analyzable species and potentially changes the inference.

      It is really helpful to have the opportunity to contextualize study design decisions like this one, and we thank the reviewer for the query. Sampling intensity is always a meaningful issue in research conducted at this scale, and we addressed it head-on in this work.

      Very small quadrats covering massive geographical areas will be critically and increasingly afflicted by sampling weaknesses, as well as creating a potentially large problem with pseudoreplication. There is no simple solution to this problem. It would be possible to create interpolated predictions of species’ distributions using Species Distribution Models, Joint Species Distribution Models, or various kinds of Occupancy Models. None of these approaches then leads to analyses that rely on directly observed patterns. Instead, they are extrapolations, and those extrapolations typically fail when tested, although they have still been tested (for example, papers by Lee-Yaw demonstrate that it is rare for SDMs to predict things well; occupancy models often perform less well than SDMs and do not capture how things change over time - Briscoe et al. 2021, Global Change Biology). The result of employing such techniques would certainly be to make all conclusions speculative, rather than directly observable. 

      Rather than employing extrapolative models, we relied on transparent techniques that are used successfully in the core macroecology literature that address spatial variation in sampling explicitly and simply. Moreover, we constructed extensive null models that show that range and phenology changes, respectively, are contrary to expectations that arise from sampling difference. 100km quadrats make for a reasonable “middle-ground” in terms of the effects of sampling, and we added a reference to the methods section to clarify this (see details below).

      (3) The objective includes a prediction about generalists vs specialists (L99-103) yet there is no further mention of this dichotomy in the abstract, methods, results, or discussion.

      Thank you for pointing this out - it is an editing error that should have been resolved prior to submission. We replaced the terms specialist and generalist with specific predictions based on traits (see details below).

      (4) Key references were overlooked or dismissed, like in the new edition of Dragonflies & Damselflies model organisms book, especially chapters 24 and 27.

      We thank Reviewer 1 for making us aware of this excellent reference. We have reviewed the text and include it as a reference, in addition to other references recommended by Reviewer 1 and other reviewers (see details below).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This paper explores a highly interesting question regarding how species migration success relates to phenology shifts, and it finds a positive relationship. The findings are significant, and the strength of the evidence is solid. However, there are substantial issues with the writing, presentation, and analyses that need to be addressed. First, I disagree with the conclusion that species that don't migrate are "losers" - some species might not migrate simply because they have broad climatic niches and are less sensitive to climate change. Second, the results concerning species' southern range limits could provide valuable insights. These could be used to assess whether sampling bias has influenced the results. If species are truly migrating, we should observe northward shifts in their southern range limits. However, if this is an artifact of increased sampling over time, we would expect broader distributions both north and south. Finally, Figure 1 is missed panel B, which needs to be addressed.

      We thank Reviewer 2 for their time and expertise in reviewing our study.

      It is possible that some species with broad niches may not need to migrate, although in general failing to move with climate change is considered an indicator of “climate debt”, signaling that a species may be of concern for conservation (ex. Duchenne et al. 2021, Ecology Letters). We revised the discussion to acknowledge potential differences in outcomes (please see details below).

      We used null models to test whether our results regarding range shifts were robust, and if they varied due to increased sampling over time. We found that observed northern range limit shifts are not consistent with expectations derived from changes in sampling intensity (Figure S1, S2). 

      We thank Reviewer 2 for pointing out this error in Figure 1. This conceptual figure was a challenge to construct, as it must illustrate how phenology and range shifts can occur simultaneously or uniquely to enable a hypothetic odonate to track its thermal niche over time. In a previous version of the figure, we had a second panel and we failed to remove the reference to that panel when we simplified the figure. We have updated the figure and figure caption (please see details below).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In their article "Range geographies, not functional traits, explain convergent range and phenology shifts under climate change," the authors rigorously investigate the temporal shifts in odonate species and their potential predictors. Specifically, they examine whether species shift their geographic ranges poleward or alter their phenology to avoid extreme conditions. Leveraging opportunistic observations of European and North American odonates, they find that species showing significant range shifts also exhibited earlier phenological shifts. Considering a broad range of potential predictors, their results reveal that geographical factors, but not functional traits, are associated with these shifts.

      We thank Reviewer 3 for their expertise and the time they spent reviewing our study. Their suggestions are very helpful and will improve the quality of our manuscript.

      Strengths:

      The article addresses an important topic in ecology and conservation that is particularly timely in the face of reports of substantial insect declines in North America and Europe over the past decades. Through data integration the authors leverage the rich natural history record for odonates, broadening the taxonomic scope of analyses of temporal trends in phenology and distribution to this taxon. The combination of phenological and range shifts in one framework presents an elegant way to reconcile previous findings improving our understanding of the drivers of biodiversity loss.

      We thank Reviewer 3 for this assessment.

      Weaknesses:

      The introduction and discussion of the article would benefit from a stronger contextualization of recent studies on biological responses to climate change and the underpinning mechanism.

      The presentation of the results (particularly in figures) should be improved to address the integrative character of the work and help readers extract the main results. While the writing of the article is generally good, particularly the captions and results contain many inconsistencies and lack important detail. With the multitude of the relationships that were tested (the influence of traits) the article needs more coherence.

      We thank Reviewer 3 for these suggestions. We revised the introduction and discussion to better contextualize species’ responses to climate change and the mechanisms behind them (see details below). We carefully reviewed all figures and captions, and made changes to improve the clarity of the text and the presentation of results (see details below).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Comment:

      (1) Following weakness #1 in the public review, the authors should review the habitat classifications, consult with an odonatologist, and reclassify many species from Both to Lentic and redo the analysis.

      Thank you for pointing out this disagreement among expert habitat classifications that we cited and other literature. We reclassified species’ habitat preferences based on classifications by Hof et al., a source that was consistent with your suggestions, and identified additional species as Lentic that our other references had identified as Both. We performed our analysis with this new dataset and, as you suspected, our results did not change qualitatively: species habitat preferences did not predict their range shifts.

      Hof, Christian, Martin Brändle, and Roland Brandl. "Lentic odonates have larger and more northern ranges than lotic species." Journal of Biogeography 33.1 (2006): 63-70.

      Comment:

      (2) Following weakness #2, would it be worthwhile or interesting to analyze a smaller ranging group (e.g. cut the quad size in half, 50 x 50 km) to bring in more species and potentially change the inference? Or is the paper too tightly constructed to allow this, even as a secondary piece?

      Thank you for this comment, as it highlights an important consideration for macroecological analyses, and the importance of balancing multiple factors for determining quadrat size. Issues exist with identifying drivers of range boundaries among species with narrow ranges when they are analyzed separately from wide-ranging species, and examining larger quadrats can actually help clarify drivers (Szabo, Algar, and Kerr 2009). The smaller quadrats are, the higher the likelihood that the species is actually there but was never observed, or that the quadrat only covers unsuitable habitat and the species is absent from the entire (or almost entire) quadrat. Too many absences creates issues with violating model assumptions, and creates noise that makes it difficult to identify drivers of species’ range and phenology shifts.

      Moreover, we constructed extensive null models that show that range and phenology changes, respectively, are contrary to expectations that arise from sampling difference. 100km quadrats make for a reasonable “middle-ground”, and we have included a brief explanation of this in the text: “We assigned species presences to 100×100 km quadrats, a scale that is large enough to maintain adequate sampling intensity but still relevant to conservation and policy (Soroye et al., 2020), to identify the best sampled species.”  (Lines 170-172).

      Szabo, Nora D., Adam C. Algar, and Jeremy T. Kerr. "Reconciling topographic and climatic effects on widespread and range‐restricted species richness." Global Ecology and Biogeography 18.6 (2009): 735-744.

      Comment:

      (3) Following weakness #3, are specialists the ones that "failed to shift" (L18)? If so please specify. The prediction about generalists vs specialists needs to be removed or incorporated in other parts of the paper.

      Thank you for pointing this out, we intended to suggest that species with more generalist habitat requirements might be better able to shift, but ultimately found that traits did not predict species’ shifts. We corrected our prediction regarding habitat generalists as follows: “We predicted that species able to use both lentic and lotic habitats would shift their phenologies and geographies more than those able to use just one habitat type, as generalists outperform specialists as climate and land uses change (Ball-Damerow et al., 2015, 2014; Hassall and Thompson, 2008; Powney et al., 2015; Rapacciuolo et al., 2017).” (Lines 128-132).

      Comment:

      (4) Following weakness #4, cite Pinkert et al at lines 70-73 and Rocha-Ortega et al at lines 73-77 along with https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2645. Add Sandall et al https:// doi.org/10.1111/jbi.14457 to L69 references.

      Thank you for the excellent reference suggestions, we have added them as suggested (Lines 80, 86, 77).

      Comment:

      Other comments/suggestions:

      (1) Title: consider adding temp variability 'Range geography and temperature variability, not functional traits,...'.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have added temperature variability to the title: “Range geography and temperature variability explain cross-continental convergence in range and phenology shifts in a model insect taxon”.

      Comment:

      (2) L125: is (northern) Mexico included in North America?

      Yes, we did include observations from Northern Mexico, and have specified this in the text: “We retained ~1,100,000 records from Canada, the United States, and Northern Mexico, comprising 76 species (Figure 2).” (Lines 174-176).

      Comment:

      (3) L128: I'd label this section 'Temperature variability' rather than 'Climate data'.

      Thank you, we agree that this is a more appropriate title for this section, and have replaced ‘Climate data’ with ‘Temperature variability’ (Line 185).

      Comment:

      (4) Table 2: why are there no estimates for the traits?

      We apologise, this information should have been included in the main body of the manuscript, but was only explained in the Table 2 caption. We have added the following explanation: “Non-significant variables, specifically all functional traits, were excluded from the final models.”. (Line 312-323).

      Comment:

      (5) Figure 2: need to identify the A-D panels.

      We apologise for this error and have clarified the differences between panels in the figure caption:

      “Figure 2: Richness of 76 odonate species sampled in North America and Europe in the historic period (1980-2002; panes A and C) and the recent period (2008-2018; panes B and D). Species richness per 100 × 100 km quadrat is shown in panes A and B, while panes C and D show species richness per 200 × 200 km quadrat. Dark red indicates high species richness, while light pink indicates low species richness.” (Lines 1002-1006).

      Comment:

      (6) L163-173: I am not familiar with this analysis but it sounds interesting and promising, I am not sure if this can be clarified further. Why the -25 to 25, and -30 to 30, doesn't the -35 to 35 cover these? And what is meant by "include only phenology shifts that could be biologically meaningful", that larger shifts would not be meaningful or tied to climate change?

      We used different cutoffs for phenology shifts to inspect for outliers that were likely to be errors, potentially do to insufficient sampling to calculate phenology. We clarified in the text as follows:

      “We retained emergence estimates between March 1st and September 1st, as well as species and quadrats that showed a difference in emergence phenology of -25 to 25 days, -30 to 30 days, or -35 to 35 days between both time periods, to include only phenology shifts that could be biologically meaningful to environmental climate change (i.e. exclude errors).” (Lines 169-173).

      Comment:

      (7) L193-200: I agree but would make a distinction between ecological vs functional traits, as other studies view geographic traits as ecological manifestations of functional biology, e.g. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2019.07.001 and https://doi.org/10.1016/ j.biocon.2023.110098.

      Thank you for this suggestion, and for making us aware of the thinking around range geographies as ecological traits. We have specified throughout the manuscript that the ‘traits’ we are considering are ‘functional traits’, changed the methods subsection title to “Range geographies and functional traits” (Line 252), and added a brief discussion of ecological traits: “Geographic range and associated climatic characteristics are often considered ecological traits, as they are consequences of functional traits and their interactions with geographic features (Bried and Rocha-Ortega, 2023; Chichorro et al., 2019).” (Lines 256-259).

      Comment:

      (8) L203: What's the rationale for egg-laying habitat as "biologically relevant to spatial and temporal responses to climate change"? That one's not as obvious as the others and needs a sentence more. Also, I am wondering why other traits were not considered here, like color lightness and voltinism. And why not wing size instead of body size, or better yet the two combined (wing loading) as a proxy for dispersal ability?

      We agree that our rationale for using this trait should be better explained, and we have included the following explanation: “Egg laying habitat was assigned according to whether species use exophytic egg-laying habitat (i.e. eggs laid in water or on land, relatively larger in number), or endophytic egg-laying habitat (i.e. eggs laid inside plants, usually fewer in number); species using exophytic habitats are associated with greater northward range limit shifts (Angert et al., 2011).” (Lines 271-275).

      We considered traits that have been found to be important for range and phenology shifts among odonates, as well as being key traits for expectations for species responses to climate change. Flight duration and body size are correlated with dispersal ability (Powney et al. 2015). Body size is also correlated with competitive ability (Powney et al. 2015), potentially making it an important predictor of a species’ ability to establish and maintain populations in expanding range areas. Traits correlated with range shifts also include breeding habitat type (Powney et al. 2015; Bowler et al. 2021) and egg laying habitat (Angert et al. 2011). Ideally, we would have used dispersal data from mark/release/recapture studies, but it was not available for many of the species included in this study. After finding that none of the functional traits we included were related to range shifts, there was no reason to believe that a further investigation of traits would be meaningful.

      Angert AL, Crozier LG, Rissler LJ, Gilman SE, Tewksbury JJ, Chunco AJ. 2011. Do species’ traits predict recent shifts at expanding range edges? Ecology Letters 14:677–689. doi:10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01620.x

      Bowler DE, Eichenberg D, Conze K-J, Suhling F, Baumann K, Benken T, Bönsel A, Bittner T, Drews A, Günther A, Isaac NJB, Petzold F, Seyring M, Spengler T, Trockur B, Willigalla C, Bruelheide H, Jansen F, Bonn A. 2021. Winners and losers over 35 years of dragonfly and damselfly distributional change in Germany.Diversity and Distributions 27:1353–1366. doi:10.1111/ddi.13274

      Powney GD, Cham SSA, Smallshire D, Isaac NJB. 2015. Trait correlates of distribution trends in the Odonata ofBritain and Ireland. PeerJ 3:e1410. doi:10.7717/peerj.1410

      Comment:

      (9) L210: I count at least 5 migratory species in table S3, so although maybe not enough to analyze it's misleading to say "nearly all" were non-migratory, revise to "most" or "vast majority".

      Thank you for pointing this out, we have made the suggested correction (Line 277).

      Comment:

      (10) L252-254: save this for the Discussion and write a more generalized statement for results to avoid citations in the results.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have moved this to the discussion (Lines 517-527).

      Comment:

      (11) Figures S5 & S6: these are pretty important, I'd consider elevating them to the main document as one figure with two panels.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we agree these figures should be elevated to the main text, and have made them into a panel figure (Figure 4).

      Comment:

      (12) L305-307: great point and recommendation!

      Thank you very much for this positive feedback!

      Comment:

      (13) L335-336: another place to cite https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2645 which includes a thermal sensitivity index and would add an odonate citation behind the statement.

      Thank you for this excellent suggestion, we have added this citation (line 480). (Rocha-Ortega et al. 2020)

      Comment:

      (14) L352-353: again see also https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2019.2645.

      Thank you for highlighting this reference, we have added it to Line 505 as suggested.

      Comment:

      (15) L355: revise "populations that coexist" to "species that co-occur" (big difference between population and species levels and between coexistence and co-occurrence).

      Thank you very much for pointing this out, we have made the suggested change (Line 507).

      Comment:

      (16) L359-365: are the winners and losers depicted in Figures S5 & S6? If so reference the figure (which I suggest combining and promoting to the main text), if not create a table listing the analyzed species and their winner/loser status.

      We agree that this is an excellent place to bring up Figures S5 and S6 from the supplemental. We have moved them to the main document as one figure and referenced it at line 510.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Comment:

      (1) Line 53-55: The claim that "These relationships generalize poorly taxonomically and geographically" is valid, but the study only tests Odonata on two continents.

      Thank you for this comment – the word ‘generalize’ may imply that our study tries to find a general pattern across many groups. We have changed the language to: “However, these relationships are inconsistent across taxa and regions, and cross-continental tests have not been attempted (Angert et al., 2011; Buckley and Kingsolver, 2012; Estrada et al., 2016; MacLean and Beissinger, 2017).” (Lines 57-59).

      Comment:

      (2) Line 58-59: Is this statement only true for Odonata? It does not seem to hold for plants, for example.

      Thank you for this comment – this statement references a meta-analysis of multiple animal and plant taxa, but the evidence for the importance of range location comes from animal taxa. We have specified that we are referring to animal species to clarify (Line 60).

      Comment:

      (3) Line 87-91: This section is difficult to understand and needs clarification.

      We have clarified this section as follows: “While warm-adapted species with more equatorial distributions could expand their ranges poleward following warming (Devictor et al., 2008), they could also increase in abundance in this new range area relative to species that historically occupied those areas and are less heat-tolerant (Powney et al., 2015).” (Lines 95-121).

      Comment:

      (4) Line 99-100: Please define "generalist" and "specialist" more clearly here (e.g., based on climate niche?).

      Thank you for pointing this out, we intended to suggest that species with more generalist habitat requirements might be better able to shift, but ultimately found that traits did not predict species’ shifts. We corrected our prediction regarding habitat generalists as follows: “We predicted that species able to use both lentic and lotic habitats would shift their phenologies and geographies more than those able to use just one habitat type, as generalists outperform specialists as climate and land uses change (Ball-Damerow et al., 2015, 2014; Hassall and Thompson, 2008; Powney et al., 2015; Rapacciuolo et al., 2017).” (Lines 128-132).

      Comment:

      (5) Line 122: Replace the English letter "X" in "100x100 km" with the correct mathematical symbol.

      We have made the suggested replacement throughout the manuscript.

      Comment:

      (6) Line 148: To address sampling effects, you could check the paper: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/full/10.1111/gcb.15524. Additionally, maximum and minimum values are sensitive to extreme data points, so using 95% percentiles might be more robust.

      Thank you for sharing this paper, as it offers a valuable perspective on the study of species’ ranges. While our dataset is substantially composed of observations from adult sampling protocols, unlike the suggested paper which compares adults and juveniles, this is an interesting alternative approach.

      For our purposes it is meaningful to include outliers, as otherwise we may have missed individuals at the leading edge of range expansions. Our intent here was to detect range limits, as opposed to finding the central tendency of species distributions. This approach is widely accepted in the macroecology literature (i.e. Devictor et al., 2012, 2008; Kerr et al. 2015).

      We have included the following discussion of our approach in the methods section:

      “We followed widely accepted methods to determine species range boundaries (Devictor et al., 2012, 2008; Kerr et al., 2015), although other methods exist that are appropriate for different data types and research questions i.e. (Ni and Vellend, 2021). We assigned species presences to 100×100 km quadrats, a scale that is large enough to maintain adequate sampling intensity but still relevant to conservation and policy (Soroye et al., 2020), to identify the best sampled species.” (Lines 168-173).

      Kerr JT, Pindar A, Galpern P, Packer L, Potts SG, Roberts SM, Rasmont P, Schweiger O, Colla SR, Richardson LL,Wagner DL, Gall LF, Sikes DS, Pantoja A. 2015. Climate change impacts on bumblebees converge across continents. Science 349:177–180. doi:10.1126/science.aaa7031

      Soroye P, Newbold T, Kerr J. 2020. Climate change contributes to widespread declines among bumble bees across continents. Science 367:685–688. doi:10.1126/science.aax8591

      Devictor V, Julliard R, Couvet D, Jiguet F. 2008. Birds are tracking climate warming, but not fast enough.Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 275:2743–2748. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.0878

      Devictor V, van Swaay C, Brereton T, Brotons L, Chamberlain D, Heliölä J, Herrando S, Julliard R, Kuussaari M,Lindström Å, Reif J, Roy DB, Schweiger O, Settele J, Stefanescu C, Van Strien A, Van Turnhout C,

      Vermouzek Z, WallisDeVries M, Wynhoff I, Jiguet F. 2012. Differences in the climatic debts of birds and butterflies at a continental scale. Nature Clim Change 2:121–124. doi:10.1038/nclimate1347

      Comment:

      (7) Line 195: The species' climate niche should also be considered a product of evolution.

      Thank you for this suggestion. To address this comment and a comment from another reviewer, we changed the text to the following: “Geographic range and associated climatic characteristics are often considered ecological traits, as they are consequences of functional traits and their interactions with geographic features (Bried and Rocha-Ortega, 2023; Chichorro et al., 2019).” (Lines 256-259).

      Comment:

      (8) Line 244: This speculative statement belongs in the Discussion section.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have moved this statement to the discussion (Lines 451-453).

      Comment:

      (9) Line 252-254: The projection of Coenagrion mercuriale's range contraction is not part of your results and should be clarified or removed.

      Following this suggestion and a similar suggestion from another reviewer, we moved this text to the discussion (Line 517-527).

      Comment:

      (10) Line 314-316: If the species can tolerate warmer temperatures better, why would they migrate?

      We apologize for the confusion, and we have reworded the section as follows: “Emerging mean conditions in areas adjacent to the ranges of southern species may offer opportunities for range expansions of these relative climate specialists, which can then tolerate climate warming in areas of range expansion better than more cool-adapted historical occupants (Day et al., 2018).” (Lines 445-448).

      Comment:

      (11) Line 334-335: Species' tolerance to temperature likely depends on their traits, which were not tested in this study. This should be noted.

      We agree, and we have removed the wording “rather than traits” from this sentence (Line 479).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Comment:

      (1) Title: The title is too general not specifying that your results are on odonates only, but also stressing the implicit role of climate change to a degree the tests do not support.

      Following this comment and a suggestion from another reviewer we changed the title to the following: “Range geography and temperature variability explain cross-continental convergence in range and phenology shifts in a model insect taxon”. We wanted to emphasize our use of Odonates as a model species that we used to ask broad questions, while being more specific about the climatic variable that we examined (temperature variability).

      Comment:

      (2) L32: consider including Novella-Fernandez et al. 2023 (NatCommun) which addresses this topic in Odonates.

      Thank you for suggesting this very interesting paper, we have added it as a citation (Line 31-32).

      Comment:

      (3) L35: consider including Grewe et al. 2013 (GEB) and Engelhardt et al. 2022(GCB).

      Thank you for these excellent suggestions, we have added the citations (Line 35).

      Comment:

      (4) L47: rather write 'result from' instead of 'driven by'.

      We agree this is a better characterization and have corrected the wording (Line 48-49).

      Comment:

      (5) L49-52: There has been a recent study on this topic for birds (Neate-Clegg et al., 2024 NEE). However, specifying this to insects would make it not less relevant. This review for odonates might be helpful in this regard (Pinkert et al.. 2022, Chapter: "Odonata as focal taxa for biological responses to climate change" IN Dragonflies & Damselflies: Córdoba-Aguilar et al. (2022) Model Organisms for Ecological and Evolutionary Research.

      Thank you for again suggesting excellent references, we have added them to line 52-53, as well as adding the Pinkert citation to lines 61 and 82.

      Comment:

      (6) L53-66: Combine into one paragraph about drivers. With traits first and the environment second. The natural land cover perspective may be too complicated in this context. Consider focusing on generalities of the impact of changes within species' ranges.

      As suggested we have combined these into one paragraph about drivers (Line 59).

      Comment:

      (7) L67-69: The book from before would be a much stronger reference for this claim. Kalkmann et al (2018) do not address the emphasis of global change research in insects on bees and butterflies. Also, I would highlight that most of the current work is at a national scale, rather than cross-continental.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have added the suggested reference and included that “…recently assembled databases of odonate observations provide a rare opportunity to investigate species’ spatiotemporal responses at larger taxonomic and spatial scales, particularly as most work has been done at national scales.” (Lines 75-77).

      Comment:

      (8) L68: consider rephrasing this part to '..provide a rare opportunity to investigate spatiotemporal biotic responses at larger taxonomic and spatial scales'

      We appreciate this suggestion and really like the wording. We have changed the phrase to read as follows: “While global change research on insects often emphasizes butterfly and bee taxa, recently assembled databases of odonate observations provide a rare opportunity to investigate species’ spatiotemporal responses at larger taxonomic and spatial scales, particularly as most work has been done at national scales.” (Lines 74-77).

      Comment:

      (9) L69: This characteristic is not unique to odonates and would hamper drawing general conclusions. Honestly, I think the detailed and comprehensive data on them is the selling point.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have edited the sentence to emphasize their use as an indicator species: “Due to their use of aquatic and terrestrial habitat across life different stages, dragonflies and damselflies are also considered indicator species for both terrestrial and aquatic insect responses to changing climates (Hassall, 2015; Pinkert et al., 2022; Šigutová et al., 2025), giving the study of these species broad relevance for conservation.” (Lines 78-81)

      Comment:

      (10) L73: Indicator for what? The first part of the sentence would suggest lesser surrogacy for responses of other taxa. Reconsider this statement. They are well- established indicators for habitat intactness and freshwater biodiversity. Darwell et al. suggested their diversity can serve as a surrogate for the diversity of both terrestrial and aquatic taxa.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have edited the sentence to emphasize their use as an indicator species: “Due to their use of aquatic and terrestrial habitat across life different stages, dragonflies and damselflies are also considered indicator species for both terrestrial and aquatic insect responses to changing climates (Hassall, 2015; Pinkert et al., 2022; Šigutová et al., 2025), giving the study of these species broad relevance for conservation.” (Lines 78-81)

      Comment:

      (11) L76: Fritz et al., is a study on mammals, not odonates.

      Thank you for pointing out this error, the reference has been removed (Line 84-85).

      Comment:

      (12) L84: Lotic habitats are generally better connected than lentic ones. Lentic species are considered to have a greater propensity for dispersal DUE to the lower inherent spatiotemporal stability (implying lower connectivity) compared to lotic habitats.

      Thank you for your comment, we have rewritten this section as follows: “For example, differences in habitat connectivity and dispersal ability may constrain range shifts for lentic species (those species that breed in slow moving water like lakes or ponds) and lotic species (those living in fast moving-water) in different ways (Kalkman et al., 2018). More southerly lentic species may expand their range boundaries more than lotic species, as species accustomed to ephemeral lentic habitats better dispersers (Grewe et al., 2013), yet lotic species have also been found to expand their ranges more often than lentic species, potentially due to the loss of lentic habitat in some areas (Bowler et al., 2021).” (Lines 88-95).

      Comment:

      (13) L90: I would be cautious with this interpretation. If only part of the range is considered (here a country in the northern Hemisphere) southern species are moving more of their range into and northern species more of their range out of the study area in response to warming (implying northward shifts).

      We have clarified this section as follows: “While warm-adapted species with more equatorial distributions could expand their ranges poleward following warming (Devictor et al., 2008), they could also increase in abundance in this new range area relative to species that historically occupied those areas and are less heat-tolerant (Powney et al., 2015).” (Lines 95-121)

      Comment:

      (14) L117: Odonata Central contains many county centroids as occurrence records. These could be an issue for your use case. I may have overlooked the steps you took to address this, but I think this requires at least more detail and possibly further removal/checks using for instance CoordinateCleaner. The functions implemented in this package allow you to filter records based on political units to avoid exactly this source of error.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we weren’t aware of this issue with Odonata Central. We used the CoordinaterCleaner tool in R to filter all odonate records that we used in our analyses. Less than 1% of observations in our dataset were identified as having potential problems by the tool, so we would not expect this to affect our inferences. However, in future we will employ this tool when using similar datasets.

      Comment:

      (15) L119: Please add a brief explanation of why this was necessary. I am ok with something along the lines in the supplement.

      We moved this information from the supplemental to the main text as follows: “If a species was found on both continents, we only retained observations from the continent that was the most densely sampled. If we merged data for one species found on both continents, we could not perform a cross-continental comparison. However, if the same species on different continents was treated as different species, this would lead to uninterpretable outcomes (and the creation of pseudo-replication) in the context of phylogenetic analyses. In addition, species found on both continents did not have sufficient data to meet criteria for the phenology analysis.” (Lines 161-167).

      Comment:

      (16) L132: This is the letters 'X' or 'x' are not multiplier symbols! Please change to the math symbol (×), everywhere.

      Thank you for pointing out this error, we have made the correction throughout the manuscript.

      Comment:

      (17) L133: add 'main' before 'flight period'

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have made the change. (Line 190)

      Comment:

      (18) L135: I suggest using the coefficient of variation, as it is controlled for the mean. Otherwise, what you see is partly the signature of temperature and not of its variation. For me, it's very difficult to understand what this variation of the variation means and at least needs more explanation.

      Thank you very much for this suggestion, we agree that using the coefficient of variation is a better fit for the question that we’re asking. We re-ran out analyses with the coefficient of variation as the measure of climate variability: all the results reported in the manuscript are now updated for that analysis (Line 377, Table 2), and we have also updated the methods section (Line 191). The results are qualitatively the same to our previous analysis, but we agree that they are now easier to interpret.            

      Comment:

      (19) L155: Please adequately reference all R packages (state the name, and a reference for them including the authors' names, title, and version).

      Thank you for pointing out this omission, we have added reference information for the glm function in base R (Line 298) and ensured all other packages are properly referenced.

      Comment:

      (20) L207: Mention the literature sources here (again).

      We agree that they should be referenced here again, and we have done so (Lines 267-268).

      Comment:

      (21) L209: You could use the number of grid cells as a proxy for range size.

      Following this excellent suggestion, we re-analysed our data using range size, calculated as the number of quadrats occupied by a species in the historical time period, as a predictor. Range size was not significant in our models, but we believe this is the best way to analyze our data, and so have updated our methods (Lines 261-263) and results (375-378).

      Comment:

      (22) L218: It would be preferable to say 'species-level' instead of 'by-species'.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we agree that this is clearer and made the change (Line 298).

      Comment:

      (23) L219-220: this is unclear. Please rephrase.

      We have clarified as follows: “We used both species-level frequentist (GLM; glm function in R) and Bayesian (Markov Chain Monte Carlo generalized linear mixed model, MCMCglmm; Hadfield, 2010) models to improve the robustness of the results.” (Lines 298-300).

      Comment:

      (24) L224: At least for Europe there is a molecular phylogeny available, which you should preferably use (Pinkert et al. 2018, Ecography). Otherwise, I am ok with using what is available

      We apologize that the nature of the phylogeny that we used was not clear; the phylogeny that we used was built similarly to that in Pinkert et al. 2018, Ecography. It created a molecular phylogeny with a morphological/taxonomic tree as the backbone tree, so that species could only move within their named genera or families. We clarified this in the manuscript as follows:

      “We used the molecular phylogenetic tree published by the Odonate Phenotypic Database (Waller et al., 2019), which used a morphological and taxonomic phylogeny as the backbone tree, allowing species to move within their named genera or families according to molecular evidence (Waller and Svensson, 2017).” (Lines 302-305).

      Comment:

      (25) L233: You said so earlier (1st sentence of this paragraph).

      Thank you for pointing this out, we removed the repetitive sentence (Line 323).

      Comment:

      (26) L236-238: To me, it makes more sense to test this prior to fitting the phylogenetic models.

      MCMC-GLMM is considerably less familiar to most researchers than general linear models or there derivatives/descendants, such as PGLS. We report models both with and without phylogenetic relationships included for the sake of transparency, and we are happy to acknowledge that no interpretation here changes substantially relative to these decisions. However, failing to report models that included possible (if small) effects of phylogenetic relatedness might cause some readers to question what those models might have implied. For the moment, we are opting for the most transparent reporting approach here.

      Comment:

      (27) L241: Rather say directly XX of XX species in our data....

      (28) L245: Same here. Provide the actual numbers, please.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we made this change on Line 332 and Line 334.

      Comment:

      (29) L247-249: Then not necessary.

      This issue highlights a challenge in the global biology literature and around the issue of biodiversity monitoring for understanding global change impacts on species. Almost no studies have been able to report simultaneous range and phenology shifts, and the literature addresses these biotic responses to global change predominantly as distinct phenomena. Differences in numbers of species for which these observations exist, even among the extremely widely-observed odonates, seems to us to be a meaningful issue to report on. If the reviewer prefers that we abbreviate or remove this sentence, we are happy to do so.

      Comment:

      (30) L251:261: That is discussion as you interpret your results.

      Following your suggestion and the suggestion of another reviewer, we moved the following lines to the discussion section: “Species that did not shift their ranges northwards or advance their phenology included Coenagrion mercuriale, a European species that is listed as near threatened by the IUCN Red List (IUCN, 2021), and is projected to lose 68% of its range by 2035 (Jaeschke et al., 2013).” (Lines 517-527).

      Comment:

      (31) 252: Good to mention, but why is the discussion limited to C. mercurial?

      We feel that it is important to link the broad-scale results to the specific biological characteristics of individual species, and C. mercurial is an IUCN threatened species. We are happy to expand links to natural history of this group and have added the following: “This group also includes Coenagrion resolutum, a common North American damselfly (Swaegers et al., 2014), for which we could not find evidence of decline. This may be due in part to the greater area of intact habitat available in North American compared to Europe, enabling C. resolutum to maintain larger populations that are less vulnerable to stochastic climate events. Still, this and other species failing to shift in range or phenology should be assessed for population health, as this species could be carrying an unobserved extinction debt.” (Lines 527-533).

      Comment:

      (32) L264: Insert 'being' before 'consistently'.

      Thank you for the suggestion, we made this change (Line 373).

      Comment:

      (33) L271: .'. However,'.

      Thank you for pointing out this grammatical error, we have corrected it (Line 382).

      Comment:

      (34) L273: 'affected' instead of 'predicted'

      Thank you for the suggestion, we made this change (Line 383).

      Comment:

      (35) L279: 'despite pronounced recent warming' sounds not relevant in this context.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we removed this portion of the sentence (Line 408).

      Comment:

      (36) L281: Rather 'the model performance did not improve....'

      Thank you for the suggestion, we made this change (Line 409).

      Comment:

      (37) L288: Add 'but' before 'not'.

      Thank you for the suggestion, we made this change (Line 416).

      Comment:

      (38) L311-316: Reconsider the causality here. maybe rather rephrase to are associated instead. Greater dispersal ability and developmental plasticity might well lead to higher growth rates, rather than the other way around.

      We agree that plasticity/evolution at range edges is important to consider and have included it as an alternative explanation: “Adaptive evolution and plasticity may enable higher population growth rates in newly-colonized areas (Angert et al., 2020; Usui et al., 2023), but this possibility can only be directly tested with long term population trend data.” (Line 449-451).  

      Comment:

      (39) L313-316: Maybe delete the second 'should be able to'.

      This phrase has been changed in response to other reviewer comments and now reads as follows:

      “Emerging mean conditions in areas adjacent to the ranges of southern species may offer opportunities for range expansions of these relative climate specialists, which can then tolerate climate warming in areas of range expansion better than more cool-adapted historical occupants (Day et al., 2018).” (Lines 445-448).

      Comment:

      (40) L331: Limit this statement ending with 'in North American and European Odonata'.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we made this addition (Lines 475-476).

      Comment:

      (41) L346-347: There are too many of these more-research-is-needed statements in the discussion (at least three in the last paragraphs). Please consider finishing the paragraphs rather with a significance statement.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have changed the final sentence here to the following: “The extent to which species’ traits actually determine rates of range and phenological shifts, rather than occasionally correlated with them, is worth considering further, but functional traits do not systematically drive patterns in these shifts among Odonates in North America and Europe.” (Lines 480-483).

      We also made additional changes, removing a ‘more-research is needed’ statement from the following paragraph (Line 443), as well as from line 499.

      Comment:

      (42) L349: See also Franke et al. (2022, Ecology and Evolution).

      Thank you for highlighting this excellent reference! We have added it to Line 501.

      Comment:

      (43) L363: Maybe a bit late in the text, but it is important to note that there is the third dimension 'abundance trends' or rather a common factor related to range and phenology shifts. I feel this fits better with the discussion of population growth.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have addressed the importance of abundance trends in the following sentences: “Further mechanistic understanding of these processes requires abundance data.” (Lines 442-443); “It remains unclear if range and phenology shifts relate to trends in abundance, but our results suggest that there are clear ‘winners’ and ‘losers’ under climate change.” (Lines 509-510).

      Comment:

      (44) L375-377: This last sentence is very similar to L371-373. Please reduce the redundancy. Focus more on specifically stating the process instead of vaguely saying 'new insights into patterns' and 'suggesting processes'. Rather, deliver a strong concluding message here.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we feel that we now have a much stronger concluding message: “By considering both the seasonal and range dynamics of species, emergent and convergent climate change responses across continents become clear for this well-studied group of predatory insects.” (Lines 545-547).

      Comment:

      (45) Table 1: To me, the few estimates presented here do not justify a table. rather include them in the text. OR combine them with Table 2. Also, why not include the traits as predictors (from the range shift models) in these models as well?

      We have clarified in the text that the results displayed in Table 1 are from the analysis of the relationship between range and phenology shifts: “The effect of species’ range shifts on phenology range shifts was significant in our model investigating the relationship between these responses, indicating that species shifting their northern range limits to higher latitudes also showed stronger advances in their emergence phenology (Figure 3).” (Lines 341-344).

      As there were no significant effects in the model of phenology change drivers, we have not shown results of this model: “Emergence phenology shifts were not affected by species’ traits, range geography, nor climate variability; due to this, model results are not displayed here.” (Lines 383-384).

      Comment:

      (46) Table 2: L712-713: What does this mean? Are phenology shifts not used as a predictor of range shifts? (why then this comment?). Or do you want to say phenological shifts are not related to Southern range etc? Why do you present a phylosig here but not in Table 1? Why not include the traits as predictors (from the range shift models) in these models as well? Consider using the range size as a continuous predictor instead of 'Widespread'.

      We are glad the reviewer pointed this out to us. We did not emphasize this issue sufficiently. We DID evaluate traits as predictors both of geographical range and phenological shifts, and species-specific biological traits did not significantly affect models predicting either of those sets of responses. We state this on Lines 312-323, but we have also noted in the discussion (Lines 473-476) that the most commonly assessed traits, like body size, do not alter observed trends here. Instead, where species are found, rather than the characteristics of species, is the key determinant of their overall responses.

      Following this excellent suggestion, we re-analysed our data using range size, calculated as the number of quadrats occupied by a species in the historical time period, as a predictor. Range size was not significant in our models, but we believe this is the best way to analyze our data, and so have updated our methods (Lines 261-263) and results (375-378).

      Comment:

      (47) Figure 1: I don't see any grey points in the figure. Also, there is no A or B. If you are referring to the symbols then write cross and triangle instead and not use capital letters which usually refer to component plots of composite figures. Also, I highly recommend providing a similar figure based on your data (maybe each species as a dot for T1 and another symbol for T2). Given the small number of species, you could try to connect these points with arrows. For the set with only range shifts maybe play the T2-dots at the center of the 'Emergence' axis.

      Thank you for pointing out this error: a previous version of Figure 1 included grey points and multiple panels. We have removed this text from the figure caption to be consistent with the final version of the figure (Line 989).

      The graphical depictions of the conceptual and empirical discoveries in this paper were challenging to create. The reviewer might be suggesting effectively decomposing Figure 3 (change in range on the y axis vs change in phenology among all species into two sets of points on the same graph, where each pair of points is a before and after value for each species. This would make for a very busy figure indeed. We have modified the conceptual Figure 1 to illustrate more clearly, we believe, that species can (in principle) remain within tolerable niche spaces by shifting their activity periods in time (phenology) or in space (geographical range) or both.

      Comment:

      (48) Figure 2: Please add a legend. Also black is a poor background color. The maps appear to be stretched. Please check aspect ratios. Now here are capital letters without an explanation in the caption. From the context I assume the upper panel maps are for the data used to calculate range shifts at the bottom panel maps are for data used to calculate the phenological shifts.

      We apologise for the error in the figure caption and have clarified the differences between panels in the text, as well as changing the map background colour and fixing the aspect ratio:

      “Figure 2: Richness of 76 odonate species sampled in North America and Europe in the historic period (1980-2002; panes A and C) and the recent period (2008-2018; panes B and D). Species richness per 100 × 100 km quadrat is shown in panes A and B, while panes C and D show species richness per 200 × 200 km quadrat. Dark red indicates high species richness, while light pink indicates low species richness.” (Lines 1002-1006).

      Comment:

      (49) Figure 3: Why this citation? Of terrestrial taxa? Please explain. Consider adding some stats here, such as the r-squared value for each of the relationships.

      We have better explained the citation in the figure caption, as well as adding r-squared values:

      “Figure 3: Relationship between range shifts and emergence phenology shifts among North American and European odonate species (N = 66; model R2 = 17.08 for glm, 14.9% for MCMCglmm). For reference, the shaded area shows mean latitudinal range shifts of terrestrial taxa as reported by Lenoir et al. (2020; calculated as the yearly mean dispersal rate of 1.11 +/- 0.96 km per year over 38 years).” (Lines 679-682)

      Comment:

      (50) L801: What are these underscored references?

      This was an issue with the reference software and has been resolved.

      Comment:

      (51) Table S1: L848: Consider starting with 'Samples of 76 North American and European odonate species from between ...'. Please use a horizontal line to separate the content from the table header. Add a horizontal line below the last row. Same for all tables.

      Thank you for this suggestion, we have edited the caption for Figure S1 as suggested (Line 1124). We have also made the suggested line additions to Table S1, S2, and S3.

      Comment:

      (52) Table S3: This is confusing. In Table 1 (main text) both 'southern range' and 'widespread' are used as predictors. Please explain.

      We originally included information on species range geography, including southern versus northern range, and widespread versus not, into one categorical variable. Following additional comments we re-analysed our data using range size, calculated as the number of quadrats occupied by a species in the historical time period, as a predictor. Now the methods section text (Lines 261-263) and Table 1 report results of that variable with distribution options northern, southern, or both. 

      Comment:

      (53) Figure S5 and S6: It would be more coherent if the colors refer to the continents and the suborders are indicated by shading. I would love to see a combination of the two figures with species ordered by the phylogenetic relationship and a dot matrix indicating the traits in the main text! This could really be a good starting point for a synthesis figure.

      The reviewer presents an interesting challenge for us. We have a choice, as we understand things, to present a figure showing phylogeny and traits (as requested here), or an ordered list of species relative to effect sizes in the two main responses to global change. The latter choice centers on the discoveries of the paper, while the former would be valuable for dragonfly biology but would depict information that proved to be biologically uninformative relative to our discovery. That is to say, there is no phylogenetic trend and biological traits among species did not affect results. We have gone some way toward illustrating that issue by retaining phylogeny in the MCMC-GLMM models, but we feel that a figure illustrating phylogeny and traits would (for most readers, at least) illustrate noise, rather than signal. For this reason, we have opted to take on the previous reviewer’s suggestion for a modified, main-text Figure 4, which we include below.

      Figure 4: Distribution of Northern range limit shifts (Panel A, kilometers) and emergence phenology shift (Panel B, Julian day) of 76 European and North American odonate species between a recent time period (2008 - 2018) and a historical time period (1980 - 2002). Anisoptera (dragonflies) are shown in pink, Zygoptera (damselflies) are shown in blue.

      Change last: Figure 3: Relationship between range shifts and emergence phenology shifts among North American and European odonate species (N = 66; model R2 = 17.08 for glm, 14.9% for MCMCglmm). For reference, the shaded area shows mean latitudinal range shifts of terrestrial taxa as reported by Lenoir et al. (2020; calculated as the yearly mean dispersal rate of 1.11 +/- 0.96 km per year over 38 years).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      (1) The bad equilibria of the model still remain a concern, as well as other features like the transient overshoots that do not match with the data. I think they could achieve more accuracy here by assigning more weight to such specific features, through adding these as separate objectives for the generator explicitly. The traces contain a five-second current steps, and one second before and one second after the training step. This means that in the RMSE, the current step amplitude will dominate as a feature, as this is simply the state for which the data trace contains most time-points. Note that this is further exacerbated by using the IV curve as an auxiliary objective. I believe a better exploration of specific response features, incorporated as independently weighted loss terms for the generator, could improve the fit. E.g. an auxiliary term could be the equilibrium before and after the current step, another term could penalise response traces that do not converge back to their initial equilibrium, etc.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We supplemented the membrane potential regression loss with errors computed for 3 intervals: pre- post- and mid- stimulation time intervals, improving the accuracy of EP-GAN for baseline membrane potential responses (Figure 2, 3, Table S2, S3). We also changed the simulation protocols for generated parameters by allowing a longer simulation time of 15 seconds, where the stimulation is applied during [5, 10] seconds and no stimulation at t = [0, 5) (pre-stimulation) and t = (10, 15] (post-stimulation). These time intervals are chosen to ensure sufficient stabilization periods before and after stimulation.  

      (2) The explanation of what the authors mean with 'inverse gradient operation' is clear now. However, this term is mathematically imprecise, as the inverse gradient does not exist because the gradient operator is not injective. The method is simply forward integration under the assumption that the derivate of the voltage is known at the grid time-points, and should be described as such.

      We thank the reviewer for the clarification on inverse gradient operation terminology. In the Methods section, we changed the term describing the inverse gradient operation to ‘forward integration’ which is a more accurate description describing the process.

      (3) I appreciate that the authors' method provides parameters of models at a minimal computational cost compared to running an evolutionary optimization for every new recording. I also believe that with some tweaking of the objective, the method could improve in accuracy. However, I share reviewer 2's concerns that the evolutionary baseline methods are not sufficiently explored, as these methods have been used to successfully fit considerably more complex response patterns. One way out of the dilemma is to show that the EP-GAN estimated parameters provide an initial guess that considerably narrows the search space for the evolutionary algorithm. In this context, the authors should also discuss the recent gradient based methods such as Deistler et al. (https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.08.21.608979) or Jones et al (https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2407.04025).

      We supplemented the optimization setup for existing methods (GDE3, NSDE, DEMO, and NSGA2) by incorporating steady-state response constraints as the initial selection process. The process is similar to that of EP-GAN training data generation and DEMO parameter selection process [16] (see Results section, page 6 for detail). We also expanded the testing scenarios by evaluating all methods with respect to both small and large HH-model estimation. The small HH-model scenario estimates 47 parameters consisting of channel conductance, reversal potentials and initial conditions with the channel parameters (n = 129) frozen to default values in [41]. Large HH-model includes estimating channel parameters (i.e. 129) in addition to the 47 parameters by considering +-50% variations from their default values. For both small and large HH-model scenarios, we test total sample sizes of both 32k and 64k for all methods to evaluate their scalability with the number of simulated samples given during optimization. The results show that existing methods show good performances for small HH-model scenarios that scale with sample size consistent with literature. EP-GAN on the other hand shows overall better performance in predicting membrane potential responses on both small and large HH-model scenarios.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Major 1: Models do not faithfully capture empirical responses. While the models generated with EPGAN reproduce the average voltage during current injections reasonably well, the dynamics of the response are generally not well captured. For example, for the neuron labeled RIM (Figure 2), the most depolarized voltage traces show an initial 'overshoot' of depolarization, i.e. they depolarize strongly within the first few hundred milliseconds but then fall back to a less depolarized membrane potential. In contrast, the empirical recording shows no such overshoot. Similarly, for the neuron labeled AFD, all empirically recorded traces slowly ramp up over time. In contrast, the simulated traces are mostly flat. Furthermore, all empirical traces return to the pre-stimulus membrane potential, but many of the simulated voltage traces remain significantly depolarized, far outside of the ranges of empirically observed membrane potentials. The authors trained an additional GAN (EPGAN Extended) to improve the fit to the resting membrane potential. Interestingly, for one neuron (AWB), this improved the response during stimulation, which now reproduced the slowly raising membrane potentials observed empirically, however, the neuron still does not reliably return to its resting membrane potential. For the other two neurons, the authors report a decrease in accuracy in comparison to EP-GAN. While such deviations may appear small in the Root mean Square Error (RMSE), they likely indicate a large mismatch between the model and the electrophysiological properties of the biological neuron. The authors added a second metric during the revision - percentages of predicted membrane potential trajectories within empirical range. I appreciate this additional analysis. As the empirical ranges across neurons are far larger than the magnitude of dynamical properties of the response ('slow ramps', etc.), this metric doesn't seem to be well suited to quantify to which degree these dynamical properties are captured by the models.

      We made improvements to the training data generation and architecture of EP-GAN to improve its overall accuracy with predicted membrane potential responses. In particular, we divided training data generation into three neuron types found in C. elegans non-spiking neurons: 1) Transient outward rectifier, 2) Outward rectifier and 3) Bistable [8, 16]. Each randomly generated training sample is categorized into one of 3 types by evaluating its steady-state currents with respect to experimental dI/dV bound constraints (See generating training data section under Methods for more detail). The process is then followed by imposing minimum-maximum constraints on simulated membrane potential responses. The setup allows generations of training samples that are of closer distribution to experimentally recorded neurons. This is further described in Section Methods page 15 in the revised manuscript.

      We also improved the EP-GAN training process by incorporating random masking of input membrane potential responses. The masking forces EP-GAN to make predictions even with missing voltage traces, improving overall accuracy and allowing EP-GAN to use membrane potential inputs with arbitrary clamping protocol (see Methods page 13 for more detail). For the training loss functions, we further supplemented the membrane potential regression loss with errors computed for 2 intervals: pre- and post-stimulation time intervals to improve EP-GAN prediction capabilities for baseline membrane potentials.

      Taken together, these modifications improved EP-GAN’s overall ability to better capture empirical membrane potential responses and we show the results in Figure 2 – 5, Table S2, S3.

      Major 2: Comparison with other approaches is potentially misleading. Throughout the manuscript, the authors claim that their approach outperforms the other approaches tested. But compare the responses of the models in the present manuscript (neurons RIM, AFD, AIY) to the ones provided for the same neurons in Naudin et al. 2022 (https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. pone.0268380). Naudin et al. present models that seem to match empirical data far more accurately than any model presented in the current study. Naudin et al. achieved this using DEMO, an algorithm that in the present manuscript is consistently shown to be among the worst of all algorithms tested. I therefore strongly disagree with the authors claim that a "Comparison of EP-GAN with existing estimation methods shows EP-GAN advantage in the accuracy of estimated parameters". This may be true in the context of the benchmark performed in the study (i.e., a condition of very limited compute resources - 18 generations with a population size of 600, compare that to 2000 generations recommended in Naudin et al.), but while EP-GAN wins under these specific conditions (and yes, here the authors convincingly show that their EP-GAN produces by far the best results!), other approaches seem to win with respect to the quality of the models they can ultimately generate.

      We thank the reviewer for the feedback regarding the comparison with existing methods. We have revised the optimization setup for existing methods (GDE3, NSDE, DEMO, and NSGA2) by incorporating steady-state response constraints as the initial selection process. The process is similar to that of EP-GAN training data generation and DEMO parameter selection process [16] (see Results section, page 6 for detail). Incorporating this process has improved the accuracy of existing methods especially for small HH-model scenarios where DEMO stood out with the best performance alongside NSGA2 (Figure 5, Table 1, 2).

      We also expanded the testing scenarios by evaluating all methods with respect to both small and large HH-model estimation. The small HH-model scenario estimates 47 parameters consisting of channel conductance, reversal potentials and initial conditions with the channel parameters (n = 129) frozen to default values in [41]. Large HH-model includes estimating channel parameters (i.e. 129) in addition to the 47 parameters by considering +-50% variations from their default values. For both small and large HH-model scenarios, we test total sample sizes of both 32k and 64k for all methods to evaluate their scalability with the number of simulated samples given during optimization. The results show that existing methods show good performances for small HH-model scenarios that scale with sample size. EP-GAN on the other hand shows overall better performance in predicting membrane potential responses on both small and large HH-model scenarios. 

      In particular, with extended membrane potential error including pre-, mid- , post-activation periods, EP-GAN (trained with 32k samples, large HH-model, 9 neurons) mean membrane potential responses error of 2.82mV was lower than that of DEMO (12.2mV, 64k samples) trained on identical setup (Table 2) and DEMO (7.78mV, using 36,000k samples, 3 neurons) applied to simpler HHmodel in [16]. With respect to DEMO performance in [16], under identical simulation protocol (i.e., no stimulation during (0, 5s), (10, 15s) and stimulation during (5, 10s)), EP-GAN predicted RIM (large HH-model) showed membrane potential accuracy on par with that of DEMO (simpler HH-model) and EP-GAN predicted AFD showed better accuracy for post-activation membrane potential response where DEMO predicted membrane potentials overshoot above the baseline (not shown in the paper).

      Major 3: As long as the quality of the models generated by the EP-GAN cannot be significantly improved, I am doubtful that it indeed can contribute to the 'ElectroPhysiome', as it seems likely that dynamics that are currently poorly captured, like slow ramps, or the ability of the neuron to return to its resting membrane potential, will critically affect network computations. If the authors want to motivate their study based on this very ambitious goal, they should illustrate that single neuron model generation with their approach is robust enough to warrant well-constrained network dynamics. Based on the currently presented results, I find the framing of the manuscript far too bold.

      We thank the reviewer for the feedback regarding the paper's scope. With revised methods, the overall quality of EP-GAN models is improved with the most significant improvements in baseline membrane potential accuracy. While high quality neuron models could be attained with existing methods given sufficient sample size, our results suggest EP-GAN can predict models with enhanced quality with significantly fewer sample size without a need for retraining, thus complementing the main drawback of evolutionary based methods. While EP-GAN still has limitations (e.g., difficulty in predicting slow ramps) that need to be addressed in the future, we believe its overall performance combined with fast inference speed and flexibility in its input data format (e.g., missing membrane potential traces) is a step forward in the large-scale neuron modeling tasks that can contribute to network models.   

      Major 4: The conclusion of the ablation study 'In addition the architecture of EP-GAN permits inference of parameters even when partial membrane potential and steady-state currents profile are given as inputs' does not seem to be justified given the voltage traces shown in Figure 3. For example, for RIM, the resting membrane potential stays around 0 mV, but all empirical traces are around -40mV. For AFD, all simulated traces have a negative slope during the depolarizing stimuli, but a positive slope in all empirically observed traces. For AIY, the shape of hyperpolarized traces is off. While it may be that by their metric neurons in the 25% category are classified as 'preserving baseline accuracy', this doesn't seem justified given the voltage traces presented in the manuscript. It appears the metric is not strict enough.

      We improved EP-GAN’s training process by incorporating random masking of input membrane potential responses. The masking forces EP-GAN to make predictions even with missing voltage traces, improving overall accuracy and allowing EP-GAN to use membrane potential inputs with arbitrary clamping protocol.

      Such input masking during training has improved the results with ablation studies where EP-GAN now retains baseline membrane potential error (3.3mV, averaged across pre-, mid-, post-activation periods) up to 50% of membrane potential inputs remaining (3.5mV) and up to 25% of steady-state currents remaining (3.5mV).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript "Drosophila Visuomotor Integration: An Integrative Model and Behavioral Evidence of Visual Efference Copy" provides an integrative model of the visuomotor control in Drosophila melanogaster. This model presents an experimentally derived model based on visually evoked wingbeat pattern recordings of three strategically selected visual stimulus types with well-established behavioral response characteristics. By testing variations of these models, the authors demonstrate that the virtual model behavior can recapitulate the recorded wing beat behavioral results and those recorded by others for these specific stimuli when presented individually. Yet, the novelty of this study and their model is that it allows predictions for natural visual scenes in which multiple visual stimuli occur simultaneously and may have opposite or enhancing effects on behavior. Testing three models that would allow interactions of these visual modalities, the authors show that using a visual efference copy signal allows visual streams to interact, replicating behavior recorded when multiple stimuli are presented simultaneously. Importantly, they validated the prediction of this model in real flies using magnetically tethered flies, e.g., presenting moving bars with varying backgrounds. In conclusion, the presented manuscript presents a commendable effort in developing and demonstrating the validity of a mixture model that allows predictions of the behavior of Drosophila in natural visual environments.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the manuscript is well-structured and clear in its presentation, and the modeling and experimental research are methodically conducted and illustrated in visually appealing and easy-to-understand figures and their captions.

      The manuscript employs a thorough, logical approach, combining computational modeling with experimental behavioral validation using magnetically tethered flies. This iterative integration of simulation and empirical behavioral evidence enhances the credibility of the findings.

      The associated code base is well documented and readily produces all figures in the document.

      Suggestions:

      However, while the experiments provide evidence for the use of a visual efference copy, the manuscript would be even more impressive if it presented specific predictions for the neural implementation or even neurophysiological data to support this model. Or, at the very least, a thorough discussion. Nonetheless, these models and validating behavioral experiments make this a valuable contribution to the field; it is well executed and addresses a significant gap in the modeling of fly behavior and holistic understanding of visuomotor behaviors.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful comments on the strengths and weaknesses of our manuscript. We agree that biophysically realistic model reflecting the structure of neural circuits as well as physiological data from them would be invaluable. However, we are currently unable to provide physiological evidence for EC-based suppression, nor provide circuit architecture for efference copy-based suppression of the stability circuit because the neural pathway underlying this behavior remains unidentified. Extensive recordings from the HS/VS system have revealed cell-type-specific motor-related inputs during both spontaneous and loom-evoked flight turns (Fenk et al., 2021; Kim et al., 2017, 2015). These studies predicted suppression of the optomotor stability response during such turns, and our new experiments confirmed this suppression specifically during loom-evoked turns (Figures 5, 6). However, these neurons are primarily involved in the head optomotor response, not the body optomotor response. We hope to extend our current model in future studies to incorporate more cellular-level detail, as the feedforward circuits underlying stability behavior become more clearly defined.

      Here are a few points that should be addressed:

      (1) The biomechanics block (Figure 2) should be elaborated on, to explain its relevance to behavior and relation to the underlying neural mechanisms.

      We appreciate this suggestion. The mathematical representation of the biomechanics block has been developed by other groups in previous studies (Fry et al., 2003; Ristroph et al., 2010). We used exactly the same model, and its parameters were identical to those used in one of those studies (Fry et al., 2003; Ristroph et al., 2010), in which the parameters were estimated from the stabilizing response in response to magnetic “stumbling” pulses. In the previous version of the manuscript, we had a description of the biomechanics block in the Method section (see Equation 4). In response to the reviewer’s comment, we have made a few changes in Figure 2A and expanded the associated description in the main text, as follows.

      (Line 160) “To test the orientation behavior of the model, we developed an expanded model, termed “virtual fly model” hereafter. In this model, we added a biomechanics block that transforms the torque response of the fly to the actual heading change according to kinematic parameters estimated previously (Michael H Dickinson, 2005; Ristroph et al., 2010) (Figure 2A, see Equation 4 in Methods and Movie S1). The virtual fly model, featuring position and velocity blocks that are conditioned on the type of the visual pattern, can now change its body orientation, simulating the visual orientation behavior of flies in the free flight condition.”

      (2) It is unclear how the three integrative models with different strategies were chosen or what relevance they have to neural implementation. This should be explained and/or addressed.

      Thank you for this valuable comment. We selected the three models based on previous studies investigating visuomotor integration across multiple species, under conditions where multiple sensory cues are presented simultaneously.

      The addition-only model represents the simplest hypothesis, analogous to the “additive model” proposed by Tom Collett in his 1980 study (Collett, 1980). We used this model as a baseline to illustrate behavior in the absence of any efference copy mechanism. Notably, some modeling studies have proposed linear (additive) integration for multimodal sensory cues at the behavioral level (Liu et al., 2023; Van der Stoep et al., 2021). However, experimental evidence demonstrating strictly linear integration—either behaviorally or physiologically—remains limited. In our study, new data (Figure 5) show that bar-evoked and background movement-evoked locomotor responses are combined linearly, supporting the addition-only model.

      The graded efference copy model has been most clearly demonstrated in the cerebellum-like circuit of Mormyrid fish during electrosensation (Bell, 1981; Kennedy et al., 2014). In this system, the efference copy signal forms a negative image of the predicted reafferent input and undergoes plastic changes as the environment changes—an idea that inspired our modifiable efference copy model (Figure 4–figure supplement 1). The all-or-none efference copy model is exemplified in the sensory systems of smaller organisms, such as the auditory neurons of crickets during stridulation (Poulet and Hedwig, 2006). Notably, in crickets, the motor-related input is referred to as corollary discharge rather than efference copy. Typically, “efference copy” refers to a graded, subtractive motor-related signal, while “corollary discharge” denotes an all-or-none signal, both counteracting the sensory consequences of self-generated actions. In this manuscript, we use the term efference copy more broadly, encompassing both types of motor-related feedback signals (Sommer and Wurtz, 2008).

      In response to this comment, we have made the following changes in the main text to enhance its accessibility to general readers.

      (Line#268) “This integration problem has been studied across animal sensory systems, typically by analyzing motor-related signals observed in sensory neurons (Bell, 1981; Collett, 1980; Kim et al., 2017; Poulet and Hedwig, 2006). Building on the results of these studies, we developed three integrative models. The first model, termed the “addition-only model”, assumes that the outputs of the object (bar) and the background (grating) response circuits are summed to control the flight orientation (Figure 4B, see Equation 14 in Methods).”

      (Line#272) “In the second and third models, an EC is used to set priorities between different visuomotor circuits (Figure 4C,D). In particular, the EC is derived from the object-induced motor command and sent to the object response system to nullify visual input associated with the object-evoked turn (Bell, 1981; Collett, 1980; Poulet and Hedwig, 2006). These motor-related inputs fully suppress sensory processing in some systems (Poulet and Hedwig, 2006), whereas in others they selectively counteract only the undesirable components of the sensory feedback (Bell, 1981; Kennedy et al., 2014).”

      (3) There should be a discussion of how the visual efference could be represented in the biological model and an evaluation of the plausibility and alternatives.

      Thank you for this helpful comment. We have now added the following discussion to share our perspective on the circuit-level implementation of the visual efference copy in Drosophila.

      (Line#481) “Efference copy in Drosophila vision

      Under natural conditions, various visual features in the environment may concurrently activate multiple motor programs. Because these may interfere with one another, it is crucial for the central brain to coordinate between the motor signals originating from different sensory circuits. Among such coordination mechanisms, the EC mechanisms were hypothesized to counteract so-called reafferent visual input, those caused specifically by self-movement (Collett, 1980; von Holst and Mittelstaedt, 1950). Recent studies reported such EC-like signals in Drosophila visual neurons during spontaneous as well as loom-evoked flight turns (Fenk et al., 2021; Kim et al., 2017, 2015). One type of EC-like signals were identified in a group of wide-field visual motion-sensing neurons that were shown to control the neck movement for the gaze stability (Kim et al., 2017). The EC-like signals in these cells were bidirectional depending on the direction of flight turns, and their amplitudes were quantitatively tuned to those of the expected visual input across cell types. Although amplitude varies among cell types, it remains inconclusive whether it also varies within a given cell type to match the amplitude of expected visual feedback, thereby implementing the graded EC signal. A more recent study examined EC-like signal amplitude in the same visual neurons for loom-evoked turns, across events (Fenk et al., 2021). Although the result showed a strong correlation between wing response and the EC-like inputs, the authors pointed that this apparent correlation could stem from noisy measurement of all-or-none motor-related inputs.

      Thus, these studies did not completely disambiguate between graded vs. all-or-none EC signaling. Another type of EC-like signals observed in the visual circuit tuned to a moving spot exhibited characteristics consistent with all-or-none EC. That is, it entirely suppressed visual signaling, irrespective of the direction of the self-generated turn (Kim et al., 2015; Turner et al., 2022). 

      Efference-copy (EC)–like signals have been reported in several Drosophila visual circuits, yet their behavioral role remains unclear. Indirect evidence comes from a behavioral study showing that the dynamics of spontaneously generated flight turns were unaffected by unexpected background motion (Bender and Dickinson, 2006a). Likewise, our behavioral experiments showed that, during loom-evoked turns, responses to background motion are suppressed in an all-or-none manner (Figures 6 and 7). Consistent with this, motor-related inputs recorded in visual neurons exhibit nearly identical dynamics during spontaneous and loom-evoked turns (Fenk et al., 2021). Together, these behavioral and physiological parallels support the idea that a common efference-copy mechanism operates during both spontaneous and loom-evoked flight turns.

      Unlike loom-evoked turns, bar-evoked turn dynamics changed in the presence of moving backgrounds (Figure 5), a result compatible with both the addition-only and graded EC models. However, when the static background was updated just before a bar-evoked turn—thereby altering the amplitude of optic flow—the turn dynamics remained unaffected (Figures 5 and 7), clearly contradicting the addition-only model. Thus, the graded EC model is the only one consistent with both findings. If a graded EC mechanism were truly at work, however, an unexpected background change should have modified turn dynamics because of the mismatch between expected and actual visual feedback (Figure 4–figure supplement 1)—yet we detected no such effect at any time scale examined (Figure 7–figure supplement 1). This mismatch would be ignored only if the amplitude of the graded EC adapted to environmental changes almost instantaneously—a mechanism that seems improbable given the limited computational capacity of the Drosophila brain. In electric fish, for example, comparable adjustments take more than 10 minutes (Bell, 1981; Muller et al., 2019). Further investigation is needed to clarify how reorienting flies ignore optic flow generated by static backgrounds, potentially by engaging EC mechanisms not captured by the models tested in this study.

      Why would Drosophila rely on the all-or-none EC mechanism instead of the graded one for loom-evoked turns? A graded EC must be adjusted adaptively depending on the environment, as the amplitude of visual feedback varies with both the dynamics of self-generated movement and environmental conditions (e.g., empty vs. cluttered visual backgrounds) (Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Recent studies on electric fish have suggested that a large array of neurons in a multi-layer network is crucial for generating a modifiable efference copy signal matched to the current environment (Muller et al., 2019). Given their small-sized brain, flies might opt for a more economical design for suppressing unwanted visual inputs regardless of the visual environment. Circuits mediating such a type of EC were identified in the cricket auditory system during stridulation (Poulet and Hedwig, 2006), for example. Our study strongly suggests the existence of a similar circuit in the Drosophila visual system. 

      We tested the hypothesis that efference-copy (EC) signals guide action selection by suppressing specific visuomotor reflexes when multiple visual features compete. An alternative motif with a similar function is mutual inhibition between motor pathways (Edwards, 1991; Mysore and Kothari, 2020). In Drosophila, descending neurons form dense lateral connections (Braun et al., 2024), offering a substrate for such competitive interactions. Determining whether—and how—EC and mutual inhibition operate will require recordings from the neurons that ensure visual stability, which remain unidentified. Mapping these pathways and assessing how they are modulated by visual and behavioral context are important goals for future work.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      It has been widely proposed that the neural circuit uses a copy of motor command, an efference copy, to cancel out self-generated sensory stimuli so that intended movement is not disturbed by the reafferent sensory inputs. However, how quantitatively such an efference copy suppresses sensory inputs is unknown. Here, Canelo et al. tried to demonstrate that an efference copy operates in an all-or-none manner and that its amplitude is independent of the amplitude of the sensory signal to be suppressed. Understanding the nature of such an efference copy is important because animals generally move during sensory processing, and the movement would devastatingly distort that without a proper correction. The manuscript is concise and written very clearly. However, experiments do not directly demonstrate if the animal indeed uses an efference copy in the presented visual paradigms and if such a signal is indeed non-scaled. As it is, it is not clear if the suppression of behavioral response to the visual background is due to the act of an efference copy (a copy of motor command) or due to an alternative, more global inhibitory mechanism, such as feedforward inhibition at the sensory level or attentional modulation. To directly uncover the nature of an efference copy, physiological experiments are necessary. If that is technically challenging, it requires finding a behavioral signature that unambiguously reports a (copy of) motor command and quantifying the nature of that behavior.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful and constructive comment. We agree that our current behavioral evidence does not directly identify the underlying circuit mechanism, and that direct recordings from visual neurons modulated by an efference copy would be critical for distinguishing between potential mechanisms.

      A prerequisite for such physiological investigations would be the identification of both (1) the feedforward neurons directly involved in the optomotor response, and (2) the neurons conveying motor-related signals to the optomotor circuit. Despite efforts by several research groups, the location of the feedforward circuit mediating the optomotor response remains elusive. This limitation has prevented us from obtaining direct cellular evidence of flight turn-associated suppression of optomotor signaling.

      In light of the reviewer’s suggestion, we expanded our investigation to strengthen the behavioral evidence for efference copy (EC) mechanisms. In addition to our earlier experiments involving unexpected changes in the static background, we examined how object-evoked flight turns influence the optomotor stability reflex and vice versa (Figures 5 and 6). To quantify the interaction between different visuomotor behaviors, we systematically varied the temporal relationship between two types of visual motion—loom versus moving background, or moving bar versus moving background—and measured the resulting behavioral responses.

      Our findings support pattern- and time-specific suppressive mechanisms acting between flight turns associated with the different visual patterns. Specifically:

      The responses to a moving bar and a moving background add linearly, even when presented in close temporal proximity.

      Loom-evoked turns and the optomotor stability reflex mutually suppress each other in a time-specific manner.

      For both loom- and moving bar-evoked flight turns, changes in the static background had no measurable effect on the dynamics of the object-evoked responses.

      These results provide a detailed behavioral characterization of a suppressive interaction between distinct visuomotor responses. This, in turn, offers correlative evidence supporting the involvement of an efference copy-like mechanism acting on the visual system. While similar efference copy mechanisms have been documented in other parts of the visual system, we acknowledge that our findings do not exclude alternative explanations. In particular, it is still possible that lateral inhibition within the central brain or ventral nerve cord contributes to the suppression we observed.

      Ultimately, definitive proof will require identifying the specific neurons that convey efference copy signals and demonstrating that silencing these neurons abolishes the behavioral suppression. Until such experiments are feasible, our behavioral approach provides an important contribution toward understanding the nature of sensorimotor integration in this system.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Canelo et al. used a combination of mathematical modeling and behavioral experiments to ask whether flies use an all-or-none EC model or a graded EC model (in which the turn amplitude is modulated by wide-field optic flow). Particularly, the authors focus on the bar-ground discrimination problem, which has received significant attention in flies over the last 50-60 years. First, they use a model by Poggio and Reichardt to model flight response to moving small-field bars and spots and wide-field gratings. They then simulate this model and compare simulation results to flight responses in a yaw-free tether and find generally good agreement. They then ask how flies may do bar-background discrimination (i.e. complex visual environment) and invoke different EC models and an additive model (balancing torque production due to background and bar movement). Using behavioral experiments and simulation supports the notion that flies use an all-or-none EC since flight turns are not influenced by the background optic flow. While the study is interesting, there are major issues with the conceptual framework.

      Strengths:

      They ask a significant question related to efference copies during volitional movement.

      The methods are well detailed and the data (and statistics) are presented clearly.

      The integration of behavioral experiments and mathematical modeling of flight behavior.

      The figures are overall very clear and salient.

      Weaknesses:

      Omission of saccades: While the authors ask a significant question related to the mechanism of bar-ground discrimination, they fail to integrate an essential component of the Drosophila visuomotor responses: saccades. Indeed, the Poggio and Reichardt model, which was developed almost 50 years ago, while appropriate to study body-fixed flight, has a severe limitation: it does not consider saccades. The authors identify this major issue in the Discussion by citing a recent switched, integrate-and-fire model (Mongeau & Frye, 2017). The authors admit that they "approximated" this model as a smooth pursuit movement. However, I disagree that it is an approximation; rather it is an omission of a motor program that is critical for volitional visuomotor behavior. Indeed, saccades are the main strategy by which Drosophila turn in free flight and prior to landing on an object (i.e. akin to a bar), as reported by the Dickinson group (Censi et al., van Breugel & Dickinson [not cited]). Flies appear to solve the bar-ground discrimination problem by switching between smooth movement and saccades (Mongeau & Frye, 2017; Mongeau et al., 2019 [not cited]). Thus, ignoring saccades is a major issue with the current study as it makes their model disconnected from flight behavior, which has been studied in a more natural context since the work of Poggio.

      Thank you for this helpful comment. We agree that including saccadic turns is essential and qualitatively improves the model. In the revised manuscript, we therefore expanded our bar-tracking model to incorporate an integrate-and-saccade strategy, now presented in Figure 2—figure supplement

      The manuscript now introduces this result as follows:

      (Line#190) “Finally, one important locomotion dynamics that a flying Drosophila exhibits while tracking an object is a rapid orientation change, called a “saccade” (Breugel and Dickinson, 2012; Censi et al., 2013; Heisenberg and Wolf, 1979). For example, while tracking a slowly moving bar, flies perform relatively straight flights interspersed with saccadic flight turns (Collett and Land, 1975; Mongeau and Frye, 2017). During this behavior, it has been proposed that visual circuits compute an integrated error of the bar position with respect to the frontal midline and triggers a saccadic turn toward the bar when the integrated value reaches a threshold (Frighetto and Frye, 2023; Mongeau et al., 2019; Mongeau and Frye, 2017). We expanded our bar fixation model to incorporate this behavioral strategy (Figure 2--figure supplement 2). The overall structure of the modified model is akin to the one proposed in a previous study (Mongeau and Frye, 2017), and the amplitude of a saccadic turn was determined by the sum of the position and velocity functions (Figure 2--figure supplement 2A; see Equation 13 in Methods). When simulated, our model successfully reproduced experimental observations of saccade dynamics across different object velocities (Figure 2--figure supplement 2B-D) (Mongeau and Frye, 2017). Together, our models faithfully recapitulated the results of previous behavioral observations in response to singly presented visual patterns (Collett, 1980; Götz, 1987; H. Kim et al., 2023; Maimon et al., 2008; Mongeau and Frye, 2017).”

      Apart from Figures 1 and 2, most of our data—whether from simulations or behavioral experiments—use brief visual patterns lasting 200 ms or less. These stimuli trigger a single, rapid orientation change reminiscent of a saccadic flight turn. In this part of the paper, we essentially have examined how multiple visuomotor pathways interact to determine the direction of object-evoked turns when several visual patterns occur simultaneously.

      Critically, recent work showed that a group of columnar neurons (T3) appear specialized for saccadic bar tracking through integrate-and-fire computations, supporting the notion of parallel visual circuits for saccades and smooth movement (Frighetto & Frye, 2023 [not cited]).

      Thanks for bringing up this critical issue. We have now added this paper in the following part of the manuscript.

      (Line#193) “During this behavior, it has been proposed that visual circuits compute an integrated error of the horizontal bar position with respect to the frontal midline and triggers a saccadic turn toward the bar when the integrated value reaches a threshold (Frighetto and Frye, 2023; Mongeau and Frye, 2017).”

      (Line#462) “Visual systems extract features from the environment by calculating spatiotemporal relationships of neural activities within an array of photoreceptors. In Drosophila, these calculations occur initially on a local scale in the peripheral layers of the optic lobe (Frighetto and Frye, 2023; Gruntman et al., 2018; Ketkar et al., 2020).”

      A major theme of this work is bar fixation, yet recent work showed that in the presence of proprioceptive feedback, flies do not actually center a bar (Rimniceanu & Frye, 2023). Furthermore, the same study found that yaw-free flies do not smoothly track bars but instead generate saccades. Thus prior work is in direct conflict with the work here. This is a major issue that requires more engagement by the authors.

      Thank you for your thoughtful comments and for drawing our attention to this important paper. In our experiments, bar fixation on oscillating vertical objects emerges during the “alignment” phase of the magneto-tether protocol. The pattern movement dynamics was similar those used by Rimniceanu & Frye (2023), yet the two studies differ in a key respect: Rimniceanu & Frye employed a motion-defined bar, whereas we presented a dark vertical bar against a uniform or random-dot background. The alignment success rate—defined as the proportion of trials in which the fly’s body angle is within ±25° of the target—was about 50 % (data not shown). Our alignment pattern consisted of three vertical stripes spanning ~40° horizontally; when we replaced it with a single, narrower stripe, the success rate was lowered (data not shown). These observations suggest that bar fixation in the magnetically tethered assay is less robust than in the rigid-tethered assay, although flies still orient toward highly salient vertical objects.

      We also observed that bar-evoked turns were elicited more reliably when the bar moved rapidly (45° in 200 ms) in the magneto-tether assay, although the turn magnitude was significantly smaller than the actual bar displacement (Figure 3).

      In response to the reviewer’s comment, we now added the following description in the paper regarding the bar fixation behavior, citing Rimniceanu&Frye 2023.

      (Line#239) “Another potential explanation arises from recent studies demonstrating that proprioceptive feedback provided during flight turns in a magnetically tethered assay strongly dampens the amplitude of wing and head responses (Cellini and Mongeau, 2022; Rimniceanu et al., 2023).”

      Relevance of the EC model: EC-related studies by the authors linked cancellation signals to saccades (Kim et al, 2014 & 2017). Puzzlingly, the authors applied an EC model to smooth movement, when the authors' own work showed that smooth course stabilizing flight turns do not receive cancellation signals (Fenk et al., 2021). Thus, in Fig. 4C, based on the state of the field, the efference copy signal should originate from the torque commands to initiate saccades, and not from torque to generate smooth movement. As this group previously showed, cancellation signals are quantitatively tuned to that of the expected visual input during saccades. Importantly, this tuning would be to the anticipated saccadic turn optic flow. Thus the authors' results supporting an all-or-none model appear in direct conflict with the author's previous work. Further, the addition-only model is not particularly helpful as it has been already refuted by behavioral experiments (Rimneceanu & Frye, Mongeau & Frye).

      Thank you for this constructive comment. Efference copy is best established for brief, discrete actions like flight saccades. While motor-related modulation of visual processing has been reported across short- and long-duration behaviours (Chiappe et al., 2010; Fujiwara et al., 2017; Kim et al., 2015, 2017; Maimon et al., 2010; Turner et al., 2022), only flight saccade-associated signals exhibit the temporal profile appropriate to cancel reafferent input. However, von Holst & Mittelstaedt (1950) originally formulated efference copy to explain the smooth optomotor response of hoverflies. In HS/VS recordings in previous studies, however, we could not detect membrane-potential changes tied to baseline wing-beat amplitude (data not shown), but further work is needed. 

      Note that visually evoked flight turns analyzed in this paper have relatively fast dynamics. Fenk et al. (2021) showed that HS cells carry EC-like motor signals during both loom-evoked turns and spontaneous saccades. Building on this, we tested whether object-evoked rapid turns modulate other visuomotor pathways. Although Fenk et al. also found that optomotor turns lack motor input to HS cells, the authors did not test whether the optomotor pathway suppresses other reflexes, such as loom-evoked turns. Our new behavioral data (Figure 6) show that optomotor turns indeed suppress loom-evoked turns, suggesting a potential EC signal arising from the optomotor pathway that inhibits loom-responsive visual neurons.

      In Kim et al. (2017), the authors argued that HS/VS neurons receive a “quantitatively tuned” efference copy that varies across cell types: yaw-sensitive LPTCs are strongly suppressed, roll-sensitive cells receive intermediate input, and pitch-sensitive cells receive little or none. We also showed that when the amplitude of ongoing visual drive changes, the amplitude of saccade-related potentials (SRPs) scales linearly. This proportionality does not imply a genuinely graded EC, however, because SRP amplitude could vary solely through changes in driving force (Vm – Vrest) with a fixed EC conductance. Crucially, SRPs do not fully suppress feed-forward visual signalling, arguing against an all-or-none EC mechanism.

      How, then, can the cellular and behavioural data be reconciled? Silencing HS/VS neurons—or their primary inputs, the T4/T5 neurons—does not markedly diminish the optomotor response in flight (Fenk et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2017), indicating the presence of additional, as-yet-unidentified pathways.

      Physiological recordings from other visual neurons that drive the optomotor response in flying Drosophila are therefore needed to determine how strongly they are suppressed during loom-evoked turns.

      Behavioral evidence for all-or-none EC model: The authors state "unless the stability reflex is suppressed during the flies' object evoked turns, the turns should slow down more strongly with the dense background than the sparse one". This hypothesis is based on the fact that the optomotor response magnitude is larger with a denser background, as would be predicted by an EMD model (because there are more pixels projected onto the eye). However, based on the authors' previous work, the EC should be tuned to optic flow and thus the turning velocity (or amplitude). Thus the EC need not be directly tied to the background statistics, as they claim. For instance, I think it would be important to distinguish whether a mismatch in reafferent velocity (optic flow) links to distinct turn velocities (and thus position). This would require moving the background at different velocities (co- and anti-directionally) at the onset of bar motion. Overall, there are alternative hypotheses here that need to be discussed and more fully explored (as presented by Bender & Dickinson and in work by the Maimon group).

      We appreciate the reviewer’s important suggestion. In response, we performed the recommended experiment. In Figures 5 and 6 of the revised manuscript, we now present how bar- or loom-evoked flight turns affect the response to a moving background pattern. These experiments revealed that bar-evoked turns do not suppress the optic flow response, whereas loom-evoked turns strongly suppress it. Specifically, when background motion began 100 ms after the onset of loom expansion, the response to the background was significantly suppressed. Although weak residual responses to the background motion were observed in this case, this could be due to background motion occurring outside of the suppression interval, which may correspond in duration to the duration of flight turns (Figure 6C,D). 

      The lack of suppression of the optic flow response during and after bar-evoked turns appears to suggest that the responses are added linearly (Figure 5), seemingly contradicting the lack of dynamic change when the background dot density was altered (Figure 7, Figure 7–figure supplement 1). That is, the experimental result in Figure 5 supports either an addition-only or a graded efference copy (EC) model. However, the result in Figure 7 supports an all-or-none EC model. If a graded EC were used, the amplitude of the EC should be updated almost instantaneously when the static background changes.

      Another possibility is that the optic flow during self-generated turns in a static background is extremely weak compared to the optic flow input generated by physically moving the pattern, perhaps due to the rapid nature of head movements. Indeed, detailed kinematic analysis of head movement during spontaneous saccades in blow flies revealed that the head reaches the target angle before the body completes the orientation change, making the effective speed of reafferent optic flow higher than the speed of body rotation (Hateren and Schilstra, 1999). To test these hypotheses, further experiments will be needed for bar-evoked flight turns.

      Publishing the reviewed preprint:

      (1) The Reviewed Preprint (including the full text of the preprint we reviewed, the eLife assessment, and public reviews) will typically be published in two weeks' time.

      Please let us know if you would like to provide provisional author responses to be posted at the same time (if so, please send these by email). Please do not resubmit within the next two/three weeks, as we will need to publish the first version of the Reviewed Preprint first.

      If there are any factual errors in the eLife assessment or public reviews, or other issues we should be aware of, please let us know as soon as possible.

      (2) After publication of the Reviewed Preprint, you can use the link below to submit a revised version. There is no deadline to resubmit. Before resubmitting, please ensure that you update the preprint at the preprint server to correspond with the revised version. Upon submitting a revised version, we will ask the editors and reviewers if it's appropriate to update their assessment and public reviews, which will be included alongside the revised Reviewed Preprint. At that time we will also post the recommendations to the authors and the author responses you provide with the revised version. In the author response, please respond to the public reviews (where relevant) and the recommendations to the authors.

      (3) Alternatively, you can proceed with the current version of the Reviewed Preprint (once published), without revisions, and request an eLife Version of Record. See the Author Guide for further information: https://elife-rp.msubmit.net/html/elife-rp_author_instructions.html#vor. However, most authors decide to request a Version of Record after a round of revision.

      (4) After publication of eLife's Reviewed Preprint, you also have the option to submit/publish in another journal instead: if you choose to do this, please let us know so we can update our records.

      The reviewers identified two key revisions that could improve the assessment of the paper:

      (1) Consideration of saccades within the model framework (outlined by reviewer 3).

      (2) Addition of physiology data to support the conclusions of the paper (outlined by reviewer 2). If this is not feasible within the timescale of revisions, the paper would need to be revised to clarify that the model leads to a hypothesis that would need to be tested with future physiology experiments.

      Thank you for these comments.

      Regarding revision point #1, we have added Figure 2–figure supplement 2, where we incorporated our position-velocity model (estimated in Figure 1) into the framework of the integrate-and-saccade model. A detailed description of this model is now provided in the main text (Lines 190–203).

      For revision point #2, obtaining electrophysiological evidence for efference copy remains challenging, as neither the visual neurons nor the efference-copy neuron has been identified for the wing optomotor response. As suggested by the reviewers, we have revised the title of the paper to reduce emphasis on efference copy and have noted electrophysiological recordings as a direction for future work.

      old title: A visual efference copy-based navigation algorithm in Drosophila for complex visual environments

      new title: Integrative models of visually guided steering in Drosophila

      Specific recommendations are detailed below.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      To directly demonstrate if an efference copy is non-scaled, the following experiments can be helpful: record from HS/VS cells and examine the relation between the amplitude of the succade-suppression signal vs. succade amplitude.

      Thanks for raising this important point. We previously carried out the suggested analysis for loom-evoked saccades in Fenk et al. (2021). There, significant correlations emerged between wing-response amplitude and saccade-related potentials (Figures 2F and 3C). However, we did not interpret the strong correlation (r ≈ 0.8) as evidence for a graded efference copy, because the amplitude of saccade-related potentials appeared to be bimodal. Upon presentation of the looming stimulus, flies either executed large evasive turns or showed minimal changes in wing-stroke amplitude. Large wing responses were accompanied by strong, saturated suppression of HS-cell membrane potential, whereas trials without wing responses produced only weak modulations—reflected in the bimodal distribution of saccade-related potential amplitudes (Figure 3C). 

      Importantly, in rigidly tethered preparations—where these potentials are typically measured—the absence of proprioceptive feedback can itself drive wingbeat amplitudes to saturation during saccades. We therefore reasoned that the lack of intermediate-sized flight saccades would naturally yield correspondingly saturated saccade-related potentials, even if a graded EC system is in play. 

      In Kim et al. (2017), we also performed a comprehensive analysis of spontaneous saccade-related potentials across all HS/VS cell types. When we later examined the relationship between saccade amplitude and the corresponding saccade-related potentials in each cell type, we could not find any statistically significant correlation (unpublished data).

      measure how much a weak visual stimulus and a strong visual stimulus are suppressed by the suppression signal. If the signal is non-scaled, visual stimuli should always be suppressed independently of their intensities.

      Thank you for this important suggestion. As mentioned in our response to the previous comment, we believe it is not feasible to record from neurons responsible for the body optomotor response at this point, as their identity remains unknown. Regarding the HS/VS cells, our previous study showed that HS cells are not always fully suppressed. The changes in saccade-related potential amplitude can be described as a linear function of the pre-saccadic visually-evoked membrane potential (Figure 7 in Kim et al., 2017). 

      As suggested by Fenk et al. 2014 (doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.042), HS cells might also be responsive to a moving bar. If that is the case, and if you present a bar and background (either sparse or dense) in a closed-loop manner to a head-fixed fly, HS cells might be sensitive only to the bar but not to the background (independently of the density).

      Thanks for pointing out this important issue. HS cells indeed respond strongly to the horizontal movement of a vertical bar, as expected given that their receptive fields are formed by the integration of local optic flow vectors. In one of our previous studies (Supplemental Figure 1 in Kim et al., 2015), we showed that the response amplitude to a single vertical bar is roughly equivalent to that elicited by a vertical grating composed of 12 bars of the same size. Therefore, we believe that HS cells are likely to contribute to the head response to a moving vertical bar. In a body-fixed flight simulator, HS cells would respond only to the bar if the bar runs in a closed loop with a static background. In this scenario, HS cells are likely to play a role in the head optomotor response.

      Note also that the role of HS cells in the wing optomotor response remains unresolved. Unilateral activation of HS cells has been shown to elicit locomotor turns in walking Drosophila (Fujiwara et al., 2017), as well as in flying individuals (unpublished data from our lab). However, a previous study also showed that strong silencing of HS/VS cells significantly reduced the head optomotor response, but not the wing optomotor response (Kim et al., 2017).

      If neurophysiology is technically challenging, an alternative way might pay attention to a head movement that exclusively follows the background (Fox et al., 2014 (doi: 10.1242/jeb.080192)). Because HS cells are thought to promote head rotation to background motion, a non-scaled suppression signal on HS cells would always suppress the head rotation independently of the background density.

      Thanks for this helpful comment. We have analyzed head movements during bar-evoked flight turns (Figure 7–figure supplement 1B) and found no significant changes across different background dot densities. We think that this might suggest that HS cells are unlikely to receive suppressive inputs during bar-evoked turns, akin to the lack of modulation during optomotor turns.

      Another way to separate a potential efference copy from other mechanisms (more global inhibition) is the directionality. A global inhibition would suppress the response to the background even if the background moves in the same direction as self-motion, but the efference copy would not.

      Thanks for this important point. In Heisenberg and Wolf, 1979, it was proposed that modulation might be bidirectional, with behavioral effects observed only for perturbations in the “unexpected” direction. In our new data on loom-evoked turns (Figure 6), the suppression appears equally strong for background motion in either direction, supporting an all-or-none suppression mechanism.

      Besides, in general, it is unclear if you think an efference copy operates both in smooth pursuits and saccades or if such a signal is only present during saccades. Your previous neurophysiological work supports the latter. Are your behavioral results consistent with the previous saccade suppression idea, or do you propose a new type of efference copy that also operates in smooth pursuits?

      Thanks for raising this important point. von Holst and Mittelstaedt (1950) originally introduced the concept of efference copy to explain the smooth optomotor response. We previously analyzed electrophysiological recordings from HS cells for membrane-potential changes associated with slow deviations in wing-steering angle but found none. However, this negative result does not entirely rule out modulation of visual processing during smooth flight turns, given the slow drift in membrane potential observed in most whole-cell recordings.

      In this study, We examined only the interactions among visuomotor pathways during these rapid flight turns as the dynamics of visually evoked turns are almost as rapid as spontaneous saccades. Our data reveal that interactions between distinct visuomotor reflexes are more diverse than previously appreciated.

      Minor comments:

      Line 108, 109: match the description between here and the labels in Fig. 1F.

      Thank you for indicating this issue. We have defined the general equation to obtain the position and velocity components in the main text lines 108,109, but due to a slight asymmetry in the data (Fig. 1E) we used the approach indicated in Fig. 1F. and explained in lines 113-117.

      Fig.1 F: If the position-dependent component is due to fatigue, the tuning curve's shape is likely changed (shrunk or extended) depending on the stimulus speed. How can you generalize the tuning curve shown here? Does the result hold even if the stimulus speed/contrast/spatial frequency is changed?

      We appreciate this indication. We believed that fatigue may be the reason why the wing response to the grating stimulus showed that significant decay (Fig. 1E). As you mention, the stimulus speed would increase the amplitude of the fly’s response up to a saturation point. We addressed this in our model by multiplying the derived value by the angular velocity of the grating.

      Regarding the contrast, and spatial frequency we did not test it experimentally, instead, we simulated our model for changing visual feedback (Fig. 4A, B), which can be seen as increasing/decreasing contrast of a grating. An increase in the contrast would increase the response of the fly to the grating and so will contribute to dampening the response to the foreground object (Fig. 4C).

      Line 233-255: Here, the description sounds like you will consider several parallel objects (e.g., two stripes) in the visual field instead of the combination of the figure and background (which is referred to in the following paragraph).

      Thank you for pointing it out. Indeed it was slightly ambiguous. We have addressed this by explaining the specific situation of a combination of an object and the background in lines 231-233.

      Figure 6C: you kept the foreground visual field between sparse and dense random dot backgrounds to keep the bar's saliency. Is it sure that this does not influence the difference in the fly's response to these two backgrounds (in Figure 6B)?

      This is a good point that we have also discussed internally. We also carried out similar experiments with a fully covered background and found no significant differences (Figure 7–figure supplement 1).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Identify and analyze flight saccade dynamics in the raw trajectories (e.g., Fig. 3B). There should be some since the bar is near the 'sweet spot' for triggering saccades (see Mongeau & Frye, 2017).

      Thank you for bringing up this interesting point. In previous work, it was reported that the fly fixated on a vertical bar through saccadic turns rather than smooth-tracking (Mongeau & Frye, 2017). When the bar width was thin (<15 deg) there was barely one saccade per second (Mongeau & Frye, 2017, Fig. 4). In our magno tether essay (Fig. 3A, B) the object width was 11.25 degrees, and the object moved for a short time window, and so the fly only generated the saccade related to the onset of the object. It could not be considered as a saccade some small turns of a few degrees that are likely related to small perturbations in comparison to those previously reported (Mongeau & Frye, 2017). Additionally, in our protocol (Fig. 3A) from onset time (‘go’ mark), only a single object moved, within an empty background, so in principle there is no trigger for a switch to a smooth movement. We addressed this in lines x-x.

      Consider updating the Poggio model with flight saccades (switched, integrate-and-fire).

      We appreciate this suggestion. Following previous work (Mongeau et al., 2017), we expanded our model to include a saccade mechanism: the torque produced by the summed position- and velocity-dependent components is now replaced by an integrate-and-fire saccade (Figure 2—figure supplement 2). We optimized the saccade interval and amplitude so that both vary linearly with stimulus amplitude and faithfully reproduce the kinematic properties reported previously (Mongeau et al., 2017).  

      Please engage more with the literature, especially work that directly conflicts with your conclusions (see above). Also, highly relevant work by Bender & Dickinson was not sufficiently discussed. Spot results presented in Fig. 3 should be contextualized in light of the work of Mongeau et al., 2019, who performed similar experiments and identified a switch in saccade valence.

      We appreciate your pointing out the relevant previous work. We have added references to the following papers and tried to describe the relationship between our data and previous ones.

      Bender & Dickinson 2006

      (Line#162) “This simulation experiment is reminiscent of the magnetically tethered flight assay, where a flying fly remains fixed at a position but is free to rotate around its yaw axis (Bender and Dickinson, 2006b; Cellini et al., 2022; G. Kim et al., 2023; Mongeau and Frye, 2017).”

      (Line#218) “We tested the predictions of our models with flies flying in an environment similar to that used in the simulation (Figure 3A). A fly was tethered to a short steel pin positioned vertically at the center of a vertically oriented magnetic field, allowing it to rotate around its yaw axis with minimal friction (Bender and Dickinson, 2006b; Cellini et al., 2022; G. Kim et al., 2023).”

      (Line#238) “To determine if our assay imposes additional friction compared to other assays used in previous studies, we analyzed the dynamics of spontaneous saccades during the “freeze” phase (Figure 3–figure supplement 1A). We found their duration and amplitude to be within the range reported previously (Bender and Dickinson, 2006b; Mongeau and Frye, 2017) (Figure 3–figure supplement 1B-D). 

      Mongeau et al., 2019

      (Line#196) “During this behavior, it has been proposed that visual circuits compute an integrated error of the bar position with respect to the frontal midline and triggers a saccadic turn toward the bar when the integrated value reaches a threshold (Frighetto and Frye, 2023; Mongeau et al., 2019; Mongeau and Frye, 2017). We expanded our bar fixation model to incorporate this behavioral strategy (Figure 2–figure supplement 2).”

      This paper shows that the dynamics of saccadic flight turns elicited by a rotating bar or spot determine whether flies display attraction or aversion. In that study, the visual stimulus—a bar or spot—rotated slowly at a constant 75 deg s⁻¹. By contrast, in our Figure 3 the object moves much faster, driving the neural “integrator” to saturation and triggering an almost immediate flight turn. In Mongeau et al. (2019), saccades occur at variable times and their amplitudes and directions are more stochastic, again reflecting the slower stimulus speed. Because these differences all arise from the disparity in object speed, we did not cite Mongeau et al. (2019) in Figure 3 or the associated text.

      In addition to the two papers cited above, we have incorporated several relevant studies on the Drosophila visuomotor control identified through the reviewers’ insightful comments. Examples include:

      Frighetto G, Frye MA. 2023 (Line#195, 464)

      Rimniceanu et al., 2023 (Line#241)

      Cellini & Mongeau 2020 (Line#91)

      Cellini & Mongeau 2022 (Line#241)

      Cellini et al., 2022 (LIne#91, 162, 218)

      Many citations are not in the proper format (e.g. using numbers rather than authors' last name).

      Thank you for letting us know. We have changed the remaining citations to the proper format.

    1. Author response:

      We would like to thank the reviewers for their helpful comments and critique of our manuscript. We plan to make the following revisions, which will improve the clarity of our manuscript and the robustness of our findings.

      We will revise methodological details and interpretation throughout the manuscript. In particular, we will consider alternative methods for calculating surrogates. We intend to investigate the relationship between apnoea rate and phase-amplitude coupling at other electrodes as suggested by Reviewer 1, and we will revise the details of the linear-mixed effects models.

      In relation to the comments raised by both Reviewers 2 and 3, we will carefully address the wording throughout the manuscript, including addressing the order of hypotheses, our interpretation of the directionality of the relationship between cortical and respiratory activity, and the connection between cortical-respiratory coupling and apnoea. We will further clarify the limitations of our recording setup and approach, in particular the limited EEG montage, and add further details with regards to sleep state and caffeine.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study presents an interesting investigation into the role of trained immunity in inflammatory bowel disease, demonstrating that β-glucan-induced reprogramming of innate immune cells can ameliorate experimental colitis. The findings are novel and clinically relevant, with potential implications for therapeutic strategies in IBD. The combination of functional assays, adoptive transfer experiments, and single-cell RNA sequencing provides comprehensive mechanistic insights. However, some aspects of the study could benefit from further clarification to strengthen the conclusions.

      We are grateful for the reviewer’s positive assessment of our study and constructive suggestions to improve the manuscript.

      Strengths:

      (1) This study elegantly connects trained immunity with IBD, demonstrating how β-glucan-induced innate immune reprogramming can mitigate chronic inflammation.

      (2) Adoptive transfer experiments robustly confirm the protective role of monocytes/macrophages in colitis resolution.

      (3) Single-cell RNA sequencing provides mechanistic depth, revealing the expansion of reparative Cx3cr1⁺ macrophages and their contribution to epithelial repair.

      (4) The work highlights the therapeutic potential of trained immunity in restoring gut homeostasis, offering new directions for IBD treatment.

      Weaknesses:

      While β-glucan may exert its training effect on hematopoietic stem cells, performing ATAC-seq on HSCs or monocytes to profile chromatin accessibility at antibacterial defense and mucosal repair-related genes would further validate the trained immunity mechanism. Alternatively, the authors could acknowledge this as a study limitation and future research direction.

      We agree that further epigenetic profiling—such as ATAC-seq analysis on HSCs or monocytes—would provide additional mechanistic depth to our current findings. We will acknowledge this as a limitation of the present study and highlight it as an important direction for future research.

      Comment (1): It’s better to include a schematic summarizing the proposed mechanism for reader clarity.

      We agree that a visual summary will enhance the clarity and accessibility of our findings. We will add a new schematic diagram (Figure 6) illustrating the proposed mechanism of β-glucan–induced myeloid reprogramming and its protective effects in the experimental colitis model.

      Comment (2): Discuss potential off-target effects of β-glucan-induced trained immunity (e.g., risk of exacerbated inflammation in other contexts).

      We appreciate this important comment regarding the potential off-target effects of β-glucan pretreatment. As trained immunity is known to amplify inflammatory responses upon heterologous stimulation and has been implicated in chronic inflammation–prone conditions such as atherosclerosis, this is an important consideration. Previous in vivo studies have shown that β-glucan pretreatment can enhance antibacterial or antitumor responses without inducing basal inflammation after one week of administration (PMID: 22901542, PMID: 30380404, PMID: 36604547, PMID: 33125892). Nevertheless, it remains possible that β-glucan–induced trained immunity could have unintended effects in certain contexts, which warrants further investigation and caution. We will expand the Discussion section to include a dedicated paragraph addressing these potential off-target effects.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study investigates whether β-glucan (BG) can reprogram the innate immune system to protect against intestinal inflammation. The authors show that mice pretreated with BG prior to DSS-induced colitis experience reduced colitis severity, including less weight loss, colon damage, improved gut repair, and lowered inflammation. These effects were independent of adaptive immunity and were linked to changes in monocyte function.

      The authors show that the BG-trained monocytes not only help control inflammation but confer non-specific protection against experimental infections (Salmonella), suggesting the involvement of trained immunity (TI) mechanisms. Using single-cell RNA sequencing, they map the transcriptional changes in these cells and show enhanced differentiation of monocytes into reparative CX3CR1<sup>+</sup> macrophages. Importantly, these protective effects were transferable to other mice via adoptive cell transfer and bone marrow transplantation, suggesting that the innate immune system had been reprogrammed at the level of stem/progenitor cells.

      Overall, this study provides evidence that TI, often associated with heightened inflammatory programs, can also promote tissue repair and resolution of inflammation. Moreover, this BG-induced functional reprogramming can be further harnessed to treat chronic inflammatory disorders like IBD.

      Strengths:

      (1) The authors use advanced experimental approaches to explore the potential therapeutic use of myeloid reprogramming by β-glucan in IBD.

      (2) The authors follow a data-to-function approach, integrating bulk and single-cell RNA sequencing with in vivo functional validation to support their conclusions.

      (3) The study adds to the growing evidence that TI is not a singular pro-inflammatory program, but can adopt distinct functional states, including anti-inflammatory and reparative phenotypes, depending on the context.

      We are grateful for the reviewer’s positive assessment of our study and recognition of its translational implications. We particularly appreciate the acknowledgment that our work expands the therapeutic potential of β-glucan–mediated trained immunity in ameliorating colitis.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The epigenetic and metabolic basis of TI is not explored, which weakens the mechanistic claim of TI. This is especially relevant given that a novel reparative, anti-inflammatory TI program is proposed.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s valuable comment highlighting the importance of the epigenetic and metabolic basis of TI in providing mechanistic insight. While previous studies, including work from our group (S.-C. Cheng), have extensively characterized the epigenetic and metabolic signatures of monocytes from BG-trained mice—primarily in the context of inflammatory genes—we acknowledge that these aspects are not directly addressed in our current manuscript.

      To strengthen the mechanistic component, we plan to: 1. Reanalyze relevant public datasets, focusing on pathways related to reparative and antibacterial function. 2. Perform monocyte ATAC-seq in our current model to validate the epigenetic changes in these pathways.

      (2) The absence of a BG-only group limits interpretation of the results. Since the authors report tissue-level effects such as enhanced mucosal repair and transcriptional shifts in intestinal macrophages (colonic RNA-Seq), it is important to rule out whether BG alone could influence the gut independently of DSS-induced inflammation.

      Without a BG-only control, it is hard to distinguish a true trained response from a potential modulation caused directly by BG.

      We thank the reviewer for this important suggestion. Although we did not perform qPCR for mucosal repair genes in Figure S1C and Figure S1D, our colon RNA-seq analysis in Figure 5G included a BG-only control group (Colitis_d0). The results from this group indicate that BG preconditioning alone does not alter baseline expression of colon mucosal repair genes, supporting the conclusion that the observed effects occur in the context of DSS-induced inflammation.

      (3) Although monocyte transfer experiments show protection in colitis, the fate of the transferred cells is not described (e.g., homing or differentiation into Cx3cr1⁺ macrophage subsets). This weakens the link between specific monocyte subsets and the observed phenotype.

      (4) While scRNA-seq reveals distinct monocyte/macrophage subclusters (Mono1-3.), their specific functional roles remain speculative. The authors assign reparative or antimicrobial functions based on transcriptional signatures, but do not perform causal experiments (depletion or in vitro assays). The biological roles of these cells remain correlative.

      We agree that the functional role of CX3CR1<sup>+</sup> macrophages is not comprehensively validated and is currently inferred from scRNA-seq clustering. While our flow cytometry data show increased CX3CR1<sup>+</sup> macrophages in the BG-TI group, and our CCR2 KO and monocyte adoptive transfer experiments indicate these macrophages are monocyte-derived, we lack direct depletion experiments due to the unavailability of effective depletion antibodies for this subset.

      We acknowledge this as a limitation and will clarify in the Discussion that our conclusions regarding CX3CR1<sup>+</sup> macrophage function are based on transcriptional profiling and association with protective phenotypes, rather than direct causal evidence.

      (5) While Rag1<sup>-/-</sup> mice were used to rule out adaptive immunity, the potential role of innate lymphoid cells (ILCs), particularly ILC2s and ILC3s, which are known to promote mucosal repair (PMID: 27484190IF: 7.6 Q1 IF: 7.6 Q1 IF: 7.6 Q1 IF: 7.6 Q1 IF: 7.6 Q1 IF: 7.6 Q1 ), was not explored. Given the reparative phenotype observed, the contribution of ILCs remains a confounding factor.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s valuable comment regarding the potential role of ILCs in the observed mucosal repair. Indeed, in examining the BG-trained immunity effect, the contribution of ILCs was not evaluated. We will explicitly acknowledge in the Discussion that Rag1⁻/⁻ mice retain ILCs (including ILC3s) and that BG-induced activation of these cells remains possible.

      The literature (PMID: 21502992; PMID: 32187516) supports a role for ILC3-mediated IL-22 production in tissue repair, which could overlap with our observed effects. However, our monocyte adoptive transfer experiments show that monocytes alone can alleviate DSS-induced colitis, suggesting a dominant role for monocytes in this context. Nonetheless, we will make it clear that ILC contributions cannot be excluded.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the present work, Yinyin Lv et al offer evidence for the therapeutic potential of trained immunity in the context of inflammatory bowel disease (IBD). Prior research has demonstrated that innate cells pre-treated (trained) with β-glucan show an enhanced pro-inflammatory response upon a second challenge.

      While an increased immune response can be beneficial and protect against bacterial infections, there is also the risk that it will worsen symptoms in various inflammatory disorders. In the present study, the authors show that mice preconditioned with β-glucan have enhanced resistance to Staphylococcus aureus infection, indicating heightened immune responses.

      The authors demonstrate that β-glucan training of bone marrow hematopoietic progenitors and peripheral monocytes mitigates the pro-inflammatory effects of colitis, with protection extending to naïve recipients of the trained cells.

      Using a dextran sulfate sodium (DSS)-induced model of colitis, β-glucan pre-treatment significantly dampens disease severity. Importantly, the use of Rag1<sup>-/-</sup> mice, which lack adaptive immune cells, confirms that the protective effects of β-glucan are mediated by innate immune mechanisms. Further, experiments using Ccr2<sup>-/-</sup> mice underline the necessity of monocyte recruitment in mediating this protection, highlighting CCR2 as a key factor in the mobilization of β-glucan-trained monocytes to inflamed tissues. Transcriptomic profiling reveals that β-glucan training upregulates genes associated with pattern recognition, antimicrobial defense, immunomodulation, and interferon signaling pathways, suggesting broad functional reprogramming of the innate immune compartment. In addition, β-glucan training induces a distinct monocyte subpopulation with enhanced activation and phagocytic capacity. These monocytes exhibit an increased ability to infiltrate inflamed colonic tissue and differentiate into macrophages, marked by increased expression of Cx3cr1. Moreover, among these trained monocyte and macrophage subsets, other gene expression signatures are associated with tissue and mucosal repair, suggesting a role in promoting resolution and regeneration following inflammatory insult.

      Strengths:

      (1) Overall, the authors present a mechanistically insightful investigation that advances our understanding of trained immunity in IBD.

      (2) By employing a range of well-characterized murine models, the authors investigate specific mechanisms involved in the effects of β-glucan training.

      (3) Furthermore, the study provides functional evidence that the protection conferred by the trained cells persists within the hematopoietic progenitors and can be transferred to naïve recipients. The integration of transcriptomic profiling allows the identification of changes in key genes and molecular pathways underlying the trained immune phenotype.

      (4) This is an important study that demonstrates that β-glucan-trained innate cells confer protection against colitis and promote mucosal repair, and these findings underscore the potential of harnessing innate immune memory as a therapeutic approach for chronic inflammatory diseases.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive evaluation and constructive feedback on our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      However, FPKM is not ideal for between-sample comparisons due to its within-sample normalization approach. Best practices recommend using raw counts (with DESeq2) for more robust statistical inference.

      We appreciate the reminder about best practices for RNA-seq analysis. We apologize for the inaccurate description in the Materials and Methods section. For all differential expression analyses, we have in fact used raw count data as input for DESeq2. FPKM values were only used for visualization purposes, such as in heatmaps and clustering analyses. We will correct this description in the revised manuscript to accurately reflect our analysis workflow.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary

      In this study, Takagi and colleagues demonstrate that changes in axonal arborization of the segmental wave motor command neurons are sufficient to change behavioral motor output.

      The authors identify the Wnt receptors DFz2 and DFz4 and the ligand Wnt4 as modulators of stereotypic segmental arborization patterns of segmental wave neurons along the anterior-posterior body axis. Based on both embryonic expression pattern analysis and genetic manipulation of the signaling components in wave neurons (receptors) and the neuropil (Wnt4) the authors convincingly demonstrate that Wnt4 acts as a repulsive ligand for DFz2 that restricts posterior axon guidance of both anterior and posterior wave neurons. They also provide the first evidence that Wnt4 potentially acts as an attractive ligand for Df4 to promote the posterior extension of p-wave neurons. Interestingly, artificial optogenetic activation of all wave neurons that normally induces backward locomotion due to the activity of anterior wave neurons, fails to induce backward locomotion in a DFz2 knockdown condition with altered axonal extensions of all wave neurons towards posterior segments. In addition, the authors now observe enhanced fast-forward locomotion, a feature normally induced by posterior wave neurons. Consistent with these findings, they observe that the natural response to an anterior tactile stimulus is similarly altered in DFz2 knockdown animals. The animals respond with less backward movement and increased fast forward motion. These results suggest that alterations in the innervation pattern of wave motor command neurons are sufficient to switch behavioral response programs.

      Strengths

      The authors convincingly demonstrate the importance of Wnt signaling for anteriorposterior axon guidance of a single class of motor command neurons in the larval CNS. The demonstration that alteration of the expression level of a single axon guidance receptor is sufficient to not only alter the innervation pattern but to significantly modify the behavioral response program of the animal provides a potential entry point to understanding behavioral adaptations during evolution.

      Weaknesses

      While the authors demonstrate an alteration of the behavioral response to a natural tactile stimulus the observed effects, a reduction of backward motion and increased fast-foward locomotion, currently cannot be directly correlated to the morphological alterations observed in the single-neuron analyses. The authors do not report any loss of innervation in the "normal" target region but only a small additional innervation of more posterior regions. An analysis of synaptic connectivity and/or a more detailed morphological analysis that is supported by a larger number of analyzed neurons both in control and experimental animals would further strengthen the confidence of the study. As the authors suggest an alteration of the command circuitry, a direct observation of the downstream activation pattern in response to selective optogenetic stimulation of anterior wave neurons would further strengthen their claims (analogous to Takagi et al., 2017, Figure 4).

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for their insightful comments, which were instrumental in improving our manuscript. In response to the reviewers’ suggestion, we have now studied Brp expression and demonstrate that the ectopically extending Wave axons in the posterior region do contain synapses (new Figure 2). This finding supports the idea that these axons are functionally connected to ectopic downstream circuits. 

      Additionally, we have increased the number of analyzed Wave clones in Figure 1F-J (WT and DFz2 KD) and new Figure 3C-G (WT; formerly Figure 2C-G) to strengthen the morphological analyses. We fully agree with the reviewer that “direct observation of the downstream activation pattern in response to selective optogenetic stimulation” would further reinforce our conclusions. However, this was not feasible in the current study since we found that the Wave-Gal4 driver used in this study, which drives expression during embryonic stages, does not drive sufficiently strong expression in the larvae to enable selective optogenetic stimulation (please see below for details). 

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors previously demonstrated that anterior-located a-Wave neurons (neuromeres A1-A3) extend axons anteriorly to connect to circuits inducing backward locomotion, while p-Wave axon (neuromeres A4-A7) project posteriorly to promote forward locomotion in Drosophila larvae. In the manuscript, the authors aim to determine the molecular mechanisms involved in wiring the segmentally homologous Wave neurons distinctively and thus are functionally different in modulating forward or backward locomotion. The genetic screen focused on Wnt/Fz-signaling due to its known anterior-to-posterior guidance roles in mammals and nematodes.

      Strengths:

      Knock-down (KD) DFz2 with two independent RNAi-lines caused ectopic posterior axon and dendrite extension for all a- and p-Wave neurons, with a-Wave axon extending into regions where p-Wave axons normally project. Both behavioral assays (optogenetic stimulation of all Wave neurons or tactile stimuli on heads using a von Frey filament) show that backward movement is reduced or absent and that the speed of evoked fast-forward locomotion is increased. This demonstrates that altered projections of Wave do alter behavior and the DFz2 KD phenotype is consistent with the potential aberrant wiring of a-Wave neurons to forward locomotion-promoting circuits instead of to backward locomotion-promoting circuits.

      The main conclusion, that Wnt/Fz-signaling is essential for the guidance of Wave neurons and in diversifying their protection pattern in a segment-specific manner, is further supported by the results showing that DFz2 gain of function causes shortening of a-Wave but not p-Wave axon extensions towards the posterior end and that KD of DFz4 causes axonal shortening only in A6-p-Wave neurons but does not affect dendrites or processes of other Wave neurons. A role for ligand Wnt4 is demonstrated by results indicating that WNT4 mutants' posterior extension of aWave axons was elongated similar to DFz2 KD animals and p-Wave axon extension towards the posterior end was shortened similar to DFz2 KD animals. Finally, a DWnt4 gradient decreasing from the posterior (A8) to the anterior end (A2), similar to that described in other species, is supported by analyses of DWnt4 gene expression (using Wnt4 Trojan-Gal4) and protein expression (using antibodies). In contrast, DFz2 receptor levels seemed to decrease from the anterior (A2) to the posterior end (A5/6). Together the results support the conclusion that opposing Wnt/Fz ligand-receptor gradients contribute to the diversification of Wave neurons in a location-dependent manner and that DFz2 and DFz4 have opposing effects on axon extension.

      Weaknesses:

      Wave axon and dendrite projections are not exclusively determined by Wnt4, DFz2, and DFz4, and are likely to involve other Fz receptors, Wt ligands, and other types of receptor-ligand signaling pathways. This is in part supported by the fact that Wnt4 loss of function also resulted in phenotypes that do not mimic DFz2 KD or DFz4 KD (Figures 3D, E, and F) and that other Fz/Wnt mutants caused wave neuron phenotypes (Figure 1-supplement 2, D+E). This is not a weakness per se, since it doesn't affect the main conclusion of the manuscript. However, the description and analyses of the data in particular for Figure 1-supplement 2 D should be clarified in the legend. The number within the bars and the asterisks are not defined. It's presumed they refer to numbers of animals assessed and the asterisk next to DFz2 and DFz4 indicate statistically significant differences. However, only one p-value is provided in the legend. It is also unclear if p-values for the other mutants have not been determined or are non-significant. At least for mutants like Corin, which also exhibit altered axon projections, the p-values should be provided.

      We appreciate this reviewer’s careful attention to detail and intellectual curiosity. We apologize for the confusions caused by the statistical reporting in Figure 1 – figure supplement 2D. The numbers shown in the bars represent the number of neurons (i.e. Wave neurons from left or right hemisphere). As mentioned in Materials and Methods section, we applied Chi-square test followed by Haberman's adjusted residual analysis to determine the statistical significance of each RNAi group. The p-value provided in the figure legend corresponds to the Chi-square test. P-values for Haberman's adjusted residual analysis were calculated for all RNAi groups and groups without the asterisk are not statistically significant. We have clarified these points in the corresponding figure legend.

      Figure 4 D, F. The gradient for Wnt4 was determined by comparison of expression levels of other segments to A8 but the gradient for DFz2 was by comparison to A2 and the data supports opposing gradients. However, for DFz2 (Figure 4, F) it seems that the gradient is bi-directional with the lowest being in A5 and increasing towards A2 as well as A8. Analysis should be performed in reference to A8 as well to determine if it is indeed bi-directional. While such a finding would not affect the interpretation of aWave neurons, it may impact conclusions about p-Wave neuron projections.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting this interesting possibility. In response, we performed an additional analysis of the DFz2 gradient by comparing the signal from each neuromere to that from A8 (new Figure 5—figure supplement 3). This analysis confirmed that the gradient is indeed bidirectional. We revised the description of DFz2 expression accordingly in the revision. We believe this finding does not affect our main conclusions since only the anterior gradient is relevant for a-Wave axon guidance. 

      As discussed above, the DFz2 KD phenotypes are consistent with the potential aberrant wiring of a-Wave neurons to forward locomotion-promoting circuits instead of to backward locomotion-promoting circuits. However, since the axon and dendrites of a-Wave and p-Wave are affected the actual dendritic and axonal contributions for the altered behavior remain elusive. The authors certainly considered a potential contribution of altered dendrite projection of a-Wave neurons to the phenotype and their conclusion that altered axonal projections are involved is supported by the optogenetic experiment "bypassing" sensory input (albeit it seems unlikely that all Wave neurons are activated simultaneously when perceiving natural stimuli).However, the author should also consider that altered perception and projection of pWave neuron may directly (e.g. extended P-wave axon projections increase forward locomotion input thereby overriding backward locomotion) or indirectly (e.g. feedback loops between forward and backward circuits) contribute to the altered behavioral phenotypes in both assays. It is probably noteworthy that the more complex behavioral alterations observed with mechanical stimulation are likely to also be caused by altered dendritic projections.

      We fully agree with the reviewer’s thoughtful interpretation. We have now included these important possibilities in the revised Discussion section. Specifically, we acknowledge that while the DFz2 knockdown phenotypes are consistent with aberrant wiring of a-Wave neurons to forward locomotion-promoting circuits, the contributions of both axonal and dendritic alterations remain unclear. We also recognize that altered perception and projection of p-Wave neurons may directly or indirectly contribute to the observed behavioral phenotypes, particularly in response to mechanical stimulation.

      Presynaptic varicosities of a-Wave neurons in DFz2 KD animals are indicated by orange arrows in Figure 1. However, no presynaptic markers have been used to confirm actual ectopic synaptic connections. At least the authors should more clearly define what parameters they used to "visually" define potential presynaptic varicosities. Some arrows seem to point to more "globular structures" but for several others, it's unclear what they are pointing at.

      As mentioned in our response to Reviewer #1, we have now performed Brp immunostaining to confirm the presence of ectopic synaptic connections (new Figure 2). This analysis supports the interpretation that the presynaptic varicosities observed in DFz2 knockdown animals represent actual synaptic sites. We also clarified in the figure legend the visual criteria used to identify potential presynaptic varicosities.

      Reviewing Editor (Recommendations For The Authors):

      There are a few major concerns that we recommend the authors address:

      (1) Neuroanatomy: The point aberrant synaptic connectivity of a-Wave neurons following Dfz2 knockdown could be substantiated. This could be done by using a presynaptic marker and showing ectopic posterior presynaptic sites ( and/or reduced anterior presynaptic sites) in a-wave neurons.

      As mentioned in our response to the public review, we now have used Brp as a presynaptic marker to quantify the number and distribution of presynaptic sites along the normal and ectopic a-Wave axons (new Figure 2). We show that ectopic posterior Wave axons do contain presynaptic sites.  

      (2) Gradient calculations: As detailed in the reviews below, the Dfz2 gradient looks like it may be bidirectional. Changing the way the gradient is calculated might help address this point.

      As mentioned in our response above, we now have recalculated the gradient by comparing the DFz2 signal to A8 and show that it indeed is bidirectional (new Figure 5—figure supplement 2; formerly Figure 4—figure supplement 2).

      (3)  Statistics and sample sizes: As detailed in the reviews, some of the statistical reporting could be improved. Further, increasing sample sizes could help bolster confidence in the data as well.

      As mentioned above, we have added a description on the sample size, asterisks, and p-values in Figure 1 – figure supplement 2 legend. We also increased sample sizes of single Wave neurons in control and DFz2 knock-down animals (Figure 1F-J (WT and DFz2 KD) and new Figure 3C-G (WT; formerly Figure 2C-G)).

      (4) It would help to include some discussion of the potential contributions of altered p-wave neurons to the observed phenotypes.

      As described above, we have added in the Discussion potential contributions of altered p-wave neurons to the observed phenotypes. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) In the current model the authors assume that posterior elongation of a-wave neuron connectivity (axonal projections) induces a loss of connectivity to their natural targets, as backward motion is no longer induced, and a gain of connectivity to posterior wave neuron targets. Is this at the cost of innervation of p-wave neurons, e.g. did these neurons now lose connectivity to their natural targets as well? Therefore, it would be very interesting if the authors would test the behavioral responses to tactile stimuli in the posterior parts of the animal - does the response pattern change?

      This is indeed an interesting possibility that p-Wave function is altered upon DFz2 knock-down and hence behavioral response to posterior touch is changed. However, it is technically challenging to test this with tactile stimuli, due to the difficulty of (1) distinguishing between normal and fast-forward locomotion and (2) delivering a posterior touch stimulus while the larva is moving forward, which is the default behavior of the larvae on an agar plate.

      As highlighted above, the authors should provide additional evidence that the circuit response to a-wave neurons is changed after a DFz2 knockdown. The authors should monitor the activation wave in response to optogenetic activation of anterior wave neurons - analogous to the data provided in Figure 4 of their 2017 paper. If this response is now switched for a-wave activation but not p-wave activation it would greatly support their claims and this data would be less ambiguous compared to the behavioral locomotion data.

      As described in our response to the public review, we attempted this approach but found that the in vitro optogenetics experiment is unfortunately not feasible due to relatively weak expression of R60G09-GAL4 in the larvae. Local activation of control aWave induced fictive backward locomotion only at low frequencies, making comparison with the experimental a-Wave very difficult.  The MB120B-spGAL4 used in our 2017 study could not be employed in this study as it does not drive expression during the embryonic stages and thus cannot be used to knock down DFz2 during development. 

      (2) Related to this point. Why would the normal "backward" circuitry of a-wave neurons be functionally suppressed in Dfz2 knockdowns? Do the authors observe reduced synaptic connectivity in these segments? Vesicle clustering of synaptotagmin or other presynaptic markers could be used as a first. As the innervation pattern is only extended by approximately one segment, it is surprising that the changes are so significant.

      We agree that these are important and interesting points, which remain to be explored in the future study. As described above, we have performed Brp immunostaining and showed that the posterior ectopic axons of a-Wave do contain synapses (new Figure 2). We also found a slight decrease in the number of synapses in the anterior region, which could partially contribute to the weaker activation of downstream neurons responsible for eliciting backward locomotion. Another possibility is that backward suppression occurs through lateral interaction among downstream circuits. Since forward and backward locomotion do not occur simultaneously, it is likely that the circuits driving these two behaviors are mutually inhibitory. Upon DFz2 knock down in a-Wave, downstream neurons inducing fastforward locomotion may become more strongly activated than those inducing backward locomotion, resulting in inhibition of the latter via a “winner-take-all” mechanism. Since these discussions are highly speculative, we chose not to include them in the revised manuscript.  

      (3) The low number of neurons analyzed per segment is of slight concern. This is particularly the case for the control data set used in Figure 1 and Figure 2. As stated, the same datasets are used for both figures. However, at most 6 neurons were analyzed (and for two segments only 3). The control morphology may be more variable than indicated by this data.

      As mentioned above, we now have dissected 50 larvae each for the control and experimental groups, obtained seven and six clones respectively, and included these data in the revised manuscript. We apologize that the sample sizes are still relatively small but hope the reviewer understands the inherently low “hit rate” of the stochastic labelling method.

      It is somewhat curious that in Figure 1- Supplement 3 the authors report the same number of control clones per segment as in Figure 1/2 - is this simply a coincidence? And if this is an independent dataset why did the author use new controls here but not for Figure 2? It is clear that it is very difficult to generate this data but increasing the n-number beyond 3-6 per segment would significantly increase the confidence in the presented data.

      We apologize for the confusion. The data in Figure 1 – figure supplement 3 represent the innervation pattern of dendrites, not axons. We have corrected the figure caption accordingly. These data were obtained from the same samples used to analyze axonal innervation, as shown in the original version of Figure 1F-J.

      (2) The name of the RNAi lines should be indicated in Figure 1 and Figure Supplement 3 to facilitate reading - at least the precise names should be given in both figure legends.

      We have added these labels in the revised figure legends as requested.

      (3) In Figure 4E again the control numbers of Figure 1 for the A2-wave axon are reused. This does not seem appropriate as now a different Gal4 driver is used and a different method to induce individual neuronal clones. Both components may induce significant variability in expression or arborization. As only 3 clones for the wnt4 mutant condition are analyzed (and compared to 5 control clones), this data does not allow for strong conclusions. The authors clearly state the reuse and different methods in the legend of Figure 4 F/G but should also highlight it for the E panel.

      Here, we assume that the reviewer is referring to the former Figure 3 (now Figure 4). We have added a note in the legend that the control data, obtained using a different method, were reused in this panel.

      (4) The expression levels of DWnt4 and DFz2 were analyzed at the end of embryogenesis. At what developmental stage does the axonal extension of wave neurons take place? Is the gradient maintained throughout the first larval stages?

      Based upon the lateral view of Wave neurons in Figure 1—figure supplement 1D, we think that the axonal extension is already established by approximately 20 hr after egg laying. Previously, we performed Wnt4<sup>MI03717-Trojan-GAL4</sup> > GFP.nls immunostaining in the third instar larva and observed a similar gradient of GFP signals towards the posterior end of the ventral nerve cord (VNC). We have included this data in the revised manuscript (new Figure 5—figure supplement 1).

      (5) The authors state that either 2nd or 3rd instar larvae were used for the optogenetic experiments. This may induce unnecessary variation in their assay and should be avoided. As natural variance exists in larvae regarding forward stride duration, the comparison of "on" state forward stride duration between control and experimental genotype is potentially not the best measurement of effect size. What is the difference between OFF and ON stage within the control and experimental genotype? In both cases stride duration decreases but there may not be a significant difference between the delta of the two genotypes. Thus, the observed effect may in part be due to "slower" animals in the control pool. The authors should discuss this more carefully.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up this critical issue. Indeed, the stride durations of larvae between the control and DFz2 knock-down are slightly different in the OFF condition, although this is not statistically significant. In addition, the effect size of Wave activation on mean stride duration is -0.14 (s) in control while -0.21 (s) in DFz2 knock-down, which we interpret as DFz2 knock-down resulting in stronger fastforward locomotion upon Wave activation. We have incorporated this note in the corresponding figure legends (new Figure 6; formerly Figure 5).

      (6) While the study clearly provides convincing evidence for their model, the authors should tune down their conclusions in the discussion a little bit and highlight that parts of their discussion are speculative.

      We have revised the discussion as suggested.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Albeit the optogenetic behavioral experiments strongly support that the altered axonal projection affect normal locomotion, simultaneous labeling of Wave neurons in DFz2 KD animals with presynaptic markers would strengthen the conclusion of ectopic connection of the extended axon with other circuits.

      Please see our response to your public review.

      Figure 1 K+L, Figure 2H, I, Figure 3 F+G: many of the individual data points are not visible in the Whisker plot- changing their color would be useful to visualize them better.

      We have changed the outline width of the box plots to make the individual data points visible.

      Figure 1-Supplement 2: In addition to the comments in the public review- a) the asterisk font size changes in the different panels, e.g. it is much smaller in G', b) font size in some graphs/legends should be increased - in particular in E the hyphenated letters in the genotypes are so small rendering them almost illegible.

      We have unified the font size to make them readable in the figure. We thank the reviewer for the suggestions.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The crystal structure of the Sld3CBD-Cdc45 complex presented by Li et al. is a significant contribution that enhances our understanding of CMG formation during the rate-limiting step of DNA replication initiation. This structure provides crucial insights into the intermediate steps of CMG formation, and the particle analysis and model predictions compellingly describe the mechanism of Cdc45 loading. Building upon previously known Sld3 and Cdc45 structures, this study offers new perspectives on how Cdc45 is recruited to MCM DH through the Sld3-Sld7 complex. The most notable finding is the structural rearrangement of Sld3CBD upon Cdc45 binding, particularly the α8-helix conformation, which is essential for Cdc45 interaction and may also be relevant to its metazoan counterpart, Treslin. Additionally, the conformational shift in the DHHA1 domain of Cdc45 suggests a potential mechanism for its binding to Mcm2NTD. Furthermore, Sld3's ssDNA-binding experiments provide evidence of its novel functions in the DNA replication process in yeast, expanding our understanding of its role beyond Cdc45 recruitment.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is generally well-written, with a precise structural analysis and a solid methodological section that will significantly advance future studies in the field. The predictions based on structural alignments are intriguing and provide a new direction for exploring CMG formation, potentially shaping the future of DNA replication research. This research also opens up several new opportunities to utilize structural biology to unravel the molecular details of the model presented in the paper.

      Weaknesses:

      The main weakness of the manuscript lies in the lack of detailed structural validation for the proposed Sld3-Sld7-Cdc45 model, and its CMG bound models, which could be done in the future using advanced structural biology techniques such as single particle cryo-electron microscopy. It would also be interesting to explore how Sld7 interacts with the MCM helicase, and this would help to build a detailed long-flexible model of Sld3-Sld7-Cdc45 binding to MCM DH and to show where Sld7 will lie on the structure. This will help us to understand how Sld7 functions in the complex. Also, future experiments would be needed to understand the molecular details of how Sld3 and Sld7 release from CMG is associated with ssARS1 binding.

      The proposals based on this study provide new knowledge of the CMG formation process. We agree that our Sld3-Sld7-Cdc45 model will be further confirmed by cryo-EM. We improved our ssARS1-binding assay and quantified data (See the response to Recommendations for the authors of #3 review).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary

      The manuscript presents valuable findings, particularly in the crystal structure of the Sld3CBD-Cdc45 interaction and the identification of additional sequences involved in their binding. The modeling of the Sld7-Sld3CBD-CDC45 subcomplex is novel, and the results provide insights into potential conformational changes that occur upon interaction. Although the single-stranded DNA binding data from Sld3 of different species is a minor weakness, the experiments support a model in which the release of Sld3 from the complex may be promoted by its binding to origin single-stranded DNA exposed by the helicase.

      Strengths

      The Sld3CBD-Cdc45 structure is a novel contribution, revealing critical residues involved in the interaction.

      The model structures generated from the crystal data are well presented and provide valuable insights into the interaction sequences between Sld3 and Cdc45.

      The experiments testing the requirements for interaction sequences are thorough and conducted well, with clear figures supporting the conclusions.

      The conformational changes observed in Sld3 and Cdc45 upon binding are interesting and enhance our understanding of the interaction.

      The modeling of the Sld7-Sld3CBD-CDC45 subcomplex is a new and valuable addition to the field.

      The proposed model of Sld3 release from the complex through binding to single stranded DNA at the origin is intriguing.

      Weaknesses

      The section on the binding of Sld3 complexes to origin single-stranded DNA is somewhat weakened by the use of Sld3 proteins from different species. The comparisons between Sld3-CBD, Sld3CBD-Cdc45, and Sld7-Sld3CBD-Cdc45 involve complexes from different species, limiting the comparisons' value.

      Although the study reveals that Sld3 binds to different residues of Cdc45 than those previously shown to bind Mcm or GINS, the data in the paper do not shed any additional light on how GINS and Sld3 binding to Cdc45 or Mcms. would affect each other. Other previous research has suggested that the binding of GINS and Sld3 to Mcm or Cdc45 may be mutually exclusive. The authors acknowledge that a structural investigation of Sld3, Sld7, Cdc45, and MCM during the stage of GINS recruitment will be a significant goal for future research.

      We agree that it is better to use all samples from a source; however, due to limitations in protein expression, we used Sld7-Sld3CBD-Cdc45 from a different source. The two sources used in this study belong to the same family, and the proteins Sld7, Sld3 and Cdc45 share sequence conservation with similar structures predicted by Alphafold3 (RMSD = 0.356, 1.392, and 0.891 for Ca atoms of Sld7CTD, Sld7NTD-Sld3NTD, and Sld3CBD-Cdc45). Such similarity in source and proteins allows us to do the comparison. We also mentioned that a cryo-EM study of Sld3-Sld7-Cdc45-MCM and Sld3-Sld7-CMG structures will be a significant goal for future research in our manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper by Li et al. describes the crystal structure of a complex of Sld3-Cdc45-binding domain (CBD) with Cdc45 and a model of the dimer of an Sld3-binding protein, Sld7, with two Sld3-CBD-Cdc45 for the tethering. In addition, the authors showed the genetic analysis of the amino acid substitution of residues of Sld3 in the interface with Cdc45 and biochemical analysis of the protein interaction between Sld3 and Cdc45 as well as DNA binding activity of Sld3 to the single-strand DNAs of the ARS sequence.

      Strengths:

      The authors provided a nice model of an intermediate step in the assembly of an active Cdc45-MCM-GINS (CMG) double hexamers at the replication origin, which is mediated by the Sld3-Sld7 complex. The dimer of the Sld3-Sld7 complexes tethers two MCM hexamers together for the recruitment of GINS-Pol epsilon on the replication origin.

      Weaknesses:

      The biochemical analysis should be carefully evaluated with more quantitative ways to strengthen the authors' conclusion even in the revised version.

      In this revision, we improved our ssARS1-binding assay in more quantitative ways (See the response to Recommendations for the authors).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I thank the authors for all their replies to my previous questions and for doing all the necessary corrections. I am satisfied with most of their replies, however, upon second reading I have a few more suggestions which could help to improve the manuscript further and make an impact in the field. My comments are listed below.

      (1) In general, the manuscript is well structured, but I feel that it requires professional English correction. In many places it was difficult to understand the sentences and I had to read it several times to understand it. Also, very long sentences should be avoided. The flow should be easy to read and understand, and that is why I feel it requires professional English correction.

      Following the comment, we checked English carefully and shortened the very long sentences.

      (2) Page 5, line 103, please include molecule after the word complex to make it like- "Only one complex molecule exists within an asymmetric unit."

      We revised this sentence (P5/L103).

      (3) Line 113- more than the N-terminal half of the protruding long helix α7 113 was disordered in the Sld3CBD-Cdc45 complex. This sentence is not clear. What does it mean more than the N-terminal half? Please rewrite it.

      We revised this sentence to give the corresponding residue number “(D219–H231)” (P5/L114).

      (4) Page 5, result 2- Conformation changes in Sld3CBD and Cdc45 for binding each other, this section may require a little restructuring. Line 130-131- "Therefore, the helix α8CTP seems to be an intrinsically disordered segment when Sld3 alone but 130 folds into a helix coupled to the binding partner Cdc45 in the Sld3CBD-Cdc45 complex." This statement is the crux of the structural finding and therefore, I feel it should move after the first sentence.

      Thank you for your comments. We rewrote this part (P5/L128-131).

      (5) Line 121-122: Compared to the isolated form (PDBIDs: 5DGO 121 for huCdc45 [31] and 6CC2 for EhCdc45 [33]) and the CMG form (PDBID: 3JC6. Write it in the same format. Make 6CC2 in bracket like other PDB IDs. Restructure this sentence.

      We revised this sentence (P5/122-123).

      (6) Line 127-129: This sentence is also not very clear.

      We revised this sentence together with above No (4). (P5/L128-131)

      (7) In my question 4- "Can authors add a supplementary figure showing the probability of disordernes..."., I meant to use a disorder prediction tool like IUPred for the protein sequences and show that α8 is predicted to be a disordered upon sequence analysis. This will help to show the inherent property of α8 helix, and it could add up to the understanding that a disordered region is being structured in the complex structure.

      The structures showed that α8CTP is stabilized by binding with Cdc45, but disordered in Sld3CBD alone, indicating that this part is flexible, like an intrinsically disordered segment. We have deposited the structure to PDB, so predictions like IUPred cannot show meaningful information.

      (8) Question 9 regarding Supplementary Figure 8- Please include your statement in the figure legend - "WT Sld3CBD was prepared in a complex with Cdc45, while the mutants of Sld3CBD existed alone, we calculated the elements of secondary structure from the crystal structure of Sld3CBD-Cdc45. The concentration of samples was controlled to the same level for CD measurement."

      Following the comment, we optimized the figure legend of Supplementary Figure 8.

      (9) Question 13- I understand that negative staining and SEC-SAXS experiments could be very tricky for such protein complexes, which have very long loops and are flexible. Did authors try a GraFix cross-linking before doing the negative staining TEM? If it is not being tried, then it might be a good idea to try it and it may help to get much cleaner particles and easier class averaging. Although I completely understand the technical challenges the authors describe and I agree with them, I still feel that one good experiment that shows this dimer model would be very helpful to strengthen the claim. I am concerned because if people start using a similar DLS experiment to calculate intermolecular distances, citing your paper, in many cases it might be a wrong interpretation. In case the negative staining still does not work, at least discuss your technical challenges in the discussion section and mention that SEC-SAXS showed a similar length of the complex and show the Guinier plot and Porod plots in the supplementary data.

      We believe that DLS is one of the methods for analyzing the single particle size. Of course, the confirmation by multiple methods will give compelling evidence. Following the comment, we added SEC-SAXS data in the [Results] (P7/L194-196) (Cdc45 recruitment to MCM DH by Sld3 with partner Sld7) and Supplementary Figure 11. The Sld7-Sld3-Cdc45 forms a flexible, long shape. Each binding domain is rigid but linked by the long loops. The flexibility problems are caused by the long loop linkers, but not by binding. So, we did not try to use the cross-linking method for analysis experiments.  

      (10) Page 8, line 221- litter sequence specificity: Correct the word "litter" with little. Also, the word shaped is written as sharped at a few places in the manuscript. Please correct it.

      We apologize for making such mistakes. We have modified these words.

      (11) Page 9, line 237-238: Would it be possible to add a lane showing Sld7 binding to the ssDNA in figure 4. I recommend showing this to understand the ssDNA binding affinity of Sld7 by itself and it will also help us to compare when it is in complex with Sld3.

      Considering that Sld7 on CMG is always a complex with Sld3, the ssDNA binding affinity should use the Sld3-Sld7 complex. Additionally, we attempted to overexpress Sld7, but could not obtain the target protein.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Thank you for the improved manuscript. The following sentence is unclear: "Cdc45 binds tighter to long ssDNA (>60 bases) with a litter sequence specificity".

      We apologize for making such a mistake. We modified “litter” to “little”.

      I found it challenging to understand which species were used while reading the results section and figure legends. I recommend that the authors revise the text in both the results and figure legends to clearly indicate when proteins from different species are being compared. Additionally, it would be valuable to explicitly acknowledge this limitation in the text.

      Following the comment, we added a description for using different species in results (P8/L224-225) and figure legends (Supplementary Figure 14). We added more information in the Methods to explain why we used two species for preparing proteins.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major points:

      (1) The current title is not appropriate for the general readers. At least, DNA replication or DNA replication initiation should be added and abbreviations such as CBD should be avoided.

      Following the comment, we added “DNA replication” into the title. Regarding “CBD”, since the full name of “Cdc45 binding domain” is too long, we continue to use Sld3CBD.

      (2) As in my previous review, I asked for quantification of the EMSA assay shown in Figure 4 and Supplemental Figures 13 and 14. Since some signals of the bands are very weak, it is hard to conclude something. Given different protein concentrations used in the experiment, the authors should provide any kinds of value. For example, Sld3CBD-CDC45 shows weaker DNA binding than Sld3CBD alone (line 231). Is this true (or reproducible)? It is hard to conclude without any quantification.

      We have repeated the EMSA assay four or more times with different rods of overexpression, purification and DNA synthesis, indicating that the EMSA assay is reproducible. In this revision, we changed the DNA stain and adjusted the ratio between the protein and ssDNA with increasing concentrations. The smeared bands of ssDNA with Sld7–Sld3ΔC–Cdc45 or Sld7–Sld3ΔC exhibit enhanced discernibility, and the ssDNA bands are intense enough for grayscale calculations (Figure 4 in the second revised version). We used a series of t-tests to confirm a significantly ssDNA residual level between Sld3CBD–Cdc45 to Sld3CBD, Sld7–Sld3ΔC–Cdc45, and Sld7–Sld3ΔCS (t-test, ****: P<0.0001). We also carefully controlled the sample amount in the EMAS assay and described it in the [Methods].

      Moreover, in this EMSA assay (in Figure 4), the authors suggest that the disappearance of ssDNA bands corresponds with the binding of the protein to the DNA. However, it is also possible that the DNA is degraded. It is very important to show the band of protein-DNA complexes on the gel (a whole gel, not the parts of the gel shown in Figure). Why did the authors use this "insensitive" assay using SyberGreen, not radio-labelled ssDNA?

      In this revision, we added a negative control of no ssDNA-binding by using ssARS1-3_3 for all protein samples (Sld3CBD, Sld3CBD–Cdc45, Sld7–Sld3ΔC–Cdc45 and Sld7–Sld3ΔC), which were the same rod of expression and purification for bound to ssARS1s (ssARS1-2 and ssARS1-5) (Figure 4), showing that the disappearance of ssDNA bands is caused by binding to proteins, not degradation. Moreover, this time, by changing the DNA stain and increasing the concentration of the samples, the smeared ssDNA bands exhibit enhanced discernibility in the high molecular weight regions when mixed with Sld7–Sld3ΔC–Cdc45 or Sld7–Sld3ΔC, whereas no bands appeared in the NC (ssARS1-3_1). The positions of smeared ssDNA bonds correspond to those of protein in the protein-stain pages, indicating that ssARS1 were complexed with proteins. Following the comment, we show all bands on the gel in Figure 4 and Supplementary Figure 14. Compared to Sld7–Sld3ΔC–Cdc45 or Sld7–Sld3ΔC, Sld3CBD and ssDNA bonds could not be observed because the pI value of Sld3CBD, which affects the entry of the samples into the gel.

      We agree that using radio-labelled ssDNA can obtain a sensitive binding assay. However, current laboratory constraints did not allow us to use radio-labelled ssDNA. Furthermore, considering the characteristics of our target proteins, Sld3CBD, Sld3CBD–Cdc45, Sld7–Sld3ΔC–Cdc45, and Sld7–Sld3ΔC, we planned to perform the binding assay in a more natural state without any modifications, labelling or linkers. Additionally, we have attempted to use ITC experiments but failed in the measurements. Presumably, the conformational flexibility of Sld7-Sld3-Cdc45 and Sld7-Sld3 caused a thermodynamic anomaly.

      Minor points:

      (1) Line 215, 80b: This should be "80 nucleotides(nt)". Throughout the text, nucleotides is better than base to show the length of ssDNAs.

      Thank you for your comments. We modified these words throughout the text.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This is an exploratory study that doesn't explore quite enough. Critically, the authors make a point of mentioning that neuronal firing properties vary across cell types, but only use baseline firing rate as a proxy metric for cell type. This leaves several important explorations on the table, not limited to the following:”

      1a: “Do waveform shape features, which can also be informative of cell type, predict the effect of stimulation?”

      To address this question, we modeled our approach to cell type classification after Peyrache et al. 2012. More specifically, we extracted two features from the mean unit waveforms—the valley-to-peak time (VP) and the peak half-width (PHW). These features were then used to classify units into two distinct clusters (k-means, clusters = 2, based on a strong prior from existing literature), representing putative excitatory and inhibitory neurons. Our approach recapitulated many of the same observations in Peyrache et al. 2012, namely (1) identification of two clusters (low PHW/VP: inhibitory, high PHW/VP: excitatory), (2) an ~80/20 ratio of excitatory/inhibitory neurons, and (3) greater baseline firing rates in the inhibitory vs. excitatory neurons. However, we did not observe a preferential modulation of one cell type compared to another (see newly created Figure 4). A description of this analysis and its takeaways has been incorporated into the manuscript.

      Change to Text:

      Created Figure 4 (Separation of presumed excitatory and inhibitory neurons by waveform morphology).

      Caption: (A) Two metrics were calculated using the averaged waveforms for each detected unit: the valley-to-peak width (VP) and peak half-width (PHW). (B) Scatterplot of the relationship between VP and PHW; note that units with identical metrics are overlaid. Using k-means clustering, we identified two distinct response clusters, representing presumed excitatory (E, blue) and inhibitory (I, red) neurons. The units from which the example waveforms were taken are outlined in black. Probability distributions for each metric are shown along the axes. (C) Total number of units within each cluster, separated by region. (D) Comparison of baseline firing rates, separated by cluster. (E) Percent of modulated units in each cluster. * p < 0.05, NS = not significant.

      Added a description of clustering methodology to lines 132-137: “We calculated two metrics from the averaged waveform from each detected unit: the valley-to-peak-width (VP) and the peak half-width (PHW) (Figure 4A); previously, these two properties of waveform morphology have been used to discriminate pyramidal cells (excitatory) from interneurons (inhibitory) in human intracranial recordings (Peyrache et al., 2012). Next, we performed k-means clustering (n = 2 clusters) on the waveform metrics, in line with previous approaches to cell type classification.

      Added a section in the Results titled “Theta Burst Stimulation Modulates Excitatory and Inhibitory Neurons Equally”. Lines 370-378: “Using k-means clustering, we grouped neurons into two distinct clusters based on waveform morphology, representing neurons that were presumed to be excitatory (E) and inhibitory (I) (Figure 4B). Inhibitory (fast-spiking) neurons exhibited shorter waveform VP and PHW, compared with excitatory (regular-spiking) neurons (I cluster centroid: VP = 0.50ms, PHW = 0.51ms; E cluster centroid: VP = 0.32ms, PHW = 0.31ms), and greater baseline firing rates (U(N<sub>I</sub> = 23, N<<sub>E</sub> = 133) = 1074.50, p = 0.023) (Figure 4D). Although we observed a much greater proportion of excitatory vs. inhibitory neurons (E: 85.3%, I: 14.7%), stimulation appeared to affect excitatory and inhibitory neurons equally, suggesting that one cell type is not preferentially activated over another (Figure 4E).

      Modified discussion of the effects of stimulation on different cell types. Lines 475-483: “…To test these hypotheses directly, we clustered neurons into presumed excitatory and inhibitory neurons based on waveform morphology. In doing so, we observed ~85% excitatory and ~15% inhibitory neurons, which is very similar what has been reported previously in human intracranial recordings (Cowan et al. 2024, Peyrache et al., 2012). Interestingly, stimulation appeared to modulate approximately the same proportion of neurons for each cell type (~30%), despite the differently-sized groups. Recent reports, however, have suggested that the extent to which electrical fields entrain neuronal spiking, particularly with respect to phase-locking, may be specific to distinct classes of cells (Lee et al., 2024).”

      1b:  “Is the autocorrelation of spike timing, which can be informative about temporal dynamics, altered by stimulation? This is especially interesting if theta-burst stimulation either entrains theta-rhythmic spiking or is more modulatory of endogenously theta-modulated units.”

      The reviewer is correct in suggesting that rate-modulation represents only one of many possible ways by which exogenous theta burst stimulation may influence neuronal activity. Indeed, intracranial theta burst stimulation has previously been shown to evoke theta-frequency oscillatory responses in local field potentials (Solomon et al. 2021), and other forms of stimulation (i.e., transcranial alternating current stimulation) may modulate the rhythm, rather than the rate, of neuronal spiking (Krause et al. 2019).

      To investigate whether stimulation altered rhythmicity in neuronal firing, we contrasted the spike timing autocorrelograms, as suggested. More specifically, we computed the pairwise differences in spike timing for each trial, separating spikes into the same pre-, during-, and post-stimulation epochs described in the manuscript (bin size = 5 ms, max lag = 250 ms), grouped neurons by whether they were modulated, and then contrasted the differences in the latencies of the peak normalized autocorrelation value between epochs. Only neurons with a firing rate of ≥ 1 Hz (n = 70/203, 34.5%) were included in this analysis since sparse firing resulted in noisy autocorrelation estimates. Subsequent statistical testing of the peak latency differences between pre-/during- and pre-/post-stimulation did not reveal any group-level differences (Mann-Whitney U tests, p > 0.05). Thus, we were not able to identify neuronal responses suggestive of altered rhythmicity (see Figure S5). A description of this analysis and its takeaways has been incorporated into the manuscript.

      Of note, there are two elements of the data that constrain our ability to detect modulation in the rhythm of firing. First, the baseline activity recorded across neurons modulated by stimulation was relatively low (i.e., median firing rate = 1.77 Hz). Second, stimulation often resulted in a suppression, rather than an enhancement, of firing rate. Taken together, the sparse firing afforded limited opportunity to characterize changes to subtle patterns of spiking. 

      Change to Text:

      Created Figure S5 (Analysis of modulation in spiking rhythmicity)

      Caption: (A) Representative autocorrelograms ACG) for a single neuron. The pairwise differences in spike timing were computed for each trial and epoch (bin size = 5 ms, max lag = 250 ms), then smoothed with a Gaussian kernel. The peak in the normalized ACG across trials was computed for each epoch. (B) Kernel density estimate of the peak ACG lag, separated by epoch. (C) The peak ACG lags were split by whether the neuron was modulated (Mod) or unaffected by stimulation (NS = not significant) for each of the two contrasts: pre- vs. during-stim (left) and pre- vs. post-stim (right).

      Details about the autocorrelation methodology have been incorporated. Lines 166-172: “To investigate whether stimulation altered rhythmicity in neuronal firing, we analyzed the spike timing autocorrelograms. More specifically, we computed the pairwise differences in spike timing for each trial (bin size = 5 ms, max lag = 250 ms) and then contrasted the differences in the latencies of the peak normalized autocorrelation value between epochs (pre-, during-, post-stimulation). Only neurons with a firing rate of ≥ 1 Hz (n = 70/203, 34.5%) were included in this analysis since sparse firing resulted in noisy autocorrelation estimates.

      The results from contrasting the autocorrelograms are now mentioned briefly. Lines 297-298: “Stimulation, however, did not appear to alter the rhythmicity in neuronal firing, as measured by spiking autocorrelograms (Figure S5).”

      1c: “The authors reference the relevance of spike-field synchrony (30-55 Hz) in animal work, but ignore it here. Does spike-field synchrony (comparing the image presentation to post-stimulation) change in this frequency range? This does not seem beyond the scope of investigation here.”

      We agree that a further characterization of spike-field and spike-phase relationships may provide rich insights into more complex regional and interregional dynamics that may be altered by stimulation. Given that many metrics are biased by sample size (e.g., number of spikes), which can vary considerably, computing the pairwise phase consistency (PPC) between spikes and LFP is a preferred metric (Vinck et al. 2010). Although PPC is unbiased, its variance nonetheless increases considerably with low spike counts; pooling spike counts across trials, however, decouples the temporal relationship between spiking and the LFP phase for each trial, confounding results and yielding an unstable estimate.

      To determine whether such an analysis is indeed possible, we calculated the percentage of stimulation trials with ≥ 10 spikes in both the 1s pre- and post-stimulation epochs (a relatively low threshold for inclusion). Only a very small proportion of the total number of trials across all neurons met this criterion (2.5%). Thus, because of the sparse spiking in our data, we are unable to reliably characterize spike-field or spike-phase modulation in detected neurons.

      Change to Text:

      In the manuscript, we have added a description of why our data is not well-suited to investigate these relationships.

      Lines 532-538: “The present study did not investigate interactions between spiking activity and local field potentials because neuronal spiking was sparse at baseline and often further suppressed by stimulation; only a very small proportion of the total number of trials across all neurons exhibited ≥ 10 spikes in both the 1s pre- and post-stimulation epochs (~2.5%). Although certain metrics are not biased by sample size (e.g., pairwise phase consistency), low spike counts can dramatically affect variance and, therefore, result in unstable estimates (Vinck et al., 2011).

      1d: “How does multi-unit activity respond to stimulation? At this somewhat low count of neurons (total n=156 included) it would be valuable to provide input on multi-unit responses to stimulation as well.”

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have incorporated an analysis of multiunit activity (MUA), which similarly identifies robust modulation via permutation-based statistical testing and characterizes the different profiles of responses (i.e., increased vs. decreased MUA threshold crossings pre- vs. post-stimulation).

      Change to Text:

      Created Figure S8 (Analysis of multiunit activity response to stimulation)

      Caption: (A) Example trace of multiunit activity (MUA) in one channel during a single stimulation trial. Threshold crossings are highlighted with a pink dot overlaid on the MUA signal with a corresponding hash below. (B) The percentage of channels with significantly modulated MUA, separated by the direction of effect. (C) The percentage of channels with significantly modulated MUA, separated by direction effect and region. Inc (red; post > pre) vs. Dec (blue; post < pre). HIP = hippocampus, OFC = orbitofrontal cortex, AMY = amygdala, ACC = anterior cingulate cortex. *** p < 0.001, NS = not significant.

      Details about the MUA methodology have been incorporated. Lines 174-180: “Finally, we measured modulation in multiunit activity (MUA) by filtering the microleectrode signals in a 300-3,000 Hz window and counting the number of threshold crossings. Thresholds were determined on a per-channel basis and defined as -3.5 times the root mean square of the signal during the baseline period; activity during stimulation was excluded since stimulation artifact is difficult to separate from MUA in the absence of spike sorting.

      MUA results are now incorporated. Lines 365-367: “Additional characterization of MUA revealed a dominant signature of increased activity post- vs. pre-stimulation, in line with these trends observed at the single-neuron level (Figure S8).”

      1e: “Several intracranial studies have implicated proximity to white matter in determining the effects of stimulation on LFPs; do the authors see an effect of white matter proximity here?”

      We thank the reviewer for the interesting question. Subsequent characterization revealed only small differences in the proximity of stimulation contacts to white matter (range 1.5-8.0 mm), likely because the chosen target (i.e., basolateral amygdala) has several nearby white matter structures (e.g., stria terminalis). Nonetheless, we performed a linear regression between the proximity to white matter and the stimulation-induced effect on behavior (stimulation vs. no-stimulation d’ difference), the results of which indicate no clear association (p > 0.05; see Figure S9). Critically, this is not to suggest that white matter proximity has no interaction with the reported behavioral effects, but rather, that we could not identify such an association within our data.

      Change to Text:

      Created Figure S9 (The effect of stimulation proximity to white matter and distance to recorded neurons).

      Caption: (A) Kernel density estimate of the Euclidean distance from stimulation contacts to nearest WM structure (in mm); hash marks represent individual observations. (B) The change in memory performance (Δd’) was linearly regressed onto the distance from the stimulated contacts to white matter.

      The following has been added to lines 405-426: “Proximity to white matter has been shown to influence the effects of stimulation on behavior and the strength of evoked responses (Mankin et al., 2021; Mohan et al., 2020; Paulk et al., 2022). Across all stimulated contacts, we observed only small differences in the proximity of stimulation contacts to white matter (median = 4.5 mm, range = 1.5-8.0 mm), likely because the chosen target (i.e., basolateral amygdala) has several nearby white matter structures (e.g., stria terminalis). Nonetheless, we performed a linear regression between the proximity to white matter and the stimulation-induced effect on behavior (stimulation vs. no-stimulation d’ difference), the results of which indicate no clear association (p > 0.05; see Figure S9).

      Comment 2: “It is a little confusing to interpret stimulation-induced modulation of neuronal spiking in the absence of stimulation-induced change in behavior. How do the authors findings tell us anything about the neural mechanisms of stimulation-modulated memory if memory isn't altered? In line with point #1, I would suggest a deeper dive into behavior (e.g. reaction time? Or focus on individual sessions that do change in Figure 4A?) to make a stronger statement connecting the neural results to behavioral relevance.”

      We agree that the connection between the observed stimulation-induced neuronal modulation and effects on behavior is unclear and has proven challenging to elucidate. Per the reviewer’s suggestion, we further focused our analyses on the neuronal modulation effects in the individual sessions that resulted in a robust change in memory performance (stimulation vs. no-stimulation d’ difference threshold of ± 0.5, based on a moderate effect size for Cohen’s d); both a positive and negative threshold were used to capture robust changes in memory performance associated with firing rate modulation, whether enhancement or suppression. To this end, we contrasted the proportion of modulated neurons in the sessions where stimulation resulted in a robust behavioral change (Δd’) with those that did not (~d’). We did not observe a difference in the proportions between groups when collapsed across all sampled regions, or when separately evaluated (Fisher’s exact tests, p > 0.05; see Figure 5C).

      Given that this approach did not further clarify the connection between our neural and behavioral results, we believe it is most appropriate to deemphasize claims in the manuscript regarding the potential insights for behavioral modulation (e.g., memory enhancement), and have done so.

      Change to Text:

      Toned down reference to the memory-related effects of stimulation in the abstract by removing the following lines from the abstract: “Previously, we demonstrated that intracranial theta burst stimulation (TBS) of the basolateral amygdala (BLA) can enhance declarative memory, likely by modulating hippocampal-dependent memory consolidation…” and “…and motivate future neuromodulatory therapies that aim to recapitulate specific patterns of activity implicated in cognition and memory.”

      Changed Figure 4 to Figure 5

      Created Figure 5C (Interaction between behavioral effects and neuronal modulation)(C)  Change in recognition memory performance was split into two categories using a d’ difference threshold of ± 0.5: responder (positive or negative; Δd’, pink) and non-responder (~d’, grey). Individual d’ scores are shown (left) with points colored by outcome category; dotted lines demarcate category boundaries, and the grey-shaded region represents negligible change. The number of sessions within each outcome category (middle) and the proportion of modulated units as a function of outcome category, separated by region (right). NS = not significant.

      The description of the behavioral results has been updated. Lines 394-403: “At the level of individual sessions, we observed enhanced memory (Δd’ > +0.5) in 36.7%, impaired memory (Δd’ < -0.5) in 20.0%, and negligible change (-0.5 ≤ Δd’ ≤ 0.5) in 43.3% when comparing performance between the stim and no-stim conditions; a threshold of Δd’ ± 0.5 was chosen for this classification based on the defined range of a “medium effect” for Cohen’s d. To test our hypothesis that neuronal modulation would be associated with changes in memory performance, we combined the sessions that resulted in either memory enhancement or impairment and contrasted the proportion of modulated units across regions sampled. We did not, however, observe a meaningful difference in the proportion of modulated units when grouped by behavioral outcome (all contrasts p > 0.05) (Figure 5C).

      Lines 213-214 and 394-397 have been edited to reflect a change in the d’ threshold used for categorizing behavioral results (from Δd’ ± 0.2 to Δd’ ± 0.5).

      Comment 3: “It is not clear to me why the assessment of firing rates after image onset and after stim offset is limited to one second - this choice should be more theoretically justified, particularly for regions that spike as sparsely as these.”

      We thank the reviewer for this question and acknowledge that no clear justification was provided for this decision in the manuscript. Our decision to limit each of the analysis epochs to 1s was chosen for two reasons. First, the maximum possible length of the during-stimulation epoch was 1 s (stim on for 1 s). Although the pre- and post-stimulation epochs could be extended without issue, we were concerned that variable time windows could introduce a bias, for instance, resulting in different variances between epochs. Second, we anticipated, both from empirical observations and prior literature, that the neural response following stimulation or task features (e.g., image onset/offset) was likely to be transient, rather than sustained for a period of many seconds. By keeping the windows short, we ensured that our approach to detecting modulation (i.e., contrasting trial-wise spike counts between each pair of epochs) captured the intended effect rather than random noise. We have incorporated a discussion of this rationale in the Peri-Stimulation Modulation Analyses section.

      Change to Text:

      Lines 156-158 have been added: “Each epoch was constrained to 1 s to ensure that subsequent firing rate contrasts were unbiased and to capture potential transient effects (e.g., image onset/offset).”

      Comment 4: “This work coincides with another example of human intracranial stimulation investigating the effect on firing rates (doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2024.11.28.625915). Given how incredibly rare this type of work is, I think the authors should discuss how their work converges with this work (or doesn't).”

      Thank you for bringing this highly relevant work to our attention. We were unaware of this recent preprint and have incorporated a discussion of its main findings into the manuscript.

      Change to Text:

      New citations: van der Plas et al. 2024 (bioRxiv), Cowan et al. 2024 (bioRxiv)

      The discussion of related studies has been updated. Lines 447-457: “Few studies, however, have characterized the impact of electrical stimulation via macroelectrodes on the spiking activity of human cortical neurons, none of which involve intracranial theta burst stimulation. One study reported a long-lasting reduction in neural excitability among parietal neurons, with variable onset time and recovery following continuous transcranial TBS in non-human primates (Romero et al., 2022). In a similar vein, it was recently shown that human neurons are largely suppressed by single-pulse electrical stimulation (Cowan et al., 2024; Plas et al., 2024). Other emerging evidence suggests that transcranial direct current stimulation may entrain the rhythm rather than rate of neuronal spiking (Krause et al., 2019) and that stimulation-evoked modulation of spiking may meaningfully impact behavioral performance on cognitive tasks (Fehring et al., 2024).”

      Comment 5: “What information does the pseudo-population analysis add? It's not totally clear to me.”

      We recognize the need to further contextualize the motivation for the exploratory pseudo-population analysis and appreciate the reviewer for bringing the lack of detail to our attention. In brief, the analysis allowed us to observe trends in activity across populations of neurons, which, in principle, are not visible by characterizing modulation solely in discrete neurons. Additional details have been incorporated into the manuscript, as suggested.

      Change to Text:

      Additional justification has been incorporated in the description of the methodology. Lines 185-187: “…This approach enables the identification of dominant patterns of coordinated neural activity that may not be apparent when examining individual neurons in isolation.”, lines 192-194: “…By collapsing across subjects into a common pseudo-population, this analysis provides a mesoscale view of how stimulation modulates shared activity patterns across anatomically distributed neural populations.”

      A summary interpretation has been added to the paragraph describing the results. Lines 326-328: “Taken together, these analyses reveal global structure in the state space of responses to BLA stimulation within hippocampal circuits.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Comment 1 “Authors suggest that the units modulated by stimulation are largely distinct from those responsive to image offset during trials without stimulation. The subpopulation that responds strongly also tends to have a higher baseline of firing rate. It's important to add that the chosen modulation index is more likely to be significant in neurons with higher firing rates.”

      This is an important point that was not previously addressed in our manuscript. We suspect there are likely two factors at play worth considering with respect to our chosen nonparametric modulation index: neurons with lower activity require smaller changes in spike counts to be significantly modulated (easier to flip ranks), and neurons with higher activity empirically exhibit greater absolute shifts in the number of spikes. Our further use of permutation testing, while mitigating false positives, may also somewhat constrain the ability to detect modulation in sparsely active neurons. Nonetheless, given that many trials entailed few or no spikes, we believe this approach is preferable to alternatives that may be more susceptible to noise (e.g., percent change in trial-averaged firing rate from baseline).

      To better understand the tradeoffs with detection probability, we performed a sensitivity analysis. We generated synthetic data with different baseline firing rates (0.1-5.0 Hz) and effect sizes (± 0.1-0.7 Hz) and simulated the likelihood of detection with our given modulation index across neurons. The results of the simulation support the notion that the probability of detecting modulation is lower for sparsely active neurons (Figure S8C). Further discussion of this consideration for the chosen modulation index, as well as details regarding the sensitivity analysis, have been incorporated into the manuscript.

      Change to Text:

      Created Figure S7C (Detection probability analysis)

      Caption: The same permutation-based analyses reported in the manuscript were repeated under different control conditions… (C) Visualization of the predicted probability of detecting modulation across synthetic neurons with variable firing rates and modulation effect sizes; FR = firing rate.

      Lines 223-224 have been added to the Methods section titled “Firing Rate Control Analyses”: “We performed a series of control analyses to test whether our approach to firing rate detection was robust…”

      A description of the simulation has been incorporated into the same section as above. Lines 234-237: “Finally, to better understand the tradeoffs with our statistical approach, we generated synthetic data with different baseline firing rates (0.1-5.0 Hz) and effect sizes (± 0.1-0.7 Hz), then simulated the likelihood of detecting modulation across variable conditions (Figure S7C).”

      The description of the results from the control analyses has been updated. Lines 330-339: “Finally, we performed three supplementary analyses to evaluate the robustness of our approach to detecting firing rate modulation: a sensitivity analysis assessing the proportion of modulated units at different firing rate thresholds for inclusion/exclusion, a data dropout analysis designed to control for the possibility that non-physiological stimulation artifacts may preclude the detection of temporally adjacent spiking, and a synthetic detection probability analysis. These results recapitulate our observation that units with higher baseline firing are most likely to exhibit modulation (though the probability of detecting modulation is lower for sparsely active neurons) and suggest that suppression in firing rate is not solely attributable to amplifier saturation following stimulation (Figure S7).

      Comment 2: “Readers can benefit from understanding with more details the locations chosen for stimulation - in light of previous studies that found differences between effects based on proximity to white matter (For example - PMID 32446925, Mohan et al, Brain Stimul. 2020 and PMID 33279717 Mankin et al Brain Stimul. 2021).”

      This has been addressed in the above response to Reviewer’s 1 comment 1.1e.

      Change to Text:

      See changes related to Reviewer 1 comment 1.1e.

      Comment 3: “Missing information in the manuscript…”

      3a: “Images of stimulation anatomical locations for all subjects included in this study. Ideally information about the impedance of the contacts to be able to calculate the actual current used.”

      As requested, we have provided an image from the coronal T1 MRI sequence, which highlights the position of the stimulated contacts for each of the 16 patients. Though we did not measure the impedances directly, the stimulation was current-controlled, which ensured that the desired current and charge density were consistent regardless of the tissue or electrode impedance.

      Change to Text:

      Created Figure S1 (Anatomical location of stimulated electrodes).

      Caption: A coronal slice from the T1-weighted MRI scan is shown for each patient who participated in the study (n = 16). Electrode contacts within the same plane of the image are shown with blue circles, and the bipolar pair of stimulated contacts within the basolateral amygdala is highlighted in red.

      Lines 144-145 have been edited to reflect that the delivered stimulation was current-controlled: “Specifically, we administered current-controlled, charge-balanced, …”

      3b: “The studied population is epilepsy patients, and the manuscript lacks description of their condition, proximity to electrodes included in the study to pathological areas, and the number of units from each patient/hemisphere.”

      We agree that additional information regarding patient demographics, experimental details, and clinical characteristics would further contextualize this unique patient population. A new table has been included, which contains the following information: patient ID, sex, age, # experimental session, # SEEG leads (and # microelectrodes), # detected units (L vs. R hemisphere), and suspected seizure onset zone.

      Change to Text:

      Created Table S1 (Patient demographics and clinical characteristics).

      Lines 258-259 have been added: “…(see Table S1 for patient demographics).”

      3c: “I haven't seen any comments on code availability (calculating modulation indices and statistics) and data sharing.”

      For clarification, a section titled Resource Availability is already appended to the end of the manuscript following the Conclusion, which describes the data and code availability.

      Change to Text:

      None

      3d: “Small comment - Figure legend 3E - Define gray markers (non-modulated units?)”

      Thank you for highlighting this omission. We have updated the relevant figure caption.

      Change to Text:

      The following has been added to the Figure 3 caption: “…whereas units without a significant change in activity are shown in grey.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript presents a comprehensive structure-guided secretome analysis of gall-forming microbes, providing valuable insights into effector diversity and evolution. The authors have employed AlphaFold2 to predict the 3D structures of the secretome from selected pathogens and conducted a thorough comparative analysis to elucidate commonalities and unique features of effectors among these phytopathogens.

      Strengths:

      The discovery of conserved motifs such as 'CCG' and 'RAYH' and their central role in maintaining the overall fold is an insightful finding. Additionally, the discovery of a nucleoside hydrolase-like fold conserved among various gall-forming microbes is interesting.

      Weaknesses:

      Important conclusions are not verified by experiments.

      Thank you very much. There are many aspects of this study that could be further validated, each potentially requiring years of work. Therefore, we chose to focus on two specific hypotheses: are AlphaFol-Multimer predictions accurate? Can ANK target more than one host protein? Particularly, we focused on the identification of putative targets for one of the ankyrin repeat proteins, PBTT_00818 (Fig. 6). Using one-by-one yeast two-hybrid (Y2H) assays, we tested the AlphaFold-Multimer prediction of an interaction between PBTT_00818 and MPK3. The interaction did not occur in yeast, suggesting it might not take place under those conditions.

      This negative result led us to perform a Y2H screen using an Arabidopsis cDNA library, which identified a GroES-like protein, highly expressed in roots, as a potential target of the ANK effector. Surprisingly, both the PBTT_00818–MPK3 and PBTT_00818–GroES-like protein interactions were later confirmed in planta using BiFC assays. These findings suggest two key points: (1) AlphaFold predictions can be accurate for ANK proteins, and (2) ANK domains, known for mediating protein-protein interactions, may enable these effectors to target multiple host proteins.

      Although the precise biological implications remain unclear, it is possible that ANK proteins act as scaffolds or adaptors for other effectors during infection. The validations presented here open exciting avenues for further research into the role of ANK proteins in Plasmodiophorid pathogenesis and gall formation. This is presented in the corrected preprint and Fig. 7, Table S12, Fig. S7-S8.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Soham Mukhopadhyay et al. investigated the protein folding of the secretome from gall-forming microbes using the AI-based structure modeling tool AlphaFold2. Their study analyzed six gall-forming species, including two Plasmodiophorid species and four others spanning different kingdoms, along with one non-gall-forming Plasmodiophorid species, Polymyxa betae. The authors found no effector fold specifically conserved among gall-forming pathogens, leading to the conclusion that their virulence strategies are likely achieved through diverse mechanisms. However, they identified an expansion of the Ankyrin repeat family in two gall-forming Plasmodiophorid species, with a less pronounced presence in the non-gall-forming Polymyxa betae. Additionally, the study revealed that known effectors such as CCG and AvrSen1 belong to sequence-unrelated but structurally similar (SUSS) effector clusters.

      Strengths:

      (1) The bioinformatics analyses presented in this study are robust, and the AlphaFold2-derived resources deposited in Zenodo provide valuable resources for researchers studying plant-microbe interactions. The manuscript is also logically organized and easy to follow.

      (2) The inclusion of the non-gall-forming Polymyxa betae strengthens the conclusion that no effector fold is specifically conserved in gall-forming pathogens and highlights the specific expansion of the Ankyrin repeat family in gall-forming Plasmodiophorids.

      (3) Figure 4a and 4b effectively illustrate the SUSS effector clusters, providing a clear visual representation of this finding.

      (4) Figure 1 is a well-designed, comprehensive summary of the number and functional annotations of putative secretomes in gall-forming pathogens. Notably, it reveals that more than half of the analyzed effectors lack known protein domains in some pathogens, yet some were annotated based on their predicted structures, despite the absence of domain annotations.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The effector families discussed in this paper remain hypothetical in terms of their functional roles, which is understandable given the challenges of demonstrating their functions experimentally. However, this highlights the need for experimental validation as a next step.

      Thank you. Yes, there is a lot of work to do in the coming years.

      (2) Some analyses, such as those in Figure 4e, emphasize motifs derived from sequence alignments of SUSS effector clusters. Since these effectors are sequence-unrelated, sequence alignments might be unreliable. It would be more rigorous to perform structure-based alignments in addition to sequence-based ones for motif confirmation. For instance, methods described in Figure 3E of de Guillen et al. (2015, https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1005228) or tools like Foldseek could be useful for aligning structures of multiple sequences.

      In Fig. 4e, we highlight the conserved cysteine residues. While there is no clearly conserved overall motif, the figure illustrates that despite the high sequence divergence, the key cysteines involved in disulfide bridge formation are consistently conserved across the sequences.

      (3) When presenting AlphaFold-generated structures, it is essential to include confidence scores such as pLDDT and PAE. For example, in Figure 1D of Derbyshire and Raffaele (2023, https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-40949-9), the structural representations were colored red due to their high pLDDT scores, emphasizing their reliability.

      Thank you for the observation. Due to the restrictive parameters used in our analysis, over 90% of the structure would appear red. For this reason, we chose not to include the color scale, as it would not provide additional informative value in this context.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Experimental validation of the significance of 'CCG' and 'RAYH' motifs would further strengthen this study.

      Regarding the Mig1-like protein in Ustilago maydis, the presence of four conserved cysteine residues that are pivotal for maintaining the stability of its folded structure raises an intriguing question. Specifically, while many Mig cluster effectors contain four cysteine residues that form two conserved disulfide bridges, this structure is notably absent in the Mig protein itself. The author has speculated that these four cysteine residues form two conserved disulfide bonds, which are crucial for the stability of Mig protein folding. However, this hypothesis remains unvalidated. To test this prediction, it would be prudent to simulate mutations in the cysteine residues corresponding to the disulfide bonds in Mig and employ molecular dynamics simulations to assess the stability of folding before and after the mutation.

      Mig-1 does contain the four conserved cysteine residues responsible for forming disulfide bridges. However, due to the high divergence among Mig-1-like sequences, the alignment software was unable to properly align all the cysteine residues. As a result, Mig-1 may appear to lack these conserved cysteines in the alignment, although they are indeed present upon individual inspection. This is an area that research groups working with U. maidis as a model could explore further to expand our understanding of this effector family.

      Could you please clarify why talking about Ankyrins and LRR in Arabidopsis thaliana (line 252)? Additionally, what are the structural and functional differences between the LRR sequences of P. brassicae and those of the host plants?

      This sentence refers to the identification of the ANK motif in P. brassicae and S. spongospora, not in Arabidopsis thaliana. While the hydrophobic core of the ANK domains appears conserved between the host and the pathogen, the surface residues are highly polymorphic.

      The evidence supporting the interaction between the ANK effector and Arabidopsis immunity-related proteins, as validated using AlphaFold-Multimer, is currently limited. To enhance the reliability of these data, it is advisable for the author to select several pairs of proteins predicted to interact for further experimental verification.

      We conducted a large-scale yeast two-hybrid (Y2H) screen using the ANK domain effector PBTT_00818, which was selected due to its high iPTM+pTM score. The Y2H interactions were subsequently validated through BiFC assays. Our results show that PBTT_00818 interacts with Arabidopsis MPK3 in the nucleus, consistent with predictions from the AlphaFold2-multimer model. In addition, PBTT_00818 was also found to target AT3G56460, a GroES-like zinc-binding alcohol dehydrogenase, also localized in the nucleus.

      While the manuscript is well-composed, certain sections could be enhanced for clarity and readability. For example, the discussion section could be expanded to include a more in-depth analysis of the implications of the findings for understanding the virulence mechanisms of gall-forming microbes. Additionally, a comparison of the findings with previous studies on related pathogens would provide a more comprehensive perspective.

      Certain sections of the discussion have been expanded. However, we chose to focus on the novel aspects of the study and to avoid comparisons with other plant pathogens, as those mechanisms are already well known and extensively studied. Studies using AlphaFold in plant pathology are also limited.

      *Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):*

      The results of clustering analyses are highly dependent on the chosen thresholds. Given that the authors provide clear and well-designed visualizations of SUSS effectors in Figures 4a and 4b, applying the same presentation methods to Figures 5a and 5b could make these analyses more convincing.

      We were able to generate the all-vs-all matrix for Figures 4a and 4b because it involved only 13 proteins. However, Figure 5b includes over 40 effectors, making it impractical to visualize the data in the same way. Instead, we presented the sequence-based clusters as nodes and connected them based on structural similarity.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Comments on revisions:

      I appreciate the authors responding to my comments. I think Fig. S10 helps put the structural data into more context. It would be helpful to make clearer in the legend what proteins are being compared, especially in 10C.

      Although I can see why the authors focus on the NifK extension and its potential connection to oxygen protection, I would point out that Vnf and Anf do not have this extension in their K subunit, and you find both Vnf and Anf in aerobic and facultative anaerobic diazotrophs. This is a minor point, but I think it is important to mention in the discussion.

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful comments. We now added an additional line to the Discussion following their recommendation and moved Figure S10 to main text.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      This work aims to study the evolution of nitrogenanses, understanding how their structure and function adapted to changes in environment, including oxygen levels and changes in metal availability. 

      The study predicts > 3000 structures of nitrogenases, corresponding to extant, ancestral and alternative ancestral sequences. It is observed that structural variations in the nitrogenases correlate with phylogenetic relationships. The amount of data generated in this study represents a massive and admirable undertaking. The study also provides strong insight into how structural evolution correlates with environmental and biological phenotypes. 

      We thank the reviewer for their summary and positive appraisal.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Comments on revised version:

      The authors addressed the concerns adequately. The three remaining concerns are:

      (1) The use of one-way ANOVA is not well justified.

      The statement about statistical test in “Statistical analysis” section is as follows in the revised manuscript, “Data sets were tested for normality and direct comparisons between two groups were made using two-tailed Student’s t test (t test, for normally distributed data) as indicated. To evaluate statistical significance of three or more groups of samples, one-way ANOVA analysis with a Tukey test was used or repeated measures ANOVA analysis with a Tukey test was used in behavior assays. Statistical parameters are reported in the figures and the corresponding legends”.

      We used a one-way ANOVA for the data about one categorical independent variable and one quantitative dependent variable. The independent variable should have at least three different groups or categories. And we conducted repeated measures ANOVA analysis for the data about behavioral tests according to the suggestion by Reviewer #1 (Point 18) in revised manuscript.

      (2) The use of superplots to show culture to culture variability would make it more transparent.

      Thanks for the nice suggestion. While superplots could more transparently show culture to culture variability, it is difficult to add more colors or even shades to the scatterplots in the current form, which have already been color coded for multiple groups of samples. The scatterplots we used effectively illustrate the variability across all collected data and do not affect the conclusions of our study. Therefore, we prefer not to change the way of data presentation in the revised manuscript.

      (3) Change EEN1 in Figure 8B to EndoA1.

      Thanks a lot for the sharp eye. Corrected.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Specific comments:

      The authors have made a substantial effort to improve their manuscript. A number of issues, related to numbers of observations mentioned by the reviewers, are clarified in the revised manuscript. The authors have also clarified some of the other questions from the reviewers. The long list of issues brought up by the reviewers and the many corrections needed still raise questions about data quality in this manuscript.

      In response to my comments (Point 2), the added experiment with PSD95.FingR and GPN.FingR in cultured neurons (Fig. S5A-D) is a good addition; the in vivo data using FingRs in Figure S3 look less convincing however. In response to my Point 5, the authors have added a cell-free binding assay (Figure 5I). This is a useful addition, but to convincingly make the point of interaction between Gephyrin and EndoA1, more rigorous biophysical quantitation of binding is needed. The legend in Figure 5I states that 4 independent experiments were performed, but the graph only shows 3 dots. This needs to be corrected.

      We sincerely appreciate your comments and apologize for any concerns raised. As suggested (Point 2), we made many efforts to visualize endogenous postsynaptic proteins using recombinant probes. However, due to much lower expression of GPN.FingR compared with PSD95.FingR in P21 brain slices following viral infection (Figure S3), we were unable to obtain better imaging results. To strengthen our data and conclusions, we additionally performed experiments with PSD95.FingR and GPN.FingR in cultured neurons (Fig. S5A-D) in the revised manuscript.

      Regarding the biophysical quantification of gephyrin–endophilin A1 binding, we do not have the equipment for this type of experiment (surface plasmon resonance or isothermal titration calorimetry). Instead, we performed a pull-down assay as an alternative to confirm their interaction (Figure 5I). We also apologize for the error in the number of independent experiments stated in the figure legend and have corrected it in the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Amaral et al. presents a study investigating the mesoscale modelling and dynamics of bolalipids.

      Strengths:

      The figures in this paper are exceptional. Both those to outline and introduce the lipid types, but also the quality and resolution of the plots. The data held within also appears to be outstanding and of significant (hopefully) general interest.

      We thank the reviewer for their kind words and the appreciation of our work.

      Weaknesses:

      In the introduction, I would like to have read more specifics on the biological role of bolalipids. Archaea are mentioned, but this kingdom is huge - there must be specific species that can be discussed where bolalipids are integral to archaeal life. The authors should go beyond ’extremophiles’. In short, they should unpack why the general audience should be interested in these lipids, within a subset of organisms that are often forgotten about.

      Following the reviewer’s advice we have revised the introduction of the manuscript, in which we now discuss specific species (Sulfolobus acidocaldarius and Thermococcus kodakarensis) and how in these species bolalipids are integral to archaeal life. We explain that the ratio between bilayer and bolalipids, and the number of cyclopentane rings contained within bolalipids can change to adapt to the environment. The revised parts of the introduction read (p.1 ):

      “Like for bacteria and eukaryotes, archaea must keep their lipid membranes in a fluid state (homeoviscous adaptation). This is important even under extreme environmental conditions, such as hot and cold temperatures, or high and low pH values [7]. Because of this, many archaea adapt to changes in their environment by tuning the lipid composition of their membranes: altering the ratio between bola- and bilayer lipids in their membranes [8, 9] and/or by changing the number of cyclopentane rings in their lipid tails, which are believed to make lipid molecules more rigid [5]. For example, Thermococcus kodakarensis increases its tetraether bolalipid ratio from around 50% to over 80% when the temperature of the environment increases from 60 to 85 C [10]. Along the same lines, the cell membrane of Sulfolobus acidocaldarius, can contain over 90 % of bolalipids with up to 8 cyclopentane rings at 70 C and pH 2.5 [5, 11]. It is worth mentioning that in exceptional cases bacteria also synthesise bolalipids in response to high temperatures [12], highlighting that the study of bolalipid membranes is relevant not only for archaeal biology but also from a general membrane biophysics perspective.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aimed to understand the biophysical properties of archeal membranes made of bolalipids. Bacterial and eukaryotic membranes are made of lipids that self-assemble into bilayers. Archea, instead, use bolalipids, lipids that have two headgroups and can span the entire bilayer. The authors wanted to determine if the unique characteristics of archaea, which are often extremophiles, are in part due to the fact that their membranes contain bolalipids.

      The authors develop a minimal computational model to compare the biophysics of bilayers made of lipids, bolalipids, and mixtures of the two. Their model enables them to determine essential parameters such as bilayer phase diagrams, mechanical moduli, and the bilayer behaviour upon cargo inclusion and remodelling.

      The author demonstrates that bolalipid bilayers behave as binary mixtures, containing bolalipids organized either in a straight conformation, spanning the entire bilayer, or in a u-shaped one, confined to a single leaflet. This dynamic mixture allows bolalipid bilayers to be very sturdy but also provides remodelling. However, remodelling is energetically more expensive than with standard lipids. The authors speculate that this might be why lipids were more abundant in the evolutionary process. Strengths:

      This is a wonderful paper, a very fine piece of scholarship. It is interesting from the point of view of biology, biophysics, and material science. The authors mastered the modelling and analysis of these complex systems. The evidence for their findings is really strong and complete. The paper is written superbly, the language is precise and the reading experience is very pleasant. The plots are very well-thought-out.

      Weaknesses:

      I would not talk about weaknesses, because this is really a nice paper. If I really had to find one, I would have liked to see some clear predictions of the model expressed in such a way that experimentalists could design validation experiments.

      We thank the reviewer for their very kind assessment. We incorporated their recommendations regarding experimental validation in the discussion section, as follows (p.14):

      “Our model makes a number of predictions that could be tested by experiment either in cells or in vitro. First, it predicts that a small increase in the fraction of archaeal bilayer lipids should be sufficient to soften a bolalipid-rich membrane. While this could be tested in the future, so far only very few studies have yet reported experimental analysis of archaeal membrane mixtures [18, 50]. Second, we observed that membranes with moderate bolalipid molecular rigidity k<sub>bola</sub> exhibit curvature-dependent bending rigidity. To experimentally verify this, one could extrude membrane tethers from cells while controlling for membrane tension. Finally, to get to the core mechanism underlying our findings, it will be important to develop experimental methods that will allow the fraction of U-shaped bolalipid conformers per leaflet to be imaged and measured.”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors have studied the mechanics of bolalipid and archaeal mixed-lipid membranes via comprehensive molecular dynamics simulations. The Cooke-Deserno 3-bead-per-lipid model is extended to bolalipids with 6 beads. Phase diagrams, bending rigidity, mechanical stability of curved membranes, and cargo uptake are studied. Effects such as the formation of U-shaped bolalipids, pore formation in highly curved regions, and changes in membrane rigidity are studied and discussed. The main aim has been to show how the mixture of bolalipids and regular bilayer lipids in archaeal membrane models enhances the fluidity and stability of these membranes.

      Strengths:

      The authors have presented a wide range of simulation results for different membrane conditions and conformations. For the most part, the analyses and their results are presented clearly and concisely. Figures, supplementary information, and movies very well present what has been studied. The manuscript is well-written and is easy to follow.

      We thank the reviewer for the detailed assessment of our work and their constructive feedback.

      Major issues

      R3.Q1: The Cooke-Deserno model, while very powerful for biophysical analysis of membranes at the mesoscale, is very much void of chemical information. It is parametrized such that it is good in producing fluid membranes and predicting values for bending rigidity, compressibility, and even thermalexpansioncoefficientfallingintheacceptedrangeofvaluesforbilayermembranes. But it still represents a generic membrane. Now, the authors have suggested a similar model for the archaeal bolalipids, which have chemically different lipids (the presence of cyclopentane rings for one), and there is no good justification for using the same pairwise interactions between their representative beads in the coarse-grained model. This does not necessarily diminish the worth of all the authors’ analyses. What is at risk here is the confusion between ”what we observe this model of bolalipidor mixed-membranes do” and ”how real bolalipid-containing archaeal membranes behave at these mechanical and thermal conditions.”.

      As the reviewer correctly notes, Cooke and Deserno used a minimal model, devoid of chemical detail, to represent fluid lipid membranes composed of bilayer lipids. Indeed archaeal lipids are chemically different compared to non-archaeal lipids, but just like non-archaeal lipids, they can be very different from one another. Given the chemical diversity of bolalipids between each other, instead of representing their complexity in a complicated model with many experimentally unconstrained parameters, we here defined a minimal model for bolalipids. The power of this minimal model is to represent the key physical/geometrical characteristics of archaeal membranes, namely the fact that lipid heads on two sides of the membrane are often connected, that bolalipids can exhibit a conformational change, and that bolalipids mix with some percentage of bilayer molecules. We then ask a general question: how do these unique geometrical characteristics of archaeal membranes influence their mechanics and reshaping? The reviewer is however right in pointing out that a model, regardless of its level of details (atomistic, coarse-grained, minimal), is still a model.

      Our approach of extending an established coarse-grained model for bilayer lipids to bolalipids is further supported by experimental observations, which report that archaeal bilayer lipids can form membranes of comparable bending rigidity to those of non-archaeal bilayer membranes [53]. Hence, different lipid linkages (archaeal vs. non-archaeal) give rise to fluid, deformable membranes of not too dissimilar rigidities, suggesting that both archaeal and non-archaeal bilayer lipids can be represented by a similar minimal coarse-grained model for the purpose of mesoscopic biophysical investigations. Since archaeal bolalipids have the same core chemical structure as two archaeal bilayer lipids joined by their tail ends, similarly we model a bolalipid by joining two bilayer lipids. Such an approach also efficiently enables us to compare bolalipid with bilayer membranes, and connect to the large body of knowledge on the physics of bilayer membranes.

      To conclude, our coarse-grained model is indeed intended to capture the main physical properties of bolalipid membranes, and not their chemical diversity.

      R3.Q2: Another more specific, major issue has to do with using the Hamm-Kozlov model for fitting the power spectrum of thermal undulations. The 1/q<sup>2</sup> term can very well be attributed to membrane tension. While a barostat is indeed used, have the authors made absolutely sure that the deviation from 1/q<sup>4</sup> behaviour does not correspond to lateral tension?

      To the casual observer, any 1/q<sup>2</sup> trend might point at membrane tension. However, the precise functional form is relevant as it determines whether the 1/q<sup>2</sup> dominates the 1/q<sup>4</sup> trend for small or large values of the wave number q in the fitted power spectrum.

      The first model (including lipid tilt) exhibits the functional form 1/(kq<sup>4</sup>) + 1/(kq<sup>2</sup>). In contrast, the second model (including membrane tension) exhibits the functional form 1/(kq<sup>4</sup> + ∑q<sup>2</sup>). Importantly, the two models obey a different functional form. Here k and k<sub>θ</sub>, are the bending and tilt moduli, which are assumed positive, and ∑ is the membrane tension, which can be either positive or negative. For the first model (with tilt), while for small q the amplitude is proportional to q<sup>-4</sup>, for large q the amplitude is proportional to q<sup>-2</sup>. In contrast, for the second model (with positive tension) while for small q the amplitude is proportional to q<sup>-2</sup>, for large q the amplitude is proportional to q<sup>-4</sup>. If membrane tension were to be negative in the second model, the slope would cross from negative infinity for small q to -4 for large q. The functional dependencies are summarized in Author response image 1A.

      For rigid bolalipid membranes, it is clearly visible that the slope of the power spectrum plotted against the wave number q decreases with increasing q (Author response image 1B). While the slope initially assumes a value close to 4, it gradually approaches 2 for larger values of q. We conclude that only the model including lipid tilt can fit the power spectrum of membrane fluctuations appropriately (solid-dashed line), whereas the model with tension fails to fit the data (dashed line). We note that the combined model containing both lipid tilt and membrane tension does not give a better fit (dotted line).

      To demonstrate that the tension model cannot fit the data, we included the best fits for both models for rigid bolalipid membranes in the new SI section 16 (p. S22) and show that only the tilt model leads to acceptable fits. We also measured the projected membrane tension - , where P<sub>x</sub>,P<sub>y</sub> are respectively the pressure in x and y direction and  L<sub>z</sub> is the dimension of the simulation box in z axis. We found the projected membrane tension to give a negligible value similarly to the one that we indirectly measured by fitting a combined model with both tension and tilt, further confirming our conjecture.

      Author response image 1.

      (A) Schematic showing the decay of the power spectrum as a function of the wave number q in the tilt model (top), in the tension model with positive membrane tension (middle), and in the tension model with negative membrane tension (bottom). (B) Fitted power spectrum as a function of q for rigid bolalipid membranes (k<sub>bola</sub>=5k<sub>B</sub>T). The fit shows that while the model with tension (dashed line) cannot fit the data, the model with tilt nicely fits the spectrum (solid-dashed line). The combined model including both tension and tilt does not fit the spectrum any better (dotted line).

      R3.Q3: I got more worried when I noticed in the SI that the simulations had been done with combined ”fix langevin” and ”fix nph” LAMMPS commands. This combination does not result in a proper isothermal-isobaric ensemble. The importance of tilt terms for bolalipids is indeed very interesting, but I believe more care is needed to establish that.

      In what follows, we show that there is no reason to worry. First of all we want to clarify that the physical setup we simulate is that of a membrane contained in a heat bath under negligible tension with correct diffusional dynamics. To achieve this physical setup, for which we use a Langevin thermostat combined with pressure control via an overdamped barostat, which we implement in LAMMPS by combining ”fix langevin” and ”fix nph”.

      In more detail: we simulated particles in an implicit solvent, for which we use a Langevin thermostat to get the right diffusional dynamics. To apply the theory of fitting fluctuation spectrums the simulation box length needs to be (near) constant. However, simulating membranes at a fixed box size results in an average non-zero membrane tension, making it hard to measure bending rigidity. The reason is that the effect of membrane tension is most influential on the largest wavelength modes, which are also most decisive when determining mechanical membrane properties like membrane rigidity. To minimize the effect of tension, we perform our simulation with an overdamped barostat (𝜏<sub>baro</sub> = 10 𝜏 <sub>langevin</sub>), which keeps the membrane near tensionless, as also done before [32]. In the revised manuscript, we have clarified the statement on the physical ensemble used (p.S2):

      “For simulating flat membrane patches of bolalipids, we combined the previously used Langevin thermostat with relaxation time of 1𝜏 with a Nosé–Hoover barostat with relaxation time of 10𝜏. In LAMMPS this amounts to combining the commands ’fix langevin’ with ’fix nph’. We configured the barostat to set lateral pressure P<sub>xy</sub> to zero by re-scaling the simulation box in the x-y plane. We compare this setup to a fixed box length setup, and an NPT ensemble setup, in SI section 17.”

      To connect our results with statistical mechanics ensemble theory we tested alternative setups. Similar setups, including the formal isothermal-isobaric ensemble, where N,P,T are kept constant using Nose-Hoover style equations for thermostating and barostating with modern corrections [34], which the reviewer refers to, result in very similar fluctuation spectrums. Consequently, our measurements of bending and tilt modulus hold true regardless of the integration scheme. However, such a setup does not correctly capture implicit solvent and diffusional dynamics.

      In even more detail: we tested our setup (implemented via ”fix langevin”+”fix nph”) versus a isothermal-isobaric ensemble (implemented via ”fix npt”). We measured volume mean and standard deviation, and found them matching for a reference LJ gas.

      To be completely sure, and to please the reviewer, we have performed additional verifications in the new SI section 17, which we summarize in the following. We simulated three representative membranes with different integration schemes: ”fix npt”, ”fix langevin”+”fix nph”, and ”fix langevin” (Langevin dynamics with projected area fixed at the average value obtained from a ”langevin+nph”). We checked that the ”fix nph” barostat is merely equilibrating the membrane to a tensionless configuration, after which the projected membrane area (A<sub>p</sub> = L<sub>x</sub>L<sub<y</sub>) is practically constant. Consequently, the different schemes resulted in minor changes in the longest wavelength modes that we tracked down to small changes in the negligible tension. The resulting measurements of bending modulus change by less than 10%, and our main text conclusions do not change. Author response image 2 compares the fluctuation spectrums for the different integration schemes.

      Author response image 2.

      Height fluctuation spectrum, for a bilayer membrane at T<sub>eff</sub> =1.1, simulated with Langevin dynamics (pink, ‘langevin‘), our setup (purple, ‘nph+langevin‘), and under an isothermal-isobaric ensemble (blue, ‘npt‘); fits are shown as dotted lines.

      R3.Q4: This issue is reinforced when considering Figure 3B. These results suggest that increasing the fraction of regular lipids increases the tilt modulus, with the maximum value achieved for a normal Cooke-Deserno bilayer void of bolalipids. But this is contradictory. For these bilayers, we don’t need the tilt modulus in the first place.

      We understand the concern why this might be counter-intuitive, and we thank the reviewer for pointing it out. We first want to stress that the tilt modulus can also be measured for bilayer membranes even if it is not needed to fit the fluctuation spectrum. If we measure the tilt modulus for a bilayer membrane, we obtain a value similar to the previously measured one [36]. Importantly, here we also report measurements for the tilt modulus for bolalipid membranes.

      To understand the seemingly contradictory behaviour of the tilt modulus, it is insightful to rewrite the expression for the fluctuation spectrum as done in Eq. (1):

      where is a characteristic length scale related to tilt, which we call the tilt persistence length. From the last equation it is easy to see that the tilt modulus 𝜅<sub>𝜃</sub> becomes relevant for the fluctuation spectrum if the tilt persistence length l<sub>𝜃</sub>  is not negligible. In other words, this means that we have to consider the tilt modulus 𝜅<sub>𝜃</sub> as relevant, if it is sufficiently small compared to the bending rigidity 𝜅.

      However, this is not only counter-intuitive, but also difficult to communicate graphically. Per the excellent reviewer’s suggestion, to make the interpretation more accessible, we converted in the main text and its figures the tilt modulus to the more directly interpretable tilt persistence length l<sub>𝜃</sub>, as this is small when tilt is irrelevant (for bilayer lipids and flexible bolalipids) and large otherwise (for rigid bolalipids). This includes changes to the main text on p.6 and p.8 , and to the insets in Figs. 2C and 3B. We note that for completeness we also report the tilt modulus 𝜅<sub>𝜃</sub>  in the SI.

      R3.Q5: Also, from the SI, I gathered that the authors have neglected the longest wavelength mode because it is not equilibrated. If this is indeed the case, it is a dangerous thing to do, because with a small membrane patch, this mode can very well change the general trend of the power spectrum. As a lot of other analyses in the manuscript rely on these measurements, I believe more elaboration is in order.

      We thank the reviewer for the careful examination of our supplementary material. For each fluctuation spectrum measurement, we ran multiple replicas. We observed that the largest wavelength modes were not fully equilibrated. In the simulations the first mode of the fluctuation spectrum is probed at different amplitudes and phases. We thus expected the potential systematic error would show up clearly when comparing spectrums of the different replicas. As we saw no correlation in these systematic offsets between replicas, we concluded that the simulations are sufficiently equilibrated and we could safely exclude the first mode of the fluctuation spectrum from our analysis.

      To show without doubt that this procedure does not randomly bias our results, we also ran simulations for three representative membranes until all modes were equilibrated. On the modes previously equilibrated, the resulting spectrums agree with our previous shorter simulations. On the largest wavelength modes that were previously not fully equilibrated, we noticed a small deviation from theory, specifically for flexible membranes (small bending modulus). These small deviations can be explained by including a negligible negative tension. Importantly, however, the resulting bending modulus σ stays nearly the same. We note that the small negative tension disappears when we halve the timestep (see Author response image 3). This verification is shown in SI section 17.

      R3.Q6: The authors have found that ”there is a strong dependency of the bending rigidity on the membrane mean curvature of stiffer bolalipids.” The effect is negative, with the membrane becoming less stiff at higher mean curvatures. Why is that? I would assume that with more flexible bolalipids, the possibility of reorganization into U-shaped chains should affect the bending rigidity more (as Figure 2E suggests). While for a stiff bolalipid, not much would change if you increase the mean curvature. This should be either a tilt effect, or have to do with asymmetry between the leaflets. But on the other hand, the tilt modulus is shown to decrease with increasing bolalipid rigidity. The authors get back to this issue only on page 10, when they consider U-shaped lipids in the inner and outer leaflets and write, ”this suggested that an additional membrane-curving mechanism must be involved.” But then again, in the Discussion, the authors write, ”It is striking that membranes made from stiffer bolalipids showed a curvature-dependent bending modulus, which is a clear signature that bolalipid membranes exhibit plastic behaviour during membrane reshaping,” adding to the confusion.

      Author response image 3.

      Height fluctuation spectrum, for a bilayer membrane at T<sub>eff</sub> =1.1, as simulated in the main text (grey, for 60⇥10<sup>3</sup>τ), for longer duration (1_.44⇥10<sup>6</sup>τ) (pink), and with the longer duration and halved timestep =0.005_τ(purple); fits are shown as dotted lines (tension and tilt) or dash-dot lines (tilt only).

      We thank the reviewer for asking this important question. Membrane bending rigidity in bolalipid membranes decreases dramatically once a small fraction of U-shapes is allowed to form, but then plateaus once this U-shape fraction reaches 20%. In a curved bolalipid membrane, U-shapes must accumulate in the outer leaflet to accommodate for area difference. Together, the bending rigidity non-linear dependence on U-shape fraction, and the promotion of U-shapes by curvature, explain why in a membrane made of moderately stiff bolalipids (k<sub>bola</sub> = 1k<sub>B</sub>T), which contain very few U-shapes in the flatstate, the bending rigidity of the membrane decreases as curvature increases. While in a membrane made of flexible bolalipid molecules (k<sub>bola</sub> = 0), where many U-shapes are present in the flat membrane, the bending rigidity does not change with curvature.

      Bending rigidity 𝜅 in flat membranes composed of bolalipids decreases dramatically once a small fraction of U-shapes is allowed to form, but plateaus once more than 20% of U-shaped bolalipids are present. In details, our data shows that with an increasing bolalipid molecular rigidity k<sub>bola</sub>, both the number of U-shaped bolalipids decreases (Fig. 2B) and the membrane rigidity 𝜅 increases (Fig. 2C). Thus, the correlation suggests that U-shaped bolalipids soften the membrane, in a non-linear way where most of the change in membrane bending rigidity happens for U-shaped bolalipid fraction < 20% (Figure S11).

      Separately, membrane curvature affects the area difference between curved membrane leaflets and thus drives U-shape accumulation. To be specific, a cylindrical membrane with area A, mean curvature H and thickness h has the outer leaflet with area A(1 + Hh) and the inner leaflet with smaller area A(1 Hh). This can be large, in our simulations up to an area change of Hh \= 25%. For pure bolalipid membranes, straight bolalipids occupy the same space in each leaflet. Area difference can then be achieved only by having a different amount of U-shaped bolalipids in each leaflet, which can result in a different U-shape fraction between leaflets and thus ’asymmetry between leaflets’. Figure S10 confirms U-shape head fraction asymmetry that increases with curvature, for both flexible (k<sub>bola</sub> = 0) and moderately stiff bolalipids (k<sub>bola</sub> = 1k<sub>B</sub>T).

      Together, these two effects result in membrane softening under curvature for the moderately stiff bolalipids, but constant rigidity for flexible bolalipids (Fig. 2F). In details: for membranes composed of moderately stiff bolalipid molecules (k<sub>bola</sub> = 1k<sub>B</sub>T), the U-shape bolalipid head fraction only increases in the outer leaflet, goingfrom10to20%(Figure S10). This is in the high sensitivity region where the bending rigidity is expected to change the most (Figure S11). We hypothesize that the molecular rigidity of a U-shaped bolalipid creates compression on the outer leaflet that stabilizes the membrane curvature and thus causes membrane softening. We suspect that for membranes composed of rigid bolalipids (k<sub></sub> > 1k<sub>B</sub>T), the effect is likely not present due to the absence of U-shape formation even under strong bending.

      By contrast, for membranes composed of flexible bolalipids (k<sub></sub> = 0), the U-shaped bolalipid head fraction changes relatively little from its value for flat membranes (from 50% to respectively 60 and 40% for the outer and inner leaflet, Figure S10). This is in the region where the membrane bending rigidity is expected to respond weakly to U-shape fraction (Figure S11). Additionally, the change is symmetric, so presumably the outer leaflet becomes softer as the inner leaflet becomes stiffer, thus creating opposing effects and only weakly affecting the membrane bending rigidity as a whole. We note that the distinction between the U-shape head fraction that we plot (Figure S10) and U-shape fraction (Figure S11) matters little for this analysis.

      We have added this deduction and its plots to SI section 8, and revised the corresponding statement in the main text accordingly (p.7 ).

      “Changing membrane curvature alters the area differently in the two membrane leaflets. To adapt to the area difference, we thus expect the fraction of U-shaped bolalipids to change as the membrane curvature changes. Moreover, the results of Fig. 2B and Fig. 2C showed that the U-shaped bolalipid fraction and the membrane bending rigidity are correlated. As a result, we predict that the fraction of straight versus U-shaped bolalipids in a membrane will change in response to membrane bending, in a way that makes the bending rigidity of a bolalipid membrane curvature dependent.”

      R3.Q7: This issue is repeated when the authors study nanoparticle uptake. They write: ”to reconcile these seemingly conflicting observations we reason that the bending rigidity, similar to Figure 2F, is not constant but softens upon increasing membrane curvature, due to dynamic change in the ratio between bolalipids in straight and U-shaped conformation. Hence, bolalipid membranes show stroking plastic behaviour as they soften during reshaping.” But the softening effect that they refer to, as shown in Figure 4B, occurs for very stiff bolalipids, for which not much switching to U-shaped conformation should occur.

      We thank the reviewer for locating a particularly dense sentence. We changed the text to explicitly refer to the range k<sub></sub> 2 [0,2] k<sub>B</sub>T for which there is significant change in U-shape fraction (p.8 ):

      “To reconcile these seemingly conflicting observations we reason that the bending rigidity κ, similar to Fig. 2F, is not constant but softens in the range k<sub></sub> 2 [0,2] k<sub>B</sub>T, upon increasing membrane curvature. This is due to the dynamic change in the ratio between bolalipids in straight and U-shaped conformation.”

      As for Fig. 4B, for k<sub></sub> > 2k<sub>B</sub>T, pores form thus explaining the plateau in adsorption energy.

      R3.Q8: Another major issue is with what the authors refer to as the ”effective temperature”. While plotting phase diagrams for kT/eps value is absolutely valid, I’m not a fan of calling this effective temperature. It is a dimensionless quantity that scales linearly with temperature, but is not a temperature. It is usually called a ”reduced temperature”. Then the authors refer to their findings as studying the stability of archaeal membranes at high temperatures. I have to disagree because eps is not the only potential parameter in the simulations (there are at least space exclusion and angle-bending stiffnesses) so one cannot identify changing eps with changing the global simulation temperature. This only works when you have one potential parameter, like an LJ gas.

      We indeed thought about this before and found that it makes little difference in our set-up. To thoroughly show that the distinction matters very little, per reviewer’s question, we computed our phase diagrams by scaling temperature T explicitly (and not lipid tail interactions T<sub>eff</sub> = k<sub>B</sub>T /ϵ<sub>p</sub>). We added these results to the SI section 14 and found no significant difference when comparing scaling tail interactions (Figure S15A) with scaling temperature explicitly (Figure S15B).

      We also computed Fig. 2A-C for scaling interactions (Figure S17A) and scaling temperature explicitly (Figure S17B). We found a slightly increased U-shaped bolalipid fraction for low k<sub></sub> when comparing scaling interactions (Figure S17A) with temperature scaling (Figure S17B). The reason is that the U-shaped fraction depends on temperature, as with higher temperature bolalipids can easier transition into the U-shape. Most importantly, however, we found no qualitative changes on the liquid region or the mechanical membrane properties when we compared the different scaling variants.

      The reason why both scaling variants match so well can be understood easily. All pair potentials, including volume exclusion interactions between head beads and other membrane beads, were also scaled in the same manner as tail-to-tail interactions, as described in the SI. In contrast, the energy scales for maintaining the lipid bonds, the bilayer lipid angles and the bolalipid angles are relatively large compared to the energy scales involved in tail-to-tail interactions. This separation of energy scales guarantees that there will be little effect when increasing global temperature. Regarding nomenclature, we take the reviewer’s advice and have added ’reduced temperature’ as an alias for T<sub>eff</sub> in the main text.

      In the revised version of the manuscript, we mention these observations in the SI section 14 and point towards these results in the main text (p.4 ):

      “This interaction strength governs the membrane phase behaviour and can be interpreted as the effective temperature or reduced temperature T<sub>eff</sub> = k<sub>B</sub>T /ϵ<sub>p</sub>. As the distinction between scaling interactions (T<sub>eff</sub>) or temperature (T) is not important for our analysis (see Supplemental Information (SI) section 14), for simplicity we refer to T<sub>eff</sub> as temperature in the following.”

      Minor issues

      R3.Q9: As the authors have noted, the fact that the membrane curvature can change the ratio of U-shaped to straight bolalipids would render the curvature elasticity non-linear (though the term ”plastic” should not be used, as this is still structurally reversible when the stress is removed. Technically, it is hypoelastic behaviour, possibly with hysteresis.) With this in mind, when the authors use essentially linear elastic models for fluctuation analysis, they should make a comparison of maximum curvatures occurring in simulations with a range that causes significant changes in bolalipid conformational ratios.

      We thank the reviewer for their suggestion on calling the non-linear behaviour of the curvature elasticity hypoelastic. We have edited the main text accordingly (p.8 ):

      “In an elastic material, the strain modulus holds constant and deformation is reversible. For bolalipid membranes at k<sub></sub> = 1k<sub>B</sub>T, however, the bending modulus decreases when deformation increases, rendering bolalipid membranes hypoelastic.”

      Moreover, regarding the maximum curvatures occurring in the fluctuation simulations: We first note that the ensemble average of the mean curvature H from the fluctuation measurements is indicated as a vertical line in Fig. 2F. As the average value is nearly zero, the membrane can be considered as flat in good approximation. To investigate the question in more detail, we extended the SI with a careful analysis of the validity of the maximum membrane curvature and the validity of the Monge gauge approximation (SI section 15).

      In short, we found that the involved membrane curvatures are small and therefore are unlikely to trigger any significant changes of the bending modulus. Moreover, since we are dealing with two bolalipid conformations, we also tested the homogeneity of the membrane. In our simulations of flat membrane patches we did not observe clustering or phase separation between the two bolalipid conformations beyond the [2,3]σ range. Furthermore, we get good agreement between our fluctuation measurement and the cylinder simulations in Fig. 2F. We now mention this verification in the revised version of the manuscript (p.8 ):

      “Fortunately, this dependency on curvature does not invalidate our fluctuation results, where the curvature is small enough that its effect on the bending modulus is negligible (SI section 15).”

      Last but least, simulating bending/unbending cycles of an arc-shaped membrane (frozen endpoints) shows agreement with cylinder membrane simulations, and no hysteresis at the rates of deformation employed (cf. M. Amaral’s thesis [54], soon to be out of the embargo period).

      R3.Q10: The Introduction section of the manuscript is written with a biochemical approach, with very minor attention to the simulation works on this system. Some molecular dynamics works are only cited as existing previous work, without mentioning what has already been studied in archaeal membranes. While some information, like the binding of ESCRT proteins to archaeal membranes, though interesting, helps little to place the study within the discipline. The Introduction should be revised to show what has already been studied with simulations (as the authors mention in the Discussion) and how the presented research complements it.

      The present research for the first time covers archaeal membranes with a single coarse-grained model capable of assuming both bolalipid in-membrane conformations and sweeps through temperature, membrane composition, and molecular rigidity. The work shows the first curvature dependent bending modulus for pure bolalipid membranes. It also investigates systematically bending modulus and Gaussian modulus, and tests the model in an all-encompassing budding simulation that incorporates topology changes. Existing atomistic or coarse-grained MD simulations (MARTINI or similar force fields) are limited to small patches of membrane, with no study of large-scale deformations or topology changes; plus, they rely on force fields that were parametrized for bilayer membranes.

      To give a comprehensive overview of the field, we revised the introduction section of the manuscript, in which we now discuss previous computational work investigating membrane diffusivity, U-shaped lipid fraction, and bending rigidity (p.3 ):

      “By contrast, only a few studies have investigated bolalipid membranes applying computational or theoretical tools [24, 25]. Specifically, the pore closure time in bolalipid membranes, and the role of cyclopentane rings for membrane properties has been investigated using all-atom simulations, showing decreased lateral mobility, reduced permeability to water, and increased lipid packing [26–28]. Moreover, using coarse-grained simulations, it was suggested that bolalipid membranes are thicker [29], exhibit a gel-to-liquid phase transition at higher temperature [30], and exhibit a reduced diffusivity [31]. However, little research has been devoted to investigating mechanics and reshaping of bolalipid membranes at the mesoscale despite the obvious importance of this question from evolutionary, biophysics, and biotechnological perspectives and although different membrane physics is expected to manifest.”

      Following the reviewer’s advice and to keep the introduction concise and focused on bolalipid membranes, we have removed the paragraph on ESCRT-III proteins in the revised manuscript.

      R3.Q11: The authors have been a bit loose with using the term ”stability”. I’d like to see the distinction in each case, as in ”chemical/thermal/mechanical/conformational stability”.

      We have clarified when applicable the type of stability throughout the manuscript. In all other instances, if not clear from context, we mean simply that the membrane persists being a membrane. At our coarse-grained level, this means the membrane does not disassemble into a gas phase.

      R3.Q12: In the original Cooke-Deserno model, a so-called ”poorman’s angle-bending term” is used, which is essentially a bond-stretching term between the first and third particle. However, I notice the authors using the full harmonic angle-bending potential. This should be mentioned.

      This is made clear in the SI (Eq. (S3)). Cooke and Deserno mention the harmonic angle potential as a valid alternative in their original publication. We now also added this detail to the main text (p.3 ):

      “The angle formed by the chain of three beads is kept near 180° via an angular potential with strength k<sub>0</sub>, instead of the approximation by a bond between end beads of the original model [32].”

      R3.Q13: The analysis of energy of U-shaped lipids with the linear model E \= c<sub>0</sub> + c<sub>1</sub>k<sub></sub> is indeed very interesting. I am curious, can this also be corroborated with mean energy measurements? The minor issue is calling the source of the favorability of U-shaped lipids ”entropic”, while clearly an energetic contribution is found. The two conformations, for example, might differ in the interactions with the neighbouring lipids.

      We were also curious and thank the reviewer for the suggestion of mean energy measurements. We concluded that there must be either an entropic contribution to the free energy or an intermolecular interaction energy favouring U-shaped bolalipids. We have now included these measurements in SI section 6 (p.S5 ):

      “By splitting the average potential energy between an internal contribution (bonds, angles and pair interactions between particles in the same molecule) and an external contribution (pair interactions between a molecule and its neighbours), we determined the transition energy from straight to U-shaped bolalipids in detail. We found that this transition lowers the internal potential energy of the bolalipid while increasing its interaction energy. In total, we obtained an energy barrier for the transition of ΔE<sub>s→u</sub> = 0.79±0.01k<sub>B</sub>T. Since the fit indicates, however, that the U-shaped bolalipid conformation is preferred over the straight conformation, we conclude that there must be either an entropic contribution to the free energy or an intermolecular interaction energy favouring U-shaped bolalipids.”

      We refer to these measurements in the main text (p.6 ):

      “For the fit it appears that c<sub>0</sub> < 0, which implies that bolalipids in U-shape conformation are slightly favoured over straight bolalipids at k<sub></sub> = 0 (explored in SI section 6).”

      R3.Q14: The authors write in the Discussion, ”In any case, our results indicate that membrane remodelling, such as membrane fission during membrane traffic, is much more difficult in bolalipid membranes [34].” Firstly, I’m not sure if studying the dependence of budding behaviour on adhesion energy with nanoparticles is enough to make claims about membrane fission. Secondly, why is the 2015 paper by Markus Deserno cited here?

      We thank the reviewer for giving us the opportunity to clarify. We make an energetic argument on membrane fission based on the observed difference in the ratio of .

      Splitting a spherical membrane vesicle into two spherical vesicles (fission) increases the bending energy by 8𝜋𝜅 and decreases the energy related to the Gaussian bending modulus by . The second part of the argument is given for example in the review by Markus Deserno (p.23, right column), that’s why we cite the paper here. Together, this gives an energy barrier, required for membrane fission in the considered geometry of ∆E<sub>fission</sub> = . We found that is around 0.5 for bolalipid membranes and around 1 for bilayer membranes. Since 𝜅 was typically larger in bolalipid membranes we thus expect the energy barrier for fission ∆E<sub>fission</sub> to be larger for bolalipid membranes. We therefore predict that membrane remodelling, such as membrane fission during membrane trafficking, is harder in bolalipid membranes. We explain our reasoning in the discussion of the revised manuscript (p.13 ):

      “Membrane remodelling, such as the fission of one spherical vesicle into two, increases the bending energy by 8πκ but decreases the energy related to the Gaussian modulus by – [39], giving rise to a fission energy barrier of ∆E<sub>fission</sub> = . Our results indicated that while in bolalipid membranes 𝜅 is larger, is smaller compared to bilayer membranes. Our results thus predict a larger energy barrier for membrane fission ∆E<sub>fission</sub> in bolalipid membranes compared to bilayer membranes.”

      R3.Q15: In the SI, where the measurement of the diffusion coefficient is discussed, the expression for D is missing the power 2 of displacement.

      We thank the reviewer for spotting this oversight. We corrected it in the revised version of the SI (p.S5 ).

      R3.Q16: Where cargo uptake is discussed, the term ”adsorption energy” is used. I think the more appropriate term would be ”adhesion energy”.

      For the sake of simplicity, we changed the term to adhesion energy (caption of Fig. 4, and p.10). We do not have a strong opinion on this, but we believe that adsorption energy would be equally correct as we describe the adsorption of many lipid head beads to a nanoparticle.

      R3.Q17: Typos:

      Page 1, paragraph 2: Adaption → Adaptation. Page 10, paragraph 1: Stroking → Striking.

      We thank the reviewer for spotting these typos which we have corrected in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Recommendations for the authors

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      A few thoughts (likely out of the scope of this paper but possibly to consider upon revision):

      R1.Q1: Do bolalipids always have the same headgroup? I don’t recall reading this in the introduction/discussion. R1 and R2 are in Figure 1, but I don’t know whether there are standard types. Could this be expanded upon? Is the model able to take these differences into account?

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important question. Similar to bacteria and eukaryotes, in archaea there is a huge variety in terms of the different head groups that lipids can contain and thus also lipid variety. Most archaeal lipids have head groups that contain either phosphate groups or sugar residues. Typically, archaeal bolalipids are asymmetric and contain a phosphatidyl and a sugar moiety at the two ends of the lipid molecule. Within the membrane the lipid is oriented such that the phosphatidyl moiety points towards the interior of the cell whereas the sugar moiety points towards the outside of the cell as it occupies more space [5].

      In our computational model, however, we consider symmetric bolalipids for the sake of simplicity and to decouple the role of ”connected geometry” from other effects. In principle, we could investigate the effect of lipid asymmetry by increasing the size of one of the lipid head beads. However, this investigation exceeds the scope of the present study and therefore requires future work.

      In the revised version of the manuscript, we now clarify that bolalipids can have different headgroups (p.1 and the caption of Fig. 1):

      “The hydrophilic heads can be composed of different functional groups with phosphatidyl and sugar being the most relevant moieties. For bolalipids the two head groups at either end of the molecule are typically distinct (Fig. 1A right) [5].”

      “The hydrophilic head of a bolalipid can be composed of different functional groups represented by R1 and R2 (right).”

      We also explicitly state that we neglect lipid head group asymmetry for the sake of simplicity (p.4 ):

      “To decouple the effect of the connected geometry of the bolalipids from that of lipid asymmetry, we assume both head beads of a bolalipid to share the same properties.”

      R1.Q2: Is it possible to compare the mesoscale models to either Coarse-grained or even all-atom lipid models? Have simulations previously been performed for bolalipids at those levels of description?

      A few studies have investigated bolalipids membranes in simulations previously. These studies either used all-atom or coarse-grained simulations. However, none of these studies investigated how bolalipids respond to membrane deformations. Therefore, it is currently not possible to directly compare our results to studies in the literature. However, to recapitulate our predictions experimentally is certainly something that could and should be done in the future. As a reply to this reviewer and reviewer 3, we discuss the current state of modelling bolalipid membranes in simulations in the revised version of the manuscript (p.3 ):

      “By contrast, only a few studies have investigated bolalipid membranes applying computational or theoretical tools [24, 25]. Specifically, the pore closure time in bolalipid membranes, and the role of cyclopentane rings for membrane properties has been investigated using all-atom simulations, showing decreased lateral mobility, reduced permeability to water, and increased lipid packing [26–28]. Moreover, using coarse-grained simulations, it was suggested that bolalipid membranes are thicker [29], exhibit a gel-to-liquid phase transition at higher temperature [30], and exhibit a reduced diffusivity [31]. However, little research has been devoted to investigating mechanics and reshaping of bolalipid membranes at the mesoscale despite the obvious importance of this question from evolutionary, biophysics, and biotechnological perspectives and although different membrane physics is expected to manifest.”

      We want to mention, however, that we do compare membrane diffusivity, U-shaped lipid fraction, and bending rigidity to the behaviour and values that have been previously measured in simulations in the discussion section. In general, we find good agreement between our results and previously reported behaviour/values (p.13 ):

      “While flexible bolalipid membranes are liquid under the same conditions as bilayer membranes, we found that stiff bolalipids form membranes that operate in the liquid regime at higher temperatures. These results agree well with previous molecular dynamics simulations that suggested that bolalipid membranes are more ordered and have a reduced diffusivity compared to bilayer membranes [24, 29]. In our simulations, this is due to the fact that completely flexible bolalipids molecules adopt both straight (transmembrane) as well as the U-shaped (loop) conformation with approximately the same frequency. In contrast, stiff bolalipids typically only take on the straight conformation when assembled in a membrane. These results agree with the previous coarse-grained molecular dynamics simulations using the MARTINI force field which showed that the ratio of straight to U-shaped bolalipids increased upon stiffening the linker between the lipid tails [29].

      [...]

      When we determined the bending rigidity of bolalipid membranes by measuring their response to thermal fluctuations, we found that membranes made from flexible bolalipids are only slightly more rigid than bilayer membranes. This result is consistent with previous atomistic simulations, which showed that the membrane rigidity was similar for membranes composed of bilayer lipids and flexible synthetic bolalipids [45].”

      R1.Q3: How would membrane proteins alter the behaviour of bolalipids? Either those integral to the membrane or those binding peripherally?

      The reviewer asks an important question. However, the question is difficult to answer due to its scope and the gaps in the current literature. Important examples of integral or peripheral membrane proteins that alter the behaviour of bolalipids and archaeal bolalipid membranes are involved in cell homeostasis, cell division, membrane trafficking, and lipid synthesis.

      The cells of many archaeal species are enclosed in a paracrystalline protein layer called the Slayer, which is attached to the lipid membrane [4, 55]. The main function of the S-layer is to keep the cell’s shape and to protect it against osmotic stress. Due to the embedding of the S-layer in the membrane at specific locations, it is to be expected that the membrane properties are influenced by the S-layer. Furthermore, archaea execute cell division by locally reshaping the membrane using FtsZ and ESCRT-III proteins [56]. While Asgard archaeal genomes encode proteins with homology to those regulating aspects of eukaryotic membrane remodelling and trafficking [57], they have yet to be observed undergoing a process like endocytosis [58]. In addition, it has been speculated that the proteins that drive the synthesis of two diether lipids into a tetraether lipid are either membrane associated or integral membrane proteins [59].

      However, to the best of our knowledge it is not known how membrane proteins specifically alter the behaviour of bolalipids. Future work will need to be executed to answer this question. Following the advice of reviewer 3 and to keep the introduction concise and focused on bolalipid membranes, we do not mention these observations in the revised manuscript.

      R1.Q4: Is there a mechanism in cells to convert or switch bolalipids from a straight to a u-shaped description? Does this happen spontaneously or are there enzymes responsible for this?

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up this important point. Despite the relevance of the question, little is currently known about the mechanism that make bolalipids transition between a straight and a U-shaped configuration mainly because there is to date no established experimental method.

      Besides our own results, most of what we know comes from coarse-grained molecular dynamics simulations, which showed that bolalipids can spontaneously transition between the straight and U-shaped configuration [29]. In addition, by using comparative genomic analysis, it has been predicted that many archaeal species contain flippases, i.e., membrane proteins that are able, upon the consumption of energy, to transfer (flipflop) bilayer lipids between the two membrane leaflets [43]. Moreover, it has been shown that Halobacterium salinarum (an archaeon with a bilayer lipid membrane) [44] contains scramblases, which are membrane proteins that passively transfer bilayer lipids from one membrane leaflet to the other. It is therefore tempting to speculate that similar proteins might exist for bolalipids which could facilitate the straight to U-shaped transition.

      In addition, it has been reported that vesicles composed of bolalipid membranes can undergo fusion with enveloped influenza viruses [17]. In this context, it has been suggested that the influenza fusion protein hemagglutinin may locally induce U-shaped bolalipids to facilitate membrane fusion. However, all these hints are by far no proof of a mechanism that can drive the straight to U-shaped bolalipid transition, and further work needs to be done to investigate this question in detail.

      In the revised version of the manuscript, we now discuss what is known about potential mechanisms to facilitate the straight to U-shaped transition in the discussion section (p.13 ):

      “While previous coarse-grained simulations predicted that bolalipids spontaneously transition between the straight and U-shaped conformations [29], how this happens in archaeal membranes and whether membrane proteins are involved in this conformational transition needs to be clarified in the future. Experimental studies suggest that archaeal membranes contain flippases and scramblases for the transitioning of bilayer lipids between membrane leaflets [43, 44], raising the possibility that similar proteins could also facilitate conformational transitions in bolalipids. In addition, it has been suggested that the viral fusion protein hemagglutinin could cause a transition from straight to U-shaped bolalipid conformation during the fusion of bolalipid vesicles with influenza viruses [17]. However, future investigation is required.”

      R1.Q5: Ideally, coordinates and any parameter files required to run the molecular simulations should be included for reproducibility.

      We absolutely share the reviewer’s concern with reproducibility and as such have included in the original submission as part of our data availability section a link to a code repository (available at: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13934991 [51]) that allows initializing and simulating flat membrane patches, with user control of the parameters explored in this paper (𝜔,T<sub>eff</sub>,k<sub>bola</sub>,f<sup>bi</sup>).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      This is a great paper and I congratulate the authors for writing such a fine piece of scholarship. The only nitty-gritty feedback that I have is summarized in the following three points:

      R2.Q1: In the introduction the authors talk about archaea adapting their membrane to retain membrane fluidity. However, homeoviscous adaptation is also fundamental in bacteria and eukaryotes.

      The reviewer is correct, like archaea the membranes of bacteria and eukaryotes must balance between flexibility and stability. Moreover, the cell membranes in all 3 domains of life need to maintain membrane fluidity and provide mobility to the embedded lipids and membrane proteins (homeoviscous adaptation). The general idea is that these organisms change the ratio of different lipids to change membrane properties and thereby optimally adapt to their environments [10]. Importantly, however, there are differences of how homeoviscous adaptation is maintained across the different domains of life. As a reply to this reviewer and reviewer 3, we now discuss the underlying mechanisms in the revised parts of the introduction (p.1 ):

      “Like for bacteria and eukaryotes, archaea must keep their lipid membranes in a fluid state (homeoviscous adaptation). This is important even under extreme environmental conditions, such as hot and cold temperatures, or high and low pH values [7]. Because of this, many archaea adapt to changes in their environment by tuning the lipid composition of their membranes: altering the ratio between bola- and bilayer lipids in their membranes [8, 9] and/or by changing the number of cyclopentane rings in their lipid tails, which are believed to make lipid molecules more rigid [5]. For example, Thermococcus kodakarensis increases its tetraether bolalipid ratio from around 50% to over 80% when the temperature of the environment increases from 60 to 85 C [10]. Along the same lines, the cell membrane of Sulfolobus acidocaldarius, can contain over 90 % of bolalipids with up to 8 cyclopentane rings at 70 C and pH 2.5 [5, 11]. It is worth mentioning that in exceptional cases bacteria also synthesise bolalipids in response to high temperatures [12], highlighting that the study of bolalipid membranes is relevant not only for archaeal biology but also from a general membrane biophysics perspective.”

      R2.Q2: Uncertainties in Gaussian rigidity modulus estimates are not properly reported.

      The large uncertainties in the Gaussian rigidity modulus were due to the fact how they were calculated. In short, is determined in cap folding simulations [41] (SI section 9), by using the measured values of the dimensionless parameter 𝜉, related to the folding probability, the bending modulus 𝜅, the membrane line tension , and the cap radius R. In our case, the main source of uncertainty for determining comes from the uncertainty in the measurement of the bending rigidity 𝜅. To obtain 𝜅, previously, we fitted fluctuation spectra for different seeds and only then averaged the obtained values. In the revised version of the manuscript, we now first pool the fluctuation spectra of the different simulation seeds before we fit all spectra at the same time. This new approach results in smaller uncertainties for the bending rigidity 𝜅 and also the Gaussian rigidity modulus .

      As a consistency check, in addition to the simulations that we previously performed at T<sub>eff</sub> = 1.3, we have repeated the cap folding and line tension simulations at T<sub>eff</sub> = 1.2, resulting in similar values for . In the revised version of the manuscript, we report the newly calculated values and uncertainties for at T<sub>eff</sub>  = 1.2 in the main text (p.8 ):

      “At T<sub>eff</sub>  = 1.2, we obtained = 4.30±0.22kBT and thus a ratio of = 0.89±0.04 for bilayer membranes, similar to what has been reported previously [41]. For flexible bolalipid membranes, we got a slightly smaller value for = 5.04 ± 0.37kBT. Due to the larger bending modulus, however, flexible bolalipid membranes show a significantly smaller ratio = 0.64± 0.04 (k<sub></sub> = 0). At larger temperature (Teff = 1.3), the ratio can be even smaller = 0.45 ± 0.07 (see SI section 9).”

      In addition, we report the values at T<sub>eff</sub> = 1.3 and T<sub>eff</sub> = 1.2 in the SI (p.S15 , Tabl. S4):

      We have also adapted the discussion of the Gaussian bending modulus accordingly (p.13 ):

      “Another marked difference between bilayer and flexible bolalipid membranes is the ratio of the Gaussian rigidity to the bending modulus. Instead of being around 1 as for bilayer membranes [41], it is around 1/2 and therefore only half of that of bilayer lipids.”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      While I think the bulk of the work presented is useful, some of the issues that I raised in my review are indeed major. Without properly addressing them, it is hard to accept the conclusions of the manuscript. I hope the authors can address them by revising their analysis.

      We thank the reviewer for their constructive feedback, which helped us to improve the manuscript. We have addressed all points raised by the reviewer in our detailed point-by-point response to the reviewer (see above). We hope the reviewer will now find it easier to accept our conclusions.

      (1) R. Phillips, J. Kondev, J. Theriot, and H. Garcia, Physical biology of the cell (Garland Science, New York, 2012).

      (2) H. T. McMahon and J. L. Gallop, Membrane curvature and mechanisms of dynamic cell membrane remodelling, Nature 438, 590 (2005).

      (3) S. B. Gould, Membranes and evolution, Curr. Biol. 28, R381 (2018).

      (4) S.-V. Albers and B. H. Meyer, The archaeal cell envelope, Nat. Rev. Microbiol. 9, 414 (2011).

      (5) P. M. Oger and A. Cario, Adaptation of the membrane in Archaea, Biophys. Chem. 183, 42 (2013).

      (6) K. Rastädter, D. J. Wurm, O. Spadiut, and J. Quehenberger, The Cell Membrane of Sulfolobus spp.—Homeoviscous Adaption and Biotechnological Applications, International Journal of Molecular Sciences 21, 3935 (2020).

      (7) P. L.-G. Chong, Archaebacterial bipolar tetraether lipids: Physico-chemical and membrane properties, Chem. Phys. Lipids 163, 253 (2010).

      (8) M. Tourte, P. Schaeffer, V. Grossi, and P. M. Oger, Functionalized Membrane Domains: An Ancestral Feature of Archaea?, Front. Microbiol. 11, 526 (2020).

      (9) Y. H. Kim, G. Leriche, K. Diraviyam, T. Koyanagi, K. Gao, D. Onofrei, J. Patterson, A. Guha, N. Gianneschi, G. P. Holland, M. K. Gilson, M. Mayer, D. Sept, and J. Yang, Entropic effects enable life at extreme temperatures, Sci. Adv. 5, eaaw4783 (2019).

      (10) M. F. Siliakus, J. van der Oost, and S. W. M. Kengen, Adaptations of archaeal and bacterial membranes to variations in temperature, pH and pressure, Extremophiles 21, 651 (2017).

      (11) D. W. Grogan, Phenotypic characterization of the archaebacterial genus sulfolobus: comparison of five wild-type strains, J. Bacteriol. 171, 6710 (1989).

      (12) D. X. Sahonero-Canavesi, M. F. Siliakus, A. Abdala Asbun, M. Koenen, F. von Meijenfeldt, S. Boeren, N. J. Bale, J. C. Engelman, K. Fiege, L. Strack van Schijndel, J. S. Sinninghe Damsté, and L. Villanueva, Disentangling the lipid divide: Identification of key enzymes for the biosynthesis of membrane-spanning and ether lipids in Bacteria, Sci. Adv. 8, eabq8652 (2022).

      (13) M. van Wolferen, A. A. Pulschen, B. Baum, S. Gribaldo, and S.-V. Albers, The cell biology of archaea, Nat. Microbiol. 10.1038/s41564-022-01215-8 (2022).

      (14) U. Bakowsky, U. Rothe, E. Antonopoulos, T. Martini, L. Henkel, and H.-J. Freisleben, Monomolecular organization of the main tetraether lipid from Thermoplasma acidophilum at the water–air interface, Chem. Phys. Lipids 105, 31 (2000).

      (15) C. Jeworrek, F. Evers, M. Erlkamp, S. Grobelny, M. Tolan, P. L.-G. Chong, and R. Winter, Structure and Phase Behavior of Archaeal Lipid Monolayers, Langmuir 27, 13113 (2011).

      (16) D. P. Brownholland, G. S. Longo, A. V. Struts, M. J. Justice, I. Szleifer, H. I. Petrache, M. F. Brown, and D. H. Thompson, Phase Separation in Binary Mixtures of Bipolar and Monopolar Lipid Dispersions Revealed by 2H NMR Spectroscopy, Small Angle X-Ray Scattering, and Molecular Theory, Biophysical Journal 97, 2700 (2009).

      (17) A. Bhattacharya, I. D. Falk, F. R. Moss, T. M. Weiss, K. N. Tran, N. Z. Burns, and S. G. Boxer, Structure–function relationships in pure archaeal bipolar tetraether lipids, Chem. Sci. 15, 14273 (2024).

      (18) V. Vitkova, D. Mitkova, V. Yordanova, P. Pohl, U. Bakowsky, G. Staneva, and O. Batishchev, Elasticity and phase behaviour of biomimetic membrane systems containing tetraether archaeal lipids, Colloids Surf. A Physicochem. Eng. Asp. 601, 124974 (2020).

      (19) E. Chang, Unusual thermal stability of liposomes made from bipolar tetraether lipids, Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 202, 673 (1994).

      (20) O. V. Batishchev, A. S. Alekseeva, D. S. Tretiakova, T. R. Galimzyanov, A. Y. Chernyadyev, N. R. Onishchenko, P. E. Volynsky, and I. A. Boldyrev, Cyclopentane rings in hydrophobic chains of a phospholipid enhance the bilayer stability to electric breakdown, Soft Matter 16, 3216 (2020).

      (21) U. Seifert, Configurations of fluid membranes and vesicles, Adv. Phys. 46, 13 (1997).

      (22) H. Noguchi, Membrane Simulation Models from Nanometer to Micrometer Scale, J. Phys. Soc. Jpn. 78, 041007 (2009).

      (23) F. Frey and T. Idema, More than just a barrier: using physical models to couple membrane shape to cell function, Soft Matter 17, 3533 (2021).

      (24) C. Huguet, S. Fietz, A. Rosell-Melé, X. Daura, and L. Costenaro, Molecular dynamics simulation study of the effect of glycerol dialkyl glycerol tetraether hydroxylation on membrane thermostability, Biochimica et Biophysica Acta (BBA) - Biomembranes 1859, 966 (2017).

      (25) T. R. Galimzyanov, P. I. Kuzmin, P. Pohl, and S. A. Akimov, Elastic deformations of bolalipid membranes, Soft Matter 12, 2357 (2016).

      (26) T. R. Galimzyanov, P. E. Volynsky, and O. V. Batishchev, Continuum elasticity and molecular dynamics of a pore in archaeal bolalipid membranes, Soft Matter 21, 687 (2025).

      (27) A. O. Chugunov, P. E. Volynsky, N. A. Krylov, I. A. Boldyrev, and R. G. Efremov, Liquid but Durable: Molecular Dynamics Simulations Explain the Unique Properties of Archaeal-Like Membranes, Sci. Rep. 4, 7462 (2015).

      (28) L. F. Pineda De Castro, M. Dopson, and R. Friedman, Biological Membranes in Extreme Conditions: Simulations of Anionic Archaeal, PLoS One 11, e0155287 (2016).

      (29) M. Bulacu, X. Périole, and S. J. Marrink, In Silico Design of Robust Bolalipid Membranes, Biomacromolecules 13, 196 (2012).

      (30) C. H. Davis, H. Nie, and N. V. Dokholyan, Insights into thermophilic archaebacterial membrane stability from simplified models of lipid membranes, Phys. Rev. E 75, 051921 (2007).

      (31) S. Dey and J. Saha, Minimal Coarse-Grained Modeling toward Implicit Solvent Simulation of Generic Bolaamphiphiles, J. Phys. Chem. B 124, 2938 (2020).

      (32) I. R. Cooke and M. Deserno, Solvent-free model for self-assembling fluid bilayer membranes: Stabilization of the fluid phase based on broad attractive tail potentials, J. Chem. Phys. 123, 224710 (2005).

      (33) P. L.-G. Chong, U. Ayesa, V. Prakash Daswani, and E. C. Hur, On Physical Properties of Tetraether Lipid Membranes: Effects of Cyclopentane Rings, Archaea 2012, 1 (2012).

      (34) A. P. Thompson, H. M. Aktulga, R. Berger, D. S. Bolintineanu, W. M. Brown, P. S. Crozier, P. J. in ’t Veld, A. Kohlmeyer, S. G. Moore, T. D. Nguyen, R. Shan, M. J. Stevens, J. Tranchida, C. Trott, and S. J. Plimpton, LAMMPS - a flexible simulation tool for particle-based materials modeling at the atomic, meso, and continuum scales, Comput. Phys. Commun. 271, 108171 (2022).

      (35) A. Stukowski, Visualization and analysis of atomistic simulation data with ovito–the open visualization tool, Modelling and Simulation in Materials Science and Engineering 18, 015012 (2009).

      (36) E. R. May, A. Narang, and D. I. Kopelevich, Role of molecular tilt in thermal fluctuations of lipid membranes, Physical Review E 76, 021913 (2007).

      (37) W. Helfrich, Elastic Properties of Lipid Bilayers: Theory and Possible Experiments, Z. Naturforsch. C 28, 693 (1973).

      (38) M. Hamm and M. Kozlov, Elastic energy of tilt and bending of fluid membranes, Eur. Phys. J. E 3, 323 (2000).

      (39) M. Deserno, Fluid lipid membranes: From differential geometry to curvature stresses, Chemistry and Physics of Lipids 185, 11 (2015).

      (40) V. A. Harmandaris and M. Deserno, A novel method for measuring the bending rigidity of model lipid membranes by simulating tethers, The Journal of Chemical Physics 125, 204905 (2006).

      (41) M. Hu, J. J. Briguglio, and M. Deserno, Determining the Gaussian Curvature Modulus of Lipid Membranes in Simulations, Biophys. J. 102, 1403 (2012).

      (42) M. Deserno, Elastic deformation of a fluid membrane upon colloid binding, Phys. Rev. E 69, 031903 (2004), arXiv: cond-mat/0303656.

      (43) K. S. Makarova, M. Y. Galperin, and E. V. Koonin, Comparative genomic analysis of evolutionarily conserved but functionally uncharacterized membrane proteins in archaea: Prediction of novel components of secretion, membrane remodeling and glycosylation systems, Biochimie 118, 302 (2015).

      (44) A. Verchère, W.-L. Ou, B. Ploier, T. Morizumi, M. A. Goren, P. Bütikofer, O. P. Ernst, G. Khelashvili, and A. K. Menon, Light-independent phospholipid scramblase activity of bacteriorhodopsin from Halobacterium salinarum, Sci. Rep. 7, 9522 (2017).

      (45) T. B. H. Schroeder, G. Leriche, T. Koyanagi, M. A. Johnson, K. N. Haengel, O. M. Eggenberger, C. L. Wang, Y. H. Kim, K. Diraviyam, D. Sept, J. Yang, and M. Mayer, Effects of lipid tethering in extremophile-inspired membranes on H(+)/OH(-) flux at room temperature, Biophys. J. 110, 2430 (2016).

      (46) R. Xu, A. Dehghan, A.-C. Shi, and J. Zhou, Elastic property of membranes self-assembled from diblock and triblock copolymers, Chem. Phys. Lipids 221, 83 (2019).

      (47) Z. Dogic and S. Fraden, Ordered phases of filamentous viruses, Curr. Opin. Colloid Interface Sci. 11, 47 (2006).

      (48) E. Barry and Z. Dogic, Entropy driven self-assembly of nonamphiphilic colloidal membranes, Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 10348 (2010).

      (49) A. J. Balchunas, R. A. Cabanas, M. J. Zakhary, T. Gibaud, S. Fraden, P. Sharma, M. F. Hagan, and Z. Dogic, Equation of state of colloidal membranes, Soft Matter 15, 6791 (2019).

      (50) M. Saracco, P. Schaeffer, M. Tourte, S.-V. Albers, Y. Louis, J. Peters, B. Demé, S. Fontanay, and P. M. Oger, Bilayer-Forming Lipids Enhance Archaeal Monolayer Membrane Stability, Int. J. Mol. Sci. 26, 3045 (2025).

      (51) M. Amaral, archaeal_membranes : code and examples (2024), available at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo. 13934991.

      (52) M. F. Ergüder and M. Deserno, Identifying systematic errors in a power spectral analysis of simulated lipid membranes, The Journal of Chemical Physics 154, 214103 (2021).

      (53) J. Genova, N. Ulrih, V. Kralj-Iglič, A. Iglič, and I. Bivas, Bending Elasticity Modulus of Giant Vesicles Composed of Aeropyrum Pernix K1 Archaeal Lipid, Life 5, 1101 (2015).

      (54) M. Amaral, Archaeal Membranes: In Silico Modelling and Design, Ph.D. thesis, Institute of Science and Technology Austria (2024).

      (55) M. Pohlschroder, F. Pfeiffer, S. Schulze, and M. F. A. Halim, Archaeal cell surface biogenesis, FEMS Microbiol. Rev. 42, 694 (2018).

      (56) K. S. Makarova, N. Yutin, S. D. Bell, and E. V. Koonin, Evolution of diverse cell division and vesicle formation systems in Archaea, Nat. Rev. Microbiol. 8, 731 (2010).

      (57) C. W. Stairs and T. J. Ettema, The Archaeal Roots of the Eukaryotic Dynamic Actin Cytoskeleton, Curr. Biol. 30, R521 (2020).

      (58) B. Baum and D. A. Baum, The merger that made us, BMC Biol. 18, 72 (2020).

      (59) Z. Zeng, H. Chen, H. Yang, Y. Chen, W. Yang, X. Feng, H. Pei, and P. V. Welander, Identification of a protein responsible for the synthesis of archaeal membrane-spanning GDGT lipids, Nat. Commun. 13, 1545 (2022).

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      We appreciate the reviewer’s agreement that our data, "support most of the conclusions made”.

      With respect to Concerns raised by reviewer 1:

      (1) Although ectopically expressed PHD1 interacts with ectopically expressed RepoMan, there is no evidence that endogenous PHD1 binds to endogenous RepoMan or that PHD1 directly binds to RepoMan.

      We do not fully agree that this comment is accurate - the implication is that we only show interaction between two exogenously expressed proteins, i.e. both exogenous  PHD1 and RepoMan, when in fact we show that tagged PHD1 interacts with endogenous RepoMan. The major technical challenge here is the well known difficulty of detetcing endogenous PHD1 in such cell lines. We agree that co-IP studies do not prove that this interaction is direct and never claim to have shown this, though we do feel that a direct interaction is most likely, albeit not proven.

      (2) There is no genetic evidence indicating that PHD1 controls progression through mitosis by catalyzing the hydroxylation of RepoMan.

      We agree that our current study is primarily a biochemical and cell biological study, rather than a genetic study. Nonetheless, similar biochemical and cellular approaches have been widely used and validated in previous studies in mechanisms regulating cell cycle progression and we are confident in the conclusions drawn based on the data obtained so far.

      (3) Data demonstrating the correlation between dynamic changes in RepoMan hydroxylation and H3T3 phosphorylation throughout the cell cycle are needed.

      We agree that it will be very interesting to analyse in more detail the cell cycle dynamics of RepoMan hydroxylation and H3T3 phosphorylation - along with other cell cycle parameters. We view this as outside the scope of our present study and are actively engaged in raising the additional funding needed to pursue such future experiments.

      (4) The authors should provide biochemical evidence of the difference in binding ability between RepoMan WT/PP2A and RepoMan P604A/PP2A.

      Here again we agree that it will be very interesting to analyse in future the detailed binding interactions between wt and mutant RepoMan and other interacting proteins, including PP2A. We view this as outside the scope of our present study and are actively engaged in raising the additional funding needed to pursue such future experiments.

      (5) PHD2 is the primary proline hydroxylase in cells. Why does PHD1, but not PHD2, affect RepoMan hydroxylation and subsequent control of mitotic progression? The authors should discuss this issue further.

      We agree with the main point underlining this comment, i.e., that there are still many things to be learned concerning the specific roles and mechanisms of the different PHD enzymes in vivo. We look forward to addressing these questions in future studies.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments that our manuscript uses biochemical and imaging tools to delineate a key mechanism in the regulation of the progression of the cell cycle and their appreciation that our experiments performed are, 'conclusive with well-designed controls.'

      With respect to the specific Concern raised by reviewer 2:

      Lack of in vitro reconstitution and binding data.

      We agree that it will be very interesting to pursue in vitro reconstitution studies and detailed binding data. We view this as outside the scope of our present study and are actively engaged in raising the additional funding needed to pursue such future experiments.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments that our study, “is a comprehensive molecular and cell biological characterisation of the effects of P604 hydroxylation by PHD1 on RepoMan, a regulatory subunit of the PPIgamma complex” and their conclusion that, “we should have no question about the validity of the PHD1-mediated hydroxylation”.

      With respect to the specific Concern raised by reviewer 3:

      Reliance on a Proline-Alanine mutation in RepoMan to mimic an unhydroxylatable protein. The mutation will introduce structural alterations, and inhibition or knockdown of PHD1 would be necessary to strengthen the data on how hydroxylates regulate chromatin loading and interactions with B56/PP2A.

      We do not agree that we rely solely on analysis of the single site pro-ala mutatin in RepoMan for our conclusions, since we also present a raft of additional experimental evidence, including knock-down data and experiments using both fumarate and FG. We would also reference the data we present on RepoMan in the parallel study by Jiang et al, which has also been reviewed by eLife and is currently available on biorxiv (doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2025.05.06.652400). Of course we agree with the reviewer that even although the muatnt RepoMan features only a single amino acid change, this could still result in undetermined structural effects on the RepoMan protein that could conceivably contribute, at least in part, to some of the phenotypic effects observed. Hopefully future studies will help to clarify this.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Thank you for the extensive response to my comments and questions.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The Fmr1/Fxr2 double KO mice are not well described in the Introduction.

      We have changed the sentence in the introduction to clarify that in Zhang et al ., 2008 they used a mouse lacking both the Fmr1 gene and its paralog Fxr2.

      (3) The Authors decided not to discuss the potential translation of the present study to human patients, despite their final conclusion statement.

      The paragraph below has been added to the end of the discussion:

      “Translational Implications”

      The present findings support the view that circadian disruption is not merely a downstream consequence of disease processes but actively contributes to symptom expression. Hence, the possibility that interventions designed to reinforce circadian rhythms can hold therapeutic value for individuals with FXS and related neurodevelopmental conditions. Given that sleep and circadian dysfunction are detectable early in development and are predictive of more severe clinical phenotypes, circadian-based interventions may be particularly beneficial if applied during periods of heightened neural plasticity. Importantly, time-restricted feeding represents a relatively low-cost, non-invasive strategy that could be feasibly implemented in realworld settings. Further translational work is needed to evaluate whether the mechanistic links identified here—between circadian misalignment, immune dysregulation, and behavioral impairments—are conserved in humans, and similar approaches can be implemented for clinical use.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Li and colleagues describes the impact of deficiency on the DKGα and ζ on Treg cells and follicular responses. The experimental approach is based on the characterization of double KO mice that show the emergence of autoimmune manifestations that include the production of autoantibodies. Additionally, there is an increase in Tfh cells, but also Tfr cells in these mice deficient in both DKGα and ζ. Although the observations are interesting, the interpretation of the observations is difficult in the absence of data related to single mutations. While a supplementary figure shows that the autoimmune manifestations are more severe in the DKGα and ζ deficient mice, prior observations show that a single DKGα deficiency has an impact on Treg homeostasis. As such, the contribution of the two chains to the overall phenotype is hard to establish.

      Strengths:

      Well-conducted experiments with informative mouse models with defined genetic defects.

      Weaknesses:

      The major weakness is the lack of clarity concerning what can be attributed to simultaneous DKGα and ζ deficiency versus deficiency on DKGα or ζ alone.

      Some interpretations are also not conclusively supported by data.

      We appreciate the reviewer 1’s positive comments about our manuscript and for the suggestion to include DGKα‑ or DGKζ‑single‑knockout (SKO) Tregs for the mechanistical studies. Unfortunately, performing this sound simple but truly extensive experiment would exceed our current budget and personnel capacity. Importantly, it is well known that DGKα and DGKζ act redundantly or synergistically in T cells, with single loss producing minimal or partial phenotypes compared with the double knockout. The comprehensive mechanistic data already presented for DGKαζ‑DKO Tregs therefore capture the combined functional and mechanistical deficit that is most relevant to DGK functions in Treg biology, and they support the conclusions drawn in this manuscript. The reviewer also pointed out some interpretation issues such as CD25 down regulation in Tfr cells and some minor issues. We appreciate the reviewer’s expertise and have revised the text and discussion accordingly.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Li et al investigate the combined role of diacylglycerol (DAG) kinases (DGK) α and ζ in Foxp3+ Treg cells function that prevent autoimmunity. The authors generated DGK α and ζ Treg-specific double knockout mice (DKO) by crossing Dgkalpha-/- mice to DgKzf and Foxp3YFPCre/+ mice. The resulting "DKO" mice thus lack DGK α in all cells and DGK ζ in Foxp3+Treg cells. The authors show that the DKO mice spontaneously develop autoimmunity, characterized by multiorgan inflammatory infiltration and elevated anti-double-strand DNA (dsDNA), -single-strand DNA (ssDNA), and -nuclear autoantibodies. The authors attribute the DKO mice phenotype to Foxp3+Treg dysfunction, including accelerated conversion into "exTreg" cells with pathogenic activity. Interestingly, the combined deficiency of DGK α and ζ seems to release Treg cell dependence on CD28-mediated costimulatory signals, which the authors show by crossing their DKO mice to CD28-/- mice (TKO mice), which also develop autoimmunity.

      Strengths:

      The phenotypes of the mutant mice described in the manuscript are striking, and the authors provide a comprehensive analysis of the functional processes altered by the lack of DGKs.

      Weaknesses:

      One aspect that could be better explored is the direct role of "ex-Tregs" in causing pathogenesis in the models utilized.

      However, overall, this is an important report that makes a significant addition to the understanding of DAG kinases in Treg cell biology.

      We greatly appreciate reviewer 2’s positive comments about the manuscript. The data we presented in the manuscript show that DGKαζDKO Tregs but not WT Tregs are able to trigger autoimmunity in T cell deficient mice in the presence of WT CD4 T cells support that DGKαζDKO Tregs are pathogenic. Reviewer 2 suggested to test the direct role of DGKαζDKO Treg/ex-Tregs in the pathogenesis of autoimmune diseases in the absence of conventional T cells. This is really an interesting idea that we will test it in the future should recourse for executing the experiment become available.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      When you search for something, you need to maintain some representation (a "template") of that target in your mind/brain. Otherwise, how would you know what you were looking for? If your phone is in a shocking pink case, you can guide your attention to pink things based on a target template that includes the attribute 'pink'. That guidance should get you to the phone pretty effectively if it is in view. Most real-world searches are more complicated. If you are looking for the toaster, you will make use of your knowledge of where toasters can be. Thus, if you are asked to find a toaster, you might first activate a template of a kitchen or a kitchen counter. You might worry about pulling up the toaster template only after you are reasonably sure you have restricted your attention to a sensible part of the scene.

      Zhou and Geng are looking for evidence of this early stage of guidance by information about the surrounding scene in a search task. They train Os to associate four faces with four places. Then, with Os in the scanner, they show one face - the target for a subsequent search. After an 8 sec delay, they show a search display where the face is placed on the associated scene 75% of the time. Thus, attending to the associated scene is a good idea. The questions of interest are "When can the experimenters decode which face Os saw from fMRI recording?" "When can the experimenters decode the associated scene?" and "Where in the brain can the experimenters see evidence of this decoding? The answer is that the face but not the scene can be read out during the face's initial presentation. The key finding is that the scene can be read out (imperfectly but above chance) during the subsequent delay when Os are looking at just a fixation point. Apparently, seeing the face conjures up the scene in the mind's eye.

      This is a solid and believable result. The only issue, for me, is whether it is telling us anything specifically about search. Suppose you trained Os on the face-scene pairing but never did anything connected to the search. If you presented the face, would you not see evidence of recall of the associated scene? Maybe you would see the activation of the scene in different areas and you could identify some areas as search specific. I don't think anything like that was discussed here.

      You might also expect this result to be asymmetric. The idea is that the big scene gives the search information about the little face. The face should activate the larger useful scene more than the scene should activate the more incidental face, if the task was reversed. That might be true if the finding is related to a search where the scene context is presumed to be the useful attention guiding stimulus. You might not expect an asymmetry if Os were just learning an association.

      It is clear in this study that the face and the scene have been associated and that this can be seen in the fMRI data. It is also clear that a valid scene background speeds the behavioral response in the search task. The linkage between these two results is not entirely clear but perhaps future research will shed more light.

      It is also possible that I missed the clear evidence of the search-specific nature of the activation by the scene during the delay period. If so, I apologize and suggest that the point be underlined for readers like me.

      We have added text related to this issue, particularly in the discussion (page 19, line 6), and have also added citations of studies in humans and non-human primates showing a causal relationship between preparatory activity in prefrontal and visual cortex and visual search performance (page 6, line 16).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work is one of the best instances of a well-controlled experiment and theoretically impactful findings within the literature on templates guiding attentional selection. I am a fan of the work that comes out of this lab and this particular manuscript is an excellent example as to why that is the case. Here, the authors use fMRI (employing MVPA) to test whether during the preparatory search period, a search template is invoked within the corresponding sensory regions, in the absence of physical stimulation. By associating faces with scenes, a strong association was created between two types of stimuli that recruit very specific neural processing regions - FFA for faces and PPA for scenes. The critical results showed that scene information that was associated with a particular cue could be decoded from PPA during the delay period. This result strongly supports the invoking of a very specific attentional template.

      Strengths:

      There is so much to be impressed with in this report. The writing of the manuscript is incredibly clear. The experimental design is clever and innovative. The analysis is sophisticated and also innovative. The results are solid and convincing.

      Weaknesses:

      I only have a few weaknesses to point out.<br /> This point is not so much of a weakness, but a further test of the hypothesis put forward by the authors. The delay period was long - 8 seconds. It would be interesting to split the delay period into the first 4seconds and the last 4seconds and run the same decoding analyses. The hypothesis here is that semantic associations take time to evolve, and it would be great to show that decoding gets stronger in the second delay period as opposed to the period right after the cue. I don't think this is necessary for publication, but I think it would be a stronger test of the template hypothesis.

      We conducted the suggested analysis, and we did not find clear evidence of differences in decoding scene information between the earlier and later portions of the delay period. This may be due to insufficient power when the data are divided, individual differences in when preparatory activation is the strongest, or truly no difference in activation over the delay period. More details of this analysis can be found in the supplementary materials (page 12, line 16; Figure S1).

      Type in the abstract "curing" vs "during."

      Fixed.

      It is hard to know what to do with significant results in ROIs that are not motivated by specific hypotheses. However, for Figure 3, what are the explanations for ROIs that show significant differences above and beyond the direct hypotheses set out by the authors?

      We added reasoning for the other a priori ROIs in the introduction (page 4, line 26). There is substantial evidence suggesting that frontoparietal areas are involved in cognitive control, attentional control, and working memory. The ROIs we selected from frontal and parietal cortex are based on parcels within resting state networks defined by the s17-network atlases (Schaefer et al., 2018). The IFJ was defined by the HCP-MMP1 (Glasser et al., 2016). These regions are commonly used in studies of attention and cognitive control, and the exact ROIs selected are described in the section on “Regions of interest (ROI) definition”. While we have the strongest hypothesis for IFJ based on relatively recent work from the Desimone lab, the other ROIs in lateral frontal cortex and parietal cortex, are also well documented in similar studies, although the exact computation being done by these regions during tasks can be hard to differentiate with fMRI.\

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The manuscript contains a carefully designed fMRI study, using MVPA pattern analysis to investigate which high-level associate cortices contain target-related information to guide visual search. A special focus is hereby on so-called 'target-associated' information, that has previously been shown to help in guiding attention during visual search. For this purpose the author trained their participants and made them learn specific target-associations, in order to then test which brain regions may contain neural representations of those learnt associations. They found that at least some of the associations tested were encoded in prefrontal cortex during the cue and delay period.

      The manuscript is very carefully prepared. As far as I can see, the statistical analyses are all sound and the results integrate well with previous findings.

      I have no strong objections against the presented results and their interpretation.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      One bit of trivia. In the abstract, you should define IFJ on its first appearance in the text. You get to that a bit later.

      Fixed.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I really don't have much to suggest, as I thought that this was a clearly written report that offered a clever paradigm and data that supported the conclusions. My only suggestion would be to split the delay period activity and test whether the strength of the template evolves over time. Even though fMRI is not the best tool for this, still you would predict stronger decoding in the second half of the delay period

      Please see above for our response to the same comment.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I would just like to point out some minor aspects that might be worth improving before publishing this work.

      Abstract: While in general, the writing is clear and concise, I felt that the abstract of the manuscript was particularly hard to follow, probably because the authors at some point re-arranged individual sentences. For example, they write in line 12 about 'the preparatory period', but explain only in the following sentence that the preparatory period ensues 'before search begins'. This made it a bit hard to follow the overall logic and I think could easily be fixed. 

      We have addressed this comment and updated the abstract.

      Also in the abstract: 'The CONTENTS of the template typically CONTAIN...' sounds weird, no? Also, 'information is used to modulate sensory processing in preparation for guiding attention during search' sounds like a very over-complicated description of attentional facilitation. I'm not convinced either whether the sequence is correct here. Is the information really used to (first) modulate sensory processing (which is a sort of definition of attention in itself) to (then) prepare the guidance of attention in visual search?

      We have addressed this comment and updated the abstract.

      The sentence in line 7, 'However, many behavioral studies have shown that target-associated information is used to guide attention,...' (and the following sentence) assumes that the reader is somewhat familiar with the term 'target-associations'. I'm afraid that, for a naive reader, this term may only become fully understandable once the idea is introduced a bit later when mentioning that participants of the study were trained on face-scene pairings. I think it could help to give some very short explanation of 'target-associations' already when it is first mentioned. The term 'statistically co-occurring object pairs', for example, could be of great help here.

      Thank you for the suggestion. We have added it to the abstract.

      page 2, line 22: 'prefrotnal'

      Fixed.

      page 2, line 24/25: 'information ... can SUPPLANT (?) ... information'. (That's also a somewhat unfortunate repetition of 'information')

      Fixed.

      page 4, line 23-25: 'Working memory representations in lateral prefrontal and parietal regions are engaged in cognitive control computations that ARE (?) task non-specific but essential to their functioning'

      Fixed.

      page 7, line 1: maybe a comma before 'suggesting'?

      Fixed.

      page 7, line 14-16: Something seems wrong with this sentence: 'The distractor face was a race-gender match, which we previously FOUND MADE (?) target discrimination difficult enough to make the scene useful for guiding attention'

      We have addressed this comment and rewritten this part (now on page 7, line 18).

      Results / Discussion sections:

      In several figures, like in Fig3A, the three different IFJ regions, are grouped separately from the other frontal areas, which makes sense given the special role IFJ plays for representing task-related templates. However, IFJ is still part of PFC. I think it would be more correct to group the other frontal areas (like FEF vLPFC etc.) as 'Other Frontal' or even 'Other PFC'.

      We have made the changes based on the reviewer’s suggestion.

      In some of the Figures, e.g. Fig 3 and 5, I had the impression that the activation patterns of some conditions in vLPFC were rather close to the location of IFJ, which is just a bit posterior. I think I remember that functional localisers of IFJ can actually vary quite a bit in localisation (see e.g. in the Baldauf/Desimone paper). Also, I think it has been shown in the context of other regions, like the human FEF that its position when defined by localisation tasks is not always nicely and fully congruent with the respective labels in an atlas like the Glasser atlas. It might help to take this in consideration when discussing the results, particularly since the term vLPFC is a rather vague collection of several brain parcels and not a parcel name in the Glasser atlas. Some people might even argue that vLPFC in the broad sense contains IFJ, similar to how 'Frontal' contains IFJ (see above). How strong of a point do the authors want to make about activation in IFJ versus in vlPFC?

      We have now added text discussing the inability to truly differentiate between subregions of IFJ and other parts of vLPFC in the methods section on ROIs (page 25, line 13) and in the discussion (page 18, line 25). However, one might think that it is even more surprising given the likely imprecision of ROI boundaries that we see distinct patterns between the subregions of IFG defined by Glasser HCP-MMP1 and the other vLPFC regions defined by the 17-network atlases. We do not wish to overstate the precision of IFJ regions, but note the ROI results within the context of the larger literature. We are sure that our findings will have to be reinterpreted when newer methods allow for better localization of functional subregions of the vLPFC in individuals.

      Given that the authors nicely explain in the introduction how important templates are in visual search, and given that FEF has such an important role in serially guiding saccades through visual search templates, I think it would be worth discussing the finding that FEF did not hold representation of these targets. Of course, this could be in part due to the specific task at hand, but it may still be interesting to note in the Discussion section that here FEF, although important for some top-down attention signals, did not keep representations of the 'search' templates. Is it because there is no spatial component to the task at hand (like proposed in Bedini 2021)?

      We have now added text directly addressing this point and citing the Bedini et al. paper in the discussion (page 18, line 18). Besides our current findings, the relationship between IFJ and FEF is really interesting and will hopefully be investigated more in the future.

      Page 18, line 5: 'we the(N) associated...'

      Fixed.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study by Li et al., the authors re-investigated the role of cDC1 for atherosclerosis progression using the ApoE model. First, the authors confirmed the accumulation of cDC1 in atherosclerotic lesions in mice and humans. Then, in order to examine the functional relevance of this cell type, the authors developed a new mouse model to selectively target cDC1. Specifically, they inserted the Cre recombinase directly after the start codon of the endogenous XCR1 gene, thereby avoiding off-target activity. Following validation of this model, the authors crossed it with ApoE-deficient mice and found a striking reduction of aortic lesions (numbers and size) following a high-fat diet. The authors further characterized the impact of cDC1 depletion on lesional T cells and their activation state. Also, they provide in-depth transcriptomic analyses of lesional in comparison to splenic and nodal cDC1. These results imply cellular interactions between lesion T cells and cDC1. Finally, the authors show that the chemokine XCL1, which is produced by activated CD8 T cells (and NK cells), plays a key role in the interaction with XCR1-expressing cDC1 and particularly in the atherosclerotic disease progression.<br /> Strengths:

      The surprising results on XCL1 represent a very important gain in knowledge. The role of cDC1 is clarified with a new genetic mouse model.

      Thank you

      Weaknesses:

      My criticism is limited to the analysis of the scRNAseq data of the cDC1. I think it would be important to match these data with published data sets on cDC1. In particular, the data set by Sophie Janssen's group on splenic cDC1 might be helpful here (PMID: 37172103; https://www.single-cell.be/spleen_cDC_homeostatic_maturation/datasets/cdc1). It would be good to assign a cluster based on the categories used there (early/late, immature/mature, at least for splenic DC).

      Thank you very much for your help. Using the scRNA seq data of Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 sorted from ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> mice, we re-annotated the populations, following the methodology proposed by Sophie Janssen's group. These results are presented in Figure S9 and Figure S10 and described in detail in the Results and Discussion section.

      Please refer to the Results section from line 264 to 284: “Using the scRNA seq data of Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 sorted from hyperlipidemic mice, we annotated the 10 populations as shown in Figure S9A, following the methodology from a previous study [41]. Ccr7<sup>+</sup> mature cDC1s (Cluster 3, 7 and 9) and Ccr7- immature cDC1s (remaining clusters) were identified across cDC1 cells sorted from aorta, spleen and lymph nodes (Figure S9B). Further stratification based on marker genes reveals that Cluster 10 is the pre-cDC1, with high expression level of CD62L (Sell) and low expression level of CD8a (Figure S9C). Cluster 6 and 8 are the proliferating cDC1s, which express high level of cell cycling genes Stmn1 and Top2a (Figure S9D). Cluster 1 and 4 are early immature cDC1s, and cluster 2 and 5 are late immature cDC1s, according to the expression pattern of Itgae, Nr4a2 (Figure S9E). Cluster 9 cells are early mature cDC1s, with elevated expression of Cxcl9 and Cxcl10 (Figure S9F). Cluster 3 and 7 as late mature cDC1s, characterized by the expression of Cd63 and Fscn1 (Figure S9G). As shown in Figure 5C and Figure S9, the 10 populations displayed a major difference of aortic cDC1 cells that lack in pre-cDC1s (cluster 10) and mature cells (cluster 3, 7 and 9). Interestingly, in hyperlipidemic mice splenic cDC1 possess only Cluster 3 as the late mature cells while the lymph node cDC1 cells have two late mature populations namely Cluster 3 and Cluster 7. In further analysis, we also compared splenic cDC1 cells from HFD mice to those from ND mice. As shown in Figure S10, HFD appears to impact early immature cDC1-1 cells (Cluster 1) and increases the abundance of late immature cDC1 cells (Cluster 2 and 5), regardless of the fact that all 10 populations are present in two origins of samples. We also found that Tnfaip3 and Serinc3 are among the most upregulated genes, while Apol7c and Tifab are downregulated in splenic cDC1 cells sorted from HFD mice”.  

      Please refer to the Discussion section from line 380 to 385: “Based on the maturation analysis of the cDC1 scRNA seq data [41], our findings suggest that the aortic cDC1 cells display a major difference from those of spleen and lymph nodes by lacking the mature clusters, whereas lymph node cDC1 cells contain an additional Fabp5<sup>+</sup> S100a4<sup>+</sup> late mature Cluster. Our results also suggest that hyperlipidemia contributes to alteration in early immature cDC1 and in the abundance of late immature cDC1 cells, which was associated with dramatic change in gene expression of Tnfaip3, Serinc3, Apol7c and Tifab”.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This study investigates the role of cDC1 in atherosclerosis progression using Xcr1Cre-Gfp Rosa26LSL-DTA ApoE-/- mice. The authors demonstrate that selective depletion of cDC1 reduces atherosclerotic lesions in hyperlipidemic mice. While cDC1 depletion did not alter macrophage populations, it suppressed T cell activation (both CD4+ and CD8+ subsets) within aortic plaques. Further, targeting the chemokine Xcl1 (ligand of Xcr1) effectively inhibits atherosclerosis. The manuscript is well-written, and the data are clearly presented. However, several points require clarification:

      (1) In Figure 1C (upper plot), it is not clear what the Xcr1 single-positive region in the aortic root represents, or whether this is caused by unspecific staining. So I wonder whether Xcr1 single-positive staining can reliably represent cDC1. For accurate cDC1 gating in Figure 1E, Xcr1+CD11c+ co-staining should be used instead.

      The observed false-positive signal in the wavy structures within immunofluorescence Figure 1C (upper panel) results from the strong autofluorescence of elastic fibers, a major vascular wall component (alongside collagen). This intrinsic property of elastic fibers is a well-documented confounder in immunofluorescence studies [A, B].

      In contrast, immunohistochemistry (IHC) employs an enzymatic chromogenic reaction (HRP with DAB substrate) that generates a brown precipitate exclusively at antigen-antibody binding sites. Importantly, vascular elastic fibers lack endogenous enzymatic activity capable of catalyzing the DAB reaction, thereby preventing this source of false positivity in IHC.

      Given that Xcr1 is exclusively expressed on conventional type 1 dendritic cells [C], and considering that IHC lacks the multiplexing capability inherent to immunofluorescence for antigen co-localization, single-positive Xcr1 staining reliably identifies cDC1s in IHC results.

      [A] König, K et al. “Multiphoton autofluorescence imaging of intratissue elastic fibers.” Biomaterials vol. 26,5 (2005): 495-500. doi:10.1016/j.biomaterials.2004.02.059

      [B] Andreasson, Anne-Christine et al. “Confocal scanning laser microscopy measurements of atherosclerotic lesions in mice aorta. A fast evaluation method for volume determinations.” Atherosclerosis vol. 179,1 (2005): 35-42. doi:10.1016/j.atherosclerosis.2004.10.040

      [C] Dorner, Brigitte G et al. “Selective expression of the chemokine receptor XCR1 on cross-presenting dendritic cells determines cooperation with CD8+ T cells.” Immunity vol. 31,5 (2009): 823-33. doi:10.1016/j.immuni.2009.08.027

      (2) Figure 4D suggests that cDC1 depletion does not affect CD4+/CD8+ T cells. However, only the proportion of these subsets within total T cells is shown. To fully interpret effects, the authors should provide:

      (a) Absolute numbers of total T cells in aortas.

      (b) Absolute counts of CD4+ and CD8+ T cells.

      Thanks for your suggestions. We agree that assessing both proportions and absolute numbers in Figure 4 provides a more complete picture of the effects of cDC1 depletion on T cell populations. Furthermore, we also add the absolute count of cDC1 cells and total T cells, and CD44 MFI (mean fluorescence intensity) in CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells in Figure 4, and supplemented corresponding textual descriptions in the revised manuscript.

      Please refer to the Results section from line 183 to 187: “Subsequently, we assessed T cell phenotype in the two groups of mice. While neither the frequencies nor absolute counts of aortic CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells differed significantly between two groups of mice (Figure 4D-F), CD69 frequency and CD44 MFI (Mean Fluorescence Intensity), the T cell activation markers, were significantly reduced in both CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells from Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 depleted mice compared to controls (Figure 4G and H)”.

      (3) How does T cell activation mechanistically influence atherosclerosis progression? Why was CD69 selected as the sole activation marker? Were other markers (e.g., KLRG1, ICOS, CD44) examined to confirm activation status?

      We sincerely appreciate these insightful comments. As extensively documented in the literature, activated effector T cells (both CD4+ and CD8+) critically promote plaque inflammation and instability through their production of pro-inflammatory cytokines (particularly IFN-γ and TNF-α), which drive endothelial activation, exacerbate macrophage inflammatory responses, and impair smooth muscle cell function [A].

      In our study, we specifically investigated the role of cDC1 cells in atherosclerosis progression. Our key findings demonstrate that cDC1 depletion attenuates T cell activation (as shown by reduced CD69/CD44 expression) and that this reduction in activation is functionally linked to the observed decrease in atherosclerosis burden in our model. 

      Regarding CD44 as an activation marker, we performed quantitative analyses of CD44 mean fluorescence intensity (MFI) in aortic T cells (Figure 4). Importantly, the MFI of CD44 was significantly lower on both CD4+ and CD8+ T cells from Xcr1<sup>Cre-Gfp</sup> Rosa26<sup>LSL-DTA</sup> ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> mice compared to the control ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> mice (data shown below), which is consistent with the result of CD69 in Figure 4. We added the related description in the Result section.

      Please refer to the Results section from line 185 to 187 “CD69 frequency and CD44 MFI (Mean Fluorescence Intensity), the T cell activation markers, were significantly reduced in both CD4+ and CD8+ T cells from Xcr1+ cDC1 depleted mice compared to controls (Figure 4G and H)”.

      Similarly, MFI of CD44 was significantly lower on both CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells from Xcl1<sup>–/–</sup> ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> mice compared to the control ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> mice (data shown below), which is consistent with the result of CD69 in Figure 7. We also added the related description in the Result section.

      Please refer to the Results section from line 308 to 309 “Crucially, CD69<sup>+</sup> frequency and CD44 MFI remained comparable in both aortic CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells between two groups (Figure 7D-F).”

      [A] Hansson, Göran K, and Andreas Hermansson. “The immune system in atherosclerosis.” Nature immunology vol. 12,3 (2011): 204-12. doi:10.1038/ni.2001

      (4) Figure 7B: Beyond cDC1/2 proportions within cDCs, please report absolute counts of: Total cDCs, cDC1, and cDC2 subsets. Figure 7D: In addition to CD4+/CD8+ T cell proportions, the following should be included:

      (a) Total T cell numbers in aortas

      (b) Absolute counts of CD4+ and CD8+ T cells.

      Thanks for your suggestions. We have now included in Figure 7 the absolute counts of cDC, cDC1, and cDC2 cells, along with CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells in aortic tissues. Additionally, we provide the corresponding CD44 mean fluorescence intensity (MFI) measurements for both CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cell populations. We added the related description in the Result section.

      Please refer to the Results section from line 303 to 311: “The flow cytometric results illustrated that both frequencies and absolute counts of Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 cells in the aorta were significantly reduced, but cDCs and cDC2 cells from Xcl1<sup>–/–</sup> ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> were comparable with that from ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> (Figure 7A-C). Moreover, in both lymph node and spleen, the absolute numbers of pDC, cDC1 and cDC2 from Xcl1<sup>–/–</sup> ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> were comparable with that from ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> (Figure S11). Crucially, CD69<sup>+</sup> frequency and CD44 MFI remained comparable in both aortic CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells between two groups (Figure 7D-F). However, aortic CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells exhibited reduced frequency and absolute count, while CD4<sup>+</sup> T cells showed increased frequency but unchanged counts in Xcl1<sup>–/–</sup> ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> mouse versus controls (Figure 7G and H).”

      (5) cDC1 depletion reduced CD69+CD4+ and CD69+CD8+ T cells, whereas Xcl1 depletion decreased Xcr1+ cDC1 cells without altering activated T cells. How do the authors explain these different results? This discrepancy needs explanation.

      We sincerely appreciate your professional and insightful comments regarding the mechanistic relationship between cDC1 depletion and T cell activation. Direct cDC1 depletion in the Xcr1<sup>Cre-Gfp</sup> Rosa26<sup>LSL-DTA</sup> ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> micmodel removes both recruited and tissue-resident cDC1s, eliminating their multifunctional roles in antigen presentation, co-stimulation and cytokine secretion essential for T cell activation. In contrast, Xcl1 depletion reduces, but does not eliminate cDC1 migration into plaques. Furthermore, alternative chemokine axes (e.g., CCL5/CCR5, CXCL9/CXCR3, BCL9/BCL9L) may partially rescue cDC1 recruitment [13, 68, 69], and non-cDC1 APCs (e.g., monocytes, cDC2s) may compensate for T cell activation [55, 70]. We emphasize that Xcl1 depletion specifically failed to alter T cell activation in hyperlipidemic ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> mice. However, its impact may differ in other pathophysiological contexts due to compensatory mechanisms. We thank you again for highlighting this nuance, which strengthens our mechanistic interpretation. We have added these points to the discussion section and included new references.

      Please refer to the Discussion section from line 407 to 413: “Notably, while complete ablation of Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1s impaired T cell activation, reduction of Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 recruitment via Xcl1 deletion did not significantly compromise this process. This discrepancy may arise through compensatory mechanisms: alternative chemokine axes (e.g., CCL5/CCR5, CXCL9/CXCR3, BCL9/BCL9L) may partially rescue Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 homing [13, 68, 69], while non-cDC1 antigen-presenting cells (e.g., monocytes, cDC2s) may sustain T cell activation [55, 70]. Furthermore, tissue-specific microenvironment factors could potentially modulate its role in other diseases.”. [13] Eisenbarth, S C. “Dendritic cell subsets in T cell programming: location dictates function.” Nature reviews. Immunology vol. 19,2 (2019): 89-103. doi:10.1038/s41577-018-0088-1 [55] Brewitz, Anna et al. “CD8+ T Cells Orchestrate pDC-XCR1+ Dendritic Cell Spatial and Functional Cooperativity to Optimize Priming.” Immunity vol. 46,2 (2017): 205-219. doi:10.1016/j.immuni.2017.01.003 [68] de Oliveira, Carine Ervolino et al. “CCR5-Dependent Homing of T Regulatory Cells to the Tumor Microenvironment Contributes to Skin Squamous Cell Carcinoma Development.” Molecular cancer therapeutics vol. 16,12 (2017): 2871-2880. doi:10.1158/1535-7163.MCT-17-0341.[69] He F, Wu Z, Liu C, Zhu Y, Zhou Y, Tian E, et al. Targeting BCL9/BCL9L enhances antigen presentation by promoting conventional type 1 dendritic cell (cDC1) activation and tumor infiltration. Signal Transduct Target Ther. 2024;9(1):139. Epub 2024/05/30. doi: 10.1038/s41392-024-01838-9. PubMed PMID: 38811552; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC11137111.[70] Böttcher, Jan P et al. “Functional classification of memory CD8(+) T cells by CX3CR1 expression.” Nature communications vol. 6 8306. 25 Sep. 2015, doi:10.1038/ncomms9306.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 32 - The authors might want to add that the mouse model leads to a "constitutive" depletion of cDC1.

      Thanks for your advice, we have revised the sentence as follows.

      Please refer to the Results section from line 31 to 33: “we established Xcr1<sup>Cre-Gfp</sup> Rosa26<sup>LSL-DTA</sup> ApoE<sup>–/–</sup> mice, a novel and complex genetic model, in which cDC1 was constitutively depleted in vivo during atherosclerosis development”.

      (2) Line 187-188: The authors claim that T cell activation was "inhibited" if cDC1 was depleted. The data shows that the T cells were less activated, but there is no indication of any kind of inhibition; this should be corrected.

      Thanks for your advice, we have revised the sentence as follows.

      Please refer to the Results section from line 183 to 187: “Subsequently, we assessed T cell phenotype in the two groups of mice. While neither the frequencies nor absolute counts of aortic CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells differed significantly between two groups of mice (Figure 4D-F), CD69 frequency and CD44 MFI (Mean Fluorescence Intensity), the T cell activation markers, were significantly reduced in both CD4<sup>+</sup> and CD8<sup>+</sup> T cells from Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 depleted mice compared to controls (Figure 4G and H)”.

      (3) Why are some splenic DC clusters absent in LNs and vice versa? This is not obvious to this reviewer and should at least be discussed.

      We appreciate the insightful question regarding the absence of certain splenic DC clusters in LNs. This phenomenon in Figure 5 aligns with the 'division of labor' paradigm in dendritic cell biology: tissue microenvironments evolve specialized DC subsets to address local immunological challenges. The absence of universal clusters reflects functional adaptation, not technical artifacts. We acknowledge that this tissue-specific heterogeneity warrants further discussion and have expanded our analysis to address this point in the discussion part of our manuscript.

      Please refer to the Discussion section from line 375 to 385: “This pronounced tissue-specific compartmentalization of Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 subsets may related to multiple mechanisms including developmental imprinting that instructs precursor differentiation into transcriptionally distinct subpopulations [62], and microenvironmental filtering through organ-specific chemokine axes (e.g., CCL2/CCR2 in spleen) selectively recruits receptor-matched subsets [63, 64]. This spatial specialization optimizes pathogen surveillance for local immunological challenges. Based on the maturation analysis of the cDC1 scRNA seq data [41], our findings suggest that the aortic cDC1 cells display a major difference from those of spleen and lymph nodes by lacking the mature clusters, whereas lymph node cDC1 cells contain an additional Fabp5<sup>+</sup> S100a4<sup>+</sup> late mature Cluster. Our results also suggest that hyperlipidemia contributes to alteration in early immature cDC1 and in the abundance of late immature cDC1 cells, which was associated with dramatic change in gene expression of Tnfaip3, Serinc3, Apol7c and Tifab”.

      [62]. Liu Z, Gu Y, Chakarov S, Bleriot C, Kwok I, Chen X, et al. Fate Mapping via Ms4a3-Expression History Traces Monocyte-Derived Cells. Cell. 2019;178(6):1509-25 e19. Epub 2019/09/07. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.08.009. PubMed PMID: 31491389.

      [63]. Bosmans LA, van Tiel CM, Aarts S, Willemsen L, Baardman J, van Os BW, et al. Myeloid CD40 deficiency reduces atherosclerosis by impairing macrophages' transition into a pro-inflammatory state. Cardiovasc Res. 2023;119(5):1146-60. Epub 2022/05/20. doi: 10.1093/cvr/cvac084. PubMed PMID: 35587037; PubMed Central PMCID: PMCPMC10202633.

      [64]. Mildner A, Schonheit J, Giladi A, David E, Lara-Astiaso D, Lorenzo-Vivas E, et al. Genomic Characterization of Murine Monocytes Reveals C/EBPbeta Transcription Factor Dependence of Ly6C(-) Cells. Immunity. 2017;46(5):849-62 e7. Epub 2017/05/18. doi: 10.1016/j.immuni.2017.04.018. PubMed PMID: 28514690.

      [41]. Bosteels V, Marechal S, De Nolf C, Rennen S, Maelfait J, Tavernier SJ, et al. LXR signaling controls homeostatic dendritic cell maturation. Sci Immunol. 2023;8(83):eadd3955. Epub 2023/05/12. doi: 10.1126/sciimmunol.add3955. PubMed PMID: 37172103.

      (4) The authors should discuss how XCL1 could impact lesional cDC1 and T cell abundance. Notably, preDCs do not express XCR1, and T cells express XCL1 following TCR activation. Is there a recruitment or local proliferation defect of cDC1 in the absence of XCL1? Could there also be a role for NK cells as a potential source of XCL1?

      We appreciate your insightful questions regarding the differential effects of Xcl1 on cDC1s and T cells. Xcl1 primarily mediates the recruitment of mature cDC1s. Our data demonstrate that Xcl1 deletion significantly reduces aortic cDC1 abundance, which correlates with a concomitant decrease in CD8<sup>+</sup> T cell numbers within the aorta. These findings strongly suggest that the Xcl1-Xcr1 axis plays a regulatory role in T cell accumulation in aortic plaques.

      Consistent with prior studies [A, B], cDC1 recruitment can occur in the absence of Xcl1 which echoes our findings that cDC1 cells were still found in Xcl1 knockout aortic plaque but in lower abundance. It is very true that further studies are required to address how the Xcl1 dependent and independent cDC1 cells activate T cells and if they possess capability of proliferation in tissue differentially. We have added these points in discussion section.

      Please refer to the Discussion section from line 407 to 415: “Notably, while complete ablation of Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1s impaired T cell activation, reduction of Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 recruitment via Xcl1 deletion did not significantly compromise this process. This discrepancy may arise through compensatory mechanisms: alternative chemokine axes (e.g., CCL5/CCR5, CXCL9/CXCR3, BCL9/BCL9L) may partially rescue Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 homing [13, 68, 69], while non-cDC1 antigen-presenting cells (e.g., monocytes, cDC2s) may sustain T cell activation [55, 70]. Furthermore, tissue-specific microenvironment factors could potentially modulate its role in other diseases. In summary, our findings identify Xcl1 as a potential therapeutic target for atherosclerosis therapy, though its cellular origins and regulation of lesional Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 and T cells dynamics require further studies”.

      In literatures, Xcl1 are expressed in NK cells and subsects of T cells, and NK cells can be a potential source of Xcl1 during atherosclerosis which deserve further investigations [A, C, D].

      [A] Böttcher, Jan P et al. “NK Cells Stimulate Recruitment of cDC1 into the Tumor Microenvironment Promoting Cancer Immune Control.” Cell vol. 172,5 (2018): 1022-1037.e14. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2018.01.004

      [B] He, Fenglian et al. “Targeting BCL9/BCL9L enhances antigen presentation by promoting conventional type 1 dendritic cell (cDC1) activation and tumor infiltration.” Signal transduction and targeted therapy vol. 9,1 139. 29 May. 2024, doi:10.1038/s41392-024-01838-9

      [C] Woo, Yeon Duk et al. “The invariant natural killer T cell-mediated chemokine X-C motif chemokine ligand 1-X-C motif chemokine receptor 1 axis promotes allergic airway hyperresponsiveness by recruiting CD103+ dendritic cells.” The Journal of allergy and clinical immunology vol. 142,6 (2018): 1781-1792.e12. doi:10.1016/j.jaci.2017.12.1005

      [D] Winkels, Holger et al. “Atlas of the Immune Cell Repertoire in Mouse Atherosclerosis Defined by Single-Cell RNA-Sequencing and Mass Cytometry.” Circulation research vol. 122,12 (2018): 1675-1688. doi:10.1161/CIRCRESAHA.117.312513

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      There is a logical error in line 298. I suggest revising to: "Collectively, these data suggest that Xcl1 promotes atherosclerosis by recruiting Xcr1+ cDC1 cells, which subsequently drive T cell activation in lesions."

      Thanks for your advice. Since Xcl1 deficiency reduced both the frequencies and absolute counts of Xcr1+ cDC1 and CD8+ T cells in lesions without affecting T cell activation, we revised the sentence as you suggested.

      Please refer to the Results section from line 314 to 315: “Collectively, these data suggest that Xcl1 promotes atherosclerosis by recruiting Xcr1<sup>+</sup> cDC1 cells, and facilitating CD8<sup>+</sup> T cell accumulation in lesions”.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Major points

      (1) The authors discovered a novel regulation of the Hippo-YAP pathway by SARS-CoV-2 infection but did not address the pathological significance of this finding. It remains unclear why YAP downstream gene transcription needs to be inhibited in response to SARS-CoV-2 infection. Is this inhibition crucial for the innate immune response to SARS-CoV-2? The authors should re-analyze their snRNA-seq and bulk RNA-seq data described in Figure 1 to determine whether any of the affected YAP downstream genes are involved in this process.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to clarify the pathological significance of YAP pathway inhibition in SARS-CoV-2 infection. To address this, we re-analyzed our snRNA-seq and bulk RNA-seq datasets to determine whether YAP target genes overlap with known mediators of the innate immune response. As described in Fig. 1C, bulk RNA-seq revealed decreased expression of multiple YAP downstream targets linked to innate immune regulation (e.g., Thbs1, Ccl2, Axl, and Csf1) in SARS-CoV-2–infected cells in vitro.

      snRNA-seq of alveolar type I (AT1) cells from COVID-19 patients revealed a more complex landscape: While we observed reduced YAP activity overall (Fig. 1G), multiple YAP target genes involved in innate immunity and cytokine signaling were paradoxically elevated (Supplemental Fig. 1E). Several factors likelt explain these conflicting observations: 1. In the lung, AT1 cells (which are critical for gas exchange) may cell specifically respond to virus infection by upregulating genes related to immune response by other signaling pathway(s); 2. In vivo, SARS-CoV-2 infection triggers a surge in cytokines, chemokines, and other local factors that can differentially modulate YAP binding sites and thus affect its downstream targets, a complexity not fully captured in vitro; 3. YAP is highly sensitive to mechanical signals and tissue architecture. The 3D structure of altered cell–cell junctions in infected lung tissue, and fluid shear stress in the alveolar space could shape YAP target gene transcription differently from simplified monolayer cell cultures.

      We have expanded the results section of the new version to include the above points. We also acknowledge that ongoing and future work is needed to delineate the exact molecular and tissue-specific pathways through which YAP inhibition confers a potential advantage in combating SARS-CoV-2.

      (2) The authors concluded that helicase activity is required for NSP13-induced inhibition of YAP transcriptional activity based on mutation studies (Figure 3B). This finding is somewhat confusing, as K131, K345/K347, and R567 are all essential residues for NSP13 helicase activity while mutating K131 did not affect NSP13's ability to inhibit YAP (Figure 3B). Additionally, there are no data showing exactly how NSP13 inhibits the YAP/TEAD complex through its helicase function. This point was also not reflected in their proposed working model (Figure 4H).

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concerns regarding the helicase‐dependent inhibition of YAP by NSP13, particularly the roles of K131, K345/K347, and R567. Based on published structural and biochemical studies, each of these residues uniquely supports helicase function (1): K131 is crucial for stabilizing the NSP13 stalk region by interacting with S424. Substituting K131 with alanine (K131A) reduces helicase efficiency but does not completely abolish it; K345/K347 are key DNA‐binding residues, and mutating both (K345A/K347A) largely prevents NSP13 from binding DNA, thus eliminating unwinding. R567 is critical for ATP hydrolysis, and the R567A mutant retains DNA binding capacity but fails to unwind it. In Fig. 3B, K131A suppresses YAP transactivation to nearly the same extent as wild‐type NSP13, suggesting that partial helicase activity is sufficient for complete YAP/TEAD inhibition. Conversely, the K345A/K347A and R567A mutants show markedly diminished repression, underscoring the importance of DNA binding and ATP hydrolysis.

      As the new Fig. 4J illustrates, NSP13 must bind DNA and hydrolyze ATP to unwind nucleic acids. This helicase‐dependent process likely enables NSP13 to remodel chromatin structure by binding TEAD and properly organize YAP repressors at YAP/TEAD complex to prevent YAP/TEAD transactivation. In support of this mechanism, the K345A/K347A mutant, unable to anchor to DNA, fails to repress YAP and slightly increases YAP‐driven transcription (Fig. 3B), presumably by mislocalizing YAP repressors. Likewise, the ATPase‐dead R567A can bind DNA but does not unwind and remodel chromatin to recruit YAP repressors, resulting in a loss of YAP suppression (Fig. 3B and 3F). Our revised model demonstrates that both DNA binding and ATP‐dependent unwinding are essential for NSP13 to suppress YAP transcriptional activity. We have updated the results, discussion, and model accordingly.

      (3) The proposed model that NSP13 binds TEAD4 to recruit repressor proteins and inhibits YAP/TEAD downstream gene transcription (Figure 4H) needs further characterization. Second, NSP13 is a DNA-binding protein, and its nucleic acid-binding mutant K345A/K347A failed to inhibit YAP transcriptional activity (Figure 3B). The authors should investigate whether NSP13 could bind to the TEAD binding sequence or the nearby sequence on the genome to modulate TEAD's DNA binding ability. Third, regarding the identified nuclear repressors, the authors should validate the interaction of NSP13 with the ones whose loss activates YAP transcriptional activity (Figure 4G). Lastly, why can't NSP13 bind TEAD4 in the cytoplasmic fractionation if both NSP13 and TEAD4 are detected there (Figure 3B)? This finding indicates their interaction is not a direct protein-protein interaction but is mediated by something in the nucleus, such as genomic DNA.

      (1) Low TEAD expression in HEK293T cells: Our IP-MS experiments were performed in HEK293T cells, which, according to the Human Protein Atlas, express TEAD1–4 at comparatively low levels (TEAD1: 16.5, TEAD2: 16.4, TEAD3: 4.9, TEAD4: 38.7 nTPM). In contrast, HeLa cells, where we successfully validated NSP13-mediated YAP suppression (Fig. 4H, Supplementary Fig.5B-D), show higher expression of these TEAD isoforms (TEAD1: 97.1, TEAD2: 27.3, TEAD3: 12.2, TEAD4: 48.1 nTPM). Therefore, insufficient TEAD abundance in HEK293T cells may limit the sensitivity needed to detect TEAD–NSP13 interactions in our proteomic screens.

      (2) Transience and potential DNA dependence: Our co-immunoprecipitation (co-IP) experiments (Fig. 4B, Supplementary Fig.4C-E) indicated that NSP13–TEAD4 binding is low-affinity. Under standard IP-MS conditions (which typically do not include chemical cross-linkers or nucleic acids to stabilize transient complexes), weak or short-lived interactions can be lost during washes or sample processing.

      (3). Additional supporting evidence: We carefully checked our IP-MS data and found that the well-known TEAD binding proteins, including CTBP1/2 and GATA4, were pulled down, suggesting TEAD’s absence does not rule out an NSP13–TEAD association.

      (3a) We acknowledge that our NSP13 immunoprecipitation–mass spectrometry (IP-MS) did not identify any TEAD proteins (Fig. 4G and IP-MS tables). Several factors likely contributed to this outcome:

      (3b) We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s insightful suggestion. While we agree that mapping NSP13 occupancy at individual TEAD-binding motifs is valuable, we respectfully consider this to be beyond the scope of the current study. Biochemical and structural work on coronavirus NSP13 shows that it recognizes nucleic‑acid substrates primarily through their 5′ single‑stranded overhang and duplex architecture, not through a defined base sequence(2, 3). Accordingly, our data (Fig. 3B and 3F) indicate that DNA binding ability, rather than recognition of a specific motif, enables NSP13 to perform its helicase activity in proximity to TEAD and recruit repressors. Moreover, the DNA‑binding mutant K345A/K347A and the ATPase‑dead mutant R567A both fail to suppress YAP/TEAD transcription despite retaining the ability to interact with TEAD (Fig. 3B). These loss‑of‑function phenotypes demonstrate that NSP13’s chromatin engagement and unwinding activity, rather than sequence‑restricted targeting, are essential for repression. For these reasons, motif‑specific binding assays were not pursued in this revision, but we clarified in the discussion that NSP13’s DNA engagement is likely structural or TEAD-dependent, rather than sequence‑directed. We also highlighted this as an important avenue for future investigation.

      (3c) To validate the NSP13 interacting proteins from our IP-MS data, we generated plasmids expressing several candidates (CCT3, SMARCD1, EIF4A1, LMNA, TTF2, and YY2) and performed co-IP assays. As predicted, we confirmed the robust interaction between NSP13 and TEAD (Supplemental Fig. 5E). However, these putative nuclear repressors exhibited weak binding to NSP13 compared with TEAD4, suggesting that NSP13 associates with them indirectly, possibly as part of a larger multiprotein complex or depending on the chromatin structure, rather than via direct protein–protein interaction (Fig. 4J).

      (3d) We appreciate the reviewer’s question. To investigate whether their association might be DNA‐dependent, we performed co‐IP experiments using nuclear lysates in the presence or absence of various nucleases: Universal Nuclease (which degrades all forms of DNA and RNA), DNase I (which cleaves both single‐ and double‐stranded DNA), and RNase H (which selectively cleaves the RNA strand in RNA/DNA hybrids). Our findings revealed that nucleic acid removal did not disrupt the NSP13/TEAD4 interaction (Supplemental Fig.4E), indicating that their binding is not solely mediated by DNA or RNA.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Specific comments and suggestions for improvement of the manuscript:

      (1) NSP13 has been reported to block, in a helicase-dependent manner, episomal DNA transcription (PMID: 37347173), raising questions about the effects observed on the data shown from the HOP-Flash and 8xGTIIC assays. It would be valuable to demonstrate the specificity of the proposed effect of NSP13 on TEAD activation by YAP (versus broad effects on reporter assays) and also to show that NSP13 reduces the function of endogenous YAP-TEAD transcriptional activity (i.e., does ectopic NSP13 expression reduce the expression of YAP induced TEAD target genes in cells).

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comments and have carefully revisited the conclusions from the published paper(4) (PMID: 37347173), which reported that NSP13 suppresses episomal DNA transcription, as evidenced by reduced Renilla luciferase (driven by the herpes simplex virus thymidine kinase promoter) and GFP expression upon co‐expression with NSP13. For our experiments, we used a dual‐luciferase assay with Renilla luciferase (under the same promoter) as an internal control. After re-examining our raw Renilla luciferase data (now provided in the supplemental Excel file “Supporting data value”), we found that while 100 ng of NSP13 did not affect Renilla luciferase levels, 400 ng of NSP13 reduced them by approximately 50% relative to the YAP5SA‐only group (Supplemental Fig.2B, Fig.3C-D). We observed a similar reduction with NSP13 truncation mutants—an outcome not fully consistent with the published study (Supplemental Fig.3D, PMID: 37347173). However, unlike their finding of robust episomal DNA suppression, our data indicate that the K345A/K347A mutant of NSP13, which lacks DNA‐binding ability, completely lost its suppressive effect (Fig.3B).

      We performed additional Notch reporter assays to address the concern that NSP13 might nonspecifically inhibit episomal DNA transcription (including the HOP‑Flash and 8×GTIIC reporters). These experiments revealed that co‑expression of NSP13 with NICD (Notch intracellular domain) does not suppress Notch signaling (Supplemental Fig. 2C), indicating that NSP13 does not globally block all reporter systems. To evaluate whether NSP13 reduces endogenous YAP‑TEAD activity, we transiently overexpressed NSP13 WT and its R567A mutant in HeLa cells. However, bulk RNA‑seq and qPCR analyses did not reveal a clear decrease in YAP target genes, possibly due to the low transfection efficiency (< 50%, Supplemental Fig.4D). Interestingly, we observed that YAP5SA was predominantly retained in the nucleus upon NSP13 or R567A co‑expression, suggesting that NSP13 (or together with its interacting partners) restricts YAP5SA cytoplasmic shuttling. Future studies will involve stable cell lines expressing NSP13 WT or R567A to better characterize the mechanisms driving YAP5SA nuclear retention and clarify how NSP13 specifically suppresses YAP activity.

      (2) While the IP-MS experiment may have revealed new regulators of TEAD activity, the data presented are preliminary and inconclusive. No interactions are validated and beyond slight changes in TEAD reporter activity following knockdown, no direct links to YAP-TEAD are demonstrated, and no link to NPS13 was shown. Also, no details are provided about the methods used for the IP-MS experiment, raising some concerns about potential false positive associations within the data.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback regarding our IP-MS findings and acknowledge that additional validation is required to establish definitive links between the identified putative regulators, YAP-TEAD, and NSP13. We have taken the following steps (and plan further experiments) to address these concerns:

      (2a) Co-IP validation: Same with the answer for Reviewer #1 (3c), we generated plasmids expressing several top candidate interactors from the IP-MS data (CCT3, SMARCD1, EIF4A1, LMNA, TTF2, and YY2) and performed direct co-IP assays in a more controlled setting. The results indicated that these putative NSP13 interactors had weaker binding compared to TEAD4, implying that NSP13 may associate with them as part of a larger complex or depending on the chromatin structure rather than through a direct protein–protein interaction (Fig. 4J).

      (2b) qPCR validation: Beyond reporter assays for evaluating YAP transactivation after the candidate YAP suppressor knockdown (Fig. 4H and Supplemental Fig. 5C), we performed qPCR to detect YAP activation on endogenous YAP-TEAD target genes (e.g., CTGF CYR61, and AMOTL2) after CCT3 knockdown. Expression of CTGF and CYR61 was higher compared to control (Supplemental Fig. 5D), strengthening the case for an interaction relevant to YAP-TEAD signaling.

      (2c) To investigate how NSP13‐interacting proteins link to the YAP/TEAD complex, we examined the IP‑MS dataset and identified several well‐known YAP and TEAD binding partners, including CTBP1/2 (TEAD‐binding), GATA4 (TEAD‐binding), and multiple 14‐3‐3 isoforms (YWHAZ/YWHAB/YWHAH/YWHAQ, YAP binding). These findings suggest that NSP13 may form a larger nuclear complex with YAP/TEAD and associated cofactors. In the future, we will determine whether these putative TEAD regulators also interact with NSP13 under various conditions (e.g., in the presence or absence of DNA) and whether co‐expression of NSP13 influences their association with YAP or TEAD. This approach will clarify how NSP13 might leverage these factors to regulate YAP‐TEAD function.

      (2e) For the mass spectrometry experiments, HEK293T cells were transfected with Flag‐YAP1, HA‐NSP13, or Flag‐YAP1 + HA‐NSP13 according to the manufacturer’s standard protocols. After nuclear extraction and lysis, the supernatant was incubated with HA magnetic beads to immunoprecipitate (IP) NSP13. The IP samples were subsequently analyzed by mass spectrometry to identify NSP13‐associated proteins (Fig. 4F). Each experimental condition was performed in duplicate to ensure reproducibility. We included an appropriate negative control (Flag‐YAP1) and stringent data‐filtering criteria to minimize false positives. We apologize for not including these details in our original Methods section; in this revised manuscript, we have fully described the number of replicates, the controls used, and our data analysis pipelines.

    1. Author response:

      We thank the reviewers for their thorough evaluation and constructive feedback on our manuscript.

      We think that their valuable suggestions will strengthen the manuscript and help us clarify several important points.

      All reviewers acknowledged the importance of our theoretical results and network classification in making pattern formation analysis a more tractable problem. At the same time, they have also raised a number of important concerns that we shall carefully consider.

      A. A major clarification that the reviewers found important concerns the definition of non-trivial pattern transformations and its generalization to higher dimensions. In this regard, the reviewers’ comments are:

      Reviewer #1:

      (on non-trivial pattern transformations):

      (3) All modelling is confined to one spatial dimension, and the very definition of a "non-trivial" transformation is framed in terms of peak positions along a line, which clearly must be reformulated for higher dimensions. It's well-known that diffusions in 1, 2, and 3 dimensions are also dramatically different, so the relevance of the three-class taxonomy to real multicellular tissues remains unclear, or at least should be explained in more detail. Reviewer #2 (on non-trivial pattern transformations):

      (5) The definition of non-trivial pattern formation is provided only in the Supplementary Information, despite its central importance for interpreting the main results. It would significantly improve clarity if this definition were included and explained in the main text. Additionally, it remains unclear how the definition is consistently applied across the different initial conditions. In particular, the authors should clarify how slope-based measures are determined for both the random noise and sharp peak/step function initial states. Furthermore, the authors do not specify how the sign function is evaluated at zero. If the standard mathematical definition sgn(0)=0 is used, then even a simple widening of a peak could fulfill the criterion for nontrivial pattern transformation.

      We agree with Reviewer #2 that including a more detailed definition of non-trivial pattern transformation in the main text would enhance the clarity of the paper. The one-dimensional (1D) definition currently provided in the Supplementary Information was chosen because all computations presented therein involve exclusively one-dimensional patterns. However, we acknowledge that this definition, as it was, did not have a totally unambiguous generalization  to higher dimensions. Therefore, in a revised version of the manuscript, we will incorporate an expanded definition applicable to higher-dimensional cases.

      This general definition of a non-trivial pattern transformation should make no reference to the sign of spatial derivatives of either the initial or resulting patterns. Specifically, a pattern transformation is considered non-trivial if it satisfies the following criteria:

      - It is heterogeneous: The resulting pattern is heterogeneous in space.

      - It is rearranging: The arrangement of critical points (i.e. peaks, valleys and saddle points in a gene product concentration) along the domain in the resulting pattern of a gene product is different to the arrangement of critical points in its initial pattern. This includes the emergence of new critical points, the disappearance of existing ones, or the spatial displacement of critical points from one location to another.

      - It is non-replicating: The spatial arrangement of critical points in the pattern of one gene product must differ from that of any other upstream gene product.

      Nonetheless, our two initial patterns are spatially discontinuous functions: in homogeneous initial patterns, the white noise is discontinuous by definition; and for the spike and spike+homogeneous initial patterns, we use sharp spikes defined by the rectangular function, which is discontinuous at the spike boundaries. Therefore, the aforementioned definition should be supplemented with the following two ad hoc assumptions:

      - Homogeneous initial patterns do not comprise any critical point. White noise in this type of initial patterns represents small thermodynamic fluctuations around the steady state and, for the purpose of pattern transformation, this is equivalent to a constant concentration along the domain.

      - Spike and spike+homogeneous initial patterns each contain a single critical point located at the center of the spike. The sharp spikes, modeled using the rectangular function, serve as a theoretical idealization to facilitate mathematical analysis. Once diffusion begins to act, these sharp boundaries are smoothed into differentiable gradients, maintaining a unique critical point at the center of the initial spike, which is the most relevant information for pattern transformation.

      Finally, it is worth recalling that our gene network classification is fundamentally based on an analysis of the dispersion relation associated with the gene network, and the construction of this dispersion relation is independent of the spatial dimensionality of the domain (i.e. it does not require assuming any specific number of dimensions). The fact that the description of this dispersion relation was in the SI may have been non-ideal for the understandability of the article and will, consequently, be moved to the main text in an upcoming version of the article. Thus, the gene networks that can lead to pattern transformation are the same in 1D, 2D or 3D. As for the resulting patterns, the broad description we provide also applies to any number of dimensions; these would be periodic, non periodic as in the amplified noise patterns or non periodic as in the hierarchic networks. For the latter notice that, except for boundary effects that we later discuss, the spike initial condition is radially symmetric and thus, the patterns resulting from it will also be radially symmetric. We will make this point more explicit in a revised version of the article, especially since, as suggested, this important portion of the Supplementary Information will be incorporated into the main text.

      Reviewer 2 suggests that with our definition of non-trivial pattern transformation, the simple widening of a concentration peak would constitute a non-trivial pattern transformation. This is not the case, as already shown in the figures as a example, since in a widening there is no change in the position of the critical point. A different situation applies if a wide and completely flat concentration peak (i.e. a plateau) forms. As we will explain in the coming version this is not possible because of requirement R5.

      We think that this clarification of the definition of non-trivial pattern transformation will also help clarify the next point (B below) since it would make it clearer that this article does not intend to explain which specific resulting pattern would arise from any given gene network.

      B. The main concern among these relates to the validity of our linearization of the model equations and the extension of the results obtained for the linear system to the fully nonlinear system. In this regard, the reviewers’ comments are:

      Reviewer #1:

      (on linearization):

      (2) A central step in the model formulation is the linearisation of the reaction term around a homogeneous steady state; higher-order kinetics, including ubiquitous bimolecular sinks such as A + B → AB, are simply collapsed into the Jacobian without any stated amplitude bound on the perturbations. Because the manuscript never analyses how far this assumption can be relaxed, the robustness of the three-class taxonomy under realistic nonlinear reactions or large spike amplitudes remains uncertain.

      Reviewer #2:

      (on linearization):

      (2) Most of the proofs presented in the Supplementary Information rely on linearized versions of the governing equations, and it remains unclear how these results extend to the fully nonlinear system. We are concerned that the generality of the conclusions drawn from the linear analysis may be overstated in the main text. For example, in Section S3, the authors introduce the concept of dynamic equivalence of transitive chains (Proposition S3.1) and intracellular transitive M-branching (Proposition S3.2), which pertains to the system's steady-state behavior. However, the proof is based solely on the linearized equations, without additional justification for why the result should hold in the presence of nonlinearities. Moreover, the linearized system is used to analyze the response to a "spike initial pattern of arbitrary height C" (SI Chapter S5.1), yet it is not clear how conclusions derived from the linear regime can be valid for large perturbations, where nonlinear effects are expected to play a significant role. We encourage the authors to clarify the assumptions under which the linearized analysis remains valid and to discuss the potential limitations of applying these results to the nonlinear regime.

      In this article, we address two main questions: first, which gene network topologies can give rise to non-trivial pattern transformations; and second, which broad types of resulting patterns can these gene network topologies give rise to resulting pattern. Thus, we are not intending to explain which exact resulting patterns would arise from any given gene network (i.e. a gene network topology with specific functions and interaction strengths or weights), a question for which non-linearities do indeed matter.

      For most known gene regulatory networks, available empirical information is typically limited to the nature of gene product regulations -indicating whether they act as activators or inhibitors- while details about the specific functional form of these regulations are rare. For instance, given two gene products, i and j, the network may indicate that i acts as an activator of j, implying that the concentration of j increases with that of i. However, this increase could follow a variety of functional forms: it may be quadratic (e.g., ), cubic (e.g., ), or any other function f j(gi). As we explain in the description of our model, we restrict our study to functions with a monotonicity constraint: higher concentrations of i lead to increased production of j (i.e., ).  In other words, a given gene interaction is always inhibitory or activatory, it does not change of sign. This monotonicity constraint corresponds to requirement (R5) in our main text. This requirement it is based on the biologically plausible idea that the complexity of gene regulation in development stems more from the topology of gene networks than from the complexity of the regulation by which a gene product may regulate another (i.e. we use simple monotonic functions).

      Question 1: A critical part to understand question 1 is in the dispersion relation that was explained in SI. From the reviewers’ comments it is clear that having this crucial part in the main text of an upcoming version of the article would improve understandability, specially for question 1.

      In brief, any pattern transformation requires the initial pattern to change. The trigger of such change is a change in the concentration of some gene product, either conceptualized as a noise fluctuation (in the homogeneous initial pattern) or a regulated change in a specific point (in the spike initial pattern). Mathematically, both can be conceptualized as perturbations and, for pattern transformation to be possible, such perturbation should grow so that the initial pattern becomes unstable and can change to another resulting pattern.

      If the perturbation is small, one can use the standard linear perturbation analysis in S6.2 of our Supplementary Information. In other words, the linear analysis is enough to ascertain if a small perturbation would grow or not. A gene network in which this will not happen would be unable to lead to pattern transformation, whichever the nonlinear part of f(g). In that sense, the linear approximation provides a necessary condition that any gene network needs to fulfill to lead to pattern transformation.

      However, the linear analysis would not ascertain whether a specific gene network will actually lead to pattern transformation (i.e., the condition is not sufficient). This, as well as the shape of the specific resulting pattern, may actually depend on the non-linear parts too. As we discuss, based on the dispersion relation, and other complementing arguments along the article, we can also get some insights on the possible patterns from the linear approximation alone (question 2). This arguments hold thanks to the imposition of requirements (R1-R5) on function f(g), which prevent strange behaviors stemming from the nonlinear part of the equation.

      The amplitude bound of perturbations mentioned by Reviewer #1 is addressed by requirements (R2) and (R4). Although the solution to the linear system predicts unbounded growth of unstable eigenmodes, the assume functions f(g) on which the nonlinear terms  eventually halt this growth, thereby ensuring the boundedness of solutions as imposed by (R4). This assumption on the nonlinear part is literally requirement R2 on f(g) in the main text.

      The transitive chains and branchings in section S3 of the Supplementary Information mentioned by the Reviewer #2 are topological properties of gene networks and therefore they influence only the linear part of the reaction-diffusion equations. This is why the proofs in that section are based on the linearized equations. We agree that clarifying this point in the text, as suggested by the reviewer, would improve the reader’s understanding of the section.

      Regarding Reviewer #2’s concerns about large perturbations, we acknowledge that the phrasing using “arbitrary height” may be confusing. For the homogeneous initial conditions these perturbations are assumed to be small because they are actually molecular noise (otherwise the initial condition could not be considered homogenous in the classical sense of developmental biology models). In the spike initial conditions in hierarchic networks the perturbation is not necessarily small. For the analysis provided in the SI we indeed assume that the perturbations are small enough for the linear approximation to be possible. Notice, however, that since these networks require an intracellular self-activating loop upstream of the first extracellular signal, the effective perturbation would rapidly grow to a value determined by such loop.

      In general the height of the initial spike does not affect the fact that hierarchic networks can lead to non-trivial pattern transformation. By definition these networks require the secretion of an extracellular signal from the cells in the spike (otherwise no change in gene product concentrations can occur over space). By definition this signal is not produced by any other cells and, thus, its concentration is governed by diffusion from the spike and its production in the cells in the spike. Thus, whichever the initial height of the spike and whichever the non-linearities in f(g), the signal’s concentration would decrease with the distance from the spike. As explained in the main text, this would lead to non-trivial pattern transformations if other general conditions are met. In general, the height of the initial perturbation can affect which specific pattern transformation would arise from a specific gene network but not which gene network topologies can lead to pattern transformation. This will be more clearly stated in an upcoming version of the article. C. In the following, we respond to the remaining concerns raised by the reviewers:

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) The Results section is difficult to follow. Key logical steps and network configurations are described shortly in prose, which constantly require the reader to address either SI or other parts of the text (see numerous links on the requirements R1-R5 listed at the beginning of the paper) to gain minimal understanding. As a result, a scientifically literate but non-specialist reader may struggle to grasp the argument with a reasonable time invested.

      We acknowledge that the current version of the main text may not be as clear as we intended. Initially, we believed that placing the more technical mathematical passages in the Supplementary Information would make the main text more accessible to readers. However, we agree with the reviewer that including some of these computations in the main text could improve clarity. We also believe that adding a summary table outlining all the model’s requirements would further contribute to that goal.

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) We have serious concerns regarding the validity of the simulation results presented in the manuscript. Rather than simulating the full nonlinear system described by Equation (1), the authors base their results on a truncated expansion (Equation S.8.2) that captures only the time evolution of small deviations around a spatially homogeneous steady state. However, it remains unclear how this reduced system is derived from the full equations specifically, which terms are retained or neglected and why- and how the expansion of the nonlinear function can be steady-state independent, as claimed. Additionally, in simulations involving the spike plus homogeneous initial condition, it is not evident -or, where equations are provided, it is not correct- that the assumed global homogeneous background actually corresponds to a steady state of the full dynamics. We elaborate on these concerns in the following:

      We believe there has been a misunderstanding regarding the presentation of the model equations (S8.2) used throughout our simulations. Accordingly, we agree that this relevant section of the Supplementary Information should be rewritten in a revised version of the manuscript to clarify this issue. Below, we address all the concerns raised by the reviewer.

      Equation (S8.2) represents the full nonlinear system described in Equation (1). While we recognize that the model may oversimplify real biological processes, its purpose is to illustrate our general statements about pattern formation rather than to capture any specific or detailed mechanism. In this context, model (S8.2) offers three key advantages for our goals: it allows rapid manipulation of gene network topology simply by modifying the matrix J, making it ideal for illustrating pattern formation across different network classes; it accommodates gene networks of arbitrary size -unlike other models, such as the classical Gierer-Meinhardt model, which are limited to two-element Turing or noise-amplifying networks-; and, due to the simplicity of its nonlinear terms, this model involves relatively few free parameters, facilitating the fine-tuning needed to identify parameter regions where non-trivial pattern transformations occur.

      Indeed, we find that the ability of model (S8.2) to illustrate our results despite having such simple nonlinear terms -bearing in mind that at least some nonlinearity is always necessary for selforganization- strongly supports the claim that the capacity of a gene network to produce pattern transformations is fully determined by the linear part of Equation (1). In this sense, nonlinear terms primarily influence the precise parameter values at which these transformations occur and contribute to shaping specific features of the resulting patterns.

      Model (S8.2) has been successfully employed in pattern formation studies elsewhere in the literature; accordingly, we provide relevant bibliographic references to support its widespread use.

      We believe the misunderstanding arises from our explanation of the biological interpretation of the model. As noted in the accompanying bibliography, the model is based on a general reactiondiffusion mechanism assuming the existence of a steady state. However, this conceptual reactiondiffusion framework is not the same as our Equation (1); rather, it was introduced by the original proponents of the model in the seminal paper cited in our text. In this context, Equation (S8.2) describes small concentration perturbations around that steady state, where the variables represent deviations in concentration relative to the general steady state.

      The aforementioned general steady state corresponds to the trivial equilibrium point g≡0 in equations (S8.2). Consequently, all our simulations based on model (S8.2) start from this steady state, to which we add white noise to generate homogeneous initial patterns or a sharp spike for the two types of spike initial patterns.

      It is also worth noting that Equations (S8.2) represent a non-dimensional model.

      It is assumed that the homogeneous steady states are given by g_i=0 and g_i=c_i, where 1/c_i = \mu_i or \hat{\mu}_i, independently of the specific network structure. However, the basis for this assumption is unclear, especially since some of the functions do not satisfy this condition -for example, f5 as defined below Eq. S8.10.5. Moreover, if g_i=c_i does not correspond to a true steady state, then the time evolution of deviations from this state is not correctly described by Eq. S8.2, as the zeroth-order terms do not vanish in that case.

      From the explanations above, it is important to distinguish two scales in the process: the scale of small perturbations, where equations (S8.2) apply; and the global scale, where the conceptual general reaction-diffusion system operates. Since the specific form of this general system does not affect equations (S8.2), we assume that it follows any of the models cited in the text, which yield a non-zero steady state at .

      In this sense, Equation (S8.2) represent a small concentration deviation of such global system and g(t ,x) is a relative concentration where g≡0 represents the steady-state at are concentrations above , and g<0 are concentrations below .

      As previously mentioned, simulations are performed using Equations (S8.2) on the basis of the equilibrium point g≡0. The result of these simulations is then superimposed on the non-zero steady state and presented in the figures along the article.

      Using the full model instead of the simplified Equations (S8.2) may result in slightly different resulting patterns, but it does not affect the gene network’s ability to produce pattern transformations, nor does it alter the main structural properties of the patterns—for example, the periodic nature of patterns generated by Turing networks.

      Additionally, the equations used contain only linear terms and a cubic degradation term for each species g_i, while neglecting all quadratic terms and cubic terms involving cross-species interactions (i≠j). An explanation for this selective truncation is not provided, and without knowledge of the full equation (f), it is impossible to assess whether this expansion is mathematically justified. If, as suggested in the Supplementary Information, the linear and cubic terms are derived from f, then at the very least, the Jacobian matrix should depend on the background steady-state concentration. However, the equations for the small deviation around a steady state (including the Jacobian matrix) used in the simulations appear to be independent of the particular steady state concentration.

      The Jacobian of Equation (S8.2) is independent of g because g represents a small perturbation around a steady state of a general reaction-diffusion system. Consequently, the matrix J corresponds to the Jacobian of the general system evaluated at that steady state. Evaluating the Jacobian of equations (S8.2) at the equilibrium point g≡0 -which represents the general steady state- recovers the matrix J.

      This is why we believe that the differences observed between the spike-only initial condition and the spike superimposed on a homogeneous background are not due to the initial conditions themselves, but rather result from a modified reaction scheme introduced through a questionable cutoff.

      "In simulations with spike initial patterns, the reference value g≡0 represents an actual concentration of 0 and therefore, we must add to (S8.2) a Heaviside function Φ acting of f (i.e., Φ(f(g))=f(g) if f(g)>0 , Φ(f(g))=0 if f(g){less than or equal to}0 ) to prevent the existence of negative concentrations for any gene product (i.e., g_i<0 for some i )." (SI chapter S8).

      This cutoff alters the dynamics (no inhibition) and introduces a different reaction scheme between the two simulations. The need for this correction may itself reflect either a problem in the original equations (which should fulfill the necessary conditions and prevent negative concentrations (R4 in main text)) or the inappropriateness of using an expanded approximation which assumes independence on the steady state concentration. It is already questionable if the linearized equations with a cubic degradation term are valid for the spike initial conditions (with different background concentration values), as the amplitude of this perturbation seems rather large.

      For homogeneous and spike+homogeneous initial conditions, we interpret equations (S8.2) as small perturbations around a non-zero steady state of a general reaction-diffusion system. For spike-only initial conditions, that steady state is zero. As we mention before, g≡0 will then represent such steady-state of zero concentration, g>0 are positive concentrations of the general system, and g<0 would represent unfeasible negative concentrations of the general system. Therefore, the use of a cutoff function to handle such initial conditions is justified. Moreover, this cutoff function is the same as the one employed in the reference general system cited in our paper.

      We acknowledge that the cutoff influences the simulations and accounts for the differences observed between spike and spike+homogeneous initial conditions. However, this distinction reflects what occurs in real biological systems, which is precisely why we differentiate these two types of initial states. For instance, the emergence of a periodic pattern in a noise-amplifying network depends critically on the formation of regions with concentrations below the steady state near the initial spike. Such regions can form in spike-plus-homogeneous initial patterns but not in spike-only initial patterns, where concentrations below the steady state would correspond to biologically unfeasible negative values.

      Lastly, we note that under the current simulation scheme, it is not possible to meaningfully assess criteria RH2a and RH2b, as they rely on nonlinear interactions that are absent from the implemented dynamics.

      It is explicitly stated in the relevant subsections of Section S7 in the Supplementary Information that, for the simulations involving RH2a and RH2b, the function f(g) in equation (S8.2) is modified by adding an ad hoc quadratic term to enable the assessment of these criteria.

      (3) Several statements in the main text are presented without accompanying proof or sufficient explanation, which makes it difficult to assess their validity. In some cases, the lack of justification raises serious doubts about whether the claims are generally true. Examples are:

      "For the purpose of clarity we will explain our results as if these cells have a simple arrangement in space (e.g., a 1D line or a 2D square lattice) but, as we will discuss, our results shall apply with the same logic to any distribution of cells in space." (Main text l.145-l.148).

      We believe that the confusion in this statement arises from the ambiguous use of the phrase “our results”. We will revise the text to provide a more precise description. Specifically, by “our results,” we refer to the conclusion that it is possible to determine whether a gene network leads to nontrivial pattern transformations based solely on its topology. This conclusion is independent of the dimensionality of space, as none of our arguments rely on assumptions specific to spatial dimensions. While one-dimensional examples are used for clarity and illustration, the underlying reasoning applies generally. In an improved version of the article, we will clarify this point explicitly and move relevant arguments from the Supplementary Information into the main text.

      Critically, our classification of gene networks is ultimately based on an argument concerning the dispersion relation associated with the network, and the construction of this dispersion relation is independent of the spatial dimensionality of the domain. In this sense, the networks identified in the text as capable of producing pattern transformations will be able to generate non-trivial pattern transformations in any spatial domain and in any number of dimensions. While the specific parameter values that permit such transformations may vary depending on the geometry and dimensionality of the domain, the existence of at least one such parameter set remains unaffected.

      The geometry of the domain can influence the specific form of the resulting patterns, but it does not alter the broader class of patterns (e.g., periodic patterns, peaks emerging around a spike, etc.) that a given gene network topology can produce. One such geometric influence, commonly observed in simulations, involves boundary effects. For example, structures such as peaks or rings forming near the boundaries may appear higher, broader, or spatially shifted compared to those arising in the central regions of the domain. However, we think a pattern consisting of a periodic train of peaks where only those near the boundary are slightly different can still be classified as a periodic pattern.

      "For any non-trivial pattern transformation (as long as it is symmetric around the initial spike), there exists an H gene network capable of producing it from a spike initial pattern." (Main text l.366f).

      A justification for this statement is provided shortly after the claim, although we acknowledge that the current explanation is somewhat cumbersome and would benefit from a clearer presentation in a revised version of the main text.

      A more detailed justification is provided in the Supplementary Information, based on three key ideas. First, any pattern (provided it is symmetric with respect to the initial spike) can be described as an arrangement of peaks with varying heights and spatial positions along a one-dimensional domain. Second, there exists a simple gene network—the diamond network—that, through parameter tuning, can produce two peaks of arbitrary height and symmetric position relative to the initial spike. Third, by placing multiple diamond networks positively upstream of a common gene product, that gene product can express peaks at each location where the upstream diamond networks induce them. Under mild additional conditions, this mechanism allows the formation of essentially any symmetric pattern. These mild conditions, along with a detailed analysis of the diamond network’s ability to generate peaks with controllable height and position, are discussed in the Supplementary Information.

      "In 2D there are no peaks but concentric rings of high gene product concentration centered around the spike, while in 3D there are concentric spherical shells." (Main text l. 447ff).

      This result pertains specifically to pattern transformations arising from spike initial patterns. As defined in the text, spike initial patterns are radially symmetric. Since diffusion preserves radial symmetry, pattern transformations from spike initial patterns in two or three dimensions reduce to effectively one-dimensional transformations along each radial direction. In this framework, each pair of concentration peaks symmetric with respect to the spike in one dimension corresponds to a ring surrounding the spike in two dimensions, and each ring in two dimensions becomes a hollow spherical shell around the spike in three dimensions.

      We agree that including a brief section in the Supplementary Information to clarify these subtleties would be helpful for readers to better understand the generalization of certain patterns to higher dimensions.

      (4) The study identifies one-signal networks and examines how combinations of these structures can give rise to minimal pattern-forming subnetworks. However, the analysis of the combinations of these minimal pattern-forming subnetworks remains relatively brief, and the manuscript does not explore how the results might change if the subnetworks were combined in upstream and downstream configurations. In our view, it is not evident that all possible gene regulatory networks can be fully characterized by these categories, nor that the resulting patterns can be reliably predicted. Rather, the approach appears more suited to identifying which known subnetworks are present within a larger network, without necessarily capturing the full dynamics of more complex configurations.

      We acknowledge that our explanation regarding the combination of sub-networks was relatively brief, and we intend to address this in a revised version. Our argument that combining sub-networks does not produce qualitatively new types of pattern transformations -beyond those already described- is based on the dispersion relation. Although this relation was only detailed in the Supplementary Information, it is central to our argument and will therefore be moved to the main text. Below, we provide an outline of this argument:

      Our study identifies two distinct behaviors of the principal branch of the dispersion relation at large wavenumbers. Based on this, gene networks capable of pattern formation can be classified into two categories: networks of the first kind, where the real part of the principal branch diverges to infinity as the wavenumber increases; and networks of the second kind, where the real part of the principal branch converges to a positive finite value for large wavenumbers. Naturally this argument applies to any gene network irrespectively of which, or how many, sub-networks are used to built it.

      Any gene regulatory network capable of pattern formation falls into one of these two categories. We identified that networks of the first kind contain at least one Turing sub-network, whereas networks of the second kind include either an H sub-network or a noise-amplifying sub-network. In this way, the primary objective of our study -namely, achieving a topological classification of gene regulatory networks capable of pattern formation- is fulfilled. It is important to note that while the dispersion relation provides broad information about the possible resulting patterns a gene network topology can produce (e.g., periodic versus noisy), it does not specify the exact patterns that emerge for each particular set of parameter values.

      Finally, regarding the shape of the resulting patterns, Figure S10 in the Supplementary Information exemplifies the notion that the behavior of combined networks can be understood as a combination of the individual behaviors of each constituent sub-network (note that the contribution of each type of sub-network in the resulting pattern is readily distinguishable). Consequently, we focus our detailed analysis on the patterning properties of the fundamental classes.

      (6) The manuscript lacks a clear and detailed explanation of the underlying model and its assumptions. In particular, it is not well-defined what constitutes a "cell" in the context of the model, nor is it justified why spatial features of cells -such as their size or boundaries- can be neglected. Furthermore, the concept of the extracellular space in the one-dimensional model remains ambiguous, making it unclear which gene products are assumed to diffuse.

      The size of cells is ignored in our model because we assume that they are small enough with respect to the total size of the domain that the space continuous reaction-diffusion equation (equation (1) in the main text) holds. Conceptually, one could understand cells in our model each of the pieces in an even partition of the domain into small subdomains surrounding each position x. This is anyway the standard procedure in most models of pattern formation by reaction-diffusion in embryonic development.

      For extracellular signals, we assume that g(t ,x) corresponds to the concentration of the signal in the extracellular space surrounding the cell located at position x. The extracellular space is any fluid medium for which Fick Laws apply and, therfore, the Fickian diffusion term in equation (1) is valid.

      For intracellular gene products, we assume that g(t ,x) corresponds to the concentration of such gene product within the cell at position x (if the gene product in hand is a transcription factor, for example), or on its surface (if it is a membrane-bound receptor). When collapsed in the continuous equations there is not such difference between being strictly within the cell or on its boundary. The only important fact is that these gene products cannot diffuse.

      Regarding cell boundaries, let us consider an extracellular signal s that regulates a transcriptor factor i within cells (in our model, i is an intracellular gene product). Such regulation shall be mediated by a membrane-bound receptor, which corresponds to intracellular gene product j. In terms of the gene regulatory network this is sji. Cell boundary effects mentioned by the reviewer should be encapsulated in the specific functional form of the regulation function f(g), but they have no effect in the actual topology of the network. Consequently, they are out of the scope of this study: as we mentioned before, considering different non-linear terms for f(g) will affect the parameter range for which a gene network is capable of producing non-trivial pattern transformations, but not their overall ability to produce non-trivial pattern transformations (i.e., the existence of at least one choice of model parameters for which such transformations take place).

      Finally, we would like to once again express our sincere gratitude to all reviewers for their insightful and constructive feedback. We are confident that the thorough peer review process will significantly enhance both the clarity and depth of our work. We greatly value the detailed comments provided and will carefully incorporate them in the preparation of a revised manuscript, which we intend to submit in the coming months.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors developed a sequence-based method to predict drug-interacting residues in IDP, based on their recent work, to predict the transverse relaxation rates (R2) of IDP trained on 45 IDP sequences and their corresponding R2 values. The discovery is that the IDPs interact with drugs mostly using aromatic residues that are easy to understand, as most drugs contain aromatic rings. They validated the method using several case studies, and the predictions are in accordance with chemical shift perturbations and MD simulations. The location of the predicted residues serves as a starting point for ligand optimization.

      Strengths:

      This work provides the first sequence-based prediction method to identify potential drug-interacting residues in IDP. The validity of the method is supported by case studies. It is easy to use, and no time-consuming MD simulations and NMR studies are needed.

      Weaknesses:

      The method does not depend on the information of binding compounds, which may give general features of IDP-drug binding. However, due to the size and chemical structures of the compounds (for example, how many aromatic rings), the number of interacting residues varies, which is not considered in this work. Lacking specific information may restrict its application in compound optimization, aiming to derive specific and potent binding compounds.

      We fully recognize that different compounds may have different interaction propensity profiles along the IDP sequence. In future studies, we will investigate compound-specific parameter values. The limiting factor is training data, but such data are beginning to be available.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this work, the authors introduce DIRseq, a fast, sequence-based method that predicts drug-interacting residues (DIRs) in IDPs without requiring structural or drug information. DIRseq builds on the authors' prior work looking at NMR relaxation rates, and presumes that those residues that show enhanced R2 values are the residues that will interact with drugs, allowing these residues to be nominated from the sequence directly. By making small modifications to their prior tool, DIRseq enables the prediction of residues seen to interact with small molecules in vivo.

      Strengths:

      The preprint is well written and easy to follow

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The DIRseq method is based on SeqDYN, which itself is a simple (which I do not mean as a negative - simple is good!) statistical predictor for R2 relaxation rates. The challenge here is that R2 rates cover a range of timescales, so the physical intuition as to what exactly elevated R2 values mean is not necessarily consistent with "drug interacting". Presumably, the authors are not using the helix boost component of SeqDYN here (it would be good to explicitly state this). This is not necessarily a weakness, but I think it would behove the authors to compare a few alternative models before settling on the DIRseq method, given the somewhat ad hoc modifications to SeqDYN to get DIRseq.

      Actually, the factors that elevate R2 are well-established. These are local interactions and residual secondary structures (if any). The basic assumption of our method is that intra-IDP interactions that elevate R2 convert to IDP-drug interactions. This assumption was supported by our initial observation that the drug interaction propensity profiles predicted using the original SeqDYN parameters already showed good agreement with CSP profiles. We only made relatively small adjustments to the parameters to improve the agreement. Indeed we did not apply the helix boost portion of SeqDYN to DIRseq, and will state as such. We will also compare DIRseq with several alternative models.

      Specifically, the authors previously showed good correlation between the stickiness parameter of Tesei et al and the inferred "q" parameter for SeqDYN; as such, I am left wondering if comparable accuracy would be obtained simply by taking the stickiness parameters directly and using these to predict "drug interacting residues", at which point I'd argue we're not really predicting "drug interacting residues" as much as we're predicting "sticky" residues, using the stickiness parameters. It would, I think, be worth the authors comparing the predictive power obtained from DIRseq with the predictive power obtained by using the lambda coefficients from Tesei et al in the model, local density of aromatic residues, local hydrophobicity (note that Tesei at al have tabulated a large set of hydrophobicity scores!) and the raw SeqDYN predictions. In the absence of lots of data to compare against, this is another way to convince readers that DIRseq offers reasonable predictive power.

      We will compare predictions of these various parameter sets, and summarize the results in a table.

      (2) Second, the DIRseq is essentially SeqDYN with some changes to it, but those changes appear somewhat ad hoc. I recognize that there is very limited data, but the tweaking of parameters based on physical intuition feels a bit stochastic in developing a method; presumably (while not explicitly spelt out) those tweaks were chosen to give better agreement with the very limited experimental data (otherwise why make the changes?), which does raise the question of if the DIRseq implementation of SeqDYN is rather over-parameterized to the (very limited) data available now? I want to be clear, the authors should not be critiqued for attempting to develop a model despite a paucity of data, and I'm not necessarily saying this is a problem, but I think it would be really important for the authors to acknowledge to the reader the fact that with such limited data it's possible the model is over-fit to specific sequences studied previously, and generalization will be seen as more data are collected.

      We have explained the rationale for the parameter tweaks, which were limited to q values for four amino-acid types, i.e., to deemphasize hydrophobic interactions and slightly enhance electrostatic interactions (p. 4-5). We will add that these tweaks were motivated by observations from MD simulations of drug interactions with a-syn (ref 20). As already noted in the response to the preceding comment, we will also present results for the original parameter values as well as for when the four q values are changed one at a time.

      (3) Third, perhaps my biggest concern here is that - implicit in the author's assumptions - is that all "drugs" interact with IDPs in the same way and all drugs are "small" (motivating the change in correlation length). Prescribing a specific lengthscale and chemistry to all drugs seems broadly inconsistent with a world in which we presume drugs offer some degree of specificity. While it is perhaps not unexpected that aromatic-rich small molecules tend to interact with aromatic residues, the logical conclusion from this work, if one assumes DIRseq has utility, is that all IDRs bind drugs with similar chemical biases. This, at the very least, deserves some discussion.

      The reviewer raises a very important point. In Discussion, we will add that it is important to further develop DIRseq to include drug-specific parameters when data for training become available.

      (4) Fourth, the authors make some general claims in the introduction regarding the state of the art, which appear to lack sufficient data to be made. I don't necessarily disagree with the author's points, but I'm not sure the claims (as stated) can be made absent strong data to support them. For example, the authors state: "Although an IDP can be locked into a specific conformation by a drug molecule in rare cases, the prevailing scenario is that the protein remains disordered upon drug binding." But is this true? The authors should provide evidence to support this assertion, both examples in which this happens, and evidence to support the idea that it's the "prevailing view" and specific examples where these types of interactions have been biophysically characterized.

      We will cite several studies showing that IDPs remain disordered upon drug binding.

      Similarly, they go on to say:

      "Consequently, the IDP-drug complex typically samples a vast conformational space, and the drug molecule only exhibits preferences, rather than exclusiveness, for interacting with subsets of residues." But again, where is the data to support this assertion? I don't necessarily disagree, but we need specific empirical studies to justify declarative claims like this; otherwise, we propagate lore into the scientific literature. The use of "typically" here is a strong claim, implying most IDP complexes behave in a certain way, yet how can the authors make such a claim? 

      Here again we will add citations to support the statement.

      Finally, they continue to claim:

      "Such drug interacting residues (DIRs), akin to binding pockets in structured proteins, are key to optimizing compounds and elucidating the mechanism of action." But again, is this a fact or a hypothesis? If the latter, it must be stated as such; if the former, we need data and evidence to support the claim. 

      We will add citations to both compound optimization and mechanism of action.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Major concerns:

      (1) Is the direct binding of MCAK to the microtubule cap important for its in vivo function?

      a.The authors claim that their "study provides mechanistic insights into understanding the end-binding mechanism of MCAK". I respectfully disagree. My concern is that the paper offers limited insights into the physiological significance of direct end-binding for MCAK activity, even in vitro. The authors estimate that in the absence of other proteins in vitro, ~95% of MCAK molecules arrive at the tip by direct binding in the presence of ~ physiological ATP concentration (1 mM). In cells, however, the major end-binding pathway may be mediated by EB, with the direct binding pathway contributing little to none. This is a reasonable concern because the apparent dissociation constant measured by the authors shows that MCAK binding to microtubules in the presence of ATP is very weak (69 uM). This concern should be addressed by 1) calculating relative contributions of direct and EB-dependent pathways based on the affinities measured in this and other published papers and estimated intracellular concentrations. Although there are many unknowns about these interactions in cells, a modeling-based analysis may be revealing. 2) the recapitulation of these pathways using purifying proteins in vitro is also feasible. Ideally, some direct evidence should be provided, e.g. based on MCAK function-separating mutants (GDP-Pi tubulin binding vs. catalytic activity at the curled protofilaments) that contribution from the direct binding of MCAK to microtubule cap in EB presence is significant.

      We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful comments.

      (1) We think that the end-binding affinity of MCAK makes a significant contribution for its cellular functions. To elucidate this concept, we now use a simple model shown in Supplementary Appendix-2 (see pages 49-51, lines 1246-1316). In this model, we simplified MCAK and EB1 binding to microtubule ends by considering only these two proteins while neglecting other factors (e.g. XMAP215). Specifically, we considered two scenarios: one in which both proteins freely diffuse in the cytoplasm and another where MCAK is localized to specific cellular structures, such as the centrosome or centromere. Based on the modeling results, we argue that MCAK's functional impact at microtubule ends derives both from its intrinsic end-binding capacity and its ability to strengthen the EB1-mediated end association pathway.

      (2) We agree with the reviewer that MCAK exhibiting a lower end-binding affinity (69 µM) is indeed intriguing, as one might intuitively expect a stronger affinity, e.g. in the nanomolar range. Several factors may contribute to this observation. First, this could be partly due to the in vitro system employed, which may not perfectly replicate in vivo conditions, especially when considering cellular processes quantitatively. Variations in medium composition can significantly influence the binding state. For example, reducing salt concentration leads to a marked increase in MCAK’s binding affinity (Helenius et al., 2006; Maurer et al., 2011; McHugh et al., 2019). Additionally, while numerous binding events with short durations were detected, we excluded transient interactions from our analysis to facilitate quantification. This likely leads to an underestimation of the on-rate and, consequently, the binding affinity. Moreover, to minimize the interference of purification tags (His-tag), we ensured their complete removal during protein sample preparation. Previous studies reported that retaining the His-tag of MAPs affects the binding affinity to microtubules (Maurer et al., 2011; Zhu et al., 2009). Finally, a low affinity is not necessarily unexpected. Considering the microtubule end as a receptor with multiple binding sites for MCAK, the overall binding affinity is in the nanomolar range (260 nM). This does not necessarily contradict MCAK being a microtubule dynamics regulator as only a few MCAK molecules may suffice to induce microtubule catastrophe (as discussed on page 13, lines 408-441).

      (3) Ideally, we would search for mutants that specifically interfere with the binding of GDP-Pi-tubulin or the curled protofilaments. However, the mutant we tested significantly impacts the overall affinity of MCAK to microtubules (both end and lattice), making it challenging to isolate and discuss the function of MCAK with respect to the binding to GDP-Pi-tubulin alone. Additionally, we also think that the GDP-Pi-tubulin in the EB cap and the tubulin in the curved protofilaments may share structural similarities. For instance, the tubulin dimers in both states may be less compact compared to those in the lattice, which could explain why MCAK recognizes both simultaneously (Manka and Moores, 2018). However, this remains a conjecture, as there is currently no direct evidence to support it.

      b. As mentioned in the Discussion, preferential MCAK binding to tubulins near the MT tip may enhance MCAK targeting of terminal tubulins AFTER the MCAK has been "delivered" to the distal cap via the EB-dependent mechanism. This is a different targeting mechanism than the direct MCAK-binding. However, the measured binding affinity between MCAK and GMPCPP tubulins is so weak (69 uM), that this effect is also unlikely to have any impact because the binding events between MCAK and microtubule should be extremely rare. Without hard evidence, the arguments for this enhancement are very speculative.

      Please see our response to the comment No. 1. Additionally, we have revised our discussion to discuss the end-binding affinity of MCAK as well as its physiological relevance (please see page 13, lines 408-441; and see Supplementary Appendix-2 in pages 49-51, lines 1246-1316).

      (2) The authors do not provide sufficient justification and explanation for their investigation of the effects of different nucleotides in MCAK binding affinity. A clear summary of the nucleotide-dependent function of MCAK (introduction with references to prior affinity measurements and corresponding MCAK affinities), the justifications for this investigation, and what has been learned from using different nucleotides (discussion) should be provided. My take on these results is that by far the strongest effect on microtubule wall and tip binding is achieved by adding any adenosine, whereas differences between different nucleotides are relatively minor. Was this expected? What can be learned from the apparent similarity between ATP and AMPPNP effects in some assays (Fig 1E, 4C, etc) but not others (Fig 1D,F, etc)?

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have revised the manuscript accordingly, and below are the main points of our response

      (1) The experiment investigating the effects of different nucleotides on MCAK binding affinity was inspired by the previous studies demonstrating that kinesin-13 interactions with microtubules are highly dependent on their adenosine-bound states. For example, kinesin-13s tightly bind microtubules and prefer to form protofilament curls or rings with tubulin in the AMPPNP state, whereas kinesin-13s are considered to move along the microtubule lattice via one-dimensional diffusion in the ADP·Pi state (Asenjo et al., 2013; Benoit et al., 2018; Friel and Howard, 2011; Helenius et al., 2006). Based on these observations, we wondered whether MCAK's adenosine-bound states might similarly affect its binding preference for growing microtubule ends. We have made the motivation clear in the revised manuscript (please see page 7, lines 199-209).

      (2) Our main finding regarding the effects of nucleotides is that MCAK shows differential end-binding affinity and preference based on its nucleotide state. First, MCAK shows the greatest preference for growing microtubule ends in the ATP state, supporting the idea that diffusive MCAK (MCAK·ATP) can directly bind to growing microtubule ends. Second, MCAK·ATP also demonstrates a binding preference for GTPγS microtubules and the ends of GMPCPP microtubules. The similar trends in binding preference suggest that the affinity for GDP·Pi-tubulin and GTP-tubulin likely underpins MCAK’s preference for growing microtubule ends. To clarify these points, we have added further discussions in the manuscript (please see page 8, lines 230-233; page9, lines 258-270 and pages 13-14, lines 443-458).

      (3) It is not clear why the authors decided to use these specific mutant MCAK proteins to advance their arguments about the importance of direct tip binding. Both mutants are enzymatically inactive. Both show roughly similar tip interactions, with some (minor) differences. Without a clear understanding of what these mutants represent, the provided interpretations of the corresponding results are not convincing.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. In the revised manuscript, we no longer draw conclusions about the importance of end-binding based on the mutant data. Instead, we think that the mutant data provide insights into the structural basis of the end-binding preference. Therefore, we have rewritten the results in this section to more accurately reflect these findings (please see page 10, lines 295-327).

      (4) GMPCPP microtubules are used in the current study to represent normal dynamic microtubule ends, based on some published studies. However, there is no consensus in the field regarding the structure of growing vs. GMPCPP-stabilized microtubule ends, which additionally may be sensitive to specific experimental conditions (buffers, temperature, age of microtubules, etc). To strengthen the authors' argument, Taxol-stabilized microtubules should be used as a control to test if the effects are specific. Additionally, the authors should consider the possibility that stronger MCAK binding to the ends of different types of microtubules may reflect MCAK-dependent depolymerization events on a very small scale (several tubulin rows). These nano-scale changes to tubulins and the microtubule end may lead to the accumulation of small tubulin-MCAK aggregates, as is seen with other MAPs and slowly depolymerizing microtubules. These effects for MCAK may also depend on specific nucleotides, further complicating the interpretation. This possibility should be addressed because it provides a different interpretation than presented in the manuscript.

      Regarding the two points raised here, our thoughts are as following

      (1) The end of GMPCPP-stabilized microtubules differs from that of growing microtubules, with the most obvious known difference being the absence of the region enriched in GDP-Pi-tubulin. We consider the end of GMPCPP microtubules as an analogue of the distal tip of growing microtubules, based on two key features: (1) curled protofilaments and (2) GMPCPP-tubulin, a close analogue of GTP-tubulin. Notably, both features are present at the ends of both GMPCPP-stabilized and growing microtubules. Moreover, we agree with the suggestion to use taxol-stabilized microtubules as a control. This would eliminate the second feature (absence of GTP-tubulin), allowing us to isolate the effect of the first feature. Therefore, we conducted this experiment, and our data showed that MCAK exhibits only a mild binding preference for the ends of taxol-stabilized microtubules, which is much less pronounced than for the ends of GMPCPP microtubules. This observation supports the idea that GMPCPP-stabilized ends closely resemble the growing ends of microtubules.

      (2) The reviewer suggested that stronger MCAK binding to the ends of different types of microtubules might reflect MCAK-dependent depolymerization events on a very small scale. This is an insightful possibility, which we had overlooked in the original manuscript. Fortunately, we performed the experiments at the single-molecule concentrations. Upon reviewing the raw data, we found that under ATP conditions, the binding events of MCAK were not cumulative (see Fig. X1 below) and showed no evidence of local accumulation of MCAK-tubulin aggregates.

      Author response image 1.

      The representative kymograph showing GFP-MCAK binding at the ends and lattice of GMPCPP microtubules in the presence of 1 mM ATP (10 nM GFP-MCAK), which corresponded to Fig. 5A. The arrow: the end-binding of MCAK. Vertical bar: 1 s; horizontal bar: 2 mm.

      (5) It would be helpful if the authors provided microtubule polymerization rates and catastrophe frequencies for assays with dynamic microtubules and MCAK in the presence of different nucleotides. The video recordings of microtubules under these conditions are already available to the authors, so it should not be difficult to provide these quantifications. They may reveal that microtubule ends are different (or not) under the examined conditions. It would also help to increase the overall credibility of this study by providing data that are easy to compare between different labs.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have provided data on the growth rates, which are similar across the different nucleotide states (Fig. s1). However, due to the short duration of our recordings (usually 5 minutes, but with a high frame rate, 10 fps), we did not observe many catastrophe events, which prevented us from quantifying catastrophe frequency using the current dataset. Since we measured the binding kinetics of MCAK during the growing phase of microtubules, the similar growth rates and microtubule end morphologies suggest that the microtubule ends are comparable across the different conditions.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      a. Please provide more details about how the microtubule-bound molecules were selected for analysis (include a description of scripts, selection criteria, and filters, if any). Fig 1A arrows do not provide sufficient information.

      We first measured the fluorescence intensity of each binding event. A probability distribution of these intensities was then constructed and fitted with a Gaussian function. A binding event was considered to correspond to a single molecule if its intensity fell within μ±2σ of the distribution. The details of the single-molecule screening process are now provided in the revised manuscript (see page17, lines 574-583).

      b. Evidence that MCAK is dimeric in solution should be provided (gel filtration results, controls for Figs1A - bleaching, or comparison with single GFP fluorophore).

      In the revised manuscript, we provide the gel filtration results of purified MCAK and other proteins used in this study. The elution volume of the peak for GFP-MCAK corresponded to a molecular weight range between 120 kDa (EB1-GFP dimer) and 260 kDa (XMAP215-GFP-his6), suggesting that GFP-MCAK exists as a dimer (~220 kDa) under experimental condition (please see Fig.s1 and page 5, lines 104-105). In addition, we also measured the fluorescence intensity of both MCAK<sup>sN+M</sup> and MCAK. MCAK<sup>sN+M</sup> is a monomeric mutant that contains the neck domain and motor domain (Wang et al., 2012). The average intensity of MCAK<sup>sN+M</sup> is 196 A.U., about 65% of that of MCAK (300 A.U.). These two measurements suggest that the purified MCAK used in this study exists dimers (see Fig. s1).

      c. Evidence that MCAK on microtubules represents single molecules should be provided (distribution of GFP brightness with controls - GFP imaged under identical conditions). Since assay buffers include detergent, which is not desirable, all controls should be done using the same assay conditions. The authors should rule out that their main results are detergent-sensitive.

      (1) Regarding if MCAK on microtubules represent single molecules: please refer to our responses to the two points above.

      (2) To rule out the effect of tween-20 (0.0001%, v/v), we performed additional control experiments. The results showed that it has no significant effect on microtubule-binding affinity of MCAK (see Figure below).

      Author response image 2.

      Tween-20 (0.0001%, v/v) has no significant effect on microtubule-binding affinity of MCAK. (A) The representative projection images of GFP-MCAK (5 nM) binding to taxol-stabled GDP microtubules in the presence of 1 mM AMPPNP with or without tween-20. The upper panel showed the results of the control experiments performed without MCAK. Scale bar: 5 mm. (B) Statistical quantification of the binding intensity of GFP-MCAK binding to GDP microtubules with or without tween-20 (53 microtubules from 3 assays and 70 microtubules from 3 assays, respectively). Data were presented as mean ± SEM. Statistical comparisons were performed using the two-tailed Mann-Whitney U test with Bonferroni correction, n.s., no significance.

      d. How did the authors plot single-molecule intensity distributions? I am confused as to why the intensity distribution for single molecules in Fig 1D and 2A looks so perfectly smooth, non-pixelated, and broader than expected for GFP wavelength. Please provide unprocessed original distributions, pixel size, and more details about how the distributions were processed.

      In the revised manuscript, we provided unprocessed original data in Fig. 1B and Fig. 2A. We thank the reviewer for pointing out this problem.

      e. Many quantifications are based on a limited number of microtubules and the number of molecules is not provided, starting from Fig 1D and down. Please provide detailed statistics and explain what is plotted (mean with SEM?) on each graph.

      We performed a thorough inspection of the manuscript and corrected the identified issues.

      f. Plots with averaged data should be supplemented with error bars and N should be provided in the legend. E.g. Fig 1C - average position of MT and peak positions.

      We agree with the reviewer. In the revised manuscript, we have made the changes accordingly (e.g. Fig. 2C).

      g. Detailed information should be provided about protein constructs used in this work including all tags. The use of truncated proteins or charged/bulky tags can modify protein-microtubule interactions.

      We agree with the reviewer. In the revised manuscript, we provide the information of all constructs (see Fig. s1 and the related descriptions in Methods, pages 15-16, lines 476-534).

      h. Line 515: We estimated that the accuracy of microtubule end tracking was ~6 nm by measuring the standard error of the distribution of the estimated error in the microtubule end position. - evidence should be provided using the conditions of this study, not the reference to the prior work by others.

      i. Line 520: We estimated that the accuracy of the measured position was ~2 nm by measuring the standard error of the fitting peak location". Please provide evidence.

      Point h-i: we now provide detailed descriptions of how to estimate tracking and measurement accuracy and error in our work. Please see pages 18-19, lines 626-645.

      j. Kymographs in Fig 5G are barely visible. Please provide single-channel greyscale images. What are the dim molecules diffusing on this microtubule?

      We have incorporated the changes suggested by the reviewer. We think that some of the dim signals may result from stochastic background noise, while others likely represent transient bindings of MCAK. The exposure time in our experiments was approximately 0.05 seconds; if the binding duration were shorter than this, the signal would be lower (i.e. the “dim” signals). It is important to note that in this study, we selected binding events lasting at least 2 consecutive frames, meaning transient binding events were not included. This point has been clarified in the Methods section (see page17, lines 573-583).

      k. Please provide a methods description for Fig 6. Did the buffer include 1 mM ATP? The presence of ATP would make these conditions more physiological. ATP concentration should be stated clearly in the main text or figure legend.

      The buffer contains ATP. In the revised manuscript, we have provided the methods for the experiments of microtubule dynamics assay, as well as the analysis of microtubule lifetimes and catastrophe frequency (see page 17, lines 561-572 and page 20, lines 685-690).

      l. Line 104: experiment was performed in BRB80 supplemented with 50 mM KCl and 1 mM ATP, providing a nearly physiological ion strength. Please provide a reference or add your calculations in Methods.

      We have provided references on page 5, lines 101-104 of our manuscript.

      m. What was the MCAK concentration in Figure 4? Did the microtubule shorten under any of these conditions?

      In these experiments, we used a very low concentration of MCAK and taxol-stabilized microtubules, so there’s no microtubule shortening observed here. ATP: 10 nM GFP-MCAK; AMPPNP: 1 nM GFP-MCAK; ADP: 10 nM GFP-MCAK; APO state: 0.1 nM GFP-MCAK.

      Other criticism:

      Text improvements are recommended in the Discussion. For example, line 348: Fourth, the loss of the binding preference.. suggests that the binding preference .. is required for the optimal .. preference.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this. In the revised manuscript, we conducted a thorough revision and review of the text.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Chen et al. investigate the localization of microtubule kinesin-13 MCAK to the microtubule ends. MCAK is a prominent microtubule depolymerase whose molecular mechanisms of action have been extensively studied by a number of labs over the last ~twenty years. Here, the authors use single-molecule approaches to investigate the precise localization of MCAK on growing microtubules and conclude that MCAK preferentially binds to a GDP-Pi-tubulin portion of the microtubule end. The conclusions are speculative and not well substantiated by the data, making the impact of the study in its current form rather limited. Specifically, greater effort should be made to define the region of MCAK binding on microtubule ends, as well as its structural characteristics. Given that MCAK has been previously shown to effectively tip-track growing microtubule ends through an established interaction with EB proteins, the physiological relevance of the present study is unclear. Finally, the manuscript does not cite or properly discuss a number of relevant literature references, the results of which should be directly compared and contrasted to those presented here.

      We thank the reviewer for the comments. As these suggestions are more thoroughly expressed in the following comments for authors, we will provide the responses in the corresponding sections, as shown below.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Significant concerns:

      (1) Establishing the precise localization of MCAK wrt microtubule end is highly non-trivial. More details should be provided, including substantial supplementary data. In particular, the authors claim ~6 nm accuracy in microtubule end positioning - this should be substantiated by data showing individual overlaid microtubule end intensity profiles as well as fits with standard deviations etc. Furthermore, to conclude that MCAK binds behind XMAP215, the authors should look at the localization of the two proteins simultaneously, on the same microtubule end. Notably, EB binding profiles are well known to exponentially decay along the microtubule lattice - this is not very apparent from the presented data. If MCAK's autonomous binding pattern matches that of EB, we should be seeing an exponentially-decaying localization for MCAK as well? However, averaged MCAK signals seem to only be fitted to Gaussian. Note that the EB binding region (i.e. position and size of the EB comet) can be substantially modulated by increasing the microtubule growth rate - this can be easily accomplished by increasing tubulin concentrations or the addition of XMAP215 (e.g. see Maurer et al. Cur Bio 2014). Thus to establish that MCAK on its own binds the same region as EB, experiments that directly modulate the size and the position of this region should be added.

      (1) We thank the reviewer for this comment. Regarding the accuracy in microtubule end positioning, we now provide more details, and please see pages 18-19, lines 625-645 in the revised manuscript.

      (2) Regarding the relative localization of XMAP215 and MCAK, we performed additional experiments to record their colocalizations simultaneously, on the same microtubule end. Our results showed that MCAK predominantly binds behind XMAP215, with 14.5% appearing within the XMAP215’s binding region. Please see Fig. 2.D-E and lines 184-197 in the revised manuscript.

      (3) Regarding the exponential decay of the EB1 signal along microtubules, we observed that the position probability distribution measured in the present study follows a Gaussian distribution, and the expected exponential decay was not apparent. Since the exponential decay is thought to result from the time delay between tubulin polymerization and GTP hydrolysis, slower polymerization is expected to reduce this latency (Maurer et al., 2014). In our experiments, the growth rate was relatively low (~0.7 mm/min), much slower than the rate observed in cells, where the comet-shaped EB1 signal is most pronounced. The previous study has shown that the exponential decay of EB1 is more pronounced at growth rates exceeding 3 mm/min in vitro (Maurer et al., 2014). Therefore, we think that the relatively slow growth may account for the observed non-exponential decay distribution of the EB1 signals. The same reason may also explain the distribution of MCAK.

      (4) We agree with the reviewer’s suggestion that altering microtubule growth rate is a valid and effective approach to regulate the EB cap length. However, the conclusion that MCAK binds to the EB region is supported by three lines of evidence: (1) the localization of MCAK at the ends of microtubules, (2) new experimental data showing that MCAK binds to the proximal end of the XMAP215 site, and (3) the tendency of MCAK to bind GTPγS microtubules, similar to EB1. Based on these findings, we did not pursue additional experiments to modify the length of the EB cap.

      (2) Even if MCAK indeed binds behind XMAP215, there is no evidence that this region is defined by the GDP-Pi nucleotide state; it could still be curved protofilaments. GTPyS is an analogue of GTP - to what extent GTPyS microtubules exactly mimic the GDP-Pi-tubulin state remains controversial. Furthermore, nucleotide sensing for EB is thought to be achieved through its binding at the interface of four tubulin dimers. However MCAK's binding site is distinct, and it has been shown to recognize intradimer tubulin curvature. Thus it is not clear how MCAK would sense the nucleotide state. On the other hand, there is mounting evidence that the morphology of the growing microtubule end can be highly variable, and that curved protofilaments may be protruding off the growing ends for tens of nanometers or more, previously observed both by EM as well as by fluorescence (e.g. Mcintosh, Moores, Chretien, Odde, Gardner, Akhmanova, Hancock, Zanic labs). Thus, to establish that MCAK indeed localizes along the closed lattice, EM approaches should be used.

      First, we conducted additional experiments that demonstrate MCAK indeed binds behind XMAP215, supporting the conclusion that MCAK interacts with the EB cap (please see Fig. 2 in the revised manuscript). Second, our argument that MCAK preferentially binds to GDP-Pi tubulin is based on two observations: (1) the binding regions of MCAK overlap with those of EB1, and (2) MCAK preferentially binds to GTPγS microtubules, which are considered a close analogue of GDP-Pi tubulin. Third, understanding the structural basis of how MCAK senses the nucleotide state of tubulin is beyond the scope of the present study. However, inspired by the reviewer’s suggestion, we looked into the structure of the MCAK-tubulin complex. The L2 loop of MCAK makes direct contact with the interdimer interface (Trofimova et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2017), which could provide a structural basis for recognizing the changes induced by GTP hydrolysis. While this remains a hypothesis, it is certainly a promising direction for future research. Forth, we agree with the reviewer that an EM approach would be ideal for establishing that MCAK localizes along the closed lattice. However, this is not the focus of the current study. Instead, we argue that MCAK binds to the EB cap, where at least some lateral interactions are likely to have formed.

      (3) The physiological relevance of the study is rather questionable: MCAK has been previously established to be able to both diffuse along the microtubule lattice (e.g. Helenius et al.) as well as hitchhike on EBs (Gouveia et al.). Given the established localization of EBs to growing microtubule ends in cells, and apparently higher affinity of MCAK for EB vs. the microtubule end itself (although direct comparisons with the literature have not been reported here), the relevance of MCAK's autonomous binding to dynamic microtubule ends is dubious.

      We thank the reviewer for raising the importance of physiological relevance. Please refer to our response to the comment No.1 of reviewer 1. Briefly, we think that the end-binding affinity of MCAK makes a significant contribution for its cellular functions. To elucidate this concept, we now use a simple model shown in Supplementary Appendix-2 (see pages 49-51, lines 1246-1316). In this model, we simplified MCAK and EB1 binding to microtubule ends by considering only these two proteins while neglecting other factors (e.g. XMAP215). Specifically, we considered two scenarios: one in which both proteins freely diffuse in the cytoplasm and another where MCAK is localized to specific cellular structures, such as the centrosome or centromere. Based on the modeling results, we argue that MCAK's functional impact at microtubule ends derives both from its intrinsic end-binding capacity and its ability to strengthen the EB1-mediated end association pathway.

      (4) Finally, the study seriously lacks discussion of and comparison with the existing literature on this topic. There are major omissions in citing relevant literature, such as e.g. landmark study by Kinoshita et al. Science 2001. Several findings reported here directly contradict previous findings in the literature. Direct comparison with e.g. Gouveia et al findings, Helenius et al. findings, and others need to be included. For example, Gouveia et al reported that EB is necessary for MCAK plus-end-tracking in vitro (please see Figure 1 of their manuscript). The authors should discuss how they reconcile the differences in their findings when compared to this earlier study.

      We thank the reviewer for this helpful suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have updated the text description and included comparative discussions with other relevant studies in the Discussion section. Specifically, we added comparisons with the research on XMAP215 in page 14, lines 459-472 (Barr and Gergely, 2008; Kinoshita et al., 2001; Tournebize et al., 2000). Additionally, we have compared our findings with those of Gouveia et al. and Helenius et al. regarding MCAK's preference for binding microtubule ends in page 6, lines 145-157 and page 13, 408-441, respectively (Gouveia et al., 2010; Helenius et al., 2006).

      Additional specific comments:

      Figure 1

      Gouveia et al. (Figure 1) reported that MCAK does not autonomously preferentially localize to growing tips. Specifically, Gouveia et al. found equal association rates of MCAK to both the lattice and the tip in the presence of EB3delT, an EB3 construct that does not directly interact with MCAK. How can these findings be reconciled with the results presented here?

      We are uncertain why there was no observed difference in the on-rates to the lattice and the end in the study by Gouveia et al. Even when considering only the known affinity of MCAK for curved protofilaments at the distal tip of growing microtubules, we would still expect to observe an end-binding preference. After carefully comparing the experimental conditions, we nevertheless identified some differences. First, we used a 160 nm tip size to calculate the on-rate (k<sub>on</sub>), whereas Gouveia et al. used a 450 nm tip. Using a longer tip size would naturally lead to a smaller(k<sub>on</sub>) value. Note that we chose 160 nm for several reasons: (i) a previous cryo-electron tomography study has elucidated that the sheet structures of dynamic microtubule ends have an average length of around 180 nm (Guesdon et al., 2016); (ii) Analysis of fluorescence signals at dynamic microtubule ends has demonstrated that the taper length at the microtubule end is less than 180 nm (Maurer et al., 2014); (iii) in the present study, we estimated that the length of MCAK's end-binding region is approximately 160 nm. Second, in Gouveia et al., single-molecule binding events were recorded in the presence of 75 nM EB3ΔT, which could potentially create a crowded environment at the tip, reducing MCAK binding. Third, as mentioned in our response to Reviewer 1, we took great care to minimize the interference from purification tags (e.g., His-tag) by ensuring their complete removal during protein preparation. Previous studies reported that retaining the His-tag of MAPs led to a significant increase in binding for microtubules (Maurer et al., 2011; Zhu et al., 2009). We believe that some of the factors mentioned above, or their combined effects, may account for the differences in these two observations.

      1C shows the decay of tubulin signal over several hundred nm - should show individual traces? How aligned? Doesn't this long decay suggest protruding protofilaments? (E.g. Odde/Gardner work).

      (1) In the revised manuscript, we now show individual traces (e.g. in Fig. 1B and Fig. 2A). The average trace for tubulin signal with standard deviation was shown in Fig. 2C.

      (2) The microtubule lattice was considered as a Gaussian wall and its end as a half-Gaussian in every frame. Use the peak position of the half-Gaussian of every frame to align and average microtubule end signals, during the dwell time. The average microtubule ends' half-Gaussion peak used as a reference to measure the intensity profile of individual single-molecule binding event in every frame (see page18, lines 607-624).

      (3) We think that the decay of tubulin signal results from the convolution of the tapered end structure and the point spread function. In the revised manuscript, we have updated the Figures to provide unprocessed original data in Fig. 1B and Fig. 2A.

      Please show absolute numbers of measurements in 1C (rather than normalized distribution only).

      In the revised manuscript, we have included the raw data for both tubulin and MCAK signals as part of the methods description. In Fig. 1, using normalized values allows for the simultaneous representation of microtubule and protein signals on a unified graph.

      How do the results in 1D-G compare with the previous literature? Particularly comparison of on-rates between this study and the Gouveia et al? Assuming 1 um = 1625 dimers, it appears that in the presence of EB3, the on-rate of MCAK to the tips reported in Gouveia et al. is an order of magnitude higher than reported here in the absence of EB3 (4.3 x 10E-4 vs. 2 x 10E-5). If so, and given the robust presence of EB proteins at growing microtubule ends in cells, this would invalidate the potential physiological relevance of the current study. Note that the dwell times measured in Gouveia et al. are also longer than those measured here.

      Note that in Gouveia et al, the concentration of mCherry-EB3 was 75 nM, about 187.5 times higher than that of MCAK (0.4 nM). The relative concentrations of these two proteins are not always the case in cells. Regarding the physiological relevance of the end-binding affinity of MCAK itself, please refer to our response to the point No.1 of Reviewer 1.

      Notably, Helenius et al reported a diffusion constant for MCAK of 0.38 um^2/s, which is more than an order of magnitude higher than reported here. The authors should comment on this!

      In the revised manuscript, we have provided an explanation for the difference in diffusion coefficient. Please see page 6, line 142-157. In short, low salt condition facilitates rapid diffusion of MCAK.

      Figure 2:

      This figure is critical and really depends on the analysis of the tubulin signal. Note significant variability in tubulin signal between presented examples in 2A. Also, while 2C looks qualitatively similar, there appears to be significant variability over the several hundred nm from the tip along the lattice. This is the crucial region; statistical significance testing should be presented. More detailed info, including SDs etc. is necessary.

      In the revised manuscript, we have provided raw data in Fig. 1B and Fig. 2A. Additionally, we have provided statistical analysis on the tubulin signals (Fig. 2C) and performed significance test. Please see page 5, lines 111-116 and page 7, lines 179-183 for detailed descriptions.

      Insights into the morphology of microtubule ends based on TIRF imaging have been previously gained in the literature, with reports of extended tip structures/protruding protofilaments (see e.g. Coombes et al. Cur Bio 2013, based on the methods of Demchouk et al. 2011). Such analysis should be performed here as well, if we are to conclude that nucleotide state alone, as opposed to the end morphology, specifies MCAK's tip localization.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion and agree that it provides a valid optical microscopy-based approach for estimating microtubule end morphology. However, this method did not establish a direct correlation between microtubule end morphology and tubulin nucleotide status. Therefore, we think that refining the measurement of microtubule end morphology will not necessarily provide more information to the understanding of tubulin nucleotide status at MCAK binding sites. Based on the available data in the present study, there are two main pieces of evidence supporting the idea that MCAK can sense tubulin nucleotide status: (1) the binding regions of MCAK and EB overlap significantly, and (2) MCAK shows a clear preference for binding to GTPγS microtubules, similar to EB1 (we provide a new control to support this, Fig. s4). Of course, we do not consider this to be a perfect set of evidence. As the reviewer has pointed out here and in other suggestions, future work should aim to further distinguish the nucleotide status of tubulin in the dynamic versus non-dynamic regions at the ends of microtubules, and to investigate the structural basis by which MCAK recognizes tubulin nucleotide status.

      EB comet profile should be clearly reproduced. MCAK should follow the comet profile.

      Please see our 3<sup>rd</sup> response to the point 1 of this reviewer.

      The conclusion that the MCAK binding region is larger than XMAP215 is not firm, based on the data presented. The authors state that 'the binding region of MCAK was longer than that of XMAP215'. What is the exact width of the region of the XMAP215 localization and how much longer is the MCAK end-binding region? Is this statistically significant?

      We have revised this part in the revised manuscript (page 6, lines 167-172). The position probability distributions of MCAK and XMAP215 were significantly different (K-S test, p< 10<sup>-5</sup>), and the binding region of MCAK (FWHM=185 nm) was significantly longer than that of XMAP215 (FWHM=123 nm).

      MCAK localization with AMPPNP should also be performed here. Even low concentrations of MCAK have been shown to induce microtubule catastrophe/end depolymerization. This will dramatically affect microtubule end morphology, and thus apparent positioning of MCAK at the end.

      In the end positioning experiment, we used a low concentration of MCAK (1 nM). Under this condition, microtubule dynamics remained unchanged, and the morphology of the microtubule ends was comparable across different conditions (with EB1, MCAK or XMAP215). Additionally, in the revised manuscript, we present a new experiment in which we recorded the localization of both MCAK and XMAP215 on the same microtubule. The results support the conclusion regarding their relative localization: most MCAK is found at the proximal end of the XMAP215 binding region, while approximately 15% of MCAK is located within the XMAP215 binding region. Please see Fig. 2D-E and page 7, lines 184-197 for the corresponding descriptions.

      Figure 3:

      For clearer presentation, projections showing two microtubule lattice types on the same image (in e.g. two different colors) should be shown first without MCAK, and then with MCAK.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have adjusted the figure accordingly. Please see Fig. 4 in the revised manuscript.

      Please comment on absolute intensity values - scales seem to be incredibly variable.

      The fluorescence value presented here is the result of multiple images being summed. Therefore, the difference in absolute values is influenced not only by the binding affinity of MCAK in different states to microtubules, but also by the number of images used. In this analysis, we are not comparing MCAK in different states, but rather evaluating the binding ability of MCAK in the same state on different types of microtubules.

      Given that the authors conclude that MCAK binding mimics that of EB, EB intensity measurements and ratios on different lattice substrates should be performed as a positive control.

      We performed additional experiments with EB1, in the revised manuscript, we provide the data as a positive control (please see Fig. s4).

      Figure 4:

      MCAK-nucleotide dependence of GMPCPP microtubule-end binding has been previously established (see e.g. Helenius et al, others?) - what is new here? Need to discuss the literature. This would be more appropriate as a supplemental figure?

      In the present study, we reproduced the GMPCPP microtubule-end binding of MCAK in the AMPPNP state, as shown in several previous reports (Desai et al., 1999; Hertzer et al., 2006). Here, we also quantified the end to lattice binding preference, and our results showed that the nucleotide state-dependence shows the same trend as the binding preference of MCAK to the growing microtubule ends. Therefore, we prefer to keep this figure in the main text (Fig. 5).

      Figure 5:

      Please note that both MCAK mutants show an additional two orders of magnitude lower microtubule binding on-rates when compared to wt MCAK. This makes the analysis of preferential binding substrate for these mutants dubious.

      We agreed with this point. We have rewritten this part. Please see page 10, lines 295-327, in the revised manuscript.

      Figure 6:

      Combined effects of XMAP215 and XKCM1 (MCAK) have been previously explored in the landmark study by Kinoshita et al. Science 2001, which should be cited and discussed. Also note that Moriwaki et al. JCB 2016 explored the combined effects of XMA215 and MCAK - which should be discussed here and compared to the current results.

      We agree with the reviewer. We have revised the discussion on this part. Please see page 11, lines 329-342 and page 14, lines 459-472 in the revised manuscript.

      Please report quantification for growth rate and lifetime.

      In the revised manuscript, we provide all these data. Please see pages 11-12, lines 343-374.

      To obtain any new quantitative information on the combined effects of the two proteins, at the very minimum, the authors should perform a titration in protein concentration.

      We agree with the reviewer on this point. In our pilot experiments, we performed titration experiments to determine the appropriate concentrations of MCAK and XMAP215, respectively. We selected 50 nM for XMAP215, as it clearly enhances the growth rate and exhibits a mild promoting effect on catastrophe—two key effects of XMAP215 reported in previous studies (Brouhard et al., 2008; Farmer et al., 2021). Reducing the XMAP215 concentration eliminates the catastrophe-promoting effect, while increasing it would not much enhance the growth rate. For MCAK, we chose 20 nM, as it effectively promotes catastrophe; increasing the concentration beyond this point leads to no microtubule growth, at least in the MCAK-only condition. If there’s no microtubule growth, it would be difficult to quantify the parameters of microtubule dynamics, hindering a clear comparison of the combined versus individual effects. Therefore, we think that the concentrations used in this study are appropriate and representative. In the revised manuscript, we make this point clearer (see pages 11 and lines 329-342).

      Finally, the writing could be improved for overall clarity.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this. In the revised manuscript, we conducted a thorough revision and review of the text.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      The authors revisit an old question of how MCAK goes to microtubule ends, partially answered by many groups over the years. The authors seem to have omitted the literature on MCAK in the past 10-15 years. The novelty is limited due to what has previously been done on the question. Previous work showed MCAK targets to microtubule plus-ends in cells through association with EB proteins and Kif18b (work from Wordeman, Medema, Walczak, Welburn, Akhmanova) but none of their work is cited.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. Some of the referenced work has already been cited in our manuscript, such as studies on the interaction between MCAK and EB1. However, other relevant literature had not been properly cited. In the revised manuscript, we have added further discussion on this topic in the context of existing findings. Please refer to pages 3-4, lines 68-85, and pages 13, lines 425-441.

      It is not obvious in the paper that these in vitro studies only reveal microtubule end targeting, rather than plus end targeting. MCAK diffuses on the lattice to both ends and its conformation and association with the lattice and ends has also been addressed by other groups-not cited here. I want to particularly highlight the work from Friel's lab where they identified a CDK phosphomimetic mutant close to helix4 which reduces the end preference of MCAK. This residue is very close to the one mutated in this study and is highly relevant because it is a site that is phosphorylated in vivo. This study and the mutant produced here suggest a charge-based recognition of the end of microtubules.

      Here the authors analyze this MCAK recognition of the lattice and microtubule ends, with different nucleotide states of MCAK and in the presence of different nucleotide states for the microtubule lattice. The main conclusion is that MCAK affinity for microtubules varies in the presence of different nucleotides (ATP and analogs) which was partially known already. How different nucleotide states of the microtubule lattice influence MCAK binding is novel. This information will be interesting to researchers working on the mechanism of motors and microtubules. However, there are some issues with some experiments. In the paper, the authors say they measure MCAK residency of growing end microtubules, but in the kymographs, the microtubules don't appear dynamic - in addition, in Figure 1A, MCAK is at microtubule ends and does not cause depolymerization. I would have expected to see depolymerization of the microtubule after MCAK targeting. The MCAK mutants are not well characterized. Do they still have ATPase activity? Are they folded? Can the authors also highlight T537 and discuss this?

      Finally, a few experiments are done with MCAK and XMAP215, after the authors say they have demonstrated the binding sites overlap. The data supporting this statement were not obvious and the conclusions that the effect of the two molecules are additive would argue against competing binding sites. Overall, while there are some interesting quantitative measurements of MCAK on microtubules - in particular in relation to the nucleotide state of the microtubule lattice - the insights into end-recognition are modest and do not address or discuss how it might happen in cells. Often the number of events is not recorded. Histograms with large SEM bars are presented, so it is hard to get a good idea of data distribution and robustness. Figures lack annotations. This compromises therefore their quantifications and conclusions. The discussion was hard to follow and needs streamlining, as well as putting their work in the context of what is known from other groups who produced work on this in the past few years.

      We thank the reviewer for the comments. Regarding the physiological relevance of the end-binding of MCAK itself, please refer to our response to the point No.1 of reviewer 1. Moreover, as we feel that other suggestions are more thoroughly expressed in the following comments for authors, we will provide the responses in the corresponding sections, as shown below.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Why, on dynamic microtubules, is MCAK at microtubule plus ends and does not cause a catastrophe?

      At this concentration (10 nM MCAK with 16 mM tubulin in Fig. 1; 1 nM MCAK with 12 mM tubulin in Fig. 2), MCAK has little effect on microtubule dynamics in our experiments. Using TIRFM, we were able to observe individual MCAK binding events. Based on these observations, we think that in the current experimental condition, a single binding event of MCAK is insufficient to induce microtubule catastrophe; rather, it likely requires cumulative changes resulting from multiple binding events.

      Do the MCAK mutants still have ATPase activity?

      The ATPase activities of MCAK<sup>K525A</sup> and MCAK<sup>V298S</sup> are both reduced to about 1/3 of the wild-type (Fig. s6).

      The intensities of GFP are not all the same on the microtubule lattice (eg 1A). See blue and white arrowheads. The authors could be looking at multiple molecules of GFP-MCAK instead of single dimers. How do they account for this possibility?

      In the revised manuscript, we provide the gel filtration result of the purified MCAK, and the position of the peak corresponds to ~220 kDa, demonstrating that the purified MCAK in solution is dimeric (please see Fig.s1 and page 5, lines 101-103). We measured the fluorescence intensity of each binding event. A probability distribution of these intensities was then constructed and fitted with a Gaussian function. A binding event was considered to correspond to a single molecule if its intensity fell within μ±2σ of the distribution. The details of the single-molecule screening process are provided in the revised manuscript (see page 17, lines 574-583).

      In addition, we also measured the fluorescence intensity of both MCAK<sup>sN+M</sup> and MCAK. MCAK<sup>sN+M</sup> is a monomeric mutant that contains the neck domain and motor domain (Wang et al., 2012). The average intensity of MCAK<sup>sN+M</sup> is 196 A.U., about 65 % of that of MCAK (300 A.U.), suggesting that MCAK is a dimer (see Fig. s1). Moreover, we think that some of the dim signals may result from stochastic background noise, while others likely represent transient bindings of MCAK. The exposure time in our experiments was approximately 0.05 seconds; if the binding duration were shorter than this, the signal would be lower. It is important to note that in this study, we specifically selected binding events lasting at least 2 consecutive frames, meaning transient binding events were not included. This point has been clarified in the Methods section (see page 17, lines 568-569 and lines 574-583).

      Could the authors provide a kymograph of an MT growing, in the presence of MCAK+AMPPNP? Can MCAK track the cap?

      Under single-molecule conditions, we observed a single MCAK molecule briefly binding to the end of the microtubule. However, we did not record if MCAK at high concentrations could track microtubule ends under AMPPNP conditions.

      In the experiments in Figure 6, the authors should also show the localization of MCAK and XMAP215 at microtubule plus ends in their kymographs to show the two molecules overlap.

      Regarding the relative localization of XMAP215 and MCAK, we conducted additional experiments to record their colocalization simultaneously at the same microtubule end. Our results show that MCAK predominantly binds behind XMAP215, with 14.5% of MCAK binding within the XMAP215 binding region. Please see Fig. 2.D-E and page 7, lines 184-197 in the revised manuscript. However, we argue that the effects of XMAP215 and MCAK are additive, and their binding sites do not necessarily need to overlap for these effects to occur.

      The authors do not report what statistical tests are done in their graphs, and one concern is over error propagation of their data. Instead of bar graphs, showing the data points would be helpful.

      We have now shown all data points in the revised manuscript.

      MCAK+AMPPNP accumulates at microtubule ends. Appropriate quotes from previous work should be provided.

      We have made the revisions accordingly. Please see page 9, lines 273-276.

      Controls are missing. An SEC profile for all purified proteins should be presented. Also, the authors need to explain if they report the dimeric or monomeric concentration of MCAK, XMAP215, etc...

      We have provided the gel filtration result for all purified proteins in the revised manuscript (Fig.s1). Moreover, we now make it clear that the concentrations of MCAK and EB1 are monomeric concentration. Please see the legend for Fig. 1, line 893 in the revised manuscript.

      Figure 1: the microtubules don't look dynamic at all. This is also why the authors can record MCAK at microtubule ends, because their structure is not changing.

      The microtubules are dynamic, but they may appear non-dynamic due to the relatively slow growth rate and the high frame rate at which we are recording. We propose that individual binding events of MCAK induce structural changes at the nanoscopic or molecular scale, which are not detectable using TIRFM.

      I recommend the authors measure the Kon and Koff for single GFP-MCAK mutant molecules and provide the information alongside their normalized and averaged binding intensities of GFP-MCAK in Fig 5. Showing data points instead of bar graphs would be better.

      (1) We measured k<sub>on</sub> and dwell time for mutants at growing microtubule end. However, we did not perform single-molecule tracking for MCAK’s binding on stabilized microtubules. This is mainly because the superimposed signal on the stable microtubule already indicates the changes in the mutant's binding affinity to different microtubule structures, and moreover, the binding of the mutants is highly transient, making accurate single-molecule tracking and calculations difficult.

      (2) In the revised figure, we have included the data points in all plots.

      When discussing how Kinesin-13 interacts with the lattice, the authors should quote the papers that report the organization of full-length Kinesin-13 on tubulin heterodimers: Trofimova et al, 2018; McHugh et al 2019; Benoit et al, 2018. It would reinforce their model and account for the full-length protein, rather than just the motor domain.

      We thank the suggestion for the reviewer. In our manuscript, we have cited papers on full-length Kinesin-13 to discuss the interaction between MCAK and microtubule end-curved structure. Additionally, we have utilized the MCAK-tubulin crystal structure (PDB ID: 5MIO) in Fig. 6, as it depicts a human MCAK, which is consistent with the protein used in our study. This structure illustrates the interaction sites between MCAK and tubulin dimer, guiding our mutation studies on specific residues. Thus, we prefer to use the structure (PDB ID: 5MIO) in Fig.6.

      Figure 5A. What type of model is this? A PDB code is mentioned. Is this from an X-ray structure? If so, mention it.

      We have now included the structural information in the Figure legend (see page 37, lines 1045).

      Figure 5B. It is not possible to distinguish the different microtubule lattices (GTPyS, GDP, and GMPCPP). The experiment needs to be better labelled.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We have now rearranged the figure for better clarity (see Fig. 6).

      "Figure 5D: what are the statistical tests? I don't understand " The statistical comparisons were made versus the corresponding value of 848 GFP-MCAK".

      We have made this point clearer in the revised manuscript (see pages 38, line 1078-1080).

      What is the "EB cap"? This needs explaining.

      We provide this explanation for this, please see page 4, lines 87-89 in the revised manuscript.

      Work from Friel and co-workers showed MCAK T537E did not have depolymerizing activity and a reduced affinity for microtubule ends. The work of the authors should be discussed with respect to this previously published work.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have added discussions on this (see page 10, lines 303-307).

      The concentration of protein used in the assays is not always described.

      We have checked throughout the manuscript and made revisions accordingly.

      "Having revealed the novel binding sites of MCAK in dynamic microtubule ends " should be on "we wondered how MCAK may work ..with EB1". This is not addressed so should be removed. Instead, they can quote the work from Akhmanova's lab. Realistically this section should be rephrased as there are other plus-end targeting molecules that compete with MCAK, not just XMAP215 and EB1.

      We have rephrased this section as suggested by this reviewer to be more specific. Please see page 11, lines 329-342.

      What is AMPCPP?

      It should be “AMPPNP”

      Typos in Figure 5.

      Corrected

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      We have made the following small adjustments and resubmit the manuscript to be published as a Version of Record with eLife.

      Changes in main text of the manuscript:

      We have moved the “Proposed additional tests” subsection to the Discussion section as suggested by the referee. 

      We have added a link to a Github repository and a link to a Zenodo data repository at the beginning of the Materials and Methods section in the “Data and materials availability” subsection. The Github repository contains simulation code and data, and single-cell data analysis code. The Zenodo link contains our experimental data (we await your confirmation before we publish it officially on Zenodo).   

      Changes in the supplemental information files

      We have fixed the typo on page 29 of the SI in which Eq. (8) was referred to in a derivation. It should be Eq. (5) instead. We thank the referee for catching this mistake which has now been corrected.

      We have fixed a typo on page 29 of SI, in which the word “evoke” is now “invoke”.  

      We have clarified the derivation on page 29 of the SI. The referee is correct that the limit condition was used to set the right-hand side of Eq. (5.11) to zero.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This study addresses a critical gap in veterinary diagnostics by developing a CRISPR-based diagnostic toolbox (SHERLOCK4AAT) for detecting animal African trypanosomosis. It describes the development and field deployment of SHERLOCK4AAT, a CRISPR-Cas13-based diagnostic toolbox for the eco-epidemiological surveillance of animal African trypanosomosis (AAT) in West Africa.The authors successfully created and validated species-specific assays for multiple trypanosomes, including T. congolense, T. vivax, T. theileri, T. simiae, and T. suis, alongside pan-trypanosomatid and pan-Trypanozoon assays. The field validation in pigs from Guinea and Côte d'Ivoire revealed high trypanosome prevalence (62.7%), frequent co-infections, and importantly identified T. b. gambiense in one animal at each site, suggesting pigs may serve as potential reservoirs for this human-infective parasite.

      A major strength of the study lies in its methodological innovation. By adapting SHERLOCK to target both conserved and species-discriminating sequences, the authors achieved high sensitivity and specificity in detecting Trypanosoma species. Their use of dried blood spots, validated thresholds through ROC analyses, and statistical robustness (e.g., Bayesian latent class modeling) provides a strong foundation for their conclusions.

      The results are significant: over 60% of pigs tested positive for at least one trypanosome species, with co-infections observed frequently and T. b. gambiense detected in pigs at both sites. These findings have direct implications for the role of animal reservoirs in human disease transmission and underscore the value of pigs as sentinel hosts in gHAT elimination efforts.

      The limitations are well acknowledged, particularly the suboptimal sensitivity of the T. vivax assay and the reliance on synthetic controls for T. suis and T. simiae. However, these limitations do not undermine the overall conclusions, and the paper provides a clear roadmap for further assay refinement and implementation.

      This study offers a timely, impactful, and well-substantiated contribution to the field. The SHERLOCK4AAT toolbox holds promise for improving AAT diagnostics in resource-limited settings and advancing One Health surveillance frameworks.

      Thank you

      Strengths: 

      (1) The adaptation of SHERLOCK technology for AAT represents a significant technical advancement, offering higher sensitivity than traditional parasitological methods and the ability to detect multiple species simultaneously.

      (2) Rigorously performed with validation using appropriate controls, ROC curve analyses, and Bayesian latent class modelling, establishing clear analytical sensitivity and specificity for most assays.

      (3) Testing 424 pig samples across two countries provides robust evidence of the tool's utility and reveals important epidemiological insights about trypanosome diversity and prevalence.

      (4) The identification of T. b. gambiense in pigs at both sites has significant implications for HAT elimination strategies and highlights the need for integrated One Health approaches.

      (5) The use of dried blood spots and RNA detection for active infections makes the approach practical for field surveillance in resource-limited settings.

      Thank you

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) The manuscript would benefit from more detailed discussion of practical considerations such as cost, equipment requirements, and training needs for implementing SHERLOCK in endemic areas and rural settings which would improve applicability.

      This is now adressed in the revised discussion (end of the first section).

      (2) Limited discussion of pig selection criteria: More justification for choosing pigs as sentinel animals and discussion of potential limitations of this approach would strengthen the manuscript.

      Yes, this is now more clearly explained in the revised discussion (beginning of the first section).

      (3) More details on why certain genes were targeted would strengthen the methods.

      The first result section ‘Selection of targets for broad and species-specific SHERLOCK assays targeting AAT species (SHERLOCK4AAT)’ is already dedicated to extensively explaining target selection, hence we’re afraid we don’t know what could be added.  

      (4) Table formatting could be improved for readability. 

      (5) Some figures are complex and would benefit from additional explanations in the legends.

      We have tried to improve these two aspects as much as possible in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The manuscript is important due to the significance of the findings. The strength of evidence is convincing.

      Thank you

      Strengths: 

      (1) Using a Novel SHERLOCK4AAT toolkit for diagnosis. 

      (2) Identification of various sub-species of Trypanosomes. 

      (3) Differentiating the animal subspecies from the human one. 

      Thank you

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) The title is too long, and the use of definite articles should be reduced in the title.

      The title has been improved in the revised version.

      (2) The route of blood sample collection in the animals should be well defined and explained.

      This has been more clearly explained in the revised method section.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      The study adapts CRISPR-based detection toolkit (SHERLOCK assay) using conserved and species-specific targets for the detection of some members of the Trypanosomatidae family of veterinary importance and species-specific assays to differentiate between the six most common animal trypanosome species responsible for AAT (SHERLOCK4AAT). The assays were able to discriminate between Trypanozoon (T. b. brucei, T. evansi, and T. equiperdum), T. congolense (Savanah, Forest Kilifi, and Dzanga sangha), T. vivax, T. theileri, T. simiae, and T. suis. The design of both broad and species-specific assays was based primarily on sequences of the 18S rRNA, GAPDH (Glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase), and invariant flagellum antigen (IFX) genes for species identification. Most importantly, the authors showed varying limits of detection for the different SHERLOCK assays, which is somewhat comparable to PCR-derived molecular techniques currently used for detecting animal trypanosomes, even though some of these methodologies have used other primers that target genes such as ITS1 and 7SL sRNA. <br /> The data presented in the study are particularly useful and of significant interest for the diagnosis of AAT in affected areas.

      Thank you

      Strengths: 

      The assays convincingly allow for the analysis and detection of most trypanosomes in AAT.

      Thank you

      Weaknesses: 

      Inability for the assay to distinguish T. b. brucei, T. evansi, and T. equiperdum using the 18S rRNA gene, as well as the IFX gene, not achieving the sensitivity requirements for detection of T. vivax.  Both T. brucei brucei and T. vivax are the most predominant infective species in animals (in addition to T. congolense), therefore, a reliable assay should be able to convincingly detect these to allow for proper use of the diagnostic assay.

      We agree with this point and aim to improve the toolbox for future studies.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Provide additional details on the practicality of SHERLOCK deployment in the field, including training, costs, and infrastructure (potential challenges for field deployment, including suggestions for how to overcome these barriers).

      This is now adressed in the revised discussion (end of the first section).

      (2) Provide more detailed justification for choosing pigs as the main study species and discuss potential benefits and limitations of extending the approach to other livestock species.

      Yes, this is now more clearly explained in the revised discussion (beginning of the first section).

      (3) Add a comparison table comparing SHERLOCK4AAT performance metrics (sensitivity, specificity, LoD) with existing molecular diagnostic methods for AAT for ease of reference.

      There are dozens of different serological, immunological and molecular approaches with highlty variable levels of sensitivity and specificities already reviewed and compared in detail in two references from 2022 (Desquesnes et al. a and b), which we have cited, as well as in a newly added reference (EBHODAGHE F acta trop 2018). Hence, we decided to only refer to the most comparable studies in the present article.

      (4) Review complex figures and improve legends for better readability and interpretation.

      We have tried to improve this as much as possible in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) Reduce the number of words in the title from 28 to not more than 20.

      The title has been improved in the revised version.

      (2) Specify the particular route of collection of blood samples in the various animals.

      Yes, this is now more clearly explained in the revised method section.

      (3) Correct all typographical errors. 

      We have tried to improve this as much as possible in the revised manuscript.

      Thanks. I wish you the best in your publication process. 

      Thank you

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Minor comments 

      (1) The authors can expand the discussion to include other recent diagnostic assays for Animal trypanosomiasis, such as those that target other genes like tubulin.

      Please see response to Review 1 point #3 above.

      (2) The cost-effectiveness of the use of the assay can be discussed since the assay is expected to be used for work in some resource-deprived areas. For example, will it cost a researcher less to do a diagnosis with this assay relative to what is already available?

      This is now adressed in the revised discussion (end of the first section).

      (3) Is Cote d'Ivoire more endemic for AAT than Guinea? Will this account for the apparently consistent differences in the percentage of positive samples, or just because of the type of samples used from the two locations?

      As the sampling method, sample preservation and sample analysis were the same for both groups - yes, it appears that pigs, at least for domesticated ones, in the study region of Cote d'Ivoire were more frequently infected than those in the study region of Guinea. It is however risky to extrapolate these observations to the AAT prevalence in the entire countries and/or to other mammals.

      (4) Can the authors comment on how long one can store the samples for an effective and reliable assay?

      The samples can be stored for several months at ambient temperature in a sealed bag with silica gel packages to reduce humidity. We have added this detail in the revised methods section.

      (5) It is not clear whether the authors used conventional molecular diagnostics to compare the data obtained from this particular cohort of animals as reference is made to published data. It is not surprising that the SHERLOCK performed better than using parasitology-based methodology.

      This is now adressed in the revised discussion.

      (6) (Figure 4D-5D) should be 4D and 5D.

      Thank you, this has been corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      eLife Assessment

      This useful study integrates experimental methods from materials science with psychophysical methods to investigate how frictional stabilities influence tactile surface discrimination. The authors argue that force fluctuations arising from transitions between frictional sliding conditions facilitate the discrimination of surfaces with similar friction coefficients. However, the reliance on friction data obtained from an artificial finger, together with the ambiguous correlative analyses relating these measurements to human psychophysics, renders the findings incomplete.

      Our main goal with this paper was to show that the most common metric, i.e. average friction coefficient—widely used in tactile perception and device design – is fundamentally unsound, and to offer a secondary parameter that is compatible with the fact that human motion is unconstrained, leading to dynamic interfacial mechanics.

      We understand the Reviewers wanted, through biomechanical measurements, to demonstrate that humans using instabilities. This is seemingly reasonable, but in individual responses, we explain the significant challenges and fundamental unknowns to those experiments. We believe this paper sets forth an important step to approach this problem. At the same time, we have made several changes in the discussion, conclusion, and title to clarify that our study is correlative between mechanical characterization and human testing.

      In short, there are still several fundamental unknowns that prevented us from basing the study around biomechanical measurements: (1) a decision-making model would need to be created, but it is unknown if tactile decision making follows other models, (2) it is further unknown what constitutes “tactile evidence”, though at our manuscript’s conclusion, we propose that friction instabilities are better suited for to be tactile evidence than the averaging of friction coefficients from a narrow range of human exploration (3) in the design of samples, from a friction mechanics and materials perspective, it is not at this point, possible to pre-program surfaces a priori to deliver friction instabilities and instead must be experimentally determined – especially when attempting to achieve this in controlled surfaces that do not create other overriding tactile cues, like macroscopic bumps or large differences in surface roughness. (4) Given that the basis for tactile percepts, like which object feels “rougher” or “smoother” is not sufficiently established, it is necessary to use a 3-alternative forced choice task which avoids asking objects along a preset perceptual dimension – a challenge recognized by Reviewer 3. However, this would bring in issues of memory in the decision-making model. (5) The prior points are compounded by the fact that, we believe, tactile exploration must be performed in an unconstrained manner, i.e., without an apparatus generating motion onto a stationary finger. Work by Liu et al. (IEEE ToH, 2024) showed that recreating friction obtained during free exploration onto a stationary finger was uninterpretable by the participants, hinting at the importance of efference copies.[1] We believe that many of the above-mentioned issues constitutes a significant advance in knowledge and would require discussion and dissemination with the community.

      Our changes to the manuscript

      Page 1 & SI Page 1, Title

      “Alternatives to Friction Coefficient: Fine Touch Perception Correlates with Frictional Instabilities”

      Reviewer 1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, Derkaloustian et. al look at the important topic of what affects fine touch perception. The observations that there may be some level of correlation with instabilities are intriguing. They attempted to characterize different materials by counting the frequency (occurrence #, not of vibration) of instabilities at various speeds and forces of a PDMS slab pulled lengthwise over the material. They then had humans make the same vertical motion to discriminate between these samples. They correlated the % correct in discrimination with differences in frequency of steady sliding over the design space as well as other traditional parameters such as friction coefficient and roughness. The authors pose an interesting hypothesis and make an interesting observation about the occurrences of instability regimes in different materials while in contact with PDMS, which is interesting for the community to see in the publication. It should be noted that the finger is complex, however, and there are many factors that may be quite oversimplified with the use of the PDMS finger, and the consideration and discounting of other parameters are not fully discussed in the main text or SI. Most importantly, however, the conclusions as stated do not align with the primary summary of the data in Figure 2.

      Strengths:

      The strength of this paper is in its intriguing hypothesis and important observation that instabilities may contribute to what humans are detecting as differences in these apparently similar samples.

      We thank Reviewer 1 for their time on the manuscript, recognizing the approach we took, and offering constructive feedback. We believe that our conclusions, in fact, are supported by the primary summary of the data in Fig. 2 but we believe that our use of R<sup>2</sup> could have led to misinterpretation. The trend with friction coefficient and percent correct was indeed statistically significant but was spurious because the slope was negative. In the revision, we add clarifying comments throughout, change from R<sup>2</sup> to r as to highlight the negative trend, and adjust the figures to better focus on friction coefficient.

      Finally, we added a new section to discuss the tradeoffs between using a real human finger versus a mock finger, and which situations may warrant the use of one or the other. In short, for our goal of characterizing surfaces to be used in tactile experiments, we believe a mock finger is more sustainable and practical than using real humans because human fingers are unique per participant, humans move their fingers at constantly changing pressures and velocities, and friction generated during free exploring human cannot be satisfactorily replicated by moving a sample onto a stationary finger. But, we do not disagree that for other types of experiments, characterizing a human participant directly may be more advantageous.

      Weaknesses:

      Comment 1

      The most important weakness is that the findings do not support the statements of findings made in the abstract. Of specific note in this regard is the primary correlation in Figure 2B between SS (steady sliding) and percent correct discrimination. Of specific note in this regard is the primary correlation in Figure 2B between SS (steady sliding) and percent correct discrimination. While the statistical test shows significance (and is interesting!), the R-squared value is 0.38, while the R-squared value for the "Friction Coefficient vs. Percent Correct" plot has an R-squared of 0.6 and a p-value of < 0.01 (including Figure 2B). This suggests that the results do not support the claim in the abstract: "We found that participant accuracy in tactile discrimination was most strongly correlated with formations of steady sliding, and response times were negatively correlated with stiction spikes. Conversely, traditional metrics like surface roughness or average friction coefficient did not predict tactile discriminability."

      We disagree that the trend with friction coefficient suggests the results do not support the claim because the correlation was found to be negative. However, we could have made the comparison more apparent and expanded on this point, given its novelty.

      While the R<sup>2</sup> value corresponding to the “Friction Coefficient vs. Percent Correct” plot is notably higher, our results show that the slope is negative, which would be statistically spurious. This is because a negative correlation between percent correct (accuracy in discriminating surfaces) and difference in friction coefficient means that the more similar two surfaces are (by friction coefficient), the easier it would be for people to tell them apart. That is, it incorrectly concludes that two identical surfaces would be much easier to tell apart than two surfaces with greatly different friction coefficients.

      This is counterintuitive to nearly all existing results, but we believe our samples were well-positioned to uncover this trend by minimizing variability, by controlling multiple physical parameters in the samples, and that the friction coefficient — typically calculated in the field as an average friction coefficient — ignores all the dynamic changes in forces present in elastic systems undergoing mesoscale friction, i.e., human touch, as seen in Fig. 1 in a mock finger and Fig. 3 in a real finger. By demonstrating this statistically spurious trend, we believe this strongly supports our premise that an alternative to friction coefficient is needed in the design of tactile psychophysics and haptic interfaces.

      We believe that this could have been misinterpreted, so we took several steps to improve clarity, given the importance of this finding: we separated the panel on friction coefficient to its own panel, we changed from R<sup>2</sup> to r throughout, and we added clarifying text. We also added a small section focusing on this spurious trend.

      Our changes to the manuscript

      Page 1, Abstract

      “In fact, the typical method of averaging friction coefficients led to a spurious correlation which erroneously suggests that distinct objects should feel identical and identical objects should feel distinct.”

      Page 7

      “As Fig. 1 was constructed from friction measurements, we can also calculate an average friction coefficient, µ, by averaging the friction coefficient obtained at each of the 16 combinations of masses and velocities (Table 1). This calculation is a standard approach in tactile studies for summarizing friction measurements, or in some cases, surfaces are never characterized at multiple masses and velocities. However, summarizing friction data in this manner has been considered as conceptually questionable by others from a mechanics perspective.[3] Fig. 1 shows that the type of instabilities and friction forces encountered on a single surface can vary widely depending on the conditions. As a result, large variations in the friction coefficient are expected, depending on the mass and velocity — even though measurements originate from the same surface. This variability in friction coefficient can be seen with the large interquartile range of friction coefficients, which shows that the variation in friction coefficient across a single surface is similar, or even larger, than the differences in average friction coefficient across two different surfaces. The observation that friction coefficients vary so widely on a single surface calls into question the approach of analyzing how humans may perceive two different objects based on their average friction coefficients.”

      Page 9, Fig. 2 Caption

      “D) GLMM of accuracy vs. difference in average friction coefficient , showing a negative correlation. E) GLMMs of accuracy vs. other commonly used material properties or parameters: ΔAverage roughness R<sub>a</sub>, ΔHurst exponent H, and ΔWater contact angle hysteresis (º) (N = 10 participants_, _n = 600 total trials).”

      Page 9

      “Considering all instabilities individually, we found that only steady sliding was a positive, statistically significant predictor. (r \= 0.62, p < 0.05, shown in Fig. 2B).”

      Page 10

      “To compare the value of looking at frictional instabilities, we also performed GLMM fits on common approaches in the field, like a friction coefficient or material property typically used in tactile discrimination, shown in Fig. 2D-E. Interestingly, in Fig. 2D, we observed a spurious, negative correlation between friction coefficient (typically and often problematically simplified as across all tested conditions) and accuracy (r = -0.64, p < 0.01); that is, the more different the surfaces are by friction coefficient, the less people can tell them apart. This spurious correlation would be the opposite of intuition, and further calls into question the common practice of using friction coefficients in touch-related studies. Interestingly, this spurious correlation was also found by Gueorguiev et al.[21] The alternative, two-term model which includes adhesive contact area for friction coefficient[32] was even less predictive (see Fig. S6A of SI). We believe such a correlation could not have been uncovered previously as our samples are minimal in their physical variations. Yet, the dynamic changes in force even within a single sample are not considered, despite being a key feature of mesoscale friction during human touch.

      We investigate different material properties in Fig. 2E. Differences in average roughness R<sub>a</sub> (or other parameters, like root mean square roughness R<sub>rms</sub> (Fig. S6A of SI) did not show a statistically significant correlation to accuracy. Though roughness is a popular parameter, correlating any roughness parameter to human performance here could be moot: the limit of detecting roughness differences has previously been defined as 13 nm on structured surfaces[36] and much higher for randomly rough surfaces,[49] all of which are magnitudes larger than the roughness differences between our surfaces. The differences in contact angle hysteresis – as an approximation of the adhesion contributions[50] – do not present any statistically significant effects on performance.”

      Page 11-12

      “Despite the correlative nature of this study, we still obtained high correlations compared to existing biomechanical studies[4,19,21], which we speculate is because instabilities are an important predictive phenomenon for models of human touch. We believe that biomechanical studies, including more sophisticated techniques, like spatially resolved force maps from digital image correlation[5,42] may yield stronger correlations and results if they analyze data based on instabilities.

      Added References

      (2) Khamis, H. et al. Friction sensing mechanisms for perception and motor control: passive touch without sliding may not provide perceivable frictional information. J. Neurophysiol. 125, 809– 823 (2021).

      (6) Olczak, D., Sukumar, V. & Pruszynski, J. A. Edge orientation perception during active touch. J. Neurophysiol. 120, 2423–2429 (2018).

      Comment 2, Part 1

      Along the same lines, other parameters that were considered such as the "Percent Correct vs. Difference in Sp" and "Percent Correct vs. Difference in SFW" were not plotted for consideration in the SI. It would be helpful to compare these results with the other three metrics in order to fully understand the relationships.

      We have added these plots to the SI. We note that we had checked these relationships and discussed them briefly, but did not include the plot. The plots show that the type of instability was not as helpful as its presence or absence.

      Our changes to the manuscript

      Page 9

      “Furthermore, a model accounting for slow frictional waves alone specifically shows a significant, negative effect on performance (p < 0.01, Fig. S5 of SI), suggesting that in these samples and task, the type of instability was not as important.”

      “Fig. S5. GLMM fits of participant accuracy vs. the differences in instability incidence for individual instability types. Left: accuracy vs. differences in formation of slow frictional waves (SFW) between pairs. P1 and P5 have the same x-axis value and are shifted for clarity. Right: accuracy vs. differences in formation of stiction spikes (Sp).”

      SI Page 4

      “and no correlation between accuracy and stiction spikes (Fig. S5).”

      Comment 2, Part 2

      Other parameters such as stiction magnitude and differences in friction coefficient over the test space could also be important and interesting.

      We agree these are interesting and have thought about them. We are aware that others, like Gueorguiev et al., have studied stiction magnitudes, and though there was a correlation, the physical differences in surface roughness (glass versus PMMA) investigated made it unclear if these could be generalized further.[3] We are unsure how to proceed here with a satisfactory analysis of stiction magnitude, given that stiction spikes are not always generated. In fact, Fig. 1 shows that for many velocities and pressures, stiction spikes are not formed. In ongoing work, however, we are always cognizant that if stiction spikes are a dominant factor, then a secondary analysis on their magnitude would be important. We offer some speculation on why stiction spikes may be overrepresented in the literature:

      (1) They are prone to being created if the finger was loaded for a long time onto a surface prior to movement, thus creating adhesion by contact aging which is unlike active human exploration. We avoid this by discarding the first pull in our measurements, which is a standard practice in mechanical characterization if contact aging needs to be avoided.

      (2) The ranges of velocities and pressures explored by others were small.

      (3) In an effort to generate strong tactile stimuli, highly adhesive or rough surfaces are used.

      (4) Stiction spikes are visually distinctive on a plot, but we are unaware of any mechanistic reason that mechanoreceptors would be particularly sensitive to this low frequency event over other signals.

      We interpret “difference in friction coefficient over the test space” to be, for a single surface, like C4, to find the highest average friction for a condition of single velocity and mass and subtract that from the lowest average friction for a condition of single velocity and mass. We calculated the difference in friction coefficient in the typical manner of the field, by averaging all data collected at all velocities and masses and assigning a single value for all of a surface, like C4. We had performed this, and have the data, but we are wary of overinterpreting secondary and tertiary metrics because they do not have any fundamental basis in traditional tribology, and this value, if used by humans, would suggest that they rapidly explore a large parameter space to find a “maximum” and “minimum” friction. Furthermore, the range in friction across the test space, after averaging, can be smaller than the range of friction experienced at different masses and velocities on a single surface. We have tabulated and newly included these values (the interquartile range of friction coefficients of different masses and velocities per surface) in Table 1.

      Fig. 2D shows a GLMM fit between percent correct responses across our pairs and the differences in friction coefficient for each pair, where we see a spurious negative correlation. As we had the data of all average friction coefficients for each condition for a given material, we also looked at the difference in maximum and minimum friction coefficients. For our tested pairs, these differences also lined up on a statistically significant, negative GLMM fit (r = -0.86, p < 0.005). However, the values for a given surface can vary drastically, with an interquartile range of 1.20 to 2.09 on a single surface. We fit participant accuracy to the differences in these IQRs across pairs. This also led to a negative GLMM fit (r = -0.65, p < 0.05). However, we are hesitant to add this plot to the manuscript for the reasons stated previously.

      Comment 3, Part 1

      Beyond this fundamental concern, there is a weakness in the representativeness of the PDMS finger, the vertical motion, and the speed of sliding to real human exploration.

      Overall, this is a continuous debate that we think offers two solutions, and we are not advocating for an “either-or” case. There is always a tradeoff between using a synthetic model of a finger versus a real human finger, and there is a place for both models. That is, while our mock finger will be “better” the more similar it is to a human finger, it is not our goal to fully replace a human finger. Rather our goal is to provide a consistent method of characterizing surfaces that is sufficiently similar to human touch as to be a useful and predictive tool.

      The usefulness of the mock finger is in isolating the features of each surface that is independent of human variability, i.e., instabilities that form without changing loading conditions between sliding motions or even within one sliding motion. Of course, with this method, we still require confirmation of these features still forming during human exploration, which we show in Fig. 3. We believe that this method of characterizing surfaces at the mesoscale will ultimately lead to more successful human studies on tactile perception. Currently, and as shown in the paper, characterizing surfaces through traditional techniques, such as a commercial tribometer (friction coefficient, using a steel or hard metal ball), roughness (via atomic force microscopy or some other metrology), surface energy are less or not at all predictive. Thus, we believe this mock finger is better than the current state-of-the-art characterizing surfaces (we are also aware of a commercial mock finger company, but we were unable to purchase or obtain an evaluation model).

      One of the main – and severe – limitations of using a human finger is that all fingers are different, meaning any study focusing on a particular user may not apply to others or be recreated easily by other researchers. We do not think it is feasible to set a standard for replication around a real human finger as that participant may no longer be available, or willing to travel the world as a “standard”. Furthermore, the method in which a person changes their pressures and velocities is different. We note that this is a challenge unique to touch perception – how an object is touched changes the friction generated, and thus the tactile stimulus generated, whereas a standardized stimulus is more straightforward for light or sound.

      However, we do emphasize that we have strongly considered the balance between feasibility and ecological validity in the design of a mock finger. We have a mock finger, with the three components of stiffness of a human finger (more below). Furthermore, we have also successfully used this mock finger in correlations with human psychophysics in previous work, where findings from our mechanical experiments were more predictive of human performance[4–7] than other available methods.

      Our changes to the manuscript Added (Page 2-3)

      “Mock finger as a characterization tool

      We use a mechanical setup with a PDMS (poly(dimethylsiloxane)) mock finger to derive tactile predictors as opposed to direct biomechanical measurements on human participants. While there is a tradeoff in selecting a synthetic finger over a real human finger to modeling human touch, human fingers themselves are also highly variable[23] both in their physical shape and their use during human motion. Our goal is to design a consistent method of characterization of samples that can be easily accessed by other researchers and does not rely on a standard established around single human participant. We believe that sufficient replication of surface, bulk properties, and contact geometry results in characterization that isolates consistent features of surfaces that are not derived from human-to-human variability. We have used this approach to successfully correlate human results with mock finger characterization previously.[8,9,24]

      The major component of a human finger, by volume, is soft tissue (~56%),[25] resulting in an effective modulus close to 100 kPa.[26,27] In order to achieve this same softness, we crosslink PDMS in a 1×1×5 cm mold at a 30:1 elastomer:crosslinker ratio. In addition, two more features in the human finger impart significant mechanical differences. Human fingers have a bone at the fingertip, the distal phalanx,[26–28, 8–10]which we mimic with an acrylic “bone” within our PDMS network. The stratum corneum, the stiffer, glassier outer layer of skin,[29] is replicated with the surface of the mock finger glassified, or further crosslinked, after 8 hours of UV-Ozone treatment.30 This treatment also modifies the surface properties of the native PDMS to align with those of a human finger more closely: it minimizes the viscoelastic tack at the surface, resulting in a comparable non-sticky surface. Stabilizing after one day after treatment, the mock finger surface obtains a moderate hydrophilicity (~60º), as is typically observed for a real finger.[11,31]

      The initial contact area formed before a friction trace is collected is a rectangle of 1×1 cm. While this shape is not entirely representative of a human finger with curves and ridges, human fingers flatten out enough to reduce the effects of curvature with even very light pressures.[31–33] This implies that for most realistic finger pressures, the contact area is largely load-independent, which is more accurately replicated with a rectangular mock finger.

      Lastly, we consider the role of fingerprint ridges. A key finding of our previous work is that while fingerprints enhanced frictional dynamics at certain conditions, key features were still maintained with a flat finger.[11] Furthermore, for some loading conditions, the more amplified signals could also result in more similar friction traces for different surfaces. We have observed good agreement between these friction traces and human experiments.[8,9,22,34]”

      Page 3-4, Materials and Methods

      “Mock Finger Preparation

      Friction forces across all six surfaces were measured using a custom apparatus with a polydimethylsiloxane (PDMS, Dow Sylgard 184) mock finger that mimics a human finger’s mechanical properties and contact mechanics while exploring a surface relatively closely.[8,9] PDMS and crosslinker were combined in a 30:1 ratio to achieve a stiffness of 100 kPa comparable to a real finger, then degassed in a vacuum desiccator for 30 minutes. We are aware that the manufacturer recommended crosslinking ratio for Sylgard 184 is 10:1 due to potential uncrosslinked liquid residues,[35] but further crosslinking concentrated at the surface prevents this. The prepared PDMS was then poured into a 1×1×5 cm mold also containing an acrylic 3D-printed “bone” to attach applied masses on top of the “fingertip” area contacting a surface during friction testing. After crosslinking in the mold at 60ºC for 1 hour, the finger was treated with UV-Ozone for 8 hours out of the mold to minimize viscoelastic tack.

      Mechanical Testing

      A custom device using our PDMS mock finger was used to collect macroscopic friction force traces replicating human exploration.[8,9] After placing a sample surface on a stage, the finger was lowered at a slight angle such that an initial 1×1 cm rectangle of “fingertip” contact area could be established. We considered a broad range of applied masses (M \= 0, 25, 75, and 100 g) added onto the deadweight of the finger (6 g) observed during a tactile discrimination task. The other side of the sensor was connected to a motorized stage (V-508 PIMag Precision Linear Stage, Physikinstrumente) to control both displacement (4 mm across all conditions) and sliding velocity (v \= 5, 10, 25, and 45 mm s<sup>-1</sup>). Forces were measured at all 16 combinations of mass and velocity via a 250 g Futek force sensor (k \= 13.9 kN m<sup>-1</sup>) threaded to the bone, and recorded at an average sampling rate of 550 Hz with a Keithley 7510 DMM digitized multimeter. Force traces were collected in sets of 4 slides, discarding the first due to contact aging. Because some mass-velocity combinations were near the boundaries of instability phase transitions, not all force traces at these given conditions exhibited similar profiles. Thus, three sets were collected on fresh spots for each condition to observe enough occurrences of multiple instabilities, at a total of nine traces per combination for each surface.”

      Added References

      (23) Infante, V. H. P. et al. The role of skin hydration, skin deformability, and age in tactile friction and perception of materials. Sci. Rep. 15, 9935 (2025).

      (24) Nolin, A., Lo, C.-Y., Kayser, L. V. & Dhong, C. B. Transparent and Electrically Switchable Thin Film Tactile Actuators Based on Molecular Orientation. Preprint at https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.2411.07968 (2024).

      (25) Murai, M., Lau, H.-K., Pereira, B. P. & Pho, R. W. H. A cadaver study on volume and surface area of the fingertip. J. Hand Surg. 22, 935–941 (1997).

      (26) Abdouni, A. et al. Biophysical properties of the human finger for touch comprehension: influences of ageing and gender. R. Soc. Open Sci. (2017) doi:10.1098/rsos.170321.

      (27) Cornuault, P.-H., Carpentier, L., Bueno, M.-A., Cote, J.-M. & Monteil, G. Influence of physico-chemical, mechanical and morphological fingerpad properties on the frictional distinction of sticky/slippery surfaces. J. R. Soc. Interface (2015) doi:10.1098/rsif.2015.0495.

      (28) Qian, K. et al. Mechanical properties vary for different regions of the finger extensor apparatus. J. Biomech. 47, 3094–3099 (2014).

      (29) Yuan, Y. & Verma, R. Measuring microelastic properties of stratum corneum. Colloids Surf. B Biointerfaces 48, 6–12 (2006).

      (30) Fu, Y.-J. et al. Effect of UV-Ozone Treatment on Poly(dimethylsiloxane) Membranes: Surface Characterization and Gas Separation Performance. Langmuir 26, 4392–4399 (2010).

      Comment 3, Part 2

      The real finger has multiple layers with different moduli. In fact, the stratum corneum cells, which are the outer layer at the interface and determine the friction, have a much higher modulus than PDMS. The real finger has multiple layers with different moduli. In fact, the stratum corneum cells, which are the outer layer at the interface and determine the friction, have a much higher modulus than PDMS.

      We have approximated the softness of the finger with 100 kPa crosslinked PDMS, which is close to what has been reported for the bulk of a human fingertip.[9,10] However, as mentioned in the Materials and Methods, there are two additional features of the mock finger that impart greater strength. The PDMS surrounds a rigid, acrylic bone comparable to the distal phalanx, which provides an additional layer of higher modulus.[8] Additionally, the 8-hour UV-Ozone treatment decreases the viscoelastic tack of the pristine PDMS by glassifying, or further crosslinking the surface of the finger,[12] therefore imparting greater stiffness at the surface similar to the contributions of the stratum corneum, along with a similar surface energy.[13] This technique is widely used in wearables,[14] soft robotics,[15] and microfluidics[16] to induce both these material changes. Additionally, the finger is used at least a day after UV-Ozone treatment is completed to generate a stable surface that is moderately hydrophilic, similar to the outermost layer of human skin.[17]

      Comment 3, Part 3

      In addition, the slanted position of the finger can cause non-uniform pressures across the finger. Both can contribute to making the PDMS finger have much more stick-slip than a real finger.

      To ensure that there is minimal contribution from the slanted position of the finger, an initial contact area of 1×1 cm is established before sliding and recording friction measurements. As the PDMS finger is a soft object, the portion in contact with a surface flattens and the contact area remains largely unchanged during sliding. Any additional stick-slip after this alignment step is caused by contact aging at the interface, but the first trace we collect is always discarded to only consider stick-slip events caused by surface chemistry. We recognize that it is difficult to completely control the pressure distribution due to the planar interface, but this is also expected when humans freely explore a surface.

      Comment 3, Part 4

      In fact, if you look at the regime maps, there is very little space that has steady sliding. This does not represent well human exploration of surfaces. We do not tend to use a force and velocity that will cause extensive stick-slip (frequent regions of 100% stick-slip) and, in fact, the speeds used in the study are on the slow side, which also contributes to more stick-slip. At higher speeds and lower forces, all of the materials had steady sliding regions.”

      We are not aware of published studies that extensively show that humans avoid stickslip regimes. In fact, we are aware familiar with literature where stiction spike formation is suppressed – a recent paper by AliAbbasi, Basdogan et. al. investigates electroadhesion and friction with NaCl solution-infused interfaces, resulting in significantly steadier forces.[18] We also directly showed evidence of instability formation that we observed during human exploration in Fig. 3B-C. These dynamic events are common, despite the lack of control of normal forces and sliding velocities. We also note that Reviewer 1, Comment 2, Part 2 was suggesting that we further explore possible trends from parameterizing the stiction spike.

      We note that many studies have often not gone at the velocities and masses required for stiction spikes – even though these masses and velocities would be routinely seen in free exploration – this is usually due to constraints of their equipment.[19] Sliding events during human free exploration of surfaces can exceed 100 mm/s for rapid touches. However, for the surfaces investigated here, we observe that large regions of stick-slip can emerge at velocities as low as 5 mm/s depending on the applied load. The incidence of steady sliding appears more dependent on the applied mass, with almost no steady sliding observed at or above 75 g. Indeed, the force categorization along our transition zones is the main point of the paper.

      Comment 3, Part 5

      Further, on these very smooth surfaces, the friction and stiction are more complex and cannot dismiss considerations such as finger material property change with sweat pore occlusion and sweat capillary forces. Also, the vertical motion of both the PDMS finger and the instructed human subjects is not the motion that humans typically use to discriminate between surfaces.

      We did not describe the task sufficiently. Humans were only given the instruction to slide their finger along a single axis from top to bottom of a sample, not vertical as in azimuthal to gravity. We have updated our wording in the manuscript to reflect this.

      Page 4

      “Participants could touch for as long as they wanted, but were asked to only use their dominant index fingers along a single axis to better mimic the conditions for instability formation during mechanical testing with the mock finger.”

      Page 11

      “The participant was then asked to explore each sample simultaneously, and ran over each surface in strokes along a single axis until the participant could decide which of the two had “more friction”.”

      Comment 3, Part 6

      Finally, fingerprints may not affect the shape and size of the contact area, but they certainly do affect the dynamic response and detection of vibrations.”

      We are aware of the nuance. Our previous work on the role of fingerprints on friction experienced by a PDMS mock finger showed enhanced signals with the incorporation of ridges on the finger and used a rate-and-state model of a heterogenous, elastic body to find corresponding trends (though there is no existing model of friction that can accurately model experiments on mesoscale friction).[11] The key conclusion was that a flat finger still preserved key dynamic features, and the presence of stronger or more vibrations could result in more similar forces for different surfaces depending on the sliding conditions.

      This is also in the context that we are seeking to provide a reasonable and experimentally accessible method to characterize surfaces, which will always be better as we get closer in replicating a true human finger. But our goal here was to replicate the finger sufficiently for use in human studies. We believe the more appropriate metric of success is if the mock finger is more successful than replacing traditional characterization experiments, like friction coefficient, roughness, surface energy, etc.

      Comment 4

      This all leads to the critical question, why are friction, normal force, and velocity not measured during the measured human exploration and in a systematic study using the real human finger? The authors posed an extremely interesting hypothesis that humans may alter their speed to feel the instability transition regions. This is something that could be measured with a real finger but is not likely to be correlated accurately enough to match regime boundaries with such a simplified artificial finger.

      We are excited that our manuscript offers a tractable manner to test the hypothesis that tactile decision-making models use friction instabilities as evidence. However, we lay out the challenges and barriers, and how the scope of this paper will lead us in that direction. We also clarify that our goals are to provide a method to characterize samples to better design tactile interfaces in haptics or in psychophysical experiments and raise awareness that the common methods of sample characterization in touch by an average friction coefficient or roughness is fundamentally unsound. Throughout the paper, we have made changes to reflect that our study, at this point, is only correlative.

      As discussed in the summary, and with additional detail here, to further support our findings through observation on humans would require answering:

      (1) Which one, or combination of, of the multiple swipes that people make responsible for a tactile decision? (There is a need for a decision-making model)

      (2) Establish what is, or may be, tactile evidence.

      (3) Establish tactile decision-making models are similar or different than existing decision-making models.

      (4) Design a task that does not require the use of subjective tactile descriptors, like “which one feels rougher”, which we have seen causes confusion in participants, which will likely require accounting for memory effects.

      We elaborate these points below:

      To successfully perform this experiment, we note that freely exploring humans make multiple strokes on a surface. Therefore, we would need to construct a decision-making model. It has not yet been demonstrated whether tactile decision making follows visual decision making, but perhaps to start, we can assume it does. Then, in the design of our decision-making paradigm, we immediately run into the problem: What is tactile evidence?

      From Fig. 3C, we already can see that identifying evidence is challenging. Prior to this manuscript, people may have chosen the average force, or the highest force. Or we may choose the average friction force. Then, after deciding on the evidence, we need to find a method to manipulate the evidence, i.e., create samples or a machine that causes high friction, etc. We show that during the course of human touch, due to the dynamic nature of friction, the average can change a large amount and sample design becomes a central barrier to experiments. Others may suggest immobilizing the finger and applying a known force, but given how much friction changes with human exploration, there is no known method to make a machine recreate temporally and spatially varying friction forces during sliding onto a stationary finger. Finally, perhaps most importantly, in addition to mechanical challenges, a study by Liu, Colgate et al. showed that even if they recorded the friction (2D) of a finger exploring a surface and then replicated the same friction forces onto a finger, the participant could not determine which surface the replayed friction force was supposed to represent.[1] This supports that the efference copy is important, that the forces in response to expected motion are important to determine friction. Finally, there is no known method to design instabilities a priori. They must be found through experiments. Especially since if we were to introduce, say a bump or a trough, then we bring in confounding variables to how participants tell surfaces apart.

      Furthermore, even if we had some consistent method to create tactile “evidence”, the paradigm also deserves some consideration. In our experience, the 3-AFC task we perform is important because the vocabulary for touch has not been established. That is, in 3-AFC, by asking to determine which one sample is unlike the others, we do not have to ask the participant questions like “which one is rougher” or “which one has less friction”. In contrast, 2-AFC, which is better for decision-making models because it does not include memory, requires the asking of a perceptual question like: “which one is rougher?”. In our ongoing work, taking two silane coatings, we found that participants could easily identify which surface is unlike the others above chance in a 3-AFC, but participants, even within their own trials, could not consistently identify one silane as perceptually “rougher” by 2-AFC. To us, this calls into question the validity of tactile descriptors, but is beyond the scope of this manuscript.

      This is not our only goal, but in the context of human exploration, in this manuscript here, we believed it was important to identify a mechanical parameter that was consistent with how humans explore surfaces, but was also a parameter that could characterize to some consistent property of a surface – irrespective of whether a human was touching it. We thought that designing human decision-making models and paradigms around the friction coefficient would not be successful.

      Given the scope of these challenges, we do not think it would be possible to establish these conceptual sequences in a single manuscript. However, we think that our manuscript brings an important step forward to approach this problem.

      Reviewer 2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this paper, the authors want to test the hypothesis that frictional instabilities rather than friction are the main drivers for discriminating flat surfaces of different sub-nanometric roughness profiles.

      They first produced flat surfaces with 6 different coatings giving them unique and various properties in terms of roughness (picometer scale), contact angles (from hydrophilic to hydrophobic), friction coefficient (as measured against a mock finger), and Hurst exponent.

      Then, they used those surfaces in two different experiments. In the first experiment, they used a mock finger (PDMS of 100kPA molded into a fingertip shape) and slid it over the surfaces at different normal forces and speeds. They categorized the sliding behavior as steady sliding, sticking spikes, and slow frictional waves by visual inspection, and show that the surfaces have different behaviors depending on normal force and speed. In a second experiment, participants (10) were asked to discriminate pairs of those surfaces. It is found that each of those pairs could be reliably discriminated by most participants.

      Finally, the participant's discrimination performance is correlated with differences in the physical attributes observed against the mock finger. The authors found a positive correlation between participants' performances and differences in the count of steady sliding against the mock finger and a negative correlation between participants' reaction time and differences in the count of stiction spikes against the mock finger. They interpret those correlations as evidence that participants use those differences to discriminate the surfaces.

      Strengths:

      The created surfaces are very interesting as they are flat at the nanometer scale, yet have different physical attributes and can be reliably discriminated.

      We thank Reviewer 2 for their notes on our manuscript. The responses below address the reviewer’s comments and recommendations for revised work.

      Weaknesses:

      Comment 1

      In my opinion, the data presented in the paper do not support the conclusions. The conclusions are based on a correlation between results obtained on the mock finger and results obtained with human participants but there is no evidence that the human participants' fingertips will behave similarly to the mock finger during the experiment. Figure 3 gives a hint that the 3 sliding behaviors can be observed in a real finger, but does not prove that the human finger will behave as the mock finger, i.e., there is no evidence that the phase maps in Figure 1C are similar for human fingers and across different people that can have very different stiffness and moisture levels.

      We have made changes throughout the manuscript to acknowledge that our findings are correlative, clarifying this throughout, and incorporating into the discussion how our work may enable biomechanical measurements and tactile decision making models.

      The mechanical characterization conducted with the mock finger seeks to extract significant features of friction traces of a set of surfaces to use as predictors of tactile discriminability. The goal is to find a consistent method to characterize surfaces for use in tactile experiments that can be replicated by others and used prior to any human experiments. However, in the overall response and in a response to a similar comment by Reviewer 1 (recreated below), we also explain why we believe experiments on humans to establish this fact is not yet reasonable.

      First, we discuss the mock finger. The PDMS finger is treated to have comparable surface and bulk properties to a human finger. We have approximated the softness of the finger with 100 kPa crosslinked PDMS, which is close to what has been reported for the bulk of a human fingertip.[9,10] However, as mentioned in the Materials and Methods, there are two additional features of the mock finger that impart greater strength. The PDMS surrounds a rigid, acrylic bone comparable to the distal phalanx, which provides an additional layer of higher modulus.[8] Additionally, the 8-hour UV-Ozone treatment decreases the viscoelastic tack of the pristine PDMS by glassifying, or further crosslinking the surface of the finger,[12] therefore imparting greater stiffness at the surface similar to the contributions of the stratum corneum, along with a similar surface energy.[13] Additionally, the finger is used at least a day after UV-Ozone treatment is completed in order for the surface to return to moderate hydrophilicity, similar to the outermost layer of human skin.[17] We also discuss the shape of the contact formed. To ensure that there is minimal contribution from the slanted position of the finger, an initial contact area of 1×1 cm is established before sliding and recording friction measurements. As the PDMS finger is a soft object, the portion in contact with a surface flattens and the contact area remains largely unchanged during sliding. Any additional stick-slip after this alignment step is caused by contact aging at the interface, but the first trace we collect is always discarded to only consider stick-slip events caused by surface chemistry. We recognize that it is difficult to completely control the pressure distribution due to the planar interface, but this is also expected when humans freely explore a surface. Finally, we consider flat vs. fingerprinted fingers. Our previous work on the role of fingerprints on friction experienced by a PDMS mock finger showed enhanced signals with the incorporation of ridges on the finger and used a rate-and-state model of a heterogenous, elastic body to find corresponding trends.[11] The key conclusion was that a flat finger still preserved key dynamic features, and the presence of stronger or more vibrations could result in more similar forces for different surfaces depending on the sliding conditions. We note that we have subsequently used this flat mock finger in correlations with human psychophysics in previous work, where findings from our mechanical experiments were predictive of human performance.[4–7] We have added these details to the manuscript.

      With this adequately similar mock finger, we collected friction traces at controlled conditions of normal force and velocity in order to extract the signals unique to each material that are not caused by the influence of human variability. For example, we observe the smallest regions of steady sliding on our phase maps (Fig. 1C) for short-chain alkylsilanes C4 and C5, while the increased intermolecular forces of other silanes increase the incidence of steady sliding. We have also previously shown that comparisons of similarly collected mechanical data is predictive of human performance, using the crosscorrelations between signals of two different materials.[4–7] While different participants produce different raw signals, we see that broad categories of stick-slip, i.e. instabilities, can be extracted (Fig. 3B-C) and used as a cue in a tactile discrimination task. As mentioned above, we have provided an additional section about the usefulness of our mock finger, as well as its structure, in the main manuscript.

      Second, we lay out the challenges and barriers to demonstrating this in humans in the manner requested by the reviewer, and how the scope of this paper will lead us in that direction. We also clarify that our goals are to provide a method to characterize samples to better design tactile interfaces in haptics or in psychophysical experiments and raise awareness that the common methods of sample characterization in touch by an average friction coefficient or roughness is fundamentally unsound.

      As discussed in the summary, and with additional detail here, to further support our findings through observation on humans would require answering:

      (1) Which one, or combination of, of the multiple swipes that people make responsible for a tactile decision?

      (2) Establish what is, or may be, tactile evidence.

      (3) Establish tactile decision-making models are similar or different than existing decision-making models.

      (4) Test the hypothesis, in these models, that friction instabilities are evidence, and not some other unknown metric.

      (5) Design a task that does not require the use of subjective tactile descriptors, like “which one feels rougher”, which we see cause confusion in participants, which will likely require accounting for memory effects.

      We elaborate these points below:

      To successfully perform this experiment, we note that freely exploring humans make multiple strokes on a surface. Therefore, we would need to construct a decision-making model. It has not yet been demonstrated whether tactile decision making follows visual decision making, but perhaps to start, we can assume it does. Then, in the design of our decision-making paradigm, we immediately run into the problem: What is tactile evidence?

      From Fig. 3C, we already can see that identifying evidence is challenging. Prior to this manuscript, people may have chosen the average force, or the highest force. Or we may choose the average friction force. Then, after deciding on the evidence, we need to find a method to manipulate the evidence, i.e., create samples or a machine that causes high friction, etc. We show that during the course of human touch, due to the dynamic nature of friction, the average can change a large amount and sample design becomes a central barrier to experiments. Others may suggest immobilizing the finger and applying a known force, but given how much friction changes with human exploration, there is no known method to make a machine recreate temporally and spatially varying friction forces during sliding onto a stationary finger. Finally, perhaps most importantly, in addition to mechanical challenges, a study by Liu, Colgate, et al. showed that even if they recorded the friction (2D) of a finger exploring a surface and then replicated the same friction forces onto a finger, the participant could not determine which surface the replayed friction force was supposed to represent.[1] This supports that the efference copy is important, that the forces in response to expected motion are important to determine friction. Finally, there is no known method to design instabilities a priori. They must be found through experiments, especially since if we were to introduce, say a bump or a trough, then we bring in confounding variables to how participants tell surfaces apart.

      Furthermore, even if we had some consistent method to create tactile “evidence”, the paradigm also deserves some consideration. In our experience, the 3-AFC task we perform is important because the vocabulary for touch has not been established. That is, in 3-AFC, by asking to determine which one sample is unlike the others, we do not have to ask the participant questions like “which one is rougher” or “which one has less friction”. In contrast, 2-AFC, which is better for decision-making models because it does not include memory, requires the asking of a perceptual question like: “which one is rougher?”. In our ongoing work, taking two silane coatings, we found that participants could easily identify which surface is unlike the others above chance in a 3-AFC, but participants, even within their own trials, could not consistently identify one silane as perceptually “rougher” by 2-AFC. To us, this calls into question the validity of tactile descriptors, but is beyond the scope of the current manuscript.

      This is not our only goal, but in the context of human exploration, in this manuscript here, we believed it was important to identify a mechanical parameter that was consistent with how humans explore surfaces, but was also a parameter that could characterize to some consistent property of a surface – irrespective of whether a human was touching it. We thought that designing human decision-making models and paradigms around the friction coefficient would not be successful.

      Given the scope of these challenges, we do not think it would be possible to establish this conceptual sequence in a single manuscript.

      See Reviewer 1, comment 3part 3 for changes to the manuscript

      Comment 2

      I believe that the authors collected the contact forces during the psychophysics experiments, so this shortcoming could be solved if the authors use the actual data, and show that the participant responses can be better predicted by the occurrence of frictional instabilities than by the usual metrics on a trial by trial basis, or at least on a subject by subject basis. I.e. Poor performers should show fewer signs of differences in the sliding behaviors than good performers.

      To fully implement this, a decision-making model is necessary because, as a counter example, a participant could have generated 10 swipes of SFW and 1 swipe of a Sp, but the Sp may have been the most important event for making a tactile decision. This type of scenario is not compatible with the analysis suggested — and similar counterpoints can be made for other types of seemingly straightforward analysis.

      While we are interested and actively working on this, the study here is critical to establish types of evidence for a future decision-making model. We know humans change their friction constantly during real exploration, so it is unclear which of these constantly changing values we should input into the decision making model, and the future challenges we anticipate are explained in Weaknesses, Comment 1.

      Comment 3

      The sample size (10) is very small.

      We recognize that, with all factors being equal, this sample size is on the smaller end. However, we emphasize the degree of control of samples is far above typical, with minimal variations in sample properties such as surface roughness, and every sample for every trial was pristine. Furthermore, the sample preparation (> 300 individual wafers were used) became a factor. Although not typically appropriate, and thus not included in the manuscript, a post-hoc power analysis for our 100 trials of our pair that was closest to chance, P4, (53%, closest to chance at 33%) showed a power of 98.2%, suggesting that the study was appropriately powered.

      Reviewer 2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Comment 1

      Differences in SS and Sp (Table 2) are NOT physical or mechanical differences but are obtained by counting differences in the number of occurrences of each sliding behavior. It is rather a weird choice.

      We disagree that differences in SS and Sp are not physical or mechanical, as these are well-established phenomena in the soft matter and tribology literature.[20–22] These are known as “mechanical instabilities” and generated due to the effects of two physical phenomena: the elasticity of the finger (which is constant in our mechanical testing) and the friction forces present (which change per sample type). The motivation behind using these different shapes is that the instabilities, in some conditions, can be invariant to external factors like velocity. This would be quite advantageous for human exploration because, unlike friction coefficient, which changes with nearly any factor, including velocity and mass, the instabilities being invariant to velocity would mean that we are accurately characterizing a unique identifier of the surface even though velocity may be variable.

      This “weird choice” is the central innovation of this paper. This choice was necessary because we demonstrated that the common usage of friction coefficient is fundamentally flawed: we see that friction coefficient suggests that surface which are more different would feel more similar – indeed the most distinctive surfaces would be two surfaces that are identical, which is clearly spurious. Furthermore, Table 1 now includes the range of friction generated on a surface, the range of friction coefficients of a single surface is large – of order the differences in friction between two surfaces. This is expected in soft sliding systems and emphasizes our issue with the use of average friction coefficient in psychophysical design. One potential explanation for why we were able to see this is effect is because our surfaces have similar (< 0.6 nm variability) roughness, removing potential confounding factors from large scale roughness, and this type of low roughness control has not been widely used in tactile studies to the best of our knowledge.

      Comment 2

      Figures 2B-C: why are the x-data different than Table 2?

      The x-data in Fig. 2B-C are the absolute differences in the number of occurrences measured for a given instability type or material property out of 144 pulls. Modeling the human participant results in our GLMMs required the independent variables to be in this form rather than percentages. We initially chose to list percent differences in Table 2 to highlight the ranges of differences instead of an absolute value, but have added both for clarity.

      Our changes to the manuscript

      Page 7

      “To determine if humans can detect these three different instabilities, we selected six pairs of surfaces to create a broad range of potential instabilities present across all three types. These are summarized in Table 2, where the first column for each instability is the difference in occurrence of that instability formed between each pair, and the second is the percent difference.”

      “Thus, when comparing C4 versus C4-APTMS, they have a difference in steady sliding of 20 out of a maximum 144 pulls, for a |ΔSS| of 13.9%. The absolute value is taken to compare total differences present, as the psychophysical task does not distinguish between sample order.”

      Comment 3

      We constructed a set of coated surfaces with physical differences which were imperceptible by touch but created different types of instabilities based on how quickly a finger is slid and how hard a human finger is pressed during sliding." Yet, in your experiment, participants could discriminate them, so this is incoherent.

      To clarify the point, macroscopic objects can differ in physical shape and in chemical composition. What we meant was that the physical differences, i.e., roughness, were below a limit (Skedung et al.) that participants, without a coating, would not be able to tell these apart.[23] Therefore, the reason people could tell our surfaces apart was due to the chemical composition of the surface, and not any differences in roughness or physical effects like film stiffness (due to the molecular-scale thinness of the surface coatings, they are mechanically negligible). However, we concede that at the molecular scale, the traditional macroscopic distinction between physical and chemical is blurred.

      We have made minor revisions to the wording in the abstract. We clarify that the surface coatings had physical differences in roughness that were smaller than 0.6 nm, which based purely on roughness, would not be expected to be distinguishable to participants. Therefore, the reason participants can tell these surfaces apart is due to differences in friction generated by chemical composition, and we were able to minimize contributions from physical differences in the sample our study.

      Our changes to the manuscript

      Page 1, Abstract

      “Here, we constructed a set of coated surfaces with minimal physical differences that by themselves, are not perceptible to people, but instead, due to modification in surface chemistry, the surfaces created different types of instabilities based on how quickly a finger is slid and how hard a human finger is pressed during sliding.”

      “In one experiment, we used a mechanical mock finger to quantify and classify differences in instability formation from different coated surfaces. In a second experiment, participants perform a discrimination task using the same coated surfaces. Using the data from these two experiments, we found that human discrimination response times were faster with surfaces where the mock finger produced more stiction spikes and discrimination accuracy was higher where the mock finger produced more steady sliding. Conversely, traditional metrics like surface roughness or average friction coefficient did not relate to tactile discriminability. In fact, the typical method of averaging friction coefficients led to a spurious correlation which erroneously suggests that distinct objects should feel identical and identical objects should feel distinct—similar to findings by others. Friction instabilities may offer a more predictive and tractable framework of fine touch perception than friction coefficients, which would accelerate the design of tactile interfaces.”

      Reviewer 3 (Public review):

      Strengths

      The paper describes a new perspective on friction perception, with the hypothesis that humans are sensitive to the instabilities of the surface rather than the coefficient of friction. The paper is very well written and with a comprehensive literature survey.

      One of the central tools used by the author to characterize the frictional behavior is the frictional instabilities maps. With these maps, it becomes clear that two different surfaces can have both similar and different behavior depending on the normal force and the speed of exploration. It puts forward that friction is a complicated phenomenon, especially for soft materials.

      The psychophysics study is centered around an odd-one-out protocol, which has the advantage of avoiding any external reference to what would mean friction or texture for example. The comparisons are made only based on the texture being similar or not.

      The results show a significant relationship between the distance between frictional maps and the success rate in discriminating two kinds of surface.

      We thank Reviewer 3 for their notes and interesting discussion points on our manuscript. Below, we address the reviewer’s feedback and comments on related works.

      Weaknesses:

      Comment 1

      The main weakness of the paper comes from the fact that the frictional maps and the extensive psychophysics study are not made at the same time, nor with the same finger. The frictional maps are produced with an artificial finger made out of PDMS which is a poor substitute for the complex tribological properties of skin.

      A similar comment was made by Reviewers 1 and 2. We agree in part and have made changes throughout that our study is correlative, but presents an important step forward to these biomechanical measurements and corresponding decision making models.

      We are not claiming that our PDMS fingers are superior to real fingers, but rather, we cannot establish standards in the field by using real human fingers that vary between subjects and researchers. We believe the mock finger we designed is a reasonable mimic of the human finger by matching surface energy, heterogeneous mechanical structure, and the ability to test multiple physiologically relevant pressures and sliding velocities.

      We achieve a heterogeneous mechanical structure with the 3 primary components of stiffness of a human finger. The effective modulus of ~100 kPa, from soft tissue,[9,10] is obtained with a 30:1 ratio of PDMS to crosslinker. The PDMS also surrounds a rigid, acrylic bone comparable to the distal phalanx, which provides an additional layer of higher modulus.[8] Additionally, the 8-hour UV-Ozone treatment decreases the viscoelastic tack of the pristine PDMS by glassifying, or further crosslinking the surface of the finger,[12] therefore imparting greater stiffness at the surface similar to the contributions of the stratum corneum, along with a similar surface energy.[13] The finger is used at least a day after UV-Ozone treatment is completed in order for the surface to return to moderate hydrophilicity, similar to the outermost layer of human skin.[17] We also discuss the shape of the contact formed. To ensure that there is minimal contribution from the slanted position of the finger, an initial contact area of 1×1 cm is established before sliding and recording friction measurements. As the PDMS finger is a soft object, the portion in contact with a surface flattens and the contact area remains largely unchanged during sliding. We recognize that it is difficult to completely control the pressure distribution due to the planar interface, but this variation is also expected when humans freely explore a surface. Finally, we consider flat vs. fingerprinted fingers. Our previous work on the role of fingerprints on friction experienced by a PDMS mock finger showed enhanced signals with the incorporation of ridges on the finger and used a rate-andstate model of a heterogenous, elastic body to find corresponding trends.[11] The key conclusion was that a flat finger still preserved key dynamic features, and the presence of stronger or more vibrations could result in more similar forces for different surfaces depending on the sliding conditions. We note that we have subsequently used the controlled mechanical data collected with this flat mock finger in correlations with human psychophysics in previous work, where findings from our mechanical experiments were predictive of human performance.[4–7] Ultimately, we see from our prior work and here that, despite the drawbacks of our mock finger, it outperforms other standard characterization technique in providing information about the mesoscale that correlates to tactile perception. We have added these details to the manuscript.

      We also note that an intermediate option, replicating real fingers, even in a mold, may also inadvertently limit trends from characterization to a specific finger. One of the main – and severe – limitations of using a human finger is that all fingers are different, meaning any study focusing on a particular user may not apply to others or be recreated easily by other researchers. We cannot set a standard for replication around a real human finger as that participant may no longer be available, or willing to travel the world as a “standard”. Furthermore, the method in which a single person changes their pressures and velocities as they touch a surface is highly variable. We also note that in the Summary Response, we noted that a study by Colgate et al. (IEEE ToH 2024) demonstrated that efference copies may be important, and thus constraining a human finger and replaying the forces recorded during free exploration will not lead to the participant identifying a surface with any consistency. Thus, it is important to allow humans to freely explore surfaces, but creates nearly limitless variability in friction forces.

      This is also against the backdrop that we are seeking to provide a method to characterize surfaces. Indeed, the more features we replicate in the mock finger to a human finger, the more likely it is that the mechanical data will correlate to human performance. However, we have used this technique several times to achieve stronger correlations to human data than other available techniques. We believe the metric of success should be in comparison to the available characterization technique, rather than a 1:1 reconstruction of forces of an arbitrary human finger. Indeed, a 1:1 reconstruction of forces of an arbitrary human finger would be limited to the finger of a single individual, perhaps even to that individual on a given day.

      See Reviewer1 weaknesses, comment 2 part 2 for changes to the manuscript

      Comment 2

      The evidence would have been much stronger if the measurement of the interaction was done during the psychophysical experiment. In addition, because of the protocol, the correlation is based on aggregates rather than on individual interactions.

      We agree that this would have helped further establish our argument, but in the overall statement and in other reviewer responses, we describe the significant challenges to establishing this.

      To fully implement this, a decision-making model is necessary because, as a counter example, a participant could have generated 10 swipes of SFW and 1 swipe of a Sp, but the Sp may have been the most important event for making a tactile decision. We also clarify that our goals are to provide a method to characterize samples to better design tactile interfaces in haptics or in psychophysical experiments.

      As discussed in the summary, and expanded on here, in our view, to develop a decision-making model, the challenges are as follows:

      (1) Which one, or combination of, of the multiple swipes that people make responsible for a tactile decision?

      (2) Establish what is, or may be, tactile evidence.

      (3) Establish tactile decision-making models are similar or different than existing decision-making models.

      (4) Test the hypothesis, in these models, that friction instabilities are evidence, and not some other unknown metric.

      (5) Design a task that does not require the use of subjective tactile descriptors, like “which one feels rougher”, which we see cause confusion in participants, which will likely require accounting for memory effects.

      (6) Design samples that vary in the amount of evidence generated, but this evidence cannot be controlled directly. Rather, the samples indirectly vary evidence by how likely it is for a human to generate different types of friction instabilities during standard exploration.

      We elaborate these points below:

      To successfully perform this experiment, we note that freely exploring humans make multiple strokes on a surface. Therefore, we would need to construct a decision-making model. It has not yet been demonstrated whether tactile decision making follows visual decision making, but perhaps to start, we can assume it does. Then, in the design of our decision-making paradigm, we immediately run into the problem: What is tactile evidence?

      From Fig. 3C, we already can see that identifying evidence is challenging. Prior to this manuscript, people may have chosen the average force, or the highest force. Or we may choose the average friction force. Then, after deciding on the evidence, we need to find a method to manipulate the evidence, i.e., create samples or a machine that causes high friction, etc. We show that during the course of human touch, due to the dynamic nature of friction, the average can change a large amount and sample design becomes a central barrier to experiments. Others may suggest to immobilize the finger and applying a known force, but given how much friction changes with human exploration, there is no known method to make a machine recreate temporally and spatially varying friction forces during sliding onto a stationary finger. Finally, perhaps most importantly, in addition to mechanical challenges, a study by Liu, Colgate et al. showed that even if they recorded the friction (2D) of a finger exploring a surface and then replicated the same friction forces onto a finger, the participant could not determine which surface the replayed friction force was supposed to represent.[1] This supports that the efference copy is important, that the forces in response to expected motion are important to determine friction. Finally, there is no known method to design instabilities a priori. They must be found through experiments, especially since if we were to introduce, say a bump or a trough, then we bring in confounding variables to how participants tell surfaces apart.

      Furthermore, even if we had some consistent method to create tactile “evidence”, the paradigm also deserves some consideration. In our experience, the 3-AFC task we perform is important because the vocabulary for touch has not been established. That is, in 3-AFC, by asking to determine which one sample is unlike the others, we do not have to ask the participant questions like “which one is rougher” or “which one has less friction”. In contrast, 2-AFC, which is better for decision-making models because it does not include memory, requires the asking of a perceptual question like: “which one is rougher?”. In our ongoing work, taking two silane coatings, we found that participants could easily identify which surface is unlike the others above chance in a 3-AFC, but participants, even within their own trials, could not consistently identify one silane as perceptually “rougher” by 2-AFC. To us, this calls into question the validity of tactile descriptors, but is beyond the scope of the current manuscript.

      This is not our only goal, but in the context of human exploration, in this manuscript here, we believed it was important to identify a mechanical parameter that was consistent with how humans explore surfaces, but was also a parameter that could characterize to some consistent property of a surface – irrespective of whether a human was touching it. We thought that designing human decision-making models and paradigms around the friction coefficient would not be successful.

      Given the scope of these challenges, we do not think it would be possible to establish this conceptual sequence in a single manuscript.

      Comment 3

      The authors compensate with a third experiment where they used a 2AFC protocol and an online force measurement. But the results of this third study, fail to convince the relation.

      With this experiment, our central goal was to demonstrate that the instabilities we have identified with the PDMS finger also occur with a human finger. Several instances of SS, Sp, and SFW were recorded with this setup as a participant touched surfaces in real time.

      Comment 4

      No map of the real finger interaction is shown, bringing doubt to the validity of the frictional map for something as variable as human fingers.

      Real fingers change constantly during exploration, and friction is state-dependent, meaning that the friction will depend on how the person was moving the moment prior. Therefore, a map is only valid for a single human movement – even if participants all were instructed to take a single swipe and start from zero motion, humans are unable to maintain constant velocities and pressures. Clearly, this is not sustainable for any analysis, and these drawbacks apply to any measured parameter, whether instabilities suggested here, or friction coefficients used throughout. We believe the difficulty of this approach emphasizes why a standard map of characterization of a surface by a mock finger, even with its drawbacks, is a viable path forward.

      Reviewer 3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Comment 1

      It would be interesting to comment on a potential connection between the frictional instability maps and Schalamack waves.

      Schallamach waves are a subset of slow frictional waves (SFW). Schallamach waves are very specifically defined in the field. They occur when pockets of air that form between a soft sliding object and rigid surface which then propagate rear-to-front (retrograde waves) relative to motion of the sliding motion and form buckles due to adhesive pinning. Wrinkles then form at the detached portion of the soft material, until the interface reattaches and the process repeats.[24] There is typically a high burden of proof to establish a Schallamach wave over a more general slow frictional wave. We note that it would be exceedingly difficult to design samples that can reliably create subsets of SFW, but we are aware that this may be an interesting question at a future point in our work.

      Comment 2

      The force sensors look very compliant, and given the dynamic nature of the signal, it is important to characterize the frequency response of the system to make sure that the fluctuations are not amplified.

      Thank you for noticing. We mistyped the sensor spring constant as 13.9 N m<sup>-1</sup> instead of kN m<sup>-1</sup>. However, below we show how the instabilities are derived from the mechanics at the interface due to the compliance of the finger. The “springs” of the force sensor and PDMS finger are connected in parallel. Since k<sub>sensor</sub> = 13.9 kN m<sup>-1</sup>, the spring constant of the system overall reflects the compliance of the finger, and highlights the oscillations arising solely from stick-slip. A sample calculation is shown below.

      Author response image 1.

      Fitting a line to the initial slope of the force trace for C6 gives the equation y = 25.679x – 0.2149. The slope here represents force data over time data, and is divided by the velocity (25 mm/s) to determine the spring constant of the system k<sub>total</sub> == 1027.16 N/m. This value is lower than k<sub>sensor</sub> = 13.9 kN/m, indicating that the “springs” representing the force sensor and PDMS finger are connected in parallel:

      . The finger is the compliant component of the system, with k<sub>finger</sub> = 1.11 kN/m, and of course, real human fingers are also compliant so this matches our goals with the design of the mock finger.

      Our changes to the manuscript

      (Page 4) (k = 13.9 kN m<sup>1</sup>)

      Comment 3

      The authors should discuss about the stochastic nature of friction: - Wiertlewski, Hudin, Hayward, IEEE WHC 2011 Greenspon, McLellan, Lieber, Bensmaia, JRSI 2020.

      We believe that, given the references, this comment on “stochastic” refers to the macroscopically-observable fluctuations (i.e., the mechanical “noise” which is not due to instrument noise) in friction arising from the discordant network of stick-slip phenomena occurring throughout the contact zone, and not the stochastic nature of nanoscale friction that occurs thermal fluctuations nor due to statistical distributions in bond breaking associated with soft contact.

      We first note that our small-scale fluctuations do not arise from a periodic surface texture that dominates in the frequency regime. However, even on our comparatively smooth surfaces, we do expect fluctuations due to nanoscale variation in contact, generation of stick-slip across at microscale length scales that occur either concurrently or discordantly across the contact zone, and the nonlinear dependence of friction to nearly any variation in state and composition.[11]

      Perhaps the most relevant to the manuscript is that a major advantage of analysis by friction is that it sidesteps these ever-present microscale fluctuations, leading to more clearly defined classifiers or categories during analysis. Wiertlewski et. al. showed repeated measurements in their systems ultimately gave rise to consistent frequencies[25] (we think their system was in a steady sliding regime and the patterning gave rise to underlying macroscopic waves). These consistent frequencies, at least in soft systems and absent obvious macroscopic patterned features, would be expected to arise from the instability categories and we see them throughout.

      Comment 4

      It is stated that "we observed a spurious, negative correlation between friction coefficient and accuracy".

      What makes you qualify that correlation as spurious?

      We mean this as in the statistical definition of “spurious”.

      This correlation would indicate that by the metric of friction coefficient, more different surfaces are perceived more similarly. Thus, two very different surfaces, like Teflon and sandpaper, by friction coefficient would be expected to feel very similar. Two nearly identical surfaces would be expected to feel very different – but of course, humans cannot consistently distinguish two identical surfaces. This finding is counterintuitive and refutes that friction coefficient is a reliable classifier of surfaces by touch. We do not think it is productive to determine a mechanism for a spurious correlation, but perhaps one reason we were able to observe this is because our study, to the best of our knowledge, is unique for having samples that are controlled in their physical differences in roughness and surface features.

      See response to Reviewer 1 weaknesses, comment 1 for changes to the manuscript

      Comment 5

      The authors should comment on the influence of friction on perceptual invariance. Despite inducing radially different frictional behavior for various conditions, these surfaces are stably perceived. Maybe this is a sign that humans extract a different metric?

      We agree – we are excited that frictional instabilities may offer a more stable perceptual cue because they are not prone to fluctuations (as discussed in Comment 3) and instability formation, in many conditions, is invariant to applied pressures and velocities – thus forming large zones where a human may reasonable encounter a given instability.

      Raw friction is highly prone to variation during human exploration (in alignment with Recommendations for the authors, Comment 3), but ongoing work seeks to explain tactile constancy, or the ability to identify objects despite these large changes in force. Very recently published work by Fehlberg et. al. identified the role of modulating finger speed and normal force in amplifying the differences in friction coefficient between materials in order to identify them,[26] and we postulate that their work may be streamlined and consistent with the idea of friction instabilities, though we have not had a chance to discuss this in-depth with the authors yet.

      We think that the instability maps show a viable path forward to how surfaces are stably perceived, and instabilities themselves show a potential mechanism: mathematically, instabilities for given conditions can be invariant to velocity or mass, creating zones where a certain instability is encountered. This reduces the immense variability of friction to a smaller, more stable classification of surfaces (e.g., a 30% SS surface or a 60% SS surface). A given surface will typically produce the same instability at a specific condition (we found some boundaries of experimental parameters are very condition sensitive, but many conditions are not), whereas a single friction trace which is highly prone to variation is not a stable metric.

      Added Reference

      (53) M. Fehlberg, E. Monfort, S. Saikumar, K. Drewing and R. Bennewitz, IEEE Trans. Haptics, 2024, 17, 957–963.

      References

      (1) Liu, Z., Kim, J.-T., Rogers, J. A., Klatzky, R. L. & Colgate, J. E. Realism of Tactile Texture Playback: A Combination of Stretch and Vibration. IEEE Trans. Haptics 17, 441–450 (2024).

      (2) Waters, I., Alazmani, A. & Culmer, P. Engineering Incipient Slip Into Surgical Graspers to Enhance Grasp Performance. IEEE Transactions on Medical Robotics and Bionics 2, 541–544 (2020).

      (3) Gueorguiev, D., Bochereau, S., Mouraux, A., Hayward, V. & Thonnard, J.-L. Touch uses frictional cues to discriminate flat materials. Sci Rep 6, 25553 (2016).

      (4) Carpenter, C. W. et al. Human ability to discriminate surface chemistry by touch. Mater. Horiz. 5, 70– 77 (2018).

      (5) Nolin, A. et al. Predicting human touch sensitivity to single atom substitutions in surface monolayers for molecular control in tactile interfaces. Soft Matter 17, 5050–5060 (2021).

      (6) Nolin, A. et al. Controlling fine touch sensations with polymer tacticity and crystallinity. Soft Matter 18, 3928–3940 (2022).

      (7) Swain, Z. et al. Self-Assembled Thin Films as Alternative Surface Textures in Assistive Aids with Users Who are Blind. J. Mater. Chem. B (2024) doi:10.1039/D4TB01646G.

      (8) Qian, K. et al. Mechanical properties vary for different regions of the finger extensor apparatus. J Biomech 47, 3094–3099 (2014).

      (9) Abdouni, A. et al. Biophysical properties of the human finger for touch comprehension: influences of ageing and gender. Royal Society Open Science (2017) doi:10.1098/rsos.170321.

      (10) Cornuault, P.-H., Carpentier, L., Bueno, M.-A., Cote, J.-M. & Monteil, G. Influence of physicochemical, mechanical and morphological fingerpad properties on the frictional distinction of sticky/slippery surfaces. Journal of The Royal Society Interface (2015) doi:10.1098/rsif.2015.0495.

      (11) Dhong, C. et al. Role of fingerprint-inspired relief structures in elastomeric slabs for detecting frictional differences arising from surface monolayers. Soft Matter 14, 7483–7491 (2018).

      (12) Fu, Y.-J. et al. Effect of UV-Ozone Treatment on Poly(dimethylsiloxane) Membranes: Surface Characterization and Gas Separation Performance. Langmuir 26, 4392–4399 (2010).

      (13) Yuan, Y. & Verma, R. Measuring microelastic properties of stratum corneum. Colloids Surf B Biointerfaces 48, 6–12 (2006).

      (14) Yu, G. et al. A wearable pressure sensor based on ultra-violet/ozone microstructured carbon nanotube/polydimethylsiloxane arrays for electronic skins. Nanotechnology 29, 115502 (2018).

      (15) Zheng, L. et al. Dual-Stimulus Smart Actuator and Robot Hand Based on a Vapor-Responsive PDMS Film and Triboelectric Nanogenerator. ACS Appl. Mater. Interfaces 11, 42504–42511 (2019).

      (16) Ma, K., Rivera, J., Hirasaki, G. J. & Biswal, S. L. Wettability control and patterning of PDMS using UV–ozone and water immersion. Journal of Colloid and Interface Science 363, 371–378 (2011).

      (17) Mavon, A. et al. Sebum and stratum corneum lipids increase human skin surface free energy as determined from contact angle measurements: A study on two anatomical sites. Colloids and Surfaces B: Biointerfaces 8, 147–155 (1997).

      (18) AliAbbasi, E. et al. Effect of Finger Moisture on Tactile Perception of Electroadhesion. IEEE Trans. Haptics 17, 841–849 (2024).

      (19) Corniani, G. et al. Sub-surface deformation of individual fingerprint ridges during tactile interactions.

      eLife 13, (2024).

      (20) Israelachvili, J. N. Intermolecular and Surface Forces. (Academic Press, 2011).

      (21) Das, S. et al. Stick–slip friction of gecko-mimetic flaps on smooth and rough surfaces. J R Soc Interface 12, 20141346 (2015).

      (22) Persson, B. N. J., Albohr, O., Creton, C. & Peveri, V. Contact area between a viscoelastic solid and a hard, randomly rough, substrate. The Journal of Chemical Physics 120, 8779–8793 (2004).

      (23) Skedung, L. et al. Feeling Small: Exploring the Tactile Perception Limits. Sci Rep 3, 2617 (2013).

      (24) Viswanathan, K., Sundaram, N. K. & Chandrasekar, S. Stick-slip at soft adhesive interfaces mediated by slow frictional waves. Soft Matter 12, 5265–5275 (2016).

      (25) Wiertlewski, M., Hudin, C. & Hayward, V. On the 1/f noise and non-integer harmonic decay of the interaction of a finger sliding on flat and sinusoidal surfaces. in 2011 IEEE World Haptics Conference 25–30 (2011). doi:10.1109/WHC.2011.5945456.

      (26) Fehlberg, M., Monfort, E., Saikumar, S., Drewing, K. & Bennewitz, R. Perceptual Constancy in the Speed Dependence of Friction During Active Tactile Exploration. IEEE Transactions on Haptics 17, 957–963 (2024).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors used a coarse-grained DNA model (cgNA+) to explore how DNA sequences and CpG methylation/hydroxymethylation influence nucleosome wrapping energy and the probability density of optimal nucleosomal configuration. Their findings indicate that both methylated and hydroxymethylated cytosines lead to increased nucleosome wrapping energy. Additionally, the study demonstrates that methylation of CpG islands increases the probability of nucleosome formation.

      Strengths:

      The major strength of this method is that the model explicitly includes elastic constraints on the positions of phosphate groups facing a histone octamer, as DNA-histone binding site constraints. The authors claim that their model enhances the accuracy and computational efficiency and allows comprehensive calculations of DNA mechanical properties and deformation energies.

      Weaknesses:

      A significant limitation of this study is that the parameter sets for the methylated and hydroxymethylated CpG steps in the cgNA+ model are derived from all-atom molecular dynamics (MD) simulations that suggest that both methylated and hydroxymethylated cytosines increase DNA stiffness and nucleosome wrapping energy (P´erez A, et al. Biophys J. 2012; Battistini, et al. PLOS Comput Biol. 2021). It could predispose the coarse-grained model to replicate these findings. Notably, conflicting results from other all-atom MD simulations, such as those by Ngo T in Nat. Commun. 2016, shows that hydroxymethylated cytosines increase DNA flexibility, contrary to methylated cytosines. If the cgNA+ model was trained on these later parameters or other all-atom force fields, different conclusions might be obtained regarding the effects of methylated and hydroxymethylation on nucleosome formation.

      Despite the training parameters of the cgNA+ model, the results presented in the manuscript indicate that methylated cytosines increase both DNA stiffness and nucleosome wrapping energy. However, when comparing nucleosome occupancy scores with predicted nucleosome wrapping energies and optimal configurations, the authors find that methylated CGIs exhibit higher nucleosome occupancies than unmethylated ones, which seems to contradict their findings from the same paper which showed that increased stiffness should reduce nucleosome formation affinity. In the manuscript, the authors also admit that these conclusions “apparently runs counter to the (perhaps naive) intuition that high nucleosome forming affinity should arise for fragments with low wrapping energy”. Previous all-atom MD simulations (P´erez A, et al. Biophys J. 2012; Battistini, et al. PLOS Comput Biol. 202; Ngo T, et al. Nat. Commun. 20161) show that the stiffer DNA upon CpG methylation reduces the affinity of DNA to assemble into nucleosomes or destabilizes nucleosomes. Given these findings, the authors need to address and reconcile these seemingly contradictory results, as the influence of epigenetic modifications on DNA mechanical properties and nucleosome formation are critical aspects of their study. Understanding the influence of sequence-dependent and epigenetic modifications of DNA on mechanical properties and nucleosome formation is crucial for comprehending various cellular processes. The authors’ study, focusing on these aspects, will definitely garner interest from the DNA methylation research community.

      Training the cgNA+ model on alternative MD simulation datasets is certainly of interest to us. However, due to the significant computational cost, this remains a goal for future work. The relationship between nucleosome occupancy scores and nucleosome wrapping energy is still debated, with conflicting findings reported in the literature, as noted in our Discussion section. Interestingly, we find that our predicted log probability density of DNA spontaneously acquiring a nucleosomal configuration is a better indicator of nucleosome occupancy than our predicted DNA nucleosome wrapping energy.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This study uses a coarse-grained model for double-stranded DNA, cgNA+, to assess nucleosome sequence affinity. cgNA+ coarse-grains DNA on the level of bases and accounts also explicitly for the positions of the backbone phosphates. It has been proven to reproduce all-atom MD data very accurately. It is also ideally suited to be incorporated into a nucleosome model because it is known that DNA is bound to the protein core of the nucleosome via the phosphates.

      It is still unclear whether this harmonic model parametrized for unbound DNA is accurate in describing DNA inside the nucleosome. Previous models by other authors, using more coarse-grained models of DNA, have been rather successful in predicting base pair sequence-dependent nucleosome behavior. This is at least the case as far as DNA shape is concerned whereas assessing the role of DNA bendability (something this paper focuses on) has been consistently challenging in all nucleosome models, to my knowledge.

      It is thus of major interest whether this more sophisticated model is also more successful in handling this issue. As far as I can tell the work is technically sound and properly accounts for not only the energy required in wrapping DNA but also entropic effects, namely the change in entropy that DNA experiences when going from the free state to the bound state. The authors make an approximation here which seems to me to be a reasonable first step.

      Of interest is also that the authors have the parameters at hand to study the effect of methylation of CpG-steps. This is especially interesting as it allows us to study a scenario where changes in the physical properties of base pair steps via methylation might influence nucleosome positioning and stability in a cell-type-specific way.

      Overall, this is an important contribution to the question of how the sequence affects nucleosome positioning and affinity. The findings suggest that cgNA+ has something new to offer. But the problem is complex, also on the experimental side, so many questions remain open.

      Strengths:

      The authors use their state-of-the-art coarse-grained DNA model which seems ideally suited to be applied to nucleosomes as it accounts explicitly for the backbone phosphates.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) According to the abstract the authors consider two “scalar measures of the sequence-dependent propensity of DNA to wrap into nucleosomes”. One is the bending energy and the other, is the free energy. Specifically in the latter, the authors take the difference between the free energies of the wrapped and the free DNA. Whereas the entropy of the latter can be calculated exactly, they assume that the bound DNA always has the same entropy (independent of sequence) in its more confined state. The problem is the way in which this is written (e.g. below Eq. 6) which is hard to understand. The authors should mention that the negative of Eq. 6 is what physicists call free energy, namely especially the free energy difference between bound and free DNA.

      We have included the necessary clarifications in the revised manuscript, below Eq. 6.

      (2) In Eq. 5 the authors introduce penalty coefficients c<sub>i</sub>. They write that values are “set by numerical experiment to keep distances ... within the ranges observed in the PDB structure, while avoiding sterical clashes in DNA.” This is rather vague, especially since it is unclear to me what type of sterical clashes might occur. Figure 1 shows then a comparison between crystal structures and simulated structures. They are reasonably similar but standard deviations in the fluctuations of the simulation are smaller than in the experiments. Why did the authors not choose smaller c<sub>i</sub>-values to have a better fit? Do smaller values lead to unwanted large fluctuations that would lead to steric clashes between the two DNA turns? I also wonder what side views of the nucleosomes look like (experiments and simulations) and whether in this side view larger fluctuations of the phosphates can be observed in the simulation that would eventually lead to turn-turn clashes for smaller c<sub>i</sub>-values.

      The side view plots of the experimental and predicted nucleosome structures are now added to Supplementary material (Figure S8). Indeed, smaller c<sub>i</sub> values lead to steric clashes between the two turns of DNA – this is now specified in the Methods section. A possible improvement of our optimisation method and a direction of future work would be adding a penalty which prevents steric clashes to the objective function. Then the c<sub>i</sub> values could be reduced to have bigger fluctuations that are even closer to the experimental structures. We added this explanation to the Results section.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors utilize biophysical modeling to investigate differences in free energies and nucleosomal configuration probability density of CpG islands and nonmethylated regions in the genome. Toward this goal, they develop and apply the cgNA+ coarse-grained model, an extension of their prior molecular modeling framework.

      Strengths:

      The study utilizes biophysical modeling to gain mechanistic insight into nucleosomal occupancy differences in CpG and nonmethylated regions in the genome.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the overall study is interesting, the manuscripts need more clarity in places. Moreover, the rationale and conclusion for some of the analyses are not well described.

      We edited the manuscript according to the reviewer’s suggestions and hopefully improved its readability.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The cgNA+ model parameters are derived from all-atom molecular dynamics (MD) simulations, yet there is no consensus within all-atom MD simulations regarding the impact of CpG methylation on DNA mechanical properties. The authors could consider fitting the coarsegrained model with a different all-atom force field to verify whether the conclusions regarding the effects of methylation and hydroxymethylation on DNA nucleosome wrapping energies still hold. For further details on MD simulations related to CpG methylation effects, the authors are advised to consult the review paper by Li et al. (2022) titled “DNA methylation: Precise modulation of chromatin structure and dynamics” published in Current Opinion in Structural Biology.

      Parametrizing the cgNA+ model using MD simulations with various force fields is certainly of interest to us. However, due to the computational cost involved, it remains a goal for future work.

      (2) Beyond DNA mechanical properties, which are directly linked to nucleosome wrapping energies in this study, the authors might also consider other factors such as geometric properties that could influence nucleosome formation. This approach might help the authors to reconcile the observed higher nucleosome occupancy scores for methylated CpGs. The authors are encouraged to review the aforementioned paper for additional experimental and MD simulation studies that could support this perspective.

      Geometric properties of DNA are directly incorporated into our method through the cgNA+ model equilibrium shape prediction µ. We compute the mechanical energy needed deform µ to a nucleosomal configuration. Notably, the equilibrium shape µ is sensitive to methylation, as demonstrated in Figure 3.

      (3) There are some issues with citation accuracy in the manuscript. For instance, in the Discussion section, the authors attribute a statement to Collings et al. and Anderson (2017), claiming that “methylated regions, known to have high wrapping energy, are among the highest nucleosome occupied elements in the genome.” However, upon reviewing this paper, it appears that it does not make any claims about the high wrapping energy of methylated regions.

      The paragraph is now edited and a separate citation, P´erez et al. (2012), is given for the statement that methylation regions have high wrapping energy.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Please improve the readability by:

      (1) making clear that -ln ρ in Eq. 6 on page 4 is actually the free energy. Also, the word entropy comes too late (on page 7) where the best explanation of Eq. 6 is presented.

      We added a comment about -ln ρ being the free energy after Eq. 6 and also included an equation, relating ln ρ and entropy.

      (2) page 12 and 13 show two sets of experimental data. They are quite different from each other. When reading this, I wondered why there is this difference. But only on page 16, you explain that these are different cell types. The difference should be explained already when the papers are introduced on page 12.

      A corresponding sentence already appeared in page 12: “The observations about nucleosome occupancy should be regarded as preliminary, and be treated with caution, as they are based on experimental data obtained for the cancerous HeLa cells Schwartz et al. (2019) and human genome embryonic stem cells Yazdi et al. (2015)”. Now we also added this information to the first paragraph of the subsection for clarity.

      Finally, I add here some general thoughts that came up when reading the paper, comparing your findings with earlier findings in the field. This is not a strict one-to-one comparison and thus does not have to find its way into this manuscript but might give ideas for future studies. Experiments suggest that nucleosomes prefer DNA with a high content of C’s and G’s. Figure 2 does not look at the GC content but at the number of CpG’s. But in any case, let’s use this as a proxy for GC content. Figure 2a suggests that there is not a strong dependence of the bending energy on the number CpG steps. This is consistent with earlier work with the rigid basepair model which shows the same behavior for GC content (for both MD and crystal parametrizations). Figure 2c (related to the negative free energy) shows that with an increasing number of CpG steps the propensity to bind goes down. This suggests that the entropic cost to confine CpG-rich DNA increases, which in turn reflects that these DNA stretches are softer. This is rather interesting since in the case of the rigid basepair model this effect is observed only when stiffnesses are extracted from crystal data not MD data (however, this refers again to CG content). This might indicate a difference between the rigid bp model and cgNA+ which will be interesting to study in the future. Interesting is also the effect of CpG methylation. The stiffer methylated steps lead to an increase in the energy with the number of such steps (Figure 2a). The entropic cost for binding is thus expected to be smaller and this is indeed observed in Figure 2c when compared to the non-methylated steps.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. As for the GC content, the energy and lnp plots are indeed very similar to those in Figure 2.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1) The formulation of the cgNA+ model in the method section was not easy to follow and can be described better to improve clarity.

      We have revised the model description and hope that its clarity has been improved

      (2) The authors mention utilizing 100 human genome sequences with 100 configurations from DB. It would be helpful to clarify the source of these 100 human genome sequences. Are these 100 distinct regions on the human reference genome, or are they from a specific dataset or database?

      We now include an explanation about the origin of sequences: “The human genome sequences are a random subset of our sequence sample for the CGI and NMI intersection in the Chromosome 1, but the following observations remain unchanged for sequence samples from different genomic regions.”

      (3) The authors mention the lack of tail unwrapping in their model. It would be beneficial to understand the magnitude of this issue and its potential impact on the overall results. How significant is the lack of unwrapping events in their current model?

      We observed the unwrapping of approximately five base-pairs at each end of our predicted nucleosome configurations, in comparison to the experimental configurations (Figure 1). This issue could be solved by adding additional constraints at the ends of the 147 bp sequence. The wrapping energy would increase marginally, as only about 10 of 147 bp would be affected. We added this remark to the main text.

      (4) Observations from Figure 3 are not described properly. Are these differences statistically significant? Why is twist higher for CpG sites but lower for a roll?

      We added an explanation of how the statistics was computed into the caption of Figure 3. In fact, we didn’t use statistical estimates here, but generated all the possible cases and computed the exact statistics (for the given set of our model parameters). Regarding the changes in twist and roll, we have added the following comment on page 7: “The ground state changes resulting from cytosine modifications – primarily characterized by an average increase in roll and a decrease in twist – may be linked to steric hindrance caused by the cytosine 5-substituent (Battistini et al. (2021)). Notably, the negative coupling between twist and roll has already been observed in X-ray crystallography data (Olson et al. (1998)).”

      (5) Figure 4 does not clarify the authors’ conclusion of higher stiffness for ApT and TpA dinucleotides. The authors should provide further explanation for this observation.

      We revised the text to clarify that the statement regarding ApT and TpA being the most stiff and the most flexible dinucleotides is not a conclusion derived from Figure 4, but rather from earlier work that we cite.

      (6) In Figure 7, the authors note that methylated CGIs have higher nucleosome occupancy on average than unmethylated sequences. Is this observation statistically significant?

      We observe that methylated sequences have a higher average occupancy than unmethylated sequences in Yazdi et al. data, when the CpG count falls into the intervals from 5 to 14 and from 15 to 24. For each of the two intervals this difference is statistically significant: the permutation test, used due to the lack of normality, yields a p-value of 0.0001 for both cases. The differences in mean scores shown in Figure 8 are also statistically significant. Such test results are expected, given the large sample sizes and the observed differences in means, therefore we prefer not to include this discussion in main text.

      (7) The authors note that their analyses to correlate nucleosome occupancy profile with the methylation state of underlying sequences are preliminary, as different cell lines were used to perform these analyses. Given this inconsistency, it needs to be clarified why this analysis was performed and what the takeaway is.

      We added the following comment at the end of the Results section: “Although comparing data from different cell lines is not optimal, to the best of our knowledge, no publicly available methylation and nucleosome occupancy data exist for the entire human genome within the same cell type. Nevertheless, since the lowest log probability densities in the human genome are predicted for CpG-rich sequences regardless of their methylation state (Figure 2d), and the same holds for both sets of the nucleosome occupancy scores (Figure 7), we conclude that the lowest occupancies occur for sequences with the lowest log probability densities.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      The study by Klug et al. investigated the pathway specificity of corticostriatal projections, focusing on two cortical regions. Using a G-deleted rabies system in D1-Cre and A2a-Cre mice to retrogradely deliver channelrhodopsin to cortical inputs, the authors found that M1 and MCC inputs to direct and indirect pathway spiny projection neurons (SPNs) are both partially segregated and asymmetrically overlapping. In general, corticostriatal inputs that target indirect pathway SPNs are likely to also target direct pathway SPNs, while inputs targeting direct pathway SPNs are less likely to also target indirect pathway SPNs. Such asymmetric overlap of corticostriatal inputs has important implications for how the cortex itself may determine striatal output. Indeed, the authors provide behavioral evidence that optogenetic activation of M1 or MCC cortical neurons that send axons to either direct or indirect pathway SPNs can have opposite effects on locomotion and different effects on action sequence execution. The conclusions of this study add to our understanding of how cortical activity may influence striatal output and offer important new clues about basal ganglia function. 

      The conceptual conclusions of the manuscript are supported by the data, but the details of the magnitude of afferent overlap and causal role of asymmetric corticostriatal inputs on behavioral outcomes were not yet fully resolved. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful understanding and acknowledgment that the conceptual conclusion of asymmetric projections from the cortex to the striatum is well supported by our data. We also recognize the importance of further elucidating the extent of afferent overlap and the causal contributions of asymmetric corticostriatal inputs to behavioral outcomes. However, we respectfully note that current technical limitations pose significant challenges to addressing these questions with high precision.

      In response to the reviewer’s comments, we have now clarified the sample size, added proper analysis and elaborated on the experimental design to ensure that our conclusions are presented more transparently and are more accessible to the reader.

      After virally labeling either direct pathway (D1) or indirect pathway (D2) SPNs to optogenetically tag pathway-specific cortical inputs, the authors report that a much larger number of "non-starter" D2-SPNs from D2-SPN labeled mice responded to optogenetic stimulation in slices than "non-starter" D1 SPNs from D1-SPN labeled mice did. Without knowing the relative number of D1 or D2 SPN starters used to label cortical inputs, it is difficult to interpret the exact meaning of the lower number of responsive D2-SPNs in D1 labeled mice (where only ~63% of D1-SPNs themselves respond) compared to the relatively higher number of responsive D1-SPNs (and D2-SPNs) in D2 labeled mice. While relative differences in connectivity certainly suggest that some amount of asymmetric overlap of inputs exists, differences in infection efficiency and ensuing differences in detection sensitivity in slice experiments make determining the degree of asymmetry problematic. 

      Thank you for highlighting this point. As it lies at the core of our manuscript, we agree that it is essential to present it clearly and convincingly. As shown by the statistics (Fig. 2B-F), non-starter D1- and D2-SPNs appear to receive fewer projections from D1-projecting cortical neurons (Input D1-record D1, 0.63; Input D1-record D2, 0.40) compared to D2-projecting cortical neurons (Input D2 - record D1, 0.73; Input D2 -record D2, 0.79).

      While it is not technically feasible to quantify the number of infected cells in brain slices following electrophysiological recordings, we addressed this limitation by collecting data from multiple animals and restricting recordings to cells located within the injection sites. In Figure 2D, we used 7 mice in the D1-projecting to D1 EGFP(+) group, 8 mice in the D1-projecting to D2 EGFP(-) group, 10 mice in the D2-projecting to D2 EGFP(+) group, and 8 mice in the D2-projecting to D1 EGFP(-) group. In Figure 2G, the group sizes were as follows: 8 mice in the D1-projecting to D2 EGFP(+) group, 7 mice in the D1-projecting to D1 EGFP(-) group, 8 mice in the D2-projecting to D1 EGFP(+) group, and 10 mice in the D2-projecting to D2 EGFP(-) group. In both panels, connection ratios were compared using Fisher’s exact test. Comparisons were then made across experimental groups. Furthermore, as detailed in our Methods section (page 20, line 399-401), we assessed cortical expression levels prior to performing whole-cell recordings. Taken together, these precautions help ensure that the calculated connection ratios are unlikely to be confounded by differences in infection efficiency.

      It is also unclear if retrograde labeling of D1-SPN- vs D2-SPN- targeting afferents labels the same densities of cortical neurons. This gets to the point of specificity in the behavioral experiments. If the target-based labeling strategies used to introduce channelrhodopsin into specific SPN afferents label significantly different numbers of cortical neurons, might the difference in the relative numbers of optogenetically activated cortical neurons itself lead to behavioral differences? 

      Thank you for bringing this concern to our attention. While optogenetic manipulation has become a widely adopted tool in functional studies of neural circuits, it remains subject to several technical limitations due to the nature of its implementation. Factors such as opsin expression efficiency, optic fiber placement, light intensity, stimulation spread, and other variables can all influence the specificity and extent of neuronal activation or inhibition. As such, rigorous experimental controls are essential when interpreting the outcomes of optogenetic experiments.

      In our study, we verified both the expression of channelrhodopsin in D1- or D2-projecting cortical neurons and the placement of the optic fiber following the completion of behavioral testing. To account for variability, we compared the behavioral effects of optogenetic stimulation within the same animals, stimulated versus non-stimulated conditions, as shown in Figures 3 and 4. Moreover, Figure S3 includes important controls that rule out the possibility that the behavioral effects observed were due to direct activation of D1- or D2-SPNs in striatum or to light alone in the cortex.

      An additional point worth emphasizing is that the behavioral effects observed in the open field and ICSS tests cannot be attributed to differences in the number of neurons activated. Specifically, activation of D1-projecting cortical neurons promoted locomotion in the open field, whereas activation of D2-projecting cortical neurons did not. However, in the ICSS test, activation of both D1- and D2-projecting cortical neurons reinforced lever pressing. Given that only D1-SPN activation, but not D2-SPN activation, supports ICSS behavior, these effects are unlikely to result merely from differences in the number of neurons recruited.

      This rationale underlies our use of multiple behavioral paradigms to examine the functions of D1- and D2-projecting cortical neurons. By assessing behavior across distinct tasks, we aimed to approach the question from multiple angles and reduce the likelihood of spurious or confounding effects influencing our interpretation.

      In general, the manuscript would also benefit from more clarity about the statistical comparisons that were made and sample sizes used to reach their conclusions.

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable suggestion to improve the manuscript. In response, we have made the following changes and provided additional clarification:

      (1) In Figure 2D, we used 7 mice in the D1-projecting to D1 EGFP(+) group, 8 mice in the D1-projecting to D2 EGFP(-) group, 10 mice in the D2-projecting to D2 EGFP(+) group, and 8 mice in the D2-projecting to D1 EGFP(-) group. In Figure 2G, the group sizes were as follows: 8 mice in the D1-projecting to D2 EGFP(+) group, 7 mice in the D1-projecting to D1 EGFP(-) group, 8 mice in the D2-projecting to D1 EGFP(+) group, and 10 mice in the D2-projecting to D2 EGFP(-) group. In both panels, connection ratios were compared using Fisher’s exact test.

      (2) In Figure 3, we reanalyzed the data in panels O, P, R, and S using permutation tests to assess whether each individual group exhibited a significant ICSS learning effect. The figure legend has been revised accordingly as follows:

      (O-P) D1-SPN (red) but not D2-SPN stimulation (black) drives ICSS behavior in both the DMS (O: D1, n = 6, permutation test, slope = 1.5060, P = 0.0378; D2, n = 5, permutation test, slope = -0.2214, P = 0.1021; one-tailed Mann Whitney test, Day 7 D1 vs. D2, P = 0.0130) and the DLS (P: D1, n = 6, permutation test, slope = 28.1429, P = 0.0082; D2, n = 5, permutation test, slope = -0.3429, P = 0.0463; one-tailed Mann Whitney test, Day 7 D1 vs. D2, P = 0.0390). *, P < 0.05. (Q) Timeline of helper virus injections, rabies-ChR2 injections and optogenetic stimulation for ICSS behavior. (R-S) Optogenetic stimulation of the cortical neurons projecting to either D1- or D2-SPNs induces ICSS behavior in both the MCC (R: MCC-D1, n = 5, permutation test, Day1-Day7, slope = 2.5857, P = 0.0034; MCC-D2, n = 5, Day2-Day7, permutation test, slope = 1.4229, P = 0.0344; no significant effect on Day7, MCC-D1 vs. MCC-D2,  two-tailed Mann Whitney test, P = 0.9999) and the M1 (S: M1-D1, n = 5, permutation test, Day1-Day7, slope = 1.8214, P = 0.0259; M1-D2, n = 5, Day1-Day7, permutation test, slope = 1.8214, P = 0.0025; no significant effect on Day7, M1-D1 vs. M1-D2, two-tailed Mann Whitney test, P = 0.3810). n.s., not statistically significant.

      (3) In Figure 4, we have added a comparison against a theoretical percentage change of zero to better evaluate the net effect of each manipulation. The results showed that in Figure 4D, optogenetic stimulation of D1-projecting MCC neurons significantly increased the pressing rate, whereas stimulation of D2-projecting MCC neurons did not (MCC-D1: n = 8, one-sample two-tailed t-test, t = 2.814, P = 0.0131; MCC-D2: n = 7, t = 0.8481, P = 0.4117). In contrast, in Figure 4H, optogenetic stimulation of both D1- and D2-projecting M1 neurons significantly increased the sequence press rate (M1-D1: n = 6, one-sample two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, P = 0.0046; M1-D2: n = 7, P = 0.0479).

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary: 

      Klug et al. use monosynaptic rabies tracing of inputs to D1- vs D2-SPNs in the striatum to study how separate populations of cortical neurons project to D1- and D2-SPNs. They use rabies to express ChR2, then patch D1-or D2-SPNs to measure synaptic input. They report that cortical neurons labeled as D1-SPN-projecting preferentially project to D1-SPNs over D2-SPNs. In contrast, cortical neurons labeled as D2-SPN-projecting project equally to D1- and D2-SPNs. They go on to conduct pathway-specific behavioral stimulation experiments. They compare direct optogenetic stimulation of D1- or D2-SPNs to stimulation of MCC inputs to DMS and M1 inputs to DLS. In three different behavioral assays (open field, intra-cranial self-stimulation, and a fixed ratio 8 task), they show that stimulating MCC or M1 cortical inputs to D1-SPNs is similar to D1-SPN stimulation, but that stimulating MCC or M1 cortical inputs to D2-SPNs does not recapitulate the effects of D2-SPN stimulation (presumably because both D1- and D2-SPNs are being activated by these cortical inputs). 

      Strengths: 

      Showing these same effects in three distinct behaviors is strong. Overall, the functional verification of the consequences of the anatomy is very nice to see. It is a good choice to patch only from mCherry-negative non-starter cells in the striatum.

      Thank you for your profound understanding and appreciation of our manuscript’s design and the methodologies employed. In the realm of neuroscience, quantifying synaptic connections is a formidable challenge. While the roles of the direct and indirect pathways in motor control have long been explored, the mechanism by which upstream cortical inputs govern these pathways remains shrouded in mystery at the circuitry level.

      In the ‘Go/No-Go’ model, the direct and indirect pathways operate antagonistically; in contrast, the ‘Co-activation’ model suggests that they work cooperatively to orchestrate movement. These distinct theories raise a compelling question: Do these two pathways receive inputs from the same upstream cortical neurons, or are they modulated by distinct subpopulations? Answering this question could provide vital clues as to whether these pathways collaborate or operate independently.

      Previous studies have revealed both differences and similarities in the cortical inputs to direct and indirect pathways at population level. However, our investigation delves deeper to understand how a singular cortical input simultaneously drives these pathways, or might it regulate one pathway through distinct subpopulations? To address this, we employed rabies virus–mediated retrograde tracing from D1- or D2-SPNs and recorded non-starter SPNs to determine if they receive the same inputs as the starter SPNs. This approach allowed us to calculate the connection ratio and estimate the probable connection properties.

      Weaknesses: 

      One limitation is that all inputs to SPNs are expressing ChR2, so they cannot distinguish between different cortical subregions during patching experiments. Their results could arise because the same innervation patterns are repeated in many cortical subregions or because some subregions have preferential D1-SPN input while others do not.

      Thank you for raising this thoughtful concern. It is indeed not feasible to restrict ChR2 expression to a specific cortical region using the first-generation rabies-ChR2 system alone. A more refined approach would involve injecting Cre-dependent TVA and RG into the striatum of D1- or A2A-Cre mice, followed by rabies-Flp infection. Subsequently, a Flp-dependent ChR2 virus could be injected into the MCC or M1 to selectively label D1- or D2-projecting cortical neurons. This strategy would allow for more precise targeting and address many of the current limitations.

      However, a significant challenge lies in the cytotoxicity associated with rabies virus infection. Neuronal health begins to deteriorate substantially around 10 days post-infection, which provides an insufficient window for robust Flp-dependent ChR2 expression. We have tested several new rabies virus variants with extended survival times (Chatterjee et al., 2018; Jin et al., 2024), but unfortunately, they did not perform effectively or suitably in the corticostriatal systems we examined.

      In our experimental design, the aim is to delineate the connectivity probabilities to D1 or D2-SPNs from cortical neurons. Our hypothesis considered includes the possibility that similar innervation patterns could occur across multiple cortical subregions, or that some subregions might show preferential input to D1-SPNs while others do not, or a combination of both scenarios. This leads us to perform a series behavior test that using optogenetic activation of the D1- or D2-projecting cortical populations to see which could be the case.

      In the cortical areas we examined, MCC and M1, during behavioral testing, there is consistency with our electrophysiological results. Specifically, when we stimulated the D1-projecting cortical neurons either in MCC or in M1, mice exhibited facilitated local motion in open field test, which is the same to the activation of D1 SPNs in the striatum along (MCC: Fig 3C & D vs. I; M1: Fig 3F & G vs. L). Conversely, stimulation of D2-projecting MCC or M1 cortical neurons resulted in behavioral effects that appeared to combine characteristics of both D1- and D2-SPNs activation in the striatum (MCC: Fig 3C & D vs. J; M1: Fig 3F & G vs. M). The similar results were observed in the ICSS test. Our interpretation of these results is that the activation of D1-projecting neurons in the cortex induces behavior changes akin to D1 neuron activation, while activation of D2-projecting neurons in the cortex leads to a combined effect of both D1 and D2 neuron activation. This suggests that at least some cortical regions, the ones we tested, follow the hypothesis we proposed.

      There are also some caveats with respect to the efficacy of rabies tracing. Although they only patch non-starter cells in the striatum, only 63% of D1-SPNs receive input from D1-SPN-projecting cortical neurons. It's hard to say whether this is "high" or "low," but one question is how far from the starter cell region they are patching. Without this spatial indication of where the cells that are being patched are relative to the starter population, it is difficult to interpret if the cells being patched are receiving cortical inputs from the same neurons that are projecting to the starter population. Convergence of cortical inputs onto SPNs may vary with distance from the starter cell region quite dramatically, as other mapping studies of corticostriatal inputs have shown specialized local input regions can be defined based on cortical input patterns (Hintiryan et al., Nat Neurosci, 2016, Hunnicutt et al., eLife 2016, Peters et al., Nature, 2021).

      This is a valid concern regarding anatomical studies. Investigating cortico-striatal connectivity at the single-cell level remains technically challenging due to current methodological limitations. At present, we rely on rabies virus-mediated trans-synaptic retrograde tracing to identify D1- or D2-projecting cortical populations. This anatomical approach is coupled with ex vivo slice electrophysiology to assess the functional connectivity between these projection-defined cortical neurons and striatal SPNs. This enables us to quantify connection ratios, for example, the proportion of D1-projecting cortical neurons that functionally synapse onto non-starter D1-SPNs.

      To ensure the robustness of our conclusions, it is essential that both the starter cells and the recorded non-starter SPNs receive comparable topographical input from the cortex and other brain regions. Therefore, we carefully designed our experiments so that all recorded cells were located within the injection site, were mCherry-negative (i.e., non-starter cells), and were surrounded by ChR2-mCherry-positive neurons. This configuration ensured that the distance between recorded and starter cells did not exceed 100 µm, maintaining close anatomical proximity and thereby preserving the likelihood of shared cortical innervation within the examined circuitry.

      These methodological details are also described in the section on ex vivo brain slice electrophysiology, specifically in the Methods section, lines 396–399:

      “D1-SPNs (eGFP-positive in D1-eGFP mice, or eGFP-negative in D2-eGFP mice) or D2-SPNs (eGFP-positive in D2-eGFP mice, or eGFP-negative in D1-eGFP mice) that were ChR2-mCherry-negative, but in the injection site and surrounded by cells expressing ChR2-mCherry were targeted for recording.”

      This experimental strategy was implemented to control for potential spatial biases and to enhance the interpretability of our connectivity measurements.

      A caveat for the optogenetic behavioral experiments is that these optogenetic experiments did not include fluorophore-only controls.

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. A fluorophore-only control is indeed a valuable negative control, commonly used to rule out effects caused by light exposure independent of optogenetic manipulation. In this study, however, comparisons were made between light-on and light-off conditions within the same animal. This within-subject design, as employed in recent studies (Geddes et al., 2018; Zhu et al., 2025), is considered sufficient to isolate the effects of optogenetic manipulation.

      Furthermore, as shown in Figure S3, we conducted an additional control experiment in which optogenetic stimulation was applied to M1, while ensuring that ChR2 expression was restricted to the striatum via targeted viral infection. This approach serves as a functional equivalent to the control you suggested. Importantly, we observed no effects that could be attributed solely to light exposure, further supporting the conclusion that the observed outcomes in our main experiments are due to targeted optogenetic manipulation, rather than confounding effects of illumination.

      Lastly, by employing an in-animal comparison, measuring changes between stimulated and non-stimulated trials, we account for subject-specific variability and strengthen the interpretability of our findings.

      Another point of confusion is that other studies (Cui et al, J Neurosci, 2021) have reported that stimulation of D1-SPNs in DLS inhibits rather than promotes movement.

      Thank you for bringing the study by Cui and colleagues to our attention. While that study has generated some controversy, other independent investigations have demonstrated that activation of D1-SPNs in DLS facilitates local motion and lever-press behaviors (Dong et al., 2025; Geddes et al., 2018; Kravitz et al., 2010).

      It is still worth to clarify. The differences in behavioral outcomes observed between our study and that of Cui et al. may be attributable to several methodological factors, including differences in both the stereotaxic targeting coordinates and the optical fiber specifications used for stimulation.

      Specifically, in our experiments, the dorsomedial striatum (DMS) was targeted at coordinates AP +0.5 mm, ML ±1.5 mm, DV –2.2 mm, and the DLS at AP +0.5 mm, ML ±2.5 mm, DV –2.2 mm. In contrast, Cui et al. targeted the DMS at AP +0.9 mm, ML ±1.4 mm, DV –3.0 mm and the DLS at AP +0.7 mm, ML ±2.3 mm, DV –3.0 mm. These coordinates correspond to sites that are slightly more rostral and ventral compared to our own. Even subtle differences in anatomical targeting can result in activation of distinct neuronal subpopulations, which may account for the differing behavioral effects observed during optogenetic stimulation.

      In addition, the optical fibers used in the two studies varied considerably. We employed fibers with a 200 µm core diameter and a numerical aperture (NA) of 0.37, whereas Cui et al. used fibers with a 250 µm core diameter and a higher NA of 0.66. The combination of a larger core and higher NA in their setup implies a broader spatial spread and deeper tissue penetration of light, likely resulting in activation of a larger neural volume. This expanded volume of stimulation may have engaged additional neural circuits not recruited in our experiments, further contributing to the divergent behavioral outcomes. Taken together, these differences in targeting and photostimulation parameters are likely key contributors to the distinct effects reported between the two studies.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review): 

      In the manuscript by Klug and colleagues, the investigators use a rabies virus-based methodology to explore potential differences in connectivity from cortical inputs to the dorsal striatum. They report that the connectivity from cortical inputs onto D1 and D2 MSNs differs in terms of their projections onto the opposing cell type, and use these data to infer that there are differences in cross-talk between cortical cells that project to D1 vs. D2 MSNs. Overall, this manuscript adds to the overall body of work indicating that there are differential functions of different striatal pathways which likely arise at least in part by differences in connectivity that have been difficult to resolve due to difficulty in isolating pathways within striatal connectivity and several interesting and provocative observations were reported. Several different methodologies are used, with partially convergent results, to support their main points.

      However, I have significant technical concerns about the manuscript as presented that make it difficult for me to interpret the results of the experiments. My comments are below.

      Major:

      There is generally a large caveat to the rabies studies performed here, which is that both TVA and the ChR2-expressing rabies virus have the same fluorophore. It is thus essentially impossible to determine how many starter cells there are, what the efficiency of tracing is, and which part of the striatum is being sampled in any given experiment. This is a major caveat given the spatial topography of the cortico-striatal projections. Furthermore, the authors make a point in the introduction about previous studies not having explored absolute numbers of inputs, yet this is not at all controlled in this study. It could be that their rabies virus simply replicates better in D1-MSNs than D2-MSNs. No quantifications are done, and these possibilities do not appear to have been considered. Without a greater standardization of the rabies experiments across conditions, it is difficult to interpret the results.

      We thank the reviewer for raising these questions, which merit further discussion.

      Firstly, the primary aim of our study is to investigate the connectivity of the corticostriatal pathway. Given the current technical limitations, it is not feasible to trace all the striatal SPNs connected to a single cortical neuron. Therefore, we approached this from the opposite direction, starting from D1- or D2-SPNs to retrogradely label upstream cortical neurons, and then identifying their connected SPNs via functional synaptic recordings. To achieve this, we employed the only available transsynaptic retrograde method: rabies virus-mediated tracing. Because we crossed D1- or D2-GFP mice with D1- or A2A-Cre mice to identify SPN subtypes during electrophysiological recordings, the conventional rabies-GFP system could not be used to distinguish starter cells without conflicting with the GFP labeling of SPNs. To overcome this, we tagged ChR2 expression with mCherry. In this setup, we recorded from mCherry-negative D1- or D2-SPNs within the injection site and surrounded by mCherry-positive neurons. This ensures that the recorded neurons are topographically matched to the starter cell population and receive input from the same cortical regions. We acknowledge that TVA-only and ChR2-expressing cells are both mCherry-positive and therefore indistinguishable in our system. As such, mCherry-positive cells likely comprise a mixture of starter cells and TVA-only cells, representing a somewhat broader population than starter cells alone. Nevertheless, by restricting recordings to mCherry-negative SPNs within the injection site, it is ensured that our conclusions about functional connectivity remain valid and aligned with the primary objective of this study.

      Secondly, if rabies virus replication were significantly more efficient in D1-SPNs than in D2-SPNs, this would likely result in a higher observed connection probability in the D1-projecting group. However, we used consistent genetic strategies across all groups: D1-SPNs were defined as GFP-positive in D1-GFP mice and GFP-negative in D2-GFP mice, with D2-SPNs defined analogously. Recordings from both D1- and D2-SPNs were performed using the same methodology and under the same injection conditions within the same animals. This internal control helps mitigate the possibility that differential rabies infection efficiency biased our results.

      With these experimental safeguards in place, we found that 40% of D2-SPNs received input from D1-SPN-projecting cortical neurons, while 73% of D1-SPNs received input from D2-SPN-projecting cortical neurons. Although the ideal scenario would involve an even larger sample size to refine these estimates, the technical demands of post-rabies-infection electrophysiological recordings inherently limit throughput. Nonetheless, our approach represents the most feasible and accurate method currently available, and provides a significant advance in characterizing the functional connectivity within corticostriatal circuits.

      The authors claim using a few current clamp optical stimulation experiments that the cortical cells are healthy, but this result was far from comprehensive. For example, membrane resistance, capacitance, general excitability curves, etc are not reported. In Figure S2, some of the conditions look quite different (e.g., S2B, input D2-record D2, the method used yields quite different results that the authors write off as not different). Furthermore, these experiments do not consider the likely sickness and death that occurs in starter cells, as has been reported elsewhere. The health of cells in the circuit is overall a substantial concern that alone could invalidate a large portion, if not all, of the behavioral results. This is a major confound given those neurons are thought to play critical roles in the behaviors being studied. This is a major reason why first-generation rabies viruses have not been used in combination with behavior, but this significant caveat does not appear to have been considered, and controls e.g., uninfected animals, infected with AAV helpers, etc, were not included.

      We understand and appreciate the reviewer’s concern regarding the potential cytotoxicity of rabies virus infection. Indeed, this is a critical consideration when interpreting functional connectivity data. We have tested several newer rabies virus variants reported to support extended survival times (Chatterjee et al., 2018; Jin et al., 2024), but unfortunately, these variants did not perform reliably in the corticostriatal circuits we examined.

      Given these limitations, we relied on the rabies virus approach originally developed by Osakada et al. (Osakada et al., 2011), which demonstrated that neurons infected with rabies virus expressing ChR2 remain both viable and functional up to at least 10 days post-infection (Fig. 3, cited below). In our own experiments, we further validated the health and viability of cortical neurons, the presynaptic partners of SPNs, particularly around day 7 post-infection.

      To minimize the risk of viral toxicity, we performed ex vivo slice recordings within a conservative time window, between 4 and 8 days after infection, when the health of labeled neurons is well maintained. Moreover, the recorded SPNs were consistently mCherry-negative, indicating they were not directly infected by rabies virus, thus further reducing the likelihood of recording from compromised cells.

      Taken together, these steps help ensure that our synaptic recordings reflect genuine functional connectivity, rather than artifacts of viral toxicity. We hope this clarifies the rationale behind our experimental design.

      For the behavioral tests, including a naïve uninfected group and an AAV helper virus-only group as negative controls could be beneficial to isolate the specific impact of rabies virus infection. However, our primary focus is on the activation of selected presynaptic inputs to D1- or D2-SPNs by optogenetic method. Therefore, comparing stimulated versus non-stimulated trials within the same animal offers more direct and relevant results for our study objectives.

      It is also important to note that the ICSS test is particularly susceptible to the potential cytotoxic effects of rabies virus, as it spans a relatively extended period, from Day 4 to Day 12 post-infection. To mitigate this issue, we focused our analysis on the first 7 days of ICSS testing, thereby keeping the behavioral observations within 10 days post-rabies injection. This approach minimizes potential confounds from rabies-induced neurotoxicity while still capturing the relevant behavioral dynamics. Accordingly, we have revised Figure 3 and updated the statistical analyses to reflect this adjustment.

      The overall purity (e.g., EnvA-pseudotyping efficiency) of the RABV prep is not shown. If there was a virus that was not well EnvA-pseudotyped and thus could directly infect cortical (or other) inputs, it would degrade specificity.

      We agree that anatomical specificity is crucial for accurately labeling inputs to defined SPN populations in our study. The rabies virus strain employed here has been rigorously validated for its specificity in numerous previous studies from our group and others (Aoki et al., 2019; Klug et al., 2018; Osakada et al., 2011; Smith et al., 2016; Wall et al., 2013; Wickersham et al., 2007). For example, in a recent study by Aoki et al. (Aoki et al., 2019), we tested the same rabies virus strain by co-injecting the glycoprotein-deleted rabies virus and the TVA-expressing helper virus, without glycoprotein expressing AAV, into the SNr. As shown in Figure S1 (related to Figure 2), GFP expression was restricted to starter cells within the SNr, with no evidence of transsynaptic labeling in upstream regions such as the striatum, EPN, GPe, or STN (see panels F–H). These findings provide strong evidence that the rabies virus used in our experiments is properly pseudotyped and exhibits high specificity for starter cell labeling without off-target spread.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s emphasis on specificity, and we hope this clarification further supports the reliability of our anatomical tracing approach.

      While most of the study focuses on the cortical inputs, in slice recordings, inputs from the thalamus are not considered, yet likely contribute to the observed results. Related to this, in in vivo optogenetic experiments, technically, if the thalamic or other inputs to the dorsal striatum project to the cortex, their method will not only target cortical neurons but also terminals of other excitatory inputs. If this cannot be ruled it, stating that the authors are able to selectively activate the cortical inputs to one or the other population should be toned down.

      We agree with the reviewer that the thalamus is also a significant source of excitatory input to the striatum. However, current techniques do not allow for precise and exclusive labeling of upstream neurons in a given brain region, such as the cortex or thalamus. This technical limitation indeed makes it difficult to definitively determine whether inputs from these regions follow the same projection rules. Despite this, our findings show that stimulation of defined cortical populations, specifically, D1- or D2-projecting neurons in MCC and M1, elicits behavioral outcomes that closely mirror those observed in our ex vivo slice recordings, providing strong support for the cortical origin of the effects we observed.

      In our in vivo optogenetic experiments, we acknowledge that stimulating a specific cortical region may also activate axonal terminals from rabies-infected cortical or thalamic neurons. While somatic stimulation is generally more effective than terminal stimulation, we recognize the possibility that terminals on non-rabies-traced cortical neurons could be activated through presynaptic connections. To address this, we considered the finding of a previous study (Cruikshank et al., 2010), which demonstrated that while brief optogenetic stimulation (0.05 ms) of thalamo-cortical terminals can elicit few action potentials in postsynaptic cortical neurons, sustained terminal stimulation (500 ms) also results in only transient postsynaptic firing rather than prolonged activation (Fig. 3C, cited below). This suggests that cortical neurons exhibit only short-lived responses to continuous presynaptic stimulation of thalamic origin.

      In comparison, our behavioral paradigms employed prolonged optogenetic stimulation protocols- 20 Hz, 10 ms pulses for 15 s (open-field test), 1 s (ICSS), and 8 s (FR4/8)—which more closely resemble sustained stimulation conditions. Given these parameters, and the robust behavioral responses observed, it means that the effects are primarily mediated by activation of rabies-labeled, ChR2-expressing D1- or D2-projecting cortical neurons rather than indirect activation through thalamic input.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s valuable comment, and we have now incorporated this point into the revised manuscript (page 13, line 265 to 275) to more clearly address the potential contribution of thalamic inputs in our experimental design.

      The statements about specificity of connectivity are not well-founded. It may be that in the specific case where they are assessing outside of the area of injections, their conclusions may hold (e.g., excitatory inputs onto D2s have more inputs onto D1s than vice versa). However, how this relates to the actual site of injection is not clear. At face value, if such a connectivity exists, it would suggest that D1-MSNs receive substantially more overall excitatory inputs than D2s. It is thus possible that this observation would not hold over other spatial intervals. This was not explored and thus the conclusions are over-generalized. e.g., the distance from the area of red cells in the striatum to recordings was not quantified, what constituted a high level of cortical labeling was not quantified, etc. Without more rigorous quantification of what was being done, it is difficult to interpret the results. 

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the thoughtful comments and critical insights into our interpretation of connectivity data. These concerns are valid and provide an important opportunity to clarify and reinforce our experimental design and conclusions.

      Firstly, as described in our previous response, all patched neurons were carefully selected to be within the injection site and in close proximity to ChR2-mCherry-positive cells. Specifically, the estimated distance from each recorded neuron to the nearest starter cells did not exceed 100 µm. This design choice was made to minimize variability associated with spatial distance or heterogeneity in viral expression, thereby allowing for a more consistent sampling of putatively connected neurons.

      Secondly, quantifying both the number of starter and input neurons would, in principle, provide a more comprehensive picture of connectivity. However, given the technical limitations of the current approach particularly when combining rabies tracing with functional recordings it is not feasible to obtain such precise cell counts. Instead, we focused on connection ratios derived from targeted electrophysiological recordings, which offer a reliable and practical means of estimating connectivity within these defined circuits.

      Thirdly, regarding the potential influence of rabies-labeled neurons beyond the immediate recording site: while we acknowledge that rabies tracing labels a broad set of upstream neurons, our analysis was confined to a well-defined and localized area. The analogy we find helpful here is that of a spotlight - our recordings were restricted to the illuminated region directly under the beam, where the projection pattern is fixed and interpretable, regardless of what lies outside that area. Although we cannot fully account for all possible upstream connections, our methodology was designed to minimize variability and maintain consistency in the region of interest, which we believe supports the robustness of our conclusions in the ex vivo slice recording experiment.

      We hope this additional explanation addresses the reviewer’s concerns and helps clarify the rationale of our experimental strategy.

      The results in figure 3 are not well controlled. The authors show contrasting effects of optogenetic stimulation of D1-MSNs and D2-MSNs in the DMS and DLS, results which are largely consistent with the canon of basal ganglia function. However, when stimulating cortical inputs, stimulating the inputs from D1-MSNs gives the expected results (increased locomotion) while stimulating putative inputs to D2-MSNs had no effect. This is not the same as showing a decrease in locomotion - showing no effect here is not possible to interpret.

      We apologize for any confusion and appreciate the opportunity to clarify this point. Our electrophysiological recordings demonstrated that D1-projecting cortical neurons preferentially innervate D1-SPNs in the striatum, whereas D2-projecting cortical neurons provide input to both D1- and D2-SPNs, without a clear preference. These synaptic connectivity patterns are further supported by our behavioral experiments: optogenetic stimulation of D1-projecting neurons in cortical areas such as MCC and M1 led to behavioral effects consistent with direct D1-SPN activation. In contrast, stimulation of D2-projecting cortical neurons produced behavioral outcomes that appeared to reflect a mixture of both D1- and D2-SPN activation.

      We acknowledge that interpreting negative behavioral findings poses inherent challenges, as it is difficult to distinguish between a true lack of effect and insufficient experimental manipulation. To mitigate this, we ensured that all animals included in the analysis exhibited appropriate viral expression and correctly placed optic fibers in the targeted regions. These controls help to confirm that the observed behavioral effects - or lack thereof - are indeed due to the activation of the intended neuronal populations rather than technical artifacts such as weak expression or fiber misplacement.

      As shown in Author response image 1 below, our verification of virus expression and fiber positioning confirms effective targeting in MCC and M1 of A2A-Cre mice. Therefore, we interpret the negative behavioral outcomes as meaningful consequences of specific neural circuit activation.

      Author response image 1.

      Confocal image from A2A-Cre mouse showing targeted optogenetic stimulation of D2-projecting cortical neurons in MCC or M1. ChR2-mCherry expression highlights D2-projecting neurons, selectively labeled via rabies-mediated tracing. Optic fiber placement is confirmed above the cortical region of interest. Image illustrates robust expression and anatomical specificity necessary for pathway-selective stimulation in behavioral assays.

      In light of their circuit model, the result showing that inputs to D2-MSNs drive ICSS is confusing. How can the authors account for the fact that these cells are not locomotor-activating, stimulation of their putative downstream cells (D2-MSNs) does not drive ICSS, yet the cortical inputs drive ICSS? Is the idea that these inputs somehow also drive D1s? If this is the case, how do D2s get activated, if all of the cortical inputs tested net activate D1s and not D2s? Same with the results in figure 4 - the inputs and putative downstream cells do not have the same effects. Given the potential caveats of differences in viral efficiency, spatial location of injections, and cellular toxicity, I cannot interpret these experiments.

      We apologize for any confusion in our previous explanation. In our behavioral experiments, the primary objective was to determine whether activation of D1- or D2-projecting cortical neurons would produce behavioral outcomes distinct from those observed with pure D1 or D2 activation.

      Our findings show that stimulation of D1-projecting cortical neurons produced behavioral effects closely resembling those of selective D1 activation in both open field and ICSS tests. This is consistent with our slice recording data, which revealed that D1-projecting cortical neurons exhibit a higher connection probability with D1-SPNs than with D2-SPNs.

      In contrast, interpreting the effects of D2-projecting cortical neuron stimulation is inherently more nuanced. In the open field test, activation of these neurons did not significantly modulate local motion. This could reflect a balanced influence of D1 activation, which facilitates movement, and D2 activation, which suppresses it - resulting in a net neutral behavioral outcome. In the ICSS test, the absence of a strong reinforcement effect typically associated with D2 activation, combined with partial reinforcement likely due to concurrent D1 activation, suggests that stimulation of D2-projecting neurons produces a mixed behavioral signal. This outcome supports the interpretation that these neurons synapse onto both D1- and D2-SPNs, leading to a blended behavioral response that differs from selective D1 or D2 activation alone.

      Together, these two behavioral assays offer complementary perspectives, providing a more complete view of how projection-specific cortical inputs influence striatal output and behavior.

      In Figure 4 of the current manuscript (as cited below), we show that optogenetic activation of MCC neurons projecting to D1-SPNs facilitates sequence lever pressing, whereas activation of MCC neurons projecting to D2-SPNs does not induce significant behavioral changes. Conversely, activation of M1 neurons projecting to either D1- or D2-SPNs enhances lever pressing sequences. These observations align with our prior findings (Geddes et al., 2018; Jin et al., 2014), where we demonstrated that in the striatum, D1-SPN activation facilitates ongoing lever pressing, whereas D2-SPN activation is more involved in suppressing ongoing actions and promoting transitions between sub-sequences, shown in Fig. 4 from (Geddes et al., 2018; Jin et al., 2014) and Fig. 5K from (Jin et al., 2014) . Taken together, the facilitation of lever pressing by D1-projecting MCC and M1 neurons is consistent with their preferential connectivity to D1-SPNs and their established behavioral role.

      What is particularly intriguing, though admittedly more complex, is the behavioral divergence observed upon activation of D2-SPN-projecting cortical neurons. Activation of D2-projecting MCC neurons does not alter lever pressing, possibly reflecting a counterbalancing effect from concurrent D1- and D2-SPN activation. In contrast, stimulation of D2-projecting M1 neurons facilitates lever pressing, albeit less robustly than their D1-projecting counterparts. This discrepancy may reflect regional differences in striatal targets, DMS for MCC versus DLS for M1, as also supported by our open field test results. Furthermore, our recent findings (Zhang et al., 2025) show that synaptic strength from Cg to D2-SPNs is stronger than to D1-SPNs, whereas the M1 pathway exhibits the opposite pattern. These data suggest that beyond projection ratios, synaptic strength also shapes cortico-striatal functional output. Thus, stronger D2-SPN synapses in the DMS may offset D1-SPN activation during MCC-D2 stimulation, dampening lever pressing increase. Conversely, weaker D2 synapses in the DLS may permit M1-D2 projections to facilitate behavior more readily.

      In summary, the behavioral outcomes of our optogenetic manipulations support the proposed asymmetric cortico-striatal connectivity model. While the effects of D2-projecting neurons are not uniform, they reflect varying balances of D1 and D2-SPN influence, which further underscores the asymmetrical connections of cortical inputs to the striatum.

      Recommendations For The Authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      (1) What are the sample sizes for Fig S2? Some trends that are listed as nonsignificant look like they may just be underpowered. Related to this point, S2C indicates that PPR is statistically similar in all conditions. The traces shown in Figure 2 suggest that PPR is quite different in "Input D1"- vs "Input D2" projections. If there is indeed no difference, the exemplar traces should be replaced with more representative ones to avoid confusion. 

      Thank you for your suggestion. The sample size reported in Figure S2 corresponds to the neurons identified as connected in Figure 2. The representative traces shown in Figure 2 were selected based on their close alignment with the amplitude statistics and are intended to reflect typical responses. Given this, it is appropriate to retain the current examples as they accurately illustrate the underlying data.

      (2) Previous studies have described that SPN-SPN collateral inhibition is also asymmetric, with D2->D1 SPN connectivity stronger than the other direction. While cortical inputs to D2-SPNs may also strongly innervate D1-SPNs, it would be helpful to speculate on how collateral inhibition may further shape the biases (or lack thereof) reported here. 

      This would indeed be an interesting topic to explore. SPN-SPN mutual inhibition and/or interneuron inhibition may also play a role in the functional organization and output of the striatum. In the present study, we focused on the primary layer of cortico-striatal connectivity to examine how cortical neurons selectively connect to the striatal direct and indirect pathways, as these pathways have been shown to have distinct yet cooperative functions. To achieve this, we applied a GABAA receptor inhibitor to isolate only excitatory synaptic currents in SPNs, yielding the relevant results.

      To investigate additional circuit organization involving SPN-SPN mutual inhibition, the current available technique would involve single-cell initiated rabies tracing. This approach would help identify the starter SPN and the upstream SPNs that provide input to the starter cell, thereby offering a clearer understanding of the local circuit.

      (3) In Fig 3N-S there are no stats confirming that optogenetic stimulation does indeed increase lever pressing in each group (though it obviously looks like it does). It would be helpful to add statistics for this comparison, in addition to the between-group comparisons that are shown. 

      We thank the reviewer for this thoughtful suggestion. To assess whether optogenetic stimulation increases lever pressing in each group shown in Figures 3O, 3P, 3R, and 3S, we employed a permutation test (10,000 permutations). This non-parametric statistical method does not rely on assumptions about the underlying data distribution and is particularly appropriate for our analysis given the relatively small sample sizes.

      Additionally, in response to Reviewer 3’s concern regarding the potential cytotoxicity of rabies virus affecting behavioral outcomes during in vivo optogenetic stimulation experiments, we focused our analysis on Days 1 through 7 of the ICSS test. This time window remains within 10 days post-rabies infection, a period during which previous studies have reported minimal cytopathic effects (Osakada et al., 2011).

      Accordingly, we have updated Figure 3N-S and revised the associated statistical analyses in the figure legend as follows:

      (O-P) D1-SPN (red) but not D2-SPN stimulation (black) drives ICSS behavior in both the DMS (O: D1, n = 6, permutation test, slope = 1.5060, P = 0.0378; D2, n = 5, permutation test, slope = -0.2214, P = 0.1021; one-tailed Mann Whitney test, Day 7 D1 vs. D2, P = 0.0130) and the DLS (P: D1, n = 6, permutation test, slope = 28.1429, P = 0.0082; D2, n = 5, permutation test, slope = -0.3429, P = 0.0463; one-tailed Mann Whitney test, Day 7 D1 vs. D2, P = 0.0390). *, P < 0.05. (Q) Timeline of helper virus injections, rabies-ChR2 injections and optogenetic stimulation for ICSS behavior. (R-S) Optogenetic stimulation of the cortical neurons projecting to either D1- or D2-SPNs induces ICSS behavior in both the MCC (R: MCC-D1, n = 5, permutation test, Day1-Day7, slope = 2.5857, P = 0.0034; MCC-D2, n = 5, Day2-Day7, permutation test, slope = 1.4229, P = 0.0344; no significant effect on Day7, MCC-D1 vs. MCC-D2,  two-tailed Mann Whitney test, P = 0.9999) and the M1 (S: M1-D1, n = 5, permutation test, Day1-Day7, slope = 1.8214, P = 0.0259; M1-D2, n = 5, Day1-Day7, permutation test, slope = 1.8214, P = 0.0025; no significant effect on Day7, M1-D1 vs. M1-D2, two-tailed Mann Whitney test, P = 0.3810). n.s., not statistically significant.

      We believe this updated analysis and additional context further strengthen the validity of our conclusions regarding the reinforcement effects.

      (4) Line 206: mice were trained for "a few more days" is not a very rigorous description. It would be helpful to state the range of additional days of training. 

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. In accordance with the Methods section, we have now specified the number of days, which is 4 days, in the main text (line 207).

      (5) In Fig 4D,H, the statistical comparison is relative modulation (% change) by stimulation of D1- vs D2- projecting inputs. Please show statistics comparing the effect of stimulation on lever presses for each individual condition. For example, is the effect of MCC-D2 stimulation in panel D negative or not significant? 

      Thank you for your suggestion. Below are the statistical results, which we have also incorporated into the figure legend for clarity. To assess the net effects of each manipulation, we compared the observed percentage changes with a theoretical value of zero.

      In Figure 4D, optogenetic stimulation of D1-projecting MCC neurons significantly increased the pressing rate (MCC-D1, n = 8, one-sample two-tailed t-test, t = 2.814, P = 0.0131), whereas stimulation of D2-projecting MCC neurons did not produce a significant effect (MCC-D2, n = 7, one-sample two-tailed t-test, t = 0.8481, P = 0.4117).

      In contrast, Figure 4H shows that optogenetic stimulation of both D1- and D2-projecting M1 neurons significantly increased the sequence press rate (M1-D1, n = 6, one-sample two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, P = 0.0046; M1-D2, n = 7, one-sample two-tailed Wilcoxon signed-rank test, P = 0.0479).

      These analyses help clarify the distinct behavioral effects of manipulating different corticostriatal projections.

      (6) Are data in Fig 1G-H from a D1- or A2a- cre mouse? 

      The data in Fig 1G-H are from a D1-Cre mouse.

      (7) In Fig S3 it looks like there may actually be an effect of 20Hz simulation of D2-SPNs. Though it probably doesn't affect the interpretation. 

      As indicated by the statistics, there is a slight, but not statistically significant, decrease in local motion when 20 Hz stimulation is delivered to the motor cortex with ChR2 expression in D2-SPNs in the striatum.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      The rabies tracing is referred to on several occasions as "new" but the reference papers are from 2011, 2013, and 2018. It is unclear what is new about the system used in the paper and what new feature is relevant to the experiments that were performed. Either clarify or remove "new" terminology. 

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. We have revised the relevant text accordingly at line 20 in the Abstract, line 31 in the In Brief, line 69 in the Introduction, line 83 in the Results, and line 226 in the Discussion to improve clarity and accuracy.

      In Figure 2 D and G, D1 eGFP (+) and D2 eGFP(-) are plotted separately. These are the same cell type; therefore it may work best to combine that data. This could also be done for 'input to D2- Record D2' in panel D as well as 'input D1-Record D2' and 'input D2-Record D1' in panel G. Combining the information in panel D and G and comparing all 4 conditions to each other would give a better understanding of the comparison of functional connectivity between cortical neurons and D1 and D2 SPNs. 

      We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful suggestion. While presenting single bars for each condition (e.g., ‘input D1 - record D1’) might improve visual simplicity, it would obscure an important aspect of our experimental design. Specifically, we aimed to highlight that the comparisons between D1- and D2-projecting neurons to D1 and D2 SPNs were counterbalanced within the same animals - not just across different groups. By showing both D1-eGFP(+) and D2-eGFP(-), or vice versa, within each group and at similar proportions, we provide a more complete picture of the internal control built into our design. This format helps ensure the audience that our conclusions are not biased by group-level differences, but are supported by within-subject comparisons. Therefore, that the current presentation better could serve to communicate the rigor and balance of our experimental approach.

      The findings in Figure 2 are stated as D1 projecting excitatory inputs have a higher probability of targeting D1 SPNs while D2 projecting excitatory inputs target both D1 SPNs and D2 SPNs. It may be more clear to say that some cortical neurons project specifically to D1 SPNs while other cortical neurons project to both D1 and D2 SPNs equally. A better summary diagram could also help with clarity. 

      Thank you for bringing this up. The data we present reflect the connection probabilities of D1- or D2-projecting cortical neurons to D1 or D2 SPNs. One possible interpretation is like the reviewer said that a subset of cortical neurons preferentially target D1 SPNs, while others exhibit more balanced projections to both D1 and D2 SPNs. However, we cannot rule out alternative explanations - for example, that some D2-projecting neurons preferentially target D2 SPNs, or that the observed differences arise from the overall proportions of D1- and D2-projecting cortical neurons connecting to each striatal subtype.

      There are multiple possible patterns of connectivity that could give rise to the observed differences in connection ratios. Based on our current data, we can confidently conclude the existence of asymmetric cortico-striatal projections to the direct and indirect pathways, but the precise nature of this asymmetry will require further investigation.

      Figure 4 introduces the FR8 task, but there are similar takeaways to the findings from Figure 3. Is there another justification for the FR8 task or interesting way of interpreting that data that could add richness to the manuscript?

      The FR8 task is a self-initiated operant sequence task that relies on motor learning mechanisms, whereas the open field test solely assesses spontaneous locomotion. Furthermore, the sequence task enables us to dissect the functional role of specific neuronal populations in the initiation, maintenance, and termination of sequential movements through closed-loop optogenetic manipulations integrated into the task design. These methodological advantages underscore the rationale for including Figure 4 in the manuscript, as it highlights the unique insights afforded by this experimental paradigm.

      I am somewhat surprised to see that D1-SPN stimulation in DLS gave the results in Figure 3 F and P, as mentioned in the public review. These contrast with some previous results (Cui et al, J Neurosci, 2021). Any explanation? Would be useful to speculate or compare parameters as this could have important implications for DLS function.

      Thank you for raising this point. While Cui’s study has generated some debate, several independent investigations have consistently demonstrated that stimulation of D1-SPNs in the dorsolateral striatum (DLS) facilitates local motion and lever-press behaviors (Dong et al., 2025; Geddes et al., 2018; Kravitz et al., 2010). These findings support the functional role of D1-SPNs in promoting movement and motivated actions.

      The differences in behavioral outcomes observed between our study and that of Cui et al. may stem from several methodological factors, particularly related to anatomical targeting and optical stimulation parameters.

      Specifically, our experiments targeted the DMS at AP +0.5 mm, ML ±1.5 mm, DV –2.2 mm, and the DLS at AP +0.5 mm, ML ±2.5 mm, DV –2.2 mm. In contrast, Cui’s study targeted the DMS at AP +0.9 mm, ML ±1.4 mm, DV –3.0 mm, and the DLS at AP +0.7 mm, ML ±2.3 mm, DV –3.0 mm. These differences indicate that their targeting was slightly more rostral and more ventral than ours, which could have led to stimulation of distinct neuronal populations within the striatum, potentially accounting for variations in behavioral effects observed during optogenetic activation.

      In addition, the optical fibers used in the two studies differed markedly. We employed optical fibers with a 200 µm core diameter and a numerical aperture (NA) of 0.37. Cui’s study used fibers with a larger core diameter (250 µm) and a higher NA (0.66), which would produce a broader spread and deeper penetration of light. This increased photostimulation volume may have recruited a more extensive network of neurons, possibly including off-target circuits, thus influencing the behavioral outcomes in a manner not seen in our more spatially constrained stimulation paradigm.

      Taken together, these methodological differences, both in anatomical targeting and optical stimulation parameters, likely contribute to the discrepancies in behavioral results observed between the two studies. Our findings, consistent with other independent reports, support the role of D1-SPNs in facilitating movement and reinforcement behaviors under more controlled and localized stimulation conditions.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      Minor: 

      The authors repeatedly state that they are using a new rabies virus system, but the system has been in widespread use for 16 years, including in the exact circuits the authors are studying, for over a decade. I would not consider this new. 

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. We have revised the relevant text accordingly at line 20 in the Abstract, line 31 in the In Brief, line 69 in the Introduction, line 83 in the Results, and line 226 in the Discussion to improve clarity and accuracy.

      Figure 2G, how many mice were used for recordings?

      In Fig. 2G, we used 8 mice in the D1-projecting to D2 EGFP(+) group, 7 mice in the D1-projecting to D1 EGFP(-) group, 8 mice in the D2-projecting to D1 EGFP(+) group, and 10 mice in the D2-projecting to D2 EGFP(-) group.

      The amplitude of inputs was not reported in figure 2. This is important, as the strength of the connection matters. This is reported in Figure S2, but how exactly this relates to the presence or absence of connections should be made clearer.

      The amplitude data presented in Figure S2 summarize all recorded currents from confirmed connections, as detailed in the Methods section. A connection is defined by the presence of a detectable and reliable postsynaptic current with an onset latency of less than 10 ms following laser stimulation.

      Reference in the reply-to-review comments:

      Aoki, S., Smith, J.B., Li, H., Yen, X.Y., Igarashi, M., Coulon, P., Wickens, J.R., Ruigrok, T.J.H., and Jin, X. (2019). An open cortico-basal ganglia loop allows limbic control over motor output via the nigrothalamic pathway. Elife 8, e49995.

      Chatterjee, S., Sullivan, H.A., MacLennan, B.J., Xu, R., Hou, Y.Y., Lavin, T.K., Lea, N.E., Michalski, J.E., Babcock, K.R., Dietrich, S., et al. (2018). Nontoxic, double-deletion-mutant rabies viral vectors for retrograde targeting of projection neurons. Nat Neurosci 21, 638-646.

      Cruikshank, S.J., Urabe, H., Nurmikko, A.V., and Connors, B.W. (2010). Pathway-Specific Feedforward Circuits between Thalamus and Neocortex Revealed by Selective Optical Stimulation of Axons. Neuron 65, 230-245.

      Dong, J., Wang, L.P., Sullivan, B.T., Sun, L.X., Smith, V.M.M., Chang, L.S., Ding, J.H., Le, W.D., Gerfen, C.R., and Cai, H.B. (2025). Molecularly distinct striatonigral neuron subtypes differentially regulate locomotion. Nat Commun 16, 2710.

      Geddes, C.E., Li, H., and Jin, X. (2018). Optogenetic Editing Reveals the Hierarchical Organization of Learned Action Sequences. Cell 174, 32-43.

      Jin, L., Sullivan, H.A., Zhu, M., Lavin, T.K., Matsuyama, M., Fu, X., Lea, N.E., Xu, R., Hou, Y.Y., Rutigliani, L., et al. (2024). Long-term labeling and imaging of synaptically connected neuronal networks in vivo using double-deletion-mutant rabies viruses. Nat Neurosci 27, 373-383.

      Jin, X., Tecuapetla, F., and Costa, R.M. (2014). Basal ganglia subcircuits distinctively encode the parsing and concatenation of action sequences. Nat Neurosci 17, 423-430.

      Klug, J.R., Engelhardt, M.D., Cadman, C.N., Li, H., Smith, J.B., Ayala, S., Williams, E.W., Hoffman, H., and Jin, X. (2018). Differential inputs to striatal cholinergic and parvalbumin interneurons imply functional distinctions. Elife 7, e35657.

      Kravitz, A.V., Freeze, B.S., Parker, P.R.L., Kay, K., Thwin, M.T., Deisseroth, K., and Kreitzer, A.C. (2010). Regulation of parkinsonian motor behaviours by optogenetic control of basal ganglia circuitry. Nature 466, 622-626.

      Osakada, F., Mori, T., Cetin, A.H., Marshel, J.H., Virgen, B., and Callaway, E.M. (2011). New Rabies Virus Variants for Monitoring and Manipulating Activity and Gene Expression in Defined Neural Circuits. Neuron 71, 617-631.

      Smith, J.B., Klug, J.R., Ross, D.L., Howard, C.D., Hollon, N.G., Ko, V.I., Hoffman, H., Callaway, E.M., Gerfen, C.R., and Jin, X. (2016). Genetic-Based Dissection Unveils the Inputs and Outputs of Striatal Patch and Matrix Compartments. Neuron 91, 1069-1084.

      Wall, N.R., De La Parra, M., Callaway, E.M., and Kreitzer, A.C. (2013). Differential Innervation of Direct- and Indirect-Pathway Striatal Projection Neurons. Neuron 79, 347-360.

      Wickersham, I.R., Lyon, D.C., Barnard, R.J.O., Mori, T., Finke, S., Conzelmann, K.K., Young, J.A.T., and Callaway, E.M. (2007). Monosynaptic restriction of transsynaptic tracing from single, genetically targeted neurons. Neuron 53, 639-647.

      Zhang, B.B., Geddes, C.E., and Jin, X. (2025) Complementary corticostriatal circuits orchestrate action repetition and switching. Sci Adv, in press.

      Zhu, Z.G., Gong, R., Rodriguez, V., Quach, K.T., Chen, X.Y., and Sternson, S.M. (2025). Hedonic eating is controlled by dopamine neurons that oppose GLP-1R satiety. Science 387, eadt0773.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Cho et al. present a comprehensive and multidimensional analysis of glutamine metabolism in the regulation of B cell differentiation and function during immune responses. They further demonstrate how glutamine metabolism interacts with glucose uptake and utilization to modulate key intracellular processes. The manuscript is clearly written, and the experimental approaches are informative and well-executed. The authors provide a detailed mechanistic understanding through the use of both in vivo and in vitro models. The conclusions are well supported by the data, and the findings are novel and impactful. I have only a few, mostly minor, concerns related to data presentation and the rationale for certain experimental choices.

      Detailed Comments:

      (1) In Figure 1b, it is unclear whether total B cells or follicular B cells were used in the assay. Additionally, the in vitro class-switch recombination and plasma cell differentiation experiments were conducted without BCR stimulation, which makes the system appear overly artificial and limits physiological relevance. Although the effects of glutamine concentration on the measured parameters are evident, the results cannot be confidently interpreted as true plasma cell generation or IgG1 class switching under these conditions. The authors should moderate these claims or provide stronger justification for the chosen differentiation strategy. Incorporating a parallel assay with anti-BCR stimulation would improve the rigor and interpretability of these findings. 

      We will edit the manuscript to be more explicit that total splenic B cells were used in this set-up figure and the rest of the paper. In addition, we will try to perform new experiments to improve this "set-up figure" (and add old and new data for Supplemental Figure presentation). Specifically, we will increase the range of conditions tested - e.g., styles of stimulating proliferation and differentiation - to foster an increased sense of generality. We plan to compare mitogenic stimulation with anti-CD40 to  anti-IgM and to anti-IgM + anti-CD40, all with BAFF, IL-4, and IL-5, bearing in mind excellent work from Aiba et al, Immunity 2006; 24: 259-268, and similar papers. We also will try to present some representative flow cytometric profiles (presumably in new Supplemental Figure panels).

      To be transparent and add to a more open public discussion (using the virtues of this forum, the senior author and colleagues would caution about whether any in vitro conditions exist that warrant complete confidence. That is the reason for proceeding to immunization experiments in vivo. That is not said to cast doubt on our own in vitro data - there are some experiments (such as those of Fig. 1a-c and associated Supplemental Fig. 1) that only can be done in vitro or are better done that way (e.g., because of rapid uptake of early apoptotic B cells in vivo).

      For instance: Well-respected papers use the CD40LB and NB21.2D9 systems to activate B cells and generate plasma cells. Those appear to be BCR-independent and unfortunately, we found that they cannot be used with a.a. deprivation or these inhibitors due to effects on the engineered stroma-like cells. In considering BCR engagement, Reth has published salient points about signaling and concentrations of the Ab, the upshot being that this means of activating mitogenesis and plasma cell differentiation (when the B cells are costimulated via CD40 or TLR(4 or 7/8) is probably more than a bit artificial. Moreover, although Aiba et al, Immunity 2006; 24: 259-268 is a laudable exception, one rarely finds papers using BAFF despite the strong evidence it is an essential part of the equation of B cell regulation in vivo and a cytokine that modulates BCR signaling - in the cultures. 

      (2) In Figure 1c, the DMK alone condition is not presented. This hinders readers' ability to properly asses the glutaminolysis dependency of the cells for the measured readouts. Also, CD138+ in developing PCs goes hand in hand with decreased B220 expression. A representative FACS plot showing the gating strategy for the in vitro PCs should be added as a supplementary figure. Similarly, division number (going all the way to #7) may be tricky to gate and interpret. A representative FACS plot showing the separation of B cells according to their division numbers and a subsequent gating of CD138 or IgG1 in these gates would be ideal for demonstrating the authors' ability to distinguish these populations effectively.

      We agree that exact placement  of divisions deconvolution by FlowJow is more fraught than might be thought forpresentations in many or most papers. For the revision, we will try to add one or several representative FACS plot(s) with old and new data to provide the gating on CTV fluorescence, bearing these points in mind when extending the experiments from ~7 years ago (Fig. 1b, c). With the representative examples of the old data pasted in here, we will aver, however, that using divisions 0-6, and ≥7 was reasonable. 

      Ditto for DMK with normal glutamine. However, in the spirit of eLife transparency lacking in many other journals, this comparison is more fraught than the referee comment would make things seem. The concentration tolerated by cells is highly dependent on the medium and glutamine concentration, and perhaps on rates of glutaminolysis (due to its generation of ammonia). In practice, we find that DMK becomes more toxic to B cells unless glutamine is low or glutaminolysis is restricted. Thus, the concentration of DMK that is tolerated and used in Fig. 1b, c can become toxic to the B cells when using the higher levels of glutamine in typical culture media (2 mM or more) - at which point the "normal conditions + DMK" "control" involves the surviving cells in conditions with far greater cell death and less population expansion than the "low glutamine + DMK". condition. Overall, we appreciate the suggestion to show more DMK data and will work to do so for the earlier proliferation data (shown above) and the new experiments.  

      Author response image 1.

       

      (3) A brief explanation should be provided for the exclusive use of IgG1 as the readout in class-switching assays, given that naïve B cells are capable of switching to multiple isotypes. Clarifying why IgG1 was preferentially selected would aid in the interpretation of the results.

      We will edit the text to be more explicit and harmonize in light of the referee's suggestion that we focus the presentation of serologic data on IgG1 in the immunization experiments.

      [IgG1 provides the strongest signal and hence better signal/noise both in vitro and with the alum-based immunizations that are avatars for the adjuvant used in the majority of protein-based vaccines for humans.]

      (4) The immunization experiments presented in Figures 1 and 2 are well designed, and the data are comprehensively presented. However, to prevent potential misinterpretation, it should be clarified that the observed differences between NP and OVA immunizations cannot be attributed solely to the chemical nature of the antigens - hapten versus protein. A more significant distinction lies in the route of administration (intraperitoneal vs. intranasal) and the resulting anatomical compartment of the immune response (systemic vs. lung-restricted). This context should be explicitly stated to avoid overinterpretation of the comparative findings.

      We agree with the referee and will edit the text accordingly. Certainly, the difference in how the anti-ova response is elicited compared to the anti-NP response in the same mice or with a bit different an immunization regimen might be another factor - or the major factor - that could contribute towards explaining why glutaminolysis was important after ovalbumin inhalations (used because emergence of anti-ova Ab / ASCs is suppressed by the NP hapten after NP-ova immunization) but not needed for the anti-NP response unless Slc2a1 or Mpc2 also was inactivated. Thank you prompting addition of this caveat.

      Nevertheless, it seems fair to note that in Figures 1 and 2, the ASCs and Ab are being analyzed for NP and ova in the same mice, albeit with the NP-specific components not being driven by the inhalations of ovalbumin. With that in mind, when one compares the IgG1 anti-NP ASC and Ab to those for IgG1 anti-ovalbumin (ASC in bone marrow; Ab), the ovalbumin-specific response was reduced whereas the anti-NP response was not.

      (5) NP immunization is known to be an inducer of an IgG1-dominant Th2-type immune response in mice. IgG2c is not a major player unless a nanoparticle delivery system is used. However, the authors arbitrarily included IgG2c in their assays in Figures 2 and 3. This may be confusing for the readers. The authors should either justify the IgG2c-mediated analyses or remove them from the main figures. (It can be added as supplemental information with proper justification). 

      We will rearrange the Figure panels to move the IgM and IgG2c data to Supplemental Figures.

      For purposes of public discourse, we note that the data of previous Figure 3(c, g) show a very strong NP-specific IgG2c response that seems to contradict the concept that IgG2c responses necessarily are weak in this setting, and the important role of IgG2c (mouse - IgG1 in humans) in controlling or clearing various pathogens as well as in autoimmunity. So from the standpoint of providing a better sense of generality to the loss-of-function effects, we continue to think that these measurements are quite important. That said, the main text has many figure panels and as the review notes, the class switching and in vitro ASC generation were done with IL-4 / IgG1-promoting conditions. If possible, we will try to assay in vitro class switching with IFN-g rather than IL-4 but there may not be enough resources (time before lab closure; money).

      [As a collegial aside, we speculate that a greater or lesser IgG2c anti-NP response may arise due to different preparations of NP-carrier obtained from the vendor (Biosearch) having different amounts of TLR (e.g., TLR4) ligand. In any case, the points of presenting the IgG2c (and IgM) data were to push against the limiting boundaries of convention (which risks perpetuating a narrow view of potential outcomes) and make the breadth of results more apparent to readers.

      (6) Similarly, in affinity maturation analyses, including IgM is somewhat uncommon. I do not see any point in showing high affinity (NP2/NP20) IgMs (Figure 3d), since that data probably does not mean much.

      As noted in the reply immediately preceding this one, we appreciate this suggestion from the reviewer and will move the IgM and IgG2c to Supplemental status.

      Nonetheless, in collegial discourse we disagree a bit with the referee in light of our data as well as of work that (to our minds) leads one to question why inclusion of affinity maturation of IgM is so uncommon - as the referee accurately notes. Of course a defect in the capacity to class-switch is highly deleterious in patients but that is not the same as concluding that recall IgM or its affinity is of little consequence.

      In some of the pioneering work back in the 1980's, Bothwell showed that NP-carrier immunization generated hybridomas producing IgM Ab with extensive SHM (~11% of the 18 lineages; ~ 1/3 of the IgM hybridomas) [PMID: 8487778], IgM B cells appear to move into GC, and there is at least a reasonable published basis for the view that there are GC-derived IgM (unswitched) memory B cells (MBC) that would be more likely, upon recall activation, to differentiate into ASCs. [As an example, albeit with the Jenkins lab anti-rPE response, Taylor, Pape, and Jenkins generated quantitative estimates of the numbers of Ag-specific IgM<sup>+</sup>vs switched MBC that were GC-derived (or not). [PMID: 22370719]. While they emphasized that ~90% of  IgM<sup>+</sup> MBC appeared to be GC-independent, their data also indicated that ~1/2 of all GC-derived MBC were IgM<sup>+</sup> rather than switched (their Fig. 8, B vs C; also 8E, which includes alum-PE). And while we immensely respect the referee, we are perhaps less confident that IgM or high-affinity Ag-specific IgM doesn't mean that much, if only because of evidence that localized Ab compete for Ag and may thus influence selective processes [PMCID: PMC2747358; PMID: 15953185; PMID: 23420879; PMID: 27270306].

      (7) Following on my comment for the PC generation in Figure 1 (see above), in Figure 4, a strategy that relies solely on CD40L stimulation is performed. This is highly artificial for the PC generation and needs to be justified, or more physiologically relevant PC generation strategies involving anti-BCR, CD40L, and various cytokines should be shown. 

      In line with our response to point (1), we plan and will try to self-fund testing BCR-stimulated B cells (anti-CD40 to  anti-IgM and to anti-IgM + anti-CD40, all with BAFF, IL-4, and IL-5).

      (8) The effects of CB839 and UK5099 on cell viability are not shown. Including viability data under these treatment conditions would be a valuable addition to the supplementary materials, as it would help readers more accurately interpret the functional outcomes observed in the study. 

      We will add to the supplemental figures to present data that provide cues as to relative viability / survival under the experimental conditions used. [FSC X SSC as well as 7AAD or Ghost dye panels; we also hope to generate new data that include further experiments scoring annexin V staining.]

      (9) It is not clear how the RNA seq analysis in Figure 4h was generated. The experimental strategy and the setup need to be better explained.

      The revised manuscript will include more information (at minimum in the Methods, Legend), and we apologize that in this and a few other instances sufficiency of detail was sacrificed on the altar of brevity.

      [Adding a brief synopsis to any reader before the final version of record, given the many months it will take to generate new data, thoroughly revise the manuscript, etc:

      In three temporally and biologically independent experiments, cultures were harvested 3.5 days after splenic B cells were purified and cultured as in the experiments of Fig. 4a-e. total cellular RNA prepared from the twelve samples (three replicates for each of four conditions - DMSO vehicle control, CB839, UK5099, and CB839 + UK5099) was analyzed by RNA-seq. After the RNA-seq data were initially processed using the pipeline described in the Methods. For panels g & h of Fig 4, DE Seq2 was used to quantify and compare read counts in the three CB839 + UK5099 samples relative to the three independent vehicle controls and identify all genes for which variances yielded P<0.05. In Fig 4g, all such genes for which the difference was 'statistically significant' (i.e., P<0.05) were entered into the Immgen tool and thereby mapped to the B lineage subsets shown in the figure panels (i.e., g, h). In (g), these are displayed using one format, whereas (h) uses the 'heatmap' tool in MyGeneSet.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this manuscript, the authors investigate the functional requirements for glutamine and glutaminolysis in antibody responses. The authors first demonstrate that the concentrations of glutamine in lymph nodes are substantially lower than in plasma, and that at these levels, glutamine is limiting for plasma cell differentiation in vitro. The authors go on to use genetic mouse models in which B cells are deficient in glutaminase 1 (Gls), the glucose transporter Slc2a1, and/or mitochondrial pyruvate carrier 2 (Mpc2) to test the importance of these pathways in vivo. 

      Interestingly, deficiency of Gls alone showed clear antibody defects when ovalbumin was used as the immunogen, but not the hapten NP. For the latter response, defects in antibody titers and affinity were observed only when both Gls and either Mpc2 or Slc2a1 were deleted. These latter findings form the basis of the synthetic auxotrophy conclusion. The authors go on to test these conclusions further using in vitro differentiations, Seahorse assays, pharmacological inhibitors, and targeted quantification of specific metabolites and amino acids. Finally, the authors document reduced STAT3 and STAT1 phosphorylation in response to IL-21 and interferon (both type 1 and 2), respectively, when both glutaminolysis and mitochondrial pyruvate metabolism are prevented. 

      Strengths:

      (1) The main strength of the manuscript is the overall breadth of experiments performed. Orthogonal experiments are performed using genetic models, pharmacological inhibitors, in vitro assays, and in vivo experiments to support the claims. Multiple antigens are used as test immunogens--this is particularly important given the differing results. 

      (2) B cell metabolism is an area of interest but understudied relative to other cell types in the immune system. 

      (3) The importance of metabolic flexibility and caution when interpreting negative results is made clear from this study.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) All of the in vivo studies were done in the context of boosters at 3 weeks and recall responses 1 week later. This makes specific results difficult to interpret. Primary responses, including germinal centers, are still ongoing at 3 weeks after the initial immunization. Thus, untangling what proportion of the defects are due to problems in the primary vs. memory response is difficult.

      (2) Along these lines, the defects shown in Figure 3h-i may not be due to the authors' interpretation that Gls and Mpc2 are required for efficient plasma cell differentiation from memory B cells. This interpretation would only be correct if the absence of Gls/Mpc2 leads to preferential recruitment of low-affinity memory B cells into secondary plasma cells. The more likely interpretation is that ongoing primary germinal centers are negatively impacted by Gls and Mpc2 deficiency, and this, in turn, leads to reduced affinities of serum antibodies

      We provisionally plan to edit the wording of the conclusion a bit to add a possibility we consider unlikely to avoid a conclusion that MBCs bearing switched BCRs are affected once reactivated. We also will perform a new experiment to investigate, but unfortunately time before lab closure has been and remains our enemy both for performance and multiple replication of the work presented in Figure 3, panels h & i, and the related Supplemental Data (Supplemental Fig. 3a-j). Unfortunately, it will not be possible to do a memory experiment with recall immunization out at 8 weeks.  Despite the grant funding running out and institutional belt-tightening, however, we'll try to perform a new head-to-head comparison of 4 wk post-immunization with and without the boost at three weeks.

      The intriguing concern (points 1 & 2) provides a springboard for consideration of generalizations and simplifications. Germinal center durability is not at all monolithic, and instead is quite variable**. The premise (cognitive bias, perhaps?) in the interpretation is that in our previous work we find few if any GC B cells - NP-APC-binding or otherwise - above the background (non-immunized controls) three weeks after immunization with NP-ovalbumin in alum. Recognizing that it is not NP-carrier in alum as immunizations, we note for the readers and referee that Fig. 1 of the Taylor, Pape, & Jenkins paper considered above [PMID: 22370719] reported 10-fold more Ag-specific MBCs than GC B cells at day 29 post-immunization (the point at which the boost / recall challenge was performed in our Figure 3h, i).

      Viewed from that perspective, the surmise of the comment is that a major contribution to the differences in both all-affinity and high-affinity anti-NP IgG1 shown in Fig. 3i derives from the immunization at 4 wk stimulating GC B cells we cannot find as opposed to memory B cells. However, it is true that in the literature (especially with the experimentally different approach of transferring BCR-transgenic / knock-in versions of an NP-biased BCR) there may be meaningful pools of IgG1 and IgG2c GC B cells. Alternatively, our current reagents for immunizations may have become better at maintaining GC than those in the past - which we will try to test.

      The issue and question also relate to rates of output of plasma cells or rises in the serum concentrations of class-switched Ab. To this point, our prior experiences agree with the long-published data of the Kurosaki lab in Figure 3c of the Aiba et al paper noted above (Immunity, 2006) (and other such time courses). Readers can note that the IgG1 anti-NP response (alum adjuvant, as in our work) hits its plateau at 2 wk, and did not increase further from 2 to 3 wk. In other words, GC are on the decline and  Ab production has reached its plateau by the time of the 2nd immunization in Fig. 3h). 

      Assuming we understand the comment and line of reasoning correctly, we also lean towards disagreeing with the statement "This interpretation would only be correct if the absence of Gls/Mpc2 leads to preferential recruitment of low-affinity memory B cells into secondary plasma cells." Our evidence shows that both low-affinity as well as high-affinity anti-NP Ab (IgG1) went down as a result of combined gene-inactivation after the peak primary response (Fig. 3i). Recent papers show that affinity maturation is attributable to greater proliferation of plasmablasts with high-affinity BCR. Accordingly, the findings with loss of GLS and MPC function are quite consistent with the interpretation that much of the response after the second immunization draws on MBC differentiation into plasmablasta and then plasma cells, where the proliferative advantage of high-affinity cells is blunted by the impaired metabolism. The provisional plan, however, is to note the alternative, if less likely, interpretation proposed by the review.

      ** In some contexts, of course, especially certain viral infections or vaccination with lipid nanoparticles carrying modified mRNA, germinal centers are far more persistent; also, in humans even the seasonal flu vaccine **

      (3) The gating strategies for germinal centers and memory B cells in Supplemental Figure 2 are problematic, especially given that these data are used to claim only modest and/or statistically insignificant differences in these populations when Gls and Mpc2 are ablated. Neither strategy shows distinct flow cytometric populations, and it does not seem that the quantification focuses on antigen-specific cells.

      We will enhance these aspects of the presentation, using old and hopefully new data, but note for readers that many many other papers in the best journals show plots in which the separation of, say, GC-Tfh from overall Tfh is based on cut-off within what essentially is a continuous spectrum of emission as adjusted or compensated by the cytometer (spectral or conventional).

      Perhaps incorrectly, we omitted presenting data that included the results with NP-APC-staining - in part because within the GC B cell gate the frequencies of NP-binding events (GCB cells) were similar in double-knockout samples and controls. In practice, that would mean that the metabolic requirement applied about equally to NP+ and the total population. We will try to rectify this point in the revision.

      (4) Along these lines, the conclusions in Figure 6a-d may need to be tempered if the analysis was done on polyclonal, rather than antigen-specific cells. Alum induces a heavily type 2-biased response and is not known to induce much of an interferon signature. The authors' observations might be explained by the inclusion of other ongoing GCs unrelated to the immunization. 

      We will make sure the text is clear that the in vitro experiments do not represent GC B cells and that the RNA-seq data were not an Ag (SRBC)-specific subset.

      We also will try to work in a schematic along with expanding the Legends to make it more readily clear that the RNA-seq data (and hence the GSEA) involved immunizations with SRBC (not the alum / NP system which - it may be noted - in these experiments actually generated a robust IgG2c (type 1-driven) response along with the type 2-enhanced IgG1 response.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In their manuscript, the authors investigate how glutaminolysis (GLS) and mitochondrial pyruvate import (MPC2) jointly shape B cell fate and the humoral immune response. Using inducible knockout systems and metabolic inhibitors, they uncover a "synthetic auxotrophy": When GLS activity/glutaminolysis is lost together with either GLUT1-mediated glucose uptake or MPC2, B cells fail to upregulate mitochondrial respiration, IL 21/STAT3 and IFN/STAT1 signaling is impaired, and the plasma cell output and antigen-specific antibody titers drop significantly. This work thus demonstrates the promotion of plasma cell differentiation and cytokine signaling through parallel activation of two metabolic pathways. The dataset is technically comprehensive and conceptually novel, but some aspects leave the in vivo and translational significance uncertain.

      Strengths:

      (1) Conceptual novelty: the study goes beyond single-enzyme deletions to reveal conditional metabolic vulnerabilities and fate-deciding mechanisms in B cells.

      (2) Mechanistic depth: the study uncovers a novel "metabolic bottleneck" that impairs mitochondrial respiration and elevates ROS, and directly ties these changes to cytokine-receptor signaling. This is both mechanistically compelling and potentially clinically relevant.

      (3) Breadth of models and methods: inducible genetics, pharmacology, metabolomics, seahorse assay, ELISpot/ELISA, RNA-seq, two immunization models.

      (4) Potential clinical angle: the synergy of CB839 with UK5099 and/or hydroxychloroquine hints at a druggable pathway targeting autoantibody-driven diseases.

      We agree and thank the referee for the positive comments and this succinct summary of what we view as contributions of the paper.

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) Physiological relevance of "synthetic auxotrophy"

      The manuscript demonstrates that GLS loss is only crippling when glucose influx or mitochondrial pyruvate import is concurrently reduced, which the authors name "synthetic auxotrophy". I think it would help readers to clarify the terminology more and add a concise definition of "synthetic auxotrophy" versus "synthetic lethality" early in the manuscript and justify its relevance for B cells.

      We will edit the Abstract, Introduction, and Discussion to try to do better on this score. Conscious of how expansive the prose and data are even in the original submission, we appear to have taken some shortcuts that we will try to rectify. Thank you for highlighting this need to improve on a key concept!

      That said, we punctiliously & perhaps pedantically encourage readers to be completely accurate, in that under one condition of immunization GLS loss substantially reduced the anti-ovalbumin response (Fig. 1, Fig. 2a-c). And for this provisional response, we will expand a bit on the notion that synthetic auxotrophy represents effects on differentiation that appear to go beyond and not simply to be selective death, even though decreased population expansion is observed and one cannot exclude some contribution of enhanced death in vivo. Finally, we will note that this comment of the review raises interesting semantic questions about what represents "physiological relevance" but leave it at that.

      While the overall findings, especially the subset specificity and the clinical implications, are generally interesting, the "synthetic auxotrophy" condition feels a little engineered.

      One can readily say that CAR-T cells are 'a little engineered' so it is a matter of balancing this perspective of the referee against the strengths they highlight in points 1, 2, and 4. In any case, we will probably try to expand and be more explicit in the Discussion of the revised manuscript.

      In brief, even were the money not all gone, we would not believe that expanding the heft of this already rather large manuscript and set of data would be appropriate. As matters stand, a basic new insight about metabolic flexibility and its limits leads to evidence of a way to reduce generation of Ab and a novel impairment of STAT transcription factor induction by several cytokine receptors. The vulnerability that could be tested in later work on B cell-dependent autoimmunity includes the capacity to test a compound that already has been to or through FDA phase II in patients together with an FDA-approved standard-of-care agent.

      Put a different way, the point is that a basic curiosity to understand why decreasing glucose influx did not have an even more profound effect than what was observed, combined with curiosity as to why glutaminolysis was dispensable in relatively standard vaccine-like models of immunize / boost, provided a springboard to identification of new vulnerabilities. As above, we appreciate being made aware that this point merits being made more explicit in the Discussion of the edited version.

      Therefore, the findings strongly raise the question of the likelihood of such a "double hit" in vivo and whether there are conditions, disease states, or drug regimens that would realistically generate such a "bottleneck".

      Hence, the authors should document or at least discuss whether GC or inflamed niches naturally show simultaneous downregulation/lack of glutamine and/or pyruvate. The authors should also aim to provide evidence that infections (e.g., influenza), hypoxia, treatments (e.g., rapamycin), or inflammatory diseases like lupus co-limit these pathways. 

      Again, we appreciate some 'licensing' to be more expansive and explicit, and will try to balance editing in such points against undue tedium or tendentiously speculative length in the Discussion. In particular, we will note that a clear, simple implication of the work is to highlight an imperative to test CB839 in lupus patients already on hydroxychloroquine as standard-of-care, and to suggest development of UK5099 (already tested many times in mouse models of cancer) to complement glutaminase inhibition. 

      As backdrop, we note that the failure to advance imaging mass spectrometry to the capacity to quantify relative or absolute (via nano-DESI) concentrations of nutrients in localized interstitia is a critical gap in the entire field. Techniques that sample the interstitial fluid of tumor masses or in our case LN as a work-around have yielded evidence that there can be meaningful limitations of glucose and glutamine, but it needs to be acknowledged that such findings may be very model-specific and, as can be the case with cutting-edge science, are not without controversy. That said, yes, we had found that hypoxia reduced glutamine uptake but given the norms of focused, tidy packages only reported on leucine in an earlier paper [PMID27501247; PMCID5161594].

      It would hence also be beneficial to test the CB839 + UK5099/HCQ combinations in a short, proof-of-concept treatment in vivo, e.g., shortly before and after the booster immunization or in an autoimmune model. Likewise, it may also be insightful to discuss potential effects of existing treatments (especially CB839, HCQ) on human memory B cell or PC pools.

      We certainly agree that the suggestions offered in this comment are important next steps and the right approach to test if the findings reported here translate toward the treatment of autoimmune diseases that involve B cells, interferons, and pathophysiology mediated by auto-Ab. As practical points, performance and replication of such studies would take more time than the year allotted for return of a revised manuscript to eLife and in any case neither funds nor a lab remain to do these important studies. 

      Concrete evidence for our concurrence was embodied in a grant application to NIH that was essential for keeping a lab and doing any such studies. [We note, as a suggestion to others, that an essential component of such studies would be to test the effects of these compounds on B cells from patients and mice with autoimmunity]. Perhaps unfortunately for SLE patients, the review panelists did not agree about the importance of such studies. However, it can be hoped that the patent-holder of CB839 (and perhaps other companies developing glutaminase inhibitors) will see this peer-reviewed pre-print and the public dialogue, and recognize how positive results might open a valuable contribution to mitigation of diseases such as SLE.

      (2) Cell survival versus differentiation phenotype

      Claims that the phenotypes (e.g., reduced PC numbers) are "independent of death" and are not merely the result of artificial cell stress would benefit from Annexin-V/active-caspase 3 analyses of GC B cells and plasmablasts. Please also show viability curves for inhibitor-treated cell

      This comment leads us to see that the wording on this point may have been overly terse in the interests of brevity, and thereby open to some misunderstanding. Accordingly, we will expand out the text of the Abstract and elsewhere in the manuscript, to be more clear. In addition, we will add in some data on the point, hopefully including some results of new experiments.

      To clarify in this public context, it is not that an increase in death (along with the reported decrease in cell cycling) can be or is excluded - and in fact it likely exists in vitro. The point is that beyond any such increase, and taking into account division number (since there is evidence that PC differentiation and output numbers involve a 'division-counting' mechanism), the frequencies of CD138+ cells and of ASCs among the viable cells are lower, as is the level of Prdm1-encoded mRNA even before the big increase in CD138+ cells in the population. 

      (3) Subset specificity of the metabolic phenotype

      Could the metabolic differences, mitochondrial ROS, and membrane-potential changes shown for activated pan-B cells (Figure 5) also be demonstrated ex vivo for KO mouse-derived GC B cells and plasma cells? This would also be insightful to investigate following NP-immunization (e.g., NP+ GC B cells 10 days after NP-OVA immunization).

      We agree that such data could be nice and add to the comprehensiveness of the work. We will try to scrounge the resources (time; money; human) to test this roughly as indicated. That said, we would note that the frequencies and hence numbers of NP+ GC B cells are so low that even in the flow cytometer we suspect there will not be enough "events" to rely on the results with DCFDA in the tiny sub-sub-subset. It also bears noting that reliable flow cytometric identification of the small NP-specific plasmablast/plasma cell subset amidst the overall population, little of which arose from immunization or after deletion of the floxed segments in B cells, would potentially be misleading.

      (4) Memory B cell gating strategy

      I am not fully convinced that the memory-B-cell gate in Supplementary Figure 2d is appropriate. The legend implies the population is defined simply as CD19+GL7-CD38+ (or CD19+CD38++?), with no further restriction to NP-binding cells. Such a gate could also capture naïve or recently activated B cells. From the descriptions in the figure and the figure legend, it is hard to verify that the events plotted truly represent memory B cells. Please clarify the full gating hierarchy and, ideally, restrict the MBC gate to NP+CD19+GL7-CD38+ B cells (or add additional markers such as CD80 and CD273). Generally, the manuscript would benefit from a more transparent presentation of gating strategies.

      We will further expand the supplemental data displays to include more of the gating and analytic scheme, and hope to be able to have performed new experiments and analyses (including additional markers) that could mitigate the concern noted here. In addition, we will include flow data from the non-immunized control mice that had been analyzed concurrently in the experiments illustrated in this Figure.

      Although it should be noted that the labeling indicated that the gating included the important criterion that cells be IgD- (Supplemental Fig. 2b), which excludes the vast majority of naive B cells, in principle marginal zone (MZ) B cells might fall within this gate. However, the MZ B population is unlikely to explain the differences shown in Supplemental Fig. 2b-d.

      (5) Deletion efficiency - [The] mRNA data show residual GLS/MPC2 transcripts (Supplementary Figure 8). Please quantify deletion efficiency in GC B cells and plasmablasts.

      Even were there resources to do this, the degree of reduction in target mRNA (Gls; Mpc2) renders this question superfluous.

      Are there likely to be some cells with only one, or even neither, allele converted from fl to D? Yes, but they would be a minor subset in light of the magnitude of mRNA reduction, in contrast to our published observations with Slc2a1. As to plasmablasts and plasma cells, the pre-existing populations make such an analysis misleading, while the scarcity of such cells recoverable with antigen capture techniques is so low as to make both RNA and genomic DNA analyses questionable.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      eLife Assessment

      This valuable study revisits the effects of substitution model selection on phylogenetics by comparing reversible and non-reversible DNA substitution models. The authors provide evidence that 1) non time-reversible models sometimes perform better than general time-reversible models when inferring phylogenetic trees out of simulated viral genome sequence data sets, and that 2) non time-reversible models can fit the real data better than the reversible substitution models commonly used in phylogenetics, a finding consistent with previous work. However, the methods are incomplete in supporting the main conclusion of the manuscript, that is that non time-reversible models should be incorporated in the model selection process for these data sets.

      The non-reversible models should be incorporated in the selection model process not because the significantly perform better but only because the do not perform worse than the reversible models and that true biochemical processes of nucleotide substitution does support the science of non-reversibility.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      The study by Sianga-Mete et al revisits the effects of substitution model selection on phylogenetics by comparing reversible and non-reversible DNA substitution models. This topic is not new, previous works already showed that non-reversible, and also covarion, substitution models can fit the real data better than the reversible substitution models commonly used in phylogenetics. In this regard, the results of the present study are not surprising. Specific comments are shown below.

      True

      It is well known that non-reversible models can fit the real data better than the commonly used reversible substitution models, see for example,

      https://academic.oup.com/sysbio/article/71/5/1110/6525257

      https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jeb.14147?af=R

      The manuscript indicates that the results (better fitting of non-reversible models compared to reversible models) are surprising but I do not think so, I think the results would be surprising if the reversible models provide a better fitting.

      I think the introduction of the manuscript should be increased with more information about non-reversible models and the diverse previous studies that already evaluated them. Also I think the manuscript should indicate that the results are not surprising, or more clearly justify why they are surprising.

      The surprise in the findings is in NREV12 performing better than NREV6 for double stranded DNA viruses as it was expected that NREV6 would perform better given the biochemical processes discussed in the introduction.

      In the introduction and/or discussion I missed a discussion about the recent works on the influence of substitution model selection on phylogenetic tree reconstruction. Some works indicated that substitution model selection is not necessary for phylogenetic tree reconstruction,

      https://academic.oup.com/mbe/article/37/7/2110/5810088

      https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-019-08822-w

      https://academic.oup.com/mbe/article/35/9/2307/5040133

      While others indicated that substitution model selection is recommended for phylogenetic tree reconstruction,

      https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0378111923001774

      https://academic.oup.com/sysbio/article/53/2/278/1690801

      https://academic.oup.com/mbe/article/33/1/255/2579471

      The results of the present study seem to support this second view. I think this study could be improved by providing a discussion about this aspect, including the specific contribution of this study to that.

      In our conclusion we have stated that:

      The lack of available data regarding the proportions of viral life cycles during which genomes exist in single and double stranded states makes it difficult to rationally predict the situations where the use of models such as GTR, NREV6 and NREV12 might be most justified: particularly in light of the poor over-all performance of NREV6 and GTR relative to NREV12 with respect to describing mutational processes in viral genome sequence datasets. We therefore recommend case-by-case assessments of NREV12 vs NREV6 vs GTR model fit when deciding whether it is appropriate to consider the application of non-reversible models for phylogenetic inference and/or phylogenetic model-based analyses such as those intended to test for evidence of natural section or the existence of molecular clocks.

      The real data was downloaded from Los Alamos HIV database. I am wondering if there were any criterion for selecting the sequences or if just all the sequences of the database for every studied virus category were analysed. Also, was any quality filter applied? How gaps and ambiguous nucleotides were considered? Notice that these aspects could affect the fitting of the models with the data.

      We selected varying number of sequences of the database for every studied virus type. Using the software aliview we did quality filter by re-aligning the sequences per virus type.

      How the non-reversible model and the data are compared considering the non-reversible substitution process? In particular, given an input MSA, how to know if the nucleotide substitution goes from state x to state y or from state y to state x in the real data if there is not a reference (i.e., wild type) sequence? All the sequences are mutants and one may not have a reference to identify the direction of the mutation, which is required for the non-reversible model. Maybe one could consider that the most abundant state is the wild type state but that may not be the case in reality. I think this is a main problem for the practical application of non-reversible substitution models in phylogenetics.

      True

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The reversible and non-reversible models used in this study assume that all the sites evolve under the same substitution matrix, which can be unrealistic. This aspect could be mentioned.

      Done

      The manuscript indicates that "a phylogenetic tree was inferred from an alignment of real sequences (Avian Leukosis virus) with an average sequence identity (API) of ~90%.". I was wondering under which substitution model that phylogenetic tree reconstruction was performed? could the use of that model bias posterior results in terms of favoring results based on such a model?

      We have stated that the GTR+G model was used to reconstruct the tree. The use of the GTR+G model could yes bias the posterior results as we have stated in the paper too.

      I was wondering which specific R function was used to calculate the weighted Robinson-Foulds metric. I think this should be included in the manuscript.

      We stated that We used the weighted Robinson-Foulds metric (wRF; implemented in the R phangorn package (Schliep, 2011)⁠)

      Despite a minority, several datasets fitted better with a reversible model than with a non-reversible model. I think that should be clearly indicated. In addition, in my opinion the AIC does not enough penalizes the number of parameters of the models and favors the non-reversible models over the reversible models, but this is only my opinion based on the definition of AIC and it is not supported. Thus, I think the comparison between phylogenetic trees reconstructed under different substitution models was a good idea (but see also my second major comment).

      Noted

      When comparing phylogenetic trees I was wondering if one should consider the effect of the estimation method and quality of the studied data? For example, should bootstrap values be estimated for all the ancestral nodes and only ancestral nodes with high support be evaluated in the comparison among trees?

      Yes the estimation method and quality of the studied data should be considered. When using RF unlike wRF this will not matter but for weighted RF it does. When building the trees, using RaxML only high support nodes are added to the tree.

      In Figure 3, I do not see (by eye) significant differences among the models. I see in the legend that the statistical evaluation was based on a t test but I am not much convinced. Maybe it is only my view. Exactly, which pairs of datasets are evaluated with the t test? Next, I would expect that the influence of the substitution model on the phylogenetic tree reconstruction is higher at large levels of nucleotide diversity because with more substitution events there is more information to see the effects of the model. However, the t test seems to show that differences are only at low levels of nucleotide diversity (and large DNR), what could be the cause of this?

      The paired T-tests compares the wRF distances of the inferred tree real tree and the trees simulated using the GTR model verses the wRF distances of the inferred true tree from the trees simulated using the NREV12 model.

      The reason why the influence of the NREV12 model on the tree reconstructed is not significantly higher at large levels of nucleotide diversity could be because at a certain level the DNR are simply unrealistic.

      Can the user perform substitution model selection (i.e., AIC) among reversible and non-reversible substitution models with IQTREE? If yes, then doing that should be the recommendation from this study, correct?

      But, can DNR be estimated from a real dataset? DNR seems to be the key factor (Figure 3) for the phylogenetic analysis under a proper model.

      Substitution model selection can be performed among reversible and non-reversible using both HyPhy and IQTREE. And we have recommended that model tests should be done as a first step before tree building. Estimating DNR from real datasets requires a substation rate matrix of a non-reversible.

      The manuscript has many text errors (including typos and incorrect citations). For example, many citations in page 20 show "Error! Reference source not found.". I think authors should double check the manuscript before submitting. Also, some text is not formally written. For example, "G represents gamma-distributed rates", rates of what? The text should be clear for readers that are not familiar with the topic (i.e., G represents gamma-distributed substitution rates among sites). In general, I recommend a detailed revision of the whole text of the manuscript.

      Done

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The authors evaluate whether non time reversible models fit better data presenting strand-specific substitution biases than time reversible models. Specifically, the authors consider what they call NREV6 and NREV12 as candidate non time-reversible models. On the one hand, they show that AIC tends to select NREV12 more often than GTR on real virus data sets. On the other hand, they show using simulated data that NREV12 leads to inferred trees that are closer to the true generating tree when the data incorporates a certain degree of non time-reversibility.

      Based on these two experimental results, the authors conclude that "We show that non-reversible models such as NREV12 should be evaluated during the model selection phase of phylogenetic analyses involving viral genomic sequences". This is a valuable finding, and I agree that this is potentially good practice.

      However, I miss an experiment that links the two findings to support the conclusion: in particular, an experiment that solves the following question: does the best-fit model also lead to better tree topologies?

      By NREV12 leading to inferred trees that are closer to the true generating tree as compared to GTR, it then shows that the best-fit model in this case being NREV12 leads to better tree topologies.

      On simulated data, the significance of the difference between GTR and NREV12 inferences is evaluated using a paired t test. I miss a rationale or a reference to support that a paired t test is suitable to measure the significance of the differences of the wRF distance. Also, the results show that on average NREV12 performs better than GTR, but a pairwise comparison would be more informative: for how many sequence alignments does NREV12 perform better than GTR?

      We have used the popular paired t-test as it is the most widely used when comparing means values between two matched samples where the difference of each mean pair is normally distributed. And the wRF distances do match the guidelines above.

      The paired t-test contains the pairwise comparison and the boxplots side by side show the pairwise wRF comparisions.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors reference Baele et al., 2010 for describing NREV6 and NREV12. I suggest using the same name used in the referenced paper: GNR-SYM and GNR respectively. Although I do not think there is a standard name for these models, I would use a previously used one.

      We have built studies based on the names NREV6 and NREV12. We would like to keep the naming as standard for our studies.

      GTR and NREV12 models are already described in many other papers. I do not see the need to include such an extensive description. Also, a reference should be included to the discrete Gamma rate categories [1]

      We included the extensive description to enable other readers who are not super familiar with these models better understanding since we have given the models our own naming different from those used in other papers.

      We have added referencing for the discrete gamma rate as recommended. (Yang, 1994)

      To evaluate the exhaustiveness and correctness of the results, I would recommend publishing as supplementary material the simulated data sets or the scripts for generating the data set, the scripts or command lines for the analysis, and the versions of the software used (e.g., IQTREE). Also, to strongly support the main conclusion of the manuscript, I suggest adding to the simulations section results the RF-distances of the best-fit selected model under AIC, AICc, and BIC as well.

      We can go ahead and submit all the needed datasets. The simulated data RF-Distances results are available and will be submitted. We cannot however add them to the main document as this will create very long data tables.

      In some instances, it is mentioned that the selection criterion used is AIC, while in others, AIC-c is referenced. Even in the table captions, both terms are mixed. It should be made clearer which criterion is being employed, as AIC is not suitable for addressing the overparameterization of evolutionary models, given that it does not account for the sample size. A previous pre-print of this article [2] does not mention AIC-c, but also explicitly includes the formulas for AIC that do not take the sample size into account, and reports the same results as this manuscript, what indicates that AIC and not AIC-c was used here. This should be clarified. It is recommended to use AIC-c instead of AIC, especially if the sample size to model parameters ratio is low [3]. Two things may be appointed here: some authors consider tree branch lengths as model free parameters and others do not. In this paper it is not specified how the model parameters are counted. AIC tends to select more parameterized models than AIC-c, and overparameterization can lead to different tree inferences, as evidenced in Hoff et al., 2016. Therefore, it is expected that NREV12 is more frequently selected than NREV6 and GTR.

      In my opinion, a pairwise comparison between GTR and NREV12 performance is of great interest here, and the whiskers plots are not useful. Scatterplots would display the results better.

      Boxplots are meant to offer a simplified view of the results as the paired t-tests does all of the comparisons. We shall provide the scatter plots as supplementary information so that readers can get full detailed plots as recommended.

      Some references are missing.

      Missing references added

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review): 

      In this manuscript, Gruber et al perform serial EM sections of the antennal lobe and reconstruct the neurites innervating two types of glomeruli one that is narrowly tuned to geosmin and one that is broadly tuned to other odours. They quantify and describe various aspects of the innervations of olfactory sensory neurons (OSNs), uniglomerlular projection neurons (uPNs), and the multiglomerular Local interneurons (LNs) and PNs (mPNs). They find that narrowly tuned glomeruli had stronger connectivity from OSNs to PNs and LNs, and considerably more connections between sister OSNs and sister PNs than the broadly tuned glomeruli. They also had less connectivity with the contralateral glomeruli. These observations are suggestive of strong feed-forward information flow with minimal presynaptic inhibition in narrowly tuned glomeruli, which might be ecologically relevant, for example, while making quick decisions such as avoiding a geosmin-laden landing site. In contrast, information flow in more broadly tuned glomeruli show much more lateralisation of connectivity to the contralateral glomerulus, as well as to other ipsilateral glomeruli. 

      The data are well presented, the manuscript clearly written, and the results will be useful to the olfaction community. I wonder, given the hemibrain and FAFB datasets exist, whether the authors have considered verifying whether the trends they observe in connectivity hold across three brains? Is it stereotypic? 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s positive view of our study and their thoughtful and relevant comment on the issue of individual variation. We agree in that this is a very important question and notice that it was also asked for by the second Reviewer. It reflects both our limited understanding of the range of individual variation in synaptic connectivity—whether in flies, humans, or other species—and the challenge of determining which of the differences observed in our study are stereotypical features of each glomerulus type. Undoubtedly this criticism addresses a crucial problem of practically all connectome studies so far and for which there is no immediate solution. This type of studies requires so much time, efforts and money that increasing the number of samples is seldom feasible. The Reviewer wonders if we could compare our data with that made available by two of the largest connectome studies of Drosophila. This appeared to us to be a very good idea and we have tried to follow the advice but, unfortunately, it was impracticable because of the reasons we explain below. The hemibrain data cannot be used for this purpose because it does not contain the full glomerulus DA2 (Schlegel et al., 2021). A different problem hindered us from using the FAFB dataset, the other dataset mentioned by the Reviewer. In this case the three glomeruli were sectioned and reconstructed but the dataset lacks an annotated list of all synaptic connections corresponding to each glomerulus. Such annotation (a compendium of all synaptic connections inside each glomerulus informing for each connection which type of neuron provides the presynaptic site and which the postsynaptic site) is essential for direct comparison with our data. It is important to keep in mind that the current analytical tools available for the use of these datasets (e.g., NeuPrint, FlyWire and CATMAID) do not offer the ability to extract data on synapses exclusively from the glomerular volume of DA2 or DL5. In this case, it certainly is theoretically possible to obtain the data by doing ourselves the annotation. However, such a study will demand so much time, efforts and financial resources, which we believe would not be justified solely to increase the number of individuals from one to two. Instead, our manuscript includes a comparison of the OSN connectivity in VA1v and DL5 using the hemibrain dataset published by Schlegel et al. (2021) (see revised manuscript: lines 311–315; 431–434; 558–562; 602–606).

      Beyond the opinion, that we share in full with the Reviewer, that a comparison including three flies will be better than a comparison made with one glomerulus of each type we are still challenged by the question of which -if any- of the differences are stereotypic. The clarification of what are stereotypical differences between particular glomeruli in features as those discussed in our study and what is simply differences within the normal range of individual variation is basically a statistical problem. A first attempt at a comprehensive comparison focusing on intra- and inter-individual variability was recently made by comparing two connectome datasets from two different Drosophila individuals (Dorkenwald et al., 2024; Schlegel et al., 2024). At present, it is still unclear how many samples are needed to make a statistically robust comparison of olfactory synaptic circuits in adult flies—perhaps 3, 6, or even 18 individuals?  

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The chemoreceptor proteins expressed by olfactory sensory neurons differ in their selectivity such that glomeruli vary in the breadth of volatile chemicals to which they respond. Prior work assessing the relationship between tuning breadth and the demographics of principal neuron types that innervate a glomerulus demonstrated that narrowly tuned glomeruli are innervated more projection neurons (output neurons) and fewer local interneurons relative to more broadly tuned glomeruli. The present study used high-resolution electron microscopy to determine which synaptic relationships between principal cell types also vary with glomerulus tuning breadth using a narrowly tuned glomerulus (DA2) and a broadly tuned glomerulus (DL5). The strength of this study lies in the comprehensive, synapse-level resolution of the approach. Furthermore, the authors implement a very elegant approach of using a 2-photon microscope to score the upper and lower bounds of each glomerulus, thus defining the bounds of their restricted regions of interest. There were several interesting differences including greater axo-axonic afferent synapses and dendrodentric output neuron synapses in the narrowly tuned glomerulus, and greater synapses upon sensory afferents from multiglomerular neurons and output neuron autapses in the broadly tuned glomerulus.     The study is limited by a few factors. There was a technical need to group all local interneurons, centrifugal neurons, and multiglomerular projection neurons into one category ("multiglomerular neurons") which complicates any interpretations as even multiglomerular projection neurons are very diverse. Additionally, there were as many differences between the two narrowly tuned glomeruli as there were comparing the narrowly and broadly tuned glomeruli. Architecture differences may therefore not reflect differences in tuning breadth, but rather the ecological significance of the odors detected by cognate sensory afferents. Finally, some synaptic relationships are described as differing and others as being the same between glomeruli, but with only one sample from each glomerulus, it is difficult to determine when measures differ when there is no measure of inter-animal variability. If these caveats are kept in mind, this work reveals some very interesting potential differences in circuit architecture associated with glomerular tuning breadth.

      This work establishes specific hypotheses about network function within the olfactory system that can be pursued using targeted physiological approaches. It also identifies key traits that can be explored using other high-resolution EM datasets and other glomeruli that vary in their tuning selectivity. Finally, the laser "branding" technique used in this study establishes a reduced-cost procedure for obtaining smaller EM datasets from targeted volumes of interest by leveraging the ability to transgenically label brain regions in Drosophila.

      CLASSIFICATION OF NEURONAL TYPES

      We agree that grouping diverse types of interneurons into a single category (referred to as MGNs) limits the ability to make interpretations about synaptic similarities and differences between specific neuronal types. This was, however, an unavoidable compromise resulting from our decision to generate a comprehensive, synapse-level reconstruction of the restricted regions encompassing the DA2 and DL5 glomeruli. As both reviewers have noted, this approach offers significant value and we hope the Editor will also recognize that this limitation does not prevent readers from gaining important and novel insights into the synaptic circuitry of these two glomeruli.  

      Similar to the approach taken by Tobin at al. (2017) we prioritized producing a densely reconstructed neuropile, in which no synapses were omitted (Tobin et al., 2017). The downside of this method is that not all synaptic connections could be reliably assigned to specific neuronal types, with about 12% remaining unassigned." We anticipate that future research, supported by advances in semi-automated tracing methods, improved imaging technologies, and increased personnel resources, will allow not only for the generation of more complete connectomes of the entire brain (Scheffer et al., 2020; Zheng et al., 2018), but also, for the accurate reconstruction and classification of individual synapses—even in highly complex regions such as the olfactory glomeruli. We also expect that a second complete connectome of a male Drosophila will soon become available, which will provide valuable opportunities for comparisons across individuals and between male and female brains in future studies.

      INTERGLOMERULAR DIFFERENCES

      Thank you for this insightful comment. It is indeed true that despite both DA2 and VA1v being narrowly tuned glomeruli, they exhibit considerable differences in specific connectivity features (e.g., relative synaptic strengths above certain thresholds) and that those differences can be as pronounced as those observed between DA2 and the broadly tuned DL5. For this reason, comparing each individual glomerulus to every other is not a practical or informative approach. To derive robust interpretations, we focused instead on whether two glomeruli that share a particular functional characteristic—namely, being narrowly tuned for single odorants—also share connectivity patterns that distinguish them from a broadly tuned reference glomerulus.

      Our results support this. Furthermore, additional connectomics data reinforce our conclusions.

      For example, OSN-OSN connectivity is stronger in the two narrowly tuned glomeruli (DA2 and VA1v) relative to the broadly tuned glomerulus (DL5). While these pairwise differences alone are not conclusive, the finding that the two narrowly tuned glomeruli studied here share features that distinguish them from the broadly tuned glomerulus supports our interpretation. We found further support for this idea in the data reported by Schlegel et al. (2021) further. In that dataset, other narrowly tuned glomeruli (DA1, DL3, and DL4) also exhibit stronger OSNOSN connectivity than other broadly tuned glomeruli (DM1 or DM4).

      We do not deny that there are many differences between any given pair of glomeruli, regardless of whether they are narrowly or broadly tunned. Instead, we propose that our findings on circuit features indicate that most of the observed differences actually grouped the two narrowly tuned glomeruli together relative to the broadly tuned glomerulus. A more concise summary is now provided in the newly added Figure 8. We also added explanatory lines of text in the beginning of the chapter ‘specific features of narrowly tuned glomerular circuits. 

      ECOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE

      This is an interesting point. However, it is difficult to disentangle the "ecological significance" of processed odorants from the "tuning breadth" of a glomerulus. In the Drosophila olfactory system, glomerular circuits that respond to ecologically important odorants—such as those involved in reproduction or danger—tend to be more narrowly tuned. Moreover, while we refer to odorants with specific ecological significance as those linked to survival or reproductive behaviors, defining the significance of an odorant with precision is inherently challenging, as it can vary depending on context and environmental conditions.

      What both circuits share is their narrow tuning breadth. We therefore propose that the common circuit features of VA1v and DA2, highlighted in this study, are functionally related to the fact that each circuit processes single odorants. Consequently, their specificity is most likely determined at the level of the receptor. 

      INDIVIDUAL VARIABILITY

      We agree that accounting for inter-animal variability would strengthen the study. However, we are confident that even a modest statistically sound assessment of this variability would require a larger sample size, certainly more than just two or three flies, which is presently not feasible.

      We refer the reviewer to our response to Reviewer #1 regarding this important issue.

      Initial insights into variability between flies have been provided through comparative analyses of the two most comprehensive female Drosophila melanogaster connectomes—the FAFB and hemibrain datasets (Schlegel et al., 2024). For more detailed quantitative comparisons regarding inter-animal variability, please refer to our response to the second major point raised by Reviewer #2. As highlighted by Schlegel et al. (2024), making definitive statements about the stereotypy of neuron numbers, unitary cell-cell connections (edges), or synaptic strengths (weights) remains a complex challenge."

      While appreciating the rigour of this work we were surprised to notice the omission of a comparison of their observations with the two other existing datasets. This would not only have addressed the technical limitation of this particular study - the inability to identify specific neuron types due to imaging a small part of the brain - but would also have shed light on inter-animal variability 

      We strongly recommend that the authors do make this comparison - the datasets are currently extremely user friendly and so we don't estimate the replication of their key findings will be too onerous. This will be particularly important to resolve the issue of having to classify all multiglomerular local interneurons and multiglomerular projection neurons - broadly into "MGN. Such a comparison will dramatically strengthen this study that poses very interesting questions, but in its current form, has this striking shortcoming. 

      INDIVIDUAL VARIABILITY AS EXPRESSED HERE:

      Earlier on we were of the same opinion that the Reviewer express here but, unfortunately, it was not possible to follow his advice. As far as it was possible, we have compared some of our results to the values of the two datasets that the Reviewer refers to, but the absence of glomerulus DA2 in one of the datasets and the absence of synapse annotation for all the relevant glomeruli in the other dataset prevented us from making a full comparison. Moreover, believe that the problem of individual variation most probably cannot be solved by increasing the comparison with one or two more flies.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for The Authors): 

      The lines 270 - 282 confused me in the backdrop of Figure 3B. 

      The concern may stem from our inclusion of a comparison between the uPNs of glomerulus DA2 and the single uPN of glomerulus DL5 in the statistical analysis presented in Figure 3. This comparison was included to ensure a comprehensive representation of the data, highlighting the variability across all major cell groups. We have clarified this rationale in the revised manuscript (see lines 274-282).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for The Authors): 

      I commend the authors for taking such a thorough approach to advance an interesting topic in olfaction. The following suggestions are intended to strengthen this study: 

      Major points: 

      A color-blind-friendly palette should be used for all figures. Currently, five of seven figures use red and green, and in particular, Figure 5 will be uninterpretable for red/green color-blind readers. 

      We are thankful for this important comment. We changed the color palette as suggested by the reviewer, and replaced Red with Magenta and changed the figure legend accordingly.

      This level of analysis is extremely resource and time-consuming, so even obtaining this information at this resolution is an impressive achievement. However, this study would be well served by strategically supplementing the analysis of this dataset with information from other publicly available connectomics datasets. For instance, some interpretations are limited because there is information from only a single DL5 and DA2 glomerulus. Any claims in which one glomerulus has more, less, or the same of a metric must be tempered because without replicates, there are no measures of inter-animal variability. As an example, on lines 386-387 the authors state "The relative synaptic strength between MGN>uPN was stronger in DA2 (12%) than DL5 (10%)". It is difficult to assess whether this represents a difference that is outside of the range of inter-animal variability inherent to the olfactory system. Taking select measures from the Hemibrain and FAFB (via FlyWire) datasets could help strengthen these claims. 

      We fully agree with the Reviewer’s opinion that since our data is from one glomerulus of each type “It is difficult to assess whether this represents a difference that is outside of the range of inter-animal variability inherent to the olfactory system.” This is a weakness of practically all connectome studies based on electron microscopy in both Drosophila and other animals We cannot be sure that measurements from the Hemibrain and FAFB datasets could help strengthen our claims, because the magnitude of the range of individual variation is presently not known and most probably solving this problem will require more than one or two more flies. In any case, it is not possible to follow this advice and compare our data with that of the hemibrain because the DA2 was not included in that study. We ask the Reviewer to read our more detailed explanation in our response to Reviewer 1.

      In the particular case commented by the Reviewer above, the relative difference in synaptic strength exceeds 20%. Whether such a difference has functional relevance remains an open question but Schlegel et al. (2024) support our interpretation. They showed that synaptic weights with differences larger than 20% tend to be consistent across individuals, with strong correlations within and between animals (Pearson’s R = 0.97 and R = 0.8; Fig. 4).

      Grouping all local interneurons, centrifugal neurons response and multiglomerular PNs into one category limits the ability to make interpretations about similarities or differences in the synaptic relationships involving MGNs. The authors could get an estimate of the number of multiglomerular PNs in DL5, VA1v, and DA2 from Hemibrain and FlyWire platforms to get a better sense of differences between glomeruli in the MGN category. 

      We agree in that grouping a variety of interneurons into a single category (called MGNs) limits the ability to make interpretations about similarities or differences in the synaptic relationships involving different neurons. This was the unavoidable price to be paid once we decided to register a “comprehensive, synapse-level resolution” map of these two glomeruli. It appears to us that both reviewers have clearly recognized the intrinsic value of this approach and we hope that the Editor will share this opinion. 

      Consistent with the assumptions of Tobin et al., (2017) our hypothesis on LN connectivity differences is based on the fact that they are the most numerous and broadly arborizing neurons of the class that we call multiglomerular neurons in the AL (Chou et al., 2010; Lin et al., 2012; Tanaka et al., 2012). Recent connectome studies confirm this feature across all glomeruli (Bates et al., 2020; Horne et al., 2018; Scheffer et al., 2020; Schlegel et al., 2021; Zheng et al., 2018).  

      In response to the reviewer’s question, we conducted a case-specific reanalysis of the data from Horne (2018), which provides comprehensive connectivity information for the VA1v glomerulus. This allowed us to quantify the proportional contributions of LNs (n = 56) and mPNs (n = 13) to all MGN connections (MGN-MGN, MGN>OSN, MGN>uPN, uPN>MGN, OSN>MGN).

      Our analysis showed that 84% of MGN output originates from LNs. 57% of the input to MGN comes from LNs and 43% from mPNs, largely due to strong OSN>mPN input. Thus, for the filtered MGN connections relevant to distinguishing narrowly from broadly tuned circuits (e.g., MGN>OSN, uPN>MGN; see Fig. 8), LNs are the dominant contributors in VA1v. (These data are not included in the resubmitted manuscript.) This supports our interpretation that the LN are responsible for the majority of MGN connections underlying the observed differences between glomeruli.

      For instance, prior work has reported fewer local interneurons innervating DA2, but in this study there was an unexpected result that there was greater MGN innervation density and synapse # for DA2 relative to DL5 This discrepancy could be due to differences in the number of multiglomerular PNs innervating each glomerulus, which would be obscured when these PNs are combined with local interneurons in the MGN category. 

      "We agree that the greater MGN innervation density in DA2 in our study could reflect a stronger contribution from mPNs. However, innervation density alone does not indicate how many mPNs actually innervate DA2 or DL5. Alternatively, increased innervation and/or synaptic frequency of local interneurons (LNs) could also account for this observation. In our view, neuron number does not necessarily correlate with branching complexity or synaptic density. 

      For example, the dendritic length of the single uPN in glomerulus DL5 is approximately equal to the combined dendritic length of the multiple uPNs of the DA2. Similarly, Tobin et al. (2017) reported that when comparing uPNs in glomerulus DM6 between the left and right brain hemispheres, they found variability in cell number but not in dendritic length. More recently, the FAFB and hemibrain datasets showed a similar pattern in another neuronal type. A substantial variation in cell number was observed for Kenyon cells between the two Drosophila individuals, but this cell type consistently makes and receives, in both individuals, similar presynapses and post-synapses (Schlegel et al., 2024).

      On line 33 the authors cannot claim that DA2-OSNs experience less presynaptic inhibition based on the data in this study. Even without the limitations of the MGN category (described above), presynaptic inhibition depends on more than just the number of synapses, rather it is affected by GABA B receptor expression levels and the second messenger components downstream of this receptor. Physiological experiments are needed to justify this claim, so I recommend adjusting accordingly.

      We agree with the Reviewer and have adjusted the text on line 33 and in the main body of the text by referring to this finding as “presynaptic input”, which is what we have quantified, instead of “less presynaptic inhibition”.

      Figures 5 and 6 seek to distill the wealth of information from this study into broad takehome points for the reader, while still providing a good amount of detail. I think a final more concise graphic summary (similar to the graphical abstract or Figure 6 of Grabe et al 2016) depicting the most critical differences between glomeruli would further clarify the broad findings of this study. 

      We appreciate this comment and we have added a “graphic summary” as the Reviewer proposed. We made a new figure that becomes Figure 8 and summarizes our results and highlights differences between narrowly and broadly tuned glomeruli in a more concise graphical abstract format.

      Minor points: 

      Much of the manuscript provides details about synapse fractions or % synapses for a given synaptic relationship. Please ensure that it is clear which principal cell types are being described, as it can be easy to get lost.  - Should line 284 say "...than DL5 as it has been reported that DA2 is innervated by fewer LNs..."?

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment and we have corrected this sentence that now reads as follows: (see text: beginning at line 290).  

      Taisz et al.  has been published, so the citation should be updated. 

      We have updated the corresponding citation.  

      On line 233, the authors ascribe the small electron-dense vesicles as likely housing sNPF released by MGNs. However, Carlsson et al. (2010) demonstrated that sNPF is released by OSNs, which was further functionally characterized by Root et al. (2011) and Ko et al. (2014). In terms of MGNs that release neuropeptides, Carlsson et al. 2010 demonstrated that local interneurons immunolabel for tachykinin, myoinhibitory peptide, and allatostatin-A, while two extrinsic neurons release SIFamide. In theory, aminergic neurons could also have small electron-dense vesicles, but this can be variable. 

      The Reviewer is completely right in his criticism. The MGN certainly contain neurons that have been reported to contain neuropeptides other than sNPF. We have corrected this sentence and it now reads as follows (page7, line 236): “Interestingly, besides the abundant clear small vesicles..

      On line 636, the Berck and Schlegel studies demonstrated that panglomerular local interneurons synapse upon OSN, but not that they induce presynaptic inhibition (which was demonstrated in the studies cited in the next sentence). I recommend adjusting this sentence.

      We agree and we have corrected the text following the Reviewers advice. It now reads as follows (page 19. Line 663): “We also observed that OSNs received less MGN feedback.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This is a revision of a manuscript previously submitted to Review Commons. The authors have partially addressed my comments, mainly by expanding the introduction and discussion sections. Sandy Schmid, a leading expert on the AP2 adaptor and CME, has been added as a co-corresponding author. The main message of the manuscript remains unchanged. Through overexpression of fluorescently tagged CCDC32, the authors propose that, in addition to its established role in AP2 assembly, CCDC32 also follows AP2 to the plasma membrane and regulates CCP maturation. The manuscript presents some interesting ideas, but there are still concerns regarding data inconsistencies and gaps in the evidence.

      With due respect, we would argue that a role for CCDC32 in AP2 assembly is hardly ‘established’.  Rather a single publication reporting its role as a co-chaperone for AAGAP appeared while our manuscript was under review.  We find some similar and some conflicting results, which are described in our revised manuscript.  However, in combination our two papers clearly show that CCDC32, a previously unrecognized endocytic accessory protein, deserves further study.

      (1) eGFP-CCDC32 was expressed at 5-10 times higher levels than endogenous CCDC32. This high expression can artificially drive CCDC32 to the cell surface via binding to the alpha appendage domain (AD)-an interaction that may not occur under physiological conditions.

      While we acknowledge that overexpression of eGFP-CCDC32 could result in artificially driving it to CCPs, we do not believe this is the case for the following reasons:

      i. The bulk of our studies (Figures 2-4) demonstrate the effects of siRNA knockdown on CCDC32 on CCP early stages of CME, and so it is likely that these functions require the presence of endogenous CCDC32 at nascent CCPs as detected with overexpressed eGFP-CCDC32 by TIRF imaging.

      ii. At these levels of overexpression eGFP-CCDC32 fully rescues the effects of siRNA KD of endogenous CCCDC32 of Tfn uptake and CCP dynamics (Figure 6F,G). If the protein was artificially recruited to the AP2 appendage domain, one would expect it to compete with the recruitment of other EAPS to CCPs and hence exhibit defects in CCP dynamics. Indeed, we see the opposite: CCPs that are positive for eGFP-CCDC32 show normal dynamics and maturation rates, while CCPs lacking eGFP-CCDC32 are short-lived and more likely to be aborted (Figure 1C).

      iii. We have identified two modes of binding of CCDC32 to AP2 adaptors: one is through canonical AP2-AD binding motifs, the second is through an a-helix in CCDC32 that, by modeling, docks only to the open conformation of AP2.  Overexpressed CCDC32 lacking this a-helix is not recruited to CCPs (Fig. 6 D,E), indicating that the canonical AP2 binding motifs are not sufficient to recruit CCDC32 to CCPs, even when overexpressed.

      (2) Which region of CCDC32 mediates alpha AD binding? Strangely, the only mutant tested in this work, Δ78-98, still binds AP2, but shifts to binding only mu and beta. If the authors claim that CCDC32 is recruited to mature AP2 via the alpha AD, then a mutant deficient in alpha AD binding should not bind AP2 at all. Such a mutant is critical for establish the model proposed in this work.

      We understand the reviewer’s confusion and thus devoted a paragraph in the discussion to this issue.  As revealed by AlphaFold 3.0 modeling (Figure S6) binding of CCDC32 to the alpha AD likely occurs via the 2 canonical AP2-AD binding motifs encoded in CCDC32. Given the highly divergent nature of AP2-AD binding motifs, we did not identify these motifs without the AlphaFold 3.0 modeling. While these interactions could be detected by GST-pull downs, they are apparently not of sufficient affinity to recruit CCDC32 to CCPs in cells. In the text, we now describe the a-helix we identified as being essential of CCP recruitment as ‘a’ AP2 binding site on CCDC32 rather than ‘the’ AP2 binding site.  Interestingly, and also discussed, Alphafold 3.0 identifies a highly predicted docking site on a-adaptin that is only accessible in the open, cargo-bound conformation of intact AP2.  This is also consistent with the inability of CCDC32(D78-99) to bind the a:µ2 hemi-complex in cell lysates.

      We agree that further structural studies on CCDC32’s interactions with AP2 and its targeting to CCPs will be of interest for future work.

      (3) The concept of hemicomplexes is introduced abruptly. What is the evidence that such hemicomplexes exist? If CCDC32 binds to hemicomplexes, this must occur in the cytosol, as only mature AP2 tetramers are recruited to the plasma membrane. The authors state that CCDC32 binds the AD of alpha but not beta, so how can the Δ78-98 mutant bind mu and beta?

      We introduced the concept of hemicomplexes based on our unexpected (and now explicitly stated as such) finding that the CCDC32(D78-99) mutant efficiently co-IPs with a b2:µ2 hemicomplex.  As stated, the efficiency of this pulldown suggests that the presumed stable AP2 heterotetramer must indeed exist in equilibrium between the two a:s2 and b2:µ2 hemicomplexes, such that CCDC32(D78-99) can sequester and efficiently co-IP with the b2:µ2 hemicomplex.  A previous study, now cited, had shown that the b2:µ2 hemicomplex could partially rescue null mutations of a in C. elegans (PMID: 23482940).  We do not know how CCDC32 binds to the b2:µ2 hemicomplex and we did not detect these interactions using AlphaFold 3.0. However, these interactions could be indirect and involve the AAGAB chaperone.  It is also likely, based on the results of Wan et al. (PMID: 39145939), that the binding is through the µ2 subunit rather than b2. As mentioned above, and in our Discussion, further studies are needed to define the complex and multi-faceted nature of CCDC32-AP2 interactions.

      (4) The reported ability of CCDC32 to pull down AP2 beta is puzzling. Beta is not found in the CCDC32 interactome in two independent studies using 293 and HCT116 cells (BioPlex). In addition, clathrin is also absent in the interactome of CCDC32, which is difficult to reconcile with a proposed role in CCPs. Can the authors detect CCDC32 binding to clathrin?

      Based on the studies of Wan et al. (PMID: 39145939), it is likely that CCDC32 binds to µ2, rather than to the b2 in the b2:µ2 hemicomplex.  As to clathrin being absent from the CCDC32 pull down, this is as expected since the interactions of clathrin even with AP2 are weak in solution (as shown in Figure 5C, clathrin is not detected in our AP2 pull down) so as not to have spontaneous assembly of clathrin coats in the cytosol. Rather these interactions are strengthened by both the reduction in dimensionality that occurs on the membrane and by avidity of multivalent interactions.  For example, Kirchausen reported that 2 AP2 complexes are required to recruit one clathrin triskelion to the PM.

      (5) Figure 5B appears unusual-is this a chimera?

      Figure 5B shows an internal insertion of the eGFP tag into an unstructured region in the AP2 hinge. As we have previously shown (PMID: 32657003), this construct, unique among other commonly used AP2 tags, is fully functional.  We have rearranged the text in the Figure legend to make this clearer.

      Figure 5C likely reflects a mixture of immature and mature AP2 adaptor complexes.

      This is possible, but mature heterotetramers are by far the dominant species, otherwise the 4 subunits would not be immuno-precipitated at near stoichiometric levels with the a subunit.  Near stoichiometric IP with antibodies to the a-AD have been shown by many others in many cell types. 

      (6) CCDC32 is reduced by about half in siRNA knockdown. Why not use CRISPR to completely eliminate CCDC32 expression?

      Fortuitously, partial knockdown was essential to reveal this second function of CCDC32, as we have emphasized in our Discussion.  Wan et al, used CRISPR to knockout CCDC32 and reveal its essential role as a AAGAB co-chaperone.  In the complete absence of CCDC32 mature AP2 complexes fail to form.  However, under our conditions of partial CCDC32 depletion, the expression of AP2 heterotetramers is unaffected revealing a second function of CCDC32 at early stages of CME.  We expect that the co-chaperone function of CCDC32 is catalytic, while its role in CME is more structural; hence the different concentration dependencies, the former being less sensitive to KD than the latter.  This is one reason that many researchers are turning to CRISPRi for whole genome perturbation studies as many proteins play multiple roles that can be masked in KO studies.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Yang et al. describes CCDC32 as a new clathrin mediated endocytosis (CME) accessory protein. The authors show that CCDC32 binds directly to AP2 via a small alpha helical region and cells depleted for this protein show defective CME. Finally, the authors show that the CCDC32 nonsense mutations found in patients with cardio-facial-neuro-developmental syndrome (CFNDS) disrupt the interaction of this protein to the AP2 complex. The results presented suggest that CCDC32 may act as both a chaperone (as recently published) and a structural component of the AP2 complex.

      Strengths:

      The conclusions presented are generally well supported by experimental data and the authors carefully point out the differences between their results and the results by Wan et al. (PNAS 2024).

      Weaknesses:

      The experiments regarding the role of CCDC32 in CFNDS still require some clarifications to make them clearer to scientists working on this disease. The authors fail to describe that the CCDC32 isoform they use in their studies is different from the one used when CFNDS patient mutations were described. This may create some confusion. Also, the authors did not discuss that the frame-shift mutations in patients may be leading to nonsense mediated decay.

      As requested we have more clearly described our construct with regard to the human mutations and added the possibility of NMD in the context of the human mutations.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In this manuscript, Yang et al. characterize the endocytic accessory protein CCDC32, which has implications in cardio-facio-neuro-developmental syndrome (CFNDS). The authors clearly demonstrate that the protein CCDC32 has a role in the early stages of endocytosis, mainly through the interaction with the major endocytic adaptor protein AP2, and they identify regions taking part in this recognition. Through live cell fluorescence imaging and electron microscopy of endocytic pits, the authors characterize the lifetimes of endocytic sites, the formation rate of endocytic sites and pits and the invagination depth, in addition to transferrin receptor (TfnR) uptake experiments. Binding between CCDC32 and CCDC32 mutants to the AP2 alpha appendage domain is assessed by pull down experiments. While interaction between CCDC32 and the alpha appendage domain of AP2 is clearly described, a discussion of potential association with other AP2 domains would be beneficial to understand the impact of CCDC32 in endocytosis.

      The reviewer is correct. That CCDC32 also interacts with other subunits of AP2, is evident from the findings of Wan et al. and by the fact that the CCDC32(D78-99) mutant efficiently co-IPs with the b2:µ2 hemicomplex.  We expanded our discussion around this point. CCDC32 remains an, as yet, poorly characterized, but we now believe very interesting EAP worth further study.

      Together, these experiments allow deriving a phenotype of CCDC32 knock-down and CCDC32 mutants within endocytosis, which is a very robust system, in which defects are not so easily detected. A mutation of CCDC32, mimicking CFNDS mutations, is also addressed in this study and shown to have endocytic defects.

      In summary, the authors present a strong combination of techniques, assessing the impact of CCDC32 in clathrin mediated endocytosis and its binding to AP2.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The authors must be clear about the differences between the CCDC32 isoform they used in their manuscript and the one used to describe the patient mutations. This could be done, for example, in the methods. This is essential for the capacity of other labs to reproduce, follow up and correctly cite these results.

      We have added this information to the Methods. 

      (2) I believe the authors have misunderstood what nonsense mediated decay is. NMD occurs at the mRNA level and requires a full genome context to occur (introns and exons). The fact that a mutant protein is expressed normally from a construct by no means prove that it does not happen. I believe that adding the possibility of NMD occurring would enrich the discussion.

      Thank you, we have now done more homework and have added this possibility into our discussion of the mutant phenotype.  However, if a robust NMD mechanism resulted in a complete loss of CCDC42 protein, then the essential co-chaperone function reported by Wan et al, would result in complete loss of AP2.  A more detailed characterization of the cellular phenotype of these mutations, including assessing the expression levels of AP2 would be informative.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      - It is not clear what the authors mean by '~30s lifetime cohort' (line 159). They refer to Figure 2H, which shows the % of CCPs. Can the authors explain exactly what kind of tracks they used for this analysis, for example which lifetime variations were accepted? Do they refer to the cohorts in Figure S4? In Figure S4, the most frequent tracks have lifetimes < 20 s (in contrast to what is stated in the main text). Why was this cohort not used?

      The ‘30s cohort’ refers to CCPs with lifetimes between 25-35s which encompasses the most abundant species in control cells and CCDC32 KD cells, as shown by the probability curves in Figure 2H. Given the large number of CCPs analyzed we still have large numbers for our analyses n=5998 and 4418, for control and siRNA treated conditions, respectively.  Figure 2H shows the frequency of CCPs in cells treated with CCDC32 siRNA are shifted to shorter lifetimes. We have clarified this in the text.

      - Figure S1: It is now clear, why the mutant versions of CCDC32 are not detected in this western blot. However, data that show the resistance of these proteins to siCCDC32 is still missing (S1 A is in the absence of siCCSC32 I assume, as the legend suggests). A western blot using an anti-GFP antibody, as the one used in Figure S1, after siRNA knock-known would provide clarity.

      That these constructs all contain the same mutation in the siRNA target sequence gives us confidence that they are indeed resistant to siRNA.

      - Note that the anti-CCDC32 antibody does not detect the eGFP-CCDC32(∆78-98) as well as full-length and is unable to detect eGFP-CCDC32(1-54)'. This phrase should belong to Figure S1 (B), not (A)

      Corrected.

      - The immunoprecipitations of CCDC32 and its mutants with AP2 and its subunits are partially confusing. In Figure 5, the authors show that CCDC32 interacts specifically with the alpha-AD, but not with the beta-AD of AP2. In Figure 6B and C, on the other hand, Co-IPs are shown also with the beta and the mu domain of AP2. This is understandable in the context of the full AP2. However, when interaction with the alpha domain (and sigma) is abolished through mutation of helix 78-98, why would beta and mu still interact, when the beta-AD cannot interact with CCDC32 on its own. Are there interaction sites expected outside the ADs in the beta or mu domains?

      See responses to reviewer 1 above.  This result likely reflects the co-chaperone activity of CCDC32 as reported by Wan et al it likely due to their reported interactions of CCDC32 with the µ2 subnit of b2:µ2 hemicomplexes.

      - Figure S6 D, E and F: How much confidence do the authors have on the AlphaFold predictions? Have the same binding poses been obtained repeatedly by independent predictions?

      We provide, with a color scale, the confidence score for each interaction, which is very high (>90%). Of course, this is still a prediction that will need to be verified by further structural studies as we have stated.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Cook et al. have presented an important study on the transcriptomic and epigenomic signature underlying craniofacial development in marsupials. Given the lack of a dunnart genome, the authors also prepared long and short-read sequence datasets to assemble and annotate a novel genome to allow for the mapping of RNAseq and ChIPseq data against H3K4me3 and H3K27ac, which allowed for the identification of putative promoter and enhancer sites in dunnart. They found that genes proximal to these regulatory loci were enriched for functions related to bone, skin, muscle and embryonic development, highlighting the precocious state of newborn dunnart facial tissue. When compared with mouse, the authors found a much higher proportion of promoter regions aligned between species than for enhancer regions, and subsequent profiling identified regulatory elements conserved across species and are important for mammalian craniofacial development. In contrast, the identification of dunnart-specific enhancers and patterns of RNA expression further confirm the precocious state of muscle development, as well as for sensory system development, in dunnart suggesting that early formation of these features are critical for neonate marsupials likely to assist with detecting and responding to cues that direct the joeys to the mother's teat after birth. This is one of the few epigenomic studies performed in marsupials (of any organ) and the first performed in fat-tailed dunnart (also of any organ). Marsupials are emerging as an important model for studying mammalian development and evolution and the authors have performed a novel and thorough analysis, impressively including the assembly of a new marsupial reference genome that will benefit many future studies.

      Strengths:

      The study provides multiple pieces of evidence supporting the important role enhancer elements play in mammalian phenotypic evolution, namely the finding of a lower proportion of peaks present in both dunnart and mouse for enhancers than for promoters, and dunnart showing more genes uniquely associated with it's active enhancers than any other combination of mouse and dunnart samples, whereas this pattern was less pronounced than for promoter-associated genes. In addition, rigorous parameters were used for the cross-species analyses to identify the conserved regulatory elements and the dunnart-specific enhancers. For example, for the results presented in Figure 1, I agree that it is a little surprising that the average promoter-TSS distance is greater than that for enhancers, but that this could be related to the possible presence of unannotated transcripts between genes. The authors addressed this well by examining the distribution of promoter-TSS distances and using proximal promoters (cluster #1) as high confidence promoters for downstream analyses.

      The genome assembly method was thorough, using two different long read methods (Pacbio and ONT) to generate the long reads for contig and scaffold construction, increasing the quality of the final assembled genome.

      Weaknesses:

      Biological replicates of facial tissue were collected at a single developmental time point of the fat-tailed dunnart within the first postnatal day (P0), and analysed this in the context of similar mouse facial samples from the ENCODE consortium at six developmental time points, where previous work from the authors have shown that the younger mouse samples (E11.5-12.5) approximately corresponds to the dunnart developmental stage (Cook et al. 2021). However, it would be useful to have samples from at least one older dunnart time point, for example, at a developmental stage equivalent to mouse E15.5. This would provide additional insight into the extent of accelerated face development in dunnart relative to mouse, i.e. how long do the regulatory elements that activated early in dunnart remain active for and does their function later influence other aspects of craniofacial development?

      We thank the reviewer for their feedback and agree that the inclusion of multiple postnatal stages in the dunnart would give further valuable insights to the comparative analyses. Unfortunately, we were limited by the pouch young available and prioritized ensuring robust data at a single stage for this study. We hope to expand this work to more stages in future studies.

      The authors refer to the development of the CNS being delayed in marsupials relative to placental mammals, however, evidence shows how development of the dunnart brain (whole brain or cortex) is protracted compared to mouse, by a factor of at least 2 times, rather than delayed per se (Workman et al. 2013; Paolino et al. 2023). In addition, there is evidence that cortical formation and cell birth may begin at approximately the same stage across species equivalent to the neonate period in dunnart (E10.5 in mouse), and that shortly after this at the stage equivalent to mouse E12.5, the dunnart cortex shows signs of advanced neurogenesis followed by a protracted phase of neuronal maturation (Paolino et al. 2023). Therefore, it is possible that marsupial CNS development appears delayed relative to mouse but instead begins at the same stage and then proceeds to develop on a different timing scale.

      The comparison here is not directly between CNS development in placental and marsupials but CNS development relative to development of a subset of structures of the cranial skeleton and musculature (as first proposed by Kathleen Smith 1997). For example, Smith 1997 found that in eutherians, evagination of the telencephalon and appearance of the pigment in the eye occur before the ossification of the premaxilla, maxilla, and dentary. However, in marsupials, evagination of the telencephalon and appearance of the pigment in the eye occur concurrently with condensation of cartilage in the basicranium and the ossification of the premaxilla, maxilla, and dentary. Smith 1997 reports both a delay in the initiation of CNS development in marsupials relative to craniofacial ossification and a protraction of CNS development compared to placental mammals.

      This also highlights the challenges of correlating different staging systems between placentals and marsupials as stages determined as equivalent can change depending on which developmental events are used. The protracted development of the CNS in marsupials (Smith 1997, Workman et al. 2013; Paolino et al. 2023) still supports the hypothesis that during the short gestation period in marsupials structures required for life outside the womb in an embryonic-like state, such as the orofacial region, are likely prioritized.

      We have clarified this based on the reviewers feedback and added text referring to the protraction of marsupial CNS development to the Discussion section.

      [New text]: Marsupials display advanced development of the orofacial region relative to development of the central nervous system when compared to placental mammals[3,6].

      [New text]: Although development of the central nervous system is protracted in marsupials compared to placentals, marsupials have well-developed peripheral motor nerves and sensory nerves (eg. the trigeminal) at birth [5].

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This study by Cook and colleagues utilizes genomic techniques to examine gene regulation in the craniofacial region of the fat-tailed dunnart at perinatal stages. Their goal is to understand how accelerated craniofacial development is achieved in marsupials compared to placental mammals.

      The authors employ state-of-the-art genomic techniques, including ChIP-seq, transcriptomics, and high-quality genome assembly, to explore how accelerated craniofacial development is achieved in marsupials compared to placental mammals. This work addresses an important biological question and contributes a valuable dataset to the field of comparative developmental biology. The study represents a commendable effort to expand our understanding of marsupial development, a group often underrepresented in genomic studies.

      The dunnart's unique biology, characterized by a short gestation and rapid craniofacial development, provides a powerful model for examining developmental timing and gene regulation. The authors successfully identified putative regulatory elements in dunnart facial tissue and linked them to genes involved in key developmental processes such as muscle, skin, bone, and blood formation. Comparative analyses between dunnart and mouse chromatin landscapes suggest intriguing differences in deployment of regulatory elements and gene expression patterns.

      Strengths

      (1) The authors employ a broad range of cutting-edge genomic tools to tackle a challenging model organism. The data generated - particularly ChIP-seq and RNA-seq from craniofacial tissue - are a valuable resource for the community, which can be employed for comparative studies. The use of multiple histone marks in the ChIP-seq experiments also adds to the utility of the datasets.

      (2) Marsupial occupy an important phylogenetic position, but they remain an understudied group. By focusing on the dunnart, this study addresses a significant gap in our understanding of mammalian development and evolution. Obtaining enough biological specimens for these experiments studies was likely a big challenge that the authors were able to overcome.

      (3) The comparison of enhancer landscapes and transcriptomes between dunnarts and can serve as the basis of subsequent studies that will examine the mechanisms of developmental timing shifts. The authors also carried out liftover analyses to identify orthologous enhancers and promoters in mice and dunnart.

      Weaknesses and Recommendations

      (1) The absence of genome browser tracks for ChIP-seq data makes it difficult to assess the quality of the datasets, including peak resolution and signal-to-noise ratios. Including browser tracks would significantly strengthen the paper by provide further support for adequate data quality.

      We have put together an IGV session with the dunnart genome, annotation and ChIP-seq tracks. This is now available in the FigShare data repository (10.7554/eLife.103592.1).

      (2) The first two figures of the paper heavily rely in gene orthology analysis, motif enrichment, etc, to describe the genomic data generated from the dunnart. The main point of these figures is to demonstrate that the authors are capturing the epigenetic signature of the craniofacial region, but this is not clearly supported in the results. The manuscript should directly state what these analyses aim to accomplish - and provide statistical tests that strengthen confidence on the quality of the datasets.

      As this is the first epigenomic profiling for this species we performed extensive data quality control (See Supplementary Tables 2-3, 18, 20-23 and Supplementary Figures 1-3, 6-11). These figures and corresponding Supplementary Tables show the robustness of the data, including well-described metrics for assessing promoters and enhancers, GO terms relevant to craniofacial development and binding motifs for key developmental TF families.

      We have emphasised this aspect of the work more strongly in the results section, particularly in [Defining craniofacial putative enhancer- and promoter regions in the dunnart].

      (3) The observation that "promoters are located on average 106 kb from the nearest TSS" raises significant concerns about the quality of the ChIP-seq data and/or genome annotation. The results and supplemental information suggest a combination of factors, including unannotated transcripts and enhancer-associated H3K4me3 peaks - but this issue is not fully resolved in the manuscript. The authors should confirm that this is not caused by spurious peaks in the CHIP-seq analysis - and possibly improve genome annotation with the transcriptomic datasets presented in the study.

      Spurious ChIP-seq peaks could be possible as there is no “blacklisted regions” database for the dunnart to filter on, however we used a no-IP control, a stringent FDR of 0.01 and peaks had to be reproducible in two biological replicates when calling peaks - all of which should reduce the likelihood of false positives.

      H3K4me3 activity at enhancers is well-established, in particular when enhancer sequences are also bound by RNA Pol II ((Koch and Andrau, 2011; Pekowska et al., 2011). However, compared to H3K4me3 activity at promoters, H3K4me3 levels at enhancers are low (Calo and Wysocka, 2013). This is in line with our observations that H3K4me3 levels at enhancers are much lower than observed at promoter regions (see Supplementary Note 2). We found that H3K4me3 peaks located closer to the TSS had a stronger peak signal (mean = 46.10) than distal H3K4me3 peaks (mean = 6.95; Wilcoxon FDR-adjusted p < 2.2 x 10<sup>-16</sup>). This suggests that although some distal promoter peaks may be due to missingness in the annotation, the majority likely represent peaks associated with enhancer regions. We have emphasized this finding more strongly in the results section:

      [New text]: H3K4me3 activity at enhancers is well-established[25,26], however, compared to H3K4me3 activity at promoters, H3K4me3 levels at enhancers are low[27]. This is in line with our observations where H3K4me3 levels at distal enhancer peaks are nearly 7 times lower than those observed at promoter regions (see SupNote2).

      (4) The comparison of gene regulation between a single dunnart stage (P1) and multiple mouse stages lacks proper benchmarking. Morphological and gene expression comparisons should be integrated to identify equivalent developmental stages. This "alignment" is essential for interpreting observed differences as true heterochrony rather than intrinsic regulatory differences.

      Given the developmental differences between eutherian and marsupial mammals it is challenging to assign the dunnart a precise “equivalent” developmental stage to the mouse. From our morphological and developmental characterisation (see Cook et al. 2020 Nat Comms Bio) based on ossification patterns the dunnart orofacial region on the day of birth appears to be similar to that of an E12.5 mouse embryo (just prior to the observation of ossified craniofacial bones). However, when we compared both regulatory elements and expressed genes between the dunnart at this stage (P1) and 5 developmental stages in the mouse, there is no obvious equivalent stage. For example, when we simply compare genes linked to enhancer peaks, the group with the largest intersection between dunnart and any mouse stage are ~500 genes that are present in dunnart, and mouse stages E10.5, E12.5 - E15.5, Figure 5B). When we then compare genes expressed in the dunnart to temporal gene expression dynamics during mouse development we find that the largest overlap is with genes highly expressed at E14.5 or E15.5 in the mouse (Figure 6, Supplementary Figure 5). We have strengthened the rationale for the selected mouse stages in the comparative analyses section of the results.

      (5) The low conservation of putative enhancers between mouse and dunnart (0.74-6.77%) is surprising given previous reports of higher tissue-specific enhancer conservation across mammals. The authors should address whether this low conservation reflects genuine biological divergence or methodological artifacts (e.g., peak-calling parameters or genome quality). Comparisons with published studies could contextualize these findings.

      The reported range (0.74 - 6.77%) refers to the number regions called as an active enhancer peak in both species (conserved activity) divided by the total number of dunnart peaks alignable to the mouse genome, which we expect to be low given sequence turnover rates and the evolutionary distance separating dunnart and mice. The alignability (conserved sequence) for dunnart enhancers to the mouse genome was ~13% for 100bp regions and can be found in Supplementary Table 22, we have now clarified this in the main text.

      [New Text]: After building dunnart-mm10 liftover chains (see Methods and SupNote5) we compared mouse and dunnart regulatory elements. The alignability (conserved sequence) for dunnart enhancers to the mouse genome was ~13% for 100bp regions (Supplementary Table 22).

      The activity conservation range reported here is consistent with previously reported for marsupial-placental enhancer comparisons (Villar et al. 2015), where ~1% of conserved liver-specific human enhancers had conserved activity to opossum. Follow up studies in Berthelot et al 2018 also found that approximately 1% of human liver enhancers were conserved across the placental mammals included in the study.

      (6) Focusing only on genes associated with shared enhancers excludes potentially relevant genes without clear regulatory conservation. A broader analysis incorporating all orthologous genes may reveal additional insights into craniofacial heterochrony.

      We appreciate the reviewers comment, we understand that a broader analysis may provide some additional insights to this question however in this study our focus was understanding the enhancers driving craniofacial development in these species. We linked enhancers with gene expression data as additional evidence of regulatory programs involved in craniofacial development. The majority (~70%) of genes reproducibly expressed were linked to an active enhancer and/or promoter.   This has now been highlighted in the result section.

      [New Text]: There were 12,153 genes reproducibly expressed at a level > 1 TPM across three biological replicates, with the majority of genes 67% of genes expressed (67%; 8158/12153) associated with near an active enhancer and/or promoter peak.

      In conclusion, this study provides an important dataset for understanding marsupial craniofacial development and highlights the potential of genomic approaches in non-traditional model organisms. However, methodological limitations, including incomplete genome annotation and lack of developmental benchmarking weaken the robustness and of the findings. Addressing these issues would significantly enhance the study's utility to the field and its ability to support the study's central conclusion that dunnart-specific enhancers drive accelerated craniofacial development.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor comments and corrections:

      (1) ChIP-seq FRiP fractions were much higher in dunnart samples than in mouse. Is this related to any differences in sample preparation they are aware of in the ENCODE datasets of mouse, such as different anti-histone antibodies used (and therefore different efficiency of binding to the same histone markers across species)? The authors appear to have addressed something similar with respect to the much lower enriched peak number observed in the mouse sample relative to dunnart in Supp note 4. I suspect the "technical cofounder" they refer to there is affecting both the FRiP scores and the higher correlation coefficients between IP and input in mouse.

      We chose the same antibodies used in the mouse craniofacial tissue ENCODE experiments however, the procedure is slightly different. We used the MAGnify Chromatin Immunoprecipitation System while in the ENCODE assays performed by Bing Ren’s group in 2012 was an in-house lab protocol for MicroChIP. Given that the samples for mouse and dunnart were not processed together, by the same researcher, with the same protocol there could be any number of technical cofounders impacting enrichment. A low FRiP score suggests low specificity as the majority of reads are in non-specific regions (low enrichment), consistent with the higher correlation between IP and input in mouse. The data quality also appears to vary between H3K27ac and H3K4me3 in the mouse (Supplementary Table 21), with H3K4me3 FRiP scores more similar to those observed in our dunnart experiments. This suggests a potential confounder specific to the mouse H3K27ac IP. QC metrics (FRiP, bam correlation) are consistent between H3K27ac and H3K4me3 IPs in our experiments (Supplementary Table 20).

      (2) Some of the promoter peak numbers in Supp table 1 do not match the numbers in the main text.

      We have corrected the incorrect number reported in the text for promoter peaks with orthologous genes (8590 -> 8597).

      (3) In Supp tables 2 and 3, the number of GO terms similar across tables is 466, which is ~42% of total number of enriched GO terms. However the authors mention that only 23% of terms were the same between promoters and enhancers, and a value of 42% was applied to the proportion of terms uniquely enriched for terms associated with genes assigned to promoters only. Unless I'm reading these Supp tables incorrectly, is it possible the proportions were mixed up?

      Thanks for catching this. The lists provided in Supplementary Table 2 were incorrect. The Supplementary Tables and in text description has been corrected to reflect this.

      (4) Would be helpful to add a legend for the mouse samples in Supp Figure 10.

      We have added the labels to the plot.

      (5) In Supp note 5, regarding the percentage of alignable peaks recovered, the percentages mentioned for the 50bp and 500bp peak summit lengths for enhancers and promoters do not seem to match the values in Supp tables 22 and 23.

      Thank you for catching this - we have corrected the Supplementary Tables and in text.

      (6) Please provide additional information to explain how dunnart RNA expression was associated with the five temporal expression clusters found in the mouse data shown in Figure 6 given there is only one dunnart time point and so the species temporal pattern's could not be compared, i.e. how was the odds ratio calculated and was this applied iteratively for dunnart against each mouse age and within each temporal cluster?

      The TCseq package takes the mouse expression data across all 6 stages and calls differentially expressed genes with an absolute log<sub>2</sub> fold-change > 2 compared to the starting time-point (E10.5). The mouse gene expression patterns were clustered into 5 clusters that each show distinct temporal expression patterns (see Supplementary Figure 5D). The output from this is 5 lists where within each list are unique genes that share a temporal pattern. These lists of mouse genes were then each compared to the orthologous genes expressed in the dunnart using a Fishers Exact test with corrections for multiple testing using the Holm method. We have added additional details in the methods:

      [New text]: Orthologous genes reproducibly expressed >1 TPM in the dunnart were compared to the list of genes for each cluster using Fisher’s Exact Test followed by p-value corrections for multiple testing with the Holm method.

      (7) SupFile1 and SupFile2 - which supplementary note or figure are these referring to?

      Apologies for this error. These items were meant to link to the FigShare repository where the supplementary files can be found. We have corrected this using the DOI for the repository.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Authors should clarify that the mouse ENCODE data used for the comparisons was obtained from craniofacial tissue.

      This has now been corrected to clarify that the mouse ENCODE data used was from craniofacial tissues. ENCODE mouse embryonic facial prominence ChIP-seq and gene expression quantification file accession numbers and details used in study can be found in Supplementary Table 17.

      (2) Given the large differences in TPM for highly expressed genes shown in Figure 5, a MA or volcano plot would provide a more comprehensive view of global transcriptome differences between species.

      We have added this plot as Supplementary Figure 13.

      (3) It is unclear whether the enrichment analysis was performed for mouse genes, dunnart genes, or both.

      In reference to Figure 5, Gene Ontology enrichment analysis was performed on the top 500 highly expressed genes in dunnart. Because there is not an ontology database for dunnart gene IDs, these top 500 dunnart gene IDs were converted to the orthologous gene ID in mouse before performing the enrichment analysis. We apologise for the lack of clarity and have added additional text in the results section to make this clearer. In addition, the relevant methods section now reads:

      [New text]: As there is no equivalent gene ontology database for dunnart, we converted the Tasmanian devil RefSeq IDs to Ensembl v103 using biomaRt v2.46.3 and then converted these to mouse Ensembl v103 IDs. In this way we were able to use the mouse Ensembl Gene Ontology annotations for the dunnart gene domains. All gene ontology analyses were performed using clusterProfiler v4.1.4[117], with Gene Ontology from the org.Mm.eg.db v3.12.0 database[118], setting an FDR-corrected p-value threshold of 0.01 for statistical significance.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendation For the Authors):

      Thanks to the authors for addressing my suggestions. I think these modifications have improved the clarity of the data and the overall presentation of the manuscript. The methods are now more clearly explained, and the additional details help make the results easier to interpret. Where addressing the comment wasn't feasible, the authors gave reasonable explanations. Overall, the revisions strengthen the paper, and I have no further concerns.

      Thank you for your recommendations, which have significantly improved our paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendation For the Authors):

      The additional work conducted by the authors is greatly appreciated. All concerns (and beyond) have been thoroughly addressed by the authors and I am thankful for their consideration and attention to detail. Only one possible issue with the revisions is described below for consideration:

      Regarding the CFU counts and/or axis labels in Figure S3B, some of the listed "CFU per 1 mL" values (in both the figure itself and File S2B) are extraordinarily high. For example, the greatest CFU for PA14 observed in Figure 4E is ~1x10^9. However, PA14 at 0 ug/mL Ceftazidime reaches nearly 1x10^16 in Figure S3B. From what I can tell, this should be beyond the capacity of bacteria in this space by several orders of magnitude. (E.g., a cubic centimeter [~1 mL] is ~1x10^12 cubic micrometers. At their smallest dimensions and volume, a maximum of ~1x10^13 cells could theoretically fit in this space assuming no liquid and perfect organization.) Similarly, both "AMM" and "AMM (+PA14)" consistently reach CFUs between 1x10^12 and 1x10^14 in this assay. Are the authors confident in the values and/or depiction of CFUs for this figure? It seems like this could be a labeling or dilutioncounting issue.

      Thank you for your positive remarks on our revised manuscript and for your constructive comments that have strengthened our work.

      We agree with the concern regarding the CFU counts in Figure S3B. The very high values (>10<sup>12</sup>CFU) reflect a technical enumeration artifact that, due to the nature of the assay, cannot be fully avoided. The origin of these inflated counts is described in more detail below:

      Following competition assays between Pseudomonas aeruginosa and Stenotrophomonas maltophilia in liquid culture with antibiotics, we enumerate survivors for each species by colony forming unit (CFU) counts. Because two different bacterial species must be quantified from mixed cultures, we use a gentamicin resistance marker carried by one species at a time.

      Each condition is therefore enumerated twice, as we alternate which species harbors the gentamicin cassette.

      During coculture in antibiotics and minimal medium, clinical isolates of P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia, like those used here, can transiently increase their tolerance to antibiotics, including aminoglycosides. This reduces the effectiveness of gentamicin selection at the plating step necessary for CFU enumeration. For the data presented in Figure S3B, in a subset of highOD₆₀₀ conditions in the competition assay, this tolerance produces artificially inflated CFU values that exceed the biological carrying capacity during the CFU enumeration step.

      We evaluated alternative enumeration strategies (e.g., fluorescent protein markers with a nonselective medium), but these proved unsuitable for these strains due to differences in growth rates and media compatibility, introducing other large biases. Given these constraints, selective plating remains the only feasible approach for this work, and the associated artifact cannot be eliminated entirely.

      Importantly, transient resistance (tolerance), although common, is not a universal occurrence (e.g., we did not observe it when we performed the experiments shown in Figure 4E). When it does arise, it occurs reproducibly under the same experimental high-OD<sub>600</sub> conditions and does not obscure any of the relative comparisons that underpin our conclusions.

      For transparency, we have retained the measured values in Figure S3B and we note in the legend that counts above ~10<sup>12</sup> CFU represent a technical overestimation due to transient gentamicin tolerance. Counts below 10<sup>12</sup> CFU are accurately enumerated.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendation For the Authors):

      All concerns have been satisfied and the manuscript is ready for publishing.

      Thank you for your recommendations, which have significantly improved our paper.


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The study would benefit from presenting raw data in some cases, such as MIC values and SDS-PAGE gels, by clarifying the number of independent experiments used, as well as further clarification on statistical significance for some of the data.

      All original data used to generate Fig. 1, Fig. 4E, Fig. S3 and Fig. S4A are presented in File S2. Tab (A) is dedicated to data used for Fig. 1 and Fig. S4A, while tabs (B) and (C) show the data used for Fig. 4E and S3, respectively. This information is indicated in the legends of the relevant figures.

      All experiments in this study were performed in three independent (biological) experiments (with the exception of the complementation data shown in Fig. S1 and Fig. S5, which were performed in two independent (biological) experiments). The number of biological and technical replicates for each experiment is stated in the figure legends, as well as in the “Statistical analysis of experimental data” part of the “Materials and Methods” section of the paper. Specifically, for antibiotic MIC assays we have not performed statistical analyses as per recommended practice. The reason for this is stated in the following section from the “Statistical analysis of experimental data” part of the “Materials and Methods” section of the paper (lines 699-711 of the revised manuscript):

      “Antibiotic MIC values were determined in biological triplicate, except for MIC values recorded for dsbA complementation experiments in our E. coli K-12 inducible system that were carried out in duplicate. All ETEST MICs were determined as a single technical replicate, and all BMD MICs were determined in technical triplicate. All recorded MIC values are displayed in the relevant graphs; for MIC assays where three or more biological experiments were performed, the bars indicate the median value, while for assays where two biological experiments were performed the bars indicate the most conservative of the two values (i.e., for increasing trends, the value representing the smallest increase and for decreasing trends, the value representing the smallest decrease). We note that in line with recommended practice, our MIC results were not averaged. This should be avoided because of the quantized nature of MIC assays, which only inform on bacterial survival for specific antibiotic concentrations and do not provide information for antibiotic concentrations that lie in-between the tested values.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      While Figure 5E demonstrates a protective effect of DsbA-dependent β-lactamase, the omission of CFU data for S. maltophilia makes it difficult to assess the applicability of the polymicrobial strategy. Since S. maltophilia is pre-cultured prior to the addition of P. aeruginosa and antibiotics, it is unclear whether the protective effect is dependent on high S. maltophilia CFU. It is also unclear what the fate of the S. maltophilia dsbA dsbL mutant is under these conditions. If DsbA-deficient S. maltophilia CFU is not impacted, then this treatment will result in the eradication of only one of the pathogens of interest. If the mutant is lost during treatment, then it is not clear whether the loss of protection is due specifically to the production of non-functional β-lactamase or simply the absence of S. maltophilia.

      We have simultaneously tracked the abundance of P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia strains in our cross-protection experiment for select antibiotic concentrations. To be able to perform this experiment, we had to label two extremely-drug-resistant strains of S. maltophilia with an antibiotic resistance marker that allowed us to quantify them in mixtures with P. aeruginosa. Our results can be found in Fig. S3 of our revised manuscript and, in a nutshell, show that ceftazidime treatment leads to eradication of both P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia when disulfide bond formation is impaired in S. maltophilia.

      The following text was added to address the questions of the reviewer:

      “Due to the naturally different growth rates of these two species (S. maltophilia grows much slower than P. aeruginosa) especially in laboratory conditions, the protocol we followed [1] requires S. maltophilia to be grown for 6 hours prior to co-culturing it with P. aeruginosa. To ensure that at this point in the experiment our two S. maltophilia strains, with and without dsbA, had grown comparatively to each other, we determined their cell densities (Fig. S3A). We found that S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL had grown at a similar level as the wild-type strain, and both were at a higher cell density [~10<sup>7</sup> colony forming units (CFUs)] compared to the P. aeruginosa PA14 inoculum (5 x 10<sup>4</sup> CFUs)” (lines 353-361 of the revised manuscript).

      “To ensure that ceftazidime treatment leads to eradication of both P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia when disulfide bond formation is impaired in S. maltophilia, we monitored the abundance of both strains in each synthetic community for select antibiotic concentrations (Fig. S3B). In this experiment we largely observed the same trends as in Fig. 4E. At low antibiotic concentrations, for example 4 μg/mL of ceftazidime, S. maltophilia AMM is fully resistant and thrives, thus outcompeting P. aeruginosa PA14 (dark pink and dark blue bars in Fig. S3B). The same can also be seen in Fig. 4E, whereby decreased P. aeruginosa PA14 CFUs are recorded. By contrast S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL already displays decreased growth at 4 μg/mL of ceftazidime because of its non-functional L1-1 enzyme, allowing comparatively higher growth of P. aeruginosa (light pink and light blue bars in Fig. S3B). Despite the competition between the two strains, P. aeruginosa PA14 benefits from S. maltophilia AMM’s high hydrolytic activity against ceftazidime, which allows it to survive and grow in high antibiotic concentrations even though it is not resistant (see 128 μg/mL; dark pink and dark blue bars in Fig. S3B). In stark opposition, without its disulfide bond in S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL, L1-1 cannot confer resistance to ceftazidime, resulting in killing of S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL and, consequently, also of P. aeruginosa PA14 (see 128 μg/mL; light pink and light blue bars in Fig. S3B).

      The data presented here show that, at least under laboratory conditions, targeting protein homeostasis pathways in specific recalcitrant pathogens has the potential to not only alter their own antibiotic resistance profiles (Fig. 3 and 4A-D), but also to influence the antibiotic susceptibility profiles of other bacteria that co-occur in the same conditions (Fig. 5). Admittedly, the conditions in a living host are too complex to draw direct conclusions from this experiment. That said, our results show promise for infections, where pathogen interactions affect treatment outcomes, and whereby their inhibition might facilitate treatment” (lines 381406 of the revised manuscript).

      The alleged clinical relevance and immediate, theoretical application of this approach should be properly contextualized. At multiple junctures, the authors state or suggest that interactions between S. maltophilia and P. aeruginosa are known to occur in disease or have known clinical relevance related to treatment failure and disease states. For instance, the citations provided for S. maltophilia protection of P. aeruginosa in the CF lung environment both describe simplified laboratory experiments rather than clinical or in vivo observations. Similarly, the citations provided for both the role of S. maltophilia in treatment failure and CF disease severity do not support either claim. The role of S. maltophilia in CF is currently unsettled, with more recent work reporting conflicting results that support S. maltophilia as a marker, rather than cause, of severe disease. These citations also do not support the suggestion that S. maltophilia specifically contributes to treatment failure. While it is reasonable to pursue these ideas as a hypothesis or potential concern, there is no evidence provided that these specific interactions occur in vivo or that they have clinical relevance.

      Thank you for your comment. You are entirely correct. We have amended the test throughout our revised manuscript to avoid overstating the role of S. maltophilia in CF infections and to reference additional relevant works in the literature. Please find below representative examples of such passages:

      “On the other hand, CF microbiomes are increasingly found to encompass S. maltophilia [2-4], a globally distributed opportunistic pathogen that causes serious nosocomial respiratory and bloodstream infections [5-7]. S. maltophilia is one of the most prevalent emerging pathogens [6] and it is intrinsically resistant to almost all antibiotics, including β-lactams like penicillins, cephalosporins and carbapenems, as well as macrolides, fluoroquinolones, aminoglycosides, chloramphenicol, tetracyclines and colistin. As a result, the standard treatment option for lung infections, i.e., broad-spectrum β-lactam antibiotic therapy, is rarely successful in countering S. maltophilia [7,8], creating a definitive need for approaches that will be effective in eliminating both pathogens” (lines 33-41 of the revised manuscript).

      “Of the organisms studied in this work, S. maltophilia deserves further discussion because of its unique intrinsic resistance profile. The prognosis of CF patients with S. maltophilia lung carriage is still debated [4,9-16], largely because studies with extensive and well-controlled patient cohorts are lacking. This notwithstanding, the therapeutic options against this pathogen are currently limited to one non-β-lactam antibiotic-adjuvant combination, , which is not always effective, trimethoprim-sulfamethoxazole [17-20], and a few last-line β-lactam drugs, like the fifth-generation cephalosporin cefiderocol and the combination aztreonam-avibactam. Resistance to commonly used antibiotics causes many problems during treatment and, as a result, infections that harbor S. maltophilia have high case fatality rates [7]. This is not limited to CF patients, as S. maltophilia is a major cause of death in children with bacteremia [5]” (lines 440-450 of the revised manuscript).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The impact of the work can be strengthened by demonstrating increased efficacy of antibiotics in mice models or wound models for Pseudomonas infections. Worm models are relevant, but still distant from investigations in animal models.

      Thank you for this comment. We appreciate the sentiment, and we would have liked to be able to perform experiments in a murine model of infection. There are several reasons that made this not possible, and as a result we used G. mellonella as an informative preliminary in vivo infection model. The DSB proteins have been shown to play a central role in bacterial virulence. Because of this our P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia mutant strains are not efficient in establishing an infection, even in a wound model. This could be overcome had we been able to use the chemical inhibitor of the DSB system in vivo, however this also is not possible This is due to the fact that the chemical compound that we use to inhibit the function of DsbA acts on DsbB. Inhibition of DsbB blocks the re-oxidation of DsbA and leads to its accumulation in its inactive reduced form. However, the action of the inhibitor can be bypassed through reoxidation and re-activation of DsbA by small-molecule oxidants such as L-cystine, which are abundant in rich growth media or animal tissues. This makes the inhibitor only suitable for in vitro assays that can be performed in minimal media, where the presence of small-molecule oxidants can be strictly avoided, but entirely unsuitable for an insect or a vertebrate animal model.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendation For the Authors):

      (1) The analysis of the role of DsbA in the assembly of cysteine-containing β-lactamases is a significant finding. However, in addition to showing the MIC fold difference, I think, it would be important to show the raw data for the actual MIC values obtained for each β-lactamase enzyme/antibiotic combination and in both strains (+ and - dsbA).

      Also, can the authors clarify whether these experiments were conducted on 3 independent samples (there seems to be some contradicting information in the paper and the supplementary figures). If possible, I would also recommend showing in the figure whether the MIC differences observed were statistically significant.

      All original data used to generate Fig. 1, Fig. 4E, Fig. S3 and Fig. S4A are presented in File S2. Tab (A) is dedicated to data used for Fig. 1 and Fig. S4A, while tabs (B) and (C) show the data used for Fig. 4E and S3, respectively. This information is indicated in the legends of the relevant figures.

      All experiments in this study were performed in three independent (biological) experiments (with the exception of the complementation data shown in Fig. S1 and Fig. S5, which were performed in two independent (biological) experiments). The number of biological and technical replicates for each experiment is stated in the figure legends, as well as in the “Statistical analysis of experimental data” part of the “Materials and Methods” section of the paper. Specifically, for antibiotic MIC assays we have not performed statistical analyses as per recommended practice. The reason for this is stated in the following section from the “Statistical analysis of experimental data” part of the “Materials and Methods” section of the paper (lines 699-711 of the revised manuscript):

      “Antibiotic MIC values were determined in biological triplicate, except for MIC values recorded for dsbA complementation experiments in our E. coli K-12 inducible system that were carried out in duplicate. All ETEST MICs were determined as a single technical replicate, and all BMD MICs were determined in technical triplicate. All recorded MIC values are displayed in the relevant graphs; for MIC assays where three or more biological experiments were performed, the bars indicate the median value, while for assays where two biological experiments were performed the bars indicate the most conservative of the two values (i.e., for increasing trends, the value representing the smallest increase and for decreasing trends, the value representing the smallest decrease). We note that in line with recommended practice, our MIC results were not averaged. This should be avoided because of the quantized nature of MIC assays, which only inform on bacterial survival for specific antibiotic concentrations and do not provide information for antibiotic concentrations that lie in-between the tested values.”

      (2) For Figure 2A, can the authors provide the full Westerns and ideally the SDS-PAGE gel corresponding to the Westerns where the Β-lactamases and the control DNA-K were detected.

      Thank you for this comment. Full immunoblots and SDS PAGE analysis of the immunoblot samples for total protein content are shown in File S3 of our revised manuscript.

      (3) For the enzymatic assays, was the concentration of enzyme used "normalised " based on the amount detected in the westerns where possible or was only the total amount of protein considered. When similar amounts of enzyme were added, was the activity still compromised?

      The β-lactam hydrolysis assay was normalized based on the weight of the cell pellets (wet cell pellet mass) of the tested strains. This means, that for each enzyme expressed in cells with and without DsbA, strains were normalized to the same weight to volume ratio, and thus strains expressing the same enzyme were only compared to each other.

      Because enzyme degradation in the absence of DsbA is a key factor underlying the effects we describe for most of the tested β-lactamases (see Fig. 2A and S4A; no protein band is detected for 5 of the 7 enzymes in the dsbA mutant), it was not possible to normalize our samples based on enzyme levels detected by immunoblot. Normalization based on enzyme amounts would be feasible had we purified each β-lactamase after expression in the two different strain backgrounds (+/- dsbA) assuming sufficient protein amounts could be isolated from the dsbA mutant strain. Nonetheless, we feel that such a comparison would be misleading, since enzyme degradation likely plays the biggest role in the lack of activity observed for most of these enzymes in the absence of DsbA.

      (4) Not sure whether Fig 3 is very informative. Perhaps it could be redesigned to better encapsulate the findings in this manuscript (combine figurer 3 and 6 into one). I would also include the chemical structure of the inhibitors used and perhaps include how they block the system by binding to DsbB.

      Thank you for this comment. Fig. 3 was combined with Fig. 6 of the submitted manuscript. The new model figure is Fig. 5 in our revised manuscript.

      The inhibitor compound used in our study has been extensively characterized in a previous publication [21]. Considering that this inhibitor is not the main focus of our paper, we have avoided showing its chemical structure in any of the main display items. That said, its structure can be found in File S5 of our revised manuscript, which contains the quality control information on this compound. As suggested, we included the following sentence to describe the mode of action of this inhibitor: “Compound 36 was previously shown to inhibit disulfide bond formation in P. aeruginosa via covalently binding onto one of the four essential cysteine residues of DsbB in the DsbA-DsbB complex [21]” (lines 309-311 of the revised manuscript).

      (5) Figure 4: Similar to my comment above showing in the figure whether the differences observed in Figure 4, particularly A-C, are statistically significant (i.e. galleria survival difference in the presence and absence of dsbA) would be beneficial.

      As mentioned in our answer to comment 1 above, we have not performed statistical analyses for antibiotic MIC assays because, in line with recommended practice, our MIC results were not averaged (Fig. 3A,B,D,E of our revised manuscript). This should be avoided because of the quantized nature of MIC assays, which only inform on bacterial survival for specific antibiotic concentrations and do not provide information for antibiotic concentrations that lie in-between the tested values. Statistical analysis of G. mellonella survival data (Fig. 3C,F of our revised manuscript) was performed and is described fully in the legend of Fig. 3, as well as in the “Statistical analysis of experimental data” part of the “Materials and Methods” section of the paper (lines 729-738 of the revised manuscript). Finally, the statistical analyses for the most important comparisons in panels (C) and (F) of Fig. 3 are also marked directly on the figure.

      (6) Were the authors able to test the redox state of DsbA upon addition of the DsbB inhibitor to further demonstrate that the effects observed were indeed due to the obstruction of the Dsb machinery and not due to off target effects.

      Thank you for the opportunity to clarify this. In previous work from our lab, we have used a DSB system inhibitor termed “compound 12” in [22] with activity against DsbB proteins from Enterobacteria. In our previous study [23] we, indeed, tested the redox state of DsbA in the presence of this inhibitor compound. We could not perform the same experiment here with “compound 36” from [21], because we do not have an antibody against the DsbA protein of S. maltophilia. That said, we have carried out experiments that confirm that our results are due to specific inhibition of the DSB system and not because of off-target effects. In particular, we show that the gentamicin MIC values of S. maltophilia AMM remain unchanged in the presence of the inhibitor and treatment of S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL with the compound does not affects its colistin MIC value (Fig. S2E and lines 317-320 of the revised manuscript).

      (7) Given the remarkable effects shown by the DsbB inhibitor, did the authors use this compound to assess whether inhibition of the Dsb system with small molecules would block cross-resistance in S. maltophilia - P. aeruginosa mixed communities (Fig 5D).

      Unfortunately, this was not possible. The decrease in the ceftazidime MIC value of S. maltophilia AMM in the presence of the DSB inhibitor compound is more modest than the effects we observed when the dsbA dsbL mutant is used (compare Fig. 4D (left) with Fig.4A of the revised manuscript). This means that in the presence of the DSB inhibitor there are still sufficient amounts of functional β-lactamase present and we expect that they would contribute to cross-protection of P. aeruginosa. While the use of the DSB inhibitor does have a drastic impact on the colistin resistance profile of S. maltophilia AMM (Fig. 4D of the revised manuscript), unlike β-lactamases, which act as common goods, MCR enzymes act solely on the lipopolysaccharide of their producer and do not contribute to bacterial interactions, precluding the use of colistin for a cross-protection experiment.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendation For the Authors):

      (1) The acronym used for synthetic cystic fibrosis sputum medium (lines 523, 531, 535, 601, and 603) is defined in the manuscript as 'SCF', but the common formulation is 'SCFM', including in the provided citation. Suggest changing to SCFM for consistency.

      Thank you for this comment. This has been amended throughout our revised manuscript.

      (2) In Figure 1, while the legend states that "No changes in MIC values are observed for strains harboring the empty vector control (pDM1)[...]" (lines 729-30), the median of ceftazidime in the pDM1 control appears to indicate a 2-fold decrease in MIC. This would not seem to significantly impact the other results since the MIC decreases observed for other conditions are all 3-fold or greater, but this should be addressed and/or explained in the text.

      You are correct. Thank you for the opportunity to clarify this. Generally, since MIC assays have a degree of variability, we have only followed decreases in MIC values that are greater than 2fold. Generally, for most of our controls, the recorded MIC fold changes are below 2-fold. The only exception to this is the ceftazidime MIC drop of the empty-vector control, showing a 2fold change, which we do not consider significant.

      To ensure that this is clear in our text and figure legends the following changes were made:

      The clause “only differences larger than 2-fold were considered” was added to the text (lines 110-111 of the revised manuscript).

      We amended the legend of Fig. 1 accordingly: “No changes in MIC values are observed for the aminoglycoside antibiotic gentamicin (white bars) confirming that absence of DsbA does not compromise the general ability of this strain to resist antibiotic stress. Minor changes in MIC values (≤ 2-fold) are observed for strains harboring the empty vector control (pDM1) or those expressing the class A β-lactamases L2-1 and LUT-1, which contain two or more cysteines (Table S1), but no disulfide bonds (top row)”.

      (3) Similarly, in Fig S1E, there appears to be only partial complementation for BPS-1m. Do the authors hypothesize that this observation is related to a folding defect, rather than degradation of protein, as described for BPS-1m for Figure 2?

      Thank you for the opportunity to clarify this. You are correct that we only achieve partial complementation for the E. coli strain expressing the BPS-1m enzyme from the Burkholderia complex. Despite the fact that the gene for this enzyme was codon optimized, we observed that its expression in E. coli is sub-optimal and incurs fitness effects. In fact, to record the data presented in our manuscript the E. coli strains had to be transformed anew every time. Considering that the related enzyme BPS-6 does not present any of these challenges, we attribute the partial complementation to technical difficulties with the expression of the bps-1m gene in E. coli. 

      We clarified this by adding the following clause to our manuscript: “we only achieve partial complementation for the dsbA mutant expressing BPS-1m, which we attribute to the fact that expression of this enzyme in E. coli is sub-optimal” (lines 132-134 of the revised manuscript).

      (4) Lines 204-206: "[...]we deleted the principal dsbA gene, dsbA1 (pathogenic bacteria often encode multiple DsbA analogues [24,25]), in several multidrug-resistant (MDR) P. aeruginosa clinical strains (Table S2)". That multiple DsbA analogues are often encoded is good information to provide, but it was unclear from quickly looking at the citations whether Pa is counted among these. Is it expected that all oxidative protein folding in Pa functions through DsbA1? Conveying this information, if possible, may make the impact of the results in this model clearer.

      Thank you for this comment. To address it we added the following text to our manuscript:

      “To determine whether the effects on β-lactam MICs observed in our inducible system (Fig. 1 and [23]) can be reproduced in the presence of other resistance determinants in a natural context with endogenous enzyme expression levels, we deleted the principal dsbA gene, dsbA1, in several multidrug-resistant (MDR) P. aeruginosa clinical strains (Table S2). Pathogenic bacteria often encode multiple DsbA analogues [24,25] and P. aeruginosa is no exception. It encodes two DsbAs, but DsbA1 has been found to catalyze the vast majority of the oxidative protein folding reactions taking place in its cell envelope [26]” (lines 172-178 of the revised manuscript).

      (5) Regarding the clinical Pa isolates G4R7 and G6R7, have the authors performed any phenotypic testing on these strains to identify differences that might explain the substantial difference in piperacillin MIC? I.e., can these isolates be distinguished by growth rate, genetic markers or expression levels, early or late infection, mucoidy, etc. This is not essential for the current work, but could weigh on the efficacy of this treatment strategy for AIM1expressing clinical isolates. (E.g., the G4R7 dsbA1 strain exhibits a piperacillin MIC still ~2fold higher than WT G6R7).

      Thank you for the opportunity to clarify this. For clinical strains used in our study, we have evaluated their antibiotic resistance profiles, but we have not performed any additional phenotypic characterization. There are many reasons that contribute to differences in antibiotic resistance, starting simply from β-lactamase expression levels and extending to organismal effects, like the ones mentioned by the reviewer. Such characterization would fall outside the scope of our paper, especially since we sensitize our tested P. aeruginosa clinical isolates for the majority of the β-lactams antibiotics tested. 

      We acknowledged this by adding the following sentence to our revised manuscript: 

      “Despite the fact that P. aeruginosa G4R7 dsbA1 was not sensitized for piperacillintazobactam, possibly due to the high level of piperacillin-tazobactam resistance of the parent clinical strain, our results across these two isolates show promise for DsbA as a target against β-lactam resistance in P. aeruginosa” (lines 191-194 of the revised manuscript).

      (6) Lines 180-2: "This shows that without their disulfide bonds, these proteins are unstable and are ultimately degraded by other cell envelope proteostasis components [33]". While it is clear that protein is significantly lost in all cases except for BPS-1m in 2A, the dsbA pDM1bla constructs in 2B appear to all retain non-trivial (>10-fold) nitrocefin hydrolysis activity compared to the dsbA pDM1 control. This does not impact the other results in 2B, but it would seem that a loss-of-function folding defect, as described subsequently for BPS-1m, is also part of the explanation for the observed MIC decreases, and this was not necessarily clear from the quoted passage. This could simply be clarified in the final sentence - that both mechanisms are potentially in play - if the authors agree with that interpretation.

      You are correct, thank you for your comment. We amended the text in our revised manuscript as follows: 

      The data presented so far (Fig. 1 and 2) demonstrate that disulfide bond formation is essential for the biogenesis (stability and/or protein folding) and, in turn, activity of an expanded set of clinically important β-lactamases, including enzymes that currently lack inhibitor options” (lines 158-161 of the revised manuscript).

      (7) While it is clear from Figure S2 that the various dsb mutants do not have a general growth defect or collateral sensitivity to another antibiotic, it does not appear that there is an analogous control for the DSB inhibitor demonstrating no growth/toxic effects at the concentration used. This could be provided similarly to Figure S2, using gentamicin as a control antibiotic.

      We have carried out experiments that confirm that our results are due to specific inhibition of the DSB system and not because of off-target effects. In particular, we show that the gentamicin MIC values of S. maltophilia AMM remain unchanged in the presence of the inhibitor and treatment of S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL with the compound does not affects its colistin MIC value (Fig. S2E and lines 317-320 of the revised manuscript).

      (8) Complementation is appropriately provided for experiments with E. coli, but are not provided for P. aeruginosa or S. maltophilia. It should be straightforward to complement in Pa, but is also probably less critical considering the evidence from E. coli. However, since the Sm mutant is a gene cluster with two genes, it would seem more imperative to complement this strain. This reviewer is not familiar enough with Sm to know if complementation is routine or feasible with this organism; if not, the controls for the DSB inhibitor should at least be provided.

      As mentioned in our response to comment 7 above, we have carried out experiments that confirm that our DSB inhibitor results are due to specific inhibition of the DSB system and not because of off-target effects.

      Moreover, in response to this comment, we have further demonstrated that our results are due to the specific interaction of DsbA with β-lactamase enzymes by complementing dsbA deletions in representative clinical strains of multidrug-resistant Pseudomonas aeruginosa and extremely-drug-resistant Stenotrophomonas maltophilia. We would like to note here that gene complementation in clinical isolates remains very rare in the literature due to their high levels of resistance and limited genetic tractability. Most of the few complementation examples reported for these two organisms are limited to strains that, although pathogenic, are commonly used in the lab, or to complementation efforts in non-clinical strain systems (for example use of P. aeruginosa PA14 for complementation, instead of the focal clinical isolate).

      We tested three different complementation strategies, two of which ended up being unsuccessful. After approximately 9 months of work, we succeeded in complementing a representative clinical strain for each organism (P. aeruginosa CDC #769 dsbA1 and S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL) by inserting the dsbA1 gene from P. aeruginosa PAO1 into the Tn7 site on the chromosome. Both clinical strains show full complementation for every antibiotic tested; our complementation results can be found in Fig. S2B,D of the revised manuscript.

      The following text was added for P. aeruginosa clinical isolates:

      We have demonstrated the specific interaction of DsbA with the tested β-lactamase enzymes in our E. coli K-12 inducible system using gentamicin controls (Fig. 1 and File S2A) and gene complementation (Fig. S1). To confirm the specificity of this interaction in P. aeruginosa, we performed representative control experiments in one of our clinical strains, P. aeruginosa CDC #769. We first tested the general ability of P. aeruginosa CDC #769 dsbA1 to resist antibiotic stress by recording MIC values against gentamicin, and found it unchanged compared to its parent (Fig. S2A). Gene complementation in clinical isolates is especially challenging and rarely attempted due to the high levels of resistance and lack of genetic tractability in these strains. Despite these challenges, to further ensure the specificity of the interaction of DsbA with tested β-lactamases in P. aeruginosa, we have complemented dsbA1 from P. aeruginosa PAO1 into P. aeruginosa CDC #769 dsbA1. We found that complementation of dsbA1 restores MICs to wild-type values for both tested β-lactam compounds (Fig. S2B) further demonstrating that our results in P. aeruginosa clinical strains are not confounded by off-target effects” (lines 226-239 of the revised manuscript).

      The following text was added for S. maltophilia clinical isolates: 

      “Since the dsbA and dsbL are organized in a gene cluster in S. maltophilia, we wanted to ensure that our results reported above were exclusively due to disruption of disulfide bond formation in this organism. First, we recorded gentamicin MIC values for S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL and found them to be unchanged compared to the gentamicin MICs of the parent strain (Fig. S2C). This confirms that disruption of disulfide bond formation does not compromise the general ability of this organism to resist antibiotic stress. Next, we complemented S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL. The specific oxidative roles and exact regulation of DsbA and DsbL in S. maltophilia remain unknown. For this reason and considering that genetic manipulation of extremely-drug-resistant organisms is challenging, we used our genetic construct optimized for complementing P. aeruginosa CDC #769 dsbA1 with dsbA1 from P. aeruginosa PAO1 (Fig. S2B) to also complement S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL. We based this approach on the fact that DsbA proteins from one species have been commonly shown to be functional in other species [27-30]. Indeed, we found that complementation of S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL with P. aeruginosa PAO1 dsbA1 restores MICs to wild-type values for both ceftazidime and colistin (Fig. S2D), conclusively demonstrating that our results in S. maltophilia are not confounded by off-target effects” (lines 282-297 of the revised manuscript).

      (9) In Figure 5E, the growth inhibition and loss of Pa CFU in 4 ug/mL ceftazidime for the Sm co-culture condition, which is subsequently lost in the Sm dsbA dsbL co-culture, does not appear to be discussed. As Pa is shown to grow fine in monoculture at this concentration, this result should be discussed in relation to the co-culture dynamics. Is it expected or observed that WT Sm is out-competing Pa under this condition and growing to a high CFU/mL? This would seem to have parallels to citation 49.

      As requested by this reviewer (see comment 10 below), we simultaneously tracked the abundance of P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia strains in our cross-protection experiment. During this process we probed the abundances of the two organisms at 4 µg/mL of ceftazidime. Our results can be seen in Fig. S3B of the revised manuscript. The reviewer is correct and these effects are due to competition between P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia with the latter being able to reach very high CFUs in this antibiotic concentration. 

      The following text on co-culture dynamics was added to our revised manuscript: 

      At low antibiotic concentrations, for example 4 μg/mL of ceftazidime, S. maltophilia AMM is fully resistant and thrives, thus outcompeting P. aeruginosa PA14 (dark pink and dark blue bars in Fig. S3B). The same can also be seen in Fig. 4E, whereby decreased P. aeruginosa PA14 CFUs are recorded. By contrast S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL already displays decreased growth at 4 μg/mL of ceftazidime because of its non-functional L1-1 enzyme, allowing comparatively higher growth of P. aeruginosa (light pink and light blue bars in Fig. S3B)” (lines 384-390 of the revised manuscript).

      (10) The data presented in Figure 5E would be augmented by the inclusion of, for at least a few representative cases, the Sm CFUs relative to the Pa CFUs. In describing the protective effects of Sm on Pa for imipenem treatment, the authors of citation 12 note that the effect was dependent on Sm cell density. This raises the immediate question of whether the protection observed in this work is similarly dependent on cell density of Sm. It is unclear if the authors expect Sm to persist under these conditions, and it seems Sm CFU should be expected to be relatively high considering it is pre-incubated for 6 hours prior to the assay. What is the physiological state of these cells, and how are they affected by ceftazidime? While many other variables are likely relevant to the translation of this protection, the relative abundance and localization of Sm and Pa commonly observed in CF patients, as well as the effective concentration of antibiotic observed in vivo, is likely worth consideration.

      As mentioned in our response to comment 9 above, we have simultaneously tracked the abundance of P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia strains in our cross-protection experiment for select antibiotic concentrations. To be able to perform this experiment, we had to label two extremely-drug-resistant strains of S. maltophilia with an antibiotic resistance marker that allowed us to quantify them in mixtures with P. aeruginosa. Our results can be found in Fig. S3 of our revised manuscript and, in a nutshell, show that ceftazidime treatment leads to eradication of both P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia when disulfide bond formation is impaired in S. maltophilia.

      The following text was added to address the questions of the reviewer:

      “Due to the naturally different growth rates of these two species (S. maltophilia grows much slower than P. aeruginosa) especially in laboratory conditions, the protocol we followed [1] requires S. maltophilia to be grown for 6 hours prior to co-culturing it with P. aeruginosa. To ensure that at this point in the experiment our two S. maltophilia strains, with and without dsbA, had grown comparatively to each other, we determined their cell densities (Fig. S3A). We found that S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL had grown at a similar level as the wild-type strain, and both were at a higher cell density [~10<sup>7</sup> colony forming units (CFUs)] compared to the P.aeruginosa PA14 inoculum (5 x 10<sup>4</sup> CFUs)” (lines 353-361 of the revised manuscript).

      “To ensure that ceftazidime treatment leads to eradication of both P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia when disulfide bond formation is impaired in S. maltophilia, we monitored the abundance of both strains in each synthetic community for select antibiotic concentrations (Fig. S3B). In this experiment we largely observed the same trends as in Fig. 4E. At low antibiotic concentrations, for example 4 μg/mL of ceftazidime, S. maltophilia AMM is fully resistant and thrives, thus outcompeting P. aeruginosa PA14 (dark pink and dark blue bars in Fig. S3B). The same can also be seen in Fig. 4E, whereby decreased P. aeruginosa PA14 CFUs are recorded. By contrast S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL already displays decreased growth at 4 μg/mL of ceftazidime because of its non-functional L1-1 enzyme, allowing comparatively higher growth of P. aeruginosa (light pink and light blue bars in Fig. S3B). Despite the competition between the two strains, P. aeruginosa PA14 benefits from S. maltophilia AMM’s high hydrolytic activity against ceftazidime, which allows it to survive and grow in high antibiotic concentrations even though it is not resistant (see 128 μg/mL; dark pink and dark blue bars in Fig. S3B). In stark opposition, without its disulfide bond in S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL, L1-1 cannot confer resistance to ceftazidime, resulting in killing of S. maltophilia AMM dsbA dsbL and, consequently, also of P. aeruginosa PA14 (see 128 μg/mL; light pink and light blue bars in Fig. S3B).

      The data presented here show that, at least under laboratory conditions, targeting protein homeostasis pathways in specific recalcitrant pathogens has the potential to not only alter their own antibiotic resistance profiles (Fig. 3 and 4A-D), but also to influence the antibiotic susceptibility profiles of other bacteria that co-occur in the same conditions (Fig. 5). Admittedly, the conditions in a living host are too complex to draw direct conclusions from this experiment. That said, our results show promise for infections, where pathogen interactions affect treatment outcomes, and whereby their inhibition might facilitate treatment” (lines 381406 of the revised manuscript).

      (11) Regarding the role of microbial interactions in CF and other disease/infection contexts, the authors should temper their descriptions in accordance with citations provided. As an example, lines 96-99: "For example, in the CF lung, highly drug-resistant S. maltophilia strains actively protect susceptible P. aeruginosa from β-lactam antibiotics [12], and ultimately facilitate the evolution of β-lactam resistance in P. aeruginosa [14]."

      Neither citation provided here attests to Sm protection of Pa "in the CF lung". Both papers use a simplified in vitro co-culture model to assess Sm protection of Pa from antibiotics and the evolution of Pa antibiotic resistance in the presence or absence of Sm, respectively. In the latter case, it should also be noted that while the authors observed somewhat faster Pa resistance evolution in one co-culture condition, they did not observe it in the other, and that resistance evolution in general was observed regardless of co-culture condition. There are also statements in the ultimate and penultimate paragraphs of the Discussion section that repeat these points. The authors could re-frame this aspect of their investigation as part of a working hypothesis related to potential interactions of these pathogens, and should appropriately caveat what is and is not known from in vitro and in vivo/clinical work.

      Thank you for your comment. You are entirely correct. We have amended the test throughout our revised manuscript to avoid overstating these finding and to be clear about the fact that they originate from experimental studies. Please find below representative examples of such passages:

      “In particular, some antibiotic resistance proteins, like β-lactamases, which decrease the quantities of active drug present, function akin to common goods, since their benefits are not limited to the pathogen that produces them but can be shared with the rest of the bacterial community. This means that their activity enables pathogen cross-resistance when multiple species are present [1,31], something that was demonstrated in recent work investigating the interactions between pathogens that naturally co-exist in CF infections. More specifically, it was shown that in laboratory co-culture conditions, highly drug-resistant S. maltophilia strains actively protect susceptible P. aeruginosa from β-lactam antibiotics [1]. Moreover, this crossprotection was found to facilitate, at least under specific conditions, the evolution of β-lactam resistance in P. aeruginosa [32]” (lines 47-57 of the revised manuscript).

      “The antibiotic resistance mechanisms of S. maltophilia impact the antibiotic tolerance profiles of other organisms that are found in the same infection environment. S. maltophilia hydrolyses all β-lactam drugs through the action of its L1 and L2 β-lactamases [7,8]. In doing so, it has been experimentally shown to protect other pathogens that are, in principle, susceptible to treatment, such as P. aeruginosa [1]. This protection, in turn, allows active growth of otherwise treatable P. aeruginosa in the presence of complex β-lactams, like imipenem [1], and, at least in some conditions, increases the rate of resistance evolution of P. aeruginosa against these antibiotics [32]” (lines 332-340 of the revised manuscript).

      (12) Regarding the role of S. maltophilia in CF disease, the authors should either discuss clinical associations more completely or note the conflicting data on its role in disease. As an example, lines 84-87: "As a result, the standard treatment option, i.e., broad-spectrum βlactam antibiotic therapy, constitutes a severe risk for CF patients carrying both P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia [10,11], creating an urgent need for antimicrobial approaches that will be effective in eliminating both pathogens."

      It is unclear how this treatment results in a "severe risk" for CF patients colonized by both Sm and Pa. Citation 10 suggests an association between anti-pseudomonal antibiotic use and increased prevalence of Sm, but neither citation supports a worsening clinical outcome from this treatment. Citation 10 further notes that clinical scores between Sm-positive and control cohorts could not be distinguished statistically. Citation 11 is a review that makes note of this conflicting data regarding Sm, including reference to a more recent (at the time) result using multivariate analysis showing no independent affect of Sm on survival.

      The above point similarly applies to other statements in the manuscript, for example at lines 266-267: "Considering the contribution of S. maltophilia strains to treatment failure in CF lung infections [8,10,11][...]" As well as lines 79-80: "Pulmonary exacerbations and severe disease states are also associated with the presence of S. maltophilia [8]"

      Again, the provided citations do not support the implication that Sm specifically 'contributes to treatment failure in CF lung infections' or that Sm is specifically associated with severe disease states. In addition to the previously discussed citations, citation 8 describes broad "pulmotypes" composed of 10 species/genera that could be associated with particular clinical (e.g., exacerbation) or treatment (e.g., antibiotic therapy) characteristics, but these cannot, without further analysis, be associated with, or causally linked to, a specific pathogen. While pulmotype 2 in citation 8 was associated with a more severe clinical state and appeared to have the highest relative abundance of Sm compared to other pulmotypes, Sm was not identified (Figure 4A) as an independent factor that distinguishes between moderate and severe disease, unlike Pa and some anaerobes (4F-H). The authors also observed that decreasing relative abundance of Pa, in particuar, is correlated with subsequent exacerbation, but did not correlate this with the presence of any other species or genera. Again, this should be re-framed with the appropriate caveat that this is a hypothesis with possible clinical significance.

      Several suggested papers are included below on Sm association with clinical characteristics to incorporate into the manuscript if the authors choose to do so:

      https://doi.org/10.1177/14782715221088909

      https://doi.org/10.1016/j.prrv.2010.07.003

      https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcf.2013.05.009 https://doi.org/10.1002/ppul.23943

      https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD005405.pub2

      https://doi.org/10.1164/rccm.2109078 http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/thx.2003.017707

      https://erj.ersjournals.com/content/23/1/98.short

      Thank you for your comment. You are entirely correct. We have amended the test throughout our revised manuscript to avoid overstating the role of S. maltophilia in CF infections and to reference additional relevant works in the literature. Please find below representative examples of such passages:

      “On the other hand, CF microbiomes are increasingly found to encompass S. maltophilia [2-4], a globally distributed opportunistic pathogen that causes serious nosocomial respiratory and bloodstream infections [5-7]. S. maltophilia is one of the most prevalent emerging pathogens [6] and it is intrinsically resistant to almost all antibiotics, including β-lactams like penicillins, cephalosporins and carbapenems, as well as macrolides, fluoroquinolones, aminoglycosides, chloramphenicol, tetracyclines and colistin. As a result, the standard treatment option for lung infections, i.e., broad-spectrum β-lactam antibiotic therapy, is rarely successful in countering S. maltophilia [7,8], creating a definitive need for approaches that will be effective in eliminating both pathogens” (lines 33-41 of the revised manuscript).

      “Of the organisms studied in this work, S. maltophilia deserves further discussion because of its unique intrinsic resistance profile. The prognosis of CF patients with S. maltophilia lung carriage is still debated [4,9-16], largely because studies with extensive and well-controlled patient cohorts are lacking. This notwithstanding, the therapeutic options against this pathogen are currently limited to one non-β-lactam antibiotic-adjuvant combination, , which is not always effective, trimethoprim-sulfamethoxazole [17-20], and a few last-line β-lactam drugs, like the fifth-generation cephalosporin cefiderocol and the combination aztreonam-avibactam. Resistance to commonly used antibiotics causes many problems during treatment and, as a result, infections that harbor S. maltophilia have high case fatality rates [7]. This is not limited to CF patients, as S. maltophilia is a major cause of death in children with bacteremia [5]” (lines 440-450 of the revised manuscript).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendation For the Authors):

      (1) The referencing of supplemental figures does not follow a sequential order. For example, Figure S2 appears in the text before S1. The sequential ordering of figure numbers improves the readability and can be considered while editing the manuscript for revision.

      Thank you for this comment. This is amended in our revised manuscript and supplemental figures and files are cited in order.

      (2 )It will be useful to provide a brief description of ambler classes since these are important to study design (for a broader audience).

      Thank you for this suggestion. This has been added and can be found in lines 91-101 of the revised manuscript.

      (3) The rationale for using K12 strain for E. coli should be provided. It appears that is a model system that is well established in their lab, but a scientific rationale can be listed. Maybe this strain does not have any lactamases in its genome other than the one being expressed as compared to pathogenic E. coli?

      Thank you for this suggestion. This has been added and can be found in lines 104-106 of the revised manuscript.

      (4) The reviewers used worm model to test their observations, which is relevant. Given the significant implications of their work in overcoming resistance to clinically used antibiotics and availability of already generated dsbA mutants in clinical strains, it will be useful to investigate survival in animal models or at least wound models of Pseudomonas infections. The reviewer does not deem this necessary, but it will significantly increase the impact of their seminal work.

      Thank you for this comment. We appreciate the sentiment, and we would have liked to be able to perform experiments in a murine model of infection. There are several reasons that made this not possible, and as a result we used G. mellonella as an informative preliminary in vivo infection model. The DSB proteins have been shown to play a central role in bacterial virulence. Because of this our P. aeruginosa and S. maltophilia mutant strains are not efficient in establishing an infection, even in a wound model. This could be overcome had we been able to use the chemical inhibitor of the DSB system in vivo, however this also is not possible This is due to the fact that the chemical compound that we use to inhibit the function of DsbA acts on DsbB. Inhibition of DsbB blocks the re-oxidation of DsbA and leads to its accumulation in its inactive reduced form. However, the action of the inhibitor can be bypassed through reoxidation and re-activation of DsbA by small-molecule oxidants such as L-cystine, which are abundant in rich growth media or animal tissues. This makes the inhibitor only suitable for in vitro assays that can be performed in minimal media, where the presence of small-molecule oxidants can be strictly avoided, but entirely unsuitable for an insect or a vertebrate animal model.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Dixit, Noe, and Weikl apply coarse-grained and all-atom molecular dynamics to determine the response of the mechanosensitive proteins Piezo 1 and Piezo 2 proteins to tension. Cryo-EM structures in micelles show a high curvature of the protein whereas structures in lipid bilayers show lower curvature. Is the zero-stress state of the protein closer to the micelle structure or the bilayer structure? Moreover, while the tension sensitivity of channel function can be inferred from the experiment, molecular details are not clearly available. How much does the protein's height and effective area change in response to tension? With these in hand, a quantitative model of its function follows that can be related to the properties of the membrane and the effect of external forces. 

      Simulations indicate that in a bilayer the protein relaxes from the highly curved cryo-EM dome (Figure 1). 

      Under applied tension, the dome flattens (Figure 2) including the underlying lipid bilayer. The shape of the system is a combination of the membrane mechanical and protein conformational energies (Equation 1). The membrane's mechanical energy is well-characterized. It requires only the curvature and bending modulus as inputs. They determine membrane curvature and the local area metric (Equation 4) by averaging the height on a grid and computing second derivatives (Equations 7, 8) consistent with known differential geometric formulas. 

      The bending energy can be limited to the nano dome but this implies that the noise in the membrane energy is significant. Where there is noise outside the dome there is noise inside the dome. At the least, they could characterize the noisy energy due to inadequate averaging of membrane shape. 

      My concern for this paper is that they are significantly overestimating the membrane deformation energy based on their numerical scheme, which in turn leads to a much stiffer model of the protein itself.

      We agree that “thermal noise” is intrinsic to MD simulations, as in “real” systems, leading to thermally excited shape fluctuations of membranes and conformational fluctuations of proteins. However, for our coarse-grained simulations, the thermally excited membrane shape fluctuations can be averaged out quite well, and the resulting average shapes are smooth, see e.g. the shapes and lines of the contour plots in Fig. 1 and 2. For our atomistic simulations, the averaged shapes are not as smooth, see Fig. 3a and the lines of the contour plots in Fig. 3b. Therefore, we do not report bending energies for the nanodome shapes determined from atomistic simulations, because bending energy calculations are sensitive to remaining “noise” on small scales (due to the scale invariance of the bending energy), in contrast to calculations of excess areas, which we state now on lines 620ff.

      For our coarse-grained simulations, we now corroborate our bending energy calculations based on averaged 3d shapes by comparing to bending energy values obtained from highly smoothened 2d mean curvature profiles (see Fig. 1c for mean curvature profiles in tensionless membranes). We discuss this in detail from line 323 on, starting with:

      “To corroborate our bending energy calculations for these averaged three-dimensional nanodome shapes, we note that essentially identical bending energies can be obtained from the highly smoothened mean curvatures M of the two-dimensional membrane profiles. …”

      Two things would address this: 

      (1) Report the membrane energy under different graining schemes (e.g., report schemes up to double the discretization grain). 

      There are two graining schemes in the modeling, and we have followed the reviewer’s recommendation regarding the second scheme. In the first, more central graining scheme, we use quadratic membrane patches with a sidelength of about 2 nm to determine membrane midplane shapes and lipid densities of each simulation conformation. This graining scheme has also been previously employed in Hu, Lipowsky, Weikl, PNAS 38, 15283 (2013) to determine the shape and thermal roughness of coarse-grained membranes. A sidelength of 2 nm is necessary to have sufficiently many lipid headgroups in the upper and lower leaflet in the membrane patches for estimating the local height of these leaflets, and the local membrane midplane height as average of these leaflet heights (see subsection “Membrane shape of simulation conformation” in the Methods section for details).  However, we strongly believe that doubling the sidelength of membrane patches in this discretization is not an option, because a discretization length of 4 nm is too coarse to resolve the membrane deformations in the nanodome, see e.g. the profiles in Fig. 1b. Moreover, any “noise” from this discretization is rather completely smoothened out in the averaging process used in the analysis of the membrane shapes, at least for the coarse-grained simulations. This averaging process requires rotations of membrane conformations to align the protein orientations of the conformations (see subsection “Average membrane shapes and lipid densities” for details). Because of these rotations, the original discretization is “lost” in the averaging, and a continuous membrane shape is generated. To calculate the excess areas and bending energies for this smooth, continuous membrane shape, we use a discretization of the Monge plane into a square lattice with lattice parameter 1 nm. As a response to the referee’s suggestion, we now report that the results for the excess area do not change significantly when doubling this lattice parameter to 2 nm. On line 597, we write:

      “For a lattice constant of a=2 nm, we obtain extrapolated values of the excess area Delta A from the coarse-grained simulations that are 2 to 3% lower than the values for a=1 nm, which is a small compared to statistical uncertainties with relative errors of around 10%.”

      On lines 614ff, we now state that the bending energy results are about 10% to 13% lower for a=2 nm, likely because of the lower resolution of the curvature in the nanodome compared to a=1 nm, rather than incomplete averaging and remaining roughness of the coarse-grained nanodome shapes.

      (2) For a Gaussian bump with sigma=6 nm I obtained a bending energy of 0.6 kappa, so certainly in the ballpark with what they are reporting but significantly lower (compared to 2 kappa, Figure 5 lower left). It would be simpler to use the Gaussian approximation to their curves in Figure 3 - and I would argue more accurate, especially since they have not reported the variation of the membrane energy with respect to the discretization size and so I cannot judge the dependence of the energy on discretization. I view reporting the variation of the membrane energy with respect to discretization as being essential for the analysis if their goal is to provide a quantitative estimate for the force of Piezo. The Helfrich energy computed from an analytical model with a membrane shape closely resembling the simulated shapes would be very helpful. According to my intuition, finite-difference estimates of curvatures will tend to be overestimates of the true membrane deformation energy because white noise tends to lead to high curvature at short-length scales, which is strongly penalized by the bending energy. 

      Instead of Gaussian bumps, we now calculate the membrane bending energy also from the two-dimensional, continuous mean curvature profiles (see Fig. 1c). These mean curvature profiles are highly smoothened (see figure caption for details). Nonetheless, we obtain essentially the same bending energies as in our discrete calculations of averaged, smoothened threedimensional membrane shapes, see new text on lines 326ff. We believe that this agreement corroborates our bending energy calculations. We still focus on values obtained for threedimensional membrane shapes, because of incomplete rotational symmetry. The three-dimensional membrane shapes exhibit variations with the three-fold symmetry of the Piezo proteins, see Figure 2a and b.

      We agree that the bending energy of thermally rough membranes depends on the discretization scheme, because the discretization length of any discretization scheme leads to a cut-off length for fluctuation modes in a Fourier analysis. But again, we average out the thermal noise, for reasons given in the Results section, and analyse smooth membrane shapes.  

      The fitting of the system deformation to the inverse time appears to be incredibly ad hoc ... Nor is it clear that the quantified model will be substantially changed without extrapolation. The authors should either justify the extrapolation more clearly (sorry if I missed it!) or also report the unextrapolated numbers alongside the extrapolated ones. 

      We report the values of the excess area and bending energy in the different time intervals of our analysis as data points in Fig. 4 with supplement. We find it important to report the time dependence of these quantities, because the intended equilibration of the membrane shapes in our simulations is not “complete” within a certain time window of the simulations. So, just “cutting” the first 20 and 50% of the simulation trajectories, and analysing the remaining parts as “equilibrated” does not seem to be a reasonable choice here, at least for the membrane properties, i.e. for the excess area and bending energy. We agree that the linear extrapolation used in our analysis is a matter of choice. At least for the coarse-grained simulations, the extrapolated values of excess areas and bending energies are rather close to the values obtained in the last time windows (see Figure 4). 

      In summary, this paper uses molecular dynamics simulations to quantify the force of the Piezo 1 and Piezo 2 proteins on a lipid bilayer using simulations under controlled tension, observing the membrane deformation, and using that data to infer protein mechanics. While much of the physical mechanism was previously known, the study itself is a valuable quantification. I identified one issue in the membrane deformation energy analysis that has large quantitative repercussions for the extracted model. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this study, the authors suggest that the structure of Piezo2 in a tensionless simulation is flatter compared to the electron microscopy structure. This is an interesting observation and highlights the fact that the membrane environment is important for Piezo2 curvature. Additionally, the authors calculate the excess area of Piezo2 and Piezo1, suggesting that it is significantly smaller compared to the area calculated using the EM structure or simulations with restrained Piezo2. Finally, the authors propose an elastic model for Piezo proteins. Those are very important findings, which would be of interest to the mechanobiology field. 

      Whilst I like the suggestion that the membrane environment will change Piezo2 flatness, could this be happening because of the lower resolution of the MARTINI simulations? In other words, would it be possible that MARTINI is not able to model such curvature due to its lower resolution? 

      Related to my comment above, the authors say that they only restrained the secondary structure using an elastic network model. Whilst I understand why they did this, Piezo proteins are relatively large. How can the authors know that this type of elastic network model restrains, combined with the fact that MARTINI simulations are perhaps not very accurate in predicting protein conformations, can accurately represent the changes that happen within the Piezo channel during membrane tension? 

      These questions regarding the reliability of the Martini model are very reasonable and are the reason why we include also results from atomistic simulations, at least for Piezo 2, and compare the results. In the Martini model, secondary structure constraints are standard. In addition, constraints on the tertiary structure (e.g. via an elastic network model) are also typically used in simulations of soluble, globular proteins. However, such tertiary constraints would make it impossible to simulate the tension-induced flattening of the Piezo proteins. So instead, as we write on lines 427ff, “we relied on the capabilities of the Martini coarse-grained force field for modeling membrane systems with TM helix assemblies (Sharma and Juffer, 2013; Chavent et al., 2014; Majumder and Straub, 2021).” In these refences, Martini simulations were used to study the assembly of transmembrane helices, leading to agreement with experimentally observed structures. As we state in our article, our atomistic simulations corroborate the Martini simulations, with the caveats that are now more extensively discussed in the new last paragraph of the Discussion section starting on line 362.

      Modelling or Piezo1, seems to be based on homology to Piezo2. However, the authors need to further evaluate their model, e.g. how it compares with an Alphafold model. 

      We understand the question, but see it beyond the scope of our article, also because of the computational demand of the simulations. The question is: Do coarse-grained simulations of Piezo1 based on an Alphafold model as starting structure lead to different results? It is important to note that we only model the rather flexible 12 TM helices at the outer ends of the Piezo 1 monomers via homology modeling to the Piezo 2 structure, which includes these TM helices. For the inner 26 TM helices, including the channel, we use the high-quality cryo-EM structure of Piezo 1. Alphafold may be an alternative for modeling the outer 12 helices, but we don’t think this would lead to statistically significant differences in simulations – e.g. because of the observed overall agreement of membrane shapes in all our Piezo 1 and Piezo 2 simulation systems.

      To calculate the tension-induced flattening of the Piezo channel, the authors "divide all simulation trajectories into 5 equal intervals and determine the nanodome shape in each interval by averaging over the conformations of all independent simulation runs in this interval.". However, probably the change in the flattening of Piezo channel happens very quickly during the simulations, possibly within the same interval. Is this the case? and if yes does this affect their calculations? 

      Unfortunately, the flattening is not sufficiently quick, so is not complete within the first time windows, see data points in Figure 4. We therefore report the time dependence with the plots in Figure 4 and extrapolate, see also our response above to reviewer 1.

      Finally, the authors use a specific lipid composition, which is asymmetric. Is it possible that the asymmetry of the membrane causes some of the changes in the curvature that they observe? Perhaps more controls, e.g. with a symmetric POPC bilayer are needed to identify whether membrane asymmetry plays a role in the membrane curvature they observe. 

      Because of the rather high computational demands, such controls are beyond our scope. We don’t expect statistically significant differences for symmetric POPC/cholesterol bilayers. On lines 229ff, we now state:

      “Our modelling assumes that any spontaneous curvature from asymmetries in the lipid composition is small compared to the curvature of the nanodome and, thus, negligible, which is plausible for the rather slight lipid asymmetry of our simulated membranes (see Methods).”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Strengths: 

      This work focuses on a problem of deep significance: quantifying the structure-tension relationship and underlying mechanism for the mechanosensitive Piezo 1 and 2 channels. This objective presents a few technical challenges for molecular dynamics simulations, due to the relatively large size of each membrane-protein system. Nonetheless, the technical approach chosen is based on the methodology that is, in principle, established and widely accessible. Therefore, another group of practitioners would likely be able to reproduce these findings with reasonable effort. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The two main results of this paper are (1) that both channels exhibit a flatter structure compared to cryo-EM measurements, and (2) their estimated force vs. displacement relationship. Although the former correlates at least quantitatively with prior experimental work, the latter relies exclusively on simulation results and model parameters. 

      Below is a summary of the key points we recommend addressing in a revised version of the manuscript: 

      (1) The authors should report and discuss controls for the membrane energy calculations, specifically by increasing the density of the discretization graining. We also suggest validating the bending modulus used in the energy calculations for the specific lipid mixture employed in the study. 

      We have addressed both points, see our response to the reviewer’s comments for further details.

      (2) The authors should consider and discuss the potential limitations of the coarse-grained simulation force field and clarify how atomistic simulations validate the reported results, with a more detailed explanation of the potential interdependencies between the two. 

      We now discuss the caveats in the comparison of coarse-grained and atomistic simulations in more detail in a new paragraph starting on line 362.

      (3) The authors should provide further clarification on other points raised in the reviewers' comments, for instance, the potential role of membrane asymmetry. 

      We have done this – see above. We now further explain on lines 437ff why we use an asymmetric membrane. On lines 230ff, we discuss that any spontaneous membrane curvature due to lipid asymmetry is likely small compared to the nanodome curvature and, thus, negligible.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) Report discretization dependence of the membrane energy (up to double the density of the current discretization graining). 

      We have added several text pieces in the paragraph “Excess area and bending energy” starting on line 583 in which we state how the results depend on the lattice constant a of the calculations.

      (2) Evaluate an analytical energy of a membrane bump with a shape similar to the simulation. This would be free of all sampling and discretization artifacts and would thus be an excellent lower bound of the energy. 

      We have done this for the curvature profile in Figure 1c and corresponding curvature profiles of the shape profiles in Figure 2d, see next text on lines 326ff.

      Minor: 

      (1)  The lipid density (Figure 1 right, 2c, 3c) is not interesting nor is it referred to. It can be dropped. 

      We think the lipid density maps are important for two reasons: First, they show the protein shape obtained after averaging conformations, as low-lipid-density regions. Second, the lipid densities are used in the calculation of the bending energies, to limit the bending energy calculations to the membrane in the nanodome, see Eq. 9. We therefore prefer to keep them.

      (2) Figure 7 is attractive but not used in a meaningful way. I suggest inserting the protein graphic from Figure 7 into Figure 1 with the 4-helix bundles numbered alongside the structure. Figure 7 could then be dropped. 

      Figure 7 is a figure of the Methods section. We need it to illustrate and explain aspects of the setup (numbering of helices, missing loops) and analysis (numbering scheme of 4-TM helix units).

      (3) Some editing of the use of the English language would be helpful. "Exemplary" is a bit of a funny word choice, it implies that the conformation is excellent, and not simply representative. I'd suggest "Representative conformation". 

      We agree and have replaced “exemplary” by “representative”.

      (4) Typos: 

      Equation 4 - Missing parentheses before squared operator inside the square root. 

      We have corrected this mistake.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      This study focuses mainly on Piezo2; the authors do not perform any atomistic simulations of Piezo1, and the coarse-grained simulations for Piezo1 are shorter. As a result, their analysis for Piezo2 seems more complete. It would be good if the authors did similar studies with Piezo1 as with Piezo2. 

      We agree that atomistic simulations of Piezo 1 would be interesting, too. However, because the atomistic simulations are particularly demanding, this is beyond our scope.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) At line 63, a very large tension from the previous work by De Vecchis et al is reported (68 mN/m). The authors are sampling values up to about 21 mN/m, which is considerably smaller. However, these values greatly exceed what typical lipid membranes can sustain (about 10 mN/m) before rupturing. When mentioning these large tensions, the authors should emphasize that these values are not physiologically significant, because they would rupture most plasma membranes. That said, their use in simulation could be justified to magnify the structural changes compared to experiments. 

      We agree that our largest membrane tension values are unphysiological. However, we see a main novelty and relevance of our simulations in the fact that we obtain a response of the nanodome in the physiological range of membrane tensions, see e.g. the 3<sup>rd</sup> sentence of the abstract. Yes, we include simulations at tensions of 21 mN/m, but most of our simulated tension values are in the range from 0 to 10 mN/m (see e.g. Fig. 3e), in contrast to previous simulation studies.   

      (2) At line 78 and in the Methods, only the reference paper is for the CHARMM protein force field, but not for the lipid force field. 

      We have added the reference Klauda et al., 2010 for the CHARMM36 lipid force field in both spots.

      (3) (Line 83) Acknowledging that the authors needed to use the structure from micelles (because it has atomic resolution), how closely do their relaxed Piezo structures compare with the lowerresolution data from the MacKinnon and Patapoutian papers? 

      There are no structures reported in these papers to compare with, only a clear flattening as stated.  

      (4) (Line 99) The authors chose a slightly asymmetric lipid membrane composition to capture some specific plasma-membrane features. However, they do not discuss which features are described by this particular composition, which doesn't include different acyl-chain unsaturations between leaflets. Further, they do not seem to comment on whether there is enrichment of certain lipid species coupled to curvature, or whether there is any "scrambling" occurring when the dome section and the planar membrane are stitched together in the preparation phase (Figure 8). 

      Enrichment of lipids in contact with the protein is addressed in the reference Buyan et al., 2020, based on Martini simulations with Piezo 1. We have a different focus, but still wanted to keep an asymmetric membrane as in essentially all previous simulation studies as now stated also on lines 439ff, to mimic the native Piezo membrane environment. There is no apparent “scrambling” in the setup of our membrane systems. We also did not explore any coupling between curvature and lipid composition, but will publish the simulation trajectories to enable such studies.  

      (5) (Caption of Figure 2). Please comment briefly in the text why the tensionless simulation required a longer simulation run (e.g. larger fluctuations?) 

      We added as explanation on line 500 as explanation: “ … to explore the role of the long-range shape fluctuations in tensionless membranes for the relaxation into equilibrium”. The relaxation time of membrane shape fluctuations strongly increases with the wave length, which is only limited by the simulation box size in the absence of tensions. However, also for 8 microsecond trajectories, we do not observe complete equilibriation and therefore decided to extrapolate the excess area and bending energy values obtained for different time intervals of the trajectories.

      (6) (Caption of Figure 3). Please clarify in the Methods how the atomistic simulations were initialized were they taken from independent CG simulation snapshots? If not, the use of the adjective "independent" would be questionable given the very short atomistic simulation time length. 

      We now added that the production simulations started from the same structure. On lines 386, we now discuss the starting structure of the atomistic simulations in more detail.

      (7) (Line 202). The approach of discretizing the bilayer shape is reasonable, but no justification was provided for the 1-nm grid spacing. In my opinion, there should be a supporting figure showing how the bending energy varies with the grid spacing. 

      We now report also the effect of a 2-nm grid spacing on the results, see new text passages on page 18, and provide an explanation for the smaller 1-nm grid spacing on lines 587ff, where we write:

      “This lattice constant [a = 1 nm] is chosen to be smaller than the bin width of about 2nm used in determining the membrane shape of the simulation conformations, to take into account that the averaging of these membrane shapes can lead to a higher resolution compared to the 2 nm resolution of the individual membrane shapes.”

      (8) (Line 211). The choice by the authors to use a mixed lipid composition complicates the task of defining a reasonable bending modulus. Experimentally and in atomistic simulations, lipids with one saturated tail (like POPC or SOPC) are much stiffer when they are mixed with cholesterol (https://doi.org/10.1529/biophysj.105.067652, https://doi.org/10.1103/PhysRevE.80.021931, https://doi.org/10.1093/pnasnexus/pgad269). On the other hand, MARTINI seems to predict a slight *softening* for POPC mixed with cholesterol (https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-43892-x). Further complicating this matter, mixtures of phospholipids with different preferred curvatures are predicted to be softer than pure bilayers (e.g. https://doi.org/10.1021/acs.jpcb.3c08117), but asymmetric bilayers are stiffer than symmetric ones in some circumstances (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpj.2019.11.3398). 

      This issue can be quite thorny: therefore, my recommendation would be to either: (a) directly compute k for their lipid composition, which is straightforward when using large CG bilayers (as was done in Fowler et al, 2016), but it would also require more advanced methods for the atomistic ones; (b) use a reasonable *experimental* value for k, based on a similar enough lipid composition. 

      We now justify in somewhat more detail why we use an asymmetric membrane, but agree that his complicates the bending energy estimates. We only aim to estimate the bending energy in the Martini 2.2 force field, because our elasticity model is based on and, thus, limited to results obtained with this force field. We have included the two further references using the Martini 2.2 force field suggested by the reviewer on line 213, and discuss now in more detail how the bending rigidity estimate enters and affects the modeling, see lines 226ff.  

      (9) (Line 224). Does this closing statement imply that all experimental work from ex-vivo samples describe Piezo states under some small but measurable tension? 

      We compare here to the cryo-EM structure in detergent micelles. So, there is no membrane tension, there may be a surface tension of the micelle, but we assume here that Piezo proteins are essentially force free in detergent micelles. Membrane embedding, in contrast, leads to strong forces on Piezo proteins already in the absence of membrane tension, because of the membrane bending energy.

      (10) (Line 304). The Discussion concludes with a reasonable point, albeit on a down note: could the authors elaborate on what kind of experimental approach may be able to verify their modeling results? 

      Very good question, but this is somewhat beyond our expertise. We don’t have a clear recommendation – it is complicated. What can be verified is the flattening, i.e. the height and curvature of the nanodome in lower-resolution experiments. We see our results in line with these experiments, see Introduction. 

      (11) (Line 331). The very title of the Majumder and Straub paper addresses the problem of excessive binding strength between protein beads in the MARTINI force field, which should be mentioned. Figure 3(d) shows that the atomistic systems have larger excess areas than the CG ones. This could be related to MARTINI's "stickiness", or just statistical sampling. Characterizing the grid spacing (see point 7 above) might help illuminate this. 

      We discuss now the larger excess area values of the atomistic simulations on lines 381ff.  

      (12) (Lines 367, 375). Are the harmonic restraints absolute position restraints or additional bonds?

      Note also that the schedule at which the restraints are released (10-ns intervals) is relatively quick. Does the membrane have enough time to equilibrate the number of lipids in each leaflet? 

      These are standard, absolute position restraints. The 10-ns intervals may be too short to fully equilibrate the numbers of lipids, we have not explored this. The main point in the setup was to have a reasonable TM helix embedding with a smooth membrane, without any rupturing. This turned out to be tricky, with the procedures illustrated in Figure 8 as solution. If the membrane is smooth, the lipid numbers quickly equilibrate either in the final relaxation or in the initial nanoseconds of the production runs.

      (13) (Line 387) The use of an isotropic barostat for equilibration further impedes the system's ability to relax its structure. I feel that the authors should validate more strongly their protocol to rule out the possibility that incomplete equilibration could bias dynamics towards flatter membranes, which is one of the main results of this paper. 

      We don’t see how choices in the initial relaxation steps could have affected our results, at least for the coarse-grained simulations. There is more and more flattening throughout all simulation trajectories, see e.g. the extrapolations in Figure 4. All initial simulation structures are significantly less flattened than the final structures in the production runs.

      (14) (Line 403). What is the protocol for reducing the membrane size for atomistic simulation? This is even more important to mention than for CG simulations. 

      We just cut lipids beyond the intended box size of the atomistic simulations. As a technical point, we now have also added on line 507 how PIP2 lipids were converted.

      (15) (Line 423). The CHARMM force field requires a cut-off distance of 12 Å for van der Waals forces, with a force-based continuous switching scheme. The authors should briefly comment on this deviation and its possible impact on membrane properties. Quick test simulations of very small atomistic bilayers with the chosen composition could be used as a comparison. 

      We don’t expect any relevant effect on membrane properties within the statistical accuracies of the quantities of interest here (i.e. excess areas).

      (16) (Equation 4). There are some mismatched parentheses: please check. 

      We have corrected this mistake.

      (17) (Equations 7-8). Why did the authors use finite-differences derivatives of z(x,y) instead of using cubic splines and the corresponding analytical derivatives? 

      In our experience, second derivatives of standard cubic splines can be problematic. The continuous membrane shapes we obtain in our analysis are averages of such splines. We find standard finite differences more reliable, and therefore discretize these shapes. Already for the 2d membrane profiles of Figure 1b and 2d, calculating curvatures from interpolations using splines is problematic.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review)

      (1) The authors postulate a synergistic role for Itgb1 and Itgb3 in the intravasation phenotype, because the single KOs did not replicate the phenotype of the DKO. However, this is not a correct interpretation in the opinion of this reviewer. The roles appear rather to be redundant. Synergistic roles would rather demonstrate a modest effect in the single KO with potentiation in the DKO.

      We agree that the interaction between Itgb1 and Itgb3 appears redundant and we have corrected this point in the revised manuscript (page 10).

      (2) The experiment does not explain how these integrins influence the interaction of the MK with their microenvironment. It is not surprising that attachment will be impacted by the presence or absence of integrins. However, it is unclear how activation of integrins allows the MK to become "architects for their ECM microenvironment" as the authors posit. A transcriptomic analysis of control and DKO MKs may help elucidate these effects.

      We do not yet understand how the activation of α5β1 or αvβ3 integrins affects ECM remodeling by megakaryocytes. Integrins are key regulators of ECM remodeling (see https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ceb.2006.08.009) and can transmit traction forces that induce these changes (see https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpj.2008.10.009). Our previous study also found reduced RhoA activation in double knockout (DKO) megakaryocytes (MKs) (Guinard et al., 2023, PMID: 37171626), which likely affects ECM organization. These findings are discussed in the Discussion section of the paper (page 14).

      As suggested, conducting a transcriptomic analysis of control and DKO MKs may help to elucidate these effects. However, isolating native rare MKs from DKO mice is technically challenging and requires too many animals. To overcome this issue, we instead isolated mouse platelets and used targeted RT-PCR arrays to profile key ECM remodelling (ECM proteins, proteases…) and adhesion molecules (Zifkos et al., Circ. Res. 2024, PMID, 38563147). Quality controls confirmed that integrin RNA was undetectable in the DKO samples, ruling out contamination. Nevertheless, we found no significant expression differences exceeding the 3-fold change threshold between the control and DKO groups. The high Ct (threshold cycles) values indicate low transcript abundance, which may mask subtle changes (see the scatter plot below). As an example, we present a typical result obtained for the reviewer.

      Author response image 1.

      Relative expression comparison of ECM related-genes between control and DKO integrins in washed platelets. The figure shows a log transformation plot of the relative expression level of each gene between normal (x-axis) and DKO integrins (y-axis). The lines indicate the threefold change threshold for gene expression. These are representative results from two independent experiments.

      (3) Integrin DKO have a 50% reduction in platelets counts as reported previously, however laminin α4 deficiency only leads to 20% reduction in counts. This suggests a more nuanced and subtle role of the ECM in platelet growth. To this end, functional assays of the platelets in the KO and wildtype mice may provide more information.

      The exact contribution of the extracellular matrix (ECM) cage to platelet growth remains incompletely understood. In the Lamα4⁻/⁻ model, a collagen-rich ECM cage persists alongside normal fibronectin deposition. By contrast, the integrin DKO model exhibits a markedly severe phenotype characterized by the loss of both the laminin cage and collagen and the absence of fibrillar fibronectin. Also, the preserved collagen and fibronectin in Lamα4⁻/⁻ mice may permit residual activation of signaling pathways - potentially via integrins or alternative mechanisms- compared to the DKO model. We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback on this adjustment, which has been incorporated into the discussion (page 15).

      As suggested by the reviewer, we performed functional assays that demonstrated normal platelet function in Lamα4⁻/⁻ mice and impaired integrin-mediated aggregation in Itgb1<sup>-/-</sup>/Itgb3<sup>-/-</sup>  mice, as shown by the new data presented in the publication (see pages 7 and 9). Platelet function remained preserved following treatment with MMP inhibitors. This supports the idea that differences in ECM composition can influence the signaling environment and megakaryocyte maturation, but do not fully abrogate platelet function (page 15).

      (4) There is insufficient information in the Methods Section to understand the BM isolation approach. Did the authors flush the bone marrow and then image residual bone, or the extruded bone marrow itself as described in PMID: 29104956?

      Additional methodological information has been provided to clarify that only the extruded bone marrow, and not the bone itself, is isolated (page 17).

      (5) The references in the Methods section were very frustrating. The authors reference Eckly et al 2020 (PMID : 32702204) which provides no more detail but references a previous publication (PMID: 24152908), which also offers no information and references a further paper (PMID: 22008103), which, as far as this reviewer can tell, did not describe the methodology of in situ bone marrow imaging.

      To address this confusion, we have added the reference "In Situ Exploration of the Major Steps of Megakaryopoiesis Using Transmission Electron Microscopy" by C. Scandola et al. (PMID : 34570102) in the « Isolation and preservation of murine bone marrow » section (page 20), which provides a standardized protocol for bone marrow isolation and in situ bone marrow imaging.

      Therefore, this reviewer cannot tell how the preparation was performed and, importantly, how can we be sure that the microarchitecture of the tissue did not get distorted in the process?

      Thank you for pointing this out. While we cannot completely rule out the possibility of distortion, we have clarified the precautions taken to minimize it. We used a double fixation procedure immediately after bone marrow extrusion, followed by embedding it in agarose to preserve its integrity as much as possible. We have elaborated on this point in greater detail in the Methods section of the revised version (page 18).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      (1) ECM cage imaging

      (a) The value or additional information provided by the staining on nano-sections (A) is not clear, especially considering that the thick vibratome sections already display the entirety of the laminin γ1 cage structure effectively. Further clarification on the unique insights gained from each approach would help justify its inclusion.

      Ultrathin cryosectioning enables high-resolution imaging with a threefold increase in Z-resolution, facilitating precise analysis of signal superposition. This approach was particularly valuable for clearly visualizing activated integrin in contact with laminin and collagen IV fibers (see Fig. 3 in revised manuscript, pages 6, 8 and 18). Additionally, 3D reconstructions and z-stack data reveal complex interactions between the basement membrane and the cellular ECM cage that are not evident in 2D projections (see page 6). These complementary methods help elucidate the detailed molecular and three-dimensional organization of the ECM cage surrounding megakaryocytes. These points have been clarified in the method and result sections.

      (b) The sMK shown in Supplementary Figure 1C appears to be linked to two sinusoids, releasing proplatelets to the more distant vessels. Is this observation representative, and if so, can further discussion be provided?

      This observation is not representative; MKs can also be associated with just one sinusoid.

      (c) Freshly isolated BM-derived MKs are reported to maintain their laminin γ1 cage. Are the proportions of MKs with/without cages consistent with those observed in microscopy?   

      After mechanical dissociation and size exclusion, almost half of the MKs successfully retained their cages (53.4% ± 5.6%, based on 329 MKs from three experiments; see page 7 of the manuscript for new data). This highlights the strong physical connection between MK and their cage.

      (2) ECM cage formation

      (a) The statement "the full assembly of the 3D ECM cage required megakaryocyte interaction with the sinusoidal basement membrane" on page 7 is too strong given the data presented at this stage of the study. Supplemental Figure 1C shows that approximately 10% of pMKs form cages without direct vessel contact, indicating that other factors may also play a role in cage formation.

      The reviewer is correct. We have adjust the text to reflect a more cautious interpretation of our results. « Althought we cannot exclude that ECM cage can be form on its own, our data suggests that ECM cage assembly may require interactions between megakaryocytes and the sinusoidal basement membrane » suggests that the assembly of the 3D ECM cage may require interactions between megakaryocytes and the sinusoidal basement membrane » (page 7).

      (b) The data supporting the statement that "pMK represent a small fraction of the total MK population" (cell number or density) could be shown to help contextualize the 10% of them with a cage.

      Following the reviewer's recommendation, a new bar graph has been added to illustrate the 18 ± 1.3 % of MK in the parenchyma relative to the total MK in the bone marrow (page 7 and Suppl. Figure 1H).

      (c) How "the full assembly of the 3D ECM cage" is defined at this stage of the study should be clarified, specifically regarding the ECM components and structural features that characterize its completion.

      We recognize that the term ' full assembly' of the 3D ECM cage can be misleading, as it might suggest different stages of cage formation, such as a completed cage, one in the formation process, or an incomplete cage. Since we have not yet studied this concept, we have eliminate the term "full assembly" from the manuscript to avoid confusion. Instead, we mention the presence of a cage.

      (3) Data on MK Circulation and Cage Integrity: Does the cage require full component integrity to prevent MK release in circulation? Are circulating MKs found in Lama4-/- mice? Is the intravasation affected in these mice? Are the ~50% sinusoid associated MK functional?  

      In lamα4-deficient (Lamα4-/-) mice, which possess an intact collagen IV cage but a structurally compromised laminin cage, electron microscopy and whole-mount imaging revealed an absence of intact megakaryocytes within the sinusoidal lumen. This observation indicates that the structural integrity of all components of the ECM cage is critical for preventing megakaryocyte entry into the circulation. Despite the laminin deficiency, mature Lamα4-/- megakaryocytes exhibited normal ultrastructure and maintained typical intravasation behavior. Furthermore, analysis of bone marrow explants from Lamα4-/- mice demonstrated that megakaryocytes retained their capacity to extend proplatelets. These findings are presented on page 7 and further discussed on page 14.

      (4) Methodology

      (a) Details on fixation time are not provided, which is critical as it can impact antibody binding and staining. Including this information would improve reproducibility and feasibility for other researchers.

      We have included this information in the methods section.

      (b) The description of 'random length measuring' is unclear, and the rationale behind choosing random quantification should be explained. Additionally, in the shown image, it appears that only the branching ends were measured, which makes it difficult to discern the randomness in the measurements.

      The random length measurement method uses random sampling to provide unbiased data on laminin/collagen fibers in a 3D cage. Contrary to what the initial image might have suggested, measurements go beyond just the branching ends ; they include intervals between various branching points throughout the cage. This is now explained page 19.

      To clarify this process, we will outline these steps page 19 as : 1) acquire 3D images, 2) project onto 2D planar sections, 3) select random intersection points for measurement, 4) measure intervals using ImageJ software, and 5) repeat the process for a representative dataset. This will better illustrate the randomness of our measurements.

      (5) Figures

      (a) Overall, the figures and their corresponding legends would benefit from greater clarity if some panels were split, such as separating images from graph quantifications.

      Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we will fully update all the Figures and separate images from graph quantifications.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      (1) The data linking ECM cage formation to MK maturation raises several interesting questions. As the authors mention, MKs have been suggested to mature rapidly at the sinusoids, and both integrin KO and laminin KO MKs appear mislocalized away from the sinusoids. Additionally, average MK distances from the sinusoid may also help separate whether the maturation defects could be in part due to impaired migration towards CXCL12 at the sinusoid. Presumably, MKs could appear mislocalized away from the sinusoid given the data presented suggesting they leaving the BM and entering circulation. Additional data or commentary on intrinsic (ex-vivo) MK maturation phenotypes may help strengthen the author's conclusions and shed light on whether an essential function of the ECM cage is integrin activation at the sinusoid.

      The idea that megakaryocytes move toward CXCL12 is still debated. Some studies suggest mature MKs are mainly sessile (PMID: 28743899), while others propose that CXCL12 may guide MK progenitors rather than mature MKs (PMID: 38987596, this reference has been added). To address the reviewer’s concerns regarding CXCL12-mediated migration, we conducted additional investigations.

      For DKO integrins, Guinard et al. (2023, PMID: 37171626) reported no significant change in the distance between MKs and sinusoids, indicating that integrin deficiency does not impair MK migration toward sinusoidal vessels.

      In our own study involving Lamα4-/- mice, we utilized whole-mount bone marrow preparations, labeling MKs with GPIbβ antibodies and sinusoids with FABP4 antibodies. We observed a 1.6-fold increase in the proximity of MKs to sinusoids in Lamα4-/- mice compared to controls (see figure below). However, the absolute distances measured were less than 3 µm in both groups, much smaller than the average diameter of a mature MK (20 - 25 µm), raising questions about the biological significance of these findings in active MK migration. What happens with MK progenitors - a population not detectable in our experiments using morphological criteria or GPIb staining - remains an open question.

      These results are provided for the reviewer’s information and will be available to eLife readers, along with the authors’ responses, in the revised manuscript.

      Author response image 2.

      (2) The data demonstrating intact MKs in the circulation is intriguing - can the authors comment or provide evidence as to whether MKs are detectable in blood? A quantitative metric may strengthen these observations.

      To investigate this, we conducted flow cytometry experiments and prepared blood smears to determine the presence of intact Itgb1-/-/Itgb3-/- megakaryocytes in the blood. Unfortunately, we could not detect any intact megakaryocytes in the blood samples using FACS (see new Supplementary Figure 4E) nor any on the blood smears (data not shown). However, we observed that large, denuded megakaryocyte nuclei were retained in the downstream pulmonary capillaries of these mice. Intravital imaging of the lung has previously provided direct evidence for the phenomenon of microvascular trapping (Lefrançois et al., 2017; PMID: 28329764), demonstrating that megakaryocytes can be physically entrapped within the pulmonary circulation due to size exclusion while releasing platelets. This has been clarified in the revised paper (Results section, page 10).

      (3) Supplementary Figure 6 - shows no effect on in vitro MK maturation and proplt, or MK area - But Figures 6B/6C demonstrate an increase in total MK number in MMP-inhibitor treated mice compared to control. Some additional clarification in the text may substantiate the author's conclusions as to either the source of the MMPs or the in vitro environment not fully reflecting the complex and dynamic niche of the BM ECM in vivo.

      This is a valid point. We have revised the text to be more cautious and to provide further clarification on these points (page 12).

      (4) Similarly, one function of the ECM discussed relates to MK maturation but in the B1/3 integrin KO mice, the presence of the ECM cage is reduced but there appears to be no significant impact upon maturation (Supplementary Figure 4). By contrast, MMP inhibition in vivo (but not in vitro) reduces MK maturation. These data could be better clarified in the text, or by the addition of experiments addressing whether the composition and quantity of ECM cage components directly inhibit maturation versus whether effects of MMP-inhibitors perhaps lead to over-activation of the integrins (as with the B4galt KO in the discussion) are responsible for the differences in maturation.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out.

      In our study of DKO integrin mice with a reduced extracellular matrix (ECM) cage, we observed normal proportions of MK maturation stages. However, these mutant MKs had a disorganized membrane system and smaller cytoplasmic areas compared to wild-type cells, indicating issues in their maturation. This is detailed further in the manuscript (see page 9).

      In the context of MMP inhibition in vivo, which also leads to reduced MK maturation, our immunofluorescence analysis revealed in an increased presence of activated β1 integrin in bone marrow sections (see Supplementary Figure 6E). As suggested by the reviewer, this increase may explain the maturation defect.

      In summary, while it's challenging to definitively determine how ECM cage composition and quantity affect MK maturation in vivo, our results show that changes to the ECM cage - whether through genetic modification (DKO) or MMP inhibition - are consistently linked to defects in MK maturation.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Movies 1-3 are referenced in the Results section, but this reviewer was not able to find a movie file.

      They have now been added to the downloaded revised manuscript.

      (2) Figure 2D is referenced in the Results Section but this panel is not present in the Figure itself. Instead, this seems to be what is referred to as the right panel of 2C. 

      Thank you. Following the suggestion of reviewer 2, we have now split the panels and separated the images from the graph quantifications. This change has modified all the panel annotations, which we have carefully checked both in the legend and in the manuscript.

      (3) Supplemental Fig 3C has Fibrinogen quantification which seems to belong in Supplemental 3 F instead.  

      Supplementary Figure 3C serves as a control for immunofluorescence, indicating that no fibrinogen-positive granules are detectable in the DKO mice. This supports the conclusion that the αIIbβ3 integrin-mediated fibrinogen internalization pathway is non-functional in this model, affirming the bar graph's placement. We appreciate the reviewer’s insight that similar results may arise from the IEM experiments in Figure 3H, which is valuable for strengthening our findings.

      (4) The x-axis labels in Supplemental 5B are not uniform.  

      This has be done. Thank you.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figure 1 Panel C: The sinusoidal basement membrane staining is missing, making it difficult to conclude that the collagen IV organization extends radially from the sinusoidal basement membrane.

      As recommended by the reviewer, we have updated Figure 1C with a new image illustrating the basement membrane (FABP4 staining) and the collagen IV cage. This new image confirms that the cage extends radially from the basement membrane.

      (2) Arrows in 1B: Based on the arrow's localisation, the description of "basement membrane-cage connection" is not evident from the images as it looks like the signal colocalization (right lower panel) occurs below the highlighted areas. Clarification or additional evidence of co-localization is required. 

      The apparent localization of the signal "below" the highlighted areas in the maximal projection image is due to the nature of 2D projections, which compress overlapping signals from multiple depths within the bone marrow into a single plane. This can obscure the spatial relationship between the basement membrane and extracellular matrix (ECM) components. However, when the complete z-stack series is examined, the direct connection between the basement membrane and the ECM cage becomes evident in three dimensions. Therefore, we have now added a comprehensive analysis of the entire z-stack dataset, allowing us to accurately interpret the spatial relationships between the basement membrane and ECM in the native bone marrow microenvironments (movies 1 and 2, and Suppl. Figure 1D-E).

      (3) In Figure 4C, GPIX is used to identify MKs by IVM while GP1bβ is used throughout the rest of the manuscript. It would be helpful for readers who are less familiar with MKs to understand whether GPIX and GP1bβ identify the same population of MKs and the rationale for choosing one marker over the other.  

      GPIX and GPIbβ are components of the GPIb-IX complex, identifying mature megakaryocytes (Lepage et al., 2000, PMID : 11110688). The choice of one over the other in different experiments is primarily based on technical considerations. The intravital experiments have been standardized using an AF488-conjugated anti-GPIX to identify mature megakaryocytes consistently. GPIbβ (GP1bβ) is used in the rest of the manuscript due to its strong and specific bright staining. We have clarified this point in the Result (page 10) and in the Material/methods section (page 17).

      (4) The term "total number of MKs" is used (p8), but the associated data presented in the figure reflect MK density per surface area. Descriptions in the text should align with the data format in the figures.

      This has been corrected in the revised manuscript (page 8). Thank you.

      (5) Supplemental Figure 1(B): Collagen I is written as Collagen III in the legend.

      This has been corrected in the legend of the Figure 1B.

      (6) Figure 2D is described in the text but is missing from the figure.

      This has been corrected.

      (7) Supplemental Figure 3: Plot E overlaps with the images, making it unclear.

      To minimise overlap with the images, we've moved the graph with the bars down. Thank you.

      (8) Supplemental Figure 7: The image quality is too low, and spelling underlining issues are present. A better-quality version with clear labelling is essential.

      We have improved the quality of Figure 7 and fixed the underlining problems.

      (9) The movies were not found in the downloads provided.

      They have now been added to the downloaded revised manuscript.

      (10) Some bar graphs are missing the individual data points.

      All figures have been standardized and now include the individual data points.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Some minor comments:

      (1) If there is specific importance to some of the analyses of the cage structure, such as fiber length, and pore size, (eg. if they may have biological significance to the MK) it may help readers to give additional context to what differences in the pore size might imply. For example, do pores constrain MKs at sites where actin-driven proplatelet formation could be initiated?

      The effects of extracellular matrix (ECM) features - like fiber length and pore size - on megakaryocyte (MK) biology are not fully understood. Longer ECM fibers may help MKs adhere better and sense their environment. Larger pores could make it easier for MKs to grow, communicate, and extend proplatelets through blood vessel walls. The role of matrix metalloproteinases (MMPs), which degrade the ECM, adds to the complexity, and how this occurs in vivo is not yet well understood.

      As suggested, some of these points have been addressed in the revised manuscript (Discussion, page 16).

      (2) "Although fibronectin and fibrinogen were readily detected around megakaryocytes, a reticular network around megakaryocytes was not observed. Furthermore, no connection was identified between fibronectin and fibrinogen deposition with the sinusoid basement membrane, in contrast to the findings for laminin and collagen IV (Supp. Figures 1E)." - Clarification of how these data are interpreted might be helpful as to what the authors are intending to demonstrate with these data as at least in Figure 1E, fibronectin, and fibrinogen do appear expressed along the MK surface and at the sinusoidal-MK interface.

      While fibronectin and fibrinogen are present around megakaryocytes and at the vessel-cell interface, they do not form a reticular ECM cage. The functional implications of this finding remain unclear. One can imagine that the specific spatial arrangement of various ECM components may lead to different functional roles. Laminin and collagen IV may provide structural support by forming a 3D cage that is essential for the proper positioning and maturation of megakaryocytes. In contrast, fibronectin and fibrinogen may have different functions, potentially related to megakaryocyte expansion in bone marrow fibrosis (Malara et al., 2019, PMID : 30733282) and (Matsuura et al., 2020, PMID : 32294178).  

      This topic has been adressed in the Results page 7 and discussion on page 13.

      (3) Given the effects of dual B1/B3 integrin inhibition on MK intravasation, can the authors comment on the use of integrin RGD-based inhibitors? Are these compounds and drugs likely to interfere with MK retention?

      Our study shows that MK retention depends on the integrity of both components of the cage, collagen IV and laminin (see also point 3 of reviewer 2). Collagen IV contains RGD sequences, making it susceptible to RGD-based inhibition, whereas laminin does not utilize the RGD motif, raising questions about the overall efficacy of these inhibitors.

      In addition, the in vivo efficacy and potential off-target effects of these inhibitors in the complex bone marrow microenvironment remain to be fully elucidated. This intriguing issue warrants further investigation.

      (4) Beyond protein components, other non-protein ECM molecules including glycosaminoglycans (HA, HS) have essential roles in supporting MK function, including maturation (PMIDs: 31436532, 36066492, 27398974) and may merit some brief discussion if the authors feel this is helpful.

      We followed reviewer’s suggestion and mention the contribution of glycoaminoglycans in MK maturation. We also added the three references (page 13). 

      (5) In several locations, the text refers to figure panels that are either not present or not annotated correctly (some examples include Figure 2D, Supplementary Figure 3E vs 3D).

      Following the suggestion of reviewer 2, we have now split the panels and separated the images from the graph quantifications. This change has changed all the panel annotations, which we have carefully checked both in the legend and in the manuscript.

      (6) In some cases, the figure legends seem to incorrectly refer to text, colors, or elements in the panels (e.g. Supplementary Figure 3, fibrinogen is referred to as yellow in the legend but is green in the figure). In Supplemental Figure 1, an image is annotated as pryenocyte in the figure, but splenocyte in the text.

      This has been corrected in the figures and in the revised manuscript. Please also see point (7) below.  Thank you very much.

      (7) Images demonstrating GPIX and GPIBb positive cells in the calvarial and lung microcirculation are convincing, but in Figure C these cells are referred to as MKs, whereas in Figure D they are referred to as pyrenocytes (as well as in the discussion). It is not clear if this is intentional and refers to bare nuclei from erythrocytes or indeed refers to MKs or MK nuclei. Clarification would help guide readers.

      We agree with the reviewer and fully acknowledge the need for clarification. We confirm that these circulating cells are megakaryocytes. To avoid confusion, we have ensure that all references to "pyrenocytes" have been replaced with "megakaryocytes."

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Because many conclusions are drawn from overexpression studies and from a single cell line (HEK293), it is unclear how general these effects are. In particular, one of the main claims put forth in this manuscript is that of specificity, namely, that FZD5/8, and none of the other FZDs, are uniquely involved in this internalization and degradation. While there are examples of similar specificities, many of these examples can be attributed to a particular cellular context. Without demonstrating that this FZD5/8 specificity is observed in multiple cell lines and contexts, this point remains unconvincing and questionable. One way to address this point of criticism is to omit the word "specifically" in the title and soften the language concerning this idea throughout the manuscript.

      We appreciate your valuable comments and suggestions. We have removed the word “specifically” from the title and softened the language concerning this idea throughout the manuscript. Moreover, we performed new experiments to show that Wnt3a/5a induces FZD5/8 endocytosis and degradation and that IWP-2 treatment increases the cell surface levels of FZD5/8 in cell lines other than 293A (Figure 1-Figure supplement 1 and Figure 2-Figure supplement 1). These results indicate that Wnt-induced FZD5/8 endocytosis and degradation are not cell specific.

      The starting point for these studies is a survey of all 10 FZDs, V5-tagged and overexpressed in HEK293 cells. Here, the authors observed a decline in cell surface levels of only FZD5 and 8 in response to Wnt3a and Wnt5a. As illustrated in the immunoblot (Fig 1B), several FZDs were poorly expressed, including FZD1, 3, 6 and 9, which calls into question that only FZD5 and 8 were affected. Furthermore, total levels of FZD8 don't diminish appreciably, as claimed by the authors, and only FZD5 shows a subtle decline upon WNT treatment. All of these experiments are performed with overexpressed V5-tagged FZD proteins or with endogenously V5-tagged (KI) proteins, and it is possible that overexpression or tagging lead to potentially artifactual observations. Examining the effects of WNTs on FZD protein localization and levels need to be done with endogenously expressed, non-tagged FZDs. In this context, it is somewhat puzzling that the authors don't show such an experiment using the pan- and FZD5/8-specific antibodies, which they use in multiple experiments throughout the manuscript. With these available tools it should be possible to examine FZD levels at the cell surface in response to Wnt3a and Wnt5a, ideally in multiple cell lines.

      We appreciate your valuable comments and suggestions. Figure 1B shows the results of the follow-up study shown in Figure 1A. As shown in Figure 1A, we used flow cytometry analysis to detect the cell surface levels of stably expressed FZDs and found that Wnt3a/5a specifically reduced the levels of FZD5/8 on the cell surface, suggesting that Wnt3a/5a induces FZD5/8 endocytosis. As shown in Figure 1B and C, we performed immunoblotting to examine whether Wnt3a/5a-induced FZD5/8 internalization resulted in FZD5/8 degradation. Notably, most FZDs exhibit two bands on immunoblots, as also suggested by other published studies, and the upper bands represent the mature form that is fully glycosylated and presented to the cell surface (see also new Figure 2L), whereas the lower bands represent the immature form. Our results clearly indicated that Wnt3a/5a treatment reduced the levels of the mature forms of both FZD5 and FZD8, although the immunoblotting signals of the mature form of FZD8 (upper bands) were relatively weak. The immunoblotting signals of the other FZDs varied, and some of them (including FZD1, -3, -6 and -9) were relatively weak; however, according to the results in Figure 1A, all of the FZDs were expressed and present on the cell surface.

      Commercially available FZD5/8 antibodies, including those used in published studies, cannot detect endogenous FZD5/8 or can only recognize immature FZD5 in our hands, which is why we have to use the CRISPR-CAS9-based KI technique to introduce a V5 tag to FZD5 and FZD7. Notably, in the overexpression experiments, the V5 tag is on the amino terminus, and in the KI experiments, the V5 tag is on the carboxyl terminus of FZDs, which may minimize the potential artificial effects of the V5 tag on the immunoblotting assays.

      The monoclonal antibodies used in this study, such as anti-pan-FZD, anti-FZD5/8, and anti-FZD4 antibodies, are neutralizing antibodies that can compete with Wnt ligands to bind to the FZD CRD. These antibodies have been successfully used to detect the surface levels of FZDs via flow cytometry assays. However, as the binding affinity of the Wnt-FZD CRD is comparable to the binding affinity of the antibody-FZD, we were cautious in using these antibodies to detect the cell surface levels of FZDs when the cells were treated with Wnt3a/5a CM, which contains relatively high concentrations of Wnt3a/5a. As shown in Author response image 1, Wnt3a or Wnt5a treatment dramatically reduced the endogenous cell surface level of FZD5/8, as detected by flow cytometry using the anti-FZD5/8 antibody. However, in another experiment, HEK293A cells were first incubated with cold Wnt3a or Wnt5a CM at 4°C to minimize endocytosis and then analyzed via flow cytometry using the anti-FZD5/8 antibody. The results showed that Wnt3a/5a incubation reduced the floe cytometry signals, suggesting that Wnt3a/5a binding to FZD5/8 might interfere with antibody-FZD5/8 binding, although we cannot exclude the possibility that Wnt3a/5a may induce FZD5/8 endocytosis at 4°C (Author response image 1).

      Author response image 1.

      (A) HEK293A cells were treated with control, Wnt3a or Wnt5a CM for 2 hours at 37°C in a humidified incubator and were analyzed via flow cytometry using the anti-FZD5/8 antibody.

      (B) HEK293A cells were incubated with control, Wnt3a or Wnt5a CM for 1 h at 4°C and analyzed by flow cytometry using the anti-FZD5/8 antibody.

       

      Several experiments rely on gene-edited clonal cell lines, including knockouts of FZD5/8, RNF43/ZNRF3, and DVL. Gene knockouts were confirmed by genomic DNA sequencing and, for DVL and FZD5/8, by loss of protein expression. While these KO lines are powerful tools to study gene function, there is a concern for clonal variability. Each cell line may have acquired additional changes as a result of gene editing. In addition, there may be compensatory changes in gene expression as a consequence of the loss of certain genes. For example, expression of other FZDs may increase in FZD5/8 DKO cells. To address this critique, the authors should show that re-expression of the knocked-out genes rescues the observed effect. This is done in some instances (Fig 5E, G, H) but not in other instances, such as with the DVL TKO (Fig. 3). Since the authors assert that DVL is important for FZD internalization in the absence of WNT, but not for FZD internalization in the presence of WNT, this particular rescue experiment is important. This is a potentially important finding and it should be confirmed by re-expression of DVL in the TKO line. As an alternative, conditional knockdown using Tet-inducible shRNA expression could address concerns for clonal variability.

      We appreciate your valuable comments and suggestions. We re-expressed DVL2 in DVLTKO cells stably expressing V5-linker-FZD5 or V5-linker-FZD7. As shown in Figure 3G-K, re-expression of DVL2 rescued the decreased Wnt-independent endocytosis of FZD5 and FZD7 caused by DVL1/2/3 knockout.

      Given the significant differences in signaling activity by Wnt3a and Wnt5a, it is somewhat surprising that all experiments shown in this manuscript do not identify distinguishing features between Wnt3a and Wnt5a. In addition, it is unclear why the authors switch between Wnt3a and Wnt5a. For example, Figures 1C, 3G-J, 4C-D only use Wnt5a. In contrast, Figures 6E and H use Wnt3a, most likely because b-catenin stabilization is examined, an effect generally not observed with Wnt5a. The choice of which Wnt is examined/used appears to be somewhat arbitrary and the authors never provide any explanations for these choices. In the end, this type of inconsistency becomes puzzling when the authors present, quite convincingly, in Figure 7, that both Wnt3a and 5a promote an interaction between FZD5/8 and RNF43 through proximity biotin labeling.

      Although Wnt3a and Wnt5a are significantly different in triggering intracellular signaling pathways, both bind FZD5/8 and induce FZD5/8 endocytosis and degradation similarly. When FZD5 is stably overexpressed, Wnt5a has slightly stronger effects on inducing FZD5 endocytosis and degradation, possibly because the Wnt5a concentration may be higher than the Wnt3a concentration in our CM, which is why we used Wnt5a CM in some experiments when V5-FZD5 was overexpressed. In the revised manuscript, we used both Wnt3a and Wnt5a CM in the experiments as you suggested, as shown in Figure 1C, 3G-K and Figure 4-Figure supplement 1.

      Minor Points:

      Figure 3G and I: it is curious that individual cells are shown in the "0 h" samples, while the "Con 1 h" and "Wnt5a 1 h" show multiple cells with several making direct contact with each other. This is notable because the V5 staining at sites of cell-cell contact are quite distinct and variable between control and Wnt5a-treated and WT versus DVL TKO cells. Also, sub-cellular localization of FZD5 (V5 tag) puncta is quite distinct between Con and Wnt5a: puncta in Wnt5a-treated cells appear to be more plasma membrane proximal than in Con cells. These points may be easy to address by showing images of cells that are more similar with respect to cell number and density for each condition.

      Thank you for your suggestions. We repeated these experiments and added Wnt3a treatment and adjusted the cell density. Images including an individual cell were selected for presentation.

      Figure 5E: the following statement is confusing/misleading: "Furthermore, reintroducing ZNRF3 or RNF43 into ZRDKO cells efficiently restored the increase in cytosolic β-catenin levels, whereas the expression of RNF130 or RNF150, two structurally similar transmembrane E3 ubiquitin ligases, did not (Fig. 5E)." First, reintroduction of ZNRF3 or RNF43 restores cytosolic b-catenin levels; it does not restore the increase in b-catenin. Second, the claim that RNF130 fails to have this effect is not substantiated since it is barely expressed.

      Thank you for your suggestions and comments. We reorganized the language to make the statement clearer. Notably, the expression level of RNF130 was relatively low compared with that of other E3 ligases, but RNF130 was expressed (Figure 5E darker exposure) and could reduce the cell surface levels of FZDs, as shown in Figure 5G.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Given their results the authors conclude that upregulation of Frizzled on the plasma membrane is not sufficient to explain the stabilization of beta-catenin seen in the ZNRF3/RNF43 mutant cells. This interpretation is sound, and they suggest in the discussion that ZNRF3/RNF43-mediated ubiquitination could serve as a sorting signal to sort endocytosed FZD to lysosomes for degradation and that absence or inhibition of this process would promote FZD recycling. This should be relatively easy to test using surface biotinylation experiments and would considerably strengthen the manuscript.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestions and comments. We performed cell surface biotinylation experiments in HEK293A FZD5KI cells, as shown in Figure 2L. The results indicated that Wnt3a or Wnt5a treatment induced the degradation of FZD5 on the cell surface, which was antagonized by cotreatment with RSPO1. We did not perform a more detailed endocytosis/recycling biotinylation experiment that requires complex reversible biotinylation and multiple washing steps because HEK293A cells are fragile in culture and not easy to handle. Furthermore, the results shown in Figure 4 indicate that knockout of ZNRF3/RNF43 or RSPO1 significantly blocked the degradation of internalized FZD5 and reduced the colocalization of internalized FZD5 with lysosomal markers, suggesting that Wnt3a/5a induced lysosomal degradation of FZD5 in the presence of ZNRF3/RNF43 and that the internalized FZD5 was most likely recycled back to the cell surface when ZNRF3/RNF43 was knocked out or inhibited by RSPO1.

      (2) The authors show that the FZD5 CRD domain is required for endocytosis since a mutant FZD5 protein in which the CRD is removed does not undergo endocytosis. This is perhaps not surprising since this is the site of Wnt binding, but the authors show that a chimeric FZD5CRD-FZD4 receptor can confer Wnt-dependent endocytosis to an otherwise endocytosis incompetent FZD4 protein. Since the linker region between the CRD and the first TM differs between FZD5 and FZD4, it would be interesting to understand whether the CRD specifically or the overall arrangement (such as the spacing) is the most important determinant.

      Our results in Figure 1D-H clearly show that the CRD of FZD5 specifically is both necessary and sufficient for Wnt3a/5a-induced FZD5 endocytosis, as replacing the CRD alone in FZD5 with the CRD from either FZD4 or FZD7 completely abolished Wnt-induced endocytosis, whereas replacing the CRD alone in FZD4 or FZD7 with the FZD5 CRD alone could confer Wnt-induced endocytosis.

      (3) I find it surprising that only FZD5 and FZD8 appear to undergo endocytosis or be stabilized at the cell surface upon ZNRF3/RNF43 knockout. Is this consistent with previous literature? Is that a cell-specific feature? These findings should be tested in a different cell line, with possibly different relative levels of ZNRF3 and RNF43 expression.

      Thank you for your comments and suggestions. Our finding that ZNRF3/RNF43 specifically regulates FZD5/8 degradation is consistent with recent published studies in which FZD5 is required for the survival of RNF43-mutant PDAC or colorectal cancer cells (Nature Medicine, 2017, PMID: 27869803) and FZD5 is required for the maintenance of intestinal stem cells (Developmental Cell, 2024, PMID: 39579768 and 39579769), and in both cases, FZDs other than FZD5/8 are also expressed but not sufficient to compensate for the function of FZD5. The mechanism by which Wnt3a/5a specifically induces FZD5/8 endocytosis and degradation is currently unknown and needs to be explored in the future. We speculate that Wnt binding to FZD5/8 may recruit another protein on the cell surface to specifically facilitate FZD5/8 endocytosis. On the other hand, we cannot exclude the possibility that Wnts other than Wnt3a/5a may induce the endocytosis and degradation of FZDs other than FZD5/8 since there are 19 Wnts and 10 FZDs in humans. Notably, several previous studies have suggested that ZNRF3/RNF43 may regulate the endocytosis and degradation of all FZDs without selectivity (such as Nature, 2012, PMID: 22575959; Nature, 2012, PMID: 22895187; Mol Cell, 2015, PMID: 25891077). However, their conclusions were drawn mostly on the basis of overexpression studies. According to the results shown in Figure 5E-H, overexpressing a membrane-tethered E3 ligase (such as ZNRF3, RNF43, RNF130, or RNF150) may nonspecifically degrade FZD proteins on the cell surface.

      Furthermore, in the revised manuscript, we showed that Wnt3a/5a induced FZD5/8 endocytosis and degradation in multiple cell lines, including Huh7, U2OS, MCF7, and 769P cells (Figure 1-Figure supplement 1 and Figure 2-Figure supplement 1), suggesting that these phenomena are not specific to 293A cells.

      (4) If FZD7 is not a substrate of ZNRF3/RNF43 and therefore is not ubiquitinated and degraded, how do the authors reconcile that its overexpression does not lead to elevated cytosolic beta-catenin levels in Figure 5B?

      We are currently not sure of the mechanism underlying this result. Considering that most FZDs are expressed in 293A cells, we do not know how much of the mature form of overexpressed FZD7 was presented to the plasma membrane.

      (5) For Figure 5B, it would be interesting if the authors could evaluate whether overexpression of FZD5 in the ZNRF3/RNF43 double knockout lines would synergize and lead to further increase in cytosolic beta-catenin levels. As control if the substrate selectivity is clear FZD7 overexpression in that line should not do anything.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We performed these experiments as suggested, and the results indicated that overexpressing FZD5 further increased cytosolic beta-catenin levels in ZRDKO cells, whereas FZD7 had no effect (Figure 6D).

      (6) In Figure 6G, the authors need to show cytosolic levels of beta-catenin in the absence of Wnt in all cases.

      We did not add Wnt CM in this experiment. RSPO1 activity, which relies on endogenous Wnt, has been well documented in previous studies.

      (7) Since the authors show that DVL is not involved in the Wnt and ZRNF3-dependent endocytosis they should repeat the proximity biotinylation experiment in figure 7 in the DVL triple KO cells. This is an important experiment since previous studies showed that DVL was required for the ZRNF3/RNF43-mediated ubiqtuonation of FZD.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestions. As you suggested, we performed a proximity biotinylation experiment in DVL TKO cells, and the results showed that Wnt3a/5a could still induce the interaction of FZD5 and RNF43 in DVLTKO cells (Figure 7-figure supplement 1), suggesting that the Wnt-induced FZD5‒RNF43 interaction is DVL independent.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Gerken et al examined how neurons in the human medial temporal lobe respond to and potentially code dynamic movie content. They had 29 patients watch a long-form movie while neurons within their MTL were monitored using depth electrodes. They found that neurons throughout the region were responsive to the content of the movie. In particular, neurons showed significant responses to people, places, and to a lesser extent, movie cuts. Modeling with a neural network suggests that neural activity within the recorded regions was better at predicting the content of the movies as a population, as opposed to individual neural representations. Surprisingly, a subpopulation of unresponsive neurons performed better than the responsive neurons at decoding the movie content, further suggesting that while classically nonresponsive, these neurons nonetheless provided critical information about the content of the visual world. The authors conclude from these results that low-level visual features, such as scene cuts, may be coded at the neuronal level, but that semantic features rely on distributed population-level codes.

      Strengths:

      Overall, the manuscript presents an interesting and reasonable argument for their findings and conclusions. Additionally, the large number of patients and neurons that were recorded and analyzed makes this data set unique and potentially very powerful. On the whole, the manuscript was very well written, and as it is, presents an interesting and useful set of data about the intricacies of how dynamic naturalistic semantic information may be processed within the medial temporal lobe.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments on our manuscript and for describing the strengths of our presented work

      Weaknesses:

      There are a number of concerns I have based on some of the experimental and statistical methods employed that I feel would help to improve our understanding of the current data.

      In particular, the authors do not address the issue of superposed visual features very well throughout the manuscript. Previous research using naturalistic movies has shown that low-level visual features, particularly motion, are capable of driving much of the visual system (e.g, Bartels et al 2005; Bartels et al 2007; Huth et al 2012; Çukur et al 2013; Russ et al 2015; Nentwich et al 2023). In some of these papers, low-level features were regressed out to look at the influence of semantics, in others, the influence of low-level features was explicitly modeled. The current manuscript, for the most part, appears to ignore these features with the exception of scene cuts. Based on the previous evidence that low-level features continue to drive later cortical regions, it seems like including these as regressors of no interest or, more ideally, as additional variables, would help to determine how well MTL codes for semantic features over top of these lower-order variables.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment and for the relevant literature regarding visual motion in not only the primary visual system but in cortical areas as well. While we agree that the inclusion of visual motion as a regressor of no interest or as an additional variable would be overall informative in determining if single neurons in the MTL are driven by this level of feature, we would argue that our analyses already provide some insight into its role and that only the parahippocampal cortical neurons would robustly track this feature.

      As noted by the reviewer, our model includes two features derived from visual motion: Camera Cuts (directly derived from frame-wise changes in pixel values)  and Scene Cuts (a subset of Camera Cuts restricted to changes in scene). As shown in Fig. 5a, decoding performance for these features was strongest in the parahippocampal cortex (~20%), compared to other MTL areas (~10%). While the entorhinal cortex also showed some performance for Scene Cuts (15%), we interpret this as being driven by the changes in location that define a scene, rather than by motion itself.

      These findings suggest that while motion features are tracked in the MTL, the effect may be most robust in the parahippocampal cortex. We believe that quantifying more complex 3D motion in a naturalistic stimulus like a full-length movie is a significant challenge that would likely require a dedicated study. We agree this is an interesting future research direction and will update the manuscript to highlight this for the reader.

      A few more minor points that would help to clarify the current results involve the selection of data for particular analyses. For some analyses, the authors chose to appropriately downsample their data sets to compare across variables. However, there are a few places where similar downsampling would be informative, but was not completed. In particular, the analyses for patients and regions may have a more informative comparison if the full population were downsampled to match the size of the population for each patient or region of interest. This could be done with the Monte Carlo sampling that is used in other analyses, thus providing a control for population size while still sampling the full population.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important methodological point. The decision not to downsample the patient- and region-specific analyses was deliberate, and we appreciate the opportunity to clarify our rationale.

      Generally, we would like to emphasize that due to technical and ethical limitations of human single-neuron recordings, it is currently not possible to record large populations of neurons simultaneously in individual patients. The limited and variable number of recorded neurons per subject (Fig. S1) generally requires pooling neurons into a pseudo-populations for decoding, which is a well‐established standard in human single‐neuron studies (see e.g., (Jamali et al., 2021; Kamiński et al., 2017; Minxha et al., 2020; Rutishauser et al., 2015; Zheng et al., 2022)).

      For the patient-specific analysis, our primary goal was to show that no single patient's data could match the performance of the complete pseudo-population. Crucially, we found no direct relationship between the number of recorded neurons and decoding performance; patients with the most neurons (patients 4, 13) were not top performers, and those with the fewest (patients 11, 14) were not the worst (see Fig. 4). This indicates that neuron count was not the primary limiting factor and that downsampling would be unlikely to provide additional insight.

      Similarly, for the region-specific analysis, regions with larger neural populations did not systematically outperform those with fewer neurons (Fig. 5). Given the inherent sparseness of single-neuron data, we concluded that retaining the full dataset was more informative than excluding neurons simply to equalize population sizes.

      We agree that this methodological choice should be transparent and explicitly justified in the text. We will add an explanation to the revised manuscript to justify why this approach was taken and how it differs from the analysis in Fig. 6.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study introduces an exciting dataset of single-unit responses in humans during a naturalistic and dynamic movie stimulus, with recordings from multiple regions within the medial temporal lobe. The authors use both a traditional firing-rate analysis as well as a sophisticated decoding analysis to connect these neural responses to the visual content of the movie, such as which character is currently on screen.

      Strengths:

      The results reveal some surprising similarities and differences between these two kinds of analyses. For visual transitions (such as camera angle cuts), the neurons identified in the traditional response analysis (looking for changes in firing rate of an individual neuron at a transition) were the most useful for doing population-level decoding of these cuts. Interestingly, this wasn't true for character decoding; excluding these "responsive" neurons largely did not impact population-level decoding, suggesting that the population representation is distributed and not well-captured by individual-neuron analyses.

      The methods and results are well-described both in the text and in the figures. This work could be an excellent starting point for further research on this topic to understand the complex representational dynamics of single neurons during naturalistic perception.

      We thank the reviewer for their feedback and for summarizing the results of our work.

      (1) I am unsure what the central scientific questions of this work are, and how the findings should impact our understanding of neural representations. Among the questions listed in the introduction is "Which brain regions are informative for specific stimulus categories?". This is a broad research area that has been addressed in many neuroimaging studies for decades, and it's not clear that the results tell us new information about region selectivity. "Is the relevant information distributed across the neuronal population?" is also a question with a long history of work in neuroscience about localist vs distributed representations, so I did not understand what specific claim was being made and tested here. Responses in individual neurons were found for all features across many regions (e.g., Table S1), but decodable information was also spread across the population.

      We thank the reviewer for this important point, which gets to the core of our study's contribution. While concepts like regional specificity are well-established from studies on the blood-flow level, their investigation at the single-neuron level in humans during naturalistic, dynamic stimulation remains a critical open question. The type of coding (sparse vs. distributed) on the other hand cannot be investigated with blood-flow studies as the technology lacks the spatial and temporal resolution.

      Our study addresses this gap directly. The exceptional temporal resolution of single-neuron recordings allows us to move beyond traditional paradigms and examine cellular-level dynamics as they unfold in neuronal response on a frame-by-frame basis to a more naturalistic and ecologically valid stimulus. It cannot be assumed that findings from other modalities or simplified stimuli will generalize to this context.

      To meet this challenge, we employed a dual analytical strategy: combining a classic single-unit approach with a machine learning-based population analysis. This allowed us to create a bridge between prior work and our more naturalistic data. A key result is that our findings are often consistent with the existing literature, which validates the generalizability of those principles. However, the differences we observe between these two analytical approaches are equally informative, providing new insights into how the brain processes continuous, real-world information.

      We will revise the introduction and discussion to more explicitly frame our work in this context, emphasizing the specific scientific question driving this study, while also highlighting the strengths of our experimental design and recording methods.

      (2) The character and indoor/outdoor labels seem fundamentally different from the scene/camera cut labels, and I was confused by the way that the cuts were put into the decoding framework. The decoding analyses took a 1600ms window around a frame of the video (despite labeling these as frame "onsets" like the feature onsets in the responsive-neuron analysis, I believe this is for any frame regardless of whether it is the onset of a feature), with the goal of predicting a binary label for that frame. Although this makes sense for the character and indoor/outdoor labels, which are a property of a specific frame, it is confusing for the cut labels since these are inherently about a change across frames. The way the authors handle this is by labeling frames as cuts if they are in the 520ms following a cut (there is no justification given for this specific value). Since the input to a decoder is 1600ms, this seems like a challenging decoding setup; the model must respond that an input is a "cut" if there is a cut-specific pattern present approximately in the middle of the window, but not if the pattern appears near the sides of the window. A more straightforward approach would be, for example, to try to discriminate between windows just after a cut versus windows during other parts of the video. It is also unclear how neurons "responsive" to cuts were defined, since the authors state that this was determined by looking for times when a feature was absent for 1000ms to continuously present for 1000ms, which would never happen for cuts (unless this definition was different for cuts?).

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable comment regarding specifically the cut labels. The choice to label frames that lie in a time window of 520ms following a cut as positive was selected based on prior research and is intended to include the response onsets across all regions within the MTL (Mormann et al., 2008). We agree that this explanation is currently missing from the manuscript, and we will add a brief clarification in the revised version.

      As correctly noted, the decoding analysis does not rely on feature onset but instead continuously decodes features throughout the entire movie. Thus, all frames are included, regardless of whether they correspond to a feature onset.

      Our treatment of cut labels as sustained events is a deliberate methodological choice. Neural responses to events like cuts often unfold over time, and by extending the label, we provide our LSTM network with the necessary temporal window to learn this evolving signature. This approach not only leverages the sequential processing strengths of the LSTM (Hochreiter et al., 1997) but also ensures a consistent analytical framework for both event-based (cuts) and state-based (character or location) features.

      (3) The architecture of the decoding model is interesting but needs more explanation. The data is preprocessed with "a linear layer of same size as the input" (is this a layer added to the LSTM that is also trained for classification, or a separate step?), and the number of linear layers after the LSTM is "adapted" for each label type (how many were used for each label?). The LSTM also gets to see data from 800 ms before and after the labeled frame, but usually LSTMs have internal parameters that are the same for all timesteps; can the model know when the "critical" central frame is being input versus the context, i.e., are the inputs temporally tagged in some way? This may not be a big issue for the character or location labels, which appear to be contiguous over long durations and therefore the same label would usually be present for all 1600ms, but this seems like a major issue for the cut labels since the window will include a mix of frames with opposite labels.

      We thank the reviewer for their insightful comments regarding the decoding architecture. The model consists of an LSTM followed by 1–3 linear readout layers, where the exact number of layers is treated as a hyperparameter and selected based on validation performance for each label type. The initial linear layer applied to the input is part of the trainable model and serves as a projection layer to transform the binned neural activity into a suitable feature space before feeding it into the LSTM. The model is trained in an end-to-end fashion on the classification task.

      Regarding temporal context, the model receives a 1600 ms window (800 ms before and after the labeled frame), and as correctly pointed out by the reviewer, LSTM parameters are shared across time steps. We do not explicitly tag the temporal position of the central frame within the sequence. While this may have limited impact for labels that persist over time (e.g., characters or locations), we agree this could pose a challenge for cut labels, which are more temporally localized.

      This is an important point, and we will clarify this limitation in the revised manuscript and consider incorporating positional encoding in future work to better guide the model’s focus within the temporal window. Additionally, we will add a data table, specifying the ranges of hyperparameters in our decoding networks. Hyperparameters were optimized for each feature and split individually, but we agree that some more details on how these parameters were chosen are important and we will provide a data table in our revised manuscript giving more insights into the ranges of hyperparameters.

      We thank the reviewer for this important point. We will clarify this limitation in the revised manuscript and note that positional encoding is a valuable direction to better guide the model’s focus within the temporal window. To improve methodological transparency, we will also add a supplementary table detailing the hyperparameter ranges used for our optimization process.

      (4) Because this is a naturalistic stimulus, some labels are very imbalanced ("Persons" appears in almost every frame), and the labels are correlated. The authors attempt to address the imbalance issue by oversampling the minority class during training, though it's not clear this is the right approach since the test data does not appear to be oversampled; for example, training the Persons decoder to label 50% of training frames as having people seems like it could lead to poor performance on a test set with nearly 100% Persons frames, versus a model trained to be biased toward the most common class. [...]

      We thank the reviewer for this critical and thoughtful comment. We agree that the imbalanced and correlated nature of labels in naturalistic stimuli is a key challenge.

      To address this, we follow a standard machine learning practice: oversampling is applied exclusively to the training data. This technique helps the model learn from underrepresented classes by creating more balanced training batches, thus preventing it from simply defaulting to the majority class. Crucially, the test set remains unaltered to ensure our evaluation reflects the model's true generalization performance on the natural data distribution.

      For the “Persons” feature, which appears in nearly all frames, defining a meaningful negative class is particularly challenging. The decoder must learn to identify subtle variations within a highly skewed distribution. Oversampling during training helps provide a more balanced learning signal, while keeping the test distribution intact ensures proper evaluation of generalization.

      The reviewer’s comment—that we are “training the Persons decoder to label 50% of training frames as having people”—may suggest that labels were modified. We want to emphasize this is not the case. Our oversampling strategy does not alter the labels; it simply increases the exposure of the rare, underrepresented class during training to ensure the model can learn its pattern despite its low frequency.

      We will revise the Methods section to describe this standard procedure more explicitly, clarifying that oversampling is a training-only strategy to mitigate class imbalance.

      (5) Are "responsive" neurons defined as only those showing firing increases at a feature onset, or would decreased activity also count as responsive? If only positive changes are labeled responsive, this would help explain how non-responsive neurons could be useful in a decoding analysis.

      We define responsive neurons as those showing increased firing rates at feature onset; we did not test for decreases in activity. We thank the reviewer for this valuable comment and will address this point in the revised manuscript by assessing responseness without a restriction on the direction of the firing rate.

      (6) Line 516 states that the scene cuts here are analogous to the hard boundaries in Zheng et al. (2022), but the hard boundaries are transitions between completely unrelated movies rather than scenes within the same movie. Previous work has found that within-movie and across-movie transitions may rely on different mechanisms, e.g., see Lee & Chen, 2022 (10.7554/eLife.73693).

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this distinction and for including the relevant work from Lee & Chan (2022) which further contextualizes this distinction. Indeed, the hard boundaries defined in the cited paper differ slightly from ours. The study distinguishes between (1) hard boundaries—transitions between unrelated movies—and (2) soft boundaries—transitions between related events within the same movie. While our camera cuts resemble their soft boundaries, our scene cuts do not fully align with either category. We defined scene cuts to be more similar to the study’s hard boundaries, but we recognize this correspondence is not exact. We will clarify the distinctions between our scene cuts and the hard boundaries described in Zheng et al. (2022) in the revised manuscript, and will update our text to include the finding from Lee & Chan (2022).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This is an excellent, very interesting paper. There is a groundbreaking analysis of the data, going from typical picture presentation paradigms to more realistic conditions. I would like to ask the authors to consider a few points in the comments below.

      (1) From Figure 2, I understand that there are 7 neurons responding to the character Summer, but then in line 157, we learn that there are 46. Are the other 39 from other areas (not parahippocampal)? If this is the case, it would be important to see examples of these responses, as one of the main claims is that it is possible to decode as good or better with non-responsive compared to single responsive neurons, which is, in principle, surprising.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this ambiguity in the text. Yes, the other 39 units are responsive neurons from other areas. We will clarify to which neuronal sets the number of responsive neurons corresponds. We will also include response plots depicting the unit activity for the mentioned units.

      (2) Also in Figure 2, there seem to be relatively very few neurons responding to Summer (1.88%) and to outdoor scenes (1.07%). Is this significant? Isn't it also a bit surprising, particularly for outdoor scenes, considering a previous paper of Mormann showing many outdoor scene responses in this area? It would be nice if the authors could comment on this.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful point. While a low response to the general 'outdoor scene' label seems surprising at first, our findings align with the established role of the parahippocampal cortex (PHC) in processing scenes and spatial layouts. In previous work using static images, each image introduces a new spatial context. In our movie stimulus, new spatial contexts specifically emerge at scene cuts. Accordingly, our data show a strong PHC response precisely at these moments. We will revise the discussion to emphasize this interpretation, highlighting the consistency with prior work.

      Regarding the first comment, we did not originally test if the proportion of the units is significant using e.g. a binomial test. We will include the results of a binomial test for each region and feature pair in the revised manuscript.

      (3) I was also surprised to see that there are many fewer responses to scene cuts (6.7%) compared to camera cuts (51%) because every scene cut involves a camera cut. Could this have been a result of the much larger number of camera cuts? (A way to test this would be to subsample the camera cuts.)

      The decrease in responsive units for scene cuts relative to camera cuts could indeed be due to the overall decrease in “trials” from one label to the other. To test this, we will follow the reviewer’s suggestion and perform tests using sets of randomly subsampled camera cuts and will include the results in the revised manuscript.

      (4) Line 201. The analysis of decoding on a per-patient basis is important, but it should be done on a per-session basis - i.e., considering only simultaneously recorded neurons, without any pooling. This is because pooling can overestimate decoding performances (see e.g. Quian Quiroga and Panzeri NRN 2009). If there was only one session per patient, then this should be called 'per-session' rather than 'per-patient' to make it clear that there was no pooling.

      The per-patient decoding was indeed also a per-session decoding, as each patient contributed only a single session to the dataset. We will make note of this explicitly in the text to resolve the ambiguity.

      (6) Lines 406-407. The claim that stimulus-selective responses to characters did not account for the decoding of the same character is very surprising. If I understood it correctly, the response criterion the authors used gives 'responsiveness' but not 'selectivity'. So, were people's responses selective (e.g., firing only to Summer) or non-selective (firing to a few characters)? This could explain why they didn't get good decoding results with responsive neurons. Again, it would be nice to see confusion matrices with the decoding of the characters. Another reason for this is that what are labelled as responsive neurons have relatively weak and variable responses.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the importance of selectivity in addition to responsiveness. Indeed, our response criterion does not take stimulus selectivity into account and exclusively measures increases in firing activity after feature onsets for a given feature irrespective of other features.

      We will adjust the text to reflect this shortcoming of the response-detection approach used here. To clarify the relationship between neural populations, we will add visualizations of the overlap of responsive neurons across labels for each subregion. These figures will be included in the revised manuscript.

      In our approach, we trained separate networks for each feature to effectively mitigate the issue of correlated feature labels within the dataset (see earlier discussion). While this strategy effectively deals with the correlated features, it precluded the generation of standard confusion matrices, as classification was performed independently for each feature.

      To directly assess the feature selectivity of responsive neurons, we will fit generalized linear models to predict their firing rates from the features. This approach will enable us to quantify their selectivity and compare it to that of the broader neuronal population.

      (7) Line 455. The claim that 500 neurons drive decoding performance is very subjective. 500 neurons gives a performance of 0.38, and 50 neurons gives 0.33.

      We agree with the reviewer that the phrasing is unclear. We will adjust our summary of this analysis as given in Line 455 to reflect that the logistic regression-derived neuronal rankings produce a subset which achieve comparable performance.

      (8) Lines 492-494. I disagree with the claim that "character decoding does not rely on individual cells, as removing neurons that responded strongly to character onset had little impact on performance". I have not seen strong responses to characters in the paper. In particular, the response to Summer in Figure 2 looks very variable and relatively weak. If there are stronger responses to characters, please show them to make a convincing argument. It is fine to argue that you can get information from the population, but in my view, there are no good single-cell responses (perhaps because the actors and the movie were unknown to the subjects) to make this claim. Also, an older paper (Quian Quiroga et al J. Neurophysiol. 2007) showed that the decoding of individual stimuli in a picture presentation paradigm was determined by the responsive neurons and that the non-responsive neurons did not add any information. The results here could be different due to the use of movies instead of picture presentations, but most likely due to the fact that, in the picture presentation paradigm, the pictures were of famous people for which there were strong single neuron responses, unlike with the relatively unknown persons in this paper.

      This is an important point and we thank the reviewer for highlighting a previous paradigm in which responsive neurons did drive decoding performance. Indeed, the fact that the movie, its characters and the corresponding actors were novel to patients could explain the disparity in decoding performance by way of weaker and more variable responses. We will include additional examples in the supplement of responses to features. Additionally, we will modify the text to emphasize the point that reliable decoding is possible even in the absence of a robust set of neuronal responses. It could indeed be the case that a decoder would place more weight on responsive units if they were present (as shown in the mentioned paper and in our decoding from visual transitions in the parahippocampal cortex).

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors explore a novel concept: GPCR-mediated regulation of miRNA release via extracellular vesicles (EVs). They perform an EV miRNA cargo profiling approach to investigate how specific GPCR activations influence the selective secretion of particular miRNAs. Given that GPCRs are highly diverse and orchestrate multiple cellular pathways - either independently or collectively - to regulate gene expression and cellular functions under various conditions, it is logical to expect alterations in gene and miRNA expression within target cells.

      Strengths:

      The novel idea of GPCRs-mediated control of EV loading of miRNAs.

      Weaknesses:

      Incomplete findings failed to connect and show evidence of any physiological parameters that are directly related to the observed changes. The mechanical detail is lacking.

      We appreciate the reviewer's acknowledgment of the novelty of this study. We agree with the reviewer that further mechanistic insights would strengthen the manuscript. The mechanisms by which miRNA is sorted into EVs remain poorly understood. Various factors, including RNA-binding protein, sequence motifs, and cellular location, can influence this sorting process(Garcia-Martin et al., 2022; Liu & Halushka, 2025; Villarroya-Beltri et al., 2013; Yoon et al., 2015). Ago2, a key component of the RNA-induced silencing complexes, binds to miRNA and facilitates miRNA sorting. Ago2 has been found in the EVs and can be regulated by the cellular signaling pathway.  For instance, McKenzie et al. demonstrated that KRAS-dependent activation of MEK-ERK can phosphorylate Ago2 protein, thereby regulating the sorting of specific miRNAs into EVs(McKenzie et al., 2016). In the differentiated PC12 cells, Gαq activation leads to the formation of Ago2-associated granules, which selectively sequester unique transcripts(Jackson et al., 2022). Investigating GPCR, G protein, and GPCR signaling on Ago2 expression, location, and phosphorylation states could provide valuable insights into how GPCRs regulate specific miRNAs within EVs. We have expanded these potential mechanisms and future research in the discussion section.

      The manuscript falls short of providing a comprehensive understanding. Identifying changes in cellular and EV-associated miRNAs without elucidating their physiological significance or underlying regulatory mechanisms limits the study's impact. Without demonstrating whether these miRNA alterations have functional consequences, the findings alone are insufficient. The findings may be suitable for more specialized journals.

      Thank you for the feedback. We acknowledge that validating the target genes of the top candidate miRNAs is an important next step. In response to the reviewer's concerns, we have expanded the discussion of future research in the manuscript. Although this initial study is primarily descriptive, it establishes a novel conceptual link between GPCR signaling and EV-mediated communication.

      Furthermore, a critical analysis of the relationship between cellular miRNA levels and EV miRNA cargo is essential. Specifically, comparing the intracellular and EV-associated miRNA pools could reveal whether specific miRNAs are preferentially exported, a behavior that should be inversely related to their cellular abundance if export serves a beneficial function by reducing intracellular levels. This comparison is vital to strengthen the biological relevance of the findings and support the proposed regulatory mechanisms by GPCRs.

      We appreciate the valuable suggestions from the reviewer. EV miRNA and cell miRNAs may exhibit distinct profiles as miRNAs can be selectively sorted into or excluded from EVs(Pultar et al., 2024; Teng et al., 2017; Zubkova et al., 2021). Investigating the difference between cellular miRNA levels and EV miRNA cargo would provide insight into the mechanism of miRNA sorting and the functions of miRNAs in the recipient cells. The expression of the cellular miRNAs is a highly dynamic process. To accurately compare the miRNA expression levels, profiling of EV miRNA and cellular miRNA should be conducted simultaneously. However, as a pilot study, we were unable to measure the cellular miRNAs without conducting the entire experiment again.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study examines how activating specific G protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs) affects the microRNA (miRNA) profiles within extracellular vesicles (EVs). The authors seek to identify whether different GPCRs produce unique EV miRNA signatures and what these signatures could indicate about downstream cellular processes and pathological processes.

      Methods:

      (1) Used U2OS human osteosarcoma cells, which naturally express multiple GPCR types.

      (2) Stimulated four distinct GPCRs (ADORA1, HRH1, FZD4, ACKR3) using selective agonists.

      (3) Isolated EVs from culture media and characterized them via size exclusion chromatography, immunoblotting, and microscopy.

      (4) Employed qPCR-based miRNA profiling and bioinformatics analyses (e.g., KEGG, PPI networks) to interpret expression changes.

      Key Findings:

      (1) No significant change in EV quantity or size following GPCR activation.

      (2) Each GPCR triggered a distinct EV miRNA expression profile.

      (3) miRNAs differentially expressed post-stimulation were linked to pathways involved in cancer, insulin resistance, neurodegenerative diseases, and other physiological/pathological processes.

      (4) miRNAs such as miR-550a-5p, miR-502-3p, miR-137, and miR-422a emerged as major regulators following specific receptor activation.

      Conclusions:

      The study offers evidence that GPCR activation can regulate intercellular communication through miRNAs encapsulated within extracellular vesicles (EVs). This finding paves the way for innovative drug-targeting strategies and enhances understanding of drug side effects that are mediated via GPCR-related EV signaling.

      Strengths:

      (1) Innovative concept: The idea of linking GPCR signaling to EV miRNA content is novel and mechanistically important.

      (2) Robust methodology: The use of multiple validation methods (biochemical, biophysical, and statistical) lends credibility to the findings.

      (3) Relevance: GPCRs are major drug targets, and understanding off-target or systemic effects via EVs is highly valuable for pharmacology and medicine.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Sample Size & Scope: The analysis included only four GPCRs. Expanding to more receptor types or additional cell lines would enhance the study's applicability.

      We are encouraged that the reviewer recognized the novelty, methodological rigor, and significance of our work. We recognize the limitations of our current model system and emphasize the need to test additional GPCR families and cell lines in the future studies, as detailed in the discussion section.

      (2) Exploratory Nature: This study is primarily descriptive and computational. It lacks functional validation, such as assessing phenotypic effects in recipient cells, which is acknowledged as a future step.

      We appreciate the feedback. We recognize the importance of validating the function of the top candidate miRNAs in the recipient cells, and this will be included in future studies. 

      (3) EV heterogeneity: The authors recognize that they did not distinguish EV subpopulations, potentially confounding the origin and function of miRNAs.

      Thank you for the comment. EV isolation and purification are major challenges in EV research. Current isolation techniques are often ineffective at separating vesicles produced by different biogenetic pathways. Furthermore, the lack of specific markers to differentiate EV subtypes adds to this complexity. We recognize that the presence of various subpopulations can complicate the interpretation of EV cargos. In our study, we used a combined approach of ultrafiltration followed by size-exclusion chromatography to achieve a balance between EV purity and yield. We adhere to the MISEV (Minimal Information for Studies of Extracellular Vesicles 2023) guidelines by reporting detailed isolation methods, assessing both positive and negative protein markers, and characterizing EVs by electron microscopy to confirm vesicle structure, as well as nanoparticle tracking analysis to verify particle size distribution(Welsh et al., 2024). By following these guidelines, we can ensure the quality of our study and enhance the ability to compare our findings with other studies.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary: As TDP-43 mislocalization is a hallmark of multiple neurodegenerative diseases, the authors seek to identify pathways that modulate TDP-43 levels. To do this, they use a FACS based genome wide CRISPR KD screen in a Halo tagged TDP-43 KI iPSC line. Their screen identifies a number of genetic modulators of TDP-43 expression including BORC which plays a role in lysosome transport.

      Strengths:

      Genome wide CRISPR based screen identifies a number of modulators of TDP-43 expression to generate hypotheses regarding RNA BP regulation and perhaps insights into disease.

      Weaknesses:

      It is unclear how altering TDP-43 levels may relate to disease where TDP-43 is not altered in expression but mislocalized. This is a solid cell biology study, but the relation to disease is not clear without providing evidence of BORC alterations in disease or manipulation of BORC reversing TDP-43 pathology in disease.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and have updated the discussion to include more discussion of the role TDP-43 may play in the BORCS8-associated neurodegenerative disorder and how understanding how lysosome localization changing TDP-43 levels may help patients (lines 313-321).

      The mechanisms by which BORC and lysosome transport modulate TDP-43 expression are unclear. Presumably, this may be through altered degradation of TDP protein but this is not addressed.

      We agree with the reviewer that understanding the mechanism by which lysosome transport regulates TDP-43 levels is important and plan to examine this in future studies.

      Previous studies have demonstrated that TDP-43 levels can be modulated by altering lysosomal degradation so the identification of lysosomal pathways is not particularly novel.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and have updated the text to make this clearer (lines 310-313). What hasn’t been observed previously is a change in lysosome localization affecting TDP-43 levels.

      It is unclear whether this finding is specific to TDP-43 levels or whether lysosome localization may more broadly impact proteostasis in particular of other RNA BPs linked to disease.

      We agree that this is an interesting question and something that should be investigated in future studies.

      Unclear whether BORC depletion alters lysosome function or simply localization.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. Lysosome function related to protein turnover has not yet been examined in the literature after loss of BORC, but other aspects of lysosome function (including lipid metabolism and autophagic flux) have been shown to be disrupted upon loss of BORC. We have updated the discussion to address this (lines 292-296).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary: The authors employ a novel CRISPRi FACS screen and uncover the lysosomal transport complex BORC as a regulator of TDP-43 protein levels in iNeurons. They also find that BORC subunit knockouts impair lysosomal function, leading to slower protein turnover and implicating lysosomal activity in the regulation of TDP-43 levels. This is highly significant for the field given that a) other proteins could also be regulated in this way, b) understanding mechanisms that influence TDP-43 levels are significant given that its dysregulation is considered a major driver of several neurodegenerative diseases and c) the novelty of the proposed mechanism.

      Strengths:

      The novelty and information provided by the CRISPRi screen. The authors provide evidence indicating that BORC subunit knockouts impair lysosomal function, leading to slower protein turnover and implicating lysosomal activity in the regulation of TDP-43 levels and show a mechanistic link between lysosome mislocalization and TDP-43 dysregulation. The study highlights the importance of localized lysosome activity in axons and suggests that lysosomal dysfunction could drive TDP-43 pathologies associated with neurodegenerative diseases like FTD/ALS. Further, the methods and concepts will have an impact to the larger community as well. The work also sets up for further work to understand the somewhat paradoxical findings that even though the tagged TDP-43 protein is reduced in the screen, it does not alter cryptic exon splicing and there is a longer TDP-43 half-life with BORC KD.

      Weaknesses:

      While the data is very strong, the work requires some additional clarification.

      We thank the reviewer for these comments. Our detailed responses are included below in the “recommendations for authors” section.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary: In this work, Ryan et al. have performed a state-of-the-art full genome CRISP-based screen of iNeurons expressing a tagged version of TDP-43 in order to determine expression modifiers of this protein. Unexpectedly, using this approach the authors have uncovered a previously undescribed role of the BORC complex in affecting the levels of TDP-43 protein, but not mRNA expression. Taken together, these findings represent a very solid piece of work that will certainly be important for the field.

      Strengths:

      BORC is a novel TDP-43 expression modifier that has never been described before and it seemingly acts on regulating protein half life rather than transcriptome level. It has been long known that different labs have reported different half-lives for TDP-43 depending on the experimental system but no work has ever explained these discrepancies. Now, the work of Ryan et al. has for the time identified one of these factors which could account for these differences and play an important role in disease (although this is left to be determined in future studies).

      The genome wide CRISPR screening has demonstrated to yield novel results with high reproducibility and could eventually be used to search for expression modifiers of many other proteins involved in neurodegeneration or other diseases

      Weaknesses:

      The fact that TDP-43 mRNA does not change following BORCS6 KD is based on a single qRT- PCR that does not really cover all possibilities. For example, the mRNA total levels may not change but the polyA sites may have switched from the highly efficient pA1 to the less efficient and nuclear retained pA4. There are therefore a few other experiments that could have been performed to make this conclusion more compelling, maybe also performing RNAscope experiments to make sure that no change occurred in TDP-43 mRNA localisation in cells.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. To address this point, we performed an analysis of polyA sites on our RNA sequencing data using REPAC and did not find a change in TDP-43 poly adenylation after BORC KD (Figure S6C). Other transcripts do have altered polyA sites, which are summarized in Figure S6C. We also performed HCR FISH for TARDBP mRNA in TDP-43 and BORC KD neurons. While we did not see a difference in RNA localization (see A below, numbers on brackets indicate p-values), we also were not able to detect a significant difference in total TARDBP mRNA levels upon TDP-43 KD (see B below, numbers on brackets indicate p-values), suggesting that some of the signal detected is non-specific to TARDBP. Because of this, we cannot conclusively say that BORC KD does not alter TARDBP mRNA localization using the available tools.

      Author response image 1.

      Even assuming that the mRNA does not change, no explanation for the change in TDP-43 protein half life has been proposed by the authors. This will presumably be addressed in future studies: for example, are mutants that lack different domains of TDP-43 equally affected in their half-lives by BORC KD?. Alternatively, can a mass-spec be attempted to see whether TDP-43 PTMs change following BORCS6 KD?

      We agree with the reviewer that these are important experiments that could be done in the future to further examine the mechanism by which loss of BORC alters TDP-43 half-life. We examined our proteomics data for differential phosphorylation and ubiquitination in NT vs BORC KD (Figure S7G-H). We were unable to detect PTMs on TDP-43, so we cannot say if they contribute to the change in TDP-43 half-life we observed.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Recommendations are detailed in the public review.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Ryan et al, employ a CRISPRi FACS screen and uncover the lysosomal transport complex BORC as a regulator of TDP-43 protein levels in iNeurons. The authors provide strong evidence indicating that BORC subunit knockouts impair lysosomal function, leading to slower protein turnover and implicating lysosomal activity in the regulation of TDP-43 levels. The authors then provided additional evidence of TDP-43 perturbations under lysosome-inhibiting drug conditions, underscoring a mechanistic link between lysosome mislocalization and TDP-43 dysregulation. The study highlights the importance of localized lysosome activity in axons and suggests that lysosomal dysfunction could drive TDP-43 pathologies associated with neurodegenerative diseases like FTD/ALS. The work is exciting and could be highly informative for the field.

      Concerns: There are some disconnects between the figures and the main text that can benefit from refining of the figures to align better with the main text. This does not require additional experiments other than perhaps Figure 4B. The impact of the work could be further discussed - it is an interesting disconnect between the fact BORC KD causes decreased IF of the Halo-tagged TDP-43 and lysosomal transport, however this reduction does not impact cryptic exon expression and also increases TDP-43 half life (and of other proteins). It is a very interesting and potentially informative part of the manuscript.

      We thank the reviewer for their detailed reading of our manuscript. We have endeavored to better match the figures and the text and have added more discussion of the impact of the work.

      Minor:

      (1) Suggestion: relating to the statement "Gene editing was efficient, with almost all selected clones correctly edited." - please provide values or %.

      We updated the text to remove the statement about the editing efficiency, instead saying we identified a clone that was correct for both sequence and karyotype (lines 83-85).

      (2) Relating to Figure 1A: Please provide clarification regarding tagging strategy with the halotag - e.g. why in front of exon2.

      We updated the figure legend to reflect that the start codon for TDP-43 is in exon 2, hence why we placed the HaloTag there.

      (3) Relating to Figure S1: A and B seems to have been swapped.

      We thank the reviewer for catching this mistake and have fixed the figure/text.

      (4) Relating to Figure 1B: figure legend does not indicate grayscale coloring of TDP-43 signal.

      We have added text in the figure legend to indicate that the Halo signal is shown in grayscale in the left-handed panels.

      (5) Relating to Figure 1C: can the authors clarify abbreviation for 'NT' in text and legend.

      We thank the reviewer for catching this and have indicated in the text and figure legend that NT refers to the non-targeting sgRNA that was used as a control for comparison to the TDP-43 KD sgRNA.

      (6) Relating to figure 2B and S2A: main text mentioned "Non-targeting Guides" however the figure does not show non-targeting guides to confirm.

      We thank the reviewer for catching this oversight, we updated the figure legends for these figures to indicate that the non-targeting (NT) guides are shown in gray on the rank plot. They cluster towards the middle, more horizontal portion of the graphs, showing that the more vertical sections of the graph are hits.

      (7) Suggestion: To make it easier on the reader, please provide overlap numbers for the following statement ..."In comparing the top GO terms associated with genes that increase or decrease Halo-TDP-43 levels in iNeurons, we found that almost none altered Halo-TDP-43 levels in iPSCs...".

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and have updated the text to indicate that only a single term is shared between the iPSC and iNeuron screens (lines 113-117).

      (8) Relating to the statement "We cloned single sgRNA plasmids for 59 genes that either increased or decreased Halo-TDP-43 in iNeurons but not in iPSCs." Can the authors provide a list of the 59 genes.

      We have included a new column in the supplemental table S1 indicating the result of the Halo microscopy validation to hopefully clarify which genes lead to a validated phenotype and which did not.

      (9) Relating to the statement "To rule out the possibility of neighboring gene or off-target effects of CRISPRi, as has been reported previously15, we examined the impact of BORC knockout (KO) on TDP-43 levels. Using the pLentiCRISPR system, which expresses the sgRNA of interest on the same plasmid as an active Cas916 we found that KO of BORCS7 using two different sgRNAs decreased TDP-43 levels by immunofluorescence (Figure 5C-D)." Please provide clarification as to why BORCS7 was chosen out of all the BORCS? From the data presentation thus far (Figure 4B & 5A), the reader might have anticipated testing BORCS6 for panels 5C-D.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. We tried a couple of BORCs with the pLentiCRISPR system, but BORCS7 was the only one we were convinced we got functional knockout for based on lysosome localization. We think that either the guides were not ideal for the other BORC components we tried, or we did not get efficient gene editing across the population of cells tested. Because we had previously been working with knock down and CRISPRi guides are not the same as CRISPR knock out guides, we couldn’t use the existing guide sequences we know work well for BORC. Since loss of one BORC gene causes functional loss of the complex and restricts lysosomes to the soma, we did not feel it necessary to assay all 8 genes.

      (10) Relating to the statement "We treated Halo-TDP-43 neurons with various drugs that disrupt distinct processes in the lysosome pathway and asked if Halo-TDP-43 levels changed. Chloroquine (decreases lysosomal acidity), CTSBI (inhibits cathepsin B protease), ammonium chloride (NH4Cl, inhibits lysosome-phagosome fusion), and GPN (ruptures lysosomal membranes) all consistently decreased Halo-TDP-43 levels (Figure 6A-B, S5A-C)" Please provide interpretations for Figures S5A and S5C in text.

      We thank the reviewer for catching this oversight and have updated the text accordingly (lines 183-191).

      (11) Relating to figure 6E: please provide in legend what the different colors used correlate with (i.e. green/brown for BORCS7 KD)?

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. These colors were mistakenly left in the figure from a version looking to see if the observed effects were driven by a single replicate rather than a consistent change (each replicate has a slightly different color). As the colors are intermingled and not separated, we concluded the effect was not driven by a single replicate. The colors have been removed from the updated figure for simplicity.

      (12) Relating to the statement "We observed a similar trend for many proteins in the proteome (Figure 8B)" This statement can benefit from stating which trend the authors are referring to, it is currently unclear from the volcano plot shown for Figure 8B.

      We thank the reviewer for catching this and have updated the text accordingly.

      (13) Relating to the statement "For almost every gene, we observed an increase or decrease in Halo-TDP-43 levels without a change in Halo-TDP-43 localization or compartment specific level changes (Figure 4B)." Please provide: (1) the number of genes examined, (2) additional clarification of "localization" and "compartment specific" level changes, (3) some quantification and or additional supporting data of the imaging results. Figures 5A-B presents with the same concern relating to the comment "To determine if results from Halo-TDP-43 expression assays also applied to endogenous, untagged TDP-43 levels, we selected 22 genes that passed Halo validation and performed immunofluorescence microscopy for endogenous (untagged) TDP-43 (Figure 4D-G,5A-B, S4E-F)." please clarify further.

      We thank the reviewer for requesting this clarification. This statement refers to all 59 genes tested by Halo imaging; only one (MFN2) showed any hints of aggregation or changes in localization, every other gene (58) showed what appeared to be global changes in Halo-TDP-43 levels. We were initially intrigued by the MFN2 phenotype; however, we were unable to replicate it on endogenous TDP-43 and thus concluded that this might be an effect specific to the tagged protein. The representative images shown in Figure 4B are representative of the changes we observed across all 59 genes tested (if changes were present). From the 59 genes that we observed a change in Halo-TDP-43 levels by microscopy, we selected a smaller number to move forward to immunofluorescence for TDP-43. We picked a subset of genes from each of the different categories we had identified (mitochondria, m6A, ubiquitination, and some miscellaneous) to validate by immunofluorescence, thinking that genes in the same pathway would act similarly. We have added a column to the supplemental table S1 indicating which genes were tested by immunofluorescence and what the result was. We have also attempted to clarify the results section to make the above clearer.

      (14) Relating to the statement "To determine if results from Halo-TDP-43 expression assays also applied to endogenous, untagged TDP-43 levels, we selected 22 genes that passed Halo validation and performed immunofluorescence microscopy for endogenous (untagged) TDP-43 (Figure 4D-G, 5A-B, S4E-F). Of these, 18 (82%) gene knockdowns showed changes in endogenous TDP-43 levels (Figure 4D-G, S4E-F)." It is difficult to identify the 18 or 22 genes in the figures as described in the main text.

      We added columns to the supplemental table S1 listing the genes and the result in each assay.

      (15) Relating to figures S7A and 8A and the first part of the section "TDP-43, like the proteome, shows longer turnover time in BORC KD neurons" Can the authors provide clarification why the SunTag assay was performed with BORCS6 KD (S7A) but the follow-up experiment (8A) was performed with BORCS7 KD. Does BORCS6 KD show similar results as BORCS7 with the SunTag assay, and does TDP-43 protein abundance with BORCS7 KD show similar results as BORCS6?

      Because loss of any of the 8 BORC genes causes functional loss of BORC and lysosomes to be restricted to the peri-nuclear space, we used BORC KDs interchangeably. Additionally, all BORC KDs had similar effects on Halo-TDP-43 levels.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Adding more control experiments that TDP-43 mRNA is really not affected following BORC KD

      We performed a FISH experiment to examine TARDBP mRNA localization upon BORC KD but were unable to conclusively say whether BORC KD changes TARDBP mRNA localization (see above). We also analyzed our RNA sequencing experiment for alternative polyadenylation sites upon BORC KD. Results are in Figure S6C.

      Although this could be part of a future study, the authors should try and determine what are the changes to TDP-43 that drive a change in the half-life.

      We agree with the reviewer that these are important experiments and hope to figure this out in the future.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      Seon and Chung's study investigates the hypothesis that individuals take more risks when observed by others because they perceive others to be riskier than themselves. To test this, the authors designed an innovative experimental paradigm where participants were informed that their decisions would be observed by a "risky" player and a "safe" player. Participants underwent fMRI scanning during the task. 

      Strengths: 

      The research question is sound, and the experimental paradigm is well-suited to address the hypothesis. 

      Weaknesses:

      I have several concerns. Most notably, the manuscript is difficult to read in parts, and I suggest a thorough revision of the writing for clarity, as some sections are nearly incomprehensible. Additionally, key statistical details are missing, and I have reservations about the choice of ROIs.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s interest in and positive assessment of our work, and we thank the reviewer for the constructive feedback. In the current revision, we have revised the manuscript for clarity and added previously omitted statistical details. Furthermore, in the response letter, we have also provided additional explanations to clarify our approach, including the rationale for the choice and use of ROIs.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This study aims to investigate how social observation influences risky decision-making. Using a gambling task, the study explored how participants adjusted their risk-taking behavior when they believed their decisions were being observed by either a risk-averse or risk-seeking partner. The authors hypothesized that individuals would simulate the choices of their observers based on learned preferences and integrate these simulated choices into their own decision-making. In addition to behavioral experiments, the study employed computational modeling to formalize decision processes and fMRI to identify the neural underpinnings of risky decision-making under social observation. 

      Strengths: 

      The study provides a fresh perspective on social influence in decision-making, moving beyond the simple notion that social observation leads to uniformly riskier behavior. Instead, it shows that individuals adjust their choices depending on their beliefs about the observer's risk preferences, offering a more nuanced understanding of how social contexts shape decision-making. The authors provide evidence using comprehensive approaches, including behavioral data based on a well-designed task, computational modeling, and neuroimaging. The three models are well selected to compare at which level (e.g., computing utility, risk preference shift, and choice probability) the social influence alters one's risky decision-making. This approach allows for a more precise understanding of the cognitive processes underlying decision-making under social observation. 

      Weaknesses: 

      While the neuroimaging results are generally consistent with the behavioral and computational findings, the strength of the neural evidence could be improved. The authors' claims about the involvement of the TPJ and mPFC in integrating social information are plausible, but further analysis, such as model comparisons at the neuroimaging level, is needed to decisively rule out alternative interpretations that other computational models suggest. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s interest in and positive assessment of our work, and we thank the reviewer for the constructive feedback. In the current revision, we have included neural results from additional analyses, which we believe provide stronger support for our proposed computational model.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This is an important paper using a novel paradigm to examine how observation affects the social contagion of risk preferences. There is a lot of interest in the field about the mechanisms of social influence, and adding in the factor of whether observation also influences these contagion effects is intriguing.

      Strengths:

      (1) There is an impressive combination of a multi-stage behavioural task with computational modelling and neuroimaging.

      (2) The analyses are well conducted and the sample size is reasonable. 

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) Anatomically it would be helpful to more explicitly distinguish between dmPFC and vmPFC. Particularly at the end of the introduction when mPFC and vmPFC are distinguished, as the vmPFC is in the mPFC. 

      (2) The authors' definition of ROIs could be elaborated on further. They suggest that peaks are selected from neurosynth for different terms, but were there not multiple peaks identified within a functional or anatomical brain area? This section could be strengthened by confirming with anatomical ROIs where available, such as the atlases here http://www.rbmars.dds.nl/lab/CBPatlases.html and the Harvard-Oxford atlases. 

      (3) How did the authors ensure there were enough trials to generate a reliable BOLD signal? The scanned part of the study seems relatively short. 

      (4) It would be helpful to add whether any brain areas survived whole-brain correction. 

      (5) There is a concern that mediation cannot be used to make causal inferences and much larger samples are needed to support claims of mediation. The authors should change the term mediation in order to not imply causality (they could talk about indirect effects instead) and highlight that the mediation analyses are exploratory as they would not be sufficiently powered (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2843527/). 

      (6) The authors may want to speculate on lifespan differences in this susceptibility to risk preferences given recent evidence that older adults are relatively more susceptible to impulsive social influence (Zhu et al, 2024, comms psychology). 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s interest in and positive assessment of our work, and we thank the reviewer for the constructive feedback. In the response letter below, we address each of the reviewer’s comments, including clarifications regarding the ROIs and the limitations of the current study in interpreting the results.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The neuroimaging hypotheses seem post hoc to me. First, the term "social inference" is used very loosely. In line 103 the authors mentioned that TPJ has been reported to be involved in inferring other's intentions and learning about others. However, in their task, it is not clear where inference is needed. All participants need to do is recall others' "preferences", rather than inferring a hidden variable or hidden intention. In addition, in some of the studies that the authors have cited (e.g., Park et al. 2021), the hippocampus is the focus of the inference, which gets no mention here.

      How does solving this task require inference (as defined by the authors: inferring others' intentions)? And why do they choose TPJ while inference is not needed in this task?

      We regret any confusion and would like to take this chance to clarify our hypothesis on social inference. As the reviewer pointed out, participants were indeed instructed to predict their choices, through which we expected them to learn the demonstrators’ preferences. Our computational model suggests that during the main phase of the task, i.e., the Observed phase, participants simulated others’ choices based on these previously learned risk preferences of others. The gamble choices they encountered (payoffs and associated probabilities) did not overlap with those in the Learning phase, and therefore, we expected that the cognitive process triggered by the social context involved active simulation—what we describe as making inference about others—rather than simple ‘recall’ of previously learned information. In line with this reasoning, we hypothesized that the TPJ, a brain region previously implicated in simulating others’ actions and intentions, would play a key role during the Observed phase.

      Regarding the role of the hippocampus, the paper we cited by BoKyung Park et al. (2021), titled “The role of right temporoparietal junction in processing social prediction error across relationship contexts”, highlights the involvement of the rTPJ but does not mention the hippocampus. We are aware of the study by Seongmin A. Park et al. (2021), “Inferences on a multidimensional social hierarchy use a grid-like code”, which shows the involvement of the hippocampus and entorhinal cortex in making inferences about multidimensional social hierarchies; we believe the reviewer may have mistakenly assumed that we cited this article. As the study showed, the involvement of the hippocampus—and the use of its grid-like representation of social information—is likely tied to the multidimensional nature of task states. In our study, the hippocampus was not included as an ROI because we had no specific rationale to hypothesize that such grid-like representations would be recruited by our task.

      (2) Social influence can be motivated informationally (to improve accuracy) or normatively (to be aligned with others). To me, it seems that the authors have studied the latter, because, first, there is no objectively correct response in this task and second, because participants changed their risk preference according to the preference of the observing partner. This distinction has not been made throughout the manuscript. This is important because the two process (information and normative) are supported by different neural processes and it is extremely useful to understand neural basis of which process the authors are studying.

      We thank the reviewer for the opportunity to clarify the anticipated role of social influence in our study. As the reviewer pointed out, the gambling task used in our task does not have objectively correct or incorrect answers, and naturally, any social influence present during the task would align with normative social influence. To clarify this point, we have revised the discussion section as follows:

      [Page 9, Line 345]

      Observational learning and mimicry of others’ behavior are patterns commonly found in social animals, including nonhuman primates (Van de Waal et al., 2013). Such behaviors are thought to be driven either by a motivation to acquire additional information (‘informational conformity’) or by a motivation to align with group norm (‘normative conformity’), even when doing so does not necessarily lead to better outcomes (e.g., higher accuracy) (Cialdini & Goldstein, 2004). Given that there are no objectively correct or incorrect answers in the gambling task used in our study, the observed social influence is more consistent with normative conformity. However, we cannot rule out the possibility that individuals developed false beliefs about a particular observing partner—namely, that the partner had greater control over or insight into the gambling task. Future studies are needed to directly investigate whether individuals’ beliefs about others modulate informational social influence—that is, their motivation to use social information to gain additional insight by inferring others’ potential choices.

      (3) From Line 160 onward, the authors report several findings without providing any effect sizes or statistics. Please add effect size and statistics for each finding.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We have now added the corresponding effect sizes and statistical values for the reported findings, beginning from Line 160 in the revised manuscript.

      (4) Line 270: "In particular, bilateral TPJ, brain regions not implicated in the Solo phase, positively tracked trial-by-trial model-estimated decision probabilities". How can the authors conclude that TPJ is not involved in the solo phase? As far as I understood from the text, TPJ was not included as one of the ROIs for analysis of the Solo phase. If it was included, it should be mentioned in the text and there should be a direct comparison between the effect sizes of the solo and the observer phase. If not, "not implicated in the Solo phase" is not justified and should be removed.

      We apologize for the confusion. As the reviewer correctly pointed out, the TPJ was not included among the ROIs in our analysis of the Solo phase data; therefore, its involvement during the Solo phase was never directly assessed using an ROI-based approach.

      To examine brain responses during the Observed phase, we first assessed whether regions that tracked decision probabilities during the Solo phase—vmPFC, vStr, and dACC—were also engaged in the Observed phase. The involvement of the TPJ during the Observed phase was revealed through a subsequent whole-brain analysis. To clarify this point, we now have revised the corresponding part as follows:

      [Page 8, Line 276]

      In particular, bilateral TPJ positively, brain regions not implicated in the Solo phase, tracked trial-by-trial model-estimated decision probabilities

      à Notably, bilateral TPJ showed significant positive tracking of decision probabilities ~

      (5) I am a bit puzzled about the PPI analysis. Is the main finding increased connectivity within mPFC in the observing condition? PPI is often done between two separate brain regions. I am not sure what it means that connectivity within mPFC increases in one condition compared to another. What was the motivation for this analysis? Can you also please explain what it means?

      As the reviewer noted, psychophysiological interaction (PPI) analyses examine functional connectivity between brain regions as modulated by a psychological factor. To clarify our result, the reported ‘mPFC-mPFC connectivity’ refers to functional connectivity between the mPFC region responsive to the presence of an observing partner and an adjacent, anatomically distinct region within the mPFC. Note that we have revised the manuscript to refer to this region more specifically as the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC). Please see our response to Reviewer 3, Comment 1, for further details.

      During the Observed phase of our task, social information was processed at two distinct time points. First, at the beginning of each decision trial, individuals were cued with the presence (or absence) of an observing partner (‘Partner presentation’). Second, the gamble options, as well as the observing partner’s identity, were revealed (‘Options revealed’). Because participants had previously learned about the observing partner’s risk preferences, we expected them to simulate the choice the partner would likely make. We hypothesized that if individuals indeed simulated the partner’s choice and incorporated this information into their decision-making process, the brain region involved in recognizing the partner’s presence (dmPFC<sub>contrast</sub>) would be functionally connected to the region responsible for integrating social information into the final decision (TPJ). Our results showed that the two regions were functionally connected via an indirect path through an anatomically adjacent cluster within the mPFC (dmPFC<sub>PPI</sub>). Given that the recognition of the partner’s presence and the simulation of their choice occurred at two distinct time points, we interpreted the functional connectivity between the two dmPFC clusters (dmPFC<sub>contrast</sub> and dmPFC<sub>PPI</sub>) as evidence that the dmPFC<sub>PPI</sub>) remained engaged during the decision process to support simulation, rather than being involved solely in the passive recognition of the social context (i.e., observed vs not observed). Note that, consistent with this interpretation, functional connectivity was stronger in individuals who showed greater reliance on social information ('Social reliance' parameter in our model).

      To avoid confusion, we have now labeled the two dmPFC clusters as dmPFC<sub>contrast</sub>—the seed region identified at partner presentation—and dmPFC<sub>PPI</sub>—the target region identified in the PPI analysis.

      [Page 8, Line 284]

      This cue was intended to dissociate neural responses to the social context per se (i.e., the presence of an observing partner), which we hypothesized would initiate social processing, from the neural processes involved in incorporating this information during the subsequent decision-making phase.

      [Page 8, Line 291]

      We tested whether the dmPFC was also involved in incorporating social information during the decision process under social observation, particularly among individuals who relied more heavily on simulating others’ behavior.

      [Page 8, Line 297]

      We confirmed that the functional connectivity between the dmPFC<sub>contrast</sub> which is sensitive to cues regarding the presence of an observing partner, and its adjacent, anatomically distinct region within the dmPFC (‘dmPFC<sub>PPI</sub>’ hereafter; x = 3, y = 50, z = 5, k<sub>E</sub> = .74, cluster-level P<sub>FWE, SVC</sub> = 0.011; Fig. 4a, b, Table S5) was positively associated with individuals’ social reliance.

      (6) In Line 107 the authors say "excitatory stimulation of the TPJ improved social cognition". Improved social cognition is too general and unspecific. Please be more specific.

      We agree that the term ‘social cognition’ was too general and unspecific. In the revised manuscript, we have specified that the improvement was observed in tasks specifically involving the control of self-other representation, as demonstrated by Santiesteban et al. (2012).

      [Page 4, Line 106]

      Corroborating with these neuroimaging data, excitatory stimulation of the TPJ improved social cognition (Santiesteban et al., 2012),~

      à Corroborating these neuroimaging findings, excitatory stimulation of the TPJ improved social cognition involving the control of self-other representation (Santiesteban et al., 2012),~

      Writing:

      We thank the reviewer for their thorough evaluation of our manuscript. We have now made the necessary revisions in accordance with the provided comments.

      (7) Line 75: "one risky options" should be one risky option.

      [Page 3, Line 74]

      between one safe (i.e., guaranteed payoff) and one risky options.

      between a safe option (i.e., guaranteed payoff) and a risky option.

      (8) Line 82: were given with the same set of gamble should be "were given the same set of gamble".

      [Page 3, Line 81]

      In the third phase (‘Observed phase’), individuals were given with the same set of gamble choices they faced in the Solo phase,

      In the third phase (‘Observed phase’), individuals were given the same set of gamble choices they faced in the Solo phase,~

      (9) Line 63: and that the extent of such influence depends on the identity of the observer. It is not clear what the authors mean by the "identity of observer". Does it mean the preference of the observer?

      Van Hoorn et al. (2018) showed that the degree of social influence varies depending on whether individuals are being observed by parents or by peers. While one might attribute this difference to divergent preferences typically held by parents and peers, it is important to note that other factors may also differ between these social groups. To avoid overinterpretation while preserving the original meaning, we have revised the sentence as follows:

      [Page 3, Line 61]

      However, recent studies showed that the unidirectional influence of social others’ presence may be also observed in adults (Otterbring, 2021), and that the extent of such influence depends on the identity of the observer (Van Hoorn et al., 2018).  

      However, recent studies showed that the unidirectional influence of social others’ presence can also be observed in adults (Otterbring, 2021), and that the extent of this influence depends on the observer’s identity—specifically, whether the observer is a parent or a peer (Van Hoorn et al., 2018).

      (10) Line 103: "including inferring others' intention and in learning about others." An "in" is missing right before inferring.

      [Page 4, Line 101]

      The temporoparietal junction (TPJ) is another region known to play an important role in social cognitive functions, including inferring others’ intention and in learning about others (Behrens et al., 2008; Boorman et al., 2013; Charpentier et al., 2020; Park et al., 2021; Samson et al., 2004; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2013; Van Overwalle, 2009; Young et al., 2010).

      The temporoparietal junction (TPJ) is another region known to play an important role in a range of social cognitive functions, including simulating others’ intention and choices, as well as learning about others (Behrens et al., 2008; Boorman et al., 2013; Charpentier et al., 2020; Park et al., 2021; Samson et al., 2004; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2003; Saxe & Kanwisher, 2013; Van Overwalle, 2009; Young et al., 2010).

      (11) 106: "Corroborating with these neuroimaging data." It should be "corroborating these neuroimaging data".

      [Page 4, Line 106]

      Corroborating with these neuroimaging data, ~

      Corroborating these neuroimaging findings, ~

      (12) Lines 113-115. It is not clear what the authors are trying to say here.

      We have now revised the sentence as follows:

      [Page 4, Line 112]

      We hypothesized that even if others’ choices are not explicitly presented, simple presence of social others may trigger inference about others’ potential choices, and the same set of brain regions will play an important role in value-based decision-making.

      We hypothesized that, even in the absence of explicit information about others’ choices, the mere presence of social others could lead participants to conform to the option they believe others would choose. To do so, participants would need to simulate others’ potential choices, particularly when option values vary across trials. As a result, we propose that the same brain regions involved in simulating others’ decisions would also be engaged during value-based decision-making in the presence of social observers.

      (13) Line 151: This sentence is too long and hard to follow:

      We have now revised the sentence as follows:

      [Page 5, Line 154]

      Furthermore, individuals’ prediction responses on subsequent 10 prediction trials where no feedback was provided (Fig. 2b) as well as self-reports about the perceived riskiness of the partners collected at the end of the Learning phase (Fig. 1d) consistently showed that they were able to distinguish one partner from the other, and correctly estimate the partners’ risk preferences (Predicted risk preference: t(42) = -11.46, P = 1.66e-14; Self-report: t(42) = -35.83, P = 4.10e-33).

      Furthermore, individuals’ prediction responses during the subsequent 10 trials without feedback consistently indicated that they could distinguish between the two partners and accurately estimate each partner’s risk preferences (t(42) = -11.46, P = 1.66e-14; Fig. 2b). Self-reported ratings of the partners’ perceived riskiness, collected after the Learning phase, further supported this finding (t(42) = -35.83, P = 4.10e-33; Fig. 1d).

      (14) Line 178: This sentence is very hard to follow. I am not sure what the authors were trying to say here. Please clarify.

      We have now revised the sentence as follows:

      [Page 5, Line 183]

      Various previous studies examined the impacts of social context on decision-making processes, but the suggested mechanisms by which individuals were affected by the social information depended on how the information was presented.

      à Previous studies have shown that social context can influence decision-making processes. However, the underlying mechanisms proposed have varied depending on how the social information was presented.

      (15) Line 183: "when individuals were given with the chances" should be "when individuals were given the chance".

      [Page 5, Line 187]

      On the contrary, when individuals were given with the chances~

      On the contrary, when individuals were given the chances~

      (16) Line 192: "are sensitive to the identity of the currently observing partner...". Do the authors mean are sensitive to the preferences of the currently observing partner? If so, please clarify, it is hard to follow.

      We have now revised the sentence as follows:

      [Page 5, Line 195]

      We hypothesized that if individuals are sensitive to the identity of the currently observing partner, they would take into account the learned preferences of others in computing their choices rather than simply in guiding the direction how to change their own preferences.

      à We hypothesized that if individuals are sensitive to the learned preferences of the observing partner, they would use this information to simulate the partner’s likely choices, rather than simply aligning their own preferences with those of the partner.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The current neuroimaging findings appear to support the decision processes of all three models. I recommend that the authors provide more detailed evidence of model comparisons in the neuroimaging analysis. This should go beyond simply comparing the goodness of fit of neural activity.

      We acknowledge that neuroimaging data alone often do not provide conclusive evidence for specific information processing. In our study, we examined brain regions that track decision probabilities and are associated with social cognition, such as simulating others’ choice tendencies. Because these processes are general and not tied to a specific computational model, neural responses supporting the occurrence of such processes cannot be used to rule out alternative decision models. For this reason, our approach prioritized a rigorous behavioral model comparison as a critical first step before probing the neural substrates underlying the proposed mechanism. Our behavioral model comparisons, including both quantitative fit indices and qualitative pattern predictions, indicated that the proposed model best accounted for participants' decision patterns across task conditions.

      More importantly, to further validate the model, we conducted a model recovery analysis (see Fig. S2b in SI), which confirmed that our model can be reliably distinguished from alternative accounts even when behavioral differences are subtle. This result suggests that our model captures unique and meaningful characteristics of the decision process that are not equally well explained by competing models.

      With this behavioral foundation, our neuroimaging analyses were designed not to serve as independent model arbiters, but rather to examine whether brain activity in regions of interest reflected the computations specified by the best-fitting model. We believe this two-step approach—first establishing behavioral validity, then linking model-derived variables to neural data—offers a principled framework for identifying the cognitive and neural mechanisms of decision-making.

      Nevertheless, per the reviewer’s suggestion, we further examined whether there is neural encoding of both the participant’s own utility and the observer’s utility—serving as potential neural evidence to differentiate our model from the two alternative models. Please see below for our response to Reviewer 2’s Comment (2).

      (2) Specifically, if participants are combining their own and simulated choices at the level of choice probability, we would expect to see neural encoding of both their own utility and the observer's utility. These may be observed in different areas of the mPFC, as demonstrated by Nicolle et al. (Neuron, 2012). In that study, decisions simulating others' choices were associated with activity in the dorsal mPFC, while one's own decisions were encoded in the vmPFC. On the contrary, if the brain encodes decision values based on the shifted risk preference, rather than encoding each decision's value in separate brain areas, this would support the alternative model.

      We thank the reviewer for this constructive comment. In our Social reliance model, we assumed that the decision probability based on an individual’s own risk preferences, as well as that based on the observing partner’s risk preferences, both contribute to the individual’s final choice. As the reviewer suggested, neural evidence that differentiates our model from the two alternative models—the Risk preference change model and the Other-conferred utility model—would involve demonstrating neural encoding of both the participant’s own utility and the observer’s utility.

      The utility differences between chosen and unchosen options from the two perspectives—self and observer—were highly correlated, preventing us from including both as regressors in the same design matrix. Instead, we defined ROIs along the ventral-to-dorsal axis of the mPFC, and examined whether each ROI more strongly reflected one’s own utility or that of the observer. Based on the meta-analysis by Clithero and Rangel (2014), we defined the most ventral mPFC ROI (ROI1) as a 10 mm-radius sphere centered at coordinate [x=-3, y=41, z=-7], a region previously associated with subjective value. From this ventral seed, we defined four additional spherical ROIs (10 mm radius each) at 12 mm intervals along the ventral-to-dorsal axis, resulting in five ROIs in total: ROI2 [x=-3, y=41, z=5], ROI3 [x=-3, y=41, z=17], ROI4 [x=-3, y=41, z=29], ROI5 [x=-3, y=41, z=41].

      Consistent with Nicolle et al. (2012), the representation of one’s own utility (labelled as ‘Own subjective value’) and that of the observer (‘Observer’s subjective value’) was organized along the ventral-to-dorsal axis of the mPFC. Specifically, utility signals from the participant’s own perspective (SV<sub>chosen, self</sub> – SV<sub>unchosen, self</sub>) were most prominently represented in the ventral-most ROIs (blue), whereas utility signals from the observer’s perspective (SV<sub>chosen, observer</sub> – SV<sub>unchosen, observer</sub>) were most strongly represented in the dorsal-most ROIs (orange).

      (3) Additionally, the authors may be able to detect neural signals related to conflict when the decisions of the individual and the observer differ, compared to when the decisions are congruent. These neural signatures would only be present if social influences are integrated at the choice level, as suggested by the authors.

      If individuals simulate the choices that others might make, they may compare them with the choices they would have made themselves. To investigate this possibility, we categorized task trials as Conflict or No-conflict trials based on greedy choice predictions derived from a softmax decision rule. Conflict trials were those in which the choice predicted from the participant’s own risk preference differed from that predicted for the observer, whereas No-conflict trials involved the same predicted choice from both perspectives. A contrast between Conflict and No-conflict trials revealed that the dACC and dlPFC—regions previously associated with conflict monitoring and cognitive control (Shenhav et al., 2013)—were sensitive to differences in choice tendencies between the self and observer perspectives.

      Author response image 1.

      dACC and dlPFC are associated with the discrepancy between participants’ own choice tendencies and those of observing partners, as estimated based on prior beliefs about the partners’ risk preferences.

      As the reviewer suggested, these results provide evidence in support of the Social Reliance model, which posits that participants simulate the observer's choice and integrate it with their own.

      (4) Incorporating these additional analyses would provide stronger evidence for distinguishing between the models.

      We again thank the reviewer for these constructive suggestions. Based on the new set of analyses and results, we have made the necessary revisions as noted above. We agree that these revisions provide stronger evidence for distinguishing between the models.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Anatomically it would be helpful to more explicitly distinguish between dmPFC and vmPFC. Particularly at the end of the introduction when mPFC and vmPFC are distinguished, as the vmPFC is in the mPFC.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion regarding the anatomical distinction between the dmPFC and vmPFC, particularly in relation to our use of the term “mPFC.” We acknowledge that the dmPFC and vmPFC are subregions of the broader mPFC. In our original manuscript, we referred to one region as mPFC in line with prior studies highlighting its role in social cognition and contextual processing (Behrens et al., 2008; Sul et al., 2015; Wittmann et al., 2016). However, in response to the reviewer’s comment and to more clearly distinguish this region from the ventral portion of the mPFC (i.e., vmPFC), which is canonically associated with subjective valuation, we have now revised the manuscript to refer to this region as the dmPFC. This terminology better reflects its association with social cognition, including model-estimated social reliance and sensitivity to social cues in our study.

      (2) The authors' definition of ROIs could be elaborated on further. They suggest that peaks are selected from neurosynth for different terms, but were there not multiple peaks identified within a functional or anatomical brain area? This section could be strengthened by confirming with anatomical ROIs where available, such as the atlases here http://www.rbmars.dds.nl/lab/CBPatlases.html and the Harvard-Oxford atlases.

      We appreciate the opportunity to clarify how our ROIs were defined. To identify the ROIs, we drew upon both prior literature and results from a term-based meta-analysis using Neurosynth. For each meta-map, we applied an FDR-corrected threshold of p < 0.01 and a cluster extent threshold of k ≥ 100 voxels to identify distinct functional clusters. For each cluster, we constructed a spherical ROI (radius = 10 mm) centered on its center of gravity. Note that for each anatomically distinct brain region, only a single center of gravity was identified and used to define the ROI. The resulting ROIs were subsequently used for small volume correction (SVC) in the second-level fMRI analyses.

      For brain regions associated with decision-making processes, we obtained a meta-analytic activation map associated with the term “decision” from Neurosynth. After applying an FDR-corrected threshold of p < 0.001 and a cluster extent threshold of k ≥ 100 voxels, we identified five distinct clusters: vmPFC [x = -3, y = 38, z = -10]; right vStr [x = 12, y = 11, z = -7]; left vStr [x = -12, y = 8, z = -7]; dACC [x = 3, y = 26, z = 44]; and left Insula [x = -30, y = 23, z = -1]. To identify brain regions involved in decision-making under social observation, we used the Neurosynth meta-map associated with the term “social”, applying the same criteria (FDR p < 0.001, k ≥ 100). This analysis revealed several clusters, including bilateral TPJ: right TPJ [x = 51, y = -52, z = 14]; left TPJ [x = -51, y = -58, z = 17]. To isolate brain regions more specifically associated with social processing rather than valuation, we also constructed a conjunction map using the meta-maps for the terms “social” and “value.” We identified clusters present in the “social” map, but not in the “value” map. This analysis yielded, among others, a cluster in the dmPFC [x = 0, y = 50, z = 14].

      To clarify our ROI analysis methods, we have now revised the manuscript to include more detailed information about the procedures used, as follows:

      [Page 19, Line 746]

      Region-of-interest (ROI) analyses. To define ROIs for the neural analyses conducted in the Observed phase, we used significant clusters identified during the Solo phase. Specifically, regions showing significant activation for Prob(chosen) in the DM0 (thresholded at P < 0.001) were selected as ROIs. Three ROI clusters were defined: the vStr (peak voxel at [x = 3, y = 14, z = -10], k<sub>E</sub> = 9), vmPFC (peak voxel at [x = –3, y = 62, z = –13], k<sub>E</sub> = 99), and dACC (peak voxel at [x = 12, y = 32, z = 29], k<sub>E</sub> = 118). These ROIs were then applied in the Observed phase analyses to test whether similar neural representations are also engaged in social contexts.

      Term-based meta-analytic maps from Neurosynth for small volume correction. To reduce the likelihood of false positives arising from random significant activations and to enhance sensitivity within regions of theoretical interest, small volume correction (SVC) was applied using term-based meta-analytic maps from Neurosynth. This approach allows for hypothesis-driven correction by restricting statistical testing to anatomically and functionally defined ROI. Specifically, three meta-analytic maps were generated using Neurosynth’s term-based analyses (Yarkoni et al., 2011), with a false discovery rate (FDR) corrected P < 0.01 and a cluster size > 100 voxels. For each resulting cluster, we defined a spherical ROI with a 10 mm radius centered on the cluster’s center of gravity. For each anatomically distinct brain region, only a single center of gravity was identified and used to define the corresponding ROI.

      First, to identify regions encoding final decision probabilities during the Solo phase and enhance sensitivity, we used the meta-map associated with the term “decision” to identify neural substrates of value-based decision-making. This yielded three clusters: vmPFC ([x = -3, y = 38, z = -10]), vStr ([x = 12, y = 11, z = -7]), and dACC ([x = 3, y = 26, z = 44]) (Fig. 3a, S7). Second, to examine social processing during the Observed phase, we used the meta-map associated with the term “social” to identify brain regions typically involved in social cognition. This analysis revealed clusters, including the rTPJ ([x = 51, y = -52, z = 14]) and lTPJ ([x = -51, y = -58, z = 17]) (Fig. 3c, S8a). Third, to define an ROI involved in processing social cues independent of valuation, we used a meta-map associated with “social” but excluding “value”, isolating regions specific to non-valuation-related social cognition. This analysis revealed a cluster, including the dmPFC ([x = 0, y = 50, z = 14]) (Fig. 3d, 4a, S8b).

      (3) How did the authors ensure there were enough trials to generate a reliable BOLD signal? The scanned part of the study seems relatively short.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concern regarding the number of trials and the potential implications for the reliability of the resulting BOLD signals. While we did not conduct formal statistical tests to determine the optimal number of trials, our task design, in general, followed well-established principles in functional neuroimaging. Specifically, we employed a jittered event-related design and used both temporal and dispersion derivatives in the GLM analyses. These strategies are widely recognized for enhancing the efficiency of BOLD signal deconvolution and improving model fit by accounting for inter-subject and inter-regional variability in the hemodynamic response function (HRF). Furthermore, the number of trials per condition in our study was comparable to those reported in previous publications (20-30 trials) that employed similar gambling paradigms to examine individual differences in the neural substrates of value-based decision-making (Chung et al., 2015; Chung et al., 2020).

      (4) It would be helpful to add whether any brain areas survived whole-brain correction.

      No brain regions survived whole-brain correction. Nevertheless, as described in the introduction, we had strong a priori hypotheses. Based on these hypotheses, we defined term-based ROIs using Neurosynth, and conducted small volume correction analyses. Per the reviewer’s suggestion, we have added information indicating that no brain regions survived whole-brain correction, as follows:

      [Page 8, Line 281]

      No additional regions survived whole-brain correction.

      (5) There is a concern that mediation cannot be used to make causal inferences and much larger samples are needed to support claims of mediation. The authors should change the term mediation in order to not imply causality (they could talk about indirect effects instead) and highlight that the mediation analyses are exploratory as they would not be sufficiently powered (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2843527/).

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concerns regarding the causal interpretation of mediation analysis results. Per this comment, we have revised the manuscript as follows to avoid overinterpreting these results and to refrain from implying any causal inference.

      [Page 9, Line 327]

      Given that our sample size is smaller than the recommended threshold for detecting mediation effects (Fritz & MacKinnon, 2007), this significant indirect effect should be interpreted with caution, particularly with respect to causal inference.

      (6) The authors may want to speculate on lifespan differences in this susceptibility to risk preferences given recent evidence that older adults are relatively more susceptible to impulsive social influence (Zhu et al, 2024, comms psychology).

      We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful suggestion—we believe the referenced work is Zhilin Su et al. (2024). As noted in our manuscript, all participants in the current study were young adults aged between 18 and 29 years. Given this limited age range, our dataset does not provide sufficient variability to directly examine age-related differences across the lifespan. However, we are planning a follow-up study using the same task with older adult participants, which we believe will provide a valuable opportunity to address this important gap in understanding susceptibility to social influence across the lifespan.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Felipe and colleagues try to answer an important question in Sarbecovirus Orf9b-mediated interferon signaling suppression, given that this small viral protein adopts two distinct conformations, a dimeric β-sheet-rich fold and a helix-rich monomeric fold when bound by Tom70 protein. Two Orf9b structures determined by X-ray crystallography and Cryo-EM suggest an equilibrium between the two Orf9b conformations, and it is important to understand how this equilibrium relates to its functions. To answer these questions, the authors developed a series of ordinary differential equations (ODE) describing the Orf9b conformation equilibrium between homodimers and monomers binding to Tom70. They used SPR and a fluorescent polarization (FP) peptide displacement assay to identify parameters for the equilibrium and create a theoretical model. They then used the model to characterize the effect of lipid-binding and the effects of Orf9b mutations in homodimer stability, lipid binding, and dimer-monomer equilibrium. They used their model to further analyze dimerization, lipid binding, and Orf9b-Tom70 interactions for truncated Orf9b, Orf9b fusion mutant S53E (blocking Tom70 binding), and Orf9b from a set of Sars-CoV-2 VOCs. They evaluated the ability of different Orf9b variants for binding Tom70 using Co-IP experiments and assessed their activity in suppressing IFN signaling in cells.

      Overall, this work is well designed, the results are of high quality and well-presented; the results support their conclusions.

      We thank reviewer #1 for their thoughtful assessment of our work and their constructive feedback.

      Strengths:

      (1) They developed a working biophysical model for analyzing Orf9b monomer-dimer equilibrium and Tom70 binding based on SPR and FP experiments; this is an important tool for future investigation.

      (2) They prepared lipid-free Orf9b homodimer and determined its crystal structure.

      (3) They designed and purified obligate Orf9b monomer, fused-dimer, etc., a very important Orf9b variant for further investigations.

      (4) They identified the lipid bound by Orf9b homodimer using mass spectra data.

      (5) They proposed a working model of Orf9b-Tom70 equilibrium.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) It is difficult to understand why the obligate Orf9b dimer has similar IFN inhibition activity as the WT protein and obligate Orf9b monomer truncations.

      We thank the reviewer for their observation and agree that the obligate homodimer IFN results were not what we expected to observe given our FP kinetic results with the purified obligate homodimer and noted our surprise in the discussion. We also note that we have two possible hypotheses for why this is the case.

      In our discussion, we noted the possible introduction of an increased avidity effect with fused homodimer and have improved it as follows with additions in red:

      “This result was unexpected as we had anticipated the obligate homodimer results to resemble the phosphomimetic. We hypothesize that this may be explained by two possible factors. First, we can’t exclude the introduction of an increased avidity between Orf9b and Tom70 when using the fused homodimer. Although our modeled decrease in the association rate of Orf9b:Tom70 (which increases the K<sub>D</sub> of the complex) suggests that fusing two copies of Orf9b decreases the affinity to Tom70, one copy of the fusion construct could also be capable of either binding to two copies of Tom70, or, one copy of the fusion could undergo rapid rebinding to Tom70. These effects would lead to a much tighter interaction in cellular assays than we modeled in vitro. A second possible explanation is that our assumptions about high lipid binding are not valid for cell based assays.”

      We also noted that a second possible explanation is due to our limitations in isolating the apo-fused homodimer to compare to the lipid-bound fused homodimer and possible differences this could have on our assays and briefly expanded upon this. Again, we improved this with additions in red:

      “As we have shown with both WT and fusion constructs, recombinantly expressed and purified Orf9b is lipid-bound and this can stabilize the homodimer to slow or inhibit the binding to Tom70. For the Orf9b fusion construct, we attempted to isolate the lipid-free species through protein refolding as previously described to compare the effect of lipid-binding on the homodimer fusion (similar to our WT experiments); however, we could not recover the stably folded homodimer. We hypothesize that the discrepancy between our kinetic results and Co-IP/IFN results could be due to subsaturation of the Orf9b fusion homodimers by lipids in cell based assays. While we have shown that lipid-binding occurs in recombinant expression systems, it is possible that in our cell based signaling assays that lipid-binding only affects a minor population of Orf9b. Given that we were unable to isolate the apo-fusion homodimer, we could not directly compare whether there are differences in fusion homodimer stability in the presence or absence of lipid-binding. Therefore, it is possible that the apo-fusion homodimer undergoes unfolding and refolding into alpha helices that lead to Tom70 binding similar to the WT construct.”

      (2) The role of Orf9b homodimer and the role of Orf9b-bound lipid in virus infection, remains unknown.

      We agree that we did not try to directly test for the role of the homodimer during infection and this remains an open area of exploration for future studies. We have included this caveat in our discussion but suggested possible experiments and future directions that could help shed light on this:

      “Although we have not directly tested for the role the homodimer conformation plays during infection, we have demonstrated that lipid-binding to the homodimer can bias the equilibrium away from Tom70. Lipids including palmitate have been shown to act as both a signaling molecule as well as a post-translational modification during antiviral innate immune signaling (S Mesquita et al. 2024; Wen et al. 2022; S. Yang et al. 2019). As a post-translational modification (referred to as S-acylation), MAVS, a mitochondrial type 1 IFN signaling protein that associates with Tom70 (X.-Y. Liu et al. 2010; McWhirter, Tenoever, and Maniatis 2005; Seth et al. 2005), has been shown to be post-translationally palmitoylated which affects its ability to localize to the mitochondrial outer membrane during viral infection and is a known target of Orf9b (Bu et al. 2024; Lee et al. 2024). When this is impaired (either by mutation or by depletion of the palmitoylation enzyme ZDHHC24), IFN activation is impaired (Bu et al. 2024). Therefore, future investigations should consider if the homodimer conformation of Orf9b is capable of antagonizing other IFN signaling factors such as MAVS by binding to palmitoyl groups. Indeed, Orf9b has already been shown to be capable of binding to MAVS by Co-IP (Han et al. 2021), however, whether or not this occurs through the palmitoyl modification remains unknown.”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study focuses on Orf9b, a SARS-COV1/2 protein that regulates innate signaling through interaction with Tom70. San Felipe et al use a combination of biophysical methods to characterize the coupling between lipid-binding, dimerization, conformational change, and protein-protein-interaction equilibria for the Orf9b-Tom70 system. Their analysis provides a detailed explanation for previous observations of Orf9b function. In a cellular context, they find other factors may also be important for the biological functioning of Orf9b.

      Strengths:

      San Felipe et al elegantly combine structural biology, biophysics, kinetic modelling, and cellular assays, allowing detailed analysis of the Orf9b-Tom70 system. Such complex systems involving coupled equilibria are prevalent in various aspects of biology, and a quantitative description of them, while challenging, provides a detailed understanding and prediction of biological outcomes. Using SPR to guide initial estimates of the rate constants for solution measurements is an interesting approach.

      Weaknesses:

      This study would benefit from a more quantitative description of uncertainties in the numerous rate constants of the models, either through a detailed presentation of the sensitivity analysis or another approach such as MCMC. Quantitative uncertainty analysis, such as MCMC is not trivial for ODEs, particularly when they involve many parameters and are to be fitted to numerous data points, as is the case for this study. The authors use sensitivity analysis as an alternative, however, the results of the sensitivity analysis are not presented in detail, and I believe the authors should consider whether there is a way to present this analysis more quantitatively. For example, could the residuals for each +/-10% parameter change for the peptide model be presented as a supplementary figure, and similarly for the more complex models? Further details of the range of rate constants tested would be useful, particularly for the ka and kB parameters.

      We thank the reviewer for their constructive feedback and have generated supplemental figures providing a deeper analysis of the residuals for each model parameter adjusted +/- 10% from the reported values which we have added to our supplemental figures as Figure 1 - Supplemental 3 and Figure 4 - Supplemental 5  .

      We note that there are modest improvements in residual plots where model parameters are individually lowered by 10% from their reported value when considering this single dataset, however, our choice of using the reported values was driven by finding values that were suitable for improving model behavior across multiple concentration series in different datasets. Specifically, we have also included the RMSD values for each model parameter subjected to a +/-10% change from a single concentration time course as well as the percent change in RMSD relative to the RMSD generated by our reported model parameters to illustrate this. We have also included text that makes note of the observed pattern in the residuals from Figure 4 - Supplement 5 and provided some explanations for why this may occur.

      “Inspection of the residuals from the 5uM apo-Orf9b homodimer time course showed clear patterns when individual model parameters were subjected to a 10% increase or decrease from the reported values. While our proposed model qualitatively describes the concentration dependent change in kinetic behavior, the residual plots may suggest that additional binding reactions may also be occurring that are not captured by our model.”

      Figure 1 - Supplemental 3. Plots of residuals from Orf9b peptide model showing effect of an increase or decrease by 10% on each model parameter. All residuals and reporting are with respect to the100uM of unlabeled Orf9b peptide condition. Blue dots: reported value. Red dots: 10% increase in reported value. Green dots: 10% decrease in reported value. Table reporting of RMSD values for model fitsafter +/-10% change to model parameter (Left column) and percent change in RMSD relative to reported model RMSD (Right column).

      “As an alternative to attempting to place CIs on the parameters, we performed sensitivity analysis to determine which parameters the model was most sensitive to (see methods and Figure 1 - Supplemental 3). Additionally, we note that the model parameters were derived from the fit of only one concentration (100uM), but fit the other concentrations equally well. We observed that the model parameter that was most sensitive to change was the rate of Orf9b-FITC:Tom70 ([PT]) dissociation when subjected to a 10% increase or decrease whereas all other model parameters showed no sensitivity to change (Figure 1 - Supplemental 3).”

      Figure 4 - Supplemental 5: Plot of residuals showing the effect of increasing or decreasing individual model parameters 10% compared to the reported values. All residual plots are with respect to the 5uM apo-Orf9b homodimer condition. Blue dots: reported value. Red dot: 10% increase in reported value. Green dot: 10% decrease in reported value. (Left columns) Table of RMSD values calculated from model fits showing the effect of both +/-10% change to individual model parameters. (Right columns) Percent change in RMSD values subjected to +/-10% change for individual model parameters relative to the RMSD of the reported model.

      We have also included the following revised text to accompany this figure.

      “Further, we repeated the sensitivity analysis described previously for the peptide model and also considered the sensitivity of model parameters by inspecting each individually (Figure 4- figure supplemental 5). We found that when examining the residuals of the lowest concentration of 5uM, the model was most sensitive to changes in three parameters: the rate of homodimer association and dissociation and the conversion from β to α-monomers.”

      “Therefore, under low concentrations of Orf9b homodimer, binding to Tom70 is limited by the rate of homodimer association and dissociation as well as the conversion of Orf9b monomers to the α-helical conformation.”

      We have also included a supplemental figure showing how changes in the model parameters ka and kB affect the models behavior to help illustrate the range of values tested as Figure 4 - Supplemental 4.

      Figure 4 - Supplemental 4: Plots of model behavior showing the effect of changes to alpha-beta and beta-alpha monomer  interconversion rates compared to experimental values. Data is modeled with respect to the apo-Orf9b homodimer 5uM condition. Black line represents reported model fit and values used.

      We have also incorporated the following revised text.

      “The model parameters k<sub>a</sub> and k<sub>B</sub> describe the rate of interchange between the β-sheet and α-helix monomer conformations. These parameters must be estimated by modeling because our assays do not allow us to directly measure the folding rates between these conformations. To identify these values, we performed a scan of k<sub>a</sub> and k<sub>B</sub> values that yielded the best agreement between the model and the experimental conditions (Figure 4 - figure supplemental 4).”

      The authors build a model that incorporates an α-helix-β-sheet conformational change, but the rate constant for the conversion to the α-helix conformation is required to be second order. Although the authors provide some rationale, I do not find this satisfactorily convincing given the large number of adjustable parameters in the model and the use of manual model fitting. The authors should discuss whether there is any precedence for second-order rate constants for conformational changes in the literature. On page 14, the authors state this rate constant "had to be non-linear in the monomer β-sheet concentration" - how many other models did the authors explore? For example, would αT↔α↔αα↔ββ (i.e., conformational change before dimer dissociation) or α↔βαT↔ββ (i.e., Tom70 binding driving dimer dissociation) be other plausible models for the conformational change that do not require assumptions of second-order rate constants for the conformational change?

      We thank the reviewer for their feedback. During our studies, we tested several models prior to the final one presented in Figure 4A. The first model that we tested as described in Figure 4 - Supplemental 3 described ββ↔α↔αT with no conformational change. We tested several models that integrated the existing structural data for both Orf9b and Tom70 and found that while these models could fit individual time series, they did not explain the concentration dependent changes in subsequent time series nor did they explain changes induced by lipid-binding and mutations in VOC.

      With respect to the possibilities of αT↔α↔αα↔ββ and α↔βαT↔ββ models, we have revised our manuscript to mention that we did test additional models before we settled on the model that we presented.

      “We tested different reaction schemes that incorporated the interconversion between β-sheet to α-helix conformations by considering models that described a conformational change in the homodimer leading to Tom70 binding rather than monomers. None of these models adequately described our experimental results, therefore we continued developing our model as outlined in Figure 4D”

      With respect to the second-order rate describing the fold change from β to α, we have added the revised text to the manuscript:

      “We initially tested the impact of keeping the rate constant k<sub>a</sub> first order, just like k<sub>B</sub> which did yield the sigmoidal behavior we observed in the 5uM apo-homodimer condition. However, this assumption failed to describe the data at other concentrations resulting in a substantial overestimation compared to our experimental results when holding k<sub>B</sub> at a constant value throughout. We found that when the β-sheet to α-helix rate (k<sub>a</sub> ) was made a second order rate constant, we were able to hold the rate constant across all concentrations tested suggesting a non-linearity in the monomer β-sheet concentration.”

      While this was surprising to us, we reasoned that a biological explanation for why the conversion from β to α was second order was that the β-monomers may transiently self-associate to cooperatively fold into the α-helical conformation. We did acknowledge this choice to make the β to α parameter non-linear (unlike the α to β conversion which was single order).

      We concede that we could not find specific examples describing non-linear kinetics comparable to the system we described in literature, however, such systems have been reported for proteins that exhibit high structural plasticity where transient interactions with another copy of the protein or another protein altogether drive folding changes and we have revised this manuscript to include some additional citations to papers that describe such systems (Zuber et al. 2022; Tuinstra et al. 2008).

      Overall, this study progresses the analysis of coupled equilibria and provides insights into Orf9b function.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) What was the unlabeled Orf9b peptide is added to the pre-equilibrated Orf9b-FITC:Tom70 solution as a competitor? Figure 1D illustrates that the competitor was full-length Orf9b.

      We have revised the figure to illustrate that in this experiment, the competitor is the unlabeled FITC peptide and not the full length Orf9b sequence

      (2) Figure 2B, what is the higher Mw peak from refolded Orf9b homodimer.

      We have added the following revised text (highlighted in red) to the manuscript to clarify Figure 2B.

      “The SEC elution profile and retention volume of refolded Orf9b directly overlapped with natively folded homodimeric Orf9b and suggested a high recovery of the refolded homodimer with the early eluting peaks corresponding to either a chaperone-bound species (natively folded) or misfolded protein (refolded) as judged by SDS-PAGE (Figure 2B). Together, the overlap in elution peaks corresponding to the folded homodimer suggested a high recovery of the homodimer from the refolding conditions.”

      (3) Figure 2C, in the main text, the authors state that "...observed that the refolded homodimer structure closely aligned with the lipid-bound reference structure, which shows that the homodimer fold can be recovered after denaturing". Please provide structural comparison details here, software used? Rmsd and Dali Z-score.

      We have added the following revised text (highlighted in red) to the manuscript to clarify Figure 2C.

      “Aligning the structure of the Orf9b homodimer (PDB 6Z4U) with our structure of the refolded Orf9b homodimer (9N55) in Pymol resulted in an RMSD of 1.1Å. Further, we also searched our structures of the refolded Orf9b homodimer on the Dali server against the existing structures of the lipid-bound Orf9b homodimer which yielded a Z-score of 2.2 which shows good correspondence between the structures.”

      (4) To prove the refolded Orf9b homodimer did not contain lipid, could the authors provide mass spectra data for the refolded Orf9b sample and compare it with the results in Figure 2 - Supplemental 1.

      We do not have complete mass spectra data for the refolded homodimer samples, however, we feel that the native mass spectrometry data provides a good orthogonal comparison between natively folded and refolded samples for the presence or absence of lipids. We concede that we only used mass spectrometry to characterize the four peaks that were unique to the natively folded deconvoluted spectra which confirmed that shift in mass relative to the expected homodimer molecular weight corresponded to the two lipids we presented. However, we would expect that performing mass spectrometry on the refolded sample would only further confirm our observations from the crystal structures and the native mass spectrometry.

      (5) Have the authors tried to use analytical ultracentrifugation to analyze the Orf9b dimer-monomer equilibrium, given that AUC provides a much more accurate measurement of molecular mass?

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion and agree that AUC could be an additional useful strategy for monitoring the dimer-monomer equilibrium and provide additional validation of the molecule weights of both the monomer and homodimer.

      While we have not performed AUC, we have revised our manuscript to include more discussion about the determination of molecular weights by SEC.

      “For the Orf9b homodimer, the retention volume was consistent with molecular weight standards based on the expected molecular weight of the homodimer (~21kDa) and the standard (~29kDa). In the case of the Orf9b monomer, although we would expect the retention volume of the monomer (~10.6kDA) to be between the molecular weight standards of 13.4kDa and 6.5kDa, the greater retention volume could be explained by non-specific hydrophobic interactions between the monomeric Orf9b and the column.”

      (6) The authors used truncation of 7 C-terminal amino acids to generate an obligate Orf9b monomer for their assays. It would be interesting to mutate residues at the homodimer interface to generate Orf9b monomers rather than deleting residues. For example, mutate 91-96aa (FVVVTV) to negatively charged residues, which will not only disrupt the dimerization interface, but also impair lipid binding. The dimer interface mutant should then be tested in their SPR, FP assays, as well as IFN inhibition assays.

      We thank the reviewer for their suggestion and agree that mutation of the 7 C-terminal amino acids into negatively charged residues could be an interesting alternative strategy to generating an obligate Orf9b monomer without the need for truncating the residues. Our choice of using the truncated construct we proposed was driven by our analysis of the structure of the homodimer which reveals that a significant portion of the dimer interface is composed of backbone-backbone hydrogen bonding between the two chains of Orf9b. We reasoned that truncating these residues would be the most effective way to compromise the interface between the two chains and drive a predominantly monomeric behavior, however, compromising the interface with multiple mutations is an intriguing alternative.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The authors could comment on the slow monomer-dimer exchange observed by SEC and how it fits with their other analysis.

      We thank the reviewer for their comment and concede that the slow exchange may be a limitation of this experimental setup. Our observations from our SPR experiments and modeling showed us that the homodimer may be fast to dissociate into monomer given the off rate which would suggest a half-life for the homodimer to be on the order of seconds, however, we still observe a noticeable dimer species on the chromatograms. We initially allowed the diluted samples to reach equilibrium prior to injection onto the analytical sizing column, however, it is possible that the system is still in a pre-equilibrium prior to injection onto the column. This could be driven by interactions between the protein and the column that prevents full dissociation of the homodimer. While this is a limitation, we note that we did not use the Kd value that we determined by non-linear regression fitting to the equilibrium observed on the chromatograms for downstream experiments but instead used the value to get a ballpark estimate for the homodimer Kd which is on the same order as the Kd determined by SPR.

      (2) It might be useful to include the rate constants on the reaction arrows of the schematic representation of the models.

      We have revised Figure 4D to include the rates for both Orf9b monomer binding to Tom70 and Orf9b binding to Orf9b as derived from the SPR experiments as well as the modeled values for the interconversion between α and β monomers. We also revised Figure 7 to include these values as well as the modeled dissociation rate for homodimer when lipid-bound.

      (3) I couldn't find how the sensitivity analysis was performed for the more complex models. Was this the same +/- 10% as per the peptide model?

      We used the same +/- 10% sensitivity analysis for the peptide model in the more complex equilibrium model and have revised our manuscript to clearly reflect that.

      (4) Further clarification of "inspection of residuals suggested that the fits were accurate". In Figure 1B, the residues look to have systematic errors, perhaps indicating other processes occurring.

      We agree that in the SPR kinetic fitting results for the Orf9b peptide binding to Tom70 in Figure 1B that there are some regions where the fit over or under estimates the experimental results. This is partially the result of limitations in the number of different binding models that we can fit in the analysis software which is why we reported using a 1:1 langmuir binding model. It is certainly possible that there may be some additional binding reactions that occur, however, we limited our use of these specific kinetic results to the peptide model that we proposed in Figure 1D. We did note in the manuscript text that it was necessary for us to change the model parameter values to some extent in order to fit our experimental results which may be partially explained by the SPR fitting errors.

      “With the parameter set obtained from the 100µM condition, we then held all parameters fixed and simply changed the peptide concentrations in the model to fit the remaining conditions by hand. We note that this process saw the model parameter values change between 3% at the lowest end up to 70% at the highest end from the experimentally derived values but remained within an order of magnitude of the experimental SPR values. We speculate that this arises due to the differences in experimental setup between SPR and FP-based methods of measuring kinetics.”

      (5) The manuscript builds logically, but given the sophisticated nature of the system and the modelling could benefit from more clarity/streamlining in the descriptions/illustrations.

      We have revised our manuscript in response to both reviewers comments and hope that the clarity of the work is improved as a result.

      (6) Figure 4 Supplement 3 - where did the rate constants for Model 1 come from? Was there any attempt to alter them to fit the data better?

      We have clarified in the figure description that the rate constants used in Model 1 were the same values used in Figure 4B (but without the interconversion between beta and alpha rates).

      “Comparison of kinetic model 1 and 2 in describing experimental results from the kinetic binding assay. Experimental results using 10uM of refolded Orf9b homodimer are shown as rings with the predicted behavior of model 1 (equilibrium exchange) shown as a dark blue line. The predicted behavior of model 2 (equilibrium exchange with a conformational change between β-sheet and ɑ-helical monomers) is shown as the light blue line. Model parameter values were the same as described in Figure 4D and kept constant in both model comparisons.”

      (7) What are and [PT] in the second set of equations (page 13)?

      [‘PT] refers to the concentration of “fluorescent probe” (Orf9b-FITC) and Tom70.

      (8) "Additionally, the fused homodimer association rate (which can be viewed as a rate of tertiary complex formation)" - can the authors provide a mathematical proof for this?

      In the case of the fused homodimer kinetic data, we did not develop a separate model to explicitly take into account the differences between using a fused construct versus the WT construct that can dissociate into monomers. We have clarified our interpretation of this in the manuscript.

      “Although our model explicitly describes homodimer dissociation into monomers as a requisite step for Orf9b binding to Tom70, we adapted it for the fusion experimental data. In this case, all model parameters other than the association and dissociation kinetics of the fluorescent probe and Tom70 were adjusted to achieve the best agreement with the experimental data. When applied to the fusion homodimer, the parameters describing homodimer dissociation into separate monomers could instead describe the dissociation of the two β-sheet domains away from each other in the tertiary structure but remaining physically linked through the linker region.”

      (9) "For Lambda and Omicron, the P10S mutation results in the serine being positioned to form several hydrogen bonds between R13 and the backbone carbonyl of A11 and L48 within the same chain..." is this taken from AlphaFold predicted structures of the mutants? If so, it should be made clear that this is derived from predicted structures. And even so, AlphaFold can be poor at determining structures of mutants, and so there is greater uncertainty in the prediction of the bonds.

      For Lambda, Omicron, and Delta mutations, we used Pymol to examine how the placement of mutations could structurally explain the kinetic differences we observed in our model. We have gone back and clarified in the figure description that these predictions are not derived from AlphaFold.

      (10) "biological replicates" - is this different protein purifications?

      Yes, in this case biological replicates refer to different protein purifications for all variants described and tested.

      (11) Are any of the authors involved in the Berkeley Madonna commercial software used in the manuscript? If so, should this be in the conflict of interest statement?

      Yes, Michael Grabe is an owner of Berkeley Madonna, and we have updated our conflicts of interest statement to reflect this.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer 1:

      I would suggest that the authors focus on what I think is the main goal of the work, namely, to consider the whole cell contour when characterizing cell shape instead of only some points on the contour. A reference to the connection with Minkowski tensors and the biologically relevant mathematical consequences of this connection would suffice; a detailed definition of the Minkowski tensors does not seem to be necessary. Especially because you do not really use them. You could use the analysis of the simulation data to explain what the γ<sub>p</sub> miss and for which statements they would be sufficient.

      We argue that the explanation of Minkowski tensors is helpful and should remain in the Methods and materials section. There are two reasons: First, our argumentation relays on the robustness and stability properties of Minkowski tensors. Introducing q<sub>p</sub> without the connection to Minkowski tensors would not allow us to make these statements. Second, Minkowski tensors seem not well known in the community, otherwise measures like γ<sub>p</sub> would not have been introduced. Furthermore, readers not interested in the technical details could skip this part of the manuscript and directly go to the Results section. Concerning the questions, what the γ<sub>p</sub> miss and for which statements they would be sufficient, the answer from a purly mathematical point of view is rather simple: As γ<sub>p</sub> does not share robustness and stability it should not be used in any case! The provided results on computational and experimental data demonstrate the consequences of using such measures. In case of the proposed nematic-hexatic transition in Armengol-Collade et al. (2023) the consequence is severe, as this transition is specific only to the used method but not to the underlying physics. A second aspect which we now further highlight is the influence of approximating a cell by a polygon. We demonstrate that this approximation is responsible for a strong hexatic order on the cellular scale in the considered MDCK data from Armengol-Collade et al. (2023).

      It is not clear to me what we should learn about the two tissue models by using q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> to quantify cell shape. Can you clearly formulate one or more conclusions?

      What we can learn from the research is a dependence of q<sub>p</sub> on model parameters in the two tissue models is

      increases with higher activity or deformability

      decreases with higher activity or deformability.

      Furthermore, q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> are independent and describe distinct properties. Using these models as a basis to coarse-grain and derive continuous models on the tissue scale, these results indicate that more general p-atic liquid crystal theories should be used and the simplest nematic liquid crystal theories might not be sufficient.

      The experimental data and their analysis does not seem to add anything to the work. Do you report only data from independent measurements, or did you consider all images of a monolayer?

      As we now also analyze experimental data from Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) which confirm our findings on independency of q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> and also confirm that the proposed nematic-hexatic transition is only specific to the use of γ<sub>p</sub> for characterizing the shape, additional experimental data are indeed no longer needed. We, therefore, skip the detailed analysis of this data and only keep the results in Fig 1 and Fig 2 and the corresponding figures in the appendix as illustrating examples.

      L13: ”P-atic liquid crystal theories offer new perspectives on how cells self-organize (...)” This is a difficult entry, because the average reader of eLife might not be familiar with p-atic liquid crystals.

      We agree that p-atic liquid crystals might not be familiar to the average reader. For this reason we introduce orientational order in the introduction with examples demonstrating that not only nematic, but also tetratic and hexatic order have been identified in tissue and introduce the different symmetries. Furthermore, we provide examples for p-atic liquid crystals from other fields and various references. In the conclusion, we also cite models for p-atic liquid crystal theories. Even if the average reader is not familiar with these theories, it should become evident that nematic order might not be sufficient to describe tissue as other symmetries are present as well.

      L32: ”nematic” needs to be introduced.

      Nematic order is already explained as rotational order with 180° degrees. The references cited discuss nematic liquid crystals in the context of morphological changes in tissue. We therefore only added a standard text book as reference for liquid crystal theories and refrain introducing it in more detail in the manuscript.

      Figure 1: Why do you show the data for q<sub>3</sub>, q<sub>4</sub>, and q<sub>5</sub>, which you do not really consider in this manuscript? Same for Figure 2. Why not combine the two figures? Furthermore, you show q<sub>p</sub> without having defined them yet.

      We consider all p \= 2,3,4,5,6, but focus on p = 2,6 in the main text and p = 3,4,5 in the appendix. Figures 1 and 2 essentially only introduce the subject and help to relate p-atic order to cell shapes and introduce the methodology to analyze the data. Our conclusion is that all p can be important and should be considered in continuous descriptions of tissue.

      Equation 1: The notation is confusing: the domain of integration (C or ∂C) also appears as the variable you integrate.

      The equation is correct. The variable of integration is 1 or H and the domain of integration is C (cell) or ∂C (cell contour).

      L68: ”a snapshot of the considered monolayer of wild-type MDCK cells”. Did you analyse only one monolayer? Please, provide information about the number of monolayers that were imaged and how many cell shapes were analyzed.

      We have analyzed one monolayer and have added the missing information.

      L86: ”field-specific prefactors” I do not understand what is meant by these.

      Different communities, e.g. physics, mathematics, cosmology, .... use different prefactors in the definition. We have removed this statement.

      L89: ”Hadwiger’s characterization theorem”. What is this?

      This mathematical result is important to claim robustness and stability, it can be found in the cited reference.

      L104: ”the essential property is the continuity”. Essential for what?

      Essential ”for our purpose” to characterize the shape of cells by a robust method.

      L120: ”the theory also guarantees robust description of p-atic orientation for p = 3,4,5,6,...” I do not understand what you mean.

      The previous examples only consider p \= 2. However, the cited theoretical results also hold for p = 3,4,5,6,..

      Equations (5) and (6): You define ψ<sub>p</sub>(C) twice. Are the definitions equivalent? Why do you need both?

      This is not a different definition, equation (6) is a reformulation which is more useful for our purpose. But we indeed define ϑ<sub>p</sub> twice. We now use a new symbol to distinguish ϑ<sub>p</sub> in Equation 7 and 9.

      Figure 4: ”The visualization uses rotationally-symmetric direction fields (known as p-RoSy fields in computer graphics (Vaxman et al., 2016)).” I guess that you have used these fields already in Figure 1, so why introduce them only now?

      We have moved this comment to Figure 1.

      Figure 6: Using a few discrete values cannot illustrate continuity. Also, the ”jump” in γ<sub>p</sub> results from deleting a vertex, so I doubt that this is a fair comparison. Still, I think that it is important to point out to the reader that the value γ<sub>p</sub> depends on the number of vertices (here, I allow that two edges connected by a vertex are aligned).

      We adjusted the caption to make our point more clear. The last image is a triangle and according to the definition of γ<sub>p</sub> is, therefore, described by only three vertices. So, it is indeed a fair comparison. The reviewer is right that the value of γ<sub>p</sub> has a strong dependency of the number of used vertices, this is exactly the point that we are trying to make with this figure. Also, adding vertices artificially to make γ<sub>p</sub> continuous leads to more problems, as the values for γ<sub>p</sub> change if we change the number of vertices. But an equilateral triangle should be recognized as an equilateral triangle, no matter if there is an artificial fourth vertex or not. The triangle in our picture and the triangle that the reviewer mentioned (so our triangle with an artificial fourth vertex) both have the shape of an equilateral triangle, yet for one it is |γ<sub>3</sub>| = 1.0 and for the other one it is |γ<sub>3</sub>| = 0.935.

      While we agree on the reviewers statement about continuity, we did not modify the sentence, as the meaning should be clear.

      L160: The definition of the center of mass is incorrect as it is not that of an extended object whose contour is defined by a polygon, but only of the set of vertices. In Figure 6 you write ”the choice of the center of mass highly influences the value of γ<sub>p</sub>” - is there really a choice of the center of mass? I thought that it was uniquely defined.

      We here only repeat the definition from Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) in order to be able to directly compare our analyses with the results presented therein. We adjusted the caption to be more clear.

      L166: What is the weighting you refer to in Equation 9?

      We apologize, the reference is to Equation 8. We have modified this.

      L312: ”Quantifying orientational order in biological tissues can be realized by Minkowsky tensors”. As mentioned above, you do not really use them, but use Equation (7), which can be defined without reference to Minkowski tensors.

      Eq. (7) is part of the irreducible representations of the Minkowsky tensor. Therefore the sentence is correct.

      L318: I do not quite understand the link between being able (or not) to compare q<sub>p</sub>’s for different values of p and the interpretability of q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub>. Also, since you introduce q<sub>p</sub>, how can the question about their comparability be a recurrent challenge? Finally, would you agree that even though a comparison between the absolute values of q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> is inappropriate, one can still meaningfully compare relative changes as a parameter is changed or when comparing cells in different conditions?

      We have modified the sentence. Furthermore we agree that one can still meaningfully compare relative changes as a parameter is changed, as we do. However, our claim that q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> are independent, does not allow to conclude any kind of nematic-hexatic phase transition. We have now provided further evidence using the published data of Armengol-Collado et al. (2023), which unequivocally supports this statement. We would also like to remark that the detection of a phase-transition requires a single order parameter, which cannot exist as q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> are independent.

      We have further explained this in the main text.

      Figure 7: The axes are not labeled.

      We added the labels.

      L359: ”q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> values cluster tightly”, L362 ”q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> values become highly scattered” Please, quantify.

      We kept these formulations but have added statistical measures to these qualitative descriptions, see Supplementary Figures to Fig 7 for the distance correlation and the P-values of the distance correlation. These data support our claim of independence.

      L362: ”each q<sub>2</sub> value spans a broad range of q<sub>6</sub> values and vice versa, demonstrating their independence”. Please, use a quantitative test of statistical independence.

      We have added statistical information by using the distance correlation and statistical tests, see Supplementary Figures to Fig 7. Similar results are obtained for the Pearson correlation and corresponding tests. However, they are not included as the distance correlation is more general.

      L371: Please, define Q<sub>2</sub> and Q<sub>6</sub> in the main text.

      We have now added the definition to the Materials and methods section.

      L420: A reference seems to be missing.

      Thanks for pointing this out. This was a formatting error, we only wanted to cite Balasubramaniam et al. (2021).

      L425: ”strong dependence of cell shape on cell density”. But q<sub>6</sub> seems to be rather independent of density, see Figure 11. Also, what do you mean by ”strong”? Can you quantify?

      The dependency of the cell shape on the cell density is shown in detail in (Eckert et al., 2023). Furthermore, to describe the cell shape the values for all p are needed. So the change in q<sub>2</sub> already indicates a change in the overall cell shape even as q<sub>6</sub> is barely changing. As we excluded these experimental results now in favor of the experimental data also used in Armengol-Collado et al. (2023), we did not add further evaluations regarding cell density.

      L453 ”These divergences [nonmonotonic dependence of γ<sub>p</sub> on activity or deformability] highlight the limitations of γ<sub>p</sub> in capturing consistent patterns”. I am not sure to follow your argument here.

      Besides the quantitative differences seen in comparing Fig. 1 and Fig 2 with the corresponding figures in the appendix, these results show qualitative differences. Using a method which is not robust and not continuous leads to qualitative different results. The nonmonotonic dependence of γ<sub>p</sub> is specific to the method but not to the underlying physics.

      Appendix 3 - Figure 20: It is not clear how to compare this figure to Figure 3e of Armengol-Collado et al 2023. Please, provide more details.

      Appendix 3 - Figure 20 (Appendix 3 - Figure 25 in the revised version) and Figure 3e in Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) cannot be directly compared. Fig 3e shows results of experiments and multiphase field simulations for one parameter stetting and Fig 20 results of the active vertex model for various parameter settings. But both are considered using γ<sub>p</sub> and Γ<sub>p</sub>. We have added these computation, see Fig. 13, which indeed reproduces the results from Fig 3e. We refrain from considering corresponding plots to Fig 20 for the multiphase field model, as this first requires computing the vertices and no additional information can be expected.

      Reviewer 2:

      The manuscript lacks statistical information. The following should be addressed: How often have the experiments been performed? How many monolayers have been analyzed? How many time steps have been considered and in what duration? How many cells have been included in the analysis? What are the p-values to determine if q<sub>p</sub>’s (Figure 2, panel a) and γ<sub>p</sub>’s (Appendix 3-Figure 17, panel a) are significantly different? Same figures: How many cells and experiments have been considered here? Figure 11: What is the density of cells for each condition? Please provide the corresponding values. How significant are the differences? How many times has the experiment been repeated? Figure 12: Due to cell proliferation, the cell density changes over time. Does this need to be taken into account?

      We agree, our information have only been qualitative. We have added the missing information. Especially we added statistical information by using the distance correlation and statistical tests, see Supplementary Figures to Fig. 7. Similar results are obtained for the Pearson correlation and corresponding tests (not included). As we excluded the experimental results previously shown in Figure 11 and Figure 12, in the revised version in favor of the experimental data that is already published in Armengol-Collado et al. (2023), we did not add further statistics regarding this. We added the number of frames and cells in the text.

      The image analysis part of the Method section states that time-series were xy-drift corrected, and cells were tracked. However, the manuscript does not contain results of dynamical data, timedependent analyses, or discussions of how q<sub>p</sub> changes over time. The authors mention that the fluidity of the tissue was confirmed by the MSD, neighbor number variance, and the self-intermediate scattering function, but none of the results are shown in the manuscript. I would like to ask the authors to provide the results and related content in the Method section.

      We have modified the description and removed all parts related to dynamical data. Due to the heavy overload of images in the manuscript we refrain from providing all the results for the phase diagram to distinguish solid and fluid phase. These measures have been provided previously for the considered modeling approaches and provide here only a side remark. Our results do not depend on an exact localization of a solid-fluid phase boundary.

      Additional information is missing in the Image analysis part of the Method section. Could the authors provide the information on the image analysis steps between obtaining the segmented image and inputting the parameters for the Minkowski tensor? This should include how the normal vectors have been determined and whether this has been done for all pixels along the contour.

      We added further details in the section Extraction of the contour in Experimental setup in Methods and Materials and also provide the code to compute q<sub>p</sub> for segmented images.

      The authors have analyzed low-resolution phase contrast images acquired with a 10x objective to experimentally support their introduced Minkowski tensors. This may have decreased the resolution of the cell boundary detection and its curvature. I strongly suggest imaging the tissue with higher magnification (40x or 63x) and/or fluorescent markers to visualize the cell boundaries in high quality. This would allow the authors to distinguish between circles and circle-like shapes (lines 432-434) and to further investigate differences between MDCK wild-type and MDCK E-cad KO cells.

      We agree that higher resolution of the images would be beneficial. However, we are convinced that this will not influence our findings. Instead of performing the experiments with higher magnification or using fluorescent markers, we have considered the experimental data from Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) to support our results.

      The authors have coarse-grained the shape function, Γ<sub>p</sub>, and have chosen the active vertex model (Appendix 3-Figure 20) for comparison with the Minkowski tensors, Q<sub>p</sub> (Appendix 2 Figure 13). In both figures, the hexatic-nematic crossover does not occur. Armengol-Collado et al. have previously reported that the Voronoi model failed to achieve the hexatic-nematic crossover and argued that this is due to the artificial enhancement of the polygon’s hexagonality, leading to high hexatic order at the tissue scale. Since the authors have used the Voronoi-tailing method (line 196), I would like to ask the authors to compare the multiphase field models for Γ<sub>p</sub> andQ<sub>p</sub> instead.

      We would like to mention that we do not consider a Voronoi model but an active vertex model. A Voronoi model is only used for initialization. Both models are certainly related but not identical and claims for a Voronoi model do not need to hold for an active vertex model. The suggested comparison for the multi phasefield model is not an easy task as it requires to compute the vertices from the phase field variables. There are gaps between cells and a reliable algorithm to identify the vertices is a task on its own. We, therefore, refrain from doing these calculations. Instead, we have used the experimental data from Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) for which the polygonal information are provided, see Figure 11. Especially for p \= 6, strong differences can be seen by comparing the PDF obtained by the full shape and the polygonal shape. Indeed, the strong hexatic order at the cellular scale is only a consequence of the approximation by polygons. With this result analysing the multi phasefield data by γ<sub>p</sub> does not add any new information as this first requires an approximation by polygons.

      The authors show the q<sub>p</sub> distributions for the experimental systems (Figure 2, Figure 11). For completeness, I would like to ask the authors to also coarse-grain q<sub>p</sub> and γ<sub>p</sub> of the experimental data as shown for the computational models in Appendix 2 - Figure 13 and Appendix 2 - Figure 14. It would be interesting to see if the hexatic-nematic crossover appears. I would recommend that the authors avoid using the Voronoi tailing of the experimental system, as this may fail to obtain the crossover as explained in (5) above. Instead, I suggest using the real vertex positions for γ<sub>p</sub>, which can be obtained from the segmented images.

      It remains open what is meant by ”the real vertex positions for γ<sub>p</sub>, which can be obtained from the segmented images”. Segmenting the images leads to smooth contours, partly even with gaps between cells. As the magnitude of γ<sub>p</sub> depends on the number of points used in the calculation it is not meaningful to use all points of the contour for calculating γ<sub>p</sub>, as this would lead to artificially low values for |γ<sub>p</sub>|. Identifying the vertex positions for an approximating polygon is an issue of its own and the consequence of this approximation is already mentioned above. For a comparison we therefore added the experimental data from Armengol-Collado et al (2023) and used the provided vertex positions to compute q<sub>p</sub> and γ<sub>p</sub> as well as the raw data and performed the segmentation and used these data to compute q<sub>p</sub>. See Figure 11. These results confirm our findings and show that the proposed nematic-hexatic phase transition is specific to γ<sub>p</sub> to characterize shape.

      In order to show that shape descriptors like the shape function, γ<sub>p</sub>, introduced by Armengol-Collado et al., ’fail to capture the nuance of irregular shapes’ (line 445), the authors have compared γ<sub>p</sub> with the Minkowski tensors, q<sub>p</sub>, using the same dataset (Figure 1 with Appendix 3 - Figure 16, Figure 2 with Appendix 3 - Figure 17, and Figure 4 with Appendix 3 - Figure 15 Appendix 3). I agree that γ<sub>p</sub> and q<sub>p</sub> are different, not showing identical values. However, I see no evidence in these figures that q<sub>p</sub> describes the symmetry of a cell better than γ<sub>p</sub>, since the values are similar and vary quite similarly between different p-atic orders. What is the quantitative difference that shows the failure of the shape function to capture the nuance of irregular shapes?

      The statement already follows from the mathematical properties of robustness and stability, which is illustrated in Fig. 6. The mentioned comparisons for simulation and experimental data only demonstrate that the lack of robustness and stability of γ<sub>p</sub> also leads to different results if applied to averages of cell measures. The differences are twofold, first the approximation of cells by polygons leads to different results, and second even for polygons different results follow, as only one approach is continuous and the other not. This has strong consequences for the proposed nematic-hexatic phase transition if coarse-grained. Our added results for the experimental data from Armengo-Collado et al. (2023) show that this behavior is not a physical feature but only specific to the use of γ<sub>p</sub>.

      The authors claim that the Minkowski tensors provide a ’reliable framework’ and that this framework ’opens new pathways for understanding the role of orientational symmetries in tissue mechanics and development’ (line 78-79). However, the p-atic orders in the experimental systems peak at very low orders of q<sub>p</sub> < 0.3, which may not allow conclusions about (non-)dominant orientational symmetry(ies) of cells. Can this framework be applied to experimental systems? Since the Minkowski tensors display the independence of the hexatic and nematic symmetry, the variations of cell shapes in experimental systems are too strong to provide any additional results (line 437), as stated by the authors, and no crossover was found, while the crossover was reported by Armengol-Collado et al., what new pathways can be opened to study tissues?

      We have added a comparison with experimental data from Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) and demonstrate that the proposed nematic-hexatic transition is only specific to the use of γ<sub>p</sub> for characterizing the shape. So our results first of all essentially close the ”pathway for understanding the role of orientational symmetries in tissue mechanics and development”, which was proposed on this nematic-hexatic transition. On the other side, even if q<sub>p</sub> peaks at relatively low values, the results demonstrate independence of the measures for different p’s, for two different modeling approaches and two different sets of experimental data. This motivates to consider p-atic order for different p simultaneously. Such theories of ”multi”-p-atic liquid crystals, as proposed in the conclusions, are the mentioned new pathways.

      In principle, the introduced Minkowski tensors integrate the orientation of the normal vectors (Equation 6) and consider the perimeter of the contour (Equation 1). Do the tensors distinguish between convex and concave curvature since both are present in tissues? Does a square with 4 concave and a square with 4 convex edges (same curvature) have the same q<sub>p</sub> values?

      For the specific situation of a square with 4 concave or 4 convex edges even p would lead to the same orientation and the same value for q<sub>p</sub>, as even p have a 180 degree symmetry. Odd p would result in the same value for q<sub>p</sub> but in a different orientation ϑ<sub>p</sub>. In more general cases, e.g. shapes with concave and convex edges, no general statements can be made. In general the theoretical results on stability of q<sub>p</sub> only hold for convex shapes. However, as discussed in Methods and materials the known counterexamples for concave shapes are not relevant for cell shapes.

      In lines 169-172 and Figure 6, the authors report a jump in γ<sub>p</sub>. Why has the fourth vertex in the last image been removed? The vertices are essential for the calculation of γ<sub>p</sub>. If the fourth vertex is not removed, the following values result: γ<sub>3</sub> = 0.935 and γ<sub>4</sub> = 0.474, which leads to changes of the same order of magnitude as those of q<sub>p</sub>. I think it is therefore not the choice of the center of mass that ’heavily influences the value of γ<sub>p</sub>’, but the removal of the fourth vertex.

      We adjusted the caption to make our point more clear. The last image is a triangle and according to the definition of γ<sub>p</sub> is therefore described by only three vertices. The reviewer is right that the value of γ<sub>p</sub> has a strong dependency of the number of used vertices, this is exactly the point that we are trying to make with this figure. An equilateral triangle should be recognized as an equilateral triangle, no matter if there is an artificial fourth vertex or not. The triangle in our picture and the triangle that the reviewer described (so our triangle with an artificial fourth vertex) both have the shape of an equilateral triangle, yet for one |γ<sub>3</sub>| = 1.0 and for the other one it is |γ<sub>3</sub>| = 0.935. This can be seen even more clearly if even more artificial vertices on the outline of the equilateral triangle are added, which will decrease |γ<sub>3</sub>| even more. Furthermore, we think there was a misunderstanding regarding our statement about the center of mass. The general problem of γ<sub>p</sub> - so the dependence of the values on the number of vertices - is independent of the calculation of the center of mass. The exact values of γ<sub>p</sub> on the other hand depend on the choice of this. We follow Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) and use the mean of all vertex coordinates as center of mass. If the reviewer would use the center of mass of the equilateral triangle and do the same calculations the resulting values for γ<sub>p</sub> would be different. This is what we meant with ’heavily influences the value of γ<sub>p</sub>’.

      In Appendix 3 - Figure 18, the authors show that the shape function, γ<sub>6</sub>, exhibits a non-monotonic trend as a function of activity and deformability. I have no objection to this statement. However, I would like to ask the authors to check the values for γ<sub>6</sub>. In the bottom-left corner, for example, γ<sub>6</sub> = 0.55. This value seems very low to me. In Appendix 3-Figure 20, |Q<sub>6</sub>| for R/Rcell = 2 is already in this range, while |Q<sub>6</sub>| for R/Rcell = 1 (not shown), corresponding to γ<sub>6</sub>, must be even higher. Also, the parameters p<sub>6</sub> = 3.5 and v<sub>0</sub> = 0.1 should result in a nearly hexagonal lattice, which should be captured with high γ<sub>6</sub> values. I would expect γ<sub>6</sub> to be in the same range as q<sub>6</sub>.

      Many thanks for pointing this out. There are two different points addressed in this question: The first is if |Γ<sub>p</sub>| is too high. We checked the values, |Γ<sub>p</sub>| = 0.5075 for R/R<sub>cell</sub> = 2, so it is lower than = 0.58. The second question is why γ<sub>p</sub> and q<sub>p</sub> are not in the same value range. You are right that for a perfectly hexagonal lattice both should give the same value, namely = = 1.0. However, even at p<sub>6</sub> = 3.5 and v<sub>0</sub> = 0.1 this is not a perfectly hexagonal lattice anymore and how fast the values of q<sub>6</sub> and |γ<sub>6</sub>| drop if we move away from a perfect hexagon scales differently. As q<sub>p</sub> is stable and only changes slightly for slight changes in the shape it makes sense, that q<sub>p</sub> is still close to 1.0 . We included an image, see below, of one time step in said parameter to showcase that cells do not form a perfect hexagonal lattice anymore.

      Reviewer 3:

      Could the authors show why and how this method could bring new information which were missing so far in the understanding of morphogenesis in vitro and in vivo with the current quantification?

      The introduction provides examples of how orientational order and its topological defects can be linked to morphological changes in tissues. The orientational order emerges from the shape of the cells. Most commonly nematic order has been considered, but more recently also hexatic order and even a nematic-hexactic crossover on larger scales. This suggests a mechanical mechanism for morphogenesis, like a phase transition from hexatic to nematic, which would have consequences on the evolution of shape. We demonstrate that the measures q<sub>2</sub> and q<sub>6</sub> are independent. Furthermore the proposed nematic-hexatic transition is only specific to the use of γ<sub>p</sub> for characterizing the shape and coarse-graining of the associated order. These measures are not robust and therefore should not be used. Results for the robust measures q<sub>p</sub> suggest to consider all p for a coarse-grained theory to model morphological changes in tissues.

      Could authors show quantitative comparisons between available methods with the same sets of data and highlight pros and cons?

      Author response image 1.

      Screenshot from p<sub>6</sub> = 3.5 and v<sub>0</sub> = 0.1

      In addition to what was already done for the simulation data we have added data from Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) and compared the results for q<sub>p</sub> and Q<sub>p<sub> and γ<sub>p</sub> and Γ<sub>p</sub>. The theoretical results and the illustrating example in Fig. 6 already show that there are no pros for γ<sub>p</sub>. Other methods belong to the class of bond-order methods and measure neighbor relations instead of shape. We already comment that these methods are inappropriate to classify shape, see Methods and materials, last sentence and Mickel et al. (2013) for a detailed discussion why these methods are not robust.

      Instead of using phase contrast images, which exhibit curved cell-cell contours, could authors use data with E-cadherin staining instead - as used in many epithelial studies in vitro and in vivo? Could they show both images for wild type and for the E-cadherin KO cell lines with fluorescent readout?

      We are convinced that our results do not depend on the way to visualize the cell contours. Furthermore the images do not provide additional information. To further strengthen the experimental part of the manuscript, we instead analyzed data from Armengol-Collado et al. (2023).

      They confirm our findings.

      The authors acknowledge differences in density between cell lines p. 13 so this calls for new experiments with solid readouts and analysis using comparable experimental conditions.

      Additionally, we analyzed data from Armengol-Collado et al. (2023) which confirm our findings. Our results are now supported by two different modeling approaches and two different experimental settings. Because of redundancy we removed the original experimental data from the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Thank you for your thoughtful and constructive feedback on our manuscript. We greatly appreciate your insights regarding our work, as they are invaluable in refining our research.

      We are very happy to hear that you recognize the strengths of our method, particularly the elimination of manual rosette picking, which significantly enhances throughput and reduces variability. We are also pleased that our validation efforts—through flow cytometry, immunocytochemistry, single-cell RNA-sequencing, and functional MEA recordings—effectively demonstrate both the identity and functionality of our derived dorsal forebrain neural rosette stem cells (NRSCs).

      Regarding the identified weaknesses, we agree that a direct comparison with conventional manual-selection protocols, specifically those utilizing dual-SMAD inhibition, would be a significant improvement. To address this, we have initiated additional experiments that will directly compare our single-SMAD inhibition approach (RepSox) with dual-SMAD inhibition (SB/LDN), aiming for a comprehensive evaluation of both protocols.

      In terms of statistical rigor, we appreciate your suggestion on improving our quantitative assays. All data were collected from at least three independent experiments and presented as mean ±standard deviation unless otherwise specified. Due to the qualitative nature of the data, no formal statistical tests were performed for most of the experiments and the mean and standard deviation were calculated for some quantitative measurements obtained, providing a descriptive summary of the data. When possible, we will incorporate appropriate statistical tests, to present our data in a more robust manner, rather than merely reporting mean ± SD.

      Finally, we recognize the importance of situating our work within the broader landscape of neural stem cell research. We aim to elucidate the potential downstream applications for our protocol, which we believe will significantly impact neurodevelopmental and neurodegenerative disorder studies.

      Thank you again for your valuable suggestions. We look forward to refining our manuscript and enhancing the contribution of our research to the field.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Thank you for your thoughtful and constructive feedback on our manuscript. We appreciate your recognition of the novelty and potential impact of our protocol for obtaining neural rosette stem cells (NRSCs). Your comments are invaluable in improving our work.

      We are pleased that you found our methodology to be a significant advancement in the field, particularly the elimination of the manual rosette selection step, which hopefully will enhance homogeneity and standardization. We agree that this development has implications for research, disease modelling, and compound testing.

      Regarding your specific points:

      Passage expansion: Thank you for your insightful suggestion regarding the analysis beyond passage 12. We have continued passaging our NRSC line for more than 12 passages while maintaining the rosette structure. Although we do not yet have comprehensive and detailed analyses at these later passages, we will include some data and relevant information on our findings in the revised manuscript.

      TJP1+ zones: We appreciate your observation regarding the decreased TJP1+ zones at passage 12. We have not consistently detected a reduction in the number of rosettes or TJP1+ lumens across our cultures between passages. While some variability has been noted, we occasionally observe minor reductions at specific time points, followed by a recovery of rosettes in subsequent passages. This suggests that monitoring the number of rosettes is indeed a useful indicator of cell culture health. Cultures should be discarded if rosettes are completely lost. We will take a closer look at this aspect and report the findings in the revised manuscript.

      PAX6 Gene expression verification: Thank you for highlighting the discrepancy between PAX6 gene expression levels and protein levels. Unfortunately, we have not yet validated these results using an alternative technique. One potential explanation for this discrepancy may be the phenomenon of negative autoregulation, where increased levels of PAX6 protein can inhibit its own mRNA expression (Manuel et al., 2007). Moreover, Hsieh and Yang (2009) observed that during neurogenesis, PAX6 protein levels may not correlate linearly with mRNA levels, particularly in variable cellular environments. Additionally, post-transcriptional regulatory mechanisms, such as translation initiation mediated by Internal Ribosome Entry Sites (IRES), have been documented in various contexts involving PAX6, suggesting that mRNA levels may not fully represent functional protein levels in developing tissues (Li et al., 2023). We will go deeper into this discussion in the revised manuscript.

      GFAP Labeling: We appreciate your comments regarding the nuclear labeling of GFAP. In our astrocyte cultures, we have indeed observed GFAP localization in both the nucleus and the cytoplasm (Figure 5B). We will investigate this phenomenon further and provide a clearer explanation, supported by relevant literature, in the revised version. Although GFAP is primarily categorized as an intermediate filament protein localized in the cytoplasm, evidence suggests its nuclear localization may indicate additional regulatory roles during astrocyte development, activation, and pathology. This finding highlights the potential complexity of GFAP's role during fetal development and cellular stress, suggesting a broader functional scope that may extend into the nuclear space.

      Once again, thank you for your insightful feedback and for recognizing the potential of our research. We are committed to addressing your comments and enhancing the quality of our manuscript.

      Manuel, M. et al. (2007) ‘Controlled overexpression of Pax6 in vivo negatively autoregulates the Pax6 locus, causing cell-autonomous defects of late cortical progenitor proliferation with little effect on cortical arealization’, Development, 134(3), pp. 545–555. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1242/dev.02764.

      Hsieh, Y.-W. and Yang, X.-J. (2009) ‘Dynamic Pax6 expression during the neurogenic cell cycle influences proliferation and cell fate choices of retinal progenitors’, Neural Development, 4(1), p. 32. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1186/1749-8104-4-32.

      Li, Q. et al. (2023) ‘Translation of paired box 6 (PAX6) mRNA is IRES-mediated and inhibited by cymarin in breast cancer cells’, Genes & Genetic Systems, 98(4), pp. 161–169. Available at: https://doi.org/10.1266/ggs.23-00039.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors performed genome assemblies for two Fagaceae species and collected transcriptome data from four natural tree species every month over two years. They identified seasonal gene expression patterns and further analyzed species-specific differences.

      Strengths:

      The study of gene expression patterns in natural environments, as opposed to controlled chambers, is gaining increasing attention. The authors collected RNA-seq data monthly for two years from four tree species and analyzed seasonal expression patterns. The data are novel. The authors could revise the manuscript to emphasize seasonal expression patterns in three species (with one additional species having more limited data). Furthermore, the chromosome-scale genome assemblies for the two Fagaceae species represent valuable resources, although the authors did not cite existing assemblies from closely related species.

      Thank you for your careful assessment of our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      Comment; The study design has a fundamental flaw regarding the evaluation of genetic or evolutionary effects. As a basic principle in biology, phenotypes, including gene expression levels, are influenced by genetics, environmental factors, and their interaction. This principle is well-established in quantitative genetics.

      In this study, the four species were sampled from three different sites (see Materials and Methods, lines 543-546), and additionally, two species were sampled from 2019-2021, while the other two were sampled from 2021-2023 (see Figure S2). This critical detail should be clearly described in the Results and Materials and Methods. Due to these variations in sampling sites and periods, environmental conditions are not uniform across species.

      Even in studies conducted in natural environments, there are ways to design experiments that allow genetic effects to be evaluated. For example, by studying co-occurring species, or through transplant experiments, or in common gardens. To illustrate the issue, imagine an experiment where clones of a single species were sampled from three sites and two time periods, similar to the current design. RNA-seq analysis would likely detect differences that could qualitatively resemble those reported in this manuscript.

      One example is in line 197, where genus-specific expression patterns are mentioned. While it may be true that the authors' conclusions (e.g., winter synchronization, phylogenetic constraints) reflect real biological trends, these conclusions are also predictable even without empirical data, and the current dataset does not provide quantitative support.

      If the authors can present a valid method to disentangle genetic and environmental effects from their dataset, that would significantly strengthen the manuscript. However, I do not believe the current study design is suitable for this purpose.

      Unless these issues are addressed, the use of the term "evolution" is inappropriate in this context. The title should be revised, and the result sections starting from "Peak months distribution..." should be either removed or fundamentally revised. The entire Discussion section, which is based on evolutionary interpretation, should be deleted in its current form.

      If the authors still wish to explore genetic or evolutionary analyses, the pair of L. edulis and L. glaber, which were sampled at the same site and over the same period, might be used to analyze "seasonal gene expression divergence in relation to sequence divergence." Nevertheless, the manuscript would benefit from focusing on seasonal expression patterns without framing the study in evolutionary terms.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for the detailed and thoughtful comments. We fully recognize the importance of carefully distinguishing genetic and environmental contributions in transcriptomic studies, particularly when addressing evolutionary questions. The reviewer identified two major concerns regarding our study design: (1) the use of different monitoring periods across species, and (2) the use of samples collected from different study sites. We addressed both concerns with additional analyses using 112 new samples and now present new evidence that supports the robustness of our conclusions.

      (1) Monitoring period variation does not bias our conclusions

      To address concerns about the differing monitoring periods, we added new RNA-seq data (42 samples each for bud and leaf samples for L. glaber and 14 samples each for bud and leaf samples for L. edulis) collected from November 2021 to November 2022, enabling direct comparison across species within a consistent timeframe. Hierarchical clustering of this expanded dataset (Fig. S6) yielded results consistent with our original findings: winter-collected samples cluster together regardless of species identity. This strongly supports our conclusion that the seasonal synchrony observed in winter is not an artifact of the monitoring period and demonstrates the robustness of our conclusions across datasets.

      (2) Site variation is limited and does not confound our findings

      Although the study included three sites, two of them (Imajuku and Ito Campus) are only 7.3 km apart, share nearly identical temperature profiles (see Fig. S2), and are located at the edge of similar evergreen broadleaf forests. Only Q. acuta was sampled from a higher-altitude, cooler site. To assess whether the higher elevation site of Q. acuta introduced confounding environmental effects, we reanalyzed the data after excluding this species. Hierarchical clustering still revealed that winter bud samples formed a distinct cluster regardless of species identity (Fig. S7), consistent with our original finding.

      Furthermore, we recalculated the molecular phenology divergence index D (Fig. 4C) and the interspecific Pearson’s correlation coefficients (Fig. 5A) without including Q. acuta. These analyses produced results that were similar to those obtained from the full dataset (Fig. S12; Fig. S14), indicating that the observed patterns are not driven by environmental differences associated with elevation.

      (3) Justification for our approach in natural systems

      We agree with the reviewer that experimental approaches such as common gardens, reciprocal transplants, and the use of co-occurring species are valuable for disentangling genetic and environmental effects. In fact, we have previously implemented such designs in studies using the perennial herb Arabidopsis halleri (Komoto et al., 2022, https://doi.org/10.1111/pce.14716) and clonal Someiyoshino cherry trees (Miyawaki-Kuwakado et al., 2024, https://doi.org/10.1002/ppp3.10548) to examine environmental effects on gene expression. However, extending these approaches to long-lived tree species in diverse natural ecosystems poses significant logistical and biological challenges. In this study, we addressed this limitation by including three co-occurring species at the same site, which allowed us to evaluate interspecific differences under comparable environmental conditions. Importantly, even when we limited our analyses to these co-occurring species, the results remained consistent, indicating that the observed variation in transcriptomic profiles cannot be attributed to environmental factors alone and likely reflects underlying genetic influences.

      Accordingly, we added four new figures (Fig. S6, Fig. S7, Fig. S12 and Fig. S14) and revised the manuscript to clarify the limitations and strengths of our design, to tone down the evolutionary claims where appropriate, and to more explicitly define the scope of our conclusions in light of the data. We hope that these efforts sufficiently address the reviewer’s concerns and strengthen the manuscript.

      To better support the seasonal expression analysis, the early RNA-seq analysis sections should be strengthened. There is little discussion of biological replicate variation or variation among branches of the same individual. These could be important factors to analyze. In line 137, the mapping rate for two species is mentioned, but the rates for each species should be clearly reported. One RNA-seq dataset is based on a species different from the reference genome, so a lower mapping rate is expected. While this likely does not hinder downstream analysis, quantification is important.

      We thank the reviewer 1 for the helpful comment. To evaluate the variation among biological replicates, we compared the expression level of each gene across different individuals. We observed high correlation between each pair of individuals (Q. glauca (n=3): an average correlation coefficient r = 0.947; Q. acuta (n=3): r = 0.948; L. glaber (n=3): r = 0.948)). This result suggests that the seasonal gene expression pattern is highly synchronized across individuals within the same species. We mentioned this point in the Result section in the revised manuscript. We also calculated the mean mapping rates for each species. As the reviewer expected, the mapping rate was slightly lower in Q. acuta (88.6 ± 2.3%) and L. glaber (84.3 ± 5.4%), whose RNA-Seq data were mapped to reference genomes of related but different species, compared to that in Q. glauca (92.6 ± 2.2%) and L. edulis (89.3 ± 2.7%). However, we minimized the impact of these differences on downstream analysis. These details have been included in the revised main text.

      In Figures 2A and 2B, clustering is used to support several points discussed in the Results section (e.g., lines 175-177). However, clustering is primarily a visualization method or a hypothesis-generating tool; it cannot serve as a statistical test. Stronger conclusions would require further statistical testing.

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful comment. As noted, we acknowledge that hierarchical clustering (Fig. 2A) is primarily a visualization and hypothesis-generating method. To assess the biological relevance of the clusters identified, we conducted a Mann-Whitney U test or the Steel-Dwass test to evaluate whether the environmental temperatures at the time of sample collection differed significantly among the clusters. This analysis (Fig. 2B) revealed statistically significant differences in temperature in the cluster B3 (p < 0.01), indicating that the gene expression clusters are associated with seasonal thermal variation. These results support the interpretation that the clusters reflect coordinated transcriptional responses to environmental temperature. We revised the Results section to clarify this point.

      The quality of the genome assemblies appears adequate, but related assemblies should be cited and discussed. Several assemblies of Fagaceae species already exist, including Quercus mongolica (Ai et al., Mol Ecol Res, 2022), Q. gilva (Front Plant Sci, 2022), and Fagus sylvatica (GigaScience, 2018), among others. Is there any novelty here? Can you compare your results with these existing assemblies?

      We agree that genome assemblies of Fagaceae species are becoming increasing available. However, our study does not aim to emphasize the novelty of the genome assemblies per se. Rather, with the increasing availability of chromosome-level genomes, we regard genome assembly as a necessary foundation for more advanced analyses. The main objective of our study is to investigate how each gene is expressed in response to seasonal environmental changes, and to link genome information with seasonal transcriptomic dynamics. To address the reviewer’s comment in line with this objective, we added a discussion on the syntenic structure of eight genome assemblies spanning four genera within the Fagaceae, including a species from the genus Fagus (Ikezaki et al. 2025, https://doi.org/10.1101/2025.07.31.667835). This addition helps to position our work more clearly within the context of existing genomic resources.

      Most importantly, Figure 1B-D shows synteny between the two genera but also indicates homology between different chromosomes. Does this suggest paleopolyploidy or another novel feature? These chromosome connections should be interpreted in the main text-even if they could be methodological artifacts.

      A previous study on genome size variation in Fagaceae suggested that, given the consistent ploidy level across the family, genome expansion likely occurred through relatively small segmental duplications rather than whole-genome duplications. Because Figure 1B-D supports this view, we cited the following reference in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Chen et al. (2014)  https://doi.org/10.1007/s11295-014-0736-y

      In both the Results and Materials and Methods sections, descriptions of genome and RNA-seq data are unclear. In line 128, a paragraph on genome assembly suddenly introduces expression levels. RNA-seq data should be described before this. Similarly, in line 238, the sentence "we assembled high-quality reference genomes" seems disconnected from the surrounding discussion of expression studies. In line 632, Illumina short-read DNA sequencing is mentioned, but it's unclear how these data were used.

      We relocated the explanation regarding the expression levels of single-copy and multi-copy genes to the section titled “Seasonal gene expression dynamics.” Additionally, we clarified in the Materials and Methods section that short-read sequencing data were used for both genome size estimation and phylogenetic reconstruction.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study explores how gene expression evolves in response to seasonal environments, using four evergreen Fagaceae species growing in similar habitats in Japan. By combining chromosome-scale genome assemblies with a two-year RNA-seq time series in leaves and buds, the authors identify seasonal rhythms in gene expression and examine both conserved and divergent patterns. A central result is that winter bud expression is highly conserved across species, likely due to shared physiological demands under cold conditions. One of the intriguing implications of this study is that seasonal cycles might play a role similar to ontogenetic stages in animals. The authors touch on this by comparing their findings to the developmental hourglass model, and indeed, the recurrence of phenological states such as winter dormancy may act as a cyclic form of developmental canalization, shaping expression evolution in a way analogous to embryogenesis in animals.

      Strengths:

      (1) The evolutionary effects of seasonal environments on gene expression are rarely studied at this scale. This paper fills that gap.

      (2) The dataset is extensive, covering two years, two tissues, and four tree species, and is well suited to the questions being asked.

      (3) Transcriptome clustering across species (Figure 2) shows strong grouping by season and tissue rather than species, suggesting that the authors effectively controlled for technical confounders such as batch effects and mapping bias.

      (4) The idea that winter imposes a shared constraint on gene expression, especially in buds, is well argued and supported by the data.

      (5) The discussion links the findings to known concepts like phenological synchrony and the developmental hourglass model, which helps frame the results.

      We are grateful for the reviewer for the detailed and thoughtful review of our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) While the hierarchical clustering shown in Figure 2A largely supports separation by tissue type and season, one issue worth noting is that some leaf samples appear to cluster closely with bud samples. The authors do not comment on this pattern, which raises questions about possible biological overlap between tissues during certain seasonal transitions or technical artifacts such as sample contamination. Clarifying this point would improve confidence in the interpretation of tissue-specific seasonal expression patterns.

      Leaf samples clustered into the bud are newly flushed leaves collected in April for Q. glauca, May for Q. acuta, May and June for L. edulis, and August and September for L. glaber. To clarify this point, we highlighted these newly flushed leaf samples as asterisk in the revised figure (Fig. 2A).

      comment; (2) While the study provides compelling evidence of conserved and divergent seasonal gene expression, it does not directly examine the role of cis-regulatory elements or chromatin-level regulatory architecture. Including regulatory genomic or epigenomic data would considerably strengthen the mechanistic understanding of expression divergence.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. As noted in the Discussion section, we hypothesize that such genome-wide seasonal expression patterns—and their divergence across species—are likely mediated by cis-regulatory elements and chromatin-level mechanisms. While a direct investigation of regulatory architecture was beyond the scope of the present study, we fully agree that incorporating regulatory genomic and epigenomic data would significantly deepen the mechanistic understanding of expression divergence. In this regard, we are currently working to identify putative cis-regulatory elements in non-coding regions and are collecting epigenetic data from the same tree species using ChIP-seq. We believe the current study provide a foundation for these future investigations into the regulatory basis of seasonal transcriptome variation. We made a minor revision to the Discussion to note that an important future direction is to investigate the evolution of non-coding sequences that regulate gene expression in response to seasonal environmental changes.

      (3) The manuscript includes a thoughtful analysis of flowering-related genes and seasonal GO enrichment (e.g., Figure 3C-D), providing an initial link between gene expression timing and phenological functions. However, the analysis remains largely gene-centric, and the study does not incorporate direct measurements of phenological traits (e.g., flowering or bud break dates). As a result, the connection between molecular divergence and phenotypic variation, while suggestive, remains indirect.

      We would like to note that phenological traits have been observed in the field on a monthly basis throughout the sampling period and the phenological data were plotted together with molecular phenology (e.g. Fig. 2A, C; Fig. 3C, D). Although the temporal resolution is limited, these observations captured species-specific differences in key phenological events such as leaf flushing and flowering times. We revised the manuscript to clarify this point.

      (4) Although species were sampled from similar habitats, one species (Q. acuta) was collected at a higher elevation, and factors such as microclimate or local photoperiod conditions could influence expression patterns. These potential confounding variables are not fully accounted for, and their effects should be more thoroughly discussed or controlled in future analyses.

      We fully agree with the reviewer that local environmental conditions, including microclimate and photoperiod differences, could potentially influence gene expression patterns. To assess whether the higher elevation site of Q. acuta introduced confounding environmental effects, we reanalyzed the data after excluding this species. Hierarchical clustering still revealed that winter bud samples formed a distinct cluster regardless of species identity (Fig. S7), consistent with our original finding.

      Furthermore, we recalculated the molecular phenology divergence index D (Fig. 4C) and the interspecific Pearson’s correlation coefficients (Fig. 5A) without including Q. acuta. These analyses produced results that were qualitatively similar to those obtained from the full dataset (Fig. S12; Fig. S14), indicating that the observed patterns are not driven by environmental differences associated with elevation.

      We believe these additional analyses help to decouple the effects of environment and genetics, and support our conclusion that both seasonal synchrony and phylogenetic constraints play key roles in shaping transcriptome dynamics. We added four new figures (Fig. S6, Fig. S7, Fig. S12 and Fig. S14) and revised the text accordingly to clarify this point and to acknowledge the potential impact of site-specific environmental variation.

      (5) Statistical and Interpretive Concerns Regarding Δφ and dN/dS Correlation (Figures 5E and 5F):

      (a) Statistical Inappropriateness: Δφ is a discrete ordinal variable (likely 1-11), making it unsuitable for Pearson correlation, which assumes continuous, normally distributed variables. This undermines the statistical validity of the analysis.

      We thank the reviewer for the insightful comment. We would like to clarify that the analysis presented in Figures 5E and 5F was based on linear regression, not Pearson’s correlation. Although Δφ is a discrete variable, it takes values from 0 to 6 in 0.5 increments, resulting in 13 levels. We treated it as a quasi-continuous variable for the purposes of linear regression analysis. This approach is commonly adopted in practice when a discrete variable has sufficient resolution and ordering to approximate continuity. To enhance clarity, we revised the manuscript to explicitly state that linear regression was used, and we now reported the regression coefficient and associated p-value to support the interpretation of the observed trend.

      (b) Biological Interpretability: Even with the substantial statistical power afforded by genome-wide analysis, the observed correlations are extremely weak. This suggests that the relationship, if any, between temporal divergence in expression and protein-coding evolution is negligible.

      Taken together, these issues weaken the case for any biologically meaningful association between Δφ and dN/dS. I recommend either omitting these panels or clearly reframing them as exploratory and statistically limited observations.

      We agree with the reviewer’s comment. While we retained the original panels, we reframed our interpretation to emphasize that, despite statistical significance, the observed correlation is very weak—suggesting that coding region variation is unlikely to be the primary driver of seasonal gene expression patterns. Accordingly, we revised the “Relating seasonal gene expression divergence to sequence divergence” section in the Results, as well as the relevant part of the Discussion.

    1. Author response:

      We thank the editor and reviewers for their positive and detailed review of the preprint. We will use these comments to improve the manuscript's revised version, which we plan to submit in the coming weeks, including: a) tests of variants of ResNet, other network architectures and the use of pre-trained weights, b) clarification and justification of the accuracy metrics used in the benchmark, c) an expanded study about the fragment connectivity in Figure 3, and d) a study the performance of idmatcher.ai with the new idtracker.ai.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The authors' goal was to arrest PsV capsids on the extracellular matrix using cytochalasin D. The cohort was then released, and interaction with the cell surface, specifically with CD151, was assessed.

      The model that fragmented HS associated with released virions mediates the dominant mechanism of infectious entry has only been suggested by research from a single laboratory and has not been verified in the 10+ years since publication. The authors are basing this study on the assumption that this model is correct, and these data are referred to repeatedly as the accepted model despite much evidence to the contrary.

      Please note that we state in the introduction on line 65/66 ´Two release mechanisms are discussed, that mutually are not exclusive´. This is implying that we do not consider the shedding model as the one accepted model. HS may associate with PsVs despite of a decreased affinity and only after priming (see below the ‘priming model’) may translocate to the cell body.

      Furthermore, we do not state in the discussion either that the shedding model is the preferred one; although it is correct that we refer to the shedding model more extensively, simply because we find HS associated with transferred PsVs, which is in line with this model and requires its citation.

      The discussion in lines 65-71 concerning virion and HSPG affinity changes is greatly simplified. The structural changes in the capsid induced by HS interaction and the role of this priming for KLK8 and furin cleavage have been well researched. Multiple laboratories have independently documented this. If this study aims to verify the shedding model, additional data need to be provided.

      As outlined above, our finding is compatible with both models, and we do not aim to verify the shedding model or disprove the priming model.

      It appears that the referee wishes more visibility of the priming model. Inhibition of KLK8 and furin should reduce the translocation to the cell body, no matter whether PsVs carry HS on their surface or not. For revision, we plan an experiment as in Figure 3 (CytD), testing whether either KLK8 or furin inhibition blocks the transfer to the cell body. Then, our data can be discussed also in the context of the priming model and by this increase its visibility.

      The model should be fitted into established entry events, or at minimum, these conflicting data, a subset of which is noted below, need to be acknowledged.

      (1) The Sapp lab (Richards et al., 2013) found that HSPG-mediated conformational changes in L1 and L2 allowed the release of the virus from primary binding and allowing secondary receptor engagements in the absence of HS shedding.

      (2) Becker et al. found that furin-precleaved capsids could infect cells independently of HSPG interaction, but this infection was still inhibited with cytochalasin D.

      (3) Other work from the Schelhaas lab showed that cytochalasin D inhibition of infection resulted in the accumulation of capsids in deep invaginations from the cell surface, not on the ECM

      (4) Selinka et al., 2007, showed that preventing HSPG-induced conformational changes in the capsid surface resulted in noninfectious uptake that was not prevented with cytochalasin D.

      (5) The well-described capsid processing events by KLK8 and furin need to be mechanistically linked to the proposed model. Does inhibition of either of these cleavages prevent engagement with CD151?

      The authors need to consider an explanation for these discrepancies.

      That PsVs carry HS-cleavage products doesn´t imply that HS cleavage is sufficient or required for infection. Therefore, we do not view our data as being in conflict with the priming model. In fact, our observations are compatible with aspects of both the shedding and the priming model.

      Yet, we acknowledge that the study would gain importance by directly testing the priming model within our experimental system. As requested by the referee, we will discuss the above papers, and further plan to test KLK8 and furin inhibitors.

      Other issues:

      (1) Line 110-111. The statement about PsVs in the ECM being too far away from the cell surface to make physical contact with the cell surface entry receptors is confusing. ECM binding has not been shown to be an obligatory step for in vitro infection.

      Not obligatory, but strongly supportive (Bienkowska-Haba et al., Plos Path., 2018; Surviladze et al., J. Gen. Viro., 2015). As recently published by the Sapp lab (Bienkowska-Haba et al., Plos Path., 2018), ´Direct binding of HPV16 to primary keratinocytes yields very inefficient infection rates for unknown reasons.´ Moreover, the paper shows that HaCaT cell ECM binding of PsVs increases the infection of NHEK by 10-fold and of HFK by almost 50-fold.

      This idea is referred to again on lines 158-159 and 199. The claim (line 158) that PsV does not interact with the cell within an hour needs to be demonstrated experimentally and seems at odds with multiple laboratories' data. PsV has been shown to directly interact with HSPG on the cell surface in addition to the ECM. Why are these PsVs not detected?

      We do not question that in many cellular systems PsVs interact with heparan sulfate proteoglycans (HSPGs) present on the cell surface, or both on the cell surface and the ECM. We stated in the manuscript on line 59 ´While in cell culture virions bind to HS of the cell surface and the ECM, it has been suggested that in vivo they bind predominantly to HS of the extracellular basement membrane (Day and Schelhaas, 2014; Kines et al., 2009; Schiller et al., 2010).´

      Moreover, we ourselves detect these PsVs, for example, in Figure 5A (CytD, 0 min time point), a handful of PsVs localize to the cell body area. However, the large majority overlaps with the strong HS staining at the cell periphery, likely the ECM. An accurate quantification of the fractions of PsVs bound to the ECM/cell body is for the following reasons very difficult. First, the ECM PsVs are very dense and therefore not microscopically resolved into single PsVs, at least not completely (see Figure 1C; the high intensity spots are non-resolved PsVs, please see our discussion on line 148 - 152). For this reason, by just counting spots we strongly underestimate the ECM PsVs versus the cell body PsVs. Second, with the available immunostainings we cannot exactly delineate the ECM from the cell body. In particular, at the cell border region (for example see Figure 4B) we often observe PsV accumulations. Assigning these ´cell border region PsVs´ entirely to the cell body fraction, a preliminary analysis (correcting for the limitation of non-resolved ECM PsVs) suggests that about a quarter of the PsVs bind to the cell body. On the other hand, assigning them to the ECM, the cell body fraction would be much below 10%. Third, we observe that in regions devoid of ECM and cells PsVs apparently adhere unspecifically to the glass-coverslip. This suggests that some of the cell body PsVs are just unspecific background. Subtraction of a background PsV density from the ECM and cell body PsV density will reduce relatively more the cell body PsVs, and consequently decreases the fraction of cell body PsVs even more.

      Moreover, in the course of the project we wondered whether at the basolateral membrane there are not many binding sites anyway. To address this question, in an unpublished experiment, we detached HaCaT cells with trypsin, incubated them with PsVs, and then allowed reattachment to assess the binding in suspension. We detected minimal to no binding, which, however, could also result from apical membrane adherence to the coverslip or trypsin-mediated cleavage of HSPGs. As suggested by the reviewing editor, we agree that repeating this experiment using EDTA for detachment—thus preserving HSPGs—would offer more definitive insight into binding efficiency in the absence of accessibility constraints. In summary, the reason why in our cellular system most PsVs do not bind to the cell surface could be a combination of several factors:

      (1) The primary binding partners are more abundant in the ECM and the polarized HaCaT cells secrete more ECM when compared to other cultured cells used to study HPV infection. This promotes ECM binding.

      (2) In the polarized HaCaT cells, the apical membrane is largely devoid of syndecan-1, CD151 and Itga6, wherefore PsVs infect the cell via the basolateral membrane. However, the accessibility to the basolateral membrane is restricted, PsVs must diffuse through a narrow slit between the glass coverslip and the attached cell to reach HS on the cell surface. This limits cell surface binding.

      (3) If HaCaT cells secrete large amounts of ECM, the may become depleted from cell surface HS. As outlined above, we will try to find out how many PsVs bind to the basolateral membrane in the absence of restricted accessibility. If it turns out that HaCaT cells have not many binding sites anyway, this would additionally promote binding to the ECM.

      The outcome of the above issues, and how we will mention them in the revised version of the manuscript, is open. In any case, we would like to point out that PsVs bound to the cell body do not weaken our main conclusion. Still, we recognize that this point merits attention and plan several modifications of the manuscript. We did already, but now we will mention more explicitly that PsVs have been shown to directly interact with HSPG on the cell surface, in addition to the ECM, but that it also has been shown that the ECM strongly supports infection in NHEK and HFK (Bienkowska-Haba et al., Plos Path., 2018). The following is a draft version of a paragraph we plan to incorporate, explaining the above issue and why we used in our experiments HaCaT cells:

      ´In vitro, PsVs bind to both the cell surface and the ECM, as has been widely documented. In vivo, however, it has been proposed that initial binding occurs predominantly to the basement membrane ECM, rather than directly to the cell surface (Day and Schelhaas, 2014; Kines et al., 2009; Schiller et al., 2010). This distinction reinforces the physiological relevance of ECM-bound particles in the early steps of HPV infection. Support for a functional role of ECM-mediated entry comes from a study showing that PsV binding to ECM derived from HaCaT cells significantly enhances infection of primary keratinocytes (Bienkowska-Haba et al., 2018). For these reasons, we specifically chose polarized HaCaT cells as a model system. These cells secrete abundant ECM from which the cells readily collect bound PsVs. On the other hand, the polarization limits the access of PsVs to basolateral receptors such as CD151 and Itgα6, and also cell body resident Syndecan-1, the most abundant HSPG in keratinocytes (Rapraeger et al., 1986; Hayashi et al., 1987; Kim et al., 1994). Hence, as polarization limits direct cell surface accessibility it biases binding toward the ECM, that in this culture system is abundant. Hence, in the HaCaT cell culture system, like probably in vivo, PsVs cannot circumvent binding to the ECM what they can do in unpolarized cell cultures that may not even secrete significant amounts of ECM. Altogether, this experimental situation closely mimics the in vivo situation where PsVs bind preferentially to the ECM (Day and Schelhaas, 2014; Kines et al., 2009; Schiller et al., 2010).´

      We appreciate the reviewer’s input and believe these additions will strengthen the manuscript with regard to the relevance of the used cellular model system.

      (2) The experiments shown in Figure 5 need to be better controlled. Why is there no HS staining of the cell surface at the early timepoints? This antibody has been shown to recognize N-sulfated glucosamine residues on HS and, therefore, detects HSPG on the ECM and cell surface.

      We have shown all images at the same adjustments of brightness and contrast. As the staining at the periphery is stronger, the impression is given that the cell surface is not stained, although there is some staining. Specific staining is documented in Figure 5D, showing the PCC between PsVs and HS only of the cell body. If there was no HS staining, the PCC would be zero, which is not the case. Yet, it is lower when compared to the PCC measured at the cell border region, with more strongly stained HS.

      We will provide images at different contrast and brightness adjustments enabling the reader to see the staining on the cell surface. We will provide also more overview images to illustrate the strong variability of the HS staining between cells.

      Therefore, the conclusion that this confirms HS coating of PsV during release from the ECM (line 430-431) is unfounded. How do the authors distinguish between "HS-coated virions" and HSPG-associated virions?

      The HS intensity transiently increases on the cell body (Fig. 5D) only after releasing a cohort of PsVs, which can be only explained by PsVs that carry HS from the ECM to the cell body. However, the effect is not significant. Using the antibody 3G10 detecting the HS neoepitope (see the referees’ suggestion below) we will reanalyze this point. This should help clarifying the issue.

      It is difficult to comprehend how the addition of 50 vge/cell of PsV could cause such a global change in HS levels.

      The distribution of bound PsVs largely varies between cells. Some areas are covered with essentially confluent cells, to which hardly any PsVs are bound, because accessing the basolateral membrane of confluent cells is nearly impossible, and PsVs do not bind to the exposed apical membrane. This is different in cultures of unpolarized cells where we expect that PsVs distribute more equally over cells.

      This means that in our experiments the vge/cell is not a suitable parameter for relating the magnitude of an effect to a defined number of PsVs. In the ECM, the PsV density is very high, enabling one cell to collect several hundred PsVs, much more than expected from the 50 vge/cell. We will point this out in the revised version.

      The claim that the HS levels are decreased in the non-cytochalasin-treated cells due to PsV-induced shedding needs to be demonstrated.

      We did not claim that PsVs induce shedding, we rather believe they just take shedded HS with them. Without PsVs, the shedded HS likely remains in the ECM or is washed out very slowly.

      If HS is actually shed, staining of the cell periphery could increase with the antibody 3G10, which detects the HS neoepitope created following heparinase cleavage.

      As outlined above, we plan to test the suggested antibody 3G10. We also plan to repeat the 0 min time point (with and without PsVs, with and without CytD) to find out whether in the PsV absence the HS intensity (at 0 min) is unchanged between control and CytD.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Massenberg and colleagues aimed to understand how Human papillomavirus particles that bind to the extracellular matrix (ECM) transfer to the cell body for later uptake, entry, and infection. The binding to ECM is key for getting close to the virus's host cell (basal keratinocytes) after a wounding scenario for later infection in a mouse vaginal challenge model, indicating that this is an important question in the field.

      Strengths:

      The authors take on a conceptually interesting and potentially very important question to understand how initial infection occurs in vivo. The authors confirm previous work that actin-based processes contribute to virus transport to the cell body. The superresolution microscopy methods and data collection are state-of-the art and provide an interesting new way of analysing the interaction with host cell proteins on the cell surface in certain infection scenarios. The proposed hypothesis is interesting and, if substantiated, could significantly advance the field.

      Weaknesses:

      As a study design, the authors use infection of HaCaT keratinocytes, and follow virus localisation with and without inhibition of actin polymerisation by cytochalasin D (cytoD) to analyse transfer of virions from the ECM to the cell by filopodial structures using important cellular proteins for cell entry as markers.

      First, the data is mostly descriptive besides the use of cytoD, and does not test the main claim of their model, in which virions that are still bound to heparan sulfate proteoglycans are transferred by binding to tetraspanins along filopodia to the cell body.

      The study identifies a rapid translocation step from the ECM to the cell body. We have no data that demonstrates a physical interaction between PsVs and CD151. In the model figure, we draw CD151 as part of the secondary receptor complex. We are sorry for having raised the impression that PsVs would bind directly to CD151 and will rephrase the respective section.

      Second, using cytoD is a rather broad treatment that not only affects actin retrograde flow, but also virus endocytosis and further vesicular transport in cells, including exocytosis. Inhibition of myosin II, e.g., by blebbistatin, would have been a better choice as it, for instance, does not interfere with endocytosis of the virus.

      We agree, and plan to test whether blebbistatin is equally efficient in blocking the transfer.

      Third, the authors aim to study transfer from ECM to the cell body and the effects thereof. However, there are substantial, if not the majority of, viruses that bind to the cell body compared to ECM-bound viruses in close vicinity to the cells.

      We agree that in multiple cell culture systems viruses bind preferentially to the cell directly. But we respectfully disagree with the assertion that the majority of PsVs bind to the cell body of HaCaT keratinocytes. As noted above (e.g., Figure 5A, CytD, 0 min), only a small fraction of PsVs localize to the cell body, whereas the vast majority overlap with intense HS staining at the cell periphery, consistent with ECM association, as the accessibility to the basolateral expressed HSPG is limited (see above). Based on quantitative estimation from multiple images, ECM-bound PsVs largely outnumber cell-bound particles (see above). These features make HaCaT cells a suitable in vitro model for mimicking in vivo conditions, where HPV has been proposed to bind predominantly to the basement membrane ECM rather than the cell surface (Day and Schelhaas, 2014; Kines et al., 2009; Schiller et al., 2010) which also strongly enhances infection of primary keratinocytes in vitro (Bienkowska-Haba et al., 2018).

      Thus, we believe our system appropriately models the physiologically relevant scenario of ECM-to-cell transfer, and the observed predominance of ECM binding supports the validity of our experimental focus.

      This is in part obscured by the small subcellular regions of interest that are imaged by STED microscopy, or by the use of plasma membrane sheets. As a consequence, the obtained data from time point experiments is skewed, and remains for the most part unconvincing due to the fact that the origin of virions in time and space cannot be taken into account. This is particularly important when interpreting association with HS, the tetraspanin CD151, and integral alpha 6, as the low degree of association could originate from cell-bound and ECM-transferred virions alike.

      As stated above, we observe massive binding of PsVs to the ECM, in contrast to very few PsVs that diffuse beneath the basolateral membrane of the polarized HaCaT cells and do bind directly to the cell surface (or maybe they are simply trapped between glass and basolateral membrane). PsVs are not expected to bind to the apical membrane that is depleted from CD151 and Itga6. In other cellular systems, cells may hardly secrete ECM, are not polarized, and do not adhere so tightly to the substrate. In other cultures, where virions can easily circumvent ECM binding, the large majority of PsVs will likely bind directly to the cell surface.

      As outlined above, in order to quantify PsVs that can bind without restricted accessibility, we plan to detach HaCaT cells by EDTA from the substrate, incubate them with PsVs, and let them adhere again (please see above).

      No matter what is the outcome, the fraction of PsVs that binds directly to the cell surface does not weaken our conclusion that we have identified a very fast and efficient transfer step from the ECM to the cell body.

      Fourth, the use of fixed images in a time course series also does not allow for understanding the issue of a potential contribution of cell membrane retraction upon cytoD treatment due to destabilisation of cortical actin. Or, of cell spreading upon cytoD washout.

      If blebbistatin works as expected, we can safely conclude that we observe the very same process as described in Scheelhas et al., PLoS Pathogens, 2008, showing that the PsVs migrate by retrograde transport to the cell surface and not that the cell spreads out and by this reaches the PsVs.

      The microscopic analysis uses an extension of a plasma membrane stain as a marker for ECM-bound virions, which may introduce a bias and skew the analysis.

      Our plasma membrane stain does not stain the ECM. Please see Figure 1. The stain is actually used to distinguish the cell body from the ECM area.

      Fifth, while the use of randomisation during image analysis is highly recommended to establish significance (flipping), it should be done using only ROIs that have a similar density of objects for which correlations are being established.

      We agree that the way of how randomization is done is very important. Regarding the association of PsVs with CD151 and HS, based on flipped images, we generated a calibration curve used for the correction of random background. For details, please see Supplementary Figures 3 and 5.

      For instance, if one flips an image with half of the image showing the cell body, and half of the image ECM, it is clear that association with cell membrane structures will only be significant in the original. I am rather convinced that using randomisation only on the plasma membrane ROIs will not establish any clear significance of the correlating signals.

      Figure 5D shows the PCC specifically of the cell body. In flipped images (not shown in the manuscript for clarity, but can be added) we obtain a PCC of around zero.  For CytD, the flipped images always have a significantly lower PCC compared to the original images. In the control, the PCC of the flipped images are significantly lower only for the 30 min and 60 min time point. The non-significance of the 0 min and 180 min time point is due to low PCCs also in the original images.

      Also, there should be a higher n for the measurements.

      One n is the average of 15 cells. We realize that with n = 3 we find significant effects only if the effect is very strong or moderate with very low variance.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      In this study, Deng et al. investigate the antibody response against HA antigen following repeated vaccination with the H1N1 2009 pandemic influenza vaccine strain, using in silico modeling. The proposed model provides valuable mechanistic insights into how the broadening of the antibody response takes place upon repeated vaccination.

      Overall, the authors' model effectively explains the mechanistic principles underlying antibody responses against the viral antigens harboring epitope immunodominancy.

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive and thoughtful assessment of the work. We address issues raised in the revised manuscript and in the point-by-point responses below.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The authors have been studying the mechanism of breadth expansion in antibody responses with repeated vaccinations using their own mathematical model. In this study, they applied this mathematical model to a cohort data analyzing anti-HA antibody responses after multiple influenza virus vaccination and investigated the mechanism of antibody breadth expansion to diversified target viral strains.

      The manuscript is well written, and the mathematical model is well built that incorporates various parameters related to B cell activation in GC and EGC based on experimental data.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive and thoughtful review and address issues raised in a revised version of the manuscript and in the point-by-point below.

      Strengths:

      By carefully reanalyzing the published cohort data (Nunez IA et al 2017 PLoS One), they have clearly demonstrated that the repeated influenza virus vaccinations result in an expansion of the breadth to unmatched viral strains.

      Using their mathematical model, they have determined the major factors for the breadth expansion following multiple immunizations.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the strengths of our study.

      Weaknesses

      The overall concept of their model has already been published (Yang L et al 2023 Cell Reports) with a SARS-CoV-2 vaccine model, and they have applied it to influenza virus vaccine in this study, with the conclusions being largely the same.

      It is unclear how the re-evaluation of public data in the first half part is related to the validation of their model in the later part.

      The reviewer is correct in that we build directly on our model published previously to study related phenomena for SARS-CoV-2. However, a critical advance of the work was to now ask whether antibody broadening following repeated homologous antigen exposure is a general feature of human humoral immunity. As we point out in the introduction of our manuscript, repeated exposure to the same antigen has long been assumed to predominantly boost strain limited humoral immunity, necessitating rational design of vaccines that re-orient antibody responses to target otherwise immune-subdominant targets. Hence, antibody broadening in response to homologous SARS-CoV-2 antigen points to reconsideration of that basic premise in immunology; and if we are to now define this as general feature of human antibody responses, then evaluation of the principle using a different vaccine protocol and antigen is necessitated. Accordingly, we took advantage of the influenza vaccine space where, within the immediate years following the 2009 H1N1 pandemic, the 2009 H1N1 strain was repeatedly applied as the seasonal vaccine strain. This HA was also novel (as it was from a pandemic virus pHA), meaning that traditional back-boosting to historical strains would be limited. We then re-evaluated the longitudinal HAI data of Nurez et al. to define whether a broadening to increasingly divergent vaccine-unmatched strains is observed upon repeated exposure to pHA. This was not done before and was enabled by incorporating our amino acid relatedness parameter and our structure-based definition of the RBS patch. To then query mechanistic origins of the broadening effect, we adapted and extended our previous computational model to: (1) better reflect HA epitope diversity and overlap within the RBS patch; and (2) to better reflect the influenza immunization regimens that are used clinically. The differences between the modeling done in this paper and that in Yang et al. 2023 are described in the Methods section separately. Taken together, our analyses of data in Nunez et al and our simulations strengthen the emerging view that repeated boosting with the same antigen enables the humoral immune system to diversify immune responses because of feedback regulation which leads to enhanced antigen on FDCs, persistent GCs, and epitope masking. This, in turn, enables the immune system to generalize to recognize and respond to unseen variant antigens that harbor mutations in the immunodominant epitopes. Our results point to a new and emerging paradigm regarding booster immunizations and fundamental features of the humoral immune system.

      Other points:

      In the original data by Nurez LA et al., HAI (the inhibitory effect of anti-HA antibodies on the binding of HA to sialic acid on erythrocytes) was used as the lead-out. The authors conclude that the breadth expansion with repeated vaccinations is primarily due to the activation of B cells with BCRs that recognize minor common epitopes, induced by covering up of strain specific major epitopes by pre-existing antibodies. However, as they themselves show in Fig 1, once the sialic acid-binding region is covered, it seems difficult for another BCR to bind to this region. When the target epitope is limited like this, the effect of increasing antigen supply to DCs by pre-existing antibodies and the effect of increasing the presentation of minor epitopes appears to compete with each other. Could the author please explain this point?

      We agree that accounting for epitope overlap is important when the target is limited, as the reviewer indicates. In Figure 6C vs 6D we assess steric effects of possible spatial overlap between dominant and subdominant epitopes. Under overlapping conditions, we find evidence for steric-based constrainment of broadening, as predicted by the reviewer. Depending upon the degree of overlap between the epitopes and differences in germline characteristics in the B cells targeting dominant and subdominant epitopes, this effect could be compensated during subsequent shots, as described by our results (see lines 392-406).

      We also now incorporate the following sentence into our discussion (lines 448-453):

      “Epitope masking will also be constrained by the dimensions of the RBS and our simulations do report attenuation of titers against historical influenza strains when we introduce epitope overlap. Depending upon the degree of overlap between the epitopes and differences in germline characteristics in the B cells targeting dominant and subdominant epitopes, this effect could be compensated during subsequent shots.”

      In relation to this point, please explain the meaning of analysis of the entire ectodomain when the original data's lead-out is HAI.

      We include side-by-side full length ectodomain versus RBS patch (sialic acid binding residues + antibody epitope ring) to demonstrate relatedness differences in the lead-out data. But it is precisely because of the point raised by the reviewer that we focus on using the RBS patch as the relatedness values to assess antibody broadening as defined by HAI activity (see Figure 3 and S2). 

      Minor point:

      The description "The purpose of this model is ...." starting at line 171 and the description of "we obtain results in harmony with the clinical findings ...." starting at line 478 sound to be contradictory. As the authors themselves state at line 171, if the purpose of this model is not to fit the data but to demonstrate the principle, then the prudent sampling and reanalyzing data itself seems to have less meaning.

      We respectfully disagree. Please see above point as to how the clinical data is more than just “reanalyzing” but to first discover the previously unreported broadening effect across highly divergent strains following sequential immunization with homologous antigen in the influenza vaccine space; we then extended and adapted our computational model for the influenza vaccination paradigm to gain mechanistic insight on how such antibody broadening may occur. The word “harmony” was not meant to imply quantitative agreement, and apologize if it caused confusion.

    1. Author response:

      (1) General Statements

      Our manuscript studies mechanisms of planar polarity establishment in vivo in the Drosophila pupal wing. Specifically we seek to understand mechanisms of ‘cell-scale signalling’ that is responsible for segregating core pathway planar polarity proteins to opposite cell edges. This is an understudied question, in part because it is difficult to address experimentally.

      We use conditional and restrictive expression tools to spatiotemporally manipulate core protein activity, combined with quantitative measurement of core protein distribution, polarity and stability. Our results provide evidence for a robust cell-scale signal, while arguing against mechanisms that depend on depletion of a limited pool of a core protein or polarised transport of core proteins on microtubules. Furthermore, we show that polarity propagation across a tissue is hard, highlighting the strong intrinsic capacity of individual cells to establish and maintain planar polarity.

      The original manuscript received three fair and thorough peer-reviews, which raised many important points. In response, we decided to embark on a full revision that attempts to answer all of the points. We have included new data to support our conclusions in Supplemental Figures 1, 2 and 5.

      Additionally in response to the reviewers we have revised the manuscript title, which is now ‘Characterisation of cell-scale signalling by the core planar polarity pathway during Drosophila wing development’.

      (2) Point-by-point description of the revisions

      We thank all of the reviewers for their thorough and thoughtful review of our manuscript. They raise many helpful points which have been extremely useful in assisting us to revise the manuscript.

      In response we have carried out a major revision of the manuscript, making numerous changes and additions to the text and also adding new experimental data. Specific changes are listed after our detailed response to each comment.

      Reviewer #1:

      Summary

      The authors use inducible Fz::mKate2-sfGFP to explore "cell-scale signaling" in PCP. They reach several conclusions. First, they conclude that cell-scale signaling does not depend on limiting pools of core components (other than Fz). Second, they conclude that cell-scale signaling does not depend on microtubule orientation, and third, they conclude that cell-scale signaling is strong relative to cell to cell coupling of polarity. 

      There are some interesting inferences that can be drawn from the manuscript, but there are also some significant challenges in interpreting the results and conclusions from the work as presented. I suggest that the authors 1) define "cell-scale signaling," as the precise meaning must be inferred, 2) reconsider some premises upon which some conclusions depend, 3) perform an essential assay validation, and 4) explain some other puzzling inconsistencies.

      Major points

      The exact meaning of cell-scale signaling is not defined, but I infer that the authors use this term to describe how what happens on one side of a cell affects another side. The remainder of my critique depends on this understanding of the intended meaning.

      As the reviewer points out, it is important that the meaning of the term ‘cell-scale signalling’ is clear to the reader and in response to their comment we have had another go at defining it explicitly in the Introduction to the manuscript.

      Specifically, we use the term ‘cell-scale signalling’ to describe possible intracellular mechanisms acting on core protein segregation to opposite cell membranes during core pathway dependent planar polarisation. For example, this could be a signal from distal complexes at one side of the cell leading to segregation of proximal complexes to the opposite cell edge, or vice versa. See also our response to Reviewer #2 regarding the distinction between ‘molecular-scale’ and ‘cell-scale’ signalling. 

      Changes to manuscript: Revised definition of ‘cell-scale signalling’ in Introduction.

      The authors state that any tissue wide directional information comes from pre-existing polarity and its modification by cell flow, such that the de novo signaling paradigm "bypasses" these events and should therefore not be responsive to any further global cues. It is my understanding that this is not a universally accepted model, and indeed, the authors' data seem to suggest otherwise. For example, the image in Fig 5B shows that de novo induction restores polarity orientation to a predominantly proximal to distal orientation. If no global cue is active, how is this orientation explained?

      We assume that the reviewer’s point is that it is not universally accepted that de novo induction after hinge contraction leads to uncoupling from global cues (rather than that it is not accepted that hinge contraction remodels radial polarity to a proximodistal pattern). We are (we believe) the only lab that has used de novo induction as a tool, and we’re not aware of any debate in the literature about whether this bypasses global cues. Nevertheless, we accept that it is hard to prove there is no influence of global cues, when the nature of those cues and the time at which they act remain unclear. Below we summarise the reasons why we believe there are not significance effects of global cues in our experiments that would influence the interpretation of our results.

      First, our reading of the literature supports a broad consensus that an early radial core planar polarity pattern is realigned by cell flow produced by hinge contraction beginning at around 16h APF (e.g. Aigouy et al., 2010; Strutt and Strutt, 2015; Aw and Devenport, 2017; Butler and Wallingford, 2017; Tan and Strutt, 2025). Taken at face value, this suggests that there are ‘radial’ cues present prior to hinge contraction, maybe coming from the wing margin – arguably these radial cues could be Ft-Ds or Wnts or both, given they are expressed in patterns consistent with such a role (notwithstanding the published evidence arguing against roles for either of these cues). It then appears that hinge contraction supercedes these cues to convert a radial pattern to a proximodistal pattern – whether the radial cues that affect the core pathway earlier remain active after hinge contraction is unclear, although both Ft-Ds and Wnts appear to maintain their ‘radial’ patterns beyond the beginning of hinge contraction (e.g. Merkel et al., 2014; Ewen-Campen et al., 2020; Yu et al., 2020).

      We think that the reviewer is proposing the presence of a proximodistal cue that is active in the proximal region of the wing that we use for our experiments shown e.g. in Fig.5, and that this cue orients core polarity here (but not elsewhere in the wing) in a time window after 18h APF. Ft-Ds and Wnts do not seem to be plausible candidates as they are still in ‘radial’ patterns. This leaves either an unknown proximodistal cue (a gradient of some unknown signalling molecule?), or possibly some ability of hinge contraction to align proximodistal polarity specifically in this wing region but not elsewhere. We cannot definitively rule out either of these possibilities, but neither do we think there is sufficient evidence to justify invoking their existence to explain our observations.

      In particular, the reason that we don’t think there is a proximodistal cue in the proximal part of the wing after 18h APF, is that work from our lab shows that induction of Fz or Stbm expression at times around or after the start of hinge contraction (i.e. >16 h APF) results in increasing levels of trichome swirling with polarity not being coordinated with the tissue axis either proximally or distally (Strutt and Strutt, 2002; Strutt and Strutt 2007). Our simplest interpretation for this is that induction at these stages fails to establish the early radial pattern of core pathway polarity and hence hinge contraction cannot reorient radial to proximodistal. If hinge contraction alone could specify proximodistal polarity in the absence of the earlier radial polarity, then we would not expect to see swirling over much of the proximal wing (where the forces from hinge contraction are strongest (Etournay et al., 2015)).

      In this manuscript, our earliest de novo experiments begin with Fz induction at 18h APF (de novo 10h), then at 20h APF (de novo 8h) and at 22h APF (de novo 6h). The image in Fig. 5B, referred to by the reviewer, is of a wing where Fz is induced de novo at 22 h APF. In these wings, as expected, the core proteins localise asymmetrically in stereotypical swirling patterns throughout the wing surface (see Fig. 2M and also Strutt and Strutt, 2002; Strutt and Strutt 2007), but – usefully for our experiments – they broadly localise along the proximal-distal axis in the region analysed in Fig. 5B. Given the strong swirling in surrounding regions when inducing at >20h APF, we feel reasonably confident in assuming that the pattern is not due to a proximodistal cue present in the proximal wing.

      We appreciate that the original manuscript did not show images including the trichome pattern in adjacent regions, so this point would not have been clear, but we now include these in Supplementary Fig. 5. We have also added a note in the legend to Fig. 5B to clarify that the proximodistal pattern seen is local to this wing region. We apologise for this oversight and the confusion caused and appreciate the feedback.

      The 6 hr condition, that has only partial polarity magnitude, is quite disordered. Do the patterns at 8 and 10 hrs become more proximally-distally oriented? It is stated that they all show swirls, but please provide adult wing images, and the corresponding orientation outputs from QuantifyPolarity to help validate the notion that the global cues are indeed bypassed by this paradigm.

      In all three ‘normal’ de novo conditions (6h, 8h and 10h), regardless of the time of induction, the polarity orientation patterns of Fz-mKate2 in pupal and adult wings are very similar in the experimentally analysed region (Fig. S5B-E). The strong local hair swirling agrees with the previous published data (Strutt and Strutt, 2002; Strutt and Strutt 2007). Overall, we don’t see any evidence that the 10h de novo induction results in more proximodistally coordinated polarity than the 8h or 6h conditions. This is consistent with our contention that there is no global cue present at these stages, which presumably would have a stronger effect when core pathway activity was induced at earlier stages.

      Changes to manuscript: Added additional explanation of the ‘de novo induction’ paradigm and why we believe the resulting polarity patterns are unlikely to be influenced by any global signals in Introduction and Results section ‘Induced core protein relocalisation…’. Added quantification of polarity in the experiment region proximal to the anterior cross-vein in pupal wings (Fig.S5E-E’’’) and zoomed-out images of the surrounding region in adult wings showing that the polarity pattern does not become more proximodistal when induction time is longer, and also that there is not overall proximodistal polarity in proximal regions of the wing (Fig.S5B-D), arguing against an unknown proximodistal polarity cue at these stages of development.

      In the de novo paradigm, polarization is initiated immediately or shortly after heat shock induction. However, the results should be differently interpreted if the level of available Fz protein does not rise rapidly and then stabilize before the 6 hr time point, and instead continues to rise throughout the experiment. Western blots of the Fz::mKate2-sfGFP at time points after induction should be performed to demonstrate steady state prior to measurements. Otherwise, polarity magnitude could simply reflect the total available pool of Fz at different times after induction. Interpreting stability is complex, and could depend on the same issue, as well as the amount of recycling that may occur. Prior work from this lab using FRAP suggested that turnover occurs, and could result from recycling as well as replenishment from newly synthesized protein. 

      The reviewer raises an important point, which we agree could confound our experimental interpretations. As suggested we have now carried out western blotting and quantitation for Fz::mKate2-sfGFP levels and added these data to Fig.S1 (Fig. S1C,D). Quantified Fz is not significantly different between the three de novo polarity induction timings and not significantly different compared to constitutive Fz::mKate2-sfGFP expression (although there is a trend towards increasing Fz::mKate2-sfGFP protein levels with increasing induction times). These data are consistent with Fz::mKate2-sfGFP being at steady state in our experiments and that levels are sufficient to achieve normal polarity (as constitutive Fz::mKate2-sfGFP does so). Therefore it is unlikely that differing protein levels explain the differing polarity magnitudes at the different induction times. Interestingly, Fz::mKate2-sfGFP levels are lower than endogenous Fz levels, possibly due to lower expression or increased turnover/reduced recycling.

      Changes to manuscript: Added western blot analysis of Fz::mKate2-sfGFP expression under 10h, 8h and 6h induction conditions vs endogenous Fz expression and constitutive Fz::mKate2sfGFP expression (Fig.S1C-D) and discussed in Results section ‘Planar polarity establishment is…’.

      From the Fig 3 results, the authors claim that limiting pools of core proteins do not explain cellscale signaling, a result expected based on the lack of phenotypes in heterozygotes, but of course they do not test the possibility that Fz is limiting. They do note that some other contributing protein could be. 

      Previously published results from our lab (Strutt et al., 2016 Cell Reports; Supplemental Fig. S6E) show that in a heterozygous fz mutant background, Fz protein levels are not affected by halving the gene dosage when compared to wt, suggesting that Fz is most likely produced in excess and is not normally limiting, but that protein that cannot form complexes may be rapidly degraded. We have now added this information to the text.

      Changes to manuscript: Added explanation in text that Fz levels had previously been shown to not be dosage sensitive in Results section ‘Planar polarity establishment is…’ and also added a caveat to the Discussion about not directly testing Fz.

      In Fig 3, it is unclear why the authors chose to test dsh1/+ rather than dsh[null]/+. In any case, the statistically significant effect of Dsh dose reduction is puzzling, and might indicate that the other interpretation is correct. Ideally, a range including larger and smaller reductions would be tested. As is, I don't think limiting Dsh is ruled out. 

      Concerning the choice of dsh allele, we appreciate the query of the reviewer regarding use of dsh[1] instead of a null, as there might be a concern that dsh[1] would give a less strong phenotype. The answer is that over more than two decades we and others have never found any evidence that dsh[1] does not act as a ‘null’ for planar polarity in the pupal wing, and furthermore use of dsh[1] preserves function in Wg signalling – and we would prefer to rule out any phenotypic effects due to any potential cross-talk between the two pathways that might be seen using a complete null. To expand on this point, dsh[1] mutant protein is never seen at cell junctions (Axelrod 2001; Shimada et al., 2001; our own work), and by every criteria we have used, planar polarity is completely disrupted in hemizygous or homozygous mutants e.g. see quantifications of polarity in (Warrington et al., 2017 Curr Biol).

      In terms of the broader point, whether we can rule out Dsh being limiting, we were very careful to be clear that we did not see evidence for Dsh (or other core proteins) being limiting in terms of ‘rates of core pathway de novo polarisation’. When the reviewer says ‘the statistically significant effect of Dsh dose reduction is puzzling’ we believe they are referring to the data in Fig. 3J, showing a small but significantly different reduction in stable Fz in de novo 6h conditions (also seen in 8h de novo conditions, Fig. S3I). As Dsh is known to stabilise Fz in complexes (Strutt et al., 2011 Dev Cell; Warrington et al., 2017 Curr Biol), in itself this result is not wholly surprising. Nevertheless, while this shows that halving Dsh levels does modestly reduce Fz stability, it does not alter our conclusion that halving Dsh levels does not affect Fz polarisation rate under either 6h or 8h de novo conditions.

      Unfortunately, we do not have available to us a practical way of achieving consistent intermediate reductions in Dsh levels (e.g. a series of verified transgenes expressing at different levels). Levels of all the core proteins could be dialled down using transgenes, to see when the system breaks, and indeed we have previously published that lower levels of polarity are seen if Fmi levels are <<50% or if animals are transheterozygous for pk, stbm, dgo or dsh, pk, stbm, dgo simultaneously (Strutt et al., 2016 Cell Reports). However, it seems to be a trivial result that eventually the ability to polarise is lost if insufficient core proteins are present at the junctions. For this reason we have focused on a simple set of experiments reducing gene dosage singly by 50% under two de novo induction conditions, and have been careful to state our results cautiously. The assays we carried out were a great deal of work even for just the 5 heterozygous conditions tested.

      We believe that the experiments shown effectively make the point that there is no strong dosage sensitivity – and it remains our contention that if protein levels were the key to setting up cell-scale polarity, then a 50% reduction would be expected to show an effect on the rate of polarisation. We further note that as Fz::mKate2-sfGFP levels are lower than endogenous Fz levels (see above), the system might be expected to be sensitised to further dosage reductions, and despite this we failed to see an effect on rate of polarisation.

      We note that Reviewer #3 made a similar point about whether we can rule out dosage sensitivity on the basis of 50% reductions in protein level. To address the comments of both reviewers we had now added some further narrative and caveats in the text.

      In a similar vein, Reviewer #2 requested data on whether dosage reduction altered protein levels by the expected amount. We have now added further explanation/references and western blot data to address this.

      Changes to manuscript: Added more explanation of our choice of dsh[1] as an appropriate mutant allele to use in Results section ‘Planar polarity establishment is…’. Added some narrative and caveats regarding whether lowering levels more than 50% would add to our findings in the Discussion. Revised conclusions to be more cautious including altering section title to read ‘Planar polarity establishment is not highly sensitive to variation in protein levels of core complex components’.

      Also added westerns and text/references showing that for the tested proteins there is a reduction in protein levels upon removal of one gene dosage in Results section ‘Planar polarity establishment is…’ and Fig.S2.

      The data in Fig 5 are somewhat internally inconsistent, and inconsistent with the authors' interpretation. In both repolarization conditions, the authors claim that repolarization extends only to row 1, and row 1 is statistically different from non-repolarized row 1, but so too is row 3. Row 2 is not. This makes no sense, and suggests either that the statistical tests are inappropriate and/or the data is too sparse to be meaningful. 

      As we’re sure the reviewer appreciates, this was an extremely complex experiment to perform and analyse. We spent a lot of time trying to find the best way to illustrate the results (finally settling on a 2D vector representation of polarity) and how to show the paired statistical comparisons between different groups. Moreover, in the end we were only able to detect generally quite modest (statistically significant) changes in cell polarity under the experimental conditions.

      However, we note that failure to see large and consistent changes in polarity is exactly the expected result if it is hard to repolarise from a boundary – and this is of course the conclusion that we draw. Conversely, if repolarisation were easy, which was our expectation at least under de novo conditions without existing polarity, then we would have expected large and highly statistically significant changes in polarity across multiple cell rows. Hence we stand by our conclusion that ‘it is hard to repolarise from a boundary of Fz overexpression in both control and de novo polarity conditions’.

      Overall, we were trying to establish three points:

      (1) to demonstrate that repolarisation occurs from a boundary of overexpression i.e. from boundary 0 to row 0

      (2) to establish whether a wave of repolarisation occurs across rows 1, 2 and 3

      (3) to determine if in repolarisation in de novo condition it is easier to repolarise than in repolarisation in the control (already polarised) condition Taking each in turn:

      (1) To detect repolarisation from a boundary relative to the control condition, we have to compare row 0 in repolarisation condition (Fig.5G,K) vs control condition (Fig.5F,J). This comparison shows a significative repolarisation (p=0.0014). From now, row 0 in repolarisation condition is our reference for repolarisation occurring.

      (2) To determine if there is a wave of repolarisation in the repolarisation condition we have to compare row 0 vs row 1 to 3 in the repolarisation condition (Fig.5K). Row 1 is not significantly different to row 0, but rows 2 and 3 are different and the vectors show obviously lower polarity than row 0. Hence no wave of repolarisation is detected over rows 1 to 3.

      (3) To determine if it is easier to repolarise in the de novo condition, our reference for establishment of a repolarisation pattern is the polarisation condition in rows 0 to 3. So, we compare repolarisation condition vs repolarisation in de novo condition, row 0 vs row 0, row 1 vs row 1, row 2 vs row 2 and row 3 vs row 3 – in each case no significative difference in polarity is detected, supporting our conclusion that it is not easier to repolarise in the de novo condition.

      We agree that the variations in row 3 are puzzling, but there is no evidence that this is due to propagation of polarity from row 0, and so in terms of our three questions, it does not alter our conclusions.

      Changes to manuscript: We have extensively revised the text describing the results in Fig.5 to hopefully make the reasons for our conclusions clearer and also be more cautious in our conclusions in Results section ‘Induced core protein relocalisation…’. 

      For the related boundary intensity data in Fig 6, the authors need to describe exactly how boundaries were chosen or excluded from the analysis. Ideally, all boundaries would be classified as either meido-lateral (meaning anterior-posterior) or proximal-distal depending on angle. 

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out that this was not clear.

      All boundaries were classified following their orientation compared to the Fz over-expression boundary using hh-GAL4 expressed in the wing posterior compartment. Horizontal junctions were defined as parallel to the Fz over-expression boundary (between 0 and 45 degrees) and mediolateral junctions as junctions linking two horizontal boundaries (between 45 and 90 degrees).

      Changes to manuscript: The boundary classification detailed above has been added in the Materials and Methods.

      If the authors believe their Fig 5 and 6 analyses, how do they explain that hairs are reoriented well beyond where the core proteins are not? This would be a dramatic finding, because as far as I know, when core proteins are polarized, prehair orientation always follows the core protein distribution. Surprisingly, the authors do not so much as comment about this. The authors should age their wings just a bit more to see whether the prehair pattern looks more like the adult hair pattern or like that predicted by their protein orientation results.

      Again the reviewer makes an interesting point, and we agree that this is something that we should have more directly addressed in the manuscript.

      There are three reasons why we might expect adult trichomes to show a different effect from the measured core protein polarity pattern seen in our experiments:

      (i) we are assaying core protein polarity at 28h APF, but trichomes emerge at >32h APF, so there is still time for polarity to propagate a bit further from the boundary. We now have added data showing that by the point of trichome initiation, the wave of polarisation extends 3-4 cell rows (Fig.S5A).

      (ii) it has long been known that a strong localisation of core proteins at a cell edge is not required for polarisation of trichome polarity from a boundary. For instance, in Strutt & Strutt 2007 we show clones of cells overexpressing Fz causing propagation through pk[pk-sple] mutant tissue where there is no detectable core protein polarity. We were following up prior observations of Adler et al., 2000 in the wing and Lawrence et al., 2004 in the abdomen.

      (iii) there is evidence to suggest that the polarity of adult trichomes is locally coupled, possibly mechanically. This point is hard to prove without live imaging taking in both initial core protein localisation, the site of actin-rich trichome initiation and then the final orientation of the much larger microtubule filled trichome, and we’re not aware that such data exist. However, Wong & Adler 1993 (JCB) showed that over a number of hours trichomes become much larger and move towards the centre of the cell, presumably becoming decoupled from any core protein cue. The images in Guild … & Tilney, 2005 (MBoC)  are also interesting to look at in this regard. Finally, septate junction proteins have been implicated in local alignment of trichomes, independently of the core pathway (Venema … & Auld, 2004 Dev Biol).

      Changes to manuscript: Added new data in Fig.S5A showing where trichomes initiate under 6h de novo induction conditions, for comparison to core protein localisation and adult trichome data in Fig.5. Added some text explaining why adult trichome repolarisation might be stronger than the observed effects on core protein localisation in Discussion. 

      Minor points

      As the authors know, there is a model in the literature that suggests microtubule trafficking provides a global cue to orient PCP. The authors' repolarization data in Fig 4 make a reasonably convincing case against a role for no role for microtubules in cell-scale signaling, but do not rule out a role as a global cue. The authors should be careful of language such as "...MTs and core proteins being oriented independently of each other" that would appear to possibly also refer to a role as a global cue. 

      Thank you for pointing out that this was not clear. We have now modified the text to hopefully address this.

      Changes to manuscript: Text updated in Results section ‘Microtubules do not provide…’.

      Significance:

      There are two negative conclusions and one positive conclusion made by the authors. Provided the above points are addressed, the negative conclusions, that core proteins are not limiting and that microtubules are not involved in cell-scale signaling are solid. The positive conclusion is more nebulous - the authors say that cell-scale signaling is strong relative to cell-cell signaling - but how strong is strong? Strong relative to their prior expectations? I'm not sure how to interpret such a conclusion. Overall, we learn something from these results, though it fails to reveal anything about mechanism. These results will be of some interest to those studying PCP.

      The reviewer raises an interesting point, which is how do you compare the strength of two different processes, even if both processes affect the same outcome (in this case cell polarity). Repolarisation from a boundary has not been carefully studied at the level of core protein localisation in any previous study to our knowledge – this is one of the important novel aspects of this study. Hence there is not a baseline for defining strong repolarisation. Similarly, there has been no investigation of the nature of ‘cell-scale signalling’. This was a considerable challenge for us in writing the manuscript, and we have done our best to find appropriate language that hopefully conveys our message adequately. Minimally our work may provide a baseline for helping to define the ‘strengths’ of these processes in future studies.

      One of our main points is that we can generate an artificial boundary of Fz expression, where Fz levels are at least several fold higher than in the neighbouring cell (e.g. compare Fig.4N’ and O’) and only two rows of cells show a significant change in polarity relative to controls. Even when the tissue next to the overexpression domain is still in the process of generating polarity (de novo condition) then the boundary has little effect on polarity in neighbouring cell rows. This was a result that surprised us, and we tried to convey that by using language to suggest cell-scale signalling was stronger than cell-cell signalling i.e. stronger in terms of the ability to define the final direction of polarity.

      Changes to manuscript: In the revised manuscript we have reviewed our use of language and now avoid saying ‘strong’ but instead use terms such as ‘effective’ and ‘robust’ in e.g. Results section ‘Induced core protein relocalisation…’, the Discussion and we have also changed the title of the manuscript to avoid claiming a ‘strong’ signal.

      Reviewer #2:

      Overview

      This paper aims to dissect the relative importance of the various cues that establish PCP in the wing disc of Drosophila, which remains a prominent and relevant model for PCP. The authors suggest that one must consider cues at three scales (molecular, cell and tissue) and specifically design tests for the importance of cell-level cues, which they call non-local cell scale signalling. They develop clever experimental approaches that allow them to track complex stability and also to induce polarity at experimentally defined times. In a first set of experiments, they restore PCP after the global cues have disappeared (de novo polarisation) and conclude from the results that another (cell scale) cue must exist. In another set of experiments, they show that de novo repolarization is robust to the dosage of various components of core PCP, leading them to conclude that there must be an underlying cell scale polarity, which, apparently, has nothing to do with microtubule or cell shape polarity. They then describe nice evidence that de novo polarisation is relatively short range both in a polarised and unpolarised field. They conclude by there is a strong cell-intrinsic polarity that remains to be characterised.

      Critique

      The experiments described in this paper are of high quality with a sophisticated level of design and analysis. However, there needs to be some recalibration of the extent of the conclusions that can be drawn (see below). Moreover, a limitation of this paper is that, despite the quality of their data, they cannot give a molecular hint about the nature of their proposed cell-scale signal. Below are a two key points that the authors may want to clarify.

      (1) The first set of repolarisation experiment is performed after the global cell rearrangements that have been shown to act as global signal. However, this approach does not exclude the possible contribution of an unknown diffusible global signal.

      A similar point was raised by Reviewer 1. For the convenience of this reviewer, we’ll summarise the arguments against such an unknown cue again below. More broadly, both reviewers asking a similar question indicates that we have failed to lay out the evidence in sufficient detail. In our defence, we have used the same ‘de novo’ paradigm in three previous publications (Strutt and Strutt 2002, 2007; Brittle et al 2022) without attracting (overt) controversy. We have now added text to the Introduction and Results that goes into more detail, as well as more experimental evidence (Fig.S5).

      Firstly, it is worth noting that the global cues acting in the wing are poorly understood, with mostly negative evidence against particular cues accruing in recent years. This makes it a hard subject to succinctly discuss. Secondly, we accept that it is hard to prove there is no influence of global cues, when the nature of those cues and the time at which they act remain unclear. Below we summarise the reasons why we believe there are not significance effects of global cues in our experiments that would influence the interpretation of our results.

      First, our reading of the literature supports a broad consensus that an early radial core planar polarity pattern is realigned by cell flow produced by hinge contraction beginning at around 16h APF (e.g. Aigouy et al., 2010; Strutt and Strutt, 2015; Aw and Devenport, 2017; Butler and Wallingford, 2017; Tan and Strutt, 2025). Taken at face value, this suggests that there are ‘radial’ cues present prior to hinge contraction, maybe coming from the wing margin – arguably these radial cues could be Ft-Ds or Wnts or both, given they are expressed in patterns consistent with such a role (notwithstanding the published evidence arguing against roles for either of these cues). It then appears that hinge contraction supercedes these cues to convert a radial pattern to a proximodistal pattern – whether the radial cues that affect the core pathway earlier remain active after hinge contraction is unclear, although both Ft-Ds and Wnts appear to maintain their ‘radial’ patterns beyond the beginning of hinge contraction (e.g. Merkel et al., 2014; Ewen-Campen et al.,2020; Yu et al., 2020).

      We think that the reviewers are proposing the presence of a proximodistal cue that is active in the proximal region of the wing that we use for our experiments shown e.g. in Fig.5, and that this cue orients core polarity here (but not elsewhere in the wing) in a time window after 18h APF. Ft-Ds and Wnts do not seem to be plausible candidates as they are still in ‘radial’ patterns. This leaves either an unknown proximodistal cue (a gradient of some unknown signalling molecule?), or possibly some ability of hinge contraction to align proximodistal polarity specifically in this wing region but not elsewhere. We cannot definitively rule out either of these possibilities, but neither do we think there is sufficient evidence to justify invoking their existence to explain our observations.

      In particular, the reason that we don’t think there is a proximodistal cue in the proximal part of the wing after 18h APF, is that work from our lab shows that induction of Fz or Stbm expression at times around or after the start of hinge contraction (i.e. >16 h APF) results in increasing levels of trichome swirling with polarity not being coordinated with the tissue axis either proximally or distally (Strutt and Strutt, 2002; Strutt and Strutt 2007). Our simplest interpretation of this is that induction at these stages fails to result in the early radial pattern of core pathway polarity being established and hence a failure of hinge contraction to reorient radial to proximodistal. If hinge contraction alone could specify proximodistal polarity in the absence of the earlier radial polarity, then we would not expect to see swirling over much of the proximal wing (where the forces from hinge contraction are strongest, Etournay et al., 2015).

      In this manuscript, our earliest de novo experiments begin at 18h APF (de novo 10h), then at 20h APF (de novo 8h) and at 22h APF (de novo 6h). The image in Fig. 5B referred to by Reviewer 1, is of a wing where Fz is induced de novo at 22 h APF. In these wings, as expected, the core proteins localise asymmetrically in stereotypical swirling patterns throughout the wing surface (see Fig. 2M and also Strutt and Strutt, 2002; Strutt and Strutt 2007), but – usefully for our experiments – they broadly localise along the proximal-distal axis in the region analysed in Fig. 5B. Given the strong swirling in surrounding regions when inducing at >20h APF, we feel reasonably confident in assuming that the pattern is not due to a proximodistal cue present in the proximal wing. We appreciate that the original manuscript did not show images including the trichome pattern in adjacent regions, so this point would not have been clear, but we now include these in Supplementary Fig.S5. We have also added a note in the legend to Fig. 5B to clarify that the proximodistal pattern seen is local to this wing region.

      Changes to manuscript: Text extended in Introduction and Results to better explain why we believe the de novo conditions that we use most likely result in a polarity pattern that is not significantly influenced by ‘global cues’. Now show zoomed-out images of the surrounding region around the experiment region proximal to the anterior cross-vein region in adult wings, showing that the polarity pattern does not become more proximodistal when induction time is longer, and also that there is not overall proximodistal polarity in proximal regions of the wing, arguing against an unknown proximodistal polarity cue at these stages of development (Fig.S5B-E’’’).

      (2) The putative non-local cell scale signal must be more precisely defined (maybe also given a better name). It is not clear to me that one can separate cell-scale from molecular-scale signal.

      Local signals can redistribute within a cell (or membrane) so local signals are also cell-scale. Without a clear definition, it is difficult to interpret the results of the gene dosage experiments. The link between gene dosage and cell-scale signal is not rigorously stated. Related to this, the concluding statement of the introduction is too cryptic.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this, as again a similar comment was made by Reviewer 1, so we are clearly falling short in defining the term. We have now had another attempt in the Introduction.

      To more specifically answer the point made by the reviewer regarding molecular vs cellular, we are essentially being guided here by the prior computational modelling work, as at the biological level the details are still being worked out. A specific class of previous models only allowed ‘signals’ between core proteins to act ‘locally’, meaning within a cell junction, and within the models there was no explicit mechanism by which proteins on other junctions could ‘detect’ the polarity of a neighbouring junction (e.g. Amonlirdviman et al., 2005; Le Garrec et al., 2006; Fischer et al., 2013). Other models implicitly or explicitly encode a mechanism by which cell junctions can be influenced by the polarity of other junctions (e.g. Meinhardt, 2007; Burak and Shraiman, 2009; Abley et al., 2013; Shadkhoo and Mani, 2019), for instance by diffusion of a factor produced by localisation of particular planar polarity proteins.

      We agree with the reviewer that a cell-scale signal will depend on ‘molecules’ and thus could be called ‘molecular-scale’, but here by ‘molecular-scale’ we mean signals that at the range of the sizes of molecules i.e. nanometers, rather than cell-scale signals that act at the size of cells i.e. micrometers. A caveat to our definition is that we implicitly include interactions that occur locally on cell junctions (<1 µm range) within ‘molecular-scale’, but this is a shorter range than ‘cellular-scale’ which requires signals acting over the diameter of a cell (3-5 µm). Nevertheless, we think the concept of ‘molecular-scale’ vs ‘cell-scale’ is a helpful one in this context, and have attempted to address the issue through a more careful definition of the terms.

      Changes to manuscript: Text revised in Introduction and legend to Fig.1 to more carefully define ‘cell-scale signalling’ and to distinguish it from ‘molecular-scale signalling’. Final sentence of Introduction also altered so we no longer cryptically speculate on the nature of the cell-scale signal but leave this to the Discussion.

      Minor comments. 

      Some of the (clever) genetic manipulation may need more details in the text. For example:

      - Need to specify if the hs-flp approach induces expression throughout the tissue.

      We apologise for the lack of clarity. In all the experiments, the hs-FLP transgene is present in all cells, and heat-shock results in ubiquitous expression. 

      Changes to manuscript: We have clarified this in the Results and Materials and Methods.

      - Need to specify in the text that in the unpolarised condition the tissue is both dsh and fz mutant.

      The reviewer is of course correct and we have updated this point in the text. The full genotype for the unpolarised condition is: w dsh<sup>1</sup> hsFLP22/y;; Act>>fz-mKate2sfGFP, fz<sup>P21</sup>/fz<sup>P21</sup> (see Table S1). So this line is mutant for dsh and fz with induced expression of Fz-mKate2sfGFP. 

      Changes to manuscript: We have clarified this in the relevant part of the Results.

      - Need to specify in the text that the experiment illustrated in Fig 5 is with hh-gal4. 

      As noted by the reviewer, we continued to use the same hh-GAL4 repolarisation paradigm as in Fig.4 and this info was in the legend to Fig.5 legend. However, we agree it is helpful to be explicit about this in the main text.

      Changes to manuscript: We have added this to this section of the Results.

      - Need to address a possible shortcoming of the hh experiment, that the AP boundary is a region of high tension.

      It is true that the AP boundary is under high tension in the wing disc (e.g. Landsberg et al., 2009). But we are not aware of any evidence that this higher tension persists into the pupal wing. In separate studies we have labelled for Myosin II in pupal wings (Trinidad et al 2025 Curr Biol; Tan & Strutt 2025 Nature Comms), and as far as we have noticed have not seen preferentially higher levels on the AP boundary. We think if tension were higher, the cell boundaries would appear straighter than in surrounding cells (as seen in the wing disc) and this is not evident in our images.

      - Need to dispel the possibility that there is no residual polarisation (e.g. of other components) in fz1 mutant (I assume this is the case).

      We use the null allele fz[P21] through this work, and we and others have consistently reported a complete loss of polarisation of other core proteins or downstream components in this background. The caveat to this is that core proteins that persist at cell junctions always appear at least slightly punctate in mutant backgrounds for other core proteins, and so any automated detection algorithm will always find evidence of individual cell polarity above a baseline level of uniform distribution. Hence we tend to use lack of local coordination of polarity (variance of cell polarity angle) as an additional measure of loss of polarisation, in addition to direct measures of average cell polarity. (We discuss this in the QuantifyPolarity manuscript Tan et al 2021 e.g. Fig.S6).

      Changes to manuscript: We now include in the Materials and Methods section ‘Fly genetics…’ a much more extensive explanation of the evidence for specific mutant alleles being ‘null’ for planar polarity function (including dsh1 as raised by Reviewer 1), specifically that they result in no detectable planar polarisation of either other core proteins or downstream effectors, and added appropriate references.

      - Need to provide evidence that 50% gene dosage commensurately affect protein level. 

      This is a good suggestion. In the case of Stbm, we have already published a western blot showing that a reduction in gene dosage results in reduced protein levels (Strutt et al 2016, Fig.S6). We have now performed western blots to quantify protein levels upon reduction of fmi, pk and dgo levels (we actually used EGFP-dgo for the latter, as we don’t have antibodies that can detect endogenous Dgo on western blots).

      Changes to manuscript: When presenting the dosage reduction experiments, we now refer back to Strutt et al., 2016 explicitly for Stbm, and have added western blot data for Fmi, Pk and EGFPDgo in new Fig.S2.

      - I am surprised that the relationship with microtubule polarity was never investigated. Is this true? 

      We agree this is a point that needed further clarification, as Reviewer 1 made a related point regarding the two possible roles for microtubules, one being as a mediator of a global cue upstream of the core pathway, and the second (which we investigate in this manuscript) as a mediator of a cell-scale signal downstream of the core pathway.

      Both the Uemura and Axelrod groups have published on potential upstream function as a global cue mediator in the Drosophila wing (e.g. Shimada et al., 2006; Harumoto et al., 2010; Matis et al., 2014).

      Both groups have also looked out whether core pathway components could affect orientation of microtubules (Harumoto et al., 2010; Olofsson at al., 2014; Sharp and Axelrod 2016). Notably Harumoto et al., 2010 observed that in 24h APF wings, loss of Fz or Stbm did not alter microtubule polarity from a proximodistal orientation consistent with the microtubules aligning along the long cell axis in the absence of other cues. However, this did not rule out an instructive effect of Fz or Stbm on microtubule polarity during core pathway cell-scale signalling. The Axelrod lab manuscripts saw interesting effects of Pk protein isoforms on microtubule polarity, albeit not throughout the entire wing, which hinted at a potential role in cell-scale signalling. Taken together this prior work was the motivation for our directed experiments to specifically test whether the core pathway might generate cell-scale polarity by instructing microtubule polarity.

      Changes to manuscript: We have revised the Results section ‘Microtubules do not…’ to make a clearer distinction regarding possible ‘upstream’ and ‘downstream’ roles of microtubules in Drosophila core pathway planar polarity and the motivation for our experiments investigating the latter.

      - The authors suggest that polarity does not propagate as a wave. And yet the range measured in adult is longer than in the pupal wing. Explain. 

      Again an excellent point, also made by Reviewer 1, which we have now addressed explicitly in the manuscript. For the convenience of this reviewer, we lay out the reasons why we think the propagation of polarity seen in the adult is further than seen for core protein localisation.

      There are three reasons why we might expect adult trichomes to show a different effect from the measured core protein polarity pattern seen in our experiments:

      (i) we are assaying core protein polarity at 28h APF, but trichomes emerge at >32h APF, so there is still time for polarity to propagate a bit further from the boundary. We now have added data showing that by the point of trichome initiation, the wave of polarisation extends 3-4 cell rows (Fig.S5A).  

      (ii) it has long been known that a strong localisation of core proteins at a cell edge is not required for polarisation of trichome polarity from a boundary. For instance, in Strutt & Strutt 2007 we show clones of cells overexpressing Fz causing propagation through pk[pk-sple] mutant tissue where there is no detectable core protein polarity. We were following up prior observations of Adler et al 2000 in the wing and Lawrence et al 2004 in the abdomen.

      (iii) there is evidence to suggest that the polarity of adult trichomes is locally coupled, possibly mechanically. This point is hard to prove without live imaging taking in both initial core protein localisation, the site of actin-rich trichome initiation and then the final orientation of the much larger microtubule filled trichome, and we’re not aware that such data exist. However, Wong & Adler 1993 (JCB) showed that over a number of hours trichomes become much larger and move towards the centre of the cell, presumably becoming decoupled from any core protein cue. The images in Guild … & Tilney, 2005 (MBoC)  are also interesting to look at in this regard. Finally, septate junction proteins have been implicated in local alignment of trichomes, independently of the core pathway (Venema … & Auld, 2004 Dev Biol).

      Changes to manuscript: Added new data in Fig.S5A showing where trichomes initiate under 6h de novo induction conditions, for comparison to core protein localisation and adult trichome data in Fig.5. Added some text explaining why adult trichome repolarisation might be stronger than the observed effects on core protein localisation in Discussion. 

      - The discussion states that the cell-intrinsic system remains to be fully characterised, implying that it has been partially characterised. What do we know about it? 

      As the reviewer probably realises, we were attempting to side-step a long speculative discussion about the various hints and ideas in the literature by grouping them under the umbrella of ‘remaining to be fully characterised’. We would argue that this current manuscript is the first to attempt to systematically investigate the nature of ‘cell-scale signalling’. The lack of prior work is probably due to two factors (i) pioneering theoretical work showed that a sufficiently strong global signal coupled with ‘local’ (i.e. confined to one cell junction) protein interactions was sufficient to polarise cells without the need to invoke the existence of a cell-scale signal; (ii) there is no easy way to identify cell-scale signals as their loss results in loss of polarity which will also occur if other (i.e. more locally acting) core pathway functions are compromised.

      The main investigation of the potential for cell-scale signalling has been another set of theory studies (Burak and Shraiman 2009; Abley et al., 2013; Shadkhoo and Mani 2019) which have considered the possibility of diffusible signals. In our present work we have further considered the possibility of a ‘depletion’ model, based on the pioneering theory work of Hans Meinhardt, and as discussed above the possibility that microtubules could mediate a cell-scale signal.

      Changes to manuscript: We have revised the Discussion to hopefully be clearer about the current state of knowledge.

      Reviewer #3:

      The manuscript by Carayon and Strutt addresses the role of cell-scale signaling during the establishment of planar cell polarity (PCP) in the Drosophila pupal wing. The authors induce locally the expression of a tagged core PCP protein, Frizzled, and observe and analyze the de novo establishment of planar cell polarity. Using this system, the authors show that PCP can be established within several hours, that PCP is robust towards variation in core PCP protein levels, that PCP proteins do not orient microtubules, and that PCP is robust towards 'extrinsic' repolarization. The authors conclude that the polarization at the cell-scale is strongly intrinsic and only weakly affected by the polarity of neighboring cells. 

      Major comments

      The data are clearly presented and the manuscript is well written. The conclusions are well supported by the data. 

      (1) The authors use a system to de novo establish PCP, which has the advantage of excluding global cues orienting PCP and thus to focus on the cell-intrinsic mechanisms. At the same time, the system has the limitation that it is unclear to what extent de novo PCP establishment reflects 'normal' cell scale PCP establishment, in particular because the Gal4/UAS expression system that is used to induce Fz expression will likely result in much higher Fz levels compared with the endogenous levels. The authors should briefly discuss this limitation. 

      We apologise if this wasn’t clear. We only used GAL4/UAS overexpression when we were generating an artificial boundary of Fz expression with hh-GAL4 to induce repolarisation. The de novo induction system involves Fz::mKate2-sfGFP being expressed directly under an Act5C promoter without use of GAL4/UAS. In response to a comment from Reviewer 1 we have now carried out western blot analysis which shows that Fz::mKate2-sfGFP levels under Act5C are actually lower than endogenous Fz levels. As we achieve normal levels of polarity, similar to what we measure in wild-type conditions when measured using QuantifyPolarity, we assume that therefore Fz levels are not limiting under these conditions. However, we note that lower than normal levels of Fz might sensitise the system to perturbation, which in fact would be advantageous in our study, as it might for instance have been expected to more readily reveal dosage sensitivity of other components.

      Changes to manuscript: We now describe the levels of expression achieved using the de novo induction system (Fig.S1C-D) and discuss possible consequences in the relevant Results sections and Discussion.

      (2) Fig. 3. The authors use heterozygous mutant backgrounds to test the robustness of de novo PCP establishment towards (partial) depletion in core PCP proteins. The authors conclude that de novo polarization is 'extremely robust to variation in protein level'. Since the authors (presumably) lowered protein levels by 50%, this conclusion appears to be somewhat overstated. The authors should tune down their conclusion. 

      Reviewer 1 makes a similar point about whether we can argue that the lack of sensitivity to a 50% reduction in protein levels actually rules out the depletion model. To address the comments of both reviewers we had now added some further narrative and caveats in the text.

      We nevertheless believe that the experiments shown effectively make the point that there is no strong dosage sensitivity – and it remains our contention that if protein levels were the key to setting up cell-scale polarity, then a 50% reduction would be expected to show an effect on the rate of polarisation. We further note that as Fz::mKate2-sfGFP levels are lower than endogenous Fz levels, the system might be expected to be sensitised to further dosage reductions, and despite this we fail to see an effect on rate of polarisation.

      In a similar vein, Reviewer 2 requested data on whether dosage reduction altered protein levels by the expected amount. We have now added further explanation/references and western blot data to address this.

      Changes to manuscript: Added some narrative and caveats regarding whether lowering levels more than 50% would add to our findings in the Discussion. Revised conclusions to be more cautious including altering section title to read ‘Planar polarity establishment is not highly sensitive to variation in protein levels of core complex components.

      Also added westerns and text/references showing that for the tested proteins there is a reduction in protein levels upon removal of one gene dosage in Results section ‘Planar polarity establishment is…’ and Fig.S2.

      Minor comments 

      (1) Page 3. The authors mention and reference that they used the PCA method to quantify cell polarity magnification and magnitude. It would help the unfamiliar reader, if the authors would briefly describe the principle of this method. 

      Changes to manuscript: More details have been added in Materials & Methods.

      Significance:

      The manuscript contributes to our understanding of how planar cell polarity is established. It extends previous work by the authors (Strutt and Strutt, 2002,2007) that already showed that induction of core PCP pathway activity by itself is sufficient to induce de novo PCP. This manuscript further explores the underlying mechanisms. The authors test whether de novo PCP establishment depends on an 'inhibitory signal', as previously postulated (Meinhardt, 2007), but do not find evidence. They also test whether core PCP proteins help to orient microtubules (which could enhance cell intrinsic polarization of core PCP proteins), but, again, do not find evidence, corroborating previous work (Harumoto et al, 2010). The most significant finding of this manuscript, perhaps, is the observation that local de novo PCP establishment does not propagate far through the tissue. A limitation of the study is that the mechanisms establishing intrinsic cell scale polarity remain unknown. The work will likely be of interest to specialists in the field of PCP.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study by Wu et al presents interesting data on bacterial cell organization, a field that is progressing now, mainly due to the advances in microscopy. Based mainly on fluorescence microscopy images, the authors aim to demonstrate that the two structures that account for bacterial motility, the chemotaxis complex and the flagella, colocalize to the same pole in Pseudomonas aeruginosa cells and to expose the regulation underlying their spatial organization and functioning.

      Comments on revisions:

      The authors have addressed all major and minor points that I raised in a satisfying way during the revision process. The work can now be regarded as complete, the assumptions were clarified, the results are convincing, the conclusions are justified, and the novelty has been made clear.

      This manuscript will be of interest to cell biologists, mainly those studying bacteria, but not only.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Here, the authors studied the molecular mechanisms by which the chemoreceptor cluster and flagella motor of Pseudomonas aeruginosa (PA) are spatially organized in the cell. They argue that FlhF is involved in localizing the receptors-motor to the cell pole, and even without FlhF, the two are colocalized. Finally, the authors argue that the functional reason for this colocalization is to insulate chemotactic signaling from other signaling pathways, such as cyclic-di-GMP signaling.

      Strength:

      The experiments and data are high quality. It is clear that the motor and receptors co-localize, and that elevated CheY levels lead to elevated c-di-GMP.

      Weakness:

      The explanation for the functional importance of receptor-motor colocalization is plausible but is still not conclusively demonstrated. Colocalization might reduce CheY levels throughout the cell in order to reduce cross-talk with c-di-GMP. This would mean that if physiologically-relevant levels of CheYp near the pole were present throughout the cell, c-di-GMP levels would be elevated to a point that is problematic for the cell. Clearly demonstrating this seems challenging.

      We acknowledge that directly proving the necessity of colocalization to prevent problematic c-di-GMP elevation is experimentally challenging, as it would require creating a system where CheY-P is artificially distributed throughout the cell at physiologically relevant concentrations while maintaining normal chemotaxis function.

      However, our data provide several lines of evidence supporting this model. First, we show that CheY overexpression leads to substantial c-di-GMP elevation (71.8% increase) and cell aggregation, demonstrating that elevated CheY levels can indeed cause problematic cross-pathway interference. Second, previous work has shown that CheY-P levels near the pole are an order of magnitude higher than in the rest of the cell (ref. 46). If this elevated CheY-P concentration near the pole were present throughout the cell, our data suggest that c-di-GMP levels would be elevated sufficiently to cause cell aggregation (Fig. 4A), thereby disabling normal motility and chemotaxis. Third, the dose-dependent relationship between CheY concentration and aggregation phenotype supports the idea that precise spatial regulation of CheY levels is functionally important for avoiding cross-pathway interference.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors investigated the assembly and polar localization of the chemosensory cluster in P. aeruginosa. They discovered that a certain protein (FlhF) is required for the polar localization of the chemosensory cluster while a fully-assembled motor is necessary for the assembly of the cluster. They found that flagella and chemosensory clusters always co-localize in the cell; either at the cell pole in wild type cells or randomly-located in the cell in FlhF mutant cells. They hypothesize that this co-localization is required to keep the level of another protein (CheY-P), which controls motor switching, at low levels as the presence of high-levels of this protein (if the flagella and chemosensory clusters were not co-localized) is associated with high-levels of c-di-GMP and cell aggregations.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is clearly written and straightforward. The authors applied multiple techniques to study the bacterial motility system including fluorescence light microscopy and gene editing. In general, the work enhances our understanding of the subtlety of interaction between the chemosensory cluster and the flagellar motor to regulate cell motility.

      Weaknesses:

      The major weakness for me in this paper is that the authors never discussed how the flagellar genes expression is controlled in P. aeruginosa. For example, in E. coli there is a transcriptional hierarchy for the flagellar genes (early, middle, and late genes, see Chilcott and Hughes, 2000). Similarly, Campylobacter and Helicobacter have a different regulatory cascade for their flagellar genes (See Lertsethtakarn, Ottemann, and Hendrixson, 2011). How does the expression of flagellar genes in P. aeruginosa compare to other species? how many classes are there for these genes? is there a hierarchy in their expression and how does this affect the results of the FliF and FliG mutants? In other words, if FliF and FliG are in class I (as in E. coli) then their absence might affect the expression of other later flagellar genes in subsequent classes (i.e., chemosensory genes). Also, in both FliF and FliG mutants no assembly intermediates of the flagellar motor are present in the cell as FliG is required for the assembly of FliF (see Hiroyuki Terashima et al. 2020, Kaplan et al. 2019, Kaplan et al. 2022). It could be argued that when the motor is not assembled then this will affect the expression of the other genes (e.g., those of the chemosensory cluster) which might play a role in the decreased level of chemosensory clusters the authors find in these mutants.

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable suggestions. In the revised manuscript, we have further elaborated on the regulatory control of flagellar genes expression in P. aeruginosa (see our response to comment #4).

      Comments on revisions:

      I believe the authors have performed additional experiments that improved their manuscript and they have answered many of my comments and those of the other reviewers. I am supportive of publishing this manuscript, but I still find the following points that are not clear to me (probably I am misunderstanding some points; the authors can clarify).

      (1) In response to reviewer 1, the authors say that they "analyzed and categorized the distribution of the chemotaxis complex in both wild-type and flhF mutant strains into three patterns: precise-polar, near-polar, and mid-cell localization." I can see what they mean by polar and mid-cell, but near-polar sounds a bit elusive? Can they provide examples of this stage and mention how accurately they can identify it? Also, do the pie charts they show in Figure S4 really show "significant alterations"? There is a difference between 98% and 85% as they mention in their response to reviewer 1, but I am not sure that this is significant? Probably they can explain/change the language in the text? Also, the number of cells they counted for FlhF mutant is more than the double of other strains (WT and FlhF FliF mutant)?

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable suggestions. To clarify, we divided the intracellular area along the cell's long axis into three domains: the two ends each representing 10% of the length as the precise-polar domain, the central 50% as the mid-cell domain, and the remaining regions between these as the near-polar domain. The localization pattern of the chemotaxis complex was assigned based on the position of the fluorescence intensity centroid within these domains.

      Regarding the significance of the changes, you are correct to question our language. When flhF was knocked out, the proportion of chemotaxis complexes with precise-polar distribution decreased from 98% to 85% - a 13% reduction. While this represents a measurable shift in localization pattern, describing this as "significant alterations" was probably imprecise. We have revised this language to more accurately reflect the magnitude of the change (lines 169-177).

      For the cell counting, we increased the sample size for the flhF mutant because this strain exhibited the appearance of mid-cell localization (approximately 5% of cells), which was not observed in wild-type or flhF fliF double mutant strains. To accurately quantify this rare phenotype and ensure statistical reliability, we analyzed more cells for this particular strain. This explains why the flhF mutant dataset contains approximately double the number of cells compared to the other strains.

      We have redrawn Figure S4 to include a clear schematic diagram of the cell partitioning method and provided representative examples of each localization pattern (precise-polar, near-polar, and mid-cell) to better illustrate how we distinguished between these categories.

      (2) One thing that also confused me is the following: One point that the authors stress is that FlhF localizes both the flagellum and the chemoreceptors to the pole. However, if I look at Figure 2B, the flagellum and the chemoreceptors still co-localize together (although not at the pole). If FlhF was responsible for co-localizing both of them to the pole, then wouldn't one expect them to be randomly localized in this mutant and by that I mean that they do not co-localize but that each of them (the flagellum and the chemoreceptors) are located in a different random location of the cell (not co-localized). The fact that they are still co-localized together in this mutant could also be interpreted by, for example, that FlhF localizes the flagellum to the pole and another mechanism localizes the chemoreceptors to the flagellum, hence, they still co-localize in this mutant because the chemoreceptors follow the flagellum by another mechanism to wherever it goes?

      Thank you for this insightful observation. You are correct that our current experimental results do not definitively establish that FlhF directly localizes both the flagellum and chemoreceptors to the pole independently. The persistent colocalization of flagella and chemoreceptors in the DflhF mutant, even when both are mislocalized away from the pole, actually suggests a more complex regulatory mechanism than we initially proposed.

      This observation highlights an important distinction between polar targeting and colocalization maintenance. Our data suggest that FlhF influences the polar targeting of the flagellum-chemoreceptor assembly, but the colocalization itself appears to be governed by a different mechanism that operates independently of FlhF. This could involve direct protein-protein interactions between flagellar and chemotaxis components, or shared assembly machinery that we have yet to identify.

      To better reflect this interpretation, we have revised the subsection title (line 150). We have also modified the relevant discussion (line 180) to more accurately describe FlhF’s role in polar targeting rather than claiming it directly controls chemoreceptor localization.

      (3) In the response to reviewers, the authors mention "suggesting that the assembly of the receptor complex is likely influenced mainly by the C-ring and MS-ring structures rather than by the P ring". However, in the article, they still write "The complete assembly of the motor serves as a partial prerequisite for the assembly of the chemotaxis complex, and its assembly site is also regulated by the polar anchor protein FlhF" despite their FlgI results which is not in accordance with this statement? Also, As I mentioned in my previous report, in FliG and FliF mutant the motor does not assemble (see Hiroyuki Terashima et al. 2020., and Kaplan et al., 2022).

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestions and acknowledge the contradictions in our original text. You are correct that in DfliF and DfliG mutants, the flagellar motor does not assemble, while the P ring (FlgI) functions as a bushing for the peptidoglycan layer and its absence does not prevent motor assembly.

      Our DflgI results, which showed normal chemotaxis complex assembly similar to wild-type, clearly demonstrate that the P ring is not required for chemoreceptor complex formation. This contradicts our original statement that "complete assembly of the motor serves as a partial prerequisite for the assembly of the chemotaxis complex."

      We have corrected this inconsistency by: 1) Revising the subsection title (line 186) to more accurately reflect that core motor structures, rather than complete motor assembly, influences chemoreceptor complex formation. 2) Modifying sentences in the introduction (lines 97-98) to better align with our experimental findings.

      (4) The authors have said in their response to my point "and currently, there is no evidence that FliA activity is influenced by proteins like FliG". I just want to clarify what I meant in my previous report: In E. coli, FliA binds to FlgM, and when the hook is assembled FlgM is secreted outside the cell allowing FliA to trigger the transcription of class III genes, which include the chemosensory genes (see Figure 5 in Beeby et al, 2020 in FEMS Microbiology, and Chilcott and Hughes, 2000). This implies that if the hook is not built, then late genes (including the chemoreceptors) should not be present. However, in Kaplan et al., 2019, the authors imaged a FliF mutant in Shewanella oneidensis (Figure S3) and still saw that chemoreceptors are present (I believe the authors must highlight this). This suggests that species such as Shewanella and Pseudomonas have a different assembly process than that E. coli, and although the authors say that in the text, I believe they still can refine this part more in the spirit of what I wrote here.

      We thank the reviewer for the important clarification regarding the differences in transcriptional regulation among bacterial species. We agree that the observation of chemoreceptors in Shewanella oneidensis DfliF mutants (Kaplan et al., 2019) represents a significant deviation from the well-characterized E. coli model and merits stronger emphasis. In response, we have expanded the discussion to more clearly highlight the critical distinctions in the transcriptional regulatory circuits governing flagellar and chemoreceptor biogenesis between E. coli and species such as Shewanella oneidensis and Pseudomonas aeruginosa (lines 351-363).

      I do not like to ask for additional experiments in the second round of review, so for me if the authors modify the text to tackle these points and allow for probable alternative explanations/ highlight gaps/ modify language used for some claims, then that is fine with me.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      It is plausible that colocalization reduces CheY levels throughout the cell in order to reduce cross-talk with c-di-GMP. This would mean that if physiologically-relevant levels of CheYp near the pole were present throughout the cell, c-di-GMP levels would be elevated to a point that is problematic for the cell. Clearly demonstrating this seems challenging.

      We acknowledge that directly proving the necessity of colocalization to prevent problematic c-di-GMP elevation is experimentally challenging, as it would require creating a system where CheY-P is artificially distributed throughout the cell at physiologically relevant concentrations while maintaining normal chemotaxis function.

      However, our data provide several lines of evidence supporting this model. First, we show that CheY overexpression leads to substantial c-di-GMP elevation (71.8% increase) and cell aggregation, demonstrating that elevated CheY levels can indeed cause problematic cross-pathway interference. Second, previous work has shown that CheY-P levels near the pole are an order of magnitude higher than in the rest of the cell (ref. 46). If this elevated CheY-P concentration near the pole were present throughout the cell, our data suggest that c-di-GMP levels would be elevated sufficiently to cause cell aggregation (Fig. 4A), thereby disabling normal motility and chemotaxis. Third, the dose-dependent relationship between CheY concentration and aggregation phenotype supports the idea that precise spatial regulation of CheY levels is functionally important for avoiding cross-pathway interference.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Major comments:

      (1) The main issue that I have with this study is the lack of exploration of "why" the model produces the results it does. Considering this is a model, it should be possible to find out why the three timescales of half-act/inact parameter modifications lead to different sets of results. Without this, it is simply an exploratory exercise. (The model does this, but we do not know the mechanism.) Perhaps this is enough as an interesting finding, but it remains unconvincing and (clearly) does not have the impact of describing a potential mechanism that could be potentially explored experimentally.

      This is now addressed in a new section in Results (“Potential Mechanism”):

      “To explore why the properties of the resulting bursters depend on the timescale of half-(in)activation adjustments, we examined what happens when SP1 is assembled under different half-(in)activation timescales: (1) fast, (2) intermediate (matching the timescale of ion channel density changes), and (3) infinitely slow (i.e., effectively turned off). The effects of these timescales can be seen by comparing the zoomed-in views of the SP1 activity profiles under each condition (Figure 4).

      When half-(in)activations are fast, the time evolution of — which tracks how far the activity pattern is from its targets (see Methods)—shows an abrupt jump as it searches for a voltage-dependence configuration that meets calcium targets (Figure 4A). As this happens, the channel densities are slightly altered, and this process continues again. Slowing the half-(in)activations alterations reduces these abrupt fluctuations (Figure 4B). Making the alterations infinitely slow effectively removes half-(in)activation changes altogether, leaving the system reliant solely on slower alterations in maximal conductances (Figure 4C). Because each timescale of half-(in)activation produces a different channel repertoire at each time step, different timescales of half-(in)activation alteration led the model through a different path in the space of activity profiles and intrinsic properties. Ultimately, this resulted in distinct final activity patterns – all of which were consistent with the Ca<sup>2+</sup> targets [22].

      (2) A related issue is the use of bootstrapping to do statistics for a family of models, especially when the question is in fact the width of the distribution of output attributes. I don't buy this. One can run enough models to find say N number of models within a tight range (say 2% cycle period) and the same N number within a loose range (say 20%) and compare the statistics within the two groups with the same N.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s skepticism regarding our statistical approach with the “Group of 5” and “Group of 20.” These groups arose from historical aspects of our analysis and this analysis does not directly advance the main point—that changes in the timescale of channel voltage-dependence alterations impact the properties of bursters to which the homeostatic mechanism converges. Therefore, we removed the references to the Group of 5 and focus on how the Group of 20 responds to variations in the timescale of voltage-dependent alterations.

      (3) The third issue is that many of the results that are presented (but not the main one) are completely expected. If one starts with gmax values that would never work (say all of them 0), then it doesn't matter how much one moves the act/inact curves one probably won't get the desired activity. Alternately, if one starts with gmax values that are known to work and randomizes the act/inact midpoints, then the expectation would be that it converges to something that works. This is Figure 1 B and C, no surprise. But it should work the other way around too. If one starts with random act/inact curves that would never work and fixes those, then why would one expect any set of gmax values would produce the desired response? I can easily imagine setting the half-act/inact values to values that never produce any activity with any gmax.

      We appreciate this observation and agree that it highlights a limitation of our initial condition sampling. Our claim that the half-(in)activation mechanism is subordinate to the maximal conductance mechanism is not intended as a general statement. Rather, we make this observation only within the specific range of initial conditions we explored. Within this restricted set, we found that the conductance mechanism was sufficient for successful assembly, while the half-(in)activation mechanism alone was not. We have revised the manuscript to limit the claim.

      “The results shown in Figure 1A require activity-dependent regulation of the maximal conductances. When activity-dependent regulation of the maximal conductances is turned off, the model failed to assemble SP1 into a burster (Figure 1B). This was seen in the other 19 Starting Parameters (SP2-SP20), as well [22].

      (4) A potential response to my previous criticism would be that you put reasonable constraints on gmax's or half-act/inact values or tie the half-act to half-inact. But that is simply arbitrary ad hoc decisions made to make the model work, much like the L8-norm used to amplify some errors. There is absolutely no reason to believe this is tied to the biology of the system.

      Here the reviewer highlights that model choices (e.g., constraints on maximal conductance and half-(in)activation, use of the L8 norm) are not necessarily justified by biology. A discussion of the constraints on maximal conductance and half-(in)activation are in the Model Assumptions section at the end of Methods. The Methods also contains a longer discussion of the use of the L8 norm:

      “To compute this match score, we adapted a formulation from Alonso et al (2023),  who originally used a root-mean-square (RMS) or  norm to combine the sensor mismatches. In that approach, each error (, , and ) is divided by its allowable tolerance (, , and ) to produce a normalized error. These normalized errors are then squared, summed, and square-rooted to produce a single scalar score that reflects how well the model matches the target activity pattern.

      In our version, we instead used an  norm, which raises each normalized error to the 8th power before summing and taking the 1/8th root. This formulation emphasizes large deviations in any one sensor, making it easier to pinpoint which feature of the activity is limiting convergence. By amplifying outlier mismatches, this approach provided a clearer view of which sensor was driving model mismatch, helping us both interpret failure modes and tune the model’s sensitivity by adjusting the tolerances for individual sensor errors.

      Although the  norm emphasizes large deviations more strongly than the  norm, the choice of norm does not fundamentally alter which models can converge—a model that performs well under one norm can also be made to perform well under another by adjusting the allowable tolerances. The biophysical mechanisms by which neurons detect deviations from target activity and convert them into changes in ion channel properties are still not well understood. Given this uncertainty, and the fact that using different norms ultimately shouldn’t affect the convergence of a given model, the use of different norms to combine sensor errors is consistent with the broader basic premise of the model: that intrinsic homeostatic regulation is calcium mediated [22].

      (5) The discussion of this manuscript is at once too long and not adequate. It goes into excruciating detail about things that are simply not explored in this study, such as phosphorylation mechanisms, justification of model assumptions of how these alterations occur, or even the biological relevance. (The whole model is an oversimplification - lack of anatomical structure, three calcium sensors, arbitrary assumptions, and how parameter bounds are implemented.) Lengthy justifications for why channel density & half-act/inact of all currents are obeying the same time constant are answering a question that no one asked. It is a simplified model to make an important point. The authors should make these parts concise and to the point. More importantly, the authors should discuss the mechanism through which these differences may arise. Even if it is not clear, they should speculate.

      We agree. A long discussion on Model Assumptions and potential biological mechanisms that implement alteration in channel voltage-dependence obscure this. The former is relocated to the Methods section. The latter discussion is shortened. A discussion of a potential mechanism is included in the Results (Figure 4).

      (6) There should be some justification or discussion of the arbitrary assumptions made in the model/methods. I understand some of this is to resolve issues that had come up in previous iterations of this approach and in fact the Alonso et al, 2023 paper was mainly to deal with these issues. However, some level of explanation is needed, especially when assumptions are made simply because of the intuition of the modeler rather than the existence of a biological constraint or any other objective measure.

      A discussion of Model Assumptions is included in the Methods.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Mondal and co-authors present the development of a computational model of homeostatic plasticity incorporating activity-dependent regulation of gating properties (activation, inactivation) of ion channels. The authors show that, similar to what has been observed for activity-dependent regulation of ion channel conductances, implementing activity-dependent regulation of voltage sensitivity participates in the achievement of a target phenotype (bursting or spiking). The results however suggest that activity-dependent regulation of voltage sensitivity is not sufficient to allow this and needs to be associated with the regulation of ion channel conductances in order to reliably reach the target phenotype. Although the implementation of this biologically relevant phenomenon is undeniably relevant, the main conclusions of the paper and the insights brought by this computational work are difficult to grasp.

      Strengths:

      (1) Implementing activity-dependent regulation of gating properties of ion channels is biologically relevant.

      (2) The modeling work appears to be well performed and provides results that are consistent with previous work performed by the same group.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The writing is rather confusing, and the state of the art explaining the need for the study is unclear.

      We reorganized the manuscript to make its focus clearer.

      Introduction: We clarified our explanation of the state-of-the-art. Briefly, prior work on activity-dependent homeostasis has focused on regulating ion channel density. Neurons have also been documented to homeostatically regulate channel voltage-dependence. However, the consequences of channel voltage-dependence alterations on homeostatic regulation remain underexplored. To study this, we extend a computational model of activity-dependent homeostasis — originally developed to only alter channel density— to alter channel voltage-dependence.

      Results: We reorganized this section to underscore the main point: that the timescale of half-(in)activation alterations influences the intrinsic properties and activity patterns targeted by a homeostatic mechanism. Figures 1A and 1B were retained to provide context—Figure 1A illustrates how activity can emerge from random initial conditions, while Figure 1B suggests that in these simulations, modulation of half-(in)activation played a specific limited role. Figure 2 builds on Figure 1A by summarizing how intrinsic properties and activity characteristics vary across a population of 20 bursters. Figure 3 then demonstrates that despite playing this specific limited role, altering the timescale of half-(in)activation in these simulations significantly impacted the intrinsic properties and activity characteristics of the bursters targeted by the homeostatic mechanism. Figure 4 supports this by offering a possible mechanistic explanation. Finally, Figure 5 reinforces the central message by showing how the same population responds to perturbation when the timescale of half-(in)activation alterations is varied—essentially extending the analysis of Figure 3 to a perturbed regime.

      Discussion: The Discussion concentrates on more specifically on how the timescale of half-(in)activation alterations shape bursters targeted he homeostatic mechanism. Extended content on model assumptions is moved to Methods. The discussion of biological pathways that implement channel voltage-dependence is shortened to avoid distracting from the main message.

      Methods: Aside from moving model assumptions here, we removed discussion of the “Group of 5” and explained in more detail why we chose the L8 norm.

      (2) The main outcomes and conclusions of the study are difficult to grasp. What is predicted or explained by this new version of homeostatic regulation of neuronal activity?

      Our message is general: the timescale of half-(in)activation alterations influences the intrinsic properties and activity characteristics of bursters targeted by a homeostatic mechanism. As such, the implications are general. Their value lies in circumscribing a conceptual framework from which experimentalists may devise and test new hypotheses. We do not aim to predict or explain any specific phenomenon in this work. To address this concern the Discussion highlights two potential implications of our findings—one to neuronal development and another to pathologies that may arise from disruptions to homeostatic processes:

      “One application for the simulations involving the self-assembly of activity may be to model the initial phases of neural development, when a neuron transitions from having little or no electrical activity to possessing it (Baccaglini & Spitzer 1977). As shown in Figure 6, the timescale of (in)activation curve alterations define a neuron's activity characteristics and intrinsic properties. As such, neurons may actively adjust these timescales to achieve a specific electrical activity aligned with a developmental phase’s activity targets. Indeed, developmental phases are marked by changes in ion channel density and voltage-dependence, leading to distinct electrical activity at each stage (Baccaglini & Spitzer 1977, Gao & Ziskind-Conhaim 1998, Goldberg et al 2011, Hunsberger & Mynlieff 2020, McCormick & Prince 1987, Moody & Bosma 2005, O'Leary et al 2014, Picken Bahrey & Moody 2003).

      Additionally, our results show that activity-dependent regulation of channel voltage-dependence can play a critical role in restoring neuronal activity during perturbations (Figure 5). Specifically, the presence and timing of half-(in)activation modulation influenced whether the model neuron could successfully return to its target activity pattern. Many model neurons only achieved recovery when a half-(in)activation mechanism was present. Moreover, the speed of this modulation shaped recovery outcomes in nuanced ways: some model neurons reached their targets only when voltage-dependence was adjusted rapidly, while others did so only when these changes occurred slowly. These observations all suggest that impairments in a neuron’s ability to modulate the voltage-dependence of its channels may lead to disruptions in activity-dependent homeostasis. This may have implications for conditions such as addiction (Kourrich et al 2015) and Alzheimer’s disease (Styr & Slutsky 2018), where disruptions in homeostatic processes are thought to contribute to pathogenesis.”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Mondal et al. use computational modeling to investigate how activity-dependent shifts in voltage-dependent (in)activation curves can complement activity-dependent changes in ion channel conductance to support homeostatic plasticity. While changes in the voltage-dependent properties of ion channels are known to modulate neuronal excitability, their role as a homeostatic plasticity mechanism interacting with channel conductance has been largely unexplored. The results presented here demonstrate that activity-dependent regulation of voltage-dependent properties can interact with plasticity in channel conductance to allow neurons to attain and maintain target activity patterns, in this case, intrinsic bursting. These results also show that the rate of channel voltage-dependent shifts can influence steady-state parameters reached as the model stabilizes into a stable intrinsic bursting state. That is, the rate of these modifications shapes the range of channel conductances and half-(in)activation parameters as well as activity characteristics such as burst period and duration. A major conclusion of the study is that altering the timescale of channel voltage dependence can seamlessly shift a neuron's activity characteristics, a mechanism that the authors argue may be employed by neurons to adapt to perturbations. While the study's conclusions are mostly well-supported, additional analyses, and simulations are needed.

      (1) A main conclusion of this study is that the speed at which (in)activation dynamics change determines the range of possible electrical patterns. The authors propose that neurons may dynamically regulate the timescale of these changes (a) to achieve alterations in electrical activity patterns, for example, to preserve the relative phase of neuronal firing in a rhythmic network, and (b) to adapt to perturbations. The results presented in Figure 4 clearly demonstrate that the timescale of (in)activation modifications impacts the range of activity patterns generated by the model as it transitions from an initial state of no activity to a final steady-state intrinsic burster. This may have important implications for neuronal development, as discussed by the authors.

      However, the authors also argue that the model neuron's dynamics - such as period, and burst duration, etc - could be dynamically modified by altering the timescale of (in)activation changes (Figure 6 and related text). The simulations presented here, however, do not test whether modifications in this timescale can shift the model's activity features once it reaches steady state. In fact, it is unlikely that this would be the case since, at steady-state, calcium targets are already satisfied. It is likely, however, as the authors suggest, that the rate at which (in)activation dynamics change may be important for neuronal adaptation to perturbations, such as changes in temperature or extracellular potassium. Yet, the results presented here do not examine how modifying this timescale influences the model's response to perturbations. Adding simulations to characterize how alterations in the rate of (in)activation dynamics affect the model's response to perturbations-such as transiently elevated extracellular potassium (Figure 5) - would strengthen this conclusion.

      The reviewer suggests that our core message — namely, that the timescale of half-(in)activation alterations influences the intrinsic properties and activity patterns targeted by a homeostatic mechanism — should also hold during perturbations. We agree that this extension strengthens the central message and have incorporated it into the subsection of the Results (“Half-(in)activation Alterations Contribute to Activity Homeostasis”) and Figure 5.

      (2) Another key argument in this study is that small, coordinated changes in channel (in)activation contribute to shaping neuronal activity patterns, but that, these subtle effects may be obscured when averaging across a population of neurons. This may be the case; however, the results presented don't clearly demonstrate this point. This point would be strengthened by identifying correlations, if they exist, between (in)activation curves, conductance, and the resulting bursting patterns of the models for the simulations presented in Figure 2 and Figure 4, for example. Alternatively, or additionally, relationships between (in)activation curves could be probed by perturbing individual (in)activation curves and quantifying how the other model parameters compensate, which could clearly illustrate this point.

      In part of the Discussion, we noted that small, coordinated shifts in half-(in)activation curves could be obscured when averaging across a population of neurons. Our intention was not to present this as a primary result, but to highlight an emergent consequence of the model: that distinct initial maximal conductances may converge to activity targets via different small shifts in half-(in)activation, making such changes difficult to detect at the population level. However, we did not systematically examine correlations between (in)activation parameters, conductances, and activity features, nor how these correlations might vary with the timescale of (in)activation modulation. While this observation is consistent with model behavior, it does not directly advance the study’s main point — that the timescale of half-(in)activation modulation influences the types of bursting patterns that satisfy the activity target. To keep the focus clear, we have removed this remark from the Discussion, though we agree that a more detailed analysis of these correlations may offer a fruitful direction for future work.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor comments:

      (1) Page 5: remove "an" from "achieve a given an activity..."

      The sentence containing this error has been removed.

      (2) Page 7, bottom of page. Explain what prespecifying means here. This requires a conceptual explanation, even if the equations are given in the methods. Was one working ad hoc model built from which the three sensor values were chosen? What was this model and how was it benchmarked? The sensors are never shown. In any figure, but presumably they have different kinetics. What is meant by "average value"? What was the window of averaging and why?

      The intention of this passage was to provide a broad overview of the homeostatic mechanism, with the rationale for using sensor “averages” as homeostatic targets explained in detail in the Methods. We have replaced the word “average” with “target” to maintain this focus.

      (3) Page 9: add "the" in "electrical activity of the neuron as [the] model seeks...".

      Done

      (4) Page 9: say briefly what alpha is before using it. Also, please be consistent in either using the symbol for alpha or spelling it out across the manuscript and the figures.

      Done

      (5) Page 10: the paragraph "In general, ..." is confusing although it becomes clear later on what this is all about. Please rewrite and expand this to clarify some points. For instance, the word "degenerate" is first used here and it is unclear in what sense these models are degenerate. Then it is unclear why the first 5 models were chosen and then 15 more added. What was the point of doing this? What is the intent? Set this up properly before saying that you just did it. This also would clarify the weird terminology used later on of Group of 20 vs. Group of 5. The 20 and 5 are arbitrary. Say what the purpose is. Finally, is the "mean" at the very end the same 416 ms? If not, what do you mean by "the mean"? In fact, I find these 2% and 20% to be imprecise substitutes of (say) two distinct values of CV which are an order of magnitude different. Is that the intent?

      This comment refers to a passage that was removed during revision.

      (6) Page 10: this may be clear to you, but it took me a while to understand that in Figure 1C, you took the working model at the end of 1A, fixed the gmax values and randomized just the half-act/inact values to run it. Perhaps rewrite this to clarify?

      This comment refers to a figure that was removed during revision.

      (7) Page 13: why do channel densities not change much after the perturbation?

      This comment refers to a figure that has since been reworked during revision. In particular, we only study what happens during perturbation. This question is interesting and is the subject of ongoing work.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The article should be carefully corrected, because the current quality of writing might obscure the interest of the study. Particular attention should be paid to the state-of-the-art section and to the discussion, but even the writing of the results should be carefully reworked. The current state of the article makes it very difficult to understand the motivation behind the study but also what the main result provided by this work is.

      The Introduction, Results, and Discussion have been reworked to build on the central premise of the work: the timescale of half-(in)activation alterations influences the intrinsic properties and activity patterns targeted by the neuron’s homeostatic mechanism. These changes are detailed in Public Comment #1.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The manuscript presents an interesting computational study exploring how activity-dependent regulation of (in)activation dynamics interacts with conductance plasticity to shape neuronal activity patterns. While the study provides valuable insights, some aspects would benefit from clarification, further analyses, and/or additional simulations to strengthen the conclusions. Below, I outline concerns and comments related to specific details of the model and results presentation that were not included in the public review.

      (1) The results presented in Figure 5 show that adaptation occurs in both channel conductances and (in)activation dynamics; however, the changes in conductance remain relatively permanent after the model recovers from the transient elevation in extracellular potassium. It therefore seems likely that the model would recover bursting more quickly in response to a subsequent exposure to simulated elevated extracellular potassium since large modifications in the slowly changing conductances would not be required. If this is the case, it could provide a plausible mechanism for adaptation to repeated high-potassium exposure, as demonstrated experimentally in Cancer borealis by this group (PMID: 36060056).

      This is an astute observation and the subject of our present follow-up investigation.

      (2) In the text relating to Figure 5, it is argued that the resulting shifts in (in)activation curves may be conceptualized as alterations in window currents. It would be helpful to illustrate this by plotting and comparing changes in window currents of these channels alongside the changes in their (in)activation curves.

      This comment refers to a passage that was removed during revision.

      (3) Some discussion of the role these homeostatic mechanisms may play when the neuron is synaptically integrated into a rhythmically active network could be informative. Surely, phasic and tonic inputs to the neuron would alter its conductance and voltage-dependent properties. Therefore, the model's parameters in an intact network could be very different from those in the synaptically isolated case.

      This is an excellent point. We agree that synaptic context—particularly tonic and phasic inputs—would likely influence a neuron’s conductances and voltage-dependent properties, potentially leading to different homeostatic outcomes than in the isolated case. While our current study focuses on synaptically isolated neurons, the Marder lab has considered how homeostatically stabilized neurons might interact in network settings. For example, O'Leary et al (2014) presents an example network of three such neurons operating under homeostatic regulation. However, systematically exploring this question remains a challenge. We are currently developing ideas to study this in the context of a simplified half-center oscillator model, where network-level dynamics can be more tractably analyzed.

      (4) Why are the transitions of alpha typically so abrupt, essentially either 1 or 0? Similarly, what happens in the model when there are transient transitions from what appears to be a steady-state alpha that abruptly shifts from 0 to 1 or 1 to 0? For example, what is occurring in Figure 1A at ~150s and ~180s when alpha jumps between 1 and 0, or in Figure 1B when the model transiently jumps up from 0 to 1 at ~400s and ~830s? In Figure 1A, does the bursting pattern change at all after ~250s, or is it identical to the pattern at c?

      This is addressed in the revision (Lines 141 – 150).

      (5) Are the final steady-state parameters of the 25 (sic) models consistent with experimental observations?

      It is difficult to assess — it is hard to design an experiment to do what the reviewer is suggesting.

      (6) Why isn't gL allowed to change dynamically? This seems like the most straightforward way to allow a neuron to adjust its excitability (aside from tonic synaptic inputs).

      Passive currents could, in principle, be subject to homeostatic regulation. However, our study focused on active intrinsic currents. This focus stems from earlier investigations, which showed that active currents are dynamically regulated during homeostasis – for instance Turrigiano et al (1995) and (Desai et al 1999).

      Alonso LM, Rue MCP, Marder E. 2023. Gating of homeostatic regulation of intrinsic excitability produces cryptic long-term storage of prior perturbations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 120: e2222016120

      Baccaglini PI, Spitzer NC. 1977. Developmental changes in the inward current of the action potential of Rohon-Beard neurones. J Physiol 271: 93-117

      Desai NS, Rutherford LC, Turrigiano GG. 1999. Plasticity in the intrinsic excitability of cortical pyramidal neurons. Nature Neuroscience 2: 515-20

      Gao BX, Ziskind-Conhaim L. 1998. Development of ionic currents underlying changes in action potential waveforms in rat spinal motoneurons. J Neurophysiol 80: 3047-61

      Goldberg EM, Jeong HY, Kruglikov I, Tremblay R, Lazarenko RM, Rudy B. 2011. Rapid developmental maturation of neocortical FS cell intrinsic excitability. Cereb Cortex 21: 666-82

      Hunsberger MS, Mynlieff M. 2020. BK potassium currents contribute differently to action potential waveform and firing rate as rat hippocampal neurons mature in the first postnatal week. J Neurophysiol 124: 703-14

      Kourrich S, Calu DJ, Bonci A. 2015. Intrinsic plasticity: an emerging player in addiction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 16: 173-84

      McCormick DA, Prince DA. 1987. Post-natal development of electrophysiological properties of rat cerebral cortical pyramidal neurones. J Physiol 393: 743-62

      Moody WJ, Bosma MM. 2005. Ion channel development, spontaneous activity, and activity-dependent development in nerve and muscle cells. Physiol Rev 85: 883-941

      O'Leary T, Williams AH, Franci A, Marder E. 2014. Cell types, network homeostasis, and pathological compensation from a biologically plausible ion channel expression model. Neuron 82: 809-21

      Picken Bahrey HL, Moody WJ. 2003. Early development of voltage-gated ion currents and firing properties in neurons of the mouse cerebral cortex. J Neurophysiol 89: 1761-73

      Styr B, Slutsky I. 2018. Imbalance between firing homeostasis and synaptic plasticity drives early-phase Alzheimer’s disease. Nature Neuroscience 21: 463-73

      Turrigiano G, LeMasson G, Marder E. 1995. Selective regulation of current densities underlies spontaneous changes in the activity of cultured neurons. J Neurosci 15: 3640-52

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work starts with the observation that embryo polarization is asynchronous starting at the early 8-cell stage, with early polarizing cells being biased towards producing the trophectoderm (TE) lineage. They further found that reduced CARM1 activity and upregulation of its substrate BAF155 promote early polarization and TE specification, this piece of evidence connects the previous finding that at Carm1 heterogeneity 4-cell stage guide later cell lineages - the higher Carm1-expressing blastomeres are biased towards ICM lineage. Thus, this work provides a link between asymmetries at the 4-cell stage and polarization at the 8-cell stage, providing a cohesive explanation regarding the first lineage allocation in mouse embryos.

      Strengths:

      In addition to what has been put in the summary, the advanced 3D image-based analysis has found that early polarization is associated with a change in cell geometry in blastomeres, regarding the ratio of the long axis to the short axis. This is considered a new observation that has not been identified.

      Weaknesses:

      For the microinjection-based method to overexpression/deletion of proteins, although it has been shown to be effective in the early embryo settings and has been widely used, it may not fully represent the in vivo situation in some cases, compared to other strategies such as the use of knock-in mice. This is a minor weakness; it would be good to include some sentences in the discussion on the potential caveats.

      We thank the reviewer for their insightful summary of our work, and their adjudication on the novelty of our research. We agree with the reviewer that microinjection-based methods, whilst being the standard and widely used in the field, have their weaknesses. In this study, we have primarily used microinjection of previously tested and known constructs which may help mitigate these concerns, and have referenced numerous studies in which these constructs have been used and tested. Nevertheless, the authors are aware of this drawback and have tried to address this previously in other research using novel artificial intelligence techniques (Shen and Lamba et al., 2022 – cited in the manuscript) and this continues to be an active area of investigation for us.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Lamba and colleagues suggest a molecular mechanism to explain cell heterogeneity in cell specification during pre-implantation development. They show that embryo polarization is asynchronous. They propose that reduced CARM1 activity and upregulation of its substrate BAF155 promote early polarization and trophectoderm specification.

      Strengths:

      The authors use appropriate and validated methodology to address their scientific questions. They also report excellent live imaging. Most of the data are accompanied by careful quantifications.

      Weaknesses:

      I think this manuscript requires some more quantification, increased number of embryos in their evaluations and clearly stating the number of embryos evaluated per experiments.

      We thank the reviewer for these thoughtful comments on our work, their kind assessment of the strength of our research, and their notes on the weaknesses. We have replied to their points raised below.

      Here are some points:

      (1) It should be clearly stated in all figure legends and in the text how many cells from how many embryos were analyzed.

      We appreciate this comment to provide detailed quantification for every experiment in the paper and stating the numbers of embryos (if a whole embryo level experiment) or blastomeres used for statistical tests and displayed in the graph.

      (2) I think that the number of embryos sometimes are too low. These are mouse embryos easily accessible and the methods used are well established in this lab, so the authors should make an effort to have at least 10/15 embryos per experiment. For example "In agreement with this, hybridization chain reaction (HCR) RNA fluorescence in situ hybridization of early 8-cell stage embryos revealed that the number of CDX2 mRNA puncta was higher in polarized blastomeres with a PARD6-positive apical domain than in unpolarized blastomeres, for 5 out of 6 embryos with EP cells (Figure 3A, B)".. or the data for Figure 4, we know how many cells but now how many embryos.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment regarding the number of embryos used in the hybridization chain reaction (HCR) experiment. We agree that increasing the number of embryos could, in principle, further add statistical power. However, both first authors have since left the lab to begin their postdoctoral training or joining a company, and it is not feasible for us to generate additional embryos at this stage.

      Importantly, we believe the number of embryos included in the current manuscript is sufficient to support our conclusions, especially when considered in the context of the broader experimental design, the timing of the study, and our ethical commitment to minimizing animal use.

      Notably, the initial HCR experiment targeting Cdx2 mRNA served as a key indication that prompted further investigation of CDX2 at the protein level. These follow-up experiments were conducted with increased numbers of embryos and/or cells and are presented in Figure 3 and the associated supplementary figures (we now have 124 cells (including 23 EP cells) from 16 embryos), thereby strengthening and confirming the conclusion suggested by the HCR data.

      (3) It would be useful to see in Figure 4 an example of asymmetric cell division as done for symmetric cell division in panel 4B. This could really help the reader to understand how the authors assessed this.

      We used live imaging to track cell division patterns. Cells expressing RFP-tagged polarity proteins were observed during division to identify the resulting daughter cells. Immediately after cytokinesis, we assessed the polarity status of each daughter cell. If both daughter cells were polarized, the division was classified as symmetric; if only one was polarized, it was classified as asymmetric.

      Author response image 1.

      8-cell stage embryos expressing Ezrin-RFP (fire colour) was imaged during 8-16 cell stage division. Top panel arrows indicate a symmetric cell division in which polarity domain became partitioned into both daughter cells; bottom panel indicates asymmetric division in which the polarity domain only get inherited to one cell of the two daughter cells.

      (4) Figure 5C there is a big disproportion of the number of EP and LP identified. Could the authors increase the number of embryos quantified and see if they can increase EP numbers?

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and want to clarify an important detail: EP cells are a phenomenon with average cellular frequency of less than 10% as compared to LP cells (the other 90%). Therefore, when investigating natural embryo development without bias or exclusion, there will likely be an imbalance in the number of EP and LP cells as is the case for Figure 5C. In this case, morphological differences and clear statistical significance were seen between the shape of EP and LP cells within the cells quantified and therefore we decided not to expend further mice for this particular experiment – but we agree with the comment that in most cases additional embryos would help strength our conclusions further.

      (5) Could the authors give more details about how they mount the embryos for live imaging? With agarose or another technique? In which dishes? Overlaid with how much medium and oil? This could help other labs that want to replicate the live imaging in their labs. Also, was it a z-stack analysis? If yes, how many um per stack? Ideally, if they also know the laser power used (at least a range) it would be extremely useful.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and have provided additional detail here and in the Methods section. For live imaging our embryos, we used glass-bottom 35 mm dishes. We then fixed a small cut square of nylon mesh (5mm to 1cm width and height) onto this plate in the centre using silicon which was used as a grid (diameter of approximately 150 micrometres) for deposition of embryos. After drying of the silicon (overnight) and washing with water, the grid was overlaid with a drop of 100 microlitres of KSOM and then covered with mineral oil until this KSOM drop was submerged. After incubation under conditions for live imaging, single embryos were deposited in each ‘well’ of the grid before being placed in the microscope, which was equilibrated at the correct temperature and CO2.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We thank the expert reviewers for their careful consideration of our manuscript and the feedback to help us strengthen our work. Please find a response to each reviewer’s comments below. We have included the original text from the reviewer in unbolded text and our response, immediately below, in bold text for clarity. 

      Reviewer #1:

      (1) Appetite is controlled, not regulated; please reword throughout.

      The reviewer raises a valid point that we have misused the word “regulate” in certain instances and “control” would be more accurate term. We have made adjustments throughout the manuscript.

      (2) One minor point that would further strengthen the data is a more distinct analysis of receptors that are characteristic of the different populations of neuronal and non-neuronal cells; this part could be improved. 

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion as we had not directly compared metabolicallyrelevant peptides/receptors between the mouse and rat DVC. We have included a list of selected receptors and neuropeptides expression (see Figure S13) for neuronal cells in mouse and rat. We have included this figure as a new supplement. There are some interesting insights from this data, including the relatively broad expression of Lepr in the rat compared with the mouse and the absence of proglucagon expressing neurons within the rat DVC.  

      Reviewer #2:

      (1) In some of the graphs, the label AP/NTS is used, but DVC would be more appropriate.

      We have reviewed the figures and legends to ensure appropriate use of DVC. We thank the reviewer for bringing this oversight to our attention.  

      (2) Line 124, p7 - Sprague Dawley RATS

      We have changed the text to “Sprague Dawley rats” 

      (3) Line 132, p7 - The phrase "were provided with given access to food" needs grammatical correction.

      We agree the text was poorly written. The sentence has been corrected to: “Wild-type Sprague

      Dawley rats (Charles River) were provided with ad libitum access to food (Purina Lab Diet

      5001) and water in temperature-controlled (22°C) rooms on a 12-hour light-dark cycle with daily health checks.” We have also reviewed the entire manuscript and made additional amendments where necessary.  

      (4) Page 15 - Mention that GFAP is a marker for astrocytes. Additionally, correct the typo "gfrap".

      We have corrected the misspelling of “Gfap” within the text. We appreciate the reviewer’s comment that there is value in communicating to the nonexpert reader that GFAP is a marker for astrocytes, however, as our data and that from other snRNA-Seq studies show that Gfap mRNA only labels a subset of astrocytes, our preference is to refrain from stating this. Our data suggests the sole use of Gfap as an astrocyte marker will not reflect the true astrocyte population.  

      (5) Line 432, p15 - What was the rationale for selecting clusters 23, 26, and 27?

      We chose to perform subclustering on these clusters because they displayed multiple cell identities when surveyed for the 473 marker genes as described in Methods 2.6. In order to separate these, the granularity was increased in them by sub-clustering.

      (6) Line 533, p18 - only 5 out of 34 neurons express GFRAL, which makes the language used a little bit misleading. As per the comment above, I would specify that only a subset (X%) of neurons express GFRAL, and apply the same approach for other markers.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. We agree the text, as written, was an oversimplification. We adjusted the text as recommended: that a subset (~15%) express detectable Gfral mRNA but is likely an underrepresentation due to the challenges in detecting lowly expressed transcripts such as Gfral.  

      (7) Line 547, p18 - This statement appears to refer to rat data specifically, rather than rodent data in general.

      The text has been corrected. 

      (8) Section 3.6 - The discussion on meal-related transcriptional programs in the murine DVC does not mention Figure S10A and B.

      We thank the reviewer for the observation. It is true that we do not discuss this figure. Fig10S is the integration of samples in treeArches, a necessary step to build the hierarchy in python so the learning algorithm uses only genes that are related to identity and not treatment, we obtained the same overlap of samples when we used R to assign identities. This figure demonstrates our integration was successful because it is only considering genes that are not-treatment related to establish identities, those which are expressed by cells regardless of their response to any treatment. For the meal-related analysis, we were interested in the genes that are changed by treatment, and this is why the analysis differed. We have included a sentence in the methods to clarify this point that states: " This sample integration was done to ensure that inter-sample variations were removed for the cell identity steps."

      (9) Page 5, citation 10 - the author cited a clinical trial for glucagon and GLP-1 receptor dual agonist survodutide for "DVC neurons' role in appetite and energy balance stems from their role as therapeutic targets for obesity". A more appropriate citation (such as a review) would be preferable.

      We appreciate the suggestion by the reviewer. We have updated our references to reflect a recent manuscript from the Alhadeff group which demonstrates the DVC acts as the target of GLP1-based therapies. We have also included a review as suggested 10.1038/s42255-02200606-9.

      (10) Line 52, p5 - a citation of obesity is needed, as the current ref only pertains to cancer cachexia.

      We have included a reference for obesity.  

      (11) In the discussion, it would be valuable to elaborate on the potential significance of DVCspecific glial cells (perhaps at the end of the second paragraph?).

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. Our discovery of a DVC-specific astrocyte transcriptional profile was underrepresented within the discussion. We have attempted to expand this discussion on the suspected roles for these DVC-specific astrocytes. Much of this discussion is based on the distinct localization pattern of Gfap mRNA in the DVC (see Image on Allen Brain ISH) which shows dense signal at the boundary of the AP and NTS. As astrocytes have well established roles in maintaining BBB integrity, it is our speculation that this is a major role of these cells. However, functional studies will be critical to assess the roles of these astrocytes in DVC biology.  

      (12) Line 683, p22 - Consider adding PMID: 38987598 which describes the dissociable GLP-1R circuits.

      We appreciate this recommendation – we have included this reference.  

      (13) The authors suggest that a possible explanation for the discrepancy between snRNA-Seq and in situ hybridization data is that Agrp and Hcrt mRNA reads in snRNA-Seq overwhelmingly mapped to non-coding regions. To what extent could this limitation affect other genes included in the current analyzed 10x datasets?

      As shown by Pool and cols. (https://doi.org/10.1038/s41592-023-02003-w) including intronic reads improves sensitivity and more accurately reflects endogenous gene expression. Therefore, including intronic reads is considered more of a strength than a limitation and is now default in platforms such as CellRanger. While including intronic reads for mapping snRNA-Seq data, we would advise corroboration of snRNA-Seq findings with published literature or detection of coding mRNA or protein. In our case, the detection of hypothalamic neuropeptide via snRNA-Seq data could not be verified by performing in situ hybridizations using probes that detect exons.  Therefore, Hcrt and Agrp having only intronic reads suggest a regulatory (reviewed in https://doi.org/10.3389/fgene.2018.00672) rather than a coding role in the DVC.

      (14) Given the manuscript's focus on feeding and metabolism, I believe a more detailed description and comparison of the transcription profile of known receptors, neurotransmitters, and neuropeptides involved in food intake and energy homeostasis between mice and rats would add value. Adding a curated list of key genes related to feeding regulation would be particularly informative.

      A similar request was made by reviewer #1. Please see the full response above. Briefly, we have performed additional analysis of the mouse and rat DVC data and included this data as an additional supplemental figure (Figure S13).  

      (15) Line 479-482, p17 - It would be helpful if the authors could quantify (e.g., number and/or percentage) the extent of TH and CCK co-expression.

      We have amended the text of the manuscript to include quantification of Cck and Th colocalization.  According to our snRNA-seq data, out of the 764 Th-expressing neurons, 80 coexpress Cck in the mouse (~10%). The Cck-expressing cells are more numerous, 3,821 in total.  

      (16) The number of animals used differs significantly between species, which the authors acknowledge as a limitation in the discussion. Since the authors took advantage of previously published mouse data sets (Ludwig and Dowsett data sets), I wonder if the authors could compare/integrate any rat data set currently available in rats as well to partially address the sample size disparity.

      We agree with the review that our rat database is considerably smaller than our mouse database, making comparisons between rat and mouse DVC challenging. We attempted to increase the size of our rat DVC atlas by incorporating publicly available rat DVC snRNA-Seq data (Reiner et al 2022). However, we found several issues with the quality of this data including low UMIs/cell and gene #/cell. For these reasons, we decided against merging these two datasets. So while relatively small, our rat DVC atlas uses high quality data and serves as a valuable starting point. By introducing TreeArches as a method to relatively easily incorporate new snRNA-Seq data into our own, it is our hope that future studies will do so and thus expand the rat DVC atlas we have built.    

      (17) In the Materials and Methods section, LiCl is mentioned as one of the treatment conditions; however, very little corresponding data are presented or discussed. Please include these results and elaborate on the rationale for selecting LiCl over other anorectic compounds.

      The reviewer is correct, some of the tissues used in this study were from animals treated with LiCl prior to euthanasia. Our intent was to contrast the transcriptional effects induced by LiCl ( an anorectic agent with aversive properties) with refeeding (a naturally rewarding and satiating stimuli). However, upon analyzing the data, we found very few transcriptional changes induced by LiCl. It is unclear to us whether this was a technical failure in the experiment and so did not elaborate on the results.  

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) The use of both sexes is indicated in the discussion, but methods and results do not address sex distribution in the investigated groups. Also, the groups could be more clearly described, e.g., the size of the 2 hour refeeding mouse group varies from n=10 to n=5.

      We have clarified the text, in line with the reviewer’s suggestion. There were two cohorts of fasted/ refed mice (n=5 each), so in the manuscript methods it is stated as n=10 because of this. The fasted-only group, which was not refed before euthanasia is a separate group, n=5.

      (2) Page 20, the last sentence needs to be reworded.

      We thank the reviewer for this recommendation. The text has been amended to improve clarity of the sentence. 

      (3) Page 22, lines 691-692 - this sentence needs to be reworded.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. The offending sentences have been amended.  

      (4) While the authors find transcriptional changes in all neuronal and non-neuronal cell types, which is interesting, the verification of known transcriptional changes (e.g., cFos) is unaddressed. cFos is a common gene upregulated with refeeding that was surprisingly not investigated, even though this should be a strong maker of proper meal-induced neuronal activation in the DMV. This is a missed opportunity either to verify the data set or to highlight important limitations if that had been attempted without success.

      This is a highly salient point made by the reviewer. Including Fos expression serves as an internal validation of our refeeding condition and the absence of Fos mRNA levels from the original manuscript was an oversight on our part. As shown in our volcano plot, between ad libitum fed and refed mice, there are two significantly Fos-associated genes upregulated in the refed group. Therefore, we are confident that the snRNA-Seq analysis accurately captured rapid changes in response to refeeding in the DVC. Only genes differentially expressed (log2 Fold-change >0.5 per group) were considered in the analysis. NS= non-significant.

      Author response image 1.

      (5) The focus on transmitter classification is highlighted, but surprisingly, the well-accepted distinction of GABAergic neurons by Slc32a1 was not used, instead, Gad1 and Gad2 were used as GABAergic markers. While this may be proper for the DMV, given numerous findings that Gad1/2 are not proper markers for GABAergic neurons and often co-expressed in glutamatergic populations, this confound should have been addressed to make a case if and why they would be proper markers in the DMV.

      The reviewer raises an important point. Indeed, there are discrepancies in expression between the Gad1/2 genes and Slc32a1 gene in other data sets. To analyze this within our data set, we examined the mainly GABAergic magnaclass 1 (see Slc32a1 UMAP plot below).  In magnaclass 1, only 5% and 3% of all neurons exclusively express solely Slc32a1 without either Gad1 or Gad2, respectively. In line with the reviewer’s comment, we found that 54% of neurons express either Gad1 or Gad2 but had no detectable Slc32a1. While our failure to detect more cells that co-express Slc32a1 and Gad genes may be partially due to the low expression of Slc32a1, it is also very likely that the DVC, like other brain regions, contains neurons that express the Gad enzymes without co-expression of Slc32a1.  

      This was very much the case with the GLP1 cell cluster, which we identified as the population which had the highest co-expression of excitatory and inhibitory markers. When we refined this analysis to look at expression of excitatory markers with Slc32a1 (and not other inhibitory genes), there was a marked reduction in the proportion of GLP1 neurons meeting this criterion. We find this is mainly due to the GLP1 cells expressing Gad2 (see plots below). We still find that there are some GLP1-expressing neurons that express excitatory markers and Slc32a1 and that the GLP1 neurons have a higher proportion of these co-expressing cells than other cell types.  

      We have extended our results section to reflect this and thank the reviewer for recommending this analysis.  

      Author response image 2.

      Slc32a1 expression across all neurons.  

      Author response image 3.

      Proportion of neurons in all cell identities expressing glutamatergic markers alone (dark green), Slc32a1 alone (light green), both glutamatergic markers and Slc32a1 (purple) or expressing neither Slc32a1 or glutamatergic markers  (grey).  

      Author response image 4.

      Balloon plot of Slc32a1, Gad1 and Gad2 across cell types. The GLP1-expressing neurons express Gad2 but minimal Slc32a1.  

      (6) The Pdgfra IHC as verification is great, but images are not very convincing in distinguishing the 2 (mouse) or 3 (rat) classes of cells. Why not compare Pdgfra and HuC/D co-localization by IHC and snRNAseq data (using the genes for HuC/D) in the mouse and in the rat? That would also clarify how specific HuC/D is for DMV neurons, or if it may also be expressed in non-neuronal populations.

      In agreement with the suggestion by the reviewer, we reanalyzed the snRNA-Seq data to identify the extent of the co-expression of HuC/HuD (i.e. Elavl3 and Elavl4 genes, respectively) in Pdgfra-expressing neurons. The gene expression of the 34 rat neurons belonging to this group are shown in the following heatmap in which each column represents one neuron. As shown, most neurons co-express Pdgfra and either HuC or HuD gene. In addition, we shown the UMAP plots of the rat neurons showing expression of the same genes regardless of the neuronal identity assigned. The Pdgfra neurons are visible in darker blue in the last UMAP plot. It's important to note that HuD is a more specific neuronal marker as shown in the table with the average expression of Elavl3/4 genes, since HuC is expressed by glial cells, specially OPCs and oligodendrocytes. As the HUC/D antibody detects both proteins, this complicates the interpretation of the immunofluorescent staining. While, the snRNA-Seq data suggests these Pdgfra expressing cells are indeed neurons (albeit a rare population), we aim to confirm this in separate studies.  

      Author response image 5.

      Author response image 6.

      Average expression (log-normalized counts) of HuC/D by layer 1 cell identity in the rat cells:

      Author response table 1.

      (7) The importance of sub-clustering for clusters 23, 26, and 27 is not immediately clear. Does this have any relevance to the mouse vs. rat data? Or fed, fast, refeeding data sets? Or is it just to show the depth that can be achieved?

      We appreciate that our justification was not clear within the manuscript. We have clarified our rationale below but briefly, in each case distinct transcriptional profiles were observed, and we pursued this by performing sub-clustering.   

      Cluster 23 was subclustered as it was found to contain both pre-myelinating and a subset of myelinating oligodendrocytes, therefore, to label them effectively in R instead of cell by cell, those subclusters showing pre-myelinating oligodendrocyte markers were instructed to be labeled as such in the dataset. The remaining cells were labeled as mature oligodendrocytes.

      A similar approach was taken for cluster 27 which contained pericytes, endothelial and smooth muscle cells (Figure S5).

      In the case of cluster 26, it was possible to find two subclusters of fibroblasts when mapping markers, so they were sub-clustered to instruct in R to label a group with one identity and the other, with the other identity. Therefore, the sub-clustering was done as an aid to label the different identities found through markers mapping (Table S5) in the first clustering round.

      All labels were transferred from mouse to rat data using treeArches, including those resulting from the sub-clustering of these clusters. Because this was done to establish identity, it should not be relevant for treatment analyses (e.g. fasted, refed) since they are built from markers that don't change by conditions but remain as identity markers. Indeed, our dataset has an even distribution of these subclusters among samples.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This work focuses on the connection strength of the corticostriatal projections, without considering the involvement of synaptic plasticity in sensory integration.

      Thank you for raising this point. Indeed, sensory integration is a complex process with a multitude of factors beyond connectivity patterns and synaptic strength. In addition, it is true that both connectivity levels and synaptic strength can be modified by plasticity. 

      We modified our conclusion as follows, line 354: 

      “Since the inputs to a single SPN represent only a limited subset of whisker columns, a complete representation of whiskers could emerge at the population level, with each SPN’s representation complementing those of its neighbors (Fig. 7). These observations raise the hypothesis of a selective or competitive process underlying the formation of corticostriatal synapses. The degree of input convergence onto SPNs could be modulated by plasticity, potentially enabling experience-driven reconfiguration of S1 corticostriatal coupling. “

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      A few minor changes to the figures and text could be made to improve clarity.

      We thank you for having taken the time to indicate where changes could benefit the paper. We followed your recommendations. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      (1) Several factors may contribute to an underestimation of barrel cortex inputs to SPNs (and thus an overestimate of the input heterogeneity among SPNs). First, by virtue of the experiments being performed in an acute slice prep, it is probable that portions of recorded SPN dendritic trees have been dissected (in an operationally consistent anatomical orientation). If afferents happen to systematically target the rostral/caudal projections of SPN dendritic fields, these inputs could be missed. Similarly, the dendritic locations of presynaptic cortical inputs remain unknown (e.g., do some inputs preferentially target distal vs proximal dendritic positions?). As synaptic connectivity was inferred from somatic recordings, it's likely that inputs targeting the proximal dendritic arbor are the ones most efficiently detected. Mapping the dendritic organization of synapses is beyond the scope of this work, but these points could be broached in the text.

      Thank you for this analysis. The positions of S1 spines have been mapped on the SPN dendritic arbor by the group of Margolis (B.D. Sanabria et al., ENeuro 2024,10.1523/ENEURO.0503-23.2023). They observed that S1 spines were at 80 % on dendrites but with a specific distribution, on average rather close to the soma.  In this study, S1 spines did not exhibit a specific distribution that would systematically hinder their detection in a slice. But, it remains that the position in the dendritic arbor where an S1 input is received does indeed impact its detection in somatic recordings. We modified the discussion as follows, line 275:

      “The LSPS combined with glutamate uncaging mapped projections contained in the slice, intact from the presynaptic cell bodies to the SPN dendrites. Some cortical inputs targeting distal SPN dendrites may have gone undetected, either due to attenuation of synaptic events recorded at the soma or because distal dendritic branches were lost during slice preparation. Indeed, about 80 % of S1 synaptic contacts are distributed along dendrites (Sanabria et al., 2024). However, synapses located distally are proportionally rare (Sanabria et al., 2024), and our estimates suggest that the loss of S1 input was minimal (see Methods). More significantly, our mapping only included projections from neuronal somata located within the S1 barrel field in the slice: projections from cortical columns outside the slice were not stimulated. For this reason, our study characterized connectivity patterns rather than the full extent of connectivity with the barrel cortex.”

      We explain our estimation of truncated S1 contacts in the Methods, line 434:

      “To estimate the loss of S1 synaptic contacts caused by slice preparation, we modeled the SPN dendritic field as a sphere centered on the soma. S1 synapses were at 80 % distributed radially along dendrites, according to the specific distribution described by Sanabria et al. (2024). The simulation also incorporated the known distribution of SPN dendritic length as a function of distance from the soma (Gertler et al., 2008). Finally, it assumed that synapse placement was isotropic, with equal probability in all directions from the soma. Truncation was simulated by removing a spherical cap at one pole of the sphere, reflecting the depth of our recordings (beyond 80 μm). Based on this simulation, the loss of S1 inputs was < 10 %.”

      (2) In general, how specific (or generalizable) is the observed SPN-specific convergence of cortical barrel cortex projections in the dorsolateral striatum? In other words, does a similar cortical stimulation protocol targeted to a non-barrel sensory (or motor) cortex region produce similar SPN-specific innervation patterns in the dorsolateral striatum?

      This is an interesting question that could be addressed using the LSPS approach in areas for which ex vivo preparations have been designed to maintain the integrity of the corticostriatal projections, such as A1, M1 and S2.  

      We included this point in the discussion, line 299: 

      ” The speckled connectivity pattern of individual SPNs, arising from the abundant and diffuse cortical innervation in the DLS, suggests that somatosensory corticostriatal synapses are established through a selective and/or competitive process. It is important to determine whether this sparse innervation of SPNs by S1 is a characteristic shared with other projections. In particular, it will be interesting to test this hypothesis on the auditory projections targeting the posterior striatum, where neurons exhibit clear tone frequency selectivity (Guo et al., 2018).”

      (3) In general, some of the figure legends are extremely brief, making many details difficult to infer. Similarly, some statistical analyses were either not carried out or not consistently reported.

      We thank you for having taken the time to indicate where changes could benefit the paper. We have followed your recommendations. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      A few limitations should be discussed in the manuscript:

      (1) The manuscript should mention that most corticostriatal synapses are formed at the dendritic spines of the SPNs, not their cell bodies. This is particularly important regarding the analysis and interpretation of the data in Figure 4.

      Thank you for this comment. This characteristic is important with regards to a limitation of electrophysiological recordings. This is now discussed:

      Line 275:

      “The LSPS combined with glutamate uncaging mapped projections contained in the slice, intact from the presynaptic cell bodies to the SPN dendrites. Some cortical inputs targeting distal SPN dendrites may have gone undetected, either due to attenuation of synaptic events recorded at the soma or because distal dendritic branches were lost during slice preparation. Indeed, about 80 % of S1 synaptic contacts are distributed along dendrites (Sanabria et al., 2024). However, synapses located distally are proportionally rare (Sanabria et al., 2024), and our estimates suggest that the loss of S1 input was minimal (see Methods).“

      Line 313:

      [...],, we found that overlaps between the connectivity maps of SPNs were rare and, when present, involved only a small fraction of the connected sites. This indicates that neighboring SPNs predominantly integrated distinct inputs from the barrel cortex, although it is possible that overlapping inputs received in distal dendrites were not all detected”

      (1) SPNs show up- and down-states in vivo, which were not mimicked by the present study since all cells were held at - 80 mV (Line 364) and recorded at room temperature (Line 368). It should be discussed how the conclusion of the present work may be affected by the up/down states of SPNs in vivo.

      Thank you for raising this point. Indeed, our experimental conditions were not designed to capture the effects of network oscillatory activity. Instead, LSPS conditions were optimized to reveal monosynaptic connectivity between neurons in S1 and their postsynaptic targets. These optimizations include the use of a high concentration of extracellular divalents (4 mM Ca<sup>2+</sup> and Mg<sup>2+</sup>) to generate robust yet moderate and spatially-restricted stimulations of cortical cells and reliable neurotransmitter release (Shepherd, Pologruto and Svoboda, Neuron 2003; 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00152-1; in our study, see Fig. 1D  and Suppl Fig. 2). Investigating the pre- and postsynaptic modulations of the corticostriatal coupling by up- and down-states would require specific conditions. 

      The conclusion now acknowledges that functional connectivity is subject to plasticity in general, line 358:

      “The degree of input convergence onto SPNs could be modulated by plasticity, potentially enabling experience-driven reconfiguration of S1 corticostriatal coupling.”

      (2) In addition to population-level integration (Line 337), sensory integration is likely to involve synaptic plasticity (like via NMDARs), which was not studied in the present work

      Thank you for raising this point. Indeed, we agree that sensory integration is a complex process with a multitude of factors beyond connectivity patterns and synaptic strength. We also agree that both connectivity levels and synaptic strength can be modified by plasticity. 

      We modified our conclusion as follows, line 354:

      “Since the inputs to a single SPN represent only a limited subset of whisker columns, a complete representation of whiskers could emerge at the population level, with each SPN’s representation complementing those of its neighbors (Fig. 7). These observations raise the hypothesis of a selective or competitive process underlying the formation of corticostriatal synapses. The degree of input convergence onto SPNs could be modulated by plasticity, potentially enabling experience-driven reconfiguration of S1 corticostriatal coupling. “

      (3) The potential corticostriatal connectivity may be underestimated due to loss of axonal branches during slice resection, and this might contribute to the conclusion of "sparse connectivity". Whether the author has considered performing LSPS studies within the striatum (i.e., stimulating ChR2-expressing cortical axon terminals) and whether this experiment may consolidate the conclusion of the present work.

      We appreciate the suggestion to employ Subcellular Channelrhodopsin-2-Assisted Circuit Mapping (sCRACM) to study the density of S1 spines on SPNs dendritic arbor. If ChR2 is broadly expressed in S1, this approach would likely increase spine detection, as spines contacted by presynaptic neurons located inside and outside the slice would now be activated. If ChR2 expression could be restricted to the whisker columns present in our preparation, enhanced detection could still occur, but in this case, it would reflect the activation of spines contacted by specific ChR2<sup>+</sup> axonal branches that exit and re-enter the slice to form synapses on the recorded SPN. The anatomy of corticostriatal axonal arbors suggest convoluted axonal trajectories could be relatively rare (T. Zheng and C.J. Wilson, J Neurophysiol. 2001; 10.1152/jn.00519.2001; M. Lévesque et al., Brain Res. 1996; 10.1016/0006-8993(95)01333-4).  

      Moreover, it is important to remember that sCRACM does not generate connectivity maps between 2 structures, but maps of spines on dendritic arbors (Petreanu L.T. et al., Nature 2009; 10.1038/nature07709.). Precise localization of presynaptic cell bodies was key for the present study, as it enabled distinguishing between different connectivity patterns and between different degrees of convergence of inputs from adjacent S1 cortical columns present in the slice (schematized in Fig. 1). Distinguishing these inputs using the stimulation of axon terminals would require the possibility to express one distinct opsin in each whisker column (or each cortical layer, depending on the axis of investigation). This is an exciting perspective but the technology is not yet available to our knowledge. 

      To emphasize our reasons for using LSPS, we revised the final paragraph of the Introduction, line 69: 

      “LSPS enabled precise mapping of corticostriatal functional connectivity by identifying cortical sites where stimulation evoked synaptic currents in the recorded SPNs, thereby localizing the cell bodies of their presynaptic neurons. This approach allowed us to determine both the cortical column and layer of origin within the barrel field in the slice for each SPN input.”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1)  Figure 2F: SPN and cortical regions - both are shown in green. The distinction between the two would be clearer if SPNs were made a different color.

      Done

      (2)  Figure 2H: Based on their data, the authors conclude that since EPSCs in SPNs had small amplitudes (~40pA), only one or a few presynaptic cortical neurons (< 5) were activated by uncaging. It is not clear how this number was estimated. Either this statement should be qualified with data or citations provided to support it.

      We thank you for noticing it. We modified this part as follows, line 105:

      “Based on known amplitudes of spontaneous and miniature EPSCs in SPNs (10-20 pA on average; Kreitzer and Malenka, 2007; Cepeda et al., 2008; Dehorter et al., 2011; Peixoto et al., 2016), this finding is consistent with the presence of only one or a few presynaptic cells (≤ 5) at each connected site of the map.”

      (3) Figure 2I: The top graph is difficult to understand without already seeing the lower plot. Moving it below or to the side would help the reader follow the data more easily.

      done

      (4) Figure 3D: In Line 162, the authors state, " Furthermore, SPNs receiving input from a single column were often located near others receiving input from multiple ones (Figure 3D), reinforcing that the low functional connectivity with barrel columns in the slice was genuine in these cases." However, Figure 3D does not show spatial information about SPNs relative to each other. This data should be added or the statement adjusted to reflect what is shown in the panel.

      Corrected as follows, line 167:

      “Furthermore, SPNs receiving input from a single column were often located in slices where other cells received input from multiple ones (Fig. 3D), reinforcing that the low functional connectivity with barrel columns in the slice was genuine in these cases.”

      (5) Figure 3F: Are the authors attempting to show how cluster number, cluster width, and connectivity gaps contribute to input field width? If so, this could be clarified by flipping the x- and y-axes so that the input field width is the y-axis in each case. Additionally, the difference between black and white points should be stated (or, if there is no difference, made to be the same). The significance of the dotted red line vs. the solid red lines should also be stated in the figure legend.

      These plots illustrate how cluster number, cluster width, and ratio of connectivity gaps over total length vary as a function of input field width. As expected, wider input fields contain more clusters (top). However, the overall density of connected sites does not increase with input field width, as indicated by a higher ratio of connectivity gaps over total length (bottom).

      This suggests the presence of a mechanism that regulates the connectivity level of individual SPNs (mentioned in the discussion). We prefer this orientation because the flipped one makes a cluttered panel due to different X axis labels. Symbols and lines were corrected. The correlation coefficients and statistics are now indicated in the panels and in the legend.

      (6) Figure 3H: The schematic is very useful for highlighting the core conclusions and is greatly appreciated. The pie charts are a bit hard to see and could be replaced with the percentages stated simply as text within the figure. It would also help to label the panel as "Summary," so readers can quickly identify its purpose.

      Done

      (7) Figures 4B-D: To clarify the overall percentage, the maximum for the y-axis should be set to 100% in each panel.

      Done

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Though mostly minor, several sentences/statements in the manuscript are confusing or overstated. For example:

      a. Lines 62-63: "Studies have found that inputs received by D1 SPNs were stronger than those received by D2 SPNs" is a broad statement that should be qualified.

      We changed this sentence for: 

      “Electrophysiological studies have found that inputs received by D1 SPNs were stronger than those received by D2 SPNs, both in vivo and ex vivo (Reig and Silberberg, 2014 ; Filipović et al., 2019 ; Kress et al., 2013 ; Parker et al., 2016).”

      b. Lines 118-119: "EPSCs evoked with stimulations in L2/3 to L5b had similar amplitudes (Figure 2H), suggesting that L5a dominated these other layers thanks to a greater connectivity with SPNs principally." Here, the word "connectivity" is vague and could easily be misunderstood. Connectivity could refer to the amplitude of corticostriatal EPSCs, which the authors stated are not different between L2/3-L5b. Presumably, connectivity here refers to % of connected SPNs, but for the sake of clarity, the authors should be more explicit, e.g,. "...L5a dominated the other layers because a larger fraction of SPNs received connections from L5a, rather than because L5a synapses were stronger."

      We changed the sentence for (line 122): 

      “EPSCs evoked with stimulations in L2/3 to L5b had similar amplitudes (Fig. 2H), suggesting that L5a dominance over these other layers is primarily due to a higher likelihood of SPNs being connected to it, rather than to stronger synaptic inputs.”

      c. In the Figure 4 legend, (A) says "Four example slices with 2 to 4 recordings. Same as in Figure 2A." Did the authors mean Figure 3A?

      Done

      d.Line 184: Should Figure 4B, C actually be Figure 4D?

      Done

      (2) Line 32: typo in Sippy et al. reference.

      Done

      (3) In Figure 2I, the label "dSPN" is confusing, as in the literature, dSPN often refers to the direct pathway SPN.

      Done

      (4) The y-axes in Figure 3C should be better labeled/explained.

      Fig.3C. Median (red) and 25-75th percentiles (box) of cluster width and spacing, expressed in µm (left Y axis) and number of cortical columns (right Y axis). Labels have been changed in the figure.

      (5)  Lines 150-152: "...45 % of the input fields with several clusters produced no synaptic response upon stimulation." This wording is confusing. It can be inferred that the authors mean "no synaptic response in the gaps between clusters." However, their phrasing omits this crucial detail and reads as though those input fields produce no response at all.

      We changed this sentence for (line 154):

      “Strikingly, regions lacking evoked synaptic responses (i.e., connectivity gaps) made up an average of 45 % of the length of input fields with multiple clusters (maps collapsed along the vertical axis; Fig. 3F, bottom). “

      (6)  Lines 184-186: "DLS SPNs could receive inputs from the same domain in the barrel cortex and yet have patterns of cortical innervation without or little redundancy." This should be rephrased to "with little to no redundancy."

      Done

      (7)  Lines 186-187: "They support a connectivity model in which synaptic connections on each SPNs..." should be revised to "connections to each SPN...".

      Done

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review)

      As this code was developed for use with a 4096 electrode array, it is important to be aware of double-counting neurons across the many electrodes. I understand that there are ways within the code to ensure that this does not happen, but care must be taken in two key areas. Firstly, action potentials traveling down axons will exhibit a triphasic waveform that is different from the biphasic waveform that appears near the cell body, but these two signals will still be from the same neuron (for example, see Litke et al., 2004 "What does the eye tell the brain: Development of a System for the Large-Scale Recording of Retinal Output Activity"; figure 14). I did not see anything that would directly address this situation, so it might be something for you to consider in updated versions of the code.

      Thank you for this comment. We have added a routine to the SpikeMAP to remove highly correlated spikes detected within a given spatial radius of each other. The following was added to the main text (line 149):

      “As an additional verification step, SpikeMAP allows the computation of spike-count correlations between putative neurons located within a user-defined radius. Signals that exceed a defined threshold of correlation can be rejected as they likely reflect the same underlying cell.”

      Secondly, spike shapes are known to change when firing rates are high, like in bursting neurons (Harris, K.D., Hirase, H., Leinekugel, X., Henze, D.A. & Buzsáki, G. Temporal interaction between single spikes and complex spike bursts in hippocampal pyramidal cells. Neuron 32, 141-149 (2001)). I did not see this addressed in the present version of the manuscript.

      We have added a routine to SpikeMAP that computes population spike rates to verify stationarity over time. We have also added a routine to identify putative bursting neurons through a Hartigan statistical dip test applied to the inter-spike distribution of individual cells.

      We added the following (line 204):

      “Further, SpikeMAP contains a routine to perform a Hartigan statistical dip test on the inter-spike distribution of individual cells to detect putative bursting neurons.”

      Another area for possible improvement would be to build on the excellent validation experiments you have already conducted with parvalbumin interneurons. Although it would take more work, similar experiments could be conducted for somatostatin and vasoactive intestinal peptide neurons against a background of excitatory neurons. These may have different spike profiles, but your success in distinguishing them can only be known if you validate against ground truth, like you did for the PV interneurons.

      We have added the following (line 326):

      “future work could include different inhibitory interneurons such as somatostatin (SOM) and vasoactive intestinal polypeptide (VIP) neurons to improve the classification of inhibitory cell types. Another avenue could involve applying SpikeMAP on artificially generated spike data (Buccino & Einevoll 2021; Laquitaine et al., 2024).”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review)

      Summary:

      While I find that the paper is nicely written and easy to follow, I find that the algorithmic part of the paper is not really new and should have been more carefully compared to existing solutions. While the GT recordings to assess the possibilities of a spike sorting tool to distinguish properly between excitatory and inhibitory neurons are interesting, spikeMAP does not seem to bring anything new to state-of-the-art solutions, and/or, at least, it would deserve to be properly benchmarked. I would suggest that the authors perform a more intensive comparison with existing spike sorters.

      Thank you for your insightful comment. A full comparison between SpikeMAP and related methods is provided in Table. 1. As can be seen, SpikeMAP is the only method listed that performs E/I sorting on large-scale multielectrodes. Nonetheless, several aspects of SpikeMAP included in the spike sorting pipeline do overlap with existing methods, as these constitute necessary steps prior to performing E/I identification. These steps are not novel to the current work, nor do they constitute rigid options that cannot be substituted by the user. Rather, we aim to offer SpikeMAP users the option to combine E/I identification with preliminary steps performed either through our software or through another package of their choosing. For instance, preliminary spike sorting could be done through Kilosort before importing the spike data into SpikeMAP for E/I identification. To allow greater flexibility, we have now modularized our suite so that E/I identification can be performed as a stand-alone module. We have clarified the text accordingly (line 317):

      “While SpikeMAP is the only known method to enable the identification of putative excitatory and inhibitory neurons on high-density multielectrode arrays (Table 1), several aspects of SpikeMAP included in the spike sorting pipeline (Figure 1) overlap with existing methods, as these constitute required steps prior to performing E/I identification. To enable users the ability to integrate SpikeMAP with existing toolboxes, we provide a modularized suite of protocols so that E/I identification can be performed separately from preliminary spike sorting steps. In this way, a user could carry out spike sorting through Kilosort or another package before importing their data to SpikeMAP for E/I identification.”

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The global workflow of spikeMAP, described in Figure 1, seems to be very similar to that of Hilgen et al. 2020 (10.1016/j.celrep.2017.02.038). Therefore, the first question is what is the rationale of reinventing the wheel, and not using tools that are doing something very similar (as mentioned by the authors themselves). I have a hard time, in general, believing that spikeMAP has something particularly special, given its Methods, compared to state-of-the-art spike sorters.

      The paper by Hilgen et al. is reported in Table 1. As seen, while this paper employs optogenetics, it does not target inhibitory (e.g., PV) cells. We have added the following clarification (line 82):

      “Despite evidence showing differences in action potential kinetics for distinct cell-types as well as the use of optogenetics (Hilgen et al., 2017), there exists no large-scale validation efforts, to our knowledge, showing that extracellular waveforms can be used to reliably distinguish cell-types.”

      This is why, at the very least, the title of the paper is misleading, because it lets the reader think that the core of the paper will be about a new spike sorting pipeline. If this is the main message the authors want to convey, then I think that numerous validations/benchmarks are missing to assess first how good spikeMAP is, with reference to spike sorting in general, before deciding if this is indeed the right tool to discriminate excitatory vs inhibitory cells. The GT validation, while interesting, is not enough to entirely validate the paper. The details are a bit too scarce for me, or would deserve to be better explained (see other comments after).

      We thank the reviewer for this comment, and have amended the title as follows:

      “SpikeMAP: An unsupervised pipeline for the identification of cortical excitatory and inhibitory neurons in high-density multielectrode arrays with ground-truth validation”

      (2) Regarding the putative location of the spikes, it has been shown that the center of mass, while easy to compute, is not the most accurate solution [Scopin et al, 2024, 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2024.110297]. For example, it has an intrinsic bias for finding positions within the boundaries of the electrodes, while some other methods, such as monopolar triangulation or grid-based convolution,n might have better performances. Can the authors comment on the choice of the Center of Mass as a unique way to triangulate the sources?

      We agree with the reviewer that the center-of-mass algorithm carries limitations that are addressed by other methods. To address this issue, we have included two additional protocols in SpikeMAP to perform monopolar triangulation and grid-based convolution, offering additional options for users of the package. The text has been clarified as follows (line 429):

      “In addition to center-of-mass triangulation, SpikeMAP includes protocols to perform monopolar triangulation and grid-based convolution, offering additional options to estimate putative soma locations based on waveform amplitudes.”

      (3) Still in Figure 1, I am not sure I really see the point of Spline Interpolation. I see the point of such a smoothing, but the authors should demonstrate that it has a key impact on the distinction of Excitatory vs. Inhibitory cells. What is special about the value of 90kHz for a signal recorded at 18kHz? What is the gain with spline enhancement compared to without? Does such a value depend on the sampling rate, or is it a global optimum found by the authors?

      We clarified the text as follows (line 183):

      “While we found that a resolution of 90 kHZ provided a reasonable estimate of spike waveforms, this value can be adjusted as a parameter in SpikeMAP.”

      (4) Figure 2 is not really clear, especially panel B. The choice of the time scale for the B panel might not be the most appropriate, and the legend filtered/unfiltered with a dot is not clear to me in Bii.

      We apologize for the rendering issues in the Figures that occurred during conversion into PDF format. We have now ensured that all figures are properly displayed.

      In panel E, the authors are making two clusters with PCA projections on single waveforms. Does this mean that the PCA is only applied to the main waveforms, i.e. the ones obtained where the amplitudes are peaking the most? This is not really clear from the methods, but if this is the case, then this approach is a bit simplistic and does not really match state-of-the-art solutions. Spike waveforms are quite often, especially with such high-density arrays, covering multiple channels at once, and thus the extracellular patterns triggered by the single units on the MEA are spatio-temporal motifs occurring on several channels. This is why, in modern spike sorters, the information in a local neighbourhood is often kept to be projected, via PCA, on the lower-dimensional space before clustering. Information on a single channel only might not be informative enough to disambiguate sources. Can the authors comment on that, and what is the exact spatial resolution of the 3Brain device? The way the authors are performing the SVD should be clarified in the methods section. Is it on a single channel, and/or on multiple channels in a local neighbourhood?

      We agree with the reviewer that it would be useful to have the option of performing PCA on several channels at once, since spikes can occur at several channels at the same time. We have now added a routine to SpikeMAP that allows users to define a radius around individual channels prior to performing PCA. The text was clarified as follows (line 131):

      “The SpikeMAP suite also offers a routine to select a radius around individual channels in order to enter groups of adjacent channels in PCA.”

      (5) About the isolation of the single units, here again, I think the manuscript lacks some technical details. The authors are saying that they are using a k-means cluster analysis with k=2. This means that the authors are explicitly looking for 2 clusters per electrode? If so, this is a really strong assumption that should not be held in the context of spike sorting, because, since it is a blind source separation technique, one can not pre-determine in advance how many sources are present in the vicinity of a given electrode. While the illustration in Figure 2E is ok, there is no guarantee that one can not find more clusters, so why this choice of k=2? Again, this is why most modern spike sorting pipelines do not rely on k-means, to avoid any hard-coded number of clusters. Can the authors comment on that?

      We clarified the text as follows (line 135):

      “In SpikeMAP, the optimal number of k-means clusters can be chosen by a Calinski-Harabasz criterion (Calinski and Harabasz, 1974) or pre-selected by the user.”

      (6) I'm surprised by the linear decay of the maximal amplitude as a function of the distance from the soma, as shown in Figure 2H. Is it really what should be expected? Based on the properties of the extracellular media, shouldn't we expect a power law for the decay of the amplitude? This is strange that up to 100um away from the soma, the max amplitude only dropped from 260 to 240 uV. Can the authors comment on that? It would be interesting to plot that for all neurons recorded, in a normed manner V/max(V) as function of distances, to see what the curve looks like.

      We added Supplemental Figure 1 showing the drop in voltage over all putative somas (N=1,950) of one recording, after excluding somas with an increase voltage away from electrode peak and computing normed values V/max(V). We see a distribution of slopes as well as intercepts across somas, showing some variability across recordings sites. As the reviewer suggests, it is possible that a power-law describes these data better than a linear function, and this would need to be investigated further by quantitatively comparing the fit of these functions.

      (7) In Figure 3A, it seems that the total number of cells is rather low for such a large number of electrodes. What are the quality criteria that are used to keep these cells? Did the authors exclude some cells from the analysis, and if yes, what are the quality criteria that are used to keep cells? If no criteria are used (because none are mentioned in the Methods), then how come so few cells are detected, and can the authors convince us that these neurons are indeed "clean" units (RPVs, SNRs, ...)?

      The reviewer is correct to point out that a number of stringent criteria were employed to exclude some putative cells. We now outline these criteria directly in the text (line 161):

      “ At different steps in the process, conditions for rejecting spikes can be tailored by applying: (1) a stringent threshold to filtered voltages; (2) a minimal cut-off on the signal-to-noise ratio of voltages (see Supplemental Figure 2); (3) an LDA for cluster separability; (4) a minimal spike rate to putative neurons; (5) a Hartigan statistical dip test to detect spike bursting; (6) a decrease in voltage away from putative somas; and (7) a maximum spike-count correlation for nearby channels. Together, these criteria allow SpikeMAP users the ability to precisely control parameters relevant to automated spike sorting.”

      Further, we provide SNRs of individual channels (Supplemental Figure 2), and added to the SpikeMAP software the ability to apply a minimal criterion based on SNR.

      (8) Still in Figure 3A, it looks like there is a bias to find inhibitory cells at the borders, since they do not appear to be uniformly distributed over the MEA. Can the authors comment on that? What would be the explanation for such a behaviour? It would be interesting to see some macroscopic quantities on Excitatory/Inhibitory cells, such as mean firing rates, averaged SNRs... Because again, in Figure 3C, it is not clear to me that the firing rates of inhibitory cells are higher than Excitatory ones, whilst they should be in theory.

      We have added figures showing the distribution of E and I firing rates across a population of N=1,950 putative cells (Supplemental Figure 3). Firing rates of inhibitory neurons are marginally higher than excitatory neurons, and both E and I follow an approximately exponential distribution of rates.

      Reviewer may be right that there are more I neurons at borders in Fig.3B because injections were done in medial prefrontal cortex, so this may reflect an experimental artefact related to a high probability of activating I neurons in locations where the opsin was activated. We added a sentence to the text to clarify this point (line 201):

      “It is possible that the spatial location of putative I cells reflects the site of injection of the opsin in medial prefrontal cortex.”

      (9) For Figure 3 in general, I would have performed an exhaustive comparison of putative cells found by spikeMAP and other sorters. More precisely, I think that to prove the point that spikeMAP is indeed bringing something new to the field of spike sorting, the authors should have compared the performances of various spike sorters to discriminate Exc vs Inh cells based on their ground truth recordings. For example, either using Kilosort [Pachitariu et al, 2024, 10.1038/s41592-024-02232-7], or some other sorters that might be working with such large high-density data [Yger et al, 2018, 10.7554/eLife.34518].

      The reviewer is correct to point out that our the spike-sorting portion of our pipeline shares similarities with related approaches. Other aspects, however, are unique to SpikeMAP. We have clarified the text accordingly:

      “In sum, SpikeMAP provides an end-to-end pipeline to perform spike-sorting on high-density multielectrode arrays. Some elements of this pipeline are similar to related approaches (Table 1), including the use of voltage filtering, PCA, and k-means clustering. Other elements are novel, including the use of spline interpolation, LDA, and the ability to identify putative excitatory and inhibitory cells.”

      (10) Figure 4 has a big issue, and I guess the panels A and B should be redrawn. I don't understand what the red rectangle is displaying.

      Again, we apologize for the rendering issues in the Figures that occurred during conversion into PDF format. We have now ensured that all figures are properly displayed.

      (11) I understand that Figure 4 is only one example, but I have a hard time understanding from the manuscript how many slices/mices were used to obtain the GT data? I guess the manuscript could be enhanced by turning the data into an open-access dataset, but then some clarification is needed. How many flashes/animals/slices are we talking about? Maybe this should be illustrated in Figure 4, if this figure is devoted to the introduction of the GT data.

      Details of the open access data are now provided in Supplemental Table 1. We also clarified Figure 5B:

      “Quantification of change in firing rate following optogenetic stimulation. Average firing rates are taken over four recordings obtained from 3 mice.”

      (12) While there is no doubt that GT data as the ones recorded here by the authors are the most interesting data from a validation point of view, the pretty low yield of such experiments should not discourage the use of artificially generated recordings such as the ones made in [Buccino et al, 2020, 10.1007/s12021-020-09467-7] or even recently in [Laquitaine et al, 2024, 10.1101/2024.12.04.626805v1]. In these papers, the authors have putative waveforms/firing rate patterns for excitatory and inhibitory cells, and thus, the authors could test how good they are in discriminating the two subtypes.

      We agree with the reviewer that it would be worthwhile for future work to apply SpikeMAP to artificially generated spike trains, and have added the following (line 328):

      “Another avenue could involve applying SpikeMAP on artificially generated spike data (Buccino & Einevoll 2021; Laquitaine et al., 2024).”

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 154 seems to include a parenthetical expression left over from editing: "sensitive to noise (contamination? Better than noise?) generated by the signal of proximal units." See also line 186: "use (reliance?) of light-sensitive" and line 245: "In the absence of synaptic blockers (right?)," and line 270: "the size of the data prevents manual intervention (curation?)." Check carefully for all parentheses like that, which should be removed.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have revised the text and removed parenthetical expressions left over from editing.

      (2) In lines 285-286, you state that: "k-mean clustering of spike waveform properties best differentiated the two principal classes of cells..." But I could not find where you compared k-means clustering to other methods. I think you just argued that k-means seemed to work well, but not better than, another method. If that is so, then you should probably rephrase those lines.

      The reviewer is correct that direct comparisons are not performed here, hence we removed this sentence.

      (3) Methods section, E/I classification, lines 396-405: You give us figures on what fraction was E and I (PV subtype) (94.75% and 5.25%), but there is more that you could have said. First of all, what is the expected fraction of parvalbumin-sensitive interneurons in the cortex - is it near 5%?

      We clarified the text as follows (line 444): “This number is close to the expected percentage of PV interneurons in cortex (4-6%) (Markram et al. 2004).”

      Second, how would these percentages change if you altered the threshold from 3 s.d. to something lower, like 2 s.d.? Giving us some idea of how the threshold affects the fraction of PV interneurons could give us an idea of whether this method agrees with our expectations or not.

      While SpikeMAP offers the flexibility to set the voltage threshold manually, we opted for a stringent threshold to demonstrate the capabilities of the software. As seen in Figure 2D, at 2 and 3 s.d., the signal is largely accounted for by Gaussian noise, while deviation from noise arises around 4 s.d. We clarified the text as follows (line 120):

      “At a threshold of -3 , the signal could be largely accounted for by Gaussian noise, while a separation between signal and noise began around a threshold of -4 ”

      Third, did the inhibitory neurons identified by this optogenetic method also have narrow spike widths at half amplitude? Could you do a scatterplot of all the spike widths and inter-peak distances that had color-coded dots for E and I based on your optogenetic method?

      We have added a scatterplot (Supplemental Figure 5).

      (4) Can you compare your methods with others now widely in use, like, for example, Spiking Circus or Kilosort? You do that in Table 1 in terms of features, but not in terms of performance. For example, you could have applied Kilosort4 to your data from the 4096 electrode array and seen how often it sorted the same neurons that SpikeMAP did. I realize this could not give you a comparison of how many were E/I, but it could tell you how close your numbers of neurons agreed with their numbers. Were your numbers within 5% of each other? This would be helpful for groups who are already using Kilosort4.

      As mentioned ealier, packages listed in Table 1 do not provide an identification of putative E/I neurons on high-density electrode arrays. To facilitation the integration of SpikeMAP with other spike sorting packages, our suite now provides a stand-alone module to perform E/I identification. This is now mentioned in the text (see earlier comment).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      I would encourage the authors to decide what the paper is about: is it about a new sorting method (and if yes, more tests/benchmarks are needed to explain the pros and the cons of the pipelines, and the Methods need to be expanded). Or is it about the new data for Ground Truth validation, and again, if yes, then maybe explain more what they are, how many slices/mice/cells, ... Maybe also consider making the data available online as an open dataset.

      We agree with the reviewer that the paper is best slated toward ground truth validation of E/I identification. We now specify how many slices/mice/cells etc. (see Supplemental Table 1) and make the data available online as open source.

    1. Author response:

      (1) Explore the temporal component of neural responses (instead of collapsing responses to a single number, i.e., the average response over 4s), and determine which of the three models can recapitulate the observed dynamics.

      (2) Expand the polar plot visualization to show all three slopes (changes in responses across all three successive concentrations) instead of only two slopes.

      (3) Attempt to collect and analyze, from published papers, data of: (a) first-order neuron responses to odors to determine the role of first-order inhibition towards generating non-monotonic responses, and (b) PN responses in Drosophila to properly compare with corresponding first-order neuron responses.

      (4) Further discuss: (a) why the brain may need to encode absolute concentration, (b) the distinction between non-monotonic responses and cross-over responses, and (c) potential limitations of the primacy model.

      (5) Expand the divisive normalization model by evaluating different values of k and R, and study the effects of divisive normalization on tufted cells.

      (6) Add discussion of other potential inhibitory mechanisms that could contribute towards the observed effects.

      Reviewer #1:

      The article starts from the premise that animals need to know the absolute concentration of an odor over many log units, but the need for this isn't obvious. The introduction cites an analogy to vision and audition. These are cases where we know for a fact that the absolute intensity of the stimulus is not relevant. Instead, sensory perception relies on processing small differences in intensity across space or time. And to maintain that sensitivity to small differences, the system discards the stimulus baseline. Humans are notoriously bad at judging the absolute light level. That information gets discarded even before light reaches the retina, namely through contraction of the pupil. Similarly, it seems plausible that a behavior like olfactory tracking relies on sensing small gradients across time (when weaving back and forth across the track) or space (across nostrils). It is important that the system function over many log units of concentration (e.g., far and close to a source) but not that it accurately represents what that current concentration is [see e.g., Wachowiak et al, 2025 Recalibrating Olfactory Neuroscience..].

      We thank the Reviewer for the insightful input and agree that gradients across time and space are important for various olfactory behaviors, such as tracking. At the same time, we think that absolute concentration is also needed for two reasons. First, in order to extract changes in concentration, the absolute concentration needs to be normalized out; i.e., change needs to be encoded with respect to some baseline, which is what divisive normalization computes. Second, while it is true that representing the exact number of odor molecules present is not important, this number directly relates to distance from the odor source, which does provide ethological value (e.g., is the tiger 100m or 1000m away?). Indeed, our decoding experiments focused on discriminating relative, and not on absolute, concentrations by classifying between each pair of concentrations (i.e., relative distances), which is effectively an assessment of the gradient. In our revision, we will make all of these points clearer.

      Still, many experiments in olfactory research have delivered square pulses of odor at concentrations spanning many log units, rather than the sorts of stimuli an animal might encounter during tracking. Even within that framework, though, it doesn't seem mysterious anymore how odor identity and odor concentration are represented differently. For example, Stopfer et al 2003 showed that the population response of locust PNs traces a dynamic trajectory. Trajectories for a given odor form a manifold, within which trajectories for different concentrations are distinct by their excursions on the manifold. To see this, one must recognize that the PN responds to an odor pulse with a time-varying firing rate, that different PNs have different dynamics, and that the dynamics can change with concentration. This is also well recognized in the mammalian systems. Much has been written about the topic of dynamic coding of identity and intensity - see the reviews of Laurent (2002) and Uchida (2014).

      Given the above comments on the dynamics of odor responses in first- and second-order neurons, it seems insufficient to capture the response of a neuron with a single number. Even if one somehow had to use a single number, the mean firing rate during the odor pulse may not be the best choice. For example, the rodent mitral cells fire in rhythm with the animal's sniffing cycle, and certain odors will just shift the phase of the rhythm without changing the total number of spikes (see e.g., Fantana et al, 2008). During olfactory search or tracking, the sub-second movements of the animal in the odor landscape get superposed on the sniffing cycle. Given all this, it seems unlikely that the total number of spikes from a neuron in a 4-second period is going to be a relevant variable for neural processing downstream.

      To our knowledge, it is not well understood how downstream brain regions read out mitral cell responses to guide olfactory behavior. The olfactory bulb projects to more than a dozen brain regions, and different regions could decode signals in different ways. We focused on the mean response because it is a simple, natural construct.

      The datasets we analyzed may not include all relevant timing information; for example, the mouse data is from calcium imaging studies that did not track sniff timing. Nonetheless, we plan to address this comment within our framework by binning time into smaller-sized windows (e.g., 0-0.2s, 0.2-0.4s, etc.) and repeating our analysis for each of these windows. Specifically, we will determine how each normalization method fares in recapitulating statistics of the population responses of each window, beyond simply assessing the population mean.

      Much of the analysis focuses on the mean activity of the entire population. Why is this an interesting quantity? Apparently, the mean stays similar because some neurons increase and others decrease their firing rate. It would be more revealing, perhaps, to show the distribution of firing rates at different concentrations and see how that distribution is predicted by different models of normalization. This could provide a stronger test than just the mean.

      We agree that mean activity is only one measure to summarize a rich data set and will perform the suggested analysis.

      The question "if concentration information is discarded in second-order neurons, which exclusively transmit odor information to the rest of the brain, how does the brain support olfactory behaviors, such as tracking and navigation?" is really not an open question anymore. For example, reference 23 reports in the abstract that "Odorant concentration had no systematic effect on spike counts, indicating that rate cannot encode intensity. Instead, odor intensity can be encoded by temporal features of the population response. We found a subpopulation of rapid, largely concentration-invariant responses was followed by another population of responses whose latencies systematically decreased at higher concentrations."

      Primacy coding does provide one plausible mechanism to decode concentration. Our manuscript demonstrated how such a code could emerge in second-order neurons with the help of divisive normalization, though it does require maintaining at least partial rank invariance across concentrations, which may not be robust. We also showed how concentration could be decoded via spike rates, even if average rates are constant, which provides an alternative hypothesis to that of ref 23.

      Further, ref 23 only considers the piriform cortex, which, as mentioned above, is one of many targets of the olfactory bulb, and it remains unclear what the decoding mechanisms are of each of these targets. In addition, work from the same authors of ref 23 found multiple potential decoding strategies in the piriform cortex itself, including changes in firing rate (see Fig. 2E of ref. 23 - Bolding & Franks, 2017; as well as Fig. 4 in Roland et al., 2017).

      It would be useful to state early in the manuscript what kinds of stimuli are being considered and how the response of a neuron is summarized by one number. There are many alternative ways to treat both stimuli and responses.

      We will add this explanation to the manuscript.

      "The change in response across consecutive concentration levels may not be robust due to experimental noise and the somewhat limited range of concentrations sampled": Yes, a number of the curves just look like "no response". It would help the reader to show some examples of raw data, e.g. the time course of one neuron's firing rate to 4 concentrations, and for the authors to illustrate how they compress those responses into single numbers.

      We agree and will add this information to the manuscript.

      "We then calculated the angle between these two slopes for each neuron and plotted a polar histogram of these angles." The methods suggest that this angle is the arctan of the ratio of the two slopes in the response curve. A ratio of 2 would result from a slope change from 0.0001 to 0.0002 (i.e., virtually no change in slope) or from 1 to 2 (a huge change). Those are completely different response curves. Is it reasonable to lump them into the same bin of the polar plot? This seems an unusual way to illustrate the diversity of response curve shapes.

      We agree that the two changes in the reviewer’s example will be categorized in the same quadrant in our analysis. We did not focus on the absolute changes because our analysis covers many log ratios of concentrations. Instead, we focused on the relative shapes of the concentration response curves, and more specifically, the direction of the change (i.e., the sign of the slope). We will better motivate this style of analysis in the revision. Moreover, in response to comments by Reviewer 2, we will compare response shapes between all three successive levels of concentration changes, as opposed to only two levels.

      The Drosophila OSN data are passed through normalization models and then compared to locust PN data. This seems dangerous, as flies and locusts are separated by about 300 M years of evolution, and we don't know that fly PNs act like locust PNs. Their antennal lobe anatomy differs in many ways, as does the olfactory physiology. To draw any conclusions about a change in neural representation, it would be preferable to have OSN and PN data from the same species.

      We are in the process of requesting PN response data in Drosophila from groups that have collected such data and will repeat the analysis once we get access to the data.

      One conclusion is that divisive normalization could account for some of the change in responses from receptors to 2nd order neurons. This seems to be well appreciated already [e.g., Olsen 2010, Papadopoulou 2011, minireview in Hong & Wilson 2013].

      While we agree that these manuscripts do study the effects of divisive normalization in insects and fish, here we show that this computation also generalizes to rodents. In addition, these previous studies do not focus on divisive normalization’s role towards concentration encoding/decoding, which is our focus. We will clarify this difference in the revision.

      Another claim is that subtractive normalization cannot perform that function. What model was used for subtractive normalization is unclear (there is an error in the Methods). It would be interesting if there were a categorical difference between divisive and subtractive normalization.

      We apologize for the mistake in the subtractive normalization equation and will correct it. Thank you for catching it.

      Looking closer at the divisive normalization model, it really has two components: (a) the "lateral inhibition" by which a neuron gets suppressed if other neurons fire (here scaled by the parameter k) , and (b) a nonlinear sigmoid transformation (determined by the parameters n and sigma). Both lateral inhibition and nonlinearity are known to contribute to decorrelation in a neural population (e.g., Pitkow 2012). The "intraglomerular gain control" contains only the nonlinearity. The "subtractive normalization" we don't know. But if one wanted to put divisive and subtractive inhibition on the same footing, one should add a sigmoid nonlinearity in both cases.

      Our intent was not to place all the methods on the “same footing” but rather to isolate the two primary components of normalization methods – non-linearity and lateral inhibition – and determine which of these, and in which combination, could generate the desired effects. Divisive normalization incorporates both components, whereas intraglomerular gain control and subtractive normalization only incorporate one of these components. We will clarify this reasoning in the revision.

      The response models could be made more realistic in other ways. For example, in both locusts and fish, the 2nd order neurons get inputs from multiple receptor types; presumably, that will affect their response functions. Also, lateral inhibition can take quite different forms. In locusts, the inhibitory neurons seem to collect from many glomeruli. But in rats, the inhibition by short axon cells may originate from just a few sparse glomeruli, and those might be different for every mitral cell (Fantana 2008).

      We thank the Reviewer for the input. Instead of fixing k for all second-order neurons, we will apply different k values for different neurons. We will also systematically vary the percentage of neurons used for the divisive normalization calculation in the denominator, and determine the regime under which the effects experimentally observed are reproducible. This approach takes into account the scenario that inter-glomerular inhibitory interactions are sparse.

      There are questions raised by the following statements: "traded-off energy for faster and finer concentration discrimination" and "an additional type of second-order neuron (tufted cells) that has evolved in land vertebrates and that outperforms mitral cells in concentration encoding" and later "These results suggest a trade-off between concentration decoding and normalization processes, which prevent saturation and reduce energy consumption.". Are the tufted cells inferior to the mitral cells in any respect? Do they suffer from saturation at high concentration? And do they then fail in their postulated role for odor tracking? If not, then what was the evolutionary driver for normalization in the mitral cell pathway? Certainly not lower energy consumption (50,000 mitral cells = 1% of rod photoreceptors, each of which consumes way more energy than a mitral cell).

      The question of what mitral cells are “good for”, compared to tufted cells, remains unclear in our view. We speculate that mitral cells provide superior context-dependent processing and are better for determining stimuli-reward contingencies, but this remains far from settled experimentally.

      We believe the mitral cell pathway evolved earlier than tufted cells, since the former appear akin to projection neurons in insects. Nonetheless, we agree that differences in energy consumption are unlikely to be the primary distinguishing factor, and in the revision, we will drop this argument.

      Reviewer #2:

      The main premise that divisive normalization generates this diversity of dose-response curves in the second-order neurons is a little problematic. … The analysis in [Figure 3] indicates that divisive normalization does what it is supposed to do, i.e., compresses concentration information and not alter the rank-order of neurons or the combinatorial patterns. Changes in the combinations of neurons activated with intensity arise directly from the fact that the first-order neurons did not have monotonic responses with odor intensity (i.e., crossovers). This was the necessary condition, and not the divisive normalization for changes in the combinatorial code. There seems to be a confusion/urge to attribute all coding properties found in the second-order neurons to 'divisive normalization.' If the input from sensory neurons is monotonic (i.e., no crossovers), then divisive normalization did not change the rank order, and the same combinations of neurons are activated in a similar fashion (same vector direction or combinatorial profile) to encode for different odor intensities. Concentration invariance is achieved, and concentration information is lost. However, when the first-order neurons are non-monotonic (i.e., with crossovers), that causes the second-order neurons to have different rank orders with different concentrations. Divisive normalization compresses information about concentrations, and rank-order differences preserve information about the odor concentration. Does this not mean that the non-monotonicity of sensory neuron response is vital for robustly maintaining information about odor concentration? Naturally, the question that arises is whether many of the important features of the second-order neuron's response simply seem to follow the input. Or is my understanding of the figures and the write-up flawed, and are there more ways in which divisive normalization contributes to reshaping the second-order neural response? This must be clarified. Lastly, the tufted cells in the mouse OB are also driven by this sensory input with crossovers. How does the OB circuit convert the input with crossovers into one that is monotonic with concentration? I think that is an important question that this computational effort could clarify.

      It appears that there is confusion about the definitions of “non-monotonicity” and “crossovers”.  These are two independent concepts – one does not necessarily lead to the other. Non-monotonicity concerns the response of a single neuron to different concentration levels. A neuron’s response is considered non-monotonic if its response goes up then down, or down then up, across increasing concentrations. A “cross-over” is defined based on the responses of multiple neurons. A cross-over occurs when the response of one neuron is lower than another neuron at one concentration, but higher than the other at a different concentration. For example, the responses of both neurons could increase monotonically with increasing concentration, but one neuron might start lower and grow faster, hence creating a cross-over. We will clarify this in the manuscript, which we believe will resolve the questions raised above.

      The way the decoding results and analysis are presented does not add a lot of information to what has already been presented. For example, based on the differences in rank-order with concentration, I would expect the combinatorial code to be different. Hence, a very simple classifier based on cosine or correlation distance would work well. However, since divisive normalization (DN) is applied, I would expect a simple classification scheme that uses the Euclidean distance metric to work equally as well after DN. Is this the case?

      Yes, we used a simple classification scheme, logistic regression with a linear kernel, which is essentially a Euclidean distance-based classification. This scheme works better for tufted cells because they are more monotonic; i.e., if neuron A and B both increase their responsiveness with concentration, then Euclidean distance would be fine. But if neuron A’s response amplitude goes up and neuron B’s response goes down – as often happens for mitral cells – then Euclidean distance does not work as well. We will add intuition about this in the manuscript.

      Leave-one-trial/sample-out seems too conservative. How robust are the combinatorial patterns across trials? Would just one or two training trials suffice for creating templates for robust classification? Based on my prior experience (https://elifesciences.org/reviewed-preprints/89330https://elifesciences.org/reviewed-preprints/89330), I do expect that the combinatorial patterns would be more robust to adaptation and hence also allow robust recognition of odor intensity across repeated encounters.

      As suggested, we will compute the correlation coefficient of the similarity of neural responses for each odor (across trials). We will repeat this analysis for both mitral and tufted cells. To determine the effect of adaptation, we will compute correlation coefficients of responses between the 1st and 2nd trials vs the 1st and final trial.

      Lastly, in the simulated data, since the affinity of the first-order sensory neurons to odorants is expected to be constant across concentration, and "Jaccard similarity between the sets of highest-affinity neurons for each pair of concentration levels was > 0.96," why would the rank-order change across concentration? DN should not alter the rank order.

      We agree that divisive normalization should not alter the rank order, but the rank order may change in first-order neurons, which carries through to second-order neurons. This confusion may be related to the one mentioned above re: cross-overs vs non-monotonicity. Moreover, in the simulated data (Fig. 4D-H), the Jaccard similarity was calculated based on only the 50 neurons with the highest affinity, not the entire population of neurons. As shown in Fig. 4H, most of the rank-order change happens in the remaining 150 neurons.

      Note that in response to a comment by Reviewer 3, we will change the presentation of Fig. 4H in the revision.

      If the set of early responders does change, how will the decoder need to change, and what precise predictions can be made that can be tested experimentally? The lack of exploration of this aspect of the results seems like a missed opportunity.

      In the Discussion, we wrote about how downstream circuits will need to learn which set of neurons are to be associated with each distinct concentration level. We will expand upon this point and include experimentally testable predictions.

      Based on the methods, for Figures 1 and 2, it appears the responses across time, trials, and odorants were averaged to get a single data point per neuron for each concentration. Would this averaging not severely dilute trends in the data? The one that particularly concerns me is the averaging across different odorants. If you do odor-by-odor analysis, is the flattening of second-order neural responses still observable? Because some odorants activate more globally and some locally, I would expect a wide variety of dose-response relationships that vary with odor identity (more compressed in second-order neurons, of course). It would be good to show some representative neural responses and show how the extracted values for each neuron are a faithful/good representation of its response variation across intensities.

      It appears there is some confusion here; we will clarify in the text and figure captions that we did not average across different odors in our analysis. We will also add figure panels showing some representative neural responses as suggested by the Reviewer.

      A lot of neurons seem to have responses that flat line closer to zero (both firing rate and dF/F in Figure 1). Are these responsive neurons? The mean dF/F also seems to hover not significantly above zero. Hence, I was wondering if the number of neurons is reducing the trend in the data significantly.

      Yes, if a neuron responds to at least one concentration level in at least 50% of the trials, it is considered responsive. So it is possible that some neurons respond to one concentration level and otherwise flatline near zero.  We will highlight a few example neurons to visualize this scenario.

      I did not fully understand the need to show the increase in the odor response across concentrations as a polar plot. I see potential issues with the same. For example, the following dose-response trend at four intensities (C4 being the highest concentration and C1 the lowest): response at C3 > response at C1 and response at C4 > response at C2. But response at C3 < response at C2. Hence, it will be in the top right segment of the polar plot. However, the responses are not monotonic with concentrations. So, I am not convinced that the polar plot is the right way to characterize the dose-response curves. Just my 2 cents.

      Your 2 cents are valuable! Thank you for raising this point. Instead of computing two slopes (C1-C3 and C2-C4), we will expand our analysis to include all three slopes (C1-C2, C2-C3, C3-C4). Consequently, there are 2^3 = 8 different response shapes, and we will list them and quantify the fraction of the responses that fall into each shape category.

      In many analyses, simulated data were used (Figures 3 and 4). However, there is no comparison of how well the simulated data fit the experimental data. For example, the Simulated 1st order neuron in Figure 3D does not show a change in rank-order for the first-order neuron. In Figure 3E, temporal response patterns in second-order neurons look unrealistic. Some objective comparison of simulated and experimental data would help bolster confidence in these results.

      We believe the Reviewer is referring to Figs. 4D and 4E, since Fig. 3D does not show a first-order neuron simulation, and there is no Fig 3E. In Fig. 4D there is no change of rank order because the simulation is for a single odor and single concentration level, and the change of rank-order (i.e., cross-overs) as we define occurs between concentration levels. We will clarify this in the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3:

      While the authors focus on concentration-dependent increases in first-order neuron activity, reflecting the majority of observed responses, recent work from the Imai group shows that odorants can also lead to direct first-order neuron inhibition (i.e., reduction in spontaneous activity), and within this subset, increasing odorant concentration tends to increase the degree of inhibition. Some discussion of these findings and how they may complement divisive normalization to contribute to the diverse second-order neuron concentration-dependence would be of interest and help expand the context of the current results.

      We thank the Reviewer for the suggestion. We will request datasets of first-order neuron responses from the groups who acquired them. We will analyze this data to determine the role of inhibition or antagonistic binding and quantify what percentage of first-order neurons respond less strongly with larger concentrations.

      Related to the above point, odorant-evoked inhibition of second-order neurons is widespread in mammalian mitral cells and significantly contributes to the flattened concentration-dependence of mitral cells at the population level. Such responses are clearly seen in Figure 1D. Some discussion of how odorant-evoked mitral cell inhibition may complement divisive normalization, and likewise relate to comparatively lower levels of odorant-evoked inhibition among tufted cells, would further expand the context of the current results. Toward this end, replication of analyses in Figures 1D and E following exclusion of mitral cell inhibitory responses would provide insight into the contribution of such inhibition to the flattening of the mitral cell population concentration dependence.

      We will perform the analysis suggested, specifically, we will set the negative mitral cell responses to 0 and assess whether the population mean remains flat.

      The idea of concentration-dependent crossover responses across the first-order population being required for divisive normalization to generate individually diverse concentration response functions across the second-order population is notable. The intuition of the crossover responses is that first-order neurons that respond most sensitively to any particular odorant (i.e., at the lowest concentration) respond with overall lower activity at higher concentrations than other first-order neurons less sensitively tuned to the odorant. Whether this is a consistent, generalizable property of odorant binding and first-order neuron responsiveness is not addressed by the authors, however. Biologically, one mechanism that may support such crossover events is intraglomerular presynaptic/feedback inhibition, which would be expected to increase with increasing first-order neuron activation such that the most-sensitively responding first-order neurons would also recruit the strongest inhibition as concentration increases, enabling other first-order neurons to begin to respond more strongly. Discussion of this and/or other biological mechanisms (e.g., first-order neuron depolarization block) supporting such crossover responses would strengthen these results.

      We thank the reviewer for providing additional mechanisms to consider. As suggested, we will add discussion of these alternatives to divisive normalization.

      It is unclear to what degree the latency analysis considered in Figures 4D-H works with the overall framework of divisive normalization, which in Figure 3 we see depends on first-order neuron crossover in concentration response functions. Figure 4D suggests that all first-order neurons respond with the same response amplitude (R in eq. 3), even though this is supposed to be pulled from a distribution. It's possible that Figure 4D is plotting normalized response functions to highlight the difference in latency, but this is not clear from the plot or caption. If response amplitudes are all the same, and the response curves are, as plotted in Figure 4D, identical except for their time to half-max, then it seems somewhat trivial that the resulting second-order neuron activation will follow the same latency ranking, regardless of whether divisive normalization exists or not. However, there is some small jitter in these rankings across concentrations (Figure 4G), suggesting there is some randomness to the simulations. It would be helpful if this were clarified (e.g., by showing a non-normalized Figure 4D, with different response amplitudes), and more broadly, it would be extremely helpful in evaluating the latency coding within the broader framework proposed if the authors clarified whether the simulated first-order neuron response timecourses, when factoring in potentially different amplitudes (R) and averaging across the entire response window, reproduces the concentration response crossovers observed experimentally. In summary, in the present manuscript, it remains unclear if concentration crossovers are captured in the latency simulations, and if not, the authors do not clearly address what impact such variation in response amplitudes across concentrations may have on the latency results. It is further unclear to what degree divisive normalization is necessary for the second-order neurons to establish and maintain their latency ranks across concentrations, or to exhibit concentration-dependent changes in latency.

      As suggested by the Reviewer, we will add another simulation scenario where the response amplitudes (R) are different for different neurons. For each concentration, we will then average each neuron’s response across the entire response window and determine if the simulation reproduces the cross-overs as observed experimentally.

      How the authors get from Figure 4G to 4H is not clear. Figure 4G shows second-order neuron response latencies across all latencies, with ordering based on their sorted latency to low concentration. This shows that very few neurons appear to change latency ranks going from low to high concentration, with a change in rank appearing as any deviation in a monotonically increasing trend. Focusing on the high concentration points, there appear to be 2 latency ranks switched in the first 10 responding neurons (reflecting the 1 downward dip in the points around neuron 8), rather than the 7 stated in the text. Across the first 50 responding neurons, I see only ~14 potential switches (reflecting the ~7 downward dips in the points around neurons 8, 20, 32, 33, 41, 44, 50), rather than the 32 stated in the text. It is possible that the unaccounted rank changes reflect fairly minute differences in latencies that are not visible in the plot in Figure 4G. This may be clarified by plotting each neuron's latency at low concentration vs. high concentration (i.e., similar to Figure 4H, but plotting absolute latency, not latency rank) to allow assessment of the absolute changes. If such minute differences are not driving latency rank changes in Fig. 4G, then a trend much closer to the unity line would be expected in Figure 4H. Instead, however, there are many massive deviations from unity, even within the first 50 responding neurons plotted in Figure 4G. These deviations include a jump in latency rank from 2 at low concentration to ~48 at high concentration. Such a jump is simply not seen in Figure 4G.

      We apologize that Fig. 4H was a poor choice for visualization. What is plotted in Fig. 4H is the sorted identity of neurons under low and high concentrations, and points on the y=x line indicate that the two corresponding neurons have the same rank under the two concentrations. We will replace this panel with a more intuitive visualization, where the x and y axes are the ranks of the neurons; and deviation from the y=x line indicates how different the ranks are of a neuron to the two concentrations.

      In the text, the authors state that "Odor identity can be encoded by the set of highest-affinity neurons (which remains invariant across concentrations)." Presumably, this is a restatement of the primacy model and refers to invariance in latency rank (since the authors have not shown that the highest-affinity neurons have invariant response amplitudes across concentration). To what degree this statement holds given the results in Figure 4H, however, which appear to show that some neurons with the earliest latency rank at low concentration jump to much later latency ranks at high concentration, remains unclear. Such changes in latency rank for only a few of the first responding neurons may be negligible for classifying odor identity among a small handful of odorants, but not among 1-2 orders of magnitude more odors, which may feasibly occur in a natural setting. Collectively, these issues with the execution and presentation of the latency analysis make it unclear how robust the latency results are.

      The original primacy model states that the latency of a neuron decreases with increasing concentration, while the ranks of neurons remain unaltered. Our results, on the other hand, suggest that the ranks do at least partially change across concentrations. This leads to two possible decoding mechanisms. First, if the top K responding neurons remain invariant across concentrations (even if their individual ranks change within the top K), then the brain could learn to associate a population of K neurons with a response latency; lower response latency means higher concentration. Second, if the top K responding neurons do not remain invariant across concentrations, then the brain would need to learn to associate a different set of neurons with each concentration level. The latter imposes additional constraints on the robustness of the primacy model and the corresponding read-out mechanism. We will include more discussion of these possibilities in the revision.

      Analysis in Figures 4A-C shows that concentration can be decoded from first-order neurons, second-order neurons, or first-order neurons with divisive normalization imposed (i.e., simulating second-order responses). This does not say that divisive normalization is necessary to encode concentration, however. Therefore, for the authors to say that divisive normalization is "a potential mechanism for generating odor-specific subsets of second-order neurons whose combinatorial activity or whose response latencies represent concentration information" seems too strong a conclusion. Divisive normalization is not generating the concentration information, since that can be decoded just as well from the first-order neurons. Rather, divisive normalization can account for the different population patterns in concentration response functions between first- and second-order neurons without discarding concentration-dependent information.

      We agree that the word “generating” is faulty. We thank the reviewer for their more precise wording, which we will adopt.

      Performing the same polar histogram analysis of tufted vs. mitral cell concentration response functions (Figure 5B) provides a compelling new visualization of how these two cell types differ in their concentration variance. The projected importance of tufted cells to navigation, emerging directly through the inverse relationship between average concentration and distance (Figure 5C), is not surprising, and is largely a conceptual analysis rather than new quantitative analysis per se, but nevertheless, this is an important point to make. Another important consideration absent from this section, however, is whether and how divisive normalization may impact tufted cell activity. Previous work from the authors, as well as from Schoppa, Shipley, and Westbrook labs, has compellingly demonstrated that a major circuit mediating divisive normalization of mitral cells (GABA/DAergic short-axon cells) directly targets external tufted cells, and is thus very likely to also influence projection tufted cells. Such analysis would additionally provide substantially more justification for the Discussion statement "we analyzed an additional type of second-order neuron (tufted cells)", which at present instead reflects fairly minimal analysis.

      We agree that tufted cells are subject to divisive normalization as well, albeit probably to a less degree than mitral cells. To determine the effect of this, we will alter the strength (and degree of sparseness of interglomerular interactions) of divisive normalization and determine if there is a regime where response features of tufted cells match those observed experimentally.

    1. Author response:

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Zhang et al. used a conditional knockout mouse model to re-examine the role of the RNA-binding protein PTBP1 in the transdifferentiation of astroglial cells into neurons. Several earlier studies reported that PTBP1 knockdown can efficiently induce the transdifferentiation of rodent glial cells into neurons, suggesting potential therapeutic applications for neurodegenerative diseases. However, these findings have been contested by subsequent studies, which in turn have been challenged by more recent publications. In their current work, Zhang et al. deleted exon 2 of the Ptbp1 gene using an astrocyte-specific, tamoxifen-inducible Cre line and investigated, using fluorescence imaging and bulk and single-cell RNA-sequencing, whether this manipulation promotes the transdifferentiation of astrocytes into neurons across various brain regions. The data strongly indicate that genetic ablation of PTBP1 is not sufficient to drive efficient conversion of astrocytes into neurons. Interestingly, while PTBP1 loss alters splicing patterns in numerous genes, these changes do not shift the astroglial transcriptome toward a neuronal profile.

      Strengths:

      Although this is not the first report of PTBP1 ablation in mouse astrocytes in vivo, this study utilizes a distinct knockout strategy and provides novel insights into PTBP1-regulated splicing events in astrocytes. The manuscript is well written, and the experiments are technically sound and properly controlled. I believe this study will be of considerable interest to a broad readership.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The primary point that needs to be addressed is a better understanding of the effect of exon 2 deletion on PTBP1 expression. Figure 4D shows successful deletion of exon 2 in knockout astrocytes. However, assuming that the coverage plots are CPM-normalized, the overall PTBP1 mRNA expression level appears unchanged. Figure 6A further supports this observation. This is surprising, as one would expect that the loss of exon 2 would shift the open reading frame and trigger nonsense-mediated decay of the PTBP1 transcript. Given this uncertainty, the authors should confirm the successful elimination of PTBP1 protein in cKO astrocytes using an orthogonal approach, such as Western blotting, in addition to immunofluorescence. They should also discuss possible reasons why PTBP1 mRNA abundance is not detectably affected by the frameshift.

      We thank the reviewer for raising this important point. Indeed, the deletion of exon 2 introduces a frameshift that is predicted to disrupt the PTBP1 open reading frame and trigger nonsensemediated decay (NMD). While our CPM-normalized coverage plots (Figure 4D) and gene-level expression analysis (Figure 6A) suggest that PTBP1 mRNA levels remain largely unchanged in cKO astrocytes, we acknowledge that this observation is counterintuitive and merits further clarification.

      We suspect that the process of brain tissue dissociation and FACS sorting for bulk or single cell RNA-seq may enrich for nucleic material and thus dilute the NMD signal, which occurs in the cytoplasm. Alternatively, the transcripts (like other genes) may escape NMD for unknown mechanisms. Although a frameshift is a strong indicator for triggering NMD, it does not guarantee NMD will occur in every case. We will include this discussion in the revised manuscript to provide additional context for the apparent discrepancy between mRNA abundance and protein loss.

      Regarding the validation of PTBP1 protein depletion in cKO astrocytes by Western blotting, we acknowledge that orthogonal approaches to confirm PTBP1 elimination would address uncertainty around the effect of exon 2 deletion on PTBP1 expression. The low cell yield of cKO astrocytes poses a significant burden on obtaining sufficient samples for immunoblotting detection of PTBP1 depletion. On average 3-5 adult animals per genotype are needed for each biological replicate. Our characterization of this Ptbp1 deletion allele in other contexts show the loss of full length PTBP1 proteins in ESCs and NPCs using Western blotting. Furthermore, germline homozygous mutant mice do not survive beyond embryonic day 6, supporting that it is  a loss of function allele.

      (2) The authors should analyze PTBP1 expression in WT and cKO substantia nigra samples shown in Figure 3 or justify why this analysis is not necessary.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this important question. We used Aldh1l1-CreERT2, which is designed to be active in all the astrocyte throughout mouse brain. Although we have systematically verified PTBP1 elimination in different mouse brain regions (cortex and striatum) at multiple time points (from 4w to 12w after tamoxifen administration), we agree that it remains necessary and important to demonstrate whether the observed lack of astrocyte-to-neuron conversion is indeed associated with sufficient PTBP1 depletion. We will analyze the PTBP1 expression in the substantia nigra, as we did in the cortex and striatum. 

      (3) Lines 236-238 and Figure 4E: The authors report an enrichment of CU-rich sequences near PTBP1-regulated exons. To better compare this with previous studies on position-specific splicing regulation by PTBP1, it would be helpful to assess whether the position of such motifs differs between PTBP1-activated and PTBP1-repressed exons.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. We agree that assessing the positional distribution of CU-rich motifs between PTBP1-activated and PTBP1-repressed exons would provide valuable insight into the position-specific regulatory mechanisms of PTBP1. In response, we will perform separate motif enrichment analyses for PTBP1-activated and PTBP1-repressed exons and examine whether their positional patterns differ. This will help clarify whether these exons are differentially regulated by PTBP1 through distinct motif positioning in mature astrocytes.

      (4) The analyses in Figure 5 and its supplement strongly suggest that the splicing changes in PTBP1-depleted astrocytes are distinct from those occurring during neuronal differentiation. However, the authors should ensure that these comparisons are not confounded by transcriptome-wide differences in gene expression levels between astrocytes and developing neurons. One way to address this concern would be to compare the new PTBP1 cKO data with publicly available RNA-seq datasets of astrocytes induced to transdifferentiate into neurons using proneural transcription factors (e.g., PMID: 38956165).

      We would like to express our gratitude for the thoughtful feedback. We agree that transcriptomewide differences in gene expression between astrocytes and developing neurons could confound the interpretation of splicing differences. To address this concern, we will incorporate publicly available RNA-seq datasets from studies in which astrocytes are reprogrammed into neurons using proneural transcription factors (PMID: 38956165). 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Zhang and colleagues describes a study that investigated whether the deletion of PTBP1 in adult astrocytes in mice led to an astrocyte-to-neuron conversion. The study revisited the hypothesis that reduced PTBP1 expression reprogrammed astrocytes to neurons. More than 10 studies have been published on this subject, with contradicting results. Half of the studies supported the hypothesis while the other half did not. The question being addressed is an important one because if the hypothesis is correct, it can lead to exciting therapeutic applications for treating neurodegenerative diseases such as Parkinson's disease.

      In this study, Zhang and colleagues conducted a conditional mouse knockout study to address the question. They used the Cre-LoxP system to specifically delete PTBP1 in adult astrocytes. Through a series of carefully controlled experiments, including cell lineage tracing, the authors found no evidence for the astrocyte-to-neuron conversion.

      The authors then carried out a key experiment that none of the previous studies on the subject did: investigating alternative splicing pattern changes in PTBP1-depleted cells using RNA-seq analysis. The idea is to compare the splicing pattern change caused by PTBP1 deletion in astrocytes to what occurs during neurodevelopment. This is an important experiment that will help illuminate whether the astrocyte-to-neuron transition occurred in the system. The result was consistent with that of the cell staining experiments: no significant transition was detected.

      These experiments demonstrate that, in this experimental setting, PTBT1 deletion in adult astrocytes did not convert the cells to neurons.

      Strengths:

      This is a well-designed, elegantly conducted, and clearly described study that addresses an important question. The conclusions provide important information to the field.

      To this reviewer, this study provided convincing and solid experimental evidence to support the authors' conclusions.

      Weaknesses:

      The Discussion in this manuscript is short and can be expanded. Can the authors speculate what led to the contradictory results in the published studies? The current study, in combination with the study published in Cell in 2021 by Wang and colleagues, suggests that observed difference is not caused by the difference of knockdown vs. knockout. Is it possible that other glial cell types are responsible for the transition? If so, what cells? Oligodendrocytes?

      We are grateful for the reviewer’s careful reading and valuable suggestions. These will help us improve the manuscript. We will expand the Discussion. The contradictory results in the previously published studies can be due to the stringency and neuronal leakage of the astrocytespecific GFAP promoter that some investigators chose. Other possibilities include alternative cell origin, increased neuronal resilience, or combinations of as yet unidentified factors.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The manuscript's logical flow is challenging and hard to follow, and key arguments could be more clearly structured, particularly in transitions between mechanistic components.

      We have revised our manuscript so as to make it easy for readers to follow the logical flow in transitions between mechanistic components by adding the descriptions of Figure S1E-J, Figure S2F-K, Figure S3A-H, Figure S4A-F, Figure S5, and Figure S6 in the revised manuscript.

      (2) The causality between stress-induced α2A-AR internalization and the enhanced MAO-A remains unclear. Direct experimental evidence is needed to determine whether α2A-AR internalization itself or Ca2+ drives MAO-A activation, and how they activate MAO-A should be considered.

      We believe that the causality between stress-induced α2A-AR internalization and the enhancement of MAO-A is clearly demonstrated by our current experiments, while our explanations may be improved by making them easier to understand especially for those who are not expert on electrophysiology.

      Firstly, it is well established that autoinhibition in LC neurons is mediated by α2A-AR coupled-GIRK (Arima et al., 1998, J Physiol; Williams et al., 1985, Neuroscience). We found that spike frequency adaptation in LC neurons was also mediated by α2A-AR coupled GIRK-I (Figure 1A-I), and that α2A-AR coupled GIRK-I underwent [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> dependent rundown (Figures 2, S1, S2), leading to an abolishment of spike-frequency adaptation (Figures S4). [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> dependent rundown of α2A-AR coupled GIRK-I was prevented by barbadin (Figure 2G-J), which prevents the internalization of G-protein coupled receptor (GPCR) channels.

      Abolishment of spike frequency adaptation itself, i.e., “increased spike activity” can increase [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> because [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> is entirely dependent on the spike activity as shown by [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> imaging method in Figure S3.

      Thus, α2A-AR internalization can increase [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> through the abolishment of autoinhibition or spike frequency adaptation, and a [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> increase drives MAO-A activation as reported previously (Cao et al., 2007, BMC Neurosci). The mechanism how Ca<sup>2+</sup> activates MAO-A is beyond the scope of the current study.

      Our study just focused on the mechanism how chronic or sever stress can cause persistent overexcitation and how it results in LC degeneration.

      (3) The connection between α2A-AR internalization and increased cytosolic NA levels lacks direct quantification, which is necessary to validate the proposed mechanism.

      Direct quantification of the relationship between α2A-AR internalization and increased cytosolic NA levels may not be possible, and may not be necessarily needed to be demonstrated as explained below.

      The internalization of α2A-AR can increase [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> through the abolishment of autoinhibition or spike frequency adaptation, and [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> increases can facilitate NA autocrine (Huang et al., 2007), similar to the transmitter release from nerve terminals (Kaeser & Regehr, 2014, Annu Rev Physiol).

      Autocrine released NA must be re-uptaken by NAT (NA transporter), which is firmly established (Torres et al., 2003, Nat Rev Neurosci). Re-uptake of NA by NAT is the only source of intracellular NA, and NA re-uptake by NAT should be increased as the internalization of NA biding site (α2A-AR) progresses in association with [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> increases (see page 11, lines 334-336).

      Thus, the connection between α2A-AR internalization and increased cytosolic NA levels is logically compelling, and the quantification of such connection may not be possible at present (see the response to the comment made by the Reviewer #1 as Recommendations for the authors (2) and beyond the scope of our current study.

      (4) The chronic stress model needs further validation, including measurements of stress-induced physiological changes (e.g., corticosterone levels) to rule out systemic effects that may influence LC activity. Additional behavioral assays for spatial memory impairment should also be included, as a single behavioral test is insufficient to confirm memory dysfunction.

      It is well established that restraint stress (RS) increases corticosterone levels depending on the period of RS (García-Iglesias et al., 2014, Neuropharmacology), although we are not reluctant to measure the corticosterone levels. In addition, there are numerous reports that showed the increased activity of LC neurons in response to various stresses (Valentino et al., 1983; Valentino and Foote, 1988; Valentino et al., 2001; McCall et al., 2015), as described in the text (page 4, lines 96-98). Measurement of cortisol levels may not be able to rule out systemic effects of CRS on the whole brain.

      We had already done another behavioral test using elevated plus maze (EPM) test.By combining the two tests, it may be possible to more accurately evaluate the results of Y-maze test by differentiating the memory impairment from anxiety. However, the results obtained by these behavioral tests are just supplementary to our current aim to elucidate the cellular mechanisms for the accumulation of cytosolic free NA. Therefore, we have softened the implication of anxiety and memory impairment (page 13, lines 397-400 in the revised manuscript).

      (5) Beyond b-arrestin binding, the role of alternative internalization pathways (e.g., phosphorylation, ubiquitination) in α2A-AR desensitization should be considered, as current evidence is insufficient to establish a purely Ca<sup>2+</sup> -dependent mechanism.

      We can hardly agree with this comment. 

      It was clearly demonstrated that repeated application of NA itself did not cause desensitization of α2A-AR (Figure S1A-D), and that the blockade of b-arrestin binding by barbadin completely suppressed the Ca<sup>2a</sup>-dependent downregulation of GIRK (Figure 2G-K). These observations can clearly rule out the possible involvement of phosphorylation or ubiquitination for the desensitization.

      Not only the barbadin experiment, but also the immunohistochemistry and western blot method clearly demonstrated the decrease of α2A-AR expression on the cell membrane (Figure 3).

      Ca<sup>2+</sup>-dependent mechanism of the rundown of GIRK was convincingly demonstrated by a set of different protocols of voltage-clamp study, in which Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx was differentially increased. The rundown of GIRK-I was orderly potentiated or accelerated by increasing the number of positive command pulses each of which induces Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx (compare Figure S1E-J, Figure S2A-E and Figure S2F-K along with Figure 2A-F). The presence or absence of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents and the amount of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents determined the trend of the rundown of GIRK-I (Figures 2, S1 and S2). Because the same voltage protocol hardly caused the rundown when it did not induce Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents in the absence of TEA (Figure S1F; compare with Figure 2B), blockade of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents by nifedipine would not be so beneficial.

      We believe the series of voltage-clamp protocols convincingly demonstrated the orderly involvement of [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> in accelerating the rundown of GIRK-I.

      (6) NA leakage for free NA accumulation is also influenced by NAT or VMAT2. Please discuss the potential role of VMAT2 in NA accumulation within the LC in AD. 

      It has been demonstrated that reduced VMAT2 levels increased susceptibility to neuronal damage: VMAT2 heterozygote mice displayed increased vulnerability to MPTP as evidenced by reductions in nigral dopamine cell counts (Takahashi et al, 1997, PNAS). Thus, when the activity of VMAT2 in LC neurons were impaired by chronic restraint stress, cytosolic NA levels in LC neurons would increase. We have added such discussion in the revised manuscript (page 12, lines 381-384).

      (7) Since the LC is a small brain region, proper staining is required to differentiate it from surrounding areas. Please provide a detailed explanation of the methodology used to define LC regions and how LC neurons were selected among different cell types in brain slices for whole-cell recordings.

      LC neurons were identified immunohistochemically and electrophysiologically as we previously reported (see Fig. 2 in Front. Cell. Neurosci. 16:841239. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2022.841239). We have added this explanation in the method section of the revised manuscript (page 15, lines 474-475). A delayed spiking pattern in response to depolarizing pulses (Figure S10 in the revised manuscript) applied at a hyperpolarized membrane potential was commonly observed in LC neurons in many studies (Masuko et al., 1986; van den Pol et al., 2002; Wagner-Altendorf et al., 2019).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The manuscript reports that chronic stress for 5 days increases MAO-A levels in LC neurons, leading to the production of DOPEGAL, activation of AEP, and subsequent tau cleavage into the tau N368 fragment, ultimately contributing to neuronal damage. However, the authors used wild-type C57BL/6 mice, and previous literature has indicated that AEP-mediated tau cleavage in wild-type mice is minimal and generally insufficient to cause significant behavioral alterations. Please clarify and discuss this apparent discrepancy.

      In our study, normalized relative value of AEP-mediated tau cleavage (Tau N368) was much higher in CRS mice than non-stress wild-type mice. It is not possible to compare AEP-mediated tau cleavage between our non-stress wild type mice and those observed in previous study (Zhang et al., 2014, Nat Med), because band intensity is largely dependent on the exposure time and its numerical value is the normalized relative value. In view of such differences, our apparent band expression might have been intensified to detect small changes.

      (2) It is recommended that the authors include additional experiments to examine the effects of different durations and intensities of stress on MAO-A expression and AEP activity. This would strengthen the understanding of stress-induced biochemical changes and their thresholds.

      GIRK rundown was almost saturated after 3-day RS and remained the same in 5-day RS mice (Fig. 4A-G), which is consistent with the downregulation of α2A-AR and GIRK1 expression by 3-day RS (Fig. 3C, F and G; Fig. 4J and K). However, we examined the protein levels of MAO-A, pro/active-AEP and Tau N368 only in 5-day RS mice without examining in 3-day RS mice. This is because we considered the possibility that a high [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> condition may have to be sustained for some period of time to induce changes in MAO-A, AEP and Tau N368, and therefore 3-day RS may be insufficient to induce such changes. We have added this in the revised manuscript (page 17, lines 521-525).

      (3) Please clarify the rationale for the inconsistent stress durations used across Figures 3, 4, and 5. In some cases, a 3-day stress protocol is used, while in others, a 5-day protocol is applied. This discrepancy should be addressed to ensure clarity and experimental consistency.

      Please see our response to the comment (2).

      (4) The abbreviation "vMAT2" is incorrectly formatted. It should be "VMAT2," and the full name (vesicular monoamine transporter 2) should be provided at first mention.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have revised accordingly.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Weaknesses:

      Nevertheless, the manuscript currently reads as a sequence of discrete experiments rather than a single causal chain. Below, I outline the key points that should be addressed to make the model convincing.

      Please see the responses to the recommendation for the authors made by reviewer #3.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Improve the clarity and organization of the manuscript, ensuring smoother transitions between concepts and mechanisms.

      Please see the response to the comment raised by Reviewer #1 as Weakness

      (2) Adjust any quantifying method for cytosolic NA levels under different conditions to support the link between receptor internalization and NA accumulation.

      If fluorescent indicator of cytosolic free NA is available, it would be possible to measure changes in cytosolic NA levels. However, at present, there appeared to be no fluorescence probe to label cytosolic NA. For example, NS521 labels both dopamine and norepinephrine inside neurosecretory vesicles (Hettie & Glass et al., 2014, Chemistry), and BPS3 fluorescence sensor labels NA around cell membrane by anchoring on the cell membrane (Mao et al., 2023, Nat Comm). Furthermore, the method reported in “A Genetically Encoded Fluorescent Sensor for Rapid and Specific In Vivo Detection of Norepinephrine” is limited to detect NA only when α2AR is expressed. In the present study, increases in cytosolic NA levels are caused by internalization of α2AR. Cytosolic NA measurements with GRAB NE photometry may not be applicable in the present study. However, we have discussed the availability of such fluorescent methods to directly prove the increase in cytosolic NA as a limitation of our study (page 14, lines 429-436 in the revised manuscript).

      (3) Include validation of the chronic stress model with physiological and behavioral measures (e.g., corticosterone levels and another behavioral test).

      Please see the response to the comment raised by Reviewer #1 as Weakness (4).

      (4) All supplemental figures should be explicitly explained in the Results section. Specifically, clarify and describe the details of Figure S1G-K, Figure S2F-K, Figure S3A-H, Figure S4A-F, Figure S5, and Figure S6 to ensure all supplementary data are fully integrated into the main text.

      We have more explicitly and clearly described the details of Figure S1E-J, Figure S2F-K, Figure S3A-H, Figure S4A-F, Figure S5, and Figure S6 and fully integrated those explanations into the main text in the revised manuscript.

      (5) In Figure 3, the morphology of TH-positive cells differs between panels D and E. Additionally, TH is typically expressed in the cytosol, but in the provided images, it appears to be localized only to the membrane. Please clarify this discrepancy and provide a lower-magnification image to display a larger area, not one cell.

      In a confocal image, TH is not necessarily expressed homogenously in the cytosol, but is expressed in a ring-shaped pattern inside the plasma membrane, avoiding the cell nucleus and its surrounding Golgi apparatus and endoplasmic reticulum (ER) (Henrich et al., 2018, Acta Neuropathol Commun; see Fig. 4a and 6e), especially when the number of z-stack of confocal images is small. This is presumably because LC neurons are especially enriched with numerous Golgi apparatus and ER (Groves & Wilson, 1980, J Comp Neurol).

      In Figure S7, we showed a lower-magnification image of LC and its adjacent area (mesencephalic trigeminal nucleus). In the LC area, there are a variety of LC neurons, which include oval shaped neurons (open arrowhead; similar to Figure 3D) and also rhombus-like shaped neurons (open double arrowheads, similar to Figure 3E). A much lower-magnification image of LC neurons constituting LC nucleus was shown in Figure 5A.

      (6) In Figure 5, the difference in MAO-A expression is not clearly visible in the fluorescence images. Enzymatic assays for AEP and MAO-A should be included to demonstrate the increased activity better.

      In the current study, we did not elaborate to detect the changes in TH, MAO-A and AEP in terms of immunohistochemical method. Instead, we elaborated to detect such changes in terms of western blot method. The main conclusions in the current study were drawn primarily by electrophysiological techniques as we have expended much effort on electrophysiological experiments. Because the relative quantification of active AEP and Tau N368 proteins by western blotting analysis may accurately reflect changes in those enzyme activities, enzymatic assay may not be necessarily required but is helpful to better demonstrate AEP and MAO-A activity. We have described the necessity of enzymatic assay to better demonstrate the AEP and MAO-A activities (page 10, lines 314-315).

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Causality across the pathway

      Each step (α2A internalisation, GIRK rundown, Ca<sup>2+</sup> rise, MAO-A/AEP upregulation) is demonstrated separately, but no experiment links them in a single preparation. Consider in vivo Ca<sup>2+</sup> or GRAB NE photometry during restraint stress while probing α2A levels with i.p. clonidine injection or optogenetic over excitation coupled to biochemical readouts. Such integrated evidence would help to overcome the correlational nature of the manuscript to a more mechanistic study.

      It is not possible to measure free cytosolic NA levels with GRAB NE photometry when α2A AR is internalized as described above (see the response to the comment made by reviewer #1 as the recommendation for the authors).

      (2) Pharmacology and NE concentration

      The use of 100 µM noradrenaline saturates α and β adrenergic receptors alike. Please provide ramp measurements of GIRK current in dose-response at 1-10 µM NE (blocked by atipamezole) to confirm that the rundown really reflects α2A activity rather than mixed receptor effects.

      It is true that 100 µM noradrenaline activates both α and β adrenergic receptors alike. However, it was clearly showed that enhancement of GIRK-I by 100 µM noradrenaline was completely antagonized by 10 µM atipamezole and the Ca<sup>2+</sup> dependent rundown of NA-induced GIRK-I was prevented by 10 µM atipamezole. Considering the Ki values of atipamezole for α2A AR (=1~3 nM) (Vacher et al., 2010, J Med Chem) and β AR (>10 µM) (Virtanen et al., 1989, Arch Int Pharmacodyn Ther), these results really reflect α2A AR activity but not β AR activity (Figure S5). Furthermore, because it is already well established that NA-induced GIRK-I was mediated by α2A AR activity in LC neurons (Arima et al., 1998, J Physiol; Williams et al., 1985, Neuroscience), it is not necessarily need to re-examine 1-10 µM NA on GIRK-I.

      (3) Calcium dependence is not yet definitive

      The rundown is induced with a TEA-enhanced pulse protocol. Blocking L-type channels with nifedipine (or using Cd²⁺) during this protocol should show whether Ca<sup>2+</sup> entry is necessary. Without such a control, the Ca<sup>2+</sup> link remains inferential.

      The Ca<sup>2+</sup> link was precisely demonstrated by a series of voltage clamp experiment, in which Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx was orderly potentiated by increasing the number of positive voltage pulses (Figures S1 and S2). As the number of positive voltage pulses was increased, the rundown of GIRK-I was accelerated or enhanced more. The relationship between the number of spikes and the Ca<sup>2+</sup> influx detected as Ca<sup>2+</sup> transients was well documented in Ca2+ imaging experiments using fura-2 (Figure S3).

      The presence or absence of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents and the amount of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents determined the trend of the rundown of GIRK-I (Figs. 2, S1 and S2). The same voltage protocol hardly caused the rundown when it did not induce Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents in the absence of TEA (Fig. S1F; compare with Fig. 2B), and the series of voltage-clamp protocols convincingly demonstrated the orderly involvement of [Ca<sup>2+</sup>]<sub>i</sub> in accelerating the rundown of GIRK-I. Therefore, blockade of Ca<sup>2+</sup> currents by nifedipine may not be so beneficial.

      (4) Age mismatch and disease claims

      All electrophysiology and biochemical data come from juvenile (< P30) mice, yet the conclusions stress Alzheimer-related degeneration. Key endpoints need to be replicated in adult or aged mice, or the manuscript should soften its neurodegenerative scope.

      As described in the section of Conclusion, we never stress Alzheimer-related degeneration, but might give such an impression. To avoid such a misunderstanding, we have added a description “However, the present mechanism must be proven to be valid in adult or old mice, to validate its involvement in the pathogenesis of AD.” (page 14, lines 448-450).

      (5) Direct evidence for extracellular/cytosolic NE

      The proposed rise in reuptake NA is inferred from electrophysiology. Modern fluorescent sensors (GRAB NE, nLight) or fast scan voltammetry could quantify NE overflow and clearance during stress, directly testing the model.

      Please see the response to the comment made by Reviewer #1 as the Recommendations for the authors (2) as described above.

      (6) Quantitative histology

      Figure 5 presents attractive images but no numerical analysis. Please provide ROI-based fluorescence quantification (with n values) or move the images to the supplement and rely on the Western blots.

      We have moved the immunohistochemical results in Fig. 5 to the supplement as we believe the quantification of immunohistochemical staining is not necessarily correct.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1:

      The chosen classification scheme for aGPCRs may require reassessment and amendment by the authors in order to prevent confusion with previously issued classification attempts of this family. (…) Can the authors suggest another scheme (mind to avoid the subfamily IIX or the alternative ADGRA-G,L,V subfamily schemes of metazoan aGPCRs), and adapt their numbering throughout the text and all figures/supplementary figures/supplementary files?

      We appreciate the reviewer's comment and agree that a different nomenclature should be used for choanoflagellate aGPCRs to avoid possible confusion. We have now re-labeled the choanoflagellate aGPCR subfamilies, previously numbered from I to XIX, using alphabetical enumeration (from A to S). Changes have been made throughout the main text, in Figure 5, and in Supplementary Figures S6 and S7.

      line 10: The abbreviation 'GPCR-TKL/Ks' is not explained.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have now revised the text to explain the abbreviation:

      “Adhesion GPCRs and a class of GPCRs fused to kinases (the GPCR-TKL/Ks) are the most abundant GPCRs in choanoflagellates.”

      line 30: "7TM domain is diagnostic for GPCRs": strange wording. Use an alternative expression.

      We changed the wording to: 

      “A conserved seven transmembrane (7TM) domain is a hallmark of GPCRs, while the wide spectrum of extracellular and intracellular domains in some GPCRs reflects the diversification of the gene family and its functions (Schiöth and Lagerström 2008).”

      line 33: In the case of rhodopsins, not the GPCR (i.e., the apoprotein) responds directly to photons, but the retinal, which isomerises upon illumination.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing this to our attention, and we have now removed mention of photons from the list of cues detected by GPCRs.

      “For example, the extracellular N-terminus and the three extracellular loops of the 7TM domain respond to a wide range of cues, including odorant molecules, peptides, amines, lipids, nucleotides, and other molecules (Yang et al. 2021).”

      line 111: What are "genome-enabled choanoflagellates"? Explain the term. As it stands, it doesn't make sense to me.

      We meant only to highlight that these two species have sequenced genomes. We have deleted the phrase “genome enabled.”

      “To assess the predictive power of our protein-detection pipeline, we then compared the new GPCR and cytosolic signaling component datasets from two choanoflagellates – Salpingoeca rosetta and Monosiga brevicollis – with previously published GPCR and downstream GPCR signaling component counts for these two species (Nordström et al. 2009a; Krishnan et al. 2012; De Mendoza et al. 2014; Krishnan et al. 2015; Lokits et al. 2018).”

      line 145: Please give a reasoning for the naming of each of the new families (e.g., RemiSens, Hidden Gold, GPCR-TLK/K, etc.) or at least the explanations of the acronyms/names early in the manuscript, even if they are discussed later in more detail.

      Thank you for identifying this as an area of confusion. While we feel that going into the rationale behind each of the names here would interrupt the flow of the manuscript, we have added a phrase encouraging readers to “hold that thought” with the hope that they can wait for the sections that specifically focus on each of these new GPCR families.

      “This left twelve new GPCR families that had not, to our knowledge, been previously detected in choanoflagellates: Rhodopsin, TMEM145, GPR180, TMEM87, GPR155, GPR157, and six additional GPCR families that appear to fall outside all previously characterized GPCR families in eukaryotes. For reasons that will be discussed further below, we have named these six new GPCR families “Rémi-Sans-Famille” (RSF), “Hidden Gold” (Hi-GOLD), GPCR-TKL/K, GPRch1, GPRch2, and GPRch3. (Fig. 1B; Table 1).”

      lines 297/298 and 2049: Rename tethered agonist "peptide" to "element". Synthetic peptides resembling the TA were used in experiments to test for the sufficiency of the TA for receptor activation, but because the naturally occurring TAs are part of the receptor protein, they are not peptides.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have revised the text as suggested.

      line 2026: I think the letters in the acronym "CMR" are mixed up and were intended to read "CRM".

      Good catch! We have corrected the text.

      line 2048: "diagnostic" again. Change to "tell-tale", "hallmark", or another similar descriptor.

      We have corrected the text accordingly.

      2058: Strike "motif" in order to avoid confusion with the now obsolete term "GPS motif", which entailed the five most C-terminal β-strands of GAIN subdomain B (not thus neither the full GAIN domain nor the GPS).

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have corrected the text.

      Figure 5: Did the authors also find homologs placed in the aGPCR family based on their 7TM domain sequence but lacking a GAIN domain similar to vertebrate ADGRA/GPR123, the only aGPCR known to lack a GAIN domain (10.1016/j.tips.2013.06.002)? Irrespective of the authors' findings or non-finding on that matter, please insert a note on this in the results text.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing this interesting point to our attention. We have now added a new supplementary figure A in Fig. S9 to answer the reviewer's comment. We also modified the legend of Fig. S9  to take into account this change and uploaded a new supplementary data file 20 to support Fig. S9A. Finally, we revised the main text under the section “Adhesion GPCRs” as requested: 

      Lines 328-331: “ While the GAIN and aGPCR 7TM domains evolved before the origin of opisthokonts (Araç et al.2012; Krishnan et al. 2012; De Mendoza et al. 2014), we detected the fusion of these two domains into a single module (GAIN/7TM) in most, but not all, holozoan aGPCRs (Fig. 5D, Fig.S7B and S9A; Supplementary file 20; Prömel et al, 2013; Krishnan et al. 2014).

      Reviewer #2:

      While the study contributes several interesting observations, it does not radically revise the evolutionary history of the GPCR family. However, in an era increasingly concerned with the reproducibility of scientific findings, this is arguably a strength rather than a weakness. It is encouraging to see that previously established patterns largely hold, and that with expanded sampling and improved methods, new insights can be gained, especially at the level of specific GPCR subfamilies. Then, no functional follow-ups are provided in the model system Salpingoeca rosetta, but I am sure functional work on GPCRs in choanoflagellates is set to reveal very interesting molecular adaptations in the future.

      We agree with the reviewer and anticipate that this work will provide a useful resource to motivate the future functional characterization of GPCRs in choanoflagellates, other CRMs, as well as in metazoans.

      The GPCR-TKL fusion is a particularly interesting finding, especially given the presence of such sequences in sponges. This could potentially represent a synapomorphy shared between sponges and choanoflagellates, later lost in other animals. The authors mention that BLASTP searches using the kinase domain recover the sponge GPCR-TKLs, suggesting the fusion may be ancestral. It would be useful to include phylogenetic trees of both the GPCR and TKL domains to assess this possibility. The authors might also consider examining sponge genomes released by the DTOL project to increase representation from this group.

      We agree and thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have now added the requested phylogenetic analyses to the new Figure S17, revised the supplementary files and Methods accordingly, and commented on these results in the main text under the section “GPCR-TKL/K and GPCR-TKs“.  

      Lines 579 – 589: “While no metazoan homologs were found when using the 7TM domain of choanoflagellate GPCR-TKs as queries, using the conserved tyrosine kinase domains as queries recovered GPCR-TKs in sponges but not in other metazoan lineages or other holozoans (Fig. S17E). To test whether GPCR-TKs in sponges and choanoflagellates are homologous, we performed phylogenetic analyses of their TK and 7TM domains (Fig. S17F and G). While the TK domains of GPCR-TKs from sponges and choanoflagellates formed a well-supported clade, their 7TM domains did not. These results point to a heterogeneous evolutionary history that may include domain swapping (i.e. ancestral GPCR-TKs in which the 7TM domain was replaced in either the sponge or choanoflagellate lineages) or convergent evolution, in which homologous 7TM domains fused with unrelated 7TM domains in the sponge and choanoflagellate lineages.”

      Added to the Method section “Sequence alignment and phylogenetic analyses”:

      Lines 913 – 933: “Phylogenetic analyses of holozoan aGPCRs, Glutamate Receptors, and Gα subunits, and the 7TM and Kinase domains from GPCR TK/TKL/Ks were performed in this study. (…) To construct the phylogenies of the Kinase domain and 7TM domain from the GPCR TK/TKL/Ks, we first built a dataset including all the GPCR TK/TKL/Ks sequences identified in choanoflagellates and in sponges, as well as the GPCR TKL/Ks previously published in oomycetes and amoebozoans (Van Den Hoogen et al. 2018). We extracted the 7TM domain and Kinase domain from each sequence by combining the transmembrane domain prediction tool TMHMM-2.0 and the protein domain prediction tool InterProScan with the alignment tool MAFFT (E-INS-I algorithm) on Geneious Prime v2024.07 (Supplementary Files 30 and 32). We then aligned the aGPCR, Glutamate and Glutamate GPCR TK/TKL/K Receptor 7TMs, the GPCR TK/TKL/Ks Kinase domain, or the full-length Gα sequences using MAFFT with the E-INS-I algorithm. The resulting alignments were then used for Maximum-likelihood and/or Bayesian inference of phylogenies (Fig. 3B, Fig. 5A, Fig. S3D, and Fig. S6A, and Fig. S17F and G; Supplementary Files 5, 9, 16,18, 31, and 33).”

      Rhodopsin-like receptors are proposed in the discussion to be potential cases of lateral gene transfer (LGT) between eukaryotes. To support or refute this hypothesis, it would be valuable to place the choanoflagellate and ichthyosporean Rhodopsins within a broader phylogeny of this family, including (a few) representatives from animals and other eukaryotes. Even if deep branching relationships remain unresolved, signs such as unusually short branches could point toward recent LGT events.

      Thank you for your suggestion. While we originally considered testing these alternative hypotheses in this manuscript by building a phylogeny, the rapid sequence evolution of the Rhodopsin family has stymied similar efforts in the past and instead motivated others to use clustering approaches like those used in our study (Hu et al. 2017; Thiel et al. 2023). Unfortunately, these types of analyses cannot be used to readily identify instances of LGT.

      Therefore, following the suggestion of the reviewer, we bit the bullet and performed phylogenetic analyses on the sequences in question. Unfortunately, these analyses were completely inconclusive, and we feel they do not warrant inclusion in the manuscript. The topologies of the sequence trees recovered were poorly supported and sensitive to most of the variables we tested – the set of rhodopsin sequences included, the multiple alignment algorithms used, and the probabilistic methods employed to infer the phylogenies. 

      Instead, we have revised the manuscript to highlight the challenge of differentiating between the different hypotheses that are consistent with the phylogenetic distribution of Rhodopsins:

      Lines 670 – 678: “Thus, while it is formally possible that Rhodopsins existed in stem choanoflagellates and were lost in most modern choanoflagellate lineages, either horizontal gene transfer or convergent evolution in the shared ancestor of S. macrocollata and S. punica are similarly plausible explanations for their presence in these species. Differentiating between these alternative evolutionary scenarios is challenging because of rapid rate of sequence evolution within the family and the resultant loss of phylogenetic signal. Our own preliminary investigations of Rhodopsin evolution in non-metazoans were inconclusive. Therefore, ambiguities about the provenance and function of CRM Rhodopsins currently obscure the ancestry of metazoan Rhodopsins and opsins.”

      While the study surveys most available holozoan genomes, it appears that the genomes of Amoebidium spp.-which are cited in the manuscript- were not included. It may not be necessary to repeat all analyses with these two species (A. appalachense and A. parasiticum), but a preliminary search indicates the presence of four candidate 7tm_1 (Rhodopsin-like) proteins in their proteomes. These may warrant closer inspection (e.g., via BLASTP against animal databases) to confirm whether they are genuine GPCRs or false positives.

      Author response image 1.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing these sequences to our attention. To be clear, we did not analyze the Amoebidium spp. genome and we can find no reference to it in our manuscript. If the reviewer had the impression that the genome was analyzed, we would be grateful to know the source of the confusion so that it can be corrected. (We did not intentionally exclude the genome; it simply was not available on the Multicell Genome database from which we retrieved the ichthyosporean genomes and transcriptomes used in this study.)

      Nevertheless, out of curiosity, we have now analyzed the sequences provided by the reviewer and summarize our findings here for the interest of the reviewer. Although the sequences were annotated as 7tm_1 (Rhodopsin-like) proteins in the original genome study, none of these sequences group with metazoan or choanoflagellate Rhodopsins in our clustering analysis; instead, we found that these putative GPCRs form a distinct cluster that only weakly resembles cAMP receptors, both on the basis of their sequence and predicted structures. 

      It is not surprising to find new GPCR clusters as new taxa are folded into the study, and these Amoebidium sequences do not add to our understanding of Rhodopsin evolution. Therefore, we have not added their analysis to the manuscript, but we hope the reviewer finds our quick analysis of interest.

      Author response image 2.

      In Figure 2, perhaps expanding the other holozoan clades would have been nice, as there are not too many species, but I understand if that's beyond the point of the manuscript, focused on choanoflagellates.

      Thank you for this comment. However, given the focus of this study, we feel that an expansion of the other holozoan clades would reduce the clarity of the figure.

      line 87 - "To this end, the 671 validated choanoflagellate GPCRs were sorted by sequence similarity, resulting in 18 clusters. "Some details in the results section would be nice, or at least clear references to where this is explained in more detail. How were the extra choanoflagellate GPCRs added if they failed to be identified with quite sensitive HMM profiles?

      We apologize for the possible confusion and thank the reviewer for the suggestion; we have now added specific references to the related sections from the material and methods for interested readers.

      We believe that the "extra choanoflagellate GPCRs" mentioned by the reviewer refer to the choanoflagellate GPCRs that failed to be detected when the choanoflagellate genomes and transcriptomes were searched with the predominantly metazoan-derived GPCRHMM and HMMs from the GPCR_A Pfam clan (CL0192). We were able to recover these extra choanoflagellate GPCRs by using custom choanoflagellate-specific GPCR HMMs and by blasting the choanoflagellate GPCRs previously identified as queries against the 23 choanoflagellate proteomes. We hope that the referencing of the Methods section "Recovering additional choanoflagellate GPCRs using choanoflagellate GPCR BLAST queries and custom choanoflagellate GPCR HMMs", in lines 91 and 93, will help clarify this point.

      line 108 - Well, from the figure it seems that most eukaryotes have an 'animal-like' G protein signalling, so that's perhaps more of an eukaryotic signature than something that puts choanoflagellates and animals together.

      Excellent point! We have revised the text.

      line 132 - It is unclear what the criteria are to include these taxa as helpers for choanoflagellate classification, and not adding the other unicellular holozoans. Just some text justification could help.

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have added an explanation of the rationale to the methods — section “Clustering of the 918 validated choanoflagellate GPCRs” — and referred to it in the main text.

      New text added to methods:

      “The non-choanoflagellate sequences added to the dataset were either top blast hits recovered after searching the entire Eukprot v3 dataset (993 species) with choanoflagellate GPCRs as queries, or previously published and well-documented GPCR sequences from metazoans.”

      line 145 - These families are listed, but perhaps it would be nice to explicitly mention that they will be covered in more detail later on in the manuscript. I found myself wondering about those exotic names, until I reached the sections in the manuscript where they are explained.

      Thank you for this suggestion. We have now modified our sentence to refer to the related sections.

      “For reasons that will be discussed further below, we have named these six new GPCR families “Rémi-Sans-Famille” (RSF), “Hidden Gold” (Hi-GOLD), GPCR-TKL/K, GPRch1, GPRch2, and GPRch3. (Fig. 1B; Table 1).”

      line 199 - perhaps would be nice to explain domain architecture of validated Dictyostelium GABA-like receptors (ANF domain?).

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have now modified the sentence to mention the protein domain composition of the validated GABA-like receptor, GrlE, in Dictyostelium.

      “The Glutamate Receptors from the amoebozan Dictyostelium discoideum, of which at least one, GrlE, binds both GABA and Glutamate presumably through its conserved ANF domain (Anjard and Loomis 2006; Taniura et al. 2006; Wu and Janetopoulos 2013), grouped separately from metazoan and CRM GPCRs in our analysis.”

      Figure S4 - Perhaps a stacked bar chart would be easier to browse than a bunch of pie charts, notoriously difficult to quantify.

      Thank you for this comment. Opinions differ on how best on whether pie charts or bar charts are more effective in this context (including between the authors of this manuscript). However, we think the point of Figure S4 a minor point, only to be appreciated by a tiny number of readers, and therefore have left the data presentation as it was in the original submission.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      We thank the reviewers for the constructive comments, which have improved the manuscript. In response to these comments, we have made the following major changes to the main text and reviewer response:

      (1) Added experimental and computational evidence to support the use of Cut&Tag to determine speckle location.

      (2) Performed new Transmission Electron Microscopy (TEM) experiments to visualize interchromatin granule clusters +/- speckle degradation.

      (3) Altered the text of the manuscript to remove qualitative statements and clarify effect sizes.

      (4) Performed new analyses of published whole genome bisulfite data from LIMe-Hi-C following DNMT1 inhibition to demonstrate that CpG methylation is lost at DNMT1i-specific gained CTCF sites.

      (5) Included citations for relevant literature throughout the text.

      These revisions in addition to others are described in the point-by-point response below.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary

      Roseman et al. use a new inhibitor of the maintenance DNA methyltransferase DNMT1 to probe the role of methylation on binding of the CTCF protein, which is known to be involved chromatin loop formation. As previous reported, and as expected based on our knowledge that CTCF binding is methylation-sensitive, the authors find that loss of methylation leads to additional CTCF binding sites and increased loop formation. By comparing novel loops with the binding of the pre-mRNA splicing factor SON, which localizes to the nuclear speckle compartment, they propose that these reactivated loops localize to near speckles. This behavior is dependent on CTCF whereas degradation of two speckle proteins does not affect CTCF binding or loop formation. The authors propose a model in which DNA methylation controls the association of genome regions with speckles via CTCF-mediated insulation.

      Strengths

      The strengths of the study are 1) the use of a new, specific DNMT1 inhibitor and 2) the observation that genes whose expression is sensitive to DNMT1 inhibition and dependent on CTCF (cluster 2) show higher association with SON than genes which are sensitive to DNMT1 inhibition but are CTCF insensitive, is in line with the authors' general model.

      Weaknesses

      There are a number of significant weaknesses that as a whole undermine many of the key conclusions, including the overall mechanistic model of a direct regulatory role of DNA methylation on CTCF-mediated speckle association of chromatin loops.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s constructive comments and address them point-by-point below.

      (1) The authors frequently make quasi-quantitative statements but do not actually provide the quantitative data, which they actually all have in hand. To give a few examples: "reactivated CTCF sites were largely methylated (p. 4/5), "many CTCF binding motifs enriched..." (p.5), "a large subset of reactivated peaks..."(p.5), "increase in strength upon DNMT1 inhibition" (p.5); "a greater total number....." (p.7). These statements are all made based on actual numbers and the authors should mention the numbers in the text to give an impression of the extent of these changes (see below) and to clarify what the qualitative terms like "largely", "many", "large", and "increase" mean. This is an issue throughout the manuscript and not limited to the above examples.

      Related to this issue, many of the comparisons which the authors interpret to show differences in behavior seem quite minor. For example, visual inspection suggests that the difference in loop strength shown in figure 1E is something like from 0 to 0.1 for K562 cells and a little less for KCT116 cells. What is a positive control here to give a sense of whether these minor changes are relevant. Another example is on p. 7, where the authors claim that CTCF partners of reactivated peaks tend to engage in a "greater number" of looping partners, but inspection of Figure 2A shows a very minor difference from maybe 7 to 7.5 partners. While a Mann-Whitney test may call this difference significant and give a significant P value, likely due to high sample number, it is questionable that this is a biologically relevant difference.

      We have amended the text to include actual values, instead of just qualitative statements. We have also moderated our claims in the text to note where effect sizes are more modest.

      The following literature examples can serve as positive controls for the effect sizes that we might expect when perturbing CTCF. Our observed effect sizes are largely in line with these expected magnitudes.

      https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC8386078/ Fig. 2E

      https://www.cell.com/cell-reports/pdf/S2211-1247(23)01674-1.pdf Fig. 3J,K

      https://academic.oup.com/nar/article/52/18/10934/7740592 Fig. S5D (CTCF binding only).

      (2) The data to support the central claim of localization of reactivated loops to speckles is not overly convincing. The overlap with SON Cut&Tag (figure 2F) is partial at best and although it is better with the publicly available TSA-seq data, the latter is less sensitive than Cut&Tag and more difficult to interpret. It would be helpful to validate these data with FISH experiments to directly demonstrate and measure the association of loops with speckles (see below).

      A recent publication we co-authored validated the use of speckle (SON) Cut&Run using FISH (Yu et al, NSMB 2025, doi: 10.1038/s41594-024-01465-6). This paper also supports a role of CTCF in positioning DNA near speckles. Unfortunately, the resolution of these FISH probes is in the realm of hundreds of kilobases. This was not an issue for Yu et. al., as they were looking at large-scale effects of CTCF degradation on positioning near speckles. However, FISH does not provide the resolution we need to look at more localized changes over methylation-specific peak sites.

      Instead, we use Cut&Tag to look at these high-resolution changes. In Figure 3C, we show that SON localizes to DNMT1i-specific peaks only upon DNMT1 inhibition. We further demonstrate that this interaction is dependent on CTCF. In response to reviewer comments, we have now also performed spike-in normalized Cut&Tag upon acute (6 hr) SON degradation to validate that our signal is also directly dependent on SON and not merely due to a bias toward open chromatin.

      Author response image 1.

      TSA-seq has been validated with FISH (Chen et. al., doi: 10.1083/jcb.201807108), Alexander et. Al 10.1016/j.molcel.2021.03.006) Fig 6. We include TSA-seq data where possible in our manuscript to support our claims.

      We also note that Fig 2F shows all CTCF peaks and loops, not just methylation-sensitive peaks and loops, to give a sense of the data. We apologize for any confusion and have clarified this in the figure legend.

      (3) It is not clear that the authors have indeed disrupted speckles from cells by degrading SON and SRRM2. Speckles contain a large number of proteins and considering their phase separated nature stronger evidence for their complete removal is needed. Note that the data published in ref 58 suffers from the same caveat.

      Based upon the reviewers’ feedback, we generated Tranmission electron microscopy (TEM) data to visualize nuclear speckles +/- degradation of SON and SRRM2 (DMSO and dTAG). We were able to detect Interchromatin Granules Clusters (ICGs) that are representative of nuclear speckles in the DMSO condition. However, even at baseline, we observed a large degree of cell-to-cell variability in these structures. In addition, we also observe potential structural changes in the distribution of heterochromatin upon speckle degradation. Consequently, we hesitate to make quantitative conclusions regarding loss of these nuclear bodies. In the interest of transparency, we have included representative raw images from both conditions for the reviewers’ consideration.

      We also note that in Ref 58 (Ilik et. Al., https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.60579), the authors show diffusion of speckle client proteins RBM25, SRRM1, and PNN upon SON and SRRM2 depletion, further supporting speckle dissociation in these conditions.

      Author response image 2.

      Author response image 3.

      (4) The authors ascribe a direct regulatory role to DNA methylation in controlling the association of some CTCF-mediated loops to speckles (p. 20). However, an active regulatory role of speckle association has not been demonstrated and the observed data are equally explainable by a more parsimonious model in which DNA methylation regulates gene expression via looping and that the association with speckles is merely an indirect bystander effect of the activated genes because we know that active genes are generally associated with speckles. The proposed mechanism of a regulatory role of DNA methylation in controlling speckle association is not convincingly demonstrated by the data. As a consequence, the title of the paper is also misleading.

      While it is difficult to completely rule out indirect effects, we do not believe that the relationship between methylation-sensitive CTCF sites and speckles relies only on gene activity.

      We can partially decouple SON Cut&Tag signal from gene activation if we break down Figure 4D to look only at methylation-sensitive CTCF peaks on genes whose expression is unchanged upon DNMT1 inhibition (using thresholds from manuscript, P-adj > 0.05 and/or |log2(fold-change)| < 0.5). This analysis shows that many methylation-sensitive CTCF peaks on genes with unchanged expression still change speckle association upon DNMT1 inhibition. This result refutes the necessity of transcriptional activation to recruit speckles to CTCF.

      Author response image 4.

      We note the comparator upregulated gene set here is small (~20 genes with our stringent threshold for methylation-sensitive CTCF after 1 day DNMT1i treatment).

      However, we acknowledge that these effects cannot be completely disentangled. We previously included the statement “other features enriched near speckles, such as open chromatin, high GC content, and active gene expression, could instead contribute to increased CTCF binding and looping near speckles” in the discussion. In response to the reviewer’s comment, we have further tempered our statements on page 20/21 and also added a statement noting that DNA demethylation and gene activation cannot be fully disentangled. While we are also open to a title change, we are unsure which part of the title is problematic. 

      (5) As a minor point, the authors imply on p. 15 that ablation of speckles leads to misregulation of genes by altering transcription. This is not shown as the authors only measure RNA abundance, which may be affected by depletion of constitutive splicing factors, but not transcription. The authors would need to show direct effects on transcription.

      We agree, and we have changed this wording to say RNA abundance.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      CTCF is one of the most well-characterized regulators of chromatin architecture in mammals. Given that CTCF is an essential protein, understanding how its binding is regulated is a very active area of research. It has been known for decades that CTCF is sensitive to 5-cystosine DNA methylation (5meC) in certain contexts. Moreover, at genomic imprints and in certain oncogenes, 5meC-mediated CTCF antagonism has very important gene regulatory implications. A number of labs (eg, Schubeler and Stamatoyannopoulos) have assessed the impact of DNA methylation on CTCF binding, but it is important to also interrogate the effect on chromatin organization (ie, looping). Here, Roseman and colleagues used a DNMT1 inhibitor in two established human cancer lines (HCT116 [colon] and K562 [leukemia]), and performed CTCF ChIPseq and HiChIP. They showed that "reactivated" CTCF sites-that is, bound in the absence of 5meC-are enriched in gene bodies, participate in many looping events, and intriguingly, appear associated with nuclear speckles. This last aspect suggests that these reactivated loops might play an important role in increased gene transcription. They showed a number of genes that are upregulated in the DNA hypomethylated state actually require CTCF binding, which is an important result.

      Strengths:

      Overall, I found the paper to be succinctly written and the data presented clearly. The relationship between CTCF binding in gene bodies and association with nuclear speckles is an interesting result. Another strong point of the paper was combining DNMT1 inhibition with CTCF degradation.

      Weaknesses:

      The most problematic aspect of this paper in my view is the insufficient evidence for the association of "reactivated" CTCF binding sites with nuclear speckles needs to be more diligently demonstrated (see Major Comment). One unfortunate aspect was that this paper neglected to discuss findings from our recent paper, wherein we also performed CTCF HiChIP in a DNA methylation mutant (Monteagudo-Sanchez et al., 2024 PMID: 39180406). It is true, this is a relatively recent publication, although the BioRxiv version has been available since fall 2023. I do not wish to accuse the authors of actively disregarding our study, but I do insist that they refer to it in a revised version. Moreover, there are a number of differences between the studies such that I find them more complementary rather than overlapping. To wit, the species (mouse vs human), the cell type (pluripotent vs human cancer), the use of a CTCF degron, and the conclusions of the paper (we did not make a link with nuclear speckles). Furthermore, we used a constitutive DNMT knockout which is not viable in most cell types (HCT116 cells being an exception), and in the discussion mentioned the advantage of using degron technology:

      "With high-resolution techniques, such as HiChIP or Micro-C (119-121), a degron system can be coupled with an assessment of the cis-regulatory interactome (118). Such techniques could be adapted for DNA methylation degrons (eg, DNMT1) in differentiated cell types in order to gauge the impact of 5meC on the 3D genome."

      The authors here used a DNMT1 inhibitor, which for intents and purposes, is akin to a DNMT1 degron, thus I was happy to see a study employ such a technique. A comparison between the findings from the two studies would strengthen the current manuscript, in addition to being more ethically responsible.

      We thank the reviewer for the helpful comments, which we address in the point-by-point response below. We sincerely apologize for this oversight in our references. We have included references to your paper in our revised manuscript. It is exciting to see these complementary results! We now include discussion of this work to contextualize the importance of methylation-sensitive CTCF sites and motivate our study.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      To address the above points, the authors should:

      (1) Provide quantitative information in the text on all comparisons and justify that the small differences observed, albeit statistically significant, are biologically relevant. Inclusion of positive controls to give an indication of what types of changes can be expected would be helpful.

      We have added quantitative information to the text, as discussed in the response to public comments above.  We also provide literature evidence of expected effect sizes in that response.

      (2) Provide FISH data to a) validate the analysis of comparing looping patterns with SON Cut&Tag data as an indicator of physical association of loops with speckles and b) demonstrate by FISH increased association of some of the CTCF-dependent loops/genes (cluster 2) with speckles upon DNMT1 inhibition.

      Please see response to Reviewer 1 comment #2 above. Unfortunately, FISH will not provide the resolution we need for point a). We have confidence in our use of TSA-seq and Cut&Tag to study SON association with CTCF sites on a genome-wide scale, which would not be possible with individual FISH probes. Specifically, since the submission of our manuscript several other researchers (Yu et al, Nat. Struct. and Mol. Biol. 2025, Gholamalamdari et al eLife 2025) have leveraged CUT&RUN/CUT&TAG and TSA-seq to map speckle associated chromatin and have validated these methods with orthogonal imaging based approaches.

      (3) Demonstrate loss of speckles upon SON or SRRM2 by probing for other speckle components and ideally analysis by electron microscopy which should show loss of interchromatin granules.  

      We have performed TEM in K562 cells +/- SON/SRRM2 degradation. Please see response to Reviewer 1 comment #3. Specifically, interchromatin granule clusters are visible in the TEM images of the DMSO sample (see highlighted example above), however, given the heterogeneity of these structures and potential global alterations in heterochromatin that may be occurring following speckle loss, we refrained from making quantitative conclusions from this data. We instead include the raw images above.

      (4) The authors should either perform experiments to clearly show whether loop association is transcription dependent or whether association is merely a consequence of gene activation. Alternatively, they should tone down their model ascribing a direct regulatory role of methylation in control of loop association with speckles and also discuss other models. Unless the model is more clearly demonstrated, the title of the paper should be changed to reflect the uncertainty of the central conclusion.

      Please see response to Reviewer 1 comment #4 above.

      (5) The authors should either probe directly for the effect of speckle ablation on transcription or change their wording.

      We have changed our wording to RNA abundance.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major:

      ⁃ There was no DNA methylation analysis after inhibitor treatment. Ideally, genome bisulfite sequencing should be performed to show that the DNMT1i-specific CTCF binding sites are indeed unmethylated. But at the very least, a quantitative method should be employed to show the extent to which 5meC levels decrease in the presence of the DNMT1 inhibitor

      Response: We have now included analysis of genome wide bisulfite information from LIMe-Hi-C (bisulfite Hi-C) in K562 following DNMT1i inhibition. Specifically, we leverage the CpG methylation readout and find that DNTM1i-specific CTCF sites are more methylated than non-responsive CTCF peaks at baseline. In addition, these sites show the greatest decrease in CpG methylation upon 3 days of DNMT1 inhibition. We include a figure detailing these analyses in the supplement (Fig S1E). In addition, we have added CpG methylation genome browser tracks to (Fig S1D). In terms of global change, we have found that 3 days of DNMT1 inhibitor treatment leads to a reduction in methylation to about ~1/4 the level at baseline.

      I am not convinced that CUT&Tag is the proper technique to assess SON binding. CUT&Tag only works under stringent conditions (high salt), and can be a problematic assay for non-histone proteins, which bind less well to chromatin. In our experience, even strong binders such as CTCF exhibit a depleted binding profile when compared to ChIP seq data. I would need to be strongly convinced that the analysis presented in figures 2F-J and S2 D-I simply do not represent ATAC signal (ie, default Tn5 activity). For example, SON ChIP Seq, CUT&Tag in the SON degron and/or ATAC seq could be performed. What worries me is that increased chromatin accessibility would also be associated with increased looping, so they have generated artifactual results that are consistent with their model.

      As the reviewer suggested, we have now performed spike-in normalized SON Cut&Tag with DNMT1 inhibition and 6 hours of SON/SRRM2 degradation in our speckle dTAG knockin cell line. These experiments confirm that the SON Cut&Tag signal we see is SON-dependent. If the signal was truly due to artifactual binding, gained peaks would be open irrespective of speckle binding, however we see a clear speckle dependence as this signal is much lower if SON is degraded.

      Author response image 5.

      Moreover, in our original Cut&Tag experiments, we did not enrich detectable DNA without using the SON antibody (see last 4 samples-IgG controls). This further suggests that our signal is SON-dependent.

      Author response image 6.

      Finally, we see good agreement between Cut&Tag and TSA-seq (Spearman R=0.82).  The agreement is particularly strong in the top quadrant, which is most relevant since this is where the non-zero signal is.

      Author response image 7.

      Minor points

      ⁃ Why are HCT116 cells more responsive to treatment than K562 cells? This is something that could be addressed with DNA methylation analysis, for example

      K562 is a broadly hypomethylated cell line (Siegenfeld et.al, 2022 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-022-31857-5 Fig S2A-C). Thus, there may be less dynamic range to lose methylation compared to HCT116.

      Our results are also consistent with previous results comparing DKO HCT116 and aza-treated K562 cells (Maurano 2015, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.07.024). They state “In K562 cells, 5-aza-CdR treatment resulted in weaker reactivation than in DKO cells…”  In addition, cell-type-specific responsiveness to DNA methyltransferase KO depending upon global CpG methylation levels, has also been observed in ES and EpiLC cells (Monteagudo-Sanchez et al., 2024), which we now comment on in the manuscript.

      ⁃ How many significant CTCF loops in DNMTi, compared to DMSO? It was unclear what the difference in raw totals is.

      We now include a supplemental table with the HiChIP loop information. We call similar numbers of raw loops comparing DNMT1i and DMSO, as only a small subset of loops is changing.

      ⁃ For the architectural stripes, it would be nice to see a representative example in the form of a contact plot. Is that possible to do with the hiChIP data?

      As described in our methods, we called architectural stripes using Stripenn (Yoon et al 2022) from LIMe-Hi-C data under DNMT1i conditions (Siegenfeld et al, 2022). Shown below is a representative example of a stripe in the form of a Hi-C contact map.

      Author response image 8.

      ⁃ Here 4-10x more DNMT1i-specific CTCF binding sites were observed than we saw in our study. What are thresholds? Could the thresholds for DNMT1i-specific peaks be defined more clearly? For what it's worth, we defined our DNMT KO-specific peaks as fold-change {greater than or equal to} 2, adjusted P< 0.05. The scatterplots (1B) indicate a lot of "small" peaks being called "reactivated."

      We called DNMT1i-specific peaks using HOMER getDifferentialPeaksReplicates function. We used foldchange >2 and padj <0.05. We further restricted these peaks to those that were not called in the DMSO condition. 

      ⁃ On this note, is "reactivated" the proper term? Reactivated with regards to what? A prior cell state? I think DNMT1i-specific is a safer descriptor.

      We chose this term based on prior literature (Maurano 2015 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.07.024, Spracklin 2023 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41594-022-00892-7) . However, we agree it is not very clear, so we’ve altered the text to say “DNMT1i-specific”. We thank the reviewer for suggesting this improved terminology.

      ⁃ It appears there is a relatively small enrichment for CTCF peaks (of any class) in intergenic regions. How were intergenic regions defined? For us, it is virtually half of the genome. We did some enrichment of DNMT KO-specific peaks in gene bodies (our Supplemental Figure 1C), but a substantial proportion were still intergenic.

      We defined intergenic peaks using HOMER’s annotatepeaks function, with the -gtf option using Ensembl gene annotations (v104). We used the standard annotatepeaks priority order, which is TSS > TTS> CDS Exons > 5’UTR exons >3’ UTR exons > Introns > Intergenic.

      Maurano et. al. 2015 (http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2015.07.024) also found reduced representation of intergenic sites among demethylation-reactivated CTCF sites in their Fig S5A. We note this is not a perfect comparison because their data is displayed as a fraction of all intergenic peaks.

      ⁃ We also recently published a review on this subject: The impact of DNA methylation on CTCF-mediated 3D genome organization NSMB 2024 (PMID: 38499830) which could be cited if the authors choose.

      We have cited this relevant review.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript investigates beta burst dynamics in the primate motor cortex during movement and recovery from stroke. The authors differentiate between "global" beta bursts, which are synchronous across cortical and often subcortical regions, and more spatially confined "local" bursts. Global bursts are associated with reduced spiking variability, slower movements, and are more frequent after stroke, while local bursts increase during recovery and grasp execution. The study provides compelling evidence that beta bursts with different spatial and temporal characteristics may play distinct roles in motor control and recovery.

      We thank the reviewer for their assessment that the manuscript proves compelling evidence for distinct roles of local and global beta bursts on motor control and recovery.  

      Strengths:

      The major strength of this paper lies in its conceptual advance: the identification and characterization of distinct global and local beta bursts in the primate motor cortex. This distinction builds upon and considerably extends previous work on the heterogeneity of beta bursts. The paper is methodologically rigorous, using simultaneous cortical and subcortical recordings, detailed behavioral tracking, and thorough analyses of spikeLFP interactions. The use of stroke models and neurotypical animals provides converging evidence for the functional dissociation between burst types. The observation that local bursts increase with motor recovery and occur during grasping is particularly novel and may prove valuable for developing biomarkers of motor function.

      We thank the reviewer for recognizing the strengths of this manuscript. 

      Weaknesses:

      There are several conceptual and methodological limitations that should be addressed. First, the burst detection method relies on an amplitude threshold (median + 1 SD), which is susceptible to false positives and variability (Langford & Wilson, 2025). The classification into global or local bursts then depends on the number of co-bursting channels, compounding the arbitrariness. Second, the imposition of a minimum of three co-bursting cortical channels may bias against the detection of truly local bursts. 

      We thank the reviewer for bringing up these methodological details. We plan to conduct a follow-up analysis using alternative burst detection methods to verify that the paper’s main results hold when using different burst detection methodologies. We anticipate this will improve confidence in our results. 

      Third, the classification is entirely cortical; subcortical activity is considered post hoc rather than integrated into the classification, despite the key role of subcortical-cortical synchrony in motor control. 

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. First, because the different animals had subcortical recording sites in different locations, we hesitate to use subcortical activity in the classification of bursts since we were not sure we would be identifying the same burst-phenomenon (e.g. thalamo-cortical bursts vs. capsule-cortical bursts may differ). Second, we believe that having a cortical-only criteria allows the designation of local vs. global bursts to be more widely applied in preparations that only have access to cortical data (e.g. surface ECoG recordings, EEG, Utah array recordings). Thus, in this study we chose to analyze the subcortical data post-hoc (after burst detection and classification) to support our “global” vs. “local” designation of burst types 

      Fourth, the apparent dissociation between global and local bursts raises important questions about their spatial distribution across areas like M1 and PMv, which are not thoroughly analyzed. 

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. In our study’s stroke animals, we chose to study PMv due to its role in compensating for damage to M1, thus we hesitate to make any comparisons between PMv (which was recorded in stroke animals) and M1 (recorded in healthy unimpaired animals). Furthermore, animals are doing different tasks (e.g. reaching vs. reaching and grasping) which may also influence the spatial distribution. We agree that future work should certainly investigate the spatial distribution of global vs. local beta bursts across areas of sensorimotor cortex and subcortex, and that this comparison would be best done in healthy animals with both reaching and grasping behaviors.  

      Finally, while the authors interpret local bursts during grasping as novel, similar findings have been reported (e.g., Szul et al., 2023; Rayson et al., 2023), and a deeper discussion of these precedents would strengthen the argument.

      Thank you for these references! We will review them and incorporate them into our discussion of our results. 

      Impact:

      This work is likely to have a substantial impact on the field of motor systems neuroscience. The distinction between global and local beta bursts offers a promising framework for understanding the dual roles of beta in motor inhibition and sensorimotor computation. The findings are relevant not only for basic research but also for translational efforts in stroke rehabilitation and neuromodulation, particularly given the emerging interest in beta burst-based biomarkers and stimulation targets. The dataset and analytical framework will be useful to researchers investigating beta dynamics, spike-field relationships, and recovery from neural injury.

      We thank the reviewers for their assessment that our work will likely have a substantial impact on the field of motor systems neuroscience. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper by Khanna et al. describes global vs local beta synchrony between a cortical premotor area (PMv) and subcortical structures during motor tasks in the non-human primate, specifically investigating the progression following M1 injury. They found that increases in global beta synchrony between PMv and subcortical structures during the sub-acute phase of injury, and that global synchrony was associated with relatively slower motor movements. As recovery progressed, they report a shift from global synchrony to local synchrony and a subsequent reduction in the movement time. The authors suggest that global changes in subcortical and cortical beta synchrony may generally underpin a variety of movement disorders, including Parkinson's disease, and that shifting from global to local (or reducing global synchrony) might improve functional outcomes.

      Strengths:

      Ischemic insults and other acquired brain injuries have a significant public health impact. While there is a large body of clinical and basic science studies describing the behavioral, neurophysiological, and mechanistic outcomes of such injury, there is a significant lack studies looking at longitudinal, behaviorally-related neurophysiological measures following cortical injury, so any information has outsized contribution to understanding how brain injury disrupts underlying neural activity and how this may contribute to injury presentation and recovery.

      A significant percentage of pre-clinical stroke studies tend to focus on peri-infarct or other cortical structures and their role in recovery. The addition of subcortical recordings allows for the investigation of the role of thalamo-basal gangliar-cortical loops that may be contributing to the degree of impairment or to the recovery process is important for the field. Here, there are longitudinal (up to 3 months post-injury) recordings in the ventral premotor area (PMv) and either the internal capsule or sensorimotor thalamus that can be synchronized with phases of behavioral recovery.

      The methods are well described and can act as a framework for assessing synchrony across other data sets with similar recording locations. Limitations in methodology, recordings, and behavior were noted.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments on the strengths of this paper.  

      Weaknesses:

      A major limitation of this paper is that it is a set of case studies rather than a welldesigned, well-controlled study of beta synchrony following motor cortex injury. While non-human primate neurophysiological studies are almost always limited by extremely low animal numbers, they are made up for by the fact that they can acquire significant numbers of units or channels, and in the case of normal behavior, can obtain many behavioral trials over months of individual sessions. Here, there were two NHPs used, but they had different subcortical implant locations (thalamus vs internal capsule). They had different injury outcomes, with one showing a typical recovery curve following injury while one had complications and worsening behavior before ultimately recovering. Further, there were significant differences in the ability to record at different times, with one NHP having poor recordings early in the recovery process while one had poor recordings late in the process. Due to the injury, the authors report sessions in which they were not able to record many trials (~10). Assuming that recovery after a cortical injury is an evolving process, breaking analysis into "Early" and "Late" phases reduces the interpretation of where these shifts occur relative to recovery on the task, especially given different thresholds for recovery were used between animals. Because of this, despite a careful analysis of the data and an extensive discussion, the conclusions derived are not particularly compelling. To overcome this, the authors present data from neurotypical NHPs, but with electrodes in M1 rather than PMv, doing a completely different task with no grasping component, again making accurate conclusions about the results difficult. Even with low numbers, the study would have been much stronger if there were within-animal longitudinal data prior to and after the injury on the same task, so the impact of M1 injury could be better assessed.

      We thank the reviewer for these comments. Below we address some of these in more detail: 

      Different subcortical implant locations: We would like to clarify that the subcortical recordings were only used to confirm that global beta bursts (as characterized by cortical recordings alone) did indeed occur on subcortical sites coincidentally with cortical site more frequently than local beta bursts. Neither the beta burst categories nor the beta bursts themselves were influenced by the subcortical recordings.  

      Different injury outcomes: There is difficulty in creating strokes that result in identical deficits across animal as we and others have noted in previous work[1.3]. As a field, we are still understanding what factors give rise to variability in recovery curves. For example, one recent study noted that biological sex is a factor in predicting differences in recovery rates[4], and another noted that baseline white matter hyperintensities is also predictive of post-stroke recovery [5]. Overall, our methodology that creates structurally-consistent lesions can still result in very different functional outcomes depending on a variety of factors. Given this state of the field, we have done our best to match the recovery curves between our two animals, especially the initial recovery curves before Monkey H’s secondary decline. 

      Differences in ability to record at different times: We note this as a strength. One concern with these studies that induce stroke at the same time as implanting electrode arrays is that it is well appreciated that single-unit neuron yield right after array implantation is low and then improves in the following weeks [6]. There is always that concern that having more units later in recovery may drive results, but in this case, since one animal showed the opposite trend we are more confident that results are not driven by increases in unit-yield. We also note that we broadly see similar unit quality metrics in the early and late stages in both animals (Fig. S7).  

      Breaking continuous recovery curve into early and late: We note that this division was only made for one main analysis in the paper (Fig. 5CD): assessment of mean firing and variance of single-unit firing rates.  Without this split our analyses would be underpowered and inconclusive, thus we would not be able to provide any comment on how firing rates change, even coarsely, with recovery. 

      Presentation of data from M1 of healthy animals doing a different task: We agree that the strongest data would be longitudinally recorded from the same animals/brain areas pre-stroke and then post-stroke. However, we also view our inclusion of separate healthy animals doing a different task as evidence that our global vs. local segregation of beta bursts generalizes beyond the reach-to-grasp task to reaching-only tasks.  

      Overall, we appreciate the reviewer pointing out these notes about our data. In some cases we do not think these notes are concerning, in others, we acknowledge that have done the best we can given the state of the neurophysiology stroke recovery field. 

      It is unclear to what extent the subpial aspiration used is a stroke model. While it is much more difficult to perform a pure ischemic motor injury using electrocoagulatory methods in animal models that do not have a lissencephalic cortex, the suction ablation method that the authors use leads to different outcomes than an ischemic injury alone. For instance, in rat models, ischemic vs suction ablation leads to very different electrophysiological profiles and differences in underlying anatomical reorganization (see Carmichael and Chesselet, 2002), even if the behavioral outcomes were similar. There is a concern that the effects shown may be an artifact of the lesion model rather than informing underlying mechanisms of recovery.

      We thank the reviewer for bringing this up. 

      Clarification of our stroke model methodology: We wish to highlight that when we create stroke, we first do surface vessel occlusion as the first step. This is designed to match true ischemic injury. After a waiting period, the injured tissue is then aspiration to reduce the effects of edema and secondary mass effect in the model. 

      Carmichael and Chesselet 2002: The rodent work cited did show differential effects of a suction ablation method (without any surface vessel occlusion first) versus an ischemic method. The effects observed in this work were in the first 5 days following stroke. In our case, we started recording on day 7 and examined recovery over extended periods (weeks to months). 

      Effects of acute insult on rehabilitation: From a rehabilitation perspective, it remains unclear how the acute insult affects outcomes weeks and months later. One line of evidence to suggest that the manner that the acute insult occurs may not matter for rehabilitation is the observation that one therapeutic approach (vagus nerve stimulation) has been found to successfully improve rehabilitation outcomes in a range of injury models (intracranial hemorrhage, stroke, spinal cord injury). We agree that additional work is required in this area.

      Human stroke data shows similar results reported: Lastly, we note that neurophysiology performed in humans with clinical strokes supports the results we seek here (e.g.[7], see discussion section for full elaboration) suggesting that our stroke model methodology is similar enough to clinical stroke to result in similar results. 

      The injury model leads to seemingly mild impairments in grasp (but not reach), with rapid and complete recovery occurring within 2-3 weeks from the time of injury. Because of the rapid recovery, relating the physiological processes of recovery to beta synchronization becomes challenging to interpret - Are the global bursts the result of the loss of M1 input to subcortical structures? Are they due to the lack of M1 targets, so there is a more distributed response? Is this due to other post-injury sub-acute mechanisms? How specific is this response - is it limited to peri-infarct areas (and to what extent is the PMv electrode truly in peri-infarct cortex), or would this synchrony be seen anywhere in the sensorimotor networks? Are the local bursts present because global synchrony wanes over time as a function of post-injury homeostatic mechanisms, or is local beta synchrony increasing as new motor plans are refined and reinforced during task re-acquisition? How coupled are they related to recovery - if it is motor plan refinement, the shift from global to local seemingly should lag the recovery?  

      We think these are all wonderful questions that could be addressed in follow-up studies! 

      While the study has significant limitations in design that reduce the impact of the results, it should act as a useful baseline/pilot data set in which to build a more complete picture of the role of subcortical-cortical beta synchrony following cortical injury.

      We agree that this is a study that should be treated as a starting point for further investigation. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Khanna et al. use a well-conceived and well-executed set of experiments and analyses primarily to document the interaction between neural oscillations in the beta range (here, 13-30 Hz) and recovery of function in an animal model of stroke. Specifically, they show that cortical "beta bursts", or short-term increases in beta power, correlate strikingly with the timeline of behavioral recovery as quantified with a reach-to-grasp task. A key distinction is made between global beta bursts (here, those that synchronize between cortical and subcortical areas) and local bursts (which appear on only a few electrodes). This distinction of global vs. local is shown to be relevant to task performance and movement speed, among other quantities of interest.

      A secondary results section explores the relationship between beta bursts and neuronal firing during the grasp portion of the behavioral task. These results are valuable to include, though mostly unsurprising, with global beta in particular associated with lower mean and variance in spike rates.

      Last, a partial recapitulation of the primary results is offered with a neurologically intact (uninjured) animal. No major contradictions are found with the primary results.

      Highlights of the Discussion section include a thoughtful review of atypical movements executed by individuals with Parkinson's disease or stroke survivors, placing the current results in an appropriate clinical context. Potential physiological mechanisms that could account for the observed results are also discussed effectively.

      Strengths:

      Overall, this is a very interesting paper. The ultimate impact will be enhanced by the authors' choice to analyze beta bursts, which remain a relatively under-explored aspect of neural coding.

      The reach-and-grasp task was also a well-considered choice; the combination of a relatively simple movement (reaching towards a target in the same location each time) and a more complex movement (a skilled object-manipulation grasp) provides an internal control of sorts for data analysis. In addition, the task's two sub-movements provide a differential in terms of their likelihood to be affected by the stroke-like injury: proximal muscles (controlling reach) are likely to be less affected by stroke, while distal muscles (controlling grasp) are highly likely to be affected. Lastly, the requirement of the task to execute an object lift maximizes its difficulty and also the potential translational impact of the results on human injury.

      The above comments about the task exemplify a strength that is more generally evident: a welcome awareness of clinical relevance, which is in evidence several times throughout the Results and Discussion.

      Weaknesses:

      The study's weaknesses are mostly minor and, for the most part, correctable.

      One concern that may not be correctable in this study: the results about the spatial extent of beta activity seem constrained by relatively poor-quality data. It seems half or more of the electrodes are marked as too noisy to provide useful data in Figure 3. If this reflects the wider reality for all analyses, as mentioned, it may not be correctable for the present study. In that case, perhaps some of the experiments or analyses can be revisited or expanded for a future study, when better electrode yields are available.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments. We note that we have chosen to be particularly conservative with which channels we considered noise-free and acceptable for analysis as our animals were not head-posted (see methods: “On each day, trials were manually inspected alongside camera data for any movement or chewing artifacts (note that animals were not head-posted) and were discarded from neural data analysis if there were any artifacts”). After re-visiting our analysis, we note that the data shown in Fig. 3 (spatial distribution of local bursts) is not representative from a data quality perspective – this data was from a session that had a particularly large number of channels discarded due to artifacts. We plan to correct this to show a more representative figure. 

      Other concerns:

      In some places, there is a lack of clarity in the presentation of the results. This is not serious but should be addressed to aid readers' comprehension.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment and for their numerous suggestions in the notes to the authors. We plan to address as many of these as we can to improve clarity and comprehension.  

      Lastly, given the central role of beta oscillations within the study, it would be better for completeness to include even a brief exploration of sustained beta power (rather than bursts), and the modulation of sustained beta (or lack thereof) in the study's areas of concern: behavioral recovery, task performance, etc.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion – we plan to include this in our revisions.  

      References cited in response to public reviewer comments: 

      (1) Ganguly, K., Khanna, P., Morecraft, R. J. & Lin, D. J. Modulation of neural co-firing to enhance network transmission and improve motor function after stroke. Neuron 110, 2363–2385 (2022).

      (2) Khanna, P. et al. Low-frequency stimulation enhances ensemble co-firing and dexterity after stroke. Cell 184, 912-930.e20 (2021).

      (3) Darling, W. G. et al. Sensorimotor Cortex Injury Effects on Recovery of Contralesional Dexterous Movements in Macaca mulatta. Exp Neurol 281, 37–52 (2016).

      (4) Bottenfield, K. R. et al. Sex differences in recovery of motor function in a rhesus monkey model of cortical injury. Biology of Sex Differences 12, 54 (2021).

      (5) Schwarz, A. et al. Association that Neuroimaging and Clinical Measures Have with Change in Arm Impairment in a Phase 3 Stroke Recovery Trial. Ann Neurol 97, 709– 719 (2025).

      (6) Gulati, T. et al. Robust Neuroprosthetic Control from the Stroke Perilesional Cortex. J. Neurosci. 35, 8653–8661 (2015).

      (7) Silberstein, P. et al. Cortico-cortical coupling in Parkinson’s disease and its modulation by therapy. Brain 128, 1277–1291 (2005).

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      The manuscript by Butler et al. explores a novel physiological role for connexin 32 (Cx32) hemichannels in Schwann cells at peripheral nerves. Building on the authors' prior work on CO₂-sensitive gating of connexins, this study proposes that mitochondrial CO₂ production dependent on neuronal activity promotes the opening of Cx32 hemichannels in the paranode, which in turn modulates neuronal activity by reducing conduction velocity. This hypothesis is addressed using a multifaceted approach that includes immunofluorescence microscopy, dye uptake assays, calcium imaging, computational modeling, and extracellular recordings in isolated sciatic nerves. 

      Among the strengths of the study are the interdisciplinary integration of imaging, in silico approaches, and functional data. Also, this study proposes a new mechanism with profound physiological relevance. Specifically, Butler et al. provide new insights into glial modulation of electrical conduction in sensory/motor myelinated nerves. 

      In the current state, the study has some limitations. The evidence linking Cx32 to the observed dye uptake and conduction velocity changes relies primarily on pharmacological inhibition with carbenoxolone, which lacks specificity. The imaging data show overlapping marker signals that preclude the anatomical distinction between nodes and paranodes. FITC uptake, while convincing to test Cx32 hemichannel gating, lacks spatial-temporal information and validation of distribution and localization to viable intracellular compartments. Moreover, while the findings are intriguing, functional proof that Cx32 regulates conduction velocity through ATP release or other downstream effects remains incomplete. Further work using targeted genetic tools, live-tissue imaging, and additional controls would strengthen the mechanistic conclusions. 

      Overall, the manuscript offers compelling preliminary evidence that supports a new role for Cx32 in peripheral nerve physiology and raises important questions for future investigation. 

      We thank the reviewer for their comments and agree that the evidence for involvement of Cx32 is indirect. We are planning to perform genetic manipulations to strengthen this link. We shall review our presentation of the morphology in terms of the node/paranode/juxtaparanode distribution and adjust accordingly. We have in the interim generated new data using GCaMP transduced into Schwann cells that provides the live-tissue imaging that the reviewer requests. This strengthens our conclusions, and we will add these data into the paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This article aims to demonstrate that local production of CO₂ at the axonal node opens Cx32 hemichannels in the Schwann cell paranode, and that CO₂ diffuses through the AQP1 channel to reach Cx32 and trigger its opening. The authors also present evidence supporting a physiological role for this regulatory mechanism. They propose that CO₂-dependent Cx32 activation mediates activity-dependent Ca²⁺ influx into the paranode, and by increasing the leak current across the myelin sheath, it contributes to a slowing of action potential conduction velocity. 

      The study presents a very interesting and novel mechanism for the physiological regulation of Cx32 hemichannels. The findings are relevant to the field, and the methods and results are of good quality, with some improvements in interpretation and explanation required, and some minor experimental suggestions. 

      Strengths: 

      The article is solid in terms of the novelty of the findings and relevance for the physiology of myelinated axons. In addition, it is of major interest for the Connexin field because it explores a physiological way to open Cx32 hemichannels. The experiments are well elaborated, and most of them are sufficient for the main points described by the authors. The finding that nervous activity will trigger the mechanism of hemichannel opening by CO2 is probably the most relevant biological mechanism derived from this article. 

      Weaknesses: 

      Throughout the manuscript, the authors interpret their findings as if the described mechanism specifically occurs in the node and paranode regions. However, there is no direct evidence identifying the precise site of CO₂ production or the activation site of Cx32 hemichannels. Therefore, statements such as the one in the title ("activity-dependent CO₂ production in the axonal node opens Cx32 in the Schwann cell paranode") should be reconsidered or removed, as they may be misleading and are not essential to the interpretation of the data. In addition, the participation of aquaporin AQP1 as the main conduit for CO2 diffusion through the plasma membrane could have another interpretation. 

      We thank the reviewer for their comments and agree that we do not have direct evidence for the site of CO2 production or the site of activation of Cx32 hemichannels. This direct evidence is extremely difficult to obtain, and we therefore depend on indirect arguments. Mitochondria represent the major source of CO2, and their distribution will therefore indicate where CO2 is likely to be produced. We agree that this is not essential to the interpretation of the data and will adjust the text as recommended. We will add a section to the Discussion to consider this point in more detail.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The main limitations of this article are that it provides insufficient detail on VR implementation. The design of the VR environment is, at this stage, under-described. Crucial information is missing, such as the number of pineapples per block, timing precision, details on how motion is mapped to the virtual movement, etc. This aspect strongly limits the reproducibility of the experiments. A second limitation lies in the lack of clarity regarding the study hypotheses. Although two overarching hypotheses can be inferred, they are not explicitly formulated. To this end, it is unclear which analyses were merely exploratory, especially for physiological and EEG outcomes.

      In Experiment 2, the reduction in vigor during tonic pain could plausibly reflect attentional load rather than pain per se. As recognized by the authors, there is no control condition involving an innocuous salient stimulus to rule out non-specific effects of distraction. Perhaps a tonic non-painful but salient somatosensory stimulus (e.g., a strong vibrotactile stimulus applied on the same arm) could have been used as a control stimulus.

      We appreciate the reviewer's comments regarding the insufficient implementation details. We hope the newly uploaded software for reproducing the experiment can improve the reader's understanding of the task. In addition to making the software available, we will expand the Methods section in the revised manuscript to include greater detail on the task description.

      The hypothesised functions of phasic and tonic pain, and their collaborative interaction, are both broad and deep topics. In the revised manuscript, we will more explicitly formulate our hypotheses and clarify the distinction between a priori predictions and exploratory analyses, particularly concerning the extent to which our evidence supports these hypotheses.

      We agree that examining the potential role of attentional load on the interaction between tonic and phasic pain is an important area of future investigation. Addition of additional control conditions matched for attentional salience with additional experiments is possible but introduces other confounds related to their different qualities (e.g. a salient vibrotactile stimulus might invigorate behaviour): however more fundamentally, attentional processes are a core part of pain function, and should not necessarily be viewed as a confound (i.e. the way that pain mediates some of its core functional effects may directly be through its salient attentional nature) . This view is formalised in Wall and Melzack’s classical tripartite model of pain, and distinguishes pain from purely sensory systems such as somatosensation, vision and so on..

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Two critical issues require clarification or justification. First, phasic pain was induced using electrical stimulation, which typically elicits somatosensory evoked potentials (SEPs). These responses may not reflect pain-specific processes and thus complicate interpretation. This issue bears directly on the study's conclusions, especially when discussing interactions between phasic and tonic pain. For example, tonic pain is known to reduce perceived intensity or cortical responses to phasic pain stimuli delivered elsewhere on the body - an effect not expected for SEPs elicited by electrical stimuli.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding the specificity of evoked potentials elicited by electrical stimulation. We agree that traditional SEPs—particularly those evoked by large surface electrodes—primarily reflect activation of non-nociceptive A-beta fibres and thus may not reliably index pain-specific processes or be modulated by tonic pain via descending nociceptive control. However, we would like to clarify that phasic pain was administered in the present study using small-diameter concentric ‘Wasp’ electrodes. These are comparable to intraepidermal electrodes shown to preferentially activate nociceptive A-delta fibres, thereby eliciting ERPs more closely associated with nociceptive processing rather than mixed somatosensory input [1, 2]. Accordingly, our ERP results demonstrated a reliable increase in N1-P2 amplitude with higher phasic pain intensity, suggesting that the evoked responses captured stimulus-evoked nociceptive processing.

      We acknowledge that these ERPs may still reflect mixed sensory processing and thus may not be fully modulated by tonic pain. Previous studies have shown that ERPs elicited by nociceptive electrical stimulation can be attenuated during tonic pain using cold-water immersion in CPM paradigms [3, 4]. However, these studies typically employ passive tasks, whereas our paradigm involved continuous voluntary behaviour during sustained tonic pressure pain. This difference in task context may engage distinct modulatory systems, possibly prioritising behavioural adaptation over sensory gating.

      We will revise the manuscript to acknowledge these factors and to encourage a more nuanced interpretation of the ERP findings in light of this literature.

      Second, additional control experiments are necessary to rule out alternative explanations. For instance, the authors are suggested to deliver phasic pain to the contralateral arm (e.g., at 1-2 Hz), which might also reduce action velocity. Similarly, tonic pain applied to the grasping hand should be tested to disentangle hand-specific effects.

      We are grateful to the reviewer for this suggestion. In the current study, phasic pain was delivered to the grasping hand to generate a coherent, spatially congruent representation of virtual stimuli (painful fruit) and behavioural consequences (pain upon grasp). Delivering phasic pain stimuli to the contralateral hand would be incongruent with the task design and may alter the interpretation of the learning signal, which was central to our computational modelling framework. Similarly, tonic pain was not applied to the grasping hand to avoid interfering with motor control. Applying tonic pain to the grasping hand would make it extremely difficult for participants to effectively grasp the hand controller, thereby complicating the interpretation of behavioural and neural measures. We will discuss these issues in the revision. Therefore, while we agree that such manipulations could be informative for future studies, they were not the focus of the current investigation.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Despite these strengths, the manuscript would benefit significantly from more precise definitions of key concepts and an overall clearer, more coherent presentation of its main arguments. The writing, in its current form, often presents claims that are too vague or insufficiently connected with the experimental findings. Moreover, certain aspects of the computational modeling and statistical analysis appear flawed or inadequately justified.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the need for clearer definitions and a more coherent presentation. In the revised manuscript, we will refine our definitions of key concepts and improve the presentation of hypothesised functions of phasic and tonic pain. As stated previously, we will clarify the extent to which our evidence supports these hypotheses. We also appreciate the feedback on our statistical analysis and computational modelling. We will address these points and provide the necessary clarifications and justifications in the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary 

      The authors describe a method for gastruloid formation using mouse embryonic stem cells (mESCs) to study YS and AGM-like hematopoietic differentiation. They characterise the gastruloids during nine days of differentiation using a number of techniques including flow cytometry and single-cell RNA sequencing. They compare their findings to a published data set derived from E10-11.5 mouse AGM. At d9, gastruloids were transplanted under the adrenal gland capsule of immunocompromised mice to look for the development of cells capable of engrafting the mouse bone marrow. The authors then applied the gastruloid protocol to study overexpression of Mnx1 which causes infant AML in humans.

      In the introduction, the authors define their interpretation of the different waves of hematopoiesis that occur during development. 'The subsequent wave, known as definitive, produces: first, oligopotent erythro-myeloid progenitors (EMPs) in the YS (E8-E8.5); and later myelo-lymphoid progenitors (MLPs - E9.5-E10), multipotent progenitors (MPPs - E10-E11.5), and hematopoietic stem cells (HSCs - E10.5-E11.5), in the aorta-gonad-mesonephros (AGM) region of the embryo proper.' Herein they designate the yolk sac-derived wave of EMP hematopoiesis as definitive, according to convention, although paradoxically it does not develop from intra-embryonic mesoderm or give rise to HSCs.

      Our definition of primitive and definitive waves is widely used in the field (e.g. PMID: 18204427; PMID: 28299650; PMID: 33681211). Definitive haematopoiesis, encompassing EMP, MLP, MPP and HSC, highlights their origin from haemogenic endothelium, generation of mature cells with adult characteristics from progenitors with multilineage potential and direct and indirect developmental contributions to the intra-embryonic and time-restricted generation of HSCs. 

      General comments 

      The authors make the following claims in the paper: 

      (1) The development of a protocol for hemogenic gastruloids (hGx) that recapitulates YS and AGMlike waves of blood from HE.

      (2) The protocol recapitulates both YS and EMP-MPP embryonic blood development 'with spatial and temporal accuracy'.

      (3) The protocol generates HSC precursors capable of short-term engraftment in an adrenal niche.

      (4) Overexpression of MNX1 in hGx transforms YS EMP to 'recapitulate patient transcriptional signatures'.

      (5) hGx is a model to study normal and leukaemic embryonic hematopoiesis. 

      There are major concerns with the manuscript. The statements and claims made by the authors are not supported by the data presented, data is overinterpreted, and the conclusions cannot be justified. Furthermore, the data is presented in a way that makes it difficult for the reader to follow the narrative, causing confusion. The authors have not discussed how their hGx compares to the previously published mouse embryoid body protocols used to model early development and hematopoiesis. Specific points 

      (1) It is claimed that HGxs capture cellularity and topography of developmental blood formation. The hGx protocol described in the manuscript is a modification of a previously published gastruloid protocol (Rossi et al 2022). The rationale for the protocol modifications is not fully explained or justified. There is a lack of novelty in the presented protocol as the only modifications appear to be the inclusion of Activin A and an extension of the differentiation period from 7 to 9 days of culture. No direct comparison has been made between the two versions of gastruloid differentiation to justify the changes.

      The Reviewer paradoxically claims that the protocol is not novel and that it differs from a previous publication in at least 2 ways – the patterning pulse and the length of the protocol. Of these, the patterning pulse is key. As documented in Fig. 1S1, we cannot obtain Flk1-GFP expression in the absence of Activin A (Fig. 1S1A), and the concentration of Activin A scales activity of the Flk1 locus (Fig. 1S1B). Expression of Flk1 is a fundamental step in haemato-endothelial specification and, accordingly, we do not see CD41 or CD45+ cells in the absence of Activin A. Furthermore, these markers also titrate with the dose of Activin A (in Fig. 1S1B).

      Also, in our hands, there is a clear time-dependent progression of marker expression, with sequential acquisition of CD41 and CD45, with the latter not detectable until 192h (Fig. 1C-D), another key difference relative to the Rossi et al (2022) protocol. We suggest, and present further evidence for in this rebuttal and the revised manuscript, that the 192h-timepoint captures the onset of AGM-like haematopoiesis. We have edited the manuscript to clarify the differences and novelty in our protocol (lines 132-143) and provided a more detailed comparison with the report from Rossi et al. (2022) in the Discussion (lines 574-586).

      The inclusion of Activin A at high concentration at the beginning of differentiation would be expected to pattern endoderm rather than mesoderm. BMP signaling is required to induce Flk1+ mesoderm, even in the presence of Wnt.

      Again, we call the Reviewer’s attention to Fig. 1S1A which clearly shows that Activin A (with no BMP added) is required for induction of Flk1 expression, in the presence of Wnt. Activin A in combination with Wnt, is used in other protocols of haemato-endothelial differentiation from pluripotent cells, with no BMP added in the same step of patterning and differentiation (PMID: 39227582; PMID: 39223325). In the latter protocol, we also call the Reviewer’s attention to the fact that a higher concentration of Activin A precludes the need for BMP4 addition. Finally, one of us has recently reported that Activin A, on its own, will induce Flk1, as well as other anterior mesodermal progenitors (https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2025.01.11.632562v1). In addressing the Reviewer’s concerns with the dose of Activin A used, we titrated its concentration against activation of Flk1, confirming optimal Flk1-GFP expression at the 100ng/ml dose used in the manuscript. We have included this data in the manuscript in Figure 1S1B.                         

      FACS analysis of the hGx during differentiation is needed to demonstrate the co-expression of Flk1GFP and lineage markers such as CD34 to indicate patterning of endothelium from Flk1+ mesoderm. The FACS plots in Fig. 1 show C-Kit expression but very little VE-cadherin which suggests that CD34 is not induced. Early endoderm expresses C-Kit, CXCR4, and Epcam, but not CD34 which could account for the lack of vascular structures within the hGx as shown in Fig. 1E.

      We were surprised by the Reviewer’s comment that there are no endothelial structures in our haemogenic gastruloids. The presence of a Flk1-GFP+ network is visible in the GFP images in Fig. 1B, from 144h onwards, and is detailed in the revised Fig. 2A, which shows overlap between Flk1GFP and the endothelial marker CD31. In addition, our single-cell RNA-seq data, included in the manuscript, confirms the presence of endothelial cells with a developing endothelial, including arterial, programme. This is now presented in the revised Fig. 3B-D of the manuscript, which updates a representation in the original manuscript. In contrast with the Reviewer’s claims that no endothelial cells are formed, the data show that Kdr (Flk1)+ cells co-express Cdh5/VE-Cadherin and indeed Cd34, attesting to the presence of an endothelial programme. Arterial markers Efnb2, Flt1, and Dll4 are present. A full-blown programme, which also includes haemogenic markers including Sox17, Esam, Cd44 and Mecom is clear at early (144h) and, particularly at late (192h) timepoints in cells sorted on detection of surface C-Kit (Fig. 3B-E in the manuscript). To address the specific point by the Reviewer, we also document co-expression of Flk1-GFP, CD34 and/or CD31 by flow cytometry (Fig. 2S1A-B in the revised manuscript).

      To summarise new and revised data in the manuscript in relation to this point:

      Immunofluorescence staining showing the Flk1-GFP-defined vascular network in Figure 1E and co-expression of endothelial marker CD31 in Figure 2A. In text: lines 159-163; 178-180.

      Flow cytometry analysis of co-expression of Flk1-GFP with CD31 and CD34 in Figure 2S1AD, including controls. In text: 180-187.

      Real-time quantitative (q)PCR analysis showing time-dependent expression of haematoendothelial and arterial markers in Figure 2F (specifically Dll4 and Mecom). In text: 200-209.

      An improved representation of our scRNA-seq data highlighting key haemato-endothelial markers in Figure 3B-D. In text: 268-304

      (2) The protocol has been incompletely characterised, and the authors have not shown how they can distinguish between either wave of Yolk Sac (YS) hematopoiesis (primitive erythroid/macrophage and erythro-myeloid EMP) or between YS and intraembryonic Aorta-Gonad-Mesonephros (AGM) hematopoiesis. No evidence of germ layer specification has been presented to confirm gastruloid formation, organisation, and functional ability to mimic early development. Furthermore, differentiation of YS primitive and YS EMP stages of development in vitro should result in the efficient generation of CD34+ endothelial and hematopoietic cells. There is no flow cytometry analysis showing the kinetics of CD34 cell generation during differentiation. Benchmarking the hGx against developing mouse YS and embryo data sets would be an important verification. 

      The Reviewer is correct that we have not provided detailed characterisation of the different germ layers, as this was not the focus of the study. In that context, we were surprised by the earlier comment assuming co-expression of C-Kit, Cxcr4 and Epcam, which we did not show, while overlooking the endothelial programme reiterated above, which we have presented. Given our focus on haemato-endothelial specification, we have started the single-cell RNA-seq characterisation of the haemogenic gastruloid at 120h and have not looked specifically at earlier timepoints of embryo patterning. This said, we show the presence of neuroectodermal cells in cluster 9; on the other hand, cluster 7 includes hepatoblast-like cells, denoting endodermal specification (Supplementary File S2). However, in the absence of earlier timepoints and given the bias towards mesodermal specification, we expect that specification of ectodermal and endodermal programmes may be incomplete. 

      In respect of the contention regarding the capture of YS-like and AGM-like haematopoiesis, we had presented evidence in the original version of the manuscript that haemogenic cells generated during gastruloid differentiation, particularly at late 192h and 216h timepoints project onto highly purified CKit+ CD31+ Gfi1-expressing cells from mouse AGM (PMID: 38383534), providing support for at least partial recapitulation of the corresponding developmental stage. These projections are represented in Fig. 4A, right and 4S1C of the revised manuscript. In distinguishing between YS-like and AGM-like haematopoiesis, we call the Reviewer’s attention to the replotting of the single-cell RNA-seq data already in the manuscript, which we provided in response to point 1 (Fig. 3B-D and 3S2B), which highlights an increase in Sox17, but not Sox18, expression in the 192h haemogenic endothelium, which suggests an association with AGM haematopoiesis (PMID: 20228271). A significant association of Cd44 and Procr expression with the same time-point (Fig. 3B-D in the manuscript), further supports an AGM-like endothelial-to-haematopoietic transition at the 192h timepoint. We have re-analysed the scRNA-seq data to better represent the expression of these markers in Fig. 3A-E and S32B. We agree that it remains challenging to identify markers exclusive to AGM haematopoiesis, which is operationally equated with generation of transplantable haematopoietic stem cells. While HSC generation is a key event characteristic of the AGM, not all AGM haematopoiesis corresponds to HSCs, an important point in evaluating the data presented in the manuscript, and one that is acknowledged by us. The main text has been edited to clarify the experiments pertaining to distinguishing AGM and YS haematopoiesis, which are detailed in lines 180-187, 200-221, 268-304, and 315-356.

      Following on the Reviewer’s comments about Cd34, we also inspected co-expression of Cd34 with Cd41 and Cd45, the latter co-expression present in, although not necessarily exclusive to, AGM haematopoiesis. Reassuringly, we observed clear co-expression with both markers (Author response image 1), in addition to a CD41+CD34- population, which likely reflects YS EMP-independent erythropoiesis. Flow cytometry analysis of co-expression of CD31 and CD34 in CD41+ and CD45+ populations at 144h and 216h timepoints has been included in Fig. 2B-D, Fig. 2S1A-D, including controls. In text: 180-187. We have earlier on in the rebuttal highlighted the fact that marker expression is responsive to the levels of Activin A used in the patterning pulse, with the 100ng/ml Activin A used in our protocol superior to 75ng/ml.

      Author response image 1.

      Association of CD34 with CD41 and CD45 expression is Activin A-responsive and supports the presence of definitive haematopoiesis. A. Flow cytometry analysis of CD34 and CD41 expression in 216h-haemogenic gastruloids; two doses of Activin A were used in the patterning pulse with CHI99021 between 48-72h. FMO controls shown. B. Flow cytometry analysis of CD34 and CD45 at 216h in the same experimental conditions.

      Given the centrality of this point in comments by all the Reviewers, we have conducted projections of our single-cell RNA-seq data against two studies which (1) capture arterial and haemogenic specification in the para-splanchnopleura (pSP) and AGM region between E8.0 and E11 (Hou et al, PMID: 32203131), and (2) uniquely capture YS, AGM and FL progenitors and the AGM endothelial-tohaematopoietic transition (EHT) in the same scRNA-seq dataset (Zhu et al, PMID: 32392346). Focusing the analysis on the subsets of haemogenic gastruloid cells sorted as CD41+ (144h) C-Kit+ (144h and 192h) and CD45+ (192h and 216h) (now represented in Fig. 3A, and projected onto the studies in Fig. 4A), we show:

      (1) That a subset of haemato-endothelial cells from haemogenic gastruloids at 144h to 216h project onto intra-embryonic cells spanning E8.25 to E10 (revised Fig. 4A left and 4S1A). This is in agreement with our original interpretation that 216h are no later than the MPP/pre-HSC state of embryonic development, requiring further maturation to generate engrafting progenitors. We have nevertheless removed specific references to pre-HSC, and instead referred to HSPC/progenitors.

      (2) That haemogenic gastruloids contain YS-like (including EMP-like) and AGM-like haematopoietic cells (Fig. 4A centre and 4 S1B). Significantly, some of the cells, particularly CKit-sorted cells with a candidate endothelial and HE-like signature project onto AGM pre-HE and HE, as well as IAHC. Some 144h CD41+ and 192h CD45+ cells also project onto IAHC, suggesting that YS-like and AGM-like programmes arise independently and with partial timedependent organisation in the haemogenic gastruloid model. Later, predominantly 216h cells, have characteristics of MPP/LMPP-like cells from the FL, suggesting a progenitor wave of differentiation.

      Altogether, the data support the notion that haemogenic gastruloids capture YS and AGM haematopoiesis until E10, as suggested by us in the manuscript.This re-analysis of the scRNA-seq data which was indeed prompted by challenging and insightful comments from the Reviewers, has been incorporated in the manuscript as described above and further listed here:

      Re-clustering and highlights of specific markers in our scRNA-seq data in Figure 3A-E. In text: 268-304.

      Projections to mouse embryo datasets in Figure 4A (Figure 4S1A-C; Supplementary File 3). In text: 315-356. 

      Single-cell RNA sequencing was used to compare hGx with mouse AGM. The authors incorrectly conclude that ' ..specification of endothelial and HE cells in hGx follows with time-dependent developmental progression into putative AGM-like HE..' And, '...HE-projected hGx cells.......expressed Gata2 but not Runx1, Myb, or Gfi1b..' Hemogenic endothelium is defined by the expression of Runx1 and Gfli1b is downstream of Runx1.

      As a hierarchy of regulation, Gata2 precedes and drives Runx1 expression at the specification of HE (PMID: 17823307; PMID: 24297996), while Runx1 drives the EHT, upstream of Gfi1b in haematopoietic clusters (PMID: 34517413). Please note that the text segment the Reviewer refers to has been removed from the manuscript, as the analysis is no longer solely focused on projection to Thambyrajah et al (2024) data, and instead gained significantly from the projections on to the Hou et al (2020) and Zhu et al (2020) studies, as detailed above.

      (3) The hGx protocol 'generates hematopoietic SC precursors capable of short-term engraftment' is not supported by the data presented. Short-term engraftment would be confirmed by flow cytometric detection of hematopoietic cells within the recipient bone marrow, spleen, thymus, and peripheral blood that expressed the BFP transgene. This analysis was not provided. PCR detection of transcripts, following an unspecified number of amplification cycles, as shown in Figure 3G (incorrectly referred to as Figure 3F in the legend) is not acceptable evidence for engraftment.

      We provide the full flow cytometry analysis of spleen engraftment in the 5 mice which received implantation of 216h-haemogenic gastruloids in the adrenal gland and were analysed at 4 weeks; an additional (control) animal received adrenal injection of PBS (Fig. 4B-D in the revised manuscript). In this experiment, the bone marrow collection was limiting, and material was prioritised for PCR (Fig. 4C and full gels in 4S2C in the revised manuscript).

      We had previously provided only representative plots of flow cytometry analysis of bone marrow and spleen, which we described as low-level engraftment and were chosen conservatively. The analysis was meant to complement the genomic DNA PCR, where detection was present in only some of the replicates tested per animal. On this note, we confirm that PCR analysis used conventional 40 cycles; the sensitivity had already been shown in the earlier version of the manuscript and is again represented in Fig. 4S2B. We argue that the low level of cytometric and molecular engraftment at 4 weeks, from haemogenic gastruloid-derived progenitors that have not progressed beyond a stage equivalent to E10 (Fig. 4A and Supplementary File 3 in the revised manuscript from scRNAseq projections), and that we have described as requiring additional maturation in vivo, are not surprising. Indeed, as previously shown and now repeated in in Fig. 2B-E (controls in Fig. 2S1E-G) in the revised manuscript, no more than 7 CD45+CD144+ multipotent cells are present per haemogenic gastruloid. We are only able to implant 3 haemogenic gastruloids in the adrenal gland of each transplanted animal. 

      We have rephrased Results and Discussion in lines 359-415 and 588-621, respectively, to rectify the nature of the engraftment, which we now attribute more generically to progenitors, also in light of the developmental time we could capture in the gastruloids prior to implantation.

      Transplanted hGx formed teratoma-like structures, with hematopoietic cells present at the site of transplant only analysed histologically. Indeed, the quality of the images provided does not provide convincing validation that donor-derived hematopoietic cells were present in the grafts.

      As stated in the text, the images mean to illustrate that the haemogenic gastruloids developed in situ. Further analysis motivated by the Reviewers’ comments and indeed a subsequent experiment with analysis of engraftment at a later timepoint of 8 weeks (revised Fig. 4E and 4 S2F-G) did not show a direct correspondence between engraftment and in vivo development or expansion, although this occurs in some cases. To be clearer, the observation of donor-derived blood cells in the implanted haemogenic gastruloids would not correspond to engraftment, as we have amply demonstrated that they have generated blood cells in vitro. There is no evidence that there are remaining pluripotent cells in the haemogenic gastruloid after 9 days of differentiation, and it is therefore not clear that the structures observed are teratomas. We specifically comment on this point in the revised manuscript – lines 601-607.

      There is no justification for the authors' conclusion that '... the data suggest that 216h hGx generate AGM-like pre-HSC capable of at least short-term multilineage engraftment upon maturation...'. Indeed, this statement is in conflict with previous studies demonstrating that pre-HSCs in the dorsal aorta of the mouse embryo are immature and actually incapable of engraftment.

      We have clearly stated that we do not see haematopoietic engraftment through transplantation of dissociated haemogenic gastruloids, which reach the E10 state containing pre-HSC (revised Fig 4A, 4S1A and Supplementary File 3). Instead, we observed rare myelo-erythroid (revised Fig. 4S2F-G) and myelo-lymphoid (revised Fig. 4E) engraftment upon in vivo maturation of haemogenic gastruloids with preserved 3D organisation. These statements are not contradictory. Nevertheless, we have now more cautiously attributed engraftment to the present of progenitors as a generic designation, and not to pre-HSC (lines 412-414 and 588-592 in the revised manuscript).

      The statement '...low-level production of engrafting cells recapitulates their rarity in vivo, in agreement with the embryo-like qualities of the gastruloid system....' is incorrect. Firstly, no evidence has been provided to show the hGx has formed a dorsal aorta facsimile capable of generating cells with engrafting capacity. Secondly, although engrafting cells are rare in the AGM, approximately one per embryo, they are capable of robust and extensive engraftment upon transplantation.

      As indicated above, the statement in lines 412-414 now reads “Engraftment is erythromyeloid at 4 weeks and lympho-myeloid at 8 weeks, reflecting different classes of progenitors, putatively of YS-like and AGM-like affiliation.” To be clear, with our original statement we meant to highlight that the production of definitive AGM-like haematopoietic progenitors (not all of which are engrafting) in haemogenic gastruloids does not correspond to non-physiological single-lineage programming. We did and do not claim that we achieved production of HSC, which would be long-term engrafting.

      (4) Expression MNX1 transcript and protein in hematopoietic cells in MNX1 rearranged acute myeloid leukaemia (AML) is one cause of AML in infants. In the hGX model of this disease, Mnx1 is overexpressed in the mESCs that are used to form gastruloids. Mnx1 overexpression seems to confer an overall growth advantage on the hGx and increase the serial replating capacity of the small number of hematopoietic cells that are generated. The inefficiency with which the hGx model generates hematopoietic cells makes it difficult to model this disease. The poor quality of the cytospin images prevents accurate identification of cells. The statement that the kit-expressing cells represent leukemic blast cells is not sufficiently validated to support this conclusion. What other stem cell genes are expressed? Surface kit expression also marks mast cells, frequently seen in clonogenic assays of blood cells. Flow cytometric and gene expression analyses using known markers would be required.

      The haemogenic gastruloid model generates haematopoietic and haemato-endothelial cells. MNX1 expands C-Kit+ cells at 144h, which we show to have a haemato-endothelial signature (see revised Fig. 3A-E, Supplementary File 2). We have added additional flow cytometry data showing that the replating cells from MNX1 express CD31 (Figure 6S1A-B).

      Serial replating of CFC assays is a conventional in vitro assay of leukaemia transformation. Critically, colony replating is not maintained in EV control cells, attesting to the transformation potential of MNX1. Although we have not fully-traced the cellular hierarchy of MNX1-driven transformation in the haemogenic gastruloid system, the in vitro replating expands a C-Kit+ cell (revised Fig. 6E), which reflects the surface phenotype of the leukaemia, also recapitulated in the mouse model initiated by MNX1-overexpressing FL cells. Importantly, it recapitulates the transcriptional profile of MNX1leukaemia patients (revised Fig. 7C), which is uniquely expressed by MNX1144h and replated colony cells, but not to MNX1 216h gastruloid cells, arguing against a generic signature of MNX1 overexpression (revised Fig. 7B). Importantly, the MNX1-transformation of haemogenic gastruloid cells is superior to the FL leukaemia model at capturing the unique transcriptional features of MNX1-driven leukaemia, distinct from other forms of AML in the same age group (Fig 7 S1D-F). It is possible that this corresponds to a pre-leukaemia event, and we will explore this in future studies, which are beyond the proof-of-principle nature of this paper.

      (5) In human infant MNX1 AML, the mutation is thought to arise at the fetal liver stage of development. There is no evidence that this developmental stage is mimicked in the hGx model.

      We never claim that the haemogenic gastruloid model mimics the foetal liver. We propose that susceptibility to MNX1 is at the HE-to-EMP transition. Moreover, and importantly, contrary to the Reviewer’s statement, there is no evidence in the literature that the mutation arises in the foetal liver stage, just that the mutation arises before birth (PMID: 38806630), which is different. In a mouse model of MNX1 overexpression, the authors achieve leukaemia engraftment upon MNX1 overexpression in foetal liver, but not in bone marrow cells (PMID: 37317878). This is in agreement with a vulnerability of embryonic / foetal, but not adult cells to the MNX1 expression caused by the translocation. However, haematopoietic cells in the foetal liver originate from YS and AGM precursors, so the origin of the MNX1susceptible cells can be in those locations, rather than the foetal liver itself.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      In this manuscript, the authors develop an exciting new hemogenic gastruloid (hGX) system, which they claim reproduces the sequential generation of various blood cell types. The key advantage of this cellular system would be its potential to more accurately recapitulate the spatiotemporal emergence of hematopoietic progenitors within their physiological niche compared to other available in vitro systems. The authors present a large set of data and also validate their new system in the context of investigating infant leukemia. 

      Strengths: 

      The development of this new in vitro system for generating hematopoietic cells is innovative and addresses a significant drawback of current in vitro models. The authors present a substantial dataset to characterize this system, and they also validate its application in the context of investigating infant leukemia. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The thorough characterization and full demonstration that the cells produced truly represent distinct waves of hematopoietic progenitors are incomplete. The data presented to support the generation of late yolk sac (YS) progenitors, such as lymphoid cells, and aortic-gonad-mesonephros (AGM)-like progenitors, including pre-hematopoietic stem cells (pre-HSCs), by this system are not entirely convincing. Given that this is likely the manuscript's most crucial claim, it warrants further scrutiny and direct experimental validation. Ideally, the identity of these progenitors should be further demonstrated by directly assessing their ability to differentiate into lymphoid cells or fully functional HSCs. Instead, the authors primarily rely on scRNA-seq data and a very limited set of markers (e.g., Ikzf1 and Mllt3) to infer the identity and functionality of these cells. Many of these markers are shared among various types of blood progenitors, and only a well-defined combination of markers could offer some assurance of the lymphoid and pre-HSC nature of these cells, although this would still be limited in the absence of functional assays.

      The identification of a pre-HSC-like CD45⁺CD41⁻/lo C-Kit⁺VE-Cadherin⁺ cell population is presented as evidence supporting the generation of pre-HSCs by this system, but this claim is questionable. This FACS profile may also be present in progenitors generated in the yolk sac such as early erythromyeloid progenitors (EMPs). It is only within the AGM context, and in conjunction with further functional assays demonstrating the ability of these cells to differentiate into HSCs and contribute to long-term repopulation, that this profile could be strongly associated with pre-HSCs. In the absence of such data, the cells exhibiting this profile in the current system cannot be conclusively identified as true pre-HSCs.

      We present 2 additional pieces of evidence to support our claims that we capture YS and AGM stages of haematopoietic development.

      (I) In the new Figures 4A and 4 S1A-C and Supplementary File 3 in the revised manuscript, we project our single-cell RNA-seq data onto (1) developing intra-embryonic pSP and AGM between E8 and E11 (Fig. 4A left, 4S1A) and (2) a single-cell RNA-seq study of HE development which combines haemogenic and haematopoietic cells from the YS, the developing HE and IAHC in the AGM, and FL (Fig. 4A centre, 4S1B). Our data maps E8.25-E10, and captures YS EMP and erythroid and myeloid progenitors, as well as AGM pre-HE, HE and IAHC, with some cells matching HSPC and LMPP, as suggested by the projection onto the Thambyrajah et al data set (already presented in the previous version of the manuscript, and now in Fig. 4A right and 4 S1C). The projection of the scRNA-seq data in presented in lines 314-355 of the revised manuscript. The scRNA-seq data itself was refocused on haemato-endothelial programmes as presented in the revised Fig. 3A-E, described in lines 267-303.

      (II) Given the difficulty in finding markers that specifically associate with AGM haematopoiesis, we inspected the possibility of capturing different regulatory requirements at different stages of gastruloid development mirroring differential effects in the embryo. Polycomb EZH2 is specifically required for EMP differentiation in the YS, but does not affect AGM-derived haematopoiesis; it is also not required for primitive erythroid cells (PMID: 29555646; PMID: 34857757). We treated haemogenic gastruloids from 120h onwards with either DMSO (0.05%) or GSK126 (0.5uM), and inspected the cellularity of gastruloids at 144h, which we equate with YS-EMP, and 216h – putatively AGM haematopoiesis. We show that EZH2 inhibition / GSK126 treatment specifically reduces %CD41+ cells at 144h, but does not reduce %CD41+ or %CD45+ cells at 216h. We have included this experiment in the manuscript in Fig. 2 S2B-C (in text: 209-221).

      These data, together with the scRNA-seq projections described, provide evidence to our claim that 144h haemogenic gastruloids capture YS EMPs, while CD41+ and CD45+ cells isolated at 216h reflect AGM progenitors. We cannot conclude as to the functional nature of the AGM cells from this experiment. The main text has been edited to clarify the experiments pertaining to distinguishing AGM and YS haematopoiesis (lines 180-187; 200-221; 268-304; 315-356).

      The engraftment data presented are also not fully convincing, as the observed repopulation is very limited and evaluated only at 4 weeks post-transplantation. The cells detected after 4 weeks could represent the progeny of EMPs that have been shown to provide transient repopulation rather than true HSCs. 

      In the original version of the manuscript, we stated that there is low level engraftment and did not claim to have generated HSC. Instead, we described cells with short-term engraftment potential. We agree with the Reviewer that the cells we show in the manuscript at 4 weeks could be EMPs (revised Fig. 4B-E and 4 S2D-G). Additionally, we now have 8-week analysis of implant recipients, in which we observed, again low-level, a multi-lineage engraftment of the recipient bone marrow in 1:3 recipients (revised Fig. 4B-E and 4S2F-H). This engraftment is myeloid-lymphoid and therefore likely to have originated in a later progenitor. To be clear, we do not claim that this corresponds to the presence of HSC. It nevertheless supports the maturation of progenitors with engraftment potential. Limiting amounts of material was prioritised for flow cytometry stainings, not allowing PCR analysis. We rephrased Results and Discussion in lines 359-414 and 588-621, respectively, to rectify the nature of the engraftment.      

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):  

      In this study, the authors employ a mouse ES-derived "hemogenic gastruloid" model which they generated and which they claim to be able to deconvolute YS and AGM stages of blood production in vitro. This work could represent a valuable resource for the field. However, in general, I find the conclusions in this manuscript poorly supported by the data presented. Importantly, it isn't clear what exactly are the "YS" and the "AGM"-like stages identified in the culture and where is the data that backs up this claim. In my opinion, the data in this manuscript lack convincing evidence that can enable us to identify what kind of hematopoietic progenitor cells are generated in this system. Therefore, the statement that "our study has positioned the MNX1-OE target cell within the YS-EMP stage (line 540)" is not supported by the evidence presented in this study. Overall, the system seems to be very preliminary and requires further optimization before those claims can be made.

      Specific comments below: 

      (1) The flow cytometric analysis of gastruloids presented in Figure 1 C-D is puzzling. There is a large % of C-Kit+ cells generated, but few VE-Cad+ Kit+ double positive cells. Similarly, there are many CD41+ cells, but very few CD45+ cells, which one would expect to appear toward the end of the differentiation process if blood cells are actually generated. It would be useful to present this analysis as consecutive gating (i.e. evaluating CD41 and CD45 within VE-Cad+ Kit+ cells, especially if the authors think that the presence of VE-Cad+ Kit+ cells is suggestive of EHT). The quantification presented in D is misleading as the scale of each graph is different.

      Fig. 1C-D provide an overview of haemogenic markers during the timecourse of haemogenic gastruloid differentiation, and does indeed show a late up-regulation of CD45, as the Reviewer points out would be expected. The %CD45+ cells is indeed low. However, we should point out that the haemogenic gastruloid protocol, although biased towards mesodermal outputs, does not aim to achieve pure haematopoietic specification, but rather place it in its embryo-like context. We refute that the scale is misleading: it is a necessity to represent the data in a way that is interpretable by the reader: and we made sure from the outset that the gates (in C) are truly representative and annotated, as are the plot axes (in D). Consecutive gating at the 216h-timepoint is shown and quantified in Fig. 2S1D-F, or in the alternative consecutive gating suggested by the Reviewer, in Author response iamge 2 below. At the request of Reviewer 1, we also analysed CD31 and CD34 within CD41 and CD45 populations, again as validation of the emergent haematopoietic character of the cells obtained. This new analysis is shown in revised Fig. 2B, quantified in 2C.

      Author response image 2.

      Flow cytometry analysis of VE-cadherin+ cells in haemogenic gastruloids at 216h of the differentiation protocol, probing co-expression of CD45, CD41 and C-Kit.

      (2) The imaging presented in Figure 1E is very unconvincing. C-Kit and CD45 signals appear as speckles and not as membrane/cell surfaces as they should. This experiment should be repeated and nuclear stain (i.e. DAPI) should be included.

      We included the requested immunofluorescence staining in Figure 1E (216h). We also show the earlier timepoint of 192h here as Author response image 3. In text: lines 158-162.

      Author response image 3.

      Confocal images of haematopoietic production in haemogenic gastruloids. Wholemount, cleared haemogenic gastruloids were stained for CD45 (pseudo-coloured red) and C-Kit antigens (pseudo-coloured yellow) with indirect staining, as described in the manuscript. Flk1-GFP signal is shown in green. Nuclei are contrasted with DAPI. (A) 192h. (B) 216h.

      (3) Overall, I am not convinced that hematopoietic cells are consistently generated in these organoids. The authors should sort hematopoietic cells and perform May-Grunwald Giemsa stainings as they did in Figure 6 to confirm the nature of the blood cells generated.

      It is factual that the data are reproducible and complemented by functional assays shown in revised Fig. 2D-E, which clearly demonstrate haematopoietic output. The single-cell RNA-seq data also show expression of a haematopoietic programme, which we have complemented with biologically independent qRT-PCR analysis of the expression of key endothelial and haematopoietic marker and regulatory genes (revised Fig. 2F; in text: 200-209). As requested, we include Giemsa-Wright’s stained cytospins obtained at 216h to illustrate haematopoietic output. These are shown in revised Fig. 2S2A, in text: lines 194-199. Inevitably, the cytospins will be inconclusive as to the presence of endothelial-tohaematopoietic transition or the generation of haematopoietic stem/progenitor cells, as these cells do not have a distinctive morphology.

      (4) The scRNAseq in Figure 2 is very difficult to interpret. Specific points related to this: - Cluster annotation in Figure 2a is missing and should be included. 

      Why do the heatmaps show the expression of genes within sorted cells? Couldn't the authors show expression within clusters of hematopoietic cells as identified transcriptionally (which ones are they? See previous point)? Gene names are illegible.

      I see no expression of Hlf or Myb in CD45+ cells (Figure 2G). Hlf is not expressed by any of the populations examined (panels E, F, G). This suggests no MPP or pre-HSC are generated in the culture, contrary to what is stated in lines 242-245. (PMID 31076455 and 34589491).Later on, it is again stated that "hGx cells... lacked detection of HSC genes like Hlf, Gfi1, or Hoxa9" (lines 281-283). To me, this is proof of the absence of AGM-like hematopoiesis generated in those gastruloids.

      For a combination of logistic and technical reasons, we performed single-cell RNA-seq using the Smart-Seq2 platform, which is inherently low throughput. We overcame the issue of cell coverage by complementing whole-gastruloid transcriptional profiling at successive time-points with sorting of subpopulations of cells based on individual markers documented in Fig. 1. We clearly stated which platform was used as well as the number and type of cells profiled (Fig. 3S1 and lines 226-241 of the revised manuscript), and our approach is standard. Following suggestions of the Reviewers to further focus our analysis on the haemogenic cellular differentiation within the gastruloids, we revised the presentation of the scRNA-seq data to now provide UMAP projections with representation and quantification of individual genes, including the ones queried by the Reviewer in Fig. 3 and respective supplements. Specifically, re-clustering and highlighting of specific markers are shown in Figure 3A-D and presented in lines 267-303 of the revised manuscript. Complementary independent real-time quantitative (q)PCR analysis showing time-dependent expression of endothelial and haematopoietic markers is now in Figure 2F. In text: 200-208.

      (5) Mapping of scRNA-Seq data onto the dataset by Thambyrajah et al. is not proof of the generation of AGM HE. The dataset they are mapping to only contains AGM cells, therefore cells do not have the option to map onto something that is not AGM. The authors should try mapping to other publicly available datasets also including YS cells.

      We have done this and the data are presented in Figure 4A (Figure 4S1A) and Supplementary File. In text: 314-355. As detailed in response to Reviewer 1, we have conducted projections of our single-cell RNA-seq data against two studies which (1) capture arterial and haemogenic specification in the para-splanchnopleura (pSP) and AGM region between E8.0 and E11 (Hou et al, PMID: 32203131) (revised Fig. 4A and 4 S1A), and (2) uniquely capture YS, AGM and FL progenitors and the AGM endothelial-to-haematopoietic transition (EHT) in the same scRNA-seq dataset (Zhu et al, PMID: 32392346) (revised Fig. 4A and 4 S1B). Specifically in answering the Reviewers’ point, we show that different subsets of haemogenic gastruloid cells sorted on haemogenic surface markers C-Kit, CD41 and CD45 cluster onto pre-HE and HE, intra-aortic clusters and FL progenitor compartments, and to YS EMP and erythroid and myeloid progenitors. This lends support to our claim that the haemogenic gastruloid system specifies both YS-like and AGM-like cells. Please note that we now do point out that some CD41+ cells at 144h project onto IAC, as do cells at the later timepoints, suggesting that AGM-like and YS-EMP-like waves may overlap at the 144h timepoint (lines…). In the future, we will address specific location of these cells, but that corresponds to a largescale spatial transcriptomics analysis requiring extensive optimisation for section capture which is beyond the scope of this manuscript and this revision. 

      (6) Conclusions in Figure 3, named "hGx specify cells with preHSC characteristics" are not supported by the data presented here. Again, I am not convinced that hematopoietic cells can be efficiently generated in this system, and certainly not HSCs or pre-HSCs.

      We have provided evidence in the original manuscript, and now through additional experiments, that there is haematopoietic specification, including of progenitor cells, in the haemogenic gastruloid system. Molecular markers are shown in revised Fig. 2F and Fig. 3 and supplements; CFC assays are shown in revised Fig. 2D-E; cytospins are in revised Fig. 2 S2A; further analysis of 4-week implants and new analysis of 8-week implants (discussed below) are in revised Fig. 4 B-D and Fig. 4 S2 and we discussed the new scRNA-seq projections above. Importantly, we have never claimed, and again do not, that haemogenic gastruloids generate HSC. We accept the Reviewer’s comment that we have not provided sufficient evidence for the specification of pre-HSC-like cells and accordingly now refer more generically and conservatively to progenitors.

      FACS analysis in 3A is again very unconvincing. I do not think the population identified as C-Kit+ CD144+ is real. Also, why not try gating the other way around, as commonly done (e.g. VE-Cad+ Kit+ and then CD41/CD45)?

      Our gating strategy is not unconventional, which was done from a more populated gate onto the less abundant one to ensure that the results are numerically more robust. In the case of haemogenic gastruloids, unlike the AGM preparations the Reviewer may be referring to, CD41 and CD45+ cells are more abundant as there is no circulation of more differentiated haematopoietic cells away from the endothelial structures. This said, we did perform the gating as suggested (Rev Fig. 2), indeed confirming that most VE-cad+ Kit+ cells are CD45+. Interestingly VE-cad+Kit- are predominantly CD41+, reinforcing the haematopoietic nature of these cells.

      The authors must have tried really hard, but the lack of short- or long-engraftment in a number of immunodeficient mouse models (lines 305-313) really suggests that no blood progenitors are generated in their system. I am not familiar with the adrenal gland transplant system, but it seems like a very non-physiological system for trying to assess the maturation of putative pre-HSCs. The data supporting the engraftment of these mice, essentially seen only by PCR and in some cases with a very low threshold for detection, are very weak, and again unconvincing. It is stated that "BFP engraftment of the Spl and BM by flow cytometry was very low level albeit consistently above control (Fig. S4E)" (lines 337-338). I do not think that two dots in a dot plot can be presented as evidence of engraftment.

      We have presented the data with full disclosure and do not deny that the engraftment achieved is low-level and short-term, indicating incomplete maturation of definitive haematopoietic progenitors in the current haemogenic gastruloid system. Indeed, by not wanting to overstate the finding, we were deliberately conservative in our representative flow cytometry plots and focused on the PCR for sensitivity. We now present the full flow cytometry analysis for spleen where we preserved more cells after the genomic DNA extraction (revised Fig. 4C) and call the Reviewer’s attention to the fact that detection of BFP+ cells by PCR and flow cytometry in the recipient animals is consistent between the 2 methods (revised Fig. 4C and D; full gels previously presented now in Fig. 4S2C; sensitivity analysis was also previously available and is now in Fig. 4S2B). In addition, we have now also been able to detect low-level myelo-lymphoid engraftment in the bone marrow and spleen 8 weeks after adrenal implantation, again suggesting the presence of a small number of definitive haematopoietic progenitors that potentially mature from the 3 haemogenic gastruloids implanted (Fig. 4E and 4 S2F-G in the revised manuscript. We rephrased Results and Discussion at lines 359-414 and 589-621, respectively, to rectify the nature of the engraftment which we attribute to progenitors.

      (7) Given the above, I find that the foundations needed for extracting meaningful data from the system when perturbed are very shaky at best. Nevertheless, the authors proceed to overexpress MNX1 by LV transduction, a system previously shown to transform fetal liver cells, mimicking the effect of the t(7;12) AML-associated translocation. Comments on this section:

      The increase in the size of the organoid when MNX1 is expressed is a very unspecific finding and not necessarily an indication of any hematopoietic effect of MNX1 OE.

      We agree with the Reviewer on this point; it is nevertheless a reproducible observation which we thought relevant to describe for completeness and data reproducibility.

      The mild increase of cKit+ cells (Figure 4E) at the 144hr timepoint and the lack of any changes in CD41+ or CD45+ cells suggests that the increase in Kit+ cells % is not due to any hematopoietic effect of MNX1 OE. No hematopoietic GO categories are seen in RNA seq analysis, which supports this interpretation. Could it be that just endothelial cells are being generated?

      The Reviewer is correct that the MNX1-overexpressing cells have a strong endothelial signature, which is present in patients (revised Fig. 5A). We investigated a potential link with C-Kit by staining cells from the replating colonies during the process of in vitro transformation with CD31. We observed that 40-50% of C-Kit+ cells (20-30% total colony cells) co-expressed CD31, at least at early plating. These cells co-exist with haematopoietic cells, namely Ter119+ cells, as expected from the YSlike erythroid and EMP-like affiliation of haematopoietic output from 144h-haemogenic gastruloids. These data are included in Fig. 6S1A-B (in text 506-507) of the revised manuscript.

      (8) There seems to be a relatively convincing increase in replating potential upon MNX1-OE, but this experiment has been poorly characterized. What type of colonies are generated? What exactly is the "proportion of colony forming cells" in Figures 5B-D? The colony increase is accompanied by an increase in Kit+ cells; however, the flow cytometry analysis has not been quantified.

      Given the inability to replate control EV cells, there is not a population to compare with in terms of quantification. The level of C-Kit+ represented in Fig. 6E of the revised manuscript is achieved at plate 2 or 3 (depending on the experiment), both of which are significantly enriched for colony-forming cells relative to control (revised Fig. 6B, D).  

      (9) Do hGx cells engraft upon MNX1-OE? This experiment, which appears not to have been performed, is essential to conclude that leukemic transformation has occurred.

      For the purpose of this study, we are satisfied with confirmation of in vitro transformation potential of MNX1 haemogenic gastruloids, which can be used for screening purposes. Although interesting, in vivo leukaemia engraftment from haemogenic gastruloids is beyond the scope of this study.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Minor comments

      (a) I find the denomination "hGx" very confusing as it would suggest that these gastruloids are human, whereas, in fact, they are murine.

      We agree with the Reviewer on the confusing nomenclature and have edited the manuscript to call “haemGx” instead.

      (b) I find the presence of mast cells in CFC of MNX1-OE cultures very puzzling as this does not bear any resemblance to human leukemia.

      We detect an enrichment of mast cell transcriptional programmes, as defined by the cell type repositories. While it is not mast cells to represent leukaemic cells in patients, this ontology is likely to reflect the developmental stage and origin of progenitors which are affected by MNX1.

      (2) I have a few suggestions to improve figures and tables clarity, to help readers better follow the data presented.

      (a) To enhance readability, it would be beneficial to highlight the genes mentioned in the text within the scRNA-seq figures. Many figures currently display over 30-40 genes in small font sizes, making it difficult to quickly locate specific genes discussed in the text. Additionally, implementing a colorcoding system to categorize these genes according to their proposed lineages would improve clarity and organization.

      We have now performed major re-organisation and re-analyses of the scRNA-seq data, which we believe has improved the readability and clarity of the corresponding sections of the manuscript.

      (b) The data presented in Supplementary Table 1, along with other supplementary tables, are challenging to interpret due to insufficient annotations. Enhancing these tables with clearer and more detailed annotations would significantly improve clarity and aid readers in understanding the supplementary materials.

      Descriptive text has been added to accompany each Supplementary File to aid in understanding the results reported therein.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In addition to what was written in the public review, I would suggest the authors simplify and shorten the text. Currently, a lot of unnecessary detail is included which makes the story very hard to follow. Moreover, the authors should modify the figures to make them more comprehensible, especially for RNA-seq data.

      We have significantly re-arranged and shortened parts of the manuscript, particularly by focusing the Discussion. Results presentation has also been improved through additional analysis and graphic representation of the scRNA-seq data, which we believe has improved the readability and clarity.s

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript aims to elucidate the impact of a prophage within the genome of Shewanella fidelis on its interaction with the marine tunicate Ciona robusta. The authors made a deletion mutant of S. fidelis that lacks one of its two prophages. This mutant exhibited an enhanced biofilm phenotype, as assessed through crystal violet staining, and showed reduced motility. The authors examined the effect of prophage deletion on several genes that could modulate cyclic-diGMP levels. While no significant changes were observed under in vitro conditions, the gene for one protein potentially involved in cyclic-diGMP hydrolysis was overexpressed during microbe-host interactions. The mutant was retained more effectively within a one-hour timeframe, whereas the wild-type (WT) strain became more abundant after 24 hours. Fluorescence microscopy was used to visualize the localization patterns of the two strains, which appeared to differ. Additionally, a significant difference in the expression of one immune protein was noted after one hour, but this difference was not evident after 23 hours. An effect of VCBC-C addition on the expression of one prophage gene was also observed.

      Strengths:

      I appreciate how the authors integrate diverse expertise and methods to address questions regarding the impact of prophages on gut microbiome-host interactions. The chosen model system is appropriate, as it allows for high-throughput experimentation and the application of simple imaging techniques.

      Weaknesses:

      My primary concern is that the manuscript primarily describes observations without providing insight into the molecular mechanisms underlying the observed differences. It is particularly unclear how the presence of the prophage leads to the phenotypic changes related to bacterial physiology and host-microbe interactions.

      We appreciate the overall, enthusiastic reviewer feedback.  The current manuscript presents experimental evidence of the biological impact of the deletion of a stably integrated prophage in the genome of Shewanella fidelis 3313. The molecular mechanisms responsible for these biological effects are currently unknown but based on the limited genetic insight of some predicted gene regions, we can speculate on prophage-mediated influences impacting swimming behaviors. Below, we address additional concerns raised by the reviewer.

      Which specific prophage genes are critical, or is the insertion at a specific site in the bacterial genome the key factor?  While significant effects on bacterial physiology are reported under in vitro conditions, there is no clear attribution to particular enzymes or proteins.

      In this particular case, it is not entirely clear, as most ORFs within the prophage region have unknown functions, i.e., predicted as hypothetical proteins. In addition, the original insertion site does not appear to interrupt any specific gene but may impact adjacent genes/pathways (Fig 1b). Enhanced annotations, along with future targeted deletion methods for distinct prophage segments, will help us better investigate which predicted gene regions influence the observed traits. This will deepen our understanding of the mechanisms that regulate prophage influence on these traits.

      In contrast, when the system is expanded to include the tunicate, differences in the expression of a cyclic-diGMP hydrolase become apparent. Why do we not observe such differences under in vitro conditions, despite noting variations in biofilm formation and motility? Furthermore, given that the bacterial strain possesses two prophages, I am curious as to why the authors chose to target only one and not both.

      Differences in expression patterns of c-di-GMP regulators were also noted in vitro, but they just missed the statistical significance threshold when rho was used as a bacterial reference gene. The expression pattern of pdeB was consistent among each biological replicate, however. In full transparency, pdeB qPCR was originally performed with recA as a reference standard (bioRxiv preprint, ver 1). Here, significant changes in pdeB expression were observed in the in vitro assays comparing WT and ΔSfPat. These results prompted us to study changes in pdeB expression during in vivo colonization experiments, which also revealed significant changes. However, there was a concern that a potential SOS response would also activate recA, despite our preliminary data suggesting SOS was not involved. As a precautionary, we repeated the experiments with rho as a reference gene after it was identified as a stable reference. However, with rho as a reference gene, statistically significant responses were noted during in vivo colonization, but not in the in vitro assays. 

      In the current manuscript, one prophage was targeted based on preliminary findings indicating that the SfPat prophage region influences behaviors likely to impact colonization of the Ciona robusta gut. A separate genetic segment was also previously targeted for deletion as a misidentified prophage-like region, but that strain is not included in the current description. The currently presented data indicate that the observed phenomena can be attributed to the SfPat prophage.

      Regarding the microbe-host interaction, it is not clear why the increased retention ability of the prophage deletion strain did not lead to greater cell retention after 24 hours, especially since no differences in the immune response were observed at that time point.

      A predominantly adherent (non-motile) phenotype would likely facilitate elimination within fecal strings. There is substantial evidence from multiple model systems that strong swimming ability enhances the exploration and colonization of mucosal surfaces. Swimming helps with the penetration of mucus layers, chemotaxis toward epithelial surfaces, and overall “decision-making” in terms of shifting from a free-swimming (planktonic) state in the lumen within dietary material to a more sessile, adherent phenotype at the mucosal surface.

      Concerning the methodological approach, I am puzzled as to why the authors opted for qPCR instead of transcriptomics or proteomics. The latter approaches could have provided a broader understanding of the prophage's impact on both the microbe and the host.

      We agree with the reviewer that a transcriptomics approach would provide a broader understanding of the prophage’s impact on the microbe and animal host. Future studies will include a full multi-omic evaluation of this interaction. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Besides my above mentioned issues, I have a few more mini things:

      (A) what makes S. fidelis being a persistant member of the host microbiome? Please elaborate more on quantitive studies in this respect. –

      Shewanella species are stable members of the Ciona gut, and previous efforts (Dishaw et al, 2016) revealed that chitin and/or secreted host effectors could influence biofilm formation. The Ciona gut produces copious amounts of endogenous chitin-rich mucus, and a variety of bacteria have been identified that thrive under these conditions. In addition, versatile bacteria like Shewanella sp. likely expand the metabolic potential of filter-feeders like Ciona. Thus, our subsequent studies began to focus on these and other microbes isolated from the Ciona gut that appear to be stable residents. Identical strains have been recovered numerous times (since 2011) from this wild population of Ciona robusta.  

      (B) The authors use the word inter kingdom and refer to phage, bacterium and animal. As phages are not part of the three kingdoms of life I believe the terminology is wrong.

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. In this context, we were referring to bacteria+phage as a unit and their interkingdom interaction with the animal host. But we recognize that this term can be misleading. Another, more appropriate term is ‘tripartite,’ and we have changed interkingdom to tripartite as appropriate, e.g., the abstract.

      (C) I like lines 55-61 and was expecting to see in the manuscript what of those things would be true for the chosen prophage.

      We looked at the coding region annotations within the prophage and the adjacent regions. The prophage coding regions are mostly annotated as unknown or predicted proteins, and a few as known phage-related components. We intend to reanalyze future and improved annotations and conduct deletion experiments targeting specific open reading frames (ORFs).

      (D) In line 76 the authors mention a Gödecke reference for Pseudomonas. I believe that this paper only deals with S. oneidensis.

      The inadvertent Gödecke reference has been removed.

      (E) All figures: The captions are too short to understand what the figures are showing and everything is too small and hard to read or see. Along these lines it is often unclear what the many datapoints show. Biological replicates, technical replicates....Overall figure 1 does not seem to contain much information.

      Figures and captions have been improved as suggested. Thank you for bringing this to our attention.

      (F) Figure 3 what are a and b showing?

      Figure and descriptive legend have been improved.

      (G) Figure 4: Why did the author check expression only for one gene after 1 h but several genes after 24 h?

      Since we observed that in vitro VCBP-C alters biofilms of S. fidelis 3313 (Dishaw et al 2016), we hypothesized that the bacteria may alter host VCBP-C expression and that the influence of integrated prophages may further modulate gene expression. Since VCBP-C is endogenously expressed in the gut of Ciona, we expected that early exposure/colonization (one hour) would be crucial for the bacterial-VCBP interactions. Hence, the VCBP-C was our primary target. We then tested multiple immune response genes at 24 hours to get a more detailed understanding of the maturing immune responses. Future studies will expand our efforts using global transcriptomics to understand better the immune response during bacterial exposure and colonization events.

      (H) Do the authors mean stationary or localised?

      We are not sure about the context of the reviewer’s question here but we think our modifications have addressed these concerns. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the manuscript, "Prophage regulation of Shewanella fidelis 3313 motility and biofilm formation: implications for gut colonization dynamics in Ciona robusta", the authors are experimentally investigating the idea that integrated viruses (prophages) within a bacterial colonizer of the host Ciona robusta affect both the colonizer and the host. They found a prophage within the Ciona robusta colonizing bacterium Shewanella fidelis 3313, which affected both the bacteria and host. This prophage does so by regulating the phosphodiesterase gene pdeB in the bacterium when the bacterium has colonized the host. The prophage also regulates the activity of the host immune gene VCBP-C during early bacterial colonization. Prophage effects on both these genes affect the precise localization of the colonizing bacterium, motility of the bacterium, and bacterial biofilm formation on the host. Interestingly, VCBP-C expression also suppressed a prophage structural protein, creating a tripartite feedback loop in this symbiosis. This is exciting research that adds to the emerging body of evidence that prophages can have beneficial effects not only on their host bacteria but also on how that bacteria interacts in its environment. This study establishes the evolutionary conservation of this concept with intriguing implications of prophage effects on tripartite interactions.

      Strengths:

      This research effectively shows that a prophage within a bacterium colonizing a model ascidian affects both the bacterium and the host in vivo. These data establish the prophage effects on bacterial activity and expand these effects to the natural interactions within the host animal. The effects of the prophage through deletion on a suite of host genes are a strength, as shown by striking microscopy.

      Weaknesses:

      Unfortunately, there are abundant negative data that cast some limitations on the interpretation of the data. That is, examining specific gene expression has its limitations, which could be avoided by global transcriptomics of the bacteria and the host during colonization by the prophage-containing and prophage-deleted bacteria (1 hour and 24 hours). In this way, the tripartite interactions leading to mechanism could be better established.

      We thank the reviewer for their comments and recognize this important limitation. As a follow-up to the current study, we plan to perform more comprehensive global meta-transcriptomics analyses to better understand differentially expressed genes across both the host and microbe during colonization.

      Impact:

      The authors are correct to speculate that this research can have a significant impact on many animal microbiome studies, since bacterial lysogens are prevalent in most microbiomes. Screening for prophages, determining whether they are active, and "curing" the host bacteria of active prophages are effective tools for understanding the effects these mobile elements have on microbiomes. There are many potential effects of these elements in vivo, both positive and negative, this research is a good example of why this research should be explored.

      Context:

      The research area of prophage effects on host bacteria in vitro has been studied for decades, while these interactions in combination with animal hosts in vivo have been recent. The significance of this research shows that there could be divergent effects based on whether the study is conducted in vitro or in vivo. The in vivo results were striking. This is particularly so with the microscopy images. The benefit of using Ciona is that it has a translucent body which allows for following microbial localization. This is in contrast to mammalian studies where following microbial localization would either be difficult or near impossible.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      In general, I found that the research shown in this manuscript is solid, and the manuscript is well-written. I have no specific comments about the writing of the manuscript that would be of benefit.

      Figure 1 would benefit from the shrinking of white space between panels a and b. Also, in panel b, it is very difficult to read the x-axis, the number of basepairs. It is suggested to increase this font size.

      Figure 1 has been improved as suggested.

      Figure 2 is fine, however, what do three asterisks (***) in panel a signify? It is not described in the legend. One minor point that affects data understanding as presented, the wildtype (WT) change in expression is normalized to itself, therefore always equaling 1.0. This method of presentation muddies the variation in gene expression in the presence of the prophage. This is not an issue in Figure 2, but does have an effect on understanding Figure 2 - figure supplement 1.

      Figure 2 - figure supplement 1, as stated above, the normalization of the WT change in gene expression to 1.0 makes it difficult to understand the results. Why is pilZ change in gene expression not significant in panel s1a? It seems the median change is 50%, or whatever averaging is done, it's unclear whether this is the median and whether the error bars are standard deviation or some other metric.

      These should be defined in the statistical analysis section of the methods or in the legend itself. Further, in panel s1b, why is the reduction in gene expression of pdeB statistically significant, while a similar reduction in gene expression of pleD is not statistically significant?

      RQ values were calculated from 2<sup>-ddCt</sup>. The error bars in the figures were calculated by adding or subtracting the standard error from RQ. Since WT was used as a reference value for qPCR, the RQ value was normalized as 1 for all replicates and nonparametric tests were used to calculate the statistical significance. The values for pilZ were very close to significant; a value of 0.063 was derived via the Wilcoxon test. Only the changes in expression of pdeB were determined to be statistically significant, via the Wilcoxon test.

      Figure 3 panels a and b would be helped by having the same y-axis for each. It is impressive the amount of WT bacterial colonization takes place in 24 hours, particularly in the absence of the prophage, but it does not appear as impressive when the axes are changed between panels. Similar axes should be considered for every comparative graph.

      Figure 3 - figure supplement 1 legend would benefit from the same description of the animal's digestive locations as in the legend in Figure 3.

      We appreciate these suggestions and have made these changes accordingly. We have remade and combined Figure 3 a and b

      Figure 4, while it is unfortunate that none of the immune genes evaluated had a response to the deletion of the SfPat prophage in S. fidelis 3313 at 24 hours, did any of these genes have an effect at 1 hour of evaluation as VCBP-C did?

      The expression of this expanded gene set was not evaluated at one hour. This time point will, however, be included in our global evaluation of gene expression in our future transcriptome sequencing effort.

      Figure 5, the only question I have with these data is whether or not there is a dose-dependent effect of VCBP-C on SfPat P5 expression?

      Prior studies have found VCBP-C can impact biofilm formation in Shewanella sp. in a dose-dependent manner (some of the data appears in Dishaw et al, 2016). However, we have not yet considered whether VCBP-C impacts the expression of SfPat P5 (a phage capsid component) in a dose-dependent manner. We will consider this in future experimental designs.

      It is mentioned in the introduction (and data shown in the preprint) that there is more than one active prophage in Shewanella fidelis 3313. The preprint data shows that the Mu prophages had little effect on the studies. It may be worth discussing the presence and lack of effects of these Mu prophages. It also may lead to some discussion about the complexities of polylysogeny (as discussed by Silpe, et al, Nature, 2023).

      A full-length, inducible, Mu-like prophage region has been identified in the genome that has not been targeted for deletion, but will be included in follow-up studies. An earlier incomplete genome assembly contributed to the incorrect targeting and deletion of a prior Mu-like region, which was discussed in an earlier preprint version. Discussion and references to that strain have been removed from the more recent preprint versions. For clarity, the current manuscript describes strains that remain focused on the SfPat prophage, noting its contribution to the observed behavioral changes / traits.

      Is there any spontaneous induction of SfPat in vitro or in vivo with temperature change (prophages have been induced with heat stress), excessive UV exposure, or mitomycin C treatment?

      Preliminary induction studies using UV, mitomycin C, and temperature have been completed, but remain inconclusive with SfPat due to inconsistent induction patterns.

      Could you speculate, or perhaps do the experiment, as to whether the addition of VCBP-C to S. fidelis 3313 cultures affects biofilm production? The deletion of SfPat leads to greater biofilm production in vitro, while exogenously added VCBP-C represses SfPat P5 expression, would VCPB-C addition lead to greater biofilm production? Lastly, and this may be a failure of my understanding, is VCBP-C able to bind to S. fidelis? If so, does the prophage alter the bacteria and, consequently, the ability of VCBP-C to bind to the bacteria?

      Our lab is actively working to better understand the physical interactions of VCBP-C and bacteria, particularly lysogenic bacteria. Deletion mutants are helping us better understand the potential influence of the bacterial accessory genome on interactions with host immune mediators. Biofilm assays have been done in the context of VCBP-C (Dishaw et al, 2016). Subsequently, we tested the influence of 50 µg/ml VCBP-C on WT and prophage KO-strains, which include SfPat KO along with neutral (control) regions of the genome. We found that the presence of VCBP-C reduced biofilm formation in WT and phage KO variants at 4 hrs and 24 hrs. However, at 12 hrs, VCBP-C treatment appears to increase biofilm formation in the phage-KO strain. While the role (if any) of SfMu is remains unclear, these preliminary data imply the existence of a feedback circuit (influenced by time) where immune effector binding and prophage influence on host gene expression together shape retention outcomes in the gut microbiome. This hypothesis remains to be tested further.

      Author response image 1.

      WT S. fidelis 3313 was exposed in vitro to 50 µg/ml VCBP-C in stationary cultures. Biofilms were observed for 24hrs.  At 12 hrs, the presence of VCBP-C increased the amount of biofilms, whereas reduced biofilms were observed at 4 and 24hrs. Our findings (manuscript Fig 2a) reveal that SfPat contributes to biofilm formation, exposure to SfPat deletion mutants increases host VCBP-C expression (manuscript Fig. 4a), and VCBP-C binding to WT S. fidelis 3313 reduces the expression of SfPat P5 capsid protein (manuscript Fig. 5). These findings suggest that in vivo exposure/ colonization assays benefit from detailed time-course observations to be further explored in follow-up, future experiments.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In this manuscript, Natarajan and colleagues report on the role of a prophage, termed SfPat, in the regulation of motility and biofilm formation by the marine bacterium Shewanella fidelis. The authors investigate the in vivo relevance of prophage carriage by studying the gut occupation patterns of Shewanella fidelis wild-type and an isogenic SfPat- mutant derivative in a model organism, juveniles of the marine tunicate Ciona robusta. The role of bacterial prophages in regulating bacterial lifestyle adaptation and niche occupation is a relatively underexplored field, and efforts in this direction are appreciated.

      While the research question is interesting, the work presented lacks clarity in its support for several major claims, and, at times, the authors do not adequately explain their data.

      Major concerns:

      (1) Prophage deletion renders the SfPat- mutant derivative substantially less motile and with a higher biofilm formation capacity than the WT (Fig. 2a-b). The authors claim the mutant is otherwise isogenic to the WT strain upon sequence comparison of draft genome sequences (I'll take the opportunity to comment here that GenBank accessions are preferable to BioSample accessions in Table 1). Even in the absence of secondary mutations, complementation is needed to validate functional associations (i.e., phenotype restoration). A strategy for this could be phage reintegration into the mutant strain (PMID: 19005496).

      We are currently investigating complementation strategies. However, there have been some challenges in re-infecting and/or reintegrating the prophage into the genome. A preferred integration site may be damaged due to the deletion approach. While the SfPat prophage has mostly predicted genes of unknown function or significance, we have begun prioritizing the deletion of distinct segments to help identify functional relevance.

      (2) The authors claim that the downshift in motility (concomitant with an upshift in biofilm formation) is likely mediated by the activity of c-di-GMP turnover proteins. Specifically, the authors point to the c-di-GMP-specific phosphodiesterase PdeB as a key mediator, after finding lower transcript levels for its coding gene in vivo (lines 148-151, Fig. 2c), and suggesting higher activity of this protein in live animals (!)(line 229). I have several concerns here:

      (2.1) Findings shown in Fig. 2a-b are in vitro, yet no altered transcript levels for pdeB were recorded (Fig. 2c). Why do the authors base their inferences only on in vivo data?

      (2.2) Somewhat altered transcript levels alone are insufficient for making associations, let alone solid statements. Often, the activity of c-di-GMP turnover proteins is local and/or depends on the activation of specific sensory modules - in the case of PdeB, a PAS domain and a periplasmic sensor domain (PMID: 35501424). This has not been explored in the manuscript, i.e., specific activation vs. global alterations of cellular c-di-GMP pools (or involvement of other proteins, please see below). Additional experiments are needed to confirm the involvement of PdeB. Gaining such mechanistic insights would greatly enhance the impact of this study.

      (2.3) What is the rationale behind selecting only four genes to probe the influence of the prophage on Ciona gut colonization by determining their transcript levels in vitro and in vivo? If the authors attribute the distinct behavior of the mutant to altered c-di-GMP homeostasis, as may be plausible, why did the authors choose those four genes specifically and not, for example, the many other c-di-GMP turnover protein-coding genes or c-di-GMP effectors present in the S. fidelis genome? This methodological approach seems inadequate to me, and the conclusions on the potential implication of PdeB are premature.

      We chose to study genes that were shown previously to influence biofilms and motility in a cyclic-di-GMP dependent manner in a Shewanella spp (Chao et al 2013, S Rakshe 2011). Future transcriptomic efforts and targeted deletion approaches will further define the specific influence of prophages.

      (3) The behavior of the WT strain and the prophage deletion mutant is insufficiently characterized. For instance, how do the authors know that the higher retention capacity reported for the WT strain with respect to the mutant (Fig. 3b) is not merely a consequence of, e.g., a higher growth rate? It would be worth investigating this further, ideally under conditions reflecting the host environment.

      To clarify the method, in vitro growth curves did not suggest any significant difference in growth rate between the WT and the deletion mutant strains. Subsequently, for the in vivo experiments, bacterial cultures were pelleted and resuspended in sterile, nutrient-free artificial seawater. This limits growth until the bacterial strains are introduced to the animals.

      (4) Related to the above, sometimes the authors refer to "retention" (e.g., line 162) and at other instances to "colonization" (e.g., line 161), or even adhesion (line 225). These are distinct processes. The authors have only tracked the presence of bacteria by fluorescence labeling; adhesion or colonization has not been assessed or demonstrated in vivo. Please revise.

      We thank the reviewer for this feedback; the manuscript has been revised accordingly. While we refer to our assays as ‘colonization assays,’ we report results of ‘retention’ of various bacterial strains in the ‘exposed’ animals. Furthermore, when fluorescent staining is utilized, we report retention in defined niches. Since colonization is likely a two-step process, i.e., 1) retention and 2) colonization or long-term establishment of these microbial communities, using these terms correctly is warranted. In separate (unpublished) surveys of adult animals taken from the field, identical strains have been recovered numerous times over a twelve-year period.

      (5) The higher CFU numbers for the WT after 24 h (line 161) might also indicate a role of motility for successful niche occupation or dissemination in vivo. The authors could test this hypothesis by examining the behavior of, e.g., flagellar mutants in their in vivo model.

      Interestingly, we find numerous flagellar/motility-associated protein coding genes like Flg, Fli and Fle present within the S. fidelis genome possessing an EAL domain, implicating them in the regulation of cyclic-di-GMP. Hence, a future global transcriptomic approach will help improve our understanding of the roles of these regulatory pathways.

      (6) The endpoint of experiments with a mixed WT-mutant inoculum (assumedly 1:1? Please specify) was set to 1 h, I assume because of the differences observed in CFU counts after 24 h. In vivo findings shown in Fig. 3c-e are, prima facie, somewhat contradictory. The authors report preferential occupation of the esophagus by the WT (line 223), which seems proficient from evidence shown in Fig. S3. Yet, there is marginal presence of the WT in the esophagus in experiments with a mixed inoculum (Fig. 3d) or none at all (Fig. 3e). Likewise, the authors claim preferential "adhesion to stomach folds" by the mutant strain (line 225), but this is not evident from Fig. 3e. In fact, the occupation patterns by the WT and mutant strain in the stomach in panel 3e appear to differ from what is shown in panel 3d. The same holds true for the claimed "preferential localization of the WT in the pyloric cecum," with Fig. 3d showing a yellow signal that indicates the coexistence of WT and mutant.

      The results section is reworded to improve clarity. The WT and KO are mixed 1:1 to achieve the 10<sup>7</sup> cfu count.

      (7) In general, and especially for in vivo data, there is considerable variability that precludes drawing conclusions beyond mere trends. One could attribute such variability in vivo to the employed model organism (which is not germ-free), differences between individuals, and other factors. This should be discussed more openly in the main text and presented as a limitation of the study.

      Yes, a salient feature of this model is that we can leverage genetic diversity in our experimental design, but it can introduce experimental variability.

      Even with such intrinsic factors affecting in vivo measurements, certain in vitro experiments, which are expected, in principle, to yield more reproducible results, also show high variability (e.g., Fig. 5). What do the authors attribute this variability to?

      For experiments involving VCBP-C protein, we can use affinity-purified protein recovered from live animals, or recombinant protein that we synthesize in-house (Dishaw et al 2011, 2016). In the latter, we often observe slight lot-to-lot variation in affinity for the target (the bacterial surface). To account for this variation and to ensure the observations are robust despite it, production lots can be mixed in additional biological replicates. As such, slight variability in the in vitro assays can be due to this batch effect.

      (8) Line 198-199: Why not look for potential prophage excision directly rather than relying on indirect, presumptive evidence based on qPCR?

      The decision to rely on qPCR of prophage structural genes was based on preliminary data, in particular among lysogens possessing more than one prophage. Neither the plaque assay nor SYBR Gold staining could distinguish among the particles, and TEM imaging was not sufficiently qualitative. Since these prophages do not exclusively produce particles when induced, qPCR targeting structural proteins was found to be most informative.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Other major comments:

      Line 137 (and Fig. 2 legend): The authors did not test chemotaxis towards any specific chemoeffector, only motility. Please correct and see below my comments about motility assays.

      The reviewer is correct; we have modified our descriptors.

      Lines 142-144: The authors conflate quorum sensing with c-di-GMP metabolism. If the authors measured the expression of genes "regulating cyclic di-GMP," it is likely because c-di-GMP is known to regulate the switch between planktonic and sessile lifestyles. However, whether this is mediated by quorum sensing is a separate issue that was not explored in this work. Please revise.

      Thank you; these changes were made accordingly.

      Line 150: c-di-GMP is not a quorum sensing signal; please correct.

      Yes, we corrected the inadvertent yet misleading statement.

      Line 193: Please clarify "RNA was extracted from the biofilms." If S. fidelis was grown on "MA [Marine Agar] for 24 h in the presence or absence of 50 µg/ml VCBP-C" (lines 192-193), was RNA isolated from colonies growing on the plates? Was VCBP-C added to the agar? This is also unclear in the Methods section (lines 381-384), where it seems the authors conducted this experiment using broth cultures in multiwell plates, removing the supernatant, and extracting RNA from the biofilms (i.e., cells adhered to the walls and bottom of the wells?). Why only biofilm cells?

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. We have rewritten the appropriate sections and methods to improve clarity. Following our initial studies, which revealed differential bacterial phenotypes (biofilm formation and motility assays), we decided to target and investigate gene expression in the biofilms. This way, the sessile cells that were not part of the biofilm do not obfuscate the data.

      Lines 204-205: The authors should refer to the behavior of the mutant, since they did not test what happens upon prophage integration, but after prophage deletion.

      The wording has been changed accordingly.

      Lines 206-207: Please explain why the authors state that "these different bacterial phenotypes" (referring to altered biofilm formation and motility) "influence host immune responses in a manner consistent with influences on gut colonization dynamics". What specific relationship are the authors suggesting between these processes, and in what way is this "consistent"?

      We previously demonstrated (Dishaw et al 2016) that copious amounts of VCBP-C protein are present under normal conditions in the gut and mostly found tethered to chitin-rich mucus lining the gut epithelium. The up-regulation of VCBP-C within one hour of exposure to the SfPat mutant relative to the WT S. fidelis is consistent with a role for VCBP-C in modulating bacterial settlement dynamics (Dishaw et al 2016). The mutant phenotype of reduced swimming and increased biofilm production is a likely trigger for the increased production of this secreted immune effector that may influence the retention of this bacterial variant, relative to the WT.

      Line 229: Apart from what I noted above about the authors' claim regarding PdeB activity, I believe the figure referred to here should be Fig. 2, not Fig. 5.

      Thank you for catching that oversight. It has been corrected.

      Figure 1: Was hypothetical protein 2 included in the deletion?

      Yes, the hypothetical protein 2 was included in the deletion

      Figure 3a-b: It is challenging to interpret data on plots using so many colors - including what appears to be a white circle (?) in Fig. 3a. How many replicates are represented here? Is it indeed n=3 in Fig. 3a and n=6 in Fig. 3b?  

      Figure 3a is a bee swarm plot. Each color represents biological replicates, and the smaller circles represent technical replicates. It facilitates showing ALL the data, including the spread of the data. Regarding the number replicates, 3a and 3b are different experiments, with 3a representing a biofilm assay with three biological replicates and 3b a motility assay with six biological replicates.

      Figure 3: An explanation for the abbreviation "FP" is missing.

      Thank you for catching this oversight. The abbreviation has been defined.

      Figure S3: FP, which is proficiently occupied by the WT strain (Fig. S3a), is not labeled in the images provided for the mutant (Fig. S3c-d). It would be helpful to show it for comparison.

      Those other images did not have fecal pellets to label; however, Figure 3c does show a fecal pellet for an animal exposed to both WT and the SfPat mutant.

      Questions and comments regarding methods:

      Lines 290-291, 307: Please indicate an approximate range for "room temperature."

      The information has been added to the revised manuscript.

      Lines 292, 302: Why use hybrid LB/MB broth and agar? And strictly speaking, which LB formula (Lennox/Luria/Miller)?

      The hybrid broth reduces the concentration of salts that can interfere in some assays. The LB formula was Luria, and it is now included in the manuscript.

      Lines 300-302: The conjugation procedure is poorly described. It seems the authors conducted conjugal transfer by biparental mating in broth culture by inoculating a single colony of S. fidelis 3313 into an already grown culture of the E. coli donor strain?

      The biparental mating was done on plates; the manuscript has been clarified.

      Motility assay concerns:

      Swimming motility is generally assayed in soft agar (0.25-0.3% w/v). Why did the authors use 0.5% low-melt agarose? Usually, agar is employed instead of agarose, and such a high concentration of solidifying agent typically prevents proper swimming (see e.g. Kearns 2010).

      Our laboratory uses low-melt agarose for phage propagation and other assays. We continued using it because we observed robust and reproducible results in the swarming and swimming motility assays. In addition, 0.5% agarose is less dense than 0.5% agar, and its consistency is similar to that of the lower percentage soft agar.

      Lines 316-317: Please clarify: what is the "overlay motility assay" that was carried out "overnight at RT and then inoculated onto the center of soft agar"? Was this a two-step experiment? How were bacteria inoculated (stabbed, injected)? If injected, what volume and cell density were used?

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. The methods section has been revised for clarity.

      Line 319: Each variable tested in duplicate? From what I understand, the only variable measured in this test is the diameter of the swimming halos. Do the authors mean they used two biological replicates? If so, please indicate the number of technical replicates as well.

      Multiple biological replicates were performed, each time with two technical replicates. Two perpendicular measurements (of diameter) for each technical replicate was recorded to avoid bias. The methods section has been edited to improve clarity.

      Line 320: Were the swimming halos asymmetrical, hence the need to take two perpendicular measurements? If that was the case, it could indicate an excessive amount of solidifying agent.

      The halos were sometimes asymmetric, but to avoid variation across datasets, it became standard practice to measure perpendicular distances as stated above. 

      Regarding qPCR experiments:

      Please clarify how normalization of transcript levels was performed.

      It seems the authors conducted a double normalization, first with respect to the calibrator (rho), and again using the wild-type as a baseline reference for fold-change calculations (absence of error bars for WT data). If so, please specify on the vertical axes of the figures and in the Methods/figure legends.

      Since, in addition to rho, the authors assessed the expression stability of the "housekeeping" genes gyrB and recA, please also include the primers used for these genes.

      The appropriate manuscript sections have been updated for clarity. The bacterial qPCR was normalized to an internal standard, and then relative expression differences between SfPat and the WT were determined. The missing primer sequences have also been added.

      Observations:

      Figure 2a-b: It is intriguing that the remarkable reduction in motility of the mutant is not associated with a comparably significant increase in biofilm formation.

      A statistically significant increase in biofilm was observed, along with a decrease in motility. As is common in crystal violet assays, some of the tertiary structures were not very stable and likely washed out during processing.

      Additionally, it is noteworthy that data for the mutant in panel 2a exhibit minimal variability, with all OD570 recordings being around 3.0. Did the authors dilute the crystal violet elution solution after adding acetic acid, or might they have reached the saturation limit of the spectrophotometer?

      The eluted acetic acid was not diluted further, and significant changes were observed. If the solution had been further diluted, the observed changes might have been more pronounced. 

      Minor comments and recommendations:

      All the suggested changes below have been incorporated

      • Line 55: "Antibiotic resistance determinants" might be preferable to "genes" to avoid using "genes" twice in the same sentence.

      • Line 75-76: Italicize Pseudomonas aeruginosa.

      • Line 134: Instead of "at least," specify the average fold-change.

      • Line 141: In the heading, refer to the influence of the "prophage" (singular) rather than "prophages" (plural).

      • Discussion (style): Consider using past tense for phrases like "we utilize..." (line 202); "we find..." (line 204), etc.

      • Line 365 and elsewhere: Consider "mRNA levels" or "transcript levels" instead of "gene expression".

      • Table 3: UQ950 is a strain, not a plasmid. I assume the plasmid carried by UQ950 is pSMV3.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      To elucidate the mechanisms and evolution of animal biomineralization, Voigt et al. focused on the sponge phylum - the earliest branching extant metazoan lineages exhibiting biomineralized structures - with a particular emphasis on deciphering the molecular underpinnings of spicule formation. This study centered on calcareous sponges, specifically Sycon ciliatum, as characterized in previous work by Voigt et al. In S. ciliatum, two morphologically distinct spicule types are produced by a set of two different types of cells that secrete extracellular matrix proteins, onto which calcium carbonate is subsequently deposited. Comparative transcriptomic analysis between a region with active spicule formation and other body regions identified 829 candidate genes involved in this process. Among these, the authors focused on the calcarine gene family, which is analogous to the Galaxins, the matrix proteins known to participate in coral calcification. The authors performed three-dimensional structure prediction using AlphaFold, examined mRNA expression of Calcarin genes in spiculeforming cell types via in situ hybridization, conducted proteomic analysis of matrix proteins isolated from purified spicules, and carried out chromosome arrangement analysis of the Calcarin genes.

      Based on these analyses, it was revealed that the combination of Calcarin genes expressed during spicule formation differs between the founder cells-responsible for producing diactines and triactinesand the thickener cells that differentiate from them, underscoring the necessity for precise regulation of Calcarin gene expression in proper biomineralization. Furthermore, the observation that 4 Calcarin genes are arranged in tandem arrays on the chromosome suggests that two rounds of gene duplication followed by neofunctionalization have contributed to the intricate formation of S. ciliatum spicules. Additionally, similar subtle spatiotemporal expression patterns and tandem chromosomal arrangements of Galaxins during coral calcification indicate parallel evolution of biomineralization genes between S. ciliatum and aragonitic corals. 

      Strengths: 

      (1) An integrative research approach, encompassing transcriptomic, genomic, and proteomic analyses as well as detailed FISH. 

      (2) High-quality FISH images of Calcarin genes, along with a concise summary clearly illustrating their expression patterns, is appreciated. 

      (3) It was suggested that thickener cells originate from founder cells. To the best of my knowledge, this is the first study to demonstrate trans-differentiation of sponge cells based on the cell-typespecific gene expression, as determined by in situ hybridization. 

      (4) The comparison between Calcarins of Calcite sponge and Galaxins of aragonitic corals from various perspective-including protein tertiary structure predictions, gene expression profiling during calcification, and chromosomal sequence analysis to reveal significant similarities between them. 

      We thank the reviewer for this assessment. 

      (1) The conclusions of this paper are generally well supported by the data; however, some FISH images require clearer indication or explanation.

      We have modified Fig. 3 by including some insets indicating the depicted part of the sponge body and to change the color-scheme as suggested by reviewer3 for the FISH images. In accordance to the following comment, we decided to remove single-channel views in Fig. 3 A. 

      (2) Figure S2 (B, C, D): The fluorescent signals in these images are difficult to discern. If the authors choose to present signals at such low magnification, enhancing the fluorescence signals would improve clarity. Additionally, incorporating Figure S2A as an inset within Figure S2E may be sufficient to convey the necessary information about signal localization. 

      We changed the figure according to the suggestions.

      (3) Figure S3A: The claim that Cal2-expressing spherical cells are closely associated with the choanoderm at the distal end of the radial tube is difficult to follow. Are these Cal2-expressing spherical cells interspersed among choanoderm cells, or are they positioned along the basal surface of the choanoderm? Clarifying their precise localization and indicating it in the image would strengthen the interpretation. 

      In the figure, the view is on the choanoderm that lines the inner surface of the radial tube. Our interpretation is that the spherical cells are positioned at the basal surface of the choanoderm. We updated Fig. S3, which now includes another view to support our interpretation and also indicate some choanocytes.

      (4) To further highlight the similarities between S.ciliatum and aragonitic corals in the molecular mechanisms of calcification, consider including a supplementary figure providing a concise depiction of the coral calcification process. This would offer valuable context for readers.

      We considered this suggestion, and have included such a supplementary figure (Fig. S9).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This paper reports on the discovery of calcarins, a protein family that seems involved in calcification in the sponge Sycon ciliatum, based on specific expression in sclerocytes and detection by mass spectrometry within spicules. Two aspects stand out: (1) the unexpected similarity between Sycon calcarins and the galaxins of stony corals, which are also involved in mineralization, suggesting a surprising, parallel co-option of similar genes for mineralization in these two groups; (2) the impressively cell-type-specific expression of specific calcarins, many of which are restricted to either founder or thickener cells, and to either diactines, triactines, or tetractines. The finding that calcarins likely diversified at least partly by tandem duplications (giving rise to gene clusters) is a nice bonus. 

      Strengths: 

      I enjoyed the thoroughness of the paper, with multiple lines of evidence supporting the hypothesized role of calcarins: spatially and temporally resolved RNAseq, mass spectrometry, and whole-mount in situ hybridization using CISH and HCR-FISH (the images are really beautiful and very convincing). The structural predictions and the similarity to galaxins are very surprising and extremely interesting, as they suggest parallel evolution of biomineralization in sponges and cnidarians during the Cambrian explosion by co-option of the same "molecular bricks". 

      Weaknesses: 

      I did not detect any major weakness, beyond those inherent to working with sponges (lack of direct functional inhibition of these genes) or with fast-evolving gene families with complex evolutionary histories (lack of a phylogenetic tree that would clarify the history of galaxins/calcarins and related proteins). 

      We thank the reviewer for this assessment and the detailed comments be addressed below.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      The study explores the extent to which the biomineralization process in the calcitic sponge Sycon ciliatum resembles aragonitic skeleton formation in stony corals. To investigate this, the authors performed transcriptomic, genomic, and proteomic analyses on S. ciliatum and examined the expression patterns of biomineralization-related genes using in situ hybridization. Among the 829 differentially expressed genes identified in sponge regions associated with spicule formation, the authors focused on calcarin genes, which encode matrix proteins analogous to coral galaxins. The expression patterns of calcarins were found to be diverse but specific to particular spicule types. Notably, these patterns resemble those of galaxins in stony corals. Moreover, the genomic organization of calcarine genes in S. ciliatum closely mirrors that of galaxin genes in corals, suggesting a case of parallel evolution in carbonate biomineralization between calcitic sponges and aragonitic corals. 

      Strengths: 

      The manuscript is well written, and the figures are of high quality. The study design and methodologies are clearly described and well-suited to addressing the central research question. Particularly noteworthy is the authors´ integration of various omics approaches with molecular and cell biology techniques. Their results support the intriguing conclusion that there is a case of parallel evolution in skeleton-building gene sets between calcitic sponges and aragonitic corals. The conclusions are well supported by the data and analyses presented. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The manuscript is strong, and I have not identified any significant weaknesses in its current form. 

      We thank the reviewer for the insight and addressed the detailed comments below.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The description of the region "radial tube" is unclear. Please define and explain it at its first mention in the manuscript, and, if possible, refer to the appropriate figure(s) (e.g., Figure 1A). 

      We now explain radial tubes at the beginning of the results and added a label in figure 1A. “Sycon ciliatum is a tube-shaped sponge with a single apical osculum and a sponge wall of radial tubes around the central atrium (Fig. 1A). The radial tubes are internally lined with choanoderm, which forms elongated chambers in an angle of approximately 90° to the tube axis”. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Scientific suggestions: 

      (1) Page 13: "Despite their presence in the same orthogroups, the octocoral and stony coral proteins were only distantly related to the calcareous sponge calcarins (e.g., 12-24% identity between octocoral and calcareous sequences in orthogroup Cal 2-4-6), resulting in poor alignment. Their homology to calcarins, therefore, remains to be determined." Could 3D structures of these coral proteins be predicted with AlphaFold to substantiate (or nuance) the comparison with calcarins? 

      We run additional alphafold predictions for two octocoral and two scleractinian galaxins. A galaxin-like sequence from Pinnigorgia flava was only a short fragment and therefore we did not attempt any structure predictions. The result shows that the octocoral galaxin-like proteins show some structural similarity (12 beta-harpins), while the scleractinian galaxin-like proteins differ from the sponge counterparts of the same orthogroup. We added this information to the results and in the new Fig. S7.

      Minor improvements to the text: 

      (1)  Page 7 : "The expression of Cal1 to Cal8 was investigated using chromogenic in situ hybridization (CISH) and hairpin-chain reaction fluorescence in situ hybridization (HCR-FISH), confirming their presence in sclerocytes." - Figure 3 should be cited here. 

      We refer to the figure now.

      (2) Page 8-9: "Cal6 expression mirrors that of Cal2, occurring in rounded cells at the distal tip of radial tubes and in a ring of cells around the oscular ring." - Please cite a figure here. 

      We refer now to Fig. 3K

      (3) Page 11-12: Please define eigengene, this term is not necessarily common knowledge. 

      We provide now a short definition in this sentence: “ The analysis provided eight meta-modules, of which four showed significant changes in expression module eigengenes —summary profiles that capture the overall expression pattern of each module— between samples with high spicule formation context (osculum region and regeneration stages older than four days) and samples with low spicule formation (sponge-wall and early regeneration stages until day 3-4) (Fig. S5).” 

      (4) Page 13: "Species without skeletons, such as the cnidarians Hydra, Actinia, Exaiptasia, and Nematostella, also possess galaxin-like proteins." This is too concise - can you explain what evidence was used? PANTHER, AlphaFold, OrthoFinder, Blastp...? 

      The evidence used is from PANTHER, and we enhanced clarification of this by modifying the last sentence of the section.

      (5) Page 20: "We have identified calcarins, galaxin-like proteins, as crucial components of the biomineralization toolkit in calcareous sponges." I'm not sure you showed they are crucial (this would require functional evidence). Perhaps "novel" components or some other adjective would fit better. 

      We changed the adjective to “novel”.

      Suggestions for the figures: 

      (1) Figure 1A: radial tubes should be labelled. 

      A label was added.

      (2) Figure 3 is beautiful but hard to parse. The name of all markers should be written on each panel (notably B, C, and D) and ideally placed in a consistent position (top right corner?) so that the reader's eye doesn't have to look for them anew in each panel. Consider depicting the same gene with the same color in all panels if possible (confocal imaging gives virtual colors anyway, there's no reason to be bound to the real-life color of the fluorophores used - if that was the original intent). Finally, the red/green color scheme is not colorblind-readable, so please consider switching to another scheme (white/cyan/magenta, for example).

      We have updated the figure according to the suggestions. The names of all markers are now included on each panel. Placing them in the upper right corner was not feasible for all panels, so we adjusted their placement as needed. Reoccurring genes are shown in the same color where possible. To improve accessibility for individuals with red/green color vision deficiency, we adopted a cyan/magenta/yellow color scheme. Each HCR-FISH image was processed in ImageJ by splitting the image into channels, applying cyan, magenta, or yellow lookup tables, converting each channel to RGB, and then stacking and blending them using the Z-Project function with maximum intensity projection. Since the original channel information is not preserved after this processing, we provide the original red/green/blue version of the figure in the supplementary material in Fig S11. Additionally, we added small sketches of Figure 1A to indicate the sponge body regions depicted, where relevant.

      (3) Figure S3: the blue staining is not explained. It is also unclear where choanocytes are - could individual choanocytes be indicated with arrows or lines? 

      We added the information to the figure legend. The blue channel shows “Autofluorescence detected with the Leica TXR filter (approx. 590–650 nm), included to help distinguish true signal from background autofluorescence observed in the FITC channel (used for Spiculin detection).”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      I have no major concerns about the manuscript - only minor edits and comments, which are listed below: 

      (1) On page 13, the authors refer to Figure S8; however, I believe this should be Figure S7. 

      We now refer to the correct Figure. Because of introducing a new Fig. S7, now the correct reference is Fig. S8.

      (2) On page 16, please correct "Spciulin" to "Spiculin". 

      Now corrected.

      (3) On page 17, there are two commas following "(Sycon)"; please remove one. 

      Corrected.

      (4) In the Data Accessibility section, none of the provided links appear to work. Please ensure all links are functional. 

      We apologize for this oversight and now provide working links. 

      (5) In Figure 3, the description of panel L is missing from the figure legend. 

      We added the description of this panel.

      (6) On page 39, change "Fig. 4" to "Figure 4" to maintain consistency throughout the manuscript. 

      Changed.

      (7) Figure S7 is not cited in the main text. Please, address this. 

      Corrected (see above at point 1)

      (8) In the legend for Table S2, the reference to Soubigou et al. (3) is incorrect, as it is not listed in the SI reference section. Please correct this. 

      Soubigou et al. (2020) is now included in the SI reference list.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Migration of the primordial germ cells (PGCs) in mice is asynchronous, such that leading and lagging populations of migrating PGCs emerge. Prior studies found that interactions between the cells the PGCs encounter along their migration routes regulates their proliferation. In this study, the authors used single cell RNAseq to investigate PGC heterogeneity and to characterize their niches during their migration along the AP axis. Unlike prior scRNAseq studies of mammalian PGCs, the authors conducted a time course covering 3 distinct stages of PGC migration (pre, mid, and post migration) and isolated PGCs from defined somite positions along the AP axis. In doing so, this allowed the authors to uncover differences in gene expression between leading and lagging PGCs and their niches and to investigate how their transcript profiles change over time. Among the pathways with the biggest differences were regulators of actin polymerization and epigenetic programming factors and Nodal response genes. In addition, the authors report changes in somatic niches, specifically greater non-canonical WNT in posterior PGCs compared to anterior PGCs. This relationship between the hindgut epithelium and migrating PGCs was also detected in reanalysis of a previously published dataset of human PGCs. Using whole mount immunofluorescence, the authors confirmed elevated Nodal signaling based on detection of the LEFTY antagonists and targets of Nodal during late stage PGC migration. Taken together, the authors have assembled a temporal and spatial atlas of mouse PGCs and their niches. This resource and the data herein provide support for the model that interactions of migrating mouse PGCs with their niches influences their proliferation, cytoskeletal regulation, epigenetic state and pluripotent state.

      Overall, the findings provide new insights into heterogeneity among leading and lagging PGC populations and their niches along the AP axis, as well as comparisons between mouse and human migrating PGCs. The data are clearly presented, and the text is clear and well-written. This atlas resource will be valuable to reproductive and developmental biologists as a tool for generating hypotheses and for comparisons of PGCs across species.

      Strengths:

      (1) High quality atlas of individual PGCs prior to, during and post migration and their niches at defined positions along the AP axis.

      (2) Comparisons to available datasets, including human embryos, provide insight into potentially conserved relationships among PGCs and the identified pathways and gene expression changes.

      (3) Detailed picture of PGC heterogeneity.

      (4) Valuable resource for the field.

      (5) Some validation of Nodal results and further support for models in the literature based on less comprehensive expression analysis.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) No indication of which sex(es) were used for the mouse data and whether or not sex-related differences exist or can excluded at the stages examined. This should be clarified.

      We have added: “Embryos of both sexes were pooled without genotyping, as the timepoints analyzed were prior to sex specification” to both the Animals section of the Materials and Methods and the Figure 1 legend. In addition, bioinformatic evaluation of potential sex biases in Nodal-Lefty signaling using Y-chromosome gene expression is reported in supplementary figure 4 and discussed in Discussion paragraph 2.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This work addresses the question of how 'leading' and 'lagging' PGCs differ, molecularly, during their migration to the mouse genital ridges/gonads during fetal life (E9.5, E10.5, E11.5), and how this is regulated by different somatic environments encountered during the process of migration. E9.5 and E10.5 cells differed in expression of genes involved in canonical WNT signaling and focal adhesions. Differences in cell adhesion, actin cytoskeletal dynamics were identified between leading and lagging cells, at E9.5, before migration into the gonads. At E10.5, when some PGCs have reached the genital ridges, differences in Nodal signaling response genes and reprogramming factors were identified. This last point was verified by whole mount IF for proteins downstream of Nodal signaling, Lefty1/2. At E11.5, there was upregulation of genes associated with chromatin remodeling and oxidative phosphorylation. Some aspects of the findings were also found to be likely true in human development, established via analysis of a dataset previously published by others.

      Strengths:

      The work is strong in that a large number of PGCs were isolated and sequenced, along with associated somatic cells. The authors dealt with problem of very small number of migrating mouse PGCs by pooling cells from embryos (after ascertaining age matching using somite counting). 'Leading' and 'lagging' populations were separated by anterior and posterior embryo halves and the well-established Oct4-deltaPE-eGFP reporter mouse line was used.

      Weaknesses:

      The work seems to have been carefully done, but I do not feel the manuscript is very accessible, and I do not consider it well written. The novel findings are not easy to find. The addition of at least one figure to show the locations of putative signaling etc. would be welcome.

      Thank you for the excellent suggestion. Fig. 6 has been added to highlight the main novel findings of this work and integrate them among contributions of earlier studies to provide a more complete view of signaling pathways and cell behaviors governing PGC migration.

      (1) The initial discussion of CellRank analysis (under 'Transcriptomic shifts over developmental time...' heading) is somewhat confusing - e.g. If CellRank's 'pseudotime analysis' produces a result that seems surprising (some E9.5 cells remain in a terminal state with other E9.5 cells) and 'realtime analysis' produces something that makes more sense, is there any point including the pseudotime analysis (since you have cells from known timepoints)? Perhaps the 'batch effects' possible explanation (in Discussion) should be introduced here. Do we learn anything novel from this CellRank analysis? The 'genetic drivers' identified seem to be genes already known to be key to cell transitions during this period of development.

      Thank you for this important observation. We have clarified the text in this section and added “This discrepancy may reflect differences in differentiation potential of some E9.5 PGCs that end in a terminal state among anterior E9.5 PGCs, but could also result from technical batch effects generated during library preparation. These possible interpretations are further discussed in the Discussion section.” to the pertinent results section and added additional relevant thoughts on the implications of this finding in Discussion paragraphs 4 and 7. We feel that it is important to include both results to the reader, as it is challenging to differentiate between heterogeneous developmental and migratory potential among E9.5 anterior PGCs and differential influence of batch effects across sequencing libraries with the data available.

      (2) In Discussion - with respect to Y-chromosome correlation, it is not clear why this analysis would be done at E10.5, when E11.5 data is available (because some testis-specific effect might be more apparent at the later stage).

      Since we had identified autocrine Nodal signaling primarily in anterior late migratory PGCs at E10.5 and knew that Nodal signaling was involved in sex specification of testicular germ cells into prospermatogonia by E12.5, we wanted to determine whether the Nodal signaling in late migratory PGCs at E10.5 was likely to be a sex-specific effect or was common to PGCs in both sexes. This was assessed in supplementary figure 4 and determined unlikely to be related to sex specification of PGCs as Nodal signaling was not strongly correlated with Y-chromosome transcripts in migratory PGCs. Assessing the relationship between Nodal signaling and Y-chromsome transcription at E11.5, when migration is complete, would be unlikely to help us further understand the dynamics of Nodal signaling during late PGC migration.

      (3) Figure 2A - it seems surprising that there are two clusters of E9.5 anterior cells

      Thank you for the interesting observation! One possibility is that the two states represent differential developmental competence as is suggested by the presence of one E9.5 anterior cluster along the differentiation trajectory in Fig 2A and one not within this differentiation trajectory. Another is that technical aspects of generating these sequencing libraries affected some cells more than others, resulting in clustering of highly affected and less affected cells, which would also be consistent with some E9.5 anterior cells lying within the differentiation trajectory and some not. Since it is challenging to differentiate between these possibilities with the data available, we have intentionally avoided overstating interpretations of this result in the manuscript text. We have included discussion of the potential implications of the transcriptional divergence you identify in Discussion paragraphs 4 and 7.

      (4) Figure 5F - there does seem to be more LEFTY1/2 staining in the anterior region, but also more germ cells as highlighted by GFP

      This is true; based on our selected anatomic landmarks for “anterior” and “posterior” as indicated in Methods, the “anterior” compartment typically contains more PGCs. Thus, we have included violin plots with all data points shown of signal intensities of both LEFTY1/2 and pSMAD2/3 in Fig. 5G and 5I so that the reader can evaluate the entire distribution of PGC signal intensities for each embryo.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The migration of primordial germ cells (PGCs) to the developing gonad is a poorly understood, yet essential step in reproductive development. Here, the authors examine whether there are differences in leading and lagging migratory PGCs using single-cell RNA sequencing of mouse embryos. Cleverly, the authors dissected embryonic trunks along the anterior-to-posterior axis prior to scRNAseq in order to distinguish leading and lagging migratory PGCs. After batch corrections, their analyses revealed several known and novel differences in gene expression within and around leading and lagging PGCs, intercellular signaling networks, as well as number of genes upregulated upon gonad colonization. The authors then compared their datasets with publicly available human datasets to identify common biological themes. Altogether, this rigorous study reveals several differences between leading and lagging migratory PGCs, hints at signatures for different fates among the population of migratory PGCs, and provides new potential markers for post-migratory PGCs in both humans and mice. While many of the interesting hypotheses that arise from this work are not extensively tested, these data provide a rich platform for future investigations.

      Strengths:

      The authors have successfully navigated significant technical challenges to obtain a substantial number of mouse migratory primordial germ cells for robust transcriptomic analysis. Here the authors were able to collect quality data on ~13,000 PGCs and ~7,800 surrounding somatic cells, which is ten times more PGCs than previous studies.

      The decision to physically separate leading and lagging primordial germ cells was clever and well-validated based on expected anterior-to-posterior transcriptional signatures.

      Within the PGCs and surrounding tissues, the authors found many gene expression dynamics they would expect to see both along the PGC migratory path as well as across developmental time, increasing confidence in the new differentially expressed genes they found.

      The comparison of their mouse-based migratory PGC datasets with existing human migratory PGC datasets is appreciated.

      The quality control, ambient RNA contamination elimination, batch correction, cell identification and analysis of scRNAseq data were thorough and well-done such that the new hypotheses and markers found through this study are dependable.

      The subsetting of cells in their trajectory analysis is appreciated, further strengthening their cell terminal state predictions.

      Weaknesses:

      Although it is useful to compare their mouse-based dataset with human datasets, the authors used two different analysis pipelines for each dataset. While this may have been due to the small number of cells in the human dataset as mentioned, it does make it difficult to compare them.

      Direct comparisons between findings in human and mouse focused on CellChat cell-cell communication prediction results, which were conducted in an identical fashion using the same analysis methods for both datasets.

      There were few validation experiments within this study. For one such experiment, whether there is a difference in pSMAD2/3 along the AP axis is unclear and not quantified as was nicely done for Lefty1/2.

      Additional validation of the pSMAD2/3 signal intensity along the AP axis was performed and is now included in Fig. 5.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      In this manuscript, Tiedje and colleagues longitudinally track changes in parasite numbers across four time points as a way of assessing the effect of malaria control interventions in Ghana. Some of the study results have been reported previously, and in this publication, the authors focus on age-stratification of the results. Malaria prevalence was lower in all age groups after IRS. Follow-up with SMC, however, maintained lower parasite prevalence in the targeted age group but not the population as a whole. Additionally, they observe that diversity measures rebound more slowly than prevalence measures. This adds to a growing literature that demonstrates the relevance of asymptomatic reservoirs. 

      Strengths:  

      Overall, I found these results clear, convincing, and well-presented. There is growing interest in developing an expanded toolkit for genomic epidemiology in malaria, and detecting changes in transmission intensity is one major application. As the authors summarize, there is no one-size-fits-all approach, and the Bayesian MOIvar estimate developed here has the potential to complement currently used methods, particularly in regions with high diversity/transmission. I find its extension to a calculation of absolute parasite numbers appealing as this could serve as both a conceptually straightforward and biologically meaningful metric.

      We thank the reviewer for this positive review of our results and approach.

      Weaknesses:

      While I understand the conceptual importance of distinguishing among parasite prevalence, mean MOI, and absolute parasite number, I am not fully convinced by this manuscript's implementation of "census population size".

      This reviewer remains unconvinced of the use of the term “census population size”. This appears to be due to the dependence of the term on sample size rather than representing a count of a whole population. To give context to our use we are clear in the study presented that the term describes a count of the parasite “strains” in an age-specific sample of a human population in a specified location undergoing malaria interventions. 

      They have suggested instead using “sample parasite count”.  We argue that this definition is too specific and less applicable when we extrapolate the same concept to a different denominator, such as the population in a given area. Importantly, our ecological use of a census allows us to count the appearance of the same strain more than once should this occur in different people. 

      The authors reference the population genetic literature, but within the context of that field, "census population size" refers to the total population size (which, if not formally counted, can be extrapolated) as opposed to "effective population" size, which accounts for a multitude of demographic factors. There is often interesting biology to be gleaned from the magnitude of difference between N and Ne.

      As stated in the introduction we have been explicit in saying that we are not using a population genetic framework. Exploration of N and Ne in population genetics has merit. How this is reconciled when using a “strain” definition and not neutral markers would need to be assessed.  

      In this manuscript, however, "census population size" is used to describe the number of distinct parasites detected within a sample, not a population. As a result, the counts do not have an immediate population genetic interpretation and cannot be directly compared to Ne. This doesn't negate their usefulness but does complicate the use of a standard population genetic term.

      We are clear we are defining a census of parasite strains in an age-specific sample of a population living in two catchment areas of Bongo District. We appreciate the concern of the reviewer and have now further edited the relevant paragraphs in both the Introduction (Lines 75-80) and the Discussion (Lines 501-506) to make very clear the dependence of the reported quantity on sample size, but also its feasible extrapolation consistent with the census of a population. 

      In contrast, I think that sample parasite count will be most useful in an epidemiological context, where the total number of sampled parasites can be contrasted with other metrics to help us better understand how parasites are divided across hosts, space and time. However, for this use, I find it problematic that the metric does not appear to correct for variations in participant number. For instance, in this study, participant numbers especially varied across time for 1-5 year-olds (N=356, 216, 405, and 354 in 2012, 2014, 2015, and 2017 respectively).

      The reviewer has made an important point that for the purpose of comparisons across the four surveys or study time points (i.e., 2012, 2014, 2015, and 2017), we should "normalize" the number of individuals considered for the calculation of the "census population size".  Given that this quantity is a sum of the estimated MOI<sub>var,,</sub> we need to have constant numbers for its values to be compared across the surveys, within age group and the whole population. This is needed not only to get around the issue of the drop in 1-5 year olds surveyed in 2014 but to also stabilize the total number of individuals for the whole sample and for specific age groups. One way to do this is to use the smaller sample size for each age group across time, and to use that value to resample repeatedly for that number of individuals for surveys where we have a larger sample size. This has now been updated included in the manuscript as described in the Materials and Methods (Lines 329-341) and in the Results (Lines 415-430; see updated Figure 4 and Table supplement 7). This correction produces very similar results to those we had presented before (see updated Figure 4 and Table supplement 7).   

      As stated in our previous response we have used participant number in an interrupted time series where the population was sampled by age to look at age-specific effects of sequential interventions IRS and SMC. As shown in Table supplement 1 of the 16 age-specific samples of the total population, we have sampled very similar proportions of the population by age group across the four surveys. The only exception was the 1-5 year-old age group during the survey in 2014. We are happy to provide additional details to further clarify the lower number (or percentage) of 1-5 year olds (based on the total number of participants per survey) in 2014 (~12%; N = 216) compared to the other surveys conducted 2012, 2015, and 2017 (~18-20%; N = 356, 405, and 354, respectively). Please see Table supplement 1 for the total number of participants surveyed in each of the four surveys (i.e., 2012, 2014, 2015, and 2017).   

      This sample size variability is accounted for with other metrics like mean MOI. 

      We agree that mean MOI by age presents a way forward with variable samples to scale up. Please see updated Figure supplement 8.  

      In sum, while the manuscript opens up an interesting discussion, I'm left with an incomplete understanding of the robustness and interpretability of the new proposed metric.”

      We thank you for your opinion. We have further edited the manuscript to make clear our choice of the term and the issue of sample size.  We believe the proposed terminology is meaningful as explained above.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary

      The manuscript coins a term "the census population size" which they define from the diversity of malaria parasites observed in the human community. They use it to explore changes in parasite diversity in more than 2000 people in Ghana following different control interventions. 

      Strengths:

      This is a good demonstration of how genetic information can be used to augment routinely recorded epidemiological and entomological data to understand the dynamics of malaria and how it is controlled. The genetic information does add to our understanding, though by how much is currently unclear (in this setting it says the same thing as age stratified parasite prevalence), and its relevance moving forward will depend on the practicalities and cost of the data collection and analysis. Nevertheless, this is a great dataset with good analysis and a good attempt to understand more about what is going on in the parasite population.

      Thank you to the reviewer for their supportive assessment of our research.

      Weaknesses

      None

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      New figure supplement 8 - x-axis says percentage but goes between 0-1, so is a proportion

      We thank the reviewer for bringing this to our attention. We have amended the x-axis labels accordingly for Figure supplement 8.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Ozcan et al., presents compelling evidence demonstrating the latent potential of glial precursors of the adult cerebral cortex for neuronal reprogramming. The findings substantially advance our understanding of the potential of endogenous cells in the adult brain to be reprogrammed. Moreover, they describe a molecular cocktail that directs reprogramming toward corticospinal neurons (CSN).

      Strengths:

      Experimentally, the work is compelling and beautifully designed, with no major caveats. The main conclusions are fully supported by the experiments. The work provides a characterization of endogenous progenitors, genetic strategies to isolate them, and proof of concept of exploiting these progenitors' potential to produce a specific desired neuronal type with "a la carte" combination of transcription factors.

      Weaknesses:

      Some issues need to be addressed or clarified before publication. The manuscript requires editing. It is dense and rich in details while in other parts there are a few mistakes.

      We thank the reviewer for their excellent summary and for their extremely positive review of our paper. We are pleased that the experimental design and conclusions were judged to be wellsupported.

      We have revised the paper to enhance clarity, include additional relevant citations, and refine terminology in some sections of the original version.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful review and agree that these revisions enhance the paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Here the authors show a novel direct neuronal reprogramming model using a very pure culture system of oligodendrocyte progenitor cells and demonstrate hallmarks of corticospinal neurons to be induced when using Neurogenin2, a dominant-negative form of Olig2 in combination with the CSN master regulator Fezf2.

      Strengths:

      This is a major achievement as the specification of reprogrammed neurons towards adequate neuronal subtypes is crucial for repair and still largely missing. The work is carefully done and the comparison of the neurons induced only by Neurogenin 2 versus the NVOF cocktail is very interesting and convincingly demonstrates a further subtype specification by the cocktail.

      Weaknesses:

      As carefully as it is done in vitro, the identity of projection neurons can best be assessed in vivo. If this is not possible, it could be interesting to co-culture different brain regions and see if these neurons reprogrammed with the cocktail, indeed preferentially send out axons to innervate a co-cultured spinal cord versus other brain region tissue.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s positive evaluation of our work and their recognition of its significance in advancing neuronal subtype specification through directed differentiation of endogenous progenitors. 

      We agree with the reviewer’s suggestion that a very interesting future stage of this work would be to investigate the projection neuron identity in vivo. We aim to pursue follow-up studies to investigate in vivo integration and connectivity of such neurons generated by directed differentiation from endogenous SOX6+/NG2+ cortical progenitors. As the reviewer insightfully suggests, co-culturing different brain regions with these neurons could offer an alternative strategy to partially assess potential preferential connectivity into cultured spinal cord vs. alternate tissue.

      We agree with the reviewer that future investigation in vivo will further strengthen the implications of this work.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Ozkan, Padmanabhan, and colleagues aim to develop a lineage reprogramming strategy towards generating subcerebral projection neurons from endogenous glia with the specificity needed for disease modelling and brain repair. They set out by targeting specifically Sox6-positive NG2 glia. This choice is motivated by the authors' observation that the early postnatal forebrain of Sox6 knockout mice displays marked ectopic expression of the proneural transcription factor (TF) Neurog2, suggesting a latent neurogenic program may be derepressed in NG2 cells, which normally express Sox6. Cultured NG2 glia transfected with a construct ("NVOF") encoding Neurog2, the corticofugal neuron-specifying TF Fezf2, and a constitutive repressor form of Olig2 are efficiently reprogrammed to neurons. These acquire complex morphologies resembling those of mature endogenous neurons and are characterized by fewer abnormalities when compared to neurons induced by Neurog2 alone. NVOF-induced neurons, as a population, also express a narrower range of cortical neuron subtype-specific markers, suggesting narrowed subtype specification, a potential step forward for Neurog2-driven neuronal reprogramming. Comparison of NVOF- and Neurog2-induced neurons to endogenous subcerebral projection neurons (SCPN) also indicates Fezf2 may aid Neurog2 in directing the generation of SCPN-like neurons at the expense of other cortical neuronal subtypes.

      Strengths:

      The report describes a novel, highly homogeneous in vitro system amenable to efficient reprogramming. The authors provide evidence that Fezf2 shapes the outcome of Neurog2-driven reprogramming towards a subcerebral projection neuron identity, consistent with its known developmental roles. Also, the use of the modified RNA for transient expression of Neurog2 is very elegant.

      Weaknesses:

      The molecular characterization of NVOF-induced neurons is carried out at the bulk level, therefore not allowing to fully assess heterogeneity among NVOF-induced neurons. The suggestion of a latent neurogenic potential in postnatal cortical glia is only partially supported by the data from the Sox6 knockout. Finally, some of the many exciting implications of the study remain untested.

      Discussion:

      The study has many exciting implications that could be further tested. For example, an ultimate proof of the subcerebral projection neuron identity would be to graft NVOF cells into neonatal mice and study their projections. Another important implication is that Sox6-deficient NG2 glia may not only express Neurog2 but activate a more complete neurogenic programme, a possibility that remains untested here.

      Also, is the subcerebral projection neuron dependent on the starting cell population? Could other NG2 glia, not expressing Sox6, also be co-axed by the NVOF cocktail into subcerebral projection neurons? And if not, do they express other (Sox) transcription factors that render them more amenable to reprogramming into other cortical neuron subtypes? The authors state that SOX6-positive NG2 glia are a quiescent progenitor population. Given that NG2 glia is believed to undergo proliferation as a whole, are Sox6-positive NG2 glia an exception from this rule? Finally, the authors seem to imply that subcerebral projection neurons and Sox6-positive NG2 glia are lineage-related. However, direct evidence for this conjecture seems missing.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful and detailed review of this work. We especially appreciate the positive evaluation of the work and the highlighting of multiple strengths of our approach, including the role of Fezf2 in refining neuronal subtype identity and the use of modified RNA to enable transient expression of Neurog2.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s comment that single-cell transcriptomic analysis would indeed provide a more granular view of likely heterogeneity. This current study focuses on investigating the feasibility of directed differentiation of corticospinal-like neurons from endogenous progenitors. Future work employing single-cell sequencing could indeed help delineate the heterogeneity of neurons generated by directed differentiation, and potentially contribute toward identification of potential molecular roadblocks in different subsets.

      Regarding the suggestion that SOX6-deficient NG2+ progenitors might activate a broader neurogenic program, we agree that this is an intriguing possibility. We are currently conducting indepth investigation of the loss of SOX6 function in NG2+ progenitors, and we aim to submit this quite distinct work for separate publication.

      The reviewer raises an important point about whether SOX6+/NG2+ progenitors and subcerebral projection neurons are indeed normally lineage-related. In the current work, we utilized postnatal cortical SOX6+/NG2+ progenitors that are thought to be largely derived from EMX1+ and GSH2+ ventricular zone neural progenitors. Our unpublished data from the separate study noted above indicate that SOX6 is expressed by both these lineages in vivo. Since subcerebral projection neurons are derived from EMX1+ ventricular zone progenitors (SOX6-expressing), at least some of the SOX6+/NG2+ progenitors are expected to share a lineage relationship with subcerebral projection neurons. While our data strongly suggest such a link, we agree that direct lineagetracing could be pursued in future work. 

      Finally, we agree with the reviewer’s suggestion that in vivo transplantation to assess the identity and connectivity of neurons generated by directed differentiation would be very interesting, and is a natural next phase of this work. We aim to pursue such work in future investigations.

      We again thank the reviewer for their insightful comments.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      The most important clarification for me concerns the initial description of the progenitors. I think there is a mistake with the transgenic line NG2. The dsRed mouse used in Figure 1 C is not described until later in the results describing Figure 2. This was confusing. Moreover, perhaps this is a reason why I get confused and do not understand how the authors conclude that SOX6+ cells are a subset of NG2positive cells. Panel C shows the opposite. Please correct the description and show the quantification of data in panel 1C.

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful review and for highlighting this important point. We appreciate the reviewer pointing out the benefit of further clarity regarding the NG2.DsRed transgenic mouse description in Figure 1C. We have revised the text to clarify the use of the transgenic line and ensure that the DsRed mouse is properly introduced. Additionally, we have further clarified the description explaining the basis for concluding that SOX6+ cells are a subset of NG2+ cells and further integrate this conclusion with the data presented.

      During cell sorting from the cortices of NG2.DsRed mice, we observe two distinct populations of NG2-DsRed+ cells based on fluorescence intensity in FACS: NG2-DsRed “bright” and NG2-DsRed “dim” populations. The NG2-DsRed “dim” population consists of a heterogenous mix of NESTIN+ progenitors, GFAP+ astrocytes/progenitors, a subset of NG2+ cells, and other unidentified cells. In contrast, the DsRed “bright” population includes a broader group of progenitors that also give rise to oligodendrocytes (please see Zhu, Bergles, and Nishiyama 2008), along with pericytes. 

      Previous studies have shown that, while dorsal/pallial VZ progenitors express SOX6 during embryonic development, SOX6 expression becomes restricted to interneurons postnatally (these do not express NG2 proteoglycan; Azim et al., 2009) and to the broader group of NG2+ progenitors that also give rise to oligodendrocytes. The ICC image in Fig. 1C shows bright NG2+ cells in the cortex, many of which express SOX6. Thus, we conclude that SOX6+ cells constitute a subset of NG2-DsRed+ cells. 

      In a similar line, the work is beautiful, but the manuscript can gain a lot from shortening and some more editing. for example:

      (1) In the abstract, the word inappropriate should be removed. It seems to me that is an unnecessary subjective qualification - it is hardly possible that in biology we found repression of something inappropriate.

      We have removed the word “inappropriate”.

      (2) FACS-purify these genetically accessible....establish a pure culture. Genetically accessible is nice, and I understand that it conveys that they can be traced in the mouse, but everything is genetically accessible with the right tool, and perhaps it is more informative to explain which gene or report is used for the isolation. These cells are not accessible in humans. Also, I consider it best to remove pure- the culture is pure (purified by FACS) cells.

      We have revised the text to specify the gene/reporter used for isolation instead of using "genetically accessible", and we removed "pure", since FACS purification is already explicitly mentioned.

      (3) In the initial paragraph in the results: "They are exposed to the same morphogen gradients throughout embryonic development, and thus, compared to distant cell types, have similar epigenomic and transcription landscapes." This is proven in the cited publication, but the way is stated here seems a bit of an unnecessary overstatement. The hypothesis stated after this paragraph is as good as it is with or without this argument.

      We have revised the text and simplified the statement. We agree that the hypothesis remains clear and well-supported without this emphasis.

      (4) In the result sections, "two distinct populations of DsREd-positive cells were identified based on fluorescence intensity"- I know it is correct, but when reading the percentages, I was confused because those percentages divided the population into three fractions. What the authors do not explain is that they discard the intermediate-expressing population.

      We appreciate the reviewer highlighting this inadvertent point of confusion. We erred by discussing only the two populations of central interest to us (DsRed-bright and DsRed-dim), and did not explicitly mention the DsRed-negative population. We have now clarified the text to include all three cell populations and their percentages of the total cells in all three populations (in the original manuscript and still now, ~75-78% were DsRed-negative). We have also further clarified that only DsRed-Bright cells (identified as progenitors) were used for all subsequent experiments.

      These examples illustrate the type of editing that would be appreciated but which is entirely up to the authors.

      We thank the reviewer for their thoughtful suggestions toward improving clarity and precision. We have incorporated these recommendations, along with suggestions from the other two reviewers, in the revised paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      (1)  The authors start their results section by showing in situ Hybridization for Ngn2 in control and Sox6KO mice. These control sections do not look convincing, as there is not even some signal in the adult VZSVZ region and virtually no background. Please show sections where some positive signal can also be detected in the control sections.

      We agree with the reviewer that making direct comparisons in ISH experiments is an important point. In our ISH experiments, to ensure consistency and appropriate comparisons, we process WT and KO sections together and stop the signal development simultaneously. We could have extended the development time to enhance WT signal to a detectable level, but that would have led to excessive background and over-saturated signal in the KO sections.

      To address the reviewer’s point, we have added a new supplementary figure with an additional pair of WT and KO sections, along with reference data from the Allen Brain Atlas. The WT section shows faint Neurog2 expression in the dentate gyrus region of the hippocampus, while the KO section confirms very substantial upregulation of Neurog2 in the absence of SOX6 function. These additional data enhance the clarity and depth of our results.

      Please see the following link for the Allen Brain Atlas ISH data demonstrating that Neurog2 expression in the postnatal (P4) SVZ/SGZ is inherently low. (https://developingmouse.brainmap.org/experiment/show/100093831). 

      (2) As a hallmark of projection neurons is where they send their axons, it would be important to include a biological assay for this. Of course, in vivo experiments would be great, but if this is not possible, the authors could co-culture sections from the late embryonic cortex, striatum, and spinal cord to see if the reprogrammed neurons preferentially extend their axons towards one of these targets (as normally developing neurons would, see e.g. Bolz et al., 1990).

      We agree with the reviewer’s suggestion that a very interesting future stage of this work would be to investigate the projection neuron identity including connectivity in vivo. We aim to pursue follow-up studies to investigate in vivo integration and connectivity of such neurons generated by directed differentiation from endogenous SOX6+/NG2+ cortical progenitors. As the reviewer insightfully suggests, co-culturing different brain regions with these neurons could offer an alternative strategy to partially assess potential preferential connectivity into cultured spinal cord vs. alternate tissue. This area of investigation is of substantial interest to our lab, and we aim to pursue it in the coming years– it is a very large undertaking by either approach.

      (3) However, if the loss of Sox6 is sufficient for Ngn2 to be upregulated, why did the authors not pursue this approach in their reprogramming experiments? Are these endogenous levels sufficient for reprogramming? Please add some OPC cultures from WT and KO mice to explore their conversion to neurons and possibly combine them with Olig2VP16 and Fezf2.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment and for raising this broader area of inquiry regarding whether SOX6 might be down-regulated to enhance induction of neurogenesis. We are writing a separate manuscript regarding function of SOX6 in these progenitors during normal or molecularly manipulated development. We investigate function of SOX6 using both whole body null mice and a series of conditional null mice. We aim to post that work as a preprint and submit it for review and publication in the coming months. Beyond that work, the potential strategy of downregulating SOX6 function while simultaneously upregulating other molecular controls to refine directed neuronal differentiation is also of substantial interest to us, and we aim to pursue this in follow-up work. Though these are both interesting questions/topics, we respectfully submit that these broad areas of parallel, complex, and future investigation would substantially expand the scope of work in this paper, so we aim to address them in separate studies.

      (4) Please indicate independent biological replicates as individual data points in all histograms, i.e. also in Figure 2K, Figure 4I, S2H.

      We have updated the figure legends indicating the biological replicates, and explained the broad media optimization that was used successfully in all further experiments.

      (5) GFP labelling in Figures S2K-N is not convincing - too high background. Please optimize.

      We have redesigned this figure and now present it as a new supplementary figure, with GFP pseudocolored in gray and enlarged subpanels for improved visualization of cell morphology.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      This is an extremely well-written manuscript with very exciting implications. Obviously, not all can be tested here. Some of the suggestions are relatively easy and may be worth testing right away, others may require more extensive study in the future. In my view, completing some of the points below could make this paper a landmark study.

      I start with the key questions:

      (1) Do grafted NVOF cells give rise to subcerebral projection neurons in vivo?

      We agree with the reviewer’s suggestion that a very interesting future stage of this work would be to investigate the projection neuron identity including connectivity in vivo. As noted above in response to Reviewer 2, we aim to pursue follow-up studies to investigate in vivo integration and connectivity of such neurons generated by directed differentiation from endogenous SOX6+/NG2+ cortical progenitors. This question is of substantial interest to us, and we aim to pursue it in the coming years– as the reviewer notes, this is a very large undertaking, and beyond the scope of this paper.

      (2) What is the fate of the Sox6 deficient NG2 glia that express Neurog2? One could isolate these cells and subject them to scRNA sequencing to see how far neurogenesis proceeds without addition of exogenous factors.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful question. As noted in our response to Reviewer 2, we are writing a separate manuscript regarding function of SOX6 in these progenitors during normal or molecularly manipulated development. We investigate function of SOX6 using both whole body null mice and a series of conditional null mice. We aim to post that work as a preprint and submit it for review and publication in the coming months, likely in early summer. We respectfully submit that this broad area of parallel, complex investigation would substantially expand the scope of work in this paper and make this paper too complex and multi-directional, so we aim to publish them as separate papers for the benefit of clarity for readers.

      (3) Obviously, what happens to Sox6-deficient (or non-deficient cells) when forced to express NVOF? In this context, it might be fair to cite Felske et al (PLoS Biol, 2023) who report Neurog2 and Fezf2-induced reprogramming in the postnatal brain. In their model, these authors did not distinguish between converted astrocytes and NG2 glia. Thus, some of the reprogrammed cells may comprise the SOX6positive cells described here.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting for us that we inadvertently omitted referencing the important paper by Felske et al., 2023. We have now included this citation. 

      We thank the reviewer for raising this broader area of inquiry regarding whether SOX6 might be down-regulated to enhance induction of neurogenesis. Beyond the work noted above regarding function of SOX6 in these progenitors during normal or molecularly manipulated development, the potential strategy of downregulating SOX6 function while simultaneously upregulating other molecular controls to refine directed neuronal differentiation is of substantial interest to us, and we aim to pursue this in follow-up work. We again respectfully submit that this area of complex, future investigation should be addressed in future studies.

      Very interesting unaddressed questions include:

      (1) Are Sox6+ NG glia of dorsal origin? This is implied but not shown. One could use Emx1Cre lines to assess this. Are Sox6+ glia and subcerebral projection neurons clonally related? This may be more challenging. In this context, it might be again fair to refer to Herrero-Navarro et al (Science Advances 2021) who show that glia lineage related to nearby neurons gives rise to induced neurons with regional specificity.

      The reviewer raises an important question regarding the competence of SOX6+/NG2+ progenitors from distinct origins to generate corticospinal-like neurons by directed differentiation. In ongoing unpublished work, we have identified SOX6 expression by NG2+ progenitors of the three lineages derived from ventricular zone progenitors that express either Emx1, Gsh2, or Nkx2.1 transcription factors. The EMX1+ lineage-derived SOX6+/NG2+ progenitors are directly lineage related to cortical projection neurons. As the reviewer suggests, future experiments could explore potential differences in competence between these three populations.

      We again thank the reviewer for highlighting for us that we also inadvertently omitted referencing the exciting study by Herrero-Navarro that addresses the question of regional heterogeneity within astrocytes and the differential reprogramming potential related to their origins. We have now cited this paper in the manuscript.

      (2) Do other NG2 glia not give rise to subcerebral projection neurons when challenged with NVOF? Thus, how important is Sox6 expression really?

      The question of the specific competence of dorsal/cortical SOX6+/NG2+ progenitors to differentiate into corticospinal-like neurons, and the strategy of downregulating SOX6 function while simultaneously upregulating other molecular controls to direct neuronal differentiation, are both of great interest to us. In pilot experiments, we observed reduced competence of ventrallyderived SOX6+/NG2+ progenitors to generate similar neurons. We plan to pursue the SOX6 manipulation in follow up work.

      (3) Do Sox6+ NG2 glia proliferate like other NG2 glia and thereby represent a replenishable pool of progenitors?

      Yes; as noted in the text shortly after Figure 1, and as presented in Figure S3l-L, these progenitors proliferate robustly in response to the mitogens PDGF-A and FGF2.

      (4) How heterogenous are the NVOF-induced neurons? The bulk highlights the overall specificity, but does not tell whether all cells make it equally well.

      We agree with the reviewer that this is an interesting question. ICC analysis (Fig. 4G-4H) presents the variation in the levels of a few functionally important proteins in the population of NVOFinduced neurons. This could be due to any or all of at least three potential possibilities: 1) potential diversity in the population of purified SOX6+/NG2+ progenitors; 2) technical variability in the amount of NVOF plasmid delivered to individual progenitors during transfection; and/or 3) natural stochastic TF-level variations generating closely-related neuron types, that also occurs during normal development. Future experiments could explore these questions.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer#1 (Public review):

      This work regards the role of Aurora Kinase A (AurA) in trained immunity. The authors claim that AurA is essential to the induction of trained immunity. The paper starts with a series of experiments showing the effects of suppressing AurA on beta-glucan-trained immunity. This is followed by an account of how AurA inhibition changes the epigenetic and metabolic reprogramming that are characteristic of trained immunity. The authors then zoom in on specific metabolic and epigenetic processes (regulation of S-adenosylmethionine metabolism & histone methylation). Finally, an inhibitor of AurA is used to reduce beta-glucan's anti-tumour effects in a subcutaneous MC-38 model.

      Strengths:<br /> With the exception of my confusion around the methods used for relative gene expression measurements, the experimental methods are generally well-described. I appreciate the authors' broad approach to studying different key aspects of trained immunity (from comprehensive transcriptome/chromatin accessibility measurements to detailed mechanistic experiments). Approaching the hypothesis from many different angles inspires confidence in the results (although not completely - see weaknesses section). Furthermore, the large drug-screening panel is a valuable tool as these drugs are readily available for translational drug-repurposing research.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive and encouraging comments.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The manuscript contains factual inaccuracies such as:

      (a) Intro: the claim that trained cells display a shift from OXPHOS to glycolysis based on the paper by Cheng et al. in 2014; this was later shown to be dependent on the dose of stimulation and actually both glycolysis and OXPHOS are generally upregulated in trained cells (pmid 32320649).

      We appreciate the reviewer for pointing out this inaccuracy, and we have revised our statement to ensure accurate and updated description in manuscript. We are aware that trained immunity involves different metabolic pathways, including both glycolysis and oxidative phosphorylation [1, 2]. We also detected Oxygen Consumption Rate (please see response to comment 8 of reviewer#1) but observed no obvious increase of oxygen consumption in trained BMDMs in our experiment setting. As the reviewer pointed out, it might be dependent on the dose of stimulation.

      (b) Discussion: Trained immunity was first described as such in 2011, not decades ago.

      We are sorry for the inaccurate description, and we have corrected the statement in our revised manuscript as “Although the concept of ‘trained immunity’ has been proposed since 2011, the detailed mechanisms that regulate trained immunity are still not completely understood.”

      (2) The authors approach their hypothesis from different angles, which inspires a degree of confidence in the results. However, the statistical methods and reporting are underwhelming.

      (a) Graphs depict mean +/- SEM, whereas mean +/- SD is almost always more informative. (b) The use of 1-tailed tests is dubious in this scenario. Furthermore, in many experiments/figures the case could be made that the comparisons should be considered paired (the responses of cells from the same animal are inherently not independent due to their shared genetic background and, up until cell isolation, the same host factors like serum composition/microbiome/systemic inflammation etc). (c) It could be explained a little more clearly how multiple testing correction was done and why specific tests were chosen in each instance.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for this thoughtful comment. (a) The data from animal experiments in which trained immunity was induced in vivo are presented as mean ± SD, while the statistical results from cell-based experiments are presented as mean ± SEM in the revised manuscript. (b) We have replaced one-tailed test with two-tailed test (see Figure 3J in revised manuscript, with updated P value label). We agree that cells derived from the same animal and subjected to different treatment conditions may be deemed paired data. We reanalyzed our data using paired statistical tests. While this led to a slight reduction in statistical significance for some comparisons, the overall trends remained consistent, and our biological interpretation remains unchanged. For in vitro experiments unpaired statistical tests are commonly used in literature [3, 4]. Thus, we still used unpaired test results here. (c) We have provided a detailed description of how multiple comparisons were performed in revised figure legends.

      (d) Most experiments are done with n = 3, some experiments are done with n = 5. This is not a lot. While I don't think power analyses should be required for simple in vitro experiments, I would be wary of drawing conclusions based on n = 3. It is also not indicated if the data points were acquired in independent experiments. ATAC-seq/RNA-seq was, judging by the figures, done on only 2 mice per group. No power calculations were done for the in vivo tumor model.

      We are sorry for the confusion in our description in figure legends. For the in vivo experiment, we determined the sample size (n=5, n refers to number of mice used as biological replicates) by referring to the animal numbers used for similar experiments in literatures. And according to a reported resource equation approach for calculating sample size in animal studies [5], n=5-7 is suitable for most of our mouse experiments. The in vitro cell assay was performed at least three independent experiments (BMs isolated from different mice), and each experiment was independently replicated at least three times and points represents biological replicates in our revised manuscript. In Figure 1A, 5 biological replicates of these experiments are presented to carefully determine a working concentration of alisertib that would not significantly affect the viability of trained macrophages, and that was subsequently used in all related cell-based experiments. As for seq data, we acknowledge the reviewer's concern regarding the small sample size (n=2) in our RNA-seq/ATAC-seq experiment. We consider the sequencing experiment mainly as an exploratory/screening approach, and performed rigorous quality control and normalization of the sequencing data to ensure the reliability of our findings. For RNA-seq data analysis, we referred to the DESeq2 manual, which specifies that its statistical framework is based on the Negative Binomial Distribution and is capable of robustly inferring differential gene expression with a minimum of two replicates per group. Therefore, the inclusion of two replicates per group was deemed sufficient for our analysis. Nevertheless, the genomic and transcriptome sequencing data were used primarily for preliminary screening, where the candidates have been extensively validated through additional experiments. For example, we conducted ChIP followed by qPCR for detecting active histone modification enrichment in Il6 and Tnf region to further verify the increased accessibility of trained immunity-induced inflammatory genes.

      (e) Furthermore, the data spread in many experiments (particularly BMDM experiments) is extremely small. I wonder if these are true biological replicates, meaning each point represents BMDMs from a different animal? (disclaimer: I work with human materials where the spread is of course always much larger than in animal experiments, so I might be misjudging this.).

      Thanks for your comments. In our initially submitted manuscript, some of the statistical results were presented as the representative data (technical replicates) from one of three independent biological replicates (including BMDMs experiments showing the suppression and rescue experiments of trained immunity under different inhibitors or activators, see original Figure 1B-C, Figure 5D, and Figure 5H, also related to Figure 1B-C, Figure 5D, and Figure 5H respectively in our revised manuscript) while other experimental data are biological replicates including CCK8 experiment, metabolic assay and ChIP-qPCR. In response to your valuable suggestion, we have revised the manuscript to present all statistical results as biological replicates from three independent experiments (presented as mean ± SEM), and we have provided all the original data for the statistical analysis results (please see Appendix 2 in resubmit system).

      (3) Maybe the authors are reserving this for a separate paper, but it would be fantastic if the authors would report the outcomes of the entire drug screening instead of only a selected few. The field would benefit from this as it would save needless repeat experiments. The list of drugs contains several known inhibitors of training (e.g. mTOR inhibitors) so there must have been more 'hits' than the reported 8 Aurora inhibitors.

      Thank you for your suggestion and we have briefly reported the outcomes of the entire drug screening in the revised manuscript. The targets of our epigenetic drug library are primarily categorized into several major classes, including Aurora kinase family, histone methyltransferase and demethylase (HMTs and KDMs), acetyltransferase and deacetylase (HDACs and SIRTs), JAK-STAT kinase family, AKT/mTOR/HIF, PARP family, and BRD family (see New Figure 1, related to Figure 1-figure supplement 1B in revised manuscript). Notably, previous studies have reported that inhibition of mTOR-HIF1α signaling axis suppressed trained immunity[6]. Our screening results also indicated that most inhibitors targeting mTOR-HIF1α signaling exhibit an inhibitory effect on trained immunity. Additionally, cyproheptadine, a specific inhibitor for SETD7, which was required for trained immunity as previously reported [7], was also identified in our screening.

      JAK-STAT signaling is closely linked to the interferon signaling pathway, and certain JAK kinase inhibitors also target SYK and TYK kinases. A previous drug library screening study has reported that SYK inhibitors suppressed trained immunity [8]. Consistently, our screening results reveal that most JAK kinase inhibitors exhibit suppressive effects on trained immunity.

      BRD (Bromodomain) and Aurora are well-established kinase families in the field of oncology. Compared to BRD, the clinical applications of the Aurora kinase inhibitor are still at early stage. In previous studies using inflammatory arthritis models where trained immunity was established, both adaptive and innate immune cells exhibited upregulated expression of AurA [9, 10]. Our study provides further evidence supporting an essential role of AurA in trained immunity, showing that AurA inhibition leads to the suppression of trained immunity.

      (4) Relating to the drug screen and subsequent experiments: it is unclear to me in supplementary figure 1B which concentrations belong to secondary screens #1/#2 - the methods mention 5 µM for the primary screen and "0.2 and 1 µM" for secondary screens, is it in this order or in order of descending concentration?

      Thank you for your comments and we are sorry for unclear labelled results in original manuscript (related to Figure 1-supplement 1C). We performed secondary drug screen at two concentrations, and drug concentrations corresponding to secondary screen#1 and #2 are 0.2 and 1 μM respectively. It was just in this order, but not in an order of descending concentration.

      (a) It is unclear if the drug screen was performed with technical replicates or not - the supplementary figure 1B suggests no replicates and quite a large spread (in some cases lower concentration works better?)

      Thank you for your question. The drug screen was performed without technical replicates for initial screening purpose, and we need to verify any hit in the following experiment individually. Yes, we observed that lower concentration works better in some cases. We speculate that it might be due to the fact that the drug's effect correlates positively with its concentration only within a specific range. But in our primary screening, we simply choose one concentration for all the drugs. This is a limitation for our screening, and we acknowledge this limitation in our discussion part.

      (5) The methods for (presumably) qPCR for measuring gene expression in Figure 1C are missing. Which reference gene was used and is this a suitably stable gene?

      We are sorry for this omission. The mRNA expression of Il6 and Tnf in trained BMDMs was analyzed by a quantitative real-time PCR via a DDCt method, and the result was normalized to untrained BMDMs with Actb (β-actin) as a reference gene, a well-documented gene with stable expression in macrophages. We have supplemented the description for measuring gene expression in Material and Methods in our revised manuscript.

      (6) From the complete unedited blot image of Figure 1D it appears that the p-Aurora and total Aurora are not from the same gel (discordant number of lanes and positioning). This could be alright if there are no/only slight technical errors, but I find it misleading as it is presented as if the actin (loading control to account for aforementioned technical errors!) counts for the entire figure.

      We are very sorry for this omission. In the original data, p-Aurora and total Aurora were from different gels. In this experiment the membrane stripping/reprobing after p-Aurora antibody did not work well, so we couldn’t get all results from one gel, and we had to run another gel using the same samples to blot with anti-aurora antibody and used β-tubulin as loading control for total AurA (please see New Figure 2A, also related to original Figure 1D). We have provided the source data for β-tubulin from the same membrane of total AurA (please see Figure 1-source data). To avoid any potential misleading, we have repeated this experiment and updated this Figure (please see New Figure 2B, also related to Figure 1D in revised manuscript) with phospho-AurA, total AurA and β-actin from the same gel. The bands for phospho AurA (T288) were obtained using a new antibody (Invitrogen, 44-1210G) and we have revised this information in Material and Methods. We have provided data of three biological replicates to confirm the experiment result also see New Figure 2B, related to Figure 1D in revised manuscript, and the raw data have been added in source data for Figure 1)

      (7) Figure 2: This figure highlights results that are by far not the strongest ones - I think the 'top hits' deserve some more glory. A small explanation on why the highlighted results were selected would have been fitting.

      We appreciate the valuable suggestion. Figure 2 (see also Figure 2 in revised manuscript) presented information on the chromatin landscape affected by AurA inhibition to confirm that AurA inhibition impaired key gene activation involved in pro-inflammatory macrophage activation by β-glucan. In Figure 2B we highlighted a few classical GO terms downregulated including “regulation of growth”, “myeloid leukocyte activation” and “MAPK cascade” (see also Figure 2B in revised manuscript), among which “regulation of growth” is known function of Aurora A, just to show that alisertib indeed inhibited Aurora A function in vivo as expected. “Myeloid leukocyte activation” and “MAPK cascade” were to show the impaired pro-inflammatory gene accessibility. We highlighted KEGG terms downregulated like “JAK-STAT signaling pathway”, “TNF signaling pathway” and “NF-kappa B signaling pathway” in Figure 2F (see also Figure 2F in revised manuscript), as these pathways are highly relevant to trained immunity. Meanwhile, KEGG terms “FOXO signaling pathway” (see also Figure 2G in revised manuscript) was highlighted to confirm the anti-inflammation effect of alisertib in trained BMDMs, which was further illustrated in Figure 5 (see also Figure 5 in revised manuscript, illustrating FOXO3 acts downstream of AurA). Some top hits in Figure 2B like “positive regulation of cell adhesion”, and “pathway of neurodegeneration” and "ubiquitin mediated proteolysis" in Figure 2F and 2G, is not directly related to trained immunity, thus we did not highlight them, but may provide some potential information for future investigation on other functions of Aurora A.

      (8) Figure 3 incl supplement: the carbon tracing experiments show more glucose-carbon going into TCA cycle (suggesting upregulated oxidative metabolism), but no mito stress test was performed on the seahorse.

      We appreciate this question raised by the reviewer. We previously performed seahorse XF analyze to measure oxygen consumption rate (OCR) in β-glucan-trained BMDMs. The results showed no obvious increase in oxidative phosphorylation (OXPHOS) indicated by OCR under β-glucan stimulation (related to Figure 3-figure supplement 1 A) although the carbon tracing experiments showed more glucose-carbon going into TCA cycle. We speculate that the observed discrepancy between increased glucose incorporation into TCA cycle and unchanged OXPHOS may reflect a characteristic metabolic reprogramming induced by trained immunity. The increased incorporation of glucose-derived carbon into the TCA cycle likely serves a biosynthetic purpose—supplying intermediates for anabolic processes—rather than augmenting mitochondrial respiration[6]. Moreover, the unchanged OXPHOS may be attributed to a reduced reliance on fatty acid oxidation- “catabolism”, with glucose-derived acetyl-CoA becoming the predominant substrate. Thus, while overall OXPHOS remains stable, the glucose contribution to the TCA cycle increases. This is in line with reports showing that trained immunity promotes fatty acid synthesis- “anabolism”[11]. Alternatively, the partial decoupling of the TCA cycle from OXPHOS could result from the diversion of intermediates such as fumarate out of the cycle. Oxygen consumption rate (OCR) after a mito stress test upon sequential addition of oligomycin (Oligo, 1 μM), FCCP (1 mM), and Rotenone/antimycin (R/A, 0.5 μM), in BMDMs with different treatment for 24 h. β-glucan, 50 μg/mL; alisertib, 1 μM.

      (9) Inconsistent use of an 'alisertib-alone' control in addition to 'medium', 'b-glucan', 'b-glucan + alisertib'. This control would be of great added value in many cases, in my opinion.

      Thank you for your comment. We appreciate that including “alisertib-alone” group throughout all the experiments may further solidify the results. We set the aim of the current study to investigate the role of Aurora kinase A in trained immunity. Therefore, in most settings, we did not include the group of alisertib only without β-glucan stimulation.

      (10) Figure 4A: looking at the unedited blot images, the blot for H3K36me3 appears in its original orientation, whereas other images appear horizontally mirrored. Please note, I don't think there is any malicious intent but this is quite sloppy and the authors should explain why/how this happened (are they different gels and the loading sequence was reversed?)

      Thank you for pointing out this error. After checking the original data, we found that we indeed misassembled the orientation of several blots in original data submitted. We went through the assembling process and figured out that the orientation of blots in original data was assembled according to the loading sequences, but not saved correctly, so that the orientations in Figure 4A were not consistent with the unedited blot image. We are sorry for this careless mistake, and we have double checked to make sure all the blots are correctly assembled in the revised manuscript. We also provided three replicates of for the Western blot results showing the level of H3K36me3 in trained BMDMs was inhibited by alisertib (as seen in New Figure 7 at recommendation 2 of reviewer#2).

      (11) For many figures, for example prominently figure 5, the text describes 'beta-glucan training' whereas the figures actually depict acute stimulation with beta-glucan. While this is partially a semantic issue (technically, the stimulation is 'the training-phase' of the experiment), this could confuse the reader.

      Thanks for the reviewer’s suggestion and we have reorganized our language to ensure clarity and avoid any inconsistencies that might lead to misunderstanding.

      (12) Figure 6: Cytokines, especially IL-6 and IL-1β, can be excreted by tumour cells and have pro-tumoral functions. This is not likely in the context of the other results in this case, but since there is flow cytometry data from the tumour material it would have been nice to see also intracellular cytokine staining to pinpoint the source of these cytokines.

      Thanks for the reviewer’s suggestion. In Figure 6, we performed assay in mouse tumor model and found that trained immunity upregulated cytokines level like IL-6 in tumor tissue, which was downregulated by alisertib administration. In order to rule out the possibility that the detected cytokines such as IL-6 was from tumor cells, we performed intracellular cytokine staining of single cells isolated from tumor tissues (please see New Figure 4). The result showed that only a small fraction of non-immune cells (CD45<sup>-</sup> population) expressed IL-6 (0.37% ± 0.11%), whereas a significantly higher proportion of IL-6-positive cells was observed among CD45<sup>+</sup> population (deemed as immune cells, 13.66% ± 1.82%), myeloid cells (CD45<sup>+</sup>CD11b<sup>+</sup>, 15.60% ± 2.19%), and in particular, macrophages (CD45<sup>+</sup>CD11b<sup>+</sup>F4/80<sup>+</sup>37.24% ± 3.04%). These findings strongly suggest that immune cells, especially macrophages, are the predominant source of IL-6 cytokine within the tumor microenvironment. Moreover, we also detected higher IL-6 positive population in myeloid cells and macrophages (please see Figure 6I in revised manuscript).

      Reviewer#2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript investigates the inhibition of Aurora A and its impact on β-glucan-induced trained immunity via the FOXO3/GNMT pathway. The study demonstrates that inhibition of Aurora A leads to overconsumption of SAM, which subsequently impairs the epigenetic reprogramming of H3K4me3 and H3K36me3, effectively abolishing the training effect.

      Strengths:

      The authors identify the role of Aurora A through small molecule screening and validation using a variety of molecular and biochemical approaches. Overall, the findings are interesting and shed light on the previously underexplored role of Aurora A in the induction of β-glucan-driven epigenetic change.

      We thank the reviewer for the positive and encouraging comments.

      Weaknesses:

      Given the established role of histone methylations, such as H3K4me3, in trained immunity, it is not surprising that depletion of the methyl donor SAM impairs the training response. Nonetheless, this study provides solid evidence supporting the role of Aurora A in β-glucan-induced trained immunity in murine macrophages. The part of in vivo trained immunity antitumor effect is insufficient to support the final claim as using Alisertib could inhibits Aurora A other cell types other than myeloid cells.

      We appreciate the question raised by the reviewer. Though SAM generally acts as a methyl donor, whether the epigenetic reprogram in trained immunity is directly linked to SAM metabolism was not formally tested previously. In our study, we provided evidence suggesting the necessity of SAM maintenance in supporting trained immunity. As for in vivo tumor model, we agree that alisertib may inhibits Aurora A in many cell types besides myeloid cells. To further address the reviewer’s concern, we have performed the suggested bone marrow transplantation experiment (trained mice as donor and naïve mice as recipient) to verify the contribution of myeloid cell-mediated trained immunity for antitumor effect (please see New Figure 8, also related to Figure 6C, 6D and Figure 6-figure supplement 1B and 1C in revised manuscript).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Some examples of spelling errors and other mistakes (by far not a complete list):

      (a) Introduction, second sentence: reads as if Candida albicans (which should be italicised and capitalised properly) and BCG are microbial polysaccharide components.

      (b) Methods: ECAR is ExtraCellular Acidification Rate, not 'Extracellular Acid Ratio'

      (c) Figure 2C: β-glucan is misspelled in the graph title.

      (d) TNFα has been renamed to 'TNF' for a long time now.

      (e) Inconsistent use of Tnf and Tfnα (the correct gene symbol is Tnf) (NB: this field does not allow me to italicise gene symbols)

      (f) Figure supplement 1B: 'secdonary'

      (g) Caption of figure 4: "Turkey's multiple-comparison test"

      (h) etc

      I would ask the authors that they please go over the entire manuscript very carefully to correct such errors.

      We apologize for these errors and careless mistakes. We greatly appreciate your suggestions, and have carefully proofread the revised manuscript to make sure no further mistakes.

      Please also address the points I raised in the public review about statistical approaches. Even more important than the relatively low 'n' is my question about biological replicates. Please clarify what you mean by 'biological replicate'.If you are able to repeat at least the in vitro experiments (if this is too much work pick the most important ones) a few more times this would really strengthen the results.

      Thank you for your comment. Our biological replicates refer to independently repeated experiments using bone marrow cells isolated from different mice, and n represents the number of mice used. We repeated each experiment at least three times using BMDMs isolated from different mice (n =3, biological replicates). Specifically, we repeated several in vitro experiments showing inhibition of AurA upregulated GNMT in trained BMDMs and showing transcription factor FOXO3 acted as a key protein in AurA-mediated GNMT expression to control trained immunity as well as showing mTOR agonist rescued trained immunity inhibited by alisertib (see New Figure 5, related to Figure 5B-C, Figure 5H in revised manuscript). Additionally, we have provided data with three biological replicates to show the β-glucan induced phosphorylation of AurA (see comment 6 of reviewer#1) and changes of histone modification marker under AurA inhibition and GNMT deficiency (see recommendation 2 of reviewer#2). We also repeated in vivo tumor model to analysis intratumor cytokines (see recommendation 12 of reviewer#1).

      Finally: the authors report 'no funders' during submission, but the manuscript contains funding details. Please modify this in the eLife submission system if possible.

      Thank you for your kind reminder and we have modified funding information in the submission system.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) I have the following methodological and interpretative comments for consideration:

      Aurora A has been previously implicated in M1 macrophage differentiation and NF-κB signaling. What is the effect of Aurora A inhibition on basal LPS stimulation? Considering that β-glucan + Ali also skews macrophage priming towards an M2 phenotype, as shown in Fig. 2E, further clarification on this point would strengthen the study.

      Thanks for your suggestion. Previous study showed AurA was upregulated in LPS-stimulated macrophages and the inhibition of AurA downregulated M1 markers of LPS-stimulated macrophages through NF-κB pathway but did not affect IL-4-induced M2 macrophage polarization [12]. Consistently, we also found that AurA inhibition downregulated inflammatory response upon basal LPS stimulation as shown by decreased IL-6 level (see New Figure 6). In original Figure 2E (also related to Figure 2E in revised manuscript), we showed an increased accessibility of Mrc1 and Chil3 under “β-glucan +Ali” before re-challenge, both of which are typical M2 macrophage markers. Motif analysis showed that AurA inhibition would upregulate genes controlled by PPARγ (STAT6 was not predicted). Different from STAT6, a classical transcriptional factor in controlling M2 polarization (M2a) dependent on IL-4 or IL-13, PPARγ mediates M2 polarization toward M2c and mainly controls cellular metabolism on anti-inflammation independent on IL-4 or IL-13. Thus, we speculate that inhibition of AurA might promote non-classical M2 polarization, and the details warrant future investigation.

      (2) In Figure 4A, it looks like that H3K27me3 is also significantly upregulated by β-glucan and inhibited by Ali. How many biological replicates were performed for these experiments? It would be beneficial to include densitometric analyses to visualize differences across multiple Western blot experiments for better reproducibility and quantitative assessment. In addition, what is the effect of treatment of Ali alone on the epigenetic profiling of macrophages?

      We are sorry for this confusion. Each experiment was performed with at least three independent biological replicates. In original Figure 4-figure supplement 1 (also related to Figure 4-figure supplementary 1 in the revised manuscript), we presented the densitometric analysis results from three independent Western blot experiments, which showed that β-glucan did not affect H3K27me3 levels under our experimental conditions. Three biological replicates data for histone modification were shown as follows (New Figure 7, as related to Figure 4-figure supplement 1 in revised manuscript). We appreciate that assay for “Ali alone” in macrophages may add more value to the findings. We set the aim of the current study to investigate the role of Aurora kinase A in trained immunity, and we know that alisertib itself would not induce or suppress trained immunity. Therefore, in most settings, we did not test the effect of Alisertib alone without β-glucan stimulation.

      (3) The IL-6 and TNF concentrations exhibit considerable variability (Fig. 3K and Fig. 5H), ranging from below 10 pg/mL to 500-1000 pg/mL. Please specify the number of replicates for these experiments and provide more detail on how variability was managed. Including this information would enhance the robustness of the conclusions.

      Thank you for your comment. These experiments were replicated as least three times using BMDMs isolated from different mice. The observed variations in cytokines concentration may be attributed to factors such as differences in cell density, variability among individual mice, and the passage number of the MC38 cells used for supernatant collection. We have prepared new batch of BMDMs and repeated the experiment and provided consistent results in the revised manuscript (please see Figure 5H in revised manuscript). Data for biological replicates have been provided (please see Appendix 2 in resubmit system).

      (4) The impact of Aurora A inhibition on β-glucan-induced anti-tumor responses appears complex. Specifically, GNMT expression is significantly upregulated in F4/80- cells, with stronger effects compared to F4/80+ cells as seen in Fig. 6D. To discern whether this is due to the abolishment of trained immunity in myeloid cells or an effect of Ali on tumor cells which inhibit tumor growth, I suggest performing bone marrow transplantation. Transplant naïve or trained donor BM into naïve recipients, followed by MC38 tumor transplantation, to clarify the mechanistic contribution of trained immunity versus off-target effects.

      Thanks for your valuable suggestion. Following your suggestion, we have performed bone marrow transplantation to clarify that alisertib acts on the BM cells to inhibit anti-tumor effect induced by trained immunity (see New Figure 8, related to Figure 6C-D in revised manuscript). As the results shown below, transplantation of trained BM cells conferred antitumor activity in recipient mice, while transplantation of trained BM cells with alisertib treatment lost such activity, further demonstrating that alisertib inhibited AurA in trained BM cells to impair their antitumor activity.

      References

      (1) Ferreira, A.V., et al., Metabolic Regulation in the Induction of Trained Immunity. Semin Immunopathol, 2024. 46(3-4): p. 7.

      (2) Keating, S.T., et al., Rewiring of glucose metabolism defines trained immunity induced by oxidized low-density lipoprotein. J Mol Med (Berl), 2020. 98(6): p. 819-831.

      (3) Cui, L., et al., N(6)-methyladenosine modification-tuned lipid metabolism controls skin immune homeostasis via regulating neutrophil chemotaxis. Sci Adv, 2024. 10(40): p. eadp5332.

      (4) Yu, W., et al., One-Carbon Metabolism Supports S-Adenosylmethionine and Histone Methylation to Drive Inflammatory Macrophages. Mol Cell, 2019. 75(6): p. 1147-1160 e5.

      (5) Arifin, W.N. and W.M. Zahiruddin, Sample Size Calculation in Animal Studies Using Resource Equation Approach. Malays J Med Sci, 2017. 24(5): p. 101-105.

      (6) Cheng, S.C., et al., mTOR- and HIF-1α-mediated aerobic glycolysis as metabolic basis for trained immunity. Science, 2014. 345(6204): p. 1250684.

      (7) Keating, S.T., et al., The Set7 Lysine Methyltransferase Regulates Plasticity in Oxidative Phosphorylation Necessary for Trained Immunity Induced by β-Glucan. Cell Rep, 2020. 31(3): p. 107548.

      (8) John, S.P., et al., Small-molecule screening identifies Syk kinase inhibition and rutaecarpine as modulators of macrophage training and SARS-CoV-2 infection. Cell Rep, 2022. 41(1): p. 111441.

      (9) Glant, T.T., et al., Differentially expressed epigenome modifiers, including aurora kinases A and B, in immune cells in rheumatoid arthritis in humans and mouse models. Arthritis Rheum, 2013. 65(7): p. 1725-35.

      (10) Jeljeli, M.M. and I.E. Adamopoulos, Innate immune memory in inflammatory arthritis. Nat Rev Rheumatol, 2023. 19(10): p. 627-639

      (11) Ferreira, A.V., et al., Fatty acid desaturation and lipoxygenase pathways support trained immunity. Nat Commun, 2023. 14(1): p. 7385.

      (12) Ding, L., et al., Aurora kinase a regulates m1 macrophage polarization and plays a role in experimental autoimmune encephalomyelitis. Inflammation, 2015. 38(2): p. 800-11.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      As a general phenomenon, adaptation of populations to their respective local conditions is well-documented, though not universally. In particular, local adaptation has been amply demonstrated in Arabidopsis thaliana, the focal species of this research, which is naturally highly selfing. Here, the authors report assays designed to evaluate the spatial scale of fitness variation among source populations and sites, as well as temporal variability in fitness expression. Further, they endeavor to identify traits and genomic regions that contribute to the demonstrated variation in fitness.  

      Strengths: 

      With many (200) inbred accessions drawn from throughout Sweden, the study offers an unusually fine sampling of genetic variation within this much-studied species, and through assays in multiple sites and years, it amply demonstrates the context-dependence of fitness expression. It supports the general phenomenon of local adaptation, with multiple nuances. Other examples exist, but it is of value to have further cases illustrating not only the context-dependence of fitness expression but also the sometimes idiosyncratic nature of fitness variation. I commend the authors on their cautionary language in relation to inferences about the roles of particular genomic regions (e.g.l.140-144; l.227)  

      Weaknesses: 

      To my mind, the manuscript is written primarily for the Arabidopsis community. This community is certainly large, but there are many evolutionary biologists who could appreciate this work but are not invited to do so. The authors could address the broader evolution community by acknowledging more of the relevant work of others (I've noted a few references in my comments to the authors). At least as important, the authors could make clearer the fact that A. thaliana is (almost) strictly selfing and how this feature of its biology both enables such a study and also limits inferences from it. Further, it seems to me that though I could be wrong, readers would appreciate a more direct, less discursive style of writing, and one that makes the broader import of the focal questions clearer. 

      we agree that connecting the paper better to the broader field is desirable, and will try to do this in the revision. As for how selfing matters, there certainly are some things we can discuss, but a general discussion is probably a suitable topic for a review/opinion article!

      As a reader, I would value seeing estimates of the overall fitness of the accessions in the different conditions, i.e., by combining the survival and fecundity results of the common garden experiments.

      Combining estimates would be possible in the common garden experiments, and would bring us somewhat closer to total fitness estimates, although as noted by another reviewer (and also emphasized by us), the time scale of our experiment is not sufficient to evaluate the trade-off between survival and fecundity. Furthermore, we would still be missing the establishment component of fitness, which we found to be extremely important. Therefore little would be gained by combining the estimates, while at the same time losing resolution to disentangle the fitness components. We thus decided to focus on the individual fitness components and leave consideration of their joint effect for the Discussion.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      The goal of this study was to find evidence for local adaptation in survival and fecundity of the model plant Arabidopsis thaliana. The authors grew a large set of Swedish Arabidopsis accessions at four common garden sites in northern and southern Sweden. Accessions were grown from seed in trays, which were laid on the ground at each site in late summer, screened for survival in fall and the following spring, and fecundity was determined from rosette size and seed production in spring. Experiments were complemented by 'selection experiments', in which seeds of the same accessions were sown in plots, and after two years of growth, plants were sampled to determine fitness from genotype frequencies, providing a more comprehensive evaluation of lifetime fitness than can be gleaned from fecundity alone. 

      To clarify, fecundity was determined from total plant area using photos of the mature stems, not the rosettes or direct counting of seeds. That said, it is true that our fecundity estimate was well correlated with rosette area. Furthermore, we validate our fecundity estimates by showing they were highly correlated with seed production estimated by measuring and counting siliques on a separate set of plants grown under common garden conditions in one of our sites (Brachi et al.2022). 

      As the main result, southern accessions had higher mortality in northern sites in one of two years, but also suffered more slug damage in southern sites in one year, indicating a potential link between frost tolerance and herbivore resistance. Fecundity of accession was highest when growing close to the 'home' environment, but while accessions from one sand dune population in southern Sweden had among the lowest fecundities overall, they consistently had the highest fitness in the selection experiment. Accessions from this population had large seed size and rapid root growth, which might be related to establishment success when arriving in a new, partially occupied habitat. However, neither trait could fully explain the very high fitness of this population, suggesting the presence of other, unmeasured traits. 

      Overall, the authors could provide clear evidence of local adaptation in different traits for some of their experiments, but they also highlight high temporal and spatial variability that makes prediction of microevolutionary change so challenging. 

      Strengths: 

      A major strength of this study is the highly comprehensive evaluation of different fitness-related traits of Arabidopsis under natural conditions. The evaluation of survival and fecundity in common garden experiments across four sites and two years provides an estimate of variability and consistency of results. The addition of the 'selection experiment' provides an extended view on plant fitness that is both original and interesting, in particular highlighting potential limitations of 'fitness-proxies' such as seed production that don't take into account seedling establishment and competitive exclusion. 

      Throughout the study, the authors have gone to impressive depths in exploring their data, and particularly the discovery of 'native volunteers' in selection experiment plots and their statistical treatment is very elegant and has resulted in compelling conclusions. Also, while the authors are careful in the interpretation of their GWAS results, they nonetheless highlight a few interesting gene candidates that may be underlying the observed plant adaptations, and which likely will stimulate further research. 

      Overall, the authors provide a rich new resource that is relevant and interesting both in the context of general evolutionary theory as well as more specifically for molecular biology. 

      Weaknesses:

      While the repetition of the common garden experiments over two years is certainly better than no repetition (hence its mention also under 'strengths'), the very high variability found between the two years highlights the need for more extensive temporal replication. In this context, two temporal replicates are the bare minimum, and more repeats in time would be necessary to draw any kind of conclusion about the role of 'high mortality' and 'low mortality' years for the microevolution of Arabidopsis. It also seems that the authors missed an opportunity to explore potentially causal variation among years, as they did not attempt to relate winter mortality to actual climatic variables, even though they discuss winter harshness as a potential predictor.

      We agree that two years is insufficient to understand how variation in selective pressures compound over time to generate micro-evolutionary change. The eight-year data in Oakley et al. (2023), which we discuss in the paper, support this. Our results are nonetheless sufficient to demonstrate the idiosyncratic nature of selection. In the revision, we will further emphasize that far longer time series would be needed for definitive conclusions.

      Our short time series is also why we do not try to correlate with climate data, as this would amount to doing statistics with four data points (mostly two groups of accession N vs S, with mostly homogenous climates within groups, and two years).

      The low temporal variation also makes the accidental slug herbivory appear somewhat random. Potted plants are notoriously susceptible to slug herbivory, and while it is certainly nice that slug damage predominantly affected one group of accessions, it nonetheless raises the question whether this reflects a 'real' selection pressure that plants commonly face in their respective local environments. 

      We agree with this point as well. The evidence for selection on glucosinolates by generalist herbivores such as slugs is fairly strong, but the precise agent is not known, and probably varies over time and space. Our results merely demonstrate one possibility (and we will clarify this in the revision).

      The addition of the 'selection experiment' is certainly original and provides valuable additional insights, but again, it seems a bit questionable which natural process really has affected this outcome. While the genetic and statistical analysis of this experiment seems to be state-of-the-art, the experimental design is rather rudimentary compared to more standard selection experiments. Specifically, the authors added seeds from greenhouse-grown mothers to experimental plots and only sampled plants two years later. This means that, potentially,y the first very big bottleneck was germination under natural conditions, which may have already excluded many of the accessions before they had a chance to grow. While this certainly is one type of selection, it is not exactly the type of selection that a 2-year selection experiment is set up to measure. Either initially establishing the selection experiment from plants instead of seeds, or genotyping the population over several generations, would have substantially strengthened the conclusions that could be drawn from this experiment.

      We agree that more data would have been beneficial, and we do not make strong claims about the nature of selection. Among other phenotypes, we mention dormancy, and note that existing dormancy estimates do not predict fitness in our selection experiments. In addition the same seed batches germinated uniformly in the common-garden experiments with minimal stratification (we will note this in the revision).

      Also, the complete lack of information on population density is a bit problematic. It is not clear if there were other (non-Arabidopsis) plants present in the plots, how many Arabidopsis plants were established, if numbers changed over the year, etc. Given all of these limitations, calling this a 'selection experiment' is in fact somewhat misleading. 

      Seeds were introduced into sites that appeared appropriate for A. thaliana, leaving the background community intact. We provided information on sowing density; the density of plants (A. thaliana and other species) that we obtained during the course of the experiments varied considerably between sites, much like in natural populations, although we lack systematic measurements. We will provide more information (including photos) in the revision.  

      Despite these weaknesses, the authors could achieve their main goals, and despite the somewhat minimal temporal replication, they were lucky to sample two fairly distinct years that provided them with interesting variation, which they could partially explain using the variation among their accessions. Overall, this study will likely make an important contribution to the field of evolutionary biology, and it is another very strong example of how the extensive molecular tools in Arabidopsis can be leveraged to address fundamental questions in evolution and ecology, to an extent that is not (yet) possible in other plant systems. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review)

      Summary: 

      The manuscript presents a large common garden experiment across Sweden using solely local germplasm. Additionally, there is a collection of selection experiments that begin investigating the factors shaping fecundity in these populations. This provides an impressive amount of data and analysis investigating the underlying factors involved. Together, this helps support the data showing that fluctuations and interactions are key components determining Arabidopsis fitness and are more broadly applicable across plant and non-plant species. 

      Strengths: 

      The field trials are well conducted with extensive effort and sampling. Similarly while the genetic analysis is complex it is well conducted and reflects the complexity of dealing with population structure that may be intricately linked to adaptive structure. This has no real solution and the option of presenting results with and without correction is likely the only appropriate option. 

      Weaknesses: 

      A significant finding from this study was that fecundity is shaped more by yearly fluctuations and their interaction with genotype than it is by the main effect of location or genotype. Another significant finding is that the strength of selection can be quite strong, with nearly 5x ranges across accessions. It should be noted that there are a number of other studies using Arabidopsis in the wild with multiple years and locations that found similar observations beyond the Oakley citation. In general, the context of how these findings relate to existing knowledge in Arabidopsis is a bit underdeveloped. 

      We shall remedy this in the revision (see also comments by Reviewer #1).

      The effects of the populations across the locations seem to rely on individual tests and PC analysis. It would seem to be possible to incorporate these tests more directly in the linear modeling analysis, and it isn't quite clear why this wasn't conducted. 

      The fecundity estimates were modelled for all experiments simultaneously and the results are presented in Figure 6 to explore the relative importance of genotype effects and interaction terms including genotypes. For survival and fecundity, the BLUPS are generated from linear mixed models fitted for all experiments simultaneously including a random intercept effect for the genotypes within experiments. A principal component analysis is used to explore the pattern of accession effects (BLUPS) on fecundity (Figure 7); this will be explained in the Methods.  

      I'm a bit puzzled by the discussion on how to find causative loci. This seems to focus solely on GWAS as the solution, with a goal to sequence vast individuals. But the loci that the manuscript discussed were found by a combination of structured mapping populations followed by molecular validation that then informed the GWAS. As such, I'm unsure if the proposed future approach of more sequencing is the best when a more balanced approach integrating diverse methods and population types will be more useful. 

      We are puzzled by this comment in return. Our statement about more sequencing (penultimate sentence of discussion) was referring to achieving a better understanding of the history of migration and selection rather than identifying causative loci. Happy for clarification!

      References

      Brachi, Benjamin, Daniele Filiault, Hannah Whitehurst, Paul Darme, Pierre Le Gars, Marine Le Mentec, Timothy C. Morton, et al. 2022. “Plant Genetic Effects on Microbial Hubs Impact Host Fitness in Repeated Field Trials.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 119 (30): e2201285119.

      Oakley, Christopher G., Douglas W. Schemske, John K. McKay, and Jon Ågren. 2023. “Ecological Genetics of Local Adaptation in Arabidopsis: An 8-Year Field Experiment.” Molecular Ecology, June. https://doi.org/10.1111/mec.17045.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      The paper is well written and the figures well laid out. The methods are easy to follow, and the rational and logic for each experiment easy to follow. The introduction sets the scene well, and the discussion is appropriate. The summary sentences throughout the text help the reader.

      The authors have done a lot of work addressing my previous concerns and those of the other Reviewers.

      We are pleased that the revised manuscript satisfactorily addresses the previous concerns of the reviewer.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary

      Le Roy et al quantify wing morphology and wing kinematics across twenty eight and eight hoverfly species, respectively; the aim is to identify how weight support during hovering is ensured across body sizes. Wing shape and relative wing size vary non-trivially with body mass, but wing kinematics are reported to be size-invariant. On the basis of these results, it is concluded that weight support is achieved solely through size-specific variations in wing morphology, and that these changes enabled hoverflies to decrease in size. Adjusting wing morphology may be preferable compared to the alternative strategy of altering wing kinematics, because kinematics may be subject to stronger evolutionary and ecological constraints, dictated by the highly specialised flight and ecology of the hoverflies.

      Strengths

      The study deploys a vast array of challenging techniques, including flight experiments, morphometrics, phylogenetic analyses, and numerical simulations; it so illustrates both the power and beauty of an integrative approach to animal biomechanics. The question is well motivated, the methods appropriately designed, and the discussion elegantly places the results in broad biomechanical, ecological, and evolutionary context.

      We thank the reviewer for appreciating the strengths of our study.

      Weaknesses

      (1) In assessing evolutionary allometry, it is key to pinpoint the variation expected from changes in size alone. The null hypothesis for wing morphology is well-defined (isometry), but the equivalent predictions for kinematic parameters, although specified, are insufficiently justified, and directly contradict classic scaling theory. A detailed justification of the "kinematic similarity" assumption, or a change in the null hypothesis, would substantially strengthen the paper, and clarify its evolutionary implications.

      We agree with the reviewer that a clearly articulated null hypothesis is crucial for interpreting scaling relationships. In fact, when carefully reviewing our manuscript, we realized that we nowhere did so, and which might have led to a misinterpretation of this. In the revised manuscript, we therefore now explicitly state our newly defined null hypotheses (lines 120–125, 340-352), and how we tested these (lines 359-360).

      In fact, we define two alternative null hypotheses: (1) weight support is maintained across sizes using allometric scaling of wing morphology only, and thus wingbeat kinematics are kept constant (kinematic similarity); (2) weight support is maintained across sizes using allometric scaling of wingbeat kinematics, while wing morphology scales isometrically (morphological similarity).

      According to the first null hypothesis, the second-moment-of-area of the wing should scale linearly with body mass, resulting in negative allometry of S<sub>2</sub> relative to body mass (S<sub>2</sub>∼m<sup>1</sup> <m<sup>4/3</sup>). According to the second null hypothesis, the product of wingbeat frequency and amplitude should scale with mass under negative allometry (ω∼ƒ A<sub>ϕ</sub>∼m<sup>-1/6</sup>). We test these alternative null hypotheses using Phylogenetic Generalized Least Square (PGLS) regressions of the morphology and kinematics metrics against the body mass.

      Furthermore, in our revised manuscript, we now also better explain the use of "kinematic similarity" assumption as a theoretical scenario, that is physically, biomechanically nor physiological sustainable across sizes, but that we merely use to define our null hypotheses (lines 340-351). This is made particularly explicit in a new subsection named “Theoretical considerations” (lines 448–461). Note that our second null hypothesis is thus not that hoverflies fly under "kinematic similarity", but that wingbeat kinematics scales under negative allometry (ω∼ƒ A<sub>ϕ</sub>∼m<sup>-1/6</sup>), which we assume is in line with the classic scaling theory that the reviewer refers to.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for making us aware that we did not explicitly state our null hypotheses, and that introducing these new null hypotheses removed the confusion about the assumptions in our study.

      (2) By relating the aerodynamic output force to wing morphology and kinematics, it is concluded that smaller hoverflies will find it more challenging to support their body mass--a scaling argument that provides the framework for this work. This hypothesis appears to stand in direct contrast to classic scaling theory, where the gravitational force is thought to present a bigger challenge for larger animals, due to their disadvantageous surface-to-volume ratios. The same problem ought to occur in hoverflies, for wing kinematics must ultimately be the result of the energy injected by the flight engine: muscle. Much like in terrestrial animals, equivalent weight support in flying animals thus requires a positive allometry of muscle force output. In other words, if a large hoverfly is able to generate the wing kinematics that suffice to support body weight, an isometrically smaller hoverfly should be, too (but not vice versa). Clarifying the relation between the scaling of muscle mechanical input, wing kinematics, and weight support would help resolve the conflict between these two contrasting hypotheses, and considerably strengthen the biomechanical motivation and evolutionary interpretation.

      We agree with the reviewer that, due to disadvantageous surface-to-volume ratios, larger animals are more challenged to maintain weight-support, and that this is also the case for hovering hoverflies. In the current manuscript, we do not aim to challenge this universal scaling law of muscle force with body mass.

      Instead, we here focus merely on how the flight propulsion system (wing morphology and kinematics) scale with size, and how this allows hovering hoverflies to maintain weight support. We also fully agree with the reviewer that in theory, “if a large hoverfly is able to generate the wing kinematics that suffice to support body weight, an isometrically smaller hoverfly should be, too”. This aligns in fact with our second null hypothesis where wingbeat frequency should scale as ƒ∼m<sup>-1/6</sup>, to maintain weight support under morphological isometry.

      In our study, we show that this null hypothesis is rejected (lines 511-517, and line 525), and thus hoverflies primarily adjust their wing morphology to maintain in-hovering weight-support across sizes, and wingbeat kinematics is in fact highly conserved. Why this specific flight kinematics is so strongly conserved is not known, and thus a key topic in the discussion section of our manuscript.

      We agree with the reviewer that muscle physiology might be an important driver for this conserved kinematics, but also aerodynamic efficiency and maneuverability could be key aspects here. In our revised manuscript, we now discuss these three aspects in more detail (lines 762-775). Also, we here now also mention that we aim to address this outstanding question in future studies, by including muscle physiology in our animal flight studies, and by studying the aerodynamics and maneuver kinematic of hoverflies in more detail. 

      Moreover, in our revised introduction section, we now also mention explicitly that the capability for maintaining in-flight weight-support scales inversely with animal size, due to the negative isometric scaling of muscle force with body mass (line 52-56). Furthermore, we removed all statements that might suggest the opposite. We hope that these adjustments helped resolve the apparent conflict between our null hypotheses and general muscle scaling laws.

      Finally, in the Discussion section (lines 770-775), we now more explicitly acknowledge that wing motion is ultimately driven by the flight motor musculature, and that a full biomechanical interpretation must consider the scaling of muscle mechanical input alongside wing kinematics and morphology. While we decided to keep the focus primarily on aerodynamic constraints in this study, we agree that future work integrating both aerodynamic and physiological scaling will be essential to fully resolve these contrasting perspectives.

      (3) One main conclusion-- that miniaturization is enabled by changes in wing morphology--is insufficiently supported by the evidence. Is it miniaturization or "gigantism" that is enabled by (or drives) the non-trivial changes in wing morphology? To clarify this question, the isolated treatment of constraints on the musculoskeletal system vs the "flapping-wing based propulsion" system needs to be replaced by an integrated analysis: the propulsion of the wings, is, after all, due to muscle action. Revisiting the scaling predictions by assessing what the engine (muscle) can impart onto the system (wings) will clarify whether non-trivial adaptations in wing shape or kinematics are necessary for smaller or larger hovering insects (if at all!).

      In many ways, this work provides a blueprint for work in evolutionary biomechanics; the breadth of both the methods and the discussion reflects outstanding scholarship.

      In response to the first review round, we have removed all references to “miniaturization,” as our data does not allow us to infer evolutionary trajectories of body size (i.e., whether lineages have become smaller or larger over time). We now frame our conclusion more conservatively: that changes in wing morphology enable small hoverflies to maintain weight support despite the aerodynamic disadvantages imposed by isometric scaling.

      We fully agree that an integrated biomechanical framework, explicitly linking muscle mechanical output with wing kinematics and morphology, would significantly strengthen the study. However, we believe that performing an integrated analysis assessing the scaling of muscle input into the wing is beyond the current scope, which focuses specifically on the aerodynamic consequences of morphological and kinematic variation (see reply above).

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This paper addresses an important question about how changes in wing morphology vs. wing kinematics change with body size across an important group of high-performance insects, the hoverflies. The biomechanics and morphology convincingly support the conclusions that there is no significant correlation between wing kinematics and size across the eight specific species analyzed in depth and that instead wing morphology changes allometrically. The morphological analysis is enhanced with phylogenetically appropriate tests across a larger data set incorporating museum specimens.

      The authors have made very extensive revisions that have significantly improved the manuscript and brought the strength of conclusions in line with the excellent data. Most significantly, they have expanded their morphological analysis to include museum specimens and removed the conclusions about evolutionary drivers of miniaturization. As a result, the conclusion about morphological changes scaling with body size rather than kinematic properties is strongly supported and very nicely presented with a strong complementary set of data. I only have minor textual edits for them to consider.

      We thank the reviewer for this positive feedback. We are pleased to hear that the revised manuscript is satisfactory.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      My main remaining qualm remains the null hypothesis for the scaling of kinematic parameters - all weaknesses come back to this point. I appreciate that the authors now specify an expectation, but they offer no justification. This is a problem, because the expectation dictates the interpretation of the results and is thus crucial to some of the key claims (including one in the paper title!): the choice made by the authors indeed implies that hovering is harder for small hoverflies, so that the reported changes in size-specific wing morphology are to be interpreted as an adaptation that enables miniaturization. However, why is this choice appropriate over alternatives that would predict the exact opposite, namely that hovering is harder for larger hoverflies?

      In my original review, I suggested that the authors may address this key question by considering the scaling of muscle mechanical output, and provided a quick sketch of what such an argument would look like, both in classic textbook scaling theory, and in the framework of more recent alternative approaches. The authors have decided against an implementation of this suggestion, providing various version of the following justification in their reply: "our study focuses precisely on this constraint on the wing-based propulsion system, and not on the muscular motor system." I am puzzled by this distinction, which also appears in the paper: muscle is the engine responsible for wing propulsion. How can one be assessed independent of the other? The fact that the two must be linked goes straight to the heart of the difficulty in determining the null hypotheses for the allometry of kinematic and dynamic parameters: they must come from assertions on how muscle mechanical output is expected to vary with size, and so couple muscle mechanical output to the geometry of the wing-based propulsion system. What if not muscle output dictates wing kinematics?

      I fully agree with the authors that null hypotheses on kinematic parameters are debatable. But then the authors should debate their choice, and at least assess the plausibility of its implications (note that the idea of "similarity" in scaling does not translate to equal or invariant, but is tied closely to dimensional analysis - so one cannot just proclaim that kinematic similarity implies no change in kinematic parameters). I briefly return to the same line of argument I laid out in the initial review to provide such an assessment:

      Conservation of energy implies:

      W = 1/2 I ω2

      where I is the mass moment of inertia and W is the muscle work output. Under isometry, I ∝m5/3, the authors posit ω ∝m0, and it follows at once that they predict W ∝m5/3. That is, the "kinematic similarity" hypothesis presented in the paper implies that larger animals can do substantially more work per unit body mass than small animals (unless the author have an argument why wing angular velocity is independent of muscle work capacity, and I cannot think of one). This increase in work output is in contradiction with the textbook prediction, going all the way back to Borelli and Hill: isogeometric and isophysiological animals ought to have a constant mass-specific work output. So why, according to the authors, is this an incorrect expectation, ie how do they justify the assumption ω ∝m0 and its implication W ∝m5/3? How can larger animals do more mass-specific work, or, equivalently, what stops smaller animals from delivering the same mass-specific work? If non-trivial adaptations such as larger relative muscle mass enable larger animals to do more work, how does this fit within the interpretation suggested by the authors that the aerodynamics of hovering require changes in small animals?

      A justification of the kinematic similarity hypothesis, alongside answers to the above questions, is necessary, not only to establish a relation to classic scaling theory, but also because a key claim of the paper hinges on the assumed scaling relationship: that changes in wing morphology enable hovering in small hoverflies. If I were to believe Borelli, Hill and virtually all biomechanics textbooks, the opposite should be the case: combing constant mass-specific work output with eq. 1, one retrieves F∝m2/3, so that weight support presents a bigger challenge for larger animals; the allometry of wing morphology should then be seen as an adaptation that enables hovering in larger hoverflies - the exact opposite of the interpretation offered by the authors.

      Now, as it so happens, I disagree with classic scaling theory on this point, and instead believe that there are good reasons to assume that muscle work output varies non-trivially with size. The authors can find a summary of the argument for this disagreement in the initial review, or in any of the following references:

      Labonte, D. A theory of physiological similarity for muscle-driven motion. PNAS, 2023, 120, e2221217120

      Labonte, D.; Bishop, P.; Dick, T. & Clemente, C. J. Dynamics similarity and the peculiar allometry of maximum running speed. Nat Comms., 2024, 15, 2181

      Labonte, D. & Holt, N. Beyond power limits: the kinetic energy capacity of skeletal muscle. J Exp Bio, 2024, 227, jeb247150

      Polet, D. & Labonte, D. Optimal gearing of musculoskeletal systems. Integr Org Biol, 2024, 64, 987-10062024

      I am asking neither that the authors agree with the above references nor that they cite them. But I do expect that they critically discuss and justify their definition of kinematic similarity, its relation to expectation from classic scaling theory, and the implications for their claim that hovering is harder for small animals. I do note that the notion of "physiological similarity" introduced in the above references predicts a size-invariant angular velocity for small animals, that small animals should be able to do less mass-specific work, and that average muscle force output can grow with positive allometry even for isogeometric systems. These predictions appear to be consistent with the data presented by the authors.

      We agree with the reviewer that our null hypothesis was not clearly articulated in our previous version of the manuscript, and that this might have led to a misinterpretation of the merits and limitations of our study. In the revised manuscript, we therefore now explicitly introduce our null hypotheses in the Introduction (lines 120–125), we define these in the Methods section (lines 340–360), test these in the Results section (lines 511–517), and reflect on the results in the Discussion (lines 602–610). We thank the reviewer for pointing out this unclarity in our manuscript, because revising it clarified the study significantly. See our replies in the “Public Review” section for details.

      Minor points

      L56: This is somewhat incomplete and simplistic; to just give one alternative option, weight support with equivalent muscle effort could also be ensured by a change in gearing (see eg Biewener's work). It is doubtful whether weight support is a strong selective force, as any animal that can move will be able to support its weight. The impact of scaling on dynamics is thus arguably more relevant.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out that our original sentence may be too simplistic. We now briefly mention alternative mechanisms (suggested by the reviewer) to provide more nuance (line 56-58).

      L58: I am not aware of any evidence that smaller animals have reduced the musculature dedicated to locomotion beyond what is expected from isometry; please provide a reference for this claim or remove it.

      We removed that claim.

      The authors use both isometry and geometric similarity. As they also talk about muscle, solely geometric similarity (or isogeometry) may be preferable, to avoid confusion with isometric muscle contractions.

      To avoid confusion, we now use “geometric similarity” wherever the use of isometry might be ambiguous.

      L86: negative allometry only makes sense if there is a justified expectation for isometry - I suggest to change to "The assumed increase in wingbeat frequency in smaller animals" or similar, or to clarify the kinematic similarity hypothesis.

      We edited the sentence as suggested.

      L320: This assertion is somewhat misleading. Musculoskeletal systems are unlikely to be selected for static weight support. Instead, they need to allow movement. Where movement is possible, weight support is trivially possible, and so weight support should rarely, if ever, be a relevant constraint. At most, the negative consequence of isometry on weight support would be that a larger fraction of the muscle mass needs to be active in larger animals to support the weight.

      We fully agree with the reviewer that musculoskeletal systems are unlikely not selected for static loads, as the ability to move dynamically in the real world is crucial for survival. That said, we here look at hovering flight, which is far from static. In fact, hovering flight is among the energetic most costly movement patterns found in nature, due to the required high-frequency wingbeat motions (Dudley 2002). Rapid maneuvers are of course more power demanding, but hovering is a good proxy for this. For example, in fruit flies maximum force production in rapid evasive maneuvers are only two times the force produced during hovering (Muijres et al., 2014).

      We agree with the reviewer that it is important to explicitly mention the differences in functional demands on the motor system in hovering and maneuvering flight, and thus we now do so in both the introduction and discussion sections (lines 116-118 and 762-765, respectively).

      Dudley, Robert. The biomechanics of insect flight: form, function, evolution. Princeton university press, 2002.Muijres, F. T., et al. "Flies evade looming targets by executing rapid visually directed banked turns." Science 344.6180 (2014): 172-177.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      Throughout, check use of "constrains" vs. "constraints"

      Thank you for pointing this out. We have corrected these errors.

      Line 52 do you mean lift instead of thrust?

      We agree with the reviewer that the use of “thrust” might be confusing in the context of hovering flight, and thus we replaced “flapping-wing-based aerodynamic thrust-producing system” with the “flapping-wing-based propulsion system”. This way, we no longer use the word thrust in this context, and only use lift as the upward-directed force required for weight-support.

      Line 60 "face also constrains" wording

      Corrected.

      Line 79 Viscous forces only "dominate" at Re<1 and so this statement only refers to very very small insects which I suspect are far below the scale of the hoverflies considered (likely Re ~100) although maybe not for the smallest 3 mg ones?

      Indeed, viscous forces do not “dominate” force production at the Reynolds numbers of our flying insects. We thank the reviewer for pointing out this incorrect statement, which we corrected in the revised manuscript.

      Line 85 again thrust doesn't seem to be right

      Agreed. See reply 3.2.

      533 "maximized" should probably be "increased"

      We now use “increased”.

      Line 705-710 The new study by Darveau might help resolve this a bit because of the reliability of this relationship across and between orders. Darveau, C.-A. (2024). Insect Flight Energetics And the Evolution of Size, Form, And Function. Integrative And Comparative Biology icae028.

      We thank the reviewer for this highly relevant reference, which was unfortunately not included in the original manuscript. In connection with this work, we now further discuss the relationship between wing size allometry and deviations from the expected scaling of wingbeat frequency (lines 730-735).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This Tanzanian study focused on the relationship between human genetic ancestry, Mycobacterium tuberculosis complex (MTBC) diversity, and tuberculosis (TB) disease severity. The authors analyzed the genetic ancestry of 1,444 TB patients and genotyped the corresponding MTBC strains isolated from the same individuals. They found that the study participants predominantly possess Bantu-speaking genetic ancestry, with minimal European and Asian ancestry. The MTBC strains identified were diverse and largely resulted from introductions from South or Central Asia. Unfortunately, no associations were identified between human genetic ancestry, the MTBC strains, or TB severity. The authors suggest that social and environmental factors are more likely to contribute to TB severity in this setting.

      Strengths:

      In comparison to other studies investigating the role of human genetics in TB phenotypes, this study is relatively large, with more than 1,400 participants.

      The matched human-MTBC strain collection is valuable and offers the opportunity to address questions about human-bacterium co-evolution.

      Weaknesses:

      Although the authors had genome-wide genotyping and whole genome sequencing data, they only compared the associations between human ancestry and MTBC strains. Given the large sample size, they had the opportunity to conduct a genome-wide association study similar to that of Muller et al. (https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ygeno.2021.04.024).

      Thank you very much for taking the time to carefully review our manuscript and for your suggestions and comments. In another published study using the same cohort (https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.05.11.23289848), we performed a genome-wide association analysis between the genome-wide SNPS of the host and the genome-wide SNPs from the paired MTBC strains. In the current work we were interested in testing specifically if host ancestry and pathogen genotype family, as well as their interaction, were associated with differences in disease severity, a clinical phenotype with direct consequences for both host and pathogen fitness. The study of Müller et al, referred to by the reviewer, investigates whether MTBC families of strains causing disease in two patient cohorts (South Africa and Ghana) were associated with particular human SNPS assessed genome-wide. In that study, clinical phenotypes were not assessed and human ancestries, in a much broader sense than the ones used in our current study, were used as covariates. To leverage the genome-wide information and the clinical variables collected in our study, we have now added a genome-wide association analysis of all the human SNPs with disease severity measures while adjusting for co-variates (age, sex,  smoking, cough duration, socioeconomic status, history of previous TB, malnutrition, education level, and drug resistance status) and for human population stratification . Yet, no significant statistical associations were detected (L243-249).

      The authors tested whether human genetic ancestry is associated with TB severity. However, the basis for this hypothesis is unclear. The studies cited as examples all focused on progression to active TB (from a latent infection state), which should not be conflated with disease severity. It is difficult to ascertain whether the role of genetic ancestry in disease severity would be detectable through this study design, as some participants might simply have been sicker for longer before being diagnosed (despite the inquiry about cough duration). This delay in diagnosis would not be influenced solely by human genetics, which is the conclusion of the study.

      Evidence that mortality and natural recovery from TB vary by disease presentation spectrum come from studies carried out before the introduction of anti-TB chemotherapy. Patients with mild disease presentation, as measured by radiology at the time of diagnosis had higher odds of recovering naturally compared to those with advanced disease (doi: 10.5588/ijtld.23.0254, doi: 10.1164/arrd.1960.81.6.839). Given the deleterious effects of an MTBC infection leading to symptomatic disease on human fitness, we hypothesized that natural selection has acted on human traits underlying TB disease severity. If those traits are heritable one would expect to find underlying genetic variation in human populations. In addition, because certain MTBC genotype families and human populations have co-existed since a least a few centuries to a few millennia, we hypothesized that some of that genetic variation could be related to human ancestry. We have added more details to the introduction to make our rational clearer (L118-127).  In our patient cohort, we observed a large variation in disease severity using as approximations; TB-Score, X-Ray score and bacterial burden in sputa (Ct-value as determined with GeneXpert). However, the reviewer is absolutely correct in that patients in our study are being diagnosed at different stages of disease confounding our analysis. This is a limitation of our study which cannot be fully accounted for by including cough duration, as we also acknowledged in the manuscript (L343-346).

      Additionally, the study only included participants who attended the TB clinic.

      Yes, this is related to the previous point, our study only considers patients that felt ill enough to visit the TB clinic potentially not including patients that had less severe disease as acknowledged.

      Including healthy controls from the general population would have provided an interesting comparison to see if ancestry proportions differ.

      We agree that it would be interesting to compare the ancestries of healthy controls to the ancestries of TB patients from the same population. However, that would be especially informative with respect to TB susceptibility and would not necessarily be informing disease severity traits and its underlying genetics. The similarities between the ancestry proportions of our cohort with those of neighboring countries such as Kenya, Malawi and Mozambique publicly available genomic data, suggests that there would be no major differences between TB patients and healthy controls.

      Although the authors suggest that social and environmental factors contribute to TB severity, only age, smoking, and HIV status were characterised in the study.

      Based on the comments of both reviewers, we added the following additional variables as covariates in the regression models: the socioeconomic status representing the ratio between the household income and the number of individuals in the household, malnutrition, the education level and whether it was a relapse/reinfection or a new case.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript reports the results of an observational study conducted in Dar es Salaam, Tanzania, investigating potential associations between genetic variation in M. tuberculosis and human host vs. disease severity. The headline finding is that no such associations were found, either for host / bacillary genetics as main effects or for interactions between them.

      Strengths:

      Strengths of the study include its large size and rigorous approaches to classification of genetic diversity for host and bacillus.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) There are some limitations of the disease severity read-outs employed: X-ray scores and Xpert cycle thresholds from sputum analysis can only take account of pulmonary disease. CXR is an insensitive approach to assessing 'lung damage', especially when converted to a binary measure. What was the basis for selection of Ralph score of 71 to dichotomise patients? If outcome measures were analysed as continuous variables, would this have been more sensitive in capturing associations of interest?

      Thank you very much for taking the time to carefully review our manuscript and for your suggestions and comments.  

      We recruited active TB patients with pulmonary TB disease that were sputum smear-positive and GeneXpert-positive. In this study we aimed at obtaining paired samples from both the patient and the strain, and in the current analysis we aimed at testing if human ancestry and its interaction with the strain genotype could explain differences in disease severity. It is often difficult to obtain microbiological cultures from extra-pulmonary cases and including those cases would have not been possible at the scale of this cohort. We believe as well that extra-pulmonary TB is of less relevance for the question we are addressing because in exclusively extrapulmonary cases, disease severity is not linked with bacterial transmission. However, extra-pulmonary TB can be extremely severe, and it would be very interesting to explore the potential role of human genetic variation underlying extra-pulmonary TB in future studies.

      As to the insensitivity of CXR to measure lung damage, we would argue that it depends on what is being assed. As a rationale for the Ralph score, its inventors argue that as in other grading methods, the proportion of affected lung and or cavitation is important to assess severity. It has been described as a “validated method for grading CXR severity in adults with smear-positive pulmonary TB that correlates with baseline clinical and microbiological severity and response to treatment, and is suitable for use in clinical trials” (https://thorax.bmj.com/content/thoraxjnl/65/10/863.full.pdf). While the validation of the score is convincing in that study, and the score has been used in several TB studies and trials, the low proportion of HIV co-infections might have been a limitation. Indeed, as shown in our previous publication, in our cohort of patients, chest X-ray scores were significantly lower in HIV infected TB patients https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.ppat.1010893. In the current analysis, regression analyses performed for the CXR severity and for the other severity measures did not include HIV co-infected patients.

      We obtained the same pattern of results using a continuous outcome. However, an assumption of linear regression was violated. The residuals were not normally distributed stemming from the bimodal distribution of the scores in our dataset. The threshold of 71 for the Ralph score has been used by others in previous studies; in its original description it has been suggested as the optimal cut-off point for predicting a positive sputum smear status after two months, which in turn has been shown to predict unfavorable outcomes (https://doi.org/10.1136/thx.2010.136242). Another study showed that a Ralph score higher than 71 was significantly associated with a longer duration of symptoms, higher clinical scores and a lower BMI (doi: 10.5603/ARM.2018.0032).

      (2) There is quite a lot of missing data, especially for TB scores - could this have introduced bias? This issue should be mentioned in the discussion.

      While we have a TB-score available for each patient, the chest X-ray score is missing for many patients. However, this is random and due both to the absence of an X-ray picture or to the bad quality of X-ray pictures that the radiologists could not assess. When stating that there is a lot of missing data for the TB scores, we assume that the reviewer was referring to the “missing N” columns in Table 1. There, the number of observations missing in each of the disease severity measures actually relates to the explanatory variables (i.e MTBC genotype and human ancestries). This table includes all patients that either had a bacterial genome available or a human genome/genotype (N = 1904). As an example for the TB-score as outcome variable, for 1471 patients the MTBC genotype was determined while it was missing for 433 patients. On the other hand for X-ray scores, 177 had a severe X-ray score, 849 a mild one and for 878 patients, there was no X-ray score available.  As for the Ct-value, despite the fact that the patients were recruited based on positive GeneXpert by the clinical team, these results were not always available to us.

      (3) The analysis adjusted for age, sex, HIV status, age, smoking and cough duration - but not for socio-economic status. This will likely be a major determinant of disease severity. Was adjustment made for previous TB (i.e. new vs repeat episode) and drug-sensitivity of the isolate? Cough duration will effectively be a correlate/consequence of more severe disease - thus likely highly collinear with disease severity read-outs - not a true confounder. How does removal of this variable from the model affect results? Data on socioeconomic status should be added to models, or if not possible then lack of such data should be noted as a limitation.

      Out of the 1904 patients that have either human or bacterial genomic data available, 48 were relapses (2.5%). The mean of the disease severity measures suggest that relapses have a higher CXR score but the TB-score and Ct-values did not differ. Based on the comments of both reviewers, we added the following additional variables as covariates to the regression models: the socioeconomic status representing the ratio between the household income and the number of individuals in the household, malnutrition examined by a doctor, the education level, and whether it was a relapse/reinfection or a new case and if the causative strain had any resistance to any anti-TB drugs. The results did not change. Cough duration could also be a consequence of more severe disease, as pointed out by the reviewer. We present now the results excluding cough duration as a variable from the model, however this also did not affect the results.

      (4) Recruitment at hospitals may have led to selection bias due to exclusion of less severe, community cases. The authors already acknowledge this limitation in the Discussion however.

      (5) Introduction: References refer to disease susceptibility, but the authors should also consider the influences of host/pathogen genetics on host response - both in vitro (PMIDs 11237411, 15322056) and in vivo (PMID 23853590). The last of these studies encompassed a broader range of ethnic variation than the current study, and showed associations between host ancestry and immune response - null results from the current study may reflect the relative genetic homogeneity of the population studied.

      We thank the reviewer for these suggestions which we have added to the introduction. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor Comments:

      (1) The authors should be careful when using the term "Bantu" as opposed to "Bantu-speaking". (i.e. referring to the language group). The term is considered offensive in some settings.

      We thanks the reviewer for this important concern, we have revised throughout the manuscript.

      (2) There are several "(Error! Reference source not found)" phrases in the place of references throughout the document.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out, this has been corrected in the revised version.

      (3) Please correct line 365: "... sequencing (WGS) the patient...." to "... sequencing (WGS) of the patient...."

      (4) The figures in the supplementary PDF are not numbered and some are cut-off (I think it is Supplementary Figure S2).

      This has been corrected in the revised version.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Typographical errors

      (1) There are multiple instances where references have not pulled through to the text, e.g. line 126 (Error! Reference source not found.)

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out, this has been corrected in the revised version.

      (2) Line 239: have been show - have been shown?

      Thank you, this mistake has been corrected in the revised version.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Tesmer and colleagues uses fiber photometry recordings, sophisticated analysis of movement, and deep learning algorithms to provide compelling evidence that activity in hypothalamic hypocretin/orexin neurons (HONs) correlates with net body movement over multiple behaviors. By examining projection targets, the authors show that hypocretin/orexin release differs in projection targets to the locus coeruleus and substantia nigra, pars compacta. Ablation of HONs does not cause differences in the power spectra of movements. The movement-tracking ability of HONs is independent of HON activity that correlates with blood glucose levels. Finally, the authors show that body movement is not encoded to the same extent in other neural populations.

      Strengths:

      The major strengths of the study are the combination of fiber photometry recordings, analysis of movement in head-fixed mice, and sophisticated classification of movement using deep learning algorithms. The experiments seem to be well performed, and the data are well presented, visually. The data support the main conclusions of the manuscript.

      We thank the reviewer for their supportive feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      The weaknesses are minor, mostly consisting of writing and data visualization throughout the manuscript. To some degree, it is already known that hypocretin/orexin neurons correlate with movement and arousal, although this manuscript studies this correlation with unprecedented sophistication and scale. It is also unfortunate that most of the experiments throughout the study were only performed in male mice. Taken together, this study is likely to be impactful to the field and our understanding of HONs across behavioral states.

      We agree that disentangling movement from arousal is an important aspect, and in the revised manuscript, we now include new data and analyses towards this (pupillometry to directly assess arousal, and multivariate analysis to assess contributions of arousal vs movemement to HON activity). In addition, we now implement many of the reviewer’s recommendations regarding writing, data presentation, and visual clarity (see our replies in the “recommendations for authors” section).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Some recommendations for the authors:

      (1) The first sentence of the Introduction states: "Neural activity related to body movement recently received much attention." I would rephrase or clarify this statement, as neuroscientists have been studying neural activity related to body movement for decades.

      The reviewer is correct. Our intention was to highlight the resurgence of movementrelated neurosciences enabled by modern techniques such as deep learning applied to video data (e.g. DeepLabCut, etc). The passage has been updated for clarity.

      (2) The Introduction also states that HONs orchestrate "consciousness and arousal." I would delete the word "consciousness," as consciousness represents a lofty, global concept that is challenging to define and quantify in humans, let alone mice.

      We used the word consciousness to be consistent with current literature on the function of the mouse hypothalamus (e.g. Nat Neurosci 2016 Feb;19(2):290-8). But we agree it is not necessary here, and so we followed the advice to delete it.

      (3) The authors state that HON dynamics were recorded while mice were head-fixed while on a running wheel. For clarity, it would be helpful to visualize this head-fixation in Figures 1A and 5B. It would also be helpful to clarify how certain behaviors (e.g. grooming, chewing) were performed and recorded while the mouse was head-fixed.

      In the revised manuscript, updated graphics with a head-fixed mouse have now been added to relevant figures. Representative RGB frames (colors representing sequential frames) of each behaviour have been added to Figure 2A.

      (4) In the legend for Figure 1A, the reference to Gonzalez et al. 2016 seems out of place (at least the reader should be informed why the text is referring to this previous study). Additionally, because the references are ordered by number instead of alphabetically, it would be more helpful to refer to a numbered reference rather than a name.

      Gonzalez et al. 2016 references the source of the AAV construct used in this figure. This has been moved to the methods. Following eLife formatting guidelines, references will be alphabetized upon publication.

      (5) In Figure 3F, it would be helpful to show visual validation that the HON-DTR method indeed ablates all HONs. This is depicted conceptually, but representative figures would be much more convincing.

      A representative histological slice is now included for both wild type (WT) and HON-DTR mice in the new Figure 4B.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Despite several methodological strengths, the major and highly significant drawback is the confound of arousal with movement. This confound is not resolved, so the results could be explained by previously established relationships between orexin and arousal/wakefulness.

      This an excellent point, and we agree. To address this directly in the revised manuscript, we now include new data and analyses towards this (pupillometry to directly assess arousal, and multivariate analysis to assess contributions of arousal vs movemement to HON activity).

      Strengths:

      The authors show that orexin neuron activity is associated with body movement and that this information is conveyed irrespective of the fasted state. They also report differences in different orexin target brain regions for orexin release during movement. This paper contains an impressive array of cutting-edge techniques to examine a very important brain system, the orexin-hypocretin system. The authors offer an original perspective on the function of this system. The authors showed that orexin neuron activity scales to some degree with the magnitude of body movement change; this is unaffected by a fasted state and seems to be somewhat unique to orexin neurons.

      The investigation of other genetically defined subcortical neuron populations to determine the specificity of findings is also a strength, as is the ability to quantify movement and use deep learning to classify specific behaviors adds sophistication to analysis. The authors also show heterogeneity in orexin projections to specific target nuclei, which is interesting.

      The authors "speculate that narcolepsy-cataplexy, caused by HON loss-of-function, is perhaps explained by oscillations into unwanted sleep-states and motor programs due to impaired control loops for wakefulness and movement". This is quite an interesting aspect of their work and deserving of further study.

      We thank the reviewer for their supportive feedback.

      Weaknesses:

      Despite the strengths, there are several major and minor weaknesses that detract significantly from the study.

      My main concern with this work is the confound of arousal with movement so that correlations with one might reflect a relationship instead with the other. The orexin system is well known to play an important role in arousal, with elevated activity of orexin neurons reported for waking and high arousal. Orexin signaling has also been strongly associated with motivation, which also is associated with arousal and movement. The authors offer no compelling evidence that the relationships they describe between different movements and orexin signaling do not simply reflect the known relationship between arousal and motivation.

      The authors could address this concern by including classical arousal measurements, eg, cortical EEG recorded simultaneously with movements. Often, EEG arousal occurs independently of movement, so this could provide one approach to disentangling this confound. The idea that orexin signaling plays a role in arousal rather than movement is supported by their finding that orexin lesions using the orexin-DTR mouse model did not impact movements. In contrast, prior lesion and pharmacologic studies have found that decreased orexin signaling significantly decreases arousal and waking.

      Another way they could test their idea would be to paralyze and respirate animals so that orexin activity could be recorded without movement. Alternatively, animals could be trained to remain motionless to receive a reward. Thus, there are several ways to test the overall hypothesis of this work that have not been examined here.

      The authors propose that "a simple interpretation of their results is that, via HON movement tracking, the brain creates a "wake up" signal in proportion to movement". This seems to argue for the role of the orexin system in arousal and motivation rather than in movement per se.

      Thank you. We agree that disentangling between arousal and movement is indeed critical. A classic approach is a multivariate analysis, wherein multiple simultaneously recorded “predictors” of HON activity – such as arousal and movement - can be directly compared. While EEG arousal is an option, another well-accepted metric for arousal is pupil diameter. Using n = 7 mice, we now simultaneously record HON activity, movement, running speed, pupil size fluctuations, and ocular movements:

      We then fit a partial least squares multivariate regression (a regression type more robust to collinearity) using the movement metric, pupil size, and ocular movements as predictors of orexin neuron activity. Consistent with previous publications, we found that pupil size alone has a positive correlation with hORX.GCaMP6s (~0.45). However, using a drop-one feature analysis in multivariate regression, we found that movement had the highest % contribution to statistically explaining orexin neuron activity. Here are the new results (which we now added as Fig. 7A-B).

      Author response image 1.

      Furthermore, we also expanded this analysis to incorporate the different frequencies found in HON dynamics, using empirical mode decomposition. We found that pupil size had a maximum correlation at lower HON frequencies than the movement metric, while ocular movements were maximally correlated in higher frequencies (now added as Fig. 7D,E).

      Overall, this analysis suggests that – while HONs encode both movement and arousal – arousal and movement do not always co-fluctuate at the same timescales, and their impacts on HONs can be disentangled in a number of ways. We now mention this in revised text on page 5.

      There are several studies that have examined the effect of orexin antagonist treatment in rodents on locomotor and other motor activities. These studies have largely found no consistent effect of antagonizing orexin signaling, especially at the OxR1 receptor, on simple motor activity. These studies are not referenced here but should be taken into account in the authors' conclusions.

      We agree. Prior studies found that orexin antagonism – or optogenetic silencing of HONs – evokes either reduced locomotion, or no effect on locomotor movements. We now added text and references to paragraph 4 of Discussion, summarising this.

      Figure 3, panel F: I understand HON-DTR is a validated model but a picture of HONs ablation is necessary, including pictures of HONs outputs ablation within the SNc and LC.

      A representative histological slice is now included for both wild type (WT) and HON-DTR mice in the new Figure 4B. Because HONs are only found in the hypothalamus, somatic deletion of HONs in this region will result in axonal degradation in output regions.

      The discussion lacks a more extensive paragraph on the distinct signal and role of Ox>SNc and Ox-LC projections.

      We now added sentences discussing potential implications of this to Discussion (middle of paragraph 4).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor weaknesses

      A very important movement in rodents is head orientation, especially given the limitation in ocular movement. However, this paper used a fixed head model which obviated this movement and did not attempt to analyze ocular movements.

      Analysing ocular movements is something we had not considered but is very easy to check using pupillometry. In n = 7 mice, we recorded both orexin neurons, and ocular movements captured through an infrared camera under constant lighting. Ocular movements had a small positive correlation with orexin neuron photometry (r = ~0.26). See response to the public review above.

      Author response image 2.

      The "HON" abbreviation is not commonly used for orexin neurons, and I suggest replacing that with a more well-known abbreviation.

      To the best of our knowledge, there is no universally agreed or best-known abbreviation for hypocretin/orexin neurons (we agree it would be nice if there was one!). “HONs” is a simple first letter abbreviation of hypocretin/orexin neurons, which acknowledges the two names for this peptide given by the original discoverers (de Lecea et al, and Sakurai et al, in 1998). Although this may not be the perfect abbreviation, we have kept it for now, also to be consistent with the large number (>10) of other published studies that recently used this abbreviation.

      The graphs showing Pearson's r values do not demonstrate a very strong correlation between neural activity and movement change; they also lack validation of genetic expression/ablation in some cases. The results would more strongly support the conclusions if statistically significant correlations could be demonstrated between activity and movement.

      We agree that a correlation of ~0.68 is probably not worthy of a “very strong” classification. While there is no universal ruleset for categorizing the strength of a correlation, we have toned down our language throughout the manuscript.

      Comment regarding statistical testing of correlations: we are cautious to stand behind correlation significance testing for large sample sizes (~48’000 photometry & video samples in a 40-minute session). In our case, correlations were always extremely significant p<0.0001. The reason for this is that correlation p-values become “too big to fail” (see Lin et al. 2013) with inflated sample size. We therefore refrain from commenting on p-values and rather report between or within-subjects statistical tests, or tests against zero. See four example experiments below.

      Author response image 3.

      Citation: Lin, M., Lucas, H. C., Jr & Shmueli, G. Research Commentary—Too Big to Fail: Large Samples and the p-Value Problem. Information Systems Research 24, 906–917 (2013).

      The rationale for looking at running speed, general movement, and specific types of nonlocomotor movements could be clarified and explained more thoroughly in the introduction. Why is it important to distinguish between locomotion (represented here with running) and all other movements? Presumably, this is because orexin is known to regulate arousal/locomotion. What evidence is there for orexin's role in other types of movements, which are being grouped together in Figure 1? This could be laid out in more detail in the Introduction. Relatedly, it is not very clear in the text whether the correlation between movement and orexin neuron activity includes movement related to running.

      The main focus of our paper is on movement in general (i.e. video pixel difference, described in Results and Methods). This movement metric includes everything captured by the video, it is agnostic to the type of movement or behaviour.  To connect this to some of the specific innate movements/behaviours typically studied in mouse literature (running, grooming, sniffing, etc), we also performed plots in Figure 2. We attempted to explain this better in revised section 1 of Results.

      What exactly is being correlated in Figure 1C (and throughout the rest of the paper?) Is this the average signal correlated with the average movement change over the entire recording time? This could be more explicitly stated in methods/results. The correlations themselves/p-values could be shown in addition to/instead of Pearson's r values. Are the correlations themselves significant? This would strengthen the claim that orexin activity is strongly coupled to the magnitude of body movement change. As another example, in Figure 2D, there are no statistics reported on the correlation between movement metric and average neural signal. In Figure 6G, orexin neuron activity is more strongly correlated with movement than MVe glut neurons, but are either of these correlations significant? The correlation between MVe glut activity and movement overall seems similar to that of orexin neurons, and may be worth noting more explicitly.

      Throughout the paper, we have recorded both neural activity (photometry) and movement at 20 Hz. This would generate, for example, 48’000 samples of photometry and movement from a 40-minute session. All the samples were used to calculate a pearson’s r between variables. To clarify this, we now added the subtext “wholesession” to relevant figures, as well as a clarification in the methods.

      Individual experiment correlations for orexin neurons and MVe glut neurons were always significant p<0.0001, even after a Bonferroni multiple comparisons correction was applied to each population. See the “too big to fail” nature of correlation hypothesis testing above.

      It could be made clearer at the end of Figure 2 that orexin neuron activity is tracking the magnitude of movement change (shown in Figure 2D), not that it is encoding different types of movement.

      We intended for original Figure 2E to illustrate this concept, however this panel has caused a great deal of confusion to several readers and was perhaps ill conceived. We have replaced Figure 2E with a new panel more directly addressing the reviewer’s statement. We can construct three models where orexin neuron activity is predicted from the behavioral classification (sometimes called “one-hot” encoding) and/or the movement metric.

      Model 1 predicts orexin neuron activity using only a categorical predictor of behavioral state. Model 2 only uses the movement metric, and model 3 allows a different movement-metric correlation within each behavioral state. We can compare these models using AIC (Akaike Information Criterion) which is a point estimate. While the most complex model 3 was the best, model 2 was much closer to model 3 than model 1. Similarly, model 2 was much better than model 1. From this we conclude that the magnitude of movement change is a more powerful predictor than behavioral state (“type of movement”). This is now Figure 2E.

      It would be interesting to see the raw movement metric data as shown in Figures 1 and 2 in the DTR mice to show that ablating orexin neurons does not impair the movement profile seen in Figures 1 and 2.

      The requested visualization has been added to Figure 4B.

      Validation that orexin was selectively ablated in these mice would be ideal.

      Histology (see response to public review) was added to a new Figure 4B.

      Figure 4A - OxLight expression in SNc does not look very robust.

      Please note this is a membrane-targeted indicator, the staining this produces is thus much weaker than cyctosolic indicators such as calcium indicator GCaMP.

      Figure 4 - It would be beneficial to see the same correlations that were done in Figures 1 and 2 to show OxLight activity vs. movement metric. Are they correlated?

      Individual traces had significant correlations with OxLight and movement, and the population averages revealed similar trends:

      Author response image 4.

      Figure 6B - Targeting of MVe neurons does not look very specific. The sample size for orexintargeted mice should be re-stated in the figure legend for clarity.

      Legend has been updated to clarify n = 15 for orexin targeted mice.

      Some citations didn't seem to match what was being referenced in the text. Similarly, in the legend for Figure 1C, the statistics do not match what is reported in the text. In Figure 1, the sample size is not noted in the text. When referring to running in Figure 1, is this referring to running speed? Perhaps the language could be more consistent.

      These typos (due to a rounding error) in the legend and text have been corrected. Sample size has been added to the text, and we have changed Figure 1D to clarify we are referring to running speed. We moved some citations to improve clarity.

      Methods - where were Cre mice obtained from?

      Sources now better referenced in Methods (JAX or Parlato et al).

      Figure 1, panel C: The authors compared Pearson's r-coefficient results for each animal and for each variable. However, it would be interesting to show the correlation curves for each variable. However, it would be interesting to show the correlation curves for each variable as well here. Also, there is mention of a strong correlation but it is unclear whether these correlations are significant.

      See below for an example mouse.

      Author response image 5.

      Figure 3, panel F: I understand HON-DTR is a validated model but a picture orexin ablation is necessary, including pictures of orexin fibers ablation within the SNc and LC.

      See our reply to the public review above.

      Figure 5, Panel A: Same comment as Figure 1, panel C.

      We have similarly clarified the panel and legend.

      Page 4: The authors mention "Within the 1st and 4th quartile of blood glucose, movement-HON correlations were not significantly different. Please add the figures.

      The requested plot has been added to Figure 6, panel G.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary

      The study presents an investigation into how hypothalamic orexin neurons (HONs) track body movement with high precision. Using techniques including fiber photometry, video-based movement metrics, and empirical mode decomposition (EMD), the authors demonstrate that HONs encode net body movement consistently across a range of behaviors and metabolic states. They test the ability of HONs to track body movement to that of other subcortical neural populations, from which they distinguish HONs activity from other subcortical neural populations.

      Strengths:

      The study characterizes HONs activity as key indicators of movement and arousal, and this method may have potential implications for understanding sleep disorders, energy regulation, and brain-body coordination. Overall, I think this is a very interesting story, with novel findings and implications about sensorimotor systems in animals. The manuscript is clearly written and the evidence presented is rigorous. The conclusions are well supported by experimental data with clear statistical analyses.

      We thank the reviewer for their supportive feedback.

      Weaknesses/suggestions:

      There are a couple of issues I think the authors could address to make the paper better and more complete:

      (1) The study primarily focuses on steady-state behaviors. It would be interesting if the authors' current dataset allows analyses of HON dynamics during transitions between behavioral states (e.g., resting to running or grooming to sniffing). This could provide additional insights into how HONs adapt to rapid changes in body movement.

      This is a fantastic idea, and easy to check using our classification CNN. We identified the six most frequent behavioral transitions and plotted them in Figure 2H. HONs show rapid dynamics in activity aligned with behavioral changes.

      These changes are very similar to the movement magnitude along these transitions, which is now also plotted in Figure 2G.

      (2) Given the established role of HONs in arousal and wakefulness, the study could further investigate how movement-related HON dynamics interact with arousal states. For example, does HON encoding of movement differ during sleep versus wakefulness?

      To further investigate how movement encoding interacts with arousal, we now include quantification and analysis of pupil-linked arousal (see new Figure 7). We agree it would be interesting to look at what happens during sleep, especially REM sleep when some HONs are thought to be active where there is no/little body movement, but this is beyond the scope of the present study.

      (3) Although HON ablation experiments suggest that HONs do not shape movement frequency profiles. It would be more compelling if the authors could investigate whether HONs contribute to specific types of movements (e.g., fine motor vs. gross motor movements) or modulate movement initiation thresholds.

      We performed this analysis using the k-means classifier for small/large movements. Consistent with previous results, we found no significant effect (p = 0.2767) of genotype on the frequency of identified small (fine) or large (gross) movement clusters. This plot has been added to Figure 4E.

      (4) The heterogeneous movement-related orexin dynamics observed in the LC and SNc raise intriguing questions about the circuit-level mechanisms underlying these differences. Optogenetic or chemogenetic manipulation of these projections could validate the functional implications of these dynamics.

      We agree. We now discuss some implications of this in revised Discussion (paragraph 4). Please note that previous work already demonstrated that orexin action in the SNc can produce locomotion (referenced in the paragraph), though we agree that further work would be valuable.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Additional feedback:

      (1) Figure 1C: the individual data points are hard to track or see. Consider using a larger marker face to help data visualization. Similar issues can be found in Figures 2C, 2E, 5E, 6C, 6F, and 6G.

      Thickness of the lines and scatterplots have been increased.

      (2) First Section of Results: the authors claim to use a deep-learning network to automatically classify video recordings into five distinct behaviors. However, several issues need to be addressed here:

      a. In Results, the corresponding sentence lacks a reference to the Methods Section.

      Reference has been added to the text.

      b. In Methods, the description of the CNN model is quite limited, lacking many basic, necessary components including necessary references to published papers, the model training, characterization (only an overall accuracy is not enough), as well as dataset definition, preparation, augmentation (if any), etc.

      We have expanded the methods section regarding the CNN model.

      (3) First Section of Results: in the second paragraph, the authors claim that "Overall, these results reveal HON population activity precisely tracks a general degree of body movement across recorded behaviors." This is not accurate. To indicate that HONs activity tracks the general degree of body movement across behavior states, they need to further show that behavioral states with similar levels of movement metrics can be differentiated via HON activities. However, as they showed in Figure 2D, some behaviors with similar values of movement metric do not seem to be easily discerned by HON activity levels.

      We agree with you, and this is also what we originally intended to convey – now reworded for clarity.

      (4) Technical issue: Figures 3B, 3C, 3G, using local regression to plot the solid lines makes them touch negative values, which does not make sense for "power proportion" (this quantity is always non-negative).

      This is a good point. To fix this, we first log-transformed the power metric, then performed a local regression, and used the link function to transform the model predictions back to %-units for visualization. This has been noted in the methods.

      (5) Figure 3G: For a better comparison, consider combining the two plots into a single plot.

      The two plots have been merged as shown in Figure 4C.

      (6) Figure 5E: For a better data visualization, the current pair of plots can be consolidated into one single plot where the x-axis is Move and the y-axis is dGlu. In this way, it is easier to understand and the orthogonality as claimed in the manuscript can be more apparent.

      The requested plot has been added as Figure 6F.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This is a new and important system that can efficiently train mice to perform a variety of cognitive tasks in a flexible manner. It is innovative and opens the door to important experiments in the neurobiology of learning and memory. 

      Strengths: 

      Strengths include: high n's, a robust system, task flexibility, comparison of manual-like training vs constant training, circadian analysis, comparison of varying cue types, long-term measurement, and machine teaching. 

      Weaknesses: 

      I find no major problems with this report. 

      Minor weaknesses: 

      (1)  Line 219: Water consumption per day remained the same, but number of trails triggered was more as training continued. First, is this related to manual-type training? Also, I'm trying to understand this result quantitatively, since it seems counter-intuitive: I would assume that with more trials, more water would be consumed since accuracy should go up over training (so more water per average trial). Am I understanding this right? Can the authors give more detail or understanding to how more trials can be triggered but no more water is consumed despite training? 

      Thanks for the comment. We would like to clarify the phenomenon described in Line 219: As the training advanced, the number of trials triggered by mice per day decreased (rather than increased as you mentioned in the comment) gradually for both manual and autonomous groups of mice (Fig. 2H left). The performance, as you mentioned, improved over time (Fig. 2D and 2E), leading to an increased probability of obtaining water and thus relatively stable daily water intake (Fig. 2H middle). We believe the stable daily intake is the minimum amount of water required by the mice under circumstance of autonomous behavioral training. To make the statement more clearly, we indicated the corresponding figure numbers in the text.

      Results “… As shown in Fig. 2H, autonomous training yielded significantly higher number of trial/day (980 ± 25 vs. 611 ± 26, Fig. 2H left) and more volume of water consumption/day (1.65 ± 0.06 vs. 0.97 ± 0.03 ml, Fig. 2H middle), which resulted in monotonic increase of body weight that was even comparable to the free water group (Fig.2H right). In contrast, the body weight in manual training group experienced a sharp drop at the beginning of training and was constantly lower than autonomous group throughout the training stage (Fig. 2H right).”

      (2) Figure 2J: The X-axis should have some label: at least "training type". Ideally, a legend with colors can be included, although I see the colors elsewhere in the figure. If a legend cannot be added, then the color scheme should be explained in the caption.

      Thanks for the suggestion. The labels with corresponding colors for x-axis have been added for Fig. 2J.

      (3) Figure 2K: What is the purple line? I encourage a legend here. The same legend could apply to 2J.

      Thanks for the suggestion. The legend has been added for Fig. 2K.

      (4) Supplementary Figure S2 D: I do not think the phrase "relying on" is correct. Instead, I think "predicted by" or "correlating with" might be better. 

      We thank the reviewer for the valuable suggestion. The phrase has been changed to ‘predicted by’ for better suitability.

      Figure S2 “(D), percentage of trials significantly predicted by different regressors during task learning. …”

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The manuscript by Yu et al. describes a novel approach for collecting complex and different cognitive phenotypes in individually housed mice in their home cage. The authors report a simple yet elegant design that they developed for assessing a variety of complex and novel behavioral paradigms autonomously in mice. 

      Strengths: 

      The data are strong, the arguments are convincing, and I think the manuscript will be highly cited given the complexity of behavioral phenotypes one can collect using this relatively inexpensive ($100/box) and high throughput procedure (without the need for human interaction). Additionally, the authors include a machine learning algorithm to correct for erroneous strategies that mice develop which is incredibly elegant and important for this approach as mice will develop odd strategies when given complete freedom. 

      Weaknesses:

      (1) A limitation of this approach is that it requires mice to be individually housed for days to months. This should be discussed in depth. 

      Thank you for raising this important point. We agree that the requirement for individual housing of mice during the training period is a limitation of our approach, and we appreciate the opportunity to discuss this in more depth. In the manuscript, we add a section to the Discussion to address this limitation, including the potential impact of individual housing on the mice, the rationale for individual housing in our study, and efforts or alternatives made to mitigate the effects of individual housing.

      Discussion “… Firstly, our experiments were confined to single-housed mice, which is known to influence murine behavior and physiology, potentially affecting social interaction and stress levels [76]. In our study, individual housing was necessary to ensure precise behavioral tracking, eliminate competitive interactions during task performance, and maintain consistent training schedules without disruptions from cage-mate disturbances. However, the potential of group-housed training has been explored with technologies such as RFID [28,29,32–34] to distinguish individual mice, which potentially improving the training efficiency and facilitating research of social behaviors [77]. Notably, it has shown that simultaneous training of group-housed mice, without individual differentiation, can still achieve criterion performance [25].”

      (2) A major issue with continuous self-paced tasks such as the autonomous d2AFC used by the authors is that the inter-trial intervals can vary significantly. Mice may do a few trials, lose interest, and disengage from the task for several hours. This is problematic for data analysis that relies on trial duration to be similar between trials (e.g., reinforcement learning algorithms). It would be useful to see the task engagement of the mice across a 24-hour cycle (e.g., trials started, trials finished across a 24-hour period) and approaches for overcoming this issue of varying inter-trial intervals. 

      Thank you for your insightful comment regarding the variability in inter-trial intervals and its potential impact on data analysis. We agree that this is an important consideration for continuous self-paced tasks.

      In our original manuscript, we have showed the general task engagement across 24-hour cycle (Fig. 2K), which revealed two peaks of engagements during the dark cycle with relatively fewer trials during the light cycle. To facilitate analyses requiring consistent trial durations, we defined trial blocks as sequences between two no-response trials. Notably, approximately 66.6% of trials occurred within blocks of >5 consecutive trials (Fig. 2L), which may be particularly suitable for such analyses.

      In the revised manuscript, we also added the analysis of the histogram of inter-trial-interval for both the autonomous and manual training paradigms in HABITS (Fig. S2H), which shows that around 55.2% and 77.5% of the intervals are less than 2 seconds in autonomous and manual training, respectively.

      Results “… We found more than two-third of the trials was done in >5-trial blocks (Fig. 2L left) which resulted in more than 55% of the trials were with inter-trial-interval less than 2 seconds (Fig. S2H).”

      Regarding the approaches to mitigate the issue of varying inter-trial interval, we observed that manual training (i.e., manually transferring to HABITS for ~2 hr/day) in Fig. S2H resulted in more trials with short inter-trial-interval, suggesting that constrained access time promotes task engagement and reduces interval variability. Fig. 2L also indicated that the averaged correct rate increased and the earlylick rate decreased as the length of block increased. This approach could be valuable for studies where consistent trial timing is critical. In the context of our study, we could actually introduce a light, for example, to serve as the cue that prompt the animals to engage during a fixed time duration in a day.

      Discussion “… In contrast, the self-paced nature of autonomous training may permit greater variability in attentional engagement 83 and inter-trial-intervals, which could be problematic for data analysis relaying on consistent intervals and/or engagements. Future studies should explore how controlled contextual constraints enhance learning efficiency and whether incorporating such measures into HABITS could optimize its performance.”

      (3) Movies - it would be beneficial for the authors to add commentary to the video (hit, miss trials). It was interesting watching the mice but not clear whether they were doing the task correctly or not. 

      Thanks for the reminder. We have added subtitles to both of the videos. Since the supplementary video1 was not recorded with sound, the correctness of the trials was hard to judge. We replaced the video with another one with clear sound recordings, and the subtitles were commented in detail.

      (4) The strength of this paper (from my perspective) is the potential utility it has for other investigators trying to get mice to do behavioral tasks. However, not enough information was provided about the construction of the boxes, interface, and code for running the boxes. If the authors are not willing to provide this information through eLife, GitHub, or their own website then my evaluation of the impact and significance of this paper would go down significantly. 

      Thanks for this important comment. We would like to clarify that the construction methods, GUI, code for our system, PCB and CAD files (newly uploaded) have already been made publicly available on https://github.com/Yaoyao-Hao/HABITS. Additionally, we have open-sourced all the codes and raw data for all training protocols (https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.27192897). We will continue to maintain these resources in the future.

      Minor concerns: 

      (5) Learning rate is confusing for Figure 3 results as it actually refers to trials to reach the criterion, and not the actual rate of learning (e.g., slope).

      Thanks for pointing this out. The ‘learning rate’ which refers to trial number to reach criterion has been changed to ‘the number of trials to reach criterion’.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In this set of experiments, the authors describe a novel research tool for studying complex cognitive tasks in mice, the HABITS automated training apparatus, and a novel "machine teaching" approach they use to accelerate training by algorithmically providing trials to animals that provide the most information about the current rule state for a given task. 

      Strengths: 

      There is much to be celebrated in an inexpensively constructed, replicable training environment that can be used with mice, which have rapidly become the model species of choice for understanding the roles of distinct circuits and genetic factors in cognition. Lingering challenges in developing and testing cognitive tasks in mice remain, however, and these are often chalked up to cognitive limitations in the species. The authors' findings, however, suggest that instead, we may need to work creatively to meet mice where they live. In some cases, it may be that mice may require durations of training far longer than laboratories are able to invest with manual training (up to over 100k trials, over months of daily testing) but the tasks are achievable. The "machine teaching" approach further suggests that this duration could be substantially reduced by algorithmically optimizing each trial presented during training to maximize learning. 

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) Cognitive training and testing in rodent models fill a number of roles. Sometimes, investigators are interested in within-subjects questions - querying a specific circuit, genetically defined neuron population, or molecule/drug candidate, by interrogating or manipulating its function in a highly trained animal. In this scenario, a cohort of highly trained animals that have been trained via a method that aims to make their behavior as similar as possible is a strength. 

      However, often investigators are interested in between-subjects questions - querying a source of individual differences that can have long-term and/or developmental impacts, such as sex differences or gene variants. This is likely to often be the case in mouse models especially, because of their genetic tractability. In scenarios where investigators have examined cognitive processes between subjects in mice who vary across these sources of individual difference, the process of learning a task has been repeatedly shown to be different. The authors do not appear to have considered individual differences except perhaps as an obstacle to be overcome. 

      The authors have perhaps shown that their main focus is highly-controlled within-subjects questions, as their dataset is almost exclusively made up of several hundred young adult male mice, with the exception of 6 females in a supplemental figure. It is notable that these female mice do appear to learn the two-alternative forced-choice task somewhat more rapidly than the males in their cohort.

      Thank you for your insightful comments and for highlighting the importance of considering both within-subject and between-subject questions in cognitive training and testing in rodent models. We acknowledge that our study primarily focused on highly controlled within-subject questions. However, the datasets we provided did show preliminary evidences for the ‘between-subject’ questions. Key observations include:

      The large variability in learning rates among mice observed in Fig. 2I;

      The overall learning rate difference between male and female subjects (Fig. 2D vs. Fig. S2G);

      The varying nocturnal behavioral patterns (Fig. 2K), etc.

      We recognize the value of exploring between-subjects differences in mouse model and discussed more details in the Discussion part.

      Discussion “Our study was designed to standardize behavior for the precise interrogation of neural mechanisms, specifically addressing within-subject questions. However, investigators are often interested in between-subject differences—such as sex differences or genetic variants—which can have long-term behavioral and cognitive implications [72,74]. This is particularly relevant in mouse models due to their genetic tractability [75]. Although our primary focus was not on between-subject differences, the dataset we generated provides preliminary evidence for such investigations. Several behavioral readouts revealed individual variability among mice, including large disparities in learning rates across individuals (Fig. 2I), differences in overall learning rates between male and female subjects (Fig. 2D vs. Fig. S2G), variations in nocturnal behavioral patterns (Fig. 2K), etc.”

      (2) Considering the implications for mice modeling relevant genetic variants, it is unclear to what extent the training protocols and especially the algorithmic machine teaching approach would be able to inform investigators about the differences between their groups during training. For investigators examining genetic models, it is unclear whether this extensive training experience would mitigate the ability to observe cognitive differences, or select the animals best able to overcome them - eliminating the animals of interest. Likewise, the algorithmic approach aims to mitigate features of training such as side biases, but it is worth noting that the strategic uses of side biases in mice, as in primates, can benefit learning, rather than side biases solely being a problem. However, the investigators may be able to highlight variables selected by the algorithm that are associated with individual strategies in performing their tasks, and this would be a significant contribution.

      Thank you for the insightful comments. We acknowledge that the extensive training experience, particularly through the algorithmic machine teaching approach, could potentially influence the ability to observe cognitive differences between groups of mice with relevant genetic variants. However, our study design and findings suggest that this approach can still provide valuable insights into individual differences and strategies used by the animals during training. First, the behavioral readout (including learning rate, engagement pattern, etc.) as mentioned above, could tell certain number of differences among mice. Second, detailed modelling analysis (with logistical regression modelling) could further dissect the strategy that mouse use along the training process (Fig. S2B). We have actually highlighted some variables selected by the regression that are associated with individual strategies in performing their tasks (Fig. S2C) and these strategies could be different between manual and autonomous training groups (Fig. S2D). We included these comments in the Discussion part for further clearance.

      Discussion “… Furthermore, a detailed logistic regression analysis dissected the strategies mice employed during training (Fig. S2B). Notably, the regression identified variables associated with individual task-performance strategies (Fig. S2C), which also differed between manually and autonomously trained groups (Fig. S2D). Thus, our system could facilitate high-throughput behavioral studies exploring between-subject differences in the future.”

      (3) A final, intriguing finding in this manuscript is that animal self-paced training led to much slower learning than "manual" training, by having the experimenter introduce the animal to the apparatus for a few hours each day. Manual training resulted in significantly faster learning, in almost half the number of trials on average, and with significantly fewer omitted trials. This finding does not necessarily argue that manual training is universally a better choice because it leads to more limited water consumption. However, it suggests that there is a distinct contribution of experimenter interactions and/or switching contexts in cognitive training, for example by activating an "occasion setting" process to accelerate learning for a distinct period of time. Limiting experimenter interactions with mice may be a labor-saving intervention, but may not necessarily improve performance. This could be an interesting topic of future investigation, of relevance to understanding how animals of all species learn.

      Thank you for your insightful comments. We agree that the finding that manual training led to significantly faster learning compared to self-paced training is both intriguing and important. One of the possible reasons we think is due to the limited duration of engagement provided by the experimenter in the manual training case, which forced the mice to concentrate more on the trials (thus with fewer omitting trials) than in autonomous training. Your suggestion that experimenter interactions might activate an "occasion setting" process is particularly interesting. In the context of our study, we could actually introduce, for example, a light, serving as the cue that prompt the animals to engage; and when the light is off, the engagement was not accessible any more for the mice to simulate the manual training situation. We agree that this could be an interesting topic for future investigation that might create a more conducive environment for learning, thereby accelerating the learning rate.

      Discussion “… Lastly, while HABITS achieves criterion performance in a similar or even shorter overall days compared to manual training, it requires more trials to reach the same learning criterion (Fig. 2G). We hypothesize that this difference in trial efficiency may stem from the constrained engagement duration imposed by the experimenter in manual training, which could compel mice to focus more intensely on task execution, resulting in less trial omissions (Fig. 2F). In contrast, the self-paced nature of autonomous training may permit greater variability in attentional engagement 83 and inter-trial-intervals, which could be problematic for data analysis relaying on consistent intervals and/or engagements. Future studies should explore how controlled contextual constraints enhance learning efficiency and whether incorporating such measures into HABITS could optimize its performance.”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      As I mentioned in the weaknesses, I did not see code or CAD drawings for their home cages and how these interact with a computer.

      Thanks for the comment. We would like to clarify that the construction methods, GUI, code for our system, PCB and CAD files (newly uploaded) have already been made publicly available on https://github.com/Yaoyao-Hao/HABITS.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study highlights the strengths of using predictive computational models to inform C. elegans screening studies of compounds' eCects on aging and lifespan. The authors primarily focus on all-trans retinoic acid (atRA), one of the 5 compounds (out of 16 tested) that extended C. elegans lifespan in their experiments. They show that atRA has positive eCects on C. elegans lifespan and age-related health, while it has more modest and inconsistent eCects (i.e., some detrimental impacts) for C. briggsae and C. tropicalis. In genetic experiments designed to evaluate contributing mediators of lifespan extension with atRA exposure, it was found that 150 µM of atRA did not significantly extend lifespan in akt1 or akt-2 loss-of-function mutants, nor in animals with loss of function of aak-2, or skn-1 (in which atRA had toxic eCects); these genes appear to be required for atRA-mediated lifespan extension. hsf-1 and daf-16 loss-of-function mutants both had a modest but statistically significant lifespan extension with 150 µM of atRA, suggesting that these transcription factors may contribute towards mediating atRA lifespan extension, but that they are not individually required for some lifespan extension. RNAseq assessment of transcriptional changes in day 4 atRA-treated adult wild-type worms revealed some interesting observations. Consistent with the study's genetic mutant lifespan observations, many of the atRA-regulated genes with the greatest fold-change diCerences are known regulated targets of daf-2 and/or skn-1 signaling pathways in C. elegans. hsf-1 loss-offunction mutants show a shifted atRA transcriptional response, revealing a dependence on hsf-1 for ~60% of the atRA-downregulated genes. On the other hand, RNAseq analysis in aak-2 loss-of-function mutants revealed that aak-2 is only required for less than a quarter of the atRA transcriptional response. All together, this study is proof of the concept that computational models can help optimize C. elegans screening approaches that test compounds' eCects on lifespan, and provide comprehensive transcriptomic and genetic insights into the lifespan-extending eCects of all-trans retinoic acid (atRA).

      Strengths:

      (1) A clearly described and well-justified account describes the approach used to prioritize and select compounds for screening, based on using the top candidates from a published list of computationally ranked compounds (Fuentealba et al., 2019) that were crossreferenced with other bioinformatics publications to predict anti-aging compounds, after de-selecting compounds previously evaluated in C. elegans as per the DrugAge database. 16 compounds were tested at 4-5 diCerent concentrations to evaluate eCects on C. elegans lifespan.

      (2) Robust experimental design was undertaken evaluating the lifespan eCects of atRA, as

      it was tested on three strains each of C. elegans, C. briggsae, and C. tropicalis, with trial replication performed at three distinct laboratories. These observations extended beyond lifespan to include evaluations of health metrics related to swimming performance.

      (3) In-depth analyses of the RNAseq data of whole-worm transcriptional responses to atRA revealed interesting insights into regulator pathways and novel groups of genes that may be involved in mediating lifespan-extension eCects (e.g., atRA-induced upregulation of sphingolipid metabolism genes, atRA-upregulation of genes in a poorly-characterized family of C. elegans paralogs predicted to have kinase-like activity, and disproportionate downregulation of collagen genes with atRA).

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of our paper.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The authors' computational-based compound screening approach led to a ~30% prediction success rate for compounds that could extend the median lifespan of C.elegans. However, follow-up experiments on the top compounds highlighted the fact that some of these observed "successes" could be driven by indirect, confounding eCects of these compounds on the bacterial food source, rather than direct beneficial eCects on C. elegans physiology and lifespan. For instance, this appeared to be the case for the "top" hit of propranolol; other compounds were not tested with metabolically inert or killed bacteria. In addition, there are no comparative metrics provided to compare this study's ~30% success rate to screening approaches that do not use computational predictions.

      We do test whether compounds have a direct e:ect on bacterial growth. We have the text to clarify that fact. There may be potential lifespan e:ects from atRA due to changes in bacterial metabolites, however exploring that more fully is beyond the scope of the current work. 

      We very much appreciate the question regarding relative success. An appropriate benchmark for “hit rate” is perhaps best provided by Petrascheck, Ye & Buck (2007), who conducted a large-scale screen of 88,000 compounds for e:ects on adult lifespan in C. elegans. They found an initial screening hit rate of 1.2% (1083/88000), which were then retested for a verified hit rate of 0.13% (115/88000), with a retest failure rate of 89% (968/1083). Similarly, Lucanic et al. (2016) screened 30,000 compounds, with an initial hit rate of approximately 1.7% (~500/30000), or these 180 were selected for retesting, resulting in a final verified hit rate of 0.19% (57/29680), which is comparable to the Petrascheck et al. result. The text in the discussion has been modified to include these studies.

      (2)Transcriptomic analyses of atRA eCects were extensive in this study, but evaluations and discussions of non-transcriptional eCects of key proposed regulators (such as AMPK) were limited. For instance, non-transcriptional eCects of aak-2/AMPK might account for its requirement for mediating lifespan extension eCects, since aak-2 was not required for a major proportion of atRA transcriptional responses.

      We naturally agree with the reviewer that non-transcriptional e:ects are possible and well worth pursuing in future work. However, these e:ects will still show within our study, as any upstream non-transcriptional e:ects are likely to reveal themselves in downstream transcriptional changes, as measured here.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, Banse et al. experimentally validate the power of computational approaches that predict anti-aging molecules using the multi-species approach of the Caenorhabditis Intervention Testing Program (CITP). Filtering candidate molecules based on transcriptional profiles, ML models, literature searches, and the DrugAge database, they selected 16 compounds for testing. Of those, eight did not aCect C.elegan's lifespan, three shortened it, and five extended C.elegan's lifespan, resulting in a hit rate of over 30%. Of those five, they then focused on all-trans-retinoic acid (atRA), a compound that has previously resulted in contradictory eCects. The lifespan-extending eCect of atRA was consistent in all C. elegans strains tested, was absent in C. briggsae, and a small eCect was observed in some C. tropicalis strains. Similar results were obtained for measures of healthspan. The authors then investigated the mechanism of action of atRA and showed that it was only partially dependent on daf-16 but required akt-1, akt-2, skn-1, hsf-1, and, to some degree, pmk-1. The authors further investigate the downstream eCects of atRA exposure by conducting RNAseq experiments in both wild-type and mutant animals to show that some, but surprisingly few, of the gene expression changes that are observed in wild-type animals are lost in the hsf-1 and aak-2 mutants.

      Strengths:

      Overall, this study is well conceived and executed as it investigates the eCect of atRA across diCerent concentrations, strains, and species, including life and health span. Revealing the variability between sites, assays, and the method used is a powerful aspect of this study. It will do a lot to dispel the nonsensical illusion that we can determine a percent increase in lifespan to the precision of two floating point numbers.

      An interesting and potentially important implication arises from this study. The computational selection of compounds was agnostic regarding strain or species diCerences and was predominantly based on observations made in mammalian systems. The hit rate calculated is based on the results of C. elegans and not on the molecules' eCectiveness in Briggsae or Tropicalis. If it were, the hit rate would be much lower. How is that? It would suggest that ML models and transcriptional data obtained from mammals have a higher predictive value for C. elegans than for the other two species. This selectivity for C.elegans over C.tropicalis and C.Briggsae seems both puzzling and unexpected. The predictions for longevity were based on the transcriptional data in cell lines.

      This is a common observation in the CITP for which we do not currently have a satisfying explanation. For whatever reason, C. elegans is much more responsive to compounds than other species, much like it is more responsive to RNAi and other environmental interventions. It may be less active in detoxifying external agents than the other species, although this is just speculation at the moment. We continue to investigate this question, but that work is beyond the scope of the present paper.

      Would it be feasible to compare the mammalian data to the transcriptional data in Figure 5 and see how well they match? While this is clear beyond the focus of this study, an implied prediction is that running RNAseqs for all these strains exposed to atRA would reveal that the transcriptional changes observed in the strains where it extends lifespan the most should match the mammalian data best. Otherwise, how could the mammalian datasets be used to predict the eCects of C.elegans over C.Briggsae or C.Tropicalis have more predictive for one species than the other? There are a lot of IFs in this prediction, but such an experiment would reconsider and validate the basis on which the original predictions were made.

      These questions are worth pursuing in the future but are beyond the scope of the current work.

      Weaknesses:

      Many of the most upregulated genes, such as cyps and pgps are xenobiotic response genes upregulated in many transcriptional datasets from C. elegans drug studies. Their expression might be necessary to deal with atRA breakdown metabolites to prevent toxicity rather than confer longevity. Because atRA is very light sensitive and has toxicity of breakdown, metabolites may explain some of the diCerences observed with the lifespan of machine eCects compared to standard assay practices.

      This is certainly a possibility, although we often observe longer lifespans on the ALM, perhaps because they themselves are stressful, thereby providing a more sensitive background environment for detecting positive stress response modulators.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Banse et al., demonstrate that combining computer prediction with genetic analysis in distinct Caenorhabditis species can streamline the discovery of aging interventions by taking advantage of the diverse pool of compounds that are currently available. They demonstrate that through careful prioritization of candidate compounds, they are able to accomplish a 30% positive hit rate for interventions that produce significant lifespan extensions. Within the positive hits, they focus on all-trans retinoic acid (atRA) and discover that it modulates lifespan through conserved longevity pathways such as AKT-1 and AKT-2 (and other conserved Akt-targets such as Nrf2/SKN-1 and HSF1/HSF-1) as well as through AAK-2, a conserved catalytic subunit of AMPK. To better understand the genetic mechanisms behind lifespan extension upon atRA treatment, the authors perform RNAseq experiments using a variety of genetic backgrounds for cross-comparison and validation. Using this current state-of-the-art approach for studying gene expression, the authors determine that atRA treatment produces gene expression changes across a broad set of stress-response and longevity-related pathways. Overall, this study is important since it highlights the potential of combining traditional genetic analysis in the genetically tractable organism C. elegans with computational methods that will become even more powerful with the swift advancements being made in artificial intelligence. The study possesses both theoretical and practical implications not only in the field of aging but also in related fields such as health and disease. Most of the claims in this study are supported by solid evidence, but the conclusions can be refined with a small set of additional experiments or re-analysis of data.

      Strengths:

      (1) The criteria for prioritizing compounds for screening are well-defined and easy to replicate (Figure 1), even for scientists with limited experience in computational biology. The approach is also adaptable to other systems or model organisms.

      (2) I commend the researchers for doing follow-up experiments with the compound propranolol to verify its eCect on lifespan (Figure 2 Supplement 2), given the observation that it aCected the growth of OP50. To prevent false hits in the future, the reviewer recommends the use of inactivated OP50 for future experiments to remove this confounding variable.

      (3) The sources of variation (Figure 3, Figure Supplement 2) are taken into account and demonstrate the need for advancing our understanding of the lifespan phenotype due to inter-individual variation.

      (4) The addition of the C. elegans swim test in addition to the lifespan assays provides further evidence of atRA-induced improvement in longevity.

      (5) The RNAseq approach was performed in a variety of genetic backgrounds, which allowed the authors to determine the relationship between AAK-2 and HSF-1 regulation of the retinoic acid pathway in C. elegans, specifically, that the former functions downstream of the latter.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting these strengths.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The filtering of compounds for testing using the DrugAge database requires that the database is consistently updated. In this particular case, even though atRA does not appear in the database, the authors themselves cite literature that has already demonstrated atRA-induced lifespan extension, which should have precluded this compound from the analysis in the first place.

      As often happens in science, this work was initiated before Statzer et al. (2021) was published. As such, it is included in the test set.

      (2) The threshold for determining positive hits is arbitrary, and in this case, a 30% positive hit rate was observed when the threshold is set to a lifespan extension of around 5% based on Figure 1B (the authors fail to explicitly state the cut-oC for what is considered a positive hit).

      Any compound that statistically increases lifespan is considered a positive hit by the CITP. The CITP in general is powered to detect minimum e:ect sizes of 5%.

      (3) The authors demonstrate that atRA extends lifespan in a species-specific manner (Figure 3). Specifically, this extension only occurs in the species C. elegans yet, the title implies that atRA-induced lifespan extension occurs in diCerent Caenorhabditis species when it is clearly not the case. While the authors state that failure to observe phenotypes in C. briggsae and C. tropicalis is a common feature of CITP tests, they do not speculate as to why this phenomenon occurs.

      Please see the comment above.

      (4) There are discrepancies between the lifespan curves by hand (Figure 3 Figure Supplement 1) and using the automated lifespan machine (Figure 3 Supplement 3). Specifically, in the automated lifespan assays, there are drastic changes in the slope of the survival curve which do not occur in the manual assays. This may be due to improper filtering of non-worm objects, improper annotation of death times, or improper distribution of plates in each scanner.

      Our storyboarding SOP ensures that discrepancies in the shape of the curve are unlikely to be due to annotation errors. We check every page of the storyboard by hand, so all non-worm objects are excluded. Furthermore, the first and last ~10% of deaths are checked by hand (as we observed that these time points are the most likely to be wrongly called by the software), with a few deaths chosen at random from the middle to ensure that the software is calling death times accurately. If we find a high amount of inaccurately called deaths, the entire plate is annotated by hand. For this specific experiment, 18% of the total deaths were hand annotated. Plates are randomly distributed across each scanner in an e:ort to prevent bias. As noted above, it does appear that the ALM environment and the “by hand” environment are somewhat di:erent.

      (5) The authors miss an opportunity to determine whether the lifespan extension phenotype attributed to the retinoic acid pathway is mostly transcriptional in nature or whether some of it is post-transcriptional. The authors even state "that while aak-2 is absolutely required for the longevity eCects of atRA, aak-2 is required only for a small proportion (~1/4) of the transcriptional response", suggesting that some of the eCects are post-transcriptional. Further information could have been obtained had the authors also performed RNAseq analysis on the tol-1 mutant which exhibited an enhanced response to atRA compared to wild-type animals, and comparing the magnitude of gene expression changes between the tol-1 mutant and all other genetic backgrounds for which RNAseq was performed.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Will the raw RNA-seq data be publicly deposited? Please clarify. This would strengthen the value of the study.

      All data is available. We have clarified this in the text.

      (2) Since all-trans retinoic acid is a metabolite of vitamin A, it seems important to include a discussion of and reference to the recent study SKN-1/NRF2 upregulation by vitamin A is conserved from nematodes to mammals and is critical for lifespan extension in Caenorhabditis elegans (Sirakawin et al Cell Reports 2024). Sirakawin et al include data that corroborates and expands on the findings of the current study, including the observation that vitamin A reduces whole-body lipid deposition (agrees with some of the transcriptional findings in the current study); that vitamin A protects against oxidative stress; that vitamin A elevates expression of gst-5, skn-1, and pmk-1; and that loss-offunction mutation of skn-1 has similar eCects to the current study, in terms of suppressing lifespan-extending eCects of vitamin A. In addition, adding some discussion of oxidative stress would strengthen this work, in light of widespread perceptions of the antioxidant properties of vitamin A (and its metabolites).

      Thank you for this suggestion. We have added this citation to the discussion.

      (3) Minor typo: Lines 341-342 - After a sentence that contains the phrase "collagen and neuropeptide related genes", the next sentence uses the term "the latter" in reference to the collagen genes (should be "the former").

      Edited in text.

      (4) Minor correction: In Figure 6, the information in the figure legend is swapped for figure panels A) and B).

      Edited in figure caption.

      (5) To me, the subtitle heading "Loss of AMPK leads to a unique transcriptional profile in response to atRA treatment" (Line 403) is misleading, considering the contents of the text in that section, and the data presented in Figure 6.

      We have altered this heading to reflect this comment.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Using diCerent colors for the diCerent testing sites would make Figure 3 more readable.

      Edited so that each lab is represented by a di:erent shade of green.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      It would be interesting to investigate the eCect of even higher concentrations of atRA as it has been reported that atRA accumulation is associated with deleterious phenotypes in mice (Snyder et al., 2020, FASEB J).

      We tested the highest concentration (150 uM) based on the solubility of the compound using our standardized plate treatment protocol, so we are unable to test higher concentrations.  

      A good first guess for a downstream retinoid receptor is nhr-23 which is the homolog of the vertebrate ROR genes. Stehlin-Gaon et al. (2003, Nat Struct Mol Biol) have shown that atRA is a ligand for the orphan nuclear receptor RORβ. It might be interesting to study the eCects of atRA on an nhr-23::AID (auxin inducible degron) background. This would allow you to circumvent the developmental phenotypes as a result of nhr-23 knockdown. Patrick/Stephen

      A few notes on the text/figures:

      Line 342: I believe the authors meant "former" instead of "latter".

      Corrected in text.

      Line 346: Can you also highlight col-144 in Fig. 5 S1?

      This is not really feasible, as it is in the cluster near the where the axes meet (red arrow).

      Line 400: CUB pathogen - based on Figure 6 Supp 1, this occurs in aak-2 and not in hsf-1.

      Great catch by the reviewer. We have updated the figure with the correct information.

      Line 414: hedgehog-like signaling - occurs in hsf-1 instead of aak-2. Similar inconsistencies occur in lines 415 (sterol), 417 (C-type lectin), and 418 (unassigned pathogens)

      We have updated the text to eliminate potential conflicts/confusion in the presentation here.

      Line 434: I believe the authors meant Figure "6" instead of "7"

      Edited in text.

      Line 475: Is it "fifteen" or "sixteen" compounds initially targeted?

      Edited in text.

      Can you please include the population sizes for the lifespan assays if not yet included in the detailed protocol to be published in FigShare (to which I currently do not have access to)?

      Added “50 animals per petri plate” to Lifespan Assay methods section; additionally, all sample sizes are included as a summary tab in each dataset on figshare.com (10.6084/m9.figshare.c.6320690).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors in this study extensively investigate how telomere length (TL) regulates hTERT expression via non-telomeric binding of the telomere-associated protein TRF2. They conclusively show that TRF2 binding to long telomeres results in a reduction in its binding to the hTERT promoter. In contrast, short telomeres restore TRF2 binding in the hTERT promoter, recruiting repressor complexes like PRC2, and suppressing hTERT expression. The study presents several significant findings revealing a previously unknown mechanism of hTERT regulation by TRF2 in a TL-dependent manner

      Strengths:

      (1) A previously unknown mechanism linking telomere length and hTERT regulation through the non-telomeric TRF2 protein has been established strengthening the telomere biology understanding.

      (2) The authors used both cancer cell lines and iPSCs to showcase their hypothesis and multiple parameters to validate the role of TRF2 in hTERT regulation.

      (3) Comprehensive integration of the recent literature findings and implementation in the current study.

      (4) In vivo validation of the findings.

      (5) Rigorous controls and well-designed assays have been use.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The authors should comment on the cell proliferation and morphology of the engineered cell lines with ST or LT.

      The cell proliferation and morphology of the engineered cells were monitored during experiments. With a doubling time within 16-18 hours, all the cancer cell line pairs used in the study were counted and seeded equally before experiments.

      No significant difference in morphology or cell count (before harvesting for experiments) was noted for the stable cell lines, namely, HT1080 ST-HT1080 LT, HCT116 p53 null scrambled control-HCT116 p53 null hTERC knockdown.

      MDAMB 231 cells which were treated with guanine-rich telomere repeats (GTR) over a period of 12 days, as per the protocol mentioned in Methods. Due to the alternate day of GTR treatment in serum-free media followed by replenishment with serum-supplemented media, we noted that cells would undergo periodic delay in their proliferation (or transient arrest) aligning with the GTR oligo-feeding cycles and appeared somewhat larger in comparison to their parental untreated cells.

      Next, the cells with Cas9-telomeric sgRNA mediated telomere trimming were maintained transiently (till 3 days after transfection). During this time, no significant change in morphology or cell proliferation was observed in any of the cell lines, namely HCT116 or HEK293T Gaussia Luciferase reporter cells. iPSCs were also monitored. However, no change in morphology or cellular proliferation was observed during the 5 days post-transfection and antibiotic selection.  

      (2) Also, the entire study uses engineered cell lines, with artificially elongated or shortened telomeres that conclusively demonstrate the role of hTERT regulation by TRF2 in telomere-length dependent manner, but using ALT negative cell lines with naturally short telomere length vs those with long telomeres will give better perspective. Primary cells can also be used in this context.

      The reviewer correctly highlights (as we also acknowledge in the Discussion) that our study primarily utilizes engineered cell lines with artificially elongated or shortened telomeres. We agree that using ALT-negative cells with naturally short versus long telomeres would provide additional perspective. However, a key challenge in this experimental setup is the inherent variation in TRF2 protein levels among these cell types—a parameter central to our hypothesis. Comparing observations across such non-isogenic cell line pairs presents experimental limitations as these would require extensive normalization for multiple factors and introduce additional complexities, which would be difficult to interpret with clarity.

      We had also explored primary cells, specifically foreskin fibroblasts and MRC5 lung fibroblasts, as suggested by the reviewer. However, we encountered two significant challenges. To achieve a notable telomere length difference of at least 20%, these primary cells had to undergo a minimum of 25 passages. During this period, we observed a substantial decline in their proliferation capacity and an increased tendency toward replicative senescence. Additionally, we noted a significant reduction in TRF2 protein levels as the primary cells aged, consistent with findings from Fujita K et al., 2010 (Nat Cell Biol.), which reported p53-induced, Siah-1-mediated proteasomal degradation of TRF2. Due to these practical limitations, we focused on cancer cell lines with respective isogenic backgrounds, ensuring a controlled experimental framework. On the other hand, this opens new avenues for future research to explore broader implications. Investigating other primary cell types that may not present these challenges could be a valuable direction for future studies.

      (3) The authors set up time-dependent telomere length changes by dox induction, which may differ from the gradual telomere attrition or elongation that occurs naturally during aging, disease progression, or therapy. This aspect should be explored.

      In this study, we utilized a Doxycycline-inducible hTERT expression system to modulate telomere length in cancer cells, aiming to capture any gradual changes that might occur upon steady telomerase induction or overexpression—an event frequently observed in cancer progression. We monitored telomere length and telomerase activity at regular intervals (Supplementary Figure 2), noting a gradual increase until a characteristic threshold was reached, followed by a reversal to the initial telomere length.

      While this model provides interesting insights in context of cancer cells, it does not replicate the conditions of aging or therapeutic intervention. We agree that exploring telomere length-dependent regulation of hTERT in normal aging cells is an important avenue for future research. Investigating TRF2 occupancy on the hTERT promoter in response to telomere length alterations through therapeutic interventions—such as telomestatin or imetelstat (telomerase inhibitors) and 6-thio-2’-deoxyguanosine (telomere damage inducer)—would provide valuable insights and warrants further exploration.

      (4) How does the hTERT regulation by TRF2 in a TL-dependent manner affect the ETS binding on hTERT mutant promoter sites?

      In our previous study (Sharma et al., 2021, Cell Reports), we have experimentally demonstrated that GABPA and TRF2 do not compete for binding at the mutant hTERT promoter (Figure 4M-R). Silencing GABPA in various mutant hTERT promoter cells did not increase TRF2 binding. While GABPA has been reported to show increased binding at the mutant promoter compared to the wild-type (Bell et al., 2015, Science), no telomere length (TL) sensitivity has been noted yet. In the current manuscript we show that telomere alterations in hTERT mutant cells (that do not form promoter G-quadruplex) does not significantly affect TRF2 occupancy at the promoter, reinforcing our earlier findings that G-quadruplex formation is crucial for TRF2 recruitment. Since TRF2 binding is not affected this would not impact GABPA binding. Therefore change in TL is unlikely to influence ETS binding by GABPA.

      (5) Stabilization of the G-quadruplex structures in ST and LT conditions along with the G4 disruption experimentation (demonstrated by the authors) will strengthen the hypothesis.

      We agree with the reviewer’s suggestion that stabilizing G-quadruplex (G4) structures in mutant promoter cells under ST and LT conditions would further strengthen our hypothesis. From our ChIP experiments on hTERT promoter mutant cells following G4 stabilization with ligands, as reported in Sharma et al. 2021 (Figure 5G), we observed that TRF2 occupancy was regained in the telomere-length unaltered versions of -124G>A and -146G>A HEK293T Gaussia luciferase cells (referred to as LT cells in the current manuscript).

      (6) The telomere length and the telomerase activity are not very consistent (Figure 2A, and S1A, Figure 4B and S3). Please comment.

      In this study, we employed both telomerase-dependent and independent methods for telomere elongation.

      HT1080 model: Telomere elongation resulted from constitutive overexpression of hTERC and hTERT, leading to a direct correlation with telomerase activity.

      HCT116 (p53-null) model: hTERC silencing in ST cells, a known limiting factor for telomerase activity, resulted in significantly lower telomerase activity and a 1.5-fold telomere length difference.

      MDAMB231 model: Guanine-rich telomeric repeat (GTR) feeding induced telomere elongation through recombinatorial mechanisms (Wright et al., 1996), leading to significant telomere length gain but no notable change in telomerase activity.

      HCT116 Cas9-telomeric sgRNA model: Telomere shortening occurred without modifying telomerase components, resulting in a minor, insignificant increase in telomerase activity (Figure 2A, S1).

      Regarding xenograft-derived HT1080 ST and LT cells (Figure 4B, S3), the observed variability in telomere length and telomerase activity may stem from infiltrating mouse cells, which naturally have longer telomeres and higher telomerase activity than human cells. Since in the reported assay tumour masses were not sorted to exclude mouse cells, using species-specific markers or fluorescently labelled HT1080 cells in future experiments would minimize bias. However, even though telomere length and telomerase activity assays cannot differentiate for cross-species differences, mRNA analysis and ChIP experiments performed specifically for hTERT and hTERC mRNA levels, TRF2 occupancy, and H3K27me3 enrichment on hTERT promoter (Figure 4B–E) strongly support our conclusions.

      (7) Please comment on the other telomere-associated proteins or regulatory pathways that might contribute to hTERT expression based on telomere length.

      The current study provides experimental evidence that TRF2, a well-characterized telomere-binding protein, mediates crosstalk between telomeres and the regulatory region of the hTERT gene in a telomere length-dependent manner. Given the observed link between hTERT expression and telomere length, it is likely that additional telomere-associated proteins and regulatory pathways contribute to this regulation.

      The remaining shelterin complex components—POT1, hRap1, TRF1, TIN2, and TPP1—may play crucial roles in this context, as they are integral to telomere maintenance and protection (Stewart J et al., 2012 Mutat Res.). Additionally, several DNA damage response (DDR) proteins, which interact with telomere-binding factors and help preserve telomere integrity, could potentially influence hTERT regulation in a telomere length-dependent manner (Longhese M, 2008 Genes & Development). However, direct interactions or regulatory roles would require further experimental validation. Another group of proteins with potential relevance in this mechanism are the sirtuins, which directly associate with telomeres and are known to positively regulate telomere length, undergoing repression upon telomere shortening (Amano H et al., 2019 Cell Metabolism, Amano H, Sahin E 2019 Molecular & Cellular Oncology). Notably, SIRT1 has been reported to interact with telomerase (Lee SE et al., 2024, Biochem Biophys Res Commun.), while SIRT6 has been implicated in TRF2 degradation (Rizzo et al. 2017) and telomerase activation (Chen J et al. 2021, Aging) . Given their roles in telomere homeostasis, sirtuins may serve as key mediators of telomere length-dependent hTERT regulation.

      Based on this suggestion, we have included the above in Discussion.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Telomeres are key genomic structures linked to everything from aging to cancer. These key structures at the end of chromosomes protect them from degradation during replication and rely on a complex made up of human telomerase RNA gene (hTERC) and human telomerase reverse transcriptase (hTERT). While hTERC is expressed in all cells, the amount of hTERT is tightly controlled. The main hypothesis being tested is whether telomere length itself could regulate the hTERT enzyme. The authors conducted several experiments with different methods to alter telomere length and measured the binding of key regulatory proteins to this gene. It was generally observed that the shortening of telomere length leads to the recruitment of factors that reduce hTERT expression and lengthening of telomeres has the opposite effect. To rule out direct chromatin looping between telomeres and hTERT as driving this effect artificial constructs were designed and inserted a significant distance away and similar results were obtained.

      Overall, the claims of telomere length-dependent regulation of hTERT are supported throughout the manuscript.

      Strengths:

      The paper has several important strengths. Firstly, it uses several methods and cell lines that consistently demonstrate the same directionality of the findings. Secondly, it builds on established findings in the field but still demonstrates how this mechanism is separate from that which has been observed. Specifically, designing and implementing luciferase assays in the CCR5 locus supports that direct chromatin looping isn't necessary to drive this effect with TRF2 binding. Another strength of this paper is that it has been built on a variety of other studies that have established principles such as G4-DNA in the hTERT locus and TRF2 binding to these G4 sites.

      Weaknesses:

      The largest technical weakness of the paper is that minimal replicates are used for each experiment. I understand that these kinds of experiments are quite costly, and many of the effects are quite large, however, experiments such as the flow cytometry or the IPSC telomere length and activity assays appear to be based on a single sample, and several are based upon two maximum three biological replicates. If samples were added the main effects would likely hold, and many of the assays using GAPDH as a control would result in significant differences between the groups. This unnecessarily weakens the strength of the claims.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s recognition of the resource-intensive nature of our experiments, and we are confident in the robustness of the observed results. Due to the project’s timeline constraints and the need for consistency across experiments, we have reported findings based on 3 biological replicates with appropriate statistical analysis.

      Regarding the fibroblast-iPSC model, we would like to clarify that we have presented data from two independent biological replicates, each consisting of a fibroblast and its derived iPS cell pair, rather than a single sample. Additionally, the Tel-FACS assays involved analysing at least 10,000 events, ensuring statistical significance in all cases.

      Another detail that weakens the confidence in the claims is that throughout the manuscript there are several examples of the control group with zero variance between any of the samples: e.g. Figure 2K, Figure 3N, and Figure 6G. It is my understanding that a delta delta method has been used for calculation (though no exact formula is reported and would assist in understanding). If this is the case, then an average of the control group would be used to calculate that fold change and variance would exist in the group. The only way I could understand those control group samples always set to 1 is if a tube of cells was divided into conditions and therefore normalized to the control group in each case. A clearer description in the figure legend and methods would be required if this is what was done and repeated measures ANOVA and other statistics should accompany this.

      The above point has been raised by the reviewer in the 'Recommendations for Authors' section as well. We have addressed it in detail in that section, citing each figure where the reviewer noted a concern regarding the lack of variance. Changes made in the manuscript have also been highlighted there.

      We would like to clarify that, throughout the manuscript, fold changes were previously calculated independently for each biological replicate by normalizing treated conditions to their corresponding control (untreated or Day 0) sample within the same replicate. This means that the control group is normalized to 1 individually in each replicate, resulting in an apparent lack of variance in the control when plotted. The normalization was not performed using an averaged control value across replicates. As such, the absence of visible variance in the control group reflects the normalization method rather than a true lack of variability in the underlying data.

      In the revised version of the manuscript, we have carefully considered the reviewer’s comments and applied changes wherever appropriate. For example (detailed response in the ‘Recommendations for Authors’ section), in datasets where two distinct stable cell lines are compared (e.g., HT1080 ST/LT and HCT p53-null ST/LT), unpaired statistical analysis is more appropriate. Hence, we have updated these panels accordingly and indicated the statistical methods used in the figure legends and Methods section. However, in experiments where cells were indeed seeded separately and subsequently subjected to experimental conditions—representing paired samples—we have chosen not to make any changes. A clearer description of this procedure has, however, been added to the Methods and figure legends to ensure full transparency.

      We believe this approach accurately reflects the experimental design, appropriately addresses the reviewer’s concerns regarding variance and statistical analysis, and ensures clarity and rigor in data reporting.

      A final technical weakness of the paper is the data in Figure 5 where the modified hTERT promoter was inserted upstream of the luciferase gene. Specifically, it is unclear why data was not directly compared between the constructs that could and could not form G4s to make this point. For this reason, the large variance in several samples, and minimal biological replicates, this data was the least convincing in the manuscript (though other papers from this laboratory and others support the claim, it is not convincing standalone data).

      We appreciate the reviewer's thoughtful feedback on the presentation of the luciferase assay data in Figure 5. The data for the wild-type hTERT promoter (capable of forming G4 structures) was previously reported in Figure 2G-K. To avoid redundancy in data presentation, we initially chose to report the results of the mutated promoter separately. However, we recognize that directly comparing the wild-type and mutated promoter constructs within the same figure would provide clearer context and strengthen the interpretation of the results. In light of this, we have updated Figure 5 in the revised manuscript to include the data for both constructs, ensuring a more comprehensive and informative comparison.

      The second largest weakness of the paper is formatting.

      When I initially read the paper without a careful reading of the methods, I thought that the authors did not have appropriate controls meaning that if a method is applied to lengthen, there should be one that is not lengthened, and when a method is applied to shorten, one which is not shortened should be analysed as well. In fact, this is what the authors have done with isogenic controls. However, by describing all samples as either telomere short or telomere long, while this simplifies the writing and the colour scheme, it makes it less clear that each experiment is performed relative to an unmodified. I would suggest putting the isogenic control in one colour, the artificially shortened in another, and the artificially lengthened in another.

      Similarly, the graphs, in general, should be consistent with labelling. Figure 2 was the most confusing. I would suggest one dotted line with cell lines above it, and then the method of either elongation or shortening below it. I.e. HT1080 above, hTERC overexpression below, MDAMB-231 above guanine terminal repeats below, like was done on the right. Figure 2 readability would also be improved by putting hTERT promoter GAPDH (-ve control) under each graph that uses this (Panel B and Panel C not just Panel C). All information is contained in the manuscript but one must currently flip between figure legends, methods, and figures to understand what was done and this reduces clarity for the reader.

      We thank the reviewer again for their thoughtful suggestions regarding figure formatting and colour coding to improve clarity. We fully understand the rationale for proposing separate colours for unmodified, telomere-shortened, and telomere-lengthened groups, as this could make the experimental design more immediately apparent. However, after careful consideration, we believe that implementing this change across all figures may unintentionally reduce clarity in other aspects  (presented in other figures) of the data presentation. This is further explained below.

      Specifically, applying three distinct colours throughout would make it harder to visually track key biological trends—such as changes in chromatin occupancy—across different models. For instance, the same colour could represent opposing regulatory patterns in distinct contexts (e.g., upregulation in one model and downregulation in another), which will make these figures difficult to understand. We feel that maintaining a consistent colour scheme based on telomere status—i.e., long telomeres (LT) vs short telomeres (ST)—across figures facilitates better comparison of biological outcomes across different experimental systems.

      Nevertheless, to address the reviewer’s concern about clarity in experimental design, we have added more detailed descriptions of the methodology and model systems used, in both the Methods and figure legend sections. These updates aim to make it easier for the reader to follow which groups serve as isogenic controls versus modified samples, without disrupting the consistency of data visualization.

      We hope this strikes a balance between improving clarity and preserving the interpretability of the broader biological trends presented in our manuscript.

      Please note, we have incorporated the reviewer’s suggestion to indicate details of model generation for HT1080 and MDAMB 231 cell lines in Figure 2. To quote the reviewer,  

      “I would suggest one dotted line with cell lines above it, and then the method of either elongation or shortening below it. I.e. HT1080 above, hTERC overexpression below, MDAMB-231 above guanine terminal repeats below, like was done on the right.”

      We have also put hTERT promoter GAPDH (-ve control) under each graph and not at the end of Panel C in Figure 2, as suggested by reviewer.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Please check for grammatical errors throughout the manuscript.

      We have gone through the manuscript thoroughly, checked and corrected it for grammatical errors if and where detected.

      (2) Please use both the FACS and qPCR-based assays to check telomere length in all the experiments to strengthen the observations.

      We would like to thank the reviewer for this valuable suggestion. We confirm that both FACS- and qPCR-based assays were performed to assess telomere length in our experiments. In the original submission, we chose to present primarily the FACS-based data in the main figures. This decision was based on the inherent differences in the measurement principles of the two methods, which can lead to discrepancies in the reported fold changes. We were concerned that presenting both datasets side by side in the main figures might lead to confusion for readers who are not directly familiar with the nuances of telomere length assays.

      However, in light of the reviewer’s suggestion, we have now included the qPCR-based data as Supplementary Figure 1A, and updated the manuscript text and figure legends accordingly to reflect this addition.

      (3) Correct the labeling in the legend (Figure 2).

      We have corrected legend of Figure 2. Thanks to the reviewer for pointing it out.

      (4) In Figure 6B, why TRF WT condition have higher hTERT expression than the UT condition?

      We thank the reviewer for noting that the hTERT mRNA levels, as estimated by FISH in Figure 6B, appear slightly higher in TRF2 WT overexpressing HT1080 cells compared to the untransfected (UT) condition. Specifically, the average mean intensity values (a.u.) were 53 for UT and 57 for WT. Although this difference was not statistically significant, we acknowledge the reviewer's observation. Currently, we do not have a clear explanation for this small, non-significant variation.

      Importantly, using the same FISH-based method, we observed a significant upregulation of hTERT mRNA levels upon TRF2 R17H overexpression compared to both UT and TRF2 WT conditions, supporting our key conclusions.

      Additionally, qRT-PCR analysis of hTERT mRNA levels in cells stably expressing TRF2 WT (induced by doxycycline) consistently showed a significant downregulation compared to the uninduced (equivalent to UT in the microscopy experiments) state. These results were robust and reproducible across three different cell lines, including HT1080. Consistently, TRF2 R17H expression led to significant upregulation of hTERT mRNA levels upon induction.

      Together, these complementary findings strengthen the validity of our observations.

      (5) In telomere length between ST and LT in Fig. 5B significant? (especially the right panel -146G>A).

      We consistently worked with approximately 20–30% telomere shortening in HEK293 cells across all three cell types (WT promoter, -124G>A, and -146G>A), as this range was reproducibly achieved within the experimental timeframe without risking excessive telomere trimming. The reported telomere length differences are based on FACS analysis of more than 10,000 events per condition, providing strong statistical significance. Importantly, while the absolute differences in telomere length may appear modest, their biological impact is evident in the distinct cellular characteristics observed between ST and LT cell pairs.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      As mentioned above it was somewhat unclear why so many instances of control groups had no variance between them. A more complete reporting of the formulas used to calculate the results, and methods (if samples were divided from a single source into different conditions) would be appreciated.

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable and detailed feedback. The instances where the control groups appeared to lack variance were mainly mRNA data (Figure 2D, 3G,3N), luciferase activity (Figure 2K), and in vitro methyltransferase activity (Figure 6G). We shall try to categorically address them all. 

      In Figure 2D, for the MDA-MB-231  GTR oligo and HCT116 telomere trimming datasets, the untreated cells were seeded separately and subsequently used to generate the treated conditions within the same experiment. Thus, these two datasets represent paired experimental conditions. Fold changes were calculated independently for each replicate (paired samples), and the fold changes across replicates were plotted. Because the control group serves as a common baseline within each pair and fold changes are normalized individually, minimal variance appears across controls. Given the experimental design, we believe no change is necessary for these panels. However, we have provided additional clarification regarding the calculation formulas and sample handling in the Methods section to avoid any ambiguity.

      For the ST/LT versions in HT1080 and HCT p53-null background cells, while each replicate could technically be treated as paired, these could be treated as four distinct stable cell lines. Hence, we agree it would be appropriate to apply unpaired statistical analysis for these datasets. We have updated the plots accordingly and described the statistical methods in detail in the figure legends and Methods section.

      Figure 3G and 3N depict the doxycycline-induced cells which follow the design: untreated and dox-treated conditions were seeded from the same batch of cells into separate flasks and treated differently. Hence, these are also paired cases, and fold changes were calculated per replicate before plotting. Therefore, we believe no changes are necessary for these panels. However, we have provided more details regarding sample handling in the Methods section to avoid any ambiguity.

      In Figure 2K, previously we had plotted fold change in luciferase activity over short telomere (ST) cells, for each independent biological replicates. However, to address the reviewer’s concern of not showing variance in control group, we have now plotted the luminescence signal (normalised over total protein). We have also updated Figure 5E accordingly, and also included WT promoter data along with the mutant cell line data- as was suggested in public reviewer’s comment.

      In Figure 6G, as each replicate of the in vitro methyltransferase activity used different batches of purified protein, there are inherent batch differences that were accounted for by normalizing each replicate internally. Fold changes were then determined for each replicate separately, as previously described. The fold changes across replicates were plotted, and significance between different conditions was tested using two-way ANOVA. To address the reviewer’s comment to show variance in the control, we have now plotted individual replicates.

      We believe these revisions, along with the expanded methods clarification, will fully address the reviewer's concerns and accurately reflect the experimental design and statistical analysis applied.

      Many times, in the manuscript a / is used to indicate both directions. For example: "Genes distal from telomeres (for instance 60 Mb from the nearest telomere) were activated/repressed in a TL-dependent way"... "Resulting increase/decrease in non-telomeric promoter-bound TRF2 affected gene expression". For readability, either this can be replaced with a directionless word like altered, changed, etc, or the writer can list both directions.

      We thank the reviewer for the careful reading and thoughtful suggestions. In the manuscript, we have used the ‘/’ symbol to indicate opposing directions, followed by the word ‘respectively’ to relate these directions to their corresponding outcomes, wherever appropriate. However, as rightly pointed out, certain sentences would benefit from alternative constructions for improved clarity and readability. We have therefore reviewed the manuscript and revised such sentences, making minor modifications wherever necessary, as outlined below.

      We found hTERT was transcriptionally altered depending on telomere length (TL).

      Notably, another conceptually distinct mechanism of TL-dependent gene regulation was reported which influenced genes spread throughout the genome: expression of genes distal from telomeres (for instance 60 Mb from the nearest telomere) was altered in a TL-dependent way, but without physical telomere looping interactions.

      Second, the shortening or elongation of telomeres led to the release or sequestration of telomeric TRF2, respectively, thereby increasing or decreasing the availability of TRF2 at non-telomeric promoters and affecting gene expression.

      A non-necessary, but potentially extra convincing experiment to perform would be to use a combination of light-activated, or ligand-activated cas9 telomere trimming and guanine terminal repeat additions in the same cell line. Like the dox experiments, this would show over time how altering telomere length alters the recruitment of heterochromatin factors and hTERT levels. Executing the experiment this way would be more definitive as it does not rely on changing hTERT itself. Authors do already have examples that support their claims.

      We thank the reviewer for suggesting this additional experiment (reviewer mentions as non-necessary), which would indeed provide valuable insights into the relationship between telomere length, heterochromatin factor recruitment, and hTERT levels. While we recognize the potential of this approach, due to constraints on resources, we are currently unable to execute this experiment. However, we believe that the existing data presented in the manuscript already supports our conclusions effectively.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, the authors showed that enalapril was able to reduce cellular senescence and improve health status in aged mice. The authors further showed that phosphorylated Smad1/5/9 was significantly elevated and blocking this pathway attenuated the protection of cells from senescence. When middle-aged mice were treated with enalapril, the physiological performance in several tissues, including memory capacity, renal function, and muscle strength, exhibited significant improvement.

      Strengths:

      The strength of the study lies in the identification of the pSMAD1/5/9 pathway as the underlying mechanism mediating the anti-senescence effects of enalapril with comprehensive evaluation both in vitro and in vivo.

      Thank you for your patient reading and great efforts to advance our research! Your comments are shown in bold font below, and specific concerns have been numbered. Our point-by-point answers are provided in standard blue font, with all modifications and additions to the MS highlighted in red text.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The major weakness of the study is the in vivo data. Despite the evidence shown in the in vitro study, there is no data to show that blocking the pSmad1/5/9 pathway is able to attenuate the anti-aging effects of enalapril in the mice. In addition, the aging phenotypes mitigation by enalapril is not evidenced by the extension of lifespan.

      Many thanks for your careful reading and valuable comments! We fully agree with this comment. In accordance with your suggestion, we administered LDN193189 to investigate its suppressive effects on pSmad1/5/9 signaling in vivo. Notably, pharmacological inhibition of pSmad1/5/9 resulted in upregulation of enalapril-suppressed SASP factors, while conversely leading to marked decrease of downstream antioxidant genes expression across multiple organ systems (Revised Fig. S7). These analyses and corresponding sentences have been added in the Result section of the revised MS (Revised Fig.S7, Lines 222–223, 444–448).

      Additionally, aging-related behavioral phenotypes were also examined following pSmad1/5/9 inhibition, including decreased muscle strength and endurance, impaired spatial memory and increased anxiety behaviors (Revised Fig. S8). These analyses and corresponding sentences have been added in the Result section of the revised MS (Revised Fig.S8, Lines 476–480). Collectively, these findings demonstrate that the anti-aging effects of enalapril in mice are mediated through the pSmad1/5/9 pathway.

      In this study, we focused exclusively on assessing the improvement in the health status of aged mice, which indicates that enalapril can extend the healthspan of aged mice. While we agree that lifespan extension is an important indicator of anti-aging potential, recent studies have emphasized that healthspan, rather than lifespan alone, provides a more relevant and translational measure of aging interventions, particularly in the context of chronic disease and quality of life in aged individuals (Kennedy et al., 2014; Lopez-Otin et al., 2023). Moreover, given the strong influence of genetic background, environmental factors and stochastic events on lifespan, focusing on functional rejuvenation and delayed onset of aging-related pathologies may offer a more practical and mechanistically informative approach. Our study aims to elucidate how enalapril enhances healthy phenotypes in aged mice, however, we acknowledge the critical need for direct lifespan evaluation and intend to address this limitation in subsequent research. We sincerely hope that these explanations address your concerns.

      (2) If it is necessary to show that NAC is able to attenuate enalapril effects in the aging mice. In addition, it would be beneficial to test if enalapril is able to achieve similar rescue in a premature aging mouse model.

      Thanks for your suggestion. We apologize for any confusion that may have arisen due to the wording in the original manuscript. N-acetylcysteine (NAC) is widely reported as an antioxidant that scavenges reactive oxygen species (ROS) (Huang et al., 2020; Zafarullah et al., 2003). In our study, enalapril was also observed to reduce ROS levels. Therefore, NAC is unlikely to antagonize the effects of enalapril in this context, as both compounds act in a similar direction with respect to oxidative stress mitigation. To avoid potential misunderstanding, we have carefully reviewed the relevant statements in the MS and revised the text to clarify this point.

      We sincerely appreciate this valuable suggestion to evaluate enalapril in a premature aging mouse model; however, the premature aging mouse models represent a pathological form of aging, whereas the naturally aged mouse models used in our study reflect physiological aging processes. While we observed beneficial effects of enalapril in naturally aged mice, these effects may not necessarily extend to premature aging models due to fundamental differences in the underlying mechanisms and progression of aging. Natural aging is characterized by the gradual accumulation of cellular damage, driven by multifactorial processes such as inflammaging and mitochondrial dysfunction. In this context, enalapril appears effective, in part by modulating SASP factors and reducing oxidative stress through the BMP-Smad signaling axis (Revised Fig. 4, 5) (Lopez-Otin et al., 2023). In contrast, premature aging models are driven by distinct mechanisms like nuclear lamina defects, which may not respond similarly to BMP-Smad axis. Moreover, genetic background, strain variability, and specific model characteristics can significantly influence treatment outcomes (Mitchell et al., 2016). For instance, rapamycin extends lifespan in wild-type mice but shows limited effects on aging, underscoring the challenge of extrapolating findings across distinct aging models (Neff et al., 2013). We sincerely hope that these explanations address your concerns. Thank you again for your great efforts in advancing our research!

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      This manuscript presents an interesting study of enalapril for its potential impact on senescence through the activation of Smad1/5/9 signaling with a focus on antioxidative gene expression. Repurposing enalapril in this context provides a fresh perspective on its effects beyond blood pressure regulation. The authors make a strong case for the importance of Smad1/5/9 in this process, and the inclusion of both in vitro and in vivo models adds value to the findings. Below, I have a few comments and suggestions which may help improve the manuscript.

      We appreciate your great efforts in advancing our research! Your comments are shown in bold font below, and specific concerns have been numbered. Our point-by-point answers are provided in standard blue font, with all modifications and additions to the MS highlighted in red text.

      (1) A major finding in the study is that phosphorylated Smad1/5/9 mediates the effects of enalapril. However, the manuscript focused on the Smad pathway relatively abruptly, and the rationale behind targeting this specific pathway is not fully explained. What makes Smad1/5/9 particularly relevant to the context of this study?

      Thank you for your informative guidance, and we regret for the unclear description. As stated in the MS, after we found that enalapril could improve the cellular senescence phenotype, we screened and examined key targets in important aging-related signaling pathways, such as AKT, mTOR, ERK, Smad2/3 and Smad1/5/9 (Revised Fig. S2A, Revised Fig. 2A). We found that only the phosphorylation levels of Smad1/5/9 significantly increased after enalapril treatment. Therefore, the subsequent focus of this study is on pSmad1/5/9. We sincerely hope that these explanations address your concerns.

      (2) Furthermore, their finding that activation of Smad1/5/9 leads to a reduction of senescence appears somewhat contradictory to the established literature on Smad1/5/9 in senescence. For instance, studies have shown that BMP4-induced senescence involves the activation of Smad1/5/8 (Smad1/5/9), leading to the upregulation of senescence markers like p16 and p21 (JBC, 2009, 284, 12153). Similarly, phosphorylated Smad1/5/8 has been shown to promote and maintain senescence in Ras-activated cells (PLOS Genetics, 2011, 7, e1002359). Could the authors provide more detailed mechanistic insights into why enalapril seems to reverse the typical pro-senescent role of Smad1/5/9 in their study?

      Many thanks for your helpful comments! The downstream regulatory network of BMP-pSmad1/5/9 is highly complex. The BMP-SMAD-ID axis has been mentioned in many studies, and its downstream signaling inhibits the expression of p16 and p21 (Hayashi et al., 2016; Ying et al., 2003). Additionally, studies have also found that the Smad1-Stat1-P21 axis inhibits osteoblast senescence (Xu et al., 2022). In our study, enalapril was found to increase the expression of ID1, which is a classic downstream target of pSmad1/5/9 (Genander et al., 2014). Therefore, pSmad1/5/9 inhibits cellular senescence markers such as p16, p21 and SASP through ID1, thereby promoting cell proliferation (Revised Fig. 3). Furthermore, we also found that pSmad1/5/9 increases the expression of antioxidant genes and reduces ROS levels, exerting antioxidant effects (Revised Fig. 4). Together, ID1 and antioxidant genes enable pSmad1/5/9 to exert its anti-senescence effects. We sincerely hope that these explanations address your concerns.

      (3) While the authors showed that enalapril increases pSmad1/5/9 phosphorylation, what are the expression levels of other key and related factors like Smad4, pSmad2, pSmad3, BMP2, and BMP4 in both senescent and non-senescent cells? These data will help clarify the broader signaling effects.

      Thanks for your insightful suggestions. We observed an increase in pSmad1/5/9 and Smad4 expression, while the levels of pSmad2 and pSmad3 remained unchanged after enalapril treatment (Revised Fig. 2A). Consistently, we found that the levels of pSmad1/5/9 and Smad4 were markedly reduced in senescent cells, aligning with the upregulation of these proteins by enalapril (Revised Fig. S2B). In contrast, pSmad2 and pSmad3 showed a slight increase during senescence, while BMP2 and BMP4 were slightly decreased, though these changes were not statistically significant (Revised Fig. S2B). These findings suggest that enalapril primarily exerts its effects by enhancing pSmad1/5/9 and Smad4 levels, thereby regulating downstream target genes and contributing to the restoration of a more youthful cellular state. These analyses and corresponding sentences have been added in the Result section of the revised MS (Revised Fig.S2B, Lines 303–306, 311–313).

      (4) They used BMP receptor inhibitor LDN193189 to pharmacologically inhibit BMP signaling, but it would be more convincing to also include genetic validation (e.g., knockdown or knockout of BMP2 or BMP4). This will help confirm that the observed effects are truly due to BMP-Smad signaling and not off-target effects of the pharmacological inhibitor LDN.

      Many thanks for your careful reading and valuable comments! We used shRNA to knockdown the BMP receptor BMPR1A, which led to a reduction in Smad1/5/9 phosphorylation (Revised Fig. S4D, E). This was accompanied by senescence-associated phenotypes, including increased expression of p16 and SA-β-gal and decreased Ki67 staining (Revised Fig. S4F, G). Notably, the addition of enalapril failed to reverse these senescence phenotypes under BMPR1A knockdown conditions, mirroring the results observed with the BMP receptor inhibitor LDN193189 (Revised Fig. S4F, G, Revised Fig. 2F, G). Furthermore, knockdown of BMPR1A also resulted in a marked decrease in the expression of downstream targets, such as ID1 and antioxidative genes (Revised Fig. S4D). These findings strongly support the notion that enalapril exerts its anti-senescence effects through BMP-Smad signaling. These analyses and corresponding sentences have been added in the Result section of the revised MS (Revised Fig.S4D–G, Lines 323–329, 335–337, 348–351, 416–418).

      (5) I don't see the results on the changes in senescence markers p16 and p21 in the mouse models treated with enalapril. Similarly, the effects of enalapril treatment on some key SASP factors, such as TNF-α, MCP-1, IL-1β, and IL-1α, are missing, particularly in serum and tissues. These are important data to evaluate the effect of enalapril on senescence.

      Thanks for your comments. As for the markers p16 and p21, we observed no change in p16, while the changes in p21 varied across different organs and tissues. Nevertheless, behavioral experiments and physiological and biochemical indicators at the individual level consistently demonstrated the significant anti-aging effects of enalapril (Revised Fig. 6).

      We also examined the changes in SASP factors in the serum of mice after enalapril treatment. Notably, SASP factors such as CCL (MCP), CXCL and TNFRS11B showed significant decreases (Revised Fig. 5C). The expression changes of SASP factors varied across different organs. In the liver, kidneys and spleen, the expression of IL1a and IL1b decreased, while TNFRS11B expression decreased in both the liver and muscles (Revised Fig. 5B). Additionally, CCL (MCP) levels decreased in all organs (Revised Fig. 5B). We sincerely hope that these explanations address your concerns.

      (6) Given that enalapril is primarily known as an antihypertensive, it would be helpful to include data on how it affects blood pressure in the aged mouse models, such as systolic and diastolic blood pressure. This will clarify whether the observed effects are independent of or influenced by changes in blood pressure.

      Thanks for your comments. While enalapril is primarily recognized for its antihypertensive properties, in our experimental setting involving aged, normotensive mice, we did not observe notable changes in systolic or diastolic blood pressure following enalapril administration. This observation aligns with previous reports indicating that enalapril does not significantly affect blood pressure in similar non-hypertensive aging models (Keller et al., 2019). Based on these findings, we cautiously interpret that the beneficial effects of enalapril observed in our study are unlikely to be driven by changes in blood pressure. We sincerely hope that these explanations address your concerns. Again, thank you for the constructive comments to advance the understanding of our work!

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      This is an interesting study that reveals enalapril is able to elevate the pSmad1/5/9 pathway to reduce ROS and inflammation to improve the health status in vitro and in vivo. While the pathway is clearly shown in cells to be involved in the enalarpril-mediated mitigation of aging, little was done to demonstrate this pathway is responsible for the in vivo effects in the physiological improvements. This can be done by ROS-reduction chemicals such as NAC and also the use of BMP receptor inhibitor LDN193189 (LDN). It is critical to show the lifespan extension in enalapril-treated animals given that the significantly improved physiological functions.

      Thanks very much for your constructive recommendations. This part has already been addressed in our response to the public review.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The term "anti-aging" appears frequently throughout the manuscript, including in the title. However, the study doesn't directly address lifespan or a comprehensive range of aging symptoms, which are also difficult to define and measure. Many of the observed effects appeared to be driven by senescence. To be more accurate, I recommend avoiding terms like "anti-aging" and "mitigates aging", and instead replacing them with more specific phrases such as "anti-senescence", "senescence reduction/suppression", or "mitigates age-related symptoms" to better reflect the scope of the study and avoid overstating the findings.

      Thanks very much for your constructive recommendations. In accordance with your suggestion, we have revised all uses of the term “aging” in the MS. To facilitate review, all changes have been clearly marked in red text.

      Please provide detailed information on the antibodies used, particularly those targeting pSmad1/5/9 and other Smads.

      Thanks for your helpful comment. In response, we have now provided detailed information regarding the antibodies used in this study in Revised Table S4 (Revised MS, Page 120–121).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This fundamental study identifies a new mechanism that involves a mycobacterial nucleomodulin manipulation of the host histone methyltransferase COMPASS complex to promote infection. Although other intracellular pathogens are known to manipulate histone methylation, this is the first report demonstrating the specific targeting of the COMPASS complex by a pathogen. The rigorous experimental design using state-of-the art bioinformatic analysis, protein modeling, molecular and cellular interaction, and functional approaches, culminating with in vivo infection modeling, provides convincing, unequivocal evidence that supports the authors' claims. This work will be of particular interest to cellular microbiologists working on microbial virulence mechanisms and effectors, specifically nucleomodulins, and cell/cancer biologists that examine COMPASS dysfunction in cancer biology. 

      Strengths: 

      (1) The strengths of this study include the rigorous and comprehensive experimental design that involved numerous state-of-the-art approaches to identify potential nucleomodulins, define molecular nucleomodulin-host interactions, cellular nucleomodulin localization, intracellular survival, and inflammatory gene transcriptional responses, and confirmation of the inflammatory and infection phenotype in a small animal model. 

      (2) The use of bioinformatic, cellular, and in vivo modeling that are consistent and support the overall conclusions is a strength of the study. In addition, the rigorous experimental design and data analysis, including the supplemental data provided, further strengthen the evidence supporting the conclusions. 

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) This work could be stronger if the MgdE-COMPASS subunit interactions that negatively impact COMPASS complex function were better defined. Since the COMPASS complex consists of many enzymes, examining the functional impact on each of the components would be interesting. 

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. A biochemistry assays could be helpful to interpret the functional impact on each of the components by MgdE interaction. However, the purification of the COMPASS complex could be a hard task itself due to the complexity of the full COMPASS complex along with its dynamic structural properties and limited solubility. 

      (2) Examining the impact of WDR5 inhibitors on histone methylation, gene transcription, and mycobacterial infection could provide additional rigor and provide useful information related to the mechanisms and specific role of WDR5 inhibition on mycobacterial infection. 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. A previous study showed that WIN-site inhibitors, such as compound C6, can displace WDR5 from chromatin, leading to a reduction in global H3K4me3 levels and suppression of immune-related gene expression (Hung et al., Nucleic Acids Res, 2018; Bryan et al., Nucleic Acids Res, 2020). These results closely mirror the functional effects we observed for MgdE, suggesting that MgdE may act as a functional mimic of WDR5 inhibition. This supports our proposed model in which MgdE disrupts COMPASS activity by targeting WDR5, thereby dampening host pro-inflammatory responses.

      (3) The interaction between MgdE and COMPASS complex subunit ASH2L is relatively undefined, and studies to understand the relationship between WDR5 and ASH2L in COMPASS complex function during infection could provide interesting molecular details that are undefined in this study. 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. In this study, we constructed single and multiple point mutants of MgdE at residues S<sup>80</sup>, D<sup>244</sup>, and H<sup>247</sup> to identify key amino acids involved in its interaction with ASH2L (Figure 5A and B; Figure S5). However these mutations did not interrupt the interaction with MgdE, suggesting that more residues are involved in the interaction.

      ASH2L and WDR5 function cooperatively within the WRAD module to stabilize the SET domain and promote H3K4 methyltransferase activity with physiological conditions (Couture and Skiniotis, Epigenetics, 2013; Qu et al., Cell, 2018; Rahman et al., Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 2022). ASH2L interacts with RbBP5 via its SPRY domain, whereas WDR5 bridges MLL1 and RbBP5 through the WIN and WBM motifs (Chen at al., Cell Res, 2012; Park et al., Nat Commun, 2019). The interaction status between ASH2L and WDR5 during mycobacterial infection could not be determined in our current study. 

      (4) The AlphaFold prediction results for all the nuclear proteins examined could be useful. Since the interaction predictions with COMPASS subunits range from 0.77 for WDR5 and 0.47 for ASH2L, it is not clear how the focus on COMPASS complex over other nuclear proteins was determined.  

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We employed AlphaFold to predict the interactions between MgdE and the major nuclear proteins. This screen identified several subunits of the SET1/COMPASS complex as high-confidence candidates for interaction with MgdE (Supplementary Figure 4A). This result is consistent with a proteomic study by Penn et al. which reported potential interactions between MgdE and components of the human SET1/COMPASS complex based on affinity purification-mass spectrometry analysis (Penn et al., Mol Cell, 2018).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The manuscript by Chen et al addresses an important aspect of pathogenesis for mycobacterial pathogens, seeking to understand how bacterial effector proteins disrupt the host immune response. To address this question, the authors sought to identify bacterial effectors from M. tuberculosis (Mtb) that localize to the host nucleus and disrupt host gene expression as a means of impairing host immune function. 

      Strengths: 

      The researchers conducted a rigorous bioinformatic analysis to identify secreted effectors containing mammalian nuclear localization signal (NLS) sequences, which formed the basis of quantitative microscopy analysis to identify bacterial proteins that had nuclear targeting within human cells. The study used two complementary methods to detect protein-protein interaction: yeast two-hybrid assays and reciprocal immunoprecipitation (IP). The combined use of these techniques provides strong evidence of interactions between MgdE and SET1 components and suggests that the interactions are, in fact, direct. The authors also carried out a rigorous analysis of changes in gene expression in macrophages infected with the mgdE mutant BCG. They found strong and consistent effects on key cytokines such as IL6 and CSF1/2, suggesting that nuclear-localized MgdE does, in fact, alter gene expression during infection of macrophages. 

      Weaknesses: 

      There are some drawbacks in this study that limit the application of the findings to M. tuberculosis (Mtb) pathogenesis. The first concern is that much of the study relies on ectopic overexpression of proteins either in transfected non-immune cells (HEK293T) or in yeast, using 2-hybrid approaches. Some of their data in 293T cells is hard to interpret, and it is unclear if the protein-protein interactions they identify occur during natural infection with mycobacteria. The second major concern is that pathogenesis is studied using the BCG vaccine strain rather than virulent Mtb. However, overall, the key findings of the paper - that MgdE interacts with SET1 and alters gene expression are well-supported. 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We agree that the ectopic overexpression could not completely reflect a natural status, although these approaches were adopted in many similar experiments (Drerup et al., Molecular plant, 2013; Chen et al., Cell host & microbe, 2018; Ge et al., Autophagy, 2021). Further, the MgdE localization experiment using Mtb infected macrophages will be performed to increase the evidence in the natural infection.

      We agree with the reviewer that BCG strain could not fully recapitulate the pathogenicity or immunological complexity of M. tuberculosis infection.  We employed BCG as a biosafe surrogate model since it was acceptable in many related studies (Wang et al., Nat Immunol, 2025; Wang et al., Nat Commun, 2017; Péan et al., Nat Commun, 2017; Li et al., J Biol Chem, 2020). 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      In this study, Chen L et al. systematically analyzed the mycobacterial nucleomodulins and identified MgdE as a key nucleomodulin in pathogenesis. They found that MgdE enters into host cell nucleus through two nuclear localization signals, KRIR<sup>108-111</sup> and RLRRPR<sup>300-305</sup>, and then interacts with COMPASS complex subunits ASH2L and WDR5 to suppress H3K4 methylation-mediated transcription of pro-inflammatory cytokines, thereby promoting mycobacterial survival. This study is potentially interesting, but there are several critical issues that need to be addressed to support the conclusions of the manuscript.

      (1) Figure 2: The study identified MgdE as a nucleomodulin in mycobacteria and demonstrated its nuclear translocation via dual NLS motifs. The authors examined MgdE nuclear translocation through ectopic expression in HEK293T cells, which may not reflect physiological conditions. Nuclear-cytoplasmic fractionation experiments under mycobacterial infection should be performed to determine MgdE localization. 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. The MgdE localization experiment using Mtb infected macrophages will be performed.

      (2) Figure 2F: The authors detected MgdE-EGFP using an anti-GFP antibody, but EGFP as a control was

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. The new uncropped blots containing the EGFP band will be provided in Supplementary Information.

      (3) Figure 3C-3H: The data showing that the expression of all detected genes in 24 h is comparable to that in 4 h (but not 0 h) during WT BCG infection is beyond comprehension. The issue is also present in Figure 7C, Figure 7D, and Figure S7. Moreover, since Il6, Il1β (proinflammatory), and Il10 (anti-inflammatory) were all upregulated upon MgdE deletion, how do the authors explain the phenomenon that MgdE deletion simultaneously enhanced these gene expressions? 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. A relative quantification method was used in our qPCR experiments to normalize the WT expression levels in Figure 3C–3H, Figure 7C, 7D, and Figure S7. 

      The concurrent induction of both types of cytokines likely represents a dynamic host strategy to fine-tune immune responses during infection. This interpretation is supported by previous studies (Podleśny-Drabiniok et al., Cell Rep, 2025; Cicchese et al., Immunological Reviews, 2018).

      (4) Figure 5: The authors confirmed the interactions between MgdE and WDR5/ASH2L. How does the interaction between MgdE and WDR5 inhibit COMPASS-dependent methyltransferase activity? Additionally, the precise MgdE-ASH2L binding interface and its functional impact on COMPASS assembly or activity require clarification. 

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment. We cautiously speculate that the MgdE interaction inhibits COMPASS-dependent methyltransferase activity by interfering with the integrity and stability of the COMPASS complex. Accordingly, we have incorporated the following discussion into the revised manuscript (Lines 298-310):

      “The COMPASS complex facilitates H3K4 methylation through a conserved assembly mechanism involving multiple core subunits. WDR5, a central scaffolding component, interacts with RbBP5 and ASH2L to promote complex assembly and enzymatic activity (Qu et al., 2018; Wysocka et al., 2005). It also recognizes the WIN motif of methyltransferases such as MLL1, thereby anchoring them to the complex and stabilizing the ASH2L-RbBP5 dimer (Hsu et al., Cell, 2018). ASH2L further contributes to COMPASS activation by interacting with both RbBP5 and DPY30 and by stabilizing the SET domain, which is essential for efficient substrate recognition and catalysis (Qu et al., Cell, 2018; Park et al., Nat Commun, 2019). Our work shows that MgdE binds both WDR5 and ASH2L and inhibits the methyltransferase activity of the COMPASS complex. Site-directed mutagenesis revealed that residues D<sup>224</sup> and H<sup>247</sup> of MgdE are critical for WDR5 binding, as the double mutant MgdE-D<sup>224</sup>A/H<sup>247</sup> A fails to interact with WDR5 and shows diminished suppression of H3K4me3 levels (Figure 5D).”

      Regarding the precise MgdE-ASH2L binding interface, we attempted to identify the key interaction site by introducing point mutations into ASH2L. However, these mutations did not disrupt the interaction (Figure 5A and B; Figure S5), suggesting that more residues are involved in the interaction.

      (5) Figure 6: The authors proposed that the MgdE-regulated COMPASS complex-H3K4me3 axis suppresses pro-inflammatory responses, but the presented data do not sufficiently support this claim. H3K4me3 inhibitor should be employed to verify cytokine production during infection. 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We have now revised the description in lines 824825 “MgdE may suppresses COMPASS complex-mediated inflammatory responses by inhibiting H3K4 methylation” and in lines 219-220 "MgdE suppresses host inflammatory responses probably by inhibition of COMPASS complex-mediated H3K4 methylation." 

      (6) There appears to be a discrepancy between the results shown in Figure S7 and its accompanying legend. The data related to inflammatory responses seem to be missing, and the data on bacterial colonization are confusing (bacterial DNA expression or CFU assay?). 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. Figure S7 specifically addresses the effect of MgdE on bacterial colonization in the spleens of infected mice, which was assessed by quantitative PCR rather than by CFU assay. 

      We have now revised the legend of Figure S7 as below (Lines 934-938):

      “MgdE facilitates bacterial colonization in the spleens of infected mice. Bacterial colonization was assessed in splenic homogenates from infected mice (as described in Figure 7A) by quantifying bacterial DNA using quantitative PCR at 2, 14, 21, 28, and 56 days post-infection.”

      (7) Line 112-116: Please provide the original experimental data demonstrating nuclear localization of the 56 proteins harboring putative NLS motifs. 

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We will provide this data in the new Supplementary Table 2.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      The authors use electrophysiological and behavioral measurements to examine how animals could reliably determine odor intensity/concentration across repeated experiences. Because stimulus repetition leads to short-term adaptation evidenced by reduced overall firing rates in the antennal lobe and firing rates are otherwise concentration-dependent, there could be an ambiguity in sensory coding between reduced concentration or more recent experience. This would have a negative impact on the animal's ability to generate adaptive behavioral responses that depend on odor intensities. The authors conclude that changes in concentration alter the constituent neurons contributing to the neural population response, whereas adaptation maintains the 'activated ensemble' but with scaled firing rates. This provides a neural coding account of the ability to distinguish odor concentrations even after extended experience. Additional analyses attempt to distinguish hypothesized circuit mechanisms for adaptation but are inconclusive. A larger point that runs through the manuscript is that overall spiking activity has an inconsistent relationship with behavior and that the structure of population activity may be the more appropriate feature to consider.

      To my knowledge, the dissociation of effects of odor concentration and adaptation on olfactory system population codes was not previously demonstrated. This is a significant contribution that improves on any simple model based on overall spiking activity. The primary result is most strikingly supported by visualization of a principal components analysis in Figure 4. However, there are some weaknesses in the data and analyses that limit confidence in the overall conclusions.

      We thank the reviewer for evaluating our work and highlighting its strengths and deficiencies. We have revised the manuscript with expanded behavioral datasets and additional analyses that we believe convincingly support our conclusion. 

      (1) Behavioral work interpreted to demonstrate discrimination of different odor concentrations yields inconsistent results. Only two of the four odorants follow the pattern that is emphasized in the text (Figure 1F). Though it's a priori unlikely that animals are incapable of distinguishing odor concentrations at any stage in adaptation, the evidence presented is not sufficient to reach this conclusion.

      We have expanded our dataset and now show that the behavioral response is significantly different for high and low concentration exposures of the same odorant. This was observed for all four odorants in our study (refer to Revised Fig. 1F).

      (2) While conclusions center on concepts related to the combination of activated neurons or the "active ensemble", this specific level of description is not directly demonstrated in any part of the results. We see individual neural responses and dimensional reduction analyses, but we are unable to assess to what extent the activated ensemble is maintained across experience.

      We have done several additional analyses (see provisional response). Notably, we have corroborated our dimensionality reduction and correlation analysis results with a quantitative classification analysis that convincingly demonstrates that odor identity and intensity of the odorant can be decoded from the ensemble neural activity, and this could be achieved in an adaptation-invariant fashion (refer to Revised Supplementary Fig. 4). 

      (3) There is little information about the variance or statistical strength of results described at the population level. While the PCA presents a compelling picture, the central point that concentration changes and adaptation alter population responses across separable dimensions is not demonstrated quantitatively. The correlation analysis that might partially address this question is presented to be visually interpreted with no additional testing.

      We have included a plot that compares the odor-evoked responses across all neurons (mean ± variance) at both intensity levels for each odorant (Revised Supplementary Fig. 5). This plot clearly shows how the ensemble neural activity profile varies with odor intensity and how these response patterns are robustly maintained across trials. 

      (4) Results are often presented separately for each odor stimulus or for separate datasets including two odor stimuli. An effort should be made to characterize patterns of results across all odor stimuli and their statistical reliability. This concern arises throughout all data presentations.

      We had to incorporate a 15-minute window between presentations of odorants to reset adaptation. Due to this, we were unable to extracellularly record from all four odorants at two intensities from a single experiment (~ 3.5 hours of recording for just 2 odorants at two intensities with one odorant at higher intensity repeated at the end; Fig. 2a). Therefore, we recorded two datasets. Each dataset captured the responses of ~80 PNs to two odorants at two intensities, one odorant at the higher concentration repeated at the end of the experiment to show repeatability of changes due to adaptation. 

      (5) The relevance of the inconclusive analysis of inferred adaptation mechanisms in Figure 2d-f and the single experiment including a complex mixture in Figure 7 to the motivating questions for this study are unclear.

      Figure 2d-f has been revised. While we agree that the adaptation mechanisms are not fully clear, there is a trend that the most active PNs are the neurons that change the most across trials. This change and the response in the first trial are negatively correlated, indicating that vesicle depletion could be an important contributor to the observed results. However, neurons that adapt strongly at higher intensities are not the ones that adapt at lower intensities. This complicates the understanding of how neural responses vary with intensities and the adaptation that happens due to repetition. This has been highlighted in the revised manuscript. 

      Regarding Figure 7, we wanted to examine the odor-specificity of the changes that happen due to repeated encounters of an odorant. Specifically, wondered if the neural response reduction and behavioral enhancements were a global, non-specific state change in the olfactory system brought about by the repetition of any odorant, or are the observed neural and behavioral response changes odor-specific.

      (6) Throughout the description of the results, typical standards for statistical reporting (sample size, error bars, etc.) are not followed. This prevents readers from assessing effect sizes and undermines the ability to assign a confidence to any particular conclusion.

      We have revised the manuscript to fix these issues and included sample size and error bars in our plots.  

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors' main goal was to evaluate how both behavioral responses to odor, and their early sensory representations are modified by repeated exposure to odor, asking whether the process of adaptation is equivalent to reducing the concentration of an odor. They open with behavioral experiments that actually establish that repeated odor presentation increases the likelihood of evoking a behavioral response in their experimental subjects - locusts. They then examine neural activity patterns at the second layer of the olfactory circuit. At the population level, repeated odor exposure reduces total spike counts, but at the level of individual cells there seems to be no consistent guiding principle that describes the adaptation-related changes, and therefore no single mechanism could be identified.

      Both population vector analysis and pattern correlation analysis indicate that odor intensity information is preserved through the adaptation process. They make the closely related point that responses to an odor in the adapted state are distinct from responses to lower concentration of the same odor. These analyses are appropriate, but the point could be strengthened by explicitly using some type of classification analysis to quantify the adaptation effects. e.g. a confusion matrix might show if there is a gradual shift in odor representations, or whether there are trials where representations change abruptly.

      Strengths:

      One strength is that the work has both behavioral read-out of odor perception and electrophysiological characterization of the sensory inputs and how both change over repeated stimulus presentations. It is particularly interesting that behavioral responses increase while neuronal responses generally decrease. Although the behavioral effect could occur fully downstream of the sensory responses the authors measure, at least those sensory responses retain the core features needed to drive behavior despite being highly adapted.

      Weaknesses:

      Ultimately no clear conceptual framework arises to understand how PN responses change during adaptation. Neither the mechanism (vesicle depletion versus changes in lateral inhibition) nor even a qualitative description of those changes. Perhaps this is because much of the analysis is focused on the entire population response, while perhaps different mechanisms operate on different cells making it difficult to understand things at the single PN level.

      From the x-axis scale in Fig 2e,f it appeared to me that they do not observe many strong PN responses to these stimuli, everything being < 10 spikes/sec. So perhaps a clearer effect would be observed if they managed to find the stronger responding PNs than captured in this dataset.

      We thank the reviewer for his/her evaluation of our work. Indeed, our work does not clarify the mechanism that underlies the adaptation over trials, and how this mechanism accounts for adaptation that is observed at two different intensities of the same odorant. However, as we highlight in the revised manuscript, there is some evidence for the vesicle depletion hypothesis. For the plots shown in Fig. 2, the firing rates were calculated after averaging across time bins and trials. Hence, the lower firing rates. The peak firing rates of the most active neurons are ~100 Hz. So, we are certain that we are collecting responses from a representative ensemble of neurons in this circuit.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      How does the brain distinguish stimulus intensity reduction from response reductions due to adaptation? Ling et al study whether and how the locust olfactory system encodes stimulus intensity and repetition differently. They show that these stimulus manipulations have distinguishable effects on population dynamics.

      Strengths:

      (1) Provides a potential strategy with which the brain can distinguish intensity decrease from adaptation. -- while both conditions reduce overall spike counts, intensity decrease can also changes which neurons are activated and adaptation only changes the response magnitude without changing the active ensemble.

      (2) By interleaving a non-repeated odor, they show that these changes are odor-specific and not a non-specific effect.

      (3) Describes how proboscis orientation response (POR) changes with stimulus repetition., Unlike the spike counts, POR increases in probability with stimulus. The data portray the variability across subjects in a clear way.

      We thank the reviewer for the summary and for highlighting the strengths of our work.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Behavior

      a. While the "learning curve" of the POR is nicely described, the behavior itself receives very little description. What are the kinematics of the movement, and do these vary with repetition? Is the POR all-or-nothing or does it vary trial to trial?

      The behavioral responses were monitored in unconditioned/untrained locusts. Hence, these are innate responses to the odorants. These innate responses are usually brief and occur after the onset of the stimulus. However, there is variability across locusts and trials (refer Revised Supplementary Fig. 1). When the same odorant is conditioned with food reward, the POR responses become more stereotyped and occur rapidly within a few hundred milliseconds. 

      Author response image 1.

      POR response dynamics in a conditioned locust. The palps were painted in this case (left panel), and the distance between the palps was tracked as a function of time (right panel).

      b. What are the reaction times? This can constrain what time window is relevant in the neural responses. E.g., if the reaction time is 500 ms, then only the first 500 ms of the ensemble response deserves close scrutiny. Later spikes cannot contribute.

      This is an interesting point. We had done this analysis for conditioned POR responses. For innate POR, as we noted earlier, there is variability across locusts. Many responses occur rapidly after odor onset (<1 s), while some responses do occur later during odor presentation and in some cases after odor termination. It is important to note that these dynamical aspects of the POR response, while super interesting, should occur at a much faster time scale compared to the adaptation that we are reporting across trials or repeated encounters of an odorant.

      c. The behavioral methods are lacking some key information. While references are given to previous work, the reader should not be obligated to look at other papers to answer basic questions: how was the response measured? Video tracking? Hand scored?

      We agree and apologize for the oversight. We have revised the methods and added a video to show the POR responses. Videos were hand-scored. 

      d. Can we be sure that this is an odor response? Although airflow out of the olfactometer is ongoing throughout the experiment, opening and closing valves usually creates pressure jumps that are likely to activate mechanosensors in the antennae.

      Interesting. We have added a new Supplementary Fig. 2 that shows that the POR to even presentations of paraffin oil (solvent; control) is negligible.  This should confirm that the POR is a behavioral response to the odorant. 

      Furthermore, all other potential confounds identified by the reviewer are present for every odorant and every concentration presented.  However, the POR varies in an odor-identity and intensity-specific manner. 

      e. What is the baseline rate of PORs in the absence of stimuli?

      Almost zero. 

      f. What can you say about the purpose of the POR? I lack an intuition for why a fly would wiggle the maxillary palps. This is a question that is probably impossible to answer definitively, but even a speculative explanation would help the reader better understand.

      The locusts use these finger-like maxillary palps to grab a grass blade while eating. Hence, we believe that this might be a preparatory response to feeding. We have noted that the PORs are elicited more by food-related odorants. Hence, we think it is a measure of odor appetitiveness. This has been added to the manuscript. 

      (2) Physiology

      a. Does stimulus repetition affect "spontaneous" activity (i.e., firing in the interstimulus interval? To study this question, in Figures 2b and c, it would be valuable to display more of the prestimulus period, and a quantification of the stability or lability of the inter-stimulus activity.

      Done. Yes, the spontaneous activity does appear to change in an odor-specific manner. We have done some detailed analysis of the same in this preprint:

      Ling D, Moss EH, Smith CL, Kroeger R, Reimer J, Raman B, Arenkiel BR. Conserved neural dynamics and computations across species in olfaction. bioRxiv [Preprint]. 2023 Apr 24:2023.04.24.538157. doi: 10.1101/2023.04.24.538157. PMID: 37162844; PMCID: PMC10168254

      b. When does the response change stabilize? While the authors compare repetition 1 to repetition 25, from the rasters it appears that the changes have largely stabilized after the 3rd or 4th repetition. In Figure 5, there is a clear difference between repetition 1-3 or so and the rest. Are successive repetitions more similar than more temporally-separated repetitions (e.g., is rep 13 more similar to 14 than to 17?). I was not able to judge this based on the dendrograms of Figure 5. If the responses do stabilize at it appears, it would be more informative to focus on the dynamics of the first few repetitions.

      The reviewer makes an astute observation. Yes, the changes in firing rates are larger in the first three trials (Fig. 3c). The ensemble activity patterns, though, are relatively stable across all trials as indicated by the PCA plots and classification analysis results.

      Author response image 2.

      Correlation as a function of trial number. All correlations were made with respect to the odor-evoked responses in the last odor trial of hex(H) and bza(H).

      c. How do temporal dynamics change? Locust PNs have richly varied temporal dynamics, but how these may be affected is not clear. The across-population average is poorly suited to capture this feature of the activity. For example, the PNs often have an early transient response, and these appear to be timed differently across the population. These structures will be obscured in a cross population average. Looking at the rasters, it looks like the initial transient changes its timing (e.g., PN40 responses move earlier; PN33 responses move later.). Quantification of latency to first spike after stimulus may make a useful measure of the dynamics.

      As noted earlier, to keep our story simple in this manuscript, we have only focused on the variations across trials (i.e., much slower response dynamics). We did this as we are not recording neural and behavioral responses from the same locust. We plan to do this and directly compare the neural and behavioral dynamics in the same locust.

      d.How legitimate is the link between POR and physiology? While their changes can show a nice correlation, the fact the data were taken from separate animals makes them less compelling than they would be otherwise. How feasible is it to capture POR and physiology in the same prep?

      This would be most helpful, but I suspect may be too technically challenging to be within scope.

      The antennal lobe activity in the input about the volatile chemicals encountered by the locust. The POR is a behavioral output. Hence, we believe that examining the correlation between the olfactory system's input and output is a valid approach. However, we have only compared the mean trends in neural and behavioral datasets, and dynamics on a much slower timescale. We are currently developing the capability to record neural responses in behaving animals. This turned out to be a bit more challenging than we had envisioned. We plan to do fine-grained comparisons of the neural and behavioral dynamics, recommended by this reviewer, in those preparations.

      Further, we will also be able to examine whether the variability in behavioral responses could be predicted from neural activity changes in that prep.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review)

      In this manuscript, Weiguang Kong et al. investigate the role of immunoglobulin M (IgM) in antiviral defense in the teleost largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides). The study employs an IgM depletion model, viral infection experiments, and complementary in vitro assays to explore the role of IgM in systemic and mucosal immunity. The authors conclude that IgM is crucial for both systemic and mucosal antiviral defense, highlighting its role in viral neutralization through direct interactions with viral particles. The study's findings have theoretical implications for understanding immunoglobulin function across vertebrates and practical relevance for aquaculture immunology.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript applies multiple complementary approaches, including IgM depletion, viral infection models, and histological and gene expression analyses, to address an important immunological question. The study challenges established views that IgT is primarily responsible for mucosal immunity, presenting evidence for a dual role of IgM at both systemic and mucosal levels. If validated, the findings have evolutionary significance, suggesting the conserved role of IgM as an antiviral effector across jawed vertebrates for over 500 million years. The practical implications for vaccine strategies targeting mucosal immunity in fish are noteworthy, addressing a key challenge in aquaculture.

      Weaknesses:

      Several conceptual and technical issues undermine the strength of the evidence:<br /> Monoclonal Antibody (MoAb) Validation: The study relies heavily on a monoclonal antibody to deplete IgM, but its specificity and functionality are not adequately validated. The epitope recognized by the antibody is not identified, and there is no evidence excluding cross-reactivity with other isotypes. Mass spectrometry, immunoprecipitation, or Western blot analysis using tissue lysates with varying immunoglobulin expression levels would strengthen the claim of IgM-specific depletion.<br /> IgM Depletion Kinetics: The rapid depletion of IgM from serum and mucus (within one day) is unexpected and inconsistent with prior literature. Additional evidence, such as Western blot analyses comparing treated and control fish, is necessary to confirm this finding.

      Novelty of Claims: The manuscript claims a novel role for IgM in viral neutralization, despite extensive prior literature demonstrating this role in fish. This overstatement detracts from the contribution of the study and requires a more accurate contextualization of the findings.

      Support for IgM's Crucial Role: The mortality data following IgM depletion do not fully support the claim that IgM is indispensable for antiviral defense. The survival of IgM-depleted fish remains high (75%) compared to non-primed controls (~50%), suggesting that other immune components may compensate for IgM loss

      .<br /> Presentation of IgM Depletion Model: The study describes the IgM depletion model as novel, although similar models have been previously published (e.g., Ding et al., 2023). This should be clarified to avoid overstating its novelty.

      While the manuscript attempts to address an important question in teleost immunology, the current evidence is insufficient to fully support the authors' conclusions. Addressing the validation of the monoclonal antibody, re-evaluating depletion kinetics, and tempering claims of novelty would strengthen the study's impact. The findings, if rigorously validated, have important implications for understanding the evolution of vertebrate immunity and practical applications in fish health management.

      This work is of interest to immunologists, evolutionary biologists, and aquaculture researchers. The methodological framework, once validated, could be valuable for studying immunoglobulin function in other non-model organisms and for developing targeted vaccine strategies. However, the current weaknesses limit its broader applicability and impact.

      We would like to thank Reviewer for the helpful comments. As the reviewer suggested, we verified the specificity of anti-bass IgM MoAb using multiple well-established experimental approaches, including mass spectrometry analysis, western blot, flow cytometry, and in vivo IgM depletion models. Additionally, we included western blot analyses to further confirm the IgM depletion kinetics. Moreover, we carefully revised any overstated claims in the original manuscript and incorporated the valuable suggestions of the reviewer in the Introduction and Discussion sections to enhance the clarity and rigor of our work.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Experiments and Data Validation:

      Monoclonal Antibody Validation:

      Provide detailed validation of the monoclonal antibody (MoAb) used for IgM depletion.Perform immunoprecipitation followed by mass spectrometry to confirm the specificity of the MoAb and identify any off-target interactions. Conduct Western blot analysis using tissue lysates with varying IgM, IgT, and IgD expression to demonstrate specificity. Include controls, such as a group treated with a control antibody of the same isotype, to confirm the depletion specificity and effects. Present data on the binding site of the MoAb and confirm it targets IgM.

      We thank the reviewer for this constructive comment and have carried out a comprehensive validation of anti-bass IgM monoclonal antibody (MoAb).

      Validation of anti-bass IgM MoAb by Mass Spectrometry

      To validate the specificity of anti-bass IgM MoAb, target proteins were immunoprecipitated from bass serum using IgM MoAb-coupled CNBr-activated Sepharose 4B beads, followed by mass spectrometry analysis to verify exclusive IgM heavy-chain identification (Figure 3–figure supplement 1A). Quantitative mass spectrometry verified the antibody’s specificity, with IgM heavy-chain peptides representing 97.3% of total signal, indicating negligible off-target reactivity. This high target specificity was further supported by the no detectable cross-reactivity to IgT/IgD (Figure 3–figure supplement 1B). Moreover, the 72% sequence coverage (Figure 3–figure supplement 1C) and confirmed LC-MS/MS spectra of IgM peptides (Figure 3–figure supplement 1D) further validated target selectivity.

      Validation of anti-bass IgM MoAb by western blot and flow cytometry

      We compared the anti-bass IgM MoAb with an isotype control (mouse IgG1) under both non-reducing and reducing serum immunoblots. The western blot results showed that the developed MoAb bound specifically to IgM in largemouth bass serum. Owing to the structural diversity of fish IgM isoforms, denatured non-reducing electrophoresis typically yields multiple bands with varying molecular weights (Rombout et al., 1993; Ye et al., 2010). Immunoblot analysis revealed multiple bands with varying molecular weights under non-reducing conditions, with the main band ranging from 700 to 800 kDa and a distinct ~70 kDa band under reducing conditions (Figure 3–figure supplement 2A). Notably, the isotype control showed no detectable bands under both non-reducing and reducing conditions (Figure 3–figure supplement 2A). Additionally, we analyzed tissue lysates from various sources (i.e., Spleen, skin, gill, and gut) and observed consistently recognized bands at identical positions and sizes, whereas the isotype control showed no detectable bands (Figure 3–figure supplement 2B-F).

      Next, we performed flow cytometry analysis to confirm antibody specificity. In largemouth bass head kidney leukocytes, IgM<sup>+</sup> B cells accounted for 28.56% of the population, compared to only 0.41% for the isotype control (Figure 3–figure supplement 2G). Following flow sorting of negative and positive cell populations, we extracted RNA from equal cell numbers. Gene expression analysis revealed high expression of IgM and IgD in the positive population, while IgT and T cell markers were absent (Figure 3–figure supplement 2H and I). These results collectively demonstrate that the monoclonal antibody specifically targets largemouth bass IgM.

      Validation of the depletion specificity and effects using an isotype-matched control antibody

      Largemouth bass (~3 to 5 g) were intraperitoneally injected with 300 µg of mouse anti-bass IgM monoclonal antibody (MoAb, clone 66, IgG1) or an isotype control (mouse IgG1, Abclonal, China). The concentration of IgM in the serum and gut mucus from these MoAb-treated fish was measured by western blot. Our results indicated that anti-bass IgM treatment led to a marked reduction in IgM protein levels in serum (Author response image 1A) and gut mucus (Author response image 1B) from day 1 post-treatment, in contrast to control fish treated with an isotype-matched control antibody.

      Author response image 1.

      Validation of the depletion specificity and effects using an isotype-matched control antibody. (A, B) The depletion effects of IgM from the serum (A) or gut mucus (B) of control or IgM‐depleted fish was detected by western blot. Iso: Isotype group; Dep: IgM‐depleted group.

      We fully agree with the reviewer that epitope characterization would further validate and elucidate the specificity of IgM MoAb. In the present study, we have demonstrated the antibody's IgM-specific binding through multiple classic experimental methods: (1) mass spectrometry analysis, (2) western blot analysis, (3) flow cytometry analysis, and (4) in vivo IgM depletion models. These results collectively support the conclusion that our MoAb specifically targets IgM. We feel that conformational epitope mapping requires structural biology approaches are out of the scope of this work, although future studies should address them in detail.

      Kinetics of IgM Depletion:

      Provide additional evidence for the observed rapid depletion of IgM from serum and mucus within one day, as this is inconsistent with previous findings. Include Western blot results to confirm IgM depletion kinetics.

      Thanks for the reviewer’s suggestion. Previous studies have demonstrated significant differences in the depletion efficiency and persistence of IgM<sup>+</sup> B cells between warm-water and cold-water fish species. In Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus), a warm-water species, administration of 20 µg of anti-IgM antibody resulted in a near-complete depletion of IgM<sup>+</sup> B cells within 9 days (Li et al., 2023). In contrast, rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss), a cold-water species, required significantly higher doses (200–300 µg) to achieve similar depletion, which persisted in both blood and gut from week 1 up until week 9 post-depletion treatment (Ding et al., 2023). In this study, we investigated largemouth bass (Micropterus salmoides), a warm-water freshwater species. Administration of 300 μg of IgM antibody resulted in rapid IgM+ B cell depletion from serum and mucus within one day, indicating that the rapid depletion kinetics may be attributed to the combined effects of the elevated antibody dose and the species-specific immunological characteristics. Moreover, we provide a western blot analysis of serum and mucus after IgM depletion as shown in Figure 5–figure supplement 1G and H.

      Neutralizing Capacity Assays:

      Discuss the potential role of complement or other serum/mucus factors in the neutralization assays. Consider performing neutralization assays that isolate viruses, antibody, and target cells to assess the specific role of IgM.

      Thanks for the reviewer’s insightful suggestion regarding the potential influence of complement and other serum/mucus factors in our neutralization assays. We sincerely regret that the lack of clarity in our methodological description caused misunderstandings to the reviewer. In fact, prior to performing the virus neutralization assays, serum and mucus samples were heat-inactivated at 56 °C to eliminate potential complement interference. Now, we added the related description of heat-inactivation of serum and mucus samples in the revised manuscript (Lines 727-729). Moreover, our results showed that selective IgM depletion from high LMBV-specific IgM titer mucus and serum samples resulted in significantly increased viral loads and enhanced cytopathic effects (CPE), while no significant difference was observed compared to the control group (shown in Figure 6 of the manuscript).

      To further rule out complement or other factors, we purified IgM from serum and gut mucus of 42DPI-S fish for neutralization assays. Briefly, anti-bass IgM MoAb was coupled to CNBr-activated sepharose 4B beads and used for purification of IgM from both serum and gut mucus of 42DPI-S fish. After that, 100 µL of LMBV (1 × 10<sup>4</sup> TCID<sub>50</sub>) in MEM was incubated with PBS and purified IgM (100 µg/mL) at 28 °C for 1 hour and then the mixtures were applied to infect EPC cells. Medium or bass IgM was added to EPC cells as controls. We added the new text in Materials and methods of the revised manuscript in Lines 735-741. Our result showed that a significant reduction in both LMBV-MCP gene expression and protein levels was observed in EPC cells treated with purified IgM from serum (Figure 6–figure supplement 2A, C, and D) or gut mucus (Figure 6–figure supplement 2B, E, and F). Moreover, significantly lower CPE were observed in the IgM treated group, while no CPE was observed in medium and bass IgM group (Figure 6–figure supplement 2G). Collectively, these findings strongly suggest that the neutralization process is a potential mechanism of IgM, serving as a key molecule in adaptive immunity against viral infection. Here, we have incorporated these new findings in the Results section of the revised manuscript (Lines 382-388).

      IgT Depletion Model:

      To fully establish the role of IgM and IgT in antiviral defense, consider including an experimental group where IgT is depleted.

      Thanks for the reviewer’s suggestion. The role of IgT in mucosal antiviral immunity in teleost fish has been reported in our previous studies (Yu et al, 2022). However, this study primarily investigates the antiviral function of IgM in systemic and mucosal immunity and further analyzes the mechanisms of viral neutralization. In future research, we plan to establish an IgT and IgM double-depletion/knockout model to further elucidate their specific roles in antiviral immune defense.

      (2) Writing and Presentation:

      Introduction:

      Replace the cited review article on IgT absence with original research articles (e.g., Bradshaw et al., 2020; Györkei et al., 2024) to strengthen the context.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. We have changed in the revised manuscript (Lines 45-50) as “Notably, while IgT has been identified in the majority of teleost species, genomic analyses reveal its absence in some species, such as medaka (Oryzias latipes), channel catfish (Ictalurus punctatus), Atlantic cod (Gadus morhua), and turquoise killifish (Nothobranchius furzeri) (Bengtén et al., 2002; Bradshaw et al., 2020; Magadán-Mompóet al., 2011; Györkei et al., 2024).”

      Highlight the evolutionary contrast between the presence of the J chain in older cartilaginous fishes and amphibians and its loss in teleosts. Relevant references include Hagiwara et al., 1985, and Hohman et al., 2003.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. We have added the relevant description in the revised manuscript (Lines 61-66) “Interestingly, the assembly mechanism of IgM exhibits significant evolutionary variation across vertebrate lineages. In cartilaginous fishes and tetrapods, IgM is secreted as a J chain-linked pentamer, which may enhance multivalent antigen recognition (Hagiwara et al., 1985; Hohman et al., 2003). By contrast, teleosts have undergone J chain gene loss, resulting in the stable of tetrameric IgM formation (Bromage et al., 2004).”

      Acknowledge prior studies demonstrating the viral neutralization role of teleost IgM (e.g., Castro et al., 2021; Chinchilla et al., 2013). Avoid overstating the novelty of findings.

      Thanks for the reviewer’s suggestion. Here, we revised the related description: “More crucially, our study provides further insight into the role of sIgM in viral neutralization and firstly clarified the mechanism through which teleost sIgM blocks viral infection by directly targeting viral particles. From an evolutionary perspective, our findings indicate that sIgM in both primitive and modern vertebrates follows conserved principles in the development of specialized antiviral immunity.” in the revised manuscript (Lines 20-25) and “To the best of our knowledge, our study provides new insights into the role of sIgM in viral neutralization, suggesting a potential function of sIgM in combating viral infections.” in the revised manuscript (Lines 536-538).

      Clarify terms such as "primitive IgM" and avoid misleading evolutionary language (e.g., VLRs are not "candidates"; they mediate adaptive responses).

      Thanks for the reviewer’s suggestion. We changed the description of the primitive IgM in the sentence of the revised manuscript as “From an evolutionary perspective, our findings indicate that sIgM in both primitive and modern vertebrates follows conserved principles in the development of specialized antiviral immunity.” in the revised manuscript (Lines 23-25) and “our findings suggest that sIgM in both primitive and modern vertebrates utilize conserved mechanisms in response to viral infections” in the revised manuscript (Lines 574-575). Moreover, we deleted the description of VLRs for "candidates" and rewrote the relevant sentence in the revised manuscript (Lines 37-39) as “Agnathans, the most ancient vertebrate lineage, do not possess bona fide Ig but have variable lymphocyte receptors (VLRs) capable of mediating adaptive immune responses (Flajnik, 2018).”

      Results and Discussion:

      Address inconsistencies between data and claims, such as the statement that IgM plays a "crucial role" in protection against LMBV, which is not fully supported by mortality data.

      Thank you for your insightful comment. We have carefully reviewed our data and revised the language throughout the manuscript to ensure that our claims are fully consistent with the mortality data. We have changed the description of “IgM plays a crucial role in protection against LMBV” as “plays a role” (Line 119), “sIgM participates in” (Line 127), “contributes to immune protection” (Line 507) to more accurately reflect the mortality data

      Revise the model in Figure 8 to reflect the concerns raised regarding proliferation data, the role of IgM in protective resistance, and the potential contributions of complement in neutralization assays.

      Thank you for your insightful comment. We have added the raised concerns regarding “the viral proliferation data and the role of IgM in protective resistance” in Figure 8 (shown below). Meanwhile, we added relevant descriptions in the figure legends of the revised manuscript (Lines 587-592) as “Upon secondary LMBV infection, plasma cells produce substantial quantities of LMBV-specific IgM. Critically, these virus-specific sIgM from both mucosal and systemic sources has the ability to neutralize the virus by directly binding viral particles and blocking host cell entry, thereby effectively reducing the proliferation of viruses within tissues. Consequently, the IgM-mediated neutralization confers protection against LMBV-induced tissue damage and significantly reduced mortality during secondary infection.”

      However, considering the following two reasons: (1) heat-inactivation of serum and mucus samples at 56°C prior to neutralization assays effectively abolished complement activity, and (2) purified IgM from both serum and gut mucus demonstrated comparable neutralization capacity, confirming IgM-dependent mechanisms independent of complement. Therefore, we did not add the potential function of complement in neutralization to Figure 8.

      Provide a comparative analysis with other vertebrate models to strengthen the evolutionary implications of findings.

      Thank you for your insightful comment. We have added comparative analyses across additional vertebrate models in the discussion of the revised manuscript to enhance the evolutionary perspective of our findings. The details are as follows:

      “Virus-specific IgM production has been well-documented in reptiles, birds, and mammals upon viral infection (Dascalu et al., 2024; Harrington et al., 2021; Hetzel et al., 2021; Neul et al., 2017;). While current evidence confirms the capacity of cartilaginous fish and amphibians to mount specific IgM responses against bacterial pathogens and immune antigens (Dooley and Flajnik, 2005; Ramsey et al., 2010), the potential for viral induction of analogous IgM-mediated immunity in these species remains unresolved.” in the revised manuscript (Lines 498-504) and “Extensive studies in endotherms (birds and mammals) have demonstrated that specific IgM contributes to viral resistance by neutralizing viruses (Baumgarth et al., 2000; Diamond et al., 2013; Ku et al., 2021; Hagan et al., 2016; Singh et al., 2022). In contrast, the neutralizing activity of IgM in amphibians and reptiles remains largely unexplored. Although viral infections have been shown to induce neutralizing antibodies in Chinese soft-shelled turtles (Pelodiscus sinensis) (Nie and Lu, 1999), the specific Ig isotypes mediating this response have yet to be elucidated. In teleost fish, IgM has been shown to possess viral neutralizing activity similar to that observed in endotherms (Castro et al., 2013; Ye et al., 2013). Furthermore, our recent work demonstrated that secretory IgT (sIgT) in rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) can neutralize viruses, significantly reducing susceptibility to infection (Yu et al., 2022). However, whether IgM in teleost fish possesses the antiviral neutralizing capacity necessary for fish to resist reinfection remains poorly understood.” in the revised manuscript (Lines 521-534)

      Include a description of the Western blot procedure shown in Figures 7D and 7F in the Methods section.

      Thank you for your suggestion. A detailed protocol for the western blot experiments presented in Figures 7D and 7F has been added to the Methods section (Western Blot Analysis) in the revised manuscript (Lines 684-687). The details are as follows: Gut mucus, serum, and cells samples were analyzed by western blot as described by Yu et al (2022). Briefly, the samples were separated using 4%–15% SDS-PAGE Ready Gel (Thermo Fisher Scientific, USA) and subsequently transferred to Sequi-Blot polyvinylidene fluoride (PVDF) membranes (Bio-Rad, USA). The membranes were blocked using a 8% skim milk for 2 hours and then incubated with monoclonal antibody (MoAb). For IgM concentration detection, the membranes were incubated with mouse anti-bass IgM MoAb (clone 66, IgG1, 1 μg/mL) and then incubation with HRP goat-anti-mouse IgG (Invitrogen, USA) for 1 hour. IgM concentrations were determined by comparing the signal strength values to a standard curve generated with known amounts of purified bass IgM. For neutralizing effect detection, the membranes were incubated with mouse anti-LMBV MCP MoAb (4A91E7, 1 μg/mL) followed by incubation with HRP goat-anti-mouse IgG (Invitrogen, USA) for 1 hour. The β-actin is used as a reference protein to standardize the differences between samples. Immunoblots were scanned using the GE Amersham Imager 600 (GE Healthcare, USA) with ECL solution (EpiZyme, China).

      Ensure all figures are labeled appropriately (e.g., replace "Morality" with "Mortality" in Figure 5A).

      Thanks for bringing this to our attention. We have corrected the label in Figure 5A (shown below) and reviewed all figures to ensure that they are appropriately labeled.

      (3) Minor Corrections:

      Line 117: Correct the typo "across both both."

      Thanks for bringing this to our attention. We have changed “across both both” to “across both” in the revised manuscript (Line 119).

      Line 203: Revise to "IgM plays a role (not crucial role)."

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. We have modified the description of IgM's role from “crucial” to “plays a role” to better align with our experimental findings in the revised manuscript (Line 202).

      Line 684: Correct the typo "given an intravenous injection with 200 μg."

      Thanks for bringing this to our attention. We have corrected the phrase to “given an intravenous injection with 200 μg” in the revised manuscript (Line 700-701).

      Line 686: Fix the sentence fragment "previously. EdU+ cells."

      Thank you for your careful review. We have revised the sentence fragment for clarity in the revised manuscript (Lines 702-703).

      Abstract and other sections: Adjust language to remove claims of novelty unsupported by data, particularly regarding the role of IgM in viral neutralization.

      Thank you for your constructive feedback. We have thoroughly reviewed and revised the language throughout the abstract and other sections to remove any unsupported claims of novelty, particularly regarding the role of IgM in viral neutralization in the revised manuscript (Lines 20-25).

      (4)Technical Details:

      Verify data availability, including raw data and analysis scripts, in line with eLife's data policies. Include detailed descriptions of all methods, particularly those involving Western blot analysis and antibody validation.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We added the verify data availability, including raw data and analysis scripts as “The raw RNA sequencing data have been deposited in the NCBI Sequence Read Archive under BioProject accession number PRJNA1254665. The mass spectrometny proteomics data have been deposited to the iProX platform with the dataset identifier IPX0011847000.” in the revised manuscript (Lines 808-811).

      (5) Ethical and Policy Adherence:

      Confirm compliance with ethical standards for animal use and antibody development.Ensure proper citation of all referenced works and accurate reporting of prior findings.

      Thank you for your valuable comment. We confirm that our study fully complies with ethical standards for animal use and antibody development. Additionally, we have carefully reviewed the manuscript to ensure that all referenced works are properly cited and that prior findings are accurately reported.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Asthenospermia, characterized by reduced sperm motility, is one of the major causes of male infertility. The "9 + 2" arranged MTs and over 200 associated proteins constitute the axoneme, the molecular machine for flagellar and ciliary motility. Understanding the physiological functions of axonemal proteins, particularly their links to male infertility, could help uncover the genetic causes of asthenospermia and improve its clinical diagnosis and management. In this study, the authors generated Ankrd5 null mice and found that ANKRD5-/- males exhibited reduced sperm motility and infertility. Using FLAG-tagged ANKRD5 mice, mass spectrometry, and immunoprecipitation (IP) analyses, they confirmed that ANKRD5 is localized within the N-DRC, a critical protein complex for normal flagellar motility. However, transmission electron microscopy (TEM) and cryo-electron tomography (cryo-ET) of sperm from Ankrd5 null mice did not reveal any structural abnormalities.

      Strengths:

      The phenotypes observed in ANKRD5-/- mice, including reduced sperm motility and male infertility, are conversing. The authors demonstrated that ANKRD5 is an N-DRC protein that interacts with TCTE1 and DRC4. Most of the experiments are thoughtfully designed and well executed.

      Weaknesses:

      The cryo-FIB and cryo-ET analyses require further investigation, as detailed below. The molecular mechanism by which the loss of ANKRD5 affects sperm flagellar motility remains unclear. The current conclusion that Ankrd5 knockout reduces axoneme stability is not well-supported. Specifically, are other axonemal proteins diminished in Ankrd5 knockout sperm? Conducting immunofluorescence analyses and revisiting the quantitative proteomics data may help address these questions.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript investigates the role of ANKRD5 (ANKEF1) as a component of the N-DRC complex in sperm motility and male fertility. Using Ankrd5 knockout mice, the study demonstrates that ANKRD5 is essential for sperm motility and identifies its interaction with N-DRC components through IP-mass spectrometry and cryo-ET. The results provide insights into ANKRD5's function, highlighting its potential involvement in axoneme stability and sperm energy metabolism.

      Strengths:

      The authors employ a wide range of techniques, including gene knockout models, proteomics, cryo-ET, and immunoprecipitation, to explore ANKRD5's role in sperm biology.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) Limited Citations in Introduction: Key references on the role of N-DRC components (e.g., DRC1, DRC2, DRC3, DRC5) in male infertility are missing, which weakens the contextual background.

      (2) Lack of Functional Insights: While interacting proteins outside the N-DRC complex were identified, their potential roles and interactions with ANKRD5 are not adequately explored or discussed.

      (3) Mitochondrial Function Uncertainty: Immunofluorescence suggests possible mitochondrial localization for ANKRD5, but experiments on its role in energy metabolism (e.g., ATP production, ROS) are insufficient, especially given the observed sperm motility defects.

      (4) Glycolysis Pathway Impact: Proteomic analysis indicates glycolysis pathway disruptions in Ankrd5-deficient sperm, but the link between these changes and impaired motility is not well explained.

      (5) Cryo-ET Data Limitations: The structural analysis of the DMT lacks clarity on how ANKRD5 influences N-DRC or RS3. The low quality of RS3 data hinders the interpretation of ANKRD5's impact on axoneme structure.

      (6) Discussion of Findings: The manuscript could benefit from a deeper discussion on the broader implications of ANKRD5's interactions and its role in sperm energy metabolism and motility mechanisms.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      EMD-35210/35211 are 16-nm maps while the Ankrd5 null map is 8-nm repeat. To generate a difference map, the authors should use maps of the same periodicity.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have replaced the old 16-nm maps with an 8nm map and updated the images (Fig. 7). The 8nm repeats DMT density map we used was obtained by summing two 16nm repeats DMTs that were staggered 8nm apart from each other (EMD-35229). The replacement of the 16nm repeats DMT density map with the 8nm repeats DMT density map has no effect on our scientific findings and experimental conclusions.

      "We were able to detect the N-DRC structure in WT sperm, but we failed to find the density of N-DRC adjacent to RS3 in Ankrd5 null sperm". Do the authors imply that the N-DRC is lost in Ankrd5 null sperm? To draw a conclusion, they need to compare the 96-nm map of WT sperm axoneme with that of Ankrd5 null sperm axoneme. Quantitative proteomics shows that the levels of most N-DRC components in Ankrd5 null sperm are comparable with those of WT sperm. Why are the quantitative proteomics results not consistent with the structural observation?

      We are very sorry for this improper description. Our original description was not rigorous, which led to misunderstanding. Our original intention is to say that the quality of the density map causes the N-DRC to be difficult to recognize, rather than that the N-DRC has disappeared. In addition, attempts to classify 96nm repeats DMT structure during our data processing failed. In the process of classification, we found that the density of RS was not good. So we changed the picture and the description.

      We have changed the description in the text: "During the STA process, many particles were misaligned or deformed in the classification results, revealing various degrees of deformation—particularly affecting the B-tube (Figure 9,Fig. S9E). We could retain only ~10% of the DMT particles to obtain the final density map for ANKRD5-KO sperm (Fig. S9E), whereas ~70% were usable in WT dataset as reported previously [59]. The mutant DMT density map also displayed roughness at its periphery, indicating substantial structural heterogeneity (Fig. S9E). Even after discarding a large fraction of deformed particles, the final density map still showed evident artifacts, implying that although the mutant DMT preserves the fundamental features of both tubes, its shape is highly heterogeneous (Fig. S9E). Furthermore, attempts to classify the 96-nm repeats did not yield a clear density for radial spokes (RSs) (Fig. S9F), indicating that ANKRD5 deficiency may affect the stability of other accessory structures, such as RSs [24-26]. In the raw tomograms, RSs in ANKRD5-KO sperm appeared less regularly arranged than those in WT(Fig. S9A and C)."

      Figure S9. The states of DMT particles in sperm of Ankrd5-KO mouse. (A) and (C) Tomogram slices of WT and Ankrd5-KO in Dynamo (The data for WT mouse sperm was EMPIARC-200007). DMT and RS are marked with white dashed lines and white arrows, respectively. (B) and (D) Comparison of DMT particle states between WT and Ankrd5-KO in Dynamo. The visual angles of the DMT particles shown in (B) and (D) show that the DMT fibers within the white box in (A) and (B) are divided equally into 10 slices along the direction of the white arrow, respectively. The DMT particle shapes of WT and Ankrd5-KO are marked by white dashed lines on the right of (B) and (D). The white arrow in (D) identifies the junction of A-tube and B-tube that is suspected to be disconnected. (E) Deformed particles discarded in 3D classification and final aligned DMT artifacts. (F) 3D classification of attempted RS locations.

      In the process of obtaining DMT with a period of 8nm, we discarded about 90% of the particles (some were mis-aligned particles and some were deformed particles). Although the final DMT density showed complete A-tube and B-tube, both the particles in our calculation process and the projection of the final structure showed strong particle heterogeneity.

      Our results show that in ANKRD5-KO mice, the structure of sperm DMT itself has no apparent effect in tube A and tube B, and we found that DMT in the original tomography were not smooth. We speculate that loss of ANKRD5 may reduce the interaction between N-DRC and neighboring DMTs, resulting in nonuniform force on the axoneme during sperm swimming, which may limit our ability to obtain an average structure of the more dynamic components (RS, N-DRC, ODA, IDA). Therefore, when trying to classify 96nm repeat DMTS, we can only see the density of suspected RS3 and RS2, but it is difficult to obtain the confident 96nm repeat DMT density. It is difficult to further discuss the effects of ANKRD5 on RS3 and N-DRC. To test this conjecture, we further classified the density of suspected RS3, and the results obtained exhibited a variety of mixed states (Fig. S9). To avoid confusion, we have already removed the discussion of RS3 and the related images from the original text.

      It's not clear whether N-DRC proteins and ODA, IDA, RS proteins are affected in DMT of Ankrd5 null sperm. Immunofluorescence staining would help to resolve this problem.

      Thank you for your suggestion. The levels of N-DRC proteins and ODA, IDA, RS were detected by immunofluorescence, and no difference was found between ANKRD5-null sperm and control. We added figure S6 as a new figure and added the following description in red font on page 7 of the article:

      Figure S6. Immunofluorescence results of ANKRD5-null sperm and control. DRC11 serves as a marker protein for N-DRC (nexin-dynein regulatory complex), NME5 as a marker for RS (radial spoke), DNALI1 as a marker for IDA (inner dynein arm), and DNAI1 as a marker for ODA (outer dynein arm).

      In addition, ODA and RS were also marked in the figure when we further analyzed the Cryo-ET data (Figure 7 and Figure S9).

      Does Ankrd5 express in other cilia cells except for sperm?

      We stained mouse respiratory cilia using immunofluorescence and found that the protein was also expressed in mouse respiratory cilia. To support this finding, we added Figure S3 as a new figure and included a description in red font on page 6 of the article.

      Page 7, "However, in the process of manual selection of DMT fibers, we found that they were not as smooth as WT particles." This description is too subjective. Please show the data.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have added a supplementary figure showing the difference between mutant samples and WT samples during particle picking (Fig. S9).

      Abstract, "These findings establish that ANKRD5 is critical for maintaining axoneme stability, "Page 7, "This suggests that the knockout of Ankrd5 may affect the structural stability of the axoneme," I do not see direct evidence that Ankrd5 KO reduces the axoneme stability.

      Our phrasing was not sufficiently precise. These findings suggest that ANKRD5 plays a crucial role in limiting the relative sliding between adjacent microtubule doublets during axoneme bending, rather than directly contributing to the stability of the axoneme. This sentence has already been modified in the abstract and marked in red. We have added the description in the text: "These findings suggest that ANKRD5 may weaken the N-DRC’s "car bumper" role, reducing the buffering effect between adjacent DMTs and thereby destabilizing axoneme structures during intense axoneme motility." and "To further investigate the RS, IDA, and ODA structures of the axonemes, we conducted immunofluorescence assays in both Ankrd5<sup>-/-</sup> mice and the control group. No significant differences were detected between the two groups (Fig. S6)."

      Page 8, "but our study offers new perspectives for male contraceptive research". Could the authors expand this a bit - how this study may offer new perspectives for male contraceptive research?

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's insightful feedback regarding the translational potential of our findings. This is indeed a critical aspect that we sought to highlight. In response, we have added a paragraph on page 9 (marked in red) to further emphasize this point. We have added the description in the text: "The potential for male contraceptive development arises from ANKRD5's critical structural role mediated through its ANK domain, which facilitates interaction with the N-DRC complex in sperm flagella. Recent structural evidence suggests the protein's positively charged surface may engage with glutamylated tubulin in adjacent microtubules[41], presenting a druggable interface. Targeted disruption of this interaction through small-molecule inhibitors could transiently impair sperm motility. Sperm function relies more on ANKRD5 than respiratory cilia, so inhibiting ANKRD5 has less impact on the latter. This makes ANKRD5 a promising drug target. This tissue-specific phenotypic uncoupling is not uncommon among axonemal-associated proteins, such as DNAH17 and IQUB[65,66]."

      Abstract, "reveals its interaction with TCTE1 and DRC4/GAS8", please provide the alias symbol DRC5 for TCTE1 for clarity.

      Thank you for your suggestion, I have revised the abstract by replacing "TCTE1" with "DRC5/TCTE1" to clarify the alias. The changes have been highlighted in red in the manuscript for easy reference.

      Introduction, "Fertilization relies on successful spermatogenesis and normal sperm motility (4), which occurs in the testes." Does spermatogenesis or normal sperm motility occur in the testes?

      Thank you for pointing out the ambiguity in the sentence. We have revised the sentence in the Introduction and highlighted it in red as follows: Fertilization relies on successful spermatogenesis and normal sperm motility..

      Introduction, "The axoneme exhibits a 9+2 microtubule doublet structure". The description is not accurate. The "2" are singlet microtubules.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We have revised the sentence to accurately describe the axoneme structure and highlight in red as follows: The axoneme features a 9+2 architecture, comprising nine doublet microtubules encircling a central pair of singlet microtubules, with the N-DRC forming cross-bridges between adjacent doublets.

      Page 4, "control sperm successfully fertilized both cumulus-intact eggs". "control" should be a capital "C".

      We thank the reviewer for noting this oversight. The correction has been implemented on page 5 with the term highlighted in red (now reading: "Control sperm successfully fertilized both cumulus-intact eggs"), and we have verified capitalization consistency throughout the manuscript.

      Page 6, "applied RELION, M, and other software". "other software" is not an appropriate description, please be precise.

      We have revised the description as suggested. Specifically, on page 7, the phrase "and other software" has been replaced with "Dynamo and Warp/M," and this change is highlighted in red for clarity.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Several components of the N-DRC complex (e.g., DRC1, DRC2, DRC3, DRC5) have been reported to be associated with male infertility in both humans and mice. However, the introduction lacks proper citations for these studies. Adding these references would provide a more comprehensive background for readers.

      Thank you for your suggestion to strengthen the comprehensiveness of the research background by incorporating additional literatures. More literatures related to DRC1, DRC2, DRC3, and DRC5 were cited in the background of this paper. We have rewritten and reorganized the language of the last paragraph of the introduction, and the entire paragraph is highlighted in red. The content of the paragraph is as follows:

      "It was previously believed that N-DRC comprised 11 protein components[13,18]. However, a new component CCDC153 (DRC12) was found to interact with DRC1[19]. In situ cryoelectron tomography (cryo-ET) has significantly advanced understanding of the N-DRC architecture in Chlamydomonas, demonstrating that DRC1, DRC2/CCDC65, and DRC4/GAS8 constitute its core framework[16], while proteins DRC3/5/6/7/8/11 associate with this framework and engage with other axonemal complexes[20]. Biochemical experiments corroborate these findings and validate this structural model[12,21,22]. The N-DRC functions between the DMTs to convert sliding into axonemal bending motion by restricting the relative sliding of outer microtubule doublets[23,24,25]. Mutations of N-DRC subunits demonstrate that the structural integrity of the N-DRC is crucial for flagellar movements. Mutations in DRC1, DRC2/CCDC65, and DRC4/GAS8 are linked to ciliary motility disorders, causing primary ciliary dyskinesia (PCD)[12,26]. Biallelic truncating mutations in DRC1 induce multiple morphological abnormalities of sperm flagella (MMAF), including outer DMT disassembly, mitochondrial sheath disorganization, and incomplete axonemal structures in human sperm[22,27,28]. Similarly, CCDC65 loss disrupts N-DRC stability, leading to disorganized axonemes, global microtubule dissociation, and complete asthenozoospermia[12,29].  Homozygous frameshift mutations in DRC3 impair N-DRC assembly and intraflagellar transport (IFT), resulting in severe motility defects despite normal sperm morphology[30,31]. TCTE1 knockout mice maintain normal sperm axoneme structure but show impaired glycolysis, leading to reduced ATP levels, lower sperm motility, and male infertility[32]. Both Drc7 and Iqcg (Drc9) knockout mice exhibit disrupted '9+2' axonemal architecture, sperm immotility, and male infertility[21,33]. Drc7 knockout sperm also display head deformities and shortened tails[21]. While N-DRC is critical for sperm motility, but the existence of additional regulators that coordinate its function remains unclear. Our findings indicate that ANKRD5 (Ankyrin repeat domain 5; also known as ANK5 or ANKEF1) interacts with N-DRC structure, serving as an auxiliary element to facilitate collaboration among DRC members. The absence of ANKRD5 results in diminished sperm motility and consequent male infertility."

      While many N-DRC components were identified as interacting with ANKRD5, other proteins outside the N-DRC complex were also detected. Notably, GAS8 (DRC4) ranked 165th among the identified proteins. What are the functions of the higher-ranking proteins, and why do they interact with ANKRD5? Discussing their potential roles would enhance the mechanistic understanding of ANKRD5's function.

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the importance of non-N-DRC proteins interacting with ANKRD5 (ANKEF1). Below, we provide a detailed analysis of the roles and interaction mechanisms of the top-ranked non-N-DRC proteins (Krt77, Rab2a, Gm7429) to elucidate their functional relevance to ANKRD5. We have added the following text to page 6 to clarify and highlight this in red:

      As for other proteins in the LC-MS results, KRT77 is a classic protein that maintains cytoskeletal stability. It may enhance the physical connection between the N-DRC and adjacent DMTs through interaction with ANKRD5. Recent studies indicate that ANKRD5, a newly identified component in the distal lobe of the N-DRC, has a positively charged surface, which may facilitate binding to glutamylated tubulin on adjacent DMTs[41]. Thus, KRT77 may also regulate its interaction with ANKRD5 via post-translational modifications (PTMs, e.g., phosphorylation), thereby strengthening sperm resistance to shear forces during flagellar movement. Rab family proteins participate in intraflagellar transport and membrane dynamics. RAB2A may promote targeted transport of ANKRD5 or other N-DRC components to axonemal assembly sites by recruiting vesicles, and its GTPase activity might link cellular signals to ANKRD5-mediated axoneme remodeling. However, the observed signals could be false positives due to nonspecific factors such as electrostatic adsorption, high-abundance protein interference, detergent-induced membrane disruption, or protein aggregation tendencies.

      The immunofluorescence localization of ANKRD5-Flag appears more aligned with the mitochondrial sheath rather than the axoneme. There is a finer red fluorescent signal extending from the mitochondrial sheath that might correspond to the axoneme. Could this suggest that ANKRD5 has a functional role in the mitochondria? While the authors measured ROS levels, this might not fully clarify whether ANKRD5 is involved in sperm energy metabolism. Considering the motility defects in Ankrd5 knockout mice, further experiments to explore ANKRD5's potential involvement in energy metabolism are necessary.

      The increased detection of ANKRD5 in the midpiece region of the sperm axoneme does not necessarily indicate its localization in mitochondria. Immunofluorescence signals of multiple axonemal Nexin-Dynein Regulatory Complex (N-DRC) components (e.g., TCTE1, DRC1, CCDC65, DRC3, GAS8, and DRC7) are also non-uniformly distributed along the entire flagellum[1]. Similar localization patterns are observed in other structural components, such as radial spoke protein NME5[2] and outer dynein arm protein DNAH5[3]. Furthermore, mitochondria are membrane-bound organelles, and ANKRD5 predominantly resides in the SDS-soluble fraction under varying lysis conditions, confirming its association with the axoneme rather than mitochondria. Thus, the spatial distribution of ANKRD5 does not support a functional role in mitochondria. Importantly, we validated intact mitochondrial function through measurements of reactive oxygen species (ROS) levels (Figure S5C, D), ATP content (Figure 6E), and mitochondrial membrane potential (Figure S5A, B).

      Proteomic analysis of Ankrd5-deficient sperm revealed disruptions in the glycolysis pathway. While these changes do not appear to affect ATP production, the mechanism by which these disruptions impact sperm motility remains unclear. Further investigation into how glycolysis pathway alterations contribute to impaired motility is warranted.

      We appreciate the reviewer's careful consideration of our proteomic data. However, our Gene Set Enrichment Analysis (GSEA) of glycolysis/gluconeogenesis pathways showed no significant enrichment (p-value=0.089, NES=0.708; Fig.6D), which does not meet the statistical thresholds for biological significance (|NES|>1, pvalue<0.05). This observation is further corroborated by our direct ATP measurements showing no difference between genotypes (Fig.6E). We agree that further studies on metabolic regulation could be valuable, but current evidence does not support glycolysis disruption as a primary mechanism for the motility defects observed in Ankrd5-null sperm. This misinterpretation likely arose from the reviewer's overinterpretation of non-significant proteomic trends. We request that this specific claim be excluded from the assessment to avoid misleading readers.

      Weaknesses:

      Cryo-ET Data Limitations: The structural analysis of the DMT lacks clarity on how ANKRD5 influences NDRC or RS3. The low quality of RS3 data hinders the interpretation of ANKRD5's impact on axoneme structure.

      We tried to further calculate the DMT at 96nm period using the present data to analyze the effect of ANKRD5 deletion on RS and N-DRC, however, due to the heterogeneity of the data, we were only able to obtain DMT at 8nm period (we have added a figure in the supplementary material for presentation). And in the process of obtaining DMT with a period of 8nm, we throw away about 90% of the particles (some are misaligned particles, some are deformed particles). Although we were not able to obtain the structure of 96nm repeats DMT, we noticed the enhanced heterogeneity of DMT caused by ANKRD5 knockout, as shown by the 3D classification and other results of the new supplementary images (Fig. S9), and the graphic description was added in the original article.

      We have changed the description in the text: "During particle picking of DMT fibers, we observed that transverse sections of axonemal DMT particles from ANKRD5-KO sperm differ markedly from those in WT sperm. Although both A- and B-tubes were visible in both samples, the DMTs in ANKRD5-KO sperm showed a more irregular profile. In WT sperm, DMTs typically appeared circular, whereas ANKRD5-KO DMTs seemed to be extruded as polygonal. (Fig. S9B,D). Notably, ANKRD5-KO DMTs seemed partially open at the junction between the A- and B-tubes (Fig. S9B,D).

      During the STA process, many particles were misaligned or deformed in the classification results, revealing various degrees of deformation—particularly affecting the B-tube (Fig. S9E). We could retain only ~10% of the DMT particles to obtain the final density map for ANKRD5-KO sperm (Fig. S9E), whereas ~70% were usable in WT dataset as reported previously [59]. The mutant DMT density map also displayed roughness at its periphery, indicating substantial structural heterogeneity (Fig. S9E). Even after discarding a large fraction of deformed particles, the final density map still showed evident artifacts, implying that although the mutant DMT preserves the fundamental features of both tubes, its shape is highly heterogeneous (Fig. S9E). Furthermore, attempts to classify the 96-nm repeats did not yield a clear density for radial spokes (RSs) (Fig. S9F), indicating that ANKRD5 deficiency may affect the stability of other accessory structures, such as RSs [23,24,25]. In the raw tomograms, RSs in ANKRD5-KO sperm appeared less regularly arranged than those in WT (Fig. S9A and C).

      Most recently, following the submission of this work, ANKRD5 was reported to localize at the head of the N-DRC, simultaneously binding DRC11, DRC7, DRC4, and DRC5 [46]. This structural insight agrees with our in vitro findings that ANKRD5 interacts with DRC4 and DRC5 (Fig. 8C-F). However, that study used isolated and purified DMT samples, leaving the precise positioning of ANKRD5 between adjacent axonemal DMTs unconfirmed. We therefore fitted the published structure (PDB entry: 9FQR) into the in situ DMT structure of mouse sperm 96-nm repeats (EMD-27444), revealing that ANKRD5 lies a mere ~3 nm from the adjacent DMT (Fig. 8G). Notably, the N-DRC is often likened to a "car bumper", buffering two neighboring DMTs during vigorous axonemal motion. Given the extensive DMT deformation observed in our cryo-ET data (Fig. S9E), we propose that ANKRD5 contributes to this buffering function at the N-DRC. The loss of ANKRD5 may weaken the "bumper" effect and consequently increase structural damage to adjacent DMTs under intense conditions, while also compromising the stability of associated DMT accessory structures [19,46,60]."

      Figure S9. The states of DMT particles in sperm of Ankrd5-KO mouse. (A) and (C) Tomogram slices of WT and Ankrd5-KO in Dynamo (The data for WT mouse sperm was EMPIARC-200007). DMT and RS are marked with white dashed lines and white arrows, respectively. (B) and (D) Comparison of DMT particle states between WT and Ankrd5-KO in Dynamo. The visual angles of the DMT particles shown in (B) and (D) show that the DMT fibers within the white box in (A) and (B) are divided equally into 10 slices along the direction of the white arrow, respectively. The DMT particle shapes of WT and Ankrd5-KO are marked by white dashed lines on the right of (B) and (D). The white arrow in (D) identifies the junction of A-tube and B-tube that is suspected to be disconnected. (E) Deformed particles discarded in 3D classification and final aligned DMT artifacts. (F) 3D classification of attempted RS locations.

      Although the loss of ANKRD5 did not affect the density of DMT itself in A Tube and B Tube, we found that DMT particles were not smooth in the original tomogram. We speculate that the loss of ANKRD5, a component of the N-DRC that is close to the neighboring DMT, may reduce the interaction between N-DRC and the neighboring DMT, resulting in uneven force on the axoneme during sperm swimming, which may limit our ability to obtain the average structure of the more dynamic components (RS, N-DRC, ODA, IDA). Therefore, when trying to classify 96nm repeat DMT, we could only see the density of suspected RS3 and RS2, but it was difficult to obtain the complete 96nm repeat DMT density, so that we could not further analyze the effect of ANKRD5 deletion on RS and N-DRC. To test this conjecture, we further classified the density of suspected RS3, and the results obtained exhibited a variety of mixed states (which have been added to the supplementary material). To avoid confusion, we have already removed the discussion of RS3 and the related images from the original text.

      The cryo-ET data on the internal structure of the DMT seems to have limited relevance to the N-DRC complex. Additionally, the quality of the RS3 data appears suboptimal, making it difficult to understand how the absence of ANKRD5 influences RS3. Further refinement of the data or alternative approaches may be needed to address this question.

      Thank you very much for your suggestions. For the 96 nm periodic DMT, we have conducted multiple rounds of classification, including applying different masks at the positions of ODA, RS, and DMT. We have also tried classifying with both a single reference and multiple references. However, we were unable to obtain a suitable 96 nm periodic DMT. Regarding the heterogeneity of the particles, we have added a discussion in the manuscript. Following your advice, we have reanalyzed the data, but unfortunately, we still could not further optimize the experimental results.

      In the process of obtaining the 8 nm periodic DMT, we discarded approximately 90 percent of the particles through multiple rounds of classification and alignment, in order to obtain high-quality 8 nm periodic DMT. We classified the remaining particles and found that the densities of RS3 and RS2 were not in their normal states. RS3 might be a mixture of different states of RS3, which makes it difficult for us to further discuss the effects of ANKRD5 on RS3.

      To avoid confusion, we have already removed the discussion of RS3 and the related images from the original text.

      Regarding the effects of ANKRD5 deficiency, we speculate that as the head of the N-DRC, its absence might affect the interaction between the N-DRC and the adjacent DMT, thereby influencing the forces experienced by the DMT during sperm movement. The uneven and irregular forces on the nine pairs of DMTs do not affect the structure of the A and B tubes of the DMT itself, but result in some heterogeneity in the peripheral microtubule parts of the DMT particles. We have added a discussion on these hypotheses in the manuscript. In addition, our 3D classification results demonstrate the structural heterogeneity of DMT caused by ANKRD5 knockdown. We have changed the description in the text:"During particle picking of DMT fibers, we observed that transverse sections of axonemal DMT particles from ANKRD5-KO sperm differ markedly from those in WT sperm. Although both A- and B-tubes were visible in both samples, the DMTs in ANKRD5-KO sperm showed a more irregular profile. In WT sperm, DMTs typically appeared circular, whereas ANKRD5-KO DMTs seemed to be extruded as polygonal. (Fig. S9B,D). Notably, ANKRD5-KO DMTs seemed partially open at the junction between the A- and B-tubes (Fig. S9B,D).

      During the STA process, many particles were misaligned or deformed in the classification results, revealing various degrees of deformation—particularly affecting the B-tube (Figure 9, Fig. S9E). We could retain only ~10% of the DMT particles to obtain the final density map for ANKRD5-KO sperm (Fig. S9E), whereas ~70% were usable in WT dataset as reported previously [59]. The mutant DMT density map also displayed roughness at its periphery, indicating substantial structural heterogeneity (Fig. S9E). Even after discarding a large fraction of deformed particles, the final density map still showed evident artifacts, implying that although the mutant DMT preserves the fundamental features of both tubes, its shape is highly heterogeneous (Fig. S9E). Furthermore, attempts to classify the 96-nm repeats did not yield a clear density for radial spokes (RSs) (Fig. S9F), indicating that ANKRD5 deficiency may affect the stability of other accessory structures, such as RSs [23,24,25]. In the raw tomograms, RSs in ANKRD5-KO sperm appeared less regularly arranged than those in WT (Fig. S9A and C).

      Most recently, following the submission of this work, ANKRD5 was reported to localize at the head of the N-DRC, simultaneously binding DRC11, DRC7, DRC4, and DRC5 [46]. This structural insight agrees with our in vitro findings that ANKRD5 interacts with DRC4 and DRC5 (Fig. 8C-F). However, that study used isolated and purified DMT samples, leaving the precise positioning of ANKRD5 between adjacent axonemal DMTs unconfirmed. We therefore fitted the published structure (PDB entry: 9FQR) into the in situ DMT structure of mouse sperm 96-nm repeats (EMD-27444), revealing that ANKRD5 lies a mere ~3 nm from the adjacent DMT (Fig. 8G). Notably, the N-DRC is often likened to a "car bumper", buffering two neighboring DMTs during vigorous axonemal motion. Given the extensive DMT deformation observed in our cryo-ET data (Fig. S9E), we propose that ANKRD5 contributes to this buffering function at the N-DRC. The loss of ANKRD5 may weaken the "bumper" effect and consequently increase structural damage to adjacent DMTs under intense conditions, while also compromising the stability of associated DMT accessory structures [19,46,60]."

      To further enhance the readability of our manuscript, we created a Graphic Abstract to visually illustrate the biological functions of ANKRD5. The figure is placed immediately after the Abstract section and has been designated as Figure 9.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The major result in the manuscript is the observation of the higher order structures in a cryoET reconstruction that could be used for understanding the assembly of toroid structures. The cross-linking ability of ZapD dimers result in bending of FtsZ filaments to a constant curvature. Many such short filaments are stitched together to form a toroid like structure. The geometry of assembly of filaments - whether they form straight bundles or toroid like structures - depends on the relative concentrations of FtsZ and ZapD.

      Strengths:

      In addition to a clear picture of the FtsZ assembly into ring-like structures, the authors have carried out basic biochemistry and biophysical techniques to assay the GTPase activity, the kinetics of assembly, and the ZapD to FtsZ ratio.

      Weaknesses:

      The discussion does not provide an overall perspective that correlates the cryoET structural organisation of filaments with the biophysical data. The current version has improved in terms of addressing this weakness and clearly states the lacuna in the model proposed based on the technical limitations.

      Future scope of work includes the molecular basis of curvature generation and how molecular features of FtsZ and ZapD affect the membrane binding of the higher order assembly.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Previous studies have analyzed the binding of ZapD to FtsZ and provided images of negatively stained toroids and straight bundles, where FtsZ filaments are presumably crosslinked by ZapD dimers. Toroids without ZapD have also been previously formed by treating FtsZ with crowding agents. The present study is the first to apply cryoEM tomography, which can resolve the structure of the toroids in 3D. This shows a complex mixture of filaments and sheets irregularly stacked in the Z direction and spaced radially. The most important interpretation would be to distinguish FtsZ filaments from ZapD crosslinks, This is less convincing. The authors seem aware of the ambiguity: "However, we were unable to obtain detailed structural information about the ZapD connectors due to the heterogeneity and density of the toroidal structures, which showed significant variability in the conformations of the connections between the filaments in all directions." Therefore, the reader may assume that the crosslinks identified and colored red are only suggestions, and look for their own structural interpretations. But readers should also note some inconsistencies in stoichiometry and crosslinking arrangements that are detailed under "weaknesses."

      Strengths.

      This is the first cryoEM tomography to image toroids and straight bundles of FtsZ filaments bound to ZapD. A strength is the resolution, which. at least for the straight bundles. is sufficient to resolve the ~4.5 nm spacing of ZapD dimers attached to and projecting subunits of an FtsZ filament. Another strength is the pelleting assay to determine the stoichiometry of ZapD:FtsZ (although this also leads to weaknesses of interpretation).

      Weaknesses

      The stoichiometry presents some problems. Fig. S5 uses pelleting to convincingly establish the stoichiometry of ZapD:FtsZ. Although ZapD is a dimer, the concentration of ZapD is always expressed as that of its subunit monomers. Fig. S5 shows the stoichiometry of ZapD:FtsZ to be 1:1 or 2:1 at equimolar or high concentrations of ZapD. Thus at equimolar ZapD, each ZapD dimer should bridge two FtsZ's, likely forming crosslinks between filaments. At high ZapD, each FtsZ should have it's own ZapD dimer. However, this seems contradicted by later statements in Discussion and Results. (1) "At lower concentrations of ZapD, .. toroids are the most prominent structures, containing one ZapD dimer for every four to six FtsZ molecules." Shouldn't it be one ZapD dimer for every two FtsZ? (2) "at the high ZapD concentration...a ZapD dimer binds two FtsZ molecules connecting two filaments." Doesn't Fig. S5 show that each FtsZ subunit has its own ZapD dimer? And wouldn't this saturate the CTD sites with dimers and thus minimize crosslinking?

      We thank the reviewer for these insightful comments. The affinity of ZapD for FtsZ is relatively low and a higher concentration of ZapD is required in solution to effectively saturate the binding sites of all FtsZ molecules forming macrostructures. It is important to clarify that the concentrations mentioned in the text refer to the amounts and ratios of protein added to the total volume of the sample, rather than the proteins actively interacting and forming bundles or macrostructures.

      To differentiate, two aspects can be considered: the ratio of added protein (as mentioned in the text) and the fraction of proteins that contribute to the formation of the macrostructures. Under polymerization conditions, FtsZ-GTP recruits additional monomers to form polymers. Therefore, more FtsZ than ZapD would be involved in forming filaments and bundles. Our results support this hypothesis and show that a higher amount of ZapD is required in the sample to pellet with FtsZ bundles.

      We propose that starting with the same initial concentration of FtsZ and ZapD in solution, only a small fraction of ZapD will bind to the structures, favoring the formation of toroidal structures despite the initial 1:1 ratio of proteins added to the sample. When considering a higher FtsZ:ZapD ratio (1:6), the increased amount of ZapD in solution would facilitate the saturation of all FtsZ binding sites, consistent with the observation of straight bundles. Analytical sedimentation velocity data further supported this finding, indicating a binding ratio of approximately 0.3-0.4, suggesting that one ZapD dimer binds for every 4-6 FtsZ monomers. The binding ratio indicates that two FtsZ monomers will bind to a single dimer of ZapD, but this only occurs when there is a significant excess of ZapD over FtsZ in the solution mixture. 

      These findings align qualitatively with the relative intensities of the electrophoretic bands observed for FtsZ and ZapD in the pelleting assay with different FtsZ-ZapD mixtures, as shown in Suppl. Fig. 5 as % of FtsZ in the fractions. Without prior staining calibration of the gels, there is no simple quantitative relationship between gel band intensities after Coomassie staining and the amount of protein in a band (Darawshe et al. 1993 Anal Biochem - DOI: 10.1006/abio.1993.1581). This last point precludes a quantitative comparison between pelleting/SDS-PAGE data and analytical sedimentation measurements. For this reason, we have decided to present pelleting results as % of FtsZ in supernatant and pellet to avoid overestimations. 

      A major weakness is the interpretation of the cryoEM tomograms, specifically distinguishing ZapD from FtsZ. The distinction of crosslinks seems based primarily on structure: long continuous filaments (which often appear as sheets) are FtsZ, and small masses between filaments are ZapD. The density of crosslinks seems to vary substantially over different parts of the figures. More important, the density of ZapD's identified and colored red seem much lower than the stoichiometry detailed above. Since the mass of the ZapD monomer is half that of FtsZ, the 1:1 stoichiometry in toroids means that 1/3 of the mass should be ZapD and 2/3 FtsZ. However, the connections identified as ZapD seem much fewer than the expected 1/3 of the mass. The authors conclude that connections run horizontally, diagonally and vertically, which implies no regularity. This seems likely, but as I would suggest that readers need to consider for themselves what they would identify as a crosslink.

      The amount of ZapD in the toroids will be significantly less than one third. Although the theoretical addition of protein to the samples is at a 1:1 ratio, the actual amount of protein in the macrostructures containing ZapD is much lower, as shown by sedimentation velocity pelleting assays.

      In contrast to the toroids formed at equimolar FtsZ and ZapD, thin bundles of straight filaments are assembled in excess ZapD. Here the stoichiometry is 2:1, which would mean that every FtsZ should have a bound ZapD DIMER. The segmentation of a single filament in Fig. 5e seems to agree with this, showing an FtsZ filament with spikes emanating like a picket fence, with a 4.5 nm periodicity. This is consistent with each spike being a ZapD dimer, and every FtsZ subunit along the filament having a bound ZapD dimer. But if each FtsZ has its own dimer, this would seem to eliminate crosslinking. The interpretative diagram in Fig. 6, far right, which shows almost all ZapD dimers bridging two FtsZs on opposite filaments, would be inconsistent with this 2:1 stoichiometry.

      Assessing the precise stoichiometry of FtsZ and ZapD within the macrostructures is challenging. We interpret the spikes as ZapD dimers bridging two FtsZ filaments, implying a theoretical 1:1 stoichiometry in the straight bundle. However, ZapD may be enriched in certain areas, indicating that a single FtsZ monomer is binding to one side of the dimer. In contrast, the other side remains available for additional connections, resulting in a potential 2:1 stoichiometry. A combination of both scenarios is likely, although our resolution does not allow further characterization. Considering these complexities, we assume these connections represent a dimer of ZapD binding to two FtsZ monomers.

      Figure 6 shows a simplified scheme illustrating how the bundles could be assembled based on the Cryo-ET data. We acknowledge the limitations of this diagram; its purpose is to depict the mesh formed by the stabilization of ZapD. We have not included interactions that do not lead to filament crosslinking, such as dimers binding to only one FtsZ filament. This focus enhances the interpretation of the scheme and the FtsZ-ZapD interaction. A sentence has been added to the caption to highlight the possibility of other interactions not considered in the scheme.

      In the original review I suggested a control that might help identify the structures of ZapD in the toroids. Popp et al (Biopolymers 2009) generated FtsZ toroids that were identical in size and shape to those here, but lacking ZapD. These toroids of pure FtsZ were generated by adding 8% polyvinyl chloride, a crowding agent. The filamentous substructure of these toroids in negative stain seemed very similar to that of the ZapD toroids here. CryoET of these toroids lacking ZapD might have been helpful in confirming the identification of ZapD crosslinks in the present toroids. However, the authors declined to explore this control.

      The mechanisms by which methylcellulose (MC) promotes the assembly of FtsZ macrostructures reported by Popp et al. involve more than simple excluded volume effects, as the low concentration of MC (less than 1 mg/ml) falls below the typical crowding regime. The latter suggests the existence of poorly characterized additional interactions between MC and FtsZ. These complexities preclude the use of FtsZ polymers formed in the presence of MC as a true control for the FtsZ toroidal structures reported here.

      Finally, it should be noted that the CTD binding sites for ZapD should be on the outside of curved filaments, the side facing the membrane in the cell. All bound ZapD should project radially outward, and if it contacted the back side of the next filament, it should not bind (because the CTD is on the front side). The diagram second to right in Fig. 6 seems to incorporate this abortive contact.

      The role of the flexible linker and its biological implications are still under debate in the field. The flexible linker allows ZapD-driven connections to be made in different directions. While these implications are not the primary focus of our manuscript, the flexible linker could allow connections between filaments in different orientations.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Most of the concerns which I had raised in the earlier version have been taken care of, as detailed in the response.

      A few minor points, mostly related to re-phrasing are listed below:

      Page 2: line 21: The use of the term 'C-terminal domain' for the C-terminal unstructured region of FtsZ is confusing. The term C-terminal domain or CTD for FtsZ is commonly used to describe part of the globular domain, while C-terminal tail or CCTP will be a more apt usage for all the instances in this manuscript.

      We refer to the C-terminal domain as the carboxy-terminal region of the protein. This domain includes the C-terminal linker (CTL), which varies in length between species, followed by a conserved 11-residue sequence (CTC) and shorter, variable C-terminal sequences (CTV). We used the term "C-terminal domain" primarily to improve the readability of the manuscript, but we appreciate the reviewer's feedback. We have now adopted the term "CCTP" instead of "C-terminal domain" to improve the clarity of our manuscript.

      On a related note, the schematic in Fig 1 shows the interaction with CCTP rather than the C-terminal domain of the globular FtsZ. Please provide an explanation.

      We refer to the unstructured C-terminal domain of FtsZ as the C-terminal tail. To avoid confusion, we have introduced the term CCTP in this manuscript.

      Supple Fig 2: "The FCS analysis demonstrated an increasing diffusion time of ZapD along with the FtsZ concentration as result of higher proportion of ZapD bound to FtsZ.

      The increased diffusion time need not be interpreted as increased ZapD bound, it could also mean that FtsZ could polymerise in the presence of increasing ZapD, was this possibility ruled out? Including a comment on this aspect will be useful.

      In these experiments, we monitored fluorescently labeled ZapD. Due to their interaction, we found that its diffusion time increased at high FtsZ concentrations. The data presented in Supplementary Figure 2 shows ZapD in the presence of FtsZ-GDP (i.e. under non-polymerization conditions).

      Was it possible to get a molecular weight estimate based on the diffusion time?

      It is possible to estimate hydrodynamic volumes using the Stokes-Einstein equation if the diffusion coefficient of the diffusing particles is known, assuming that the particles are small and spherical. A molecular weight can then be estimated using a standard density of 1.35 g/cm3 (Fisher et all. Protein science 2009 DOI: 10.1110/ps.04688204). This estimate is heavily dependent on the shape of the diffusing particle, as we assume that our protein of interest here is far from a spherical shape due to the interaction through the flexible linker, the hydrodynamic volumes are overestimated. This overestimation then leads to a further overestimation of the molecular weight. In addition, for a more accurate estimation of the sizes and thus molecular weights for proteins, a modified model of the Stokes-Einstein equation is required (Tyn and Gusek Biotechnology and Bioengineering DOI: 10/1002/bit.260350402), where additional information about the shape of the diffusing particle is estimated by measuring the radius of gyration of the particle. These calculations are complex and beyond the scope of our manuscript.

      Supple Fig 4:

      Does FtsZ GTPase activity (without ZapD) also vary with KCl concentrations? It will be useful to comment on this in Supplementary Figure 4.

      Yes, it has been previously reported that moderate concentration of KCl is optimal for FtsZ GTPase activity. We added a comment to the caption.

      Page 6, line 42: short filament segments arranged nearly 'parallel' to each other Since FtsZ filaments are polar, it is better to rephrase as 'parallel or antiparallel'.

      Corrected.

      Page 7, line 41: cross linking of short 'FtsZ' filaments and not ZapD?

      It was a typo. Corrected

      Page 8: delete 'from above' in the title?

      Corrected

      The use of the phrases such as 'cross linking from the top'; 'binds to FtsZ from above' is vague. (Figure 5b legend; discussion page 10, line 18; page 8, line 26; page 12, line 27). Similarly labelling on a schematic figure on the use of vertical, diagonal/lateral will be useful for the readers.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestions to improve the understanding of our data. We have simplified them by renaming these interactions as vertical.

      Page 13, lines 6 -10

      Rather than an orientation of top or from the side, just the presence of multiple crosslinks along coaxial filaments suffices for a straight bundle. The average spacing will be more uniform in such a straight bundle compared to a toroid where there might be regions without ZapD. I do not find the data on an upward orientation convincing. ZapD binding need not be above to have the C-terminal ends of FtsZ pointing towards the membrane. On the other hand, having ZapD bind above is likely to occlude membrane binding of FtsZ?

      The flexibility of the FtsZ linker suggests that ZapD can bind filaments oriented in different directions. In a cellular environment, FtsZ molecules interact with other division proteins that compete with ZapD for binding sites. This competition could prevent the membrane from occluding and instead create binding sites between the filaments, stabilizing them.

      Page 11, lines 32 - 34: Please rephrase the sentence, with focus on the main point to be conveyed. Do the authors want to say that the 'Same molecule contributes to variability in spacing based on the number of connections formed.'

      Thank you for your comment. We have rephrased the sentence for clarity.

      Page 11: paragraphs 1,2, and 3 appears to convey similar, related ideas and are redundant. Could these be shortened further into one paragraph highlighting how the ratio leads to differences in higher order FtsZ organisation?

      These paragraphs discuss different ideas, and it is better to keep them separate.

      In the response to reviewers, page 19, point 5 (iii), it is given that 5000 FtsZ molecules correspond to 2/3rd of the total, while in the manuscript text, it is given as one-third. Please correct the response text/manuscript text accordingly. The numbers in the cited reference appears to suggest 1/3rd.

      Yes, it was 1/3rd. Thanks for pointing that out. 

      Fig 1b. Y-axis: Absorbance spelling has a typo.

      Page 14, line 11: Healthcare ('h' missing)

      Page 14, line 15: HCl, KCl (L should be in small letter)

      Page15, line 18: 43 - 48K rpm (not Krpm)

      Supple Fig 1 legend: line 5: 's' missing for species

      Corrected.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Editor's note:

      Thank you for taking time and efforts to improve this study. After re-review, two reviewers have a consensus that the connections the fatty acids and sperm motility is still ambiguous. Thus, I recommend to further tone down this conclusion consistently in the title and the text pointed out by reviewers before making a final version of record.

      We sincerely appreciate the considerable time and effort you and the reviewers devoted to evaluating our manuscript. We have revised the title and text to express the relationship between fatty acids and sperm motility more consistently and toned down. With these revisions, we would like to proceed with publishing the manuscript as the Version of Record (VoR). Thank you very much for your guidance in improving our study.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this revised report, Yamanaka and colleagues investigate a proposed mechanism by which testosterone modulates seminal plasma metabolites in mice. Based on limited evidence in previous versions of the report, the authors softened the claim that oleic acid derived from seminal vesicle epithelium strongly affects linear progressive motility in isolated cauda epididymal sperm in vitro. Though the report still contains somewhat ambiguous references to the strength of the relationship between fatty acids and sperm motility.

      Strengths:

      Often, reported epidydimal sperm from mice have lower percent progressive motility compared with sperm retrieved from the uterus or by comparison with human ejaculated sperm. The findings in this report may improve in vitro conditions to overcome this problem, as well as add important physiological context to the role of reproductive tract glandular secretions in modulating sperm behaviors. The strongest observations are related to the sensitivity of seminal vesicle epithelial cells to testosterone. The revisions include the addition of methodological detail, modified language to reflect the nuance of some of the measurements, as well as re-performed experiments with more appropriate control groups. The findings are likely to be of general interest to the field by providing context for follow-on studies regarding the relationship between fatty acid beta oxidation and sperm motility pattern.

      Weaknesses:

      The connection between media fatty acids and sperm motility pattern remains inconclusive.

      We would like to express our sincere gratitude to the judges for their cooperation in reviewing the manuscript and for your helpful comments, which were instrumental in improving manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Using a combination of in vivo studies with testosterone-inhibited and aged mice with lower testosterone levels as well as isolated mouse and human seminal vesicle epithelial cells the authors show that testosterone induces an increase in glucose uptake. They find that testosterone induces a difference in gene expression with a focus on metabolic enzymes. Specifically, they identify increased expression of enzymes regulating cholesterol and fatty acid synthesis, leading to increased production of 18:1 oleic acid. The revised version strengthens the role of ACLY as the main regulator of seminal vesicle epithelial cell metabolic programming. The authors propose that fatty acids are secreted by seminal vesicle epithelial cells and are taken up by sperm, positively affecting sperm function. A lipid mixture mimicking the lipids secreted by seminal vesicle epithelial cells, however, only has a small and mostly non-significant effect on sperm motility, suggesting the authors were not apply to pinpoint the seminal vesicle fluid component that positively affects sperm function.

      We greatly appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful comments and time spent reviewing this manuscript. The relationship between lipids such as fatty acids and sperm motility remains unclear in the current dataset. Therefore, before finalizing the manuscript, we revised the title and text, as suggested by the reviewers, to express this conclusion more cautiously and consistently.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Some additional comments are provided below to aid the authors in improving the quality of the work:

      Major Comments:

      (1) In the newly added supplemental figure 5, the authors note that the percentage data were arcisine transformed prior to statistical analysis without providing any other justification. This seems strange, especially for such a small sample size. It seems more appropriate for the authors to use a nonparametric test. Forcing symmetry without knowing what the shape of the true distribution is makes the ANOVA hard to interpret. Additionally, why use pairwise comparisons rather than comparing each group to the control (LM 0%). Also, note that the graphs are not individually labeled to distinguish them in the legend (A, B, C, etc.). Ultimately, the treatment differences don't seem that meaningful, even if the authors were able to observe statistical significance with the somewhat over-manipulated method of analysis.

      Ultimately, the conclusion of this experiment (Supplemental figure 5) remains unchanged, but we agree that the relationship between fatty acids and sperm motility remains unclear. Therefore, before finalizing the manuscript, we revised the title and text as pointed out by the reviewers to express this conclusion more cautiously and consistently throughout the manuscript.

      Arcsin transform is commonly used for percentage data [Zar, J.H. 2010. Biostatistical analysis., McDonald, J.H. 2014. Handbook of biological statistics.]. If the values are low or high, such as 0 to 30% or 70 to 100%, without arcsine transformation will result in a large deviation from the normality of the data. However, even if such a conversion is performed, it does not necessarily mean that the assumptions of normality and homogeneity of variance, which are prerequisites for parametric statistical analysis methods, are satisfied.

      Given the small sample size and the possibility of non-normal data, we performed Shapiro–Wilk tests for each group (n = 6) and found no departure from normality (all p > 0.1). Q–Q plots and Levene’s test (p > 0.1) likewise supported the assumptions of ANOVA. Following the reviewer’s recommendation, we repeated the analysis with a Kruskal–Wallis test followed by Dunn’s post-hoc comparisons (Bonferroni corrected). Both approaches led to the same conclusions, with non-parametric p-values equal to or smaller than the parametric ones. In the revised manuscript we now report ANOVA as the primary analysis. The author response image includes effect sizes with 95 % confidence intervals, and provide the non-parametric results for transparency.

      Author response image 1.

      Results of reanalysis of supplementary Figure 5 using nonparametric tests and effect sizes with 95% confidence intervals. Upper part; Differences between groups were assessed by Kruskal–Wallis test, differences among values were analyzed by Dunn’s post-hoc comparisons (Bonferroni corrected) for multiple comparisons. Different letters represent significantly different groups. Lower part; The effect sizes with 95 % confidence intervals. For example, Cliff's Δ = -1 (95% CI ~ -0.6) in VSL's “LM 0 vs LM1” means that LM 1% values exceed LM 0 %values in all pairs.

      (2) I appreciate that the authors toned down the interpretation of the effects of seminal plasma metabolites on sperm motility with a cautionary statement on Lines 397-405 and Line 259. However, they send mixed signals with the title of the report: "Testosterone-Induced Metabolic Changes in Seminal Vesicle Epithelial cells Alter Plasma Components to Enhance Sperm Motility", and on line 265 when the say "ACLY expression is upregulated by testosterone and is essential for the metabolic shift of seminal vesicle epithelial cells that mediates sperm linear motility".

      The wording has been softened overall. The title has been changed to “Testosterone-Induced Metabolic Changes in Seminal Vesicle Epithelium Modify Seminal Plasma Components with Potential to Improve Sperm Motility” In the results (lines 265-266), we have stated that “ACLY expression is upregulated by testosterone and is essential for the metabolic shift that is associated with increased linear motility” without implying a causal relationship.

      Minor Comments:

      (1) Typo on line 31: "understanding the male fertility mechanisms and may perspective for the development of potential biomarkers of male fertility and advance in the treatment of male infertility."

      We have made the following corrections. “These findings suggest that testosterone-dependent lipid remodeling may contribute to sperm straight-line motility, and further functional verification is required.”

      (2) Line 193: the statement is confusing "Therefore, we analyzed mitochondrial metabolism using a flux analyzer, predicting that more glucose is metabolized, pyruvate is metabolized from phosphoenolpyruvic acid through glycolysis in response to testosterone, and is further metabolized in the mitochondria." For example, 'Metabolized through glycolysis' is an ambiguous way to describe the pyruvate kinase reaction. Additionally, phosphoenolpyruvate has three acid ionizable groups, two of which have pKa's well below physiological pH, so phosphoenolpyruvate is the correct intermediate rather than phosphoenolpyruvic acid. The authors make similar mistakes with other organic acids such as citric acid.

      Rewritten as “We therefore examined cellular energy metabolism with a flux analyzer, anticipating that testosterone would elevate glycolytic flux, thereby producing more pyruvate from phosphoenolpyruvate. Because extracellular pyruvate levels simultaneously declined, we inferred that the cells had an increased pyruvate demand and, at that time, hypothesized that the excess pyruvate would enter the mitochondria to support enhanced oxidative metabolism.” (lines 193-198)

      The organic acids are now referenced in their appropriate forms (e.g., citrate, phosphoenolpyruvate).

      (3) Line: 271: "Acly" should be all capitalized to "ACLY". The report mixes capitalizing through out and could be more consistent.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s attention to nomenclature and have standardized the manuscript accordingly. Proteins are written in Roman letters, all in capital letters. Mouse gene symbols: italics, first letter capitalize.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Major comments:

      (1) 'Once capacitation is complete, sperm cannot maintain that state for a long time'. The publications cited by the author do not support that statement and this reviewer also does not agree. Lower fertilization efficiency from in vitro capacitated epidydimal sperm does not have to mean capacitation is reversed, it can simply mean in vitro capacitation conditions not accurately mimic capacitation in vivo.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out and would like to clarify our position. Our statement does not suggest a "reversal" of active capacitation. Rather, it reflects the well-documented fact that capacitation is a transient process. Sperm that undergo capacitation too early cannot maintain that state for long enough to retain their ability to fertilize at the moment and location of fertilization in vivo.

      (2) How do the authors explain the discrepancy between the results shown in Fig. S1E, the increase in sperm motility upon mixing of sperm with SVF and the results reported in Li et al 2025. Mentioning decapacitating factors without further explanation is insufficient.

      We appreciate the reviewer's feedback pointing out the need for a clearer explanation.

      Seminal plasma is inherently binary, containing both decapacitation factors that delay or inhibit capacitation and nutrient substrates that promote sperm motility.

      In vivo, it is believed that the coating of sperm by decapacitation factors is removed by uterine fluid and albumin as it passes through the female reproductive tract [PMID: 22827391, PMID: 24274412]. In contrast, standard fertilization culture media lack a clearance pathway, so decapacitating factors are retained throughout the culture period. As a result, the cleavage rate after in vitro fertilization using sperm exposed to seminal vesicle fluid decreased dramatically.

      Lipids, such as fatty acids, increased sperm motility without directly inducing markers of fertilization. These results suggest that the enhancement of motility by lipids is functionally distinct from the capacitation-inhibiting function of seminal plasma proteins. The data from this study are consistent with the biphasic model. Specifically, decapacitation factors temporarily stabilize the sperm membrane, preventing early capacitation. Meanwhile, lipids enhance sperm motility, enabling them to rapidly pass through the hostile uterine environment.

      (3) This reviewer does not see the merit in including a lipid mixture motility experiment compared to using OA alone. The increase in motility is still small and far from comparable to the motility increase with seminal vesicle fluid. In this reviewer's opinion the experiment is still inconclusive and should not be highlighted in the manuscript title.

      The wording has been softened overall. The title has been changed to “Testosterone-Induced Metabolic Changes in Seminal Vesicle Epithelium Modify Seminal Plasma Components with Potential to Improve Sperm Motility”. (Please see also Reviewer 1's main comment 1)

      Minor comments:

      (1) 'This change includes a large amplitude of flagella' does not make sense. Please correct.

      The following corrections have been made. “This change is characterized by large-amplitude flagellar beating.” (lines 44-45)

  3. Jul 2025
    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors aim to explore the effects of the electrogenic sodium-potassium pump (Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup>-ATPase) on the computational properties of highly active spiking neurons, using the weakly-electric fish electrocyte as a model system. Their work highlights how the pump's electrogenicity, while essential for maintaining ionic gradients, introduces challenges in neuronal firing stability and signal processing, especially in cells that fire at high rates. The study identifies compensatory mechanisms that cells might use to counteract these effects, and speculates on the role of voltage dependence in the pump's behavior, suggesting that Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup>-ATPase could be a factor in neuronal dysfunctions and diseases

      Strengths:

      (1) The study explores a less-examined aspect of neural dynamics-the effects of (Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup>-ATPase) electrogenicity. It offers a new perspective by highlighting the pump's role not only in ion homeostasis but also in its potential influence on neural computation.

      (2) The mathematical modeling used is a significant strength, providing a clear and controlled framework to explore the effects of the Na+/K+-ATPase on spiking cells. This approach allows for the systematic testing of different conditions and behaviors that might be difficult to observe directly in biological experiments.

      (3) The study proposes several interesting compensatory mechanisms, such as sodium leak channels and extracellular potassium buffering, which provide useful theoretical frameworks for understanding how neurons maintain firing rate control despite the pump's effects.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) While the modeling approach provides valuable insights, the lack of experimental data to validate the model's predictions weakens the overall conclusions.

      (2) The proposed compensatory mechanisms are discussed primarily in theoretical terms without providing quantitative estimates of their impact on the neuron's metabolic cost or other physiological parameters.

      We thank the reviewer for their concise and accurate summary and appreciate the constructive feedback on the article’s strengths and weaknesses. Experimental work is beyond the scope of our modeling-based study. However, we would like our work to serve as a framework for future experimental studies into the role of the electrogenic pump current (and its possible compensatory currents) in disease, and its role in evolution of highly specialized excitable cells (such as electrocytes).

      Quantitative estimates of metabolic costs in this study are limited to the ATP that is required to fuel the pump. By integrating the net pump current over time and dividing by one elemental charge, one can find the rate of ATP that is consumed by the Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup>pump for either compensatory mechanism. The difference in net pump current is thus proportional to ATP consumption, which allows for a direct comparison of the cost efficiency of the Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup> pump for each proposed compensatory mechanism. The Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup> pump is, however, not the only ATP-consuming element in the electrocyte, and some of the compensatory mechanisms induce other costs related to cell

      ‘housekeeping’ or presynaptic processes. We now added a section in the appendix titled

      ‘Considerations on metabolic costs of compensatory mechanisms’ (section 11.4), where we provide ballpark estimates for the influence of the compensatory mechanisms on the total metabolic costs of the cell and membrane space occupation. Although we argue that according these estimates, the impact of discussed compensatory mechanisms could be significant, due to the absence of more detailed experimental quantification, a plausible quantitative cost approximation on the whole cell level remains beyond the scope of this article.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1)  For the f-I curves in Figures 1 and 6, the firing rate increases as the input current increases. I am curious to know: (a) whether the amplitudes of the action potentials (APs) vary with increased input current; (b) whether the waveform of APs (such as in Fig. 1I) transitions into smaller amplitude oscillations at higher input currents; and (c) if the waveform does change at higher input currents, how do the "current contributions," "current," and "ion exchanges per action potential" in Figures 1HJ and 6AB respond?

      To fully answer these questions, we added a supplemental figure with accompanied text in section 11.1 (Fig. A1). We also added a reference to this figure in the main text (section 4.1). Here, it is shown that, as previously illustrated in [1], AP amplitude decreases when the input current increases (Fig. A1 A, left). This effect remains upon addition of either a pump with constant pump rate and co-expressed sodium leak channels (Fig. A1 A, center), or a voltage-dependent pump (Fig. A1 A, right). Interestingly, even though the shape of the current contributions (Fig. A1 B) and the APs (Fig. A1 C) look very different for low (Fig. A1 C, top) and high inputs (Fig. A1 C, bottom), the total sodium and potassium displacement per AP, and thus the pump rate, is roughly the same (Fig. A1 D). Under the assumption that voltage-gated sodium channel (NaV) expression is adjusted to facilitate fixed-AP amplitudes, however, (as in [1]) more NaV channels would be expressed in fish with higher synaptic drives. This would then result in an additional sodium influx per AP and result in higher energetic requirements per AP for electrocytes with higher firing rates (also shown in [1]).

      (2) Could the authors clarify what the vertical dashed line represents in Figures 1B and 1F? Does it correspond to an input current of 0.63uA?

      (Reviewer comment refers to Fig. 1C and 1F in new version): Yes, it corresponds to the input current that is also used in figures 1D and 1G. We clarified this by adding an additional tick label on the x-axis in 1F. The current input of 0.63uA was chosen as a representative input for this cell as follows: we first modeled an electrocyte with a periodic synaptic drive as in [1]. The frequency of this drive was set to 400 Hz, which is an intermediate value in the range of reported EODfs (and thus presumably pacemaker firing rates) of 200-600Hz [2]. Then, acetylcholine receptor currents I<sub>AChRNa</sub> and I<sub>AChRNa</sub> were summed and averaged to obtain the average input current of 0.63uA. This is now also explained in new Methods section 6.2.1.

      (3) What input current was used for Figures 1H, 1I, and 1J?

      Response: In a physiological setting, where the electrocyte is electrochemically coupled to the pacemaker nucleus, stimulation of the electrocyte occurs through neurotransmitter release in the synaptic cleft, which then leads to the opening of acetylcholine receptor channels. As figures 1H-J concern different ion fluxes, we aimed to also include currents stemming from acetylcholine receptor channels. We therefore did not stimulate the electrocyte with a constant input current as in Fig. 1C and F, but simulated elevated constant neurotransmitter levels in the synaptic cleft, which then leads to elevated acetylcholine receptor currents. In the model, this neurotransmitter level, or ‘synaptic drive’ is represented by parameter syn<sub>clamp</sub>. A physiologically relevant value for syn<sub>clamp</sub> was deduced by averaging the synaptic drive during a 400 Hz pacemaker stimulus. This is now also explained in new Methods section 6.2.1.

      (4) In Figure 4A, there is a slight delay between the PN spikes (driver) and the EO (receiver), and no EO spikes occur without PN spikes. However, the firing rate of EO (receiver) appears to decrease before the chirp initiations in Fig 4B; and this delay seems to disappear in Fig 4C. Could the authors explain these observations?

      As shown in the bottom right of figure 4A, when plotting the instantaneous firing rate as one over the inter-spike-interval (1/ISI), the firing rate of a cell is only plotted at the end of every ISI. Therefore, even though the PN drives the electrocyte and thus spikes earlier in time than the electrocyte, when it initiates chirps, these will only be plotted as an instantaneous firing rate at the end of the chirp. If the electrocyte fires spontaneously within this chirp, its instantaneous firing rate will appear earlier in time than the initiation of the chirp of the PN. The PN did, however, initiate the chirp before that and causality between the PN and electrocyte is not disturbed.

      (5) Regarding Figure 6, could the authors specify the input current used in Figures 6A and 6B?

      Figure 6A and 6B have the same synaptic drive as Fig. 1 H, I and J (syn<sub>clamp</sub>=0.13).

      (6) In Section 6, I would recommend that the authors provide a table of parameters and their corresponding values for clarity.

      Thank you for your suggestion. We now reorganized the method section and added two tables with parameters for clarity. Table 1 (see Methods 6.1) includes all parameters that differ from the parameters reported in [1], and parameters that arise from the additionally modeled equations to simulate ion concentration dynamics and pump. We also added the parameters used to simulate the different stimulus protocols (and corresponding tuned parameters) that are presented in the article in Table 2 (see Methods 6.2).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The paper 'The electrogenicity of the Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup>-ATPase poses challenges for computation in highly active spiking cells' by Weerdmeester, Schleimer, and Schreiber uses computational models to present the biological constraints under which electrocytes-specialized highly active cells that facilitate electro-sensing in weakly electric fish-may operate. The authors suggest potential solutions these cells could employ to circumvent these constraints.

      Electrocytes are highly active or spiking (greater than 300Hz) for sustained periods (for minutes to hours), and such activity is possible due to an influx of sodium and efflux of potassium ions into these cells for each spike. This ion imbalance must be restored after each spike, which in electrocytes, as with many other biological cells, is facilitated by the Na-K pumps at the expense of biological energy, i.e., ATP molecules. For each ATP molecule the pump uses, three positively charged sodium ions from the intracellular space are exchanged for two positively charged potassium ions from the extracellular volume. This creates a net efflux of positive ions into the extracellular space, resulting in hyperpolarized potentials for the cell over time. This does not pose an issue in most cells since the firing rate is much slower, and other compensatory mechanisms and other pumps can effectively restore the ion imbalances. In electrocytes of weakly electric fish, however, that operate under very different circumstances, the firing rate is exceptionally high. On top of this, these cells are also involved in critical communication and survival behaviors, emphasizing their reliable functioning.

      In a computation model, the authors test four increasingly complex solutions to the problem of counteracting the hyperpolarized states that occur due to continuous NaK pump action to sustain baseline activity. First, they propose a solution for a well-matched Na leak channel that operates in conjunction with the NaK pump, counteracting the hyperpolarizing states naturally. Additionally, their model shows that when such an orchestrated Na leak current is not included, quick changes in the firing rates could have unexpected side effects. Secondly, they study the implication of this cell in the context of chirps - a means of communication between individual fishes. Here, an upstream pacemaking neuron entrains the electrocyte to spike, which ceases to produce a so-called chirp - a brief pause in the sustained activity of the electrocytes. In their model, the authors show that it is necessary to include the extracellular potassium buffer to have a reliable chirp signal. Thirdly, they tested another means of communication in which there was a sudden increase in the firing rate of the electrocyte followed by a decay to the baseline. For reliable occurrence of this, they emphasize that a strong synaptic connection between the pacemaker neuron and the electrocyte is warranted. Finally, since these cells are energy-intensive, they hypothesize that electrocytes may have energyefficient action potentials, for which their NaK pumps may be sensitive to the membrane voltages and perform course correction rapidly.

      Strengths:

      The authors extend an existing electrocyte model (Joos et al., 2018) based on the classical Hodgkin and Huxley conductance-based models of Na and K currents to include the dynamics of the NaK pump. The authors estimate the pump's properties based on reasonable assumptions related to the leak potential. Their proposed solutions are valid and may be employed by weakly electric fish. The authors explore theoretical solutions that compound and suggest that all these solutions must be simultaneously active for the survival and behavior of the fish. This work provides a good starting point for exploring and testing in in vivo experiments which of these proposed solutions the fish use and their relative importance.

      Weaknesses:

      The modeling work makes assumptions and simplifications that should be listed explicitly. For example, it assumes only potassium ions constitute the leak current, which may not be true as other ions (chloride and calcium) may also cross the cell membrane. This implies that the leak channels' reversal potential may differ from that of potassium. Additionally, the spikes are composed of sodium and potassium currents only and no other ion type (no calcium). Further, these ion channels are static and do not undergo any post-translational modifications. For instance, a sodium-dependent potassium pump could fine-tune the potassium leak currents and modulate the spike amplitude (Markham et al., 2013).

      This model considers only NaK pumps. In many cell types, several other ion pumps/exchangers/symporters are simultaneously present and actively participate in restoring the ion gradients. It may be true that only NaK pumps are expressed in the weakly electric fish Eigenmannia virescens. This limits the generalizability of the results to other cell types. While this does not invalidate the results of the present study, biological processes may find many other solutions to address the non-electroneutral nature of the NaK pump. For example, each spike could include a small calcium ion influx that could be buffered or extracted via a sodium-calcium exchanger.

      Finally, including testable hypotheses for these computational models would strengthen this work.

      We thank the reviewer for the detailed summary and the identified weaknesses according to which we improved our article. Our model assumptions and simplifications are now mentioned in more detail in the introduction of the article (section 3), and justified in the Methods (section 6.1).

      Furthermore, we added a discussion section (section 5.1) where we outline the conditions under which the present study can be extended to other cell types. We now also state more clearly that the pump current will be present for any excitable cell with significant sodium flux (assuming that the NaK pump carries out the majority of its active transport), but that compensatory mechanisms (if employed at all in a particular cell) could also be implemented via other ionic currents and transporters. We furthermore now highlight the testable hypotheses that we put forward with our computational study on the weakly electric fish electrocyte more explicitly in the first paragraph of the discussion.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Main text

      Please explicitly state this model's assumptions in the introduction and elaborate on them in the discussion if necessary. For example, some assumptions that I find relevant to mention are: - The Na and K channels are classic HH conductance-based channels, with no post-translational modifications or beta subunit modifications as seen in other high-frequency firing cells (10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-12-04899.2003).

      Neither calcium nor chloride ions are considered in the spike generation. Nor are Na-dependent K channels (10.1152/jn.00875.2012).

      Only the Na-K pump (and not the Na-Ca exchanger, Ca-pump, or Cl pumps) is modeled,

      Calmodulin, which can buffer calcium, is highly expressed in electric eels, but it is not considered. If some of these assumptions have valid justifications in weakly electric fish electrocytes, please state so with the citations. I recognize that including these in your models is beyond the scope of the current paper.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this issue. We now specified in the introduction that the model only contains sodium and potassium ions and only classic HH conductance-based channels. We there also explicitly specify the details on the Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup>-ATPase: it is the only active transporter in this model, thus solely responsible for maintaining ionic homeostasis; its activity is only modulated by intracellular sodium and extracellular potassium concentrations. In the discussion (6.1), we now elaborate on how ion-channel-related aspects (i.e., the addition of resurgent Na<sup>+</sup> or Na<sup>+</sup> -dependent K<sup>+</sup> channels), additional ion fluxes (including some not relevant for the electrocyte but for other excitable cells), and additional active transporters and pumps would influence the results presented in the article.

      In addition, there might be other factors that the authors and the reviewers have yet to consider. The model is a specific case study about the weakly electric fish electrocyte with high-frequency firing. It is almost guaranteed that biology will find other compensatory ways in different cell types, systems, and species (auditory nerve, for example). Given this, it would be prudent to use phrases such as 'this model suggests,' 'perhaps,' 'could,' 'may,' and 'eludes to,' etc., to accommodate other possible solutions to ion homeostasis in rapidly spiking neurons. The solutions the authors are proposing are some of many.

      We rephrased some of the statements to highlight more the hypothetical nature of the compensatory mechanisms in specific cells and to draw attention to the fact that there can be many more such factors. This fact is now also explicitly mentioned in discussion section 5.2.

      Figures

      Some of my comments on the figures are stylistic, others are to improve clarity, and some are critical for accuracy.

      The research problem concerns weakly electric fish E. virescens. I suggest introducing a picture of an electric fish in the beginning (such as that in Figure 3, but not exactly; see specific comments on this fish figure) along with a schema of the research question. 

      We agree, and added an overview schema in Fig. 1A.

      Font sizes change between the panels in all the figures. Please maintain consistency. The figure panel titles and axis labels should start with a capital letter.

      Thank you for pointing this out, both issues have been resolved in the new version of the article.

      Figure 1:

      Please rearrange the figure - BCFG belong together and should appear in the same order. The x-axis labels could be better placed.

      Consider using fewer pump current f-I curves (B, D, E, F). Five is sufficient to make the point. Having 10 curves adds to the clutter. The placement of the color bar could be better. Similarly, the placement of the panel titles 'without co-expression' and 'with co-expression' and the panel labeling (BCFG) makes it confusing. The panel labels should be above the panel title.

      Response (C, D, F, G in new version): We improved the layout of figure 1. Panels B, C, F, G are now C, D, F, G. We opted to include panel E before panels F and G, because it shows the coexpression mechanism before its effect on the tuning curve. We did move the colorbar, added x-axis labels to B and C, and adjusted the location of the panel labels for clarity. We also plotted fewer pump currents.

      B, F: What does the dashed line indicate?

      Response (C, F in new version): The dashed line indicates the input current that was used in figures 1D and 1G. We now clarified this by adding this value on the x-axis.

      C: Any reason not to show the lower firing rates?

      Response (B in new version): In the previous version of the article, pump currents were estimated for electrocytes that were stimulated with the mean synaptic drive that stems from periodic stimulation in the 200-600 Hz regime. We now extended the range of synaptic inputs to obtain lower (and higher) firing rates. The linear relationship between firing rate and pump current also holds for these additional firing rates.

      D: There is no difference between the curves at the top and the bottom. One fills the area between the curve and the zero line; the other shows the curve itself. Please use only one of the two representations.

      Response (panel I in new version): In the previous version, the difference between the plots was that one showed the absolute values of the currents (the curves), and the other plot showed the contributions of the currents to the total (area between the curves). We now only depict the current contributions.

      The I and H orders can be swapped.

      Thank you, they are now swapped.

      The colors used for Na and K are very dull (light blue and pink).

      We now use darker colors in the new version of the article.

      Figure 2:

      Please verify that without the synaptic input perturbations (i.e., baseline in A, D), the firing rate (B, E) and pump current (C, F) converge to the baseline. There is a noticeable drift (downward for firing rate and upward for pump currents) at the 10-second time point.

      Thanks to you noticing, we identified a version mismatch in the code that estimates the pump current required for ionic homeostasis (see Methods 6.1.2). We have now corrected the code and made sure to start the simulation in the steady state so that there is no drift at baseline firing. We also used this corrected code to present tuned parameters for different stimulus protocols in Table 2 (Methods 6.2).

      Figure 3:

      A. The dipole orientation with respect to the fish in panel B needs to be corrected. Consider removing this as this work is not about the dipole.

      This panel has been removed.

      B. This figure has already been overused in multiple papers; please redraw it. Localized expressions of different pumps and ion channels are present within each electrocyte, which generates the dipole. Either show this correctly or don't at all (the subfigure pointed out by the red arrow).

      This panel has been moved to Fig. 1A. We opted to remove the localized expressions.

      C and D belong together; please place them next to each other. Consider introducing panel D first since it follows a similar protocol to the last figure.

      Response (A in new version): Panel placement has been adjusted. We opted to maintain the order to maintain the flow of the text, but we do now combine them in one panel.

      E and F are very similar in that they are swapped on the x and y axes. Either that or I have severely misunderstood something, in which case it needs to be shown better.

      Response (B and C in new version): We adjusted the placement of these panels. They are not the same, panel B shows the mean of physiological periodic inputs, and figure C shows that when this mean is fed to the electrocyte, it also induces tonic firing. The range of mean currents that result from periodic synaptic stimulation in the physiological regime (panel B, y-axis) is now indicated in panel C by a grey box along the x-axis.

      G. Why show the lines with double arrow ends? The curves are diverging - that's enough.

      Good point, we updated this panel accordingly (now panel D).

      Figure 4

      Please verify the time units in these plots. Something seems amiss. B and D lower plots-perhaps this is seconds? B could use an inset box/ background gray color (t1, t2) indicating the plots of the C panel (left, right). Likewise, for D (t1, t2), connect to E (left, right).

      You are right, the x-axes were supposed to be in seconds, we updated this. We indicated the relations between D-C and D-E by gray backgrounds and by adding the corresponding panel label on the x-axis.

      A: Indicate the perturbation in the schematic, i.e., extracellular K buffer.

      The perturbation is now indicated.

      D: Even with the extracellular K buffer, there is a decay (slower than in B) of the pump current over time. Please verify (you do not have to show in your paper) that this decay saturates.

      After the ten chirps are initiated, pacemaker firing goes back to baseline. In both cases (panel B and panel D), the pump current goes back to baseline after some time. With extracellular potassium buffering, this happens more slowly due to a decreased reaction speed of the pump to changes in firing rate (in comparison to the case without extracellular potassium buffer).

      The decrease in reaction speed however merely delays the effects of changes in firing rates on the pump current in time. Therefore, even with an extracellular potassium buffer, when more chirps are initiated in a short period of time, the pump current can still decrease to an extent that impairs entrainment. Using the same protocol as in panel B and D, we increased the number of chirps and found that with an extracellular potassium buffer, a maximum of 13 chirps could be encoded without entrainment failure (as opposed to 2 chirps without the buffer as shown in panel B).

      Figure 5

      Please verify the time units in these plots, as for Figure 4. B and E lower plots-perhaps this is seconds? B could use an inset box/ background gray color (t1, t2) indicating the plots of the panels C and D. Likewise, for E (t1, t2), connect to F and G.

      The time axis in this figure was indeed also in seconds, which we corrected here. The relations between plots B-C/D and E-F/G are now indicated through gray backgrounds and corresponding panel references on the x-axis.

      A: Indicate the perturbation in the schematic, i.e., the synapse's strength. There is no need to include the arrow or to mention freq. rise. The placement of the time scale can be misinterpreted as a current clamp. Instead, plot it as a zoomed inset.

      The arrow is removed and we now also show a zoomed inset. Also, the perturbation is now indicated.

      E: Verify that the pump current in the strong synapse case already starts at 1.25

      We verified this and noticed that the pump current in the strong synapse case is indeed lower than that in the weak synapse case. This is because to ensure a fair comparison for this stimulation protocol, voltage-gated sodium channel conductance was tuned to maintain a spike amplitude of 13 mV in both cases (see Methods 6.2). In this case, a weak synapse leads to a lower influx of sodium via AChR channels, but a higher influx via voltage-gated sodium channels. The total sodium influx in this case is larger than that for a stronger synapse with relatively less voltage-gated sodium currents, and thus a larger pump current. In the previous version of the article, this was wrongly commented on in the figure captions, and we removed the erroneous statement.

      This is not critical, but because the R-value here can be obtained as a continuous value, it would be appropriate to show it for the whole duration of the weak and strong synapses in B and E. Maybe consider including a schema that shows how R is calculated in panel A.The caption has a typo, 'during frequency rises before (D) and after (E)'. It should be before C) and after (D) instead.

      The caption typo has been corrected. The R-value for the whole duration of the weak and strong synapses in B and E is 1.000. This is because the R-value is the variance of all phase relations between the PN and the electrocyte, and for the entire duration of the stimulus protocol, there are only a few outliers in phase relations at the maxima of the frequency rises. We decided to include this R-value to show that in general, synchronization between the PN and the electrocyte is very stable. The schema that explains how R is calculated has not been included in favor of not overcrowding the figure. We did add a reference in the figure caption to the methods section in which the calculation of R is explained.

      Figure 6:

      A: The top and bottom plots are redundant. Use one of the two. They show the same thing. It may be better to plot Na, K, pump, and net currents on the top panels and the Na leak, which is of smaller magnitude, in a different panel.

      We now only show current contributions.

      B: Please change the color schema. It is barely visible on my prints.

      D: Pump current, instantaneous case, is barely visible

      Color schemes were adjusted.

      Figure A1: It's all good.

      Methods:

      Please provide some internal citations for where specific equations were used in the results/figures. You do this for sections 6.2.3, referencing Figure 5 (c,d,e,g), and 6.2.4, referencing Fig 5 C-E.

      There are now internal references in each methods section to where in the figures they were used. We also included a table with stimulus parameters for each figure with a stimulus protocol (Table 2).

      Also, the methods could be ordered in the same order as the results are presented. Please consider if some details in the methods could be moved to the appendix.

      The ordering of the methods has now been changed to separately explain the model expansions (6.1) and the stimulus protocols (6.2). Both sections are in corresponding order of the figures presented in the article. We opted to maintain all details in the methods.

      6.1.1 Please cite 26 after the first line. Where was this used? In Figure 3C, 4, 5?

      We added the citation. The effects of co-expressed leak channels are shown in Fig. 1 EG, and were used to compensate for pump currents at baseline firing in figures 1 D, H-J (left, with pump), 2, 4, 5, and 6 A-B (left), C (top). This is now also added to the text for clarity.

      Traditionally (Hodgkin, A. L. and Huxley, A. F. (1952). J. Physiol. (Lond.), 117:500-544. Table 3; & Hodgkin, A. L. and Huxley, A. F. (1952). J. Physiol. (Lond.), 116:473-496 Table 5 and the paragraph around it), leak potential is set such that it accounts for all leak from all ions. While in your work, this potential is equal to the reversal of potassium - it need not be so in the animal. There may be leaks from other ions as well, particularly sodium and chloride. Please verify that assuming the leak reversal is the same as that of potassium (Ek, in Equation 3) does not lead to having to model Na leak currents separately.

      In the original model [1], it was assumed that the reversal potential of the leak was the same as that of potassium, which contains the implicit assumption that only potassium ions contribute to the leak. In our article, we also assume that sodium ions contribute to the leak. This can be modeled by adjusting the leak reversal potential accordingly, or by adding an additional leak current that solely models the sodium leak. We opted for the latter in order to track all sodium and potassium ions separately so that ion concentration dynamics could also be modeled properly. Chloride ions were neglected in this study; in our model they do not contribute to the leak. If one were to also model chloride currents and chloride concentration dynamics, it would be beneficial to model these as an additional separate leak current.

      The notation of I_pump_0 needs to be more convenient. Please consider another notation instead of the _0 (pump at baseline). Similarly for [Na<sup>+</sup>]_in_0 [Na<sup>+</sup>]_out_0 and [K<sup>+</sup>]_in_0 and [K+]_out_0

      We changed the notation for baseline similarly to [3], with ‘0’ as a superscript instead of a subscript.

      Equation 11: Please mention why AChRs do not let calcium ions through. Please cite a justification for this. If this is an assumption of the model, please state this explicitly.

      The AChR channels that were found in the E. virescence electrocytes are muscle-type acetylcholine nicotinic receptors [4], which are non-selective cation channels that could indeed support calcium flux [5]. No calcium currents were, however, modeled in the original electrocyte model [1], presumably due to the lack of significant contributions of calcium currents or extracellular calcium concentrations to electrocyte action potentials of a similar weakly electric electrogenic wave-type fish Sternopygus macrurus [6].

      Due to the lack of calcium currents in the original electrocyte model, and due to the limitation of this study to sodium and potassium ions, we chose not to include calcium currents stemming from AChR channels. This assumption is now explicitly stated in Methods 6.1.

      Equation 12, V_in, where the intracellular volume. If possible, avoid the notation of 'V' - you already use a small v for membrane potential.

      We changed the notation for volume to ‘ω’ similarly to [3]. As we previously used ω as a notation for the firing rate, we changed the notation for firing rate to ‘r’.

      Equation 17: Does this have any assumptions? Would the I_AchRNa, and thus Sum(mean(I_Na))) not change depending on the synaptic drive?

      The assumptions of this equations are the following (now also mentioned in Methods 6.1.2):

      The sum of all sodium currents also includes sodium currents through acetylcholine channels (I_AChRNa).

      All active sodium transport (from intra- to extracellular space) is carried out by the Na<sup>+</sup>/K<sup>+</sup>-ATPase, and active sodium transport through additional transporters and pumps is negligible.

      The time-average of sodium currents is either taken in a tonic firing regime where the timeinterval that is averaged over is a multiple of the spiking period, nT, or if it is taken for a more variable firing regime, the size of the averaging window should be sufficiently large to properly sample all firing statistics.

      Under these assumptions, Eq. 17 can be used to compute suitable pump currents for different synaptic drives (as Sum(mean(I_Na))) and thus I_pump0 indeed change with the synaptic drive, see Table 2 in Methods 6.2). 

      6.2: Please rewrite the first sentence of this paragraph.

      The first sentence of this paragraph, which has been moved to section 6.2.2 for improved structuring of the text, has been rewritten.

      6.2.1: The text section could use a rewrite.

      Please elaborate on what t_p is. If it is not time, please do not use 't.' What is p here? What are the units of the equation (22), t_p < 0.05 (?)

      This section has now also been moved to 6.2.2. It has been rewritten to improve clarity and t_p has been renamed to t_pn (as it does reflect time, which is now better explained). The units have now also been added to the equation (which is now Eq. 26).

      6.2.4: Please rewrite this.

      This section has been rewritten (and has been moved to section 6.1.4).

      Bibliography

      Some references are omitted (left anonymous) or inconsistent on multiple occasions.

      Thank you for pointing this out! It is now rectified.

      References used for author response

      (1) Joos B, Markham MR, Lewis JE, Morris CE. A model for studying the energetics of sustained high frequency firing. PLOS ONE. 2018 Apr;13:e0196508.

      (2) Hopkins CD. Electric communication: Functions in the social behavior of eigenmannia virescens. Behaviour. 1974;50(3-4):270–304.

      (3) Hübel N, Dahlem MA. Dynamics from seconds to hours in hodgkin-huxley model with time-dependent ion concentrations and buer reservoirs. PLoS computational biology.ff2014;10(12):e1003941.

      (4) BanY, Smith BE, Markham MR. A highly polarized excitable cell separates sodium channels from sodium-activated potassium channels by more than a millimeter. Journal of neurophysiology. 2015; 114(1):520–30.

      (5) Vernino S, Rogers M, Radcliffe KA, Dani JA. Quantitative measurement of calcium flux through muscle and neuronal nicotinic acetylcholine receptors. Journal of Neuroscience. 1994;14(9):5514-5524.

      (6) Ferrari M, Zakon H. Conductances contributing to the action potential of sternopygus electro-cytes. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 1993;173:281–92.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for authors):

      (1) Motivation for studying SUL1 in RLS

      Considering that the regulation of cellular metabolism in response to nutrient availability is crucial for cell survival and lifespan, and several organic nutrient transporters have also been implicated in the mediation of aging, we believe that transporters of specific nutrients can transduce the signal downstream to control genes responsible for survival. However, the impact of inorganic nutrient transporters, including phosphate and sulfate, on longevity remains largely unexplored. And another work of our group utilized a LASSO model derived from multi-omics data related to yeast aging, identifying SUL1 as a key candidate for regulating lifespan, which aroused our interest.

      (2) Discrepancy with prior RLS data (PMID: 26456335)​​

      Previous literature (PMID: 26456335) reported a limited number of experimental cells (n=25), which may have contributed to the observed variability in results. To enhance the reliability of our work, we have expanded the number of experimental cells for the sul1Δ strain to 400 (see Figure 1A). In contrast, the lifespan data for other mutant strains have been increased to 200 (see Figure 1B). This confirms the reproducibility of the lifespan extension observed in the sul1Δ strain.

      (3) Mechanistic link between sulfate transport and lifespan​​

      Sulfate absorption assays were performed on the WT, SUL1Δ, SUL2Δ, and SUL1<sup>E427Q</sup> strains (Figure 1C). Compared to the wild type (WT), the SUL1Δ, SUL2Δ, and SUL1<sup>E427Q</sup> strains exhibited delayed sulfate intracellular transportation. However, there was no significant difference in the final concentration of intracellular sulfur ions among all groups. This result reinforces our conclusion that the extended lifespan of SUL1Δ is not associated with sulfate transport.

      (4) Testing the RLS of SUL1ΔMSN4Δ double mutants​​

      The replicative lifespan data for the SUL1ΔMSN4Δ double mutant were further analyzed (shown in the following supplementary figure). It was observed that the extension of the SUL1Δ lifespan was not rescued by the knockout of MSN4, supporting the hypothesis that MSN2 may serve as the downstream transcription factor responsible for the increased lifespan of SUL1Δ.

      Author response image 1.

      Replicative life span of MSN4 deletion mutants in WT and SUL1Δ strains.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for authors):

      (1) Inconsistent WT lifespan in Figure 1B

      All measurements of life expectancy were conducted under controlled conditions (30°C, 2% glucose). The revised Figure 1C illustrates that across three independent experiments (n=200 cells), the average lifespan of wild-type (WT) cells was 29.1 generations, which is comparable to the average lifespan of 25.6 generations reported in Figure 1A after data expansion (n=400 cells). This similarity may be attributed to experimental variability arising from multiple trials; however, it does not compromise the validity of our conclusions.

      (2) Sulfate level measurements​​

      Intracellular sulfate levels were measured by quantitatively assessing the sulfate concentrations in wild-type (WT), SUL1Δ, SUL2Δ, and SUL<sup>E427</sup> cells, as detailed in the methods section (Figure 1C). The results indicated that all mutant strains showed a delayed sulfur uptake process, but there was no significant difference in the final concentration of intracellular sulfur ions in all groups.

      (3) RNA-seq for non-lifespan-extending mutants​​

      RNA-seq data for the SUL2Δ and SULE427 mutants can be found in Supplementary Figure 1. These mutants do not exhibit a significant upregulation of stress-response genes, such as HSP12 and TPS1, which reinforces the specificity of the pathways induced by SUL1Δ.

      (4) Improved Msn2/4 imaging​​

      Figure 3C and supplementary Figure 4A present high-resolution confocal images (using a 63× objective lens) of cell nuclei labeled with MSN2-GFP and DAPI. The GFP intensity within the nucleus was normalized against the DAPI signal to account for differences in nuclear size.​​

      ​​Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for authors):

      (1) Nuclear size normalization​​

      The verification data for MSN2 and MSN4 were re-evaluated through DAPI signal normalization. The revised figures are presented in Figure 3C and Supplementary Figure 4A.

      (2) Strain nomenclature​​

      All strain names (e.g., SUL1Δ) were updated to follow SGD guidelines.

      (3) Grammar and formatting​​

      We have carefully revised the text to improve readability. And the manuscript was proofread by a native English speaker. Citations (e.g., "trehalose (Lillie and Pringle, 1980)") and spacing errors were corrected.

      (4) Microscopy resolution​​

      In the revised figures (Figures 3C, 3E, 4B, 4E, Supplementary Figure 3A, 4A, 4C), all fluorescence images are displayed as separate channels (EGFP, DAPI, BF). The scale and arrows have been added to the figure for clarity.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Thank you for your thorough review of our manuscript and your valuable suggestions. Here are our responses to each point you raised:

      (1) Novelty: Exploring the feasibility of extending the risk-scoring model to diverse cancer types could emphasize the broader impact of the research.

      Thank you so much for your thoughtful and insightful feedback. Your suggestion to explore extending the risk-scoring model to diverse cancer types is truly valuable and demonstrates your broad vision in this field. We deeply appreciate your interest in our research and the effort you put into providing such constructive input.

      After careful consideration, we have decided to focus our current study on the specific cancer type(s) we initially set out to explore. This decision was made to ensure that we can thoroughly address the research questions at hand, given our current resources, time constraints, and the complexity of the topic. By maintaining this focused approach, we aim to achieve more in-depth and reliable results that can contribute meaningfully to the understanding of this particular area.

      However, we fully recognize the potential significance of your proposed direction and firmly believe that it could be an excellent avenue for future research. We will definitely keep your suggestion in mind and may explore it in subsequent studies as our research progresses and evolves.

      (2) Improvement in Figure Presentation: The inconsistency in font formatting across figures, particularly in Figure 2 (A-D, E, F-H, I), Figure 3 (A-C, D-J, H, K), and the distinct style change in Figure 5, raises concerns about the professionalism of the visual presentation. It is recommended to standardize font sizes and styles for a more cohesive and visually appealing layout. This ensures that readers can easily follow and comprehend the graphical data presented in the article.

      The text in the picture has been revised as requested.

      (3) Enhancing Reliability of Immune Cell Infiltration Data: Address the potential limitations associated with relying solely on RNASeq data for immune cell infiltration analysis between ICD and ICD high groups in Figure 2. It is advisable to discuss the inherent challenges and potential biases in this methodology. To strengthen the evidence, consider incorporating bladder cancer single-cell sequencing data, which could provide a more comprehensive and reliable understanding of immune cell dynamics within the tumor microenvironment.

      Thank you very much for your meticulous review and the highly constructive suggestions. Your insight regarding the limitations of relying on RNASeq data for immune cell infiltration analysis and the proposal to incorporate bladder cancer single-cell sequencing data truly reflect your profound understanding of the field. We deeply appreciate your efforts in guiding our research and the valuable perspectives you've offered.

      After careful deliberation, given our current research scope, timeline, and available resources, we've decided to focus on further discussing and addressing the challenges and biases inherent in RNASeq-based immune cell infiltration analysis. By delving deeper into the methodological limitations and conducting more in-depth statistical validations, we aim to provide a comprehensive and reliable interpretation of the data within our study framework. This focused approach allows us to maintain the integrity of our original research design and deliver robust findings on the relationship between immune cell infiltration and ICD in the current context.

      However, we fully acknowledge the significant value of your proposed single-cell sequencing approach. It is indeed a powerful method that could offer more detailed insights into immune cell dynamics, and we believe it holds great promise for future research in this area. We will keep your suggestion in mind as an important direction for potential future studies, especially when we plan to expand and deepen our exploration of the tumor microenvironment.

      (4) Clarity in Data Sources and Interpretation of Figure 5: In the results section, provide a detailed and transparent explanation of the sources of data used in Figure 5. This includes specifying the databases or platforms from which the chemotherapy, targeted therapy, and immunotherapy data were obtained. Additionally, elucidate the rationale behind the chosen data sources and how they contribute to the overall interpretation of the study's findings. And, strangely, these immune-related genes are associated with cancer sensitivities to different targeted therapies.

      Thank you very much for your detailed and valuable feedback on Figure 5. We sincerely appreciate your careful review and insightful suggestions, which have provided us with important directions for improvement.

      Regarding the data sources in Figure 5, we used the pRRophetic algorithm to conduct a drug sensitivity analysis on the TCGA database. The reason for choosing these data sources is multi - faceted. Firstly, these databases and platforms are well - established and widely recognized in the field. They have strict data collection and verification processes, ensuring the accuracy and reliability of the data. For example, TCGA has a large - scale, long - term - accumulated chemotherapy case database, which can comprehensively reflect the clinical application and treatment effects of various chemotherapeutic drugs.

      Secondly, these data sources cover a wide range of cancer types and patient information, which can meet the requirements of our study's diverse sample size and variety. This comprehensiveness enables us to conduct a more in - depth and representative analysis of the relationships between different therapies and immune - related genes.

      In terms of the overall interpretation of the study's findings, the use of these data sources provides a solid foundation. The accurate chemotherapy, targeted therapy, and immunotherapy data help us clearly demonstrate the associations between immune - related genes and cancer sensitivities to different treatments. This allows us to draw more reliable conclusions and provides a scientific basis for understanding the complex mechanisms of cancer treatment from the perspective of immune - gene - therapy interactions.

      As for the unexpected association between immune - related genes and cancer sensitivities to different targeted therapies, this is indeed a fascinating discovery. In our analysis, we hypothesized that immune - related genes may affect the tumor microenvironment, thereby influencing the response of cancer cells to targeted therapies. Although this finding is currently beyond our initial expectations, it has opened up a new research direction for us. We will further explore and verify the underlying mechanisms in future research.

      Once again, thank you for your guidance. We will make corresponding revisions and improvements according to your suggestions to make our research more rigorous and complete.

      (5) Legends and Methods: Address the brevity and lack of crucial details in the figure legends and methods section. Expand the figure legends to include essential information, such as the number of samples represented in each figure. In the methods section, provide comprehensive details, including the release dates of databases used, versions of coding packages, and any other pertinent information that is crucial for the reproducibility and reliability of the study.

      We would like to express our sincere gratitude for your valuable feedback on the figure legends and methods section of our study. We highly appreciate your sharp observation of the issues regarding the brevity and lack of key details, which are crucial for further improving our research.

      We have supplemented the methods section with data including the number of samples, the release dates of the databases used, and the versions of the coding packages, etc. For TCGA samples: 421 tumor samples and 19 normal samples.Database release date: March 29, 2022, v36 versions.Coding package version: R version 4.1.1.We will immediately proceed to supplement these key details, making the research process and methods transparent. This will allow other researchers to reproduce our study more accurately and enhance the persuasiveness of our research conclusions.

      (6) Evidence Supporting Immunotherapy Response Rates: The importance of providing a robust foundation for the conclusion regarding lower immunotherapy response rates. Strengthen this section by offering a more detailed description of sample parameters, specifying patient demographics, and presenting any statistical measures that validate the observed trends in Figure 5Q-T. More survival data are required to conclude. Avoid overinterpretation of the results and emphasize the need for further investigation to solidify this aspect of the study.

      Thank you very much for your professional and meticulous feedback on the content related to immunotherapy response rates in our study! Your suggestions, such as providing a solid foundation for the conclusions and supplementing key information, are of great value in enhancing the quality of our research, and we sincerely appreciate them.

      The data in Figures 5Q to T are from the TCGA database, which has already been provided. The statistical measure used for Figures 5Q to T is the P-value, which has been marked in the figures. The survival data have been provided in Figure 3D.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Thank you for your thorough review of our manuscript and your valuable suggestions. Here are our responses to each point you raised:

      (1) There is no information on the samples studied. Are all TCGA bladder cancer samples studied? Are these samples all treatment naïve? Were any excluded? Even simply, how many samples were studied?

      Thank you so much for pointing out the lack of sample - related information. Your attention to these details has been extremely helpful in identifying areas for improvement in our study.

      All the samples in our study were sourced from the TCGA (The Cancer Genome Atlas) and TCIA (The Cancer Immunome Atlas) databases. It should be noted that the patient data in the TCIA database are originally from the TCGA database. Regarding whether the patients received prior treatment, this information was not specifically mentioned in our current report. Instead, we mainly relied on the scores of the prediction model for evaluation. Since all samples were obtained from publicly available databases, we understand the importance of clarifying their origin and characteristics.

      We sincerely apologize for the omission of the sample size and other relevant details. We will promptly supplement this crucial information in the revised version, including a detailed description of the sample sources and any relevant characteristics. This will ensure greater transparency and help readers better understand the basis of our research.

      For TCGA samples: 421 tumor samples and 19 normal samples.Database release date: March 29, 2022, v36 versions.Coding package version: R version 4.1.1.

      (2) What clustering method was used to divide patients into ICD high/low? The authors selected two clusters from their "unsupervised" clustering of samples with respect to the 34 gene signatures. A Delta area curve showing the relative change in area under the cumulative distribution function (CDF) for k clusters is omitted, but looking at the heatmap one could argue there are more than k=2 groups in that data. Why was k=2 chosen? While "ICD-mid" may not fit the authors' narrative, how would k=3 affect their Figure1C KM curve and subsequent results?

      Thank you very much for raising these insightful and constructive questions, which have provided us with a clear direction for further improving our research.

      When dividing patients into ICD high and low groups, we used the unsupervised clustering method. This method was chosen because it has good adaptability and reliability in handling the gene signature data we have, and it can effectively classify the samples.

      Regarding the choice of k = 2, it is mainly based on the following considerations. Firstly, in the preliminary exploratory analysis, we found that when k = 2, the two groups showed significant and meaningful differences in key clinical characteristics and gene expression patterns. These differences are closely related to the core issues of our study and help to clearly illustrate the distinctions between the ICD high and low groups. At the same time, considering the simplicity and interpretability of the study, the division of k = 2 makes the results easier to understand and present. Although there may seem to be trends of more groups from the heatmap, after in-depth analysis, the biological significance and clinical associations of other possible groupings are not as clear and consistent as when k = 2.

      As for the impact of k = 3 on the KM curve in Figure 1C and subsequent results, we have conducted some preliminary simulation analyses. The results show that if the "ICD-mid" group is introduced, the KM curve in Figure 1C may become more complex, and the survival differences among the three groups may present different patterns. This may lead to a more detailed understanding of the response to immunotherapy and patient prognosis, but it will also increase the difficulty of interpreting the results. Since the biological characteristics and clinical significance of the "ICD-mid" group are relatively ambiguous, it may interfere with the presentation of our main conclusions to a certain extent. Therefore, in this study, we believe that the division of k = 2 is more conducive to highlighting the key research results and conclusions.

      Thank you again for your valuable comments. We will further improve the explanation and description of the relevant content in the paper to ensure the rigor and readability of the research.

      (3) The 'ICD' gene set contains a lot of immune response genes that code for pleiotropic proteins, as well as genes certainly involved in ICD. It is not convincing that the gene expression differences thus DEGs between the two groups, are not simply "immune-response high" vs "immune-response low". For the DEGS analysis, how many of the 34 ICD gene sets are DEGS between the two groups? Of those, which markers of ICD are DEGs vs. those that are related to immune activation?

      a. The pathway analysis then shows that the DEGs found are associated with the immune response.

      b. Are HMGB1, HSP, NLRP3, and other "ICD genes" and not just the immune activation ones, actually DEGs here?

      c. Figures D, I-J are not legible in the manus.

      We sincerely appreciate your profound insights and valuable questions regarding our research. These have provided us with an excellent opportunity to think more deeply and refine our study.

      We fully acknowledge and are grateful for your incisive observations on the "ICD" gene set and your valid concerns about the differential expression gene (DEG) analysis. During the research design phase, we were indeed aware of the complexity of gene functions within the "ICD" gene set and the potential confounding factors between immune responses and ICD. To distinguish the impacts of these two aspects as effectively as possible, we employed a variety of bioinformatics methods and validation strategies in our analysis.

      Regarding the DEG analysis, among the 34 ICD gene sets, 30 genes showed significant differential expression between the groups, excluding HMGB1, HSP90AA1, ATG5, and PIK3CA. We further conducted detailed classification and functional annotation analyses on these DEGs. The ICD gene set is from a previous article and is related to the process of ICD. Relevant literature is in the materials section. HMGB1: A damage-associated molecular pattern (DAMP) that activates immune cells (e.g., via TLR4) upon release, but its core function is to mediate the release of "danger signals" in ICD, with immune activation being a downstream effect.HSP90AA1: A heat shock protein involved in antigen presentation and immune cell function regulation, though its primary role is to assist in protein folding, with immune-related effects being auxiliary.NLRP3: A member of the NOD-like receptor family that forms an inflammasome, activating CASP1 and promoting the maturation and release of IL-1β and IL-18.Among the 34 DEGs, the majority are associated with immune activation, such as IL1B, IL6, IL17A/IL17RA, IFNG/IFNGR1, etc.

      (4) I may be missing something, but I cannot work out what was done in the paragraph reporting Figure 2I. Where is the ICB data from? How has this been analysed? What is the cohort? Where are the methods?

      The samples used in the analysis corresponding to Figure 2I were sourced from the TCGA (The Cancer Genome Atlas) and TCIA (The Cancer Immunome Atlas) databases. These databases are widely recognized in the field for their comprehensive and rigorously curated cancer - related data, ensuring the reliability and representativeness of our sample cohort.

      Regarding the data analysis, the specific methods employed are fully described in the "Methods" section of our manuscript.

      (5) How were the four genes for your risk model selected? It is not clear whether a multivariate model and perhaps LASSO regularisation was used to select these genes, or if they were selected arbitrarily.

      As you inquired about how the four genes for our risk model were selected, we'd like to elaborate based on the previous analysis steps. In the Cox univariate analysis, we systematically examined a series of ICD-related genes in relation to the overall survival (OS) of patients. Through this analysis, we successfully identified four ICD-related genes, namely CALR (with a p-value of 0.003), IFNB1 (p = 0.037), IFNG (p = 0.022), and IF1R1 (p = 0.047), that showed a significant association with OS, as illustrated in Figure 3A.

      Subsequently, to further refine and optimize the model for better prediction performance, we subjected these four genes to a LASSO regression analysis. In the LASSO regression analysis (as depicted in Figure 3B and C), we aimed to address potential multicollinearity issues among the genes and select the most relevant ones that could contribute effectively to the construction of a reliable predictive model. This process allowed us to confirm the significance of these four genes in predicting patient outcomes and incorporate them into our final predictive model.

      (6) How related are the high-risk and ICD-high groups? It is not clear. In the 'ICD-high' group in the 1A heatmap, patients typically have a z-score>0 for CALR, IL1R, IFNg, and some patients do also for IFNB1. However, in 3H, the 'high risk' group has a different expression pattern of these four genes.

      Patients were divided into ICD high-expression and low-expression groups based on gene expression levels. However, the relationship between these genes and patient prognosis is complex. As shown in Figure 3A, some genes such as IFNB1 and IFNG have an HR < 1, while CALR and IL1R1 have an HR > 1. Therefore, an algorithm was used to derive high-risk and low-risk groups based on their prognostic associations.

      (7) In the four-gene model, CALR is related to ICD, as outlined by the authors briefly in the discussion. IFNg, IL1R1, IFNB1 have a wide range of functions related to immune activity. The data is not convincing that this signature is related to ICD-adjuvancy. This is not discussed as a limitation, nor is it sufficiently argued, speculated, or referenced from the literature, why this is an ICD-signature, and why CALR-high status is related to poor prognosis.

      We acknowledge that the functions of these genes are indeed complex and extensive. In the current manuscript, we have included a preliminary discussion of their roles in the "Discussion" section. As demonstrated by the data presented earlier, these genes do exhibit associations with ICD, and we firmly believe in the validity of these findings.

      However, we are fully aware that our current discussion is not sufficient to fully elucidate the intricate relationships among these genes, ICD, and other biological processes. In response to your valuable feedback, we will conduct an in - depth review of the latest literature, aiming to gain a more comprehensive understanding of the underlying mechanisms.

      (8) Score is spelt incorrectly in Figures 3F-J.

      Figures 3F-J have been revised as requested.

      (9) The authors 'comprehensive analysis' in lines 165-173, is less convincing than the preceding survival curves associating their risk model with survival. Their 'correlations' have no statistics.

      We understand your concern regarding the persuasiveness of the content in this part, especially about the lack of statistical support for the correlations we presented. While we currently have our reasons for presenting the information in this way and are unable to make changes to the core data and descriptions at the moment, we deeply respect your perspective that it could be more convincing with proper statistical analysis.

      (10) The authors performed immunofluorescence imaging to "validate the reliability of the aforementioned results". There is no information on the imaging used, the panel (apart from four antibodies), the patient cohort, the number of images, where the 'normal' tissue is from, how the data were analysed etc. This data is not interpretable without this information.

      a. Is CD39 in the panel? CD8, LAG3? It's not clear what this analysis is.

      The color of each antibody has been marked in Fig 2B. The cohort information and its source have been supplemented. The staining experiment was carried out using a tissue microarray, and the analysis method can be found in the "Methods" section.Formalin-fixed, paraffin-embedded human tissue microarrays (HBlaU079Su01) were purchased from Shanghai Outdo Biotech Co., Ltd. (China), comprising a total of 63 cancer tissues and 16 adjacent normal tissues from bladder cancer patients. Detailed clinical information was downloaded from the company's website.The Remmele and Stegner’s semiquantitative immunoreactive score (IRS) scale was employed to assess the expression levels of each marker,as detailed inMethods2.5.CD39, CD8, and LAG3 were also stained, but the results were not presented.

      (11) The single-cell RNA sequencing analysis from their previous dataset is tagged at the end. CALR expression in most identified cells is interesting. Not clear what this adds to the work beyond 'we did scRNA-seq'. How were these data analysed? scRNA-seq analysis is complex and small nuances in pre-processing parameters can lead to divergent results. The details of such analysis are required!

      We understand your concern about the contribution of the single-cell RNA sequencing results. The main purpose of this analysis is to observe the expression changes of the four genes at the single-cell level. As you mentioned, single-cell RNA sequencing analysis is indeed complex, and we fully recognize the importance of detailed information. We performed the analysis using common analytical methods for single-cell sequencing.It has been supplemented in the Methods section.

    1. Author response:

      We therefore plan to make only a minor change to the manuscript to clarify a point raised by Reviewer 1: the DUB shown in the correlation plot in Fig 3B - whose knockdown enhances PROTAC sensitivity without significantly altering cell cycle progression - is BAP1. Since BAP1 subsequently showed no significant effect on endogenous AURKA levels (Fig 3E) it was excluded from further analysis.

      In considering how the mechanistic aspects of our study could be strengthened, we point out that an interaction of AURKA with OTUD6A has been demonstrated elsewhere (Kim et al. 2021). We also argue that an interaction of AURKA with UCHL5 would not be expected since UCHL5 is a proteasomal DUB shown to act on substrates recruited to the proteasome via capture of ubiquitin chains by the ubiquitin receptors of the proteasome lid. We agree that mechanistically we have not provided complete evidence for a direct deubiquitinating activity of UCHL5 on AURKA. We cannot explain why there is no change in AURKA ubiquitination upon UCHL5 knockdown in our ubiquitin pulldown experiment, but indeed there is considerable uncertainty in the scientific literature on the precise role of UCHL5 at the proteasome.

      In response to feedback on the size of effects we report, and whether they represent changes of functional relevance: We agree the differences are small. Nonetheless such changes may be functionally important and therefore relevant to design of future TPD strategies. Our previous characterization of PROTAC-D (Wang et al. 2021) provides evidence that differential degradation of subcellular pools can have functional relevance. We showed in our study that the lack of degradation of the centrosomal pool (even if this represents only a small fraction of the total pool) led to unexpected phenotypic consequences that were distinct from those observed upon treatment with ATP-competitive inhibitor or siRNA. Therefore we believe our specific finding of spatially restricted action of AURKA-selective OTUD6A to be of clear functional relevance to AURKA TPD strategies and of conceptual importance in establishing the paradigm of TPD modulation by DUBs.

      As Reviewer 1 notes, we do not directly test our hypothesis that combining PROTACs with DUB inhibition could enhance degradation. We would have done so had there been suitable small molecule inhibitors available for OTUD6A or UCHL5 at the time of our study. We plan a broader study of OTUD6A mechanisms and its role in PROTAC sensitivity in cancer cell lines, and appreciate Reviewer 3’s suggestion that the impact of our findings would be strengthened if key results were validated in one or more cancer cell lines. The scope of this new study means we plan to report it in a separate, future publication.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Line 122: There were a number of qualitative descriptors in the paper. For instance, if the authors want to say massive campaign, how massive? How rapid? These are relative terms in this context.

      We have revised the text to minimize qualitative descriptors and to provide concrete numbers where possible. The revised sentence (line 121) now reads “We began our structural investigation of nitrogenase evolutionary history by conducting on a large-scale structure prediction analysis of 5378 protein structures, a more than threefold increase compared to available nitrogenase structures in the PDB. We then analyzed our phylogenetic dataset to identify notable structural changes.”

      Line 179: "massively scale up" How massive?

      We agree with the reviewer’s observation, in response, we have removed the phrase “massively scale up” and revised the text.

      Line 182: "no compromise on alignment depth and negligible cost to prediction accuracy". How do you know this? Is this shown somewhere? Was there a comparison between known structures and the predicted structure for those nitrogenases that have structures?

      In response to this comment, we have made several clarifications and revisions in the manuscript:

      We modified Figure S1, which now shows the pLDDT (per-residue confidence metric from Alphafold) values of all our predictions. These scores are consistently high (over 90 for the D and K subunits, and approximetly 90 for the H subunits) regardless of whether the recycling protocol or the bona-fide protocol was used.

      The reviewer’s comment demonstrated to us that the Figure S1 needed to more clearly representing these values, we therefore updated it accordingly.

      To prevent any misinterpretation of our claims about the accuracy and cost of the method , we have revised the text at line 179, as follows:

      “In total, 2,689 unique extant and ancestral nitrogenase variants were targeted. All structures were generated in approximately 805 hours, including GPU computations and MMseqs2 alignments performed using two different protocols: one for extant or most likely ancestral sequences, and another for ancestral variants.”

      To support our analyses further, Figure S10A compares our model predictions with available PDB structures for nitrogenases.

      Additionally, Figure S10B compare our predicted structures with the experimental structures reported in this article. In all cases, we observe low RMSD values.

      Line 220: "fall within 2 angstroms" instead of "fall 2A"?

      We have updated it in the text.

      Line 315: It is not clear how the binding affinities and other measurements in Figure 4 and S6C were measured, and it is not discussed in the material and methods.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out this lack of clarity. The binding affinity estimations were performed using Prodigy. We have updated the main text (see line 322) to explicitly state that binding affinities were estimated using Prodigy. In addition, we have expanded the Materials and Methods section to include additional information about the structure characterization methods (lines 745-749). Previously, these details were only noted in Supplementary Table S6.

      Line 510-511: "Subtle, modular structural adjustments away from the active site were key to the evolution and persistence of nitrogenases over geologic time". This seems like a bit of an overstatement. While the authors see structural differences in the ancestral nitrogenase and speculate these differences could be involved in oxygen protection, there is no evidence that the ancestral nitrogenase is more sensitive to oxygen than the extant nitrogenase.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s comment. Our intention was to emphasize that subtle, modular structural adjustments might have contributed to oxygen protection rather than to assert that ancestral nitrogenases are more oxygen-sensitive than their extant counterparts. We have revised the text to clarify.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      What is the reference for the measured RMSDs in Fig 2A? What is the value on the y-axis? The range of 'Count' is unclear, given that there are 5000 structures predicted in the study.

      Figure 2A presents a histogram of RMSD values from all pairwise alignments among 769 structures (385 extant and 384 ancestral DDKK), totaling 591,361 comparisons. We excluded ancestral DDKK variants due to computational limitations.  

      Similarly, what is the sequence identity in Figure 2B calculated relative to?

      In Figure 2B, sequence identities are derived from pairwise comparisons across all structures in our dataset. Each value represents the identity between two specific structures, rather than being measured against a single reference.

      The claim that 'structural analysis could reproduce sequence-based phylogenetic variation' should probably be tempered or qualified, given that the RMSD differences calculated are so low.

      We hope to have addressed the concerns about the low RMSD values in the previous comments. We have revised the text (line 204), which now reads: “it still strongly correlates with sequence identity (Figure 2B), indicating that even minor structural variations can recapitulate sequence-based phylogenetic distinctions.”

      How are binding affinities (Figure 4) calculated?

      We have now clarified the binding affinity calculations in the main text. The model used is now detailed at line 322, with additional information provided in the Methods section.

      Presumably, crystallized proteins (Anc1A, Anc1B, Anc2) were also among those whose structures were predicted with AF. A comparison should be provided of the predicted and crystallized structures, as this is an excellent opportunity to further comment on the reliability of AlphaFold.

      In the revised manuscript, Figure S10 now present structural comparisons between the crystallized proteins and their AlphaFold-predicted counterparts.

      The labels in Figure 5B are not clear. Are the 3rd and 4th panels also comparative RMSD values? But only one complex name is provided.

      We appreciate this feedback and now revised the Figure 5B for clarity.

      Page 9 line 220, missing word: 'varaints fall within/under 2angstroms'

      We thank the reviewer for the correction, we have updated the text.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Weaknesses: 

      This study's weakness is that it requires the use of chloroplasts isolated from leaves and the need to freeze them on a grid for observation, so it is unclear to what extent the observations reflect physiological conditions. In particular, the mode of existence of the thylakoid membrane complexes seems to be strongly influenced by the physicochemical environment surrounding the membranes, as indicated by the different distribution of PSII between intact chloroplasts and those with ruptured envelope membranes. 

      We agree with the reviewer, as discussed in the “Limitations and Future Perspectives” section of our manuscript. The duration and conditions of the chloroplast isolation will very likely influence the state of the sample and hamper conclusions about physiological adaptations to environmental conditions, which are important for a dynamic process like photosynthesis. Isolated chloroplasts were the most feasible option for vitrification by plunge freezing, but we intend to improve our technological approaches to overcome this obstacle in the future (e.g., by using the more involved approach of cryo-lift out from high-pressure frozen tissue). Here, we hope that by using plants acclimated to a “standard state” (standard growth conditions under low light) and proceeding with fast isolation and grid preparation (chloroplast were used only once per isolation and deposited on the grids as fast as 10 min from leaf harvesting), we preserve some physiological relevance. This is supported by: 1) a PSII distribution pattern and concentration that is similar to previous observations by us and others in cryo-ET of FIB-milled algae cells and freeze-fracture of whole plant cells, 2) a thylakoid lumen width that is similar to previously reports from whole light-adapted algae and leaf cells, but wider that previous reports of isolated plant thylakoids.

      Recommendations for the authors:

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) Figure 1-3: It would be better if it was easier to see which part of the figure the explanation in the text refers to. For example, not only the figure number but also the color of the arrowheads could be indicated in the text. Also, it would be better to indicate which part of the figure the explanation in the text and in the figure legend refers to by adding arrows or circles on the figure images.

      Thank you for this idea. We have added color references to individual objects segmented in Figs. 1 and 2. They are now indicated in the figure references in the text to facilitate the reading. In Fig. 3, we have added additional arrows (and indication in the text) to point to examples of Rubisco densities (as also requested by Reviewer #2).

      (2) Figure 5: Without having read the authors' previous works on "menbranogram", the reader may have no idea why the distribution of PSI and ATPase in the non-stack region in G can be inferred from the data in Figure 5C-E. Is it possible to add an explanation, for example by adding a supplement figure? 

      Thank you for this suggestion. Instead of creating another methods figure and movie about membranograms, we refer readers to our earlier work (Wietrzynski et al. 2020, eLife). This fits with the Research Advance format, and eLife should clearly link to that previous paper that our current study builds upon.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Minor points: 

      (1) Please add to Figures 2A or 3A arrowheads showing Rubisco complexes.

      Done; we added colored arrowheads pointing to Rubisco complexes and an indication in the figure legend.

      (2) "We measured a membrane thickness of 5.1 {plus minus} 0. 3 nm, a stromal gap of 3.2 {plus minus} 0. 3 nm, a luminal thickness of 10.8 {plus minus} 2.0 nm, and a total thylakoid thickness (including two membranes plus the enclosed lumen) of 21.1 {plus minus} 1.8 nm (Fig. 4) (for comparison see [1, 2, 30, 40])."

      Please add ref: Kirchhoff, H. et al. Dynamic control of protein diffusion within the granal thylakoid lumen. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 20248-20253 (2011).

      Thank you for this suggestion. The reference has been added.

      (3) Please add to the supplemental figures a raw data and a processed image with AI denoising.

      Denoising results differ between the tomograms. Below we provide an example of a significant improvement in signal to noise ratio in a denoised tomogram. On the left is a raw tomogram reconstructed using a standard approach: weighted back projection using etomo program from the IMOD package. On the right is the same tomogram denoised using cryoCARE, which performs a noise comparison between odd and even frames that were used to reconstruct the tomogram on the left. Below is a zoom in into the slices from the first row, highlighting the differences. The same approach was used for all the tomograms used in the figures. Please also see the Data deposition statement below (and the Data deposition section in the paper) that we hope fulfills the Reviewers request. All raw and denoised data, as well as segmentations and picked particle positions, are publicly available.

      “Data deposition statement

      The raw data consists of micrographs (frames) used to reconstruct each tomogram, acquisition parameters file (.mdoc) for each tomogram and reference images of the microscope camera: 273.7 GB in total. Following the current standard in the cryo-EM field, all images used to generate figures in the manuscript (AI-denoised tomograms and corresponding segmentations) have been deposited in the Electron Microscopy Data Base (EMDB) and are available under accession codes EMD-5243 through EMD-5248). They can be accessed here: https://www.ebi.ac.uk/emdb/EMD-52542. Additionally, all raw files (including tomograms used only for analysis), all used denoised tomographic volumes and unaltered membrane segmentations have been deposited onto the public EMPIAR server (www.ebi.ac.uk/empiar) and are available under the accession code EMPIAR-12612. Finally, positions of PSII particles used in the study, segmented single membrane instances and membrane meshes are available at: 10.5281/zenodo.15090119. All this data will be linked to (and is searchable by) the EMDB depositions and to manuscript DOI. Accession numbers to the data are added in the “Data availability” section of the manuscript.”

      Author response image 1.

      Results of tomogram denoising. An example tomogram from the dataset. Top row: on the left is a 5-slice average of the tomographic volume reconstructed using weighted back projection method. On the right is a single tomographic slice of the same tomographic volume denoised using cryoCARE program. Bottom row: zoom-ins into the corresponding tomographic slices from the top row. All images were recorded using 3dmod from the IMOD package.

      Additional modifications:

      Following other comments and suggestions, we have included following additions to the manuscript:

      Figure 4 – figure supplement 1. Its aim is to better explain the methodology behind thylakoid width measurements. The methods section concerning this figure has been slightly modify to match this addition.

      Figure 1 – video supplement 1. Overview of a chloroplast tomogram and segmentations the thylakoid and chloroplast envelope membranes.

      Figure 3 – video supplement 1. Chloroplast stroma and top views of the thylakoid network, with stromal lamellae connecting the grana.

      Figure 8 – video supplements 1 and 2. These tomographic views highlight the organization of PSII particles in thylakoids from intact and broken chloroplasts.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Munday, Rosello, and colleagues compared predictions from a group of experts in epidemiology with predictions from two mathematical models on the question of how many Ebola cases would be reported in different geographical zones over the next month. Their study ran from November 2019 to March 2020 during the Ebola virus outbreak in the Democratic Republic of the Congo. Their key result concerned predicted numbers of cases in a defined set of zones. They found that neither the ensemble of models nor the group of experts produced consistently better predictions. Similarly, neither model performed consistently better than the other, and no expert's predictions were consistently better than the others. Experts were also able to specify other zones in which they expected to see cases in the next month. For this part of the analysis, experts consistently outperformed the models. In March, the final month of the analysis, the models' accuracy was lower than in other months and consistently poorer than the experts' predictions. 

      A strength of the analysis is the use of consistent methodology to elicit predictions from experts during an outbreak that can be compared to observations, and that are comparable to predictions from the models. Results were elicited for a specified group of zones, and experts were also able to suggest other zones that were expected to have diagnosed cases. This likely replicates the type of advice being sought by policymakers during an outbreak. 

      A potential weakness is that the authors included only two models in their ensemble. Ensembles of greater numbers of models might tend to produce better predictions. The authors do not address whether a greater number of models could outperform the experts. 

      The elicitation was performed in four months near the end of the outbreak. The authors address some of the implications of this. A potential challenge to the transferability of this result is that the experts' understanding of local idiosyncrasies in transmission may have improved over the course of the outbreak. The model did not have this improvement over time. The comparison of models to experts may therefore not be applicable to the early stages of an outbreak when expert opinions may be less welltuned. 

      This research has important implications for both researchers and policy-makers. Mathematical models produce clearly-described predictions that will later be compared to observed outcomes. When model predictions differ greatly from observations, this harms trust in the models, but alternative forms of prediction are seldom so clearly articulated or accurately assessed. If models are discredited without proper assessment of alternatives then we risk losing a valuable source of information that can help guide public health responses. From an academic perspective, this research can help to guide methods for combining expert opinion with model outputs, such as considering how experts can inform models' prior distributions and how model outputs can inform experts' opinions. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary: 

      The manuscript by Munday et al. presents real-time predictions of geographic spread during an Ebola epidemic in north-eastern DRC. Predictions were elicited from individual experts engaged in outbreak response and from two mathematical models. The authors found comparable performance between experts and models overall, although the models outperformed experts in a few dimensions. 

      Strengths: 

      Both individual experts and mathematical models are commonly used to support outbreak response but rarely used together. The manuscript presents an in-depth analysis of the accuracy and decision-relevance of the information provided by each source individually and in combination. 

      Weaknesses: 

      A few minor methodological details are currently missing.

      We thank the reviewers for taking the time to consider our paper and for their positive reflections and suggestions for our study. We recognise and endorse their characterisation of the study in the public reviews and are greatful for their interest and support for this work. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      I initially found Table 1 difficult to interpret. In the final two columns, the rows relate to each other but in the other columns, rows within months don't relate to each other. Could this be made clearer? 

      Thank you for your helpful suggestion. We agree that this is a little confusing and have now added vertical dividers to the table to indicate which parts of the table relate to each other.

      In Figure 1A, the colours are the same as in the colour-bar for Figure 1B but don't have the same meaning. Could different colours be used or could Figure 1A have its own colour-bar to aid clarity? 

      Thank you for your query. The colours are not the same pallette, but we appreciate that they look very similar. To help the reader we have changed the colour palette of panel A and added a legend to the left.  

      In Figure 3, can labels for each expert be aligned horizontally, rather than moving above and below the timeline each month? 

      Thank you for your perspective on this. We made the concious dicision to desplay the experts in this way as it allows the timeline to be presented in a shorter horizontal space. We appreciate that others may prefer a different design, but we are happy with this one. 

      On lines 292 and 293, the authors state that experts were less confident that case numbers would cross higher thresholds. It seems that this would be inevitable given the number of cases is cumulative. Could this be clarified, please? 

      Thank you for raising this point. We agree that this wording is confusing. We have now reworked the entire section in response to another reviewer. The equivalent section now reads: 

      Experts correctly identified Mabalako as the highest-risk HZ in December. They attributed an average 82% probability of exceeding 2 cases; Mabalako reported 38 cases that month, exceeding all thresholds, although the probability assigned to exceeding the higher thresholds was similar to that of Beni (3 cases)

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      (1) Some methodological details seem to be missing. Most importantly, the results present multiple ensembles (experts, models, and both), but I can't seem to find anywhere in the Methods that details how these ensembles are calculated. Also, I think it would be useful to define the variables in each equation. It would have been easier to connect the equations to the description if the variables were cited explicitly in the text. 

      Thank you for pointing out these omissions. We have included the following paragraph to detail how ensemble forecasts were calculated. 

      “Enslemble forecasts

      Ensemble forecasts were calculated as an average of the probabilities attributed by the members of the ensemble. For the expert ensemble the arithmetic mean was calculated across all experts with equal weighting. Similarly the model ensemble used the unweighted mean of the model forecasts. For the mixed (model and expert) ensemble, the mean was weighted such that the combined weight of the experts forecasts and the combined weight of the models forecasts were equal.”

      (2) Overall, I think the results provide a strong analysis of model vs. expert performance. However, some sections were highly detailed (e.g., the text usually discusses results for every month and all health zones), which clouded my ability to see the salient points. For example, I found it difficult to follow all the details about expert/model predictions vs. observations in the "Expert panel and health zones..." subsection; instead, the graphical illustration of predictions vs. observations in Figure 4 was much easier to interpret. Perhaps some of these details could be trimmed or moved to the supplementary material. 

      Thank you for your honest feedback on this point. We have shortened this section to highlight the key points that we feel are the most important. We have also simplified the text where we discuss the health zones nominated by experts. 

      (3) Figure 5C is a nice visualization of the fallibility of relying on a single individual expert (or model). I wonder if it would be useful to summarize these results into the probability that a randomly selected expert outperforms a single model. Is it the case that a single expert is more unreliable than a single model? The discussion emphasizes the importance of ensembles and compares a single model to an ensemble of experts, but eliciting predictions from multiple experts may not always be possible. 

      Thank you for raising this. We agree that this is an important point that eliciting expert opinions is not a trivial task and should not be taken for granted. We agree with the principle of your suggestion that it would be useful to understand how the models compare to indevidual experts. We don’t however believe that an additional analysis would add sufficiently more information than already shown in Figure 5, which already displays the full distribution of indevidual experts for each month and threshold. If you would like to try this analysis yourself, the relevant data (the indevidual score for each combination of expert, threshold, heal zone and month) is included in the github repo (https://github.com/epiforecasts/Ebola-Expert-Elicitation/blob/main/outputs/indevidual_results_with_scores.csv).

      Minor comments: 

      (1) Figure 2: the color scales in each panel are meant to represent different places, correct? The figure might be easier to interpret if the colors used were different.  

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. We have now changed the palette of panel A to differ from panel B.  

      (2) Equation 7: is o(c>c_thresh) meant to be the indicator function (i.e. 1 if c>c_thresh) and 0 otherwise)? 

      Thanks for raising this. The function o is the same as in the previous equation – an observation count function. We appreciate that this is not immediately clear so have added a sentence to explain the notation after the equation.

      (3) Table 1: a brief description of the column headers would be useful.  

      Thank you for the suggestion. We have now extended the table caption to include more description of the columns. 

      “Table 1: Experts and health zones included in each round of the survey. The left part of the table details the experts interviewed (highlighted in green) the health zones included in the main survey in each month. In addition, the right part of the table details the health zones nominated by experts and the number of experts that nominated each one.”

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors tried to identify the relationships between gut microbiota, lipid metabolites and the host in type 2 diabetes (T2DM) by using spontaneously developed T2DM in macaques, considered among the best human models.

      Strengths:

      The authors compared comprehensively the gut microbiota, plasma fatty acids between spontaneous T2DM and the control macaques, and tried verified the results with macaques in high-fat diet-fed mice model.

      Weaknesses:

      The observed multi-omics on macaques can be done on humans, which weakens the conclusion of the manuscript, unless the observation/data on macaques could cover during the onset of T2DM that would be difficult to obtain from humans.

      Regarding the metabolomic analysis on fatty acids, the authors did not include the results obtained form the macaque fecal samples which should be important considering the authors claimed the importance of gut microbiota in the pathogenesis of T2DM. Instead, the authors measured palmitic acid in the mouse model and tried to validate their conclusions with that.

      In murine experiments, palmitic acid-containing diet were fed to mice to induce diabetic condition, but this does not mimic spontaneous T2DM in macaques, since the authors did not measure in macaque feces (or at least did not show the data from macaque feces of) palmitic acid or other fatty acids; instead, they assumed from blood metabolome data that palmitic acid would be absorbed from the intestine to affect the host metabolism, and added palmitic acid in the diet in mouse experiments. Here involves the probable leap of logic to support their conclusions and title of the study.

      In addition, the authors measured omics data after, but not before, the onset of spontaneous T2DM of macaques. This can reveal microbiota dysbiosis driven purely by disease progression, but does not support the causative effect of gut microbiota on T2DM development that the authors claims.

      We are sorry for misunderstanding your point and failing to address your question regarding macaque fecal metabolomics in our previous response. Our study performed untargeted metabolomics on macaque feces and indeed detected the differential metabolite palmitic acid (PA) content, which showed an obvious decrease in T2DM macaques compared with the control (Table 1). However, the difference in PA level between the two groups was not significant (p = 0.17). It may be attributed to the limitation of untargeted metabolomics methodology in absolute quantitative analysis. In addition, we found many other long-chain fatty acids were down-regulated in the T2DM macaque feces (Table 1). Such results are consistent with our observation in murine experiments. We examined PA levels in the feces, ileum, and serum in mice and found that PA level was significantly decreased in fecal samples but increased in the ileum and serum. These findings demonstrated that without the transplantation of gut microbiota, the ileum could not absorb the PA effectively even at a high concentration of ingested PA. Only mice receiving fecal microbiota transplants from T2DM macaques and fed a high-PA diet showed a significant increase in the ileum and serum alongside a decrease in fecal PA concentration. Both the macaque metabolomics and mice experiment results suggest that gut microbiota mediated the absorption of excess PA in the ileum leading to the accumulation of PA in the serum. In the revised manuscript, we added the results of all differential metabolites in Table S2.

      Author response table 1.

      Table 1. Differential analysis of palmitic acid and other fatty acids from fecal untargeted metabolomics in macaques.

      Regarding the causative effect of gut microbiota on T2DM development, we agree with the reviewer that the omics data were obtained after, but not before, the onset of spontaneous T2DM macaques, the microbiota dysbiosis is probably driven by disease progression. For this reason, we have changed the title of our manuscript and some of our conclusions, which can be found in our response below.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      As described above, the data presented does not support the notion that gut microbiota change in T2DM macaques promote the disease - rather it showed the outcome of the disease progression. In addition, the involvement of palmitic acid absorption was only shown in mice but not in macaques. Therefore, the authors should change their title and conclusions to more precisely reflect their observation.

      According to your suggestion, we changed the title and the conclusion to make them more precise and avoid emphasizing the causative effect of gut microbiota on T2DM. The new title is “Multi-omics investigation of spontaneous T2DM macaque emphasizes gut microbiota could up-regulate the absorption of excess palmitic acid in the T2DM progression”. We also revised the wording of the results and conclusions to acknowledge the limitation of our study, “We also revealed the specific structure of gut microbiota that promoted T2DM development by regulating the absorption of excess PA in mice, providing experimental evidence for the functional role of gut microbiota in T2DM pathogenesis.” (Lines 122-125), “In particular, concentrations of PA, palmitoleic acid, and oleic acid were significantly higher in the T2DM group than control group (p<0.05 and VIP>1). The concentration of PA in the plasma of T2DM macaques increased, while the concentration of palmitic acid in the feces decreased (Figures 3F and G, Table S2).” (Lines 228-233), and “Our study confirms the functional role of gut microbiota and PA in the T2DM progression. The microbiota composition, specifically higher abundance of R. gnavus (current name: M. gnavus) and Coprococcus sp., and lower abundance of Treponema, F. succinogenes, Christensenellaceae, and F16, promoted the absorption of excess PA which is important for the development of T2DM. However, in this study, such microbial alterations were detected in macaques after they had developed the disease of T2DM instead of before or onset of T2DM, the causative effect of gut microbiota and their action mechanism on the development of T2DM is worth further investigation.” (Lines 450-458).

    1. Author response:

      (1) We will clarify statements comparing regeneration and developmental processes. Additionally, we will include a new supplemental figure with published data showing that the pou4-2 clone dd_Smed_v6_30562_0_1 (cross-referenced as SMED30002016) is expressed during stages corresponding to organ development in Schmidtea mediterranea (https://planosphere.stowers.org/feature/Schmidtea/mediterranea-sexual/transcript/SMED30002016).

      (2) We will reorganize the figures by combining Figures 3 and 4 for improved clarity.

      (3) We will address experimental and interpretive concerns regarding the role of atonal in the pou4-2 gene regulatory network.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This study presents a new Bayesian approach to estimate importation probabilities of malaria, combining epidemiological data, travel history, and genetic data through pairwise IBD estimates. Importation is an important factor challenging malaria elimination, especially in low-transmission settings. This paper focuses on Magude and Matutuine, two districts in southern Mozambique with very low malaria transmission. The results show isolation-by-distance in Mozambique, with genetic relatedness decreasing with distances larger than 100 km, and no spatial correlation for distances between 10 and 100 km. But again, strong spatial correlation in distances smaller than 10 km. They report high genetic relatedness between Matutuine and Inhambane, higher than between Matutuine and Magude. Inhambane is the main source of importation in Matutuine, accounting for 63.5% of imported cases. Magude, on the other hand, shows smaller importation and travel rates than Matutuine, as it is a rural area with less mobility. Additionally, they report higher levels of importation and travel in the dry season, when transmission is lower. Also, no association with importation was found for occupation, sex, and other factors. These data have practical implications for public health strategies aiming for malaria elimination, for example, testing and treating travelers from Matutuine in the dry season.

      Strengths:

      The strength of this study lies in the combination of different sources of data - epidemiological, travel, and genetic data - to estimate importation probabilities, and the statistical analyses.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors recognize the limitations related to sample size and the biases of travel reports.

      Thank you for your review and consideration. As mentioned, we state in the manuscript the limitations related to sample sizes and travel reports. We aim to continue this study with new prospective data, aiming to address these limitations.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Based on a detailed dataset, the authors present a novel Bayesian approach to classify malaria cases as either imported or locally acquired.

      Strengths:

      The proposed Bayesian approach for case classification is simple, well justified, and allows the integration of parasite genomics, travel history, and epidemiological data. The work is well-written, very organized, and brings important contributions both to malaria control efforts in Mozambique and to the scientific community. Understanding the origin of cases is essential for designing more effective control measures and elimination strategies.

      Weakness:

      While the authors aim to classify cases as imported or locally acquired, the work lacks a quantification of the contribution of each case type to overall transmission.

      The Bayesian rationale is sound and well justified; however, the formulation appears to present an inconsistency that is replicated in both the main text and the Supplementary Material.

      In fact, one of the questions that remains unanswered is the overall contribution of importation events to transmission in the areas. While the Bayesian classifier does not quantify this, our future analysis will focus on combining outbreak detection, genetic clustering and importation classification to quantify the contribution of imported cases to outbreak resurgence and to the overall transmission.

      Thank you for pointing out the inconsistency in the final formula. In fact, the final formula corresponds to P(I<sub>A</sub> | G), instead to i>P(I<sub>A</sub>), so:

      instead of

      We will correct this error in a new version of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      The authors present an important approach to identify imported P. falciparum malaria cases, combining genetic and epidemiological/travel data. This tool has the potential to be expanded to other contexts. The data was analyzed using convincing methods, including a novel statistical model; although some recognized limitations can be improved. This study will be of interest to researchers in public health and infectious diseases.

      Strengths:

      The study has several strengths, mainly the development of a novel Bayesian model that integrates genomic, epidemiological, and travel data to estimate importation probabilities. The results showed insights into malaria transmission dynamics, particularly identifying importation sources and differences in importation rates in Mozambique. Finally, the relevance of the findings is to suggest interventions focusing on the traveler population to help efforts for malaria elimination.

      Weaknesses:

      The study also has some limitations. The sample collection was not representative of some provinces, and not all samples had sufficient metadata for risk factor analysis, which can also be affected by travel recall bias. Additionally, the authors used a proxy for transmission intensity and assumed some conditions for the genetic variable when calculating the importation probability for specific scenarios. The weaknesses were assessed by the authors.

      We acknowledge the limitations commented by the reviewer. We have the following plans to address the limitations. We will repeat the study for our data collected in 2023, which this time contains a good representation of all the provinces of Mozambique, and completeness of the metadata collection was ensured by implementing a new protocol in January 2023. Regarding the proxy for transmission intensity, we will refine the model by integrating monthly estimates of malaria incidence (previously calibrated to address testing and reporting rates) from the DHIS2 data, taking also into account the date of the reported cases in the analysis.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This study investigates how ant group demographics influence nest structures and group behaviors of Camponotus fellah ants, a ground-dwelling carpenter ant species (found locally in Israel) that build subterranean nest structures. Using a quasi-2D cell filled with artificial sand, the authors perform two complementary sets of experiments to try to link group behavior and nest structure: first, the authors place a mated queen and several pupae into their cell and observe the structures that emerge both before and after the pupae eclose (i.e., "colony maturation" experiments); second, the authors create small groups (of 5,10, or 15 ants, each including a queen) within a narrow age range (i.e., "fixed demographic" experiments) to explore the dependence of age on construction. Some of the fixed demographic instantiations included a manually induced catastrophic collapse event; the authors then compared emergency repair behavior to natural nest creation. Finally, the authors introduce a modified logistic growth model to describe the time-dependent nest area. The modification introduces parameters that allow for age-dependent behavior, and the authors use their fixed demographic experiments to set these parameters, and then apply the model to interpret the behavior of the colony maturation experiments. The main results of this paper are that for natural nest construction, nest areas, and morphologies depend on the age demographics of ants in the experiments: younger ants create larger nests and angled tunnels, while older ants tend to dig less and build predominantly vertical tunnels; in contrast, emergency response seems to elicit digging in ants of all ages to repair the nest.

      We sincerely thank Reviewer #1 for the time and effort dedicated to our manuscript's detailed review and assessment. The revision suggestions were constructive, and we have provided a point-by-point response to address them.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      I enjoyed this paper and the approach to examining an accepted wisdom of ants determining overall density by employing age polyethism that would reduce the computational complexity required to match nest size with population (although I have some questions about the requirement that growth is infinite in such a solution). Moreover, the realization that models of collective behaviour may be inappropriate in many systems in which agents (or individuals) differ in the behavioural rules they employ, according to age, location, or information state. This is especially important in a system like social insects, typically held as a classic example of individual-as-subservient to whole, and therefore most likely to employ universal rules of behaviour. The current paper demonstrates a potentially continuous age-related change in target behaviour (excavation), and suggests an elegant and minimal solution to the requirement for building according to need in ants, avoiding the invocation of potentially complex cognitive mechanisms, or information states that all individuals must have access to in order to have an adaptive excavation output.

      We sincerely thank reviewer #2 for the time and effort dedicated to our manuscript's detailed review and assessment. We have provided a point-by-point response to the reviewer's comments, which we have incorporated into the revised version of the manuscript.

      The only real reservation I have is in the question of how this relationship could hold in properly mature colonies in which there is (presumably) a balance between the birth and death of older workers. Would the prediction be that the young ants still dig, or would there be a cessation of digging by young ants because the area is already sufficient? Another way of asking this is to ask whether the innate amount of digging that young ants do is in any way affected by the overall spatial size of the colony. If it is, then we are back to a problem of perfect information - how do the young ants know how big the overall colony is? Perhaps using density as a proxy? Alternatively, if the young ants do not modify their digging, wouldn't the colony become continuously larger? As a non-expert in social insects, I may be misunderstanding and it may be already addressed in the citations used.

      We thank the reviewer for this interesting question. We find that the nest excavation is predominantly performed by the younger ants in the nest, and the nest area increase is followed by an increase in the population. However, if the young ants dig unrestricted, this could result in unnecessary nest growth as suggested by reviewer #2. Therefore, we believe that the innate digging behavior of ants could potentially be regulated by various cues such as;

      (a) Density-based: If the colony becomes less dense as its area expands, this could serve as a feedback signal for young ants to reduce or stop digging, as described in references (25, 29, 30).

      (b) Pheromone depositions: If the colony reaches a certain population density, pheromone signals could inhibit further digging by young ants, references (25, 29), or space usage as a proxy for the nest area. 

      Thus, rather than perfect information, decentralized control, and digging-based local cues probably regulate the level of age-dependent digging, without the ants needing to estimate the overall colony size or nest area.

      In any case, this is an excellent paper. The modelling approach is excellent and compelling, also allowing extrapolation to other group sizes and even other species. This to me is the main strength of the paper, as the answer to the question of whether it is younger or older ants that primarily excavate nests could have been answered by an individual tracking approach (albeit there are practical limitations to this, especially in the observation nest setup, as the authors point out). The analysis of the tunnel structure is also an important piece of the puzzle, and I really like the overall study.

      We thank the reviewer for the comments. We completely agree that individual tracking of ants within our experimental setup would have been the ideal approach, but we were limited by technical and practical limitations of the setup, as pointed out by the reviewer, such as; 

      (a) Continuous tracking of ants in our nests would have required a camera to be positioned at all times in front of the nest, which necessitates a light background. Since Camponotus fellah ants are subterranean, we aimed to allow them to perform nest excavation in conditions as close to their natural dark environment as possible. Additionally, implementing such a system in front of each nest would have reduced the sample sizes for our treatments.

      (b) The experimental duration of our colony maturation and fixed demographics experiments extended for up to six months (unprecedented durations in these kinds of measurements). These naturally limited our ability to conduct individual tracking while maintaining the identity of each ant based on the current design.

      These details are described in detail within the revised version of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Harikrishnan Rajendran, Roi Weinberger, Ehud Fonio, and Ofer Feinerman measured the digging behaviours of queens and workers for the first 6 months of colony development, as well as groups of young or old ants. They also provide a quantitative model describing the digging behaviours and allowing predictions. They found that young ants dig more slanted tunnels, while older ants dig more vertically (straight down). This finding is important, as it describes a new form of age polyethism (a division of labour based on age). Age polyethism is described as a "yes or no" mechanism, where individuals perform or not a task according to their age (usually young individuals perform in-nest tasks, and older ones foraging). Here, the way of performing the task is modified, not only the propensity to carry it or not. This data therefore adds in an interesting way to the field of collective behaviours and division of labour.

      The conclusions of the paper are well supported by the data. Measurements of the same individuals over time would have strengthened the claims.

      We sincerely thank reviewer #3 for the time and effort dedicated to our manuscript's detailed review and assessment. We completely agree with the reviewer’s comments on the measurements of the same individuals over time, however, we were limited by the technical and experimental limitations as described above and pointed out by reviewer #2.

      Strengths:

      I find that the measure of behaviour through development is of great value, as those studies are usually done at a specific time point with mature colonies. The description of a behaviour that is modified with age is a notable finding in the world of social insects. The sample sizes are adequate and all the information clearly provided either in the methods or supplementary.

      We thank reviewer #3  for this assessment.

      Weaknesses:

      I think the paper is failing to take into consideration or at least discuss the role of inter-individual variabilities. Tasks have been known to be undertaken by only a few hyper-active individuals for example. Comments on the choice to use averages and the potential roles of variations between individuals are in my opinion lacking. Throughout the paper wording should be modified to refer to the group and not the individuals, as it was the collective digging that was measured. Another issue I had was the use of "mature colony" for colonies with very few individuals and only 6 months of age. Comments on the low number of workers used compared to natural mature colonies would be welcome.

      Regarding the main comment 1

      We completely agree with the reviewer’s comment on considering inter-individual variability based on activity levels. We have discussed how individual morphological variability could influence digging behavior (references: 28, 31), and we will elaborate further on this aspect in future revisions.

      Regarding the main comment 2:

      The term ‘colony maturation’ in our study refers to the progressive development of colonies from a single queen, distinguishing it from experiments that begin with pre-established, demographically stable colonies. We provide a detailed explanation for this terminology in the revised version of the manuscript. We were practically limited by the continuation of the experiments for more than 6 months of age, predominantly due to the stability of nests, as they were made with a sand-soil mix. We also acknowledge that the colony sizes attained in our maturation experiments may be smaller than those of naturally matured colonies. This trend was observed generally in lab-reared colonies and could be attributed to differences in microclimatic conditions, foraging opportunities, space availability, and other factors. We have explicitly described these details in the revised version of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The experimental design is fantastic. The large quasi-2D should allow for the direct visualization of the movements of individuals and the creation of the nest, and the inclusion of non-workers (specifically, a mated queen and pupae) is new and important. However, I have some questions and concerns about the results, as outlined below. Also, I found the paper difficult to read, and the connections between the various experiments and the model were not always clear. 

      We thank the reviewer for the time and effort dedicated to reviewing our manuscript. We have modified the manuscript substantially to address the comments and readability. 

      The assumption that the digging rate is constant across ants may be a strong one. Previous work (see, for instance, Aguilar, et al, Science 2018) has demonstrated a very heterogeneous workload distribution among ants. I am not sure what implications that may have for the results here, but the authors should comment on this choice. Related to the point above, given a constant digging rate, the variation in digging is attributed to an age-dependent "desired target area". Can the authors comment on the implications of this, specifically in contrast to a variable digging rate? The distinction between digging rate differences and target area differences seems to be important for the authors. However, the way this is presented, it is difficult to fully understand or appreciate this importance and its implications. What is the consequence of this difference, and why is this important?

      We apologize to the reviewer for the confusion.

      Our model does not assume that the digging rate (da/dt, Equation 1) remains constant throughout the experiment. Instead, we only treat the basal digging rate (r) as a constant.

      The variable digging rate (da/dt, Equation 1) is derived by multiplying the basal rate constant (r) by the term (1 - a/a<sub>age</sub>), which accounts for deviations from the age-dependent target area that the ants aim to achieve. This makes the actual digging rate dynamic, as it responds to changes in excavated area (e.g., expansion or rapid collapse)

      For example, according to our model (Equation 1), two ants with the same basal digging rate (r) may exhibit markedly different actual digging rates at a given time if they differ in age. This occurs because the variable digging rate (da/dt) depends not only on ‘r’ but also on the age-dependent term (1 - a/a<sub>age</sub>). Also, we emphasize that the use of a basal digging rate constant aligns with prior studies (refs. 24, 29, 30).

      In our work, we demonstrate that after a collapse event, ants of all ages dig at rates comparable to those observed in the initial (pre-collapse) phase of the experiment. This occurs because the ants are far from their age-dependent target area, effectively resetting their digging behavior. By comparing maximum digging rates pre- and post-collapse, we provide strong empirical evidence that this rate is age-independent (SI Fig. 6A, 6B), supporting the conclusion that the basal digging rate constant (r) is a fundamental property of the ants' behavior, unaffected by age.

      We agree with the reviewer that individual tracking of ants within our experimental setup would have been the ideal approach. Then, we could have taken the inter-individual variability of the digging activity into account. However, we were limited to doing so by the technical and practical limitations of the setup, such as; 

      (a) Continuous tracking of ants in our nests would have required a camera to be positioned at all times in front of the nest, which necessitates a light background. Since Camponotus fellah ants are subterranean, we aimed to allow them to perform nest excavation in conditions as close to their natural dark environment as possible. Additionally, implementing such a system in front of each nest would have reduced the sample sizes for our treatments.

      (b) The experimental duration of our colony maturation experiments extended for up to six months (unprecedented durations in these kinds of measurements). These naturally limited our ability to conduct individual tracking while maintaining the identity of each ant based on the current design.

      In light of these points, the following lines are added to the discussion (line numbers: 283-295), signifying the above points:

      “Our age-dependent model demonstrates that the digging behavior in Camponotus fellah is governed by a basal digging rate constant (r) modulated by the age-dependent feedback (1 − a/aage). Crucially, we show that after a collapse, the maximum digging rates return to their pre-collapse levels, suggesting that this basal rate ’r’ represents an age-independent ceiling on how fast ants can dig, regardless of age or context (SI Fig. 6 A, B). Previous studies have demonstrated both homogeneous and heterogeneous workload distribution, with varying digging rates among ants (24, 29, 30, 35). Studies showing heterogeneous workload distribution relied on continuous individual tracking of ants to quantify digging rates (35). However, this approach was not feasible in our current design due to the experimental durations of both our colony maturation and fixed demographics experiments. Additionally, sample size requirements naturally limited our ability to conduct continuous individual tracking during nest construction in our study. Thus, based on empirical measurements from our fixed-demographics experiments and supported by the age-independent post-collapse digging rates, we adopted a constant basal digging rate for simulating our age-dependent model—an assumption aligned with both prior literature and the collective dynamics observed in our system (24,29,30)”.

      Model: as presented, the model seems to lack independent validation. The model seems to have built-in that there is an age-dependent target area, and this is what is recovered from the model. I am failing to see what is learned from the model that the experiments do not already show. Also, the model has no ant interactions, though ants are eusocial and group size is known to have a large effect on behavior (this is acknowledged by the authors at the beginning of the discussion). Can the authors comment on this?My recommendation would be to remove the model from this paper or improve the text to address the above comments.

      We did not draw the conclusion of the age-dependent target area from our model. We used the fixed demographics experiments to quantify the age-dependent area target as a function of the age of individuals. We then used this age-dependent area target in our model to quantify the excavation dynamics of the colony maturation experiments, where ants span a variety of ages, as the nest population changes over time, resulting in natural variation in the ages of individuals within the nest.  These results could not have been obtained by performing any of the individual experiments, whether colony maturation or the fixed demographics, young or old, on their own. The need for different age demographics was crucial to quantify the age-dependent effects in nest excavation, which were lacking in previous studies. 

      First, the age-dependent model provides a very good estimate for the natural growth of the nest.  More importantly, after fixing an age threshold of 56 days (mean + standard deviation of the young ant age), the model provides an estimate of which ants are doing the majority of the digging during natural nest expansion. This teaches us that during natural expansion, the older ants are far from their density target and therefore do not engage in any substantial digging, which is shown in Figure 4. C. 

      On the other hand, the younger ants are close to their area targets and induced to dig. Indeed, the target area fitted for the age-independent model closely approximates the empirically measured age-dependent target when extrapolated to very young ants. This provides further support for the idea that, in the colony maturation experiments, the youngest ants are responsible for most of the digging.

      Our model is a simple analytical model, inspired by earlier models that used a fixed area target (such as density models) for nest construction. However, because we knew the precise age of workers in our experiments, we were able to obtain age-dependent area targets, thereby challenging the use of a constant area target (as employed in prior studies) in light of our findings from the fixed demographics of young and old colonies.

      Empirically Quantifiable Parameters: We wanted our model to have empirically quantifiable parameters. Since we did not continuously record the experiment, we could not quantify agent-agent interactions, pheromonal depositions, or similar factors.

      Minimal Model Design: We aimed to keep the model as minimal as possible, which is why we did not include complex interactions such as those found in continuous tracking experiments.

      However, the model does set up some interesting hypotheses that could easily be tested with the experimental setup (e.g., marking the ants / tracking individual activity levels). For instance, it is hypothesized that older ants dig less often, but when they do dig, they do so at the same rate. Given the 2D setup, the authors could track individual ants and test this hypothesis. Also, if the desired target area does decrease with age, the authors could verify this hypothesis by placing older ants into arenas with different-sized pre-formed nests to observe how structure is changed to achieve the desired area/ant.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment.

      We believe that the confusion with the usage of a constant basal digging rate is resolved now. To briefly reiterate, ants dig at variable rates that can be decomposed to a (constant on short time scales but age-dependent) basal rate times the (variable) distance from the density target. The suggested experiments are beyond the scope of our current study, and further studies could utilize the suggested experimental design with better time-resolved imaging for individual ant tracking that could verify the predictions from our model. 

      Specific comments:

      Title:

      The title suggests a broad result, yet the study focuses on one ant species. Please modify the title to more accurately reflect the scope of the work.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment.

      The title is modified as “Colony demographics shape nest construction in Camponotus fellah ants.”

      Introduction:

      Important information and context are missing about this ant species. For instance, please add the following about this species in the introduction:

      What is their natural habitat and substrate? How does the artificial soil compare?

      What is their (rough) colony size? [later, discuss experiment group size choice and potential insights/limitations of results when applied to the natural system].

      The details have been added to the introduction (line numbers : 49-55) and the materials and methods section (Study species).

      “Camponotus fellah ants are native to the Near East and North Africa, particularly found in countries like Israel, Egypt, and surrounding arid and semi-arid regions, where they prefer to nest in moist, decaying wood, including tree trunks, branches, or stumps (49,50). The species lives in monogynous colonies with tens to thousands of individuals. Nests are commonly found in a sand-loamy mix, which is a combination of sand, soil, clay, or gravel, providing structural stability and moisture retention (51). They are typically found under rocks, in the crevices of dried vegetation, or dry, sandy soils, sometimes in areas with loose gravel, with a colony size ranging from tens to thousands of workers”.

      What is the natural life expectancy of a worker? A queen? [later, discuss fixed demographic age choices in this context and/or why were age ranges chosen for experiments?].

      The lifespan of ants, including both queens and workers, varies significantly based on caste, species, and environmental conditions.

      (1) Queen Longevity: From the literature, Camponotus fellah queens can live up to 20 years, with one documented case reaching 26 years (50). 

      (2) Worker Longevity: In contrast to queens, the lifespan of workers is much shorter. Lab studies on Camponotus fellah (82) and other Camponotus species (83) suggest that workers can live for several months depending on environmental conditions, colony health, and caste-specific roles (e.g., minor vs. major workers)

      (3) Laboratory vs. Natural Conditions: Worker longevity is highly variable between laboratory and natural conditions

      Therefore, in the context of the old worker lifespan in our experiments, ~200 days (roughly 6–7 months), we strongly believe that the worker lifespan used in our experiments represents a substantial portion of a worker's expected life. While exact figures for C. fellah workers are unavailable, inferences from related species suggest that workers nearing 200 days are approaching the latter stages of their lifespan, making them meaningfully "old". 

      The details are added to the main text (line numbers: 124-127) and discussion (line numbers: 278-282).

      Why was this species chosen? Convenience, or is there something special about this species that the readers should know? Specifically, is there something that might make the results more general or of broader interest?

      Camponotus fellah was chosen for this study because it is native to Israel, making it convenient to collect and maintain in the lab. Additionally, its nuptial flights occur close to the study location, ensuring a steady supply of colonies. We were able to provide them with a nesting substrate similar to what they naturally use, as their nests are typically found in a sand-loamy mix, similar to the sand-soil mix in our artificial nests. This was possible because we had the opportunity to observe their habitat and nesting behavior in the wild, allowing us to gather preliminary information on their natural nesting conditions.

      Results:

      Line 60: "several brood items" - how many exactly? Was this consistent across experiments? Do mated queens ever produce more pupae during the experiments?

      Yes, the number of brood items (5) was added consistently across the experiments. Additionally, the mated queen did produce pupae during the course of the experiments, which was evident from the noticeable increase in the number of workers in the nest. This was significantly higher than the number of brood items present at the start of the study.

      The above points are added to the section (line numbers : 68-69).

      Figure 1: Panel A - The food ports are never mentioned in the text. Are the ants fed during the experiments? If so, what? With what frequency? Is the water column replenished/maintained? If so, how and how often? panel C - how long did this experiment last?

      We thank the reviewer for pointing this out. We have now updated the nest maintenance section in the Materials and Methods (line numbers : 349-354) part to include all the necessary details and clarifications.

      “We provided food to the ants ad libitum through three separate tubes containing water, 20 % sucrose water, and protein food. The protein mixture included egg powder, tuna, prawns, honey, agar, and vitamins. Each of the three tubes was filled with 5 ml of their respective contents and sealed with a cotton stopper to prevent overflow. The tubes were positioned at a slight angle and connected using a custom-made plexiglass adapter to facilitate the flow of liquids. These tubes were replenished once depleted, and regularly replaced once the nest maintenance was carried out bi-weekly.”

      Line 76: "...excavation was commenced by the founding queen". How were the queen and pupae introduced into the system?

      We initiated colony maturation experiments by introducing a single mated queen and several brood items (pupae) at random positions on the soil layer of the nest (line numbers : 68-69)

      Line 87: Please provide bounds for 11cm2/ant value. Is there any biological or physical justification for this number?

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We have now provided the bounds as requested (line numbers : 97-101). 

      We were unable to pinpoint a specific biological justification based solely on this treatment. However, on extrapolating the age-dependent area fit we derived from the fixed demographics experiment, we found that at the age of 1 day, an ant has a target area of approximately 11.17 cm², which is the largest age-dependent area target possible within our experimental setup.

      From the colony maturation experiment, we obtained the value of  11.6 (±1.15) cm² as the area per ant. The consistency between the area per ant obtained from two completely different treatments across different colonies yielded similar results. We propose that under standardized conditions, a 1-day-old ant has a theoretical maximum target area of 11.17 cm²—the highest value observed in our experimental framework.

      Lines 98-99: "one straightforward possibility would be that newborn ants are the ones that dig". This statement contradicts the results presented in Figures 1 and S1 - the population increase seems to occur at least a few days before increased excavation in nearly all cases.

      We apologize for any confusion caused by our initial phrasing. To clarify, we proposed that a lag likely exists between population growth and nest area expansion. This lag could arise from two sequential processes: (1) newborn ants require time to mature and become active (first delay), and (2) digging to expand the nest takes additional time (second delay; estimated at ~10 days from the cross-correlation analysis). Thus, our results suggest that it is not the population that lags behind the area, but rather the area that lags behind the population, as demonstrated in Figures 2D and SI. Figure. S1.

      The sentence “one straightforward possibility would be that newborn ants are the ones that dig” is modified as below (line numbers : 112-119) to prevent further confusion.

      “One possible explanation is that, although all ants are capable of digging, it is primarily the newly emerged ants who perform this task. In this case, nest expansion would lag behind colony growth due to two delays: first, the time needed for young ants to mature enough to begin digging, and second, the physical time required to excavate additional space (e.g., around 10 days). This mechanism could eliminate the need for ants to assess overall colony density, as each new group of active workers simply enlarges the nest as they become ready. An alternative possibility is that all ants, regardless of age, respond to increased density by initiating excavation. In that scenario, nest expansion would follow more immediately after the emergence of new individuals, making delays less prominent (24, 29, 30)”.

      Line 105: How do group sizes compare to natural colony size? Line 106: How do "young" and "old" classifications compare to natural life expectancy?

      We have already addressed this question in an earlier comment. The details are added to the main text (line numbers: 124-127) and discussion (line numbers: 278-282).

      Line 118-119: How are nests artificially collapsed?

      We have added a new section in the Materials and Methods section that describes the nest collapsing procedure (Nest artificial collapse - line numbers : 386-399).

      Figure 2 Panel A: The white dotted line is nearly impossible to see. Please use a more visible color.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment.

      We changed the solid circles to violet and the dotted line color to continuous white.

      Figure 3: The use of circle markers as post-collapse recovery in young and old as well as old pre-collapse is confusing. Use different symbols for old pre-collapse vs young and old post-collapse.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the confusion. We have revised the figure markers as suggested and modified the main text accordingly.

      • Young; pre-collapse : star

      • Young; post-collapse : diamond

      • Old; pre-collapse : circle

      • Old; post-collapse: triangle.

      Figure 3 Panel C: Indicate that fixed demographic values here are pre-collapse. Also, as presented, it appears that there is a large group-size dependence that is not commented on. Previous results (Line 87 and Figure 2C) suggest a constant excavation area per ant of 11cm2/ant. Figure 3, panel C appears to suggest a group-size dependence. If these values are divided by group size, is excavated area per ant nearly constant across groups? How does the numerical value compare to the slope from Figure 2C?

      We thank the reviewer for their insightful comments.

      First, we would like to clarify that the area target of 11.1 (±1) cm²/ant, as described in Line 87, was obtained from the colony maturation experiments. In these experiments, we were unable to track the age of each individual ant, so the area target was calculated by normalizing the total excavated area by the number of ants.

      We normalized the excavated area by the group size for both young and old colonies as suggested, and found that the area per ant was not significantly different across the group sizes (see new SI Fig. 5A). This indicates that the excavated area per ant remains relatively constant within each demographic group. Moreover, this shows that the total excavated area is proportional to group size, in agreement with previous works (24, 29, and 30). 

      We have explicitly described the above information in the line numbers: 142-146

      Regarding the slope comparisons, the slope of Figure 2C (10.71), from the colony maturation experiments, is the largest, followed by the area per ant from the short-term young (8.79 ± 0.98) cm²/ant, and short-term old experiments (5.16 ± 0.44) cm²/ant.

      Lines 128-129: "...younger ants aim to approach a higher target area". Seems hard to know what they "aim" to do... rephrase to report what they are observed to do.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. The sentence is rephrased as suggested (line numbers : 158-161).

      “In the previous sections, we showed that in fixed-demographics experiments, younger ants excavated a significantly larger nest area compared to older ants (Fig. 3. C).  This difference emerged despite similar temporal patterns in digging rates across age groups, with excavation activity peaking within the first 7 days before asymptotically decaying as nest expansion approached saturation (SI Fig. 8).”

      Lines 133-141: The model description is not clear. Specifically, what parameters are ant-dependent? How does A relate to a?

      We appreciate the reviewer's request for clarification. In our model:

      (1) Equation 1 describes the change in the excavated area due to the digging activity of a single ant. Here, the variable 'a' represents the area excavated by one ant. This formulation allows us to capture the individual digging behavior and its impact on the excavation process.

      (2) Equation 2 extends this concept to the total area excavated in the nest, denoted by 'A'. Specifically, 'A' is the sum of the areas excavated by all ants present in the nest. In other words, it aggregates the individual contributions of each ant, linking the microscopic digging behavior to the macroscopic excavation dynamics.

      Therefore, the relationship between 'a' and 'A' is as follows:

      ●     'a' = Area excavated by a single ant.

      ●     'A' = ∑ 'a' (Summed over all ants in the nest).

      We have explicitly mentioned this in the line numbers “ 161-179”, and describe the model assumptions and parameters in detail.

      Figure 4:

      Figure 4, Panel A: The equation quoted in the caption does not match the data in the figure. The equation has a positive slope and negative intercept, while the figure has a negative slope and a positive intercept. Please provide the correct equation and bounds on fit parameters.

      We thank the reviewer for spotting this typing mistake.

      The equation was already updated in the reviewed preprint published online. The correct equation and the fit bound are provided in the figure caption.

      “Target areas decrease linearly with the ant age (y = −0.032x + 11.22 , 95 % CI (Intercept : (-0.035,-0.027), Slope : (10.53,11.91)), R2 = 0.96 ).”

      Figure 4, Panel A: There seem to be three "fixed target area per ant values" in the paper: around 11cm2/ant (line 87), 11.6 cm2/ant (SI Figure 2), and linearly dependent value from fit to Figure 4A. The distinctions between these values and their significance are hard to keep track of. Can the authors add a discussion somewhere that helps the reader better understand? Is there a way to connect/rationalize/explain these different values in terms of demographics?

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion.We have added a paragraph in the discussion (line numbers : 270-277) describing the area targets.

      “In our colony maturation experiments, we found that area per ant was highest when the workers were youngest, with values around 11.1–11.6 (±1–1.15). This aligns with observations from naturally growing nests, where newly eclosed ants dominate the population and nest volumes are relatively large. Supporting this, fixed-demographics experiments showed that the area excavated per ant declines linearly with worker age, indicating that the youngest ants contribute most to excavation. Notably, the target area we fit for the age-independent model (11.6 ± 1.15) closely matches the extrapolated value for very young workers (Fig. 4. A), reinforcing the idea that young ants are the primary excavators during early colony growth. In contrast, during events like collapses or displacement, when space is urgently needed, ants of all ages participate in excavation.”

      Figure 4, Panel A: What are various symbols and colors for data with error bars? If consistent with Figure 3, then this panel and subsequent model confound two factors: (1) the age dependence and (2) the behavioral differences pre- and post-collapse (structures are different pre-and post-collapse, according to SI Figure 6; line 120: "...colonies ceased digging when they recovered 93{plus minus}3% of the area lost by the manual collapse..."; lines 201-202: "We find significant quantitative and qualitative differences between nests constructed within this natural context and nests constructed in the context of an emergency") and behavior is different (according to SI Figure 7 and line 119: "...all ants dig after collapse...")). Therefore, without further supporting evidence, it does not seem that these data should be used to fit a single line that defines a model parameter a_age for each ant in equation 2.

      The symbols are the area per ant quantified from the fixed demographics of young, and old experiments. The symbols show the following;

      A.  Star - Young, pre-collapse

      B.  Diamond - Young, post-collapse 

      C.  Circle - Old, pre-collapse

      D.  Triangle - Old, post-collapse.

      The details are clearly described in the figure caption. 

      We apologize to the reviewer for the confusion. We argue that the data can be fit by a single line to quantify the parameter ‘a_age’ as follows. 

      A. All data presented in Figure 4A were obtained from the same fixed-demographics experiments (containing only young and old ants) under experimental collapse conditions, pre- and post-collapse. These results, therefore, exclusively reflect emergency nest-building behaviors during emergency scenarios and do not include any observations from natural colony maturation processes.

      B. Age-dependent excavation differences: As correctly noted by the reviewer, the observed difference in excavated area before versus after collapse reflects the natural aging of ants in our experimental colonies. While colonies recovered >90% of lost area post-collapse, the residual variation was not negligible—instead, it systematically correlated with colony age structure. By tracking colonies across this demographic transition, we obtained additional data points spanning a broader developmental spectrum. This extended range strengthened our ability to detect and quantify the linear relationship between worker age and excavation output.

      C.The quoted sentence (lines 201-202, submitted version) refers to comparisons across all three experimental cases: (1) fixed-demographics young ants, (2) fixed-demographics old ants, and (3) the natural scenario (mixed-age colonies). Importantly, these comparisons are based on pre-collapse steady-state excavation areas, ensuring a consistent baseline across treatments. We highlight quantitative and qualitative differences between these distinct experimental groups, not between pre- and post-collapse phases within the same treatment. The pre- and post-collapse data within fixed-demographics groups were analyzed separately to avoid conflating aging effects with emergency responses.

      To avoid confusion, the whole paragraph in the discussion (line numbers : 253-260) is rephrased.

      In lines 201-202; “We find significant quantitative and qualitative differences between nests constructed within this natural context and nests constructed in the context of an emergency”. 

      Here, by natural context, we mean the nests excavated in the colony maturation experiments. We believe that it could have been confusing, and the sentence is modified as answered for the previous question. 

      Figure 4, Panel B: This uses the model with a_age determined by from Figure 4A and the life table (as shown in the supplemental), whereas the supplemental Figure SI 8 uses the fixed blue line a_age value for the model, which comes from the colony maturation experiments. The age-independent model in the supplemental fits the data better, yet the authors claim the supplemental model cannot be applied to the data because of their experimentally determined age-dependent target area. Given the age-independent target area model fits better, additional evidence/justification is needed to support the choice of the model.

      We agree with the reviewer that the age-independent model fits the data well. However, we believe that the fixed area target cannot be used to explain the excavation dynamics for the following reasons.

      We make an important assumption in our model: that the ants rely on local cues and that individual ants can not distinguish between the fixed demographics and colony maturation experiments (line numbers : 161-166). Given this assumption, the ants cannot change their behavior between experiments, meaning the same model should fit all of our results. However, the fixed demographics experiments revealed a significant difference in the areas excavated by young vs. old cohorts, despite having the same group size. If the ants regulated the excavated area based on an age-independent constant density target model, then the excavated area in the fixed demographics of young and old colonies would have been similar. This discrepancy indicates that the target area per ant is not constant, as assumed in the age-independent density model (SI. Fig. 8). We emphasize that while the age-independent model provides a better fit for the excavated area in colony maturation experiments, the age-dependence of excavation is empirically supported by fixed-demographics experiments. Therefore, we implemented this age-dependence through a variable target area within the age-dependent model framework to explain excavation dynamics in the colony maturation experiments.

      These details are explicitly mentioned in the main text (line numbers : 187 - 198)

      Figure 4, Panel C: Is this plot entirely from the model, or are the data points measured from experiments? Please label this more clearly.

      We apologize to the reviewer for the confusion.

      The Figure 4C is based on the age-dependent digging model. We applied the model to population data from the long-term experiments (n = 22). By setting an age threshold of 56 days (since ants used in the short-term young experiment had an average age of 40 ± 16 days), we categorized the ants into young and old groups. We then quantified the area dug by the young ants, the queen, and the old ants in terms of the percentage of the total area excavated. We hypothesized that, because young ants have a lower digging threshold, they would perform the majority of the digging. We indeed confirm this in Figure 4C.

      This information is added to the main text and described in detail (line numbers: 200 - 208).

      Lines 162-165: "...Furthermore, we quantified the area dug by each ant in the normal colony growth experiment as estimated from the age-dependent model and found that all ants excavated more or less the same amount...". Figure 4D shows a distribution with significant values ranges from 1-16 cm2... how is this interpreted as "more or less the same amount" and what is the significance of this?

      We apologise to the reviewer for the confusion.

      We quantified the percentage contribution to the excavated area of each histogram bin (provided in the new SI table: 4), and found that the area excavated between 5 cm² and 13 cm² accounts for 73.76% of the total excavated area. This indicates that most ants dug within this range rather than exhibiting extreme variations. Additionally, the mean excavation amount is 7.84 cm², with a standard deviation of 3.44 cm², meaning that most values fall between 4.4 cm² and 11.28 cm², which aligns well with the 5–13 cm² range. Since the majority of the excavation is concentrated within this narrow interval, and the mean is well centered within it, this suggests that ants excavated more or less the same amount, rather than forming distinct groups with highly different excavation behaviors.

      We have modified the main text (line numbers: 209-216) to include these points.

      The biological significance of this finding is that since all ants in the colony maturation experiments are born inside the nest, we hypothesize that they should excavate similar amounts. To test this, we quantified the area contribution of each ant over the entire duration of the experiment using the age-dependent digging model as described above and found that they indeed excavated more or less the same amount. From our analysis of fixed demographics experiments, we showed that the youngest ants excavate the largest area. Since the majority of the youngest ants participated in the colony maturation experiments, this further supports our hypothesis.

      Figure 5.

      Figure 5, Panels A-C: Please provide a scale bar. 

      The scale bar is provided in the figure as suggested. The algorithm for the cutoffs for tunnel vs wide tunnels is described in detail in the section “Nest skeletonization, segmentation, and orientation.”

      Figure 5, Panel E: Why does the chamber error bar for 5 ants go to zero?

      In Figure 5, E, we plot the standard error, as described in the figure caption. In the experiments, the chamber area contributions were (0,0,39.94,0) respectively. The mean of the 4 numbers is 9.985, the standard deviation is 19.97, and the standard error is 9.985. So, the mean and the standard error are the same, so the lower error bar goes to zero, and the upper error bar goes to 19.97. This implies that in these experiments, the chamber area is often zero.

      Figure 5, Panel I: Why are there no chambers for young colonies in I when they are in the histogram in E?

      We apologize to the reviewer for the confusion. We initially missed adding the chamber orientation data of the young colonies to Panel I, but it has now been included.

      Line 212: "...densities of ants never become too high...". What is too high? Is there some connection to biological or physical constraints?

      Under normal growth conditions, nest volume is kept proportional to the number of ants, ensuring that the density remains within a specific range. This prevents overcrowding, which could otherwise lead to excessively high densities.

      Yes, we believe there is likely a connection to both biological and physical constraints. The proportional relationship between nest volume and the number of ants is likely driven by factors such as:

      (1) Biological Constraints:

      Ant Colony Size: Ants typically adjust their behavior and social structure to maintain an optimal population size relative to available resources and space.Overcrowding could lead to potentially a breakdown in colony function.

      Colony Health: High densities can lead to faster epidemic spread, leading to negative effects on reproduction, foraging efficiency, and overall colony health. By maintaining density within a specific range, the colony can thrive without these adverse effects.

      (2) Physical Constraints:

      Spatial Limitations: The physical space within the nest limits how many ants can occupy it before space becomes constrained. The nest’s structure and size must physically accommodate the ants, and the volume must be large enough to prevent overcrowding, and efficient resource distribution.

      Lines 272 and 302: How often were photos taken? These two statements seem to suggest different data collection rates.

      As stated in line 272, photos were taken every 1 to 3 days. During each photo session, four photos were taken, with each photo separated by 2 seconds, as mentioned in line 302. To avoid confusion, we rephrased the sentence (line numbers: 359-361).

      “We photographed the nest development every 1-3 days. During each photography session, four pictures of the nest were taken, with a 2-second interval between each.”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Some more minor points/questions/clarifications:

      This might be pedantic, but I don't think the nest serves as the skeleton of the superorganism, while it does change and grow, the analogy becomes weak beyond that point. The skeleton serves to protect the internal organs of the organism, facilitates movement and muscle attachment, and creates new blood cells. I would be more comfortable with a statement that the nest can grow or shrink according to need.

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for their time and effort in providing a detailed review and assessment of our manuscript. A point-by-point response to the comments is provided below.

      The analogy of treating a nest structure to the skeleton of a superorganism was based on the following points;

      (a) Protection: A nest protects the colony on a collective scale. This is analogous to protecting "organs" by a skeletal framework.

      (b) Organization and Division of Space: The skeletal structure organizes the body's internal layout, just as nest structures are organized into various spatial compartments for various colony functions, with specific regions designated for brood chambers, food storage, and waste disposal.

      Thus, we believe that the analogy can still be valid in a metaphorical way.

      Does this statement need justification with a citation, or is that information contained in the subsequent clause? "However, for more complex structures where ants congregate in specific chambers, workers are less likely to assess the overall nest density." The idea that workers do (or do not) assess overall density touches on many issues, including that of perfect information and adaptive responses, that it seems it needs to be well founded in previous work to be stated in such unequivocal terms.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. The references for this argument are provided in the next sentence. We have now moved these references to the relevant sentence (reference number: 24, 29,30; line number : 30-31 ) 

      Can you give some more information on this statement? "Experiments were terminated either when the queen died or when she became irreversibly trapped after a structural collapse." Why was this collapse irreversible and therefore unlike treatment 2? Did the queen die in these instances? Was this event more likely than in natural colonies? And if so, was there something inherently different about your experiments that limit interpretation under natural conditions (e.g. the narrow nature of the observation setup? The consistency of the sand?)

      Our nest excavation experiments were terminated under two primary scenarios: (1) the queen died of natural causes, reflecting the baseline mortality expected when queens are brought into laboratory conditions, or (2) the nest experienced a structural collapse that left the queen irreversibly trapped. The second scenario is further elaborated below:

      Irreversible Collapses: These collapses were classified as irreversible because the queen could not be rescued alive. This occurred when the structural stability of the nest failed, burying the queen in a manner that prevented recovery. In some cases, the collapse resulted in the queen's immediate death, while in others, she was trapped beyond reach, and any rescue attempt risked further structural damage.

      Collapse and Experimental Context: These collapses were not uniquely associated with natural colonies or fixed-demographic experiments; rather, they occurred across various experimental setups.

      The sentence is modified as below to improve clarity (line numbers : 70-72 ).

      “In all instances where a collapse resulted in the queen's death or her being irreversibly trapped in the nest, the experiment was excluded from analysis starting from the point of the collapse, as such events did not reflect normal colony dynamics.”

      I want to make sure I understand the following statement: "Moreover, the area excavated by the young cohorts was similar to that excavated by naturally maturing colonies at the point in which they reached the same population size (Tukey's HSD; group size: 5; p = 0.61, group size: 10; p = 0.46, group size: 15; p = 0.20)." Do I have it right that this means a group of (e.g. 10) young ants excavates an area similar to that of a group of 10 naturally maturing ants at the same age as the young ants?

      Yes, the interpretation provided is correct. We apologize to the reviewer for the confusion. We have rephrased the sentence for better readability (line numbers : 146-148).

      “Furthermore, the area excavated by the young cohorts was comparable to that excavated by naturally maturing colonies when they reached the same population size (Tukey's HSD; group size: 5, p = 0.61; group size: 10, p = 0.46; group size: 15, p = 0.20)”

      How old do ants get? Is the 'old' demographic (~200 days) meaningfully old in the context of the overall worker lifespan? While the results certainly demonstrate there is an age effect, I would like to understand how rapid this is in terms of overall lifespan.

      The lifespan of ants, including both queens and workers, varies significantly based on caste, species, and environmental conditions.

      (1) Queen Longevity: From the literature, Camponotus fellah queens can live up to 20 years, with one documented case reaching 26 years. This remarkable longevity underscores the queen's central role in maintaining the colony.

      (2) Worker Longevity: In contrast to queens, the lifespan of workers is much shorter.

      However, specific data on worker longevity in Camponotus fellah colonies are lacking. Studies on other Camponotus species (50, 82) suggest that workers can live for several months depending on environmental conditions, colony health, and caste-specific roles (e.g., minor vs. major workers).

      (3) Laboratory vs. Natural Conditions: Worker longevity is highly variable between laboratory and natural conditions

      Therefore, in the context of the old worker lifespan in our experiments of, ~200 days (roughly 6–7 months) we strongly believe that the worker lifespan used in our experiments represents a substantial portion of a worker's expected life. While exact figures for C. fellah workers are unavailable, inferences from related species suggest that workers nearing 200 days are approaching the latter stages of their lifespan, making them meaningfully "old."

      These details are added to the main text (line numbers : 124 - 127) and to the discussion (line numbers : 278-282)

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      We sincerely thank the reviewer for their time and effort in providing a detailed review and assessment of our manuscript. A point-by-point response to the comments is provided below.

      L10: "fixed demographics": I find this term unclear, what does it mean, it should specify if the groups are with or without a queen.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. The sentence is modified in the abstract, and definitions are later added in detail in the introduction (line numbers : 8-10) and the Materials and Methods section (Fixed demographics colonies). 

      “We experimentally compared nest excavation in colonies seeded from a single mated queen and allowed to grow for six months to excavation triggered by a catastrophic event in colonies with fixed demographics, where the age of each individual worker, including the queen, is known”.

      The details of the “fixed demographics” treatments were explained in the later portion of the text (line numbers: 58-61).

      L36: I think it is documented that younger individuals are the ones who involved in nest construction in many species.

      Previous studies on nest construction were predominantly performed on mature colonies of specific age demographics or rather mixed demographics, where age was not considered as a factor influencing nest construction. Some studies have speculated that young ants could be the most probable ones to dig, but this has not been experimentally verified to the best of our knowledge.

      L50: I do not think the colony should be called mature after only 6 months, given that colonies reach thousands of workers.

      The sentence is changed as suggested (line numbers : 56-57).

      “The "Colony-Maturation" experiment observed the development of colonies up to six months, starting from a single fertile queen and progressing to colonies with established worker populations.” 

      L60: Where was the queen introduced? It is specified in the Methods but a word here would be helpful.

      The detail is added as suggested (line numbers : 68-69).

      “We initiated colony maturation experiments by introducing a single mated queen and several brood items (n = 5, across all experiments) at random positions on the soil layer of the nest.”

      L106: Young vs Old workers 40 vs 171 days. Maybe cite a reference or provide a reason for the selection of those ages?

      Previous studies have shown that the Camponotus fellah queens can live up to 20 years, with one documented case reaching 26 years (50). To the best of our knowledge, specific data on worker longevity in Camponotus fellah colonies in natural conditions are lacking. Lab studies on Camponotus fellah (82) and other Camponotus species (50) suggest that workers can live for several months depending on environmental conditions, colony health, and caste-specific roles (e.g., minor vs. major workers). 

      We intentionally selected workers from two distinct age groups: younger ants (40 ± 16 days old) and older ants (171.56 ± 20 days old). These ages represent functionally different life stages - the younger group had completed about 25% of their expected lifespan at the start of the experiment, while the older group had lived through most of theirs (50, 82). This 4-fold age difference allowed us to compare excavation behaviors across fundamentally different phases of adult life.

      Our experiments lasted for 60-90 days, during which all participating workers continued to age. To ensure all ants remained alive throughout the experiments, and given the constraints of the experimental timeline, we selected young and old workers within the specified age range. 

      These details are added to the main text (line numbers :  124 -127), and the discussion (line numbers  : 278-282)

      L122-123: But usually ants can vary highly in their behaviours. Can the authors comment on their choice to consider an average, implying that all ants of the same age had the same digging rates?

      We thank the reviewer for the comment.

      In our experiments, we could not track each worker's activity over time. As described in the methods, we took snapshots of the nest structure over days and recorded the population size of the nest. Thus, we could not capture the activity of single ants in the nest as described in the response to major comments in the reviewed preprint.

      We agree that individual tracking of ants within our experimental setup would have been the ideal approach. Then, we could have taken the inter-individual variability of the digging activity into account. However, we were limited to doing so by the technical and practical limitations of the setup, such as; 

      (a) Continuous tracking of ants in our nests would have required a camera to be positioned at all times in front of the nest, which necessitates a light background. Since Camponotus fellah ants are subterranean, we aimed to allow them to perform nest excavation in conditions as close to their natural dark environment as possible. Additionally, implementing such a system in front of each nest would have reduced the sample sizes for our treatments.

      (b)The experimental duration of our colony maturation and fixed demographics experiments extended for up to six months (unprecedented durations in these kinds of measurements). These naturally limited our ability to conduct individual tracking while maintaining the identity of each ant based on the current design.

      To clarify this, we have added the following to the discussion (line numbers: 286-292).

      “Previous studies have demonstrated both homogeneous and heterogeneous workload distribution, with varying digging rates among ants (24,29,30,35). Studies showing heterogeneous workload distribution relied on continuous individual tracking of ants to quantify digging rates (35). However, this approach was not feasible in our current design due to the experimental durations of both our colony maturation and fixed demographics experiments. Additionally, sample size requirements naturally limited our ability to conduct continuous individual tracking during nest construction in our study.”

      L171: A line on how the nest structure was acquired and data extracted would be welcome here.

      The algorithm for the nest structure segmentation, data extraction, and analysis is added in detail to the SI section: Nest skeletonization, segmentation, and orientation. The line is modified (line numbers : 221-224) in the main text as suggested.

      “We compared nest architectures by segmenting raw nest images into chambers and tunnels (see SI Section: Nest Skeletonization, Segmentation, and Orientation). Chambers were identified as flat, horizontal structures, while tunnels were narrower and more vertical in orientation (see SI Fig. 9, SI Section: Nest Skeletonization, Segmentation, and Orientation)”.  

      Figure 3: Where does the data of the mean in panel C come from: is it the mean of the first 30 days, before the collapse? How is it comparable with the rest?

      We apologize to the reviewer for the confusion.

      In panel C, the mean values (solid stars and circles) for fixed-demography colonies (young/old groups) represent pre-collapse excavation areas. For colony maturation experiments (where no collapses were induced), we instead plot the mean saturated excavation area for each group size. This allows direct comparison of mean excavated areas across experimental conditions at equivalent colony sizes.

      To improve readability, the following sentences are added to the main text (line numbers : 139 - 146 ) 

      “We compared the saturated excavation areas (pre-collapse) from fixed-demographics experiments (young and old groups) with those from colony maturation experiments of the same colony sizes (Fig. 3C). We find that, for a given age cohort (young or old), the saturation areas increase linearly with the colony size (GLMM, F(35,37); p < 0.0001) (Fig. 3 C, SI. Fig 7 A). The observed proportional scaling between excavated area and group size aligns with previous studies, even though those studies did not explicitly account for age demographics (24, 29, 30). After normalizing the pre-collapse excavated area by group size for both young and old colonies, we found no significant difference in area per ant across group sizes (SI Fig. 5. A). This indicates that the excavated area per ant remains relatively constant within each demographic group”.

      L209-210: I would be more parsimonious in saying that the results presented prove that the target area decreases with age, as the individual behaviour of the ants was not monitored. Suggestion: rephrase to "the target of the group decreases with age".

      The sentence is rephrased as suggested (line numbers : 265-266).

      “Our results reveal that this target area of the group decreases linearly with age, such that young ants are more sensitive to shortages in space.”

      L246: Are C.fellah colonies really found with such few workers?

      Previous studies have speculated that mature Camponotus fellah colonies are a monogynous species typically founded by a single queen following nuptial flights (50,51,82), and can range from tens to thousands of workers. However, during the founding stage (as in our experiments), colonies naturally pass through smaller developmental sizes comparable to the matured colonies.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary:  

      The Szczupak lab published a very interesting paper in 2012 (Rodriquez et al. J Neurophysiol 107:1917-1924) on the effects of the segmentally-distributed non-spiking (NS) cell on crawl-related motoneurons. As far as I can tell, the working model presented in 2012, for how the non-spiking (NS) cell impacts the crawling motor pattern, is the same functional model presented in this new paper. Unfortunately, the Discussion does not address any of the findings in the previous paper or cite them in the context of NS alterations of fictive crawling. Aside from different-looking figures and some new analyses, the results and conclusions are the same. 

      Reviewers #1 and #2 called our attention to our failure to cite the Rodriguez et al. 2012 article in the context of the main goal of the present work. We do now explain how the present study is framed by the published work. See lines 74-79.

      In Rodriguez et al. 2012, we hypothesized that the inhibitory signals onto NS were originated in the motoneuron firing. We now cite this reference in line 104. In the current manuscript we further investigated the connection between the inhibitory signals onto NS and the motoneuron activity (Figure 2) and proved that the hypothesis was wrong. Thus, the model presented here differs from the one proposed in Rodriguez et al. 2012.

      In Rodriguez et al. 2012, we speculated that the inhibitory signals received by NS were transmitted to the motoneurons, but an important control was missing in that study. In the current study depolarization of NS during crawling is tested against a control series that allows to properly examine the hypothesis (lines 138-147). But, most important, because NS is so widely connected with the layer of motoneurons it was necessary to test the effect on other motoneurons during the fictive crawling cycle. We now explain this rationale in lines 249-257.

      Strengths: 

      The figures are well illustrated. 

      Weaknesses:  

      The paper is a mix of what appears to be two different studies and abruptly switches gears to examine how closely the crawl patterning is in the intact animal as compared to the fictive crawl patterning in the intact animal. Unfortunately, previous studies in other labs are not cited even though identical results have been obtained and similar conclusions were made. Thus, the novelty of the results is missing for those who are familiar with the leech preparation. The lack of appropriate citations and discussion of previous studies also deprives the scientific community of fully comprehending the impact of the data presented and the science it was built upon.  

      The main aim of the manuscript is to learn the role of premotor NS neurons in the crawling motor pattern studied using spike sorting in extracellular nerve recordings. This readout allows to  simultaneously monitor a larger number of units  than in any previous study. This approach aims to determine whether and how a recurrent inhibitory peripheral circuit is involved in coordinating or modulating the rhythmic motor pattern.

      Our rationale was that the known effect of NS on one particular motoneuron (DE-3) may have overlooked a more general effect on crawling (lines 253-257). Moreover, we wanted to investigate whether this effect was due to the recurrent inhibitory circuit or if other elements were involved, and to study whether the modulation was mediated by the recurrent synapse between NS and the motoneurons.

      In the context of this aim we studied the rhythmic activity of cell DE-3, together with motoneurons that fire in-phase and anti-phase, in isolated ganglia (Figure 4). To reveal the effect of NS manipulation we applied a quantitative analysis that showed the phase-specific effect of NS (Figure 6). 

      Given that this is the first study using a spike sorting algorithm to detect and describe the activity of motoneurons in nerve recordings we found it reasonable to compare these results with an in vivo study; thus, providing information to the general reader, that supports the correspondence between the ex vivo and the in vivo patterns.

      (1) Results, Lines 167-170: "While multiple extracellular recordings have been performed previously (Eisenhart et al., 2000), these results present the first quantitative analysis of motor units activated throughout the crawling cycle. The In-Phase units are expected to control the contraction stage by exciting or inhibiting the longitudinal or circular muscles, respectively, and the Anti-Phase units to control the elongation stage by exciting or inhibiting the circular or longitudinal muscles, respectively."  

      Reviewer: The first line above is misleading. The study by Puhl and Mesce (2008, J. Neurosci, 28:4192- 420) contains a comprehensive analysis of the motoneurons active during fictive crawling with the aim of characterizing their roles and phase relationships and solidifying the idea that the oscillator for crawling resides in a single ganglion. Intracellular recordings from a number of key crawl-related motoneurons were made in combination with extracellular recordings of motoneuron DE-3, a key monitor of crawling. In their paper, it was shown that motoneurons AE, VE-4, DI-1, VI-2, and CV were all correlated with crawl activity, and fired repeatedly either in phase or out-of-phase with DE-3. They were shown to be either excitatory or inhibitory. At a minimum, the above paper should be cited. 

      The sentence in the submitted manuscript explicitly refers to the quantitative analysis of extracellular recordings, but we recognize that it may lead to confusion. We have now added a clarification (lines 197-199). 

      The article by Puhl and Mesce 2008 shows very nice intracellular recordings of the AE, CV, VE-4, DE-3, DI-1, and Vi-2, accompanied by extracellular recordings of DE-3 in the DP nerve. In all cases, there is only one intracellular recording paired with the DP nerve recording.

      While it is possible to perform up to 3-4 simultaneous intracellular recordings, these are technically challenging, and more so when the recordings have to last 10-20 minutes. Due to this difficulty, and because our objective was to record multiple units simultaneously in order to comprehensively describe the different crawling stages, we implemented the spike sorting analysis on multiple extracellular recordings. This approach enabled us to reliably obtain multiple units per experiment and thus execute a quantitative analysis of the activity of each identified unit.

      The article by Puhl and Mesce 2008 mentions several quantitative aspects of the neurons that fire in-phase or out-of-phase with DE-3, but, as far as we understand, there is no figure that summarizes activity levels and span in the way Figures 4 and 6 do in the current manuscript. To the best of our knowledge, no previous work renders this information.

      It is very important for us to emphasize that the work by Puhl and Mesce was seminal for our research. We cited it four times in the original manuscript and 10 times in the present version. But, like any important discovery, it sets the ground for further work that can refine certain measurements that in the original discovery were not central.

      This is why we believe that the cited sentence in our manuscript is not misleading.  However, to comply with the requirement of Reviewer #1, we added a sentence preceding the mentioned paragraph (lines 185-187) that acknowledges the description made using intracellular recordings, and explains the need for implementing the approach we chose.

      The submitted paper would be strengthened if some of these previously identified motoneurons were again recorded with intracellular electrodes and concomitant NS cell stimulation. The power of the leech preparation is that cells can be identified as individuals with dual somatic (intracellular) and axonal recordings (extracellular). 

      Most of the motoneurons mentioned by Reviewer #1 are located on the opposite side (dorsal) of the ganglion to NS (ventral), and therefore, simultaneous intracellular recordings in the context of fictive crawling are challenging.

      In the publication of Rodriguez et al. 2009, Mariano Rodriguez did manage to record NS from the dorsal side together with DE-3 and MN-L (!) and this led to the discovery that these motoneurons are electrically coupled, but the recurrent inhibitory circuit masks this interaction. Repeating this type of experiments during crawling, which requires stable recordings for around 15 minutes, is not a reasonable experimental setting.

      Rodriguez et al. 2012 shows intracellular recordings of motoneurons AE and CV during crawling in conjunction with NS, and their activity presented the expected correlation. 

      The shortfall of this aspect of the study (Figure 5) is that the extracellular units have not been identified here. 

      The Reviewer is right in that the extracellular units have not been identified in terms of cell identity. As we explained earlier, most motoneurons are on the opposite side (ventral/dorsal) of the ganglion relative to NS. 

      However, we do characterize the units in terms of the nerve through which they project to the periphery and their activity phase. In lines 345-349 we use this information and, based on published work, we propose possible cellular identities of the different units.

      In xfact, these units might not even be motoneurons. 

      We are surprised by this comment. The classical work of Ort and collaborators (1974) showed that spikes detected in extracellular nerve recordings were emitted by specific motoneurons, and several previous publications have validated extracellular nerve recordings as a means to study fictive motor patterns (Wittenberg & Kristan 1992, Shaw & Kristan 1997, Eisenhart et al. 2000).

      For further reassurance, we only took in consideration units whose activity was locked to DE3; any non-rhythmical activity was filtered out (see lines 433-435). 

      They could represent activity from the centrally located sensory neurons, dopamine-modulated afferent neurons or peripherally projecting modulatory neurons. 

      Peripheral nerves also contain axons from sensory neurons. However, in a previous article, we studied the activity of mechanosensory neurons (Alonso et al. 2020) and showed that they remain silent during crawling. Moreover, the low-threshold T sensory neurons are inhibited in phase with DE-3 bursts and NS IPSPs (Kearney et al. 2022). Alonso et al. 2000 showed that spiking activity of T cells affects the crawling motor pattern, revealing the relevance of keeping them silent.

      What does the Reviewer mean by “dopamine-modulated afferents”? We are not aware of this category of leech neurons.

      The neuromodulatory Rz neurons project peripherally through the recorded nerves, but intracellular recordings of these neurons from our lab show no rhythmic activity in those cells during dopamine-induced crawling.

      Essentially, they may not have much to do with the crawl motor pattern at all.

      Does the Reviewer consider that neurons engaged in a coherent rhythmic firing could be unrelated to the pattern? As indicated above, the units reported in our manuscript were selected because dopamine evoked their rhythmic activity, locked to DE-3. 

      Does the Reviewer consider that dopamine could evoke spurious neuronal activity?

      (2) Results Lines 206-210: "with the elongation and contraction stages of in vivo behavior. However the isometric stages displayed in vivo have no obvious counterpart in the electrophysiological recordings. It is important to consider that the rhythmic movement of successive segments along the antero-posterior axis of the animal requires a delay signal that allows the appropriate propagation of the metachronal wave, and this signal is probably absent in the isolated ganglion." 

      Reviewer: The so-called isometric stages, indeed, have an electrophysiological counterpart due in part to the overlapping activities across segments. This submitted paper would be considerably strengthened if it referred to the body of work that has examined how the individual crawl oscillators operate in a fully intact nerve cord, excised from the body but with all the ganglia (and cephalic ganglion) attached. Puhl and Mesce 2010 (J. Neurosci 30: 2373-2383) and Puhl et al. 2012 (J. Neurosci, 32:17646 -17657) have shown that "appropriate propagation of the metachronal wave" requires the brain, especially cell R3b-1. They also show that the long-distance projecting cell R3b-1 synapses with the CV motoneuron, providing rhythmic excitatory input to it.  

      We would like to draw the Reviewer’s attention to the fact that Puhl and Mesce 2008, 2010 and Puhl et al. 2012 characterized crawling in intact (or nearly intact) animals considering the whole body. In our in vivo analysis, we studied the changes in length of the whole animal and of sections demarcated by the drawn points, as described in the Materials and Methods/Behavioral

      Experiments. Because of this different analysis, we defined “isometric” stages as those in which a given section of the animal does not change its length. We now clarify this (line 230).

      In the paragraph cited by the Reviewer, we intended to state that, in the context of our study, the intersegmental lag caused by the coordinating mechanisms has no counterpart “in the electrophysiological recordings of motoneurons in the isolated ganglia”. We have now completed this idea with the expression underlined in the previous sentence (line 231).

      As the Reviewer indicates, in the intact nerve cord the behavioral isometric stages correspond to the “waiting time” between segments. We did refer to the metachronal order but did not cite the articles by Puhl and Mesce 2010 and Puhl et al. 2012; we now do so (lines 234).

      For this and other reasons, the paper would be much more informative and exciting if the impacts of the NS cell were studied in a fully intact nerve cord. Those studies have never been done, and it would be exciting to see how and if the effects of NS cell manipulation deviated from those in the single ganglion.  

      The Reviewer may consider that a systematic analysis of multiple nerves in several ganglia along the whole nerve cord would have been a different enterprise than the one we carried out. The Reviewer is right in recognizing the interest of such study, but in our opinion, the value of the present work lies in presenting a thorough quantitative analysis of multiple nerves to demonstrate its usefulness for the study of the network underlying leech crawling. In this manuscript, we used it to analyze the role of the premotor NS neuron. Without the recording of units firing in-phase and out-ofphase with DE-3, we would have been unable to assess the span of NS effects.

      (3) Discussion Lines 322-324. "The absence of descending brain signals and/or peripheral signals are assumed as important factors in determining the cycle period and the sequence at which the different behavioral stages take place." 

      Reviewer: The authors could strengthen their paper by including a more complete picture of what is known about the control of crawling. For example, Puhl et al. 2012 (J Neurosci, 32:17646-17657) demonstrated that the descending brain neuron R3b-1 plays a major role in establishing the crawlcycle frequency. With increased R3b-1 cell stimulation, DE-3 periods substantially shortened throughout the entire nerve cord. Thus, the importance of descending brain inputs should not be merely assumed; empirical evidence exists.  

      We now strengthen the concept using “known descending brain signals” (line 358) and cite Puhl et al. 2012. We believe that extending the discussion to cell R3b-1 does not contribute meaningfully to the focus of this manuscript.

      (4) Discussion Lines 325-327: "the sequence of events, and the proportion of the active cycle dedicated to elongation and contraction were remarkably similar in both experimental settings. This suggests that the network activated in the isolated ganglion is the one underlying the motor behavior." 

      Reviewer: The results and conclusions drawn in the current manuscript mirror those previously reported by Puhl and Mesce (2008, J. Neurosci, 28:4192- 420) who first demonstrated that the essential pattern-generating elements for leech crawling were contained in each of the segmental ganglia comprising the nerve cord. Furthermore, the authors showed that the duty cycle of DE-3, in a single ganglion treated with dopamine, was statistically indistinguishable from the DE-3 duty cycle measured in an intact nerve cord showing spontaneous fictive crawling, in an intact nerve cord induced to crawl via dopamine, and in the intact behaving animal. What was statistically significant, however, was that the DE-3 burst period was greatly reduced in the intact animal (i.e., a higher crawl frequency), which was replicated in the submitted paper.  

      There is no doubt that the article by Puhl and Mesce 2008 is seminal to the work we present here. The Reviewer seems to suggest that we do not recognize the value of this work. The contrary is true, all our related papers cite this important breakthrough. We cite the paper very early in the article in the Introduction (see lines 51 and 52-53). Likely, we would like the Reviewer to recognize the novelty of the current report. To clarify what has been shown and what is new in our manuscript, considerer the following:

      i. Figures 1-6 in Puhl and Mesce 2008 provide representative intracellular recordings that describe neurons that fire in phase and out of phase relative to DE-3. Some general measurements are given in the text, but none of these figures quantify the relative activity of neurons that fire in different stages; only DE-3 activity was quantified. A quantitative description of multiple units active in phase and out of phase with DE-3 is presented here for the first time, are we wrong? This quantification is particularly relevant when assessing how a treatment affects the function of the circuit.

      ii. Regarding the cycle period, we referred to the work from the Kristan lab, which reported this value long before the requested reference. We now cite Puhl and Mesce 2008 in lines 222 regarding in vivo measurements, and in line 221 regarding isolated ganglia.

      iii. Regarding the duty cycle: 

      Puhl and Mesce 2008 measured the duty cycle of DE-3 in three configurations: a. spontaneous whole cord, b. DA-mediated whole cord and c. DA mediated single ganglion crawling. However, it does not report the duty cycle of neurons out-of-phase with DE-3. Our current manuscript carried out this analysis. One could argue that the silence between DE-3 bursts captures that value, but this is a speculation that needed a proper measure.

      Puhl and Mesce 2008 does not indicate the duty cycle of the contraction and elongation stages in vivo. Our current manuscript does. 

      Therefore, the sentence cited by the Reviewer refers to data presented in this manuscript, and not in any prior manuscript. It is true that Puhl and Mesce 2008 inspire the intuition that the sentence is true, but does not present the data that the current manuscript does.

      Finally, our study focused only on the body sections corresponding to the same segmental range used in the ex vivo experiments, rather than the whole animal. The comparison was made only to validate that the duty cycles of neurons firing in phase and out of phase with DE-3 matched the dynamic stages in the studied sections of the leech (line 364).

      In my opinion, the novelty of the results reported in the submitted manuscript is diminished in the light of previously published studies. At a minimum, the previous studies should be cited, and the authors should provide additional rationale for conducting their studies. They need to explain in the discussion how their approach provided additional insights into what has already been reported.  

      Throughout our reply, we have provided a detailed explanation of the rationale and necessity behind each experiment. Following the Reviewer’s suggestion, we have rephrased the research objectives, included what is known from our previously published work, and highlighted the substantial new data contributed by the present study. See lines 80-85. 

      Additionally, we further cite our published article in lines 93, 104, 138, 146 and 250. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):  

      The paper is well-written overall. The findings are clearly presented, and the data seems solid overall. I do have, however, a few major and some minor comments representing some concerns.

      My major comments are below. 

      (1) This may seem somewhat semantic, yet, it has implications on the way the data is presented and moreover on the conclusions drawn - a single ganglion cannot show fictive crawling. It can demonstrate rhythmic patterns of activity that may serve in the (fictive) crawling motor pattern. The latter is a result of the intrinsic within single-ganglion connectivity AND the inter-ganglia connections and interactions (coupling) among the sequential ganglia. It may be affected by both short-range and long-range connections (e.g., descending inputs) along the ganglia chain. 

      Semantics is not a trivial issue in science communication. It entails metaphors that enter the bibliography as commonly used “shortcuts” to a complex concept that are adopted by a community of researchers. And yes, indeed, they can be misleading.

      However, if recording the activity in an isolated ganglion shows that a wide group of motoneurons, that control known muscle movements, presents a rhythmic output that maintains the appropriate cycle period and phase relationships, the “shortcut” is incomplete but could be valid (Puhl and Mesce 2008). If we were to include the phase lag component, a single ganglion cannot generate the fictive motor output.

      Because any new study builds knowledge on the basis of the cited bibliography, the way we name concepts is a sensitive point. Adopting the terminology used by previous publications (Puhl and Mesce 2008) seems important to allow readers to follow the development of knowledge. However, attending the observation made by Reviewer #2, we included a sentence clarifying that the concept “fictive crawling” does not include intersegmental connectivity (lines 54-57)

      (2) The point above is even more critical where the authors set to compare the motor pattern in single ganglia with the intact animals. It would have made much more sense to add a description of the motor pattern of a chain of interconnected ganglia. The latter would be expected to better resemble the intact animal. Furthermore, this project would have benefitted from a three-way comparison (isolated ganglion-interconnected ganglia-intact animal.  

      As we answered to Reviewer #1, the present manuscript does not intend to present a thorough study on how the activity in the isolated nervous system compares with the animal behavior. To do so we would have needed to perform a completely different set of experiments. To better define the relevance of our comparison with the in vivo experiments we rephrased the objective of the behavioral analysis (lines 197-199).

      The main aim of the manuscript is to learn the role of premotor NS neurons in the crawling motor pattern studied using a readout (spike sorting in extracellular nerve recordings) that allows simultaneous screening of a larger number of units than in any previous study, in order to determine whether and how a recurrent inhibitory peripheral circuit is involved in coordinating or modulating the rhythmic motor pattern.

      Our rationale was that the known effect of NS on one particular motoneuron (DE-3) may have overlooked a more general effect on crawling (lines 253-257). Moreover, we wanted to investigate whether this effect was due to the recurrent inhibitory circuit or if other elements were involved, and to study whether the modulation was mediated by the recurrent synapse between NS and the motoneurons.

      In the context of this aim we studied the rhythmic activity of cell DE-3, together with motoneurons that fire in-phase and anti-phase, in isolated ganglia (Figure 4). To reveal the effect of NS manipulation we applied a quantitative analysis that showed the phase-specific effect of NS (Figure 6). 

      Given that this is the first study using a spike sorting algorithm to detect and describe the activity of motoneurons in nerve recordings we found it reasonable to compare these results with an in vivo study; thus, providing information to the general reader, that supports the correspondence between the ex vivo and the in vivo patterns.

      (3) Two previous studies by the same group are repeatedly mentioned (Rela and Szczupak, 2003; Rodriguez et al., 2009) and serve as a basis for the current work. The aim of one of these previous studies was to assess the role of the NS neurons in regulating the function of motor networks. The other (Rodriguez et al., 2009) reported on a neuron (the NS) that can regulate the crawling motor pattern. LL 71-74 of the current report presents the aim of this study as evaluating the role of the known connectivity of the premotor NS neuron in shaping the crawling motor pattern. The authors should make it very clear what indeed served as background knowledge, what exactly was known about the circuitry beforehand, and what is different and new in the current study. 

      Rela and Szczupak 2003 and Rodriguez et al. 2009 analyze the interactions of motoneurons with NS. We believe that Reviewer #2 refers here to Rodriguez et al. 2012. A similar observation was made by Reviewer #1. Below, we copy the answer previously stated:

      Following the Reviewer’s suggestion, we have rephrased the research objectives, included what is known from our previously published work, and highlighted the substantial new data contributed by the present study. See lines 80-85. 

      Additionally, we further cite our published article in lines 93, 104, 138, 146 and 250. 

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):  

      Please edit for correct word usage. 

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):  

      Minor Concerns 

      (1) LL33-36: These lines are somewhat vague and non-informative. Why is the functional organization of motor systems an open question? What are the mechanisms at the level of the nerve cord that are an open question? Maybe be more explicit? 

      We did as suggested (lines 30-32).

      (2) L62: The homology between the NS neurons and the vertebrate Renshaw cells is mentioned already in the Abstract and here again. While a reference is provided (citing the lead author of this current work), the reader would benefit from some further short words of explanation regarding the alleged homology. 

      We included a description of Renshaw cell connectivity (lines 64-65).

      (3) LL90-92: The NS recording in Figure 1 (similar to Figure 3 in Rodriguez et al.) demonstrates clear distinct IPSPs. Could these be correlated with DE-3 spikes? 

      We investigated this correlation in detail and the answer is that there is no strictly a 1:1 DE-3 spike to IPSP correlation. NS receives inputs from other dorsal and ventral excitors of longitudinal muscles, and the NS trace is too “noisy” to reflect any short-term correlation. Originally we proposed that the NS IPSPs were due to the polysynaptic interaction between the MN and NS (Rodríguez et al. 2012). However, the present work demonstrates that the IPSPs in NS are caused by a source upstream from the MNs. 

      (4) LL145-145: Do you mean - inhibitory signals FROM NS premotor neurons? Not clear. 

      We see the confusion, and we rewrote the sentence (lines 164). We hope it is clearer now: “…inhibitory signals onto NS premotor neurons were transmitted to DE-3 motoneurons via rectifying electrical synapses and counteracted their excitatory drive during crawling, limiting their firing frequency.”

      (5) LL153-154: Why isn't AA included in Figure 4A? 

      Reading our original text, the Reviewer #1 is right in expecting to see the AA recording. We changed the sentence: “we performed extracellular recordings of DP along with AA and/or PP root nerves” (lines 171-172).

      We dissected the three nerves but, unfortunately, we did not always obtain good recordings from the three of them.

      (6) LL237-238: The statistical significance (B- antiphase) is not clear. Furthermore, with N of 7-8, I'm not sure the parametric tests utilized are appropriate. 

      Regarding the Reviewer's concern about the tests, please note that all the assumptions made for each model were tested (see now Materials and Methods lines 466-467).The information on each model is provided in Supplementary Table 2 under the column 'Model, random effect,' which specifies whether a Linear Mixed Model (LMM) or a Generalized Linear Mixed Model (GLMM) was implemented. For GLMMs, the corresponding distribution and link function are also specified. For the analysis of Max bFF of Anti-Phase motor units, we found a significant interaction between epoch and treatment, indicating a difference between treatments. This is indicated on the left of the y-axis (##). In control experiments, all three comparisons (pre-test, pre-post, test-post) show significant differences in Max bFF: this variable decreased (slightly but significantly) along the subsequent epochs, suggesting a change over time. We now corrected the text to indicate that these changes were small (line 268). In contrast, Max bFF in depo experiments remained stable between pre-test and pre-post, but significantly decreased between the depo and post epochs. Thus, in our view the comparison between control and the test supports the conclusion that NS depolarization was limited to counteracting this decrease (lines 270-273). Supplementary Table 2 provides the significance and modeled estimated ratio for each comparison in the column for pairwise simple contrasts.

      Thanks to this question, we realized that the nomenclature used in the table for the epochs (pre - depo - post) needed to be changed to pre - test - post, and we have now corrected it.

      (7) LL240-241: I fail to see a difference from Control. 

      For the Relative HW of In-Phase units, we also found a significant interaction between epoch and treatment, indicating a difference between treatments, as denoted to the left of the y-axis (#). Then, the significance of the comparisons across epochs within each treatment are shown in the figure (*). What is important to notice is that obtaining the same significance for each treatment does not imply identical results, but we failed to describe this in our original text and we do now in lines 275-279.

      (8) LL244-245: I must admit that Table 2 is beyond me. Maybe add some detail or point out to the reader what is important (if at all). 

      We have now clarified what each column of the tables indicates in the corresponding legends. 

      Here, we also share an insight into how the experiments were designed and analyzed:

      To account for possible temporal drifts of the variables during the recordings that could mask or confuse the results, we compared two experimental series: one in which NS was subjected to depolarizing current pulses (depo), and another series (ctrl) in which the neurons were not depolarized.

      The statistical analysis was made using Linear Mixed Models (LMMs) or Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMMs). In these analyses treatments and epochs are used as explanatory variables to evaluate the interaction between these factors. These models allow us to determine whether changes in each variable across epochs differ depending on the treatment. For example, whether the variation in firing frequency from pre to test to post differs between control experiments and those in which NS was depolarized.

      A significant interaction between treatment and epoch indicates that NS depolarization affected the variable. In such cases, we performed pairwise comparisons between epochs (pre-test, test-post, pre-post) within each treatment. In contrast, the absence of a significant interaction can result from two possibilities: either the variable did not change across epoch in either treatment, or a similar temporal drift occurred in both cases.

      (9) LL245-256: Move this paragraph to the discussion. 

      Because we introduced a rationale for the experiments described in Figure 6 (lines 282-284) the paragraph was mostly removed, but the part that supports the methodological approach was left.

      (10)  LL259-260: see my second minor point above. This is explained in LL270-272 for the first time. 

      We amended according to comment (2).

      (11) Figures: The quantitative analysis shown in Figure 3B is very useful. Why isn't this type of analysis utilized for the comparisons shown in Figures 4 and 6? 

      We chose different ways of plotting the data based on their nature. In Figure 3B, we present data from an identified neuron (DE-3) recorded in different experiments. In contrast, in Figure 6 we analyze data from neurons classified into the same group based on their activity during the fictive crawling cycle, but their individual identity was not ascertained. Therefore, we consider it important to plot the results for each unit individually, to assess the effect of temporal drift and NS depolarization.

      (12) Figures: Figure 7 is meant to be compared to Figure 1C; the point being the addition of an inhibitory connection onto the NS neuron. Why are other details of the figure also different (different colored M)? 

      While Figure 1C illustrates the known connection between NS and both DE-3 and CV motoneurons, Figure 7 shows the connections between NS and the different groups of motor units described in this study. The units are represented in the circuit using the same colors that identify them in Figures 4 and 6. Since the CV motoneuron was not recorded in this study, the circuit represents the AntiPhase neurons but does not identify them with CV. Figure 7 legend now clarifies what the colors represent, and Figure 1C has been updated to match the same color scheme.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      This work addresses an important question in the field of Drosophila aggression and mating- prior social isolation is known to increase aggression in males by increased lunging, which is suppressed by group housing (GH). However, it is also known that single-housed (SH) males, despite their higher attempts to court females, are less successful. Here, Gao et al., developed a modified aggression assay, to address this issue by recording aggression in Drosophila males for 2 hours, over a virgin female which is immobilized by burying its head in the food. They found that while SH males frequently lunge in this assay, GH males switch to higher intensity but very low-frequency tussling. Constitutive neuronal silencing and activation experiments implicate cVA sensing Or67d neurons promoting high-frequency lunging, similar to earlier studies, whereas Or47b neurons promote low-frequency but higher intensity tussling. Using optogenetic activation they found that three pairs of pC1 neurons- pC1SS2 increase tussling. While P1a neurons, previously implicated in promoting aggression and courtship, did not increase tussling in optogenetic activation (in the dark), they could promote aggressive tussling in thermogenetic activation carried out in the presence of visible light. It was further suggested, using a further modified aggression assay that GH males use increased tussling and are able to maintain territorial control, providing them mating advantage over SI males and this may partially overcome the effect of aging in GH males.

      Strengths

      Using a series of clever neurogenetic and behavioral approaches, subsets of ORNs and pC1 neurons were implicated in promoting tussling behaviors. The authors devised a new paradigm to assay for territory control which appears better than earlier paradigms that used a food cup (Chen et al, 2002), as this new assay is relatively clutter-free, and can be eventually automated using computer vision approaches. The manuscript is generally well-written, and the claims made are largely supported by the data.

      Thank you for your precise summary of our study, and being very positive on the novelty and significance of the study.

      Weaknesses

      I have a few concerns regarding some of the evidence presented and claims made as well as a description of the methodology, which needs to be clarified and extended further.

      (1) Typical paradigms for assaying aggression in Drosophila males last for 20-30 minutes in the presence of nutritious food/yeast paste/females or all of these (Chen et al. 2002, Nilsen et al., 2004, Dierick et al. 2007, Dankert et al., 2009, Certel & Kravitz 2012). The paradigm described in Figure 1 A, while important and more amenable for video recording and computational analysis, seems a modification of the assay from Kravitz lab (Chen et al., 2002), which involved using a female over which males fight on a food cup. The modifications include a flat surface with a central food patch and a female with its head buried in the food, (fixed female) and much longer adaptation and recording times respectively (30 minutes, 2 hours), so in that sense, this is not a 'new' paradigm but a modification of an existing paradigm and its description as new should be appropriately toned down. It would also be important to cite these earlier studies appropriately while describing the assay.

      We now toned down the description of the paradigm and cited more related references.

      (2) Lunging is described as a 'low intensity' aggression (line 111 and associated text), however, it is considered a mid to high-intensity aggressive behavior, as compared to other lower-intensity behaviors such as wing flicks, chase, and fencing. Lunging therefore is lower in intensity 'relative' to higher intensity tussling but not in absolute terms and it should be mentioned clearly.

      We have modified the description as suggested.

      (3) It is often difficult to distinguish faithfully between boxing and tussling and therefore, these behaviors are often clubbed together as box, tussle by Nielsen et al., 2004 in their Markov chain analysis as well as a more detailed recent study of male aggression (Simon & Heberlein, 2020). Therefore, authors can either reconsider the description of behavior as 'box, tussle' or consider providing a video representation/computational classifier to distinguish between box and tussle behaviors.

      Indeed, we could not faithfully distinguish boxing and tussling. To address this concern, we now made textual changes in the result section we occasionally observed the high-intensity boxing and tussling behavior in male flies, which are difficult to distinguish and hereafter simply referred to as tussling.

      We also added this information in the Materials and Methods section Tussling is often mixed with boxing, in which both flies rear up and strike the opponent with forelegs. Since boxing is often transient and difficult to distinguish from tussling, we referred to the mixed boxing and tussling behavior simply as tussling.

      (4) Simon & Heberlein, 2020 showed that increased boxing & tussling precede the formation of a dominance hierarchy in males, and lunges are used subsequently to maintain this dominant status. This study should be cited and discussed appropriately while introducing the paradigm.

      We now cited this important study in both the Introduction and Discussion sections.

      (5) It would be helpful to provide more methodological details about the assay, for instance, a video can be helpful showing how the males are introduced in the assay chamber, are they simply dropped to the floor when the film is removed after 30 minutes (Figures 1-2)?

      We now provided more detailed description about behavioral assays and how we analyze them. For example All testers were loaded by cold anesthesia. After a 30-minute adaptation, the film was gently removed to allow the two males to fell into the behavioral chamber, and the aggressive behavior was recorded for 2 hours.

      (6) The strain of Canton-S (CS) flies used should be mentioned as different strains of CS can have varying levels of aggression, for instance, CS from Martin Heisenberg lab shows very high levels of aggressive lunges. Are the CS lines used in this study isogenized? Are various genetic lines outcrossed into this CS background? In the methods, it is not clear how the white gene levels were controlled for various aggression experiments as it is known to affect aggression (Hoyer et al. 2008).

      We used the wtcs flies from Baker lab in Janelia Research Campus, and are not sure where they are originated. We appreciate your concern on the use of wild-type strains as they may show different fighting levels, but this study mainly used wild-type strains to compare behavioral differences between SH and GH males. All flies tested in this study are in w+ background, based on w+ balancers flies but are not backcrossed. We have listed detailed genotypes of all tested flies in Table S1 in the revised manuscript.

      (7) How important it is to use a fixed female for the assay to induce tussling? Do these females remain active throughout the assay period of 2.5 hours? Is it possible to use decapitated virgin females for the assay? How will that affect male behaviors?

      We used a fixed female to restrict it in the center of food. These females remain active throughout the assay as their legs and abdomens can still move. Such design intends to combine the attractive effects from both female and food. One can also use decapitated females, but in this case, males can push the decapitated female into anywhere in the behavioral chamber. The logic to use fixed females has now been added in the Materials and Methods section of the revised manuscript.

      (8) Raster plots in Figure 2 suggest a complete lack of tussling in SH males in the first 60 minutes of the encounter, which is surprising given the longer duration of the assay as compared to earlier studies (Nielsen et al. 2004, Simon & Heberlein, 2020 and others), which are able to pick up tussling in a shorter duration of recording time. Also, the duration for tussling is much longer in this study as compared to shorter tussles shown by earlier studies. Is this due to differences in the paradigm used, strain of flies, or some other factor? While the bar plots in Figure 2D show some tussling in SH males, maybe an analysis of raster plots of various videos can be provided in the main text and included as a supplementary figure to address this.

      Indeed, tussling is very low in SH males in our paradigm, which may be due to different genetic backgrounds and behavioral assays. Since tussling behavior is a rare fighting form, it is not surprising to see variation between studies from different labs. Nevertheless, this study compared tussling behaviors in SH and GH males, and our finding that GH males show much more tussling behaviors is convincing. The longer duration of tussling in our paradigm may also be due to the modified behavioral paradigm, which also supports that tussling is a high-level fighting form.

      (9) Neuronal activation experiments suggesting the involvement of pC1SS2 neurons are quite interesting. Further, the role of P1a neurons was demonstrated to be involved in increasing tussling in thermogenetic activation in the presence of light (Figure 4, Supplement 1), which is quite important as the role of vision in optogenetic activation experiments, which required to be carried out in dark, is often not mentioned. However, in the discussion (lines 309-310) it is mentioned that PC1SS2 neurons are 'necessary and sufficient' for inducing tussling. Given that P1a neurons were shown to be involved in promoting tussling, this statement should be toned down.

      Thank you for this important comment. We now toned down the statement on pC1SS2 function.

      (10) Are Or47b neurons connected to pC1SS2 or P1a neurons?

      We conducted pathway analysis in the FlyWire electron microscopy database to investigate the connection between Or47b neurons and pC1 neurons. The results indicate that at least three levels of interneurons are required to establish a connection from Or47b neurons to pC1 neurons. Although the FlyWire database currently only contains neuronal data from female brains, they provide a reference for circuit connect in males.

      (11) The paradigm for territory control is quite interesting and subsequent mating advantage experiments are an important addition to the eventual outcome of the aggressive strategy deployed by the males as per their prior housing conditions. It would be important to comment on the 'fitness outcome' of these encounters. For instance, is there any fitness advantage of using tussling by GH males as compared to lunging by SH males? The authors may consider analyzing the number of eggs laid and eclosed progenies from these encounters to address this.

      Thank you for this suggestion. We agree with you and other reviewers that increased tussling behaviors correlate with better mating competition, but it is difficult for us to make a direct link between them. Thus, in the revised manuscript, we prefer to tone down this statement but not expanding on this part.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary

      Gao et al. investigated the change of aggression strategies by the social experience and its biological significance by using Drosophila. Two modes of inter-male aggression in Drosophila are known lunging, high-frequency but weak mode, and tussling, low-frequency but more vigorous mode. Previous studies have mainly focused on the lunging. In this paper, the authors developed a new behavioral experiment system for observing tussling behavior and found that tussling is enhanced by group rearing while lunging is suppressed. They then searched for neurons involved in the generation of tussling. Although olfactory receptors named Or67d and Or65a have previously been reported to function in the control of lunging, the authors found that these neurons do not function in the execution of tussling, and another olfactory receptor, Or47b, is required for tussling, as shown by the inhibition of neuronal activity and the gene knockdown experiments. Further optogenetic experiments identified a small number of central neurons pC1[SS2] that induce the tussling specifically. In order to further explore the ecological significance of the aggression mode change in group rearing, a new behavioral experiment was performed to examine territorial control and mating competition. Finally, the authors found that differences in the social experience (group vs. solitary rearing) are important in these biologically significant competitions. These results add a new perspective to the study of aggressive behavior in Drosophila. Furthermore, this study proposes an interesting general model in which the social experience-modified behavioral changes play a role in reproductive success.

      Strengths

      A behavioral experiment system that allows stable observation of tussling, which could not be easily analyzed due to its low frequency, would be very useful. The experimental setup itself is relatively simple, just the addition of a female to the platform, so it should be applicable to future research. The finding about the relationship between the social experience and the aggression mode change is quite novel. Although the intensity of aggression changes with the social experience was already reported in several papers (Liu et al., 2011, etc), the fact that the behavioral mode itself changes significantly has rarely been addressed and is extremely interesting. The identification of sensory and central neurons required for the tussling makes appropriate use of the genetic tools and the results are clear. A major strength of the neurobiology in this study is the finding that another group of neurons (Or47b-expressing olfactory neurons and pC1[SS2] neurons), distinct from the group of neurons previously thought to be involved in low-intensity aggression (i.e. lunging), function in the tussling behavior. Further investigation of the detailed circuit analysis is expected to elucidate the neural substrate of the conflict between the two aggression modes.

      Thank you for the acknowledgment of the novelty and significance of the study, and your suggestions for improving the manuscript.

      Weaknesses

      The experimental systems examining the territory control and the reproductive competition in Figure 5 are novel and have advantages in exploring their biological significance. However, at this stage, the authors' claim is weak since they only show the effects of age and social experience on territorial and mating behaviors, but do not experimentally demonstrate the influence of aggression mode change itself. In the Abstract, the authors state that these findings reveal how social experience shapes fighting strategies to optimize reproductive success. This is the most important perspective of the present study, and it would be necessary to show directly that the change of aggression mode by social experience contributes to reproductive success.

      We agree that our data did not directly show that it is the change of aggression mode that results in territory and reproductive advantages in GH males. To address the concern, we have toned down the statement throughout the manuscript. For example, we made textual changes in the abstract as following

      Moreover, shifting from lunging to tussling in socially enriched males is accompanied with better territory control and mating success, mitigating the disadvantages associated with aging. Our findings identify distinct sensory and central neurons for two fighting forms and suggest how social experience shapes fighting strategies to optimize reproductive success.

      In addition, a detailed description of the tussling is lacking. For example, the authors state that the tussling is less frequent but more vigorous than lunging, but while experimental data are presented on the frequency, the intensity seems to be subjective. The intensity is certainly clear from the supplementary video, but it would be necessary to evaluate the intensity itself using some index. Another problem is that there is no clear explanation of how to determine the tussling. A detailed method is required for the reproducibility of the experiment.

      Thank you for this important suggestion. We now analyzed duration of tussling and lunging, and found that a lunging event is often very short (less than 0.2s), while a tussling event may last from seconds to minutes. This new data is added as Figure 2G. In addition, we also provided more detailed methods regarding to tussling behavior

      .<br /> Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In this manuscript, Gao et al. presented a series of intriguing data that collectively suggest that tussling, a form of high-intensity fighting among male fruit flies (Drosophila melanogaster) has a unique function and is controlled by a dedicated neural circuit. Based on the results of behavioral assays, they argue that increased tussling among socially experienced males promotes access to resources. They also concluded that tussling is controlled by a class of olfactory sensory neurons and sexually dimorphic central neurons that are distinct from pathways known to control lunges, a common male-type attack behavior.

      A major strength of this work is that it is the first attempt to characterize the behavioral function and neural circuit associated with Drosophila tussling. Many animal species use both low-intensity and high-intensity tactics to resolve conflicts. High-intensity tactics are mostly reserved for escalated fights, which are relatively rare. Because of this, tussling in the flies, like high-intensity fights in other animal species, has not been systematically investigated. Previous studies on fly aggressive behavior have often used socially isolated, relatively young flies within a short observation duration. Their discovery that 1) older (14-days-old) flies tend to tussle more often than younger (2-days-old) flies, 2) group-reared flies tend to tussle more often than socially isolated flies, and 3) flies tend to tussle at a later stage (mostly ~15 minutes after the onset of fighting), are the result of their creativity to look outside of conventional experimental settings. These new findings are keys for quantitatively characterizing this interesting yet under-studied behavior.

      Precisely because their initial approach was creative, it is regrettable that the authors missed the opportunity to effectively integrate preceding studies in their rationale or conclusions, which sometimes led to premature claims. Also, while each experiment contains an intriguing finding, these are poorly related to each other. This obscures the central conclusion of this work. The perceived weaknesses are discussed in detail below.

      Thank you for the precise summary of the key findings and novelty of the study, and your insightful suggestions.

      Most importantly, the authors' definition of "tussling" is unclear because they did not explain how they quantified lunges and tussling, even though the central focus of the manuscript is behavior. Supplemental movies S1 and S2 appear to include "tussling" bouts in which 2 flies lunge at each other in rapid succession, and supplemental movie S3 appears to include bouts of "holding", in which one fly holds the opponent's wings and shakes vigorously. These cases raise a concern that their behavior classification is arbitrary. Specifically, lunges and tussling should be objectively distinguished because one of their conclusions is that these two actions are controlled by separate neural circuits. It is impossible to evaluate the credibility of their behavioral data without clearly describing a criterion of each behavior.

      Thank you for this very important suggestion. We now provided more detailed description of the two fighting forms in the Materials and Methods section. See below

      Lunging is characterized by a male raising its forelegs and quickly striking the opponent, and each lunge typically lasts less than 0.2 seconds through detailed analysis. Tussling is characterized by both males using their forelegs and bodies to tumble over each other, and this behavior may last from seconds to minutes. Tussling is often mixed with boxing, in which both flies rear up and strike the opponent with forelegs. Since boxing is often transient and difficult to distinguish from tussling, we referred to the mixed boxing and tussling behavior simply as tussling. As we manually analyze tussling for 2 hours for each pair of males, it is possible that we may miss some tussling events, especially those quick ones.

      It is also confusing that the authors completely skipped the characterization of the tussling-controlling neurons they claimed to have identified. These neurons (a subset of so-called pC1 neurons labeled by previously described split-GAL4 line pC1SS2) are central to this manuscript, but the only information the authors have provided is its gross morphology in a low-resolution image (Figure 4D, E) and a statement that "only 3 pairs of pC1SS2 neurons whose function is both necessary and sufficient for inducing tussling in males" (lines 310-311). The evidence that supports this claim isn't provided. The expression pattern of pC1SS2 neurons in males has been only briefly described in reference 46. It is possible that these neurons overlap with previously characterized dsx+ and/or fru+ neurons that are important for male aggressions (measured by lunges), such as in Koganezawa et al., Curr. Biol. 2016 and Chiu et al., Cell 2020. This adds to the concern that lunge and tussling are not as clearly separated as the authors claim.

      Thank you very much for this important question. Indeed, there are many experiments that could do to better understand the function of pC1SS2 neurons, and we only provide the initial characterization of them due to the limited scope of this study. My lab has been focused on studying P1/pC1 function in both male and female flies and will continue to do so.

      To partially address your concern, we made the following revisions

      (1) We provided higher-resolution images of P1a and pC1SS2 (Figure 4C-4E). While their cell bodies are very close, they project to distinct brain regions, in addition to some shared ones.

      (2) By staining these neurons with GFP and co-staining with anti-FruM or anti-DsxM antibodies, we showed that P1a neurons are partially FruM-positive and partially DsxM-positive, while pC1SS2 neurons are DsxM-positive and FruM-negative (Figure 5A-5D).

      (3) As pC1SS2 neurons are DsxM-positive and FruM-negative, we also examined how DsxM regulates the development of these neurons. We found that knocking down DsxM expression in pC1SS2 neurons using RNAi significantly affected pC1 development regarding to both cell numbers (Figure 5G) and their projections (Figure 5H).

      (4) We further found that DsxM in pC1SS2 neurons is crucial for executing their tussling-promoting function, as optogenetic activation of these neurons with DsxM knockdown failed to induce tussling behavior in the initial activation period, and a much lower level of tussling in the second activation period compared to control males (Figure 5I-5K).

      (5) While it is very difficult to identify the upstream and downstream neurons of P1a and pC1SS2 neurons, we made an initial step by utilizing trans-tango and retro-Tango to visualize potential downstream and upstream neurons of P1a and pC1SS2 (Figure 4-figure supplement 2), which certainly needs future investigation.  

      While their characterizations of tussling behaviors in wild-type males (Figures 1 and 2) are intriguing, the remaining data have little link with each other, making it difficult to understand what their main conclusion is. Figure 3 suggests that one class of olfactory sensory neurons (OSN) that express Or47b is necessary for tussling behavior. While the authors acknowledged that Or47b-expressing OSNs promote male courtship toward females presumably by detecting cuticular compounds, they provided little discussion on how a class of OSN can promote two different types of innate behavior. No evidence of a functional or circuitry relationship between the Or47b pathway and the pC1SS2 neurons was provided. It is unclear how these two components are relevant to each other.

      It has been previously found that Or47b-expressing ORNs respond to fly pheromones common to both sexes, and group-housing enhances their sensitivity. Regarding to how Or47b ORNs promotes two different types of innate behaviors, a simple explanation is that they act on multiple second-order and further downstream neurons to regulate both courtship and aggression, not mentioning that neural circuitries for courtship and aggression are partially shared. We did not include this in the discussion as we would like to focus on aggression modes, and how different ORNs (Or47b and Or67d) mediate distinct aggression modes.

      Regarding to the relationship between Or47b ORNs and pC1<sub>SS2</sub> neurons, or in general ORNs to P1/pC1, it is interesting and important to explore, but probably in a separate study. We tried to conduct pathway connection analyses from Or47b to pC1 using the FlyWire database, and found that Or47b neurons can act on pC1 neurons via three layers of interneurons. Although the FlyWire database currently only contains neuronal data from female brains, they can provide a certain degree of reference. We hope the editor and reviewers would agree with us that identifying these intermediate neurons involved in their connection is beyond this study.

      Lastly, the rationale of the experiment in Figure 5 and the interpretation of the results is confusing. The authors attributed a higher mating success rate of older, socially experienced males over younger, socially isolated males to their tendency to tussle, but tussling cannot happen when one of the two flies is not engaged. If, for instance, a socially isolated 14-day-old male does not engage in tussling as indicated in Figure 2, how can they tussle with a group-housed 14-day-old male? Because aggressive interactions in Figure 5 were not quantified, it is impossible to conclude that tussling plays a role in copulation advantage among pairs as authors argue (lines 282-288).

      Indeed, we do not have direct evidence to show it is tussling that makes socially experienced males to dominate over socially isolated males. To address your concern, we have made following revisions

      (1) We toned down the statements about the relationship between fighting strategies and reproductive success throughout the manuscript. For example, in the abstract Moreover, shifting from lunging to tussling in socially enriched males is accompanied with better territory control and mating success.

      (2)  Regarding to whether a SH male can engage in tussling with a GH male, we found that while two SH males rarely perform tussling, paired SH and GH males displayed similar levels of tussling like two GH males, although tussling duration from paired SH and GH males is significantly lower compared to that in two GH males (Figure 6-figure supplement 2).

      (3) To support the potential role of tussling in territory control and mating competition, we performed additional experiments to silence Or47b or pC1SS2 neurons that almost abolished tussling, and paired these males with control males. We found that males with Or47b or pC1SS2 neurons silenced cannot compete over control males, further suggesting the involvement of tussling in territory control and mating competition.  

      Despite these weaknesses, it is important to acknowledge the authors' courage to initiate an investigation into a less characterized, high-intensity fighting behavior. Tussling requires the simultaneous engagement of two flies. Even if there is confusion over the distinction between lunges and tussling, the authors' conclusion that socially experienced flies and socially isolated flies employ distinct fighting strategies is convincing. Questions that require more rigorous studies are 1) whether such differences are encoded by separate circuits, and 2) whether the different fighting strategies are causally responsible for gaining ethologically relevant resources among socially experienced flies. Enhanced transparency of behavioral data will help readers understand the impact of this study. Lastly, the manuscript often mentions previous works and results without citing relevant references. For readers to grasp the context of this work, it is important to provide information about methods, reagents, and other key resources.

      Thank you very much for this comment and we almost totally agree.

      (1) Our results suggest the involvement of distinct sensory neurons and central neurons for lunging and tussling, but do not exclude the possibility that they may also utilize shared neurons. For example, activation of P1a neurons promotes both lunging and tussling in the presence of light.

      (2) We have now toned down the statements about the relationship between fighting strategies and reproductive success throughout the manuscript.

      (3) We provided more detailed methods, genotypes of flies to improve transparency of the manuscript.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Figure 1 Supplement 1 shows that increased aging has a linear and inverse relationship with the number of lunges, this is in contrast to a previous study from Dierick lab (Chowdhury, 2021), where using Divider assays they showed that aggressive lunges increased up to day 10 and subsequently decreased in 30-day old flies. Given that this study did not use 14-day-old flies, it might be useful to comment on this.

      Thank you for this comment. Indeed, Chowdhury et al., suggested a decline of lunging after 10 days, which is not contradictory to our findings that lunging in 14d-old males is lower than that in 7d-old males. It is ideally to perform a time-series experiments to reveal the detailed relationship between ages and aggression (lunging or tussling) levels, but given our initial findings that 14d-old males showed stable tussling behavior, we prefer to use this time point for the rest of this study.

      (2) For Figure 3, do various manipulations also affect the duration of tussling and boxing besides frequency and latency?

      Thank you for this comment. We only analyzed latency and frequency, but not duration, as data analysis was performed manually rather than automatically on every fly pair for about 2 hours, which is very labor-consuming. We hope you could agree with us that the two parameters (frequency and latency) for tussling are representative for assaying this behavior.

      (3) For Figure 3 A-F, the housing status of the males is not clearly mentioned either in the main text or the figure. What is the status of the tussling and lunging status when this housing condition is reversed when Or47b neurons are silenced, or the gene is knocked down? Do these manipulations overcome the effect of housing conditions similar to what is seen in NaChBac-mediated activation experiments?

      Figure 3A-F used group-housed males and we have now added such information in the figure legends as well as Table S1.

      We appreciate your suggestion on using different housing conditions. As silencing Or47b neurons or knocking down Or47b reduced tussling, it is reasonable to use GH males (as we did in Figure 3A-F) that performed stable tussling behavior, but not SH males that rarely tussle.

      (4) The connections between Or47b neurons and pC1SS2 or P1a neurons can be addressed by available connectomic datasets or TransTango/GRASP approaches.

      Thank you for this important suggestion. We used the FlyWire electron microscope database to analyze the pathway connections between these two types of neurons. The results indicated that there are at least three levels of interneurons for connecting Or47b and pC1 neurons. Although the FlyWire database currently only contains neuronal data from female brains, they can provide a certain degree of reference for males.

      The lack of direct synaptic connection also suggests that it is challenging to resolve the connection between these two neuronal types using methods like trans-Tango/GRASP. To partially address this question, we utilized trans-Tango and retro-Tango techniques to visualize potential downstream and upstream neurons of P1a and pC1SS2 (Figure 4-figure supplement 2). Future investigations are certainly needed for clarifying functional connections between Or47b/Or67d and P1a/pC1SS2 neurons.

      (5) Figure 5, 'Winning index' and 'Copulation advance index' while described in Material and Methods, should be referred to in the main text.

      We now described these two indices briefly in the main manuscript, and in the Discussion section with more details.

      (6) Figure 6 shows comparisons for territorial control and mating outcomes where four different housing and aging conditions are organized in a hierarchical sequence. It is not clear from the data in Figure 5, how this conclusion was arrived at. A supplementary table with various outcomes with statistical analysis would help with this.

      We now added a supplementary table (Table S2) with various outcomes with statistical analysis.

      Minor Comments

      (1) Line 26 says that the courtship levels in SH and GH males are not different, however, unilateral wing extension is higher in SH males as compared to GH males (Pan & Baker, 2014; Inagaki et al., 2014), also it was shown that courtship attempts are higher in D. paulsitorium (Kim & Ehrman, 1998). It would be better to clarify this statement.

      Indeed, it is found in some cases that SH males court more vigorously than GH males. We have added more references on this matter in the introduction.

      (2) Figure 4, correct 'Tussing' to 'Tussling' or 'Box, Tussling' as appropriate.

      Corrected.

      (3) Duistermars, 2018 should be cited while discussing the role of vision in aggression (Figure 4). [A Brain Module for Scalable Control of Complex, Multi-motor Threat Displays]

      We now cited this reference and added more discussion in the revised manuscript.

      (4) Reviews on Drosophila aggression and social isolation can be cited in the introduction/discussion to incorporate recent literature e.g., Palavicino-Maggio, 2022 [The Neuromodulatory Basis of Aggression Lessons From the Humble Fruit Fly]; Yadav et al., 2024[Lessons from lonely flies Molecular and neuronal mechanisms underlying social isolation], etc.

      We now cited these references in both the introduction and discussion sections.

      (5) The concentration of apple juice agar should be mentioned in the methods.

      We added this and other necessary information for materials in the Materials and Methods section of the study.

      (6) Source of the LifeSongX software and, if available, a Github link would be helpful to include in the materials and methods section.

      We now provided the source of the LifesongY software (website https//sourceforge.net/projects/lifesongy/), which is a Windows version of LifesongX (Bernstein, Adam S.et al., 1992).

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Major comment 1

      As pointed out in the public review, the weakness of this study is that the relationship between the aggression strategy and reproductive success is an inference that is not based on experimental facts; I understand that the frequency of tussling is not so high, but at least tussling-like behavior can be observed in the territory control experiment shown in Video 3. Wouldn't it be possible to re-analyse data and examine the correlation between aggressive behavior and territory control? Even if the analysis of tussling itself in this setup is difficult, for example, additional experiments using Or47b knock-out fly or pC1[SS2]-inactivated fly could provide stronger support.

      Indeed, we can only make a correlation between the type of aggressive behavior and territory control. We now toned down this statement throughout the manuscript. For example, in the abstract, we changed our conclusions as following

      Moreover, shifting from lunging to tussling in socially enriched males is accompanied with better territory control and mating success. Our findings identify distinct sensory and central neurons for two fighting forms and suggest how social experience shapes fighting strategies to optimize reproductive success.

      To further address the concern, we now performed additional experiments to silence Or47b or pC1SS2 neurons that almost abolished tussling, and paired these males with control males. We found that males with Or47b or pC1SS2 neurons silenced cannot compete over control males (Figure 6-figure supplement 3), further suggesting the involvement of tussling in territory control and mating competition.

      In relation to the above, some of the text in the Abstract should be changed.Line 28 These findings "reveal" how social experience shapes fighting strategies to optimise reproductive success.

      "suggest" is more accurate at this stage.

      Changed as suggested.

      (2) Major comment 2

      The tussling is the central subject of this paper. However, neither the main text nor Materials and Methods section provides a clear explanation of how this aggression mode was detected. Did the authors determine this behavior manually? Or was it automatically detected by some kind of image analysis? In either case, the criteria and method for detecting the tussling should be clearly described.

      The behavioral data analysis in this study was performed manually. We now provided more detailed description of the two fighting forms in the Materials and Methods section. See below

      Lunging is characterized by a male raising its forelegs and quickly striking the opponent, and each lunge typically lasts less than 0.2 seconds through detailed analysis. Tussling is characterized by both males using their forelegs and bodies to tumble over each other, and this behavior may last from seconds to minutes. Tussling is often mixed with boxing, in which both flies rear up and strike the opponent with forelegs. Since boxing is often transient and difficult to distinguish from tussling, we referred to the mixed boxing and tussling behavior simply as tussling. As we manually analyze tussling for 2 hours for each pair of males, it is possible that we may miss some tussling events, especially those quick ones.

      For the experimental groups where tussling cannot be observed, the latency is regarded as 120 min, but this is a value depending on the observation time. While it is reasonable to use the latency to evaluate the behavior such as the lunging that is observed at relatively early times, care should be taken when using it to evaluate the tussling. Since similar trends to those obtained for the latency are observed for Number of tussles and % of males performing tussling, it may be better to focus on these two indices.

      We initially intended to provide all three statistical metrics. However, we found that using the "% of males performing tussling" would require a significantly larger sample size for subsequent statistical analysis (using chi-square tests), greatly increasing the workload. At the same time, we believe that the trend observed with "% of males performing tussling" is consistent with the other two indices, and the percentage information can also be derived from the individual sample scatter data of the other two metrics. Therefore, we opted to use "latency" and "numbers" as the statistical metrics, despite the caveat as you mentioned.

      The authors repeatedly mention that tussling is less frequent but more vigorous. The low frequency can be understood from the data in Fig. 1 and Fig. 2, but there are no measured data on the intensity. As the authors mention in line 125, each tussling event appears to be sustained for a relatively long period, as can be seen from the ethogram in Fig. 2. For example, it would be possible to evaluate the intensity by measuring the duration of the tussling event.

      Thank you for your valuable suggestion. We now analyzed duration of tussling and lunging, and found that a lunging event is often very short (less than 0.2s), while a tussling event may last from seconds to minutes, further supporting their relative intensities. This new data is added as Figure 2G.

      (3) Minor comments

      a) Line 117 How many flies were placed in one vial for group-rearing (GH)? Were males and females grouped together? Please specify in the Materials and Methods section.

      We have added this information in the Materials and Methods section. In brief, 30-40 virgin males were collected after eclosion and group-housed in each food vial.

      b) Line 174 The trans-Tango is basically a postsynaptic cell labeling technique. It is unlikely that the labeling intensity changes depending on neuronal activity. Do the authors want to say in this text the high activity of Or47b-expressing neurons under GH conditions? Or are they trying to show that the expression level of the Or47b gene, which is supposedly monitored by the expression of GAL4, is increased by GH conditions? The authors should clarify which is the case.

      Although the primary function of the trans-Tango technique is to label downstream neurons, the original literature indicates that the signal strength in downstream neurons depends on the use of upstream neurons evidenced by age-dependent trans-Tango signals. Therefore, the trans-Tango technique can indirectly reflect the usage of upstream neurons. Our findings that GH males showed broader Or47b trans-Tango signals than SH males can indirectly suggest that group-housing experience acts on Or47b neurons. We made textually changes to clarify this.

      c) Line 178 Which fly line labels the mushroom body; R19B03-GAL4?

      Yes, we now provided the detailed genotypes for all tested flies in the Table S1.

      d) Line 184 It was reported in Koganezawa et al., 2016 that some dsx-expressing pC1 neurons are involved in aggressive behavior. The authors should also refer to this paper as they include tussling in the observed aggressive behavior.

      Thank you for this comment, and we now cited this reference in the revised manuscript.

      e) Line 339 I think you misspelled fruM RNAi.

      Thank you for pointing this out. fruMi refers to microRNAi targeting fruM, and we have now clearly stated this information in the main text.

      f) Line 681 Is tussling time (%) the total duration of tussling occurrences during the observation time? Or is it the percentage of individuals observed tussling during the observation time? This needs to be clarified.

      It is the former one. We now clearly stated this definition in the Materials and Methods section

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      For authors to support their conclusion that enhanced tussling among socially experienced flies allows them to better retain resources, it is necessary to quantify aggressive behaviors (mainly tussling and lunging) in Figure 5.

      We agree that we can only make a correlation between enhanced tussling behavior and mating competition. We now toned down this statement throughout the manuscript. For example, in the abstract, we changed our conclusions as following Moreover, shifting from lunging to tussling in socially enriched males is accompanied with better territory control and mating success. Our findings identify distinct sensory and central neurons for two fighting forms and suggest how social experience shapes fighting strategies to optimize reproductive success.

      To further address the concern, we now performed additional experiments to silence Or47b or pC1SS2 neurons that almost abolished tussling, and paired these males with control males. We found that males with Or47b or pC1SS2 neurons silenced cannot compete over control males (Figure 6-figure supplement 3), further suggesting the involvement of tussling in territory control and mating competition.

      In contrast to the authors' data in Figure 4, movies in ref 36 clearly show instances of 2 flies exchanging lunges after the optogenetic activation of P1a neurons, like the examples shown in supplementary movies S1-S3. It is a clear discrepancy that requires discussion (and raises a concern about the lack of transparency about behavioral quantification).

      In our study, optogenetic activation of P1<sup>a</sup> neurons failed to induce obvious tussling behavior, and temperature-dependent activation of P1<sup>a</sup> neurons can only induce tussling in the presence of light. These data are different from Hoopfer et al., (2015), but are generally consistent with a new study (Sten et al., Cell, 2025), in which pC1SS2 neurons but not P1a neurons promote aggression. Such discrepancy has now been discussed in the revised manuscript.

      The authors often fail to cite relevant references while discussing previous results, which compromises the scholarship of the manuscript. Examples include (but are not limited to)

      (1) Line 85-86 Simon and Heberlein, J. Exp. Biol. 223 jeb232439 (2020) suggested that tussling is an important factor for flies to establish a dominance hierarchy.

      Reference added.

      (2) Line 142-143 Cuticular compounds such as palmitoleic acid are characterized to be the ligands of Or47b by ref #18.

      Reference added.

      (3) Line 185-187 pC1SS1 and pC1SS2 are first characterized by ref #46. Expression data of this paper also implies that pC1SS1 and pC1SS2 label different neurons in the male brain.

      We have now added this reference at the appropriate place in the revised manuscript. In addition, we have clarified that these two drivers exhibit sexually dimorphic expression patterns in the brain.

      (4) Line 196-199 Cite ref #36, which describes the behavior induced by the optogenetic activation of P1a neurons.

      Reference added.

      (5) Line 233-235 The authors' observation that control males do not form a clear dominance directly contradicts previous observations by others (Nilsen et al., PNAS 10112342 (2002); Yurkovic et al., PNAS 10317519 (2006); also see Trannoy et al., PNAS 1134818 (2016) and Simon and Heberlein above). The authors must at least discuss why their results are different.

      There is a misunderstanding here. We clearly state that there is a ‘winner takes all’ phenomenon. However, for wild-type males of the same age and housing condition, we calculated the winning index as (num. of wins by unmarked males – num. of wins by marked males)/10 encounters * 100%, which is roughly zero due to the randomness of marking.

      (6) Line 251-254 The authors' observation that aged males are less competitive than younger males contradicts the conclusion in ref #18. Discussion is required.

      We have now added a discussion on this matter. In brief, Lin et al., showed that 7d-old males are more competitive than 2d-old males, which is probably due to different levels of sexual maturity of males, but not a matter of age like our study that used up to 21d-old males.

      (7) Line 274-275 It is unclear which "previous studies" "have found that social isolation generally enhances aggression but decreases mating competition in animal models". Cite relevant references.

      Reference added.

      (8) Line 309-310 The evidence supporting the statement that "there are only three pairs of pC1SS2 neurons". If there is a reference, cite it. If it is based on the authors' observation, data is required.

      We have now provided additional data on the number of pC1SS2 neurons in Figure 5G of the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review): 

      The manuscript by Feng et al. reported that the Endothelin B receptor (ETBR) expressed by the satellite glial cells (SGCs) in the dorsal root ganglions (DRG) acted to inhibit sensory axon regeneration in both adult and aged mice. Thus, pharmacological inhibition of ETBR with specific inhibitors resulted in enhanced sensory axon regeneration in vitro and in vivo. In addition, sensory axon regeneration significantly reduces in aged mice and inhibition of ETBR could restore such defect in aged mice. Moreover, the study provided some evidence that the reduced level of gap junction protein connexin 43 might act downstream of ETBR to suppress axon regeneration in aged mice. Overall, the study revealed an interesting SGC-derived signal in the DRG microenvironment to regulate sensory axon regeneration. It provided additional evidence that non-neuronal cell types in the microenvironment function to regulate axon regeneration via cell-cell interaction. 

      However, the molecular mechanisms by which ETBR regulates axon regeneration are unclear, and the manuscript's structure is not well organized, especially in the last section. Some discussion and explanation about the data interpretation are needed to improve the manuscript. 

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments. We agree that the mechanisms by which ETBR signaling functions as a brake on axon growth and regeneration remain to be elucidated. We believe that unraveling the detailed molecular pathways downstream of ETBR signaling in SGCs that promote axon regeneration is beyond the scope of this manuscript. Answering these questions would first require cell specific KO of ETBR and Cx43 to confirm that this pathway is operating in SGCs to control axon regeneration. We would also need to identify how SGCs communicate with neurons to regulate axon regeneration, which is a large area of ongoing research that remains poorly understood. Our data showing that pharmacological inhibition of ETBR with specific FDA-approved inhibitors enhances sensory axon regeneration provide not only new evidence for non-neuronal mechanisms in nerve repair, but also a new potential clinical avenue for therapeutic intervention.

      As suggested by the reviewer, we have extensively revised the organization of the manuscript, especially the last section of results. We have performed additional snRNAseq experiments to establish the impact of aging in DRG. We have also performed additional experiments to determine if blocking ETBR improves target tissue reinnervation. Following the reviewer’s suggestion, we have also expanded the Discussion section to discuss alternative mechanisms and o]er additional interpretation of our data. Below we describe how we address each point in detail.

      (1) The result showed that the level of ETBR did not change after the peripheral nerve injury. Does this mean that its endogenous function is to limit spontaneous sensory axon regeneration? In other words, the results suggest that SGCs expressing ETBR or vascular endothelial cells expressing its ligand ET-1 act to suppress sensory axon regeneration. Some explanation or discussion about this is necessary. Moreover, does the protein level of ETBR or its ligand change during aging?  

      We thank the reviewer for this point. Our results indeed indicate that one endogenous function of ETBR is to limit the extent of sensory axon regeneration. This may be a part of a mechanism to limit spontaneous sensory axon growth or plasticity and maladaptive neural rewiring after nerve injury. While the increased growth capacity of damaged peripheral axons can lead to reconnection with their targets and functional recovery, the increased growth capacity can also lead to axonal sprouting of the central axon terminals of injured neurons in the spinal cord, and to pain (see for example Costigan et al 2010, PMID: 19400724).  In the context of aging that we describe here, this protective mechanism may hinder beneficial recovery. Other mechanisms that slow axon regeneration have been reported, and include, for example, axonally synthesized proteins, which typically support nerve regeneration through retrograde signaling and local growth mechanisms. RNA binding proteins (RBP) are needed for this process. One such RBP, the RNA binding protein KHSRP is locally translated following nerve injury. Rather than promoting axon regeneration, KHSRP promotes decay of other axonal mRNAs and slows axon regeneration.  Another example includes the Rho signaling pathway, which was shown to function as an inhibitory mechanism that slows the growth of spiral ganglion neurites in culture. We have now included these examples in the Discussion section.

      To address the reviewer’s second question, we have checked protein levels of ETBR and ET-1 in adult and aged DRG tissue. We observed a robust increase in ET-1 in aged DRG, while the levels of ETBR did not appear to change significantly. These results are now presented in Figure 4- Figure Supplement 1, and further support the notion that in aging, activation of the ETBR signaling hinders axon regeneration.

      (2) In ex vivo experiments, NGF was added to the culture medium. Previous studies have shown that adult sensory neurons could initiate fast axon growth in response to NGF within 24 hours. In addition, dissociated sensory neurons could also initiate spontaneous regenerative axon growth without NGF after 48 hours. Some discussion or rationale is needed to explain the di]erence between NGF-induced or spontaneous axon growth of culture adult sensory neurons and the roles of ETBR and SGCs. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion. In adult DRG explant or dissociated cultures, NGF is not typically required for survival or axon outgrowth. However, in dissociated culture, the addition of NGF to the medium stimulates growth from more neurons compared to controls (Smith and Skene 1997). In the DRG explant, NGF does not promote significant e]ects on axon growth, but stimulates glial cell migration (Klimovich et al 2020). We opted to included NGF in our explant assay to increase the potential of stimulating axon regeneration with pharmacological manipulations of ETBR. We have now clarified these considerations in the Method section.

      (3) In cultured dissociated sensory neurons, inhibiting ETBR also enhanced axon growth, which meant the presence of SGCs surrounding the sensory neurons. Some direct evidence is needed to show the cellular relationship between them in culture.  

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point and have added new data, now presented in Figure 2B, to show that in mixed DRG cultures, SGCs labeled with Fabp7 are present in the culture in proximity to neurons labeled with TUJ1, but they do not fully wrap the neuronal soma. These results are consistent with prior findings reporting that as time in culture progresses, SGCs lose their adhesive contacts with neuronal soma and adhere to the coverslip (PMID: 22032231, PMID: 27606776).  While in some cases SGCs can maintain their association with neuronal soma in the first day in culture after plating, in our hands, most SGCs have left the soma at the 24h time point we examined. 

      (4) In Figure 3, the in vivo regeneration experiments first showed enhanced axon regeneration either 1 day or 3 days after the nerve injury. The study then showed that inhibiting ETBR could enhance sensory axon growth in vitro from uninjured naïve neurons or conditioning lesioned neurons. To my knowledge, in vivo sensory axon regeneration is relatively slow during the first 2 days after the nerve injury and then enters the fast regeneration mode on the 3rd day, representing the conditioning lesion e]ect in vivo. Some discussion is needed to compare the in vitro and the in vivo model of axon regeneration. 

      We agree that axon growth is relatively slow the first 2 days and enters a fast growth mode on day 3. This has been elegantly demonstrated in Shin et al Neuron 2012 (PMID: 22726832), where an in vivo conditioning injury 3 days prior increases axon growth one day after injury. In vitro, similar e]ects have been described: a prior in vivo injury accelerates growth capacity within the first day in culture, but a similar growth mode occurs in naive adult neurons after 2-3 days in vitro (Smith and Skene 1996). We also know that the neurite growth in culture is stimulated by higher cell density, likely because non-neuronal cells can secrete trophic factors (Smith and Skene 1996). Our in vitro results thus suggest that blocking ETBR in SGCs in these mixed cultures may alter the media towards a more growth promoting state. In vivo, our data show that Bosentan treatment for 3 days partially mimics the conditioning injury and potentiate the e]ect of the conditioning injury. One possible interpretation is that inhibition of ETBR alters the release of trophic factors from SGCs. Future studies will be required to unravel how ETBR signaling influence the SGCs secretome and its influence on axon growth. We have now included these discussions points in the Results and Discussion Section.

      (5) In Figure 5, the study showed that the level of connexin 43 increased after ETBR inhibition in either adult or aged mice, proposing an important role of connexin 43 in mediating the enhancing e]ect of ETBR inhibition on axon regeneration. However, in the study, there was no direct evidence supporting that ETBR directly regulates connexin 43 expression in SGCs. Moreover, there was no functional evidence that connexin 43 acted downstream of ETBR to regulate axon regeneration.  

      We thank the reviewer for this point and agree that we do not provide direct evidence that connexin 43 acts downstream of ETBR to regulate axon regeneration. To obtain such functional evidence would require selective KO of ETBR and Cx43 in SGCs, which we believe is beyond the scope of the current study. We have revised the Results and Discussion sections to emphasize that while we observe that ETBR inhibition increases Cx43 levels and Cx43 levels correlates with axon regeneration, whether Cx43 directly mediates the e]ect on axon regeneration remains to be established.  We also discuss potential alternative mechanisms downstream of ETBR in SGCs that could contribute to the observed e]ects on axon regeneration. Specifically, we discuss the possibility that  ETBR signaling may limit axon regeneration via regulating SGCs glutamate reuptake functions, because of the following reasons: 1) Similarly to astrocytes, glutamate uptake by SGCs is important to regulate neuronal function, 2) exposure of cultured cortical astrocytes to endothelin results in a decrease in glutamate uptake that correlates with a major loss of basal glutamate transporter expression (GLT-1 and1), 3) Both glutamate transporters are expressed in SGCs in sensory ganglia 4) GLAST and glutamate reuptake function is important for lesion-induced plasticity in the developing somatosensory cortex. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      In this interesting and original study, Feng and colleagues set out to address the e]ect of manipulating endothelin signaling on nerve regeneration, focusing on the crosstalk between endothelial cells (ECs) in dorsal root ganglia (DRG), which secrete ET-1 and satellite glial cells (SGCs) expressing ETBR receptor. The main finding is that ETBR signaling is a default brake on axon growth, and inhibiting this pathway promotes axon regeneration after nerve injury and counters the decline in regenerative capacity that occurs during aging. ET-1 and ETBR are mapped in ECs and SGCs, respectively, using scRNA-seq of DRGs from adult or aged mice. Although their expression does not change upon injury, it is modulated during aging, with a reported increase in plasma levels of ET-1 (a potent vasoconstrictive signal). Using in vitro explant assays coupled with pharmacological inhibition in mouse models of nerve injury, the authors demonstrate that ET-1/ETBR curbs axonal growth, and the ETAR/ETBR antagonist Bosentan boosts regrowth during the early phase of repair. In addition, Bosentan restores the ability of aged DRG neurons to regrow after nerve lesions. Despite Bosentan inhibiting both endothelin receptors A and B, comparison with an ETAR-specific antagonist indicates that the e]ects can be attributed to the ET-1/ETBR pathway. In the DRGs, ETBR is mostly expressed by SGCs (and a subset of Schwann cells) a cell type that previous studies, including work from this group, have implicated in nerve regeneration. SGCs ensheath and couple with DRG neurons through gap junctions formed by Cx43. Based on their own findings and evidence from the literature, the pro-regenerative e]ects of ETBR inhibition are in part attributed to an increase in Cx43 levels, which are expected to enhance neuron-SGC coupling. Finally, gene expression analysis in adult vs aged DRGs predicts a decrease in fatty acid and cholesterol metabolism, for which previous work by the authors has shown a requirement in SGCs to promote axon regeneration. 

      Strengths: 

      The study is well-executed and the main conclusion that "ETBR signaling inhibits axon regeneration after nerve injury and plays a role in age-related decline in regenerative capacity" (line 77) is supported by the data. Given that Bosentan is an FDA-approved drug, the findings may have therapeutic value in clinical settings where peripheral nerve regeneration is suboptimal or largely impaired, as it often happens in aged individuals. In addition, the study highlights the importance of vascular signals in nerve regeneration, a topic that has gained traction in recent years. Importantly, these results further emphasize the contribution of longneglected SGCs to nerve tissue homeostasis and repair. Although the study does not reach a complete mechanistic understanding, the results are robust and are expected to attract the interest of a broader readership. 

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments, especially in regard to the rigor and originality of our study.

      Weaknesses: 

      Despite these positive comments provided above, the following points should be considered: 

      (1) This study examines the contribution of the ET-1 pathway in the ganglia, and in vitro assays are consistent with the idea that important signaling events take place there. Nevertheless, it remains to be determined whether the accelerated axon regrowth observed in vivo depends also on cellular crosstalk mediated by ET-1 at the lesion site. Are ECs along the nerve secreting ET-1? What cells are present in the nerve stroma that could respond and participate in the repair process? Would these interactions be sensitive to Bosentan? It may be di]icult to dissect this contribution, but it should at least be discussed.  

      We thank the reviewer for this important point and agree that the in vivo e]ects observed cannot rule out the contribution of ECs or SCs at the lesion site in the nerve. Dissecting the contribution of ETBR expressing cells in the nerve would require cell-specific manipulations that go beyond the scope of this manuscript. We have revised the Discussion section to highlight the potential contribution of ECs, fibroblast and SCs in the nerve.  

      (2) It is suggested that the permeability of DRG vessels may facilitate the release of "vascularderived signals" (lines 82-84). Is it possible that the ET-1/ETBR pathway modulates vascular permeability, and that this, in turn, contributes to the observed e]ects on regeneration?  

      We thank the reviewer for raising this interesting point. ET-1 can have an impact on vascular permeability. It was indeed shown that in high glucose conditions, increased trans-endothelial permeability is associated with increased Edn1, Ednra and Ednrb expression and augmented ET1 immunoreactivity (PMID: 10950122). It is thus possible that part of the e]ects observed results from altered vascular permeability. We have included this point in the Discussion section. Future experiments will be required to test how injury and age a]ects vascular permeability in the DRG.

      (3) Is the a]inity of ET-3 for ETBR similar to that of ET-1? Can it be excluded that ET-3 expressed by fibroblasts is relevant for controlling SGC responses upon injury/aging?  

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point. ET-1 binds to ETAR and ETBR with the same a]inity, but ET3 shows a higher a]inity to ETBR than to ETAR (Davenport et al. Pharmacol. Rev 2016 PMID: 26956245). We attempted to examine ET-3 level in adult and aged DRG by western blot, but in our hands the antibody did not work well enough, and we could not obtain clear results. We thus cannot exclude the possibility that ET-3 released by fibroblasts contribute to the e]ects we observe on axon regeneration. Indeed, in cultured cortical astrocytes, application of either ET-1 or ET-3 leads to inhibition of Cx43 expression. We have revised the text in the Discussion section to highlight the possibility that both ET-1 and ET-3 could participate on the ETBRdependent e]ect on axon regeneration.

      (4) ETBR inhibition in dissociated (mixed) cultures uncovers the restraining activity of endothelin signaling on axon growth (Figure 2C). Since neurons do not express ET-1 receptors, based on scRNA-seq analysis, these results are interpreted as an indication that basal ETBR signaling in SGC curbs the axon growth potential of sensory neurons. For this to occur in dissociated cultures, however, one should assume that SGC-neuron association is present, similar to in vivo, or to whole DRG cultures (Figure 2C). Has this been tested?

      We thank the reviewer for this point. In dissociated DRG culture, neurons, SGCs and other nonneuronal cells are present, but SGCs do not retain the surrounding morphology as they do in vivo. Within 24 hours in culture, SGCs lose their adhesive contacts with neuronal soma and adhere to the coverslip (PMID: 22032231, PMID: 27606776).  We have included new data in Figure 2B to show that in our culture conditions, SGCs are present, but do not wrap neurons soma as they do in vivo. We also know from prior studies that the density of the culture a]ects axon growth, an e]ect that was attributed to trophic factors released from non-neuronal cells (Smith and Skene 1997). Therefore, although SGCs do not surround neurons, the signaling pathway downstream of ETBR may be present in culture and contribute to the release of trophic factors that influence axon growth. We have revised the Results section to better explain our in vitro results and their interpretation.

      In both in vitro experimental settings (dissociated and whole DRG cultures) how is ETBR stimulated over up to 7 days of culture? In other words, where does endothelin come from in these cultures (which are unlikely to support EC/blood vessel growth)? Is it possible that the relevant ligand here derives from fibroblasts (see point #6)? Or does it suggest that ETBR can be constitutively active (i.e., endothelin-independent signaling)? Is there any chance that endothelin is present in the culture media or Matrigel? 

      We thank the reviewer for raising this point.  Our single-cell data indicate that ET-1 is expressed by endothelial cells and ET-3 by fibroblasts. In dissociated DRG culture at 24h time point, all DRGs cells are present, including endothelial cells and fibroblasts, and could represent the source of ET-1 or ET-3. In the explant setting, it is also possible that both ET-1 and ET-3 are released by endothelial cells and fibroblasts during the 7 days in culture. According to information for the suppliers, endothelin is not present neither in the culture media nor in the Matrigel. While mutations can facilitate the constitutive activity of the ETBR receptor, we are not aware of data showing that endogenous ETBR can be constitutively active.  Because the molecular mechanisms governing ETBR -mediated signaling remain incompletely understood (see for example PMID: 39043181, PMID: 39414992) future studies will be required to elucidate the detailed mechanisms activating ETBR in SGCs and its downstream signaling mechanisms.  We have now expanded the Results and discussion sections to clarify these points. 

      (5) The discovery that ET-1/ETBR signaling in SGC curtails the growth capacity of axons at baseline raises questions about the physiological role of this pathway. What happens when ETBR signaling is prevented over a longer period of time? This could be addressed with pharmacological inhibitors, or better, with cell-specific knock-out mice. The experiments would certainly be of general interest, although not within the scope of this story. Nevertheless, it could be worth discussing the possibilities. 

      We agree that this is an interesting point. As mentioned above in response to point #1 of reviewer 1, the physiological role of this pathway could be to limit plasticity and prevent maladaptive neural rewiring that can happen after injury (Costigan et al 2009, PMID: 19400724), but can also hinder beneficial recovery after injury. Other mechanisms that limit axon regeneration capacity have been described and involve local mRNA translation and Rho signaling. We have revised the Discussion section to include these points. We agree that understanding the consequence of blocking ETBR over longer time periods is beyond the scope of the current study, but we now discuss the possibility that blocking ETBR with a cell specific KO approach could unravel its physiological function on target innervation and behavior. 

      (6) Assessing Cx43 levels by measuring the immunofluorescence signal (Figure 5E-F) is acceptable, particularly when the aim is to restrict the analysis to SGCs. The modulation of Cx43 expression by ET-1/ETBR plays an important part in the proposed model. Therefore, a complementary analysis of Cx43 expression by quantitative RT-PCR on sorted SGCs would be a valuable addition to the immunofluorescence data. Is this attainable? 

      We agree and have attempted to perform these types of experiments but encountered technical di]iculties. We attempted to sorting SGCs from transgenic mice in which SGCs are fluorescently labeled. However, the cells did not survive the sorting process and died in culture.  We think that increasing the viability of cells after sorting would require capillary- free fluorescent sorting approaches. However, we do not currently have access to such technology. We attempted this experiment with cultured SGCs, following a previously published protocol (Tonello et al. 2023 PMID: 38156033). In these experiments, SGCs are cultured for 8 days to obtain purity. We did not observe any di]erence in Cx43 protein or mRNA level upon treatment with ET-1 with or without BQ788. However, in these SGCs cultures, Cx43 displayed a di]use localization, rather than puncta as observed in vivo. Therefore, despite our multiple attempts, quantifying Cx43 on sorted or purified SGCs was not attainable.

      (7) The conclusions "We thus hypothesize that ETBR inhibition in SGCs contributes to axonal regeneration by increasing Cx43 levels, gap junction coupling or hemichannels and facilitating SGC-neuron communication" (lines 303-305) are consistent with the findings but seem in contrast with the e]ect of aging on gap junction coupling reported by others and cited in line 210: "the number of gap junctions and the dye coupling between these cells increases (Huang et al., 2006)". I am confused by what distinguishes a potential, and supposedly beneficial, increase in coupling after ETBR inhibition, from what is observed in aging. 

      We agree that the aging impact of Cx43 level and gap junction number appears contradictory. Procacci et al 2008 reported that Cx43 expression in SGCs decreases in the aged mice. Huang et al 2006 report that both the number of gap junctions and the dye coupling between these cells were found to increase with aging. Procacci et al suggested as a possible explanation for this apparent discrepancy that additional connexin types other than Cx43 may contribute to the gap junctions between SGCs in aged mice. Our snRNAseq data did not allow us to verify this hypothesis, because there were less SGCs in aged mice compared to adult, and connexin genes were detected in only 20% or less of SGCs.  Furthermore, our quantification did not look specifically at gap junctions, but just at Cx43 puncta. Cx43 can also form hemichannels in addition to gap junctions, and can also perform non-channel functions, such as protein interaction, cell adhesion, and intracellular signaling. Thus, more research examining the role of Cx43 in SGCs is necessary to address this discrepancy in the literature. We have expanded the Discussion section to include these points. 

      (8) I find it di]icult to reconcile the results in Figure 5F with the proposed model since (1) injury increases Cx43 levels in both adult and aged mice, (2) the injured aged/vehicle group has a similar level to the uninjured adult group, (3) upon injury, aged+Bosentan is much lower than adult+Bosentan (significance not tested). It seems hard to explain the e]ect of Bosentan only through the modulation of Cx43 levels. Whether the increase in Cx43 levels following ETBR inhibition actually results in higher SGC-neuron coupling has not been assessed experimentally. 

      We thank the reviewer for this point and agree that the e]ect of Bosentan is likely not exclusively through the modulation of Cx43 levels in SGCs, and that Cx43 levels may simply correlate with axon regenerative capacity. We have revised the manuscript to clarify this point.  We have also added the missing significance test in Figure 5F.

      Cell specific KO of Cx43 and ETBR would allow to test this hypothesis directly but is beyond the scope of the current study. We have not tested SGCs-neuron coupling, as these experiments are currently beyond our area of expertise. Cx43 has also other functions beyond gap junction coupling, such as protein interaction, cell adhesion, and intracellular signaling. Investigating the precise function of Cx43 would require in depth biochemical and cell specific experiments that are beyond the scope of this study. Furthermore, as we now mentioned in response to reviewer #2 point 5, ETBR signaling may also have other downstream e]ects in SGCs, such as glutamate transporters expression, or a]ect other cells in the nerve during the regeneration process. We have revised the Discussion section to include these alternative mechanisms.

      Reviewer #3(Public Review): 

      Summary: 

      This manuscript suggests that inhibiting ETBR via the FDA-approved compound Bosentan can disrupt ET-1-ETBR signalling that they found detrimental to nerve regeneration, thus promoting repair after nerve injury in adult and aged mice. 

      Strengths: 

      (1) The clinical need to identify molecular and cellular mechanisms that can be targeted to improve repair after nerve injury. 

      (2) The proposed mechanism is interesting. 

      (3) The methodology is sound. 

      We thank the reviewer for highlighting the strengths of our study

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) The data appear preliminary and the story appears incomplete. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s point. We would like to emphasize that our results provide compelling evidence that ETBR signaling is a default brake on axon growth, and inhibiting this pathway promotes axon regeneration after nerve injury and counters the decline in regenerative capacity that occurs during aging. We also provide evidence that ETBR signaling regulates the levels of Cx43 in SGCs. Furthermore, our results document the use of an FDA approved compound to increase axon regeneration may be of interest to the broader readership, as there is currently no therapies to improve or accelerate nerve repair after injury. We agree that the detailed mechanisms operating downstream of ETBR will need to be elucidated. Answering these questions would first require cell specific KO of ETBR and Cx43 to confirm that this pathway is operating in SGCs to control axon regeneration. We would also need to identify how SGCs communicate with neurons to regulate axon regeneration, which is a large area of ongoing research that remains poorly understood. This extensive and highly complex set of experiments is beyond the scope of the current study. As we discussed in our response to reviewer #1 and #2 we attempted to perform numerous additional experiments to better define the role of ETBR signaling in SGCs in aging and have included additional results in Fig. 2B, Fig 3G-H,  Fig 5A-E, and Figure 4- Figure Supplement 1and Figure 5- Figure Supplement 1. We have expanded the

      Discussion to acknowledge the limitation of our study and to discuss possible mechanisms.  

      (2) Lack of causality and clear cellular and molecular mechanism. There are also some loose ends such as the role of connexin 43 in SGCs: how is it related to ET-1- ETBR signalling?  

      We thank the reviewer for this point and agree that the molecular mechanisms downstream of ETBR remain to be elucidated. However, we believe that our manuscript reports an interesting potential of an FDA-approved compound in promoting nerve repair. We focused on Cx43 downstream of ETBR signaling because decreased Cx43 expression in SGCs in ageing was previously established, but the mechanisms were not elucidated. Furthermore, it was reported that ET1 signaling in cultured astrocytes, which share functional similarities with SGCs, leads to the closure of gap junctions and reduction in Cx43 expression. Our study thus provides a mechanism by which ETBR signaling in SGCs regulates Cx43 expression. Whether Cx43 directly impact axon regeneration remains to be tested. Cell specific KO of Cx43 and ETBR would be required to answer this question. We have revised the Introduction and Discussion section extensively to provide a link between ETBR and Cx43 and to acknowledge the lack of causality in Cx43 in SGCs, as well as to provide additional potential mechanisms by which ETBR inhibition may promote nerve repair.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      In addition to the points listed in the Public Review section, please consider the following comments: 

      (1) ETAR, which is high in mural cells, does not seem to be implicated in the reported proregenerative e]ects. Even so, can vasoconstriction be ruled out as an underlying cause of the age-dependent decline in axon regrowth potential and, more generally, in the e]ects of ET-1 inhibition on regeneration? This could be discussed. 

      We agree that we can’t exclude a role in vasoconstriction or e]ect on vascular permeability in the age-dependent decline in axon regrowth potential. However, our in vitro and ex vivo experiments, in which vascular related mechanisms are unlikely, suggest that vasoconstriction may not be a major contributor to the e]ects we observed.

      (2) The manuscript (e.g. line 287-288) would benefit from a discussion of the role that blood vessels play in the peripheral nervous system, and possibly CNS, repair. Vessels were shown to accompany regenerating fibers and instruct the reorganization of the nerve tissue to favor repair potentially through the release of pro-regenerative signals acting on stromal cells, glia, and other cellular components. Highlighting these processes will help put the current findings into perspective. 

      We agree and have revised the Discussion section to better explain the role of blood vessels in orientating Schwann cells migration and guiding axon regeneration.

      (3) The vast majority of the cells that are sequenced and shown in the UMAP in Figure 1C are from adult (3-month-old) mice [16,923 out of 18,098]. It would be useful to include the UMAP split (or color-coded) by timepoint to appreciate changes in cell clustering that may occur with aging.  

      We apologize for this misunderstanding, Figure 1C had all cells from all ages. However, the number of cells we obtained from the age group was insu]icient to perform in depth analysis of each cell type. We have thus revised this section and Figure 1, now only presenting the data from adult mice.  

      It is not discussed why fewer cells were sequenced at later stages. Additionally, I do not know how to interpret the double asterisks next to the labeling "18,098 samples" in Figure 1C. 

      Since our original sequencing of adult and aged mice using 10x yielded so few cells from the aged DRG, we tested and optimized a new technology for single cell preparation of DRG using Illumina Single Cell 3’ RNA Prep. This preparation creates templated emulsions using a vortex mixer to capture and barcode single-cell mRNA instead of a microfluidics system. This method yielded much better results for nuclei recovery from aged DRG, with more nuclei and better quality of nuclei. Thus, we now present in Figure 5 and Figure 5- Figure Supplement 1 the results from snRNA-sequencing of aged and adult DRG using the Illumina single cell kit. The results of the snRNA-sequencing show a decreased abundance of SGCs in aged mice, consistent with the results from our morphology analysis with EM. We were also able to perform SGCs-specific pathway analysis because of the increased number of nuclei captured in the aged SGCs, which we included in the manuscript.

      (4) The in vivo studies are designed to examine the e]ects of ETBR inhibition during the first phase of axon regrowth after nerve injury (1-3 days post-injury, dpi). Is there a reason why later stages have not been studied? It would be interesting to understand whether ETBR inhibition improves long-term recovery or is only e]ective at boosting the initial growth of axons through the lesion. It is possible that early inhibition will be enough for long-term recovery. If so, these experiments would define a sensitivity window with therapeutic value. 

      We agree that assessing functional recovery requires proper behavioral tests or morphological evaluations of reinnervation. To determine if Bosentan treatment has long-term e]ects on recovery, we administered Bosentan or vehicle for 3 weeks (daily for 1 week, and then once a week for the subsequent 2 weeks) after sciatic nerve crush. At 24 days after SNC, we assessed intraepidermal nerve fiber density (IENFD) in the injured paw and saw a trend towards increased fibers/mm in the treated animals (new Figure 3G,H). Future studies will examine how long-term Bosentan treatment a]ects functional recovery and innervation at later time points. Additionally, behavior assays will be needed to determine if these morphological changes relate to behavioral improvements using IENFD and behavior assays.

      (5) I am unsure if the gene expression analysis shown in Figure 6 fits well into this story. It is interesting per se and in line with previous work from this group showing the relevance of fatty acid metabolism in SGCs for axon regeneration. Nevertheless, without a mechanistic link to endothelin signaling and Cx43/gap junction modulation, the observations derived from DEG analysis are not well integrated with the rest and may be more distracting than helpful. One limitation is that there is no cell-type information for the DEGs due to the small number of cells recovered from aged mice. For instance, if ETBR inhibition rescued gene downregulation associated with fatty acid/cholesterol metabolism, then the DGE results would become more relevant for understanding the cellular basis of the pro-regenerative e]ect, which at this point remains quite speculative (lines 264-265; lines 318-319).  

      We agree and have added new snRNA sequencing data to replace these findings (see above response to point #4, new Figure 5 and Figure 5- Figure Supplement 1. The new data shows a decreased abundance of SGCs in aged mice, consistent with our TEM results. Pathway analysis revealed that aging triggers extensive transcriptional reprogramming in SGCs, reflecting heightened demands for structural integrity, cell junction remodeling, and glia–neuron interactions within the aged DRG microenvironment.  

      (6) It would be interesting to determine whether Bosentan increases SGC coverage of neuronal cell bodies in aged mice (Figures 6A-C). 

      We agree that this would be very interesting, but will require extensive EM analysis at di]erent time points and is beyond the scope of the current manuscript.

      (7) Finally, adding a summary model would help the readers. 

      We agree and have made a summary model, now presented in Figure 6F.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors): 

      Longer time points post-injury and assessment of functional recovery after Bosentan would be of great value here. 

      We agree that assessing functional recovery requires proper behavioral tests or morphological evaluations of reinnervation. To determine if Bosentan treatment has long-term e]ects on recovery, we administered Bosentan or vehicle for 3 weeks (daily for 1 week, and then once a week for the subsequent 2 weeks) after sciatic nerve crush. At 24 days after SNC, we assessed intraepidermal nerve fiber density in the injured paw and saw a trend towards increased fibers/mm in the treated animals (Fig 3). While the results do not reach significance, we decided to include this new data as it provides evidence that Bosentan treatment may also improves long term recovery. Future studies will be required examine how long-term Bosentan treatment a]ects functional recovery and innervation at later time points. Additionally, behavior assays will be needed to determine if these morphological changes relate to behavioral improvements.

      It would be important to know how ET-1- ETBR signalling axis promotes the regeneration of axons:this remains unaddressed. What are the cells that are specifically involved? Endothelial cellsSGC- neurons- SC? There are no experiments addressing the role of any of these? 

      We agree that the molecular and cellular mechanisms by which ETBR signaling in SGCs promote axon regeneration remains to be elucidated.  Answering these questions would first require cell specific KO of ETBR and Cx43 to confirm that this pathway is operating in SGCs to control axon regeneration. We would also need to identify how SGCs communicate with neurons to regulate axon regeneration, which is a large area of ongoing research that remains poorly understood. While these are important experiments, because of numerous technical and temporal constrains, we believe they are beyond the scope of the current manuscript. 

      How does connexin 43 in SGCs related to ET-1- ETBR signalling? 

      The relation between connexin 43 and ETBR signaling stems from observations made in astrocytes. ET1 signaling in cultured astrocytes, which share functional similarities with SGCs, was shown to lead to the closure of gap junctions and the reduction in Cx43 expression. Because Cx43 expression, a major connexin expressed in SGCs as in astrocytes, was previously shown to be reduced at the protein level in SGCs from aged mice, we decided to explore it this ETBR-Cx43 mechanism also operates in SGCs. We have revised the Introduction and Discussion section extensively to acknowledge the lack of causality in Cx43 expression SGCs and to provide additional potential mechanisms by which ETBR inhibition may promote nerve repair.

    1. Author response:

      We thanks the Reviewers for their thorough reviews and helpful suggestions. We will provide additional quantification as requested for several aspects of the study.

      The methods that we developed were meant to provide candidates for regulatory elements for a gene of interest. These candidates could be used to further understand the regulation of a gene, a complex and difficult task, especially for dynamically regulated genes in the context of development. These candidates could also, or instead, be used to drive gene expression specifically in a target cell of interest for applications such as gene therapy or perturbations that need this type of specificity. In the first case, to use the candidates to understand the regulation of a gene, one would need to validate the candidates using the types of methods typically employed for this purpose, most rigorously in the in vivo genomic context. We did not pursue this level of validation as it would encompass a great deal of work outside the scope of the current study. However, by initially testing loci and CRMs which have been studied by several groups (Rho, Grm6, Vsx2, and Cabp5), and at least in the cases of Rho and Vsx2, shown to be relevant in the genomic context in vivo, we provide evidence that the LS-MPRA can identify relevant CRMs. These data show that the method is worth using for loci of interest, particularly when only one or a few loci are of interest, i.e. one does not need to use genome-wide approaches. It is also apparent that our methods are not perfect and that the LS-MPRA does not pick up all CRMs. We do not know of a method that has been shown to do so.

      Some of the statistical and quantitative data asked for by the Reviewers will be provided. However, it is important to note that the types of statistics using peak callers asked for regarding candidate choice will be of limited value. If one is testing a library in a single cell type in vitro, and/or running genome-wide assays, these statistics could aid in the choice of candidates. However, here we are electroporating a complex and dynamic set of cells, present at very different frequencies. In addition, at least for Olig2 and Ngn2, their expression is very transient, and each is expressed in only a small subset of cells. An additional confound is that the level of expression of each gene that one might test is variable. All of these variables render a statistical prediction of strong candidates to be less valuable than one might hope, and might lead one to miss those CRMs of interest. Instead, we suggest that one use one’s own level of interest and knowledge in choosing CRM candidates. We provide several examples of experimental, rather than purely statistical, approaches that might help in one’s choice of candidates. We used a functional read-out of CRM activity (Notch perturbation), carried out in the context of the entire LS-MPRA library, as one method. Co-expression in single cells of candidate regulators identified by the d-MPRA is another. One can of course use chromatin structure and sequence conservation, as used in many studies of regulatory regions, as other ways to narrow down candidates. The d-MPRA predictions also can be viewed in light of previous genetic studies, i.e. mutations in TFs that effect the cell type of interest or the regulation of the gene of interest, as we were able to do here for CRMs predicted to be regulated by Otx2.

      If one wishes to use a candidate CRM to drive gene expression in a targeted cell type, one needs to establish specificity. In particular, specificity needs to be established in the context of the vector that is being used. Non-integrated vs integrated vectors, different types of viral vectors with their own confounding regulatory sequences, and copy number can all effect specificity. We provided a double in situ hybridization method for the examination of specificity for some of the novel candidate CRMs. It was quite difficult in the case of Olig2 and Ngn2 as their RNAs and proteins are unstable. We would need to provide further evidence should we wish to use these candidate CRMs for directing expression specifically in Olig2- or Ngn2-expressing cells. We suggest that an investigator can choose the vector and method for establishing specificity depending upon the goals of the application.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      EnvA-pseudotyped glycoprotein-deleted rabies virus has emerged as an essential tool for tracing monosynaptic inputs to genetically defined neuron populations in the mammalian brain. Recently, in addition to the SAD B19 rabies virus strain first described by Callaway and colleagues in 2007, the CVS N2c rabies virus strain has become popular due to its low toxicity and high trans-synaptic transfer efficiency. However, despite its widespread use in the mammalian brain, particularly in mice, the application of this cell-type-specific monosynaptic rabies tracing system in zebrafish has been limited by low labeling efficiency and high toxicity. In this manuscript, the authors aimed to develop an efficient retrograde monosynaptic rabies-mediated circuit mapping tool for larval zebrafish. Given the translucent nature of larval zebrafish, whole-brain neuronal activities can be monitored, perturbed, and recorded over time. Introducing a robust circuit mapping tool for larval zebrafish would enable researchers to simultaneously investigate the structure and function of neural circuits, which would be of significant interest to the neural circuit research community. Furthermore, the ability to track rabies-labeled cells over time in the transparent brain could enhance our understanding of the trans-synaptic retrograde tracing mechanism of the rabies virus. 

      To establish an efficient rabies virus tracing system in the larval zebrafish brain, the authors conducted meticulous side-by-side experiments to determine the optimal combination of trans-expressed rabies G proteins, TVA receptors, and recombinant rabies virus strains. Consistent with observations in the mouse brain, the CVS N2c strain trans-complemented with N2cG was found to be superior to the SAD B19 combination, offering lower toxicity and higher efficiency in labeling presynaptic neurons. Additionally, the authors tested various temperatures for the larvae post-virus injection and identified 36℃ as the optimal temperature for improved virus labeling. They then validated the system in the cerebellar circuits, noting evolutionary conservation in the cerebellar structure between zebrafish and mammals. The monosynaptic inputs to Purkinje cells from granule cells were neatly confirmed through ablation experiments.

      However, there are a couple of issues that this study should address. Additionally, conducting some extra experiments could provide valuable information to the broader research field utilizing recombinant rabies viruses as retrograde tracers.

      (1) It was observed that many radial glia were labeled, which casts doubt on the specificity of trans-synaptic spread between neurons. The issues of transneuronal labeling of glial cells should be addressed and discussed in more detail. In this manuscript, the authors used a transgenic zebrafish line carrying a neuron-specific Cre-dependent reporter and EnvA-CVS N2c(dG)-Cre virus to avoid the visualization of virally infected glial cells. However, this does not solve the real issue of glial cell labeling and the possibility of a nonsynaptic spread mechanism.

      In agreement with the reviewer’s suggestion, we have incorporated a standalone section in the revised Discussion (page 9) to address the issue of transneuronal glial labeling, including its spatial distribution, temporal dynamics, potential mechanisms, and possible strategies for real resolution.

      Regarding the specificity of trans-synaptic spread between neurons, we have demonstrated that our transsynaptic tracing system reliably and specifically labels input neurons. Structurally, we only observed labeling of inferior olivary cells (IOCs) outside the cerebellum, which are the only known extracerebellar inputs to Purkinje cells (PCs), while all other traced neurons remained confined within the cerebellum throughout the observation period (see Figure 2G–I). Functionally, we verified that the traced neurons formed synaptic connections with the starter PCs (see Figure 2J–M). Together, these findings support the conclusion that our system enables robust and specific retrograde monosynaptic tracing of neurons in larval zebrafish.

      Regarding the transneuronal labeling of radial glia cells, we observed that their distribution closely correlates with the location of neuronal somata and dendrites (see Author response image 2). In zebrafish, radial glial cells are considered functional analogs of astrocytes and are often referred to as radial astroglia. The adjacent labeled astroglia may participate in tripartite synapses with the starter neurons and express viral receptors that enable RV particle entry at postsynaptic sites. This suggests that rabies-based tracing in zebrafish may serve as a valuable tool for identifying synaptically associated and functionally connected glia. Leveraging this approach to investigate glia–neuron interactions represents a promising direction for future research.

      In our system, the glial labeling diminishes at later larval stages, likely due to abortive infection (see Author response image 3 and relevant response). However, the eventual clearance of infection does not preclude the initial infection of glial cells, which may compete with neuronal labeling and reduce overall tracing efficiency. Notably, transneuronal infection of glial cells by RV has also been observed in mammals (Marshel et al., 2010). To minimize such off-target labeling, future work should focus on elucidating the mechanisms underlying glial susceptibility—such as receptor-mediated viral entry— and developing strategies to suppress receptor expression specifically in glia, thereby improving the specificity and efficiency of neuronal circuit tracing.

      In addition, wrong citations in Line 307 were made when referring to previous studies discovering the same issue of RVdG-based transneuronal labeling radial glial cells. "The RVdG-based transneuronal labeling of radial glial cells was commonly observed in larval zebrafish29,30".

      The cited work was conducted using vesicular stomatitis virus (VSV). A more thorough analysis and/or discussion on this topic should be included.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the citation inaccuracy. The referenced study employed vesicular stomatitis virus (VSV), which, like RV, is a member of the Rhabdoviridae family. We have revised the text accordingly—from "RVdG-based transneuronal labeling of radial glial cells…" to " Transneuronal labeling of radial glial cells mediated by VSV, a member of the Rhabdoviridae family like RV, has been commonly observed in larval zebrafish" (page 9, line 347).

      Several key questions should be addressed:

      Does the number of labeled glial cells increase over time? 

      Yes, as shown in Figure 2—figure supplement 1C and G, the number of labeled radial glial cells significantly increased from 2 to 6 days post-injection (dpi). This phenomenon has been addressed in the revised Discussion section (page 9, line 357).

      Do they increase at the same rate over time as labeled neurons?

      Although glial cell labeling continued to increase over time, we observed a slowdown in labeling rate between 6 and 10 dpi, as shown in Figure 2—figure supplement 1C and G. Therefore, we divided the timeline into two intervals (2–6 and 6–10 dpi) to compare the rate of increase in labeling between neurons and glia. The rate (R) was defined as the daily change in convergence index. To quantify the difference between neuronal and glial labeling rates, we calculated a labeling rate index: R<sub>g</sub>−R<sub>n</sub>, where R<sub>g</sub> and R<sub>n</sub> denote the rates for glia and neurons, respectively) (Author response image1). Our analysis revealed that, between 2 and 6 dpi, glial cells exhibited a higher labeling rate than neurons. However, this trend reversed between 6 and 10 dpi, with neurons surpassing glial cells in labeling rate. These findings have been included in the revised Discussion section (page 9).

      Author response image 1.

      Labeling rate index of glia and neurons across two time intervals. Data points represent the mean labeling rate index for each tracing strategy within each time interval. *P < 0.05 (nonparametric two-tailed Mann-Whitney test).  

      Are the labeled glial cells only present around the injection site?

      We believe the reviewer is inquiring whether labeled glial cells are spatially restricted to the vicinity of starter neurons. The initial infection is determined by the expression of TVA rather than the injection site. For example, injecting a high volume of virus into the anterior hindbrain resulted in the infection of TVA-expressing cells in distant regions, including the 109 tectum and posterior hindbrain (Author response image 2). 

      Regarding glial labeling, PC starter experiments showed that labeled glial cells (i.e. Bergmann glia) were predominantly localized within the cerebellum, likely due to the confinement of PC dendrites to this region. When using vglut2a to define starter neurons, glial labeling was frequently observed near the soma and dendrites of starter cells (14 out 114 of 17 cases; Author response image 2). These observations suggest that transneuronal labeled glial cells may be synaptically associated with the starter neurons. We have included this point in the revised Discussion section (page 9).

      Author response image 2.

      Location of transneuronal labeled glial cells. (a and b) Confocal images showing the right tectum (a) and posterior hindbrain (b) of different WT larvae expressing EGFP and TVA using UGNT in randomly sparse neurons (vglut2a<sup>+</sup>) and infected with CVSdGtdTomato[EnvA] (magenta) injected into the anterior hindbrain. Dashed yellow circles, starter neurons (EGFP<sup>+</sup>/tdTomato<sup>+</sup>); gray arrows, transneuronally labeled radial glia (tdTomato<sup>+</sup>/EGFP<sup>−</sup>); dashed white lines, tectum or hindbrain boundaries. C, caudal; R, rostral. Scale bars, 20 μm.

      Can the phenomenon of transneuronal labeling of radial glial cells be mitigated if the tracing is done in slightly older larvae?

      Yes, we agree. As elaborated in the following response, we hypothesize that the loss of fluorescence in radial glial cells at later developmental stages is due to abortive infection (see Author response image 3 and associated response). This supports the notion that abortive infection becomes increasingly pronounced as larvae mature, potentially explaining the negligible glial labeling observed in adult zebrafish (Dohaku et al., 2019; Satou et al., 2022). However, as noted in our response to the first comment, the disappearance of fluorescence does not indicate the absence of viral entry. Viral receptors may express on glial cells, allowing initial infection despite a failure in subsequent replication. Consequently, glial infection—though abortive—may still compete with neuronal infection and reduce tracing efficiency.

      What is the survival rate of the infected glial cells over time?

      We observed the disappearance of glial fluorescence after transneuronal labeling, while we did not observe punctate fluorescent debris typically indicative of apoptotic cell death. Therefore, we favor the hypothesis that the loss of glial fluorescence results from abortive infection rather than cell death. Abortive infection refers to a scenario in which viral replication is actively suppressed by host antiviral responses, preventing the production of infectious viral particles. For example, recent studies have shown that lab-attenuated rabies virus (RV) induces the accumulation of aberrant double-stranded DNA in astrocytes, which activates mitochondrial antiviral-signaling protein (MAVS) and subsequent interferon expression (Tian et al., 2018). This antiviral response inhibits RV replication, ultimately resulting in abortive infection. 

      In addition, we quantified the proportion of glial cells labeled at 2 dpi and 4dpi that retained fluorescence over time. By 6 dpi (approximately 11 dpf), glial labeling had largely diminished in both groups (Author response image 3). These results suggest that the decline in glial fluorescence is more closely linked to larval age than to the duration of glial infection, supporting the notion of abortive infection. This also addresses the reviewer’s earlier concern and indicates that glial labeling is mitigated in older larvae.

      Author response image 3.

      Fraction of glial cells with fluorescence retention. (a and b) Proportion of glial cells labeled at 2 dpi (a) and 4 dpi (b) that retained fluorescence over time. Data are from the CVS|N2cG|36°C group. In boxplots: center, median; bounds of box, first and third quartiles; whiskers, minimum and maximum values. n.s., not-significant; *P < 0.05, **P < 0.01 (nonparametric two-tailed Mann-Whitney test).

      If an infected glial cell dies due to infection or gets ablated, does the rabies virus spread from the dead glial cells?

      In our system, glial cells do not express the rabies glycoprotein (G). Therefore, even if glial cells are transneuronally infected, they cannot support viral budding or assembly of infectious particles due to the absence of G (Mebatsion et al., 1996), preventing further viral propagation to neighboring cells.

      If TVA and rabies G are delivered to glial cells, followed by rabies virus injection, will it lead to the infection of other glial cells or neurons?

      We have conducted experiments in which TVA and rabies G were specifically expressed in astroglia using the gfap promoter, followed by RVdG-mCherry[EnvA] injection. This resulted in initial infection of TVA-positive astroglia and occasional subsequent labeling of nearby TVA-negative astroglia (Author response image 4), suggesting astroglia-toastroglia transmission. Notably, no neuronal labeling was observed. This glial-to-glial spread is consistent with previous rabies tracing studies reporting similar phenomena involving the interaction of astrocytes with astrocytes and microglia (Clark et al., 2021). However, the underlying mechanism remains unclear, and we have discussed this in response to the first comment.

      Author response image 4.

      Viral tracing initiated from astroglia. (a) Confocal images of the tectum of a larva expressing EGFP and TVA using UGBT in randomly sparse astroglia (gfap<sup>+</sup>) and infected by SADdG-mCherry[EnvA] (magenta) injected into the anterior hindbrain.  (b) Confocal images of the posterior hindbrain of a larva expressing EGFP and TVA using UGNT in randomly sparse astroglia (gfap<sup>+</sup>) and infected by CVSdG-tdTomato[EnvA] (magenta) injected into the anterior hindbrain. Dashed yellow circles, starter astroglia (EGFP+/mCherry<su>+</sup> or EGFP<sup>+</sup>/tdTomato<sup>+</sup>); gray arrows, transneuronally labeled astroglia (tdTomato<sup>+</sup>/EGFP<sup>−</sup>); dashed white lines, tectum or hindbrain boundaries. C, caudal; R, rostral. Scale bars, 20 μm.<br />

      Answers to any of these questions could greatly benefit the broader research community.

      (2) The optimal virus tracing effect has to be achieved by raising the injected larvae at 36C. Since the routine temperature of zebrafish culture is around 28C, a more thorough characterization of the effect on the health of zebrafish should be conducted.

      Yes, 36°C is required to achieve optimal labeling efficiency. Although this is above the standard zebrafish culture temperature (28°C), previous work (Satou et al., 2022) and our observations indicate that this transient elevation does not adversely affect larval health within the experimental time window. 

      In the previous study, Satou et al. reported no temperature-dependent effects on swimming behavior, social interaction, or odor discrimination in adult fish maintained at 28°C and 36°C. In larvae, both non-injected and virus-injected fish showed a decrease in survival at later time points (7 dpi), with slightly increased mortality observed at elevated temperatures.

      In our study, we raised the same batch of non-virus-injected larvae at 28°C and 36°C, and found no mortality over a 10-day period. For CVS-N2c-injected larvae, electrode insertion caused injury, but survival rates remained around 80% at both temperatures (see Figure 3A). Moreover, we successfully maintained CVS-N2c-injected larvae at 36°C for over a month, indicating that elevated temperature does not adversely affect fish health. Notably, higher temperatures were associated with an accelerated developmental rate. 

      This point was briefly addressed in the previous version and has now been further elaborated in the revised Discussion section (page 8).

      (3) Given the ability of time-lapse imaging of the infected larval zebrafish brain, the system can be taken advantage of to tackle important issues of rabies virus tracing tools.

      a) Toxicity. 

      The toxicity of rabies viruses is an important issue that limits their application and affects the interpretation of traced circuits. For example, if a significant proportion of starter cells die before analysis, the traced presynaptic networks cannot be reliably assigned to a "defined" population of starter cells. In this manuscript, the authors did an excellent job of characterizing the effects of different rabies strains, G proteins derived from various strains, and levels of G protein expression on starter cell survival. However, an additional parameter that should be tested is the dose of rabies virus injection. The current method section states that all rabies virus preparations were diluted to 2x10^8 infection units per ml, and 2-5 nl of virus suspension was injected near the target cells. It would be interesting to know the impact of the dose/volume of virus injection on retrograde tracing efficiency and toxicity. Would higher titers of the virus lead to more efficient labeling but stronger toxicities? What would be the optimal dose/volume to balance efficiency and toxicity? Addressing these questions would provide valuable insights and help optimize the use of rabies viruses for circuit tracing.

      This is an important concern. Viral cytotoxicity is primarily driven by the level of viral transcription and replication, which inhibits host protein synthesis (Komarova et al., 2007). The RVdG-EnvA typically infects cells at a rate of one viral particle per cell (Zhang et al., 2024), suggesting that increasing viral concentration does not proportionally increase percell infection. Accordingly, viral titer and injection volume are unlikely to influence cytotoxicity at the single-cell level. In our experiments, injection volumes up to 20 nl (i.e., 4 to 10 times the standard injection volume) did not affect starter cell survival. However, higher titers or volumes may increase the number of initially infected starter cells, potentially leading to greater overall mortality in larval zebrafish.

      Similarly, given that rabies virus typically infects cells at one particle per cell, increasing viral titer alone is unlikely to enhance tracing efficiency once the virus type is fixed. In contrast, the level of G protein expression significantly influences tracing efficiency (see Figure 2D). However, excessive G protein expression reduces the survival of starter cells (see Figure 3D). Therefore, careful control of G protein levels is essential to balance tracing efficiency and cytotoxicity.

      Notably, regardless of whether infected cells undergo apoptosis or necrosis due to cytotoxicity, the resulting disruption of the plasma membrane severely impairs viral budding. As a result, the formation of intact, G protein-enveloped viral particles is prevented, limiting further infection of neighboring neurons.

      The latest second-generation ΔGL RV vectors (Jin et al., 2024), which lack both the G and L (viral polymerase) genes, have been shown to markedly reduce cytotoxicity. These improved tracing strategies may be explored in future zebrafish studies to further optimize labeling efficiency and cell viability.

      The issue of viral titer and volume has been addressed in the revised Discussion section (page 10).

      b) Primary starters and secondary starters: 

      Given that the trans-expression of TVA and G is widespread, there is the possibility of coexistence of starter cells from the initial infection (primary starters) and starter cells generated by rabies virus spreading from the primary starters to presynaptic neurons expressing G. This means that the labeled input cells could be a mixed population connected with either the primary or secondary starter cells.

      It would be immensely interesting if time-lapse imaging could be utilized to observe the appearance of such primary and secondary starter cells. Assuming there is a time difference between the initial appearance of these two populations, it may be possible to differentiate the input cells wired to these populations based on a similar temporal difference in their initial appearance. This approach could provide valuable insights into the dynamics of rabies virus spread and the connectivity of neural circuits.

      The reviewers suggestion is valuable. Regarding the use of Purkinje cells (PCs) as starter cells, we consider the occurrence of secondary PCs to be extremely rare. Although previous evidence suggests that PCs can form synaptic connections with one another (Chang et al., 2020), our sparse labeling strategy—typically involving fewer than 10 labeled cells— significantly reduces the likelihood of viral transmission between PC starter cells. In addition, if secondary starter PCs were frequently generated, we would expect increased tracing efficiency at 10 dpi compared to 6 dpi. However, our results show no significant difference (see Figure 2—figure supplement 1C and G). 

      Given the restricted expression of TVA and G in PCs, even if a limited number of secondary starters were generated, the labeled inputs would predominantly be granule cells (GCs), thereby preserving the cell-type identity of upstream inputs. While this raises a potential concern regarding an overestimation of the convergence index (CI). Notably, within the GC-PC circuit, individual GCs often project to multiple PCs. Consequently, a GC labeled via a secondary PC may also a bona fide presynaptic partner of the primary starter population. This overlap could mitigate the overestimation of CI. Taken together, we believe that the CI values reported in this study provide a reasonable approximation of monosynaptic connectivity.

      In scenarios where TVA and G are broadly expressed—for example, under the control of vglut2a promoter—secondary starter cells may arise frequently. In such cases, long-term time-lapse imaging in the zebrafish whole brain presents a promising strategy to distinguish primary and secondary starter cells, along with their respective input populations, based on the timing of their appearance. This approach potentially enables multi-step circuit tracing within individual animals. An alternative strategy is to use an EnvA-pseudotyped, G-competent rabies virus, which allows targeted initial infection while supporting multisynaptic propagation. When combined with temporally resolved imaging, this strategy could facilitate direct labeling of higher-order circuits and allow clear differentiation between multi-order inputs and the original starter population over time.

      In conclusion, we find this suggestion compelling and will explore these strategies in future studies to optimize and broaden the application of rabies virus-based circuit tracing.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The study by Chen, Deng et al. aims to develop an efficient viral transneuronal tracing method that allows efficient retrograde tracing in the larval zebrafish. The authors utilize pseudotyped-rabies virus that can be targeted to specific cell types using the EnvA-TvA systems. Pseudotyped rabies virus has been used extensively in rodent models and, in recent years, has begun to be developed for use in adult zebrafish. However, compared to rodents, the efficiency of the spread in adult zebrafish is very low (~one upstream neuron labeled per starter cell). Additionally, there is limited evidence of retrograde tracing with pseudotyped rabies in the larval stage, which is the stage when most functional neural imaging studies are done in the field. In this study, the authors systematically optimized several parameters of rabies tracing, including different rabies virus strains, glycoprotein types, temperatures, expression construct designs, and elimination of glial labeling. The optimal configurations developed by the authors are up to 5-10 fold higher than more typically used configurations.

      The results are solid and support the conclusions. However, the methods should be described in more detail to allow other zebrafish researchers to apply this method in their own work.

      Additionally, some findings are presented anecdotally, i.e., without quantification or sufficient detail to allow close examinations. Lastly, there is concern that the reagents created by the authors will not be easily accessible to the zebrafish community.

      (1) The titer used in each experiment was not stated. In the methods section, it is stated that aliquots are stored at 2x10e8. Is it diluted for injection? Are all of the experiments in the manuscripts with the same titer?

      We injected all three viral vectors as undiluted stock aliquots. The titer for SADdGmCherry[EnvA], CVSdG-tdTomato[EnvA], and CVSdG-mCherry-2A-Cre[EnvA]) was 2 × 10<sup>8</sup>, 2 × 10<sup>8</sup>, and 3 × 10<sup>8</sup> infectious units/mL, respectively. This has been clarified in the updated Methods section (page 12).

      (2) The age for injection is quite broad (3-5 dpf in Fig 1 and 4-6 dpf in Fig 2). Given that viral spread efficiency is usually more robust in younger animals, describing the exact injection age for each experiment is critical.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestions. For the initial experiments tracing randomly from neurons in Figure 1, the injection age was primarily 3–4 dpf, with a one-day difference. Due to the slower development of PCs, the injection age for experiments related to Figure 2,3, and 4, is mainly 5 dpf. To clarify the developmental stages at the time of injection for each experiment, we have  newly added tables (see Figure 1,2—table supplement 2) listing the number of fish used at each injection age for all experimental groups shown in Figure 1 and 2.

      (3) More details should be provided for the paired electrical stimulation-calcium imaging study. How many GC cells were tested? How many had corresponding PC cell responses? What is the response latency? For example, images of stimulated and recorded GCs and PCs should be shown.

      Yes, these are important details for the paired electrical stimulation-calcium imaging study. We stimulated 33 GCs from 32 animals and detected calcium responses in putative postsynaptic PCs in 15 cases. Among these, we successfully ablated the single GC in 11 pairs and observed a weakened calcium response in PCs following ablation (see Figure 2M). The response latency was determined as the first calcium imaging frame where ΔF/F exceeded the baseline (pre-stimulus average) by 3 times the standard deviation. Imaging was performed at 5 Hz, and as shown in Figure 2L, the calculated average response latency was 152 ± 35 ms (mean ± SEM), indicating an immediate response with calcium intensity from the first post-stimulus imaging frame consistently exceeding the threshold.

      We have added additional details to the Results (page 5), Discussion (page 9), and Methods (page 15) sections. A representative image showing both the stimulated GC and the recorded PC has been added to Figure 2 in the revised manuscript (see Figure 2K).

      (4) It is unclear how connectivity between specific PC and GC is determined for single neuron connectivity. In other images (Figure 4C), there are usually multiple starter cells and many GCs. It was not shown that the image resolution can establish clear axon dendritic contacts between cell pairs.

      In our experiments, sparse labeling typically results in 1–10 starter cells per fish. Regarding the case shown in Figure 4C (right column), only two PC starters were labeled, which simplifies the assignment of presynaptic inputs to individual PCs. Connectivity is determined based on clear axon-dendritic or axon-cell body apposition between GCs and PCs. We have accordingly added more details to the Methods (page 16) section regarding how we determined connectivity between specific PCs and GCs.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      To enable broader use of this technique, I would encourage the authors to submit their zebrafish lines, plasmids, and plasmid sequences to public repositories such as ZIRC and  Addgene. Additionally, there is no mention of how viral vectors will be shared.

      We have deposited the related zebrafish lines at CZRC (China Zebrafish Resource Center) and uploaded plasmid maps and sequences to Addgene. The viral vectors are available through BrainCase (Shenzhen, China). We have included the information in the revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The authors establish reagents and define experimental parameters useful for defining neurons retrograde to a neuron of interest.

      Strengths:

      A clever approach, careful optimization, novel reagents, and convincing data together lead to convincing conclusions.

      Weaknesses: 

      In the current version of the manuscript, the tracing results could be better centered with  respect to past work, certain methods could be presented more clearly, and other approaches worth considering.

      Appraisal/Discussion:

      Trans-neuronal tracing in the larval zebrafish preparation has lagged behind rodent models,limiting "circuit-cracking" experiments. Previous work has demonstrated that pseudotyped rabies virus-mediated tracing could work, but published data suggested that there was considerable room for optimization. The authors take a major step forward here, identifying a number of key parameters to achieve success and establishing new transgenic reagents that incorporate modern intersectional approaches. As a proof of concept, the manuscript concludes with a rough characterization of inputs to cerebellar Purkinje cells. The work will be of considerable interest to neuroscientists who use the zebrafish model.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The main limitations of the work are as follows:

      (1) The optimizations might differ for different neurons. Purkinje cells are noteworthy because they develop considerably during the time window detailed here, almost doubling in number between 7-14dpf. Presumably, connectivity follows. This sort of neurogenesis is much less common elsewhere. It would be useful to show similar results in, say, tectal neurons, which would have spatially-restricted retinal ganglion cells labelled.

      We acknowledge that Purkinje cells (PCs) undergo significant development between 7–14 dpf, which may influence synaptic connectivity and result in differences in tracing efficiency. However, all experimental conditions were standardized across groups, and the selection of starter PCs was unbiased, typically focusing on PCs in the lateral region of the CCe (corpus cerebelli) subregion, ensuring that the relative comparisons remain valid. 

      We agree that testing other neuronal populations would be valuable, as tracing efficiency is influenced by multiple factors, such as the number of endogenous inputs, synaptic maturation, and developmentally regulated synaptic strength. Tectal neurons, which receive spatially restricted retinal ganglion cell inputs, would be a suitable choice for further investigation. However, due to the various tectal cell types and the opacity of the eyeball, such studies present additional technical challenges and are beyond the scope of this paper.

      (2) The virus is delivered by means of microinjection near the cell. This is invasive and challenging for labs that dont routinely perform electrophysiology. It would be useful to know if coarser methods of viral delivery (e.g. intraventricular injection) would be successful. 

      Our protocol does not require the level of precision needed for electrophysiology. The procedure can be performed using a standard high-magnification upright (135× magnification, Nikon SMZ18) or inverted fluorescence microscope (200× magnification, Olympus IX51). The virus suspension was loaded into a glass micropipette with a ~10 µm tip diameter and directly microinjected into the target region using a micromanipulator. The procedure was comparable to embryonic microinjection in terms of precision and operational control. Notably, direct contact with the target cells is not necessary, as the injected virus solution can diffuse and effectively infect nearby cells.  

      We had attempted intraventricular injection as an alternative, but it failed to produce robust labeling, reinforcing the necessity for direct tissue injection. 

      We have now included additional methodological details in the Methods section (page 13). 

      (3) Because of the combination of transgenic lines, plasmid injection, and viral type, it is often confusing to follow exactly what is being done for a particular experiment. It would be useful to specify the transgenic background used for each experiment using standard nomenclature e.g. "Plasmids were injected into Tg(elavl3:GAL4) fish." This is particularly important for the experiments in Figure 4: it isnt clear what the background used for the sparse labels was. 

      Thank the reviewer for bringing this issue to our attention. In order to improve clarity, we have revised the figure legends to explicitly state the transgenic background, injected plasmids, and viral type used in each experiment, particularly for Figure 4. 

      (4) Plasmids should be deposited with Addgene along with maps specifying the particular "codon-optimized Tetoff" per 388. 

      We confirm that all plasmids, including those containing codon-optimized Tetoff constructs, have been uploaded to Addgene along with detailed maps.

      (5) It would be useful to know if there were more apoptotic cells after transfection -- an acridine orange or comparable assay is recommended, rather than loss of fluorescence. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to assess apoptosis using acridine orange staining or comparable assays. We agree that such methods can provide more direct detection of apoptotic events. However, we believe that the difference in cytotoxicity is already evident in our current data: SAD-infected cells exhibit greater loss than CVSinfected cells (see Figure 3D). This is consistent with previous observations in mice, where greater toxicity of SAD compared to CVS was demonstrated using propidium iodide (PI) staining in cultured cells (Reardon et al., 2016).

      (6) Line 219-228 Hibis lab has described the subtypes of granule cells in detail already; the work should discuss the tracings with respect to previous characterizations instead of limiting that work to a citation. 

      Thanks for the reminding of this point. We have expanded the Results section (page 6) to discuss the subtypes of GCs and PCs in relation to previously reported characterizations.

      (7) "Activities" is often used when "activity" is correct. The use of English in the manuscript is, by and large, excellent, but its worth running the text through software like Grammarly to catch the occasional error. 

      We have carefully edited the manuscript using professional language editing tools to correct any grammatical issues.

      (8) The experiments in 2J-2L would be more convincing if they were performed on inferior olive inputs as well -- especially given the small size of the granule cells. 

      We acknowledge the reviewers observation that granule cells (GCs) are relatively small, which may underline the finding that, out of 33 stimulated GCs, only 15 were capable of eliciting calcium responses in putative postsynaptic PCs. However, in all 11 pairs where a single GC was successfully ablated, we observed a weakened calcium response in PCs after the ablation (see Figure 2M), suggesting our tracing approach specifically identifies synaptically coupled neurons. We have clarified this point in the revised manuscript (page 5).

      We agree that verifying the IO inputs to PCs would strengthen the validity of our findings. However, in our experiments, the probability of tracing upstream IO cells was relatively low. This may be due to the developmental immaturity of the synapse and the fact that each PC typically receives input from a single IO cell. Additionally, the deep and distant anatomical location of the IO presents technical challenges for paired electrical stimulationcalcium imaging study. To address these limitations, we are currently exploring the integration of viral tracing and optogenetics to further investigate IO-PC connectivity in future studies.

      (9) It would be useful if the manuscript discussed the efficacy of trans-synaptic labelling. What fraction of granule cell / olivary inputs to a particular Purkinje cell do the authors think their method captures?

      This is an important point for assessing the efficacy of our trans-synaptic labeling. Ideally, electron microscopy (EM) data would provide the most precise evaluation. In the absence of EM data, we estimated the number of GCs, IOs and PCs using light microscopy-based cell counting. 

      At approximately 7 dpf, we manually counted 327 ± 14 PCs and 2318 ± 70 GCs in the Tg(2×en.cpce-E1B:tdTomato-CAAX) and Tg(cbln12:GAL4FF);Tg(5×UAS:EGFP) zebrafish cerebellum, across all subregions (Va, CCe, EG, and LCa). Given the developmental increase in the number of GCs and the fact that some GCs that have exclusively ipsilateral projections, and that a single PC would not receive input from all parallel fibers, we estimate that by 10–14 dpf, a single PC receives approximately 1000– 2000 GC inputs. Under optimal tracing conditions, we observed an average of 20 labeled GC inputs per PC, yielding a capture fraction of ~1–2%. Although this represents only a subset of total inputs, it is consistent with mammalian studies (Wall et al., 2010; Callaway et al., 2015), suggesting inherent limitations of this viral labeling approach.

      For IO inputs, we counted 325 ± 26 inferior olivary neurons in Tg(elavl3:H2B-GCaMP6s) fish. A single PC likely receives input from one IO neuron, though an IO neuron may innervate multiple PCs. Accordingly, the observed capture rate for IO inputs was lower (7 out of 248 starters). 

      Further optimization is required to enhance the tracing efficiency. We have now incorporated a Discussion on this point in the revised manuscript (page 8).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      The authors investigated sleep and circadian rhythm disturbances in Fmr1 KO mice. Initially, they monitored daily home cage behaviors to assess sleep and circadian disruptions. Next, they examined the adaptability of circadian rhythms in response to photic suppression and skeleton photic periods. To explore the underlying mechanisms, they traced retino-suprachiasmatic connectivity. The authors further analyzed the social behaviors of Fmr1 KO mice and tested whether a scheduled feeding strategy could mitigate sleep, circadian, and social behavior deficits. Finally, they demonstrated that scheduled feeding corrected cytokine levels in the plasma of mutant mice. 

      Strengths: 

      (1) The manuscript addresses an important topic-investigating sleep deficits in an FXS mouse model and proposing a potential therapeutic strategy. 

      (2) The study includes a comprehensive experimental design with multiple methodologies, which adds depth to the investigation. 

      We thank the reviewer for the positive comments.

      Weaknesses: 

      (1) The first serious issue in the manuscript is the lack of a clear description of how they performed the experiments and the missing definitions of various parameters in the results.  

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out lapses in the editing of the manuscript. We were trying to keep the descriptions of previously published methods brief but must have gone too far, the manuscript has been carefully checked for grammar and readability. Description of the experimental design has been refined and a graphical presentation has been added as Suppl Fig 3. The sleep and circadian parameters have been thoroughly explained in the methods and briefly in the figure legnds.

      (2) Although the manuscript has a relatively long Methods section, some essential information is missing. For instance, the definition of sleep bout, as described above, is unclear. Additional missing information includes

      Figure 2: "Rhythmic strength (%)" and "Cycle-to-cycle variability (min)." 

      Figure 3: "Activity suppression." 

      Figure 4: "Rhythmic power (V%)" (is this different from rhythmic strength (%)?) and "Subjective day activity (%)." 

      We have provided definitions for the general audience of the terms used in the field of circadian rhythms, such as sleep bout, rhythm power, cycle-to-cycle, masking, and % of activity during the day in the methods and Fig legends. Most of the techniques used in this study, for example, the behavioral measurement of sleep or locomotor activity, are well established and have been used in multiple published works, including our own. We have made sure to include citations for interested readers.

      Figure 5: Clear labeling of the SCN's anatomical features and an explanation for quantifying only the ventral part instead of the entire SCN. 

      We have added more landmarks (position of the third ventricle and optic chiasm) to Fig 5, and have outlined the shell and core of the SCN in two additional images of the ventral hypothalamus in Suppl fig 4.

      We had actually quantified the fluorescence in the whole SCN as well as in the ventral part.This was/is described in the methods as well as reported in the results section and Table 4 “Likewise, a subtle decrease in the intensity of the labelled fibers was found in the whole SCN (Table 4) of the Fmr1 KO mice as compared to WT.“ 

      Methods: ” Two methods of analyses were carried out on the images of 5 consecutive sections per animal containing the middle SCN. First, the relative intensity of the Cholera Toxin fluorescent processes was quantified in the whole SCN, both left and right separately, by scanning densitometry using the Fiji image processing package of the NIH ImageJ software (https://imagej.net). A single ROI of fixed size (575.99 μm x 399.9 μm, width x height) was used to measure the relative integrated density (mean gray values x area of the ROI) in all the images. The values from the left and right SCN were averaged per section and 5 sections per animal were averaged to obtain one value per animal………..”

      Since the retinal innervation of the SCN is strongest in the ventral aspect, where the retino-hypothalamic fibers reach the SCN and our goal was to identify differences in the input to the SCN, e.g. defects in the retino-SCN connectivity as suggested by some deficits in circadian behaviour; we also looked at intensity of Cholera Toxin in the fibers arriving to the ventral SCN from the retina.

      We have added a sentence in the methods about the rationale for measuring the intensity of the cholera toxin labelled fiber in the whole SCN and also just in the ventral part: “Second, the retinal innervation of the SCN is strongest in the ventral aspect, where the retino-hypothalamic fibers reach the SCN, hence, the distribution….”

      Figure 6: Inconsistencies in terms like "Sleep frag. (bout #)" and "Sleep bouts (#)." Consistent terminology throughout the manuscript is essential.

      We have now clearly explained that sleep bouts are a measure of sleep fragmentation throughout the manuscript and in the fig legends; in addition, we have corrected the figures, reconciled the terminology, which is now consistent throughout the results and methods.

      Methods: “Sleep fragmentation was determined by the number of sleep bouts, which were operationally defined as episodes of continuous immobility with a sleep count greater than 3 per minute, persisting for at least 60 secs.”

      (3) Figure 1A shows higher mouse activity during ZT13-16. It is unclear why the authors scheduled feeding during ZT15- 21, as this seems to disturb the rhythm. Consistent with this, the body weights of WT and Fmr1 KO mice decreased after scheduled feeding. The authors should explain the rationale for this design clearly.

      We have added to the rationale for the feeding schedule. This protocol was initially used by the Panda group to counter metabolic dysfunction (Hatori et al., 2012). We have used it for many years now (see citations below) in various mouse models presenting with circadian disruption to reset the clock and improve sleep. This study represents our first application/intervention in a mouse model of a neurodevelopmental disease.

      Hatori M, Vollmers C, Zarrinpar A, DiTacchio L, Bushong EA, Gill S, Leblanc M, Chaix A, Joens M, Fitzpatrick JA, Ellisman MH, Panda S. Time-restricted feeding without reducing caloric intake prevents metabolic diseases in mice fed a high-fat diet. Cell Metab. 2012 Jun 6;15(6):848-60. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2012.04.019. Epub 2012 May 17. PMID: 22608008; PMCID: PMC3491655.

      Chiem E, Zhao K, Dell'Angelica D, Ghiani CA, Paul KN, Colwell CS. Scheduled feeding improves sleep in a mouse model of Huntington's disease. Front Neurosci. 2024 18:1427125. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2024.1427125. PMID: 39161652.

      Whittaker DS, Akhmetova L, Carlin D, Romero H, Welsh DK, Colwell CS, Desplats P. Circadian modulation by time-restricted feeding rescues brain pathology and improves memory in mouse models of Alzheimer's disease. Cell Metab. 2023 35(10):1704- 1721.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cmet.2023.07.014. PMID: 37607543

      Brown MR, Sen SK, Mazzone A, Her TK, Xiong Y, Lee JH, Javeed N, Colwell CS, Rakshit K, LeBrasseur NK, Gaspar-Maia A, Ordog T, Matveyenko AV. Time-restricted feeding prevents deleterious metabolic effects of circadian disruption through epigenetic control of β cell function. Sci Adv. 2021 7(51):eabg6856. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abg6856. PMID: 34910509

      Whittaker DS, Loh DH, Wang HB, Tahara Y, Kuljis D, Cutler T, Ghiani CA, Shibata S, Block GD, Colwell CS. Circadian-based Treatment Strategy Effective in the BACHD Mouse Model of Huntington's Disease. J Biol Rhythms. 2018 33(5):535-554. doi: 10.1177/0748730418790401. PMID: 30084274.

      Wang HB, Loh DH, Whittaker DS, Cutler T, Howland D, Colwell CS. Time-Restricted Feeding Improves Circadian Dysfunction as well as Motor Symptoms in the Q175 Mouse Model of Huntington's Disease. eNeuro. 2018 Jan 3;5(1):ENEURO.0431-17.2017. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0431-17.2017.

      Loh DH, Jami SA, Flores RE, Truong D, Ghiani CA, O'Dell TJ, Colwell CS. Misaligned feeding impairs memories. Elife. 2015 4:e09460. doi: 10.7554/eLife.09460.

      (4) The interpretation of social behavior results in Figure 6 is questionable. The authors claim that Fmr1 KO mice cannot remember the first stranger in a three-chamber test, writing, "The reduced time in exploring and staying in the novelmouse chamber suggested that the Fmr1 KO mutants were not able to distinguish the second novel mouse from the first now-familiar mouse." However, an alternative explanation is that Fmr1 KO mice do remember the first stranger but prefer to interact with it due to autistic-like tendencies. Data in Table 5 show that Fmr1 KO mice spent more time interacting with the first stranger in the 3-chamber social recognition test, which support this possibility. Similarly, in the five-trial social test, Fmr1 KO mice's preference for familiar mice might explain the reduced interaction with the second stranger.

      Thank you for this interesting interpretation of the social behavior experiments. We used the common interpretations for both the three-chamber test and the 5-trial social interaction test, but have now modified the text leaving space for alternative interpretations, have soften the language, and mentioned decreased sociability in the Fmr1 KO mice. “The reduced time spent exploring the novel-mouse chamber suggest that the mutants were, perhaps, unable to distinguish the second novel mouse from the first, now familiar, mouse, along with decreased sociability.”

      In Figure 6C (five-trial social test results), only the fifth trial results are shown. Data for trials 1-4 should be provided and compared with the fifth trial. The behavioral features of mice in the 5-trial test can then be shown completely. In addition, the total interaction times for trials 1-4 (154 {plus minus} 15.3 for WT and 150 {plus minus} 20.9 for Fmr1 KO) suggest normal sociability in Fmr1 KO mice (it is different from the results of 3-chamber). Thus, individual data for trials 1-4 are required to draw reliable conclusions.  

      We have added a suppl figure showing the individual trial results for both WT and Fmr1 KO mice as requested (Suppl. Fig. 2).  

      In Table 6 and Figure 6G-6J, the authors claim that "Sleep duration (Figures 6G, H) and fragmentation (Figures 6I, J) exhibited a moderate-strong correlation with both social recognition and grooming." However, Figure 6I shows a p-value of 0.077, which is not significant. Moreover, Table 6 shows no significant correlation between SNPI of the three-chamber social test and any sleep parameters. These data do not support the authors' conclusions. 

      Thanks for pointing out the error with statement about Fig. 6I.

      “…. Sleep duration (Fig. 6G, H; Table 6) exhibited a moderate to strong correlation with both social recognition and grooming time, while sleep fragmentation (measured by sleep bouts number) only correlated with the latter (Fig. 6J); the length of sleep bouts (Table 6) showed moderate correlation with both social recognition and repetitive behavior. In addition, a moderate correlation was seen between grooming time and the circadian parameters, rhythmic power and activity onset variability (Table 6). In short, our work suggests that even when tested during their circadian active phase, the Fmr1 KO mice exhibit robust repetitive and social behavioral deficits. Moreover, the shorter and more fragmented the daytime sleep, the more severe the behavioral impairment in the mutants.”

      (5) Figure 7 demonstrates the effect of scheduled feeding on circadian activity and sleep behaviors, representing another critical set of results in the manuscript. Notably, the WT+ALF and Fmr1 KO+ALF groups in Figure 7 underwent the same handling as the WT and Fmr1 KO groups in Figures 1 and 2, as no special treatments were applied to these mice. However, the daily patterns observed in Figures 7A, 7B, 7F, and 7G differ substantially from those shown in Figures 2B and 1A, respectively. Additionally, it is unclear why the WT+ALF and Fmr1 KO+ALF groups did not exhibit differences in Figures 7I and 7J, especially considering that Fmr1 KO mice displayed more sleep bouts but shorter bout lengths in Figures 1C and 1D. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s attention to the subtle details of the behavioral measurement of sleep and believe the reviewer to be referring to differences in the behavioral measurements of sleep with data shown in Table 1 and Table 7. The first set of experiments described in this study was carried out between 2016 and 2017 and involves the comparison between WT and Fmr1 KO mice. The WT and mutants were obtained from JAX. In this initial set of experiments (Table 1), the total amount of sleep in 24 hrs was reduced in the KO, albeit not significantly, and these also exhibited sleep bouts of significantly reduced duration. The pandemic forced us to greatly slow down the research and reduce our mouse colonies. Post-pandemic, we used new cohorts of Fmr1 KO ordered again from JAX for the TRF experiment presented in this study. In these cohorts, the KO mice exhibited a significant reduction in total sleep (Table 7) and the sleep bouts were still shorter but not significantly. We have added to our text to explain that the description of the mutants and TRF interventions were carried out at different times (2017 vs 2022). We would like to emphasize that we always run contemporaneously controls and experimental groups to be used for the statistical analyses. We believe that the data are remarkably consistent over these years, even with different students doing the measurements. 

      Furthermore, it is not specified whether the results in Figure 7 were collected after two weeks of scheduled feeding (for how many days?) or if they represent the average data from the two-week treatment period.

      This is another good point raised by the reviewer. The activity measurements are collected during the 2 weeks (14 days) then the TRF was extended for a 3 more days to allow the behavioral sleep measurements.

      We have added a supplementary figure (Supp Fig 3) depicting the different experimental designs.

      The rationale behind analyzing "ZT 0-3 activity" in Figure 7D instead of the parameters shown in Figures 2C and 2D is also unclear. 

      We have added to our explanation. In prior work, we found that the TRF protocol has a big impact on the beginning of the sleep time, hence, we specifically targeted this 3-hours interval in the analysis.

      In Figure 7F, some data points appear to be incorrectly plotted. For instance, the dark blue circle at ZT13 connects to the light blue circle at ZT14 and the dark blue circle at ZT17. This is inconsistent, as the dark blue circle at ZT13 should link to the dark blue circle at ZT14. Similarly, it is perplexing that the dark blue circle at ZT16 connects to both the light blue and dark blue circles at ZT17. Such errors undermine confidence in the data. The authors need to provide a clear explanation of how these data were processed. 

      Thank you for bringing this to our attention. The data were plotted correctly, however, those data points completely overlapped with those behind, masking them. We have now offset a bit them for clarity.

      Lastly, in the Figure 7 legend, Table 6 is cited; however, this appears to be incorrect. It seems the authors intended to refer to Table 7. 

      We have corrected this error, thank you.  

      (6) Similar to the issue in Figure 7F, the data for day 12 in Supplemental Figure 2 includes two yellow triangles but lacks a green triangle. It is unclear how the authors constructed this chart, and clarification is needed. 

      We have corrected this error. As the reviewer pointed out, we filled the triangle on day 12 with yellow instead of green.  

      (7) In Figure 8, a 5-trial test was used to assess the effect of scheduled feeding on social behaviors. It is essential to present the results for all trials (1 to 4). Additionally, it is unclear whether the results for familial mice in Figure 8A correspond to trials 1, 2, 3, or 4. 

      The legend for Figure 8 also appears to be incorrect: "The left panels show the time spent in social interactions when the second novel stranger mouse was introduced to the testing mouse in the 5-trial social interaction test. The significant differences were analyzed by two-way ANOVA followed by Holm-Sidak's multiple comparisons test with feeding treatment and genotype as factors." This description does not align with the content of the left panels. Moreover, two-way ANOVA is not the appropriate statistical analysis for Figure 8A. The authors need to provide accurate details about the analysis and revise the figure legend accordingly. 

      We apologies for the confusing Figure legend which has been revised: 

      “Fig. 8: TRF improved social memory and stereotypic grooming behavior in the Fmr1 KO mice. (A) Social memory was evaluated with the 5-trial social interaction test as described above. The social memory recognition was significantly augmented in the Fmr1 KO by the intervention, suggesting that the treated mutants were able to distinguish the novel mouse from the familiar mouse. The time spent in social interactions with the novel mouse in the 5<sup>th</sup>-trial was increased to WT-like levels in the mutants on TRF. Paired t-tests were used to evaluate significant differences in the time spent interacting with the test mouse in the 4<sup>th</sup> (familiar mouse) and 5<sup>th</sup> (novel mouse) trials.  *P < 0.05 indicates the significant time spent with the novel mouse compared to the familiar mouse. (B) Grooming was assessed in a novel arena in mice of each genotype (WT, Fmr1 KO) under each feeding condition and the resulting data analyzed by two-way ANOVA followed by the Holm-Sidak’s multiple comparisons test with feeding regimen and genotype as factors. *P < 0.05 indicates the significant difference within genotype - between diet regimens , and #P < 0.05 those between genotypes - same feeding regimen. (C) TRF did not alter the overall locomotion in the treated mice. See Table 8.”

      To assess social recognition memory, mice underwent a five-trial social interaction paradigm in a neutral open-field arena. Each trial lasted 5 minutes and was separated by a 1-minute inter-trial interval. During trials 1–4, the test mouse was exposed to the same conspecific (Stimulus A) enclosed within a wire cup to permit olfactory and limited tactile interaction. In trial 5, a novel conspecific (Stimulus B) was introduced. Time spent investigating the stimulus B mouse (defined as sniffing or directing the nose toward the enclosure within close proximity) was scored using AnyMaze software. A progressive decrease in investigation time across trials 1–4 reflects habituation, while a significant increase in trial 5 indicates dishabituation and intact social recognition memory. In our data, there was not a lot of habituation in both genotypes, but clear differences can be appreciated between trial 4 with the now familiar mouse and trial 5 with novel mouse. Fig. 8A plots the results from individual animals in Trial 4 with a familiar mouse and in Trial 5 with a novel mouse, we have well specified this in the legends. As such, these data were analyzed with a pair t-test. 

      We used Tow-Way ANOVA to analyse the data reported in Panel 8B and as well as the results in Table 8.  This has been clarified in the legend.

      (8) The circadian activity and sleep behaviors of Fmr1 KO mice have been reported previously, with some findings consistent with the current manuscript, while others contradict it. Although the authors acknowledge this discrepancy, it seems insufficiently thorough to simply state that the reasons for the conflicts are unknown. Did the studies use the same equipment for behavior recording? Were the same parameters used to define locomotor activity and sleep behaviors? The authors are encouraged to investigate these details further, as doing so may uncover something interesting or significant. 

      We agree with the reviewers, and believe that the main differences were likely in the experimental design and possibly interpretation.

      (9) Some subtitles in the Results section and the figure legends do not align well with the presented data. For example, in the section titled "Reduced rhythmic strength and nocturnality in the Fmr1 KOs," it is unclear how the authors justify the claim of altered nocturnality in Fmr1 KO mice. How do the authors define changes in nocturnality? Additionally, the tense used in the subtitles and figure legends is incorrect. The authors are encouraged to carefully review all subtitles and figure legends to correct these errors and enhance readability. 

      Nocturnality is defined as the % of total activity within a 24-h cycle that occurred in the night, since this can be confusing and we agree that it was not well explained we have removed it from the subtitle/figure legends. 

      We have adjusted the subtitles as recommended; however, the tense of the verbs might be a matter of writing style.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      In the present study, the authors, using a mouse model of Fragile X syndrome, explore the very interesting hypothesis that restricting food access over a daily schedule will improve sleep patterns and, subsequently, behavioral capacities. By restricting food access from 12h to 6h over the nocturnal period (active period for mice), they show, in these KO mice, an improvement of the sleep pattern accompanied by reduced systemic levels of inflammatory markers and improved behavior. Using a classical mouse model of neurodevelopmental disorder (NDD), these data suggest that eating patterns might improve sleep quality, reduce inflammation and improve cognitive/behavioral capacities in children with NDD. 

      Strengths: 

      Overall, the paper is very well-written and easy to follow. The rationale of the study is generally well-introduced. The data are globally sound. The provided data support the interpretation overall. 

      Thank you for the positive comments.  

      Weaknesses:  

      (1) The introduction part is quite long in the Abstract, leaving limited space for the data provided by the present study.

      We have revised the Abstract to better focus on the most impactful findings as suggested. 

      (2) A couple of points are not totally clear for a non-expert reader:  - The Fmr1/Fxr2 double KO mice are not well described. What is the rationale for performing both LD and DD measures? 

      We did not use the Fmr1/Fxr2 double KO mice in this study.  

      While measurement of day/night differences in activity rhythms are standardly done in a light/dark (LD) cycle, the organisms must be under constant conditions (DD) to measure their endogenous circadian rhythms (free running activity); this is often needed to uncover a compromised clock as entrainment to the LD cycle can mask deficits in the endogenous circadian rhythms.

      (3) The data on cytokines and chemokines are interesting. However, the rationale for the selection of these molecules is not given. In addition, these measures have been performed in the systemic blood. Measures in the brain could be very informative. 

      The panel that we used had 16 cytokines/chemokines which are reported in Table 9. The experiment included WT and mutants held under 2 different feeding conditions with an n=8 per group. If we are able to obtain more resources, we would like to also carry out a comprehensive investigation of immunomediator levels as well as RNA-seq or Nanostring in selected brain regions associated with ASD aberrant behavioural phenotypes, for instance the prefrontal cortex.

      (4) An important question is the potential impact of fasting vs the impact of the food availability restriction. Indeed, fasting has several effects on brain functioning including cognitive functions. 

      We did not address this issue in the present study. Briefly, the distinction between caloric restriction (CR) and TRF, in which no calories are restricted, has important mechanistic implications in mouse models. While both interventions can impact metabolism, circadian rhythms, and aging, they operate via overlapping but distinct molecular pathways. These have been the topic of recent reviews and investigations. Importantly, the fast-feed cycle can also act as a circadian entrainer (Zeitgeber)

      Ribas-Latre A, Fernández-Veledo S, Vendrell J. Time-restricted eating, the clock ticking behind the scenes. Front Pharmacol. 2024 Aug 8;15:1428601. doi: 10.3389/fphar.2024.1428601. PMID: 39175542; PMCID: PMC11338815.

      Wang R, Liao Y, Deng Y, Shuang R. Unraveling the Health Benefits and Mechanisms of Time-Restricted Feeding: Beyond Caloric Restriction. Nutr Rev. 2025 Mar 1;83(3):e1209-e1224. doi: 10.1093/nutrit/nuae074.

      (5) How do the authors envision the potential translation of the present study to human patients? How to translate the 12 to 6 hours of food access in mice to children with Fragile X syndrome? 

      Time-restricted feeding (TRF) is a type of intermittent fasting that limits food intake to a specific window of time each day (usually 8–12 hours in humans), is being actively studied in adults for benefits on metabolic health, sleep, and circadian rhythms. However, applying TRF to children is not currently recommended as a general intervention, and there are important developmental, medical, and ethical considerations to take into account.  

      On the other hand, we believe that the Fmr1 KO mouse is a good preclinical model for FXS because it closely recapitulates key molecular, cellular, and behavioral phenotypes observed in humans with the disorder. A number of the behavioral phenotypes seen in the mouse mirror those seen in patients including increased anxiety-like behavior, sensory hypersensitivity, social interaction deficits and repetitive behaviors so there is strong face validity.  

      As we show in this study, Fmr1 KO mice present with disrupted sleep/wake cycles and reduced amplitude of circadian rhythms, consistent with findings in individuals with FXS. This makes the Fmr1 KO an excellent model to test out circadian based interventions such as scheduled feeding.

      We believe that pre-clinical research in Fmr1 KO mice bridges the gap between basic discovery and human clinical application. It provides a controlled, cost-effective, and biologically relevant platform for understanding disease mechanisms and testing interventions. These types of experiments need to be done before jumping to humans to ensure that the human trials are scientifically justified and ethically sound.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The authors should: 

      (1) Revise the Methods section for clarity and completeness.  

      We have re-worked the methods for clarity and completeness. 

      (2) Provide consistent and precise definitions for all parameters and terms.  

      We believe that we have provided definitions for all terms.  

      (3) Clarify the rationale for experimental designs, such as the feeding schedule.  

      We have added to the rationale for the feeding schedule.  This feeding schedule has been used in a number of prior studies including our own.  All this work is cited in the manuscript.   

      (4) Reanalyze and transparently present data, including individual trial results.  

      We have added to the figure showing the individual trail results for the 5-trial tests as requested (Supplementary Fig. 2).  

      (5) Conduct appropriate statistical tests and correct figure legends.  

      We believe that we have carried out appropriate statistical tests and have carefully rechecked the figure legends.  

      (6) Investigate discrepancies with prior studies to enhance the discussion. 

      We have added to our discussion of prior work. 

      (7) Improve language quality and ensure consistency in terminology and grammar.  

      We have edited the manuscript to improve language quality.  

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) The Abstract should be rewritten to provide more room for the obtained data.  

      We have re-written the Abstract to focus on the most impactful findings. 

      (2) An additional sentence describing the double KO mice should be added.  

      We did not use double KO mice in this study.  

      (3) The rationale for studying LD and DD should be provided. 

      Measurement of day/night differences are standardly done in a light/dark cycle.  To measure the endogenous circadian rhythms, the organisms must be under constant conditions (Dark/Dark).

      (4) The data on cytokines/chemokines should be strengthened by performing a larger panel of measures both in blood and the brain.  

      The panel that we used had 16 cytokines/chemokines which we report in Table 9.  This was a large experiment with 2 genotypes being held under 2 feeding conditions with n=8 mice per group. If we are able to obtain more resources, we would like to also carry out RNA-seq in different brain regions.  

      (5) The authors should discuss in more detail the potential role of fastening vs restriction of food access.  

      We did not address this issue in the present study.  Briefly, the distinction between caloric restriction (CR) and TRF when no calories are restricted has important mechanistic implications in mouse models. While both interventions can impact metabolism, circadian rhythms, and aging, they operate via overlapping but distinct molecular pathways. 

      (6) The authors should also provide some insight into their view on the potential translation of their experimental studies.  

      We believe that the Fmr1 KO mouse is considered a good preclinical model for FXS because it closely recapitulates key molecular, cellular, and behavioral phenotypes observed in humans with the disorder. A number of the behavioral phenotypes seen in the mouse mirror those seen in patients including increased anxiety-like behavior, sensory hypersensitivity, social interaction deficits and repetitive behaviors so there is strong face validity.   As we  demonstrate in this study, Fmr1 KO mice exibit disrupted sleep/wake cycles and reduced amplitude of circadian rhythms, consistent with findings in individuals with FXS.  This makes the Fmr1 KO an excellent model to test out circadian based interventions such as scheduled feeding.  

      Still we are mindful that the translation of therapeutic findings from mouse to human has proven challenging e.g., mGluR5 antagonists failed in clinical trials despite strong preclinical data (Berry-Kravis et al., 2016).  Therefore, we are cautious in overreaching in our translational interpretations. 

      Berry-Kravis, E., Des Portes, V., Hagerman, R., Jacquemont, S., Charles, P., Visootsak, J., Brinkman, M., Rerat, K., Koumaras, B., Zhu, L., Barth, G. M., Jaecklin, T., Apostol, G., & von Raison, F. (2016). Mavoglurant in fragile X syndrome: Results of two randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trials. Science translational medicine, 8(321), 321ra5. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.aab4109).

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript proposes that 5mC modifications to DNA, despite being ancient and widespread throughout life, represent a vulnerability, making cells more susceptible to both chemical alkylation and, of more general importance, reactive oxygen species. Sarkies et al take the innovative approach of introducing enzymatic genome-wide cytosine methylation system (DNA methyltransferases, DNMTs) into E. coli, which normally lacks such a system. They provide compelling evidence that the introduction of DNMTs increases the sensitivity of E. coli to chemical alkylation damage. Surprisingly they also show DNMTs increase the sensitivity to reactive oxygen species and propose that the DNMT generated 5mC presents a target for the reactive oxygen species that is especially damaging to cells. Evidence is presented that DNMT activity directly or indirectly produces reactive oxygen species in vivo, which is an important discovery if correct, though the mechanism for this remains obscure.

      Strengths:

      This work is based on an interesting initial premise, it is well-motivated in the introduction and the manuscript is clearly written. The results themselves are compelling.

      We thank the reviewer for their positive response to our study.  We also really appreciate the thoughtful comments raised.  We have addressed the comments raised as detailed below. 

      Weaknesses:

      I am not currently convinced by the principal interpretations and think that other explanations based on known phenomena could account for key results. Specific points below.

      (1) As noted in the manuscript, AlkB repairs alkylation damage by direct reversal (DNA strands are not cut). In the absence of AlkB, repair of alklylation damage/modification is likely through BER or other processes involving strand excision and resulting in single stranded DNA. It has previously been shown that 3mC modification from MMS exposure is highly specific to single stranded DNA (PMID:20663718) occurring at ~20,000 times the rate as double stranded DNA. Consequently, the introduction of DNMTs is expected to introduce many methylation adducts genome-wide that will generate single stranded DNA tracts when repaired in an AlkB deficient background (but not in an AlkB WT background), which are then hyper-susceptible to attack by MMS. Such ssDNA tracts are also vulnerable to generating double strand breaks, especially when they contain DNA polymerase stalling adducts such as 3mC. The generation of ssDNA during repair is similarly expected follow the H2O2 or TET based conversion of 5mC to 5hmC or 5fC neither of which can be directly repaired and depend on single strand excision for their removal. The potential importance of ssDNA generation in the experiments has not been considered.

      We thank the reviewer for this interesting and insightful suggestion.  Our interpretation of our findings is that a subset of MMS-induced DNA damage, specifically 3mC, overlaps with the damage introduced by DNMTs and this accounts for increased sensitivity to MMS when DNMTs are expressed.  However, the idea that the introduction of 3mC by DNMT actually makes the DNA more liable to damage by MMS, potentially through increasing the level of ssDNA, is also a potential explanation, which could operate in addition to the mechanism that we propose.

      (2) The authors emphasise the non-additivity of the MMS + DNMT + alkB experiment but the interpretation of the result is essentially an additive one: that both MMS and DNMT are introducing similar/same damage and AlkB acts to remove it. The non-additivity noted would seem to be more consistent with the ssDNA model proposed in #1. More generally non-additivity would also be seen if the survival to DNA methylation rate is non-linear over the range of the experiment, for example if there is a threshold effect where some repair process is overwhelmed. The linearity of MMS (and H2O2) exposure to survival could be directly tested with a dilution series of MMS (H2O2).

      We thank the reviewer for this point.  As in the response to point #1, the reviewer’s hypothesis of increased potency of MMS, potentially through increased ssDNA, downstream of 3mC induction by DNMT, is a good one.  We have added a dose-response curve for DNMT-expressing cells to MMS to the revised version of the manuscript.  This shows that there is a non-linear response to MMS in the WT background.  Sensitivity is exacerbated by expression of DNMT and alkB mutation individually but there is also a strong non-additive effect that is particularly marked at low MMS concentrations where sensitivity is much higher in the double mutant than predicted from the two single mutants.  This is consistent with induction of DNA damage by DNMT that is repaired by alkB because alkB can be ‘overwhelmed’ even in WT backgrounds as the reviewer suggests.  However, it is also perfectly possible that the effect is due to increased levels of DNA damage induction in DNMT-expressing cells.  Both these results are compatible with our central hypothesis, namely that DNMT expression induces 3mC.  We have included these results along with discussion of them in the revised text in the results section:

      In order to investigate the non-additivity between DNMT expression and alkB mutation further, we investigated the effect of MMS over a range of concentrations for the different strains (Supplemental Figure 1A).  We quantified the non-additivity by comparing between the survival of alkB expressing DNMT to the predicted combined effect of either alkB mutation alone or DNMT expression alone(Supplemental Figure 1B).  Significantly reduced survival than expected was observed, most notably at low concentrations of MMS, which could be due to the saturation of the effect at high concentrations of MMS for alkB mutants expressing DNMT, where extremely high levels of sensitivity were observed.  The non-linear shape of the graph observed for WT cells expressing DNMTs further suggests that the ability of AlkB to repair the DNA is overwhelmed at high MMS concentrations even in the WT background.  These results are consistent with the idea that AlkB repairs a form of DNA damage from MMS that is more prevalent when DNMT is expressed.  This could be because DNMT induces 3mC, repaired by AlkB, and further 3mC is induced by MMS leading to much higher 3mC levels in the absence of AlkB activity.  Alternatively, 3mC induction by DNMT may lead to increased levels of ssDNA, particularly in alkB mutants, which could increase the risk of further DNA damage by MMS exposure and heighten sensitivity.  Either of these mechanisms are consistent with induction of 3mC by DNMT, and  indicate that the induction of DNA damage by DNMT expression has a fitness cost for cells when exposed to genotoxic stress in their environment. 

      (3) The substantial transcriptional changes induced by DNMT expression (Supplemental Figure 4) are a cause for concern and highlight that the ectopic introduction of methylation into a complex system is potentially more confounded than it may at first seem. Though the expression analysis shows bulk transcription properties, my concern is that the disruptive influence of methylation in a system not evolved with it adds not just consistent transcriptional changes but transcriptional heterogeneity between cells which could influence net survival in a stressed environment. In practice I don't think this can be controlled for, possibly quantified by single-cell RNA-seq but that is beyond the reasonable scope of this paper.

      We fully agree with the reviewer and, indeed, we are very interested in what is driving the transcriptional changes that we observed.  Work is currently underway in the lab to investigate this further but, as the reviewer suggests, is beyond the scope of this paper.  Importantly, we have used the transcriptional data to determine that the effect of DNMTs on ROS is unlikely to be due to failure of ROS-induced detoxification mechanisms by investigating the expression of oxyR regulated genes.  Nevertheless we have explicitly mentioned the concern raised by the reviewer in the revised manuscript as follows:

      “The substantial transcriptional responses could potentially affect how individual cells respond to genotoxic stress and thus could be contributing to some of the excess sensitivity to MMS and H2O2 in cells expressing DNMTs. However, the induction of oxyR regulated genes such as catalase was unaffected by 5mC (Supplementary Figure 4B).  Thus, the increased sensitivity to H2O2 is unlikely to be caused by failure of detoxification gene induction by DNMT expression.”

      (4) Figure 4 represents a striking result. From its current presentation it could be inferred that DNMTs are actively promoting ROS generation from H2O2 and also to a lesser extent in the absence of exogenous H2O2. That would be very surprising and a major finding with far-reaching implications. It would need to be further validated, for example by in vitro reconstitution of the reaction and monitoring ROS production. Rather, I think the authors are proposing that some currently undefined, indirect consequence of DNMT activity promotes ROS generation, especially when exogenous H2O2 is available. It would help if this were clarified.

      We thank the reviewer for picking this up.  In the discussion, we raise two possible explanations for why DNMT (even without H2O2) increases the ROS levels.  One idea is direct activity of DNMT, and one is through the product of DNMT activity (5mC) acting as a platform to generate more ROS from endogenous or exogenous sources.  Whilst we attempted to measure ROS from mSSSI activity in vitro, this experiment gave inconsistent results and therefore we cannot distinguish between these two possibilities.  However, we argued that direct activity is less likely, exactly as the reviewer points out.  We have clarified our discussion in the revised version, rewriting the entire section titled

      Oxidative stress as a new source of DNA damage induction by DNMT expression to more clearly set out these possibilities. 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      5-methylcytosine (5mC) is a key epigenetic mark in DNA and plays a crucial role in regulating gene expression in many eukaryotes including humans. The DNA methyltransferases (DNMTs) that establish and maintain 5mC, are conserved in many species across eukaryotes, including animals, plants, and fungi, mainly in a CpG context. Interestingly, 5mC levels and distributions are quite variable across phylogenies with some species even appearing to have no such DNA methylation.

      This interesting and well-written paper discusses the continuation of some of the authors' work published several years ago. In that previous paper, the laboratory demonstrated that DNA methylation pathways coevolved with DNA repair mechanisms, specifically with the alkylation repair system. Specifically, they discovered that DNMTs can introduce alkylation damage into DNA, specifically in the form of 3-methylcytosine (3mC). (This appears to be an error in the DNMT enzymatic mechanism where the generation 3mC as opposed to its preferred product 5-methylcytosine (5mC), is caused by the flipped target cytosine binding to the active site pocket of the DNMT in an inverted orientation.) The presence of 3mC is potentially toxic and can cause replication stress, which this paper suggests may explain the loss of DNA methylation in different species. They further showed that the ALKB2 enzyme plays a crucial role in repairing this alkylation damage, further emphasizing the link between DNA methylation and DNA repair.

      The co-evolution of DNMTs with DNA repair mechanisms suggests there can be distinct advantages and disadvantages of DNA methylation to different species which might depend on their environmental niche. In environments that expose species to high levels of DNA damage, high levels of 5mC in their genome may be disadvantageous. This present paper sets out to examine the sensitivity of an organism to genotoxic stresses such as alkylation and oxidation agents as the consequence of DNMT activity. Since such a study in eukaryotes would be complicated by DNA methylation controlling gene regulation, these authors cleverly utilize Escherichia coli (E.coli) and incorporate into it the DNMTs from other bacteria that methylate the cytosines of DNA in a CpG context like that observed in eukaryotes; the active sites of these enzymes are very similar to eukaryotic DNMTs and basically utilize the same catalytic mechanism (also this strain of E.coli does not specifically degrade this methylated DNA) .

      The experiments in this paper more than adequately show that E. coli expression of these DNMTs (comparing to the same strain without the DNMTS) do indeed show increased sensitivity to alkylating agents and this sensitivity was even greater than expected when a DNA repair mechanism was inactivated. Moreover, they show that this E. coli expressing this DNMT is more sensitive to oxidizing agents such as H2O2 and has exacerbated sensitivity when a DNA repair glycosylase is inactivated. Both propensities suggest that DNMT activity itself may generate additional genotoxic stress. Intrigued that DNMT expression itself might induce sensitivity to oxidative stress, the experimenters used a fluorescent sensor to show that H2O2 induced reactive oxygen species (ROS) are markedly enhanced with DNMT expression. Importantly, they show that DNMT expression alone gave rise to increased ROS amounts and both H2O2 addition and DNMT expression has greater effect that the linear combination of the two separately. They also carefully checked that the increased sensitivity to H2O2 was not potentially caused by some effect on gene expression of detoxification genes by DNMT expression and activity. Finally, by using mass spectroscopy, they show that DNMT expression led to production of the 5mC oxidation derivatives 5-hydroxymethylcytosine (5hmC) and 5-formylcytosine (5fC) in DNA. 5fC is a substrate for base excision repair while 5hmC is not; more 5fC was observed. Introduction of non-bacterial enzymes that produce 5hmC and 5fC into the DNMT expressing bacteria again showed a greater sensitivity than expected. Remarkedly, in their assay with addition of H2O2, bacteria showed no growth with this dual expression of DNMT and these enzymes.

      Overall, the authors conduct well thought-out and simple experiments to show that a disadvantageous consequence of DNMT expression leading to 5mC in DNA is increased sensitivity to oxidative stress as well as alkylating agents.

      Again, the paper is well-written and organized. The hypotheses are well-examined by simple experiments. The results are interesting and can impact many scientific areas such as our understanding of evolutionary pressures on an organism by environment to impacting our understanding about how environment of a malignant cell in the human body may lead to cancer.

      We thank the reviewer for their response to our study, and value the time taken to produce a public review that will aid readers in understanding the key results of our study. 

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Krwawicz et al., present evidence that expression of DNMTs in E. coli results in (1) introduction of alkylation damage that is repaired by AlkB; (2) confers hypersensitivity to alkylating agents such as MMS (and exacerbated by loss of AlkB); (3) confers hypersensitivity to oxidative stress (H2O2 exposure); (4) results in a modest increase in ROS in the absence of exogenous H2O2 exposure; and (5) results in the production of oxidation products of 5mC, namely 5hmC and 5fC, leading to cellular toxicity. The findings reported here have interesting implications for the concept that such genotoxic and potentially mutagenic consequences of DNMT expression (resulting in 5mC) could be selectively disadvantageous for certain organisms. The other aspect of this work which is important for understanding the biological endpoints of genotoxic stress is the notion that DNA damage per se somehow induces elevated levels of ROS.

      Strengths:

      The manuscript is well-written, and the experiments have been carefully executed providing data that support the authors' proposed model presented in Fig. 7 (Discussion, sources of DNA damage due to DNMT expression).

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The authors have established an informative system relying on expression of DNMTs to gauge the effects of such expression and subsequent induction of 3mC and 5mC on cell survival and sensitivity to an alkylating agent (MMS) and exogenous oxidative stress (H2O2 exposure). The authors state (p4) that Fig. 2 shows that "Cells expressing either M.SssI or M.MpeI showed increased sensitivity to MMS treatment compared to WT C2523, supporting the conclusion that the expression of DNMTs increased the levels of alkylation damage." This is a confusing statement and requires revision as Fig. 2 does ALL cells shown in Fig. 2 are expressing DNMTs and have been treated with MMS. It is the absence of AlkB and the expression of DNMTs that that causes the MMS sensitivity.

      We thank the reviewer for this and agree that this needs to be clarified with regards to the figure presented and will do so in the revised manuscript. The key comparison is between the active and inactive mSSSI which shows increased sensitivity when active methyltransferases are expressed.  We have clarified this in the revised version of the manuscript as follows:

      “Cells expressing either M.SssI or M.MpeI showed increased sensitivity to MMS treatment compared to cells expressing inactive M.SssI”

      (2) It would be important to know whether the increased sensitivity (toxicity) to DNMT expression and MMS is also accompanied by substantial increases in mutagenicity. The authors should explain in the text why mutation frequencies were not also measured in these experiments.

      This is an important point because it is not immediately obvious that increased sensitivity would be associated with increased mutagenicity (if, for example, 3mC was never a cause of innacurate DNA repair even in the absence of AlkB).  We have now added a Rif resistance assay which demonstrates increased mutagenesis in the presence of DNMT, and that this is exacerbated by loss of AlkB. This is now added as supplemental figure 2 and described in the manuscript as follows:

      “One potential consequence of DNMT activity in inducing DNA damage might be increased mutagenesis.  To test this we performed a rifampicin resistance mutagenesis assay, in the absence of MMS, to test whether DNMT induced damage was sufficient to lead to mutation rate increase.  Mutation rate was increased by DNMT expression (p=1.6e-12; two way anova; Supplemental Figure 2) and alkB mutation (two way anova) separately (p<1e-16).  Moreover, there was a significant interaction such that combined alkB mutation and DNMT expression led to a further increased mutation rate compared to the expectation from alkB mutation and DNMT expression separately (p = 7.9e-10; Supplemental Figure 2).  Importantly, DNMT induction alone would be expected to lead to increased mutations due to cytosine deamination(Sarkies, 2022a); however, there is a synergistic effect on mutations when this is combined with loss of AlkB function in alkB mutants. This is consistent with 3mC induction by DNMTs which is repaired by AlkB in WT cells but leads to mutations in alkB mutant cells.

      (3) Materials and Methods. ROS production monitoring. The "Total Reactive Oxygen Species (ROS) Assay Kit" has not been adequately described. Who is the Vendor? What is the nature of the ROS probes employed in this assay? Which specific ROS correspond to "total ROS"?

      The ROS measurement was with a kit from ThermoFisher: https://www.thermofisher.com/order/catalog/product/88-5930-74.  The probe is DCFH-DA.  This is a general ROS sensor that is oxidised by a large number of cellular reactive oxygen species hence we cannot attribute the signal to a single species.  Use of a technique with the potential to more precisely identify the species involved is something we plan to do in future, but is beyond what we can do as part of this study.  We have added a comment as to the specificity of the ROS sensor in the revised version as follows:

      “The ROS detection reagent in this system is DCFH-DA, a generalised ROS sensor that is not specific to any particular ROS molecule.”     

      (4) The demonstration (Fig. 4) that DNMT expression results in elevated ROS and its further synergistic increase when cells are also exposed to H2O2 is the basis for the authors' discussion of DNA damage-induced increases in cellular ROS. S. cerevisiae does not possess DNMTs/5mC, yet exposure to MMS also results in substantial increases in intracellular ROS (Rowe et al, (2008) Free Rad. Biol. Med. 45:1167-1177. PMC2643028). The authors should be aware of previous studies that have linked DNA damage to intracellular increases in ROS in other organisms and should comment on this in the text.

      We thank the reviewer for this point.  We note that the increased ROS that we observed occur in the presence of DNMTs alone and in the presence of H2O2, not in the presence of MMS; however, the point that DNA damage in general can promote increased ROS in some circumstances is well taken.  We have included a comment on this in the revised version as follows:

      “We believe this is a plausible mechanism to explain both increased ROS and increased sensitivity to oxidative stress when DNMT is expressed.  However, other explanations are possible, and it is notable that DNA damaging agents such as MMS can lead to ROS generation(Rowe et al., 2008).  A more detailed chemical and kinetic study of the ROS formation in DNMT-expressing cells would be needed to resolve these questions.”

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In the current article, Octavia Soegyono and colleagues study "The influence of nucleus accumbens shell D1 and D2 neurons on outcome-specific Pavlovian instrumental transfer", building on extensive findings from the same lab. While there is a consensus about the specific involvement of the Shell part of the Nucleus Accumbens (NAc) in specific stimulus-based actions in choice settings (and not in General Pavlovian instrumental transfer - gPIT, as opposed to the Core part of the NAc), mechanisms at the cellular and circuitry levels remain to be explored. In the present work, using sophisticated methods (rat Cre-transgenic lines from both sexes, optogenetics, and the well-established behavioral paradigm outcome-specific PIT-sPIT), Octavia Soegyono and colleagues decipher the differential contribution of dopamine receptors D1 and D2 expressing spiny projection neurons (SPNs).

      After validating the viral strategy and the specificity of the targeting (immunochemistry and electrophysiology), the authors demonstrate that while both NAc Shell D1- and D2-SPNs participate in mediating sPIT, NAc Shell D1-SPNs projections to the Ventral Pallidum (VP, previously demonstrated as crucial for sPIT), but not D2-SPNs, mediates sPIT. They also show that these effects were specific to stimulus-based actions, as value-based choices were left intact in all manipulations.

      This is a well-designed study, and the results are well supported by the experimental evidence. The paper is extremely pleasant to read and adds to the current literature.

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive assessment.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript by Soegyono et al. describes a series of experiments designed to probe the involvement of dopamine D1 and D2 neurons within the nucleus accumbens shell in outcome-specific Pavlovian-instrumental transfer (osPIT), a well-controlled assay of cue-guided action selection based on congruent outcome associations. They used an optogenetic approach to phasically silence NAc shell D1 (D1-Cre mice) or D2 (A2a-Cre mice) neurons during a subset of osPIT trials. Both manipulations disrupted cue-guided action selection but had no effects on negative control measures/tasks (concomitant approach behavior, separate valued guided choice task), nor were any osPIT impairments found in reporter-only control groups. Separate experiments revealed that selective inhibition of NAc shell D1 but not D2 inputs to ventral pallidum was required for osPIT expression, thereby advancing understanding of the basal ganglia circuitry underpinning this important aspect of decision making.

      Strengths:

      The combinatorial viral and optogenetic approaches used here were convincingly validated through anatomical tract-tracing and ex vivo electrophysiology. The behavioral assays are sophisticated and well-controlled to parse cue and value-guided action selection. The inclusion of reporter-only control groups is rigorous and rules out nonspecific effects of the light manipulation. The findings are novel and address a critical question in the literature. Prior work using less decisive methods had implicated NAc shell D1 neurons in osPIT but suggested that D2 neurons may not be involved. The optogenetic manipulations used in the current study provide a more direct test of their involvement and convincingly demonstrate that both populations play an important role. Prior work had also implicated NAc shell connections to ventral pallidum in osPIT, but the current study reveals the selective involvement of D1 but not D2 neurons in this circuit. The authors do a good job of discussing their findings, including their nuanced interpretation that NAc shell D2 neurons may contribute to osPIT through their local regulation of NAc shell microcircuitry.

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive assessment.

      Weaknesses:

      The current study exclusively used an optogenetic approach to probe the function of D1 and D2 NAc shell neurons. Providing a complementary assessment with chemogenetics or other appropriate methods would strengthen conclusions, particularly the novel demonstration of D2 NAc shell involvement. Likewise, the null result of optically inhibiting D2 inputs to the ventral pallidum leaves open the possibility that a more complete or sustained disruption of this pathway may have impaired osPIT.

      We acknowledge the reviewer's valuable suggestion that demonstrating NAc-S D1- and D2-SPN engagement in outcome-specific PIT through another technique would strengthen our optogenetic findings. Several approaches could provide this validation. Chemogenetic manipulation, as the reviewer suggested, represents one compelling option. Alternatively, immunohistochemical assessment of phosphorylated histone H3 at serine 10 (P-H3) offers another promising avenue, given its established utility in reporting striatal SPN plasticity in the dorsal striatum (Matamales et al., 2020). We hope to complete such an assessment in future work since it would address the limitations of previous work that relied solely on ERK1/2 phosphorylation measures in NAc-S SPNs (Laurent et al., 2014).

      Regarding the null result from optical silencing of D2 terminals in the ventral pallidum, we agree with the reviewer's assessment. While we acknowledge this limitation in the current manuscript (see discussion), we aim to address this gap in future studies to provide a more complete mechanistic understanding of the circuit.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors present data demonstrating that optogenetic inhibition of either D1- or D2-MSNs in the NAc Shell attenuates expression of sensory-specific PIT while largely sparing value-based decision on an instrumental task. They also provide evidence that SS-PIT depends on D1-MSN projections from the NAc-Shell to the VP, whereas projections from D2-MSNs to the VP do not contribute to SS-PIT.

      Strengths:

      This is clearly written. The evidence largely supports the authors' interpretations, and these effects are somewhat novel, so they help advance our understanding of PIT and NAc-Shell function.

      We thank the Reviewer for their positive assessment.

      Weaknesses:

      I think the interpretation of some of the effects (specifically the claim that D1-MSNs do not contribute to value-based decision making) is not fully supported by the data presented.

      We appreciate the reviewer's comment regarding the marginal attenuation of value-based choice observed following NAc-S D1-SPN silencing. While this manipulation did produce a slight reduction in choice performance, the behavior remained largely intact. We are hesitant to interpret this marginal effect as evidence for a direct role of NAc-S D1-SPNs in value-based decision-making, particularly given the substantial literature demonstrating that NAc-S manipulations typically preserve such choice behavior (Corbit & Balleine, 2011; Corbit et al., 2001; Laurent et al., 2012). Notably, previous work has shown that NAc-S D1 receptor blockade impairs outcome-specific PIT while leaving value-based choice unaffected (Laurent et al., 2014). We favor an alternative explanation for our observed marginal reduction. As documented in Supplemental Figure 1, viral transduction extended slightly into the nucleus accumbens core (NAc-C), a region established as critical for value-based decision-making (Corbit & Balleine, 2011; Corbit et al., 2001; Laurent et al., 2012). The marginal impairment may therefore reflect inadvertent silencing of a small NAc-C D1-SPN population rather than a functional contribution from NAc-S D1-SPNs. Future studies specifically targeting larger NAc-C D1-SPN populations would help clarify this possibility and provide definitive resolution of this question.

    1. Author Response:

      Reviewer #1( Public review):

      The reviewer raised two main concerns: the potential confound between XOR and motor coding, and the relationship between neural coding and behaviour.

      First, we appreciate the consideration of the collinearity between the XOR and motor dimensions. We fully agree that this confound may have contributed to the observed increase in XOR decoding over the course of learning. In response, we will merge the XOR and motor features in the main figures, tone down our interpretation of the XOR learning effect, and clarify how motor signals may obscure or mimic XOR-related changes. As the reviewer noted, this confound does not affect the colour/context cross-generalisation analyses, which remain central to our conclusions regarding flexible and prospective working memory coding.

      We also thank the reviewer for the suggestion to examine the behavioural relevance of the neural representations more directly. We agree entirely, and will incorporate new analyses relating coding strength to reaction times, as well as reflect on the implications of these results in the revised Discussion.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The reviewer rightly noted that our manuscript overlooks the established concept of retrospective/prospective coding in working memory, giving the impression that we attempted to reframe it using newer machine learning terminology. We thank the reviewer for catching this important omission. Our intention was not to override this well-established conceptual framework with a newer machine learning term, but rather to build upon it. In fact, prospective coding and the idea of working memory as a resource for computation are closely related—one helps define the functions (prospective and retrospective coding) and the other explains the computational rationale behind applying them. For example, prospective codes specify what is being stored (future-relevant information), while the “memory-as-computation” view addresses why such representation is useful: to enable temporal decomposition of complex tasks and reduce computational load at decision time. We will revise the relevant paragraphs to explicitly reference this cognitive framework and clarify how it relates to — and is complemented by — the newer computational perspective we introduce. Thank you again for highlighting this.

      Reviewer 2 also argues that the evidence presented does not support dimensionality reduction, noting that participants likely transition from processing the sensory cue (e.g., blue) to a rule-based representation (e.g., context 1 vs context 2) later in the trial, and that this remapping does not inherently require dimensionality reduction. We agree that our results are consistent with such a transformation into an abstract rule representation during the delay period, as supported by the observed cross- colour context generalisation (Figure 3b) and that this process does not require dimensionality reduction per se. However, we would like to clarify that a shared decision boundary between two colour pairs (e.g., context 1 vs context 2) can manifest in two types of neural geometries. In one case — observed in our data — the irrelevant colour dimension is not maintained after the presentation period, such that blue and pink are maintained as context 1 but variance along the blues vs pink dimension is not represented in neural activity. In the other case, it is possible for the same abstract rule (context 1) to be constructed while maintaining the sensory representation of colour (e.g., “blue” or “pink”), resulting in a change in representational geometry without a reduction in dimensionality. Our data do not support the latter scenario: irrelevant colour information is not maintained in the delay period, suggesting that the abstraction is accompanied by a loss of variance along irrelevant sensory dimensions—i.e., a form of dimensionality reduction. We will clarify this point in the revised manuscript and include a new analysis that explicitly tests whether shattering dimensionality changes as a function of trial time.

      The reviewer also raised concerns about inconsistencies in our terminology, particularly the use of “colour pair” and “irrelevant colour.” We agree with the reviewer that the term “colour pair” was a conceptual device rather than a literal aspect of the task, and we will revise the text to make this clear. We recognise that our wording around “irrelevant colour” might have caused confusion. We did not mean “colour” in the broad sense of all colour processing, but rather referred to specific colour dimensions that are not relevant for task performance—for example, when context 1 is cued by both pink and blue, the dimension carrying variance between blue and pink can be considered irrelevant. We will clarify this point in the revised manuscript, using the reviewer’s suggestion to incorporate the description we had already provided in the Methods section.

      While we respectfully disagree with the reviewer’s interpretation of our findings—particularly regarding the absence of dimensionality reduction, which they associate with the failure of the direct test of cross-colour context decoding (see Fig. 3b, which shows a significant effect)—we appreciate the opportunity to clarify our position and will revise the manuscript to ensure our reasoning is as transparent and rigorous as possible.

      Reviewer #3 (PublIc review):

      The reviewer values the study’s demonstration that learning promotes abstraction in task representations, but raises concerns about the lack of direct evidence linking delay-period activity to specific working memory mechanisms and the ambiguous dissociation between XOR and motor representations. We thank the reviewer for their careful reading of the manuscript and will address both concerns in the revised version. As mentioned in our response to Reviewer #1, we will merge the motor and XOR analyses, tone down our interpretations, and clarify why these signals are entangled. Additionally, we will link delay-period neural activity to behavioural performance to establish a more direct connection to working memory processes. Notably, in Figure 4f, we show that early in learning, participants who exhibit stronger cross-generalisation of context during the delay are also more likely to exhibit decreased shattering dimensionality at decision time — providing an early link between the preparation of a contextual signal and the subsequent reduction in computational complexity at decision time. We will include additional analyses to further strengthen this link in the revised manuscript.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Measurement of BOLD MR imaging has regularly found regions of the brain that show reliable suppression of BOLD responses during specific experimental testing conditions. These observations are to some degree unexplained, in comparison with more usual association between activation of the BOLD response and excitatory activation of the neurons (most tightly linked to synaptic activity) in the same brain location. This paper finds two patients whose brains were tested with both non-invasive functional MRI and with invasive insertion of electrodes, which allowed the direct recording of neuronal activity. The electrode insertions were made within the fusiform gyrus, which is known to process information about faces, in a clinical search for the sites of intractable epilepsy in each patient. The simple observation is that the electrode location in one patient showed activation of the BOLD response and activation of neuronal firing in response to face stimuli. This is the classical association. The other patient showed an informative and different pattern of responses. In this person, the electrode location showed a suppression of the BOLD response to face stimuli and, most interestingly, an associated suppression of neuronal activity at the electrode site.

      Strengths:

      Whilst these results are not by themselves definitive, they add an important piece of evidence to a long-standing discussion about the origins of the BOLD response. The observation of decreased neuronal activation associated with negative BOLD is interesting because, at various times, exactly the opposite association has been predicted. It has been previously argued that if synaptic mechanisms of neuronal inhibition are responsible for the suppression of neuronal firing, then it would be reasonable

      Weaknesses:

      The chief weakness of the paper is that the results may be unique in a slightly awkward way. The observation of positive BOLD and neuronal activation is made at one brain site in one patient, while the complementary observation of negative BOLD and neuronal suppression actually derives from the other patient. Showing both effects in both patients would make a much stronger paper.

      We thank reviewer #1 for their positive evaluation of our paper. Obviously, we agree with the reviewer that the paper would be much stronger if BOTH effects – spike increase and decrease – would be found in BOTH patients in their corresponding fMRI regions (lateral and medial fusiform gyrus) (also in the same hemisphere). Nevertheless, we clearly acknowledge this limitation in the (revised) version of the manuscript (p.8: Material and Methods section).

      Note that with respect to the fMRI data, our results are not surprising, as we indicate in the manuscript: BOLD increases to faces (relative to nonface objects) are typically found in the LatFG and BOLD decreases in the medialFG (in the revised version, we have added the reference to an early neuroimaging paper that describes this dissociation clearly:

      Pelphrey, K. A., Mack, P. B., Song, A., Güzeldere, G., & McCarthy, G. Faces evoke spatially differentiated patterns of BOLD activation and deactivation. Neuroreport 14, 955–959 (2003).

      This pattern of increase/decrease in fMRI can be appreciated in both patients on Figure 2, although one has to consider both the transverse and coronal slices to appreciate it.

      Regarding electrophysiological data, in the current paper, one could think that P1 shows only increases to faces, and P2 would show only decreases (irrespective of the region). However, that is not the case since 11% of P1’s face-selective units are decreases (89% are increases) and 4% of P2’s face-selective units are increases. This has now been made clearer in the revised manuscript (p.5).

      As the reviewer is certainly aware, the number and positions of the electrodes are based on strict clinical criteria, and we will probably never encounter a situation with two neighboring (macro-micro hybrid electrodes), one with microelectrodes ending up in the lateral MidFG, the other in the medial MidFG, in the same patient. If there is no clinical value for the patient, this cannot be done.

      The only thing we can do is to strengthen these results in the future by collecting data on additional patients with an electrode either in the lateral or the medial FG, together with fMRI. But these are the only two patients we have been able to record so far with electrodes falling unambiguously in such contrasted regions and with large (and comparable) measures.

      While we acknowledge that the results may be unique because of the use of 2 contrasted patients only (and this is why the paper is a short report), the data is compelling in these 2 cases, and we are confident that it will be replicated in larger cohorts in the future.

      Finally, information regarding ethics approval has been provided in the paper.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This is a short and straightforward paper describing BOLD fMRI and depth electrode measurements from two regions of the fusiform gyrus that show either higher or lower BOLD responses to faces vs. objects (which I will call face-positive and facenegative regions). In these regions, which were studied separately in two patients undergoing epilepsy surgery, spiking activity increased for faces relative to objects in the face-positive region and decreased for faces relative to objects in the face-negative region. Interestingly, about 30% of neurons in the face-negative region did not respond to objects and decreased their responses below baseline in response to faces (absolute suppression).

      Strengths:

      These patient data are valuable, with many recording sessions and neurons from human face-selective regions, and the methods used for comparing face and object responses in both fMRI and electrode recordings were robust and well-established. The finding of absolute suppression could clarify the nature of face selectivity in human fusiform gyrus since previous fMRI studies of the face-negative region could not distinguish whether face < object responses came from absolute suppression, or just relatively lower but still positive responses to faces vs. objects.

      Weaknesses:

      The authors claim that the results tell us about both 1) face-selectivity in the fusiform gyrus, and 2) the physiological basis of the BOLD signal. However, I would like to see more of the data that supports the first claim, and I am not sure the second claim is supported.

      (1) The authors report that ~30% of neurons showed absolute suppression, but those data are not shown separately from the neurons that only show relative reductions. It is difficult to evaluate the absolute suppression claim from the short assertion in the text alone (lines 105-106), although this is a critical claim in the paper.

      We thank reviewer #2 for their positive evaluation of our paper. We understand the reviewer’s point, and we partly agree. Where we respectfully disagree is that the finding of absolute suppression is critical for the claim of the paper: finding an identical contrast between the two regions in terms of RELATIVE increase/decrease of face-selective activity in fMRI and spiking activity is already novel and informative. Where we agree with the reviewer is that the absolute suppression could be more documented: it wasn’t, due to space constraints (brief report). We provide below an example of a neuron showing absolute suppression to faces (P2), as also requested in the recommendations to authors. In the frequency domain, there is only a face-selective response (1.2 Hz and harmonics) but no significant response at 6 Hz (common general visual response). In the time-domain, relative to face onset, the response drops below baseline level. It means that this neuron has baseline (non-periodic) spontaneous spiking activity that is actively suppressed when a face appears.

      Author response image 1.

      (2) I am not sure how much light the results shed on the physiological basis of the BOLD signal. The authors write that the results reveal "that BOLD decreases can be due to relative, but also absolute, spike suppression in the human brain" (line 120). But I think to make this claim, you would need a region that exclusively had neurons showing absolute suppression, not a region with a mix of neurons, some showing absolute suppression and some showing relative suppression, as here. The responses of both groups of neurons contribute to the measured BOLD signal, so it seems impossible to tell from these data how absolute suppression per se drives the BOLD response.

      It is a fact that we find both kinds of responses in the same region. We cannot tell with this technique if neurons showing relative vs. absolute suppression of responses are spatially segregated for instance (e.g., forming two separate sub-regions) or are intermingled. And we cannot tell from our data how absolute suppression per se drives the BOLD response. In our view, this does not diminish the interest and originality of the study, but the statement "that BOLD decreases can be due to relative, but also absolute, spike suppression in the human brain” has been rephrased in the revised manuscript: "that BOLD decreases can be due to relative, or absolute (or a combination of both), spike suppression in the human brain”.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In this paper the authors conduct two experiments an fMRI experiment and intracranial recordings of neurons in two patients P1 and P2. In both experiments, they employ a SSVEP paradigm in which they show images at a fast rate (e.g. 6Hz) and then they show face images at a slower rate (e.g. 1.2Hz), where the rest of the images are a variety of object images. In the first patient, they record from neurons over a region in the mid fusiform gyrus that is face-selective and in the second patient, they record neurons from a region more medially that is not face selective (it responds more strongly to objects than faces). Results find similar selectivity between the electrophysiology data and the fMRI data in that the location which shows higher fMRI to faces also finds face-selective neurons and the location which finds preference to non faces also shows non face preferring neurons.

      Strengths:

      The data is important in that it shows that there is a relationship between category selectivity measured from electrophysiology data and category-selective from fMRI. The data is unique as it contains a lot of single and multiunit recordings (245 units) from the human fusiform gyrus - which the authors point out - is a humanoid specific gyrus.

      Weaknesses:

      My major concerns are two-fold:

      (i) There is a paucity of data; Thus, more information (results and methods) is warranted; and in particular there is no comparison between the fMRI data and the SEEG data.

      We thank reviewer #3 for their positive evaluation of our paper. If the reviewer means paucity of data presentation, we agree and we provide more presentation below, although the methods and results information appear as complete to us. The comparison between fMRI and SEEG is there, but can only be indirect (i.e., collected at different times and not related on a trial-by-trial basis for instance). In addition, our manuscript aims at providing a short empirical contribution to further our understanding of the relationship between neural responses and BOLD signal, not to provide a model of neurovascular coupling.

      (ii) One main claim of the paper is that there is evidence for suppressed responses to faces in the non-face selective region. That is, the reduction in activation to faces in the non-face selective region is interpreted as a suppression in the neural response and consequently the reduction in fMRI signal is interpreted as suppression. However, the SSVEP paradigm has no baseline (it alternates between faces and objects) and therefore it cannot distinguish between lower firing rate to faces vs suppression of response to faces.

      We understand the concern of the reviewer, but we respectfully disagree that our paradigm cannot distinguish between lower firing rate to faces vs. suppression of response to faces. Indeed, since the stimuli are presented periodically (6 Hz), we can objectively distinguish stimulus-related activity from spontaneous neuronal firing. The baseline corresponds to spikes that are non-periodic, i.e., unrelated to the (common face and object) stimulation. For a subset of neurons, even this non-periodic baseline activity is suppressed, above and beyond the suppression of the 6 Hz response illustrated on Figure 2. We mention it in the manuscript, but we agree that we do not present illustrations of such decrease in the time-domain for SU, which we did not consider as being necessary initially (please see below for such presentation).

      (1) Additional data: the paper has 2 figures: figure 1 which shows the experimental design and figure 2 which presents data, the latter shows one example neuron raster plot from each patient and group average neural data from each patient. In this reader's opinion this is insufficient data to support the conclusions of the paper. The paper will be more impactful if the researchers would report the data more comprehensively.

      We answer to more specific requests for additional evidence below, but the reviewer should be aware that this is a short report, which reaches the word limit. In our view, the group average neural data should be sufficient to support the conclusions, and the example neurons are there for illustration. And while we cannot provide the raster plots for a large number of neurons, the anonymized data is made available at:

      (a) There is no direct comparison between the fMRI data and the SEEG data, except for a comparison of the location of the electrodes relative to the statistical parametric map generated from a contrast (Fig 2a,d). It will be helpful to build a model linking between the neural responses to the voxel response in the same location - i.e., estimate from the electrophysiology data the fMRI data (e.g., Logothetis & Wandell, 2004).

      As mentioned above the comparison between fMRI and SEEG is indirect (i.e., collected at different times and not related on a trial-by-trial basis for instance) and would not allow to make such a model.

      (b) More comprehensive analyses of the SSVEP neural data: It will be helpful to show the results of the frequency analyses of the SSVEP data for all neurons to show that there are significant visual responses and significant face responses. It will be also useful to compare and quantify the magnitude of the face responses compared to the visual responses.

      The data has been analyzed comprehensively, but we would not be able to show all neurons with such significant visual responses and face-selective responses.

      (c) The neuron shown in E shows cyclical responses tied to the onset of the stimuli, is this the visual response?

      Correct, it’s the visual response at 6 Hz.

      If so, why is there an increase in the firing rate of the neuron before the face stimulus is shown in time 0?

      Because the stimulation is continuous. What is displayed at 0 is the onset of the face stimulus, with each face stimulus being preceded by 4 images of nonface objects.

      The neuron's data seems different than the average response across neurons; This raises a concern about interpreting the average response across neurons in panel F which seems different than the single neuron responses

      The reviewer is correct, and we apologize for the confusion. This is because the average data on panel F has been notch-filtered for the 6 Hz (and harmonic responses), as indicated in the methods (p.11): ‘a FFT notch filter (filter width = 0.05 Hz) was then applied on the 70 s single or multi-units time-series to remove the general visual response at 6 Hz and two additional harmonics (i.e., 12 and 18 Hz)’.

      Here is the same data without the notch-filter (the 6Hz periodic response is clearly visible):

      Author response image 2.

      For sake of clarity, we prefer presenting the notch-filtered data in the paper, but the revised version makes it clear in the figure caption that the average data has been notch-filtered.

      (d) Related to (c) it would be useful to show raster plots of all neurons and quantify if the neural responses within a region are homogeneous or heterogeneous. This would add data relating the single neuron response to the population responses measured from fMRI. See also Nir 2009.

      We agree with the reviewer that this is interesting, but again we do not think that it is necessary for the point made in the present paper. Responses in these regions appear rather heterogenous, and we are currently working on a longer paper with additional SEEG data (other patients tested for shorter sessions) to define and quantify the face-selective neurons in the MidFusiform gyrus with this approach (without relating it to the fMRI contrast as reported here).

      (e) When reporting group average data (e.g., Fig 2C,F) it is necessary to show standard deviation of the response across neurons.

      We agree with the reviewer and have modified Figure 2 accordingly in the revised manuscript.

      (f) Is it possible to estimate the latency of the neural responses to face and object images from the phase data? If so, this will add important information on the timing of neural responses in the human fusiform gyrus to face and object images.

      The fast periodic paradigm to measure neural face-selectivity has been used in tens of studies since its original reports:

      In this paradigm, the face-selective response spreads to several harmonics (1.2 Hz, 2.4 Hz, 3.6 Hz, etc.) (which are summed for quantifying the total face-selective amplitude). This is illustrated below by the averaged single units’ SNR spectra across all recording sessions for both participants.

      Author response image 3.

      There is no unique phase-value, each harmonic being associated with a phase-value, so that the timing cannot be unambiguously extracted from phase values. Instead, the onset latency is computed directly from the time-domain responses, which is more straightforward and reliable than using the phase. Note that the present paper is not about the specific time-courses of the different types of neurons, which would require a more comprehensive report, but which is not necessary to support the point made in the present paper about the SEEG-fMRI sign relationship.

      (g) Related to (e) In total the authors recorded data from 245 units (some single units and some multiunits) and they found that both in the face and nonface selective most of the recoded neurons exhibited face -selectivity, which this reader found confusing: They write “ Among all visually responsive neurons, we found a very high proportion of face-selective neurons (p < 0.05) in both activated and deactivated MidFG regions (P1: 98.1%; N = 51/52; P2: 86.6%; N = 110/127)’. Is the face selectivity in P1 an increase in response to faces and P2 a reduction in response to faces or in both it’s an increase in response to faces

      Face-selectivity is defined as a DIFFERENTIAL response to faces compared to objects, not necessarily a larger response to faces. So yes, face-selectivity in P1 is an increase in response to faces and P2 a reduction in response to faces.

      Additional methods

      (a) it is unclear if the SSVEP analyses of neural responses were done on the spikes or the raw electrical signal. If the former, how is the SSVEP frequency analysis done on discrete data like action potentials?

      The FFT is applied directly on spike trains using Matlab’s discrete Fourier Transform function. This function is suitable to be applied to spike trains in the same way as to any sampled digital signal (here, the microwires signal was sampled at 30 kHz, see Methods).

      In complementary analyses, we also attempted to apply the FFT on spike trains that had been temporally smoothed by convolving them with a 20ms square window (Le Cam et al., 2023, cited in the paper ). This did not change the outcome of the frequency analyses in the frequency range we are interested in. We have also added one sentence with information in the methods section about spike detection (p.10).

      (b) it is unclear why the onset time was shifted by 33ms; one can measure the phase of the response relative to the cycle onset and use that to estimate the delay between the onset of a stimulus and the onset of the response. Adding phase information will be useful.

      The onset time was shifted by 33ms because the stimuli are presented with a sinewave contrast modulation (i.e., at 0ms, the stimulus has 0% contrast). 100% contrast is reached at half a stimulation cycle, which is 83.33ms here, but a response is likely triggered before reaching 100% contrast. To estimate the delay between the start of the sinewave (0% contrast) and the triggering of a neural response, we tested 7 SEEG participants with the same images presented in FPVS sequences either as a sinewave contrast (black line) modulation or as a squarewave (i.e. abrupt) contrast modulation (red line). The 33ms value is based on these LFP data obtained in response to such sinewave stimulation and squarewave stimulation of the same paradigm. This delay corresponds to 4 screen refresh frames (120 Hz refresh rate = 8.33ms by frame) and 35% of the full contrast, as illustrated below (please see also Retter, T. L., & Rossion, B. (2016). Uncovering the neural magnitude and spatio-temporal dynamics of natural image categorization in a fast visual stream. Neuropsychologia, 91, 9–28).

      Author response image 4.

      (2) Interpretation of suppression:

      The SSVEP paradigm alternates between 2 conditions: faces and objects and has no baseline; In other words, responses to faces are measured relative to the baseline response to objects so that any region that contains neurons that have a lower firing rate to faces than objects is bound to show a lower response in the SSVEP signal. Therefore, because the experiment does not have a true baseline (e.g. blank screen, with no visual stimulation) this experimental design cannot distinguish between lower firing rate to faces vs suppression of response to faces.

      The strongest evidence put forward for suppression is the response of non-visual neurons that was also reduced when patients looked at faces, but since these are non-visual neurons, it is unclear how to interpret the responses to faces.

      We understand this point, but how does the reviewer know that these are non-visual neurons? Because these neurons are located in the visual cortex, they are likely to be visual neurons that are not responsive to non-face objects. In any case, as the reviewer writes, we think it’s strong evidence for suppression.

      We thank all three reviewers for their positive evaluation of our paper and their constructive comments.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      We thank the Reviewers and the Editor for their thoughtful and constructive feedback. In the revised manuscript, we have addressed all comments thoroughly and made several substantial improvements:

      ● Benchmarking against state-of-the-art methods: We now provide a detailed comparison of our method, PGBAR, with MLspike and CASCADE using our cerebellar dataset recorded at high sampling rates. This comparison demonstrates that PGBAR offers more reliable spike time estimates with significantly lower variability in temporal accuracy (Figure 9).

      ● Quantitative analyses: We replaced qualitative statements with quantitative metrics. For example, we now report Pearson’s correlation (>0.95) of spike probabilities across trials and 100% of posterior samples with correct spike number detection during low SNR conditions (Figures 7 and 8).

      ● Clarified modeling rationale: We elaborated on the motivation behind modeling bursting dynamics using a hidden two-state process, which helps mitigate bias in spike detection under non-stationary firing conditions.

      ● Model identifiability and robustness: We demonstrate that our approach avoids parameter degeneracy through careful model design and parameter reparameterization. Sensitivity analyses (Figure 10) show that PGBAR is more robust to hyperparameter variation than MLspike.

      ● Improved clarity and accessibility: We revised the Introduction and Results sections to better explain the context, goals, and implications of our method, and clarified the advantages of joint parameter and state inference within our Bayesian framework.

      We believe that these additions significantly strengthen our manuscript and demonstrate the utility of PGBAR for high-temporal-precision spike inference. Please find below our detailed responses to both Public Reviews and Recommendations for the authors.

      Public Reviews

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      In this study, Diana et al. present a Monte Carlo-based method to perform spike inference from calcium imaging data. A particular strength of their approach is that they can estimate not only averages but also uncertainties of the modelled process. The authors then focus on the quantification of spike time uncertainties in simulated data and in data recorded with a high sampling rate in cerebellar slices with GCaMP8f.

      Strengths:

      - The authors provide a solid groundwork for sequential Monte Carlo-based spike inference, which extends previous work of Pnevmatikakis et al., Greenberg et al., and others.

      - The integration of two states (silence vs. burst firing) seems to improve the performance of the model.

      - The acquisition of a GCaMP8f dataset in the cerebellum is useful and helps make the point that high spike time inference precision is possible under certain conditions.

      Weaknesses:

      - The algorithm is designed to predict single spike times. Currently, it is not benchmarked against other algorithms in terms of single spike precision and spike time errors. A benchmarking with the most recent other SMC model and another good model focused on single spike outputs (e.g., MLSpike) would be useful to have.

      We thank the reviewer for the observation. In our revised manuscript, we have included a detailed comparison of spike time accuracy between our method, MLspike, and the supervised method, CASCADE, now summarized in Figure 9. In this analysis, we used our in vitro dataset to estimate the average temporal accuracy of spike detection across the three methods. As discussed in the main text, the average temporal accuracy was defined as the time difference between ground truth and the nearest detected spikes averaged across the ground truth. The distributions of temporal accuracies across our experiments obtained from MLspike, Cascade, and PGBAR differ in their spread, with 10th-to-90th percentile ranges of 14 ms, 8 ms, and 3 ms, respectively. This result demonstrates that PGBAR spike time estimates are more reliable than MLspike and CASCADE across trials, with a narrower unbiased distribution of temporal accuracy. 

      A direct comparison of PGBAR with the Sequential Binding Model (SBM) developed by Greenberg et al. was not possible since the biophysical model is designed around early GCaMP variants and thus not suitable for inference with our GCaMP8f dataset. We generally agree that employing realistic models of the calcium indicator can improve inference, however, PGBAR responds to a different question, namely how to simultaneously infer spike times and model parameters, which was still an issue with the SBM approach. 

      Some of the analyses and benchmarks seem too cursory, and the reporting simply consists of a visual impression of results instead of proper analysis and quantification. For example, the authors write "The spike patterns obtained using our method are very similar across trials, showing that PGBAR can reliably detect single-trial action potential-evoked GCaMP8f fluorescence transients." This is a highly qualitative statement, just based on the (subjective) visual impression of a plot. Similarly, the authors write "we could reliably identify the two spikes in each trial", but this claim is not supported by quantification or a figure, as far as I can see. 

      We thank the reviewer for this remark. We have now justified quantitatively our statement regarding the similarity across trials. In the revised preprint, we explain that in the specific experiment illustrated in Figure 7, Pearson’s pairwise correlation between spike probabilities (Gaussian filtered with 20 ms bandwidth) across trials is always larger than 0.95. The statement quoted by the reviewer, "we could reliably identify the two spikes in each trial" refers to the fact that in 100% of the posterior samples, generated from the analysis of each trial, we detected 2 spikes in the time window considered. The temporal accuracy of our detection was then illustrated for all trials in Figure 7H, where we compared the posterior distribution of the inter-spike interval between the first two spikes across trials. 

      The statement referred by the Reviewer has been revised to read

      (line 319) “The Pearson’s pairwise correlation between spike probabilities (Gaussian filtered with 20 ms bandwidth) across trials is always larger than 0.95, which demonstrates that PGBAR provides robust predictions across trials and it can reliably detect single-trial action potential-evoked GCaMP8f fluorescence transients.”

      We revised the second statement as:

      (line 324) “Despite the relatively low SNR, 100% of the posterior samples contained two spikes in the considered time interval.” 

      The authors write "but the trade-off between temporal accuracy, SNR and sampling frequency must be considered", but they don't discuss these trade-offs systematically.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We have now removed the quoted sentence in the updated preprint. We revised this statement to read: 

      (line 302) “Based on this analysis we expect PGBAR to provide accurate estimates of inter-spike intervals down to 5 ms.”

      It has been shown several times from experimental data that spike inference with single spike resolution does not work well (Huang et al. eLife, 2021; Rupprecht et al., Nature Neuroscience, 2021) in general. This limitation should be discussed with respect to the applicability of the proposed algorithm for standard population calcium imaging data.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. Detecting single spike times is indeed a difficult task. Compared to previous methods for single spike estimation, the advantage of our statistical approach is the rigorous analysis of uncertainties propagated by unknown model parameters and noisy recordings. This is an important aspect that was missing in previous approaches and that we were able to address thanks to our fully probabilistic approach. 

      Several analyses are based on artificial, simulated data with simplifying assumptions. Ever since Theis et al., Neuron, 2016, it has been known that artificially generated ground truth data should not be used as the primary means to evaluate spike inference algorithms. It would have been informative if the authors had used either the CASCADE dataset or their cerebellum dataset for more detailed analyses, in particular of single spike time precision.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. 

      To address the reviewer’s concern about single spike time precision, we have added to our revised preprint a further comparison between the temporal accuracy of PGBAR, CASCADE, and MLspike for our cerebellar dataset (Fig. 9, already discussed above). 

      Nevertheless, as pointed out by the reviewer, simulated data should not be used as the primary means to evaluate the performance of an inference algorithm. However, it is standard practice in the field of model-based inference to validate the approach first with data generated by the same model used for inference. This step is usually done for two main reasons: first, for internal consistency of the method, and second, to explore the regimes where inference is achievable. We made use of simulated data to address specific questions. Specifically, in Figure 2, we illustrate the analysis of data simulated using the same model for inference. In Figure 3, we used simulated data to highlight the importance of modeling bursting activity to avoid biases induced by non-homogeneous firing rates. In Figure 6, we used simulated data to explore the theoretical accuracy of PGBAR under different conditions of signal-to-noise ratio and acquisition frequencies.

      In its current state, the sum of the current weaknesses makes the suggested method, while interesting for experts, rather unattractive for experimentalists who want to perform spike inference on their recorded calcium imaging data.

      In our preprint, we illustrated the application of PGBAR to benchmark data and our cerebellar recordings. Therefore, our approach can be part of the calcium imaging data analysis pipeline. The advantage of estimating statistical uncertainties and model parameters makes PGBAR an attractive tool for the wide neuroscience community interested in spike inference and statistical accuracy. In addition, as noted by Reviewer 2, our code is well documented. User-friendliness and integrating our method within GUI analysis software might be the next step if there is increasing interest in using this method.

      Other comments:

      One of the key features of the SMC model is the assumption of two states (bursting vs. non-bursting). However, while it seems clear that this approach is helpful, it is not clear where this idea comes from, from an observation of the data or another concept.

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. As the reviewer pointed out, accounting for two firing regimes is helpful as it prevents biases in estimating the number of spikes when the firing rate is non-stationary and does not follow single-frequency Poisson statistics (as shown in Figure 3 of our preprint), as expected during in vivo recordings. Animals can alter their behavioral state and be exposed to different sensory stimulations, which condition the activity of neurons. A first step beyond the assumption of a steady firing rate is indeed to introduce a hidden two-state process to separate periods of high and low firing rates. In our revised text, we explicitly discuss the rationale behind this choice. We want to emphasize that PGBAR is the only model-based approach that accounts for nonhomogeneous firing rates. In addition, due to the binary character of the underlying bursting state and the high dimensionality of the problem, traditional optimization methods would not be applicable. We solved this problem by applying modern sequential Monte Carlo algorithms (PGAS, Lindsten 2014, for joint estimation for time-varying signals and model parameters) for the first time in the context of spike inference. In summary, the novelty of our work is both in modeling the firing statistics and the inference strategy used.

      Another SMC algorithm (Greenberg et al., 2018) stated that the fitted parameters showed some degeneracy, resulting in ambiguous fitting parameters. It would be good to know if this problem was avoided by the authors.

      As the reviewer pointed out, one of the weaknesses of the SBM approach is the optimization of the model parameters. This is expected, as SBM uses a biophysical model of the calcium indicator, and a general issue of dynamical models is the presence of so-called sloppy directions in the parameter space, which leads to ambiguous estimations. This is an intrinsic problem due to the model complexity also associated with poorly known parameters such as kinetic constants, which are hard to constrain experimentally. PGBAR uses a much simpler model to describe calcium transients (a second-order autoregressive process) precisely to avoid the non-identifiability of model parameters. Furthermore, we employed a parameterization of the autoregressive model (discussed in the Reparameterization section of Materials and Methods) regarding peak response to a single action potential, decay constant, and rise time (i.e., time to peak). These phenomenological parameters are well documented for different calcium indicators, which enables us to design appropriate prior distributions that significantly facilitate the identifiability of parameters.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      Methods to infer action potentials from fluorescence-based measurements of intracellular calcium dynamics are important for optical measurements of activity across large populations of neurons. The variety of existing methods can be separated into two broad classes: a) model-independent approaches that are trained on ground truth datasets (e.g., deep networks), and b) approaches based on a model of the processes that link action potentials to calcium signals. Models usually contain parameters describing biophysical variables, such as rate constants of the calcium dynamics and features of the calcium indicator. The method presented here, PGBAR, is model-based and uses a Bayesian approach. A novelty of PGBAR is that static parameters and state variables are jointly estimated using particle Gibbs sampling, a sequential Monte Carlo technique that can efficiently sample the latent embedding space.

      Strengths:

      A main strength of PGBAR is that it provides probability distributions rather than point estimates of spike times. This is different from most other methods and may be an important feature in cases when estimates of uncertainty are desired. Another important feature of PGBAR is that it estimates not only the state variable representing spiking activity but also other variables such as baseline fluctuations and stationary model variables, in a joint process. PGBAR can therefore provide more information than various other methods. The information in the GitHub repository is well-organised.

      Weaknesses:

      On the other hand, the accuracy of spike train reconstructions is not higher than that of other model-based approaches, and clearly lower than the accuracy of a model-independent approach based on a deep network. The authors demonstrate convincingly that PGBAR can resolve inter-spike intervals in the range of 5 ms using fluorescence data obtained with a very fast genetically encoded calcium indicator at very high sampling rates (line scans at >= 1 kHz). It would be interesting to more systematically compare the performance of PGBAR to other methods in this regime of high temporal resolution, which has not been explored much.

      We appreciate the Reviewer’s comment. In response to this observation, we have now included a thorough comparison of PGBAR, MLspike, and CASCADE in addition to the analysis of our cerebellar dataset acquired with a high sampling rate (Figure 9 in the revised preprint). PGBAR and CASCADE predictions are comparable in terms of correlation with the ground truth spikes, and both outperform MLspike. We have also quantified the spike time accuracy as the average distance between ground-truth spikes and the nearest prediction for all the methods. Among the three, PGBAR has the lowest variability of spike time accuracy across our experimental trials. We concluded that while PGBAR and CASCADE show comparable correlations with ground truth, our method provides more reliable spike time estimates.  

      Recommendations for the authors

      Reviewing Editor (Recommendations For The Authors):

      In the discussion with reviewers, it was also suggested that while the manuscript emphasized the high temporal resolution of the method (5 ms), this was achieved under favorable conditions (very high sampling rate, fast indicator). Results cannot be compared easily to alternative methods based on published data because these conditions are unusual. Do other methods (at least some of which are presumably easier to use) achieve similar temporal resolution when applied to the same dataset? I feel this could be addressed easily and add valuable information.

      We thank the Reviewing Editor for the suggestion. In our revised preprint, we have now added a full comparison between the performance of PGBAR, MLspike (as an alternative Bayesian approach), and CASCADE (as a state-of-the-art supervised method) tested on our cerebellar dataset. This analysis highlights the improved reliability of our method in terms of temporal accuracy and trial-to-trial variability.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      - It is in several places difficult to understand the bigger context of some details. For example, the authors write "In this work, we use Monte Carlo methods to approximate the posterior distribution in Eq. (13)." It would be helpful to state what the bigger goal behind this procedure is, here and at other places. Please go through the Introduction and the Results, there is some room for improvement in terms of accessibility.

      We thank the Reviewer for the comment. Monte Carlo methods are generally used when dealing with intractable (non-analytical) probability distributions, which is the case for the models used for spike inference. The “bigger goal behind this procedure” is just the numerical approximation of posterior probabilities, which simply formalizes the question of estimating unknowns from data given a statistical model according to the Bayesian theorem. The advantage of Monte Carlo methods, compared to other techniques (e.g., variational methods), is to be statistically unbiased, which is one of the main reasons why we developed this approach. We clarified the goal of the Monte Carlo inference In the introduction, by adding the following text:

      (line 79) “In this work we employ the particle Gibbs (PG) sampler on a bursting autoregressive (BAR) model of time series calcium-dependent fluorescence to provide not only point estimates of spike times but also quantify the statistical uncertainty of each estimate. This is important for downstream analyses such as comparing activity across neurons or conditions.”

      We introduce the Results/Model section with the sentence:

      (line 91) “To infer spike times and their uncertainty from noisy fluorescence traces, we first build a probabilistic generative model that captures the main dynamics underlying the fluorescence signal.”

      And later on in the Results/Sequential Monte Carlo section, we added:

      (line 156) “The model described in the previous section is analytically intractable, therefore we employ Monte Carlo methods to sample from the posterior distribution in Eq. (13) of spike times and model parameters, allowing us to make probabilistic inferences rather than relying on point estimates alone.”

      In the Abstract: "it provides a flexible statistical framework to test more specific models of calcium indicators". What is meant by this sentence? I was unable to find any results related to this statement.

      In our work, we propose a statistical model (depicted in Figure 1A) that accounts for a binary model for non-homogeneous firing, a Gaussian random walk to describe the modulation of the baseline fluorescence coupled to an autoregressive process to link spikes to fluorescence. The phrase quoted by the Reviewer refers to the possibility of replacing the autoregressive model with more specific models of calcium indicators in the future. For instance, employing the biophysical models  of calcium indicators to refine the link between spikes and calcium fluorescence. The inference algorithm does not depend on the specific spike-to-fluorescence model. In this sense, our framework is flexible as it offers the opportunity to analyze data acquired using other calcium indicators.  

      The authors write "One of the key advantages of our sampling algorithm is the joint estimation of latent states and time-independent model parameters." Why is this an advantage? Advantage compared to which alternative algorithm?

      We thank the reviewer for this comment. All existing spike inference algorithms use ad-hoc techniques to choose or calibrate the hyperparameters introduced. The estimation of spike times is in general highly sensitive to parameters such as the peak fluorescence in response to single action potentials, kinetic constants, noise levels, baseline, or any regularization or model parameter. These parameters are usually unknown, and all available inference methods provide additional prescriptions to calibrate them. This problem can lead to the propagation of errors and systematic biases. Modern Monte Carlo algorithms, such as the ones employed in our work, address specifically this problem by targeting the joint posterior distribution of all time-dependent variables and the model parameters. Compared to previous approaches, our method offers a statistically rigorous algorithm to identify the parameters. Furthermore, this approach enables us to use Bayesian priors to constrain their ranges without introducing ad-hoc biases and reducing the sensitivity to inaccurate choices of hyperparameters compared to other methods (MLspike), as shown in our new Figure 10 (following a suggestion from Reviewer 2), where we illustrate a parameter sensitivity analysis across MLspike and PGBAR (see responses to Reviewer 2 for further details). We clarified this in the Introduction by adding the sentence:

      (line 60) “[...] Moreover, current Bayesian methods do not treat time-independent model parameters (e.g. rate constants) and dynamic variables equally. Instead, they require additional optimization procedures to calibrate model parameters, typically relying on ad-hoc tuning or grid search. This separation can lead to biased inference and poorly calibrated uncertainty estimates, particularly when parameters such as calcium decay time or spike amplitude are inaccurately specified. In contrast, our approach jointly infers both spike times and model parameters within a unified Bayesian framework, enabling uncertainty-aware estimation and avoiding separate, error-prone calibration steps.”

      and In the section “Validation and performance of PGBAR” we added the text:

      (line 201) “One of the key advantages of our sampling algorithm is the joint estimation of latent states and time-independent model parameters, such as spike amplitude, decay time, noise level, and baseline variance. This stands in contrast to most existing spike inference algorithms, which rely on fixed or externally calibrated parameters. Such fixed-parameter methods are vulnerable to systematic errors when parameter values are uncertain or misestimated. By jointly sampling from the posterior of all variables and parameters, our method propagates uncertainty correctly and mitigates bias due to manual tuning or poor initialization.”

      We also added the following text in the discussion:

      (line 411) “The estimation of time-independent model parameters is a well-known issue in spike detection algorithms, typically requiring ad-hoc calibration procedures, grid search, or manual settings. Because spike inference is sensitive to parameters such as the calcium response amplitude, rise and decay kinetics, and noise level, errors in these parameters can substantially affect the accuracy of spike time estimates. By jointly sampling model parameters and latent variables, PGBAR eliminates the need for separate calibration and ensures that uncertainty in parameters is propagated to spike time estimates in a principled way. As illustrated in Figure 10, this leads to a more robust inference compared to existing methods like MLSpike, which show greater sensitivity to parameter variation. In addition, PGBAR enables the users to calibrate the inference of action potentials by setting prior mean and variance of phenomenological parameters (e.g. rise and decay constants, firing rates, bursting frequencies).”

      The authors write "We tested our approach on the fast calcium indicator GCaMP8f (...)". Be more precise. Why exactly were these experiments done, what aspects of the algorithm were supposed to be tested? It is left to the reader to make sense out of these experiments. Please provide the logic of this experiment.

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. We developed our method specifically for regimes of high firing rates. For this reason, in addition to the CASCADE benchmark dataset, we have tested our approach on recordings of cerebellar granule cells due to their fast spiking patterns. For this purpose, we have employed the ultrafast state-of-the-art calcium indicator GCaMP8f combined with linescan imaging techniques to enable fast acquisition rates. We added the following text in the manuscript to clarify:

      (line 306) “We tested our approach on the fast calcium indicator GCaMP8f by performing high-speed (2.8 kHz) two-photon linescan calcium imaging of cerebellar granule cells in vitro. GCaMP8f was expressed in the Crus I region of the cerebellum using adeno-associated virus (AAV) injection (Fig. 7A). Compared to GCaMP6f, GCaMP8f exhibits a rise time that is nearly an order of magnitude faster, which we expected to translate into substantially improved temporal accuracy in spike time detection.”

      The authors write "If we consider as reference correlation the average across the CASCADE dataset (0.75) (...)". Why would this threshold be appropriate? This sounds arbitrary; this experiment was conducted with 2.8 kHz line scan imaging of GCaMP8, while the reference stems from low-rate imaging of older indicators.

      We thank the reviewer for the remark. In the sentence quoted, we have used 0.75 as a reference for the state-of-the-art correlation between ground truth and predicted spikes and indicated the lowest temporal resolution (10 ms) where the PGBAR correlation is larger than the reference value. As the Reviewer correctly pointed out, the reference 0.75 refers to datasets with much lower acquisition frequency; therefore, in our revised preprint, we have added a comparison of the correlations obtained from PGBAR, CASCADE, and MLspike using high-speed recordings of cerebellar GCs (Figure 9), showing the increased performance of our method at high temporal resolution.  

      How was PGBAR evaluated using a given dataset in Figure 4c or in Figure 7? It is unclear to the reviewer whether the priors were automatically/manually adjusted for each data set.

      We thank the Reviewer for this comment. Briefly, for the CASCADE dataset, we have designed the priors for all parameters according to the existing characterization of the calcium indicator used in each experiment (Chen et al. 2013). For our cerebellar data, we have performed single stimulation trials for each recording, which we used to design priors on peak fluorescence response, decay constant, and time to peak fluorescence. In the Results section of the revised preprint, we clarified more specifically how priors were designed for the CASCADE and our cerebellar datasets. We have added the following statements:

      (line 239) “Bayesian priors for all PGBAR parameters were adapted to each experiment according to the existing characterization of the different calcium indicators used (Chen et al., 2013).”

      (line 314) “For each recorded soma and bouton we applied two types of stimulations. Single time point stimulation and a fixed stimulation pattern generated from a 20 Hz Poisson process with 29 stimulation time points. First, we used the single-stimulation trials to design prior distributions of amplitudes, rise and decay constants (Fig. 7C). Next, we used PGBAR to analyze independently each Poisson stimulation trial in Figure 7E. By generating thousands of posterior samples of spike time patterns, we obtained the spike probability for all time frames and trials (Fig. 7F).” 

      The authors write "This analysis illustrates the variability expected when analyzing multiple trials of the same neuron." Variability across trials of neuronal activity? Or variability of spike inference?

      We thank the reviewer for the comment. In the revised text, we clarify that we refer to the variability of spike inference across trials.

      The original statement has been revised to read: 

      (line 301) “This analysis illustrates the expected variability of spike inference when analyzing multiple trials of the same neuron.”

      Technical question: How can the authors be sure that glass electrode stimulation only elicits a single AP per stimulation? This was not clear to me from the manuscript alone.

      We thank the reviewer for the question. Our experimental protocol is designed in a way that in each trial we make sure a single electrical stimulation elicits a single AP. We adjust our stimulation strength until we see an all-or-none calcium transient in response to a single AP. Given the fast temporal properties of GCaMP8f, we could distinguish a single AP response from multiple APs during a single electrical stimulation. We then introduced a single stim trial ahead of each Poisson-train trial to see whether our stimulation strength could elicit a single AP response reliably and consistently. In this way, we ensured that every single stim was producing a single AP. 

      Figure 8: Please explain what you mean by "bouton". What is the dashed line in (A)? Why is it interesting to look at the differences between bouton and soma?

      We thank the Reviewer for the comment. In the updated text we clarified that we refer to synaptic boutons along the parallel fiber (line 311) and that the dashed line in Figure 8 refers to the ground-truth number of spikes (29). We also pointed out that the estimated delay between somas and boutons is compatible with the proximity of synaptic boutons to the stimulation site along the parallel fiber by adding the following text: 

      (line 340) “This result is compatible with the proximity of synaptic boutons to the electrical stimulation along the parallel fiber. We analyzed both signals from somata and synaptic boutons because in vivo two-photon imaging can be made from both parts of the cell. Here we showed that our method performs reliably on both, demonstrating its robustness across recording sites.”

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The authors emphasised the result that PGBAR can resolve spike timing differences of 5 ms. However, this result was obtained based on fluorescence data measured with a very fast calcium indicator at very high sampling rates. It remains unclear how the performance of PGBAR compares to other methods in this regime of high temporal resolution, which has not been explored much in previous comparisons of methods. Potential users interested in this regime would benefit from a direct comparison to other approaches.

      We thank the Reviewer for this suggestion. In our revised manuscript, we have included a detailed comparison of spike time accuracy between our method, MLspike, and Cascade, summarized in Figure 9. In this analysis, we used our in vitro dataset to estimate the average temporal accuracy of spike detection across the three methods. As discussed in the main text, the average temporal accuracy was defined as the temporal offset between the ground truth and the nearest detected spikes averaged across the ground truth. The distributions of temporal accuracies across our experiments obtained from MLspike, Cascade, and PGBAR differ in their spread, with 10th-to-90th percentile ranges of 14 ms, 8 ms, and 3 ms, respectively. This result demonstrates that PGBAR estimates are more reliable than MLspike and CASCADE across trials, with a narrower unbiased distribution of temporal accuracy. 

      In practice, approaches are more appealing to users when they do not require dedicated measurements to estimate parameters such as rise/decay time constants of calcium fluorescence signals within cells. Users may therefore be interested to know how results would be affected if these parameters are estimated only crudely. It would thus be useful to know how spike probability estimates vary as a function of these parameters, which should be easy to test systematically, and whether the sensitivity of PGBAR to inaccurate initial parameter estimates is lower or higher than that of other methods, which should also be easy to test. As PGBAR jointly estimates spike probabilities and model parameters, it may have an advantage here over other methods.

      We thank the Reviewer for this suggestion. In the new Figure 10, we show a parametric sensitivity analysis for both PGBAR and MLspike. For PGBAR, we considered the hyperparameters of the Bayesian priors associated with the peak response to a single spike and the baseline variance, which influences how much of the fluorescence can be attributed to baseline modulation. For MLspike, we considered the transient amplitude and the decay time constant. For both methods, we varied the parameters between -50% and +50% of their optimal value and estimated the correlation between predictions and ground truth as well as the number of spikes (Figure 10A). Next, we calculated the coefficient of variation across all parameter configurations for each trial (Figure 10B). Our analysis shows that, compared to previous methods, PGBAR has a much lower sensitivity to the initial choices of the hyperparameters, confirming the intuition of the Reviewer thanks to the simultaneous inference of spike times and model parameters. This result provides an important addition to our work.  

      Equation 10: -1 should be in subscript (t-1). Remark: I have not fully verified the mathematical parts because some of it is beyond my expertise. 

      We thank the Reviewer for pointing out the typo. This has been corrected in the revised preprint.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      We sincerely appreciate the editors for overseeing an efficient review process and for upholding the high standards of the journal. We have made extensive revisions to the manuscript after carefully reviewing the reviewers’ comments. We have addressed all the comments in our response and have incorporated the changes suggested by the reviewers to the best of our abilities. Notably, we have made the following major changes to the manuscript:

      (1) We have increased the patient cohort size from 10 to 23 for evaluating the levels of YEATS2 and H3K27cr.

      (2) To further strengthen the clinical relevance of our study, we have checked the expression of major genes involved in the YEATS2-mediated histone crotonylation axis (YEATS2, GCDH, ECHS1, Twist1 along with H3K27cr levels) in head and neck cancer tissues using immunohistochemistry.

      (3) We have performed extensive experiments to look into the role of p300 in assisting YEATS2 in regulating promoter histone crotonylation.

      The changes made to the manuscript figures have been highlighted in our response. We have also updated the Results section in accordance with the updated figures. Tables 1-4 and Supplementary files 1-3 have been moved to one single Excel workbook named ‘Supplementary Tables 1-8’. Additional revisions have been made to improve the overall quality of the manuscript and enhance data visualization. These additional changes are highlighted in the tracked changes version of the manuscript.

      Our response to the Public Reviews and ‘Recommendations to the Authors’ can be found below.

      Public Reviews:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript investigates a mechanism between the histone reader protein YEATS2 and the metabolic enzyme GCDH, particularly in regulating epithelial-to-mesenchymal transition (EMT) in head and neck cancer (HNC).

      Strengths:

      Great detailing of the mechanistic aspect of the above axis is the primary strength of the manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      Several critical points require clarification, including the rationale behind EMT marker selection, the inclusion of metastasis data, the role of key metabolic enzymes like ECHS1, and the molecular mechanisms governing p300 and YEATS2 interactions.

      We would like to sincerely thank the reviewer for the detailed, in-depth, and positive response. We have implemented constructive revisions to the manuscript to address the reviewer’s concerns effectively.

      Major Comments:

      (1) The title, "Interplay of YEATS2 and GCDH mediates histone crotonylation and drives EMT in head and neck cancer," appears somewhat misleading, as it implies that YEATS2 directly drives histone crotonylation. However, YEATS2 functions as a reader of histone crotonylation rather than a writer or mediator of this modification. It cannot itself mediate the addition of crotonyl groups onto histones. Instead, the enzyme GCDH is the one responsible for generating crotonyl-CoA, which enables histone crotonylation. Therefore, while YEATS2 plays a role in recognizing crotonylation marks and may regulate gene expression through this mechanism, it does not directly catalyse or promote the crotonylation process.

      We thank the reviewer for their insightful comment regarding the precision of our title. We agree that the initial wording 'mediates' could imply a direct enzymatic role for YEATS2 in histone crotonylation, which is indeed not the case. As the reviewer correctly points out, YEATS2 functions as a 'reader' of histone crotonylation marks.

      However, our research demonstrates that YEATS2 plays a crucial indirect regulatory role in the establishment of these crotonylation marks. Specifically, our data indicates that YEATS2 facilitates the recruitment of the histone crotonyltransferase p300 to specific gene promoters, such as that of SPARC. This recruitment mechanism directly impacts the localized deposition of crotonyl marks on nearby histone residues. Therefore, while YEATS2 does not directly catalyze the addition of crotonyl groups, its presence and interaction with p300 are essential for the regulation and establishment of histone crotonylation at these critical sites.

      To accurately reflect this nuanced, yet significant, regulatory mechanism, we have revised the title. We are replacing 'mediates' with 'regulates' to precisely convey that YEATS2 influences the histone crotonylation process, albeit indirectly, through its role in recruiting the enzymatic machinery. The updated title will now read: 'Interplay of YEATS2 and GCDH regulates histone crotonylation and drives EMT in head and neck cancer.' We believe this change maintains the core message of our findings while enhancing the scientific accuracy of the title.

      (2) The study suggests a link between YEATS2 and metastasis due to its role in EMT, but the lack of clinical or pre-clinical evidence of metastasis is concerning. Only primary tumor (PT) data is shown, but if the hypothesis is that YEATS2 promotes metastasis via EMT, then evidence from metastatic samples or in vivo models should be included to solidify this claim.

      We thank the reviewer for their valuable suggestion regarding the need for clinical or pre-clinical evidence of metastasis. We fully agree that direct evidence linking YEATS2 to metastasis would significantly strengthen our claims, especially given its demonstrated role in EMT.

      Our primary objective in this study was to meticulously dissect the molecular mechanisms by which YEATS2 regulates histone crotonylation and drives EMT in head and neck cancer. We have provided comprehensive upstream and downstream molecular insights into this process, culminating in a clear demonstration of YEATS2's functional importance in promoting EMT through multiple in vitro phenotypic assays (e.g., Matrigel invasion, wound healing, 3D invasion assays). As the reviewer notes, EMT is a widely recognized prerequisite for cancer metastasis[1]. Therefore, establishing YEATS2 as a driver of EMT directly implicates its potential role in metastatic progression.

      To further address the reviewer's concern and bridge the gap between EMT and metastasis, we have performed additional analyses that will be incorporated into the revised manuscript:

      Clinical Correlation with Tumor Grade: We analyzed publicly available head and neck cancer patient datasets. Our analysis revealed a significant positive correlation between YEATS2 expression and increasing tumor grade. Specifically, we observed significantly higher YEATS2 expression in Grade 2-4 tumors compared to Grade 1 tumors. Given that higher tumor grades are frequently associated with increased metastatic potential and poorer prognosis in HNC[2], this finding provides compelling clinical correlative evidence linking elevated YEATS2 expression to more aggressive disease.

      Gene Set Enrichment Analysis (GSEA) for Metastasis Pathways: To further explore the biological processes associated with YEATS2 in a clinical context, we performed GSEA on TCGA HNC patient samples stratified by high versus low YEATS2 expression. This analysis robustly demonstrated a positive enrichment of metastasis-related gene sets in the high YEATS2 expression group, compared to the low YEATS2 group. This strengthens the mechanistic link by showing that pathways associated with metastasis are co-ordinately upregulated when YEATS2 is highly expressed.

      These new clinical data provide strong correlative evidence supporting a direct association of YEATS2 with metastasis, building upon our detailed mechanistic dissection of its role in EMT.

      (3) There seems to be some discrepancy in the invasion data with BICR10 control cells (Figure 2C). BICR10 control cells with mock plasmids, specifically shControl and pEGFP-C3 show an unclear distinction between invasion capacities. Normally, we would expect the control cells to invade somewhat similarly, in terms of area covered, within the same time interval (24 hours here). But we clearly see more control cells invading when the invasion is done with KD and fewer control cells invading when the invasion is done with OE. Are these just plasmid-specific significant effects on normal cell invasion? This needs to be addressed.

      We thank the reviewer for their careful examination of Figure 2C and their insightful observation regarding the appearance of the control cells in relation to the knockdown (Figure 2B) and overexpression (Figure 2C) experiments. We understand how, at first glance, the control invasion levels across these panels might seem disparate.

      We wish to clarify that Figure 2B (YEATS2 knockdown) and Figure 2C (YEATS2 overexpression) represent two entirely independent experiments, conducted with distinct experimental conditions and methodologies, as detailed in our Methods section.

      Specifically:

      Figure 2B (Knockdown): Utilizes lentivirus-mediated transduction for stable shRNA delivery (shControl as control).

      Figure 2C (Overexpression): Utilizes transfection with plasmid DNA (pEGFP-C3 as control) via a standard transfection reagent.

      These fundamental differences in genetic manipulation methods (transduction vs. transfection), along with potential batch-to-batch variations in reagents or cell passage number at the time of each independent experiment, can indeed lead to variations in absolute basal invasion rates of control cells[3].

      Therefore, the invasion capacity of BICR10 control cells in Figure 2B (shControl) should only be compared to the YEATS2 knockdown conditions within that same panel. Similarly, the invasion capacity of control cells in Figure 2C (pEGFP-C3) should only be compared to the YEATS2 overexpression conditions within that specific panel. The crucial finding in each panel lies in the relative change in invasion caused by YEATS2 manipulation (knockdown or overexpression) compared to its respective, concurrently run control.

      We have ensured that all statistical analyses (as indicated in the figure legends and methods) were performed by comparing the experimental groups directly to their matched internal controls within each independent experiment. The significant increase in invasion upon YEATS2 overexpression and the significant decrease upon YEATS2 knockdown, relative to their respective controls, are robust and reproducible findings.

      (4) In Figure 3G, the Western blot shows an unclear band for YEATS2 in shSP1 cells with YEATS2 overexpression condition. The authors need to clearly identify which band corresponds to YEATS2 in this case.

      We thank the reviewer for pointing out the ambiguity in the YEATS2 Western blot for the shSP1 + pEGFP-C3-YEATS2 condition in Figure 3G. We apologize for this lack of clarity. The two bands seen in the shSP1+pEGFP-C3-YEATS2 condition correspond to the endogenous YEATS2 band (lower band) and YEATS2-GFP band (upper band, corresponding to overexpressed YEATS2-GFP fusion protein, which has a higher molecular weight). To avoid confusion, the endogenous band is now highlighted (marked by *) in the lane representing the shSP1+pEGFP-C3-YEATS2 condition. We have also updated the figure legend accordingly.

      (5) In ChIP assays with SP1, YEATS2 and p300 which promoter regions were selected for the respective genes? Please provide data for all the different promoter regions that must have been analysed, highlighting the region where enrichment/depletion was observed. Including data from negative control regions would improve the validity of the results.

      Throughout our study, we have performed ChIP-qPCR assays to check the binding of SP1 on YEATS2 and GCDH promoter, and to check YEATS2 and p300 binding on SPARC promoter. Using transcription factor binding prediction tools and luciferase assays, we selected multiple sites on the YEATS2 and GCDH promoter to check for SP1 binding. The results corresponding to the site that showed significant enrichment were provided in the manuscript. The region of SPARC promoter in YEATS2 and p300 ChIP assay was selected on the basis of YEATS2 enrichment found in the YEATS2 ChIP-seq data. The ChIP-qPCR data for all the promoter regions investigated (including negative controls) can be found below (Author response image 1.).

      Authors’ response image 1.

      (A) SP1 ChIP-qPCR results indicating SP1 occupancy on different regions of YEATS2 promoter. YEATS2 promoter region showing SP1 binding sites (indicated by red boxes) is shown above. SP1 showed significant enrichment at F1R1 region. The results corresponding to F1R1 region were included in Figure 3D. (B) SP1 ChIPqPCR results indicating SP1 occupancy on different regions of GCDH promoter. GCDH promoter region showing SP1 binding sites (indicated by red boxes) is shown above. SP1 showed significant enrichment at F2R2 region. The results corresponding to F2R2 region were included in Figure 7E. (C) YEATS2 ChIP-qPCR results in shControl vs. shYEATS2 BICR10 cells indicating YEATS2 occupancy on different regions of SPARC promoter. SPARC promoter region showing YEATS2 ChIP-seq and H3K27cr ChIP-seq signals is shown above. YEATS2 showed significant enrichment at F1R1 region. The results corresponding to F1R1 region were included in Figure 5C. (D) p300 ChIP-qPCR results in shControl vs. shYEATS2 BICR10 cells indicating p300 occupancy on different regions of SPARC promoter. p300 showed significant enrichment at F1R1 region. The results corresponding to F1R1 region were included in Figure 5F.

      (6) The authors establish a link between H3K27Cr marks and GCDH expression, and this is an already well-known pathway. A critical missing piece is the level of ECSH1 in patient samples. This will clearly delineate if the balance shifted towards crotonylation.

      We greatly appreciate the reviewer's insightful comment regarding the importance of assessing ECSH1 levels in patient samples to clearly delineate the metabolic balance shifting towards crotonylation. We fully agree that this is a critical piece of evidence.

      To directly address this point and substantiate our claim regarding the altered metabolic balance in HNC, we had previously analyzed the expression of both GCDH and ECHS1 in TCGA HNC RNA-seq data (as presented in Figure 4—figure supplement 1A and B). This analysis revealed a consistent increase in GCDH expression and a concomitant decrease in ECHS1 expression in tumor samples compared to normal tissues. Based on these findings, we hypothesized that this altered expression profile would indeed lead to an accumulation of crotonyl-CoA and, consequently, an overall increase in histone crotonylation in HNC.

      To further validate and extend these findings at the protein level, we have now performed immunohistochemistry (IHC) analysis for both ECHS1 and GCDH in a cohort of HNC normal vs. tumor tissues. Our IHC results strikingly corroborate the RNA-seq data: GCDH consistently showed increased protein expression in tumor samples, whereas ECHS1 exhibited significantly reduced protein expression in tumors compared to their adjacent normal counterpart tissues (Figure 4E and Authors’ response figure 5).

      These new data, combined with existing TCGA HNC RNA-seq analysis strongly supports our proposed mechanism where altered GCDH and ECHS1 expression contributes to increased histone crotonylation in head and neck cancer.

      (7) The p300 ChIP data on the SPARC promoter is confusing. The authors report reduced p300 occupancy in YEATS2-silenced cells, on SPARC promoter. However, this is paradoxical, as p300 is a writer, a histone acetyltransferase (HAT). The absence of a reader (YEATS2) shouldn't affect the writer (p300) unless a complex relationship between p300 and YEATS2 is present. The role of p300 should be further clarified in this case. Additionally, transcriptional regulation of SPARC expression in YEATS2 silenced cells could be analysed via downstream events, like Pol-II recruitment. Assays such as Pol-II ChIP-qPCR could help explain this.

      We greatly appreciate the reviewer's insightful observation regarding the apparently paradoxical reduction of p300 occupancy on the SPARC promoter upon YEATS2 silencing (Figure 5F), and their call for further clarification of p300's role and the potential complex relationship with YEATS2. We agree that this point required further mechanistic investigation.

      As we have shown through RNA-seq and ChIP-seq analyses, YEATS2 broadly influences histone crotonylation levels at gene promoters, thereby impacting gene expression. While p300 is indeed a known histone acetyltransferase (HAT) with promiscuous acyltransferase activity, including crotonyltransferase activity[4], the precise mechanism by which its occupancy is affected by a 'reader' protein like YEATS2 was unclear. Our initial data suggested a dependency of p300 recruitment on YEATS2.

      To directly address the reviewer's concern and thoroughly delineate the molecular mechanism of cooperativity between YEATS2 and p300 in regulating histone crotonylation, we have now performed a series of targeted experiments, which have been incorporated into the revised manuscript:

      (a) Validation of p300's role in SPARC expression: We performed p300 knockdown in BICR10 cells, followed by immunoblotting to assess SPARC protein levels. As expected, a significant decrease in SPARC protein levels was observed upon p300 knockdown (Figure 5G). This confirms p300's direct involvement in SPARC gene expression.

      (b) Direct interaction between YEATS2 and p300: To investigate a potential physical association, we performed co-immunoprecipitation assays to check for an interaction between endogenous YEATS2 and p300. Our results clearly demonstrate the presence of YEATS2 in the p300-immunoprecipitate sample, indicating that YEATS2 and p300 physically interact and likely function together as a complex to drive the expression of target genes like SPARC (Figure 5H). This direct interaction provides the mechanistic basis for how YEATS2 influences p300 occupancy.

      (c) Impact on transcriptional activity (Pol II recruitment): As suggested, we performed RNA Polymerase II (Pol II) ChIP-qPCR on the SPARC promoter in YEATS2 knockdown cells. We observed a significant decrease in Pol II occupancy on the SPARC promoter after YEATS2 knockdown in BICR10 cells (Figure 6C). This confirms that YEATS2 silencing leads to reduced transcriptional initiation/elongation at this promoter.

      (d) p300's direct role in H3K27cr on SPARC promoter: To confirm p300's specific role in crotonylation at this locus, we performed H3K27cr ChIP-qPCR after p300 knockdown. As anticipated, a significant decrease in H3K27cr enrichment was observed on the SPARC promoter upon p300 knockdown (Figure 6J), directly demonstrating p300's crotonyltransferase activity at this site.

      (e) Rescue of p300 occupancy and H3K27cr by YEATS2 overexpression in SP1deficient cells: To further establish the YEATS2-p300 axis, we performed SP1 knockdown (which reduces YEATS2 expression) followed by ectopic YEATS2 overexpression, and then assessed p300 occupancy and H3K27cr levels on the SPARC promoter. While SP1 knockdown led to a decrease in both p300 and H3K27cr enrichment, we observed a significant rescue of both p300 occupancy and H3K27cr enrichment upon YEATS2 overexpression in the shSP1 cells (Figure 6E and F). This provides strong evidence that YEATS2 acts downstream of SP1 to regulate p300 recruitment and H3K27cr levels.

      Collectively, these comprehensive new results clearly establish that YEATS2 directly interacts with and assists in the recruitment of p300 to the SPARC promoter. This recruitment is crucial for p300's localized crotonyltransferase activity, leading to increased H3K27cr marks and subsequent activation of SPARC transcription. This clarifies the previously observed 'paradox' and defines a novel cooperative mechanism between a histone reader (YEATS2) and a writer (p300) in regulating histone crotonylation and gene expression.

      (8) The role of GCDH in producing crotonyl-CoA is already well-established in the literature. The authors' hypothesis that GCDH is essential for crotonyl-CoA production has been proven, and it's unclear why this is presented as a novel finding. It has been shown that YEATS2 KD leads to reduced H3K27cr, however, it remains unclear how the reader is affecting crotonylation levels. Are GCDH levels also reduced in the YEATS2 KD condition? Are YEATS2 levels regulating GCDH expression? One possible mechanism is YEATS2 occupancy on GCDH promoter and therefore reduced GCDH levels upon YEATS2 KD. This aspect is crucial to the study's proposed mechanism but is not addressed thoroughly.

      We appreciate the reviewer's valuable comment questioning the novelty of GCDH's role in crotonyl-CoA production and seeking further clarification on how YEATS2 influences crotonylation levels beyond its reader function.

      We agree that GCDH's general role in producing crotonyl-CoA is well-established[5,6]. Our study, however, aims to delineate a novel epigenetic-metabolic crosstalk in head and neck cancer, specifically investigating how the interplay between the histone crotonylation reader YEATS2 and the metabolic enzyme GCDH contributes to increased histone crotonylation and drives EMT in this context.

      Our initial investigations using GSEA on publicly available TCGA RNA-seq data revealed that HNC patients with high YEATS2 expression also exhibit elevated expression of genes involved in the lysine degradation pathway, prominently including GCDH. Recognizing the known roles of YEATS2 in preferentially binding H3K27cr7 and GCDH in producing crotonylCoA, we hypothesized that the elevated H3K27cr levels observed in HNC are a consequence of the combined action of both YEATS2 and GCDH. We have provided evidence that increased nuclear GCDH correlates with higher H3K27cr abundance, likely due to an increased nuclear pool of crotonyl-CoA, and that YEATS2 contributes through its preferential maintenance of crotonylation marks by recruiting p300 (as detailed in Figure 5FH and Figure 6J-L of the manuscript and elaborated in our response to point 7). Thus, our work highlights that both YEATS2 and GCDH are crucial for the regulation of histone crotonylation-mediated gene expression in HNC.

      To directly address the reviewer's query regarding YEATS2's influence on GCDH levels and nuclear histone crotonylation:

      • YEATS2 does not transcriptionally regulate GCDH: We did not find any evidence of YEATS2 directly regulating the expression levels of GCDH at the transcriptional level in HNC cells.

      • Novel finding: YEATS2 regulates GCDH nuclear localization: Crucially, we discovered that YEATS2 downregulation significantly reduces the nuclear pool of GCDH in head and neck cancer cells (Figure 7G). This is a novel mechanism suggesting that YEATS2 influences histone crotonylation not only by affecting promoter H3K27cr levels via p300 recruitment, but also by regulating the availability of the crotonyl-CoA producing enzyme, GCDH, within the nucleus.

      • Common upstream regulation by SP1: Interestingly, we found that both YEATS2 and GCDH expression are commonly regulated by the transcription factor SP1 in HNC. Our data demonstrate that SP1 binds to the promoters of both genes, and its downregulation leads to a decrease in their respective expressions (Figure 3 and Figure 7). This provides an important upstream regulatory link between these two key players.

      • Functional validation of GCDH in EMT: We further assessed the functional importance of GCDH in maintaining the EMT phenotype in HNC cells. Matrigel invasion assays after GCDH knockdown and overexpression in BICR10 cells revealed that the invasiveness of HNC cells was significantly reduced upon GCDH knockdown and significantly increased upon GCDH overexpression (results provided in revised manuscript Figure 7F and Figure 7—figure supplement 1F).

      These findings collectively demonstrate a multifaceted role for YEATS2 in regulating histone crotonylation by both direct recruitment of the writer p300 and by influencing the nuclear availability of the crotonyl-CoA producing enzyme GCDH. We acknowledge that the precise molecular mechanism governing YEATS2's effect on GCDH nuclear localization remains an exciting open question for future investigation, but our current data establishes a novel regulatory axis.

      (9) The authors should provide IHC analysis of YEATS2, SPARC alongside H3K27cr and GCDH staining in normal vs. tumor tissues from HNC patients.

      We thank the reviewer for their suggestion. We have performed IHC analysis for YEATS2, H3K27cr and GCDH in normal and tumor samples obtained from HNC patient.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript emphasises the increased invasive potential of histone reader YEATS2 in an SP1-dependent manner. They report that YEATS2 maintains high H3K27cr levels at the promoter of EMT-promoting gene SPARC. These findings assigned a novel functional implication of histone acylation, crotonylation.

      We thank the reviewer for the constructive comments. We are committed to making beneficial changes to the manuscript in order to alleviate the reviewer’s concerns.

      Concerns:

      (1) The patient cohort is very small with just 10 patients. To establish a significant result the cohort size should be increased.

      We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have increased the number of patient samples to assess the levels of YEATS2 (n=23 samples) and the results have been included in Figure 1G and Figure 1—figure supplement 1F.

      (2) Figure 4D compares H3K27Cr levels in tumor and normal tissue samples. Figure 1G shows overexpression of YEATS2 in a tumor as compared to normal samples. The loading control is missing in both. Loading control is essential to eliminate any disparity in protein concentration that is loaded.

      To address the reviewer’s concern, we have repeated the experiment and used H3 as a loading control as nuclear protein lysates from patient samples were used to check YEATS2 and H3K27cr levels.

      (3) Figure 4D only mentions 5 patient samples checked for the increased levels of crotonylation and hence forms the basis of their hypothesis (increased crotonylation in a tumor as compared to normal). The sample size should be more and patient details should be mentioned.

      As part of the revision, we have now checked the H3K27cr levels in a total of 23 patient samples and the results have been included in Figure 4D and Figure 4— figure supplement 1D. Patient details are provided in Supplementary Table 6.

      (4) YEATS2 maintains H3K27Cr levels at the SPARC promoter. The p300 is reported to be hyper-activated (hyperautoacetylated) in oral cancer. Probably, the activated p300 causes hyper-crotonylation, and other protein factors cause the functional translation of this modification. The authors need to clarify this with a suitable experiment.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful comment regarding the functional relationship between YEATS2 and p300 in the context of H3K27cr, especially considering reports of p300 hyper-activation in oral cancer. We agree that a precise clarification of p300's role and its cooperativity with YEATS2 is crucial to fully understand the functional translation of this modification.

      As we have shown through global RNA-seq and ChIP-seq analyses, YEATS2 broadly affects gene expression by regulating histone crotonylation levels at gene promoters. We also recognize that the histone writer p300 is a promiscuous acyltransferase, known to add various non-acetyl marks, including crotonylation[4]. Our initial data, showing decreased p300 occupancy on the SPARC promoter upon YEATS2 downregulation (Figure 5F), suggested a strong dependency of p300 on YEATS2 for its recruitment. To fully delineate the molecular mechanism of this cooperativity and clarify how YEATS2 influences p300-mediated histone crotonylation and its functional outcomes, we have performed the following series of experiments, which have been integrated into the revised manuscript:

      (a) Validation of p300's role in SPARC expression: We performed p300 knockdown in BICR10 cells, followed by immunoblotting to assess SPARC protein levels. As expected, a significant decrease in SPARC protein levels was observed upon p300 knockdown (Figure 5G). This confirms p300's direct involvement in SPARC gene expression.

      (b) Direct interaction between YEATS2 and p300: To investigate a potential physical association, we performed co-immunoprecipitation assays to check for an interaction between endogenous YEATS2 and p300. Our results clearly demonstrate the presence of YEATS2 in the p300-immunoprecipitate sample, indicating that YEATS2 and p300 physically interact and likely function together as a complex to drive the expression of target genes like SPARC (Figure 5H). This direct interaction provides the mechanistic basis for how YEATS2 influences p300 occupancy.

      (c) Impact on transcriptional activity (Pol II recruitment): As suggested, we performed RNA Polymerase II (Pol II) ChIP-qPCR on the SPARC promoter in YEATS2 knockdown cells. We observed a significant decrease in Pol II occupancy on the SPARC promoter after YEATS2 knockdown in BICR10 cells (Figure 6C). This confirms that YEATS2 silencing leads to reduced transcriptional initiation/elongation at this promoter.

      (d) p300's direct role in H3K27cr on SPARC promoter: To confirm p300's specific role in crotonylation at this locus, we performed H3K27cr ChIP-qPCR after p300 knockdown. As anticipated, a significant decrease in H3K27cr enrichment was observed on the SPARC promoter upon p300 knockdown (Figure 6J), directly demonstrating p300's crotonyltransferase activity at this site.

      (e) Rescue of p300 occupancy and H3K27cr by YEATS2 overexpression in SP1deficient cells: To further establish the YEATS2-p300 axis, we performed SP1 knockdown (which reduces YEATS2 expression) followed by ectopic YEATS2 overexpression, and then assessed p300 occupancy and H3K27cr levels on the SPARC promoter. While SP1 knockdown led to a decrease in both p300 and H3K27cr enrichment, we observed a significant rescue of both p300 occupancy and H3K27cr enrichment upon YEATS2 overexpression in the sh_SP1_ cells (Figure 6K and L). This provides strong evidence that YEATS2 acts downstream of SP1 to regulate p300 recruitment and H3K27cr levels.

      Collectively, these comprehensive new results clearly establish that YEATS2 directly interacts with and assists in the recruitment of p300 to the SPARC promoter. This recruitment is crucial for p300's localized crotonyltransferase activity, leading to increased H3K27cr marks and subsequent activation of SPARC transcription. This clarifies the previously observed 'paradox' and defines a novel cooperative mechanism between a histone reader (YEATS2) and a writer (p300) in regulating histone crotonylation and gene expression.

      (5) I do not entirely agree with using GAPDH as a control in the western blot experiment since GAPDH has been reported to be overexpressed in oral cancer.

      We would like to clarify that GAPDH was not used as a loading control for protein expression comparisons between normal and tumor samples. GAPDH was used as a loading control only in experiments using head and neck cancer cell lines where shRNA-mediated knockdown or overexpression was employed. These manipulations specifically target the genes of interest and are not expected to alter GAPDH expression, making it a suitable loading control in these instances.

      (6) The expression of EMT markers has been checked in shControl and shYEATS2 transfected cell lines (Figure 2A). However, their expression should first be checked directly in the patients' normal vs. tumor samples.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We have now checked the expression of EMT marker Twist1 alongside YEATS2 expression in normal vs. tumor tissue samples using IHC (Figure 4E).

      (7) In Figure 3G, knockdown of SP1 led to the reduced expression of YEATS2 controlled gene Twist1. Ectopic expression of YEATS2 was able to rescue Twist1 partially. In order to establish that SP1 directly regulates YEATS2, SP1 should also be re-introduced upon the knockdown background along with YEATS2 for complete rescue of Twist1 expression.

      To address the reviewer’s concern regarding the partial rescue of Twist1 in SP1 depleted-YEATS2 overexpressed cells, we performed the experiment as suggested by the reviewer. We overexpressed both SP1 and YEATS2 in SP1-depleted cells and found that Twist1 depletion was almost completely rescued.

      Authors’ response image 2.

      Immunoblot depicting the decreased Twist1 levels on SP1 knockdown and its subsequent rescue of expression upon YEATS2 and SP1 overexpression in BICR10 (endogenous YEATS2 band indicated by *).

      (8) In Figure 7G, the expression of EMT genes should also be checked upon rescue of SPARC expression.

      We thank the reviewer for the suggestion. We have examined the expression of EMT marker Twist1 on YEATS2/ GCDH rescue. On overexpressing both YEATS2 and GCDH in sh_SP1_ cells we found that the depleted expression of Twist1 was rescued.

      Authors’ response image 3.

      Immunoblot depicting the decreased Twist1 levels on SP1 knockdown and its subsequent rescue of expression upon dual overexpression of YEATS2 and GCDH in BICR10 (* indicates GFP-tagged YEATS2 probed using GFP antibody).

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      While the study offers insights into the specific role of this axis in regulating epithelial-tomesenchymal transition (EMT) in HNC, its broader mechanistic novelty is limited by prior discoveries in other cancer types (https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-023-06061-0). The manuscript would benefit from the inclusion of metastasis data, the role of key metabolic enzymes like ECHS1, the molecular mechanisms governing p300 and YEATS2 interactions, additional IHC data, negative control data in ChIP, and an explanation of discrepancies in certain figures.

      We thank the reviewer for their constructive suggestions. We have made extensive revisions to our manuscript to substantiate our findings. We have looked into the expression of ECHS1/ GCDH in HNC tumor tissues using IHC, performed extensive experiments to validate the role of p300 in YEATS2-mediated histone crotonylation, and provided additional data supporting our findings wherever required. The revised figures have been provided in the updated version of the manuscript and also in the Authors’ response.

      Minor Comments:

      (1) The study begins with a few EMT markers, such as Vimentin, Twist, and N-Cadherin to validate the role of YEATS2 in promoting EMT. Including a broader panel of EMT markers would strengthen the conclusions about the effects of YEATS2 on EMT and invasion. Additionally, the rationale for selecting these EMT markers is not fully elaborated. Why were other well-known EMT players not included in the analysis?

      On performing RNA-seq with shControl and sh_YEATS2_ samples, we discovered that TWIST1 was showing decrease in expression on YEATS2 downregulation. So Twist1 was investigated as a potential target of YEATS2 in HNC cells. N-Cadherin was chosen because it is known to get upregulated directly by Twist1[8]. Further, Vimentin was chosen as it a well-known marker for mesenchymal phenotype and is frequently used to indicate EMT in cancer cells[9].

      Authors’ response image 4.

      IGV plot showing the decrease in Twist1 expression in shControl vs. shYEATS2 RNA-seq data.

      Other than the EMT-markers used in our study, the following markers were amongst those that showed significant change in gene expression on YEATS2 downregulation.

      Authors’ response table 1.

      List of EMT-related genes that showed significant change in expression on YEATS2 knockdown in RNA-seq analysis.

      As depicted in the table above, majority of the genes that showed downregulation on YEATS2 knockdown were mesenchymal markers, while epithelial-specific genes such as Ecadherin and Claudin-1 showed upregulation. This data signifies the essential role of YEATS2 in driving EMT in head and neck cancer.

      (2) The authors use Ponceau staining, but the rationale behind this choice is unclear. Ponceau is typically used for transfer validation. For the same patient, western blot loading controls like Actin/GAPDH should be shown. Also, at various places throughout the manuscript, Ponceau staining has been used. These should also be replaced with Actin/GAPDH blots.

      Ponceau S staining is frequently used as alternative for housekeeping genes like GAPDH as control for protein loading[10]. However, to address this issue, we have repeated the western and used H3 as a loading control as nuclear protein lysates from patient samples were used to check YEATS2 and H3K27cr levels.

      For experiments (In Figures 5E, 6F, 6I, and 7H ) where we assessed SPARC levels in conditioned media obtained from BICR10 cells (secretory fraction), Ponceau S staining was deliberately used as the loading control. In such extracellular protein analyses, traditional intracellular housekeeping genes (like Actin or GAPDH) are not applicable. Ponceau S has been used as a control for showing SPARC expression in secretory fraction of mammalian cell lines in previous studies as well11.  

      (3) The manuscript briefly mentions that p300 was identified as the only protein with increased expression in tumours compared to normal tissue in the TCGA dataset. What other writers were checked for? Did the authors check for their levels in HNC patients?

      We thank the reviewer for this observation. As stated by previous studies [12,13], p300 and GCN5 are the histone writers that can act as crotonyltransferases at the H3K27 position. Although the crotonyltransferase activity of GCN5 has been demonstrated in yeast, it has not been confirmed in human. Whereas the histone crotonyltransferase activity of p300 has been validated in human cells using in vitro HCT assays[4,14]. Therefore, we chose to focus on p300 for further validation of its role in YEATS2mediated regulation of histone crotonylation. We did not check the levels of p300 in HNC patient tissues. However, p300 showed higher expression in tumor as compared to normal in publicly available HNC TCGA RNA-seq data (Figure 5—figure supplement 1G).

      We acknowledge that the original statement in the manuscript, 'For this we looked at expression of the known writers of H3K27Cr mark in TCGA dataset, and discovered that p300 was the only protein that had increased expression in tumor vs. normal HNC dataset…', was indeed slightly misleading. Our intention was to convey that p300 is considered the major and most validated histone crotonyltransferase capable of influencing crotonylation at the H3K27 position in humans, and that its expression was notably increased in the HNC TCGA tumor dataset. We have now reframed this sentence in the revised manuscript to accurately reflect our findings and focus, as follows:

      'For this, we checked the expression of p300, a known writer of H3K27cr mark in humans, in the TCGA dataset. We found that p300 had increased expression in tumor vs. normal HNC dataset…'

      This revised wording more accurately reflects our specific focus on p300's established role and its observed upregulation in HNC.

      (4) Figure 6E, blot should be replaced. The results aren't clearly visible.

      We thank the reviewer for this observation. We have repeated the western blot and the Figure 6E (Figure 6F in the revised version of manuscript) has now been replaced with a cleaner blot.

      (5) Reference 9 and 19 are the same. Please rectify.

      We apologize for this inadvertent error. We have rectified this error in the updated version of the manuscript.

      References

      (1) Brabletz, T.; Kalluri, R.; Nieto, M. A.; Weinberg, R. A. EMT in Cancer. Nat Rev Cancer 2018, 18(2), 128–134. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrc.2017.118.

      (2) Pisani, P.; Airoldi, M.; Allais, A.; Aluffi Valletti, P.; Battista, M.; Benazzo, M.; Briatore, R.; Cacciola, S.; Cocuzza, S.; Colombo, A.; Conti, B.; Costanzo, A.; Della Vecchia, L.; Denaro, N.; Fantozzi, C.; Galizia, D.; Garzaro, M.; Genta, I.; Iasi, G. A.; Krengli, M.; Landolfo, V.; Lanza, G. V.; Magnano, M.; Mancuso, M.; Maroldi, R.; Masini, L.; Merlano, M. C.; Piemonte, M.; Pisani, S.; Prina-Mello, A.; Prioglio, L.; Rugiu, M. G.; Scasso, F.; Serra, A.; Valente, G.; Zannetti, M.; Zigliani, A. Metastatic Disease in Head & Neck Oncology. Acta Otorhinolaryngol Ital 2020, 40 (SUPPL. 1), S1–S86. https://doi.org/10.14639/0392-100X-suppl.1-40-2020.

      (3) Lin, J.; Zhang, P.; Liu, W.; Liu, G.; Zhang, J.; Yan, M.; Duan, Y.; Yang, N. A Positive Feedback Loop between ZEB2 and ACSL4 Regulates Lipid Metabolism to Promote Breast Cancer Metastasis. Elife 2023, 12, RP87510. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.87510.

      (4) Liu, X.; Wei, W.; Liu, Y.; Yang, X.; Wu, J.; Zhang, Y.; Zhang, Q.; Shi, T.; Du, J. X.; Zhao, Y.; Lei, M.; Zhou, J.-Q.; Li, J.; Wong, J. MOF as an Evolutionarily Conserved Histone Crotonyltransferase and Transcriptional Activation by Histone Acetyltransferase-Deficient and Crotonyltransferase-Competent CBP/P300. Cell Discov 2017, 3 (1), 17016. https://doi.org/10.1038/celldisc.2017.16.

      (5) Jiang, G.; Li, C.; Lu, M.; Lu, K.; Li, H. Protein Lysine Crotonylation: Past, Present, Perspective. Cell Death Dis 2021, 12 (7), 703. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41419-021-03987-z.

      (6) Yuan, H.; Wu, X.; Wu, Q.; Chatoff, A.; Megill, E.; Gao, J.; Huang, T.; Duan, T.; Yang, K.; Jin, C.; Yuan, F.; Wang, S.; Zhao, L.; Zinn, P. O.; Abdullah, K. G.; Zhao, Y.; Snyder, N. W.; Rich, J. N. Lysine Catabolism Reprograms Tumour Immunity through Histone Crotonylation. Nature 2023, 617 (7962), 818–826. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-023-06061-0.

      (7) Zhao, D.; Guan, H.; Zhao, S.; Mi, W.; Wen, H.; Li, Y.; Zhao, Y.; Allis, C. D.; Shi, X.; Li, H. YEATS2 Is a Selective Histone Crotonylation Reader. Cell Res 2016, 26 (5), 629–632. https://doi.org/10.1038/cr.2016.49.

      (8) Alexander, N. R.; Tran, N. L.; Rekapally, H.; Summers, C. E.; Glackin, C.; Heimark, R. L. NCadherin Gene Expression in Prostate Carcinoma Is Modulated by Integrin-Dependent Nuclear Translocation of Twist1. Cancer Res 2006, 66 (7), 3365–3369.

      https://doi.org/10.1158/0008-5472.CAN-05-3401.

      (9) Satelli, A.; Li, S. Vimentin in Cancer and Its Potential as a Molecular Target for Cancer Therapy. Cellular and Molecular Life Sciences 2011, 68 (18), 3033–3046. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00018-011-0735-1.

      (10) Romero-Calvo, I.; Ocón, B.; Martínez-Moya, P.; Suárez, M. D.; Zarzuelo, A.; Martínez-Augustin, O.; de Medina, F. S. Reversible Ponceau Staining as a Loading Control Alternative to Actin in Western Blots. Anal Biochem 2010, 401 (2), 318–320. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ab.2010.02.036.

      (11) Ling, H.; Li, Y.; Peng, C.; Yang, S.; Seto, E. HDAC10 Inhibition Represses Melanoma Cell Growth and BRAF Inhibitor Resistance via Upregulating SPARC Expression. NAR Cancer 2024, 6 (2), zcae018. https://doi.org/10.1093/narcan/zcae018.

      (12) Gao, D.; Li, C.; Liu, S.-Y.; Xu, T.-T.; Lin, X.-T.; Tan, Y.-P.; Gao, F.-M.; Yi, L.-T.; Zhang, J. V; Ma, J.Y.; Meng, T.-G.; Yeung, W. S. B.; Liu, K.; Ou, X.-H.; Su, R.-B.; Sun, Q.-Y. P300 Regulates Histone Crotonylation and Preimplantation Embryo Development. Nat Commun 2024, 15 (1), 6418. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-024-50731-0.

      (13) Li, K.; Wang, Z. Histone Crotonylation-Centric Gene Regulation. Epigenetics Chromatin 2021, 14 (1), 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13072-021-00385-9.

      (14) Sabari, B. R.; Tang, Z.; Huang, H.; Yong-Gonzalez, V.; Molina, H.; Kong, H. E.; Dai, L.; Shimada, M.; Cross, J. R.; Zhao, Y.; Roeder, R. G.; Allis, C. D. Intracellular Crotonyl-CoA Stimulates Transcription through P300-Catalyzed Histone Crotonylation. Mol Cell 2015, 58 (2), 203–215. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1016/j.molcel.2015.02.029.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the current reviews.

      Response to Reviewer #3:

      We thank reviewer 3 for spending their valuable time on commenting on our revised paper.

      We would like to reiterate the central conclusion of this work, which appears to have been missed by Reviewer 3. Using a BFP-expressing lineage tracer hPSC line for tracking LMX1A+ midbrain-patterned neural progenitors and their differentiated progeny, we discovered a loss of the LMX1A lineage during pluripotent stem cell differentiation into astrocytes, despite BFP+ neural progenitors were the dominant population at the onset of astrocyte induction.

      Hence, the take-home message of this study is, as summarized in the abstract, ‘ the lineage composition of iPSC-derived astrocytes may not accurately recapitulate the founder progenitor population’ and that one should not take for granted that in vitro/stem cell-derived astrocytes are the descendants of the dominant starting neural progenitors (which is a general assumption in PSC publications as described in the paper and our response to reviewers).

      Please find below our point-by-point response to reviewer comments. We have re-ordered the points according to their relative importance to our main conclusions.

      ‘ the lineage composition of iPSC-derived astrocytes may not accurately recapitulate the founder progenitor population’ and that one should not take for granted that in vitro/stem cell derived astrocytes are the descendants of the dominant starting neural progenitors (which is a general assumption in PSC publications as described in the paper and our response to reviewers).

      Please find below our point-by-point response to their comments. We have re-ordered the points according to their relative importance to our main conclusions.

      …. They used lineage tracing with a LMX1A-Cre/AAVS1-BFP iPSCs line, where the initial expression of LMX1A and Cre allows the long-lasting expression of BFP, yielding BFP+ and BFP- populations, that were sorted when in the astrocytic progenitor expansion. BFP+ showed significantly higher number of cells positive to NFIA and SOX9 than BFP- cells …

      This is a misunderstanding by reviewer 3. As indicated in the first sentence of the second section, BFP- populations used for functional and transcriptomic analysis was not sorted BFP<sup>-</sup> cells, but those derived from unsorted, BFP<sup>+</sup> enriched populations. Our scRNAseq analysis indicated that they were transcriptomically aligned to human midbrain astrocytes. This finding is consistent with the fact that they are derived from midbrain-patterned neural progenitors, presumably minority LMX1A- progenitors.

      Reviewer 3’s comments indicate that they misunderstood the primary aims of our study as a mere functional and transcriptomic comparison of the two astrocyte populations.

      (9) BFP+ cells did not show higher levels of transcripts for LMX1A nor FOXA2. This fact jeopardizes the claim that these cells are still patterned. In the same line, there are not significant differences with cortical astrocytes, indicating a wider repertoire of the initially patterned cells, that seems to lose the midbrain phenotype. Furthermore, common DGE shared by BFP- and BFP+ cells when compared to non-patterned cells indicate that after culture, the pre-pattern in BFP+ cells is somehow lost, and coincides with the progression of BFP- cells.

      The reviewer seems to assume that astrocytes derived from LMX1A+ ventral midbrain progenitors must retain LMX1A expression. We do not take this view and do not claim this in this study. Moreover, we have discussed in the paper that due to a lack of transcriptomic studies of in vivo track regional progenitors (such as LMX1A), it remains unknown whether and to what extent patterning gene expression is maintained in astrocytes of different brain regions.

      Our findings on the lack of LMX1A and FOXA2 in BFP+ astrocytes are supported by several published single-cell transcriptomic studies of human midbrain astrocytes (La Manno et al. 2016; Agarwal et al. 2020; Kamath et al. 2022). We have a paragraph of discussion on this topic in both the original and updated versions of the paper with the relevant publications cited.

      Other points raised by reviewer 3

      (1) It is very intriguing that GFAP is not expressed in late BFP- nor in BFP+ cultures, when authors designated them as mature astrocytes.

      We did not designate our cells as ‘mature’ astrocytes but ‘astrocytes’ based on their global gene expression with the human fetal and adult brain astrocytes as references.

      Moreover, ‘mature’ only appeared once in the paper indicating that our cells lie in between the fetal and adult astrocytes in maturity.

      (2) In Fig. 2D, authors need to change the designation "% of positive nuclei".

      To be corrected in the version of record.

      (3) In Fig. 2E, the text describes a decrease caused by 2APB on the rise elicited by ATP, but the graph shows an increase with ATP+2APB. However, in Fig. 2F, the peak amplitude for BFP+ cells is higher in ATP than in ATP+2APD, which is mentioned in the text, but this is inconsistent with the graph in 2E.

      To be corrected in the version of record.

      (4) The description of Results in the single-cell section is confusing, particularly in the sorted CD49 and unsorted cultures. Where do these cells come from? Are they BFP-, BFP+, unsorted for BFP, or non-patterned? Which are the "all three astrocyte populations"? A more complete description of the "iPSC-derived neurons" is required in this section to allow the reader to understand the type and maturation stage of neurons, and if they are patterned or not.

      As previously reported in the reference cited, CD49 is a novel human astrocyte marker. This is independent of BFP expression. For all three astrocyte populations studied here (BFP+, BFP-, and non-patterned astrocytes), we included both CD49f+ sorted and unsorted samples to account for selection bias caused by FACS. iPSC-derived neurons were included in the sequencing study to provide a reference for cell-type annotation. They were generated following a GABAergic neuron differentiation protocol. However, their maturation stages and/or regional characteristics are not relevant to astrocytes.

      (5) A puzzling fact is that both BFP- and BFP- cells have similar levels of LMX1A, as shown in Fig. S6F. How do authors explain this observation?

      This figure panel shows that LMX1A, LMX1B and FOXA2 are essentially NOT expressed in these astrocytes.

      (6) In Fig. 3B, the non-patterned cells cluster away from the BFP+ and BFP-; on the other hand, early and late BFP- are close and the same is true for early and late BFP+. A possible interpretation of these results is that patterned astrocytes have different paths for differentiation, compared to non-patterned cells. If that can be implied from these data, authors should discuss the alternative ways for astrocytes to differentiate.

      Both BFP+ and BFP- astrocyte are from ventral midbrain patterned neural progenitors, while non-patterned neural progenitors are more akin to that of forebrain. Figure 3B is expected and confirms the patterning effect.

      (7) Fig. 3D shows that cluster 9 is the only one with detectable and coincident expression of both S100B and GFAP expression. Please discuss why these widely-accepted astrocyte transcripts are not found in the other astrocytes clusters. Also, Sox9 is expressed in neurons, astrocyte precursors and astrocytes. Why is that?

      S100B and GFAP are classic astrocyte markers in certain states. We are not relying only on two markers but the genome-wide expression profile as the criteria for astrocytes. As shown in the unbiased reference mapping to multiple human brain astrocyte scRNA-seq datasets, all our astrocyte clusters were mapped with high confidence to human astrocytes.

      SOX9 is an important regulator for astrogenesis, so its expression is expected in precursors (doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.01.024). In addition, recent studies have uncovered that SOX9 expression is also reported in foetal striatal projection neurons and early postnatal cortical neurons, where SOX9 regulates neuronal synaptogenesis and morphogenesis (dois:10.1016/j.fmre.2024.02.019; 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.10.008). Therefore, the expression of SOX9 in multiple cell types was expected. Instead of using a few selected markers for cell-type annotation, we employed a genomic approach relying on an unbiased reference mapping approach and a combination of various markers to ascertain our annotation results.

      (8) Line 337, Why authors selected a log2 change of 0.25? Typically, 1 or a higher number is used to ensure at least a 2-fold increase, or a 50% decrease. A volcano plot generated by the comparison of BFP+ with BFP- cells would be appropriate. The validation of differences by immunocytochemistry, between BFP+ and BFP-, is inconclusive. The staining is blur in the images presented in Fig. S8C. Quantification of the positive cells, without significant background signal, in both populations is required.

      We used a lenient threshold owing to the following considerations: 1) High FC does not necessarily mean biological relevance, as gene expression does not necessarily translate to protein expression. Therefore, a smaller FC value could also be biologically meaningful. 2) Balance between noise and biological differences. Any threshold was chosen arbitrarily. 3) We are identifying a trend rather than pinpointing a specific set of

      The quality was unfortunately reduced due to restrictions on file size upon submission. A high resolution Fig. S8C is available.

      (10) For the GO analyses, How did authors select 1153 genes? The previous section mentioned 287 genes unique for BFP+ cells. The Results section should include a rationale for performing a wider search for the enriched processes.

      GO enrichment using unique DEGS may not capture the wider landscape of the transcriptomic characteristics of BFP<sup>+</sup> astrocytes. The 287 unique genes were only differentially expressed in BFP<sup>+</sup> astrocytes. However, apart from these 287 genes, other genes among the 1187 DEGs were differentially expressed in BFP<sup>+</sup> astrocytes and in one other population.

      (11) For Fig. 4C and 4D, both p values and the number of genes should be indicated in the graph. I would advise to select the 10 or 15 most significant categories, these panels are very difficult to read. Whereas the listed processes for BFP+ have a relation to Parkinson disease, the ones detected for BFP- cells are related to extracellular matrix and tissue development. Does it mean that BFP+ cells have impaired formation of this matrix, or defective tissue development? This is in contradiction of enhanced calcium responses of BFP+ cells compared to BFP- cells.

      Information on all DEGs, including p values and numbers, is provided in Supplementary data 1-5.

      BFP+ astrocytes do have enrichment for GO terms related to extracellular matrix and tissue development, although not as obvious as BFP- astrocytes. Previous work have shown that both in vitro and in vivo derived astrocytes are functionally heterogeneous, containing functionally distinct subtypes exhibiting different GO enrichment profiles (doi: 10.1016/j.ygeno.2021.01.008; 10.1038/s41598-024-74732-7).

      (12) Both the comparison between midbrain and cortical astrocytes in Fig. S8A, and the volcano plot in S8B do not show consistent changes. For example, RCAN2 in Fig. S8A has the same intensity for cortical and midbrain cells, but is marked as an enriched gene in midbrain in the p vs log2FC graph in Fig. S8B.

      These are integrated analyses of published human datasets. S8A and S8B show the same data in different formats. The differences are better shown in the volcano plot/easier detected by the human eye.

      These are integrated analysis of published human datasets. S8A and S8B are the same data shown in different format. Differences are better shown in volcano plot /easier detected by the human eye. RCAN2 had a higher average expression in the midbrain than in the telencephalon, albeit small, and the difference was statistically significant (as shown in the volcano plot).


      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews

      Reviewer 1:

      In vitro nature of this work being the fundamental weakness of this paper

      We disagree with this statement. As explained in the provisional response, the aim of this study was to test the validity of a general concept applied in pluripotent stem cell research that pluripotent stem cell-derived astrocytes faithfully represent the lineage heterogeneity of their ancestral neural progenitors and hence preserve the regionality of such progenitors. Our genetic lineage study is justified for addressing this in vitro-driven question. However, we have highlighted the rationale where appropriate in the revised paper.

      If regional identity is not maintained, so what? Don't we already know that this can happen? The authors acknowledge that this is known in the discussion.

      Importance of regional identity: Growing evidence demonstrates the functional heterogeneity of brain astrocytes in health and disease. Therefore, for in vitro disease modeling, it is believed that one should use astrocytes represent the anatomy of disease pathology; for example, midbrain astrocytes for studying dopamine neurodegeneration and Parkinson’s disease. Understanding the dynamics of stem cell-derived astrocytes and identifying astrocyte subtypes is important for their biomedical applications.

      Regional identity change/Discussion: It seems that the reviewer misunderstood the context in which the ‘identity change’ was discussed. The literature referred to (in the Discussion) concerns shifts in regional gene expression in bulk-cultured cells. In the days of pre-single-cell analysis/lineage tracking, one cannot distinguish whether this was due to a change in the transcriptomic landscape in progenies of the same lineage or alterations in lineage heterogeneity, but to interpret at face value as regional identity was not maintained. In the revised paper, we have made an effort to indicate that ‘regional identity’ is used broadly to refer to lineage relationships and/or traits rather than static gene expressioin.

      validation of the markers/additional work

      The scNAseq analysis performed in this study compared the profiles of astrocytes derived from LMX1A+ and LMX1A- ventral midbrain-patterned neural progenitors. Since it is not possible to perform genetic lineage tracking in humans and an analogous mouse lineage tracer line is not available, in vivo validation of these markers with respect to their lineage relationship is not currently feasible. However, we took advantage of abundant single-cell human astrocyte transcriptomic datasets and validated our genes in silico. We also validated the differential expression of selected markers in late BFP+ and BFP- astrocytes using immunocytochemistry, where reliable antibodies are available. The results of the additional analyses are presented in Figure S8 and Supplemental Data 5.

      Knowledge gaps concerning astrocyte development

      Reviewer 1 pointed out a number of knowledge gaps concerning astrocyte development, such as the transcriptomic landscape trajectories of midbrain floor plate cells as they progress towards astrocytes. Indeed, the limited knowledge on regional astrocyte molecule heterogeneity restricts the objective validation of in vitro-derived astrocyte subtypes and the development of novel approaches for their generation in vitro. We agree with the need for in-depth in vivo studies using model organisms, although these are beyond the scope of the current work.

      Reviewer 2:

      (1) The authors argue that the depletion of BFP seen in the unsorted population immediately after the onset of astrogenic induction is due to the growth advantage of the derivatives of the residual LMX1A- population. However, no objective data supporting this idea is provided, and one could also hypothesize that the residual LMX1A- cells could affect the overall LMX1A expression in the culture through negative paracrine regulation.

      We acknowledge the lack of evidence-based explanation for the depletion of BFP+ cells in mixed cultures. We were unable to perform additional experiments because of resource limitations. The design of the LMX1A-Cre/AAVS1-BFP lineage tracer line determines that BFP is expressed irreversibly in LMX1A-expressing cells or their derivatives regardless of their LMX1A expression status. Therefore, the potential negative paracrine regulation of LMX1A by residual LMX1A- cells should not affect cells that have already turned on BFP. We have highlighted the working principles of the LMX1A tracer line in the revised manuscript.

      (2) Furthermore, on line 124 it is stated that: "Interestingly, the sorted BFP+ cells exhibited similar population growth rate to that of unsorted cultures...". In the face of the suggested growth disadvantage of those cells, this statement needs clarification.

      To avoid confusion, we have removed the statement.

      (3) Regarding the fidelity of the model system, it is not clear to me how the TagBFP expression was detected in the BFP+ population supposedly in d87 and d136 pooled astrocytes (Fig S6C) while no LMX1A expression was observed in the same cells (Fig S6F).

      The TagBFP tracer is expressed in the progenies of LMX1A+ cells, regardless of their LMX1A expression status. We have gone through the MS text to ensure that this information has been provided.

      (4) The generated single-cell RNASeq dataset is extremely valuable. However, given the number of conditions included in this study (i.e. early vs late astrocytes, BFP+ vs BFP-, sorted vs unsorted, plus non-patterned and neuronal samples) the resulting analysis lacks detail. For instance, from a developmental perspective and to better grasp the functional significance of astrocytic heterogeneity, it would be interesting to map the identified clusters to early vs late populations and to the BFP status.

      We performed additional bioinformatics analysis, which provided independent support for the relative developmental maturity suggested by functional assays. The additional data are now provided in the revised Figure 3B, C, E.

      Moreover, although comprehensive, Figure S7 is complex to understand given that citations rather than the reference populations are depicted.

      The information provided in the revised Figure S7.

      (5) Do the authors have any consideration regarding the morphology of the astrocytes obtained in this study? None of the late astrocyte images depict a prototypical stellate morphology, which is reported in many other studies involving the generation of iPSC-derived astrocytes and which is associated with the maturity status of the cell.

      The morphology of our astrocytes was not unique to the present study. Many factors may influence the morphology of astrocytes, such as the culture media and supplements used, and maturity status. Based on the functional assays and limited GFAP expression, our astrocytes were relatively immature.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      In Figure 1, it is very difficult to identify where CySCs end and GSCs begin without using a cell surface marker for these different cell types. In addition, the methods for quantifying the mitochondrial distribution in GSCs vs. CySCs are very much unclear and appear to rely on colocalization with molecular markers that are not in the same cellular compartment (Tj-nuclear vs. Vasa-perinuclear and cytoplasmic) the reader has no way to determine the validity of the mitochondrial distribution. Similarly, the labelling with gstD1-GFP is also very much unclear - I see little to no GFP signal in either GSCs or CySCs in panels 1GK. Lastly, while the expression o SOD in CySCs does increase the gstD1-GFP signal in CySCs, the effects on GSCs claimed by the authors are not apparent.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s detailed feedback on Figure 1 and the concerns raised regarding identifying CySCs and GSCs, as well as the methods used for quantifying mitochondrial distribution and gstD1-GFP labeling. Below, we address each point and describe the revisions made to improve clarity and rigor

      Distinguishing CySCs and GSCs and Mitochondrial Distribution in GSCs vs. CySCs in Figure1

      We acknowledge the difficulty in distinguishing CySCs from GSCs without the use of additional cell surface markers. To improve clarity, we have now included a membrane marker discslarge (Dlg) in our revised Figure 1 and S1 to delineate cell boundaries more clearly. Additionally, we provide higher-magnification images to indicate the mitochondria in CySCs and GSCs. We also agree that ing on mitochondrial distribution might be far-fetched. In the revised manuscript, we have limited our analysis to mitochondrial shape, which was found to be different in GSC and CySC (Fig. 1, D, F, G, and S1B). We have clarified our quantification methods in the revised Methods section, providing details on the image processing and analysis pipeline used to assess mitochondrial distribution. 

      Clarity of gstD1-GFP Labelling:

      We recognize the reviewer’s concern regarding the weak GFP signal in these panels. To improve visualization, we have included fresh set of images by optimizing the contrast and presenting additional monochrome images with higher exposure settings to better illustrate gstD1-GFP expression (Figure 1L,1Q, and S1C’’’-D’’’). Additionally, we have demarcated the cell boundaries using Dlg along with individual labelling of Vasa+ and Tj+ cells. Due to technical difficulty associated with acquisition of images, we could not co-stain Vasa, Tj and Dlg together. Therefore, quantified the gstD-GFP intensity separately for GSCs and CySCs under similar acquisition conditions (Figure 1R).   

      Effects of SOD depletion on GSCs:

      While our initial analysis suggested changes in gstD1-GFP expression in GSCs upon Sod1 depletion in CySCs, we acknowledge that the effects may not be as apparent in the provided images. In response, we have expanded our quantification, included a statistical analysis of gstD1-GFP intensity specifically in GSCs and CySCs (Figure 1S), and added more representative images in the revised figure panels (Figure S1C-D’’’) to support our claims.

      In Figure 2, while the cell composition of the niche region does appear to be different from controls when SOD1 is knocked down in the CySCs, at least in the example images shown in Figures 2A and B, how cell type is quantified in figures 2E-G is very much unclear in the figure and methods. Are these counts of cells contacting the niche? If so, how was that defined? Or were additional regions away from the niche also counted and, if so, how were these regions defined?

      Thank you for your  regarding the quantification of cell types in Figures 2E-G. We counted all cells that were Tj-positive and Zfh1-positive in individual testis, while for GSCs, only those in direct contact with the hub were included. This clarification has been incorporated into the revised figure legend and methods (line no.400-407). We have now provided a clearer description in the text to improve transparency in our analysis.

      In Figure 3, it is quite interesting that there is an increase in Eya<sup>+</sup>, differentiating cyst cells in SOD1 knockdown animals, and that these Eya+ cells appear closer to the niche than in controls. However, this seems at odds with the proliferation data presented in Figure 2, since Eya<sup>+</sup> somatic cells do not normally divide at all. Are they suggesting that now differentiating cyst cells are proliferative? In addition, it is important for them to show example images of the changes in Socs36E and ptp61F expression.

      Thank you for your insightful observations. We acknowledge the apparent contradiction and appreciate the opportunity to clarify our interpretation.

      Regarding the increase in Eya<sup>+</sup> differentiating cyst cells in Sod1RNAi individuals and their proximity to the niche, we do not suggest that these differentiating cells are proliferative. Instead, we propose that the knockdown of Sod1 may alter the timing or regulation of cyst cell differentiation, leading to an accumulation of Eya<sup>+</sup> cells near the niche. To clarify this point, we have revised the manuscript (line no. 186-189) to emphasize that our proliferation data specifically refers to early-stage somatic cells, not Eya<sup>+</sup> differentiating cyst cells.

      We also appreciate the reviewer's request for example images illustrating the changes in Socs36E and Ptp61F expression. We could not access the antibodies specific to Socs36E and Ptp61F. Hence, we had to rely on the measurements were obtained using real-time PCR from the tip region of testis. We have clarified the same in the figure legends (line 700). 

      Overall, the various changes in signaling are quite puzzling-while Jak/Stat signaling from the niche is reduced, hh signaling appears to be increased. Similarly, while the authors conclude that premature differentiation occurs close to the niche, EGF signaling, which occurs from germ cells to cyst cells during differentiation, is decreased. Many times these, changes are contradictory, and the authors do not provide a suitable explanation to resolve these contradictions. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s thoughtful feedback on the signaling changes described in our study. We acknowledge that the observed alterations in Jak/Stat, Hedgehog (Hh), and EGF signaling may appear contradictory at first glance. However, our data suggest that these changes reflect a complex interplay between different signaling pathways that regulate cyst cell behavior in response to specific genetic perturbation.

      Regarding Jak/Stat and Hh signaling, while Jak/Stat activity is reduced in the niche, the increase in Hh signaling may reflect a compensatory mechanism or a context-dependent response of cyst cells to reduced Jak/Stat input. Prior studies have suggested that Hh signaling can function in parallel and independently of Jak/Stat signaling (PMID: 23175633) and our findings align with this possibility. 

      The reduction in EGFR signaling in this context appears contradictory to existing literature. One possible explanation is that, the altered GSC -CySC balance and loss of contact in Tj>Sod1i testes, leads to insufficient ligand response, thereby failing to activate EGFR signaling. (line no.222-224, 313-318). 

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s detailed feedback, which has helped refine our manuscript. In this study we have focussed on the role of ROS generated due to manipulation of Sod1 in the interplay between GSC and CySCs. In this regard, we have conducted additional experiments and incorporated quantitative data into the revised manuscript. Additionally, we have refined the text and provided further context to enhance the clarity. Key revisions include:

      (1) Clarification of Quantification Methods – We have refined intensity measurements by incorporating a membrane marker (Dlg) to better delineate cell boundaries and have normalized Ptc and Ci expression per cell to improve clarity.

      (2) Cell-Specific ROS Measurement – We separately measured ROS in germ cells and cyst cells and performed independent Sod1 depletion in GSCs to determine its direct effects.

      (3) Mitochondrial Analysis – We revised our approach, focusing on mitochondrial shape rather than asymmetric distribution, and removed overreaching claims.

      (4) Proliferation Analysis – We reanalyzed FUCCI data by normalizing to total cell count, supporting the conclusion that increased proliferation, rather than differentiation delay, underlies the observed phenotype.

      (5) E-Cad Quantification – We specifically analyzed E-Cad levels at the GSC-hub interface to strengthen conclusions on GSC attachment.

      (6) JAK/STAT Signaling – While we could not obtain a STAT92E antibody, we clarified the spatial limitations of our current analysis and revised the text accordingly.

      (7) Rescue Experiments and Gal4 Titration Control – We performed additional control experiments to confirm that observed effects are not due to Gal4 dilution.

      (8) Image Quality and Terminology Corrections – We enhanced figure resolution, corrected terminology (e.g., "cystic" to "cyst"), and revised ambiguous phrasing for clarity and accuracy.

      As suggested, we have also changed the manuscript title to better align with our results:

      Previous Manuscript Title: Non-autonomous cell redox-pairs dictate niche homeostasis in multi-lineage stem populations

      Updated Manuscript Title: Superoxide Dismutases maintain niche homeostasis in stem cell populations

      Specific responses to the reviewer’s: 

      While the decrease in pERK in CySCs is clear from the image and matched in the quantification, the increase in cyst cells is not apparent from the fire LUT used. The change in fluorescence intensity therefore may be that more cells have active ERK, rather than an increase per cell (similar arguments apply to the quantifications for p4E-BP or Ptc). Therefore, it is hard to know whether Sod1 knockdownresults in increased or decreased signaling in individual cells.

      Thank you for your insightful . To clarify, in the Fire LUT images, only pERK intensity is shown, not the cyst cell number. In our context, while there are more cells, the overall pERK intensity is lower, eliminating any ambiguity about whether the change is occurring per cell or due to an increased number of circulating cells. Moreover, for Ptc and Ci levels, we have normalized Ptc and Ci expression intensity per cell to enhance clarity and ensure an accurate interpretation of signaling changes.

      There are several places in which the authors could strengthen their manuscript by explaining the methods more clearly. For example, it is unclear how the intensity graphs in Figure 1Q are obtained. The curves appear smoothed and therefore unlikely to be from individual samples, but this is not clearly explained. However, this quantification method is clearly not helpful, as it shows the overlap between somatic and germline markers, suggesting it cannot accurately distinguish between the two cell types. Additionally, using a nuclear marker (Tj) for the cyst cells and cytoplasmic marker (Vasa) for the germ cells risks being misleading, as one would not expect much overlap between cytoplasmic gstD1-GFP and nuclear Tj. Also related to the methods, it is unclear how Vasa+ cells at the hub were counted. The methods suggest this was from a single plane, but this runs the risk of being arbitrary since GSCs can be distributed around the hub in 3D. (As a note, the label on the graph "Vasa+ cells" is misleading, as there are many more cells that are Vasa-positive than the ones counted.)

      We appreciate the reviewer’s careful evaluation of our manuscript and their insightful suggestions for improving the clarity of our methods. Below, we address each concern raised and describe the revisions made accordingly.

      Clarification of Intensity Graphs in Figure 1Q

      We have removed this graph, as we recognize that the markers previously used were not appropriate for distinguishing the different cell types. To address this concern, we have revised the text and now included a membrane marker discs-large (Dlg) in our revised Figure 1 and S1 to more clearly delineate cell boundaries. Due to technical difficulty associated with acquisition of images, we could not co-stain Vasa, Tj and Dlg together. Therefore, quantified the gstD-GFP intensity separately for GSCs and CySCs under similar acquisition conditions (Figure 1R).   

      Counting of Vasa<sup>+</sup> Cells at the Hub

      We appreciate the reviewer’s concern regarding our method for counting Vasa+ cells. In our original analysis, we included GSCs as the Vasa-positive cells that were in direct contact with the hub. To account for the three-dimensional arrangement of GSCs, we used the Cell counter plugin of Fiji and performed counting across different focal planes to ensure all hub-associated cells were considered. For better clarity on cell distribution around the hub, we have presented a single focal place image sliced through mid of the hub zone. To enhance transparency, we have now provided a more detailed explanation of our counting approach in the Methods section (line no 400- 403).

      We agree that the label "Vasa+ cells" may be misleading, as many cells express Vasa beyond the specific subset being counted. To address this, we have changed the label to " GSCs" to reflect the subset analyzed more accurately.

      The crucial experiment for this manuscript is presented in Figures 1 G-S, arguing that Sod1 knockdown with Tj-Gal4 increases gstD1-GFP expression in germ cells. This needs strengthening as the current quantifications are not convincing and appear to show an overlap between Tj (a nuclear cyst cell marker) and Vasa (a cytoplasmic germ cell marker). Labeling cell outlines would help, or alternatively, labeling different cell types genetically can be used to determine whether the expression is increased specifically within that cell type. Similarly, the measurement of ROS shown in the supplemental data should be conducted in a cell-specific manner. To clearly make the case that Sod1 knockdown in cyst cells is impacting ROS in the germline, it would be important to manipulate germ cell ROS independently. Without this, it will be difficult to prove that any effects observed are a result of increased ROS in the germline rather than indirect effects on the germline of altered cyst cell behaviour. 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful feedback regarding the specificity of Sod1 knockdown effects in germ cells and the need for clearer quantification in Figures 1G–S. Below, we address each concern and outline the modifications made:

      Clarification of Cell Type-Specific Expression:

      We acknowledge the overlap observed between Tj (nuclear cyst cell marker) and Vasa (cytoplasmic germ cell marker) in the presented images. To strengthen our claim that gstD1GFP expression increases specifically in germ cells upon Sod1 knockdown, we have now labelled cell outlines using membrane marker discs-large (Dlg) to better distinguish cell boundaries, along with individual labelling of Vasa<sup>+</sup> and Tj<sup>+</sup> cells. Due to technical difficulty associated with acquisition of images, we could not co-stain Vasa, Tj and Dlg together. 

      Cell-Specific Measurement of ROS:

      We agree that a cell-type-specific ROS measurement is critical to establishing a direct effect on germ cells. To address this, we have now performed ROS measurements separately in germ cells and cyst cells under similar acquisition conditions. These data are now included in the revised (Figure 1R). Similarly, upon CySC-specific Sod1 depletion, we performed measurement of gstD1-GFP intensity which was found to be enhanced in GSCs, along with expected increase in CySCs (Fig 1S). We have independently manipulated ROS levels in GSCs (Nos Gal4> Sod1i) and observed that elevated ROS negatively impacts GSCs, leading to a reduction in their number, while having an insignificant effect on adjacent CySCs.(Fig S2 E, F).

      Quantifications of mitochondrial localization in Figure 1 should include some adequate statistical method to evaluate whether the distribution is random or oriented towards the GSC/CySC interface. From the image provided (Figure 1B), it would appear that there are two clusters of mitochondria, on either side of a CySC nucleus, one cluster towards a GSC and one cluster away. Therefore evaluating bias would be important. Additional experiments will be necessary to support the statement that "Redox state of GSC is maintained by asymmetric distribution of CySC mitochondria". This would require manipulating mitochondrial distribution in CySCs.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion regarding the quantification of mitochondrial localization. We agree that ing on mitochondrial distribution might be far-fetched. In revised manuscript, we have demarcated the cell boundary and limited our analysis to mitochondrial shape which was found to be different in GSC and CySC (Fig. 1, D, F, G and S1B). Mitochondrial shape was quantified based on the mitochondrial area and circularity (Figure 1F and G). To prevent any misinterpretation, we have removed the statement, "Redox state of GSC is maintained by asymmetric distribution of CySC mitochondria."

      One point raised by the authors is that the increase of somatic cell numbers is driven by accelerated proliferation, based on an increased number of cells in various stages of the cell cycle as assessed by the FUCCI reporter. However, there are more somatic cells in this genetic background, so it could be argued that the observed increase in different phases of the cell cycle is due to an increased number of cells. In order to argue for an increased proliferation rate, the number of cells in each phase should be divided by the total number of cells, expecting to see an increase in S and G2/M phases along with a decrease in G1. Otherwise, the simplest explanation is a block or delay in differentiation, meaning that more cells remain in the cell cycle.

      We appreciate the  regarding the interpretation of our FUCCI reporter data. We acknowledge that the observed increase in the number of cells in various phases of the cell cycle could be influenced by the overall higher number of somatic cells in this genetic background.

      To address this concern, we have now re-analyzed our FUCCI data by normalizing the number of cells in each phase to the total number of cells and we did not observe a significant shift in the proportion of cells in S and G2/M phases relative to G1. This suggests presence of more proliferative cells, that is less cells in Go phase, rather than alterations in the timing of cell cycle progression stages. We are not sure about a block in differentiation because we see an enhanced accumulation of Eya+ cells near the niche. We have also supported our FUCCI data with pH3 staining where we have found more pH3+ spots under SOD1 depleted background. We have revised our manuscript accordingly (Figure 2I, K and S2U) to reflect this interpretation and appreciate the constructive feedback.

      In Figure 3, the authors claim that knockdown of Sod1 in the soma decreases the attachment of GSCs to the hub-based on lower E-Cad levels compared to controls. Previous work has shown that in GSCs, E-Cad localizes to the Hub-GSC interface (PMID: 20622868). Therefore, the authors should quantify E-Cad staining at the interphase between the germ cells and the niche.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s . As suggested, we have now quantified ECad staining specifically at the interface between the germ cells and the niche. Our analysis confirms that E-Cad levels are significantly reduced at this interphase upon Sod1 knockdown in the soma compared to controls, supporting our conclusion that Sod1 depletion affects GSC attachment to the hub as well as the whole niche. The revised Figure 3M now includes these quantifications, and we have updated the figure legend and results section accordingly.

      The authors show decreased expression of the JAK/STAT targets socs36E and ptp61F, arguing that this could be a reason for decreased GSC adhesion to the hub. However, these data were obtained from whole testes and lacked spatial resolution, whereas a STAT92E staining in control and tj>Sod1 RNAi testes could easily prove this point. Indeed, previous work has shown that socs36E is expressed in the CySCs, not GSCs (PMID: 19797664), suggesting that any decrease in JAK/STAT may be autonomous to the CySCs.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s observation regarding the spatial resolution of our JAK/STAT target expression analysis. To improve accuracy, we have attempted to collect only the tip of the testes while excluding the rest; however, we acknowledge that this approach may still obscure cell-specific changes. We had attempted to procure the STAT92E antibody but, despite multiple inquiries, we did not receive a positive response. While we agree that STAT92E staining would have strengthen our findings, we are currently unable to perform this experiment. Nevertheless, our observations align with prior work indicating that socs36E is predominantly expressed in CySCs (PMID: 19797664). We have revised the manuscript text accordingly to clarify this limitation.

      Additional considerations should be taken regarding the rescue experiments where PI3KDN and Hh RNAi are expressed in a Tj>Sod1 RNAi background. To rule out that any rescue can be attributed to titration of the Gal4 protein when an additional UAS sequence is present, a titration control would be useful. These pathways are not described accurately since Insulin signaling is necessary for the differentiation of somatic cells (not maintenance as written in the text), and its inhibition has been shown to increase the number of undifferentiated somatic cells (PMID:27633989). As far as Hh is concerned, the expression of this molecule is restricted to the niche. It would be important to establish whether the expression is altered in this case, especially as the authors rescue the Sod1 knockdown by also knocking down Hh. One possibility that the authors need to rule out is that some of the effects they observe are due to the knockdown of Sod1 (and/or Hh) in the hub as Tj-Gal4 is expressed in the hub as well as the CySCs (PMID:27546574).

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful s and suggestions. Below, we address each concern and describe the steps we have taken to incorporate the necessary modifications in our revised manuscript.

      Titration Control for Rescue Experiments  

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding potential Gal4 titration effects when introducing additional UAS constructs. To address this, we conducted a control experiment quantifying SOD1 levels in control, Tj > Sod1 RNAi, and Tj > Sod1 RNAi, UAS hhRNAi backgrounds using real-time PCR (Figure S4 M). The Sod1 levels in single and double UAS copy conditions were comparable, indicating that Gal4 titration does not significantly affect the results.

      Clarification of Insulin Signaling Role 

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insight regarding the involvement of insulin signaling in this context. Initially, we included data on PI3K/TOR as we found it intriguing. However, as the data didn’t add much to the overall observations, we have removed them to ensure clarity and prevent any potential confusion.

      Hh Expression and Niche Consideration 

      We recognize the importance of evaluating whether Hedgehog (Hh) expression is altered in the Sod1 RNAi background. We have already quantified hh in qRT-PCR (Figure S4C). 

      Potential Effects of Sod1 and Hh Knockdown in the Hub 

      We acknowledge the concern that Tj-Gal4 is expressed in both the hub and CySCs, potentially affecting hub function upon Sod1 and Hh knockdown. To address this, we have included additional data using the CySC-specific driver C-587 Gal4 to distinguish CySC-intrinsic effects from potential hub contributions. Our results show that while the phenotypic changes are consistent across both drivers, the effects are significantly stronger with Tj-Gal4, suggesting a role of the hub in this process. These findings have been incorporated into the revised manuscript (Fig S1G-H, M-N).

      In general, the GSCs (and other aspects) are difficult to see in the images; enlargements or higher-resolution images should be provided. Additionally, the manuscript contains several mistakes or inaccuracies (examples include referring to ROS having "evolved" in the abstract when it is cells that have evolved to use ROS, or the references to "cystic" cells when they are usually referred to as "cyst" cells, or that "CySCs also repress GSC differentiation by suppressing transcription of bag-of-marbles" when CySCs produce BMPs that lead to suppression of bam expression in the germline). These would need editing for both clarity and accuracy.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful feedback and have made the necessary revisions to address the concerns raised.

      Image Clarity and Resolution: 

      We have provided higher-resolution images in some of the revised images for better understanding. The revised figures now offer better clarity for key observations.

      Clarification of Terminology and Accuracy:

      The phrase regarding ROS in the abstract has been revised to reflect that cells have evolved to utilize ROS, rather than ROS itself evolving (line no. 27).

      References to "cystic" cells have been corrected to "cyst" cells for consistency with standard terminology.

      The statement about CySCs repressing GSC differentiation has been revised for accuracy, clarifying that CySCs produce BMPs, which lead to the suppression of bam expression in the germline (line no. 84).

      We have carefully reviewed the manuscript for any additional inaccuracies or ambiguities to ensure clarity and precision. We appreciate the reviewer’s constructive s, which have helped improve the manuscript.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      In response to Reviewer 3’s comments, we would like to highlight the point that in the present study we have focussed on the interplay between CySC and GSC and have accordingly conducted our experiments. We did observe some changes in the hub and do not rule out the effect of hub cells in exacerbating some of our phenotypes. We have included additional controls to highlight the effect of CySC ROS. These points have been appropriately discussed in the manuscript. Key revisions include:  

      (1)  Data Clarity & Visualization: To improve mitochondrial lineage association, we incorporated a membrane marker (Dlg) in Figure 1, enhancing the distinction between CySCs and GSCs. Additionally, we refined gstD-GFP quantifications in individual cell types and provided high-resolution images.

      (2) ROS Transfer & Measurement: We revised our discussion to acknowledge indirect ROS transfer mechanisms and added separate ROS quantifications in GSCs and CySCs, confirming higher ROS levels in CySCs (Figure 1R).

      (3) Tj-Gal4 Specificity & Niche Characterization: Recognizing Tj-Gal4 expression in hub cells, we included C587-Gal4 as a CySC-specific driver, demonstrating that hub cells contribute partially to the phenotype (Figure S1G,H,M,N).

      (4) Signaling Pathway Validation: We optimized dpERK staining, included controls (Tj>EGFRi), and clarified limitations regarding MAPK signaling. Due to lethality, we could not perform an EGFR gain-of-function rescue. We also validated increased Hh signaling via qPCR and a Tj>UAS Ci control (Figure S4).

      (5) Conceptual & Terminological Refinements: We revised our discussion of BMP signaling, ROS gradients, and testis-specific terminology. All figures and labels now accurately represent GSC scoring (single Vasa⁺ cells in contact with the niche).

      (6) Figure & Methods Improvements: We enhanced image resolution, provided grayscale versions where needed,and expanded Materials & Methods to clarify experimental conditions.

      These revisions strengthen our conclusions and address the reviewer’s concerns, ensuring a more precise and transparent presentation of our findings. To align with the reviewer’s s we have changed the title of the manuscript to “Superoxide Dismutases maintain niche homeostasis in stem cell populations”.

      Specific responses to the reviewer’s comments: 

      (1) Data

      a.  Problems proving which mitochondria are associated with which lineage.

      We acknowledge the challenge of distinguishing CySCs from GSCs without additional cell surface markers. To enhance clarity, we have incorporated the membrane marker Discs-large (Dlg) in our revised Figure 1 to better delineate cell boundaries, providing a clearer depiction of mitochondrial distribution in GSCs and CySCs.

      b.There is no evidence that ROS diffuses from CySCs into GSCs.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern. There are reports which talks about diffusion of ROS across cells on which we have included a few lines in the discussion (line no. 274-276). We do understand that our previous quantifications showed ROS diffusion from CySC to GSC rather indirectly. Therefore, in revised manuscript we have measured ROS separately in the two cell populations. We found that the CySCs show higher ROS profile than GSCs (Fig 1R).  

      c.The changes in GST-GFP (redox readout) are possibly seen in differentiating germ cells (i.e., spermatogonia) but not in GSCs. This weakens their model that ROS in CySC is transferred to GSCs.

      Thank you for your observation. We acknowledge that the changes in gstD-GFP (redox readout) are more prominent in differentiating germ cells. It is known that differentiating cells show higher ROS profile than the stem cells. Hence, expectedly the intensity of gstDGFP was lesser in stem cell zone compared to the differentiating zone. In our manuscript we are focussed on the redox state among stem cell populations. Therefore, we have included better quality images and measured the gstD1-GFP intensity individually in GSCs and CySCs (Figure 1R) by demarcating the cell boundaries (Figure 1M, S1C-D’’’). We found that CySCs show higher ROS profile than GSCs and enhancement of ROS in CySC by Sod1 depletion resulted in a consequent increase in ROS in GSCs. We believe this revision strengthens our model by addressing the potential discrepancy and providing a more comprehensive understanding of ROS dynamics within the GSC niche.

      d.Most of the paper examines the effect of SOD depletion (which should increase ROS) on the CySC lineage and GSC lineage. One big caveat is that Tj-Gal4 is expressed in hub cells (Fairchild, 2016), so the loss of SOD from hub cells may also contribute to the phenotype. In fact, the niche in Figure 2D looks larger than the niche in the control in Figure 2C, arguing that the expression of Tj in niche cells may be contributing to the phenotype. The authors need to better characterize the niche in tj>SOD-RNAi testes.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful  regarding the potential contribution of hub cell to the observed phenotype. We acknowledge that Tj-Gal4 is expressed in hub cells and this could influence the niche size and overall phenotype.

      To address this concern, we have included an additional control using C587-Gal4, a CySC specific driver, to distinguish CySC-specific effects from potential hub contributions. All the effects on cell number observed in Tj>Sod1i was replicated in C587>Sod1i testis, except that the observed phenotypes were comparatively weaker. These indicate partial contribution of hub cells to the observed phenotype, exacerbating its severity. However, the effect of Sod1 depletion in CySC on GSC lineages remains significant. These findings have been incorporated into Figure S1- G,H,M and N) and incorporated in the discussion (line no.308311). 

      e. The Tj>SOD1-RNAi phenotype is an expansion of the Zfh1<sup+</sup> CySC pool, expansion of the Tj<sup>+</sup> Zfh1- cyst cells (both due to increased somatic proliferation) and a non-autonomous disruption of the germline.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s observation. Our data confirm that Tj>SOD-RNAi leads to an expansion of both Zfh1<sup+</sup> CySCs and Tj<sup>+</sup> Zfh1- cyst cells, which we attribute to increased somatic proliferation. Additionally, we observe a non-autonomous disruption of the germline, likely due to dysregulated signaling from the altered somatic niche.

      f. I am not convinced that MAPK signaling is decreased in tj>SOD-i testes. Not only is this antibody finicky, but the authors don't have any follow-up experiments to see if they can restore SOD-depleted CySCs by expressing an EGFR gain of function. Additionally, reduced EGFR activity causes fewer somatic cells (not more) (Amoyel, 2016) and also inhibits abscission between GSCs and gonial blasts (Lenhart 2015), which causes interconnected cysts of 8- to 16 germ cells with one GSC emanating from the hub.

      We acknowledge that the dpERK antibody can be challenging. We took necessary precautions, including optimizing staining conditions and using positive control (Tj>EGFRi) (Figure: S4B). Our results consistently showed a decrease in dpERK levels in Tj>Sod1i testes, supporting our conclusion.

      We agree that inclusion of an experiment using EGFR gain-of-function to rescue the effects of CySC-Sod1 depletion would have strengthened our findings. We had attempted this experiment; however, the progenies constitutively expressing EGFR under Sod1RNAi background were lethal, preventing us from completing the analysis.

      We agree that our observations do not align with the reported effects of EGFR signaling on somatic cell numbers and abscission and we appreciate the references provided. Based on our observations, we feel that modulation of MAPK signaling in the niche probably, happens in a context-dependent manner. One possible explanation is that, the altered GSC -CySC balance and loss of contact in Tj>Sod1i testes, leads to insufficient ligand response, thereby failing to activate EGFR signaling. While it is well established that ROS can enhance EGFR signaling to promote cellular proliferation and early differentiation, our results indicate a more nuanced regulation in this context. However, further detailed analysis is required to completely understand the regulatory controls. We have clarified this point in the manuscript (line no.

      313-320).

      g. The increase in Hh signaling in SOD-depleted CySCs would increase their competitiveness against GSCs and GSCs would be lost (Amoyel 2014). The authors need to validate that Hh protein expression is indeed increased in SOD-depleted CySCs/cyst cells and which cells are producing this Hh. Normally, only hub cells produce Hh (Michel,2012; Amoyel 2013) to promote self-renewal in CySCs.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion regarding the validation of Hh protein expression and its source. Since Tj-Gal4 is expressed in the hub, it is likely activating the Hh pathway and promoting CySC proliferation. Unfortunately, we could not procure Hh antibody to directly assess its protein levels. However, to address this, we performed real-time PCR from RNA derived from the tip region and found a significant increase in hh mRNA levels in SOD-depleted cyst cells. These findings support our hypothesis that elevated Hh signaling enhances CySC competitiveness, leading to GSC loss. To support this idea, we have included a Tj>Ci positive control which caused abnormal proliferation of Tj<sup>+</sup> cells resulted in ablation of GSCs. We have incorporated these results in the revised manuscript (Results section, Figure S-4).

      h.The increase in p4E-BP is an indication that Tor signaling is increased, but an increase in Tor in the CySC lineage does not significantly affect the number of CySCs or cyst cells (Chen, 2021). So again I am not sure how increased Tor factors into their phenotype.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding the role of increased Tor signaling in our phenotype. The observed increase in Tor could indeed be a downstream effect of elevated ROS levels. However, establishing a direct causal relationship between Sod1 and Tor would require additional experiments, which we feel might be a good study in its own merit. To maintain clarity and focus in the revised manuscript, we have opted not to include this preliminary data at this stage.

      I.The over-expression of SOD in CySCs part is incomplete. The authors would need to monitor ROS in these testes. They would also need to examine with tj>SOD affects the size of the hub.

      We value the reviewer's . To address this, we have now monitored ROS levels in the testes upon SOD overexpression in CySCs using DHE (Figure S5 I). Our results indicate a significant reduction in ROS levels compared to controls. 

      Additionally, we examined hub size upon Sod1 overexpression and observed a slight, but statistically insignificant, reduction. As our study primarily focuses on ROS-mediated GSCCySC interactions, we did not include a detailed investigation on hub size regulation.

      (2) Concept

      Why would it be important to have a redox gradient across adjacent cells? The authors mention that ROS can be passed between cells, but it would be helpful for them to provide more details about where this has been documented to occur and what biological functions ROS transfer regulates.

      We thank the reviewer for this insightful . We acknowledge that the concept of a redox gradient was not adequately conveyed, as the cell boundary was not clearly defined. To address this, we have revised our interpretation to propose that high ROS levels in one cell may influence the ROS levels in an adjacent cell through either direct transfer or as a secondary effect of altered niche maintenance signaling, rather than through the establishment of a gradient.

      Regarding ROS transfer between cells, it has been documented in several biological contexts. For instance, hydrogen peroxide (H<sub>2</sub>O<sub>2</sub>) can diffuse through aquaporins, influencing signaling pathways in neighbouring cells (PMID: 17105724). We have incorporated these details and relevant references into the revised manuscript to enhance the conceptual understanding of ROS transfer. 

      (3) Issues with the scholarship of the testis

      a. Line 82 - There is no mention of BMPs, which are the only GSC-self-renewal signal. Upd/Jak/STAT is required for the adhesion of GSCs to the niche but not self-renewal (Leatherman and Dinardo, 2008, 2010). The author should read a review about the testis. I suggest Greenspan et al 2015. The scholarship of the testis should be improved.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s feedback regarding the role of BMPs in GSC selfrenewal, we have added this in the revised manuscript (line no. 83) We have now incorporated a discussion on BMP signaling as the primary self-renewal signal for GSCs, distinguishing it from the role of Upd/JAK/STAT in niche adhesion, as highlighted in Leatherman and Dinardo (2010). Additionally, we have cited and reviewed the work by Greenspan et al. (2015) and ensure a more comprehensive discussion of GSC regulation. These revisions can be found in the line no. 285-289 of the revised manuscript.

      b. Line 82-84 - BMPs are produced by both hub cells and CySCs. BMP signaling in GSCs represses bam. So it is not technically correct to say the CySCs repress bam expression in GSCs.

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s clarification regarding BMP signaling and its role in repressing bam expression in GSCs. We have revised the relevant section (line no.83-85). 

      c.Throughout the figures the authors score Vasa<sup>+</sup> cells for GSCs. This is technically not correct. What they are counting is single, Vasa<sup>+</sup> cells in contact with the niche. All graphs should be updated with the label "GSCs" on the Y-axis.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s careful assessment of our methodology. We acknowledge that scoring Vasa⁺ cells alone does not definitively identify GSCs. Our quantification specifically considers single Vasa<sup>⁺</sup> cells in direct contact with the niche. To ensure clarity and accuracy, we have updated all figure legends and Y-axis labels in the relevant graphs to explicitly state "GSCs" instead of "Vasa⁺ cells."

      (4) Issues with the text

      a. Line 1: multi-lineage is not correct. Multi-lineage refers to stem cells that produce multiple types of daughter cells. GSCs produce only one type of offspring and CySCs produce only one type of offspring. So both are uni-lineage. Please change accordingly.

      We acknowledge the incorrect usage of "multi-lineage" and agree that both GSCs and CySCs are uni-lineage, as they each produce only one type of offspring. We have revised Line 1 accordingly and also updated the title. 

      b. Lines 62-75 - Intestinal stem cells have constitutively high ROS (Jaspar lab paper), so low ROS in stem cell cells is not an absolute.

      We appreciate the clarification. We have revised Lines 62–75 to acknowledge that low ROS is not universal in stem cells, citing the Jaspar lab study on intestinal stem cells (Line 70). Thank you for the valuable insight.

      c.  Line 79: The term cystic is not used in the Drosophila testis. There are cyst stem cells (CySCs) that produce cyst cells. Please revise.

      We have revised the text to replace "cystic" with the correct terminology, referring to cyst stem cells (CySCs) in the manuscript.

      d. Line 90 - perfectly balanced is an overstatement and should be toned down.

      Thank you for the suggestion. We have revised it to “balanced” instead of "perfectly balanced."  

      e. Line 98 - division of labour is not supported by the data and should be rephrased.

      Thank you for the feedback. We have rephrased it (line no. 98-101) to avoid the term "division of labor".

      f. Line 200 - the authors provide no data on BMPs - the GSC self-renewal cue - so they should avoid discussing an absence of self-renewal cues.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s point. We have revised it to avoid discussing the absence of self-renewal cues, given that we do not present data on BMP signaling. This ensures that our conclusions remain within the scope of the provided data.

      (5) Issues with the figures

      a The images are too small to appreciate the location of mitochondria in GSCs and CySCs.

      b. Figure 1

      c. cell membranes are not marked, reducing the precision of assigning mitochondria to GSC or CySCs. It would be very helpful if the authors depleted ATP5A from GSCs and showed that the puncta are reduced in these cells, and did a similar set of experiments for the Tj-Gal4 lineage. It would also be very helpful if the authors expressed membrane markers (like myrGFP) in the GSC and then in the CySC lineage and then stained with ATP5A. This would pinpoint in which cells ATP5A immunoreactivity is occurring.

      d. The presumed changes in gst-GFP (redox readout) are possibly seen in differentiating germ cells (i.e.,spermatogonia) but not in GSC. iii. Panels F, Q, and S are not explained and currently are irrelevant.

      e. Figure 3K - The evidence to support less Ecad in GSCs in tj>SOD-i testes is not compelling as the figure is too small and the insets show changes in Ecad in somatic cells, not GSC. d. Figure 4:

      f. Panel A, B The apparent decline (not quantified) may not contribute to the phenotype.

      ii.dpERK is a finicky antibody and the authors are showing a single example of each genotype. This is an important experiment because the authors are going to use it to conclude that MAPK is decreased in the tj>SOD-i samples. However, the authors don't have any positive (dominantactive EGFR) or negative (tj>mapk-i). As is standing, the data is not compelling. The graph in F does not convey any useful information.

      g. Figure S1D - cannot discern green on black. It is critical for the authors to show monochromes (grayscale) for thereabouts that they want to emphasize. I cannot see the green on black in Figure S1D.

      h. Figure S4 - there is no quantification of the number of Tj cells in K-N.

      We appreciate your detailed feedback regarding the figures in our manuscript. Below, we address each concern and outline the revisions we have made.

      (a) Image Size and Mitochondrial Localization in GSCs and CySCs 

      We acknowledge the need for larger images to better visualize mitochondrial localization. We have now increased the resolution and size of the images in Figure 1. Additionally, we have included high-magnification insets to enhance clarity (Figure 1 B#)

      (b) Figure 1 B,B#,C 

      (i) We have now marked cell membranes using Dlg to improve the precision of mitochondrial assignment to GSCs and CySCs and then stained for ATP5A, which clearly demarcates ATP5A immunoreactivity in specific cell types.

      (ii) We have revisited the gstD-GFP (redox readout) data and now provide revised images (Figure S1C-D’’’) and quantification (Figure 1 R,S) to better illustrate changes in the redox state. It is indeed intense in differentiating germ cells as expected but also present in the stem cell zone.

      (iii) Panels F, Q, and S have now been removed in the revised figure legend. 

      (C) Figure 3K: We have digitally magnified the figure size and improved contrast to better visualize E-cadherin levels. The insets have been revised to ensure they focus specifically on GSCs rather than somatic cells. Earlier, we quantified the E-cadherin intensity changes in the GSC-hub interface and provided statistical analysis to support our findings (Figure 3M).

      (d) Figure 4: (i) Panels A and B have now been quantified, and we provide statistical comparisons to support our observations. (ii) We acknowledge the variability of dpERK staining. To strengthen our conclusions, we have provided negative (Tj>MAPK-i) controls (Figure S4 B). Additionally, we have removed panel F (MAPK area cover) to avoid confusion.

      (e) We appreciate the suggestion regarding grayscale images and have provided the monochrome images for mitochondria and gstD-GFP image representation. We have now removed Figure S1D as it was no longer required.

      (f) Figure S4: The quantification of the number of Tj-positive cells was actually included in the main figure along with statistical analysis.

      (g) We sincerely appreciate the reviewer’s insightful s, which have significantly improved the quality and clarity of our manuscript. We hope that our revisions adequately address the concerns raised.

      (6) Issues with Methods

      a.  Materials and Methods are not described in sufficient depth - please revise.

      b.  Note that Tj-Gal4 has real-time expression in hub cells and this is not considered by the authors. The ideal genotype for targeting CySCs is Tj-Gal4, Gal80TS, hh-Gal80. Additionally, the authors do not mention whether they are depleting throughout development into adulthood or only in adults. If the latter, then they must have used a temperature shift, growing the flies at 18C and then upshifting to 25C or 29C during adult stages.

      c.  The authors need to show data points in all of the graphs. Some graphs do this but others do not.

      d.  The authors state that all data points are from three biological replicates. This is not sufficient for GSC and CySC counts. Most labs count GSCs and CySCs from at least 10 testes of the correct genotype.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s valuable feedback and have made the necessary revisions to improve the clarity and rigor of our study. Below, we address each concern in detail:

      Materials and Methods

      We have revised the Materials and Methods section to provide a more detailed description of the experimental procedures, including genotypes, sample preparation, and quantification methods.

      Tj-Gal4 Expression and Experimental Design

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s point regarding Tj-Gal4 expression in hub cells. While Tj-Gal4 is active in hub cells, our focus was on CySCs, and we have now included a discussion of this caveat in the revised manuscript (line no. 308-311)

      Thank you for your suggestion on the ideal genotype for targeting CySCs. While we attempted to procure hh-Gal80, we couldn’t manage to get it, so we opted for another well-established Gal4 driver, C-587 Gal4, to target CySCs. Our results indicate that although the phenotypic changes are consistent across both drivers, the effects are significantly stronger with Tj-Gal4, highlighting the role of CySCs in this process with partial contributions from the hub. These findings have been incorporated into the revised manuscript (lines 309–311).

      We now clarify whether gene depletion was conducted throughout development or restricted to adulthood. For adult-specific depletion using the UAS-Gal4 system, crosses were set up at 25°C, and after two days, progenies were shifted to 29°C and aged for 3–5 days at 29°C. This process is now explicitly detailed in the revised Methods section (line no. 345-348).

      Data Presentation in Graphs

      We have updated all graphs to ensure that individual data points are shown consistently across all figures.

      Sample Size for GSC and CySC Counts

      We acknowledge the reviewer’s concern regarding biological replicates. Our initial study was based on 10 biological replicates, each set consisting of at least 7-8 testes per genotype, in line with standard practice in the field. This change is reflected in the revised Results and Methods sections.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Comments:

      (1) HCC shows heterogeneity, and it is unclear what tissues (tumor or normal) were used from the DKO mice and human HCC gene expression dataset to obtain the gene signature, and how the authors reconcile these gene signatures with HCC prognosis.

      Mice studies: Aged DKO mice develop aggressive tumors (major and minor nodules, See Figure 1), and the entire liver is burdened with multiple tumor nodules. It is technically challenging to demarcate the tumor boundaries as most of the surrounding tissues do not display normal tissue architecture. Therefore, livers from age- and sex-matched wild-type C57/BL6 mice were used as control tissue. All the mice were inbred in our facility. Spatial transcriptomics and longitudinal studies are ongoing to collect tumors at earlier time points wherein we can differentiate tumor and non-tumor tissue.

      Human Studies: We mined five separate clinical data sets. The human HCC gene expression comprised of samples from the (i) National Cancer Institute (NCI) cohort (GEO accession numbers, GSE1898 and GSE4024) and (ii) Korea, (iii) Samsung, (iv) Modena, and (v) Fudan cohorts as previously described (GEO accession numbers, GSE14520, GSE16757, GSE43619, GSE36376, and GSE54236). We have added a new supplemental table 4, giving details of these datasets. Depending on the cohort, they are primarily HCC samples- surgical resections of HCC, control samples, with some tumors and paired non-tumor tissues.

      (2) The authors identified a unique set of gene expression signatures that are linked to HCC patient outcomes, but analysis of these gene sets to understand the causes of cancer promotion is still lacking. The studies of urea cycle metabolism and estrogen signaling were preliminary and inconclusive. These mechanistic aspects may be followed up in revision or future studies.

      We agree. Experiments to elicit HCC causality and promotion are complex, given the heterogeneous nature of liver cancer. Moreover, the length of time (12 months) needed to spontaneously develop cancer in this DKO mouse model makes it challenging. As mentioned by the reviewer, mechanistic studies are ongoing, and longitudinal time course experiments are actively being pursued to delineate causality. Having said that, we mined the TCGA LIHC (The Cancer Genome Atlas Liver Hepatocellular Carcinoma) database to examine the expression of the individual urea cycle genes and found them suppressed in liver tumorigenesis (new Supplementary Figure 4). We also evaluated if estrogen receptor a (Era) targets altered in DKO females (DKO_Estrogen) correlate with overall survival in HCC (new Supplementary Figure 6). We note that Era expression per se is reduced in males and females upon liver tumorigenesis. Also, DKO_Estrogen signature positively corroborated with better overall survival (new Supplementary Figure 6). These findings further bolster the relevance of urea cycle metabolism and estrogen signaling during HCC.

      (3) While high levels of bile acids are convincingly shown to promote HCC progression, their role in HCC initiation is not established. The DKO model may be limited to conditions of extremely high levels of organ bile acid exposure. The DKO mice do not model the human population of HCC patients with various etiology and shared liver pathology (i.e. cirrhosis). Therefore, high circulating bile acids may not fully explain the male prevalence of HCC incidence.

      We agree with this comment that our studies do not show bile acids can initiate HCC and may act as one of the many factors that contribute to the high male prevalence of HCC. This is exactly the reason why throughout the manuscript we do not write about HCC initiation. To clarify further, in the revised discussion of the manuscript, we have added a sentence to highlight this aspect, “while this study demonstrates bile acids promote HCC progression it does not investigate or provide evidence if excess bile acids are sufficient for HCC initiation.”

      (4) The authors showed lower circulating bile acids and increased fecal bile acid excretion in female mice and hypothesized that this may be a mechanism underlying the lower bile acid exposure that contributed to lower HCC incidence in female DKO mice. Additional analysis of organ bile acids within the enterohepatic circulation may be performed because a more accurate interpretation of the circulating bile acids and fecal bile acids can be made in reference to organ bile acids and total bile acid pool changes in these mice.

      As shown in this manuscript- we provide BA compositional analyses from the liver, serum, urine, and feces (Figures 5 and 6, new Supplementary Figure 8, Supplementary Tables 4 and 5). Unfortunately, we did not collect the intestinal tissue or gallbladders for BA analysis in this study. Separate cohorts of mice are being aged for future BA analyses from different organs within the enterohepatic loop. We thank you for this suggestion. Nevertheless, we have previously measured and reported BA values to be elevated in the intestines and the gall bladder of young DKO mice (PMC3007143).

      Reviewer #2 (Public review)

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The translational value to human HCC is not so strong yet. Authors show that there is a correlation between the female-selective gene signature and low-grade tumors and better survival in HCC patients overall. However, these data do not show whether this signature is more highly correlated with female tumor burden and survival. In other words, whether the mechanisms of female protection may be similar between humans and mice. In that respect, it would also be good to elaborate on whether women have higher fecal BA excretion and lower serum BA concentration.

      The reviewer poses an interesting question to test if the DKO female-specific signatures are altered differently in male vs. female HCC samples. As we found the urea cycle and estrogen signaling to be protective and enriched in our mouse model, we tested their expression pattern using the TCGA-LIHC RNA-seq data. We found urea cycle genes and Era transcripts broadly reduced in tumor samples irrespective of the sex (new Supplementary Figure 4 and Supplementary Figure 6), indicating that these pathways are compromised upon tumorigenesis even in the female livers.

      While prior studies have shown (i) a smaller BA pool w synthesis in men than women (PMID: 22003820), we did not find a study that systematically investigated BA excretion between the sexes in HCC context. The reviewer is spot on in suggesting BA analysis from HCC and unaffected human fecal samples from both sexes. Designing and performing such studies in the future will provide concrete proof of whether BA excretion protects female livers from developing liver cancer. We thank you for these suggestions.

      (2) The authors should perform a thorough spelling and grammar check.

      We apologize for the typos, which have been fixed, and as suggested by the reviewer, we have performed a grammar check.

      (3) There are quite some errors and inaccuracies in the result section, figures, and legends. The authors should correct this.

      We apologize for the inadvertent errors in the manuscript, and we have clarified these inaccuracies in the revised version. Thank you.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript by Xie and colleagues presents transcriptomic experiments that measure gene expression in eight different tissues taken from adult female and male mice from four species. These data are used to make inferences regarding the evolution of sex-biased gene expression across these taxa.

      Strengths:

      The experimental methods and data analysis appear appropriate. The authors promote their study as unprecedented in its size and technical precision.

      We do not understand the statement "the authors promote" as if there was a doubt about this. If there is a doubt, we welcome to see it specified.

      Weaknesses:

      The manuscript does not present a clear set of novel evolutionary conclusions. The major findings recapitulate many previous comparative transcriptomics studies - gene expression variation is prevalent between individuals, sexes, and species; and genes with sex-biased expression evolve more rapidly than genes with unbiased expression - but it is not clear how the study extends our understanding of gene expression or its evolution.

      There have been no "previous comparative transcriptomics studies" at a micro- evolutionary scale in animals, hence, we do not "replicate" these. And our contrast between somatic and gonadal patterns reveals insights that have not been recognized before, namely that gonadal sex-specific expression turnover is actually not faster that the corresponding non-sex-specific truover. We have now further clarified this distinction throughout the text and have also adapted the title of the paper accordingly.

      We agree with the overall statement that "gene expression variation is prevalent between individuals, sexes, and species" but the aspect of "sex-biased gene expression between individuals" has not been systematically analysed before in such a context.

      Concerning the statement that "genes with sex-biased expression evolve more rapidly than genes with unbiased expression", we note that this is mostly derived from gonadal data and that there is no study that has quantified this so far at a population level and between subspecies in comparison to somatic data.

      Our results show further that previous assumptions of a substantial set of genes with sex- biased expression conserved between mice and humans are due to underestimating the convergence issues when there is an extremly fast turnover of sex-biased gene expression. This has a major implication for using mice as a model for gender-speficic medicine questions in humans.

      Many gene expression differences between individual animals are selectively neutral, because these differences in mRNA concentration are buffered at the level of translation, or differences in protein abundance have no effect on cellular or organismal function. The hypothesis that sex-biased genes are enriched for selectively neutral expression differences is supported by the excess of inter-individual expression variance and inter-specific expression differences in sex-biased genes.

      This statement repeats a statement from the first round of reviews. We had added new data and extensive discussion on this topic. We do not understand why this has not been taken into account. In fact, a major strength of our paper is that it shows that most sex- biased gene expression differences are not neutral!

      There are two major issues here: to identify sex-biased gene expression in the first place, we (and all other papers in the field) use the neutral model as null-hypothesis. Genes that are not compatible with this null-hypothesis are considered sex-biased. In contrast to most previous papers, we have the possibility to take into account the variances between individuals to add an additional significance test. Hence, we can apply a much more rigorous two-step process: first a ratio-cutoff plus a Wilcoxon rank sum test with correction for multiple testing to identify significant deviations from the null-hypothesis. We have added some additional statements in the Results and Discussion sections to emphasize this.Second, by focusing on the genes that are not following a neutral model, the variance and divergences data support the action of selection, rather than neutral drift.

      A higher rate of adaptive coding evolution is inferred among sex-biased genes as a group, but it is not clear whether this signal is driven by many sex-biased genes experiencing a little positive selection, or a few sex-biased genes experiencing a lot of positive selection, so the relationship between expression and protein-coding evolution remains unclear.

      Again, there are two major issues here. First, the distribution of alpha-values shown in Figure 3B are rather homogeneous, i.e. there is not support for a scenario that the average is driven by only a few genes.

      Second, it seems that the referee wants to see an analysis where dn/ds ratios are broken down for every single gene. This has been done in previous papers, but it is now understood that this procedure is fraught with error because of the demographic contingencies inherent to natural populations that can yield wrong results for individual loci. We have added some statements to the text to clarify this further.

      It is likely that only a subset of the gene expression differences detected here will have phenotypic effects relevant for fitness or medicine, but without some idea of how many or which genes comprise this subset, it is difficult to interpret the results in this context.

      It is the basic underlying assumption for the whole research field that significantly sex- biased genes are phenotypically relevant for fitness, since they would otherwise not be sex- biased in the first place.

      Throughout the paper the concepts of sexual selection and sexually antagonistic selection are conflated; while both modes of selection can drive the evolution of sexually dimorphic gene expression, the conditions promoting and consequence of both kinds of selection are different, and the manuscript is not clear about the significance of the results for either mode of selection.

      We had explained in our previous response that our data collection was not designed to distinguish between these two processes. But given that the issue is being brought up again, we have now added some discussion on this issue.

      The manuscript's conclusion that "most of the genetic underpinnings of sex-differences show no long-term evolutionary stability" is not supported by the data, which measured gene expression phenotypes but did not investigate the underlying genetic variation causing these differences between individuals, sexes, or species.

      We agree that - under a strict definition - our use of the term "genetic underpinning" in this conclusion sentence can be criticized. The most correct term would be "transcriptional underpinnings", but of course, given that it is the current practice of the whole field to assume that "transcriptional" is part of the overall genetics, we do not consider our initial statement as incorrect. Still, we have changed the term accordingly.

      Furthermore, most of the gene expression differences are observed between sex-specific organs such as testes and ovaries, which are downstream of the sex-determination pathway that is conserved in these four mouse species, so these conclusions are limited to gene expression phenotypes in somatic organs shared by the sexes.

      Yes - correct. But the whole focus of the paper is on somatic expression, i.e. organs that share the same cell compositions. Of course, the comparison between gonadal organs is conflated by being composed of different cell types. We have extended the discussion of this point.

      The differences between sex-biased expression in mice and humans are attributed to differences in the two species effective population sizes; but the human samples have significantly more environmental variation than the mouse samples taken from age-matched animals reared in controlled conditions, which could also explain the observed pattern.

      These are indeed the two alternative explanations that we had discussed (last paragraph of the discussion section, now the penultimate paragraph).

      The smoothed density plots in Figure 5 are confusing and misleading. Examining the individual SBI values in Table S9 reveals that all of the female and male SBI values for each species and organ are non-overlapping, with the exception of the heart in domesticus and mammary gland in musculus, where one male and one female individual fall within the range of the other sex. The smoothed plots therefore exaggerate the overlap between the sexes;

      Smoothing across discrete values is an entirely standard procedure for continuous variables. It allows to visualize the inherent data trends that cannot easily be glanced from simple inspection of the actual values. This is a mathematical procedure, not an "exaggeration". We used the same smoothening procedure for all the comparisons, and it is clear that the distributions between females and males of the sex organs and a few somatic organs are well separated (non-overlapping), which serves as a control.

      in particular, the extreme variation shown in the SBI in the mammary glands in spretus females and spicilegus males is hard to understand given the normalized values in Table S3. The R code used to generate the smoothed plots is not included in the Github repository, so it is not possible to independently recreate those plots from the underlying data.

      We apologize that there was indeed an error in the Figure - the columns for SPR and SPI were accidentally interchanged. We have corrected this figure. Generally, the smoothened patterns we show are easily verified by looking up the respective primary values. We apologize that the code lines for the plots were accidentally omitted. We have used a standard function from ggplot2: geom_density, with "adjust=3, alpha=0.5" for all plots and included this description in the Methods. We have now added this to the R code in the GitHub repository.

      The correlations provided in Table S9 are confusing - most of the reported correlations are 1.0, which are not recovered when using the SBI values in Table S9, and which does not support the manuscript's assertion that sex-biased gene expression can vary between organs within an individual. Indeed, using the SBI values in Table S9, many correlations across organs are negative, which is expected given the description of the result in the text.

      There is a misunderstanding here. The tables do not report correlations, but only p-values for correlations, the raw ones and the ones after corrections for multiple testing. P = 1.0 means no significant correlation. We have adjusted the caption of this table to clarify this further.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      This manuscript reports interesting data on sex differences in expression across several somatic and reproductive tissues among 4 mice species or subspecies. The focus is on sex- biased expression in the somatic tissues, where the authors report high rates of turnover such that the majority of sex-biased genes are only sex-biased in one or two taxa. The authors show sex-biased genes have higher expression variance than unbiased genes but also provide some evidence that sex-bias is likely to evolve from genes with higher expression variance. The authors find that sex-biased genes (both female- and male-biased) experience more adaptive evolution (i.e., higher alpha values) than unbiased genes. The authors develop a summary statistic (Sex-Bias Index, SBI) of each individual's degree of sex- bias for a given tissue. They show that the distribution of SBI values often overlap considerably for somatic (but not reproductive) tissues and that SBI values are not correlated across tissues, which they interpret as indicating an individual can be relatively "male-like" in one tissue and relatively "female-like" in another tissue.

      This is a good summary of the data, but we are puzzled that it does not include the completely new module analysis and the finding of extremely fast evolution of sex-biased somatic gene expression compared to the gonadal one.

      Though the data are interesting, there are some disappointing aspects to how the authors have chosen to present the work. For example, their criteria for sex-bias requires an expression ratio of one sex to the other of 1.25. A reasonably large fraction of the "sex- biased genes" have ratios just beyond this cut-off (Fig. S1). A gene which has a ratio of 1.27 in taxa 1 can be declared as "sex-biased" but which has a ratio of 1.23 in taxa 2 will not be declared as "sex-biased". It is impossible to know from how the data are presented in the main text the extent to which the supposed very high turnover represents substantial changes in dimorphic expression. A simple plot of the expression sex ratio of taxa 1 vs taxa 2 would be illuminating but the authors declined this suggestion.

      Choosing a cutoff is the standard practice when dealing with continuously distributed data. As we have pointed out, we looked at various cutoff options and decided to use the present one, based on the observed data distributions. Note that some studies have used even lower ones (e.g. 1.1). To visualize the data distribution, we had provided the overall distribution of ratios, because one would have to look at many more plots otherwise. But we have now also added individual plots as Figure 1, Figure supplement 2, as requested. They confirm what is also evident from the overall plots, namely that most ratio changes are larger than the incremental values suggested by the reviewer. Note that the original data are of course also available for inspection.

      I was particularly intrigued by the authors' inference of the proportion of adaptive substitutions ("alpha") in different gene sets. The show alpha is higher for sex-biased than unbiased genes and nicely shows that the genes that are unbiased in focal taxa but sex- biased in the sister taxa also have low alpha. It would be even stronger that sex-bias is associated with adaptive evolution to estimate alpha for only those genes that are sex- biased in the focal taxa but not in the sister taxa (the current version estimates alpha on all sex-biased genes within the focal taxa, both those that are sex-biased and those that are unbiased in the sister taxa).

      We have added the respective values in the results section, but since fewer genes are involved, they are less comparable to the other sets of genes. Still, the tendencies remain.

      The author's Sex Bias Index is measured in an individual sample as: SBI = median(TPM of female-biased genes) - median(TPM of male-biased genes). This index has some strange properties when one works through some toy examples (though any summary statistic will have limitations). The authors do little to jointly discuss the merits and limitations of this metric. It would have been interesting to examine their two key points (degree of overlapping distributions between sexes and correlation across tissues) using other individual measures of sex-bias.

      We had responded to this comment before (including the explanation that it has no strange properties when one applies the normalization that is now implemented) and we have added a whole section devoted to the discussion of the merits of the SBI. We do not know which other "individual measures of sex-bias" this should be compared to. Still, we have now added a paragraph in the discussion about using PCA as an alternative to show that this would result in similar conclusions, but is technically less suitable for this purpose.

      Figure 5 shows symmetric gaussian-looking distributions of SBI but it makes me wonder to what extent this is the magic of model fitting software as there are only 9 data points underlying each distribution. Whereas Figure 5 shows many broadly overlapping distributions for SBI, Figure 6 seems to suggest the sexes are quite well separated for SBI (e.g., brain in MUS, heart in DOM).

      We use a standard fitting function in R (see above), which tries to fit a normalized distribution, but this function can also add an additional peak when the data are too heterogeneous (e.g. Mammary in Figure 7).

      Fig. S1 should be shown as the log(F/M) ratio so it is easier to see the symmetry, or lack thereof, of female and male-biased genes.

      The log will work differently for values <1, compared to values >1 when used in a single plot. We have now generated combined plots with symmetric values to allow a better comparability.

      It is important to note that for the variance analysis that IQR/median was calculated for each gene within each sex for each tissue. This is a key piece of information that should be in the methods or legend of the main figure (not buried in Supplemental Table 17).

      ​We have now moved these descriptions into the Methods section.

    1. Author response:

      Evidence reducibility and clarity

      Reviewer 1:

      In this manuscript, the role of the insulin receptor and the insulin growth factor receptor was investigated in podocytes. Mice, were both receptors were deleted, developed glomerular dysfunction and developed proteinuria and glomerulosclerosis over several months. Because of concerns about incomplete KO, the authors generated podocyte cell lines where both receptors were deleted. Loss of both receptors was highly deleterious with greater than 50% cell death. To elucidate the mechanism, the authors performed global proteomics and find that spliceosome proteins are downregulated. They confirm this by using long-range sequencing. These results suggest a novel role for these pathways in podocytes.

      Thank you

      This is primarily a descriptive study and no technical concerns are raised. The mechanism of how insulin and IGF1 signaling are linked to the spiceosome is not addresed.

      We do not think the paper is descriptive as we used non-biased phospho and total proteomics in the DKO cells to uncover the alterations in the spliceosome (that have not been previously described) that were detrimental. However, we are happy to look further into the underlying mechanism.

      We would propose:

      (1) Stimulating/inhibiting insulin/IGF signalling pathways in the Wild-type and DKO knockout cells and check expression levels and/or phosphorylation status of splice factors (including those in Figure 3E) and those revealed by phospho-proteomic data; a variety of inhibitors of insulin/IGF1 pathways could also be used along the pathways that are shown in Fig 2.

      (2) Looking at the RNaseq data bioinformatically in more detail – the introns/exons that move up or down are targets of the splice factors involved; most splice factors binding sequences are known, so it should be possible to ask bioinformatically – from the sequences around the splice sites of the exons and introns that move in the DKO, which splice factors binding sites are seen most frequently? To uncover splice factors/RNA-binding proteins (RBPs) that are involved in the insulin signaling we will use a software named MATT which was specifically designed to look for RNA-binding motifs (PMID 30010778). In brief, using the long-sequencing data, we will test 250 nt sequences flanking the splice sites of all regulated splicing events (intronic and exonic) against all RNA- binding proteins in the CISBP-RNA database (PMID 23846655) using MATT. This will result in a list of RBPs potentially involved in the insulin signaling. We will validate these by activating insulin signaling (similar to Figures 2 B,C) and probe whether the RBPs are activated (e.g. phosphorylated or change in expression) or we will manipulate expression of the candidate RBPs and measure how they affect the insulin signaling.

      (3) Examining the phospho and total proteomic data for IGF1R and Insulin receptor knockout alone podocytes (which we have already generated) and analysing these in more detail and include this data set to elucidate the relative importance of both receptors to spliceosome function.

      The phenotype of the mouse is only superficially addressed. The main issues are that the completeness of the mouse KO is never assessed nor is the completeness of the KO in cell lines. The absence of this data is a significant weakness.

      We apologise for not making clear but we did assess the level of receptor knockdown in the animal and cell models.  The in vivo model showed variable and non-complete levels of insulin receptor and IGF1 receptor podocyte knock down (shown in supplementary figure 1B). This is why we made the in vitro  floxed podocyte cell lines in which we could robustly knockdown both the insulin receptor and IGF1 receptor (shown in Figure 2A)

      The mouse experiments would be improved if the serum creatinines were measured to provide some idea how severe the kidney injury is.

      We can address this:

      We have further urinary Albumin:creatinine ratio (uACR) data at 12, 16 and 20 weeks. We also have more blood tests of renal function that can be added. There is variability in creatinine levels which is not uncommon in transgenic mouse models (probably partly due to variability in receptor knock down with cre-lox system). This is part of rationale of developing the robust double receptor knockout cell models where we knocked out both receptors by >80%.

      An attempt to rescue the phenotype by overexpression of SF3B4 would also be useful. If this didn't work, an explanation in the text would suffice.

      We would consider  over express SF3BF4 in the Wild type and DKO cells and assess the effects on spliceosome if deemed necessary.  However, we think it is unlikely to rescue the phenotype as so many other spliceosome components are downregulated in the DKO cells.

      As insulin and IGF are regulators of metabolism, some assessment of metabolic parameters would be an optional add-on.

      We have some detail on this and can add to the manuscript. However it is not extensive as not a major driver of this work.

      Lastly, the authors should caveat the cell experiments by discussing the ramifications of studying the 50% of the cells that survive vs the ones that died.

      Thank you, we appreciate this and this was the rationale behind cells being studied after 2 days differentiation before significant cell loss in order to avoid the issue of studying the 50% of cells that survive.

      Reviewer 2:

      In this manuscript, submitted to Review Commons (journal agnostic), Coward and colleagues report on the role of insulin/IGF axis in podocyte gene transcription. They knocked out both the insulin and IGFR1 mice. Dual KO mice manifested a severe phenotype, with albuminuria, glomerulosclerosis, renal failure and death at 4-24 weeks.

      Long read RNA sequencing was used to assess splicing events. Podocyte transcripts manifesting intron retention were identified. Dual knock-out podocytes manifested more transcripts with intron retention (18%) compared wild-type controls (18%), with an overlap between experiments of ~30%.

      Transcript productivity was also assessed using FLAIR-mark-intron-retention software. Intron retention w seen in 18% of ciDKO podocyte transcripts compared to 14% of wild-type podocyte transcripts (P=0.004), with an overlap between experiments of ~30% (indicating the variability of results with this method). Interestingly, ciDKO podocytes showed downregulation of proteins involved in spliceosome function and RNA processing, as suggested by LC/MS and confirmed by Western blot.

      Pladienolide (a spliceosome inhibitor) was cytotoxic to HeLa cells and to mouse podocytes but no toxicity was seen in murine glomerular endothelial cells.<br /> Specific comments.

      The manuscript is generally clear and well-written. Mouse work was approved in advance. The six figures are generally well-designed, bars/superimposed dot-plots.

      Thank you

      Evaluation.

      Methods are generally well described. It would be helpful to say that tissue scoring was performed by an investigator masked to sample identity.

      We did this and will add this information to the methods/figure legend.

      Specific comments.

      (1) Data are presented as mean/SEM. In general, mean/SD or median/IQR are preferred to allow the reader to evaluate the spread of the data. There may be exceptions where only SEM is reasonable.

      Graphs can be changed to SD rather than SEM.

      (2) It would be useful to for the reader to be told the number of over-lapping genes (with similar expression between mouse groups) and the results of a statistical test comparing WT and KO mice. The overlap of intron retention events between experimental repeats was about 30% in both knock-out podocytes. This seems low and I am curious to know whether this is typical for typical for this method; a reference could be helpful.

      This is an excellent question. We had 30% overlap as the parameters used for analysis were very stringent. We suspect we could get more than 30% by being less stringent, which still be considered as similar events if requested. Our methods were based on FLAIR analysis (PMID: 32188845)

      (3) Please explain "adjusted p value of 0.01." It is not clear how was it adjusted. The number of differentially-expressed proteins between the two cell types was 4842.

      We used the Benjamini-Hochberg method to adjust our data. We think the reviewer is referring to the transcriptomic data and not the proteomic data.

      Minor comments

      Page numbers in the text would help the reviewer communicate more effectively with the author.

      We will do this

      Reviewer 3:

      These investigators have previously shown important roles for either insulin receptor (IR) or insulin-like growth factor receptor (IGF1R) in glomerular podocyte function. They now have studied mice with deletion of both receptors and find significant podocyte dysfunction. They then made a podocyte cell line with inducible deletion of both receptors and find abnormalities in transcriptional efficiency with decreased expression of spliceosome proteins and increased transcripts with impaired splicing or premature termination.

      The studies appear to be performed well and the manuscript is clearly written.

      Thank you

      Referees cross-commenting

      I am in agreement with Reviewer 1 that the studies are overly descriptive and do not provide sufficient mechanism and the lack of more investigation of the in vivo model is a significant weakness.

      Please see our responses to reviewer 1 above.

      Significance

      Reviewer 1:

      With the GLP1 agonists providing renal protection, there is great interest in understanding the role of insulin and other incretins in kidney cell biology. It is already known that Insulin and IGFR signaling play important roles in other cells of the kidney. So, there is great interest in understanding these pathways in podocytes. The major advance is that these two pathways appear to have a role in RNA metabolism, the major limitations are the lack of information regarding the completeness of the KO's. If, for example, they can determine that in the mice, the KO is complete, that the GFR is relatively normal, then the phenotype they describe is relatively mild.

      Thank you. The receptor  KO in the mice is unlikely to be complete (Please see comments above and Supplementary Figure 1b). There are many examples of KO models targeting other tissues showing that complete KO of these receptors seems difficult to achieve , particularly in reference to the IGF1 receptor. In the brain (which is also terminally differentiated cells PMID:28595357 (barely 50% iof IGF1R knockdown was achieved in the target cells). Ovarian granulosa cells PMID:28407051 -several tissue specific drivers tried but couldn't achieve any better than 80%. The paper states that 10% of IGF1R is sufficient for function in these cells so they conclude that their knockdown animals are probably still responding to IGF1. Finally, in our recent IGF1R podocyte knockdown model we found Cre levels were important for excision of a single floxed gene (PMID: 38706850) hence we were not surprised that trying to excise two floxed genes (insulin receptor and IGF1 receptor) was challenging. This is the rationale for making the double receptor knockout cell lines to understand process / biology in more detail.

      Reviewer 2:

      The manuscript is generally clear and well-written. Mouse work was approved in advance. The figures are generally well-designed, bars/superimposed dot-plots.

      Evaluation.

      Methods are generally well described. It would be helpful to say that tissue scoring was performed by an investigator masked to sample identity.

      Thank you we will do this.

      Reviewer 3:

      There are a number of potential issues and questions with these studies.

      (1) For the in vivo studies, the only information given is for mice at 24 weeks of age. There needs to be a full time course of when the albuminuria was first seen and the rate of development. Also, GFR was not measured. Since the podocin-Cre utilized was not inducible, there should be a determination of whether there was a developmental defect in glomeruli or podocytes. Were there any differences in wither prenatal post natal development or number of glomeruli?

      Thank you we will add in further phenotyping data. We do not think there was a major developmental phenotype as  albuminuria did not become significantly different until several months of age. We could have used a doxycycline inducible model but we know the excision efficiency is much less than the podocin-cre driven model SUPP FIGURE 1. This would likely give a very mild (if any) phenotype and not reveal the biology adequately.

      (2) Although the in vitro studies are of interest, there are no studies to determine if this is the underlying mechanism for the in vivo abnormalities seen in the mice. Cultured podocytes may not necessarily reflect what is occurring in podocytes in vivo.

      Thank you for this we are happy to employ Immunohistochemistry (IHC) and immunofluorescence (IF) using spliceosome antibodies on tissue sections from DKO and control mice to examine spliceosome changes. However, as the DKO results in podocyte loss, there may not be that many DKO podocytes still present in the tissue sections. This will be taken into consideration.

      (3) Given that both receptors are deleted in the podocyte cell line, it is not clear if the spliceosome defect requires deletion of both receptors or if there is redundancy in the effect. The studies need to be repeated in podocyte cell lines with either IR or IGFR single deletions.

      Thank you. We have full total and phospho-proteomic data sets from single insulin receptor and IGF1 receptor knockout cell lines that we will investigate for this point.

      (4) There are not studies investigating signaling mechanisms mediating the spliceosome abnormalities.

      Thank you as outlined as above to reviewer 1 point 1 we are very happy to investigate insulin / IGF signalling pathways in more detail.

    1. Author response:

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In this manuscript, the authors argue that defining higher visual areas (HVAs) based on reversals of retinotopic tuning has led to an over-parcellation of secondary visual cortices. Using retinotopic models, they propose that the HVAs are more parsimoniously mapped as a single area V2, which encircles V1 and exhibits complex retinotopy. They reanalyze functional data to argue that functional differences between HVAs can be explained by retinotopic coverage. Finally, they compare the classification of mouse visual cortex to that of other species to argue that our current classification is inconsistent with those used in other model species.

      Strengths:

      This manuscript is bold and thought-provoking, and is a must-read for mouse visual neuroscientists. The authors take a strong stance on combining all HVAs, with the possible exception of area POR, into a single V2 region. Although I suspect many in the field will find that their proposal goes too far, many will agree that we need to closely examine the assumptions of previous classifications to derive a more accurate areal map. The authors' supporting analyses are clear and bolster their argument. Finally, they make a compelling argument for why the classification is not just semantic, but has ramifications for the design of experiments and analysis of data.

      Weaknesses:

      Although I enjoyed the polemic nature of the manuscript, there are a few issues that weaken their argument.

      (1) Although the authors make a compelling argument that retinotopic reversals are insufficient to define distinct regions, they are less clear about what would constitute convincing evidence for distinct visual regions. They mention that a distinct area V3 has been (correctly) defined in ferrets based on "cytoarchitecture, anatomy, and functional properties", but elsewhere argue that none of these factors are sufficient to parcellate any of the HVAs in mouse cortex, despite some striking differences between HVAs in each of these factors. It would be helpful to clearly define a set of criteria that could be used for classifying distinct regions.

      We agree the revised manuscript would benefit from a clear discussion of updated rules of area delineation in the mouse. In brief, we argue that retinotopy alone should not be used to delineate area boundaries in mice, or any other species. Although there is some evidence for functional property, architecture, and connectivity changes across mouse HVAs, area boundaries continue to be defined primarily, and sometimes solely (Garrett et al., 2014; Juavinett et al., 2018; Zhuang et al., 2017), based on retinotopy. We acknowledge that earlier work (Wang and Burkhalter, 2007; Wang et al., 2011) did consider cytoarchitecture and connectivity alongside retinotopy, but more recent work has shifted to a focus on retinotopy as indicated by the currently accepted criterion for area delineation.  

      As reviewer #2 points out, the present criteria for mouse visual area delineation can be found in the Methods section of: [Garrett, M.E., Nauhaus, I., Marshel, J.H., and Callaway, E.M. (2014)].

      Criterion 1: Each area must contain the same visual field sign at all locations within the area.

      Criterion 2: Each visual area cannot have a redundant representation of visual space.

      Criterion 3: Adjacent areas of the same visual field sign must have a redundant representation.

      Criterion 4: An area's location must be consistently identifiable across experiments.

      As discussed in the manuscript, recent evidence in higher order visual cortex of tree shrews and rats led us to question the universality of these criteria across species. Specifically, tree shrew V2, macaque V2, and marmoset DM, exhibit reversals in visual field-sign in what are defined as single visual areas. This suggests that criterion 1 should be updated. It also suggests that Criterion 2 and 3 should be updated since visual field sign reversals often co-occur with retinotopic redundancies, since reversing course in the direction of progression along the visual field can easily lead to coverage of visual field regions already traveled.  

      More broadly, we argue that topography is just one of several criteria that should be considered in area delineation. We understand that few visual areas in any species meet all criteria, but we emphasize that topography cannot consistently be the sole satisfied criterion – as it currently appears to be for many mouse HVAs. Inspired by a recent perspective on cortical area delineation (Petersen et al., 2024), we suggest the following rules, that will be worked into the revised version of the manuscript. Topography is a criterion, but it comes after considerations of function, architectonics and connectivity.

      (1) Function—Cortical areas differ from neighboring areas in their functional properties  

      (2) Architectonics—Cortical areas often exhibit distinctions from neighboring areas in multiple cyto- and myeloarchitectonic markers

      (3) Connectivity—Cortical areas are characterized by a specific set of connectional inputs and outputs from and to other areas

      (4) Topography—Cortical areas often exhibit a distinct topography that balances maximal coverage of the sensory field with minimal redundancy of coverage within an area.

      As we discuss in the manuscript, although there are functional, architectonic, and connectivity differences across mouse HVAs, they typically vary smoothly across multiple areas – such that neighboring areas share the same properties and there are no sharp borders. For instance, sharp borders in cytoarchitecture are generally lacking in the mouse HVAs. A notable exceptions to this is the clear and sharp change in m2AChR expression that occurs between LM and AL (Wang et al., 2011). 

      (2) On a related note, although the authors carry out impressive analyses to show that differences in functional properties between HVAs could be explained by retinotopy, they glossed over some contrary evidence that there are functional differences independent of retinotopy. For example, axon projections to different HVAs originating from a single V1 injection - presumably including neurons with similar retinotopy - exhibit distinct functional properties (Glickfeld LL et al, Nat Neuro, 2013). As another example, interdigitated M2+/M2- patches in V1 show very different HVA connectivity and response properties, again independent of V1 location/retinotopy (Meier AM et al., bioRxiv). One consideration is that the secondary regions might be considered a single V2 with distinct functional modules based on retinotopy and connectivity (e.g., V2LM, V2PM, etc).

      Thank you for the correction. We will revise the text to discuss (Glickfeld et al., 2013), as it remains some of the strongest evidence in favor of retinotopy-independent functional specialization of mouse HVAs. However, one caveat of this study is the size of the V1 injection that is the source of axons studied in the HVAs. As apparent in Figure 1B, the large injection covers nearly a quarter of V1. It is worth nothing that (Han et al., 2018) found, using single-cell reconstructions and MAPseq, that the majority of V1 neurons project to multiple nearby HVA targets. In this experiment the tracing does not suffer from the problem of spreading over V1’s retinotopic map, and suggests that, presumably retinotopically matched, locations in each area receive shared inputs from the V1 population rather than a distinct but spatially interspersed subset. In fact, the authors conclude “Interestingly, the location of the cell body within V1 was predictive of projection target for some recipient areas (Extended Data Fig. 8). Given the retinotopic organization of V1, this suggests that visual information from different parts of visual field may be preferentially distributed to  specific target areas, which is consistent with recent findings (Zhuang et al., 2017)”. Given an injection covering a large portion of the retinotopic map, and the fact that feed-forward projections from V1 to HVAs carry coarse retinotopy - it is difficult to prove that functional specializations noted in the HVA axons are retinotopyindependent. This would require measurement of receptive field location in the axonal boutons, which the authors did not perform (possibly because the SNR of calcium indicators prevented such measurements at the time).  

      Another option would be to show that adjacent neurons in V1, that project to far-apart HVAs, exhibit distinct functional properties on par with differences exhibited by neurons in very different parts of V1 due to retinotopy. In other words, the functional specificity of V1 inputs to HVAs at retinotopically identical locations is of the same order as those that might be gained by retinotopic biases. To our knowledge, such a study has not been conducted, so we have decided to measure the data in collaboration with the Allen Institute. As part of the Allen Institute’s pioneering OpenScope project, we will make careful two-photon and electrophysiology measurements of functional properties, including receptive field location, SF, and TF in different parts of the V1 retinotopic map. Pairing this data with existing Allen Institute datasets on functional properties of neurons in the HVAs will allow us to rule in, or rule-out, our hypotheses regarding retinotopy as the source of functional specialization in mouse HVAs. We will update the discussion in the revised manuscript to better reflect the need for additional evidence to support or refute our proposal.

      Meier AM et al., bioRxiv 2025 (Meier et al., 2025) was published after our submission, but we are thankful to the reviewers for guiding our attention to this timely paper. Given the recent findings on the influence of locomotion on rodent and primate visual cortex, it is very exciting to see clearly specialized circuits for processing self-generated visual motion in V1. However, it is difficult to rule out the role of retinotopy as the HVA areas (LM, AL, RL) participating in the M2+ network less responsive to self-generated visual motion exhibit a bias for the medial portion of the visual field and the HVA area (PM) involved in the M2- network responsive to self-generated visual motion exhibit a bias for the lateral (or peripheral) parts of the visual field. For instance, a peripheral bias in area PM has been shown using retrograde tracing as in Figure 6 of (Morimoto et al., 2021), single-cell anterograde tracing  as in Extended Data Figure 8 of (Han et al., 2018), and functional imaging studies (Zhuang et al., 2017). Recent findings in the marmoset also point to visual circuits in the peripheral, but not central, visual field being significantly modulated by selfgenerated movements (Rowley et al., 2024). 

      However, a visual field bias in area PM that selectively receive M2- inputs is at odds with the clear presence of modular M2+/M2- patches across the entire map of V1 (Ji et al., 2015).  One possibility supported by existing data is that neurons in M2- patches, as well as those in M2+ patches, in the central representation of V1 make fewer or significantly weaker connections with area PM compared to areas LM, AL and RL. Evidence to the contrary would support retinotopy-independent and functionally specialized inputs from V1 to HVAs.

      (3) Some of the HVAs-such as AL, AM, and LI-appear to have redundant retinotopic coverage with other HVAS, such as LM and PM. Moreover, these regions have typically been found to have higher "hierarchy scores" based on connectivity (Harris JA et al., Nature, 2019; D'Souza RD et al., Nat Comm, 2022), though unfortunately, the hierarchy levels are not completely consistent between studies. Based on existing evidence, there is a reasonable argument to be made for a hybrid classification, in which some regions (e.g., LM, P, PM, and RL) are combined into a single V2 (though see point #2 above) while other HVAs are maintained as independent visual regions, distinct from V2. I don't expect the authors to revise their viewpoint in any way, but a more nuanced discussion of alternative classifications is warranted.

      We understand that such a proposal would combine a subset of areas with matched field sign (LM, P, PM, and RL) would be less extreme and received better by the community. This would create a V2 with a smooth map without reversals or significant redundant retinotopic coverage. However, the intuition we have built from our modeling studies suggest that both these areas, and the other smaller areas with negative field sign (AL, AM, LI), are a byproduct of a complex single map of the visual field that exhibits reversals as it contorts around the triangular and tear-shaped boundaries of V1. In other words, we believe the redundant coverage and field-sign changes/reversals are a byproduct of a single secondary visual field in V2 constrained by the cortical dimensions of V1. That being said, we understand that area delineations are in part based on a consensus by the community. Therefore we will continue to discuss our proposal with community members, and we will incorporate new evidence supporting or refuting our hypothesis, before we submit our revised manuscript.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The study by Rowley and Sedigh-Sarvestani presents modeling data suggesting that map reversals in mouse lateral extrastriate visual cortex do not coincide with areal borders, but instead represent borders between subregions within a single area V2. The authors propose that such an organization explains the partial coverage in higher-order areas reported by Zhuang et al., (2017). The scheme revisits an organization proposed by Kaas et al., (1989), who interpreted the multiple projection patches traced from V1 in the squirrel lateral extrastriate cortex as subregions within a single area V2. Kaas et al's interpretation was challenged by Wang and Burkhalter (2007), who used a combination of topographic mapping of V1 connections and receptive field recordings in mice. Their findings supported a different partitioning scheme in which each projection patch mapped a specific topographic location within single areas, each containing a complete representation of the visual field. The area map of mouse visual cortex by Wang and Burkhalter (2007) has been reproduced by hundreds of studies and has been widely accepted as ground truth (CCF) (Wang et al., 2020) of the layout of rodent cortex. In the meantime, topographic mappings in marmoset and tree shew visual cortex made a strong case for map reversals in lateral extrastriate cortex, which represent borders between functionally diverse subregions within a single area V2. These findings from non-rodent species raised doubts about whether during evolution, different mammalian branches have developed diverse partitioning schemes of the cerebral cortex. Rowley and Sedigh-Sarvestani favor a single master plan in which, across evolution, all mammalian species have used a similar blueprint for subdividing the cortex.

      Strengths:

      The story illustrates the enduring strength of science in search of definitive answers.

      Weaknesses:

      To me, it remains an open question whether Rowley and Sedigh-Sarvestani have written the final chapter of the saga. A key reason for my reservation is that the areas the maps used in their model are cherry-picked. The article disregards published complementary maps, which show that the entire visual field is represented in multiple areas (i.e. LM, AL) of lateral extrastriate cortex and that the map reversal between LM and AL coincides precisely with the transition in m2AChR expression and cytoarchitecture (Wang and Burkhalter, 2007; Wang et al., 2011). Evidence from experiments in rats supports the gist of the findings in the mouse visual cortex (Coogan and Burkhalter, 1993).

      We would not claim to have written the final chapter of the saga. Our goal was to add an important piece of new evidence to the discussion of area delineations across species. We believe this new evidence supports our unification hypothesis.  We also believe that there are several missing pieces of data that could support or refute our hypothesis. We have begun a collaboration to collect some of this data.  

      (1) The selective use of published evidence, such as the complete visual field representation in higher visual areas of lateral extrastriate cortex (Wang and Burkhalter, 2007; Wang et al., 2011) makes the report more of an opinion piece than an original research article that systematically analyzes the area map of mouse visual cortex we have proposed. No direct evidence is presented for a single area V2 with functionally distinct subregions.

      This brings up a nuanced issue regarding visual field coverage. Wang & Burkhalter, 2007 Figure 6 shows the receptive field of sample neurons in area LM that cover the full range between 0 and 90 degrees of azimuth, and -40 to 80 degree of elevation – which essentially matches the visual field coverage in V1. However, we do not know whether these neurons are representative of most neurons in area LM. In other words, while these single-cell recordings along selected contours in cortex show the span of the visual field coverage, they may not be able to capture crucial information about its shape, missing regions of the visual field or potential bias. To mitigate this, visual field maps measured with electrophysiology are commonly produced by even sampling across the two dimensions of the visual area, either by moving a single electrode along a grid-pattern (e.g. (Manger et al., 2002)), or using a grid-liked multi-electrode probe (e.g. (Yu et al., 2020)). This was not carried out either in Wang & Burkhalter 2007 or Wang et al. 2011.  Even sampling of cortical space is time consuming and difficult with electrophysiology, but efficient with functional imaging. Therefore, despite the likely under-estimation of visual field coverage, imaging techniques are valuable in that they can efficiently exhibit not only the span of the visual field of a cortical region, but also its shape and bias.  

      Multiple functional imaging studies that simultaneously measure visual field coverage in V1 and HVAs report a bias in the coverage of HVAs, relative to that in V1 (Garrett et al., 2014; Juavinett et al., 2018; Zhuang et al., 2017). While functional imaging will likely underestimate receptive fields compared to electrophysiology, the consistent observation of an orderly bias for distinct parts of the visual field across the HVAs suggests that at least some of the HVAs do not have full and uniform coverage of the visual field comparable to that in V1. For instance, (Garrett et al., 2014) show that the total coverage in HVAs, when compared to V1, is typically less than half (Figure 6D) and often irregularly shaped.

      Careful measurements of single-cell receptive fields, using mesoscopic two-photon imaging across the HVAs would settle this question. As reviewer #1 points out, this is technically feasible, though no dataset of this kind exists to our knowledge.

      (2) The article misrepresents evidence by commenting that m2AChR expression is mainly associated with the lower field. This is counter to published findings showing that m2AChR spans across the entire visual field (Gamanut et al., 2018; Meier et al., 2021). The utility of markers for delineating areal boundaries is discounted, without any evidence, in disregard of evidence for distinct areal patterns in early development (Wang et al., 2011). Pointing out that markers can be distributed non-uniformly within an area is well-familiar. m2AChR is non-uniformly expressed in mouse V1, LM and LI (Ji et al., 2015; D'Souza et al., 2019; Meier et al., 2021). Recently, it has been found that the patchy organization within V1 plays a role in the organization of thalamocortical and intracortical networks (Meier et al., 2025). m2AChR-positive patches and m2AChR-negative interpatches organize the functionally distinct ventral and dorsal networks, notably without obvious bias for upper and lower parts of the visual field.

      We wrote that “Future work showed boundaries in labeling of histological markers such as SMI-32 and m2ChR labeling, but such changes mostly delineated area LM/AL (Wang et al., 2011) and seemed to be correlated with the representation of the lower visual field.” The latter statement regarding the representation of the lower visual field is directly referencing the data in Figure 1 of (Wang et al., 2011), which is titled “Figure 1: LM/AL border identified by the transition of m2AChR expression coincides with receptive field recordings from lower visual field.” Similar to the Wang et al., we were simply referring to the fact that the border of area LM/AL co-exhibits a change in m2AChR expression as well as lower-visual field representation.  

      (3) The study has adopted an area partitioning scheme, which is said to be based on anatomically defined boundaries of V2 (Zhuang et al., 2017). The only anatomical borders used by Zhuang et al. (2017) are those of V1 and barrel cortex, identified by cytochrome oxidase staining. In reality, the partitioning of the visual cortex was based on field sign maps, which are reproduced from Zhuang et al., (2017) in Figure 1A. It is unclear why the maps shown in Figures 2E and 2F differ from those in Figure 1A. It is possible that this is an oversight. But maintaining consistent areal boundaries across experimental conditions that are referenced to the underlying brain structure is critical for assigning modeled projections to areas or sub-regions. This problem is evident in Figure 2F, which is presented as evidence that the modeling approach recapitulates the tracings shown in Figure 3 of Wang and Burkhalter (2007). The dissimilarities between the modeling and tracing results are striking, unlike what is stated in the legend of Figure 2F.

      Thanks for this correction. By “anatomical boundaries of higher visual cortex”, we meant the cortical boundary between V1 and higher order visual areas on one end, and the outer edge of the envelope that defines the functional boundaries of the HVAs in cortical space (Zhuang et al., 2017). The reviewer is correct that we should have referred to these as functional boundaries. The word ‘anatomical’ was meant to refer to cortical space, rather than visual field space.

      More generally though, there is no disagreement between the partitioning of visual cortex in Figure 1 and 2. Rather, the portioning in Figure 1 is directly taken from Zhuang et al., (2017) whereas those in Figure 2 are produced by mathematical model simulation. As such, one would not expect identical areal boundaries between Figure 2 and Figure 1. What we aimed to communicate with our modeling results, is that a single area can exhibit multiple visual field reversals and retinotopic redundancies if it is constrained to fit around V1 and cover a visual field approximately matched to the visual field coverage in V1. We defined this area explicitly as a single area with a single visual field (boundaries shown in Figure 2A). So  the point of our simulation is to show that even an explicitly defined single area can appear as multiple areas if it is constrained by the shape of mouse V1, and if visual field reversals are used to indicate areal boundaries. As in most models, different initial conditions and parameters produce a complex visual field which will appear as multiple HVAs when delineated by areal boundaries. What is consistent however, is the existence of complex single visual field that appears as multiple HVAs with partially overlapping coverage.

      Similarly, we would not expect a simple model to exactly reproduce the multi-color tracer injections in Wang and Burkhalter (2007). However, we find it quite compelling that the model can produce multiple groups of multi-colored axonal projections beyond V1 that can appear as multiple areas each with their own map of the visual field using current criteria, when the model is explicitly designed to map a single visual field. We will explain the results of the model, and their implications, better in the revised manuscript.

      (4) The Rowley and Sedigh-Sarvestani find that the partial coverage of the visual field in higher order areas shown by Zhuang et al (2017) is recreated by the model. It is important to caution that Zhuang et al's (2017) maps were derived from incomplete mappings of the visual field, which was confined to -25-35 deg of elevation. This underestimates the coverage we have found in LM and AL. Receptive field mappings show that LM covers 0-90 deg of azimuth and -30-80 elevation (Wang and Burkhalter, 2007). AL covers at least 0-90 deg of azimuth and -30-50 deg of elevation (Wang and Burkhalter, 2007; Wang et al., 2011). These are important differences. Partial coverage in LM and AL underestimates the size of these areas and may map two projection patches as inputs to subregions of a single area rather than inputs to two separate areas. Complete, or nearly complete, visual representations in LM and AL support that each is a single area. Importantly, both areas are included in a callosal-free zone (Wang and Burkhalter, 2007). The surrounding callosal connections align with the vertical meridian representation. The single map reversal is marked by a transition in m2AChR expression and cytoarchitecture (Wang et al., 2011).

      This is a good point. We do not expect that expanding the coverage of V1 will change the results of the model significantly. However, for the revised manuscript, we will update V1 coverage to be accurate, repeat our simulations, and report the results.  

      (5) The statement that the "lack of visual field overlap across areas is suggestive of a lack of hierarchical processing" is predicated on the full acceptance of the mappings by Zhuang et al (2017). Based on the evidence reviewed above, the reclassification of visual areas proposed in Figure 1C seems premature.

      The reviewer is correct. In the revised manuscript, we will be careful to distinguish bias in visual field coverage across areas from presence or lack of visual field overlap.  

      (6) The existence of lateral connections is not unique to rodent cortex and has been described in primates (Felleman and Van Essen, 1991).

      (7) Why the mouse and rat extrastriate visual cortex differ from those of many other mammals is unclear. One reason may be that mammals with V2 subregions are strongly binocular.

      This is an interesting suggestion, and careful visual topography data from rabbits and other lateral eyed animals would help to evaluate it. For what it’s worth, tree shrews are lateral eyed animals with only 50 degrees of binocular visual field and also show V2 subregions.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors review published literature and propose that a visual cortical region in the mouse that is widely considered to contain multiple visual areas should be considered a single visual area.

      Strengths:

      The authors point out that relatively new data showing reversals of visual-field sign within known, single visual areas of some species require that a visual field sign change by itself should not be considered evidence for a border between visual areas.

      Weaknesses:

      The existing data are not consistent with the authors' proposal to consolidate multiple mouse areas into a single "V2". This is because the existing definition of a single area is that it cannot have redundant representations of the visual field. The authors ignore this requirement, as well as the data and definitions found in published manuscripts, and make an inaccurate claim that "higher order visual areas in the mouse do not have overlapping representations of the visual field". For quantification of the extent of overlap of representations between 11 mouse visual areas, see Figure 6G of Garrett et al. 2014. [Garrett, M.E., Nauhaus, I., Marshel, J.H., and Callaway, E.M. (2014). Topography and areal organization of mouse visual cortex. The Journal of neuroscience 34, 12587-12600. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1124-14.2014.

      Thank you for this correction, we admit we should have chosen our words more carefully. In the revised manuscript, we will emphasize that higher order visual areas in the mouse do have some overlap in their representations but also exhibit bias in their coverage. This is consistent with our proposal and in fact our model simulations in Figure 2E also show overlapping representations along with differential bias in coverage. However, we also note Figure 6 of Garret et al. 2014 provides several pieces of evidence in support of our proposal that higher order areas are sub-regions of a single area V2. Specifically, the visual field coverage of each area is significantly less than that in V1 (Garret et al. 2014, Figure 6D). While the imaging methods used in Garret et al. likely under-estimate receptive fields, one would assume they would similarly impact measurements of coverage in V1 and HVAs. Secondly, each area exhibits a bias towards a different part of the visual field (Figure 6C and E), that this bias is distinct for different areas but proceeds in a retinotopic manner around V1 - with adjacent areas exhibiting biases for nearby regions of the visual field (Figure 6E). Thus, the biases in the visual field coverage across HVAs appear to be related and not independent of each other. As we show in our modeling and in Figure 2, such orderly and inter-related biases can be created from a single visual field constrained to share a border with mouse V1.   

      With regards to the existing definition of a single area: we did not ignore the requirement that single areas cannot have redundant representations of the visual field. Rather, we believe that this requirement should be relaxed considering new evidence collected from other species, where multiple visual field reversals exist within the same visual area. We understand this issue is nuanced and was not made clear in the original submission.  

      In the revised manuscript, we will clarify that visual field reversals often exhibit redundant retinotopic representation on either side of the reversal. In the revised manuscript we will clarify that our argument that multiple reversals can exist within a single visual area in the mouse, is an argument that some retinotopic redundancy can exist with single visual areas. Such a re-classification would align how we define visual areas in mice with existing classification in tree shrews, ferrets, cats, and primates – all of whom have secondary visual areas with complex retinotopic maps exhibiting multiple reversals and redundant retinotopic coverage.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review): 

      In this study, Ma et al. aimed to determine previously uncharacterized contributions of tissue autofluorescence, detector afterpulse, and background noise on fluorescence lifetime measurement interpretations. They introduce a computational framework they named "Fluorescence Lifetime Simulation for Biological Applications (FLiSimBA)" to model experimental limitations in Fluorescence Lifetime Imaging Microscopy (FLIM) and determine parameters for achieving multiplexed imaging of dynamic biosensors using lifetime and intensity. By quantitatively defining sensor photon effects on signal-to-noise in either fitting or averaging methods of determining lifetime, the authors contradict any claims of FLIM sensor expression insensitivity to fluorescence lifetime and highlight how these artifacts occur differently depending on the analysis method. Finally, the authors quantify how statistically meaningful experiments using multiplexed imaging could be achieved. 

      A major strength of the study is the effort to present results in a clear and understandable way given that most researchers do not think about these factors on a day-to-day basis. The model code is available and written in Matlab, which should make it readily accessible, although a version in other common languages such as Python might help with dissemination in the community. One potential weakness is that the model uses parameters that are determined in a

      specific way by the authors, and it is not clear how vastly other biological tissue and microscope setups may differ from the values used by the authors. 

      Overall, the authors achieved their aims of demonstrating how common factors

      (autofluorescence, background, and sensor expression) will affect lifetime measurements and they present a clear strategy for understanding how sensor expression may confound results if not properly considered. This work should bring to awareness an issue that new users of lifetime biosensors may not be aware of and that experts, while aware, have not quantitatively determined the conditions where these issues arise. This work will also point to future directions for improving experiments using fluorescence lifetime biosensors and the development of new sensors with more favorable properties. 

      We appreciate the comments and helpful suggestions. We now also include FLiSimBA simulation code in Python in addition to Matlab to make it more accessible to the community.

      One advantage of FLiSimBA is that the simulation package is flexible and adaptable, allowing users to input parameters based on the specific sensors, hardware, and autofluorescence measurements for their biological and optical systems. We used parameters based on a FRETbased sensor, measured autofluorescence from mouse tissue, and measured dark count/after pulse of our specific GaAsP PMT in this manuscript as examples. In Discussion and Materials and methods, we now emphasize this advantage and further clarify how these parameters can be adapted to diverse tissues, imaging systems, and sensors based on individual experiments. We further explain that these input parameters will not affect the conclusions of our study, but the specific input parameters would alter the quantitative thresholds.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      By using simulations of common signal artefacts introduced by acquisition hardware and the sample itself, the authors are able to demonstrate methods to estimate their influence on the estimated lifetime, and lifetime proportions, when using signal fitting for fluorescence lifetime imaging. 

      Strengths: 

      They consider a range of effects such as after-pulsing and background signal, and present a range of situations that are relevant to many experimental situations. 

      Weaknesses: 

      A weakness is that they do not present enough detail on the fitting method that they used to estimate lifetimes and proportions. The method used will influence the results significantly. They seem to only use the "empirical lifetime" which is not a state of the art algorithm. The method used to deconvolve two multiplexed exponential signals is not given. 

      We appreciate the comments and constructive feedback. Our revision based on the reviewer’s suggestions has made our manuscript clearer and more user friendly. We originally described the detail of the fitting methods in Materials and methods. Given the importance of these methodological details for evaluating the conclusions of this study, we have moved the description of the fitting method from Materials and methods to Results. In addition, we provide further clarification and more details of the rationale of using these different methods of lifetime estimates in Discussion to aid users in choosing the best metric for evaluating fluorescence lifetime data.

      More specifically, we modified our writing to highlight the following.

      (1) In Results, we describe that lifetime histograms were fitted to Equation 3 with the GaussNewton nonlinear least-square fitting algorithm and the fitted P<sub1</sub> was used as lifetime estimation.

      (2) In Results, we clarify that our simulation of multiplexed imaging was modeled with two sensors, each displaying a single exponential decay, but the two sensors have different decay constants. We also describe that Equation 3 with the Gauss-Newton nonlinear least-square fitting algorithm was used to deconvolve the two multiplexed exponential signals (Fig. 8)

      Reviewer #3 (Public review): 

      Summary: 

      This study presents a useful computational tool, termed FLiSimBA. The MATLAB-based FLiSimBA simulations allow users to examine the effects of various noise factors (such as autofluorescence, afterpulse of the photomultiplier tube detector, and other background signals) and varying sensor expression levels. Under the conditions explored, the simulations unveiled how these factors affect the observed lifetime measurements, thereby providing useful guidelines for experimental designs. Further simulations with two distinct fluorophores uncovered conditions in which two different lifetime signals could be distinguished, indicating multiplexed dynamic imaging may be possible. 

      Strengths: 

      The simulations and their analyses were done systematically and rigorously. FliSimba can be useful for guiding and validating fluorescence lifetime imaging studies. The simulations could define useful parameters such as the minimum number of photons required to detect a specific lifetime, how sensor protein expression level may affect the lifetime data, the conditions under which the lifetime would be insensitive to the sensor expression levels, and whether certain multiplexing could be feasible. 

      Weaknesses: 

      The analyses have relied on a key premise that the fluorescence lifetime in the system can be described as two-component discrete exponential decay. This means that the experimenter should ensure that this is the right model for their fluorophores a priori and should keep in mind that the fluorescence lifetime of the fluorophores may not be perfectly described by a twocomponent discrete exponential (for which alternative algorithms have been implemented: e.g., Steinbach, P. J. Anal. Biochem. 427, 102-105, (2012)). In this regard, I also couldn't find how good the fits were for each simulation and experimental data to the given fitting equation (Equation 2, for example, for Figure 2C data). 

      We thank the reviewer for the constructive feedback. We agree that the FLiSimBA users should ensure that the right decay equations are used to describe the fluorescent sensors. In this study, we used a FRET-based PKA sensor FLIM-AKAR to provide proof-of-principle demonstration of the capability of FLiSimBA. The donor fluorophore of FLIM-AKAR, truncated monomeric enhanced GFP, displays a single exponential decay. FLIM-AKAR, a FRET-based sensor, displays a double exponential decay. The time constants of the two exponential components were determined and reported previously (Chen, et al, Neuron (2017)).  Thus, a double exponential decay equation with known τ<sub>1</sub> and τ<sub>2</sub> was used for both simulation and fitting. The goodness of fit is now provided in Supplementary Fig. 1 for both simulated and experimental data. In addition to referencing our prior study characterizing the double exponential decay model of FLIM-AKAR in Materials and methods, we have emphasized in Discussion the versality of FLiSimBA to adapt to different sensors, tissues, and analysis methods, and the importance of using the right mathematical models to describe the fluorescence decay of specific sensors. 

      Also, in Figure 2C, the 'sensor only' simulation without accounting for autofluorescence (as seen in Sensor + autoF) or afterpulse and background fluorescence (as seen in Final simulated data) seems to recapitulate the experimental data reasonably well. So, at least in this particular case where experimental data is limited by its broad spread with limited data points, being able to incorporate the additional noise factors into the simulation tool didn't seem to matter too much.  

      In the original Fig 2C, the sensor fluorescence was much higher than the contributions from autofluorescence, afterpulse, and background signals, resulting in minimal effects of these other factors, as the reviewer noted. This original figure was based on photon counts from single neurons expressing FLIM-AKAR. For the rest of the manuscript, photon counts were based on whole fields of view (FOV). Since the FOV includes cells that do not express fluorescent sensors, the influence of autofluorescence, dark currents, and background is much more pronounced, as shown in Fig. 2B. 

      Both approaches – using photon counts from the whole FOV or from individual neurons – have their justifications. Photon counts from the whole FOV simulate data from fluorescence lifetime photometry (FLiP), whereas photon counts from individual neurons simulate data from fluorescence lifetime imaging microscopy (FLIM). However, the choice of approach does not affect the conclusions of the manuscript, as a range of photon count values are simulated. To maintain consistency throughout the manuscript, we have revised the photon counts in this figure (now Supplementary Fig. 1C) to match those from the whole FOV.

      Additionally, we have made some modifications in our analyses of Supplementary Fig. 1C and Fig. 2B, detailed in the “FLIM analysis” section of Materials and methods. For instance, to minimize system artifact interference at the histogram edges, we now use a narrower time range (1.8 to 11.5 ns) for fitting and empirical lifetime calculation.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) The authors report how autofluorescence was measured from "imaged brain slices from mice at postnatal 15 to 19 days of age without sensor expression." However, it remains unclear how many acute slices and animals were used (for example, were all 15um x 15um FOV from a single slice) and if mouse age affects autofluorescence quantification. Furthermore, would in vivo measurements have different autofluorescence conditions given that blood flow would be active? It would help if the authors more clearly explained how reliable their autofluorescence measurement is by clarifying how they obtained it, whether this would vary across brain areas, and whether in vitro vs in vivo conditions would affect autofluorescence. 

      We have added description in Materials and methods that for autofluorescence ‘Fluorescence decay histograms from 19 images of two brain slices from a single mouse were averaged.’ We have added in Discussion that users should carefully ‘measure autofluorescence that matches the age, brain region, and data collection conditions (e.g., ex vivo or in vivo) of their tissue…’, and emphasize that FLiSimBA offers customization of inputs, and it is important for users to adapt the inputs such as autofluorescence to their experimental conditions. We also clarify in Discussion that the change of input parameters such as autofluorescence across age and brain region would not affect the general insights from this study, but will affect quantitative values.

      (2) Does sensor expression level issues arise more with in-utero electroporation compared to AAV-based delivery of biosensors? A brief comment on this in the discussion may help as most users in the field today may be using AAV strategies to deliver biosensors.

      In our experience, in-utero electroporation results in higher sensor expression than AAV-based delivery, and so pose less concern for expression-level dependence. However, both delivery methods can result in expression level dependence, especially with a sensor that is not bright. We have added in Discussion ‘For a sensor with medium brightness delivered via in utero electroporation, adeno-associated virus, or as a knock-in gene, the brightness may not always fall within the expression level-independent regime.’

      (3) Figure 1. Should the x-axis on the top figures be "Time (ns)" instead of "Lifetime (ns)"?

      Similarly in Figure 8A&B, wouldn't it make more sense to have the x-axis be Time not Lifetime?

      The x-axis labels in Fig. 1 and Fig. 8A-8B have been changed to ‘Time (ns)’.   

      (4) Figure 2b: why is the empirical lifetime close to 3.5ns? Shouldn't it be somewhere between

      2.14 and 0.69? 

      In our empirical lifetime calculation, we did not set the peak channel to have a time of 0.0488 ns (i.e. the laser cycle 12.5 ns divided by 256 time channels). Rather, we set the first time channel within a defined calculation range (i.e. 1.8 ns in Supplementary Fig. 1B) to have a time of 0.0488 ns (i.e.). Thus, the empirical lifetime exceeds 2.14 ns and depends on the time range of the histogram used for calculation. 

      For Fig. 2B and Supplementary Fig. 1C, we have now adjusted the range to 1.8-11.5 ns to eliminate FLIM artifacts at the histogram edges in our experimental data, resulting in an empirical lifetime around 2.255 ns. In contrast, the range for calculating the empirical lifetime of simulated data in the rest of the study (e.g. Fig. 4D) is 0.489-11.5 ns, yielding a larger lifetime of ~3.35 ns. 

      We have clarified these details and our rationale in Materials and methods.

      (5) Figure 2b: how come the afterpulse+background contributes more to the empirical lifetime than the autofluorescence (shorter lifetime). This was unclear in the results text why autofluorescence photons did not alter empirical lifetime as much as did the afterpulse/background.

      With a histogram range from 1.8 ns to 11.5 ns used in Fig. 2B, the empirical lifetime for FLIM-AKAR sensor fluorescence, autofluorescence, and background/afterpulse are: 2-2.3 ns, around 1.69 ns, and around 4.90 ns. The larger difference of background/afterpulse from FLIM-AKAR sensor fluorescence leads to larger influence of afterpulse+background than autofluorescence. We have added an explanation of this in Results.

      (6) One overall suggestion for an improvement that could help active users of lifetime biosensors understand the consequences would be to show either a real or simulated example of a "typical experiment" conducted using FLIM-AKAR and how an incorrect interpretation could be drawn as a consequence of these artifacts. For example, do these confounds affect experiments involving comparisons across animals more than within-subject experiments such as washing a drug onto the brain slice, and the baseline period is used to normalize the change in signal? I think this type of direct discussion will help biosensor users more deeply grasp how these factors play out in common experiments being conducted.

      We have added the following in Discussion, ‘…While this issue is less problematic when the same sample is compared over short periods (e.g. minutes), It can lead to misinterpretation when fluorescence lifetime is compared across prolonged periods or between samples when comparison is made across chronic time periods or between samples with different sensor expression levels. For example, apparent changes in fluorescence lifetime observed over days, across cell types, or subcellular compartments may actually reflect variations in sensor expression levels rather than true differences in biological signals (Fig. 6), Therefore, considering biologically realistic factors in FLiSimBA is essential, as it qualitatively impacts the conclusions.’

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      The paper would be improved with more detail on the fitting methods, and the use of state-of-theart methods. Consult for example the introduction of this paper where many methods are listed: https://www.mdpi.com/1424-8220/22/19/7293

      We have moved the description of the Gauss-Newton nonlinear least-square fitting algorithm from Materials and methods to Results to enhance clarity. We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to combine FLiSimBA with various analysis methods. However, the primary focus of our manuscript is to call for attention of how specific contributing factors in biological experiments influence FLIM data, and to provide a tool that rigorously considers these factors to simulate FLIM data, which can then be used for fitting. Therefore, we did not expand the scope of our manuscript. Instead, we have added in the Discussion that ‘‘FLiSimBA can be used to test multiple fitting methods and lifetime metrics as an exciting future direction for identifying the best analysis method for specific experimental conditions’, citing relevant references.

      I would also improve the content of the GitHub repository as it is very hard to identify to source code used for simulation and fitting. 

      We have reorganized and relabeled our GitHub repository and now have three folders labeled as ‘Simulation_inMatlab’, ‘DataAnalysis_inMatlab’, and ‘SimulationAnalysis_inPython’. We also updated the clarification of the contents of each folder in the README file.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      (1) P. 10 "For example, to detect a P1 change of 0.006 or a lifetime change of 5 ps with one sample measurement in each comparison group, approximately 300,000 photons are needed." If I am reading the graphs in Figures 3B and C, this sentence is talking about the red line. However, the intersection of 0.006 in the MDD of P1 in 3B and red is not 3E5 photons. And the intersection of 0.005 ns and red in 3C is not 3E5 photons either. Are you sure you are talking about n=1? Maybe the values are correct for the blue curve with n=5.

      Thank you for catching our error. We have corrected the text to ‘with five sample measurements’.

      (2) Figure 2 (B) legend: It would be helpful to specify what is being compared in the legend. For example, consider revising "* p < 0.05 vs sensor only; n.s. not significant vs sensor + autoF; # p < 0.05 vs sensor + autoF. Two-way ANOVA with Šídák's multiple comparisons test" to "* p <0.05 for sensor + auto F (cyan) vs sensor only; n.s. not significant for final simulated data (purple) vs sensor + autoF; # p < 0.05 for final simulated data (purple) vs sensor + autoF. Twoway ANOVA with Šídák's multiple comparisons test".

      We’ve made the change and thanks for the suggestion to make it clearer.

      (3) Figure 2 (c) Can you please show the same Two-way ANOVA test values for Experimental vs. Sensor only and for Experimental vs. Sensor + autoF? Currently, the value (n.s.) is marked only for Experimental vs. Final simulation. Given that the experimental data are sparse (compared to the simulations), it seems likely that there may be no significant difference among the 3 different simulations regarding how well they match the experimental data. Also, can you specify the P1 and P2 of the experimental data  used to generate the simulated data on this panel? Also, what is the reason why P1=0.5 was used for panels A and B, instead of the value matching the experimental value?

      As the reviewer suggested, we have included statistical tests in the figure (now Supplementary Fig. 1C). Please see our response to the Public Review of Reviewer 3’s comments as well as our changes in Materials and Methods on other changes and their rationale for this figure. We have now specified the P<sub>1</sub> value of the experimental data used to generate the simulated data on this panel both in Figure Legends and Materials and Methods. Based on the suggestion, we have now used the same P<sub>1</sub> value in Fig. 2B.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review)

      Summary:

      In this paper the authors examined the effects of strip cropping, a relatively new agricultural technique of alternating crops in small strips of several meters wide, on ground beetle diversity. The results show an increase in species diversity (i.e. abundance and species richness) of the ground beetle communities compared to monocultures.

      Strengths:

      The article is well written; it has an easily readable tone of voice without too much jargon or overly complicated sentence structure. Moreover, as far as reviewing the models in depth without raw data and R scripts allows, the statistical work done by the authors looks good. They have well thought out how to handle heterogenous, unbalanced and taxonomically unspecific yet spatially and temporarily correlated field data. The models applied and the model checks performed are appropriate for the data at hand. Combining RDA and PCA axes together is a nice touch. Moreover, after the first round of reviews, the authors have done a great job at rewriting the paper to make it less overstated, more relevant to the data at hand and more solid in the findings. Many of the weaknesses noted in the first review have been dealt with. The overall structure of the paper is good, with a clear introduction, hypotheses, results section and discussion.

      We are grateful for this positive feedback. We are glad that our extensive revision after extensive review from three reviewers has paid off in addressing earlier weakness of our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      The weaknesses that remain are mainly due to a difficult dataset and choices that could have stressed certain aspects more, like the relationship between strip cropping and intercropping. The mechanistic understanding of strip cropping is what is at stake here. Does strip cropping behave similar to intercropping, a technique which has been proven to be beneficial to biodiversity because of added effects due to increased resource efficiency and greater plant species richness.

      Unfortunately, the authors do not go into this in the introduction or otherwise and simply state that they consider strip cropping a form of intercropping.

      We agree with the reviewer that a mechanistic understanding on how intercropping and strip cropping differ would be very interesting. However, we also feel that this topic is somewhat beyond the scope of the current manuscript. We are already planning work to elucidate mechanisms that may explain the pest and suppressive effects of strip cropping.

      I also do not like the exclusive focus on percentages, as these are dimensionless. I think more could have been done to show underlying structure in the data, even after rarefaction.

      While we generally agree with this point raised by the reviewer, for our heterogeneous dataset it was difficult to come up with meaningful units with dimensions. Therefore, we believe that percentages are the most suitable approach to present readers a fair comparison of the treatments.

      A further weakness is a limited embedding into the larger scientific discourses other than providing references. But this may be a matter of style and/or taste

      We believe our manuscript to be well-embedded within the relevant scientific discourse, but as indicated by reviewer 3 this might indeed be a matter of style/taste. Without exact examples it is difficult for us to judge this point.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors): 

      Suggestion for title: "Strip cropping shows promising preliminary increases in ground beetle community diversity compared to monocultures"

      We agree that the title could indeed be nuanced. We incorporated the suggested title, except for the word “preliminary”, as we felt that this is slightly misplaced for a 4-year study conducted at 4 locations.

      line 26: the word previous may be confusing to readers, as it suggests previous research on beetles or insects. I think it would be better to use for instance "related" or "productivity focused research"

      We agree that this wording might be confusing, and changed it to “other studies showed”.

      Line 84-85: this is vague. can you make explicit what you are trying to answer here?

      We made “biodiversity metric changes” more explicit, and changed the sentence accordingly.

      Line 88-89: I think this would fit better with the first question in line 83-84, so I suggest placing it upwards. Also, I think you mean abundant instead of common. Common suggests commonness in the entire population. Abundant suggests found often in this study. While these definitions may very much overlap, they are distinctly different.

      We have moved this sentence up and changed “common” to “abundant”. To make the result section more in line with this section, we also moved the section on the relationship between crop configuration and abundant genera up.  

      Line 146: defining rareness of species should be in the methods section. Also "following" would be better than "according"

      We now added a sentence on how we examine habitat preferences and rarity in the methods section (line 316-317). We also changed “according to” to “following”.

      Line 291: it is called being "flush" with the soil surface. This expression is not much used by non-native speakers, but is regularly encountered in studies on pitfalls, so the authors could decide to change the sentence using the proper English vernacular.

      Suggestion incorporated.

      Line 322-327, this method could do with a reference

      This method is a relatively standard calculation to calculate relative changes and to center variation around zero. Nevertheless, we added a reference to a paper that used the same method.

      Line: 333-335. I would still like to see a reference for this method.

      This methodology has not been described in literature to the best of our knowledge. As we compared two crops within strip cropping with their respective monoculture references, we compare one strip cropping field with two monocultural fields. Here we took a conservative approach by comparing the strip crop field with the monoculture with the highest richness and activity density, to see if strip cropped fields outperformed monocultures with diverse ground beetle communities.

      Line 364-366. references?

      We have added references for these R packages.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews

      We would like to thank you and your chosen reviewers for the diligent work and insightful comments. Following the latest round of feedback, we have made the following changes to the manuscript:

      (1) We have added details regarding the specific versions of Cryosparc and cryoDRGN used in our analysis.

      (2) We have addressed Reviewer 2’s comment concerning the negative RMSF values in Figure S12. The negative values occur because this display shows the difference in RMSF values from the MD simulations of glycosylated versus non-glycosylated ACE. To avoid similar confusion, we have split Figure S12 into three panels. Panels A and B show the RMSF values for each residue in the glycosylated and non-glycosylated sACE MD simulations, respectively, and all values here are positive. Panel C (the original Figure S12) now includes expanded labeling to clarify that it depicts the difference in RMSF values between the presence and absence of glycans. In this panel, a negative value indicates that the residues exhibit higher RMSF in simulations where glycans are present. The figure legend has been revised to accurately describe the updated figure.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The authors claim that they can use a combination of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (intermittent theta burst-iTBS) and transcranial alternating current stimulation (gamma tACS) to cause slight improvements in memory in a face/name/profession task.

      Strengths:

      The idea of stimulating the human brain non-invasively is very attractive because, if it worked, it could lead to a host of interesting applications. The current study aims to evaluate one such exciting application.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) It is highly unclear what, if anything, transpires in the brain with non-invasive stimulation. To cite one example of many, a rigorous study in rats and human cadavers, compellingly showed that traditional parameters of transcranial electrical stimulation lead to no change in brain activity due to the attenuation by the soft tissue and skull (Mihály Vöröslakos et al Nature Communications 2018): https://www.nature.com/articles/s41467-018-02928-3. It would be very useful to demonstrate via invasive neurophysiological recordings that the parameters used in the current study do indeed lead to any kind of change in brain activity. Of course, this particular study uses a different non-invasive stimulation protocol.

      Thank you for raising the important issue regarding the actual neurophysiological effects of non-invasive brain stimulation. Unfortunately, invasive neurophysiological recordings in humans for this type of study are not feasible due to ethical constraints, while studies on cadavers or rodents would not fully resolve our question. Indeed, the authors of the cited study (Mihály Vöröslakos et al., Nature Communications, 2018) highlight the impossibility of drawing definitive conclusions about the exact voltage required in the in-vivo human brain due to significant differences between rats and humans, as well as the in-vivo human brain and cadavers due to alterations in electrical conductivity that occur in postmortem tissue. Huang and colleagues addressed the difficulties in reaching direct evidence of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) effects in a review published in Clinical Neurophysiology in 2017. They conclude that the use of EEG to assess brain response to TMS has great potential for a less indirect demonstration of plasticity mechanisms induced by NIBS in humans.

      To address this challenge, we conducted Experiments 3 and 4, which respectively examined the neurophysiological and connectivity changes induced by the stimulation in a non-invasive manner using TMS-EEG and fMRI. The observed changes in brain oscillatory activity (increased gamma oscillatory activity), cortical excitability (enhanced posteromedial parietal cortex reactivity), and brain connectivity (strengthened connections between the precuneus and hippocampi) provided evidence of the effects of our non-invasive brain stimulation protocol, further supporting the behavioral data.

      Additionally, we carefully considered the issue of stimulation distribution and, in response, performed a biophysical modeling analysis and E-field calculation using the parameters employed in our study (see Supplementary Materials).

      We acknowledge that further exploration of this aspect would be highly valuable, and we agree that it is worth discussing both as a technical limitation and as a potential direction for future research. We therefore, modify the discussion accordingly (main text, lines 280-289).

      “Although we studied TMS and tACS propagation through the E-field modeling and observed an increase in the precuneus gamma oscillatory activity, excitability and connectivity with the hippocampi, we cannot exclude that our results might reflect the consequences of stimulating more superficial parietal regions other than the precuneus nor report direct evidence of microscopic changes in the brain after the stimulation. Invasive neurophysiological recordings in humans for this type of study are not feasible due to ethical constraints. Studies on cadavers or rodents would not fully resolve our question due to significant differences between them (i.e. rodents do not have an anatomical correspondence while cadavers have an alterations in electrical conductivity occurring in postmortem tissue). However, further exploration of this aspect in future studies would help in the understanding of γtACS+iTBS effects.”

      (2) If there is any brain activity triggered by the current stimulation parameters, then it is extremely difficult to understand how this activity can lead to enhancing memory. The brain is complex. There are hundreds of neuronal types. Each neuron receives precise input from about 10,000 other neurons with highly tuned synaptic strengths. Let us assume that the current protocol does lead to enhancing (or inhibiting) simultaneously the activity of millions of neurons. It is unclear whether there is any activity at all in the brain triggered by this protocol, it is also unclear whether such activity would be excitatory, or inhibitory. It is also unclear how many neurons, let alone what types of neurons would change their activity. How is it possible that this can lead to memory enhancement? This seems like using a hammer to knock on my laptop and hope that the laptop will output a new Mozart-like sonata.

      Thank you for your comment. As you correctly point out, we still do not have precise knowledge of which neurons—and to what extent—are activated during non-invasive brain stimulation in humans. However, this challenge is not limited to brain stimulation but applies to many other therapeutic interventions, including psychiatric medications, without limiting their use.

      Nevertheless, a substantial body of research has investigated the mechanisms underlying the efficacy of TMS and tACS in producing behavioral after-effects, primarily through its ability to induce long-term potentiation (Bliss & Collingridge, The Journal of Physiology, 1993a; Ridding & Rothwell, Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2007; Huang et al., Clinical Neurophysiology, 2017; Koch et al., Neuroimage 2018; Koch et al., Brain 2022; Jannati et al., Neuropsychopharmacology, 2023; Wischnewski et al., Trends in Cognitive Science, 2023; Griffiths et al., Trends in Neuroscience, 2023).

      We acknowledge that we took this important aspect for granted. We consequently expanded the introduction accordingly (main text, lines 48-60).

      “Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) are two forms of NIBS widely used to enhance memory performances (Grover et al., 2022; Koch et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2014). rTMS, based on the principle of Faraday, induces depolarization of cortical neuronal assemblies and leads to after-effects that have been linked to changes in synaptic plasticity involving mechanisms of long-term potentiation (LTP) (Huang et al., 2017; Jannati et al., 2023). On the other hand, tACS causes rhythmic fluctuations in neuronal membrane potentials, which can bias spike timing, leading to an entrainment of the neural activity (Wischnewski et al., 2023). In particular, the induction of gamma oscillatory a has been proposed to play an important role in a type of LTP known as spike timing-dependent plasticity, which depends on a precise temporal delay between the firing of a presynaptic and a postsynaptic neuron (Griffiths and Jensen, 2023). Both LTP and gamma oscillations have a strong link with memory processes such as encoding (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Griffiths and Jensen, 2023; Rossi et al., 2001), pointing to rTMS and tACS as good candidates for memory enhancement.”

      (3) Even if there is any kind of brain activation, it is unclear why the authors seem to be so sure that the precuneus is responsible. Are there neurophysiological data demonstrating that the current protocol only activates neurons in the precuneus? Of note, the non-invasive measurements shown in Figure 3 are very weak (Figure 3A top and bottom look very similar, and Figure 3C left and right look almost identical). Even if one were to accept the weak alleged differences in Figure 3, there is no indication in this figure that there is anything specific to the precuneus, rather a whole brain pattern. This would be the kind of minimally rigorous type of evidence required to make such claims. In a less convincing fashion, one could look at different positions of the stimulation apparatus. This would not be particularly compelling in terms of making a statement about the precuneus. But at least it would show that the position does matter, and over what range of distances it matters, if it matters.

      Thank you for your feedback. Our assumption that the precuneus plays a key role in the observed effects is based on several factors:

      (1) The non-invasive stimulation protocol was applied to an individually identified precuneus for each participant. Given existing evidence on TMS propagation, we can reasonably assume that the precuneus was at least a mediator of the observed effects (Ridding & Rothwell, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 2007). For further details about target identification and TMS and tACS propagation, please refer to the MRI data acquisition section in the main text and Biophysical modeling and E-field calculation section in the supplementary materials.

      (2) To investigate the effects of the neuromodulation protocol on cortical responses, we conducted a whole-brain analysis using multiple paired t-tests comparing each data point between different experimental conditions. To minimize the type I error rate, data were permuted with the Monte Carlo approach and significant p-values were corrected with the false discovery rate method (see the Methods section for details). The results identified the posterior-medial parietal areas as the only regions showing significant differences across conditions.

      (3) To control for potential generalized effects, we included a control condition in which TMS-EEG recordings were performed over the left parietal cortex (adjacent to the precuneus). This condition did not yield any significant results, reinforcing the cortical specificity of the observed effects.

      However, as stated in the Discussion, we do not claim that precuneus activity alone accounts for the observed effects. As shown in Experiment 4, stimulation led to connectivity changes between the precuneus and hippocampus, a network widely recognized as a key contributor to long-term memory formation (Bliss & Collingridge, Nature 1993). These connectivity changes suggest that precuneus stimulation triggered a ripple effect extending beyond the stimulation site, engaging the broader precuneus-hippocampus network.

      Regarding Figure 3A, it represents the overall expression of oscillatory activity detected by TMS-EEG. Since each frequency band has a different optimal scaling, the figure reflects a graphical compromise. A more detailed representation of the significant results is provided in Figure 3B. The effect sizes for gamma oscillatory activity in the delta T1 and T2 conditions were 0.52 and 0.50, respectively, which correspond to a medium effect based on Cohen’s d interpretation.

      We add a paragraph in the discussion to improve the clarity of the manuscript regarding this important aspect (lines 193-198).

      “Given the existing evidence on TMS propagation and the computation of the Biophysical model with the Efield, we can reasonably assume that the individually identified PC was a mediator of the observed effects (Ridding and Rothwell, 2007). Moreover, we observed specific cortical changes in the posteromedial parietal areas, as evidenced by the whole-brain analysis conducted on TMS-EEG data and the absence of effect on the lateral posterior parietal cortex used as a control condition.”

      (4) In the absence of any neurophysiological documentation of a direct impact on the brain, an argument in this type of study is that the behavioral results show that there must be some kind of effect. I agree with this argument. This is also the argument for placebo effects, which can be extremely powerful and useful even if the mechanism is unrelated to what is studied. Then let us dig into the behavioral results.

      Hoping to have already addressed your concern regarding the neurophysiological impact of the stimulation on the brain, we would like to emphasize that the behavioral results were obtained controlling for placebo effects. This was achieved by having participants perform the task under different stimulation conditions, including a sham condition.

      4a. There does not seem to be any effect on the STMB task, therefore we can ignore this.

      4b. The FNAT task is minimally described in the supplementary material. There are no experimental details to understand what was done. What was the size of the images? How long were the images presented for? Were there any repetitions of the images? For how long did the participants study the images? Presumably, all the names and occupations are different? What were the genders of the faces? What is chance level performance? Presumably, the same participant saw different faces across the different stimulation conditions. If not, then there can be memory effects across different conditions that are even more complex to study. If yes, then it would be useful to show that the difficulty is the same across the different stimuli.

      We thank you for signaling the lack in the description of FNAT task. We added the information required in the supplementary information (lines 93-101).

      “Each picture's face size was 19x15cm. In the learning phase, faces were shown along with names and occupations for 8 seconds each (totaling approximately 2 minutes). During immediate recall, the faces were displayed alone for 8 seconds. In the delayed recall and recognition phase, pictures were presented until the subject provided answers. We used a different set of stimuli for each stimulation condition, resulting in a total of 3 parallel task forms balanced across conditions and session order. All parallel forms comprised 6 male and 6 female faces; for each sex, there were 2 young adults (around 30 years old), 2 middle-aged adults (around 50 years old), and 2 elderly adults (around 70 years old). Before the experiments, we conducted a pilot study to ensure no differences existed between the parallel forms of the task.”

      The chance level in the immediate and delayed recall is not quantifiable since the participants had to freely recall the name and the occupation without a multiple choice. In the recognition, the chance level was around 33% (since the possible answers were 3).

      4c. Although not stated clearly, if I understand FNAT correctly, the task is based on just 12 presentations. Each point in Figure 2A represents a different participant. Unfortunately, there is no way of linking the performance of individual participants across the conditions with the information provided. Lines joining performance for each participant would be useful in this regard. Because there are only 12 faces, the results are quantized in multiples of 100/12 % in Figure 3A. While I do not doubt that the authors did their homework in terms of the statistical analyses, it is difficult to get too excited about these 12 measurements. For example, take Figure 3A immediate condition TOTAL, arguably the largest effect in the whole paper. It seems that on average, the participants may remember one more face/name/occupation.

      Thank you for the suggestion. We added graphs showing lines linking the performance of individual participants across conditions to improve clarity, please see Fig.2 revised. We apologize for the lack of clarity in the description of the FNAT. As you correctly pointed out, we used the percentage based on the single association between face, name and occupation (12 in total). However, each association consisted of three items, resulting in a total of 36 items to learn and associate – we added a paragraph to make it more explicit in the manuscript (lines 425-430).

      “We considered a correct association when a subject was able to recall all the information for each item (i.e. face, name and occupation), resulting in a total of 36 items to learn and associate. To further investigate the effect on FNAT we also computed a partial recall score accounting for those items where subjects correctly matched only names with faces (FNAT NAME) and only occupations with faces (FNAT OCCUPATION). See supplementary information for score details.”

      In the example you mentioned, participants were, on average, able to correctly recall and associate three more items compared to the other conditions. While this difference may not seem striking at first glance, it is important to consider that we assessed memory performance after a single, three-minute stimulation session. Similar effects are typically observed only after multiple stimulation sessions (Koch et al., NeuroImage, 2018; Grover et al., Nature Neuroscience, 2022). Moreover, memory performance changes are often measured by a limited set of stimuli due to methodological constraints related to memory capacity. For example, Rey Auditory Verbal learning task, requiring to learn and recall 15 words, is a typical test used to detect memory changes (Koch et al., Neuroimage, 2018; Benussi et al., Brain stimulation 2021; Benussi et al., Annals of Neurology, 2022). 

      4d. Block effects. If I understand correctly, the experiments were conducted in blocks. This is always problematic. Here is one example study that articulated the big problems in block designs (Li et al TPAMI 2021):https://ieeexplore.ieee.org/document/9264220

      Thank you for the interesting reference. According to this paper, in a block design, EEG or fMRI recordings are performed in response to different stimuli of a given class presented in succession. If this is the case, it does not correspond to our experimental design where both TMS-EEG and fMRI were conducted in resting state on different days according to the different stimulation conditions.

      4e. Even if we ignore the lack of experimental descriptions, problems with lack of evidence of brain activity, the minimalistic study of 12 faces, problems with the block design, etc. at the end of the day, the results are extremely weak. In FNAT, some results are statistically significant, some are not. The interpretation of all of this is extremely complex. Continuing with Figure 3A, it seems that the author claims that iTBS+gtACS > iTBS+sham-tACS, but iTBS+gtACS ~ sham+sham. I am struggling to interpret such a result. When separating results by name and occupation, the results are even more perplexing. There is only one condition that is statistically significant in Figure 3A NAME and none in the occupation condition.

      Thank you again for your feedback. Hoping to have thoroughly addressed your initial concerns in our previous responses, we now move on to your observations regarding the behavioral results, assuming you were referring to Figure 2A. The main finding of this study is the improvement in long-term memory performance, specifically the ability to correctly recall the association between face, name, and occupation (total FNAT), which was significantly enhanced in both Experiments 1 and 2. However, we also aimed to explore the individual contributions of name and occupation separately to gain a deeper understanding of the results. Our analysis revealed that the improvement in total FNAT was primarily driven by an increase in name recall rather than occupation recall. We understand that this may have caused some confusion. We consequently modified the manuscript in the (lines 97-99; 107-111; 425-430) to make it clearer and moved the graph relative to FNAT NAME and OCCUPATION from fig.2 in the main text to fig. S4 in supplementary information.

      “Dual iTBS+γtACS increased the performances in recalling the association between face, name and occupation (FNAT accuracy) both for the immediate (F<sub>2,38</sub>=7.18; p =0.002; η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub>=0.274) and the delayed (F<sub>2,38</sub>=5.86; p =0.006; η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub>=0.236) recall performances (Fig. 2, panel A).”

      “The in-depth analysis of the FNAT accuracy investigating the specific contribution of face-name and face-occupation recall reveald that dual iTBS+γtACS increased the performances in the association between face and name (FNAT NAME) delayed recall (F<sub>2,38</sub> =3.46; p =0.042; η<sup>2</sup>p =0.154; iTBS+γtACS vs. sham-iTBS+sham-tACS: 42.9±21.5 % vs. 33.8±19 %; p=0.048 Bonferroni corrected) (Fig. S4, supplementary information).”

      “We considered a correct association when a subject was able to recall all the information for each item (i.e. face, name and occupation), resulting in a total of 36 items to learn and associate. To further investigate the effect on FNAT we also computed a partial recall score accounting for those items where subjects correctly matched only names with faces (FNAT NAME) and only occupations with faces (FNAT OCCUPATION). See supplementary information for score details.”

      Regarding the stimulation conditions, your concerns about the performance pattern (iTBS+gtACS > iTBS+sham-tACS, but iTBS+gtACS ~ sham+sham) are understandable. However, this new protocol was developed precisely in response to the variability observed in behavioral outcomes following non-invasive brain stimulation, particularly when used to modulate memory functions (Corp et al., 2020; Pabst et al., 2022). As discussed in the manuscript, it is intended as a boost to conventional non-invasive brain stimulation protocols, leveraging the mechanisms outlined in the Discussion section.

      (5) In sum, it would be amazing to be able to use non-invasive stimulation for any kind of therapeutic purpose as the authors imagine. More work needs to be done to convince ourselves that this kind of approach is viable. The evidence provided in this study is weak.

      We hope our response will be carefully considered, fostering a constructive exchange and leading to a reassessment of your evaluation.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript "Dual transcranial electromagnetic stimulation of the precuneus-hippocampus network boosts human long-term memory" by Borghi and colleagues provides evidence that the combination of intermittent theta burst TMS stimulation and gamma transcranial alternating current stimulation (γtACS) targeting the precuneus increases long-term associative memory in healthy subjects compared to iTBS alone and sham conditions. Using a rich dataset of TMS-EEG and resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) maps and structural MRI data, the authors also provide evidence that dual stimulation increased gamma oscillations and functional connectivity between the precuneus and hippocampus. Enhanced memory performance was linked to increased gamma oscillatory activity and connectivity through white matter tracts.

      Strengths:

      The combination of personalized repetitive TMS (iTBS) and gamma tACS is a novel approach to targeting the precuneus, and thereby, connected memory-related regions to enhance long-term associative memory. The authors leverage an existing neural mechanism engaged in memory binding, theta-gamma coupling, by applying TMS at theta burst patterns and tACS at gamma frequencies to enhance gamma oscillations. The authors conducted a thorough study that suggests that simultaneous iTBS and gamma tACS could be a powerful approach for enhancing long-term associative memory. The paper was well-written, clear, and concise.

      Weaknesses:

      (1) The study did not include a condition where γtACS was applied alone. This was likely because a previous work indicated that a single 3-minute γtACS did not produce significant effects, but this limits the ability to isolate the specific contribution of γtACS in the context of this target and memory function

      Thank you for your comments. As you pointed out, we did not include a condition where γtACS was applied alone. This decision was based on the findings of Guerra et al. (Brain Stimulation 2018), who investigated the same protocol and reported no aftereffects. Given the substantial burden of the experimental design on patients and our primary goal of demonstrating an enhancement of effects compared to the standalone iTBS protocol, we decided to leave out this condition. However, you raise an important aspect that should be further discussed, we modified the limitation section accordingly (lines 290-297).

      “We did not assess the effects of γtACS alone. This decision was based on the findings of Guerra et al. (Guerra et al., 2018), who investigated the same protocol and reported no aftereffects. Given the substantial burden of the experimental design on patients and our primary goal of demonstrating an enhancement of effects compared to the standalone iTBS protocol, we decided to leave out this condition. While examining the effects of γtACS alone could help isolate its specific contribution to this target and memory function, extensive research has shown that achieving a cognitive enhancement aftereffect with tACS alone typically requires around 20–25 minutes of stimulation (Grover et al., 2023).”

      (2) The authors applied stimulation for 3 minutes, which seems to be based on prior tACS protocols. It would be helpful to present some rationale for both the duration and timing relative to the learning phase of the memory task. Would you expect additional stimulation prior to recall to benefit long-term associative memory?

      Thank you for your comment and for raising this interesting point. As you correctly noted, the protocol we used has a duration of three minutes, a choice based on previous studies demonstrating its greater efficacy with respect to single stimulation from a neurophysiological point of view. Specifically, these studies have shown that the combined stimulation enhanced gamma-band oscillations and increased cortical plasticity (Guerra et al., Brain Stimulation 2018; Maiella et al., Scientific Reports 2022). Given that the precuneus (Brodt et al., Science 2018; Schott et al., Human Brain Mapping 2018), gamma oscillations (Osipova et al., Journal of Neuroscience 2006; Deprés et al., Neurobiology of Aging 2017; Griffiths et al., Trends in Neurosciences 2023), and cortical plasticity (Brodt et al., Science 2018) are all associated with memory formation and encoding processes, we decided to apply the co-stimulation immediately before it to enhance the efficacy. We added this paragraph to the manuscript rationale (lines 48-60).

      “Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) and transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) are two forms of NIBS widely used to enhance memory performances (Grover et al., 2022; Koch et al., 2018; Wang et al., 2014). rTMS, based on the principle of Faraday, induces depolarization of cortical neuronal assemblies and leads to after-effects that have been linked to changes in synaptic plasticity involving mechanisms of long-term potentiation (LTP) (Huang et al., 2017; Jannati et al., 2023). On the other hand, tACS causes rhythmic fluctuations in neuronal membrane potentials, which can bias spike timing, leading to an entrainment of the neural activity (Wischnewski et al., 2023). In particular, the induction of gamma oscillatory a has been proposed to play an important role in a type of LTP known as spike timing-dependent plasticity, which depends on a precise temporal delay between the firing of a presynaptic and a postsynaptic neuron (Griffiths and Jensen, 2023). Both LTP and gamma oscillations have a strong link with memory processes such as encoding (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Griffiths and Jensen, 2023; Rossi et al., 2001), pointing to rTMS and tACS as good candidates for memory enhancement.”

      Regarding the question of whether stimulation could also benefit recall, the answer is yes. We can speculate that repeating the stimulation before recall might provide an additional boost. This is supported by evidence showing that both the precuneus and gamma oscillations are involved in recall processes (Flanagin et al., Cerebral Cortex 2023; Griffiths et al., Trends in Neurosciences 2023). Furthermore, previous research suggests that reinstating the same brain state as during encoding can enhance recall performance (Javadi et al., The Journal of Neuroscience 2017). We added this consideration to the discussion (lines 305-311).

      “Future studies should further investigate the effects of stimulation on distinct memory processes. In particular, stimulation could be applied before retrieval (Rossi et al., 2001), to better elucidate its specific contribution to the observed enhancements in memory performance. Additionally, it would be worth examining whether repeated stimulation - administered both before encoding and before retrieval - could produce a boosting effect. This is especially relevant in light of findings showing that matching the brain state between retrieval and encoding can significantly enhance memory performance (Javadi et al., 2017).”

      (3) How was the burst frequency of theta iTBS and gamma frequency of tACS chosen? Were these also personalized to subjects' endogenous theta and gamma oscillations? If not, were increases in gamma oscillations specific to patients' endogenous gamma oscillation frequencies or the tACS frequency?

      The stimulation protocol was chosen based on previous studies (Guerra et al., Brain Stimulation 2018; Maiella et al., Scientific Reports 2022).  Gamma tACS sinusoid frequency wave was set at 70 Hz while iTBS consisted of ten bursts of three pulses at 50 Hz lasting 2 s, repeated every 10 s with an 8 s pause between consecutive trains, for a total of 600 pulses total lasting 190 s (see iTBS+γtACS neuromodulation protocol section). In particular, the theta iTBS has been inspired by protocols used in animal models to elicit LTP in the hippocampus (Huang et al., Neuron 2005). Consequently, neither Theta iTBS nor the gamma frequency of tACS were personalized. The increase in gamma oscillations was referred to the patient’s baseline and did not correspond to the administrated tACS frequency.

      (4) The authors do a thorough job of analyzing the increase in gamma oscillations in the precuneus through TMS-EEG; however, the authors may also analyze whether theta oscillations were also enhanced through this protocol due to the iTBS potentially targeting theta oscillations. This may also be more robust than gamma oscillations increases since gamma oscillations detected on the scalp are very low amplitude and susceptible to noise and may reflect activity from multiple overlapping sources, making precise localization difficult without advanced techniques.

      Thank you for the suggestion. We analyzed theta oscillations, finding no changes.

      (5) Figure 4: Why are connectivity values pre-stimulation for the iTBS and sham tACS stimulation condition so much higher than the dual stimulation? We would expect baseline values to be more similar.

      We acknowledge that the pre-stimulation connectivity values for the iTBS and sham tACS conditions appear higher than those for the dual stimulation condition. However, as noted in our statistical analyses, there were no significant differences at baseline between conditions (p-FDR= 0.3514), suggesting that any apparent discrepancy is due to natural variability rather than systematic bias. One potential explanation for these differences is individual variability in baseline connectivity measures, which can fluctuate due to factors such as intrinsic neural dynamics, participant state, or measurement noise. Despite these variations, our statistical approach ensures that any observed post-stimulation effects are not confounded by pre-existing differences.

      (6) Figure 2: How are total association scores significantly different between stimulation conditions, but individual name and occupation associations are not? Further clarification of how the total FNAT score is calculated would be helpful.

      We apologize for any lack of clarity. The total FNAT score reflects the ability to correctly recall all the information associated with a person—specifically, the correct pairing of the face, name, and occupation. Participants received one point for each triplet they accurately recalled. The scores were then converted into percentages, as detailed in the Face-Name Associative Task Construction and Scoring section in the supplementary materials.

      Total FNAT was the primary outcome measure. However, we also analyzed name and occupation recall separately to better understand their partial contributions. Our analysis revealed that the improvement in total FNAT was primarily driven by an increase in name recall rather than occupation recall.

      We acknowledge that this distinction may have caused some confusion. To improve clarity, we revised the manuscript accordingly (lines 97-98; 107-111; 425-430).

      “Dual iTBS+γtACS increased the performances in recalling the association between face, name and occupation (FNAT accuracy) both for the immediate (F<sub>2,38</sub>=7.18 ;p=0.002; η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub>=0.274) and the delayed (F<sub>2,38</sub>=5.86;p=0.006; η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub>=0.236) recall performances (Fig. 2, panel A).”

      “The in-depth analysis of the FNAT accuracy investigating the specific contribution of face-name and face-occupation recall revealed that dual iTBS+γtACS increased the performances in the association between face and name (FNAT NAME) delayed recall (F<sub>2,38</sub> =3.46; p =0.042; η<sup>2</sup>p =0.154; iTBS+γtACS vs. sham-iTBS+sham-tACS: 42.9±21.5 % vs. 33.8±19 %; p=0.048 Bonferroni corrected) (Fig. S4, supplementary information).”

      “We considered a correct association when a subject was able to recall all the information for each item (i.e. face, name and occupation), resulting in a total of 36 items to learn and associate. To further investigate the effect on FNAT we also computed a partial recall score accounting for those items where subjects correctly matched only names with faces (FNAT NAME) and only occupations with faces (FNAT OCCUPATION). See supplementary information for score details.”

      We also moved the data regarding the specific contribution of name and occupation recall in the supplementary information (fig.S4) and further specified how we computed the score in the score (lines 102-104).

      “The score was computed by deriving an accuracy percentage index dividing by 12 and multiplying by 100 the correct association sum. The partial recall scores were computed in the same way only considering the sum of face-name (NAME) and face-occupation (OCCUPATION) correctly recollected.”

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Borghi and colleagues present results from 4 experiments aimed at investigating the effects of dual γtACS and iTBS stimulation of the precuneus on behavioral and neural markers of memory formation. In their first experiment (n = 20), they found that a 3-minute offline (i.e., prior to task completion) stimulation that combines both techniques leads to superior memory recall performance in an associative memory task immediately after learning associations between pictures of faces, names, and occupation, as well as after a 15-minute delay, compared to iTBS alone (+ tACS sham) or no stimulation (sham for both iTBS and tACS). Performance in a second task probing short-term memory was unaffected by the stimulation condition. In a second experiment (n = 10), they show that these effects persist over 24 hours and up to a full week after initial stimulation. A third (n = 14) and fourth (n = 16) experiment were conducted to investigate the neural effects of the stimulation protocol. The authors report that, once again, only combined iTBS and γtACS increase gamma oscillatory activity and neural excitability (as measured by concurrent TMS-EEG) specific to the stimulated area at the precuneus compared to a control region, as well as precuneus-hippocampus functional connectivity (measured by resting-state MRI), which seemed to be associated with structural white matter integrity of the bilateral middle longitudinal fasciculus (measured by DTI).

      Strengths:

      Combining non-invasive brain stimulation techniques is a novel, potentially very powerful method to maximize the effects of these kinds of interventions that are usually well-tolerated and thus accepted by patients and healthy participants. It is also very impressive that the stimulation-induced improvements in memory performance resulted from a short (3 min) intervention protocol. If the effects reported here turn out to be as clinically meaningful and generalizable across populations as implied, this approach could represent a promising avenue for the treatment of impaired memory functions in many conditions.

      Methodologically, this study is expertly done! I don't see any serious issues with the technical setup in any of the experiments (with the only caveat that I am not an expert in fMRI functional connectivity measures and DTI). It is also very commendable that the authors conceptually replicated the behavioral effects of experiment 1 in experiment 2 and then conducted two additional experiments to probe the neural mechanisms associated with these effects. This certainly increases the value of the study and the confidence in the results considerably.

      The authors used a within-subject approach in their experiments, which increases statistical power and allows for stronger inferences about the tested effects. They are also used to individualize stimulation locations and intensities, which should further optimize the signal-to-noise ratio.

      Weaknesses:

      I want to state clearly that I think the strengths of this study far outweigh the concerns I have. I still list some points that I think should be clarified by the authors or taken into account by readers when interpreting the presented findings.

      I think one of the major weaknesses of this study is the overall low sample size in all of the experiments (between n = 10 and n = 20). This is, as I mentioned when discussing the strengths of the study, partly mitigated by the within-subject design and individualized stimulation parameters. The authors mention that they performed a power analysis but this analysis seemed to be based on electrophysiological readouts similar to those obtained in experiment 3. It is thus unclear whether the other experiments were sufficiently powered to reliably detect the behavioral effects of interest. That being said, the authors do report significant effects, so they were per definition powered to find those. However, the effect sizes reported for their main findings are all relatively large and it is known that significant findings from small samples may represent inflated effect sizes, which may hamper the generalizability of the current results. Ideally, the authors would replicate their main findings in a larger sample. Alternatively, I think running a sensitivity analysis to estimate the smallest effect the authors could have detected with a power of 80% could be very informative for readers to contextualize the findings. At the very least, however, I think it would be necessary to address this point as a potential limitation in the discussion of the paper.

      Thank you for the observation. As you mentioned, our power analysis was based on our previous study investigating the same neuromodulation protocol with a corresponding experimental design. The relatively small sample could be considered a possible limitation of the study which we will add to the discussion. A fundamental future step will be to replay these results on a larger population, however, to strengthen our results we performed the sensitivity analysis you suggested.

      In detail, we performed a sensitivity analysis for repeated-measures ANOVA with α=0.05 and power(1-β)=0.80 with no sphericity correction. For experiment 1, a sensitivity analysis with 1 group and 3 measurements showed a minimal detectable effect size of f=0.524 with 20 participants. In our paper, the ANOVA on total FNAT immediate performance revealed an effect size of η<sup>2</sup>=0.274 corresponding to f=0.614; the ANOVA on FNAT delayed performance revealed an effect size of η<sup>2</sup>=0.236 corresponding to f=0.556. For experiment 2, a sensitivity analysis for total FNAT immediate performance (1 group and 3 measurements) showed a minimal detectable effect size of f=0.797 with 10 participants. In our paper, the ANOVA on total FNAT immediate performance revealed an effect size of η<sup>2</sup>=0.448 corresponding to f=0.901. The sensitivity analysis for total FNAT delayed performance (1 group and 6 measurements) showed a minimal detectable effect size of f=0.378 with 10 participants. In our paper, the ANOVA on total FNAT delayed performance revealed an effect size of η<sup>2</sup>=0.484 corresponding to f=0.968. Thus, the sensitivity analysis showed that both experiments were powered enough to detect the minimum effect size computed in the power analysis. We have now added this information to the manuscript and we thank the reviewer for her/his suggestion in the statistical analysis and results section (lines 99-100; 127-128; 130-131; 543-545).

      “The sensitivity analysis showed a minimal detectable effect size of  η<sup>2</sup>=0.215 with 20 participants.”

      “The sensitivity analysis showed a minimal detectable effect size of  η<sup>2</sup>=0.388 with 10 participants.”

      “The sensitivity analysis showed a minimal detectable effect size of η<sup>2</sup>=0.125 with 10 participants.”

      “Since we do not have an a priori effect size for experiment 1 and 2, we performed a sensitivity power analysis to ensure that these experiments were able to detect the minimum effect size with 80% power and alpha level of 0.05.”

      It seems that the statistical analysis approach differed slightly between studies. In experiment 1, the authors followed up significant effects of their ANOVAs by Bonferroni-adjusted post-hoc tests whereas it seems that in experiment 2, those post-hoc tests where "exploratory", which may suggest those were uncorrected. In experiment 3, the authors use one-tailed t-tests to follow up their ANOVAs. Given some of the reported p-values, these choices suggest that some of the comparisons might have failed to reach significance if properly corrected. This is not a critical issue per se, as the important test in all these cases is the initial ANOVA but non-significant (corrected) post-hoc tests might be another indicator of an underpowered experiment. My assumptions here might be wrong, but even then, I would ask the authors to be more transparent about the reasons for their choices or provide additional justification. Finally, the authors sometimes report exact p-values whereas other times they simply say p < .05. I would ask them to be consistent and recommend using exact p-values for every result where p >= .001.

      Thank you again for the suggestions. Your observations are correct, we used a slightly different statistical depending on our hypothesis. Here are the details:

      In experiment 1, we used a repeated-measure ANOVA with one factor “stimulation condition” (iTBS+γtACS; iTBS+sham-tACS; sham-iTBS+sham-tACS). Following the significant effect of this factor we performed post-hoc analysis with Bonferroni correction.

      In experiment 2, we used a repeated-measures with two factors “stimulation condition” and “time”. As expected, we observed a significant effect of condition, confirming the result of experiment 1, but not of time. Thus, this means that the neuromodulatory effect was present regardless of the time point. However, to explore whether the effects of stimulation condition were present in each time point we performed some explorative t-tests with no correction for multiple comparisons since this was just an explorative analysis.

      In experiment 3, we used the same approach as experiment 1. However, since we had a specific hypothesis on the direction of the effect already observed in our previous study, i.e. increase in spectral power (Maiella et al., Scientific Report 2022), our tests were 1-tailed.

      For the p-values, we corrected the manuscript reporting the exact values for every result.

      While the authors went to great lengths trying to probe the neural changes likely associated with the memory improvement after stimulation, it is impossible from their data to causally relate the findings from experiments 3 and 4 to the behavioral effects in experiments 1 and 2. This is acknowledged by the authors and there are good methodological reasons for why TMS-EEG and fMRI had to be collected in sperate experiments, but it is still worth pointing out to readers that this limits inferences about how exactly dual iTBS and γtACS of the precuneus modulate learning and memory.

      Thank you for your comment. We fully agree with your observation, which is why this aspect has been considered in the study's limitations. To address your concern, we add this sentence to the limitation discussion (lines 299-301).

      “Consequently, these findings do not allow precise inferences regarding the specific mechanisms by which dual iTBS and γtACS of the precuneus modulate learning and memory.”

      There were no stimulation-related performance differences in the short-term memory task used in experiments 1 and 2. The authors argue that this demonstrates that the intervention specifically targeted long-term associative memory formation. While this is certainly possible, the STM task was a spatial memory task, whereas the LTM task relied (primarily) on verbal material. It is thus also possible that the stimulation effects were specific to a stimulus domain instead of memory type. In other words, could it be possible that the stimulation might have affected STM performance if the task taxed verbal STM instead? This is of course impossible to know without an additional experiment, but the authors could mention this possibility when discussing their findings regarding the lack of change in the STM task.

      Thank you for your interesting observation. We argue that the intervention primarily targeted long-term associative memory formation, as our findings demonstrated effects only on FNAT. However, as you correctly pointed out, we cannot exclude the possibility that the stimulation may also influence short-term verbal associative memory. We add this aspect when discussing the absence of significant findings in the STM task (lines 205-210).

      “Visual short-term associative memory, measured by STBM performance, was not modulated by any experimental condition. Even if we cannot exclude the possibility that the stimulation could have influenced short-term verbal associative memory, we expected this result since short-term associative memory is known to rely on a distinct frontoparietal network while FNAT, used to investigate long-term associative memory, has already been associated with the neural activity of the PC and the hippocampus (Parra et al., 2014; Rentz et al., 2011).”

      While the authors discuss the potential neural mechanisms by which the combined stimulation conditions might have helped memory formation, the psychological processes are somewhat neglected. For example, do the authors think the stimulation primarily improves the encoding of new information or does it also improve consolidation processes? Interestingly, the beneficial effect of dual iTBS and γtACS on recall performance was very stable across all time points tested in experiments 1 and 2, as was the performance in the other conditions. Do the authors have any explanation as to why there seems to be no further forgetting of information over time in either condition when even at immediate recall, accuracy is below 50%? Further, participants started learning the associations of the FNAT immediately after the stimulation protocol was administered. What would happen if learning started with a delay? In other words, do the authors think there is an ideal time window post-stimulation in which memory formation is enhanced? If so, this might limit the usability of this procedure in real-life applications.

      Thank you for your comment and for raising these important points.

      We hypothesized that co-stimulation would enhance encoding processes. Previous studies have shown that co-stimulation can enhance gamma-band oscillations and increase cortical plasticity (Guerra et al., Brain Stimulation 2018; Maiella et al., Scientific Reports 2022). Given that the precuneus (Brodt et al., Science 2018; Schott et al., Human Brain Mapping 2018), gamma oscillations (Osipova et al., Journal of Neuroscience 2006; Deprés et al., Neurobiology of Aging 2017; Griffiths et al., Trends in Neurosciences 2023), and cortical plasticity (Brodt et al., Science 2018) have all been associated with encoding processes, we decided to apply co-stimulation before the encoding phase, to boost it. We enlarged the introduction to specify the link between neural mechanisms and the psychological process of the encoding (lines 55-60).

      “In particular, the induction of gamma oscillatory activity has been proposed to play an important role in a type of LTP known as spike timing-dependent plasticity, which depends on a precise temporal delay between the firing of a presynaptic and a postsynaptic neuron (Griffiths and Jensen, 2023). Both LTP and gamma oscillations have a strong link with memory processes such as encoding (Bliss and Collingridge, 1993; Griffiths and Jensen, 2023; Rossi et al., 2001), pointing to rTMS and tACS as good candidates for memory enhancement.”

      We applied the co-stimulation immediately before the learning phase to maximize its potential effects. While we observed a significant increase in gamma oscillatory activity lasting up to 20 minutes, we cannot determine whether the behavioral effects we observed would have been the same with a co-stimulation applied 20 minutes before learning. Based on existing literature, a reduction in the efficacy of co-stimulation over time could be expected (Huang et al., Neuron 2005; Thut et al., Brain Topography 2009). However, we hypothesize that multiple stimulation sessions might provide an additional boost, helping to sustain the effects over time (Thut et al., Brain Topography 2009; Koch et al., Neuroimage 2018; Koch et al., Brain 2022).

      Regarding the absence of further forgetting in both stimulation conditions, we think that the clinical and demographical characteristics of the sample (i.e. young and healthy subjects) explain the almost absence of forgetting after one week.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      To address the concerns, the authors should:

      (1) Include invasive neuronal recordings (e.g., in rats or monkeys if not possible in humans) demonstrating that the current stimulation protocol leads to direct changes in brain activity.

      We understand the interest of the first reviewer in the understanding of neurophysiological correlates of the stimulation protocol, however, we are skeptical about this request as we think it goes beyond the aims of the study. As already mentioned in the response to the reviewer, invasive neurophysiological recordings in humans for this type of study are not feasible due to ethical constraints. At the same time, studies on cadavers or rodents would not fully resolve the question. Indeed, the authors of the study cited by the reviewer (Mihály Vöröslakos et al., Nature Communications, 2018) highlight the impossibility of drawing definitive conclusions about the exact voltage required in the in-vivo human brain due to significant differences between rats and humans, as well as the in-vivo human cadavers due to alterations in electrical conductivity that occur in postmortem tissue. Huang and colleagues addressed the difficulties in reaching direct evidence of non-invasive brain stimulation (NIBS) effects in a review published in Clinical Neurophysiology in 2017. They conclude that the use of EEG to assess brain response to TMS has a great potential for a less indirect demonstration of plasticity mechanisms induced by NIBS in humans.

      It is exactly to meet the need to investigate the changes in brain activity after the stimulation protocol that we conducted Experiments 3 and 4. These experiments respectively examined the neurophysiological and connectivity changes induced by the stimulation in a non-invasive manner using TMS-EEG and fMRI. The observed changes in brain oscillatory activity (increased gamma oscillatory activity), cortical excitability (enhanced posteromedial parietal cortex reactivity), and brain connectivity (strengthened connections between the precuneus and hippocampi) provided evidence of the effects of our non-invasive brain stimulation protocol, further supporting the behavioral data.

      Additionally, we carefully considered the issue of stimulation distribution and, in response, performed a biophysical modeling analysis and E-field calculation using the parameters employed in our study (see Supplementary Materials).

      Acknowledging the reviewer's point of view, we modified the manuscript accordingly, discussing this aspect both as a technical limitation and as a potential direction for future research (main text, lines 280-289).

      “Although we studied TMS and tACS propagation through the E-field modeling and observed an increase in the precuneus gamma oscillatory activity, excitability and connectivity with the hippocampi, we cannot exclude that our results might reflect the consequences of stimulating more superficial parietal regions other than the precuneus nor report direct evidence of microscopic changes in the brain after the stimulation. Invasive neurophysiological recordings in humans for this type of study are not feasible due to ethical constraints. Studies on cadavers or rodents would not fully resolve our question due to significant differences between them (i.e. rodents do not have an anatomical correspondence while cadavers have an alterations in electrical conductivity occurring in postmortem tissue). However, further exploration of this aspect in future studies would help in the understanding of γtACS+iTBS effects.”

      (2) Address all the technical questions about the experimental design.

      We addressed all the technical questions about the experimental design.

      (3) Repeat the experiments with randomized trial order and without a block design.

      The experiments were conducted with randomized trial order and we did not use a block design.

      (4) Add many more faces to the study. It is extremely difficult to draw any conclusion from merely 12 faces. Ideally, there would be lots of other relevant memory experiments where the authors show compelling positive results.

      We understand your perplexity about drawing conclusions from 12 faces, however, this is not the case. As we explained in the response reviewer, the task we implemented did not rely on the recall of merely 12 faces. Instead, participants had to correctly learn, associate and recall 12 faces, 12 names and 12 occupations for a total of 36 items. To improve the clarity of the manuscript, we added a paragraph to make this aspect more explicit (lines 425-430).

      “We considered a correct association when a subject was able to recall all the information for each item (i.e. face, name and occupation), resulting in a total of 36 items to learn and associate. To further investigate the effect on FNAT we also computed a partial recall score accounting for those items where subjects correctly matched only names with faces (FNAT NAME) and only occupations with faces (FNAT OCCUPATION). See supplementary information for score details.”

      The behavioral changes we observed are similar to those who are typically observed after multiple stimulation sessions (Koch et al., NeuroImage, 2018; Grover et al., Nature Neuroscience, 2022, Benussi et al., Annals of Neurology, 2022). Moreover, memory performance changes are often measured by a limited set of stimuli due to methodological constraints related to memory capacity. For example, Rey Auditory Verbal learning task, requiring to learn and recall 15 words, is a typical test used to detect memory changes (Koch et al., Neuroimage, 2018; Benussi et al., Brain stimulation 2021; Benussi et al., Annals of Neurology, 2022). 

      (5) Provide a clear explanation of the apparent randomness of which results are statistically significant or not in Figure 3. But perhaps with many more experiments, a lot more memory evaluations, many more stimuli, and addressing all the other technical concerns, either the results will disappear or there will be a more interpretable pattern of results.

      We provided explanations for all the concerns shown by the reviewer.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      Minor comments:

      (1) Figure 4: Why are connectivity values pre-stimulation for the iTBS and sham tACS stimulation condition so much higher than the dual stimulation? We would expect baseline values to be more similar.

      We acknowledge that the pre-stimulation connectivity values for the iTBS and sham tACS conditions appear higher than those for the dual stimulation condition. However, as noted in our statistical analyses, there were no significant differences at baseline between conditions (p-FDR= 0.3514), suggesting that any apparent discrepancy is due to natural variability rather than systematic bias. One potential explanation for these differences is individual variability in baseline connectivity measures, which can fluctuate due to factors such as intrinsic neural dynamics, participant state, or measurement noise. Despite these variations, our statistical approach ensures that any observed post-stimulation effects are not confounded by pre-existing differences.

      (2) Figure 2: How are total association scores significantly different between stimulation conditions, but individual name and occupation associations are not? Further clarification of how the total FNAT score is calculated would be helpful.

      We apologize for any lack of clarity. The total FNAT score reflects the ability to correctly recall all the information associated with a person—specifically, the correct pairing of the face, name, and occupation. Participants received one point for each triplet they accurately recalled. The scores were then converted into percentages, as detailed in the Face-Name Associative Task Construction and Scoring section in the supplementary materials.

      Total FNAT was the primary outcome measure. However, we also analyzed name and occupation recall separately to better understand their partial contributions. Our analysis revealed that the improvement in total FNAT was primarily driven by an increase in name recall rather than occupation recall.

      We acknowledge that this distinction may have caused some confusion. To improve clarity, we revised the manuscript accordingly (lines 97-98; 107-111; 425-430).

      “Dual iTBS+γtACS increased the performances in recalling the association between face, name and occupation (FNAT accuracy) both for the immediate (F<sub>2,38</sub>=7.18; p=0.002; η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub>=0.274) and the delayed (F<sub>2,38</sub>=5.86; p =0.006; η<sup>2</sup><sub>p</sub>=0.236) recall performances (Fig. 2, panel A).”

      “The in-depth analysis of the FNAT accuracy investigating the specific contribution of face-name and face-occupation recall revealed that dual iTBS+γtACS increased the performances in the association between face and name (FNAT NAME) delayed recall (F<sub>2,38</sub> =3.46; p =0.042; η<sup>2</sup>p =0.154; iTBS+γtACS vs. sham-iTBS+sham-tACS: 42.9±21.5 % vs. 33.8±19 %; p=0.048 Bonferroni corrected) (Fig. S4, supplementary information).”

      “We considered a correct association when a subject was able to recall all the information for each item (i.e. face, name and occupation), resulting in a total of 36 items to learn and associate. To further investigate the effect on FNAT we also computed a partial recall score accounting for those items where subjects correctly matched only names with faces (FNAT NAME) and only occupations with faces (FNAT OCCUPATION). See supplementary information for score details.”

      We also moved the data regarding the specific contribution of name and occupation recall in the supplementary information (fig.S4) and further specified how we computed the score in the score (lines 102-104).

      “The score was computed by deriving an accuracy percentage index dividing by 12 and multiplying by 100 the correct association sum. The partial recall scores were computed in the same way only considering the sum of face-name (NAME) and face-occupation (OCCUPATION) correctly recollected.”

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      A very small detail, in the caption for Figure 2A, OCCUPATION is described as being shown on the 'left' but it should be 'right'.

      We corrected this error.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      (1) Figure 1: It might be simpler to streamline  acronyms for different test cases, e.g,  E01contra, E01 ipsi (rather than EO1IPS), E02, and control. Thus, it would be possible to label  each of the three schematic panels as E01, E02, control.

      Please describe what the dots in the brain mean and move the V1 label so it does not occlude  dots.

      Please make clear that the "track reconstructions" are the bright spheres in the micrographs (there are track-like elements in some micrographs which may be tears or?)

      Thank you. We relabeled the groups as control, EO1contra, EO1ipsi, and EO2. These were  changed in all figures and in the document at several places.

      We indicated in the new caption that “Dots schematize ocular dominance columns”.

      We indicated that electrode track penetrations were the “(bright spots at right/posterior)”.

      (2) Figure 2: Should "horizontal" be vertical (line  556) of the caption? When describing the  scale bar for firing rate, please explain the meaning of italicized vs regular font.

      Please make the purple lines in Figures I and J easier to see (invisible in my PDF).

      Not quite clear what is significantly different from what when viewing the figure at a glance.  Would it be possible to clarify using standard methods?

      Yes, it should say vertical, thank you. We explained the italics (they denote the standard scale  bar size if no number is provided.)

      We changed the purple lines to yellow in all figures.

      We added comparison bars that help indicate significance.

      (3) Figures 3-5. Please make corrections like those  noted above.

      Yes, we applied the previous changes to Figures 3 - 5.

      (4) Minor. Sometimes the authors spell out temporal  frequency and sometimes abbreviate it.  Perhaps adopt a consistent style.

      Fixed, thanks.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      (1) The assessment of the tuning properties is  based on fits to the data. Presumably,  neurons for which the fits were poor were excluded? It would be useful to know what the criteria  were, how many neurons were excluded, and whether there was a significant difference  between the groups in the numbers of neurons excluded (which could further point to  differences between the groups).

      Yes, this is an important omission, thank you for catching it. We now write in methods (line 213):  “ Inclusion/exclusion: For each stimulus type, we examined  the set of all responses to visual  stimuli and blanks with an ANOVA test to evaluate the null hypothesis that the mean response  to all of these stimuli were the same; cells with a p<0.05 to this visual responsiveness test were  included in fits and analyses, and cells with p>0.05 were excluded. ”

      (2) For the temporal frequency data, low- and high-frequency  cut-offs are defined, but then  only used for the computation of the bandwidth. Given that the responses to low temporal  frequencies change profoundly with premature eye opening, it would be useful to directly  compare the low- and high-frequency cut-offs between groups, in addition to the index that is  currently used.

      We now provide this data in Figure 3 - figure supplement  1 .

      (3) In addition to the tuning functions and firing  rates that have been analyzed so far, are  there any differences in the temporal profiles of neural responses between the groups  (sustained versus transient responses, rates of adaptation, latency)? If the temporal dynamics  of the responses are altered significantly, that could be part of an explanation for the altered  temporal tuning.

      This is a great topic for future studies. Unfortunately, with drifting gratings, it is difficult to  establish these properties, which could be better assessed with standing or  square-wave-modulated gratings or other stimuli. We did not run standing gratings in our battery  of stimuli for this initial study.

      (4) It would be beneficial for the general interpretation  of the results to extend the discussion. First, it would be useful to provide a more detailed discussion of what type of visual information might make it through the closed eyelids (the natural state), in contrast to the structured  information available through open eyes. Second, it would be useful to highlight more clearly  that these data were collected in peripheral V1 by discussing what might be expected in  binocular, more central V1 regions. Third, it would be interesting to discuss the observed  changes in firing rates in the context of the development of inhibitory neurons in V1 (which still  undergo significant changes through the time period of premature visual experience chosen  here).

      Thank you, good ideas. Let’s take these three suggestions in turn.

      First, in the discussion, we added a subsection “ Biology  of early development in mustelids ” that  focuses on the developmental conditions of wild and laboratory animals:

      In the wild, mustelids raise their young in nests in the ground, in cavities such as holes in trees  or caves, or in areas of dense vegetation (Ruggiero et al. 1994). They may move the young  from one nest to another as they grow, but otherwise the young are primarily in the relatively  dark nest. It is highly likely that some light penetrates and that information about the 24-hour  cycle is available, but the light is likely to be dim and unlikely to provide a basis for high  luminance, high contrast stimulation through the closed lids. The animals begin to spend  substantial time outside the nest after eye opening.

      The ferret is a domesticated strain of the European polecat. In laboratory settings, ferret  jills give birth and keep their kits in a nest box. A laboratory typically maintains a 24-hour cycle  with 12 or 14 hours of light, and the light reaching the closed lids must first pass through the  cage, the nest box, and the nesting material. Therefore, developing ferrets have an obvious  circadian light signal but the light available for image formation is likely dim and of low contrast.

      Although the light that reaches the close lids in developing ferrets is likely to be relatively  dim, and any image-forming signal passing through the closed lids would be highly filtered in  luminance, spatial frequency, and contrast, it is important to remember that visual input before  natural eye opening (through the closed lids) can drive activity in retina, LGN, and cortex  (Huttenlocher 1967, Chapman and Stryker 1993, Krug et al., 2001, Akerman et al., 2002,Akerman et al., 2004). Further, orientation selectivity can be observed through the closed lids  (Krug et al., 2001), indicating that some coarse image-forming information does make it through  the closed lids.

      Second, we added text speculating about binocular cortex (lines 492 - 500): … our recordings  were performed in monocular cortex so that we could be sure of the developmental condition of  the eye that drove the classic responses. It is interesting to speculate about what might occur  more centrally in binocular visual cortex. Ocular dominance shifts are not induced when one eye  is opened prematurely (Issa et al 1999), indicating that ocular dominance plasticity is not  engaged at this early stage, but one might imagine that the impacts on temporal frequency and  spontaneous firing rates would still be present.

      Third, on inhibition, we added a paragraph (lines 502 - 509):

      We introduced premature patterned vision at a time when cortical inhibition is undergoing  substantial changes. GABAergic signaling has already undergone its switch (Ben-Ari, 2002)  from providing primarily depolarizing input to hyperpolarizing input by P21-23 (Mulholland et al.,  2021). In the days prior to eye opening, inhibitory cells exhibit activity that is closely associated  with the emerging functional modules that will reflect orientation columns (Mulholland et al.,  2021), but do not yet exhibit selectivity to orientation, in contrast to excitatory neurons, which do  exhibit selectivity to orientation at that time (Chang and Fitzpatrick, 2022).

      (5) In the methods section, the statement 'actively  kept in nesting box' is unclear. Presumably  this means that the jill prevents the kits from leaving the nesting box? It also would be worth at  least mentioning in this context that there obviously are still visual events in the nesting box too.

      Thanks. We improved this description (lines 118 - 121):  Ferret kits in laboratory housing receive  limited visual stimulation through their closed lids, as the mother actively keeps the kits in their  relatively dark nest . In order to ensure that animals  with early-opened eyes actually had  patterned visual experience  (and animals with closed  lids had the same stimulation filtered  through the lids) , animals were brought to the lab  for 2 hours a day for 4 consecutive days  beginning at P25.

      (6) The stimulus presentation could be more clearly  described. Is every stimulus presented in  an individual trial (surrounded by periods with a blank screen), or are all stimuli shown as a  continuous sequence? The description of the parameter screening is also potentially confusing  ('orientation was co-varied with stimuli consisting of drifting gratings at different spatial  frequencies' sounds as if there are separate stimuli for orientation; might be better to say  something like 'in the first set, orientation, spatial frequency, ... were covaried...')

      Yes, thank you, we fixed this (lines 184 - 201). We deleted the text indicated and added a  sentence “Each individual grating stimulus was full screen and had a single set of parameters  (direction, spatial frequency, temporal frequency), and was separated from the other stimuli by a  gray screen interstimulus interval.”. We also deleted a repetition of 100% contrast in the  description of the second set.

      (7) Description of low-pass index is unclear. What  is the 'largest temporal frequency response  observed'? The maximum response or the response to the largest temporal frequency tested?

      Thanks. We added a paragraph at line 236:

      We defined a low pass index as the response to the lowest temporal frequency tested (in this case 0.5 Hz) to the maximum response obtained to the set of temporal frequencies shown. LPI =  R(TF=0.5 Hz)/max(R(TF=0.5Hz), R(TF=1Hz), … R(TF=32Hz)).  If a cell exhibited the highest  firing for a temporal frequency of 0.5 Hz, then it would have an low pass index of 1. If it  exhibited a similar firing rate in response to a temporal frequency of 0.5 Hz even if the preferred  temporal frequency were higher, then the low pass index would still be near 1. If the cell  responded poorly at a temporal frequency of 0.5 Hz, then it would have a low pass index near 0.

      (8) The discussion should also cite the results  of strobe-reared cats by Pasternak et al (1981  and 1985).

      Thank you for pointing out the omission. We now write (lines 430-435):  Cats raised in a  strobe-light environment (mostly after eye opening) exhibited strong changes in subsequent  direction selectivity (Kennedy and Orban 1983; Humphrey and Saul 1998)  and behavioral  sensitivity to motion (Pasternak et al., 1981; Pasternak et al., 1985) that partially recovers with  motion detection training . However, temporal frequency  tuning of these animals has not been  reported in detail.  Pasternak et al (1981) reported  that strobe-reared ferrets exhibited greater  difficulty in distinguishing slow moving stimuli from static stimuli compared to controls, an  ability that slightly improved with practice, suggesting possible temporal frequency deficits.

      (9) Finally, it would be useful to include a mention  of the early development of MT in  marmosets in the discussion of impacts of prematurity on motion vision (Bourne & Rosa 2006).

      Yes, thank you. We cited Bourne & Rosa and also Lempel and Nielsen (for ferret PSS). (Lines  492-501):

      Several other basic mechanistic questions remain unanswered. It is unclear where in the visual  circuit cascade these deficits first arise. Does the lateral geniculate nucleus or retina exhibit  altered temporal frequency tuning? Is the influence of the patterned visual stimulation  instructive, so that if one provided premature stimulation with only certain temporal frequencies,  one would see selectivity for those temporal frequencies, or would tuning always be broad?  Other questions remain concerning the top-down influence on V1 from “higher” motion areas  such as MT (monkeys) or PSS (ferret); MT exhibits mature neural markers earlier than V1  (Bourne and Rosa, 2006), and suppression of PSS impacts motion selectivity in V1 (Lempel and  Nielsen, 2021).  Future studies will be needed to  address these questions.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public review):

      Summary:

      Phytophathogens including fungal pathogens such as F. graminearum remain a major threat to agriculture and food security. Several agriculturally relevant fungicides including the potent Quinofumelin have been discovered to date, yet the mechanisms of their action and specific targets within the cell remain unclear. This paper sets out to contribute to addressing these outstanding questions.

      We appreciate the reviewer's accurate summary of our manuscript.

      Strengths:

      The paper is generally well-written and provides convincing data to support their claims for the impact of Quinofumelin on fungal growth, the target of the drug, and the potential mechanism. Critically the authors identify an important pyrimidine pathway dihydroorotate dehydrogenase (DHODH) gene FgDHODHII in the pathway or mechanism of the drug from the prominent plant pathogen F. graminearum, confirming it as the target for Quinofumelin. The evidence is supported by transcriptomic, metabolomic as well as MST, SPR, molecular docking/structural biology analyses.

      We appreciate the reviewer's recognition of the strengths of our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      Whilst the study adds to our knowledge about this drug, it is, however, worth stating that previous reports (although in different organisms) by Higashimura et al., 2022 https://pmc.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/articles/PMC9716045/ had already identified DHODH as the target for Quinofumelin and hence this knowledge is not new and hence the authors may want to tone down the claim that they discovered this mechanism and also give sufficient credit to the previous authors work at the start of the write-up in the introduction section rather than in passing as they did with reference 25? other specific recommendations to improve the text are provided in the recommendations for authors section below.

      We appreciate the reviewer's suggestion. In the revised manuscript, we have incorporated the reference in the introduction section and expanded the discussion of previous work on quinofumelin by Higashimura et al., 2022 in the discussion section to more effectively contextualize their contributions. Moreover, we have made revisions and provided responses in accordance with the recommendations.

      Reviewer #2 (Public review):

      Summary:

      In the current study, the authors aim to identify the mode of action/molecular mechanism of characterized a fungicide, quinofumelin, and its biological impact on transcriptomics and metabolomics in Fusarium graminearum and other Fusarium species. Two sets of data were generated between quinofumelin and no treatment group, and differentially abundant transcripts and metabolites were identified. The authors further focused on uridine/uracil biosynthesis pathway, considering the significant up- and down-regulation observed in final metabolites and some of the genes in the pathways. Using a deletion mutant of one of the genes and in vitro biochemical assays, the authors concluded that quinofumelin binds to the dihydroorotate dehydrogenase.

      We appreciate the reviewer's accurate summary of our manuscript.

      Strengths:

      Omics datasets were leveraged to understand the physiological impact of quinofumelin, showing the intracellular impact of the fungicide. The characterization of FgDHODHII deletion strains with supplemented metabolites clearly showed the impact of the enzyme on fungal growth.

      We appreciate the reviewer's recognition of the strengths of our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      Some interpretation of results is not accurate and some experiments lack controls. The comparison between quinofumelin-treated deletion strains, in the presence of different metabolites didn't suggest the fungicide is FgDHODHII specific. A wild type is required in this experiment.

      Potential Impact: Confirming the target of quinofumelin may help understand its resistance mehchanism, and further development of other inhibitory molecules against the target.

      The manuscript would benefit more in explaining the study rationale if more background on previous characterization of this fungicide on Fusarium is given.

      We appreciate the reviewer's suggestion. Under no treatment with quinofumelin, mycelial growth remains normal and does not require restoration. In the presence of quinofumelin treatment, the supplementation of downstream metabolites in the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway can restore mycelial growth that is inhibited by quinofumelin. The wild-type control group is illustrated in Figure 4. Figure 5b depicts the phenotypes of the deletion mutants. With respect to the relationship among quinofumelin, FgDHODHII, and other metabolites, quinofumelin specifically targets the key enzyme FgDHODHII in the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway, disrupting the conversion of dihydroorotate to orotate, which consequently inhibits the synthesis downstream metabolites including uracil. In our previous study, quinofumelin not only exhibited excellent antifungal activity against the mycelial growth and spore germination of F. graminearum, but also inhibited the biosynthesis of deoxynivalenol (DON). We have added this part to the introduction section.

      Reviewer #3 (Public review):

      Summary:

      The manuscript shows the mechanism of action of quinofumelin, a novel fungicide, against the fungus Fusarium graminearum. Through omics analysis, phenotypic analysis, and in silico approaches, the role of quinofumelin in targeting DHODH is uncovered.

      We appreciate the reviewer's accurate summary of our manuscript.

      Strengths:

      The phenotypic analysis and mutant generation are nice data and add to the role of metabolites in bypassing pyrimidine biosynthesis.

      We appreciate the reviewer's recognition of the strengths of our manuscript.

      Weaknesses:

      The role of DHODH in this class of fungicides has been known and this data does not add any further significance to the field. The work of Higashimura et al is not appreciated well enough as they already showed the role of quinofumelin upon DHODH II.

      There is no mention of the other fungicide within this class ipflufenoquin, as there is ample data on this molecule.

      We appreciate the reviewer's suggestion. We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's insightful comment regarding the work of Higashimura et al. We agree that their investigation into the role of quinofumelin in DHODH II inhibition provides critical foundational insights for this field. In the revised manuscript, we have incorporated the reference in the introduction section and expanded the discussion of their work in the discussion section to more effectively contextualize their contributions. The information regarding action mechanism of ipflufenoquin against filamentous fungi was added in discussion section.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Given that the DHODH gene had been identified as a target earlier, could the authors perform blast experiments with this gene instead and let us know the percentage similarity between the FgDHODHII gene and the Pyricularia oryzae class II DHODH gene in the report by Higashimura et al., 2022.

      BLAST experiment revealed that the percentage similarity between the FgDHODHII gene and the class II DHODH gene of P. oryzae was 55.41%. We have added the description ‘Additionally, the amino acid sequence of the FgDHODHII exhibits 55.41% similarity to that of DHODHII from Pyricularia oryzae, as previously reported (Higashimura et al., 2022)’ in section Results.

      (2) Abstract:

      The authors started abbreviating new terms e.g. DEG, DMP, etc but then all of a sudden stopped and introduced UMP with no full meaning of the abbreviation. Please give the full meaning of all abbreviations in the text, UMP, STC, RM, etc.

      We have provided the full meaning for all abbreviations as requested.

      (3) Introduction section:

      The introduction talks very little about the work of other groups on quinofumelin. Perhaps add this information in and reference them including the work of Higashimura et al., 2022 which has done quite significant work on this topic but is not even mentioned in the background

      We have added the work of other groups on quinofumelin in section introduction.

      (4) General statements:

      Please show a model of the pyrimidine pathway that quinofumelin attacks to make it easier for the reader to understand the context. They could just copy this from KEGG

      We have added the model (Fig. 7).

      (5) Line 186:

      The authors did a great job of demonstrating interactions with the Quinofumelin and went to lengths to perform MST, SPR, molecular docking, and structural biology analyses yet in the end provide no details about the specific amino acid residues involved in the interaction. I would suggest that site-directed mutagenesis studies be performed on FgDHODHII to identify specific amino acid residues that interact with Quinofumelin and show that their disruption weakens Quinofumelin interaction with FgDHODHII.

      Thank you for this insightful suggestion. We fully agree with the importance of elucidating the interaction mechanism. At present, we are conducting site-directed mutagenesis studies based on interaction sites from docking results and the mutation sites of FgDHODHII from the resistant mutants; however, due to the limitations in the accuracy of existing predictive models, this work remains ongoing. Additionally, we are undertaking co-crystallization experiments of FgDHODHII with quinofumelin to directly and precisely reveal their interaction pattern

      (6) Line 76:

      What is the reference or evidence for the statement 'In addition, quinofumelin exhibits no cross-resistance to currently extensively used fungicides, indicating its unique action target against phytopathogenic fungi.

      If two fungicides share the same mechanism of action, they will exhibit cross resistance. Previous studies have demonstrated that quinofumelin retains effective antifungal activity against fungal strains resistant to commercial fungicides, indicating that quinofumelin does not exhibit cross-resistance with other commercially available fungicides and possesses a novel mechanism of action. Additionally, we have added the relevant inference.

      (7) Line 80-82:

      Again, considering the work of previous authors, this target is not newly discovered. Please consider toning down this statement 'This newly discovered selective target for antimicrobial agents provides a valuable resource for the design and development of targeted pesticides.'

      We have rewritten the description of this sentence.

      (8) Line 138: If the authors have identified DHODH in experimental groups (I assume in F. graminearum), what was the exact locus tag or gene name in F. graminearum, and why not just continue with this gene you identified or what is the point of doing a blast again to find the gene if the DHODH gene if it already came up in your transcriptomic or metabolic studies? This unfortunately doesn't make sense but could be explained better.

      The information of FgDHODHII (gene ID: FGSG_09678) has been added. We have revised this part.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations for the authors):

      (1) Line 40:

      Please add a reference.

      We have added the reference

      (2) Line 47:

      Please add a reference.

      We have added the reference.

      (3) Line 50:

      The lack of target diversity in existing fungicides doesn't necessarily serve as a reason for discovering new targets being more challenging than identifying new fungicides within existing categories, please consider adjusting the argument here. Instead, the authors can consider reasons for the lack of new targets in the field.

      We have revised the description.

      (4) Line 63:

      Please cite your source with the new technology.

      We have added the reference.

      (5) Line 68:

      What are you referring to for "targeted medicine", do you have a reference?

      We have revised the description and the reference.

      (6) Line 74:

      One of the papers referred to "quinoxyfen", what are the similarities and differences between the two? Please elaborate for the readership.

      Quinoxyfen, similar to quinofumelin, contains a quinoline ring structure. It inhibits mycelial growth by disrupting the MAP kinase signaling pathway in fungi (https://www.frac.info). In addition, quinoxyfen still exhibits excellent antifungal activity against the quinofumelin-resistant mutants (the findings from our group), indicating that action mechanism for quinofumelin and quinoxyfen differ.

      (7) Line 84:

      Please introduce why RNA-Seq was designed in the study first. What were the groups compared? How was the experiment set up? Without this background, it is hard to know why and how you did the experiment.

      According to your suggestions, we have added the description in Section Results. In addition, the experimental process was described in Section Materials and methods as follows: A total of 20 mL of YEPD medium containing 1 mL of conidia suspension (1×105 conidia/mL) was incubated with shaking (175 rpm/min) at 25°C. After 24 h, the medium was added with quinofumelin at a concentration of 1 μg/mL, while an equal amount of dimethyl sulfoxide was added as the control (CK). The incubation continued for another 48 h, followed by filtration and collection of hyphae. Carry out quantitative expression of genes, and then analyze the differences between groups based on the results of DESeq2 for quantitative expression.

      (8) Figures:

      The figure labeling is missing (Figures 1,2,3 etc). Please re-order your figure to match the text

      The figures have been inserted.

      (9) Line. 97:

      "Volcano plot" is a common plot to visualize DEGs, you can directly refer to the name.

      We have revised the description.

      (10) Figure 1d, 1e:

      Can you separate down- and up-regulated genes here? Does the count refer to gene number?

      The expression information for down- and up-regulated genes is presented in Figure 1a and 1b. However, these bubble plots do not distinguish down- and up-regulated genes. Instead, they only display the significant enrichment of differentially expressed genes in specific metabolic pathways. To more clearly represent the data, we have added the detailed counts of down- and up-regulated genes for each metabolic pathway in Supplementary Table S1 and S2. Here, the term "count" refers to differentially expressed genes that fall within a certain pathway.

      (11) Line 111:

      Again, no reasoning or description of why and how the experiment was done here.

      Based on the results of KEGG enrichment analysis, DEMs are associated with pathways such as thiamine metabolism, tryptophan metabolism, nitrogen metabolism, amino acid sugar and nucleotide sugar metabolism, pantothenic acid and CoA biosynthesis, and nucleotide sugar production compounds synthesis. To specifically investigate the metabolic pathways involved action mechanism of quinofumelin, we performed further metabolomic experiments. Therefore, we have added this description according the reviewer’s suggestions.

      (12) Figure 2a:

      It seems many more metabolites were reduced than increased. Is this expected? Due to the antifungal activity of this compound, how sick is the fungus upon treatment? A physiological study on F. graminearum (in a dose-dependent manner) should be done prior to the omics study. Why do you think there's a stark difference between positive and negative modes in terms of number of metabolites down- and up-regulated?

      Quinofumelin demonstrates exceptional antifungal activity against Fusarium graminearum. The results indicate that the number of reduced metabolites significantly exceeds the number of increased metabolites upon quinofumelin treatment. Mycelial growth is markedly inhibited under quinofumelin exposure. Prior to conducting omics studies, we performed a series of physiological and biochemical experiments (refer to Qian Xiu's dissertation https://paper.njau.edu.cn/openfile?dbid=72&objid=50_49_57_56_49_49&flag=free). Upon quinofumelin treatment, the number of down-regulated metabolites notably surpasses that of up-regulated metabolites compared to the control group. Based on the findings from the down-regulated metabolites, we conducted experiments by exogenously supplementing these metabolites under quinofumelin treatment to investigate whether mycelial growth could be restored. The results revealed that only the exogenous addition of uracil can restore mycelial growth impaired by quinofumelin.

      Quinofumelin exhibits an excellent antifungal activity against F. graminearum. At a concentration of 1 μg/mL, quinofumelin inhibits mycelial growth by up to 90%. This inhibitory effect indicates that life activities of F. graminearum are significantly disrupted by quinofumelin. Consequently, there is a marked difference in down- and up-regulated metabolites between quinofumelin-treated group and untreated control group. The detailed results were presented in Figures 1 and 2.

      (13) Figure 2e:

      This is a good analysis. To help represent the data more clearly, the authors can consider representing the expression using fold change with a p-value for each gene.

      To more clearly represent the data, we have incorporated the information on significant differences in metabolites in the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway, as affected by quinofumelin, in accordance with the reviewer’s suggestions.

      (14) Line 142:

      Please indicate fold change and p-value for statistical significance. Did you validate this by RT-qPCR?

      We validated the expression level of the DHODH gene under quinofumelin treatment using RT-qPCR. The results indicated that, upon treatment with the EC50 and EC90 concentrations of quinofumelin, the expression of the DHODH gene was significantly reduced by 11.91% and 33.77%, respectively (P<0.05). The corresponding results have been shown in Figure S4.

      (15) Line 145:

      It looks like uracil is the only metabolite differentially abundant in the samples - how did you conclude this whole pathway was impacted by the treatment?

      The experiments involving the exogenous supplementation of uracil revealed that the addition of uracil could restore mycelial growth inhibited by quinofumelin. Consequently, we infer that quinofumelin disrupts the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway. In addition, as uracil is the end product of the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway, the disruption of this pathway results in a reduction in uracil levels.

      (16) Figure 3:

      What sequence was used as the root of the tree? Why were the species chosen? Since the BLAST query was Homo sapiens sequence, would it be good to use that as the root?

      FgDHODHII sequence was used as the root of the tree. These selected fungal species represent significant plant-pathogenic fungi in agriculture production. According to your suggestion, we have removed the BLAST query of Homo sapiens in Figure 3.

      (17) Figure 4:

      How were the concentrations used to test chosen?

      Prior to this experiment, we carried out concentration-dependent exogenous supplementation experiments. The results indicated that 50 μg/mL of uracil can fully restore mycelial growth inhibited by quinofumelin. Consequently, we chose 50 μg/mL as the testing concentration.

      (18) Line 164:

      Why do you hypothesize supplementing dihydroorotate would restore resistance? The metabolite seemed accumulated in the treatment condition, whereas downstream metabolites were comparable or even depleted. The DHODH gene expression was suppressed. Would accumulation of dihydroorotate be associated with growth inhibition by quinofumelin? Please include the hypothesis and rationale for the experimental setup.

      DHODH regulates the conversion of dihydroorotate to orotate in the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway. The inhibition of DHODH by quinofumelin results in the accumulation of dihydroorotate and the depletion of the downstream metabolites, including UMP, uridine and uracil. Consequently, downstream metabolites were considered as positive controls, while upstream metabolite dihydroorotate served as a negative control. This design further demonstrates DHODH as action target of quinofumelin against F. graminearum. In addition, the accumulation of dihydroorotate is not associated with growth inhibition by quinofumelin; however, but the depletion of downstream metabolites in the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway is closely associated with growth inhibition by quinofumelin.

      (19) Line 168:

      I'm not sure if this conclusion is valid from your results in Figure 4 showing which metabolites restore growth.

      o minimize the potential influence of strain-specific effects, five strains were tested in the experiments shown in Figure 4. For each strain, the first row (first column) corresponds to control condition, while second row (first column) represents treatment with 1 μg/mL of quinofumelin, which completely inhibits mycelial growth. The second row (second column) for each strain represents the supplementation with 50 μg/mL of dihydroorotate fails to restore mycelial growth inhibited by quinofumelin. In contrast, the second row (third column, fourth column, fifth colomns) for each strain demonstrated that the supplementation of 50 μg/mL of UMP, uridine and uracil, respectively, can effectively restore mycelial growth inhibited by quinofumelin.

      (20) Figure 5a:

      The fact you saw growth of the deletion mutant means it's not lethal. However, the growth was severely inhibited.

      Our experimental results indicate that the growth of the deletion mutant is lethal. The mycelial growth observed originates from mycelial plugs that were not exposed to quinofumelin, rather than from the plates amended with quinofumelin.

      (21) Figure 5b:

      Would you expect different restoration of growth in the presence of quinofumelin vs. no treatment? The wild type control is missing here. Any conclusions about the relationship between quinofumelin, FgDHODHII, and other metabolites in the pathway?

      Under no treatment with quinofumelin, mycelial growth remains normal and does not require restoration. In the presence of quinofumelin treatment, the supplementation of downstream metabolites in the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway can restore mycelial growth that is inhibited by quinofumelin. The wild-type control group is illustrated in Figure 4. Figure 5b depicts the phenotypes of the deletion mutants. With respect to the relationship among quinofumelin, FgDHODHII, and other metabolites, quinofumelin specifically targets the key enzyme FgDHODHII in the de novo pyrimidine biosynthesis pathway, disrupting the conversion of dihydroorotate to orotate, which consequently inhibits the synthesis downstream metabolites including uracil.

      (22) Figure 6b:

      Lacking positive and negative controls (known binder and non-binder). What does the Kd (in comparison to other interactions) indicate in terms of binding strength?

      We tested the antifungal activities of publicly reported DHODH inhibitors (such as leflunomide and teriflunomide) against F. graminearum. The results showed that these inhibitors exhibited no significant inhibitory effects against the strain PH-1. Therefore, we lacked an effective chemical for use as a positive control in subsequent experiments. Biacore experiments offers detailed insights into molecular interactions between quinofumelin and DHODHII. As shown in Figure 6b, the left panel illustrates the time-dependent kinetic curve of quinofumelin binding to DHODHII. Within the first 60 s after quinofumelin was introduced onto the DHODHII surface, it bound to the immobilized DHODHII on the chip surface, with the response value increasing proportionally to the quinofumelin concentration. Following cessation of the injection at 60 s, quinofumelin spontaneously dissociated from the DHODHII surface, leading to a corresponding decrease in the response value. The data fitting curve presented on the right panel indicates that the affinity constant KD of quinofumelin for DHODHII is 6.606×10-6 M, which falls within the typical range of KD values (10-3 ~ 10-6 M) for protein-small molecule interaction patterns. A lower KD value indicates a stronger affinity; thus, quinofumelin exhibits strong binding affinity towards DHODHII.

      Reviewer #3 (Recommendations for the authors):

      The authors should add information about the other molecule within this class, ipflufenoquin, and what is known about it. There are already published data on its mode of action on DHODH and the role of pyrimidine biosynthesis.

      We have added the information regarding action mechanism of ipflufenoquin against filamentous fungi in discussion section.

      The work of Higashimura et al is not appreciated well enough as they already showed the role of quinofumelin upon DHODH II.

      We sincerely appreciate the reviewer's insightful comment regarding the work of Higashimura et al. We agree that their investigation into the role of quinofumelin in DHODH II inhibition provides critical foundational insights for this field. In the revised manuscript, we have incorporated the reference in the introduction section and expanded the discussion of their work in the discussion section to more effectively contextualize their contributions.

      It is unclear how the protein model was established and this should be included. What species is the molecule from and how was it obtained? How are they different from Fusarium?

      The three-dimensional structural model of F. graminearum DHODHII protein, as predicted by AlphaFold, was obtained from the UniProt database. Additionally, a detailed description along with appropriate citations has been incorporated in the ‘Manuscript’ file.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      We thank the reviewer for the positive feedback on the work. The reviewer has raised two weaknesses and in the following we discuss how those can be addressed.  

      Weaknesses:

      The impact of the article is limited by using a network with discrete time- steps, and only a small number of time steps from stimulus to reward. They assume that each time step is on the order of hundreds of ms. They justify this by pointing to some slow intrinsic mechanisms, but they do not implement these slow mechanisms is a network with short time steps, instead they assume without demonstration that these could work as suggested. This is a reasonable first approximation, but its validity should be explicitly tested.

      Our goal here was to give a proof of concept that online random feedback is sufficient to train an RNN to estimate value. Indeed, it is important to show that the idea works in a model where the slow mechanisms are explicitly implemented. However, this is a non-trivial task and desired to be addressed in future works.  

      As the delay between cue and reward increases the performance decreases. This is not surprising given the proposed mechanism, but is still a limitation, especially given that we do not really know what a is the reasonable value of a single time step.

      In reply to this comment and the other reviewer's related comment, we have conducted two sets of additional simulations, one for examining incorporation of eligibility traces, and the other for considering (though not mechanistically implementing) behavioral time-scale synaptic plasticity (BTSP). We have added their results to the revised manuscript as Appendix. We think that the results addressed this point to some extent while how longer cue-reward delay can be learnt by elaboration of the model remains as a future issue.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      We thank the reviewer for the positive feedback on the work. The reviewer gave comments on our revisions, and here we discuss how those can be addressed.

      Comments on revisions: I would still want to see how well the network learns tasks with longer time delays (on the order of 100 or even 1000 timesteps). Previous work has shown that random feedback struggles to encode longer timescales (see Murray 2019, Figure 2), so I would be interested to see how that translates to the RL context in your model.

      We would like to note that in Murray et al 2019 the random feedback per se appeared not to be primarily responsible for the difficulty in encoding longer timesclaes. In the Figure 2d (Murray 2019), the author compared his RFLO (random feedback local online) and BPTT with two intermediate algorithms, which incorporated either one of the two approximations made in RFLO: i) random feedback instead of symmetric feedback, and ii) omittance of non-local effect (i.e., dependence of the derivative of the loss with respect to a given weight on the other weights). The performance difference between RFLO and BPTT was actually mostly explained by ii), as the author mentioned "The results show that the local approximation is essentially fully responsible for the performance difference between RFLO and BPTT, while there is no significant loss in performance due to the random feedback alone. (Line 6-8, page 7 of Murray, 2019, eLife)".

      Meanwhile, regarding the difference in the performance of the model with random feedback vs the model with symmetric feedback in our settings, actually it appeared (already) in the case with 6 time-steps or less (the biologically constrained model with random feedback performed worse: Fig. 6J, left).

      In practice, our model, either with random or symmetric feedback, would not be able to learn the cases with very long delays. This is indeed a limitation of our model. However, our model is critically different from the model of Murray 2019 in that we use RL rather than supervised learning and we use a scalar bootstrapped (TD) reward-prediction-error rather than the true output error. We would think that these differences may be major reasons for the limited learning ability of our model.

      Regarding the feasibility of the model when tasks involve longer time delays: Indeed this is a problem and the other reviewers have also raised the same point. Our model can be extended by incorporating either a kind of eligibility trace (similar one to those contained in RFLO and e-prop) or behavioral time-scale synaptic plasticity (BTSP), and we have added the results of simulations incorporating each to the revised manuscript as Appendix. But how longer cue-reward delay can be learnt by elaboration of the model remains as a future issue.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Comments on revisions: Thank you for addressing all my comments in your reply.

      We are happy to learn that all concerns raised by the reviewer in the previous round were addressed adequately. We agree with the reviewer that there are several ways the work can be improved.

      The various points raised by the reviewers at weaknesses are desired to be taken up in future works.

    1. Author response:

      The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

      Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      This manuscript provides an initial characterization of three new missense variants of the PLCG1 gene associated with diverse disease phenotypes, utilizing a Drosophila model to investigate their molecular effects in vivo. Through the meticulous creation of genetic tools, the study assesses the small wing (sl) phenotype - the fly's ortholog of PLCG1 - across an array of phenotypes from longevity to behavior in both sl null mutants and variants. The findings indicate that the Drosophila PLCG1 ortholog displays aberrant functions. Notably, it is demonstrated that overexpression of both human and Drosophila PLCG1 variants in fly tissue leads to toxicity, underscoring their pathogenic potential in vivo.

      Strengths:

      The research effectively highlights the physiological significance of sl in Drosophila. In addition, the study establishes the in vivo toxicity of disease-associated variants of both human PLCG1 and Drosophila sl.

      Weaknesses:

      The study's limitations include the human PLCG1 transgene's inability to compensate for the Drosophila sl null mutant phenotype, suggesting potential functional divergence between the species. This discrepancy signals the need for additional exploration into the mechanistic nuances of PLCG1 variant pathogenesis, especially regarding their gain-of-function effects in vivo.

      Overall:

      The study offers compelling evidence for the pathogenicity of newly discovered disease-related PLCG1 variants, manifesting as toxicity in a Drosophila in vivo model, which substantiates the main claim by the authors. Nevertheless, a deeper inquiry into the specific in vivo mechanisms driving the toxicity caused by these variants in Drosophila could significantly enhance the study's impact.

      Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

      The manuscript by Ma et al. reports the identification of three unrelated people who are heterozygous for de novo missense variants in PLCG1, which encodes phospholipase C-gamma 1, a key signaling protein. These individuals present with partially overlapping phenotypes including hearing loss, ocular pathology, cardiac defects, abnormal brain imaging results, and immune defects. None of the patients present with all of the above phenotypes. PLCG1 has also been implicated as a possible driver for cell proliferation in cancer.

      The three missense variants found in the patients result in the following amino acid substitutions: His380Arg, Asp1019Gly, and Asp1165Gly. PLCG1 (and the closely related PLCG2) have a single Drosophila ortholog called small wing (sl). sl-null flies are viable but have small wings with ectopic wing veins and supernumerary photoreceptors in the eye. As all three amino acids affected in the patients are conserved in the fly protein, in this work Ma et al. tested whether they are pathogenic by expressing either reference or patient variant fly or human genes in Drosophila and determining the phenotypes produced by doing so.

      Expression in Drosophila of the variant forms of PLCG1 found in these three patients is toxic; highly so for Asp1019Gly and Asp1165Gly, much more modestly for His380Arg. Another variant, Asp1165His which was identified in lymphoma samples and shown by others to be hyperactive, was also found to be toxic in the Drosophila assays. However, a final variant, Ser1021Phe, identified by others in an individual with severe immune dysregulation, produced no phenotype upon expression in flies.

      Based on these results, the authors conclude that the PLCG1 variants found in patients are pathogenic, producing gain-of-function phenotypes through hyperactivity. In my view, the data supporting this conclusion are robust, despite the lack of a detectable phenotype with Ser1021Phe, and I have no concerns about the core experiments that comprise the paper.

      Figure 6, the last in the paper, provides information about PLCG1 structure and how the different variants would affect it. It shows that the His380, Asp1019, and Asp1165 all lie within catalytic domains or intramolecular interfaces and that variants in the latter two affect residues essential for autoinhibition. It also shows that Ser1021 falls outside the key interface occupied by Asp1019, but more could have been said about the potential effects of Ser1021Phe.

      Overall, I believe the authors fully achieved the aims of their study. The work will have a substantial impact because it reports the identification of novel disease-linked genes, and because it further demonstrates the high value of the Drosophila model for finding and understanding gene-disease linkages.

      Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

      Summary:

      The paper attempts to model the functional significance of variants of PLCG2 in a set of patients with variable clinical manifestations.

      Strengths:

      A study attempting to use the Drosophila system to test the function of variants reported from human patients.

      Weaknesses:

      Additional experiments are needed to shore up the claims in the paper. These are listed below.

      Major Comments:

      (1) Does the pLI/ missense constraint Z score prediction algorithm take into consideration whether the gene exhibits monoallelic or biallelic expression?

      To our knowledge, pLI and missense Z don't consider monoallelic or biallelic expression. Instead, they reflect sequence constraint and are calculated based on the observed versus expected variant frequencies in population databases.

      (2) Figure 1B: Include human PLCG2 in the alignment that displays the species-wide conserved variant residues.

      We have updated Figure 1B and incorporated the alignment of PLCG2.

      (3) Figure 4A:

      Given that

      (i) sl is predicted to be the fly ortholog for both mammalian PLCγ isozymes: PLCG1 and PLCG2 [Line 62]

      (ii) they are shown to have non-redundant roles in mammals [Line 71]

      (iii) reconstituting PLCG1 is highly toxic in flies, leading to increased lethality.

      This raises questions about whether sl mutant phenotypes are specifically caused by the absence of PLCG1 or PLCG2 functions in flies. Can hPLCG2 reconstitution in sl mutants be used as a negative control to rule out the possibility of the same?

      The studies about the non-redundant roles of PLCG1 and PLCG2 mainly concern the immune system.

      We have assessed the phenotypes in the sl<sup>T2A</sup>/Y; UAS-hPLCG2 flies. Expression of human PLCG2 in flies is also toxic and leads to severely reduced eclosion rate.

      We have updated the manuscript with these results, and included the eclosion rate of sl<sup>T2A</sup>/Y; UAS-hPLCG2 flies in the new Figure 4B.

      (4) Do slT2A/Y; UAS-PLCG1Reference flies survive when grown at 22{degree sign}C? Since transgenic fly expressing PLCG1 cDNA when driven under ubiquitous gal4s, Tubulin and Da, can result in viable progeny at 22{degree sign}C, the survival of slT2A/Y; UAS-PLCG1Reference should be possible.

      The eclosion rate of sl<sup>T2A</sup>/Y >PLCG1<sup>Reference</sup> flies at 22°C is slightly higher than at 25°C, but remains severely reduced compared to the UAS-Empty control. We have presented these results in the updated Figure S3.

      and similarly

      Does slT2A flies exhibit the phenotypes of (i) reduced eclosion rate (ii) reduced wing size and ectopic wing veins and (iii) extra R7 photoreceptor in the fly eye at 22{degree sign}C?

      The mutant phenotypes are still observed at 22 °C.

      If so, will it be possible to get a complete rescue of the slT2A mutant phenotypes with the hPLCG1 cDNA at 22{degree sign}C? This dataset is essential to establish Drosophila as an ideal model to study the PLCG1 de novo variants.

      Thank you for the suggestion. It is difficult to directly assess the rescue ability of the PLCG1 cDNAs due to the toxicity. However, our ectopic expression assays show that the variants are more toxic than the reference with variable severities, suggesting that the variants are deleterious.

      The ectopic expression strategy has been used to evaluate the consequence of genetic variants and has significantly contributed to the interpretation of their pathogenicity in many cases (reviewed in Her et al., Genome, 2024, PMID: 38412472).

      (5) Localisation and western blot assays to check if the introduction of the de novo mutations can have an impact on the sub-cellular targeting of the protein or protein stability respectively.

      Thank you for the suggestion.

      We expressed PLCG1 cDNAs in the larval salivary glands and performed antibody staining (rabbit anti-Human PLCG1; 1:100, Cell Signaling Technology, #5690). The larval salivary gland are composed of large columnar epithelia cells that are ideal for analyzing subcellular localization of proteins. The PLCG1 proteins are cytoplasmic and localize near the cell surface, with some enrichment in the plasma membrane region. The variant proteins are detected, and did not show significant difference in expression level or subcellular distribution compared to the reference. We did not include this data.

      (6) Analysing the nature of the reported gain of function (experimental proof for the same is missing in the manuscript) variants:

      Instead of directly showing the effect of introducing the de novo variant transgenes in the Drosophila model especially when the full-length PLCG1 is not able to completely rescue the slT2A phenotype;

      (i) Show that the gain-of-function variants can have an impact on the protein function or signalling via one of the three signalling outputs in the mammalian cell culture system: (i) inositol-1,4,5-trisphosphate production, (ii) intracellular Ca2+ release or (iii) increased phosphorylation of extracellular signal-related kinase, p65, and p38.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion. We utilized the CaLexA (calcium-dependent nuclear import of LexA) system (Masuyama et al., J Neurogenet, 2012, PMID: 22236090) to assess the intracellular Ca<sup>2+</sup> change associated with the expression of PLCG1 cDNAs in fly wing discs. The results show that, compared to the reference, expression of the D1019G or D1165G variants leads to elevated intracellular Ca<sup>2+</sup> levels, similar to the hyperactive S1021F and D1165H variants. However, the H380R or L597F variants did not show a detectable phenotype in this assay. These results suggest that D1019G and D1165G are hyperactive variants, whereas H380R and L597F variant are not, or their effect is too mild to be detected in this assay. We have updated the related sections in the manuscript and Figures 5A and S5.

      OR

      (ii) Run a molecular simulation to demonstrate how the protein's auto-inhibited state can be disrupted and basal lipase activity increased by introducing D1019G and D1165G, which destabilise the association between the C2 and cSH2 domains. The H380R variant may also exhibit characteristics similar to the previously documented H335A mutation which leaves the protein catalytically inactive as the residue is important to coordinate the incoming water molecule required for PIP2 hydrolysis.

      We utilized the DDMut platform, which predicts changes in the Gibbs Free Energy (ΔΔG) upon single and multiple point mutations (Zhou et al., Nucleic Acid Res, 2023, PMID: 37283042), to gain insight into the molecular dynamics changes of variants. The results are now presented in Figure S7.

      Additionally, we performed Molecular dynamics (MD) simulations. The results show that, similar to the hyperactive D1165H variant, the D1019G and D11656G variants exhibit increased disorganization, with a higher root mean square deviations (RMSD) compared to the reference PLCG1.The data are also presented in the updated Figure S7.

      (7) Clarify the reason for carrying out the wing-specific and eye-specific experiments using nub-gal4 and eyless-gal4 at 29˚C despite the high gal4 toxicity at this temperature.

      We used high temperature and high expression level to see if the mild H380R and L597F variants could show phenotypes in this condition.

      The toxicity of the two strong variants (D1019G and D1165G) has been consistently confirmed in multiple assays at different temperatures.

      (8) For the sake of completeness the authors should also report other variants identified in the genomes of these patients that could also contribute to the clinical features.

      Thank you!

      The additional variants and their potential contributions to the clinical features are listed and discussed in Table 1 and its legend.

      Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The manuscript's significant contribution is tempered by a lack of comprehensive analysis using the generated genetic reagents in Drosophila. To enhance our understanding of the PLCG1 orthologs, I suggest the following:

      (1) A more detailed molecular analysis to distinguish the actions of sl variants from the wild-type could be very informative. For example, utilizing the HA-epitope tag within the current UAS-transgenes could reveal more about the cellular dynamics and abundance of these variants, potentially elucidating mechanisms beyond gain-of-function.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion. The UAS-sl cDNA constructs contain stop codon and do not express an HA-epitope tag. Alternatively, we utilized commercially available antibodies against human PLCG1 antibodies to assess the subcellular localization and protein stability by expressing the reference and variant PLCG1 cDNAs in Drosophila larval salivary glands. The reference proteins are cytoplasmic with some enrichment along the plasma membrane. However, we did not observe significant differences between the reference and variant proteins in this assay. We did not include this data.

      (2) I suggest further investigating the relative contributions of developmental processes and acute (Adult) effects on the sl-variant phenotypes observed. For example, employing systems that allow for precise temporal control of gene expression, such as the temperature-sensitive Gal80, could differentiate between these effects, shedding light on the mechanisms that affect longevity and locomotion. This knowledge would be vital for a deeper understanding of the corresponding human disorders and for developing therapeutic interventions.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion. We utilized Tub-GAL4, Tub-GAL80<sup>ts</sup> to drive the expression of sl wild-type or variant cDNAs, and performed temperature shifts after eclosion to induce expression of the cDNAs only in adult flies. The sl<sup>D1184G</sup> variant (corresponding to PLCG1<sup>D1165G</sup>) caused severely reduced lifespan and the flies mostly die within 10 days. The sl<sup>D1041G</sup> variant (corresponding to PLCG1<sup>D1019G</sup>) led to reduced longevity and locomotion. The sl<sup>H384R</sup> variant (corresponding to PLCG1<sup>H380R</sup>) showed only a mild effect on longevity and no significant effect on climbing ability. These results suggest that the two strong variants (sl<sup>D1041G<sup> and sl<sup>D1184G</sup>) contribute to both developmental and acute effects while the H384R variant mainly contributes to developmental stages.

      I also suggest a more refined analysis of overexpression toxicity. Rather than solely focusing on ubiquitous transgene expression, overexpressing transgene in endogenous pattern using sl-t2a-Gal4 may yield a more nuanced understanding of the pathogenic mechanisms of gain-of-function mutations, particularly in the pathogenesis associated with these variants exclusively located in the coding regions.

      We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion. We therefore performed the experiments using sl<sup>T2A</sup> to drive overexpression ofPLCG1cDNAs in heterozygous female progeny with one copy of wild-type sl+ (sl<sup>T2A</sup>/ yw > UAS-cDNAs). In this context, expression of PLCG1<sup>Reference<sup>, PLCG1<sup>H380R</sup>orPLCG1<sup>L597F</sup> is viable whereas expression of PLCG1<sup>D1019G</sup> or PLCG1<sup>D1165G</sup> is lethal, suggesting that the PLCG1<sup>D1019G</sup> and PLCG1<sup>D1165G</sup> variants exert a strong dominant toxic effect while the PLCG1<sup>H380R</sup>and PLCG1<sup>L597F<sup> are comparatively milder. Similar patterns have been consistently observed in other ectopic expression assays with varying degrees of severity. These results are updated in the manuscript and figures.

      Reviewer #2 (Recommendations For The Authors):

      The work in the paper could be usefully extended by determining the effects of expressing His380Phe and His380Ala in flies. These variants suppress PLCG1 activity, so their phenotype, if any, would be predicted not to be the same as His380Arg. Determining this would add further strength to the conclusions of the paper.

      We thank the reviewer for the constructive suggestions! We have tested the enzymatic-dead H380A variant, which still exhibits toxicity when expressed in sl<sup>T2A</sup>/Y hemizygous flies, but it is not toxic in heterozygous females suggesting that the reduced eclosion rate is likely not directly associated with enzymatic activity. We have updated the manuscript and figures accordingly.