16,205 Matching Annotations
  1. Jul 2018
    1. On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:

      A manuscript version is available at handle. The paper was commented on by Thomas Chalmers http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0895-4356(96)00190-4 to which a response was published http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0895-4356(96)00191-6, which is also available at handle


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 10, Matthew Katz commented:

      Organ preservation as a treatment strategy effectively demonstrated chemoradiation as the standard of care for anal canal cancer. RTOG proved the point in 83-14 [Sischy B, 1989]. The RTOG phase III trial above confirmed the importance of mitomycin-C as part of the treatment regimen, which remains the standard in 2014.

      With the rising incidence of HPV+ anal cancer, which may have a better prognosis, an important question will be whether we should lessen treatment intensity. Further trials will be needed to see if it's possible to lessen treatment toxicity without compromising cancer control.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Jun 13, Karl Broman commented:

      This is the first paper to consider interval mapping for binary traits in experimental crosses. Somewhat surprisingly, genetic mapping in humans focused on binary traits and not quantitative ones, while mapping in experimental crosses focused on quantitative traits and not binary ones. The method is developed in the context of marker covariates, and so it is a bit more technically challenging to understand than it might be, but this paper had a lot of influence on my understanding of QTL mapping.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 07, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Reader,

      The myocardial sinusoids are not a phantom regardless of whether knowledge about them is.

      Arteriosinusoidal vessels described by Wearn connect to myocardial sinusoids. The myocardial sinusoids are often pathologically altered and readily apparent on microscopy in pulmonary atresia with intact ventricular septum.

      Dr. Wearn's work: http://bit.ly/JTWearn The myocardial sinusoids are not phantom as they connect to the arteriosinusoidal vessels. The vessels of Wearn include the arteriosinusoidal and the arterioluminal vessels of the heart. Wearn's distinguished Harvey lecture of 1940 describes the difference between the "Thebesian veins" and the "AL & AS" vessels (vessels of Wearn). Related references: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1933738/ http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/22704295 http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23332812 https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/419324443068874752


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Aug 21, Robert P O'Shea commented:

      The PubMed title of this paper is incorrect. It should be "The extraordinarily rapid disappearance of entoptic images" with two ts in "entropic", as in the paper itself. Entopic is a medical term meaning in the usual place, as opposed to ectopic, which means in the wrong place (e.g., ectopic pregnancy). "Entoptic" means in the eye, requiring the Greek root "optic".


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Sep 11, Bill Cayley commented:

      More evidence that "less-medical" primary care can actually be MORE cost-effective (and perhaps better in other ways: https://lessismoreebm.wordpress.com/?s=primary


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 24, John Sotos commented:

      [Written in 1996] Speculations regarding Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (CJD) acquired from beef containing the infectious agent that causes bovine spongiform encephalopathy [1] have resulted in dramatic health policy measures and serious concerns in the lay public about dietary beef intake in Britain. We believe these concerns are out of proportion to the actual risk of CJD, especially when compared to other established risks of beef consumption, such as the development of coronary heart disease (CHD). We therefore sought to estimate a beef-eater’s risk of CJD and CHD based on currently available data.

      The ten cases of CJD became manifest over a 20 month span in 1994-1995 [1]. If we suppose a uniform incubation period of 10 years for CJD, then all 10 cases were infected during a 20 month period in 1984-1985, when the mean beef intake among the 56 million Britons was 181.5 g/person/week [2] (roughly equivalent to two hamburgers per week). Assuming the attack rate remains constant, an average Briton would have a 1 in 933,000 chance of developing CJD from 10 years of beef-eating. The risk per hamburger is roughly 1 in one billion.

      To determine the beef-attributable risk of CHD, we first calculate the change in serum cholesterol that results when the fat and cholesterol in lean beef [3] are consumed in place of isocaloric carbohydrate or protein. Using the average daily caloric intake in Britons (2040 kcal) [2], the Hegsted equation [4] predicts a 2.1 mg/dl increase in serum cholesterol when 181.5 g beef/week are consumed. For a 30 year-old non-smoking, non-diabetic man with a total cholesterol of 150 mg/dl, an HDL cholesterol of 45 mg/dl, and a systolic blood pressure of 120 mmHg, the Framingham equation [5] predicts that a 2.1 mg/dl increase in serum cholesterol sustained over 10 years raises the risk of CHD during this period by 1 chance in 5300. Thus the CHD/CJD relative risk is approximately 175!

      Although our calculations are approximations at best, they clearly show that the risk from fat and cholesterol in beef is, for most of the adult population, orders of magnitude greater than the risk from neurotropic particles contained therein. Other diseases linked to fat intake, such as stroke and colon cancer, have not been considered here. Based on these data we believe that beef-eaters should care for their coronaries, rather thsn perseverate on prions.

      [1] Will RG, Ironside JW, Zeidler, et al. A new variant of Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease in the UK. Lancet. 1996;347:921-925.

      [2] Ministry of Agriculture, Fisheries and Food. National Food Survey 1994. London: Her Majesty’s Stationery Office, 1995.

      [3] American Heart Association Cookbook. Ballantine Books. New York, NY, 1986.

      [4] Hegsted OM. Serum-cholesterol response to dietary cholesterol: a re-evaluation. Am J Clin Nutr. 1986;44:299-305.

      [5] Anderson KM, Wilson PWF, Odell PM, et al. An updated coronary risk profile: a statement for health professionals. Circulation. 1991; 83:356-362.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Jul 23, Jonathan Eisen commented:

      I am posting some materials associated with this paper on Figshare (alignments, trees, color figures, etc).

      See https://figshare.com/projects/1995_Eisen_RecA_Evolution_JME/15035

      Jonathan Eisen


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 02, Robert Eibl commented:

      I worked at that time in that lab and I completed the very first isogenic SV129 K.O. constructs for the planned partial K.O of the CD44 gene. Part of my work contributed to the paper above and was acknowledged within the article: "The sizes of the PCR products derived from the phage clones allows an estimate of the intron sizes. These results have been indepentently confirmed by mapping the restriction sites for more than twenty restriction enzymes on (lambda) phage inserts(R.Eibl, unpublished results)."


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2014 Nov 16, Robert Eibl commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 21, Tom Kindlon commented:

      References:

      1 Chalder T, Berelowitz G, Pawlikowska T, Watts L, Wessely S, Wright D, Wallace EP: Development of a Fatigue Scale. J Psychosom Res 1993, 37:147-153.

      2 Neu D, Hoffmann G, Moutrier R, Verbanck P, Linkowski P, Le Bon O. Are patients with chronic fatigue syndrome just 'tired' or also 'sleepy'? J Sleep Res. 2008 Dec;17(4):427-31. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00679.x.

      3 Goudsmit, EM., Stouten, B and Howes, S. Fatigue in myalgic encephalomyelitis. Bulletin of the IACFS/ME, 2008, 16, 3, 3-10. https://web.archive.org/web/20140719090603/http://www.iacfsme.org/BULLETINFALL2008/Fall08GoudsmitFatigueinMyalgicEnceph/tabid/292/Default.aspx

      4 Goldsmith LP, Dunn G, Bentall RP, Lewis SW, Wearden AJ. Correction: Therapist Effects and the Impact of Early Therapeutic Alliance on Symptomatic Outcome in Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. PLoS One. 2016 Jun 1;11(6):e0157199. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0157199. eCollection 2016. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157199.s001 (CSV form: https://www.mediafire.com/file/rvh3brmgoaznude/Goldsmith+2015+FINE+data+journal.csv )

      5 A dataset from the PACE trial. Released following a freedom of information request. https://sites.google.com/site/pacefoir/pace-ipd_foia-qmul-2014-f73.xlsx Readme file: https://sites.google.com/site/pacefoir/pace-ipd-readme.txt.

      6 Morriss RK, Wearden AJ, Mullis R. Exploring the validity of the Chalder Fatigue scale in chronic fatigue syndrome. J Psychosom Res. 1998 Nov;45(5):411-7.

      7 Loge JH, Ekeberg O, Kaasa S. Fatigue in the general Norwegian population: normative data and associations. J Psychosom Res 1998; 45: 53-65. CrossRef | PubMed

      8 Van Kessel K, Moss-Morris R, Willoughby, Chalder T, Johnson MH, Robinson E, A randomized controlled trial of cognitive behavior therapy for multiple sclerosis fatigue, Psychosom. Med. 2008; 70:205-213.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2018 Jan 21, Tom Kindlon commented:

      My feedback on content of Common Data Elements (Fatigue) - Myalgic Encephalomyelitis/Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (ME/CFS) CDE Public Review

      I submitted this to the NIH and thought I would also post it here.

      Suggested Change: Don't use the Chalder fatigue questionnaire

      Rationale: The Chalder Fatigue questionnaire has two separate scoring systems, bimodal (0-11) and Likert (0-33) [1]. Some of the issues raised below are more significant with one system rather than the other.

      (i) Doubts about the validity of two of the items in the questionnaire as means to measure fatigue:

      The item “Do you have problems starting things” seems as though it could relate more to motivation or some other issue rather than fatigue specifically.

      The item “Do you feel sleepy or drowsy” relates more to sleepiness than fatigue. Sleepiness and fatigue are not necessarily the same thing [2].

      Most studies that used the Chalder fatigue scale do not give details of scores on individual items but one study [3] reported the following in participants with ME: “Focusing on the individual items revealed that 86.8% of the questions making up the physical fatigue subscale received near maximal or maximum scores. The items which received the greatest number of low scores were question 3 (‘do you feel sleepy or drowsy’) and question 4 (‘do you have problems starting things’).”

      (ii) Ceiling effects are a significant issue when the Chalder fatigue questionnaire is used with patients with ME and CFS score, particularly with bimodal scoring:

      A study of those with ME [3] found that “Fifty per cent of the patients recorded the maximum score using the bimodal method and 77% recorded the two highest scores [i.e. either 10 or 11].” In the FINE and PACE trials, 76% (147/193) and 65% (417/640) respectively of CFS participants reported the highest score [11] at baseline using bimodal scoring [4,5].

      With regards to Likert scoring, a study of those with ME found that there was some evidence of a ceiling effect in those who were severely affected (more details were not reported but the average score for those severely affected was 30.55 (SD: 2.66)). In the FINE and PACE trials 29.1% (57/196) and 14.5% (93/640) of the participants with CFS respectively scored the maximum score of 33 at baseline.

      There is also a 14-item version of the instrument with three extra items. A study of 136 individuals with CFS looking at Likert scoring found there was near-maximal scoring on 6 of the 8 physical fatigue items [6].

      The authors of the ME study [3] noted with regards to bimodal scoring that there was a “marked overlap between those who rated themselves as moderately or severely ill. These findings are indications of a low ceiling.” This could lead to the questionnaire failing to detect patients moving from being severely to moderately affected and vice versa.

      Furthermore, if patients are already at a ceiling score at the start of the intervention, the questionnaire cannot detect their getting worse. This could mean that evidence of harm would not be recorded. Also, this phenomenon could affect measures of efficacy: if a certain percentage of patients improved and the same percentage worsened to a similar level, this could show up as an average improvement because the scores for those who got worse would not change if they were already at the ceiling level.

      This could also make interventions that caused a significant number of deteriorations seem better than those that caused fewer. For example, consider a scenario in which one intervention caused a certain percentage of patients to improve while the same percentage, who began at the maximum score, worsened by the same amount. If another intervention caused half the number of patients to both improve and worsen, the average numerical improvement for the first intervention would be twice that of the second, even though rationally the scores should be the same.

      (iii) Discussion of the ability of respondents to mark symptoms as occurring “less than usual”:

      The fact that participants can rate their fatigue symptoms as occurring “less than usual” can lead to some odd results with Likert scoring of the Chalder scale (it is not an issue with its bimodal scoring). People who have no fatigue problems should generally score 11/33, indicating that they had problems ‘no more than usual’. And, indeed, a study in Norway found that those in the category “No disease/current health problem” had a mean score of 11.2 [7].

      However, a study found that people with "multiple sclerosis fatigue" after an intervention reported an average fatigue score of 7.80 – that is, lower than 11; this score also showed lower fatigue than that of a healthy, nonfatigued comparison group in the study [8]. It is very unlikely to be true that patients with multiple sclerosis fatigue at baseline ended the study with lower fatigue than healthy people. Scores of less than 11 were also reported by those with CFS in the FINE and PACE trial [4,5].

      I will explore further now how pooling the scores of people who give scores of less than 11 with other scores can give odd results. Say 75% of participants gave a Likert score of 4 and 25% gave a score of 24. This would be an average score of 9 which is a better score than the score of 11 that healthy people report. However, it is likely that people who scored 4 on the scale were confused by the peculiar option on the Chalder questionnaire that allows them to rate themselves as having fewer problems with fatigue than when they were last well (choosing that option is the only way to get a score below 11). If they really meant to say that they had no more fatigue than when they were last well, then their score should really have been similar to that of the average healthy person, at 11.2. Substituting this score instead of 4 in this example would give an average score for the group of 14.4, a worse score than what healthy people score. The latter is, I believe, a better representation of what the average fatigue score for the group would be: that is, if a significant percentage still had significant fatigue, than the overall fatigue level should be worse on average than a healthy group, not better. This shows that the ability to have better scores than healthy people doesn’t just affect the validity of individual scores, it also affects the validity of overall mean scores.

      [The references for this comment are in my reply below]


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:

      A manuscript version of the paper is available at the Helsinki University institutional repository: http://hdl.handle.net/10250/8076


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Aug 20, Daniel Weiss commented:

      This is a paper which is often cited to tout the robust sensitivity of the Western blot for diagnosis of Lyme disease. However, a careful, critical reading of this paper reveals major flaws. The Western blot is, on the contrary quite insensitive for the diagnosis.

      I will limit my comments to study patients without recent erythema migrans. Most workers agree that an erythema migrans rash is diagnostic on its own, and that serologic tests are unnecessary as well as unreliable at this stage of the disease. Dressler performed both a prospective and retrospective study to evaluate two-tier testing. Study subjects were recruited from the authors’ Lyme disease clinic from 1990-1991. To participate they had to live in “an area endemic for Lyme disease”. The G39/40 strain of B. burgdorferi was used to generate antigens for the ELISA assay.

      Dressler’s prospective study included 54 highly-selected patients with Lyme disease. The prospective group included patients with Lyme arthritis and those with “inactive” Lyme disease. Many patients had a history of the EM rash. Neuroborreliosis was diagnosed in 39 patients; these patients had CSF pleocytosis, increased CSF protein or EMG evidence of an axonal polyneuropathy not caused by other known disease. Ten of the 39 with neuroborreliosis were declared to be “inactive” although this term was not clearly defined. Importantly, only 21 of the 39 were positive by ELISA—a sensitivity of 54%. Of those cases of “active Lyme disease” who had neuroborreliosis, the sensitivity was 58%.

      There were 57 patients with an indeterminate ELISA. One third of those who were thought by the investigators to have “active Lyme disease” had a negative Western blot. Thus, the two-tier test performed poorly even in patients whom expert physicians (Dr. Steere and colleagues) were sure had “active Lyme disease”.

      In the retrospective study of Dressler et. two-tiered testing was performed on a highly-selected patient cohort. ELISA was negative in two of 25 with Lyme encephalopathy or polyneuropathy; two more patients’ ELISAs were read as indeterminate. Western blot testing in four of the 25 showed a minimal or absent response. Thus, the sensitivity of the test in these patients chosen because the authors were convinced they had Lyme disease reached only 84%.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Reader,

      This excellent study of quail embryos by Poelmann et al. also identified the vessels of Wearn.

      The impression was that the vessels of Wearn are only connected to the right ventricle. However, Wearn identified the arteriosinusoidal and arterioluminal connecting to each of the four heart chambers. http://bit.ly/JTWearn

      The vessels of Wearn may be appear less significant in healthy individuals, but in disease states such has PAIVS, or AA/MS, they appear more significant.

      For additional commentary, please see.

      https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/404941362908241920/

      My interest is in accurate medical terminology, and I ask that others collaborate towards this end.

      Comments and suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you kindly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 May 21, Kay Dickersin commented:

      We need to make sure this article and others are archived properly so that they can be accessed by others. This journal was ahead of its time, and contains important articles.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 23, Pedro Mendes commented:

      Gepasi is now hosted at http://www.gepasi.org . However this software has been succeeded by COPASI Hoops S, 2006 which is at http://www.copasi.org


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 21, Gary Ward commented:

      This report describes a simple yet elegant series of experiments demonstrating that the T. gondii parasitophorous vacuole membrane (PVM) acts as a sieve, allowing small molecules (up to ~1300 Da) to pass from the cytosol into the vacuolar space. The researchers microinjected a panel of fluorescent probes either into the cytosol of infected cells or into the parasitophorous vacuole and observed bi-directional, passive transport across the PVM. These experiments provided an important new insight into the interface between the intracellular parasite and the host cell. The likely presence of nonselective membrane channels in the PVM is analogous to the outer membrane of Gram-negative bacteria, in which membrane porins allow passive transport of nutrients and metabolites into the bacterial periplasmic space.

      These results from these experiments are consistent with characterization of the vacuole membrane in host cells infected with the related apicomplexan parasites, Plasmodium falciparum [Desai et al, Nature 362 (1993); Desai and Rosenberg, PNAS 94 (1997)] and Eimeria nieschulzi [Werner-Meier and Entzeroth, Parasitol Res 83 (1997)]. The obligate intracellular microsporidian Encephalitozoon cuniculi also resides within an intracellular vacuole capable of passive transport of fluorescent peptides up to 1100 Da while 10 kDa fluorescently-labeled dextrans are excluded [Rönnebäumer et al, Eukaryot Cell 7 (2008)]. The presence of non-selective membrane channels and the non-fusogenic nature of the parasitic vacuoles established by these phylogenetically distant organisms point to a similar adaptation to an intracellular parasitic lifestyle.

      Posted by Gary Ward on behalf of the University of Vermont Toxoplasma Journal Club (UVM ToxoJC); members include Jenna Foderaro, Anne Kelsen, Shruthi Krishnamurthy, Jacqueline Leung, Pramod Rompikuntal, Luke Tilley & Gary Ward


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Mar 03, Md. Shahidul Islam commented:

      An extensively updated review on this topic is available: M. S. Islam. Calcium signaling in the islets. In: Islets of Langerhans, edited by M. S. Islam, Dordrecht, Heidelberg, New York, London:Springer, 2014, p. 605-632. ISBN 978-9400766853


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 May 26, Israel Hanukoglu commented:

      Originally we had named this protein as LL5.

      This protein has been named as Pleckstrin homology-like domain family B member 1 (Phldb1).

      The Uniprot link for this protein is http://www.uniprot.org/uniprot/Q63312


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Aug 17, Lydia Maniatis commented:

      Short version:

      The authors say: "We hypothesize that how our stimuli are perceived depends on spatial frequency. Our hypothesis is confirmed. (P.S. The effect is size invariant.)"

      Translation: "We hypothesize that how our stimuli are perceived depends on spatial frequency. How our stimuli are perceived DOES NOT depend on spatial frequency. Our spatial frequency hypothesis is falsified."

      a + (-a) = 0

      (The discrete addition of the phrase "at one size" into the abstract, as though this did not upend the entire position, is really quite extraordinary.)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2016 Apr 11, Lydia Maniatis commented:

      It is worth asking why, on the one hand, Solomon and Pelli (1994) seem to be asking questions about initial steps in vision - asking about "visual filters," and, on the other, referring, not to "perception" of letters, but to "identification" or "recognition." In order to identify or recognise, we have to first perceive, but this step is skipped over not only by these researchers but by those contemporary investigators who consider themselves "psychophysicists" in general.

      The reason is this: These investigators insist on treating perception as a problem of detection, such that they can recycle the signal detection models first developed by radar researchers.

      Thus, they assume that identification/recognition would be perfect if it were not for “internal noise” in the system, which produces uncertainty in the outcome. This noise is the random firing of neurons, base rates etc. This claim about internal noise and perception has never been tested, nor (to my knowledge) have these researchers proposed a way to test or measure the claim, but it is assumed.

      It is also assumed that we can add “external noise” to a stimulus and thus counterbalance the “internal noise.” This “external noise” can take the form of variance of the stimulus pool, splotches superimposed on the stimulus (as here), etc. Anything that can be described as “unpredictable.”

      If we do this, then there is an equation that we can use that allows us to measure “performance” regardless of the task, as long as we have a value to plug in for our “noise” term.

      If we can assume that the “external noise” is much bigger than the “internal noise” then we can use this equation whose output is labelled “E” for efficiency, by plugging in our “external noise” value.

      Clear so far?

      What is wrong with this story is also the reason researchers adopting it don't refer to “perception.”

      The stimulus, which consists of retinal photoreceptors excited by disconnected photons pouring in from the environment does not contain “letters” or any other objects to be “detected.” Only photons are detected by the system. It is the response of the nervous system that organizes these points, based on rules (instantiated in the system) that have been selected for over evolutionary time to produce a good enough match between the objects out there and the objects we perceive. We can understand the importance of sophisticated rules if we consider how easy it is for humans to read the letters of “captchas” used by websites to ensure you're not a robot, but which programmers have a hard time beating. (This is actually a crude and somewhat misleading example, because even seeing the entire captcha without being able to recognize the letters it “contains” is a feat of perceptual organization). So the visual system creates a percept by adding structure to structureless data, and the number of these potential structurings is infinite. The limiting factors are the rules.

      So to see a letter, even as a physical shape, implies an enormously complicated process of perceptual organization, based on inferences that constitute the output of the system. What we “identify” or “recognize” is the product of these processes. We are not “detecting” something that is there a priori, in the stimulus, the disconnected photons. Seeing splotches, or any other feature of the percept, is also the result of the same organizing processes. There's no principle justifying calling part of the percept “noise” and another part “signal” if what we are interested in are basic perceptual processes. It is relatedly a problem to overlook that some kinds of “noise” will, for organizational reasons, interfere more with the “signal” that we have in mind than other kinds, something that the makers of “captchas” know well.

      But all of these facts are silently overlooked by investigators who choose to treat perception as a detection problem involving imaginary spatial filters and arbitrarily defined “noise”. Which is truly extraordinary.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2016 Mar 12, Lydia Maniatis commented:

      Continuation of previous:

      Translation: Forget everything we've assumed so far about spatial filters. We don't have special detectors sensitive to the frequency of light bands falling across the retina! Visual angle doesn't matter. The visual system has special detectors for detecting the number of light/dark cycles per (familiar) "letter" - (about 3), actually a range of cycles, since some letters are quite wide (e.g. m's) and others quite narrow (e.g. l's). The processes implicit in organizing points of light into letter shapes so that we can recruit the relevant letter-sensitive channels are not relevant. Our data plus assumptions prove it.

      I think this front-page Nature article has zero credible content or face-validity.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2016 Mar 12, Lydia Maniatis commented:

      The claims the authors make in this publication lack both empirical and logical support. To make what is perhaps the most clear point, the very tenuous assumptions underlying the project are themselves contradicted by the authors own statements. This is represented as a refinement of the fundamental assumption, but in fact it pulls the rug out from under it. Each step of the way, definite assertions are made which citations provided do not support.

      The train of thought (with comments) is this:

      Authors: “We hear periodic sounds, or tones, by means of parallel auditory filters, each tuned to a band of temporal frequency (Fletcher, 1940)”

      From MIT online Encyclopedia of Cognitive Sciences: To account for the obtained data, Fletcher proposed a “critical band” that likened the ear to a bandpass filter (or, to encompass the entire frequency range, a set of contiguous, overlapping bandpass filters). The proposed “auditory filter” is a theoretical construct that reflects frequency selectivity present in the auditory system...”

      Translation: The auditory system exhibits frequency selectivity. We could find no more convincing citations with respect to auditory filters between 1940 and 1994.

      Authors: “...and we see periodic patterns, or gratings, by means of parallel visual filters (Campbell and Robson, 1968).”

      From Robson and Campbell, 1968: “Thus, it seems that we cannot satisfactorily model the over-all visual system by a simple peak detector following a spatial filter...As a modification of this theory, we may assume...Thus, we may suppose that the visual system behaves not as a single detector mechanism preceded by a single broadband spatial filter but as a number of independent detector mechanisms each preceded by a relatively narrowband filter 'tuned' to a different frequency...Such a model could account for our findings...”

      Translation: Robson and Cambell's (1968) results did not bear out their original predictions, and they speculated as to possible alternative accounts. Between 1968 and 1994, no firmer conclusions were apparently available.

      Note also that the analogy between frequency of pressure waves and the “frequency of periodic patterns (gratings)” is very loose. What is being claimed, specifically, is that there is a class of visual neurons that are excited by stimuli with a specific, sine-wave-type luminance structure across a region of space (more specifically, across a region of the retina), and that they are sensitive to the frequency of these spatial luminance alterations. But frequency of pressure waves is a fundamental feature of the energy that interacts with our sensory system to initiate perception. Grating patterns, on the other hand, are just one of an infinite number of possible patterns of light impinging on our retinas. If assumptions about vision are to rest on analogy, then frequency of light waves would seem more appropriate.

      Authors: “Do these visual filters participate in everyday tasks such as reading and object recognition?”

      Translation: We assume the existence of special, “low-level” mechanisms for detecting grating patterns of light on the retina. (Campbell and Robson, 1968 specified visual angle, i.e. extent on the retina, in defining their patterns). We wonder if these exist exclusively to detect grating patterns, or whether they are engaged in seeing other things as well. We're particularly interested in the perception of familiar objects that we can name upon seeing. The possibility that the visual system may possess “low-level” neurons interested only in ecologically irrelevant grating patterns but not in objects seems to us to be not only credible but testable and worth testing...

      Authors: “After all, grating visibility only requires the distinguishing of pattern from blank, whereas object recognition, for example letter identification, requires classification by the observer into one of many learned categories.”

      Translation: It's possible that letter identification doesn't implicate mechanisms used for seeing “pattern from blank,” or for seeing unfamiliar letters. To the extent that such mechanisms might, in fact, be implicated in letter recognition, we presume they consist of spatial filters.

      Authors: “Here, we make use of results from hearing research (Patterson, 1974).”

      Translation: An ad hoc model constructed on the basis of hearing experiments is an appropriate model for testing our conceptually confused variant of an assumption about the visual system based on another analogy to audition. (From Patterson, 1974: From Patterson (1974): “Threshold for a pulsed tone was measured as a function of its distance in frequency from the edge of a broad band of noise with very sharp skirts. Tone frequency was held constant at 0.5, 1.0, 2.0, 4.0, or 8.0 kHz while the position of the noise edge was varied about the frequency of the tone. The spectrum level of the noise was 40 dB. As expected, tone threshold decreased as the distance between the tone and the noise edge increased, and the rate of decrease was inversely related to tone frequency. The data were used in conjunction with a simple model of masking to derive an estimate of the shape of the auditory filter. A mathematical expression was found to describe the filter, and subsequently, this expression was used to predict the results reported by several other investigators.”)

      Authors: We find that letter-identification and grating detection filters are identical, showing that the recognition of these objects at one size is mediated and constrained by a single visual filter, or 'channel.'

      Translation: Our “single visual filter” conclusions contradict the speculations of Campbell and Robson (1968) (the authors we cited as support for the existence of filters to begin with) i.e. that “we may suppose that the visual system behaves not as a single detector mechanism preceded by a single broadband spatial filter but as a number of independent detector mechanisms each preceded by a relatively narrowband filter 'tuned' to a different frequency.” We don't know if our results on "letters" apply to other shapes. The varied shapes and proportions of "letters" have no bearing on the issue. For the visual system, "letters" is a special category of its own.

      Authors: Surprisingly, our noise-masking paradigm shows that the same channel performs both low-level detection of narrow-band gratings and high-level identification of broad-band letters.

      Translation: Uncorroborated assumptions inspired by analogies to electrical circuits and sound systems, which ignore the fact of neural inhibition and the principles of perceptual organisation, allowed us to show that hypothetical grating-detectors are responsible for the processes involved not only in perceiving, but also in recognizing letters. (Clearly a breakthrough for memory research as well as perception.) Also, we are arbitrarily splitting the part of the pattern we are showing observers into “noise” and “signal” terms, and assume that the visual system agrees with our analysis and segregates letters from splotches, without recourse to any organizing constraints (e.g. figure-ground rules). (Also, see again comment above re: single channel.)

      Authors: (Blurb to Figure 1): Note that changes in viewing distance, from 3 to 60cm, hardly affect the visibility of any given letter, indicating that the channel scales with letter size. (cont'd in next comment).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:

      A manuscript version of the paper is available at the Helsinki University institutional repository: http://hdl.handle.net/10250/8077


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 May 03, Hugues BEDOUELLE commented:

      For an unknown reason, this article is absent from the archives of "Protein Engineering Design and Selection". A pdf file of the article can be found at URL: http://hugues.bedouelle.free.fr


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 30, Harri Hemila commented:

      Believing that the 2% average effect of vitamin E on mortality is valid for all ATBC participants is an example of ecological fallacy.

      The ATBC Study, Alpha-Tocopherol, Beta Carotene Cancer Prevention Study Group., 1994 reported that the average mortality in participants administered vitamin E was not different from the non-vitamin E group with RR = 1.02 (95% CI 0.95, 1.09).

      However, a subsequent subgroup analysis found that the effect of vitamin E was not uniform over all the ATBC Study participants. The combination of age and dietary vitamin C modified the effect of vitamin E so that there was strong evidence of heterogeneity in the effect of vitamin E over 6 subgroups (P = 0.0005) Hemilä H, 2009. Furthermore, given that baseline age modified the effect of vitamin E, a further analysis by the participants’ follow-up age was done and a significant difference in the effect of vitamin E on mortality was observed before and after the age of 71 years (P = 0.03) Hemilä H, 2011.

      Ecological fallacy occurs where data about a group is used to conclude information about an individual. Thus, assuming that the +2% average effect of vitamin E on mortality in the ATBC Study is valid for all ATBC participants is an example of ecological fallacy. A substantial proportion of the ATBC Study participants are inconsistent with the average effect.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Jul 23, Jonathan Eisen commented:

      I am posting some notes and other materials relating to this paper on Figshare. See https://figshare.com/account/home#/projects/15032.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Oct 26, EDWARD BERRY commented:

      The potato hinge protein has a perfect CX9CX3C----CX3CX9C motif which by homology with vertebrate structures should form a helix hairpin with disulfides holding the two arms together. This is the motif recognized by the MIA40/CHCHD4 disulfide relay import system. And like other MIA40 substrates, the hinge protein ends up in the inter-membrane space (attached to cyt c1 of the bc1 complex). I wonder if anyone has checked whether Hinge is imported by the MIA40 system.

      In vertebrates one of the middle cysteines is lost, some residues are inserted, spoiling the perfect cannonical MIA40 import sequence. Only two disulfides are formed, and there is a short leader sequence although it is not a typical mitochondrial targeting sequence.

      In Saccharomyces, all but two of the cysteines are lost, and the hairpin is clamped by a single disulfide. Could this represent a switch from one import mechanism to another over the course of evolution?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Sep 20, Dr Yasmin Momin commented:

      mucoepidermoid carcinoma of ovary arising in dermoid is a rare case we reported


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Mar 23, Misha Koksharov commented:

      There is a later paper on this topic with important critical comments: Dworak M, 2011


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 May 30, Thomas Heston commented:

      The data presented in this study do not support ageism but rather illustrate a primary feature of statistics: as the sample size increases, you are more likely to find a statistically significant p-value. First of all, look at admission rates to the coronary care unit (CCU). If there was a gender bias, then a greater proportion of men (or women) without acute myocardial infarction would be admitted to the CCU compared to the other sex. This study shows clearly that sexism in CCU admission rates is not present. In 1990, 53% of men admitted to the CCU did not have an infarction compared to 52% of women, i.e. over half of the people admitted to the CCU did not have a myocardial infarction yet received CCU level care. This difference is not statistically significant. Then, in 1992 it was found that 48% of men admitted to the CCU did not have infarction compared to 54% of women. This difference was statistically significant, indicating that a greater proportion of women vs men admitted for suspected myocardial infarction received CCU level care. If anything, this represents sexism against men, not against women. But if we look at the data closer, we find that quite a few more people were admitted for suspected infarction in 1992 compared to 1990. This larger sample size means that we are more likely to find statistically significant differences. So while no statistically significant difference was found with the lower sample size (n=1473) in 1990, the higher sample size in 1992 (n=1967) resulted in a statistically significant difference. This is not surprising, this is just how statistical analysis works: large sample sizes are much more likely than small sample sizes to show statistically significant differences. To highlight this further, look at when the authors broke down their data by age (see Table 4). When broken down by age, no significant differences were found, but when all ages were combined, a statistically significant gender difference was found in 1990 and in 1992 in terms of thrombolytic therapy. This does not indicate that the differences are explained by age, but rather indicates that small sample sizes are less likely to show statistically significant differences compared to large sample sizes. The data does show a difference in rate of thrombolysis that is interesting, with an associated difference in mortality rates. My interpretation of this data is that a higher percentage of men vs women with confirmed infarction received thrombolytic therapy, and a lower percentage of men vs women with confirmed infarction died. Unlike the CCU admission rates which are not very illuminating and do clear show gender or age bias, the rate of thrombolysis suggests a bias towards more aggressive treatment of men with infarction (likely starting with differences in their emergency room care Heston TF, 1992). Furthermore, the data also suggest that thrombolysis works, as a lower in-hospital mortality rate was seen for men vs women. However, mortality was not broken down by whether or not the patient had received thrombolysis, so a meaningful analysis of mortality by gender and thrombolytic therapy is not possible.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Jun 13, Karl Broman commented:

      The idea is obvious in retrospect, but it wasn't at the time; this paper made a big difference.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Aug 31, Santosh Kondekar commented:

      I request the authors to comment on the link below: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/511337#cm511337_73741 Please share your experience if you had come across any accidental single heavy dose of amikacin. Thank you.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Jun 14, Michael Schatz commented:

      This is a seminal paper in genome assembly, precisely defining the unitigging algorithm that forms the basis of all overlap-layout-consensus assemblers since.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Jan 04, S A Ostroumov commented:

      New theory is formulated: the role of membrane potential as a multi-functional regulator of membrane proteins, in biomembranes of various types.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 25, Harri Hemila commented:

      A secondary analysis of this study has been published in Hemilä H, 2013, DOI. The secondary analysis calculated that the incidence of all asthma attacks was reduced by RR = 0.22 (95% CI 0.06 to 0.81), and the incidence of severe and moderate asthma attacks was reduced by RR = 0.11 (95% CI 0.02 to 0.48).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Thank you for the excellent article. Please provide your kind attention to the distinction between the Thebesian veins and the vessels of Wearn.

      http://bit.ly/Thebesius http://bit.ly/JTWearn

      The Thebesian veins may indeed be involved in drainage of the "dye/contrast" into the ventricles. The authors report what is presumably the vessels of Wearn “direct pathways of drainage into the right ventricle from the septal artery.” That depends on the definition of the term “direct.” If it means before the capillary beds, then it is the vessels of Wearn. If it means all connections excluding the coronary sinus, then it may include both the vessels of Wearn and the Thebesian veins.

      For additional commentary on the subject, please see the subsequent link. https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/419196829339639808

      http://bit.ly/vasaThebesii

      Comments or suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you very much.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:

      A manuscript version of the paper is available at the Helsinki University institutional repository: http://hdl.handle.net/10250/135149


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 25, Harri Hemila commented:

      A secondary analysis of this study has been published in Hemilä H, 2013. In the tetanus patients aged 1 to 12 years, vitamin C treatment was associated with a 100% reduction in case fatality rate (95% CI from -100% to -94%). In tetanus patients aged 13 to 30 years, vitamin C treatment was associated with a 45% reduction in case fatality rate (95% CI from -69% to -5%).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Feb 16, Lydia Maniatis commented:

      Another example of the problem discussed by Teller can be found in Pelli (1990), who is proposing a model of perception:

      "In order to make the model as general as possible, yet still be able to measure its parameters, we need threeassumptions,or constraints. First we assume that the observer's level of performance increases monotonically with the contrast of the signal (when everything else is fixed). This guarantees that there will be a unique threshold. Secondly, as indicated on the diagram by the prefix contrast-invariant we assume that the calculation performed is independent of the contrast of the effective stimulus, which is its immedi- ate input. Together, assumptions 1 and 2 are a linking hypothesis. They imply that the observer's squared contrast threshold (which we can measure) is pro- portional to the effective noise level (which is inaccessible). Thirdly, we assume that the equivalent input noise is independent of the amplitude of the input noise and signal, or at least that it too is contrast- invariant, independent of the contrast of the effective image. These assumptions allow us to use two threshold measurements at different external noise levels to estimate the equivalent noise level. In effect, the assumptions state that the proportionality con- stant and the equivalent noise Neq are indeed constant, independent of the contrast of the effective image. These three assumptions are just enough to allow us to make psychophysical measurements that uniquely determine the parameters of our black-box model. Our model makes several testable predictions, as will be discussed below."

      It's worth noting that the main function of the rationale seems to be one of practical convenience.

      Theoretically, Pelli (1990) is proposing to make the case that: "the idea of equivalent input noise and a simplify- ing assumption called 'contrast invariance' allow the observer's overall quantum efficiency (as defined by Barlow, 1962a) to be factored into two components: transduction efficiency (called 'quantum efficiency of the eye' by Rose, 1948) and calculation efficiency..."

      Although he claims his model makes testable predictions, he also states that they had not, as of publication been tested.

      Pelli and Farrell (1999) seem to be referencing the untested, two-component model when they state that: "it is not widely appreciated that visual sensitivity is a product of two factors. By measuring an additional threshold, on a background of visual noise, one can partition visual sensitivity into two compo- nents representing the observer鳠efficiency and equivalent noise. Although they require an extra threshold measurement, these factors turn out to be invariant with respect to many visual parameters and are thus more easily characterized and understood than their product, the traditional contrast threshold."

      No references are provided to suggest the proposal has been corroborated. This problem is not remedied by the subsequent statement that: "Previous authors have presented compelling theoreti- cal reasons for isolating these two quantities in order to understand particular aspects of visual function (refs. 3 㠱3)," since all of the references predate the Pelli (1990) claims. Conveniently, the authors "ignore theory, to focus on the empirical properties of the two factors, especially their remarkable invari- ances, which make them more useful than sensitivity"). Ignoring theory unfortunately seems to be a hallmark of modern vision science.

      In this way, Pelli papers over a theoretical vacuum via technical elaboration of an untested model.

      Both Pelli (1990) and Pelli and Farrell (1999) are referenced by more recent papers as a support for the use of the "Equivalent noise" model.

      Pelli (1990) is cited by Solomon, May & Tyler (2016), without a further rationale for adopting the model (I've commented on that article here: https://pubpeer.com/publications/62E7CB814BC0299FBD4726BE07EA69).

      Dakin, Bex, Cass and Watt (2009) cite Pelli and Farrell (1999), their rationale being that the model "has been widely used elsewhere."

      I feel inclined to describe that what is going on here (it is not uncommon) as a kind of "theory-laundering," where ideas are proposed uncritically, then uncritically repeated, then become popular, and their popularity acts as a substitute for the missing rationale. Is this science?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2016 Feb 15, Lydia Maniatis commented:

      In reading a number of papers in visual perception, I repeatedly noticed authors using the term "linking hypothesis." "Linking hypotheses" - or assertions about how some experimental effect was linked to some cause -would be "introduced" without any ado, i.e. casually, without a rationale. It seemed as though we were supposed to take these "hypotheses" as givens in the interpretation of data, which didn't seem right. I googled the term "linking hypothesis" looking for some kind of formal definition, but didn't find anything of that kind. I did, however, find this article, which confirmed all my worst suspicions. I've only looked over as yet, but here is the final, jaw-dropping paragraph:

      "Perhaps we should include more often in our publications a paragraph or two making our linking propositions explicit. It will be important to define the ranges of intended applicability of individual linking propositions, the kinds of support they receive from prior experiments, their consistency with other broadly accepted linking propositions, the constraints they place on the composite map, the ancil- lary assumptions involved and the overall fit of the linking propositions into the current theoretical net- work of visual science. Within a few years, enough such descriptions should become available that it would be possible to refer to and build upon earlier explications, rather than starting from scratch each time. Similarly, perhaps reviewers could be encouraged to use the explicitness and potential values of linking propositions as one of the criteria of excellence for papers in which arguments containing link- ing propositions are explicitly or implicitly made."

      Essentially, this paragraph reveals that, at least as of the time the article was written, vision "scientists" were basing research on arbitrary claims for which they did NOT define ranges of intended applicability; defend with respect to previous research findings; ensure that they were consistent with accepted ideas (or at least how they challenged these); explain their general implications and connection to the current theoretical framework. They were, rather, "starting from scratch" on the basis of arbitrary assertions. This is not a recognisable picture of the scientific method.

      It doesn't appear that Teller's hopes for correction of these problems were realised. As I read the literature, the culture of "linking hypotheses" and all that that entails has become normalised. In fact, it may have gotten worse, as it has combined with the fashion for model-fitting in vision science. Thus, data collected is arbitrarily assumed to be "linked" to underlying processes, and the data are fitted to an algorithm, which, due to the high degree of predictability of the general outcomes (e.g. that attention will limit performance) and large number of free parameters, will always be achieved "more or less," without, of course, having general applicability.

      I've criticised a number of articles from this unfortunate new (maybe not so new) tradition (a few examples below):

      https://pubpeer.com/publications/90941136CC181AFE4896477BF5BB44

      https://pubpeer.com/publications/62E7CB814BC0299FBD4726BE07EA69

      https://pubpeer.com/publications/F0FDE1805AE5BDE583E3883A8AF39C

      A particularly devilish aspect of this approach is that the absence of intellectual/theoretical rigor is masked not only by incoherence but also by mathematics, which may confuse or scare off potential critics. But the same thing applies to math as to computing: Garbage in, garbage out. The rationale is the thing.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Oct 20, David Mage commented:

      This is an excellent review. However, it seems to have overlooked the paper "Neonatal mortality, the male disadvantage" by Naeye RL, Burt LS, Wright DL, Blanc WA, Tatter D, in Pediatrics. 1971 Dec;48(6):902-6, PMID: 5129451, that concluded that the male excess in infant mortality "must" be X-linked. Naeye et al.'s conclusion that an X-linkage is involved has been supported by work following this 1983 paper as shown in the following:

      Is excess male infant mortality from sudden infant death syndrome and other respiratory diseases X-linked? Mage DT, Donner EM. Acta Paediatr. 2014 Feb;103(2):188-93. doi: 10.1111/apa.12482. Epub 2013 Dec 20. PMID: 24164639

      The X-linkage hypotheses for SIDS and the male excess in infant mortality. Mage DT, Donner M. Med Hypotheses. 2004;62(4):564-7. PMID: 15050108

      The fifty percent male excess of infant respiratory mortality. Mage DT, Donner EM. Acta Paediatr. 2004 Sep;93(9):1210-5. PMID: 15384886

      A genetic basis for the sudden infant death syndrome sex ratio. Mage DT, Donner M. Med Hypotheses. 1997 Feb;48(2):137-42.PMID: 9076695

      Female resistance to hypoxia: does it explain the sex difference in mortality rates? Mage DT, Donner M. J Womens Health (Larchmt). 2006 Jul-Aug;15(6):786-94. Review. PMID: 16910910


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jun 15, Salzman Lab Journal Club commented:

      This of course is a landmark paper, clearly and concisely demonstrating that RNA alone can catalyze reactions. A finding that is a bit obscure here is that nicked circle IVS has mobility similar to linear IVS minus 15 nt. A later paper by the Cech group showed that this is because the autocyclization reaction cleaves off the 5'-most 15 nt of the linear IVS; they posit that this may limit the reversibility of the splicing reaction. This paper introduced the term "ribozyme", which the authors used in distinction to "enzyme" since the RNA reaction was not multiple-turnover; however these days it has expanded to include any RNA catalysts.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Dec 05, S A Ostroumov commented:

      Full text online free: Reconstitution of biological molecular generators of electric current. J Biol Chem 251: 7059-7065. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/23071084


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Oct 29, Jamie Horder commented:

      This 1981 report of 5 cases was the first published description of the syndrome that was soon to be known as AIDS. It concluded:

      "The above observations suggest the possibility of a cellular-immune dysfunction related to a common exposure that predisposes individuals to opportunistic infections."


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jul 25, Richard Sauerheber commented:

      We now know that both calcium and magnesium ions at millimolar concentrations decrease the fluidity of biologic membranes. Inside cells, where calcium is at only micromolar concentrations and magnesium at millimolar concentrations, magnesium ion would be involved, particularly since the inner half bilayer phospholipids have negatively charged groups even at slightly acidic pH (6.9) inside cells. The outer half of the bilayer is also susceptible to effects of both calcium and magnesium at millimolar concentrations in extracellular fluid, where phosphate groups on phospholipids would be negatively charged at alkaline pH (7.3) of extracelluluar fluid. The detailed control of the lipid fluidity of membranes in vivo appears to be quite sophisticated.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jun 09, Israel Hanukoglu commented:

      This article is the second in the series of first keratin sequences. The first one, https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6186381 reported the first sequence of a type I keratin.

      This second article reported for the first time the sequence of a type II keratin and predicted the secondary structural domains of the protein by computerized analysis. These predictions were confirmed by an analysis of the crystal structure of a segment of this keratin in coiled-coil form (Lee et al., PMID:22705788).

      The coiled-coiled structure of keratins is presented in an article (http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bmb.20746 ) that also presents a model that can be viewed in Proteopedia.

      http://proteopedia.org/wiki/index.php/Keratin


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 11, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Reader,

      Please provide your kind consideration to the following reasons that some of the described "lymphatic structures" may be "myocardial sinusoids."

      Quoted from the article: “The subepicardial and subendocardial systems in dogs and pigs communicate via transmyocardial channels” … "in humans these communications have not been identified with certainty." The subendocardial network of lymphatics described in both animals and humans in this study have many similarities to the myocardial sinusoids described by Wearn.

      http://bit.ly/JTWearn

      Tracing to subepicardial lymphatics was possible in dogs/pigs, but not in humans. This may be explained by the hypothesis that some of the observed “lymphatics” in both animals and humans were “myocardial sinusoids.” Caution should be exercised before assuming that all the irregular subendocardial channels reported in this study were lymphatics. Presumably, many of them were myocardial sinusoids and not lymphatics.

      Whether these myocardial sinusoids communicate with subendocardial lymphatics may be a reasonable area for further study.

      My opinion is that accurate anatomic terminology is a basic principle underlying good medical science, and I ask others to consider whether the aforementioned definitions are appropriate. If this comment is not helpful, please let me know how it might be improved. Comments and suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you very much.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 07, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Reader,

      Please see an annotated image from the article. https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/419272033118085120

      Comments and suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you kindly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Thank you for the excellent article.

      The authors write “intramyocardial channels normally joining the coronary arteries to the ventricular chambers.” Wearn described these ventriculocoronary arterial connections in addition to atriocoronary arterial connections. The eponym and only pronoun applied to these connections is the vessels of Wearn.

      http://bit.ly/JTWearn

      Finegold et al. astutely acknowledged the vessels of Wearn (connections) as a possible etiology for their findings. For further information related to PAIVS and the connections, please see:

      http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/23332812 https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/419317173689909248

      Comments or suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you kindly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Aug 31, Santosh Kondekar commented:

      Though amikacin is known to be toxic at non therapeutic doses; I feel the toxicity with single high dose is questionable.

      A fourteen month old child of a hospital staff nurse had presented to us with loose motions. Injection amikacin was advised along with routine supportive therapy for hydration. The dose of amikacin was advised as 7.5mg/kg/dose. The child had oliguria since last 12 hours. The amikacin injection was supposed to be kept on hold in view of same. Due to some miscommunication and advise being followed by a junior staff nurse and being unsuperwised; the child received an accidental overdose of intravenous amikacin bolus as 750 mg instead of 75.0 mg. The mishap was immediately noted by the mother. Mother being a staff nurse alerted the consultant and nephrologist. Immediately, blood was collected for renal profile, blood gas and amikacin levels. There was no acidosis on ABG and BUN was 24g/dl and Cr was 0.9. The child showed no worsening of symptoms nor any new symptoms like irritability,cry or seizures. A collective decision of consultant and parents was made to wait and watch. parents were informed all toxicities of amikacin in details. Child passed urine in subsequent hour. A repeat renal profile was sent after 6 hours and 24 hours. BUN was 22 and Serum Creatinine was 0.8. An audiometry was done after three months. A decision to perform dialysis was averted. Serum amikacin levels couldnot be done.

      I do feel that; in normal renal profile, an accidental overdose upto ten times therapeutic dose in a child should not be a reason to panic and an apprehensive decision to perform hemodialysis can be avoided.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Dec 05, S A Ostroumov commented:

      Full text online free : Electrogenesis by bacteriorhodopsin incorporated in a planar phospholipid membrane. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/18316493


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 25, Harri Hemila commented:

      A secondary analysis of this study has been published in Hemilä H, 2008, which is available also as a manuscript version, Handle. There is statistically highly significant interaction between gender and vitamin C effect on common cold incidence (P = 0.0001), so that vitamin C reduced cold incidence in males with RR = 0.63 (95% CI 0.50 to 0.78), but not in females (95% CI 0.95 to 1.61).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Mar 19, Nigam Shah commented:

      The first paper to propose the idea of building a cohort of 'patients like mine'.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jun 02, thomas samaras commented:

      Additional information on height, CHD, and longevity is available from these recent publications.

      Samaras TT. Shorter height is related to lower cardiovascular disease risk—A narrative review. Indian Heart Journal 2013; 65: 66-71.

      Samaras, TT. Is short height really a risk factor for coronary heart disease and stroke mortality? A review. Med Sci Monit 2004; 10(4): RA63-76.

      Samaras TT. Evidence from eight different types of studies showing that smaller body size is related to greater longevity. Journal of Scientific Research & Reports. 2014: 3 (16): 2150-2160, 2014; article no. JSRR.2014.16.003.

      Samaras TT. Human Scaling and Body Mass Index. In: Samaras TT (ed): Human Body Size and the Laws of Scaling: Physiological Performance, Growth, Longevity and Ecological Ramifications. New York: Nova Science Pub; 2007: pp 17-32.

      He Q, Morris BJ, Grove JS, Petrovitch H, Ross W, Masaki KH, et al. Shorter men live longer: Association of height with longevity and FOXO3 genotype in American men of Japanese ancestry. Plos ONE 9(5): e94385. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094385.

      Salaris L, Poulain M, Samaras TT. Height and survival at older ages among men born in an inland village in Sardinia (Italy), 1866-2006. Biodemography and Social Biology, 58:1, 1-13.

      Bartke A. Healthy Aging: Is Smaller better? A mini-review. Gerontology 2012; 58:337-43.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 31, Denise N Slenter commented:

      The pathway outlined in Figure 1, is available as free machine readable data in WP4210 in WikiPathways: https://www.wikipathways.org/index.php/Pathway:WP4210 . This pathway can be used for data analysis with e.g. Pathvisio and Cytoscape!


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jun 25, Jacques Barbet commented:

      This is an incredibly useful paper to calculate multiple equilibria in solution based on the mass action law.

      The method uses a partition function that should be used to teach students.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jul 27, Richard Sauerheber commented:

      (from the author) Many have argued that insulin stimulates glucose transport in sensitive cells by recruiting already pre-synthesized glucose transport molecules that are inside the cell waiting to be inserted after insulin binds to cell surface receptors. This however would be an energy-requiring and relatively slow process, to insert water soluble transport protein sections through the hydrophobic lipid bilayer. The function of insulin is to conserve energy after eating a meal, storing glucose for future energy needs in the form of glycogen, as well as other foodstuffs, and to quickly react to meal eating, and then to quickly deactivate energy storage between meals. So energy use, such as that required to insert transporters into an already-formed membrane would be wasteful and a contradiction in function. Also, the insulin stimulation of glucose transport is very rapid, finished within a minute, and the reverse, the deactivation of the transport rate, occurs quickly after insulin washes away, as when insulin levels are miniscule between meals. Removal of transporters from a membrane, to be stored inside the cell for later use again when the next meal is consumed, is a cumbersome notion. The data here point to a much more sophisticated and efficient design, where allosteric conformational changes can cause rapid activation and deactivation cycles in transporter activity when insulin is present and then absent, as during and then between meals throughout the day in the in vivo setting. For all the published data suggesting to investigators this is not a proper view, understand that such presented evidence is not compelling or beyond reasonable doubt, including microscopic observations of what are thought to be widespread intracellular transporters waiting for repeated, rapid re-insertion into, and re-removal from, the plasma membrane. The view described here caused my NIH grant renewal in the 1980's to be rejected. My research position eventually was terminated and I went into teaching (which I fortunately dearly love). Meanwhile, I stand by these observations and continue to teach them.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Aug 09, Matthew Katz commented:

      This is a seminal article in cancer care. NSABP B-06 demonstrated the safety of organ-preserving surgery in breast cancer and established a role for radiation therapy in early breast cancer. At the time there were questions about whether radiation was even necessary, but the segmental mastectomy alone arm fared worse. There results have held up at 20 years, also published the NEJM. Fisher B, 2002

      We still have more to refine on patient selection, treatment approach and other factors. But NSABP B-06 remains an example of how clinical trials can give patients treatment options without compromising cure rates.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2015 Jan 21, David Keller commented:

      Vitamin C was administered orally in this study

      When vitamin C is ingested orally, the serum concentration is regulated to a maximum of about 300 microM/L. Intravenous administration of vitamin C bypasses enteric control mechanisms, allowing plasma concentrations of up to 20,000 microM/L to be achieved. Alternative practitioners claim special benefits at the supra-physiologic concentrations which can only be achieved with IV administration. This comment is not meant to support or dispute their claims, it is placed here for the reference of researchers who do not have access to the NEJM archives, since the route of administration is not mentioned in the PubMed abstract.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2016 Jan 04, S A Ostroumov commented:

      Keywords (additional): Halobacterium halobium, Rhodospirillum rubrum, membrane potential, bioenergetics, bacterial photosynthesis,


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2016 Jan 04, S A Ostroumov commented:

      ABSTRACT: A unique short review of innovative landmark articles that discovered and proved the membrane potential across the membranes of the bacteria that use light as a source of energy, namely, the bacteria with traditional type of photosynthesis with bacteriochlorophyll and the bacteria with a new type of energetics that is based on bacteriorhodopsin.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2015 Dec 10, S A Ostroumov commented:

      Abstract is available, as well as the list of cited publications: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/22754808


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Nov 24, John Sotos commented:

      In a recent letter to the Journal Guarino describes an apparently novel method of detecting ascites: the diaphragm of the stethoscope is placed just superior to the pubic crest of a patient who has been standing for three minutes or more, and the physician determines by auscultation the level where the timbre of the “finger-flicking” percussion note changes from dull to loud (1). A similar technique, alas, was described in the 1930s by two French physicians (2) (3). Without mentioning the three-minute caveat, they found that if the physician auscultated in the iliac fossa of a standing patient with ascites, and percussed the abdomen from superior to inferior, the percussion note had a brusque augmentation at the top of the zone liquidienne (2).

      More interestingly, however, the thrust of their articles is directed at a technique they considered far more sensitive and convenient in detecting ascites: listening with the stethoscope bell in one iliac fossa while percussing the contralateral fossa with a “finger pulled back like a hammer” (doigt recourbé en marteau) (3). The presence of ascites is indicated by a distinctive double note in response to percussion (le double bruit ascitique), whereas a single note is heard in normal patients. Abdominal distention due to large ovarian cysts (3) or “enormous urinary retention” (2), the authors say, yields a single sound, while, in cases of hydatidiform cyst, a second sound is present, but of a different character (l’echo hydatique) (2). The authors further claim that, with the patient standing, their method is capable of demonstrating ascites of volume small enough to have otherwise escaped clinical detection. We have ourselves heard the ascitic double note, which the French authors considered the sonic manifestation of a fluid wave (3).

      Two hundred years ago, Marie Antoinette’s milliner is reported to have said, “There is nothing new except what is forgotten” (4). The advent of computer-based bibliographic retrieval systems has dramatically improved mankind’s scientific “memory,” but has not perfected it. Thus, there still is (and, most likely, always will be) a vast corpus of pre-electronic knowledge, searchable only by means that are both painful and tedious. Unfortunately, when dealing with an art as ancient as the physical examination, the scrutiny of this literature demands added attention (5).

      John G. Sotos, MD

      Iredell W. Iglehart III, MD

      (1) Guarino JR. Auscultatory percussion to detect ascites. N Engl J Med. 1986 Dec 11;315(24):1555-6. Pubmed 3785320

      (2) Lian C, Odinet J. De l’existence d’un double bruit par la percussion abdominale dans l’ascite. Societé Medicale des Hospitaux de Paris. 1931; 55: 1402-1408.

      (3) Lian C, Odinet J. Le double bruit ascitique et le signe de la matité horizontale dans la station debout. Presse Medicale. 1934; 42: 1337-1338.

      (4) Bartlett J (comp.). Familiar Quotations. 13th ed. Boston: Little, Brown, 1955; 1002.

      (5) Sotos JG. Diagnosis of fractures of the hip and pelvis (continued). N Engl J Med. 1983 Apr 21;308(16):971. Pubmed 6835304


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Jun 19, Robert Tibshirani commented:

      This paper, along with

      Kung-Yee Liang and Scott Zeger (1986). "Longitudinal data analysis using generalized linear models". Biometrika 73 (1): 13–22.

      introduced the Generalized Estimating equation (GEE) approach for modelling repeated or grouped observations. Full probability models for such data are often hard to specify, so GEEs finesse the issue by including a within subject or group correlation matrix in the usual score equations for a generalized linear model. The GEE is an important advance that has had much impact on statistical practice.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Jan 27, Guillaume Filion commented:

      This study reports that the sex-determining factor is the transcription factor ZFY. The claims were later shown to be erroneous and the sex-determining factor was identified as the transcription factor SRY (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/1695712). Expressing SRY in the XX mouse "Randy" made it develop as a phenotypic male (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/2030730), confirming that SRY is indeed the sex-determining factor. A historical summary of the events was published 25 years after the birth of Randy (https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/27190031).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 23, Harri Hemila commented:

      A manuscript version of the paper is available at the Helsinki University institutional repository: http://hdl.handle.net/10250/135150


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Reader,

      These diffuse (minute, aka fine) connections may be consistent with the vessels of Wearn. Since we cannot definitively exclude an arteriosinusoidal vessel, using the term "vessels of Wearn" is appropriate.

      It is also possible that some of the connections enter the capillary bed before entering the heart chamber. If that is the case, then it is possible that some of the connections are Thebesian connections.

      For serial histologic sections, Wearn studied the arteriosinusoidal and arterioluminal vessels utilizing cold celloidin that was too thick to enter the capillaries. Wearn did note that when India ink was injected into the coronary arteries it would enter the heart chambers through numerous minute connections. Since, the ink would not permit serial histologic sections with 3D reconstruction as was used for the arteriosinusoidal and arterioluminal vessels, we may not have sufficient information to specify whether the connections are Arterio-capillary-cameral, or Arterio-capillary-venule-cameral (The venule-cameral connection would be a Thebesian vein).

      Thus, when the diffuse connections are identified, we do not have sufficient data to determine whether they are arteriosinusoidal only, or whether some are arteriosinusoidal and some enter the capillary bed before entering the heart chamber. In addition, if some of the connections were arterioluminal vessels, then we would have an even more difficult time naming the radiographic finding. Thus, the term vessel of Wearn has been proposed. For additional commentary, please see. https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/404825015771619328 and https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/413860163033247744

      Comments, disagreements, and suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you kindly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Dec 10, Kath Wright commented:

      Other search filters are available from the InterTASC Information Specialists' Sub-Group Search Filter Resource at https://sites.google.com/a/york.ac.uk/issg-search-filters-resource/home


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Jan 08, Brett Snodgrass commented:

      Dear Reader,

      Please consider that approximately 37 years after Wearn's distinguished Harvey lecture, the arterioluminal and arteriosinusoidal vessels (vessels of Wearn), are reported as veins.

      Morphological and Functional Alterations of the Coronary Circulation. Harvey Lecture, 1940 - http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC1933738/

      Annotations, commentary, and links to additional related references are found at the following Twitter link:

      https://twitter.com/BrettSnodgrass1/status/419530221927141376

      Comments and suggestions are welcome.

      Thank you kindly.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2013 Jun 18, Jill Barnholz-Sloan commented:

      One of the seminal papers in population stratification bias and admixture effects. This paper was one of the first to describe individual admixture testing and its potential effects and linkage disequilibrium.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2014 Nov 24, Guillaume Filion commented:

      For a retrospective of the ideas behind BALST together with some comments by David Lipman, you can check out the blog post "Once upon a BLAST" at the following link. http://blog.thegrandlocus.com/2014/06/once-upon-a-blast


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Feb 02, Jim Woodgett commented:

      Probably shouldn't comment on a co-authored paper, but I regrettably missed this Drosophila homologue of ZesteWhite3/Shaggy that is involved in fruitfly male fertility and has the (great) name mojoless: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/17179138


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 31, Varshil Mehta commented:

      If there are any new treatment options available, please mail me on varshil91@gmail.com, since we are actively trying to find new therapies as the older ones are getting exhaustive. Regards, Varshil Mehta


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 30, Jim Woodgett commented:

      Characterization of a novel GSK3-like kinase (GlkA) in slime mold that is related to GskA and (similarly to GskA) acts downstream of cAMP in regulating chemotaxis. Rescue data show increased expression of GskA in glkA nulls is unable to complement (unlike re-expression of GlkA) and vice versa. Paper includes comparisons with mammalian isoforms (GSK-3 alpha and beta) and finds GskA target, Daydreamer, is also targeted by GlkA. A huge amount of data in this study including iTRAQ comparative proteomics. Would be good to know if lithium inhibits glkA.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 26, Jason Climer commented:

      Zutshi et al present an interesting framework that explains the phenomenon of grid cell phase precession as a code for movement direction to reduce path integration error in the grid cell circuit. While the hypothesis that precession stabilizes the grid cell code is an intriguing one, there are several caveats to their interpretations from extant data that should be considered.

      Phase precession in the open field has been characterized by several groups (Climer et al, 2013; Jeewajee et al, 2013; Reifenstein et al, 2014). Notably, all three groups demonstrated that precession relative to the distance traveled occurs faster as an animal’s path crosses further away from the center of the field. This causes sequences to sweep past each other in a way that would make decoding the distance traveled more complex. It may be possible to encode additional trajectory information in this family of sequences, but how the brain would read that out physiologically remains an open question.

      There are other minor concerns that are relevant to the field. Attractor network dynamics have yet to generate phase precession during open field movement. The claim that the slower spatial sequences in the absence of temporal sequences would be “adequate if travelling in a particular direction for distances that correspond to approximately the minimum grid spacing” may be misleading, as the ability of the brain to read out such long timescale dynamics has not been demonstrated.

      Altogether, it is an interesting idea, and it will be interesting to see what testable predictions fall out of it.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 19, Christopher Southan commented:

      The PubChem mappings (which have associated bioassay data, vendors and some patent matches) have been compiled at https://cdsouthan.blogspot.se/2018/01/new-year-new-antimalarial-mappings.html


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 14, Erick H Turner commented:

      We appreciate the authors' covering this important topic. However, the search method might have missed some relevant publications. It did cover this 2012 paper of ours...

      • Turner EH, Knoepflmacher D, Shapley L. Publication Bias in Antipsychotic Trials: An Analysis of Efficacy Comparing the Published Literature to the US Food and Drug Administration Database. PLoS Med 2012;9:e1001189. Available at https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1001189

      ...but the following two papers of ours, which employed the same methodology, were not included:


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 11, Tony Buffington commented:

      Of all the comorbidities, was endometriosis the ONLY problem associated with an increased risk for IC?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 11, Jim Woodgett commented:

      This is a comprehensive and up to date review of small molecule inhibitor classes of GSK-3 and is to be commended for recognizing the two isoform distinctions in the introduction. Some sections still refer to GSK-3beta specific effects (e.g tau phosphorylation where GSK-3alpha is essentially equally happy to phosphorylate this microtubule -associated protein that is implicated in Alzheimer pathology). All of the inhibitors listed in the review block both isoforms.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 16, Alessandro Rasman commented:

      I think that these two recent papers could be useful to understand better the reasons about the failure of the Brave Dreams trial:

      Factors influencing the hemodynamic response to balloon angioplasty in the treatment of outflow anomalies of internal jugular veins http://www.jvsvenous.org/article/S2213-333X(17)30336-0/pdf

      Optimism, enthusiasm, responsibility http://www.jvsvenous.org/article/S2213-333X(17)30335-9/pdf


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 26, Benjamin W Chaffee commented:

      We appreciate the detailed reading of our publication.

      In response, we note that a risk factor need not account for the majority of cases to be an important public health concern. Of all drivers involved in fatal traffic accidents in the United States, 79% were not impaired by alcohol.[1] Nonetheless, drinking and driving demands public health attention because of its indisputable and preventable contribution to traffic fatalities.

      All risk factors for youth smoking that can be addressed through effective public policy merit attention, particularly if those factors are increasing in prevalence.

      [1] National Highway Traffic Safety Administration. Traffic Safety Facts 2012 Data. DOT HS 811 870. https://crashstats.nhtsa.dot.gov Impaired by alcohol defined as blood alcohol concentration 0.08 or above.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2018 Jan 18, Brad Rodu commented:

      Using youth data from Waves 1 and 2 of the Population Assessment of Tobacco and Health (PATH) survey, Watkins et al. reported that “[a]ny use of e-cigarettes, hookah, noncigarette combustible tobacco, or smokeless tobacco was independently associated with cigarette smoking 1 year later. Use of more than 1 product [polyuse] increased the odds of progressing to cigarette use.”

      Watkins et al. focused almost exclusively on odds ratios, which is problematic because the size of the referent group of never tobacco users at Wave 1 (n= 9,058) is 36 to 97 times the size of the other tobacco use groups. This obscures the relative contributions of the groups to the outcomes, but they can be estimated using baseline numbers and adjusted odds ratios (Model 4) from eTable 10:

      219 = 9058x + 255(2.12x) + 189(2.15x) + 114(3.08x) + 93(1.53x)

      Of the 219 subjects reporting cigarette use in the past 30 days at follow-up, the numbers (percent) from each mutually exclusive tobacco-use group at baseline are: 175 never users (79.9%), 11 e-cigarette-only users (5.0%), 8 hookah-only users (3.7%), 7 other combustibles-only users (3.2%), 3 smokeless-only users (1.4%), and 15 polyusers (6.8%). These numbers are derived from the adjusted odds ratios and may vary slightly from the actual survey numbers, but the relative contributions of the groups will not change.

      While teens who try one tobacco product are more likely to try another, the dominant gateway to cigarettes in the PATH youth survey was from no previous tobacco use.

      Disclosure: BR is supported by unrestricted grants from tobacco manufacturers to the University of Louisville and by the Kentucky Research Challenge Trust Fund.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 08, Massimiliano Tognolini commented:

      Compound 1 (2,5-dimethylpyrrolyl benzoic acid) has been used as an Eph/ephrin inhibitor to assess the role of Eph/ephrin system virus entry. Unfortunately, compound 1 is PAINS (Pan Assay INterference substance) giving positive results in a large number of biological assays (Baell JB, 2010). The compound is highly reactive and spontaneously forms a mixture of non-specifically reactive polymers (Zhu W, 2013). Discoverer of Compound 1 themselves retracted their observation by publishing: "...the newly synthesized compound 1 did not show detectable inhibition of ephrin-A5 binding to EphA4-Fc in ELISAs or EphA2 phosphorylation in cells stimulated with ephrin-A1 Fc. ...when left exposed to air at room temperature in dry form, compound 1 acquired a progressively darker brown color. Concomitantly, the compound became progressively more active in ELISAs measuring inhibition of ephrin-A5-EphA4 binding.” (Noberini R, 2011). Scientists were advised about the use of compound 1 by the writer and colleagues in 2014 (Tognolini M, ACS chem neuroscience and Lodola A, JVI). In conclusion compound 1 IS NOT a RELIABLE EPH/EPHRIN ANTAGONIST.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 03, Rajat Rohatgi commented:

      Hi Pete, You are absolutely correct-- Atthog is a tetraspan protein, but does not belong to the tetraspanin family. Atthog is related to the claudin-like group of tetraspan proteins, but the term "tetraspanin" is specific to a different family. Thank you for bringing this error in nomenclature to our attention so promptly. We apologize for any confusion this may have caused. A correction is forthcoming.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2018 Jan 02, Pete Monk commented:

      Atthog is not a tetraspanin protein. Tetraspanins (TSPAN) are a specific gene family (https://www.genenames.org/cgi-bin/genefamilies/set/768). Atthog is unrelated to the TSPAN family but has 4 putative transmembrane domains; such proteins are known generically as tetraspans.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 21, Peter Rogan commented:

      The results reported in Table S1 of the different bioinformatic methods were difficult for us to assess. For example, why were there no bioinformatic analyses for c.426+415_4780dup(insGATCGCAGTGA)? Our analysis includes this mutation. Model cutoffs for these bioinformatic methods are defined arbitrarily because they are based on underlying datasets with unpublished or unknown content; furthermore, the binding site models are not easily reproduced, in part because they are not actually based on binding site affinities (Rogan PK, 2013).

      The details of the methods and source data we use to derive our information weight matrices and the matrices themselves are available (Rogan PK, 2003). The information contents of splice recognition sites or exons are expressed in units of bits, which have been formally proven to be related to binding site affinity through the second law of thermodynamics (Schneider TD, 1997, Rogan PK, 1998). In fact, relative entropy used by maxEntscan, violates the triangle inequality which is a fundamental requirement of the second law (Schneider TD, 1999). These articles demonstrate the cutoff for true binding sites is very close to the theoretical minimum of zero bits (Delta G = 0). We have also demonstrated this thermodynamic threshold holds for other types of binding sites (Lu R, 2017).

      Our pipeline for NGS data analysis has been validated extensively (Shirley BC, 2013, Viner C, 2014, Dorman SN, 2014, Caminsky NG, 2016, Mucaki EJ, 2016, Yang XR, 2017, Dos Santos ES, 2017). The URL of the MutationForecaster pipeline is given in the document linked to our previous PubMed Commons post .


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2018 Jan 17, Kathleen B M Claes commented:

      Dear dr Rogan, thank you very much for your constructive comments. It is very interesting to learn that your exon definition-based mRNA splicing analyses are in agreement with our cDNA analyses for all variants we studied (an overview is provided in Suppl Table S1 of our paper - not S3). I read the detailed comments on the URL you referred to. How easy can this approach be implemented in an NGS data analysis pipeline? Can you define cut-offs in this program to indicate when cDNA analysis is warranted?

      I also would like to thank you for alerting us about the typing error for the Multi-exon duplication in BRCA2 - the correct nomenclature for this duplication is indeed c.426+415_4780dup{insGATCGCAGTGA}. We corrected this in the final proofs.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2018 Jan 12, Peter Rogan commented:

      Twenty one BRCA1 and BRCA2 mRNA splice site variants were analyzed by semi-quantitative RT-PCR, with commercial software that scores putative splice sites by ad hoc methods, and with bioinformatic models based on Adaboost and Random Forest, which are general machine learning approaches. The authors cited our review on interpretation of splicing mutations (Caminsky N, 2014), however the analytic approach described in that paper was not evaluated. As an update to our previous BRCA mutation study (Mucaki EJ, 2011), we carried out information theory-based splicing analysis of all potential splicing mutations listed in Supplemental Table S3. The splicing consequences of all variants were accurately predicted by information analysis. We also report results of exon definition-based mRNA splicing mutation analysis (Mucaki EJ, 2013), which infers relative abundance of wild type and mutated splice isoforms from total splicing information content of each prospective exon. Due to length limitations in PubMed Commons commenting system, detailed results for each variant are described in: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1146708

      Also, during our analysis, some inconsistencies in mutation designation or interpretation were noted in the paper: (1) The complex BRCA2 duplication described in this article (c.425+415_4780dup[insGATCGCAGTGA]) is sometimes referred to as "c.426-415_4780dup[insGATCGCAGTGA]" (e.g. the title of Figure 5, and Suppl. Table S3), which are not congruent mutations. The true mutation is likely the former, as the Figure 5 legend describes an mRNA splice form that includes 293nt of intron 4. If the duplication was c.426-415_4780dup[insGATCGCAGTGA], the intron inclusion would only be 205nt long. (2) We report an additional inconsistency in regards to Figure 5: The legend of Figure 5E describes a splice form where a truncated exon 11 junctions with the aforementioned 11nt insertion. However, the diagram and the electropherogram in Figure 5e shows exon 11 (ending at c.2398) sharing a junction with the beginning of exon 5. The latter is most likely the correct isoform, as an acceptor is not predicted at the junction between c.4780 and the 11nt insertion.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 29, John-Olov Jansson commented:

      Response to Klaus: We thank Dr Klaus for her useful comment regarding possible sex differences in the response to increased loading. We have consistently found the effect of increased loading to be similar in female and male mice on high fat diet. However, we have not evaluated possible sex differences in the response to increased loading in non-obese rodents given standard diet. One possible explanation for previous inconsistent results could be that the effect of loading in these studies was not evaluated in obese mice on high fat diet, an often used model for clinical obesity.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2018 Jan 23, Susanne Klaus commented:

      I would like to comment on two points. (i) I feel that the discussion would have profited from comparing this study to previous studies using the same experimental approach, i.e. weight implantation to investigate the role of weight per se in body weight regulation (1-3). (ii) Since our previous study using the same experimental approach showed considerable sex differences in the reaction of mice to an implanted weight load it is unfortunate that in all except one experiment the sex of the animals used was not indicated and the question of possible sex differences not even mentioned. This makes it very hard to relate the presented data to previous investigations regarding the existence of a ponderostat (here termed “gravitostat”). In our study published in 2004 (1) we implanted weight loads both into male and female FVB mice resulting in drastical sex difference in long term body weight regulation observed over a period of 14 weeks. While male mice showed a partial long term compensatory decrease in body weight due to decrease of fat mass in response to an increased weight load, female mice regained biological body mass within 3 to 4 weeks after the initial weight loss due to surgery. Furthermore, contrary to males, female mice did not reduce food intake and body fat in response to an implanted weight load in the long term. These gender-specific differences in body mass regulation point to different strategies in males and females to cope with a situation affecting body mass and energy demands suggesting a higher efficiency of energy conservation in female compared to male mice.

      1. Wiedmer P, Boschmann M, & Klaus S (2004) Gender dimorphism of body mass perception and regulation in mice. J Exp Biol 207(Pt 16):2859-2866.
      2. Adams CS, Korytko AI, & Blank JL (2001) A novel mechanism of body mass regulation. J Exp Biol 204(Pt 10):1729-1734.
      3. Wiedmer P, Boschmann M, & Klaus S (2002) Weight per se influences body mass regulation differently in male and female mice. J Gravit Physiol 9(1):P189-190.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 13, Raphael Stricker commented:

      An Outdated View of Lyme Neuroborreliosis

      Raphael B. Stricker, MD. Union Square Medical Associates, San Francisco, CA, USA. rstricker@usmamed.com

      The article by Halperin entitled "Diagnosis and Management of Lyme Neuroborreliosis" attempts to address "widespread misconceptions" about the clinical phenomenology, diagnostic approach and response to treatment of neuroborreliosis. Unfortunately the article promotes these misconceptions by ignoring progress in Lyme disease research over the past 20 years. In fact, with the exception of some cosmetic details such as mention of CxCL13, a nonspecific marker of neuroborreliosis, and C6 peptide testing, a relatively insensitive test for Lyme disease, this article could have been written in 1997. It is equally inaccurate and simplistic now, as described below.

      The article uses an interesting approach to promote "Lyme denialism", the view that Lyme disease is a trivial illness that is easily diagnosed and treated as opposed to the worldwide epidemic of an often debilitating disease that affects more than 300,000 new cases annually in the USA alone (1). The discussion points to extensive uncertainties about various aspects of the neurological disease, and then concludes that, despite these worrisome unanswered questions, diagnosis and treatment of neuroborreliosis is relatively simple. There is no discussion of Borrelia cystic forms, biofilms or persister cells that complicate diagnosis and treatment. Relapsing fever Borrelia (RFB) and RFB-like strains such as B. miyamotoi and B. mayonii are brushed aside because "very few human infections" with these organisms have been identified, mainly because routine clinical testing for these Borrelia species does not exist. There is no mention of SPECT brain scans that may be useful in diagnosis of brain inflammation in neuroborreliosis patients (2), and tickborne coinfections such as Babesia microti, Babesia duncani, Anaplasma, Ehrlichia, Bartonella and Rickettsia that may cause significant neurological complications are completely ignored. Thus the article inhabits a simplistic world that was already obsolete 20 years ago.

      A good example of the misinformation contained in this document is the statement that the erythema migrans (EM) "bullseye" rash is found in "90% of infected children" with Lyme disease. This exaggerated EM prevalence is based on a study that defines Lyme disease according to CDC surveillance criteria, which rely heavily on the EM rash for diagnosis. Thus due to this circular reasoning, patients with an EM rash were significantly over-represented in the study. A more realistic contemporary clinical study of neuroborreliosis in children put the rate of EM rash at 27% (3). This more realistic EM prevalence indicates that diagnosis may not be that simple in children with neuroborreliosis.

      The article restates the myth that two-tier Lyme testing has "high sensitivity and specificity" for diagnosis. This statement ignores evidence for the dismal sensitivity of CDC-sanctioned two-tier testing (4,5). For patients with neuroborreliosis, the allegedly high test sensitivity is based on circular reasoning: to be included in this category, patients were required to have positive Lyme testing, and then they had positive Lyme testing! This type of faulty reasoning is presented uncritically throughout the article.

      The article relies heavily on information from the review by Steere et al. (6), which has an extensive critique in PubMed Commons. Many of those criticisms apply to this article as well.

      References

      (1) Stricker & Johnson, PLoS Pathog 10(1): e1003796.

      (2) Donta et al, Clin Nucl Med. 2012;37:e219.

      (3) Bingham et al, Pediatrics. 1995;96:1053.

      (4) Stricker et al, Expert Rev Anti Infect Ther. 2005;3:155.

      (5) Cook & Puri, Int J Gen Med. 2017;10:113.

      (6) Steere et al, Nature Rev Dis Primers. 2016;2:16090.

      Disclosure: RBS is a member of the International Lyme and Associated Diseases Society (ILADS) and a director of LymeDisease.org. He has no financial or other conflicts to declare.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 06, Ivan Buljan commented:

      Dear Hilda,

      Thank you for your comments. You have made several excellent observations and we are glad to clarify those issues.

      As it was addressed in the limitations, high dropout rate was present in trials with consumers and physicians, while there was none in student trials. Unlike the student’s trial, where we tested the efficacy of the formats, our intention was to test their effectiveness (the real world application). We believe that our results are representative because in the real world there will be a significant amount of patients and physicians who would not want to read the CSR. It is true that in the student trial there was no difference in reading experience between PLS and infographics, but although there was significant difference between infographic and PLS in consumer and physician trials, it has to be admitted (as we did in the article) that the difference was very small (only couple of points on reading experience scale, which poses the question whether that is a “clinical significance”).

      There were differences between infographics and PLS concerning the number of numerical expressions, but it cannot be claimed that that was the only reason for the dropout from the trials. Participants may have refused to participate in the trials even before reading the text, or before giving the answers about the summary. Also, even with different number of numerical expressions, numeracy was the predictor of results for all formats, meaning that participant with higher numeracy levels were better in understanding the results in a summary format even when fewer numerical expressions were used.

      Although there were differences in the formats with regard to the presentation of quality of the evidence between infographic and PLS. We accounted for that when scoring the results. In the scoring of the answers, we scored the answer as correct if the answer described the studies with terms like “low quality of evidence”, “the quality of studies differed greatly”, “the strength of the evidence from the studies differed”, “the studies were too small”, whereas the incorrect answers would be such as “the quality of evidence was very good”, “there was no differences in quality of studies” or “all studies produced equal strength of the evidence”. The result was that there was no difference between formats in number of correct answers on that question in any of the three trials.

      As for the metaanalysis, we had all individual level data from the three trials, so it was not necessary to use metaanalysis to estimate the effect. We presented the pooled results from all three trials.

      The ways of presentation of evidence to public using infographic are still not well explored. We do not know whether the symbols used in this research were appropriate to consumers for the purposes they are intended to. To explore that, we would need to ask participant their opinion about the symbols used in the infographic, which would require a qualitative approach, which is underway.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2018 Jan 01, Hilda Bastian commented:

      It is great to see randomized trials to test the effects of an infographic. However, I have concerns with the interpretation of the results of this set of 3 trials. The abstract states that these were randomized trials of 171 students, 99 consumers, and 64 doctors. However, those are the numbers of people who completed the knowledge and reading experience questions, not the number randomized: 171 students, 212 consumers, and 108 doctors were randomized. The extremely high dropout rate (e.g. 53% for consumers) leaves only the trial in students as a reliable base for conclusions. And for them, there was no difference in knowledge or reported reading experience - they did not prefer the infographic.

      The authors point out that the high dropout rate may have affected the results for consumers and doctors, especially as they faced a numeracy test after being given the infographic or summary to read. That must have skewed the results. In particular, since the infographic (here) has such different content to the plain language summary (here), this seems inevitably related to the issue of numeracy: the plain language summary is almost number-free, while the infographic is number-heavy (an additional 16 numerical expressions).

      The knowledge test comprised 10 questions, one of which related to the quality of the evidence included in the systematic review. The infographic and plain language summary contained very different information on this. The article's appendix suggests that the correct answer expected was included in the infographic but not in the plain language summary. It would be helpful to know whether this affected the knowledge scores for readers of the plain language summary.

      Cohen's d effect sizes are not reported for the 3 trials separately, and given the heterogeneity in those results, it is not accurate to use the combined result to conclude that all 3 participant groups preferred the infographic and reading it. (In addition, the method for the meta-analysis of effect sizes of the 3 trials is not reported.)

      The specific summary and infographic, although high quality, also point to some of the underlying challenges in communicating with these media to consumers. For example, the infographic uses a coffin as pictograph for mortality, which I don't believe is appropriate in patient information. This highlights the risks inherent in using graphic elements where there aren't well-established conventions. Both the infographic and the plain language summary focus on information about the baby's wellbeing and the birth - but not the impact of the intervention on the pregnant woman, or their views of it. Whatever the format, issues remain with the process of determining the content of research summaries for consumers. (I have written more about the evidence on infographics and this study here.)

      Disclosure: The Cochrane (text) plain language summaries were an initiative of mine in the early days of the Cochrane Collaboration, when I was a consumer advocate. Although I wrote or edited most of those early Cochrane summaries, I had no involvement with the one studied here.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2017 Dec 28, Karen Woolley commented:

      In a consumer-focused world, focusing on consumer preferences is understandable, if not desirable. If an infographic approach to summarising health information appeals to consumers of health information without interfering with knowledge translation, should the Plain Language Expectations for Authors of Cochrane Summaries (PLEACS) be updated to consider the use of graphics? Currently, the PLEACS guidelines focus primarily on text - the use of data visualisation / graphics is not mentioned. The skills of designers and editors can complement the skills of medical writers to produce high-quality plain-language documents. If PubMed Commons made it easier to post graphics (vs hyperlinking to graphics like this https://twitter.com/KWProScribe/status/946247110600540160), then it could serve as a logical, free, readily accessible, international repository for visually engaging plain-language summaries...that could sit right under their matching scientific (and less consumer-friendly) publications. Disclosures: Financial: I am a paid employee of Envision Pharma Group, which provides medical communication services and technology solutions. I have shares in Johnson & Johnson and have been a government-appointed director on the board of 5 hospitals. Nonfinancial: I am an active member and past director of associations that advocate for ethical publication practices. I am a research partner with international patient leaders and advocacy organisations.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 23, Rajarshi Guha commented:

      We've also released an application to explore scaffold trends at https://tripod.nih.gov/ste

      In contrast to the paper, it doesn't generate fits to the raw trend data. However, it does allow you to visualize trends for any substructure, rather than the fixed set employed in the paper. (The bookmarks in the top right list a number of well known scaffolds)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 14, Randi Pechacek commented:

      David Coil referenced this paper in a blog on microBEnet praising the extent of the research conducted.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 13, Anthony Michael commented:

      This paper reports that the single-celled green alga Chlamydomonas reinhardtii accumulates the polyamine putrescine and to a lesser extent norspermidine, with low amounts of 1,3-diaminopropane, spermidine and spermine. It also reports that arginine decarboxylase (ADC) activity is detected at a level 20% that of ornithine decarboxylase (ODC), two enzymes involved in the alternative initial step of putrescine biosynthesis. The authors also tentatively suggest that norspermidine could be synthesized by the aspartate beta-semialdehyde pathway, a bacterial polyamine biosynthetic pathway.

      The detection of ADC activity in C. reinhardtii is intriguing because its genome does not encode a homologue of the plant ADC. Rather, C. reinhardtii encodes two paralogues of ODC (XP001697502 and XP00698872) that according to ChloroP analysis, each contain a chloroplast targeting sequence. The authors applied the irreversible, active site suicide inhibitor of ADC, difluoromethylarginine (DFMA) to growing cells and then tested the effect of this inhibitor on activities of ADC and ODC. Against expectation of the authors, DFMA inhibited ODC activity, and the authors reasoned that this was due to DFMA being converted by arginase to difluoromethylornithine (DFMO), a specific inhibitor of ODC. Indeed, the authors found that cell extract supernatant and the pelleted fraction contained arginase activity. Surprisingly, the authors did not question whether the same phenomenon was happening when they assayed ADC activity. ADC activity was determined by measuring the release of [14C] CO2 from [14C] arginine. However, if arginase was converting [14C] arginine to [14C] ornithine, then any ODC activity would release [14C] CO2 from [14C] ornithine that had been formed from [14C] arginine. This would give the impression that there is ADC activity when in fact the [14C] CO2 was being release by ODC activity, in essence, the assay was detecting a phantom ADC activity, particularly relevant in the absence of an ADC homologue in the C. reinhardtii genome. This is a well-known pitfall with detecting plant ADC activity, and the best way to unambiguously detect ADC activity is to measure the product agmatine (decarboxylated arginine) that is produced by ADC activity on arginine.

      The authors also reported that C. reinhardtii accumulated norspermidine and spermine, and proposed that norspermidine is produced by an equivalent of the bacterial aspartate beta-semialdehyde-dependent pathway. Both spermidine and spermine in flowering plants are synthesized by dedicated spermidine and spermine synthases. An isomer of spermine, thermospermine, is synthesized by a dedicated thermospermine synthase. In Arabidopsis, spermine synthase has very likely evolved from gene duplication of spermidine synthase, which happened at the origin of flowering plants, whereas thermospermine synthase was likely acquired endosymbiotically from the cyanobacterial progenitor of the chloroplast. C. reinhardtii does not encode more that one spermidine synthase homologue, i.e., there is no spermine synthase present. However, a homologue of the Arabidopsis Acl5 thermospermine synthase is present (XP_001696651). As spermine and thermospermine have the same molecular mass and are difficult to separate by HPLC, the absence of a spermine synthase homologue, and the presence of a thermospermine synthase homologue, strongly suggests that the authors detected thermospermine rather than spermine in C. reinhardtii. The presence of thermospermine, and the application of Occam’s razor, then eliminates the requirement for a specific norspermidine biosynthetic pathway that is based on the bacterial asparate beta-semialdehyde-dependent pathway. This is because a known member of the plant polyamine oxidase family produces norspermidine from the oxidation of thermospermine (Sagor et al., (2015) The polyamine oxidase from lycophyte Selaginella lepidophylla (SelPAO5), unlike angiosperms, back converts thermospermine to norspermidine. FEBS Letts, 589, 3071-3078).

      In summary, I respectfully suggest that the authors’ own data indicate that the ADC activity detected by the CO2 release assay may be an artifact of: arginase activity on the [14C] arginine substrate to produce [14C] ornithine, and subsequent ODC activity on the resultant [14C] ornithine. The spermine they detect is likely to be thermospermine, which would explain the presence of the thermospermine catabolite, norspermidine. The effect that the authors found, of the spermidine synthase inhibitor cyclohexylamine, in increasing C. reinhardtii putrescine levels and reducing both spermidine and norspermidine levels suggests that inhibition of spermidine synthesis diminishes thermospermine levels. Thermospermine is made by aminopropylation of spermidine, and therefore the amount of the thermospermine catabolite norspermidine is also reduced.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 12, Morten Oksvold commented:

      In this study the authors use artesunate at concentrations ranging from 50 to 400 microM. They argue that the cytotoxic effects of artesunate on normal cells represent a problem for use of the drug in cancer therapy.

      The problem is that the authors study biological effects of artesunate at concentrations which are not physiological relevant. We are using artesunate at concentrations raging from 1 to 5 microM in B-cell lymphoma, where most cells have an IC50 below 1 microM. Maxium serum levels in malaria patients treated with artesunate have been measured to 1-3 microM.

      Artesunate concentrations of 50 microM and above will never be relevant in clinical settings and the study by Li X et al. is therefore misleading and irrelevant.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 19, Andrea Messori commented:

      Eight gene therapies are already supported by a published clinical trial

      by Andrea Messori

      HTA Unit, ESTAR

      50135 Firenze, Italy

      The study by Rangarajan and co-workers shows that gene therapies can be successful for a disease condition where the deficient factor is coded for by a very large gene. As of December 31, 2017, a published clinical trial is already available for the following 8 gene therapies:

      -Voretigene neparvovec (LUXTURNA, Spark Therapeutics): trial by Russell et al 2017; approved by FDA [2].

      -Tisagenlecleucel (KYMRIAH, Novartis): trial by Oncologic Drugs Advisory Committee[3]; approved by FDA [4].

      -Axicabtagene ciloleucel (YESCARTA, Gilead): trial by Neelapu et al [5]; approved by FDA [6].

      -STRIMVELIS (GSK): trial by Cicalese et al 2016; approved by EMA [7].

      -Gene therapy with a high-specific-activity factor IX variant: trial by George et al. 2017 [9].

      -Valoctocogene roxaparvovec: trial by Rangarajan et al 2017 [10].

      -Single-dose gene-replacement therapy Spinal Muscular Atrophy: trial by Mendell et al 2017 [11].

      -Hematopoietic stem-cell gene therapy for cerebral adrenoleukodystrophy: trial by Eichler et al 2017 [12].

      References

      1) Russell S, Bennett and co-workers for Cerebral Adrenoleukodystrophy J, Wellman JA, Chung DC, Yu ZF, Tillman A, Wittes J, Pappas J, Elci O, McCague S, Cross D, Marshall KA, Walshire J, Kehoe TL, Reichert H, Davis M, Raffini L, Georg Spinal Muscular Atrophy e LA, Hudson FP, Dingfield L, Zhu X, Haller JA, Sohn EH, Mahajan VB, Pfeifer W, Weckmann M, Johnson C, Gewaily D, Drack A, Stone E, Wachtel K, Simonelli F, Leroy BP, Wright JF, High KA, Maguire AM. Efficacy and safety of voretigene neparvovec (AAV2-hRPE65v2) in patients with RPE65-mediated inherited retinal dystrophy: a randomised, controlled, open-label, phase 3 trial. Lancet. 2017 Aug 26;390(10097):849-860. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31868-8.

      2) FDA. FDA approves hereditary blindness gene therapy. Nat Biotechnol. 2018 Jan 10;36(1):6. doi: 10.1038/nbt0118-6a.

      3) Novartis. Oncologic Drugs Advisory Committee Briefing Document. Tisagenlecleucel (CTL019) for the Treatment of Pediatric and Young Adult Patients with Relapsed/Refractory B-Cell Acute Lymphoblastic Leukemia. https://www.fda.gov/downloads/advisorycommittees /committeesmeetingmaterials/drugs /oncologicdrugsadvisorycommittee/ucm566168.pdf , July 12, 2017. Accessed September 11, 2017.

      4) Bach PB, Giralt SA, Saltz LB. FDA Approval of Tisagenlecleucel: Promise and Complexities of a $475 000 Cancer Drug. JAMA. 2017 Nov 21;318(19):1861-1862. doi:10.1001/jama.2017.15218.

      5) Neelapu SS, Locke FL, Bartlett NL, Lekakis LJ, Miklos DB, Ja for a totalcobson CA, Braunschweig I, Oluwole OO, Siddiqi T, Lin Y, Timmerman JM, Stiff PJ, Friedberg JW, Flinn IW, Goy A, Hill BT, Smith MR, Deol A, Farooq U, McSweeney P, Munoz J, Avivi I, Castro JE, Westin JR, Chavez JC, Ghobadi A, Komanduri KV, Levy R, Jacobsen ED, Witzig TE, Reagan P, Bot A, Rossi J, Navale L, Jiang Y, Aycock J, Elias M, Chang D, Wiezorek J, Go WY. Axicabtagene Ciloleucel CAR T-Cell Therapy in Refractory Large B-Cell Lymphoma. N Engl J Med. 2017 Dec 28;377(26):2531-2544. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1707447.

      6) FDA. U.S. Food & Drug Administration. YESCARTA (axicabtagene ciloleucel), Axicabtagene Ciloleucel (YESCARTA, Gilead). https://www.fda.gov/BiologicsBloodVaccines/CellularGeneTherapyProducts/ApprovedProducts/ucm581222.htm

      7) Cicalese MP, Ferrua F, Castagnaro L, Pajno R, Barzaghi F, Giannelli S, Dionisio F, Brigida I, Bonopane M, Casiraghi M et al. Update on the safety and efficacy of retroviral gene therapy for immunodeficiency due to adenosine deaminase deficiency. Blood 2016;128: 45 – 54.

      8) Aiuti A, Roncarolo MG, Naldini L. Gene therapy for ADA-SCID, the first marketing approval of an ex vivo gene therapy in Europe: paving the road for the next generation of advanced therapy medicinal products. EMBO Mol Med. 2017 Jun;9(6):737-740. doi: 10.15252/emmm.201707573. PubMed PMID: 28396566; URL https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC5452047/pdf/EMMM-9-737.pdf

      9) George LA, Sullivan SK, Giermasz A, Rasko JEJ, Samelson-Jones BJ, Ducore J, Cuker A, Sullivan LM, Majumdar S, Teitel J, McGuinn CE, Ragni MV, Luk AY, Hui D, Wright JF, Chen Y, Liu Y, Wachtel K, Winters A, Tiefenbacher S, Arruda VR, van der Loo JCM, Zelenaia O, Takefman D, Carr ME, Couto LB, Anguela XM, High KA. Hemophilia B Gene Therapy with a High-Specific-Activity Factor IX Variant. N Engl J Med. 2017 Dec 7;377(23):2215-2227. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1708538.

      10) Rangarajan S, Walsh L, Lester W, Perry D, Madan B, Laffan M, Yu H, Vettermann C, Pierce GF, Wong WY, Pasi KJ. AAV5-Factor VIII Gene Transfer in Severe Hemophilia A. N Engl J Med. 2017 Dec 28;377(26):2519-2530. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa1708483.

      11) Mendell JR, Al-Zaidy S, Shell R, Arnold WD, Rodino-Klapac LR, Prior TW, Lowes L, Alfano L, Berry K, Church K, Kissel JT, Nagendran S, L'Italien J, Sproule DM, Wells C, Cardenas JA, Heitzer MD, Kaspar A, Corcoran S, Braun L, Likhite S, Miranda C, Meyer K, Foust KD, Burghes AHM, Kaspar BK. Single-Dose Gene-Replacement Therapy for Spinal Muscular Atrophy. N Engl J Med. 2017 Nov 2;377(18):1713-1722. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1706198.

      12) Eichler F, Duncan C, Musolino PL, Orchard PJ, De Oliveira S, Thrasher AJ, Armant M, Dansereau C, Lund TC, Miller WP, Raymond GV, Sankar R, Shah AJ, Sevin C, Gaspar HB, Gissen P, Amartino H, Bratkovic D, Smith NJC, Paker AM, Shamir E, O'Meara T, Davidson D, Aubourg P, Williams DA. Hematopoietic Stem-Cell Gene Therapy for Cerebral Adrenoleukodystrophy. N Engl J Med. 2017 Oct 26;377(17):1630-1638. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1700554.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 12, D Majerowicz commented:

      In my opinion, a significant control group is missing in this study. The control group should not be a chow diet fed, but a chow diet supplement with olive oil, soy oil or any other oil of this kind. This control would exclude the possibility that the obtained results were caused by a high-fat diet and not by the canola oil. In the current study design, it is impossible to say if the significant effect came from the overall increase in dietary fat or from the specific fatty acid composition of the canola oil.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 18, Stuart RAY commented:

      Replacing "to" with "toward" might have been clearer.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Dec 17, Stuart RAY commented:

      This is an interesting report, but the wording of claims in the title and abstract seems sensational and likely to mislead some readers; in particular, saying that they observed "evolution of SIVcpz to pandemic HIV-1" might lead some to believe that the result was a strain much more similar to HIV-1. In fact, as the authors note in Fig 1D, SIVcpz strain MB897 has less than 70% sequence similarity with the most similar HIV-1M strain compared, thus there are more than 3000 (30% of 10kb) genomic differences and it remains unclear what further changes would be needed to make the mutated SIVcpz capable of spreading among humans. Even when there is no intent to mislead, words must be carefully chosen to avoid confusion.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 19, Harald HHW Schmidt commented:

      Unfortunately, fundamental mistake in here. Not all NOX produce superoxide, but NOX4 produces hydrogen peroxide, which has different characteristics and function. One cannot put all ROS into one class. Very limited literature insight on NOX4 and angiogenesis.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 16, Wichor Bramer commented:

      Dear Helge,

      Thanks for your comment. Good to hear our research is helpful.

      In our analysis we did not really differentiate between which specific database delivered the unique references that were coming from Web of Science. All I can see is the databases we subscribe to are these:

      Science Citation Index Expanded (1975-present) Social Sciences Citation Index (1975-present) Arts & Humanities Citation Index (1975-present) Conference Proceedings Citation Index- Science (1990-present) Conference Proceedings Citation Index- Social Science & Humanities (1990-present) Emerging Sources Citation Index (2015-present)

      I have the idea it is mostly the first two databases that contributed the most to the references, but that is only a wild guess and not at all based on objective research.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Dec 15, Helge Knüttel commented:

      This is a very nice and helpful work based on a broad empirical basis. It will have an impact on my future searching for systematic reviews.

      The autors' main conclusion is that at least the databases/search engines Embase, MEDLINE (incl. ahead-of-print publications), Web of Science Core Collection and Google Scholar should be used for searching in a systematic review (plus some more depending on the topic). However, there is no data in the article what content contained in Web of Science (WoS) Core Collection contributed to the search results analyzed in the article. Therefore, it is not clear what content of Wos Core Collection the authors' recommendation relates to.

      Web of Science Core Collection is a brand name for, well, a collection of databases. Currently, there are at least six citation indexes (Science Citation Index Expanded, Social Sciences Citation Index, Arts & Humanities Citation Index, Emerging Sources Citation Index, Book Citation Index, Conference Proceedings Citation Index) and two chemical indexes (Current Chemical Reactions, Index Chemicus) sold under the general term Web of Science Core Collection. These may contribute to relevant search results in highly varying degrees depending on the research question. This is of relevance as the set of databases accessible to a subscribing institution is not fixed. Rather, it is "You get what you pay for." In addition and unlike other databases a running subscription to one of the citation indexes will not give access to content from all previous years. "Archive" content will need to be purchased separately. As a result, the term "Web of Science Core Collection" may stand for a very broad range of database content.

      It would be helpful if the authors could specify what WoS content their analysis and recommendation relates to.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 13, Xiangbing Meng commented:

      I highly recommend readers to further read the following two references. Xiangbing Meng

      Induction of mitotic cell death by overriding G2/M checkpoint in endometrial cancer cells with non-functional p53. Meng X, Laidler LL, Kosmacek EA, Yang S, Xiong Z, Zhu D, Wang X, Dai D, Zhang Y, Wang X, Brachova P, Albitar L, Liu D, Ianzini F, Mackey MA, Leslie KK. Gynecol Oncol. 2013 Mar;128(3):461-9. doi: 10.1016/j.ygyno.2012.11.004. Epub 2012 Nov 9. PMID:23146687 PMCID:PMC3612022

      Strategies for Molecularly Enhanced Chemotherapy to Achieve Synthetic Lethality in Endometrial Tumors with Mutant p53. Meng X, Dizon DS, Yang S, Wang X, Zhu D, Thiel KW, Leslie KK. Obstet Gynecol Int. 2013;2013:828165. doi: 10.1155/2013/828165. Epub 2013 Dec 7. PMID 24381593 PMCID: PMC3871910


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 04, Anthony Michael commented:

      This paper tackles an interesting problem: whether Salmonella needs to be able to N-acetylate the polyamines spermidine or spermine for successful intracellular replication in human cells. Acetylation of endogenous spermidine in Salmonella might be required to prevent excess/ deleterious accumulation of spermidine. Acetylation by Salmonella of host-derived spermine might detoxify a potentially physiologically disruptive metabolite.

        Until this paper, the tetraamine spermine has never been reproducibly detected in Enterobacteria, however, the authors find that spermine is present in Salmonella at 8.0 mM and is 40 times more abundant than spermidine in Salmonella cells. Such a high molar ratio of spermine to spermidine has never been found in any organism, not even in seminal fluid, where spermine phosphate is sufficiently concentrated to result in some crystallization. This is an extraordinary discovery if reproducible, although it is more likely that some technical problem lies at the heart of this unique finding.
      
         It should be noted that the authors grew the Salmonella in LB medium, a rich, putrescine- and spermidine-containing medium. To determine the native polyamines of a given bacterial species, it is necessary to grow the bacterium in polyamine-free, chemically-defined medium because most bacteria can take up polyamines from the environment. There is barely detectable spermine in LB medium, and so the presence of spermine at 8 mM is all the more remarkable (this is likely a higher concentration of spermine than found in the human host cells, which have a dedicated spermine synthase not found in Salmonella). I suggest the authors use LC-MS to detect the mass of tetrabenzoylated spermine in their benzoylated extracts of Salmonella. Finally, the authors have not demonstrated that the spermidine N-acetyltransferase (speG) gene deletion results in any change in N-acetylspermidine levels. As the Salmonella speG protein has not been previously biochemically characterized, it would be prudent to seek orthogonal proof that the changes ascribed to the speG gene deletion are due to its enzymatic activity as a spermidine N-acetyltransferase. If spermine is reproducibly found to be present in Salmonella at 8.0 mM, I am more than happy to admit that everything that I know is wrong.
      


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 07, Peter Rogan commented:

      We have analyzed this mutation with the Automated Splice Site and Exon Definition Analysis server (ASSEDA). The 1 nt deletion in the splice donor of exon 20 reduces the strength of this site from 11.5 -> 4.1 bits. (100/[2<sup>7.4</sup> bits] = 0.6% binding affinity)

      The information theory-based approach used in ASSEDA predicts isoform abundance and computes the fold changes in binding affinity from mutations (Mucaki EJ, 2013), which corresponds to the degree of exon skipping in this case. The reduction in splice site strength is much greater than the estimates given by the ad hoc methods used in the paper. LOH was not complete; some of the observed expression may have been derived from the contaminating normal allele. In fact, had the loss of function in splice site recognition only been 25-40% according to the paper, it could have been classified as a variant of unknown significance, or possibly as benign (as we suggested in Mucaki EJ, 2011).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Feb 01, Tim Mathes commented:

      We thank Maria-Inti Metzendorf for her comments. We wrote that 2 full-texts were unavailable. By unavailable we mean that the full-text was not available anywhere. For sure, we tried to obtain all articles also via a document delivery services or by contacting the study authors. We assumed that it is not necessary to mention all sources that were used for obtaining full-texts. We chose the term not available, instead of not obtainable because one can never be 100% certain that a full-text don’t exist anywhere. The two mentioned cases were abstracts from the Cochrane Colloquium that were very likely never published as full-texts at the time point of the search. The abstracts did not provide sufficient information to allow an adequate data extraction. The studies were therefore excluded. Both authors are experienced in developing search strategies. Moreover, we tested the search strategy by checking whether the relevant publications known to us were identified before conducting the search (see article Methods section). In the comment no studies are mentioned that were missed by our search strategy. Therefore, it is only an assumption that the search strategy is not sensitive. We suppose this idea has come up because we only used “systematic”sb. In our experience this subset strategy is very sensitive. Our experience has also shown that this subset strategy is even more sensitive than some of the validated search filters for systematic reviews (e.g. health-evidence.ca filter: https://bmcmedresmethodol.biomedcentral.com/articles/10.1186/1471-2288-12-51). It might be always argued that sensitivity could be further increased. However, scientific manpower is not inexhaustible. So, in our opinion also precision of the search strategy is an important feature. However, we welcome suggestions on how to improve our search strategy, although we are confident that our search strategy is sufficient and did retrieve all relevant studies. We also appreciate comments or suggestions for studies we might have missed.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2018 Jan 24, Maria-Inti Metzendorf commented:

      I would like to congratulate the authors on preparing a methodological review on the important topic of data extraction methods in systematic reviews (SR). They report that 11 potentially relevant articles were identfied in the screening process. After full-text evaluation 3 articles were excluded because they did not meet eligiblity criteria, and further 2 articles were excluded as the full-text was not available. Therefore, the authors could only base their analysis on 6 studies and conclude that the evidence base to inform the review´s question is sparse. Given this conclusion I wonder why the authors did not acquire the full-text of the above mentioned 2 articles (via document delivery services or by contacting the authors), as these could have further informed their review. In addition, the review would also have benefited from involving an information specialist who could have checked the search and improved its sensitivity.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 30, Israel Hanukoglu commented:

      Some images from this article has been included in the "Beauty of Proteins" Collection at https://plus.google.com/collection/Ar9AME with a brief summary of the findings.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 19, Stuart RAY commented:

      The methods for figure 1 in this manuscript are incorrect; while the text says that RDP4 was used to generate figure 1, it's evident from the figure itself (text at top) that SimPlot was used.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 24, Shashi Seshia commented:

      This is an Open Access article under the terms of the Creative-Commons Attribution-Non Commercial License. Readers should be able to download the article without paying any fee.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 17, Ivan Shatsky commented:

      The review contains an extensive bibliography and in general could be useful for many readers, especially those who are taking their first steps in studying the molecular mechanisms of translation initiation in eukaryotes. The main and serious drawback of the article is an incomplete and distorted presentation of the current status of cellular IRES-elements. I already wrote in Pub Med Commons critical notes on the article from the same lab (Xue S et al. 2015. Nature 517 (7532):33-38)devoted to the same problem but, unfortunately, I was not heard. This review,again,does not say anything about a severe controversy in this field, it does not even mention the fact that the vast majority, if not all,of the cellular IRESs proposed to date must be validated since they all have been indentified using the method of bicistronic DNA constructs, a method extremely prone to almost unavoidable artifacts.

       Most of the works on individual cellular IRES quoted in the review belong to the period from the late 1990s to the end of the first decade of 2000s. In recent years, however, several articles have been published that point to uncertainties of our knowledge on cellular IRES, describe various pitfalls in the approaches used to identify them and suggest methods to avoid artifacts and obtain reliable information. The main requirement in these approaches is to use various RNA constructs, rather than bicistronic DNA, to transfect cells and assess the level of their expression. These approaches and stringent criteria to analyze  experimental  data were described in several of our papers (see, for instance, Andreev et al. 2009. Nucleic Acids Res. 37: 6135–6147; Shatsky et al. 2010. Mol.Cells 30: 285–293; Andreev et al. 2012. FEBS Lett. 586: 4139–4143; Terenin et al. 2017. Cell Mol Life Sci. 74: 1431–1455) and employed in our lab to verify IRESs in c-Myc, Apaf-1 and LINE-1 mRNAs. The authors of this NRMCB article probably found these papers not worthy of attention since they are not quoted. The only exception is reference 101 which represents the article with a detailed analysis of the status of cellular IRES by 2013. The analysis was performed by Richard Jackson, a widely known expert in eukaryotic translation (Jackson 2013. Cold Spring Harb.Perspect. Biol. 5: a011569–a011569.) Remarkably,although this reference is present in the text, nothing is said about the actual content and conclusions of Jackson's work. 
      
       The authors note that “over 100 proposed IRES-containing mRNAs have been reported” but “after decades of work, few examples have been well characterized”. However, the authors do not express any surprise why the progress is that slow, why the methods which have been successfully used to validate viral IRESs are not employed or not applicable to characterize cellular IRESs. The authors also note that putative cellular IRESs have “few structural similarities to each other” and that “their mechanisms of action are largely unknown”. Again, the authors do not ask the question:  why for many years now we have not seen positive data on the validation of cellular IRES? Maybe most of these mysterious structures are simply not IRESs, and we must look for alternative mechanisms? Meanwhile, scientific journals, including top ranking ones, go on publishing papers with novel unvalidated cellular IRESs, thereby only thickening the fog in this area 
      
      In short, it seems to me that this section of the review is written in an overly optimistic manner. It is unlikely to be useful for professionals in the translation mechanisms and may be  misleading for new generation of researchers who do not have a sufficient background in the field.
      


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 23, Richard Holliday commented:

      The oral health risks of tobacco smoking are well known and it is critically important we continue to build the evidence base around novel nicotine products so we can accurately inform our patients of the relative risks. It is great to see more research published on this topic.

      After reading this paper, I was left confused with regards to tobacco smoking within the e-cigarette (EC) consumer group. The study population is described as being made up of ‘former cigarette smokers’ and ‘current EC users’.

      The ‘former smokers’ groups were defined as:

      • Smoked >100 cigarettes in their lifetime.
      • Been abstinent (self-reported) for at least 6 months and not more than 2 years.

      The ‘EC consumers’ group were defined as:

      • EC consumers were ‘smoking’ EC for at least 6 months.<br>

      My concern with the study design and interpretation is that there has been inadequate control of confounding factors, particularly tobacco smoking. ‘Dual use’ (i.e. using both combustible tobacco and EC) is very common, currently around 45% in the UK. The study inclusion criteria do not specify that those in the EC consumer group need to be abstinent from tobacco smoking nor do they include any method of biochemically validating this (e.g. expired air carbon monoxide or salivary/urine biomarkers).

      This confounding factor makes the interpretation of the results very challenging. The comparison is between ‘former smokers’ and ‘users of combustible tobacco and EC’. Unfortunately, you can’t differentiate out the effect of the EC with this design. The papers conclusions ‘Our results show that e-cigarettes are linked to three types of inflammatory lesions in the oral cavity’ hence appear invalid.

      I also felt it would have been useful to have a ‘tobacco smoking (without EC)’ comparator group and I was unsure why this was not included or presented. This would have allowed relative risk to be communicated to tobacco smokers.

      I encourage the authors to clarify these points. Many thanks.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Feb 05, Johanna McEntyre commented:

      We do have a mechanism to do this ("External links") - we just need the databases to participate - something we are keen to see and will be encouraging within the context of ELIXIR over the foreseeable future. e.g. Here are all the papers cited in IntAct: http://europepmc.org/search?query=(HAS_INTACT:y)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2018 Feb 03, Egon Willighagen commented:

      One thing that has been bugging me for some years now (see this write up) is that it is really hard to figure out which databases capture data in articles. An example is WikiPathways which has knowledge extracted from almost 20 thousand articles (unique PubMed IDs), but it is nearly impossible to figure out which databases have data related to some article.

      What would the database community need to do for Europe PMC to use links between articles and database entries?


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2018 Feb 02, Christopher Southan commented:

      OK, but AWK users assume sources are current by default. Any chance of cajoling EPO to re-instate the feed? (would arguably < ELIXIR value)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2018 Jan 30, Johanna McEntyre commented:

      There are over 4.2 million patents in Europe PMC, which we thought justified a passing mention in the article. No implication other than that was intended.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    5. On 2018 Jan 10, Christopher Southan commented:

      While the advances described for EPMC are impressive, it remains somewhat misleading to imply European Patent Office biotechnology abstracts as a current resource, since the feed ceased in 2012. Would be great if EPO could backfill these which would then uniquely facilitate // searching of patents and literature.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 05, Hiskias Keizer commented:

      Choosing physiological salt as a control for multiple intra-peritoneal CLA-triglyceride injections makes it impossible to decipher which of the effects as noted in this article were caused by CLA and which were caused by intra-peritoneal dosing triglycerides in general.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 29, Michael Hoffman commented:

      I read this paper with interest and welcome its innovative new experimental method.

      Readers may be interested in our 2016 preprint, "Modeling methyl-sensitive transcription factor motifs with an expanded epigenetic alphabet" (Viner et al. 2016, bioRxiv, https://doi.org/10.1101/043794). In our paper, we also extended the four-symbol DNA alphabet to include additional symbols for methyl-C and guanine paired to methyl-C, and used position weight matrices (PWMs) based on this expanded alphabet. Our paper also used sequence logos with the expanded alphabet. The use of an expanded epigenetic alphabet for an energy PWM (ePWM) in the 2017 Zuo et al. paper is an interesting development.

      In 2015, we added custom alphabet support to most tools in the MEME Suite to allow use of this expanded alphabet. We hope that those tools will be useful for many.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 24, Dorothy V M Bishop commented:

      It is great to hear that the authors are conducting a large scale follow-up study: I would urge them to do this as a Registered Report, or at least to pre-register their hypotheses. Yes, small samples are not inherently bad, but, as demonstrated by Ramus in his 2017 paper,it is a sad reality that most findings from small studies in this areas fail to replicate in larger samples - perhaps not surprisingly given heterogeneity of dyslexia. There is a danger that, aware of this history, we might throw out the baby with the bathwater and dismiss genuine, important findings. But it is hard to know which findings are solid because most researchers in this area currently fail to distinguish hypothesis-testing and exploratory studies. A proper, large-scale prospective study with hypotheses identified a priori would be extremely valuable.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Dec 21, Christa Müller-Axt commented:

      We here respond to the questions and concerns raised by Franck Ramus (23th of Nov 2017). We organize our response around four topics: (i) sample size and statistical power, (ii) interpretation of effect sizes, (iii) specific predictions and correlation analyses, and (iv) current status and future directions.

      (i) Sample size and statistical power: Franck Ramus argues that our study lacks statistical power due to the modest sample size (i.e., n=12 dyslexics and n=12 matched controls). First, as recommended by Button et al. (2013), we have explicitly acknowledged and discussed the modest sample size in our manuscript (see pp. 3694 & 3696). Second, given the well-known statistical problems associated with modest sample sizes, we took measures to reproduce our results in additional analyses, as laid out in the paper (i.e., surface-based analyses, see pp. 3694 & e3). While we are aware that these analyses do not constitute an independent replication, they further provide confidence in our main results, as they make it unlikely that the findings are merely due to methodological specificities. Third, the Button et al. (2013) paper elicited a more detailed discussion on sample size issues and neither Button et al. (2013) themselves nor any of the comments and papers in response to Button et al. (2013) suggested that small sample size studies should be dismissed altogether (see e.g., Bacchetti P., 2013; Quinlan P.T., 2013 for a more detailed discussion of these arguments).

      (ii) Interpretation of effect sizes: Franck Ramus questions the plausibility of our results, specifically because the observed effect size for the group difference in left V5/MT-LGN connectivity is larger (d=1.28) than the observed effect size for the associated behavioral difference in rapid automatized naming (RAN) for letters and numbers (composite score: d=1.16). We were surprised by this remark. A larger effect size for left V5/MT-LGN connectivity differences than for behavioral differences on RAN could be explained by a number of reasons, e.g.: 1) altered V5/MT-LGN connectivity might also affect other cognitive processes that are not captured by RAN measures, 2) RAN is supported by additional brain networks, other than V5/MT-LGN connectivity alone, that contribute to task performance, 3) recruitment of compensatory mechanisms that partially substitute altered V5/MT-LGN connectivity to enable RAN performance. In our view, all of these mechanisms (as well as their combinations) seem plausible given the complex and nonlinear nature of the human brain and given the multi-component nature of the RAN. Therefore, we do not see how the apparent difference in reported effect sizes between the behavioral and neural measures would undermine the plausibility of our results.

      (iii) Specific predictions and correlation analyses: Franck Ramus asks whether we had specific predictions about the correlations reported in our paper. We had indeed specific predictions about the RAN and reading comprehension scores. As described in the paper (pp. 3693 & 3695), these predictions were based on a previous finding from our research group that only these two scores correlated with sensory thalamus function in dyslexics in an fMRI study (Diaz et al., 2012). The correlation in that study was only found for RAN and reading comprehension in dyslexics, but not in controls (Diaz et al., 2012). Hence, in the present paper we tested the specific hypothesis that RAN and reading comprehension scores are related to V5/MT-LGN connectivity in dyslexics. Franck Ramus states that there are other core symptoms of dyslexia. He asks whether we tested correlations of V5/MT-LGN connectivity with more behavioral measures than reported, without appropriately correcting for multiple tests. We agree that dyslexia also encompasses other core symptoms (e.g., spelling and reading speed deficits). We did, however, not test for correlations with these or other behavioral measures, as we did not have specific predictions about them. It should go without saying that we do not do selective reporting of test results. Our predictions about the correlations with RAN and reading comprehension related to the dyslexia group, while we did not expect a correlation in the control group. We therefore consider it valid to apply a Bonferroni correction for the two tested correlations within each group separately. We did not compute correlations across the whole sample of dyslexics and controls together, as this would not have addressed our hypothesis (i.e., that RAN and reading comprehension scores are related to V5/MT-LGN connectivity in individuals with developmental dyslexia).

      (iv) Current status and future directions: We appreciate that Franck Ramus highlighted the issue of the replication crisis. Indeed, while we think it is important not to dismiss results merely based on sample size, we agree that novel research findings necessitate replication. For this reason, our research lab is currently conducting a large scale follow-up study for replication as well as testing of further hypotheses generated by the present findings. We hope that the present study will inspire further research on potential deficits at the level of the sensory thalami and cortico-thalamic connections in the dyslexia field – a field that is currently dominated by an almost exclusive cerebral-cortex-centric view.

      Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S. J., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365‑376. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475

      Bacchetti, P. (2013). Small sample size is not the real problem. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(8), 585-585. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475-c3

      Quinlan, P. T. (2013). Misuse of power: in defence of small-scale science. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(8), 585-585. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475-c1

      Díaz, B., Hintz, F., Kiebel, S. J., & von Kriegstein, K. (2012). Dysfunction of the auditory thalamus in developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(34), 13841-13846. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1119828109


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2017 Nov 24, Dorothy V M Bishop commented:

      I agree with Franck's comment. This seems yet another example of a high impact journal favouring newsworthiness of a result over methodological quality. See my earlier blogpost on this topic:

      https://figshare.com/articles/High-impact_journals_where_newsworthiness_trumps_methodology/5631748


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    4. On 2017 Dec 12, Jose M. Moran commented:

      Great analysis! completely agree!


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    5. On 2017 Nov 23, Franck Ramus commented:

      This study seems severely underpowered, which is surprising for such a frequent disorder as dyslexia, and given that we are in the midst of a replication crisis (e.g., Button et al. 2013; Ramus et al. 2017).

      With 12 participants per group, this study had 29% chance to observe a moderate group difference (d=0.6). Here, the significant result is due to a huge group difference (d=1.28) in the left V5/MT-LGN connectivity. Even larger than the corresponding behavioural difference in RAN letters (d=1.27) and numbers (d=0.95) (from Table S1). How plausible is it that there should be such a large brain difference between two groups of dyslexic and control individuals, even larger than the cognitive symptoms that this brain difference is presumed to underlie?

      Similarly, with 12 dyslexic individuals, only huge correlations greater than 0.576 could be significant. Luckily this study observed a correlation of 0.588 between left V5/MT-LGN connectivity and RAN (using a one-tailed test and correcting for two tests), but not with reading comprehension. But what about the other behavioural variables, spelling and reading speed? Are they not core symptoms of dyslexia, even more so than RAN? Do they not rely on visual abilities? Were the a priori predictions so specific to RAN and reading comprehension, that correlations with spelling and reading speed were not even tested? If those predictions had been preregistered, this might be credible. Alternatively, were those correlations tested, but not taken into account in the correction for multiple tests? (not even mentioning correlations within the control group, or across the two groups)

      Button, K. S., Ioannidis, J. P. A., Mokrysz, C., Nosek, B. A., Flint, J., Robinson, E. S. J., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(5), 365‑376. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3475 Ramus, F., Altarelli, I., Jednoróg, K., Zhao, J., & Scotto di Covella, L. (2017). Neuroanatomy of developmental dyslexia : pitfalls and promise. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.08.001


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 21, Franz Schelling commented:

      2-dimensional statistics can't come to terms with 4-dimensional hemodynamic problems such as those illustrated by the Unmistakable MS lesions which reflect retrograde venous impacts - as those shown in tell-tale MRI series

      Please share a non-factional look at Dominik Meier's unquestionably stellar MRI sequences of a patient with multiple sclerosis.

      Isn't https://medium.com/@franzschelling/what-deals-the-brain-such-blows-synchronous-at-least-double-hits-in-an-exemplary-case-of-292877844500 or http://bit.ly/2hu3g9N proving without any shadow of a doubt:

      Such cases of multiple sclerosis are biomechanically primed hemodynamic and obviously venous conditions. Which need to be established as such, to be addressed with the adequate technical equipment and topical expertise.

      F. Schelling, M.D.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Nov 20, Alessandro Rasman commented:

      I think that these two recent papers could be useful to understand better the reasons about the failure of this trial:

      Factors influencing the hemodynamic response to balloon angioplasty in the treatment of outflow anomalies of internal jugular veins http://www.jvsvenous.org/article/S2213-333X(17)30336-0/pdf

      Optimism, enthusiasm, responsibility http://www.jvsvenous.org/article/S2213-333X(17)30335-9/pdf


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 23, Alessandro Rasman commented:

      I think that these two recent papers could be useful to understand better the reasons about the failure of the trial Brave Dreams:

      Factors influencing the hemodynamic response to balloon angioplasty in the treatment of outflow anomalies of internal jugular veins http://www.jvsvenous.org/article/S2213-333X(17)30336-0/pdf

      Optimism, enthusiasm, responsibility http://www.jvsvenous.org/article/S2213-333X(17)30335-9/pdf


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 09, David Keller commented:

      "The most common cause of death was PD"?

      It is notoriously difficult to get physicians to complete death certificates properly, especially when it comes to concisely and accurately identifying the cause of death. Some clinicians list the cause of death as "cardiopulmonary arrest" in nearly all cases, because (they argue) the cessation of pulse and breathing is one of the final events in most deaths, and thus is the most proximate cause of death. This type of error in identifying the cause of death is akin to "missing the forest for the trees", and interferes with research that would benefit from a cause of death that really answered the question: "what disease entity was the main reason this patient died on this admission?"

      On the other hand, we have doctors who are too vague and imprecise in identifying the cause of death, as in this study. Assigning "Parkinson disease" (PD) as the cause of death tells us little in most cases. The fact that a patient had advanced PD when he died does not mean that he died from the direct effects of PD, which is unusual. It would be better to identify a more proximate cause of death, such as stroke associated with recent onset of atrial fibrillation (for example) and list "advanced Parkinson disease" as an "other illness present at death". This would give researchers who use the death certificate a more accurate designation of the proximal prime mover in the patient's death at this time, and also allow for investigations into associations between that cause of death, and the patient's underlying PD.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 20, Doug Altman commented:

      This paper reports a simple descriptive survey of reports of randomised trials published in dermatology journals. It is not a meta-epidemiological study, which is a meta-analytic summary of subgroup analyses across multiple meta-analyses – see for example Sterne et al (Stat Med 2002) [PMID: 12111917].


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 18, Christopher Southan commented:

      As a more convenient alternative to supplementary data or figshare, NAR were enlightened in accepting pointers to our own blog posts (http://www.guidetopharmacology.org/news.jsp). We will thus now be able to update these

      Note also this paper follows on from our 2016 effort https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/26464438 so please switch to this 2018 one for future citations


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 21, Samira Vesali commented:

      Natural conception and reproductive experiences in cancer survivors who stored reproductive material before their treatment In the October 2017 issue of human reproduction (vol: 17, Page:1-8), Hammarberg K, 2017, published an opinion piece arguing of 301 respondents who had tried to achieve pregnancy since completing cancer treatment almost all had succeeded, in most cases through natural conception,Hammarberg K, 2017. There are some worthwhile points were not considered in this study, even as limitations or reasons for caution, which are bias influencing their findings. First, relatively response rate. It is not possible to know whether people with better or worse survival were more or less likely to participate. Our thoughts on this issue is that cancer patients undergoing advanced stages likely did not tend to participate in the survey. As well, those with malignancy normally get more extensive chemotherapy, which can damage their reproductive system. Evidence has been shown that different chemotherapy regimens can affect the reproductive system differently, Long JP, 2016. However, cancer patients who were on malignant stages have likely lower life expectancy and less hope childbearing after their treatment. It is possible they are less inclined to look for and benefit from fertility preservation options to store their cryopreserved material. Second, we believe that it is important to view the findings within the context of the cancer type, stage and chemotherapy regimens of the participants, because all the mentioned factors can lead to use or not use the frozen reproductive material and are effective in the participants’ ability to get pregnant naturally (possibly mild chemotherapy which can preserve some of the ovarian reserve). Third, the cancer patients studied were recruited from 1995 to 2014, but as we know Human Fertility and Embryology Authority (HFEA) has given permission to the usage of frozen eggs in United Kingdom (UK) from 2000, Wise J, 2000, and American Society of Reproductive Medicine (ASRM) removed the label of “experimental” from oocyte freezing in 2013,Practice Committees of American Society for Reproductive Medicine., 2013 . So, giving single vision to all frozen materials of participants during this time period maybe is not correct. Earlier, the patients only could enjoy form the cryopreserved embryo and sperm, then ovarian tissue and more recently oocytes. Looking at the table II, the pregnancy achievement is presented as “as a result of ART with stored material” but is not presented separately. It is not clear that all usage of stored material are adequately cryopreserved gametes or embryos or not. Fourth, the essential problem we see is that age can be another confounding factor in this survey. Many of the people who have never tried to get pregnant may be in advanced age, but we do not know the age of those who did not respond or never tried to get pregnant. Reza Omani Samani1, Samira Vesali1* 1 Department of Epidemiology and Reproductive Health, Reproductive Epidemiology Research Center, Royan Institute for Reproductive Biomedicine, ACECR, Tehran, Iran Correspondence address: Department of Epidemiology and Reproductive Health, Royan Institute for Reproductive Biomedicine, Tehran, Iran. E-mail: samiravesali@yahoo.com.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 19, Gwinyai Masukume commented:

      The authors define this pregnant patient’s Haemoglobin (Hb) of 8.9 g/dL as mild anemia. However according to the World Health Organization (WHO) this is moderate anemia (Hb < 10 but >= 7 g/dL) (WHO, 2011).

      It is suggested that theirs is the first case report describing an HIV infected pregnant patient with uncorrected Tetralogy of Fallot. In 2011 Solanki and co-authors reported such a case Solanki SL, 2011.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 12, Matthew Bjerknes commented:

      Haber AL, 2017 claim as novel their observation that a type of tuft cell expresses "the pan-immune marker CD45, which was not previously associated with non-haematopoietic cells" (see abstract). It is important to correct the oversight that the fact that tuft cells express PTPRC, encoding the pan-immune marker CD45, was first reported by Bezençon C, 2008.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 20, Tanai Cardona commented:

      I had suggested a similar experiment in a blog post back in the day (June 19, 2013): http://tanaiscience.blogspot.co.uk/2013/06/ethanol-production-using-heterocyst.html

      Glad to see it worked and that my prediction for better ethanol production in the heterocysts wasn't too crazy after all. In any case, using cyanobacteria to produce biofuels might need rates many times higher for it to become of any use at all. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4364286/

      Well done! All the best, Tanai


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 18, Anthony Michael commented:

      This article addresses the degradation of the polyamine spermine (Spm) by a strain of Bacillus subtilis used in the fermentation of soybeans to produce the Japanese fermented foodstuff “natto”. The authors have paraphrased half a sentence from a review of mine that dramatically misrepresents the whole sentence and meaning. In their article, the authors state, “However, it is widely accepted that bacteria do not produce Spm (Michael, 2016) and direct degradation of Spm by B. subtilis (natto) is unproven.” In fact, my review stated, “It had been accepted dogma that bacteria do not produce spermine (77); however, that supposition was incorrect, and in fact Spm has been detected in diverse bacteria (25).” (Michael, A.J. (2016) Polyamines in Eukaryotes, Bacteria and Archaea. J. Biol. Chem. 291, 14896-903 PMID: 27268252). In an age of fake news, this could be construed as fake citation.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 31, John Sotos commented:

      "The Popeye sign," described in the Journal as a marked bulge in the upper arm after rupture of the biceps tendon (1), should more correctly have been called "a Popeye sign," given that the term "Popeye arm" has been applied to patients with fascio-scapulo-humeral muscular dystrophy who have wasting of the biceps combined with preserved deltoid and forearm musculature (2).

      The confusion is understandable to anyone familiar with Popeye-the-Sailorman's complex and dynamic upper limb anatomy. In his baseline state, Popeye exhibited the forearm muscular hypertrophy common among sailors of his era (the 1930s), for whom scraping paint was a frequent duty (3). To science's detriment, the instantaneous biceps hypertrophy that Popeye experienced after oral spinach ingestion remains physiologically unexplained.

      Thus, for maximal clarity, an eponymical purist would use the terms "pre-spinach Popeye sign" and "post-spinach Popeye sign," respectively, in cases of fascio-scapulo-humeral muscular dystrophy and biceps tendon rupture.

      (1) Yoshida N, Tsuchida Y. "Popeye" sign. Images in clinical medicine. N Engl J Med. 2017; 377: 1976.

      (2) Case records of the Massachusetts General Hospital, Case 40-1991. N Engl J Med. 1991; 325: 1026-1035.

      (3) Hornfischer JD. The Last Stand of the Tin Can Sailors. New York: Bantam Dell, 2005. Page 90.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 16, Erick H Turner commented:

      The authors make mention of two approaches for dealing with publication bias. However, both of these can be classified as statistical approaches for use when the only data to which one has access is the published literature. Another approach: simply procure the missing data. Instead of relying on manuscript cohorts, one can look further “upstream” to inception cohorts of clinical trials. Examples of sources include regulatory data (e.g. Drugs@FDA), grants databases, and ClinicalTrials.gov.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 17, Giorgio Cattoretti commented:

      In 2014 [PMID: 24794148] we demonstrated that a pH2 Glycine buffer and a boiling 6M Urea buffer fail to remove primary antibodies in an exquisitely affinity-dependent fashion. Secondary antibodies increase the primary antibody affinity, further preventing the antibody complex removal with those buffers. Antibodies were not "degraded " to the point of not being recognizable by a second step, as shown in our paper. Later this year [PMID: 28692376 ] we published two separate and efficient antibody removal technique which preserve tissue antigenicity entirely over ten cycles and more. The method allows whole slide immunostaining with routine reagents and open source analysis tools as per this manuscript, but with far more markers per slide (50 or more). This manuscript describes a partially efficient two-step method for “detachment” but little rationale for the choice of buffers and none of the multiple previous references dealing with antibody removal (see our 2014 manuscript for a list). The fact that the Authors reproduce our previous observations is comforting; unfortunately misses the scientific rationale we provided and the solution of the problem we published.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 17, Xinlai Cheng commented:

      Here is the gRNA sequence information for Stat3 Double Nickase Plasmid (h) : sc-400027-NIC:

      sc-400027-NIC Stat3 Double Nickase Plasmid (h): gcctagatcggctagaaaac sc-400027-NIC Stat3 Double Nickase Plasmid (h): gttgggcgggcctccaatgc

      we can also provide you our established cell line. Let me know if you want


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Nov 15, Nirajkumar Makadiya commented:

      Hi,

      What sgRNA sequences were used in the KO cell line generation?

      Thank you!


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 19, Kenneth Witwer commented:

      This study suggests that approximately 20 microRNAs (miRNAs) in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) may distinguish between different responses to exercise or between sedentary controls and individuals living with chronic fatigue syndrome (CFS, also known as myalgic encephalomyelitis, or ME) and Gulf War Illness (GWI). Funded by recent NIH efforts to support research into several related, poorly-understood and debilitating conditions, this report represents a large and commendable undertaking in terms of patient recruitment (182 individuals) and sample collection. Unfortunately, experimental and analytical issues limit interpretation of the results. I would like to highlight several of these here in the hopes of stimulating rigorous follow-up as appropriate.

      1) The choice of a threshold of 35 for a clearly noisy qPCR array. A Cq threshold (to distinguish noise from real signal) was assigned to Cq=35, higher than might have been supported by the negative controls. 82 "positive" negative control features were found in this dataset, with an average Cq of 35.6. 34 of these data points were below Cq 35, averaging 33.1, and several amplified before 25.9. This is a high false positive signal, raising concerns about specificity of the array and suggesting that a lower threshold might have been appropriate.

      2) The noise threshold was effectively ignored by adjusting all values above 35 (PCR noise) to 35. This replaced noise with "data" points and introduced a block of artificially invariable values. In a re-analysis after discarding Cq>35, at least half of the reported differences fall away or are smaller. Specifically, results for the following miRNAs appear to be affected by the Cq=35 adjustment: miR-99b-5p, miR-423-5p, miR-204-5p, miR-30d-5p; let-7i-5p, miR-200a-5p, and miR-93-3p (these three miRNAs also do not reach the 2/3 detection threshold in the exercise groups); miR-19b-3p, miR-505-3p, miR-532-5p, and miR-186-3p (for the latter, only one and two Cq<35 data points were gathered for the SC and CFS groups, respectively); and miR-22-3p and miR-9-3p (detected in only one group). miR-22-3p may remain significant, but the real difference between groups is about half the reported value.

      3) A miRNA was considered "expressed" in any one of the seven groups only if it was found in 2/3 of all samples. Unfortunately, this threshold was then ignored in analysis, when even miRNAs that were "expressed" in only one group were compared between groups. In some cases, miRNAs with only one or two amplifications in a group were reported (like miR-186-3p).

      4) No validation or measures of technical variability. First, there is no assessment of technical variability. The qPCR array gives one reading per miRNA per sample, with the exception of a negative control, read twice. Reading the same sample on three or four arrays would show how precise the readings are. Some of the miRNAs have up to 15-cycle detection ranges (even with Cq=35 as the cutoff) within groups. This represents a >32,000-fold range if it is an accurate measure of abundance. As a result, the purported differences between groups are often overshadowed by large standard deviations. It is unclear how much of this variability is technical, thus it is difficult to assign any differences to biology. Second, no findings are validated by individual assays. Instead of, or in addition to, technical array repeats, individual qPCR assays are routinely used in RNA biomarker studies to confirm profiling findings. These experiments are relatively cheap and permit multiple technical replicates for each sample. Third, no spike-ins are reported (RNA extraction efficiency) nor is there quality control of RNA prior to array measurements. While I can understand that the authors's enthusiasm might have spurred them to proceed without some standard procedures, it is perplexing that reviewers (and editors) would not ask for these crucial experiments.

      Despite these critiques, there are several findings that appear to be supported by a reanalysis (excluding Cq>35). These include the finding of 40-fold decreased abundance of miR-328 in CSF of exercised versus non-exercised individuals (although this is not restricted to any particular disease group), and, to a lesser extent (8-fold), miR-608. While validation is needed, and ideally a before-and-after study (in animals if this is too demanding in humans), at least these miRNAs may justify follow-up. Several other miRNAs are also significantly different between the two groups, but with large variability, diminishing their likely utility as biomarkers. However, with most of the purported CFS or GWI differences confounded by the thresholding strategies, and with no validation, additional evidence will be needed before other conclusions can be drawn. Notably, there was no discernible pattern of neuronal, oligodendrocyte (providing myelin sheath), or inflammation-associated miRNAs, which one might expect with damage or inflammation associated with CFS/ME or the acetylcholinesterase-linked mechanisms for GWI that is mentioned, so I am doubtful that this avenue of investigation will be fruitful.

      Patients of CFS/ME not only have to live with this condition, they have been through a lot in recent years with irreproducible science. I would thus encourage the authors both to moderate speculation about CFS or GWI-associated miRNAs for now and to proceed with and report the results of blinded qPCR quantification. I would recommend using the standard stem-loop RT/TaqMan assays for this. Indeed, I would be glad to examine samples along with you in a blinded fashion if it would be helpful.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 22, Sander Houten commented:

      This paper focuses on the role of KLF14 in the regulation of hepatic gluconeogenesis in mice. The authors show that Klf14 mRNA and KLF14 protein expression in mouse liver is induced after overnight fasting. The authors furthermore show that Klf14 mRNA and KLF14 protein expression increased in liver of C57BL/6 mice on high fat diet, db/db mice and ob/ob mice when compared to control animals. The authors continue to demonstrate that the promoter of peroxisome proliferator-activated receptor-gamma coactivator 1alpha (Pgc-1alpha), a transcriptional regulator of gluconeogenesis, contains one KLF14-binding site. In subsequent experiments the authors use adenovirus-mediated KLF14 overexpression and knockdown in isolated mouse hepatocytes and in vivo in mice, which further indicated that KLF14 modulates hepatic gluconeogenesis (Wang L, 2017).

      I would like to point out that there is little evidence to support the hypothesis that KLF14 plays an important role in adult mouse liver biology. We found no evidence for expression of KLF14 in adult mouse liver as we were unable to amplify Klf14 cDNA, did not find Klf14 mapped reads in liver RNA sequencing data and found no specific signal upon immunoblotting (Argmann CA, 2017). Our data on the absence of Klf14 expression in liver are consistent with previously published work by others (Parker-Katiraee L, 2007) and publicly available data sources. We also investigated the physiological functions of KLF14 by studying a whole body KO mouse model and focused on the metabolic role of this transcription factor in mice on chow and high fat diet. Our results indicate that KLF14 does not play a role in the development of diet-induced insulin resistance in male C57BL/6 mice (Argmann CA, 2017).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 15, Franz Schelling commented:

      Congratulations on this pioneering study! I would highly appreciate being granted a reprint (F. Schelling, Fingstr. 32, A-6974 Gaissau, Austria) or electronic copy of the entire paper (dr.franz.schelling@aon.at)


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 12, Jose M. Moran commented:

      None


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Dec 12, Jose M. Moran commented:

      Dear Alvaro, I think that your comments deserve a Letter to the Editor.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    3. On 2017 Nov 09, Alvaro Alonso commented:

      This paper reports that warfarin users had lower overall incidence rates of cancer than non-users in a large population-based cohort. The association was present for most cancers.

      One major limitations in this analysis, that puts into question the validity of the results, is the lack of adjustment for potential confounders. The reported models seem to only adjust for age and sex. The authors did not consider any other factor that may influence the decision to prescribe or not warfarin. It has been established that frail and sicker individuals are less likely to receive oral anticoagulation with warfarin, even if indicated, and to discontinue treatment (see for example PMID 19151165, 26277091 and 27471198).

      In summary, before concluding that a particular medication has a blanket anticancer effect, I would expect a more careful consideration of confounding.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 22, NephJC - Nephrology Journal Club commented:

      The trial of Tolvaptan in Later-Stage Autosomal Dominant Polycystic Kidney Disease was discussed on November 14th and 15th 2017 on #NephJC, the open online nephrology journal club. Introductory comments written by Ian Logan are available at the NephJC website here and a patient perspective is available here.

      160 people participated in the discussion with an impressive 1215 tweets.

      The highlights of the tweetchat were:

      • In general, tolvaptan is still not routinely used, limited by lack of approval, concerns regarding cost-effectiveness, and patient tolerance in presymptomatic CKD.

      • This was a well-designed study but one year may be too short an interval to evaluate at given that ADPKD is a lifelong disease.

      • The benefit is mainly driven by stage 3a group. In stage 3b & 4 the change in GFR decline was between 0.78-0.81 ml/min

      • The results overall are promising for Tolvaptan particularly if it can delay ESRD by a few years to allow time for pre-emptive transplant but concerns still remain regarding patient selection, tolerance, and the haemodynamic drop in GFR mediated by the drug.

      Transcripts of the tweetchats, and curated versions as storify are available from the NephJC website.

      Interested individuals can track and join in the conversation by following @NephJC or #NephJC on twitter, liking @NephJC on facebook, signing up for the mailing list, or just visit the webpage at NephJC.com.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 10, Arthur Yin Fan commented:

      Great news! This month our article has been selected as one of ten articles for the November 2017 Atlas Awards Nominations: "Acupuncture’s Role in Solving the Opioid Epidemic: Evidence, Cost-Effectiveness, and Care Availability for Acupuncture as a Primary, Non-Pharmacologic Method for Pain Relief and Management–White Paper 2017″ (Arthur Yin Fan is the first author, and Dr.David Miller is the correspondence author, our colleague Sarah Faggert also a co-author---there are 14 authors across the United States).

      As is stated on the Elsevier Atlas Awards homepage: “Each month the Atlas Advisory Board are sent a selection of 10 articles to choose their winning Atlas article. The articles are shortlisted by Elsevier from across journal portfolios based on their potential social impact. We are delighted to present the entire monthly shortlist and congratulate the authors of the nominated articles.” While the voting is still in progress, we are still very excited to even be nominated. This marks the first time that an acupuncture article has been nominated for the Elsevier Atlas Award.You may click on the following link to take you the Elsevier Atlas Awards Nominations page: https://www.elsevier.com/connect/atlas/nominations.

      We will let you know should our article win!

      Nominations Each month the Atlas Advisory Board are sent a selection of 10 articles to choose their winning Atlas article. ELSEVIER.COM


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 04, James M Heilman commented:

      One additional comment regarding the selection of questions used for the quizes. While SG and I selected them, we did so in a random manner.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 17, Jos Verbeek commented:

      The phrase in the background of the abstract of this article that there is 'no consolidated evidence for workplace interventions to reduce or prevent neck pain' is at odds with the four Cochrane reviews that appear in the box of 'similar articles' to the right of the abstract. Given that only English language articles were included, it is unclear what this systematic review adds to the body of evidence already available.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 21, Ralph Brinks commented:

      The authors state that Hoyer and Brinks set up "a hypothetical (micro-simulated) population" with "a linear increase of diabetes incidence to age 70 years". It is not true that we performed a micro-simulation. Instead, we used analytical relations between incidence and prevalence as cited, e.g. Brinks R, 2014. The linear increase of the age-specific incidence is an approximation to the values reported in the original paper. We agree that this an approximation only.

      The key point we made is that ignoring mortality yields to biased estimates of the incidence. This point has not been - and cannot be - undermined by the authors. Thus, we strongly discourage the use of the Styblo method whenever mortality matters.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 03, Helmi BEN SAAD commented:

      Please note that the 1st author correct name is "Ben Saad H" not "Saad HB".


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Oct 29, Wichor Bramer commented:

      Dear Hilda,

      Thank you for your comments and your additional clarification via e-mail upon my request.

      From your comments and clarification I understand that the nature and differences of the ESM and DAH searches might not be clear to you, and therefore also not to the other readers of the article. Let me start by explaining this in more detail.

      The ESM searches were librarian-mediated searches created by me upon requests of researchers at our institute, Erasmus MC in Rotterdam, the Netherlands. The DAH searchers were librarian-mediated searches created by other information specialist upon requests of researchers at other academic hospitals in the Netherlands. The researchers published the outcomes of the searches as systematic reviews between January 2014 and June 2016. The topics of the reviews differ, as can be seen in table 1 in the article. Though we did not check that for all ESM and DAH reviews, the reviews were probably executed and reported according to the PRISMA standards. The review authors were responsible for the inclusion process, and the evaluation of the relevance of the retrieved articles.

      This leads to an answer to your two questions:

      1) The ESM and DAH reviews differ on topics. Therefore there is no direct comparison of two searches (at different times), which would require the exclusion of references retrieved based on a later search date. The ESM and DAH reviews are published in the same period, therefore we can conclude that the searches had been performed in the same period as well (though we did not analyze this).

      2) As the both the ESM and the DAH searches are done at the requests of researchers, it was the responsibility of the review authors, all topic experts, to assess the relevance of the retrieved references. The reviews authors were not involved in the research, neither were they informed of the inclusion of their review in this research. Inclusion was only done after the review was published, therefore to the best of our knowledge, there was no bias in the inclusion process of the ESM and DAH reviews.

      We do acknowledge that the weakness of this study is that a direct comparison is impossible, since there are different searches on different topics. We therefore have started with a follow up research in which we create three simultaneous searches on a topic for a systematic reviews by three independent information specialists. All results will then be combined for the researchers to review the retrieved references for relevance.

      We will take your comments into account by the execution and writing of that research, as we have done with your earlier comments on a previous research article (a comparison of Medline, Embase and Google Scholar) which have influenced the data analysis and reporting of this research.

      Once again thank you for your comments, and looking forward to discussing our research in the future with you.

      Kind regards, Wichor


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    2. On 2017 Oct 28, Hilda Bastian commented:

      It would be useful if the authors could provide detail on two key issues not described in the paper. The first is the method for excluding identified references that were published subsequent to the date of the original searches.

      The second is how eligibility for study inclusion was assessed for the ESM group, and by whom. This is a key outcome measure, that is also highly susceptible to bias. A method for reducing this bias, for example, would be assessment by more than one assessor independent of those conducting the searches, blinded to the search strategy by which the study had been identified.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 30, Donald Forsdyke commented:

      CRISPR SPACERS PROVIDE "JUNK" VLA RNAs

      A "peculiarity of human thinking" invokes sad head-shaking in some quarters. It is argued, not only that "the vast majority of low abundant transcripts are simply junk," but also that such junk is "simple" (1). Those led to think that junk DNA serves the organism (i.e. can under some conditions be functional and hence selectively advantageous) are labelled "determinists." They can scarcely be distinguished from "ID believers"! There is no mention of the two-decade-old view that very low abundance transcripts (VLA RNAs) represent an intracellular antibody-like repertoire, for which much evidence has since accumulated (2-4).

      For microorganisms, the CRISPR system provided a clear example of the functionality of the transcription of their spacer "junk DNA." Ledford notes that the system "adapts to, and remembers, specific genetic invaders in a similar way to how human antibodies provide long-term immunity after an infection" (5). Just as we have germline cascades of V genes that confer immunological specificity on B and T lymphocytes, so microorganisms have their germline spacers that confer a similar specificity on their RNA populations. However, the functionality of an individual spacer "sense" transcript is only tested when a virus with a specific "antisense" sequence enters the cell. Transcription is conditional. The selective advantage can only emerge when the corresponding pathogen attacks.

      Thus, the analytical problem is not so "simple" as showing by experimental DNA deletion that the transcript of a specific eukaryotic gene is functional, or as dismissively postulating a requirement for "unacceptably high birth rates." Deletion of a single human V-region gene could show no selective effect if no corresponding pathogens invaded the body. Even if there were such an invasion, other V-regions would likely be able to compensate for the deletion. Similarly, deleting a segment of "junk" DNA is unlikely to impact survival if some of the wide spectrum of alternative "junk" transcripts can compensate for this defect in the RNA antibody-like repertoire.

      1.Sverdlov E (2017) Transcribed junk remains junk if it does not acquire a selected function in evolution. BioEssays doi: 10.1002/bies.201700164. Sverdlov E, 2017

      2.Cristillo AD, Mortimer JR, Barrette IH, Lillicrap TP, Forsdyke DR (2001) Double-stranded RNA as a not-self alarm signal: to evade, most viruses purine-load their RNAs, but some (HTLV-1, Epstein-Barr) pyrimidine-load. J Theor Biol 208:475-491. Cristillo AD, 2001

      3.Forsdyke DR, Madill CA, Smith SD (2002) Immunity as a function of the unicellular state: implications of emerging genomic data. Trends Immunol 23:575-579. Forsdyke DR, 2002

      4.Forsdyke DR (2016) Evolutionary Bioinformatics. 3rd edition. Springer, New York, pp. 279-303.

      5.Ledford H (2017) Five big mysteries about CRISPR’s origins. Nature 541:280-282. Ledford H, 2017


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2018 Jan 13, Randi Pechacek commented:

      Kaisa Koskinen, first author of this paper, wrote a blog post on microBEnet explaining some background to the research.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 06, Alexander Kraev commented:

      This is an interesting case of active nescience, because both authors were aware, even before their work started, of the fact that the PLN transgenic strain they used had a peculiar genomic structure that resulted in the presence of two resident wildtype PLN gene copies and 13 transgenic mutant (R9C) PLN gene copies under the control of alpha-MHC promoter. This situation can never, even theoretically, occur in a human patient (https://doi.org/10.1101/075671). Yet, they chose to compare it to another transgenic strain, expressing another gene with the same promoter cassette, wherein the transgene structure and genomic location are unknown. The authors state that "Although these mice are DCM models, their etiology are different from each other", which is based on the assumption that different genes are used in the two transgenes. The original descriptions of the transgenes both lack data on transgene localization and also lack the minimally necessary controls, such as an analysis of two or more transgenic lines produced with the same DNA construct. The readers should interpret the data in this paper with extreme caution, since some or all of the "universality in failing hearts" found between the two strains may stem from the use of the common expression cassette, and/or its genomic location. However, if these data are ever validated by the use of a more advanced transgenic method, this paper would serve as a good reference of the extent of conceptual misdirection the lack of transgene genomic characterization can induce.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 18, Amos Bairoch commented:

      It is a very bad idea to call this cell line "KB". KB is a very well known HeLa contaminated cell line established in 1955 and distributed by many cell collections. See: https://web.expasy.org/cellosaurus/CVCL_0372


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Oct 29, David Keller commented:

      No placebo control, blinding or randomization, nor confidence intervals for the purported benefits

      Patients with Parkinson disease (PD) are uniquely susceptible to the placebo effect due to the role of dopamine in expectation effects. Studies which do not include placebo control, double-blinding and randomization of subjects to therapy, cannot claim to provide more than anecdotal data.

      Medical cannabis has been legalized in many states, but remains on Schedule 1 as an illegal drug at the national level. Because the commercial cultivation of cannabis is also illegal, most is supplied by underworld criminal sources. High levels of herbicides such as paraquat have been measured in marijuana, exposure to which is a risk factor for PD. I would insist on cannabis grown under safe legal conditions before risking exposure of PD patients to unknown levels of herbicides.

      It is difficult to imagine how an intoxicant like cannabis, which impairs so many neurological functions, could reduce falls in PD patients. PD patients are also susceptible to dementia, and the long-term dementia risk of cannabis intoxication is not known.

      As a clinician, I cannot at this time place any credence in these reported benefits of cannabis for the treatment of PD.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Dec 24, David Keller commented:

      Treating pain versus preventing addiction - finding a balance

      In their Viewpoint editorial, titled "Underlying Factors in Drug Overdose Deaths", Dowell and colleagues use the term "prescription opioids" to refer to all opioids that can be obtained by prescription, without distinguishing how they were, in fact, obtained. This nomenclature conflates two very distinct and different categories of opioid users: pain patients taking opioids prescribed by their physician, and persons who abuse opioids without medical indication or supervision. The latter should be designated "abusers of prescription opioids" to avoid creating a false impression of elevated risk from the proper use of prescribed opioids.

      The Figure showing alarming increases in "prescription opioid overdose deaths" does not reveal what fraction of these deaths occur in persons who obtain prescription opioid medication by illegal means, such as buying or stealing pills from acquaintances or drug dealers, again inflating the risk of death from the proper use of prescribed opioids.

      Overdose deaths should not be reported as "related to prescription opioids" when coexisting levels of alcohol, benzodiazepines, heroin or other illegal narcotics are found in the blood. Deaths from polysubstance abuse creates a falsely high impression of the risk of prescribed opioids when taken as directed.

      Doctors know full well that "unnecessary exposure to prescription opioids" is to be avoided, but would the crisis of "prescription opioid overdose deaths" end, even if all "prescription opioids" were outlawed completely? Hint: heroin overdoses did not end when heroin was made illegal. Overdoses of "prescription opioids" by drug abusers will not end, regardless of how diligently doctors reduce the therapeutic use of opioids.

      Pain patients are questioned as if they were criminals for needing strong pain medicine, by doctors wary of the legal and regulatory risk of "excessive prescribing" of opioids. Under-treatment of pain becomes inevitable, because clinicians lack time for the extensive documentation, discussions, warnings, contracts, testing, database queries and other precautions which are advised with each opioid prescription.


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.

    1. On 2017 Nov 23, Franck Ramus commented:

      There is at least one more problem with this study that Mark Seidenberg did not point out in his critique (http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=35144). It is that the two groups were tested sequentially: first the 30 controls, then the 30 dyslexics. This is generally avoided in experimental psychology (and biology as well), because this is so prone to experimenter bias (unwittingly influencing differentially the members of each group), as well as to apparatus bias (e.g., apparatus parameters changing at some point, making these parameters confounded with group membership).


      This comment, imported by Hypothesis from PubMed Commons, is licensed under CC BY.